The Languages and Linguistics of Indigenous North America: A Comprehensive Guide, Vol 1 9783110600926, 9783110597981

This handbook provides broad coverage of the languages indigenous to North America, with special focus on typologically

145 75 119MB

English Pages 767 [769] Year 2023

Report DMCA / Copyright

DOWNLOAD PDF FILE

Table of contents :
Preface
Table of contents
List of North American families, languages, and dialects
Maps
I Sounds and sound structure
1 Acoustic phonetics
2 Articulatory phonetics
3 Tone
4 Segmental phonology
5 Prosodic morphology
6 Word prosody
7 Prosody beyond the word
II Words
8 What is a word?
9 Word classes
III Sentences
10 Syntax within the clause
11 Negatives
12 Questions and requests in North American languages
13 Information structure
14 Clause-combining: Relative clauses
15 Clause combining: Syntax of subordination and complementation
16 Switch-reference and event cohesion
IV Discourse
17 Verbal art
18 Conversation structure
V Meaning
19 Lexicalization and lexical meaning
20 Lexicography
21 Evidentiality
22 Pluractionality and distributivity
23 Mass and count nouns
24 Sense of place: Space, landscape, and orientation
25 A sense of time and world
26 Pragmatics
VI Languages over space and time
27 Languages as dynamic systems: How grammar can emerge
28 Language contact and linguistic areas
29 Language classification
30 Archival-based sociolinguistic variation
31 Community-based sociolinguistic variation
Recommend Papers

The Languages and Linguistics of Indigenous North America: A Comprehensive Guide, Vol 1
 9783110600926, 9783110597981

  • 0 0 0
  • Like this paper and download? You can publish your own PDF file online for free in a few minutes! Sign Up
File loading please wait...
Citation preview

The Languages and Linguistics of Indigenous North America WOL 13.1

The World of Linguistics

Editor Hans Henrich Hock

Volume 13.1

The Languages and Linguistics of Indigenous North America A Comprehensive Guide Volume 1 Edited by Carmen Dagostino, Marianne Mithun, and Keren Rice

ISBN 978-3-11-059798-1 e-ISBN (PDF) 978-3-11-060092-6 e-ISBN (EPUB) 978-3-11-059869-8   Library of Congress Control Number: 2023932794   Bibliographic information published by the Deutsche Nationalbibliothek The Deutsche Nationalbibliothek lists this publication in the Deutsche Nationalbibliografie; detailed bibliographic data are available on the Internet at http://dnb.d-nb.de.   © 2023 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston   Cover image: jmatkins / iStock / Getty Images Plus Typesetting: Dörlemann Satz GmbH & Co. KG, Lemförde Printing and binding: CPI books GmbH, Leck www.degruyter.com



Preface 

 V

Preface This handbook is intended to provide broad coverage of topics of interest to linguists in general, and more specifically to community and academic scholars engaged in the study and revitalization of North American Indigenous languages. Particular attention has been given to new ideas and recent trends in research, to features of the languages that are typologically unusual or unusually well-developed in comparison with others outside of the area, and topics of special importance to communities. The general chapters include broad cross-linguistic coverage of each area and surveys of current work on the topic, as well as points that may be useful for language revitalization purposes. Many address topics that received less attention in earlier accounts, such as syntax, discourse, language change, and contact effects. Given the current blossoming of community-centered research (Bischoff and Jany 2018) and the formation of ever greater numbers of Indigenous linguists, the editors would like to ensure that this work is of value to the communities involved in language maintenance and revitalization. The volume is divided into two main parts, the first on general topics, and the second on revitalization and sketches of languages and families. Volume  1 describes different levels of structure: sounds and sound structures (acoustic phonetics, articulatory phonetics, tone, segmental phonology, prosodic phonology, word prosody, prosody beyond the word), words (identifying words, word classes), sentences (syntax within the clause, syntax beyond the clause, negation, questions and requests, information structure, relative clauses, subordination and complementation, switch reference and event cohesion), discourse (verbal art, conversation structure), and meaning (lexicalization and lexical meaning, lexicography, evidentiality, pluractionality and distributivity, mass versus count nouns, space, landscape, and orientation) and pragmatics. Following that are sections on language over time and space (how grammar emerges, language classification, language contact and linguistic areas, archival-based sociolinguistic variation, and community-based sociolinguistic variation). Volume 2 contains sections devoted to topics of importance in language acquisition and revitalization (outcomes of Mentor-Apprentice programs, child and child-directed speech, language pedagogies, digital tools for revitalization, the use of archival materials for language reclamation, and changing notions of fieldwork), followed by sketches of families and isolates. The geographic area over which the languages are spoken is extensive, extending from the Arctic in the north to the US border with Mexico in the south although some include Mexico in their definition of this area (Siddiqi, Barrie, Gillon, Haugen, and Mathieu 2020). This is the area traditionally covered in works on North American languages, such as Boas’ Handbook of North American Indian languages (1911, 1922), Voegelin and Voegelin’s Languages of the World: Native America fascicle one, Languages of the world: Native America fascicle two (1964, 1965), Campbell and Mithun’s The Languages of Native North America: A Historical and Comparative Assessment (1979), Campbell’s American Indian Languages: The Historical Linguistics of Native America (1997), Goddard’s Handbook of North American Indians 17: Languages (1996), and Mithun’s The https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-201

VI 

 Preface

Languages of Native North America (1999), as well as the Routledge Handbook of North American Languages (2020) edited by Siddiqi et al. In general, we have opted for a greater number of shorter chapters rather than fewer longer ones, with the goal of covering as many relevant topics as possible while striving for user friendliness, though we recognize that the chapters necessarily vary somewhat in their accessibility and interest to different audiences. We were somewhat selective about languages and families, and if we did not include your favorite one, we hope that you will excuse us. It is an exciting time, with knowledge and ideas constantly evolving.

References Bischoff, Shannon T. & Carmen Jany (eds.). 2018. Insights from Practices in Community-Based Research. (Trends in Linguistics Studies and Monographs 319). Berlin/Boston: De Gruyter Mouton. Boas, Franz (ed.). 1911. Handbook of the American Indian languages, Part 1. (Bureau of American Ethnology Bulletin 40). Washington. Boas, Franz (ed.). 1922. Handbook of American Indian languages Part 2. (Bureau of American Ethnology 40). Washington. Campbell, Lyle. 1997. American Indian Languages: The Historical Linguistics of Native America. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Campbell, Lyle & Marianne Mithun (eds.). 1979. The Languages of Native America: A Historical and Comparative Assessment. Austin: University of Texas Press. Goddard, Ives (ed.). 1996. Handbook of North American Indians Volume 17: Languages, 137–157. Washington: Smithsonian Institution. Mithun, Marianne. 1999/2001. The Languages of Native North America. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Siddiqi, Daniel, Michael Barrie, Carrie Gillon, Jason Haugen & Eric Matthieu. 2020. Routledge Handbook of North American Languages. London and New York: Routledge. Voegelin, Carl F. & Florence M. Voegelin. 1964. Languages of the World: Native America fascicle one (Anthropological Linguistics). Vol. 6. Bloomington, IN: Anthropology Department, Indiana University. Voegelin, Carl F. & Florence M. Voegelin. 1965. Languages of the World: Native America fascicle two (Anthropological Linguistics). Vol. 7. Bloomington, IN: Anthropology Department, Indiana University.

Table of contents Volume 1 Preface    V List of North American families, languages, and dialects  Maps   XLI

I Sounds and sound structure Sonya Bird, Rae Anne Claxton, and Tess Nolan 1 Acoustic phonetics   3 Heather Bliss, Sonya Bird, and Bryan Gick 2 Articulatory phonetics   39 Hiroto Uchihara 3 Tone   63 Colleen M. Fitzgerald and Matthew K. Gordon 4 Segmental phonology   89 Suzanne Urbanczyk 5 Prosodic morphology  Matthew K. Gordon 6 Word prosody 

 109

 135

Siri G. Tuttle 7 Prosody beyond the word 

II Words Fernando Zúñiga 8 What is a word?  Daniel W. Hieber 9 Word classes 

 205

 183

 155

 XIII

VIII 

 Table of contents

III Sentences George Aaron Broadwell 10 Syntax within the clause 

 247

Elly van Gelderen 11 Negatives   267 Olga Lovick 12 Questions and requests in North American languages  Anna Berge 13 Information structure 

 283

 305

Tim Thornes 14 Clause-combining: Relative clauses 

 323

Amy Dahlstrom 15 Clause combining: Syntax of subordination and complementation  Andrew McKenzie 16 Switch-reference and event cohesion 

 363

IV Discourse Anthony K. Webster 17 Verbal art   385 Olivia N. Sammons 18 Conversation structure 

 421

V Meaning Sally Rice 19 Lexicalization and lexical meaning  Sally Rice 20 Lexicography 

 479

 453

 345



Tyler Peterson 21 Evidentiality 

Table of contents 

 497

Robert Henderson 22 Pluractionality and distributivity  Andrea Wilhelm 23 Mass and count nouns 

 511

 527

Gary Holton and Andrea L. Berez-Kroeker 24 Sense of place: Space, landscape, and orientation  Sihwei Chen and Lisa Matthewson 25 A sense of time and world 

 547

 577

Elizabeth Bogal-Allbritten 26 Pragmatics   599

VI Languages over space and time Marianne Mithun 27 Languages as dynamic systems: How grammar can emerge  Sarah Thomason 28 Language contact and linguistic areas  Hannah J. Haynie 29 Language classification 

 647

 669

Justin Spence 30 Archival-based sociolinguistic variation  Kayla Palakurthy 31 Community-based sociolinguistic variation 

 689

 701

 619

 IX

X 

 Table of contents

Volume 2 Preface  

 V

VII Language revitalization Onowa McIvor, Peter Jacobs, and Barbara Jenni 32 Reviving languages: Outcomes of a Mentor-Apprentice style learning study   719 Melvatha R. Chee and Ryan E. Henke 33 Child and child-directed speech in North American languages 

 741

Kari A. B. Chew, Wesley Y. Leonard, and Daisy Rosenblum 34 Decolonizing Indigenous language pedagogies: Additional language learning and teaching   767 Ashleigh Surma and Christina L. Truong 35 Digital tools for language revitalization 

 789

Megan Lukaniec 36 Using archival materials for language reclamation 

 807

Lorna Wanosts’a7 Williams and Ewa Czaykowska-Higgins 37 Changing notions of fieldwork   823

VIII Language families and isolates Richard Compton 38 Inuit-Yupik-Unangan: An overview of the language family  Leslie Saxon 39 Dene – Athabaskan  Will Oxford 40 Algonquian  Nicole Rosen 41 Michif 

 951

 931

 875

 843



Table of contents 

Clarissa Forbes 42 Tsimshianic 

 985

T̕łat̕łaḵuł Patricia Rosborough and Daisy Rosenblum 43 Wakashan Languages   1013 Honoré Watanabe 44 Salish   1053 Philip T. Duncan, Valerie (Lamxayat) Switzler, and Henry B. Zenk 45 Chinookan family, with special reference to Kiksht and notes on Chinuk Wawa   1115 Joana Jansen 46 Sahaptian 

 1139

Andrew Garrett, Susan Gehr, Erik Hans Maier, Line Mikkelsen, Crystal Richardson, and Clare S. Sandy 47 Karuk   1169 M. Ryan Bochnak, Emily A. Hanink, and Alan Chi Lun Yu 48 Wáˑšiw   1201 Eugene Buckley 49 Pomoan   1223 Carmen Dagostino 50 California languages: Isolates and other languages  Timothy P. Henry-Rodriguez 51 Chumashan   1275 Amy Miller 52 Yuman 

 1303

Eric Elliott and David Leedom Shaul 53 Uto-Aztecan   1333 Logan Sutton 54 Kiowa-Tanoan 

 1361

 1247

 XI

XII 

 Table of contents

Logan Sutton and Armik Mirzayan 55 Caddoan   1407 Armik Mirzayan 56 Sketch of the Siouan Language Family  Daniel W. Hieber 57 Chitimacha 

 1519

Judith M. Maxwell and Patricia Anderson 58 Tunica   1545 Jack B. Martin 59 Muskogean 

 1577

Marianne Mithun and Ryan DeCaire 60 Iroquoian   1601 Raoul Zamponi 61 Unclassified languages 

 1627

 1649 List of Authors  Index of languages and varieties Index of names and subjects

 1447

List of North American families, languages, and dialects The following is a list of families, languages, and dialects mentioned in the handbook. It is not an exhaustive list of all North American languages and dialects, but just those mentioned in the various chapters. Only North American languages are included here. The list includes the names as they appear in the chapters, in addition to any alternate names and spellings. While we do not intend to perpetuate misnomers and misspellings, we opted to include names that appear in major publications and those still in use so that relevant resources can be identified. We recognize that these names are likely going to change over time. For some languages and dialects, multiple names are widely used, and rather than choosing a single main entry, we decided to include each name as a main entry. Names that include cardinal directions, toponyms, etc. are listed once and cross-referenced (e.g., Apache, Plains -> See: Plains Apache). The third column reflects the genetic affiliation of particular languages and dialects. We chose to list the language family, as well as major branches for some of the larger families. If a name refers to a family, it is noted as such. Families, languages, dialects

Alternate names and spellings

Language family (branch)

‘Wuik̓ala

‘Uik̓ala, Ooweekyala, Oowekyala, Oweek’ala, Oweke(e)no

Wakashan (North Wakashan/ Kwakiutlan)

’Iipay

Ipai; Diegueño; Varieties: Mesa Grande, Barona, Santa Ysabel, Iñaja-Cosmit, San Pasqual

Yuman

’Nak̓wala

Dialect of Kwak’wala

Wakashan (North Wakashan/ Kwakiutlan)

’Uik̓ala

Wakashan (North Wakashan/ Kwakiutlan)

Abenaki, Eastern

See: Eastern Abenaki

Abenaki, Western

See: Western Abenaki

Achumawi

Achomawi

Palaihnihan

Acoma

Keresan

Adai

Isolate

Ahtna

Atna, Atnakenaege’, Copper River, Mednovskiy

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Akokisa

Accokesaws, Arkokisa, Orcoquiza

Unclassified

https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-202

XIV 

 List of North American families, languages, and dialects

Families, languages, dialects

Alternate names and spellings

Alabama

Alibamu, Albaamaha, Albama, Alabamer, Muskogean Alabamu, Alaba, Albaamo, Aibamo, Aybamo, Halbama, Holbama, Alebamah, Alebamon, Albaamo innaałiilka, Limanu

Aleut

Anangax

Algic

Language family (branch)

Inuit-Yupik-Unangan Algic family

Algonquian

Algonkian

Branch of Algic

Algonquin

Anishinàbemiwin; Variety of Ojibwe

Algic (Algonquian)

Almosan

Hypothetical stock: Almosan (Algonquian-Wakashan)

Alsea

Isolate

Alutiiq

Alutiiq Alaskan Yupik; Pacific Gulf Yupik

Inuit-Yupik-Unangan

Amerind

Hypothetical superstock

Amotomanco

Unclassified

Anishininimowin

Oji-Cree

Apache, Jicarrilla

See: Jicarilla Apache

Apache, Lipan

See: Lipan Apache

Apache, Plains

See: Plains Apache

Apache, San Carlos

See: San Carlos Apache

Apache, Western

See: Western Apache

Apachean

Apache

Algic (Algonquian)

Subbranch of Na-Dene (Dene)

Apalachee

Muskogean

Apalachee-Spanish pidgin

Pidgin (Muskogean/ Indo-European)

Aranama

Anames, Jaranames, Juranames, Xaramenes

Unclassified

Arapaho

Hinónoʼeitíít

Algic (Algonquian)

Arikara Arizona Tewa

Caddoan Tewa, Hopi-Tewa

Asiatic Eskimo

Kiowa-Tanoan Inuit-Yupik-Unangan

Atakapa

Yukhiti

Isolate

Athabaskan

Athabascan, Athapascan, Athapaskan, Athabaskan, Dene

Branch of Na-Dene



Families, languages, dialects

List of North American families, languages, and dialects 

Alternate names and spellings

Ati Piman

 XV

Language family (branch) Uto-Aztecan (Southern, Tepiman)

Atikamekw

Cree, Attikamek, Tête de Boule, Attimewk, Atihkamekw, Atikamek, Attikamekw

Algic (Algonquian)

Atsugewi

Dialects: Hat Creek, Dixie Valley

Palaihnihan

Aztec-Tanoan

Hypothetical stock

Babine

Witsuwit’en, Nedut’en, Northern Carrier Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Baffin Inuktitut

Baffin Island Inuit, Qikiqtaaluup nigianimiutut

Bannock Barbareño Chumash

Inuit-Yupik-Unangan Uto-Aztecan (Numic)

šmuwič, Barbareño

Bay Miwok

Chumashan (Southern, Central) Utian (Miwokan)

Bayogoula

Bayagola, Bayagoula, Baya-Ogoula, Bayugla, Bayuk-okla

Unclassified

Bearlake

Sahtúot’i̜ ne yatǐ̜ , Dene, North Slavey

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Bella Coola

nuxalk

Salishan

Bidai

Beadeye, Bedias, Bidey, Viday

Unclassified

Biloxi

Siouan-Catawban (Siouan, Ohio Valley/Southeastern)

Bitterroot Salish

Seliš, Flathead, Montana Salish

Salishan

Blackfoot

Pikanii, Siksiká, Siksika, Blackfeet, Niitsipowahsin, Pied Noir

Algic (Algonquian)

Caddo

Hasinay; Varieties: Hainai, Kadohadacho, Natchitoches, Yatasi

Caddoan

Caddoan

Caddoan family

Cahita

Uto-Aztecan (Yoemian/Cahitan)

Cahuilla

Ivilyuat; Dialects: Desert, Pass, Mountain Uto-Aztecan (Takic, Cupan)

Calusa

Calloosa, Caloosa, Caloose, Calos, Carlos Unclassified

Campo

See: Northeastern Kumeyaay

Carrier

Dakelh; varieties: Cheslatta, Lheidli, Saik'uz, Stuart Lake

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Cascades

Dialect of Upper Chinook, Watlala

Chinookan

Catawba

Siouan-Catawban (Catawban)

XVI 

 List of North American families, languages, and dialects

Families, languages, dialects

Alternate names and spellings

Language family (branch)

Cayuga

Gayogo̱hó꞉nǫʼ

Iroquoian (Northern)

Cayuse

Unclassified

Central Alaskan Yup’ik

Yugtun, Central Alaskan Yupik, Central Yupik, Yupik, Yupʼik, Yugtun, Yugcestun

Inuit-Yupik-Unangan

Central Pomo

Varieties: Hopland/Shanel, Yokaya/ Ukiah, Point Arena-Manchester/Boya, Anderson Valley

Pomoan

Central Siberian Yupik

St. Lawrence Island Yupik

Inuit-Yupik-Unangan

Chalon

Utian (Costanoan/Ohlone)

Chehalis, Lower

See: Lower Chehalis

Chehalis, Upper

See: Upper Chehalis

Chemakum

Chimakum, Chimacum

Chimakuan

Chemehuevi

Southern Paiute

Uto-Aztecan (Numic)

Cherokee

Jalagi, Tsalagi, ᏣᎳᎩ

Iroquoian (Southern)

Cherokee, Western

See: Western Cherokee

Cheyenne

Tsėhésenėstsestȯtse, Tsitsistas

Algic (Algonquian)

Chickasaw

Chikashshanompa’

Muskogean

Chico Maidu

Maiduan

Chikashshanompa’

Chickasaw

Muskogean

Chilcotin

Tsinlhqut’in, Tsilhqut’in, Tsilhqot’in, Tsilhqút’ín

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Chimariko

Varieties: New River (Chimalakwe), Trinity River, South Fork

Isolate

Chinook

Chinookan; Languages: Lower Chinook/ Chinookan family Coastal Chinook, Kathlamet/Katlamat /Cathlamet, Upper Chinook/Kiksht/ Columbia Chinook

Chinook Jargon

Chinook Wawa, Chinuk Wawa, Chinook Jargon, Wawa, Jargon, Chinook

Pidgin

Chinook Proper

Chinook, Tsinuk, Shoalwater Chinook

Chinookan

Chinook, Lower

See: Lower Chinook

Chinook, Upper

See: Upper Chinook

Chinuk Wawa

Chinook Wawa, Chinook Jargon, Wawa

Pidgin



List of North American families, languages, and dialects 

 XVII

Families, languages, dialects

Alternate names and spellings

Language family (branch)

Chipewyan

Dëne, Dene, Dené, Dene yatié, Dënesųłiné, Dënesųłiné Yatié, Dënësųłinë,́ Dëne Súline, Dene Sǫłiné, Denesųłiné, Denesuline, Dene Súline, Dene Sounlhine, Dene Soun'line, Dëne Dédlıné, Tetsǫ́t’ıné, Montagnais

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Chiricahua Apache

Na-Dene (Dene, Apachean)

Chitimacha

Isolate

Chiwere

Baxoje-Jiwere (includes Ioway, Oto, and Missouria)

Siouan-Catawban (Siouan, Mississippi Valley)

Choctaw

Chahta, Chahta Anumpa

Muskogean

Chumashan

Chumashan family

Clackamas

Clackamas Chinook, Clawiwalla; dialect of Upper Chinook

Chinookan

Clallam

Klallam, Na’klallam, S’klallam, N@ xʷsň’ay’@múc@n, nəxʷsƛʼayʼəmʼúcən

Salishan

Classical Nahuatl

Aztec

Uto-Aztecan

Clatsop

Klatsop, Tlatsop, Chinook, Tsinuk

Chinookan

Coahuilteco

Isolate

Coast Miwok

Utian (Miwokan)

Coast Tsimshian

Tsmksian, Tsimshian, Coast Tsimshian, Sm’algya̲x; dialect of Lower Tsimshianic

Cocopa

See: Cocopah, Cucapá

Cocopah

Kwapá, Kwikapa, Cocopa, Arizona Cocopa

Yuman

Coeur d’Alene

Snchitsu’umshtsn (snčícuʔumšcn)

Salishan (Interior Salish, Southern)

Columbian

Moses-Columbia, nxaʔamxcín (Varieties: Salishan (Interior Salish, Columbia, Wenatchi) Southern)

Colville-Okanagan

Nsyilxcən; Varieties: Northern/Lakes, Southern/Colville

Salishan (Interior Salish, Southern)

Comanche

Nʉmʉ Tekwapʉ

Uto-Aztecan (Numic)

Comecrudo Comox

Tsimshianic

Comecrudan Comox-Sliammon; Varieties: Comox (Island); Sliammon-Homalco-Klahoose (Mainland)

Salishan (Coast Salish)

XVIII 

 List of North American families, languages, and dialects

Families, languages, dialects

Alternate names and spellings

Language family (branch)

Comox-Sliammon

Comox, ʔayʔaǰuθəm; Ay-Ay-Ju-­ ThumEy7a7juuthem; Varieties: Comox (Island); Sliammon-Homalco-Klahoose (Mainland)

Salishan (Coast Salish)

Congaree Coosan

Unclassified Coos

Coosan family

Cora(n)

Uto-Aztecan (Southern, Coracholan-Nahuan)

Cotoname

Isolate

Cowlitz

ƛʼpúlmixq, sƛʼpúlmš

Cree

Salishan (Tsamosan) Algic (Algonquian)

Cree, Plains

See: Plains Cree

Cree, Swampy

See: Swampy Cree

Cree, Woods

See: Woods Cree

Creek

Muskogee, Muscogee, Mvskoke, Seminole

Muskogean

Crow

Apsáalooke, Apsaáloka

Siouan-Catawban (Siouan)

Cruzeño

Island Chumash

Chumashan (Southern)

Cucapá

Kuapá, Cucupá

Yuman

Cup’ik

Variety of Central Alaskan Yupik, Cup’ig, Yup’ikm Yup’ik, Yupic

Inuit-Yupik-Unangan

Cupan

Branch of Uto-Aztecan

Uto-Aztecan

Cupeño

Uto-Aztecan (Takic, Cupan)

Cusabo

Cusabe, Cusabee, Cusaboe, Coosaboys, Corsaboy

Unclassified

Dakelh

Varieties: Cheslatta, Lheidli, S̱aik'uẕ, Stuart Lake

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Dakota

Dakhótiyapi, Dakȟótiyapi, Santee-Sisseton, Sioux, Dakhota, Santee

Siouan-Catawban (Siouan)

Dakota, Stoney

See: Stoney Dakota

Dakotan

Očhéthi Šakówiŋ - “Seven Council Fires” Siouan-Catawban (Siouan, (Dakota ~ Dakhota (Santee-Sisseton), Mississippi Valley) Bdewákhaŋthuŋwaŋ, Waȟpékhute, Waȟpéthuŋwaŋ, Sisíthuŋwaŋ, Dakota ~ Dakhota (Yankton-Yanktonai), Iháŋktȟuŋwaŋ, Iháŋktȟuwaŋna, Lakota ~ Lakhota ~ Lakȟóta), Nakota ~ Nakhota ~ Assiniboine, Nakoda ~ Stoney



List of North American families, languages, and dialects 

 XIX

Families, languages, dialects

Alternate names and spellings

Language family (branch)

Dane-zaa

Beaver

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Danezāgé’

Denek’éh, Kaska

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Deg Xinag

Deg Hit’an

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Delaware

Two languages: Munsee, Lenape

Algic (Algonquian)

Dena’ina

Tanaina

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Dene

Term used as an alternative for Athabas­ Na-Dene (Dene, Northern) kan family; also used for language varieties spoken in Mackenzie Valley regions and elsewhere. Also: Slavey, Slave, Chipewyan

Dëne

Dene, Dené, Dene yatié, Dënesųłiné, Na-Dene (Dene, Northern) Dënesųłiné Yatié, Dënësųłinë,́ Dëne Súline, Dene Sǫłiné, Denesųłiné, Denesuline, Dene Súline, Dene Sounlhine, Dene Soun'line, Dëne Dédlıné, ­Tetsǫ́t’ıné, Montagnais, Chipewyan

Dëne Dédliné

Variety of Chipewyan, Dëne, Dene Sųłiné Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Dene Ké

Two languages: Slave or Kaska

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Denek’éh

Kaska, Danezāgé’

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Dene Sųłiné

Dëne, Dené, Dene yatié, Dënesųłiné, Na-Dene (Dene, Northern) Dënesųłiné Yatié, Dënësųłinë,́ Dëne Súline, Dene Sǫłiné, Denesųłiné, Denesuline, Dene Súline, Dene Sounlhine, Dene Soun'line, Dëne Dédlıné, ­Tetsǫ́t’ıné, Montagnais, Chipewyan

Dene-Dinjii

Term used for languages of the Macken- Na-Dene (Dene, Northern) zie Valley regions, including Gwich’in

Dene-Yeniseian

Hypothetical stock: Dene-Yeniseian

Dhegihan

Languages: Omaha – Ponca, Omaha ~ UmoNhOn, Osage, Kansa ~ Kaw, Quapaw

Sub-branch of Siouan-Catawban (Siouan, Mississippi Valley)

Diegueño

See: ’Iipay, Jamul Tiipay, La Huerta, Los Conejos, Northeastern Kumeyaay

diitiidʔaaʔtx̣

Ditidaht, Nitinaht, Nitinat, Ditidaht-Pacheedaht

Wakashan (South Wakashan, Nootkan)

Diné bizaad

Diné, Navajo, Navaho

Na-Dene (Dene, Apachean)

Ditidaht

Nitinaht

Wakashan (South Wakashan/ Nootkan)

XX 

 List of North American families, languages, and dialects

Families, languages, dialects

Alternate names and spellings

Language family (branch)

Dogrib

Tłı̨ chǫ, Tłı̨ chǫ Yatıì, Tlicho, Tlinchon, Doné, Thlingcha-dinneh, Doné

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

East Cree

iiyiyuu ayimuun, iiyiyiuyimuwin, iinuuay- Algic (Algonquian) imuwin, iiyiyuuayimuwin

Eastern Abenaki

Penobscot, Caniba

Eastern Pomo

Kulanapo; Varieties: Habematolel/Upper Pomoan Lake, Big Valley, Robinson Rancheria

Algic (Algonquian)

Erie

Iroquoian (Northern)

Eskimo-Aleut

Inuit-Yupik-Unangan family

Esselen

Huelel

Isolate

Eudeve

Dohema

Uto-Aztecan (Opatan)

Eurasiatic

Hypothetical stock: Eurasiatic

Eyak

dAXunhyuuga’

Na-Dene

Fort Ware Sekani

Kwadacha Tsek’ene

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Fox

Mesquakie (Meskwaki), Mesquakie-Sauk, Mesquakie-Sauk-Kickapoo, Sauk-Fox

Algic (Algonquian)

Gitksan

Gitxsan, Gitxsen, Gitxsanimaax, Giatikshan, Gityskyan, Gitxsanimx̲, Gitxsanimax̲, Gitxsenimx̲, Gitsenimx̲, Gyaanimx̲, Nass-Gitksan; dialect of Nass-Gitksan

Tsimshianic

Greenlandic

Greenlandic Kalaallisut; dialects: Kalaal- Inuit-Yupik-Unangan lisut/West Greenlandic; Tunumiisut/East Greenlandic; Inuktun; Thule

Greenlandic, West

See: West Greenlandic

Gros Ventre

Ananin, Ahahnelin, Ahe, A’ani, Atsina

Algic (Algonquian)

Guale

Unclassified

Guarijio

Uto-Aztecan (Southern, Tarahumaran)

Gwich’in

Kutchin, Loucheux, Takudh, Tukidh, Tukudh, Dinju Zhuh K’yuu

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Haida

Haidah, Xaad Kil, X̱aat Kíl, X̱aadas Kíl, Isolate X̱aayda Kil, Xaaydaa Kil; Varieties: Skittagetan, Haidah, Tolewa Skittagits, Tahlewah Haidah, Queen Charlotte’s Island, Masset, Skidegate

Haida, Northern

See: Northern Haida



List of North American families, languages, and dialects 

 XXI

Families, languages, dialects

Alternate names and spellings

Haíɫzaqvḷa

Híɫzaqvḷa, Hailhzaqvla, Haílhzaqvḷa, Wakashan (North Wakashan/ Heiltsuk, Heiltsuq, Heiltsukv, Heiltsukvla, Kwakiutlan) Bella Bella

Halkomelem

Holkomelem, Hul’q’umi’num’, Halq̓eméylem, Hul̓q̓umín̓um̓, hən̓q̓əmin̓əm; Varieties: Chilliwack/ Upriver (halqʼəméyləm), Musqueam/Downriver (hənʼqʼəmínʼəmʼ), Cowichan/Island (həlʼqʼəminʼəmʼ)

Halkomelem, Upriver

See: Upriver Halkomelem

Hän

Han-Kutchin, Dawson, Moosehide

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Hanis

Hanis Coos

Coosan

Hare

K’áshogot’ine xedə́, Dene, North Slavey

Na-Dene (Dene)

Havasupai

Havasu Baa Gwaawa

Yuman

Heiltsuk

Haíɫzaqv

Wakashan (North Wakashan/ Kwakiutlan)

Hidatsa

Hiraacá

Siouan-Catawban (Siouan, Missouri Valley)

Híɫzaqv

Heiltsuk, Hailhzaqvla, Bella Bella, Heiltsuk-Oweek’ala, Belbellah, Heiltsuk-Oowekyala

Wakashan (North Wakashan/ Kwakiutlan)

Hinono’eitiit

Arapaho

Algic (Algonquian)

ḥiškʷiiʔatḥ

Hesquiaht (dialect of nuučaan̓uł)

Wakashan (South Wakashan/ Nootkan)

Hitchiti Hocąk

Language family (branch)

Salishan (Coast Salish)

Muskogean Hocank, Hoocągra, Hochunk, Ho-Chunk, Siouan-Catawban (Mississippi Winnebago Valley Siouan)

Hokan

Hypothetical stock: Hokan

Hokan-Siouan

Hypothetical stock: HokanSiouan

Hopi

Hopílavayi

Uto-Aztecan

Hualapai

Walapai, Hwalbáy

Yuman

Huichol Hul’q’umi’num’

Uto-Aztecan (Coracholan-­ Nahuan) Hul̓q̓umín̓um̓; Halkomelem; Island Halkomelem

Salishan (Coast)

XXII 

 List of North American families, languages, and dialects

Families, languages, dialects

Alternate names and spellings

Language family (branch)

Hupa

Hoopa, Na꞉tinixwe Mixine꞉wheʼ

Na-Dene (Dene, Pacific Coast)

Ichishkíin, Yakima

See: Yakima Ichishkíin

Innu

Montagnais, Innu-aimun

Algic (Algonquian)

Inuinnaqtun

Western Canadian Inuktun, Kangiryuarmiutun

Inuit-Yupik-Unangan

Inuit

Inuinnaqtun; Inuktitut; Inuktun; Inuttitut; Inuttut; Inuvialuktun

Inuit-Yupik-Unangan

Inuit-Yupik-Unangan

Eskimo-Aleut, Eskaleut

Inuit-Yupik-Unangan family

Inuktitut

Inuit, ᐃᓄᒃᑎᑐᑦ, Eastern Canadian Inuktitut, Inuktitut, Inuttitut, Inuttut

Inuit-Yupik-Unangan

Inuktitut, North Baffin

See: North Baffin Inukitut

Inuktitut, Tarramiut

See: Tarramiut Inukitut

Inupiaq

Iñupiaq, Iñupiatun, Inupik, Inupiak, Inupiat, North Alaskan Inuktitut, North Alaskan Inupiaq, North Alaskan Inupiat, North Alaskan, Inupiatun, Northwest Alaska Inupiat, Seward Peninsula Inupiaq

Inuit-Yupik-Unangan

Inuvialuktun

Western Canadian Inuktun, Inuktun

Inuit-Yupik-Unangan

Iroquoian

Iroquoian family

Jamul Tiipay

Jamul Diegueño, Tipai, Kumeyaay

Yuman

Ja’a

Tipey Aa, Tipei, Ha’a, Kumiay, Kumiai, Kumeyaay

Yuman

Jicarilla Apache

Na-Dene (Dene, Apachean)

Jova

Uto-Aztecan

Kalaallisut

West Greenlandic, Greenlandic

Kalapuyan

Inuit-Yupik-Unangan Kapaluyan family

Kalispel

Spokane

Salishan (Interior Salish, Southern)

Kanien’kéha’

Mohawk, Kanyen’kéha

Iroquoian (Northern)

Kansa

Kaw, Dhegiha Siouan

Siouan-Catawban (Siouan)

Kanyen’kéha

Mohawk, Kanien’kéha’

Iroquoian (Northern )

Karankawa

Isolate

Karkin

Utian (Costanoan/Ohlone)

Karuk

Karok

Isolate



List of North American families, languages, and dialects 

 XXIII

Families, languages, dialects

Alternate names and spellings

Language family (branch)

Kashaya Pomo

Kashaya, Kashia, Southwestern Pomo (obsolete); Varieties: Stewart’s Point, Fort Ross, Haupt Ranch

Pomoan

K’áshogot’ine xedə́

Hare dialect of North Slavey, variety of Mackenzie Valley Dene

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Kaska

Denek’éh, Dene K’é, Danezāgé’

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Kathlamet

Katlamat, Cathlamet, Wakaikam,

Chinookan

Kato

Cahto

Na-Dene (Dene, Pacific Coast)

Kaw

Siouan

Kawaiisu

Uto-Aztecan (Numic)

Keres

Keresan

Isolate

Keresiouan

Hypothetical stock: Keresiouan

Kickapoo

Algic (Algonquian)

Kiksht

Upper Chinook

Chinookan

Kiliwa

Kiliwi, Koleeu, Ko’lew, Quiligua

Yuman

Kiowa

Cáuijògà/Cáuijò:gyà, Cáuígú, [Gáuigú, kɔ́ygú

Kiowa-Tanoan

Kiowa-Tanoan

Kiowa-Tanoan family

Kitanemuk

Uto-Aztecan (Takic/Serrano)

Kitsai

Kichai, Keechi

Caddoan

Klallam

nəxʷsƛʼáyʼəmʼucən, Clallam

Salishan (Coast Salish)

Klamath-Modoc

Varieties: Klamath, Modoc, Lutuamian

Isolate

Koasati

Coushatta, Koasáti

Muskogean (Eastern Muskogean)

Ko’alh

Ku’alh, Ko’al, Kw’aal

Yuman

Konkow

Maiduan

Konomihu

Shastan

Koyukon

Denaakk’e, Ten’a

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Ktunaxa

Kutenai, Kootenay, Ksanka, Kootenai

Isolate

Kumeyaay

See: ’Iipay, Los Conejos, Northeastern Kumeyaay, Jamul Tiipay, La Huerta, Nejí, Ja’a, Peña Blanca, San Antonio Necua, San Jose de la Zorra, San José de Tecate

Kumeyaay, Northeastern

See: Northeastern Kumeyaay

XXIV 

 List of North American families, languages, and dialects

Families, languages, dialects

Alternate names and spellings

Language family (branch)

Kutenai

Ksanka, Ktunaxa, Kootenai, Kootenay

Isolate

Kwadacha Tsek’ene

Fort Ware Sekani

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Kw’ally

kʷʔaƚʸ, Kw’ally

Yuman

Kwak’wala

Kʷak’ʷala , Kwakwala, Kwakiutl, Wakashan (North Wakashan/ Kwagu’ł, Kwaguł, Kwagulth, Kwagu’tl, Kwakiutlan) Kwakwaka’wakw, Kwakwakawakw, Bakwumk’ala, Bak’wa̱mk’ala, Bak̓ʷəmk̓ala, Kwakwaka’wakw, Kwagiutl; (Varieties: ‘Nak̕wala, Gwat̕sala, G̱ut̕sala, Kwak̓wala, Liq’wala)

Kwatsáan

Quechan, Yuma, Kwtsaan

Kyuquot

Yuman Wakashan (South Wakashan/ Nootkan)

La Huerta

Tipay, Tipai, Cuchimí, Kumiay, Kumeyaay Yuman

Laguna Keres

Dialect of Keres

Lake Miwok

Isolate Utian (Miwokan)

Lakhota

Lakota, Lakhota, Lakȟótiyapi, Lakotiyapi, Siouan-Catawban (Siouan) Teton, Teton Sioux

Laurentian

St. Laurence Iroquois

Iroquoian (Northern)

Lekwungen

lək̓ʷəŋiʔnəŋ, Songhees; Songish; Northern Straits Salish

Salishan (Coast Salish)

Lenape

Lenape Delaware

Algic (Algonquian)

Lillooet

Sƛ̓áƛ̓imxəc, St̓át̓imcets; Varieties: Upper/ Salishan (Interior Salish, NorthFountain/Fraser River, Lower/Mount ern) Currie

Lingít

Tlingit

Lipan Apache

Na-Dene Na-Dene (Dene, Apachean)

Los Conejos

Kumeyaay, Diegueño, ’Iipay/Tiipay

Yuman

Loup

Nipmuck

Algic (Algonquian)

Lower Chehalis

ɬəwʼálʼməš (Varieties: Humptulips, Wynochee, Westport-Shoalwater)

Salishan (Tsamosan Salish)

Lower Chinook

Coastal Chinook; dialects: Shoalwater/ Chinook proper, Clatstop

Chinookan

Luiseño

Luiseño-Juaneño

Uto-Aztecan (Takic, Cupan)



List of North American families, languages, and dialects 

 XXV

Families, languages, dialects

Alternate names and spellings

Language family (branch)

Lushootseed

xʷəlšucid, dxʷləšúcid, Puget Sound Salish, Skagit-Nisqually, Dxʷl@šucid, Northern Lushootseed, Northern Puget Sound Salish, dxʷləšúcid; Varieties: Northern (Skagit, Snohomish); Southern (Duwamish-Suquamish, Puyallup, Nisqually

Salishan (Coast Salish)

Macro-Algonquian

Hypothetical stock: Macro-Algonquian

Macro-Siouan

Hypothetical stock: Macro-Siouan

Mackenzie Valley Dene

Varieties of Dene spoken in the Na-Dene (Dene, Northern) ­Mackenzie Valley; Dene, Dene K’é, Slave, Slavey, North Slavey, South Slavey, K'áshogot'ine xedə́, Shúhtaot’ine yatí, Sahtú Dene, Deh Cho, Deh Gáh, MV Dene

Mahican

Algic (Algonquian)

Maidu

Northeastern Maidu, Mountain Maidu

Maidu, Mountain

See: Mountain Maidu

Maiduan

Maidun

Maiduan family

Mainland Comox

Comox-Sliammon

Salishan (Coast Salish)

Makah

Qʷiqʷidiččaq

Wakashan (South Wakashan/ Nootkan)

Maliseet

Malecite

Algic (Algonquian)

Mandan

Maiduan

Siouan-Catawban (Siouan)

Maricopa

Piipaash, Pee-Posh, Cocomaricopa; ­ Xalychidom (Halchidhoma), ­Kavelychidom (Kavelchadom)

Yuman

Massachusett

Natick, Wampanoag, Wôpanâak

Algic (Algonquian)

Mattole

Mattole-Bear River

Na-Dene (Dene, Pacific Coast)

Mayo

Dialect of Cáhita

Uto-Aztecan (Tarahumaran)

Mednyj Aleut

Copper Island Aleut; bilingual mixed language [Aleut + Russian]

Mixed language

Menominee

Menomini, Oma͞ eqnomenew

Algic (Algonquian)

Mesa Grande ’Iipay

See: ’Iipay

XXVI 

 List of North American families, languages, and dialects

Families, languages, dialects

Alternate names and spellings

Language family (branch)

Meskwaki

Mesquakie, Fox, Meskwaki-Sauk-Kickapoo

Algic (Algonquian)

Mi’kmaq

Mi’gmaq, Mi’kmaw, Micmac

Algic (Algonquian)

Miami-Illinois

Myaamia

Algic (Algonquian)

Michif

Métchif, Mitchif, Chippewa Cree, Michif Mixed language; generally clasCree, Heritage Michif, Southern Michif, sified as Algic (Algonquian) not-the-real Cree, Métif; bilingual mixed language [Cree + French], French Cree, Southern Michif

Mikasuki

Miccosukee, Hitchiti

Muskogean

Miluk

Miluk Coos

Coosan

Minto Lower Tanana

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Miwok

Miwokan, Moquelumnan

Miwok, Plains

See: Plains Miwok

Miwok, Sierra

See: Sierra Miwok

Mobilian Jargon

Mobilian trade language, Mobilian Trade Jargon, Chickasaw-Choctaw trade language; Yamá, Moquelumnan

Pidgin

Mohave

Mojave, Hamakhav, Upriver Yuman

Yuman

Mohawk

Kanienʼkéha’, Kanyen’kéha

Iroquoian (Northern)

Mohegan-Pequot-Montauk

Utian (Miwokan)

Algic (Algonquian)

Mojave

See: Mohave

Molalla

Molala, Molele, Molalla-Sahaptian, Molalla

Molalla-Sahaptian

Mono

Owens Valley Paiute

Uto-Aztecan (Numic)

Mono, Western

See: Western Mono

Montagnais

Innu-aimun

Algic (Algonquian)

Montana Salish

Flathead, Kalispel–Pend d’Oreille, SelišQl’ispé, Salish-Pend d’Oreille, Séliš

Salishan (Southern Interior Salish)

Monyton

Moniton

Siouan-Catawban (Siouan, Ohio Valley/Southeastern)

Moose Cree

Algic (Algonquian)

Moses-Columbia

Nxaʔamxčín , Nxa’amxcin

Salishan

Mountain Dene

Shúhtaot’ine yatī; variety of North Slavey, Mackenzie Valley Dene

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)



List of North American families, languages, and dialects 

 XXVII

Families, languages, dialects

Alternate names and spellings

Language family (branch)

Mountain Maidu

Maidu

Maiduan

Multnomah

Dialect of Upper Chinook. Wakanasisi, Wacanassisi

Chinookan

Munsee

Munsee Delaware

Algic (Algonquian)

Muskogean

Muskhogean

Muskogean family

Muskogee

Muscogee, Creek, Mvskoke, Creek-Seminole, Muskokee, Mvskoke, Maskoke, Seminole dialect of Creek

Muskogean (Eastern)

Musqueam

Hən̓q̓əmin̓əm̓, variety of Halkomelem

Salishan

Mutsun

Costanoan, Ohlone (as in “Mutsun Ohlone”)

Utian (Costanoan)

Mutsun-Awaswas

Mutsun

Utian (Costanoan)

MV Dene

See: Mackenzie Valley Dene

Na-Dene (Dene)

Mvskoke

Muskogee, Creek, Muscogee, Creek-Seminole

Muskogean (Eastern)

Myaamia

myaamiaataweenki, Miami, Miami-Illinois, Illinois, Maumee, myaamia, Twightwee, Wea

Algic (Algonquian)

Na-Dene

Nadene, Athabaskan-Eyak-Tlingit

Na-Dene family

Nakota

Assiniboine

Siouan-Catawban (Siouan)

Nansemond

Unclassified

Nanticoke

Algic (Algonquian)

Narragansett

Algic (Algonquian)

Naskapi

Algic (Algonquian)

Natchez

Isolate

Naukan

Naukanski, Naukan(ski) Siberian Yupik

Inuit-Yupik-Unangan

Navajo

Dine, Diné, Dineh, Diné bizaad, Navaho, Naabeehó bizaad

Na-Dene (Dene, Apachean)

Nedut’en

Witsuwit’en, Northern Carrier, Babine

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Nee’aanèegn’

Upper Tanana

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

nēhinawēwin

ᓀᐦᐃᓄᐍᐏᐣ, Swampy Cree, n-Dialect

Algic (Algonquian)

Nêhiyawêwin

ᓀᐦᐃᔭᐍᐏᐣ, Plains Cree, Y-Dialect

Algic (Algonquian)

Nejí

Tipey Aa, Tipei, Kumiay, Kumiai, ­Kumeyaay

Yuman

XXVIII 

 List of North American families, languages, and dialects

Families, languages, dialects

Alternate names and spellings

Neutral Nevome

Iroquoian (Northern) Onabas Piman

New River Shasta Nez Perce

Uto-Aztecan (Tepiman) Shastan

Nez Percé, Nimiipuutímt, Niimiipuutímt, Sahaptian Nimipuutímt, Niimi’ipuutímt, Niimíipu, Nuumiipuutimt, Numí, Chopunnish, titoqatímt

Nez Perce Jargon Nimipuutímt

Language family (branch)

No affiliation See: Nez Perce

Nisenan

Maiduan

Nisg̱a’a

Dialect of Nass-Gitksan: Nishga, Niska’, Tsimshianic Nisk’a’, Nass, Nisgha, Nisk’a, Nishka, Niska, Nasqa’, Nisgha, Nisg̲a’amx̲, Lisims

Nishnaabemwin

Jibwemwin, Odawa, Ottawa, Chippewa, Eastern Ojibwa, Ojibway, Ojibwe

Algic (Algonquian)

Nitinaht

Nitinaht, Nitinat, Ditidaht, Southern Nootkan

Wakashan (South Wakashan/ Nootkan)

Nlaka’pamux

Thompson; Thompson River Salish; Nłeʔkepmxcín

Salishan (Northern Interior Salish)

Nomlaki

Wintu

Wintuan (Northern)

Nooksack

Lhéchalosem (ɬə́čələsəm)

Salishan (Coast Salish)

Nootka

Nuu-chah-nulth, Nuuchahnulth, nuučaan̓uɫ, Nutka

Wakashan (South Wakashan/ Nootkan)

North Baffin Inuktitut

Qikiqtaaluk uannangani

Inuit-Yupik-Unangan

North Slavey

Variety of Mackenzie Valley Dene. Dene, Slave, K'áshogot'ine xedə́, Shúhtaot’ine, Sahtú Dene

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Northeastern Kumeyaay

Diegueño, Kumeyaay (proper), Tipai; Varieties: Campo, Imperial Valley, La Posta, Baron Long, Manzanita, ­Sycuan, ’Ewiiaapaayp (Cuyapaipe)

Yuman

Northeastern Pomo

Salt Pomo, Stony Creek, Stonyford

Pomoan

Northeastern Yavapai

Prescott Yavapai, Yavpé, Yavape

Yuman

Northern Carrier

Witsuwit’en, Nedut’en, Babine

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Northern Haida

Dialect of Haida

Isolate

Northern Paiute

Paviotso, Numu, Northern Paiute-­ Bannock

Uto-Aztecan (Numic)



List of North American families, languages, and dialects 

 XXIX

Families, languages, dialects

Alternate names and spellings

Language family (branch)

Northern Pomo

Redwood Cañon; Varieties: Pinoleville, Guidiville, Potter Valley, Coyote Valley, Sherwood Valley/Little Lake, Little Noyo River

Pomoan

Northern Straits

SENĆOŦEN, Saanich, Straits, NorthSalishan (Coast Salish) ern Straits Salish; Varieties: Sooke (T’Sou-ke), Saanich (SENĆOŦEN), Songhees/Songish (Lekwungen), Semiahmoo/ SEMYOME, Samish/ Xws7ámeshqen, Lummi (Xwlemi’chosen)

Northern Tepehuan

Uto-Aztecan (Southern, Tepiman)

Northern Tutchone

Dän kʼí

Northern Yuman

See: Hualapai, Havasupai, Yavapai

Nottoway

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Iroquoian (Northern)

Nsyilxcen

n̓səl̓xcin̓, Okanagan, Colville-Okanagan, Nsyilxcən, Nsyilxcn, nsyílxcən, nsəlxcin

Nuu-wee-ya’

Three-dialect cluster including varieties Na-Dene (Dene, Pacific Coast) in s. Oregon and n. California. CoquilleRogue River-Tututni-Tolowa-Chetco-Galice-Applegate/Dakubeh

Nuuchahnulth

Nuu-chah-nulth; Nootka, Nuučaan̓uɫ, Nutka, Aht, West Coast, T’aat’aaqsapa; Varieties: Barkley, Central, Northern

Wakashan (South Wakashan/ Nootkan)

nuxalk

Bella Coola

Salishan

Nxa’amxcin

Moses Columbia Salish, Columbian Salish, Nxaʔamxčín

Salishan (Interior Salish, Southern)

O’odham

Papago, Pima-Papago

Uto-Aztecan (Tepiman)

Salishan (Interior Salish, Southern)

Obispeño

Chumashan (Northern)

Occaneechi

Siouan-Catawban (Siouan, Ohio Valley/Southeastern)

Ofo

Siouan-Catawban (Siouan, Ohio Valley/Southeastern)

Ohlone

Costanoan

Utian (Costanoan/Ohlone)

Oji-Cree

Variety of Ojibwe

Algic (Algonquian)

Ojibwe

Anishinaabemowin, Nishnaabemwin, Algic (Algonquian) Ojibway, Ojibwa, Otchipwe, Mississauga, Saulteaux, Chippeway, Southwestern Chippewa, Daawaamwin

XXX 

 List of North American families, languages, and dialects

Families, languages, dialects

Alternate names and spellings

Language family (branch)

Okanagan

Nsyilxcən, Nsyilxcn, Nsəlxcin, Nsilxcín, Colville-Okanagan

Salishan (Interior Salish, Southern)

Okwanuchu

Shastan

Omaha

Omaha-Ponca

Siouan-Catawban (Siouan)

Oneida

Onʌyota’a:ka

Iroquoian (Northern)

Onondaga

Onoñdaʼgegáʼ nigaweñoʼdeñʼ

Iroquoian (Northern)

Oob No’ok

Uto-Aztecan (Tepiman)

Osage

Siouan-Catawban (Siouan)

Paipai

Paʔipa:y, Pa’ipai, Pa Ipai, Pai Pai, Akwa’ala

Paiute, Northern

See: Northern Paiute

Paiute, Southern

See: Southern Paiute

Pamlico

Carolina Algonquian

Pamunkey Passamaquoddy

Algic (Algonquian) Unclassified

Passamaquoddy-Maliseet

Patwin

Algic (Algonquian) Wintuan

Pawnee

Chaticks si chaticks, Cahriksicahriks, Cahiksicahiks; Varieties: Skidi, Skiri, South Band

Pee-Posh

See: Maricopa

Peña Blanca Pentlatch

Yuman

Caddoan

Yuman pənƛʼáč, pentl̓ach

Penutian

Salishan (Coast Salish) Hypothetical stock: Penutian

Peoria

Miami-Illinois

Algic (Algonquian)

Petun

Tionontati, Hkionontate

Iroquoian (Northern)

Picuris Northern Tiwa

Picuris Tiwa; Varieties: Tiwa, Picurís, Northern Tiwa

Kiowa-Tanoan

Pidgin Delaware

Pidgin Unami, Delaware Jargon, Trader’s Pidgin Jargon

Pidgin Inuit

There were two different ones.

Pidgin

Piipaash

See: Maricopa

Yuman

Pima

Pima Bajo; Mountain Pima; Lowland Pima; Nevome

Uto-Aztecan

Plains Apache

Na-Dene (Dene, Apachean)



List of North American families, languages, and dialects 

 XXXI

Families, languages, dialects

Alternate names and spellings

Language family (branch)

Plains Cree

ᓀᐦᐃᔭᐍᐏᐣ nēhiyawēwin, Nêhiyawêwin

Algic (Algonquian)

Plains Indian Sign Language

Pidgin

Plains Miwok

Utian (Miwokan)

Pochutec

Uto-Aztecan (Southern, Coracholan-Nahuan)

Pomo

Pomoan

Pomo, Eastern

See: Eastern Pomo

Pomo, Kashaya

See: Kashaya Pomo

Pomo, Northeastern

See: Northeastern Pomo

Pomo, Northern

See: Northern Pomo

Pomo, Southeastern

See: Southeastern Pomo

Pomo, Southern

See: Southern Pomo

Pomoan

Pomo

Pomoan family

Potawatomi

Pottawatomie, Bodéwadmi, Bodéwadmimwen, Bodéwadmi Zheshmowen,

Algic (Algonquian)

Powhatan

Virginia Algonquian

Algic (Algonquian)

Purisimeño Ql'ispé

Pomoan family

Chumashan (Southern, Central) Upper Pend d’Oreille

Quapaw

Salishan (Southern Interior Salish) Siouan-Caddoan (Siouan)

Quechan

See: Kwatsáan

Quileute

Quillayute

Chimakuan

Quinault

kʷínayɬ (Varieties: Queets, Quinault)

Salishan (Tsamosan Salish)

qʷi·qʷi·diččaq

Makah

Wakashan (South Wakashan/ Nootkan)

Rio Grande Tewa

Varieties: Tewa, Nambé, Nanbé, Ohkay, Kiowa-Tanoan San Juan, Pojoaque, Santa Clara, Northern Tewa, San Ildefonso, San I [sæ̨n ˀaɪ], Tesuque

Ritwan

California Algic

Rumsen Saanich Sahaptian

Hypothetical branch of Algic Utian (Costanoan/Ohlone)

Northern Straits Salish, SENĆOŦEN

Salishan (Coast Salish) Sahaptian family

XXXII 

 List of North American families, languages, and dialects

Families, languages, dialects

Alternate names and spellings

Sahaptin

Ichishkíin, Ichishkin, ’Ichishkin, ‘Ichish- Sahaptian kíin, Chíshkin, Shahaptin, Shawpatin; Variesties: Northeast dialects: Palouse, Palus, Peluuspem, Walla Walla, Walúula, Priest Rapids, Wanapam, Lower Snake, Wawyukma, Naxiyampam, Chamnapam; Northwest dialects: Klickitat, X̱ waɬx̱waypam, Klikitat, Kittitas, Pshwanwapam, Yakima,Yakama, Mamachatpam, Wanapum, Taytnapam, Upper Cowlitz, Taitnapam, Upper Nisqually, Mishálpam; Southern/Columbia River dialects: Umatilla, Imatalam, Rock Creek, Q’miɬama, John Day, Celilo, Wayamɬama, Tenino, Tinaynu, Tygh Valley, Warm Springs); Includes Tanino, Umatilla, Walla Walla, Yakama

Sahaptin, Yakima

See: Yakima Sahaptin

Sahtúot’i̜ ne yati̜

Sahtugot’ine yati, Sahtú, Bearlake, Dene; variety of Mackenzie Valley Dene

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Salinan

Dialects: Antoniano, Migueleño

Isolate

Salish

Salishan

Salishan family

Salishan, Southern Interior

See: Southern Interior Salishan

Samala

Ineseño

San Antonio Necua San Carlos Apache

Language family (branch)

Chumashan (Southern, Central) Yuman

Variety of Western Apache

San Francisco Bay Costanoan

Na-Dene (Dene, Apachean) Utian (Costanoan)

San José de la Zorra

Tipey Aa, Kumiay, Kumiai, Kumeyaay

Yuman

San José de Tecate

Tipai, Kumiay, Kumiai, Kumeyaay

Yuman

Santee

Dakota

Siouan-Catawba (Siouan)

Saponi

Sappony

Siouan-Catawban (Siouan, Ohio Valley/Southeastern)

Sarcee

Tsúut’ínà, Tsuut'ina, Sarsi

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Sauk

Meskwaki-Sauk-Kickapoo, Sac

Algic (Algonquian)

Sechelt

Shashishalhem (šášíšáɬəm), She shashishalhem

Salishan (Coast Salish)

Secwepemctsín

Shushwap, Shuswap, Secwepemc, At-nah, Shooswap, səxwəxcín, Secwepemctsía

Salish (Interior Salish, Northern)



List of North American families, languages, and dialects 

 XXXIII

Families, languages, dialects

Alternate names and spellings

Language family (branch)

Sekani

Tse’khene, Tsek’ene

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Sekani, Fort Ware

See: Fort Ware Sekani

Seliš

Séliš, Bitterroot Salish, Flathead, Montana Salish

Seminole

Salishan (Interior Salish, Southern) Muskogean

SENĆOŦEN

Sənčaθən, Saanich, Northern Straits Salish

Salish (Coast Salish)

Seneca

Onödowáʼga꞉

Iroquoian (Northern)

Seri

Comcaac, Comcáac, Comcáackg, ConIsolate caac, Concáac, Congcaac, Cuncaac, Komkak, Konkaak, Kunkaahac, Kunkaak, Könkáak

Serrano

Uto-Aztecan (Takic, Serran)

Sewee

Unclassified

Shasta

Shasta proper

Shasta, New River

See: New River Shasta

Shawnee She shashishalhem

Shastan

Algic (Algonquian) Sechelt

Shoccoree-Eno

Salishan (Coast Salish) Unclassified

Shoshone

Shoshoni, Shoshoni-Gosiute, Neme ta̲ikwappe, Sosoni’ ta̲ikwappe,

Uto-Aztecan (Numic)

Shúhtaot’ine yati

Mountain Dene; variety of North Slavey, Mackenzie Valley Dene

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Shuswap

Secwepemctsín (səxʷepməxcín)

Salishan (Interior Salish, Northern)

Siberian Yupik

Bering Strait Yupik, Central Siberian Yupik, Saint Lawrence Island Yupik, Sivuqaghmiistun, Yoit, Yuit

Inuit-Yupik-Unangan

Sierra Miwok

Moquelumnan, Miwuk

Utian (Miwokan)

Sierra Miwok, Southern

See: Sourthern Sierra Miwok

Siletz

Salishan (Tillamook/hutyéyu)

Siletz Dee-ni

Tolowa

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Siouan

Branch of Siouan-Catawban

Siouan-Catawban family

Siouan-Catawban Sirenik

Siouan-Catawban family Sirenikski

Inuit-Yupik-Unangan

XXXIV 

 List of North American families, languages, and dialects

Families, languages, dialects

Alternate names and spellings

Language family (branch)

Siuslaw

Sayúskla, Siuslawan

Isolate

Sḵwx̱wu7mesh

Squamish, Sḵwx̱wu7mesh snichim

Salishan (Coast Salish)

Slave

Dene, Mackenzie Valley Dene, Slavey, Slavé, Slavi, North Slavey, South Slavey, Dené Tha, Dené, Deh Cho, Deh Gáh, Dene K’é, Got’ine, Mackenzian See also: North Slavey, South Slavey

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Slavey Jargon

Slave Jargon

Pidgin

Sliammon

Comox-Sliammon, Éy7á7juuthem, ʔayʔaǰuθəm

Salishan (Coast Salish)

Sm’algya̱x

See: Coast Tsimshian

Snchitsu’umshtsn

Coeur d’Alene

Salishan (Interior Salish, Southern)

Solano

Unclassified

Sonoran O’otam

Uto-Aztecan (Tepiman)

Souriquois Jargon

Pidgin

South Slavey

Variety of Mackenzie Valley Dene. Dene, Dené, Dehcho Dene, Dené Tha, Deh Cho, Deh Gáh, Slave, Slavey

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Southeastern Pomo

Varieties: Sulphur Bank/Elem, Lower Lake/Koi

Pomoan

Southeastern Yavapai

Kewevikopaya, Kwevkapaya, Kewevkopaya, Kewevkapaya

Yuman

Southern Interior Salishan

Branch of Salishan

Southern Paiute

Southern Paiute-Ute, Colorado River Uto-Aztecan (Numic) Numic, Ute-Chemehuevi; Dialects: Chemehuevi, Southern Paiute, Northern Ute

Southern Pomo

Salmonhole, West Creek, Dry Creek/ Mihilakhawna, Cloverdale, Lytton, ­Graton, Gallinoméro

Southern Sierra Miwok

Pomoan

Utian (Miwokan)

Southern Tiwa

Varieties: Tiwa, Isleta, Sandia, Ysleta del Sur, Ysleta D

Kiowa-Tanoan

Southern Tsimshian

Sgüüx̣s (dialect of Tsimshian

Tsimshianic

Southern Tutchone

Dän kʼè

Na-Dene (Dene)

Southern Ute

Uto-Aztecan (Numic)



Families, languages, dialects

List of North American families, languages, and dialects 

Alternate names and spellings

Southwestern/Southeastern Tepehuan

 XXXV

Language family (branch) Uto-Aztecan (Tepiman)

Spokane

Npoqínišcn

Salishan (Interior Salish, Southern)

Squamish

Sḵwx̱wú7mesh, sqʷx̣ʷúʔməš, sníchim

Salishan (Coast Salish)

St’át’imcets

Sƛ̓áƛ̓imxəc, St̓át̓imcets, Lillooet, Lillooet Salish, Lil’wat7úlmec (dialect)

Salishan (Interior Salish, Northern)

Stoney Dakota

Siouan-Catawban (Siouan)

Straits, Northern

See: Northern Straits

Susquehannock

Conestoga, Andaste

Swampy Cree

Iroquoian (Northern) Algic (Algonquian)

Tagish

Den k'é

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Tahltan

Tāłtān

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Tāłtān

Tahltan

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Takelma

Takilma

Isolate

Tanacross

Neeʼaandegʼ

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Tanana

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Tanana, Minto Lower

See: Minto Lower Tanana

Tanana, Upper

See: Upper Tanana

Tanpachoa

Unclassified

Taos Northern Tiwa

Varieties: Tiwa, Taos, Northern Tiwa

Tarramiut Inuktitut

Canadian Inuit, Inuit, Inuktut, Nunavim- Inuit-Yupik-Unangan miutut, Nunavik dialect of Inuktitut

Tawasa Teguima

Kiowa-Tanoan

Timucuan Opata

Tepecano

Uto-Aztecan Opatan) Uto-Aztecan (Tepiman)

Tepehuan, Northern

See: Northern Tepehuan

Tepehuan, Southwestern/ Southeastern

See: Southwestern/Southeastern Tepehuan

Tetsǫ́t’ıné

Tetsǫ́t’ıné yatıé; variety of Dene Sųłiné/ Chipewyan

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Tewa

Tano, dialect of Arizona Tewa

Kiowa-Tanoan

Tewa of Arizona

Arizona Tewa, Hopi-Tewa

Kiowa-Tanoan

Tewa, Rio Grande

See: Rio Grande Tewa

XXXVI 

 List of North American families, languages, and dialects

Families, languages, dialects

Alternate names and spellings

Thompson

Nlaka’pamux, Nɫeʔkepmxcín, NtlakapSalishan (Interior Salish, muk, Thompson River Salish, Thompson Northern) Salish; Varieties: Lytton/Canyon, Nicola Valley; Varieties: Western, Eastern

Thompson River

Thompson, Thompson Salish

Salishan (Interior Salish, Northern)

Thompson Salish

Nlaka’pamux, Ntlakapmuk, Thompson, Thompson River Salish, Nɬeʔkepmxcín

Salishan (Interior Salish, Northern)

Tiipay

Sometimes used as a cover term for any and all Kumeyaay languages other than ’Iipay; see Kumeyaay. See also Jamul Tiipay

Tipai

See: Tiipay, La Huerta, Northeastern Kumeyaay, San José de Tecate

Tillamook

Hutyéyu

Timucua

Language family (branch)

Salishan Timucuan

Tiwa, Picuris Northern

See: Picuris Northern Tiwa

Tiwa, Southern

See: Southern Tiwa

Tiwa, Taos Northern

See: Taos Northern Tiwa

Tlingit

Tlingít, Lingít, Łingít, Lingit, Lingít yoo x̲’atángi

Na-Dene

Tłı̨ chǫ

Tłı̨ chǫ Yatıì, Dogrib, Thlingcha-dinneh

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Tohono O’odham

O’odham; Papago-Pima, Papago

Uto-Aztecan (Piman)

Tolkapaya Yavapai

Tolkepaya, Upland Yuman, Western Yavapai

Yuman

Tongva

Gabrielino, Gabrieleño

Uto-Aztecan (Takic)

Tonkawa

Isolate

Towa

Varieties: Jemez, Pecos

Kiowa-Tanoan

Tsek’ene

Sekani; varieties include Kwadacha Tsek’ene, Fort Ware Sekani

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Tse’khene

Tsek’ene, Sekani

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Trader Navajo Tsilhq’otin

Pidgin Chilcotin, Tsinlhqut’in, Tsilhqut’in, Tsilhqút’ín

Tsimshian Tsimshian, Coast

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern) Tsimshianic family

See: Coast Tsimshian



List of North American families, languages, and dialects 

Families, languages, dialects

Alternate names and spellings

Tsimshian, Southern

See: Southern Tsimshian

Tsúut’ínà

Tsúūt’ínà, Tsuut’ina, Sarsi, Sarcee

 XXXVII

Language family (branch)

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Tubar

Uto-Aztecan (Southern, Tarahumaran)

Tübatulabal

Uto-Aztecan

Tümpisa

Timbisha, Timpisha, Tumbisha, Panamint

Uto-Aztecan (Numic)

Tumpisa Shoshone¨

Timbisha, Panamint, Koso

Uto-Aztecan (Numic)

Tunica

Tanico, Tonica, Tunihka, Tunixka, Yoroniku, Luhchi Yoroni, Yuron

Isolate

Tunumiisut

Inuit-Yupik-Unangan

Tuscarora

Ske:rù:ręˀ

Iroquoian (Northern)

Tutchone, Northern

See: Northern Tutchone

Tutchone, Southern

See: Southern Tutchone

Tutelo

Yesanechi

Siouan-Catawban (Siouan, Southeastern/Ohio Valley)

Tututni

Variety of Nuu-wee-ya’

Na-Dene (Dene, Pacific Coast)

Twana

sqʷuqʷúʔbəšq, Tuwaduq; Varieties: Quilcene, Skokomish

Salishan (Coast Salish)

Twulshootseed

Dxʷləšucid, Xʷəlšucid, Lushootseed, Puget Salish, Puget Sound Salish, Skagit-Nisqually

Salishan (Coast Salish)

Ucwalmícwts

Lil’wat7úl

Salishan

Unami

Delaware

Algic (Algonquian)

Unangam Tunuu

Aleut

Inuit-Yupik-Unangan

Upper Chehalis

Q̉ʷay̓áyiłq̉; Varieties: Satsop, Oakville Chehalis, Tenino Chehalis

Salishan (Tsamosan Salish)

Upper Chinook

Kiksht, Columbia Chinook; dialects: Multnomah, Clackamas, Cascades, White Salmon, Hood River, Wasco-Wishram

Chinookan

Upper Tanana

Nee’aaneegn’, Nee’aanèegn’, Nee’aanèègn’

Na-Dene (Dene, Northern)

Upriver Halkomelem

Halq̓eméylem, Holkomelem, Halkomelem

Salishan (Coast Salish)

Ute

Southern Paiute

Uto-Aztecan (Numic)

XXXVIII 

 List of North American families, languages, and dialects

Families, languages, dialects

Alternate names and spellings

Language family (branch)

Ute, Southern

See: Southern Ute

Utian

Miwok-Costanoan

Utian family

Uto-Aztecan

Uto-Aztekan

Uto-Aztecan family

Uumarmiut

Inuit-Yupik-Unangan

Ventureño

Chumashan (Southern, Central)

Wáˑšiw

See: Washo

Waiilatpuan Wailaki

Hypothetical Cayuse-Molala family Eel River Athabaskan

Wakashan

Na-Dene (Dene, Pacific Coast) Wakashan family

Walapai

See: Hualapai

Wampanoag

Wôpanâak, Massachusett

Wappo

Algic (Algonquian) Yukian (Yuki-Wappo)

Wasco-Wishram

Dialects of Upper Chinook, Wasco, Wishram

Chinookan

Washo

Washoe; wá꞉šiw ʔítlu, Wašišiw, Wa:šiw, Wáˑšiw

Isolate

Wendat

Huron

Iroquoian (Northern)

Wenro

Neutral

Iroquoian (Northern)

West Greenlandic

Kalaallisut

Inuit-Yupik-Unangan

Western Abenaki

Algic (Algonquian)

Western Apache

San Carlos, San Carlos Apache, Cibecu, White River Apache, Ndee, Coyotero, Nnee biyáti’

Na-Dene (Dene, Apachean)

Western Cherokee

ᏣᎳᎩ ᎦᏬᏂᎯᏍᏗ, Tsalagi Gawonihisdi, dialect of Cherokee

Iroquoian (Southern)

Western Mono

Monache

Uto-Aztecan (Numic)

Western Yavapai

See: Tolkapaya Yavapai

Wichita

Kitikiti’sh, Kirikirish, Kirikir’is; Varieties: Tawakoni, Waco

Wikchamni Yokuts Winnebago

Yokutsan Ho-Chunk

Wintu Wintuan

Caddoan

Siouan-Catawban (Siouan) Wintuan (Northern)

Wintun

Wintuan family



List of North American families, languages, and dialects 

 XXXIX

Families, languages, dialects

Alternate names and spellings

Language family (branch)

Wipuhk’a’bah

Wipukpaya, Verde Valley Yavapai

Yuman

Witsuwit’en

Nedut’en, Babine, Babine Carrier, North- Na-Dene (Dene, Northern) ern Carrier

Wiyot

Wishosk

Algic

Woccon

Siouan-Catawban (Catawban)

Woods Cree

Algic (Algonquian)

Wôpanâôt8âôk

Wampanoag, Wôpanâak

Wyandot

Algic (Algonquian) Iroquoian (Northern)

X̄a’islak̓ala (Kitimaat)-X̄enaksialak̓ala (Kitlope)

Haisla, X̣aʔislak’ala, Kitimat, Kitlope, Northern Kwakiutl

Wakashan (North Wakashan/ Kwakiutlan)

X̲aad Kil

Masset Haida, X̲aad kíl, dialect of Haida

Isolate

xʷməθkʷəy̓əm

Musqueam, Halkomelem

Salishan (Coast Salish)

Yakima Ichishkíin

Yakama, Yakima Sahaptin

Sahaptian

Yakima Sahaptin

Yakima Ichishkiin

Sahaptian

Yana

Varieties: Northern Yana, Central Yana, Southern Yana

Isolate

Yaqui

Hiaki, Yoeme, Yoem Noki

Uto-Aztecan

Yavapai

Varieties: Kwevikopaya (Southeastern Yavapai), Prescott (Northeastern Yavapai, Yavpé); Wipuhk’a’bah (Verde Valley Yavapai); Tolkapaya (Western

Yuman

Yawelmani

Yowlumne, Yauelmani

Yokutsan

Yoeme

Uto-Aztecan (Yoemian/Cahitan)

Yokuts

Yokutsan; Varieties: Poso Creek, General Yokutsan family Yokuts (Buena Vista and Nim-Yokuts)

Yokuts, Wikchamni

See: Wikchamni Yokuts

Yokutsan

Yokuts; Varieties: Poso Creek, General Yokuts (Buena Vista and Nim-Yokuts)

Yokutsan family

Yuchi

Euchee

Isolate

Yuki

Ukiah, Ukomno’m; Other varieties: Huchnom, Coast Yuki

Yukian/Yuki-Wappo)

Yuma

See: Kwatsáan

Yuman

Yuman Yuman, Northern

See: Northern Yuman

Yup’ik, Central Alaskan

See: Central Alaskan Yup’ik

XL 

 List of North American families, languages, and dialects

Families, languages, dialects

Alternate names and spellings

Language family (branch)

Yup’ik

Yupik, Yupiik, Yupiaq, Yupiat, Yugcestun, Inuit-Yupik-Unangan Yugtun, Central Alaskan, Central Alaskan Yup’ik

Yupik, Central Siberian

See: Central Siberian Yupik

Yupik, Siberian

See: Siberian Yupik

Yurok

Chillula, Mita, Pekwan, Rikwa, Sugon, Weitspek, Weitspekan

Algic

Zuni

Zuñi, Shiwi’ma, Ashiwi, Shiwi, Zuñi

Isolate

ʔayʔaǰuθəm

Comox-Sliammon, Comox, Island Comox, Mainland Comox, Tla A’min (Homalco/Xwemalhkwu–Klahoose– Sliammon/Tla A-min)

Salishan (Coast Salish)

Maps Introduction The following maps serve to illustrate the approximate territories of the languages and language families covered in this handbook. The maps stem from the Handbook of North American Indians, a series of handbooks published by the Smithsonian Institution over a thirty-year period in the late twentieth century.1 The maps are not intended as an authoritative representation of the territories, but rather as a general illustrative addition to the volume.2 Mapping in the Handbook of North American Indians began in 1978 and was based on the map by anthropologist Harold E. Driver with changes proposed by editors and chapter authors in the various volumes. The maps may cover different time periods (Ives Goddard, p.c.). While in the original source the maps were labeled “Key to Tribal Territories”3 and later “Key to Chapter Coverage”, the labels have been removed here for reprinting. The names of the languages and language groups shown on the maps have not been changed from the original source for reprinting in this volume, and they may not always represent current language names. The chapters covering language sketches and the list of languages included in this handbook can be consulted for more recently updated language names. The sequence of the maps included here follows the sequence of the Handbook of North American Indians series. It is arranged by culture and geographic area starting in the Arctic and moving south and then east. Maps from volumes 5-15 are included. In addition, an enlarged version of the whole continent map is included. It is a revised version of the map published in volume 17 of the Handbook of North American Indians and by the University of Nebraska Press. This map is included separately as a fold-out at the end of book and online here: https://www.degruyter.com/document/isbn/9783110597981/html

Acknowledgements We would like to extend our deepest gratitude to Laurie Burgess at the Department of Anthropology at the Smithsonian Institution for granting us permission to re-print the maps in this handbook. We are also eternally grateful to Ives Goddard for his helpful 1  Some maps that were originally produced decades ago have been redrafted, as noted by cartographer Dan Cole (p.c.). 2  The disclaimer published in the handbook volume 14 (page viii) can be applied more generally to all maps. 3  As noted by Ives Goddard (p.c.), the label “Key to Tribal Territories” was misleading. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-203

XLII 

 Maps

insights and guidance in obtaining permissions and for providing us with invaluable background information about the maps, which is included in the introduction and notes accompanying the maps. We would also like to thank the cartographer Dan Cole for graciously providing the digital files of the maps for re-printing.

References Native Languages and Language Families of North America. Compiled by Ives Goddard. Revised and enlarged edition, with additions and corrections, 1999. Department of Anthropology, National Museum of Natural History, Smithsonian Institution.  Native Languages and Language Families of North America. Compiled by Ives Goddard. 1999. Studies in the Anthropology of North American Indians Series. Sheet map, folded. University of Nebraska Press. Sturtevant, William C. ed. 1971. Handbook of North American Indians: Arctic. Volume 5. David Damas, volume editor. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution. Sturtevant, William C. ed. 1982. Handbook of North American Indians: Subarctic. Volume 6. June Helm, volume editor. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution. Sturtevant, William C. ed. 1990. Handbook of North American Indians: Northwest Coast. Volume 7. Wayne Suttles, volume editor. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution. Sturtevant, William C. ed. 1978. Handbook of North American Indians: California. Volume 8. Robert F. Heizer, volume editor. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution. Sturtevant, William C. ed. 1979. Handbook of North American Indians: Southwest. Volume 9. Alfonso Ortiz, volume editor. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution. Sturtevant, William C. ed. 1983. Handbook of North American Indians: Southwest. Volume 10. Alfonso Ortiz, volume editor. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution. Sturtevant, William C. ed. 1986. Handbook of North American Indians: Great Basin. Volume 11. Warren L. D’Azevedo, volume editor. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution. Sturtevant, William C. ed. 1998. Handbook of North American Indians: Plateau. Volume 12. Jr. Deward E. Walker, volume editor. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution. Sturtevant, William C. ed. 2001. Handbook of North American Indians: Plains. Volume 13. Raymond J. DeMallie, volume editor. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution. Sturtevant, William C. ed. 2004. Handbook of North American Indians: Southeast. Volume 14. Raymond Fogelson, volume editor. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution. Sturtevant, William C. ed. 1979. Handbook of North American Indians: Northeast. Volume 15. Bruce G. Trigger, volume editor. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution. Sturtevant, William C. ed. 1996. Handbook of North American Indians: Languages. Volume 17. Ives Goddard, volume editor. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution.



Maps 

 XLIII

Map of the languages and major dialects in the Arctic culture area

Source: Department of Anthropology, National Museum of Natural History, Smithsonian Institution. ­ Published in the Handbook of North American Indians: Arctic. Volume 5: ix. [Note: Some language names have been updated since originally published in the Handbook.]

XLIV 

 Maps

Map of the languages and dialects in the Subarctic culture area

Source: Department of Anthropology, National Museum of Natural History, Smithsonian Institution. ­ Published in the Handbook of North American Indians: Subarctic. Volume 6: ix.



Maps 

 XLV

Map of the languages and language groups on the Northwest Coast To the left: North.

To the right: South.

Source: Department of Anthropology, National Museum of Natural History, Smithsonian Institution. ­Published in the Handbook of North American Indians: Northwest Coast. Volume 7: ix.

XLVI 

 Maps

Map of the languages and language groups in California

Source: Department of Anthropology, National Museum of Natural History, Smithsonian Institution. P ­ ublished in the Handbook of North American Indians: California. Volume 8: ix.



Maps 

 XLVII

Map of the languages and language groups in the Southwest

Source: Department of Anthropology, National Museum of Natural History, Smithsonian Institution. ­ Published in the Handbook of North American Indians: Southwest. Volumes 9 and 10: ix. [Note: There are some updates on the map since it was originally published in the handbook.]

XLVIII 

 Maps

Map of the languages in the Great Basin

Source: Department of Anthropology, National Museum of Natural History, Smithsonian Institution.­ Published in the Handbook of North American Indians: Great Basin. Volume 11: ix.



Maps 

 XLIX

Map of the languages and language groupings in the Plateau

Source: Department of Anthropology, National Museum of Natural History, Smithsonian Institution. ­ Published in the Handbook of North American Indians: Plateau. Volume 12: ix.

L 

 Maps

Map of the languages, dialects, and language groupings in the Plains

Source: Department of Anthropology, National Museum of Natural History, Smithsonian Institution. ­ Published in the Handbook of North American Indians: Plains. Volume 13.1 and 13.2: ix.



Maps 

 LI

Map of the languages, dialects, and regional groupings in the Southeast

Source: Department of Anthropology, National Museum of Natural History, Smithsonian Institution. ­ Published in the Handbook of North American Indians: Southeast. Volume 14: ix.

LII 

 Maps

Map of the languages, dialects, and regional groupings in the Northeast

Source: Department of Anthropology, National Museum of Natural History, Smithsonian Institution. ­ Published in the Handbook of North American Indians: Northeast. Volume 15: ix.

I Sounds and sound structure

Sonya Bird, Rae Anne Claxton, and Tess Nolan

1 Acoustic phonetics

Abstract: Acoustic phonetics is the subfield of linguistics dedicated to studying the details of speech sounds, by examining their sound waves. Although speech sounds have been studied for over 100 years, there are not many studies of North American Indigenous languages. In this chapter, we describe acoustic phonetics as it relates to Indigenous language revitalization and creating new speakers (Section 1.1). We discuss how to use existing and new recordings to document the acoustic features of speech (Section  1.2). We summarize acoustic phonetic work that exists on North American Indigenous languages, including studies on vowels, consonants, word-level prosody (the rhythm of individual words), and sentence-level intonation (how whole sentences sound) (Section 1.3). We end by discussing how people are coming together for community-based acoustic phonetic research on Indigenous languages, and how it is a key component of language documentation and revitalization (Section 1.4).

1.1 Introduction At first glance, languages around the world share many of the same sounds. For example, English (Germanic) and Hul’q’umi’num’ (Salish) both have a word written ten (‘mother’ in Hul’q’umi’num’). If we listen carefully though, we can notice that even though the spelling is the same, the sounds are slightly different: 1 is longer and more tense in Hul’q’umi’num’ than in English (spelled [e] vs. [ɛ] in the International Phonetic Alphabet, see Figure 2). Of course, languages also have sounds that are entirely different from those used in other languages, both in spelling and in pronunciation. For example, the sound spelled in the Hul’q’umi’num’ word ([t͡ɬʼam]; ‘to be enough’) does not exist in English. Unfamiliar sounds can be challenging for new speakers, especially if they do not know what exactly to pay attention to in trying to hear and pronounce them. Documenting a language’s sounds using acoustic phonetics and articulatory phonetics (see Bliss, Bird, and Gick, this volume) can help us understand the details of these sounds, and this can help us support new speakers in their language work. Phonetics is the subfield of linguistics that is focused on documenting and comparing the details of speech sounds, both within a language and across the languages of the world. Whereas articulatory phonetics (Bliss, Bird, and Gick, this volume) focuses on the actual articulation of speech, acoustic phonetics focuses on the sound waves that result from articulation. Both help us further our understanding of the human capacity for

1 Linguists use at least three sets of brackets: are used for spelling; /../ are used for sounds as they are encoded in our minds (see Chapter 5); [..] are used for sounds as they are actually pronounced. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-001

4 

 Sonya Bird, Rae Anne Claxton, and Tess Nolan

speech – what sounds we are able to make and how we use them to express ourselves. For Indigenous language documentation and revitalization, acoustic and articulatory phonetic research also creates a foundation for developing teaching materials that can help learners achieve what they think of as ‘authentic’ pronunciation. Many learners of Indigenous languages take very seriously their responsibility of speaking in a way that honors their elders (Bird and Kell 2017; Jenni et al. 2017; Bird and Miyashita 2019), but they are often lacking in resources to help them (McIvor 2015). Relatively few opportunities exist to interact with first language (L1, mother tongue) speakers and popular teaching approaches like Total Physical Response (Asher 1977) and Where Are Your Keys (Gardner 2019) tend to de-emphasize pronunciation. Analyzing the details of speech can be very technical work (see Sections 1.2.3 and 1.3) and can never replace working face-to-face with speakers. Nonetheless, it can play a valuable role in language learning, especially for learners at the intermediate to advanced level who are ready to fine-tune their speaking and listening skills. It allows us to “zoom in” on very specific aspects of speaking and listening, in the comfort of our own homes, in a space where we can really examine the details of speech without feeling like we’re putting ourselves and other speakers on the spot or making anyone repeat things over and over again. The examples provided throughout this chapter are transcribed using the International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA). The IPA was created by linguists to transcribe all the sounds in the world’s languages. Figures 1 and 2 summarize the symbols used for the most common consonants (Figure 1) and vowels (Figure 2) across languages. Both charts are organized based on articulation, and are described more fully in Bliss, Bird, and Gick (this volume). In the consonant chart, the columns correspond to where the sound is produced in the mouth/throat (place of articulation), from the lips (at the left) to the larynx (at the right). The rows correspond to how constricted the sound is (manner of articulation), from completely constricted stops (no air gets through, e.  g., [p]) to unconstricted resonants (air flows freely, e.  g., [l]). Within each cell, the two symbols represent sounds that differ only in what the vocal folds are doing; the sound on the left (e.  g., [p]) is pronounced without vocal fold vibration (voiceless), while the sound on the right (e.  g., [b]) is pronounced with vocal fold vibration (voiced). In the vowel chart, vowels are organized based on where the tongue is within the mouth, along two dimensions: frontback (towards the front of the mouth vs. towards the back of the mouth) and close-open (high to the palate vs. low on the mouth floor). Additional charts and lists of other IPA symbols and diacritics can be found on the website of the International Phonetic Association: (https://www.internationalphoneticassociation.org/IPAcharts/IPA_charts_2018. html). While the IPA is the standard alphabet used by the international community of linguists, in North America – at least in the Pacific Northwest – it is more common to use the North American Phonetic Alphabet (NAPA). The alphabets are very similar, with the exception of a few symbols (NAPA = IPA): x̣ = χ; ḥ = ħ; ƛʼ = t͡ɬʼ; s, c (or š, č) = ʃ, t͡ʃ; y = j; ṣ, c̣, ḷ, ị, ụ, ə̣, ạ = s̠ , c̠ , l̠ , i̠ , u̠ , ə̠, a̠ .



 5

Acoustic phonetics 

THE INTERNATIONAL PHONETIC ALPHABET (revised to 2015)

PHABET (revised to 2015)

CONSONANTS (PULMONIC)

Bilabial Labiodental

Plosive

Postalveolar Retroflex

Nasal

Palatal

Velar

Dental

Uvular

© 2015 IPA

Alveolar Postalveolar Retroflex

© 2015 IPA

Pharyngeal

Palatal

Velar

Uvular

Pharyngeal

Glottal

Glottal

Trill Tap or Flap Fricative Lateral fricative

Approximant Lateral approximant

Symbols to the right in a cell are voiced, to the left are voiceless. Shaded areas denote articulations judged impossible.

CONSONANTS (NON-PULMONIC)

VOWELS

Fig. 1: Basic (pulmonic) consonant chart (Source: https://www.internationalphoneticassociation.org/ Front Central Clicks Voiced implosives Ejectives content/ipa-chart, available under a Creative Commons Attribution-Sharealike 3.0 Unported License. Close Source: Copyright 2018 International Phonetic Bilabial Bilabial Examples: Association)

Back

left are voiceless. Shaded areas denote articulations judged impossible. Dental

(Post)alveolar VOWELS

s

Palatoalveolar Front

Dental/alveolar

Bilabial

Palatal

Dental/alveolar

Velar

Alveolar lateral Close

Central

Uvular

Close-mid

Back

Velar Alveolar fricative

OTHER SYMBOLS

eolar

icative

Voiceless labial-velar fricative Close-mid

Voiced alveolar lateral flap

Voiced labial-palatal approximant

Simultaneous

Open-mid Voiceless epiglottal fricative Voiced epiglottal fricative Epiglottal plosive

atives

ral flap

nd

ulations ymbols ary.

Open

Open

Alveolo-palatal fricatives

Voiced labial-velar approximant

Open-mid

SUPRASEGMENTALS

and

Primary stress

Affricates and double articulations can be represented by two symbols joined by a tie bar if necessary.

Secondary stress Long

DIACRITICS Some diacritics may be placed above a symbol with a descender, e.g. Voiceless

Where symbols appear in pairs, the one to the right represents a rounded vowel.

Where symbols appear in pairs, the one to the right represents Breathy voiced a rounded vowel. Dental

Voiced

Creaky voiced

Apical

Less rounded

Secondary stress Palatalized

Nasal release

Half-long Extra-short Minor (foot) group

Major (intonation) group SUPRASEGMENTALS Fig. 2: Vowel chart (Source: www.internationalphoneticassociation.org/content/ipa-chart, available under Aspirated Linguolabial Laminal Syllable a Creative Commons Attribution-Sharealike 2018break International Primary stress 3.0 Unported License. Source: Copyright More rounded Labialized Nasalized Linking (absence of a break) Phonetic Association) Advanced

Long Velarized

Lateral release

TONES AND WORD ACCENTS

Many Indigenous language communities in North America use LEVEL an Extra alphabetCONTOUR that is or or Rising Retracted Pharyngealized No audible release Half-long high adapted from either the Roman alphabet, the IPA, or the NAPA. Being able to read difol with a descender, e.g. High Falling Centralized Velarized or pharyngealized Extra-short ferent alphabets makes it much easier to access materials put togetherMid by differentHigh linDental rising Mid-centralized Raised ( = voiced alveolar fricative) Lowto Minor (foot)on group guists over time. In our work Salish languages, we have found that it is useful Low rising Syllabic Lowered ( = voiced bilabial approximant) Apical Extra Risingcreate conversion charts that includegroup IPA, NAPA, and the local writing system(s), so that Major (intonation) low falling Non-syllabic Advanced Tongue Root Downstep Global rise Laminal we have a handy reference to help decode various written materials. As an example, Syllable break Rhoticity Retracted Tongue Root Upstep Global fall Nasalized Table 1 provides a small section of a conversion chart that includes the alphabets for two Linking (absence of a break) neighboring Salish languages spoken on Southern Vancouver Island (Hul’q’umi’num’ Typefaces: Doulos SIL (metatext); Doulos SIL, IPA Kiel, IPA LS Uni (symbols) Nasal release TONES AND WORD ACCENTS (HUL) and SENĆOŦEN (DEA), NAPA, and IPA). The SENĆOŦEN orthography was created Lateral release LEVEL CONTOUR by the late Dave Elliott, W̱SÁNEĆ elder. DEA stands for Dave Elliott Alphabet; it uses all Extra or of four or Rising No audiblecapital release letters and a set high diacritics. High

= voiced alveolar fricative) = voiced bilabial approximant)

Mid Low

Extra low

Downstep

Falling

High rising Low rising Risingfalling

Global rise

6 

 Sonya Bird, Rae Anne Claxton, and Tess Nolan

Tab. 1: Sample conversion chart: Hul’q’umi’num’(HUL), SENĆOŦEN (DEA), NAPA, IPA HUL

DEA

NAPA

IPA

o u  p’ lh ch kw xw

O  E  B  Ƚ  Ć  Ȼ  X̱

a ə  p̓ ɫ  č kʷ x̣ʷ

ɑ ə  p’ ɬ  t͡ʃ kʷ χʷ

The remainder of this chapter is structured as follows: we start by presenting methodological considerations in using and creating recordings that form the basis of acoustic analysis, how to make use of existing recordings, what to consider when making new recordings, and how to process and analyze these to learn about pronunciation (Section 1.2). We summarize acoustic phonetic work that has focused on North American Indigenous languages, focusing on some of the more unusual features of these languages (Section  1.3). We end the chapter with a brief discussion of new ways of conducting collaborative (university-community) acoustic phonetic research (Section 1.4).

1.2 Methods and materials produced 1.2.1 Using existing recordings and making new ones Our detailed understanding of speech sounds (reflected in the charts above) comes from analyzing speakers’ articulations (articulatory phonetics) and the resulting speech sound waves (acoustic phonetics), in addition to careful listening of course (auditory phonetics). Some sounds are easier to grasp referring directly to articulation, e.  g., ones with complicated tongue positions like English [r] and [l]; others are easier to grasp referring to the acoustic signal (the sound waves), e.  g., ones with a tightening in the throat like ejective [t’] (see Section  1.3.2.2). Combining auditory, acoustic, and articulatory phonetics gives us the best chance to document speech in a thorough, comprehensive way. In this chapter, we focus on acoustic phonetics (Bliss, Bird, and Gick, this volume, focuses on articulatory phonetics). Studying speech sound waves involves using specialized software (see below) to analyze audio recordings. These can either be existing, for example legacy recordings made with previous generations of speakers, or they can be made new. Existing recordings are immensely valuable for studying speech, especially in cases where there are currently no first language speakers. The earliest recordings of Indigenous



Acoustic phonetics 

 7

languages of North America were made in the late 1800s using wax cylinders (https:// www.historymuseum.ca/cmc/exhibitions/tresors/barbeau/mbf0300e.html). Since then, recording technology has evolved substantially, from magnetic tape recordings (reelto-reel and cassette) to digital media (DAT, minidisc, CD, DVD). Nowadays, recordings normally consist of digital audio files (WAV, MP3 and other formats), created and saved directly on hard drives and/or servers. The key to preserving and using recordings made on older media is digitization. Older media can easily become unusable, either because they deteriorate over time or because the equipment required to play back the recordings is no longer available. Many institutions have large collections of older recordings. For example, comprehensive collections of early wax cylinder recordings are housed at the Library of Congress in the United States and the Canadian Museum of Civilization in Canada. Other collections are housed more locally, e.  g., the California Language Archive (http://cla.berkeley. edu/) contains a large collection of early recordings of Californian languages, thanks to a partnership with the IRENE project at the University of California, Berkeley (http:// irene.lbl.gov/). In addition, many universities, libraries and museums have the necessary equipment and capacity to digitize and archive recordings made using media that is now outdated. Partnerships between communities and these institutions can facilitate storing and accessing audio collections, and various funding opportunities exist to support such partnerships2. Existing recordings can provide us with wonderful breadth in terms of content, especially if they are of naturalistic speech (e.  g., stories, conversations), allowing for fruitful exploration of all kinds of speech features. Working with these recordings can be relatively time-consuming (although thoroughly engaging!) in that it is often necessary to listen to (and transcribe) hours of recordings to hear the specific speech feature you are interested in. Where possible, working on existing recordings with fluent speakers (often elders) is very valuable; they can provide context and translations for what was recorded, and can also notice things that newer speakers may not notice about the speech styles of individual speakers, for example. In some cases, existing recordings may simply not contain enough examples to allow for an in-depth study of something you are interested in. This is where new recordings are useful: because they can be designed with a specific focus in mind, they allow for in-depth study (the trade-off being that we may miss important patterns by intentionally eliciting speech rather than letting it emerge naturally). Another advantage in making

2 Funding opportunities change over time, so research on the possibilities must be done at the time communities are ready to prepare an application. In 2021, some national-level possibilities include the Phillips Fund (American Philosophical Society), Jacobs Fund (Whatcom Museum), Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (SSHRC; in Canada), and Dynamic Language Infrastructure/Documenting Endangered Languages (DLI/DEL) program (jointly funded by the National Science Foundation [NSF] and the National Endowment for the Humanities [NEH] in the US). Other funding opportunities may exist within specific institutions.

8 

 Sonya Bird, Rae Anne Claxton, and Tess Nolan

new recordings is that they provide the opportunity to work with fluent speakers, which can be a very rich experience. For example, in recording the details of pronunciation with elders, learners often have the opportunity to get valuable feedback on their own pronunciation. New recordings can take some time and effort to set up in terms of tailoring them to elicit specific aspects of speech but, once they are made, processing the recordings for analysis is relatively quick. In some contexts, making new recordings is not feasible (e.  g., if there are currently no speakers of the language). Recording quality may also be a determining factor in choosing whether to work with existing or new recordings: if existing recordings were made on older media and/or were not made with acoustic phonetic research in mind, they may not be of high enough quality for acoustic analysis. Ultimately, the choice of whether to use existing or new recordings will differ depending on the research topic and on the feasibility of accessing or creating different types of recordings. In general, using a combination of the two types of recording can be very successful: existing recordings for breadth and new recordings for depth. One thing that we have learnt from our work together is that it is important not to underestimate how much time and effort it takes to review and process recordings (whatever the medium/media) and to get them in a format that is usable for different purposes. If possible, we recommend including someone in your team who is tech-savvy and can help to turn recordings into resources for community-based language learning, e.  g., an app for listening to the language on your phone.

1.2.2 Methods for making new recordings 1.2.2.1 Recording procedures 1.2.2.1.1 Recording devices New recordings can be made using a range of devices. The Zoom H4N is currently (2020) a relatively inexpensive and high quality recorder (https://www.zoom-na.com/), and is very popular in linguistic documentation work. Recordings can also be made directly onto desktop or laptop computers, ideally using an external microphone (examples include the Rode NT-1 A, NT-USB, Sennheiser MKH 416 P48U3, or the Blue Yeti USB microphone) and free recording software like Audacity (https://www.audacityteam. org/). Nowadays, recordings can even be made on handheld devices. Paul De Decker and Jennifer Nycz (2011) compared the quality of sound recordings made on four devices: a professional Roland Edirol R-09 recorder, a first-generation iPhone, a Macbook Pro, and a Mino Video recorder, both original and uploaded to and then downloaded from YouTube. Compared to the baseline of the Edirol recorder, the iPhone and Macbook Pro were able to accurately record the lower frequencies (in vowels, the first two formants – see Section 1.3.2.1), but beyond that became unreliable. De Decker and Nycz found that the Mino Video recorder and the YouTube videos significantly altered and compressed



Acoustic phonetics 

 9

the audio of the recordings such that they were considerably warped and therefore unusable for acoustic analysis. In our opinion, even though microphones in non-dedicated consumer electronics have improved in recent years, it is not recommended to use them for significant, faithful, speech recording. In making recordings, the main factor to consider is what is called the signal to noise ratio (Ladefoged 2003): the difference in loudness between the sound you want to record (signal) and everything else (noise, including clocks, refrigerators, TVs, lawnmowers, etc.). Acoustic analysis requires audio recordings of relatively high quality, meaning a relatively loud signal compared to whatever background noise might be present. In a very quiet room and with talkers with relatively strong (loud) voices, it may be possible to get by without an external microphone, for example using the built-in microphone of the Zoom H4N. In a louder setting and/or with quieter speakers, or in cases where we want to record multiple speakers, one or more external microphones may be needed, and careful thought must go into choosing the appropriate one(s). 1.2.2.1.2 Microphones There are two main features to consider when it comes to microphones for recording speech: microphone type and polarity. Type refers to the way the microphone takes the physical property of sound (changes in air pressure) and converts it to electronic signals. There are a number of different types of microphones; the best type for speech is a condenser microphone. More specifically, small-diaphragm condenser microphones are better than large-diaphragm ones for their increased and more life-like responsiveness to speech sounds. These microphones (also called capacitor microphones) are good at capturing the more subtle acoustic properties of speech, but they do require an external power source. This power source (called “phantom power”3) will usually come from (a) the recorder (where it can be turned on in the settings, as is the case with Zoom recorders), (b) a powered pre-amp or computer (usually turned on with a switch), or (c) a battery pack included with the microphone itself. The second feature is microphone polarity. Polarity refers to the microphone’s sensitivity to sounds coming from different directions. Different polarities have different advantages and disadvantages, and different applications. For example, an omnidirectional microphone will record with equal loudness sounds from all the way around it (360 degrees). This can be useful for group conversations where there are not enough microphones for each individual but can be detrimental in noisy situations where just one person is being recorded. A cardioid polarity microphone is highly sensitive to sounds directly in front of the microphone, and very insensitive to sounds elsewhere. This makes it great for recording a single speaker while minimizing other sounds in the

3 On recorders and pre-amps phantom power can be labeled either “phantom power” or “48V” for the 48 volts that it provides.

10 

 Sonya Bird, Rae Anne Claxton, and Tess Nolan

environment (the signal to noise ratio mentioned above), but it is not so good if you are also interested in other speakers or ambient sound. If recording multiple speakers, it is best to have several cardioid microphones, one for each speaker (or group). Some microphones can be switched between different polarities, and this will often be indicated as a “multi-pattern” microphone. High quality microphones usually connect to a recorder or pre-amp with a special cable called an XLR cable. For example, the Zoom H4N mentioned above has two XLR ports for cables to connect to external microphones. Likewise, the Focusrite Scarlett 2i2 pre-amp has two ports for microphones. Some microphones (such as the Rode NT-USB or the Blue Yeti) connect directly to the computer via USB (which should be indicated in their title). Recorders and recording software allow various methods for recording audio from microphones into different channels. The most common are mono and stereo. In mono, all audio from all microphones is recorded to one channel; in stereo, the audio from at least two microphones is recorded into two channels. Some recorders, such as the Zoom H6N, allow multi-channel recording with up to four external microphones. Audio information from one microphone in one channel can be separated from audio information in another channel, allowing separate analysis if needed. The simplest recorders, with just one built-in microphone, allow (effectively) only mono recordings. The more microphones and microphone ports available on a recorder or a pre-amp, the greater the number of channels there are available for recording. This can often be reflected in the product name for a recorder, for example the Zoom H4N has (potentially) four microphones, two built-in and two ports for external microphones, while the Zoom H6N has (potentially) six microphones, two built-in and four ports for external microphones. These devices (or ones like them) are ideal for recording a group of elders in conversation, for example. Note that if you are recording to multiple devices rather than through multiple microphones on a single device, you will have to synchronize the recordings after the fact, for processing. This can be done by creating a loud sound, such as a clap, that appears at the beginning on all recordings. The recordings can then be synched later, by lining up the loud sound. 1.2.2.1.3 Audio file types Recorders often save recorded audio to a removable SD card or some other on-board storage, which can then be transferred to a computer. There is no quality difference between these storage types, though there can be quantity differences in terms of amount of storage available. The two most common audio file types found on portable recorders or accessible in recording software are uncompressed and compressed types. Uncompressed audio means that, within the given parameters for recording, all the acoustic information picked up by the microphone will be faithfully encoded into a digital file and be available for later analysis. No data or information is lost through the encoding of sound into digital information. This type commonly includes the WAV and AIFF file



Acoustic phonetics 

 11

types.4 Compressed audio includes both lossless and lossy file types. Lossless formats are compressed, that is, the data they hold have been manipulated from the original signal to create a smaller file size. They rely on algorithms to do this compression in a manner that preserves the full psycho-acoustic information of the original uncompressed format (meaning the human ear cannot normally tell that the file has been compressed). Examples of common lossless formats include FLAC and ALAC. Lossy formats involve the same kind of manipulation and transformation but result in an even smaller file sizes and greater information loss. Common types of lossy audio are MP3 or AAC formats. While compression saves on file size and therefore storage space, compression also entails loss of information (regardless of whether the listener can tell or not). We strongly recommend that original audio recordings be saved and stored in an uncompressed format (not MP3), unedited, and that this uncompressed format be used for analysis – see Dry (2009) for a wider discussion on the best practices of data and materials preservation. Whenever possible, decisions about recording set-ups should be made with guidance from a sound expert who is knowledgeable in the currently available technology. 1.2.2.1.4 Adding video recording Although the focus here has been on audio recordings, our experience is that we can learn a lot by being able to see as well as hear fluent speakers. Two kinds of video recordings are useful: (1) zoomed-in videos of speakers’ faces, to see things like lip rounding (see Bliss, Bird, and Gick,this volume, Section 2.3.1) and (2) zoomed-out videos of whole speakers, to see things like hand gestures and eyebrow movements, which are also part of how we communicate. In terms of pedagogical applications, a growing body of literature in language learning and teaching points to the benefits of multimodal speech perception (Bliss et al. 2018; Olson 2014). For any new recordings, it is worth considering simultaneous audio and video recording.

1.2.2.2 Elicitation materials and techniques If new recordings are being made, there are several options in terms of how to elicit speech. For basic information on the pronunciation of specific sounds, word list elicitations are useful. For example, a speaker might be asked to read a list of words, each one illustrating a different sound in the language. The Illustrations of the International Phonetic Association (IPA 1999; see Section 1.3.1) are largely based on word list elicitations. These short descriptions are designed to give the reader a fairly good sense of how the individual sounds of a language are pronounced.

4 Technically WAV refers to the file format that can hold many different types of metadata and audio at different levels of compression, but in speech recording “saving to WAV” usually means saving in an uncompressed format

12 

 Sonya Bird, Rae Anne Claxton, and Tess Nolan

To understand how a language sounds above the level of individual words, longer speech samples are needed. For example, to compare the intonation patterns of statements (e.  g., ‘he went to the store.’) and questions (e.  g., ‘he went to the store?’), whole sentences need to be elicited. Often this is done using props like pictures or storyboards (Figure 3) (Caldecott and Koch 2014). The website http://totemfieldstoryboards.org/ has an ever-growing collection of storyboards that can be used for research and teaching. Note that storyboards and similar visual props do not need to be highly professional-looking or digital; hand drawn pictures on notecards (Figure 3b) work very well. Speakers like them, they can be edited on the fly as needed, and they do not rely on technology (that sometimes crashes).5

(a) (b) Fig. 3: Examples of visual props to elicit conversational speech: (a) Picture with bunny holes that are movable and can be added/taken away to elicit ordinal numbers in a story where Coyote is looking for Rabbit (artwork created by co-author Claxton for a story elicitation session with elder Dr. Sti’tum’at Ruby Peter in September 2019); (b) Notecard from a story about borrowing a car (original source, following Creative Commons license CC-BYNC-SA 2.0: Caldecott and Koch [2014, Figure 2, p. 223]).

It is worth putting thought into designing elicitation sessions so that the recordings can be used for multiple research and pedagogical purposes. This is especially important if time with fluent speakers is limited. For example, stories and conversations can be used to study all kinds of things, from the tiniest details of speech (e.  g., how the high front vowel /i/ transitions into the back consonant /q/; see Section 1.3.2.1) to the broadest patterns (e.  g., sentence intonation; see Section 1.3.2.3). At the same time, they also provide a valuable cultural resource for communities, which can be used in any number of ways. Along these lines, as feasible and appropriate, it is always a good idea to record whole sessions with elders (rather than just a specific task). You just never know what might come up, and what unexpected things you might learn from these sessions!

5 Marion Caldecott, personal communication (May 5, 2020).



Acoustic phonetics 

 13

One thing that is important to remember about focused elicitation sessions is that speakers will not always sound the same in these recordings as they do in more natural settings, when they are speaking spontaneously. This means that how learners experience spoken language based on elicited speech may not translate to how they experience it in the ‘real world’: learners may find that they understand elicited speech better than they do more natural speech; similarly, they may find that they are expected to speak in community in a way that does not match how they speak in more careful sessions with their teachers and elders. This is partly because elicitation sessions tend not to capture the living nature of spoken language, and partly because speakers (often teachers/elders) participating in elicitation sessions are often particularly careful to create clear recordings, knowing that they will be used in part for pedagogical purposes. In any case, it is important to keep in mind that while doing focused pronunciation work is one component of language learning, it will never replace learning in community, and in a more holistic way. Finally, we must always remember that any recordings we make should be made available to the speakers and their community (as appropriate) in a timely manner. For example, when learners participate in recording sessions, they have very practical goals in mind; they expect the recordings to be processed quickly, so that they can get feedback sooner rather than later on their pronunciation. It is our responsibility to make this our priority as well.

1.2.2.3 Speakers Existing acoustic phonetic studies of North American Indigenous languages are by and large based on the speech of one or two first language (L1) speakers. It can be of great value though – in terms of linguistic inquiry and in terms of pedagogical applications – to compare pronunciation across different speakers (e.  g., Fortier 2019). In the SENĆOŦEN-speaking community for example, young adults joke about sounding like 80 year old men, because the legacy recordings that have helped them learn their language were mostly recordings of elderly men (Bird and Kell 2017). Making recordings with speakers of all ages and across genders, in a range of tasks and activities, will ensure that we document the language as thoroughly as possible. Documenting the pronunciation of new speakers with varying fluency levels can be particularly useful, again for theoretical and practical reasons. There is an overwhelming focus on English in the second language (L2) pronunciation literature (Lee, Jang, and Plonsky 2015). Given how complex the sound systems are in many North American Indigenous languages, looking into pronunciation among learners of these languages will undoubtedly shed important light on L2 pronunciation as a whole. From the perspective of Indigenous language revitalization, documenting the speech of new speakers will help us determine what challenges they face, so that we can create tools and techniques to help them overcome these challenges.

14 

 Sonya Bird, Rae Anne Claxton, and Tess Nolan

No matter who is being recorded, it is important to follow appropriate protocols and best practices during the recording session. In working with elders, it is our responsibility to take care of them while they are working. This includes knowing what they like to eat and drink, being aware of dietary restrictions (e.  g., diabetes or allergies) when we provide snacks, keeping an eye on their energy levels, being flexible when it comes to taking breaks vs. pushing through on a task, and making sure they have what they need to work (elicitation materials in large font sizes, spare batteries for hearing aids, notepads and pens, etc.). Many elders like to know ahead of time what they will be focusing on in a particular session, so getting materials to them in advance is important. In working with learners, we must always keep in mind that we are putting them on the spot by focusing on their pronunciation. Documenting their speech might feel uncomfortable and even intrusive, especially if they are sensitive about sounding different from their elders and/or peers. We must always proceed carefully, mindful of how learners are feeling, and keeping in mind that any form of feedback, no matter how constructive it is, can be discouraging. This section has focused on recording speech for future analysis (see Section 1.3.3). This kind of work is beneficial in two ways: First, it allows us – especially learners – to go off and figure out the details of speech offline, without the pressure of ‘getting it right’ in front of others. Second, it takes some burden off elders and teachers, in that they do not have to keep repeating the same things over and over again, as we try to make sense of what we are hearing. In an ideal situation, recordings are made by language learners, and contribute directly and immediately to their own learning process. With this in mind, we would like to end with a word of advice on doing pronunciation work with elders: remember that you are not necessarily going to see progress overnight. Your elders and teachers might offer you pronunciation tips and strategies that you are not immediately able to make use of. Take them away and work with them; give yourself time and patience, and practice, practice, practice. At the same time, try not to be shy when you are working with your teachers and elders; your time with them is precious, and you will get the most out of it if you are willing to take chances and make mistakes. In our experience, it works well to pick one thing to focus on, and to let your mentor(s) know what it is so they can help you. They will check your pronunciation, and will help you progress in a steady, step-by-step way. For example, if you are struggling with a particular sound, they may start you off with familiar words that you are comfortable saying, and help you work towards harder words. By balancing face to face work with work based on recordings, you will be able to make the most of different resources you have to work with.

1.2.3 Analyzing audio recordings acoustically Acoustic phonetic data analysis can be quite technical, including detailed quantitative measurements of various acoustic parameters like vowel formants (Kharlamov and



Acoustic phonetics 

 15

Oberly 2019) and pitch contours (Martin and Johnson 2002) – see Section 1.3.2. This need not be the case though; qualitative visual inspection of the waveform and spectrogram associated with speech can be enough to clarify how speech sounds differ from one another, and this kind of work does not require a lot of technical expertise (Bird et al. 2019; Fish and Miyashita 2017). In terms of quantitative analysis, Ladefoged (2003) provides a useful summary of some of the acoustic features that are commonly measured, including pitch, loudness, and length (Ladefoged 2003: Chapter 4), vowel characteristics (Ladefoged 2003: Chapter 5), consonant characteristics (Ladefoged 2003: Chapter 6), and phonation types (Ladefoged 2003: Chapter 7). Studies illustrating some of these features and related measurements are discussed in Section 1.3.2 below. Note that, although technology-based analysis of speech can be very useful, it should not replace using our ears. Acoustic analysis provides an additional way of experiencing speech, one that can be incorporated into the process of understanding speech rather than one that replaces other approaches. In terms of acoustic data analysis tools, perhaps the most commonly used is Praat (http://www.fon.hum.uva.nl/praat/), a freely downloadable tool created by Paul Boersma and David Weenink at the University of Amsterdam (Boersma and Weenink 2022). Praat allows for visual inspection of waveforms, spectrograms, and pitch and intensity (loudness) contours of speech, as well as for taking quantitative measurements. Each (or all) of the visual displays can be paired with one or more “textgrids,” which can be used to segment and label the speech stream. Figure 4 below provides a sample waveform (upper panel), spectrogram (middle panel), and two textgrids6 (lower panel) of the word ‘acoustics’, as pronounced by Sonya Bird (co-author). Superimposed on the spectrogram are also the pitch contour (flat-ish lines) and intensity (loudness) contour (loopy lines). Such visuals can help us identify what the acoustic features of different sounds are. For example, we can see that the middle vowel ou [u] has the highest pitch and intensity (the pitch and intensity contours are higher up on the image), reflecting the fact that this vowel carries the primary, word-level stress (see 3.2.3). We can also see that the two k sounds in “acoustics” differ from one another: the first k ([kh]), which precedes the stressed vowel ou ([u]), has a lot of “aspiration” (visually: white noise) on it; this is the puff of air you can feel if you place your hand just in front of your lips and pronounce the word. The second k ([Ɂk]), which follows the unstressed vowel i ([ɪ]), looks quite different – it is not aspirated like the first one, and is preceded by spreading out and irregularity of the vertical lines – the pitch pulses – at the end of the vowel [ɪ] and before the airflow stops completely for [k]. This is called glottalization or laryngealization. Articulatorily, it corresponds to a tightening in the throat (a constriction at the glottis, more precisely), like the sound in the middle of uh-oh.7 6 Upper textgrid: English alphabet; lower textgrid: phonetic transcription using the International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA). 7 Each pitch pulse corresponds to one glottal pulse, or one open-close cycle of the vocal folds, as they vibrate to produce voiced speech sounds like vowels.

16 

 Sonya Bird, Rae Anne Claxton, and Tess Nolan

Fig. 4: Visual displays of the English word ‘acoustics’: top = waveform; middle = spectrogram (with superimposed pitch contour [flat-ish lines] and amplitude contour [big loopy]); bottom = textgrid for labeling sound files (upper: English orthography; lower: phonetic [IPA] transcription).

Praat also has a scripting function, which allows for automatically extracting and exporting measurements to an Excel spreadsheet for analysis. Another handy feature of Praat is that its textgrids are compatible with the annotation tiers in ELAN (ELAN 2019), a very popular tool used for annotating video and audio resources (https://tla. mpi.nl/tools/tla-tools/elan/). This means that it is possible to go between Praat and ELAN fairly seamlessly.

1.3 Acoustic phonetic descriptions of North American languages Based on many of the methods outlined in Section 1.2 above, different types of phonetic descriptions have been created for Indigenous languages of North America. In searching for such descriptions, we used three methods: (a) keyword searches (e.  g., Salish + phonetics) through library and online databases, (b) searches within key journals focused on acoustic phonetics (e.  g., Journal of Phonetics) and North American Indigenous languages (e.  g., International Journal of American Linguistics), and (c) searches within reference sections of individual journal articles, including comprehensive summary articles like Gordon (2017) and McDonough and Whalen (2008). Based on our search, we created a database of existing acoustic studies, which allowed us to identify broad trends in acoustic phonetic research on Indigenous languages of North America, as well



Acoustic phonetics 

 17

as identify a number of studies that describe particularly unusual features of North American languages and/or provide useful models for future work. In the following two sections, we start by summarizing the types of acoustic phonetic resources that exist (Section 1.3.1); we then describe a number of acoustic phonetic studies of North American languages, as a sample of what exists, and as inspiration of what more could be done (Section 1.3.2).

1.3.1 Types of resources The majority of published acoustic phonetic studies on Indigenous languages of North America are relatively recent (since 2000), and not all language families or possible areas of study within acoustic phonetics are equally represented in the literature. The language families that have been most thoroughly studied are Salish (especially individual sounds, syllables, and prosody), Dene, and Muskogean (especially tone and pitch-accent). The majority of studies we found are on segmental phonetics: the acoustic features of consonants and vowels, and how they interact. These include descriptions of systems as a whole (common with studies on vowels) and ones about specific sounds or sets of sounds (common with studies on consonants). The remaining studies cover areas such as the acoustics of syllables and syllable rhythm, word-level prosody (stress, tone, and pitch-accent; see also Uchihara, this volume; Gordon, this volume), and sentence-level prosody and intonation (see also Tuttle, this volume). Perhaps the most readily available resources to community members are descriptions of sounds found in dictionaries and grammars of individual languages. For example, the SENĆOŦEN dictionary (Montler 2018) has a four-page description of the language’s consonants and vowels, including acoustic and articulatory features. In the past, such descriptions have often been somewhat opaque for non-specialists (very jargon-heavy), but this is changing, with linguists becoming increasingly aware of their responsibility to make their work accessible to the communities they work with (compare, for example, the descriptions in Montler’s 2018 dictionary with those in his (1986) Outline of the Morphology and Phonology of Saanich, North Straits Salish). In our experience, grammars rarely include thorough acoustic phonetic documentation of individual sounds, let alone of other pronunciation features, like word-level prosody or sentential intonation. For these, we must look to more specialized publications. The International Phonetic Association (https://www.internationalphoneticassoci ation.org) publishes thorough phonetic descriptions of individual languages in Illustrations of the International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA). First appearing in the Handbook of the IPA (1999), they are now published as short articles in regular volumes of the Journal of the International Phonetic Association. Audio files are also available for the Illustrations (https://richardbeare.github.io/marijatabain/ipa_illustrations_all.html). So far, only eight Illustrations have been written on Indigenous languages of North America: Spokane (Carlson and Esling 2001); Nuuchahnulth (Carlson, Esling, and Fraser 2001); Chickasaw

18 

 Sonya Bird, Rae Anne Claxton, and Tess Nolan

(Gordon, Munro, and Ladefoged 2001); Jicarilla Apache (Tuttle and Sandoval 2002); Mono (Olson 2004); Mono Lake Northern Paiute (Babel, Houser, and Toosarvandani 2012); Sahaptin (Hargus and Beavert 2014); and Gitksan (Brown et al. 2016). Similarly, broad phonetic descriptions of specific languages have been published in the Journal of Phonetics (Taff et al. (2001) on Aleut; Flemming, Ladefoged, and Thomason (2008) on Montana Salish), Anthropological Linguistics (Gordon, Munro, and Ladefoged (2000) on Chickasaw; Maddieson, Smith, and Bessell (2001) on Tlingit), and International Journal of American Linguistics (Gordon et al. (2001) on Western Apache; Gordon, Martin, and Langley (2015) on Koasati). A number of these were originally produced by researchers affiliated with the University of California Los Angeles’ Phonetics Laboratory (e.  g., Gordon (1996) on Hupa), thanks to a National Science Foundation (NSF) grant Sounds of the World’s Languages awarded to Peter Ladefoged and Ian Maddieson, two of the founding fathers of modern phonetics (Gordon 2017). Aside from broad phonetic descriptions, journals like the Journal of Phonetics and the International Journal of American Linguistics have occasionally published scholarly papers on specific sounds of North American Indigenous languages, for example Bird et al. (2008) on glottalized resonants and Dart (1993) on Tohono O’odham stops and fricatives. The proceedings of the International Conference of Salish and Neighboring Languages have a number of acoustic studies on languages of the Pacific Northwest (e.  g., Bessell (1997) on St’át’imcets; Jacobs (2007) on Skwxwu7mesh), and there are also studies on North American languages in the proceedings of the International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (e.  g., Ingram and Rigsby (1987) on Gitksan). A handful of doctoral dissertations and master’s theses include acoustic phonetics of North American languages (e.  g., Oberly (2008) on Southern Ute; Haynes (2010) on Numu; Neal (2018) on Kawaiisu). Finally, we have also come across two books that include acoustic phonetics, both on Dene languages: Athabaskan Prosody (Hargus and Rice 2005) and Navajo Sound System (McDonough 2003). In general, acoustic phonetic studies of North American languages are still fairly few and far between (McDonough and Whalen 2008), especially ones written in a way that is accessible to community members. However, there is an increasing call for phonetic resources that are accessible, for use in pronunciation learning and teaching (Fish and Miyashita 2017; Bird and Miyashita 2019). We are hopeful that the landscape is shifting in this respect.

1.3.2 Illustrative acoustic phonetic studies In this section we describe a small set of studies that reflect the types of acoustic phonetic research that has been done on North American Indigenous languages. These studies were chosen to cover a range of language families (Uto-Aztecan, Dene, Salish, Eskimo-Aleut, and Muskogean) and focus on a range of areas in acoustic phonetics, from vowels and consonants to word-level prosody and sentential intonation. Many of them also provide good models for future studies on similar features in other languages.



Acoustic phonetics 

 19

Within each subsection, we start with a small summary called What you need to know, for the benefit of readers who are not so interested in the technicalities of acoustic phonetic research but are looking for some information on the kinds of features to pay attention to in teaching and learning pronunciation.

1.3.2.1 Vowels What you need to know Two things are good to keep in mind when thinking about vowels: (1) Vowels can sound different in different languages, even if they are spelled the same way. Vowels can also sound different within a language, e.  g., different elders and families might use different vowel variations. It is good to take the time to learn how different people you interact with pronounce their vowels, so that you can make conscious decisions about what variation(s) you want to use, and how to be consistent in your pronunciation. (2) Vowels can also sound different next to different consonants. Sometimes, when consonants are hard to tell apart, listening to the vowels can give you good clues to what the consonants are. This is the case with /k/ and /q/ (see 1.3.2.2), for example. Again, it is good to take the time to learn how the vowels sound in different contexts, so that you can incorporate this information into your speaking and listening practice. Many existing studies of vowels are acoustic descriptions of a language’s whole vowel system (Bessell 1997; Barthmaier 1998; Tuttle, Lovick, and Núñez-Ortiz 2011; Muehlbauer 2012; Nelson 2013; Nolan 2017; Fortier 2019; Kharlamov and Oberly 2019). These generally include vowel charts (see Figure 2), which show how vowels are distributed in an articulatory and acoustic space defined by the first two formants (F1 and F2) of each vowel (Ladefoged and Johnson 2015). Formants are bands of frequencies that are amplified by the shape of the vocal tract (see Figure 4, vowels [ə], [u] and [ɪ]). Broadly speaking, F1 is correlated with how close the tongue is to the palate (see Bliss, Bird, and Gick, this volume, Section 2.2.3): close vowels like [i] have a low F1; open vowels like [a] have a high F1. F2 is correlated with how far back the tongue is: front vowels like [i] have a high F2; back vowels like [u] (or [ə] and [a] in Figure 1) have a low F2. Vowels can be plotted with F1 on the y-axis and F2 on the x-axis, and with the origin point {0,0} at the top right (Figure 5a). With this lay-out, the acoustic distribution of vowels reflects where they are produced in the mouth, assuming a side view of the mouth with the lips at the left (Figure 5b). As an example, Figure 5 provides the vowel space of lək̓wəŋən (Central Salish) as spoken by a single speaker in the late 1960s (Nolan 2017). One thing that is interesting to notice is that lək̓wəŋən has a typologically unusual vowel space: it is asymmetrical, in that there is no mid back vowel [o] that corresponds to the mid front vowel [e], and [u] (not represented here) is uncommon, found primarily in borrowed words. While four-vowel systems are relatively common, they normally include at least one non-low back vowel (Gordon et al. 2001). Additionally, while the acoustic and artic-

20 

 Sonya Bird, Rae Anne Claxton, and Tess Nolan

ulatory spaces of [a] and [ə] overlap, they are distinctive in duration; [a] (as well as [i] and [e]) are on average ~180 milliseconds long, while [ə] is between ~100 and ~120 milliseconds on average (Nolan 2017).

(a) acoustic space

(b) articulatory space

Fig. 5: Acoustic (a) and corresponding articulatory (b) space of lək̓wəŋən vowels. Original source for 5(a) (following Creative Commons license CC-BY-NC-SA 2.0): Nolan (2017), Figure 4.9(d) “Lekwungen vowel space across three time points and a mean measure” (p. 74)

From the perspective of vowel typology, one of the most interesting aspects of acoustic descriptions is the detail often provided about how vowels vary as a function of adjacent consonants (Bessell 1997; Bird and Leonard 2009; Nolan 2017; Fortier 2019), position in the word (Gordon and Munro 2007), whether they are in a stressed position or not (Shahin and Blake 2004; Blake and Shahin 2008), etc. Kharlamov and Oberly (2019) documented the acoustic details of vowels in Southern Ute, a Uto-Aztecan language spoken in southeastern Colorado. They measured F1 and F2 for all the vowels and their systematic phonetic variations, or allophones.8 Among other things, they found that /ø/ had a significantly raised F1 (indicating a lowered tongue position) and lowered F2 (indicating a backed tongue position) before velars and uvulars (‘back of the mouth’ sounds), but not after them. Conversely, /a/ had a significantly raised F2 (indicating a fronted tongue position) after palatal (‘front of the mouth’) sounds /j/, /i/, or /ø/ but not before /j/, /i/, or /ø/. In other words, in Southern Ute, velar and uvular consonants tend to draw the previous vowel (/ø/ in particular) down and back, whereas palatal sounds tend to draw the following vowel (at least /a/) forward.9 Kharlamov and Oberly’s findings mirror those

8 See Fitzgerald, this volume, for a definition of ‘allophone’. 9 See Bliss, Bird, and Gick, this volume, for definitions of terms used to describe the place of articulation, like velar, uvular, and palatal.



Acoustic phonetics 

 21

of Nolan (2017), who found that in lək̓wəŋən (Central Salish), post-velar consonants also had the greatest influence on preceding vowels, whereas palatal consonants had the greatest influence on following vowels. Linguists use the term co-articulation to refer to the influence that adjacent sounds have on one another. Together, Kharlamov and Oberly’s (2019) and Nolan’s (2017) studies suggest that the observed asymmetry between palatal and velar/uvular consonants in their co-articulatory effects holds across languages and can be explained by physiological restrictions shared by speakers around the world. Not all co-articulatory effects are universal though. Bird and Leonard (2009) showed that, even within a single language family, co-articulatory patterns can vary. They considered existing descriptions of /iq/ and /qi/ sequences in Salish languages. These sequences are inherently challenging to pronounce because they require the tongue to move between opposite positions: front and high for /i/ and low and back for /q/ (see Gick and Wilson 2006). They found that co-articulatory effects differed across languages. In general, Central Salish languages were described as exhibiting asymmetrical co-articulatory effects: /qi/ sequences were pronounced with a lowered/retracted vowel, e.  g., [qe], whereas /iq/ sequences were pronounced with /i/ intact, but with a transitional element between /i/ and /q/, e.  g., [i-əq]. On the other hand, Interior Salish languages were described as exhibiting symmetrical co-articulatory effects: /qi/ and /iq/ were both either pronounced with a lowered/ retracted vowel ([qe] and [eq]) or with a transitional element ([q-ə-i] and [i-ə-q]). Figure 6 provides examples of two different versions of /qi/ within the Central Salish language SENĆOŦEN, pronounced by two different speakers. These show that real speech is in fact more variable than the generalizations above indicate. In 6a, we see that the vowel starts out as [i] (low F1 and high F2) but then transitions into a more central vowel [ə] before [q], with F1 raising and F2 lowering into [q]; in 6b, we see a stable, lowered and retracted [e] sound before [q] (raised F1 and lowered F2 compared to [i]). Knowing what strategies first language (L1) speakers use in their pronunciation can be very useful from a pedagogical perspective. Bird (2018) found that L1 speakers used a range of strategies to pronounce /iq/ and /qi/ sequences, all of which maintained the uvular place of /q/. In contrast, most second language (L2) speakers fronted the uvular /q/ to a velar [k] position, for example pronouncing /iq/ as [ik]. Having documented the differences between the pronunciations of L1 and L2 speakers, we can explain to L2 speakers what strategies L1 speakers use, so that they can choose among them rather than using a strategy that loses the /q/ sound (see also Bliss, Bird, and Gick, this volume, Section 2.3.2).

22 

 Sonya Bird, Rae Anne Claxton, and Tess Nolan

(a) Transitional vowel [iəq] in /st’iqəl/ (SDIKEL, ‘bog’)

(b) Stable, lowered and retracted [eq], in /st’iqəl/ (SDIKEL, ‘bog’) Fig. 6: Example strategies used for /iq/ articulation in SENĆOŦEN: [iəq] with a ­transitional vowel (a) vs. [eq] with a stable, lowered vowel (b). Adapted with ­permission (following Creative Commons license CC-BY-NC-SA 2.0) from Bird (2018), Figures 2 and 3.



Acoustic phonetics 

 23

1.3.2.2 Consonants What you need to know Especially for adults, it can be hard to learn how to hear and pronounce a lot of new consonants. Here are a few tips we have learnt in our own learning process: (1) Break down learning into steps: learn to hear a new sound first, then learn to pronounce it, and then learn where to use it (in what words). Celebrate the small victories along the way! (2) Languages of the Pacific Northwest in particular have a lot of consonants that might not be familiar to you. On top of this, they can occur together, in long strings (“clusters”). The elders we work with always tell us to slow down when we start speaking, to give each consonant its place. (3) Start with learning the sounds in short, simple words, and gradually build to more complex words and sentences. When you are learning to pronounce consonant clusters, try not to put vowels between them. (4) Once we have learned a sound that is unique to our language, we sometimes start to overuse it, to sound really authentic. This happens a lot with ejectives, for example (see below). When you are able to reliably pronounce a consonant, pay attention to what words have it – this is the next step in mindfully learning to hear and speak the language. In some languages, just a small change in a sound, e.  g., glottalization (see below), can change the tense, or even the entire meaning of a word. This is the case for Hul’q’umi’num’ (Central Salish), and we have found that it is useful to learn and practice minimal pairs. Research on consonants tends to focus on specific sounds or sets of sounds that are not found in the dominant language of the area, usually English. Examples include stops, fricatives, and affricates (Dart 1993; Gordon et al. 2002; Holton 2001; McDonough and Wood 2008), post-velar consonants (Bessell 1993; McDowell 2004; Wilson 2007; Bird 2016), ejectives (Ingram and Rigsby 1987; Maddieson, Smith, and Bessell 2001; Wright, Hargus and Davis 2002; Ham 2008; Nelson 2010; Bird 2016), and glottalized resonants (Bird et al. 2008; Bird 2011). Many existing studies on consonants, including those listed above, have been done on languages of the Pacific Northwest, which have particularly rich consonant inventories. In this section we focus on the acoustic characteristics of sounds that involve some kind of constriction or tightening at the throat: ejective stops and glottalized resonants. The Americas are home to over half of the world’s languages that have ejective consonants, and they are found in almost every language of western North America from northern California to Alaska (Maddieson 2013). Ejective stops have been divided into two types, based on their articulatory and acoustic properties: strong/stiff/tense and weak/slack/lax (Lindau 1984). Strong ejectives have a strong ‘popping’ sound when they are released, while weak ejectives are lacking this pop, and sound more like creaky voiced (laryngealized) sounds. The exact pronunciation of ejectives varies across languages (Lindau 1984) as well as within languages (based on factors like word position; Percival 2019), and even within speakers (Wright, Hargus, and Davis 2002). A single North American language – Tlingit (Na Dene, spoken in South Eastern Alaska (US) and Western British Columbia and Yukon (Canada)) – has ejective fricatives (Maddieson,

24 

 Sonya Bird, Rae Anne Claxton, and Tess Nolan

Smith, and Bessell 2001; see also Bliss, Bird, and Gick, this volume, Section 2.3.5); these are extremely rare across languages of the world (Ladefoged and Maddieson 1996). Figure 7 provides an example of a strong (a) vs. weak (b) version of /t’/, in two repetitions of the SENĆOŦEN word DIL,EḴ /t’íl’əq/ (‘strawberry’) pronounced by a fluent speaking elder (SENĆOŦEN ejectives vary quite a bit in their realization). In the strong version, /t’/ is released with a strong burst (vertical band of noise), followed by a relatively long period of silence (no noise) before the start of the following vowel (this is also seen with the sudden drop in intensity between the burst and the vowel); the regularly-spaced pitch pulses (vertical lines) and relatively high pitch contour at the beginning of the vowel show that it begins with regular (modal) voicing. In the weak version, there is no silence between the /t’/ and the following vowel (there is only a slight dip in intensity),

(a) Strong /t’/ in SENĆOŦEN DIL,EḴ /t’íl’əq/ (‘strawberry’)

(b) Weak /t’/ in SENĆOŦEN DIL,EḴ /t’íl’əq/ (‘strawberry’) Fig. 7: Word-initial strong (a) vs. weak (b) /t’/ in two repetitions of the SENĆOŦEN word /t’íl’əq/ (DIL,EḴ ‘strawberry’) pronounced by a fluent speaking elder.



Acoustic phonetics 

 25

and creakiness can be observed at the beginning of the vowel, seen as spread out and irregular pitch pulses and a relatively low pitch contour (see also Figure 4 above). Wright, Hargus and Davis (2002) studied the acoustic features of /t’/ in a single language – Witsuwit’en, a Dene language spoken in the central interior of British Columbia, Canada. Their study provides an excellent model for studying ejectives, because they used a comprehensive set of measurements to characterize /t’/ (compared to plain /t/). Auditorily, they heard variation in production similar to that illustrated in Figure 7 for SENĆOŦEN: some /t’/ tokens were very strong and popping, others sounded more aspirated (breathy), and yet others were weak and sounded like voiceless stops followed by creaky voice. Ultimately, the acoustic measures they took placed Witsuwit’en /t’/ in the weak category, although the authors argued that a binary distinction could not fully capture the range of productions observed. Interestingly, pitch movement during the following vowel differed between men and women, illustrating how important gender can be in influencing the details of speech (see Johnson, Ladefoged, and McDonough (1993) on men and women’s pronunciation of Navajo vowels). Wright, Hargus, and Davis’ (2002) study alongside others that have described ejectives (Flemming, Ladefoged, and Thomason 2008; Ham 2008; Percival, Bird and Gerdts 2018) in Indigenous languages of North America shows us that ejectives do not necessarily fall into neat strong vs. weak categories, and that variation can be substantial both within and across speakers. One particularly noteworthy feature of ejectives in the context of language revitalization is that, at least in some languages, they seem to be strengthening over time: in cases where they have traditionally been described as weak, they are now produced as relatively strong (Bird 2020). In addition, learners are using them more than their elders do (Babel 2009; Bird, Gerdts, and Leonard 2016; Haynes 2010). This shift likely results from a combination of the pedagogical context in which the sounds are being transmitted (Saito and van Poeteren 2012; Uther, Knoll, Burnham 2006) and factors related to cultural identity (Gatbonton, Trofimovich, and Segalowitz 2011; Haynes 2010; Nance et al. 2016). Another set of sounds that involve a tightening of the throat is glottalized resonants (GRs). These are resonant consonants which include both an articulation in the upper vocal tract (e.  g., at the lips for [m]) and constriction in the larynx (and sometimes the pharynx) (see Bliss, Bird, and Gick, this volume, Section 2.3.3). GRs are relatively rare sounds, found in only 20 of the 317 languages sampled by Maddieson (1984), but they are common in the languages of the Pacific Northwest. What is interesting about GRs is that glottalization varies both within and across languages, along two dimensions: (a) the timing of laryngeal constriction relative to the resonant articulation and (b) the phonetic realization of glottalization. Bird et al. (2008) document the timing between the laryngeal and resonant (oral) articulations in GRs in three different languages: St’át’imcets and NɬeɁkepmxcin (both Interior Salish), and Nuuchahnulth (Wakashan). They show that GRs are consistently pre-glottalized (e.  g., [Ɂm]) in Nuuchahnulth, meaning glottalization occurs at the beginning of the resonant; they are consistently post-glottalized (e.  g., [mɁ]) in NɬeɁkepmxcin, meaning glottalization occurs at the end of the resonant;

26 

 Sonya Bird, Rae Anne Claxton, and Tess Nolan

in St’át’imcets, their timing varies as a function of syllable position: pre-glottalized at the beginning of syllables (in onset position), post-glottalized at the end of syllables (in coda position), and mid-glottalized ([mɁm]) in intervocalic position (between two vowels). Figure 8 provides another example, from Hul’q’umi’num’ (Central Salish). In

(a) Pre-glottalized [Ɂl] in si’lu [siɁlə] (‘grandparent’)

(b) Post-glottalized [lɁ] in sisul’u [sisəlɁə] (‘grandparent-diminutive’)) Fig. 8: Pre-glottalized [Ɂl] in Hul’q’umi’num’ si’lu [siɁlə] (‘grandparent’) (a) vs. post-glottalized [lɁ] in sisul’u [sisəlɁə] (‘grandparent- DIM’) (b), as pronounced by co-author Rae Anne Claxton.



Acoustic phonetics 

 27

this language, full vowels (/a e i o u/) attract glottalization towards them; conversely, stressed /ə/ repels glottalization away from it. Thus, in Figure 8(a), /l’/ is pre-glottalized in si’lu [siɁlə] (‘grandparent’); in Figure 8(b), it is post-glottalized in the diminutive form sisul’u [sisəlɁə] (‘grandparent-diminutive’). Note the slight creakiness in [l] adjacent to the glottal stop [Ɂ] in both cases, also seen in Figures 4 and 7. Similar to ejectives, GRs also vary in terms of the phonetic realization of glottalization (Bird 2011). In some languages, glottalization seems to always be realized as a full glottal stop, e.  g., Hul’q’umi’num’ (see Figure 8). In other languages, glottalization is more variable and often realized as mild creakiness, especially in unstressed environments, as for /l’/ in SENĆOŦEN DIL,EḴ /t’íl’əq/ ‘strawberry’ (Figure 7). The languages of the Pacific Northwest in particular have contributed immensely to our understanding of glottal articulations. As with other examples provided in Section 1.3, documenting the variation we see in ejectives and glottalized resonants is helpful not only from the perspective of phonetic typology, but also from the perspective of pronunciation teaching and learning: knowing how these sounds are pronounced by fluent speakers can help learners achieve what they think of as ‘authentic’ pronunciation. Because these sounds tend to be relatively easy to “see” on visual displays like Figures 7 and 8, they are good candidates for incorporating visual tools into learning (Olson 2014): learners can be taught to record themselves in Praat (see Section 1.2 above) and try to approximate an Elder’s speech visually as well as auditorily. From our experiences, this is an engaging way to learn the details of pronunciation, because learners can get a very concrete sense of what they are doing differently from their Elders, and how to approximate their speech more closely (Bird et al. 2019). Before moving on to prosody and intonation (Section 1.3.2.3), it is worth mentioning that many Indigenous languages of North America have length contrasts, either in their vowel systems (Apache (Gordon et al. 2001); Aleut (Taff et al. 2001); Chickasaw (Gordon 2000, Gordon et al. 2001); Plains Cree (Muehlbauer 2012); Upper Tanana Athabascan (Tuttle, Lovick, and Núñez-Ortiz 2011); Washo (Yu 2008)) or consonant systems (Washo, (Yu 2008)). In these languages, sounds include long and short versions of the same vowel or consonant. For example, Gordon and Munro (2007) studied the vowel length distinction in Chickasaw, across four different speakers and between short and long /a, aː/, /o, oː/, and /i, iː/. In Chickasaw, vowel length alone can distinguish between two different words, e.  g., /iʃtalali/ (‘I bring it here’) vs. /iʃtalaːli/ (‘you set it upright’) differ in only the length of the vowel in the second-to-last syllable. In many languages with length contrasts, a short/long contrast is the whole story. In Chickasaw however, things are a bit more complex, in that short vowels can also be lengthened in prominent word positions (see Tuttle, this volume, on word prosody), leading to an intermediate vowel length. For example, the second short /a/ in /iʃtalali/ can be lengthened to [aˑ] because of its position in the word and the word-level prosody, leading to the pronunciation [iʃtalaˑli]. Gordon and Munro's results show short vowels are ~ 92 milliseconds long on average, the long vowels are ~155 milliseconds long on average, and the lengthened short vowels are ~140 milliseconds long on average.

28 

 Sonya Bird, Rae Anne Claxton, and Tess Nolan

1.3.2.3 Prosody and intonation What you need to know In language classes, we tend to focus listening and speaking work on individual sounds and words, but out in the “real world” (in community), intonation – the flow of whole sentences – is also very important. Learning the rhythm and flow of a language can be done somewhat separately from learning the consonants and vowels. A good way to learn is to replace all the syllables in a sentence with . For example, “the picture is beautiful” (with seven syllables) can be pronounced “ma MA ma ma MA ma ma” (capital letters mark prominent syllables). Doing this lets learners abstract away from challenging sounds, and focus on the flow of whole sentences – what syllables are more emphasized, and how emphasis is created. Once we have the flow down, we can start adding the consonants and vowels back in. Prosody and intonation refer to the rhythmic flow of words and sentences. Though less researched than segmental phonetics (vowels and consonants), prosody and ­intonation have also been the focus of a small number of studies. At the word level (see Tuttle, this volume, and Uchihara, this volume), the most commonly studied features are lexical tone and pitch accent (Martin and Johnson 2002; Gordon 2008; Uchihara 2013; Rice 2014; Hirata-Edds and Herrick 2017). For example, Martin and Johnson (2002) studied what they call “tonal accent” in Muskogee Creek, a Muskogean language spoken in eastern Oklahoma. Their goal was to acoustically confirm the auditory-based descriptions made by earlier researchers, in particular Haas (1977). Haas described three tones (level, falling, and rising) that could be borne by “key” (prominent) syllables in a word, creating grammatical (aspectual) contrasts. For example, the difference in tones creates the difference in aspect in three forms of the stem apoːk- ‘sit (of three or more)’: level tone [apóːkiːs] ‘we are (here),’ falling tone [apôːkiːs] ‘we have sat down, are in a sitting position,’ and rising tone [apǒːⁿkiːs] ‘we keep sitting and sitting’ (Martin and Johnson 2002, p. 29). Corroborating Haas’ descriptions, Martin and Johnson found that (1) level tones maintained a generally level pitch that slightly decreased over time; (2) falling tones varied by position: in initial position they had a rise and then a fall and, in medial and final positions, they gradually declined; (3) rising tones rose to a peak before slightly falling. Additionally, in words with multiple key syllables, tone was slightly lower on any key syllable following the initial one. Martin and Johnson also reported on a number of cases where Haas’ phonetic descriptions did not exactly match their acoustic findings. This raises two points to consider when doing acoustic work to corroborate earlier descriptive work: (1) the language may change over time or place (e.  g., if speakers are from different communities), leading to observed discrepancies and (2) acoustic analysis does not always match up perfectly with auditory analysis (what we hear), for various reasons (Bird and Leonard 2009). In our experience, automatic pitch tracking in Praat is somewhat unreliable, and therefore should be confirmed with manual measurements and with our ears. Martin and Johnson’s



Acoustic phonetics 

 29

study is a good example of how the tools available now allow us to confirm and also go beyond previous work, giving us more insight into the fine phonetic details of speech, especially in ways that the ear may not pick up on unaided. In terms of prosody beyond the word (see Zúñiga, this volume), one common topic is question intonation, which refers to the flow of questions, including the rising and falling of pitch (Taff et al. 2001; Jacobs 2007; Oberg 2007; Koch 2008). It has been claimed that there is a universal tendency for languages to indicate yes/no questions with either a rising pitch (as in English ‘they are going to the store?’) or an overall higher pitch, and to indicate statements with a falling pitch (as in English ‘they are going to the store.’) or an overall lower pitch (Ladd 1981). Several North American languages show the latter pattern: an overall raised pitch in yes/no questions compared to statements. For example, Jacobs (2007) compared the intonation of declarative statements vs. yes/no questions in Skwxwu7mesh, a Central Salish language spoken in southwestern British Columbia, Canada. He measured (a) the overall change in pitch from the beginning to the end of the sentence and (b) the average pitches on the first two and last two syllables. He showed that both statements and questions had a fall in pitch from the start of the sentence to the end (Figure 9). In statements, the pitch fell consistently across the syllables, whereas in questions, the pitch initially rose through the question marker (first syllable) and then fell through the rest of the question, leading to an overall raised pitch compared to statements. Questions in Skwxwu7mesh are also marked by the presence of a question particle, making statements and questions syntactically distinct, and avoiding confusion between similar intonation patterns.

Fig. 9: Pitch contours of Skwxwu7mesh declaratives and yes/no questions. Original source (following Creative Commons license CC-BYNC-SA 2.0): Jacobs (2007), Table 9 “Comparison of pitch”, p. 247.

30 

 Sonya Bird, Rae Anne Claxton, and Tess Nolan

In their work on Aleut, an Eskimo-Aleut language spoken in southwestern Alaska,10 Taff et al. (2001) found that both statements and yes/no questions had falling intonation, and rises in pitch word-internally occurred in both types of sentences as well. A slight rise in the final content word was seen in questions for some speakers, but not for others, and this rise was also seen in some declaratives, so Taff et al. did not consider this feature to be distinctive of yes/no questions. They also compared the median pitches of statements and questions for each speaker and found that the pitch range of the questions for each speaker was within the range of their declarative sentences, neither higher nor lower. Taff et al.’s study shows that North American languages can “buck the trend” so to speak when it comes to assumed universals about intonation. The common theme emerging from the brief literature review presented in Section 1.3 is that Indigenous languages of North America have much to offer in terms of the richness of their phonetic structures. Research documenting these patterns has contributed and will continue to contribute greatly to our understanding of cross-linguistic as well as language-specific patterns of phonetic realization. In addition, it has provided a robust foundation for developing pedagogical resources for teaching and learning the details of pronunciation, which is necessarily language specific.

1.4 New wave of phonetic research: ­community-based and collaborative As mentioned in Section 1.3.1, much of the existing phonetic work on Indigenous languages of North America is highly technical, and as a result not terribly accessible to community members and language learners and teachers. Nonetheless, the studies cited in Section 1.3.2 have hopefully shown that conducting phonetic research can be valuable from the perspective of teaching and learning pronunciation in the context of language revitalization. In this final section, we offer our thoughts on how to conduct phonetic work in an accessible and collaborative way. Donna Gerdts (2010) talks about the linguist as one member of a language revitalization team. Indeed, in our experience, the most valuable kind of acoustic phonetic work combines the expertise of linguists, Elders and teachers, and learners (see also Bird and Miyashita 2019). Elders, teachers, and learners are in the best position to identify the challenges that learners face in speaking and hearing their language(s). These challenges most often correspond to features that differ from those of the majority language (English in our case) and that are particularly interesting from the perspective of phonetic typology. Community members also know best how to design research methods

10 Their study was in fact a very broad one, which covered consonant properties, vowel space, wordlevel stress, and sentence intonation.



Acoustic phonetics 

 31

for studying speech in culturally appropriate ways. Linguists (phoneticians) have the technical expertise to analyze the acoustic features of speech instrumentally. Once the phonetic details are understood, the team can work together to determine how best to make the research findings accessible and useful for teachers and learners. Naatosi Fish and Mizuki Miyashita (Fish and Miyashita 2017, Fish 2018)’s “pitch art” – stylized pitch contours of Blackfoot (Algonquian) tonal melodies (Figure 10) – is an excellent example of how teamwork can lead to engaging tools for teaching and learning the phonetic details of speech.

Fig. 10: Pitch Art of the Blackfoot word makóyi (wolf). Copied with permission from Fish (2018).

Collaborative phonetic work like Fish and Miyashita’s pitch art project has also been documented by Herrick et al. (2015) working on Cherokee and Bird et al. (2019) working on Hul’q’umi’num’. A recent Special Session at the International Conference of the Phonetic Sciences (Billington and Bird 2019) brought together linguists working with Indigenous and minority language communities around the world to share ideas about best practices for ensuring that we continue to work together in the most effective ways to support communities’ efforts in sound-related work, as part of larger language maintenance and revitalization efforts. We are enthusiastic about these continued partnerships and their contribution to our understanding of phonetic structures across the languages of the world and also to our ability to create resources for teaching and learning pronunciation. Acknowledgements: HÍSW̱ḴE SI,IÁM, huy tseep q’u sii’em’, and thank you to our community partners, who continue to shape the way we go about conducting acoustic phonetic work. Hul’q’umi’num’: late Dr. Sti’tum’at Ruby Peter, Swustanulwut Delores Louie, Dr. Donna Gerdts, Hul’q’umi’num’ Language & Culture Society, and students in the Hul’q’umi’num’ Language Academy. SENĆOŦEN: late Ivan Morris Sr. and late Ray Sam, Tye Swallow, teachers and students in the W̱,SENĆOŦEN IST program. Thank you also to the editors and reviewers for your valuable feedback.

32 

 Sonya Bird, Rae Anne Claxton, and Tess Nolan

References Cited in the chapter Asher, James J. 1977. Learning another language through actions: The complete teacher’s guidebook. Los Gatos, CA: Sky Oaks Productions. Babel, Molly. 2009. The phonetic and phonological effects of obsolescence in Northern Paiute. In James Stanford & Dennis R. Preston (eds.), Variation in indigenous minority languages, 23–46. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Babel, Molly, Michael J. Houser & Maziar Toosarvandani. 2012. Illustrations of the IPA: Mono Lake Northern Paiute. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 42(2). 233–243. Barthmaier, Paul. 1998. Lushootseed vowels: A preliminary phonetic analysis. In Papers for the International Conference on Salish and Neighboring Languages (ICSNL) 33, 1–11. The University of British Columbia. Vancouver, BC. Bessell, Nicola. 1993. Towards a phonetic and phonological typology of post-velar articulation. Vancouver, BC: University of British Columbia dissertation. Bessell, Nicola. 1997. St’át’imcets vowels and consonants: A phonetic study. In Papers for the International Conference on Salish and Neighboring Languages (ICSNL) 32, 64–85. The University of British Columbia. Vancouver, BC. Bird, Sonya. 2011. The nature of laryngealization in St’át’imcets laryngealized resonants. International Journal of American Linguistics 77(2). 159–184. Bird, Sonya. 2016. Pronunciation change in SENĆOŦEN: A acoustic study of /k, kw, kw’, q, q’, qw qw’/ across generations of speakers. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Acoustical Society of America. Salt Lake City, UT. Bird, Sonya. 2018. Phonetics in the context of language revitalization: Exploring pronunciation strategies. In Lisa Matthewson, Erin Guntly, & Michael Rochement (eds.), Wa7 xweysás i nqwal’utteníha i ucwalmícwa: He loves the people’s languages. Essays in honour of Henry Davis, 493–508. (University of British Columbia Occasional Papers in Linguistics 6). Vancouver, BC: University of British Columbia. Bird, Sonya. 2020. Pronunciation among Adult Indigenous Language Learners: the case of SENĆOŦEN /t’/. Journal of Second Language Pronunciation. 6(2). 148–179. Bird, Sonya, Marion Caldecott, Fiona Campbell, Bryan Gick & Patricia A. Shaw. 2008. Oral-laryngeal timing in glottalized resonants. Journal of Phonetics 36. 492–507. Bird, Sonya, Rae Anne Claxton, Luschiimtunaat Sonya Charlie, S-hwutstus Harvey George, Maida Percival & Sq’utxulenuhw George Seymour. 2019. Seeing speech: Teaching and learning Hul’q’umi’num’ pronunciation with Praat. Paper presented at the International Conference on Language Documentation and Conservation. Honolulu, HI. Bird, Sonya, Donna Gerdts & Janet Leonard. 2016. The realization of obstruents across speakers of Hul’q’umi’num’. Canadian Acoustics 44(3). 134–135. Bird, Sonya & Sarah Kell. 2017. The role of pronunciation in SENĆOŦEN language revitalization. Canadian Modern Language Review 73(4). 538–569. Bird, Sonya & Janet Leonard. 2009. The universality of articulatory conflict-resolution: Evidence from Salish languages. Northwest Journal of Linguistics 3(2). 1–29. Bird, Sonya & Mizuki Miyashita. 2019. Teaching phonetics in the context of Indigenous language revitalization. Proceedings of the 2018 International Symposium on Applied Phonetics (ISAPh), 39–44. https:// www.isca-speech.org/archive/ISAPh_2018/pdfs/07.pdf Billington, Rosey & Sonya Bird. 2019. Special Session at the 19th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS): Phoneticians in partnership with communities in language revitalization and maintenance. Melbourne, Australia, August 5–9.



Acoustic phonetics 

 33

Blake, Susan & Kimary Shahin. 2008. On the phonetics of schwa in Sliammon (M. Comox Salish): Implications for the representation of Salish vowels. Canadian Acoustics 36(4). 42–43. Bliss, Heather, Sonya Bird, Strang Burton, PEPAḴIYE Ashley Cooper & Bryan Gick. 2018. Seeing speech: Ultrasound-based multimedia resources for pronunciation learning in Indigenous languages. Language Documentation & Conservation 12. 315–338. Bliss, Heather, Sonya Bird & Bryan Gick. 2023 [this volume]. Articulatory phonetics. In Carmen Dagostino, Marianne Mithun & Keren Rice (eds.), The Languages and Linguistics of Indigenous North America: A Comprehensive Guide. Berlin & Boston: De Gruyter Mouton. Boersma, Paul & David Weenink. 2022. Praat: Doing phonetics by computer [Computer program]. Version 6.2.10, retrieved 17 March 2022 from http://www.praat.org Brown, Jason, Henry Davis, Michael Schwan & Barbara Sennott. 2016. Illustrations of the IPA: Gitksan. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 46(3). 367–378. Caldecott, Marion & Karsten Koch. 2014. Using mixed media tools for eliciting discourse in Indigenous languages. Language Documentation & Conservation 8. 209–240. https://scholarspace.manoa.hawaii. edu/bitstream/10125/24603/1/caldecott.pdf Carlson, Barry & John H. Esling. 2001. Illustrations of the IPA: Spokane. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 30(1–2). 97–102. Carlson, Barry, John H. Esling & Katie Fraser. 2001. Illustrations of the IPA: Nuuchahnulth. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 31(2). 275–279. Dart, Sarah N. 1993. Phonetic properties of O’odham stop and fricative contrasts. International Journal of American Linguistics 59(1). 16–37. De Decker, Paul & Jennifer Nycz. 2011. For the record: Which digital media can be used for sociophonetic analysis? University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics 17(2). 51–59. https://repository.upenn. edu/pwpl/vol17/iss2/7 Dry, Helen Aristar. 2009. Preserving digital language materials: Some considerations for community initiatives. In Wayne Harbert, Sally McConnell-Ginet, Amanda Miller, & John Whitman (eds.), Language and Poverty, 202–222. Bristol: Multilingual Matters. ELAN (Version 5.6) [Computer software]. 2019, June 04. Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics. https://tla.mpi.nl/tools/tla-tools/elan/ Fish, Naatosi. 2018. Pedagogy of pitch in L2 Blackfoot. University of Montana Undergraduate Theses and Professional Papers. 193. https://scholarworks.umt.edu/utpp/193/ Fish, Naatosi & Mizuki Miyashita. 2017. Guiding pronunciation of Blackfoot melody. In Jon Reyhner, Joseph Martin, Louise Lockard & Willard Sakiestewa Gilbert (eds.), Honoring Our Teachers, 203–210. Flagstaff, AZ: Northern Arizona University. Flemming, Edward, Peter Ladefoged & Sarah Thomason. 2008. Phonetic structures of Montana Salish. Journal of Phonetics 36. 465–491. Fortier, Kyra. 2019. An acoustic investigation of vowel variation in Gitksan. Victoria, BC: University of Victoria MA thesis. Gardner, Evan. 2019. Where Are Your Keys. https://whereareyourkeys.org/ Gatbonton, Elizabeth, Pavel Trofimovich & Norman Segalowitz. 2011. Ethnic group affiliation and patterns of development of a phonological variable. The Modern Language Journal 95(2). 188–204. Gerdts, Donna. 2010. Beyond expertise. The role of the linguist in language revitalization programs. In Lenore Grenoble & N. Louanna Furbee (eds.), Language documentation practice and values, 173–192. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Gick, Brian & Ian Wilson. 2006. Excrescent schwa and vowel laxing: Cross-linguistic responses to conflicting articulatory targets. In Louis Goldstein, Douglas H. Whalen & Catherine T. Best (eds.), Laboratory Phonology VIII, 635–659. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter.

34 

 Sonya Bird, Rae Anne Claxton, and Tess Nolan

Gordon, Matthew. 1996. The phonetic structures of Hupa. In Ian Maddison (ed.), Fieldwork studies of targeted languages IV. (University of California Los Angeles Working Papers in Phonetics 93). 164–187. Los Angeles, CA: University of California. Gordon, Matthew. 2008. Pitch accent timing and scaling in Chickasaw. Journal of Phonetics 36. 521–535. Gordon, Matthew. 2017. Phonetic and phonological research on Native American languages: Past, present, and future. International Journal of American Linguistics 83(1). 79–110. Gordon, Matthew, Paul Barthmaier & Kathy Sands. 2002. A cross-linguistic acoustic study of voiceless fricatives. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 32(2). 141–174. Gordon, Matthew, Jack B. Martin & Linda Langley. 2015. Some phonetic structures of Koasati. International Journal of American Linguistics 81(1). 83–118. Gordon, Matthew & Pamela Munro. 2007. A phonetic study of final vowel lengthening in Chickasaw. International Journal of American Linguistics 73(3). 293–330. Gordon, Matthew, Pamela Munro & Peter Ladefoged. 2000. Some phonetic structures of Chickasaw. Anthropological Linguistics 42(3). 366–400. Gordon, Matthew, Pamela Munro & Peter Ladefoged. 2001. Illustrations of the IPA: Chickasaw. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 31(2). 287–290. Gordon, Matthew, Brian Potter, John Dawson, Willem de Reuse & Peter Ladefoged. 2001. Phonetic structures of Western Apache. International Journal of American Linguistics 67(4). 415–448. Haas, Mary R. 1977. Tonal accent in Creek. In Larry M. Hyman (ed.), Studies in stress and accent, 195–208. (Southern California Occasional Papers in Linguistics 4). Los Angeles, CA: University of Southern California. Ham, SooYoun. 2008. Tsilhqut’in ejectives: A descriptive phonetic study. Victoria, BC: University of Victoria MA thesis. Hargus, Sharon & Keren Rice (eds.). 2005. Athabaskan Prosody. (Current Issues in Linguistic Theory 269). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins. Hargus, Sharon & Virginia Beavert. 2014. Illustrations of the IPA: Northwest Sahaptin. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 44(3). 319–342. Haynes, Erin. 2010. Phonetic and phonological acquisition in endangered languages learned by adults: A case study of Numu (Oregon Northern Paiute). Berkeley, CA: University of California Berkeley dissertation. Holton, Gary. 2001. Fortis and lenis fricatives in Tanacross Athapaskan. International Journal of American Linguistics 67(4). 396–414. Ingram, John & Bruce Rigsby. 1987. Glottalic stops in Gitksan: An acoustic analysis. 11th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS XI), 134–137. Hirata-Edds, Tracy & Dylan Herrick. 2017. Building tone resources for second language learners from phonetic documentation: Cherokee examples. Language Documentation & Conservation 11. 289–304. Herrick, Dylan, Marcellino Berardo, Durbin Feeling, Tracy Hirata-Edds & Lizette Peter. 2015. Collaborative documentation and revitalization of Cherokee tone. Language Documentation & Conservation 9. 12–31. International Phonetic Association. 1999. Handbook of the International Phonetic Association: A guide to the use of the International Phonetic Alphabet. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Jacobs, Peter. 2007. Intonation of yes/no questions in Skwxwú7mesh. In Kristin M. Jóhannsdóttir & Martin A. Oberg (eds.), Papers for the International Conference on Salish and Neighboring Languages (ICSNL) 42, 236–255. (University of British Columbia Working Papers in Linguistics 20). Vancouver, BC: University of British Columbia. Jenni, Barbara, Adar Anisman, Onowa McIvor & Peter Jacobs. 2017. An exploration of the effects of mentor-apprentice programs on mentors’ and apprentices’ wellbeing. International Journal of Indigenous Health 12(2). 25–42. Johnson, Keith, Peter Ladefoged & Joyce McDonough. 1993. Do women listen to men’s voices with men’s mouths in mind? The Journal of the Acoustical Society 93. 2298. Doi: 10.1121/1.406484



Acoustic phonetics 

 35

Kharlamov, Viktor & Stacey Oberly. 2019. Phonetics of Southern Ute vowels. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 49(2). 1–19. Koch, Karsten. 2008. Intonation and focus in Nlhe7kepmxcin (Thompson River Salish). Vancouver, BC: University of British Columbia dissertation. Ladd, D. Robert. 1981. On intonational universals. In Terry Meyers, John Laver & John M. Anderson (eds.), The cognitive representation of speech, 389–397. (Advances in Psychology 7). Amsterdam & New York: North Holland Publishing Company. Ladefoged, Peter. 2003. Phonetic data analysis: An introduction to fieldwork and instrumental techniques. Malden, MA: Blackwell. Ladefoged, Peter & Keith Johnson. 2015. A Course in Phonetics. 7th edn. Stamford, CT: Cengage Learning. Ladefoged, Peter & Ian Maddieson. 1996. The Sounds of the World’s Languages. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell. Lee, Junkyu, Juhyun Jang & Luke Plonsky. 2015. The effectiveness of second language pronunciation instruction: A meta-analysis. Applied Linguistics 36(3). 345–366. Lindau, Mona. 1984. Phonetic differences in glottalic consonants. Journal of Phonetics 12. 147–55. Maddieson, Ian. 1984. Patterns of sound. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. Maddieson, Ian. 2013. Glottalized Consonants. In Matthew S. Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. https://wals.info/chapter/7 Maddieson, Ian, Caroline L. Smith & Nicola Bessell. 2001. Aspects of the phonetics of Tlingit. Anthropological Linguistics 43(2). 135–176. Martin, Jack B. & Keith Johnson. 2002. An acoustic study of “tonal accent” in Creek. International Journal of American Linguistics 68(1). 28–50. McDonough, Joyce. 2003. The Navajo sound system. (Studies in Natural Language & Linguistics 55). Dordrecht, Boston, London: Kluwer Academic Publishers. McDonough, Joyce, Douglas H. Whalen. 2008. Editorial: The phonetics of native North American languages. Journal of Phonetics 36. 423–426. McDonough, Joyce & Valerie Wood. 2008. The stop contrasts of the Athabaskan languages. Journal of Phonetics 36. 427–449. McDowell, Ramona. 2004. Retraction in Montana Salish lateral consonants. Vancouver, BC: University of British Columbia MA thesis. McIvor, Onowa. 2015. Adult Indigenous language learning in Western Canada: What is holding us back? In Kathryn A. Michel, Patrick D. Walton, Emma Bourassa & Jack Miller (eds.), Living our languages: Papers from the 19th Stabilizing Indigenous Languages Symposium, 37–49. Ronkonkoma, NY: Linus Learning. Montler, Timothy. 1986. An outline of the morphology and phonology of Saanich, North Straits Salish. (University of Montana Occasional Papers in Linguistics 4). Missoula, MT: University of Montana Linguistics Laboratory. http://saanich.montler.net/Outline/index.htm Montler, Timothy. 2018. SENĆOŦEN: A dictionary of the Saanich language. Seattle, WA: University of Washington Press. Muehlbauer, Jeffrey. 2012. Vowel spaces in Plains Cree. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 42(1). 91–105. Nance, Claire, Wilson McLeod, Bernadette O’Rourke & Stuart Dunmore. 2016. Identity, accent aim, and motivation in second language users: New Scottish Gaelic speakers’ use of phonetic variation. Journal of Sociolinguistics 20(2). 164–191. Neal, Thomas. 2018. A phonetic description of the Kawaiisu language. Tucson, AZ: University of Arizona dissertation. Nelson, Katherine. 2010. Ejectives in Nez Perce. Santa Barbara Papers in Linguistics 21. 1–19. Nelson, Katherine. 2013. The Nez Perce vowel system: A phonetic analysis. Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics 19. 1–9.

36 

 Sonya Bird, Rae Anne Claxton, and Tess Nolan

Nolan, Tess. 2017. A phonetic investigation of vowel variation in Lekwungen. Victoria, BC: University of Victoria MA thesis. Oberg, Martin. 2007. Intonation contours in St’át’imcets. In Kristin M. Jóhannsdóttir & Martin A. Oberg (eds.), Papers for the International Conference on Salish and Neighboring Languages (ICSNL) 42, 357–370. (University of British Columbia Working Papers in Linguistics 20). Vancouver, BC. Oberly, Stacey. 2008. A phonetic analysis of Southern Ute with a discussion of Southern Ute language policies and revitalization. Tucson, AZ: University of Arizona dissertation. Olson, Daniel J. 2014. Benefits of visual feedback on segmental production in the L2 classroom. Language Learning and Technology 18(3). 173–192. Olson, Kenneth. 2004. Illustrations of the IPA: Mono. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 34(2). 233–238. Percival, Maida. 2019. Contextual variation in the acoustics of ejective stops. Proceedings of ICPhS 2019. Percival, Maida, Sonya Bird & Donna Gerdts. 2018. Laryngeal contrasts in first and second language speakers of Hul’q’umi’num’. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Acoustical Society of America. November 5–9, Victoria, BC. Rice, Keren. 2014. On beginning the study of the tone system of a Dene (Athabaskan) language: Looking back. Language Documentation & Conservation 8. 690—706. Saito, Kazuya & Kim van Poeteren. 2012. Pronunciation-specific adjustment strategies for intelligibility in L2 teacher talk: results and implications of a questionnaire study. Language Awareness 21(4). 369–385. Shahin, Kimary & Susan Blake. 2004. A Phonetic Study of Schwa in St’át’imcets (Lillooet Salish). In Donna Gerdts & Lisa Matthewson (eds.), Studies in Salish Linguistics in honour of M. Dale Kinkade, 311–327. (University of Montana Occasional Papers in Linguistics 17). Missoula, MT: University of Montana Press. Taff, Alice, Lorna Rozelle, Taehong Cho, Peter Ladefoged, Moses Dirks & Jacob Wegelin. 2001. Phonetic structures of Aleut. Journal of Phonetics 29. 231–271. Tuttle, Siri G., Olga Lovick & Isabel Núñez-Ortiz. 2011. Vowels of Upper Tanana Athabascan. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 41(3). 283–312 Tuttle, Siri G. & Merton Sandoval. 2002. Illustrations of the IPA: Jicarilla Apache. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 32(1). 105–112. Uchihara, Hiroto. 2016. Tone and accent in Oklahoma Cherokee. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Uther, Maria, Monja Angelika Knoll & Denis Burnham. 2006. Do you speak E-NG-LI-SH? A comparison of foreigner- and infant-directed speech. Speech Communication 49. 2–7. Wilson, Ian. 2007. The effects of post-velar consonants on vowels in Nuu-chah-nulth: Auditory, acoustic, and articulatory evidence. Canadian Journal of Linguistics/Revue canadienne de linguistique 52(1). 43–70. Wright, Richard, Sharon Hargus & Katharine Davis. 2002. On the categorization of ejectives: Data from Witsuwit’en. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 32(1). 43–77. Yu, Alan C. L. 2008. The phonetics of quantity alternation in Washo. Journal of Phonetics 36. 508–520.

Other readings of interest Linguistics and Indigenous language revitalization Czaykowska-Higgins, Ewa. 2009. Research models, community engagement, and linguistic fieldwork: Reflections on working within Canadian Indigenous communities. Language Documentation & Conservation 3(1). 15–50. Fitzgerald, Colleen M. 2017. Motivating the documentation of the verbal arts: Arguments from theory and practice. Language Documentation & Conservation 11. 114–132.



Acoustic phonetics 

 37

Fitzgerald, Colleen M. 2017. The sounds of Indigenous language revitalization. Plenary address, 91st Annual General Meeting of the Linguistic Society of America. January 5–8, Austin, TX. Percival, Maida, Heather Bliss & Murray Schellenberg. 2017. Methodological trade-offs for dual-purpose phonetic fieldwork. Canadian Acoustics 45(3). 184–185.  Rice, Keren. 2010. The linguist’s responsibilities to the community of speakers: Community-based research. In Lenore A. Grenoble & N. Louanna Furbee (eds.), Language documentation: Practice and values, 25–36. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

Methods Bird, Sonya. 2011. Phonetic fieldwork in the Pacific Northwest. International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS) 17. Hong Kong.

Other broad phonetic descriptions Mithun, Marianne. 1999. The languages of Native North America. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Nelson, Katherine. 2013. An acoustic analysis and cross-linguistic study of the phonemic inventory of Nez Perce. Houston, TX: Rice University dissertation.

Consonants Bird, Sonya. 2004. Lheidli intervocalic consonants: Phonetic and morphological effects. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 34(1). 69–91. Kingston, John. 1985. The phonetics and phonology of the timing of oral and glottal events. Berkeley CA: University of California Berkeley dissertation. Kingston, John. 1990. Articulatory binding. In John Kingston & Mary E. Beckman (eds.), Between the grammar and physics of speech, 406–434. (Laboratory Phonology I). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Stonham, John & Eun-Sook Kim. 2008. Labialization in Nuuchahnulth. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 38(1). 25–50.

Vowels Herrick, Dylan. 2011. On Comanche’s central mid vowel. International Journal of American Linguistics 77(3). 373–396. Oberly, Stacey & Viktor Kharlamov. 2015. The phonetic realization of devoiced vowels in the Southern Ute language. Phonetica 72. 1–19. Russell, Kevin. 2008. Sandhi in Plains Cree. Journal of Phonetics 36. 450–464.

38 

 Sonya Bird, Rae Anne Claxton, and Tess Nolan

Syllables Bird, Sonya & Ewa Czaykowska-Higgins. 2016. Parsing Salish consonant clusters. In Martin J. Ball & Nicole Müller (eds.), Challenging Sonority: Cross-linguistic evidence, 159–199. Sheffield, UK: Equinox Publishing Home. Hargus, Sharon & Virginia Beavert. 2002. Yakima Sahaptin clusters and epenthetic [i]. Anthropological Linguistics 44(3). 231–277.

Word-level prosody/intonation Gordon, Matthew. 2004. A phonological and phonetic study of word-level stress in Chickasaw. International Journal of American Linguistics 70(1). 1–32. Gordon, Matthew & Edmundo Luna. 2004. An intergenerational study of Hupa stress. Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (BLS) 30. 105–117.

Heather Bliss, Sonya Bird and Bryan Gick

2 Articulatory phonetics

Abstract: While the field of articulatory phonetics dates back millennia, throughout most of the field’s history, investigations have been largely impressionistic. With many relatively recent and continually evolving technological innovations, the tools and methods we can use to analyze speech articulation have opened up the space for new discoveries and approaches. Moreover, these technologies are increasingly affordable and portable, meaning that they can be used “in the field,” in community contexts with speakers of Indigenous languages in North America. As new technologies develop, our awareness is concurrently growing about the importance and necessity of conducting ethical and engaged research with Indigenous communities. In this chapter we survey tools and methods that have been used for articulatory research with Indigenous languages in North America, with a focus on how these tools and methods can enrich and enhance community-based research. Articulatory data can be an important part of both language documentation and language revitalization, as it can provide detailed information about the complexities and variation in linguistic sound systems, which can be used to assist learners in reclaiming their languages. As technologies and perspectives continue to evolve, the potential for articulatory phonetic research to inform language revitalization practices continues to grow.

2.1 Introduction Often, we talk about speech as sound, focusing on the acoustic signal associated with it (see Bird, Nolan, and Claxton, this volume). To understand sound though, we need to have an understanding of the movements responsible for creating it. Articulatory Phonetics is the subfield of linguistics focused on studying the articulations underlying speech sounds: movements of the tongue, the jaw, the larynx, and other structures used in speech production. Documenting the articulatory characteristics of speech tells us about the ways that humans are able to manipulate their vocal apparatus to create sounds unique to their languages. It also helps with teaching and learning new sounds not present in the language(s) we are familiar with. As detailed throughout this chapter, articulatory phonetic studies complement acoustic phonetic studies by providing detail on how we to produce speech sounds by manipulating our articulators. Unlike acoustic phonetics (see Bird, Nolan, and Claxton, this volume), articulatory phonetics requires technology that has historically been relatively inaccessible, particularly for use in Indigenous communities, because of cost, lack of portability, and the need for specialized expertise. However, as technology becomes increasingly affordable, portable, and user-friendly, new opportunities are emerging for exploring the articulatory properties of Indigenous languages and for developing resources for communities https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-002

40 

 Heather Bliss, Sonya Bird and Bryan Gick

engaged in language documentation and revitalization, in particular ones focused on developing oral proficiency among new speakers. In this chapter, we start by introducing the field of Articulatory Phonetics, and how it can be a useful component of Indigenous language documentation (Section 2.2). We then describe some of the tools used for articulatory phonetic research, and how they can be applied in Indigenous language research (Section 2.3). We end with a brief discussion of areas of future research, in terms of articulatory features to explore as well as methods for doing this following collaborative, community-engaged models of research (Section 2.4).

2.2 What is articulatory phonetics and why is it important in Indigenous language research? 2.2.1 A brief history of the field of articulatory phonetics Articulatory phonetics has a long history, going back over two millennia. Many of the familiar terms and concepts we use today to describe speech sounds – including “articulator,” “place of articulation,” “stop,” “fricative,” “vowel,” and many others  – were reintroduced by 19th-century British School phoneticians from the works of ancient Indian scholars such as Pāṇini and Patañjali (see Allen 1953). Notwithstanding its long history and technological advances, the ways we use articulatory phonetics to describe languages has not changed much in the course of its 2500-year history. Articulatory phonetic descriptions have long been a core part of the grammatical descriptions of Indigenous languages (e.  g., Goddard 1907), though the phonetic portions of traditional grammars have often been very brief and impressionistic. Even recently, many descriptions of how speech sounds are produced seem more impressionistic than scientific. One survey of twenty descriptive grammars published between 1989 and 2000 revealed that the mean percentage of content devoted to sound systems is under 10 % and the large majority of this is phonological, not phonetic (Maddieson 2002). In recent years, however, technological advances have begun to allow us to “look inside” the moving vocal tract to gain a better understanding of the kinds of movements we make when we speak. As Rice (2006: 239) observes: “Many recent grammars…contain far more discussion of phonetics than do older grammars. …Technological advances have allowed phonetics to become a core part of linguistic training in a way that was difficult even fifteen years ago.” The particular technologies researchers have used have had an important influence on the shape of modern articulatory phonetics. Unlike the study of acoustic phonetics (see Bird, Nolan, and Claxton, this volume), in which large quantities of data can be easily recorded and analyzed, articulatory phonetics often relies on more involved, labor-intensive procedures for recording and analyzing data. As a result, articulatory



Articulatory phonetics 

 41

phoneticians are more likely to produce smaller case studies, or studies with a focus on individual variability. In some ways, this research tradition is well suited to Indigenous languages with small populations of speakers; unlike acoustic studies, there is less of a need to justify having small participant groups, as is true for all sciences with case-study design (see Bird 2011). We are better equipped today than ever before to tackle some of the big questions in the field of articulatory phonetics. Some of these include: What is the relationship between sounds and movements in speech? What kinds of basic physical processes or mechanisms do we use when we produce speech? Where do these come from and how do they develop in children? How do we coordinate the complex movements of speech sounds and transitions between speech sounds? What are the range and sources of variation in articulatory patterns? How can an understanding of the physical mechanisms of speech assist language learners (e.  g., how can speech articulations be visualized to assist language teaching and learning)? To address any of these questions, it is essential to investigate a wide diversity of languages, particularly those that have not been traditionally included in typological studies. As an example, the very idea that “sound” is fundamental to speech is based on observations of widely spoken and widely studied mainstream languages. It was only recently that an instrumental study of two Indigenous languages in North America – Oneida (Iroquoian) and Blackfoot (Algonquian)  – showed that entire syllables can be produced with no sound at all (see Gick et al. 2012). This and other studies will be described in more detail below.

2.2.2 Articulatory phonetics and Indigenous language ­documentation and revitalization Articulatory phonetic research can be expensive, laborious, and in some cases invasive for speakers. Why should we do this work then? From a theoretical standpoint, documenting the articulatory mechanisms used in languages across the world expands our understanding of the range of possibilities for human speech. Indigenous languages in North America provide numerous examples of the articulators in action through their use of ejectives, pharyngeals, soundless articulations, and so much more (McDonough and Whalen 2008; Gordon 2017). These languages are therefore immensely valuable in terms of what they can teach us about the human capacity for speech. From a documentation standpoint, articulatory recordings (e.  g., video, ultrasound) provide an extra layer of detail beyond what is available in audio recordings, just as audio recordings provide an extra layer of detail beyond written records. From a language revitalization standpoint, articulatory phonetic research can also provide valuable tools for supporting teaching and learning pronunciation (see also Bird, Nolan, and Claxton, this volume). In his discussion of phonetic documentation, Maddison (2002: 420) states:

42 

 Heather Bliss, Sonya Bird and Bryan Gick

An ideal to have in mind is to provide enough phonetic information that a reader would be able to sound like a native speaker. A grammar writer might imagine he or she is describing the speech of the last generation of fluent speakers and aims to do it well enough that their grandchildren will be able to learn to sound like them and react like them to hearing the language spoken.

Currently in North America, many language revitalization efforts are focused on creating new speakers (see, e.  g., McIvor and McCarty 2016). These speakers take very seriously their responsibility of pronouncing their language(s) in a way that honors their Elders (Bird and Kell 2017; Jenni et al. 2017), and documentation of the quality described by Maddison can provide valuable resources for them. Immersion-based approaches like Total Physical Response (TPR; Asher 1977) and Where Are Your Keys (WAYK; https:// whereareyourkeys.org) provide excellent opportunities for learners to hear and speak in a holistic way, but other approaches are necessary to tackle the details of pronunciation once learners are ready for them. One approach that is gaining increasing popularity, and that is directly based on articulatory phonetic research, is speech visualization, or “seeing speech,” which involves looking directly at what speakers are doing with their articulators (e.  g., their tongues in ultrasound imaging) as they speak (see Bliss, Abel, and Gick 2018; Bliss et al. 2018). This approach complements similar work in acoustic phonetics, described in Bird, Nolan, and Claxton (this volume). We know that speech processing involves multiple modalities (e.  g., Catford and Pisoni 1970; Navarra and Soto-Faraco 2007), and incorporating them into pedagogical techniques can be of great benefit to learners, especially when it comes to understanding articulations that are not easily accessible because they occur deep in the vocal tract, e.  g., “back of the mouth” sounds like uvular and pharyngeal consonants.

2.2.3 Articulatory phonetics as the basis for phonetic descriptions and charts Linguists generally describe speech sounds based on how they are articulated. Consonants are normally described in reference to three primary dimensions: voicing, place, and manner. In its most basic version, voicing refers to whether the vocal folds are vibrating (voiced sounds) or not (voiceless sounds). Voicing also covers other, more complex articulations in the larynx, like those involved in making ejective stops and glottalized resonants (see Bird, Nolan, and Claxton, this volume, Section 1.3.2.2), or voice qualities like creaky voice (vocal fry) or breathy voice (see Figure 8). Place refers to where in the vocal tract the sound is being made. Figure 1 provides a schematic view of the vocal tract, labeled with the key landmarks in speech articulation, from the lips at the front/ top to the larynx at the back/bottom. Manner refers to how constricted the sound is, and consequently how freely air can flow through the vocal tract during the sound, e.  g., stop consonants are sounds where the articulators are completely closed, stopping airflow altogether; resonant consonants are ones where the articulators remain relatively far apart, allowing air to flow freely through the vocal tract.



Articulatory phonetics 

 43

Fig. 1: Midline view of the human vocal tract showing some important anatomical landmarks for speech. (Source: Adapted from an image by Patrick J. Lynch, medical illustrator (CC BY 2.5 [http://creativecommons. org/licenses/by/2.5]), via Wikimedia Commons.)

As a shorthand for phonetic descriptions of sounds, linguists use the International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA). For example, the symbol [k] (also written in the Roman alphabet as ) is used for the sound we describe as a voiceless velar stop, e.  g., in the English word kayak [kajak], borrowed from the Inuit word qayak [qajaq]. It is produced without vocal fold vibration (voicing = voiceless), by bringing the tongue body up against the back of the hard palate (place = velar) completely, thus completely stopping the airflow for a short time (manner = stop). Note that in the Inuit word, the first and last consonants are uvular [q] sounds, pronounced further back in the mouth than English [k]. This kind of sound adaptation is very common, and it happens naturally when a language borrows a word from another language that has an unfamiliar sound in it (Andersson, Sayeed, and Vaux 2017). Figure 2 summarizes the most common consonants found in the world’s languages, organized in an IPA chart. The columns correspond to places of articulation, from the front of the vocal tract on the left to the back of the vocal tract on the right. The rows correspond to manners of articulation, from the most constricted at the top to the least constricted at the bottom. Within each cell, the sound on the left is voiceless and the one on the right is voiced.

44 

 Heather Bliss, Sonya Bird and Bryan Gick

THE INTERNATIONAL PHONETIC ALPHABET (revised to 2015) CONSONANTS (PULMONIC)

© 2015 IPA

Bilabial Labiodental

Dental

Alveolar Postalveolar Retroflex

Palatal

Velar

Uvular

Pharyngeal

Glottal

Plosive Nasal Trill Tap or Flap Fricative Lateral fricative

Approximant Lateral approximant

Symbols to the right in a cell are voiced, to the left are voiceless. Shaded areas denote articulations judged impossible.

CONSONANTS (NON-PULMONIC)

VOWELS

Fig. 2: Basic (pulmonic) consonant chart (Source: www.internationalphoneticassociation.org/content/ipaBack Front Central Clicks Voiced implosives Ejectives chart, available under a Creative Commons Attribution-Sharealike 3.0 Unported License. Source: Copyright Close Bilabial Association)Examples: 2018Bilabial International Phonetic Dental

Dental/alveolar

Bilabial

(Post)alveolar

Palatal

Dental/alveolar

Close-mid

Palatoalveolar Velar Velar Vowels are also described articulatorily, usually in terms of tongue height and backOpen-mid Alveolar lateral or high Uvular Alveolar fricative 1 Close ness. vowels are ones with the tongue high up close to the palate; open or low vowels are ones with the tongue low against the floor of the mouth. Front vowels OTHER SYMBOLS Open have the tongue relativelyAlveolo-palatal far forward in the mouth; back vowels have the tongue Voiceless labial-velar fricative fricatives Where symbols appear in pairs, the one to the right represents a rounded vowel. or relatively far back. Figure 3 provides the vowels in a space that corresponds more Voiced labial-velar approximant Voiced alveolar lateral flap lessVoiced to these tongue-based distinctions. The vowel /i/, for example, is a close (or high) SUPRASEGMENTALS labial-palatal approximant Simultaneous and front vowel. A third dimension used for categorizing vowels is rounding, which refers Primary stress Voiceless epiglottal fricative Affricates and double articulations stress vowel; it to whether orfricative not the lips are rounded. For example, /y/ is also a Secondary close front Voiced epiglottal can be represented by two symbols joined by a tie bar if necessary. Long can also differ in differs from Epiglottal plosive /i/ in that it is rounded whereas /i/ is unrounded. Vowels Half-long terms of whether or not air can get out through the nose; in nazalized vowels, the velum DIACRITICS Some diacritics may be placed above a symbol with a descender, e.g. Extra-short is lowered, so that air escapes through the nose as well as through the mouth. Nasalized Voiceless Breathy voiced Dental Minor (foot) group vowels can be found inCreaky Dene languages, as Apical well as in languages like French. Voiced voiced Major (intonation) group The articulatory categories introduced here form the basis of phonological categoAspirated Linguolabial Laminal Syllable break ries,More discussed in Fitzgerald (this volume). Nasalized In our work with community members, we rounded Labialized Linking (absence of a break) have found that introducing the articulatory basis of sounds can help them grasp not Less rounded Palatalized Nasal release TONES AND WORD ACCENTS only how to pronounceVelarized these sounds, but also why they pattern the way they do in the Advanced Lateral release LEVEL CONTOUR Extra language. To this end, it can be useful to create schemas of the sound inventories that or or Rising Retracted Pharyngealized No audible release high areCentralized tailored to specificVelarized languages. Figure 4 provides an example from Hul’q’umi’num’, High Falling or pharyngealized High Mid Island and a Central Salish language spoken on the east side of Southern Vancouver on rising Mid-centralized Raised ( = voiced alveolar fricative) Low Low rising theSyllabic adjacent islands. Hul’q’umi’num’ 34bilabial consonants, 21 of which are not found in Lowered ( has = voiced approximant) Extra Risingfalling low English. Being able to Advanced summarize Non-syllabic Tongue Rootthem using visuals like this is particularly helpful, Downstep Global rise given all the possibilities. Rhoticity Retracted Tongue Root Upstep Global fall Typefaces: Doulos SIL (metatext); Doulos SIL, IPA Kiel, IPA LS Uni (symbols)

1 Note that describing vowels only in terms of what the tongue is doing (linguo-centric model) is a simplification of speech; in fact, jaw height and pharynx positioning also play important roles in vowel production and speech more generally (Esling 2005).



Articulatory phonetics 

 45

Fig. 3: Vowel chart within the vocal tract. (Source: Vocal tract image adapted from https://commons. wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Midsagittal_diagram_ unlabeled.svg. Public domain. Vowel chart image from www.internationalphoneticassociation.org/ content/ipa-chart, available under a Creative Commons Attribution-Sharealike 3.0 Unported License. Source: Copyright 2018 International Phonetic Association.)

Fig. 4: Hul’q’umi’num’ (Coast Salish) consonant chart showing anatomical landmarks corresponding to places of articulation. Red arrows indicate the points of contact (most often between tongue and palate) for different sounds. Consonants are written in the Hul’q’umi’num’ alphabet. Vocal tract image source as in Figure 3.

46 

 Heather Bliss, Sonya Bird and Bryan Gick

2.3 Tools for articulatory phonetics and their ­applicability in Indigenous language research and revitalization There is not enough articulatory data from North American languages to say anything broadly typological (but see Gordon 2017 for some generalizations based on articulatory and acoustic data). Rather than survey languages and/or classes of sounds, in this section, we survey tools and methods, with a focus on why they are useful and how they can be used effectively and ethically. For more general resources describing articulatory phonetic methodologies, we refer interested readers to Gick, Wilson, and Derrick (2013), Podesva and Zsiga (2013), and Ladefoged (2003).

2.3.1 Video Video is increasingly used as a basic recording method in language documentation to capture extralinguistic data such as gestures and facial expressions, as well as metadata variables surrounding the recording situation such as time of day, recording location, other people present, etc. (Ladefoged 2003; Margetts and Margetts 2012). However, video is easily overlooked as a tool for articulatory phonetic documentation, research, and pedagogy, despite being the most affordable and accessible tool described in this paper. Video can be used for measuring lip movements and lip aperture, which can inform investigations of vowel height differences (Gick et al. 2012; Bliss and Gick 2017), as well as distinctions between labial consonants and degrees of lip rounding (see Gordon, Martin, and Langley 2015 on Koasati). In addition to its utility as a research tool, video is useful as a pedagogical tool as it can be used to make data and images from ultrasound (and possibly other tools) more interpretable for untrained learners (see Bliss et al. 2018). Pedagogical applications are discussed in more detail in section 2.3.2 below. Here we focus on video as a research tool and briefly describe methods that have been used to measure lip aperture in two Indigenous languages in North America. Gick et al. (2012) report on two studies that measured lip aperture in Oneida and Blackfoot as a way to investigate “soundless” vowels  – sounds that are produced by moving the lips and tongue but that do not make any audible sound, and so cannot be measured acoustically. For example, the correct way to say the Oneida word akohta’ (meaning ‘her shoes’) is to pronounce the first two syllables [agoht] and then to mouth the final vowel [a] while making no sound at all. If you were just listening (or, say, talking on the phone) to a speaker of Oneida, without looking at the speaker’s face to see the mouth opening for the final [a], you might well think the word ended with a final [t]. The usual way to write these “soundless” vowels (and sometimes consonants) in Oneida is with an underline, so that the word for ‘her shoes’ would be written akohta. One speaker



Articulatory phonetics 

 47

of Oneida (according to Gick et al. 2012) described these sequences as: “you say it, but you don’t say it.” In the Oneida and Blackfoot studies, video was a valuable tool for capturing “soundless” sequences. In both studies, a camcorder2 mounted on a tripod was focused on a speaker’s mouth as they produced words or phrases. Video editing software was later used to extract frames from the video (e.  g., at the time corresponding to the midpoint of the vowel), and the lip opening was measured, either according to area (Oneida) or according to the vertical distance between reference points in the upper and lower lips (Blackfoot). Bliss and Gick (2017) used a similar method for investigating speaker variation in vowel articulations in Blackfoot, but they measure vertical and horizontal distances as a means to determine lip aperture (see Figure 5 below). The choice between these different measures depends on camera angle. In all three studies, pixels were converted to millimeters using known quantities in the frame; Maddieson (2002) suggests having a ruler in the frame for such purposes.

Fig. 5: Horizontal and vertical lines measuring lip aperture in Blackfoot

2 Margetts and Margetts (2012) note that even some professional-grade video cameras do not have professional audio inputs, and that most or all consumer cameras do not either. They advise using a mid-range “prosumer” camera with a secondary audio recording device to capture audio and visual streams in basic language documentation. For the purposes of measuring lip movements, a lower range consumer camera will likely suffice. As Smartphones and other readily accessible video-recording technologies become available, we expect the opportunities for using video in articulatory phonetic research will continue to grow.

48 

 Heather Bliss, Sonya Bird and Bryan Gick

2.3.2 Ultrasound There is a long history of ultrasound being used as a speech research tool, but only in the past 15–20 years has it been used in research and documentation of Indigenous North American languages. Historically, ultrasound was expensive and confined to a laboratory, but it has become increasingly portable and affordable over the past two decades (Gick 2002); there are now a number of handheld devices on the market that can connect to a laptop computer or smartphone. These developments have led to questions about how ultrasound could be used in the community-based settings effectively and ethically (see Gick, Bird, and Wilson 2005), and have opened the door for ultrasound to be used outside the laboratory and in Indigenous communities for articulatory phonetic documentation and research. With ultrasound, a transducer is held underneath a speaker’s chin, and a high-frequency sound is transmitted through the tongue and reflected back to the transducer, creating a 2-dimensional image of the tongue. Figure 6 provides two still images extracted from an ultrasound video, comparing the tongue position of the velar stop [k] (left) and the uvular stop [q] (right) (see Figures 2 and 4 above). The white line corresponds to the surface of the tongue, with the tongue tip at the right. The [k]~[q] contrast can be difficult to hear and to pinpoint acoustically (see Bird, Nolan, and Claxton, this volume, Section 1.3.2.2), but it is easy to see with ultrasound; the tongue body comes straight up for [k] but up and back (towards the uvula) for [q]. This contrast is very common in the languages of the Pacific Northwest (e.  g., as in the Halq’eméylem (Coast Salish) words kálti [kalti] ‘candy’ and qá:l [qa:l] ‘to steal’), and we have found that “seeing” the articulation of the two sounds using ultrasound is beneficial for learners (Bird & Miyashita 2019).

Fig. 6: Ultrasound tongue contours of [k] (left) vs. [q] (right). Tongue tip is on the right. Red dots indicate where the tongue is highest in the mouth.

Ultrasound is particularly useful for imaging sounds that are differentiated along the mid-sagittal plane (side view of the head, as in Figure 1): vowel articulations, the place of articulation of coronal and dorsal consonants, and consonants that require coordination of different parts of the tongue such as lateral (“l-like”) and rhotic (“r-like”) sounds. For all of these sounds, it is easy to see the tongue contour and to get a good sense of what is hap-



Articulatory phonetics 

 49

pening where in the mouth. It is also possible to turn the transducer 90 degrees and image the tongue in the cross-sectional plane (front view of the head). In this view, it is possible to see what the sides and centre of the tongue are doing, which is especially relevant for differentiating lateral sounds from coronal sounds, as in Hul’q’umi’num’ (Coast Salish) [s] (e.  g., in situn [sitǝn] ‘basket’) from [ɬ] (e.  g., in lheel [ɬeːl] ‘to go ashore’) (see Figure 2). Ultrasound is non-invasive, and speakers who use ultrasound often report that they enjoy the process of seeing their tongues “in action” (see, e.  g., Bliss et al. 2018 for discussion). The only drawback is that not all speakers image equally well. To enhance the ultrasound image, a thin layer of conductive gel is applied to the transducer. Because ultrasound imaging produces more or less clear images for different people, some speakers may also need to apply gentle pressure between the transducer and the neck, which may create mild discomfort for some speakers. To generate a clear image and minimize movements that can skew measurements, the speaker’s head and the transducer should ideally be stabilized, which can be achieved, for example, with a foam head rest affixed to a wall (see Gick et al. 2012) and an adjustable arm clamped to a chair to support the transducer (see Bliss et al. 2018). While there are numerous more invasive and more expensive stabilization tools and techniques that have been used by researchers, working in community settings – particularly with Elders – may require compromising on stabilization for the sake of the speakers’ comfort (see Percival, Bliss, and Schellenberg 2017). Moreover, as documented by Gick, Bird, and Wilson (2005), a simple stabilization set-up (such as that described above) can suffice to collect reliable data in the field. In Indigenous languages in North America, ultrasound has been used to document and describe a wide range of sounds and phenomena in a diverse range of languages. In combination with video (see 2.3.1), Gick et al. (2012) used ultrasound to study soundless vowels in Blackfoot and Oneida. As mentioned above, in these languages, vowels are articulated without any accompanying acoustic signal, meaning they can only be studied with articulatory methods. In combination with video recordings of speakers’ lips (see section 2.3.1), ultrasound recordings were used to demonstrate that different soundless vowels have distinct tongue shapes. Bliss and Gick (2017) expand on the 2012 study to include four additional speakers from two different Blackfoot dialects. Although the speakers exhibited certain differences in their tongue contours, all maintained a robust distinction between soundless /a/ and soundless /i/ vowels, as in the words ki’sómma [kiʔsumːa] ‘moon (proximate)’ and ki’sómmi [kiʔsumːi] ‘moon (obviative)’.3 Iskarous, McDonough, and Whalen (2012) also investigate variation – but amongst the productions of the velar fricative /x/ by a single speaker of Navajo (Athabaskan). They found that the tongue shape for this consonant varied considerably across tokens,

3 Proximate and obviative – the categories distinguished by soundless –a and –i in Blackfoot – are morphosyntactic categories observed in all Algonquian languages. See Chapter 59: Algonquian for details.

50 

 Heather Bliss, Sonya Bird and Bryan Gick

and was influenced by the following vowel (e.  g., /xi/ sequences were further front than /xa/ sequences). Ultrasound has also been used to investigate VC and CV sequences in Salish languages, particularly in cases where the consonant has a co-articulatory effect on a neighboring vowel (see also Bird, Nolan, and Claxton, this volume, Section  1.3.2.1 on vowel-consonant co-articulation). Interior Salish languages are well-attested for having vowel retraction: uvular and pharyngeal consonants produced at the far back of the mouth trigger the preceding vowel to be retracted, i.e., produced further back in the mouth as well. Retraction is also attested as a secondary feature with some other consonants. McDowell (2004) was the first to conduct an articulatory study of this retraction phenomenon, focusing on lateral (“l-like”) sounds in Montana Salish. She finds that all four laterals exhibit retraction and co-articulatory effects, but they differ according to whether the retraction involves the tongue dorsum (back of the tongue) or the tongue root (see Figure 1 above). Namdaran (2006) and Hudu (2008) investigate retraction in another Interior Salish language, St’át’imcets, documenting articulatory properties of both inherently and secondarily retracted consonants and their co-articulated vowels. Bird, Leonard, and Moisik (2010) and Bird (2012) also look at VC and CV sequences in Salish, but in a different branch of the language family, and from a different perspective. They investigate the articulatory mechanisms that speakers of SENĆOŦEN (Coast Salish) use to produce /iq/ and /qi/ sequences, as in the words SḴITEW [sqitǝw] ‘mermaid’ and ȾIḴT [tθ’iqt] ‘ivory-billed woodpecker.’4 These sequences involve an “articulatory conflict,” because uvular /q/ is produced far at the back of the mouth, and high vowel /i/ is produced at the front. Bird (2012) finds that one speaker uses a tongue rolling strategy that allows him to pronounce the sequences without compromising the vowel or the uvular closure. In general, articulatory studies tend to be more involved than acoustic studies, both in terms of data collection and data processing. For this reason, ultrasound studies are sometimes done as focused follow-ups to broader acoustic studies, to add a deeper understanding of a particular aspect of pronunciation that is difficult to characterize from acoustic analysis. In the case of Bird (2012), ultrasound imaging explained why, in some cases, the speaker appeared to be pronouncing /qi/ and /iq/ without making any adjustments to ease his pronunciation (see Bird, Nolan, and Claxton, this volume, Section 1.3.2.1). It turned out he was making adjustments, but they were not perceptible in auditory or acoustic analysis. This kind of discovery is important not only for documentation and description, but also for language pedagogy, as it can provide learners with tips on how to overcome articulatory conflicts in their language. Towards the view of contributing to pedagogical materials for language revitalization, Bliss et al. (2018) describe their collaborations with three Salish communities

4 The SENĆOŦEN alphabet was created by the late Dave Elliott Sr. It uses all capital letters in combination with 4 diacritics (https://wsanecschoolboard.ca/sencoten-language/).



Articulatory phonetics 

 51

in British Columbia to develop ultrasound-enhanced videos that can help learners of SENĆOŦEN, Halq’eméylem, and Secwepemc to pronounce challenging sounds, particularly “back-of-the-mouth” sounds. These videos combine a profile view of a speaker’s head with an ultrasound image of their tongue, effectively making the tongue visible and salient for the learner. A screenshot of one of the videos is given in Figure 7; see also https://enunciate.arts.ubc for more ultrasound video resources, including an interactive IPA chart. Percival, Bliss, and Schellenberg (2017) adopt Bliss et al.’s method to produce ultrasound-enhanced videos of Hän (Athabaskan) speakers. This project focused not only on developing pedagogical resources but also documenting the articulatory properties of the complex consonant inventory, which includes 5–6 coronal contrasts and numerous affricates. With a more direct focus on pedagogy, Bird and Miyashita (2019) describe using ultrasound and other phonetic tools with Blackfoot (Algonquian) and Hul’q’umi’num’ (Coast Salish) language learners as a pronunciation aid. In our experiences working with Indigenous community members using ultrasound imaging, we found that it can be a useful tool for developing awareness of what is going on inside the mouth during speech production; learners are enthusiastic to use it and they often reflect on how helpful it is to see inside the mouth. Whether or not it actually benefits their pronunciation (specifically of the target sounds and/or more generally) is yet unclear, but studies of other languages (such as Cantonese, see Bliss et al. 2017) suggest that it does.

Fig. 7: Ultrasound-enhanced video screenshot for SENĆOŦEN.

2.3.3 Endoscopy In linguistic research, endoscopy – and, more precisely, laryngoscopy – is used to image the (lower) pharynx and larynx, to study different voice qualities (Esling et al. 2019), as well as sounds that are pronounced deep within the throat, with engagement of the pharyngeal and laryngeal structures (Esling, Fraser, and Harris 2005). There are two types of

52 

 Heather Bliss, Sonya Bird and Bryan Gick

endoscopes: rigid and flexible. Rigid endoscopes have a camera and light at the end of a rigid tube (approximately the size of a pencil). They are inserted into the very back of the mouth, such that the camera looks down at the pharynx and larynx. Flexible (fibre optic) endoscopes have much thinner, more flexible tubes. They are inserted through the nose and fed through the nasal passage and down through the pharynx as far as just above the larynx (or anywhere above this). Fibre optic endoscopes are more versatile in terms of their placement, and they interfere much less with articulation (Esling, Fraser, and Harris 2005; Podesva and Zsiga 2013). They are therefore generally more useful as a tool for characterizing speech articulations. Figure 8 provides an example of fibreoptic laryngoscopy data, showing the laryngeal articulator during a period of breathy voicing. Further images and explanations of laryngeal structures, states of the larynx, and phonation types may be found in Esling et al. (2019); companion materials, including videos, are available for free through the Cambridge University Press website.5

Fig. 8: Example of fibreoptic laryngoscopy data. Image shows a slight tightening of the laryngeal articulator during a period of breathy voicing. (Courtesy of John Esling, Department of Linguistics, University of Victoria, Canada).

Up until now, linguistic endoscopy research has generally been done in a laboratory setting. However, a quick Google search (conducted in spring 2019) shows that handheld endoscopy systems are now on the market and relatively inexpensive, potentially allowing for research in communities as well. Nonetheless, compared to other imaging techniques like video and ultrasound, endoscopy is somewhat invasive and, at least at some universities, ethics boards require that flexible endoscopes be inserted by a

5 From the following link, click on Resources > Resources > Companion files https://www.cambridge. org/ca/academic/subjects/languages-linguistics/phonetics-and-phonology/voice-quality-laryngealarticulator-model?format=HB.



Articulatory phonetics 

 53

medical professional. In short, serious thought should be put into deciding whether to incorporate endoscopy into articulatory phonetic research. The only endoscopic studies of Indigenous languages in North America that we know of have focused on Nlaka’pamux (Interior Salish) and Nuu-chah-nulth (Wakashan). These two language families feature pharyngeal and glottal sounds, which are produced very deep in the throat (similar to some of the Arabic sounds, for example), and which would be very difficult to accurately describe without directly looking at the articulatory mechanisms involved (Carlson, Esling, and Harris 2004; Esling 2003; Esling, Fraser, and Harris 2005). For these languages, endoscopy has been an extremely valuable component of language documentation. For example, Esling, Fraser, and Harris (2005) examines glottal stops, pre-glottalized resonants, and pharyngeal consonants in the Ahousaht dialect of Nuu-chah-nulth (Wakashan). The study is based on the pronunciation of two speakers, co-author Katie Fraser and her sister Łuuta Qamiina. The paper itself includes several sequences of still images (extracted from video) showing the articulatory features of Nuu-chah-nulth’s pharyngeal, glottalized, and glottal sounds, including which structures are involved (the arytenoid cartilages, the vocal folds, the ventricular folds (false vocal folds), the epilaryngeal tube, and the glottis) and their degree of constriction across sounds. Esling, Fraser, and Harris’ findings have made major contributions to our understanding of how laryngeal and pharyngeal structures contribute to speech, and of the vast range of configurations they can adopt – and consequently the vast range of sounds they can generate.

2.3.4 Palatography Palatography involves tracking points of contact between the tongue and the roof of the mouth, either at a single point in time (static palatography) or over time (dynamic palatography) (Anderson 2008). Static palatography has been used for many decades to study speech sounds, e.  g., by Goddard (1907, 1912) in his work on Dene languages Hupa and Dene Sułiné. It involves painting a non-toxic, dark substance (often a mix of olive oil and powdered charcoal) on the surface of the tongue, and then using a mirror to observe where on the roof of the mouth the substance has left traces after articulating a specific sound (Anderson 2008; Ladefoged 2003).6 In our experience, the methodology is somewhat tricky: words have to be chosen carefully, so that only the sounds of interest create contact. The procedure is also fairly time-consuming since the tongue must be repainted after each utterance. One advantage of static palatography is that it is inex6 A related methodology is linguography, which involves painting the same substance on the roof of the mouth and inside surfaces of the upper teeth, instead of the tongue. Anderson (2008) notes that this method may be challenging, as the palate is less accessible to paint than the tongue (and may be ticklish for some speakers), but it may also be simpler than palatography as it does not require the use of a mirror to view the contact points.

54 

 Heather Bliss, Sonya Bird and Bryan Gick

pensive and portable. We have found that younger speakers and learners really engage in palatography sessions (in one session that co-author Bird was involved with, learners were streaming live on Facebook!). It is a good option for articulatory research done outside of laboratory settings and when the research budget is small. Figure 9 provides a close-up of a static palatography set-up, with the mirror showing the charcoal traces of tongue contact at the front and sides of the palate (for the English sound [θ], as produced by an 8-year old speaker).

Fig. 9: Static palatography image of charcoal traces left by the tongue on the palate during [θ] as pronounced by an 8-year old English speaker, seen through a mirror inserted into the mouth.

Another advantage of palatography is that it records contact points between the tongue and the hard structures (teeth and palate) above it, something that ultrasound, for example, is unable to do. In some cases, being able to image the points of contact on the tongue and on the palate simultaneously is very useful. For example, in Coast Salish languages, /θ/ (often spelled ) is articulated differently than in English, even though the sounds have generally been transcribed the same way in the IPA, and the contrast between /θ/ and /s/ is quite difficult to perceive for people unfamiliar with these sounds (see Mellesmoen 2018 on Comox-Sliammon). To teach learners to pronounce these sounds, it would be very handy to know what part of the tongue makes contact with what part of the upper teeth/palate, something that palatography (as opposed to ultrasound) can help with. Dart (1991) used static palatography to describe Tohono O’odham (Uto-Aztecan) sounds that involve raising the tongue front, as in the words a’ada [ˈaʔad̪a] ‘a kind of ritual’ and aḍawi [aˈdawi̥ ] ‘buffalo gourd.’ Her findings confirmed, for example, that the “dental” (front) /d̪/ and the “alveolar” (back) /d/ differed in the following ways: (a) which part of the tongue made contact with the roof of the mouth  – tongue tip and blade for /dental d̪/ and tongue tip only for /alveolar d̪/, (b) where contact was made – at the upper teeth for /d̪/ and behind the upper teeth for /d/, and (c) how large the area of contact was – greater (longer) for /dental d̪/ than for /alveolar d̪/. Avelino and Kim (2003) also use static palatography to investigate similar contrasts in Pima, a related language. The detailed descriptions generated by Dart’s (1991) and Avelino and Kim’s



Articulatory phonetics 

 55

(2003) research could be very useful for explaining to new speakers how to articulate these sounds, which are important to distinguish in Tohono O’odham, but correspond to a single /d/ sound in English and Spanish (the colonial languages spoken in Tohono O’odham territory). Dynamic palatography, or electropalatography (EPG), involves creating customized false palates for individual speakers that have electrodes embedded in them in a gridlike fashion (see Figure 10). These palates are inserted in the speakers’ mouths, and track contact over time within each cell of the grid (Ladefoged 2003; Gick, Wilson, and Derrick 2013). Having to create custom palates is often not feasible, for various reasons such as cost and time, and so EPG has not often been used in-community. No studies exist that have used EPG to study North American languages, but it has been used to investigate Aboriginal languages in Australia (Tabain, Fletcher, and Butcher 2011). Many Australian languages have extensive sets of sounds articulated with the front of the tongue against the roof of the mouth, which differ in precisely which part of the tongue makes contact, and where along the roof of the mouth. EPG has provided valuable information on the details of these sounds; again, this information can be used in pedagogical applications as well as to broaden our understanding of phonetic typology.

Fig. 10: A custom palate (left) and tongue-palate contact patterns (right) using electropalatography. The visualizations of contact patterns, from the Kay Palatometer Database CSL 4333, indicate (using small blue squares) where the tongue is touching the palate for a male English speaker during (middle image) and after (right image) the release of an alveolar stop consonant [t].

2.3.5 Aerodynamic methods Aerodynamic methods track the airflow and/or air pressure inside a speaker’s mouth during the production of speech sounds. This is particularly helpful for determining airstream mechanism, e.g, whether a given sound is ejective (produced with an explosive release of pressure created by a glottalic airstream) or not (pulmonic airstream). Maddieson (2002) notes that the term “glottalized” is used in many phonetic descriptions but is imprecise in that it has been used to refer to both ejective consonants as well as non-ejective consonants preceded or followed by constriction of the glottis. Understand-

56 

 Heather Bliss, Sonya Bird and Bryan Gick

ing what is meant by glottalization in phonetic descriptions can be investigated using aerodynamic methods that measure airflow and air pressure.7 Aerodynamic methods are not new; Goddard (1907, 1912) investigated air pressure in Hupa and Dene Sułiné using a now-archaic tool known as a kymograph. The kymograph (literally ‘wave drawing’) included flexible tubing with a rubber diaphragm on one end and a reed pen on the other, which would make tracings on a rotating drum in response to air pressure changes created by holding the diaphragm against the mouth (oral airflow), larynx (laryngeal activity), or up the nose (nasal airflow). Today, an oral or nasal mask with a plastic tube is still used, but instead of a kymograph recording pressure changes on a rotating drum, these same changes are picked up by a transducer and sent to a computer with specialized software that can digitize them. This kind of system for measuring air pressure/flow may be suitable for community-based research as it is highly portable, but it is costly (see http://www.sciconrd.com/price.aspx) and may require specialized training to interpret the results. Its operation is fairly simple; speakers produce words or phrases with the plastic tube between their lips, the opening of which rests lightly at the roof of the mouth. While not particularly invasive, some researchers report that it can be difficult to position the tube so it is not blocked by the tongue or obstructed with saliva. The nasal mask may be uncomfortable for some speakers, but it can be removed between recordings. In studies of North American languages, aerodynamic methods have been largely used to investigate the properties of consonants that have been described as ejectives (see also Bird, Nolan, and Claxton, this volume, Section 1.3.2.2). For instance, Maddieson, Smith, and Bessell (2001) measure the intra-oral pressure of a class of consonants described as ejective fricatives in words such as x’áax’ [x’aːx’] ‘crabapple’ in Tlingit (Dene) and compare it with that of non-ejective (pulmonic) fricatives in words such as xaak [xaːk] ‘empty seashell.’ They note that ejective fricatives are typologically rare and are not attested in any other North American language. Moreover, there is reason to question whether these sounds are truly ejective; Bessell (1996) notes that they are longer in duration than would be expected for a true ejective fricative, given that the volume of air would be relatively small for a sound produced with glottal closure and the release time would in turn be relatively short. However, the results of Maddieson, Smith, and Bessell’s (2001) study indicate that Tlingit does have ejective fricatives that are distinguishable from regular (non-ejective) fricatives by having a higher intra-oral pressure and a differ-

7 Maddieson (2002) also uses aerodynamic methods to determine that a consonant contrast previously described as “fortis” versus “lenis” in Leggbo (a Niger-Congo language of Africa) is in fact a distinction between singleton and geminate consonants. We know of no similar studies for North American languages, but we note that this methodology may be useful for enhancing phonetic descriptions of some North American languages.



Articulatory phonetics 

 57

ent pattern of release.8 Flemming, Ladefoged, and Thomason (2008) use similar methods to compare ejective and non-ejective stops and affricates in Montana Salish, and they also report higher pressure for the ejectives than non-ejectives. Finally, McDonough and Tucker (2012) use contemporary methods to replicate Goddard’s (1912) aerodynamic study of Dene Sułiné consonants, including the lateral affricate ejective [tɬ’] as in the word [tɬ’iːze] ‘fly.’ Their results are consistent with Goddard’s initial findings; all speakers consistently produced canonical ejectives, supporting the efficacy of Goddard’s kymographic methods and Dene Sułiné’s resistance to sound change, even after 100 years.

2.3.6 Other tools There are many other tools of modern articulatory phonetics that have not yet been brought to bear on studies of the Indigenous languages in North America. For the most part, this is because these require either highly invasive or highly involved laboratorybased procedures. The most common of these procedures involve either point-tracking or imaging techniques. Point-tracking techniques, such as magnetometry, x-ray microbeam, or optical tracking, involve attaching small sensors to different parts of the vocal tract (e.  g., tongue, lips, jaw or face) and tracking their movement over time. Depending on the type of tracking device, sensors can be tracked using either a magnetic field (as in magnetometry), x-ray (as in x-ray microbeam), or reflected light (as in optical tracking). These methods all involve expensive, highly specialized equipment that is normally only available at highend research centers, and none are portable. Some of the more intensive imaging techniques used in laboratory-based articulatory phonetics research include x-ray, computed tomography (CT), and structural magnetic resonance imaging (structural MRI). These techniques can produce very detailed images, and some (CT and MRI) can even image in 3 dimensions (“volumetrics”), but they have some serious drawbacks when it comes to investigating Indigenous languages. For example, both x-ray and CT present significant health concerns, while for CT and MRI, speakers need to sit or lie still for an extended period of time. All three methods are generally available only in a clinical hospital or research university setting, and all are expensive and require a team of professionals to operate.

8 An anonymous reviewer questions whether aerodynamic methods can distinguish between true ejectives versus consonant-glottal stop sequences. While Maddieson, Smith, and Bessel (2001) do not discuss this directly, they do note that Tlingit has both types of sounds and that the difference between them is in regards to the timing of the glottal release (see also Howe and Pulleyblank 2001 on Nuu-chah-nulth). Given that aerodynamic methods can track release times, we speculate that they could be used to distinguish between ejectives and consonant-glottal stop sequences.

58 

 Heather Bliss, Sonya Bird and Bryan Gick

2.4 Conclusions and future directions This chapter has surveyed articulatory phonetic tools and methods that can and have been used in the description, documentation, and revitalization of Indigenous languages in North America. While not a new field, articulatory phonetics is continually evolving, not only in terms of the research technologies employed but also in terms of researchers’ perspectives on community engagement. Regarding the former, as new innovations lead to improved tools and technologies, there is increased potential to conduct articulatory phonetic research in the field and with communities in ethical and effective ways (see Gick, Bird, and Wilson 2005). Regarding the latter, there is a growing awareness of the importance and necessity for community-based articulatory research (e.  g., Czaykowska-­Higgins 2009; Rice 2010; Fitzgerald 2017; Bird and Miyashita 2019). As technologies become increasingly portable, affordable and user-friendly, Indigenous communities can take the lead in conducting articulatory research on questions of direct relevance to their own linguistic needs and interests. While detailed phonetic documentation is a crucial ingredient in Indigenous language revitalization, so is the creation of new speakers, who can benefit from pedagogical applications of articulatory research to improve their pronunciation and carry their languages forward in an authentic way. As more Indigenous communities engage in articulatory phonetic research and pedagogy, the field will continue to shift and evolve. Acknowledgments: Nitsiko’tahsi’taki, HÍ,SW̱ḴE SI,IÁM, huy tseep q’a’ sii’em’, and thank you to our community and affiliated partners. Blackfoot: Ikino’motstaan Noreen Breaker, late Tootsinam Beatrice Bullshields, Tony Black Water, Natalie Creighton, and Piohkomiaaki Rachel Ermineskin. Halq’eméylem: Strang Burton and Siyamiyateliyot Elizabeth Phillips. Hul’q’umi’num’: Dr. Sti’tu’mat Ruby Peter, Delores Louie, Dr. Donna Gerdts, Hul’qumi’num’ Language & Culture Society, and students in the Hul’q’umi’num’ Language Academy. SENĆOŦEN: late Ivan Morris Sr. and late Ray Sam, Tye Swallow, and teachers and students in the W̱,SENĆOŦEN IST program. Thank you also to the editors and reviewers for their helpful feedback.

References Allen, W. Sidney. 1953. Phonetics in ancient India. (London Oriental Series 1). London: Oxford University Press. Anderson, Victoria B. 2008. Static palatography for language fieldwork. Language Documentation & Conservation 2(1). 1–27. Andersson, Samuel, Oliver Sayeed & Bert Vaux. 2017. The phonology of language contact. Oxford Handbooks Online. Asher, James J. 1977. Learning another language through actions: The complete teacher’s guidebook. Los Gatos, CA: Sky Oaks Productions.



Articulatory phonetics 

 59

Avelino, Heriberto & Sahyang Kim. 2003. Variability and constancy in the articulation and acoustics of Pima coronals. In Pawel Nowak, Corey Yoquelet & David Mortensen (eds.), Proceedings of the Twenty-ninth annual meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, 43–54. Ann Arbor: Sheridan Books. Bessell, Nicola J. 1996. Tlingit F’: Ejective or glottalized? Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Society for the Study of Indigenous Languages of Americas, San Diego, January 4–7. Bird, Sonya. 2011. Phonetic fieldwork in the Pacific Northwest. Proceedings of the International Conference of Phonetic Sciences XVII. 76–79. Bird, Sonya. 2012. Cool thing about ultrasound #17: Now I can pronounce /hiqət/! In Joel Dunham, John Lyon & Natalie Weber (eds.), Proceedings of the 47th International Conference on Salish and Neighbouring Languages (UBC Working Papers in Linguistics), 1–12. Vancouver, BC: University of British Columbia. Bird, Sonya & Sarah Kell. 2017. The role of pronunciation in SENĆOŦEN language revitalization. Canadian Modern Language Review 73(4). 538–569. Bird, Sonya, Janet Leonard & Scott Moisik. 2010. A motion vector analysis of tongue motion in SENĆOŦEN / qV/ and /Vq/ sequences. Paper presented at Ultrafest V, Haskins Laboratories, March 19–21. Bird, Sonya & Mizuki Miyashita. 2019. Teaching phonetics in the context of Indigenous language revitalization. Proceedings of the 2018 International Symposium on Applied Phonetics (ISAPh), 39–44. https://www.isca-speech.org/archive/ISAPh_2018/pdfs/07.pdf Bliss, Heather, Jennifer Abel & Bryan Gick. 2018. Computer-assisted visual articulation feedback in L2 pronunciation instruction: A review. Journal of Second Language Pronunciation 4(1). 129–153. Bliss, Heather, Sonya Bird, PEPAKIYE Ashley Cooper, Strang Burton & Bryan Gick. 2018. Seeing speech: Ultrasound-based multimedia resources for pronunciation learning in Indigenous languages. Language Documentation & Conservation 12. 315–338. Bliss, Heather, Lauretta Cheng, Murray Schellenberg, Zoe Lam, Raymond Pai & Bryan Gick. 2017. Ultrasound Technology and its Role in Cantonese Pronunciation Teaching and Learning. In Mary O’Brien & John Levis (eds.), Proceedings of the 8th Annual Conference on Pronunciation in Second Language Learning and Teaching (PSLLT), 33–46. Bliss, Heather & Bryan Gick. 2017. Blackfoot final vowels: What variation and its absence can tell us about communicative goals. In Betsy Sneller (ed.), Select proceedings of New Ways of Analyzing Variation (NWAV) 45. (Penn Working Papers in Linguistics 23[2]). Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania. https:// repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/vol23/iss2/6/ (accessed 09 August 2019). Carlson, Barry F., John H. Esling & Jimmy G. Harris. 2004. A laryngoscopic phonetic study of Nlaka’pamux (Thompson) Salish glottal stop, glottalized resonants, and pharyngeals. In Donna Gerdts & Lisa Matthewson (eds.), Studies in Salish Linguistics in Honor of M.D. Kinkade, 58–71. Missoula, MT: University of Montana. Catford, J. C. & David B. Pisoni. 1970. Auditory versus articulatory training in exotic sounds. The Modern Language Journal 54(7). 477–481. Czaykowska-Higgins, Ewa. 2009. Research models, community engagement, and linguistic fieldwork: Reflections on working within Canadian indigenous communities. Language Documentation & Conservation 3(1). 15–50. Dart, Sarah. 1991. Articulatory and acoustic properties of apical and laminal articulations. Los Angeles: University of California Los Angeles dissertation. Esling, John H. 2003. Glottal and epiglottal stop in Wakashan, Salish and Semitic. Proceedings of the 15th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences 2. 1707–1710. Esling, John. 2005. There Are No Back Vowels: The Laryngeal Articulator Model. Canadian Journal of Linguistics 50(1/2/3/4). 13–44. Esling, John H., Katie Fraser & Jimmy G. Harris. 2005. Glottal stop, glottalized resonants, and pharyngeals: A reinterpretation with evidence from a laryngoscopic study of Nuuchahnulth (Nootka). Journal of Phonetics 33(4). 383–410.

60 

 Heather Bliss, Sonya Bird and Bryan Gick

Esling, John, Scott Moisik, Allison Benner & Lise Crevier-Buchman. 2019. Voice quality: The laryngeal articulator model. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Fitzgerald, Colleen. 2017. The sounds of Indigenous language revitalization. Plenary address presented at the 91st annual meeting of the Linguistic Society of America, Austin, January 5–8. Flemming, Edward, Peter Ladefoged & Sarah Thomason. 2008. Phonetic structures of Montana Salish. Journal of Phonetics 36. 465–491. Gick, Bryan. 2002. The use of ultrasound for linguistic phonetic fieldwork. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 32(2). 113–122. Gick, Bryan, Sonya Bird & Ian Wilson. 2005. Techniques for field application of lingual ultrasound imaging. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 19(6–7). 503–514. Gick, Bryan, Heather Bliss, Karin Michelson & Bosko Radanov. 2012. Articulation without acoustics: “Soundless” vowels in Oneida and Blackfoot. Journal of Phonetics 40. 46–53. Gick, Bryan, Ian Wilson & Donald Derrick. 2013. Articulatory phonetics. Maldon, MA and Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell. Goddard, Pliny Earle. 1907. The phonology of the Hupa language. Part I: the individual sounds. Berkeley: University of California Press. Goddard, Pliny Earle. 1912. Chipewyan texts and analysis of the Cold Lake dialect, Chipewyan. (Anthropological Papers of the American Museum of Natural History 10). New York: Published by order of the Trustees of the American Museum of Natural History. Gordon, Matthew. 2017. Phonetic and phonological research on native American languages: Past, present, and future. International Journal of American Linguistics 83(1). 79–110. Gordon, Matthew, Jack Martin & Linda Langley. 2015. Some phonetic structures of Koasati. International Journal of American Linguistics 81(1). 83–118. Howe, Darin & Doug Pulleyblank. 2001. Patterns and timing of glottalisation. Phonology 18. 45–80. Hudu, Fusheini. 2008. The low vowel and retraction in St’at’imcets: An ultrasound investigation. SKY Journal of Linguistics 21. 67–81. Iskarous, Khalil, Joyce McDonough & D. H. Whalen. 2012. A gestural account of the velar fricative in Navajo. Laboratory Phonology 3(1). 195–210. Jenni, Barbara, Adar Anisman, Onowa McIvor & Peter Jacobs. 2017. An exploration of the effects of mentor-apprentice programs on mentors’ and apprentices’ wellbeing. International Journal of Indigenous Health 12(2). 25–42. Ladefoged, Peter. 2003. Phonetic data analysis: An introduction to fieldwork and instrumental techniques. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing. Maddieson, Ian. 1998. Collapsing vowel harmony and double-articulated fricatives: Two myths about Avatime phonology. In Ian Maddieson & Thomas J. Hinnebusch (eds.), Language history and linguistic description in Africa, 155–166. Trenton NJ: Africa World Press. Maddieson, Ian. 2002. Phonetics in the field. In Julie Larson & Mary Paster (eds.), Proceedings of the twentyeighth annual meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, 411–429. Ann Arbor: Sheridan Books. Maddieson, Ian, Caroline L. Smith & Nicola Bessell. 2001. Aspects of the phonetics of Tlingit. Anthropological Linguistics 43. 135–176. Margetts, Anna & Andrew Margetts. 2012. Audio and video recording techniques for linguistic research. In Nicholas Thieberger (ed.), The Oxford handbook of linguistic fieldwork, 13–53. Oxford: Oxford University Press. McDonough, Joyce M. & Benjamin Tucker. 2012. Replicating P. E. Goddard: A contemporary airflow and EGG study of Dene Sułiné. University of Rochester Working Papers in the Language Sciences 4(1). 45–56. McDonough, Joyce M. & D. H. Whalen. 2008. Editorial: The phonetics of native North American languages. Journal of Phonetics 36. 423–426. McDowell, Ramona. 2004. Retraction in Montana Salish lateral consonants: An ultrasonic study. Vancouver: University of British Columbia MA thesis.



Articulatory phonetics 

 61

McIvor, Onowa & Teresa L. McCarty. 2016. Indigenous bilingual and revitalization-immersion education in Canada and the USA. In Ofelia García & Angel Lin Cham (eds.), Encyclopedia of language and education, vol. 5: Bilingual and multilingual education, 1–17. Basel, Switzerland: Springer International. Mellesmoen, Gloria. 2018. An acoustic comparison of /θ/ and /s/ in Comox-Sliammon. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 144(3). 1939–1939. Namdaran, Nahal. 2006. Retraction in St’at’imcets: An ultrasonic investigation. Vancouver: University of British Columbia MA thesis. Navarra, Jordi & Salvador Soto-Faraco. 2007. Hearing lips in a second language: visual articulatory information enables the perception of second language sounds. Psychological Research 71. 4–12. Podesva, Robert J. & Elizabeth Zsiga. 2013. Sound recordings: Acoustic and articulatory data. In Robert J. Podesva & Devyani Sharma (eds.), Research methods in linguistics, 169–194. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Percival, Maida, Heather Bliss & Murray Schellenberg. 2017. Methodological trade-offs for dual-purpose phonetic fieldwork. Canadian Acoustics 45(3). 184–185. Rice, Keren. 2006. Let the language tell its story? The role of linguistic theory in writing grammars. In Felix K. Ameka, Alan Dench & Nicholas Evans (eds.), Catching language: The standing challenge of grammar writing, 235–268. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Rice, Keren. 2010. The linguist’s responsibilities to the community of speakers: Community-based research. In Lenore A. Grenoble & N. Louanna Furbee (eds.), Language documentation: Practice and values, 25–36. Philadelphia: John Benjamins. Tabain, Marija, Janet Fletcher & Andrew Butcher. 2011. An EPG study of palatal consonants in two Australian languages. Language & Speech 54(2). 265–282.

Hiroto Uchihara

3 Tone

Abstract: Tone is an integral part of some indigenous languages spoken in North America, even if it may not be a common feature. Tone can present challenges due to the difficulty of learning to hear tones, of establishing tonal contrasts, and the complexity of tonal phonology. However, tone can be an integral part of the language, serving to convey both lexical and grammatical functions and thus cannot be ignored. With adequate training and basic knowledge of tones, tone can be transcribed and represented in collaboration with the speakers. This chapter describes such a methodology and how tones can be represented in the orthography. In addition, this chapter surveys the functions of tones, tone processes, and interactions of tones with other parts of the grammar, as well as the diachrony of tones in the indigenous languages of North America.

3.1 Introduction Tone is an integral part of some indigenous languages spoken in North America,1 even if it may not be a common feature. Tone can present challenges for community members as well as for linguists working on documentation and revitalization, due to the difficulty of learning to hear tones, of establishing tonal contrasts, and the complexity of tonal phonology. Tone is a linguistic use of pitch; speakers of all languages use pitch to encode linguistic information, but its function can be fundamentally different depending on the language. In a tonal language pitch can distinguish different words or different forms of the same word, while in English or French speakers use pitch for intonation to convey pragmatic information. If a speaker of Oklahoma Cherokee says ga̋:du with a rising pitch on the first syllable it means ‘bread’, while if he says gadú with level low pitch on the first syllable it means ‘on top of’; in this way, pitch contributes to the difference in meaning. On the other hand, an English speaker can pronounce the word bread with any pitch, without changing its meaning, but they can differ in the pragmatics; if a speaker pronounces the word bread with a falling pitch contour, he may be answering a question, ‘Do you want bread or rice?’, while if a speaker pronounces bread with a rising pitch contour, a speaker may be asking a question. More specifically, the definition of a canonical tonal language is where each syllable is specified for a tone (Hyman 2006); that is, speakers need to know the tone of each syllable. Typical examples of tonal languages are Mandarin and Vietnamese. Among North 1 In this chapter, as in other chapters of the book, “North America” refers to the regions that currently fall within the territories of the United States and Canada; Mexico is excluded from the discussion, despite its geographical and linguistic contiguity. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-003

64 

 Hiroto Uchihara

American languages, many Dene/Athabaskan languages qualify as true tonal languages in this sense, since each syllable is specified for a tone. However, there are also less canonical tonal languages in North America. First, in some languages tonal contrasts are more restricted, such as only on accented (or stressed) syllables: this is the case in some Northern Iroquoian languages (Chafe 1977; Michelson 1988), Takelma (Sapir 1912), and some Uto-Aztecan languages such as Hopi (Jeanne 1982) and Yaqui (Demers et al. 1999). Secondly, there are so-called pitch-accent languages (Hyman 2009; Gordon 2014), which employ pitch to encode lexical and grammatical information as in tonal languages, but where each syllable is not specified for a tone but rather one syllable is specified for accent and the pitch on the other syllables is predictable (a typical pitch-accent language is Tokyo Japanese). This is the case with some Algonquian languages such as Arapaho and Cheyenne, Siouan languages such as Crow and Dhegiha, Muskogean languages and possibly Kiowa-Tanoan languages. Tonal languages in North America include the following: Kiowa-Tanoan languages (Sutton, this volume), Lingít/Tlingit and some Dene/Athabaskan languages (Saxon, this volume), some Algonquian such as Montagnais and Kickapoo (Oxford, this volume), Cherokee (Iroquoian; Mithun and DeCaire, this volume), Halq̓eméylem/Upriver Halkomelem (Salishan; Watanabe, this volume), Heiltsuk (Wakashan; Rosenblum and Rosborough, this volume), Haida, and other more marginally tonal languages in the senses described above. Tonal inventories of these languages are typically simple: they mostly contrast two pitch levels, low and high (and possibly the combinations of these two tones, rising and falling). Some languages are reported to have three pitch levels, low, mid and high and their combinations, including Tsuut’ina/Sarcee (Dene/Athabaskan; Sapir 1925; Cook 1978; McDonough et al. 2013) and Haida (Hori 1996). Other languages may have more complex inventories, such as Oklahoma Cherokee (Uchihara 2016). The organization of the rest of this chapter is as follows. First, I will discuss methodologies for establishing and identifying tonal contrasts, which will be crucial in documentation projects (§ 3.2). I will further discuss issues in the representations of tones, both orthographic and linguistic in § 3.3. As was already mentioned, tone can serve to distinguish different words or different forms of the same word. § 3.4 discusses such functions of tones. Tone can further be complicated by various tonal processes (§ 3.5), and interactions with non-tonal phonology and morphology (§ 3.6). Many tonal languages in North America developed tones recently. § 3.7 discusses how tones can develop from various sources.

3.2 Methodologies for establishing and identifying tones For most of the languages spoken in North America basic descriptions of tones already exist. However, sometimes we would like to check validity of the previous studies, or we

Tone 

 65

might suspect that a new system with new tonal patterns is emerging, and thus we feel the necessity for establishing tonal contrasts. In addition, even when a basic description of tones exists, often we do not always know what tone each word has. In this section, we will explore various methodologies for establishing tonal contrasts and identifying tones of each word. There is a fallacy that speakers easily ‘hear’ tones, and otherwise only those with absolute pitch can hear tones. This is not the case; it is very rarely the case that speakers can hear tones without any training (even in languages with fairly simple two-level pitch accent languages such as Tokyo Japanese). On the other hand, most people, whether speaker or non-speaker, or linguist or non-linguist, can learn to hear tones employing certain methodologies (Snider 2018: 34), as we will see in this section. Another misconception is that one can establish tonal contrasts and identify tones of each word by using acoustic tools such as Praat (to be discussed later). Tonal contrasts are categorical representations in speakers’ minds, which acoustic tools cannot access (Cruz & Woodbury 2014: 515; Snider 2018: 34). Linguists have been wary of overreliance on acoustic tools since they became available; for instance, Kenneth Pike remarks: instrumental records… do not contribute greatly to their analysis since (1) it is the relative pitch of tonemes which is significant, rather than their absolute pitch, and (2) tonemes change under various conditions, so that the intervals do not remain fixed in such a way that they may be mechanically discovered; instruments merely record gross fluctuation, rather than analyzing it in terms of deviations of units within a system. (Pike 1948: 21).

How, then, can one establish tonal contrasts and identify tones of each word? Pike (1948), Cruz & Woodbury (2014) and Snider (2018) detail methodologies for establishing tonal contrasts. The languages they are interested in and familiar with are Otomanguean languages spoken in Mexico and Bantu languages in Africa, and their typological tonal characteristics are distinct from those in North American languages. However, we can apply their methodologies to these languages as well. Their methodology is developed so that we can know how speakers categorize each tone pattern; acoustically, speakers physically produce various pitch patterns with different frequencies, but what we are interested here is how speakers categorize which pitch patterns as the ‘same’ and which pitch patterns as ‘different’. This is the same with the segments; both Spanish speakers and English (or Japanese) speakers physically produce [s] and [z], but Spanish speakers categorize these sounds to belong to the same category /s/ and produce [z] when an adjacent consonant is voiced, while English or Japanese speaker as two distinct categories, /s/ and /z/. By looking at acoustic realizations alone, we would not be able to determine how speakers categorizes these sounds. In (1) I summarize the steps that they propose to follow in establishing and identifying tones:

66 

(1)  

         



 Hiroto Uchihara

Steps for establishing tonal contrasts I. Find tonal minimal pairs, that is pairs of words which are segmentally identical, differentiated only by tones. This would confirm at least how many and which tone patterns are contrastive. II. Group words according to the phonological characteristics which can affect the perception of tones. This includes the following:   a. The number of syllables: monosyllabic (one syllable), disyllabic (two syllables), trisyllabic (three syllables), etc.   b. Vowel length: short or long   c. Glottalization: whether the vowel is followed by a glottal stop or not.   d. Syllable structure: open syllable (no final consonant in the coda, such as me) vs. closed syllable (syllable ends in a consonant, such as meat); if there is coda, resonant (consonants such as n, m, l, r) vs. obstruent coda (consonants such as p, b, t, d, k, g, etc.). III. Within the groups established in (II), classify each word according to the tone patterns. This can be achieved by comparing two words in sequence, asking speakers whether the two words have the same tonal patterns, or asking them to ‘hum’ or ‘whistle’ these words to see if they do have the same tonal patterns.

As an illustration, we can try to establish tonal patterns in Oklahoma Cherokee. First, consider Oklahoma Cherokee minimal pairs or near minimal pairs which contrast only by tones. Here, we are still not concerned with the specifics of the tones, especially their labels such as ‘high’ or ‘low’, or how they are written in the orthography, but rather we concentrate on how these words are (mostly) differentiated solely by tones; see § 3.3 on how to represent tones. (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)

a. a. a. a. a.

ama ‘water’ higa ‘Eat it!’ ǐ:ga ‘noon’ ga̋:du ‘bread o:hni ‘back’

b. b. b. b. b.

á:ma ‘salt’ híga ‘You just ate it’ i:ga ‘day’ gadú ‘on top of’ ò:hna ‘lawn’

Next, limiting ourselves to disyllabic words (words with two syllables) with a short vowel in the first syllable, we find two groups of words, most of which belong to the first group. Here we disregard whether the first syllable is glottalized (marked with the symbol ʔ) or not: (7)  

A ada ‘wood’, ama ‘water’, daksi ‘turtle’, joʔi ‘three’, kwana ‘peach’, sali ‘persimmon’, sihgwa ‘pig’, sohi ‘hickory nut’, svkta ‘apple’, tehga ‘toad’, tsgili ‘ghost’, wahya ‘wolf’, yansa ‘buffalo’

(8)  

B  áʔni ‘strawberry’, táʔli ‘two’

Tone 

 67

When we ask speakers to pronounce the words in A in a sequence, they should all have the same ‘melody’. On the other hand, if we mix words from class B, their melodies should sound somewhat different. Often speakers can recognize whether the words have the same ‘melody’ or not. Another thing to note here is that we are still not labeling each tonal pattern as terms like low, mid, high, falling, but rather abstract labels such as A or B. Sometimes, speakers and linguists are quick to label each tonal category with concrete tonal labels such as ‘low’, ‘high’, etc., but labels are not important at this point; what is important is whether the tonal melody of one word is the same as the tonal melody of the model word. In fact, Ken Pike and Thom Smith-Stark warn us of the danger of labelling: up to this point there has been no essential need for tonal transcription. It is the grouping as such which has been important… Serious efforts to make an early “correct” transcription only tend to confuse us. (Pike 1948: 53) Whenever anyone said “these ones are mid-high to low, these ones are high to mid-low,” Thom smiled and said, “Never mind about that; as long as the tone sounds the same in each pile!” (Cruz & Woodbury 2014: 501 – 502)

The following are words with a long vowel in the first syllable; here, we find five melodies: (9)  

A  a:dla ‘rubber’, do:sa ‘mosquito’, gi:hli ‘dog’, i:ga ‘day, i:ya ‘pumpkin’, ji:sdu ‘rabbit’, lo:lo ‘locust’, nv:da ‘moon, sun’, su:li ‘buzzard’, yv:gi ‘fork’

(10)  

B  á:ma ‘salt’, gv́:na ‘turkey’, jí:sgwa ‘bird’, ó:si ‘stove’

(11)  

C  kǎ:hwi ‘coffee’, nǔ:na ‘potato’, sě:lu ‘corn’, tǔ:ya ‘bean’, wě:sa ‘cat’, yǒ:na ‘bear

(12)  

D  dlà:yhga ‘bluejay’, nv̀:ya ‘rock’, sv̀:gi ‘onion’

(13)  

E  a̋:ta ‘young woman’, ga̋:du ‘bread’, gő:la ‘winter’, i̋:ga ‘noon’

When we encounter new disyllabic words, we can identify their tonal patterns by comparing their tones with the model words listed above. This methodology is easier to apply for languages where the shape of the root is constant, such as CV or CVCV, such as Otomanguean languages spoken in Mexico. On the other hand, tonal languages spoken in North America tend to be polysynthetic (that is, one ‘word’ can contain information that would be conveyed in a phrase or a sentence in English) and thus polysyllabic; the longer the word is, the harder it is to find other words which have exactly the same number of syllables and other phonological characteris-

68 

 Hiroto Uchihara

tics to compare with. However, unlike languages in Mesoamerica, Asia or Africa, tonal systems of North American languages generally involve only a few tonal patterns (§ 3.1); this makes it easier to apply the methodology for establishing tonal contrasts. After establishing tonal contrasts following the methodology described above, one can confirm the results with acoustic software such as Praat; such acoustic tools are especially useful for labelling each tonal pattern established employing the methodology above with concrete tonal labels such as low, high, high-falling, etc. Praat can be downloaded at http://www.fon.hum.uva.nl/praat/. If you open a recording with Praat and click on ‘view and edit’, you will see a screen like the following. This is word [a:˨˩giʔ˦gv:˧˦ʔi˥˩] ‘I ate it’ in Oklahoma Cherokee; we can see the low fall tone on the first syllable, a relatively high tone on the second, and the drastic pitch fall on the last syllable. Tone, or pitch, is drawn as a blue line.

Fig. 1: Pitch trace, spectrogram and spectrum of [a:˨˩giʔ˦gv:˧˦ʔi˥˩] ‘I ate it’ in Oklahoma Cherokee (Junior Scraper, speaker)

3.3 Representation of tones As we have seen, tone is a linguistic use of pitch. This section discusses how tones can be represented in orthography, if community members decide to represent them. In developing orthographic systems, speakers may decide whether or not they would like to represent tones. Note that not all the tonal languages with orthographic conventions represent tones in their orthography. For instance, various dialects of Japanese have pitch accent contrasts, but they are never represented in orthography. Representing tones in orthography can have advantages and disadvantages. If tone is contrastive, that is if tone distinguishes different words and forms of words (§ 3.4),

Tone 

 69

ambiguity can be avoided if all the tones are represented. Moreover, representing all the tonal contrasts will be helpful for second-language learners and linguists. On the other hand, having to represent tones in all syllables all the time may discourage some speakers from writing their language, since as we have seen above, identifying tones is not easy for all the speakers. See Bird (1998), Bernard et al. (2002) and Roberts et al. (2019), among others, on whether marking tone helps comprehension in tonal languages spoken in Africa. If we decide to represent tone in orthography, how do we represent it? Snider (2018: Ch.1) classifies various systems of tone representation into the following major systems: the Chao system (or the IPA system), which includes the numeric version of the Chao system; the Pike system; and the diacritic system.2 First, the Chao system is shown in (14) and illustrated in (15) with an example from Oklahoma Cherokee: (14)          

Chao system  ˥ Extra high ˦ High ˧ Mid ˨ Low ˩ Extra Low

(15)    

Oklahoma Cherokee   a˧dv:˧˩ne:˧˦li:˦˥sgi   ‘actor’ 

˩˥ rising ˥˩ falling ˦˥ high rising ˩˧ low rising ˧˥˩ rising-falling

The Chao system has a numeric version; here, the higher numbers correspond to higher pitch, illustrated in (17) with a Cherokee example: (16)          

Numeric Chao system 55 Extra high 15 rising 44 High 51 falling 33 Mid 45 high rising 22 Low 23 low rising 11 Extra Low 343 rising-falling

(17)    

Oklahoma Cherokee a3dv:31ne:34li:45sgi ‘actor’

The Pike system, commonly used among Mesoamericanists (mainly in the previous century) also employs numbers to represent tones, but the numbers are reversed, so that the higher numbers represent lower pitch; (18) is the same Cherokee word with this representation: 2 Snider (2018) further introduces the bar system, but here I omit the discussion of this system since this system is seldom employed for North American languages.

70 

(18)    

 Hiroto Uchihara

Cherokee a3dv:35ne:32li:21sgi ‘actor’

Lastly, the diacritic system employs accent diacritics above vowels, illustrated in (20): (19)          

Diacritic system a̋ Extra high ǎ rising á High â falling ā Mid   à Low   ȁ Extra Low  

(20)    

Oklahoma Cherokee ādv̀:ně:li̋ :sgi ‘actor’

Each system has its own advantages and disadvantages. The Chao (IPA) system and numeric systems might be precise, but they are harder to type and might be unintuitive for community members. On the other hand, the diacritic system might be easier to type but it can be difficult to represent all the tonal contrasts sometimes, such as various types of falling (high-falling, mid-falling) or rising tones (mid-rising, mid-falling). In this chapter, I employ the diacritic system to represent tones. This is first because it is conventional for the majority of the tonal languages spoken in North America, and second because North American languages rarely have complex tone inventories (with the possible exception of Oklahoma Cherokee), with usually two tone levels and contours combining them and thus accent diacritics can represent all of the tonal contrasts in these languages fairly easily. In general, I follow the conventions used in the original sources. Such sources usually use the diacritic system presented in (19), except that with systems with more than two levels (such as Tsuut’ina/Sarcee or Oklahoma Cherokee), the intermediate tone is left unmarked, rather than representing it with a macron (ā). Even within the diacritic system, there can be great variation. For instance, for Oklahoma Cherokee, at least seven orthographic systems have been proposed and employed in the linguistic literature; the first ‘Pulte & Feeling (1975) system’ corresponds to the numeric Chao system, while the remaining six systems are all diacritic systems, with differences in how to represent vowel length or which tones to represent or not (in some systems the low tone is left unmarked, since this tone is the least marked tone phonologically; cf. § 3.5.3).

Tone 

 71

Tab. 1: Comparison of Oklahoma Cherokee tonal orthography systems   V length

(i) Pulte & Feeling (1975)

(ii) Scancarelli (1987)

Munro (1996)

(iii) Feeling et al. (2003), Uchihara (2009)

MontgomeryAnderson (2008)

(iv) Community orthography

Modified community orthography

short

LF (short) L  H  SH (short)

ạ1 ạ2 ạ3 ạ4

ȁ à  á  a̋

– à  á  –

à  a  á  a̋

à  a  á  –

à  ā á   

à  a  á   

long

LF LL HH LH HL SH

a1 = a21 a2 a3 a23 a32 a4

ȁ: à: á: ǎ: â: a̋:

àa àà áá àá áà áa

àà/àa aa áá aá áa a̋a̋

aà aa áa aá áà áá

à: ā: á: á̄: á̄: a̋:

à: a: á: ǎ: â: a̋:

A word ‘actor’, for example, would be represented as follows according to each system. It might be important for community members and linguists to agree on how tones can be represented, to avoid future confusion. (21)              

‘actor’ in Oklahoma Cherokee a. Pulte & Feeling (1975) b. Scancarelli (1987) c. Munro (1996) d. Feeling et al. (2003) e. Montgomery-Anderson (2008) f. Community orthography g. Modified community orthography

ạ2dv1ne23li4sgi àtv̏:ně:li̋ :ski àdv̀vnèélíiski atv̀vn ̀ eéli̋ i̋ ski atvv̀neélííski ādv̀:nēˊ:li̋ :sgi adv̀:ně:li̋ :sgi

3.4 Functions of tones In a tonal language, tones can be used to distinguish lexical items (§ 3.4.1), or to encode grammatical information (§ 3.4.2).

3.4.1 Lexical contrast Tones can be used to distinguish different lexical items, as segments do. For instance, see the following minimal pairs and triplets which are distinguished solely by tones.

72 

 Hiroto Uchihara

First in (22) from Oklahoma Cherokee, the second syllable has a low tone in (a) and a high tone in (b), which contribute to the difference in meaning. Similarly, in (23) from K’ashógot’i̜ ne Xədə́/Hare (variety of Sahtú Dene/North Slavey, Dene/Athabaskan), the low tone in (a) and the high tone in (b) is the only difference between the two lexical items. Finally, in (24) from Kiowa (Kiowa-Tanoan), the high tone in (a), the low tone in (b), and the falling tone in (c) all contributes to the difference in meaning: (22)    

Cherokee ((Uchihara 2016: 3) a. gaʔdvsga b.   ‘I am growing’  

  (23)    

        K’ashógot’ine Xədə́ (Rice 2014: 692) a. sa b. sá   ‘bear’  ‘beaver’

(24)    

Kiowa (Miller 2018: 43) a. kˀɔ́:   ‘cold’

b.  

gaʔdv́sga ‘I am hanging it up’

kˀɔ ̀: ‘to lay there’

   

   





   

   

c.  

kˀɔ̂: ‘knife, cut’

3.4.2 Grammatical use of tones Tones can also be used to encode grammatical information, such as tense, aspect or person. In some languages, the person category can be encoded solely by the differences in tones. For instance, in Oklahoma Cherokee (25), for some verbs the only difference between the 1sg and the 3sg forms is the tone, namely the lowfall tone and the low tone in the first syllable. In some Dene/Athabaskan languages, such as Diné bizaad/Navajo (26) or Tsuut’ina/Sarcee (27), the 2sg and 3sg forms are differentiated by tones on the prefix ni-: (25)    

Cherokee (Feeling 1975) a. gò:hlv:sga   ‘I make it’

(26)    

Dene bizaad/Navajo (Goosen 1995: 4) a. nílį́ b. nilį́   ‘you are’   ‘he is’

(27)    

Tsuut’ina/Sarcee (Cook 1978: 171) a. níɣá b. niɣá   ‘you will grow up’   ‘he will grow up’

b.  

go:hlv:sga ‘he makes it’

Tone can also encode the tense/aspect/mode categories in North American languages. For instance, the punctual and imperative forms in Oklahoma Cherokee are distin-

Tone 

 73

guished by a tone difference in the penultimate syllable (28); the non-iterative and iterative aspect in Oklahoma Cherokee are encoded by a tone difference in the first syllable, in addition to vowel length (29); finally, the imperfective and perfective aspects in Dene/ Athabaskan languages such as Tsuut’ina/Sarcee (30) and Diné bizaad/Navajo (31) can be differentiated by tones of the stem or the prefix: (28)    

Cherokee (Uchihara 2016: 3) a. ho:hwe:lv̂:ga (Uchihara 2016: 3)   ‘you just wrote it’

(29)    

Oklahoma Cherokee (Pulte & Feeling 1975: 254) a. ù:go:hé:ʔi (Feeling 1975: 252) b. úgo:hé:ʔi   ‘he reportedly saw it’   ‘He reportedly saw it again’

(30)    

Tsuut’ina/Sarcee (Cook 1978: 169) a. -kˀós   ‘to throw (imperfective)’

(31)    

Dine bizaad/Navajo (McDonough 1999: 509) a. yicha b.   ‘he is crying’  

b.  

b.  

ho:hwe:lv̀:ga ‘Write it!’

-kˀòs ‘to throw (perfective)’ yícha ‘he cried’

Finally, tone can mark nominal number in Montagnais Algonquian (Cowan 1983): in (32), the singular form has a high tone on the last syllable (a) while in plural it has a low tone (b). (32)    

Montagnais (Cowan 1983: 68) a. ustá:xw b. ustà:xw ‘ax’   ‘axes’

In summary, tone can serve to convey both lexical and grammatical information. Thus, in a way, ‘tone can do everything segments and non-tonal prosodies can do’ (Hyman 2011: 214).

3.5 Tonal processes and constraints In § 3.2, we took a look at how tone melodies of each word can be identified, and in § 3.3 we saw how tones can be represented in the orthography. However, when such words are placed in contexts or are inflected, their tones can undergo changes. For instance, in Diné bizaad/Navajo, “I am walking along” is yìšá:ɬ, with a high tone on the last syllable, but its negated form, “I am not walking along”, is dò: yìšâ:ɬ=dà (Leer 2001: 82); here, the syllable šâ:ɬ now has a high-low falling tone. This is due to a tonal process that changes the lexical tones, which we will take a look in § 3.5.1. Some such tonal processes can be motivated by cross-linguistically common tonal constraints, that is certain tonal

74 

 Hiroto Uchihara

sequences are not preferred in general (§  3.5.2). Lastly, some tonal processes can be better understood by employing the concept of tonal markedness (§ 3.5.3).

3.5.1 Tonal processes For North American languages, the most common tonal process is tone spreading, where a tone of one tone-bearing unit (such as syllable) spreads to another tone-bearing unit. The example above from Diné bizaad/Navajo illustrates spreading of a low tone, where the low tone of the enclitic =dà spreads to the preceding long vowel with a high tone, forming a falling tone (Leer 2001). Here, the first line represents how speakers pronounce, and the second line shows a more abstract level of analysis which shows how each word is pronounced in isolation. Thus, the syllable šâ:ɬ has a falling tone in the first line, as pronounced by speakers, while it has šá:ɬ, a high tone, in the second line, reflecting how it is pronounced without the following =dà. Between these two lines we see a line connecting the syllable dà in the first line and the symbol L which represents a low tone, as well as a dashed line which connects this L with the syllable šâ:ɬ. This means that the original low tone on dà spreads to the preceding syllable šá:ɬ, which originally has a high tone. Due to this low tone that spreads from the following syllable dà, the syllable šâ:ɬ now forms a high-low falling tone.3 (33)            

Diné bizaad/Navajo dò: yìšâ:ɬ=dà (Leer 2001: 82)      L dò: yìšá:ɬ=dà neg I.am.walking.along=neg ‘I am not walking (along)’

The conventions for representing tones as in (33) may look complex at first glance, but such a representation is employed in many linguistics works on tones; such a representation is called autosegmental representation (Goldsmith 1976). It can be useful for learners in understanding why the tones are not always as expected. (33) above shows that the low tone spreads leftward in Diné bizaad. A low tone in Diné bizaad can also spread rightward from any high tone verb prefix to a short prefix syllable that does not have a high tone (Leer 2001: 81). Thus, the low tone on xà= ‘up/ out’ in (34) spreads for two syllables. Again, the solid line represents where the tone is lexically associated to, and the broken lines indicate where they spread to: 3 In the examples, the first line shows the surface forms, while the second line (if any) shows the segmented form with lexical representations. The third line provides the gloss. The abbreviations used in this chapter are as follows: a: set A prefix (agentive-series); an: animate; cmp: compact; du: dual; ex: exclusive; foc: focus; ind: indicative; neg: negative; obj: object; pct: punctual; prs: present; sg: singular; subj: subject.

Tone 

(34)            

 75

Diné bizaad/Navajo (Leer 2001: 81) xàxònìštšà:d        L   xà=xo-ni-š-l-tšà:d   up/out=areal.obj.-thematic.prefix-1sg.subj.-thematic.prefix-fluff.up   ‘I fluff up the soil’

Such tone spreading processes are reported in other Dene/Athabaskan languages (Hoijer 1943; Cook 1989; Rice 1989; Leer 2001, Holton 2005; Rice & Hargus 2005), Oklahoma Cherokee (Lindsey 1985; Uchihara 2016: Ch.6), Kiowa (Watkins 1984; Harbour 2003; Miller 2018: 96  ff.), Heiltsuk (Wilson 1987) and Haida (Hori 1996). When tone shifts from one tone-bearing unit (such as syllable) to another, this results in tone displacement. For instance, in Dakelh/Carrier (Dene; Athabaskan), the negative enclitic =íloh has a high tone on its first syllable (indicated with H in the second line), as shown in (35a), but this high tone is shifted to the preceding stem with certain nouns (Story 1989: 105). Below, the unlinked line is indicated with =, meaning that this syllable no longer has a high tone. Tone displacement is also reported in Fort Nelson Slavey (Dene/Athabaskan; Rice 1989) and Oklahoma Cherokee (Uchihara 2016: Ch.6). (35)            

Dakelh/Carrier (Story 1989: 105) a. xoh=íloh                        H   xoh-íloh   goose-neg   ‘not a goose’

b.          

lhéz=iloh             =                        H lhez-íloh dust-neg ‘not dust’

In some languages, some morphemes are associated with a floating tone. A floating tone is not realized on the morpheme it is associated to but rather is realized on the adjacent morpheme or a word (which is commonly known as tone sandhi) when they are combined. For instance, in Oklahoma Cherokee, some stems are associated with a floating high tone which docks onto the preceding morpheme, most commonly a pronominal prefix. As can be seen in (36b), the pronominal prefix ci- 1sg.a is lexically low-toned; however, when followed by verb stems such as -H:tlo:- ‘strap’, which is associated with a floating high tone (indicated with a raised H), the pronominal prefix receives a high tone as in (36a).4

4 In the examples from Oklahoma Cherokee, the first line has letters such as d, j, g, while the second line has t, c or k corresponding to them. This is because the first line reflects the community-based orthography, while the second line reflects the conventions employed by linguists.

76 

(36)          

 Hiroto Uchihara

Oklahoma Cherokee (Feeling 1975: 92, 97) a. jí:dlo:híha b.       H   ci- :tlo:-híh-a     1sg.a-strap-prs-ind     ‘I am strapping it.’  

ji:jagalí:ʔa   ci-:cakal-íh-a 1sg.a-rip-prs-ind ‘I am ripping it.’

When grammatical information is expressed by tones (see § 3 4.2 above), this could be also due to a morpheme that consists only of a floating tone. This is the case with the iterative prefix in Cherokee that we saw in (29b), or the possessive suffix in Dënesu̜ ɬiné/ Chipewyan and Tłı̨ chǫ/Dogrib (Saxon & Wilhelm 2016). Finally, in some languages a boundary tone is assigned to the last tone bearing unit (such as syllable) of a (phonological) word, to mark the end of a word or a phrase. For instance, in Oklahoma Cherokee, the word-final vowels are not pronounced in general, but when one is pronounced, this syllable usually has a high tone (Lindsey 1985: 125, 168; Haag 2001: 414; Johnson 2005: 17), even if lexically this vowel does not have a high tone, as shown in (37). Here, the final syllable hli is pronounced with a high tone; that this high tone is only phonetic (and thus not represented in the orthography) is indicated by putting these forms in the square brackets []. That the lexical tone of this syllable is low is justified by the form in (37b), where the word ‘dog’ is followed by a focus enclitic =dv́: and thus the syllable hli is no longer final in the word. A word-final boundary tone is also found in some Dene/Athabaskan languages (Rice 1989). (37)        

Oklahoma Cherokee (DF 1972) a. [gi:ɬí]   gi:hli   dog   ‘dog’

b.      

[gi:ɬidʌ́ :] gi:hli=dv́: dog=foc ‘(it is the) dog’

3.5.2 Tonal constraints Some of the tonal processes discussed in §  3.5.1 are motivated by cross-linguistically common tonal constraints; that is, certain tonal sequences are not generally preferred. First, in general a sequence of the same tones in the adjacent syllables that are not due to spreading is avoided (the Obligatory Contour Principle; Leben 1973; Myers 1997). For instance, in Oklahoma Cherokee, when two morphemes with a high tone come next to each other, one of them has to be deleted (Uchihara 2016: 110). Thus, in (38), the forms in (a) and (b) share the verb root -stoóʔ- ‘crush’, with the lexical high tone as can be observed in (a), but when preceded by another high tone as in (b), this high tone is deleted. Here, the deletion of tone is indicated by = across the line which links H, a high tone, to the syllable sdo:

Tone 

(38)            

Oklahoma Cherokee (Feeling 1975: 48, 17) a. à:sdó:ʔa b.                 H     a-stoóʔ-a     3sg.a-crush:prs-ind     ‘He is crushing it.’  

 77

à:gî:sdoʔa               =           H          H a-kíi(ʔ)+stoóʔ-a 3sg.a-eat+crush:prs-ind ‘He is chewing it.’

Another cross-linguistic tendency is to avoid a tonal dip (*Trough; Yip 2002: 137; Hyman 2009: 229). For instance, again in Oklahoma Cherokee, a sequence of high-low-high tone is avoided (Uchihara 2016: 123).

3.5.3 Tonal markedness In a tonal language, not all the tones have an equal status. Some tones are more common than others; some tones may trigger tonal processes more than others. For instance, in Oklahoma Cherokee, the low tone is the most frequent tone in the language and never affects tones of other syllables. Reflecting this fact, in some orthographic representation of tones in Oklahoma Cherokee, the low tone is not marked with any diacritic, as we saw in § 3.3. In such a case, we say that the low tone in Oklahoma Cherokee is unmarked, underspecified or that syllables that carry a low tone are toneless (Hyman 2001). Unmarked tones are those which are phonologically not active (for instance, they do not spread to other syllables), and/or occur more frequently than other tones (Maddieson 1976: 30; Hyman 2001). The status of markedness can change over time or vary between varieties: Leer (2001) presents evidence that Lingít/Tlingit and Southern Dene/Athabaskan languages were originally low-marked, but the marked tone has shifted to the high tone (Leer 2001). Tones which are superficially identical can differ at a more abstract level. Such is the case of abstract tonal contrasts. Such analysis has been proposed for Dakelh/Carrier (Story 1989: 104  ff.), Neeʼaandegʼ/Tanacross (Holton 2005: 260  ff.) and Oklahoma Cherokee (Cornelius 2018). For instance, in Oklahoma Cherokee, a high tone spreads to the preceding mora (roughly speaking, a short vowel has one mora and a long vowel has two moras) if complex phonological and morphological conditions are met (cf. § 3.5.1) as in (39a), but in some cases this high tone fails to spread to the preceding mora even though the conditions are met (39b). Cornelius (2018) attributes this difference in behavior to the abstract contrast between the underlying low tone and the underlying underspecified tone, both of which are realized as a low tone on surface. That is, in (39a), the syllable hwee is underspecified (or carries no tone) and thus no letter below hwee in the second line, while in (39b) the syllable nvv carries a low tone, represented with the letter L in the second line.5 5 The asterisk (*) means that the forms are not pronounced this way.

78 

 Hiroto Uchihara

(39)  

Oklahoma Cherokee (Feeling 1975) a. goohweélíʔa6

b.

nvvwóoti (*nvv́wóoti)

   

   

   

         L       H ‘medicine’

                    H ‘he is writing’

3.6 Interactions with other parts of the grammar Tone is not independent from the rest of the phonology and grammar, and it can i­ nteract with other parts of the grammar: phonology, such as syllable structure or accent (§ 3.6.1); morphology, or the internal structure of words (§ 3.6.2).

3.6.1 Interactions with phonology Tones can interact with syllable types. For instance, contour tones (rising and falling tones) can only occur on long vowels, as in Cherokee (Lindsey 1985) or Tsuut’ina/Sarcee (Cook 1978). Tones can also interact with the accent (or stress). Thus, in some languages tone is contrastive only on accented syllables, as in Northern Iroquoian (Chafe 1977; Michelson 1988) and Yaqui (Uto-Aztecan; Demers et al. 1999). In the following examples from Kanienʼkéha/Mohawk, a Northern Iroquoian language, the accent is assigned to the penultimate syllable, where the high tone in (40) contrasts with a falling tone in (41): (40)    

Kanien’kéha/Mohawk (Michelson 1988: 54) í:weʔs ‘she, it is walking around’

(41)    

Kanien’kéha/Mohawk (Michelson 1988: 54) ì:reʔs ‘he goes’

Tone can also attract accent or vice versa. Thus, in the K’ashógot’i̜ ne Xədə́/Hare variety of Sahtú Dene/North Slavey, a high tone is attracted to the accented syllable (Rice 1990); on the other hand, in Neeʼaandegʼ/Tanacross (Holton 2000), and Tse’khene/Sekani (Hargus 2005) high tone attracts accent. The interaction of tone and accent is also reported for Crow (Siouan; Kaschube 1954) and Heiltsuk (Kortlandt 1975).

6 In these examples the long vowels are represented with doubling the vowels instead of a colon, so that the tonal process can easily be seen.

Tone 

 79

3.6.2 Interactions with morphology Some tonal processes discussed in §  3.5 can be morphologically conditioned; that is, the application of such processes can be limited to certain units (or domains), such as morphemes, words, or phrases. For instance, high tone spreading is restricted to the stem domain in Oklahoma Cherokee, which excludes pronominal prefixes (Uchihara 2016: Ch.7). In (42a), the high tone associated with the syllable gí spreads leftward by one mora due to the high tone spreading discussed in § 3.5.1 above, forming a lowhigh rising tone. On the other hand, in (42b) the high tone on the syllable gí cannot spread to the preceding syllable because this syllable belongs to the pronominal prefix oostii- 1du.ex.a, which is outside of the domain of high tone spreading in Oklahoma Cherokee. The domain boundary is indicated by a square bracket ([). (42)              

a.

Oklahoma Cherokee (DF, July 2013) Spreading à:sdǐ:gíʔa                H aa-[stiik-íʔ-a 3sg.a-eat.lg-prs-ind ‘He is eating it (something long).’

b.

OK Cherokee (Feeling 1975:47) No spreading ò:sdi:gíʔa                 H oostii-[k-íʔ-a 1du.ex.a-eat-prs-ind ‘He and I are eating it.’

Similarly, in Kiowa, the domain of high tone spreading excludes pronominal prefixes (Harbour 2003: 548; Miller 2018). Thus, the high tone of the last syllable of the incorporated kíísɔ́ ‘afternoon’ spreads to the first syllable of the verb stem dęįkˀɔ́ɔ́ ‘lie asleep’ in (43a), while the high tone of the pronominal prefix á- fails to spread (43b): (43)              

Kiowa (Harbour 2003: 548) a. Spreading   kíísɔ́dę́įḱ ˀɔ́ɔ́                    H       [kíísɔ́+dęįkˀɔ́ɔ́       afternoon+lie.asleep       ‘?’

b.

No Spreading ádęįkˀɔ́ɔ́ (*ádę́įḱ ˀɔ́ɔ́)   H á-[dęįkˀɔ́ɔ́ they.an-lie.asleep ‘They lie asleep’

Tone spreading in Dene/Athabaskan languages are also sensitive to the morphological structures (Story 1989; Rice 1989). For instance, in Fort Nelson Slavey, high tone spreading is only applied within the word and thus applies in compounds which form one word (44a), but it fails to apply in phrasal compounds which form two words (44b) (Rice 1989: 240). A similar morphological conditioning of the tone spreading is also reported in Crow (Siouan; Matthews 1959).

80 

(44)              

 Hiroto Uchihara

Fort Nelson Slavey (Athabaskan) (Rice 1989: 240) a. Spreading b. No Spreading   [sáhdhéh] [dechį][jíh] (*dechį́jíh)                                  H                                    H      sah+dhéh    dechį jíh      bear+skin    wood hook      ‘bearskin’    ‘wood hook’

3.7 Diachrony of tones Not all tonal languages have always been tonal. Most tonal languages spoken in North America have recently developed tones from some segments (§ 3.7.1) or syllable structure through accent (§ 3.7.2). Their original source is traceable in many cases through internal reconstruction or the comparative method. This development of tones from other sources is called tonogenesis.

3.7.1 Tonogenesis from segments In the languages of North America, the most common segmental source of tones is the glottal stop. It has been well known that glottal stop has tonal effects on the preceding vowel (Hombert et al. 1978; Kingston 2011). A glottal stop itself can induce a higher tone; this is the case with some Dene/Athabaskan languages such as Slavey or Dënesu̜ ɬiné/ Chipewyan (Krauss 2005) and Montagnais Algonquian (Cowan 1983). A glottal stop can also induce a lower tone, via creakiness; this is the case with some other Dene/Athabaskan languages such as Tłı̨ chǫ/Dogrib or Diné bizaad/Navajo (Krauss 2005); Northern Iroquoian (Michelson 1988); Keres (isolate; Miller 1964: 17–18); Hopi (Manaster Ramer 1986); Takelma (Sapir 1912: 20); Quileute (Chimakuan; Hoard 1993); Heiltsuk (Rath 1986; Wilson 1987); Coast Tsimshian (Sasama 1997); and Sanya-Henya Lingít/Tlingit (Leer 1991: 12–18). Note that some Dene/Athabaskan languages have developed a low tone and others a high tone from a glottal stop (so-called ‘tonal flip-flop’). Even dialects of the same language have developed opposite tones, as in Dän kʼè/Northern Tutchone and Dän kʼí/ Southern Tutchone (Krauss 2005). For instance, the word for ‘arrow’ in Dän kʼè/Northern Tutchone is k’éʔ with a high tone, while in Dän kʼí/Southern Tutchone it has a low tone, k’àʔ (Krauss 2005: 94). More extreme cases are Oklahoma Cherokee (Uchihara 2009; 2016) or Halq̓eméylem/Upriver Halkomelem (Brown 2004), which has developed both higher and lower tone from a glottal stop depending on the morphological and phonological contexts. For instance, in Oklahoma Cherokee, one and the same morpheme

Tone 

 81

can alternate between a high tone and a lowfall tone, depending on the morphological context. Thus, in (45), both forms share the stem which can be internally reconstructed as *-yooʔst- ‘break (compact)’, but has an allomorph (another form of the same morpheme in another context) with the lowfall tone in the imperative form in (a) while in the punctual form it has a high tone (b). (45)        

a.        

Oklahoma Cherokee (Feeling 1975) hiyò:sda hi-yòò(ʔ)st-Ø-a 2sg.a-break.cmp-pct-ind ‘Break it (compact)!’

b.        

Oklahoma Cherokee (EJ 2011) hiyó:sda hi-yóo(ʔ)st-Ø-a 2sg.a-break.cmp-pct-ind ‘You just broke it (compact)’

Another common segmental source of tones is fricatives. A glottal fricative h has induced a lower tone in Montagnais (Algonquian; Cowan 1983), Northern Iroquoian (Michelson 1988), and Hopi (Uto-Aztecan; Manaster Ramer 1986); recall that Montagnais and Northern Iroquoian have also developed tones from a glottal stop. In Kickapoo (Algonquian; Gathercole 1983), fricatives θ, s, h induced a lower tone on the preceding vowel, and thus now speakers can employ whistle speech to communicate with each other, where segmental information is absent but the information is carried by the tones (Voorhis 1971).7

3.7.2 Tonogenesis from syllable structure and accent Tones can also develop differently depending on syllable structure or accent, as in Haida (Isolate; Hori 1996; Enrico 1998) or Heiltsuk (Wakashan; Wilson 1987). In Heiltsuk, the original stressed syllables acquired a high tone, while other syllables now have a low tone (Wilson 1987). In Haida, a high tone is found when the syllable has a long vowel, as in (46a) or (46b), or a sequence of a vowel + resonant, that is w, y, l, ɬ, m, n or ŋ as in (46c). A low tone is found on a word-initial syllable with a short vowel without any consonant in the final position of the syllable, as in (46d), or when this short vowel is followed by an obstruent, that is b, d, g, ɢ, p, t, k, q, t’, k’, q’, ʔ, sɬ, x, χ, or h as in (46e). A short syllable which follows a low-pitched syllable has a low tone, due to low-tone spreading (see § 3.5.1 of this chapter for tone spreading), as shown in (46  f ). Otherwise, a mid tone is found; that is, with a short syllable not followed by any consonant or a short syllable followed by an obstruent, which is not word-initial or preceded by a low-toned syllable. This is exemplified by the second syllable of the form in (46g):

7 This is practiced by courting adolescents in the Nacimiento community in Mexico. The whistle is produced by “cupping the hands and placing them together to form a chamber into which they blow with the lips placed against the knuckles of the thumbs. The pitch is controlled by lifting the fingers from the back of the chamber” (Voorhis 1971: 238).

82 

(46)              

 Hiroto Uchihara

Haida a. /ɬguu/ [ɬgú:] ‘heron’ b. /kiis/ [kí:s] ‘pus’ c. /qayd/ [qáyt] ‘tree’ d. /qu/ [qò:] ‘sea otter’ e. /mad/ [màt] ‘mountain goat’ f. /q’aχada/ [q’à:χàdà] ‘dogfish’ g. /ɬtalga/ [ɬtál̩ ga] ‘nest’

The generalization appears to be that in an accented (mostly initial) syllable, a high tone is found when the rhyme (the vowel and the following final consonant if there is one within the syllable) is heavy (a long vowel or a sequence of a vowel + a resonant), while a low tone is found when the rhyme is light (a short vowel or a sequence of a short vowel + an obstruent); in Haida, coda obstruent is not counted for the syllable weight, which is typologically common (Gordon 2006). In a non-accented syllable a mid tone is assigned, which could be the unmarked tone (cf. § 3.5.3), unless a low tone spreads from the preceding syllable.

3.8 Conclusion As we have seen in this chapter, tone can be challenging both for community members and for linguists working on such languages. However, tone can be an integral part of the language, serving to convey both lexical and grammatical functions (§ 3.4) and thus cannot be ignored. With adequate training and basic knowledge of tones, tone can be transcribed and represented in collaboration with the speakers (§ 3.2, § 3.3). Tonal contrasts can emerge from other sources such as segments or accent (§ 3.7). Tone in North American languages is still underdocumented and understudied. These languages can have typologically and theoretically interesting characteristics which are not reported in tonal languages of other parts of the world, and thus documentation and analysis of tonal systems of such languages is urgent for both language revitalization and linguistics. Acknowledgements: I am thankful to Karin Michelson, two anonymous reviewers and the editors of the volume, especially Keren Rice, for their feedback on this chapter.

Tone 

 83

3.9 References 3.9.1 General Beckman, Mary. 1986. Stress and Non-Stress Accent. Dordrecht: Foris. Bernard, Russell., George Mbeh & Penn Handwerker. 2002. Does marking tone make tone languages easier to read? Human Organization 61(4). 339–349. Bird, Steven 1999. When Marking Tone Reduces Fluency: An Orthography Experiment in Cameroon. Language and Speech 42. 83–115. Buckley, Eugene. 2019. Stress, tone and accent. In Ch. 3 of Daniel Siddiqui, Michael Barrie, Jessica Coon, Carrie Gillon, Jason Haugen & Eric Mathieu (eds.), Routledge Handbook of North American Languages, 68–90. New York, NY and Oxford: Routledge. Caballero, Gabriela & Matthew Gordon. 2021. Prosody in North American Indian languages. In Carlos Gussenhoven & Aoju Chen (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Language Prosody, 395–407. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Goldsmith, John A. 1976. Autosegmental Phonology. Cambridge, MA: Massachussetts Institute of Technology dissertation. Gordon, Matthew. 2001. A typology of contour tones. Studies in Language 25(3). 423–462 Gordon, Matthew. 2006. Syllable weight: phonetics, phonology, typology. New York, NY and Oxford: Routledge. Gordon, Matthew. 2014. Disentangling stress and pitch accent: Toward a typology of prominence at different prosodic levels. In Harry van der Hulst (ed.), Word stress: Theoretical and typological issues, 83–118. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Gordon, Matthew. 2016. Consonant-Tone Interactions: A Phonetic Study of Four Indigenous Languages of the Americas. In Avelino, Heriberto, Matt Coler & Leo Wetzels (eds.), The Phonetics and Phonology of Laryngeal Features in Native American Languages, 129–156. Leiden: Brill. Gordon, Matthew. 2017. Phonetic and phonological research on Native American Languages: past, present, and future. International Journal of American Linguistics 83(1). 79–101. Hyman, Larry. 2001. Private tone in Bantu. In Shigeki Kaji (ed.), Cross-linguistic studies of tonal phenomena, 237–257. Tokyo: Institute for the Study of Languages and Cultures of Asia and Africa. Hyman, Larry. 2006. Word-prosodic typology. Phonology 23. 225–257. Hyman, Larry. 2009. The representation of tone. In UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report (2009). 108–132. Hyman, Larry. 2009. How (not) to do phonological typology: a case of pitch-accent. Language Sciences 31. 213–238. Hyman, Larry. 2011. Tone: is it different? In John Goldsmith, Jason Riggle & Alan Yu (eds.), The handbook of phonological theory, 197–239. 2nd edn. Oxford: Blackwell Maddieson, Ian. 1976. Universals of tone. In Joseph Greenberg (ed.), Universals of human language, vol. 2: Phonology, 335–365. Stanford, California: Stanford University Press. Roberts, David, Dana Basnight-Brown & Valentin Vydrin. 2019. Marking tone with punctuation: an orthography experiment in Eastern Dan. In Yannis Haralambous (ed.), Proceedings of the conference Grafematik: Graphemics in the 21st century from graphemes to knowledge, 293–327. Brest, France: Fluxus. Snider, Keith. 2018. Tone Analysis for Field Linguists. Summer Institute of Linguistics. Yip, Moira. 2002. Tone. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

84 

 Hiroto Uchihara

3.9.2 Na-Dene Cook, Eung-Do. 1971. Vowels and tones in Sarcee. Language 47. 164–179. Cook, Eung-Do. 1989. Chilcotin tone and verb paradigms. In Eung-Do Cook & Keren Rice (eds.), Athabaskan linguistics,145–98. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Goossen, Irvy. 1995. Dine Bizaad: Speak, Read, Write Navajo. Salina bookshelf. Hargus, Sharon. 1985. Lexical Phonology of Sekani. Los Angeles, CA: University of California, Los Angeles dissertation. Hargus, Sharon. 2005. Prosody in two Athabaskan languages of northern British Columbia. In Sharon Hargus & Keren Rice (eds.), Athabaskan Prosody, 393–423. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Hargus, Sharon & Keren Rice (eds.). 2005. Athabaskan Prosody. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Holton, Gary. 2000. The Phonology and Morphology of the Tanacross Athabaskan Language. Santa Barbara, CA: University of California, Santa Barbara dissertation. Holton, Gary. 2005. Pitch, tone and intonation in Tanacross. In Sharon Hargus & Keren Rice (eds.), Athabaskan Prosody, 249–275. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Hoijer, Harry. 1943. Pitch accent in the Athapaskan languages. Language 19. 38–41. Hung, Beverly. 1959. On the phonemic status of Navaho stress. Anthropological Linguistics 19. 20–3. Kingston, John. 2005. The phonetics of Athabaskan tonogenesis. In Sharon Hargus & Keren Rice (eds.), Athabaskan Prosody, 136–184. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Krauss, Michael E. 2005. Athabaskan Tone. In Sharon Hargus & Keren Rice (eds.), Athabaskan Prosody, 55–136. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Landar, Herbert. 1980. On stress in Apachean languages. International Journal of American Linguistics 46. 227–30. Leer, Jeff. 1991. The schetic categories of the Tlingit verb. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago dissertation. Leer, Jeff. 1999. Tonogenesis in Athabaskan. In Shigeki Kaji (ed.), Cross-linguistic studies of Tonal Phenomena, Tonogenesis, Typology, and Related Topics, vol.1, 37–66. Tokyo: Institute of the Study of the Languages and the Cultures of Asia and Africa. Leer, Jeff. 2001. Shift of tone markedness in Northern Tlingit and Southern Athabaskan. In Shigeki Kaji (ed.), Proceedings of Symposium: Cross-Linguistic Studies of Tonal Phenomena, Tonogenesis, Japanese Accentology, and Other Topics, 61–86. Tokyo: Institute of the Study of the Languages and the Cultures of Asia and Africa. McDonough, Joyce. 1999. Tone in Navajo. Anthropological Linguistics 41(4). 503–39. McDonough, Joyce. 2003. Navajo sound system. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic. McDonough, Joyce, Jared O’Loughlin & Christopher Cox. 2013. An investigation of the three tone system in Tsuut’ina (Dene). The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 133. 3571. https://doi. org/10.1121/1.4806542 Pike, Eunice. 1986. Tone contrast in Central Carrier (Athapaskan). International Journal of American Linguistics 52. 411–18. Rice, Keren. 1976. Hare phonology. Toronto, ON: University of Toronto dissertation. Rice, Keren. 1989. The Phonology of Fort Nelson Slave Tone: Syntactic Implications. In Eung-Do Cook & Keren Rice (eds.), Athabaskan linguistics, 229–264. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Rice, Keren. 1990. Prosodic constituency in Hare (Athapaskan): Evidence for the foot. Lingua 82. 201–245. Rice, Keren. 2014. On beginning the study of the tone system of a Dene (Athabaskan) language: Looking back. Language Documentation & Conservation 8. 890–706. Rice, Keren & Sharon Hargus. 2005. Introduction. In Sharon Hargus & Keren Rice (eds.), Athabaskan Prosody, 1–45. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Sapir, Edward. 1925. Pitch accent in Sarcee, an Athapaskan language. Journal de la Société des Americanistes de Paris n.s. 17. 185–205.

Tone 

 85

Saxon, Leslie & Andrea Wilhelm. 2016. The “possessed noun suffix” and possession in two Northern Dene (Athabaskan) languages. International Journal of American Linguistics 82(1). 35–70 Story, Gillian. 1989. A Report on the Nature of Carrier Pitch Phenomena: With Special Reference to the Verb Prefix Tonomechanics. In Eung-Do Cook & Keren Rice (eds.), 1989, Athabaskan linguistics, 99–144. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Tuttle, Siri. 2003. Archival Phonetics: Stress and Tone in Tanana Athabaskan. Anthropological Linguistics 45(3). 316–336

3.9.3 Kiowa-Tanoan Harbour, Daniel. 2003. The Kiowa case for feature insertion. Natural Language and Linguistic Theories 21(3). 543–578. Miller, Taylor. 2018. The Phonology-Syntax Interface and Polysynthesis: A Study of Kiowa and Saluteaux Ojibwe. Newark, DE: University of Delaware dissertation. Sivertsen, Eva. 1956. Pitch problems in Kiowa. International Journal of American Linguistics 22. 117–130. Watkins, Laurel. 1980. A grammar of Kiowa. Lawrence, KS: University of Kansas dissertation.

3.9.4 Iroquoian Chafe, Wallace. 1977. Accent and related phenomena in the Five Nations Iroquois languages. In Larry Hyman (ed.), Studies in stress and accent, 169–81. (Southern California Occasional Papers in Linguistics 4). Los Angeles, CA: University of Southern California. Cornelius, Samantha. 2018. Prosodic Phonology in Oklahoma Cherokee. Arlington, TX: University of Texas at Arlington dissertation. Haag, Marcia. 2002. Cherokee tone associations with overt morphology. Chicago Linguistic Society 37(2). 413–423. Herrick, Dylan, Marcellino Berardo, Durbin Feeling, Tracy Hirata-Edds & Lizette Peter. Collaborative Documentation and Revitalization of Cherokee Tone. Language Documentation and Conservation 9. 12–31. Hirata-Edds, Tracy & Dylan Herrick. 2017. Building Tone Resources for Second Language Learning from Phonetic Documentation: Cherokee Examples. Language Documentation & Conservation 11. 289–304. Johnson, Keith. 2005. Tone and Pitch Accent in Cherokee Nouns. UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report (2005). http://www.linguistics.berkeley.edu/phonlab/annual_report/2005/Johnson1-48. pdf#search='Cherokee Tone’ Lindsey, Geoffrey. 1985. Intonation and interrogation: tonal structure and the expression of a pragmatic function in English and other languages. Los Angeles, CA: University of California, Los Angeles dissertation. Lindsey, Geoffrey. 1987. Cherokee Pitch Phonology. Ms. London: University College at London. ms. Michelson, Karin. 1988. A comparative study of Lake-Iroquoian accent. Dordrecht: Kluwer. Uchihara, Hiroto. 2009. High Tone in Oklahoma Cherokee. International Journal of American Linguistics 75(3). 317–36. Uchihara, Hiroto. 2016. Tone & Accent in Oklahoma Cherokee. Oxford Studies of Endangered Languages. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Wright, Richard. 1996. Tone and Accent in Oklahoma Cherokee. In Pamela Munro (ed.), Cherokee papers from UCLA, 11–22. (University of California Occasional Papers in Linguistics 16). Los Angeles: University of California, Los Angeles.

86 

 Hiroto Uchihara

3.9.5 Algonquian Bogomolets, Ksenia. 2023. Accent and tone in Plains Algonquian languages. In Ksenia Bogomolets & Harry van der Hulst (eds.), Word prominence in languages with complex morphology, 219–247. Oxford: Oxford University Press Cowan, William. 1983. The development of suprasegmental inflections in Montagnais. International Journal of American Linguistics 49. 64–71. Franz, Donald. 1972b. The origin of Cheyenne pitch accent. International Journal of American Linguistics 38. 223–5. Gathercole, Geoffrey. 1983. Tonogenesis and the Kickapoo tonal system. International Journal of American Linguistics 49. 72–6. Leman, Wayne. 1981. Cheyenne pitch rules. International Journal of American Linguistics 47. 283–309. Martin, Pierre. 1980. Des tons en montagnais? Cahier de Linguistique de l’Université du Québec 10. 175–94. Voorhis, Paul. 1971b. Notes on Kickapoo whistle speech. International Journal of American Linguistics 37. 238–43.

3.9.6 Siouan Gordon, Raymond, Jr. 1972. Pitch accent in Crow. International Journal of American Linguistics 38. 191–200. Kaschube, Dorothea. 1954. Examples of tone in Crow. International Journal of American Linguistics 20. 34–6. Kim, Michael. 1996. The tonal system of accentual languages. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago dissertation. Matthews, George. 1959. On tone in Crow. International Journal of American Linguistics 25. 135–6.

3.9.7 Uto-Aztecan Demers, Richard, Fernando Escalante & Eloise Jelinek. 1999. Prominence in Yaqui words. International Journal of American Linguistics 65. 40–50. Jeanne, LaVerne Masayesva. 1982. Some phonological rules of Hopi. International Journal of American Linguistics 48(3). 245–70. Manaster Ramer, Alexis. 1986. Genesis of Hopi tones. International Journal of American Linguistics 52. 154–60.

3.9.8 Pacific Northwest Brown, Jason C. 2004. Some tonogenetic properties of Upriver Halkomelem. In Lea Harper & Carmen Jany (eds.), Proceedings from the Seventh Workshop on American Indigenous Languages, 40–48. Santa Barbara: University of California, Santa Barbara Enrico, John. 1990. Masset Haida phonology. Berkeley, CA: University of California, Berkeley dissertation. Enrico, John. 1991. The lexical phonology of Masset Haida. (Alaska Native Language Center-Research Paper 8). Alaska Native Language Center, University of Alaska Fairbanks: Fairbanks, Alaska. Hoard, James. 1993. Quileute tones. In Mattina Montler & Timothy Montler (eds.), American Indian Linguistics and Ethnography in Honor of Laurence C. Thompson, 417–28. Missoula, MT: University of Montana. Hori, Hirofumi. 1994. Pitch in Skidegate Haida. In Osahito Miyaoka (ed.), Languages of the North Pacific Rim, 197–215. Sapporo: Hokkaido University. Hori, Hirofumi. 1996. Pitch assignment rules in Skidegate Haida. Gengo Kenyku 110. 28–51.

Tone 

 87

Kortlandt, Federik H. H. 1975. Tones in Wakashan. Linguistics 146. 31–34. Rath, John. 1985. Predictable and unpredictable tones in Heiltsuk Wakashan. Papers for the International Conference on Salish (and Neighbo(u)ring) Languages 20. 313–24. Sasama, Fumiko. 1997. A report on Coast Tsimshian ‘interrupted vowels’. Languages of the North Pacific Rim 2. 47–60. Sapporo: Hokkaido University. Wilson, Stephen. 1987. The development of tones in Heiltsuk. Berkeley Linguistics Society 13. 321–9.

3.9.9 Takelma Sapir, Edward. 1912. The Takelma Language of Southwestern Oregon. Handbook of American Indian Languages. Washington: Government Printing Office. Shipley, William. 1969. Proto-Takelman. International Journal of American Linguistics 35. 226–230.

Colleen M. Fitzgerald and Matthew K. Gordon

4 Segmental phonology

Abstract: This chapter explores some of the special characteristics of sounds (or segments) in North American languages. There are numerous ways in which the sounds of North American languages are special; for example, certain types of consonants that are common crosslinguistically do not occur in some languages in North America, e.  g. nasals, bilabials, while there are other sounds that are relatively rare in languages of the world but that are more widespread in North American languages, e.  g. ejectives, creaky voiced sonorants. These consonants combine in many languages to form potentially elaborate syllables. Because North American languages are known for their complexity in word structure, there are also many alternations in the realization of segments that depend on position in a word and neighboring sounds. Language revitalization efforts benefit from a better understanding of these special characteristics. Indigenous communities often focus on learners sounding “right”, achieving an accent that closely approximates fluent first language speech and minimizes “accentedness” (Munro and Derwing 2015, Bird and Kell 2017). An essential part of that is understanding the distinctiveness of the pronunciation of ancestral languages. Those distinctive aspects of sound systems are the focus of this chapter.

4.1 Introduction The sound systems of North American indigenous languages exhibit some relatively unique patterns of consonants and vowels, termed “segments”, in comparison with other languages of the world. These patterns have been understudied and possess characteristics that are dramatically different from English, Spanish, and French, the most widely spoken languages in North America. Increasing knowledge of the sound systems found in North American languages is beneficial for communities seeking to reclaim their languages. The next section (section  4.2) highlights some of the special and rare patterns observed in the inventory of segments found in particular North American languages, language families, and geographic regions. Section 4.3 delves into how sounds can affect each other and trigger changes of different kinds. Section 4.4 explores how segments may be combined to form larger units, known as syllables. The chapter concludes by synthesizing implications of the study of segments for language revitalization.

https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-004

90 

 Colleen M. Fitzgerald and Matthew K. Gordon

4.2 Consonants and vowels Considerable diversity exists in the inventory of segments found in languages of the world, both in the number and type of segments observed. For example, North American English has about 36 segments (24 consonants and 12 vowels), where the exact number varies between dialects and speakers, while Latin American Spanish varieties have approximately 22 segments (17 consonants and 5 vowels). North American indigenous languages vary considerably in their segment inventories, with some having fewer than English and Spanish and others having considerably more. For example, the Iroquoian language Seneca has 16 segments (9 consonants and 7 vowels), while the Na-Dene language Dëne Sųłinë́ (Chipewyan) has 63 segments (39 consonants and 24 vowels). The wide variation between languages in their number of segments is matched by comparable diversity in the type of segments that occur. Because of their diversity, the study of North American indigenous languages makes a significant contribution to the field of typology, which is concerned with identifying which properties are common, uncommon, and absent across languages and explaining the reasons for these distributions. Sections 4.2.1–4.2.3 explore these properties further for segment inventories in North America.

4.2.1 Segment inventories While a large portion of this chapter will focus on consonants and their properties, it is notable that one typological rarity fairly well-attested in North American languages, and more generally in the languages of the Americas, is related to their vowel systems. Native American languages often display relatively small-sized vowel inventories (Maddieson 2013g). For example, Central Alaskan Yup’ik (an Eskimo-Aleut language of Alaska), and the two languages of the southeastern United States, Chickasaw (a Muskogean language) and Caddo (a Caddoan language), all have vowel systems with only three different vowel qualities, which are distinguished through tongue position (height and backness) and the presence vs. absence of lip rounding. Chickasaw has the low central unrounded vowel /a/, the high front vowel unrounded /i/ and the mid back rounded vowel /o/ (Munro and Willmond 1994: ix), while the vowel systems of Central Alaskan Yup’ik (Jacobson 1984) and Caddo (Chafe 2018) consist of /a/ and /i/, but instead of /o/, they have the high back rounded vowel /u/. All three languages, however, have variants of these three basic vowel qualities differing in other dimensions. One of these dimensions is length, where short vowels contrast with long vowels that are produced by stretching out the short vowel without changing its quality (i.  e. tongue and lip position). For example, the Central Alaskan Yup’ik word /juk/ (spelled yuk in the Yup’ik orthography) ‘one person’ has a short /u/ while the word /juːk/ ‘two people’ (spelled yuuk) has a long /uː/ (Jacobson 1984:8), which is represented with the International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA) length markː. Chickasaw



Segmental phonology 

 91

and Caddo (but not Central Alaskan Yup’ik) also distinguish vowels based on whether they are oral or nasal(ized). Oral vowels are the default case cross-linguistically, while nasal vowels are produced by venting air through the nose, as for the vowels in French words like lent ‘slow’, pronounced [lã], or long ‘long’, pronounced [lõ]. In Chickasaw, the middle vowel in /takõlo/ ‘peach’ and the first vowel in /ĩnakfiʔ/ brother (of a woman)’ are nasalized. The combination of length and nasality is also responsible for the large number of vowels in Dëne Sųłinë́ mentioned above, as there are six vowel qualities all of which can occur in short and long variants and oral and nasal form, i.  e. there are both short and long oral vowels and both short and long nasal vowels. Turning to consonants, several languages with unusually small inventories of consonants are found in North America, e.  g. Iroquoian languages. For example, Lukaniec (2018) analyzes Wendat, an Iroquoian language, as having nine consonants, which is a small consonant inventory in comparison with the vast majority of the world’s languages (Maddieson 2013h). The consonants of Wendat appear in (1), where each consonant is represented using a sound of the International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA), a universal set of symbols that encodes any sound found in any language of the world. Most IPA symbols correspond to those used in writing systems based on the Latin alphabet, such as those used for English and most North American indigenous languages, but there are some differences; a couple of common ones are mentioned below. The columns in (1) reflect place of articulation, i.  e. where in the mouth a sound is produced, such that sounds at the left have more forward articulations than those to their right. Labial sounds, those produced at the lips, thus appear on the far left, while sounds produced at the larynx (or glottis), which contains the vocal folds, fall on the right side. In between the lips and larynx are different places of articulation, including the alveolar ridge (the bony ridge behind the upper teeth), which is contacted by the tongue blade in the production of sounds like /t/, /s/, /n/, and /r/, the hard palate, the point of contact for the tongue for the sound /j/, and the velum, which serves as the contact point for /k/ and /w/ and is the gateway to the nasal cavity—when raised, the velum blocks off the nasal chamber; when lowered, the result is a nasal sound like the alveolar /n/ and nasalized vowels, as in Chickasaw (see above). Rows in the chart in (1) encode manner of articulation, or the degree of constriction in the vocal tract. Plosives have a complete closure in the oral cavity and a closed velum, such that no air escapes through the nose, whereas nasals also have a complete oral occlusion but a lowered velum that allows venting of air through the nose. (Note that plosives are also often termed stops, a category that technically includes nasal consonants as well since there is a closure of the mouth for both plosives and nasals.) Finally, approximants have a relatively open vocal tract without air passing through the nose; acoustically, they are characterized by regular (termed “periodic”) vocal fold vibration (see chapter 1 on acoustics). Note that some sources draw a further division among approximants between the two glides /j, w/ and the liquids, which include lateral sounds, such as /l/, and rhotic sounds such as /ɹ/ (the r-type sound of English) and the Spanish trilled /r/.

92 

(1)

 Colleen M. Fitzgerald and Matthew K. Gordon

Wendat consonant inventory (Lukaniec 2018: 43) Labial Alveolar Palatal Velar Plosive t k Fricative s Nasal n Approximant ɹ j w

Glottal ʔ h

The majority of the consonants in Wendat roughly correspond to sounds in English with the exception of /ʔ/, the symbol for glottal stop, a sound that is produced by closing the vocal folds, as in the middle of the English word uh-oh where it is pronounced but not spelled. It may also be noted that there are two sounds in (1) that are found in English but spelled differently in the Wendat orthography. The palatal approximant (glide) /j/ is written as ‘y’ and is pronounced like English ‘y’ (e.  g. in the word yam), a convention also seen in the Central Alaskan Yup’ik example presented earlier. The alveolar approximant (rhotic) /ɹ/ is written ‘r’ and pronounced like English ‘r’ (e.  g. in the word rat). Wendat is also of interest because it lacks labial plosives. Of the 567 languages categorized by Maddieson (2013e), fewer than 1 % lack a labial consonant—and all are from North America, including another Iroquoian language, Oneida, and Wichita (Kirikirʔi:s), a Caddoan language related to Caddo. Another uncommon feature of Wichita (besides its lack of labial consonants) that contributes to its small inventory of 10 consonants, is its lack of nasal consonants (Rood 1975), a feature shared with its fellow Caddoan language, Pawnee (Parks and Pratt 2008). An absence of nasal consonants is a more general areal feature (see chapter 28 on language contact and linguistic areas) of many Pacific Northwest languages (which neither Wichita nor Pawnee are), including the Salish languages Twana and Lushootseed, the Chimakuan language Quileute, and the Wakashan languages Makah and Nitinat (Thompson and Thompson 1972). Historically, these languages are argued to have had nasals, but from that earlier stage, the sounds developed into voiced stops, leading to this inventory gap. At the other end of the inventory size continuum are the large consonant inventories (34 or more consonants per Maddieson 2013h), spread out in the Pacific Northwest across several unrelated language families. The Na-Dene language Dëne Sųłinë́

mentioned earlier has 39 consonants. The Coast Tsimshian language Sm’algy̠a̠ x has a similarly large consonant inventory with 38 distinct consonants. We will examine this language a bit further below in section 4.2.2, since Sm’algy̠a̠ x also has some consonant types found infrequently in other languages.

4.2.2 Cross-linguistically underattested segments Maddieson (2013f) identifies several kinds of “uncommon” segment types: pharyngeals, labial-velars, clicks, and “th” sounds (his categorization of dental/alveolar non-stri-



 93

Segmental phonology 

dent fricatives as in the English word think). The first two of these categories occur in Native American languages and will be discussed in this section, alongside several other cross-linguistically less common segment types that are found in many languages of North America: uvulars, glottalized nasals and approximants, ejectives, and laterals. Pharyngeals are produced by retracting the very back of the tongue, the tongue root, toward the rear wall of the pharynx (or back of the throat). Pharyngeal sounds occur in only 4.1 % of the languages surveyed in Maddieson (2013f). The Interior Salish language, Snchitsu’umshtsn (Coeur d’Alene) includes the pharyngeal /ʕ/, described in teaching materials by the fluent speaker Lawrence Nicodemus (1975: xvi) as having “no equivalent” in English, “best learned by listening to native speakers”. Observations in Doak and Montler (2000) suggest that that pharyngeal consonants are not commonly produced nor perceived in the second language pronunciation (and the writing) of Snchitsu’umshtsn. Another type of consonant produced far back in the mouth are uvulars, which involve a constriction, either a closure as for a plosive or a narrowing as for a fricative, at the uvula, the dangling appendage visible at the back of the throat. Maddieson’s survey (2013f) indicates that uvulars are found roughly twice as frequently as pharyngeals (which still makes them relatively rare), and he notes they often occur in the indigenous languages of the western part of the United States and Canada. The Wakashan language Nuuchahnulth, spoken in British Columbia, Canada, has two pharyngeal consonants, /ʕ ħ/, which occur in fairly robust distribution in the language and are derived historically from uvular fricatives and glottalized uvular stops (Carlson, Esling, and Fraser 2001). The consonant inventory for Sm’álgyax (Coast Tsimshian) is presented in (2) and features uvulars among its 38 distinct consonants, one of the largest consonant inventories found across the world’s languages. (2)

Sm’álgyax consonant inventory (adapted from Mulder 1994: 20) Labial Alveolar Palataliz- Velar Labialized velar ed velar Plosive:  Ejective pʼ tʼ k jʼ kʼ kwʼ j  Voiceless p t k k kw  Voiced b d gj g gw Affricate:  Ejective tsʼ  Voiceless tsʼ  Voiced dzʼ Fricative s ɬ Nasal:  Plain Voiced m n  Glottalized ʔm ʔn

Uvular Glottal

qʼ q ɢ

ʔ

χ

h

94 

 Colleen M. Fitzgerald and Matthew K. Gordon

Approximant: Plain Voiced Glottalized

l ʔl

j ʔj

ɰ ʔɰ

w ʔw

The labels for the charts in (1) and (2) encode categories like place and manner of articulation (both of which were discussed earlier), as well as other dimensions involving the configuration of the vocal folds and the source of the air for the consonants. We discuss here those additional dimensions applicable to plosives and return in section 4.2.3 to those affecting nasals and approximants. Voiced sounds are produced with vibration of the vocal folds, whereas voiceless sounds have no vocal fold vibrations. Ejectives involve a more complex orchestration of timing relations between the vocal folds and the oral constriction. They are produced by closing the vocal folds, as for glottal stop, at about the same time as the oral closure for the consonant is produced and then raising the entire larynx upward to compress the air between the closed vocal folds and the oral closure for the consonant. Finally, the oral closure and then shortly afterwards the vocal fold closure are released, allowing air to rush out of the mouth to produce a distinctive “popping” sound that makes ejectives one of the most auditorily salient type of segments. In ejectives, the source of the air responsible for the sound, termed the airstream mechanism, is not the lungs as in most other sounds, but the cavity between the vocal folds and the consonant’s constriction. Sm’álgyax has a majority of its consonants articulated in the back of the mouth, at the velum or further back. Sm’álgyax also has a variety of complex segments. Complex segments have two articulations, articulations that could represent a sequence of two distinct segments, except that these two articulations pattern as a single unit in the language. Complex segments include affricates, which start with a stop that releases into a fricative, often at the same place of articulation. English has two affricates, /ʧ/, sometimes phonetically represented as /č/ and heard at the beginning of chat, and /dʒ/, sometimes phonetically represented as /ǰ/ and heard at the beginning and end of judge. Unlike the English affricates, which are produced between the palatal and alveolar regions, Sm’álgyax’s affricates are alveolars and include an ejective. Another type of complex segment in Sm’álgyax are the glottalized nasals and approximants / ʔm ʔn ʔl ʔj ʔw ʔɰ /. Glottalized nasals and approximants are particularly common in the northwestern portion of North America (Maddieson 2013c). Finally, complex segments are also reflected in the velar segments that occur in (2) with secondary articulations, a j-like palatalization, represented by a superscripted /j/, e.  g. /kj/, and a w-like labialization, represented by a superscripted labial-velar approximant /w/, e.  g. /kw/. It may also be noted that Sm’álgyax has a voiceless lateral fricative, which is produced by lowering one or both sounds of the tongue just slightly so the air passing over the tongue creates noise, similar to the /l/ if you produce the English word clay in slow motion. Near the far end of the large and typologically intriguing consonant inventories is Tlingit, a Na-Dene language with 42 consonants shown in (3).



(3)

Segmental phonology 

 95

Tlingit consonant inventory (adapted from Maddieson, Smith and Bessell 2001: 139) Alveo- Post- Pala- Late- Velar Lablzd Uvular Lablzd Glottal velar uvular lar alveo- tal ral lar Plosive:  Ejective tʼ kʼ kwʼ qʼ qwʼ ʔ w w  Unaspirated t k k q q  Aspirated th kh kwh qʰ qwh Fricative:  Plain s š ɬ x xw χ χw h w w  Ejective sʼ ɬʼ xʼ x ʼ χʼ χ ʼ Affricate:  Ejective tsʼ ʧʼ tɬʼ  Unaspirated ts ʧ tɬ h h  Aspirated ts ʧ tɬh Nasal n Approximant j w

None of the Tlingit consonants are labials, but complex segments, uvulars and laterals are abundant. The 10 uvulars include plain and labialized uvular plosives and fricatives. In addition to featuring ejectives, the Tlingit consonant inventory includes an aspiration contrast for the plosives; the unaspirated plosives are simple voiceless ones whereas the aspirated ones are associated with a puff of air when they are released, much like the English initial plosives in pan, tan, and can. Tlingit is also typologically remarkable in possessing ejective fricatives, which are found in only 2.2 % of languages according to the UCLA Phonological Segment Inventory Database (UPSID) (http://web.phonetik.unifrankfurt.de/upsid_info.html). Ejective fricatives present an articulatory challenge since they require simultaneously a tight enough constriction to allow for pressure to build up for the salient ejective release while also allowing for enough air to escape through the constriction to produce the turbulent airflow required for a fricative. One of the ejective fricatives of Tlingit is /ɬʼ/, even though Tlingit lacks the far more common lateral approximant /l/. Tlingit is noteworthy in having five laterals, none of which are approximants. Among North American indigenous languages, Ahtna (a Na-Dene language of Alaska), Nuuchahnulth, Kutenai (a language isolate spoken in Idaho and Montana), and Kiowa (a Tanoan language of Oklahoma) also lack a sonorant /l/, but have laterals produced using other manners of articulation.

96 

 Colleen M. Fitzgerald and Matthew K. Gordon

4.2.3 Phonation and voicing In this section, we focus on properties of sounds that involve the vocal folds. Many languages employ a contrast between voiceless and voiced sounds. A voicing contrast is particularly common for plosives as in Sm’álgyax. Less common are voiced vs. voiceless distinctions for other types of sounds. For example, many languages have only voiceless fricatives and not voiced ones as in Tlingit. (This differs from English, which does contrast voiceless and voiced fricatives, e.  g. the pair of words sip vs. zip.) There is an aerodynamic reason for the bias in favor of voiceless fricatives. Vocal fold vibration is dependent on supraglottal air pressure, above the vocal folds, being lower than subglottal pressure below the vocal folds since air flows from areas of higher pressure to areas of lower pressure. Fricatives, however, require sufficient supraglottal pressure behind the oral constriction to create the turbulent air flow requisite for a fricative. What is particularly rare is for a language to make a voicing distinction for consonants other than plosives but not for plosives. Mesa Grande ’Iipa (Diegueño), a Yuman language spoken in southern California, is thus unusual in having a voicing contrast only for laterals; this contrast occurs at both the alveolar (4a) and palatal (4b) places of articulation. It may be noted that the voiceless laterals are fricated in Mesa Grande ’Iipa as is typical across languages. (4)

Mesa Grande ’Iipa voicing contrast (Langdon 1966: 32–33) Voiced laterals Voiceless laterals a. xəlul ‘he plays the flute’ wəl̥ itʃ ‘he is bad’ b. wəʎak ‘he lies down’ xəʎ̥aː ‘moon’

Another feature relevant to the vocal folds is aspiration, which we saw earlier in the discussion of Tlingit. Aspiration is produced when the vocal folds are open (abducted) widely enough apart to allow substantial airflow from the lungs into the oral cavity but still close enough together to create turbulence as the air passes through the vocal folds. Consonants can differ in whether the aspiration precedes or follows the consonant with which it is associated. In Tlingit (and also English), there are post-aspirated plosives, meaning that the vocal fold abduction gesture extends after the release of the oral closure for the plosive. In some languages, however, plosives can be preaspirated, meaning that the vocal fold opening gesture precedes the oral constriction. Preaspiration is considerably rarer than postaspiration across languages. Tohono O’odham (Papago), a Uto-Aztecan language of southern Arizona and Sonora, Mexico, is a language with preaspiration; it occurs with voiceless plosives preceding a vowel (5a) but does not affect voiced stops (5b). The lack of preaspiration with voiced stops is expected since the vocal fold abduction gestures needed for aspiration is incompatible with the vocal fold adduction gesture necessary for the vocal fold vibration associated with voicing.



(5)

Segmental phonology 

 97

Preaspiration in Tohono O’odham stops (Fitzgerald 1996) a. máhkai ‘doctor’ máːgina ‘car (Spanish loan)’ b. wáːhpag ‘hole’ (plural) čaːbo ‘short-legged one’

There is significant variation across languages in the specific aspects of preaspiration, such as how much frication occurs and what degree of voicing occurs. Tohono O’odham’s Uto-Aztecan relatives like Southern Paiute, Chemehuevi, Ute (Miller, Elzinga, and McLaughlin 2005), Hopi (Whorf 1946) and Rarámuri (Tarahumara; Valiñas 2001) also have preaspiration, as do the Central Algonquian languages Menominee, Fox, Cree and Ojibwa (Bloomfield 1935). While it is most common for stops and fricatives to be voiceless, for vowels, it is the opposite—voiceless vowels are almost always limited to specific contexts, in particular adjacent to voiceless consonants. A number of indigenous languages, particularly in the western United States, devoice vowels, especially Uto-Aztecan languages. Southern Ute speakers regularly devoice unstressed vowels (see chapter 6 on stress) before a voiceless consonant (Oberly and Kharlamov 2015); this devoicing process is a feature that has been a challenge in language revitalization for learners (Oberly 2013). The examples in (6) illustrate vowel devoicing from words written in both the IPA (on the left) and the tribal orthography (on the right). Voiceless vowels are indicated with an open circle under the vowel in the IPA and with underlining in the orthography. Note that the IPA symbol ˈ indicates that the following syllable is stressed (see chapter 6 on stress). (6)

Devoicing in Southern Ute (Oberly and Kharlamov 2015: 3) a. ʧi̥ ˈʧiɣɛ chi̱ chige ‘to be hard’ b. tɯ̥ˈkwa tü̱kwa ‘to be deep’ c. ko̥ˈpokitiː ko̱pokitii ‘to cause to break’ d. ku ̥ˈpɛnapɯ ku︬ penapü ‘bat, drum stick’

Voicing may also be subject to modifications based on the configuration of the vocal folds. These differences in voicing are often termed phonation types and include a common kind found in languages of North America, creaky voicing. In creaky phonation, the tension of the vocal folds is increased, resulting in vibratory cycles that are irregular and lower in fundamental frequency or pitch (see chapter 1 on pitch). The Wakashan language, Kwakw’ala (Kwakiutl) contrasts regular or modal voiced nasals (7a), with creaky voiced nasals, indicated by a tilde under the nasal (7b). It may be noted that consonants produced with glottal constriction, including ejectives (see above), are often termed glottalized and may be represented with a superscripted glottal stop. For example, the creaky voiced /m̰ n̰/ in Kwakw’ala may alternatively be represented as /ˀm ˀn/. (7)

Creaky nasals in Kwakw’ala (Gordon and Ladefoged 2001: 387) a. nəm ‘one’ naka ‘drinking’ b. n̰an̰əm̰a ‘nine’ n̰ala ‘day’

98 

 Colleen M. Fitzgerald and Matthew K. Gordon

Contrasts involving creaky voicing are underattested cross-linguistically, but Gordon and Ladefoged (2001) observe they occur in a number of indigenous languages in the northwest, including Montana Salish, Hupa, and Kashaya Pomo (see Howe and Pulleyblank 2001 for more on glottalization in languages of North America).

4.3 Phonological processes In discussing the voiceless vowels of Southern Paiute, it was noted that voiceless vowels are limited to certain contexts, in particular before a voiceless consonant. Another condition on voiceless vowels in Southern Paiute is that they are unstressed. In other words, vowels that are either stressed or not adjacent to a voiceless consonant do not become voiceless. In fact, the same vowel may be voiceless in one form of a word and voiced in another form if the context changes between the two forms. This situation often arises when a morpheme, a basic element of meaning, is added to a word and creates alternations in the realization of sounds. This can be seen in the Southern Ute forms in (8), in which the base forms in (8a) and (8c) end in a voiceless vowel but their counterparts with a suffix, the plural -u in (8b) and the locative -vwan ‘on’ in (8d), have a voiced vowel in place of the original voiceless vowel. (8)

Voicing alternation in Southern Ute vowels (Oberly and Kharlamov 2015: 4) a. mamaʧi̥ mamachi ‘woman’ b. mamaʧiu mamachiu ‘women’ c. tɯkaɁnapɯ̥ tüka’napü̲ ‘table’ d. tɯkaɁnapɯvwan tüka’napüvwan ‘on the table’

This alternation, or phonological process, illustrates how sound patterns change when morphological processes lead to new or different phonological environments by creating different combinations of sounds in different places. In (8), the word-final environment is removed when morphemes are added. These phonological processes can produce differences that fluent speakers may not be conscious of, but their absence is noticeable and perceivable as sounding “different”. Returning again to Nuuchahnulth, the glottalized nasals /ˀm ˀn/ (along with their glottalized glide counterparts /ˀj ˀw/) may trigger creakiness in the vowels occurring on both sides of the glottalized resonant (Carlson, Esling, and Fraser 2001). This a natural process resulting from the overlap of the constriction gesture for the glottalized nasals with adjacent vowels. A similar type of process triggered by gestural overlap, but of a different type of gesture, is observed in English when the lowered velum position for a nasal consonant spills over to an adjacent vowel creating a nasalized vowel. For example, the vowel in the word tan is nasalized but this nasalization is predictable from the environment, namely being adjacent to a nasal consonant. This differs from the nasalization in Chickasaw or French, which can distinguish words with different



Segmental phonology 

 99

meanings, e.  g. the French pair beau ‘beautiful’ (phonetically [bo]) vs. bon ‘good’ (phonetically [bõ]). Consonants can also be affected by context. Klamath, a language of California and Oregon, allows plosives to contrast three ways: voiceless unaspirated, voiceless aspirated, and ejective. However, voiceless aspirated and ejective plosives surface as voiceless unaspirated plosives before another obstruent (9c, f). The aspiration or ejective property is retained when the plosive occurs before vowels (9a, d) or sonorants (9b, e). (9)

Ejectives and Aspirated Plosives in Klamath (Blevins 1993: 246) Stem mphet’ ‘floats’ a. C’V mphet’iːqi ‘floats up’ b. C’S mphet’wa ‘floats in water’ c. C’O mphetplanʧ’a ‘floats downstream’ Stem pheʧh ‘foot’ d. CʰV pheʧʰiːqi ‘puts a foot over’ e. CʰS pheʧʰwa ‘puts a foot into the water’ f. CʰO pheʧk’wa ‘puts a foot across’

The examples of alternations thus far have involved a segment being affected by an adjacent one. Alternations can sometimes operate at a distance. One such example of a long-distance alternation is provided by vowel harmony, a process involving spreading of certain properties of a vowel, e.  g. frontness or rounding, from one vowel to another across adjacent consonants. In languages with vowel harmony, vowels can be divided into two (or potentially more depending on how many properties are involved) groups that are mutually exclusive in a word, i.  e. all vowels in a word belong to one set or the other set but may not be mixed between the two groups. Vowel harmony is observed in Nimipuutímt (Nez Perce), a Sahaptin language of the western U.  S. on the Columbia Plateau. The language has five vowels, /i e a o u/ that can be separated into two sets on the basis of their patterning: if one set of vowels (called the dominant series) is present, the other vowels (called the recessive set of vowels) do not appear in the word, with a few exceptions (Aoki 1966). The examples in (10) show how /a o/ belong to the dominant set and trigger a shift of the vowel in the prefix neʔ- (10a vs. 10b) and the suffix -eʔ (10c vs. 10d). (10)

Vowel Harmony in Nimipuutímt (Aoki 1966: 759–60) a. neʔ-ˈmeχ ‘my paternal uncle’ b. naʔ-ˈtoːt ‘my father’ c. ˈmeq-eʔ ‘paternal uncle!’ d. ˈtoːt-aʔ ‘father! ’

Vowel harmony occurs in a number of Native North American languages, including Salish languages like Snchitsu’umshtsn (Doak 1992), and the Pomoan language, Kashaya (Oswalt 1961, Walker 2013).

100 

 Colleen M. Fitzgerald and Matthew K. Gordon

Consonants may also be involved in long distance harmony processes. Tahltan, a Na-Dene language, displays harmony among coronal consonants (i.  e., articulations using the blade or tip of the tongue). In the Tahltan consonant inventory in (11) all the consonants from dental to postalveolar (delimited by bold lines) are coronal consonants, and the subset of those in shaded columns are the ones that participate in consonant harmony. (11)

Tahltan consonant inventory (adapted from Shaw 1991: 144) Labial Dental Lateral Inter- Alveo- Postalve- Velar Labializdental lar olar ed velar b d dl dð dz dz g gw t tɬ tθ ts ʧ k kw tʼ tɬʼ tθʼ tsʼ ʧʼ kʼ k wʼ ɬ l m

n, ˀn

θ ð

s z

ʃ ʒ

x ɣ

j

w

xwʼ ɣw

Uvular Glottal ɢ q qʼ ʁ χ

ʔ h

While the process is not limited to the first person singular subject marker ‘I’ -s-, that morpheme shows how the different sets of coronals act in this system. In (12), the first person morpheme is in bold, showing that it surfaces as -θ- in (12a–b), as -ʃ- in (12c–d), and as -s- in (12e–f). The determining factor for which variant appears is in whether there are any coronals from one of the shaded sets in (11). If there is an interdental or postalveolar consonant to the right of the first person -s- marker, then the -s- matches the place of articulation of that consonant but otherwise remains -s- if there are no interdental or postalveolar consonants to its right. It may be noted that the triggering sound may be either immediately adjacent to the first person morpheme, as in (12c–d) or may be separated from it, as in (12a–b). (12)

Coronal Harmony in Tahltan (morpheme /s/, “I”/1st sg. subject; Shaw 1991: 144) a. dɛθkʷʊθ ‘I cough’ b. mɛθɛθɛθ ‘I’m wearing (on feet)’ c. hudiʃʧa ‘I love them’ d. nɛʃjɛɬ ‘I love them’ e. ɛsdan ‘I love them’ f. sɛsxɛɬ ‘I’m going to kill it’

The Na-Dene language family has various patterns of coronal consonant harmony, as do other languages also have consonant harmony, such as Chumashan languages (Shaw 1991, Mithun 1997).



Segmental phonology 

 101

4.4 Syllable structure The previous sections examined consonants and vowels in two general ways: the patterns of the inventories of each in a given language (and across languages), and the influences that segments wield on each other’s features as word-building processes generate new segment sequences. This section describes how consonants and vowels are organized into larger units, known as syllables. Vowels are typically the core elements (often termed the nucleus) of syllables, potentially preceded by consonants (where they are termed the syllable onset) and/or followed by consonants (where they are termed the syllable coda). In Karuk (see chapter 47 on Karuk), syllables consist of “[a]ny consonant plus an immediately following vowel, plus any immediately following consonant that is not immediately followed by a vowel” (Bright 1957: 11). The syllabification patterns in Karuk are shown for one syllable words in (13a–b) and two syllable words in (13c–g). The period symbol . is used to indicate boundaries between the different syllables, while C stands for any consonant, V for any short vowel, and Vː for any long vowel. Note that the acute accent mark in certain forms marks tone, which is not relevant to the discussion of syllable structure (see chapter 3 on tone). (13)

Syllables in Karuk (Bright 1957) Syllable Shape One Syllable a. tas CVC b. xuːn CVːC

c. d. e. f. g.

Two Syllables sára púraf ʔamkir pusjaːh túːjʃip

CV.CV CV.CVC CVC.CVC CVC.CVːC CVːC.CVC

Gloss ‘fence’ ‘acorn mush’

‘bread’ ‘deer’ ‘table’ ‘toyon berry’ ‘mountain’

The maximal syllable in Karuk is CVC and syllables without an onset consonant are prohibited. This kind of syllable structure is complex, but not overly so, and in the survey of languages in Maddieson (2013j), roughly half of the languages display moderate complexity in syllables. In contrast, a language with maximally simple syllable structure starts with a single onset consonant, but does not have any coda consonants. Syllable position plays an important role in the sound patterns of many languages of the world, since many restrict certain consonants to either onsets or coda position. In the Na-Dene language Deg Xinag, there is a three-way contrast between voiceless unaspirated, voiceless aspirated, and ejective plosives in onset position but a two-way contrast between voiceless and voiced plosives in coda position. Conversely, there is a tripartite distinction between plain voiced, glottalized, and voiceless

102 

 Colleen M. Fitzgerald and Matthew K. Gordon

nasals in coda position but only plain voiced nasals in the syllable onset (Hargus 2012: 165–6). Returning to Karuk, the syllable complexity is strictly regulated. When word-building processes lead to sequences of segments that are not permissible as syllables in Karuk, phonological processes alter those sequences to conform to the syllable canon of CV(C) (where the parentheses around the C indicate optionality of the coda consonant). In (14), vowel-initial stems in Karuk contrast with consonant-initial stems. There are two prefixes, nani- ‘my’ and mu- ‘his’, which end in a vowel. While this does not create any challenges for stems that begin with a consonant, vowel-initial stems show a syllable repair strategy for many speakers, who say mú-psi:h instead of the expected form mu-apsi:h and naní-psiːh instead of expected naní-apsiːh (though some speakers do say mú-ʔapsi:h and naní-ʔapsiːh). (14)

Vowel Deletion in Karuk (Bright 1957) V-initial stem Gloss C-initial stem a. ápsiːh ‘leg’ típah b. naní-psiːh ‘my leg’ nani-típah c. mú-psiːh ‘his leg’ mu-típah

Gloss ‘brother, male cousin’ ‘my brother, male cousin’ ‘his brother, male cousin’

What is the problem, in terms of syllable structure, with a form like *mu-apsiːh? Karuk does not allow onsetless syllables and repairs them by eliminating the first vowel of the stem in mú-psiːh. It may be noted that that is not the only available strategy for avoiding onsetless syllables in cases of vowel sequences (a situation that is called hiatus) arising at morpheme boundaries—another option, which many languages opt for, would be to insert a consonant between the vowels. In fact, the careful reader may have noticed that the form ápsiːh ‘leg’ begins with a vowel, which would appear to violate the requirement that syllables have an onset consonant. As it turns out, speakers add a glottal stop to words that would otherwise begin with a vowel. Thus, ápsiːh is realized as ʔápsiːh. This process of adding a sound is called epenthesis. The Yakima variety of Sahaptin, a Sahaptian language of Washington and Oregon, displays more complex syllable structure, with certain kinds of consonant clusters permitted in the syllable onset and coda, e.  g. skɨtks ‘fringe’, and sɨt’χws ‘Brodiaea hyacinthine’ (Hargus and Beavert 2002). However, these consonant clusters display an internal organization such that sonorants (also termed resonants), which are a class of consonants with periodic (i.  e. non-noisy) energy (see chapter 1) that includes approximants and nasals, must be closer to the vowel nucleus than obstruents, a set of consonants that is characterized by noisy energy and includes fricatives and plosives. An obstruent followed by a sonorant is thus possible in the onset (15a–c) and a sonorant followed by an obstruent is viable as a coda cluster (15d–e), but the opposite order is not permitted in either position. Onset and coda clusters, including those consisting of two obstruents (15f–k), are allowed subject to these sonority sequencing principles. Following Hargus and Beavert’s practice, the examples in (15) are schematized with O for obstruent and S for sonorant.



(15)

Segmental phonology 

 103

Consonant Clusters in Yakima Sahaptin (Hargus and Beavert 2002: Appendix) Cluster type Gloss a. χmɨɬ OSɨO ‘ridged’ b. ʧʼmɨt OSɨO ‘red elderberry, Sambucus racemosa’ c. ʃnɨm OSɨS ‘Crataegus douglassi’ d. ʃɨlk OɨSO ‘cricket’ e. kwɨnʧ OɨSO ‘tree moss’ f. ɬɨɬχ OɨOO ‘dirt’ g. ʦʼɨps OɨOO ‘released, popped off’ h. χɨʧʼk OɨOO ‘removing quickly’ i. kʼpɨs OOɨO ‘cold’ j. wɨtk SɨOO ‘half, piece’ k. qw’ʃɨm OOɨS ‘mischievous’

The patterns of syllable structure in Yakima Sahaptin illustrate a trend identified by Maddieson (2013j) of a positive correlation between syllable complexity and inventory size such that languages with more segments tend to allow greater syllable complexity. English shares with Yakima Sahaptin a high threshold for syllable complexity in terms of number of consonants, e.  g. in words like strengths (phonetically /stɹɛŋθs/) and squelched (/skwɛlʧt/). However, English allows far fewer sonority plateaus or reversals involving a higher sonority consonant closer to the nucleus. Consequently, the combinations of consonants occurring in many North American indigenous languages present challenges to learners acquiring both a new place of articulation, airstream mechanism, or phonation type while simultaneously deploying that new articulatory dimension in a complex syllable.

4.5 Conclusions The incredible segmental diversity of the indigenous languages of North America plays an essential role in the cross-linguistic understanding of what is possible in phonology, not only for these languages, but abstractly for language more generally. Especially noteworthy are the consonant systems, from the size of the inventory, the number of contrasting places of articulation, or the permissible consonant clusters, among other features. Some of these aspects are also vulnerable to change or loss as languages have fewer fluent first language speakers. The contrast between glottalized and non-glottalized consonants is one example. While first language speakers of Dëne Sųɬiné (Chipewyan) contrast three sets of stops, /d t tʼ/, Cook (2005) observes that adults whose first language is English, but who have learned Dëne Sųɬiné deglottalize the ejectives (so producing /tɬ/ rather than /tɬʼ/). Recent research has developed a better understanding for effective teaching of indigenous languages (i.  e., Hermes 2007, Johnson 2014, Czaykowska-Higgins, Burton,

104 

 Colleen M. Fitzgerald and Matthew K. Gordon

Marinakis, McIvor 2017, McIvor, Jacobs, and Jenni 2018; see also chapter 34). That literature is growing, but only a small proportion attends to the pronunciation and phonological implications for indigenous language revitalization. One challenge is that more documentation and analysis of the phonetics and phonology of indigenous languages is needed (Whalen, Di Canio, and Shaw 2011, Fitzgerald 2017b). Most of the world’s languages have been studied in far less detail than English or other major world languages so there is certainly a need for more phonetic and phonological documentation and analysis of the sounds systems of indigenous languages. Another challenge, as demonstrated in this chapter, is that the sound systems of North American indigenous languages differ anywhere from slightly to substantially from those of English, Spanish or French (Whalen, Di Canio, and Shaw 2011; Bird and Miyashita 2018). Accordingly, achieving a pronunciation that sounds like the elders is challenging because it means being able to perceive and produce potentially a large number of novel contrasts, e.  g. the difference between velars and uvulars, between plain and ejective stops, modal voiced and creaky voiced nasals, etc., and being able to articulate all of these differences when such consonants occur consecutively (Bird and Kell 2017). Despite these challenges, communities focused on language revitalization can draw from a number of important principles grounded in the research on second language pronunciation for other languages. Evidence suggests that pronunciation should be explicitly taught, even at the beginning of instruction (Thomson and Derwing 2015, Bird and Kell 2017). Language instruction benefits from an understanding of the linguistic aspects of sound structure like the segmental inventory or topics covered in this and the other phonetic and phonological chapters of this volume. Exposure to authentic language use, especially steady streams of spoken language from elders in conversation or even stories plays a role in helping learners perceive the different sounds and where word and phrasal breaks occur (Derwing and Munro 2014: 50–51, as well as studies cited there). Using culturally grounded materials like those that are part of the verbal arts tradition of that language, such as genres of storytelling, song, or poetry (Fitzgerald and Hinson 2015; Fitzgerald 2017a) is one way to provide that authentic input. Finally, resources exist for communities and linguists, such as the Teaching Pronunciation in Indigenous Languages course (Teaching Pronunciation for Indigenous Languages 2018) that has been taught several times at the Institute on Collaborative Language Research or through collaborative projects in teaching and research that focus on phonetics, phonology, or pronunciation cited here.



Segmental phonology 

 105

References Aoki, Haruo. 1966. Nez Perce vowel harmony and proto-Sahaptian vowels. Language 42. 759–767. Bird, Sonya & Sarah Kell. 2017. The role of pronunciation in SENĆOŦEN language revitalization. The Canadian Modern Language Review/La Revue Canadienne des language vivantes. 73(4). 538–569. Bird, Sonya & Mizuki Miyashita. 2018. Teaching phonetics in the context of Indigenous language revitalization. Proceedings of the International Symposium on Applied Phonetics (ISAP). 39–44. doi 10.21437/ ISAPh.2018-7 Blevins, Juliette. 1993. Klamath Laryngeal Phonology. International Journal of American Linguistics 59. 237–280 Bloomfield, Leonard. 1935. On the sound-system of Central Algonquian. Language 1. 130–156. Bright, William. 1957. The Karok language. Volume 13. Berkeley, CA: University of California publications in linguistics. Carlson, Barry F., John H. Esling & Katie Fraser. 2001. Illustrations of the IPA: Nuuchahnulth. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 31(2). 275–279. Chafe, Wallace. 2018. The Caddo Language: A grammar, texts, and dictionary based on materials collected by the author in Oklahoma between 1960 and 1970. Petoskey, MI: Mundart Press. Cook, Eung-Do. 2006. The patterns of consonantal acquisition and change in Chipewyan (Dëne Sųłiné). International journal of American linguistics 72(2). 236–263. Czaykowska-Higgins, Ewa, Strang Burton, Onowa McIvor & Aliki Marinakis. 2017. Supporting Indigenous language revitalisation through collaborative post-secondary proficiency-building curriculum. In Wesley Y. Leonard & Haley De Korne (eds.), Language Documentation and Description. Vol 14, 136–159. London: EL Publishing. Derwing, Tracey & Murray J. Munro. 2014. Once you have been speaking a second language for years, it’s too late to change your pronunciation. In Linda Grant & Donna M. Brinton (eds.), Pronunciation myths: applying second language research to classroom teaching, 34–55. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press. Doak, Ivy. 1992. Another Look at Coeur d’Alene Harmony. International Journal of American Linguistics 58(1). 1–35. Doak, Ivy & Timothy Montler. 2000. Orthography, lexicography, and language change. In Nicholas Ostler & Blair Rudes (eds.), Endangered Languages and Literacy. Proceedings of the Fourth FEL Conference, 132–138. Bath, England: FEL. Fitzgerald, Colleen M. 1996. Aspiration in Tohono O’odham. paper presented at the LSA Annual Meeting, San Diego, CA, January 4–7, 1996. Fitzgerald, Colleen M. & Joshua D Hinson. 2015. Using listening workshops to integrate phonology into language revitalization:  Learner training in Chickasaw pronunciation. Paper presented at the 4th International Conference on Language Documentation and Conservation, University of Hawai’i, Honolulu, Hawai’i, February 26–March 1, 2015. Fitzgerald, Colleen M. 2017a. Motivating the documentation of the verbal arts: Arguments from theory and practice. Language Documentation and Conservation 11. 114–132. Online http://hdl.handle. net/10125/24728 (accessed 10 December 2019). Fitzgerald, Colleen M. 2017b. The Sounds of Indigenous Language Revitalization. Invited plenary address at the Annual Meeting of the Linguistic Society of America. Austin, TX, January 5–8, 2017. www.youtube. com/watch?v=wrPe_6KdoOo&list=PLc4TBef_CiuokIawF2lrXL2q6vyP4IIs7 (accessed 3 February 2017). Gordon, Matthew K. & Peter Ladefoged. 2001. Phonation types: a cross-linguistic overview. Journal of phonetics 29(4). 383–406. Haas, Mary R. 1940. Tunica. (Handbook of American Indian Languages, vol. 4, no. 40, part 4a). New York: JJ Augustin.

106 

 Colleen M. Fitzgerald and Matthew K. Gordon

Hargus, Sharon. 2012. Deg Xinag rounding assimilation: a case study in phonologization. Laboratory Phonology 3.1. 163–193. Hargus, Sharon & Virginia Beavert. 2002. Predictable versus underlying vocalism in Yakima Sahaptin. International Journal of American Linguistics 68(3). 316–340. Hermes, Mary. 2007. Moving toward the language: Reflections on teaching in an Indigenous immersion school. Journal of American Indian Education 46(3). 54–71. Howe, Darin & Douglas Pulleyblank. .2001. Patterns and timing of glottalisation. Phonology 18. 45–80. Doi: 10.1017/S0952675701004018. Jacobson, Steven. 1984. Yup’ik Eskimo Dictionary. Fairbanks: University of Alaska, Alaska Native Language Center. Johnson, M. K. (Sʔímlaʔxw). (2014). yaʕ̓tmín cqw əlqwilt nixw, uł nixw, ul nixw, I need to speak more, and more, and more: Okanagan-Colville (Interior Salish) Indigenous second-language learners share our filmed narratives. Language Documentation and Conservation 8. 136–167. Kaisse, Ellen M. & Patricia A. Shaw. 1985. On the theory of Lexical Phonology. Phonology 2(1). 1–30. Langdon, Margaret H. 1966. A Grammar of Diegueño: The Mesa Grande Dialect. Berkeley,CA: University of California dissertation. https://escholarship.org/uc/item/08t8n3db (accessed 31 December 2019). Lukaniec Megan. The elaboration of verbal structure: Wendat (Huron) verb morphology. Santa Barbara, CA: University of California, Santa Barbara dissertation. Maddieson, Ian. 2013a. Voicing and Gaps in Plosive Systems. In Matthew S. Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. http://wals.info/chapter/5 (accessed 17 February 2019). Maddieson, Ian. 2013b. Uvular Consonants. In Matthew S. Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. http://wals.info/chapter/6 (accessed 17 February 2019). Maddieson, Ian. 2013c. Glottalized Consonants. In Matthew S. Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. http://wals.info/chapter/7 (accessed 17 February 2019). Maddieson, Ian. 2013d. Lateral Consonants. In Matthew S. Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. http://wals.info/chapter/8 (accessed 17 February 2019). Maddieson, Ian. 2013e. Absence of Common Consonants. In Matthew S. Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. http://wals.info/chapter/18 (accessed 17 February 2019). Maddieson, Ian. 2013f. Presence of Uncommon Consonants. In Matthew S. Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. http://wals.info/chapter/19 (accessed 17 February 2019). Maddieson, Ian. 2013g. Vowel Quality Inventories. In Matthew S. Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. http://wals.info/chapter/2 (accessed 20 October 2019). Maddieson, Ian. 2013h. Consonant Inventories. In Matthew S. Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. http://wals.info/chapter/1 (accessed 20 October 2019). Maddieson, Ian. 2013i. Voicing in Plosives and Fricatives. In Matthew S. Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. http://wals.info/chapter/4 (accessed 2 November 2019). Maddieson, Ian. 2013j. Syllable Structure. In Matthew S. Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. https://wals.info/chapter/12 (accessed 17 February 2019).



Segmental phonology 

 107

Maddieson, Ian, Caroline L. Smith, & Nicola Bessell. 2001. Aspects of the Phonetics of Tlingit. Anthropological Linguistics 43(2). 135–176. http://www.jstor.org/stable/30028779 (accessed 9 November 2019). McIvor, Onowa, Peter Jacobs & Barbara Jenni. 2018. Adult Indigenous contributions to reviving languages in British Columbia through Mentor-Apprentice style learning: Research Report. Victoria, British Columbia, Canada: University of Victoria. Retrieved from www.netolnew.ca/resources. Mithun, Marianne. 1999. The Languages of Native North America. New York: Cambridge University Press. Miller, Wick R., Dirk Elzinga & John E. McLaughlin. 2005. Preaspiration and gemination in Central Numic. International journal of American linguistics 71(4). 413–444. Mulder, Jean G. 1994. Ergativity in Coast Tsimshian (Sm’algyax). Volume 124. Berkeley,CA: University of California Press. Munro, Murray J. & Tracy M. Derwing. 2015. A prospectus for pronunciation research in the 21st century: A point of view. Journal of Second Language Pronunciation 13. 11–42. Munro, Pamela & Catherine Willmond. 1994. Chickasaw: An Analytical Dictionary. Norman, OK: University of Oklahoma Press. Nicodemus, Lawrence. 1975. Snchitsu’umshtsn: The Coeur d’Alene Language: a Modern Course, Albuquerque, NM Southwest Research Associates. Available online at https://plateauportal.libraries.wsu.edu/digitalheritage/snchitsuumshtsn-coeur-dalene-language-page-xvi-xvii (accessed 8 November 2019). Oberly, Stacey. 2013. The phonetic correlates of Southern Ute stress. In Shannon Bischoff, Amy V. Fountain, Deborah Cole & Mizuki Miyashita (eds.), The persistence of language: constructing and confronting the past and present in the voices of Jane H. Hill, 85–106. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Oberly, Stacey & Viktor Kharlamov. The phonetic realization of devoiced vowels in the Southern Ute language. Phonetica 72(1). 1–19. Oswalt, Robert L. 1961. A Kashaya grammar (Southwestern Pomo). Berkeley, CA: University of California dissertation. Parks, Douglas R. & Lula Nora Pratt. 2008. A Dictionary of Skiri Pawnee. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska in Cooperation with the American Indian Studies Research Institute, Indiana University. Rood, David S. 1975. The implications of Wichita phonology. Language 51(2). 315–337. Shaw, Patricia A. 1991. Consonant harmony systems: The special status of coronal harmony. In Carole Paradis & Jean-François Prunet (eds.), Phonetics and phonology, vol. 2: The special status of coronals: internal and external evidence, 125–57. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Thomson, Ron I. & Tracy M. Derwing. 2015. The Effectiveness of L2 Pronunciation Instruction: A Narrative Review. Applied Linguistics 36(3). 326–344. Thompson, Laurence C. & M. Terry Thompson. 1972. Language universals, nasals, and the Northwest coast. In M. Estellie Smith (ed.), Studies in Linguistics in Honor of George L. Trager, 441–456. The Hague: Mouton. Teaching Pronunciation for Indigenous Languages. 2018. Workshop offered at the Institute on Collaborative Research (CoLang). Instructors, Ewa Czaykowska-Higgins & Colleen Fitzgerald, instructors. Gainesville: University of Florida. https://colang.lin.ufl.edu/home/colang-2018-workshops/ (accessed 17 February 2019). Valiñas, Leopoldo. 2001. Lengua, dialectos e identidad étnica en la Sierra tarahumara. In Claudia Molinari & Eugeni Porras (eds.), Identidad y cultura en la sierra Tarahumara, 105–125. Mexico: Instituto Nacional de Antropología e Historia. Walker, Neil A. 2013. A Grammar of Southern Pomo. An Indigenous Language of California. Santa Barbara, CA: University of California, Santa Barbara dissertation. Whalen, D. H., Christian T. DiCanio & Patricia A. Shaw. 2011. Phonetics of endangered languages. Acoustics Today 7. 35–42. Whorf, Benjamin. 1946. The Hopi Language, Toreva Dialect. In Harry Hoijer et al (ed.), Linguistic Structures of Native America, 158–183. (Viking Fund Publications in Anthropology 6). New York: Cornelius Osgood.

Suzanne Urbanczyk

5 Prosodic morphology Abstract: Prosodic morphology refers to a wide range of processes to make new words by reference to the sound patterns in a word, such as adding syllables or deleting or changing the order of segments. These processes are less common than simply adding segments to indicate a new meaning, but are found in many Indigenous languages of North America. This chapter provides an overview of the kinds of prosodic morphology processes found as well as a brief outline of how understanding these patterns can aid in teaching and learning new words.

5.1 Introduction Words are made of sounds and have meanings. Some words are simple, in terms of having just a few sounds and one meaning. Other words can be more complex, with several identifiable meanings and sound groupings. In many languages of North America, new words can be created by a variety of ways that do not involve simply adding an affix such as a prefix like re- or a suffix like -s to the edge of a base, as in the word re-write-s. A base is the part of the word to which some word formation processes apply. For example, in Hiaki (Uto-Aztecan) the way to express habitual meaning involves copying the first syllable of the base, as illustrated in (1) below. This chapter provides an overview of a range of prosodic morphology patterns, as found in the languages of North America, starting with a general introduction to the field. The bulk of the chapter illustrates the many ways that prosodic structure is relevant to understanding word patterns. It wraps up by discussing some ways that concepts like syllable structure can be helpful in teaching and learning word patterns like this.

5.1.1 Word patterns The sets of words in (1–6) below show a range of ways that a new meaning can be expressed by reference to units like syllables. Relevant portions of the word are underlined and syllable boundaries are indicated with a period, where it could be helpful. The linguistic terms used to refer to the patterns are indicated above each example. For example, in (1) the first syllable is copied to express that the action is ‘habitual’, and this way to make new words, by having a copy of part of a word is knowns as reduplication. The underlined portion of the words below indicate what is added. As can be seen, it looks like an exact copy of the first syllable.

https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-005

110 

(1)    

 Suzanne Urbanczyk

Reduplication – ‘habitual’ Hiaki (Haugen 2014: 511) vu.sa vu.vu.sa ‘awaken’ či.ke či.či.ke ‘comb one’s hair’

In Southern Sierra Miwok (Miwok), the syllable structure of the base changes, either by making a consonant or a vowel longer or shorter (long consonants and vowels are represented with a colon following the symbol for the long segment). The base (or stem) has a specific target or shape it needs to be, depending on the suffix that is attached. For example, notice that in Stem 3 in (2), the second consonant is always long, and in Stem 4, the second consonant is always followed by another consonant. (2)  

Templatic morphology – Southern Sierra Miwok (Broadbent 1964: 38) Stem 1 Stem 2 Stem 3 Stem 4 meaning

     

mus:alo:tkowta-

musahlotkowat-

mus:aʔlot:uʔkow:at-

musʔalotʔukowta-

‘to be ashamed’ ‘to catch’ ‘to bump into’

In Dakota (Siouan), an affix meaning ‘I’ (indicated with underlining) is added inside the root, going after the first syllable of the word as in (3). (3)    

Infixation – Dakota, 1st person (Boas & Deloria, 1939) na.pcu ‘swallow’ na-wa-pca ‘I swallow it’ la.k’o.ta ‘Lakota’ la-ma-k’o.ta ‘I am a Lakota’

In Koasati (Muskogean), plural meaning is expressed by removing part of a syllable. The part of the syllable removed is underlined in the first column below in (4). (4)  

Subtractive morphology – Koasati ‘plural’ rhyme deletion (Kimball, 1991) singular plural meaning

     

pi.táf-fi-n a.ko.co.fót-li-n ti.wáp-li-n si.mát-li-n

pít-li-n a.ko.cóf-fi-n tíw-wi-n sím-mi-n

‘to slice up the middle’ ‘to jump down’ ‘to open something’ ‘to cut up tanned skin’

Lushootseed (Salish) shows that a vowel change can occur when copying a syllable to indicate the meaning of ‘diminutive’, as illustrated in (5). The accent indicates that the vowel is stressed. (5)    

Ablaut – Lushootseed diminutive reduplication (Urbanczyk, 2006) ǰə́səd ‘foot’ ǰí-ǰəsəd ‘little foot’ tədzíl ‘lie in bed’ tí-tədzil ‘lie in bed for a little while’



Prosodic morphology 

 111

And finally, in Saanich (Salish) the syllable structure changes in yet another way, by reordering the consonant and vowel, to indicate an action is occurring now as in (6). (6)    

Metathesis – Saanich ‘actual’ (Montler, 1986) θk̓ʷə́t ‘straighten it out’ θə́k̓ʷt ‘straightening it out’ ƛ̓pə́x̣ scatter’ ƛ̓ə́px̣ ‘scattering’

The shape, location and conditions of these changes are frequently described in terms of syllables and rhythmic patterns, or what is often referred to as prosodic structure. The next section discusses some key concepts related to prosody and prosodic morphology.

5.1.2 Prosodic morphology One way to see how prosodic structure is important to understanding word structure comes from taking a closer look at the Hiaki word for ‘awaken’ as represented in (7) below. The base has two syllables indicated by the symbol σ above the segments. The segments that are grouped into syllables are linked to the syllables by lines. Next to vusa ‘awaken’, one can see that the ‘habitual’ is formed by copying the first syllable. The syllable that is affixed is indicated with shading. Notice that it is an exact copy of the syllable that comes after it. We can describe this pattern as prefixing a syllable. (7)    

    σ         σ      vusa ‘awaken’

  →   

    σ          σ         σ         vuvusa ‘habitual’

When looking at the word patterns (morphology) above, the changes that are associated with a new meaning can be understood in terms of prosodic structure, and fall under the domain of prosodic morphology. As one can see, the concept of prosodic unit, such as a syllable, is central to describing the patterns and a brief explanation of what these prosodic units are is useful to understanding the field. In essence, prosodic structure is relevant to understanding how segments are grouped into larger units, such as syllables. Syllables are then grouped into larger prosodic units, called feet, in which one syllable is more prominent than the other, and which frequently reflects the rhythmic pattern of the language. Feet are essential units in the prosodic word. Just as there can be more than one syllable in a word, so too, there can be more than one foot. The following prosodic hierarchy illustrates the various prosodic units and what the larger category is above it. The most elemental prosodic unit is a mora, which can be referred to as a timing or a weight unit. Notice that a mora (µ) is below the syllable; only some segments in a syllable are linked to moras, and more about this will be discussed below. The symbols and abbreviations used to represent these prosodic units are indicated at the far left.

112 

(8)        

 Suzanne Urbanczyk

Prosodic Hierarchy (Selkirk 1980) PWd prosodic word Ft foot σ  syllable µ  mora

McCarthy and Prince (1986, 1990) proposed that meaningful units (morphemes) can be prosodic units; they developed several principles to account for some of the processes illustrated in (1–6). The basic idea is that rather than affixes and bases being composed solely in terms of segments, affixes and bases can also be prosodic units, which lack segments: they are empty prosodic units (or templates), that need to be filled by segments. A template is a meaningful prosodic unit, like syllable or foot, but does not have any segments in it. The cornerstone of this approach to understanding words like this is the Prosodic Morphology Hypothesis (PMH) and is defined as follows. (9)  

Prosodic Morphology Hypothesis (McCarthy and Prince 1990: 209) Templates are defined in terms of the authentic units of prosody: mora (µ), syllable (σ), foot (Ft), prosodic word (PWd) and so on.

Other principles refer to how the prosodic units are filled with segments. There are also ways to identify the base, by referencing prosodic structure, as for example with the infixing pattern in (3). The patterns found in the languages of North America illustrate a wide range of ways in which prosodic units can be added to bases (as affixes) or can be targets to which processes apply (as bases) and we will look at patterns for each of these in sections 2 and 3 below. The process of reduplication involves doubling segments to indicate meaning, and there are some interesting patterns that arise when more than one reduplicative or prosodic morpheme is added to a word, which is the focus of section 4.

5.2 Prosodic units as affixes One of the key ways that prosodic morphemes are used to express meaning is when they are affixes. The basic insight here is that any type of prosodic unit can be associated with meaning and then affixed. Empty prosodic units (those that have no segments) gain segmental content by copying segments from the base to which they are attached. This section outlines patterns found to support each level of the prosodic hierarchy, starting with the mora, and building up to the syllable, then the foot and prosodic word.



Prosodic morphology 

 113

5.2.1 Mora affixation A mora can be described as a timing unit, representing vowel and consonant length; it is also a unit of weight, which is relevant to locating stress (Hyman, 1985; Chapter 7, by Gordon). Let’s start by first discussing the relevant parts of a syllable. There are three main parts to a syllable. The onset is the first part of the syllable, coming before the vowel, and is always at least one consonant. The nucleus is the centre of the syllable and is usually a vowel, and the coda refers to any consonants at the end of a syllable. The nucleus and the coda are sometimes collectively called the rhyme. Interestingly, onsets do not play a role in determining where stress falls and so are not ever moraic. The following illustrates how length is encoded via moraic structure, with simple syllables. Following convention, C is used to represent any consonant and V is used to represent any vowel. The difference between a short and long vowel is in the number of moras: one for a short vowel and two for a long vowel, as in (10.a). (10)                            

a.             b.            

Moras as timing units Vowel length   Short Long σ  σ      µ   C V Consonant length Short       σ        σ             µ       µ   C V C V

 µµ   C   V   Long  σ   σ             µ µ    µ   C V C  V

A short consonant is linked to just one position in the syllable, as in (10.b). Notice that it is only linked to the onset of the syllable – there are no moras for short consonants. On the other hand, long, or geminate consonants are represented in (10.b) and are linked to the mora and coda of the syllable before it, as well as the onset of the second syllable. The double linking represents double length. A range of patterns have been analyzed as affixation of a mora: vowel lengthening, consonant gemination, reduplication, epenthesis, subtractive morphology, and metathesis (Saba Kirchner, 2010; Zimmermann, 2017). The following examples from Alabama (Muskogean) straightforwardly show how adding a mora either results in a lengthened vowel (11.a) or a lengthened consonant (11.b), depending on the syllable structure and size of the base. Recall that length is represented by having a colon after the symbol that is long. For instance, in the forms in (11.a), the second to last vowel of the base is long in

114 

 Suzanne Urbanczyk

the imperfect, and in (11.b) the consonant that begins the second to last syllable is long in the imperfect. The affected segments are indicated with shading. (11)  

Alabama imperfect (Hardy & Montler, 1988) base imperfect gloss a.           b.          

Vowel lengthening hofna hó:fna ‘smell’ isko í:sko ‘drink’ isi í:si ‘take, catch’ coba có:ba ‘big’ ibakpila ibakpí:la ‘turn upside down’ Consonant lengthening (gemination) bala:ka bál:a:ka ‘lie down’ coko:li cók:o:li ‘sit down’ ilkowatli ilków:atli ‘move’ ataka:li atak:a:li ‘hang one object’ afinapli afin:apli ‘lock up’

The following diagrams illustrate how adding a mora lengthens the vowel (12.a) as well as the consonant (12.b). The affixed mora is indicated with shading under the column labeled ‘imperfect’. Notice that the shaded mora is affixed inside the first syllable – it is the rightmost mora in the first syllable. In (12.a) the mora affix is linked to a vowel – so the vowel becomes long – it has two moras. In (12.b), the mora affix is linked to a consonant – so is realized as a long consonant (a geminate). The length of the vowel or consonant is represented by how many moras it is linked to. (12)

mora affixation

                         

a.               b.      

Vowel lengthening base imperfect     σ               σ     σ                 σ               µ      µ           µµ          µ     c o b a c o   b a     Consonant gemination base imperfect     σ    σ             σ     σ    σ             σ               µ       µµ        µ          µµ  µµ        µ





c o k   o  l i

c ó k   o  l i

          ‘big’           ‘sit down’



Prosodic morphology 

 115

This way of representing the affix as a mora helps to explain the two different changes of lengthening as coming from the same input – a segmentally empty prosodic unit that needs to be filled. Because there are multiple ways to fill a mora as above, affixing a mora can also account for different forms of a morpheme (allomorphs), depending on what the segmental content of the base is. Other ways to fill a mora can be seen in Kwak’wala (Wakashan) in which the mora is realized as either vowel lengthening, when the initial syllable is one mora as in (13.a) when the first syllable of the base is a light syllable, or as reduplication, if the initial syllable is two moras – or heavy – as (13.b). Notice that nasal consonants n and m make the first syllable heavy, equivalent to a long vowel. There is independent evidence from stress that plain sonorant consonants are moraic, but obstruents and glottalized resonants are not. Kwak’wala shows that languages vary as to which coda consonants can be moraic. (13)                  

a.       b.        

Kwak’wala (Wakashan, Saba Kirchener 2013: 240) Vowel lengthening c̓əx →  c̓a:x.m̓u:t ‘singe’ ̓kəxʷ ̓ ̓ →  ka:xʷ.mu:t ‘suck with whole mouth’ t̓əs →  t̓a:s.m̓u:t ‘crack barnacles’ Reduplication wən →  wən.wə.mu:t ‘drill with auger’ ma: →  ma:.mə.mu:t ‘(serpent) crawl’ c̓a:s →  c̓a:.c̓əs.mu:t ‘eel grass’ ̓kəmƛ →  k̓əm.k̓əɫ.m̓u:t ‘adze’

Another example of how mora affixation is associated with a range of allomorphs comes from Saanich (Salish): reduplication (14.a), metathesis (14.b), and glottal stop insertion/ infixing (14.c) have all be analyzed as prefixing a mora (Stonham, 1994). (14)                        

Straits (SENĆOŦEN) actual allomorphy (Montler, 1986; Stonham, 1994) a. reduplication   t̓ᶿéʔ ‘be on top’ t̓ᶿét̓ᶿəʔ ‘riding (a horse)’   s-kʷúl ‘school’ s-kʷúkʷəl ‘going to school’   ɫík̓ʷsən ‘trip’ ɫíɫək̓ʷsən̓ ‘tripping’ b. metathesis   sx̣ə́t ‘push it’ sə́xṭ ‘pushing it’   θk̓ʷə́t ‘straighten it out’ θə́k̓ʷt ‘straightening it out’   ƛ̓pə́x̣ scatter’ ƛ̓ə́px̣ ‘scattering’ c. glottal stop infix (epenthesis)   čáq̓ʷəŋ ‘sweat’ čáʔqʷəŋ̓ ‘sweating’   wéqʷəs ‘yawn’ wéʔqəs ‘yawning’   xʷítəŋ ‘jump’ xʷíʔtəŋ̓ ‘jumping’

116 

 Suzanne Urbanczyk

How the mora is filled depends on the segments in the base. Affixing a mora provides a way to see what these different patterns have in common. While they look like completely different processes, the range of patterns can be explained as filling an affixed empty mora.

5.2.2 Syllable reduplication A frequent shape of reduplicative morpheme is a syllable. As was seen above, there are light (one mora) and heavy (two moras) syllables. Each of these is attested in reduplication patterns. Examples of both light and heavy syllable reduplication are found in Plains Cree (Algonquian). Ahenekew & Wolfhart (1983, p. 370) note that the long vowel of the reduplicant is “typically followed by devoicing”, indicated by /h/ below. The reduplicant (copied portion) is indicated with underlining below. (15)      

a. b. c.

Plains Cree (Ahenekew & Wolfart, 1983, p. 371) Base pimohte:‘walk along’ Light syllable ni-pa-pimohta:n ‘I am walking along’ Heavy syllable ni-pa:h-pimohta:n ‘I walk off and on’

In the examples above, only the first consonant is copied, as the vowel quality is [a]. The different shapes arise because a monomoraic syllable is affixed in (15.b), while a bimoraic syllable is affixed in (15.c). The difference in length is then achieved because the segmental quality of the vowel links to the empty moras in the syllable to fill the two moras, and creates a long vowel in (15.c) (Marantz, 1982; McCarthy & Prince, 1986). Hiaki (Uto-Aztecan) illustrates an instance of true syllable copying, where the first syllable of the base is copied (Haugen, 2014). If the stem begins with an open, codaless syllable, the whole syllable is copied (16.a). If the base stem begins with a closed syllable, as in (16.b), then that syllable including the coda is copied. (16)                    

a.         b.        

Syllable copying reduplication: Hiaki habitual (Haugen, 2014: 511) CV.CV-initial stems vu.sa vu.vu.sa ‘awaken’ či.ke či.či.ke ‘comb one’s hair’ ču.pa ču.ču.pa ‘grow (transitive verb)’ he.wi.te he.he.wi.te ‘agree’ CVC.CV-initial stems vam.se vam.vam.se ‘hurry’ čep.ta čep.čep.ta ‘jump over’ čuk.ta čuk.čuk.ta ‘cut with a knife or saw’ hit.ta hit.hit.ta ‘make a fire’

Hiaki syllable reduplication is rarer than other types of syllable reduplication: the shape of syllable doesn’t matter – it is copied exactly as it is in the base. So, it is proposed that



Prosodic morphology 

 117

the affix is a syllable – it gets segments by copying the first syllable. No specification of weight is needed, as the weight varies. The example of ‘awaken’ was presented with full syllable structure in (7).

5.2.3 Foot/prosodic word reduplication When the reduplicant is two syllables in size, this often corresponds with being a foot in size. There are two basic types of prosodic feet, identifiable in terms of which syllable is more prominent (Hayes, 1995). When the initial syllable is emphasized (the strong syllable or head) it is a trochaic foot. When the final syllable is stronger (the head), it is an iambic foot. Each of these foot types can have various properties, including being monosyllabic. The basic classification and representation of feet as iambic and trochaic is provided below. Trochaic feet are binary, where the two elements are either having two syllables (for a syllabic trochee, as in 17.a) or having two moras (for a moraic trochee, as in 17.b). For languages with the system in (17.a), it doesn’t matter what kind of syllables they are, only that two syllables are grouped into a foot. Iambic feet are always quantity sensitive, which means that the number of moras is important, not the number of syllables. There is a preference for a light (one mora)-heavy (two moras) pattern, as seen in the initial foot type in (17.c). The stressed or strong syllable is indicated with an S below it. (17)                                      

a.         b.             c.            

Basic inventory of prosodic feet (Hayes, 1995) Syllabic trochee Ft         σ σ     S      Moraic trochee Ft Ft     σ σ σ     µ µ µ µ   S  S   Iamb Ft Ft Ft σ σ

σ σ

σ

µ µ µ S

µ µ S

µ µ S

118 

 Suzanne Urbanczyk

The following examples of Yurok (Algic) ‘repetitive’ reduplication illustrate what appears to be a moraic trochee. All the different types of moraic trochee are found in the language. The copied portion can be two light syllables (18.a) or two different types of heavy syllable: CVC- (18.b) and CV:- (18.c). The choice of reduplicant form depends on segmental properties of the base (Garrett, 2001). (18)                  

a.     b.     c.    

Yurok ‘repetitive’ reduplication (Garrett, 2001) Bisyllabic meno:ʔ ‘pull anything’ meno-meno:tek̓ ‘I repeatedly pull’ yekʷoh(s-) ‘to fold’ yekʷo-yekʷoh ‘to fold several things’ CVCkep̓eɫ ‘housepit’ kep̓-kep̓eɫ ‘there are several housepits’ ket̓ey ‘to park, moor’ ket̓-ket̓ey- ‘to lie (boats)’ CV:maʔepet‘to tie up’ ma:-maʔepet‘to tie right up’ moʔohkeloyt ‘to make into a ball’ mo:-moʔohkeloyt ‘make several balls’

In all the forms above, CVCV, CVC and CV: are different ways to fill a trochaic foot affix. So, while it might seem that there are three distinct patterns, they can all be understood as a trochaic foot. The reduplicated words below from Fox (Algonquian) illustrate that bisyllabic size can correspond to a prosodic word. Recall from the introduction that there is a hierarchy of how prosodic units are built, where syllables can be grouped into feet and that feet are grouped into prosodic words. Teasing apart what is a foot vs. a prosodic word involves looking at what happens at the ends of words and seeing if that happens at the end of the reduplicant. If end of the reduplicant has the same properties as the end of a word, it is likely a prosodic word. Dahlstrom (1997, p. 218) observes that the second syllable of the reduplicant has the same patterning as syllables at the ends of words: both end with a short vowel, regardless of what the second syllable of the base is. Notice that even if the vowel in the second syllable of the base is long, as in (19.a) or is long and followed by a consonant, as in (19.b), the second syllable of the reduplicant always has the shape CV. (19)    

Fox prosodic word reduplication (Dahlstrom, 1997) a. či:.pi-či:.pi:.kʷe:-wa ‘he winks’ b. ko.kʷa-ko.kʷa:š.ke:-wa ‘he is jerked’

Having looked at ways that prosody is relevant to adding meaningful content to a base, we now turn to patterns in which segments are deleted and how that can be described in terms of syllable structure as well.



Prosodic morphology 

 119

5.2.4 Subtractive morphology & truncation There are two general ways that material is deleted to indicate a new meaning in a word. In one, the segments associated with a prosodic unit are deleted. This is generally referred to as subtractive morphology: the meaning is indicated by subtracting a prosodic unit. In the other, the base is reduced in size to a prosodic unit; the segments not associated with a larger prosodic unit are deleted, which is generally referred to as truncation. Muskogean languages have subtractive morphology, with deletion of a specific prosodic unit, such as a rhyme or coda (Fitzgerald, 2016), as seen in the Koasati words indicating ‘plural’ meaning below. The deleted part of the word from the ‘singular’ is indicated with shading and underlining. Notice that this is the end of the syllable: either the rhyme (20.a) or coda (20.b). (20)    

Koasati ‘plural’ (Kimball, 1991) a. Rhyme-Deletion   singular plural

                 

              b.  

pitáf-fi-n akocofót-li-n tiwáp-li-n simát-li-n ataká:-li-n albití:-li-n apoɬó:-ka-n Coda-Deletion singular

pít-li-n akocóf-fi-n tíw-wi-n sím-mi-n aták-li-n albít-li-n apóɬ-ka-n

‘to slice up the middle’ ‘to jump down’ ‘to open something’ ‘to cut up tanned skin’ ‘to hang something’ ‘to place on top of’ ‘to sleep with someone’

plural

gloss

       

       

asikóp-li-n atóf-ka-n kacáɬ-ɬi-n akapós-kan

asiko:-li-n ató:-ka-n kacá:-li-n akapó:-kan

‘to breathe’ ‘to melt’ ‘to bite something’ ‘to be pinched’

gloss

Another way that segments are deleted to indicate meaning is called truncation. In this case, rather than stating what is deleted in terms of syllable parts (like codas), the process involves deletion of everything so that the new word has a specific prosodic shape. An example of truncation from Central Alaskan Yup’ik (Eskimo-Aleut) illustrates the pattern, in which the target shape is a foot or prosodic word. These are names and so that is why they are all spelled with a capital letter.

120 

 Suzanne Urbanczyk

(21)  

Central Alaskan Yup’ik (Woodbury, 1985) Full noun Proximal Vocative

       

Aŋukaɣnaq Nupiɣak Kalixtuq Aɣnaɣayaq

Aŋ ~ Aŋuk Nup ~ Nupix/Nupik Kal ~ Kalik Aɣən

The variation in the number of syllables, as seen in the first form is because feet can have more than one shape, as noted above. A couple of points are worth noting here. First, these forms illustrate how the prosodic hierarchy works, from the prosodic word to the segments. Each form is a prosodic word (indicated as PWd below), which is exactly one foot. Second, the structure of the foot is different, but both forms are well-formed iambic feet. Recall from (17.c) that iambic feet can be one or two syllables. (22)

PWd

PWd



Ft

Ft



σ 

σ σ



µ µ

µ  µ µ



a ŋ

a ŋ u k

5.2.5 Prosodic repairs In some cases, there are changes or alternations to a base that appear to be motivated by improving the prosodic structure: these are referred to as prosodic repairs. For example, copying a single consonant has been analyzed as providing an onset to create wellformed syllables. There is a well-known preference for syllables to begin with consonants rather than vowels, and so copying a consonant helps to make it a better syllable. The following example from Spokane (Salish) illustrates that the repetitive morpheme /-e-/ can be an infix, if the stem begins with a cluster (23.a). If the stem begins with a single consonant, that consonant is copied to provide an onset for the repetitive /-e-/, as in (23.b). The copied segment is indicated with shading. (23)          

Spokane (Bates & Carlson 1992) a. Infix   /-e-, šl̓-n̓-t-ən̓/ š-e-l̓n̓tén̓

     

rep, chop-ctr-tr-1sgTrS ‘I chopped it up repeatedly’ /-e-, lč̓-n̓-t-ən̓/ l̓-e-č̓n̓tén̓ rep, tie-ctr-tr-1sgTrS ‘I tied it over and over’



         

Prosodic morphology 

b.        

Consonant reduplication /-e-, šl̓/ rep, chop /-e-, nič̓-n̓-t-əxʷ/ rep, cut-ctr-tr-2sgTrS

 121

še-šíl̓ ‘chopped repeatedly’ n̓e-n̓íč̓n̓txʷ ‘you kept cutting’

A second type of prosodic repair is vowel strengthening or ablaut and it has been motivated to avoid having schwas be said with stress or more emphasis when they are reduplicated (Urbanczyk, 2001). The following examples from Lushootseed (Salish) illustrate that the change of schwa to /i/ is motivated by stress, rather than being a segment associated with diminutive CV- reduplication. The stressed syllable is indicated with an accent mark above the vowel. Crucially, the vowel is only changed when stress falls on the reduplicant vowel (which is indicated with shading), as in (24.a). If stress falls on a schwa that is not reduplicated, it stays as schwa. If stress occurs later in the word, as in (24.b), the schwa is retained. (24)            

Lushootseed diminutive reduplication (Bates, Hess & Hilbert, 1994; Urbanczyk, 2006) a. Stressed reduplicant   ǰə́səd ‘foot’ ǰí-ǰəsəd ‘little foot’ z z   təd íl ‘lie in bed’ tí-təd il ‘lie in bed for a little while’ b. Unstressed reduplicant   ƛ̓əládiʔ ‘sound, noise’ ƛ̓ə-ƛ̓əládiʔ ‘little noise’   táləɫ ‘nephew/neice’ tə-táləɫ ‘little nephew/neice’

As one can see, when the stress mark is on the shaded vowel, it is [i], but when the shaded vowel is not stressed, the vowel is [ə]. There are some good reasons proposed or why schwa avoids being stressed, mostly related to it being a neutral vowel.

5.2.6 Summary This section illustrated a range of patterns related to how the affixation of segmentally empty prosodic units can account for reduplication, metathesis, and deletion. This not only illustrates an important insight about prosodic morphology, but it also maintains a concatenative approach to morphology, especially in the face of what might otherwise be seen as evidence against that approach. We have also seen how having well-formed prosodic structure can account for when consonants are copied and also when vowel quality changes with the addition of a new meaningful unit to a base. The hypothesis that segmentally empty prosodic units are affixes, differs from other research, which aims to derive the prosodic shapes from constraints on morpho-prosodic well-formedness (Downing, 2005; Kennedy 2008; McCarthy & Prince 1999; Urbanczyk, 2006). In this view, the prosodic shapes of morphemes are emergent, rather

122 

 Suzanne Urbanczyk

than directly listed in the input to the creation of a word. We turn next to looking at how prosodic structure is relevant in having well-formed bases to which morphological operations may apply.

5.3 Prosodic units as bases Two ways that prosodic units are relevant to morphological bases include the location of infixes and requiring certain prosodic shapes of bases. Each of these will be examined in turn.

5.3.1 Infixing location Yu (2007) defines an infix as an affix that separates a morpheme into two parts that would not otherwise occur independently. This is necessary to distinguish infixing from other types of internal modification, like the ablaut pattern discussed above. Yu’s typological survey of the location of infixes revealed that they are located in noticeable, or salient positions, which he refers to as pivots. There are two classes of pivots: edge pivots and prominence pivots. The use of the term ‘edge’ is meant to refer to the first and last parts of words, and prominence is related to stress. Yu provides the following list of potential pivot locations. The parentheses identify positions which have been observed by others, which Yu did not find unequivocal evidence to support. (25)  

Potential pivot locations (Yu, 2007, p. 67) Edge pivots Prominence pivots

           

First consonant First vowel (First syllable) Last syllable Last vowel (Last consonant)

Stressed foot Stressed syllable Stressed vowel      

An example of an infix found with an edge pivot is the first-person subject marker ‘I’ in Dakota (Siouan): it comes after the first vowel, which also happens to be the first syllable in the words below. (26)        

Dakota 1st person (Yu, 2007: 102, citing Boas & Deloria 1941, Moravscik 1977) ʔi.kto.mi ‘Iktomi’ ʔi-ma-kto.mi ‘I am Iktomi’ na.pcu ‘swallow’ na-wa-pca ‘I swallow it’ la.k’o.ta ‘Lakota’ la-ma-k’o.ta ‘I am a Lakota’ na.wi.zi ‘jealous’ na-wa-wi.zi ‘I am jealous’



Prosodic morphology 

 123

An example of a prominence pivot can be found with infixing reduplication. The language isolate Washo illustrates infixing to the stressed syllable, to indicate plural meaning. A mora is attached and receives its segmental content by copying segments from the base, similarly to Kwak’wala reduplication. Notice that stress (marked with an accent over the stressed vowel) in all the words below is on the second to last syllable, regardless of whether or not there is reduplication. The words in (27.a) illustrate that the segments copied are adjacent, and the words in (27.b) show that a coda consonant can be skipped in order to copy the onset and nucleus of the stressed syllable. (27)  

Washo plural infixing reduplication (Jacobsen, 1974; Winter, 1970)   singular plural gloss

             

a.       b.    

ʔélel gúšuʔ dámal bókoŋ ʔéw.šiʔ nén.t̓uš mók.go

ʔe-lé-lel gu-šú-šuʔ da-má-mal bo-kó-koŋ ʔe.-ší-w.šiʔ ne.-t̓ú-n.tuš-u mo.-gó-k.go

‘mother’s father’ ‘pet’ ‘to hear’ ‘to snore’ ‘father’s brothers’ ‘old women’ (u=nominalizing suffix) ‘shoe’

5.3.2 Stem modifications based on prosodic units In addition to prosodic units determining what can be affixed to a word and where an affix is placed, prosodic units can also serve to require a particular shape and size of the base. The patterns either relate to expansion of the stem to some target size or to a requirement that affixes be attached to a particular type of base (where the base adjusts its syllable structure for a particular affix). In terms of stem expansion, Cupeño (Uto-Aztecan) habilitative meaning is expressed by identifying the stressed foot (indicated in square brackets on the left) and expanding the form, so the stressed syllable is followed by two syllables. The expansion occurs by copying the vowel and inserting a glottal stop [ʔ] onset. The copied portion is indicated by underlining on the right, and the two syllables after the stressed syllable are shaded. (28)              

Cupeño habilitative (Hill, 1970; McCarthy & Prince, 1990; Crowhurst, 1994)   Verb Stem Habilitative   a. [čál] čá-ʔaʔa-l ‘husk’   [tə́w] tə́-ʔəʔə-w ‘see’   hə[lyə́p] həlyə́-ʔəʔə-p ‘hiccup’   kə[láw] kəlá-ʔaʔa-w ‘gather wood’ b. [páčik] páči-ʔi-k ‘leach acorns’   [čáŋnəw] čáŋnə-ʔə-w ‘be angry’

124 

         

 Suzanne Urbanczyk

c.   d.    

[pínəʔwəx] [xáləyəw] čí hú ʔáyu

pínəʔwəx xə́ləyəw číʔ húʔ ʔáyu

‘sing enemy songs’ ‘fall’ ‘gather’ ‘fart’ ‘want’

As one can see, there is no affix per se, but rather the ‘habilitative’ meaning is expressed by the expansion of the base to meet a target size. How many vowels are copied depends on the number of syllables in the base and where stress falls, but the output is the same: the stressed syllable is followed by two unstressed syllables, as indicated by shading. In (28.a) two syllables are added, because the base has final stress. In (28.b), only one syllable is added, because stress is on the second to last syllable in the verb stem. In (28.c), the stressed syllable is already followed by two syllables, so no copying is needed. And interestingly, in (28.d), the verb stems all end in vowels, so a different pattern is used. Languages in which affixes require the base to have a specific shape include Zuni (isolate), Sierra Miwok (Utian), and Yowlumne (Yokuts). In Sierra Miwok, there are several stem shapes which change, depending on the suffix, and the root’s composition of consonants and vowels. The following illustrates four different stem shapes found. These examples show that different bases can have different shapes, depending on the class, changing the length of the vowel or consonant or adding extra material. (29)  

Templatic morphology – Southern Sierra Miwok (Broadbent 1964, p. 38) Stem 1 Stem 2 Stem 3 Stem 4 meaning

     

mus:alo:tkowta-

musahlotkowat-

mus:aʔlot:uʔkow:at-

musʔalotʔukowta-

‘to be ashamed’ ̓to catch’ ‘to bump into’

As you can see above, each class has a set pattern regarding the length of consonants, such as geminating the second consonant in the third class.

5.3.3 Summary This survey has illustrated a range of ways in which prosodic structure is relevant in determining the base of some morphological operation: either by identifying the location of infixes, or by adjusting the shape of the base for a particular affix. Some patterns such as truncation also show how bases can be adjusted to have a specific shape to express a particular meaning, an example of that was given from Yup’ik in (21). When learning new words that use prosodic morphology, it may appear that the processes are not familiar or that there are many different forms to keep track of. However, once one can see that prosodic structure is relevant, one can make equivalences between forms, which could be useful thus provide a guide for how to create new words.



Prosodic morphology 

 125

5.4 Multiple prosodic morphemes and multiple exponence Much of the work in prosodic morphology has focused on how a single morpheme or meaning is realized. A growing body of research has started to explore what happens when more than one prosodic morpheme is added or there are multiple parts to the prosodic morphemes. This section outlines some of the patterns that occur when more than one prosodic morpheme is added to a word.

5.4.1 Double reduplications Zimmermann (2021) outlines a typology of patterns found when there is more than one reduplicative morpheme used to form a new word, referring to these as multiple reduplication. She observes three patterns: all reduplicants surface as expected (faithful multiple reduplication), only a single reduplicant surfaces (avoidance of multiple reduplication), or all reduplicants surface, but one is smaller than expected (subtracting multiple reduplication). She identifies multiple reduplication occurring in Algonquian, Salish and Wakashan languages, as well as languages spoken outside of North America. Examples of faithful multiple reduplication are found in Thompson (Salish) and Fox (Algonquian). Interestingly, the outermost reduplicant appears to copy the adjacent segments, as can be seen in the Fox example below. The base word is presented in (30.a). Notice that in (30.b) the prosodic morpheme only copies the consonant; instead of copying the vowel, a fixed long vowel /a:/ occurs, as indicated with shading. The form in (30.c) illustrates that the second reduplicative morpheme is a bisyllabic foot, faithfully copying the vowels in the adjacent base. In (30.d) we see the pattern with both reduplicative morphemes. (30)        

Faithful multiple reduplication in Fox (Dahlstrom, 1997, 206–207) a. wi:tamaw-e:wa ‘he tells him’ b. wa:-wi:tamaw-e:wa ‘he tells him over and over’ c. wi:ta-wi:tamaw-e:wa ‘he keeps telling him’ d. wa:wi-wa:-wi:tamaw-e:wa ‘he keeps telling him over and over’

When the bisyllabic reduplicant is located before the /C1a:-/ reduplicant in (30.d), it copies the long vowel of the reduplicant, not the root vowel. This shows that the outermost reduplication occurs after the first one is added; otherwise it would be puzzling as to how both reduplicants can have the fixed /a:/ vowel. This discussion of attaching the prosodic morphemes in a specific order will also be relevant below. Other examples of faithful multiple reduplication can be found in Salish languages, such as Thompson (Broselow, 1983). The basic stem is given in (31.a). A CVC- reduplicant indicates ‘distributive’ meaning in (31.b). Diminutive is CV- (31.c), and both are as

126 

 Suzanne Urbanczyk

expected when combined in (31.d): the vowels and consonants are as expected, and the shapes are also as expected. Notice that it looks like the ‘distributive’ skips over the ‘diminutive’ in order to copy the second consonant of the root. (31)        

Faithful multiple reduplication in Thompson (Broselow 1983, Thompson & Thompson 1991) a. sil ‘calico’ b. sil-síl ‘calico, distributive’ c. sí-sil’ ‘calico, diminutive’ d. sil-sí-sil’ ‘calico, distributive-diminutive’

Broselow (1983) notes that this skipping effect is because the ‘diminutive’ is actually an infix. It is attached to the stressed syllable, as can be seen with longer words with non-initial stress, shown in the examples below. (32)    

Thompson infixing reduplication a. qʷintwáxʷ ‘talk to each other’ b. qʷintwáw’xʷ ‘talk to each other, diminutive’

The ‘distributive’ is a true prefix, so must attach to the root. Subtracting multiple reduplication is defined as: “One of the reduplicants is smaller than its form in isolation” (Zimmermann, 2021, p. 542). An example of this pattern can be found in Lushootseed (Salish), with ‘distributive’ reduplication: this morpheme is shaded in the examples below to show how it changes depending on where it occurs in the word. The ‘distributive’ usually has the shape CVC as in (33.b). When it comes after ‘diminutive’ Ci- reduplication in (33.c), it has its usual CVC shape, as illustrated in (33.d). However, when the ‘distributive’ precedes ‘diminutive’, it is reduced in size to CV-, as in (33.e). (33)          

Subtracting multiple reduplication in Lushootseed (Bates, Hess & Hilbert 1994) a. bədaʔ ‘child, offspring’   b. bəd-bədaʔ ‘children’ ‘distributive’ c. bíʔ-bədaʔ ‘young child’ ‘diminutive’ d. bí-bəd-bədaʔ ‘litter (of animals); dolls’ ‘diminutive-distributive’ e. bí-bi-bədaʔ ‘young children’ ‘distributive-diminutive’

The examples below verify that this is in fact reduplication, rather than affixation, as was suggested by a reviewer. The only difference in the patterns is that there is deletion of the base vowel with ‘diminutive’ reduplication. (34)        

Subtracting multiple reduplication in Lushootseed (Bates, Hess & Hilbert 1994) pástəd ‘white person, Caucasian’   pas-pastəd ‘many white folks’ ‘distributive’ pa-pstəd ‘white child, white friend’ ‘diminutive’ pa-pa-pstəd ‘many white children’ ‘distributive-diminutive’



Prosodic morphology 

 127

The reduction in size of the ‘distributive’ to CV- from CVC- has been argued by Urbanczyk (2001) to be due to purely phonological processes: it can be attributed to avoiding doubled consonants or geminates. Lushootseed doesn’t have any geminates, and the ill-formed *bíb-bi-bədaʔ would be independently ruled out because a sequence of two segments like bb is never found in the language (contra Broselow 1983 and Zimmermann 2021). The following section looks at cases in which two reduplicative morphemes are avoided.

5.4.2 Coalescence of prosodic morphemes Turning to avoidance of multiple reduplication, Zimmermann (2021) refers to this as coalescence and discusses examples from Wakashan languages. In Nuu-chah-nulth (Wakashan), when more than one morpheme is present that is associated with reduplication, only one reduplicant arises: the two prosodic units are fused by coalescence. Nuu-chah-nulth, like other Wakashan languages, has a number of stem changes associated with various suffixes. These stem changes include triggering reduplication, lengthening or shortening the vowel in the base or the reduplicant, and also some segmental changes (not indicated here). The changes that the suffix triggers in the base are indicated in the second line below, where R indicates reduplication, and L indicates that the base or reduplicant vowel is lengthened. Multiple reduplications occur, but only in very restricted contexts, as illustrated in (35). Stonham (2007) presents evidence that when multiple reduplication does happen, it is when the reduplicative morphemes are associated with different stages of word formation. As in the example below, the word for ‘sea otter’ is created first, with reduplication being triggered by the suffix meaning ‘look for’. Then, once this is created, the word is fully formed and then it can be pluralized by having CV- reduplication. (35)        

Multiple reduplication in Nuu-chah-nulth (Stonham, 2007) k̓ʷa-k̓ʷa-k̓ʷaƛ̓iiḥ RED-RED-k̓ʷaƛ-iiḥ PL-RED-sea.otter-look.for[R] ‘sea otter hunters’

So, the first reduplication occurs when the stem is created, the second reduplication occurs after this, when the final word is created. However, if two suffixes are added that trigger reduplication at the same level, only one reduplication is found. This can be seen in the word in (36) below. Notice that it has two suffixes that trigger reduplication, but only one reduplicant (underlined) occurs in the word. The reduplicant has a long vowel as resulting from coalescing the two prosodic units associated with the two suffixes ‘at.leg’ and ‘really’.

128 

(36)        

 Suzanne Urbanczyk

Avoidance of multiple reduplication in Nuu-chah-nulth1 (Kim, 2003) ƛ̓uu-ƛ̓uukʷan̓łap RED-ƛuk-aan̓uł[R+L]-apa[RL+L] RED-broad-at.leg-really ‘His legs are really big’

An interesting feature of this avoidance pattern is that when reduplication co-occurs with a suffix it can be omitted if needed, as it is not the sole marker of meaning. Plural reduplication, as in (35), is one of the only reduplication patterns that does not co-occur with a suffix. This raises the question of whether the avoidance of multiple reduplication occurs when reduplication co-occurs with an affix, or whether the effect is related to avoiding multiple reduplications altogether. Some examples from Halkomelem (Salish), discussed in § 5.4.3, seem to suggest that multiple reduplication is avoided when it is the sole indicator of meaning. Another pattern of coalescence can be found in Halkomelem (Salish). The prosodic morphemes for ‘plural’ and ‘imperfective’ are coalesced when they co-occur with some bases. The base form of the verb is provided in (37.a). The imperfective is CV- reduplication in (37.b), with plural being a CəC- prefix, as in (37.c). (37)        

Halkomelem (Salish) coalescence of prosodic morphemes (Hukari, 1978) a. t̓íləm ‘sing’   b. t̓í-t̓ələm̓ ‘singing’ imperfective c. t̓əl-t̓íləm ‘they sing’ plural d. t̓íl-t̓ələm̓ ‘they are singing’ plural-imperfective

When both morphemes are combined, the result is CVC- reduplication with initial stress as in (37.d), indicated with shading. It should be pointed out that the ‘imperfective’ for the most part is associated with initial stress, so this looks like the prosodic unit associated with imperfective coalesces with the syllable associated with plural reduplication to result in CVC- initially stressed word.

5.4.3 Avoiding reduplication with allomorph selection A fourth pattern of how meanings are expressed with multiple prosodic morphemes has been identified (Mellesmoen and Urbanczyk, 2021). Halkomelem (Salish) illustrates a pattern in which reduplication is avoided if another allomorph can be chosen to express the meaning. The ‘imperfective’ has a range of allomorphs, including reduplication

1 An anonymous reviewer has asked for examples of intermediate words. A search of available sources including Rose (1980), the source for many of Stonham’s (2007) examples, has not revealed any words that would be relevant, where they have the same root or base and the relevant suffixes to trigger reduplication.



Prosodic morphology 

 129

(38a), metathesis (38b), h- prefixation (38c), ablaut (38d) and glottal stop insertion/infixing (38e; Hukari, 1978), as illustrated below. The relevant portion of the word that shows the allomorph is indicated with shading. (38)                    

Halkomelem imperfective allomorphy (Hukari & Peter, 1995) a. reduplication   t̓íləm ‘sing’ t̓i-t̓ələm̓ ‘singing’ b. metathesis   pqʷát ‘break it’ páqʷt ‘breaking it’ c. h- prefixation   mə́q̓ət ‘swallow it’ hə́m̓q̓ət ‘swallowing it’ d. ablaut   č̓ə́kʷx̣ ‘fry’ č̓ékʷx̣ ‘frying’ e. infix   hésəm ‘sneeze’ héʔsəm ‘sneezing’

Likewise, the ‘plural’ has several different allomorphs. Two of these allomorphs involve reduplication, and one does not. In (39a), the plural is C1əC2- reduplication and in (39b) it is a -C1i- infix. As can be seen in (39c), plural meaning is expressed with a /-l̓-/ infix. (39)                  

Halkomelem plural allomorphy (Hukari, 1978) a. C1əC2- reduplication   lémət ‘look at him/her/it’ ləm-lémət   t̓íləm ‘sing’ t̓əl-t̓íləm b. -C1i- infixing reduplication   kʷəmləxʷ ‘root’ kʷə-kʷí-mləxʷ   šə́yəɫ ‘older sibling’ šə-ší-yəɫ c. -l- infix   técəl ‘arrive, reach’ tél̓əcəl   méčəs ‘match’ mél̓əčəs

‘look at them’ ‘they sing’ ‘roots’ ‘older siblings’ ‘they arrive’ ‘matches’

When these prosodic morphemes are combined with ‘diminutive’ reduplication on verbs (which is always reduplicative), the range of allomorphs found with the plural is reduced to just the /-l̓-/ infix, avoiding the reduplicative forms. The plural is indicated with shading to illustrate where it occurs in the word (the location of the epenthetic schwa varies). (40)      

Halkomelem multiple morphemes (Hukari, 1978)   diminutive-imperfective diminutive-imperfective-plural a. ɬiʔɬəɬə́nəm̓ ‘weaving’ ɬəliʔɬəɬə́nəm̓ ‘weaving’ b. piʔpaqʷt ‘breaking it’ pəliʔpaqʷt ‘breaking them’

While allomorphy is related to choosing one of several forms to express a meaning, there are also patterns in with several forms are required to express a single meaning. We turn to this next.

130 

 Suzanne Urbanczyk

5.4.4 Multiple exponence: Affix-triggered reduplication Multiple exponence refers to when a single meaning is expressed by multiple realizations within a word (Harris, 2017, p. 9). There is very little research on multiple exponence and prosodic morphology, so the following discussion represents a preliminary overview, which should be subject to verification by a balanced typological survey. In some languages, reduplication co-occurs with the addition of a segmentally specified affix. This affix-triggered reduplication can be found in the Wakashan languages, as illustrated in the Nuu-chah-nulth examples in (35 – 36) above. For example, in (35), the suffix meaning ‘look for’ triggers copying the first consonant and vowel of the base. The striking feature about affix-triggered reduplication is that it seems to be found only with languages which have independently motivated reduplication as well as a very large inventory of affixes. For example, in Nuu-chah-nulth, reduplication on its own indicates plurality, but there are approximately 80 suffixes which trigger reduplication (as well as other alternations to the base). Nuu-chah-nulth has over 400 affixes, and approximately 20 % of the suffixes are also associated with reduplication. A preliminary investigation of this pattern found that most of the suffixes that trigger reduplication have homophonous or near homophonous counterparts, so reduplication may serve to disambiguate the suffixes from each other (Lee and Urbanczyk, 2006). The following is a partial list of some of the homophonous affixes in Nuu-chah-nulth, with the kind of change indicated after the meaning. (41)  

Nuu-chah-nulth homophonous affixes (Sapir and Swadesh, 1939; Lee and Urbanczyk, 2006)   form meaning modification to base

             

a.   b.   c.    

-aɬc̓a -aɬc̓a -im -im -paɬ -paɬ -paɬ

‘at fault’ ‘at an upright surface’ ‘through an aperture’ ‘… thing’ ‘on each side’ ‘in the same group with’ ‘season of’

reduplication, vowel lengthening no change reduplication, vowel lengthening no change reduplication vowel lengthening no change

While this pattern is interesting, much further work is needed to document the specific details of the words as well as to verify if the observation holds that languages with affix-triggered reduplication also have homophonous affixes, and this is not an accidental correlation. To summarize, this section has reviewed some examples of multiple prosodic morphemes. A general typology of multiple reduplication as found by Zimmerman (2021) was presented, along with other evidence that reduplication is avoided when possible.



Prosodic morphology 

 131

5.5 Relevance to language revitalization A key issue that arises in the context of language revitalization, is to identify the best way to teach and learn these patterns. Some of the patterns seem quite complex when first looking at them, and they often reflect important and common meanings that are acquired or learned early, like plurality or that an action is on-going. For example, the Halkomelem ‘imperfective’ forms in (38) express a very common meaning in the language. Viewing the range of changes as adding a syllable or a mora helps to see what gets added to the word, when it is not simply a suffix with consonants and vowels in it, like English -ing. There is very little published research on effective ways to teach and learn new words that have prosodic morphemes, even for languages outside North America that have reduplication and which have many speakers and learners (for example Chinese). Some recent work on this is outlined in the hopes of inspiring others to share their approaches more widely. Tohono O’odham (Uto-Aztecan) makes use of several patterns of reduplication to express plurality. Beer, Cruz, Hirrel and Kerfoot (2014) describe several patterns found that depend on the syllable shape, in particular the number of moras in the base, vs. the reduplication pattern. They outline a path to follow to teach the pattern, where students first learn to identify consonants and vowels and what counts as moraic in Tohono O’odham, without using formal linguistic terms. They then provide a series of exercises to help learners identify the patterns, asking learners to predict the correct form. This provides a way for learners to be able to find the patterns and apply them to new forms. Related to learning patterns in this way, I recently had the opportunity to co-develop some fun ways to present Halkomelem imperfective patterns with a cohort of language learners. One example uses flash cards for Halkomelem, in which the base form is written on one side of a card, while the ‘imperfective’ form is written on the other side (Claxton, Urbanczyk, & Hul’q’umi’num’ Language Academy, 2019). Once we did this and developed a game, learners expressed that it was easier to internalize the pattern by playing a game, trying to match the form with the meanings, rather than just going through all the rules. Other ideas one can use are to make schemas of word patterns, using symbols like C and V, and filling the blanks with the correct letter, to create new words. It also might be helpful to link some of the patterns of repetition in reduplication with other culturally significant patterns of repetition, such as with knitting or weaving (Urbanczyk, 2021). This is an important and understudied area of language revitalization, and it is hoped that by presenting the patterns with representations of the prosodic morphemes, people will start to share their ideas and successes with teaching and learning these patterns.

132 

 Suzanne Urbanczyk

5.6 Conclusions The proposal that morphemes can be prosodic units has been a key development in understanding a range of word formation processes which are not strictly affixational. Some of the patterns found can be described as affixing prosodic units such as moras, syllables, or feet. Doing so allows one to see that there is just one affix, but it is pronounced differently, depending on what the sounds of the base are. Prosodic structure and patterns are also relevant to identifying bases for some affixes. Multiple reduplication patterns can be found in several language families (Algonquian, Salish, Wakashan), and some general patterns regarding how these are realized was discussed as well. A few final thoughts about how these patterns may be taught was also shared.

References Ahenekew, Freda & Hans Christoph Wolfart. 1983. Productive reduplication in Plains Cree. In William Cowan (ed.), Proceedings of the Algonquian Conference, 369 – 378. Ottawa, ON: Carleton University Press. Bates, Dawn & Barry F. Carlson. 1992. Simple syllables in Spokane Salish. Linguistic Inquiry 23. 653–659. Bates, Dawn, Thomas. M. Hess & Vi Hilbert. 1994. Lushootseed dictionary. Seattle: University of Washington Press. Beers, Keiko, Robert Cruz, Laura Hirrel & Iphigenia Kerfoot. 2014. Describing reduplication patterns in Tohono O’odham with language learners in mind. Proceedings of the High Desert Linguistics Society Conference, 43–55. University of New Mexico. Boas, Franz & Ella C. Deloria. 1941. Dakota grammar. (Memoirs of the National Academy of Sciences 23. part 2). Washington, DC: US Government Publishing Office. Broadbent, Sylvia M. 1964. The Southern Sierra Miwok language. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Broselow, Ellen. 1983. Subjacency in morphology: Salish double reduplication. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 1. 317 – 346. Claxton, Rae Anne, Suzanne Urbanczyk & Hul’q’umi’num’ Language Academy. 2019. Hul’q’umi’num’ verb classes: Phonological word schemas. Presentation at 54th International Conference on Salish and Neighbouring Languages, Victoria, BC. Crowhurst, Megan. 1994. Foot extrametricality and template mapping in Cupeño. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 12. 177–201. Dahlstrom, Amy. 1997. Fox (Mesquakie) reduplication. International Journal of American Linguistics 63(2). 205 – 226. Downing, Laura. 2006. Canonical forms in prosodic morphology. Oxford UK: Oxford University Press. Fitzgerald, Colleen. 2016. Morphology in the Muskogean languages. Language and Linguistic Compass 10. 681 – 700. Garrett, Andrew. 2001. Reduplication and infixation in Yurok: morphology, semantics and diachrony. International Journal of American Linguistics 67. 264–312. Hardy, Heather K. & Timothy R. Montler. 1988. Imperfective gemination in Alabama. International Journal of American Linguistics 54. 399 – 415. Harris, Alice. 2017. Multiple exponence. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Haugen, Jason. 2014. Hiaki (Yaqui) reduplication redux: morphological and prosodic targets in syllabic reduplication. International Journal of American Linguistics 80(4). 507 – 532. Hayes, Bruce. 1995. Metrical stress theory: principles and case studies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.



Prosodic morphology 

 133

Hill, Jane H. 1970. A peeking rule in Cupeño. Linguistic Inquiry 1. 534–539. Hukari, Thomas. 1978. Halkomelem nonsegmental morphology. Proceedings of the 13th International Conference on Salish and Neighbouring Languages, 157 – 209. https://lingpapers.sites.olt.ubc.ca/ icsnl-volumes/. Hukari, Thomas & Ruby Peter. 1995. The Cowichan dictionary of the Hul’q’umi’num’ dialect of the Coast Salish people. Duncan BC: Cowichan Tribes. Hyman, Larry. 1985. A theory of phonological weight. Dordrecht: Foris Publisher. Kennedy, Robert. 2008. Evidence for morphoprosodic alignment in reduplication. Linguistic Inquiry 39. 589–614. Kim, Eun-Sook. 2003. Theoretical issues in Nuu-chah-nulth phonology and morphology. Ph.D. thesis, University of British Columbia. Kimball, Geoffrey D. 1991. Koasati grammar. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press. Lee, Sunghwa & Suzanne Urbanczyk. 2006. Reduplication and vowel lengthening/shortening in Nuu-chah-nulth. Presentation at 2nd International Conference on Wakashan Languages, Victoria, BC: University of Victoria. Marantz, Alec. 1982. Re reduplication. Linguistic Inquiry 13. 483–545. McCarthy, John & Alan Prince. 1986. Prosodic morphology. Manuscript. University of Massachusetts, and Brandeis University. McCarthy, John & Alan Prince. 1990. Foot and word in prosodic morphology: The Arabic broken plural. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 8. 209 – 283. McCarthy, John & Alan Prince. 1999. Faithfulness and identity in prosodic morphology. In R. Kager, H. van der Hulst & W. Zonneveld (eds.), The prosody morphology interface, 218–309. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Mellesmoen, Gloria & Suzanne Urbanczyk. 2021. Avoiding multiple reduplication without Integrity. In D. K. E. Reisinger & M. Huijsmans (eds.), Proceedings of 37th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics, 169–178. Boston MA: Cascadilla Press. Montler, Timothy R. 1986. Morphology and phonology of Saanich. Missoula, MT: University of Montana Press. Moravscik, Edith. 1977. On rules of infixing. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Linguistics Club. Saba Kirchner, Jesse. 2010. Minimal reduplication. Ph.D. thesis, UC Santa Cruz. Saba Kirchner, Jesse. 2013. Minimal reduplication and reduplicative exponence. Morphology 23. 227–243. Sapir, Edward & Morris Swadesh. 1939. Nootka texts: tales and ethnological narratives, with grammatical notes and lexical materials. Philadelphia, PA: Linguistic Society of America, University of Pennsylvania. Selkirk, Elisabeth O. 1980. The role of prosodic categories in English word stress. Linguistic Inquiry 11. 563 – 605. Stonham, John. 1994. Combinatorial morphology. (Current Issues in Linguistic Theory). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Stonham, John. 2007. Nuu-chah-nulth double reduplication and Stratal Optimality Theory. Canadian Journal of Linguistics 52. 105 – 130. Thompson, Lawrence. C. & M. Terry Thompson. 1991. The Thompson language. Missoula, MT: University of Montana Occasional Papers in Linguistics. Urbanczyk, Suzanne. 2001. Patterns of reduplication in Lushootseed. New York, NY: Garland. Urbanczyk, Suzanne. 2006. Reduplicative form and the root-affix asymmetry. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 24. 179–240. Urbanczyk, Suzanne. 2021. A note on how weaving and knitting can enhance learning Salish reduplication patterns. In M. Desmarais & J. Wu (eds.), Working Papers of the Linguistics Circle of the University of Victoria, Vol. 31(1), 148–156. Victoria, BC: University of Victoria. Woodbury, Anthony. 1985. Meaningful phonological processes: A consideration of Central Alaskan Yupik Eskimo prosody. Unpublished manuscript. University of Texas, Austin. Yu, Alan. 2007. A natural history of infixation. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

134 

 Suzanne Urbanczyk

Zimmermann, Eva. 2017. Morphological length and prosodically defective morphemes. Oxford UK: Oxford University Press. Zimmermann, Eva. 2021. Two is too much…in the phonology!: A phonological account of unfaithful multiple reduplication” The Linguistic Review 38(3). 537–572. https://doi.org/10.1515/tlr-2021-2075

Matthew K. Gordon

6 Word prosody Abstract: Many phonological properties operate over the domain of the word. For example, stress is typically bounded by the word, as are other phenomena sensitive to word edges, including tone, prosodic minimality conditions, harmony processes, and segmental alternations. North American Indian languages have provided a fertile ground for studying word level prosody due to their characteristic high degree of morphological synthesis, which gives rise to long words that supply a rich backdrop for word-bounded phonological properties. This chapter examines phonetic and phonological aspects of word prosody in North American Indian languages from both a synchronic and diachronic perspective, with a particular focus on stress and metrical structure, areas of research in which North American languages have greatly informed typological knowledge.

6.1 Introduction Word prosody refers to features of a language’s sound system that are sensitive to the domain of the word. For example, stress is typically bounded by the word, as are other phenomena sensitive to word edges, including tone, word minimality conditions, harmony processes, and segmental alternations. North American Indigenous languages have provided a fertile ground for studying word level prosody due to their characteristic high degree of morphological synthesis, which gives rise to long words that supply a rich backdrop for word-bounded phonological properties. Word prosody is also an important area of study from the point of view of language acquisition since accurate production of prosodic features is crucial for intelligibility and comprehensibility. This chapter examines phonetic and phonological aspects of word prosody in North American Indian language from both a synchronic and diachronic perspective, with a particular focus on stress and metrical structure, areas of research in which North American languages have greatly informed typological knowledge.

6.2 Prosodic structure of the word A prosodic word is composed of one or more syllables, which in turn consist typically of a vowel nucleus (syllable peak) and one or more consonants on either side of the nucleus (see Fitzgerald & Gordon this volume, on syllable structure). A language may also provide evidence for a unit smaller than the word comprised of a group of two adjacent syllables. This unit, the foot, is diagnosed primarily through stress, which is https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-006 https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-000

136 

 Matthew K. Gordon

increased prominence associated with certain syllables in a word. For example, in the English noun INsight, the first syllable is stressed whereas the verb inCITE minimally differs in its pronunciation in having stress on the second syllable. Longer words often have a secondary stress that is less prominent than the primary stress but stronger than other syllables in a word. For example, the word HEliCOPter has two stresses, a primary stress on the first syllable and a secondary stress rhythmically placed two syllables away on the third syllable. Speakers of some languages, including English, have strong intuitions about stress that are often accessible through the tapping test in which the speaker taps once while saying a word. Typically, a single tap will align with the stressed syllable of a word and a second tap, if introduced, will fall on a secondary stressed syllable. In other languages, however, speakers’ intuitions about stress may be less salient and other phonetic diagnostics of stress may be necessary to consider. It is common to characterize rhythmic stress patterns in terms of disyllabic feet, where the foot corresponds to the groupings commonly used to describe many poetic traditions, e.  g., iambic pentameter in Shakespearean verse and dactylic hexameter in Greek and Latin poetry. For example, the word HEliCOPter can be divided into two feet indicated by parentheses, i.  e., (HEli)(COPter). In this word, the two feet are termed trochaic feet since they consist of a stressed syllable followed by an unstressed syllable. Feet are discussed further in section 6. A schematic diagram of the prosodic structure of the word (HEli)(COPter) appears in Figure 1 (see Urbanczyk 2023 [this volume] for more on the prosodic structure of the word). Word Ft

Ft

Syll

Syll

Syll

Syll

HE

li

COP

ter

Fig. 1: The prosodic structure of the word ‘helicopter’

6.3 Phonetic correlates of stress There are many physical correlates of stress. In some languages, consonants and vowels have noticeably different realizations depending on stress. Stressed syllables are typically associated with stronger or hyperarticulated sounds and unstressed syllables are characterized by weaker or hypoarticulated sounds. For example, in English, voiceless stops (sounds produced without vibration of the vocal folds) are aspirated (i.  e., produced with a puff of air after release) before stressed vowels (though not after /s/) and most unstressed vowels centralize to schwa. Thus, in the word apPENdix, the /p/ (in



Word prosody 

 137

bold) is aspirated in the onset of the stressed second syllable but the same /p/ is not aspirated in APpenDECtomy because it occurs before an unstressed vowel. The vowel in the second syllable also alternates in quality between the two words as a function of stress; it is realized with a more front quality [ɛ] when stressed in appendix but as a central, schwa-like, vowel [ə] when unstressed in appendectomy. Stress is typically realized phonetically through one or more acoustic and articulatory properties, including greater duration, increased intensity (loudness), higher fundamental frequency (pitch), and greater gestural displacement and/or velocity (see Bird, Nolan, and Claxton, this volume, and Bliss, Bird, and Gick, this volume, for more on acoustic and articulatory features, respectively, of North American Indian languages).

Pitch (Hz)

250

100



0

1.212

Time (s)

ˈs a

p



b

u

ˈb

u

Fig. 2: Spectrograms (frequency range from 0–8kHz), pitch track (in blue) and intensity trace (in red) for the Lakota words /ˈsapa/ ‘to know,’ with initial stress, and /buˈbu/ ‘husky,’ with final stress1

The acoustic exponents of stress can be visualized using displays like the one in Figure 2, which shows a spectrogram, pitch track (in blue), and intensity trace (in red) for the pair of Lakota (Siouan; North and South Dakota) words /ˈsapa/ ‘to know’ with initial stress

1 Figure is based on the following recording from the UCLA Phonetics Archive: http://archive.phonetics. ucla.edu/Language/LKT/lkt_word-list_1983_01.wav

138 

 Matthew K. Gordon

and /buˈbu/ ‘husky’ with stress on the second syllable. A spectrogram provides information about the distribution of energy across different frequencies, where darker areas indicate greater energy. Time is on the x-axis and frequency on the y-axis in a spectrogram. The pitch track (in blue) is useful for looking at the fundamental frequency of the voice, the property which, in English, distinguishes the end of yes/no questions (ending in a pitch rise) and statements (ending in a pitch fall). Finally, the intensity trace (in red) shows the volume (or loudness) of the voice. The mark above and to the left of the stressed syllable indicates stress in the International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA). In the word with initial stress on the left, the vowel in the first syllable has higher pitch (acoustically equivalent to fundamental frequency) and greater intensity than the second vowel. The second vowel is also realized with creaky phonation (indicated with a subscripted tilde), evident from the increased distance between the vertical lines in the spectrogram. Creaky phonation is commonly associated with unstressed syllables, particularly in final position. The second word, on the other hand, has higher pitch and greater intensity on the second vowel, which is also longer and lacks creakiness. Acoustic correlates of stress vary from language to language (see Gordon and Roettger 2017 for a survey) and may not all converge on stressed syllables. For example, stressed vowels in Witsuwit’en (Na Dene; British Columbia) are longer, have greater intensity and higher fundamental frequency (pitch) than their unstressed counterparts (Hargus 2005), whereas duration and intensity but not fundamental frequency are used to signal stress in Witsuwit’en’s close linguistic and geographic relative Sekani (Hargus 2005).

6.4 Typology of stress systems in North America An interesting and productive area of prosodic research involves the typology of stress systems. The investigation of stress on a broad cross-linguistic basis sheds light on the diversity of stress systems along multiple dimensions: its function (e.  g., to distinguish words with different meanings or to mark the edges of words to facilitate the comprehension of connected speech), its location (e.  g., near the left or right edge of a word), its relationship to other prosodic features (e.  g., tone), and its rhythmic properties. This section examines how a relatively small number of parameters can be used to describe the superficially vast array of stress systems found throughout the world, including in North America.

6.4.1 Phonemic and morphological stress Languages differ not only in how stress is realized but also in the principles governing the location of stress within a word. A fundamental division can be drawn between lan-



Word prosody 

 139

guages in which stress is phonemic and used to distinguish words or morphemes with different meanings and those in which stress is predictable based on prosodic properties. Phonemic stress is observed in some North American languages, e.  g., Yuchi (isolate; Oklahoma), in which the words ˈʃaja ‘squirrel’ and ʃaˈja ‘weeds’ (Wagner 1933: 309) differ only in their stress patterns. The location of stress for each word in this pair must be learned along with the sounds. Words that may differ not only in terms of stress but also in terms of sounds can also diagnose phonemic stress. For example, the Lakota pair of words in figure 2 both contain the same string of consonant-vowel-consonant-vowel (CVCV) but differ in stress position. The parallel word structure but divergent stress pattern suggests that stress is phonemic and must be learned for each Lakota word. In Lakota, there are thus many pairs of words with the same syllable structures but different stress positions (1). (1)    

Lakota stress distinctions (Rood and Taylor 1996) a. Initial stress:ˈmaza ‘iron’, ˈmila ‘knife’, ˈtʰoka ‘enemy’ b. Final stress: paˈha ‘hill, mountain’, tʰeˈzi ‘belly’, hoˈhu ‘bone’

Lakota also exemplifies another function of stress: to signal a grammatical contrast, as in the distinction between the non-active verb xopˈxopa ‘to be good-looking’ and its active counterpart ˈxopxopa ‘to pose, try to appear one’s best’ (Boas and Deloria 1941: 38). In many languages, stress is sensitive to morphological structure. In some languages, particular morphemes may either attract stress or reject stress, whereas in others, the distinction between root morphemes and affixes (prefixes, suffixes) is important in predicting stress. For example, many Interior Salish languages display intricate relationships between morphology and stress (see Czaykowska-Higgins 1998 for an overview of stress in Salish languages). Nxa’amxcin (Salish; Washington) exemplifies the ways morphology can influence stress (Czaykowksa-Higgins 1993). Prefixes in Nxa’amxcin thus reject stress while roots preferentially attract stress over certain suffixes (recessive suffixes). However, certain suffixes (dominant suffixes) pull stress off the root. Certain suffixes and roots also have the ability to force stress rightward off of an immediately following suffix.

6.4.2 Prosodically predictable stress In many languages, stress is neither a property of individual words (as in Lakota) nor morphemes (as in Nxa’amxcin) but rather is predictable from the prosodic structure of a word. In these languages with predictable stress, there are two fundamental dimensions to which a language’s stress system may be sensitive: the distance of a syllable from a word edge and the role of the internal structure, or weight, of a syllable in positioning stress.

140 

 Matthew K. Gordon

6.4.2.1 Fixed stress Considering the first of these dimensions, syllable location, many languages position stress a fixed and predictable distance from a word edge. For example, stress in Mohawk (Iroquoian; Ontario, Quebec, New York) falls on the penultimate (second-to-last) syllable of most words, as shown in (2) (Chafe 1977; Michelson 1988; Mithun in press). (2)      

Penultimate stress in Mohawk (Mithun in press) (syllable boundaries marked by.) a. ak.ˈhsot.ha ‘my grandmother’ b. a.kon.ˈhahw.ha ‘my son-in-law’ c. wa.ke.niaʔ.ˈtat.hens ‘I am thirsty.’

To take an example of stress oriented toward the left edge of a word, stress is fixed on the initial syllable in Southeastern Pomo, as shown in (3) (Pomoan; California) (Moshinsky 1974; Buckley 2013). (3)      

Initial stress in Southeastern Pomo (Buckley 2013) a. ˈxela ‘friend’ b. ˈxelataj ‘friends’ c. ˈhaliqmattat ‘(two) discuss, plan’

6.4.2.2 Weight-sensitive stress On the other hand, there are many languages in which stress is not fixed on the same syllable in all words but is sensitive to syllable structure, or syllable weight. Aspects of syllable structure that can play a role in predicting the location of stress include whether the syllable contains a long vowel or not and whether it ends in a consonant or not. Syllables ending in a consonant (a coda consonant), are considered closed syllables (CVC), while those ending in a vowel are open syllables. Most languages in which syllable weight impacts stress fall into one of two types: either only syllables containing a long vowel (CVV) are stress- attracting (i.  e., heavy) or both syllables containing a long vowel or a coda consonant are heavy. Aleut (Eskimo-Aleut; Alaska, Russia) is an example of the former type of stress system in which syllables with long vowels are heavy. Stress may fall on either the penultimate or the final syllable of a word depending on the structure of the final two syllables (Taff et al. 2001). In most cases, stress is on the penult (4a). However, if the final syllable contains a long vowel and the penult does not, the final syllable, the heavier one, attracts stress (4b). (4)    

Syllable weight in Aleut (Taff et al. 2001: 91–92) (ː indicates that the vowel is long) a. Penult stress: ˈsi.ʧiŋ ‘four’, a.ˈmax.six ‘spend the night’, a.ˈðaː.ðaː ‘their father’ b. Final stress: si.ˈʧiːŋ ‘nine’



Word prosody 

 141

Hopi (Uto-Aztecan; Arizona) (Jeanne 1982) also has a weight-sensitive stress system, but unlike Aleut it treats not only CVV but also CVC as heavy (5). Stress falls on either the first or the second syllable: the first syllable if it is closed by a consonant or contains a long vowel (5a), otherwise the second syllable (5b). (5)    

Syllable weight in Hopi (Jeanne 1982: 253–54) a. Initial stress: ˈnap.na ‘shirt’, ˈnaː.tɨ.ho.ta ‘to hurt oneself’. b. Second syllable stress: ko.ˈjo.ŋo ‘turkey’, qø.ˈtø.som.pi ‘headband’

The form ˈnap.na ‘shirt’ demonstrates that CVC is heavy in Hopi since it attracts stress in initial position. This may be compared with the Aleut form in (4) ˈsi.ʧiŋ ‘four’, in which the final CVC fails to attract stress. Another property of syllables that can also influence stress is vowel quality. Most commonly, this sensitivity to vowel quality manifests itself as a distinction between schwa and peripheral vowels (vowels other than schwa), where schwa is more resistant to stress. For example, in Lushootseed (Salishan; Washington), stress falls on the leftmost peripheral vowel in a word (6a) and on the first syllable in words consisting only of schwa (6b) (Bianco 1995). Note that morphology also plays a role in Lushootseed stress, as in many other Salish languages (see section 5.1). (6)    

Syllable weight in Lushootseed (Bianco 1995: 128) a. jə.ˈla.ʧiʔ ‘both hands’, ˈda.da.tut ‘morning’ b. ˈp’ə.ʧ’əb ‘bobcat’, ˈχə.gʷəd ‘blackberry’

As the form jə.ˈla.ʧiʔ shows, the attraction of stress to the left edge of the word is superseded by a stronger prohibition against stress on schwa. Only in words where there is no peripheral vowel does schwa attract stress, as in ˈp’ə.ʧ’əb. Weight distinctions are often encoded by linguists as differences in the number of timing units, or moras, associated with syllables (Hayes 1989). Syllables that are heavy (i.  e., stress-attracting) have more moras than those that are light. Most short vowels have one mora, while long vowels have two moras, i.  e., CVµVµ. Languages differ in whether they assign a mora to a syllable-final (coda) consonant or not. In Aleut, which treats only long vowels as heavy, coda consonants do not receive a mora, whereas in Hopi, which treats syllables with either a long vowel or a coda consonant as heavy, coda consonants carry a mora, e.  g., Aleut CVµC vs. Hopi CVµCµ. Heavy syllables under this approach are those with two moras, whereas light syllables have fewer than two. Moraic theory can be extended to account for stress in languages like Lushootseed if one assumes that schwa is different from other vowels in not having any mora associated with it. Heavy syllables thus have at least one mora in Lushootseed whereas light syllables have no moras (under the assumption that coda consonants are also non-moraic in Lushootseed). A key feature of weight-sensitive stress is that it is virtually never sensitive to the structure of the syllable onset, i.  e., onset consonants do not have a mora. A syllable is thus equally heavy regardless of whether it has two or more onset consonants.

142 

 Matthew K. Gordon

6.4.3 The role of final consonants in stress systems An interesting twist on weight-sensitive stress is observed in languages in which syllables require extra weight to count as heavy in final position. For example, in Stoney Dakota (Siouan; Alberta), the final syllable is stressed only if it is closed by two consonants; otherwise, the penultimate syllable is stressed even if the final syllable is closed by a single consonant (Shaw 1985). Stress is thus final in a.ˌki.da.ˈbinʧ ‘they looked at it’, but falls on the penult in a.ˌki.da.ˈbik.taʧ (Shaw 1985: 189). Note that secondary stress (marked with ˌ) falls predictably on the second syllable, an inheritance from the dominant pattern of primary stress found in closely related Lakota (see section 5.1). The requirement that the final syllable have two coda consonants to attract stress is typically treated as an instance of extrametricality (Hayes 1989), the application of a more stringent type of weight criterion at the periphery of a word. Extrametricality is largely limited to final syllables, though it has been claimed to also apply at the left edge of the word in Kashaya Pomo (Pomoan; California) (Buckley 1994).

6.4.4 Tone-driven stress A rarer type of property that predicts stress is tone in certain languages that possess both lexical tone (see Uchihara, this volume, on tone) and stress. Tone corresponds to pitch but is used to describe languages in which words are distinguished through pitch differences. Cross-linguistically, in languages with tone-driven stress, high tones and tones involving a high starting or ending point (i.  e., falling or rising tones) preferentially attract stress over lower tones (De Lacy 2004), a pattern that also holds in North American languages, e.  g., in Xaad Kil (isolate; Alaska, British Columbia) (Enrico 1991) and Fort Ware Sekani (Na-Dene; British Columbia) (Hargus 2005). For example, in Fort Ware Sekani, the low-toned final syllable rejects stress in ˈxə́nə ̀s ‘raft’, whereas the hightoned final syllable is stressed in tɬ’ə́ˈnə́s ‘snake’ (Hargus 2005: 412).

6.4.5 Hybrid stress systems Stress systems in many languages do not fit neatly into a single category but rather reflect a hybrid system employing elements of multiple types. For example, even Lakota, which was introduced earlier as an example of a language with phonemic and morphological stress, has stress on the second syllable in the majority of words, vestiges of which are also observed in the secondary stress patterns of related Dakota (see section 6.4.3). Similarly, Chimariko (isolate; California) (Jany 2009) positions stress on the penultimate syllable of the root but also stresses long vowels not in the penult, a system that reflects a combination of fixed stress, morphological stress, and weight-sensitive stress. Fort Ware Sekani (Hargus 2005) is sensitive to a complex array of factors, including



Word prosody 

 143

morphology (stems attract stress over affixes), vowel quality (more peripheral vowels attract stress over schwa), tone (high tone attracts stress over low tone), and position (stress is pulled to the left if other factors are controlled for).

6.4.6 Rhythmic stress Another dimension along which stress systems differ concerns the number of stresses per word. In most of the languages discussed thus far, there is a single stress per word. In many languages, however, words, especially when they have many syllables, may contain multiple stresses occurring at rhythmic intervals. For example, the seven syllable Central Alaskan Yupik (Eskimo-Aleut, Alaska) word qayangecigsugnarquq, phonetically [qa.ˈjaː.ŋə.ˈʧiq.suʁ.ˈnaχ.quq], ‘he will probably get a kayak’ (Miyaoka 1985: 61) displays a rhythmic prominence pattern involving stress on alternating even-numbered syllables. Rhythmic stress systems can be classified according to two parameters: the edge of the word at which the rhythmic stress pattern originates and whether the first syllable at that edge is stressed or not. Combining these two dimensions yields four basic types of stress patterns: left-to-right rhythm starting with a stressed syllable, left-to-right rhythm starting with an unstressed syllable, right-to-left rhythm commencing with a stressed syllable, and right-to-left rhythm initiating with an unstressed syllable. All four of these patterns are attested in North America and are distinguished by considering a range of longer words with an odd and even number of syllables. Osage (Siouan; Oklahoma) instantiates the second system (left-to-right beginning with an unstressed syllable) (7a) (Altshuler 2009). Kutenai/Ktunaxa (Isolate; Montana, Idaho, British Columbia) reflects the mirror image of the Osage pattern (7b) (Garvin 1948). Tübatulabal (Uto-Aztecan; California) is similar to Kutenai in parsing the word from right-to-left but differs in starting with a stressed syllable (7c) (Voegelin 1935). Finally, Cahuilla (Uto-Aztecan; California) employs a left-to-right parse beginning with a stressed syllable (7d) (Seiler 1965). Note that the IPA symbol ˌ represents a secondary, or less prominent, stress in a word. (7)        

Rhythmic stress patterns a. Osage: xoː.ˈʦo.ðiːb.ˌrɑ̃ ‘smoke cedar’, ɑ̃ː.ˈwɑ̃.lɑː.ˌxy.ɣe ‘I crunch up my own (e.  g., prey) with teeth’ b. Kutenai/Ktunaxa: ˌkqa.qa.ˈnaɬk.qaːʦ ‘automobile’, ˌhu.qaɬ.ˌwiyn.ʔoːɬʔ.ˈxup.xa ‘I want to know’ c. Tübatulabal: pɨ.ˌtɨt.pɨ.ˌtɨː.di.ˈnat ‘he is turning it over repeatedly’, ˌɨm.bɨŋ.ˌwi. ba.ˈʔat ‘he is wanting to roll string on his thigh’ d. Cahuilla: ˈtax.mu.ˌʔat ‘song’, ˈtax.mu.ˌʔaʔ.tih ‘song (objective case)’

As the examples above illustrate, the primary or main stressed syllable is almost always the stressed syllable closest to the edge at which the rhythmic pattern originates. Thus, in left-to-right systems, the leftmost stress is the primary one, whereas in right-to-left systems, the rightmost stress is the main one.

144 

 Matthew K. Gordon

6.5 Foot structure The foot was introduced in section 6.2 in the context of the English word (HEli)(COPter), which consists of a trochaic foot, meaning the stressed (or head) syllable precedes the unstressed syllable. There is another common type of foot that has the opposite structure: the iamb, which consists of an unstressed syllable followed by a stressed syllable. Left-to-right stress systems beginning with an unstressed syllable, as in Osage, are thus iambic, whereas those beginning with a stressed syllable, e.  g., Cahuilla, are trochaic. Right-to-left systems starting with an unstressed syllable, e.  g., Kutenai, are also trochaic, whereas those beginning with a stressed syllable, e.  g., Tübatulabal, have iambic feet.

6.5.1 Iambic/trochaic asymmetries Comparison of iambic and trochaic stress, particularly those spoken in North America where iambic stress is disproportionately common relative to other areas of the world, suggests an interesting rhythmic asymmetry in the two types of systems. Iambic systems typically lengthen stressed syllables as in the Central Alaskan Yupik word (maˈɬuː)(suˈtuː) (ɬiˈniː)(luˈni) ‘he apparently always hunted for beached sea mammals’ (Woodbury 1987: 696), which displays lengthening of vowels in non-final stressed syllables, or the second syllable in each iambic foot (feet surrounded by parentheses). Iambic lengthening in Yupik (as in many other languages) is suppressed in final syllables and in syllables closed by a consonant, which are already heavy without vowel lengthening by virtue of the syllable-final consonant, e.  g., (uˈtəχ)(tənˈʁil)(ŋuʁˈni) ‘(ones) who do not come home (loc.)’ (Woodbury 1987: 696). Iambic lengthening can alternatively affect the consonant immediately following stressed vowels. For example, in Lenape (Algonquian; United States) (Goddard 1982), a voiceless consonant other than /h/ is lengthened (i.  e., geminated) after a stressed short vowel. Thus, the /t/ after the stressed vowel in the word (nəˈmət)(təˈmeː) ‘I follow a trail’ surfaces as long unlike its counterpart before a stressed vowel (məˈtə)(meː) ‘he follows a trail’ (Goddard 1979: xiii). The post-stress consonant lengthening can be interpreted as a strategy for increasing the duration of the stressed syllable if one assumes the standard analysis in which a long (geminate) consonant is shared between two syllables. On the other hand, in (mə.ˈtə.)(meː), unlike (nə.ˈmət.)(tə.ˈmeː), the short /t/ belongs only to the second syllable. Note that the /m/ after the stressed vowel in the first word does not lengthen because it is a voiced consonant. The lengthening process observed in the iambic stress languages of Central Alaskan Yupik and Lenape offers support for the hypothesis that the optimal iamb consists of a light unstressed syllable followed by a heavy stressed syllable (Prince 1990). Unlike iambic stress systems, trochaic systems are less consistent in the pervasiveness and magnitude of the lengthening effect. Trochaic stress languages thus limit stress-induced lengthening to the primary stressed syllable in a word and the length-



Word prosody 

 145

ening effect, where it occurs, is characteristically smaller than that observed in iambic languages. This durational asymmetry between iambic and trochaic stress systems is often termed the Iambic-Trochaic Law (see Hayes 1995 and Hyde 2011 for overviews), which has analogs in musical grouping preferences and has been amply documented in psycholinguistic experiments. When presented with sequences of nonce syllables that are durationally equivalent but alternate between more intense and less intense, e.  g., [DA da DA da DA] (where greater intensity is indicated via capital letters), listeners prefer to group them into binary units in which the first syllable is perceived to be stressed, i.  e., (DA da) (DA da) DA, On the other hand, when presented with alternating sequences that are durationally imbalanced but equivalent in intensity, e.  g., [daa da daa da daa], the preference is for groupings in which the second member is the long syllable, i.  e., daa (da daa) (da daa). The relevance of the grouping asymmetry in different genres suggests a deeper basis for the Iambic-Trochaic Law that transcends the modality of language. The relationship between stress and length is bidirectional in many languages with rhythmic stress. Not only do syllables lengthen when stressed but syllables that are heavy also attract stress. In such languages with rhythmic weight-sensitive stress, the alternating stress pattern is interrupted by heavy syllables that attract stress even when adjacent to another stress. For example, in Chickasaw (Muskogean; Oklahoma) (Munro and Willmond 1994, 2005; Gordon 2004), which employs a left-to-right iambic stress pattern, all closed syllables and syllables containing a phonemic long vowel attract stress and the left-to-right alternating stress pattern resumes after the heavy syllable. As in Central Alaskan Yupik, non-final stressed vowels in open syllables lengthen in Chickasaw. Thus, in the word, (ˌtok)(saˌliː)(liˈtok) ‘I worked’, the first syllable is stressed by virtue of its weight whereas the third vowel is stressed by the rhythmic stress pattern (and lengthened because it is stressed). The final syllable is both heavy and in a rhythmically prominent position. The attraction of stress by heavy syllables creates the potential for multiple adjacent stresses in a Chickasaw word, e.  g., (ˌok)(ˌʧaː)(ˌlin)(ˈʧiʔ) ‘savior’, which has four consecutive stressed syllables.

6.5.2 Metrical structure beyond stress In all of the examples discussed thus far, the evidence for metrical feet has come from stress and the rhythmic grouping preferences comprising the Iambic-Trochaic Law. In certain languages, phenomena other than stress provide the most compelling support for foot structure. One such language is Muskogee (Muskogean; Florida), in which the occurrence of high tone is predictable from an iambic metrical parse oriented toward the left edge of a word. In Muskogee words (lacking a lexically assigned high tone), a predictable high tone spans the domain between the leftmost and the rightmost foot head in a word (Haas 1977; Martin 2011). For example, in the words (nokó)(sótʃí) ‘bear cub’, (awá)(nájí)ta ‘to tie to’ (Martin 2011: 75), a high tone plateau extends from the

146 

 Matthew K. Gordon

second syllable to the rightmost strong syllable: the final syllable in (nokó)(sótʃí) and the penultimate syllable in (awá)(nájí)ta. In the word (í:)(káná) ‘land’, the high tone span begins with the first syllable, which is the leftmost strong syllable due to its heavy status. Vowel deletion (syncope) and shortening patterns can also provide evidence for metrical structure. Tonkawa (isolate; Texas, Oklahoma, New Mexico) displays complementary processes of vowel shortening and deletion that can be accounted for with reference to metrical feet (Gouskova 2003). Deletion targets short vowels in even-numbered non-final syllables, while shortening makes phonemic long vowels short in the same context (Hoijer 1933, 1946). For example, the second (long) vowel is shortened and the fourth (short) vowel is lost in the word /ke-taː-notoso-oʔs/ ‘he stands with me’, which surfaces as (ˈketa)(ˈnot)( ˈsoʔs). Gouskova (2003) suggests that both processes are driven by foot well-formedness conditions stemming from the trochaic metrical parse of Tonkawa. Long vowels shorten to avoid a trochaic foot in which the unstressed syllable is heavier than the stressed syllable, e.  g., (ˈketa) instead of *(ˈketaː), reflecting a more general strong cross-linguistic bias against unstressed heavy syllables. Short vowels delete to create monosyllabic feet in which the stressed syllable is heavy, e.  g., (ˈnot) instead of *(noto), in keeping with the cross-linguistic preference for stressed syllables to be heavy. Poetry and songs can also provide evidence for metrical structure. In her analysis of Tohono O’odham (Uto-Aztecan; Arizona, Mexico) songs, Fitzgerald (1998) shows that recurring characteristics of song lines can be captured by assuming that the song meter consists of trochaic feet. First, neither the second nor the last syllable of a line is stressed and, second, adjacent stresses are prohibited. Both constraints follow if one assumes that lines adhere to a trochaic parse and that there is a requirement that song lines begin and end with a trochaic foot. The trochaic parse is enforced through a process of vacuous reduplication (copying of sounds; see Urbanczyk 2023 [this volume] for discussion of reduplication) whose only function is to ensure that the song is metrically well-formed. The role of reduplication in the song’s verse is apparent through comparison of words appearing in a song with the spoken equivalent lacking reduplication. For example, the word ˈwaw ‘rock’ surfaces as ˈwa-wai before a word beginning with a stressed syllable in a song (Fitzgerald 1998: 24).

6.6 Prominence at different levels One of the challenges intrinsic to the study of stress is the intertwined relationship between word level stress and prominence associated with larger prosodic units. Although stress is typically conceived as a word level property, many descriptions of stress are based on words uttered in isolation, a context in which the word is equivalent to the entire utterance. Prosodic units larger than the word have their own prominence characteristics, which often are realized through the same acoustic cues as word stress.



Word prosody 

 147

For example, intonation is a phrase and utterance level feature that is conveyed by pitch, which is the perceptual correlate of fundamental frequency, the same acoustic property used to signal stress in many languages. The use of pitch as a cue to both word stress and phrasal intonation can make it difficult to determine whether a prominence on a syllable is attributed to stress or to intonation. For example, the question Is it a mammal? has a pitch rise on the second syllable of mammal even though the first syllable is stressed. This terminal pitch rise is a general property of yes/no questions for many speakers of American English. The stress on the first syllable of mammal is more easily perceived when mammal is placed in non-final position of an utterance where there is no terminal pitch rise, e.  g., A mammal couldn’t live in that climate. The stress on the first syllable of mammal is even more apparent when the word is focused, as in the utterance A fox is a mammal not a reptile. As these examples of the word mammal in different contexts illustrate, the same word can have very different prosodic realizations depending on properties related to the utterance. Stress patterns may accordingly differ depending on the position of a word in an utterance. For example, word-final syllables in Central Alaskan Yupik are unstressed when followed by a large prosodic boundary even if they are predicted to be stressed by the weight-sensitive iambic stress system that treats CVV as heavy. The effect of a word’s position can be observed by comparing the first word of the phrase in (8a), which has a large boundary separating the words, with the first word of the phrase in (8b), which has a small boundary between the words. Conversely, word-final syllables followed by another word in the same phrase are stressed in Yupik even if not predicted to be stressed by the typical stress rules treating (non-initial) CVC as light (8c). (8)                  

The effect of phrasal position on stress in Central Alaskan Yupik (Miyaoka 1985) a. nuˈnaːkaː taˈmaːna   his land that (extended one)   ‘that (extended one) is his (emphasis) land’ b. nuˈnaːˌkaː taˈmaːna   his land that (extended one)   ‘that (extended one) is his land’ c. qaˈjaːˌmun teˈkiːtuq   to the kayak he came   ‘he came to the kayak’ (cf. phrase-final *qaˈjaːmun)

In summary, it is important to consider the prosody of a word in varied contexts to determine which syllable or syllables are consistently prominent and can be reliably treated as stressed (see Tuttle, this volume for more on the relationship between word prosody and prosody associated with larger units).

148 

 Matthew K. Gordon

6.7 Prosodic evolution Like other linguistic properties, stress and metrical structure can evolve over time due to either internal changes or due to contact with other languages. The evolution of stress systems in languages of North America has been the subject of relatively little study but there is enough comparative research on certain language families to suggest various mechanisms of prosodic development. In a study of Yupik varieties, Leer (1985) examines the development from a relatively simple left-to-right weight-sensitive iambic stress system, still observed in Central Siberian Yupik, to more complex versions in which the core rhythmic and weight elements of the inherited system are preserved but have been supplemented with additional features. For example, Alaskan Yupik varieties have introduced a modification to the weight system whereby initial closed syllables containing a short vowel (CVC) attract stress. Central Alaskan Yupik varieties have extended this attraction of stress by closed syllables to positions to the right of the initial syllable, where a CVC syllable preceding a non-final CV syllable attracts stress away the following CV. The progression in the weight of CVC can be observed in comparing the stress of the word for ‘my big boat’ in three Yupik varieties: aŋˈjaχpaka in Central Siberian Yupik, ˈaŋjaχˈpaːka in the Norton Sound dialect of Alaskan Yupik, and ˈaŋˈjaχpaka in Central Alaskan Yupik (Jacobson 1985: 21), where the lack of stress on the final syllable reflects the pre-boundary realization (see section 6.6). Northern Iroquoian languages display a different type of prosodic development based on a shift in directionality of stress assignment. The ancestor language to modern Northern Iroquoian languages, Proto-Northern Iroquoian, originally positioned stress on the penultimate (second-to-last) syllable (Chafe 1977; Foster 1982; Michelson 1988). This system is essentially preserved in Mohawk (Chafe 1977; Mithun in press) and Tuscarora (Williams 1976). Cayuga and Seneca, however, have introduced a left-to-right iambic system (see section 6.5.1 on iambs) that places stress on the syllable immediately preceding the penult (the antepenult) if it is an even-numbered one counting from the beginning of the word. The difference in the location of stress between Mohawk and Tuscarora, on the one hand, and Cayuga and Seneca, on the other hand, can be seen in the cognate set for ‘mind, spirit’ in (9). Note that the circumflex ˆ in the Mohawk form indicates falling tone, (see Uchihara 2023 [this volume] for more on tone) a reflex of the following /h/ that is still preserved in the Tuscarora form. (9)            

Stress differences in Northern Iroquoian languages (Julian 2010: 558–9) a. Penultimate stress in Mohawk and Tuscarora   Mohawk: oɁniˈkũ̂ːɹaɁ   Tuscarora: oɁtiˈkə̃hrɛh b. Iambic stress in Cayuga and Seneca   Cayuga: kɁaˈnikõhaɁ   Seneca: hoɁˈnikɔɛ̃ Ɂ ̃



Word prosody 

 149

Comparison of stress in Northern Iroquoian languages demonstrates how related languages may follow different evolutionary paths in the development of their prosodic systems. In this case, a relatively simple shift from counting syllables from right-to-left to left-to-right by Cayuga and Seneca speakers produced a divergence in the location of stress from their linguistic relatives speaking Mohawk and Tuscarora.

6.8 Prosody and language contact The relative malleability of stress systems often makes it difficult in practice to distinguish between interlanguage convergence attributed to internal sources and convergence due to language contact (see Rice 2010 for discussion of this issue in the context of stress in North American languages). The potential for language contact to influence stress is exemplified, however, by Michif, a mixed language arising through contact between Plains Cree (Algonquian; Saskatchewan, Alberta, Manitoba, Montana) and French speakers in Manitoba during the 19th century (Rhodes 1977, 1986; Rosen 2006, 2007). Michif’s stress system shows elements of both the French and the Cree systems of stress. The basic pattern is for stress to fall on odd-numbered syllables in a word counting from the right edge. The choice of which stress is the primary one in words with multiple stresses varies as a function of word length. In words with three syllables, the shortest word shape that can support more than one stress, the primary stress is on the final syllable (10a), a pattern that mirrors French. In words of at least four syllables, the second to the last stress, the one on the antepenult (third-to-last syllable in a word), is the primary one, a pattern that corresponds to Cree stress (10b). (10)    

Michif stress (Rosen 2007: 219–220) a. Three-syllables: ˌmiʧʊˈwak ‘they are eating’, ˌʃɔkɔˈla ‘brown’ b. Four-syllable and longer: nɪˈmiʧʊˌnɑn ‘we are eating’, ˌkɪmiˈʧʊnɑˌwɑw ‘you (pl.) are eating’

Rosen (2007) suggests that the Michif divergence in the location of primary stress as a function of word length is attributed to a difference in the characteristic length of words with a French source vs. those of Cree origin. Because words sourced from French, predominantly nouns, were typically no more than three syllables, the French system of primary stress on the final syllable was adopted in trisyllabic words. On the other hand, the polysyllabic words, predominantly verbs that came from Cree, followed the Cree system in which primary stress falls on the antepenultimate syllable. This difference in primary stress placement only emerges in words with at least three syllables, as both French and Cree words have only a single (primary) stress on the final syllable in words shorter than three syllables. As Rosen points out, a key feature of the Michif stress system is its internal coherence: primary stress is predictable from word length and not directly from the language from which a word originates. Plains Cree words with three

150 

 Matthew K. Gordon

or fewer syllables thus have primary stress on the final syllable, e.  g., ˌmiʧʊˈwak ‘they are eating’ (Rosen 2007: 219), while French words with more than three syllables position primary stress on the antepenult, e.  g., oˈtomɔˌbil (Rosen 2007: 220). As the consistency of stress across different strata of the lexicon demonstrates, the influence of the source language’s stress system exerts its impact indirectly through the probabilistic correlation between word length and its source.

6.9 Conclusions: The importance of word prosody Although prosodic properties play a very important role in language, they often receive less attention in language instruction than segmental properties such as consonants and vowels. As we have seen, in many languages, differences in the location of stress can differentiate words that are otherwise identical, as in the Yuchi minimal pair ˈʃaja ‘squirrel’ vs. ʃaˈja ‘weeds’. Even if a language does not distinguish any pairs of words solely on the basis of stress, stress still plays an important role in facilitating effective communication, which is a critical component in language learning (see Derwing and Munro 2009 for an overview of issues related to communication for second language learners). Numerous studies have shown that accuracy in the production of stress patterns among second language learners is a better predictor of both intelligibility, the ability to be understood by others, and comprehensibility, the perceived naturalness or nativelikeness of speech, than other properties such as individual sounds and syllables (Anderson-Hsieh, Johnson, and Koehler 1992; Hahn 2004; Isaacs and Trofimovich 2012). Furthermore, because stress typically occurs at or near a word edge, it can be used as a marker of word boundaries aiding in the chunking of the string of speech into individual words. The importance of stress placement can easily be observed if one tries to interpret an English sentence in which stress is inaccurately placed, e.  g., eLEphants are mamMALS with stress on the second rather than the first syllable of elephants and on the second rather than the first syllable of mammals. Stress can also impact the realization of consonants and vowels in significant ways that impact meaning and intelligibility. For example, the rhythmic lengthening of stressed vowels in Yupik and Chickasaw discussed in section 6.5.1 creates vowels that are identical in length to (or virtually identical to) phonemic long vowels. Thus, the second rhythmically lengthened vowel in the Yupik word qaˈjani ‘his own kayak’ is equivalent in length to the phonemic long vowel in the word qaˈjaːni ‘in his own kayak’ (Jacobson 1985: 30) potentially leading to homophony or near homophony between words. On a deeper level, prosody is a crucial component of a language’s identity. The overall sound of a language depends heavily upon the stress, timing, and rhythm of individual words that form the building blocks of phrases, sentences, and larger discourses (see Buckley 2020 for more on word prosody in North American languages).



Word prosody 

 151

References Altshuler, Daniel. 2009. Osage fills the gap: The quantity insensitive iamb and the typology of feet. International Journal of American Linguistics 75. 365–398. Anderson-Hsieh, Janet, Ruth Johnson & Kenneth Koehler. 1992. The relationship between native speaker judgments of nonnative pronunciation and deviance in segmental, prosody, and syllable structure. Language Learning 42. 529–555. Bianco, Violet. 1995. Stress in Northern Lushootseed – a preliminary analysis. In Papers for the 30th (XXX) International Conference on Salish and Neighbouring Languages, 127–36. Victoria: University of Victoria. http://lingpapers.sites.olt.ubc.ca/files/2018/03/1995_Bianco.pdf (accessed 5 July 2020). Bird, Sonya, Tess Nolan & Rae Anne Claxton. [this volume]. Acoustic phonetics. In Carmen Dagostino, Marianne Mithun & Keren Rice (eds.), The languages and linguistics of Indigenous North America: A comprehensive guide. Berlin & Boston: De Gruyter Mouton. Bliss, Heather, Sonya Bird & Bryan Gick. [this volume]. Articulatory phonetics. In Carmen Dagostino, Marianne Mithun & Keren Rice (eds.), The languages and linguistics of Indigenous North America: A comprehensive guide. Berlin & Boston: De Gruyter Mouton. Boas, Franz & Ella Deloria. 1941. Dakota grammar. (Memoirs of the National Academy of Sciences 23). Washington: U.S. Government Printing Office. Buckley, Eugene. 1994. Persistent and cumulative extrametricality in Kashaya. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 12. 423–464. Buckley, Eugene. 2013. Prosodic structure in Southeastern Pomo stress. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Society for the Study of the Indigenous Languages of the Americas. https://www.ling. upenn.edu/~gene/papers/Buckley2013_pse_stress.pdf (accessed 5 July 2020). Buckley, Eugene. 2020. Stress, tone, and pitch accent. In Daniel Siddiqi, Michael Barrie, Carrie Gillon, Jason Haugen & Éric Mathieu (eds.), The Routledge handbook of North American languages, 68–90. New York: Routledge. Chafe, Wallace. 1977. Accent and related phenomena in the Five Nations Iroquois languages. In Larry Hyman (ed.), Studies in stress and accent, 169–81. (Southern California Occasional Papers in Linguistics 4). Los Angeles: University of Southern California. https://dornsife.usc.edu/assets/sites/56/docs/ SCOPIL4-studies-in-stress-and-accent.pdf (accessed 5 July 2020). Chafe, Wallace. 2014. A grammar of the Seneca language. UC Berkeley: University of California Press. Czaykowksa-Higgins, Ewa. 1993. Cyclicity and stress in Moses Columbia Salish (Nxa’amxcin). Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 11. 197–278. De Lacy, Paul. 2004. Markedness conflation in Optimality Theory. Phonology 21. 1–55. Derwing, Tracey M. & Murray J. Munro. 2009. Putting accent in its place: Rethinking obstacles to communication. Language Teaching 42. 476–490. Enrico, John. 1991. The lexical phonology of Masset Haida. ANLC-RP 8. Fitzgerald, Colleen. 1998. The meter of Tohono O’odham songs. International Journal of American Linguistics 64. 1–36. Foster, Michael. 1982. Alternating weak and strong syllables in Cayuga words. International Journal of American Linguistics 48. 59–72. Garvin, Paul. 1948. Kutenai 1: Phonemics. International Journal of American Linguistics 14. 37–43. Goddard, Ives. 1979. Delaware verbal morphology. New York: Garland Goddard, Ives. 1982. The historical phonology of Munsee. International Journal of American Linguistics 48. 16–48. Gordon, Matthew. 2004. A phonological and phonetic study of word-level stress in Chickasaw. International Journal of American Linguistics 70. 1–32. Gordon, Matthew & Timo Roettger. 2017. Acoustic correlates of word stress: A cross-linguistic survey. Linguistics Vanguard 3.1. https://doi.org/10.1515/lingvan-2017-0007.

152 

 Matthew K. Gordon

Gouskova, Maria. 2003. Deriving economy: Syncope in Optimality Theory. Amherst, MA: University of Massachusetts, Amherst dissertation. Available online on the Rutgers Optimality Archive, ROA 610, http://roa.rutgers.edu. Haas, Mary. 1977. Tonal accent in Creek. In Larry Hyman (ed.), Studies in stress and accent, 195–208. (Southern California Occasional Papers in Linguistics 4). Los Angeles, CA: University of Southern California. Hahn, Laura. 2004. Primary stress and intelligibility: Research to motivate the teaching of suprasegmentals. TESOL Quarterly 38(2). 201–223. Hargus, Sharon. 2005. Prosody in two Athabaskan languages of northern Brittish Columbia. In Sharon Hargus & Keren Rice (eds.), Athabaskan prosody, 393–423. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Hayes, Bruce. 1995. Metrical stress theory: principles and case studies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Hayes, Bruce. 1989. Compensatory lengthening in moraic phonology. Linguistic Inquiry 20. 253–306. Hoijer, Harry. 1933. Tonkawa: An Indian language of Texas. In Franz Boas & Harry Hoijer (eds.), Handbook of American Indian languages. Glückstadt; New York: J.J.: Columbia University Press. Hoijer, Harry. 1946. Tonkawa. In Cornelius Osgood (ed.), Linguistic structures of Native America, 289–311. New York: Viking Fund Publications in Anthropology. Hyde, Brett. 2011. The iambic-trochaic law. In Marc van Oostendorp, Colin Ewen, Elizabeth Hume & Keren Rice (eds.), The Blackwell companion to phonology, vol. II, 1052–1077. West Sussex, UK: Wiley-Blackwell. Isaacs, Talia & Pavel Trofimovich. 2012. Deconstructing comprehensibility: Identifying the linguistic influences on listeners’ L2 comprehensibility ratings. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 34. 475–505. Jacobson, Steven. 1985. Siberian Yupik and Central Yupik prosody. In Michael Krauss (ed.), Yupik Eskimo prosodic systems: Descriptive and comparative studies, 25–46. Fairbanks, Alaska: Alaska Native Language Center. Jany, Carmen. 2009. Chimariko grammar: Areal and typological perspective. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Jeanne, La Verne. 1982. Some phonological rules of Hopi. International Journal of American Linguistics 48. 245–270. Julian, Charles. 2010. A history of the Iroquoian languages. Winnipeg: University of Manitoba dissertation. Leer, Jeff. 1985. Prosody in Alutiiq (the Koniag and Chugach dialects of Alaskan Yupik). In Michael Krauss (ed.), Yupik Eskimo prosodic systems: Descriptive and comparative studies, 77–134. Fairbanks, Alaska: Alaska Native Language Center. Martin, Jack B. 2011. A grammar of Creek (Muskogee). University of Nebraska Press. Michelson, Karin. 1988. A comparative study of Lake-Iroquoian accent. Dordrecht: Kluwer. Mithun, Marianne. in press. A grammar of Mohawk. Miyaoka, Osahito. 1985. Accentuation in Central Alaskan Yupik. In Michael Krauss (ed.), Yupik Eskimo prosodic systems: Descriptive and comparative studies, 51–76. Fairbanks: Alaska Native Language Center. Moshinsky, Julius. 1974. A grammar of Southeastern Pomo. Berkeley: University of California Press. Munro, Pamela & Catherine Willmond. 1994. Chickasaw: An analytical dictionary. Norman: University of Oklahoma Press. Munro, Pamela & Catherine Willmond. 2005. Chikashshanompa’ kilanompoli’. Los Angeles: UCLA Academic Publishing. Prince, Alan. 1990. Quantitative consequences of rhythmic organization. Parasession on the Syllable in Phonetics and Phonology Chicago Linguistic Society 26. 355–398. Rhodes, Richard. 1977. French Cree: A case of borrowing. Actes du huitième congrès des algonquinistes, 6–26. Ottawa: Carleton University Press. Rhodes, Richard. 1986. Métchif: A second look. Proceedings of the 16th Congress of Algonquianists, 287–296. Ottawa: Carleton University Press.



Word prosody 

 153

Rice, Keren. 2010. Accent in the native languages of North America. In Harry van der Hulst, Rob Goedemans & Ellen van Zanten (eds.), A survey of word accentual patterns in the languages of the world, 155–248. Berlin & Boston: De Gruyter Mouton. Rood, David S. & Allan R. Taylor. 1996. Sketch of Lakhota, a Siouan Language. Handbook of North American Indians, vol. 17: Languages, 440–482. Washington DC: Smithsonian Institution. Rosen, Nicole. 2006. Language contact and Michif stress assignment. STUF 59(2). 170–190. Rosen, Nicole. 2007. Domains in Michif phonology. Toronto: University of Toronto dissertation. Seiler, Hansjakob. 1965. Accent and morphophonemics in Cahuilla and Uto-Aztecan. International Journal of American Linguistics 31. 50–9. Shaw, Patricia. 1985. Coexistent and competing stress rules in Stoney (Dakota). International Journal of American Linguistics 51. 1–18. Taff, Alice, Lorna Rozelle, Taehong Cho, Peter Ladefoged, Moses Dirks, & Jacob Wegelin. 2001. Phonetic structures of Aleut. Journal of Phonetics 29. 231–271. Uchihara, Hiroto. [this volume]. Tone in North American Languages. In Carmen Dagostino, Marianne Mithun & Keren Rice (eds.), The languages and linguistics of Indigenous North America: A comprehensive guide. Berlin & Boston: De Gruyter Mouton. Urbanczyk, Suzanne. 2023 [this volume]. Prosodic morphology. In Carmen Dagostino, Marianne Mithun & Keren Rice (eds.), The languages and linguistics of Indigenous North America: A comprehensive guide. Berlin & Boston: De Gruyter Mouton. Voegelin, Charles Frederick. 1935. Tübatulabal grammar. University of California Publications in American Archaeology and Ethnology 34. 55–190. Wagner, Günter. 1933–1938. Yuchi. In Franz Boas (ed.), Handbook of American Indian languages, vol. 3, 292–384. New York: Columbia University Press. Williams, Marianne Mithun. 1976. A grammar of Tuscarora. New York: Garland Press. Woodbury, Anthony. 1987. Meaningful phonological processes: a consideration of Central Alaskan Yupik Eskimo prosody. Language 63. 685–740.

Siri G. Tuttle

7 Prosody beyond the word Abstract: This chapter will discuss the study of prosodic effects that extend beyond single words in connected speech. These patterns are realized in contrasts in pitch, duration, and loudness. They represent both extensions of and interactions between different prosodic systems such as word stress, lexical tone, and phrase and utterance intonation. While it can be difficult to pull together information of this type from written sources on North American Indigenous languages, there is valuable information to be gathered even from the oldest grammatical descriptions. More recently, linguists increasingly address the prosody of connected speech in their descriptions.

7.1 It’s all about how you say it When we listen to a traditional story told by an elder, we hear more than the sounds of consonants and vowels. We hear how they use their voices to let us know when a new sentence is beginning; when a piece of the speech is important; and even where we are in the story. Consider this example from a Lower Tanana Dene story told by Moses Charlie in the early 1960s (Charlie, Krauss and Kari 1991).

Fig. 1: From “Raven and Bear” by Moses Charlie (Charlie, Krauss and Kari 1991)

The pitch track above the printed words in this example represents some aspects of the performance of this piece of a story. The wavy lines in the middle box of the graphic show how the storyteller lengthened and shortened his words and raised or lowered https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-007

156 

 Siri G. Tuttle

his pitch. The pitch track is generated using a phonetic analysis program called Praat (Boersma and Weenink 2018; see Bird, Nolan, and Claxton, this volume) so it can be analyzed quantitatively. However, it can also be understood intuitively. Longer lines mean a longer time taken to say a word; higher marks mean a syllable or a word was pronounced with higher pitch in this recording. Notice in this example how the first tsoni ‘brown bear’ takes longer to say than the next one, and there is a pause after it. The storyteller repeats the word. The grizzly is the main antagonist in this story, a very important character. The introduction of this character gets special treatment, both in words – the repetition – and in the prosodic treatment of the words. However, the storyteller also remembers to stress the first syllable in the word, not the second, using effects of length and pitch, so that word prosody is respected as well. Prosody is letting us know about all these things at once. This example also shows the stretchability of prosodic structure – in this case, intonational boundary marking. Notice again the first pronunciation of tsoni ends with an intonational low tone before the pause – but when it is included in a longer utterance (the second pronunciation), the final low pitch is assigned to the word at the end of that utterance (nokhuniłtsinh). Thus, some of the prosodic effects that we hear in connected speech relate directly to linguistic information: they let us know when words and utterances are beginning and ending. However, they may also encode information that is called paralinguistic – letting us know more about the speaker’s attitude, intention, and feelings. When we use recorded stories as linguistic data, we have to sort out the linguistic from the paralinguistic in order to make generalizations. In the case of prosody, we also have to sort out effects that relate to syllables and words from effects driven by the composition of longer utterances. It can be difficult to decide where to start, especially if the prosodic systems of the language we are studying have not been fully described. This chapter therefore connects to all the other chapters in this book that deal with prosody. In words, prosody organizes stress and tone patterns to provide cues to word identity, and also cues the listener to word boundaries (see Gordon, this volume, on word prosody and Uchihara, this volume, on tone for descriptions and examples from numerous languages). Prosody also organizes speech in domains that are, or can be, much larger than a prosodic word. Some prosodic patterns play out over shorter or longer stretches of speech, from a single syllable to a long utterance. Intonation is one of these systems. However, it is impossible to figure out what intonation is doing in a language unless we know what prosodic phenomena exist at the word level as well. For this reason, the examples in this chapter will involve interaction among prosodic systems: word stress, tone, and intonation. Despite the importance of phrase and utterance-level prosody to the understanding and use of language, this field of study is still developing in Native American linguistics.



Prosody beyond the word 

 157

In this chapter, many of the examples come from the author’s work with Dene (ath) languages, which provide a wealth of opportunities to study the interactions between prosodic systems.

7.2 Prosody beyond the word What we observe most often when we study prosody is contrast and variation in three major phenomena of spoken language: the relative pitches at which we hear words spoken, the relative length of syllables, and the relative loudness of different syllables. Pitch is a given in any speech, because each of our voices have a basic pitch, reflecting its fundamental frequency. All syllables take time to utter, because each must be uttered in turn. And loudness, reflecting the acoustic amplitude of our utterances, has to be there or we cannot hear each other. Prosody is the study of how we use these basic properties of human speech in communication. What is fascinating about it is that despite the universality of the cues of prosody, the ways it is used are very particular to individual languages. Beckman and Venditti (2011: 486) refer to prosody as a “multidimensional taxonomy of phonetic form in relationship to linguistic function.” As such, its study requires us to be aware of all the possible functions related to each measurable cue. Among these are boundary creation (helping the listener find the edges of words or larger units), realization of prominence (marking the most important syllable in a string of language) and lexical identification (serving as a required element for distinguishing one morpheme from another). Considering only these three functions, three possible cues, and several possible types of prosodic systems, we can see some of the many dimensions to which Beckman and Venditti refer (Table 1). Tab. 1: Functions and Cues of Prosodic Systems  

F0 (Pitch)

Duration (Length)

Intensity (Loudness)

Boundary Creation

Boundary tones (phrases) Edge-aligned stress (words)

Intonational slowing (phrases)

Intonational fading (phrases)

Word ­identification

Tone, stress, accent

Stress

Increased loudness: Stress in words

Prominence

Stress, emphasis

Stress, emphasis

Stress, emphasis

The use of the same auditory cues to signal different prosodic systems, as shown in Table  1, can result in a high level of complexity. It is problematic for learners and describers of languages that prosodic patterns of this type are not always fully described, and their interaction even less often. There are several reasons for this. It can be very

158 

 Siri G. Tuttle

challenging to provide this description because it requires knowledge of each of the prosodic systems that might be present, and tools to tease them apart. But there may also be a bias toward more intensive study of the linguistic elements that are encoded in orthography – that is, segmental elements that can be spelled using commonly available symbols. Such bias may have consequences for the creation of reference works that are used to build pedagogical materials. This in turn may exacerbate the issues that can arise from too great dependence on written language in teaching (see Chew, Leonard, and Rosenblum, this volume). Prosodic patterns operate within domains of application: stress patterns usually play out within words, and tone systems also have word-level patterns. However, when words are combined into phrases, both stress and tone systems may apply patterns in the larger domain as well, and these patterns may be similar to or different from those that apply within words. When words are joined into phrases, and phrases into longer utterances, prosodic phenomena that operate within different domains may also come into competition. This happens when systems that employ the same phonetic means of expression – pitch, duration, or loudness – need to use the same tone-bearing unit (a vowel, say, or a syllable, where the pitch can be heard) for their expression. In some cases, conflicting values for a phonetic cue may all be expressed, a phenomenon called co-articulation; in other cases, one system’s expression may obscure the expression of another system. A high lexical tone at the end of an intonational phrase may be affected by intonational final lowering, for example. Word or phrase stress may use pitch as part of its expression, and the pitch required may obscure lexical tone. Other prosodic patterns include vowel harmony systems, in which vowels must agree for some feature within a particular domain. They may also include nasalization patterns, and rhotacization patterns. These patterns work much the same way as those that use pitch, loudness and timing as their auditory cues, in that their domains are not defined by the boundaries of single linguistic units. However, these systems are realized segmentally, rather than in pitch and timing. Robins (1958, 1967) describes a vowel harmony process in the Algic language Yurok (Algic; yur), which makes any non-high vowel in a prefix be pronounced as [ɚ], a rhotic or r-like vowel, if it precedes the rhotic vowel in the first syllable of the stem word. In this example from Robins’ 1958 grammar, the initial syllable of ‘my saliva’ is pronounced with the rhotic vowel written as [ɹ], harmonizing with the following rhotic in the first stem syllable. (1)    

Yurok vowel harmony (Robins 1958:26) (Robins’ orthography)   (ʔ)nelɹhpɹyeł ~ (ʔ)nɹlɹhpɹyeł ‘my saliva’

Harmony processes are considered prosodic because they do not just affect vowels and consonants that are adjacent to each other but work across syllable boundaries (as in 1) and even across word boundaries.



Prosody beyond the word 

 159

Prosody beyond the word, even more than word-level prosody, is often not notated in writing systems. Conventional devices like punctuation can be used to suggest boundaries within which prosodies may operate. Usually, the choice is made to keep wordlevel spelling consistent, rather than to transcribe prosody using the writing system. More general guidelines for reading, or phonological rule statements, may be provided to help learners. For this reason, information about prosody beyond the word is sometimes found buried in the caveats presented by researchers when they are describing word-level systems or attempting to tease one prosodic system apart from another. For example, Krauss (2005: 105) wrote of the Dene languages Gwich’in (gwi) and Hän (haa), that “both…show phrase-final lowering of unmarked pitch, apparently neutralizing the contrast between marked and unmarked…” and of Lower Tanana (taa), another language with sparse low tone (p.  119) that “verb stem tone is (apparently) neutralized in the usual final position”, though he did not specify intonational final lowering as the reason for this neutralization. Thus, while it is not always possible to find an article that explicitly references prosody beyond the word, it is often possible to find clues in other linguistic descriptions to systems that need investigation, as they emerge as confounds in studies of other systems. Another example from Moses Charlie’s Lower Tanana Raven and Bear story, recorded by Krauss himself, shows the source of his observation. In Figure 2, a low-toned syllable ends the utterance, and in Figure 3, a non-tonal syllable is in the same position. Measurement in Praat shows that these two syllables are both pronounced at about the same pitch – by the same speaker, in the same narrative. The numbers in Hz (Hertz) represent measurement of the acoustic property fundamental frequency, which we hear as pitch.

Fig. 2: Low toned syllable in utterance-final position in Lower Tanana (Charlie, Krauss and Kari 1991)

160 

 Siri G. Tuttle

Fig. 3: Non-low syllable in utterance-final position in Lower Tanana (Charlie, Krauss and Kari 1991)

The relevant information lies in the pitch track over these two utterances. Both end with a low pitch on the last syllable. However, the final syllable in Figure 2 is lexically low-toned, meaning that it can be expected to sound lower than the last one in Figure 3. However, in this intonational final position, the two are almost identical in pitch. (There is a difference in the voice quality, which is historically related to Lower Tanana’s low tone – but the difference does not show up as a difference in pitch.) This example shows how important it can be to learn about all the prosodic systems in a language we are studying. Because prosodic systems with different domains may use the same acoustic cues, it is possible to draw false conclusions if we are thinking only about lexical tone patterns, or only about stress, or only about intonation.

7.3 Methods in prosodic research There are numerous methods and tools available for the investigation of prosodic systems. This applies to the selection or collection of data, its analysis, and the implications of research for practical applications. Xu Yi (2011) describes the development of international prosodic research as a passage through a number of different methods, including introspection, transcription, hypothesis-testing, and more sophisticated quantitative modeling, on the way to predictive knowledge – which is what learners of a language hope to gain, in order to speak correctly. Researchers in Native American prosodic research have employed the same methods with the same goal. However, language endangerment and language shift make the status of prosodic data an especially crucial element in this research.



Prosody beyond the word 

 161

As in other aspects of linguistic research, direct elicitation, structured narration, and the recording of spontaneous speech are all useful tools in data collection. Direct elicitation of words rarely gives enough information for a description of prosodic effects over phrases or utterances. Elicitation can interfere with the collection of valid data. There is, for example, the lab-speech effect, caused by lack of context and speaker discomfort, which can cause a speaker’s prosody to come out distorted in any number of ways. However, prosodic interaction itself can interfere with the gathering of word-level information. Studying spontaneous or semi-spontaneous narration offers both benefits and risks. The benefits include access to authentic and spontaneous connected speech. For some situations, where few first-language speakers remain, narration recorded and archived in earlier decades may provide the only opportunity to hear fluent speech. The risks include the possibility of confusing constraints on performance, such as markers of register or genre, with constraints on ordinary language. Because fluent texts may have been recorded by relatively few narrators in some endangered language situations, there is also the possibility of one person’s performative style being understood to represent the state of prosody for the language’s population – or to be taken as evidence for significant language change. For this reason, whenever possible we want to review narrative audio (and when possible, video) with the original narrator in order to correct possible confusions. When the original narrator is not available, family members or others who had a chance to hear the narrator in person can be extremely helpful in interpretation, even when they themselves do not feel they could tell a traditional story in the language with confidence. Some studies of phrasal prosody (Mirzayan 2010, for example) use both elicited data and spontaneous narration. This study of Lakhota prosody is based on archival narrative recordings from the early 1970s, as well as recordings by the author done between 2006 and 2009. In the author’s work, recordings include spontaneous speech, but also scripted phrases and conversation, as well as elicited words and phrases (Mirzayan 2010: 32). An example from Mirzayan’s study (Figure 4) shows how multilayered prosodic analysis needs to be. The graphic shows the waveform generated by the program Praat (Boersma and Weenink 2018). Below the waveform, a pitch track shows the relative height of pitches in the recorded utterance. The next tier down is marked using the ToBI prosodic annotation system (Beckman and Elam 1997), which marks “pitch accents” that reflect intonational patterns. These high-pitched phrase-level accents line up with the word-level stresses. The tier below the ToBI annotation shows the segments of the utterance, divided by syllables; below that we see the words of the utterance. Since high and low pitches can be contributed to an utterance by both word-level and utterance-level requirements, this layered annotation style can be highly informative, though it is, of course, also very time-consuming to create. As a dissertation, Mirzayan’s work has room for a few very complete graphics demonstrating the analysis. Many other studies, including those that modern work depends on, contain no such intuitively readable display.

162 

 Siri G. Tuttle

300

F0 (Hz)

255 210 165 120 (L)

75

ma

H* štíŋ maštíŋča

0

L ča

H* phu



L

^H*

L



=phuté

la líla

Time (s)

!H*

L%

ó

tah óta 2.705

Fig. 4: Mirzayan (2010: 82): a Lakhota sentence about buffalo berries.

Despite the brevity of some older descriptions of phrasal prosody, no description should be ignored. Historically, prosodic description has been reported based on accumulated intuitive observation, including qualitative study of recorded narration. Prosodic facts may be presented very briefly as part of much longer and more complex descriptions of segmental phonology and morphology. Still, even these descriptions can encapsulate crucial facts about pronunciation. These generalizations are not based on first impressions, but often on decades of work by a linguist with a language community. For example, Pitkin’s (1984) grammar of the Wintu language of Northern California (Wintuan, wnw) contains only three pages that describe prosody above word level, but they include information about smaller and larger domains, effects of pitch, duration and pause, and phrasal accent. It is no accident that the grammar is based on nearly thirty years of experience with the language. Sivertsen (1956) considers multiple issues that make the phonemic status of suprasegmentals in Kiowa (kio) hard to define. Her work was based on data collection at the 1954 Summer Institute of Linguistics at the University of Oklahoma, with a number of Kiowa participants. She approaches the description of phrase-level prosody as a problem of sorting confounds, but this does not prevent the inclusion of several generalizations, including that of final lowering and downdrift (p. 122), raising of pitch on high-toned syllables “under special emphasis” (p. 123) and patterns of laryngealization on open syllables, involving interaction with the tonal system (p. 124). This work is qualitative but includes instrumental measurement. Quantitative and qualitative methods are both useful, in the study of prosody beyond the word. These represent two different, and equally important, means of arriving at generalizations. Quantitative studies measure and count observations, and then filter the measurements through statistical models to reduce the possibility of observer



Prosody beyond the word 

 163

bias or results that are due to chance. Qualitative studies demonstrate and describe phenomena learned through uncounted, but valid observations. These two styles of working with prosodic data complement each other. Usually, the results are consistent across methodologies, when they can be compared (Tuttle 2003). Both quantitative and qualitative methods can use instrumental measurement. Acoustic graphics such as those shown in Figures 1–4 can represent typical or exemplary tokens of a phenomenon, where the linguist has learned through experimental measurement and statistics; they may also represent good examples of phenomena learned about primarily through auditory study. In both cases, the linguist is showing an example that they believe correctly represents their generalization. Digital tools for data collection, analysis, description, and demonstration of prosodic patterns have become increasingly accessible. The pitch tracks shown in Figures 1–4, for example, were derived from the program Praat (Boersma and Weenink 2018; see Bird, Nolan, and Claxton, this volume). It takes only a few minutes to make these measurements and create a graphic display to demonstrate them using this free program. Affordable access to programs like this has only become available in the last twenty years, and this has changed the study of sound in language in important ways. For one thing, it means that when we make a claim about pitch, length, quietness, or some other phonetic effect, we can back it up, sometimes with quantitative data and often with intuitively communicative images. For another, it means that when we work with prosodic systems, we really need to provide this phonetic evidence – because we can. The end result can become a body of descriptive work that is both scientifically reproducible, and adaptable to applications like language learning and teaching. Researchers working with Dene languages have used quantitative methods in a number of studies: Holton (2005) for Tanacross (tcb), Lovick and Tuttle (2012) for Dena’ina (tfn), Tuttle (1998) for Lower and Middle Tanana, Bird (2004) for Lheidli (crx), Berez (2011) for Ahtna (aht), Alderete and Bob (2005) for Tahltan (tht), Tuttle (2005) for Apache (apw), and Palakurthy (2019) for Diné Bizaad (Navajo, nav) have used such methods. Researchers of Salish languages have also employed quantitative methods to explore prosodic patterns: Caldecott (2016; St’át’imcets, lil) follows on a burst of research on prosody for this language family, including works by Barthmaier (2004; Okanagan, oka), Beck (1991; Lushootseed, lut) Caldecott and Czaykowska-Higgins (2012; Nxaʔamčín/ Moses-Columbian Salish, sal), among others. Kinkade and Mattina (1996) offer several useful caveats for those wishing to embark on prosodic description using published and archived written narratives from the languages of North America. They point out (p.  253) that “Non-speaking specialists in a language may be able to make some evaluation of texts based on comparisons between texts narrated by different speakers, observation of certain stylistic features or complexities, or vocabulary selection, but even then expectations based on Euro-American literary traditions may color their judgments.” They also note that published texts are

164 

 Siri G. Tuttle

often “severely edited” to make them easier to read. Those who wish to understand the prosody of the performances on which these texts are based should, if at all possible, obtain the recordings from which they were transcribed and compare the two versions of the recorded language. Sometimes, an original narration is corrected or adapted by the narrator in further work on the story. There can be multiple layers of information in recorded field sessions discussing the story, as well as in the original performance recording. When available, all versions should be compared. Recorded narration is not the only form of verbal art that can be used to study prosody (see Webster, this volume). The study of verse has been used as a method for learning about word stress in many languages, because of the tendency for poetic forms to match word prominence to metrical prominence. Musical rhythms can be thought of as analogous to poetic meter in some cases, making text setting another potential area of study in the understanding of prominence in language. Lower Tanana song lyrics show that word prominence can be linked to intentionally created structure as well as the naturally occurring rhythms of utterances. Tuttle (2012), a study of text-setting in songs from the village of Minto (Lower Tanana), finds that syllables that would carry main stress in speech – mostly syllables that correspond with the roots of words – are the syllables most likely to be linked to a moving pitch, a note held over more than one beat, or a higher pitch than a neighboring syllable. In Figure 5, two lines of a short song from a Lower Tanana story are analyzed for their rhythm and pitch movement in relation to word stress. The lines are: sech’etthila’ selotl’ogh no’i’oyh, yozronh eya, yozronh ‘Give me my ax back! Clear sky, clear sky.’ In this song, which follows the rhythm of speech, word roots are stressed and are also associated with higher pitches and longer-held notes (Tuttle 2012). In the table in Figure 5, syllables in the song lyric are lined up with other aspects of the song: whether the pitch changes on a syllable, whether the pitch in the melody goes up or down, how long a syllable is held for, whether the syllable is placed on a downbeat in the rhythm. By carefully listening to multiple recordings and paying attention to how singers place words in melodies, it is possible not only to learn about song making, but also to uncover facts about the language that did not show up in conversational registers. In this case, it was the importance of word roots to the stress pattern in the language.



Prosody beyond the word 

 165

Yozronh, line 1 Pitch movement D/U Beats Linguistic stress Segmental Gloss

high D 1 X se my

lower U 1 . ch'e its

flat D 1 X tthi chop

flat U 1 . la' poss

flat D 1 X se my

flat U 1 . lo band

flat DU 2 X tl'ogh to

flat DU 2 X e voc

lower DU 2 X ya voc

Yozronh, lines 2- 3 Pitch movement D/U Beats Linguistic stress Segmental Gloss

higher lower D U 1 1 X X no ’i back you

flat DU 2 X ’oyh give

flat flat D UDU 1 2-3 X X yo zronh sky clear

higher D 1 X yo sky

flat UDU 2-3 X zronh clear

Fig. 5: Two lines from a Lower Tanana song (Tuttle 2012), annotated

7.4 Intonational boundaries Intonation organizes several different kinds of functions in a language. Intonation groups parts of an utterance and signals beginnings and endings; it may also be used for emphasis and contrast. In some languages, it can encode mood (declarative, interrogative, imperative). Intonational patterns can also distinguish speech registers and genres of expression. Boundaries occur at the edges of units. Prosodic units have been suggested to exist in a hierarchy – reaching from the level of the syllable to multi-sentence groupings, something like paragraphs in written language (Nespor and Vogel 1986). Intonation does not always play a role in differentiating smaller from larger units, but some studies have found intonational differences between larger utterances and phrases within those utterances. Beginnings and ends of intonational units – utterances, and sometimes prosodic phrases within utterances – are often marked by an intonational pattern involving a change in pitch, slowing of speech rate, or a decrease in volume. A common finding is “final lowering.” Declarative utterances in many languages, including some Native American languages, have this pattern, in which the final syllable of an utterance carries a lower tone than the rest of the syllables. Caldecott and Czaykowska-Higgins (2012), for example, report final lowering as well as low initial boundary tones in Salish languages. Gordon, Martin, and Langley (2015) describe the prosodic system of Koasati (cku), a Muskogean language spoken in Louisiana and Tennessee, using a database of words

166 

 Siri G. Tuttle

spoken in citation form, find that the one-word utterances in their data set show evidence of phrase-final tones – high or low, depending on the speaker. The data in this study consists of one-word utterances. Figure 6, from Gordon, Martin, and Langley, shows the sharp fall in pitch at the end of a Koasati utterance. They interpret this pitch pattern as intonational.

Fig. 6: Koasati elicitation, showing final lowering

Final lowering is not uncommon in Dene languages. Lovick and Tuttle (2012: 310) found final lowering in the Alaskan Dene language Dena’ina; Tuttle (1998, 2003) discusses utterance-final lowering in Lower Tanana, in an instrumental study using both elicited and text data. Final lowering may be found in Dene languages outside Alaska, as well. De Reuse (2006: 58) notes a final lowering effect in San Carlos Apache, which seems to co-articulate with high tones, creating a falling tone where a high toned syllable occurs at the end of an utterance. If an utterance ends with a low pitch, and the speaker begins a new utterance with a higher pitch, both beginning and end of utterance can be said to be intonationally marked. Berez (2011) finds that high or “reset” pitch marks the beginning of utterances in Ahtna narration. Final lengthening, sometimes referred to as “rhyme lengthening” because both consonants and vowels may be extended, is reported for Dena’ina by Lovick and Tuttle 2012, in intermediate intonational units (abbreviated “IU” in the graphic, and smaller than their “story units, ” abbreviated as “SU”). Palakurthy (2019) finds final lengthening at the ends of intonational units in a set of Navajo recorded narratives, as well as reset at the beginnings of units. This paper also reports significant pause at the ends of syntactic, as opposed to intonational units, a finding that harks back to the work of Landar



Prosody beyond the word 

 167

Fig. 7: Rhyme length in prosodic domains in Dena’ina (Lovick and Tuttle 2011)

(1963), a work that sought to organize Navajo grammar based on intonational group­ ings. Figure 7 shows a graphic representing statistical findings for the Dena’ina study (Lovick and Tuttle 2012: 315) on rhyme lengthening in intermediate intonational units: In this study, as in many others, statistics are used to reduce the probability that listener bias will affect generalizations about acoustic measurements. The boxplot in Figure 7 shows the median length (the line in the middle of the boxes) of syllable rhymes in milliseconds (thousandths of a second) as well as the variability of the findings (the height of the boxes). The position of the median line in the 4-IU-Fin position (position 4, Intonational-Unit-final) demonstrates the difference in rhyme length for this comparison. Statistical diagrams like this one provide information in graphic shorthand. They do not substitute for evaluation of the statistical studies themselves but serve to illustrate patterns that emerge with quantitative analysis. Boundary tones can be used to indicate other distinctions as well as to delimit utterances. In some cases, boundary markings may differ for different utterance types – as in English, where certain types of questions may be marked differently from declaratives. Lovick (2020) considers intonational and other accentual patterns in Upper Tanana Dene (tau) in the process of describing lexical tone effects. She finds (p. 153) that the low lexical tone in the Northway dialect of this language can be obscured by high-rising intonation in yes-no questions. Declaratives in this language show declination, or a gradual fall in pitch. Lovick (this volume) provides numerous other examples of intonational marking of different types of questions in various languages of North America. Final lengthening – lengthening of certain segments or slowing of speech rate at the ends of utterances  – has also been described for Chickasaw (cic) in Gordon and

168 

 Siri G. Tuttle

Munro (2007). Final vowels in Chickasaw words are lengthened whether the words are included in a phrase, end a phrase, or end a larger intonational unit. These vowels also become breathy, and the duration of the breathiness increases with the size of the domain ended by the vowel. To summarize: intonation structures utterances using relative pitch, loudness, and length. In the Native American languages discussed in this section, pitch and length have been the cues that define intonational boundaries.

7.5 Lexical tone beyond the word Lexical tone, in languages that have it, distinguishes words, different forms of words, and morphemes from one another (Uchihara, this volume). As seen in Table 1 (repeated here), tone is one of the linguistic systems that uses voice pitch as a cue. When words that have lexical tones are combined, tonal systems also play out in larger domains, such as phrases and utterances. Because lexical tone shares pitch as a cue with intonation, and sometimes with word stress, interactions between these different pitch-using systems can also change tonal contours. Tab. 1: (repeated). Functions and Cues of Prosodic Systems  

F0 (Pitch)

Duration (Length)

Intensity (Loudness)

Boundary ­Creation

Boundary tones (phrases) Edge-aligned stress (words)

Intonational slowing (phrases)

Intonational fading (phrases)

Word identification

Tone, stress, accent

Stress

Increased loudness: Stress in words

Prominence

Stress, emphasis

Stress, emphasis

Stress, emphasis

I follow van der Hulst (2011) and Welmers (1973) in defining a language with tone as one in which “both pitch phonemes and segmental phonemes enter into the composition of at least some morphemes.” but also including languages that allow affixes that consist solely of tones. This means that the word “tone” in this chapter refers to pitch effects that are associated with particular morphemes or with particular morpheme-level ­meanings. These definitions of tone all have to do with words, not with domains outside the word. However, tonal phenomena do extend to the phrasal level, and also often interact with the other prosodic systems that can use pitch as a cue: stress and intonation. Though Welmers (1973) was writing for researchers of African languages, his observations on tone and its relationship with other prosodic systems are valid for North



Prosody beyond the word 

 169

American languages as well. He points out that languages that use lexical tone may have many syllables that get their pitch from another prosodic system, and that tone is not always lexically significant (p. 79). Some of the tonal phenomena encountered outside the domain of the word include tonal spreading, downstep, downdrift, and interaction with intonational use of pitch, as discussed below. Tonal spreading, when a tone that exists on one syllable changes the tonal value of syllables near it, can occur both within and between words. Holton (2005: 252) describes word-internal spreading of tones in Tanacross, an Alaska Dene language with high lexical tone. This phenomenon results in a co-articulation of tones, resulting in rising or falling tone in certain morphologically complex contexts. Spreading across word boundaries has also been documented in the Dene languages of Alaska; Ritter (1990), for example, points out rightward spreading of Hän low tone from lexical items immediately preceding non-tonal verbal prefixes. Downstep is a phrasal tonal phenomenon in which high tones following certain other tones are pronounced with relatively lower pitch. Thompson and Thompson (1966) describe downstep in Tillamook (til); while a pitch pattern is referred to as stress, and particular accents followed by downstep or de-accentuation are described under intonation, the patterns are all shown over sequences without spaces, which indicates a judgment that they are “words.” Miner (1979) describes downstep in Winnebago (Ho-Chunk; Siouan, win) as a terracing effect over utterances, causing the pitches of word accents to occur at successively lower levels. In both the Ho-Chunk and the Tillamook case, the downstep effect is described as affecting word accent or stress, rather than lexical tone. However, the effect is the same, as pitches that are assigned at the level of the word are realized differently depending on their position in a phrase or utterance. Martin and Johnson (2002) describe tonal downstep in Creek (Muskogean, mus) as being triggered between words by a stress-assigned low pitch at the beginnings of words. Thus, when there are two words with high tones, the second high tone will be relatively lower than the first due to interaction with the stress system. These authors also describe downdrift: this is a gradual declination in the pitch of tonal syllables, towards a low at the end of an utterance. Tuttle (1999) found downdrift in the low tones of San Carlos Apache – but no clear downdrift or downstep in the high tones of the same data set, in a study intended to uncover intonational patterns. Figure 8 shows average pitch for high and low tones in a set of recordings of the sentences used in the experiment. The high tones cluster within a range of ten Hertz – between 120 and 130 Hertz in the male speaker’s voice. The low tones, however, fall from about 105 to 80 Hertz over the course of the utterances, suggesting that high and low tones seem to be treated differently in this language. Without studying tones in the context of longer utterances, this kind of pattern is much harder to observe. In example (2) below, a tone-intonation interaction is shown that demonstrates a competitive relationship between two prosodic systems: low tone, which in this lan-

170 

 Siri G. Tuttle

Fig. 8: Average high and low tones in different utterance positions in a Western Apache experimental set

guage is found sprinkled sparsely through utterances, and an intonational pitch that can be used to signal utterance type. A rising pitch at the end of yes-no questions in Lower Tanana is often used by bilingual speakers of the language in place of the low-toned question marker –(h)i, and this intonation can override low tone marking.’ The result can be a question contour that is very familiar to English speakers (M, H and L here refer to pitch levels Mid, High and Low): (2)                      



Lexical tone vs. question intonation – Lower Tanana (Session notes November 17, 2005) a. Contour when lexical tone is realized:                  M          H          L                  L       L   Khuzrunhts’e ghestà’   Well                                         PFV:1s.sbj-sleep:pfv   ‘I slept well’ b. Contour with question marker realized:                  M           H        L               M           H              L              M     M   L   Nenh chukhw, khuzrunhts’e ghintà’ì’   You         also                     well                                        PFV:2sSUB-sleep:PFV-Question   ‘And you, did you sleep well?’ c. Contour with question intonation on verb stem:                M           H        L               M           H              L                           M   H   Nenh chukhw, khuzrunhts’e ghintà’ì’ You         also                     well                                        PFV:2sSUB-sleep:PFV:Question And you, did you sleep well?’



Prosody beyond the word 

 171

The variation among these possibilities pits an intonational contour (which may or may not have been borrowed from English and favors a high pitch at the end of the question) against a tonal melody (which realizes the low tone on the low-toned question marker). Sometimes, the lexical-tonal version of the question wins out, and version (b) is heard; sometimes, the intonational pattern in (c) is realized instead. Both patterns are understood to convey the same meaning. Holton (2005: 273), a qualitative study, reports interaction between lexical tone and intonation in Tanacross, which he shows to have distinctive high and low tones. He finds that final lowering interacts with the high lexical tone in this language, with final lowering obscuring high tone in utterance-final syllables.

Fig. 9: Pitch track for declarative with high tone stem in Tanacross (Holton 2005). Figure 9 illustrates ­Holton's (2005:269) analysis of the interaction of high tone and final lowering in Tanacross. While the high tone here is expressed, its expression is seen in the early part of the utterance-final syllable, while the utterance-final low is expressed at the end of the syllable, creating a falling tone.

Similar phenomena are found in neighboring Lower Tanana. In Figures (10) and (11), two “versions” of a word are shown. The word is shath, which means ‘wart’. The speaker first gives the word as a translation, in citation form, in Figure 10. Notice that because it happens at the end of the utterance, it is pronounced with a low pitch (155 Hz). In Figure 11, recorded just after (10), she pronounces the word in context, in the middle of a sentence. Now, the word is pronounced at 183 Hz, which is quite a bit higher. If you were testing the word to find out if it bore low lexical tone, what would you decide based on Figure 10 or Figure 11? This is an example of how we need to examine words in different contexts to understand their patterns.

172 

 Siri G. Tuttle

Fig. 10: Lower Tanana shath ‘wart’ in citation

Fig. 11: Lower Tanana shath ‘wart’ in sentence context

Summary: distinctive tones assigned to words participate in prosodic interactions in larger domains as well. Sometimes, this interaction results in loss of information, as when an intonational pitch overrides a lexical tone. Assimilation or spreading, downstep, downdrift, and interaction with intonational contours are all documented in Native American languages.



Prosody beyond the word 

 173

7.6 Stress beyond the word Stress is the term used for patterns of prominence within words (see Gordon, this volume). When stress systems are predictable, their regular patterns may be assigned on the basis of syllable position, syllable quantity, rhythmic pattern, or some combination of these. However, stress may also be assigned to particular morpheme types (such as roots), or to a syllable in a certain position in a word. When words are placed in larger context, stress assignment can change due to constraints on stress class or lapse. However, phrases and utterances are also domains for prominence. In longer stretches of speech, certain word stresses are often promoted to the level of the “phrase accent,” a term originated by Gösta Bruce in his study of Swedish accentual patterns. Mirzayan’s (2010) study of Lakhota (lkt) documents phrasal accents in this Siouan language. Mirzayan’s work also shows how transcriptional techniques developed for very different languages can be adapted for the languages of North America. The ToBI (Tones and Break Indices) system was developed for English by Mary Beckman and others (see Beckman and Elam (1997), and it has also been adapted by Sun-Ah Jun (1993) for Korean and other languages. In fluent communication, speech rate is much quicker than in elicitation or teacher-pronunciation. This can mean that in a somewhat longer sequence, one word stress will be more prominent than others. In Lower Tanana, each word is stressed on its root, as well as on any full vowel and closed syllable. The most prominent word stress falls on the last stress in the word – usually a word root. In a multiple-word utterance, as shown in the Lower Tanana sentence in (3), the most prominent of these word stresses is the last one, and the others may be reduced in prominence. The ticks before stressed syllables indicate primary phrasal stress when high, and secondary stress when low. (3)          

Lower Tanana (Session Notes, November 17, 2005) Khedoch’ekhwdel’ikhde ghesdò’ kheˌdoˌch’ekhwˌdelˈ’ikhde ˌghesˈdò’ SchoolatPFV:1sSUB-sit:PFV ‘I sat in school’

Word stress in this language is cued by length of the vowel; a stressed vowel is nearly twice as long as an unstressed vowel (Tuttle 1998). The stem syllable -dò’ will have the strongest stress in the word. Because it comes at the end of the utterance as well, this syllable will also be the most prominent – and longest – in the utterance. Studies of stress often include consideration of both citation, or slow-speech form, and of connected speech in the form of spontaneous text or elicited reading. The results of studying material from these two sources may differ because of phrase-level patterns, so it is important to pay attention to the source of data and methods used for analysis. By studying a list of words recorded in isolation, boundary markings of declarative or

174 

 Siri G. Tuttle

list-like utterances can be learned. By examining the same words in connected speech, both word-level and utterance-level prosody can be clarified. Descriptions of intonational phenomena such as phrase accent may refer to fastspeech phenomena rather than connected speech. For example, Mesquakie (sac) (Voorhis 1971) is reported by Voorhis to have both deliberate and casual forms of speech, with the deliberate forms known at that time only by older speakers. There are numerous rhythmic adjustments described in this article as effects of rapid or casual speech, which might also be considered effects of word-level prosody. This article is somewhat unusual in that that fast-speech forms are described. Many descriptions of North American languages idealize away from these effects, a practice that has made the task of language learners far more difficult as they try to reconcile the speech they hear with idealized language as recorded in grammars. Caldecott (2016) demonstrates a phrase or “nuclear” accent in St’át’imcets (Salishan, lil), which occurs on the last full vowel of an utterance and makes clear that the nuclear accent is not used to mark focus, and not associated with the particle used to mark focus. Phrase accents are not always realized with higher pitch. In (3) above, we saw how word-level prominence is promoted to utterance-level prominence in connected speech in Lower Tanana. However, this prominence does not coincide with high pitch, due to the complex relationships among the language’s prosodic systems. Final lowering provides intonational boundary marking, so a final syllable will be pronounced at lower pitch as well as being lengthened, no matter its lexical tone, and still carry primary stress prominence. In (4), two low tones, one in denigi and one on the mode morphology of the verb, draw tones downward, as does the final intonational low tone. Syllables preceding and following low tones are high in comparison. The stress and pitch patterns are shown in (4). The L, M and H stand for low, mid and high pitch. (4)            

Lower Tanana (Session Notes, November 17, 2005) L                      L     H     H     M    L        L    L Denìgi k’osenàthdeyo Deˈnìgi ˌk’oseˌnàthdeˈyo Moose around-3S-1sOBJ-PFV-follow ‘The moose followed me around.’

The pattern in (4) shows how high pitches associate with a syllables that are stressed, but may not bear primary stress; both tonal and intonational patterns provide more of the pitch contour than stress does. There is much more that can be discovered about stress patterns in phrases. Because the acoustic correlate, or audible cue, for stress in languages can differ widely, different effects may be expected and should be explored. As in all areas of prosody in connected speech, it is clear here that isolating stress-related effects from those associated with lexical tone or intonation can be tricky. Patience and an open mind are very important to discovering patterns.



Prosody beyond the word 

 175

7.7 Information structure and intonation The study of information structure intersects with intonation when categories important to the tracking of information in discourse are signaled with prosodic contrasts – changes in pitch, duration or intensity (see Berge on information structure, this volume). Higher or changed pitch are often associated with focused categories in the languages that have been studied, but there are also other strategies for calling the listener’s attention to a segment of speech. Intonational marking of different kinds of focus is found in some North American languages. As Berge (this volume) shows, this term can be used to describe elements in an utterance that provide new (as opposed to given) information, or elements that contrast with alternative elements already present in the conversation. The contrasted-not-contrasted distinction interacts with other important informational distinctions, particularly Given-New. Givenness is a property of linguistic items that are already present in the discourse, while Newness is a property of items that are introduced; after introduction, they also become Givens (Krifka 2008). Katz and Selkirk (2011), looking at English intonation, also found differences in the expression of prominence for items that were New versus those that appeared in Contrast. Applying the techniques used for languages like English to the Native languages of North America requires attention to information structure strategies that do not involve prosodic marking. Fery, Skopeteas and Hoernig’s 2010 study of a variety of worldwide languages found that strategies for marking Newness and Contrast varied among syntactic, morphological, and prosodic expression. It is significant that the unity these authors find among such focusing strategies is that of moving focus toward the end of an utterance and keeping given objects toward the beginning. This pattern has been reported for languages of the Americas as well: Harbour et al (2012), for example, investigates information structure in Kiowa entirely in relation to the linear presentation of noun phrases, without consideration of prosodic marking, and finds a relationship between postverbal placement of noun phrases and transition points between phases of narrative. The authors suggest that further research into clause prosody would be a useful development. Methods for studying information structure in the field have been advanced by innovative empirical research, as discussed in Bohnemeyer (2015). Structured narrative, structured elicitation through the use of games, and other strategies for eliciting rarely used constructions assist in discovering how information structure is conveyed. Because both new information and contrasting information can be marked morphologically or lexically, languages do not always use intonation to indicate the presence of focus. However, some do. Mirzayan (2010) found that in addition to a lexical marker for focus in Lakhota, an intonational pattern including a high accent was frequently, but not invariably present. Dene languages employ multiple strategies for expressing newness and contrast, including manipulation of word order, morphological marking, and prosodic marking.

176 

 Siri G. Tuttle

Lovick and Tuttle (2013) compared texts in three Alaska Dene languages and found morphological, syntactic, and prosodic strategies used for marking newness and contrast. When prosodic prominence is used, higher pitch can be involved, but interactions with low tone (present in both Upper and Lower Tanana) can affect the expression of information structure. However, the use of lexical markers that encode information structure is common in all three languages. Clearly the availability of this strategy for tracking information structure makes prosody redundant in many cases.

7.8 Additional considerations The study of prosody in connected speech can be complicated by the previous experience of researchers. Because prosodic patterns are not usually written, and are governed by subconscious knowledge, it can be particularly difficult to catch ourselves when we (again subconsciously) apply assumptions about prosodic patterns that would be correct for our first language, but not for the language we are studying. It is very easy for our first language prosodic systems to interfere in our understanding of the functions of the prosody we are listening to. For English speakers, it is easy to confuse tone or accent for stress or prominence, since we do not use pitch distinctively in English. It is also easy for us to misinterpret pitch or voice quality effects for markers of speaker attitude, when they may be doing an entirely different job in the language. Understanding of social expectations in the language community is essential to working with prosodic questions. In the present context of language endangerment, methodologies are emerging that can bring together these different areas of knowledge. Prosodic research especially benefits from research directed by language community members, who have a great deal of social and pragmatic understanding even when they have not had the opportunity to become fluent speakers of their community’s language. Non-community members with complementary skills can learn to adjust their questions and analytic approach to follow the lead of these researchers.

7.9 Conclusion In this chapter several aspects of phrase-level prosody have been addressed: those expressing some aspect of a lexical tone system; those involved with stress; and those involved in intonation. Examples of interaction between word-level and phrase or utterance level systems have also been discussed. Examples of the study of prosody beyond the word include many descriptions that start with the goal of describing one prosodic system, but where it is found that some other system, competing for the means of expression, must be described in order to get



Prosody beyond the word 

 177

to the system that is the intended focus of the study. However, numerous descriptions of prosodic phenomena focus on larger domains for functions involving pitch, rhythm, and variation in intensity. The resolution of competing prosodic systems from languages in contact is a fruitful area for research that has not been fully explored for most North American languages. The reason for this may lie in respect for conservative versions of the languages, and for some learners and researchers, in a desire to master or understand the language as spoken by those least affected by language contact. So many aspects of language find expression in prosody that in order to learn about the form or function of a system, it is very important to keep track of context. For this reason, despite potentially difficult conditions where languages are endangered, it is important to continue the study of prosody, especially if members of the speech community can design and carry out research. This does not mean, of course, that we should stop doing our homework before we make new recordings, or that we should ignore the analyses of previous researchers. We should also be making use of archived narrative. Bringing old narration back to speakers and learners for commentary, re-narration and discussion can create a rich environment for learning about the function and form of prosody, as well as many other topics of interest. It can also stimulate the production of new narration that can add to archived material for study. For many languages of North America, prosodic study is incomplete. There are many opportunities not only to describe prosodic systems, but to define the questions learners, teachers and linguists will be asking about these systems for a long time. Naturally, we worry about describing prosody that may have been introduced through language contact, or that may show differences from patterns described long ago. The possibility of linguistic change should not lesson our curiosity but intensify it. If we are lucky, earlier observations are available for comparison. But if we are the first ones to ask, that is a delightful privilege as well.

References Barthmaier, Paul. 2004. Intonation units in Okanagan. In Donna Gerdts & Lisa Matthewson (eds.), Studies in Salish linguistics in honor of M. Dale Kinkade, 30–34. (University of Montana Occasional Papers in Linguistics 17). Missoula: University of Montana. Beck, David. 1999. Words and prosodic phrasing in Lushootseed. In T. Allan Hall & Ursula Kleinhenz (eds.), Studies on the phonological word, 23–46. Amsterdam: Benjamins. Beckman, Mary E. & Gayle Ayers Elam. 1997. Guidelines for ToBI Labeling. Columbus, Ohio: Ohio State University Research Foundation. Beckman, Mary E. & Jennifer J. Venditti. 2011. Intonation. In John Goldsmith, Jason Riggle & Alan C. L. Yu (eds.), Handbook of Phonological Theory, 485–532. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell. Berez, Andrea. 2011. Prosody as a genre-distinguishing feature in Ahtna: A quantitative approach. Functions of Language 18(2). 210–236.

178 

 Siri G. Tuttle

Bird, Sonya. 2004. Lheidli intervocalic consonants: Phonetic and morphological effects. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 34(1). 69–91. Boersma, Paul & David Weenink. 2018. Praat: doing phonetics by computer [computer program]. Version 6.0.37, retrieved 14 March 2018 from http://www.praat.org/. Bohnemeyer, Jürgen. 2015. A Practical Epistemology for Semantic Elicitation in the Field and Elsewhere. In M. Ryan Bochnak & Lisa Matthewson (eds.), Methodologies in Semantic Fieldwork, 13–46. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Bruce, Gösta. 1977. Swedish Word Accent in Sentence Perspective. Lund: Gleerup. Caldecott, Marion. 2016. St’át’imcets intonation contours: a preliminary study. Canadian Journal of Linguistics/Revue canadienne de linguistique 61(2). 119–155. doi: 10.1017/cnj.2016.11 Caldecott, Marion, & Ewa Czaykowska-Higgins. 2012. Prosodic phrasing in Nxaʔamxčín (Salish) declarative clauses. Canadian Acoustics 40(3). 16–17. Chafe, W. 1976. Givenness, contrastiveness, definiteness, subjects, topics and points of view. In Charles N. Li (ed.), Subject and topic, 27–55. New York: Academic Press. Charlie, Moses, Michael E. Krauss & James Kari. 1991. The Moses Charlie Collection. Manuscript. Fairbanks: Alaska Native Language Archive: TNMN981K1991d. DeReuse, Willem. 2006. A Practical Grammar of San Carlos Apache. München: LINCOM Europa. Gordon, Matthew & Pamela Munro. 2007. A phonetic study of final vowel lengthening in Chickasaw. International Journal of American Linguistics 73. 293–330. Gordon, Matthew, Jack B. Martin & Linda Langley. 2015. Some Phonetic Structures of Koasati. International Journal of American Linguistics 81(1). 83–118. Harbour, Daniel, Laurel J. Watkins & David Adger. 2012. Information structure, Discourse Structure, and Noun Phrase Position in Kiowa. International Journal of American Linguistics 78(1). 97–126. Holton, Gary. 2005. Pitch, Tone and Intonation in Tanacross. In Sharon Hargus & Keren Rice (eds.), Athabaskan Prosody, 249–274. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Jun, Sun-Ah. 1993. The Phonetics and Phonology of Korean Prosody. PhD dissertation. Ohio State University. Kinkade, M. Dale & Anthony Mattina. 1996. Discourse. In William C. Sturtevant (ed.), Handbook of North American Languages. Vol. 17: Languages, 244–274. Washington: Smithsonian Institution. Landar, Herbert. 1963. Navaho Syntax. Ph.D. dissertation, Yale University. Lovick, Olga. 2020. A Grammar of Upper Tanana. Volume 1. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Lovick, Olga & Siri Tuttle. 2012. Prosody of Dena’ina Narrative Discourse. International Journal of American Linguistics 78(3). 293–334. Martin, Jack & Keith Johnson. 2002. An Acoustic Study of “Tonal Accent” in Creek. International Journal of American Linguistics 68(1). 28–50. Miner, Kenneth L. 1979. Dorsey’s Law in Winnebago-Chiwere and Winnebago Accent. International Journal of American Linguistics 45(1). 25–33. Mirzayan, Armik. 2010. Lakota Intonation and Prosody. Linguistics Graduate Theses & Dissertations. Paper 7. Ph.D. dissertation. University of Colorado. Nespor, Marina & Irene Vogel. 1986. Prosodic Phonology. Dordrecht: Foris Publications. Palakurthy, Kayla. 2019. Prosody in Diné Bizaad Narratives: A Quantitative Investigation of Acoustic Correlates. International Journal of American Linguistics 85(4). 49–531. Pitkin, Harvey. 1984. Wintu Grammar. (University of California Publications in Linguistics 94). Berkeley: University of California Press. Ritter, John. 1989. Han Literacy Session, Whitehorse, Yukon, December, 1989. Manuscript. Fairbanks: Alaska Native Language Archive. HN976R1989. Sivertsen, Eva. 1956. Pitch Problems in Kiowa. International Journal of American Linguistics 22(2). 117–130. Thompson, Laurence & M. Terry Thompson. 1966. A Fresh Look at Tillamook Phonology. International Journal of American Linguistics 32(4). 313–319.



Prosody beyond the word 

 179

Tuttle, Siri G. 2012. Language and music in the songs of Minto, Alaska. In Niclas Burenhult, Arthur Holmer, Anastasia Karlsson, Håkan Lundström & Jan-Olof Svantesson (eds.), Language Documentation and Description, vol 10: Special Issue on Humanities of the lesser-known: New directions in the description, documentation and typology of endangered languages and musics, 82–112. London: SOAS. Tuttle, Siri G. 2008. Phonetics and word definition in Ahtna Athabaskan. Linguistics 46(2). 439–470. Tuttle, Siri G. 1999. Preliminary findings on intonation in San Carlos Apache. Paper presented at the 1999 Athabaskan Languages Conference, Albuquerque, New Mexico. Tuttle, Siri G. 1998. Metrical and tonal structures in Tanana Athabaskan. Ph.D dissertation, Washington: University of Washington. Tuttle, Siri & Olga Lovick. 2013. Prosodic marking of information structure in Alaskan Athabascan text. Workshop on Phonetic/phonological typology and fieldwork, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig. August 14, 2013. Voorhis, Paul H. 1971. New Notes on the Mesquakie (Fox) Language, International Journal of American Linguistics 37(1). 63–75. Xu, Yi. 2011. Speech Prosody: a methodological review. Journal of Speech Sciences 1(1). 85–115.

II Words

Fernando Zúñiga

8 What is a word? Abstract: The recent literature on wordhood has shown the importance of disentangling patterns of sound (the phonological domain) from patterns of grammar (the morphosyntactic domain) for defining units that can be meaningfully called “words.” The present paper outlines the main issues involved when identifying, analyzing, and classifying these phonological words and grammatical words from the perspective of Indigenous North American languages. It summarizes what we already know about how diverse and elusive they can be, as well as the areas where we need further research to learn more about complex and heterogeneous phenomena related to word(-like) units.

8.1 Introduction Both language-internally and cross-linguistically, there are different kinds of units that linguists, teachers, and learners like to call words, and it is important (i) to tell them apart terminologically and analytically, and (ii) to see that they show significant, and perhaps also systematic, variation. The present paper outlines the main issues raised by such structural units in the context of Indigenous North American languages. Section 2 presents the two main wordhood domains, namely sound structure and syntagmatic relations (i.  e., how units are chained together to build phrases, clauses, and sentences), while Section 3 addresses some possible relationships between units identified in each domain. Section 4 deals with the subtypes of grammatical words found along two particular dimensions that have received much attention in the literature, namely the morphological make-up of grammatical words and the kinds of grammatical words distinguished by the lexicon and the grammar of a language (i.  e., “word classes” or “parts of speech”). Section  5 closes the paper with some summarizing remarks and suggestions for further research.

8.2 Wordhood domains Different units of linguistic structure can be distinguished in two domains: phonology and morphosyntax. Intermediate-level units called “words” are normally expected to show not only higher autonomy than smaller units but also stronger cohesion than larger units, in either domain. Before discussing these domains, however, some comments on orthographic “words” are in order. The definitions of kinds of “words” introduced in this section are summarized in Table 1 below for ease of reference.

https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-008

184 

 Fernando Zúñiga

Tab. 1: Selected kinds of units Orthographic word

In some writing systems, uninterrupted string of graphic symbols preceded and followed by some boundary sign (e.  g., space, punctuation mark, etc.)

Phonological word

Unit of phonology, smaller than an intonational phrase and larger than a segment, defined by phonological rules and features

Grammatical word

Unit of grammar (= morphology and syntax), smaller than a phrase and larger than a morpheme, defined by grammatical behavior

Morpheme

Minimal form-meaning pair in language; it can be either autonomous or bound in grammar; it may or may not correspond to a simple phonological unit

Clitic

Morpheme with a mixed autonomy status (e.  g., an autonomous grammatical word that is part of a phonological word)

8.2.1 Orthographic words Separating orthographic words (e.  g., through spaces) is only about one thousand years old (Saenger 1997). Most importantly, even though the use of spaces is connected to some analysis of linguistic structures, orthographic words are conventional and related to other kinds of words rather idiosyncratically. The treatment given to compound words and to functional elements varies significantly. For instance, Spanish object clitics are written separately before verbs but as parts of the verb when they follow, as in e.  g., me lo da ‘s/he gives it to me’ vs. dámelo ‘give it to me!’, but this does not reflect any difference in the linguistic status of those elements. Compare also English bedroom with living-room and pairs like instead of and in spite of. Arbitrary though orthographic words generally are, normal language users, language learners and teachers, and linguists are accustomed to them. The influence of the orthographic conventions of English, French, and Spanish in North America is considerable to this very day, however—which poses several problems in the present context. First, those conventions are not perfectly designed or implemented to meet the complex needs of their own writing communities; especially the conventions of English and French are quite idiosyncratic and difficult to master. Second, many Indigenous languages show structures, both in their phonology and their grammar, that are so different from those of the European languages of reference, that devising and implementing suitable (European-inspired) writing systems and orthographies for them is a challenging task. Moreover, norms are subject to competing motivations that are not always easy to harmonize; striking a satisfactory balance between a convention that is relatively easy to learn (and therefore probably close in many concrete and abstract respects to the convention of whatever European language is dominant in primary school) and a convention that does justice to the Indigenous language regarding not only its structures but also its cultural and political context (which may depart in numerous respects from



What is a word? 

 185

some of the familiar European conventions) is usually a formidable job for interdisciplinary teams of people intimately familiar with the language and its speakers. Without going into specifics here, it seems that consistency is the single most important requisite for a useful and successful writing convention. Considering structural issues in particular, and irrespective of whether the script is alphabetic or syllabic, boundary marks like spaces should ideally separate units of grammar, which is the implicit default option in most conventions. Spaces are not normally used to separate exclusively units of sound; they are only variably and irregularly used with this function. Trying to use spaces for both purposes in any kind of principled way would probably lead to numerous inconsistencies and difficulties that would most likely incentivize speakers not to write at all (see Section 3).

8.2.2 Phonological words At their simplest, phonological words consist of smaller units of the stream of speech called segments (consonants and vowels). Sequences of segments smaller than phonological words are morae, syllables, and feet (see Urbanczyk 2023 [this volume]); they make up what the specialized literature calls the “metrical hierarchy.” For instance, the English phonological word segments /s/, /e/, /g/, /m/, /e/, /n/, and /t/ can be grouped in two syllables /seg/ and /ment/, which constitutes a foot /seg.ment/, which is also a phonological word. Sequences of phonological words constitute larger units like clitic groups, phonological phrases, and intonational phrases; this is the “prosodic hierarchy” (see Gordon [this volume]; Tuttle [this volume]). They can be defined language-specifically by segmental and prosodic features/rules. Segmental features include structural and phonotactic constraints. For instance, verbal phonological words are minimally bisyllabic in Mohawk (Iroquoian), i.  e., they consist of at least two syllables; smaller units are parts of verbal phonological words (Adler 2016). Example (1) shows how an initial i and a medial e appear on a verb form that would otherwise have no vowels (*kks); these vowels do not contribute to the unit’s meaning but merely make it a formally admissible phonological word: (1)        

Mohawk (Iroquoian; Marianne Mithun, p.c.) Í:keks. i-k-e-k-s proth-1sg.A-ep-eat-hab ‘I eat.’

Moreover, phonological-unit boundaries are frequently restricted. For instance, most consonant clusters can occur only medially in a phonological word in Cayuga (Iroquoian); initially and finally in a phonological word, they are severely constrained, e.  g., kr, sn, and tsy can occur only initially and medially, and nr, and kth can occur only medially (Dyck 1999). Lastly, segments are often realized differently at phonological word

186 

 Fernando Zúñiga

boundaries. In Cup’ik (Eskimoan), for instance, stem-final uvular and velar sounds are realized as continuants word-medially (or before a sonorant-initial enclitic), as plosives word-finally and utterance-finally (or before non-sonorant-initial enclitic), or as either continuant or plosive word-finally when followed by a sonorant-initial phonological word (Woodbury 2002). Prosodic features include the sensitivity of tone and stress assignment to phonological-unit boundaries. For instance, morphemes in Arapaho (Algonquian) bear different pitches that are redistributed at the phonological word level (Cowell and Moss 2008: 22–40). In (2), the verb stem ciinén- ‘put (something) down’ has high pitch on its short second syllable; this surfaces normally if the following syllable is low pitched, as in (a) (iinén-o becomes iinéno), but the pitch pattern is altered—in order to avoid inadmissible sequences of high-pitch syllables—if the following syllable is long and high pitched, as in (b) (iinén-óú becomes íínenóú). Such redistribution occurs within phonological words, not in larger units. (2)                

Arapaho (Algonquian; Cowell and Moss 2008: 29, my glosses) a. Ceniinénowoo.   ciinén-owoo   put.down-1sg   ‘I am putting it down.’ b. Ceníínenóú’u.   ciinén-óú’u   put.down-3pl   ‘They are putting it down.’

Fixed stress is not widespread in Indigenous North American languages, but it can be found on the first syllable (e.  g., in Comanche; Charney 1993: 40–44) or the penultimate syllable (e.  g., in Sm’álgyax; Dunn 1995: 5) (see also Michelson 1988 for Lake Iroquoian). Other relevant phenomena include segmental harmony, i.  e., where phonological segments agree in a particular phonological feature; vowel harmony is especially frequent (e.  g., in Sahaptian, Interior Salishan, Washo, and Yokutsan; Mithun 1999: 478–479, 494, 557, 568), but consonant harmony is also found (e.  g., coronal harmony in Athabaskan; Hansson 2007). For an example of vowel harmony, see in (3) how the verb stem wé·yik‘go across’ and the 1st-person-singular affix -se (a) both adapt their “recessive” e vowel to the “dominant” a vowel of the recent-past suffix in (b). This process is found within phonological words, but not in larger phonological units. (3)        

Nez Perce (Sahaptian; Aoki 1966: 760, my glosses) a. Wé·yikse.   wé·yik-se   go.across-1sg   ‘I am going down.’



       

What is a word? 

b.      

 187

Wá·yiksaqa. wé·yik-se-qa go.across-3pl-rec.pst ‘I went across recently.’

The usual kinds of segmental and prosodic rules used to define phonological words are summarized in Table 2 below. Tab. 2: Summary of potentially relevant phonological word regularities Structural ­constraints

Do some units show a minimal number of morae, syllables, or feet? → If so, these units may be phonological words.

Phonotactic ­constraints

Can/must some segments (not) appear at the edge of some units? Are some segments regularly realized differently at the edge of some units? → If so, these units may be phonological words.

Tone/stress

Within what kind of unit does tone and/or stress become (re)assigned? → These units may be phonological words.

Harmony phenomena

Within what kind of unit do consonants, vowels, or features (e.  g., nasality, rhoticity) harmonize? → These units may be phonological words.

At least two factors complicate this simple picture. First, since the prosodic hierarchy does not necessarily include the metrical hierarchy, having the phonological word as the place where both meet cannot be taken for granted. See Selkirk (1986), Inkelas (1989, 1993) and Dyck (2009) for arguments in favor of treating these hierarchies as independent from each other. Second, and more importantly, phonological words need be neither present nor unambiguous everywhere; languages may have one kind of phonological word, several, or none (Schiering, Bickel, and Hildebrandt 2010). For instance, the string in (4) from Cup’ik as analyzed by Woodbury (2002) can be phonologically grouped in two different ways (according to different sets of rules). Small phonological words exclude, while large phonological words include, enclitics like the reportative =gguq: (4)            

Cup’ik (Woodbury 2002: 91–93) small phonological word

added unit

large phonological word mallu-ssu-tu-llini-luni gguq beached.whale-hunt-always-apparently-3sg.app rep Mallussutulliniluni-gguq. ‘S/he is apparently always hunting beached whales, it is said.’

     

188 

 Fernando Zúñiga

8.2.3 Grammatical words Grammatical words consist of smaller units called morphemes and appear in larger units (e.  g., phrases and clauses). They can be defined language-specifically by criteria based on structural cohesion and behavior. Before presenting these criteria, however, some introductory remarks on morphemes are in order. The morpheme is the smallest meaningful unit of a language.1 For instance, the English expression the ducklings consists of four morphemes: the, duck, -ling, and -s. Morphemes are typically classified as roots (which carry specific, “lexical,” semantic content; here: duck) and affixes (which carry general, “grammatical,” semantic content; here: -ling and -s), sometimes further distinguishing clitics (which, as discussed in Section 3.1, are diverse; here: possibly the). In many languages, roots can be expanded to form bases/stems via compounding and/or affixation; in languages that allow simpler units, roots, stems, and grammatical words may exactly correspond to each other (like English duck). Regarding structural cohesion, languages show morphemic strings that show integrity along several parameters, namely insertion potential, displacement potential, fixed ordering, and cross-slot dependencies (Bickel and Zúñiga 2017). Insertion potential stipulates the conditions under which an element can interrupt the string. Restrictive grammatical words thus defined would ideally be uninterruptible, and noun incorporation—a possible instance of “free forms (=words) interrupting (other) words”—has been sometimes seen as potentially posing a problem (see, e.  g., the brief, and incomplete, discussion in Julien 2002: 34–35 of some Pawnee data). Nevertheless, it is extremely rare to find true “free forms” as nominal incorporates in Indigenous North American languages (Marianne Mithun, p.c.); incorporates are normally stems, rather than fullfledged grammatical words.2 Displacement potential refers to constitutive elements of words being moved or copied by a general operation independently of each other, and fixed ordering refers to a particular order not being determined arbitrarily (e.  g., rather than by semantics or pragmatics). An instance of the latter can be seen in (5) from Blackfoot; the verb root ssi ‘wipe’ can appear in the simple stems ssi-i ‘wipe (something inanimate)’ and ssi ‘wipe (something animate)’, but also in the complex stem ssi-ika-asi ‘wipe one’s feet’, with the so-called medial -ika ‘foot’. The order ssi-ika is fixed; *ika-ssi is unacceptable.

1 Many studies use the term morph instead of morpheme, while others use them interchangeably, but there is no consensus as to how to best employ this pair—which is, at least in principle, roughly parallel to phone (or segment) and phoneme. I use the more familiar term morpheme here. 2 Incorporated stems that are routinely also grammatical words are much less rare in Indigenous South American languages.



(5)        

What is a word? 

 189

Blackfoot (Algonquian; Dunham 2009: 5, glosses adapted) Anná Conrad áíssiikawatsii. ann-wa Conrad á-ssi-ika-atsi dem-prox.sg C. ipfv-wipe-foot-ai ‘Conrad is washing his feet.’

Lastly, cross-slot dependencies refer to cases where neither constitutive element can be deleted without simultaneously deleting another element. For instance, in (6), the negative prefix Imáát- requires the concomitant appearance of the non-affirmative suffix for singular 1st or 2nd person -hpa: (6)                

Blackfoot (Algonquian; Frantz 2009: 18, 82, glosses adapted) a. Nitáóoyi.   nit-á-ooyi   1-ipfv-eat.ti   ‘I am eating.’ b. Nimáátáóoyihpa.   n-Imáát-á-ooyi-hpa   1-neg-ipfv-eat.ti-naff.1sg/2sg   ‘I am not eating.’

Constraints on displacement, re-ordering, and deletion are widespread in North American language families showing highly complex verb morphology, like Algonquian, Athabaskan, and Iroquoian. Future studies might show specific tests for grammatical words to be useful in the North American context, even though they do not yield cross-linguistically consistent results. First, the impossibility of eliding units smaller than grammatical words in coordination (also called cross-slot dependency diagnostic) works well in many languages. The second test is a special case of the displacement potential and concerns extractability (i.  e., the possibility of displacing a noun from its original location in a larger nominal unit), which can distinguish Noun-Noun compounds from Noun Phrases (as in Italian; Bisetto and Scalise 1999: 38–39).3 Nevertheless, extractability is probably best seen as a property of (referential) phrases and can be restricted in some languages (Haspelmath 2011: 52). The third test concerns anaphoric islandhood, i.  e., expressions whose parts cannot be anaphorically related to other parts of the sentence. Outbound anaphora (i.  e., an external pronoun referring to an element of a grammatical word) is primarily determined by semantic and pragmatic factors (Ward, Sproat, and McKoon 1991). For instance, in English weapons of mass destruction are controversial because its effects are

3 In the compound nave ospedale ‘hospital boat’, ospedale is unextractable: *ospedale, hanno costruito una nave _ ‘hospital, they have built a _ boat’ is ungrammatical. In the noun phrase trasporto dei passeggeri ‘passenger transport’, by contrast, dei passeggeri is extractable: dei passeggeri, è efficiente il trasporto _ ‘of the passengers, the transportation is efficient’ is grammatical.

190 

 Fernando Zúñiga

terrible, the anaphoric pronoun its cannot refer to destruction; in cases like McCartyites are now puzzled by him, the pronoun him can refer to McCarthy, because the latter is a name and therefore referential (Haspelmath 2011: 51). By contrast, inbound anaphora (i.  e., an internal, referential, pronoun within a grammatical word) is impossible in some languages (e.  g., English: *McCarthy was happy that many him-ites were at the meeting is ungrammatical) but possible in others (e.  g., Georgian: Ševardnaʒe icnobs? K’i, imis-iania ‘Does she know Shevardnaze? Yes, she is a him-ite’; Harris 2006: 199). Another criterion concerns morphophonological idiosyncrasies, which are allegedly common in stem-affix combinations but excluded in host-clitic combinations (Zwicky and Pullum 1983: 505). This cross-linguistic tendency can be relevant language-specifically; in Blackfoot, for instance, some noun stems show a slightly different form after prefixes, like ponoká- ‘elk’ appearing as -innoka- after sik- ‘black’ in siksínnoka- ‘black elk’ (Frantz 2009: 78–80). See also § 8.3.1 for more on cliticization. Other cohesion-related criteria used to identify grammatical words appear to be less useful. One criterion is similar to an old “semantic word” notion but is nowadays usually framed in terms closer to Bloomfield’s (1933: 178) “Minimal Free Forms”: can a given unit occur on its own as a well-formed, complete utterance? In principle, this test helps to distinguish larger units that can occur on their own (whether grammatical words or phrases) from smaller units that cannot (e.  g., affixes), but its implementation is often difficult. In English, for instance, autonomous units exclude not only functional elements like articles and prepositions but also some transitive verbs, which are not free forms but are traditionally regarded as “words.” Moreover, the test cannot adequately distinguish between compounds and phrases (e.  g., English compounds like firewater or blood(-)red consist of free forms that would qualify as phrases). Two further criteria are even more problematic. The first concerns deviations from one-to-one correspondences between meanings and forms (zero marking, multiple exponence, cumulative exponence, and morphomic patterns), which would apply to affixes.4 Nevertheless, since all four deviation types exist in syntax as well (Spencer 2001), the test does not distinguish affixes from grammatical words (Haspelmath 2011: 54–59). The second criterion is related to the interruptibility criterion mentioned above and is based on the possibility of splitting up units larger than, or identical to, grammat4 Zero marking consists in one meaning being expressed by the absence of form; consider, for instance, the opposition between the Blackfoot stems sina-aki- ‘draw, write’ (ai), sina-i- ‘draw/take a picture of (inanimate)’ (ti), and sina- ‘draw/take a picture of (animate)’ (ta), where the latter stem has a zero socalled “final.” In other words, the absence of form carries a meaning analogous to the one carried by the non-zero forms -aki and -i in the other two stems (Frantz & Russell 2017: 252). Multiple exponence consists in several forms expressing one meaning, e.  g., Latin rēg-s-ī (spelled rexi) ‘I directed’, where the perfect is encoded three times, viz. by stem vowel length (ē), by a temporal-aspectual suffix (-s), and by the person-number suffix (-ī) (Haspelmath 2011: 46). Cumulative exponence consists in one form expressing several meanings, e.  g., Blackfoot -wa, which usually encodes both ‘singular’ and ‘proximate’. Lastly, morphomic patterns consists in one form expressing no meaning, e.  g., Blackfoot m and n at the end of some nominal stems in some forms but not in others (pokón-wa ‘ball’ vs. pokó-íksi ‘balls’) (Frantz 2009: 11).



What is a word? 

 191

ical words via pauses (Hockett 1958: 166, Langacker 1972: 41). It is unclear, however, whether this is better understood as a phonological word criterion (Dixon and Aikenvald 2002: 24). Most probably, pausing is not closely related to language structure per se but derives from production and processing conventions (Haspelmath 2011: 37). Finally, a grammatical word criterion related to behavior rather than cohesion concerns the selectivity of dependent elements. Affixes tend to be highly selective as to their possible hosts (e.  g., only nominal or verbal stems), whereas function words (e.  g., prepositions) are frequently less so. This criterion is used often, but non-selective nonwords (“edge inflection,” Zwicky 1987; “promiscuous inflection,” Stump 2001: 126) pose a potential problem. There are non-selective tonal markers, for instance, which cannot be words because they are not segmental units.5 Similarly, English ’s is non-selective but triggers haplology (i.  e., the deletion of identical consecutive strings, as in English the kids’ ideas), which is typical of morphological markers. Simply accepting non-selective markers as parts of words—as many studies do—renders the selectivity test useless (Haspelmath 2011: 47). The most useful criteria for defining grammatical words presented above are summarized in Table 3 below. Tab. 3: Summary of potentially useful grammatical word diagnostics Insertion potential

Can strings of adjacent elements be interrupted by autonomous elements? → If insertion potential is high, those adjacent elements probably belong to ­different grammatical words.

Displacement potential

Can either morpheme in a string be moved/copied independently of the other? → If they can, those adjacent elements probably belong to different grammatical words.

Fixed ordering

Is a particularstring order arbitrarily fixed, or does it allow the converse order with a different meaning? → If the order is fixed, the adjacent elements in the string may belong to the same grammatical word.

Cross-slot dependencies

Can either morpheme in a string be deleted without simultaneously deleting another morpheme? → If they cannot, those elements may belong to the same grammatical word.

5 For instance, in Òko, a Benue-Congo language of Nigeria, the locative is expressed as a high tone on the first syllable, cf. ùgbègbèn ‘mirror’ vs. úgbègbèn ‘in the mirror’ (Atoyebi 2009: 94).

192 

 Fernando Zúñiga

8.3 Relationships between phonological words and grammatical words Even though phonological words and grammatical words that have the same form can be found in natural languages, such matches cannot be simply assumed to hold everywhere. Mismatches seem to be rather frequent, but they have not been systematically studied from a typological perspective. Preliminary exploration of phonological and “long” grammatical words suggests that mismatches between phonological words and grammatical words probably show considerable variation (Bickel and Zúñiga 2017). I limit myself here to outlining some important kinds of mismatch in the North American context, starting with elements that show grammatical autonomy but limited phonological autonomy (§ 8.3.1) and then proceeding to other mismatches (§ 8.3.2).

8.3.1 Grammatical autonomy with limited phonological autonomy One possible mismatch between phonological and morphosyntactic structures consists in elements showing high grammatical autonomy but limited phonological autonomy. Special cases thereof are known as clitics. The term ‘clitic’ is used in fairly disparate ways in the linguistic literature. Older descriptive studies use it to refer to units that somehow fall between words and affixes— typically without being technical about the definition of words. Recent theoretical and/ or comparative studies use the term more technically,6 to refer to prosodically deficient grammatical words (e.  g., Klavans 1985).7 Some studies in this vein see clitics as having the distribution of (functional) grammatical words and the form and prosodic properties of affixes. More precisely, Spencer and Luís (2012) define their canonical clitic as follows. Distributionally, words and canonical clitics occur non-selectively at the edge of phrases and show wide scope in coordination, while affixes have limited host choice and narrow scope in coordination. Formally, affixes and canonical clitics are usually prosodically unspecified monomoraic consonant-vowel syllables that are dependent, while words are only minimally monomoraic consonant-vowel syllables that are subject to phonotactic constraints. According to this view, in English the canonical clitic auxil6 Recent studies in the Chomskyan tradition also have a technical definition, distinguishing as they do between clitics and affixes rather sharply. They typically employ a bundle of parameters shown by well-behaved instances of either category, including formal boundedness, obligatoriness, position, and tam-conditioned allomorphy (see, e.  g., Nevins 2011 and Woolford 2016). 7 Occasionally, studies distinguish between kinds of bound morphemes based on morphosyntactic cohesion only, without employing phonological criteria but using the term clitic nonetheless. For instance, Watanabe defines “clitics” in Mainland Comox (Salishan) as showing higher insertion and displacement potential than “affixes,” and as not showing morphophonological idiosyncrasies (2003: 38–47).



What is a word? 

 193

iary ’s in he’s come differs both from the suffix s in books and from the strings book and books, which are both grammatical words. Some scholars resort to additional criteria to justify treating particular elements as something different from both words and affixes, frequently without providing detailed prosodic and grammatical characterizations for such elements. Most notably—and potentially problematically, see the end of § 8.2.3—, these criteria include a grammatical autonomy property, namely selectivity: non-selective grammatical elements are routinely classified as clitics (see, e.  g., Mithun 1999: 39). For instance, the Plains Cree (Algonquian) elements ni and ki are typically treated as clitics because they can occur on both verbs (e.  g., nimâcîn ‘I hunt’ and kimâcîn ‘you (sg) hunt’) and nouns (e.  g., nitem ‘my horse/dog’ and kitem ‘your (sg) horse/dog’). Other common criteria are whether the elements in question can be assigned a part of speech, and whether they have a dedicated position in the clause; residual “particles” on the one hand, and second-position or clause-final elements on the other, are prone to clitic treatment (e.  g., the Lakota interrogative marker he in (7) below). Older studies also consider whether the elements in question have “full” counterparts that occur under complementary conditions; those that do not are less frequently regarded as clitics. Sometimes cliticization is defined more widely. For instance, while some clitics appear to be canonical clitics and form part of a phonological word defined by phonological features and rules, others, which is presumably more common, are “simply added—as an extra, unstressed syllable—to a fully articulated phonological word after all processes and rules have applied” (Dixon and Aikhenvald 2002: 25). I call the latter annexed clitics here. Different kinds of elements frequently receive clitic treatment across languages. For instance, so-called enclitics are closer to canonical clitics in Lillooet (Salishan) but to annexed clitics in Creek (Muskogean). The former (e.  g., specifiers like ti= and =a in ti=n-citxw=a [art=my-house=reinf] ‘my house’, or the question marker =ha in nuk’wʔ-ancíh-as=ha? [help-tr-you-he=q] ‘did he help you?’; Van Eijk 1997: 46–47) tend to remain unstressed, but they are within the domain of stress of phonological words and can bear stress under appropriate circumstances (Van Eijk 1997: 17). By contrast, some Creek case markers like subjective =(i)t in sokha-há:tka=t ‘opossum (sbj)’ and nonsubjective =(i)n in sokha-há:tka=n ‘opossum (nsbj)’ are outside the domain of stress (Martin 2011: 80). These elements also appear depending on several grammatical, semantic, and pragmatic factors and occur at the edge of NPs rather than nouns (Martin 2011: 336–343). Perhaps even more frequently, different kinds of elements receive underspecified clitic treatment in the same language. For instance, recent grammatical descriptions of Lakota (Siouan) treat as enclitics a number of post-verbal elements that express notions related to polarity, tense, aspect, modality, and evidentiality (e.  g., Rood and Taylor 1996: 473  f and Ullrich 2011: 820  f ). These elements, however, are phonologically heterogeneous: some are integrated into the verb and closer to canonical clitics (e.  g., unreal/ potential ktA and negative šni) while others are autonomous and perhaps best seen as annexed clitics or even nonclitic particles (e.  g., evidential kéyA and male assertive

194 

 Fernando Zúñiga

yeló). The examples in (7) illustrate two such elements. The unreal marker ktA in (a) is clearly part of the preceding phonological word ú ‘come’ (regarding stress, nasalization, and other phonological processes/rules). By contrast, the hearsay evidential kéyA in (b) bears its own stress and shows other signs of autonomy; it originated in a Proto-Siouan verb *ke·he ‘say (the preceding)’, and it can also be found in a plural form kéyapi, which suggests it has grammaticalized only incompletely (Rankin et al. 2002: 197): (7)                

Lakota (Siouan; Ullrich 2011: 821–822, my glosses) a. Ú kta he?   ú=ktA=he   come=unreal=q   ‘Will he come?’ b. Tokhéčela máni kéye.   tokhečéla máni kéyA   barely walk evid   ‘It is said he was barely walking.’

8.3.2 Other mismatches between phonology and morphosyntax One possible case of mismatch consists in elements showing high phonological autonomy but limited grammatical autonomy. I have called such mirror images of clitics as found in Mapudungun (unclassified, South America) anti-clitics (Zúñiga 2014). Simple instances thereof would be long, cohesive grammatical words that consist of several phonological words, but it is still rather unclear whether complex grammatical words in Eskimoan, Wakashan, Salishan, Algonquian, or Athabaskan can actually behave in this way. To my knowledge, there are no comparative studies on such phenomena. The verbal grammatical words of Blackfoot may be such a case of structurally cohesive units that are phonologically looser. Consider the verbal strings given in (8): (8)                      

Blackfoot (Algonquian) a. Nitsikákomimmotsspinnaan(a). nit-iikakomimm-oti-hpinnaan   1-love.ta-inv-1pl   ‘We (excl) are loved.’ (Frantz 2009: 61) b. Nóko’siksi áyo’kaayaawa.   n-óko’s-iksi á-yo’kaa-yi-aawa   1-kid-anim.pl ipfv-sleep.ai-3pl-3pl   ‘My kids are sleeping.’ (Fox and Frantz 1979: 152; glosses are adapted and tones added) c. Áánistsisa ikkámáakaaistoosi.   waanIt-is ikkám-yáak-waaistoo-si   say.ta-2sg→3.imp if-fut-come.ai-3.sbjv   ‘Ask him if he will come.’ (Frantz 2009: 138)



 195

What is a word? 

Many studies on Algonquian languages regard elements like nit- (8a) and n- (8b) as proclitics, mainly based on some interpretation of the selectivity criterion. Some elements following the verb stem, like -oti and -hpinnaan in (8a), -yi in (8b), and -is in (8c), are routinely considered affixes. Others, like -aawa ‘3pl’ in (8b) and -si ‘3.sbjv’ in (8c), are often considered enclitics, the former on syntactic grounds—it appears only under certain syntactic (and pragmatic) conditions—and the latter on phonological grounds—it is less tightly integrated into the preceding unit—(see Stacy 2004 and references therein). As in Lakota (see § 8.3.1), such “enclitics” are apparently heterogeneous regarding their phonological autonomy; upon closer inspection, -si and other comparable elements may well turn out to be anti-clitics. Regarding their grammatical autonomy and leaving aside the selectivity criterion, Blackfoot verb complexes (including personal markers before and after the stem) seem to be rather tight units. Another case where a systematic kind of mismatch is not analyzable as run-of-themill cliticization comes from Kwak’wala (as already noted in Anderson 1984). Consider (9); the elements da ‘det’ and laχ ənoχ ‘prep 1pl’ are phonologically hosted by the units preceding them but morphosyntactically hosted by the units following them: (9)            

Kwak’wala (Wakashan; Janzen 2011: 102) phonological unit

phonological unit

grammatical unit

phonological unit

grammatical unit

ləmis gaχuʔi da ʔixpoʔom a  then come det fruit obj Ləmis gaχu’ida ʔixpo’omalaχənoχ gukw. ‘Then the fruit was brought up to our house.’

phonological unit grammatical unit

laχ prep

ənoχ 1pl

gukw house

The comparative literature calls such elements ditropic clitics (Embick and Noyer 1999, Cysouw 2005). They seem to occur in several languages of the Northwest Coast, but much more work is needed on them. To the extent that descriptive studies address them, they use terms like “ambivalent affixes” for them (see, e.  g., Suttles 2004: 28–29 for a short mention of such (optionally) encliticized elements in Musqueam [Salishan]).

8.4 Selected parameters of variation of grammatical words This section presents two important parameters of variation of grammatical words, namely (i) their internal make-up and (ii) the overall word classes into which they fall. The former is one of the central topics of morphological typology, has been debated in detail since the mid-1990s, and is the object of § 8.4.1. The latter has become highly

196 

 Fernando Zúñiga

controversial in the 20th century —particularly since the 1970s—, and would merit a comprehensive treatment, but I limit myself to sketching its essentials in § 8.4.2.

8.4.1 Morphological make-up Regarding the internal structure of grammatical words, Indigenous North American languages show variation (Sapir 1921: Chh. V–VI; Mithun 1999: Ch.2; Zúñiga 2017, 2019). While grammatical words that potentially consist of a high number of morphemes are frequent, this is not universal in the region. Moreover, those that do have such “polysynthetic” grammatical words show different subtypes thereof. First, morphemic arrangement can be templatic (e.  g., in Athabaskan and Iroquoian) or layered (e.  g., in Eskimoan). In the former, roots and affixes occur in specific positions and in an invariant order with respect to the other morphemes; in the latter, at least some affixes can occur in different positions, showing semantic and grammatical scope over the material to their left. The examples in (10) (slightly adapted from Mithun 1999: 42–43) illustrate these two subtypes. The templatic arrangement in (a) has both the verb root (-e ‘go’) and the affixes around it occupying specific slots in a fixed order. The layered arrangement in (b) has the verb root (ayag- ‘go’) occupy the leftmost position, but the modal suffix -yugnarqe ‘probably’ can immediately follow either the verbal root and its future marker (ayag-ciq- ‘will go’) or the verbal suffix and its past tense marker, (-ni-llru ‘claimed’), with a difference in meaning: (10)                  

a.         b.        

Tuscarora (Iroquoian) Yaʔnə̨́:tsyə̨:t. y-aʔ-ʔn-ə̨ts-ye-e-t trans-fct-du-rep-indf.a-go-pfv ‘They two went back there.’ Yup’ik (Eskimoan) Ayagciqsugnarqnillruuq. ayag-ciq-yugnarqe-ni-llru-u-q go-fut-probably-claim-pst-ind-3sg ‘He said he would probably go.’

       

Ayagciqnillruyugnarquq. ayag-ciq-ni-llru-yugnarqe-u-q go-fut-claim-pst-probably-ind-3sg ‘He probably said he would go.’

Second, the morphemes combined into grammatical words may belong to one clause (the default case) or to separate clauses (e.g, in Wakashan and Eskimoan). The examples in (11) from Pittman (2009: 141) illustrate cases where a lexical suffix even allows the complex construction to accommodate a new argument. In the Nuuchahnulth example (a), the intransitive verbal root waʔič- ‘sleep’ combines with the transitive verbal suffix -ʕiƛ ‘come upon’ to form a single grammatical word; note that it is the lexical suffix that introduces the new 1st-person-singular subject. Similarly, in the Inuktitut example (b), the transitive verbal root niri- ‘eat’ licenses two arguments (Miali ‘Mary’ and tuktu ‘(the) caribou’) and the transitive verbal suffix -nira ‘say’ introduces the new subject Jaani



What is a word? 

 197

‘John’. In both cases, the verbal roots and the verbal suffixes head their own (subordinate and superordinate) clauses, which have been conjoined: (11)                

a.         b.      

Nuuchahnulth (Wakashan) Waʔičʕiƛitsiš Ken. waʔič-ʕiƛ-mit-siiš Ken sleep-come.upon-pst-1sg.ind K. ‘I found Ken sleeping.’ North Baffin Inuktitut (Eskimoan) Jaani-up niri-qqau-nira-lauq-taa tuktu J.-erg eat-rec.pst-say-rem.pst-3sg→3sg caribou[abs] ‘(A while ago) John said that Mary was eating the caribou.’

Miali-mu. M.-all

This parameter of variation has received comparatively little attention and needs to be explored in greater detail (see Zúñiga 2019 and references therein). Lastly, morphemes of different kinds can be combined into grammatical words: roots may combine with grammatical affixes (as in many North American and Indo-European languages), lexical affixes (e.  g., in Eskimoan, Wakashan, Salishan, Chimakuan, and Tsimshianic), and/or other roots (“incorporation,” e.  g., in Iroquoian, Caddoan, Siouan, Kiowa-Tanoan, Uto-Aztecan, Athabaskan, Tsimshianic, and several unclassified languages). For instance, the nominal root nayir ‘seal’ combines with the lexical affix -cur ‘hunt’ in the Yup’ik grammatical word in (12a); the Onondaga (Iroquoian) grammatical word in (12b) is different: nominal -ʔseht- ‘vehicle’ is a stem (it consists of ʔse- ‘drag’ plus the nominalizer -ht; Marianne Mithun, p.c.) and verbal -hninu ‘buy’ is a root, and either can occur as the sole lexical element of its own grammatical word. (12)                  

a.         b.        

Yup’ik (Mithun 1999: 49) Nayircurtuq. nayir-cur-tuq seal-hunt-3sg.ind ‘He is seal-hunting.’ Onondaga (Iroquoian; Woodbury 1975: 17) Tom waʔhaʔsehtahní·nuʔ. Tom waʔ-ha-ʔseht-a-hninu-ʔ T. pst-3sg.m.A-vehicle-ep-buy-tam ‘Tom bought a vehicle.’

There is much to say about lexical affixation in North American languages, but I limit myself to outlining the basics here (see Mattissen 2006 and Mithun 1997, 1999: 48–56, 2017 for more details). First, note that the mirror image of (12a) is also found; in (13), the verbal root k’wəs- ‘burn’ combines with the affix -cəs ‘hand’:

198 

(13)        

 Fernando Zúñiga

Musqueam (Salishan; Gerdts 1998: 95) Ni cən k’wəscəs. ni cən k’wəs-cəs aux 1sg.sbj burn-hand ‘I burned my hand.’

Thus, Indigenous North American languages differ regarding the formal status of elements that are semantically comparable to roots, words, or even phrases in many other languages: their grammatical autonomy may vary considerably, and even if they are bound, they can range from root-like to affix-like. Some of the latter are noun-, adjective-, or adverb-like (e.  g., Musqueam -cəs), can be found in Northwest-Coast languages and in Algonquian, and express notions like location, direction, instruments, manner, degree, quantification, chronology, and body parts. Others are verb-like (e.  g., Yup’ik -cur-), can be found in Eskimoan, Wakashan, and Algonquian, and show semantics that ranges from general (e.  g., ‘exist’, ‘have’, ‘make’) to specific (e.  g., ‘hunt’, ‘eat’, ‘smell like’). In any given language, lexical suffixes may have non-affixal counterparts (which may be etymologically related to them or not), and the former are often more general (e.  g., metaphorically and/or metonymically extended) than the latter. Like incorporated roots/ stems, lexical affixes are both vocabulary-extending and discourse-structuring elements, backgrounding as they do ancillary or established information. Most present-day scholars think lexical affixes probably originated in compounds. For instance, either the verbal or the nominal element in erstwhile Verb+Noun compounds retained the capability of being used as an independent stem while the companion element became affixal; so-called bipartite stems (Jacobsen 1980) arose when both elements of erstwhile Verb+Verb compounds became dependent.

8.4.2 Parts of speech There is no universally accepted answer to the twofold question of what parts of speech (notions like nouns, verbs, etc.) are and how they are identified. Some scholars—often those working in the Chomskyan tradition (e.  g., Baker 2003)—see parts of speech as universal notions. Others—often those working in the functional-typological tradition (e.  g., Croft 2000)—regard them as language-specific but showing strong cross-linguistic tendencies. Approaches to parts of speech also differ as to the role played by prototypicality and/or gradience in their definition (e.  g., Aarts 2007). See Polinsky (2012) for a recent illustration of the main approaches. Languages evidently differ regarding the morphological possibilities and/or necessities of their grammatical words: the formal marking of case, number, gender, person, tense, aspect, etc. shows significant cross-linguistic variation, often even among closely related languages. Morphologically, most languages of Native North America clearly distinguish not only between function and content words but also between different



What is a word? 

 199

subtypes of the latter; these language-specific subtypes can be termed, in the spirit of the model outlined in § 8.2, morphological nouns, morphological verbs, etc. Syntactically, languages often distinguish between elements that can head noun phrases from those that head clauses, which could in turn be termed syntactic nouns, syntactic verbs, etc. As with phonological words and grammatical words, matches between morphological units and syntactic units do exist, but they cannot be assumed to hold everywhere: mismatches exist as well. Several language groups of Indigenous North America have figured prominently in the debates about parts of speech—most notably: Eskimoan, Iroquoian, Salishan, Wakashan, and Chimakuan (see Mithun 1999: 56–67). The issue has often been framed in terms of, and even reduced to, the nature and the degree of distinction between nouns and verbs. The relevant descriptive questions asked in the literature are: (i) can most content grammatical words (including proper names) be used referentially and predicatively, (ii) do such grammatical words need special morphology to perform each task, and (iii) is such morphology available to most roots/stems or just to subsets thereof? Irrespective of the theoretical use to which the answers are put, there cannot be any doubt on the variation encountered in North American languages regarding this issue. Athabaskan and Algonquian languages, for instance, do not normally do what Northwest Coast languages famously can; even though the determiner tit crucially differentiates between the predicative string preceding it and the referential one following it in Upper Chehalis, the structural symmetry shown by pairs like (14) is striking nonetheless: (14)            

Upper Chehalis (Salishan; Kinkade 1983: 30) a. ʔAc-táw-ɬ tit ʔac-lə́pxw-ɬ.   stat-big-intr det stat-hole-intr   ‘The hole is big.’ b. ʔAc-lə́pxw-ɬ tit ʔac-táw-ɬ.   stat-hole-intr det stat-big-intr   ‘The big one is a hole.’

8.5 Conclusions The previous sections have taken studies from the 2000s onwards on the relevant phenomena seriously: when using the term “word” to address linguistic units, one needs to distinguish between different kinds of words, both within and across languages. The first parameters of variation of words are domain-related (i.  e., phonology and morphosyntax), and we saw that phonological and grammatical words can, but need not, match. Several subtypes of grammatical words can be distinguished, based on which kinds of morphemes are combined into words, and how, as well as how many clauses are involved.

200 

 Fernando Zúñiga

Native North American languages show variation regarding all these parameters, and the emerging picture is a complex one. Long polymorphemic grammatical words are not hard to find, and comparative work seems to confirm the traditional impression that many languages of the region share this general feature (see, e.  g., Bickel and Nichols 2013 for a worldwide survey of inflectional synthesis on verbs). Incorporation and lexical affixation, by contrast, are hallmarks of the languages of some areas and families only; other languages of the region show neither. Both templatic and layered grammatical words are robustly represented in the region; multiclausal polysynthesis and a tenuous correlation between different morphological grammatical words and syntactic grammatical words (i.  e., “weak noun-verb distinction”), under-described though both phenomena are typologically, seem to occur only in few linguistic groups. Comparative work on different kinds of grammatical words and their distribution is still in its infancy, and such analysis of phonological words has not been conducted yet. Further research on these topics will help determine patterns, formulate generalizations, and possibly also identify more cases of mismatches between different kinds of words. We have also much to learn from future work on the origins and the development of different kinds of grammatical words and phonological words. Regarding the issue of parts of speech, rather than arguing in favor or against the adequacy of the received pigeonholes, descriptive and comparative work should focus on the regularities and the oddities found in the lexicon and grammar of the languages under scrutiny. Detailed inventories of subtypes of nouns, verbs, adverbs, and particles, as well as their formal properties, are particularly interesting and useful. Acknowledgements: I am grateful to the editors of the present volume and two anonymous reviewers for their comments on earlier versions of this article.

Abbreviations ABS AI ALL ANIM APP ART AUX CL DET DU EP ERG FCT FUT HAB

absolutive animate intransitive allative animate appositional article auxiliary classifier determiner dual epenthesis ergative factual future habitual

IC IMP IND INDF.A INTR INV IPFV NAFF NEG NSBJ OBJ PFV PL PREP PROTH

initial change imperative indicative indefinite agent intransitive inverse imperfective nonaffirmative negative nonsubject objective perfective plural preposition prothesis



PST Q  REC REINF REM REP SBJ SBJV SG

What is a word? 

past question recent reinforcing remote repetitive subject subjunctive singular

SO STAT TA TAM TI TR TRANS UNREAL

 201

sticklike object stative transitive animate tense-aspect-modality transitive inanimate transitive translocative unreal

References Aarts, Bas. 2007. Syntactic gradience: The nature of grammatical indeterminacy. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Adler, Jeffrey. 2016. The nature of conspiracy: Implications for parallel versus serial derivation. Santa Cruz: University of California at Santa Cruz MA thesis. Aikhenvald, Alexandra. 2002. Typological parameters for the study of clitics, with special reference to Tariana. In R. M. W. Dixon & Alexandra Aikhenvald (eds.), Word: A cross-linguistic typology, 42–78. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Anderson, Stephen. 1984. Kwakwala syntax and the Government-Binding theory. In Eung-Do Cook & Donna Gerdts (eds.), The syntax of Native American languages, 21–75. New York: Academic Press. Aoki, Haruo. 1966. Nez Perce vowel harmony and Proto-Sahaptian vowels. Language 42(4). 759–767. Atoyebi, Joseph Dele. 2009. A reference grammar of Oko. Leipzig: University of Leipzig dissertation. Baker, Mark. 2003. Lexical categories: Verbs, nouns, and adjectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Bickel, Balthasar & Johanna Nichols. 2013. Inflectional synthesis of the verb. In Matthew Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. http://wals.info/chapter/22 Bickel, Balthasar & Fernando Zúñiga. 2017. The ‘word’ in polysynthetic languages: Phonological and syntactic challenges. In Michael Fortescue, Marianne Mithun, & Nicholas Evans (eds.), The Oxford handbook of polysynthesis, 158–185. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Bisetto, Antonietta & Sergio Scalise. 1999. Compounding: morphology and/or syntax? In Lunella Mereu (ed.), Boundaries of morphology and syntax, 31–48. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Bloomfield, Leonard. 1926. A set of postulates for the science of language. Language 2(3). 153–164. Bloomfield, Leonard. 1933. Language. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston Charney, Jean Ormsbee. 1993. A grammar of Comanche. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Cowell, Andrew & Alonzo Moss, Sr. 2008. The Arapaho language. Boulder: University Press of Colorado. Croft, William. 2000. Parts of speech as language universals and as language-particular categories. In Petra Vogel & Bernard Comrie (eds.), Approaches to the typology of word classes, 65–102. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Cysouw, Michael. 2005. Morphology in the wrong place: A survey of preposed enclitics. In Wolfgang Dressler, Dieter Kastovsky Oskar Pfeiffer, & Franz Rainer (eds.), Morphology and its demarcations, 17–37. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Dixon, R. M. W. & Alexandra Aikhenvald. 2002. Word: A typological framework. In R. M. W. Dixon & Alexandra Aikhenvald (eds.), Word: A cross-linguistic typology, 1–41. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Dunham, Joel. 2009. Noun incorporation in Blackfoot. Manuscript. University of British Columbia.

202 

 Fernando Zúñiga

Dunn, John Asher. 1995. Sm’algyax: A reference dictionary and grammar for the Coast Tsimshian language. Seattle: University of Washington Press/Sealaska Heritage Foundation. Dyck, Carrie. 1999. Cayuga syllable structure. Linguistica Atlantica 21. 69–105. Dyck, Carrie. 2009. Defining the word in Cayuga (Iroquoian). International Journal of American Linguistics 75(4). 571–605. Embick, David & Rolf Noyer. 1999. Locality in post-syntactic operations. In David Embick & Rolf Noyer (eds.), Papers in morphology and syntax, Cycle two, 265–317. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Fortescue, Michael. 2017. What are the limits of polysynthesis? In Michael Fortescue, Marianne Mithun & Nicholas Evans (eds.), The Oxford handbook of polysynthesis, 115–134. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Fox, Jacinta & Donald Frantz. 1979. Blackfoot clitic pronouns. In William Cowan (ed.), Papers of the Tenth Algonquian Conference, 152–166. Ottawa: Carleton University. Frantz, Donald. 2009. Blackfoot grammar. 2nd ed. Toronto: Toronto University Press. Frantz, Donald & Norma Jean Russell. 2017. Blackfoot dictionary of stems, roots, and affixes. 3rd ed. Toronto: University of Toronto Press. Gerdts, Donna. 1998. Incorporation. In Andrew Spencer & Arnold Zwicky (eds.), The handbook of morphology, 84–100. Oxford: Blackwell. Hansson, Gunnar Ólafur. 2007. On the evolution of consonant harmony: The case of secondary articulation agreement. Phonology 24(1). 77–120. Harris, Alice. 2006. Revisiting anaphoric islands. Language 82(1). 114–130. Haspelmath, Martin. 2011. The indeterminacy of word segmentation and the nature of morphology and syntax. Folia Linguistica 45(1). 31–80. Hockett, Charles. 1958. A course in modern linguistics. New York: MacMillan. Inkelas, Sharon. 1989. Prosodic constituency in the lexicon. San Francisco: Stanford University dissertation. Inkelas, Sharon. 1993. Deriving cyclicity. In Sharon Hargus & Ellen Kaisse (eds.), Phonetics and phonology, Vol. 4: Studies in lexical phonology, 75–110. San Diego: Academic Press. Jacobsen, William. 1980. Washo bipartite verb stems. In Kathryn Klar, Margaret Langdon & Shirley Silver (eds.), American Indian and Indoeuropean studies, 85–99. The Hague: Mouton. Janzen, Jonathan. 2011. Phrase- and word-level prosody in Kwak’wala. University of British Columbia Working Papers in Linguistics 30. 101–107. Julien, Marit. 2002. Syntactic heads and word formation. New York: Oxford University Press. Kempf, Luise. 2010. In erober: vnd plünderung der Statt: Wie Ellipse von Wortteilen entstand. Beiträge zur Geschichte der deutschen Sprache und Literatur 132(3). 343–365. Kinkade, M. Dale. 1983. Salish evidence against the universality of ‘noun’ and ‘verb’. Lingua 60. 25–40. Klavans, Judith. 1985. The independence of syntax and phonology in cliticization. Language 61(1). 95–120. Langacker, Ronald. 1972. Fundamentals of linguistic analysis. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. Lewis, Geoffrey. 1967. Turkish grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Martin, Jack B. 2011. A Grammar of Creek (Muskogee). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Mattissen, Johanna. 2006. The ontology and diachrony of polysynthesis. In Dieter Wunderlich (ed.), Advances in the theory of the lexicon, 287–353. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. https://doi. org/10.1515/9783110197815.287 Mattissen, Johanna. 2017. Sub-types of polysynthesis. In Michael Fortescue, Marianne Mithun, & Nicholas Evans (eds.), The Oxford handbook of polysynthesis, 70–98. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Michelson, Karin. 1988. A comparative study of Lake-Iroquoian accent. Dordrecht: Kluwer. Mithun, Marianne. 1997. Lexical affixes and morphological typology. In Joan Bybee, John Haiman, & Sandra Thompson (eds.), Essays on language function and language type, 357–371. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Mithun, Marianne. 1999. The languages of Native North America. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Mithun, Marianne. 2017. Polysynthesis in North America. In Michael Fortescue, Marianne Mithun, & Nicholas Evans (eds.), The Oxford handbook of polysynthesis, 235–259. Oxford: Oxford University Press.



What is a word? 

 203

Nevins, Andrew. 2011. Multiple agree with clitics: Person complementarity vs. omnivorous number. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 29(4). 939–971. Piggott, Glyne L. 1995. Epenthesis and syllable weight. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 13(2). 283–326. Pittman, Christine. 2009. Complex verb formation revisited: Restructuring in Inuktitut and Nuu-chah-nulth. In Marc-Antoine Mahieu & Nicole Tersis (eds.), Variations on polysynthesis: The Eskaleut languages, 135–147. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Polinsky, Maria (ed.). 2012. Are lexical categories universal? The view from Chamorro. Special issue of Theoretical Linguistics 38.2. Rankin, Robert, John Boyle, Randolph Graczyk & John Koontz. 2002. A synchronic and diachronic perspective on ‘word’ in Siouan. In R. M. W. Dixon & Alexandra Aikhenvald (eds.), Word: A cross-linguistic typology, 180–204. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Rood, David & Allan Taylor. 1996. Lakhota. In Ives Goddard (ed.), Handbook of North American Indians, vol. 17: Languages, 440–482. Washington: Smithsonian Institution. Sadock, Jerrold. 1986. Some notes on noun incorporation. Language 62(1). 19–31. Saenger, Paul. 1997. Space between words: The origins of silent reading. Stanford: Stanford University Press. Sapir, Edward. 1921. Language: An introduction to the study of speech. New York: Harcourt Brace. Schiering, René, Balthasar Bickel & Kristine Hildebrandt. 2010. The prosodic word is not universal, but emergent. Journal of Linguistics 46(3). 657–709. Selkirk, Elisabeth. 1986. On derived domains in sentence phonology. Phonology Yearbook 3. 371–405. Spencer, Andrew. 2001. The paradigm-based model of morphosyntax. Transactions of the Philological Society 99(2). 279–314. Spencer, Andrew & Ana Luís. 2012. The canonical clitic. In Dunstan Brown, Marina Chumakina, & Greville Corbett (eds.), Canonical morphology and syntax, 123–150. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Stacy, Elizabeth. 2004. Phonological aspects of Blackfoot prominence. Calgary: University of Calgary MA thesis. Stump, Gregory. 2001. Inflectional morphology: A theory of paradigm structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Suttles, Wayne. 2004. Musqueam reference grammar. Vancouver: University of British Columbia Press. Ullrich, Jan. 2011. New Lakota dictionary: Lakȟótiyapi-English, English-Lakȟótiyapi & incorporating the Dakota dialects of Yankton-Yanktonai, Santee-Sisseton. 2nd ed. Bloomington: Lakota Language Consortium. Urbanczyk, Su. 2023 [this volume]. Prosodic morphology. In Carmen Dagostino, Marianne Mithun & Keren Rice (eds.), The languages and linguistics of Indigenous North America: A comprehensive guide. Berlin & Boston: De Gruyter Mouton. Van Eijk, Jan. 1997. The Lillooet Language: Phonology, Morphology, Syntax. Vancouver: University of British Columbia Press. Ward, Gregory; Richard Sproat & Gail McKoon. 1991. A pragmatic analysis of so-called anaphoric islands. Language 67(3). 439–474. Watanabe, Honoré. 2003. A morphological description of Sliammon, Mainland Comox Salish, with a sketch of syntax. Osaka: Osaka Gakuin University, Faculty of Informatics, Endangered Languages of the Pacific Rim. Woodbury, Anthony. 1975. Onondaga noun incorporation: Some notes on the interdependence of syntax and semantics. International Journal of American Linguistics 41(1). 10–20. Woodbury, Anthony. 2002. The word in Cup’ik. In R. M. W Dixon & Alexandra Aikhenvald (eds.), Word: A cross-linguistic typology, 79–99. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Woolford, Ellen. 2016. Two types of portmanteau agreement: Syntactic and morphological. In Géraldine Legendre, Michael Putnam, Henriëtte de Swart & Erin Zaroukian (eds.), Optimality-Theoretic syntax, semantics, and pragmatics: From uni- to bidirectional optimization, 111–135. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

204 

 Fernando Zúñiga

Zúñiga, Fernando. 2014. (Anti-)cliticization in Mapudungun. Morphology 24(3). 161–175. Zúñiga, Fernando. 2017. On the morphosyntax of indigenous languages of the Americas. International Journal of American Linguistics 83(1). 111–139. Zúñiga, Fernando. 2019. Polysynthesis: A review. Language and Linguistics Compass 13.4. Zwicky, Arnold. 1987. Suppressing the Zs. Journal of Linguistics 23(1). 133–148. Zwicky, Arnold & Geoffrey Pullum. 1983. Cliticization vs. inflection: English n’t. Language 59(3). 502–513.

Daniel W. Hieber

9 Word classes Abstract: This chapter is an introduction to word classes (parts of speech) in indigenous North American languages. It explains theoretical approaches to the study of word classes (language-particular vs. typological) as well as how word classes are classified (lexical vs. functional classes and open vs. closed classes). The core of the chapter is a survey of the word classes commonly found in North American languages. It covers the lexical categories of noun, verb, adjective, and adverb, and the functional categories of adposition, article, auxiliary, particle, and pronoun. There are however many outstanding questions in word-class research. Two of the most prominent ones – locus of categoriality and the noun-verb distinction – are discussed in detail in the latter half of this chapter. The diversity of North American languages continues to challenge our understanding of the nature of word classes.

9.1 Introduction Word classes (traditionally called parts of speech) are groups of words in a language that fill similar slots in an utterance (Croft 2001: 11) and share some linguistic properties, whether those properties are semantic, syntactic, or morphological (Anward, Moravcsik, and Stassen 1997: 171–172; Anward 2001: 726; Rijkhoff 2007: 709; Schachter and Shopen 2007: 1–2; van der Auwera and Gast 2011: 166).1 For example, the class of words that can fill the slot in the utterance the big _____ are typically called “nouns” in English. Noun, verb, and adjective are the best-known classes, but linguists argue for the existence of many others. Languages vary in the number of word classes they have, the characteristics that define those classes, and the number of words that fall into each class (Schachter and Shopen 2007: 1; Velupillai 2012: 122; Smith 2015). Native North American languages have a unique part to play in research on word classes. These languages challenge traditional conceptions of word classes because they do not cleanly map onto the categories of Greek and Latin, which were thought to be universal (Anward, Moravcsik, and Stassen 1997: 167; Vogel and Comrie 2000: xiii). As a result, early Americanist linguists sought to analyze languages on language-internal

1 This definition is intentionally broad, because linguists disagree – often fundamentally – on what word classes are, and how to define them in particular languages (see § 2). Bernard Comrie (p.c.) points out that the present definition could include inflectional classes or valence classes, which are not traditionally considered distinct parts of speech. The tradition in linguistics is that the term word class refers to categories like noun, verb, pronoun, etc. (Haspelmath 2001: 16538). However, some linguists, particularly those that adopt the perspective of construction grammar (see especially Croft 2001), happily accept different inflectional classes or valence classes as types of word classes. See § 2 for more detail. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-009

206 

 Daniel W. Hieber

evidence alone, rather than impose grammatical models from other languages and traditions (Sapir 1921: 125). The subsequent quest to accurately describe Native American languages on their own terms motivated – and continues to motivate – a large portion of the research into the nature of linguistic categories. An understanding of word classes is useful to speakers and language learners because knowing the category of a word provides speakers with information about how that word is used. The part of speech of a word can indicate which affixes that word is allowed to take, how that word combines with other words or affixes to create new words, and what roles that word can play in a sentence, among other information. This chapter has two primary goals: 1) to introduce the study of word classes with a focus on current approaches 2) to highlight the unique place and contribution of native North American languages in this research Section 2 presents two competing theories of word classes. Section 3 explains the main types of word classes: lexical vs. functional and open vs. closed. Section 4 is a brief survey of some common word classes. Section 5 summarizes two central issues in word class research in North American languages specifically. Section 6 concludes by summarizing the distinct contribution of North American languages to the study of word classes.

9.2 Theories of word classes Today, there are two diametrically opposed perspectives on the nature of word classes (Croft 2001: 63). The first, more traditional approach, argues that individual languages have large, cohesive word classes such as noun, verb, and adjective, but that these categories vary considerably across languages, with perhaps some languages lacking certain categories entirely. Researchers that adopt this perspective differ as to whether they view word classes as clearly defined or fuzzy and prototypical, but they agree that it is possible to define and describe major categories for every language. This is the particularist (that is, language-particular) approach to word classes. The second approach argues that the behaviors of individual words in a language are so diverse that it is impossible to formulate broad definitions for word classes. In this approach, languages do not have major word classes like noun, verb, and adjective. Instead they have a proliferation of tiny categories or constructions. The major word classes are emergent and epiphenomenal (Croft 1991: 30; Croft 2005: 436), arising from the human propensity to view the world through the cognitive prototypes of objects, actions, and properties, and the fact that discourse is fundamentally a sequence of referents and predicates (Sapir 1921: 119; Croft 1991: 124). This cognitive propensity is reflected in various subtle ways in the grammars of all languages. This is the typological (that is, crosslinguistic), constructional, or functional prototype approach to word classes (Croft 2001: 102–104).



Word classes 

 207

It is impossible to discuss word classes without at least implicitly committing to one of these two perspectives. Nearly all the studies referenced in this chapter adopt the particularist approach to word classes. The typological approach to word classes is still fairly recent, and little research has looked at North American languages from a constructional perspective (though see Hieber [2018] and Vigus [2018]). However, since this chapter is a crosslinguistic survey, I adopt the typological approach here. When I use terms like noun or verb in describing a language, I am referring to crosslinguistic prototypes or comparative concepts (Haspelmath 2010), rather than making a claim about the existence or nonexistence of that particular part of speech in that particular language.

9.3 Types of word classes Word classes are typically described along two dimensions: they may contain lexical (“content”) words or grammatical (“function”) words, and they may be open to new members or closed to new members. This section describes each of these types.

9.3.1 Content words vs. function words One way to describe word classes is in terms of the meanings of their words, dividing them into lexical categories or functional categories (Haspelmath 2001; Rijkhoff 2007). Lexical categories contain “content words” which prototypically refer to things, events, or properties in the world. Below are some lexical words in Arapaho (Algonquian). Section § 4.1 discusses lexical categories in more depth. (1)                            

Arapaho (Algonquian) (Cowell and Moss 2008: 56, 61, 74–75) hébes ‘beaver’ hébesii ‘beavers’ wóxhoox ‘horse’ woxhóóxebii ‘horses’ ho’óeet ‘clay’ ho’óeetno ‘(clay-based) ceremonial paints’ bes ‘wood’ béxo ‘sticks’ biixúút ‘shirt’ nebiixúút ‘my shirt’ nííhooyóúʼu ‘they (inanimate) are yellow’ nííhoonéíh(i)t ‘s/he (animate) is yellow) nonóóhowó’ ‘I see him/her’ neihoownoohówoo ‘I don’t see him/her’

208 

 Daniel W. Hieber

In contrast, functional categories contain words which indicate grammatical relationships or specify features about content words. These are called “function words”, and they typically have abstract meanings. Below are some function words in Creek (Muskogean). (2)          

Creek (Muskogean) (Martin 2011: 304, 331–332, 357–359, 360–362) leykauxiliary verb, ‘be (while sitting)’ hoyɬauxiliary verb, ‘be (while standing)’ wa:kk- auxiliary verb, ‘be (while lying)’ =ta:t(i) focus of attention =a:t(i) referential (definite/emphatic)

The first three words in (2) are auxiliary verbs  – words which provide additional information about a main verb (see § 4.2.3). In Creek, auxiliary verbs express aspect, possibility, or strength of assertion (Martin 2011: 298). The last two words in (2) are enclitics – morphemes which behave phonologically like suffixes, but syntactically like independent words, with scope over the entire phrase. While =ta:t(i) attaches to noun phrases and indicates that the noun is the focus of attention or topic of the discourse, =a:t(i) attaches to verb phrases to indicate definiteness or identifiability (Martin 2011: 357, 360). North American languages have a great diversity of functional categories like these. Section § 4.2 describes several common ones in more detail. The terms lexical category and functional category are not used the same way by all researchers. Both lexical category and functional category are sometimes used to refer to word classes as defined here (e.  g., Payne 1997: 32). Sometimes word class is used to refer to lexical categories (Rijkhoff 2007: 710). It is also common to use the term grammatical categories for word classes, although this term more typically refers to formally marked features of a word such as person, tense, or number (Crystal 2008: 68–69; 186–187; Trask 1993: 122). Another related term is syntactic categories; this is sometimes used in the equivalent sense of lexical categories, sometimes in the broader sense of word classes (see Rauh [2010] for an extended discussion). It is helpful to be aware of these terminological differences when reading linguistic research. The distinction between lexical and functional categories is not always a clear one. Adpositions (prepositions and postpositions) often have both lexical and functional uses (Haspelmath 2001: 16539; Smith 2015: 178). Consider the two uses of the word by in English in (3). (3)    

English (Indo-European) (Corpus of Contemporary American English; Davies 2020) a. Remember the last time you passed by your favorite park b. If your life was destroyed by the money that paid for this thing

In example (a), by is lexical, meaning ‘next to’ or ‘in proximity to’. In (b), by is functional, marking the agent of a passive clause. Adpositions in Chitimacha (isolate) also have both lexical and functional uses. In (4), the postposition hix may mean ‘with; by means of’ (its lexical sense, in (a)) or mark the agent of a transitive verb (its functional sense, in (b)).



(4)                

Word classes 

 209

Chitimacha (isolate) (Swadesh 1939a: A10k.2, A11c.10) a. hus mahci kuh hix qapx nehpapuyna   hus mahci kuh hix qapx neh-pa-puy-na   3sg tail feather with refl cover-caus-hab-nf.pl   ‘they adorn themselves with his tail feathers’ b. qix hix hi koomicukix   qix hix hi kow-ma-cu(y)-ki-x   1sg erg and call- plact-irr-1sg.a-cond   ‘if I call them’

The reason for this gradation between lexical and functional uses of the same word is that function words derive historically from lexical words, a process known as grammaticalization (Hopper and Traugott 2003). A language will often retain the older, lexical meaning alongside the newer, functional meaning. Another example of the cline between lexical and functional uses of a word is the use of words meaning ‘sit’, ‘stand’, and ‘lie’ as auxiliary verbs indicating progressive or continuative aspect in Siouan (Mithun 1999: 115–116), some Muskogean languages (Munro 1984; Broadwell 2006: 209–211), and Chitimacha (Hieber 2019: 350–352), among others. Example (5) shows lexical and functional uses of ‘sit’, ‘stand’, and ‘lie’ in Mandan (Siouan). (5)                                    

Mandan (Siouan) (Kennard 1936: 31) a. wɛ́rɛx nakóc   wɛ́rɛx nak-oc   pot sit-prs   ‘A pot was there (sitting).’ b. mah ísɛkanakeròmakoc   ‘he was (sitting) making an arrow’ c. múixtɛ ̀na tɛ́romakoc   múi-xtɛ-na tɛ-romakoc   village-big-emph stand-narr.pst   ‘there was a big village’ d. ptáhakekaʼ   ‘he was running around (upright)’ e. máːta makómakoc   máːta mak-omakoc   river lie-narr.pst   ‘the river was there’ f. miníxamakɛkaʼ   ‘he was playing (prone)’

210 

 Daniel W. Hieber

9.3.2 Open classes vs. closed classes Another way to describe word classes is by how open they are to new members. Open classes are typically large and have new words added to them frequently, whereas closed classes are typically small, limited to a fixed set of words, and add new members only slowly and infrequently (often through grammaticalization) (Robins 2014: 214–215; Schachter and Shopen 2007: 3; Velupillai 2012: 115). English articles, for example, are a closed class of just two words (the and a/an), while English nouns are in principle unlimited, with more words added all the time. There is gradation here as well: English prepositions are generally considered a closed class even though they constitute a large group of words (greater than 100 members), because new prepositions are not created easily. Nonetheless, new prepositions do occasionally arise. For example, prepositional uses of the word absent (as in the utterance absent those ropes, we’d float to a new and faraway place [COCA]) arose only in the 1940s (Harper 2020). In North American languages, one somewhat common closed class of words is the preverb category, words which form a semantic unit with their verb, and often indicate things like direction or aspect (Los et al. 2012: Ch. 1).2 Chitimacha has a closed set of 10 preverbs, shown below in (6) (Hieber 2018). By contrast, Menominee (Algic) has a large open class of preverbs (Bloomfield 1962: 214). (6)                  

Chitimacha (isolate) (Hieber 2018: 19) hi ‘to, there’ his ‘back to, back there’ kap ‘up, beginning, becoming’ kaabs ‘back up’ ka ‘across’ kas ‘back across, apart, reverse’ ni ‘down’, definite qap ‘here, coming’ qapx ‘back here, coming back’, reflexive, reciprocal

While open classes tend to be lexical ones and closed classes tend to be functional ones, this is just a tendency (Velupillai 2012: 115). Some Australian languages (Dixon 1976: 615–768; Dixon 1980: 280–281) and Papuan languages (Foley 1986: 113–118) have small, closed classes of verbs (Anward 2001: 728). However, I know of no North American language which has a closed class of verbs like this. Closed adjective classes are likewise less common in North America. In a balanced sample of 27 languages in Mexico and north2 In some language families, the term preverb is used for certain types of verbal prefixes with lexical meanings, rather than for syntactically distinct words. This is the case in many Dene languages, for example (Rice & de Reuse 2017: Sec. 23.2.2). Interestingly, the functions and meanings of these affixal preverbs are similar to those of syntactically free preverb classes in other languages, suggesting that preverbs are a coherent typological class whose morphological boundedness is a cline.



Word classes 

 211

ward, Velupillai (2012: 127–128) finds that 7 have a closed adjective class. Velupillai analyzes most languages in the sample as lacking an adjective class entirely (17 languages), and the few languages with an open adjective class are constrained to Mesoamerica (3 languages). Cupeño (Uto-Aztecan) has fewer than 100 adjectives (though Hill [2005: 202] notes that “the classes of adjectives and adverbs are not closed by structural principle but simply have relatively few members”). In Wichita (Caddoan), property concepts are expressed through verbs; however, a handful of words behave like inflected noun stems rather than inflected verb stems. The only words in this category are Riwa·c ‘big’, Rikic ‘little’, riya·s ‘old’, and colors such as khac ‘white’ and kʷah·c ‘red’ (Rood 1996: 594–595).

9.4 Word classes in North American languages This section describes the major lexical categories (§ 4.1) and a sample of functional categories (§ 4.2) in North American languages from a crosslinguistic perspective, in keeping with the functional-typological approach presented in § 2.

9.4.1 Lexical classes The four most widely-discussed lexical classes are nouns, verbs, adjectives, and adverbs. This section briefly defines each in turn.

9.4.1.1 Nouns Nouns are words whose prototypical function is to refer to things (Croft 1991: 51–52; 2001: 66, 89). The best exemplars are “time-stable” entities such as people, places, and things (Givón 2001: 51), but nouns frequently refer to non-prototypical concepts as well, such as abstract ideas and feelings. Distributionally, the prototypical function of nouns is to serve as a participant in a clause, or as the head of a noun phrase that does so. Typologically, nouns regularly have special forms or markers for the grammatical categories of number, possession, definiteness/specificity, noun class (“gender”), or case/ grammatical relations (Croft 1991: 79; Haspelmath 2001: 16541; Dixon 2010: 54–55; Velupillai 2012: 125). However, every one of these features may be marked on verbs as well, meaning that the presence of these grammatical categories is not a failproof diagnostic for distinguishing nouns from verbs. I demonstrate a few such cross-cutting examples in the remainder of this section. Number: On nouns, number marking indicates plurality of the referent; analogously, some languages have a kind of number marking on verbs called pluractionality (also event number or verbal number). Pluractionality indicates that the event happened

212 

 Daniel W. Hieber

multiple times, or that the action was distributed among multiple participants (Mithun 1988: 215–218; Mattiola 2020). Most North American languages surveyed by Mattiola (2020) have pluractional morphology. Possession: While many languages indicate a possessive relationship between two nouns by marking either the possessor noun or the possessed noun, Nuuchahnulth (a.k.a. Nootka; Wakashan) also allows possessive marking on verbs. When the possessive suffix -ʔa·k appears on nouns, it indicates that the noun is possessed by the subject of the clause. When the suffix appears on verbs, it indicates that the subject of the verb is the possessor of the noun phrase. The two examples in (7) illustrate this contrast. Possession is not an exclusively nominal category. (7)                

Nuuchahnulth (Wakashan) (Nakayama 2001: 128) a. ʔaapḥiiʔiš ɬuucmaakqs   ʔaːp-ḥi·-ʔi·š ɬuːcma-ʔa·k-qs   kind-dur-ind.3 wife-poss-sbj.1sg   ‘My wife is kind.’ b. ʔaapḥiiʔaks ɬuucma   ʔaːp-ḥi·-ʔa·k-s luːcma   kind-dur-poss-1sg wife   ‘My wife is kind.’

Definiteness: Verbs may have special morphology indicating that the speaker is referring to a definite (identifiable) action, or a definite/indefinite participant involved in the action. Chitimacha, for example, has a preverb ni which marks the verb as definite. The verb gast- ‘plant’ is transitive, usually taking an object, but with the preverb ni it becomes intransitive and means ‘plant it’, indicating that there is some definite thing being planted, whose identity can be understood from context. Definiteness is therefore also not criterial of a noun category. Noun Class: Nouns in many languages take affixes that signify some inherent property about the item, such as its animacy, spatial orientation, or shape (Mithun 1999: 95–103), separating nouns into classes. For example, the Iroquoian and Algonquian languages make a morphological distinction between animate and inanimate nouns, with different affixes for each. In Yurok (Algic), however, adjectives also make this distinction (see §  4.1.3; Robins 1958: 93–95). Dene languages have an entire set of classificatory verbs whose stems change based on the countability, number, animacy, and shape/consistency of their absolutive argument (Jaker, Welch, and Rice 2019: 497). Classification is therefore not a category unique to nouns.



Word classes 

 213

9.4.1.2 Verbs Verbs are words whose prototypical function is to predicate – that is, to state something about a referent (Searle 1969: 23–24; Croft 1991: 109–111; Croft 2001: 66, 89). The best exemplars of verbs are actions, events, and processes (Givón 2001[1984]: 52), but verbs frequently convey static meanings as well, such as location or knowledge. Distributionally, the prototypical function of verbs is to serve as the head of a clause. Typologically, it is common for verbs to mark the grammatical categories of tense, aspect, mood, polarity (negative/positive), evidentiality (source of knowledge), epistemic modality (attitude towards the statement), event number, verb class, or grammatical relations (information about the participants in the clause and their relations to one another) (Croft 1991: 79; Haspelmath 2001: 16541; Dixon 2010: 52–54). In § 4.1.1, I noted that nouns also indicate grammatical relations; it is quite common for North American languages to indicate grammatical relations on both nouns and verbs. An important difference is that markers of grammatical relations on nouns indicate their own role in the clause, while markers of grammatical relations on verbs indicate the role of its arguments in the clause. The grammatical categories most commonly associated with verbs may be found on other categories as well. Since the noun-verb distinction is treated at length in § 5.2, I will mention just two cross-cutting cases here: Although tense is the most canonical grammatical category associated with verbs, Makah (Wakashan) nouns may also appear with tense markers. Compare the predicative and referential uses of the tense marker in (8). (8)            

Makah (Wakashan) (Jacobsen 1979: 113) a. baʔasʔu   house-pst-ind.3   ‘It was a house.’ b. baʔasʔuq   house-pst-art   ‘the former house’

Similarly, although aspect is also canonically associated with verbs, Nuuchahnulth (also Wakashan) allows aspect markers on nouns: (9)                

Nuuchahnulth (Wakashan) (Nakayama 2001: 48) a. ḥaaḥuupač̓ak   ḥaːḥuːp-(y)a-č̓ak   instructing-cont-instrument   ‘This is a teaching.’ b. ḥaaw̓iɬaƛ̓aƛ qaaḥma   ḥaːw̓iɬaƛ-ʼaƛ qaaḥma   young.man-tel name   ‘Qaahma was a young man.’

214 

 Daniel W. Hieber

When nouns are used in this non-prototypical way, they are inherently durative, and may only form existential, classifying, or identifying expressions (Nakayama 2001: 48).

9.4.1.3 Adjectives Adjectives are words whose prototypical function is to modify – that is, to specify additional features, qualities, or attributes of a referent (Searle 1969: 23–24; Croft 1991: 109–111; Croft 2001: 66, 89; Schachter and Shopen 2007: 13). Adjectives always modify nouns; words which modify other kinds of items are classified as adverbs (see § 4.1.4). The best exemplars of adjectives are words which attribute properties having to do with value, dimension, age, speed, physical property, and color (Dixon 1977), but adjectives may convey a vast diversity of concepts depending on the language. Adjectives may have distinct forms for comparatives (taller), superlatives (tallest), and equatives (as tall as). Adjectives are not the only way that languages can convey information about attributes and properties. They may have verbs meaning ‘have quality X’, or nouns meaning ‘thing with quality X’. Consequently, adjectives crosslinguistically tend to associate with either nouns or verbs (Croft 1991: 131; Wetzer 1996: 19; Croft 2000: 94). This is especially true for North American languages – there are few if any morphosyntactic devices dedicated to modification. However, words for property concepts usually exhibit behaviors which are different from other words in their class, often justifying the recognition of a subclass of verbs or nouns. There are North American languages with a large, open class of adjectives such as Chitimacha (see discussion in § 5.1) or Central Pomo (Pomoan) (Mithun 1999: 474), but this is rare. Slightly more common are languages with a small, closed class of adjectives. In a sample including 23 North American languages, Velupillai (Velupillai 2012: 128) finds only 6 of those languages have a distinct but closed class of adjectives. Southern Paiute (Uto-Aztecan) has only about a dozen adjectives, for the concepts large, small, long, short, new, old, good, high, strong, hard, and cold (Sapir 1930: 77–79). We have already seen the small class of adjectives in Wichita (Rood 1996: 594–595). Tłı̨ cho Yatıì (a.k.a. Dogrib, Na-Dene) likewise has a closed set of 20 adjectives which are distinguished by their lack of inflectional morphology (Welch 2016). Most North American languages arguably lack an adjective class, such that property concepts are a subcategory of noun or verb or divided between both. Only a few North American languages encode property concepts as nouns; most languages code property concepts as a subclass of verbs. Rincón Luiseño (Uto-Aztecan) is one language which codes some property concepts like nouns. While most modifiers in Luiseño are derived from verbs, the most prototypical property concepts take noun endings, e.  g., yoot ‘large’ and kiháat ‘small’ (Kroeber and Grace 1959: 59). The following examples illustrate the morphological similarity between nouns and adjectives. (Note that the



Word classes 

 215

“absolutive” suffix3 in these examples has various realizations – here -ch, -l, or -sh – and that the plural of ‘girl’ is irregular/suppletive.) (10)                        

Rincón Luiseño (Uto-Aztecan) (Elliott 1999: 27–28) a. nawítma-l yawáywi-sh   girl-abs pretty-abs   ‘pretty girl’ b. nánatma-l-um yawáywi-ch-um   girl-abs-pl pretty-abs-pl   ‘pretty girls’ c. Yaʼá-sh tóow-q nawítma-l-i yawáywi-ch-i.   man-abs see-prs.sg girl-abs-obj pretty-abs-obj   ‘The man sees a pretty girl.’ d. péshli-chal yawáywi-chal   dish-ins pretty-ins   ‘with the pretty dish’

The Maidu (Maidun) and Cherokee (Iroquoian) languages are like the Chitimacha language mentioned above in that they contain an open class of adjectives, all of which are formed from verbs (Dixon [1911: 716–717] for Maidu; Lindsey and Scancarelli [1985], Chafe [2012], and Barrie and Uchihara [2019] for Cherokee). Unlike Chitimacha, however, these languages use nominal rather than adjectival affixes for modifiers. Adjectives in these languages are therefore a subclass of nouns which are all derived from verbs. By far the most common way to encode property concepts in North American languages is as a subclass of verbs. The following examples illustrate the use of such property concepts in a selection of languages, comparing them to action verbs in the same language. (11)            

Navajo (Na-Dene) (Young and Morgan 1980: 216, 290) a. yi-sh-cha   ipfv-1sg-cry   ‘I am crying’ b. ni-s-neez   n.ipfv-1sg-tall   ‘I am tall’

3 In the North American tradition, the term absolutive sometimes refers to the default or unmarked form of a noun rather than the single argument of an intransitive verb (as most linguists use the term today). Grammatical descriptions of Luiseño often use this former, more traditional sense of the term. I retain that usage in the examples here.

216 

 Daniel W. Hieber

(12)            

Quileute (Chimakuan) (Andrade 1933: 267, 257) a. čaːč-a-ø   fly-dur-3abs   ‘it is/was flying’ b. tsiʔda-ʔa-ø   handsome-dur-3abs   ‘he is handsome’

(13)            

West Greenlandic (Eskaleut) (Fortescue 1984: 120, 121) a. isir-puq ingil-luni-lu   come_in-3sg.ind sit_down-4sg.ctmp-and   ‘she came in and sat down’ b. illu-at kusanar-puq   house-3pl.poss pretty-3sg.ind   ‘their house is pretty and warm’

Many North American languages make a distinction in their verbal person marking between agents – the argument in the clause that performs, effects, instigates, or controls the event – and patients – the argument in the clause which lacks one or more of these properties. In languages with property verbs that exhibit agent-patient marking, property verbs often use patient person markers, although this is just a typological tendency. Examples include Alabama (Muskogean; Wetzer [1996: 216–217]), Kiowa (Kiowa-Tanoan; Wetzer [1996: 187]), Lakota (Siouan; Pustet [2002: 388]), and Mojave (Yuman; Wetzer [1996: 187]), among many others. Central Pomo is one North American exception to this tendency: basic adjectives appear with agent markers unless they are inchoative (‘becoming’) (Mithun 1991: 521). As mentioned, property verbs often exhibit slightly different behavior from prototypical event verbs within a language. The most common difference is that property verbs are limited in the range of inflectional possibilities they can take (what Croft [1991; 2000; 2001; 2003; 2010] calls their behavioral potential). They may be limited to the stative or durative aspects, for example. Another common difference is that property verbs may modify nouns directly, but event verbs require some type of additional nominalizing or relativizing morphology to modify nouns. For instance, in a thorough review of evidence for adjective classes in several Siouan languages, Helmbrecht (2006; in progress) reports that in Hocank (a.k.a. Winnebago; Siouan) nouns may be modified using relative clauses. Relative clauses in Hocank are structurally nearly identical to noun phrases in the language. They typically require a determiner and person inflection, and they may take tense and aspect marking. However, when a property word is used to modify a noun, it does not require a determiner, is never inflected for person, and never takes tense or aspect marking. In Choctaw (Muskogean), property words are morphological verbs, but there are clear semantic regularities which set them apart from other verbs. In Muskogean languages, verb stems undergo certain phonological changes such as nasalization, h-inser-



Word classes 

 217

tion, lengthening, etc. to indicate their aspect. These sets of phonological changes are called grades in the Muskogean literature. When applied to property words, however, these grades have the semantic effect of intensifiers or comparatives rather than aspect (Haag 1995, 1997). Secondly, when these property words appear after nouns, they optionally show nominal morphology, with a penultimate pitch accent and final glottal stop (Broadwell 2006: 223). In the case of Nuuchahnulth, Nakayama (2001) argues that the adjective class is a discourse tendency rather than a clearly-defined set of properties that pick out a mutually exclusive set of words. He reports that adjectivals are words which do not take objects, and which may be combined with nominals to form a phrase (Nakayama 2001: 50). They may however sometimes also serve directly as arguments. A more unusual pattern of behavioral differences for property concepts occurs in Yurok, where numbers and about eleven roots expressing property concepts vary the form of their stem (that is, their stems are suppletive) based on the category of the noun they modify. The categories include human, animal, plant (non-tree), tree, stringlike, flat, and others. Each one of these stems in Yurok potentially has a different form of the stem for each one of these categories. Example (14) shows the different stems for ‘big’ and ‘small’. (14)

Yurok (Algic) (Robins 1958: 93–95)



Category

‘big’

‘small’

               

human beings animals and birds round things body parts, utensils, clothes stringlike things flat things houses boats

peloy plɹʔɹy ploy(keloy) plep plep ploks pleʔloy pleyteloy

cey(kel) cɹykɹʔɹy ceykoh cey(kel) cey(kel) cey(kel) ceykoh cey(kel)

In other languages there seems to be no substantive behavioral differences between property words and event verbs. In Seneca (Iroquoian), words expressing property concepts do belong to a subclass of verbs that are limited to stative aspect, but there are numerous other, non-property verbs which also belong to this class (Chafe 2012). Example (15) lists representative sets of property words and event words in Seneca, both of which are restricted to the stative aspect.

218 

(15)                              

 Daniel W. Hieber

Seneca (Iroquoian) (Chafe 2012: 13–14) a. osde’ ‘it’s heavy’   otgi’ ‘it’s dirty’   odö:sgwi:h ‘it’s wrinkled’   o:ni:yöh ‘it’s hard, tough’   ojiwagëh ‘it’s sour, bitter’   hohsë:h ‘he’s fat’   hodí’gyö’ ‘he’s shy’ b. otga:h ‘it’s making a noise’   owë h́ de’ ‘it has something added to it’   hotö:de’ ‘he hears it’   honö́hdö’ ‘he knows it’   hóío’de’ ‘he’s working’   hohse’ ‘he’s riding on its back’   ho:wísdagá’de’ ‘he has a lot of money’   ho’áshägéhde’ ‘he’s carrying a basket on his back’

Chafe (2012) considers seven possible criteria that might identify a class of adjectives in Seneca (and by extension all of Northern Iroquoian) and finds that all the criteria are subject to the same problem in that they include non-property concepts as well. Finally, some languages distribute property concepts across multiple word classes. In Chinook (Chinookan), words expressing speed, color, and a few words for human propensity are particles, while words expressing age are verbs, and words expressing dimension, value, and other human propensity concepts are nouns (Dixon 1977: 53–54). In sum, the encoding of property concepts in North American languages shows tremendous diversity. Some languages have a large, open class of distinct adjectives, others have a small closed class, but in most North American languages property words are a subset of either nouns or verbs. And in a few cases, even the existence of such a subclass is difficult to motivate.

9.4.1.4 Adverbs Adverbs, like adjectives, are words whose prototypical function is modify; however, they differ from adjectives in that adjectives only modify nouns, while adverbs may modify essentially anything else (Haspelmath 2001: 16543; Velupillai 2012: 130), including verbs (run quickly), adjectives (quite happy), other adverbs (very quickly), prepositions (right out), noun phrases (quite the party), entire utterances (unfortunately), but not individual nouns (*dog quickly) (Velupillai 2012: 130). Semantically, adverbs prototypically convey meanings such as manner (quickly), degree (extremely), time (now), location (there), or evidential/epistemic attitude (probably, frankly) (Quirk et al. 1985; Velupillai 2012: 130). Like adjectives, adverbs may have distinct forms for comparatives (faster), superlatives (fastest), and equatives (as fast as).



Word classes 

 219

Adverbial constructions are not strongly grammaticalized in North American languages. In a recent handbook of North American languages, discussions of adverbs appear only sparingly, and the term “adverb” does even not appear in the index (Siddiqi et al. 2020).4 Languages use a variety of other strategies for conveying prototypical manner concepts instead. In Dëne Sųłıné (Na-Dene), locative nouns may function as adverbs (Cook 2004: 303), and adverbs in most Dene languages are derived from relative clauses (Jaker, Welch, and Rice 2019: 497). Chitimacha has a set of suffixes expressing manner, including -di ‘doing horizontally’, -duwa ‘doing suddenly’, -kint ‘by pushing’, and -ti ‘by handling’. In Nuuchahnulth adverbial concepts like ‘also’, ‘for two days’, and ‘still’ are encoded with intransitive predicates (Nakayama 2001: 51–53). Nuuchahnulth also has a number of lexical suffixes expressing location, such as -ʽis ‘being on the beach’, -ʼas ‘being on the ground’, -ʼa· ‘being on the rock’, or -ʽiɬ ‘being in the house’. Otherwise, adverbial concepts are expressed through verb serialization. The examples below show serial verbs expressing manner, time, and location, respectively. (16)                              

Nuuchahnulth (Wakashan) (Nakayama 2001: 100) a. ƛawaʔiiʔaƛquuč kʷaačiƛ   ƛawa-ʔiː-ʼaƛ-quː-č kʷaː-či(ƛ)   near-reaching-tel-cond.3-infer move.backwards-mom   he.would.go.near move.backwards   ‘[While he was dancing] he would go near [him] moving backwards.’ b. qiis waɬyuu   qiː-s waɬ-yu·   for.long-1sg go.home-done   I.for.long at.home   ‘For a long time I stayed at home.’ c. yacaaqtuu ʔucačiƛ ʔuuƛ̓aqči   yac-a·qtu· ʔu-ca-či(ƛ) ʔuuƛ̓aqči   step-going.over it-going.to-mom name   walked.over.the.hill went.there Odlaqutla   ‘They went over [the high land] to Odlaqutla.’

9.4.2 Functional classes As mentioned in § 3.1, North American languages exhibit a large variety of function words. This section covers just a few functional classes for which North American languages exhibit unique or interesting behaviors – adpositions, articles, auxiliaries, particles, and pronouns. 4 This point is a comment on the structure of North American languages rather than a criticism of the volume’s coverage.

220 

 Daniel W. Hieber

9.4.2.1 Adpositions Adpositions are words that govern a noun phrase and signal a relationship between the noun phrase and another word in the clause (Hagège 2010: 1; Kurzon and Adler 2008: 2). It can be difficult to distinguish adpositions from case markers since they both signal relationships between elements of a clause, and there is often a diachronic and synchronic cline between them resulting from grammaticalization (Hagège 2010: Sec. 2.2). Adpositions may also be clitics (Hagège 2010: 18). There are several types of adpositions: prepositions precede the noun phrase, postpositions follow the noun phrase, and circumpositions consist of two elements, one which precedes the noun phrase and one which follows it. Shoshone (Uto-Aztecan) has another type called an inposition which occurs inside the noun phrase (Dryer 2013c). Many second-position clitics could also be considered a type of inposition. Perhaps because North American languages signal many of the relationships among participants using affixes (especially verbal affixes), adpositions are not a robust word class in most North American language families. Even when present in a language, they are sometimes a marginal class, only lightly grammaticalized. Creek (Muskogean), for example, has just a small set of nouns such as naɬkapá ‘middle’ which have grammaticalized into postpositions indicating location (Martin 2011: 147–148). Not all languages have adpositions however (Croft 1991: 144). In Nuuchahnulth, relationships between referents are always communicated by predicates, as shown below. (17)          

Nuuchahnulth (Wakashan) (Nakayama 2001: 53) šišaa ʔuuʔatup kʷakuucuk šiš-(y)a· ʔu-ʽatup kʷakuːc-uk clean-cont it-doing.for grandchild-poss cleaning doing.for.them her.grandchildren ‘She would peel them for her grandchildren.’

9.4.2.2 Articles As mentioned in § 4.1.1, one of the categories that can be indicated on a noun is definiteness. One way languages do this is by using definite/indefinite articles (such as the, a/ an in English). Lakota has a set of two definite articles and three indefinite articles with slightly different uses, exemplified in (18). Articles in Lakota follow their noun phrase rather than precede it. (18)          

Lakota (Siouan) (Van Valin 1977: 63) a. ki ‘the’ definite b. kʔũ ‘the aforementioned’ definite c. wã ‘a (specific)’ indefinite d. wãẑi ‘one (nonspecific)’ indefinite e. cha ‘a (contrastive)’ indefinite



Word classes 

 221

Quileute (Chimakuan) only has an indefinite article (Andrade 1933: 246). Chitimacha uses a demonstrative adjective as a definite article, a common strategy in North America (Dryer 2013b).

9.4.2.3 Auxiliaries Auxiliary verbs are verbs which provide grammatical information about the main verb they accompany (Velupillai 2012: 146). In example (19) from Southern Pomo (Pomoan), the future tense marker appears on the auxiliary verb yo- rather than the predicate kac:i ‘cold’. (19)      

Southern Pomo (Pomoan) (Walker 2013: 356) kac:i yo-kʰ:e cold aux-fut ‘it will be cold’

While the grammatical features that auxiliary verbs carry typically include tense, aspect, person, number, etc., a prevalent feature of North American languages is that auxiliary verbs may also provide information regarding the spatial orientation of their subjects – usually sitting, standing, or lying position. This is especially common in the Siouan languages (Mithun 1999: 115–116) and the languages of the U.S. Southeast (Campbell 1997: 342). See § 3.1 above for an illustration of positional auxiliary verbs in use in Siouan.

9.4.2.4 Particles Language descriptions often include a word class called particles, but particles are not a coherent typological class. The term “particle” is a morphological term, typically referring to words which are invariable and/or do not have inflectional morphology (Crystal 2008: 352). However, the functions of uninflectable words are not consistent across languages. Particles in Algonquian cover a wide array of functions such as quantifiers, numerals, adjectives, adverbs, prepositions, and conjunctions (Oxford 2007; 2019: 511). Particles in Chitimacha, on the other hand, are used for preverbs, postpositions, negation, topic marking, discourse markers, and interjections. There is no typological prototypical core to particles as a word class.

9.4.2.5 Pronouns Pronouns are words that either refer to discourse participants (I, you, s/he), refer anaphorically to referents that are activated in the discourse (Kibrik 2011: 73), or otherwise stand in for nouns. Pronouns referring to discourse participants are called personal

222 

 Daniel W. Hieber

pronouns, while the others are sometimes called proforms (Bhat 2004: 5). Personal pronouns may be syntactically free words (free pronouns), affixes on the verb (bound pronouns), or clitics (clitic pronouns). The following example from Mohawk (Iroquoian) includes both the free pronoun í:se’ ‘you’ and the bound pronoun sa- ‘you’. (20)                  

Mohawk (Iroquoian) (Mithun 2013: 292) Í:seʼ tókaʼ wà:kehreʼ tókaʼ thé:nenʼ íseʼ tokaʼ waʼ-k-ehr-eʼ tokaʼ thenenʼ you maybe fact-1sg.a-think-pfv maybe something you maybe I thought maybe something sarì:waienʼ ne ahsheriʼwanón:tonhseʼ. sa-rihw-a-ien-ʼ ne a-hshe-riʼwanonton-hs-eʼ 2sg.p-matter-have-stat the irr-2sg/fi-ask-ben.appl-pfv you issue have the you would ask her ‘I thought that you might have some questions to ask her.’

Southern Pomo has clitic pronouns rather than affixes: (21)        

Southern Pomo (Pomoan) (Walker 2013: 229) mihyanakʰ:eʔwamt̯ aʔa mihyana-kʰ:e=ʔwa=mt̯ a=ʔa kill-fut=cop.evid=2sg.p=1sg.a ‘I’m going to kill you.’

All languages have free pronouns, irrespective of whether they also have bound or clitic pronouns. In North American languages, discourse participants are predominantly expressed using bound pronouns on the verb (Dryer 2013a). In these languages, the functions of the pronouns are divided between the bound and free forms. The bound pronouns are used to refer to and track referents in the discourse, while the free pronouns accomplish the various other functions, such as focus/emphasis, cleft constructions, topicalization, antitopicalization, etc. (Mithun 2003; 2013). There are many types of proforms, including demonstrative (22), indefinite (23), interrogative (24), possessive (25), and relative (26), among others. (22)        

Demonstrative: Potawotami (Algic) (Lockwood 2017: 58) Apte ode gminen. apte ode gminen half dem.near I.give.you ‘I’ll give you half of this.’

(23)      

Indefinite: Seneca (Iroquoian) (Chafe 2015: 118) Ëké:owi’ sö:ga:’. I’ll tell them someone ‘I’ll tell someone.’



Word classes 

(24)        

Interrogative: Southern Pomo (Pomoan) (Walker 2013: 231) čaʔ:aʔkam:u ʔaṭʰ:a ʔahsoduy čaʔ:a=ʔka=m:u ʔaṭʰ:a ʔahso-duy-∅ who=inter=3sg gravel throw.many.small-dir-pfv ‘who threw the gravel?’

(25)      

Possessive: Creek (Muskogean) (Martin 2011: 144) ca-nâːki-t ôː-s 1sg.p-thing-t be.fgr-ind ‘It’s mine.’

(26)      

Relative: Tuscarora (Iroquoian) (Mithun 2012: 270) Thwé:ʼn waʼkayęʼnaʼnitʼúthahs haʼ káhneʼ kayęʼnęʼnę́nhyahr. all he put them to sleep the who they are guarding him ‘He put to sleep all those who were guarding him.’

 223

9.5 Issues in word-class research This section describes the most prominent themes in research on word classes in North America. The difficulties in determining word classes in North American languages are decidedly different from those presented by languages in other areas of the world. For North American languages, there are three recurring questions in the study of word classes and lexical categories in particular: 1) at what level a word is categorized (root, stem, or entire inflected word; § 5.1), 2) whether a given language distinguishes noun and verb (§ 5.2), and 3) whether a given language has an adjective category (which has already been discussed in § 4.1.3). The widespread (but not ubiquitous) presence of (poly)synthesis in North American languages (Mithun 2017b: 235; Rice [this volume]) means that a morphological distinction between nouns, verbs, and, when present, adjectives, is often quite clear. Words tend to have multiple affixes indicating their word class. In the following example from Nez Perce (Sahaptian), there are a tense marker and a perfective aspect marker – both categories typically associated with verbs. (27)      

Nez Perce (Sahaptian) (Deal 2010: 57) hi-pe-nees-ex-n-e 3.sbj-plSBJ-plobj-see-pfv-rem.pst ‘they saw us/you (pl.)/them’ 

Similarly, nouns in Nez Perce are marked for case (their role in the sentence) (Deal 2010: 32), a feature which is typically associated with nouns. Given these clear morphological distinctions, it may seem surprising that there could be any ambiguity regarding word classes in North American languages. Nonethe-

224 

 Daniel W. Hieber

less, the potential for ambiguity in lexical categories can occur at the root, stem, or even whole word level, and words may be categorized differently at different levels (Jacobsen 1979: 100; Mithun 1999: 56; Haag 2006: 143; Lois et al. 2017: 102; Mithun 2017a: 155; Clemens 2019: 372). Section  5.1 shows how this ambiguity surfaces at these different levels in the languages of North America, and how categorization depends on the level of analysis (root, stem, or word).

9.5.1 Locus of categoriality In morphologically complex languages, words have an internal structure, so that some morphemes are more central to the core meaning of the word than others. The morpheme that provides the core sense of a word is called the root. For example, in Chitimacha the root ni- ‘water’ is used as the base for a number of different words, including nen- ‘go out of water’, nicwa- ‘approach water’, nitgext- ‘dump into water’, niduwa- ‘fall into water’, and others (Swadesh 1939b: 44). Each of the forms just listed are called stems, defined as the part of the word which serves as the basis for all its inflected forms. The stem nicwa-, for example, serves as the base for the inflected forms nicwi ‘s/he approaches water’, nicwicuki ‘I will approach water’, nicwipuyna ‘they used to approach water’, etc. Each of these inflectional possibilities is called an inflected word. Words may be categorized differently depending on whether one is analyzing the root, stem, or inflected word. In the West Greenlandic (Eskaleut) language, the lexical category of a word is typically obvious at all three levels. In example (28) the nominal root aamaruti- ‘coal’ takes various suffixes which create new stems, changing the word at different points from a noun to a verb and back again. Affixes which change the class of a word are called derivational affixes. At each step of derivation in West Greenlandic, the category of the word is clear. (28)        

West Greenlandic (Eskaleut) (Fortescue 1984: 315) aamaruti-ssar-siur-vi-tua-a-suq coal-fut-look.for-place-only-be-intr.ptcp n>n>v>n>n>v>n ‘which is the only place for getting coal’

In other North American languages, roots do not seem to be categorized for word class. In these languages, stems can be categorized but roots cannot. Haag (2006) argues that Cherokee is one such language. Cherokee has many words which are composed of multiple roots compounded together; however, it is impossible to determine what the category of the resulting compound will be based on the roots. The roots are simply put together in a way that makes sense for their meanings, and then a suffix is added that clarifies the lexical category (Haag 2006: 138). Example (29) shows two roots in Cherokee.



(29)    

Word classes 

 225

Cherokee (Iroquoian) (Haag 2006: 137) a. -jaʔt- ‘attach asymmetrically at an indentation’ b. -húú- ‘stoma, opening’ (allomorph -ʔúú)

Example (30) shows four compounds that can be formed using these roots. (30)      

Cherokee (Iroquoian) (Haag 2006: 137–138) a. tii-húú-jaʔt-î   pl.obj-opening-attached-thing   ‘lunchbox (with two handles)’

                   

b.     c.       d.    

a-húú-jaʔt-î sg-opening-attached-thing ‘pitcher’ jii-ʔúú-jaʔt-v́vkâ 1sg-opening-attached-imm.pst ‘I just now attached a handle to something (e.  g., a bucket)’ ‘I just now caught something by the mouth with a hook or attachment.’ tee-jíí-ʔúú-jaʔt-v́vkâ pl.obj-1sg-opening-attached-imm.pst ‘I just now attached something with more than one handle to something.’

Though in each case the stem is formed from the same combination of roots, in (a) and (b) the result is a noun, and in (c) and (d) the result is a verb. Haag takes this and other evidence to suggest that lexical categorization is not relevant to Cherokee roots, only stems. A similar situation occurs in Algonquian languages, in which lexical stems are formed of a combination of up to three components, called initial, medial, and final in the Algonquian literature (Goddard 1990; Macaulay and Salmons 2017; Lockwood 2017: 63–64; Oxford [this volume]). The initial is generally considered the root of the word, but it is the final component which determines the lexical category of the stem. Roots in Algonquian languages are therefore unspecified for lexical category. Examples (31) and (32) demonstrate how the same initial (shown in boldface) can be used to form either a noun or verb stem in Ojibwe and Menominee (both Algonquian languages). (31)                

Ojibwe (Algic) (Nichols 2020) a. miskozi   miskw-izi   red-3sg.ind   ‘it is red’ b. miskobag   miskw-bagw   red-leaf   ‘red leaf’

226 

(32)                

 Daniel W. Hieber

Menominee (Algic) (Monica Macaulay, p.c.) a. maehkuakom   maehkw-akom   red-skin/hide/covering/garment   ‘red blanket’ b. maehkīhotaw   maehkw-hot-a‑w   red-paint‑theme-3sg   ‘s/he paints it red’

In the Ojibwe example in (31), the same initial miskw- ‘red’ is used to form both a noun ‘red leaf’ and a verb ‘it is red’, while in the Menominee example in (32) the initial maehkw- ‘red’ is likewise used to form both the noun ‘red blanket’ and the verb ‘s/he paints it red’. Thus, in Algonquian it is only stems which are categorized for lexical category, not the root components. In some languages, even the stem can be neutral or ambiguous with respect to lexical category. Frachtenberg (1922: 318) claims that any stem in Coos (Coosan) may be used either nominally or verbally as appropriate. This is illustrated in (33). (33)                                          

Coos (Coosan) (Frachtenberg 1922: 329, 330, 328) a. poːʷkw-is   slave-nmlz   ‘slave’ b. ŋ-poːʷkw-its   1sg-enslave-tr   ‘I enslaved him’ c. huːʷmis   ‘woman’ d. n̥-huːʷmis-its   1sg-marry-tr   ‘I marry (her)’ e. tsoːweˣtɬ   ‘grease’ f. n̥-tsoːʷˣtɬ-ts   1sg-grease-tr   ‘I greased it’ g. tɬʼkwiː   ‘blanket’ h. tɬʼkwi-t   cover-tr   ‘she covered (them) with blankets’



Word classes 

 227

For the Tonkawa (isolate) language, Hoijer (1933: 23–24) famously claimed, “To apply the classificatory notion of “parts of speech” to Tonkawa would do extreme violence to the spirit of the language.” He provides the following example as evidence of his claim: (34)            

Tonkawa (isolate) (Hoijer 1946: 297) a. notox-ʔaː-la   hoe-def-nom.sg   ‘the hoe’ b. notx-o-ʔ   hoe-decl-3.prs   ‘he hoes it’

Andrade (1933: 179) likewise analyzes Quileute as a language where stems may be used as either noun or verb, assuming their function in context. In other languages, such as Hopi (Uto-Aztecan), most stems are specified for category, but a subset are ambivalent and may be used as either noun or verb (Whorf 1946: 163). Even fully-inflected wordforms with clear morphological marking of their class may nonetheless blur the distinction between noun and verb. In many North American languages, fully-inflected morphological verbs may be used as nominals without any special affixes or other modification, as the following examples illustrate. (35)                    

Chitimacha (isolate) (Swadesh 1939b: 56; Swanton 1920: 17) a. dzampuyna   dza-m-puy-na   thrust-plact-hab-nf.pl   ‘they usually thrust/spear (with it)’   ‘spear’ b. pamtuyna   pa-m-tuy-na   ford-plact-hab-nf.pl   ‘they usually cross (it)’   ‘bridge’

(36)                    

Cayuga (Iroquoian) (Mithun 2000: 200) a. o̜tekho̜nyáʔthaʔ   ye-ate-khw-o̜ni-aʔt-haʔ   indf.a-refl-meal-make-ins-ipfv   ‘one makes a meal with it’   ‘restaurant’ b. kao̜tanéhkwi   ka-ro̜t-a-nehkwi   neut.a-log-ep-haul.ipfv   ‘it hauls logs’   ‘horse’

228 

(37)              

 Daniel W. Hieber

Navajo (Na-Dene) (Young 1989: 316) a. tsinaaʼeeɬ   tsi(n)-naaʼeeɬ   wood-it.moves.about.floating   ‘ship, boat’ b. chahaɬheeɬ   it.is.dark   ‘darkness’

For Cayuga (and other Iroquoian languages), some morphological verbs have been so fully lexicalized as nouns that they may no longer be used with their verbal meanings. The default meaning of kao̜tanéhkwi for Cayuga speakers is ‘horse’, not ‘it hauls logs’. Other verbs may retain both uses, while others lack any nominal meaning at all. Morphological verbs in Iroquoian therefore each sit on a cline from fully verbal to fully nominal, with many cases in between (Mithun 2000). In other languages, fully-inflected nouns and verbs can appear superficially similar, taking affixes of the exact same form, but nonetheless belong to clearly distinct word classes. In Central Alaskan Yup’ik, for example, the forms of noun inflections are a subset of the forms of verb inflections (Sadock 1999: 386). That is, noun endings all look like verb endings (but not vice versa), and even have similar functions, as the following examples illustrate: (38)            

Central Alaskan Yup’ik (Eskaleut) (Mithun 2017a: 161) a. qaya-q ‘kayak’ sg   kaigtu-q ‘he/she/it is hungry’ sg b. qaya-k ‘two kayaks’ du   kaigtu-k ‘they two are hungry’ du c. qaya-t ‘three or more kayaks’ pl   kaigtu-t ‘they all are hungry’ pl

Possessive suffixes on nouns likewise share their forms with transitive person suffixes on verbs: (39)                    

Central Alaskan Yup’ik (Eskaleut) (Mithun 2017a: 161) a. angya-qa ‘my boat’ 1sg/3sg   ner’a-qa ‘I am eating it’ 1sg/3sg b. angya-gka ‘my two boats’ 1sg/3du   ner’a-gka ‘I am eating both of them’ 1sg/3du c. angya-nka ‘my boats’ 1sg/3pl   ner’a-nka ‘I am eating them’ 1sg/3pl d. angya-a ‘his/her boat’ 3sg/3sg   nera-a ‘he/she/it is eating it’ 3sg/3sg e. angya-k ‘his/her two boats’ 3sg/3du   ner’a-k ‘he/she/it is eating both of them’ 3sg/3du



   

Word classes 

f.  

angya-i nera-i

‘his/her boats’ ‘he/she/it is eating them’

 229

3sg/3pl 3sg/3pl

However, any transitive verb whose object is not a third person has suffixes which never appear in nominal inflections, such as the examples in (40). (40)        

Central Alaskan Yup’ik (Eskaleut) (Sadock 1999: 386) takua-anga ‘s/he sees me’ 3sg/1sg takua-atigut ‘s/he sees us’ 3sg/1pl takua-akkit ‘I see you (sg.)’ 1sg/2sg takua-rma ‘you (sg.) see me’ 2sg/1sg

The reason for these similarities is that many verbal inflections arose historically from nominalizations (Jacobson 1982; Woodbury 1985; Mithun 2008; Berge 2016). This is an example of a process known as insubordination, where subordinate clauses or noun phrases are reanalyzed as main clauses (Evans 2007; Mithun 2008; Evans and Watanabe 2016). Despite having a common origin as noun suffixes, verbal and nominal endings in Yup’ik are now nonetheless two distinct sets of affixes belonging to different word classes. Another case of superficial similarity between nouns and verbs comes from Menominee: (41)                

Menominee (Algic) (Monica Macaulay p.c.) a. askēhnen     askēhnen-w     be.fresh-3sg     ‘it is fresh/raw’   b. askēhnen   askēhnen-w   be.fresh-nmlz   ‘raw thing’

       (Monica Macaulay p.c.)

While the words in (41) have the same surface and underlying forms,5 this is merely a historical accident; the third person -w suffix and the nominalizing -w suffix are unrelated. Not only the category label, but the size of the category can vary depending on the level of analysis. Lindsey and Scancarelli (1985), for example, argue that Cherokee has a large, open class of adjectives when considering the level of the inflected word, but a small, closed class of adjectives when considering the level of the root. More drastically, Chitimacha lacks adjective stems entirely, but nonetheless has an open class of adjectives at the word level. All adjectives in Chitimacha are formed by adding an adjectivizing suffix to a verb stem, as shown in the examples in (42). 5 Note that the final /w/ in both examples is lost due to a synchronic process of final consonant cluster reduction.

230 

(42)            

 Daniel W. Hieber

Chitimacha (isolate) (Swadesh 1939b) bixtigi ‘industrious’ affix.

An independent word (also referred to as particle) may become cliticized and affixed, after which the negative is renewed. That presumably happened in (4), where te- represents the older negative that is now attached to the verb and tąʔą is the renewal. Grammaticalization followed by renewal can be seen as a cycle and, in the case of negatives, it is called the Negative Cycle (as in Jespersen 1917). Mithun (2016; 2018b) provides examples of both grammaticalization and renewal in a number of NA languages and I too will use this cycle to describe the variation among closely related languages. Another example of variation due to grammaticalization and renewal is found in Yuman. Taking a snip from the map in Figure 1, we see Jamul Tiipay listed as having a

Negatives 

 271

particle (dark blue), Hualapai and Diegueño with a negative auxiliary (light blue), and Maricopa as having double negation (yellow).

Hualapai    Maricopa       Diegueño/Kumeyaay          Jamul Tiipay

Fig. 2: Variation in Yuman negatives

This variety shows the result of a Yuman Negative Cycle. The Jamul Tiipay from is an original negative verb with a switch reference marker, i.  e., maaw; the Hualapai and Diegueño/Kumeyaay negatives are still inflected verbs; and the Maricopa original negative verb aw is now a suffix –ma with a clause marker (w)aly- as its renewal. This reconstruction is from Munro (1974a) and will be discussed more in section 4.

11.3 Negatives and word order In this section, I discuss the position of the negative in the sentence as early (in the clause) or as late. For the most part, the position of the negative depends on the position of the verb. Negation typically precedes the verb (Dryer 2013b) and therefore will be sentence-initial in verb-initial languages and more towards the end in verb-final languages. The languages of North America are no exception to this pattern. All 11 VSO languages listed in WALS are negative-initial, namely Kalispel, Sahaptin, Quileute, Makah, Halkomelem, Musqueam, Squamish, Kyuquot, Heiltsuk, Bella Coola, and Nisgha. An example from one of these languages, Squamish, is shown in (9), and an example from Nuuchahnulth, a verb-initial language not included in the WALS sample, is shown in (10). (9)      

Squamish (Salish) (Kuipers 1967: 194, cited after Kroeber 1999: 156) háw q-ʔan-c’ic’áp’ ti-scíʔs neg irr-1S-work art-today ‘I don’t work today.’

272 

(10)        

 Elly van Gelderen

Nuuchahnulth (Wakashan) (Nakayama 2001: 119) wikiimits wik -i:p -it -s neg obtaining pst 1S ‘I didn’t catch anything’

The sole VOS language listed in WALS, Chinook, is negative-initial as well. This pattern of verb-initial with the negative in pre-verbal position is also known from Austronesian and Celtic languages. The negative appears in initial position with other word orders as well. WALS (map 144P) gives a few examples, e.  g., Northern Paiute, Pima Bajo, Mono, Wichita, Cupeño, Kiowa, and Comanche, as (11) shows, where the verb is in final position. (11)      

Comanche (Uto-Aztecan) (Charney 1993: 220, gloss simplified) ke nɨɨ toHtɨn-kahtu miʔaRƗ neg 1S [name]-toward go ‘I didn’t go to Lawton.’

Languages that place verbs in final position often have a later negative, either preceding or following the verb. The 24 North American languages in the WALS atlas (Dryer 2013b) that have the negation placed in final position are all OV. This pattern of verb-last and late negation is also known from other languages, e.  g., from those in the Indo-European family. The North American OV-languages where negation is last include Hare (6), Chipewyan (7), Osage, Lakhota, and Diegueño (12); on the other hand, Hopi, as in (13), Tlingit, and Cahuilla have a negative that precedes the final verb. (12)      

Diegueño/Kumeyaay (Yuman) (Munro 1976a, cited after Langdon 1970) ʔ-a:m-x ʔ-ma:w 1-go-irr 1-neg ‘I don’t/didn’t go.’

(13)      

Hopi (Uto-Aztecan) (Kalectaca 1978: 41) I’ nagahu qa wehe. this medicine neg spill ‘This medicine didn’t spill.’

The negative in (12) develops from a negative verb as Munro (1974a: 37) argues. Note the inflection which makes it an auxiliary verb (light blue in Figure 2), and not a particle (dark blue). In some languages, as shown in Figure 1, there are two negative markers. In Athabascan, the sentence initial negative (e.  g., doo in (2)) is the more recent renewal and the final negative (e.  g., da in (2)) is the older one. Similar renewals can be found in Hare (6) and Apache (14), where doo is optional.

Negatives 

(14)      

 273

(Western) Apache (Bray and The White Mountain Apache Tribe 1998: 109) (doo) nchad da neg 2S.cry neg ‘Don’t you cry.’

This development fits the Negative Cycle, mentioned around (6). Again, Mithun (2016; 2018b) presents other such cases. Concluding, we can say that the negative is typically close to the verb: in initial position when the verb is initial and final when the verb is final. Renewed negation results in the double marking of Figure 1/Table1.

11.4 Negative verbs and auxiliaries Payne (1985: 222) argues that the origin of a negative marker is often the negative of the verb ‘to be’ and Givón (1978) mentions a negative verb approximating ‘to lack’ as a source. Mithun (2018b: 315–320) provides examples of negatives deriving from verbs meaning ‘to die/not exist’ and I will add others. NA languages show many negatives that are still verbs, e.  g., Diegueño (12), or that occur in structures that consist of two clauses, e.  g., Salish and Algonquian, discussed below. The latter is evidence of the verbal nature of the negative. In this section, I first sketch the reconstruction in Yuman mentioned before and in Uto-Aztecan and Tanoan that show the negative is verbal in origin. I then look at Salish and Algonquian for evidence of more than one clause, with the negative originating in a cleft construction. I finish with Athabascan, Muskogean and geographically related isolates, and EskimoAleut.

11.4.1 Yuman, Uto-Aztecan, and Tanoan Compare the data from Maricopa in (1) to those in Diegueño in (12) and Mohave in (15). This shows the negative affixes -ma and -mo in Maricopa and Mohave, respectively, but an independent verb in Diegueño. (15)      

Mohave (Yuman) (Munro 1976a: 106 with gloss adapted) ʔnyeč ʔ-iyem-mo-t-m I  1-go-neg-emph-real ‘I didn’t go’

Munro (1976a) reconstructs (16), where aw is a verb that has a clause to its left. The –m in the embedded clause marks that the subject is different from that of the main clause (also known as a switch reference marker).

274 

(16)    

 Elly van Gelderen

(S O V]-m aw       neg-Verb ‘It is not that (I went)’

In Diegueño (and Tolapaya Yavapai), the negative main verb is still inflected but, in Maricopa and Mohave, it is a suffix. Munro (1976: 65–8) shows that Mohave has the complex verbal negative suffix -m-ot- because, when the realis marker p-č is used in (17), the negative is split. The -m is therefore an earlier Different Subject marker. (17)      

Mohave (Yuman) (Munro 1976a: 106 with gloss adapted) ʔnyeč ʔ-iyem-m-p-o-t-č I  1-go-neg-real-neg-emph-real ‘I didn’t go’

More evidence that what remains in –o/-a:w/-a is originally a verb *–aw, with an embedded clause to its left is that the realis marker of the entire sentence, which is lexically determined by the main verb, is determined by the negative, i.  e., it has to be –m, as in (15), or –p-č, as in (17); it cannot be –k, even if the lower clause requires that. Uto-Aztecan uses particles for which Langacker (1977: 33) suggests two reconstructed forms, *ka and *ka-y. This is clear from comparing negation in some of the languages, e.  g., either káa in Yaqui (Dedrick and Casad 19992), ki in River Warihío (Armendáriz 2007: 1063), qa in Hopi (12) (Kalectaca 1978: 39), and ke in Comanche (Charney 1993: 220) or k…y, e.  g., qay in Luiseño (Hyde 1971: 13), kai in Shoshone (Crum and Dayley 1993: 11), Cupeño qay (Jacobs 1972: 71), Cahuilla kilye (Hioki 1971: 24), and Northern Paiute kai (Nichols 1974: 228). Miller (1967: 49) also reconstructs *ka and *kai. According to Langacker, the latter is a negative and the verb ‘to be’, forming a negative cleft, as in ‘It is not the case that …’.4 Kroskrity (1984) shows that, in Arizona Tewa, a Tanoan language, there are two negative markers, the prefix we- and the suffix –dí, as in (18). The suffix –dí is also used for subordinate clauses in (19). This dual use points to a bi-clausal origin of the negative where we in (18) was the main negative and mun the subordinate verb, perhaps cleftlike, as in ‘It was not the case that the man saw the woman’. The original subordinator was then reanalyzed as part of the negative complex.

2 Although Yaqui káa can have enclitic subject pronouns when it appears clause-initially. 3 River Warihío procliticizes on the lexical verb and cliticizes to ‘te in existential and copular constructions. 4 Langacker (1977: 33) cites an unpublished paper by Munro (1974b) with another possible bi-clausal reconstruction where *-y is a nominalizer with an optional case suffix *ta for forms such as Tubatulabal haainda.

Negatives 

(18)      

Arizona Tewa (Kiowa-Tanoan) (Kroskrity 1984: 95) sen kwiyó we-mán-mun-dí man woman neg-3/3-see-neg ‘The man didn’t see the woman.’

(19)      

Arizona Tewa (Kiowa-Tanoan) (Kroskrity 1984: 95) he’i sen na-mɛn- dí ‘o-yohk’ó that man 3-go-sub 1-be.asleep ‘When that man went, I was asleep.’

 275

In fact, all Tanoan languages (except Jemez) have a prefix and suffix where the latter is identical with a subordinator. The data for Picuris Tiwa are given in (20). (20)      

Picuris Tiwa (Kiowa-Tanoan) (Zaharlick 1977: 238, cited after Kroskrity 1984: 96) ‘u-wa-wəle-mᶒ 3P-neg-go-sub.pres ‘They are not going.’

11.4.2 Salish and Algonquian Salishan languages have many constructions where a negative is followed by a subordinate structure, which is indicative of the negative’s earlier verbhood (see Kroeber 1999: 99; 108; 138; 155–9; 204; 207; 213; 230–2). For instance, in Kalispel (21), the verb following the negative has a possessive inflection and nominalizer, which means the verb looks like an earlier embedded structure. The i- possessive and s- nominalizer only occur with a negative particle, which currently no longer looks like a verb. (21)      

Kalispel (Salish) (Vogt 1940: 75, cited after Kroeber 1999: 108) tá i-s-ən-té neg 1s.pos-nom-in-think ‘I don’t think so,’

Davis (2005: 50) argues that Proto-Salish had two patterns that “probably involved a biclausal structure” with a negative verb selecting either a nominalized complement, as in (21), or a subordinate clausal one, marked by CJ (conjunctive) in (22). (22)      

Comox (Salish) (Kroeber 1999: 156) xwuxwaʔ=č huǰ-an neg=1s finish-1s.cj ‘I am not finished yet.’

276 

 Elly van Gelderen

Algonquian languages are well-known for having some negators that choose the conjunct verb form (typical for embedded clauses) rather than the independent one. MacKenzie (1992) summarizes the negatives for the Cree-Montagnais-Naskapi continuum. A variant of nama is used with independent verbs and eka with conjunct verbs in Plains Cree, Moose Cree, and Swampy Cree. Montagnais has the innovative apu, typically with a conjunct verb, instead of nama. Variation has been reported as to independent and conjunct forms with the negatives.

11.4.3 Athabascan, Eskimo-Aleut, Muskogean and neighboring isolates As mentioned in connection with Athabascan in section 3, a number of scholars reconstruct the negative in (6) and (7) (either particle or suffix) as a verb. So, yíle in (6) and hilɛ in (7) are seen as erstwhile negative auxiliaries or copula verbs. For reasons of space, I will not go into this more here, but see Leer (2000: 123), Rice (1989: 1108, n. 1), and van Gelderen (2011: 325–6). This verbal origin is also the reason that they vary for tense, mood, and aspect (TMA), as will be shown in section 5. The renewal mentioned around (14) is not of verbal origin. According to Fortescue (1999), Eskimo-Aleut also uses a negative enclitic based on a copular stem, the u- in –ulax in (23). Other tenses/aspects use different markers. (23)      

Aleut (Eskimo-Aleut) (Fortescue 1999, fn 5) awa-za-ĝ-ulax work-hab-3s.neg ‘He never works.’

In Alabama and Koasati, Muskogean languages, the negative is complex because of the relationship between agreement and the negative form. This connection is to be expected if the negative is originally a verb or auxiliary. Haas (1977) argues for such a connection on the basis of the negative verbs present in Chitimacha, an isolate in the same ‘Gulf’ region. Montler and Hardy (1991: 16) do not agree and argue that the origin as an auxiliary is not so much “by weight of evidence” but they do not “offer a better reconstruction”. From a grammaticalization point of view, Haas’ reconstruction makes sense. Chitimacha has a clearly inflected negative auxiliary, as in (24), as well as an uninflected variant in (25). The lexical verb kaakwi in (24) is clearly marked as non-finite (through GER(und)) whereas the lexical verb in (25) is inflected for person and number and the negative is a particle. (24)      

Chitimachi (isolate) (Hieber 2015: 3, unpublished material from Swadesh) we qaxinjadi cuntk hi waytm kaakwi gay-ik. old man about more know.ger neg-1sg ‘I do not know any more about the old man.’

Negatives 

(25)      

 277

Chitimachi (isolate) (Hieber 2015: 3, unpublished material from Swadesh) waqax qam haanaa ne kaakw-iki gan. others what they.happen even know-1sg neg ‘I do not know what happened to the others.’

In this section, several North American families/languages have been shown to have a negation pattern focused on the negative verb. Although the Uralic languages, some Nilo-Saharan languages, and a few others also show auxiliary negation on the WALS map, this pattern is not that wide-spread. Austronesian negation is ambiguous between particle and verb as well. Related to the verbal status of the negative is the specialization of the negative for TMA which I will turn to next.

11.5 Negatives specialized for tense, mood, and aspect (TMA) Languages of North America specialize as to whether their negative modifies a particular tense or aspect or mood. In main and subordinate clauses, the negative may be different because the mood of the main clause is independent and that of the subordinate is usually dependent. Cross-linguistically, a very frequent special negative is the prohibitive mood (a negative imperative), and I will start with the prohibitive in Zuni. I then examine Athabascan and Central Pomo negatives that are marked for aspect. Languages can have the regular negative in the imperative or a special one. Van der Auwera and Lejeune’s (2013) map shows that NA languages are varied. Maricopa, Diegueño, Pomo, Klamath, Lakhota, Wichita and others use the same negative for declaratives and imperatives but Zuni, Acoma, Oneida, Northern Paiute, Mandan, and others use a different form. For instance, Zuni, an isolate, distinguishes between imperative/permissive verbs on the one hand and verbs inflected for person on the other (Newman 1965: 73), which are declarative. The former have the negative particle ʔełł, as in (26), whereas the latter have kwaʔ, as in (27), both in clause-initial position. In addition, there is a form of naʔma that attaches to the verb in both (26) and (27). (26)      

Zuni (isolate) (Newman 1965: 74) ʔełł tešlan-naʔma-ø neg afraid-neg-imp ‘Don’t be afraid.’

(27)      

Zuni (isolate) (Newman 1965: 74) kwaʔ tešlan-nam-ka neg afraid-neg-pst ‘He wasn’t afraid.’

278 

 Elly van Gelderen

Bunzel (1935: 513–5) adds more on the variation of naʔma when used with present tense and certain aspects. It is optional when the negative subjunctive is marked by a suffix –cukwa in addition to the initial kwaʔ, as in (28). (28)      

Zuni (isolate) (Bunzel 1935: 514; Bunzel 1933: 57, line 97) k waʔ ał-cukwa neg sleep-neg.subj ‘She will not sleep (if the baby cries).’

As for the connection between TMA and the negative, we have already seen some evidence for that link in the previous section. For example, Picuris Tiwa (20), Aleut, and Muskogean make such connections. Here I discuss Athabascan and Pomoan. Leer (2000: 103) finds a connection between negation and verbal aspect in Athabascan. In many languages, e.  g., Ahtna (29), the negative replaces conjugation markers, such as the ø-imperfective, with a negative s- prefix, as shown in the non-negative imperfective (30). (29)      

Ahtna (Athabascan) (Kari 1992: 123) ‘ele’ k’e-s-t’aaz-e neg indef-neg-cut-neg ‘He isn’t cutting something.’

(30)      

Ahtna (Athabascan) (Kari 1992: 123) k’e-t’aas indef-cut.impf ‘He’s cutting it.’

In addition to the –s, there is a negative suffix –e, probably a remnant of a copula, and a renewed form ‘ele’. Mithun (1998) presents data from Central Pomo (Pomoan) where two negatives (generally5) specialize for imperfective or perfective aspect. The čhów negative in (31) marks the perfective whereas the t̪ hín in (32) is used for everything else. (31)      

Central Pomo (Pomoan) (Mithun 1998: 78) ʔaˑ čá-ˑn-ka-w čhów. 1.agt run-impf-caus-prf neg ‘I didn’t drive.’

(32)      

Central Pomo (Pomoan) (Mithun 1998: 78) t̪ áwhal yhé-t̪ -aʔ t̪ hín =ka. work do-multiple-impf.pl neg=inferential ‘They must not be working.’

5 As Mithun (1998) points out, there is fluidity depending on pragmatic choices.

Negatives 

 279

This section has provided examples of languages where the negative depends on the mood or aspect of the clause. This is to be expected if the negative derives from a verb or auxiliary, which are marked for TMA. Next, I turn to negative pronouns which can turn a sentence into a negative as well.

11.6 Negative indefinite pronouns In this section, I show that the negative indefinite in NA languages is often based on an indefinite pronoun and may occur with sentential negation. The WALS map (Haspelmath 2013) shows that all NA languages listed use an additional sentential negation but that is hard to argue in languages like Navajo, as I will show. Haspelmath’s (1997) overview of how indefinite pronouns are marked in a sample of 140 languages includes eight NA languages. His NA data show that all, except Slave, have interrogative-based positive indefinites. For instance, in O’odham (Uto-Aztecan), heḍai means ‘who’ but, when combined with the negative pi, it means ‘nobody.’ The same is true in Hopi (33). This word has to occur at the beginning of the clause, unlike the sentential negative qa in (34), which occurs before the verb. Yaqui and Tümpisa Shoshone follow a similar pattern, according to Haspelmath. (33)      

Hopi (Uto-Aztecan) (Malotki 1979: 123, Haspelmath 1997: 251) Qa-háqaqw kwii-kwitsi. neg-where rdp-smoke ‘Smoke comes out nowhere.’

(34)      

Hopi (Uto-Aztecan) (Kalectaca 1978: 41) I’ nagahu qa wehe. this medicine neg spill ‘This medicine didn’t spill’.

In Mohave (35), there is a special kind of negative that occurs with indefinites, so again no special negative indefinites appear. (35)      

Mohave (Yuman) (Munro 1976b: 71) kuč ʔ-ičo:-poʔa:və something 1-make-neg ‘I am not making anything.’

The indefinites are really positive on their own. That is true in Navajo as well where the negative indefinite háída also translates as ‘someone’ so the sentential negation is responsible for the negative indefinite meaning, as in (36).

280 

(36)      

 Elly van Gelderen

Navajo (Athabascan) (Hale and Platero n.d.) doo háída níyáa da neg someone arrived neg ‘Nobody has arrived.’

Indefinites go through cycles, from indefinites with sentential negation, as in (35), to negative indefinites, as in (33), to adding another negative (not shown). Cf. Willis (2010) for a cycle involving these pronouns. In short, cross-linguistically in the NA languages, there are no special negative indefinites. The data show that a negative is either affixed to an indefinite pronoun, as in Hopi, or that, as in Mohave, the indefinite occurs in a negative clause.

11.7 Conclusion All languages have negative markers, and North American languages are no exception. The negatives can be affixes or particles and are often placed close to the verb. In many of the languages we have discussed, negatives show evidence of (still) being verbs or auxiliaries or having (once) been verbal. The negative form also differs depending on the tense, mood, and aspect (TMA) of the clause. Finally, negative indefinites are often formed by combining the negative with an indefinite or interrogative pronoun.

References Armendáriz, Rolando. 2007. A Grammar of River Warihío. Munich: Lincom. Bray, Dorothy & the White Mountain Apache Tribe. 1998. Western Apache-English Dictionary. Tempe: Bilingual press. Bunzel, Ruth. 1933. Zuni Texts. New York: G.E Stechert. Bunzel, Ruth. 1935. Zuni. New York: G.E Stechert. Charney, Jean Ormsbee. 1993. A Grammar of Comanche. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Cook, Eung-Do. 2004. A Grammar of Dëne Sųłiné (Chipewyan). Winnipeg: Algonquian and Iroquoian Linguistics. Crum, Beverly & Jon Philip Dayley. 1993. Western Shoshoni Grammar. Boise State University. Dahl, Östen. 1979. Typology of sentence negation. Linguistics 17. 79–106. Davis, Henry. 2005. On the syntax and semantics of negation in Salish. International Journal of American Linguistics 71(1). 1–55. Dedrick, John & Eugene Casad. 1999. Sonora Yaqui Language Structures. Tucson: University of Arizona Press. Dryer, Matthew. 2013a. Order of Negative Morpheme and Verb. In Matthew Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online, Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. (Available online at http://wals.info/chapter/143, Accessed on 2018-05-28.)  Dryer, Matthew. 2013b. Position of Negative Morpheme with Respect to Subject, Object, and Verb. In Matthew Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig:

Negatives 

 281

Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. (Available online at http://wals.info/chapter/144, Accessed on 2018-05-28.) Dryer, Matthew. 2013c. Negative Morphemes. In Matthew Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. (Available online at http://wals.info/chapter/112, Accessed on 2018-06-13.)  Fortescue, Michael. 1999. The rise and fall of polysynthesis in the Eskimo-Aleut family. In Nicholas Evans & Hans-Jürgen Sasse (eds.), Problems of polysynthesis, 257–75. Berlin: Akademie Verlag. Gelderen, Elly van. 2008a. The Negative Cycle. Linguistic Typology 12(2). 195–243. Gelderen, Elly van. 2008b. Cycles of Negation in Athabaskan. Working Papers in Athabaskan Languages 7. 49–64. Gelderen, Elly van. 2011. The Linguistic Cycle. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Givón, Talmy. 1978. Negation in Language. In Peter Cole (ed.), Syntax & Semantics 9, 69–112. New York: Academic Press. Gordon, Lynn. 1986. Maricopa Morphology and Syntax. Berkeley: University of California Press. Haas, Mary. 1977. From auxiliary verb phrase to inflectional suffix. In Charles Li (ed)., Mechanisms of Syntactic Change, 525–38. Austin: University of Texas Press. Hale, Ken & Paul Platero. n.d. Negative Polarity Expressions in Navajo. Unpublished manuscript. http:// lingphil.mit.edu/papers/hale/papers/hale014.pdf Haspelmath, Martin. 1997. Indefinite Pronouns. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Haspelmath, Martin. 2013. Negative Indefinite Pronouns and Predicate Negation. In Matthew Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online, Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. (Available online at http://wals.info/chapter/115, Accessed on 2018-08-25.) Hieber, Daniel. 2015. To be not or to not be. Unpublished manuscript. (https://files.danielhieber.com/ publications/Chitimacha-negation-behavioral-profile/pdf.pdf) Hioki, Kojiro. 1971. Zur Beschreibung des Systems der Klitika im Cahuilla. Unpublished manuscript. Hyde, Villiana. 1971. Introduction to the Luiseno Language. Banning: Malki Museum Press. Jacobs, Roderick. 1972. Syntactic Change: a Cupan Case Study. UCSD Dissertation. Jespersen, Otto. 1917 [1966]. Negation in English and other Languages. Copenhagen: A.F. Høst. Kalectaca, Milo. 1978. Lessons in Hopi. Tucson: University of Arizona Press. Kari, James. 1992. Some Concepts in Ahtna Athabaskan word formation. In Mark Aronoff (ed.), Morphology Now, 107–131. Albany: SUNY Press. Kopris, Craig Alexander. 2001. A Grammar and Dictionary of Wyandot. PhD SUNY Buffalo. Kroeber, Paul. 1999. The Salish Language Family. Lincoln: The University of Nebraska Press. Kroskrity, Paul. 1984. Negation and subordination in Arizona Tewa. International Journal of American Linguistics 50. 94–104. Kuipers, Aert. 1967. The Squamish Language. The Hague: Mouton. Langacker, Ronald. 1977. Studies in Uto-Aztecan Grammar, I. Arlington: SIL. Langdon, Margaret. 1970. A Grammar of Diegueño: The Mesa Grande Dialect. Berkeley: University of California. Leer, Jeff. 2000. The Negative/Irrealis category in Athabaskan-Eyak-Tlingit. In Ted Fernald & Paul Platero (eds.), The Athabaskan Languages, 101–138. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Li, Fang Kuei 1967. Chipeywan. In C. Osgood & H. Hoijer (Eds.), Linguistic Structures of Native America. Viking Fund Publications 6: 398-423. [reprinted from 1946] MacKenzie, Marguerite 1992. Negative Markers in East Cree and Montagnais. Papers of the 23rd Algonquian Conference, Carleton Ottawa. 274–284. Malotki, Ekkehart. 1979. Hopi-Raum: Eine sprachwissenschaftliche Analyse der Raumvorstellungen in der Hopi-Sprache. Tübingen: Gunter Narr Verlag. Miestamo, Matti. 2003. Clausal Negation: A Typological Study. Helsinki: University of Helsinki Doctoral Dissertation.

282 

 Elly van Gelderen

Miller, Amy. 2001. Grammar of Jamul Tiipay. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Miller, Wick. 1967. Uto-Aztecan Cognate Sets. Berkeley: University of California. Mithun, Marianne. 1995. Affixation and Morphological Longevity. In Geert Booij & Jaap van Marle (eds.), Yearbook of Morphology 1994, 73–97. Dordrecht: Springer. Mithun, Marianne. 1998. Fluid Aspects of Negation in Central Pomo. In Leanne Hinton & Pamela Munro (eds.), Studies in American Indian Languages, 77–86. Berkeley: University of California Press. Mithun, Marianne. 2016. What cycles when and why? In Elly van Gelderen (ed.), Cyclical Change Continued, 19–45. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Mithun, Marianne. 2018a. Negation against a polysynthetic backdrop: Mohawk. Presentation at Syntax of the World’s Languages 8, Paris, 3–5 September. Mithun, Marianne. 2018b. Shaping typology through grammaticalization: North America. In Heiko Narrog & Bernd Heine (eds.), Grammaticalization from a typological perspective, 309–336. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Montler, Timothy & Heather Hardy. 1991. The phonology of negation in Alabama. IJAL 57(1). 1–23. Munro, Pamela. 1974a. Topics in Mohave Syntax. La Jolla, CA: UCSD Dissertation. Munro, Pamela. 1974b. On the Morphology of Shoshonean Negatives. Unpublished manuscript. Munro, Pamela 1976a. Subject Copying, Auxiliarization, and Predicate Raising: The Mojave Evidence. International Journal of American Linguistics 42.2: 99–112. Munro, Pamela. 1976b. Mojave Syntax. New York: Garland. Nakayama, Toshihide. 2001. Nuuchanulth (Nootka) Morphosyntax. Berkeley: University of California. Newman, Stanley. 1965. Zuni Grammar. Albuquerque: UNM Press. Nichols, Michael. 1974. Northern Paiute Historical Grammar. University of California Berkeley Dissertation. Payne, Thomas. 1985. Negation. In Timothy Shopen (ed.), Language typology and syntactic description, vol. I, 197–242. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Rice, Keren. 1989. A Grammar of Slave. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. van der Auwera, Johan. 2010. On the diachrony of negation. In Larry Horn (ed.), The Expresssion of negation, 73–101. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. van der Auwera, Johan & Ludo Lejeune (with Valentin Goussev). 2013. The Prohibitive. In Matthew Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. (Available online at http://wals.info/chapter/71, Accessed on 2018-10-09.) Vogt, Hans. 1940. The Kalispel Language. Oslo: Det Norske Videnskaps Akademie. Willis, David. 2010. Negative polarity and the Quantifier Cycle. http://rhssl1.uni-regensburg.de/SlavKo/ conferences/gces/abstracts/Willis.pdf Young, Robert W. & William Morgan. 1987. The Navajo Language: A Grammar and Colloquial Dictionary. Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press. Zaharlick, Ann Marie. 1977. Picuris syntax. Washington D.C.: American University Dissertation.

Olga Lovick

12 Questions and requests in North American languages Abstract: Questions and requests are speech acts that call upon the hearer to do something. Questions typically ask for a verbal response, while requests usually elicit a non-verbal response. Both are important in language teaching and often among the first expressions that a learner acquires. Questions fall into two groups: polar questions, which can be answered with “yes” or “no”; and content questions, which require a more elaborate answer. Depending on the language, there are different types of requests, depending on factors such as identity of the addressee, politeness, etc. This paper provides an overview over the formation of different types of questions and requests in Indigenous languages of North America. Common strategies include intonation, (verbal) inflection or special markers. The paper closes with a brief discussion of how questions and requests can be used instead of one another.

12.1 Introduction Questions and requests are related types of speech acts that call upon the hearer to do something (Searle 1976: 11). The first type, questions, calls on the hearer to provide a verbal response of some sort—to answer the question. The second type, requests1, calls on the hearer to perform (or to refrain from performing) a certain activity. The difference between these speech acts, then, lies in the type of action that the speaker wants the hearer to perform, as illustrated in (1) and (2), where a question and a request are paired with possible responses. (1)    

Question A: Do you like chocolate? B: Yes./No./Only milk chocolate./…

(2)    

Request A: Pass the salt, please. B: [Passes the salt to A]

1 This type of speech act is often called “command” or “imperative” in the literature. I use “request” instead of “command” due to the hierarchical connotations of the latter term. The term “imperative” is used here for the linguistic (default) form used to express a request. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-012

284 

 Olga Lovick

Of course, it is possible to answer questions non-verbally (e.  g., by nodding or shaking one’s head) and requests verbally (e.  g., by saying “Sorry, I don’t see the salt anywhere.”), but (1) and (2) are prototypical examples. Questions and requests are very common in everyday interactions and thus important for learners working towards fluency. They also are used prominently in several approaches often used in Indigenous language teaching: Hinton, Florey, Gessner and Manatowa-Bailey (2018: 128) point to the importance of questions and requests in the Master-Apprentice approach; and the Total Physical Response approach by Asher (1969) has the explicitly stated goal of improving language students’ comprehension of spoken requests. In this chapter, I review basic types of questions (§ 2) and requests (§ 3) and describe how they can be formed in different Indigenous languages. In § 4, I look at how questions can be used to form requests and vice versa.

12.2 Questions Questions call on the hearer to provide a verbal response; they are a request for information. Depending on the response being sought, two types of questions can be distinguished. Polar questions are used when a speaker seeks to confirm or disconfirm a certain state of affairs, as in (1) above. Because they typically allow a limited set of answers such as “yes” and “no”, they are also known as yes/no questions. Content questions are used when the speaker is attempting to obtain a missing piece of information. If I notice that cookies have disappeared from the cupboard, I might ask a question such as (3). (3)

Who ate the cookies?

Content questions always contain a question word, such as who, what, when, where, why, or how. In English, this type of question is also known as wh-question.

12.2.1 Polar questions Polar questions can be formed in several different ways. In a few languages, they differ from a declarative by intonation only. This is illustrated using a sentence pair from Upper Tanana (Dene). The declarative clause in (4a) has gently declining pitch across the utterance, while the question in (4b) has a sharp rise on the last syllable. In both clauses, the word order is the same and no special question marker is used in (4b).2 2 The following abbreviations are used in this paper: 1/2–first person inclusive, AG–agent, ALL–allative, ANIM–animate, ASRT–assertion, CONCESS–concessive, CONT–continuative, DAT–dative, DEL–de-



(4)            

Questions and requests in North American languages 

 285

Upper Tanana (Dene) (Roy H. David, Sr. p.c. May 28, 2013, Avis Sam p.c. August 12, 2009) a. Ay chih dindeey heldeeł.       and also people 3pl.sbj:eat:ipfv     ‘And they eat people.’ b. Shoh aldeeł?         bear 2pl.sbj:eat:ipfv     ‘Do you (addressing plural) eat bear?’





This strategy is used in almost a fifth of all languages worldwide but seems to be quite rare in North America according to Dryer (2013a). Much more common, both typologically and in North America, is the use of question particles, e.  g., in Nakota (Siouan). No special intonation is used in this language, even though the question particle he is often omitted, and the word order is identical to a declarative (5). (5)      

Nakota (Siouan) (Cumberland 2005: 377) Tʰaspą yúta he? apple 3ag:eat q  ‘Did s/he eat an apple?’

In Lakhota (Siouan), the form of the question particle depends on the gender of the speaker: male speakers use hųwo, female ones hųwé (Trechter 1995: 48–49).3 Some languages have a special interrogative mood paradigm. Lupardus (1982: 194–195) shows that in Alabama (Muskogean), the interrogative mood causes nasalization of the stem-final vowel. This is accompanied by a prosodic pattern “consist[ing] of a drop followed by a slight rise, all on the final syllable” (p. 195), as illustrated in (6). While only about a quarter of the world’s languages use special interrogative inflection, Dryer (2013a) suggests that this is not at all uncommon in the languages of North America.

layed, DEM–demonstrative, DEON–deontic, DES–desire, DIR–direction, DUR–durative, EMPH–emphatic, FOC–focus, FUT–future, GEN–genitive, IMM–immediate, IMP–imperative, INAM–inanimate, INCEP– inceptive, INST–instrumental, INTENS–intensifier, IPFV–imperfective, IRR–irrealis, ITER–iterative, MOD–modal, NEG–negation, NMLZR–nominalizer, NONREF–non-referential, OBJ–object, OBL–oblique, OPT–optative, PAST–past, PAT–patient, PL—plural, POL–polite, POSS–possessor, POT–potential, PROH– prohibitive, PROX–proximate, PUNC–punctual, Q–question, QUOT–quotative, REF–referential, SBJ– subject, SEQ–sequential, SG–singular, SPEC–specific, SR–switch reference, STV–stative, TR–transitive, TRNS–translocative, UNR–unrealized. Most grammatical glosses are identical to those in the source; only a few were standardized. 3 Trechter (1995:48) also notes that the female question particle is largely obsolete.

286 

(6)                

 Olga Lovick

Alabama (Muskogean) (Lupardus 1982: 195) a. ispalõ   is-(i)pa-lo-~   2sg.ag-eat-fut-q   ‘Will you eat it?’ b. ákpolo   ák-(i)pa-o-lo   neg.1sg.ag-eat-neg-fut   ‘I will not eat it’

The expected (or preferred) answer may have an influence on the form of a question. Rice (1989) notes for example that questions formed with the question particle in Slave (Dene) are neutral regarding the possible answers (p. 1123  f.), while intonation questions in that language are used when the speaker expects an affirmative answer (p. 1128).

12.2.2 Content questions All content questions contain an interrogative word. As alread noted by Sadock and Zwicky (1985: 184), languages differ enormously in the number of interrogative words and the distinctions they make. Cook (2004: 99) lists seven interrogative stems in Dëne Sųłiné, translating to ‘who, what, when, where, why, how’, and ‘how many’. Cumberland (2005: 381) lists twelve interrogative words (plus additional derived forms) in Nakota; this language distinguishes, for example, between realized versus unrealized when asking about the time of an event (7). This distinction is also attested in some Dene languages (see, e.  g., Lovick 2020: 280 for Upper Tanana). (7)            

Nakota (Cumberland 2005: 383) a. Tóhą hí he?   when:past 3ag:arrive q   ‘When did he arrive?’ b. Tohą́ n hí-pi-kta   when:fut 3ag:arrive-pl-pot   ‘When will they arrive?’

he? q 

More unusual is the interrogative inventory of Ute (Uto-Aztecan). This language distinguishes twelve interrogative pronouns depending on subjecthood, animacy, number, and referentiality (Givón 2011: 316). (8) illustrates the number contrast for the animate non-referential subject and object pronouns ‘what kind of (animate)’. As Givón (2001: 316) points out, the expected answer to this type of question identifies the “type of individual, not its specific reference”.



(8)            

Questions and requests in North American languages 

 287

Ute (Uto-Aztecan) (Givón 2001: 316) a. ’u-vwa-tʉ ’íni ’uni’ni(-y)?   there-at-dir q:nonref:anim:subj do/be-imm   ‘What kind (of a person/animate) is there? b. ’u-vwa-tʉ ’ini-u ’uni’ni-kya-y?   there-at-dir q:nonref:anim:subj-pl do/be-pl-imm   ‘What kind (of persons/animates) are there?

The non-subject stems participate in a number of morphological processes: they can be suffixed with one of several case markers or can join with a postposition or a verb, yielding complex forms such as those in (9). (9)            

Ute (Uto-Aztecan) (Givón 2001: 321) a. ’agha-naagha karʉ-i?   q:inan:obj-in sit-imm   ‘Where-in is s/he sitting?’ b. ’agha-vaa-tukhwa paghay’wa-y?   q:inan:obj-at-go walk-imm   ‘Where-to is (s/he) walking?

Thus, languages differ not only in the number of interrogative stems, but also in the number of forms these stems can occur in. One important distinction typologically concerns the position of the interrogative word in the sentence. In many languages the interrogative word occurs obligatorily in clause-initial position. This is illustrated in (10) for Onondaga (Iroquoian). While the word order in that language is generally rather free (Woodbury 2017: 348–349), the interrogative word always occurs in initial position (p. 321). (10)      

Onondaga (Iroquoian) (Woodbury 2017: 322) Nwa·hóʔdęʔ naʔ snaʔjyęhá·wiʔ? what asrt 2sg.ag:pail:carry:stv ‘What is that pail you are carrying?’

Clause-initial interrogative words seem to be the dominant type in North America; typologically, this word order appears to occur in about one third of cases (see also Dryer 2013b). In languages such as Nakota (Siouan), the interrogative word occurs in situ, i.  e., in the same place where the phrase it replaces would ordinarily occur. When asking about the patient, tuwé ‘who’ occurs in its regular position between the agent noun phrase and the verb (11). The question marker he (discussed already in 2.1) is optional. (11)      

Nakota (Siouan) (Cumberland 2004: 379) John tuwé į-yų́ ǧa he? John who stv-3ag>3pat:ask q ‘Whom did John ask?’

288 

 Olga Lovick

In some languages, such as Slave (Dene; Rice 1989: 1158–1160) or Osage (Siouan, Quintero 2004: 471), interrogative words may either be fronted or occur in situ with no apparent difference in meaning. Such mixed systems are quite rare in the world’s languages (Dryer 2013b). In many languages, the question word is followed by a focus marker. In Nishnaabemwin (Algonquian), the “emphatic focussing particle” dash is so common that it often fuses with the question word (Valentine 2001: 979). The question word wenen ‘who’, for example, coalesces with dash to wenesh (12). (12)      

Nishnaabemwin (Algonquian) (Valentine 2001: 980) Wenesh maanda debendang zhibiihgaans? who:foc this 3sg.prox>3sg:own pencil ‘Whose pencil is this?’

Another strategy that is common in Indigenous languages is the use of interrogative verbs in content questions. This is described in detail by Munro (2012) for the Takic subgroup of Uto-Aztecan (13), where this appears to be a highly productive process. (13)      

Mountain Cahuilla (Uto-Aztecan) (Munro 2012: 278) Hem-hí-yax-we? 3pl.sbj-ind-say-dur ‘What are they saying?’

Munro (2012) also notes the existence of interrogative verbs in Muskogean and Yuman languages. Interrogative verbs also attested in several Salish languages (14). (14)      

St’át’imcets (Salish) (van Eijk 1997: 134) kaxw kánəm 2sg.sbj be.what.state ‘How are you doing?’

In Musqueam (Salish), a substantial inventory of interrogative verbs is derived from a few basic stems (Suttles 2003: 395). Interrogative verbs can also be formed by incorporating a question word into a form of the verb ‘to be’ as done in Dëne Sųłiné (Dene). The verb in (15) is inflected for second person singular subject and imperfective mode. (15)        

Dëne Sųłiné (Dene) (Cook 2004: 99) ʔedlǫ́ lyeʔá   ʔedlá-ǫlye ʔá what-2sg.sbj:be.named:ipfv q ‘What is your name?’



Questions and requests in North American languages 

 289

12.3 Requests Requests can be categorized along several different dimensions (Aikhenvald 2010), such as the identity of the addressee of the request (the person intended to carry out the action; in this sense, the term “addressee” is not necessarily identical to the term “hearer”), § 3.1, or polarity (whether the request is to do something or to abstain from doing something), § 3.2. Many languages make additional distinctions, § 3.3. It is important to note here that these dimensions may intersect, and that languages can have many different ways of phrasing a request depending on the context. Finally, some languages have a dedicated set of non-verbal expressions that are used in requests, § 3.4. In this section, I do not offer comparisons on how frequent a particular feature is typologically or even within North America. This is because the World Atlas of Language Structures draws different distinctions than I do in this chapter. Additionally, while question formation is discussed in somewhat comparable fashion in reference grammars, the same cannot be said for requests; information on this topic is often spotty.

12.3.1 Identity of the addressee A prototypical request involves getting the hearer to do something; Aikhenvald (2010: 18) calls such second-person requests canonical. The most basic, or common, form of a canonical request is often called an imperative; I follow this practice here. Requests with a first person (usually plural, and usually inclusive, i.  e., including the second person) addressee are often called hortatives or exhortatives. Jussives is a common label for requests with a third person addressee.

12.3.1.1 Canonical imperatives Canonical imperatives are in many languages formally unmarked and thus the shortest possible verb form. Aikhenvald (2010: 19) comments that this lack of formal marking possibly reflects their functionally unmarked status as the most common type of request. Unmarked canonical requests are attested in many North American languages. In Northern Paiute (Uto-Aztecan, Thornes 2017: 151), canonical requests consist of the bare stem. Subject number may be expressed by suppletive (number-marked) stems (16) or by the use of overt pronouns (p. 153). (16)      

Northern Paiute (Uto-Aztecan) (Thornes 2017: 151) Yadua! talk.sg ‘Speak!’ (addressing singular)

290 

 Olga Lovick

Another common strategy is to simply use a second person form. In Slave (Dene), the second person imperfective functions as canonical imperative (17). (17)      

Slave (Dene) (Rice 1989: 1109) Kágodahwhi! 2pl.sbj:go.out:ipfv ‘Go outside!’ (addressing plural)

Other languages employ a special imperative marker. The Aleut (Eskaleut) suffix -da, -ada -‘imp’ in (18) never co-occurs with an overt subject but is interpreted as second person singular or plural depending on context (Golovko 2001: 301). (18)      

Aleut (Eskaleut) (Golovko 2001: 301) Qangu-da! come.in-imp ‘Come in!’ (addressing singular or plural)

Aleut additionally employs a special intonation pattern involving lowered pitch and a lengthened final syllable (p. 302). In Onondaga (Iroquoian), an imperative suffix combines with an agent marker (Woodbury 2017: 101). The second person singular agent marker has a special imperative form (19). (19)        

Onondaga (Iroquoian) (Woodbury 2017: 101) Sęni·hęh́! s-ęni·hę-h’ 2sg.imp-stop-imp ‘Stop!’ (addressing singular)

Another group of languages has a special imperative verb paradigm or set of paradigms. In the Algonquian language family, this is known as the ‘imperative order’, which contrasts structurally and functionally with the independent and conjunct orders as illustrated in Table 1 for Innu. Tab. 1: Innu (Algonquian) paradigms of ‘work’, indicative mood, present tense (Baraby 2017: 56)  

independent

conjunct

imperative

1SG 2SG 3SG 4SG 1PL 1/2PL 2PL 3PL

nit-atusse-n tshit-atusse-n atusse-u atusse-nua nit-atusse-nan tshit-atusse-nan tshit-atusse-nau atusse-uat

atusse-ian atusse-in atusse-t atusse-niti atusse-iat atusse-iaku atusse-ieku atusse-ht

–  atusse –  –  –  atusse-tau atusse-ku – 



Questions and requests in North American languages 

 291

Outside the Algonquian language family, imperative paradigms are also reported for Klamath (Isolate; Stegnij 2001: 81) and several Eskaleut languages.

12.3.1.2 (Ex)hortatives Hortatives (or exhortatives) have a first-person addressee. In many languages, including Innu (Algonquian) in Table 1, they are limited to the first-person plural including the second person, e.  g., atussetau ‘let’s (you and I) work’. In this language, the hortative belongs to the same imperative paradigm as second-person requests. In languages such as Ute (Uto-Aztecan), hortative and imperative formations differ. Second person requests use the bare stem plus an optional emphatic affix, while hortatives employ an irrealis suffix and a first person inclusive subject marker (20). (20)            

Ute (Uto-Aztecan) (Givón 2011: 305, 307) a. Yugwi!   pl:sit   ‘Sit!’ (addressing several) b. wʉ́ ʉka-qha-paa-rawi!   work-pl-irr-1/2pl ‘Let’s (you and we) work!’

Some languages do not have a dedicated strategy for the formation of hortatives. In Moses-Columbia Salish, imperatives contain the enclitic =(t)aʔ and no subject inflection (21a). Hortatives can be formed in several ways, e.  g., by adding a modal marker (21b) or by adding an ‘unrealized’ verbal prefix (21c). (21)                  

Moses-Columbia (Salish) (Mattina 1999: 2, 13) a. ʔác’x̌-nt=(t)aʔ   look.at-tr=imp   ‘Look at it!’ b. saʔk ʔác’x̌-nt-m   deon look.at-tr-1pl.sbj   ‘Let’s look at it!’ c. t’íl’ ka-s-ʔác’x̌-nt-m   emph unr-s-look.at-tr-1pl.sbj   ‘Let’s look at it!’

292 

 Olga Lovick

Some languages limit hortatives to non-singular subjects (including minimally the speaker and the hearer), but not all of them do. Koasati, for instance, has a dedicated first person singular hortative suffix illustrated in (22).4 (22)        

Koasati (Muskogean) (Kimball 1991: 263) /má:mik cimmą́ hkaták/ má:mik cim-mán,h,ka-ták then 2sg.dat-tell,h:grade-1sg.sbj:imp ‘Then let me tell you about this!’

Singular hortatives can be difficult to translate into English. In his discussion of Uummarmiut hortatives, Lowe (1985: 155) notes that “[i]t is as if the speaker, talking to himself and trying to make up his mind whether he should do something or not, was urging himself to finally do it”, illustrating it with (23): (23)      

Uummarmiut (Eskaleut) (Lowe 1985: 155) niuqqar-la-nga have.tea-opt-1sg.sbj ‘I might as well have tea!’

Hortatives are not attested in all languages; e.  g., Cumberland (2005) does not report their existence in Nakota (Siouan).

12.3.1.3 Jussives Jussives are requests directed at a third person, i.  e., neither the speaker nor the hearer. They may belong to the same paradigm as canonical requests and hortatives as for example in Klamath. In this language, imperative markers for all three persons belong to what Stegnij (2001: 81) terms “class 23” suffixes, an obligatory class of modal and tense markers. A hortative form is shown in (24a), a jussive form in (24b). A canonical imperative is given in (26) below. (24)            

Klamath (Isolate) (Stegnij 2001: 86, 85) a. hak hay sle?-i:k     emph excl see-imp:1sg     ‘let me see it!’ a. q’ay mis sa sle:-tgi   neg 2sg.o 3pl.s see-imp.3sg   ‘do not let them see you!’

4 “h-grade” in the gloss means that this form uses a particular stem form instead of the simple root. This language distinguishes between root imperatives and h-grade imperatives; Kimball (1991: 269) notes that h-grade imperatives “are used to request an action rather than to order it”.



Questions and requests in North American languages 

 293

Jussives may also belong to a different paradigm. In Slave (Dene), the basic imperative uses the imperfective mode (17), while hortatives and jussives both employ the optative (25). (25)      

Slave (Dene) (Rice 1989: 1111) dezǫa kare nágoguyeh! children outside 3pl.sbj:play:opt ‘Let the children play outside!’

Like hortatives, jussives do not exist in all languages; Innu for example does not have them (see Table 1) nor does Northern Paiute (Thornes 2017: 161).

12.3.2 Polarity All requests surveyed so far were directives that called upon someone to do something. There are also negative directives that call upon someone to not do something. These are often called prohibitives. Prohibitives can be compositional or non-compositional. Compositional prohibitives consist of a regular imperative, hortative, or jussive negated by the same negator used in standard negation (negation of a declarative clause). Klamath uses compositional prohibitives characterized by the cooccurrence of the second-person imperative suffix, which occurs in affirmative and negative requests alike, and the standard negator q’ay (26). (26)            

Klamath (Stegnij 2001: 81, 88) a. Gida ?is Gis-lG-i!   here 2sg>1sg step-down-2sg:imp   ‘Here you step down on me!’ b. Q’ay del-ditgo:l-i!   neg look-out.from.under-2sg:imp   ‘Don’t look out!’ (addressing one)

   

Typologically more common are non-compositional prohibitives. Van der Auwera, Lejeune, and Goussev (2013) note that these may differ from standard-negated imperatives with respect to the negator, the verb form, or both. Upper Tanana (Dene) illustrates non-compositional prohibitives. Standard negation is achieved by negative inflection plus the negative particle k’a(t’eey), while prohibitives use affirmative inflection and the prohibitive particle sǫ’ (27). While imperatives are usually in the imperfective, prohibitives are almost always in the optative (Lovick and Tuttle 2019: 131).

294 

(27)                  

 Olga Lovick

Upper Tanana (Dene) (Lovick and Tuttle 2019: 131) a. Ha’áát tsät įįtthèèł ch’a.   out wood 2sg.sbj:chop:ipfv foc   ‘Chop wood outside!’ (addressing one) b. Sǫ’ tsät ǫǫtthèèl!     proh wood 2sg.sbj:chop:opt     ‘Don’t chop wood!’ (addressing singular) c. K’àt’eey tsät į ̀į ̀tthéél de’…   neg wood 2sg.sbj:chop:pfv:neg if   ‘If you (sg.) had not chopped wood…’

The distinctions noted for affirmative requests may be available in negative ones. Onondaga, for example, allows negative jussives (as well as negative hortatives), (28). (28)                

Onondaga (Iroquoian) (Woodbury 2017:106) a. ahgwi tho hęhséh!   ahgwi tho h-ę-hs-e-h   proh loc trns-fut-2sg.a-go-imp   ‘don’t go there!’ (addressing singular) b. ahgwí ęhatdó·gah!     ahgwi ę-h-atdog-ah     proh fut-3m.sg.a-notice-imp     ‘he shouldn’t notice!’

Other distinctions, such as that between immediate and delayed requests in Algonquian (see § 3.3.2) are only available in affirmative requests, not in negative ones.

12.3.3 Other distinctions Many languages make additional distinctions within the functional domain of requesting. Different forms may be used depending on politeness, urgency, the relationship of speaker and addressee and so forth (§ 3.3.1). Some languages draw distinctions that have to do with the timing of a request (§ 3.3.2). More rarely, the form of a request is influenced by properties of the speaker or addressee (§ 3.3.3).

12.3.3.1 Softening and strengthening a request The forms described in § 3.1 and 3.2 are often perceived as somewhat rude, and many languages have requesting strategies that may be more appropriate in certain circumstances. Thornes (2017: 154) finds that in Northern Paiute, a request involving a verb form inflected for tense accompanied by a second person pronoun plus a deontic modal



Questions and requests in North American languages 

 295

enclitic (28) is perceived as “softer” than one using the uninflected form illustrated in (15) above. (29)      

Northern Paiute (Uto-Aztecan) (Thornes 2017: 154) ɨɨ=sakwa ka=i=mia-si i=puh-nagi! you=mod obl=1=go-seq 1=inst/eye-chase ‘You should keep watching me where I go!’

Another strategy identified by Thornes (2017: 155) involves the bare stem imperative preceded by an intensifier, “indicat[ing] a kind of emphatic permissive command that lends an immediacy to the expected action” (29): (30)      

Northern Paiute (Uto-Aztecan) (Thornes 2017: 155) ɨnɨ mia! nɨ mai yongo-kwi,… intens go I  dem evening-fut ‘Go ahead and go! I’ll stay here for the night,…’

Lovick (2016) notes that in Upper Tanana, requests in the imperfective mode signal that the speaker expects the request to be complied with, while those in the optative do so to a much lesser degree. The imperfective request in (31a) has the force of an order, while the optative request in (31b) has the force of a suggestion. (31)            

Upper Tanana (Dene) (Lovick 2016: 285, 286) a. An staanįįdaay!   away:all 2sg:sg.go.away:iter:ipfv:imp   ‘Go away! (addressing one) b. Shyiign t’axoh natonshya’!   down:all finally 2sg:sg.go:iter:opt   ‘It is time for you to go back down!’ (addressing one)

In Aleut, on the other hand, optative requests are stronger than plain imperatives, although Golovko (2001: 307) additionally notes that intonation can also strengthen or soften a request. Crow (Siouan) has three strategies (Graczyk 2007: 151–153). The “simple imperative” in (32a) contains the imperative suffix -h; some verbs additionally contain a second person agent prefix.5 The “mild or polite imperative” additionally contains the suffix -kawe (32b). The “emphatic imperative” contains a suffix -wa and “adds a note of insistence: ‘do it or else!’” (32c). (This kind of marker is sometimes called “apprehensive”, cf. Aikhenvald (2010: 226)).

5 The presence of the second person agent marker seems to depend on the initial syllable of the verb stem. It is absent in (31a, b) but present in the form in (31c), where the verb begins with an unstressed i-vowel.

296 

(32)

 Olga Lovick

a.     b.     c.    

Crow (Siouan) (Graczyk 2007: 151, 152, 153) Baláxi-h! sing-imp ‘Sing!’ (addressing one) Disshi-káwe-h! dance-pol.imp-imp ‘Please dance!’ (addressing one) d-iháwi-wa-h 2sg.ag-sleep-emph.imp-imp ‘Sleep (or else)!’ (addressing one)

In a similar vein Givón (2011: 308) describes a “strong obligative mode” for Ute, composed of the irrealis nominalizing suffixes. This mode may be used in affirmative and negative forms (33). (33)            

Ute (Uto-Aztecan) (Givón 2011: 308) a. wʉ́ ʉka-vaa-na-mʉ   work-irr-nmlzr-2sg   ‘you must work’ (addressing one) b. ’ʉmʉy-aqh-’uru ka-’ini-vaa-’wa-na   2sg/gen-it-that neg-do-irr-neg-nmlzr   ‘you must not do this’ (addressing one)

12.3.3.2 Time of execution Some languages distinguish requests based on when they should be carried out. The imperative order in Algonquian languages allows the speaker to distinguish between immediate requests, to be carried out immediately, and delayed ones, to be carried out at a later time (34).6 (34)            

Nishnaabemwin (Algonquian) (Valentine 2001: 991, 993) a. Zhngishni-n doopwining.     lie.prone-2sg:imp:imm on.table     ‘Lie down on the table.’ (addressing one) b. Baamaa waabang bi+zhaa-kan…   later tomorrow come-2sg:imp:del   ‘Come tomorrow…’ (addressing one)

6 In the Algonquianist tradition, the hyphen is usually used to separate preverbs, while I use it in this paper to indicate morpheme boundaries. (33b) contains a preverb that I separated from the rest of the form using +.



Questions and requests in North American languages 

 297

Baraby (2017: 67–74) suggests that in Innu (also Algonquian), forms similar to (33b) should be more appropriately analyzed as evidential imperatives, arguing that the action is to be carried out in the absence of the speaker. In Asiatic Eskimo7, imperatives can take the continuative marker, which contributes the meaning “urging to continue/not to terminate an action in progress” (Vaxtin 2001: 133): (35)            

Asiatic Eskimo (Eskaleut) (Vaxtin 2001: 133) a. negh-i   eat-imp:2sg   ‘Eat!’ (addressing one) b. negh-aq-i   eat-cont-imp:2sg   ‘Go on eating!’ (addressing one)

A similar construction is reported by Quintero (2004: 297) for Osage (Siouan). Rice (1989: 1109–1110) reports that Hare (a variety of Slave; Dene) has a timing distinction within negative directives. Those in the optative mode warn against an activity not yet begun, while those in the imperfective direct an individual to stop the activity (36). (36)            

Hare (Dene) (Rice 1989: 1110) a. ʔelá dewǫtl’éle!   boat 2sg.sbj:paint:opt:neg   ‘Don’t paint the boat!’ (addressing one, painting has not yet begun) b. ʔelá dįtl’éle!   boat 2sg.sbj:paint:ipfv:neg   ‘Stop painting the boat!’ (addressing one)

Koasati (Muskogean) seems to be particularly rich in timing distinctions within the domain of requesting. Kimball (1991, chap. 6) reports a distinction between first delayed (37b), second delayed (37c), and polite continuing imperatives (37d), all of which differ in turn from the bare root canonical imperative (37a). (37)        

Koasati (Muskogean) (Kimball 1991: 264, 267, 268) a. /ip!/   i:pa-Ø-DEL   eat-2sg.imp-phr-term   ‘eat!’ (addressing singular)

7 This is the language name used by Vaxtin; I suspect that this is the language known these days as “Siberian Yup’ik”.

298 

                       

 Olga Lovick

b.       c.       d.      

/amawí:cįh/ am-awí:ci-Ø-˛Vh 1sg.dat-help-2sg.imp-delay ‘help me later!’ (addressing singular) /taɬą́ :hah/ taɬ-:˛V́hah weave(imp)-sec.delay ‘weave it a lot later!’ /ɬopótcitik/ ɬopót-ci-tik pass.through-2sg.sbj-pol ‘please continue on passing through!’

12.3.3.3 Indexing speaker or addressee properties In some languages the form of a request depends on certain properties of the speaker or addressee. Lakhota, for example, uses different request particles depending on the speaker’s gender. This intersects with distinctions regarding the relationship between speaker and addressee. Table 2 replicates a summary table provided by Trechter (1995: 57). Her table also includes gender-marked enclitics that are not used in requests; these have been omitted here. Tab. 2: Gender-marked request enclitics in Lakhota (Trechter 1995: 57)  

Male

Female

Command Familiar command Entreaty

yo yetho ye

ye nitho na

Eastern Pomo (Pomoan) employs a special form when making a request of an in-law to display respect. The semantically unmarked form of a request with a singular addressee employs the imperative suffix -im (38a). If the addressee is an in-law, the plural imperative suffix -me is used with a singular stem (38b). For plural (non-in-law) addressees, the plural imperative suffix combines with a plural stem and/or a plural agent suffix (38c). (38)      

Eastern Pomo (Pomoan) (McLendon 1996: 530)   kaˑki-m!   sit.down:sg-sg.imp   ‘Sit down!’ (addressing one)



           

Questions and requests in North American languages 

b.     c.    

 299

kaˑki-me! sit.down:sg-pl.imp ‘Sit down!’ (addressing daughter in-law) naˑphó-k-aki-me! several.to.be.located-punc-pl.ag-pl.imp ‘Sit down!’ (addressing several)

Eastern Pomo in fact distinguishes a very large number of different types of requests. In addition to the basic form in (38a, c) and the in-law form in (38b), McLendon (1996) mentions a “polite imperative” which involves the concessive clitic (39a), “instructions” (which seem to function similar to delayed requests, 39b), “requests” (which seem to be more polite, 39c), and “supplications” (used in prayers, 39d). (This language also has “exhortations”, i.  e., hortatives in the terminology used in this paper.) (39)                            

Eastern Pomo (Pomoan) (McLendon 1996: 530, 531) a. xaˑká=bi yóx-k’i-l-im   flint=concess become-do/make-dur-sg.imp   ‘Please make an arrowpoint!’ (addressing one) b. xódaˑmal máˑ-l ʔáˑm čiki-pʰila   after.awhile you(pl)-pat thing happen-if(sr)   kuˑnúˑla-bùˑc’ike-heʔmìˑpal maˑ xaˑdí-yaki-baʔè   Coyote-Old.Man-spec(pat) you(pl.ag) go.get-pl.ag-subjunc   ‘After awhile, if something (bad) happens to you, you should go get Old Man Coyote.’ (addressing several) c. má=ti diˑt’áq-aʔèˑle   you(sg.ag)=subjunc think-cond   ‘would you think (about) it?’ d. yowx, maˑʔáy=ti moˑkoˑš   prayer.opening food=subjunc become-des   ‘May it turn into food!’

It is unclear how common languages with such rich inventories in request forms are. Reference grammars tend to list the more common forms, but, as noted by Aikhenvald (2010: 2), “[t]he possibilities for [the expression of] commands are immense, and openended”.

12.3.4 Imperative-only lexemes Many languages in North America have special imperative-only lexemes, non-verbal expressions used exclusively in requests (Aikhenvald 2010: 317). They exist in Musqueam (Salish; Suttles 2003: 470–474), Ute (Uto-Aztec; Givón 2011: 429), Koasati (Muskogean, Kimball 1991: 504–507), Innu (Algonquian, Baraby 2017: 63–64), Upper Tanana (Dene,

300 

 Olga Lovick

Lovick 2020: 302–305), and probably many other languages. The semantic range of these lexemes is considerable, as comparison of the inventories in Koasati (40) and Innu (41) shows. (40)        

Koasati (Muskogean) (Kimball 1991: 504–507) a. hé:! ‘go on!’ b. himá:kǫ! ‘wait a minute!’ c. kappá:l! ‘shut up!’ d. máh! ‘listen!’

(41)                

Innu (Algonquian) (Baraby 2017: 63) a. matshi! ‘go ahead, go away!’ b. ashtam! ~ashtamite! ~mua! ‘come here!’ c. akua! ‘watch out! be careful!’ d. eshku! ‘wait a minute! hang on! stop a second!’ e. kata! ‘wait!’ f. eka pitama! ‘wait, just a moment!’ g. ushte! ‘move further away! get away from here!’ h. eku! ‘let’s go!’

12.4 Questions and requests: using one as the other The preceding sections provide an overview over both the formal range of question and request formation in North American languages and over the range of distinctions made by individual languages. This overview is by necessity too brief and cursory in many places. Before closing, however, I want to draw attention to one interesting way in which questions and requests interact in many languages. At the outset, I noted that questions and requests have in common that they direct the actions of another person. In light of this, it is not surprising that oftentimes the functions of questions and requests overlap, allowing one to be used to express the other. Givón (2011: 314–315) observes that in Ute, negative polar questions often serve as polite requests. (42)      

Ute (Uto-Aztecan) (Givón 2011: 315) …   Mʉ́ ni-áa nʉ́ nay kach ’u-vwaa-tʉ             2pl.s-q 1sg.o neg there-at-dir ‘… Won’t you please carry me there?…’

nɵ́ ɵ’wa-way-ura?… carry-neg-be

McLendon (1996: 531) observes the very same strategy for Eastern Pomo, and Watahomigie, Bender, and Yamamoto (1982: 130) for Hualapai (Yuman). In Hualapai, there is additionally a systematic ambiguity between a certain type of negative request and a type of negative question—a conventionalized way of forming an affirmative request.



Questions and requests in North American languages 

 301

The two sentences in (43) consist of the same morphosyntactic material and differ only in intonation: the prohibitive is characterized by falling intonation, while the negative question/affirmative request has rising intonation (Watahomigie, Bender, Watahomigie, Yamamoto, Mapatis, Powskey, and Steele 2001: 130). (43)            

Hualapai (Yuman) (Watahomigie et al. (2001: 131) a. Miya:m-a mđé!      go-imp neg:asrt     ‘Don’t go!’ (addressing one) b. Miya:m-a mđé!    go-imp neg:asrt     ‘Why don’t you go? (i.  e., Go!)’ (addressing one)

↘ ↗

Lovick and Tuttle (2019) report that in Upper Tanana and Koyukon (both Dene), one conventionalized strategy to avoid uttering a prohibitive is to question the addressee’s behavior (44). (44)      

Upper Tanana (Dene) (Lovick and Tuttle 2019: 144) Dii xah ch’a utüh tidhįį’ia tl’aan why foc 3sg:over 2sg:step:incep:pfv and ‘Why did you step over it when you got there?’

ni’įįhaał? 2sg:sg.arrive:ipfv:prog

In Inuktitut, we find the opposite. Changing the intonation of a canonical imperative to typical question intonation with rising pitch makes it into a “polite question” (45). (45)      

Inuktitut (Eskaleut) (Mallon 1991: 21) tiituriit? drink.tea-imp:2sg ‘Drink tea?’

This tendency towards functional overlap between questions and requests is attested in many languages across the globe.

12.5 Summary In this chapter, strategies for the formation of different types of questions and requests in the Indigenous languages of North America were reviewed. Questions and requests are common speech acts in routine interactions and are central to several approaches used in Indigenous language teaching. Two types of questions can be distinguished. Polar questions are questions that typically elicit a “yes” or “no” answer. In some languages, polar questions are marked only by a special intonation pattern. More common is the use of question particles or special interrogative paradigms.

302 

 Olga Lovick

Content questions query a missing piece of information; they always contain a question word. The languages surveyed here differ considerably with respect to the number of question words. Another difference concerns the placement of the question word. In many languages, the question word always occurs in clause-initial position; in other languages, it occurs in the position of the phrase it replaces. In a few languages, either order is attested with no apparent change in meaning. Several Indigenous languages have interrogative verbs. In the domain of requesting, many types of distinctions can be drawn. One is the addressee: the addressee of a canonical request is the hearer (second person). The most common form of a canonical request is often called the imperative. In some languages, imperatives are bare stems; in other languages, special imperative morphemes or even paradigms exist. In yet another group, second-person subject forms are co-opted as imperatives. Requests directed at the first person (hortatives) and at the third person (jussives) are also common. In some languages, they belong to the same paradigms as imperatives, but this is not always the case. One can also distinguish requests to do something from those not to do something; the latter type is called prohibitives here. Prohibitives sometimes are simply imperatives combined with the same negative marker(s) as used in declarative sentences, but more frequently, they use special forms. In many languages (Indigenous and other) there are ways to soften or strengthen requests. Some languages distinguish immediate from delayed or general requests or use different forms depending on the identity of speaker or addressee. A number of languages have special imperative-only lexemes that can only be used in requests. Since requests and questions both direct the addressee to provide a (nonverbal or verbal) response, it is not surprising that in many languages, there is functional overlap between these types of speech acts. Thus, learning about question and request formation in any language (Indigenous or not!) does not merely involve learning to construct the forms: one also needs to learn when and how to use them in an appropriate fashion, which will be different for every language.

References Aikhenvald, Alexandra Y. 2010. Imperatives and commands. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Asher, James J. 1969. The total physical response approach to second language learning. The Modern Language Journal 53(1). 3–17. Baraby, Anne-Marie. 2017. Imperatives and evidentiality in Innu. In Daniël Van Olmen & Simone Heinold (eds.), Imperative and directive strategies, 53–77. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Cook, Eung-Do. 2004. A grammar of Dëne Sųłiné (Chipewyan). Algonquian and Iroquoian Linguistics – Special Athabaskan Number, Memoir 17. Winnipeg: Algonquian/Iroquoian Linguistics.



Questions and requests in North American languages 

 303

Cumberland, Linda A. 2005. A grammar of Assiniboine: A Siouan language of the Northern Plains. PhD diss., Indiana University. Dryer, Matthew S. 2013a. Polar questions. In Matthew S. Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. https:// wals.info/chapter/116. Dryer, Matthew S. 2013b. Position of interrogative phrases in content questions. In Matthew S. Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. https://wals.info/chapter/93. Givón, Talmy. 2011. Ute reference grammar. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company. Golovko, Evgenij V. 2001. Imperative in Aleut. In Viktor S. Xrakovskij (ed.), Typology of imperative constructions, 300–314. Munich: LINCOM. Graczyk, Randolph. 2007. A grammar of Crow: Apsáaloke Aliláau. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Hinton, Leanne, Margaret Florey, Suzanne Gessner & Jacob Manatowa-Bailey. 2018. The Master-Apprentice language learning program. In Leanne Hinton, Leena Marjatta Huss & Gerald Roche (eds.), The Routledge Handbook of Language Revitalization, 127–136. New York: Routledge. Kimball, Geoffrey D. 1991. Koasati grammar. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Lovick, Olga. 2016. Įįjih and request formation in Upper Tanana Athabascan: Evidence from narrative text. Anthropological Linguistics 58(3). 248–298. Lovick, Olga. 2020. A grammar of Upper Tanana, Volume 1: Phonology, lexical classes, morphology. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Lovick, Olga & Siri G. Tuttle. 2019. Video elicitation of negative directives in Alaskan Dene languages: Reflections on methodology. In Aimée Lahaussois & Marine Vuillermet (eds.), Methodological tools for linguistic description and typology. Language Documentation & Conservation, Special Publication No. 16, 125–154. Lowe, Ronald. 1985. Uummarmiut uqalungiha ilihaurr̂ û tikrâ ngit: Basic Uummarmiut Eskimo Grammar. Inuvik: Committee for Original Peoples Entitlement. Lupardus, Karen Jaque. 1982. The language of the Alabama Indians. PhD diss., University of Kansas. Mallon, Mick. 1991. Introductory Inuktitut reference grammar. Montreal: Arctic College – McGill University Inuktitut Text Project. Mattina, Nancy. 1999. Moses-Columbia Imperatives and Interior Salish. Anthropological Linguistics 41(1). 1–27. McLendon, Sally. 1996. Sketch of Eastern Pomo, a Pomoan language. In Ives Goddard (ed.), Handbook of North American Indians. Volume 17: Languages, 507–550. Washington: Smithsonian. Munro, Pamela. 2012. Interrogative verbs in Takic. UCLA Working Papers in Linguistics, Theories of Everything 17(32). 274–284. Quintero, Carolyn. 2004. Osage Grammar. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Rice, Keren. 1989. A Grammar of Slave. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter. Sadock, Jerrold M. & Arnold M. Zwicky. 1985. Speech act distinctions in syntax. In Timothy Shopen (ed.), Language typology and syntactic description. Volume I: Clause structure, 155–196. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Searle, John R. 1976. A classification of illocutionary acts. Language in Society 5(1). 1–23. Stegnij, Viktor A. 2001. Imperative constructions in Klamath. In Viktor S. Xrakovskij (ed.), Typology of imperative constructions, 78–97. Munich: LINCOM. Suttles, Wayne. 2003. Musqueam reference grammar. Vancouver: UBC Press. Thornes, Tim. 2017. On the heterogeneity of Northern Paiute directives. In Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald & R. M. W. Dixon (eds.), Commands: A cross-linguistic typology, 146–168. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Trechter, Sara. 1995. The pragmatic functions of gender deixis in Lakhota. PhD diss., University of Kansas. Valentine, J. Randolph. 2001. Nishnaabemwin reference grammar. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.

304 

 Olga Lovick

Van der Auwera, Johan, Ludo Lejeune & Valentin Goussev. 2013. The prohibitive. In Matthew S. Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. https://wals.info/chapter/71. Van Eijk, Jan. 1997. The Lillooet language: Phonology, morphology, syntax. Vancouver: UBC Press. Vaxtin, Nikolaj B. 2001. Imperative sentences in Asiatic Eskimo. In Viktor S. Xrakovskij (ed.), Typology of imperative constructions, 129–144. Munich: LINCOM. Watahomigie, Lucille J., Jorigine Bender, Philbert Watahomigie, Sr., Akira Y. Yamamoto, Elnor Mapatis, Malinda Powskey & Josie Steele. 2001. Hualapai reference grammar. Expanded and revised edition. Osaka: Endangered Languages of the Pacific Rim. Watahomigie, Lucille J., Jorigine Bender & Akira Y. Yamamoto. 1982. Hualapai reference grammar. Los Angeles: American Indian Studies Center, UCLA. Woodbury, Hanni. 2017. A reference grammar of the Onondaga language. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.

Anna Berge

13 Information structure Abstract: The way people decide to present what they want to say reflects the choices they make about what it is they want to focus on, or what they think other people in a conversation know. The study of how people package the information they want to convey in a sentence is called information structure. Some important concepts in the study of information structure include the notions of focus (what a speaker considers particularly important information), topic (what a speaker is talking about), and word order (what information a speaker decides to present first). Indigenous languages, being so different syntactically to widely spoken languages like English, have much to offer to studies of information structure. For example, how are notions such as focus and topic indicated when a sentence may consist of a single word? In this chapter, I present the basic notions of and approaches to information structure and the challenges that Indigenous languages have posed to studies of information structure, then conclude with a brief look at why an understanding of information structure is helpful for language revitalization efforts.

13.1 Introduction Most discussions of syntactic structure in a language focus on how words are combined into phrases, clauses, and sentences. Another way to look at a sentence or clause, however, is how information is packaged within it: how new vs. old information is indicated, how important or newsworthy information is differentiated from backgrounded information, how the use of nominal forms of verbs or verbal forms of nouns affects the interpretation of the message, and so forth. This approach to understanding language is called Information Structure. The packaging of information happens at all levels; thus, we can ask whether or not a language has a single word for a concept, such as caused events. In English, we have separate words for ‘to die’ and ‘to cause to die,’ i.  e. ‘to kill.’ In Kalaallisut (Eskaleut), the same concepts are formed from the same lexeme: toqu- ‘to die,’ toquC- ‘to kill’ (ultimately from toqu-t- ‘die-caus’). Information structure, however, specifically refers to information packaging within sentences. In (1a–1c) from Kalaallisut (Eskaleut), the same information (tamuatsivaaq ‘seal skin and fat chewing gum’) is packaged in four different ways in three consecutive clauses: as an incorporated noun in (1a), as a part of the verb and the verb ending in (1b), as an independent pronoun taanna ‘that one’ also in (1b), and as an independent noun in (1c). (1)    

Kalaallisut (Eskaleut) (Berge 2011:61–62) a. tamuatsivaar-tor-luta tassa   tamuatsivaaq-eat-1pl.ct that.is

https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-013

306 

           

 Anna Berge

b.     c.    

tassa taanna tamua-rujoor-tar-parput that.is that.one.abs chew-casually-hab-1pl.subj/3sg.obj.ind ‘…we used to chew it continually’ tamuatsivaa-mik taasar-parput uagut taqqava-ni tamuatsivaaq-inst call-1pl.subj/3sg.obj.ind 1.pl.abs south-loc ‘…we in the south used to call it “tamuatsivaaq”’

Why do this? Varying the way we speak makes a story more interesting. All languages have ways of manipulating the way that information is packaged. The study of information structure is relatively new in terms of the field of linguistics, and major ideas and advances in our understanding thereof are still being developed, but several components of information structure are generally accepted as fundamental: word order, or the order in which we present information; topic, or what it is we are talking about, and focus, or the new information we want to communicate about the topic. In section 2, I will develop these concepts in more detail. The theory of information structure was initially developed for languages very different from the Indigenous languages of North America. Over the past several decades, theories of information structure have been greatly enriched and informed by the studies of Indigenous languages. Several characteristics shared by many of these languages have especially contributed to the discussion, including different word orders, nominal marking systems, and different concepts of word and sentence boundaries. In section 3, I will address the contributions that Indigenous languages have made to these aspects of information structure. In the process, I hope to show how important an understanding of information structure is to the study of the Indigenous languages, and why this is vital to revitalization efforts.

13.2 What is information structure? The concept of a formal level of grammatical structure based on the organization of information rather than words has a long tradition in fields such as philosophy or logic. Within linguistics, this concept was first developed and found useful for characterizing the relatively flexible word order of Slavic languages (cf. Firbas 1964), as in (2a) and (2b), which differ in the order of the subject (ya ‘I’) and object (knigi ‘books’): (2)            

Russian (Firbas 1964) a. Ya knig-i   1nom.sg book-acc.pl   ‘I love books’ b. knig-i ya   book-acc.pl 1nom.sg   ‘books, I love (them)’

lyubly-u love-1sg.pres lyubly-u love-1sg.pres



Information structure 

 307

Changing the expected word order of a sentence without changing grammatical marking allows a speaker to highlight something; what comes first in a sentence is typically what the speaker wants to talk about, or the theme, or topic, and what comes after is what the speaker wants to say about it, or the rheme, or comment. As the importance of information structure was recognized, it was taken up in studies of other language types, including English. Word order is less free in English, and variations tend to incur a change in structure, as in the addition of the pronoun ‘them’ in the translation of (2b). These early discussions focused on languages with typically separate verbs and arguments, or in other words, languages with structures often quite different from those of the Indigenous languages of America. How does one talk about information structure in languages with sentences that frequently consist of a single word, as in Unangam Tunuu (Eskaleut) aniqduĝikuqing ‘I have a child’? Nevertheless, it is widely accepted that we all make choices about how we structure the presentation of information, or how we package the information, in order to convey how important one piece of information is with respect to another, regardless of the type of language we speak.1 Different languages impose different strategies for expressing what is important on the speakers, but all languages have a variety of strategies for doing so. In (3a) and (3b) from Unangam Tunuu, information structure strategy relies not on word order but on decisions about word building: (3)            

Unangam Tunuu (Berge 2016:144–145) a. aniqdu-ĝi-ku-qing   child-have-ind-1sg   ‘I have a child.’ b. aniqdu-x̂ mata-ku-qing   child-abs.sg have-ind-1sg   ‘I have a child.’

Whenever there are two different ways of communicating what appears to be the same information, the information is in fact not the same (Mithun 1983). There are subtle differences between these two sentences, best explained as a function of speaker and hearer presuppositions about why the speaker is giving this information, what he/she expects the hearer to understand, etc. (Berge 2016). The analytic structure in (3b) is found in a wider variety of contexts today, whereas the incorporated structure in (3a)

1 This is evident in the long history of studies of related sentences, encompassing many syntactic and semantic theories, e.  g. Chomskyan transformational and generative grammar, Fillmore and Kay’s studies of semantic roles in different sentence types, Foley and Van Valin’s functionalist approaches to syntax, and speech act theories, e.  g. regarding the effects of different performative verbs such as ‘command’ vs. ‘request’ on the meaning of a sentence. Information Structure is one approach to understanding the way people package information in a clause; cross-linguistic studies such as those by Vallduví and Engdahl (1996) highlight the different structural options available to speakers for packaging the same information.

308 

 Anna Berge

often has either special connotations or restrictions in use. Aniqduĝikuqing can mean ‘I have a child’ or ‘I am with child’ (i.  e. pregnant); and although in principle, it can mean ‘I have children,’ the analytic structure is preferred for plurals: aniqdun matakuqing ‘I have children.’ Information structure, therefore, requires us as linguists to not just understand the sentence structure, but also the factors involved in the choice of sentence structure. These include understanding presuppositions and knowledge shared between the speaker and hearer (pragmatics), the flow of information negotiated during a conversation (conversational analysis), or the choice of words (lexical analysis) or perspective (semantics) that best convey the speaker’s meaning. Key works on information structure in which these concepts are elaborated include Chafe (1976), Lambrecht (1994), Vallduví (1991, 1995), Gundel (1988), and Krifka (2006) among others. Although information structure is an important component of linguistic knowledge, there is no agreement on either the key elements of information structure or their definitions. This, unfortunately, makes it difficult to compare information structure between languages in general, and even more so between the many less-widely studied languages. Nevertheless, there are concepts commonly taken up in most studies, including especially word order, focus, and topic.

13.2.1 Word order One of the main premises of information packaging is that we limit the amount of information in any given utterance to something manageable to the hearer; one pervasive idea is that an utterance preferably includes no more than one new piece of information. For example, saying ‘John likes Mary’ pragmatically suggests that both ‘John’ and ‘Mary’ are new participants, and in Cayuga (Iroquoian), a sentence with both ‘John’ and ‘Mary’ overtly expressed would seem odd. In normal speech, speakers are more likely to have introduced one of the two previously, and therefore to say either (4a), (4b), or (4c) (4)                  

Cayuga (Iroquoian) (Mithun, 1992:18) a. Mary shakó-n̥ǫ̥hwe’-s   Mary he/her-like-hab   ‘He likes Mary’ b. John shakó-n̥ǫ̥hwe’-s   John he/her-like-hab   ‘John likes her’ c. Shakó-n̥ǫ̥hwe’-s   he/her-like-hab   ‘He likes her’

This necessarily results in an utterance that contrasts the new information with the already known, or given information. This is the basis of much terminology in the literature, e.  g., the following pairs of terms, which are not necessarily synonymous and which



Information structure 

 309

may overlap in some ways: theme/rheme; given/new; backgrounded/foregrounded information; and topic/comment, or topic/focus. The contrast between new and given information is expressed in many ways, but one of the most obvious is through word order. Because we can only say one word at a time, what gets expressed first sets the tone for what follows. In a neutral statement (with no particular emphasis or unexpected use of language), the order of given vs. new information is assumed to be constant, and a change in the expected word order is a signal to pay special attention. In many early studies, the claim was that given information precedes new information; thus, in the statement ‘Mary is making some cookies,’ ‘Mary’ is given information (using the name ‘Mary’ with no other context assumes that she is known to the hearer), and ‘is making some cookies’ is new (hence the use of the indefinite ‘some’). This order has been found in Yaqui (Uto-Aztecan) (Guerrero and Bellero 2010). However, new and/or important information precedes given and/or backgrounded information in languages as different as Ojibwa (Algonquin), Ute (Numic), Tuscarora (Iroquoian), Coos (Coosan) and Unangam Tunuu (Eskaleut) (Mithun 1992:17, Mithun 1983, Berge 2015). In Unangam Tunuu, one or more of the verbal arguments is likely to be pronominally marked on the verb; this may result in a sentence consisting only of the verb, as in (3a), or in a sentence with the new information first, as in (5b): (5)            

Unangam Tunuu (Edna Floyd speaker, Berge field notes 2013) a. Kiin ing’ suxuraada-n agu-na-n?   Who these cookie-pl make-part-3pl   ‘Who made these cookies?’ b. Mariiya-m agu-laana-ngin.   Mary-erg make-recent.past-3pl.an   ‘Mary made them’

Word order studies have grown in sophistication and complexity, in part as a result of data from Indigenous languages. The order given first/new last may not be appropriate for all languages, or in all circumstances; and neither may a description that relies on overt or expressed noun phrases to the exclusion of information packaging within the verb. Nevertheless, normal discourse typically involves more than one-word sentences, and the distinction between given/new, important/background, etc. is valid and useful. Even in extremely polysynthetic languages like Kalaallisut, while each clause may consist of a verb only, it more frequently includes particles, adverbs, and postpositional phrases, as in (1a)-(1c). Furthermore, word order variations have important effects in any language: moving a word to the front of a clause may signal what the sentence will be about (i.  e. the topic) or it may signal new information (i.  e. the focus), whereas moving a word to the very end may signal an antitopic, a different kind of emphasis, a reminder of given information that is only expressed inflectionally on the verb, etc. In Koyukon (Na-Dene), Tlingit (Na-Dene), and Unangam Tunuu, all verb-final languages, moving a noun phrase after the verb signals that it is old but important information, whereas at least in the latter two languages, moving a noun phrase before the clause

310 

 Anna Berge

signals that it is new and important information (cf. Thompson 2000, Crippen 2014, and Berge 2016 for the respective languages). Word order thus interacts with the notions of focus and topic.

13.2.2 Focus Focus and topic are often discussed with reference to and in opposition to each other, but they are separate concepts. Perhaps one of the most widely cited definitions of focus is that of Lambrecht (1994), for whom focus is new information about what is being talked about, or topic. He distinguishes three types of focus. The location of focus in a sentence can be an argument of the verb, such as a subject or object; in (5a) and (5b), the focus is on the subject (kiin ‘who’ and Mariyam ‘Mary’, respectively). Focus on an argument is often contrastive, as in ‘Is John or Mary making some cookies?’ ‘Mary’. When the predicate of the sentence is in focus, we tend to have a typical topic-comment structure, where the subject is the topic, or given information, and the rest of the sentence is the new information, as in ‘making some cookies’ in ‘Mary is making some cookies’. Finally, the sentence as a whole may describe new information, as in the answer to ‘What’s going on?’ ‘Mary’s making some cookies’. Another influential understanding of focus emphasizes the role of contrast rather than newness. For Krifka (2006), focus is “the presence of alternatives that are relevant for the interpretation of linguistic expressions”; in ‘Who’s making some?’ there are a set of relevant people who could be making cookies, while in ‘What’s going on?’ there is a set of relevant activities that could be happening. Whether focus is about newness or contrast, the linguistic indication of focus may include the use of stress, a particle, a cleft structure (using a complex sentence to focus one element, as in ‘It’s Mary that’s making the cookies’), or moving an argument, either to the right or to the left of the clause. These two understandings of focus are not mutually exclusive. In (6a), from Meskwaki (Algonquian), the focus (in bold) is the new information, or comment; in (6b), the focus is contrastive (it is also a cleft structure): (6)            

Meskwaki (Algonquian) (Dahlstrom, 2003:152) a. ni∙na a∙kwi wi∙h-na∙kwa∙-ya∙nini   I  not fut-leave-1   ‘As for me, I’m not leaving’ b. a∙kwi ni∙na wi∙h-na∙kwa∙-ya∙nini   not I  fut-leave-1   ‘I’M not leaving; it’s not me who’s leaving’



Information structure 

 311

13.2.3 Topic The term topic has also been applied to a variety of phenomena. It is commonly defined as the given part of a sentence, and thus it is often correlated with a) the first part of a sentence, as discussed above; b) the subject, since most world languages have a word order in which the first element is the subject; c) a primary (preferably animate) participant, since that is what is likely to be the subject of the discourse and therefore already given; d) backgrounded information, since what is given or known tends not to be highlighted; e) pronouns, since given information need not be repeated in full. All of these are illustrated with ‘she’ in the following: ‘Mary is in the kitchen. She is baking a cake.’ The topic in (6a) is ni∙na ‘I’; it is also the first element, the subject, and the given information. A topic can be focused (because it is being contrasted, or it is a new topic, or it is made more prominent by some form of movement, such as fronting, cf. Wilbur 2012:464; Gundel 1988:296); this is the case in (6b). On the other hand, we can think of topic as what the sentence is about; this is often correlated with the notion of contextual frames: by stating the topic directly, the speaker signals that he or she is assuming that the hearer shares a set of expectations generally associated with the topic. For example, in talking about baking, we make assumptions about the types of food, equipment, people and/or personality types, etc. involved in baking. Some languages, such as Lakota (Siouan) and Haida, are known as topic-prominent languages; they mark this kind of topic grammatically, with a topic particle or displacement of the topic (often to the left of the sentence). In Haida (7), the topic chiin iiwaanda ‘big fish’ is the first element in the sentence and is indicated by the following particle uu: (7)      

Haida (Eastman and Edwards 1983:58) chiin iiwaandaa uu l  guulaagan fish big top she likes ‘Big fish are what she likes.’

13.2.4 Identifying focus and topic Focus and topic are identified in a variety of ways at all levels of grammatical structure. Thus, focus can be indicated phonologically by stress, as in the English translation of (6b). Morphologically, a topic may simply be indicated by a pronominal inflection on the verb, as in the indication of the subject ‘I’ in the inflectional ending -qing in (3a-b) or by a particular case marker, determiner, or enclitic; contrastive focus may be indicated by the decision to use a separate expression, rather than a fused or incorporated structure (3a-b). Syntactically, a topic or focused element may be in front of a sentence, clefted, or expressed with some other non-neutral structure (6b). Lexically, either focus or topic or both may be indicated by a particle. In Western Apache (Na-Dene), both focus and topic are distinctively marked: =(h)í is an enclitic that can mark topics (8a-b), and =go an enclitic that can mark focus (8d); (8c) is a neutral statement:

312 

(8)                        

 Anna Berge

Western Apache (Na-Dene) (de Reuse 2001:14) a. Gósé=hí ápos yíyą̄ą̄   dog=top apple it.ate.it   ‘Speaking about the dog, it ate the apple.’ b. Gósé ápos=í yíyą̄ą̄   dog apple=top it.ate.it   ‘Speaking about the apple, the dog ate it.’ c. Shiye’ hish’į̄ į̄ ni’   my.son I.see.him past   ‘I saw my son.’ d. Shiye’=go hish’į̄ į̄ ni’   my.son=foc I.see.him past   ‘It is my son that I saw (and not someone else).’

Such clear marking of focus and topic is uncommon, however, and in many languages, a particle is optional. In Kalaallisut, for example, tassa ‘that is’ indicates that a neighboring phrase is focused (1a-b), but a phrase may be focused without the use of this particle. It can often be confusing to distinguish between topic and focus because the two are not necessarily mutually exclusive, and many strategies used for one may also be used for the other. Frequently, multiple strategies are used, and since these strategies overlap, it is unclear what indicates topic and what indicates focus, if these can be isolated. For example, both fronting and the use of a particle may indicate topic, but they are also common strategies for focus. This is further compounded by different uses of the terms topic and focus in the literature, as well as by different understandings of their characteristics over time. In describing topic in Haida, for example, Eastman and Edwards (1983) suggest that all topics are contrastive, and that sentence-initial elements not marked by the topic marker uu are focused, but not contrastive, as we see by comparing (7) with (9): (9)        

Haida (Eastman and Edwards 1983:58) chiin iiwaandaa l  guulaagan fish big she likes Focus   Subj Verb ‘She likes big fish.’

However, contrast is often reserved for discussions of focus. Distinguishing the two terms is especially confusing when comparing strategies cross-linguistically: where one language uses one particular strategy for signaling topics, another might use the same strategy for focused elements. Finally, the needs of information structure frequently clash with those of a different level of grammatical structure, as in when both sentential subjects and topics are generally first in a sentence, but the topic is something other than the subject.



Information structure 

 313

13.3 Indigenous language studies and information structure The Indigenous languages of America have challenged earlier understandings of information structure in several ways, as a result of which our understanding of information structure has been enormously enriched. Although there are still relatively few studies of information structure in Indigenous languages, compared to others, information structure is a major topic of interest in studies of semantics in these languages (Matthewson 2017:6). As a result of studies of these languages, theories of information structure have had to address issues such as 1) verbal inflection, its effect on word expression and word order, and the very notion of basic word order, 2) the different nominal and head marking systems, and 3) different levels of morphological synthesis, the notion of a unit of meaning within a sentence, and indeed the notion of a sentence.

13.3.1 Word order We typically speak of word order with respect to subject, object, and verb: SOV, SVO, VOS, etc. Almost all possible word orders are found in the North America; we speak of Ojibwa or Haida as having VOS order, for example, and Na-Dene and Eskaleut languages as having SOV word order. Discussing word order involves assumptions that there is a basic word order, and that subject and object are overtly expressed. Mithun (1992) argues that some languages, such as Coos and Cayuga, may not have a basic word order, and that preferred word order in any give sentence is related to the pragmatic or communicative needs at the time of utterance. On the other hand, some languages are claimed to have relatively flexible word order, but they have been found to have very definite preferences; Yaqui, for example, strongly disprefers word orders in which the object precedes the subject (Guerrero and Belloro 2010:123). Basic word order is in opposition to non-neutral word order, which implies fronting, postposing, or extraction of a subject or object from the sentence. A language with basic SOV word order might allow non-neutral orders such as OSV (fronting of O), OVS (postposing of the subject), or S, OV (extraction of the subject, signaled through intonation, indexing,…). In Unangam Tunuu, a language that has generally been characterized as having fixed SOV word order, constructions with an extracted topic (10) may actually be more natural in casual conversation. (The object is said to be extracted here both because of its position before the subject (O, SV) and its inflectional marking): (10)      

Unangam Tunuu (Bergsland 1997:143) ayx̂aasi-x̂ anu-m aga-ti-ku-u boat-abs current-rel take-caus-ind-3sg/3sg.an ‘the boat, the current took it’ = ‘the boat was taken by the current’

314 

 Anna Berge

In many languages, the subject and sometimes the object are indexed on the verb with pronominal inflection. A language with pronominal indexing on the verb may allow the subject or object to be unexpressed if it is understood from context (4c). In this case, a clause may have no overt noun phrases; this tends to be the case in mid discourse, when the participants have already been mentioned, and the focus is on an activity described by the verb. A clause may, however, have one, two, or more rarely three overt noun phrase arguments. Time and again, it has been reported that a sentence with two or more overt noun phrases is rare in discourse (Mithun 1992:20; Berge 2009, 2011), and even disallowed (Beck 2010:42) in part because too much information is being introduced simultaneously, and in part because speakers prefer not to repeat previously introduced information. Far more frequently, a sentence will have at most one overt argument, which may be either a topic or a focused element. Thus, in a language with a verb final or verb initial basic word order, neutral word order by itself may not be a good indicator of topic or focus. In consecutive clauses (11a-c) from Unangam Tunuu, the topic is x̂ulustaakam uluu ‘seal meat,’ but both the topic and some new information riisax̂ ‘rice’ are focused by being overtly expressed and sentence initial: (11)                  

Unangam Tunuu (Susie Merculief speaker, Berge field notes 2007) a. …x̂ulustaaka-m ulu-u kakliitka-x̂si-ĝi-i   bachelor.fur.seal-rel meat-3sg.posm meatball-make-pas.part-3sg.posm   ‘having made seal meatballs’ b. …x̂ulustaaka-m ulu-u su-x̂ta-lix   bachelor.fur.seal-rel meat-3sg.posm take-cont-conj   ‘using seal meat’ c. riisa-m una-ĝi-i su-x̂ta-lix…   rice-rel cook-pas.part-3sg.posm take-cont-conj   ‘using cooked rice’

In combination with other factors, however, such as whether or not an argument is overt, a subject or object, in neutral or non-neutral order with respect to the verb (cf. (10)), accompanied by a particle (cf. (8a-d), or (7) vs. (9)), and so forth, word order can be an indicator of topic or focus status. Many languages in North America show a very strong tendency, perhaps even requirement, for overt focus and topics, whether newly introduced, contrastive, or continued, to be toward the beginning of a sentence. In Kiowa (Tanoan), a verb-final language with relatively fluid word order, focus and topic must precede the verb (12a-b); they cannot follow the verb (12c); and they must precede a verbal particle (e.  g. an) if one is present (12d), as opposed to non-topics, which would be found after a verbal particle: (12)      

Kiowa (Tanoan) (Harbour et al, 2012:104) a. Focus: Hâatêl gɔ–góp?     who.q 3s:2s–hit.pf     ‘Who hit you?’



               

Information structure 

b.     c.     d.  

Focus:     Focus:     Topic:  

 315

Carl ę́ –góp. Carl 3s:1s–hit.pf ‘Carl hit me.’ *Ę́góp Carl 3s:1s–hit.pf Carl ‘Carl hit me.’   Kɔ́ígú an ém–thɔ̨ɔ̨-téttɔ hágyá Kiowa.i hab 3a:refl–story-tell-impf sometime ɔnk!îi gya–dɔ́ɔ́mêi nɔ past 3p–be.evid and.df ‘The Kiowas tell a story about a past time when .  .  .’

In Unangam Tunuu too, both focus and topic tend to be sentence-initial, but there are interesting differences between them arising from factors such as argument and overtness. For example, an overt object is almost always new, or focused, whereas an overt subject may be equally new or given. These tendencies correlate with the overwhelming preference for objects to come before the verb, and the frequent occurrence (up to 30 % in conversations) of subjects after the verb, or postposition of subjects. Postposed elements, on the other hand, tend to function differently, although there is a great deal of variation in how they are interpreted. In many cases, they are arguments that in other circumstances might have been unexpressed. Harbour et al. (2012:113) see them as having a role in discourse structure, beyond the sentence; Mithun (2018) calls them antitopics, noting that in Cayuga, they are often intonationally separate from the sentence, and Berge (2011) finds the same for Kalaallisut. In Unangam Tunuu, however, they are almost always pronominal subjects and so much a part of the sentence that they are becoming cliticized to the preceding verb. In Yaqui, too, they are definite, identifiable, and previously introduced in the discourse. In Koyukon (13), they are almost always definite, human, and associated with important participants. (13)      

Koyukon (Thompson 2000:231) Huyeɬ gheel beyeenhooldlet go tsook’aale and.then 3s.became.angry dem old.woman ‘And then the old woman became angry.’

Tersis and Carter-Thomas (2005:494) find the same for Tunumiisut (Eskaleut), but they also show that postposing can be a focusing strategy that results in topic-comment word order. North American Indigenous languages show that the formulation of topic-comment or given-new characterization of information structure is simplistic. Czipionka (2007) finds a correlation between word order and focus, with a greater tendency to have focus first in SOV languages. However, North American languages are not uniform (Yaqui, an SOV language, has topic-comment order). Furthermore, they have shown that languages with flexible word order do, in fact, have word order requirements, and freedom is constrained by information structure and pragmatic factors.

316 

 Anna Berge

13.3.2 Case marking Nouns are often marked to differentiate them from other nouns. One common type of differentiation is case, which is the inflectional marking on nouns that allows a speaker to distinguish the various roles that nouns play within sentences, such as subject and direct object. Some languages, such as Na-Dene languages, make little to no use of case marking, while in other languages, case marking is much more extensive. There are a number of case marking systems. Nominative-accusative systems, in which sentential subjects are inflected in one way (with nominative case) and direct objects in another (with accusative case) are well-known from studies of European languages, including the Slavic languages described by early studies of information structure and a relatively small number of North American languages such as Miwok (Utian), Maidu (Maiduan), Yaqui, or the Yuman languages. The latter two groups are unusual in having a special marker for the nominative case, whereas the vast majority of nominative-accusative languages have marking on the accusative case. In many of the former type of nominative-accusative languages, nominative has been found to coincide with topic. Unfortunately, there is little discussion of information structure or discourse structure in North American marked-nominative languages. Handschuh (2014) notes that a noun being topicalized or focused in Maidu may have an emphatic marker and sentence-initial position (14), but this is not enough to conclude that subjects are topics: (14)      

Maidu (Handschuh 2014:126, quoting Dixon 1911:711) sü-m has nik do’kan dog-nom emph 1sg.acc bite ‘The dog bit me.’

Another system, known as ergative-absolutive case marking, is found in languages along the west coast, such as Eskaleut languages, Tsimshian, and Chinook. In ergative-absolutive systems, the subject of an intransitive verb and the object of a transitive verb are marked in the same way (with absolutive case), and the subject of a transitive verb is marked differently (with ergative case); in other words, case does not match up with the syntactic categories of subject and object, but it does reflect information structure. This pattern of case marking seems to reflect the introduction of new information in discourse (Du Bois 1987). In extended discourse, however, absolutive case occurs far more frequently than ergative case does, and in most cases, it does not coincide with new information, but rather with topic. This has been argued for Inuit (Tersis and Carter-Thomas 2005, Berge 2011, Johns and Kucerova 2017). Ergative subjects in Kalaallisut are infrequently expressed, but when they are, they are focused, and not topical within the clause; in Unangam Tunuu, they are specifically non-topics (Berge 2009). A very different type of nominal marking involves differentiating the roles of 3rd person participants in discourse (i.  e. participants other than the speaker and hearer). In some languages, gender is an important distinction, although gender does not always relate to information structure as discussed here. In others, the distinction is between



Information structure 

 317

more and less important or prominent participants, with prominence often correlated with animacy (how participants are ranked on a hierarchy from animate to inanimate). The most prominent 3rd person argument in a sentence is designated as proximate, while all other 3rd person arguments are obviative. This type of distinction is found in some Salishan languages, Kutenai, and Algonquian languages; Navajo and some other Na-Dene languages have a similar distinction, without marking on the nouns. There is a consistently close association between information structure and obviative-proximate marking, with proximate often associated with topic (Junker 2004; Zúñiga 2014); obviative has been described as a non-topic. Proximate and obviative are distinct from subject and object; proximate in neutral clauses is the subject (15a), but the obviative can be a subject (15b), in which case a special indication is made on the verb, as illustrated in East Cree (Algonquian), a VOS language: (15)            

East Cree (Algonquian) (Junker 2004: 349–350) a. miyeyim-e-u uyuuh atim-h uu awaash   like.ta-dir-3 this.obv dog-obv this.prox child.prox   ‘a child likes dogs’ b. miyeyim-iku-u uyuuh awaash-ah uu atim   like.ta-inv-3 this.obv child-obv this.prox dog.prox   ‘this child likes this dog’

Almost all word orders are found in natural discourse and either or both subjects and objects can precede the verb. Fronted noun phrases are said to be focused (16a). However, when both precede the verb, the obviative, or non-topic, cannot be first (16b): (16)            

East Cree (Algonquian) (Junker 2004: 349) a. uu awaash uyuuh atim-h   this.prox child.prox this.obv dog-obv   ‘It is this child that likes this dog’ b. * uyuuh atim-h uu awaash   this.obv dog-obv this.prox child.prox   ‘It is this child that likes this dog’

miyeyim-e-u like.ta-dir-3 miyeyim-e-u like.ta-dir-3

In Plains Cree (Algonquian), this restriction is even more pronounced: even if only one noun phrase is in non-neutral position, the obviative cannot precede the proximate (17a-b): (17)            

Plains Cree (Algonquian) (Bliss 2005:61) a. Am-a nínaa-wa íístini-m an-i í’ksisako-yi   dem-prox man-prox cut-dir dem-obv meat-obv   ‘The man cut the meat’ b. *An-i í’ksisako-yi íístini-m am-a nínaa-wa   dem-obv meat-obv cut-dir dem-prox man-prox   ‘The man cut the meat/The meat was cut by the man’

318 

 Anna Berge

Thus, nominal marking in many North American languages very clearly has an important role to play in information structure, distinct from word order yet obviously related. Each nominal marking system makes different distinctions which appear to correlate the default nominal marking (case or person) with topic; but a satisfactory and comprehensive cross-linguistic comparison of topic and focus remains to be done.

13.3.3 Polysynthesis Languages differ in how much information they pack into individual words, and in the types and methods of packaging that information. If words can be analyzed as having component parts, they can be thought of as synthetic, which means ‘put together’. Some languages allow or require quite a few component parts to be put together to form words, in which case they are known as polysynthetic. (18) illustrates polysynthesis in Wichita: (18)      

Wichita (Caddoan) (Mattissen 2004:204, citing Rood 1976:75) kiya:kí-riwa:c-ʔáras-a-ri-kita-ʔa-hí:rik-s evid.aor:3-big-meat-coll.u-tr-top-come-iter-impf ‘by making many trips, he carried the large (quantity of) meat up into it [the tree]’

Although polysynthesis is found elsewhere, it is particularly well represented in the Americas. There are different understandings of what constitutes polysynthesis (Mithun 2009, 2017), and there are different types of polysynthesis, but key to the discussion here is that many ideas are packaged into a single word, and this must have an implication for word order, sentence structure, and so forth. If only one unit of new information is preferentially introduced per sentence, how does this work in polysynthetic languages, and what is a unit of information? Is a word a clause, and is a sentence (one or more clauses) the same across languages? In some ways, polysynthesis may not be as problematic to information structure as it seems. In terms of the units of meaning in a clause, even in a highly polysynthetic structure, many of the elements of meaning give flavor to the clause, but the actual new content may not exceed what we otherwise find in non-polysynthetic clauses. Although (18) is decontextualized, the glosses suggest that most of the information is grammatical, leaving ‘big meat’ and ‘come’ as the places to look for new information. As for word order, a one-word clause may include a topic indexed on the verb and proposition-level focus; and many clauses in fact have multiple words and phrases that indicate relative levels of focus or backgrounding. In other ways, polysynthesis presents some interesting challenges to theories of information structure. For example, sentences in polysynthetic languages frequently consist of chains of clauses, and therefore tend to resemble paragraphs, rather than the short, decontextualized examples found in this article. They may reflect topic continuity in discourse (Berge 2011), but information structure is specifically supposed to be a



Information structure 

 319

feature of the syntax. Another characteristic of polysynthesis is the ability to incorporate independent words into a verbal structure, as in the independent noun aniqdux̂ ‘child’ as in (3a), or ʔáras ‘meat’ in (18). Incorporation therefore affects the flow of information. It may be a mechanism for introducing a new topic (Berge 2011; Mithun 2009); but not all incorporated nouns are new, nor are they all topical (Mithun 1983; Berge 2011). Incorporation is one of many options for managing information. Both the effects of clause chaining and incorporation on information structure need more investigation.

13.4 Conclusions Information structure is an important, if still imperfectly understood part of linguistic knowledge. Time and again, concepts such as neutral and non-neutral word orders, topic tracking, strategies to focus noteworthy information, and choices in the packaging of information within words and sentences have been shown to have explanatory value in understanding the grammar of Indigenous languages, often capturing insights that do not seem to be answered by appeal to syntax or morphology alone. There appear to be some interesting correlations or trends that need further investigation: for example, there is a preference for a verbal argument to the right of the sentence to be given information, or backgrounded, rather than new; nominal marking, regardless of the marking system, seems to mark topic in the languages illustrated here; and even in highly polysynthetic languages, patterns such as limiting the amount of new information in a clause are evident. Information packaging is complex, and as important as cross-linguistic studies are in improving our understanding of information structure, we also need to expect and study variation in information structure within a given language. For example, because of the linear nature of speech, all languages have strategies to manipulate word order for effect. We can therefore expect there to be neutral word orders and unexpected word orders that will catch the attention of the listener in some way. A good speaker will play with his or her options to do so, and we can therefore expect differences in information structure between speakers of a language, with more eloquent or creative speakers using a wider variety of strategies, or with some speakers preferring some strategies over others. Therefore, true knowledge of a language must include an understanding of that language’s information packaging strategies. In addition to cross-linguistic and language internal differences in information structure, we can expect differences in information structure across time within a language, as a result of either changes in notions of aesthetics, or perhaps of changes in other parts of the grammar, either through language internal developments or as a result of language contact. Change is inevitable, it is a healthy part of language adaptation through time. But information structure may also be affected by language loss and replacement, with the structure of a dominant or prestigious language affecting that of

320 

 Anna Berge

the affected language. As such, further studies of information structure in Indigenous languages are desperately needed, especially for language maintenance and revitalization. information structure has rarely been prominent in discussions of language revitalization and the teaching of heritage languages, and efforts have generally focused on learning vocabulary and the generation of simple conversations. But learning a language also requires learning its unique approach to packaging concepts, or the way speakers manipulate the language for effect.

References Beck, David. 2010. Communicative Structure in Lushootseed Syntax: Thematicity and Focalization. In José Camacho, Rodrigo Gutiérrez-Bravo & Liliana Sánchez (eds.), Information Structure in Indigenous Languages of the Americas: Syntactic Approaches, 39–64. Berlin & Boston: De Gruyter Mouton. Berge, Anna. 2009. Tracking topics: A comparison of “topic” in Aleut and Greenlandic discourse. In Marc-Antoine Mahieu & Nicole Tersis (eds.), Variations in Polysynthesis: The Eskaleut Languages, 185–200. (Typological Studies in Language 86.) Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Berge, Anna. 2011. Topic and Discourse Structure in West Greenlandic Agreement Constructions. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Berge, Anna. 2013. Object Reduction in Aleut. Transitivity and Its Related Phenomena. Asian and African Languages and Linguistics 7. 5–23. Berge, Anna. 2014. A Comparison of Information Structure in Greenlandic and Aleut. Workshop on Information Structure in Head Marking Languages, Nijmegen, March 28–29. Berge, Anna. 2016. Pribilof Anĝaĝigan Tunungin/The Way We Talk in the Pribilofs. Fairbanks: Alaska Native Language Center. Berge, Anna. 2016. Polysynthesis in Aleut (Unangam Tunuu). In Tokusu Kurebito (ed.), Linguistic Typology of the North 3, 1–38. Tokyo: ILCAA. Bergsland, Knud. 1997. Aleut Grammar. Fairbanks: Alaska Native Language Center. Bliss, Heather. 2005. Topic, Focus, and Point of View in Blackfoot. In John Alderete, Chung-hye Han & Alexei Kochetov (eds.), Proceedings of the 24th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics, 61–69. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project. Carol M. Eastman & Elizabeth A. Edwards. 1983. Pragmatic factors and Haida syntax, Word 34(2). 57–65. DOI: 10.1080/00437956.1983.11435737. Dahlstrom, Amy. 2003. Focus Constructions in Meskwakwi (Fox). In Miriam Butt & Tracy Holloway King (eds.), Proceedings of LFG03 ConferenceUniversity at Albany, State University of New York, 145–163. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications. De Reuse, Willem. 2001. Topic, focus, definiteness, specificity, and referential prominence in Western Apache noun phrases and relative clauses. In Jeanie Castillo (ed.), Santa Barbara Papers in Linguistics 11, Proceedings from the fourth Workshop on American Indigenous Languages July 6–8, 2001, 13–19. Santa Barbara: UCSB Department of Linguistics. Du Bois, John W. 1987. The Discourse Basis of Ergativity. Language 63/4. 805–855. Eastman, Carol M. & Elizabeth A. Edwards. 1983. Pragmatic factors and Haida syntax. Word 34(2). 57–65. DOI: 10.1080/00437956.1983.11435737. Firbas, Jan. 1964. On Defining the Theme in Functional Sentence Analysis. In Josef Vachek (ed.), Travaux Linguistiques de Prague 1: L’École de Prague d’Aujourd’hui, 267–280. Prague: Editions de l’Académie Tchécoslovaque des Sciences. Givón, Talmy. 1983. Topic Continuity in Discourse. Amsterdam: Benjamins.



Information structure 

 321

Gundel, Jeanette K. 1988. Universals of topic-comment structure. In Michael Hammond, Edith A. Moravcsik & Jessica R. Wirth (eds.), Studies in Syntactic Typology, 209–240. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Guerrero, Lilián & Valeria A. Belloro. 2010. On word order and information structure in Yaqui. In José Camacho, Rodrigo Gutiérrez-Bravo & Liliana Sánchez (eds.), Information Structure in Indigenous Languages of the Americas: Syntactic Approaches, 117–139. Berlin & Boston: De Gruyter Mouton. Handschuh, Corinna. 2014. A Typology of Marked-S Languages. (Studies in Diversity Linguistics 1). Berlin: Language Science Press. http://langsci-press.org/catalog/book/18. Harbour, Daniel, Laurel J. Watkins & David Adger. 2012. Information Structure, Discourse Structure, and Noun Phrase Position in Kiowa. International Journal of American Linguistics 78(1). 97–126. Johns, Alana & Ivona Kučerova. 2017. On the Morphosyntactic Reflexes of Information Structure in the Ergative Patterning of Inuit Language. In Jessica Coon, Diane Massam, Lisa Demena Travis (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Ergativity, 397–418. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI 10.1093/ oxfordhb/9780198739371.013.17. Junker, Marie-Odile. 2004. Focus, Obviation, and Word Order in East Cree. Linga 114. 345–365. Krifka, Manfred. 2006. Basic Notions of Information Structure. In Caroline Féry, Gisbert Fanselow & Manfred Krifka (eds.), Interdisciplinary Studies on Information Structure 06, 13–56. (ISIS/Working Papers of the SFB 632). Potsdam: Universtitätsverlag Potsdam. Lambrecht, Knud. 1994. Information Structure and Sentence Form: Topic, Focus, and the Mental Representation of Discourse Referents. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Matthewson, Lisa. 2017. Semantics in Indigenous American Languages: 1917–2017 and Beyond. International Journal of American Linguistics 83(1). 141–172. Mattissen, Johanna. 2004. A Structural Typology of Polysynthesis. WORD 55(2). 189–216. Mithun, Marianne. 1983. The Genius of Polysynthesis. In James S. Thayer (ed.), North American Indians: Humanistic Perspectives, 221–242. (University of Oklahoma Papers in Anthropology 24.2). Norman, OK: University of Oklahoma Press. Mithun, Marianne. 1992. Is basic word order universal? In Doris L. Payne (ed.), Pragmatics of Word Order Flexibility, 15–61. (Typological Studies in Language 22). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Mithun, Marianne. 2009. Polysynthesis in the Arctic. In Marc-Antoine Mahieu & Nicole Tersis (eds.), Variations on Polysynthesis: The Eskimo-Aleut Languages, 3–18. (Typological Studies in Language 86). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Mithun, Marianne. 2018. Factors behind variation in marking information structure: Contributions from Central Pomo. In Evangelia Adamou, Katharina Haude & Martine Vanhove (eds.), Information Structure in Lesser-Described Languages: Studies in Prosody and Syntax, 119–156. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/slcs.199.05mit. Tersis, Nicole & Shirley Carter-Thomas. 2005. Integrating syntax and pragmatics: word order and transitivity variations in Tunumiisut. International Journal of American Linguistics 71(4). 445–472. Thompson, Chad L. 2000. Iconicity and Word Order in Koyukon Athabaskan. In Theodore B. Fernald & Paul R. Platero (eds.), The Athabaskan Languages: Perspectives on a Native American Language Language Family, 228–251. (Oxford Studies in Anthropological Linguistics). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Vallduví, Enric & Elisabet Engdahl. 1996. The Linguistic Realization of Information Packaging. Linguistics 34. 454–519. Zúñiga, Fernando. 2014. Inversion, Obviation, and Animacy in Native Languages of the Americas: Elements for a Cross-Linguistic Survey. Anthropological Linguistics 56. 3/4. 334–355.

322 

 Anna Berge

Further Reading Dalrymple, Mary & Irina Nikolaeva. 2011. Objects and Information Structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Foley, William. 2007. A Typology of Information Packaging in the Clause. In Timothy Schopen (ed.), Language Typology and Syntactic Description, 362–446. 2nd edn. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Tim Thornes

14 Clause-combining: Relative clauses Abstract: This chapter presents a survey of constructions in the languages of Native North America that could properly be considered relative clauses. Relative clauses are subordinate clauses or clause-like structures that describe nouns and either fully occupy the position of a noun phrase in a sentence or appear as dependents of a noun phrase, either modifying or delimiting the head noun or associated statement. From the perspective of language acquisition, one may consider the primary function of a restrictive relative clause as assisting the speaker in directing the attention of the listener to a particular entity or type—an identifying function.

14.1 Introduction: What is a relative clause? A language requires some means for speakers to identify and describe entities in a discourse. For example, we can imagine two people in conversation at a busy dog park, and one person says, “That dog looks thirsty!” The other person asks, “Oh! Which one?” “The black lab that’s standing near the gate over there,” is the response. The questioner’s request for help in narrowing down the list of possible referents of “that dog” has led to a response that supplies more specifics. These specifics include breed (it’s a lab) and color (black), but also, as a busy dog park no doubt has more than one black lab in it, a statement of the posture and location (it’s standing near the gate) of the referent “dog.” By specifying something about the dog—in this case, where it is and what it is doing— attention has been directed to a particular dog. The dog is identified in this case not only by color, breed, etc., but by relating it to a proposition. This proposition is what is known as a relative clause, indicated between [square brackets] in example (1a): (1a)

{The dog [that’s standing near the gate over there]} looks thirsty.

In this example, we consider the noun “dog” to be the head noun (in boldface), since it is the entity (or referent) that is being described by the relative clause. Head nouns have two roles in a complex sentence containing a relative clause, 1) the role of the head noun inside the relative clause and 2) its role in the main clause. The head noun plus relative clause together form a {noun phrase}, surrounded by curly brackets.1 It is important to note that sentence (1a) consists of essentially two statements expressed as two clauses. These include a relative clause (a type of subordinate or dependent clause) whose function is to help the listener identify a specific dog, and a main clause whose function is to convey the primary information (to assert something) about the dog so identified (i.  e. that it looks thirsty). 1 We will treat the determiner “the” as part of the noun phrase {NP} as well. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-014

324 

 Tim Thornes

Notice that the entire noun phrase, including the relative clause, serves in the role of subject of the main clause, which expresses the main statement and includes a verb, “looks,” and a predicate adjective, “thirsty.” In most cases, the role of the complex noun phrase (the one containing the relative clause) in the main clause does not make a difference in the grammatical form of the relative clause itself. Note: (1b)

Do you see {the dog [that’s standing near the gate over there]}?

The noun phrase in curly brackets is identical in form to the one in example (1a), despite the fact that it is serving in the role of object of the main clause verb “see” in example (1b). In both examples, one imagines that the speaker felt that this additional information regarding the dog was necessary in order to both describe and identify one particular dog to the listener. Consider, by comparison, the following example from the Siouan language, Hidatsa: (2)      

Hidatsa (Missouri River; Siouan) (Park 2012:517) { masúga [agu–mii–náhci–s] } adáàsi–hgua dog rel–1b–bite–def outside–loc ‘The dog that bit me is outside.’

háhgu–c be.around–decl

In (2), masúga ‘dog,’ is the head noun, followed by the complex verb form aguwiiráhcis operating as a relative clause meaning ‘(one) that bit me.’ Perhaps the context involves my friend wondering why I am nervous about leaving his house to go home, and so I offer him (2) by way of explanation. At any rate, the relative clause narrows the referential range of all possible dogs to a particular one. We can consider masúga as playing a role in both the relative clause (the biter) and the main clause (the one outside). It is the role in the former that is important here, as that is the role that determines what type of relative clause it is (see section 14.3). In some cases, we find relative clauses that do not serve an identifying function by narrowing reference to a specific entity, but instead simply provide additional information about an entity whose identity is already known. Consider: (3)

Felix, [who works at the capitol], has been lobbying for the latest farm bill.

In this example, the proper noun, Felix, does not require further description to assist in identifying him, since we know his name. The relative clause in square brackets in example (3) is what is known as a non-restrictive relative clause type, since the relative clause construction here does not serve the purpose of restricting the range of possibilities to some entity. That is, reference to Felix’ work is not required in order for us to know who has been lobbying for the farm bill (although it may explain why he has). We do not put Felix in bold, since it is not, properly speaking, the head of a noun phrase that includes the non-restrictive relative clause.2 By comparison, the relative clause type

2 Also, being set off by commas, it is not part of the same intonational phrase as the would-be head noun.



Clause-combining: Relative clauses 

 325

exemplified in (1a-b) and (2) is called a restrictive relative clause. It is this latter type that we will be most concerned with in this chapter. As a brief note of caution, I think it important to acknowledge the influences of methodology on relative clause structures as they are represented and described in the languages of Native North America. The practice of direct elicitation, which usually involves asking speakers for the translational equivalents of engineered sentences in English, say, into the target language, increases the possibility of calques from English. That is to say, when one presents for translation an English example of a relative clause, it increases the likelihood that the translation equivalent will share grammatical properties in common with English. On the other hand, in searching a corpus of spontaneous speech for examples of relative clause structures, many of those studying Native North American languages find them to be either rare or absent.

14.2 Parameters of variation This chapter presents a survey of grammatical constructions in the languages of Native North America that are or could be properly considered relative clauses or, alternatively, serve the typical functions associated with relative clauses. Although we find a great deal of variation across the languages of the continent, this variation occurs within a limited set of parameters. We have already seen a couple of these parameters of variation at play in the discussion of examples in the introduction. Here are a few of the ways in which relative clauses may vary grammatically from language to language: 1) the position of the head noun vis a vis the relative clause, 2) the presence or absence of a special relativizer or relative pronoun, 3) the use of a specialized verb form or verbal morphology particular to relative clauses, 4) how the grammatical role of the head noun within the relative clause is identified (“recovered”), and 5) whether there are restrictions on tense or other distinctions that can be coded in the relative clause, in other words, the degree to which relative clauses resemble main clauses in finiteness (section 14.4). We will draw some connections between these parameters, since they interact with each other within the ecology of the various languages. Some theoretical issues will also be touched upon, but the goal of the present chapter is to lay a groundwork of facts, not a settlement of theory-specific concerns. For the student and teacher of Native North American languages, I hope that this survey provides for a deeper understanding of this complex area of grammar while also advancing possibilities for teaching and learning these interesting languages.

326 

 Tim Thornes

14.2.1 Presence and position of the head noun 14.2.1.1 External heads One parameter of variation is the position of the head noun with respect to the relative clause. In our English examples above, the head noun always comes before the relative clause (i.  e. to its left), and the relative clause may therefore be called a left-headed relative clause.3 The examples below also demonstrate this feature: (4)      

Northern Paiute (Uto-Aztecan) (Thornes 2003:464) { umɨ [kai u= pidzabi–dɨ] } ɨmɨ–nɔ tuʔi they not 3sg:acc= like–nmlz them–with try ‘Those that didn’t like it would try to fight with them.’

(5)      

Creek (Muskogean) (Martin 2011:392) { axéy ifá [ a:–hóyL–a:ti–t ] } lopéyc–i:–t that dog dir–stand.fgr–ref–t nice–dur–t ‘That dog standing over there is friendly.’

(6)      

Okahoma Cherokee (Iroquoian) (Montgomery-Anderson 2008:523) { a–skaya [ ji–jii–ali–hnohehtiisk–vv’i ] } a–ahnika 3a–man rel–1a.an–mdl–talk.with:inc–exp/sub 3A–leave:imm ‘The man that I was talking to left.’

na–koiwɨnai–ʔyakwi mm–fight.against–hab

ó:–s be.fgr–ind

The example in (4) has the pronoun umɨ ‘they’ acting as the noun phrase head whose identity is restricted to some subset of those present (in this case, those who were attending an Indian boarding school). In (5) the subject noun phrase of the main clause (in curly brackets { } ) headed by ifá ‘dog’ has a modifying relative clause (in square brackets [ ] ) a:hóyLa:tit ‘standing over there.’ Head nouns can be understood to play two roles in these complex sentences: 1) their grammatical role in the main clause and 2) their role in the relative clause. In both (4) and (5), the head nouns in bold are playing the role of subject in both the main clause and their respective relative clauses. This is not a necessary circumstance, however. In (6), the head noun askaya ‘man’ is followed by the restrictive relative clause jijiialihnohehtiiskvv’i ‘that I was talking to’ in square brackets. The head noun of the complex noun phrase ‘the man that I was talking to’ is playing the role of indirect object in the relative clause, while the entire noun phrase headed by askaya is serving the role of subject of the main clause centered on the verb of the main clause aahnika ‘he left.’

3 Many authors prefer to call these post-nominal relative clauses, based on the position of the relative clause with respect to the head noun. Since much of our focus is on the presence or position of the head noun, our preference will be to label relative clause types with respect to the property, or parameter, of “headedness.”



Clause-combining: Relative clauses 

 327

Although our examples thus far are left-headed, it turns out that headedness is a flexible concept in Cherokee, which allows for right-headed relative clauses as well, as in (7): (7)            

Okahoma Cherokee (Iroquoian) (Montgomery-Anderson 2008:523) { [ ji–kinii–atuuliisk–oi ] altiithla } rel–1b.dl–want:cmp–hab\sub car kaayuula khilo uu–hwas–ei already someone 3b–buy:cmp–nxp ‘The car we want has already been bought.’ lit. ‘The car that we want, someone already bought it.’

The right-headed relative clause type is found much less frequently in the languages of Native North America. Most examples I have found that illustrate this type demonstrate it as an alternative to the left-headed variety, as we have seen with Cherokee (and is reported as rare, but possible, in Creek, cf. Martin 2011). Ktunaxa, widely known as Kutenai,4 a language isolate5 of the northern Idaho panhandle and neighboring British Columbia, favors right-headed relative clauses as in (8): (8)      

Ktunaxa (isolate) (Dryer 2007:202) wiɬqaʔ–ni {[niʔ k=u big–indic def subord=1.subj ‘the woman that I saw was tall’

wukat] see

paɬkiy} woman

Notice here that the head noun paɬkiy ‘woman’ follows to the right of the relative clause. Unlike Cherokee, flexibility of head position appears not to be a characteristic of relative clauses in Ktunaxa. Tlingit (Na-Dene) represents another case whereby right-headedness has been analyzed as strict (Crippen 2012)6. To be clear, a strong preference for right-headed relative clauses is not at all uncommon in the world’s languages. Rather, it represents something of an anomaly in the indigenous languages of North America.

14.2.1.2 Internal heads In most cases of left- versus right-headedness, there is an assumption that the head noun is outside of the relative clause (external), as we have seen, especially where there is flexibility with respect to the order of the head and the relative clause, as in Cherokee. There are, however, a significant number of languages of Native North America that

4 The preferred name for the language is Ktunaxa (Matthew Dryer, in personal communication). 5 A language isolate is one for which a relationship to other languages in a language family has not been established or is in question. 6 I thank Ashlyn Nutting for alerting me to the Tlingit data and analysis in Crippen (2012).

328 

 Tim Thornes

offer examples of head nouns appearing inside the relative clause (internal). The following examples from Osage (Siouan), Navajo (Athabaskan), and Jamul Tiipay (Yuman) demonstrate the internally-headed relative clause type. (9)    



Osage (Siouan) (Quintero 2004:467) {[sitó̩i̩ níhkaši wi̩ a–ø–chí–api–ðe]} yesterday man a  prev–a3s–arrive.here–pl–decl John–a akxaíi–ø–ø–ðe–api–ðe John–syl subj prev–p3s–a3s–see–pl–decl ‘John saw the man that came yesterday’

(10)      

Navajo (Athabaskan) (Platero 1974:204) {[tl’éédá̩á̩’ ashkii aLhá̩á̩’–á̩á̩]} last.night boy 3sg.impfv.snore–rel.past ‘The boy who was snoring last night will speak.’

(11)      

Jamul Tiipay (Yuman) (Miller 2001:210) {[Manweel kaamiis txiil]}–pu nyaach Manuel shirt wear–dem i+sj ‘The shirt Manuel is wearing, I sewed it.’

yádooLtih fut.3sg.speak

shuukwil sew

In (9), the head noun níhkaši ‘man’ appears surrounded by other elements of the relative clause. This is also the case with ashkii ‘boy’ and kaamiis ‘shirt,’ in (10) and (11), respectively. One characteristic of relative clauses with internal heads is that, in most cases, the head nouns appear in the same position inside the relative clause as they would in an ordinary main clause, e.  g. before or after the predicate. Chimariko, a language isolate from northern California, also has internally-headed relative clauses, as is most clearly in evidence in example (12), although the source analyzes (13) as an example as well. (12)      

Chimariko (isolate) (Jany 2013:433) {[mo’a phuncar h–uwa–tku–rop ]} yesterday woman 3–go–dir–dep ‘That woman who came yesterday told me.’

(13)      

Chimariko (isolate) (Jany 2013:433) {[h–iman–tamo–rop map’un ]} h–i’am–ta 3–fall–dir–dep that.one 3–beat–der ‘Those fellows that went down got beaten.’

pha’yi–nip thus.say–pst

These examples also illustrate flexibility in the placement of the head noun within the relative clause as preceding the relative clause verb huwatkurop in (12) while following the verb himantamorop in (13). In both cases, the head noun bears the intransitive subject relation to the relative clause. According to Jany (2013:434), elements within a noun phrase in Chimariko show significant flexibility, while still remaining together as a



Clause-combining: Relative clauses 

 329

syntactic unit, or constituent. The dependent marking suffix –rop consistently indicates the status of the verb as that of a relative clause. Flexibility is also noted in the case of Jamul Tiipay, which allows “pre-posing” (movement to the beginning of the clause) of head nouns, as in the following: (14)      

Jamul Tiipay (Yuman) (Miller 2001:210) { [vineen nyaap wich]–pe–ch } m–aaxway–x–s poison me+abs have–dem–sj 3/2–kill–irr–emp ‘The poison that I have will kill you.’

By comparing the position of the object here to that in (11) above, we see that the object “poison” precedes the subject in (14), as opposed to the object “shirt” following it in (11). Languages with flexible word order—and there are many such languages in Native North America—typically use variable word order for pragmatic, rather than strictly grammatical, purposes (see Broadwell (this volume) and Berge (this volume)). Highlighting the relative importance of certain characters in the discourse, for example, is one common function of flexibility in word order.

14.2.1.3 Headlessness Finally, we will consider relative clause structures that appear without an overt head noun—what are commonly called either headless relative clauses or, alternatively, “free” relatives. In these cases, the relative clause itself is sufficient for narrowing down the range of possible referents, as in the following examples from English and Northern Paiute: (15)

I’ll have { [what she’s having.] }

(16)      

Northern Paiute (Uto-Aztecan) (Thornes 2003:502) { [ tɨ=tɨ–da–kwɨhɨ–na ] } pɨnau owi–tu hani–ʔyakwi logo= apass–IP/feet–get–ptcp again there–to do–hab ‘.  . what it grabs (with its claws), (it) brings back to there.’

In example (15) the reference to “what she’s having” narrows the range of choices (e.  g. at a restaurant) to another person’s order. In (16), from a traditional narrative about a flying monster that carries people away, the reference is narrowed to what or whoever is captured. Most of the languages surveyed allow for headless relative clause structures. Examples (17)-(21) illustrate the diverse range of languages that share this property. (17)      

Blackfoot (Algonquian) (Franz 1991:129) n–Imaat–ssksino–a:–yi {[om–iksi k–omoht–yIIstap–oo–at–a:–iksi]} 1–neg–know–dir–3pl that–3pl 2–from–away–go–fin–dir–3pl ‘I don’t know those you went away from.’

330 

 Tim Thornes

(18)      

Saanich (Salishan) (Montler 1993:260) kwən–nəxw = sən {[kwsə tɬ’itɬ’əw]} see–trans = 1sg:subj dem escaping ‘I saw the one who was getting away.’

(19)      

Wappo (isolate; proposed Yukian) (Thompson, et.al. 2006:122) {[maʔa mi thal mes–ta]} ah paʔe–siʔ just 2sg what make–pst:dep 1sg:nom eat–fut ‘I’ll eat whatever you made.’

(20)      

Creek (Muskogean) (Martin 2011:393) {[iLki–acól–i léyk–a:t]} L–óLho:y–ati:–s 3pat.father–old–I sit.sg.fgr–ref dir–reach.du.lgr–past5–ind ‘They got to where their elderly father lived.’

(21)      

Ktunaxa (isolate) (Dryer 2007:202) wiɬqaʔ–ni {[niʔ k=u wukat]} big–indic def subord=1.subj see ‘the person that I saw was tall’ or ‘the thing that I saw was big’

For some languages, like Blackfoot and Northern Paiute (according to Franz 1991 and Thornes 2012, respectively), it is reported that clearly headed relative clauses appear only rarely in natural discourse.7 We turn now to another feature of variation across relative clause structures, namely, the use of a specialized relativizer or relative pronoun, usually to introduce a relative clause but also correlating with other features of grammatical expression.

14.2.2 Presence of a relativizer or another boundary identifier The presence of a special relativizer or relative pronoun often serves to identify the boundary of a relative clause and, in some cases, to determine the role of the target entity within the relative clause. Looking back at the English examples above, “that” serves as relativizer in (1a) and (1b), while “who” is a relative pronoun in example (3). The main difference between the two is that a relativizer is generally fixed in form, typically serving only to indicate the presence or boundary of a relative clause. A relative pronoun, on the other hand, is sensitive to other features, like animacy, humanness, or number, that are assigned to the head noun. Note, for example, that while you can say “the man that” or “the man who”

7 It has been suggested that there may be a correlation between a high frequency in the use of headless relative clauses and the nominalization strategy discussed in section 14.4.2 (cf. Comrie and Estrada-Fernandez 2012).



Clause-combining: Relative clauses 

 331

in English, one cannot say “*the house who,” since a house is not animate. The invariant relativizer “that” can be used with any head noun type, but the relative pronoun changes form, depending upon these and other features. What counts as animate, or human, is a judgment that speakers of English make all the time. For some, “the dog who” is okay (dogs being closely associated with humans), but “the starfish who” is strange (despite being technically animate), unless one is referring to an animated character in a movie with human-like characteristics. Yakima Ichishkíin, a Sahaptian language of the Columbia Plateau region, has subordinate clauses, including relative clauses, which appear to be identical to an ordinary main clause (cf. Jansen, 2010:396–397). The only distinction from main clauses is the presence of a subordinating prefix (or clitic) ana– (what we could consider a relativizer REL), as in the following: (22)          

Yakima Ichishkíin (Sahaptian) (Jansen 2010:400) ku awkú pa–tamápaysh–ta {[ana–kuunak húuy and then 3sg.s –report.on–fut rel–obj in.vain pá–talax̱itk–sha]} inv–discipline–impv ‘And then they will report on that one who is disciplining them in vain.’

This boundary phenomenon appears to be the minimal requirement for identifying relative clauses as well as other subordinate clause types. Note that ana– in (22) attaches to the object pronominal kuunak to form a kind of relative pronoun translating roughly as ‘the one that.’ We see it elsewhere attached to interrogative pronouns like ‘who’ or ‘where’ as in the following: (23)    



Yakima Ichishkíin (Sahaptian) (Jansen 2010:399) {[ana–shín i–wiláalakw–ta pɨnmikínk tmíyu–t–ki]} rel–who 3sg.s–outrace–fut 3sg.pn.inst plan–nzr–inst awkú kushk i–wá–ta then winner 3sg.s–cop–fut Whoever comes up with the best plan (to create a night) will be the winner.’

In keeping with the English translation, ana– could be interpreted as ‘–ever’ as with ana–shín ‘whoever,’ ana–mún ‘whenever,’ and ana–miɬ ‘however many,’ among others. Notice, too, that both (22) and (23) represent examples of headless relative clauses. In Crow (Siouan), there are two “basic relativizers,” according to Graczyk (2007:254), which also appear as prefixes to the verb of the relative clause. Their forms are sensitive to whether or not the subject versus some other notional role is being played by the head noun in the relative clause. For subject relative clauses, the relativizer ak– is used, as in the following:

332 

(24)      

 Tim Thornes

Crow (Siouan) (Graczyk 2007:253) {hinne bacheé–m [ak–óopiia–sh]} is–bilé this man–det rel–smoke–det 3:poss–fire ‘this man who was smoking’s fire was burning down’

awá–ss–dee–m earth–goal–go–ds

Here, we can identify bacheém ‘the man’ as the one doing the smoking, the subject of the relative clause. It is isbilé ‘his fire’ that is the subject of the main clause verb awássdeem ‘going (burning) down.’ Recall that it is the notional role of the head noun in the relative clause that typically affects various aspects of the grammar of relative clauses. As another example of the use of ak–, consider the following headless relative clause: (25)      

Crow (Siouan) (Graczyk 2007:253) {[Baáhpuuo ak–kukaa–húua–sh]} Pryor rel–source–come–det ‘the one who came from Pryor…’

Despite the absence of a true head noun in (25), we still understand the relative clause to be identifying a target entity playing the role of subject with respect to the verb ‘come.’ Not all relative clause types require a relativizer in Crow, however. In many cases, like the following object relative clause, it is sufficient to use a determiner suffix to mark the right boundary of a relative clause: (26)      

Crow (Siouan) (Graczyk 2007:262) {shikáaka–m [xapíi–o–sh]} kuú–k boy–det lost–caus.pl–det come.back–decl ‘the boy that they lost has come back’

In (26), the definite determiner (det) –sh marks the boundary of the noun phrase whose head noun is shikáakam ‘the boy’ playing the role of object to the relative clause verb ‘lost.’ Note that the complex noun phrase shikáakam xapíiosh ‘the boy they lost’ is subject of the main clause verb kuúk ‘come back.’ Slave, a Northern Athabaskan language, uses separate words that Rice (1989) refers to as complementizers both to signal the final boundary of a relative clause and to indicate whether or not the head noun is definite. By definite, we mean that something is known to both the speaker and the hearer. Compare the following examples: (27)            

Slave (Athabaskan) (Rice 1989:1320) a. {ɬue [ɬan˜ihdé síi]} seghán˜ilã   fish 3.killed.3pl:obj comp 3.gave.1sg>3pl:obj   ‘he gave me the fish he killed’ b. {ɬue [ɬan˜ihdé líi]} seghán˜ilã   fish 3.killed.3pl:obj comp 3.gave.1sg>3pl:obj   ‘he gave me whatever fish he killed’



Clause-combining: Relative clauses 

 333

The complementizer síi in (27a) signals that the head noun is definite (referring to some particular, known bunch of fish), while líi in (27b) indicates that it is indefinite (e.  g. an unknown quantity or a variety of fish that is involved). Montgomery-Anderson (2008) reports that Oklahoma Cherokee represents a somewhat unusual case of boundary marking with regard to relative clauses. Cherokee relative clauses appear with both a relativizer (REL) (formally referred to as a “prepronominal prefix,” ji–) and a distinctive high-fall tone on the rightmost long vowel of the relative clause verb. Although tone is not an unusual property in the indigenous languages of North America (see Uchihara, this volume), this particular grammatical use of tone is otherwise unattested, as far as I am aware, for these languages.8

14.3 Relative clause types We began this chapter by looking at language-particular features in section 14.2, where we explored some basic ways in which relative clause formation can differ from language to language. Now we will look at how these features interact with one another to help determine what role the key referent (either in the form of a head noun or the understood target entity of a headless relative clause) is playing within the relative clause itself. Several times in our discussion, we have made a point of identifying the grammatical role of the head noun within the relative clause. This role is distinct from the role of the complex noun phrase in the main clause. This duality of roles is important to track, as it is most often the case that the role of the head noun (or target entity in the case of headlessness) is essential for describing the form and function of relative clauses. To clarify, consider the following English examples: (28)  

a. b.

{The dog [that chased the cat]} bit me. {The cat [(that) the dog chased]} got away.

Subject relative clause Object relative clause

These represent two distinct relative clause types, a subject relative clause (the head noun “dog” plays the role of subject of “chased”) in (28a) and an object relative clause (the head noun “cat” plays the role of object of “chased”) in (28b). In the subject relative clause of (28a), the relativizer “that” does the important work of marking the initial boundary of a relative clause “that chased the cat.” Note that without it, the sentence reads rather oddly, as one is led to believe that “the dog chased the cat” is the main clause. With the object relative clause in (28b), on the other hand, the relativizer is optional (and so appears in parentheses), and without it, we still recognize the role of the head noun in the relative clause, since “the cat” has been moved from its expected

8 The use of tone as an identifier of relative clauses is found elsewhere in the world, however, as in the Ebembe (Bantu; Niger Congo) language of eastern Congo and adjacent Tanzania (Iorio 2014:150  ff), where a verb’s final vowel in a relative clause carries high tone.

334 

 Tim Thornes

object position following the verb “chased.” The change in the position of the object noun leaves what linguists often refer to as a gap in the relative clause-level grammar. This gap is one strategy English has for identifying the role of the head noun in the relative clause. The relative clause verb “chase” requires an object to follow it, which, in the case of (28b), precedes it as the head of the relative clause. In the languages of Native North America, several factors operate to minimize the use of the gap strategy. The prevalence of internally-headed and headless relative clauses entails either that the head noun itself occupies the syntactic position where one may find a gap in other languages, or that there is no head noun available for “gapping.” In the case of external heads, the preponderance are left-headed, which essentially leaves the head of a subject relative clause in its usual position in the main clause. Finally, the extensive cross-indexing of participants on the verbs of both relative and main clauses in many Native North American languages leaves open the question as to whether one ought to consider syntactic gaps as such since the capacity these indexing patterns have for avoiding ambiguity is evident in the morphology. Since the gap strategy is largely unavailable on both practical and theoretical grounds, most Native North American languages use different means for indicating the role of a head noun in the relative clause. Considering Yakima Ichishkíin examples (22) and (23) above, we see that the relativizing prefix ana– can be found attached to an interrogative pronoun that is itself sensitive to which participant is being relativized. In the earlier examples, the target entity for relativization was left underspecified, since the relative clauses were of the headless variety. As another example, this time of an internally-headed relative clause, consider: (29)    



Yakima Ichishkíin (Sahaptian (Jansen 2010:399) ku kuuk awkú á–sɨ́ nwi–ya and now then 3o–say–pst {[ana–túun sapsikwʼa–t i–náktux̱–ɨnm–a]} rel–what.obj teach–nmlz 3sg.s–carry.back–cisl–pst ‘that’s when he spoke the teaching he brought back’

Here, the nominalized verb sapsikw’at ‘teaching’ serves as the object of the relative clause. Again, we see the importance of classifying relative clauses for an understanding of many aspects of relative clause grammar as compared to basic, or main clause grammar. Studies of relative clauses, especially studies designed to make broad comparisons between and among languages from all over the world, have taken pains to see what sorts of limitations there may be on particular relative clause types (like subject, object, indirect object, etc.). Some languages only allow for subject relative clauses,9

9 This has been most famously described for Malagasy, an Austronesian language of Madagascar (Keenan and Comrie 1977).



Clause-combining: Relative clauses 

 335

for example, while others, like English and Crow, allow head nouns to play nearly any grammatical role within the relative clause. Given the introductory nature of this survey and space restrictions, a more detailed comparison of how or whether such restrictions play out in the languages of Native North America will be set aside for another study. More important, for our purposes, is to explore the different ways various relative clause types may be expressed in the grammars of the individual languages.

14.4 Finiteness and nominalization Typical English relative clauses, like those in examples (1), (3), and (28), do not use special verb forms or harbor restrictions on indicating things like tense or aspect—what would otherwise distinguish them from main clauses. Instead, tense can be indicated within relative clauses, as in “the dog that’s standing” (present) versus “the dog that stood” (past) or “the dog that will stand” (future). Relative clauses that replicate many of the features, including tense marking, of main clauses are said to be finite, while those that show more restrictions in form are referred to either as less finite or even fully non-finite. Many linguists consider finiteness to be a scalar concept, meaning that there can be degrees of finiteness. As such, different subordinate clause types or relative clauses in different languages (related or not) may show variation in which or how many finite properties they share with independent clauses.

14.4.1 Finite properties in relative clauses By way of grasping the notion of finiteness further, consider that in English the form of the verb in a direct command, like ‘Go!’ or ‘Leave that alone!’ is almost completely non-finite. One cannot say ‘*Went!’ (past) or ‘*Will leave that alone!’ (future). Clausal complements in English illustrate variation in finiteness (see Dahlstrom, this volume, on subordinate clauses and complementation), as in ‘I told her to go’ or ‘I hope she will leave.’ The complement-taking verbs “tell” and “hope” have different finiteness requirements of their complements. The complement of “tell” requires an infinitive form of the verb. The complement of “hope,” on the other hand, cannot appear in the infinitive, but maintains the finite properties of the independent main clause, ‘she will leave.’ The forms of the pronouns that follow the complement-taking verbs are different as well. Following “tell” is “her,” the object form, behaving as the object of “tell.” Following “hope,” however, we have “she,” the subject form, in its role as subject of the complement clause. We could, on the basis of these two simple examples, conclude that the complement of “tell” is less finite than the complement of “hope.” That is, a hope-complement shares more properties with a fully finite main clause.

336 

 Tim Thornes

Let us now consider finiteness with regard to relative clauses in Native North American languages. As examples of relative clauses that are highly finite, note the following from the Salishan language Saanich. (30)      

Saanich (Salishan) (Montler 1993:256) ʔəw’ Xchi–t = sən { kwsə asp know–trans = 1sg:subj dem ‘I know the man who hit you.’

swəy’qaʔ male

[t’əm’–ə–sə ]} hit–trans–2obj

(31)      

Saanich (Salishan) (Montler 1993:258) ʔəw’ Xchi–t = sən { kwsə asp know–trans = 1sg:subj dem ‘I know the man who you hit.’

swəy’qaʔ male

[t’əm’–ət–əxw ]} hit–trans–2subj

There appears to be little to distinguish relative clauses from main clauses in Saanich. Indeed, Montler (1993) prefers the more general term attributive construction in describing these construction types based on the function of these structures to attribute particular features to some entity. The difference between the subject relative clause in (30) and the object relative clause in (31) can only be determined by the verb agreement with a second person object in the former, leaving swəy’qaʔ as the subject, and a second person subject in the latter with swəy’qaʔ the object.10 Other Salishan language specialists assert that relative clauses are simply not present in some languages of the family, either because one does not find a unique syntactic structure that can be referred to as such—one that is distinct from other structures that perform attributing functions or from general clausal subordination—or that there is little, if any, structural difference between a relative clause within a main clause and a sequence of two main clauses. This is due in large part to the highly finite nature of subordinate clauses generally in Salishan languages. Thompson River Salish arguably has relative clauses of both the headed (32) and headless (33) varieties, according to Kroeber (1997). Consider: (32)      

Thompson River (Salishan) (Kroeber 1997:385) y’e–mín–ne { he seʔlís t  ə  [qwəz–t–exw ] } good–trz–1s.ts art knife att art use–trz–2s.ts ‘I like the knife that you use/used.’

(33)      

Thompson River (Salishan) (Kroeber 1997:385) pze–ne {ɬ [xwuy’ kn–ci–s] } meet–1s.ts art fut help–trz+2s.obj–3.ts ‘I met the one who is going to help you.’

10 This can lead to potential ambiguities in the case of only third person participants as third person categories are typically unmarked.



Clause-combining: Relative clauses 

 337

It is interesting that in headed relative clauses, both Saanich and Thompson River Salish appear to place the verbal predicate after other elements of the clause, while in main clauses, the preference appears to be verb initial. Whether this sort of pre-posing is true for relative clauses more generally in Salishan merits exploration.11 Athabaskan languages are also noted for having strongly finite subordinate, including relative, clauses. In Bearlake, a Northern Athabaskan language, object marking on the main clause verb is missing when the object position is filled by an object relative clause. (34)      

Bearlake (Athabaskan) (Rushforth and Gorbet 1989:457) ’Ehkee { tLi̩ [nadéhtLa] } ghái̩ i̩ dá boy dog 3leave.perf 3.see.perf ‘The boy saw the dog that left (to return).’

Without object-marking on ‘see,’ the complex noun phrase containing the relative clause must be interpreted as the syntactic object of the main clause. Navajo, another Athabaskan language, carries a relativizing suffix –á̩á̩ that both marks the final boundary of the relative clause and indicates past tense, as we saw in example 10). As a counterpart to –á̩á̩, the relativizing suffix –ígíí indicates that the time frame for the relative clause is the present, as in the headless relative clause in 35): (35)      

Navajo (Athabaskan) (Platero 1974:204) { [KinLání–góó deeyáh–ígíí] } bééhonisin Flagstaff–to 3sg.go–rel.nonpast 3sg(obj).imperf.1sg(subj).know ‘I know (the person) who is going to Flagstaff.’

Morphologically, the only difference between relative clauses in Navajo and main clauses appears to be the use of these tense-sensitive relativizing suffixes. Platero (1974:203) notes that the non-past form –ígíí also functions as a general nominalizer, perhaps placing Navajo relative clauses on a scale of slightly reduced finiteness. At the non-finite end of the scale, we can take into account those subordinate clauses that bear more resemblance to noun phrases than finite clauses. We turn now to nominalization proper, a key strategy for forming relative clauses in several languages and language families of Native North America.

11 A general preference for left-headedness may be due, in part at least, to the connection between relative clause and like structures and a discourse function like topic or focus (a la Schachter 1973). The fronting of constituents is a common strategy for indicating discourse prominence.

338 

 Tim Thornes

14.4.2 Nominalization as strategy Relative clause structures that are the least finite share numerous features one expects of a noun phrase, rather than a clause. These features are well-known from numerous typological studies (cf. Keenan and Comrie 1977, Givón 1990, 2012, and Andrews 2007). Noun- or noun phrase-like structures that derive from verb- or clause-like structures are referred to as nominalizations. A simple nominalization that changes the word class (part of speech) of a verb into a noun can be illustrated with the verb “teach” and the noun “teacher.” Adding the suffix –er to the verb results in a noun meaning “one who teaches.” Northern Paiute, an indigenous Uto-Aztecan language of the northern Great Basin region of the western U.S. also uses a suffix –dɨ for the same task, turning the verb tɨničui ‘teach; tell stories’ into the noun tɨničuidɨ ‘teacher; story-teller.’ Northern Paiute and other Native North American languages, however, extend the use of the nominalization process toward turning an entire clause into a complex noun phrase. The examples in (36a) and (36b) illustrate this process: (36)            

Northern Paiute (Uto-Aztecan) (Thornes 2003:428) a. su=tɨhɨča oʔo wɨnnɨ   nom=deer there stand.sg   ‘The deer is standing out there.’ b. nɨ { ka=tɨhɨča [oʔo wɨnnɨ–dɨ] } punni   I  acc=deer there stand.sg–nmlz see:dur   ‘I see the deer that’s standing out there.’

In example (36a), we have a fully finite main clause. The simple form of the verb wɨnnɨ ‘stand.sg’ indicates that the act of standing is presently ongoing. In (36b), with the addition of the nominalizing suffix (nmlz), we have a structure that best translates as a relative clause in English. The head noun, tɨhɨča ‘deer,’ is being modified and referentially restricted by the relative clause in square brackets, [oʔo wɨnnɨdɨ]. We could translate this relative clause as ‘the one standing out there’ just as we could interpret tɨničuidɨ as ‘the one who teaches.’12 Looking back at examples (24) and (25) from Crow, it is not a coincidence that both ak– and ala– function as nominalizers as well as relativizers. In both Crow and Northern Paiute, the form of the nominalizer is a reflection of the relative clause type. Just as –dɨ in Northern Paiute appears on verbs in subject relative clauses, so, too, does ak– in Crow. In order to form a non-subject relative clause in Crow, the prefix ala– is used, while in Northern Paiute, the nominalizing participle suffix –na appears, as in:

12 It doesn’t work too well, however, to interpret the bracketed noun phrase in 38) to mean ‘over there stander.’



(37)      

Clause-combining: Relative clauses 

 339

Northern Paiute (Uto-Aztecan) (Thornes 2003:428) su={ miidɨ [ ɨ= kuhani—na ]} kai toki kamma nom= meat your= cook–ptcp neg correct taste ‘The meat you cooked doesn’t taste right.’

Aside from the use of a nominalizing suffix, a major feature of nominalized clauses in Northern Paiute and many other languages is that the subject of the relative clause appears in its possessive form. In (37) the ‘your’ possessor form ɨ= is the would-be subject of cook. Possession in noun and pronoun forms is a feature of noun phrases, not of clauses. In Mohave and other Yuman languages, the prefix kw– that appears on the verb of a subject relative clause does double duty as subject/agent nominalizer as well. In the Yuman case, however, instead of having a counterpart for non-subject relative clauses, the subject nominalizer kw– contrasts with its absence. Compare the following examples from Mesa Grande Diegueño: (38)            

Mesa Grande Diegueño (Yuman) (Dryer 2007:200) a. {[’ehatt gaat kw–akewii]}= ve= ch nye–chuukuw   dog cat rel.subj–chase]=def=subj 1obj –bite   ‘the dog that chased the cat bit me’ b. {[’ehatt gaat akewii]}= ve= ch chepam   dog cat chase]=def=subj get.away   ‘the cat that the dog chased got away’

The order of subject, object, and verb remains the same in both examples, a reflection of the SOV (subject-object-verb) word order in main clauses. The only difference in form between the two relative clauses is in the presence of the kw– prefix in (38a), signaling that subject of the relative clause ’ehatt ‘dog’ is the head noun of the complex noun phrase in curly brackets. The absence of this prefix in (38b) indicates that it is the object gaat ‘cat’ that is the head. So it is the combination of subject relative marking and word order that allows us to identify the head noun and its role in the relative clause. The Uto-Aztecan language Cupeño also bears key features of nominalization in forming relative clauses, including restrictions on verb finiteness. As Hill (2003) points out, Cupeño “verb constructions in complement and relative clauses are marked only for mood, not tense (116, emphasis added).” Note the following: (39)    



Cupeño (Uto-Aztecan) (Hill 2003:298) Aya { ataxa–m [ pe–m kwaw–in–t–am ] } then person–pl det–pl call–in–npn–pl ramaada–’i paas mekwel–pe’–men–wen. ramada–o thrice go.around–3pl–in.pl–pipl ‘And the people who were invited went three times around the ramada.’

340 

 Tim Thornes

Nominalized relative clauses typically have one or more of three key characteristics of non-finiteness: 1) restricted finite verb coding (e.  g. lack of tense; mood marking only in Cupeño), 2) the presence of a determiner to mark the boundary and definiteness of the entity identified by the relative clause, and 3) possessive/genitive case forms on participants (especially the subject role). Wappo, a California language isolate, expresses relative clause-like properties of modification and narrowing reference with internally-headed, dependent clauses. Subject relative clause: (40)      

Wappo (isolate) (Thompson, Park, and Li 2006:115–116) { [ cek’ew olol ] –i } i  peh–khiʔ dem man dance:dep –nom 1sg look:at–stat ‘The man who’s dancing is looking at me.’

Object relative clause: (41)      

Wappo (isolate) (Thompson, Park, and Li 2006:115–116) { [ i chuya t’um –t ] –i } shoy’i–khiʔ 1sg house buy –pst:dep –nom burn–stat ‘The house that I bought burned down.’

It is not readily apparent that these examples are in fact internally-headed, since in both cases, either a pronoun or a demonstrative precedes the head noun (perhaps as proclitic). In (40), the demonstrative appears to be functioning as a determiner, as does, arguably, the first-person singular pronoun in (41). This pronoun functions elsewhere as an inalienable possessor form. By this analysis, it may be possible to propose that what the authors call simply a “suffix for verbs in dependent clauses (xvii)” is actually a suffix indicating a nominalization. Chimariko, another isolate of California, illustrates many of the core features of nominalization in the formation of relative clauses as well. In Chimariko relative clauses, there is a complete lack of tense, aspect, or mood marking (the latter two of which are required on main clauses)—taking finite restrictions a step further than Cupeño, which still can be marked for mood, as we have seen. The dependent marker –rop, as we have seen above in examples (12) and (13) and as discussed in Jany (2013), could thereby be interpreted as a nominalizer.



Clause-combining: Relative clauses 

 341

14.5 Summary and questions What I have attempted in this survey is to explore a few broad issues with respect to relativization phenomena in this large and diverse linguistic landscape. First among these is the presence and position of a head noun whose modification or identification is the main function of the relative clause. Second has been the identification of distinct relative clause types and how the role of the head noun or, barring one, the target entity, is understood from the grammatical cues present in the language. Finally, we have explored the presence, in full or in part, of properties of nominalization as a strategy for forming structures one could reasonably claim to be relative clauses as distinct from other grammatical constructions. As a related issue, finiteness can serve to mask structural differences between relative and main clauses. Relative clauses have held great interest among typologists and syntactic theorists, with parameters for comparison and variation established for Standard Average European (SAE) and a few other well-known languages, usually with extensive literary traditions. Some such parameters either 1) are ambiguous when applied to Native North American languages or 2) do not apply to some of these languages, particularly when grammatical relations and alignment patterns are not of the SAE variety. Grammatical studies of Native North American languages have suffered, until relatively recently, from a narrow focus on morphological phenomena and a reliance upon direct elicitation as a barometer for the presence of certain readily identified, and identifiable, syntactic structures. The assumption not only of the presence of such structures in the target language under study, but of a similar, if not identical, set of associated functions, has often remained unquestioned. Thornes (2012) and Álvarez González (2016) focus on the referential function of (mostly headless) relative clauses in Uto-Aztecan languages over the modifying function, in keeping with what Álvarez González refers to as “the prototypical function of nominalization (135).” Indeed, Bickel (2011) asserts most simply that “relative constructions turn a propositional expression into a referential one (428).” It turns out that the distribution of the so-called “nominalization strategy” for forming relative clauses is scattered across the languages surveyed, but also appears to be a family trait, for example, in the Yuman, Uto-Aztecan, and Siouan families. Input from a greater range of languages is necessary both for establishing characteristics of relative clauses among related languages, and to look for possible areal influences from long term contact between languages. As relative clause structures appear to be particularly prone to syntactic calquing and other grammatical developments, like the use of interrogative pronouns as relative pronouns (Mithun 2012), one might expect to find specific features to be present across well-known linguistic areas like the Pacific Northwest, northern California, or the North American Southeast.

342 

 Tim Thornes

References Álvarez González, Albert. 2016. ‘The evolution of gramatical nominalizations in Cahita languages.’ In Claudine Chamoreau & Zarina Estrada-Fernandez (eds.), Finiteness and Nominalization, 107–139. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Andrews, Avery D. 2007. Relative clauses. In Timothy Shopen (ed.), Language Typology and Syntactic Description. Vol. 2: Complex Constructions, 206–236. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Bickel, Balthasar. 2011. Grammatical Relations. In Jae Jung Song (ed.), Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Typology, 399–444. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Chamoreau, Claudine & Zarina Estrada-Fernandez. 2016. Finiteness and Nominalization. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Comrie, Bernard & Zarina Estrada-Fernández (eds.). 2012. Relative Clauses in the Americas. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Comrie, Bernard & Tania Kuteva. 2005. Relativization strategies. In Matthew S. Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. (Available online at http://wals.info/chapter/96, Accessed on 2019-01-09.) Crippen, James A. 2012. Exploring Tlingit relative clauses: Morphology and syntax. University of British Columbia. https://tlingitlanguage.com/wp-content/uploads/2015/01/Crippen-2012-relativeclauses.pdf Dryer, Matthew S. 2013. Relationship between the Order of Object and Verb and the Order of Relative Clause and Noun. In Matthew S. Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. (Available online at http://wals.info/chapter/96, Accessed on 2019-01-09.) Dryer, Matthew S. 2007. Noun phrase structure. In Timothy Shopen (ed.), Language Typology and Syntactic Description. Vol. 2: Complex Constructions, 151–205. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Frantz, Donald G. 1991. Blackfoot Grammar. Toronto: University of Toronto Press. Givón, Talmy. 2012. Toward a diachronic typology of relative clauses. In Bernard Comrie & Zarina EstradaFernández (eds.), Relative Clauses in the Americas, 3–25. Amsterdam: John Benamins. Givón, Talmy. 1990. Syntax: A Functional-Typological Introduction. Vol. 2. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Graczyk, Randolph. 2007. A Grammar of Crow. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Hill, Jane. A Grammar of Cupeño. Berkeley: University of California Press. Iorio, David E. 2014. Subject and Object Marking in Bembe. Ph. D. dissertation, University of Newcastle upon Tyne. Jansen, Joana Worth. 2010. A Grammar of Yakima Ichishkíin/Sahaptin. Ph. D. dissertation, University of Oregon, Eugene. Jany, Carmen. 2011. Clausal Nominalization as Relativization Strategy in Chimariko. International Journal of American Linguistics 77. 429–443. Keenan, Edward & Bernard Comrie. 1977. Noun phrase accessibility and universal grammar. Linguistic Inquiry 8. 63–99. Kroeber, Paul D. 1997. Relativization in Thompson River Salish. Anthropological Linguistics 39(3). 376–422. Langacker, Ronald 1977. Studies in Uto-Aztecan Grammar, I. Arlington: SIL. Martin, Jack B. 2011. A Grammar of Creek (Muskogee). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Miller, Amy. 2001. Grammar of Jamul Tiipay. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Mithun, Marianne. 2012. Questionable relatives. In Bernard Comrie & Zarina Estrada-Fernández (eds.), Relative Clauses in the Americas, 269–300. Amsterdam: John Benamins. Mithun, Marianne 1999. The Languages of Native North America. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Montgomery-Anderson, Brad. 2008. A Reference Grammar of Oklahoma Cherokee. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Kansas, Lawrence.



Clause-combining: Relative clauses 

 343

Montler, Timothy. 1993. Relative clauses and other attributive constructions in Saanich. In Anthony Mattina (eds.), American Indian Linguistics and Ethnography in honor of Laurence C. Thompson, 241–63. (Occasional papers in linguistics). Missoula, MT: University of Montana. Munro, Pam. 1976. Mojave Syntax. New York: Garland. Nakayama, Toshihide. 2001. Nuuchahnulth (Nootka) Morphosyntax. Berkeley: University of California Press. Park, Indrek. 2012. A Grammar of Hidatsa. Ph. D. dissertation. Indiana University, Bloomington. Platero, Paul. 1974. The Navajo relative clause. International Journal of American Linguistics. 40. 202–46. Quintero, Carolyn. 2004. Osage Grammar. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Rushforth, Scott & Larry Gorbet. 1989. Notes on Bearlake Athapaskan relative clauses. International Journal of American Linguistics. 55. 455–67. Rice, Keren 1989. A Grammar of Slave. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Schachter, Paul. 1973. Focus and relativization. Language. 49. 19–46. Starks, Donna. 1995. Subordinate clauses in Woods Cree. International Journal of American Linguistics. 61. 312–27 Thompson, Sandra A., Joseph Sung-Yul Park & Charles N. Li. 2006. A Reference Grammar of Wappo. (University of California Publications in Linguistics 138). Berkeley: University of California Press. Thornes, Tim. 2012. Functional underpinnings of diachrony in relative clause formation: the nominalization-relativization connection in Northern Paiute. In Bernard Comrie & Zarina Estrada-Fernández (eds.), Relative Clauses in the Americas, 147–170. Amsterdam: John Benamins. Thornes, Tim. 2003. A Northern Paiute Grammar with Texts. Ph. D. dissertation. University of Oregon, Eugene. Whaley, Lindsay. 2011. Syntactic Typology. In Jae Jung Song (ed.), Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Typology, 465–486. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Amy Dahlstrom

15 Clause combining: Syntax of subordination and complementation Abstract: This chapter examines the phenomenon of subordination and how it is realized in the indigenous languages of North America. We will start by defining terms and looking at examples of different types of subordinate clauses, then consider ways in which various North American languages indicate that a clause is subordinate. One way in which languages can differ is whether the subordinate material is expressed in a full clause or one that is reduced in some way. Moreover, languages may exhibit different patterns of case marking, word order, or negation in subordinate clauses compared to the patterns found in main clauses. The final section considers some tricky cases in which it may be hard to tell whether a specific clause is a main clause or a subordinate clause.

15.1 Some definitions Let us start by defining some terms. A clause consists of a predicate (usually a verb), the arguments required by the predicate (for example, a subject and an object), plus optional modifiers providing information about the time of the event (for example “yesterday”) or the reason for the action, etc. Subordinate clauses are clauses in which the whole clause functions either as an argument or as a modifier in another clause, the main clause. In the Cherokee example in (1), the underlined portion is a subordinate clause expressing the time of the main clause:1 1 Abbreviations in the examples: 0 inanimate, 1p 1person exclusive, 3r third person reflexive, a Set A pronominal prefixes (Cherokee), a actor (Lakota), a A case (Northern Pomo), a transitive subject (Yupik), abl ablative, abs absolutive, Acomp adverbial complementizer, adv adverb, agt agent, ai Animate Intransitive verb, all allative, and andative, aor aorist, app applicative, area areal pronoun, art article, aux auxiliary, b Set B pronominal prefixes, C complementizer, c1 changed conjunct 1, cl clitic, cmp completive, cn conjunction, cnsq consequential, conj conjunct, ctl control, cvb converb, decl declarative, def definite, det determiner, di ditransitive suffix, ds different subject, dst distributive, e ergative, erg ergative, evid evidential, f feminine, fut future, ger gerund, hab.pres habitual present, hsy hearsay, I intransitive (Cherokee), I agreement class I (Choctaw), ic initial change, impv imperfective, inc incompletive, ind indicative, inf infinitive, infin infinitive, int.part interrogative participle, ipf imperfective, ipfv imperfective, irr irrealis, loc locative, lv linking vowel, m masculine, nc non-determinative connective, ncbr non-clause-bounded reflexive, neg negative, nom nominative, ns nonsingular, nxp nonexperienced past, nz nominalizer, o direct object, ob object, obj object, obl oblique, obv obviative, opt optative, ord ordinal, osp oblique-subject participle, part participle, pasv passive, pat patient, perf perfective, pf perfective, pft perfective, pl plural, po possessive, pp postposition, pr present, prc present continuous, pst, pt past, q yes/no question marker, refl reflexive voice, rel relative, rroot relative root, s singular https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-015

346 

(1)        

 Amy Dahlstrom

Cherokee (Montgomery-Anderson 2015: 338) sanaale yijayééja jalagi hadahntesgéesdi sanaale yi-ja-yéej-a jalagi hi-adanvhtesg-éesdi morning irr-2b-wake(i):cmp-cvb Cherokee 2a-think:inc-pft ‘In the morning when you wake up, think Cherokee!’

The main clause is ‘think Cherokee!’ and could be used on its own. The underlined part is a clause (it has a verb ‘wake up’, a subject ‘you’, and a modifier ‘in the morning’) and that whole clause functions as a modifer in the main clause, identifying the time when you should ‘think Cherokee!’. Notice that the underlined part (the subordinate clause) could not be said in isolation, except as a fragment answering a question like “When should I think Cherokee?” In the next section we will see a number of ways in which languages can mark clauses to indicate that they are subordinate: in (1) the subordinate clause is marked by a “converb” suffix -a on the subordinate clause verb, as well as by a change in tone on the verb. The opposite of subordination is coordination, where two (or more) simple clauses are combined into a single sentence and each half of the sentence is of equal importance. (2) is a Cherokee example of coordination: (2)        

Cherokee (Montgomery-Anderson 2015: 316) aniisgay aàníina aniichúújahno anii-sgaya anii-na anii-chúúja=hno 3a.ns-man 3a.ns-sit(ns):prc 3a.ns-boy=cn ‘The men are sitting and the boys are standing.’

aàniidóòna anii-dóòna 3a.ns-stand(ns):prc

(2) contains two main clauses: that is, each clause in (2) could be used on its own as a simple clause; moreover, the activity described by each clause in (2) is given equal weight. The part of (2) that means ‘and’ is the enclitic conjunction =hno, which attaches to the first word of the second clause. (Enclitics and proclitics are discussed in section 2.3 below.)

15.1.1 Types of subordinate clause: complement vs. adjunct Subordinate clauses like the one in (1) are called adjunct or adverbial clauses: they perform functions similar to simple adverbs identifying the time or reason or other circumstance related to the main clause. Another type of subordinate clause is known

(Choctaw, Northern Pomo, Southern Paiute) s subject (Nishga), s. someone/something, s3 subsidiary 3rd person, sap speech act participant, sbj subject, sg singular, sm subordinator marker, sr switch reference, ss same subject, su subject, sub subordinator, \sub subordinate tone, temp temporal, top topic, tr transitive, transloc translocative, u undergoer, vai verb, Animate Intransitive, vis visible, vti verb, Transitive Inanimate, X unspecified subject, wh question word.



Clause combining: Syntax of subordination and complementation 

 347

as a complement clause, a clause which expresses the subject or object of the verb of the main clause. For example, consider (3), also from Cherokee: (3)        

Cherokee (Montgomery-Anderson 2015: 319) uùnaduulis jalagi uuniiwooniíhisdi uunii-aduuli=s jalagi uunii-wooniíhisdi 3b.ns-want:prc=q Cherokee 3b.ns-speak:inf ‘Do they want to speak Cherokee?’

The underlined part of (3) is a complement clause, here functioning as the object of ‘want’. Like the adjunct clause in (1), the complement clause in (3) cannot be used on its own as an independent clause. Complement clauses are different from adjunct clauses, however, in that adjunct clauses are optional: in (1) the speaker has chosen to give extra information about the time when the main clause takes place. Complement clauses are not optional – if the underlined portion of (3) were omitted the remaining portion would not be a complete sentence.

15.1.2 Types of complements 15.1.2.1 Embedded statements Let us look at additional examples of complement clauses. Complement clauses often express statements, as in the Kutenai example in (4): (4)              

Kutenai (Morgan 1991: 445) Qa ʔupxni mi¢’qaqas qa ‿ ʔupx ‿ ni mi¢’qaqas neg ‿ see/know ‿ ind chickadee niʔs ksakiɬ hakiɬwi¢kiɬiɬ niʔ-s k ‿ sak-iʔɬ‿ hakiɬ-wi¢ki-ɬ-iɬ the-s3 sm ‿ still-adv ‿ keep-watch-di-pasv ‘The chickadees don’t know (that) they are being watched.’

The statement ‘they are being watched’ in (4) is what the chickadees don’t know.

15.1.2.2 Embedded questions It is also possible to use a complement clause to report a question that a subject asked or wondered about as in the Washo example in (5):

348 

(5)

 Amy Dahlstrom

Washo (The Washo Project Online Dictionary) béverli gó:beʔ hálaŋa hé:š yák’aš-i Beverly coffee still Q  warm-ipfv gó:beʔ métuʔ-mámaʔ-i coffee cold-finish-ipfv ‘Beverly asked if the coffee was still warm, but the coffee was cold.’

ʔ-í:d-i-š 3.sbj-say-ipfv-sr

The question ‘is the coffee still warm?’ is what Beverly asked. (The second line of (5) is a coordinate clause, conjoined with ‘Beverly asked…’.)

15.1.2.3 Future or hypothetical events Complements of verbs like ‘want to’ or ‘try to’ express future events or hypothetical events. We already saw a Cherokee example of this type in (3); another, from Northern Pomo, is in (6): (6)      

Northern Pomo (O’Connor 1992: 36) wayʔe duhu man natka early leave 3sf.A try ‘She tried to leave early.’

In (6), ‘leave early’ is what the subject of (6) tried to do.

15.1.3 Types of adjuncts Examples (3)-(6) illustrated types of complement clauses; now let us look at different types of adjunct clauses that a language might employ.2

2 Not discussed in this chapter are relative clauses, which are subordinate adjunct clauses which modify a head noun, as opposed to the subordinate adjunct clauses discussed here, which modify the main clause. For example, in the following Sahaptin sentence the underlined portion meaning ‘who rode across’ is a relative clause modifying ‘that man’, providing more information about which man is referred to. (i)      

Sahaptin (Rigsby and Rude 1996: 688) ín=aš á=qʼinu-šan-a kwaaná I=1sg 3abs-see-impv-pst that.obj ‘I saw that man who rode across.’

ɨwínš-na man-obj.sg

Relative clauses are discussed in Thornes, this volume.

ana-pɨ́n rel-3sg

i-qásu-yayč-a 3nom-on.horse-cross-pst



Clause combining: Syntax of subordination and complementation 

 349

15.1.3.2 Temporal adjuncts As we saw earlier in the Cherokee example in (1), adjunct clauses often provide information about the time of the event reported in the main clause relative to the time of another event. A similar example is seen in the Tonkawa sentence in (7): (7)          

Tonkawa (Hoijer & Wier 2018: 50) ʔawas-wa·-ka ya·c-ayco-na-l-ʔok meat-obv-nom.pl look-up-abl-3-when he-ylap-an-cʔel-ʔa·-yʔik yele·la-k-laknoʔo. refl-stand-ger-top-def-all sit-part-evid ‘When the buffalo looked up, he was sitting on top of the tree.’

Other temporal adjuncts may express temporal relations such as ‘while…’, ‘before…’, and ‘after…’.

15.1.3.2 Reason clauses Another semantic type of adjunct clause is one that identifies the reason for the event in the main clause occurring, as in the Northern Pomo example in (8): (8)      

Northern Pomo (O’Connor 1992: 257) tiʔ xama diṭhal-kan mo:w ncbr.obl foot hurt-Acomp 3sm.A ‘He’s not dancing because his foot hurts.’

khemane-nha dance-neg

According to O’Connor (1992: 39), the suffix -kan, glossed Acomp for adverbial complementizer, indicates that “[a]ction in suffixed clause precedes action in main clause, and main clause event is seen as resulting from event in suffixed clause.” (The abbreviation ncbr, non-clause-bounded reflexive, indicates that the possessor of the foot is the same as the person who is not dancing.)

15.1.3.3 Purpose clauses Other adjunct clauses make the goal or purpose of the action of the main clause explicit, as in the Haida example in (9): (9)      

Haida (Enrico 2003: 1045) dang-ga tla.ad-ee-ran hl you-pp help-infin-for I ‘I’ve come in to help you.’

qats’a-ang come.in-pr

In (9), the speaker asserts that he or she has come in in order to help the addressee.

350 

 Amy Dahlstrom

15.1.3.4 Concessive clauses Another type of adjunct clause is often translated in English with ‘although’ or ‘even though’, expressing a state of affairs that contrasts with what is expressed in the main clause. (10)          

Nishga (Tarpent 1987: 418) c’ə n’í[t]=ɬ qásq’an-ə-n=ɬ ɬáq’askw although that’s=nc dislike.food-ctl-2s=nc seaweed ʔi: məq’ap ḳíp-t and 2e must.eat.s.-3 ‘Even though you dislike seaweed, you have to eat it.’

In the Nishga example in (10) the speaker knows that the addressee does not like seaweed, but asserts that the addressee must eat it nevertheless.

15.1.3.5 Conditional clauses The final type of adjunct clause exemplified here is the conditional clause, or ‘if clause’. A conditional adjunct clause identifies a hypothetical state of affairs; the main clause expresses the consequence of that hypothetical condition: (11)      

San Carlos Apache (de Reuse 2006: 337, glosses added from de Reuse’s word list) Ńch’ii=yúgo, doo dadányu nahikai da it.is.windy=if neg outside we.(pl.).are.(around) neg ‘If it is windy we don’t go outside.’

In the Apache example in (11), the hypothetical condition is ‘if it is windy’ and the consequence of that condition is expressed in the main clause: ‘we don’t go outside’.

15.2 How are clauses identified as subordinate? Now that we have a sense of the range of functions subordinate clauses play in a sentence, let us investigate how languages distinguish subordinate clauses from main clauses and how different types of subordinate clauses might be identified.

15.2.1 Complementizer or particle Some languages indicate that a clause is subordinate by using a separate small word, often called a particle or a complementizer. (English uses this strategy with comple-



Clause combining: Syntax of subordination and complementation 

 351

mentizers like that or whether introducing complement clauses and words like when or if introducing various types of adjunct clauses.) An example of this strategy was seen above in the Nishga concessive clause in (10), which is introduced by a separate particle glossed ‘although’. More examples of particles can be seen in the Nez Perce examples in (12): (12)            

Nez Perce (Deal 2015: 412, 410) a. ke kaa Angel-nim   C  then Angel-erg   ‘when Angel calls them’ b. ke-x kaa Angel-nim   C-1 then Angel-erg   ‘when Angel calls me’

hi-nees-cewcew-téetu 3subj-O.pl-call-hab.pres hi-cewcew-téetu 3subj-call-hab.pres

In (12) the combination of ke and kaa indicate that the clause is a temporal adjunct clause, glossed ‘when’. See Aoki (1970: 126–127) for the wide range of particles found in Nez Perce subordinate clauses. As Aoki points out, some of the particles can be the host for subject and/or object inflection. In (12b), for example, the particle ke is followed by -x, which indicates the first person singular object of the verb ‘call’.

15.2.2 Affix Many of the languages of North America exhibit complex morphology on the verb, with agreement for both subject and object, incorporated objects, incorporated adverbial material, etc. (see Zúñiga, this volume, and Broadwell, this volume) It is therefore not surprising that some of the languages of North America indicate that a clause is subordinate by adding an affix to the verb of the subordinate clause. We have already seen several instances of this strategy in the examples above: for example, the Cherokee example in (1) has a “converb” suffix -a attached to the adjunct clause, and the Tonkawa example in (7) includes a suffix -ʔok glossed ‘when’. Languages with switch reference (see McKenzie, this volume), where the switch reference system extends to subordinate clauses, often have paired suffixes marking specific types of subordinate clauses. In such systems, one member of the pair of suffixes indicates that the subject of the subordinate clause is the same as the subject of the main clause and the other member of the pair indicates that the subject of the subordinate clause is different from the subject of the main clause. Consider the pair of Choctaw sentences below: (13)      

Choctaw (Broadwell 2006: 263) a. Kaah sa-nna-haatokoosh, iskali’ ittahobli-li-tok   car 1sI-want-because:ss money save-1sI-pt   ‘Because I wanted a car, I saved money.’

352 

     

 Amy Dahlstrom

b.    

Kaah banna-haatoko, iskali’ ittahobli-li-tok car want-because:ds money save-1sI-pt ‘Because he wanted a car, I saved money.’

Both sentences of (13) exhibit adjunct clauses of the reason type, and both adjunct clauses are identified by a suffix on the verb of the subordinate clause. In (13a) the suffix expressing ‘because’ is -haatokoosh while in (13b) the suffix glossed ‘because’ is -haatako. The suffix in (13a) also indicates ‘same subject’ – that is, the subject of ‘want’ and the subject of ‘save’ are the same person. In (13b) the suffix indicates ‘different subject’, because the subject of ‘want’ is not the same as the subject of ‘save’.

15.2.3 Clitic As discussed in Zúñiga (this volume), clitics resemble affixes in being phonologically dependent on a host word to be pronounced, but in other respects have properties of separate words. Some languages of North America identify subordinate clauses by using a clitic, rather than an affix or a separate particle. A clitic which precedes the host it attaches to is called a proclitic; one which follows the host is called an enclitic. An example of an enclitic was seen in (2), with the Cherokee conjunction =hno ‘and’. For an example of a proclitic consider the following Caddo example, in which a proclitic nat appears in a temporal adjunct clause: (14)        

Caddo (Melnar 2004: 94–95) nappáwdihšiyah … nahašʔnáwwá·yáh nat#wa-wid(i)-ih-šiyah nak ašnáw-wa-yáh temp.sub#pl-arrive-and-transloc.perf transloc.ind#meal-pl-eat ‘When they arrived there, … they would eat something there.’

dikaʔháy dikaʔháy something

The symbol # after nat indicates that the usual word-internal phonological processes of Caddo do not apply at that boundary, reflecting a difference between clitics and ordinary affixes.

15.2.4 Special paradigms of subject/object agreement In other languages, the difference between main and subordinate clauses may be indicated by the use of a separate paradigm, that is, the set of affixes agreeing with the subject (and object, in some languages). For example, in the Algonquian language Meskwaki third person singular is expressed by the suffixes -w-a in the independent indicative paradigm, which is only used for verbs in main clauses. See (15a):



(15)                                

Clause combining: Syntax of subordination and complementation 

 353

Meskwaki (Dahlstrom 2000: 76–78)   mahkate·wi·wa   mahkate·wi·-w-a   fast-3-sg   ‘he/she fasts; he/she fasted’ [main clauses only] b. mahkate·wi·tehe   mahkate·wi·-t-ehe   fast-3-mode.suffix   ‘if he/she had fasted, …’  c. e·hmahkate·wi·či   e·h-mahkate·wi·-t-i   aorist-fast-3-mode.suffix   that he/she fasts; that he/she fasted.’ d. me·hkate·wi·či   ic-mahkate·wi·-t-i   ic-fast-3-mode.suffix   ‘when he/she fasted, …’

Other paradigms, the conjunct forms, are used mostly in subordinate clauses.3 A sampling of conjunct paradigms is shown in (15b-d): in all three forms third person is expressed by the suffix -t, which is palatalized to [-č] if it is followed by [-i], as in (15c-d). Other parts of the conjunct verbs indicate what role the subordinate clause plays. For example, (15b) is a contrary-to-fact ‘if’ clause, (15c) is a complement clause which could be used with a main clause such as ‘I know…’, and (15d) is an adjunct clause expressing ‘when’ in the past. These functions are identified with a combination of the final suffix (glossed mode suffix in (15b-d) above) and what appears on the left edge of the verb. On the left edge of the verb there are three possibilities: nothing at all (15b), a prefix e·h-, glossed aorist (15c), or a process known as initial change (ic) in the Algonquianist literature which changes the length and quality of the vowel of the first syllable of the verb. In (15d) initial change has changed the short [a] of the stem to a long [e·]. Note that for the purpose of glossing examples, initial change is represented as if it were a prefix on the left edge of the verb stem, both in the morphological breakdown in line 2 and in the glosses in line 3. In each form in (15b-d), neither the mode suffix at the right edge of the verb nor what appears on the left edge of the verb (if anything) can be identified as marking the verb’s function on its own: each of the mode suffixes appears in other conjunct paradigms, as does the aorist prefix and initial change. It is the combination of the material on the left and right edges of the verb that indicates the function of the verb’s clause. See Dahlstrom (2000: 76–78) for more discussion.

3 See section 5 for discussion of conjunct forms used in main clauses.

354 

 Amy Dahlstrom

15.2.5 Nominalization In some languages of North America, forming a subordinate clause involves turning the clause into a noun.4 Many Salish languages employ this strategy (Kroeber 1999: 100): (16)      

Lushootseed (Kroeber 1999: 102) ʔi ‿cən ʔa·-t kwθə ‿dáktə aux ‿ 1s.su.cl call.tr art ‿ doctor ‘I called a doctor to look me over’

kwə s‿ -ƛ’èm-ə-θ-ám’š-s art ‿ nz-look-lv-tr-1s.ob-3.po

In (16) the final word of the sentence expresses a purpose clause ‘[for him] to look me over’. The purpose clause is formed by turning the whole clause into a noun, indicated by the prefix s- (in boldface) on the verb ‘look’; s- is glossed nz for nominalizer. As a result of the nominalization, the subject of ‘look’ is expressed as a possessor (the boldfaced suffix -s at the right edge of the nominalized clause). The purpose clause can thus be translated literally as “his looking me over”. Another consequence of turning the purpose clause into a noun is that the subordinated material is introduced with an article kwə, appearing on the left of the nominalized clause.

15.2.6 Tonal contour The tonal language Cherokee uses tone to identify subordinate clauses. All subordinate verbs in Cherokee bear a “highfall” tone; for some subordinate clauses this is the only indication that they are subordinate. (17)        

Cherokee (Montgomery-Anderson 2015: 342–343) duùhwahtvvhe taliine aániihlinaʔééʔi dee-uu-hwahtvvh-e tali-iine anii-hlinaʔ-éeʔi dst-3b-find:cmp-nxp two-ord 3a.ns-sleep(ns):inc-nxp\sub ‘He found them asleep again.’

The gloss \sub indicates highfall tone, here on the nonexperienced past suffix. According to Montgomery-Anderson (2015: 467), the highfall tone rises higher than regular high tone, and falls a little at the end.

4 Nominalization as a strategy in relative clause formation is also discussed in some detail in Thornes, this volume.



Clause combining: Syntax of subordination and complementation 

 355

15.2.7 No marking There sometimes is no special marking identifying a clause as a subordinate clause. Watkins (1984: 235) states that some complement clauses in Kiowa are “simply juxtaposed to the main clause.” (18)          

Kiowa (Watkins 1984: 235) nɔ́·-p’ì· ę́-tét my-sister [(2,3sg/agt):1sg/pat:∅/obj]-tell/pf á-k’ì·-dè Carnegie-kù ∅-bá·n-ê· her-husband-poss Carnegie-to [3sg]-go-ipf/hsy ‘My sister told me that her husband was going to Carnegie.’

15.2.8 Embedded questions The special type of complement clause expressing a question often contains a question particle, if the embedded question is a yes-no question, or a question word, if the embedded question is of the question word type. Here are examples of an embedded yes-no question and an embedded question word question from Slave: (19)      

Slave (Rice 1989: 1175) John [ ʔeyi t’eere sú húhshu ] kodįhshǫ́ le that girl Q  3 opt.marry   3 know.neg ‘John doesn’t know if that girl is getting married.’

(20)      

Slave (Rice 1989: 1181) [  ʔamíi ʔat’į ]  keodįhsǫ́ le   who 3 is   3 knows area.neg ‘She doesn’t know who it was.’

See also the Washo example of an embedded yes-no question in (5). Meskwaki, on the other hand, employs its rich system of verbal paradigms (discussed earlier in 2.4) to indicate that a subordinate clause is an embedded question. In (21) there is no independent question word corresponding to ‘who’. Instead, the subordinate verb is identified as an embedded question by the combination of initial change on the first syllable of the compound verb plus the suffixes on the verb ‘eat’. Furthermore, the final suffix -a on the verb ‘eat’ indicates that the element being questioned is the subject of the verb ‘eat’.5

5 See Dahlstrom 2019 for a detailed discussion of these Meskwaki forms, known as interrogative participles in the Algonquianist literature.

356 

(21)              

 Amy Dahlstrom

Meskwaki (Dahlstrom 2019: 77) e·hwe·pi–nana·tohtawi·nameki .  .  . e·h-we·pi–nana·tohtaw-i·nameki .  .  . aor-begin–ask-X>1p/aor e·škike·hi–mi·čikwe·na ic-aški–=ke·hi –mi·či-kwe·n-a ic-first.time–=and –eat-3>0/int.part-3 ‘They (unspecified) began to ask us .  .  . and who ate it first.’

15.3 Full clause or reduced? Besides the differences illustrated in the preceding section regarding the strategies used to indicate that a given clause is subordinate, there are also differences seen among the languages of North America in terms of whether the subordinate clause expresses the full range of information that would be found in a main clause. Some languages, such as the Algonquian language Blackfoot and the Salish language Halkomelem, have no infinitive forms (Ritter and Wiltschko 2004). In other words, every subordinate clause in those languages expresses the full range of information found in main clauses. Other languages exhibit various restrictions on what grammatical information is expressed in a subordinate clause. For example, Rood (1996: 590) reports that Wichita marks fewer tense/aspect distinctions in subordinate clauses than are found in main clauses.

15.3.1 Infinitives Some North American languages are described as having infinitive forms of the verb in some subordinate clauses. Lakota is an example: (22)        

Lakota (Ullrich 2018: 16) Inúŋwaŋ iblútȟe. inúŋwAŋ i-bl-(y)útȟA swim try-1sg.a-stem ‘I tried to swim.’

In (22) the main clause verb ‘try’ is infixed with a first singular agent morpheme but the verb of the subordinate clause has no marking for subject. Nevertheless, the subject of ‘try’ is understood to also be the subject of ‘swim’ in the Lakota example, just as it is in the English translation which uses an infinitive form of ‘swim’. Similar examples were seen above in (6) for Northern Pomo and (9) for Haida.



Clause combining: Syntax of subordination and complementation 

 357

A different sense of the term “infinitive” is used in Montgomery-Anderson’s (2015) description of Cherokee. Cherokee infinitives have prefixes identifying the subject of the verb of the subordinate clause but do not indicate the tense/aspect of the verb.6 (23)        

Cherokee (Montgomery-Anderson 2015: 75) aànehldi uudagesv uuyoohuúsehdííʔi a-anehldi uudagesv uu-yoohuúsehdííʔi 3a-try:prc weight 3b-lose:inf ‘He is trying to lose weight.’

Another example of a Cherokee infinitive was seen above in (3).

15.3.2 Both main and subordinate predicates in a single verb The most extreme example of reducing the subordinate “clause” is found in languages which permit incorporation of complement predicates into the main clause verb. For example, in the Yupik example in (24) the main clause predicate is ‘say’ (in boldface) and the predicate of the complement of ‘say’ is ‘wait for’, which is expressed as part of the same verb: (24)      

Central Alaskan Yupik (Woodbury 2017: 555) atanqe-ciq-ni-llru-ateng ama-ni wait.for-future-say-past-cnsq.3sga+3r.plO there-loc ‘Because he said that (he) will wait for them there’

Notice that Yupik allows separate tense markers for the two predicates in (24): ‘wait for’ is future tense and ‘say’ is past tense. Another example of this type can be seen in the Meskwaki example in (21), where ‘begin’ and ‘ask’ are compounded into a single verb.

15.4 Interactions with case-marking, word order, and negation The difference in syntactic contexts between main clauses and subordinate clauses can have an effect on other parts of the grammatical system of a language. For example, the Uto-Aztecan language Southern Paiute exhibits different case-marking patterns depending on whether the clause is main or subordinate. Subjects in main clauses take nominative case, while subjects in subordinate clauses take oblique case.

6 Examples like the Cherokee forms in (3) and (23) suggest that finiteness is perhaps best thought of as a matter of degree, rather than a clear-cut opposition between finite and non-finite.

358 

(25)          

 Amy Dahlstrom

Southern Paiute (adapted from Bunte 1986: 283) nʉ’ pʉsuchuxwai-yʉ-ak Johni-ung kiritsi-ang I.nom know-pr-3vis John.obl-art cat.obl-art arungwa-ngkʉ-kai-naya-anga-n uy-app-perf-osp.obl-3svis-1s I know (it) that John bought the cat for me.’

In (25) the first-person subject of the main clause is in nominative case, while ‘John’, the subject of the complement clause, takes an oblique case marker. The basic word order pattern of a language may also differ depending on whether the clause is a main clause or a subordinate clause. For Quileute, Andrade (1933: 278) reports, “In the main clause the normal order is (1) verb, (2) subject, (3) object. In the subordinate clause the subject precedes the verb.” (26)            

Quileute (Andrade 1933: 280, 285) toqò·l dâ·kil yik hadós·t’ot’ ki’ k’ade´’ya’a´k replied then the elder.sister the her.younger.sister “hé.sekłli tca’à· ha’ tcè·k u t’łotóloo’t tas há.kutax̣a‘.” I.prefer yonder that large star 3rd.person.conditional come ‘Then the elder girl said to her younger sister, “I should prefer that big star yonder would come.”’

In (26) the first clause is a main clause, with the subject (in boldface) following the verb. In the second line there is a complement clause in which the subject ‘that big star yonder’ (in boldface) precedes the verb of the complement clause, ‘come’. Another example of how the difference between main and subordinate clauses can affect other parts of the language is in the expression of negation. In Potawatomi, two different strategies are used to negate a verb, depending on whether the verb is in a main clause or in a subordinate clause. The following examples are from the Forest County, Wisconsin, dialect of Potawatomi: (27)                

Potawatomi (Lockwood 2017: 115) a. Jo wi nin nwi-byasi   jo=wi nin n-wi-bya-si   neg I  1-fut-come.vai-neg   ‘I’m not coming tomorrow.’ b. ga-bwa-wje-bontawat   ic.gi-pwa-wje-bonet-awat   pst-neg-rroot-quit.vti-3.pl.conj   ‘why they did not quit it’

wabek wabek tomorrow

(27a) is a main clause; negation here is expressed with a negative particle jo plus a suffix -si on the verb (both in boldface). In (27b), however, the subordinate clause is negated with a preverb pwa compounded with the verb and there is no negative morpheme suffixed to the verb.



Clause combining: Syntax of subordination and complementation 

 359

15.5 Some tricky cases Most of the time it is easy to decide whether a particular clause in a language is a main clause or a subordinate clause. There are, however, a few tricky cases to be aware of, which will be covered in this section.

15.5.1 “Cosubordination” Occasionally languages have constructions which seem to exhibit features of both coordination and subordination; the term which has been coined for such constructions is cosubordination. An example can be seen in the Siouan language Crow. Crow has a switch reference system, but the system operates only on coordinate clauses. In the Crow construction which is described as cosubordination, chains of clauses are connected by switch reference markers, with only the final clause of the chain bearing a marker indicating the speech act type: (28)          

Crow (Graczyk 2007: 402) alápasshi-ss-basaa-(a)k dáakbachee-sh hii-ák kukaaaaxp-ák direction-goal-run-ss his.son-det reach-ss embrace-ss óhchikaap-ak iispáschi-k huu-k   greet-ss kiss-decl say.pl-decl   ‘he ran toward him, he reached his son, he hugged him, he greeted him, he kissed him’ (Lk 15:28)

The same-subject markers on the non-final clauses are in boldface, as is the declarative speech act marker -k on the final verb of the chain, ‘kiss’. (The final word in (28) is a reportative evidential which also has the declarative speech act marker.) The effect of the declarative marker extends over the entire chain, and the non-final clauses cannot be used on their own — features which suggest subordination. However, the Crow switch reference system does not otherwise appear on subordinate clauses, only on coordinate clauses, making constructions like (28) difficult to classify as involving either subordination or coordination. For a further example of a language analyzed as exhibiting cosubordination, see Jacobsen (1992), a lengthy discussion of various subordinate and cosubordinate constructions in the Wakashan language Nootka.

15.5.2 Formally subordinate constructions used in main clauses In some languages, clauses bearing morphology which usually indicates that the clause is subordinate can used as main clauses in certain contexts. For example, the Algonquian language Plains Cree exhibits a similar distinction between independent and

360 

 Amy Dahlstrom

conjunct paradigms as the one discussed above in 2.4: in Cree, second person singular is indicated by a prefix ki- plus a suffix -n in the independent paradigm, used only in main clauses (29a), while conjunct verbs require a suffix -yan to express second person singular (29b). (29b) shows that a verb bearing conjunct inflection can be used in a main clause: (29)                

Plains Cree (Cook 2014: 140) a. kinôhtêhkatân cî   ki-nôhtêhkatê-n cî   2-hungry.vai-sap q    ‘Are you hungry?’ [independent inflection] b. ê- nôhtêhkatêyan cî   ê- nôhtêhkatê-yan cî   c1-hungry.vai-2 q    ‘Are you hungry?’ [conjunct inflection]

According to Cook (2014: 140), the form in (29a), with the independent inflection normally used for main clauses, is used to ask someone “out of the blue” if they are hungry, such as when someone has come to visit. The form in (29b), using conjunct morphology otherwise found on subordinate clauses, is used when the context creates a presupposition relevant to the utterance, perhaps if the addressee is rummaging in the fridge looking for food.

15.5.3 Historical change/reanalysis In the section above we saw that verbs bearing subordinate clause marking may be used in a main clause in certain contexts. For some languages, such variation between subordinate clause functions and main clause functions eventually results in certain constructions being reanalyzed as being main clauses only.7 As a result, the verbal morphology which originally marked the clause as being subordinate still appears on the newly reanalyzed clause, even though the clause is no longer subordinate. An example of this can be seen in the Algonquian language Menominee, where question-word questions (as opposed to yes-no questions) require conjunct inflection on the verb. (30)      

Menominee (Johnson and Macaulay 2015: 344) Tāq kēs–mēcek? wh pst–eat.ai.3conj ‘What did he eat?’

7 See van Gelderen, this volume for this type of historical change involving negative particles.



Clause combining: Syntax of subordination and complementation 

 361

As in other Algonquian languages, the primary function of conjunct morphology in Menominee is to indicate that the clause is subordinate. Given that, we might expect the translation of (30) to be something like “What is the thing which he ate?”, with ‘eat’ appearing in a subordinate clause. Johnson and Macaulay (2015), however, present evidence which indicates that the verb in questions like (30) is not part of a subordinate clause but is instead the main clause verb of the question. Menominee questions are therefore an instance where the syntax of the construction has changed over time, reanalyzing an originally subordinate clause to be a main clause, but where the morphology of the construction still reflects the syntax of the older construction.8

15.6 Conclusion This chapter has defined various types of subordination, surveyed ways in which subordination may be indicated, shown how the syntactic context of subordination can affect other parts of a language’s linguistic system, and ended by pointing out a few pitfalls that analysts of a language may need to watch out for in classifying clauses as subordinate.

References Andrade, Manuel J. 1933. Quileute. In Franz Boas (ed.), Handbook of American Indian languages 3, 151–292. New York: Columbia University Press Aoki, Haruo. 1970. Nez Perce grammar. University of California Publications in Linguistics. Berkeley: University of California Press. Broadwell, George Aaron. 2006. A Choctaw reference grammar. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Bunte, Pamela A. 1986. Subordinate clauses in Southern Paiute. International Journal of American Linguistics 52. 275–300. Cook, Clare. 2014. The clause-typing system of Plains Cree: Indexicality, anaphoricity, and contrast. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Dahlstrom, Amy. 2000. Morphosyntactic mismatches in Algonquian: Affixal predicates and discontinuous verbs. In Arika Okrent & John Boyle (eds.), Proceedings from the panels of the Chicago Linguistic Society’s thirty-sixth meeting, 63–87. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society. Dahlstrom, Amy. 2019. Embedded questions in Meskwaki: Syntax and information structure. In Monica Macaulay & Margaret Noodin (eds.), Papers of the 48th Algonquian Conference 69–85. East Lansing: Michigan State University Press.

8 Another example of subordinate clauses being reanalyzed as main clauses can be seen in auxiliary verb constructions in Yuman languages, where the main verb and the auxiliary are separated by the suffix which otherwise marks ‘same-subject’ in a switch reference system. In such constructions the verb which is now the main verb must have originated as the verb of a subordinate clause, subordinated to the verb which now functions merely as an auxiliary (McKenzie 2015: 436).

362 

 Amy Dahlstrom

Deal, Amy Rose. 2015. A note on Nez Perce verb agreement, with sample paradigms. In Natalie Weber, Erin Guntly, Zoe Lam & Sihwei Chen (eds.), Proceedings of the International Conference on Salish and Neighbouring Languages 50, 389–413. Vancouver: University of British Columbia Working Papers in Linguistics. Enrico, John. 2003. Haida syntax. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Graczyk, Randolph. 2007. A grammar of Crow. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Hoijer, Harry & Thomas R. Wier. 2018. Tonkawa texts: A new linguistic edition. Norman: University of Oklahoma Press. Jacobsen, William H., Jr. 1992. Subordination and cosubordination in Nootka: Clause combining in a polysynthetic verb-initial language. In Robert D. Van Valin (ed.), Advances in Role and Reference Grammar, 235–274. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Kroeber, Paul D. 1999. The Salish language family: Reconstructing syntax. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Johnson, Meredith, & Monica Macaulay. 2015. A monoclausal analysis of Menominee wh-questions. International Journal of American Linguistics 81. 337–377. Lockwood, Hunter Thompson. 2017. How the Potawatomi language lives: A grammar of Potawatomi. Madison: University of Wisconsin dissertation. McKenzie, Andrew. 2015. A survey of switch reference in North America. International Journal of American Linguistics 81. 409–448. Melnar, Lynette R. 2004. Caddo verb morphology. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Montgomery-Anderson, Brad. 2015. Cherokee reference grammar. Norman: University of Oklahoma Press. Morgan, Lawrence. 1991. A description of the Kutenai language. Berkeley: University of California dissertation. O’Connor, Mary Catherine. 1992. Topics in Northern Pomo grammar. New York: Garland. de Reuse, Willem J. & Philip Goode. 2006. A practical grammar of the San Carlos Apache language. (LINCOM studies in Native American linguistics 51). Munich: Lincom Europa. Rice, Keren. 1989. A grammar of Slave. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Rigsby, Bruce & Noel Rude. 1996. Sketch of Sahaptin, a Sahaptian language. In Ives Goddard (ed.), Handbook of North American Indians, Volume 17, 666–692. Washington, DC: Government Printing Office. Ritter, Elizabeth, & Martina Wiltschko. 2004. The lack of tense as a syntactic category: Evidence from Blackfoot and Halkomelem. In J. C. Brown & Tyler Peterson (eds.), Proceedings of the 39th International Conference on Salish and Neighbouring Languages 14, 341–370. Vancouver: University of British Columbia Working Papers in Linguistics. Rood, David. 1996. Sketch of Wichita, a Caddoan language. In Ives Goddard (ed.), Handbook of North American Indians, Volume 17, 580–608. Washington, DC: Government Printing Office. Tarpent, Marie-Lucie. 1987. A grammar of the Nisgha language. Victoria: University of Victoria dissertation. Ullrich, Jan. 2018. Modification, secondary predication, and multi-verb constructions in Lakota. Düsseldorf: Heinrich Heine University dissertation. The Washo Project. Online dictionary. Chicago: University of Chicago. http://washo.uchicago.edu/dictionary/ dictionary.php (accessed 27 May 2019). Watkins, Laurel J. 1984. A grammar of Kiowa. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Woodbury, Anthony. 2017. Polysynthesis in Central Alaskan Yup’ik. In Michael Fortescue, Marianne Mithun & Nicholas Evans (eds.), The Oxford handbook of polysynthesis, 536–559. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Andrew McKenzie

16 Switch-reference and event cohesion Abstract: This chapter considers some of the ways that grammars encode how speakers build discourse structures larger than sentences, focusing on switch-reference systems. Canonically these indicate that the clauses share subjects, but findings in many North American languages show that switch-reference indicates various kinds of event cohesion as well. The chapter also discusses different clause types that switch-reference can be found with and how those types affect the kinds of cohesion available. Finally, it compares switch-reference to a number of phenomena that have been linked to it over the years.

16.1 Introduction Most grammatical study emphasizes how clauses and sentences are built, but people do not generally utter sentences in isolation. Rather, we situate sentences as part of a larger conversation (or discourse) between speech participants. In the course of a conversation, we will link certain sentences together as involving the same theme, event, or topic— that is, how much cohesion they have. We also set other sentences as distinct from one another. Adverbials like then or while fulfill this purpose, but in many of the languages of the world, specific forms in the grammar provide speakers with the means to indicate cohesion across clauses. In the languages of North America, one very common form that marks cohesion is called switch-reference, which usually indicates that the two clauses have the same grammatical subject or different subjects. For instance, in Kiowa (Kiowa-Tanoan), the connective meaning ‘when’ has forms that indicate switch-reference (SR). The form in (1a) indicates that the clauses share the ‘same subject (SS)’, and the form in (1b) indicates they each have a ‘different subject (DS)’.1 (1)      

Kiowa (Christina Simmons, p.c.) a. tôy à=héːbà=tsę ː̀ èm=bǫ́ ː   house.in 1sg=enter.pfv=when.SS 1sg>2sg=see.pfv   ’When I went into the house, I saw you.’

1 Glossing conventions follow Leipzig rules, with the following additions and clarifications: !: surprise, A: agent, D: dative/applicative argument, decl: declarative mood, DS: different subject/situation, dur: durative, evid: indirect evidentiality, exp: personal experience evidential, h: h-grade aspect, INC: Incomplete aspect, indic: indicative mood, irr: irrealis mood, mv: middle voice, obl: oblique, OR: open reference, Pma: perfective, male addressee, prev: previously mentioned, ptcp: participle, S: subject, seq, sequential in time, sim: simultaneous, spec: specific, SS: same subject/situation, TEM: temporal pivot. Some glosses from various sources have been adjusted for clarity or to standardize abbreviations. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-016

364 

     

 Andrew McKenzie

b.    

tôy à=héːbà=ę ː̀ ę́=bǫ́ ː house.in 1sg=enter.pfv=when.DS 2sg>1sg=see.pfv ’When I went into the house, you saw me.’

SR markers are not usually translated, since English and other European languages lack them. However, their use is crucial for fluent conversation in the languages they appear in, so care must be taken to document and understand them.

16.2 Cohesion via role identity When thinking about cohesion, we want to keep in mind exactly what is being held as cohesive. It is typically held that a marker of cohesion actually indicates some kind of semantic identity across clauses. That is, the same particular role in each clause is filled by the same real-world object.2 A marker of non-cohesion actually indicates some kind of semantic non-identity across clauses; the same role is fulfilled by different real-world objects. Thus, in (2), the -ku suffix in Tümpisa Shoshone (Numic) indicates that the two clauses’ time arguments are filled by the same interval in time (sim), while -ka/ha indicates two distinct intervals (3): (2)        

Tümpisa Shoshone (Dayley 1989: 347–8) a. ümaku tammü kahni kuppa weekikkwantu’ih   rain-sim.DS we.incl house in enter.pl-going to   ‘When it rains, we’re going to go in the house.’ b. the time of it raining = the time of us going into the house

(3)        

 Tümpisa Shoshone (Dayley 1989: 347–8) a. Nümmü [tatsa naakkiha] nümmü supe toya mantu mi’a   we.excl we.incl get-seq.DS in there mountain to go   When [=after] it gets summer, we go there to the mountains.’ b. the time of it getting summer ≠ the time of us going to the mountains

When we consider cohesion across clauses in terms of identity of a particular role-filling item, we can try to organize these markers based on the roles, and we observe that usually, different kinds of semantic objects fill different roles.

2 If one is talking about unreal situations or hypotheticals, these ‘real-world’ objects might also be hypothetical.



Switch-reference and event cohesion 

 365

16.2.1 Subject identity A common form of cohesion across clauses involves subject identity. For instance, in some subordinate clauses in Chickasaw (Muskogean) the −t suffix indicates that the subject of the main clause is the same as the subject of the subordinate clause. The nasalvowel suffix −V indicates that the subjects are distinct. This bivalent marker is called switch-reference, and in (4) it is the only overt difference between the two sentences. (4)            

Chickasaw (Munro 2017: 122) a. Amposhi’ achifa-kmat kashooch-a’ni   dish wash-if:SS dry-can   ‘if she1 washes the dishes she1 can dry them’ b. Amposhi’ achifa-kma kashooch-a’ni   dish wash-if:DS dry-can   ‘if she1 washes the dishes she2/*1 can dry them’

Switch-reference was first named by Jacobsen (1967) when describing the Washo (Isolate) and Tonkawa (Isolate) languages, and it was swiftly found in dozens of other languages in North America and then around the world (Austin 1981; Haiman & Munro 1983; Roberts 1997; McKenzie 2015; van Gijn 2016). Many American languages allow arguments to be unexpressed; sentences might just consist of a verb. Because of this, switch-reference can tell listeners who is doing what when there are two people involved. For instance, the Crow (Siouan) sentence uá dappeék can mean that someone killed his wife, or that his wife killed someone. In a sentence with SR, this important distinction becomes clear. (5)            

Crow (Graczyk 2007: 417) a. bachée-sh húu-laa uá ∅-∅-dappeé-k   man-det come-SS his:wife 3O-3A-kill-decl   ‘The man came and killed his wife.’ b. bachée-sh húu-m uá ∅-∅-dappeé-k   man-det come-DS his:wife 3O-3A-kill-decl   ‘The man came and his wife killed him.’

This effect can be very helpful in a narration. In the following excerpt from a Kiowa story, seven consecutive sentences (b-g) start with an SR-marker. Each marker helps listeners understand the action, since the subject is not expressed verbally in any of them, and the overt nouns are not case-marked. (6)          

Kiowa (P. McKenzie n.d. “Poolant’s Killing”) a. pólą́ ːtè tsę̂ ː á=p’ɔ́y-hyèl … t’ɔ́khɔ́y+k’íː mɔ́n   Poolant horse 3sS›3sD=be lost-evid White+man probably á=pɔ ̀ːdôː 3sA›3sO›3sD=care for ‘Poolant had lost a horse … a White man must have been watching it for him.’

366 

                                        

 Andrew McKenzie

b.     c.     d.     e.  

f.     g.    

nɔ ̀ hɔ́ndéʔ+tʰàːlyìː ∅=pɔ ̀ː+kʰɔ̂ː+tòl-hèl and:DS some+boy 3sA›3sO=bring+get+send.pfv-evid ‘So he sent a boy to bring it back.’ (DS → Poolant) nɔ ̀ mɔ́n ∅=ɔ́ːgyá+tɔ ̀ː and:DS probably 3sS=refuse to relinquish+act.pfv ‘But he refused to give the horse up.’ (DS → the White man) nɔ ̀ hègɔ́ ɔ̨́ːg̣ɔ ̀ gyà=ây-hyèl and:DS then self 3sA›3pO=start.off-evid ‘So then he went [to get it] himself.’ (DS → Poolant) gɔ ̀ mɔ́n háyá t’ɔ́khɔ́y+k’íː mɔ́ː and:SS probably somehow White+man somewhat ∅=éppátéː+dɔ ̀ːpèː 3sA›3sO=with force+ask.pfv ‘He must have somehow tried to coerce the White man a bit. (SS → Poolant) nɔ ̀ hègɔ́ mɔ́n ∅=sɔ̨́ɔ̨ ːdè ̀ and:DS then probably 3sS=become angry.pfv ‘and then he got angry’ (DS → the White man) gìgɔ́ ∅=thɔ́ttè-hèl gɔ ̀ ∅=hól-hèl and:SS|then 3sA›3sO=shoot.pfv-evid and:SS 3sA›3sO=kill.pfv-evid ‘and shot him and killed him’ (SS → the White man)

Switch-reference is very common in North America and will form the focus of the rest of this chapter. We will see how it interacts with other expressions of cohesion, and how it often ignores subjects in favor of vaguer forms of cohesion.

16.2.2 Events and times Switch-reference in many languages can ignore subjects altogether, to mark a continuity between the events of the clause rather than the subject. This is sometimes called ‘non-canonical switch-reference.’ Watkins (1993) finds that in Kiowa, SS marking can be used with different subjects when the two events are linked. (7)          

Kiowa (Watkins 1993: 149) a. Kathryn gyà=gút gɔ ̀ Esther=àl K. 3sA›3pO=write.pfv and:SS E.=also   ‘Kathryn wrote a letter and Esther wrote one, too.’ b. Kathryn gyà=gút nɔ ̀ Esther=àl   K. 3sA›3pO=write.pfv and:DS E.=also   ‘Kathryn wrote a letter and Esther wrote one, too.’

gyà=gút 3sA›3pO=write.pfv gyà=gút 3sA›3pO=write.pfv

McKenzie (2012) investigates this link and reports that speakers like (7a) if for instance the letters are being talked about as parts of a campaign under discussion, like writing



Switch-reference and event cohesion 

 367

the governor to pardon a particular person. If there is no such campaign, or even if there is, but it is not significant to the discussion, then DS marking is chosen (7b). DS marking can also indicate that the events being described are distinct, even if the clauses’ subjects are identical. This is the case in Kiowa and also in Lakota. (8)      

Lakota (Siouan) (Dahlstrom 1982: 73) mazophiyeta wa’i yũkhã čhuwe wãblake store-to 1.go and:DS sister 1.see ‘I went to the store and I saw my sister.’

In the terms we use here, we can say that these cases involve ordinary switch-reference, but with an event or situation as the locus of cohesion instead of the subject. Events and situations are real-world objects that we can describe or refer to with anaphora. Events can be verb arguments, and they can be related to each other as identical or not. SS marking in cases like these indicates that the event (or situation) that the first clause describes is the same event as the event that the second clause describes. DS marking indicates that the clauses describe different events or situations that are not being joined in this sentence. In (7a), the speaker chooses the same event and applies the two sentences to it; the letter-writings are parts of that event. In (7b), the speaker chooses each letter-writing event separately. This choice is schematized in Figure 1. e₃ = e₁ + e₂ + … e₃ : the letter-writing campaign

e₁ : Kathryn writes a letter

e₂ : Esther writes a letter

e₃ – Kathryn wrote a letter and.SS e₃ – Esther wrote one, too

e₁ – Kathryn wrote a letter and.DS e₂ – Esther wrote one, too

Fig. 1: Speaker’s choice of event continuity and its effect on SR marking.

It can be hard for investigators to ‘see’ what the difference is between these choices, but native speakers clearly distinguish them. SS marking is used to track events in several ways in a number of other languages. One use is to ‘zoom in’ on a scene. In the Crow sentence in (9), a boy looks over at the campsite, and the events he sees are linked by SS. (9)          

Crow (Graczyk 2007: 415) chiláakshe shikáakee-sh asall-ák kuss-íkaa-lee-m ashé ah-ak morning boy-det go out.SS goal-look-!-DS lodge many-SS bilaxpáake chiwakálaa-(a)k dahkú-m people go back and forth-SS continue-DS ‘in the morning the boy went out, he looked in the direction of [the old campsite], and to his surprise, there were lots of lodges, and people going back and forth.’

368 

 Andrew McKenzie

Another event-oriented use of SR is described by Mithun (1993), who notes that the Central Pomo (Pomoan) sentence (10), uttered as part of a legal discussion, describes two clauses “packaged as a single event explaining the lack of documents, even though their subjects were different.” (10)          

Central Pomo (Mithun 1993: 132) mu:l ʔe khe pápil=ʔel s-ts’á-ba that copula 1.obl paper=the with.liquid-destroyed-and:SS ʔa: qów=mča-w=ʔkhe č’o-č=ya 1A out=throw-pfv=fut happen-semelfactive=exp ‘My papers got wet and I just had to throw them away’

čalél just

Mithun proposes that SR only marks event cohesion, and it just so happens that subject sharing is a key component of two events being cohesive enough for SS marking. In narratives, DS marking can also be used with event orientation, indicating the boundary between two episodes in a story, as exemplified in (11), from the Mandan (Siouan) language. DS links two clauses with the same subject but marks the story’s boundary between the landing and the traveling. Once the second part begins, SS marking links Coyote’s actions. (11)        

Mandan (Mixco 1997:248) kipxeak kirątɛrį kasi:wįowąkoʔš ki-pxe-ak ki-rątE-rį ka-si:-wį-o:wąk-oʔš mv-land-DS mv-get up-SS inceptive-travel-prog-npst-Pma ‘Coyote landed, got up, and started traveling.’

kirųwąʔkšis ki-rųwąʔk-ši-s ?-man-good-def

It can be difficult to determine what drives speakers to choose to describe one event or two when linking two clauses. Pustet (2013) quantitatively analyzes the use of SR in Lakota narratives, finding that subject identity, identity of the event’s time, contrast, and probability all play a role in choosing SS or DS. Mithun (2020) discusses similar factors playing roles in Pomoan languages, along with possible routes of the markers’ origins.

16.3 Additional morphology of switch-reference This section discusses some of the morphology of switch-reference in more detail, focusing on the general traits one can expect to find if it is present in a language.

16.3.1 Diagnosing switch-reference Switch-reference (SR) markers can be diagnosed as such if they have all of the following features, based on McKenzie (2015: 418).



Switch-reference and event cohesion 

 369

1. They occur at or near a clause juncture, including coordination or subordination. 2. They have two values in a complementary pair, such that the forms have identical meanings except for their SR value. 3. The SR value depends solely on the reference of the subject or prominent argument of each joined clause.3 The complementary forms are known as ‘same subject’ and ‘different subject’ forms, or SS and DS. Some of these pairs are shown in Table 1. Notably, the SS/DS abbreviations are used even when the prominent argument is not the subject. Tab. 1: Complementary pairs of SR markers Language

SS

DS

Choctaw (Muskogean | cho) Mojave (Yuman | mov) Crow Tohono O’odham (Piman | ood)

-t -k -laa c 

-V̨ (nasal vowel) -m -m ku

Not all SR morphemes have all three properties on this list. For instance, a handful of languages only have a DS form, like Washo -š. No language has been observed to have only SS forms. Some languages have what researchers call “open reference” (OR), where the SS form can only occur with same subjects, while an ‘open’ form can be used with same or different subjects. One such language is Northern Pomo (Pomoan), where –(e)n is SS (12a), but -da is open reference (12b-c). One might suspect this is actually a form of non-canonical or event-related SR; McKenzie’s (2015) survey lists these forms as ‘non-canonical’, requiring more investigation. (12)            

a.     b.    

Northern Pomo (O’Connor 1993: 231, 232) ša-nam maʔa-n man mo:wal baʔol-e fish-spec eat-SS 3s.fem 3s.masc call-pres ‘While she1 ate the fish she1/*2 called him.’ ša-nam maʔa-da man mo:wal baʔol-e fish-spec eat-DS 3s.fem 3s.masc call-pres ‘She1 vomited because she2/*1 ate the fish.’

3 The term ‘prominent argument’ is a bit vague because the mechanism of prominence varies across languages. In some languages the prominent argument is very clearly the grammatical subject of the verb. In others, it can be a topic (i.  e., an argument of a discourse), while in some (like Choctaw), a topical “broad subject” can be the focus of SR rather than the actual grammatical subject (Broadwell 2006). In languages where the event or situation argument is the focus of SR, that is the prominent argument.

370 

     

 Andrew McKenzie

c.    

tiyi ša-nam maʔa-kan logophor fish-det eat-because.OR ‘She1 vomited because she1 ate the fish.’

maːdal 3s.fem.pat

yat-ye vomit-past

Most SR morphemes do have all the features on this list. For cases where not all of them are met, if a morpheme has two out of three of the features, it is likely switch-reference. McKenzie (2015) discusses how some systems described with the term ‘switch-reference’ do not meet these structural criteria of a distinct morpheme, even though they do involve a mandatory switch in reference. For instance, Enrico uses ‘switch-reference’ to describe a Haida phenomenon where coreference depends on the presence or absence of a pronoun. Woodbury (1983) describes a “fourth-person” marking in Central Yup’ik that appears in instances of a third-person that is not co-referent to an anaphoric third person. Similar systems are found in other languages of the Eskimo-Aleut family (Pittman 2005, Berge 2011). However, they do not meet criterion 2, and their restriction to third person suggests rather that they are in the family of pronominal anaphors. They do feature in marking cohesion across clauses, however, and are discussed in Section 6. Many languages have multiple forms of SR markers, such as Kiowa, whose SR markers are fused with different connectives (Table 2). Each set of SR markers forms a complementary pair. The first two are sentential conjunctions, and the second two are adverbial subordinators. Tab. 2: Kiowa SR forms (Watkins 1984: 236)  

SS

DS

‘and’ ‘and’ (contrary to expectation) ‘when’ ‘if, as’

gɔ ̀ k’ɔ ̀t =tsę ː̀ =gɔ ̀

nɔ ̀ ɔ ̀t =ę ː̀ =nɔ ̀

16.3.2 Fusion with other morphemes SR markers in many languages fuse with another morpheme, creating a portmanteau. The SR markers thus seem to indicate two or more things at once. We saw this with Kiowa in the previous section, where the SR marker also indicates some sentential connection. As linguists we often will separate the SR meaning from the other meaning in our analysis, the way we often do with fused tense, aspect, or mood markers, because phenomena like temporal connectives or conjunctions operate independently from indications of reference. In Chemehuevi (Numic), the SR markers also indicate simultaneity or sequentiality. Between those two values, and the SS/DS pair, the language has four distinct markers of SR (Table 3).



Switch-reference and event cohesion 

 371

Tab. 3: Chemeheuvi SR markers (Press 1979:108)  

SS

DS

same event time (sim) different event time (seq)

-ga(i) -c(i)

-g(u) -k(a)

The Chemehuevi case is also interesting because it demonstrates how tricky it can be to rely on linguistic descriptions without further investigation. Linguists over the years have used the same terms in different ways, and that difference can cause confusion to a reader in our time. In the case of Chemehuevi, Press (1979) does not use the term ‘switch-reference’, but simply states that some adverbial clause markers depend on whether the subject is the same as that of the main clause. We also see that the marker appears at a clause boundary, occurs in a complementary pair, and indicates subject identity or non-identity. These are all signs of switch-reference, so we can consider it as SR, even if the author did not. When we consider the description of the time relation in the SR markers, we also must take care. Press (1979: 108) describes the same time (sim) SR markers as contemporaneous due to being durative, and different time (seq) markers as momentaneous (which leads to a sequential reading). However, Press then states that the ‘momentaneous’ markers “refer to antecedent actions”— things that had previously occurred. Press claims that these adverbial markers also indicate whether or not the clauses’ ‘tense’ matches, but this cannot be the case, as Press points out elsewhere that Chemehuevi subordinating clauses are not marked at all for tense and are interpreted with the tense of the main clause (13). The clauses therefore always have the same tense, so “different tense” marker could not be used here. We can see from the description that the marker indicates that the clauses’ event times are distinct. They are non-contemporaneous, as one occurs after the other is complete. (13)      

Chemehuevi (Press 1979: 108) Ann ijapaka-c tɨrawaiʔi-kwai-vɨ Ann be scared-SS.seq dash-away-past ‘Ann got scared and ran away.’

This discussion is not intended to cast doubt or aspersions on previous work, but to warn readers about encountering terminology. Modern usage of the term tense usually does not apply to the event time (when the action actually happens) but rather to the topic or reference time (the time frame the sentence is about). However, this usage did not become widespread until after much of the descriptive literature was already published, including the description of Chemehuevi. When examining a language’s reference materials, a reader should keep this kind of usage difference in mind.

372 

 Andrew McKenzie

16.3.3 Where switch-reference is found Switch-reference is found in languages in many parts of North America. McKenzie (2015) finds SR in nearly 70 languages and dialects in indigenous languages of the US and northern Mexico. Many language groups lack it altogether, notably the Algonquian, Iroquoian, and the Athapaskan groups. In many language groups, virtually every language has it, including the Numic, Yuman, and Muskogean groups. However, one of SR’s interesting features worldwide is that it spreads through contact from one language to its neighbors (Austin 1981). In North America, this kind of diffusion explains why many language families have one or a few members that use SR, while the rest do not. Among the Siouan languages, for instance, languages whose speakers were in contact with speakers of SR languages often came to have SR, including Crow in the North (near Numic communities) and Biloxi in the south (near Muskogean communities). However, those that were not in such contact never adopted it, like Osage or Kansa. Kiowa is an even more striking case, for it is the only Kiowa-Tanoan language with SR, having changed from its speakers’ close contact with the Crow and the Comanche, who both speak SR languages. Another consequence of areal diffusion is that we cannot assume that a language has SR forms or does not simply based on the languages related to it. On the other hand, the presence of SR markers can reveal interesting pieces of a language’s history (Mithun 2020).

16.4 Switch-reference with subordination Switch-reference frequently occurs with subordinate clauses. The SR marker appears at the end of the subordinate clause, usually attached to or fused with the subordinating connective. It connects the embedded clause to the main clause and relates its subject to the main clause subject: SS if the subjects are identical and DS if they are not. In this section’s examples, subordinate clauses are enclosed within [square brackets]. (14)      

Tümpisa Shoshone (Dayley 1989: 348) Puhakantün puuhawinna [ung kamma-ku] shaman cure you.acc be sick-when.DS ‘The shaman cures you when you’re sick’

If there are multiple subordinate clauses in a single sentence, each clause’s SR marker compares its subject to the main clause subject, not to that of any adjacent subordinate clause.



(15)      

Switch-reference and event cohesion 

Maricopa (Yuman) (Gordon 1983:93) [’iipaa-ny-sh nya-vaa-k]A [’ayuu ’rav-m]B man-det-subject when-come-SS something 1-rav-DS ‘When the man came, he helped me because I was sick.’

 373

ny-wik-k 3>1-help-aspect

16.4.1 Adjunct clauses Switch-reference is commonly found with adverbial clauses, subordinate clauses that modify the event of the main verb. These include when-clauses, if-clauses, and asclauses. (16)      

Mojave (Munro 1976: 44–5) nya-avač-ku:ʔe:-kum ahwer-k when-arrive.pl-poor.pl-DS fence in-SS ‘When the [poor] parents had a chance to come in, they [the Whites] had fenced the place off’

(17)      

Choctaw (Broadwell 2006:292) Tiballichi-li-kmã am-anooli-h err-1s-if:DS 1sD-tell-tense ‘If I make a mistake, tell me.’ Choctaw

(18)      

Tolkapaya Yavapai (Yuman) (Hardy & Gordon 1980:185) qwaloyawa ’-nwirk-k ’-wu-m [ssah-a ’um-t-m] chicken 1>3-cook-SS 1-do-inc spoil-irr neg-tem-DS ‘I cooked the chicken before it spoiled.’

16.4.2 Complement clauses Complement clauses are arguments to the verb, usually a verb expressing a mental state like think or know, or a verbal action like say or tell. SR can occur with complement clauses in some languages, though not very many. (19)      

Choctaw (Broadwell 2006: 271) Lynn-at ik-ikháan-o-h [iy-aachĩ-ka-t] Lynn-nom agr-know-neg-tense go-irr-comp-SS ‘Lynn does not know that she will go’

374 

 Andrew McKenzie

16.4.3 Relative clauses It is uncommon for relative clauses to bear SR marking, but it does occur. Choctaw and Chickasaw permit SR marking to be used in place of the case-marking that the relative clause normally bears. (20)      

Choctaw (Broadwell 2006:299) [Ofi’ ipiita-li-k-aash-mã] dog feed-1s-tense-prev-dem.DS ‘The dog that I fed ran away’

(21)      

Chickasaw (Gordon & Munro 2017:13) [Ofi’ ipita-li-kaash-oot] isso-li-tok dog feed-1s-prev-focus.SS hit-1s-past ‘I hit the dog I fed’

balii-t run-ptcp

kaniiya-h go away-tense

Washo employs DS marking with internally-headed relative clauses. (22)      

Washo (Peachey 2006:7) k’ák’aʔ dá: ∅-gé:gel-i-š-ge ∅-yá:m-aʔ heron there 3-sit-ipfv-DS-3O 3-speak to-aorist ‘She spoke to a heron who was sitting there.’

It is not yet known why SR appears so much more frequently with adverbial clauses than with complement or relative clauses, or why any particular language employs SR on one type but not another.

16.5 Switch-reference with other clause types Switch-reference is not restricted to the juncture of subordinate clauses to main clauses. It occurs in many languages in coordinating constructions, and its properties in those cases differ slightly from those of SR with subordination. It also occurs with clausechains, where its properties again differ slightly.

16.5.1 Sentential coordination A good number of languages use SR with sentential coordination, where two complete sentences are joined together. The SR marker is fused with the conjunction in such cases, like in Washo.



(23)      

Switch-reference and event cohesion 

 375

Washo (Jacobsen 1967) píteliʔ yát’umuwaʔ-aʔ ʔ-i-š-da géwe geʔišúwam-i-da lizard go down in-aorist X-ipfv-ds-loc Coyote pursue him-ipfv-ds ‘The lizard went in and then Coyote pursued him and.  .  .’

Coordination contrasts with subordination in several ways. With coordination, the SR markers appear at the beginning of the clause (clause-initial) attached to the conjunction, while with subordination they appear at the end of the clause (clause-final) near the subordinating morpheme. The subject of a coordinated SR clause is compared with the previous clause that it is coordinated with, and there is no skipping of main clauses (see (6)). Also, coordination allows for non-canonical SR in many languages (7a), but subordinating SR is strictly canonical.

16.5.2 Clause-chaining Many languages use switch-reference with clause chains. Clause-chaining is a type of clause linking where a series of incomplete ‘medial’ clauses are chained one after another and capped off by a main ‘final’ clause. (24)      

Central Pomo (Mithun 1993:121) ʔa: čáw=yó-ba máti ʔ-čhá:-č-ba maʔá 1A in=go-SS down by gravity-sit-inch-SS food ‘I came into the house, (I) sat down, and (I) started to eat.’

qa:-yúʔč’i-w biting-begin-pfv

Clause-chains skirt the line between subordination and coordination. Clause-chaining behaves like subordination in many ways. A chained clause’s tense and aspect are determined by the main clause. In a sentence with a long chain of clauses, all of their tense and aspect values depend on those of the main clause. However, chained clauses also behave like coordination in many ways. For instance, anaphora proceeds in a linear fashion, rather than from main clause to subordinate. Since their clause structure is distinct from coordination and subordination, chained structures are translated into English as coordination, subordination, or other structures altogether, as we can see in the sequences in (25), in the unrelated languages Jamul Tiipay (Yuman) and Creek (Muskogean). (25)            

a.

b.

Jamul Tiipay (Yuman) (Miller 2001:240) servees me-si-x-pu m-aa-chm uuyaaw beer 2-drink-irr-dem 2-go-DS know ‘I know you went there to drink beer’ w-amp-ch ’al’al-ch w-aam-s 3-walk-SS wobble-SS 3-go away-emphatic ‘He staggered away’

376 

             

 Andrew McKenzie

c.

d.      

chaw-k uuyaaw-x fix-irr.SS know-irr ‘I will find out how to fix it (or, I will know how and I will fix it)’ Creek (Muskogean) (Martin 2011:315) ísti hamk-ín faccí:ca-n háhy-i:-t person one-DS judge-acc make.h-agr-SS ‘We’ll make one person the judge…’

With SR, clause-chains skirt the line as well. An SR marker appears at the end of the chained clause, as it would in a subordinate clause. However, SR’s meaning behaves likes it does with coordination, in that it compares the medial clause to the next one, whether it is another chained clause or the main clause. Also, SR can be non-canonical with clause-chains, like it can with coordination.

16.5.3 Pro-verbs Another structure that carries SR is the pro-verb. A pro-verb is a verb linking two sentences together, and which stands in place of the main verb of the first of the two sentences. The Tonkawa example in (26) demonstrates this effect. The two sentences are linked by the verb translated as do so, which bears the SR marker. (26)          

Tonkawa (Hoijer 1949: 43) hostaxso:n xilipa:nanoklaknoʔo ha:ʔako:nwa:ʔa:la. in the morning he always went out hunting.evid that man ʔe:lʔila ʔaweykak kwa:lowkak ya:lo:nanklaknoʔo he so doing.SS many deer big ones he always killed them.evid ‘That man went out hunting every morning, it is said. So doing, he always killed many big deer, it is said.’

Muskogean languages routinely use pro-verbs in discourse. For instance, the Creek verb mom ‘be so’ is used to link two sentences, often in narratives. (27)                      

a.       b.     c.  



Creek (Martin 2011: 354–355) isti hámk-it inókk-i:-t wâ:kk-ati:-s person one-nom sick-dur-SS lie.sg-histpst-ind ‘A man lay sick.’ mo:m-it i-heywa ó:c-i:-t ô:m-ati:-s be so-SS one-nom exist-dur-SS be.sg-histpst-ind ‘And he had a wife.’ mo:m-it hatâm ifá-n sólk-i:-n ó:c-i:-t be so-SS again dog-acc many-dur-DS exist-dur-SS ô:m-ati:-s be.sg-histpst-ind ‘And he also had many dogs’



Switch-reference and event cohesion 

 377

Pro-verbs are used in languages that do not have SR on coordinating conjunctions. SR on coordinating conjunctions occurs at the start of its sentence, allowing a link to previous sentences. SR on subordinate or chained clauses is clause-final, so that link is not really feasible. It is thus likely that the use of pro-verbs offers a strategy that allows a switch-reference effect between main clauses in languages whose grammar only allows it between a main and subordinate or chained clause.

16.6 Other structures This section discusses a few other methods of indicating coherence across sentences. These methods are sometimes linked to switch-reference, and sometimes described as switch-reference, but they do not meet the diagnostic criteria for it.

16.6.1 Obviation Obviation is a distinct system of grammar that is widely found in some regions and language families, marking arguments based on their relative discourse prominence. Obviation has been linked to switch-reference on multiple occasions (Hale 1992; Wichmann 2007; Muehlbauer 2012), because it indicates a kind of cohesion throughout a discourse. However, obviation is a distinct system with several differences from SR, with its own mechanisms. These differences are summarized in Table 4. More obviously, there exist languages with both obviation and switch-reference, like Hopi (Uto-Aztecan) (Hale 1992). Tab. 4: Comparison of switch-reference and obviation property

SR

obviation

1. at clause juncture 2. occurs in complementary pair 3. depends on reference or cohesion marked on nouns restricted to 3rd person interacts with agreement depends on discourse prominence marked on conjunctions or subordinating connectives

yes yes yes no no no no yes

no yes no yes yes yes yes no

While obviation and switch-reference can co-occur, such co-occurrence might be a rare accident. The language groups most widely known for obviation are Algonquian (see Oxford, this volume) and Athapaskan (see Saxon, this volume), and they all lack

378 

 Andrew McKenzie

switch-reference, with the likely exception of Arapaho (Algonquian) (Cowell & Moss 2008), whose speakers spent many years interacting with speakers of SR-languages on the Northern Plains.

16.6.2 Special kinds of reflexives In the Eskimo-Aleut languages, some person suffixes on embedded verbs and possessors vary depending on whether they co-refer with the main clause subject. (28)      

Central Yup’ik (Woodbury 1983: 296) wangkugneng-tawaam tangvakuneng aavurciiqut us.du-however if they (3.refl) see they will be amused ‘If they see the two of us, they will be amused’

(29)      

West Greenlandic (Fortescue 1991: 53) nulia-ni takuaa wife-3.refl see-3sg.agt/3s.obj.indic ‘hei saw hisi wife’

This marking is sometimes described as “fourth person” or as “switch-reference” (Woodbury 1983, Fortescue 1991; Pittman 2005; Berge 2011), although it differs in crucial ways from switch-reference morphology (Table 5). Tab. 5: Comparison of switch-reference with Central Yup’ik special reflexives property

SR

special reflexive

1. at clause juncture 2. occurs in complementary pair 3. depends on reference or cohesion marked on nouns restricted to 3rd person interacts with agreement marked on conjunctions or subordinating connectives

yes yes yes no no no yes

no no yes yes yes yes no

This form also appears on objects and can be considered a “special reflexive,” more like long-distance anaphors (cf. Bok-Bennema 1991) than switch-reference. Berge (2011) shows that these anaphors can refer back to a discourse topic rather than the main subject (30–31), and that is also a feature of long-distance anaphors cross-linguistically (Kitagawa 1981, Xu 1994). In (30–31), the previously established topic is cigarettes, and that is what the co-referent marker (3coref) points back to, rather than the 1st singular subject of the previous clause.



Switch-reference and event cohesion 

 379

(30)        

West Greenlandic (Berge 2011: 202–3) 1987-arsimi tassanngaannarsuaq pujortarunnaarama 1987-ars-mi tassannga-innaq-suaq pujortaq-junnaaq-gama 1987-years-loc from.then.on-only-big smoke-no.more-1sg.causative ‘in 1987 from then on I stopped smoking [I no longer smoked]’

(31)        

West Greenlandic (Berge 2011: 203) uanga cigaritsip ajuleramimga uanga cigaritsi-p ajor-leq-gaminga 1sg cigarette-relative be.bad-begin-3coref.sg.subj/1sg.obj.causative ‘cigarettes didn’t like me anymore [began to be bad for me]’

At a deep level of the grammar, it is likely these special reflexives emerge from the same sorts of syntactic phenomena that switch-reference emerges from (Baker & Camargo Souza 2019). However, in a descriptive sense this is a distinct phenomenon of clause cohesion.

16.7. Conclusion This chapter has introduced the concept of switch-reference and discussed how it expresses different kinds of cohesion between clauses. The most common way is marking whether two clauses’ subjects are identical, but it can also mark identity of events or situations from one sentence to another. The chapter also showed the variety of ways that switch-reference can appear in different North American languages. Switch-reference’s meaning, function, and frequency all contribute to its importance in discourses like conversations and narratives. This importance makes SR vital to understand for communities interested in maintaining and revitalizing their languages. SR is a key component of putting sentences together, and thus a key component of teaching learners how to build conversations. It is difficult to translate, though, and even including its meaning in a translation can be very awkward. Due to this difficulty, teachers cannot rely on comparison to the structures of more familiar languages when discussing SR. It has to be described on its own terms by people familiar with how it works, and it is hoped that this chapter can help people gain that familiarity.

References Austin, Peter. 1981. Switch-Reference in Australia. Language 57(2). 309–334. Baker, Mark C. & Livia Camargo Souza. 2019. Switch-Reference in American Languages: A Synthetic Overview. In Daniel Siddiqi, Michael Barrie, Carrie Gillon, Jason Haugen & Eric Mathieu (ed.), The Routledge Handbook of North American Languages, 210–232. London: Taylor and Francis.

380 

 Andrew McKenzie

Berge, Anna. 2011. Topic and Discourse Structures in West Greenlandic Agreement Constructions. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press. Bok-Bennema, Reineke. 1991. Case and Agreement in Inuit. Berlin: Foris Publications. Broadwell, George Aaron. 2006. A Choctaw Reference Grammar. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press. Cowell, Andrew & Alonzo Moss, Sr. 2008. The Arapaho Language. Boulder, CO: University Press of Colorado. Dahlstrom, Amy. 1982. A functional analysis of switch-reference in Lakhota. In Kevin Tuite, Robinson Schneider & Robert A. Chametzky (eds.), Chicago Linguistics Society (CLS) 18. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Dayley, Jon. 1989. Tümpisa (Panamint) Shoshone grammar. University of California Publications in Linguistics Vol. 115. Berkeley, California: University of California Press. Fortescue, Michael. 1991. Switch reference anomalies and ‘topic’ in West Greenlandic: A case of pragmatics over syntax. In Jef Verschueren (ed.), Levels of Linguistic Adaptation, 53–80. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Gijn, Rijk van. 2016. Switch-reference in South America. In Rik van Gijn (ed.), Switch-Reference 2.0. (Typological Studies in Language 114). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins. Gordon, Lynn. 1983. Switch-reference, clause order, and interclausal relationships in Maricopa. In John Haiman & Pamela Munro (eds.), Switch-reference in Universal Grammar, 83–104. Philadelphia: John Benjamins. Gordon, Lynn & Pamela Munro. 2017. Relative clauses in Western Muskogean languages. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics 2(1). 30. Graczyk, Randolph. 2007. A Grammar of Crow. Lincoln, NE: U of Nebraska Press. Haiman, John & Pamela Munro (eds.). 1983. Switch-reference and Universal Grammar. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Hale, Ken. 1992. Subject obviation, switch-reference, and control. In Richard K. Larson, Sabine Iatridou, Uptal Lahiri & James Higginbotham (eds.), Control and Grammar. Studies in Linguistics and Philosophy, 51–77. Dordrecht: Kluwer. Hardy, Heather K. & Lynn Gordon. 1980. Types of adverbial and modal constructions in Tolkapaya. International Journal of American Linguistics 46(3). 183–196. Hoijer, Harry. 1949. Tonkawa syntactic suffixes and anaphoric particles. Southwestern Journal of Anthropology 5(1). 37–55. Jacobsen, William H., Jr. 1967. Switch-Reference in Hokan-Coahuiltecan. In Dell Hymes & William Bittle (eds.), Studies in Southwestern Linguistics, 238–263. The Hague: Mouton. Kitagawa, Chisato. 1981. Anaphora in Japanese: Kare and Zibun. In Ann Farmer & Chisato Kitagawa (eds.), Coyote Papers 2: Proceedings of the Arizona Conference on Japanese, 61–75. Tucson, AZ: University of Arizona Linguistics Circle. Martin, Jack. 2011. A Grammar of Creek (Muskogee). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press. McKenzie, Andrew. 2012. The role of contextual restriction in reference-tracking. Amherst, MA.: University of Massachusetts Amherst dissertation. McKenzie, Andrew. 2015. A survey of switch-reference in North America. International Journal of American Linguistics 81(3). 409–448. McKenzie, Parker. n.d. Account of Poolant’s killing. Recorded by Laurel Watkins. Oklahoma Historical Society Research Center. Miller, Amy. 2001. A Grammar of Jamul Tiipay. The Hague: Mouton de Gruyter. Mithun, Marianne. 1993. Switch-reference: Clause combining in Central Pomo. International Journal of American Linguistics 59(2). 119–136. Mithun, Marianne. 2020. Inside contact-stimulated grammatical development. In Patience Epps, Danny Law & Na’ama Pat-El (eds.), Historical Linguistics and Endangered Languages: Exploring Diversity in Language Change. Routledge Series in Historical Linguistics. New York: Routledge.



Switch-reference and event cohesion 

 381

Mixco, Maurício. 1997. Mandan Switch Reference: A Preliminary View. Anthropological Linguistics 39(2). 220–298. Muehlbauer, Jeffrey. 2012. The relation of switch-reference, animacy, and obviation in Plains Cree. International Journal of American Linguistics 78(2). 203–238. Munro, Pamela. 1976. Two Stories by Nellie Brown (Mojave). In Yuman Texts, Margaret Langdon ed. Supplement to the International Journal of American Linguistics 1:3. University of Chicago Press. p. 43–50. Munro, Pamela. 2017. Chickasaw quantifiers. In Denis Paperno & Edward L. Keenan (eds.), Handbook of Quantifiers in Natural Language. Vol. II, 113–201. Amsterdam: Springer Netherlands. O’Connor, Mary Catherine. 1993. Disjoint reference and pragmatic inference: Anaphora and switch reference in Northern Pomo. In William A. Foley (ed.), The role of theory in language description, 215–242. (Trends in Linguistics, Studies and Monographs 69). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Peachey, Robert M. 2006. On switch-reference and the Internally-Headed Relative Clause construction in Washo. Unpublished manuscript. Press, Margaret L. 1979. Chemehuevi: A Grammar and Lexicon. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Pustet, Regina. 2013. Switch-reference or coordination? A quantitative approach to clause linkage in Lakota. International Journal of American Linguistics 79(2). 153–188. Roberts, John. 1997. Switch-Reference in Papua New Guinea. In Andrew Pauley (ed.), Papers in Papuan Linguistics No. 3, 101–241. Canberra, ACT, Australia: Australian National University. van Gijn, Rik. see Gijn, Rijk van. Watkins, Laurel. 1984. A Grammar of Kiowa. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press. Watkins, Laurel. 1993. The Discourse Function of Kiowa Switch-Reference. International Journal of American Linguistics 59(2). 137–164. Wichmann, Søren. 2007. The reference-tracking system of Tlapanec: Between obviation and switchreference. Studies in Language 31(1). 801–827. doi: 10.1075/sl.31.4.04wic. Woodbury, Anthony C. 1983. Switch reference, syntactic organization, and rhetorical structure in Central Yup’ik Eskimo. In John Haiman & Pamela Munro (eds.), Switch-reference in Universal Grammar, 291–315. Philadelphia: John Benjamins. Xu, Liejiong. 1994. The antecedent of ziji. Journal of Chinese Linguistics 22(1). 115–137.

IV Discourse

Anthony K. Webster

17 Verbal art Abstract: This chapter outlines some of the important linguistic and cultural issues concerning Native American verbal art. While not attempting to be exhaustive, this chapter does attempt to highlight a variety of verbal artistic traditions. After providing a definition of verbal art and discussing some of the ethical issues concerning documenting verbal art, the chapter turns to an overview of various topics related to verbal art. Topics include the importance of performance for thinking about verbal art (1), the question of locally understood genres (2), poetic structuring (3), the ways speech play provides key poetic devices (4), and the evaluative criteria by which verbal art is interpreted (5). Particular focus is placed on concerns with punning, ideophony, poetry, song, narratives, placenames, and parallelism (of various kinds). At the end of the chapter is an extended bibliography for further readings.

Introduction Native American verbal art covers a diverse range of discursive practices. This chapter outlines a number of key aspects of such practices. It does not pretend to be inclusive of the full range of that diversity. As a useful heuristic, Sherzer and Woodbury (1987: 8) define verbal art this way: “Verbal art” is a community’s own conception of what in language use is aesthetically or rhetorically pleasing, the forms and processes that members of the community label or otherwise demonstrate they consider to be verbally artistic.

To add to this and to sharpen our focus a bit, it is well to keep in mind that verbal art as a cover term is a bit misleading. First, verbal art is often multimodal, including gesture (Wiget 1987) or sign languages such as Plains Indian Sign Language (Farnell 2002) or various inscriptive practices such as O’odham calendar sticks (see Darling and Lewis 2007; Schermerhorn 2019). Second, verbal art should not be an a priori assumption. Rather, what is and is not considered verbal art according to local aesthetic standards needs to be investigated and not imputed. Third, any Western derived distinction between “art” and other modes of action—ritual or otherwise—needs to be dispensed with. Native American verbal art can do multiple things simultaneously and local theories of language functions need to be attended to. It is simply not the case for many Native American aesthetic traditions to talk of “art for art’s sake”—rather verbal art is often used to restore the world, cure the sick, or otherwise affect changes in the world. Such a recognition leads, then, to a realization that there may be limits on the kinds of verbal art that can be documented or shared. Among the Zuni (ISO zun), for example, https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-017

386 

 Anthony K. Webster

there is a distinction (B. Tedlock 1995) between tso’ya and attsani (the beautiful and the dangerous), where beautiful things—including certain genres of verbal art—can be shared and dangerous things should not be shared. Some genres are public, other genres private. Likewise, certain genres may be off limits to certain people based on gender, as is the case among the Mvskoke (ISO mus) (Innes 2010) or based on community status (whether or not one is initiated into certain ritual societies), as described for the Tewa of Arizona (ISO tew) (Kroskrity 2012). Even genres, like Navajo Coyote stories, that have been much documented, may need to be returned and destroyed after the death of the narrator (Toelken 1998). Such considerations should remind us of the social nature of verbal art and, as well, of our ethical obligations towards such verbal art and the people who create such verbally artistic discourse. Much of this research has been done under the name of ethnopoetics (Hymes 1981), for purposes here we can think of the ethno- as highlighting the need for ethnography (that is, talking with people about their verbal art) and the -poetics as indicating some attention to the literary devices used in verbal art (Webster 2020). In what follows I will discuss the issue of performance (1), then the question of locally understood genres (2), then I will discuss poetic structuring (3), I turn after that to a discussion of various poetic devices (4), and I conclude by discussing the evaluative criteria by which verbal art is interpreted (5).

17.1 Performance Performance is a key concept in thinking about Native American verbal art because it challenges us to think beyond a text-centered view of verbal art. Rather, it forces researchers to confront the situated nature of the doing of verbal art (Bauman 1984). Such performances are emergent as well, sensitive to and creative of context (Bauman 1984). Narrators may adjust the narrative according to who is present or how the audience responds. Verbal art becomes, then, less about the product, but rather the process, the doing-things with languages and gestures (and whatever else may be used) in the sphere of meaningful behavior. Textualized versions, that is versions written down, of verbal art often obscure the performance features of loudness, prosody, pacing, accompanying gestures, and the like (see Section 3.2). Verbal art is enacted. In many of the early texts collected by the Boasians, the audience was often the narrator and the anthropologist or linguist, sometimes as well an interpreter.1 Contrary to what might be

1 Briefly, the “Boasians” refers to anthropologists and linguists trained by the anthropologist Franz Boas and later his students in the early twentieth century. Documenting Indigenous languages was a cornerstone of the early tradition of Boasian anthropology. Such work was done before the wide-spread use of sound recording devices and so the forms of verbal art were often taken down through the slow process of elicitation. Here the anthropologist and/or linguist (the distinction was not so clear at that



Verbal art 

 387

assumed, such texts often reveal the ways that performers used such interactions for complex, often locally meaningful, forms of social action. Verbal art is here in the service of reflexive commentary on the interactional moment—that is, it is a way for the narrator to comment on the participants involved (including the anthropologist). Darnell (1989) provides a useful analysis of the various contextual issues that informed—and constrained in certain respects—the performance of a Cree narrative (ISO cre). Wiget (1987) provides a detailed analysis of a video-recorded Hopi Coyote narrative told by Helen Sekaquaptewa (ISO hop). Having said this, it is also the case that some genres (discussed below) are more or less open to creative adaptations. Some genres, ideally, are to be performed verbatim—that is, they are assumed to be verbatim reproductions of prior performances and are evaluated accordingly.

17.2 Genres It is sometimes assumed that verbal art must be long and elaborate. But the question of what constitutes verbal art in a particular community is, in fact, an empirical question. It is a question of genre. Some genres are rather lengthy, taking hours to complete. Other genres, the skillful deployment of a place-name for example, may be rather fleeting. So too the use of puns—a form of speech play that can be mobilized in the service of verbal art—can be fleeting. Some genres are meant to be repeated verbatim. Certain songs, for example, tend to be relatively stable over time, even if the ordering of songs may differ from performance to performance (Bahr 1986; Bahr et al 1997, Loether 1993). Other genres are understood to be more ephemeral. An assumption inherited from the Boasian tradition saw verbal art as residing in some static whole text, the complete narrative. But verbal art need not be a single “complete” narrative. Scollon (2009: 261) reports that, “within the Athabaskan storytelling tradition one doesn’t waste words or insult one’s listener by telling somebody something he or she already knows. A truly knowledgeable person really only requires an allusion to the story.” Such skillful allusions, then, constitute a site of verbal artistry. Likewise, Kendall (1979: 146) notes that for Yavapai and Hualapai (ISO yuf), Coyote tales—a particular genre that involves the trickster figure Coyote—were meant to be elliptical—the idea was that people knew the stories and could fill in various parts. Such expectations about the shape and form of various narratives need to be investigated.

time) wrote the narrative in a phonetic system (later a phonemic system) while the Indigenous narrator told the narrative (often slowly and often repeatedly). Given the emphasis of the Boasians on using the texts to create a grammar and dictionary, careful attention to the verbal artistry of the narratives was often not done (see Epps, Webster, Woodbury 2017 for a history of this tradition). The narratives were almost always treated as prose. Songs were treated as poetry. Ethnopoetic work, to be discussed below, has suggested that such narratives are better represented as a series of lines and verses and not as prose.

388 

 Anthony K. Webster

Some genres may have recognizable names—that is to say, they are locally categorized as distinct genres. Various Native communities differ in the elaboration of such named genres. For example, Shaul (2002: 6) gives the following list of named genres among the Hopi (see also Sekaquaptewa, Hill, and Washburn 2015): (1)  

Hopi (Shaul 2002: 6) tuuwutsi (stories) (a subcategory istutuwutsi ‘coyote stories’)

     

tsa’alawu (announcements)   wimtsa’alawu (ritual announcements)   qa wimtsa’alawu (secular announcements)

     

unangwvàasi (prayers) wimlavayi (ritual speech) lavay’oyi (speech)

           

taawi (song)   puwati/puwvitstawi/titaptawi (lullabies)   tsakotawi (children’s game songs)   tuwutstawi (story songs)   sosokwtawi (gambling songs)   ngumantawi (grinding songs)

   

yewatawi (songpoems)   katsintawi (kachina songs)

Among Zuni, narratives fall into two broad categories: chimiky’ana’kowa (origin stories and considered true) and telepnaawe (tales and considered fiction) (Tedlock 1983: 159– 160). Cup’ik (ISO esu) narrative traditions are also divided into two broad categories: Qanemcit ‘narratives’ and Qulirat ‘tales’. Qanemcit are understood to have originated with a particular person, often based on real people; whereas Qulirat are said to have originated with remote ancestors (this seems their defining feature) (Woodbury 1984: 13). Genres may often be demarcated by formal styles, a co-occurrence, that is, among various linguistic features (formality being a local categorization). Consider, for example, the Seneca (ISO see) ways of speaking described by Chafe (1993). Chafe highlights the ways Seneca speaking styles diverge along four dimensions (prosody, formulaicness, sentence structuring, and source of knowledge), as seen in Table 1: Tab. 1: Dimensions of Seneca Styles of Speaking (adapted from Chafe 1993: 86)  

Conversation

Good Message

Thanksgiving Speech

Prosody Formulaicity Sentences Epistemology

Free Low Fragmented Uncertain

Somewhat stylized Moderate Somewhat integrated Unexpressed

Highly stylized High Highly integrated Certain



Verbal art 

 389

In Seneca, then, the Thanksgiving Speech is marked by way of a particular formal style that includes highly stylized prosody, formulaic utterances, a highly integrated sentence structure, and explicit use of epistemic particles such as wai(h) (‘indeed’, ‘for sure’) and verbs such as tkaye:i’ (‘it is a fact’) (Chafe 1993: 86). Genres are often recognized by way of opening and closing formulas (Zuni, Eastern Pomo). In Kumeyaay, spoken in southern California and Baja California (ISO dih), for example, trickster tales begin with the following formulaic opening: (2)          

Kumeyaay (Field and Cuero 2012: 325) Ke’nápa nyuuchs It’s an old story. Nyuuch yúsa. It’s old. Nyuu, It’s old, nyuu yus ’i mat. it’s old, I say. Ke’nápa nyuuch nyáasa: It’s an old story I am telling you.

In Zuni telapnanne, they often begin with the formulaic opening so’nahchi said loudly and concludes with the formulaic closing lee semkonikya (Tedlock 1983: 160–161). In both cases, neither word is meant to be understood referentially or semantically; rather it indexes that a telapnanne is about to begin or to end. It creates, then, a narrative frame. Inside the frame, one is to understand what is being said as a part of the genre telapnanne. Genres can also be recognized by the use of genre signature devices (particular linguistic forms that must be present often throughout a particular genre) (Shaul 2002: 7). In both Hopi (a Uto-Aztecan language) and Tewa of Arizona (a Kiowa-Tonoan language), traditional narratives employ an evidential particle (respectively yaw and ba) which occurs in almost every clause of a narrative outside of quoted speech. Here is an example of the use of ba ‘so they say’. I follow Kroskrity’s (2012: 162–163) formatting with the one exception that I have bolded ba when it occurs: (3)      

Tewa of Arizona (Kiowa-Tanoan) (Kroskrity 2012: 162–163) Kídí di-da-kelen ‘haedi ba And then, after they got quite strong ‘a:khon-ge-pe’e ba over to the plain, so ‘óóbé-khwóóli-n-di im-bi yiyá-‘in-di. they were flown by their mothers.

When we think of verbal art as performance we realize, as well, that non-linguistic features can also signal genres. For example, some narrative traditions could only be told at night and in the winter—such was the case for Coyote stories among the Navajo (Toelken and Scott 1981). Recalling the two broad genres among the Zuni, chimiky’ana’kowa can be told at all times of the year, while telepnaawe should be told only at night and during the winter (Tedlock 1983: 160).

390 

 Anthony K. Webster

17.2.1 Names In some Native communities, place-names and/or personal names can be considered forms of verbal art. Among the Western Apache (ISO apw), there is a genre of verbal art called yałti’ bee’ízhí (speaking with names) (Basso 1996: 80). When this genre of verbal art occurs, place-names are “fixed up” (náyidlé). That is to say, they occur both in their long form and with an emphatic enclitic (=né ‘at this very place’). Thus, T’iis Tl’áh ‘Olį í ̨ ’́ (Water Flows Inward Under a Cottonwood Tree) becomes, in this genre, T’iis Bitł’ah Tú ‘Olį ń é (Basso 1996: 90). Here is an example of “speaking with names” from Basso (1996: 79). While place-names are considered aesthetically pleasing ways of speaking, the genre is also used for interpersonal and moral instruction, and revolves around the uttering of the fixed-up place-names. In this case, two Apache women (Lola and Emily) are attempting to comfort another Apache woman (Louise) (I have followed the formatting conventions of Basso 1996: 79). Notice that in the examples below, the enclitic =né is variously realized as =yé (in Lola’s first turn and Emily’s reply) and then as =né (in Lola’s second turn) (I have bolded the enclitic). (4)          

Apache (Basso 1996: 79) Lola: Tséé Hadigaiyé yú ‘ágodzaa. (It happened at Line of White Rocks Extends Up And Out, at this very place!) [Pause: 30–45 seconds] Emily: Ha’aa. Túzhį’ Yaahigaiyé yú ‘ágodzaa. (Yes. It happened at Whiteness Spreads Out Descending To Water, at this very place!) [Pause: 30–45 seconds] Lola: Da’aníí. K’is Deeschii’ Naaditiné yú ‘ágodzaa. (Truly. It happened at Trail Extends Across A Red Ridge With Alder Trees, at this very place!)

While in “Speaking with Names,” a single place name may be uttered, its force comes from being associated with ‘ágodzaahí (historical tales) (Basso 1996: 49). Indeed, one key genre feature of Western Apache narratives is that they begin at named places (Basso 1996). Such uses of place-names to anchor narratives is quite common and can also be found among the Eastern Pomo (ISO peb) (McClendon 1977) and the Zuni (Tedlock 1983), for example. In each case, the use of the place-names grounds the narrative in a particular named and knowable world. Personal names too can be forms of verbal art. Whiteley (1998) describes the way Hopi names are forms of literature. They are, according to Whiteley (1998: 107), “individually authored poetic compositions that comprise a literary genre.” Northern Yuman (ISO yuf) personal names can also be understood as compressed texts, in need of interpretation and exegesis (Kendall 1980).



Verbal art 

 391

17.2.2 Puns While puns are not often considered in the Western tradition as forms of verbal art, in many Native communities, puns are a highly valued aesthetic practice. They can either stand alone, as fleeting puns in conversations, or they can be used in other genres (from narratives to emceeing at Powwows to rituals). This is yet another place where speech play and verbal art come together. Puns can be of two sorts. The first sort are what we can call intralingual puns (these are puns within a language): (5)    

Seneca (Chafe 1998: 187) agwásgane:s ‘we wish’ agwás gane:s ‘very long penis’

Note the way two words can be punned with one single word. The second sort of puns we can call interlingual puns (these are puns that work because they cross languages). The following example is a rather old pun on the Navajo Nation. (6)    

Navajo (ISO nav) (Webster 2010: 291) television ‘television’ télii alizhgo ‘urinating donkey’

Gelo (1999) and Palmer (2003) report the Kiowa (ISO kio) and English interlingual pun of bót ‘cow innards’ with ‘boat’ leading to such puns as love bót (after the television show The Love Boat). Puns can both stand alone as forms of verbal art or can occur in a variety of verbally artistic genres (see below).

17.2.3 Songs Among the more studied aspects of Native American verbal art are the song traditions. Loether (1993) notes that, for Western Mono (ISO mnr), songs are one place where Indigenous languages may continue after their use in day-to-day interactions has ceased. Important linguistic analysis of specific song traditions can be found, for example, in Hinton (1984) on Havasupai (ISO yuf) songs and in Enrico and Stuart (1996) on Northern Haida (ISO hdn) songs. Hinton (1980) and Frisbie (1980) have discussed the role of vocables—semantically meaningless syllables—as structuring devices in the song traditions of Havasupai and Navajo, respectively (see also Hymes 1981). The early ethnomusicological work on Navajo song traditions by McAllester (1954, 1980a) highlighted a crucial aesthetic sensibility: namely, that for many Navajos, “beauty is that which does something” (McAllester 1954: 72). McAllester (1980a) outlines a number of the poetic devices found in Navajo songs, including repetition, interruption, alternation, ambiguity, imitation, identification, and continuation. Both Hinton (1990) for Havasupai and Fitzgerald (1998) for Tohono O’odham (ISO ood) have shown that meter can be found in Native North American verbally artistic traditions.

392 

 Anthony K. Webster

A number of song traditions use a special song language (see Bahr 1984). Such song languages may manipulate everyday words by adding syllables or they may reduce grammatical details (deleting grammatical affixes or particles, for example). Shoshoni (ISO shh) song traditions often omit the subject, leaving the line ambiguous, and will use obscure or obsolete words as well (Crum, Crum, and Dayley 2001: 19–20). To give a sense of the changes involved in song languages, below are the first lines of the First Snake Song as sung by Ray Winnie in 1957. The song language is first, then everyday Navajo, and then an English translation (following McAllester’s 1980 presentation). Note the addition of syllables like -ye, -yoya, -yeye, and the reduction of long vowels to short vowels. Note as well the use of vocables at the end of the song lyrics for the first verse. (7)      

Navajo (McAllester’s 1980: 3) K’a dine dighini-ye, k’a K’ad dinééh dighiní k’ad Now man holy now

     

Tł’iyitso Tł’iistsoh Snake big

yołtełigo yołtéełgo holding while

dine dinééh man

nohani yo-he nohaanííyá to us has come

dighini-ye dighiní holy

nohani-yoya-yeye nohaanííyá to us has come

yaha ‘eye neya ŋeye ŋa   yaha ‘eye neya ŋeye ŋa

Here we have seen how ordinary Navajo can be changed into song language (through the addition of syllables and the reduction of vowel length). Thus song language is a manipulated form of everyday language—it is a form of speech play, in that respect in the service of verbal art. Bahr (1983) and Bahr, Paul, and Joseph (1997) point out, in discussing Tohono O’odham songs, that it is necessary to produce four versions: a transcript of the song language, an ordinary language version, an ordinary English language version and, importantly, an English song language version. Here is an example of an O’odham Ocean song (Bahr 1991:544). For the sake of brevity, I have not included the ways Bahr indicates various rhythmic features of the song. Note as well, as with the Navajo example, the song language includes more syllables than the ordinary language version. (8)          

O’odham (Bahr 1991:544) Song Language Ka ci me su na ni Ka ci me su na ni ku kugi me Toi ta ge da mai Ma ma to ne wu pa him

         

Ordinary Language and Literal Translation Ka:cim su:dagi Staying water Ka:cim su:dagi kukughim Staying water terminating T-oidag da:m Our-field on-top Mamtod wurpahim. Seaweed tossing.



         

Verbal art 

 393

Free Translation STAYING WATER, STAYING WATER EXTENDS, OUR FIELDS’ TOPS TOSSED WITH SEAWEED

Bahr translates the song language into ordinary O’odham. He then translates that ordinary O’odham into a literal English translation (represented directly below the ordinary O’odham). Finally, the literal English translation is translated into a free English translation (the All caps indicating this is song language). Much recent work on Native North American song traditions has been published. See, for example, Browner 2009 and Levine and Robinson 2019 for useful recent edited volumes.

17.2.4 Stories As we saw above concerning the genres of Hopi verbal art, narratives too are often divided into locally relevant genres. Such genres may be recognizable, as discussed earlier, based on form (genre signatures, for example) or content or function—or in combination (genres need not always be clearly distinguishable). Basso (1996: 48–51) provides a useful breakdown of Western Apache narrative genres. (9)            

Western Apache narrative genres (Basso 1996: 48–51) nagoldi’é ‘narrative story’   Major categories   godiyįhgo nagoldi’é (myth)   ‘ágodzaahí (historical tales)   nłt’éégo nagoldi’é (saga)   ch’idii (gossip)

     

     

Minor categories ma’ highaałyú nagoldi’é (Coyote stories) binííbaa’ nagoldi’é (seduction tales)

The major categories can be organized based on when the narrative is set, and on the function or purpose of the narrative, as illustrated in Table 2:

394 

 Anthony K. Webster

Tab. 2: Genre Distinctions for Major Categories of Western Apache narratives (after Basso 1996: 50) Narrative Category

Temporal Locus of Events

Purpose

godiyįhgo nagoldi’é (myth)

godiyąąná’ (in the beginning) doo’ánííná (long ago) dííj̢í̢ígo (modern times) k’ad (now)

to enlighten, to instruct

‘ágodzaahí (historical tales) nłt’éégo nagoldi’é (saga) ch’idii (gossip)

to criticize, to warn, to “shoot” to entertain, to engross to inform, to malign

17.2.5 Poetry Verbal art does not remain static. New genres may emerge, inspired and informed by other genres. Societies, cultures, and verbally artistic traditions are not hermetically sealed. For example, a number of Native Americans write poetry. This poetry is influenced by both larger traditions of poetry (Western or Eastern), and by locally relevant verbally artistic traditions. Contemporary Native American poets write and perform in English, local varieties of English, their Indigenous languages, or in some combination of them. Among the Tohono O’odham poems are called ha-cegĭtodag (‘thoughts’) (Zepeda 2019: 8). Some Navajos classify contemporary poetry as a form of hane’ (‘narrative’, ‘story’) (Webster 2009). The use of punning and ambiguity more generally are important features in Navajo verbal art and life, and can also be found in the poetry of a number of Navajo poets. Many of the poems in Rex Lee Jim’s (1995) saad are predicated on puns. So, for example, a poem may play with the ambiguity of na’astsǫǫsí (‘mouse’, ‘the one that goes about sucking’), which can be heard as náá’ásts’ǫǫs (‘to perform a sucking ceremony again’) (see Webster 2018: 32).

17.3 Poetic structuring Following the lead of Melville Jacobs (1959), a number of scholars have sought to rethink Native American narrative traditions in ways other than prose. One particularly fruitful school of thought has been ethnopoetics. This work has sought, through a variety of presentational formatting principles, to highlight various poetic structuring devices, often presenting such narratives in a format resembling Western poetry (thus narratives were presented in lines). Early work, especially work focused on narrative traditions taken down in dictation, tended to focus on recurrent forms such as particles and form-content parallelism (Hymes 1981, 2003). Lines were then defined by such recurrent forms. Other work, often focusing on performances that were recorded with audio-recordings, highlighted prosody and pause structure (Tedlock 1983). Lines here are defined by pause structure. Much work has sought to combine the insights of both “particle” and



Verbal art 

 395

“pause” (see Bright 1979; Woodbury 1985, 1987; Kroskrity 1985, 1993; McClendon 1982; V. Hymes 1987; Doak 1991; Watahomigie and Yamamoto 1998.). I turn now to the use of particles, both initials and finals, as line creating devices. It is important to stress that such uses need to be investigated and not imputed. Likewise, while I have divided the discussion into initials and finals, this does not mean that the two are mutually exclusive. There is often an interplay between, for example, initial adverbial particles and a clause final quotative particle (Hymes 2003). The two work in conjunction to create lines. Likewise, Kimball (1993: 5) notes that lines in Koasati (ISO cku) narratives are created by way of the use of a switch-reference suffix -n and/ or initial conjunctions.

17.3.1 Particles In this section, I highlight the use of particles and enclitics in the creation of meaningful discursive units, following the lead of Hymes (1981). In general, Hymes’s method sought to discern various structuring principles of narratives. The narratives could then be represented as a series of lines, verses, stanzas, acts, and scenes.

17.3.1.1 Initials Initials are words or particles that appear at the beginning of a clause. They are often referred to as discourse particles. Initials, as we will see below, often have an adverbial quality to them. They are one key site for the poetic structuring of discourse. Hymes’s (1981; 2003) early work on Chinookan narrative structuring focused on the use of initial particles in the creating of verses (a grouping of lines). For example, in Wasco-Wishram (ISO wac), the initial particles aga kwapt (‘now then’), nawit (‘straightaway’), agawit’a (‘now again’), kwadau (‘and’, ‘before’), and qidau (‘that is the way’) were shown to be crucial in the structuring of narratives (Hymes 2003: 217). In Upper Chehalis (Salish) (ISO cjh), speaker Silas Heck uses the initial particle húy (‘and so’) as a major structural device (Kinkade 1987: 283). In Ojibwe (Algonquian) (ISO oji) narratives, mii (‘so’) and mii dash (‘and then’) play a major structural role and act as a key rhetorical device (Ghezzi 1993; Valentine 1995; Spielmann 1998). Spielmann (1998: 201), following the work of Valentine (1995), suggests that “mii marks the advancement of the storyline.” Its use doesn’t just signal particular discourse units (lines, stanzas or verses), but also keys the listener into important moments of plot advancement. Ghezzi (1993) qualifies this by suggesting that the use of mii dash may be a part of a particular narrator’s style. We will see this again when we look at reduplication in Southern Paiute (ISO ute) narratives, where reduplication is a poetic option, actualized by individual narrators as a part of their style (Bunte 2002). It is, of course, only possible to attend to individual style when it can be held up in relief against the options other narrators have taken.

396 

 Anthony K. Webster

17.3.1.2 Finals Finals are forms that occur at the end of clauses. In the discussion below, they are either enclitics or quotatives that are clause final. In Chiricahua Apache (Athabaskan) (ISO apm) Coyote narratives, the narrative enclitic =ná’a ‘so they say’ is often used at the end of every clause outside of quoted speech. It serves at least three discursive functions: 1) it places the narrative within the voice of authority; 2) it acts as a genre signature; and 3) it creates meaningful discursive units that—following Hymes (1981)—we can call lines (Webster 1999). In Maidu (ISO nmu), the sentence final-quotative ac’ójʔam ‘it is said’ occurs throughout Maidu narratives (Nevins 2017: 30). Here again, it functions both as a genre signature device and as a structuring principle, creating lines through its rhythmic repetition. Such interplay between grammatical functions and poetic structuring are examples of what Sherzer (1990: 18) calls the poeticization of grammar.

17.3.1.3 A Karuk example Here, then, is an example of the representation of a narrative based on the Hymesian principles of measured verse. The example comes from Bright’s (1984) work on Karuk (ISO kyh). The narrative was told by Mrs. Mamie Offield and taken down in dictation— without a tape-recorder—by Bright in 1951. Verses are identified by the use of initial particles such as víri (‘so’), kári xás (‘and then’) or yakún (‘you see’) (Bright 1984: 137). Lines are identified by predications, subordination, quotations, or shifts in word order (where, for example, a noun phrase occurs after the verb, Bright takes this to be a line marking device) (Bright 1984: 137). Here is the opening scene of Mrs. Mamie Offield’s ‘Coyote Gives Salmon and Acorns to Humans’. Verses are numbered on the left. Lines are indented within a verse. Extra loud materials are indicated by all-caps. Notice as well that the narrative begins at a named and knowable location. (10)                  

Karuk version with glosses (Bright 1984: 137) (1) ʔáxxak ʔasiktâ·n kunʔí·nanik     two women they used to live                 kustá·ras         sisters                     ʔame·kyá·ra·m.         at Amekyaram          



Verbal art 

                     

(2)           (3)        

                 

English Translation (1) Two women used to live,     sisters,         at Amekyaram.         (2) And then they said,     ‘NOBODY’s going to eat salmon.         (3) You see, we’ve hidden it,     that salmon.’

 397

kári xás kunpi·p,   and then they said           ‘PÚRA kára vúra ʔá·ma ʔa·mtíhe·šara.   nobody just salmon won’t eat         yukún tánupíššunva,   you see we’ve hidden it           paʔá·ma.’     the salmon

Bright’s presentation highlights the repeated use of initial particles (kári xás in verse 2 and yukún in verse 3). It also suggests something of the cadence, the rhythm of the narrative performance. A point we will now turn to.

17.3.2 Pause structuring and performance features Another poetic device is the use of pause structure (that is the way lines are created through the use of pauses). Tedlock (1972, 1983) developed a formatting practice that would highlight both the pause structuring and other performance features. Pause being one of a suite of performance features that Tedlock identified in Zuni narratives. Yet, while pause is one feature among others, it was for Tedlock the organizing feature— it is what gave the narratives their poetic structuring (their organization into lines and not sentences or paragraphs). Below is the opening of Andrew Peynetsa’s narration of ‘Coyote and Junco’ (Tedlock 1972: 76–77). I present the Zuni and then Tedlock’s translation. In the example, · indicates a pause of two seconds or so. The — indicates a holding of the vowel for roughly two seconds. Words in small type are said softly, while words in caps are said loudly. Split lines are said in a chant-like manner. Note the formulaic opening (which Tedlock does not translate) and, again, the locating of the narrative at a specific place.

398 

 Anthony K. Webster

(11)                              

Zuni (Tedlock 1972: 76–77)   Zuni version SON’AHCHI.          NOO—   SONTI I TE.            ·   SHOPLHUWAYAL’AN SIL’OKYATTSIK KY’AKWAPPA taachi SUSKI suski lak a’l iimulhan holh cha’lliye. Cha’llappa

   

English translation SON’AHCHI.        NOO— SONTI I TE.          · AT STANDING ARROWS OLD LADY JUNCO HAD HER HOME and COYOTE Coyote was there at Sitting Rock with his children. He was with his children

Notice that the narrative performance begins with loudness and then slowly settles into a more level narrative style. This, as Tedlock describes, is a way to indicate, to frame, that a performance has begun. At the end of the narrative, there will again be much loudness as the narrative frame is closed. Within the frame is the narrative world. Thus the opening and closing bracket the narrative, setting it apart from everyday discourse.

17.3.3 Rhetorical structuring Woodbury (1985, 1987) has argued that the interaction between various ways of poetically organizing verbal art can be communicatively and aesthetically meaningful. That is, rather than seeing “particles” and “pauses” as competing organizing devices, we need to explore the ways they do and do not interact. Woodbury highlights five types of interacting features of rhetorical-structure: 1) prosodic phrasing; 2) pause phrasing; 3) syntactic phrasing; 4) adverbial-particle phrasing; and 5) numerically constrained form-content parallelism (Woodbury 1987: 193). Numerically-constrained form-con-



Verbal art 

 399

tent parallelism concerns the ways that various forms within a narrative (from lines to scenes to the repetition of actions or songs) fall into two broad numerical constraints. As Hymes (1980:9) notes: In Zuni, Karok, Takelma, and Tonkawa, the formal pattern is built up of pairs and fours. In the Chinookan languages, and in the neighboring Sahaptin languages, the formal pattern is built up of threes and fives.

That is to say, that in Tonkawa (ISO tqw) narratives told by John Rush Buffalo (Hymes 1987), events tend to happen in twos and fours. There are, as well, two brothers or two sisters, four brothers or four sisters. An action is repeated four times for success. An action repeated three times suggests incompleteness. In Chinookan languages, on the other hand, the third time is the charm, as it were. Such patterning can be global—that is, it influences an entire narrative—or it can be more local in its appearance—that is, a particular scene may depend on the completion of an action four times. Woodbury (1985, 1987) goes on to argue that such rhetorical-structuring serves four overlapping functions: “organization of information in discourse”, “expression of affective meanings”, “indexing genre”, and “regulation of dialogic interaction” (1985: 179). Finally, where there is misalignment between various components of rhetorical-structure, this may be communicatively significant. Thus, non-alignment can be meaningful.

17.4 Poetic devices (speech play and verbal art) One of the more substantial limitations of the early work on Native American verbal art was a lack of attention to linguistic features that might, in subtle ways, serve key rhetorical purposes. Much recent work has attended to such linguistic features. Hymes (1981), for example, reevaluated the use of expressive prefixes in Takelma (ISO tkm) narratives dealing with bears. He showed that the shift between voiceless laterals in the speech of bears and the addition of a voiceless alveolar fricative s in the speech of both bear and coyote together expressed differing degrees of social distance from those actors. The lateral ł, linked as it was with neighboring Athabaskans, expressed a greater social distance for the Takelma than did the use of s, which was linked with the neighboring (and perhaps distantly linguistically related) Siuslaw. Attention to speech play, the manipulating of linguistic forms for aesthetic purposes, within such verbal artistic traditions also highlights the limits of interpretations of verbal art that failed to account for such speech play. Berman (1992) has shown, for example, how Boas fundamentally misunderstood a Kwakw’ala (ISO kwk) text because he failed to recognize the lewd puns involved (reminding us, as well, that the elicitation of texts was also a site of performance, an interactional event [Hymes 1981; Silverstein 1996]). Here I will only list and exemplify a few such poetic devices.

400 

 Anthony K. Webster

17.4.1 Reportative elements We have already seen the use of various quotative, reportative, or narrative elements in examples from Tewa, Hopi, Chiricahua Apache, and Maidu. To give a sense of the distribution of such devices as a line creating device, here are examples from a Natchez kinha·wiciʔiš (‘tale’) which use a reportative affix (Kimball calls it a quotative), variously realized as šu-, ši-, or š- (ISO ncz) (Kimball 2013: 432). As with the other examples, in traditional narratives the reportative occurs regularly outside of quoted speech; within quoted speech it does not regularly occur (Kimball does not translate the reportative, though it is bolded). (12)        

Natchez (Kimball 2013: 432). cu· tololkop lešukuk cu· tololkop le-šu-ku-k tree hollow stand-qt-aux-conn ‘There was a hollow tree standing, and…’

       

ma·k ka·ʔe·ca·šici. ma·k ka·ʔe·ca·-ši-ci there PVB-enter(pl)-qt-aux ‘there they all entered’

While Kimball does not translate the reportative device, the use of the reportative device places that narrative within the sphere of traditional knowledge (it is not something that has been personally seen, rather it is something one has been told) and it also creates, through its recurrence (a form of parallelism, see below), poetic lines (compare with the use of ba in Arizona Tewa). In Navajo, the reportative jiní ‘they say’ tends to occur more frequently at the beginning and ending of narratives, thus framing the narrative as coming from the voice of tradition (Toelken and Scott 1981; Webster 2009). It can be dropped, though narrators differ about this, in the middle portions of a narrative. Like the Natchez example, the reportative does not normally occur within quoted speech.

17.4.2 Reduplication Among the kinds of stylistic and poetic devices that can be found in the literatures of Native North America is reduplication. Bunte (2002) provides a useful discussion of the use of reduplication in Paiute verbal art. First, in Southern Paiute, reduplication (normally of the initial syllable on either a verb or a noun) indicates a distributive sense on nouns. On verbs, it can indicate a distributive, momentaneous/inceptive, or iterative meaning. Thus, reduplication is grammatical in Southern Paiute. Here is the beginning of a narrative told by Grace Lehi that uses parallelism to set up the availability of a number of women (a particularly important plot point). The reduplicated forms have been bolded following the presentation by Bunte (2002: 7):



(13)      

Verbal art 

 401

Southern Paiute (Bunte 2002: 7) pingwaru̥kwapʉxa u̥kwah máma’utsing Lots of women got married, wife-become-pl-npst quot woman=redup-anim-pl imʉra’atʉm nána’aintsing, Lots of girls, like these here, this.kind giri-redup-anim-pl máma’utsing. Lots of women.

While a number of Southern Paiute narrators used reduplication to highlight key plot points in the narrative, other narrators used reduplication to focus on important concepts that transcend, in a sense, a particular narrative. Thus, Tony Tillohash repeatedly reduplicates the verb ‘arrived’ (píchi̥ pʉxa, pipichi̥ pʉxa [arrive=redup-npst], and píchixw’aipʉxa [arrive=redup-go.away-npst]). Bunte argues that this highlights the cosmological importance of movement for Paiutes. Things, as it were, are always in motion.

17.4.3 Ideophony Ideophones are sound iconic forms that attempt to simulate or enact some quality (movement, sound, texture, etc). The use of ideophony is much described for verbally artistic traditions of South America, Africa, and SE Asia, but it has not been a robust topic of interest in Native North American verbal art. Navajo ideophony is used in a variety of genres of verbal art, from narratives to jokes to place-names to chants to songs to contemporary poetry (Webster 2009). Below are two examples of ideophony from Navajo Coyote narratives told by Curly Tó Aheedlíinii to Father Berard Haile. Notice the use of the verb of sounding (yiists’ą́ą́’) that follows the ideophones (-ts’a’). This is a common way to frame the use of ideophones—it acts as a way of quoting sound. I have bolded the ideophones. (14)        

Navajo (Haile 1984: 104) Nikináázdeestał, ńt’éé’ dil, dil, yiists’ą́ ą́’ jiní   T’áadoo tó diists’ą́ ą́’ da jiní When he stamped his feet again, there was the sound dil, dil, they say.   There was no sound of the river, they say.

(15)        

Navajo (Haile 1984: 94) “Díí shoókę́zíłę́ń” jiní   Hááhgóóshį ,́ jįįz jįįz yiits’a’go “This is for me,” they say.   !!! Listen to that jįįz jįįz sound!

In the first example, the use of the ideophones (dil, dil) acts as evidence that Elk has carried Porcupine across the river. Now, being on solid ground, Porcupine can kill Elk. In 15), Skunk, after tricking Coyote and partaking of nicely cooked Prairie Dogs, throws down the bones for Coyote to eat. The jiiz, jiiz is the sound of crumbling and crushing of the bones as Coyote is left to eat them.

402 

 Anthony K. Webster

Example (15) also shows another poetic device found in Navajo narratives: hááhgóóshį í ̨ .́ Toelken and Scott (1981: 109) comment on the use of hááhgóóshį í ̨ ́ which they gloss as ‘!!!’. This device is used to mark crucial sections of the narratives—saying, essentially, pay attention to this! In this respect, the device calls into relief key moments of the narrative and as such it functions as a metanarrative exhortation (Nuckolls 1992: 74).

17.4.4 Parallelisms Parallelism, be it phonological, morphological, lexical, or syntactic, is an important structuring device for a variety of genres of verbal art. It is found in much Native North American verbal art, though one needs always to explore its functions. At its most basic, parallelism is repetition with variation. Meter, as described for Tohono O’odham (Fitzgerald 1998) and Havasupai (Hinton 1990) song traditions, is a form of parallelism. The meter repeats, while the words may vary. Here is an example of the meter of a Havasupai narrative song (Hinton 1990: 58). The crucial point here is the highly constrained nature of the stress pattern—that is, the metrical structure, as seen in Figure 1.

Fig. 1: Havasupai Song Meter (from Hinton 1990: 58)

Note in this example that each line takes the same stress pattern—that is the meter recurs—but the words of the song are not the same in each line. This creates a metrical pattern for the song.



Verbal art 

 403

Other kinds of parallelism, from grammatical to lexical, can also be discerned in Native North American verbal art. Lexical parallelism involves the repetition of particular lexical items, grammatical parallelism the repetition of grammatical structures. Note that the two are not mutually exclusive. In Navajo ceremonialism, there is much parallelism, where the same grammatical form is repeated with only the substitution of a lexical item (Reichard 1944; McAllester 1980; Field and Blackhorse 2002). Field and Blackhorse (2002: 224) provide this example from a sodizin (‘prayer’) text documented by Reichard (1944). (16)            

Navajo (Field and Blackhorse 2002: 224) chánah nishlį í ̨ ́ dooleeł, ‘I will be healthy, shikee biyá ních’i doo, Wind will be beneath my feet, shijáád biyá ních’i doo, Wind will be beneath my legs, sits’íís biyá ních’i doo, Wind will be beneath my body, shíni’ biyá ních’i doo, Wind will be beneath my mind, shinéé’ biyá ních’i doo Wind will be beneath my voice’

Notice that the parallelism has a direction to it—namely that there is an upward movement to the parallelism. That is, the lexical substitutions begin at the feet and then move upward. This upward movement is common in Navajo verbal art. Here is an example of the upward movement from a Coyote narrative told by John Watchman to Edward Sapir (Sapir and Hoijer 1942). The text has been formatted to highlight the use of jiní (discussed above) as a line marking device and to highlight the parallelism in the text (see Webster and Mitchell 2012). (17)  



Navajo (Webster and Mitchell 2012: 152) “Shikégizhdę́ę’́ tó hada’nłxoshle’!” T’áá’áko bik’egizhdę́ę’́ , hada’nłxosh, jiní. “Shibid bíighahgo tó neel’ą́ ąle’!” T’áá’áko bibid tó bíneel’ą́ , silį í ̨ ’́ . “Shiigháán t’ééidasitą́ ągo yishdloshle’!” ní, jiní. T’áá’áko bíígháán t’éidasitą́ , jiní. “Shijaa’ t’éeyá háát’i’le’!” ní, jiní. T’áá’áko bijaa’ t’éeyá háát’i’, jiní. “My toes, I wish that water would come bubbling between!” Just so, between his toes, it came bubbling up, they say. “My belly, I wish water would come to that level!” Just so, it reached the level of his belly. “My back, I wish I could trot along with it at that level!” he said, they say. Just so, his back, it reached that level, they say. “My ears, I wish only that they stuck out!” he said, they say. Just so, his ears, only they stuck out, they say.

404 

 Anthony K. Webster

Such parallelism within a Coyote story intertextually links with the structure of sodizin (‘prayer’). This is an important part of researching verbal art, recognizing what is novel in a performance and what is instead a gesture to prior discourse and/or other genres.

17.4.5 Metaphor Metaphors are used in a variety of verbally artistic genres. Sekaquaptewa, Hill and Washburn (2015) provide a rich discussion of the use of metaphors in Hopi song traditions. Here is an example from a song recorded by Natalie Curtis in 1903 and presented by Sekaquaptewa, Hill and Washburn (2015: 52). Note the use of vocables at the end of the song. (18)  



Hopi (Sekaquaptewa, Hill and Washburn 2015: 52) Humisimanatuy uuyive amunawita tuvevolimanatu naangöyimani. Taatawyuyuwinani. Lee lehe la. Aahaa yoowi aha ha. Along the corn blossom maidens in the planted field, the butterfly maidens of various colors will go along chasing one another. [You] will be dancing and singing songs. (Vocables.)

Sekaquaptewa, Hill and Washburn (2015: 52) describe the metaphors used here as follows, “both the corn blossoms and the butterflies are metaphorically likened to maidens, that is, young women, since they both hold the promise of the future.” Basso (1990) argues that metaphors—while ubiquitous—need to be understood in light of locally relevant interpretative frameworks. He analyzes a Western Apache genre of verbal art called goyągo yałti’ ‘wise words’ (Basso 1990: 59). The crucial piece to interpreting such metaphors is that they are to be understood as negative statements. Thus, for example, the use of the Western Apache metaphor doolé ‘ichi’kíí ‘at’éé ‘butterflies are girls’ as a “wise word” suggests particular ways to understand this metaphor and the negative social behavior it evokes (i.  e., that they chase around after each other and act crazy and mindlessly) (Basso 1990: 65–66). Brucks and Lovick (2019: 116) note that the association of butterflies with losing one’s moral sense can also be found among the Upper Tanana Athabascan (ISO tau). A similar metaphor can also be found in use among Navajos. Thus, this particular metaphor seems to have a degree of stability across Athabaskan/Dene speaking peoples. Elsewhere, Lovick (2012) points out that for Upper Tanana Athabascan there are two types of metaphors, those grounded in observation and those grounded in myth. Field (2009) has investigated the mythic and metaphorical relations for a particular Navajo verb stem. In both cases, the understanding of metaphors needs to be placed within a larger mythic context.



Verbal art 

 405

17.4.6 Quoted speech Much narrative discourse is founded on the deployment of quoted speech. That is to say, the narratives are often based on verbal interactions (Jacobs 1959). Kroeber (1995) highlights the ways that characterizations are accomplished in a Kalispel (Salish) (ISO spo) narrative told by One-Eyed Tom in 1937 to Hans Vogt. The two main protagonists, Thunder and Rabbit, contrast in how much quoted speech they are given. Rabbit is presented as verbally adroit, Thunder as verbally isolated. Rabbit succeeds in the narrative while Thunder loses his wife to Rabbit. Another way that characterizations can be suggested is through stylized speech (Egesdal 1992; Kimball 2012). Among the Thompson Salish (ISO thp), Coyote’s speech is often marked by the use of the suffix =ólk’ (‘stigma’) (Egedsal 1992: 28). (19)      

Thomspon Salish (Egedsal 1992: 28) kén-m nke ł  n/qwic’tn=ólk’-kt happen-mdl cjr ep lcl/spouse.of.deceased.sibling=stigma-1p.PSV “Something must have happened to our in-law.”

In Natchez “cannibal speech” (Kimball 2012), the k replaces the glottal stop in the first-person optative verbal prefix (ʔa-): (20)        

Natchez (Kimball 2012: 275) kapiškʷą· ka-pi-škʷ-a·-n 1opt(cannibal)-pl-eat-inc-phr.term ‘Let us [cannibals] eat him!’

Likewise, certain phonological changes are also associated with particular actors in a narrative. Egesdal (1992: 31) describes a number of examples of such styled speech. For example, Raven (an important mythic figure on the Northwest Coast), “replaces b and d with the corresponding non-native nasals m and n in Lushootseed, substitutes š for x̥ and nasalizes vowels in Makah, inserts -čx̥- into words in Nootka, inserts šx̥ in Nitinaht, and prefixes š- to words in Quileute.” Beyond the Northwest Coast, in Cocopah narratives, ly is infixed into the speech of Coyote (Langdon 1978). Such processes are quite common and allow characters to be recognized without direct attribution. Simply hearing the way in which a character speaks, then, allows a listener to recognize who is speaking. It is also, as discussed in Section 5, part of the artistry of the narrative—creating involvement in the narrative telling.

17.4.7 Crossing languages One key insight that has arisen from such research on verbal art is the way that certain rhetorical forms can cross languages. Narratives told in Apache English, for example,

406 

 Anthony K. Webster

show certain rhetorical parallels with narratives told in Apache (Nevins 2012). Such rhetorical forms have also been documented in Tillamook English (Hymes 1993) and in Chinuk Wawa (ISO chn) narratives (Hymes 1990 and Hymes and Zenk 1987). This, of course, does not mean that everything transfers across languages, but certain rhetorical patternings—a focus on twos and fours or threes and fives (Hymes 1990, 1993), the use of initial particles (Nevins 2012) and quotative devices (Kroskrity 2012)—can be found to cross languages. We should note, however, that not all linguistic forms or poetic devices can be or are transferred across languages (Woodbury 1998). It is also the case that the presence of multiple languages creates particular verbally artistic possibilities. We have already noted interlingual punning among Navajos in Section 2.2. Code-switching, to take another example, can be used as a poetic device (see Basso 1979). The switch in language can be rhetorically and aesthetically meaningful. In the narrative performances by Angela Sidney, Annie Ned, and Kitty Smith documented by Cruikshank (1990), narratives are told in local varieties of English, but crucial cultural concepts—like placenames, clan names, or the names of important people, for example—are said in Tlingit (ISO tli), Tagish (ISO tgx) or Southern Tutchone (ISO tce). Such code-switching serves as a pedagogical model of learning the names, of “getting the words right” (Cruikshank 1990: 346). In addition, Choctaw prophetic riddles are predicated on the interplay between Choctaw (ISO cho) and English (Mould 2002: 409–410).

17.5 Ethnoliterary criticism One final issue lingers. It is perhaps the most substantial of issues, certainly the most ethnographic. What makes a particular instance of verbal art good? That is, what are the evaluative criteria that people use to appraise a particular performance? Obviously, this is an issue that requires ethnographic investigation. Just as genres cannot be assumed a priori, so too what makes a particular narrative “good” can also not be assumed a priori. Much work still needs to be done on ethnoliterary criticism (Mitchell and Webster 2011). Working with the Koasati, Kimball (1993) was able to discern aesthetic criteria for the narratives. They included the use of parallelism (especially when an action is partially restated in the subsequent line), the use of quoted speech, “the use of long lines to highlight important semantic content in the narrative,” and, finally, “clarity of language” (Kimball 1993: 7). Among the Arizona Tewa, Kroskrity (2012: 162–164) found the ethnoliterary criteria for good stories to involve: 1) péé;yu’u tú ‘story words’ (especially the abundant use of the evidential ba ‘so they say’); 2) the use of archaic words; 3) stylized facial expressions; 4) paralinguistic and prosodic voice effects that often imitate the voices of characters; 5) use of khaw’ or thematic songs; and 6) “Carrying it hither…which can be paraphrased as situating the narrative for the present audience.” Shoshoni scholar Beverly Crum (1980: 17) discusses three key features of Shoshoni song poems (Newe Hupia): 1) use of the diminutive suffix -ttsi, which



Verbal art 

 407

creates a general “warm feeling”; 2) certain sounds changes concerning geminated (or lengthened) consonants; and 3) based on the previous two poetic devices, a certain ambiguity is created in the song lyrics. Finally, Crum (1980: 19) critiques a certain tradition in anthropology that failed to recognize the importance of song traditions for the Shoshoni and thus failed to appreciate an important aesthetic practice. We ignore verbal art at our peril—the peril of missing the things that give delight and pleasure to people in and through verbal art. It is simply not the case that one can look at verbal art documented by prior generations of linguists and anthropologists, or, for that matter, verbal art that has been documented more recently, and understand the interpretative frameworks, the ways of making sense, of that verbal art. How do we understand, for example, a Navajo poem? Some understanding of the role of punning in Navajo aesthetic traditions seems warranted. But there are a host of other issues that inform such poetry—from ideophony to place-names to the felt relations towards languages (Webster 2009; Mitchell and Webster 2011). Likewise, in many Indigenous traditions, the understanding of a narrative is a pragmatic matter—that is to say, it is a matter of the context of the performance. These rather obvious points, but at times easily forgotten points, should remind us that verbal art is not just a question of linguistic devices, but rather something along the lines of an entire aesthetic tradition, often as well, that aesthetic tradition intersects with a set of moral and ethical traditions. To paraphrase literary theorist Kenneth Burke, verbal art, like all literature, is equipment for living. One task then, is to understand in what ways it is such equipment for living. And this, bringing us back full circle to a “community’s own conception” (Sherzer and Woodbury 1987: 8), can only be done by talking with people. More research on the evaluative criteria, the ethnoliterary criticism, of verbally artistic traditions seems warranted (Epps, Webster, and Woodbury 2017). Acknowledgements: I thank Pattie Epps and Tony Woodbury for comments on an earlier draft of this chapter. I thank Bradley Graupner for help tracking down various materials.

References Bahr, Donald. 1983. A Format and Method for Translating Songs. Journal of American Folklore 96(380). 170−182. Bahr, Donald. 1986. Pima Swallow Songs. Cultural Anthropology 1(2). 171−187. Bahr, Donald. 1991. Papago ocean songs and wi:gita. Journal of the Southwest 33(4). 539−556. Bahr, Donald, Lloyd Paul & Vincent Joseph. 1997. Ants and Orioles: Showing the Art of Pima Poetry. Salt Lake City: Utah UP. Basso, Keith. 1979. Portraits of “The Whiteman.” Cambridge: Cambridge UP. Basso, Keith. 1990. Western Apache Language and Culture. Tucson: University of Arizona Press. Basso, Keith. 1996. Wisdom Sits In Places. Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press. Bauman, Richard. 1984. Verbal Art as Performance. Chicago: Waveland Press.

408 

 Anthony K. Webster

Berman, Judith. 1992. Oolachan-Woman’s Robe: Fish, Blankets, Masks, and Meaning in Boas’s Kwakw’ala Texts. In Brian Swann (ed.), On the Translation of Native American Literatures, 125−162. Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian. Bright, William. 1979. A Karok Myth in Measured Verse: the Translation of a Performance. Journal of California and Great Basin Anthropology 1. 117−123. Bright, William. 1984. American Indian Linguistics and Literature. Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter. Browner, Tara. (ed.). 2009. Music of the First Nations: Tradition and Innovation in Native North America. Urbana: University of Illinois Press. Brucks, Caleb & Olga Lovick. 2019. Losing one’s way: Geographical and moral lessons in the Butterfly Story in Upper Tanana Athabascan. In Gary Holton & Thomas F. Thornton (eds.), Language and Toponymy in Alaska and Beyond: Papers in Honor of James Kari, 97−120. (Language Documentation & Conservation Special Publication no. 17). Honolulu: University of Hawai‘i Press. Bunte, Pamela. 2002. Verbal Artistry in Southern Paiute Narratives: Reduplication as a Stylistic Process. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 12(1). 3−33. Chafe, Wallace. 1993. Seneca Speaking Styles and the Location of Authority. In Jane Hill & Judith Irvine (eds.), Responsibility and Evidence in Oral Discourse, 72−87. Cambridge: Cambridge UP. Chafe, Wallace. 1998. Polysynthetic Puns. In Leanne Hinton & Pamela Munro (eds.), Studies in American Indian Languages: Description and Theory, 87−89. Berkeley: University of California Press. Cruikshank, Julie, Angela Sidney, Kitty Smith & Annie Ned. 1990. Life Lived like a Story. Lincoln: Nebraska UP. Crum, Beverly. Crum, Beverly. 1980. Newe Hupia—Shoshoni Poetry Songs. Journal of California and Great Basin Anthropology Papers in Linguistics (2). 1−23. Crum, Beverly, Earl Crum & Jon Dayley. 2001. Newe Hupia: Shoshoni Poetry Songs. Lincoln: Nebraska UP. Darling, J. Andrew & Barnaby Lewis. 2007. Songscapes and Calendar Sticks. In Suzanne Fish & Paul Fish (eds.), The Hohokam Millennium, 131−139. Santa Fe: School of American Research. Darnell, Regna. 1989. Correlates of Cree Narrative Performance. In Richard Bauman & Joel Sherzer (eds.), Explorations in the ethnography of speaking, 315−336. Cambridge: Cambridge UP. Doak, Ivy. 1991. Coeur d’Alene Rhetorical Structure. Texas Linguistic Forum (32). 43−70. Egesdal, Steven. 1992. Stylized Characters’ Speech in Thompson Salish Narrative. University of Montana Occasional Papers in Linguistics (9). Enrico, John & Wendy Bross Stuart. 1996. Northern Haida Songs. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Epps, Patience, Anthony K. Webster & Anthony C. Woodbury. 2017. A Holistic Humanities of Speaking: Franz Boas and the continuing centrality of texts. International Journal of American Linguistics 83(1). 41−78. Farnell, Brenda. 2002. Dynamic Embodiment in Assiniboine (Nakota) Storytelling. Anthropological Linguistics 44(1). 37−64. Field, Margaret. 2009. Metaphor, Mythology, and a Navajo Verb: The Role of Cultural Constructs in the Lexicography of Endangered Languages. Anthropological Linguistics 51(3–4). 296−302. Field, Margaret & Taft Blackhorse Jr. 2002. The Dual Role of Metonymy in Navajo Prayer. Anthropological Linguistics 44(3). 217−230. Field, Margaret & Jon Meza Cuero. 2012. Kumeyaay Oral Tradition, Cultural Identity, and Language Revitalization. Oral Tradition 27(2). 319−332. Fitzgerald, Colleen M. 1998. The meter of Tohono O’odham songs. IJAL 64(1). 1−36. Frisbie, Charlotte. 1980. Vocables in Navajo Ceremonial Music. Ethnomusicology 24(3). 347−392. Gelo, Daniel. 1999. Powwow Patter: Indian Emcee Discourse on Power and Identity. Journal of American Folklore 112(443). 40−57. Ghezzi, Ridie. 1993. Tradition and Innovation in Ojibwe Storytelling: Mrs. Marie Syrette’s ‘The Orphans and Mashos.’ In Arnold Krupat (ed.), New Voices in Native American Literary Criticism, 37−76. Washington, DC: Smithsonian. Haile, Fr. Berard. 1984. Navajo Coyote Tales. Lincoln: Nebraska UP.



Verbal art 

 409

Hinton, Leanne. 1980. Vocables in Havasupai Song. In Charlotte Frisbie (ed.), Southwestern Indian Ritual Drama, 275−306. Albuquerque: New Mexico UP. Hinton, Leanne. 1984. Havasupai songs: A Linguistic Perspective. Tubingen: Gunter Narr Verlag. Hinton, Leanne. 1990. Song Metrics. Proceedings of the Sixteenth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society: Special Session on General Topics in American Indian Linguistics. 51−60. Hymes, Dell. 1980. Particle, Pause and Pattern in American Indian Narrative Verse. American Indian Culture and Research Journal 4(4). 7−51. Hymes, Dell. 1981. In Vain I Tried to Tell You. Philadelphia: Pennsylvania UP. Hymes, Dell. 1990. Thomas Paul’s Sametl: Verse Analysis of a (Saanich) Chinook Jargon Text. Journal of Pidgin and Creole Language 5(1). 71−106. Hymes, Dell. 1993. In Need of a Wife: Clara Pearson’s ‘Split-His-(Own)-Head’. In Anthony Mattina & Timothy Montler (eds.), American Indian Linguistics and Ethnography in Honor of Laurence C. Thompson, 127−162. Missoula: Montana UP. Hymes, Dell. 2003. Now I know only that far. Lincoln: Nebraska UP. Hymes, Dell & Henry Zenk. 1987. Narrative Structure in Chinook Jargon. In Glenn Gilbert (ed.), Essays in Honor of John Reinecke, 445−465. Honolulu: Hawai’i UP. Hymes, Virginia. 1987. Warm Springs Sahaptin narrative analysis. In Joel Sherzer & Anthony Woodbury (eds.), Native American Discourse: Poetics and Rhetoric, 62−102. Cambridge: Cambridge UP. Innes, Pamela. 2010. Ethical problems in archival research: Beyond accessibility. Language & Communication 30(3). 198−203. Jacobs, Melville. 1959. The Content and Style of an Oral Literature: Clackamas Chinook Myths and Tales. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Jim, Rex Lee. 1995. saad. Princeton: Princeton Collections of Western Americana. Kendall, Martha. 1979. Wolf and Coyote an Upland Yuman Text. Amerindia 4. 127−147. Kendall, Martha. 1980. Exegesis and Translation: Northern Yuman Names as Texts. Journal of Anthropological Research 36. 261−273. Kimball, Geoffrey. 1993. Koasati Narrator and Narrative. In Arnold Krupat (ed.), New Voices In Native American Literary Criticism, 3−35. Washington, DC: Smithsonian. Kimball, Geoffrey. 2012. Natchez Cannibal Speech. IJAL 78(2). 273−279. Kimball, Geoffrey. 2013. The Woman who was a Fox: The structure of a Natchez oral narrative. IJAL 79(3). 421−437. Kinkade, M. Dale. 1987. Bluejay and His Sister. In Brian Swann & Arnold Krupat (eds.), Recovering the Word, 255−296. Berkeley: California UP. Kroeber, Paul. 1995. Rhetorical Structure of a Kalispel Narrative. Anthropological Linguistics 37(2). 119−140. Kroskrity, Paul. 1985. Growing with Stories: Line, Verse, and Genre in an Arizona Tewa Text. Journal of Anthropological Research 41. 183−199. Kroskrity, Paul. 1993. Language, History and Identity: Ethnolinguistic Studies of the Arizona Tewa. Tucson: Arizona UP. Kroskrity, Paul. 2012. Growing with Stories: Ideologies of Storytelling and the Narrative Reproduction of Arizona Tewa Identities. In Paul Kroskrity (ed.), Telling Stories in the Face of Danger: Language Renewal in Native American Communities, 151−183. Norman: University of Oklahoma Press. Langdon, Margaret. 1978. Animal Talk in Cocopa. International Journal of American Linguistics 44. 10−16. Levine, Victoria Lindsay & Dylan Robinson (eds.). 2019. Music and Modernity among First Peoples of North America. Middletown: Wesleyan University Press. Loether, Christopher. 1993. Niimina Ahubiya: Western Mono Song Genres. Journal of California and Great Basin Anthropology 15(1). 48−57. Lovick, Olga. 2012. Walking like a porcupine, talking like a raven: Figurative Language in Upper Tanana Athabascan. In Anna Idstrӧm & Elisabeth Piirainen (eds.), Endangered Metaphors, 103–121. Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

410 

 Anthony K. Webster

McAllester, David. 1954. Enemy Way Music: A study of Social and Esthetic Values as seen in Navajo Music. Papers of the Peabody Museum of American Archaeology and Ethnology, Harvard University. 41(3). Cambridge: Peabody Museum. McAllester, David. 1980. The First Snake Song. In Stanley Diamond (ed.), Theory and Practice: Essays Presented to Gene Weltfish, 1−27. New York: Mouton. McLendon, Sally. 1977. Cultural Presuppositions and discourse analysis. In Muriel Saville-Troike (ed.), Linguistics and Anthropology: Georgetown University Round Table on Languages and Linguistics, 153−189. Washington DC: Georgetown UP. McLendon, Sally. 1982. Meaning, Rhetorical Structure, and Discourse Organization in Myth. In Deborah Tannen (ed.), Analyzing Discourse: Text and Talk. Georgetown University Round Table on Languages and Linguistics, 284−305. Washington, DC: Georgetown UP. Mitchell, Blackhorse & Anthony K. Webster. 2011. “We don’t know what we become”: Navajo ethnopoetics and an expressive feature in a poem by Rex Lee Jim. Anthropological Linguistics 53(3). 259−286. Mould, Tom. 2002. Prophetic Riddling: A Dialogue of Genres in Choctaw Performance. Journal of American Folklore 115(457/458). 395−421. Nevins, M. Eleanor. 2012. Ndah Ch’ii’n. In David Kozak (ed.), Inside Dazzling Mountains: Southwest Native Verbal Art, 197−239. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Nevins, M. Eleanor. 2017. World-making stories: Maidu language and community renewal on a shared California landscape. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Nuckolls, Janis. 1992. Sound Symbolic Involvement. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 2(1). 51−80. Palmer Jr., Gus. 2003. Telling Stories the Kiowa Way. Tucson: University of Arizona Press. Reichard, Gladys. 1944. Prayer: The Compulsive Word. American Ethnological Society Monograph 7. Seattle: Washington UP. Sapir, Edward & Harry Hoijer. 1942. Navaho Texts. Iowa City: Linguistic Society of America. Schermerhorn, Seth. 2019. Walking to Magdalena: Personhood and Place in Tohono O’odham Songs, Sticks, and Stories. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Scollon, Ronald. 2009. This Is What They Say: Stories. Seattle: University of Washington Press. Sekaquaptewa, Emory, Kenneth Hill & Dorothy Washburn. 2015. Hopi Katsina Songs. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Shaul, David Leedom. 2002. Hopi Traditional Literature. Albuquerque: New Mexico UP. Sherzer, Joel. 1990. Verbal Art in San Blas: Kuna Culture Through Its Discourse. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Sherzer, Joel & Anthony Woodbury. 1987. Introduction. In Joel Sherzer & Anthony Woodbury (eds.), Native American Discourse: Poetics and Rhetoric, 1−16. Cambridge: Cambridge UP. Silverstein, Michael. 1996. The Secret Life of Texts. In Michael Silverstein & Greg Urban (eds.), Natural Histories of Discourse, 81−105. Chicago: Chicago UP. Spielmann, Roger. 1998. “You’re So Fat!”: Exploring Ojibwe Discourse. Toronto: Toronto UP. Tedlock, Barbara. 1995. Aesthetics and Politics: Zuni War God Repatriation and Kachina Representation. In Brenda Jo Bright (ed.), Looking High and Low, 151−172. Tucson: University of Arizona Press. Tedlock, Dennis. 1972. Finding the Center. New York: The Dial Press. Tedlock, Dennis. 1983. The Spoken Word and the Work of Interpretation. Philadelphia: Pennsylvania UP. Toelken, Barre. 1998. The Yellowman Tapes, 1966–1997. Journal of American Folklore 111(442). 381−391. Toelken, Barre & Tacheeni Scott. 1981. Poetic Retranslation and the ‘Pretty Languages’ of Yellowman. In Karl Kroeber (ed.), Traditional Literatures of the American Indians, 65−116. Lincoln: Nebraska UP. Valentine, Lisa Philips. 1995. Making it Their Own: Severn Ojibwe Communicative Practices. Toronto: University of Toronto. Watahomigie, Lucille & Akira Yamamoto. 1998. Discourse Routines in Hualapai Stories. In Leanne Hinton & Pamela Munro (eds.), Studies in American Indian Languages, 57−71. Berkeley: University of California Press.



Verbal art 

 411

Webster, Anthony K. 1999. Sam Kenoi’s Coyote Stories: Poetics and Rhetoric in some Chiricahua Apache Narratives. American Indian Culture and Research Journal 23(1). 137–163. Webster, Anthony K. 2009. Explorations in Navajo Poetry and Poetics. Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press. Webster, Anthony K. 2010. A Note on Navajo Interlingual Puns. International Journal of American Linguistics 76(2). 289−298. Webster, Anthony K. 2018. The Sounds of Navajo Poetry: A Humanities of Speaking. New York: Peter Lang. Webster, Anthony K. 2020. Learning to be Satisfied: Navajo poetics, a chattering chipmunk, and ethnopoetics. Oral Tradition 34. 73−104. Webster, Anthony K. & Mitchell Blackhorse. 2012. John Watchman’s Ma’ii dóó Gólízhii. In David Kozak (ed.), Inside Dazzling Mountains: Southwest Native Verbal Art, 150−172. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Whiteley, Peter. 1998. Rethinking Hopi Ethnography. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Press. Wiget, Andrew. 1987. Telling the Tale: A performance analysis of a Hopi Coyote Story. In Brian Swann & Arnold Krupat (eds.), Recovering the Word, 297−338. Berkeley: California UP. Woodbury, Anthony. 1984. Cev’armiut Qanemciit Qulirait-llu: Eskimo narratives and Tales from Chevak, Alaska. Fairbanks: Alaska Native Language Center, University of Alaska. Woodbury, Anthony. 1985. The Function of Rhetorical Structure: A Study of Central Alaskan Yupik Eskimo Discourse. Language in Society 14. 153−190. Woodbury, Anthony. 1987. Rhetorical Structure in a Central Alaskan Yupik Eskimo Traditional Narrative. In Joel Sherzer & Anthony C. Woodbury (eds.), Native American Discourse: Poetics and Rhetoric, 176–239. Cambridge: Cambridge UP. Woodbury, Anthony. 1998. Documenting rhetorical, aesthetic, and expressive loss in language shift. In Lenore Grenoble & Lindsay Whaley (eds.), Endangered Languages, 234–258. Cambridge: Cambridge UP. Zepeda, Ofelia (ed.). 2019. When it rains/mat hekid o ju: ‘O’odham Ha-Cegĭtodag/Tohono O’odham and Pima Poetry. Tucson: University of Arizona Press.

Further references of interest Anderson, Jeffrey. 2005. Arapaho Ghost Dance Songs. In Brian Swann (ed.), Algonquian Spirit, 448−471. Lincoln: Nebraska UP. Anderson, Jeffrey. 2006. The Poetics of Tropes and Dreams in Arapaho Ghost Dance Songs. In Sergei Kan & Pauline Turner Strong (eds.), New Perspectives on Native North America, 122−161. Lincoln: Nebraska UP. Astrov, Margot. 1950. The conception of motion as the psychological leitmotif of Navaho life and literature. Journal of American Folklore 63(247). 45−56. Axelrod, Melissa & Jule Gómez de García. 2007. Repetition in Apachean Narrative Discourse: From Discourse Structure to Language Learning in Morphologically Complex Languages. Language, Meaning and Society 1. 107−142. Bahr, Donald. 1975. Pima and Papago Ritual Oratory. San Francisco: The Indian Historian Press. Bahr, Donald. 1980. The Role of Rhythm in ‘Cementing’ Meaning in Piman Songs. In Mary Ritchie Key (ed.), The Relationship of Verbal and Nonverbal Communication, 119−123. The Hague: Mouton Publishers. Bahr, Donald. 1987. Pima Heaven Songs. In Brian Swann & Arnold Krupat (eds.), Recovering the Word, 198−246. Berkeley: California UP. Bahr, Donald. 1991. A Grey and Fervent Shamanism. Journal de la Société des Americanistes 77. 7−26. Bahr, Donald. 2011. How Mockingbirds Are: O’odham Ritual Orations. Albany: SUNY Press. Bahr, Donald (ed.). 2001. O’odham Creation & Related Events: As told to Ruth Benedict in 1927 in Prose, Oratory, and Song. Tucson: Arizona UP.

412 

 Anthony K. Webster

Bahr, Donald, Joseph Giff & Manuel Havier. 1979. Piman Songs on Hunting. Ethnomusicology 23(2). 245−296. Bahr, Donald & J. Richard Haefer. 1978. Song in Piman Curing. Ethnomusicology 22(1). 89−122. Basso, Ellen. 1978. The enemy of every tribe: “Bushman” images in Northern Athapaskan narratives. American Ethnologist 5(4). 690−709. Beck, David & David Bennett. 2007. Extending the Prosodic Hierarchy: Evidence from Lushootseed Narrative. Northwest Journal of Linguistics 1(1). 1−37. Belin, Esther G., Jeff Berglund, Connie Jacobs & Anthony K. Webster (eds.). 2021. The Diné Reader: An Anthology of Navajo Literature. Tucson: University of Arizona Press. Berez, Andrea. 2011. Prosody as a genre-distinguishing feature in Ahtna: A quantitative approach. Functions of Language 18(2). 210−236. Bierwert, Crisca. (ed.) 1996. Lushootseed Texts: An Introduction to Puget Salish Narrative Aesthetics. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Brightman, Robert. 1989. Tricksters and Ethnopoetics. IJAL 52(2). 179−203. Browner, Tara. 2000. Making and Singing Pow-wow Songs: Text, Form, and the Significance of Culture-based Analysis. Ethnomusicology 44(2). 214−233. Browner, Tara. 2004. Heartbeat of the People: Music and Dance of the Northern Pow-wow. Urbana: Illinois UP. Bunte, Pamela. 2006. Narrative, Place, and Identity in a Southern Paiute Community. Santa Barbara Papers in Linguistics 18. Bunte, Pamela. 2009. “You Keep Not Listening with Your Ears!”: Language Ideologies, Language Socialization, and Paiute Identity. In Paul Kroskrity & Margaret Field (eds.), Native American Language Ideologies, 172−189. Tucson: University of Arizona Press. Bunte, Pamela. 2012. “You’re Talking English, Grandma”: Language Ideologies, Narratives, and Southern Paiute Linguistic and Cultural Reproduction. In Paul Kroskrity (ed.), Telling Stories in the Face of Danger: Language Renewal in Native American Communities, 44−59. Norman: University of Oklahoma Press. Carr, Gerald & Barbra Meek. 2013. The poetics of language revitalization: Text, performance, and change. Journal of Folklore Research 50 (1−3). 191−216. Chafe, Wallace. 1998. Verbal Artistry in a Seneca Folktale. Proceedings from the First Workshop on American Indigenous Languages. Santa Barbara Papers in Linguistics 8. 42−56. Collins, James. 2009. The place of narrative in human affairs: the implications of Hymes’s Amerindian work for understanding text and talk. Text & Talk 29(3). 325−345. Clements, William. 1996. Native American Verbal Art. Tucson: Arizona UP. Clements, William. 2002. Oratory in Native North America. Tucson: University of Arizona Press. Cruikshank, Julie. 1998. The Social Life of Stories. Lincoln: Nebraska UP. Cruikshank, Julie. 1999. The Social Life of Texts: Editing on the Page and in Performance. In Laura Murray & Keren Rice (eds.), Talking on the Page: Editing Aboriginal Oral Texts, 97−119. Toronto: University of Toronto Press. Dahlstrom, Amy. 2003. Warrior Power from an Underwater Spirit: Cultural and Linguistic Aspects of an Illustrated Meskwaki Text. Anthropological Linguistics 45. 1−56. Dahlstrom, Amy. 2006. The Syntax of Algonquian Ethnopoetics. Papers of the Algonquian Conference/Actes du congres des algonquinistes 37. 131−147. Dahlstrom, Amy. 2015. Highlighting Rhetorical Structure through Syntactic Analysis: An Illustrated Meskawki Text by Alfred Kiyana. In David Costa (ed.), New Voices for Old Words: Algonquian Oral Literatures, 118−197. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Dauenhauer, Nora Marks & Richard Dauenhauer. 1987. Haa Shuká, Our Ancestors: Tlingit Oral Narratives. Seattle: Washington UP. Dauenhauer, Nora Marks & Richard Dauenhauer. 1995. Oral Literature Embodied and Disembodied. In Uta Quasthoff (ed.), Aspects of Oral Communication, 91−111. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. Dauenhauer, Nora Marks & Richard Dauenhauer. 1998. Technical, emotional, and ideological issues in reversing language shift: examples from Southeast Alaska. In Lenore Grenoble & Lindsay Whaley (eds.), Endangered Languages, 57−98. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.



Verbal art 

 413

Dauenhauer, Nora Marks & Richard Dauenhauer. 1999. The paradox of talking on the page: Some aspects of the Tlingit and Haida experience. In Laura Murray & Keren Rice (eds.), Talking on the Page: Editing Aboriginal Oral Texts, 3−41. Toronto: University of Toronto Press. Dauenhauer, Nora Marks & Richard Dauenhauer. 2001. Tracking ‘Yuwaan Gagéets’: A Russian Fairy Tale in Tlingit Oral Tradition. In Larry Evers & Barre Toelken (eds.), Native American Oral Traditions: Collaboration & Interpretation, 58−91. Logan: Utah State University Press. Dauenhauer, Nora Marks & Richard Dauenhauer. 2011. Translating Tense and Aspect in Tlingit Narratives. In Brian Swann (ed.), Born in the Blood: On Native American Translation, 301−325. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Dayley, Jon. 2002. Special language in Shoshoni poetry songs. Survey of California and Other Indian Languages. Report 12. 98−107. Dorsey, James O. 1892. Siouan Onomatopes. American Anthropologist 5(1). 1−8. Dunn, John. 1989. Tsimshian Poetics. In Mary Ritchie Key & Henry Hoenigswald (eds.), General and Amerindian Ethnolinguistics, 395−406. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Eckert, Penelope & Russell Newmark. 1980. Central Eskimo Song Duels: A Contextual Analysis of Ritual Ambiguity. Ethnology 19(2). 191−211. Edwards, Elizabeth. 1995. “It’s an Ill Wind…”. In Michael Dürr, Egon Renner & Wolfgang Oleschinski (eds.), Language and Culture in Native North America, 245−252. Berlin: Lincom. Edwards, Elizabeth & Carol Eastman. 1983. qaao qaao: A Haida Traditional Narrative, or Quoth the Raven ‘Nevermore’. Working Papers for the 18th International Conference on Salish and Neighboring Languages, 54−79. Seattle: University of Washington. Edwards, Elizabeth & Carol Eastman. 1995. ‘Fried Bread’: A Recipe for the structure of Haida Oral Narrative. In Michael Dürr, Egon Renner & Wolfgang Oleschinski (eds.), Language and Culture in Native North America, 253−264. Berlin: Lincom. Epps, Patience, Anthony K. Webster, Anthony C. Woodbury. 2023. Documenting Speech Play and Verbal Art: A Tutorial. [Special issue]. Language Documentation & Conservation. Evers, Larry & Felipe Molina. 1987. Yaqui Deer Songs. Arizona: Arizona UP. Evers, Larry & Felipe Molina. 1990. Wo’i Bwikam: Coyote Songs. Tucson: Chax Press. Evers, Larry & Felipe Molina. 1998. Like this it stays in your hands: Collaboration and Ethnopoetics. Oral Tradition 13(1). 15−57. Evers, Larry and Toelken, Barre. 2001. Native American Oral Traditions: Collaboration and Ethnopoetics. Logan: Utah State UP. Evers, Larry and Zepeda, Ofelia. 1995. Home Places. Tucson: Arizona UP. Farnell, Brenda. 1995. Do you see what I mean: Plains Indian Sign Talk and the Embodiment of Action. Austin: Texas UP. Field, Margaret. 2012. Kumai Stories: Bridges between the Oral Tradition and Classroom Practice. In Paul Kroskrity (ed.), Telling Stories in the Face of Danger: Language Renewal in Native American Communities, 115−126. Norman: University of Oklahoma Press. Field, Margaret. 2018. Sacred Water and Water-Dwelling Serpents: What Can Yuman Oral Tradition Tell Us about Yuman Prehistory? Journal of the Southwest 60(1). 2−25. Field, Margaret (ed.). 2019. Mii Namak Nyamak Kweyiwpo/Huellas del Pasado Hacia el Futuro/Footsteps from the Past Into the Future: Jwanya Kumiai Kuwak/Cuentos Kumiai de Baja California/Kumeyaay Stories of Baja California. A Trilingual Collection in Kumeyaay, Spanish, & English. San Diego: San Diego State University Press. Fienup-Riordan, Ann & Lawrence Kaplan. 2007. Words of the Real People: Alaska Native Literature in Translation. Fairbanks: University of Alaska Press. Fitzgerald, Colleen M. 2017. Motivating the documentation of the verbal arts: Arguments from theory and practice. Language Documentation & Conservation 11. 114−132. Foster, Michael. 1974. From the Earth to the Sky: An Ethnographic Approach to Four Longhouse Iroquois Speech Events. Canadian Ethnology Service Paper No. 20. National Museum of Man Mercury Series. Ottawa: National Museums of Canada.

414 

 Anthony K. Webster

Fowler, Catherine & Harold Abel. 2000. Northern Paiute Prayer: Some Features of the Genre. In Eugene Casad & Thomas Willet (eds.), Uto-Aztecan: Structural, Temporal and Geographical Perspectives, 43−49. Hermosillo: Universidad de Sonora. Frisbie, Charlotte. 1980. Ritual Drama in the Navajo House Blessing Ceremony. In Charlotte Frisbie (ed.), Southwestern Indian Ritual Drama, 161−198. Albuquerque: New Mexico UP. Goertz, Jolynn Amrine (ed.). 2018. Chehalis Stories. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Haag, Marcia. 2011. Toward Literature: Preservation of Artistic Effects in Choctaw Texts. In Brian Swann (ed.), Born in the Blood: On Native American Translation, 348−369. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Haag, Marcia (ed.). 2016. A Listening Wind: Native Literature from the Southeast. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Henne, Richard. 2009. Verbal Art: A Case for Education. Anthropology and Education Quarterly 40(4). 331−349. Henne-Ochoa, Richard & Richard Bauman. 2015. Who is Responsible for Saving the Language? Performing Generation in the Face of Language Shift. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 25(2). 128−150. Hill, Jane & Ofelia Zepeda. 1993. Mrs. Patricio’s Troubles: The distribution of responsibility in an account of personal experience. In Jane Hill & Judith Irvine (eds.), Responsibility and Evidence, 197−225. London: Cambridge UP. Hill, Williard William. 1943. Navajo Humor. Menasha: George Banta. Hill, Williard William & Dorothy Hill. 1945. Navaho Coyote Tales and their position in the Southern Athabaskan Group. Journal of American Folklore 58 (203). 317−343. Hinton, Leanne. 1994. Flutes of Fire. Berkeley: Heyday Books. Hinton, Leanne. 1996. The New Old Ladies’ Songs: Functional Adaptation of Hualapai Music to Modern Contexts. In Dan Slobin, Julie Gerhardt, Amy Kyratzis & Jiansheng Guo (eds.), Social Interaction, Social Context, and Language, 529−544. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers. Hinton, Leanne & Lucille Watahomigie (eds.). 1984. Spirit Mountain: An Anthology of Yuman Story and Song. Tucson: Arizona UP. Howard, Victoria. 2021. Clackamas Chinook Performance Art: Verse Form Interpretations, Catharine Mason (ed.). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Humphreys, Paul. 1982. The Tradition of Song Renewal Among the Pueblo Indians of North America. American Indian Culture and Research Journal 6(1). 9−24. Hymes, Dell. 1965. Some North Pacific Coast Poems: A Problem in Anthropological Philology. American Anthropologist 67(2). 316−341. Hymes, Dell. 1979. How to Talk Like a Bear in Takelma. IJAL 45(2). 101−106. Hymes, Dell. 1983. Victoria Howard’s ‘Gitskux and His Older Brother’: A Clackamas Chinook Myth. In Smoothing the Ground: Essays on Native American Oral Literature. Berkeley: University of California Press. 129−170. Hymes, Dell. 1984. The Earliest Clackamas Text. IJAL 50(4). 358−383. Hymes, Dell. 1987. Tonkawa Poetics: John Rush Buffalo’s ‘Coyote and Eagle’s Daughter.’ In Joel Sherzer & Anthony Woodbury (eds.), Native American Discourse: Poetics and Rhetoric, 17−61. Cambridge: Cambridge UP. Hymes, Dell. 1995. Na-Dene Ethnopoetics, a Preliminary Report: Haida and Tlingit. In Michael Dürr, Egon Renner & Wolfgang Oleschinski (eds.), Language and Culture in Native North America, 265−311. Berlin: Lincom. Hymes, Dell. 1998. Reading Takelma Texts. Bloomington: Trickster Press. Hymes, Dell. 2000. Sung Epic and Native American Ethnopoetics. In Lauri Honko (ed.), Textualization of Oral Epics, 291−342. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Hymes, Dell & William Seaburg. 2013. Chinookan Oral Literature. In Robert Boyd, Kenneth Ames & Tony Johnson (eds.), Chinookan Peoples of the Lower Columbia, 163−180. Seattle: University of Washington Press.



Verbal art 

 415

Hymes, Virginia & Hazel Supah. 1992. How long ago we got lost. Anthropological Linguistics 34(1−4). 73−83. Innes, Pamela. 2006. The interplay of genres, gender, and language ideology among the Muskogee. Language in Society 35(3). 231−259. Jackson, Jason. 2002. Gender Reciprocity and Ritual Speech among the Yuchi. In Lisa Lefler & Fredric Gleach (eds.), Southern Indians and Anthropologists, 89−106. Athens, GA: University of Georgia Press. Jackson, Jason & Victoria Levine. 2002. Singing for Garfish: Music and Woodland Communities in Eastern Oklahoma. Ethnomusicology 46(2). 284−306. Jackson, Jason Baird & Mary S. Linn. 2000. Calling in the Members: Linguistic Form and Cultural Context in a Yuchi Ritual Speech Genre. Anthropological Linguistics 42(1). 61−80. Jim, Rex Lee. 1989. Áhí Ni’ Nikisheegiizh. Princeton: Princeton Collections of Western Americana. Jim, Rex Lee. 1998. Dúchas Táá Kóó Diné. Beal Feirste, Ireland: Au Clochan. Jim, Rex Lee. 2019. Saad Lá Tah Hózhóón: A Collection of Diné Poetry. Flagstaff: Salina Bookshelf. Jim, Rex Lee & Anthony K. Webster. 2022. Native North America: Notes Towards a Dialogical Ethnopoetics. In Svenja Völkel and Nico Nassenstein (eds.), Approaches to Language and Culture, 385-424. Berlin: Mouton. Keeling, Richard. 1992. Cry for Luck: Sacred Song and Speech among the Yurok, Hupa, and Karok Indians of Northwestern California. Berkeley: University of California Press. Kenoyer, Louis, Jedd Schrock, & Henry Zenk. 2017. My Life, Louis Kenoyer. Corvallis: Oregon State University Press. Kimball, Geoffrey. 2010. Koasati Traditional Narratives. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Kimball, Geoffrey. 2022. Yukhíti Kóy: A Reference Grammar of the Atakapa Language. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Kozak, David. 2004. Whirlwind Songs. In Brian Swann (ed.), Voices from the Four Directions: Contemporary Translations of the Native Literatures of North America, 340−349. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Kozak, David (ed.). 2012. Inside Dazzling Mountains: Southwest Native Verbal Art. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Kozak, David & David Lopez. 1999. Devil Sickness and Devil Songs: Tohono O’odham Poetics. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Press. Kroeber, Karl (ed). 1981. Traditional Literatures of the American Indian. Lincoln: Nebraska UP. Kroskrity, Paul. 1992. Arizona Public Announcements: Form, Function, and Linguistic Ideology. Anthropological Linguistics 34(1−4). 104−116. Kroskrity, Paul. 1998. Discursive convergence with a Tewa evidential. In Jane Hill, P. J. Mistry & Lyle Campbell (eds.), The Life of Language, 25−34. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Kroskrity, Paul. 2009. Embodying the Reversal of Language Shift: Agency, Incorporation, Language Ideological Change in the Western Mono Community of Central California. In Paul Kroskrity & Margaret Field (eds.), Native American Language Ideologies, 190−210. Tucson: University of Arizona Press. Kroskrity, Paul. 2009. Narrative Reproductions: Ideologies of Storytelling, Authoritative Words, and Generic Regimentation in the Village of Tewa. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 19(1). 40−56. Kroskrity, Paul. 2010. The Art of Voice: Understanding the Arizona Tewa Inverse in Its Grammatical, Narrative, and Language-Ideological Contexts. Anthropological Linguistics 52(1). 49−79. Kroskrity, Paul. 2013. Discursive Discriminations in the Representation of Western Mono and Yokuts Stories: Confronting Narrative Inequality and Listening to Indigenous Voices in Central California. Journal of Folklore Research 50 (1−3). 145−174. Kroskrity, Paul (ed). 2012. Telling Stories in the Face of Danger: Language Renewal in Native American Communities. Norman: University of Oklahoma Press. Kroskrity, Paul & Anthony K. Webster (eds.). 2015. The Legacy of Dell Hymes: Ethnopoetics, Narrative Inequality, and Voice. Bloomington: Indiana University Press. Krupat, Arnold (ed). 1993. New Voices in Native American Literary Criticism. Washington, DC: Smithsonian.

416 

 Anthony K. Webster

Kwachka, Patricia. 1992. Discourse structures, cultural stability, and language shift. International Journal of the Sociology of Language 93. 67−73. Lang, Julian. 1994. Ararapíkva Creation Stories of the People: Traditional Karuk Indian Literature from Northwestern California. Berkeley: Heyday Books. Langen, Toby. 1989–90. The Organization of Thought in Lushootseed (Puget Salish) Literature: Martha Lamont’s ‘Mink and Changer.’ MELUS 16. 77−94. Langen, Toby. 1995. Nostalgia and Ambiguity in Martha Lamont’s ‘Crow and her Seagull Slaves. In Amritjit Singh, Joseph Skerrit & Robert Hogan (eds.), Memory and Cultural Politics. Boston: Northeastern UP. Langen, Toby & Marya Moses. 2001. Reading Martha Lamont’s Crow Story Today. In Larry Evers & Barre Toelken (eds.), Native American Oral Traditions, 92−129. Logan: Utah State UP. Lassiter, Luke. 1998. The Power of Kiowa Song. Tucson: Arizona UP. Levine, Victoria Lindsay & Bruno Nettl. 2011. Strophic Form and Asymmetrical Repetition in Four American Indian Songs. In Michael Tenzer & John Roeder (eds.), Analytical and Cross-Cultural Studies in World Music, 288−315. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Loether, Christopher. 1990. Ceremony as Performance: The Western Mono Cry-Dance. Journal of California and Great Basin Anthropology 12(2). 215−230. Lovick, Olga & Siri Tuttle. 2012. The prosody of Dena’ina narrative discourse. IJAL 78(3). 293−334. Luthin, Herbert. 2002. Surviving Through the Days: Translations of Native California Stories and Songs. Berkeley: California UP. Luthin, Herbert & Leanne Hinton. 2003a. The Story of Lizard. In Karl Kroeber & Clifton Kroeber (eds.), Ishi in Three Centuries, 293−317. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Luthin, Herbert & Leanne Hinton. 2003b. The Days of a Life: What Ishi’s Stories Can Tell Us About Ishi. In Karl Kroeber & Clifton Kroeber (eds.), Ishi in Three Centuries, 318−354. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Malone, Joseph. 1997. On Reduplication in Ojibwa. Anthropological Linguistics 39(3). 437−458. Martin, Jack. 2010. How to Tell a Creek Story in Five Tenses. IJAL 76(1). 43−70. Martin, Jack, Margaret McKane Mauldin & Juanita McGirt (eds.). 2004. Totkv Mocvse: New Fire: Creek Folktales by Earnest Gouge. Norman: Oklahoma UP. (With companion DVD Creek Folktales by Earnest Gouge as read by Margaret McKane Mauldin. Norman: Oklahoma UP). Mather, Elsie & Phyllis Morrow. 1998. There are no more words to the story. Oral Tradition 13(1). 200−242. Matthews, Washington. 1894. Songs of Sequence of the Navajos. Journal of American Folklore 7(26). 185−194. McAllester, David. 1980a. Hogans: Navajo Houses and House Songs. Middletown: Wesleyan UP. McAllester, David. 1980b. Shootingway, an Epic Drama of the Navajos. In Charlotte Frisbie (ed.), Southwestern Indian Ritual Drama, 199−237. Prospect Heights: Waveland Press. McCarty, Teresa L., Sheilah E. Nicholas, Kari A. B. Chew, Natalie G. Diaz, Wesley Y. Leonard & Louellyn White. 2018. Hear Our Languages, Hear Our Voices: Storywork as Theory and Praxis in Indigenous-Language Reclamation. Dædalus 147(2). 160−172. McIlwraith, Thomas. 2012. ‘We Are Still Didene’: Stories of Hunting and History from Northern British Columbia. Toronto: University of Toronto Press. Mishler, Craig. 1984. Telling About Bear: A Northern Athapaskan Men’s Riddle Tradition. Journal of American Folklore 97(383). 61−68. Mishler, Craig. 2013. The Blind Man and the Loon: The Story of a Tale. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Moore, Robert. 1993. Performance form and the voices of characters in five versions of the Wasco Coyote Cycle. In John Lucy (ed.), Reflexive Language, 213−240. Cambridge: Cambridge UP. Moore, Robert. 2009. From Performance to Print, and Back: Ethnopoetics as social practice in Alice Florendo’s corrections to ‘Raccoon and His Grandmother.’ Text & Talk 29(3). 296−324. Moore, Robert. 2013. Reinventing Ethnopoetics. Journal of Folklore Research 50 (1−3). 13−39. Mould, Tom. 2003. Choctaw Prophecy: A Legacy for the Future. Tuscaloosa: Alabama UP. Mould, Tom. 2004. Choctaw Tales. Jackson: Mississippi UP. Mulder, Jean. 1994. Structural Organization in Coast Tsimshian Music. Ethnomuiscology 38(1). 81−125.



Verbal art 

 417

Murray, Laura J. & Keren Rice (eds.). 1999. Talking on the Page: Editing Aboriginal Oral Texts. Toronto: University of Toronto Press. Nevins, M. Eleanor. 2013. Lessons from Fort Apache: Beyond Language Endangerment and Maintenance. Malden: Wiley-Blackwell. Nevins, M. Eleanor & Thomas Nevins. 2004. He Became an Eagle. In Brian Swann (ed.), Voices from the Four Directions, 283−302. Lincoln: Nebraska UP. Nevins, M. Eleanor & Thomas Nevins. 2012. They Don’t Know How to Ask: Pedagogy, Storytelling, and the Ironies of Language Endangerment on the White Mountain Apache Reservation. In Paul Kroskrity (ed.), Telling Stories in the Face of Danger: Language Renewal in Native American Communities, 129−150. Norman: University of Oklahoma Press. O’Connor, Mary Catherine. 1990. Third-Person Reference in Northern Pomo Conversation: The Indexing of Discourse Genre and Social Relations. IJAL 56(3). 377−409. O’Neill, Sean. 2006. Mythic and Poetic Dimensions of Speech in Northwestern California: From Cultural Vocabulary to Linguistic Relativity. Anthropological Linguistics 48(4). 305−334. O’Neill, Sean. 2012. The Politics of Storytelling in Northwestern California: Ideology, Identity, and Maintaining Narrative Distinction in the Face of Cultural Convergence. In Paul Kroskrity (ed.), Telling Stories in the Face of Danger: Language Renewal in Native American Communities, 60−89. Norman: University of Oklahoma Press. O’Neill, Sean. 2015. Translating Oral Literature in Indigenous Societies: Ethnic Aesthetic Performances in Multicultural and Multilingual Settings. In Paul Kroskrity & Anthony Webster (eds.), The Legacy of Dell Hymes: Ethnopoetics, Narrative Inequality, and Voice, 206−239. Bloomington: Indiana University Press. O’Neill, Sean. 2016. The politics of language contact in northwestern California: Maintaining diversity in the face of cultural convergence. International Journal of the Sociology of Language 240. 53−85. O’Neill, Sean. 2017. The Howling Muse: Chasing Coyote Tales in Northwestern California. Cahiers de littérature orale 81. 135−165. Palmer, Andie. 2005. Maps of Experience: The Anchoring of Land to Story in Secwepemc Discourse. Toronto: University of Toronto Press. Palmer Jr., Gus. 2012. Kiowa Stories Express Tribal Memory, Ideology and Being. In Paul Kroskrity (ed.), Telling Stories in the Face of Danger: Language Renewal in Native American Communities, 23−43. Norman: University of Oklahoma Press. Palmer Jr., Gus (ed.). 2018. When Dream Bear Sings: Native Literatures of the Southern Plains. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Parks, Douglas & Raymond DeMallie. 1994. Plains Indian Native Literatures. In Karl Kroeber (ed.), American Indian Persistence and Resurgence, 106−148. Durham: Duke UP. Peterson, Leighton C. & Anthony K. Webster. 2013. Speech Play and Language Ideologies in Navajo Terminology Development. Pragmatics 23(1). 93−116. Preston, Richard. 2002. Cree Narrative. 2nd edn. Montreal: Mc-Gill-Queen’s University Press. Radin, Paul. 1915. Literary Aspects of North American Mythology. Ottawa: Government Printing Bureau. Radin, Paul. 1926. Literary Aspects of Winnebago Mythology. Journal of American Folklore 39(151). 18−52. Radin, Paul. 1956. The Trickster. New York: Schocken Press. Richland, Justin. 2010. ‘They Did It Like a Song’: Ethics, Aesthetics, and Tradition in Hopi Legal Discourse. In Michael Lambek (ed.), Ordinary Ethics: Anthropology, Language, and Action, 249−270. New York: Fordham University Press. Ridington, Robin & Jillian Ridington. 2006. When You Sing It Now, Just Like New: First Nations Poetics, Voices, and Representations. Lincoln: Nebraska UP. Ridington, Robin, Jillian Ridington, Patrick Moore, Kate Hennessy & Amber Ridington. 2011. Ethnopoetic Translation in Relation to Audio, Video, and New Media Representations. In Brian Swann (ed.), Born in the Blood: On Native American Translation, 211−241. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Rigsby, Bruce. 1970. A Note on Gitksan Speech-Play. IJAL 36(3). 212−215.

418 

 Anthony K. Webster

Samuels, David. 2004. Putting a Song on Top of It. Tucson: Arizona UP. Samuels, David. 2009. Singing Indian Country. In Tara Browner (ed.), Music of the First Nations, 141−159. Urbana: University of Illinois Press. Sapir, Edward. 1910. Song Recitative in Paiute Mythology. Journal of American Folklore 23(90). 455−472. Sapir, Edward. 1915. Abnormal Types of Seech in Nootka. Canada, Geological Survey, Memoir 62, Anthropological Series No. 5. Ottawa: Government Printing Bureau. Sapir, Edward. 1932. Two Navajo Puns. Language 8. 217−219. Sarris, Greg. 1993. Keeping Slug Woman Alive. Berkeley: California UP. Scales, Christopher. 2012. Recording Culture: Powwow Music and the Aboriginal Recording Industry on the Northern Plains. Durham: Duke University Press. Scollon, Ronald. 1976. The Framing of Chipewyan Narrative Performances: Titles, initials and finals. Working Papers in Linguistics 7(4). 97−107. Department of Linguistics, University of Hawaii. Scollon, Ronald. 1977. Two Discourse markers in Chipewyan. IJAL 43(1). 60−64. Scollon, Ronald. 1979. The Context of the Informant Narrative Performance. Canadian Ethnology Paper 52. Ottawa: National Museum of Man. Scollon, Ronald. 1985. The Sequencing of Clauses in Chipewyan narrative. In Johanna Nichols & Anthony Woodbury (eds.), Grammar inside and outside the clause, 113−131. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Scollon, Ronald & Suzanne Scollon. 1979. Linguistic Convergence: An Ethnography of Speaking at Fort Chipewyan. New York: Academic Press. Sekaquaptewa, Emory & Dorothy Washburn. 2004. They go along singing: Reconstructing the Hopi Past from Ritual Metaphors in Song and Image. American Antiquity 69(3). 457−486. Shepard, Alice. 1997. In My Own Words: Stories, Songs and Memories of Grace McKibbin, Wintu. Berkeley, CA: Heyday Press. Sherzer, Joel & Anthony Woodbury (eds.). 1987. Native American Discourse: Poetics and Rhetoric. Cambridge: Cambridge UP. Shimkin, Dmitri. 1947. Wind River Shoshone Literary Forms. Journal Washington Academy of Sciences 37. 329−352. Shipley, William. 1988. Maidu literary style. In William Shipley (ed.), In Honor of Mary Haas, 705−714. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Swann, Brian (ed.) 1983. Smoothing the Ground. Berkeley: California UP. Swann, Brian (ed.). 1992. On the Translation of Native American Literatures. Washington D.C.: Smithsonian Press. Swann, Brian (ed.). 1994. Coming To Light. New York: Vintage Press. Swann, Brian (ed.). 2004. Voices from the Four Directions. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Swann, Brian (ed.). 2005. Algonquian Spirit. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Swann, Brian (ed.). 2011. Born in the Blood: On Native American Translation. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Swann, Brian (ed.). 2014. Sky Loom: Native American Myth, Story, and Song. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Swann, Brian & Arnold Krupat (eds.). 1987. Recovering the Word. Berkeley: California UP. Tedlock, Barbara. 1980. Songs of the Zuni Kachina Society: Composition, Rehearsal, and Performance. In Charlotte Frisbie (ed.), Southwestern Indian Ritual Drama, 7−36. Albuquerque: School of American Research. Tedlock, Dennis. 1988. The Witches Were Saved: A Zuni Origin Story. Journal of American Folklore 101(401). 312−320. Tedlock, Dennis. 2002. Written in Sound: Translating the Multiple Voices of the Zuni Storyteller. In Marc Shell (ed.), American Babel: Literatures of the United States from Abnaki to Zuni, 238−260. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Thornton, Thomas. 2008. Being and Place Among the Tlingit. Seattle: University of Washington Press.



Verbal art 

 419

Toelken, Barre. 1987. Life and Death in Navajo Coyote tales. In Brian Swann & Arnold Krupat (eds.), Recovering the Word, 388−401. Berkeley: California UP. Toelken, Barre. 1996. From Entertainment to Realization in Navajo Fieldwork. In Bruce Jackson & Edward D. Ives (eds.), The World Observed: Reflections on the Fieldwork Process, 1−17. Urbana: University of Illinois Press. Toelken, Barre. 2003. The Anguish of Snails. Logan: Utah State UP. Toelken, Barre. 2004. Beauty Behind Me; Beauty Before. Journal of American Folklore 117(466). 441−445. Tuttle, Siri. 2011. Language and music in the songs of Minto, Alaska. In Jan-Olof Svantesson, Niclas Burenhult, Arthur Holmer, Anastasia Karlsson & Håkan Lundström (eds.), Language Documentation and Description (Volume 10), 82−112. London: Hans Rusing Endangered Languages Project. Underhill, Ruth. 1934. Vocabulary and Style in an Indian Language. American Speech 9(4). 279−282. Underhill, Ruth. 1993. Singing for Power: The Song Magic of the Papago Indians of Southern Arizona. Tucson: Arizona UP. Underhill, Ruth, Donald Bahr, Baptisto Lopez, Jose Pancho & David Lopez. 1979. Rainhouse and Ocean: Speeches for the Papago Year. Flagstaff: Museum of Northern Arizona. Valentine, J. Randolph. 1996. ‘Amik Anicinaabewigoban’: Rhetorical Stuctures in Albert Mowatt’s Telling of an Algonquin Tale. In John Nichols & Arden Ogg (eds.), nikotwâsik iskwâhtêm, pâskihtêpayih! Studies in Honor of H. Christoph Wolfart, 387−428. (Algonquian and Iroquoian Linguistics, Memoir 13). Winnipeg, Manitoba: Algonquian and Iroquoian Linguistics. Vander, Judith. 1982. The Song Repertoire of Four Shoshone Women: A Reflection of Cultural Movements and Sex Roles. Ethnomusicology 26(1). 73−83. Vander, Judith. 1997. Shoshone Ghost Dance Religion: Poetry Songs and Great Basin Context. Urbana: University of Illinois Press. Webster, Anthony K. 1999. Lisandro Mendez’s ‘Coyote and Deer’: On narrative structures, reciprocity, and interactions. American Indian Quarterly 23(1). 1−24. Webster, Anthony K. 2006. On speaking to him (Coyote): The Discourse functions of the yi-/bi- alternation in some Chiricahua Apache narratives. Southwest Journal of Linguistics 25(2). 143−160. Webster, Anthony K. 2008. Running Again, Roasting Again, Touching Again: On repetition, heightened affective expressivity and the utility of linguaculture in Navajo and beyond. Journal of American Folklore 121(482). 441−472. Webster, Anthony K. 2015. Intimate Grammars: An Ethnography of Navajo Poetry. Tucson: University of Arizona Press. Webster, Anthony K. 2015. The poetry of sound and the sound of poetry: Navajo poetry, phonological iconicity, and linguistic relativity. Semiotica 207. 279−301. Webster, Anthony K. 2016. The Art of Failure in Translating a Navajo Poem. Journal de la Société des Américanistes 102(1). 9−41. Webster, Anthony K. 2017. ‘So It’s Got Three Meanings dil dil:’ Seductive ideophony and the sounds of Navajo poetry. Canadian Journal of Linguistics/Revue canadienne de linguistique 62(2). 173−195. Wiget, Andrew. 1980. Sayatasha’s Night Chant: A Literary Textual Analysis of a Zuni Ritual Poem. American Indian Culture and Research Journal 4(1&2). 99−140. Woodbury, Anthony. 1995. The Poetics and Rhetoric of Conversational Overlap in a Sample of Yup’ik Men’s House Speech. Texas Linguistic Forum 36. 77−97. Woodbury, Anthony. 2019. He suffocates me: A Playful dimension of exact transcription, and of being an iluraq. [Special issue]. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology. 29(2): 155−160. Zepeda, Ofelia. 1982. O’odham Ha-Cegitodag/Pima and Papago Thoughts. IJAL 48(3). 320−326. Zepeda, Ofelia. 1983. A Papago Grammar. Tucson: Arizona UP. Zepeda, Ofelia. 1995. Ocean Power. Tucson: Arizona UP. Zepeda, Ofelia. 1997. Jewed ‘I-hoi: Earth Movements. Tucson: Kore Press. Zolbrod, Paul. 1984. Diné Bahane. Albuquerque: New Mexico UP.

Olivia N. Sammons

18 Conversation structure Abstract: Everyday conversation represents a core facet of human interaction and expression, and its documentation and analysis provide the potential for a richer understanding of language. Conversation is also increasingly important to language revitalization, where conversational proficiency and the expansion of language usage into increased domains is often a primary goal (Amery 2009; Miyashita & Hirata-Edds forthcoming). Nevertheless, everyday conversation has remained both underdocumented and understudied for many Indigenous languages, representing a significant gap in the documentary record (Amery 2009; Berge 2010). The documentation and analysis of everyday conversation in North American Indigenous languages has only recently begun to receive more dedicated attention, in part due to recent advances in recording technologies, increasing emphasis on documentary representativeness (Biber 1993; Himmelmann 2006; Sankoff 1988), and growing recognition of the importance of connected, spontaneous speech in interactive contexts (Mithun 2001). This chapter provides an overview of recent developments in this area, surveying several conversation documentation projects in North America and considering the relevance of conversation to linguistic research and language revitalization.

18.1 Introduction This chapter discusses conversation structure in the Indigenous languages of North America. While many North American Indigenous languages and language families have an appreciable tradition of documentation and description, the study of conversation is noticeably absent from most such sources, mentioned only in passing, if at all. This may be due in part to the position of conversation outside of the classic Boasian trilogy of grammars, texts, and dictionaries that has influenced the documentation of North American Indigenous languages. However, this has recently begun to change, as evidenced by recent panels organized by the Committee on Endangered Languages and their Preservation and the Linguistic Society of America (Documenting conversation, 2014) and by the American Anthropological Association (Interactional approaches to language documentation, 2015), in which several presentations focused on conversation in North American languages. This chapter aims to cast a wide net over such research, providing examples of a range of approaches to understanding conversation in North American Indigenous languages.

https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-018

422 

 Olivia N. Sammons

18.2 The importance of conversation Conversation forms an important part of the overall documentation of a language because it provides rich examples of how people communicate with one another in their everyday lives, a central element of the human experience. Beyond that, everyday talk is often of a different nature than what is found in lexical and grammatical elicitation and single-speaker narratives, and can help reveal what is unique about a language (Mithun forthcoming).

18.2.1 Importance for language description and linguistic theory Conversation presents an opportunity to observe linguistic phenomena that are often unattested or that would otherwise be difficult to observe through other methods. For example, certain important construction types, such as imperatives, hortatives, interjections, evidential markers, and referring expressions, are much more robustly represented in conversation than in monologues and elicitation, where declaratives predominate (Mithun 2014). Similarly, the prosody found in conversational speech is typically more complex and multifaceted than that found in monologues or elicited speech, which involve the careful pronunciation of words and/or sentences in isolation (Mithun 2001: 36–7). Beyond individual grammatical phenomena, conversation also allows us to observe how discourse is structured in a given language (e.  g., how turn-taking and repair are negotiated), as well as the role of other paralinguistic features, such as gaze and gesture, as discussed in Section 4 below.1 One aspect of grammar that conversation is especially helpful in illustrating is the use of evidential markers, which indicate the source and reliability of the information being conveyed in the utterance. These can be seen in the following excerpt from a spontaneous conversation in Central Pomo, a Pomoan language from northern California: (1)        

Central Pomo inferential evidential: Florence Paoli, speaker (Mithun 2001: 46) Shirleywet̪ ̓ našóyya ʔdúč̓ka… ̓ Shirley=wet̪ našóy=ya ʔdú-č̓-ʔ=ka Shirley=poss young.lady=new.topic marry-sml-prf=inferential ‘He must have married Shirley’s daughter’ [That’s why I didn’t understand at first.]

1 In fact, Nyman & Leer (1993: xxii) argue that a “rich inventory of hand gestures[,] (…) eye and head movements and changes of posture” represent part of the linguistic system of Tlingit, showing noteworthy differences from the gestural system of English.



       

Conversation structure 

Mu·lʔkaman Shirleywet̪ ̓ našóy Mu·l=ʔ=ka=man Shirley=wet̪ ̓ našóy that=cop=inferential=that Shirley=poss young.lady ‘It must have been Shirley’s daughter he married’

 423

ʔdúč̓ ʔdú-č̓-ʔ marry-sml-prf

Here, we see that the inferential evidential =ka is used twice, both in ʔdúč̓ka… ‘must have married’ and mu·lʔkaman ‘must have been’. In both cases, this marker indicates that the speaker is inferring the information that is being conveyed, rather than having firsthand knowledge or experience of it. Central Pomo has a range of markers such as these, some of which are listed in Table 1: Tab. 1: Some Central Pomo evidentials (from Mithun 2001: 45–7) Evidential Marker

Meaning

=ka =ma =ya =la

inferential (i.  e., the speaker is inferring the information being conveyed) factual (i.  e., the information is established general knowledge) experiential (i.  e., the speaker witnessed the event firsthand) performative (i.  e., the speaker performed the action being expressed)

As one can imagine, markers such as these are often difficult to elicit because their use is largely unconscious. When asked directly to translate English prompts from which approximate their meaning (e.  g., ‘I heard that…’ or ‘It is a known fact that…’, etc.), speakers tend to use whole sentences or clauses, rather than using the markers themselves (Mithun 2001: 47). Conversation is thus useful in allowing markers such as these to be observed in their natural context, without the interference of translation from English or another lingua franca. The constructional richness of conversational data noted by Mithun (2001; 2014) has practical consequences for language description as well, often contributing to more empirically adequate analyses. For example, in Potawatomi, an Algonquian language with roots in the Great Lakes region, previous studies of clause typing that were based largely on narrative arrived at the conclusion that one particular inflectional paradigm commonly associated with dependent clauses (i.  e., the conjunct order) was essentially the default in the language, leaving only a limited role for other inflectional paradigms (specifically the independent order). Buszard-Welcher (2003; 2010), however, notes a much different distribution when considering conversational data. She found that the conjunct order appeared much less frequently than had been previously reported, and argued that it was in fact a special verbal mode characteristic of narrative, but not of Potawatomi speech in general.2 In this case, consideration of examples of conversation 2 Although the previous researcher, Charles Hockett, did acknowledge that there were differences between narrative and conversational style, this is not mentioned in his work other than as a brief note,

424 

 Olivia N. Sammons

served to enhance the overall description of Potawatomi, presenting a fuller characterization of the range of functions and usage contexts in which both inflectional paradigms are attested.3 These same kinds of imbalances in the documentary record can also have consequences for the characterization of entire languages. For example, Michif, an Indigenous contact language spoken in Canada and the United States, is commonly described as having a split in its lexicon, whereby nouns are derived from French and verbs from Plains Cree (Bakker 1997: 3). As Sammons (forthcoming; 2019) notes, however, this generalization is based almost entirely on elicited, introspective, and monologic sources, with essentially no representation from interactive speech. These types of sources tend to be highly monitored by speakers, who pay close attention to their performance and aim to ensure that it conforms to speech norms (e.  g., avoiding code switching, as well as constructions and/or pronunciations which might not be considered within the speech community to be “proper” or “good” language). When more casual, interactive speech is brought into consideration, however, a more complex distribution of source languages across Michif constructions is observed:

and the differences were not explored any further, presumably due to limitations of time and recording capability (Buszard-Welcher 2010: 70). 3 It should be noted that the results reported here are specific to Potawatomi, and should not necessarily be assumed to be true of other Algonquian languages. Some studies of clause-typing systems in other Algonquian languages have come to different results. For instance, Starks (1994) found that conjunct verbs were used more frequently in main clauses than independent order verbs in both conversation and narrative in Woods Cree: Tab.: Distribution of inflectional paradigms in main clauses in Woods Cree (after Starks 1994: 305)  

Conversation

Narrative

Independent Conjunct Imperative Total

 45 %  48 %  14 % 100 %

 23 %  75 %   2 % 100 %

Initial investigations of Plains Cree have found a similar distribution (Mithun, p.c.). As one reviewer suggested, this may be in part due to language contact, as the previous studies of Potawatomi mentioned here (e.  g., Hockett 1939; Hockett 1948a; Hockett 1948b, inter alia) were based on narratives collected in the late 1930s and 1940s, while Buszard-Welcher’s analysis is based on data collected in the late 1990s and early 2000s (Buszard 2003). It is possible that, in earlier stages of the language, the conjunct may have been used for a much wider range of discourse purposes, many of which did not align with English subordination, and that, over time, a rise in bilingualism and contact with English has reduced the range of functions for which the conjunct is used. Without historical conversational data which could be compared against contemporary usage, however, it is difficult to explore this hypothesis further.



Conversation structure 

(2)    

Michif (2013-07-24: Sammons, forthcoming) Lawrence Fleury: So, taanshi o mataeñ?   ‘So, how are you this morning?’

   

Mervin Fleury:  

Taret, taret, tama sii maeñ ka-pakamaham. ‘Stop, stop, she will clap (lit: hit) her hands first.’

   

   

Ok, ka-maatstaanaan la. ‘Ok, we’ll start now.’

 425

In (2), the French-origin command form taret ‘stop’ is used rather than a Cree-origin verb form, which would be expected under the split lexicon characterization. In addition, it is not atypical to find French-origin lexical items being used as verb stems in interactive speech, with otherwise Cree-origin morphology, as shown in (3): (3)    

Cree (Norman Fleury; 2013–08–24) ka-lizheuniwiiyaan ‘when I was young’

In this example, the lexical content of the verb zheun is derived from French jeune ‘young, youth’, rather than an equivalent verb form derived from Cree. Just as importantly, spoken corpus data reveal no shortage of English-origin lexical items participating in these same constructions, as shown in (4): (4)      

Cree (Mervin Fleury; 2013–07–17) ka-learniiyen li kachim avik ayi, when.you’re.learning the catechism with uh ‘when you’re learning catechism with the sisters’

lii, the

lii the

soer sisters

In this example, the English-origin form learn is being used as the verb stem and appears with Cree-origin verbal affixes. Although instances such as these do occasionally occur in non-interactive speech, as noted by Bakker (1997: 114–116), they are generally treated as being marginal or atypical. They are much more difficult to ignore in conversational speech, however, suggesting that this association between nouns, verbs, and their source languages may not be as clear-cut as one would otherwise be led to believe. This characterization has a direct impact on the overall typological profile of Michif, particularly in relation to other contact languages, where the composition of the lexicon is often one important factor in cross-linguistic comparison (cf. Michaelis et al. 2013). As we have seen, conversation is helpful in constructing a well-rounded, overall picture of a language, which has consequences for linguistic theory and description. When linguistic descriptions are based on a representative sample of how a language is used in practice across a wide sample of speech situations and genres—including conversation—we are more likely to observe phenomena which may be less attested in other forms of speech. Resulting descriptions will then be more accurate and can also

426 

 Olivia N. Sammons

reveal what is most typical or frequent in a language, which is helpful in determining what should be given priority in language learning (cf. Himmelmann 2006: 7 on issues of representativeness in language documentation in general).

18.2.2 Importance for language revitalization, education, and wellness Beyond the descriptive, typological, and theoretical implications discussed above, documentation of conversation is also relevant to language revitalization and education. As noted by Levinson (1983: 284), conversation is “the form in which we are all first exposed to language—the matrix for language acquisition”. From a practical perspective, spontaneous, interactive speech between fluent first-language speakers represents exactly the kind of language that many learners want to be exposed to, learn from, and emulate. In terms of content, conversation provides a window into what speakers talk about in their everyday lives, as well as topics (cultural or otherwise) that might not have arisen through other documentation activities (Mithun forthcoming). Beyond showing what speakers say, conversation also shows us how they choose to say it. That is, conversation provides ample, contextualized examples of a host of grammatical features, frequent collocations, formulaic language, idiomatic expressions, and the conventional use of sentence fragments, while also exposing learners to native pronunciation and prosodic patterns. Conversation is also important for learners on a social level, providing models of linguistic and cultural practice as they work to establish their identity as emerging speakers (Sammons forthcoming). It is also becoming increasingly apparent that natural conversation can be helpful in the realm of language education. Hermes, Bang, and Marin (2012) discuss how conversation is used to incorporate culture into a language curriculum, stating that conversation can be particularly helpful to educators in creating realistic discourses that can be used outside of the classroom, as opposed to artificially-constructed “school talk”. This is especially true when such production takes place within the community and actively involves heritage learners (389). Finally, recent years have seen a growing awareness of the link between Indigenous language use and wellness. Preliminary studies and reports have shown that there are physical, mental, and emotional health benefits that can be derived from Indigenous language use and learning (see, e.  g., Jenni et al. 2017; Holton 2015; McIvor 2013; Oster et al. 2014; Whalen, Moss & Baldwin 2016, among others). Many of the contexts in which these wellness benefits are realized are ones that involve or are enriched by conversational interaction. For example, by their very nature, Master-Apprentice programs, which pair adult learners with fluent Elders to complete various activities conducted in the language (Hinton 2002), involve a high degree of conversational interaction. Likewise, language nests, which provide immersive environments for young children with the goal of creating new speakers, are also spaces characterized largely by spontaneous,



Conversation structure 

 427

natural interaction. Other important wellness benefits associated with Indigenous language use are the result of a sense of community and identity that is fostered through language transmission and continuity efforts (Taff et al. 2018: 863; Whalen, Moss & Baldwin 2016: 3). Again, these kinds of communal ties are often centered around conversation. In a similar way, there is also a strong desire on the part of learners to be able to interact with Elders, to “communicate cross-generationally for love and understanding” (Taff et al. 2018: 873), which also requires some degree of conversational proficiency in the language. While different language initiatives may involve different forms and genres of language use, conversation increasingly plays a key role in realizing many communities’ language reclamation goals, which, in turn, helps to address some of the effects of trauma caused by language oppression (Taff et al. 2018: 873) and foster greater individual and community well-being. Despite these many benefits, conversation is often the least documented speech genre, leaving a considerable gap in the record when language learners and teachers wish to consider how mundane topics and ideas are expressed. A desire for examples of authentic conversation in their languages has been voiced repeatedly by Indigenous learners, as it is precisely this ordinary, day-to-day language that learners most need to be able to express themselves in their daily lives (Amery 2009: 142; Taff 2013; Grant & Ahlers forthcoming; Sammons forthcoming). These perspectives from Indigenous language revitalization and education are increasingly reflected in language documentation recommendations, with many practitioners now advocating for the planned inclusion of a wide range of communicative activities and situational language routines (cf. Himmelmann 2006; Himmelmann 2008; Amery 2009). The reasons for this imbalance in the documentary record, however, are not hard to see. In many cases, the logistics of recording conversation can be challenging. Meetings between speakers may need to be facilitated, sometimes across significant geographical distances (Sammons 2019); individuals who no longer use the language on a regular basis may find it difficult to maintain a conversation (McCreery 2015); and elderly speakers may not have the stamina to participate in interactions for extended periods of time. In addition, everyday talk is often seen as not being worthy of documentation in comparison to myths, traditional stories, and cultural texts—both by speakers and by researchers—because it is so commonplace (Amery 2009: 138). Conversation is also typically more complex than other forms of speech, often filled with speech overlaps, turn-taking, false starts, hesitations, and the like across multiple speakers, making it much more challenging and time-consuming to transcribe (Gaenszle 2010; Lüpke 2009; Rosenblum & Sammons 2014; Sammons forthcoming). Finally, technological barriers have also affected the documentation of interactional speech. Until the 1960s and 1970s, when audio recording equipment became more widely available, linguists primarily relied on dictation and/or texts written out by native speakers, methods which were limited in their ability to accurately capture the full character of spontaneous conversation (Boas 1917: 2; Buszard-Welcher 2010).

428 

 Olivia N. Sammons

18.3 Case studies in documenting conversation In this section, I present profiles of three recent projects which have documented conversational speech in an Indigenous language of North America—Tlingit (Section 3.1); Kawaiisu (Section 3.2); and Michif (Section 3.3).4 These descriptions aim to provide a sense of the scope that these kinds of conversation-focused projects generally have, as well as their motivations, methodologies, and challenges, for others who may be interested in embarking on similar projects. All three projects share broadly similar goals, aiming to record, transcribe, and translate some number of hours of conversation in the language; as well as to engage community members in different parts of this work, providing opportunities for both language learning and training in language documentation and linguistics. Even though these aims are generally similar across projects, the ways in which they are approached and realized may differ, as discussed below.

18.3.1 “Woosh een áyá yoo x̱’atudli.átk – We’re talking conversation” “Woosh een áyá yoo x̱’atudli.átk – We’re talking conversation” is a project that focused on documenting conversation in Tlingit, a Na-Dene language spoken in southeast Alaska and parts of Yukon and British Columbia, Canada.5 Although a large body of narrative, lexical, and grammatical documentation of the Tlingit language already existed prior to the start of this project, these materials included virtually no recordings of spontaneous, everyday conversations which were accessible to learners. At the same time, as of 2018, there were only 60–100 first-language Tlingit speakers, providing additional motivation to record fluent conversation among fluent speakers while this was still possible (Taff, Wallace & Mason forthcoming). One aspect of the Tlingit conversation project that sets it apart from many other documentation initiatives is the size and diversity of the group of contributors involved. In addition to the 61 Tlingit speakers who participated in recorded conversations and offered advice on transcription and translation, several dozen other members of the language community, academic partners, and university students contributed to project activities, from assisting with recording sessions, managing technology, and supporting 4 The three highlighted projects are only a few of the increasing number of conversation-focused initiatives involving Indigenous languages in North America. Though beyond the scope of this chapter, several other recent projects have focused on documenting conversation in North American Indigenous languages as well, including Ahtna (Mithun 2009)Aleut (Taff 2006), Arapaho (Cowell 2011), Blackfoot (Miyashita & Hirata-Edds Forthcoming; Bliss & Wiltschko 2018), Kʷakʷ’ala (Rosenblum 2015), Navajo (Mithun 2009; Eisman 2015; Navajo Language Academy), Ojibwe (Hermes 2012; Hermes et al. 2014; Hermes 2016), and Tsuut’ina (Starlight, Moore & Cox 2018), among others. See references for further information. 5 This project ran from 2007–2013 and was funded by two grants from the Documenting Endangered Languages (DEL) program of the U.S. National Science Foundation.



Conversation structure 

 429

the logistics of arranging meetings in communities throughout southeastern Alaska and the Yukon, to recommending Tlingit speakers to work with and conversation topics that were closely aligned with communities’ needs and interests. Given the ambitious scope of this project and the range of knowledge and abilities required, this broad base of participation proved important to the overall success of the project: no one group or individual had all the skills necessary to complete this work, but, working together, the project team was able to accomplish its goals. Recordings took place throughout the year in a total of ten communities. An effort was made to limit recording sessions to two or three speakers, as it was found that when more speakers were included, multiple conversations tended to take place simultaneously. This in turn made the recording significantly more difficult to understand and annotate due to speech overlap. After recordings were completed, two separate phases of annotation were conducted: the first concentrating on English translations, and the second on Tlingit transcriptions. Annotations were produced using ELAN (Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics 2020), a standard tool for time-aligned annotation in documentary linguistics. Using this software helped to ensure that the transcripts would be text-searchable for future users, and that certain portions of the recordings could be replayed as many times as users would like. As in other conversation projects, annotation represented a significant time investment: members of the project team found that it took on average one hour to translate five minutes of Tlingit conversation into English, while it took roughly one hour to transcribe one minute of Tlingit speech (cf. Rosenblum & Sammons 2014 for estimates on transcription time). One of the primary challenges encountered in the project was identifying speakers who would be willing to participate and helping them to feel comfortable enough to be recorded. Some speakers were often hesitant to participate because they no longer felt comfortable using the language, whether because they had previously been criticized for the way in which they spoke, or because they felt “rusty”. In many cases, it took multiple recording sessions to help speakers overcome any sort of uneasiness and feel comfortable enough to speak freely. In particular, the team found that some of these issues could be addressed by shifting the focus of speakers away from their central role in these conversations to a non-speaking participant, such as a baby. Not only did the presence of babies provide a welcome distraction for the speakers, but it also often gave rise to “baby talk”, a form of speech which is often underdocumented among Indigenous languages (Taff, Wallace & Mason forthcoming). Through this project, a total of 48.5 hours of conversation were recorded and annotated in both Tlingit and English. Subtitled videos of these recordings are available to Tlingit language learners and the wider public on the Tlingit Conversation website,6

6 https://www.uas.alaska.edu/arts_sciences/humanities/alaska-native-studies/alaska-native-languages/ tlingit-language-conversation-project.html

430 

 Olivia N. Sammons

with master copies of all materials housed in two archives. A sample from a conversation transcribed in ELAN is provided in Figure 1 below, where speakers G̱uneiwtí Marsha Hotch (left) and Kaaxkʼwei Evelyn Hotch (right) are discussing the use of mountain goat wool in weaving (Taff 2013; Taff, Wallace & Mason forthcoming):

Fig. 1: Tlingit conversation transcript sample (Taff, Wallace & Mason forthcoming)

Through this project, a core group of language community members developed skills in language documentation (e.  g., audiovisual recording techniques, transcription methods), archiving, and linguistics, while also improving their own Tlingit language proficiency. This project is also noteworthy in that it demonstrates the importance of including “super learners” (sometimes referred to as passive speakers, semi-speakers, fluent understanders, etc.) in documentation projects (Taff, Wallace & Mason forthcoming). These individuals are typically members of a generation that was exposed to the language at a very early age and came to understand it quite well, but whose range of production is limited. The Tlingit team observed that involving this group of individuals in the project helped them to make great strides in their language, with some even beginning to dream and speak in the language again. Finally, by being able to play the videos produced by the project on repeat, learners who would otherwise have limited exposure to fluent Tlingit speech were able to gain additional exposure without placing undue burden on the Elders. This project thus played a part in a larger, ongoing process of language healing:



Conversation structure 

 431

Documenting conversation is, itself, a forum for language use and language healing. Every time a language is used, the neural firings in the speakers’ and listeners’ brains are strengthened. Every conversation in a language makes it more likely that another conversation will happen. I believe in the value of documenting conversation. Yes, the stories. Yes, the songs. Yes, the oratory. But conversation, too, is not trivial; it is rich, from the heart. It is how we connect with each other to carry on our lives and our cultures (Taff, Wallace & Mason forthcoming).

18.3.2 “Kawaiisu conversations and landscapes” Beginning in 2012, members of the Kawaiisu community, led by the Kawaiisu Language and Cultural Center (KLCC), embarked on an 18-month project to document conversation in nuwa abigip, or Kawaiisu, a Uto-Aztecan language spoken in Kern County, California. This project was driven by an observed need in the community to address gaps in previous documentation, which had focused primarily on elicitation of lexical items and grammatical structure, as well as by a desire to help language learners increase their proficiency. At the time the project began, there were five remaining first-language speakers of Kawaiisu; this period thus represented a critical window in which it was still possible to document fluent, interactive conversation between native speakers (Grant & Ahlers forthcoming). The project team consisted of eight members, including three Elder native speakers, three learner/teachers, and two linguistic consultants. The project aimed to record conversations related to the landscape and history of the Kawaiisu people, specifically during the period of 1940–1985, when the Kawaiisu community was still somewhat geographically cohesive. Recording took place in various locations, including areas in which traditional adobe homes were built, traditional skills were practiced, gardens were cultivated, and siblings were born and raised (Grant & Ahlers forthcoming). During the sessions, video recordings were made from two vantage points to facilitate the observation of gestures and expressions in the context of conversation, and also to document the overall environment in which conversations were taking place. These recordings were then brought into ELAN for transcription, translation, and analysis by members of the project team. An example of a portion of a completed transcript is provided in Figure 2:

432 

 Olivia N. Sammons

Fig. 2: Kawaiisu conversation excerpt (Grant & Ahlers forthcoming)

Grant and Ahlers (forthcoming) estimate that it took approximately two hours for team members with a high degree of language proficiency to transcribe a single minute of recorded conversation. All project materials have been archived at the Survey of California and Other Indian Languages at the University of California, Berkeley, and a sample of these recorded conversations is available on the Kawaiisu Language and Cultural Center’s website. One challenge faced by team members was that the speakers lived at some distance from one another and were no longer accustomed to using the language on a daily basis. This led to conversations initially being somewhat stilted, often with long stretches in which English was primarily used. The team addressed this by arranging for recording sessions to take place on the traditional homeland of the Kawaiisu people, in locations which had historical and/or cultural significance. This resulted in much richer and more natural language use: Because the speakers were interacting with each other each month in places where, in their earlier lives, they always spoke Kawaiisu, their use of the language became more fluid, and they became more comfortable staying in Kawaiisu for longer periods of time (Grant & Ahlers forthcoming).

This project produced sixty hours of fluent Kawaiisu conversation, along with detailed transcriptions (including morphemic analysis) for eight of those hours. Another important outcome of the project was the development of stylesheets indicating the team’s protocol for transcription and analysis so that transcripts would be consistent regard-



Conversation structure 

 433

less of who was doing the transcription. These were developed and refined collaboratively throughout the project by Kawaiisu speakers, community researchers, and linguistic consultants. In addition to these more tangible outputs, major strides were made in the area of capacity development within the community, as community researchers were trained in the use of computers and relevant software, transcription methods, and linguistic analysis. Community researchers involved in recording and transcription also increased their language proficiency, and first-language Kawaiisu speakers became more comfortable using the language for longer periods of time. Finally, team members gained insight into the use and interpretation of Kawaiisu verbal suffixes, while also becoming more familiar and comfortable with the use of linguistic terminology (Grant & Ahlers forthcoming).

18.3.3 Michif conversation project Our final case study involves the documentation of conversation in Michif, an Indigenous contact language spoken by the Métis in western Canada and the northern United States. This multi-year project produced approximately 37 hours of audiovisual recordings of spontaneous Michif conversation in several different varieties and communities. As in the Tlingit and Kawaiisu projects, the Michif conversation project focused on recording natural discourse involving small groups of speakers in community-based settings. Elder speakers were consulted in determining who to involve in these conversation sessions and which topics should be covered. Once recordings were completed, time-aligned annotation of these conversations was undertaken in both oral and written form using ELAN and SayMore software (Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics 2020; Brugman & Russel 2004; SIL International 2019). An excerpt from one such transcript is provided in Figure 3, representing a conversation between speakers Harvey Pelletier (left), Norman Fleury (right), and Mervin Fleury (middle), who is sharing a humorous childhood story:

434 

 Olivia N. Sammons

Fig. 3: Michif conversation excerpt (from Sammons 2015a, session dmv-2012-10-13-03; http://hdl.handle. net/2196/00-0000-0000-000F-59FA-4)

Two interesting features of this project include the use of conversation facilitators and conversation prompts, both of which were implemented in part to help conversation flow freely between speakers in the language. One to two first-language speakers of Michif would typically facilitate recording sessions, helping to address any lulls in the conversation and ensuring that speakers stayed in the language as much as possible. In some cases, Elders had specific topics in mind to discuss (often relating to Métis culture and history), and in others, the conversations were much more open-ended. There were, however, times when it was initially difficult to get conversation going. When this occurred, prompts were provided in the form of StoryCorps questions.7 This would often help to jumpstart a conversation between speakers, which then continued quite naturally. Preliminary analysis of these conversations not only reveals the use of novel imperative forms in the language, but also is also being used to inform our understanding of Michif nominal classification systems (Sammons 2019). In this section, we have seen conversation projects operating in a number of Indigenous communities and language families. These projects have adopted a range of approaches and sizes of teams, from groups of a hundred or more collaborators (Tlingit), to individual researchers working in collaboration with Elders and community language activists (Michif). Virtually all of these projects have produced both audio and video recordings of conversations, placing particular emphasis on video as a crucial

7 See https://storycorps.org/great-questions/ for further information about the questions recommended by StoryCorps. Thanks to Mary Linn for suggesting this resource.



Conversation structure 

 435

component of documentation in providing information not only about the physical and social contexts in which these interactions take place, but also about conventionalized uses of gesture, gaze, facial expressions, and more. In some cases, conversations proceeded on their own, while in others, prompts were provided. Such projects demonstrate that the challenges that are typically involved in documenting conversation can often be overcome by creative and collaborative means—and the outcomes of this work suggest that this is well worth doing.

18.4 What can we learn from conversation in North American languages? Once documentation of conversation has been developed, as in the preceding projects, there is much that can be learned from the careful study of these materials from a range of distinct analytical perspectives. As Basso (1988: 123) notes, The resources of a language, together with the varieties of action facilitated by their use, acquire meaning and force from the sociocultural contexts in which they are embedded, and therefore, as every linguist knows, the discourse of any speech community will exhibit a fundamental character—a genius, a spirit, an underlying personality—which is very much its own.

In this section, we discuss several studies which examine conversation in Indigenous languages of North America within a range of traditions, including conversation analysis (4.1), Indigenous ways of learning and interacting (4.2), and ethnography (4.3).8

8 It should be noted that the overview presented here is not meant to be comprehensive, but instead to highlight some recent studies in this area, as well as the kinds of information that can be observed through conversation. While conversation has typically received less attention than traditional narratives in the literature, there is nevertheless an important history of research on conversation which covers multiple language groups. Some works of note include Woodbury (1995), which examines speech overlap in Yup’ik; Kroskrity (1993), which includes an examination of code-switching in Tewa conversation; Field (2007), which explores increments in Navajo conversation, O’Connor (1990), which investigates third person reference in Northern Pomo conversation, and Spielmann (2017), which examines interactional patterns in Ojibwe.

436 

 Olivia N. Sammons

18.4.1 Conversation analysis Conversation analysis (CA) emerged in the 1960s as an approach to “describe, analyze and understand talk as a basic and constitutive feature of human social life” (Sidnell 2010: 1). CA is empirical in nature, placing a high priority on naturally occurring data which are systematically recorded, transcribed, and analyzed to discover the organizational properties of talk across turns between speakers. Some areas that CA is concerned with include how participants take and organize turns (turn-taking), identify and repair problems (conversational repair), and assign and realize actions (action formation) within an interaction.9 Recent studies, such as the ones discussed below, have begun to apply this framework to discourse in North American Indigenous languages, offering insights into how conversation in these languages is organized.

18.4.1.1 Topic shift, gesture, and language ideologies in Arapaho One aspect of conversation which CA allows us to observe is the interplay between speech and other semiotic resources such as gaze, body positioning, pointing, and gesture. Several studies exploring the dynamics of Arapaho conversation in relation to these points have taken place in recent years.10 One such study, Cowell (forthcoming), uses a multi-modal approach to examine topic shift and initiation in Arapaho conversation, with several noteworthy preliminary findings. One such finding relates to body position. When given the option, Arapaho speakers consistently positioned themselves adjacent to one another rather than facing one another directly, such that their gaze was shared towards a triangulated point, as shown in Figure 4 below:

9 See Clift (2016), Have (2007), Hutchby & Wooffitt (1998), Schegloff (2007), Sidnell (2010), and Sidnell & Stivers (2012), among others, for further reading on CA. 10 The studies described here draw on examples from the Arapaho Conversational Database (Cowell 2011), which includes video documentation of over thirty hours of Arapaho discourse across more than 60 speakers, from 2000–present. Some of the conversations recorded happened organically, occurring in various locations on the Wind River Reservation in Wyoming. Many recordings took place in areas where the language was often spoken (e.  g., the tribal college, preschool immersion programs, community meetings, ceremonies, and friends visiting one another). In other cases, arrangements were made for conversations to take place between speakers. The participants tended to know one another prior to the recording, either as acquaintances, friends, or family members. Further information on this corpus, which is accessible online through the Endangered Languages Archive (ELAR), University of London, at http://elar.soas.ac.uk/deposit/0194, can be found in Cowell (forthcoming). Other studies based on this corpus include Sandoval (2013) and (2016), which both explore the relationship between gesture and speech in Arapaho.



Conversation structure 

 437



a. b. Fig. 4: Default gaze position (Cowell forthcoming)

The two images pictured above are from two separate conversations. Upon further observation, it became apparent that this adjacent configuration was the default, “home” position (Sacks & Schegloff 2002) for Arapaho speakers when interacting with one another. Speakers occasionally departed from this position when responding to topic shifts or initiations, or when problems arose in the conversation (e.  g., dispreferred responses to topic shifts and/or initiations), but they promptly returned to it after the issue was resolved. What is significant is that this conversational behavior shows a marked difference from typical Euro-American and wider cross-linguistic norms, but it shows similarities to conversational behavior that has been described for speakers of Tzeltal Mayan (Cowell forthcoming, citing; Rossano 2013). Cowell (forthcoming) also identifies correlations between lexical and syntactic devices used to introduce and/or change topics, as well as the use of gaze in response to these topic shifts. While topic initiations and shifts are typically accomplished verbally in Arapaho, verbal responses to these shifts are rare, with addressees instead performing a shift in gaze. Cowell further observes that this lack of obligation on the part of addressees to provide a verbal response to a topic shift is cross-linguistically unusual, and thus serves as an example of a ‘non-canonical’ sequence initiation (forthcoming). Cowell (2015) also considers conversational data in relation to ideologies around the avoidance of both pointing and direct eye contact in Indigenous language communities in North America. Despite commonly-held ideologies to the contrary, examination of a single minute of recorded video conversation from the Arapaho Conversational Database (Cowell 2011) shows multiple instances of Arapaho speakers using pointing to various ends (e.  g., to refer to themselves, addressees, third person referents, and locations) (Cowell 2015). Likewise, multiple instances of speakers making direct eye contact with one another can also be found in the database. Such instances demonstrate that both pointing and direct eye contact can not only be observed, but can perhaps even be considered to be a regular part of Arapaho discourse. Cowell does note, however, that the functions that pointing and making direct eye contact serve are particular to

438 

 Olivia N. Sammons

Arapaho and do not necessarily align with those employed by individuals operating within a North American English framework.11 Further, Cowell finds that both pointing and direct eye contact are linked to moments in the discourse in which affirmation is being sought and/or miscommunication has occurred. For example, while a minor glance appears to be the preferred response to a polar question in Arapaho, Cowell finds two types of cases in which an addressee instead makes eye contact with a speaker. In the first, the speaker appears to be seeking either affirmation of a claim or position, or permission to act from the addressee. In the second, there is a polar question that is misunderstood, unexpected, and/or results in unanticipated follow-up questions. Both types of cases result in more marked shifts in gaze, typically involving sustained eye contact with the addressee (Cowell 2015; Cowell forthcoming). Thus, while it is true that unmarked discourse in Arapaho does not involve direct eye contact between speakers, the assumption that using direct eye contact in conversation signals confrontation or an act of aggression, as has often been suggested, is not accurate either. Rather, Arapaho speakers make use of eye contact as an interactional resource when seeking “mutual support and affirmation, and/or re-establishing shared stances or epistemic positions”. Thus, rather than being used to incite conflict, eye contact “is fundamentally an effort to repair the situation and avoid such conflict” (Cowell forthcoming). The combined preliminary results of these studies demonstrate that there are established, conventionalized patterns in Arapaho discourse which differ from North American English norms and provide evidence that brings commonly held language ideologies into question. In both cases, the result is a more refined and accurate characterization of the language and how it is used in practice. Finally, it is worth noting that in all of the examples provided here, the interrelationship of verbal speech with semiotic resources such as body position, gaze, pointing, and eye contact is very much at play, thus underscoring the need for video documentation which allows for the observation of multimodality (Cowell forthcoming).

18.4.1.2 Self-repair in Wichita Mirzayan (2008) examines self-repair initiation strategies in Wichita, a Caddoan language spoken in the area of Anadarko, Oklahoma. Conversational repair refers to a set of strategies that speakers may use to address any issues related to speaking, understanding, and communicating that arise within an interaction (e.  g., halting, pausing, correcting, or abandoning an utterance). This can be initiated either by the speaker (self-repair), or by another participant (Schegloff, Jefferson & Sacks 1977; Sidnell 2016; 11 For example, differences in hand shape and personal space have been identified in pointing behavior; see Sandoval (2013) for a description of the markedness of thumb vs. forefinger pointing in Arapaho in relation to participation space.



Conversation structure 

 439

Fox, Hayashi & Jasperson 1996). Using a CA approach, the author of this study analyzes a 15-minute sample of a 28-minute conversation between three speakers of Wichita, with the goal of identifying the forms of repair being used, and how they might interact with the morphology and syntax of the language. Two findings from this study are especially noteworthy. The first of these involves phonetic aspects of self-repair strategies in Wichita. Mirzayan (2008) finds that there are no restrictions on the type or position of segment which is susceptible to self-repair by cut-off, and that glottal closure cutoff can be used to initiate repair in vocalic environments. This finding challenges the wider claim that languages with phonemic glottal stops do not use glottal cut-off to initiate self-repair (352). Second, Mirzayan finds that when Wichita speakers initiate self-repair within a verbal construction, they tend to restart the word-in-progress, recycling the bound morphemes and at times inserting a new one, rather than attempting to rephrase it partway through (349): (5)                

Wichita recycling: (Mirzayan 2008: 350) 888 Pickard °aːkoːʔa Roland éːkinnakiroːkh(a) – éːkinnaréːkiroːkhárʔiki(h)° 889   Roland said “what kind of songs has she done?” 890 Other (()) 891 Provost (taʔa)ckiroːkhaːr(ʔa) 892   ?? 893 Pickard (taʔa)ckiroːkhaːrʔa 894   ?? 895   0.34

In this conversation, the speakers Bertha Pickard and Bertha Provost are discussing songs. In line 888, Pickard attempts to produce the utterance éːkinnaréːkiroːkhárʔiki(h) ‘what kind of songs has she done?’, but initially only gets partway through the word before cutting herself off. When she respeaks it, however, she recycles from the beginning of the entire word, rather than simply adding the missing morphemes necessary to complete the utterance. All but one of the instances of recycling found in this study occurred from word-initial position. This strong preference for recycling from the beginning of a complex word rather than partway through is likely due to the complexity of Wichita morphology, including constraints on the order of bound morphemes and their morphophonology. This is similar to what has been found for Kickapoo, a polysynthetic Algonquian language spoken primarily in Oklahoma in the United States and in Nacimiento, Mexico. In her (1994) study, Gomez de Garcia examines self-repair strategies based on the speech of a single speaker, finding that self-repair occurred only within word stems, and never during verbal affixation (Mirzayan 2008: 319). Overall, this study shows that Wichita has access to some of the same strategies to initiate self-repair found in other languages, though they are realized differently (352). These findings also speak to issues of polysynthesis and cross-linguistic patterns in self-repair. As previous research on repair strategies has tended to focus on non-pol-

440 

 Olivia N. Sammons

ysynthetic languages, examination of the ways in which conversational repair is executed in polysynthetic languages such as Wichita provides important insights into conversational analysis research more broadly.

18.4.1.3 Syntactic and prosodic patterns in Upper Tanana Conversation is especially helpful in illustrating the intonation and prosody of a language. Whereas isolated sentences produced through elicitation tend to demonstrate less varied intonation, utterances that occur within a conversation are embedded within a pragmatic context and show a more natural prosodic contour (i.  e., the rhythm, speed, intensity, and pitch of an utterance): While a given sentence, taken in isolation, can be translated using a default, neutral intonation, a dialogue creates a meaningful context in which a certain prosodic contour naturally comes to mind – whether it encodes surprise, amusement, anger, or any other emotion suitable to the given dialogue situation (François 2019: 169).

This is seen in Lovick & Tuttle (2012), which examines syntactic and prosodic features used in turn construction and turn allocation in Upper Tanana, a Dene (Athabaskan) language spoken in eastern interior Alaska and western Yukon Territory. The authors set out to identify the syntactic and prosodic criteria for turn-construction in the language, particularly in relation to turn-holding and turn-yielding. They accomplish this by observing the prosody and syntax around syntactic completion points, or points where “a structure has been produced which is syntactically independent from […] its following context” (Lovick & Tuttle 2012: 138, citing Auer 1996: 60). In Upper Tanana, this occurs primarily through the use of main clause verbs and clause-final particles. The authors find that speakers are keenly aware of these syntactic completion points, which is demonstrated in the dataset in a number of ways. First, speakers tend to refrain from taking the floor until the current speaker produces a syntactic completion point, as illustrated in (6): 6)      

Turn-taking and syntactic completion points in Upper Tanana (from Lovick (2006), as reproduced in Lovick & Tuttle (2012: 145–6) 1  AS A:Y (0.3)   and   ‘and’

     

2     

↑ay ↑xA shinahOlnikand because they used to tell me ‘and because they used to tell me’

(0.7)  

     

3     

nahatA’ ay our fathers and ‘our fathers and’

(1.4)  

ch’Ale uh; foc



Conversation structure 

     

4     

aą; yes ‘and’

     

5     

↑ts’ĄĄ ↓t’iin hiiushyiit Ay the people there and ‘the people there, and, there’

     

6     

deltth’iik henAy, they used to live they say ‘they used to live, they say’

     

7     

     

8     

CD whatswhat ‘what’

 441

(3.7)     ushyiit. there

(0.7)  

(3.3)  

(0.9)    

che’ t’iin iin? tail people pl ‘the tailed people’

(0.9)  

In (6), speakers AS and CD are discussing a volcanic eruption that occurred in 1911. Although a syntactic completion point is present at the end of line 1, speaker CD does not take the floor, instead allowing speaker AS to elaborate on what has been told to her. CD then waits until the next syntactic completion point (in this case, the verb henay ‘they say’) to speak, despite the presence of multiple pauses preceded by falling intonation in lines 2–5. It is rare to find instances of turn-taking outside of a syntactic completion points, which can thus be seen as a turn-yielding device in Upper Tanana conversation. Prosodic features of syntactic completion found by the authors include decreased pitch and intensity, which also appear to be turn-yielding devices. Second, speakers are found to strategically avoid uttering a syntactic completion point as a means of holding the floor. This turn-holding technique is referred to as syntactic non-completion, or delayed syntactic completion: One of our most striking findings is that in conversation…Upper Tanana speakers make extensive use of delayed syntactic completion. By stringing subordinate clauses (containing either a relativized verb or a subordinating complementizer) together, and by avoiding main clauses, speakers can hold their turns relatively effortlessly (Lovick & Tuttle 2012: 161).

This use of syntactic means to accomplish interactional ends may be connected to insubordination (i.  e., the use of clauses that are formally subordinate as main clauses; Evans 2007) and the kinds of discourse-level dependencies discussed by Mithun (2008) for Navajo, where the subordinating clitic =go is used to indicate dependency to larger stretches of discourse (Lovick & Tuttle 2012: 149). While Upper Tanana does not have a cognate morpheme for =go, relativization is used extensively for similar purposes, suggesting that the phenomena of syntactic non-completion in Upper Tanana and insub-

442 

 Olivia N. Sammons

ordination in Navajo may be related, and may both be motivated in part by the functions they serve in conversation.12

18.4.2 Indigenous ways of learning and interaction In addition to offering a special vantage point onto the linguistic features of North American Indigenous languages, conversation can also teach us about Indigenous ways of learning and interaction. As one example, Hermes, Engman, & Roach (2019) discuss the documentation of intergenerational conversation between Elders and youth in Ojibwemowin while taking forest walks together. Transcription of audiovisual recordings of such walks, produced by point-of-view cameras and dedicated audio recorders worn by each participant, were used by the authors to consider the production of meaning through interactions in the language both on and with the land, adopting a learning science approach to interactional analysis. Though still in the early stages of analysis, several themes have already begun to emerge from this work. Perhaps one of the most significant is the notion of the land as a participant in the interaction. Through the process of transcription, it became apparent that there was a type of interaction with the land that was not being represented in the initial transcripts, which only included lines for each of the speakers. The team found that these initial transcripts were somewhat “flat”, and that a piece of the conversation was missing. Consequently, they decided to add the land to the transcripts as an autonomous participant, complete with turns, as shown in Figure 5:

Fig. 5: Transcript sample with land as participant (Hermes, Engman & Roach 2019)13 12 See also Cable (2011) for further discussion of insubordination in Tlingit, Haida, and Gitksan. 13 Y2 = Youth 2, L = Land



Conversation structure 

 443

This decision was motivated by a view of the land as an animate entity with agency, as well as the observation that speakers in the interaction were acting in relation to the land. Members of the project team noted that adding the land as a participant provided context which was otherwise missing from the conversation. It also enabled them to represent a kind of turn-taking, in which speakers were “reading” or consulting with the land. Overall, team members found that this approach helped them to gain insights they would not otherwise have had, and offered a “more indigenous way of doing research” (Hermes, Engman & Roach 2019). Other themes that have emerged through the analysis are those of naming and pitching in. For example, there are some instances in the recordings in which a youth asks an Elder a direct question, usually the name of something they have come across on the land. In one case, this was a fiddlehead fern, and in another, a pinecone. Although the youth asks the Elder the names for these things, a direct answer is never given. Instead, the Elder offers a description of the pinecone, describing its life cycle, how it has changed color, how easy it is to pull the needles out, and so on. The point of view cameras show the youth looking from the Elder to the plant, then back to the Elder. Meanwhile, the Elder mimics with his body what the youth is doing with the land (pulling on the pinecone needles). Together, the Elder and youth are “pitching in” to co-construct knowledge with and about the land, even though a specific classification label is not given. They take turns embodying both each other and the land. The question is taken by the Elder as an invitation to collaborate, rather than to put himself an expert position which would create a hierarchy between himself and the youth. Unlike in a classroom setting, where direct questions are typically met with direct answers, this is a situation in which expertise is flattened among all participants and learning is achieved jointly. These findings demonstrate the varied ways in which Indigenous knowledge is constructed and underscore the importance of getting on the land to look for clues and continue learning how Indigenous knowledge is constructed through interaction beyond the bounds of classroom walls (Hermes, Engman & Roach 2019).

18.4.3 Ethnographic perspectives on conversation Finally, as the linguistic space in which day-to-day life and culture are enacted, conversation also serves as an important source of insight for ethnographic studies of language. In his 1988 study, Basso adopts an ethnographic approach to discussing the role of place-naming in Western Apache conversation. In particular, Basso (1988) explores the practice of “speaking with names,” which employs place names as a means of referring to and representing narratives associated with those places, thus “condensing into compact form their essential moral truths” for use in expressing social meaning (121). According to Basso (1988), Apache conversation (’ilch’ị’ yádaach’ilti’) is comprised of a series of pictorial depictions exchanged between speakers. The best narrators are those who encourage their listeners to “travel in their minds”, visualizing what is being

444 

 Olivia N. Sammons

communicated, rather than those who speak too much and interfere with others’ ability to think (109–110). In addition, a location must be explicitly mentioned in all Apache stories which spatially anchors them and allows listeners to create their own mental pictures of what is being described (Basso 1988: 110). The practice of “speaking with names” occurs when speakers use special forms of place names in conversation with one another to situate the event being depicted, evoking for the hearer the ancestral wisdom of traditional stories associated with that location. The use of these specialized place names alone brings to the foreground essential moral truths and narratives associated with that location (Basso 1988: 121). When used in this way, these place names occur in their full form, often with an emphatic suffix which translates roughly as “right here!”, or “at this very place!” (Basso 1988: 114). This practice is only considered appropriate in a limited range of circumstances. In more ordinary situations, abbreviated forms of place names are used instead of the full forms (Basso 1988: 113–114). This practice allows speakers to “acknowledge a regrettable circumstance without explicitly judging it, to exhibit solicitude without openly proclaiming it, and to offer advice without appearing to do so” (Basso 1988: 114). This study reveals how understanding the use of place names in this specialized form of conversational discourse about the landscape reinforces both community members’ individual sense of place and how they are situated within it, as well as a shared understanding of the world (Basso 1988: 101).

18.5 Conclusion In this chapter, we have seen why the process and products of documenting conversation in North American Indigenous languages can be beneficial to both linguistic theory and language revitalization, providing valuable opportunities for language learning and a fuller appreciation of the cultural and linguistic practices of Indigenous communities. While spontaneous interaction has long been underrepresented in the documentation and description of Indigenous languages in North America, both the case studies of recent documentation projects surveyed in this chapter and the results of conversation-focused studies from a range of research traditions demonstrate the value of dedicated attention to conversation. Due to the scarcity of previous conversational documentation and the standing challenge of developing new conversational materials, many studies in this area are based on relatively small samples of conversation (e.  g., a single conversational exchange). Ongoing documentation, description, and revitalization initiatives continue to address these issues in many North American Indigenous language communities, expanding the conversational resources available for many languages and integrating them more deeply into language learning, teaching, and research programs, thus setting the stage for conversational documentation to play a more central role in North American Indigenous languages in the future.



Conversation structure 

 445

Acknowledgements: I am grateful to the many Michif speakers I have worked with over the years, in particular Verna DeMontigny for her constant encouragement and assistance with translation, as well as Mervin Fleury, Harvey Pelletier, and Norman Fleury. I would also like to thank the editors, reviewers, and Christopher Cox for helpful comments on earlier drafts of this manuscript, as well as Nicholas Williams and Richard Sandoval for fruitful discussion of these topics. Part of the Michif work cited here was supported by the Hans Rausing Endangered Languages Project (IGS 0151, 2011–2014).

References Amery, Rob. 2009. Phoenix or Relic? Documentation of Languages with Revitalization in Mind. Language Documentation & Conservation 3(2). http://scholarspace.manoa.hawaii.edu/handle/10125/4436 (23 February, 2019). Auer, Peter. 1996. On the prosody and syntax of turn-continuations. In Elizabeth Couper-Kuhlen & Margaret Selting (eds.), Prosody in Conversation, 57–100. Cambridge: Cambridge UP. Bakker, Peter. 1997. A Language of Our Own: The Genesis of Michif, the Mixed Cree-French Language of the Canadian Metis. [Rev. ed.]. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Basso, Keith H. 1988. “Speaking with Names”: Language and Landscape among the Western Apache. Cultural Anthropology 3(2). 99–130. Berge, Anna. 2010. Adequacy in documentation. In Lenore A. Grenoble & Louanna Furbee (eds.), Language documentation: Practice and values, 51–66. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Biber, Douglas. 1993. Representativeness in corpus design. Literary and Linguistic Computing 8. 243–257. Bliss, Heather & Martina Wiltschko. 2018. Stsíkiistsi ki stsíkiistsi: The ubiquity of Blackfoot demonstratives in discourse. Presented at the Workshop on the Discourse Functions of Demonstratives, Universitetet i Oslo. Boas, Franz. 1917. Introductory. International Journal of American Linguistics 1. 1–8. Brugman, Hennie & Albert Russel. 2004. Annotating multi-media/multi-modal resources with ELAN. In Maria Teresa Lino, Maria Francisca Xavier, Fátima Ferreira, Rute Costa & Raquel Silva (eds.), Proceedings of the 4th International Conference on Language Resources and Language Evaluation, 2065–2068. Paris: European Language Resources Association. Buszard, Laura Ann. 2003. Constructional Polysemy and Mental Spaces in Potawatomi Discourse. Berkeley, California, United States: University of California, Berkeley phdthesis. Buszard-Welcher, Laura. 2010. Necessary and sufficient data collection: Lessons from Potawatomi legacy documentation. In Lenore A. Grenoble & Louanna Furbee (eds.), Language documentation: Practice and values, 67–74. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Cable, Seth. 2011. Insubordination in Tlingit: An Areal Effect? Northwest Journal of Linguistics 5(1). 1–38. Clift, Rebecca. 2016. Conversation Analysis. Reprint edition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Cowell, Andrew. 2011. A Conversational Database of the Arapaho Language in Video Format. London: SOAS, Endangered Languages Archive. https://elar.soas.ac.uk/Collection/MPI189644 (1 April, 2020). Cowell, Andrew. 2015. Language Myths, Language Documentation and Language Specificity: Pointing, Eye Contact, and Polar Questions in Arapaho. Presented at the Language in the Present, Victoria, BC. Cowell, Andrew. forthcoming. Documenting multi-modality in conversation: Theoretical and practical considerations around the example of topic initiation. In Richard A. Sandoval, Nicholas Williams & Olivia N. Sammons (eds.), Interactional approaches to language documentation (in preparation). Cowell, Andrew. forthcoming. Language myths, language documentation and language specificity: Pointing, eye contact and polar questions in Arapaho.

446 

 Olivia N. Sammons

Eisman, Kayla. 2015. Marking the Unexpected: Evidence from Navajo to Support a Metadiscourse Domain. UC Santa Barbara. https://escholarship.org/uc/item/0mg3r7f4 (25 February, 2021). Evans, Nicholas. 2007. Insubordination and its uses. In Irina Nikolaeva (ed.), Finiteness, 366–431. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Field, Margaret. 2007. Increments in Navajo conversation. Pragmatics : quarterly publication of the International Pragmatics Association 17(4). 637–646. https://doi.org/10.1075/prag.17.4.07fie. Fox, Barbara, Makoto Hayashi & Robert Jasperson. 1996. Resources and repair: A cross-linguistic study of syntax and repair. Studies in Interactional Sociolinguistics 13. 185–237. https://doi.org/10.1017/ CBO9780511620874.004. François, Alexandre. 2019. A proposal for conversational questionnaires. 321–329. Gaenszle, Martin. 2010. Documenting ceremonial dialogues: An in vitro performance and the problem of textualisation. In Imogen Gunn & Mark Turin (eds.), Language documentation and description, vol. 8, 66–82. London: HRELP. Gomez de Garcia, Jule Marie. 1994. Communicative strategies in conversational Kickapoo. United States – Colorado: University of Colorado at Boulder Ph.D. http://search.proquest.com/docview/304121354/ abstract/A4FBA66CD4CA49D9PQ/2 (2 June, 2020). Grant, Laura & Jocelyn C. Ahlers. forthcoming. Benefits of Community-driven documentation focused on interactional language use. In Richard A. Sandoval, Nicholas Williams & Olivia N. Sammons (eds.), Interactional approaches to language documentation (in preparation). Have, Paul ten. 2007. Doing conversation analysis. (Introducing Qualitative Methods). 2nd ed. Los Angeles, Calif. : SAGE. Hermes, Mary. 2012. Documenting Chippewa [ciw] Conversation and Training Indigenous Scholars. https:// www.nsf.gov/awardsearch/showAward?AWD_ID=1346905 (9 October, 2019). Hermes, Mary. 2016. Understanding Learning Mechanisms and Language Acquisition through Intergenerational Conversations in Southwestern Ojibwe, a Native American language. https://www.nsf.gov/ awardsearch/showAward?AWD_ID=1346905 (11 October, 2019). Hermes, Mary, Megan Bang & Ananda Marin. 2012. Designing Indigenous Language Revitalization. Harvard Educational Review; Cambridge 82(3). 381–402,437–438. Hermes, Mary, Mel M. Engman & Kevin Roach. 2019. Walking the Land: Documenting intergenerational, conversational Ojibwemowin in the Forest. Presented at the International Conference on Language Documentation & Conservation, Honolulu, HI. http://hdl.handle.net/10125/44850. Hermes, Mary, Rose Tainter, Nancy Jones & Margaret Porter. 2014. Ojibwe Conversations. https:// conservancy.umn.edu/handle/11299/163235 (23 February, 2019). Himmelmann, Nikolaus P. 2006. Language documentation: what is it and what is it good for? In Jost Gippert, Nikolaus P. Himmelmann & Ulrike Mosel (eds.), Essentials of Language Documentation. (Trends in Linguistics. Studies and Monographs [TiLSM]), vol. 178, 1–30. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter. Himmelmann, Nikolaus P. 2008. Reproduction and preservation of linguistic knowledge: Linguistics’ response to language endangerment. Annual Review of Anthropology 37. 337–350. Hinton, Leanne. 2002. How to Keep Your Language Alive: A Commonsense Approach to One-on-One Language Learning. Illustrated edition. Heyday Books: Heyday. Hockett, Charles. 1939. Potawatomi Syntax. Language 15(4). 235–248. https://doi.org/10.2307/409107. Hockett, Charles F. 1948a. Potawatomi I: Phonemics, Morphophonemics, and Morphological Survey. International Journal of American Linguistics 14(1). 1–10. Hockett, Charles F. 1948b. Potawatomi II: Derivation, Personal Prefixes, and Nouns. International Journal of American Linguistics 14(2). 63–73. Holton, Gary. 2015. Language and Wellbeing. Linguistic Society of America Committee for Endangered Language Preservation Blog. https://www.linguisticsociety.org/comment/1160#comment-1160 (29 March, 2021).



Conversation structure 

 447

Hutchby, Ian & Robin Wooffitt. 1998. Conversation Analysis: Principles, Practices and Applications. Cambridge: Wiley. Jenni, Barbara, Adar Anisman, Onowa McIvor & Peter Jacobs. 2017. An Exploration of the Effects of Mentor-Apprentice Programs on Mentors’ and Apprentices’ Wellbeing. International Journal of Indigenous Health 12(2). 25–42. https://doi.org/10.18357/ijih122201717783. Kroskrity, Paul V. 1993. Language, history, and identity: ethnolinguistic studies of the Arizona Tewa. Tucson: University of Arizona Press. Levinson, Stephen C. 1983. Pragmatics (Cambridge Textbooks in Linguistics). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://www.cambridge.org/core/books/pragmatics/6D0011901AE9E92CBC1F5F21 D7C598C3 (10 June, 2020). Lovick, Olga. 2006. Documentation of Upper Tanana Athabascan. Fairbanks: Alaska Native Language Archive. Lovick, Olga & Siri G. Tuttle. 2012. Conversation in Upper Tanana Athabascan: Syntactic and prosodic patterns. In Jan-Olaf Svantesson, Niclas Burenhult, Arthur Holmer, Anastasia Karlsson & Håkan Lundström (eds.), Language Documentation and Description, vol. 10, 132–176. London: HRELP. http:// www.elpublishing.org/itempage/117 (23 February, 2019). Lüpke, Frederike. 2009. Data collection methods for field-based language documentation. In Peter K. Austin (ed.), Language Documentation and Description, vol. 6, 53–100. London: SOAS. http://www. elpublishing.org/PID/ 071. Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics. 2020. ELAN. Nijmegen: The Language Archive. https://tla.mpi.nl/ tools/tla-tools/elan/ (22 February, 2016). McCreery, Dale. 2015. You want me to just talk? – getting conversations from a single speaker. Presented at the Language in the Present, Victoria, BC. McIvor, Onowa. 2013. Protective effects of language learning, use and culture on the health and wellbeing of Indigenous people in Canada. In Mary Jane Norris, Erik Anonby, Marie-Odile Junker, Nicholas Ostler & Donna Patrick (eds.), FEL XVII: Endangered languages beyond boundaries: Community connections, collaborative approaches and cross-disciplinary research: Proceedings of the 17th FEL conference, 123–131. Ottawa, ON.: Foundation for Endangered Languages in association with Carleton University. https:// dspace.library.uvic.ca//handle/1828/11929 (29 March, 2021). Michaelis, Susanne Maria, Philippe Maurer, Martin Haspelmath & Magnus Huber (eds.). 2013. APiCS Online. https://apics-online.info/ (15 June, 2020). Mirzayan, Armik. 2008. A preliminary study of same-turn self-repair initiation in Wichita conversation. In K. David Harrison, David S. Rood & Arienne Dwyer (eds.), Lessons from Documented Endangered Languages. 317–354. (Typological Studies in Language 78). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins Publishing Company. Mithun, Marianne. 2001. Who shapes the record: The speaker and the linguist. In Paul Newman & Martha Ratliff (eds.), Linguistic Fieldwork, 34–54. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/ CBO9780511810206.003. Mithun, Marianne. 2008. The Extension of Dependency Beyond the Sentence. Language 84(1). 69–119. Mithun, Marianne. 2009. Athabascan Spoken Language Corpora: Ahtna (aht) and Navajo (nav). https:// www.nsf.gov/awardsearch/showAward?AWD_ID=0853598 (25 February, 2021). Mithun, Marianne. 2014. The value of good conversation. Presented at the Annual meeting of the Linguistic Society of America, Minneapolis, MN. http://www.linguisticsociety.org/sites/default/files/MithunValue_of_Good_Conversation.pdf (2 January, 2014). Mithun, Marianne. forthcoming. The value of good conversation. In Richard A. Sandoval, Nicholas Williams & Olivia N. Sammons (eds.), Interactional approaches to language documentation (in preparation). Miyashita, Mizuki & Tracy Hirata-Edds. Forthcoming. “Guided conversation” for language documentation. In Richard A. Sandoval, Nicholas Williams & Olivia N. Sammons (eds.), Interactional approaches to language documentation (in preparation).

448 

 Olivia N. Sammons

Navajo Language Academy. Conversations. Navajo Conversations. https://www.swarthmore.edu/SocSci/ tfernal1/nla/convers.htm (13 May, 2021). Nyman, Elizabeth & Jeff Leer. 1993. Gágiwduł.àt: Brought Forth to Reconfirm. The Legacy of a Taku River Tlingit Clan. Whitehorse, YT: Yukon Native Language Centre. O’Connor, Mary Catherine. 1990. Third-Person Reference in Northern Pomo Conversation: The Indexing of Discourse Genre and Social Relations. International Journal of American Linguistics 56(3). 377–409. Oster, Richard T., Angela Grier, Rick Lightning, Maria J. Mayan & Ellen L. Toth. 2014. Cultural continuity, traditional Indigenous language, and diabetes in Alberta First Nations: A mixed methods study. International Journal for Equity in Health 13(92). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12939-014-0092-4. https:// go-gale-com.proxy.library.carleton.ca/ps/i.do?p=AONE&sw=w&issn=14759276&v=2.1&it=r&id=GALE%7 CA539642933&sid=googleScholar&linkaccess=abs (9 March, 2021). Rosenblum, Daisy. 2015. Multimodal documentation of interactive speech in Kwakw’ala. London: SOAS, Endangered Languages Archive. https://elar.soas.ac.uk/Collection/MPI1029690 (1 April, 2020). Rosenblum, Daisy & Olivia N. Sammons. 2014. Documenting multimodal interaction: Workflows, data management, and archiving. Presented at the Annual meeting of the Linguistic Society of America, Minneapolis, MN. http://www.linguisticsociety.org/sites/default/files/Mithun- Value_of_Good_ Conversation.pdf (2 January, 2014). Rossano, Federico. 2013. Gaze in Conversation. In Jack Sidnell & Tanya Stivers (eds.), The Handbook of Conversation Analysis, 308–329. Chichester, U.K.: Wiley-Blackwell. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/ abs/10.1002/9781118325001.ch15 (30 March, 2021). Sacks, Harvey & Emanuel Schegloff. 2002. Home position. Gesture 2(2). 133–146. https://doi.org/10.1075/ gest.2.2.02sac. Sammons, Olivia. 2019. Nominal classification in Michif. Edmonton, Alberta: University of Alberta Doctoral dissertation. Sammons, Olivia N. 2015a. Documenting Michif Variation. Endangered Languages Archive. http://hdl.handle. net/2196/00-0000-0000-000E-D15E-3. Sammons, Olivia N. 2015b. Enriching the record: The role of conversation in language documentation. Presented at the Language in the Present, Victoria, BC. Sammons, Olivia N. forthcoming. Enriching the record: The role of conversation in language documentation. In Richard A. Sandoval, Nicholas Williams & Olivia N. Sammons (eds.), Interactional approaches to language documentation (in preparation). Sandoval, Richard. 2016. Gesture-Speech Bimodalism in Arapaho Grammar: an Interactional Approach. https:// scholar.colorado.edu/ling_gradetds/61. Sandoval, Richard A. 2013. The forefinger/thumb alternation in Arapaho pointing: Participation space as a frame of reference. Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society 39(1). 412–424. https://doi. org/10.3765/bls.v39i1.3896. Sankoff, David. 1988. Problems of representativeness. In Ulrich Ammon, Norbert Dittmar & Klaus J. Mattheier (eds.), Sociolinguistics: An international handbook of the science of language and society, 899–903. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. Schegloff, Emanuel A. 2007. Sequence Organization in Interaction: A Primer in Conversation Analysis. 1 edition. Cambridge ; New York: Cambridge University Press. Schegloff, Emanuel, Gail Jefferson & Harvey Sacks. 1977. The Preference for Self-Correction in the Organization of Repair in Conversation. Language 53. 361–382. https://doi.org/10.2307/413107. Sidnell, Jack. 2010. Conversation Analysis: An Introduction. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. Sidnell, Jack. 2016. Conversation Analysis. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Linguistics. https://oxfordre. com/linguistics/view/10.1093/acrefore/9780199384655.001.0001/acrefore-9780199384655-e-40 (24 February, 2021). Sidnell, Jack & Tanya Stivers. 2012. The Handbook of Conversation Analysis. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.



Conversation structure 

 449

SIL International. 2019. SayMore: Language documentation productivity. http://saymore.palaso.org/ (22 February, 2016). Spielmann, Roger. 2017. “You’re So Fat!”: Exploring Ojibwe Discourse. Toronto: University of Toronto Press,. Starks, Donna. 1994. Planned vs Unplanned Discourse: Oral Narrative vs Conversation in Woods Cree. Canadian Journal of Linguistics/Revue canadienne de linguistique 39(4). 297–320. https://doi.org/10.1017/ S0008413100015437. Starlight, Bruce, Patrick Moore & Christopher Cox. 2018. Documenting conversations in Tsuut’ina. London: SOAS, Endangered Languages Archive. https://elar.soas.ac.uk/Collection/MPI1257379 (1 April, 2020). Taff, Alice. 2006. Unangam Tunuu (Aleut Language) Conversation Corpus. London: SOAS, Endangered Languages Archive. http://elar.soas.ac.uk/deposit/0027 (15 September, 2018). Taff, Alice. 2013. Woosh een áyá yoo x̱ʼatudli.átk, the Tlingit conversation collection. Presented at the Sharing our Knowledge Clan Conference, Juneau, AK. Taff, Alice, Melvatha Chee, Jaeci Hall, Millie Yéi Dulitseen Hall, Kawenniyóhstha Nicole Martin & Annie Johnston. 2018. Indigenous language use impacts wellness. In Kenneth L. Rehg & Lyle Campbell (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Endangered Languages. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Taff, Alice, Liana Wallace & Louise Mason. forthcoming. Woosh een áyá yoo x̱’atudli.átk – We’re talking conversation. In Richard A. Sandoval, Nicholas Williams & Olivia N. Sammons (eds.), Interactional approaches to language documentation (in preparation). Whalen, D. H., Margaret Moss & Daryl Baldwin. 2016. Healing through language: Positive physical health effects of indigenous language use. F1000Research 5. 852. https://doi.org/10.12688/ f1000research.8656.1. Woodbury, Anthony C. 1995. The poetics and rhetoric of overlap in a sample of Yup’ik men’s house speech. In Proceedings of the third annual Symposium About Language and Society (SALSA), vol. 35, 77–97. Austin: Texas Linguistic Forum. https://www.academia.edu/26089817/ Woodbury_Anthony_C_1995_The_poetics_and_rhetoric_of_overlap_in_a_sample_of_Yupik_mens_ house_speech_Proceedings_of_the_Third_Annual_Symposium_about_language_and_society_Austin_ SALSA_Texas_Linguistic_Forum_35_77_97 (26 February, 2021).

V Meaning

Sally Rice

19 Lexicalization and lexical meaning Abstract: The Indigenous languages of Native North America are largely morphologically complex, with the verb often taking on the lion’s share of morphological marking and meaning-making. The languages differ by family in terms of (i) the range of inflectional and derivational options on both nouns and verbs, (ii) the extensiveness, ordering, and transparency (if more agglutinating) or opacity (if more fusional) of morphological affixation, and (iii) the size of the root inventory available for word formation. Nevertheless, there are a number of commonalities across the continent in terms of lexicalization patterning that, broadly understood, could aid communities and linguists involved in language documentation, analysis, and pedagogical materials development for language revitalization. This chapter addresses aspects of lexicalization for both universal concepts as well as terms of acculturation (brought about by contact with neighboring Indigenous groups, with the first wave of settler-colonizers, and, ultimately, with modernity), semantic shift, and the role of figurativity (in the case of common metaphors and metonymies) in extending the lexicon.

19.1 Overcoming expectations about semantic compositionality and stable form-meaning mappings in lexico-semantic analysis Anyone encountering an unfamiliar language feels lost in a sea of sound and meaning confusion. Both linguists and learners tend to cling to the same analytical buoy, thinking it will keep them afloat until they can start to “swim” in the language.1 This heuristic support is based on a set of idealized assumptions, monosemy and compositionality, that a unit of language––be it morpheme, word, or lexical item––has a stable form and a stable meaning (monosemy) and that it combines with other units in a straightforward and predictable way across different contexts of use (compositionality). In reality, and especially in the case of language documentation, analysis, and pedagogy, form-meaning pairings are anything but stable and one-to-one and the combination of one morpheme or word or lexical item with another is anything but straightforward and predictable. This chapter tackles the situation of lexical meaning in Native North American languages and bases much of the discussion on two different assumptions about meaning

1 I am grateful to the many speakers of diverse Indigenous languages of Native North America who have taught me so much about semantics and lexicalization over the years. Thanks, too, to several anonymous reviewers and the editors of this volume for valuable suggestions and commentary. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-019

454 

 Sally Rice

in language: polysemy (many meanings associated with one from because of language change or different contexts of use)2 and idiomaticity or partial compositionality (fixed, multi-component expressions with figurative or un-figure-out-able meanings)3 The fact, too, that Native North American (NNA) languages are oral––having a very short history of writing mastered by only a few speakers and spoken by relatively small and local bands of people––means that the segmentation of a stream of speech into discrete words or word parts will be difficult for outsiders, variation across speakers will be common, meaning and usage patterns will be highly contingent on context and culture, and language change will be relatively fast. It also means we will be capturing conversation and the telling of stories (and not highly edited text) as we document a language, analyze it, and prepare to teach it to non-speakers. Speakers and learners should not despair at this lexico-semantic fact of life––that meaning is not necessarily stable across contexts of use and, moreover, is generally incommensurate across languages. Compared to one another, individual languages either over- or under-specify meaning relations in their lexicon and, thus, depend on pragmatics to fill in any blanks in the case of vagueness or to distinguish between senses in the case of ambiguity. We will return elsewhere to these notions of semantic specification and equivalence, but bear in mind that the lexical situation in many NNA languages is that a typical word may be very complex morphologically and rather idiomatic and figurative semantically. The sheer fact that many NNA languages are morphologically complex (and many are polysynthetic) means that the major word type (or part of speech) in these languages––the verb––is often at the cusp, unit-wise, between complex word and simple sentence. Linguistically speaking, word formation and meaning is usually the domain of the lexicon, while sentence formation and interpretation is typically relegated to the grammar of a language. The division of labor between grammar and lexicon is arbitrary and one challenge in documenting NNA languages is capturing the richness of lexical meaning, word formation, collocation, and variation, recognizing that some “words” are really formulaic phrases or small sentences that could helpfully be represented in a dictionary and not left for a grammar. Moreover, lexicalization and lexical meaning is truly at the nexus of how ancestors construed the world (cognition), how knowledge of society and the environment is encapsulated (culture), how modern speakers have extended old forms for new purposes (language change), and how languages are similar to and different from one another (typology).

2 Polysemy is not the same thing as either homonymy, whereby one form has many unrelated meanings and any sound similarity is merely incidental, or allomorphy, whereby one meaning may be associated with multiple forms. 3 Semantic analysis may be complicated by the fact that some languages feature a fair amount of morphological fusion or portmanteau morphemes that carry multiple meanings in one form.



Lexicalization and lexical meaning 

 455

19.2 Factors driving lexicalization In the previous section, the long-standing division in linguistics was introduced between grammar and lexicon or, put another way, between form and meaning, rule and list, or aspects of language which are more regular and syntactic than idiosyncratic and lexical. For most of the world’s languages, this separation in focus and explanation is not fully supportable since regularities and irregularities abound, regardless of the size of the linguistic unit (e.  g. morpheme, word, phrase, or clause). However, since this chapter is about lexical phenomena in NNA languages, we might start with some all-purpose tenets to give us a firmer handle on this very broad topic of lexicalization and lexical meaning.4 First of all, a language’s lexicon contains a mix of universal tendencies in word formation and some very language-specific patterns. In doing language documentation (and, ultimately, dictionary-making), it is important to have some familiarity with the now well-established lexical semantic and lexical typological literature that has revealed some of the predominant universal and particular tendencies in lexicalization (see Talmy 1985, Frawley 1992, Koch 2001, Evans 2010, and the collected papers in Vanhove 2008 and Riemer 2016, for good overviews). The term lexicalization simply refers to the way semantic notions are “carved up” and “packaged” in a language’s lexical items, be they morphemes, words, or fixed expressions (see Pawley 1985, Brinton and Traugott 2005). Lexicalization is where we really come to terms with conceptual ideologies and cognitive packaging in a language. For example, some languages have separate words for hand and finger,5 while others have a single word generally denoting arm that

4 I deliberately avoid using the term lexical semantics here since, as a subdiscipline in linguistics, it tends to have a focus on inter-word relationships rather than on the meaning of lexical items per se. Typical phenomena described from a lexical semantic point of view include relationships such as synonymy (words with similar meaning such as the English sofa and couch); homonymy (words with no similarity in meaning despite a similarity in pronunciation, as in for/fore/four, or spelling, as in plant ‘a botanical entity’ vs. plant ‘a factory’); antonymy (words with somewhat opposite meanings, as in cold/hot, alive/ dead); meronymy (words which are related by a part/whole relationship, such that finger could be considered a meronym of hand; hypernymy (words which subsume other words by category, such that tree could be a hypernym of oak or spruce) or hyponymy (words which instantiate a more general category, such that beagle and poodle are designated hyponyms of dog). These between-word relationships are of particular interest to psycholinguists who are concerned with semantic networks and how a word can prime another related word experimentally for a speaker, to anthropologists who are interested in folk biological, kinship, or other taxonomic systems that bring out related vocabulary within a domain of experience (see § 2.4), as well as to second language teachers who hope to accelerate vocabulary development in learners by drawing simple associations between sets of words. It must be stressed that these inter-word relationships are more language-specific than universal. Some good, general overviews of lexical semantics for the interested reader include: Cruse 1986, Aitchison 2003, Lieber 2004, and Goddard and Wierzbicka 2014. 5 The use of small capital letters here represents a schematic concept, because a gloss––usually indicated by single quotes in linguistics––gives too much credence to the meaning of the analytical language equivalent.

456 

 Sally Rice

subsumes hand and finger. These differences in naming major and minor body parts provides an apt illustration of how distinct languages differentially carve meaning at the joints, either very generally or more specifically. Some of the major factors that influence lexicalization in any given language––which aspects of meaning get encoded, which get left out, which get encoded independently, or which get conflated with other concepts––are described in the rest of § 2.

19.2.1 The size of the root inventory Basic word-building morphemes or items from the endolexicon (or core vocabulary–– the oldest, smallest, and usually least derived part of a language’s lexicon) are presumed to be fairly universal, such as the notions intended to be captured in a Swadesh list. These may include body-part terms, immediate kin terms, some pronouns, terms for the environment, as well as for flora and fauna that are prevalent worldwide, e.  g. tree, dog, or fly).6 Although such concepts may seem relatively equivalent across languages, they may not be. A good example is the arm notion discussed above; another example is a word like sister or cousin, which may be distinguished on the basis of age or sex vis-à-vis the speaker or relationships to other kinfolk. This inventory size factor could be thought of as the available ingredients that each language brings to the task of word formation. For NNA languages with smallish root inventories, there is necessarily a robust reliance on derivation and recombination in order to attain the full panoply of semantic expression. A small root inventory guarantees that items will be re-used extensively and undergo shifts in meaning, possibly leading to vague, figurative, if not fully opaque senses. A large root inventory, especially for certain domains of experience, may be more specialized from the outset and meaning distinctions may be far richer than those in the language of analysis.

19.2.2 The range of available word-formation devices The morphological devices prevalent in word formation typically include conversion, compounding, affixation (usually through derivational morphology), reduplication, incorporation, periphrasis, and apposition. This combinatorial factor could be seen as akin to a set of recipes that each language exploits for the purpose of word formation. NNA languages, being largely morphologically complex and verb-oriented, frequently 6 These types of terms are considered referential––they pick out an entity and make reference to it. Terms for relational predicates (such as verbs and adpositions or other root items marking states, processes, and activities or spatial, possessive, attributive, or broad associative relations) are rarely included in Swadesh-type lists of universal, core vocabulary since the semantic scope of relational items is rarely comparable from one language to another.



Lexicalization and lexical meaning 

 457

derive nouns from verbs so we might add nominalization/deverbalization to processes that create nouns (and possibly adjectives and adverbs or their structural equivalents) from verbs. Many languages have robust verbal denominalization constructions as well and create verbs of varying degrees of semantic transparency out of nouns, usually by adding the lightest or most schematic of “verby” morphology to a noun stem. Obviously, morphosyntactic patterning options in a language either enhance or constrain its word-formation potential and how specific or vague the meaning of the resulting expression is.

19.2.3 The nature of the concept being lexicalized Lived experience contains a mix of natural and artifactual, animate and inanimate, concrete and abstract, objective and subjective, specific and generic, and referential and relational notions, and these conceptual differences often have ramifications for the way in which basic building-block morphemes will be combined with other morphemes and enter into a lexicalization for a concept. Put another way, different semantic fields (e.  g. lexicalizations for semi-universal or widespread concepts such as dog, beaver, or caterpillar versus terms of acculturation such as wednesday, automobile, or hospital) will engender different semantic strategies for their lexicalization. It is here that we see some interesting commonalities in the ways in which metaphors vs. metonymies are deployed for lexicalizing old, new, encountered, or introduced concepts. For example, worldwide, terms for nipple tend to derive from terms for breast, not vice versa––the former body-part term referring to a smaller and more functionally specialized part than the more salient basic-level term. Often, another smaller body-part term enters into a compound with breast and the resulting metaphorical lexicalization for nipple might literally (etymologically) be something like ‘breast-neck’, ‘breast-nose’, or ‘breasthead-tip’ as in Lakota [Siouan]: azéphiŋkpa (< azé-pȟá-íŋkpa, ‘breast-head-small.end’).7 The introduced concepts, horse and rice, will frequently also be construed metaphorically in NNA languages and may be lexicalized, when glossed literally, as ‘big dog’ or ‘little bug/maggot’ since the new concept looks like a type of older, more familiar one. On the other hand, table may be framed in a more metonymic way, as ‘the thing one eats on’, by which some process (eat) stands for the instrument that participates in the process. This factor is especially relevant for many of the referential lexicalizations we find in NNA languages, as illustrated in § 3.2 below.

7 All Lakota examples come from the New Lakota Dictionary (2nd edition) by the Lakota Language Consortium (2011). The Navajo examples come from Young and Morgan (1987); the Hupa examples are from Baldy (1996); all N. Paiute-Bannock examples come from Liljeblad et al. (2012); and the Dene Sųłiné and Tsuut’ina examples are from the author’s own fieldnotes. Citations for all other language examples are given in the text.

458 

 Sally Rice

19.2.4 Universal tendencies in conceptualization and lexicalization Much anthropological linguistic work in the 1960s and 1970s documented cross-linguistic trends in onomasiology (or how concepts are named), specifically detailing how languages conflate or separate categories in their lexicon and whether terms for more universal items in a set will be named by simpler and more unanalyzable terms, while more language-specific terms will be named by more composite or secondary lexemes. (See Berlin, Breedlove, and Raven 1973 and Brown 1977 on general principles of folk biological nomenclature, and Brown et al. 1976 and Witkowski and Brown 1978 for non-biological naming principles). Relatedly, the now famous color studies of Berlin and Kay (1969) produced an implicational hierarchy showing that languages tend to indicate the same expansion of color distinctions depending on the size of their color term inventory (in other words, the fewer color terms available in a language, the more likely they will mark the same set of universal colors). They proposed the following evolutionary sequence for the expression of distinct color terms cross-linguistically, assuming that the forms towards the left of the sequence are most likely to be lexicalized in a language using a basic, unanalyzable term (where X ≺ Y means that X is lexicalized before and possibly more simply than Y): white green yellow purple/ & ≺  red ≺  or ≺  or ≺  blue ≺  brown ≺  orange/ black yellow green pink/grey

Similarly, Brown and Witkowski (1981: 318) posited the following lexical encoding sequence for folk botanical life forms: “grerb” bush vine or other [no life ≺  tree ≺  = grass + herb, ≺  or ≺  bush + ≺  plant forms] or just grass vine grass types

More recently, Wilkins (1996) looked at trends in the naming of body parts, especially terms that derive from other body-part words (such as leg or knee ➔ bone)8, from lexemes for their physiology (walk ➔ leg), or even from similarly shaped entities in the environment (branch ➔ arm). Wilkins (1996: 274) offered the following implicational hierarchy for the naming of “parts of a person” when that naming is figurative (based on a metonymy or a metaphor) and involves semantic extension (note, the left-most change types are the most common):

8 Here, X ➔ Y means that X is the original meaning and Y is the extended, derived, or replacement meaning.



intrafield ­metonymic changes (skin ➔ body)

Lexicalization and lexical meaning 

≺ 

interfield ≺  ­metonymic changes (smell ➔ nose)

interfield ­metaphoric changes (spear ➔ penis)

≺ 

 459

intrafield ­metaphoric changes (anus ➔ mouth).

Here, intrafield means within the domain of the body, while interfield means across domains of experience, such as a physiology-to-anatomy or plants/artifacts-to-body. Generally, metaphor involves conceptual mapping across different fields or domains of experience based on some sort of perceived similarity (X is like Y), while metonymy involves conceptual mapping within a field or domain, as when a part stands for a whole or a whole for a part, or when two parts are contiguous (X stands for Y). Studies like these about cross-language regularities give us insight into generalized patterns of human cognition and categorization that have ramifications for how a particular language might encode experience in its lexicon and––importantly for documentation purposes––which concepts are likely to be basic-level items and simple morphologically and which are likely to be derived and complex. More pertinent to our concerns about lexicalization in NNA languages is the fact that a huge swath of the average lexicon is figurative.

19.2.5 Conceptual strategies particular to a language, family, or area The notion of idiosyncratic conceptual strategies (more fully discussed and exemplified in § 3), also called “cognitive style” (Hymes 1961) or “cognitive set” (Heath and McPherson 2009), mainly pertains to more relational lexicalizations rather than referential. The general idea is that languages, either genetically or areally, tend to frame events in particular ways, rendering cross-linguistic semantic comparison highly difficult. Hymes (1961: 35–36) describes these cognitive styles as pervasive and systematic within a language and notes the case of Navajo, in which the concept of motion is especially prevalent, as is preparedness in Hopi, while point of origin and point of terminus is a particular feature of Upper Chinook [Chinookan]. This framing could also take the form of which phase of an event (initial, medial, or final) enters into its lexicalization or whether individual event phases are encoded at all versus larger macro-event properties (see Bohnemeyer et al. 2007 for some cross-linguistic comparisons). A simple English example of such an “event phase metonymy”, is found in the periphrastic and highly colloquial expression for drink a beer: crack open a cold one. Only the initial phase is lexicalized (not the actual drinking); moreover, beer is referred to with the very generic expression, cold one, which is nevertheless completely interpretable within the collocation. Similarly, the relational concept hunt moose could be framed in English as set out for moose to focus on the initial phase of the hunting process or bag a moose to focus on the result. As Kroskrity (2015: 150) points out, our challenge in providing lexical entries

460 

 Sally Rice

for verbs in dictionaries of NNA languages is “understanding how cultural values saturate even the simplest of verbs.” Another type of conceptual set at play is the tendency to prefer either specific-over-generic encoding or generic-over-specific. For example, many European languages have general all-purpose verbs for eat and drink, while many NNA languages distinguish these processes more specifically on the basis of the nature of the eater, the manner of the eating, and the nature of the object being eaten, to the point of having large inventories of eating and drinking verb stems. This is especially true of Athapaskan or Dene languages (used here interchangeably; see Landar 1964 and Rice 2009). Other processes or relational predications that are especially robust lexically in NNA languages have to do with posture verbs, verbs of motion, verbs of handling and transfer, verbs of moving or carrying an object by different body parts, and verbs of putting on or taking off clothing. Rhodes (1986) cites the case of Ojibwe [Algonquian] verbs of speaking, listing the many ways in which the language has differentiated different communicative situations in its lexicon. These lexicalization decisions reflect biases in the construal of the world. There is every reason to believe that many categories of experience can be carved up or packaged in ways unique to a language or linguistic area. Thus, we must stay light on our semantic feet in order to appreciate the actual range of meaning conveyed in a particular language’s lexical items and not imbue too much of the analytical language’s semantic structure on the target language’s lexicon. This is critical when trying to describe, analyze, or define demonstrative, spatial, or directional systems in another language (see Levinson 2003), not to mention tense, aspect, and mood systems (see Mithun 1999: Chap. 3).

19.2.6 The emergence of grammatical material from formerly lexical material Most NNA languages are characterized by having a limited part-of-speech inventory–– often just nouns, verbs, and particles9 (recognizing, of course, that a vast array of affixes can create items that look more like adjectives or adverbs in European languages, among other lexical categories). The origin of particles and some of the affixes and clitics that end up doing more functional or grammatical work in a language rather than lexical

9 A particle generally refers to an uninflected, usually monomorphemic item with a pragmatic, discursive, or otherwise interactional function, such as marking a question, negation, uncertainty, stance, etc. Particles often derive from more semantically rich or “lexical” material, but through processes of grammaticalization, they often lose morphological complexity and take on more pragmatic force. Such particles are rarely elicited by linguists who might only be engaged in translation exercises with bilingual speakers. However, particles abound in natural, connected discourse. Their high frequency of use and strong indicator of fluency in a language necessitate their inclusion in a dictionary, no matter how difficult a definition or translation equivalent might be.



Lexicalization and lexical meaning 

 461

or semantic work is an area of great interest to many linguists, as well as speakers who start to see discrepancies semantically across different uses of a particle or bound morpheme once they start to scrutinize their language in printed form, taking it “off-line”, so to speak, and comparing visually across multiple instances. In short, all languages have items which are polyfunctional and which have probably grammaticalized from lexical material (see Svorou 1994, Hopper and Traugott 2003, and Kuteva et al. 2019 for good cross-linguistic overviews of lexical sources for grammaticalized target items). Typically, tense/aspect markers, spatial and temporal markers, evidentials, conjunctions and complementizers, relativizers, comparative devices, discourse particles, as well as hesitation devices tend to derive morphologically from more lexically transparent notions across the world’s languages. Some of the most typologically robust grammaticalization pathways see body-part terms turning into adpositions, demonstratives into complementizers/relativizers and focus particles, adpositions into conjunctions, allative/ablative markers into possessives, and question words into indefinite pronouns. There has been relatively little published research expressly on grammaticalization in NNA languages, but ample evidence of semantic shift from the more lexical to the more grammatical is found in many grammars, dictionaries, as well as published papers. A few noteworthy grammaticalization studies of NNA languages include Rankin (1977) on the extension of posture verbs in Siouan languages to become auxiliaries, noun classifiers and determiners; Linn (1994) on posture verbs in the isolate, Yuchi/Euchee, also becoming noun classifiers and determiners; Cook (1999) on postpositions becoming conjunctions in Dene Sųłiné [Dene], and Sutton (2014) on the development in Tanoan languages of intensifiers, concessives, evidentials, and other more functional material emerging from lexical sources. This type of semantic shift should not be overlooked in analyses and dictionary entries since it demonstrates how profoundly languages can change over time and still be considered intact.

19.2.7 Borrowing due to contact with speakers of other languages Close and friendly contact between speakers of languages of different stocks does not necessarily result in lexical borrowing (consider the centuries-long contact between the Kiowa [Tanoan] and the Plains Apache [Dene] which did not yield much lexical transference), while strained contact between Indigenous peoples or between Indigenous peoples and colonizers does not necessarily preclude borrowing. Borrowing may take the form of wholesale adoption of a foreign term so that it becomes a loanword, which is like borrowing an ingredient from another language. Borrowing may also involve the adoption of a foreign lexicalization pattern while retaining native vocabulary; this is called a calque or loan translation and is like borrowing a recipe, but not the ingredients that go into the recipe. Two common examples of widely borrowed loanwords in NNA languages involve the adoption of the Dutch hog-calling term [ku:š-ku:š] (Goddard 1974:155) or the French cat-calling terms minou or pus which, discounting any phono-

462 

 Sally Rice

tactic accommodations or reduplications, frequently name the introduced animal concepts pig/hog and domestic cat, respectively. Conversely, the U.S. state name, Oklahoma, was borrowed wholesale into English from Choctaw [Muskogean] from the compound oklahumma (< okla-humma, people-red) (Cutler 1994:186). In English, it is unanalyzable, as loanwords tend to be. As for calques borrowed into NNA languages, Sammons (2009: 52) gives examples from Sauk [Algonquian] of two English-based calques for raincoat (kêmiyâni-pîthehkâhi, ‘rain-coat’) and wheelchair (têtepithâhi-papîni ‘wheel-chair’), respectively. Brown (1999: 25) cites the Crow [Siouan] word kukúmbirè (< kukúwe-bilí, squash-water) which is supposed to be a calque based on English watermelon. There is a robust literature on borrowing of lexical items into and out of NNA languages that will not be summarized here (however, see Cutler 1994, Callaghan and Gamble 1996, and Brown 1999 for excellent treatments and copious examples). Except in extraordinary cases of creoles, jargons, and the singular case of near-wholesale intertwining of Cree/Ojibwe and French lexica in Michif [Algonquian], borrowed vocabulary have made up a small minority of the average NNA language’s lexicon and usually with respect to very specialized domains of experience: household items associated with colonialism (e.  g. lantern), commerce (e.  g. money), terms for missionary-related religious practice (e.  g. mass), and foreign food stuffs (e.  g. coffee). More often than not, speakers of NNA languages lexicalized terms of acculturation brought about by migration, trade, changes in the environment, or colonization via language-internal means, borrowing neither ingredients nor recipes from other languages, but re-fashioning the lexical resources already available for new expressive purposes. Much of § 3 takes up this point. Semantic notions briefly alluded to here affecting a language’s lexicon, such as inventory size, cognitive style, meaning lightness/density or vagueness/specificity, semantic shift, and evolution from more lexical to more grammatical function, takes us to the heart of how a language is put together meaning-wise. Revealing where words and expressions come from and how they come to mean what they do in certain contexts is the way learners and semi-speakers start to “crack the code” of their language. It is also how strong speakers and Elders can see patterns in their language’s lexicalizations so that they can go on and expand their language’s vocabulary in traditional ways for more contemporary purposes. Resisting certain kinds of borrowing from the outside is, after all, a sign of resilience and refashioning existing morphemes for new purposes is a sign of resourcefulness––two core values endemic to Indigenous peoples across the continent.

19.3 Lexicalization patterns in Native North American languages Given the fact that meaning is not always consistent out of context, that composition is not always straightforward, and that languages have an array of resources––both



Lexicalization and lexical meaning 

 463

universal and particular––for lexicalizing concepts (be they referential or relational), let us now consider some major lexicalization tendencies found across the languages of Native North America. A number of properties affecting the way in which these languages lexicalize concepts are probably not unique to the continent, but merely reflect the confluence of factors briefly discussed above such as a small formative inventory and extensive morphological recombination. All in all, we should expect that most words other than particles to contain multiple morphemes (bearing in mind that monomorphemic particles are probably highly reduced forms of historically lexical material). Moreover, the fact that a vast number of words are polymorphemic means that they are likely to be figurative, and figurativity can lead to opacity of meaning which brings with it definite challenges in how historic etymologies and literal glosses could or should be represented in community-focused dictionaries. What follows are some of the major trends in lexicalization. The representation of these trends in dictionaries and documentary materials often serves as a strong semantic bootstrap for adult learners and speakers alike as it allows for the internal analysis of words and phrases that may have only been considered holistically or literally before.

19.3.1 A preference, conceptually, for marking the specific over the generic In anthropological terms, we could say that there are often few unique beginners or abstract hypernyms in NNA languages such as animal, color, or celestial body. These languages tend to lexicalize at the level of the basic category term (after Rosch et al. 1976), with terms for concepts such as dog, red, or star standing alongside, respectively, terms for concepts such as {wolf, snake}, {black, white}, or {sun, moon}. In short, there may be few superordinate category terms equivalent to thing, mammal, or plant; instead, lexical resources may have been poured into creating subordinate hyponyms (e.  g. types of woodpeckers, types of cousins). This preference for the specific over the generic may also be realized in the verbal lexicon. Many NNA language families are famous for their sets of classificatory verbs wherein there may be no general-purpose verb such as sit, stand, give, break, cut, carry, move, or eat, but rather suppletive sets (highly irregular and unrelated forms which seem to form a system inflectionally) for, say, ‘eating meat’, ‘eating mushy matter’, or ‘eating berries’. In languages with classificatory verbs, the different verb stems tend to differentiate between kinds of event participants (such as animate, stick-like, contained, or rounded objects) either acting on or acted upon in the event. (See the following for studies about classificatory verb systems in NNA languages: Davidson, Elford, and Hoijer (1963) in Athapaskan; Haas 1948 in Creek [Muskogean]; Berman 1990 in Kwakw’ala [Wakashan]; and Blankenship 1997 in Cherokee [Iroquoian].) Moreover, NNA languages often have individual roots or all-purpose verbal affixes that specifically designate an action being carried out principally by different body parts, as in pushing/

464 

 Sally Rice

poking with the hands vs. pushing/poking with the feet/head/elbow/back. Likewise, two-legged walking/running may be distinguished from four-legged. There may be no go or come verbs but rather verbs for ‘moving away from speaker/hearer’, ‘moving to speaker/hearer’, ‘moving back to speaker/hearer’, ‘moving outwards across a threshold’, ‘moving inwards across a threshold’, and so on. There are, nevertheless, exceptions to this observation that NNA languages tend to lexicalize at the level of the particular rather than the all-purpose, as discussed below in § 3.4 with respect to “light” predicates.

19.3.2 A high degree of figurativity brought about by semantic shift and lexical redeployment Because of the smallish root inventories in NNA languages, there is much recycling and recombination of forms, leading inevitably to a great deal of figurativity in the lexicon on the way to satisfying the need for culturally full lexical expression. Rarely are these figurative lexicalizations (involving metaphor, metonymy, or a combination of the two) completely idiosyncratic or “one-off”. Instead, one can usually detect patterns in the deployment of a figurative trope, as in the well-known case of automotive part-naming (partonymy, also known as meronymy) discussed by Basso (1967: 472) for Western Apache [Dene], which involves, under the appropriate contexts of use, the wholesale metaphorical mapping through simple semantic extension of human or animal bodypart terms onto “analogous” parts of a car or truck: -gan ‘hand/arm’ ➔ ‘front wheel’, -chį ‘nose’ ➔ ‘hood’, -ze’ ‘mouth’ ➔ ‘gas-pipe opening, -bid ‘stomach’ ➔ ‘gas tank’, and so on. It is not just terms of acculturation that receive figurative treatment in NNA languages. As suggested above in § 2.3 for the Lakota nipple word, languages often extend or otherwise redeploy roots for perfectly universal concepts, such as body parts. As it happens, the Lakota skull word, natáhu (< natá-hú), literally ‘head-leg’, also involves a compound with multiple body-part terms, but this time displays an intrafield metonymy, to use Wilkins’ (1996) terminology, as described in §  2.4. The word for skull is based on a semantic broadening that the original word for leg has undergone to also mean any type of bone. Three Dene languages, Tututni, Tsilhqot’in, and Carrier, display a similar semantic shift; etymologically speaking, they all encode skull as ‘head-knee’ since the original knee morpheme (rather than leg as in the Lakota case) has broadened to also mean bone in these languages (see Snoek 2015). By contrast, the Koasati [Muskogean] skull word is isbaktalí. Although no analysis is given in the dictionary (Kimball 1994), it is straightforward enough to deduce that it has been lexicalized via compounding from isbakkí ‘head’ and talí ‘stone; rock’, thus making it an interfield metaphor based on hardness similarity between the outer bone of the head and a stone. Such associations based on similarity in shape or other physical attributes can lead to figurative lexicalizations driven by metaphor, whereas associations based on proximity within a referential domain or on functional participation within an event or relational domain can lead to lexicalizations driven by metonymy. Whether physical



Lexicalization and lexical meaning 

 465

similarity, proximity, or participation are particularly salient within a lexicalization depends on a number of factors including the nature of the concept being lexicalized and whether it is native to a culture or introduced by outsiders or through migration. In the referential sphere, so-called natural kinds (animals, plants, elements in the natural world) tend to evoke a perceptual property (what an object looks like) or habitual behavior (how it moves or behaves) lexicalization, while artifacts (objects made by humans for some purpose) generally invoke a process––either the process of its creation or the process of its utility (see Brown 1999 for extensive analysis of terms of acculturation in NNA languages). This may be especially true for objects of acculturation (pear, money, bicycle), but it does not hold across the board, as with universal or indigenous concepts (turtle, rainbow), which may be lexicalized by a variety of figurative tropes, as these are among the oldest concepts in a language. Let us now take a look at a few disparate lexicalization paths taken by different languages for similar concepts. The natural kind concept, pear, is a fruit introduced by colonization across the continent, although it bears a strong similarity to an apple or other large tree fruit that would have been widely endemic. Here are three different routes taken by NNA languages for lexicalizing pear, although they are all based on shape considerations: In Mohawk [Iroquoian], shape considerations enter into the lexicalization and it is named metaphorically via a compound of a saliently shaped artifact term, a jug, and a generic fruit term: kátshe’ káhi (< ka-tshe’ ka-ahi, neut-jug neut-fruit), lit. ‘jug fruit’ (Mithun 2009: 580). In Navajo [Dene], the lexicalization is even more elliptical since there need not be any apple (or fruit) word to hang the rest of the description on. A pear is conventionally bitsee’ hólóní (< bi-tsee’ hólǫ́=í, its-tail it.exists=nmlz), lit. ‘the one that has a tail’. This involves both a metonymy and a metaphor since the tail part references the fruit whole, but the fruit whole is framed as a type of animal––an entity with a tail, after all! An especially evocative (and metaphoric) lexicalization can be found in the Blackfoot [Algonquian] word for pear: ómahkínaotohon (< ómahkína-(m)otohon, old.man-heel), lit. ‘old man’s heel’ (http://dictionary.blackfoot.atlas-ling.ca/). As in Navajo, the fruit is likened to a body-part term. The artifactual concept, bicycle, also has taken several different paths in its lexicalization in NNA languages. In Passamaquoddy-Maliseet [Algonquian] (Francis and Leavitt 2008), there is a version that is a direct loanword from English: payoshihkul ‘bicycle’, but there is another version that demonstrates a language-internal means of lexicalizing a culturally foreign concept: lipokomasut/pomipokomut, lit. ‘something that glides along’. This framing around a verb suggests a process-for-instrument metonymy. As such, it is similar to the Lakota word, hunáhomnipi (< hu-ná-homni-pi, leg/tire-foot-spin-nmlz), lit. ‘the tires that spin with the foot’. Note that the ‘leg’ morpheme hu- has extended metaphorically to mean ‘tire’ in the language, reminiscent of the Western Apache case discussed above in which animate body-part terms also lexicalize vehicle parts. Words for the concept, money (meaning modern coins or bills, not traditional trade items such as dentalia shells), are equally varied in their lexico-semantic composition across the continent. In Hupa [Dene], we find a pure process-for-instrument metonymy

466 

 Sally Rice

based on a verb: miłky’o:xe:t (< mił-ky’o:-xe:t, with.it-things-3sg.buys), lit. ‘with it, he buys things’. Its functional utility is being highlighted, not its physical nature as metal coin or paper bill. By contrast, the Lakota word for ‘money’ is mázaská (< máza-ská, metal-white), lit. ‘white metal’, a generic-for-specific metonymy (not all white metal is money, after all). In N. Paiute-Bannock [Numic, Uto-Aztecan], there is an English loanword, mooniɁi, but the language-internal lexicalization is based on paper money: puhidɨboni (< puhi-dɨ-boopɨ, green/blue-nmlz-writing [or paper.with.writing.on.it]), lit. ‘green paper’. It is based on a double metonymy: writing stands for what it is written on (paper) and green paper generically stands for the specific type of green paper, money. The indigenous-to-the-Americas and natural kind concept, turtle, is frequently lexicalized figuratively, possibly because turtles were not encountered in northern parts of the continent. However, they were clearly not introduced via colonization. In N. Paiute-Bannock, the word for ‘turtle’ is pabayagotsaaɁa (< paba-wagotsaɁa, aug-frog), lit. ‘big frog’, arguably, an intrafield metaphor, which would be the rarest type. In Dene Sųłiné, the expression is gu detth’eni ( ‘button(s)’ for the French (whose coats bore prominent buttons). There are usually very interesting stories to tell in the names NNA language speakers have for themselves and for others and the lexicalization paths behind the ethnonyms can be analytically edifying for learners. Finally, the use of sobriquets (or nicknames) has been very prevalent in NNA societies, possibly because the use of Indigenous personal names was disrupted due to colonization. Personal names, although mainly used referentially and not as terms of address, usually consisted of descriptive phrases that may have had an obscure metaphorical meaning. Golla (2011: 222) provides such an example in Hupa [Dene]: misče:-nint’ik’ ‘fog extends in a string (dim)’. Traditionally, such names were given by Elders and may have changed at different stages of life, such as before and after a puberty ceremony or other initiation rite. Reclaiming personal names is an important component of language ideology and cultural identity (see French and French 1996 on lexicalization and cultural practices for bestowing personal names in NNA languages). Furthermore, almost universally, vocatives (the term used to address someone rather than to refer to him or her) were just possessed kin terms, as in mičhíŋkši (mi-čhíŋkší, my-son) in Lakota or shitaa’ (shi-taa’, my father) in Navajo, and in the small familial bands that most people traditionally functioned within, nearly all encountered people were kin. Jacobsen 1995 details the sound symbolic changes in Nootka/Nuu-chah-nulth [Wakashan] personal names depending on whether they are being used as vocatives or as referential items. Unlike the giving of personal names, nicknaming has persisted into contemporary times by strong speakers of NNA languages. These terms are usually referential and typically used by non-relatives. Initially, they may have been coined spontaneously in some informal context, then they just stuck to a person and became widely used. Sobriquets usually describe salient personal attributes or habits of a person. Sapir (1923) reported many examples of sobriquets in Dene languages, noting that they generally involve a nominalized (what he called “relativized”) stative verb, such as the Dene Sųłiné tthik’edhi (< tthik’edh=i, head-be.bald=nmlz), lit. ‘the one whose head is bald’ or “Baldy” or the Navajo gą k’isi (< gą k’is=i, arm be.cut=nmlz), lit. ‘the one whose arm is cut (off)’ or “One-armed”. Young and Morgan (1987) contains a long list of clan names, personal names, and sobri-



Lexicalization and lexical meaning 

 473

quets in Navajo for both tribal members and for historical figures; especially notable is Dágháilchįįh (< dá-ghá-yilchįįh, lip-hair-he.smells.it), lit. ‘he smells his mustache’ or “Mustache Smeller” for Adolph Hitler.

19.4 Common lexicalization strategies beyond the word in NNA languages Lexicalization is generally regarded as being indifferent to the idea of the word since the focus is on elements which have received lexical status in a language as holistic lexical items even though they themselves might form part of a phrase or clause. This attitude is especially fitting for the analysis of lexicalization patterns in NNA languages since many lexical items are in fact multi-word expressions which have nevertheless achieved a kind of unit status in a particular language. Notably, we can talk about apposition (separate words or phrases juxtaposed in a fixed order), what Traugott and Trousdale (2013) refer to as constructionalization (which can be thought of as how fixed expressions come to be in a language, which often are opaque semantically, just like idioms), and idioms (a phrase or clause whose pragmatic interpretation is wholly at odds with its syntactico-semantic formation). In a very general sense, all of these describe lexicalizations larger than a word that, although phrasal, encapsulate a concept much as a word-level lexical item might in another language. Some examples of apposition come from Dene languages. The Tsuut’ina word for beaver, discussed previously in §  3.3, michà dikòdí (< mi-chà di-kòd=í, 3sg-tail 3sg. impf-be.broad=nmlz), lit.‘the one that its tail, it is broad’ is an appositive construction, as is the concept bruise in Dene Sųłiné, which is lexicalized in a structurally similar way: denetth’i háretł’ezi (< dene-tth’i há-detł’ez=i, person-flesh out-it.is.blue=nmlz), lit. ‘a person’s skin, it’s what comes out blue’. Another common lexicalization that involves apposition is for listing one’s age. What in English would be a simple copular sentence, as in I am eighteen, is framed as iłk’édįghįch’adheł segháye (< ił-k’é-dįghį-ch’a-dheł se-gháye, refl-on-four-from-ten 1sg-winter), lit. ‘four-on-itself-from-ten (=18) my-winters’ or ‘18, my winters’. There is no verb in this sentence, only two juxtaposed nominals, but this is the conventional way of conveying one’s age, as it is in many languages across the continent. Similar to apposition is lexicalization via constructionalization. In short, this describes a round-about and usually highly figurative descriptive phrase that has the structural appearance of a well-formed sentence. These patterns are commonly associated with attributive expressions about emotional or physiological states. Critically, the translation equivalent might be a single word, such as (be) hungry, scared, lonesome, or (have) cancer. These are lexicalized as follows in Dene Sųłiné: Sedzie hįdhų (< se-dzie hį-dhų, 1sg-heart impf.3sg-be.numb), lit. ‘my heart, it is numb’, fig. ‘I am hungry’; Bedzierełtth’er (< be-dzie-deł-tth’er, 3sg-heart-perf.3sg.-start.off), lit. ‘his/her

474 

 Sally Rice

heart took off’, fig. ‘s/he is scared’; Ą́nihiʔá (< ą́-ni-hi-ʔá, away-mind-perf.1sg-handle. round.object), lit. ‘I moved my mind to the wild’, fig. ‘I am lonesome’; Gu sedak (< gu se-dak, worm/bug 1sgO-impf.3sg.devour), lit. ‘bugs are eating me’, fig. ‘I have cancer.’ Not unlike the preceding examples are true idioms and other examples of formulaic language. These may be syntactically elliptical and even repetitive, as much “high” or ceremonial language is, as in the following Lakota expression: čhaŋlí ahí, čhaŋlí yušká, lit. ‘to bring tobacco, to untie tobacco’, fig. ‘to make peace’. The documentation of idioms generally requires the collection of natural, connected discourse, as happens in conversation between highly fluent speakers. Such expressions are lexical since they have status as holistic and semantically synthetic constructions. Their mastery is associated with a strong sense of pragmatic competency for semi-speakers and learners and it is important that they be included in dictionaries and phrasebooks.

19.5 Lexicalization for the lexicon (i.  e., a dictionary) This brief survey of common lexicalization strategies and practices in world languages (§  2) and NNA languages in particular (§§  3–4) is really preparatory for the production of a dictionary, perhaps the most commonly recognized and widely used product of language documentation. At the heart of every bilingual dictionary is the quest for meaning, either in the form of What does this word mean? or How do I say this in the language? A quote by Richard Rhodes nicely sums up the basic set of tensions that language documentarians and lexicographers must grapple with as they come to terms with lexical meaning in a language they likely do not speak: The basic problem in the study of exotic languages [sic] is to understand in the meaning of morphologically complex forms the balance between the contribution of the semantics of component morphemes and the contribution of pragmatics and extension. On the one hand, if we pay too much attention to the pragmatic glosses our native speakers provide, we can miss both the range of reference of a form and the close semantic relationships it participates in. If, on the other hand, we follow the morphology too slavishly, we will miss a form’s range(s) of focus. And worse, the fact that we have accounted for all the parts of the structure of a form will lead us to think that we have accounted for its meaning (Rhodes 1986: 8).

He is writing with a caution about monosemy and compositionality, the two assumptions identified at the beginning of this chapter that can get language learners, lexical semanticists, and lexicographers into analytical trouble if clung to for too long. At this point, it must be obvious that today’s and tomorrow’s dictionaries of NNA languages have to be collaborative efforts with many different types of knowledge included, none of which can emerge from just a single person’s vision or imperfect purview. More positively, it is perhaps better to end with a quote from Wittgenstein (1953: 20), which seems addressed to both speakers and linguists as they proceed to analyze lexical meaning and to render it lexicographically for others: “the meaning of a word is its use in the language.”



Lexicalization and lexical meaning 

 475

References Afable, Patricia O. & Madison S. Beeler. 1996. Place-Names. In Ives Goddard (ed.), Handbook of North American Indians, Vol. 17: Languages, 185–199.Washington: Smithsonian Institution Aitchison, Jean. 2003. Words in the mind: An introduction to the mental lexicon. 3rd edn. Oxford: Blackwell. Aoki, Haruo. 1995. Symbolism in Nez Perce. In Leanne Hinton, Johanna Nichols & John J. Ohala (eds.), Sound symbolism, 15–22. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Baldy, Ray (ed.). 1996. Hupa language dictionary: Na: tinixwe mixine: whe’. 2nd edn. Hoopa, CA: Hoopa Valley Tribal Council. Basso, Keith H. 1967. Semantic aspects of linguistic acculturation. American Anthropologist 69. 471–477. Berlin, Brent & Paul Kay. 1969. Basic color terms: Their universality and evolution. Berkeley: University of California Press. Berlin, Brent, Dennis E. Breedlove & Peter H. Raven. 1973. General principles of classification and nomenclature in folk biology. American Anthropologist 75. 214–242. Berman, Judith. 1990. Notes on shape classifiers in Kwakw’ala. Proceedings of the International Conference on Salish and Neighboring Languages 25. 37–60. Blankenship, Barbara. 1997. Classificatory verbs in Cherokee. Anthropological Linguistics 39(1). 92–110. Bohnemeyer, Jürgen, Nicholas J. Enfield, James Essegbey, Iraide Ibarretxe-Antuñano, Sotaro Kita, Friederike Lüpke & Felix K. Ameka. 2007. Principles of event segmentation in language: The case of motion events. Language 83(3). 495–532. Brinton, Laurel J. & Elizabeth C. Traugott. 2005. Lexicalization and language change. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Brown, Cecil H. 1977. Folk botanical life-forms: Their universality and growth. American Anthropologist 79(2). 317–342. Brown, Cecil. 1999. Lexical acculturation in Native American languages. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Brown, Cecil, John Kolar, Barbara Torrey, Tipawan Trươong-Quang & Phillip Volkman. 1976. Some general principles of biological & non-biological folk classification. American Ethnologist 3. 73–85. Brown, Cecil H. & Stanley R. Witkowski. 1981. Figurative language in a universalist perspective. American Ethnologist 8. 596–615. Callaghan, Catherine A. & Geoffrey Gamble. 1996. Borrowing. In Ives Goddard (ed.), Handbook of North American Indians, Vol. 17, 111–136. Washington: Smithsonian Institution. Cook, Eung-Do. 1999. Conjoined clauses are postpositional phrases in Chipewyan. Proceedings of WSCLA-4, 11–16. Vancouver: UBC Working Papers in Linguistics. Cruse, D. A. 1986. Lexical semantics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Cutler, Charles L. 1994. O brave new words! Native American loanwords in current English. Norman & London: University of Oklahoma Press. Davidson, William, L.W. Elford & Harry Hoijer. 1963. Athapaskan classificatory verbs. In Harry Hoijer et al. (eds.), Studies in the Athapaskan languages, 30–41. Berkeley: University of California Press. Evans, Nicholas. 2010. Semantic typology. In Jae Jung Song (ed.), The Oxford handbook of linguistic typology, 504–533. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Francis, David A. & Robert M. Leavitt. 2008. A Passamaquoddy-Maliseet dictionary. Orono, Maine: University of Maine Press. Frawley, William. 1992. Linguistic semantics. New York & London: Routledge. French, David H. & Katherine S. French. 1996. Personal names. In Ives Goddard (ed.), Handbook of North American Indians, Vol. 17, 200–221. Washington: Smithsonian Institution. Gerdts, Donna B. & Stephen A. Marlett. 2008. Introduction: The form and function of denominal verb constructions. International Journal of American Linguistics 74(4). 409–422. Goddard, Cliff & Anna Wierzbicka. 2014. Words and meanings: Lexical semantics across domains, languages, and cultures. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

476 

 Sally Rice

Goddard, Ives. 1974. Dutch loanwords in Delaware. In Herbert C. Kraft (ed.), A Delaware Indian Symposium 153–160. (Anthropological Series 4). Harrisburg: The Pennsylvania Historical and Museum Commission. Goddard, Ives (ed.). 1996. Languages. Handbook of North American Indians, Vol. 17. Washington: Smithsonian Institution. Golla, Victor. 2011. California Indian languages. Berkeley & Los Angeles: University of California Press. Haas, Mary. 1948. Classificatory verbs in Muskogee. International Journal of American Linguistics 14. 244–246. Heath, Jeffrey & Laura McPherson. 2009. Cognitive set and lexicalization strategy in Dogon action verbs. Anthropological Linguistics 51(1). 38–63. Hopper, Paul J. & Elizabeth Closs Traugott. 2003. Grammaticalization. 2nd edition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Hymes, Dell. 1961. On typology of cognitive styles in language (with examples from Chinookan). Anthropological Linguistics 3(1). 22–55. Jacobsen, William H., Jr. 1995. Nootkan vocative vocalism and its implications. In Leanne Hinton, Johanna Nichols & John J. Ohala (eds.), Sound symbolism, 23–39. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Kimball, Geoffrey D. (with the assistance of Bel Abbey, Martha John, & Ruth Poncho). 1994. Koasati dictionary. Lincoln & London: University of Nebraska Press. Koch, Peter. 2001. Lexical typology. In Martin Haspelmath (ed.), Language typology and language universals: An international handbook, Vol 2, 1142–78. Berlin: de Gruyter. Kroskrity, Paul V. 2015. Designing a dictionary for an endangered language community: Lexicographic deliberations, language ideological considerations. Language Documentation and Conservation 9. 140–157. Kuteva, Tania, Bernd Heine, Bo Hong, Haiping Long, Heiko Narrog, & Seonhee Rhee (eds.). 2019. World lexicon of grammaticalization (2nd edition). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Lakota Language Consortium. 2011. New Lakota dictionary. 2nd edition. Bloomington: Lakota Language Consortium. Landar, Herbert. 1964. Seven Navaho verbs of eating. International Journal of American Linguistics 30. 94–96. Levinson, Stephen. 2003. Space in language and cognition: Explorations in cognitive diversity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Lieber, Rochelle. 2004. Morphology and lexical semantics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Liljeblad, Sven, Catherine S. Fowler & Glenda Powell. 2012. The Northern Paiute-Bannock dictionary. Salt Lake City: University of Utah Press. Linn, Mary. 1994. Positionals in Yuchi/Euchee. Proceedings of the 1994 Mid-America Linguistics Conference, 576–587. Minnesota: University of Minnesota Linguistics Department. Mithun, Marianne. 1999. The languages of Native North America. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Mithun, Marianne. 2009. Iroquoian: Mohawk. In Rochelle Lieber & Pavol Štekauer (eds.), The Oxford handbook of compounding, 564–583. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Oswalt, Robert L. 1971. Inanimate imitatives in Pomo. In Jesse Sawyer (ed.), Studies in American Indian languages, 175–190. Berkeley: University of California Press. Oswalt, Robert L. 2002. Interjections in Kashaya. UC Berkeley Survey Reports, Survey of California and other Indian Languages. https://escholship.org/uc/item/1m04k210 (accessed 18 June 2019). Pawley, Andrew 1985. Lexicalization. In Deborah Tannen & James E. Alatis (eds.), Languages and linguistics: The interdependence of theory, data, and application [GURT 1985], 98–120. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press. Rankin, Robert. 1977. From verb to auxiliary to noun classifier and definite article: Grammaticalization of the Siouan verbs ‘sit’, ‘stand’, ‘lie’. In R.L. Brown, K. Houlihan, L. Hutchinson & A. MacLeish (eds.), Proceedings of the 1976 Mid-America Linguistics Conference, 273–283. Minnesota: University of Minnesota Linguistics Department.



Lexicalization and lexical meaning 

 477

Rhodes, Richard. 1986. The semantics of the Ojibwa verbs of speaking. International Journal of American Linguistics 52(1). 1–19. Rice, Sally. 2009. Athapaskan eating and drinking verbs and constructions. In John Newman (ed.), The linguistics of eating and drinking, 109–152. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Rice, Sally. 2012. “Our language is very literal:” Figurative expression in Dene Sųłiné [Athapaskan]. In Anna Idstrom & Elisabeth Piirainen (eds.), Endangered metaphors, 21–76. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Rice, Sally. 2014. Corporeal incorporation and extension in Dene Sųłiné [Athapaskan] lexicalization. In Matthias Brenzinger & Iwona Kraska-Szlenk (eds.), The body in language: Comparative studies of linguistic embodiment, 71–97. Leiden: Brill. Rice, Sally. 2017. Phraseology and polysynthesis. In Michael Fortescue, Marianne Mithun & Nicholas Evans (eds.), The Oxford handbook of polysynthesis, 203–214. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Riemer, Nick (ed.). 2016. The Routledge handbook of semantics. Abingdon/New York: Routledge. Rosch, Eleanor, Carolyn B. Mervis, Wayne D. Gray, David M. Johnson, & Penny Boyes-Braem. 1976. Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology 8: 382–439. Sammons, Olivia. 2009. Updating the Sauk lexicon: Strategies and implications for language revitalization. In Daisy Rosenblum & Carrie Meeker (eds.), Santa Barbara Papers in Linguistics, vol. 20, 46–59. Sapir, Edward. 1923. A type of Athabaskan relative. International Journal of American Linguistics 2(3/4). 136–142. Silverstein, Michael. 1995. Relative motivation in denotational and indexical sound symbolism of Wasco-Wishram Chinookan. In Leanne Hinton, Johanna Nichols & John J. Ohala (eds.), Sound symbolism, 40–60. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Snoek, Conor. 2015. The lexical semantics of Athapaskan anatomical terms: A historical-comparative study. Unpublished PhD dissertation. University of Alberta. Sutton, Logan. 2014. Kiowa-Tanoan: A synchronic and diachronic study. Unpublished PhD dissertation. University of New Mexico. Svorou, Soteria. 1994. The grammar of space. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Talmy, Leonard. 1985. Lexicalization patterns: Semantic structure in lexical forms. In Timothy Shopen (ed.), Language typology and syntactic description. Vol. 3: Grammatical categories and the lexicon, 57–149. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Thornton, Thomas F. 1997. Anthropological studies of Native American place naming. American Indian Quarterly 21(2). 209–228. Traugott, Elizabeth Closs & Graeme Trousdale. 2013. Constructionalization and constructional changes. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Vanhove, Martine (ed.). 2008. From polysemy to semantic change: Towards a typology of lexical semantic associations. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Wilkins, David. 1996. Semantic change and the search for cognates. In Mark Durie & Malcolm Ross (eds.), The Comparative Method reviewed: Regularity and irregularity in semantic change, 264–304. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Witkowski, Stanley & Cecil Brown. 1978. Lexical universals. Annual Review of Anthropology 7. 427–451. Wittgenstein, Ludwig. 1953. Philosophical investigations. Oxford: Basil Blackwell. Young, Robert W. & William Morgan. 1987. The Navajo Language. Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press.

Sally Rice

20 Lexicography Abstract: This chapter addresses aspects of lexical representation for morphologically complex and oral languages like those spoken in Native North America, especially in the context of community-based dictionary development for purposes of language revitalization. It summarizes some exemplary models for overall dictionary organization and illustrates different practices in terms of content and format found in individual dictionary entries for a small range of lexical types across a handful of languages. It advocates for the inclusion of lexical units smaller and larger than single words as well as both literal and figurative glosses in documentary by-products such as dictionaries and phrasebooks so as to help learners “crack the code”, morphosemantically speaking, of their heritage language.

20.1 The tall order: Representing lexica at, above, and below the word in dictionaries for revitalization Elsewhere in this handbook, some of the special properties of the languages of Native North America (NNA) have been covered that have definite consequences for lexicalization patterning and for lexical representation––as in dictionaries, texts, and pedagogical materials. In particular, the brief survey of common lexicalization strategies and practices in world languages, especially in NNA languages, as covered in the chapter on Lexicalization and Lexical Meaning, is really a preamble to a more pressing concern: How to represent lexical patterning in the most commonly understood product of language documentation and the most widely consulted instrument of language revitalization: the dictionary? Frawley, Hill, and Munro (2002: 22) regard a dictionary as “a thousand pages of ideas and history, a guide to the mind and world of a people.” In the context of language revitalization, a dictionary has enormous implications for literacy, preservation, history, and discovery. This is a tall order for the kind of knowledge that resides in the best dictionaries, and this chapter builds the case for trying to include a wide range of information in what might be a targeted but single-effort, multi-purpose, communitybased, and, hopefully, linguistically informed dictionary.1 NNA languages have historically been oral, but they are increasingly only being taught in classroom situations, requiring a new dependency on literacy. They often

1 I acknowledge with gratitude the thoughtful comments and suggestions of several anonymous reviewers as well as the volume editors. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-020

480 

 Sally Rice

have sound inventories unlike major European languages, requiring special fonts and often unfamiliar diacritics for sound representation in print. However, the resulting orthographies may be neither fully conventionalized nor widely accepted, and representational issues (e.  g., what letter to use for a sound, whether to represent a word by rapid or careful pronunciation, whether to reflect allophonic variation at the expense of morphological transparency or vice versa, whether to segment an expression into individual words or hyphenate between syllables of complex words) can take center stage in a community language program and never be satisfactorily resolved (see Rice and Saxon 2002 for a fuller discussion of representational issues related to orthography). In the extreme case, many NNA languages are polysynthetic, but at the very least, most are highly complex, morphologically speaking. Therefore, roots, stems, affixes, and clitics are arguably as important pedagogically as fully inflected, “pronounceable” words or independent lexical items that older non-literate speakers or Elder consultants may be most comfortable with. Because of the typical complex word structure, there may be many recurring affixes that make an alphabetical listing (either by the beginning or the end of a word) not very helpful or practical. Root/stem elements–– generally the semantically richest part of a word––may be embedded in the middle of a word or a word may contain multiple stem elements, as in a compound, that may further complicate an alphabetical or thematic listing. In the case of verbs, since they are often so highly inflected and rarely permit free-standing citation forms as in English (e.  g. stand, jump), many linguists have argued that the least inflected form of a verb––typically the third person singular, present tense or imperfective aspect form––should stand as the headword entry (or lemma) for the verb, rather than listing all the major inflected forms (or lexemes). This may prove difficult for highly suppletive classificatory verbs or very light predicates which never appear without a wide range of lexical affixes that truly flesh out their meaning (see the chapter on Lexicalization and Lexical Meaning for a description and examples of these two sources of irregularity and ambiguity). Some dictionaries may provide sample conjugation sets for verbs or verb classes, but in some languages, the full paradigmatic range of a verb could yield hundreds of inflected (and derived) forms, regardless of how frequent or rare all of those forms might be. In the chapter on Lexicalization and Lexical Meaning, the past documentary practice of providing full verb paradigms in dictionaries is challenged. The reasoning behind minimizing the inclusion of verb paradigms is that they occupy a lot of space, show highly skewed frequency distributions in actual speech (that is, typically only a few inflected forms of a verb are ever widely used), are tiresome for speakers to produce, and divert attention from more pressing lexical semantic or discursive/pragmatic issues that may never get addressed. Some under-represented areas in the typical NNA language dictionary include the meaning of recurrent discourse particles or formulaic collocations and idioms that are highly context-specific and whose appropriate use is a good marker of proficiency in a language.

Lexicography 

 481

20.2 Types of dictionaries While linguistic resources (word lists, lists of root morphemes, dictionaries, grammars, phrasebooks, transcriptions of conversations and spontaneous narratives, as well as edited text collections) might only be minimal or non-existent for the majority of NNA languages, contemporary documentation and revitalization projects often have the feel of re-inventing the wheel or starting from scratch even when there is a wealth of material in and about the language available. This is especially so because, more often than not, these projects are increasingly collaborative efforts between linguists and speakers, and the desires of the community increasingly take precedence over the intentions of the linguist to describe and analyze a language for the profession, as frequently was the case in the past. Formerly, linguists writing for other linguists often resorted to esoteric nomenclature or obscure theoretical points because they either saw their role as in service to the language (not the speakers) or they did not yet feel the full threat of language shift. In any case, we are now operating in an active climate of language reclamation and community-based research, which has had major consequences for promoting better community-academic engagement and data-collection that does not rely exclusively on elicitation. See Kroskrity (2015) and K. Rice (2018) for powerful testimonies on exposing and resolving competing motivations between documentary linguists and community language activists on what a dictionary is, what kinds of information it contains, and what should be included or left out. With more ethically grounded interaction between language activists and linguists at the fore, informed by more readily available best-practice protocols and a burgeoning set of training venues for speakers and heritage learners (such as AILDI, Breath of Life, CILLDI, COLANG, NILI, among others) and dissemination venues (such as SILS, ICLDC, or individual language family conferences),2 as well as the ever-present and shared desire for language sustainability and revitalization, dictionaries clearly are not what they used to be. The Boasian three-pronged approach to language documentation (grammar, dictionary, and collection of interlinearized texts) increasingly looks antiquated, compartmentalized, and self-limiting. Many modern dictionaries have incorporated all three documentary components and are better for having done so. They may be audio-visual (with sounds and images); they may be more collocational or phrasebook-like; they may contain a grammatical sketch and many example utterances; and they may have derived from a multi-genre corpus containing transcripts of narratives and conversations, as well as edited texts. Every year, more powerful and user-friendly data management software becomes available that helps those who are not particularly tech-savvy to organize language materials into a variety of lexicographic formats. In addition, modern concordancers that can interrogate a corpus are essential in lexicography for producing

2 The full names of these acronyms and the URLs for these venues and others can be found in the references.

482 

 Sally Rice

word lists (both alphabetically and by frequency)3; for returning actual examples of morphemes, words, or entire phrases in context; for helping with sense disambiguation; for demonstrating genre, register, generational, gender, or dialectal differences in usage; and for identifying recurring expressions or other formulaic language that can help learners begin to develop conversational skills (see S. Rice 2018 on the role of linguistic corpora in language documentation, including lexicography). Most community-based language revitalization projects begin with plans to create a dictionary that will be somewhere between a word list and an encyclopedia in scope. Such a dictionary carries a heavy load of expectancy that it will and can satisfy a very large and heterogeneous constituency: speakers, learners, and linguists––in short, both language insiders and outsiders. Nevertheless, we should not really talk in the singular about “a” dictionary, but rather accept that in this digital-technological age, the lexicographic material and analyses collected by a community-based documentation project team can be purposefully transposed into many different formats depending on the content, the audience, and the purpose of the intended lexical resource. Lexicographic by-products can vary along numerous dimensions, as shown in Table 1. Going forward, any new community-based dictionary emerging out of a documentation project aimed at revitalization will likely feature hybridized components of many of the dictionary types listed in Table 1. It will probably be bilingual, descriptive, pedagogical, analytical, variationist, both mono- and multi-media, and eventually on-line. There are many excellent sources in the literature that address best practices or principles for making dictionaries or other pedagogically informed lexical materials that either focus on or are relevant for NNA languages. The collection of chapters in Frawley, Hill, and Munro (2002), Durkin (2016), and Hanks and de Schryver (2017) are good places to start, as are more community-directed publications such as the Mohawk Language Steering Committee Report (1993) and Kell (2014). Anderson’s (2017) dissertation on developing a dictionary for Tunica and Genee and Junker’s (2018) account of creating an on-line suite of lexicographic tools and by-products for Blackfoot provide excellent models of dictionary projects based on responsible and reciprocal community engagement. Sadly, though, much lexicographic research has focused on dictionaries for major world languages or on the minority or Indigenous languages of Africa or Australasia, many of which have much different structural properties and word-formation patterns than the languages of NNA, not to mention different rates of literacy, speaker densities, and economies of scale. These are all factors that affect the funding and human resources that can be committed to a dictionary project within a community––typically a highly collaborative and hopefully mutually supporting undertaking that may take many years to complete while drawing constant criticism within the community from speakers, educators, young users, and tribal leaders, all of whom may have 3 A concordancer is a basic type of computer program that searches a digital language corpus and produces various types of analyses such as word lists, frequency counts, keywords in context, and lists of recurring collocations.

Lexicography 

 483

differing expectations. The volume edited by Frawley, Hill, and Munro (2002) is essential as a resource, focusing as it does on lexicographic best practices for the languages of the Americas based on first-hand experience. In particular, their introductory chapter on ten issues for dictionary-making is an excellent set of considerations for any community-based dictionary project team to consider at the outset.

20.3 Attributes of good dictionaries Because of the highly complex morphological nature of many NNA languages (both in terms of the grammatical material that combines to form synthetic verbs and the numerous lexical and derivational affixes that can enter into word formation across the lexicon), much debate and decision-making concerning the organization of dictionaries revolves around whether to list roots, stems, and affixes at all––perhaps in a separate section of the dictionary––or to list them alongside whole words in the target language, giving them their own entry. At the crux of this determination is whether to list only pronounceable items or to give status to recurrent morphological building blocks––of which there are many in the typical NNA language––so as to better facilitate language learning and language analysis. Should a dictionary team decide to only list whole words, then one can also ask, why not list fixed expressions that are larger than words, too? In many cases, these have lexical status and may be of equal importance to learners for developing conversational fluency. In other words, why not meld a phrasebook model into the dictionary? Most recent dictionaries also include a grammatical sketch, after all. In the chapter on Lexicalization and Lexical Meaning, some common lexical patterns above the word level are described. Should these types of constructions not also be included? Croft and Sutton (2016) make the case that constructional frames––as in little templates for word- and phrase-formation––also have a place in dictionaries. In a nutshell, this is akin to including both ingredients (morphemes) and recipes (word and phrasal patterns) in a dictionary.4 Another decision point about dictionary-making concerns how to organize derived words and multiple senses of a single word: as nested or as separate entries? The multiple decision points that a lexicography team must face are always local and cannot be resolved in the absolute. However, a few general observations can be made based on a survey of some of the major NNA language dictionaries published since the early 1990s, 4 To extend this dictionary=cookbook metaphor just a little, new-style dictionaries might also consider adding “menus” (information about collocation or which expressions go with other expressions) and “seasonal cooking ideas” (information about genre and register); in short, much more information about usage considerations. The best cookbooks have photographs and indices and sidebar tutorials for stepby-step generic assembly or cooking techniques and the best dictionaries can do the same by including grammatical sketches, templates for different word-formation protocols, cultural information, and the like.

484 

 Sally Rice

Tab. 1: Types of dictionaries frequently associated with documentation projects for NNA languages depending on language, target audience/purpose, content, scope, and medium of presentation.  

language

   

audience



Dictionary Types monolingual in target language only; this is the type of dictionary associated with major world languages and is the ideal goal for any language with a large, literate, and growing group of speakers; it may contain pronunciation and part-of-speech information, but the focus is on definitions (including for multiple senses of an entry); it may also include etymologies, but rarely does it give sample sentences showing usage (monolingual dictionaries often take usage facts for granted)

bilingual usually a bi-directional dictionary with one section organized by the target language and a second by the language of analysis; most products of language revitalization/sustainability projects are bilingual

comparative multi-lingual, listing only cognate forms (usually in IPA format rather than a practical orthography) with minimalistic meaning across the language family; it possibly includes some reconstructed forms for a hypothesized proto-language; comparative dictionaries have a special appeal to typologists and historical linguists, as well as documentary linguists hoping for insight into a language family; literate speakers also report a fascination with comparative (or cognate) word lists

pedagogical/special purpose aimed at children or possibly adult learners and translators; it may be constituted as a simple glossary, giving definitions (perhaps by illustration) for a contained word list, as in an accompanying text (for pedagogical purposes) or by a particular subject area (as in a medical dictionary, a glossary of terms related to land rights, or ethnobotanical terms); pedagogical dictionaries may contain collocations, fixed expressions, and other multi-word units that have conversational utility so as to develop fluency in the learner, whereas special-purpose dictionaries can aid translators negotiating between older speakers and, say, governmental agencies

descriptive/reference aimed at both speakers and non-speakers, with definitions containing pronunciation and part-of-speech information, possible etymologies, perhaps a short grammatical sketch, cross-reference to synonyms/ paraphrases and antonyms, example expressions containing the lexical entry, and numerous cross-references to other forms; ample inclusion of sample expressions as a way of handling ambiguity, or exemplifying discourse particles, evidentials, and other hard-to-define lexicosyntactic phenomena

analytical aimed primarily at linguists and highly interested speakers; the terminological nomenclature may be dense and it may include much phonological and grammatical information of a theoretical and/or typological nature with dubious relevance to revitalization; there is usually much cross-referencing across related items; importantly, it generally lists bound morphemes (be they roots/stems or affixes and clitics) alongside free-standing lexical items, as well as collocations that have the semantic force of a lexical item

Lexicography 

 485

Tab. 1 (continued)

Dictionary Types



official/standardized gives one form (pronunciation and spelling) and meaning for each entry or sub-entry, ignoring dialectal or regional variation for the purpose of promoting a standardized form of the language

   

dialectal/variationist gives equal treatment, as far as possible, to historical, regional, generational, and perhaps gender differences (“varieties”) in pronunciation, usage, and meaning

monomedia text only, often printed; best for reference

multimedia text and sound; best for learners

on-line either mono- or multimedia; best for widest distribution on interactive websites or cellphone apps

scope



format



when language documentation and revitalization efforts began to eclipse more analytical pursuits among field linguists. First and foremost, the best dictionaries are as accessible as possible to the widest range of users. This means that an entry should have something for everyone––a pronunciation guide; part-of-speech and grammatical class information; isolated root/ stem/affix/clitic components; basic definition(s); morphological break-down, if relevant; literal and figurative glosses; sample expressions in the language with the key word highlighted, accompanied by a free gloss, and sourced to the contributor or speech community; dialectally variant forms and meanings, if available, and also sourced to a speaker; meta-commentary from Elders or the strongest available speakers about usage or socio-cultural-historical significance of the entry; pointers to synonyms or morphologically related items; and possibly even an etymology. This may seem especially cumbersome for each entry, but the individual entry components could be presented in different fonts, colors, or font sizes and styles so they can be scanned quickly and relevant information driving each unique act of look-up can either be homed in on or ignored, as needed. Indentation or use of columns, special symbols, and good spacing between entries also help a user navigate the thicket of all this information. There will necessarily be a lot of cross-referencing and, consequently, much redundancy in a dictionary whose entries contain multiple fields of content, but this goes with the territory of providing multiple access points and multiple routes of inquiry to the widest range of users. In this digital, hypertext era, space limitations are not the problem they once were. Moreover, one of the best, recent, community-based, print dictionaries of a NNA language, the New Lakota Dictionary (Lakota Language Consortium 2011) is large––a

486 

 Sally Rice

little over 1,000 pages, sturdy, and attractive. It is also rich in what it includes in an entry, in its clear and helpful formatting and use of graphical symbols, and in its extensive cross-referencing (three sample entries from this dictionary are presented in Table 2). One cannot over-emphasize the role of usage in compiling and organizing a practical dictionary aimed at community members and heritage learners, as well as the scientific community. It must be grounded in what speakers actually say and do with the language, thus putting the target language front and center, not the language of analysis. Lexical data collection may have started from simple word lists of objects and basic events (or even verb paradigms!) elicited for the dictionary, but it should not end there. The bulk of the entries should come from collections of natural discourse (not just elicitation), be that text collections, transcriptions of narrative, or most optimally for learners, transcriptions of conversations between fluent speakers. Such a collection procedure guarantees that entries will not be pre-determined and that any and all useful expressions gleaned from spontaneous language use will be represented in the dictionary, including exclamations, interjections, hesitation devices, greetings, transactional terms (here, take this; what?), terms for agreeing or disagreeing with another speaker, discourse particles, idioms, stock expressions (such as how to start and end a story), as well as other more expected referential material (typically, nouns) and relational material (typically, verbs) more easily compiled through elicitation. Moreover, by including a lot of specific cultural and historical commentary about the meaning and usage of a term, or by excluding vocabulary of a sacred or secret nature, the dictionary will never lose sight of being based on usage and, ultimately, usefulness as determined by the community. Assuming that any community-based dictionary in this era of language reclamation and revitalization will be aimed at non-speaking learners but encapsulating as much as possible of the language and cultural knowledge still retained by speakers, some of whom may be quite elderly, it is important that the language of analysis component (usually English, Spanish, or French in the North American context) not be too prominent. The “reverse” part of the dictionary should definitely follow the target language section and should be shorter and more schematic in scope. More than anything, it should be a pointer back to the target language listings, which should contain enough explanation, exemplification, and cross-referencing information that helps users navigate within the target language section and not back-and-forth across the two language sections. Of course, other parts of the dictionary will necessarily be in the language of analysis and will probably precede the target language listings. These parts include general instructions on using the dictionary, a guide to reading sample entries, a list of contributors with linguistically relevant biographical information (such as the community they were raised in, the languages they speak, birth year, etc.), a list and explanation of abbreviations and symbols (which should be no more than a single page, using very plain language to explain), a very short grammatical sketch, a general guide to the orthography and phonetic alphabet used within a given entry, a set of references, and perhaps separate indices organized by roots and affixes in the target language listed

Lexicography 

 487

with the most schematic of meanings. It is important that these language-of-analysis sections not overwhelm the entries themselves.

20.4 Attributes of good dictionary entries It is fine and well to describe what should go into an optimal lexical entry in a dictionary of a Native North American language, but much more persuasive to show some actual entries that can be compared in terms of presentation and information load. Table 2 presents the entries from seven fairly recent dictionaries to three translation equivalents from English: pear, table, and hey; in other words, two likely figurative (and multi-morphemic) lexicalizations for terms of acculturation, and an attention-getting interjection––in nearly all languages, a noun, a deverbalized noun, and a particle, respectively. The purpose of Table 2 is to provide a quick and superficial comparison across three fairly “simple” lexical items (simpler than an average verb in any of these languages) in seven unrelated languages to show the range of representational decisions that the lexicographic teams associated with these dictionaries settled on. The reader is encouraged to consider the content and format of these entries both as a potential learner and as a potential lexicographer––in other words, to consider both the ease of extraction and packaging of information within an individual lexical entry. In some cases, pointers to other related entries have been listed which were not in the original. They augment the original entries by providing valuable information that aids in figuring out the origins of the lexicalization. One might also consider the absence of an entry for hey in two of the languages (or, indeed, the absences of any similar type of interjection or phatic device, such as wow or other expression of surprise) and what this says about data source or target audience. The Lakota dictionary provides a different type of usage fact (other than sample utterances) by including symbols representing the most highly frequent and most commonly recognized morphemes in the language. All in all, there are time and space constraints that each lexicographic project team must operate within. However, there are also a wide range of possibilities out there for enriched and enriching dictionary entries. Any team is encouraged to spend time looking at an array of dictionaries in related and unrelated languages before settling on a general organizational, content, and formatting strategy.

488 

 Sally Rice

20.5 How an understanding of lexicalization ­tendencies in NNA languages can make complex languages come alive for speakers and learners Lexicography, like transcription, is an exercise in rendering a spoken language into print. Putting a world on paper (or computer screen) is like putting some plant or animal tissue under a microscope: individual components can be isolated and examined and possibly identified, but textual representation is no substitution for actual speech. This transposition with language from sound to orthographic form can be very powerful for literate speakers who often gain tremendous insights about their language when they can separate out individual morphemes in a complex word or when highly frequent “everyday Tab. 2: Dictionary entries (or partial entries) for the target language equivalents of two terms of acculturation (pear and table), the former a natural kind and the latter an artifact, and a common attention-getting interjection (hey). Sources are: Navajo [dene/athapaskan] (Young and Morgan 1987], Plains Cree [algonquian] (Wolfart and Ahenakew 1998), Oneida [iroquoian] (Michelson et al. 2002), Klallam [salishan] (Montler 2012), Nez Perce [sahaptian] (Aoki 1994), Lakota [siouan] (Lakota Language Consortium 2011), and Hopi [uto-aztecan] (Hill and Malotki 1997). In all cases, formatting (or lack thereof) reflects the original entry.  

Comparing Dictionary Entries

Plains Cree

Navajo

  ‘pear’

‘table’

‘hey’

bitsee’ hólóní, pear (bitsee’, its tail + hólǫ́, it exists + -nligature + -í, the one: the one that has a tail).

bikáá’adání, table (for eating) (bikáá’, on it + ’adą́, eating takes place + -n-, ligature + -í, the one).

héí! Hey! (shouting to attract attention).

kâ-cîposicik inm pears [i.  e., plural form of cîposi– vai, lit. pointy ones]

mîcisowinâhtikw– ni dining table, table

––––––––

[author note, there is a related entry: cîposi– vai be pointed]

[author note, related entries on the same page: mîciso– vai eat, have a meal; feed (e.  g., bird); chew the cud (e.  g., ruminant) mîcisowin– ni eating, meal, eating-habits, food

Lexicography 

 489

Tab. 2 (continued)  

Comparing Dictionary Entries

  ‘pear’

‘table’

‘hey’

-atekhwahlakhw-/-atekhwahlaʔtsl- v. > n. table. atekhwahlákhwaʔ table. akwatekhwahlákhwaʔ my table, satekhwahlákhwaʔ your table. With -ʔke locative suffix: atekhwahlakhwá∙ke on the table...... waʔkatekhwahlaʔtslú∙nekeʔ I moved the table. Composed of: -ate- semireflexive, -khw- food, -hel-/-hl- set on top of, place on, -hkw- instrumental or -ʔtsl- nominalizer. Derived Bases: yutekhwahlaʔtslolóktaʔ tablecloth.

––––––––

páas 〖√paas √pear〗pear. [from English ‘pears’] {nəxʷčxíkʷt cə n̉páas. Split your pear. (es)} VAR: páa (es)

c̉aʔc̉ítən 〖√c ̉iʔ =ci=tənaas √upon= food=instr〗table. (mjt; lc; es)}… {ƛ’čaʔwíyǝt ʔaʔ cǝ c ̉aʔc̉ítən. Put it under the table. (es) | céʔyǝt ʔaʔ cǝ ćaʔćítən. Put it on the table. (es)…} VAR: ćaʔćítn (es) VAR: c ̉aʔčítən (abt)

ʔée 〖ʔée hey〗[interjection] hey. {ʔée, ʔǝstúʔŋət ʔaỷ ʔǝn̉sqʷiʔ ʔaʔ tə xʷənʔáŋ? Hey, why are you talking that way? (aa)}

k͗ócac N pointed. k͗watsatsk͗wátsats ‘sharp’ is recorded (Swadesh 1930:1.14) in a Downriver dialect of Nez Perce as used by Cayuse Indians of Oregon. It is possible that the form historically had an initial *k͗ʷ. … k͗ók͗cac a pear; so named for the “pointed” shape of the fruit

hipinwé∙s eating place, restaurant, inn, dining table, kitchen

hó∙m P ahoy, hello, a call to attract attention hó∙m or hóm kawóʔ koná wá∙qoʔ “há∙mtiʔc hó∙m náma kú∙s” hiwalatiyó∙xna. And then he shouted as he waited, “Hello there. What are you doing?” HA35.101

Oneida

yotsheʔtʌ́∙tuheʔ v. > n. pear. Composed of: yo- neuter patient, -tshe-/-tsheʔt- bottle, jar, quart, -hlʌʔtu-/-ʌʔtube attached, hanging, -heʔ habitual.

Nez Perce

Klallam

[author note: literally, ‘the hanging bottle one’?]

[author note: elsewhere, one finds the entry for hipí and nwé∙s, given, in part, as follows: hipí VS (1) to eat, (2) with a reflexive prefix to eat oneself. The act of eating or biting oneself, even accidentally, was believed to turn the person into a cannibal. nwé∙s vsts place of …-ing; …hipínwe∙s dining hall, restaurant: “eating place.” Cf. hipí VS. (vsts refers to a deverbative thematic substantive suffix, as given in the abbreviations; in other words, it’s a place nominalizer)]

490 

 Sally Rice

Tab. 2 (continued)  

Comparing Dictionary Entries

  ‘pear’

‘table’

‘hey’

(tȟaspáŋ + pȟestola) N pear. Lit: ‘sharp-pointed apple.’

wágnawotapi [L] (2, in a distributive sense).’

Before moving on to discussing how distributivity is marked in Native American languages, I want to use this example to illustrate how distributivity is related to pluractionality. Note that for (11b) to be true there must be a plurality of events. If each child 𝑥 has to have the property that ‘I played with 𝑥’, then there will be at least as many playing events as there are children. Moreover, note that da- is verbal derivational morphology, so it appears to satisfy this additional requirement of our strict definition of pluractionals. So, is da- a distributivity operator or a pluractionality operator? There is only one hard and fast test, which is to see how event-based da- is. The critical fact is that certain distributors require distinct events in a stronger way than others do. For instance, suppose I take bag of pecans and place them on a scale. In English, I can say I weighed every pecan, but it is infelicitous to say I weighed each pecan. The latter distributivity operator strongly prefers a scenario where each pecan is weighed in a



Pluractionality and distributivity 

 521

distinct event (Tunstall, 1998). This is much closer to a pluractional reading, and so it would be critical to test da- in these kinds of scenarios to see whether we want to call it a pluractional. Beyond testing whether the distributivity operator is event-based, ideally to be classified as a pluractional, we should be able to find some other property that the operator shares with pluractionals more widely. For instance, we could see whether it is only acceptable with predicates of events and not stative verbs, which is a property pluractionals often have. Similarly, we could look and see whether it is accepted with verbs of all aktionsart classes because, as we have seen, the distribution of pluractionals is often constrained by lexical aspect. In the meantime, distributivity marking on verbs provides a true edge case between pluractionality and distributivity. What makes distributivity a broader phenomenon only partially overlapping with pluractionality is that it is not marked only on verbs. In fact, now that we have these notions of key and share, we can classify languages and expressions in those languages by whether distributivity is marked on the key or the share. The Navajo example in (11) is clearly an example of share-marking. Because the share is canonically the verb phrase, which can be quite large and varied, share-marking can occur on various expressions. In Navajo the verb can be marked, but we also commonly see share marking on verb phrase arguments. Consider the case of Piipaash in (12). Here the quantifier ma.t-čaamxperm ‘all’ bears the morpheme -xper which can also acts as share marking on verbs of the kind we saw in Navajo in (11). Here, though, it occurs inside the noun phrase ‘all the sticks’, and as a share, enforces the distributive interpretation of the subject ipač xvikk ‘two men’, as the translation indicates. (12)      

Piipash (Yuman)    Ipač xvikk ?ii ma.t-čaamxperm man-pl:nom 3-two-sg-ss stick refl-3-pl-dist.share-ds ‘(Each of) two men carried all the sticks.’

(Gil, 1995, ex. 39b) paayšík. s-carry-dl-real

A special case of share-marking an argument is that well-known case of so-called distributive indefinites. This is an extremely common strategy, more common that what we saw in (12), and suggests that there is something special about the semantics of indefinites lending them to distributivity marking. Tlingit provides the best-studied example of this phenomenon for Native American languages (Cable, 2014). We see an example in (13). Here the numeral nás’k ‘three’ bears a special distributivity morpheme -gáa, which derives a numeral that requires a distributive interpretation of the sentence. Note that crosslinguistically distributive numerals are often formed via reduplication (see e.  g., Henderson 2014). (13)      

Tlingit (Na-Dene)  (Cable, 2014, ex. 3b) Nás’gigáa xáat has aawasháat. three.DIST fish pl.3o.pfv.3S.catch ‘They caught three fish each’ or ‘They caught three fish each time.’

522 

 Robert Henderson

It is important to see that in this example, the distributive numeral is share-marked. That is, it is not the case that we distribute over the fish, requiring that each individual fish be caught by them. No, instead, the two available readings require that each object belonging to the key stand in relation to a share with three (distinct fish). In the first reading, the key is they. Each individual must satisfy the key caught three fish. In the second reading, the key is some contextually supplied temporal extent. Each time must have the share property of being a time in which “they caught three fish”. As discussed in Henderson (2014), crosslinguistically distributive numerals are often ambiguous with respect to their keys, which can be expressions that denote worlds, times, events, individuals, etc. That said, there appears to be an implicational universal that if such a share marker allows distributivity over worlds, it allows it over times/events, and it if allows it over the latter, it allows it over keys that denote individuals. Besides the head of the share, the verb, or one of its arguments, we also see share marking through adverbial elements. Choctaw provides a nice example, where quantifiers come in noun phrase internal and verb phrase internal forms, including distributive quantifiers. In (14) the distributivity item áyyokaalih is external to the noun hattak ‘man’, and is instead in the VP (which can be detected through fronting facts as discussed in Broadwell 2006, p. 227). (14)      

Choctaw (Muskogean)  Hattak áyyokaali-h písa-li-h. man all:distr-tns see:n-1si-tns ‘I saw each person.’

(Broadwell, 2006, p. 231, ex. 55)

There is another important class of distributivity operators that often appear as verbal adjuncts, which I treat below under the heading simultaneous distributivity, but we will see that it is different than the simple share marking we see in examples like (14). For now, we will move on to the second major category of distributivity marking, that is, marking the expression that is divided up. This kind of marking almost always appears in the guise of quantifiers with distributive lexical semantics, sometimes in addition to other quantificational meaning. We see a few examples in (15–16). (15)      

Inuit  uqalimaaga-it atu-nit titiqqa-li-it book-pl each-3pl.acc letter-which.has-pl ‘Each of the books has letters in it.’

(Compton, 2004, ex. 48)

(16)      

St’át’imcets (Salishan)  (Matthewson, 1998, p. 324, ex. 2a.) zi7zeg’ i  wa7 píx-em’ kwámem ku míxalh each pl.det prog hunt-intr take(red) det bear ‘Each of the hunters caught a bear.’ (they caught one each).

It is important here to pause and emphasize the difference between distributive indefinites, which are a kind of share-marking, and key-marking distributive quantifiers.



Pluractionality and distributivity 

 523

Indefinites are themselves usually, at least descriptively, considered quantifiers. This means that in the difference between (16) and (13) we see clearly the difference between keys and shares. In (16), we have to take then hunters—the key—and split them up and require each to satisfy the predicate ‘caught a bear’. It is this key that is marked. In (13), ‘three fish’ is marked, but it isn’t the key. We don’t take those three fish, split them up, and require each to satisfy the predicate ‘be caught by them’. Instead, what we split up is the ‘they’, the key, which is not marked. Each individual in the group that ‘they’ denotes must then satisfy the share, namely, ‘catch three fish’, which includes the distributive operator. We turn now to the final case of distributive expressions that we will consider here. These expressions are special because they do not fit clearly into the key-share paradigm. Instead, these expressions express simultaneous distributivity. We see an example here from the Comox-Sliammon. Comox-Sliammon has a series of distributivity items built on numerals that roughly translate as num-by-num in English. (17)      

Comox-Sliammon a. paɁapyaɁ ‘one by one’ b. saɁasyaɁ ‘two by two’ c. etc.

 (Mellesmoen, 2018, ex. 14)

Crucially, Mellesmoen 2018 notes that these distributors not only distribute over an individual argument, but also place conditions on the event. In particular, (18) is felicitous if Daniel and Kaining arrive at the same time, but through different doors. In contrast, it is infelicitous if they arrive at the same time through the same door. That is, this distributor requires at least spatially distinct events, in addition to distributing over an argument. (18)      

Comox-Sliammon (Salishan) paɁapyaɁ qwəl təs-uɬ Kaining dist come reach-pst Kaining ‘Daniel and Kaining arrive one by one.’

  higa and  

 (Mellesmoen, 2018, ex. 12) Daniel Daniel  

The fact that expressions such as these simultaneously place constraints on both the event the verb denotes and an argument of that verb is why we called it simultaneous distributivity. This simultaneous effect also degrades the key-share relationship. In a standard distributive construction, the distributor only places conditions on the key. For this reason these expressions are special, and should be classified apart. In fact, in virtue of the fact that such expressions tend to require a plurality of events and structure those events in the way pluractionals do, expression like paɁapyaɁ in (18) have been called pluractional adverbials. We saw in the previous section why I am hesitant to call non-derivational morphology pluractional, but being treated thusly by previous authors confirms that these expressions are not ordinary distributivity operators.

524 

 Robert Henderson

22.5 Documenting distributivity Distributivity is much better studied than pluractionality, and is a somewhat more unified phenomenon. Thus, a general plan for describing the distributivity operators in a language is a bit easier. The first step would be an investigation of distributive predication. That is, verbs can be inherently distributive (like die), inherently collective (like gather), or are so-called ‘mixed’ predicates, allowing collective or distributive readings (like lift). Moreover, it is common to find two kinds of collective predicates, ones like gather and ones like be numerous, the latter of which are ‘stronger’ in the sense that they are infelicitous with more distributivity items. We can see this in English, where a distributive quantifier like everyone is ungrammatical with be numerous (e.  g., #Everyone is numerous), but grammatical with gather (e.  g., Everyone gathered in the park). Once verbal predicates have been categorized, one can move to looking for distributivity operators. The reason why it is helpful to know about different predicate types is that we are more likely to elicit distributivity operators if we initially work with so-called ‘mixed-predicates’. The reason is that these expressions are ambiguous between distributive and collective interpretations, and so one can lead speakers into disambiguating the two readings with distributivity operators. Here it can be helpful to use act-out tasks. For instance, if we learn that ‘touch’ is a mixed predicate, bringing a set of items to touch in various ways while the speaker describes the actions would be one way to elicit distributivity items without asking for translation from a contact language, which can bias the kinds of structures that are produced—for instance, if the contact language mostly has key-marking, one might only elicit key-marking with a translation task. Just as with pluractionals, it can be helpful to consider any reduplicative morphological processes. While less commonly marked through reduplication than pluractionality, there are cases of distributivity marked through reduplication, like the Tlingit case discussed in the previous section. Finally, once a set of distributivity items has been uncovered, one can investigate the strength of distributivity, that is, what can it distribute over. We want to know, for a given distributivity operator, whether it can have a distributive key that is a distributive predicate, mixed predicate, gather -type collective predicate, or a be-numerous-type collective predicate, where these are ordered by how strongly they resist distributivity. Finally, it is important to know whether the distributors in question require event-based readings in the sense discussed above when we considered the difference between pluractionality and distributivity.

22.6 Summary and Challenges This chapter has provided a high-level crosslinguistic survey of pluractionality and distributivity in the Native North American languages. An additional goal was to present



Pluractionality and distributivity 

 525

strategies for determining the types of pluractionality and distributivity available in languages for which those categories have not been extensively documented. This second goal grows out of the fact that comprehensive semantic documentation of distributivity and pluractionality in Native North American languages is limited. So, for instance, while we have a fairly good understanding of the semantic parameters along which pluractional and distributive operators vary across languages, we cannot say anything about how different types of distributivity and pluractionality are distributed across languages and families in Native North America. Doing this kind of basic description is a necessary prerequisite to this larger challenge of understanding the typology and areal distribution of pluractionality and distributivity in the Native North American languages. It is my hope that this chapter can serve as a springboard to this next step in documentation and description of pluractionality and distributivity in these languages.

References Broadwell, George Aaron. 2006. A choctaw reference grammar. University of Nebraska Press. Lincoln, Nebraska. Cable, Seth. 2014. Distributive numerals and distance distributivity in tlingit (and beyond). Language 90. 562–606. Champollion, Lucas. 2017. Parts of a whole: Distributivity as a bridge between aspect and measurement, volume 66. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Compton, Richard. 2004. On quantifiers and bare nouns in inuktitut. Toronto Working Papers in Linguistics 23. Cusic, David. 1981. Verbal plurality and aspect. Doctoral Dissertation, Stanford University. Garrett, Andrew. 2001. Reduplication and infixation in Yurok: morphology, semantics, and diachrony. International Journal of American Linguistics 67. 264–312. Gil, David. 1995. Universal quantifiers and distributivity. In Quantification in natural languages, 321–362. Springer. Emmon Bach el al. (eds) Berlin, Germany. Henderson, Robert. 2012. Ways of pluralizing events. Doctoral Dissertation, University of California, Santa Cruz. Henderson, Robert. 2014. Dependent indefinites and their post-suppositions. Semantics and Pragmatics 7(9). 1–67. Henderson, Robert. 2017. Swarms: Spatiotemporal grouping across domains. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 35. 161–203. Jacobson, Steven A. 1984. Yup’ik Eskimo Dictionary. Fairbanks: Alaska Native Language Center, University of Alaska. Junker, Marie-Odile. 1994. Reduplication in east cree. Algonquian Papers-Archive 25. 265–273. Lachler, Jordan. 2006. A grammar of laguna keres. Albuquerque: The University of New Mexico. Majid, Asifa & Stephen C. Levinson. 2011. The senses in language and culture. The Senses and Society 6. 5–18. Matthewson, Lisa. 1998. Determiner systems and quantificational strategies: Evidence from Salish. World Theses 1. The Hague: Holland Academic Graphics. Mellesmoen, Gloria. 2018. A one (morpheme) by one (morpheme) approach to paapya:-v-as a temporal pluractional infix in comox-sliammon. UBCWPL 143. In Papers for the International Conference on Salish and Neighbouring Languages 53, University of British Columbia Working Papers in Linguistics 47, Marianne Huijsmans, Roger Lo, Daniel Reisinger, and Oksana Tkachman (eds.).

526 

 Robert Henderson

Mithun, Marianne. 1988. Lexical categories and the evolution of number marking, 211–234. New York: Academic Press. Newman, Paul. 1980. The classification of chadic within afroasiatic. Leiden: Universitaire Pers Leiden. Swanton, J.R. 1911. Haida. Handbook of American Indian languages. volume Bureau of American Ethnology Bulletin (40). Washington D.C: US Government Printing Office. Tufvesson, Sylvia. 2011. Analogy-making in the semai sensory world. The Senses and Society 6. 86–95. Tunstall, Susanne Lynn. 1998. The interpretation of quantifiers: semantics & processing. Doctoral Dissertation, University of Massachusetts at Amherst. Watkins, Laurel Jayne. 1980. A grammar of Kiowa. Doctoral Dissertation, University of Kansas. Winter, Yoad. 2002. Flexibility principles in boolean semantics: The interpretation of coordination, plurality, and scope in natural language. Cambridge, Massachusetts MIT press. Wood, Esther Jane. 2007. The semantic typology of pluractionality. Doctoral Dissertation, University of California, Berkeley. Yazzie, Helen Yellowman et al. 2000. Da: the Navajo distributive plural preverb. In T. Fernald & K. Hale (eds.), Diné Bizaad Naalkaah: Navajo language investigations, working papers on endangered and less familiar languages 3, 141–160. Cambridge, MA: MIT.

Andrea Wilhelm

23 Mass and count nouns Abstract: This chapter gives a short survey of how the count/mass distinction manifests in North American Indigenous languages. I will show that unlike in European languages, number inflection does not necessarily play a prominent role in the distinction. North American Indigenous languages use their own unique morphosyntactic and lexical resources, which include not only the noun phrase but also the verb. I also emphasize the need for more in-depth study of this interesting phenomenon, both on the morphosyntactic and the semantic level, and make some methodological suggestions.

23.1 Introduction For the purposes of this chapter, count nouns are defined as those which can be counted, i.  e., combined on their own with numerals, e.  g., one person, two months, three stones, four chickens, ten shoes (Wierzbicka 1988, Chierchia 2010, Rothstein 2017). Mass nouns (also referred to as non-count nouns or uncountable nouns) are defined as nouns which cannot be counted, i.  e., not combined on their own with numerals, for example, *one sand, *two clothing(s), *three money(s), *four beef(s), *ten groceries. They need additional linguistic material in order to combine with numerals: one grain of sand, two pieces of clothing, three forms of money, four pounds of beef, ten bags of groceries. In many European languages, count and mass nouns differ systematically in a number of salient morphosyntactic properties and are therefore treated as distinct classes of nouns (e.  g., Jespersen 1924:188–199, Huddleston & Pullum 2002:333–354 for English). In English, for example, only mass nouns can occur without a determiner in a sentence, compare I saw sand and *I saw shoe. Count nouns require a determiner or must be in the plural form: I saw a shoe or I saw shoes. English count and mass nouns also take different subsets of determiners and quantifiers: (1)          

Count a shoe many shoes *much shoe(s) few shoes every shoe

Mass *a sand *many sand(s) much sand *few sand(s) *every sand

Most saliently, perhaps, count nouns inflect for the grammatical category number— they have singular and plural forms: person – persons, month – months, stone – stones, chicken – chickens, shoe – shoes. Mass nouns do not; they occur only in singular form or only in plural form: sand – *sands, clothing – *clothings, money – *monies, beef – *beefs, https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-023

528 

 Andrea Wilhelm

*grocery – groceries.1 European languages thus make a morphosyntactic or grammatical distinction between count and mass nouns. Are there notions or concepts that underlie the morphosyntactic behaviour? Intuitively, count nouns refer to individuated items and that is why they can be counted, while mass nouns refer to substances without clear divisions or boundaries and therefore cannot be counted (cf. Wierzbicka 1988). For example, I can add to sand or divide sand into smaller amounts and still call it sand. If, on the other hand, I divide a shoe into parts, I am no longer left with something I could call shoe. It would be called a piece of a shoe or perhaps the heel, sole, and so on. I will call this property of count but not mass nouns atomicity (cf. Rothstein 2017, Doetjes 2013, Huddleston & Pullum 2002, see also Bunt 1985:45–46).2 It is however a matter of debate whether the count/mass distinction has a semantic foundation in atomicity (or a similar property). For example, arguably the mass nouns rice and clothing refer to an accumulation of atomic items (grains of rice, pieces of clothing). Conversely, there are count nouns that do not seem to be atomic, e.  g., one (large) herd can be divided into two (smaller) herds. And there are many, many nouns which act mass in mass contexts and count in count contexts, cf. rope (here’s some rope / here’s a rope), chocolate (I ate some chocolate / two chocolates), fence (this stretch of fence is rotten / your yard needs a fence), fear (much fear / many fears). These so-called flexible nouns are polysemous, in that they have an atomic sense and one that is not atomic. Nouns in other languages do not necessarily behave like English nouns. For example, information is mass in English (much information) but count in German (viele Informationen ‘many informations’). Tools is count in English (one/five/ten tools) but its Dënesųłiné counterpart layúé is not (*ɂįlághe ‘one’/*sǫlághe ‘five’/*honéna ‘ten’ layúé). And unlike English wheat, Dënesųłiné łéstł’olá has a count use, e.  g., hunéna łéstł’olá ‘ten [kernels of] wheat’. This shows us two things: First, it is not something about the real-world referents which makes a noun count or mass. Count/mass is a property of nouns (or noun phrases), not things. Second, we cannot use the translation of a noun into another language to determine if it is count or mass (and atomic or not) — we need to determine the grammatical and semantic properties of a noun based on how it behaves in its own language. This has implications for Indigenous language documentation and revitalization. For example, language lessons cannot be designed based on English or another contact language. We need to teach nouns in natural sentences or discourses

1 Sands and monies are acceptable on special interpretations, namely ‘extensive area of sand; time’ and ‘currencies’, that are not derivable by simple semantic pluralization from the singular. 2 Formally, atomicity can be defined as follows (cf. Landman 1989:561): (i) Given set A closed under sum formation, set members a, b, c etc., and the part-of relation ≤   Atom: a is an atom in A iff for every b ∊ A: if b ≤ a then b = a.   Atomic: A is atomic iff for all b ∊ A: there is an a ∊ At such that a ≤ b, where At is the set of atoms in A. Set A is part of a formal semantic model for noun denotations. Atomicity thus is a semantic property, saying something about how count nouns may be conceptualized.



Mass and count nouns 

 529

which reveal the nouns’ properties and common usages, and such examples should also be part of dictionaries. Very few descriptions of Indigenous languages address the count/mass distinction. This chapter offers a representative selection of those descriptions which do exist and highlights the interesting ways in which Indigenous languages of North America encode countability. We will see that while not all languages distinguish count and mass nouns morphosyntactically, the available evidence suggests that all languages have nouns with atomic and nouns with nonatomic denotations. By presenting a range of phenomena, I also hope to give language revitalizers and documenters some ideas how to probe the countability nature of nouns in their own language. We start with languages in which nouns (or some other element in the noun phrase) show number inflection, because this property has been so important in previous discussions (section 2), then move on to languages without number inflection (section 3), verbal systems for making a count/mass distinction (section 4), methodological remarks (section 5), and conclude in section 6.

23.2 Languages with number inflection (in the nominal domain) 23.2.1 Nishnaabemwin Nishnaabemwin, a language of the Ojibwe or Anishinaabemowin group in the Algonquian family, is spoken in southern Ontario and into the U.S., roughly from around Lake Huron east to Lake Ontario. Nishnaabemwin has a morphosyntactic count/mass distinction, but the number of mass nouns is very small, much smaller than, for example, in English. Mass nouns are those which cannot be pluralized in Nishnaabemwin and are incompatible with numerals and elements such as the quantifier gakina ‘every’ (Valentine 2001:182, Mathieu 2012:186). Examples of count nouns are bagaan(ag) ‘nut(s)’, miigwan(ag) ‘feather(s)’, maanadikoshens(ag) ‘goat(s)’, akwaandawaagan(an) ‘ladder(s)’, makizin(an) ‘mocassin(s)’ (Mathieu 2012:183  f ); some mass nouns are doodooshaaboo ‘milk’, negaw ‘sand’, ziitaagan (also zhiiwtaagan) ‘salt’, waasgan(g) ‘pepper’, bkwezhgan ‘bread’, goon ‘snow’, bmaadziwin ‘life’, azhdaadwin ‘mutual revenge’ (Valentine 2001:182). Semantically, the mass nouns refer to abstract things or substances while the count nouns refer to individuated items (Valentine 2001:182). We can say that count denotations are atomic in Nishnaabemwin while mass denotations are not. The number of mass nouns is so small because many nouns which refer to a substance in the singular may also refer, in the plural, to units of that substance. For example, semaa ‘tobacco’ – semaag ‘wads or pieces of tobacco’, mashkosiw ‘grass’ – mashkosiwag ‘blades of grass’, (a)ki ‘earth’ – akiin ‘bits of earth’, bkwezhgan ‘bread’ – bkwezhganan

530 

 Andrea Wilhelm

‘slices or pieces of bread’ (Mathieu 2012:184, 186). Note that the units are idiosyncratic to the noun. This is different from the general mass-to-count mapping mechanisms known from English, ‘standard servings of X’ (she ordered a beer) and ‘different kinds of X’ (they make three different beers). The idiosyncracy of the units suggests that they are part of the lexical information of a given noun. These nouns are thus polysemous, as already noted in Rhodes (1985, 1990), and fall in the category of flexible nouns.3 Mathieu (2012) proposes that these nouns undergo a singulative derivation, i.  e., a process which derives a ‘single unit’ meaning from the substance meaning, and that this derivation is reflected in a shift from inanimate to animate gender. That this is a derivational process accounts for the fact that the unit meanings are idiosyncratic to each noun and must be lexically stored. Nishnaabemwin also has plural-only nouns, for example biiwekdamaagnan ‘(wood) shavings’, bootsan ‘boots’ (Valentine 2001:182), ndahiiman ‘my clothes, my stuff’ (Rhodes 1985:xiii). For Valentine (2001:182) these nouns are “basically thought of as consisting of a group of similar objects or a set of objects” and therefore inherently plural. This is reminiscent of aggregate nouns, which are discussed at the end of section 4.2.3. We do not find out if these Nishnaabemwin nouns are compatible with a numeral.

23.2.2 Nez Perce In Nez Perce, a Sahaptian language spoken in Idaho, Washington and Oregon, nominal number is not necessarily seen on the noun itself. However, all nouns can combine with plural- marked adjectives, showing that all nouns can occur in the plural (or rather, in a plural noun phrase):4 (2)      

Nez Perce (Deal 2017:143–144; emphasis mine) a. Himeeq’is ’itet’es-pe hii-we-s ki-kuckuc   big bag-in 3sbj-be-prs pl-small   ‘In the big bag there are little eggs.’

taam’am egg

3 Rhodes (1990:153) proposes that all substance nouns are polysemous, can be pluralized, and that therefore there is no morphosyntactic count/mass distinction in the language. I have decided to follow the more recent work of Valentine and Mathieu, which does find nonpluralizable/non-polysemous nouns, and also probes the count/mass distinction more deeply. An anonymous reviewer has suggested that the researchers’ different perspectives may be due to dialect differences. This is unlikely, however, as Valentine’s and Rhodes’ data come from many of the same communities. If anything, perhaps we are witnessing language change between the 1980’s and 2000’s. 4 Glosses and abbreviations of all examples have been adapted from the sources to conform to the Leipzig Glossing Rules. Other abbreviations used: cisloc = cislocative, evid = evidential, hum = human, in = inanimate, mom = momentaneous aspect, prf = perfect/perfective aspect (Nez Perce), rem.past = remote past tense.



Mass and count nouns 

   

b.





 531

(context: discussing road construction) He’ilpe’ilp sitx̂ hii-we-s x̂uysx̂uys ’iskit-pe pl.red mud 3sbj-be-prs slippery road-on ‘There are red muddy spots that are slippery on the road.’

Likewise, all nouns combine directly with numerals for counting: (3)                          

Nez Perce (Deal 2017:136–138) a. kii lepit ciickan   dem two blanket   ‘these two blankets’ b. (context: discussing a nosebleed)   Lepit kike’t hi-sseew-n-e   two blood 3sbj-drip-prf-rem.past   ‘Two drops of blood fell.’ c. (context: making sandwiches)   Linguist: Pii-’ni-m lepit ’ipeex̂!     2/1-give-cisloc.imp two bread     (lit. ‘Give me two bread!’)   Linguist: “What would you give me?“   Speaker: “If I heard that, I’d probably figure you wanted slices.”

In the plural and with numerals, substance-denoting nouns are interpreted as a portion or unit of that substance and, as with Nishnaabemwin, the unit is specific to that noun and context. Rothstein (2010, 2017) argues that atomic interpretations of nouns (if they are possible at all) are always relative to the given context. This context can be linguistic, such as morphemes and other words, or extralinguistic/situational. Note the situational contexts in which (2b) and (3b–c) emerged. Deal suggests that Nez Perce has a very general mechanism which maps the substance meaning into a unit, i.  e. atomic, denotation in certain contexts. Another context for atomic interpretations is what is sometimes called count adjectives (Quine 1960), e.  g. small, large, round. These are adjectives which in languages like English are infelicitous with substance-denoting nouns, e.  g. (the) small cat but #(the) small mud.5 In Nez Perce, such adjectives are felicitous with all nouns but result in different interpetations: himeeq’is picpic ‘(the) big cat’, himeeq’is kuus ‘(the) big portion of water’ (Deal 2017:145). However, Deal (2017) shows that even though all nouns can occur with numerals, count adjectives, and in plural noun phrases, that does not mean that there is no semantic count/mass distinction. Other contexts reveal that some Nez Perce nouns have inherently count meanings.

5 # indicates semantic ill-formedness rather than ungrammaticality.

532 

 Andrea Wilhelm

One such context is quantifiers. In Nez Perce, count nouns need to occur in a plural noun phrase in order to combine with a quantifier, mass nouns do not: (4)                        

  a.     b.     c.     d.    

Nez Perce (Deal 2017:149–151) miil’ac-wa ha-ham/*haama few/little-hum pl-man/*sg.man ‘a few men’ ?? miil’ac ta’c / ti-t’ac wix̂si’likeecet’es ?? few/little good/ pl-good chair ‘a few good chairs’     miil’ac cimuuxcimux lalx̂ few/little black coffee ‘a little black coffee’ miil’ac x̂ayx̂ayx̂ mayx few/little white sand ‘a little white sand’

Deal suggests that this is because the quantifiers require a cumulative noun denotation. Nouns like lalx̂ ‘coffee’ and mayx ‘sand’ are inherently cumulative, in that one can add more sand or coffee and still call the result lalx̂ and mayx respectively. Haama ‘man’ and wix̂si’likeecet’es ‘chair’ do not behave like this; if one adds more men or more chairs one needs to use a plural noun phrase. This indicates that without the pluralization, nouns like haama and and wix̂si’likeecet’es are not cumulative but denote (single) atoms, i.  e., they are semantically count.6

23.2.3 Michif Michif, spoken in Canada’s prairie provinces, is a mixed language. Part of the vocabulary and grammar are from Cree and Ojibwe (Algonquian) and part from French (Indo-European). The language has both French-style and Algonquian-style number marking. Gillon & Rosen (2016) show that in Michif, French-derived nouns display a clear morphosyntactic count/mass distinction while Algonquian-derived nouns do not. Numerals, the singular indefinite determiner aeñ, pluralization, and the quanitfier paahpeeyak ‘one by one’ are ungrammatical with a subset of French-derived nouns—those with substance and other mass-type meanings. Algonquian-derived nouns are grammatical with these elements, but certain nouns shift from a substance to a portion or unit interpretation. The units are idiosyncratic to the noun. Here are some nouns with aeñ, plural article lii and the plural demonstrative oñhiñ:

6 Note that this analysis suggests that Nez Perce count nouns denote single atoms (singular interpretation), unless there is plural marking somewhere in the noun phrase.



Mass and count nouns 

(5)        

Michif (Gillon & Rosen 2016:127–128) French-derived count nouns: garsoñ ‘boy’, lii ‘bed’ a. aeñ garsoñ ‘a boy’ b. lii garsoñ ‘(the) boys’ c. lii lii oñhiñ ‘these beds’ [det.pl bed dem.prox.in.pl]

(6)        

Michif (Gillon & Rosen 2016:127–128) French-derived mass nouns: mañzhi ‘food’, diloo ‘water’ a. *aeñ mañzhi b. *lii mañzhi c. *diloo oñhiñ

(7)        

Michif (Gillon & Rosen 2016: 131–133) Algonquian-derived object-denoting noun: takwaminaan ‘chokecherry’ a. aeñ takwaminaan ‘a chokecherry’ b. lii takwaminaan ‘(the) chokecherries’ c. (lii) takwaminaan-a oñhiñ ‘these chokecherries’

(8)        

Michif (Gillon & Rosen 2016:132–133) Algonquian-derived substance-denoting noun: tominikan ‘oil’ a. aeñ tominikan ‘one bottle of oil’ b. lii tominikan ‘oils/greases’ c. tominikan-a oñhiñ ‘these oils’ (i.  e., 2 bottles)

 533

The Algonquian-derived substance nouns might be polysemous, as in Nishnaabemwin, or there may be a general mapping mechanism as in Nez Perce. What matters is that they do have a nonatomic interpretation (perhaps along with an atomic one) while the object-denoting nouns do not. The syntactic environments in which the nouns occur are identical and thus cannot be responsible for the different behaviours of the French-derived and Algonquian-derived nouns. It appears that the nouns themselves have inherited their count/mass characteristics from their language of origin and stored them in the Michif lexicon. These characteristics include morphosyntactic ones as in (5)–(6), and semantic characteristics, specifically the availability of atomic and nonatomic denotations.

23.2.4 Other languages There are other languages where most if not all nouns appear to inflect for number, for example, Upriver Halkomelem and St’át’imcets, both from the Salish family and home to SW British Columbia, and Blackfoot, an Algonquian language spoken in the western plains of North America (Davis 2014, Frantz 1991, Wiltschko 2012). In Central Alaskan Yup’ik, an Eskimoan language, at least some substance nouns can take the dual or plural (Mithun 1999:80). On the other hand, in Zuni, a language isolate of the American south-

534 

 Andrea Wilhelm

west, mass and abstract nouns seem to appear only in the plural (Newman 1965:31–33, 55–56), suggesting a clear morphosyntactic distinction. More information is needed for these languages.

23.3 Languages without number inflection (in the nominal domain) We now turn to languages in which nouns or noun phrases do not show number marking. In such languages, a noun’s meaning comprises singular and plural (and any number value), that is, the nouns are number-neutral. We will see that differences between mass and count nouns still exist; they just do not manifest in grammatical number. Number inflection thus is not a prerequisite for a count/mass distinction.

23.3.1 Dënesųłıné Dënesųłıné, a Dene or Athabaskan language spoken north of the Canadian prairies, has no number inflection for nouns or other nominal elements such as demonstratives. Nouns have the same form in singular and plural contexts (Wilhelm 2008:46). The language clearly distinguishes count and mass nouns; only the former are directly compatible with numerals: (9)     (10)    

Dënesųłıné nouns compatible with numerals (Wilhelm 2008:48): tthe ‘stone, pipe’, ke ‘shoe’, ɂerıhtł’ı́schëné ‘pen, pencil’, dechën ‘stick, bush, tree’, jíechogh ‘apple (or similar-size fruit)’, tł’uli ‘rope’, ɂerihtł’ı́s ‘sheet(s) of paper, book’, thanakódhi ‘car’, yeh/yoh ‘house’, dejúli ‘mosquito’, sas ‘bear’, łį ‘dog’, dëne ‘person, people’ Dënesųłıné nouns incompatible with numerals (Wilhelm 2008:48): lı́gofı́ ‘coffee’, jı́etué ‘wine’, ɂejëretth’úé ‘milk’, bëŕ ‘meat’, thay ‘sand’, dedhay ‘salt’, suga ‘sugar’, dzą ‘mud’, tłës ‘lard/grease/fuel’, yath ‘snow’, yú ‘cloth/clothing’, dą́ ‘hunger, starvation’, beł ‘sleep, sleepiness’

The mass nouns denote substances, liquids and some abstract concepts, while the count nouns denote discrete entities. I conclude that the semantic basis for the count/mass distinction in Dënesųłiné is atomicity. Some mass nouns can have an atomic interpretation in certain contexts. For example, the use of numerals with tthígha ‘(human) head hair’ is strange, unless a context is evoked in which individual hairs are salient. In a crime scene context where an investigator has found two hairs it would be acceptable to say náke thhígha ‘two (human) head hairs’. Another noun that behaves like this is jíe ‘berries’.



Mass and count nouns 

 535

23.3.2 Haida Haida, a language isolate spoken on Haida Gwaii and in nearby coastal areas, has no number inflection on nouns or in noun phrases and there is also no number agreement on verbs (Enrico 2003:23–24). Nonetheless, there is a clear count/mass distinction, as per the patterning of quantifiers and classifiers (Enrico 2005:xxxi). The quantifier 7ihlii ‘some of’ requires a count noun (Enrico 2005:xxxi). For exam7 ple: (11)      

Haida (Enrico 2003:739; emphasis mine) xa 7ihlii-gw@ gina aw’ll gud ’laa? dog some.of-Q thing sweet like ‘So some dogs like sweet things?’ (Skidegate dialect)

7ihlii belongs to a class of quantifiers applying only to nouns denoting pluralities of atoms, cf. xa ‘dog’ in (11). This class includes the Haida numerals (Enrico 2003:744), which in turn suggests that the numerals also require a (plural) atomic noun denotation, thus a count noun. The numerals may appear with or without classifiers, cf. (12a) vs. (12b): (12)    

   

Haida (Enrico 2003:753, emphasis mine) a. “haw.isan kun sdang xà.aw-7uhlaa-.ang qasa.a-ang,“ hin ’la   again whale two lie-in.morning.going.to-prs thus 3 saaw-aa-n. say-evid-pst ‘“Two whales are going to lie (there) again in the morning,” he said.’ (Masset dialect) b. “qiihlgaa q’ii-sdang-ga dii sdahla-ang,” hin-.uu. ’la 7aww   dish clf-two-PP I  need-prs thus-foc 3  mother saaw-aa-n. say-evid-pst ’ ‘“I need three [sic] bowls,” his mother said.’ (Masset dialect)

The numerals and the quantifier 7ihlii are evidence for a morphosyntactic count/mass distinction. The other main criterion for count nouns is having a “lexically specified” classifier (Enrico 2005:xxxi). In other words, if a noun has a classifier lexically associated with it, it is count. For example, the classifier for kaa.a ‘car’ must be hlra ‘two-dimensional extended object consisting of parallel one-dimensional rigid members lying’ (Enrico 2005:liii), and t’aw ‘object extended in one dimension, broadening out at one end, spatulate object’ is the classifier for spoons (Enrico 2005:191).

7 A period between two vowels indicates an empty consonant slot, and @ = a low vowel.

536 

 Andrea Wilhelm

In Wilhelm (2008) I argued that numeral classifiers do not create the atoms for counting count nouns, they simply make them explicit (see also Doetjes 1997). The optionality of the Haida classifiers supports this. I also argued that classifiers which specify a container or measure (e.  g., spoon, pound) create units for counting mass nouns. This seems to be the case in Haida as well, for example with t’aw ‘spatulate object’ and the noun súgaa ‘sugar’: (13)      

Haida (Enrico 2003:786) súgaa t’aw-sdang xaw-rii ’la 7isda-gan. sugar clf-two coffee-into 3 put-pst ‘He put two spoons of sugar in the coffee.’ (Masset dialect)

While mass nouns may appear with classifiers, they are not lexically associated with them (Enrico 2005:xxxi). Likely a mass noun like súgaa can be used with different classifiers in different contexts like counting bags, cups, or lumps of sugar. The same classifier may occur with count and mass nouns and thus classifiers do not morphosyntactically distinguish count/mass. They do have a different semantic function in each case.

23.4 Verbal systems for making the distinction Many North American Indigenous languages are head-marking (Nichols 1992). At the clausal level, this means that grammatical notions are marked on the verb (or auxiliary) rather than on the verb’s arguments. This is also true for the count/mass distinction; North American Indigenous languages utilize verbs for the distinction in ways unavailable to European languages.

23.4.1 Verbal number Verbal number refers to number as an inherent component of a verb’s meaning. For example, Koasati, a Muskogean language of Louisiana and Texas, has distinct verbs for the same situation, depending on whether one or several entities are involved: (14)            

Koasati (Kimball 1991:451, 454) a. pokkó-k cokkó:l   ball-sbj sit.sg   ‘There is a ball.’ b. pokkó-k cikkí:k   ball-sbj sit.pl   ‘There are some balls.’



Mass and count nouns 

 537

Verbal number is not number agreement. In (14), the noun for ‘ball’ is invariant, as Koasati does not have nominal number (Kimball 1991:403), and the verb has nothing to agree with. Verbal number is the key criterion for the count/mass distinction in Koasati. (Compatibility with numerals is not discussed.) Nouns “are divided into two categories, count and mass nouns, the former consisting of objects that can be individually numerated, the latter of objects occurring only as groups. The morphological effect of this categorization is that mass nouns require a plural verb, when one is available” (Kimball 1991:450): (15)            

Koasati (Kimball 1991:450, 455) a. kafí-k itta-cikkí:k   coffee-sbj loc-sit.pl   ‘There is a heap of coffee on the ground.’ b. a:nipó-k cikki:k   prepared_meat-sbj sit.pl   ‘There is a heap of meat.’

Koasati mass nouns include, for example, acitiká ‘rope, hatkasí ‘sheet’, holikfá ‘clothing’, honnó ‘skirt’, na:sincá:ka ‘paper’, baksá ‘string’, hapí ‘salt’, hiplí ‘snow’, ittohissi ‘leaves’, kopíhka ‘trash’, okłí ‘earth’, pahí ‘grass’, sancó ‘sand’, talcosí ‘pebbles’, tilkó ‘flour’ (p. 450). Some other languages with verbal number are Kumeyaay (Langdon 1970) and other Yuman languages, Shoshone (Dayley 1989), Haida (Enrico 2003), Dënesųłıné (Cook 2004:240–242) and other Dene/Athabaskan languages; however, the relevance for the count/mass distinction is generally not documented. For Dënesųłiné, see section 4.2.3.

23.4.2 Verbal classifiers and classificatory verbs 23.4.2.1 Koasati In addition to verbal number, Koasati has positional verbs which select objects of a certain shape, size, orientation, and so on. The ‘sit’ verbs in (14) and (15) above are examples; they indicate arrangement in a heap with mass nouns while with count nouns they reflect that the objects themselves are roundish (Kimball 1991:454). Interestingly, the singular ‘sit’ verb cokkó:lin can be used with nouns for cloth and paper items to indicate that the item is lying somewhere balled up. Mass nouns are not normally compatible with singular verbs, but it appears that cokkó:lin can evoke a context for a (singular) atomic denotation:8

8 Kimball glosses cokkó:l as ‘lie balled up’ here rather than ‘sit’, but it is the same verb as in (14) above.

538 

(16)      

 Andrea Wilhelm

Koasati (Kimball 1991:460) holikfá-k itta-cokkó:l clothing-sbj loc-lie_balled_up ‘The clothing lies balled up on the floor.’

23.4.2.2 Haida Besides accompanying numerals and certain quantifiers, the Haida classifiers may also occur as prefixes with some verbs. In this position, the classifiers help interpret the number-neutral noun as singular, plural, or mass. In (17a) the classifier dla specifies that xa.a refers to a single duck; in (17b) the classifier xa leads to an interpretation of plural ducks in a group. In (18), the mass noun sablii ‘bread’ is interpreted as ‘loaf (of bread)’ when the verb contains the classifier sk’a. (17)            

Haida (Enrico 2003:561) a. huu xa.a dla-gang-gang.   there duck clf-float-prs   ‘A duck is floating there.’ (Masset dialect) b. huu xa.a xa-gang-gang.   there duck clf-float-prs   ‘Several ducks are floating there in a single group.’ (Masset dialect)

(18)      

Haida (Enrico 2003:25, emphasis mine) sablii-gee dii-ga ’la sk’asdla-gan. bread-def me-to 3 hand-pst ‘He handed me the (long cylindrical loaf) of bread.’ (Masset dialect)

We could say that in (18) an atomic interpretation of sablii is achieved through the verbal classifier. Enrico emphasizes that the classifiers do not agree with some feature of the noun, but that they are “meaningful elements, and in many instances they provide fairly specialized information about the associated argument” (p. 25). 23.4.2.3 Dënesųłiné Like other Athabaskan/Dene languages, Dënesųłiné has a set of abstract verb roots which “classify” their absolutive argument according to shape, dimension, texture, and number (cf. Carter 1976, Cook 1986, 2004, Davidson et al. 1963, Rice 1998). Most handling verbs and many verbs of location or existence are formed from these roots. Nouns are conventionally associated with a particular root or roots, but other roots can be chosen for semantic or rhetorical effect. The classificatory verb roots broadly reflect the count/mass distinction. In (19) we see some of the roots occurring primarily with nouns denoting discrete objects (atomic



Mass and count nouns 

 539

denotations), either single (a–b) or plural objects (c). These nouns are also compatible with numerals. Some of the roots occurring primarily with nouns denoting unbounded substances (nonatomic denotations) are shown in (20). These nouns are incompatible with numerals and the ‘object’ classificatory roots, cf. (21). They may however occur with roots specifying an object in a certain container, (22).9 (19)    

Dënesųłiné a. tthe   stone/pipe

   

b.  

sas bear

   

c.  

tthe stone/pipe

(20)            

Dënesųłiné a. ?  yath yeghą nínįdzáy     snow 4obj.to adv.mom.3sbj.handle_granular_object.pfv     ‘s/he brought snow to him/her’ (no container specified) b.   dzą yegáh nínįtłe(gh)     mud 4obj.by adv.mom.3sbj.handle_mushy_matter.pfv     ‘s/he spattered/splattered mud near him/her, s/he plopped the mud near him/her’ (no container specified)

(21)                        

Dënesųłiné a. #  sǫlághe yath       five snow   b. #  yath seghą nínįɁą     snow 1sg.obj.to adv.mom.3SBJ.handle_single_round_object.pfv     (intended: ‘s/he brought me one snowflake/grain of snow’) c. #  yath nínila       snow adv.mom.1sg.sbj.handle_plural_objects.pfv     (intended: ‘I brought some snowflakes’) d. #  sǫlághe dzą       five mud   e. #  dzą yeghą nínįla     mud 4obj.to adv.mom.3sbj.handle_pural_objects.pfv

yeghánįɁą 4obj.to.mom.3sbj.handle_single_round_object.pfv ‘S/he gave him/her a pipe.’ seghánįłtį 1sg.obj.to.mom.3sbj.handle_single_animate_object.pfv ‘s/he gave me a bear’ yeghánįla 4obj.to.mom.3sbj.handle_plural_objects.pfv ‘s/he gave him/her several pipes’

9 For simplicity, the verbs are not broken down into morphemes, but the classificatory verb root is bolded. The example in (20a) is slightly questionable on pragmatic grounds—it’s unusual to transport snow without container. However, if such a situation is to be described, the root -dzáy is used.

540 

(22)            

 Andrea Wilhelm

Dënesųłiné a. yath níniłtą   snow adv.1sg.sbj.handle_object_in_closed_container.pfv   ‘I brought some snow in a container’ b. dzą yeghą nínįką   mud 4obj-to adv.mom.3sbj.handle_object_in_shallow_container.pfv   ‘s/he brought him/her some mud (in a small, shallow dish)’

With polysemous nouns, the classificatory verbs help determine interpretation. For example, naídí ‘medicine’ with the ‘shallow container’ verb root -ką is liquid medicine, with the ‘granular object’ root -dzáy is a powder or crystals, with the ‘single round object’ root -Ɂą is a single pill, and with the ‘plural objects’ root -la is several pills. All these occurred in the same sentence frame naídí tth’áy yé + classificatory verb of existence ‘there is medicine in the dish’ (tth’áy yé = ‘dish in’). As is by now familiar, mass nouns may receive an atomic interpretation in certain contexts. For example, while dedhay ‘salt’ is usually nonatomic (e.  g., cannot be counted with numerals, typically occurs with ‘closed container’ verbs), in the context of a ‘plural objects’ verb (and cows) it receives the atomic denotation ‘blocks of salt’. The unit (‘blocks’) is idiosyncratic to the noun. (23)        

Dënesųłiné Ɂejëre ghą dedhay nínįla cow to salt adv.mom.3sbj.handle_pural_objects.pfv ‘s/he brought blocks of salt to the cows’ [speaker comment: “automatically brings up image of blocks of salt”]

The classificatory verbs also reveal that Dënesųłiné has aggregate nouns, for example, layúé ‘tools’ (mentioned in section 1), núneshaze insert single end quotation mark ‘groceries’, ɂasí ɂahełdéłi ‘garbage’, beghąxólé ‘his/her clan’, bëŕ ‘meat’. These nouns are morphosyntactically mass by the numeral test, but they are typically used with ‘plural objects’ verbs rather than a mass or container verb, cf. (24a–b). A few of them are even used with a ‘single object’ classificatory verb, cf. (24c). (24)                  

Dënesųłiné a. belayúé setł’ághįla   3.tools 1sg.obj.pfv.3sbj.handle_plural_objects.pfv   ‘he handed me his tools’ b. bëŕ thela   meat ipfv.3sbj.plural_objects_exist   ‘there’s meat laying there (several pieces)’ c. bëŕ yeghą nínįɁą   meat 3obj.to adv.mom.3sbj.handle_single_round_object.pfv   ‘he brought her some meat (one piece only)’



Mass and count nouns 

 541

This recalls English aggregate nouns like clothing and furniture, which denote “a variety of objects united by their shared function” (Huddleston & Pullum 2002:336). While they are mass by the morphosyntactic criteria of English, on some semantic tests they act as if they have atomic denotations. For example, they do combine with count adjectives and the meaning of the adjective is applied to each member of the aggregate, not to the aggregate as a whole: large furniture, extra-small clothing. It appears that just like the count adjectives, Dënesųłiné classificatory verbs are sensitive to noun semantics in ways that numerals are not.10 Overall, verbal ways of making the count/mass distinction may be semantic— (in)compatible meanings, selectional restrictions—rather than morphosyntactic.

23.5 Visual methods of getting at noun meaning The previous sections have made it clear that countability manifests differently in different languages, depending on each language’s morphosyntactic and lexical resources. I have emphasized the importance of not only examining the distribution of nouns, but also of paying careful attention to the meanings of nouns as they are revealed in combination with grammatical elements or lexical items. In this section, I offer some additional, nonlinguistic tools for understanding noun meanings.

23.5.1 Objects and pictures Objects and pictures are excellent for understanding concrete nouns. Speakers can show what a noun means by manipulating or pointing to real-world referents in naturalistic environments. This could involve traditional activities and objects as well as nontraditional ones. For example, in a kitchen while preparing or eating food, one could talk about flour, salt, milk, water, butter, oil, herbs, various containers (spoons, cups, bowls, pots, jars, etc.), some of which may do double duty as measuring devices, utensils and cutlery, meats, fish, eggs, dough, vegetables and fruits, and so on. Time in the natural environment, perhaps on traditional land, can help understand nouns for plants and vegetation, animals and game, animal excrement, soil, rock and rocks, sand, mud, water 10 Some speculation: The aggregate nouns might not combine with numerals in Dënesųłiné because only like things can be counted, while the members of aggregates are heterogeneous. It is also possible that the classificatory verbs supply more semantic context than numerals and that this gives them access to the individual members. Rothstein (2017) would say that these nouns while having natural atoms are semantically nonatomic and that the classificatory verbs (just like count adjectives) are sensitive to natural, not semantic, atomicity. Barner & Snedeker (2005) would keep the semantics simple but allow a mismatch between semantics and morphosyntax, for example that aggregate nouns have atomic denotations but are blocked from count syntax.

542 

 Andrea Wilhelm

in various occurrences (streams, lakes, oceans, puddles, drops), land features such as woods, “bush”, bogs, beaches, types of precipitation and weather, and so on. Many activities can also be done in a classroom or office setting if props are brought in, or one can use pictures (photos or drawings) of objects. For example, Deal (2017) gave speakers actual clay to use when talking about clay and showed pictures for objects such as flour. Another possibility is to have speakers choose the “most appropriate” pictures (those which capture the meaning of a noun best), perhaps from an online image search, or have them draw the meanings of nouns. Drawing has the advantage of not being influenced by what the internet or supplied pictures have to offer. One could also use short videos. Where it does not occur spontaneously, one can throw in questions in which nouns are combined with numerals, quantifiers, certain verbs, pluralized, etc.—depending on what the resources of the language are. Speakers’ reactions will reveal what is acceptable, and discussion about meaning and appropriate use may ensue.

23.5.2 Experiments The count or mass character of nouns can also be examined with experiments. A wellknown method (Barner & Snedeker 2005) uses quantity judgments. Speakers are presented with pictures of the same kinds of item(s) arranged on two sides and told that the items on the one side belong to one person and those on the other side to another person. Some pictures will be of objects and some of substances. Crucially, there will be a higher number of the items on the one side of the picture and a higher combined volume of the items on the other side, as shown in Figure 1. Speakers are then asked “Who has more X?”, where X is the noun denoting the item(s). If speakers point to the side with more items, they have judged the quantity of the item based on numerosity, i.  e., number of individual items. If they point to the other side, they have judged the quantity based on overall volume. Barner & Snedeker (2005) found that in English, count nouns were consistently judged on numerosity, indicating that they have atomic denotations. Mass nouns referring to substances were consistently judged on volume, indicating nonatomic denotations. Aggregate nouns such as clothing, silverware were judged on numerosity even though they are morphosyntactically mass. This could indicate that aggregate nouns have atomic denotations (Barner & Snedeker’s conclusion) or that the quantity judgments are not based on atomic denotations but on something else. Finally, flexible nouns such as stone, rope are judged based on volume when they appear in mass syntax (Who has more rope?) and based on numerosity when they appear in count syntax (Who has more ropes?), showing that in these cases morphosyntax plays a disambiguating role. Deal (2017) conducted this experiment in Nez Perce using photos of items. The experiment confirmed the existence of nouns with atomic denotations in Nez Perce, and the fact that substance nouns have inherently nonatomic denotations but can be



Mass and count nouns 

(Who has more GROCERIES?)

(Who has more CIGARETTES?)

 543

(Who has more FABRIC?)

Fig. 1

mapped onto an atomic denotation via count syntax. Deal did not test aggregate nouns and we do not have information at this point whether they exist in Nez Perce.

23.6 Conclusion This chapter has attempted to show that the count or mass character of a noun may impact its linguistic behaviour and is worth paying attention to in language documentation. The following questions should be asked: a. Is a certain morphosyntactic distribution of nouns (e.  g., with numerals, number inflection, quantifiers, determiners, classifiers, certain verbs) best characterized in terms of count/mass? (Or rather in terms of some other notion, e.  g., animacy, gender, shape?) b. If yes, are there other morphosyntactic features which show the same (or similar) division of nouns into count and mass? c. In a given linguistic environment (e.  g., numerals, plural, count adjectives, quantifiers, classifiers, certain verbs), are there systematic differences in interpretation of nouns (e.  g., ‘object’ vs. ‘unit of a substance’) that could be characterized as atomic/ nonatomic? d. After checking count and mass environments, are there nouns which have only atomic interpretations, or only nonatomic interpretations (e.  g., cannot be mapped to the other type)? It will be important to look at a sizeable sample of nouns in order to make valid generalizations. The sample should include nouns referring to discrete objects (humans, animals, plants, inanimate objects), to substances, liquids and diffuse matter, and to aggregates. It should also include small objects which usually occur in a mass (e.  g., hair, lentils, sand). And it should include abstract nouns, which have been quite neglected in count/mass research. Another type of noun which is often neglected is those referring to entities consisting of two symmetrical parts, such as scissors, pants, nose/nostrils. For example, in English scissors is plural-only and marginal in count contexts (*a scissors,

544 

 Andrea Wilhelm

three scissors), thus mass, while in German we have the count noun eine Schere ‘one [pair of] scissors (sg)’ – drei Scheren ‘three [pairs of] scissors (pl)’. The Yup’ik equivalent nuussicuak ‘scissors’ (lit., ‘little pair of knives’) is inherently dual. No singular form exists; however the plural is used to refer to several pairs of scissors (Mithun 1999: 80). To understand the behaviour of a given noun, both rich contexts, as partly discussed in section 5, and out-of-the blue contexts will be instructive (see the ‘hair’ example in section 3.1). It will be helpful and may indeed be crucial to include negative evidence (e.  g., incompatibility with a numeral). Ideally one will combine naturalistic and elicited documentation and also pay attention to collocations, registers/genres, and social norms. Creating a corpus of a sizeable number of nouns with rich information on each, as outlined above, is in and of itself a very valuable piece of language documentation. It blends into lexicography work and could provide excellent material for a dictionary. It should be clear in light of our discussion that one-word translations of a list of words from a contact language into an Indigenous language (e.  g., arrow: k’á, arthritis: tth’ɛn ʔɛlghą́ nat’a eyahi, ashes: kɛsłezɛ́, Elford & Elford 1998:2) does not make for adequate documentation, language teaching, or an adequate dictionary, as it provides no information on the (naturalistic) uses of a noun. A particular danger is that language learners will use a noun based on how they would use its contact language translation. If (a), or (a) and (b), are answered in the positive, the language has a morphosyntactic count/mass distinction. In this case, relevant information should be included in dictionaries. For example, Rhodes (1985) specifies the plural form of nouns which have one (the citation form is singular) and also specifies which nouns are plural-only. If (a) and (b) are answered in the negative but (c) and (d) in the positive, the language has a semantic count/mass distinction. Particularly interesting for linguistic theory will be languages without any count/ mass distinction (if they exist). This would be languages in which all nouns are polysemous (flexible), or languages in which all nouns are mass (nonatomic denotations) or all nouns are count (atomic denotations). In closing, two things should be borne in mind. First, the count/mass phenomena discussed in this chapter are not intended to be exhaustive. It will be important not to limit oneself to the notions and tools presented here, but to find the resources for understanding countability specific to each language. Second, while I believe that all languages can refer to substances and discrete objects and that likely all languages semantically distinguish count/atomic and mass/nonatomic nouns, count/mass is not going to be an important organizing feature in every language. Languages may well organize their grammar and lexicon predominantly along other notions. We saw an example of this with the Dënesųłiné classificatory verbs, which divide nouns into about ten categories, not only mass and count. Investigation of the count/mass distinction should not come at the expense of documenting a language’s inherent categorizations. ??



Mass and count nouns 

 545

Acknowledgements: I want to thank the editors of this volume for inviting me to contribute this chapter, and the anonymous reviewers for their comments. My thinking on the count/mass distinction has been shaped deeply by what I have learned about the Dënesųłiné language, and I want to thank my teachers, Shirley Cardinal, John Janvier, Lynda Minoose, as well as Agnes Gendron and the late Ernest Ennow, Nora Matchatis, and Marlene Piché, all of Cold Lake First Nations, Canada. For the examples from other languages, I used the libraries of the University of Alberta and the University of Victoria extensively. I want to acknowledge this service provided by our public institutions of higher education. I also want to acknowledge the support of my linguistics colleagues, too many to name, but in particular Hotze Rullmann, with whom I have talked about semantics, including the count/mass distinction, the most. However, I alone am responsible for the content of this chapter. Finally, this chapter contains some of my own original research, for which I gratefully acknowledge funding from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (SSHRC grants 756-2005-0324 and 410-2009-1101).

References Barner, David & Jesse Snedeker. 2005. Quantity judgments and individuation: evidence that mass nouns count. Cognition 97. 41–66. Bunt, Harry C. 1985. Mass terms and model-theoretic semantics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Carter, R.M. 1976. Chipewyan classificatory verbs. International Journal of American Linguistics 42. 24–30. Chierchia, Gennaro. 2010. Mass nouns, vagueness and semantic variation. Synthese 2010. 99–149. Cook, Eung-Do. 1986. Athapaskan classificatory verbs. Amerindia 11. 11–23. Cook, Eung-Do. 2004. A grammar of Dëne Sųłiné (Chipewyan). (Memoir 17: Special Athabaskan Number). Winnipeg: Algonquian and Iroquoian Linguistics. Dayley, Jon P. 1989. Tümpisa (Panamint) Shoshone grammar. Berkeley: University of California Press. Davidson, W., Leon M. Elford & Harry Hoijer. 1963. Athapaskan classificatory verbs. In Harry Hoijer et al. (eds.), Studies in the Athapaskan languages, 30–41. Berkeley: University of California Press. Davis, Henry. 2014. Count and mass in St’át’imcets. Paper presented at SULA (Semantics of Under-Represented Languages in the Americas) 8, University of British Columbia, 16–18 May. Deal, Amy Rose. 2017. Countability distinctions and semantic variation. Natural Language Semantics 25. 125–171. Doetjes, Jenny. 2013. Count/mass distinctions across languages. In Claudia Maienborn, Klaus von Heusinger & Paul Portner (eds.), Semantics: An international handbook of natural language meaning. Vol. 3, 2559–2580. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. Doetjes, Jenny. 1997. Quantifiers and Selection. On the Distribution of Quantifying Expressions in French, Dutch and English. The Hague: Holland Academic Graphics dissertation. Elford, Leon M. & Elford, Marjorie. 1998. Dene (Chipewyan) dictionary. Prince Albert, SK: Northern Canada Mission Distributors. Enrico, John. 2003. Haida syntax (2 volumes). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Enrico, John. 2005. Haida dictionary: Skidegate, Masset, and Alaskan dialects (2 volumes). Fairbanks, AK: Alaska Native Language Center. Frantz, Donald. 1991. Blackfoot grammar. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.

546 

 Andrea Wilhelm

Gillon, Carrie & Nicole Rosen. 2016. Critical mass in Michif. Journal of Pidgin and Creole Languages 31. 113–140. Huddleston, Rodney & Geoffrey K. Pullum. 2002. The Cambridge grammar of the English language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Jespersen, Otto. 1924. The philosophy of grammar. London: Allen & Unwin. Kimball, Geoffrey D. 1991. Koasati grammar. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Landman, Fred. 1989. Groups (I–II). Linguistics and Philosophy 12. 559–605, 723–744. Langdon, Margaret. 1970. A grammar of Diegueño: the Mesa Grande dialect. Berkeley: University of California Press. Mathieu, Eric. 2012. On the count/mass distinction in Ojibwe. In Diane Massam (ed.), Count and mass across languages, 172–198. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Mithun, Marianne. 1999. The languages of Native North America. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Newman, Stanley. 1965. Zuni grammar. Albuquerque, NM: University of New Mexico Press. Nichols, Johanna. 1992. Linguistic diversity in space and time. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Quine, W.V.O. 1960. Word and object. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Rhodes, Richard. 1985. Eastern Ojibwa-Chippewa-Ottawa dictionary. Berlin: Mouton. Rhodes, Richard A. 1990. Lexical hierarchies and Ojibwa noun derivation. In Savas L. Tsohatzidis (ed.), Meanings and prototypes: Studies in linguistic categorization, 151–158. London: Routledge. Rice, Sally. 1998. Giving and taking in Chipewyan: The semantics of THING-marking classificatory verbs. In John Newman (ed.), The linguistics of giving, 97–134. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company. Rothstein, Susan. 2010. Counting and the count/mass distinction. Journal of Semantics 27. 343–397. Rothstein, Susan. 2017. Semantics for counting and measuring. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Valentine, Randy. 2001. Nishnaabemwin reference grammar. Toronto, ON: University of Toronto Press. Wierzbicka, Anna. 1988. Oats and wheat: mass nouns, iconicity, and human categorization. In Anna Wierzbicka (ed.), The Semantics of grammar, 499–560. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Wilhelm, Andrea. 2008. Bare nouns and number in Dëne Sųłiné. Natural Language Semantics 16. 39–68. Wiltschko, Martina. 2012. Decomposing the count/mass distinction: Evidence from languages that lack it. In Diane Massam (ed.), Count and mass across languages, 146–171. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Gary Holton and Andrea L. Berez-Kroeker

24 Sense of place: Space, landscape, and orientation Abstract: The languages of Native North America reflect deep connections to place, as evidenced in systems of orientation, landscape classification, and place naming strategies. Along with language, land is a defining feature of Native North American identity. To be Diné or Inupiaq or Kanien’kehá:ka is to identify with Diné or Inupiaq or Kanien’kehá:ka country. Language gives shape to the land and reveals a unique and storied landscape. Rather than relying on cardinal directions, many Native North American languages make use of systems of orientation which are anchored in the landscape. Landscape terminologies recognize culturally-significant distinctions, such as the Siberian Yupik distinction between sea ice which is safe to walk on and sea ice which is dangerous. Place-naming strategies also differ markedly from those found in colonizing languages. Many Native North American languages reject commemorative names in favor of ecological or topographic names. In this chapter we provide examples of some of the typical spatial features of North American languages, while also highlighting features which are unique and typologically unusual as compared to non-North American languages. We conclude with a discussion of the role of sense of place in language maintenance and reclamation efforts, including community-based mapping initiatives and participatory geographic information systems.

24.1 Introduction Space and place are fundamental to the human experience. Our conceptualization of place—our “creation” of place—is mediated through language and reflected in several areas of grammar: in the structure of spatial orientation systems; in the delineation of landscape categories; and in the organization of place names and place-naming strategies. While spatial language is found in languages across the world, sense of place is particularly important in North America: “In a number of [Native North American] languages, particularly in the Northwest and the North, spatial distinctions are elaborately developed. In some they clearly reflect the topographic contexts in which the languages are spoken.” (Mithun 1999: 132)

Moreover, the study of space has deep roots in the history of North American linguistics (cf. Sapir 1912), with the result that sense of place has arguably featured more prominently in research on North American languages than in perhaps any other linguistic region outside Austronesia. Hence, this short chapter can only hope to scratch the surface of the large amount of research on spatial language North America. Accordingly, https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-024

548 

 Gary Holton and Andrea L. Berez-Kroeker

in this chapter we provide a broad overview of the various factors underpinning sense of place in the Native languages of North America. Like the landscapes in which the languages are spoken, there is an enormous diversity of spatial language in North America. We focus here on five domains of grammar, each discussed in turn: spatial orientation (§ 2); demonstratives (§ 3); topological relations (§ 4); landscape classification (§ 5); and place naming (§ 6). Each of these domains has received extensive attention in the typological literature; however, the coverage for North America is uneven, and our examples reflect this. The goal in a short chapter like this is not to be comprehensive but rather to give some flavor of the great variety which exists. Perhaps because of its close tangible connection to the land, the study of place, landscape and spatial orientation in North America has always had a strong applied component. While the importance of community-based research is increasingly recognized within linguistics more broadly (cf. Bischoff & Jany 2018), there is an especially long history of community-based research on place. This is particularly true in the domain of place names documentation, where most research tends to be bottom-up, driven by community needs and undertaken within and often by the community (cf. Holton 2015). The scope of this work is vast and rapidly growing; we limit our discussion in § 7 to community-based mapping initiatives. We conclude in § 8 with a discussion of efforts toward decolonizing place in North America, focusing in particular on official place naming efforts.

24.2 Spatial orientation systems North American languages often rely heavily on what Levinson (2003) has called absolute frames of reference to express spatial orientation. Such systems specify the location of a referent independent of the orientation of either the observer or the reference object (ground). The most familiar of such systems are the cardinal directions translated as ‘north’, ‘south’, ‘east’, ‘west’ determined by the transit of the sun; but many North American languages make use of other reference points for absolute orientation. Riverine and coastal systems of orientation are especially prominent. Crucially, while such systems may be anchored in environmental features, their usage extends beyond those features. Thus, the “upstream” and “upcoast” axes differ from the equivalent lexical items in English in that they define a spatial coordinate system rather than referring strictly to local features of the river or coast. Lushootseed (Coast Salish) contrasts an upstream-downstream axis with an orthogonal toward vs. away from water axis. For each of the resulting four directions there is both a locative and a dynamic (translocative) root, as in Table 1. The locative forms can be further derived with prefixes dxw- ‘toward’, tul- ‘from’, and lił- ‘by what route’. Even more elaborated riverine systems can be found in the Dene (Athabaskan) languages. Dene directionals consist of a root indicating direction upriver, downriver,



Sense of place: Space, landscape, and orientation 

 549

Tab. 1: Lushootseed directional terms (Hess 1979: 371)  

locative

dynamic

‘upstream’ ‘downstream’ ‘away from water’ ‘toward water’

q’ixw ʔáłx̌ad t’áq’t čáʔkw

təyil qwic čúbə k’wíł

toward water and away from water, as in Coast Salish. To this root is added a suffix indicating specific location, general area, movement toward, and movement away; and a prefix indicating distance from the deictic center. The full Dene directional system consists of nine directional roots, as exemplified by Ahtna: nae’ ‘upriver, behind’, daa’ ‘downriver’, ngge’ ‘from water, upland’, tsen ‘toward water, lowland’, naan ‘across’, tgge’ ‘up vertically’, igge’ ‘down vertically’, ’an ‘away, off’, and nse’ ‘ahead’ (see Figure 1).1 These terms take their meaning with regard to the flow of a local river of some cognitive and cultural importance (Berez 2011: 33).

Fig. 1: Schematic representation of Ahtna riverine directional stems (Berez 2011: 34).

1 Eight of these directional stems can be reconstructed to Proto-Dene; reconstruction of the ‘up vertically’ term is problematic (Leer 1989).

550 

 Gary Holton and Andrea L. Berez-Kroeker

In contrast to Dene, in Karuk (classified by some as Hokan) the riverine directionals do not form a distinct grammatical class but are contained within a larger set of derivational suffixes with abstract spatial meaning. Many of these spatial suffixes, including the riverine directionals, occur in pairs denoting motion toward and away from the deictic center. Thus, the stem -rupu express motion toward a point downriver, while the stem -raː expresses motion from a point downriver, and hence in the opposite, upriver direction (see Table 2). The downriver-upriver axis is distinct from downhill-uphill axis, though the contrast is neutralized for the down direction in the terms expressing motion toward the decitic center (see also Garrett et al, this volume). Tab. 2: Karuk riverine directionals (Macaulay 2004: 86) from deictic center

toward deictic center



-rupu -unih -uraː -rôvu -kaθ -kara

-raː -raː -faku -várak -rina -ríPaː

‘downriver’ ‘downhill’ ‘uphill’ ‘upriver’ ‘across body of water’ ‘horizontally away from center of a body of water’

In addition to these riverine terms Karuk has numerous more abstract terms referring to direction, e.  g., -sip ‘up to the height of a person or less’, -iš ‘down from the height of a person or less’, -ura: ‘up to a considerable height’, -unih ‘down from a considerable height’, -ku ‘onto a vertical surface’, -taku ‘onto a horizontal surface’, -fúruk ‘into an enclosed space’, -rúpuk ‘out of an enclosed space’ (Bright 1957, cited in Mithun 1999). In riverine directional systems, orientation depends crucially on the speaker’s chosen reference stream. In Ahtna speakers ascribe to minor streams and tributaries the spatial grid of the major river. In practical terms, this means speakers will shift their mental coordinate system when crossing from one drainage to another to match the flow of the major river in the new drainage (Berez 2011). As with other types of spatial orientation, riverine usage may be conventionalized in certain contexts. A particularly common conventialization occurs with the usage of riverine directionals inside a house. Across many North American cultures, houses are traditionally oriented with the entrance facing the river, so that the direction toward the back of the house, away from the entrance is conventionalized as ‘away from the river’. This conventionalization may hold even when the house does not in fact face the river (cf. Holton 2022 on Tanacross). Wakashan languages express elaborate spatial distinctions via lexical suffixes. Of the roughly 400 lexical suffixes in Nuuchahnulth, approximately 150 are used to indicate topological or spatial relations which situate referents with respect to body parts, man-made objects and geographic features. Nuuchahnulth is spoken along the west coast of Vancouver Island, along steep wooded fjords, and many of these lexical suffixes specify coastal orientation which references this unique landscape, for example, distin-



Sense of place: Space, landscape, and orientation 

 551

guishing the direction up the sound (i.  e., toward land) from the direction out to sea, and the direction upcoast from downcoast. (1)                

Selected Nuuchahnulth “site” suffixes (Davidson, Matthew 2002: 384) -(w)ač ‘at the margin along the water’ -ačišt ‘on the sea’ -’aˑqƛ’iːɬ ‘upsound, to the east’ -asuˑ ‘under, in liquid (esp. water)’ -c’aˑtu ‘on the water, out to sea’ -(c)smu(ɬ) ‘along the bank’ -(c)csuˑʔis ‘far out at sea; at a distant place out over the sea; on the horizon’ -(c)swaqƛi ‘downstream’

(2)                    

Selected Nuuchahnulth “path” suffixes (Davidson, Matthew 2002: 368) -aˑ ‘go out to sea’ -aˑqtuˑ ‘move across; extending across’ -aˑt ‘move downstream, out of the woods; extending downward’ -ciːq ‘move along the shore -inʡatu ‘go up the coast’ -n’iˑq ‘down a slope’ -(c)saˑa ‘come to land’ -(c)stiˑs ‘move into the interior’ (perf.) -(c)suḥta ‘come out into the open’ -wiːʔis ‘go down to the coast’

Many North American languages employ cardinal terms for spatial orientation at larger geographic scales, with one axis determined by the path of the sun and an orthogonal axis contrasting cold and warm weather. In Seneca (Iroquoian), these cardinal terms are transparently analyzable: (3)        

Seneca cardinal directions (Chafe 2014: 97) tgäː́ hgwitgë’s ‘east (literally,‘where the sun rises’) ̀ hegäːhgwë’s ‘west’, (literally, ‘‘where the sun sets’) joneːnö’ ‘south’ (literally, ‘where it is warm’) otówe’geh ‘north’ (literally, ‘‘where it is cold’)

Seneca cardinal direction terms may occur with the directional clitic =gwaːh ‘toward’ to indicate motion in one of the cardinal directions, as in (4). (4)          

Seneca cardinal direction with directional enclitic (Chafe 2014: 247) nä:h da’áöh neh, emph impossible namely t-gäː́ hgw-itgë’-s=gwa:h h-aa-hën-en-’ cis-nsg.agt-sun-emerge-hab=toward trans-hyp-m.pl.agt-go.somewhere-pct ‘It was impossible for them to go toward the east.’

552 

 Gary Holton and Andrea L. Berez-Kroeker

In Hopi the cardinal points are determined not by the path of the sun but rather the location of the sunrise and sunset at the winter and summer solstices. At the latitude where Hopi is spoken (approximately 35 degrees north), these directions correspond roughly to the English intercardinals. These four cardinal directions are in turn embedded within a system which includes the vertical directions as well. (5)            

Hopi direction terms (McCluskey 1977: 178) kwini’wi ‘summer sunset’ (southwest) te’vyûña ‘winter sunset’ (southwest) ta’tyûka ‘winter sunrise’ (northeast) ho’poko ‘summer sunrise’ (southeast) o’mi ‘above’ at’kyami ‘below’

Across languages of the Plains and Southwest the cardinal direction terms may exhibit symbolic and spiritual associations with colors, animals, plants and landscape, though the particular associations vary across the languages (Harrington 1916). Different systems of spatial orientation may be used at different geographic scales. In Inuit-Yupik languages a coastal system is used at larger scales, beyond the local village. Direction up and down the coast is contrasted with an orthogonal axis toward vs away from the coast (cf. Fortescue 2011). Thus, Yup’ik (Inuit-Yupik) contrasts uavet ‘rightward along the coast facing the water’, kiavet ‘leftward along the coast facing the water’, piavet ‘away from the coast’, and kanavet ‘toward the coast’. However, at local and intermediate scales a much more elaborated demonstrative system is employed to indicate spatial orientation. In this system the relevant anchor in the landscape is not the coast but rather geophysical elevation (cf. Burenhult 2008). In addition to elevation, the directional roots in Yup’ik encode two additional dimensions which Jacobson (1984) calls “indicatibility” and “accessibility” (see Table 3). Indicatibility indicates the extent (roughly, the width) of a referent. The “restricted” form of the directional is used if you can point with a static finger; the “extended” form is used if you have to move your finger horizontally to indicate extent—this includes objects in motion. The “obscured” form indicates locations that cannot be pointed to at all, for example, locations which are hidden behind a landscape feature. The interpretation of the accessibility dimension varies somewhat according to direction. For the upward direction, the more accessible form ping- indicates geophysical elevation, as in ‘up a slope’, while the less accessible form pik- indicates non-geophysical, that is, ‘vertically up’. But the down direction does not behave in an analogous way. The more accessible form kat- is used for both geophysical and vertical, while the less accessible form has been specialized to mean ‘downriver’ and ‘toward an exit’. For the terms meaning ‘level’, the less accessible form indicates a location beyond a barrier which could be referred to with an extended demonstrative, such as a river or a fence. This leads to useful distinctions, such as ‘across a river but visible’ (restricted, less accessible) vs. ‘on this side of river but not visible’ (obscured, accessible). For the



Sense of place: Space, landscape, and orientation 

 553

‘in/out’ and ‘here/there’ directions, the accessibility dimension distinguishes between the poles. Tab. 3: Yup’ik demonstrative roots, showing more accessible forms (left member of each pair) and less accessible forms (Jacobson 1984: 187).2    

   

indicatibility

elevation

down level up in/out here/there

extended

restricted

obscured

un-/ungaug-/agpaug-/pagqaug-/qagma-/tama-

kat-/uging-/ikping-/pikkiug-/keggu-/tau-

cam-/cakmang-/akmpam-/pakmqam-/qakmim-/taim-

Languages like Lushootseed, Ahtna, and Karuk are renowned not only for their inventory of riverine directionals but also for the frequent usage of these directionals. In particular, directionals are not merely used to reference location but tend to be pervasive within spontaneous speech and may serve to structure the discourse. Directionals may be obligatory in many contexts in which they would not be required in other languages. In the following passage from a Karuk Coyote story, directional terms occur affixed to each successive verb. (6)      

Karuk directionals (bolded) in use (Storyteller: Nettie Reuben; Bright 1957: 164) Viri vaˑ vúra ‘upi-kyíviv-raˑ thus so emph he.fell-from.downriver.to.here ‘So he fell over.’

   

Vássihkam backwards



‘He fell backwards into the river.’

     

Kári xás yúruk then and far.downriver ‘And he floated downriver.’

     

čavúra tá yíˑv tóˑpθívruˑh-varak finally punctual far floated.back-from.downriver.to.here ‘Finally he floated, a long ways downriver back to here.’

’úˑθ toward.center.of.water

’úkyiˑm-kar he.fell-horizontally.away.from.center. of.water

‘uθívruˑh-rup he.floated-hence.downward  

2 While the Yup’ik demonstratives indicate location, they exist alongside a semantically parallel system of motion verbs.

554 

 Gary Holton and Andrea L. Berez-Kroeker

In the Upper Tanana travel narrative example in (7), directionals serve to ground the narrative in the real places that are being described. (7)      

Upper Tanana (Dene) directionals (bolded) usage by Mrs. Sherry Barnes (Brucks 2015: 58) Nts’ą̈’ ahnegn’ chih Naambia Niign ahnegn’ and upland.all also Northway upland ‘And upland to Naambia Niign, upland there [they went] too.’

     

Hatthän’ ’eeł iidlaak naxach’ihdelshyeek. downland.all trap he.set they.went.hunting ‘They would go hunting with traps down there.’

         

Hahda’ Beaver Lake uda’ du’ shta’ iin downriver.all   downriver.all thus father pl nohts’ay mänh deedlay other.side lake plural.sit ‘Downstream, downstream to that Beaver Lake my father and the others [went] to the other side of the lakes down there.’

         

Ay tl’aan chih Herman Kessler moosi’ chih hanaat and then also   he.is.called also across.punct ahne’ nee-shyah ts’änh ahne’   upriver.all our-house from upriver.all   ‘And then Herman Kessler also [moved] across upriver there, upriver from our [old] house.’

The repeated use of spatial orientation terms in discourse does more than merely situate referents in space but rather serves to create a sense of place. Speakers of Karuk and Upper Tanana and other North American languages with rich directional systems are able to draw on extensive paradigms of directional forms in order to infuse a narrative with orientation, effectively embedding the narrative onto the landscape. In other words, spatial orientation systems provide a key tool with which speakers of Native North American languages “constitute their landscapes and take themselves to be connected to them” (Basso 1996: 54).

24.3 Demonstrative systems Demonstratives indicate relative distance from a reference point, typically taken to be the location of the speaker. In contrast to spatial orientation systems described in the preceding section, demonstratives systems do not necessarily locate referents along an angular direction. That is, demonstratives need not incorporate spatial coordinate systems. However, demonstrative systems in Native North American languages often



 555

Sense of place: Space, landscape, and orientation 

indicate additional non-spatial dimensions beyond the proximity of the referent. In addition to distinguishing at least three distances (i.  e., close to the speaker, away from the speaker, and even farther from the speaker), Ojibwe (Algonkian) demonstratives also indicate number and animacy (i.  e., obviation), though not all distinctions are maintained by all speakers (see Table 4). Tab. 4: Ojibwe demonstratives (Valentine 2001)  

animate

  proximal (‘this/these’) distal (‘that/those’) remote (‘that/those over there’)

singular maaba wa widi

plural gonda giwi giwi

inanimate obviative nanda niwi niwi

singular maanda wi widi

plural nanda niwi niwi

Demonstrative systems often encode additional semantic distinctions which cut across grammatical categories. This is the case with Ojibwe animacy distinctions Table 4 above. Further examples can be found among positional verbs, a pervasive feature of most Siouan and many other Plains languages. The verbs, which minimally distinguish sitting, lying and standing posture, may be used as auxiliaries to classify events/states; but they may also combine to form complex demonstratives which classify the nominal referent. The basic Hidatsa (Siouan) demonstratives make a three-way distance distinction, as shown in Table 5. Tab. 5: Hidatsa demonstratives (Park 2012: 383)  

pronominal

determiner

‘this’ ‘that’ ‘that over there’

hiri gúá haríà

-hee éèhgua íàhgua

An additional eight middle distance (“mesiodistal”) positional demonstratives are derived (at least historically) from positional verbs ‘sit’, ‘stand’, ‘lie’, ‘move around’. (8)              

Hidatsa positional demonstratives (Park 2012: 399) éèraha ‘that one (standing over there)’ éèrahaa ‘that one (who just left / is going)’ éèwagi ‘that one (lying over there)’ éèraga ‘that one (sitting over there)’ éèrahgu ‘that one over there’ éèruwa ‘that one (walking around over there)’ íàrahaa ‘that one (who just left / is going)’

556 

 Gary Holton and Andrea L. Berez-Kroeker

In Blackfoot (Algic), in addition to the animacy and number distinctions found in Algonkian demonstratives may encode movement or visibility of the referent with respect to the speaker. Tab. 6: Partial Blackfoot demonstrative template (after Schupbach 2013: 42) Stem

Animacy

Movement/Visibility

am ‘proximal’ ann ‘medial’ om ‘distal’

-wa ‘proximate singular’ -yi ‘obviative singular’ -yi ‘inanimate singular’ -iksi ‘animate plural’ -istsi ‘inanimate plural’

-ma ‘stationary’ -ya ‘moving away from speaker’ -ka ‘moving toward speaker’ -hka ‘invisible, indiscernible’

Example (9) makes use of two complex demonstratives to reference the place ‘where the bear was killed’. The distal demonstrative om-yi-wa is marked as inanimate and stationary, referring to the location. The medial demonstrative ann-wa-hka is marked as proximate animate and invisible. In this case the referent (the dead bear) is known to both the speaker and hearer; the use of the invisible demonstrative form to reference the bear emphasizes its lifelessness. (9)          

Blackfoot demonstratives in use (Uhlenbeck 1912, quoted in Schupbach 2013: 91) itap-oo-t om-yi-ma it-i’nit-hp-yi toward-go.anim.intr-ipv dist-inam.sg-stat deictic-kill-cnj-inam.sg ann-wa-hka kiááyo-wa   med-3.sg-invs bear-3.sg   ‘Go over there where that bear was killed.’

As noted in the preceding section, Yup’ik demonstratives obligatorily encode a distinction which is similar to the Blackfoot movement/visibility parameter, categorizing referents as extended (moving, long, or of large extent); restricted (stationary, localized, visible); or obscured (stationary, indistinct or invisible). Three distances are distinguished: near speaker, near hearer, and away from both speaker and hearer. The latter category is further specified according to absolute spatial orientation. These forms may be further inflected for number (singular, dual, plural) and case (absolutive, relative, localis, terminalis, ablative, vialis, aequalis, vocative).3 Table 7 shows only the singular forms for the absolutive case.

3 See Jacobson (1995) for a description of Yup’ik cases and their uses.



Sense of place: Space, landscape, and orientation 

 557

Tab. 7: Yup’ik demonstratives (absolutive singular) (Jacobson 1984: 658–60) extended

restricted

obscured



man’a tamana augna agna qaugna qagna un’a unegna paugna pagna

una tauna ingna ikna kiugna keggna kan’a ugna pingna pikna

    amna akemna qamna qakemna camna cakemna pamna pakemna

‘this, near speaker’ ‘that, near hearer’ ‘that over there’ ‘that across a significant feature of topography’ ‘inland/upriver’ ‘that outside’ ‘that toward river, down below’ ‘that downriver, toward sea, toward exit’ ‘that up, away from river, behind’ ‘that above’

The unspecified distal demonstrative ingna in (10a) makes no reference to direction, whereas the distal demonstrative pakemna in (10b) indicates that the referent (‘dog’) lies in the upward direction. (10)            

Yup’ik demonstrative distinctions (Jacobson 1984: 658–60) a. ingna qimugta naca-mnek keggmia-ngqer-tuq   that.over.there dog hat-1sg/3sg thing.held.in.teeth-have-3sg.indic   ‘That dog over there has my hat in its teeth.’ b. pakemna tutma-lria cakneq avirlur-tuq   that.above.abs walk.on-nmlz very make.noise-3sg.indic   ‘The one walking around up there is making a lot of clattering noise.’

While most demonstrative systems make a simple two- or three-way distance contrast (Mithun 1999: 132), more elaborate contrasts are found as well, referencing distance from the hearer as well as the speaker. The Maricopa (Yuman) system shows a five-way distinction, including two proximal distances. (11)          

Maricopa (Yuman) demonstrative distinctions (Gordon 1986: 55) vda ‘near, in hand’ da ‘near’ va ‘medial’ sva ‘distant’ aas ‘distant, out of sight’

The Koasati (Muskogean) system reflects a six-way contrast based on both distance from the speaker and distance from the hearer. (12)      

Koasati demonstrative distinctions (Kimball 1991: 486) yólli/yín/yá ‘proximal’ mǎːfa ‘near speaker’ yaʔá ‘near hearer’

558 

     

 Gary Holton and Andrea L. Berez-Kroeker

má ‘distal’ yáːfa ‘away from both speaker and hearer’ akkó ‘far away from both speaker and hearer’

As noted at the beginning of this section, in contrast to spatial orientation systems, demonstrative systems to not necessarily specify an angular search domain. However, demonstrative systems may express multiple distance distinctions, as in Maricopa and Koasati, and they may also interact with other dimensions of grammar, as in Blackfoot and Yup’ik. Together, these aspects of demonstrative systems in North American languages allow very precise location of referents with respect to the deictic center.

24.4 Topological relations The expression of topological relations differs from both spatial orientation systems (§ 2) and demonstrative systems (§ 3) in that they need not encode angular direction nor distance. Many Native North American languages express topological relations using adpositions, such as in Ahtna (Dene) ka ‘in search of’, Lakhota (Siouan) ohomni ‘around’, and Innu-aimun (Algonquian) tâkut ‘on top’ in (13). (13)                        

Topological relations expressed by adpositions a. Ahtna (Kari 1990: 234)   deniigi ka sta-ni-yaa   moose in.search.of out-pfv-walk   ‘He went out to get a moose.’ b. Lakhota (Sikos 2010)   čʰũkaške ti ohomni he   fence house around exists   ‘There is a fence around the house.’ c. Innu-aimun (Oxford 2007: 134)   Anita tâkut tetapuâkanit nîmuat   the.loc on.top couch.loc dance.3P   ‘The kids are dancing on the couch.’

auâssat child.3P

However, the syntactic status of these forms is not always clear. Apparent adpositions expressing topological relations in Choctaw (Muskogean) behave like nouns. For example, in (14) the nominal case marker attaches to the postposition paknaka ‘top’, not to the noun aaka’ ‘table’. Similarly, in (14b) the quantifier -ka ‘all’ follows the postposition but is interpreted as quantifying the object of the postposition. (14)      

Choctaw postpositions (Broadwell 2006: 254, 6) a. aaka’ paknaka-yat homma-h   table top-nom red-tns   ‘The top of the table is red.’



     

Sense of place: Space, landscape, and orientation 

b.    

aaí̠ pa’ nóta’ mó̠ma-ka̲ table under all-comp:ds ‘I looked under all the tables.’

 559

pisa-li-h see-1sg-tns

Similarly, some Innu-aimun (Algonkian) prepositions are noun-like in that they sometimes take demonstratives. In (15a-b) the prepositional phrases do not contain demonstratives, but the prepositional phrases in (15c-d) do. (15)                        

Innu-aimon prepositions (Oxford 2007: 154) a. Âkû mishtikut kâshû   behind tree.loc hide.3sg   ‘S/he is hiding behind a tree.’ b. Ûshte Nûsh uîtshuât nipimûtenân   beyond Rose 3.house.3pl.loc 1.walk.1pl   ‘We walked past Rose’s house.’ c. Anite shek u mishtikut tâuat   the.loc under tree.loc be.3pl   ‘They’re under the trees.’ d. Nimessipimîkâshinân nete tetâut   1.run.out.of.gas.1pl that.loc halfway.along   ‘We ran out of gas halfway along the road.’

meshkanât road.loc  

Furthermore, Innu-aimun has classificatory prepositions, in which a prepositional morpheme attaches to a nominal classificatory base that “expresses properties of the preposition’s reference point, or ground, the semantic role normally played by a preposition’s independent NP complement” (Oxford 2007: 169), as in (16). (16)    

Innu-aimun (Oxford 2007: 169) âpitûâshku ‘midway along a stick-like object’ (âpitû- ‘midway along’ + -âshku ‘sticklike object’)

Topological relations may also be expressed via affixes attached to nominal or verbal roots. Nisga’a (Tsimshianic) contains an exceedingly large inventory of location and motion enclitics which facilitate a very precise characterization of the semantics of topological relationships. (17)      

Nisga’a location proclitic (Tarpent 1987: 517) luː=sy̓èːn-[t]-ɬ c̓im-t̓áx̣ in=bottom-[3]-nc in-lake ‘the bottom of the lake’

The range of meanings encoded by Nisga’a location and motion proclitics can be seen in Table 8 below.

560 

 Gary Holton and Andrea L. Berez-Kroeker

Tab. 8: List of Nisga’a location and motion proclitics (Tarpent 1987: 518–44) luː ‘in, inside’ ʔakʷiː ‘outside, at the side’ ksə ‘out of’ ci̓ lim ‘into’ qalksə ‘through’ [t]qa̓ n ‘opening, ajar’ laqam ‘into’ ̓ tukʷs ‘out of (from the top)’ ni̓ ː ‘on (horizontal)’ laqaltip ‘under (horizontal)’ kiltip ‘under the bottom (of round object)’ m’in ‘upward (vertically)’ ̓ tip ‘downward’ pax̣ ‘uphill’ y’aqa ‘downhill’ [t]q’ali ‘upstream’ ḳisə ‘downstream’ caqa ‘from one end to the other’ sqa ‘across obstacle’ laːx̣ ‘equally at both ends’ ̓ kaːɬ ‘pulling to one side’ na̓ ː ‘conspicuously against a background’ qa̓ ltix ‘deep in woods’ y’aws ‘from confied/hidden to open’ ḳici̓ ɬ ‘into confined/hidden’ ̓ ḳila ‘past’ hakʷin ‘closer’ wətin  ‘further’ caqam ‘towards’ ʔukʷs ‘outward’ ̓ [t]kə ‘dropping down’ [t]qa̓ l ‘flush against’ saː ‘off, away’

kʷilks ‘back’ spə ‘horizontally off’ x̣lip ‘at end’ x̣cə̓  ‘crosswise’ wisin ‘lengthwise’ tx̣as ‘along the length’ x̣pə ‘at halfway point’ ̓ skʷin ‘at end of line/row’ ḳimiː ‘to the rear’ ̓ tim ‘away from’ ḳinaː ‘left behind’ loqa̓ liː ‘behind’ [t]qa̓ ː ‘tottering’ ɬim ‘in front’ luː=spa[qa]yt ‘in between’ ̓ [t]ku ‘around in circle’ ̓ u ‘all ̓ [t]ḳutḳ around’ haspaː ‘concave side up’ pəlx̣sim ‘concave side down’ ̓ ɬ ‘upside down’ ḳinx̣ haltim  ‘up from ground, from lying position’ [t]qa̓ yks ‘close to ground’ ḳitiː ‘stopping’ pa[aq]yt ‘in middle, in half crosswise’ ̓ hatix ‘in half lengthwise’ ḳis ‘from one place to another’ kʷɬiː ‘all over, randomly’ ̓ a̓ l ‘all over, covering’ ḳilq stax̣ ‘on one side only’ sax̣ ‘gathering together’ x̣ɬim ‘going around’ lukʷɬə ‘right under’ ma̓ qɬə ‘right over’

Some languages may express direction of motion via translocative and cislocative verbal affixes, indicating motion toward and away from the speaker, respectively. (18)            

Translocative and cislocative prefixes in Cherokee (Iroquoian) (MontgomeryAnderson 2008) a. ihlkv ta-aki-aloos-vv́ʔi   tree cis2-1b-fall:cmp-exp   ‘I fell from the tree.’ b. wi-uu-nvjiithl-éʔi naʔv   trans-3b-fall.headfirst:cmp-trm:cmp-dvb near   ‘He fell head first near it.’  



Sense of place: Space, landscape, and orientation 

 561

The examples presented in this section represent just a small sample of the variety of methods by which North American languages express topological relations. For additional examples see Mithun (1999: 132–51).

24.5 Landscape feature classification The domain of landscape exhibits much variation across languages. In particular, in contrast to other semantic domains such as flora and fauna, the classification of landscape is more likely to be driven by human affordance, that is, by how it is used by humans (cf. Levinson 2008: 257). Landscape “is not land, it is not nature, and it is not space” (Ingold 2000: 190) but rather “even the simplest environmental influence is either supported or transformed by social forces” (Sapir 1912: 226). The greatly varying landscapes of North America yield a range of different landscape terminologies among North American languages, reflecting different ways of knowing and journeying through the landscape. The variety of Inupiaq spoken in the Arctic community of Kiŋigin (Wales), alongside Bering Strait, distinguishes nearly one hundred terms referring to sea ice as a landscape feature. Thus, qaimġuq ‘small berm of frozen slush ice’ is distinguished from iuniq ‘pressure ridge made of broken ice’ from tunfuruaq ‘grounded pressure ridge’ (Weyapuk, Jr. & Krupnik 2012). At the other end of the Inuit dialect chain in the community of Kangiqsualujjuaq, Nunavik, Heyes (2011) documents 71 terms referring to sea ice, including specialized terminology to refer to the shape of the underside of the sea ice—which is only directly observable at times of extreme low tide.4 While the physical landscape may influence landscape terminology within language, the conceptualization of landscape is ultimately driven by human affordances. Kiŋigin and Kangiqsualujjuaq sea ice terminologies reflect not just the fact that the sea freezes in those locations but also—and especially—that those communities have an intimate cultural, economic, and spiritual connection to sea ice. These connections may differ in different communities. In particular, “people from different language groups/ cultures have different ways of conceptualizing landscape, as evidenced by different terminology and ways of talking about and naming landscape features” (Mark, Turk & Stea 2007: 16). When two different language communities share physical territory, their linguistic conceptualizations of the shared landscape may differ. In Western Alaska the Inuit-Yupik and Dene language families shared a long border across the foothills of the

4 Counting lexemes in a polysynthetic language is notoriously fraught, and the notion of “Inuit words for snow” in particular has become something of a cautionary tale in the study of linguistic relativity (Martin 1986; Pullum 1989). The point here is less the actual number of lexemes (which may be subject to debate) but rather the fact that these Inuit varieties encode detailed knowledge of the arctic environment (Kaplan 2004).

562 

 Gary Holton and Andrea L. Berez-Kroeker

Alaska Range, yet their landscape and streamscape terminologies differ significantly. Dene languages classify elevation features according to scale, whereas Inuit-Yupik languages emphasize shape over scale. For example, the Yup’ik root peng- yields forms ranging from pengunquq ‘mound’ to pengurpall’er ‘great big hill’, as well as non-landscape vocabulary such as pengigarneret ‘hives’ (Jacobson 2012). The crucial semantic component here is the convex shape, not the size of the feature. In the domain of streamscape, Dene languages delineate a landscape feature *kæq’, typically translated as ‘river mouth’. But the feature known as *kæq’ refers to much more than the hydrologic merging of two streams. Indeed, *kæq’ may refer to a location where a river emerges from the mountains into a flat, without any merging of streams (Holton 2011). No equivalent river mouth term is found in Inuit-Yupik languages. Instead, the concept of river mouth is conveyed metaphorically using the same term which would be used to refer to an anatomical mouth or an entry or opening such as a kayak hatch (< Proto Inuit-Yupik *paðə). Dene *kæq’ is frequently incorporated into place names, reflecting the importance of the river mouth as a settlement location. Thus, the place known in English as Stony River Village is Teggalgum ‘of stone’ in Yup’ik but Gidighuyghatno’ Xidochagg Qay’ (literally, ‘mouth-of-distant-river village’) in Deg Xinag, reflecting -chaag < Proto-Dene *kæq’. Language provides a way of conceptualizing landscape. Landscape classification often takes advantage of existing grammatical structures. In Seri (isolate), complex landscape terms are formed by combining one of four classificatory terms— hast ‘stone’, hax ‘fresh water’, xepe ‘seawater’, or hant ‘land’—with either a definite article with posture semantics, a nominalized form of an intransitive verb, or a relational noun.5 The result is a landscape classification system based on substances: stone, fresh water, seawater and land. In (19a-c) the substance term hax occurs with three different modifiers, resulting in three landscape feature terms. In (19c-d) the same modifier actim ‘cut off’ occurs with two different substance classifiers, resulting in two different landscape features. (19)                  

Seri complex landscape terms (O’Meara & Bohnemeyer 2008: 326–7) a. hax qu-imej   fresh.water sbj.nmlz-flow.sg   ‘stream, flowing arroyo’ b. hax c-aacoj   fresh.water sbj.nmlz-big.sg   ‘lake’ c. hax c-actim   fresh.water sbj.nmlz-cut.off.sg   ‘lagoon’

5 Seri is spoken in Mexico some 250 km south of the US border, so is technically outside of the scope of this volume. In terms of cultural history, however, Seri is very much a North American language.



     

Sense of place: Space, landscape, and orientation 

d.    

 563

xepe c-actim seawater sbj.nmlz-cut.off.sg ‘lagoon, tidal pool’

The inventory of monomorphemic nouns in Seri is relatively small, thus the landscape vocabulary makes use of an overarching typological feature of the language to construct complex landscape terms. As a result, every geographic entity is classified according to one of the four substance terms, providing a unique Seri conceptualization of the landscape. Landscape terminology may be closely tied to Indigenous knowledge of the landscape which can have vital consequences. This is true of Siberian Yupik sea ice terminology, which like its sister language Inupiaq, discussed above, contains dozens of terms distinguishing different varieties of sea ice. Siberian Yupik is spoken both on the coast of Chukotka and on St. Lawrence Island in Bering Strait. For much of the year sea ice surrounds St. Lawrence Island, and travel on and through sea ice is crucial to survival in the region. Sea ice terminology is largely based on human affordances. When qenu ‘newly formed slush ice’ packs together and begins to freeze on top it is referred to as qateghrapak. “You can see it from the distance because it is of truly white color. It is not safe to walk on and is vulnerable for one to fall through” (Oozeva et al. 2004: 41). When this qenu collides and doubles up in overlapping layers it is known as qaspighun, and this ice is safe to walk on (see Figure 2).

Fig. 2: Siberian Yupik sea ice terms: qaspighun (left) ‘safe to walk on’ and qateghrapak ‘dangerous’ (Oozeva et al. 2004)

564 

 Gary Holton and Andrea L. Berez-Kroeker

24.6 Place names and place naming strategies Place names and place naming strategies have figured prominently in research on Native North American languages.6 Since place naming is a deliberate act, place names are often taken to be non-arbitrary associations of sound and meaning, revealing something about the nature of human relationship to the environment. Many English place names in North America tend to be commemorative, referring to a person with only a vague relationship to the named place; or duplications of an existing place, as in “New York.” In contrast, Native North American place names include rich morphology which conveys significant information about the named place---a feature which Hunn (1996) calls “descriptive force.” ‘This is not merely an outsiders’ observation of Native place naming strategies. Reflecting on Dene place naming, Chief Peter John of Minto, Alaska recalled that “People used to name these things as they see them” (John & Krupa 1996). Similarly, Western Apache speakers remark, “That place looks just like its name” (Basso 1984). In Inuinnait (Inuit-Yupik) of Northwestern Canada nearly half of the 1007 place names documented by Collignon (2006) refer to either a landscape feature (e.  g., Imnaryuaq ‘the high cliff’), a morphological analogy as to what a landscape feature looks like (e.  g., Pualrinak ‘like a mitten’), or a shape (e.  g., Angmalukataaq ‘the little round one’). Yet a nearly equal amount of the names—some 40 %—represent not descriptions of the place but rather reference to activities or accidental, unintentional events. Examples include Havviurvik ‘the place to make knives’, Alliakhaqhiurvik ‘the place to search for material to make sledges’, Nauhirvik ‘the place where the women wait while the men are out hunting’. A sizeable portion of such names refer to specific subsistence activities, e.  g., Ivaturlik ‘the place that has duck nests’ and Nattiqtuuq ‘there are many seals’. Some names refer to accidental events or very specific occurrences, e.  g., Ilgaavik ‘where a pair of glasses was lost’. Nevertheless, there is significant variation in place-naming strategies across the languages of Native North America. For example, Dene languages show a strong tendency toward binomial names, combining a specific term with a landscape generic. Thus, the Ahtna name Yidateni Tl’aa is formed from the specific term yidateni ‘jaw trail’ combined with the landscape generic term tl’aa ‘headwaters’. This same specific may combine with numerous other landscape generic terms, reflecting what Kari has dubbed a “generative geographic capacity” (Kari 2010). In contrast, in Sahaptin only about 3 % of place names are binomial. A more typical Sahaptin strategy is to form place names with possessive constructions, as in k’amamul-nmí łámtax, literally ‘bald eagle’s head’ (Hunn 1996: 13). Languages also differ in the ability to coin new names. Although Yup’ik names are sometimes claimed to be of great antiquity, Fienup-Riordan cites numerous examples of 6 There are many excellent studies of place names and place naming practices in North America. In addition to those references mentioned in this section, see also Cowell & Moss (2003) on Arapaho, Meadows (2008) on Kiowa, and Whiteley (2011) on Hopi, for some excellent case studies.



Sense of place: Space, landscape, and orientation 

 565

recently coined, whimsical names, noting that “some places were named simply to make us smile” (2011: xxix). Thus, the Yup’ik place Kass’aq, literally ‘white person’, is so named simply because a white person lived there. In contrast, such recently coined, whimsical names are almost entirely absent in Dene languages (Holton 2022). Rather, Dene names are predominantly landscape based, generated in clusters within the domain of the riverine valley. Native place names are not mere geographic waypoints which situate a referent in space. Names create place and generate social meaning. As noted by Basso, “relationships to places are lived most often in the company of other people” (1996: 56–7). Basso recounts a Western Apache (Dene) conversation in which meaning is conveyed almost entirely with place names. In the excerpt in (20), Lola and Emily are attempting to console Louise, whose younger brother Robert had become ill the night before. Some people had attributed her brother’s illness to the fact that he had carelessly stepped on a snakeskin, but Louise’s brother saw no cause for concern. Lola’s reference to the name Tsé Hadigai evokes a story that occurred at this place—a story of a girl who, disobeying her grandmother’s instructions, took a shortcut and ended up getting bitten by a snake. Similarly, Emily’s reference to the name Túzhį’ Yahigai evokes another story—this time a story in which a man becomes unlucky in hunting as a result of not exercising caution and respect. And finally, Lola’s mention of the name K’is Deeschii’ Naaditiné references a story of a newly married man who suffers as a result of violating a menstrual taboo. This third story cautions against impulsive behavior, but it is also funny; hence, the mention of this place name evokes laughter from Louise and serves to resolve the tension of the situation. (20)                                  

Western Apache place names in discourse (Basso 1988: 105) Louise: shidizhé…   ‘My younger brother…’ Lola: Tsé Hadigaiyé yú’ ágodzaa.   ‘it happened at line of white rocks extends upward and out, at this very place!’ Emily: …(30-45 seconds) Ha’aa. Túzhį’ Yahigaiyé yú’ágodzaa.   ‘Yes. It happened at whiteness spreads out descending to water,     at this very place!’ Lola: …(30-45 seconds) Da’ aníí. K’is Deeschii’ Naaditiné yú’ágodzaa.   ‘Truly. It happened at trail extends across a long red ridge with alder trees,     at this very place!’ Louise: [laughs softly] Robert: gozhǫǫ doleeł.   ‘Pleasantness and goodness will be forthcoming.’ Lola: gozhǫǫ doleeł.   ‘Pleasantness and goodness will be forthcoming.’ Louise: shidizhé bíni’éshid ne góshé?   ‘My younger brother is foolish, isn’t he, dog?’

566 

 Gary Holton and Andrea L. Berez-Kroeker

The stories which took place at Tsé Hadigai and Túzhį’ Yahigai are cautionary tales, but in each case the protagonist survives, humbled but wiser. The names thus serve to console Louise and allay her worries. In Western Apache this form of discourse is recognized as a distinct genre, known as yałti’ bee’ízhi, literally, ‘speaking with names’. Another example of the use of place names in discourse is what Kari (2011) has termed “elite travel narratives” in Dene languages. These narratives are a kind of virtual guided tour in which the speaker discusses, in sequential order, all the meaningful and hence named locations along a given route. A single narrative may cover over one hundred miles of river and/or trail and is often interspersed with personal memories and descriptions of how each site was used seasonally for camping and hunting (Berez-Kroeker 2018: 76). A fine example of an elite travel narrative is Ahtna speaker Jim Tyone’s account of traveling in 1912 from Tyone Village to Knik, Alaska, a distance of some 170 miles. Recorded in 1981, nearly 70 years after the trip occurred, Tyone mentions 37 place names in geographic order over the span of a ten-minute narrative. The locations of the named places are numbered in Figure 3.

Fig. 3: Jim Tyone’s 1912 travel route from Tyone to Knik (Kari & Fall 2004: 225)



Sense of place: Space, landscape, and orientation 

 567

24.7 Reclaiming a sense of place through community mapping initiatives The complex Indigenous landscapes referenced and brought about through discursive acts like travel narration are being increasingly made visible through participatory mapping projects in Indigenous communities worldwide. The rise in recent years of technologies and methods for participatory mapping—the development of maps by a community with or without the assistance of professional cartographers—has enabled the documentation of community knowledge of the land in tangible ways that were not possible before. Participatory frameworks are often facilitated by web-based mapping—or cybercartography—which offers a dynamic and interlinked alternative to traditional two-dimensional paper-based cartography. “Audio, video, and text recordings of oral histories can be intertwined with photographs and drawings, allowing narratives to emerge through the user’s navigation of the atlas, and eventually, through his/her input into the atlas” (Aporta et al. 2014: 242). In this way web-based participatory approaches can help to overcome one of the fundamental challenges of Indigenous cartography, namely, that placing a name on a map may serve to divorce that name from the cultural context in which the name is embedded. As with landscape more generally, places derive meaning through human affordance: “A place owes its character to the experiences it affords to those who spend time there—to the sights, sounds and indeed smells that constitute its specific ambience. And these, in turn, depend on the kinds of activities in which its inhabitants engage.” (Ingold 2000: 192)

The Nunaliit Cybercartographic Atlas Framework (http://nunaliit.org) provides a webbased participatory platform for communities to manage geolinguistic data in culturally responsible ways. Developed by the Geomatics and Cartographic Research Centre at Carleton University, Nunaliit is a “bottom up” technology which employs a schemaless, extensible database so as not to impose predefined categories or templates (Hayes, Pulsifer & Fiset 2014). In particular, the platform does not determine what is mapped or how it is represented. For example, in developing the Nunaliit-based Kitikmeot Place Names Atlas, the community deliberately avoided documenting place names in isolation. Instead, the project focused on mapping oral stories, in which the names were embedded (Keith, Crockatt & Hayes 2014: 222). Atlases based on the Nunaliit framework can be constantly revised and updated, making them better able to reflect the dynamic nature of Indigenous knowledge (Aporta et al. 2014: 242). And Nunaliit atlases are also inherently participatory, allowing users to contribute content directly and comment and amend content submitted by others. Nunaliit has been adopted by dozens of communities in North America, especially in Northern Canada.

568 

 Gary Holton and Andrea L. Berez-Kroeker

Participatory frameworks also serve as a kind of action research, supporting efforts in conservation and planning: “Because of the potential of such participatory mapping projects to bring forth indigenous knowledge and perspectives, they are increasingly becoming central to participatory approaches to biodiversity conservation and land-use planning in areas populated by indigenous people.” (Sletto 2012: 13)

An example of this kind of planning-centered mapping project is use-and-occupancy mapping, the documentation of physical and cultural use of the natural environment and resources. One such use-and-occupancy project was undertaken in the Tsleil-Waututh Nation in and around Vancouver, Canada (George-Wilson & Aberley 2009). In the 1990s, Chief Leonard George charged his community with a vision to “absolutely and irrevocably place a Tsleil-Waututh ‘face’ back on the territory of his people” (George-Wilson & Aberley 2009: 16), which led to a broad use-and-occupancy mapping project. With funding from the British Columbia Ministry of Forests, in 1998 members of the project team embarked on doing map-biography interviews, in which participants were recorded telling stories of participating in various land-use activities (eg hunting, fishing) while also locating these events on maps. The narratives were transcribed and the maps digitized, and the map-biography interviews of 90 Tsleil-Waututh participants were integrated into a geographic information system. The result was a rich database of locations within the Tsleil-Waututh territory paired with multiple narratives about how those places have been used. In addition to the tangible benefits of having many detailed maps demonstrating Tseil-Waututh use and occupancy, the project also brought a less tangible but nonetheless important benefit, “[t]he knowledge of the elders was honoured and preserved, and the prideful curiosity of our youth was piqued” (George-Wilson & Aberley 2009: 18). Use-and-occupancy mapping can also impact resource management policies. In Southeast Alaska, spatiotemporal mapping of the Tlingit sac roe herring fishery was critical in convincing the Alaska Department of Fish and Game to implement a nearly eight-fold increase in the size of the area closed to commercial harvest. This change represents a shift from a single-species “maximum sustained yield” model of resource management toward an Indigenous model of “cultivated abundance,” in which herring spawning grounds are managed for consistent rather than maximum yields (Thornton & Kitka 2015). Beyond resource management, use-and-occupancy mapping plays a crucial role in land rights and treaty negotiations. The 1971 Alaska Native Claims Settlement Act granted Native Alaskans the right to select lands based on their cultural history and significance. As a result a dedicated office was created within the Bureau of Indian Affairs in order to map traditional sites and record related oral histories (Pratt 2009). While use-and-occupancy mapping does not always resolve land claims in ways that benefit Native peoples, it does serve as a way of asserting agency over Native lands, what Schreyer (2016) has termed “performative stewardship.”



Sense of place: Space, landscape, and orientation 

 569

Participatory mapping need not rely heavily on Western concepts of cartography, but rather represents an opportunity to create maps and GIS that are Indigenously-centered, relying on Indigenous knowledge and history. Rather than being a tool for marginalization, mapping becomes a path for reinforcing community and territory (cf. Sletto 2012; Brown & Kyttä 2018; Cochrane & Corbett 2018). One project that seeks to integrate storytelling and place-making is the Mapping Indigenous Los Angeles project (https://mila.ss.ucla.edu/). This project aims to center the experiences of Indigenous people living in the Los Angeles area through maps linked to historical and personal narratives, images, and videos. Some experiences of the original inhabitants of the region, the Gabrielino Tongva, are represented in the project, but story maps have been contributed that reflect the histories and experiences of other tribes from southern California, Indigenous peoples of Latin America living in diaspora, and Pacific Islander communities. The project exemplifies the power of participatory mapping to reinforce social bonds in and between communities, especially in times of great historical change. Finally, participatory mapping technologies can be used to present “the complexities of indigenous landscapes. Indigenous land tenure and boundaries are fluid, overlapping, and changing; Indigenous conceptions of space reflect complex social relations, and the meanings of landscapes are interwoven with spiritual relationships” (Sletto 2012: 14). The Native Land project (https://native-land.ca/), a community-mapping project on a macro scale, captures the fluid and overlapping historical, territorial, and linguistic boundaries through the transparent layers that users can toggle on and off. Started in 2015, Native Land is an interactive map where users can explore Indigenous territories, languages and treaties in North America (as well as much of Central and South America, Greenland, Hawai‘i, Guam and Australia). Users can toggle information layers, and clicking on the map brings up links to details on tribes and nations, languages, and treaties. Figure 4 shows the Native Land interface after a user has clicked on a location in present-day western United States. Links in the left-hand text box lead to internal pages containing more information about languages, territories and treaties relevant to the selected location. Each of these internal pages contains links to external sources including tribe and nation web pages and in some cases, to historical documents. The Native Land project is based in Canada and, since December 2018, has been run by an Indigenous Board of Directors and an Advisory Council. The information in the project is continually updated and corrected with input from the international community.

570 

 Gary Holton and Andrea L. Berez-Kroeker

Fig. 4: Screenshot from Native Land after a user has clicked on a location in present-day western United States (https://native-land.ca/).

24.8 De-colonizing place in North America Sense of place in North America has been greatly affected by centuries of colonial efforts to eradicate Native languages and cultures in a deliberate attempt to break the ties to place. Nearly three decades ago Hawaiian scholar Kihei deSilva noted, “we live in a time of un-naming, in a time when old names for the land, names given in honor, happiness, and sorrow have been set aside…” (quoted in Louis 2011). And while many names of Native origin have been adopted by English and French speakers in North America (see Bright 2004), connections with contemporary Native communities are rarely acknowledged. However, there are signs that this process of un-naming is beginning to reverse. Native lands are becoming increasingly recognized and visible among both Native and non-Native populations in North America. Moreover, Native names have long been seen as crucial to land claims issues in both the United States and Canada (Wonders 1987). One of the first initiatives undertaken following the passing of the 1971 Alaska Native Claims Settlement Act was the creation of a dedicated office within the Bureau of Indian Affairs to document place names and toponymic knowledge (Pratt 2009). While these efforts have been valuable, they took place mostly behind the scenes, with little direct public impact. In contrast, more recent efforts have focused not merely on documenting Native place names but on seeking official recognition. These efforts acknowledge



Sense of place: Space, landscape, and orientation 

 571

the inherent symbolic value of place names as part of an effort to decolonize the North American landscape. As Kanien’kehá:ka scholar Gerald Taiaiake Alfred notes: “It’s important to reclaim the names, because names are symbolic of the attitudes and ideas people bring—of their relationship to the land and each other. If all indigenous names are erased, and never respected, it shows the white society’s view of indigenous people: that we no longer exist; we’ve been erased…” (Alfred, quoted in Ball 2013)

Many significant renamings have occurred in recent times. In 2009 the Canadian government recognized Haida Gwaii as the official name of the island group formerly known as Queen Charlotte Islands, named by British Captain George Dixon in 1778 after his ship, the Queen Charlotte, itself named after the wife of King George III. The name Haida Gwaii itself is an English transliteration of the actual Haida name, X̱aayda gwaay, meaning literally ‘islands of the Haida people’. A similar example within the United States is the official recognition of Denali, a transliteration of the Koyukon name Denaalee, which in 2015 replaced the former Mt. McKinley as the name for North America’s highest peak. However, the Denali case is in many ways an outlier, as the name has long been widely used by non-Native Alaskans and could arguably be said to be an English name of Native origin, as opposed to an actual Native name. Perhaps more significant is the 2016 adoption of the name Draanjik River, from the Gwich’in name Draanjik, literally ‘cache river’. In contrast to Denali, the Native name for this major river in eastern Alaska was never widely known outside the Gwich’in community. Yet, since its official renaming the Gwich’in name is now used routinely in popular media, commercial business and government, often without any reference to the former English name. Renaming efforts have not been limited to natural geographic features but have also affected names for towns and villages. In 2016 the town of Barrow, Alaska was renamed to its traditional Inupiaq name Utqiaġvik.7 As an official census-designated place, this name has quickly gained wide usage among both Native and non-Native peoples in Alaska (though the uvular fricative is often represented without the diacritic). With a population of over 4000, Utqiaġvik is the largest town in Alaska to have an official Native name spelled in an official orthography, and it may well be the largest such in the US. Sometimes renaming efforts result in a compromise which replaces colonial names but fails to recognize Native names. In 2016 Black Elk Peak was adopted as the official name for the highest point in South Dakota, replacing the name Harney Peak. The earlier name, first officially adopted in 1907, commemorated General William Harney, an Army cavalry officer whose reputation for brutality against Native people was widely known even at the time. In 1855 the New York Times commented, “The lamentable butcheries of Indians by Harney’s command on the Plains have excited the most painful feelings” (Whitney 2016). Given this history, the motivation for changing the name was clear, but

7 https://edits.nationalmap.gov/apps/gaz-domestic/public/summary/1398635

572 

 Gary Holton and Andrea L. Berez-Kroeker

the campaign to change the name met with much resistance. The eventual decision to replace it with the name of the Lakota leader known in English as Black Elk represented a compromise, forgoing official recognition of the traditional Lakota (Siouan) name of the peak, Hiŋháŋ Káǧa, literally ‘owl maker’. Some renaming efforts have failed to achieve even partial success. The name Mt. Rainier, referring to the highest peak in the Pacific Northwest, was bestowed by explorer George Vancouver in the 1790’s to honor a fellow member of the British Royal Navy. Efforts to replace this with the Salish name Tahoma have been repeatedly rejected by the U.S. Board of Geographic Names, citing lack of broad usage (Seattle Times Editorial Board 2015). In other words, changing an official name within the US context requires support from the colonizing population itself—something which exists for Denali but apparently not for Tahoma. In some cases legal restrictions prohibit adoption of Indigenous names. For example, the Rural Addressing Initiative of the Navajo Nation explicitly prohibits the adoption of Navajo names for roads on the Navajo Reservation, arguing that this could hinder emergency response (Webster 2014: 385). Resistance to reclaiming Indigenous names is not limited to places of geographic prominence. Efforts to rename Mount Douglas, a 225 meter high hill in Victoria, British Columbia, to its SENĆOŦEN (Coast Salish) name, PKOL ([pq’áls]), have so far been unsuccessful (Ball 2013). In spite of these setbacks, efforts to reclaim Native place names and de-colonize the North American landscape continue. “Place-names are not neutral, but fully implicated in concerns about who has and does not have the right (and power) to name” (Webster 2017: 253). While often construed as mere symbolic acts, official place naming efforts are having a real impact on the linguistic landscape. Canadian and US laws require that official names be used in certain official contexts, such as government maps, road signs, and reports. This results in an increased presence of Native language in the public sphere—both Native and non-Native. During an intense wildfire season in Alaska in 2019 news reports repeatedly referred to the Dranjiik River—marking perhaps the first time in over a century that Gwich’in language had been used by non-Native people in the public domain. Official place naming is thus contributing toward restoring a Native American sense of place in North America, de-colonizing a landscape which has long been viewed as terra nullius by outsiders.

24.9 References Brown, Greg & Marketta Kyttä. 2018. Key issues and priorities in participatory mapping: Toward integration or increased specialization? Applied Geography 95. 1–8. Cochrane, Logan & Jon Corbett. 2018. Participatory mapping. In Jan Servaes (eds.), Handbook of Communication for Development and Social Change, 1–9. Singapore: Springer. Oxford, Will. 2007. Towards a grammar of Innu-aimun particles. Memorial University of Newfoundland. Aporta, Claudio, Ingrid Kritsch, Alestine Andre, Kristi Benson, Sharon Snowshoe, William Firth & Del Carry. 2014. The Gwich’in Atlas: Place Names, Maps, and Narratives. In D.R. Fraser Taylor & Tracey P. Lauriault



Sense of place: Space, landscape, and orientation 

 573

(eds.), Developments in the Theory and Practice of Cybercartography: Applications and Indigenous Mapping, 229–244. Amsterdam: Elsevier. Ball, David P. 2013. A People’s Geography of BC: Renaming of Saanich mountain latest in campaign to reclaim indigenous landmarks. Tyee News. Vancouver, BC. Retrieved from https://thetyee.ca/ News/2013/05/27/Peoples-Geography-BC/ Basso, Keith H. 1984. Western Apache place name hierarchies. In Elizabeth Tooker (ed.), Naming Systems: Proceedings of the American Ethnological Society, vol. 4, 78–94. Washington, D.C.: American Ethnological Society. Basso, Keith H. 1988. ‘Speaking with names’: Language and landscape among the Western Apache. Cultural Anthropology 3(2). 99–130. Basso, Keith H. 1996. Wisdom Sits in Places: Notes on a Western Apache Landscape. In Steven Feld & Keith H. Basso (eds.), Senses of Place, 53–90. Santa Fe, NM: School of American Research Press. Berez, Andrea L. 2011. Directional Reference, Discourse, and Landscape in Ahtna. University of California Santa Barbara dissertation. Berez-Kroeker, Andrea L. 2018. Directional Reference in Discourse and Narrative: Comparing indigenous and non-indigenous genres in Ahtna. In Gary Holton & Thomas Thornton (eds.), Language and Toponymy in Alaska and Beyond, 75–95. Honolulu and Fairbanks: University of Hawaii Press and Alaska Native Language Center. Bischoff, Shannon & Carmen Jany (eds.). 2018. Insights from Practices in Community-Based Research. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. Bright, William. 1957. The Karok Language. Berkeley: University of California Press. Bright, William. 2004. Native American Placenames of the United States. Norman: University of Oklahoma Press. Broadwell, George Aaron. 2006. A Choctaw Reference Grammar. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Brown, Greg & Marketta Kyttä. 2018. Key issues and priorities in participatory mapping: Toward integration or increased specialization? Applied Geography 95. 1–8. Brucks, Caleb. 2015. The creation of narrative space: The directional system of Upper Tanana. University of Regina dissertation. Burenhult, Niclas. 2008. Spatial coordinate systems in demonstrative meaning. Linguistic Typology 12. 99–142. Chafe, Wallace. 2014. A Grammar of the Seneca Language. Berkeley: University of California Press. Retrieved from https://www.ucpress.edu/book/9780520286412/a-grammar-of-the-seneca-language Cochrane, Logan & Jon Corbett. 2018. Participatory mapping. In Jan Servaes (ed.), Handbook of Communication for Development and Social Change, 1–9. Singapore: Springer. Collignon, Béatrice. 2006. Knowing Places: The Inuinnait, Landscapes, and the Environment. Transl. by L. W. Müller-Wille. Edmonton: Canadian Circumpolar Institute. Cowell, Andrew & Alonzo Moss. 2003. Arapaho Place Names in Colorado: Form and Function, Language and Culture. Anthropological Linguistics 45(4). 349–389. Davidson, Matthew. 2002. Studies in Wakashan (Nootkan) Grammar. Buffalo: University of New York at Buffalo dissertation. Fienup-Riordan, Ann (ed.). 2011. Qaluyaarmiuni Nunamtenek Qanemciput: Our Nelson Island Stories. Seattle: University of Washington Press. Fortescue, Michael. 2011. Orientation Systems of the North Pacific Rim. Copenhagen: Museum Tuscalanum Press. George-Wilson, Leah D. & Doug Aberley. 2009. Using your maps: Tsleil-Waututh case study. In Terry N. Tobias (ed.), Living Proof: The Essential Data Collection Guide for Indigenous Use-and-Occupancy Map Surveys, 13–30. Vancouver: Ecotrust Canada. Gordon, Lynn. 1986. Maricopa Morphology and Syntax. Berkeley: University of California Press. Harrington, John Peabody. 1916. The Ethnogeography of the Tewa Indians. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office.

574 

 Gary Holton and Andrea L. Berez-Kroeker

Hayes, Amos, Peter L. Pulsifer & J.P. Fiset. 2014. The Nunaliit cybercartographic atlas framework. In D. R. Fraser Taylor & Tracey P. Lauriault (eds.), Developments in the Theory and Practice of Cybercartography, 129–140. Amsterdam: Elsevier. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-62713-1.00009-X Hess, Thom. 1979. A Comparison of Marine and Riverine Orientation Vocabulary in Two Coast Salish Languages. Anthropological Linguistics 21(8). 363–378. Heyes, Scott. 2011. Cracks in the knowledge: Sea ice terms in Kangiqsualujjuaq, Nunavik. The Canadian Geographer 55(1). 91–107. Holton, Gary. 2011. Differing conceptualizations of the same landscape: The Athabaskan and Eskimo language boundary in Alaska. In David M. Mark, Andrew G. Turk, Niclas Burenhult & David Stea (eds.), Landscape in Language, 225–237. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Holton, Gary. 2015. Initial thoughts on developing a registration authority for Alaska Native place names. Unpublished Manuscript., Fairbanks. Retrieved from http://hdl.handle.net/11122/7855 Holton, Gary. 2022. Place naming strategies in Inuit-Yupik and Dene languages in Alaska. In Kenneth L. Pratt & Scott Heyes (eds.), Memory and Landscape: Indigenous Responses to a Changing North, 276–296. Athabasca: Athabasca University Press. Hunn, Eugene. 1996. Columbia Plateau Indian place names: What can they teach us? Linguistic Anthropology 6(1). 3–26. Ingold, Tim. 2000. The Perception of the Environment: Essays on Livelihood, Dwelling and Skill. Psychology Press. Jacobson, Steven A. 1984. Semantics and morphology of demonstratives in Central Yup’ik Eskimo. Inuit Studies 8. 185–192. Jacobson, Steven A. 1995. A Practical Grammar of the Central Alaskan Yup’ik Eskimo Language. Fairbanks: Alaska Native Language Center. Jacobson, Steven A. 2012. Yup’ik Eskimo Dictionary. 2nd edition. Fairbanks: Alaska Native Language Center. John, Peter & David J. Krupa. 1996. The Gospel According to Peter John. Fairbanks: Alaska Native Knowledge Network. Kaplan, Lawrence D. 2004. Inuit snow terms: Folk wisdom or linguistic fact? Presented at the 14th Inuit Studies Conference, Arctic Institute of North America. Kari, James. 1990. Ahtna Athabaskan Dictionary. Fairbanks: Alaska Native Language Center. Kari, James. 2010. Ahtna Travel Narratives: A Demonstration of Shared Geographic Knowledge among Alaska Athabascans. Fairbanks: Alaska Native Language Center. Kari, James. 2011. A case study of Ahtna Athabascan geographic knowledge. In David M. Mark, Andrew G. Turk, Niclas Burenhult & David Stea (eds.), Landscape in Language, 261–274. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Kari, James & James Fall. 2004. Shem Pete’s Alaska 2nd ed. Fairbanks: University of Alaska Press. Keith, Darren, Kim Crockatt & Amos Hayes. 2014. The Kitikmeot Place Name Atlas. In D.R. Fraser Taylor & Tracey P. Lauriault (eds.), Developments in the Theory and Practice of Cybercartography, 219–228. Amsterdam: Elsevier. Kimball, Geoffrey D. 1991. Koasati Grammar. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Leer, Jeff. 1989. Directional systems in Athapaskan and Na-Dene. In Eung-Do Cook & Keren D. Rice (eds.), Athapaskan Linguistics: Current Perspectives on a Language Family, 575–622. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Levinson, Stephen C. 2003. Space in Language and Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Levinson, Stephen C. 2008. Landscape, seascape and the ontology of places on Rossel Island, Papua New Guinea. Language Sciences 30(3). 256–290. Louis, Renee Pualani. 2011. Hawaiian storied place names: Re-placing cultural meaning. In David M. Mark, Andrew G. Turk, Niclas Burenhult & David Stea (eds.), Landscape in Language, 167–186. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Macaulay, Monica. 2004. On the Karuk directionals. In Marc Ettlinger, Nicholas Fleisher & Park-Doob, Mischa (eds.), 30th Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society. Special Session on the Morphology of Native American Languages, 85–101. Berkeley: Berkeley Linguistics Society.



Sense of place: Space, landscape, and orientation 

 575

Mark, David M., Andrew G. Turk & David Stea. 2007. Progress on Yindjibarndi ethnophysiography. In Stephan Winter, Matt Duckham, Lars Kulik & Benjamin Kuipers (eds.), Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Spatial Information Theory, 1–19. Melbourne: Springer. Martin, Laura. 1986. “Eskimo words for snow”: A case study in the genesis and decay of an anthropological example. American Anthropologist 88(2). 418–423. McCluskey, Stephen C. 1977. The Astronomy of the Hopi Indians. Journal for the History of Astronomy 8(3). 174–195. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/002182867700800302 Meadows, William C. 2008. Kiowa Ethnogeography. Austin: University of Texas Press. Retrieved from https:// utpress.utexas.edu/books/meaeth Mithun, Marianne. 1999. The Languages of Native North America. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Montgomery-Anderson, Brad. 2008. A Reference Grammar of Oklahoma Cherokee. University of Kansas dissertation. O’Meara, Carolyn & Jürgen Bohnemeyer. 2008. Complex landscape terms in Seri. Language Sciences 30(2–3). 316–339. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langsci.2006.12.006 Oozeva, Conrad, Chester Noongwook, George Noongwook, Christina Alowa & Igor Krupnik. 2004. Watching Ice and Weather Our Way; Sikumengllu Eslamengllu Esghapalleghput. Washington, D.C.: Arctic Studies Center, Smithsonian Institution. Oxford, Will. 2007. Towards a grammar of Innu-aimun particles. St. John’s: Memorial University of Newfoundland dissertation. Park, Indrek. 2012. A Grammar of Hidatsa. Bloomington: Indian University dissertation. Pratt, Kenneth L. 2009. A history of the ANCSA 14(h)(1) program and significant reckoning points, 1975–2008. In Kenneth L. Pratt (ed.), Chasing the Dark: Perspectives on Place, History and Alaska Native Land Claims, Vol. 1: Shadowlands, 3–43. Anchorage, Alaska: United States Department of the Interior, Bureau of Indian Affairs, ANCSA Office. Pullum, Geoffrey K. 1989. The great Eskimo vocabulary hoax. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 7(2). 275–281. Sapir, Edward. 1912. Language and environment. American Anthropologist 14(2). 226–242. Schreyer, Christine. 2016. Taku River Tlingit Genres of Place as Performatives of Stewardship. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 26(1). 4–25. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/jola.12109 Schupbach, Shannon Scott. 2013. The Blackfoot demonstrative system: Function, form, and meaning. University of Montana. Seattle Times Editorial Board. 2015. After McKinley, it’s time to consider renaming Rainier. Seattle Times. Retrieved from https://www.seattletimes.com/opinion/editorials/after-mckinley-its-time-to-considerrenaming-rainier-2/ Sikos, Les. 2010. Locative Constructions in Lakhota: Evidence for/against “universal conceptual categories” in spatial topology. Colorado Research in Linguistics 1–38. Sletto, Bjørn. 2012. Indigenous Rights, Insurgent Cartographies, and the Promise of Participatory Mapping. Portal, Issue 7, 2012. Tarpent, Marie-Lucie. 1987. A Grammar of the Nishga Language. University of Victoria dissertation. Thornton, Thomas F. & Harvey Kitka. 2015. An indigenous model of a contested Pacific herring fishery in Sitka, Alaska. International Journal of Applied Geospatial Research (IJAGR) 6(1). 94–117. Uhlenbeck, C. C. 1912. A new series of Blackfoot texts from the southern Peigans Blackfoot Reservation, Teton County, Montana. Amsterdam: Johannes Müller. Retrieved from https://www.worldcat.org/title/ new-series-of-blackfoot-texts-from-the-southern-peigans-blackfoot-reservation-teton-countymontana/oclc/514003 Valentine, Randy. 2001. Nishnaabemwin Reference Grammar. University of Toronto Press. Webster, Anthony K. 2014. Dif’g’one’ and semiotic calquing: A Signography of the linguistic landscape of the Navajo Nation. Journal of Anthropological Research 70(3). 385–410.

576 

 Gary Holton and Andrea L. Berez-Kroeker

Webster, Anthony K. 2017. Why Tséhootsooí does not equal ‘Kit Carson Drive’: Reflections on Navajo place names and the inequalities of language. Anthropological Linguistics 59(3). 239–262. DOI: https://doi. org/10.1353/anl.2017.0009 Weyapuk, Jr. Winton & Igor Krupnik. 2012. Kifikmi Sigum Qanuq Ilitaavut/Wales Inupiaq Sea Ice Dictionary. Washington, D.C: Smithsonian Arctic Studies Center. Whiteley, Peter. 2011. Hopi place value: Translating a landscape. In Brian Swann (ed.), Born in the Blood: On Native American Translation, 84–108. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Whitney, Stu. 2016, August 12. In defense of Black Elk Peak. Argus Leader. Sioux Falls, SD. Retrieved from https://www.argusleader.com/story/news/columnists/stu-whitney/2016/08/12/harney-peak-black-elkdaugaard-thune-south-dakota-native-american-stu-whitney/88627694/ Wonders, William C. 1987. Native claims and place names in Canada’s Western Arctic. The Canadian Journal of Native Studies 7(1). 111–120.

Sihwei Chen and Lisa Matthewson

25 A sense of time and world Abstract: Tense marking is used to express the location of situations within time; aspect marking expresses how situations unfold through time, and modal elements concern all kinds of possible situations that are different from the actual world. This chapter highlights the range of variation in the systems of tense, aspect, and modality in North American languages. Our survey shows that North American languages possess a range of different tense systems: the languages often appear to be tenseless, but may also contain optional tenses, multiple tenses, or even silent tenses. Most of the languages are rich in their aspects, and the inventory of aspectual distinctions they mark is diverse. Modal systems tend to lexicalize modal flavour (e.  g., using different words for inferences as opposed to obligations), rather than encoding modal strength. The study of these phenomena in the languages indigenous to North America has contributed greatly to our understanding of language typology and has motivated in-depth semantic studies. The study of these languages has also shed light on properties which are shared across languages, but which in other languages are not obvious on the surface.

25.1 Introduction Every language can talk about situations that are separate from the present time and from the actual world. Temporal separation is achieved by means of tense and aspect markers, where tense locates situations relative to the speech time, and aspect characterizes the temporal profile of situations. World separation relies on modal expressions. The grammar employed for tense, aspect, and modality varies considerably across languages. This chapter highlights the features of North American languages in these three areas. We cannot address all languages, or even all families, of the continent. Our discussion touches on Algonquian, Chumashan, Eskimo-Aleut, Iroquoian, Muskogean, Na-Dene, Pomoan, Sahaptian, Salishan, Tsimshianic, Uto-Aztecan, and Wakashan languages, plus two language isolates. See also Matthewson (2017) on tense, aspect, and modality in American languages. We show that most languages of North America lack an inflectional paradigm for tense, and that the languages that do have overt tense marking may not obligatorily use it. On the other hand, some of the languages encode fine-grained temporal distinctions in their tense systems. Unlike tenses, aspects are often overtly expressed, but the inventory and the semantic properties they encode vary from language to language; we review some of the interesting patterns. Lastly, we provide an overview of modal systems and show that there is a tendency for modals to only lexicalize the type of modality, leaving the strength (i.  e., necessity or possibility) up to the context. We also discuss the interaction of modality with other categories, including evidentiality, temporality, and mood. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-025

578 

 Sihwei Chen and Lisa Matthewson

25.2 Tense 25.2.1 Introduction to tense Tense is a grammatical category that relates the time of an utterance to a reference time, which is intuitively the time about which a claim is being made (Reichenbach 1947; Klein 1994). The reference time may be distinct from the time of the situation being described, as shown in (1). The reference time of B’s statement—the time B is making a claim about—is when B looked into the room (e.  g., 9 pm). The actual flickering of the light could have lasted for much longer; it could even still be flickering today. The job of the past tense marking (on was) is to indicate that the reference time is in the past. (1)

A: B:

What did you notice when you looked into the room? The light was flickering. (Matthewson 2006:675; adapted from Klein 1994)

Many North American languages lack a grammatical paradigm for tense, and languages with tense marking may not obligatorily use it, or conversely may encode more finegrained distinctions. This section reviews these types of variation and discusses issues worth paying attention to when studying tenses in North American languages.

25.2.2 Types of languages without a tense paradigm Many North American languages allow sentences without any marking of tense. An important question is whether the temporal reference of sentences in the language is grammatically constrained, even when there seems to be no tense marking. We discuss two broad types of languages based on this criterion.

25.2.2.1 Reference times are restricted St’át’imcets (Lillooet, Salishan) is an example of a language where the distinction between a non-future vs. a future reference time is rigidly encoded for every sentence (Matthewson 2006; see also Davis and Matthewson 2009 on tense in Salishan). For example, (2a-b) show that sentences without any tense marking can be interpreted as either present or past, but not future.1 A future interpretation requires overt marking, for example by the element kelh in (3).

1 The type of predicate influences the default interpretations; see e.  g., Bar-el (2005); Bar-el, Davis and Matthewson (2005); Matthewson (2006).



A sense of time and world 

(2)

a.

       

b.      

(3)

St’át’imcets (Matthewson 2006: 678) Say’sez’-lhkán kelh. play-1sg.sbj prosp ‘I will play.’ / ≠ ‘I played.’/ ≠ ‘I am playing.’

 579

St’át’imcets (Matthewson 2006: 676) Sáy’sez’-lhkan.2 play-1sg.sbj ‘I played.’ / ‘I am playing.’ St’át’imcets (Matthewson 2006: 677) *Sáy’sez’-lhkan natcw. play-1sg.sbj tomorrow Intended for ‘I will play tomorrow.’

The fact that sentences without future marking are strictly restricted to non-future interpretations suggests that there is a silent non-future tense marker. A non-future/ future tense system is also observed in Gitksan (Tsimshianic, Jóhannsdóttir and Matthewson 2007), Tłı̨ chǫ Yatıì (Na-Dene, Welch 2015), and Tlingit (Na-Dene, Burge 2017; Cable 2017a). Blackfoot (Algonquian) is another language that obligatorily marks future, but it may have a finer tense distinction that is only revealed by some predicates. As exemplified in (4), eventive predicates (in the absence of imperfective marking) are only interpreted in the past in Blackfoot (unlike in St’át’imcets, cf. (2a); Reis Silva and Matthewson 2007).3,4 (4)

Blackfoot (Reis Silva and Matthewson 2007: 200) nítsskiita. nit-ihkiita 1sg-cook ‘I cooked.’ / ≠ ‘I am cooking.’

2 Abbreviations not in the Leipzig Glossing Rules: Ø: inanimate proximate, agt: grammatical agent, circ: circumstantial (modal), cisl: cislocative, cm: conjugation marker, contr: contrastive, cn: connective, cnj: conjunctive, conj: conjunction, d.poss: d-possessive, dst.pst: distant past, epis: epistemic, ii: series ii pronoun, imm: immediate, inan: inanimate, inch: inchoative, inf: inferential evidential, ipc: indeclinable particle, ipv: indeclinable preverb, lc: limited control, mid: middle, mod: modal, nctr: non-control, pat: grammatical patient, pn: proper noun, possib: possibility, ptc: particle, pr: pronoun, prosp: prospective, que: interrogative mood, redup: reduplication, rl: realis, rpt: reportative evidential, sr: switch reference, st: stative, sub: subordinator, th: thematic, vai: animate intransitive verb, vii: inanimate intransitive verb, vti: transitive inanimate verb, wtn: direct/witness evidential. We use the orthographies of original sources for all cited data, but some glosses have been standardized. 3 Eventive predicates typically describe dynamic situations (such as play and cook) and form a contrast with stative predicates (such as know, hungry and tall). 4 See Ritter and Wiltschko (2004, 2005, 2009, 2014) and Wiltschko (2003) for a tenseless analysis of Blackfoot and Halkomelem (Salishan); see Matthewson (2005) for a reply to Wiltschko (2003).

580 

 Sihwei Chen and Lisa Matthewson

Compared to the above languages, Wá∙šiw is different in that it allows future interpretations without overt future morphology in certain contexts (Bochnak 2016): (5)

Wá∙šiw (Bochnak 2016: 256) Context: You want to know what Mona will cook for dinner later today. hut’aŋahéːš móːna ʔíːbikhayi dewp’áwɨt démlulewe hut’aŋa-heːš mona ʔ-iːbik’-ha-i dewp’awɨd d-emlu-lewe what-q Mona 3-be.cooked-caus-ind evening d.poss-eat-for ‘What will Mona prepare for dinner?’

25.2.2.2 Reference times are unrestricted In contrast to the languages discussed so far, some North American languages do not have tense systems that place restrictions on reference times. Main clauses in Northern Paiute (Uto-Aztecan) are one such case; in (6), the verb form does not change regardless of the reference time (although temporal reference can be disambiguated by a time adverb; Toosarvandani 2016). Toosarvandani argues, however, that clause-chained clauses in Northern Paiute mark (relative) tense distinctions. (6)  

Northern Paiute (Toosarvandani 2016: 863) Idzi’i/Mu’a tɨ=kaadzi madabbui-wɨnnɨ. yesterday/tomorrow refl=car fix-prog ‘He was/will be fixing his car yesterday/tomorrow.’

In other languages, temporal reference may be derived by other grammatical means, for example aspect or mood, as argued for Kalaallisut Inuktitut (Eskimo-Aleut, Shaer 2003; Bittner 2005), Tarramiut Inuktitut (Eskimo-Aleut, Swift 2004; Bohnemeyer and Swift 2004), and Mohawk (Iroquoian, Baker and Travis 1997). (However, for a tensed analysis of the Inuktitut dialects spoken in South Baffin and Igloolik, see Hayashi and Spreng 2005; Hayashi 2011.) Temporal reference is sometimes analyzed as being derived by pragmatic inferences, as in Navajo (Na-Dene, Smith, Perkins and Fernald 2003, 2007). Under this approach, default tense interpretations are derived by general inference procedures, relying in part on aspectual information. For example, (7) contains an imperfective-marked eventive predicate and is interpreted with present tense by default, while (8) contains a perfective-marked eventive and is interpreted as past by default. (7)  

Navajo (Smith et al. 2007: 61) Biih yish’nééh. 3-into 1sbj-ipfv-crawl ‘I’m crawling into it.’



(8)  

A sense of time and world 

 581

Navajo (Smith et al. 2007: 62) Hooghan binishishnish. hogan 3-on-1sbj-pfv-work ‘I did some work on a hogan.’

25.2.3 Optional temporal markers Recent research highlights some past markers that behave like tenses but are not obligatory, as in SENĆOŦEN (Northern Straits Salish, Turner 2011), Kwak’wala (Wakashan, Greene 2013), Tłı̨ chǫ Yatıì (Na-Dene, Welch 2015), Wá∙šiw (Bochnak 2016), and Tlingit (Cable 2017a). (9a–b) show that in Tlingit, a simple unmarked sentence can describe a present or past state, but a sentence bearing past marking (realized by the decessive suffix -een) is also interpreted as past (with a cessation inference that the state has ceased to hold at the utterance time).5 It can be difficult to tell optional tenses apart from other grammatical categories; see, for example, Davis and Matthewson (2003) and Matthewson (2006) for analysis of an apparent optional past marker as a distal demonstrative adverb in St’át’imcets. (9)

a.

   

b.

Tlingit (Cable 2017a: 656) Kuwak’éi. ipfv.weather.be.nice ‘The weather is/was nice.’ Tlingit (Cable 2017a: 637, citing Leer 1991: 464) Kuk’éiyeen. ipfv.weather.be.nice.pst ‘The weather was nice (but turned bad).’

25.2.4 Multiple tenses Some North American languages have more than one tense for past or future reference times—Creek (Muskogean, Martin 2010), Baffin Inuktitut (Eskimo-Aleut, Hayashi 2011; Hayashi and Oshima 2015), and Kwak’wala (Greene 2013) are examples. (10) illustrates the distinction between the simple past =xdi and the distant past =aɬ in contemporary Kwak’wala.6

5 A related phenomenon is the so-called ‘discontinuous past’ (Plungian and van der Auwera 2006). 6 These tense markers in Kwak’wala are optional, as discussed in Section 25.2.3.

582 

 Sihwei Chen and Lisa Matthewson

(10)

a.



    b.    

Kwak’wala (Greene 2013: 74) yi-’yak̓antam=xd=ox=da bibi-bagwanam ɬanswaɬ redup-talk-pst=2loc=det redup-man yesterday ‘The men were talking to one another yesterday.’ Kwak’wala (Greene 2013: 72) dzalxw-’id-oɬ=i=da giwas run-inch-dst.pst=3loc=det deer ‘A long time ago the deer ran.’

Interesting questions for all languages with multiple past or future tense markers include how these multiple tenses divide the labour of marking the past/future, and whether they are associated with temporal intervals that are distinct or overlapping.

25.2.5 Further issues We have seen that tenses in North American languages vary in whether they are pronounced or silent, in the precise restrictions they place on reference times, and in whether they belong to a paradigm of obligatory tenses. Variation beyond these dimensions that merits further study concerns whether tenses are absolute or relative (whether they always anchor the reference time to the speech time or possibly to some other time), or the way they establish temporal reference (i.  e., whether they refer to a time, as temporal pronouns do, or quantify over times).7 Lastly, tense inflection on nominals has been discussed for many North American languages, including Kwak’wala (Boas 1947), Halkomelem (Salishan, Burton 1997; Wiltschko 2003) and St’át’imcets (Matthewson 2005). Example (11) shows one of the effects of past marking on nominals in Halkomelem, that the referent of the noun phrase is deceased.8 (11)  

Halkomelem (Burton 1997: 73) ímex te-l sí:la-lh walk det-1sg.poss grandfather-pst ‘My late grandfather walked.’

7 Mithun (1999: 160–162) discusses North American languages that show tense relativity such as Barbareño Chumash (Chumashan). Aonuki (2021) provides detailed discussion of these fine-grained tense issues in Gitksan. 8 See also Mithun (1999: 154–156) for discussion of Yup’ik (Eskimo-Aleut), Barbareño Chumash and Wá∙šiw, and Nordlinger and Sadler (2004, citing Hockett 1958) for Potawatomi (Algonquian).



A sense of time and world 

 583

25.3 Aspect 25.3.1 Introduction to aspect Aspect describes the temporal profile of situations, and it does so via two types of information: lexical aspect and viewpoint/grammatical aspect.9 Lexical aspect refers to classes of verbs which share certain aspectual properties. For example, Mary was running entails that it is true that Mary ran, but Mary was building a house does not necessarily mean that Mary built a house; the two predicates run and build a house represent two common lexical aspectual classes, activities and accomplishments respectively. Viewpoint aspect is defined by how the event time relates to the reference time. For example, He cooked, He was cooking, and He had cooked place the time of the cooking event fully within, overlapping with, or preceding a past reference time respectively. It is in principle possible for an aspect to associate with any tense, as in, for example, He was cooking vs. He is cooking, which are past and present progressive respectively. Most North American languages have rich aspectual systems; these differ from each other not only in the inventory of aspectual categories but also in the semantic properties that individual aspects encode.

25.3.2 Lexical aspect Substantial work has identified four common lexical aspectual classes: activities (e.  g., ‘run’), accomplishments (e.  g., ‘build a house’), achievements (e.  g., ‘arrive’), and states (e.  g., ‘be angry’) (Vendler 1957; Dowty 1979; Smith 1997; a.o.). There are some cross-linguistically robust semantic features that are used to distinguish the lexical classes. For example, whether a situation is stative or eventive (cf. footnote 3) separates lexical states from the other three classes; other features include durativity (whether an event is punctual or spans over some time) and telicity (whether an event contains a specified final endpoint). Diagnosing these features, however, requires language-specific tools (e.  g., Wilhelm 2007 on Dëne Sųłiné (Na-Dene); Bar-el 2015 on Skwxwú7mesh (Squamish, Salishan)). Note that lexical aspectual classes may interact with morphological categories that are traditionally known as aspectual, as in Navajo (Smith 1996, 1997; Midgette 1996). The semantics of the same aspectual class may also vary across languages. For example, while John fixed his canoe means that John’s canoe is fixed, this is not the case with the accomplishment verb p’ayakantas ‘fix’ in Skwxwú7mesh, as in (12) (see 9 This way of viewing aspect is compositional, such that a predicate always takes both types of aspect when situated in a sentence. Aspectual information may also be classified without making the lexical and grammatical distinction; see for example Frawley (1992).

584 

 Sihwei Chen and Lisa Matthewson

also Bar-el, Davis and Matthewson 2005; Kiyota 2008; Jacobs 2011 for St’át’imcets, Skwxwú7mesh and SENĆOŦEN). (12)  

Skwxwú7mesh (Bar-el 2005: 81) na p’ayak-ant-as ta John ta snexwilh-s rl heal-tr-3erg det John det canoe-3poss i  huy-nexw-as. ptc finish-tr(lc)-3erg ‘He fixed his canoe but he didn’t finish (fixing) it.’

welh conj

haw neg

k-as irr-3cnj

Based on a range of facts including a similar absence of required culminations for accomplishment-type predicates, Greene (2013) proposes that Kwak’wala lacks an accomplishment class altogether, with typical activity predicates like ‘laugh’ forming a single class along with typical accomplishments like ‘make a canoe’. Some Salishan languages have a class of verbs that involve a stage of turning into a state, termed inchoative states (such as ‘get angry’) (Bar-el 2005; Kiyota 2008). For example, in SENĆOŦEN, t’ečəq’ means ‘get angry’ rather than ‘be angry’, since (13) is only possible in a scenario where Jack was not mad before, but became mad upon hearing the news.10 Activities in SENĆOŦEN and other Salishan languages may also be inchoative; see Bar-el (2005); Kiyota (2008). However, see Turner (2014) for an alternative analysis, which derives the effects from viewpoint aspect instead. (13)  

SENĆOŦEN (Kiyota 2008: 42) t’ečəq’ tə Jack kws kwɬ tə-nəxw-s mad det Jack sub prf hear-nctr-3sg ‘Jack got mad when he heard the news.’

tə det

sqwəl’qwəl’. news

The meaning of achievements also varies. In Northern Paiute, achievements can describe a result state, in addition to an instantaneous change of state (Toosarvandani 2014). For example, with imperfective aspect (glossed as ‘dur’), the verb mi’a ‘leave’ differs from tsibu’i ‘emerge’ in including the state that follows the leaving event (and thus it is incompatible with ‘He hasn’t left yet’): (14)  

Northern Paiute (Toosarvandani 2014: 111) Amamu’a su=naatsi’i mi’a. #Yaisi kaisu morning nom=boy leave.dur       ptc not.yet ‘This morning, the boy left. He hasn’t left yet.’

mia-maggw-ɨ-hu. leave-compl-pfv

10 Inchoative states can also be derived through affixes in SENĆOŦEN; see Kiyota (2008) and Turner (2011).



A sense of time and world 

(15)  

 585

Northern Paiute (Toosarvandani 2014: 111) Su=naatsi’i nabagia-na-ggw tsibu’i. Yaisi kaisu tsibui-maggw-ɨ-hu. nom=boy bathe-nmlz-loc emerge.dur ptc not.yet emerge-compl-pfv ‘The boy is getting out of the bathtub. He hasn’t gotten out yet.’

Some languages have aspectual classes other than the four core ones. Slave (Na-Dene), for example, has semelfactive verbs, such as Ø-gha ‘shake’ and h-we ‘cut’, which behave like activities in lacking an inherent endpoint and like achievements in being punctual (Rice 2000).

25.3.3 Viewpoint aspect 25.3.3.1 Perfective Perfectives are traditionally understood as aspects which place a situation wholly within the reference time. This captures the fact that events with final points (such as accomplishments and achievements) culminate within the reference time. For example, Mary built a house last year means that the house was finished within last year. However, perfectives in some North American languages allow accomplishments not to reach their final points. In Skwxwú7mesh and other Salishan languages, perfective accomplishments allow non-completion (see, e.  g., (5) above and the references cited there). In contrast, perfective aspect in Dëne Sųłiné is argued to always convey completion (Wilhelm 2007). In (16), a perfective activity event (‘jump’) cannot be continued.11 (16)

#      

Dëne Sųłiné (Wilhelm 2007: 50) yághelgus-ú ʔa̜łú̜ yálgus. yá#ghe-l-gus ʔú ɂa̜łú̜ yá#l-gus. th-#cm-clf-stem:jump and still th#clf-jump ‘He finished jumping and he’s still jumping.’

Another type of perfective is found in Kwak’wala; Greene (2013) analyzes the aspectual suffix -x’id as a perfective which requires only the initial transition into the event to be placed within the reference time.

11 See Axelrod (1993), Young (2000) and Rice (2000) for the completion feature of perfectives in other Na-Dene languages.

586 

 Sihwei Chen and Lisa Matthewson

25.3.3.2 Imperfective Imperfectives, at their most basic, place the reference time within the run-time of the event; for example, Mary was studying at noon places noon within Mary’s studying time. However, imperfectives in the languages of North America display a complex range of interpretations. For example, the imperfective morpheme á- in Blackfoot regularly gives rise to both in-progress and habitual readings, as in (17) (Dunham 2007, 2008). (17)

Blackfoot (Dunham 2008: 3) nit-á-o’tsisii. 1-ipfv-smoke(vai) i. ‘I smoke’ (habitual) ii. ‘I am smoking’ (in-progress)

In Skwxwú7mesh, there are at least two forms that express imperfectivity: the marker wa allows in-progress and habitual readings while the CV- reduplicant is restricted to in-progress readings (Bar-el 2005). Alabama (Muskogean) has a morphological process with an array of apparently different semantic effects, including augmentation (‘getting …-er’), attenuation, permanency of a state and continuation of an event (Hardy and Montler 1988). Hardy and Montler argue that all these uses can be unified as an imperfective which signals an absence of ‘totality’. Northern Paiute has a ‘durative gemination’ process which gives rise to typical imperfective interpretations including event-in-progress, continuous, and habitual, but also has a surprising completed reading with certain verbs (Toosarvandani 2017; see (14–15) above).

25.3.3.3 Other viewpoint aspects Perfect aspect, represented in English by have V-ed, does not appear to be a salient viewpoint aspect in North American languages (see e.  g., Dahl and Velupillai 2013). There are isolated examples of aspects which have been analyzed as perfects; see Bybee, Perkins and Pagliuca (1994) and references therein, and see Kiyota (2008) for discussion of a perfect in SENĆOŦEN. Prospective aspect, as in (18) from Plains Cree (Algonquian), places an event after some relevant reference point, either the speech time or some other time.



(18)  

A sense of time and world 

Plains Cree (Wolvengrey 2006: 402) wîpîkopayin- anima kîspin âhkami- tahkisk- -aman prosp vii (Øsg) pr-Øsg ipc ipv vti 2sg(-Ø)   break that if keep kick s.t.   ‘That’s going to break if you keep kicking it. (Please stop kicking it!)’

 587

-i cond  

North American languages make a range of other aspectual distinctions. Examples include inceptive (focusing on the beginning of an event (19)), inchoative (enforcing some initial change (20)), durative (extending an event in time), and iterative (marking repetition of an event).12 (19)

Koyukon (Na-Dene) (Axelrod 1993: 99, cited in Mithun 1999: 169) taatlkon’. te-le-ł-kon’ inceptive-pfv-clf-rain.dur ‘It started to rain.’

(20)  

Central Pomo (Pomoan) (Mithun 1999: 219) kaṭúl-aq-a•d=a to•. bony-inch-ipfv=imm 1sg.patient ‘I am losing weight (literally: becoming skinny).’

25.4 Modality 25.4.1 Introduction to modality Modals allow speakers to talk about non-actual situations. For example, (21a), which contains no modals, makes a claim about the real world. We can find out whether it is true simply by checking the facts. In contrast, the modal sentences in (21b) talk about necessities, possibilities, or desires. (21)

a. b.

The prime minister of New Zealand is a woman. The prime minister of New Zealand must/might/could/should be a woman.

Modals convey (at least) two types of information, which are often called flavour and strength. Flavour refers to the type of reasoning involved. For example, epistemic flavour has to do with the speaker’s beliefs, knowledge, or inferences, while deontic flavour has to do with rules or requirements. Strength has to do with how strong the claim is.

12 See Henderson (this volume) for iterative aspect and its relation to pluractionality.

588 

 Sihwei Chen and Lisa Matthewson

(22)



strong

weak



epistemic The P.M. must be a woman. (I have good evidence)

The P.M. could be a woman. (I have some evidence)



deontic

The P.M. could be a woman. (The rules allow it)

The P.M. must be a woman. (The rules require it)

One of the most interesting findings about modals in North American languages is that the systems are not organized in the same way as in more well-studied languages when it comes to flavour and strength. For an overview of modality in North American languages, see Huijsmans and Murray (2019).

25.4.2 Modal flavour It is very typical for modals in North American languages to be restricted to a particular flavour. An example is Gitksan. The modal imaa in (23) is epistemic, and is appropriate in contexts where the statement is based on the speaker’s evidence. The modal da’akhlxw in (24) allows a range of non-epistemic interpretations, including ability readings, as here. Peterson (2010) and Matthewson (2013) give evidence that each modal only allows these particular flavours. (23)

Gitksan (Matthewson 2013: 360) Context: Joe left the meeting looking really green in the face and sweaty. Someone asks you why he left. Yugw=imaa=hl siipxw-t. ipfv=epis.mod=cn sick-3sg.ii ‘He must have been sick.’

(24)  

Gitksan (Matthewson 2013: 371) Da’akhlxw-i-s Henry circ.possib.mod-tr-pn Henry ‘Henry is/was able to cook.’

dim prosp

jam-t. cook-3sg.ii

Modals with particular flavour are found in a range of families across the ­continent, including Salishan: St’át’imcets (Rullmann, Matthewson and Davis 2008; Davis, Matthewson and Rullmann 2009), Nsyilxcən (Okanagan, Menzies 2013), ʔayʔaǰuθəm (Comox-Sliammon, Reisinger 2018; Huijsmans 2022), SENĆOŦEN (Chen et al. 2017) and Halkomelem (Chen et al. 2017), Algonquian: Blackfoot (Reis Silva 2009; Louie 2015), Na-Dene: Tlingit (Cable 2017b; Burge 2017), Iroquoian: Mohawk (Mithun 2016), Sahaptian: Nez Perce (Deal 2011), Na-Dene: Navajo (Bogal-Albritten 2016), and the isolate Ktunaxa (Chen et al. 2017).



A sense of time and world 

 589

25.4.3 Modal strength Modals in many North American languages allow varying strengths. Consider the epistemic modal mat in Nsyilxcən (Okanagan, Salishan), which in (25) can express either a weak (25a) or strong (25b) claim (Menzies 2013): (25)  



Nsyilxcən (Menzies 2013: 24–25) mat talí ʔilx̆wt. epis.mod very hungry i. ‘She might be hungry.’ (Context: You don’t hear my stomach growling and I’m not in the room. It is supper time and we have been working for a while.) ii. ‘She must be hungry.’ (Context: We are working away and my stomach starts to growl.)

A related phenomenon is found in Nez Perce (Sahaptian). Deal (2011) shows that the non-epistemic modal o’qa in this language appears to have variable strength; for example, o’qa is used to ask for permission in (26) (a weak interpretation), but is translated by should in (27), which appears to be a strong interpretation. However, Deal argues that o’qa is better analyzed as unambiguously weak (with apparent strong readings arising due to the lack of certain pragmatic inferences which would rule them out). (26)

Nez Perce (Deal 2011: 566) Context: How a student should ask a teacher for permission. wéet-eex kiy-ó’qa ’áatinwas-x? q-1 go-circ.mod bathroom-to ‘Can I go to the bathroom?’

(27)

Nez Perce (Deal 2011: 564) Context: A discussion of how young people speak quickly, making them hard to understand. ’i’yéwki hi-pa-c’íix̂-no’qa. slowly 3sbj-sbj.pl-speak-circ.mod ‘They should speak slowly.’

Languages whose modals systematically have variable strength include most of those listed in the previous sub-section with the exceptions of Tlingit and Mohawk. Within a single language, not all modals necessarily pattern the same way with respect to whether strength is fixed or variable. Nsyilxcən, which as shown in (25) has a variable-strength modal, also has an epistemic modal which is restricted to weak strength (Menzies 2013). Blackfoot likewise displays a mix of variable- and fixed-strength modals (Louie 2015; Chen et al. 2017). Gitksan shows a systematic split in the modal system: epistemic modals allow variable strength, while non-epistemic ones do not (Peterson 2010; Matthewson 2013).

590 

 Sihwei Chen and Lisa Matthewson

In Tlingit, most modals lexically have fixed strength (Cable 2017b; Burge 2017). For instance, the dubitative particle gwál is accepted in a weak epistemic context such as (28a) but rejected in a strong context like (28b) when careful attention is paid to the certainty level; gwál is also invariably translated into English using weak modals like maybe. Tlingit also illustrates a type of modal system with a limited set of modal elements. (28)





a.

Tlingit (Cable 2017b: 629) Context: You are looking for your cat, who is hiding in one of three baskets: a red one, a yellow one, or a blue one. You haven’t checked any baskets yet, and you make a wild guess that he might be in the red one.   Gwál x’aan yáx yateeyi kákw yíkx’ awdlisín.   epis.possib.mod red ipfv.3sg.be.rel basket inside pfv.3sg.hide   ‘Maybe he hid in the basket that is red.’ b. Tlingit (Cable 2017b: 635) Context: You are looking for your cat, who is hiding in one of three baskets: a red one, a yellow one, or a blue one. You’ve checked the red one and the yellow one, and he isn’t hiding there. The only remaining possibility is that he’s hiding in the blue basket.  #  Gwál wé x’éishx’w kákw a tóot áa.   epis.possib.mod that blue basket inside.it ipfv.3sg.sit   ‘He must (might) be sitting in that blue basket.’

Finally, an extremely underspecified or vague modal is found in Wá∙šiw. Bochnak (2015a,b) argues that the modal -eʔ not only allows variable strength (like the other North American languages discussed above), it also allows variable flavour: for example, in (29), -eʔ is interpreted as a strong deontic modal but in (30), it is like a weak epistemic modal. (29)

Wá∙šiw (Bochnak 2015a: 7) Context: A friend comes to visit, and brings her dog along. You don’t want the dog to come in the house. súku baŋáya ʔéʔišgi k’éʔi. suku baŋaya ʔ-eʔ-i-š-gi k’-eʔ-i. dog outside 3-cop-ipfv-sr-rel 3-mod-ipfv ‘The dog has to stay outside.’

(30)

Wá∙šiw (Bochnak 2015a: 9) Context: You hear a knock at the door. You can’t see who it is, but can see that the person looks about the same height as Beverly. bévali k’éheligi k’éʔi bevali k’-eʔ-hel-i-gi k’-eʔ-i. Beverly 3-cop-sub-ipfv-rel 3-mod-ipfv ‘It might be Beverly.’



A sense of time and world 

 591

For typological research on modality, including in North American languages, see for example van der Auwera and Ammann (2013).

25.4.4 Interaction of modality with other categories 25.4.4.1 Modality and evidentiality Evidentiality is the grammatical encoding of a speaker’s information source.13 Examples of direct/witness, reportative, and inferential evidential marking from Cheyenne (Algonquian) are given in (31). (31)

a.



b.

Cheyenne (Murray 2016: 493–494, citing Leman 2011: 43, 51) É-hoo’koho-Ø / nėse. 3-rain-wtn / rpt.sg.inan ‘It’s raining, I witnessed / I hear.’ Cheyenne (Murray 2016: 494, citing Leman 2011: 50) Mó-hoo’kȯhó-hané-he. q+3-rain-neg-inf ‘It’s raining, I gather.’ or ‘It must be raining.’

Evidentiality interacts with modality because some evidentials can be analyzed as epistemic modals, with extra specifications about evidence. This idea means that an inferential evidential conveys something like ‘might’ or ‘must’, with the extra information that the speaker is making their modal claim based on inference. There has been extensive debate about the best way to analyze evidentials—including how many of them are modals—and North American languages have featured prominently in the debate. Murray (2010, 2014, 2016, 2017), for example, argues that Cheyenne evidentials are not modals, while Matthewson, Davis and Rullmann (2007) and Matthewson (2011, 2012) argue that St’át’imcets evidentials are modals, and Peterson (2010) argues that Gitksan possesses both modal and non-modal evidentials.

25.4.4.2 Modality and temporality The truth of a modal statement can be dependent on time. For example, He has to leave expresses a present obligation, but He had to leave expresses a past obligation. North American languages tend not to grammatically distinguish present from past modal times; this holds for many Salishan languages (St’át’imcets, SENĆOŦEN, Halkomelem (Chen et al. 2017) and ʔayʔaǰuθəm (Reisinger 2018)), as well as Ktunaxa (Chen et al.

13 See Peterson (this volume) for discussion of evidentiality; see also Huijsmans and Murray (2019).

592 

 Sihwei Chen and Lisa Matthewson

2017), Blackfoot (Reis Silva 2009; Louie 2015; Chen et al. 2017), and Gitksan (Matthewson 2013; Chen et al. 2017; Rullmann and Matthewson 2018). A Gitksan example was given in (24). An example with epistemic modality (and evidentiality) from ʔayʔaǰuθəm is given in (32). The sentence is acceptable either with present- or past-time evidence for the inference. (32)



ʔayʔaǰuθəm (Reisinger 2018: 217) č̓ə~č̓ɬ=č̓a. ipfv~rain=inf ‘It might / must be raining.’ or ‘(It sounded like) it might have been raining.’ Good in context: You wake up and hear pattering on the roof. Good in context: This morning, you heard some pattering on the roof. It sounded like it might have been raining. Later you found out that your uncle was on the roof, fixing some holes.

25.4.4.3 Modality and mood Mood refers to the grammatical marking of the status of a proposition with respect to either its function in discourse (declarative, interrogative, imperative), or (roughly) its reality status (realis vs. irrealis, or indicative vs. subjunctive) (Mithun 2016). North American languages display mood marking of both types. Kalaallisut is an example of a mood system of the first type (Bittner 2005); see also Murray (2016) on Cheyenne. In (33), the ending on the verb reflects the difference between a declarative, an interrogative and an imperative. (33)  

Kalallisut (Bittner 2005: 343) a. Sinip-pu-q. b.   sleep-ind.iv-3s     ‘He is/was asleep.’

Sinip-pa? sleep-que.3s ‘Is/was he asleep?’

c.    

Sinig-(niar)-it! sleep-(please)-imp.2s ‘(Please) go to sleep!’

An example of a realis/irrealis system is Mohawk. Mithun (2016) shows that irrealis marking is found in embedded clauses referring to unrealized events, in negated sentences, conditionals and counterfactuals, as in (34), in modal contexts and with optatives. See also Baker and Travis (1997) for analysis of irrealis in Mohawk. (34)  

Mohawk (Mithun 2016: 229) Tóka’ ki aontaki’teròn:take’ iáh ki’ nì:’i thaonkwattó:ken. toka’ ki: aa-onta-k-i’teront-ak-e’ iah ki’ if this irr-cisl-1sg.agt-be.there-cont-pfv not in.fact th-aa-wakw-at-tok-en contr-irr-1sg.pat-mid-notice-st ‘If I had been home I wouldn’t have noticed.’

n=i’i the=1



A sense of time and world 

 593

Mood interacts closely with both modality and tense. For example, irrealis mood is often analyzed as conveying modal semantics, while future marking can include, or consist entirely of, irrealis marking.

25.5 Conclusion Attention to North American languages has produced many insights about how languages can refer to situations that are displaced from the present time and/or from the actual world. In the areas of tense, aspect and modality we find great variety across the continent, but we also find common trends. We have seen that North American languages usually have a small number of (or no) overt tenses, but are often rich in viewpoint aspects. Many of the languages have lexical aspectual systems which differ from Indo-European languages in their details. And modal systems tend to be organized in terms of modal flavour, rather than modal strength. Underlying the rich variety, we also see core similarities which these languages share with unrelated ones from different continents. For example, while many North American languages appear to be tenseless, some of those apparently tenseless languages show evidence for silent tenses, which are similar in meaning to tenses in more well-studied language groups. Conversely, the study of phenomena in North American languages has in many instances shed light on properties which in other languages are not obvious on the surface. In spite of the progress that has been made, it is extremely urgent that researchers continue to devote focused attention to the subtle semantic properties of North American languages. We have as yet understood only a tiny portion of what these languages have to teach us. And the most important and interesting findings will come from targeted, hypothesis-driven fieldwork.

References Aonuki, Yurika 2021. Relative pronominal tense: Evidence from Gitksan, Japanese, and English. Vancouver, Canada: University of British Columbia MA thesis. Axelrod, Melissa 1993. The Semantics of time: Aspectual categorization in Koyukon Athabaskan. Lincoln, NB: University of Nebraska Press. Baker, Mark & Lisa Travis. 1997. Mood as verbal definiteness in a “tenseless” language. Natural Language Semantics 5. 213–269. Bar-el, Leora Anne. 2005. Aspect in Skwxwú7mesh. Vancouver, Canada: University of British Columbia dissertation. Bar-el, Leora Anne. 2015. Documenting and classifying aspectual classes across languages. In Lisa Matthewson & Ryan Bochnak (eds.), Methodologies in semantic fieldwork, 75–109. New York: Oxford University Press.

594 

 Sihwei Chen and Lisa Matthewson

Bar-el, Leora Anne, Henry Davis & Lisa Matthewson. 2005. On non-culminating accomplishments. In L. Bateman & C. Ussery (eds.), Proceedings of the thirty-fifth North Eastern Linguistics Society (NELS 35), 87–102. Amherst, MA: GLSA Publications. Bittner, Maria. 2005. Future discourse in a tenseless language. Journal of Semantics 22. 339–388. Boas, Feanz. 1947. Kwakiutl grammar. Transactions of the American Philosophical Society 37(3). 201–377. Bochnak, M. Ryan. 2015a. Underspecified modality in Washo. In Natalie Weber & Sihwei Chen (eds.), Proceedings of the eighteenth and nineteenth Workshop on Structure and Constituency in the Languages of the Americas (WSCLA 18&19), 3–17. Amherst, MA: UBCWPL. Bochnak, M. Ryan. 2015b. Variable force modality in Washo. In Thuy Bui & Deniz Özyıldız (eds.), Proceedings of the forty-fifth annual meeting of the North East Linguistic Society (NELS 45), 105–114. Amherst, MA: GLSA. Bochnak, M. Ryan. 2016. Past time reference in a language with optional tense. Linguistics and Philosophy 39(4). 247–294. Bogal-Albritten, Elizabeth. 2016. Building meaning in Navajo. Boston, MA: University of Massachusetts, Amherst dissertation. Bohnemeyer, Jürgen & Mary Swift 2004. Event realization and default aspect. Linguistics and Philosophy 27. 263–296. Burge, Heather. 2017. Prospective aspect in Tlingit. Vancouver, Canada: University of British Columbia MA thesis. Burton, Strang. 1997. Past tense on nouns as death, destruction and loss. In Kiyomi Kusumoto (ed.), Proceedings of The North East Linguistic Society 27, 65–77. University of Massachusetts, Amherst: GLSA. Bybee, Joan L., Revere Perkins & William Pagliuca. 1994. The evolution of grammar: Tense, aspect and modality in the languages of the world. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Cable, Seth. 2017a. The implicatures of optional past tense in Tlingit and the implications for “discontinuous past.” Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 35(3). 635–681. Cable, Seth. 2017b. The expression of modality in Tlingit: a paucity of grammatical devices. International Journal of American Linguistics 83. 619–678. Chen, Sihwei, Vera Hohaus, Rebecca Laturnus, Meagan Louie, Lisa Matthewson, Hotze Rullmann, Ori Simchen, Claire K. Turner & Jozina Vander Klok. 2017. Past possibility cross-linguistically: Evidence from 12 languages. In Ana Arregui, Maria-Luisa Rivero & Andres Salanova (eds.), Modality across syntactic categories, 236–287. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Dahl, Östen & Viveka Velupillai. 2013. The perfect. In Matthew S. Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.), The world atlas of language structures online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. http://wals.info/chapter/68 (accessed 12 June 2019) Davis, Henry & Lisa Matthewson. 2003. A note on remote: The temporal enclitic tu7 in St’át’imcets. In J. C. Brown & Michele Kalmar (eds.), Papers for the 38th International Conference on Salish and Neighbouring Languages (ICSNL 38), 47–64. University of British Columbia Working Papers in Linguistics. Davis, Henry & Lisa Matthewson. 2009. Issues in Salish syntax and semantics. Language and Linguistics Compass 3/4. 1097–1166. Davis, Henry, Lisa Matthewson & Hotze Rullmann. 2009. A unified modal semantics for out-of-control in St’át’imcets. In Lotte Hogeweg, Helen de Hoop & Andrej L. Malchukov (eds.), Cross-linguistic semantics of tense, aspect and modality, 206–244, John Benjamins. Deal, Amy Rose. 2011. Modals without scales. Language 87(3). 559–585. Dowty, David. 1979. Word meaning and Montague grammar: The semantics of verbs and times in generative semantics and in Montague’s PTQ. Dordrecht: Reidel. Dunham, Joel. 2007. The morpheme á- in Blackfoot: Understanding imperfectivity. In Amy Rose Deal (ed.), Proceedings of the fourth meeting on the Semantics of Under-represented Languages of the America (SULA 4) (University of Massachusetts Occasional Papers in Linguistics 35), 49–56. Missoula, MT: University of Montana Press.



A sense of time and world 

 595

Dunham, Joel. 2008. A unified analysis of the habitual and in-progress readings of á in Blackfoot. Ms., University of British Columbia. Frawley, William. 1992. Linguistic semantics. Hillsdale, NJ, Hove and London: Lawrence Erlbaum. Greene, Hannah. 2013. Verb classes in Kwak’waka. Vancouver, Canada: University of British Columbia MA thesis. Hardy, Heather K. & Timothy R. Montler. 1988. Imperfective gemination in Alabama. International Journal of American Linguistics 54. 399–415. Hayashi, Midori. 2011. The structure of multiple tenses in Inuktitut. Toronto, Canada: University of Toronto dissertation. Hayashi, Midori & Bettina Spreng. 2005. Is Inuktitut tenseless? In Claire Gurski (ed.), Proceedings of the 2005 annual meeting of the Canadian Linguistic Association (CLA 2005), 1–12. University of Ottawa: Cahiers Linguistiques d’Ottawa. Hayashi, Midori & David Y. Oshima. 2015. How multiple past tenses divide the labor: The case of South Baffin Inuktitut. Linguistics 53. 773–808. Hockett, Charles F. 1958. A course of modern linguistics. New York, NY: Macmillan. Huijsmans, Marianne. 2022. Second-position clitics, from morphosyntax to semantics: The ʔayʔaǰuθəm (Comox-Sliammon) perspective. Vancouver, Canada: University of British Columbia dissertation. Huijsmans, Marianne & Sarah Murray. 2019. Evidentials and modals. In Michael Barrie, Carrie Gillon, Jason Haugen, Eric Mathieu & Daniel Siddiqi (eds.), Handbook of North American languages, 290–309. New York and London: Routledge. Jacobs, Peter. 2011. Control in Skwxwúmesh. Vancouver, Canada: University of British Columbia dissertation. Jóhannsdóttir, Kristín M. & Lisa Matthewson. 2007. Zero-marked tense: The case of Gitxsan. In Emily Elfner & Martin Walkow (eds.), Proceedings of the thirty-seven annual meeting of the North East Linguistic Society (NELS 37), 299–310. Amherst, MA: GLSA. Kiyota, Masaru. 2008. Situation aspect and viewpoint aspect: From Salish and Japanese. Vancouver, Canada: University of British Columbia dissertation. Klein, Wolfgang. 1994. Time in language. London: Routledge. Leman, Wayne. 2011. A reference grammar of the Cheyenne language. Raleigh, N.C.: Lulu Press. Leer, Jeff. 1991. The schetic categories of the Tlingit verb. Chicago: University of Chicago dissertation. Louie, Meagan. 2015. The temporal semantics of actions and circumstance in Blackfoot. Vancouver, Canada: University of British Columbia dissertation. Martin, Jack B. 2010. How to tell a Creek story in five past tenses. International Journal of American Linguistics 76. 43–70. Matthewson, Lisa. 2005. On the absence of tense on determiners. Lingua 115(12). 697–735. Matthewson, Lisa. 2006. Temporal semantics in a superficially tenseless language. Linguistics and Philosophy 29. 673–713. Matthewson, Lisa. 2011. On apparently non-modal evidentials. In Olivier Bonami & Patricia Cabredo Hofherr (eds.), Empirical issues in syntax and semantics 8, 333–357. Colloque de Syntaxe et Sémantique de Paris. http://www.cssp.cnrs.fr/eiss8/ (accessed 15 May 2020) Matthewson, Lisa. 2012. Evidence about evidentials: Where fieldwork meets theory. In Britta Stolterfoht & Sam Featherston (eds.), Empirical approaches to linguistic theory: Studies of meaning and structure, 85–114. Berlin: De Gruyter. Matthewson, Lisa. 2013. Gitksan modals. International Journal of American Linguistics 79(3). 349–394. Matthewson, Lisa. 2017. Semantics in indigenous American languages: 1917–2017 and beyond. International Journal of American Linguistics 83(1). 141–172. Matthewson, Lisa, Henry Davis & Hotze Rullmann. 2007. Evidentials as epistemic modals: Evidence from St’át’imcets. In Johan Rooryck (ed.), Linguistic variation yearbook 7, 201–254. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company. Menzies, Stacey. 2013. Nsyilxcen modality. Vancouver, Canada: University of British Columbia MA thesis.

596 

 Sihwei Chen and Lisa Matthewson

Midgette, Sally. 1996. Lexical aspect in Navajo: The telic property. In Eloise Jelinek, Sally Midgette, Keren Rice & Leslie Saxon (eds.), Athabaskan language studies: Essays in Honor of Robert W. Young, 305–330. Albuquerque, NM: University of New Mexico Press. Mithun, Marianne. 1999. The languages of native North America. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Mithun, Marianne. 2016. Modality and mood in Iroquoian. In Jan Nuyts & Johan Van Der Auwera (eds.), The Oxford handbook of modality and mood, 223–257. Oxford University Press. Murray, Sarah E. 2010. Evidentiality and the structure of speech acts. New Brunswick, New Jersey: Rutgers University dissertation. Murray, Sarah E. 2014. Varieties of update. Semantics and Pragmatics 7(2). 1–53. Murray, Sarah E. 2016. Evidentiality and illocutionary mood in Cheyenne. International Journal of American Linguistics 82(4). 487–517. Murray, Sarah E. 2017. The semantics of evidentials. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Nordlinger, Rachel & Louisa Sadler. 2004. Nominal tense in crosslinguistic perspective. Language 80. 776–809. Peterson, Tyler. 2010. Epistemic modality and evidentiality in Gitksan at the semantics-pragmatics interface. Vancouver, Canada: University of British Columbia dissertation. Plungian, Vladimir A., and Johan van der Auwera. 2006. Towards a typology of discontinuous past marking. Sprachtypologie und Universalienforschung 59(4). 317–349. Reichenbach, Hans. 1947. Elements of symbolic logic. Berkeley: UC Press. Reis Silva, M. Amélia 2009. Circumstantial modality in Blackfoot. Paper presented at Chronos 9, the 9th International Conference on Tense, Aspect and Modality, Université Paris-Diderot, September 2–4, 2009. Reis Silva, Maria Amélia & Lisa Matthewson. 2007. An instantaneous present tense in Blackfoot. In Amy Rose Deal (ed.), Proceedings of the fourth meeting on the Semantics of Under-represented Languages in the Americas (SULA 4) (University of Montana Occasional Publications in Linguistics 35), 197–213. Missoula, MT: University of Montana Press. Reisinger, Daniel. 2018. Modality in Comox-Sliammon. In Marianne Huijsmans, Roger Lo, Daniel Reisinger & Oksana Tkachman (eds.), Papers for the 53rd International Conference on Salish and Neighbouring languages (ICSNL 53), 197–227. Vancouver, Canada: University of British Columbia Working Papers in Linguistics. Rice, Keren. 2000. Morpheme order and semantic scope: Word formation in the Athapaskan Verb. Cambridge University Press. Ritter, Elizabeth & Martina Wiltschko. 2004. The lack of tense as a syntactic category: Evidence from Blackfoot and Halkomelem. In J. C. Brown & Tyler Peterson (eds.), Papers for the 39th International Conference on Salish and Neighboring Languages (ICSNL 39), 341–370. UBCWPL 14, Vancouver, Canada: University of British Columbia. Ritter, Elizabeth & Martina Wiltschko. 2005. Anchoring events to utterances without tense. In John Alderete, Chung-hye Han & Alexei Kochetov (eds.), Proceedings of the twenty-fourth West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics (WCCFL 24), 343–351. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project. Ritter, Elizabeth & Martina Wiltschko. 2009. Varieties of INFL: TENSE, LOCATION and PERSON. In Jeroen van Craenenbroeck (ed.), Alternatives to cartography, 153–201. Berlin: de Gruyter. Ritter, Elizabeth & Martina Wiltschko. 2014. The composition of INFL: An exploration of tense, tenseless languages, and tenseless constructions. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 32. 1331–1386. Rullmann, Hotze & Lisa Matthewson. 2018. Towards a theory of modal-temporal interaction. Language 94(2). 281–331. Rullmann, Hotze, Lisa Matthewson & Henry Davis. 2008. Modals as distributive indefinites. Natural Language Semantics 16(4). 317–357. Shaer, Benjamin. 2003. Toward the tenseless analysis of a tenseless language. In Jan Anderssen, Paula Menendez-Benito & Adam Werle (eds.), Proceedings of the second meeting on the Semantics of Under-represented Languages in the Americas (SULA 2), 139–156. Amherst, MA: GLSA.



A sense of time and world 

 597

Smith, Carlota S. 1996. Aspectual categories in Navajo. International Journal of American Linguistics 62. 227–263. Smith, Carlota S., 1997. The parameter of aspect. 2nd edn. (Studies in Linguistics and Philosophy 43). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer. Smith, Carlota S. Ellavina T. Perkins & Theodore B. Fernald. 2003. Temporal interpretation in Navajo. In Jan Anderssen, Paula Menendez-Benito & Adam Werle (eds.), Proceedings of the second meeting on the Semantics of Under-represented Languages in the Americas (SULA 2), 175–192. Amherst, MA: GLSA. Smith, Carlota S., Ellavina T. Perkins & Theodore B. Fernald. 2007. Time in Navajo: Direct and indirect interpretation. International Journal of American Linguistics 73(1). 40–71. Swift, Mary D. 2004. Time in child Inuktitut: A developmental study of an Eskimo-Aleut language. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter. Toosarvandani, Maziar. 2014. Achievements in Northern Paiute. In Jérémy Pasquereau (ed.), Proceedings of the eighth meeting on the Semantics of Under-represented Languages of the Americas (SULA 8), 105–120. Amherst, MA: GLSA. Toosarvandani, Maziar. 2016. The temporal interpretation of clause chaining in Northern Paiute. Language 92(4). 850–889. Toosarvandani, Maziar. 2017. The imperfective semantics of durative gemination in Northern Paiute. International Journal of American Linguistics 83(3). 1–35. Turner, Claire K. 2011. Representing events in Saanich (Northern Straits Salish): The interaction of aspect and valence. Guildford, UK: University of Surrey dissertation. Turner, Claire K. 2014. Perfective activities in Saanich. In Hannah Greene (ed.), Proceedings of the seventh meeting on the Semantics of Under-represented Languages of the America (SULA 7), 205–220. Amherst, MA: GLSA. van der Auwera, Johan & Andreas Ammann. 2013. Overlap between situational and epistemic modal marking. In Matthew S. Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.) The World Atlas of Language Structures online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. http://wals.info/chapter/76 (accessed 16 June 2019) Vendler, Zeno. 1957. Verbs and times. The Philosophical Review 66(2). 143–160. Welch, Nicholas. 2015. Obligatory future in a Dene language. International Journal of American Linguistics 81. 41–81. Wilhelm, Andrea. 2007. Telicity and durativity: A study of aspect in Dëne Sųłiné (Chipewyan) and German. New York: Routledge. Wiltschko, Martina. 2003. On the interpretability of tense on D and its consequences for case theory. Lingua 113. 659–696. Wolvengrey, Arok. 2006. Prospective aspect in the western dialects of Cree. International Journal of American Linguistics 72(3). 397–407. Young, Robert W. 2000. The Navajo verb system: An overview. Albuquerque, NM: University of New Mexico Press.

Other readings of interest Bittner, Maria. 2011. Time and modality without tenses or modals. In Renate Musan & Monika Rathers (eds.), Tense across languages, 147–188. Tübingen: Niemeyer. Bittner, Maria. 2014. Temporality: Universals and variation. Oxford: Wiley Blackwell. Chafe, Wallace. 1995. The realis-irrealis distinction in Caddo, the Northern Iroquoian languages, and English. In Joan L. Bybee & Suzanne Fleischman (eds.), Modality in grammar and discourse, 349–366. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

598 

 Sihwei Chen and Lisa Matthewson

Condoravdi, Cleo 2002. Temporal interpretation of modals: Modals for the present and the past. In David I. Beaver, Luis D. Casillas Martinez, Brady Z. Clark & Stefan Kaufmann (eds.), The construction of meaning, 59–88. Stanford: CSLI Publications. Enç, Murvet. 1996. Tense and modality. In Shalom Lappin (ed.), The handbook of contemporary semantic theory, 345–358. London: Blackwell. Faller, Martina. 2002. Semantics and pragmatics of evidentials in Cuzco Quechua. Stanford, CA: Stanford University dissertation. Faller, Martina 2003. Propositional- and illocutionary-level evidentiality in Cuzco Quechua. In Jan Anderssen, Paula Menéndez-Benito & Adam Werle (eds.), Proceedings of the second meeting on the Semantics of Under-represented Languages of the America (SULA 2), 19–33. Amherst, MA: GLSA. Faller, Martina 2011. A possible worlds semantics for Cuzco Quechua evidentials. In Nan Li & David Lutz (eds.), Proceedings of the twentieth conference on Semantics and Linguistic Theory (SALT 20), 660–683. Ithaca: CLC Publications. Garrett, Andrew. 2001. Reduplication and infixation in Yurok: Morphology, semantics and diachrony. International Journal of American Linguistics 67. 264–312. Harley Heidi & Maria Flavez Leyva. 2009. Form and meaning in Hiaki (Yaqui) verbal reduplication. International Journal of American Linguistics 75. 233–272. James, Deborah. 1991. Preterit forms in Moose Cree as markers of tense, aspect, and modality. International Journal of American Linguistics 57. 281–297. Kroeber, Paul D. 1988. Inceptive reduplication in Comox and Interior Salishan. International Journal of American Linguistics 54. 141–167. Matthewson, Lisa. 2016. Modality. In Aloni Maria & Paul Dekker (eds.), The Cambridge handbook of formal semantics, 525–559. Cambridge University Prese. Palmer, F.R. 1986. Mood and modality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Peterson, Tyler. 2012. The role of the ordering source in Gitksan epistemic modals. In Elizabeth Bogal-Allbritten (ed), Proceedings of the sixth meeting on the Semantics of Under-represented Languages in the Americas (SULA 6), 171–192. Amherst, MA: GLSA. Peterson, Tyler. 2018. Evidentiality and epistemic modality in Gitksan. In Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald (ed.), The Oxford handbook of evidentiality, 463–489. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Portner, Paul. 2018. Mood. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Rice, Keren. 2012. Review of telicity and durativity: A study of aspect in Dëne Sųłiné (Chipewyan) and German by Andrea Wilhelm. International Journal of American Linguistics 78. 132–137. Tonhauser, Judith. 2015. Cross-linguistic temporal reference. Annual Review of Linguistics 1(1). 129–154. Wilhelm, Andrea. 2003a. The grammatization of telicity and durativity in Dëne Sųłiné (Chipewyan) and German. Calgary, Canada: University of Calgary dissertation. Wilhelm, Andrea. 2003b. Quasi-telic perfective aspect in Dëne Sułiné (Chipewyan). In Robert B. Young & Yuping Zhou (eds.), Proceedings of the thirteenth conference on Semantics and Linguistic Theory (SALT 13), 310–327. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University. Willie, MaryAnn. 1996. On the expression of modality in Navajo. In Eloise Jelinek, Sally Midgette, Keren Rice & Leslie Saxon (eds.), Athabaskan language studies: Essays in Honor of Robert W. Young, 331–347. Albuquerque, NM: University of New Mexico Press.

Elizabeth Bogal-Allbritten

26 Pragmatics

Abstract: Pragmatics is the study of the interaction between language and the contexts in which it is used. This chapter focuses on three areas of pragmatics that have been enriched by the study of Indigenous languages of North America. First, conversational implicatures, which are aspects of meaning that are suggested when a sentence is used in a particular context and which go beyond the sentence’s literal meaning. Second, the linguistic conventions that speakers use to signal politeness. Third, presuppositions, which are the background assumptions that must hold in order for certain expressions or constructions to make sense in the context. The relevance of pragmatics for language revitalization and documentation projects is also discussed.

26.1 Introduction All languages provide their users with ways to convey a multitude of meanings. However, speakers of all languages—including Indigenous languages of North America—also have the ability to convey meanings beyond what their utterances themselves express. For example, on the face of it, (1) is a question about the addressee’s physical abilities. This literal meaning is based on the individual meanings of can, open, window, etc. and the use of the question word order. But if someone says this sentence to you, it is unlikely that you will answer by saying yes, you are physically able to do so. Instead, you will understand it as a request for you to actually go and open the window. (1)  

Can you open the window? Context: It is very warm in the room. You are standing closest to the window, which is closed.

You might also have the intuition that this sentence is more polite than other, more direct alternatives to it, like (2). Both of these sentences express requests, but they do so in different ways and would be appropriate when speaking to different people or in different circumstances. (2)

Open the window!

We have subtler intuitions about appropriate language use as well. Fluent English speakers will most likely feel that there is something “off” about saying sentence (3) if we had not previously established that Anne ate something other than mutton: (3)

Anne also ate mutton.

https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-026

600 

 Elizabeth Bogal-Allbritten

In a context where this information has not been established, (4) would be more natural since it gets across the same basic information as (3) does—Anne ate mutton—without assuming that we all know that she ate something else, too. (4)

Anne ate mutton.

When you decide between (1) and (2) or between (3) and (4), you are using language in a way that pays attention to context, which is the environment in which an utterance is produced, including the physical and temporal setting, the knowledge and beliefs of the participants, the identities and relationships of the participants, and the broader linguistic context of the utterance (e.  g. is it part of a conversation, speech, or story?). Topics in language use that involve the interaction of linguistic structure and context are in the domain of pragmatics. Researchers who are interested in pragmatics ask how we interpret linguistic structures through the lens of context, and how the context can influence whether a particular linguistic structure feels natural and appropriate. This chapter looks at three topics in pragmatics and shows how Indigenous languages of North America have enriched understanding of each topic. First, we will consider conversational implicatures, which are meanings that are suggested when an utterance is used in a particular context, but which are not said explicitly. Our understanding that (1) expresses a request, rather than a question, is an example of the kind of meaning expressed through conversational implicature. Second, we will look at some of the strategies that speakers use to be polite or show respect, including the use of questions like (1) to make polite requests. Third, we will examine presuppositions, which are the requirements that certain linguistic structures place on the context. For example, (3) presupposes that Anne ate something else in addition to mutton and, as a result, does not feel natural when said in a context where this had not been established as background information. This chapter does not look at all pragmatic phenomena that have been studied in Indigenous North American languages. Readers who want to know more about pragmatics as a field might find Ariel’s (2014) short overview of interest, as well as longer textbooks by Levinson (1983), Grundy (1995), Ariel (2010), and Birner (2013). In addition, five other chapters in this volume address data relevant to pragmatics. Berge’s chapter looks at how speakers use word order and other strategies to arrange information in sentences and indicate which pieces of information are new or particularly important in the context. Sammons’ chapter looks at the linguistic strategies that speakers use when organizing information in longer conversations. Lovick’s chapter discusses speech acts, which are uses of language that bring about actions; she focuses on requests, which prompt the addressee to carry out some action, and questions, which prompt the addressee to give an answer. Finally, two chapters consider expressions relating to place (Berez-Kroeker and Holton) and time (Chen and Matthewson). The interpretation of certain expressions—known as deictic expressions— in these semantic domains depends on context; for instance, the place and time of speaking will determine which night last night refers to and which location over there picks out.

Pragmatics 

 601

26.2 Conversational implicatures The first topic in pragmatics we will discuss is conversational implicatures, which are those aspects of meaning that are not explicitly said but which the speaker intends the listener to figure out on the basis of what he or she knows about the context. The sentence Can you open the window? is a classic example of a sentence that carries a conversational implicature. As we discussed already, a listener will generally understand this sentence as a request to do something—to open the window—rather than as a question about physical abilities. However, there is nothing about the linguistic form of Can you open the window? that directly tells us to interpret it as a request rather than a question. After all, we would interpret many other sentences with the same word order and morphemes (e.  g. the verb can) as questions about abilities: (5)

Can you count to 100 in Diné Bizaad?

What is it about a question like Can you open the window? that makes it feel like a request? Context is the key. While sentence (5) talks about an ability that not everyone has, Can you open the window? is about a much more trivial ability: In most contexts, it would be strange to ask whether someone is capable of opening a window, since that is something we assume most people can manage. This oddness suggests that the speaker intended what she said to mean something else in the context. What we know about the context—the room is very warm and you are standing by the closed window—helps us to figure out what this “something else” might be. It is not surprising that people would want to cool down a hot room, and you are a likely person to help fix this problem since you are standing next to the window. Putting these pieces together, you work out the conversational implicature and you open the window. As language users, we are able to work out conversational implicatures for unfamiliar sentences used in unfamiliar contexts. Linguists and philosophers have developed a number of theories about the general strategies or principles that give us this ability, which can feel almost like mindreading. One influential theory was developed by Grice (1975, 1989). Grice proposed that language users do their best to use language in a cooperative, collaborative way and assume that the people they interact with are doing likewise. Grice suggested that cooperative speakers follow the following general principle: (6)

Cooperative principle: “Make your conversational contribution such as is required at the stage at which it occurs, by the accepted purpose or direction of the talk exchange in which you are engaged.” (Grice 1989: 26)

Grice claims that speakers who are following the cooperative principle do their best to obey the following maxims:1 1 Grice is far from the last word on conversational implicatures. Authors in the “neo-Gricean” tradition, including Horn (1984) and Levinson (2000), simplified Grice’s maxims. An influential alternative is Sperber and Wilson’s (1986) Relevance Theory, which, replaces Grice’s maxims with a principle of relevance.

602 

(7)        

 Elizabeth Bogal-Allbritten

a. Quality: Try to make your contribution one that is true and something for which you have evidence. b. Quantity: Make your contribution as informative as is required for the current purposes of the exchange, and do not give more information than is needed. c. Relation: Be relevant. d. Manner: Express yourself in a way that is clear and easy to understand. Do not use obscure, ambiguous, or longwinded language.

As we will see below, when speakers assume that the person they are talking to is trying to follow the cooperative principle and its associated maxims, this helps them to figure out what conversational implicatures are carried by an utterance. While Grice developed this theory on the basis of English and did not make claims about its applicability to other languages, other researchers have suggested that it might be used to calculate conversational implicatures in all languages (Green 1990, von Fintel and Matthewson 2008). The idea that Grice’s theory might be universally applicable is a big claim, however, and should be investigated in detail for many more languages. Fernald et al.’s (2000) detailed study of this question for one language—Diné (Navajo; Dene/Athabaskan)—shows us how Indigenous languages of North America can help us to determine just how universally applicable Grice’s theory is. In the case of Diné, the answer seems to be a positive one: Fernald et al. observe that while individuals from Diné and English speech communities have different cultural and interactional norms, Grice’s theory seems to explain how speakers from both communities figure out what meaning someone intends to convey to them. In both languages, different maxims play a greater or lesser role in determining different kinds of conversational implicatures. In the following exchange, Quantity takes center stage: (8)        

Diné (Navajo; Dene/Athabaskan) (adapt. Fernald et al., 2000: 20) a. Anne: Did you eat all the pie? b. Ben: Masdéél ałníídóó yíyą́ą́’   pie half 3obj.1sbj.eat.pfv   ‘I ate half the pie.’

Ben only explicitly says what happened to half of the pie, but Anne understands him to mean I did not eat all the pie. She comes to this conclusion by assuming that Ben is a cooperative speaker who is doing his best to obey Grice’s maxims. In order to satisfy Quantity, what Ben said must have been the most informative answer he knew to be true. If he actually ate the entire pie, he should have said so to begin with, since that would have been a more informative way to describe the situation than I ate half the pie. Because he did not say this, Anne concludes he must have meant I did not eat all the pie. While I ate half the pie may very strongly suggest the conversational implicature I did not eat all the pie, this is a suggestion only. We know this because it is possible to explicitly deny that the implicature is true, as shown below:

Pragmatics 

(9)

 603

I ate half the pie…in fact, I ate all of it.

While someone who says this is not being very cooperative—again, if they ate all the pie, they should have said so to start with—they are not contradicting themselves in the way that they would have been if they said I ate half the pie but I did not eat any pie. This is a contradiction because if you say you ate half the pie, it is necessarily true you ate some pie. There is no way around this. But if you say you ate half the pie, it is only strongly suggested that you did not eat the whole pie. Following a statement with a denial of the conversational implicature it carries is sometimes called canceling the implicature. The ability of implicatures to be canceled is a useful diagnostic for determining if you are dealing with a conversational implicature or a part of the sentence’s fundamental and logical meaning. In both Diné and English, conversational implicatures can also arise when speakers intentionally violate one of the maxims in order to send a message to the people they are talking to. Consider the following example: (10)      

Diné (Navajo; Dene/Athabaskan) Asdzáán dah ‘iistł’ǫ́ yíznoołhał woman loom 3obj.3sbj.beat.prog ‘The woman is batting away at the loom.’

(adapt. Fernald et al. 2000: 22)

As Fernald et al. observe, fluent speakers would normally use the verb atł’ó ‘S/he is weaving’ to describe someone making a rug or blanket at a loom. But the speaker here has used an unexpected verb—yíznoołhał can describe the beating of many different sorts of objects—and this violates Manner. But this violation is intentional: The speaker used this odd wording to get the listener to compute a conversational implicature. Someone who hears (10) will most likely understand that the speaker meant that the woman was not weaving well at all. While Fernald et al. (2000) is the most extensive exploration of Grice’s theory of conversational implicatures for a single Indigenous language of North America, studies of other Indigenous languages have focused on how Grice’s maxim of Quantity in particular may help us to explain why certain kinds of meanings do, or do not, arise in different languages. We will start by looking at mirative meaning. Specialized mirative markers, which indicate that information is new and surprising to the speaker, are found in a number of Indigenous languages of North America. These languages include Caddo (Caddoan) where, as originally discussed by Melnar (2004) and subsequently reported by Aikhenvald (2012), the mirative (or admirative adm, as glossed here) prefix hús- is used to express surprise: (11)      

Caddo (Caddoan) hús-ba-ʔa=sa-yi-k’awih-saʔ adm-1.ben-irr-name-know-prog ‘My goodness he knows my name!’

(Aikhenvald 2012: 449)

604 

 Elizabeth Bogal-Allbritten

Specialized mirative morphemes have also been argued to exist in Dene/Athabaskan languages including Western Apache (de Reuse 2003), Hare (deLancey 2001), and Diné (Eisman 2015). In languages that lack specialized mirative markers, however, other morphemes can be used in particular kinds of contexts to express mirative meaning. In many languages, evidentials—morphemes that indicate what kind of evidence a speaker has for making a claim—are used to express mirative meaning under special conversational circumstances. For instance, Peterson (2010) writes that in Gitksan (Gitxsan; Tsimshian) the morpheme n̓akw usually expresses indirect evidence. It has this meaning when used in contexts like the one below, where the speaker did not witness or experience firsthand the event she is describing, but instead is inferring what happened on the basis of clues: (12)          

Gitksan (Tsimshian) (Peterson 2010: 252) n̓akwhl witxwt John n̓akw=hl witxw=t John evid=cnd arrive=pnd John ‘John must be here,’ ‘Looks like John’s here.’ Context: The speaker sees John’s pickup parked outside in the driveway but he hasn’t seen John yet.

However, Peterson observes that n̓akw can also be used in contexts where the speaker has witnessed an event directly. In such contexts, n̓akw comes to express mirativity. The use of n̓akw in the following context indicates that the speaker is surprised to see John: (13)          

Gitksan (Tsimshian) n̓akwhl witxwt John n̓akw=hl witxw=t John evid=cnd arrive=pnd John ‘John’s here!’, ‘I see John’s here!’ Context: The speaker sees John standing in the doorway.

(Peterson 2010: 253)

Peterson argues that the literal meaning of n̓akw is an inferential evidential as used in (12), but that a mirative interpretation arises when the speaker intentionally violates Quantity. Peterson proposes that, on an inferential meaning for n̓akw, what the speaker says in (13) is true but not very informative: If the speaker can see John standing there, presumably everyone else can as well. This violation of Quantity leads listeners to conclude that the speaker intended the sentence to be interpreted in some other way. According to Peterson, one alternative interpretation is that the speaker is expressing her surprise at John’s unexpected appearance. This alternative interpretation—the conversational implicature carried by (13)—makes an informative contribution since other people in the context may not have known the speaker was surprised to see John. Next, we will consider how conversational implicatures can affect modal meaning, where modal marker expresses what is possible or necessary in light of evidence, rules,

Pragmatics 

 605

goals, or other relevant considerations in the context. The notion of comparing statements in terms of how informative they are is important here, just as it was in our discussion of why I ate half the pie carries the conversational implicature I did not eat all the pie. English has distinct modal morphemes to describe what is possible or allowed (e.  g. can, might) and what is necessary or required (e.  g. have to, must, should). The use of a possibility modal (e.  g. can) can give rise to the conversational implicature that the same sentence with a necessity modal (e.  g. have to) would be false: (14)  

You can take these blankets. Conversational implicature: You do not have to take these blankets.

Just like with half and all, Quantity is relevant to working out this conversational implicature. In order to satisfy Quantity, the speaker of (14) must have been making the strongest statement she knew to be true or for which she had evidence. Since she chose to say (14) instead of the more informative alternative You have to take these blankets, the listener concludes that this alternative statement is false; otherwise, the speaker should have said it to start with. Just as it was possible to cancel the conversational implicature seen earlier—I ate half the pie…in fact, I ate all the pie—it is also possible to cancel the conversational implicature that arises for (14). While (15) feels like an uncooperative and rather strange thing to say, it is not a contradiction. (15)

You can take these blankets…in fact, you have to take them.

But where English has separate modals like can and have to, Nez Perce (Sahaptian) has a single modal expression, o’qa. Deal (2011) reports that speakers use this modal both to translate prompts that describe what is possible (16) and to translate prompts that describe what is necessary (17): (16)      

Nez Perce (Sahaptian) (Deal 2011: 563) kíye kíne pa-wc’áa-yo’qa ‘íiq’o kíye pa-ckilíi-toq-o’qa we here sbj.pl-stay-mod or 1pl sbj.pl-return-back-mod Prompt: You are traveling with somebody and the two of you can’t decide whether to spend the night there or to go home. You want to say, “Well, look, we could stay here, or we could go tonight.”

(17)      

Nez Perce (Sahaptian) kíye pe-ckilíi-toq-oq’a kulaawit-ásx we sbj.pl-return-back-mod dark-before Prompt: We have to get home before it gets dark.

(Deal 2011: 563)

Deal (2011) proposes that o’qa is fundamentally a possibility modal like English can, but that sentences with o’qa do not carry the same sort of conversational implicature that we saw for can, namely that a stronger alternative statement with have to is false. Deal argues that this meaning is missing for o’qa because it lacks a counterpart on par with have to. Using Grice’s theory, we can explain the behavior of o’qa by saying that, because

606 

 Elizabeth Bogal-Allbritten

there is no stronger alternative with which to compare o’qa in Nez Perce, Quantity does not come into play and thus no conversational implicature arises.2

26.3 Politeness While Grice’s theory is a good starting place for explaining how people use language, it does not provide all of the answers we need for why speakers choose to use certain sentences. Recall Can you open the window?, the example we started the chapter with. We saw that this question will be understood as expressing a request in most contexts. However, if our only tool is Grice’s basic theory as outlined above, this meaning is a little puzzling. Because a dedicated imperative form like Open the window! would be a much clearer way to express a request, we expect this sentence to be preferred according to Grice’s maxim of Manner. Why would someone make a request in an indirect way by using a sentence with the shape of a question? A number of authors have argued that politeness is the key to this puzzle. One of these authors was Searle (1979), who suggested that indirect requests like (1) are more polite than imperatives for two reasons. First, unlike an imperative, (1) does not make an assumption about the addressee’s abilities. Second, (1) gives the addressee the option—or at least the illusion of the option—to refuse to do as requested. In Searle’s words, using (1) means that “compliance can be made to appear a free act rather than obeying a command” (1979: 48). Searle’s reasoning here closely reflects Lakoff’s (1973) pragmatic “politeness principle,” which includes the rules “Don’t impose” and “Give options,” both of which have relevance here. Later authors took up the question of how universal the notion of politeness is. Leech (1983) included a degree of flexibility in his approach by breaking politeness down into additional maxims that, he suggested, might have different levels of relative importance in different speech communities or cultures. Leech’s maxim of approbation says that speakers should try to make as few negative statements about other people as possible. This maxim could explain why a speaker of Diné might use the indirect statement in (10) rather than commenting on the weaver’s (lack of) skill in a more direct way. Leech claims that the maxim of tact—roughly, avoid imposing on other people— is the most important maxim for English-speaking communities, and could also explain why someone might use (1). By contrast, Brown and Levinson’s (1987) theory of politeness is based on a universal concept of “face” (see also Goffman 1967) and says that speakers try to speak in a way that will preserve their own “negative face” (i.  e. speak in a way that accomplishes their goals) while also preserving the addressee’s “positive face” (i.  e.

2 As Deal (2011) notes, Grice’s cooperative principle and maxim of Quantity is not the only way to explain how implicatures related to different statements’ comparative strength can arise. For an alternative theory of such implicatures, see Chierchia 2006.

Pragmatics 

 607

speak in a way that does not put the addressee in a position where he or she might look bad). On this theory, a speaker might use (1) because it achieves a good balance between these two goals, communicating the speaker’s desires while not putting the addressee in a position where she feels she must act. It is not clear exactly how universal or culturally specific the conventions related to politeness are. Wierzbicka (1985), for instance, argues that Brown and Levinson’s universal notion of face cannot capture all of the differences that are observes across communities of speakers. In this section, we will explore how requests or commands are made in a polite way in Indigenous languages of North America.3 Understanding these structures is important not only for pragmatic theory—since we can learn how languages differ in their politeness conventions—but also for language revitalization and pedagogy, since teaching these conventions to students may be a priority. Different Indigenous languages use different strategies to issue commands or make requests that are less direct and more polite. In Southern Pomo (Pomoan), the conditional construction can be used to form polite commands. (18)        

Southern Pomo (Pomoan) ʔay:ak̓oʔwenṭ̓ oʔma ʔay:á=ɛo=ʔwen=ṭ̓ o=ʔma 1pl=com=?=contrast=2sg.agt ‘You ought to lie with us.’

mi:ṭi:ba mi:ṭi-:ba lie-cond

(Walker 2013: 316)

Presumably, (18) is felt to be polite for the same kinds of reasons that were relevant to (1): Both structures give the addressee the option to refuse or ignore the request in a way that would not be possible with a true imperative. A similar line of reasoning might also explain the use of demonstratives in place of second-person pronouns in polite requests in Central Alaskan Yupik: (19)        

Central Alaskan Yupik (Eskimo–Aleut) Mak-ten ≠ am’ u-suuq! get_up-opt.2sg.hurry this-voc.sg ‘You, now get up.’ Literally: ‘This one, now get up.’

(Miyaoka 2012: 359)

Since the second-person pronoun is not used, the addressee could choose to interpret (19) as directed at someone else, once again giving her the option to ignore or refuse doing what has been requested of her. The avoidance of second-person pronouns is a relatively common strategy to indicate politeness crosslingustically (Helmbrecht 2013). Speakers of Arapaho (Algonquian) can choose between several structures to make commands or express requests with varying degrees of indirectness and politeness. As

3 See Lovick’s chapter (this volume) for more discussion of polite alternatives to imperatives, including the use of questions to make requests or give commands.

608 

 Elizabeth Bogal-Allbritten

Cowell (2007) discusses, a speaker’s choice of structure is shaped by Arapaho beliefs concerning relationships and age, gender, and ceremonial roles, and a cultural preference to maintain harmony in the community. Cowell illustrates this with an example of language used during a sweat lodge ceremony, which involved a leader, several assistants to the leader, and several other participants. When another individual entered and found that there was no place for him to sit, Cowell reports that the leader used the following indirect imperative structure: (20)      

Arapaho (Algonquian) ceenokú-hee sit(ai)-3.imper.indir ‘Let/Have him sit down.’

(Cowell 2007: 50)

However, there were other structures the leader might have used instead, at least in theory: (21)      

Arapaho (Algonquian) ceenóku sit_down(ai) ‘Sit down!’

(Cowell 2007: 50)

(22)      

Arapaho (Algonquian) ceenokun-ún sit_someone_down(ta)-3.imper ‘Sit him down.’

(Cowell 2007: 50)

Cowell suggests that neither of these constructions would have been appropriate in the context. While the intransitive imperative in (21) is, in a sense, the most efficient since it only involves two individuals—the ceremony leader and the newly arrived man— it would be inappropriate since it incorrectly implies that the individual who had just entered was reluctant to be seated and just needed to be told to do so by the ceremony leader. However, the transitive imperative (22) would also be inappropriate in this context since the assistant—the addressee of the command—was younger and the individual who entered—the person who would be acted upon—was a respected older man. Cowell reports that because of this age difference, Arapaho speakers felt that (22) would suggest that the older man lacked the agency to act on his own and that the younger man was somehow in a position to make him act. Given the importance of age to social relationships and order in Arapaho society, putting the young assistant in this position “would have placed [him] in a socially untenable position, and would have put the necessary ceremony even further in jeopardy” (Cowell 2007: 51). By contrast, the indirect imperative in (20) avoids this violation of cultural norms by asking the young assistant to “act ‘on the world’ rather than on the arrivee to bring about the desired result” (Cowell 2007: 50). We have seen how the major theories of politeness give us ways to explain why indirect requests or commands will typically be perceived as more polite than direct

Pragmatics 

 609

ones. However, two Algonquian languages demonstrate that this is not universally true, or, perhaps, that different language communities have different conceptions of what structures are most direct. Cowell (2007) notes for Arapaho that verbs marked for future tense can also be used to make requests, and that these requests are not perceived as rude. This is surprising from the perspective of English, where giving an order with a future-tensed verb would likely be perceived as quite rude, or only suitable for giving orders to children. A similar observation also holds in Ojibwe (Algonquian), where future-marked forms like the following are perceived as polite requests: (23)      

Ojibwe (Algonquian) (Rhodes 1988: 167)1 Giin idash, Amik, abwiin giga-babaa-biigwandaanan ‘And, Beaver, why don’t you go around and chew up the paddles.’ Literally: ‘And you, Beaver, will go around and chew up the paddles.’

It is not the case, Rhodes notes, that Ojibwe speakers lack ways to make indirect requests, but rather that direct speech like (23) can also be used to convey politeness. Rhodes (1988) and Cowell (2007) give different explanations for why Ojibwe and Arapaho speakers convey politeness with direct forms. Rhodes suggests that in Ojibwe communities, cooperation is assumed and, as a result, speakers do not feel it is necessary to use structures explicitly designed to preserve their addressee’s positive face, to use Brown and Levinson’s (1987) terminology. Instead, speakers use constructions that emphasize negative face that are designed to accomplish the speaker’s goals in a more efficient manner. For Arapaho, Cowell suggests that future-marked verbs are perceived as polite since they make the utterance feel less like a command and more like “a recognition of the strong authority of the person, who cannot be commanded, or prevented from acting” (2007: 57). Regardless of the explanation, though, both languages demonstrate that what may be perceived as very direct and impolite in one language (such as English) can have quite a different feeling in other languages.

26.4 Presuppositions The final topic in pragmatics that we will discuss is rather different from the previous two, but still involves the interaction of linguistic structure and context. An important part of the context is the set of background beliefs, knowledge, and assumptions that are shared by all of the participants in a conversation; these beliefs, knowledge, and assumptions are collectively called the common ground (Stalnaker 1973, 2002). Some expressions and constructions introduce (or “trigger”) presuppositions, which means they are only natural and appropriate if the common ground contains particular infor-

1 This example was not glossed in the original.

610 

 Elizabeth Bogal-Allbritten

mation. Menominee (Algonquian) has certain presupposition triggers, including mesek ‘also’, that will be familiar to speakers of English and many other languages: (24)      

Menominee (Algonquian) Ohpa͞ enyak mesek nep̝ ōnāwak potatoes also 1sg_sbj.3obj.put_in_pot ‘I also put potatoes in the kettle.’

(Johnson et al. 2015: 164) ahka͞ ehkoh kettle.loc

Johnson et al. (2015) write that this sentence was used in a context where other items put into the kettle had already been named. We expect that if (24) were said in a context where it was clear nothing else had been put in the kettle, the listener would be confused, and it would have felt odd to use this sentence in that context. If the intended presupposition is not, in fact, part of the common ground, the result is referred to as presupposition failure. In English, if a conversational participant feels that there has been presupposition failure, then she can question or object in certain ways. One way of objecting is through the use of a phrase like Hey, wait a minute! I didn’t know that… (von Fintel 2001). For instance, we can imagine someone replying to the English translation of (24) in the following way if the common ground did not support the intended presupposition. (25)

Hey, wait a minute! I didn’t know that you had put anything in the kettle yet!

Hey, wait a minute… is an odd way to respond to information that is not presupposed. For instance, (26) would be an odd way to reply to (24), since the addition of potatoes is new information contributed by that sentence, rather than information that was presupposed to already exist in the common ground: (26)

Hey, wait a minute! I didn’t know that you put potatoes in!

However, Indigenous languages of the Pacific Northwest show that the Hey, wait a minute… response to presupposition failure is not a universal one. Matthewson (2006) reports for St’át’imcets (Lillooet; Salishan) that consultants did not spontaneously object to the inappropriate use of presupposition triggers using a phrase along those lines. The following example shows how speakers responded to múta7 ‘again’ when it was used in a context where the presupposition (Henry won the lottery before) was not part of the common ground: (27)            

St’át’imcets (Lillooet; Salishan) (Matthewson 2006: (9)) A: t’cum múta7 k  Henry l-ta lottery-ha   win(intr) again det Henry in-det lottery-det   ‘Henry won the lottery again.’ B: o, áma   oh good Context: Interlocutors [conversational participants—EBA] all know that Henry is not a millionaire.

Pragmatics 

 611

Matthewson (2006) considers several possible explanations for why St’át’imcets speakers might not object to presupposition triggers in the same way that English speakers do. One possible explanation has to do with cultural differences. According to Matthewson, St’át’imc culture values listening over directly challenging or questioning what you do not understand; perhaps this preference makes St’át’imcets speakers reluctant to challenge presuppositions. However, Matthewson notes several reasons why this is not the most plausible explanation. First, while there is a cultural prohibition against younger people challenging or questioning elders, there is no prohibition against elders challenging younger people, like herself. Second, the elders Matthewson was working with were otherwise comfortable with challenging and correcting errors in the context of linguistic elicitation sessions. For instance, Matthewson writes that St’át’imcets speakers did not hesitate to challenge or question other kinds of linguistic issues—such as unclear reference of nouns—even while they appeared to ignore presupposition failure. This is illustrated below, where t’it ‘also’ is expected to trigger the presupposition that someone else is in the hospital, too: (28)            

St’át’imcets (Lillooet; Salishan) A: wá7 t’it ta n-snúk’w7-a   be also det 1sg.poss-friend-det   ‘My friend is also in the hospital.’ B: swat ku núk’wa7-sa   who det friend-2sg.poss   ‘Who is your friend?’

l-ta in-det

(Matthewson 2006: (16)) qwenúcw-alhcw-a sick-place-det

Another possible explanation is that, unlike English also and again, St’át’imcets t’it and múta7 are not presupposition triggers at all. However, this does not seem quite right either: While St’át’imcets speakers never spontaneously challenged presuppositions with a phrase like Hey, wait a minute… they were willing to question or deny the presupposition when explicitly asked to comment on the appropriateness of the relevant expressions. Matthewson (2006) argues that both English and St’át’imcets have presupposition triggers, but that these triggers encode different requirements on the context in the two languages. In English, presuppositions impose restrictions on the common ground. As a result, a presupposition in English fails if the relevant background information is not known by all participants in the conversation. In St’át’imcets, by contrast, presuppositions only reflect the speaker’s own view of the context. We predict (27) and (28) to be fine as long as the speaker is aware of the relevant background information. This shows us that presupposition triggers may interact with context in different ways in different languages. However, this is not the only possible explanation for differences in the properties of potential presupposition triggers in English and Indigenous languages of the Pacific Northwest. Littell (2016) discusses how cleft constructions are responded to in Kwak’wala (Wakashan). In English, cleft constructions like (29) are said to presuppose

612 

 Elizabeth Bogal-Allbritten

exhaustivity. This means that they are only appropriate in contexts where the so-called “pivot” of the cleft (e.  g. you) is an exhaustive, or complete, list of everyone or everything in the context that can be described by the second half of the construction (e.  g. got a goat). (29)  

It’s you who got a goat. Presupposition: No one else got a goat.

Because of this presupposition in English, it does not feel natural to follow the cleft construction with a continuation that mentions some other person who got a goat. However, Littell (2016) demonstrates that speakers of Kwak’wala can do just that: (33)                

Kwak’wala (Wakashan) (adapt. Littell 2016: 130–131) suʔəm loɬ x̌a məlx̌ƛu, loɬʔəmx̌əʔən x̌a su=ʔm loƛ x̌a məlx̌ƛu loƛ=ʔm=x̌a=n x̌a be.2.ver get acc mountain.goat get.ver.add_foc.1 acc məlxƛu məlx̌ƛu mountain.goat ‘It’s you who got a goat, and I got a goat, too.’ Context: We were passing around a box of animal crackers, each person taking one. Two people found a goat (an elicitor and a speaker); other people found other animals. The elicitor asks the speaker who got a goat.

Similar observations have been made for clefts in other languages of the Pacific Northwest, including three Salishan languages: Northern Straits Salish (Davis et al. 2004; Matthewson 2006), Nɬeʔkepmxcin (Thompson River Salish; Koch 2008), and Okanagan Salish (Lyon 2013). Littell (2016) suggests that clefts in these languages may not, in fact, presuppose exhaustivity at all. Instead, exhaustivity is merely suggested, perhaps through conversational implicatures, like those discussed in section 2.

26.5 The importance of pragmatics for ­documentation and revitalization To this point, our discussion has focused on how North American languages have enriched linguists’ understanding of how pragmatics works. But the study of pragmatic phenomena can also have value for individuals and communities whose primary focus is on language teaching and revitalization. The acquisition of pragmatic intuitions is a key part of becoming a speaker who can communicate in a natural and appropriate way. However, pragmatic intuitions can be challenging to acquire in a typical classroom environment since pragmatics is so dependent on context. A language learner will collect very different kinds of pragmatic “data” depending on if she hears the language

Pragmatics 

 613

being used to tell a funny story, to politely ask someone to pass a plate of food, to speak respectfully to an elder, or to make conversation with a stranger. In most classroom environments, it will be hard for language learners to become familiar with how the language should be used in all of these contexts; as a result, language learners may have an incomplete understanding of the language’s pragmatic conventions. In response, some communities have developed teaching methods designed to foster pragmatic competence by exposing language learners to language as it is used in practice. For instance, in the Onkwawén:na Kentyókhwa immersion program for adult learners of Kanyen’kéha (Mohawk; Iroquoian), students encounter the language being used in many different contexts, including in films and different kinds of written texts, such as legends, non-fiction narratives, and comedic stories. According to Green and Maracle (2018), students in these programs learn not only the syntax of Kanyen’kéha, but also its pragmatic conventions. Language-learning programs based on Hinton’s (2001) Master–Apprentice (or Mentor–Apprentice) model seek to develop language learners’ pragmatic competence by putting them in situations where they need to use the language to talk about and carry out concrete tasks, which are contexts where it is crucial for them to understand a speaker’s intended meaning. In the same spirit, a handbook developed for Tlingit (Athabaskan-Eyak-Tlingit) language communities mentions the importance of the “communicative/task-based approach” to language learning. On this approach, language learners “are not taught about the language, but instead are taught to do things in the language. For example, instead of learning weather vocabulary, a learner learns how to have a conversation about the weather” (Sealaska Heritage 2017: 15–16). Pragmatics can be a valuable area to document from the perspective of mitigating the effects of language contact. While some pragmatic conventions may be the same in many different languages, conventions that are closely tied to cultural or social norms may show more variation between different speech communities. Pragmatic conventions are not immune from change in situations of language contact. This may be particularly true for politeness conventions, like those discussed in section 4. Crago et al. (1993) interviewed mothers of young children in two Inuit communities where Inuktitut (Eskimo–Aleut) was under pressure from English. Even though words like nakurmik ‘thank you’ and ilali ‘you’re welcome’ exist in modern Inuktitut, older Inuit community members would not traditionally use these terms to signal politeness to each other. However, the researchers found that younger Inuit mothers were encouraging their children to use please and thank you, as well as their Inuktitut equivalents. If a language community decides that change in pragmatic conventions is not desirable, then understanding and documenting these conventions can give the community ideas for pedagogical materials that encourage the maintenance of these conventions.

614 

 Elizabeth Bogal-Allbritten

26.6 Conclusions In this chapter, we have looked at how Indigenous languages of North America have enriched the understanding of three topics in pragmatics, namely conversational implicatures, politeness conventions, and presuppositions. In the realm of conversational implicatures, Indigenous languages suggest that the general principles originally proposed for English may be valid across many more languages. For politeness, we have seen that, due to the interaction of cultural and social norms with politeness conventions, different kinds of structures may be perceived as polite or impolite by different language communities. Finally, the study of presupposition in Indigenous languages suggests that theories developed on the basis of English alone may be too restrictive to account for how speakers of other languages use and react to presupposition triggers. We have also seen that issues in pragmatics are not only important for linguists. The acquisition of pragmatic competence is an important step for language learners, but it can be difficult to accomplish since it requires observing the language being used across many contexts and by many different speakers. Language revitalization and education programs that emphasize practical situations and language in context allow new speakers to acquire the pragmatic rules they need to use language in an appropriate way. Acknowledgements: I thank the editors and two anonymous reviewers for their detailed feedback, which greatly improved the paper. Any remaining errors are my own.

References Aikhenvald, Alexandra Y. 2012. The essence of mirativity. Linguistic Typology 16. 435–485. Ariel, Mira. 2010. Defining Pragmatics. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Ariel, Mira. 2014. Pragmatics: Inference for language. In Carol Genetti (ed.), How Languages Work: An Introduction to Language and Linguistics, 180–199. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Birner, Betty J. 2013. Introduction to Pragmatics. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell. Brown, Penelope & Stephen C. Levinson. 1987. Politeness: Some Universals in Language Usage. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Chierchia, Gennaro. 2006. Broaden your views: Implicatures of domain widening and the ‘logicality’ of language. Linguistic Inquiry 37. 535–590. Cowell, Andrew. 2007. Arapaho imperatives: Indirectness, politeness and communal ‘face’. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 17(1). 44–60. Crago, Martha B., Betsy Annahatak & Lizzie Ningiuruvik. 1993. Changing patterns of language socialization in Inuit homes. Anthropology and Education Quarterly 23(3). 205–223. Davis, Henry, Lisa Matthewson & Scott Shank. 2004. Clefts vs. nominal predicates in two Salish languages. In Donna Gerdts & Lisa Matthewson (eds.), Studies in Salish Linguistics in Honor of M. Dale Kinkade. Missoula, MT: University of Montana Press. Deal, Amy Rose. 2011. Modals without scales. Language 87(3). 559–585. deLancey, Scott. 2001. The mirative and evidentiality. Journal of Pragmatics 33. 369–382.

Pragmatics 

 615

de Reuse, Willem. J. 2003. Evidentiality in Western Apache (Athabaskan). In Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald & Robert M. W. Dixon (eds.), Studies in Evidentiality, 79–100. (Typological Studies in Language 54). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Eisman (Palakurthy), Kayla. 2015. Marking the Unexpected: Evidence from Navajo to Support a Metadiscourse Domain. Master’s thesis, University of California, Santa Barbara. Fernald, Theodore B., Lorene Legah, Alyse Neuendorf, Ellavina Tsosie Perkins & Paul Platero. 2000. Implicature and presupposition in Navajo. In Theodore B. Fernald & Ken L. Hale (eds)., Working Papers on Endangered and Less Familiar Languages, volume 3, 17–29. Cambridge, MA: MITWPL. Fintel, Kai von. 2001. Would you believe it? The king of France is back! Presuppositions and truth value intuitions. In Marga Reimer & Anne Bezuidenhout (eds.), Descriptions and Beyond, 315–341. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Fintel, Kai von & Lisa Matthewson. 2008. Universals in semantics. The Linguistic Review 25. 139–201. Goffman, Erving. 1967. On face-work. Interaction Ritual: Essays on Face-to-Face Behavior, 5–45. New York: Pantheon Books. Green, Jeremy & Owennatékha Brian Maracle. 2018. The root-word method for building proficient secondlanguage speakers of polysynthetic languages: Onkwawén:na Kentyókhwa Adult Mohawk Language Immersion Program. In Leanne Hinton, Leena Huss & Gerald Roche (eds.), The Routledge Handbook of Language Revitalization, 146–155. New York: Routledge. Grice, Herbert Paul. 1975. Logic and conversation. In Peter Cole & Jerry L. Morgan (eds.), Syntax and Semantics 3: Speech Acts, 41–58. New York: Academic Press. Grice, Herbert Paul. 1989. Studies in the Ways of Words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Grundy, Peter. 1995. Doing Pragmatics. New York: Hodder Education. Helmbrecht, Johannes. 2013. Politeness distinctions in pronouns. In Matthew S. Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig. Available online: http://wals.info/chapter/45 Hinton, Leanne. 2001. The master–apprentice language learning program. In Leanne Hinton & Ken Hale (eds.), The Green Book of Language Revitalization in Practice, 217–226. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Horn, Laurence R. 1984. Towards a new taxonomy for pragmatic inference: Q-based and R-based implicature. In Deborah Schiffrin (ed.), Meaning, Form and Use in Context, 11–42. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press. Johnson, Meredith, Monica Macaulay, Bryan Rosen & Rachel Wang. 2015. A survey of Menominee word order. In Monica Macaulay & J. Randolph Valentine (eds.), Papers of the 43rd Algonquian Conference, 154–178. Albany, NY: SUNY Press. Koch, Karsten. 2008. Intonation and Focus in Nɬeʔkepmxcin (Thompson River Salish). Doctoral dissertation, University of British Columbia. Lakoff, Robin. 1973. Language and woman’s place. Language in Society 2(1). 45–79. Leech, Geoffrey. 1983. Principles of Pragmatics. Longman: Longman. Levinson, Stephen C. 1983. Pragmatics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Levinson, Stephen C. 2000. Presumptive Meanings: The Theory of Generalized Conversational Implicature. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Littell, Patrick. 2016. Focus, Predication, and Polarity in Kwak’wala, Doctoral dissertation, University of British Columbia. Lyon, John. 2013. Predication and Equation in Okanagan Salish: The Syntax and Semantics of Determiner Phrases. Doctoral dissertation, University of British Columbia. Matthewson, Lisa. 2006. Presuppositions and cross-linguistic variation. In Christopher Davis, Amy Rose Deal & Youri Zabbal (eds.), Proceedings of the Thirty-sixth Annual Meeting of the North East Linguistic Society, 63–76. Amherst, MA: GLSA. Melnar, Lynette R. 2004. Caddo Verb Morphology. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.

616 

 Elizabeth Bogal-Allbritten

Peterson, Tyler Roy Gösta. 2010. Epistemic Modality and Evidentiality in Gitksan at the Semantics–Pragmatics Interface. Doctoral dissertation, University of British Columbia. Rhodes, Richard A. 1988. Ojibwa politeness and social structure. In William Cowan (ed.), Papers of the Nineteenth Algonquian Conference, 165–174. Ottawa: Carleton University Press. Rullmann, Hotze, Lisa Matthewson & Henry Davis. 2008. Modals as distributive indefinites. Natural Language Semantics 16. 317–357. Sealaska Heritage. 2017. Tlingit Language Mentor–Apprentice Handbook: Bridging Challenges to Fluency through Partnerships. Available online: http://www.sealaskaheritage.org/sites/default/files/tlingitment orapprenticehandbook%20final.pdf Searle, John R. 1979. Expression and Meaning: Studies in the Theory of Speech Acts. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Stalnaker, Robert. 1973. Presuppositions. Journal of Philosophical Logic 2. 447–457. Stalnaker, Robert. 2002. Common ground. Linguistics and Philosophy 25. 701–721. Sperber, Dan, & Deirdre Wilson. 1986. Relevance: Communication and Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Wierzbicka, Anna. 1985. Different cultures, different languages, different speech acts: Polish vs. English. Journal of Pragmatics 9(2). 145–178. Young, Robert. W. & William Morgan. 1987. The Navajo Language. Albuquerque, NM: University of New Mexico Press.

VI Languages over space and time

Marianne Mithun

27 Languages as dynamic systems: How grammar can emerge Abstract: The value of traditional languages as repositories of culture is well recognized: they embody the codification of patterns of thought passed from generation to generation. Many grammatical distinctions recur in languages around the globe, but others are richly developed only in some. The differences are no accident. They emerge out of what speakers choose to say the most often in everyday speech. Frequently-recurring turns of phrase can become routinized; words within them may lose their individual salience, erode in form, and gain more abstract meanings. Differences in vocabulary are also no accident. Speakers create terms for concepts they consider nameworthy. Languages indigenous to North America provide excellent examples of both processes. Understanding them brings home the fact that languages are not simply interchangeable: each represents a history, stretching over millennia, of what speakers have chosen to say the most often.

27.1 Routinization, erosion of form, loss of lexical independence, extension of meaning A great many grammatical markers such as conjunctions, prepositions, prefixes, suffixes, etc., begin life as full words. Usually their origins can no longer be discerned, but on occasion, they still survive in the language. A well-known example described in Bybee & Pagliuca 1987 is the English future tense. We know from historical records that English will was originally a verb signaling intention, a meaning that persists in some of its uses today. Over time, there was a gradual extension in meaning: if someone willed something to happen, it was likely to happen in the future. Specification of future tense is highly frequent in English, so the form will has been eroding; it is now often reduced to the enclitic =ll, a reduced form merging with the previous word, as in she’ll. The development of grammatical markers can take thousands of years, and the oldest historical records of languages indigenous to North America go back only several centuries at best. But many clues to processes of development can still be seen within the modern languages.

27.1.1 From verbs to postpositions to suffixes Hopi, a member of the Uto-Aztecan family spoken in northeastern Arizona, contains a set of postpositions, much like English prepositions, that can orient a referent in space. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-027

620 

 Marianne Mithun

(Postpositions are like prepositions, except that they follow their objects.) The Hopi postpositions add such meanings as ‘at’, ‘along’, ‘to’, ‘from’, and ‘through or along’. (1)          

Hopi postpositions with the noun ‘field’ (Hopi Dictionary Project 1998: 874) paasat ep ‘at/in a point on the field’ paasat angqw ‘from the field’ paasat aw ‘to the field’ paasat ang ‘along an area of the field’ paasat ánawit ‘along the course of the field’

The postpositions originated in verbs, some of which still persist in the language. A verb angqw ‘come from’, for example, is the source of the postposition angqw ‘from’. (2)      

Hopi verb angqw ‘come from’ (Hopi Dictionary Project 1998: 25) Nuy tuuving-tota, hìntiqw oovi nu’ angqw-niqw. 1sg.acc ask-distr why so 1sg.nom come.from-subord ‘They asked me why I came from there.’

The meaning of the verb itself has been extended metaphorically to mean ‘recover, revive’. The meaning of the postposition has also been extended metaphorically, from indicating space to time, a common kind of development. (3)      

Hopi postposition angqw ‘from’ (Hopi Dictionary Project 1998: 25) Nalöstala-t angqw nu’ na’sastiva. fourth.day-acc from 1sg begin.to.prepare ‘From the fourth day on I started preparing (for the wedding)’.

It has also been extended to designate origins and sources. (4)      

Hopi verb angqw ‘come from’ (Hopi Dictionary Project 1998: 25) Kopatsoki pay wunavutsi-t angqw yukiw-ta-ngwu tablita piece.of.board-acc from made-caus-hab ‘A tablita (headdress) is made from a piece of board.’

The Hopi postpositions still show other traces of their verbal origins. Verbs can contain prefixes identifying their objects: inùu-pungyala ‘keep wanting to hang around with me’, ùu-pungyala ‘keep wanting to hang around with you (sg)’, àa-pungyala ‘keep wanting to hang around with him or her’, etc. (Hopi Dictionary Project 1998: 881). Like their verbal sources, postpositions begin with a pronominal object prefix: inu-ngaqw ‘from me’, uu-ngaqw ‘from you’, aa-ngaqw ‘from him or her’. (Hopi Dictionary Project 1998: 5). The postposition angqw actually contains two parts: a-ngqw it-from, that is, ‘from it’. Some other postpositions are angk ‘after, behind, following’, aqlaq ‘near, by’, angq ‘from’, atpiq ‘under’, atsva ‘above’, atsveq ‘on top of’, and amuutsave ‘between’ (Malotki 1978: 113). They form a paradigmatic set, all occurring in the same constructions. They also have what are termed ‘extreme’ forms, distinguishing more distant referents as in paasat angqaqw ‘from the field distantly perceived’.



Languages as dynamic systems: How grammar can emerge 

 621

But all development did not stop with postpositions. Some of the postpositions have fused with the noun before them as suffixes, losing their independence as separate words; the complex words then eroded in substance. (5)          

Hopi suffixes with paasa ‘field’ (Hopi Dictionary Project 1998: 874) pas-ve ‘at/in a point on the field’ pas-ngaqw ‘from the field’ pas-mi ‘to the field’ pas-va ‘along an area of the field’ pas-aanawit ‘along the course of the field’

Like their postposition sources, the suffixes also have ‘extreme’ forms: pasngahaqaqw ‘from the field distantly perceived’. The key role of frequency of use in shaping grammar is illustrated by similar developments in Lakota, a Siouan language of the Northern Plains. Pustet (2000, 2008) lists 45 Lakota postpositions and traces the sources of 15 of them to verbs still in the language. Among them are iyúweɣa ‘to cross’ > iyuwex ‘across’; kaxlóka ‘to pierce’, kaxlóg ‘through’; and kičhiča ‘to be with’, kičhí ‘with’ (comitative). Many of the postpositions have developed more diverse and abstract meanings and have eroded in form. The verb ognáka ‘to put in’, for example, has given rise to the postposition ogná ‘in, inside, among, according to’. An important point is that individual words do not develop into grammatical markers in isolation. As Pustet points out, the Lakota postpositional constructions apparently developed out of frequently-occurring sequences of two verbs: serial verb constructions. A construction like that in (6) apparently originated in a sequence of verbs like ognáka ‘put in’, just seen above, followed by mani ‘walk’. (6)      

Lakota postpositional construction (Pustet 2000: 176) Lená oyáte ki ogná ma-wá-ni. dem.pl people def among walk-1sg.agt-walk ‘It is among the people I am walking’ = ‘I am walking among these people.’

In Lakota, the main participants in an event are identified by markers on the verb: ma-wá-ni ‘I am walking’, ma-ya-ni ‘you are walking’. As in many other languages, however, third persons ‘he’, ‘she’, ‘him’, ‘her’, and ‘it’ are not marked: máni ‘(he or she) is walking’. The verb ognáka can thus mean ‘(he or she) put (it) into (it). Though this word developed more general meanings and was reduced in form as it evolved into the postposition ogná ‘in, inside, among, according to’, it still appears with pronominal prefixes, like wičhi- ‘them’ in (7). (7)      

Lakota postpositional construction (Pustet 2000: 176) Wičhíy-ogna ma-wá-ni. 3pl.obj-among walk-1sg.agt-walk ‘It is among them I am walking’ > ‘I am walking among them.’

622 

 Marianne Mithun

The other postpositions similarly appear with pronominal prefixes. In general these match the prefixes on verbs, with just a few small differences. The overwhelming majority of Lakota postpositions have now developed still further and also occur as suffixes. (8)            

Lakota postposition and suffix (Pustet 2008: 275) a. Mní(-ki) mahél yąká-pi.   water(-def) in sit-pl   ‘They are sitting in the water.’ b. Mni-máhel yąká-pi   water-in sit-pl   ‘They are sitting in the water.’

While the postpositions bear stress of their own, the suffixes do not, though they may bear stress as the second syllable of a word. The suffixes also cannot co-occur with the definite article kiŋ or its fast-speech form ki. An important aspect of developments of this kind is the role of frequency of use: the markers that have developed from postpositions into suffixes are the most frequent ones. But it is not their frequency in isolation: it is the frequency of the collocations they occur in. This can be seen in the modern language. The most frequent noun – postposition combinations have developed into noun-suffix combinations. As Pustet notes, all but 8 of the markers occur as suffixes to the noun thí ‘house’, for example. And the frequency of a particular collocation can affect the shapes of the markers. The suffix -mahel ‘in’ is sometimes shortened to -ma, but only in the most frequent phrase it appears in, thi-má ‘house-in’ = ‘inside the house’. The role of the larger speech context can also be seen in Navajo, a language of the Dené (Athabaskan-Eyak-Tlingit) family, spoken over a wide area of Arizona, New Mexico, and Colorado. Navajo has a large inventory of postpositions, which contain pronominal prefixes like those in Hopi and Lakota. The postpositions, with prefixes, can appear on their own or following a noun, as in (9) ‘in the hogan’. (9)          

Navajo postposition (Dolly Hermes Soulé, speaker p.c.) Ááji éí hooghan yii’ nánijiah. áá=ji éí hooghan y-ii’ ná-nii-jah there=toward that hooghan 3-in repeatedly-down-multiple.recline. iterative over there they hogan in it they customarily sleep ‘They slept over there in the hogan.’

Some of the postpositions have developed further into affixes, but in a different way from those in Hopi and Lakota. They have become attached to a following verb as prefixes.



(10)          

Languages as dynamic systems: How grammar can emerge 

 623

Navajo complex verb (Dolly Hermes Soule, speaker p.c.) Nihijish biihdahoɬnííɬ. nihi-jish b-iih-da-hw-oh-ɬ-nííɬ 2pl.poss-suitcase 3-into-distr-indef-2pl.sbj-tr-propel.several.objects.mom. ipfv your suitcase you all put them into them ‘Put them into your suitcases.’

27.1.2 From nouns to affixes via compounding As can be seen in the last examples, verbs in Navajo and other Dené languages can be elaborate. They consist of a final verb stem (now a fused combination of a verb root and aspect/mood marker), plus various prefixes indicating transitivity, subject, object, and more. The verb structure developed gradually, piece by piece, over time, as prefixes were added one by one to the beginning of verbs (Mithun 2011, 2017). Those prefixes closest to the stem, apparently the first to become attached and thus the oldest, have quite abstract meanings, roughly indicating transitivity, like ɬ- in (10). They are substantially reduced in form, just a consonant, voicing of an adjacent sound, or nothing at all. Immediately before these prefixes are first and second person subject pronominal prefixes, like oh- ‘you all’ in (10). Pronouns ‘I’ and ‘you’ are of course highly frequent in speech: people tend to talk most about themselves and those they are conversing with. Prefixes that can appear immediately before these pronouns are generally still quite reduced in form, often a single consonant with the vowel i, like ni-, si-, or yi-, and their meanings can be difficult to pinpoint. But prefixes furthest from the stem, the most recently added, show somewhat less reduced forms: a wider range of consonants followed by any vowel, long or short, oral or nasal, with low or high tone, and sometimes more than one syllable. Their meanings can also be more concrete and specific, like k’é- ‘friendly relations, good terms’. (11)        

Navajo prefix (Young & Morgan 1987: 50) K’énáhásdlį́ į’ k’é-ná-há-s-d-lį́ į́ ’ good.terms-reversionary-generic-pfv-intr-come.into.being.mom.pfv ‘Peace was restored, peace returned, war ended.’

These more concrete prefixes apparently followed a slightly different pathway of development than those that developed from postpositions seen in the previous section. At an earlier time there was productive noun incorporation, the compounding of a noun stem with a verb stem to form a new verb stem (N + V > V). This process is still productive in related languages. The Navajo prefix k’é-, related to the noun -k’ei ‘kinsman, relative’, apparently developed along this pathway. The lexical sources of some other prefixes still exist in the language.

624 

(12)                        

 Marianne Mithun

Some Navajo verb prefixes (Young & Morgan 1987: 42–57) Prefix Lexical source ’a’ą́‘into a hole or burrow’ ’a’áán ‘burrow’ ’ana‘war, warfare’ ’anaa’ ‘war’ dá’ak’ę-‘into a field’ dá’ák’eh ‘field’   k’a‘wound’ k’aa’ ‘arrow, missile, bullet’ -lák’ee‘hand’ ′-lák’eh ‘hand, hand area’ ɬe‘into dirt or ashes’ -ɬeezh ‘dirt’ ta(h)‘body’ -tah ‘body’ t’a‘wing’ t’a’ ‘wing’ yá-/yé‘into the air’ yá ‘sky’ wó’ą‘over an edge’ wó’ąą ‘over beyond an edge,       over the crest of a hill’

Differences in the sequencing of these processes, the fusion of markers on the one hand and the abstraction of meaning and erosion of substance on the other, can result in different kinds of morphological structures early on (Mithun 1998). Affixes that developed from stems in compounds, like these Navajo prefixes, can have more concrete, specific, lexical-like meanings at early stages. Those that became more abstract and eroded while still separate words, like the Hopi and Lakota postpositions, can have more grammatical meanings early on.

27.2 Special elaboration Some grammatical distinctions recur in languages around the world. Many languages have past tense suffixes on verbs, for example, and many have plural suffixes on nouns, though certainly not all do. But some languages show distinctions that are less common cross-linguistically, and these are not accidental. They reflect what speakers of these languages have chosen to express particularly frequently over long periods of time. Some special distinctions found in languages indigenous to North America are described here.

27.2.1 Salishan suffixes Languages of the Salishan family, which extends from Northern British Columbia south into Oregon, and from the Pacific Coast east into Montana, show rich inventories of what have been termed lexical suffixes, suffixes with often relatively concrete, noun-like meanings, such as ‘head’, ‘mouth’, ‘nose’, ‘throat’, ‘shoulder’, ‘hand’, ‘baby’, ‘firewood’, ‘container’, ‘land’, ‘food’, ‘meat’, ‘clothing’, ‘shirt’, ‘hat’, ‘building’, ‘friend’, ‘ground floor’, and more. The inventories vary from language to language, but some languages contain



Languages as dynamic systems: How grammar can emerge 

 625

over a hundred. Examples here are drawn from Klallam, described in fine detail in Montler 2012. Klallam is a Central Coast Salish language spoken along the north coast of the Olympic Peninsula of what is now Washington State, and on the southern tip of Vancouver Island in what is now British Columbia (Montler 2012: vii). Montler isolated 98 lexical suffixes in the language, which vary in productivity; only about 50 are used regularly. They occur both in words used to refer and words used to predicate. (13)        

Klallam suffix -əqsən ‘nose’ (Montler 2012: 431) sxʷɬiɬq’ʷáyəqsən sxʷ-ɬy-ɬq’ʷ-ay-aqsən for-pl-hollow-extenser-nose ‘nostrils’

(14)        

Klallam suffix -əqsən ‘nose’ (Montler 2012: 93) c’əŋ’əqst c’əŋ’-əqsən-t bite-nose-transitive ‘bite someone or something on the nose’

They are clearly suffixes: they cannot serve as the basis for a word, but must always follow a root or larger stem. Though they appear to have the kinds of concrete meanings typical of roots, most have a range of meanings, some more abstract. The suffix -əqsən ‘nose’, for example, is also used to evoke various pointed things, including a point of land. (15)        

Klallam suffix -əqsən ‘nose, point’ (Montler 2012: 131) čšə́qsən čəsəʔ-əqsən two-nose ‘two-pronged fish spear used for flounder and crab.’

(16)        

Klallam suffix -əqsən ‘nose, point’ (Montler 2012: 334) q’tə́qsən q’t-əqsən around-nose ‘go around a point’

This kind of root-suffix construction is quite old, occurring in all 23 languages of the family. It is apparently descended from compounds in which the first member was either a noun or verb stem, and the second member a noun root. Over time, roots that occurred the most frequently as the second members of such compounds took on more diffuse and abstract meanings and eroded in substance. Most of these source noun roots no longer occur as independent nouns on their own, but traces of some can still be discerned, either within the same language or in a related one. A Klallam example is the root cúcən ‘mouth’ and the suffix -ucən.

626 

 Marianne Mithun

(17)        

Klallam root cúcən ‘mouth’ (Montler 2012: 83) k’ʷc’ə́təŋ ce n’cúcən. k’ʷc’-t-ŋ ce n’-cucən be.made.crooked-transitive-pass det 2sg.poss-mouth ‘Your mouth gets twisted [when seeing a ghost].’

(18)                

Klallam suffix -ucən ‘mouth’ (Montler 2012: 811–812) Complex stem   Initial root ʔəmxʷ-úcən ‘harvest fruit’ ʔəmxʷčaʔčaʔy-úcən ‘talk too much’ čaʔ.rdp-yq’t-úcən ‘follow the beach’ q’tkʷən-úcən ‘sing spirit song’ kʷənƛ’əm’-úcən ‘say right’ ƛ’ən’maʔkʷ-úcən ‘kissing’ məʔkʷmək’ʷ-úcən’ ‘eating leftovers’ mk’ʷ-

  ‘harvest’ ‘work-dim ‘around’ ‘enrapture’ ‘bump’ ‘lump.impf ‘claim’

The semantic contribution of the suffix -ucən is not always obvious. It seems to invoke ideas of food and eating, speaking and singing, and even kissing. It even shows a metaphorical extension to ‘edge’. Montler notes that ‘the two meanings ‘mouth’ and ‘edge’ are probably related in the view of the body as a container with the mouth as its rim/ edge’ (2012: 818). It also shows evidence of the kind of phonological erosion that often occurs with highly frequent forms over time. The ‘mouth’ suffix appears as -ucən/-əcen/-cən/-uc/-əc. (19)        

Klallam suffix -əcən ‘mouth, edge’ (Montler 2012: 60) ʔiyəcən ʔiy-ucən beside-mouth ‘to be at the edge’

(20)        

Klallam suffix -cən ‘mouth, edge’ (Montler 2012: 224) ɬp’céʔnə’ ɬəp-ucin-ŋ slip.off-mouth-middle-3pl ‘We two will go check them’

       

aitarcitaciarluku. aitar-cir-taciar-lu-ku gaping.open-caus-test-subordinative-r>3sg ‘to see if there’s an opening (between ice floes).’

634 

 Marianne Mithun

       

Manaryugyaqua manar-yug-yaaqe-u-nga fish.with.hook-want-in.vain-intr.ind-1sg ‘I want to go fishing but’

       

qenumun atraryuumiitua qenu-mun atrar-yuumir-ite-u-a slush.ice-trm.sg descend-yearn-not-intr.ind-1sg ‘I don’t want to go down there to the ice.’

taugaam, taugaam however  

The languages have enormous inventories of suffixes, numbering in the hundreds. Many have meanings similar to suffixes in other languages, like the future -ciiqe- above. But many have surprisingly lexical-like meanings. (39)        

Yup’ik verb (Elena Charles, speaker p.c.) Akikiungeqatartua. aki-kiur-nge-qatar-tu-nga money-prepare.something.to.be-begin-fut-intr.ind-1sg ‘I am going to start making objects for money.’

The rich dictionary in Jacobson 2012 provides a wealth of examples. Many of the suffixes have verb-like meanings, like -carte- ‘to hit in/on the’, as in yaqu-cart-aa ‘s/he winghit it’ = ‘S/he hit it in the wing’, -ci- ‘to buy’, -(u)saar- ‘to speak the language of’, and -ninarqe- ‘to smell or taste like’. Others have noun-like meanings, like -iq ‘animal that dwells in’, as in qunguq ‘grave’, qungu-iq ‘arctic fox, small bug found on drying fish or fur’. Many function like adjectives or adverbs in other languages, like -kuaq ‘leftover’, as in arucetaaq ‘dried fish skin’, arucetaar-kuaq ‘uneaten, leftover dried fish skin’, and -qtarar(ar)- ‘slowly and with difficulty because of disability’, as in qaner- ‘speak’, qaneqtara’ar-tuq ‘s/he is speaking slowly and haltingly (e.  g. because of a stroke)’. Some of the suffixes are added to noun bases, some to verb bases, and some to both. Some create new noun bases, some create new verb bases, and some create both. The term for ‘car’, for example, consists of a noun root nuna ‘land’, followed by a suffix -kuar- ‘go by way of’ forming a new verb base ‘go by way of land’, followed in turn by a suffix -cuun ‘device for’ forming a new noun base ‘device for going by way of land’. (40)        

Yup’ik noun (George Charles, speaker p.c.) nunakuarcuutka nuna-kuar-cuun-ka land-go.by.way.of-device.for-1sg>3sg ‘my car’

Such structures are pervasive throughout the family and can be reconstructed for their common ancestor, so they are thousands of years old. Because of the lexical-like meanings of the suffixes, it appears that they developed along lines similar to those in the Salishan and Wakashan languages, from the second members of compounds. A closer look



Languages as dynamic systems: How grammar can emerge 

 635

at their distribution shows that many have just begun to undergo the kinds of meaning changes typical of the development of grammatical markers over time. The suffix -cur/-ssur-, for example, attached to noun bases, can add the meaning ‘hunt’. But it actually has a wider range of meanings. (41)              

Yup’ik suffix -cur-/-ssur- ‘to hunt, seek, check’ (Jacobson 2012: 757) tuntu ‘caribou’ tuntussurtuq ‘s/he is hunting caribou’ kayangu ‘egg’ kayangussurtuq ‘s/he is gathering eggs’ neqa ‘fish’ neqsurtuq ‘s/he is fishing by gillnet’ kuvya ‘fishnet’ kuvyassurtuq ‘s/he is checking the fishnet’ aki ‘money’ akissurtuq ‘s/he is prospecting’ kumakaq ‘birch tree fungus’ kumakarcurtuq ‘s/he is harvesting birch tree       fungus (as chewing tobacco)’

The suffix -tur- can be translated ‘eat’, but also ‘wear’, ‘use’, and more (Jacobson 2012: 888). The suffix -te- can be translated ‘catch’ after a noun for a game animal, but also ‘obtain’, after some other nouns, and ‘go to’ after a noun for a place, and ‘spend’ or ‘for N to pass’ after a noun for a time (Jacobson 2012: 877). The inventories of suffixes in languages of this family, like those of others, reflect what speakers have said most often over the course of their development. Some reflect the environment, like the array of those for ‘hunt’, and -ir- ‘have cold’, which is added to noun stems for body parts. (42)        

Yup’ik suffix -ir- ‘to have cold’ (Jacobson 2012: 767) it’gairtua it’ga-ir-tu-a foot-have.cold-intr.ind-1sg ‘I am foot-cold’ = ‘my feet are cold’

Another suffix -ir-, added to a noun for a game-capturing device, adds the meaning ‘set’, as in negaq ‘snare’, negirtuq ‘s/he is setting a snare’ (Jacobson 2012: 766). Not all of grammar emerges out of the physical environment. Yup’ik is rich in suffixes indicating endearment, which are added to both noun and verb bases and can be pervasive in speech. They do not necessarily indicate feelings toward the specific noun or verb base they are attached to, but rather a person who is part of the scene. The suffix -turluq- in (43) was conveying affection toward the speaker’s relative, not the money. (43)              

Yup’ik endearment suffix (Elena Charles, speaker p.c.) Nayagam-ll’ akiuturlui nayagaq-m=llu aki-un-turluq-i younger.sister-gen=too money-supply.of-poor.dear-3sg>3pl atuqenka. atur-ke-nka use-tr.participial-1sg>3pl ‘I continue to use dear Nayagaq’s money.’

tua-i, tuai then

636 

 Marianne Mithun

Attention to interpersonal relations is even reflected in the wealth of causative suffixes, whose meanings are carefully differentiated by Jacobson. There is a general causative -vkar-/-cete ‘to let, allow, cause, or compel one to’, but also -car- ‘to try to induce to’, -cetaar- ‘to try to cause one to’, -cir- ‘to wait for object to’, -ciar- ‘to wait patiently for object to’, -narqe- ‘to tend to cause one to’, -naite- ‘to tend not to cause one to’, -narcar- ‘to try to cause one to or induce one to’, -rqe- ‘to intentionally or deliberately cause one to (often repeatedly)’, and -te- ‘to act on one so as to cause it to do the action unproductively’. One example is in (44). (44)        

Yup’ik causative (Elizabeth Ali, speaker p.c.) Ilaliunarqut. ila-liur-narqe-u-t associate-work.or.play.with-tend.to.cause.one.to-intr.ind-3pl ‘They are friendly.’

As noted, root-suffix combinations can come to be processed as single units, whose meanings may not correspond precisely to their components. The root ila- in (44) is variously translatable as ‘part of, one of, some of, relative, family member, associate’. The suffix -liur- is translatable as ‘be occupied with, work or play with, cook’. But together they form a derived verb base ilaliur- ‘be sociable’, one that speakers recognize as part of the language. Frequent sequences of suffixes can also come to be processed as single units with recognizable meanings. The causative suffix -narqe- ‘tend to cause one to’ in (44) developed from a combination of -nar- ‘cause’, no longer productive on its own, and -rqe- ‘time after time’.

27.3 Continuing grammatical development Once a word has become an affix, it may continue to undergo semantic and formal development, becoming even more abstract in meaning and/or eroding in form. Among the Yup’ik derivational suffixes with verb-like meaning is -u- ‘be’, which is added to noun bases. (45)        

Yup’ik suffix -u- ‘be’ Asveruuq. asver-u-u-q walrus-be-intr.ind-3sg ‘It is a walrus.’

As Jacobson points out (2012: 889), this suffix triggers complex phonological alternations. (Preceding voiced velar continuants are dropped if they occur between single vowels of which at least the first is full, final e is dropped from preceding bases, and a velar nasal ng is added after bases ending in a vowel.) As noted earlier, all Yup’ik verbs



Languages as dynamic systems: How grammar can emerge 

 637

contain an inflectional ending consisting of a mood marker and pronominal suffix. The intransitive indicative mood suffix -u- can be seen in many examples above. This mood suffix shows exactly the same idiosyncratic phonological behavior as the suffix -u- ‘be’. Though this derivational suffix -u- ‘be’ was already in place within the verb morphology, it continued to evolve into the inflectional indicative suffix, used to mark basic statements, which is now part of an obligatory paradigm. Other examples can be seen in the Iroquoian family of eastern North America. The family consists of two main branches, Southern Iroquoian, represented only by Cherokee, and Northern Iroquoian, made up of all the other languages. All Iroquoian languages contain a cislocative prefix t(a)-, ‘here, hither, this direction’. Because it appears in essentially the same forms with this meaning in all of the languages, it can be reconstructed for their common ancestor, Proto-Iroquoian. (Though voicing is not distinctive, it is distinguished in the Cherokee community orthographies: the sound written t in Tuscarora and Mohawk here is the same as that written d in Cherokee.) (46)    

Cherokee cislocative (Pulte & Feeling 1975: 251) Aʔi. ‘He is walking.’ Da-yaʔi. ‘He is walking (in the direction of the speaker)’

(47)    

Tuscarora cislocative (Elton Greene, speaker p.c.) wehrú:ʔnyę:. ‘He has thrown it (over there).’ t-hrú:ʔnyę:. ‘He has thrown it (here).’

(48)    

Mohawk cislocative (Annette Kaia’titáhkhe’ Jacobs, speaker p.c.) Ia’sá:ti! ‘Throw it (over there)!’ Ta-sá:ti! ‘Throw it here!’

In just one language, Cherokee, the meaning of the prefix has been extended from the realm of space to that of time: it now also serves as a future tense marker. Metaphorical extension from space to time is of course not uncommon cross-linguistically. It is a kind of abstraction in meaning, and, as here, can be accompanied by a shift from derivation to inflection. (49)    

Cherokee future (Pulte & Feeling 1975: 249) Gawoniha. ‘He is speaking.’ Da-gawonisi. ‘He will speak.’

A similar development can be seen in the Northern languages, where a translocative prefix ‘away, thither’ has developed a use as a past tense marker (Mithun 2017).

638 

 Marianne Mithun

27.4 The speech community The frequency of patterns of expression, which in turn shapes grammatical systems, can be affected by a variety of factors. One that has been important in shaping languages indigenous to North America is multilingualism. Language contact can, particularly over a long period of time, shape grammatical structures, categories, and distinctions, whether or not there is replicated vocabulary. Hints of such influence can be seen in the Salishan and Wakashan languages discussed in Sections 2.1 and 2.2. There is no evidence that the Salishan and Wakashan families are related, but they are spoken in neighboring areas on the Northwest Coast. Over time, parallel morphological structures have developed, with large inventories of lexical suffixes. Some other examples of the role of language contact in shaping grammatical systems are pronominal distinctions and evidentiality.

27.4.1 Inclusive and exclusive first persons A number of languages indigenous to North America distinguish inclusive and exclusive first persons: inclusive forms include the listeners (‘you and I’), and exclusive forms exclude them (‘he/she/they and I’). In Wikchamni Yokuts, there are no third person pronouns, but singulars, duals, and plurals are distinguished in all cases, as are inclusive and exclusive first persons. (50)    

Wikchamni Yokuts subject pronouns (Gamble 1978: 101) naʔ ‘I’ 1sg   maʔ ‘you’ 2sg  

     

mak’ naʔak’ maʔak’

‘you and I’ ‘he or he and I’ ‘you two’

1+2.du 1+3.du 2du

inclusive dual exclusive dual  

     

may’ naʔan maʔak

‘you all and I’ ‘they and I’ ‘you all’

1+2.pl 1+3.pl 2pl

inclusive plural exclusive plural  

In other languages inclusive and exclusive first persons are distinguished only in some forms. In Santee (Siouan), as in other Siouan languages, there is a special form only for inclusive duals, ‘you and I’. The speaker and hearer are categorized as a single unit, not marked as plural. All other groups consisting of two or more are marked as plurals with the suffix -pi.



Languages as dynamic systems: How grammar can emerge 

 639

(51)        

Santee (Dakota) agent pronominal prefixes (Martha St. John, speaker p.c.) iȟpe-wa-ye ‘I threw it away it away’ 1sg iȟpé-ya-ye ‘you threw it away’ 2sg iȟpé-uŋ-ya ‘you and I threw it away’ 1+2.du iȟpé-ye ‘s/he threw it away’ 3sg

     

iȟpé-uŋ-ya-pi iȟpé-ya-ya pi iȟpé-ya-pi

(52)        

Santee (Dakota) patient pronominal prefixes (Martha St. John, speaker p.c.) bduǧó ‘I’m tired’ 1sg duǧó ‘you’re tired’ 2sg ųŋ-yúǧo ‘you and I are tired’ 1+2du yuǧó ‘s/he is tired’ 3sg

     

uŋ-yúǧo-pi d-uǧó-pi yuǧó-pi

‘we threw it away’ ‘you all threw it away’ ‘they threw it away’

‘we’re tired’ ‘you all are tired’ ‘they’re tired’

1+2.pl, 1pl 2dp 3dp

1+3du, 1pl 2dp 3dp

(As noted earlier for Lakota, third persons are unmarked. Stem-initial merges with the preceding first and second person patient prefixes. Forms are listed here in the community orthography used for the Lakota dialect, with representing nasalization on the preceding vowel, the voiced velar fricative [x], and the voiced velar fricative [ɣ].) The inclusive/exclusive distinction is not limited to North America, but it is pervasive across the continent, appearing in Algonquian, Iroquoian, Siouan, Caddoan, Yokutsan, Wintuan, Palaihnihan, and Chinookan languages, and in Yuchi, Washo, Chimariko, Coos, Siuslaw, Alsea, and others. In many cases it appears in just some languages of a family. Within the Muskogean family it appears only in Hitchiti, Mikasuki, and Seminole; within Kiowa-Tanoan it appears only in Kiowa; within Uto-Aztecan it appears only in languages of the Numic branch and the neighboring Tübatulabal; within Utian it appears only in the Miwok branch; within Yuki-Wappo it appears only in Yuki; within Sahaptian it appears only in Sahaptin; within Salishan it appears only in one dialect of Shuswap; and within Wakashan it appears only in the Northern branch. As Jacobsen (1980) points out, it is pervasive in certain geographic areas, occurring in all languages of the Great Basin culture area (Nevada and adjacent California, Oregon, Idaho, Utah, Wyoming, Colorado, and New Mexico), and in languages contiguous with them. The Great Basin languages include those of the Numic branch of Uto-Aztecan (Northern Paiute, Southern Paiute, Kawaiisu, Ute, Shoshone) and the genealogically unrelated Washo. Immediately west of the Numic languages and Washo, in the Sierras and San Joaquin Valley of California, are the unrelated Palaihnihan languages Achumawi and Atsugewi, the Miwok and Yokutsan languages, and the non-Numic Uto-Aztecan language Tübatulabal. Related languages beyond these areas lack the distinction.

640 

 Marianne Mithun

It appears in another cluster of unrelated languages on the Oregon coast, Coos, Siuslaw, and Alsea, then again further north along the Columbia River in the Chinookan family and the unrelated but contiguous Sahaptin, which in turn borders the Numic language Northern Paiute. Finally, still further north, the distinction appears in the Northern Wakashan languages (including Kʷak’ʷala) but not Southern Wakashan, and in one dialect of the Salishan language Shuswap but not other Salishan languages. As noted by Jacobsen, Kʷak’ʷala was once spoken in adjacent territories, before the intrusion of the Chilcotin (Dené). The distribution suggests that at least in some languages, the development of the distinction may have been stimulated by contact. Bilingual speakers accustomed to distinguishing inclusive from exclusive first persons in one of their languages might seek to replicate it in the other, exploiting means already present in this second one. In most cases, since forms themselves were not usually transferred, and since philological records of North American languages go back no more than a few centuries at best, evidence of the role of contact can only be circumstantial. But some cases point strongly in that direction. As noted, the Kiowa language is the only member of its family, Kiowa-Tanoan, to distinguish inclusive first persons. The recent history of the Kiowa provides a likely explanation for its appearance. Speakers of all of the other languages of the family have been in the Southwest for a long time, but the Kiowa now reside in the southern Plains. Watkins recounts what is known of their location since 1700. Mooney (1898) places the Kiowas near the head of the Missouri River around 1700 and traces their migration east to the Black Hills of South Dakota, during which time they established friendly ties with the Crow, and then gradually south to the Platte and beyond to the area between Arkansas and Red Rivers. Before 1700, however, we have no record of the Kiowas. Today, they live primarily in southwestern Oklahoma, and especially in Caddo, Kiowa, and Comanche counties (Watkins 1984:1)

The Kiowa would have thus moved through territories now occupied by speakers of Siouan, Algonquian, and Caddoan languages. They are now surrounded by speakers of Arapaho (Algonquian) on the northwest, Pawnee (Caddoan) on the north, Kansa (Siouan) on the northeast, Osage (Siouan) on the east, Wichita (Caddoan) on the south, and Comanche (Uto-Aztecan) on the west. All of these languages distinguish first person inclusives. The Kiowa inclusive distinction appears innovative. The material within the language that speakers exploited to express it is still clear. According to Watkins, ‘First person non-singular inclusive is distinguished from exclusive for agents only and is indicated by the second person plural prefixes’ (1984: 113).



Languages as dynamic systems: How grammar can emerge 

 641

27.4.2 Evidentiality A number of North American languages have evidential markers, which indicate the source and sometimes, by extension, the degree of certainty on the part of the speaker of the information expressed. Some rich systems are found in languages of the Pomoan family, indigenous to Northern California. They have been described for Kashaya Pomo (Oswalt 1961), Southern Pomo (Walker 2020), Central Pomo (Mithun 2020), Northern Pomo (O’Connor 1992), Eastern Pomo (McLendon 2003), and Southeastern Pomo (Moshinsky 1974). Examples here are drawn from Central Pomo. Central Pomo contains two tightly attached evidential suffixes, -ya for direct observation and -nme for auditory evidence, and two loosely attached suffixes, -la for performatives (those actions the speaker is certain of because he or she is performing them himself or herself) and -wiya personal effect (those situations the speaker is certain of because he or she was affected personally). There are three second-position sentential clitics: an inferential =ʔka, a hearsay/reportative and quotative =ʔdó(ma), and a factual =ʔma, which indicates that the sentence states a generally-known or certain fact. Various other particles can add evidential connotations as well. (53)      

Central Pomo direct observation (Winifred Leal, speaker p.c.) Mé:n=ʔti q’dí ʔóm-ya mu:l. so=but good appear-dir.observation 3sg ‘But he looked good.’

(54)      

Central Pomo performative (Frances Jack, speaker p.c.) Mt̯ o maqó-w-la! 2sg.pat find-pfv-performative ‘I found you!’

(55)      

Central Pomo personal affect (Frances Jack, speaker p.c.) Maʔá=čo mdál-wiya. food=for die.pfv-personal.affect ‘I’m starving.’

(56)          

Central Pomo inferential (Eileen Oropeza, speaker p.c.) Mé:n ʔi-n ʔ=ka mu:l yal tí:-kʰet̯ ’ cá ʔ=mi: so be-ipfv cop=infer 3sg.agt 1pl.pat 3r.sg-poss house cop=there hlá:n-ka-w díy-an. multiple.go-caus-pfv order-ipfv.sg ‘That must be why he was telling us to come to his house.’

(57)      

Central Pomo hearsay (Winifred Leal, speaker p.c.) Sarah kiy ʔṭʰál ʔdoma ʔ=mu:l. too sick hearsay cop=3 ‘Sarah is sick too, they say.’

642 

(58)      

 Marianne Mithun

Central Pomo factual (Frances Jack, speaker p.c.) Bó:-qʰa-: ʔiʔ=ma báya:-k’u: qʰa-: west=water-loc cop=fac man-child water-loc ‘A boy drowned on the Coast.’

s-ná:m sucking-drown

ʔe. cop

These evidential markers are old, many cognate across the Pomoan languages, dating to the time of their common ancestral language; their diachronic sources can no longer be discerned. California is a strong linguistic area; numerous structural parallelisms have developed across family lines. The region around the Pomoan homeland is a particularly strong subarea. There are long histories of intermarriage across languages family lines, and multilingualism. The closest neighbors are the Wappo to the south, the Lake Miwok (Utian) to the southeast, the Wintuan to the east, and the Yuki to the north. Wappo and Yuki may be remotely related in a group termed Yukian. The other languages are all considered genealogically distinct. None of the neighbors have elaborate evidential systems comparable to those of the Pomoans. Lake Miwok has just a hearsay particle weno ‘it is said’ (Callaghan 1963: 251). But it has no cognates in related languages outside the area. Wappo similarly has just a hearsay particle kʰon’, used in legends, which does not match any marker in its only possible relative Yuki. Radin (1929: 168) suggests that the particle may be descended from a verb *ka ‘talk’. Yuki has four evidentials: two glossed as hearsay markers and two as inferential markers (Balodis 2016: 270–278, 362). The origins of three of the markers are still clear. The Yuki hearsay clitic =ʔi/=ʔi:/=ʔiy/=ʔey, apparently developed from a verb ʔi:m-/ʔe:m- ‘say’. The inferential clitic =hąli ‘it seems’ apparently developed from a verb hąl- ‘hear’. The inferential šiloʔ, midway between a particle and clitic, still has the same form as a verb šiloʔ ‘appear to, seem to, be the same’. These evidentials thus appear to be relatively recent developments. Evidentials in the Wintuan languages are also relatively young. They are described in Schlichter 1986, Pitkin 1984, Whistler 1986, and Lawyer 2015. The Wintuan family consists of Northern and Southern branches, each comprising multiple dialects. The Northern language Wintu contains four evidential suffixes. One, -ntʰEr, is termed a nonvisual sensorial by Schlichter. [It is] used if the speaker wishes to indicate that the statement he is making describes a fact known to him through one of his senses other than vision, i.  e. his hearing, feeling, taste, smell, touch, or any kind of intellectual experience of a sixth sense. It is used when talking about the supernatural (Lee 1941), and for prophecy; for predicting future events, which are somehow felt as being imminent. (Schlichter 1986: 47)

Schlichter carefully traces its source to a contraction of a verb mut- ‘hear, feel, sense, perceive’ and a passive suffix -hEr-, so ‘it is sensed’. A second Wintu evidential is a hearsay suffix -ke:, which Schlichter links to a form *keCV ‘maybe, must be’, proposing that it changed its meaning to hearsay when it occurred in past time contexts. A third



Languages as dynamic systems: How grammar can emerge 

 643

Wintu evidential is an inferential suffix –:re:, which she traces to the combination of a subordinator -r on a complement clause with a following verb ‘see’. A fourth Wintu evidential is a suffix -ʔel, which she terms an expectational, indicating ‘that the speaker believes his proposition to be true because of his experience with similar situations, regular patterns, or repeated circumstances common in human life’ (1986: 52). This she traces to a verb meaning something like ‘it exists’. For Southern Wintuan, Lawyer describes just one true evidential, a hearsay particle. He notes that it is attested only in the Hill Patwin dialects, but it can be traced to a verb ʔupu ‘say’, found only in the River Patwin and South Patwin dialects (Lawyer 2015: 306–307). Whistler lists five additional suffixes in Hill Patwin that could be seen to add some evidential meaning. The first two, -mtʰer/-mutʰer (realis) and -mte/-mute: (irrealis) ‘tentative inference’, imply insufficient ground for certain knowledge. These are cognate with the Wintu non-visual sensory evidential -ntʰer, but Whistler notes that in Patwin the element of sensory evidence is absent, so that they indicate only speaker uncertainty. A third, -boti/-beti, is an indirect evidential suggesting knowledge based on other than direct sensory evidence. It includes hearsay, logical inference, and inference based on circumstance or appearance. This Whistler traces to an auxiliary -bo/-be ‘to be (locational). A fourth, -mʔa/-muʔa indicates confident inference, ‘must have’. It developed from the subjunctive suffix -m/-mu plus an auxiliary -ʔa ‘have’. A fifth, -monʔa indicates circumstantial inference, based on appearance, ‘appears to be’. It is still a full verb meaning ‘to do like, be like’. All thus have traceable, often very recent histories. Schlichter points out that all of the evidentials in the family are of recent origin. Evidentials are not an old Wintun trait. Only -ntʰEr has a cognate with similar functions in Patwin (Whistler 1986), the only surviving related language. The other Patwin evidentials were created independently from different forms already present in the language, … perhaps because Patwin has neighbors with evidentials, the Pomo languages. (Schlichter 1986: 48)

This pattern of evidential development indicates that the shaping of grammar through frequency of expression can involve more than one language. If distinctions are made particularly often in one language, bilinguals are likely to transfer such frequencies into another language they speak, even if they do not transfer the actual forms. Speakers accustomed to specifying the source of their information in a Pomoan language, for example, might replicate this tendency when speaking another language, even if they draw on resources native to that second language to accomplish this with paraphrases: ‘they say’, ‘it must be that’, etc. Particularly if there are large numbers of bilinguals, over a substantial period of time, this added frequency of expression in the second language can set in motion processes of grammaticalization there. In fact, a number areas of North America show the effects of just such processes, as detailed in the chapter by Thomason in this volume on language contact.

644 

 Marianne Mithun

27.5 Conclusion Languages are constantly evolving, as speakers extend them for expressive purposes, routinizing frequently-recurring patterns of expression, coining new terms for nameworthy concepts, and replicating distinctions they are accustomed to specifying in other languages they speak. As elements of recurring sequences of words lose their lexical independence and their individual meanings fade, speakers may have varying consciousness of the original meanings of the elements of grammatical constructions. Klallam speakers may no longer think of the literal meaning ‘two-nose’ when they mention čšə́qsən ‘two-pronged fish spear used for flounder and crab’ or the literal meaning ‘slip.offmouth’ when they use the verb ɬp’céʔnə’ ‘stutter, have a strong accent or slurred speech’. Kʷak’ʷala speakers may not be thinking ‘long horizontal opening around indoor’ when they use the verb k̓acʔstogaʔliɬ ‘lay a stick or broom toward the door on the floor’. Yup’ik speakers may not be thinking ‘they tend to cause one to work or play with an associate’ when they use the verb ilaliunarqut ‘they are friendly’. Cherokee speakers are surely not thinking of spatial direction when they use the future prefix ta-, descended from a cislocative ‘hither’. But speakers of Iroquoian languages have sometimes remarked that they often sense ‘shadow meanings’ in their words, that they see images in their minds. Grammar and vocabulary are shaped by what speakers have chosen to say over long periods of time, often reflecting their unique environmental, social, and cultural circumstances. This fact is of major importance for communities dedicated to passing along their heritage to future generations. Of course languages must continue to evolve to meet the needs of their speakers. But the idiomatic turns of phrase, grammatical distinctions, special vocabulary, and even what must be specified and what can be left unsaid, constitute a rich, irreplaceable heritage to be cherished.

References Balodis, Uldis. 2016. Yuki Grammar, with sketches of Huchnom and Coast Yuki. (University of California Publications in Linguistics 151). Berkeley: University of California. Boas, Franz. 1947. Kwakiutl Grammar, with Glossary of the Suffixes. In Helen Boas Yampolsky & Zellig S. Harris (eds.), Transactions of the American Philosophical Society 37(3), 203–377. Philadelphia: The American Philosophical Society. Boas, Franz. 1948. Kwakiutl Dictionary. In Helene Boas Yampolsky (ed.), American Philosophical Society MS #30 W 1a.21. Philadelphia. Bybee, Joan & William Pagliuca. 1987. The evolution of future meaning. In Anna Giacalone-Ramat, Onofrio Carruba & Guiliano Bernini (eds.), Papers from the 7th International Conference on Historical Linguistics, 109–122. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Callaghan, Catherine. 1963. A grammar of the Lake Miwok Language. Berkeley: University of California Ph.D. dissertation in linguistics Eijk, Jan van. 1997. The Lillooet language: Phonology, Morphology, Syntax. Vancouver: University of British Columbia Press.



Languages as dynamic systems: How grammar can emerge 

 645

Gamble, Geoffrey. 1978. Wikchamni grammar. (University of California Publications in Linguistics 89). Berkeley: University of California. Hinkson, Mercedes Quesney. 1999. Salishan lexical suffixes: A study in the conceptualization of space. Vancouver: Simon Fraser University Ph.D. dissertation in linguistics. Hopi Dictionary Project. 1998. Edited by Kenneth C. Hill. Tucson: University of Arizona Press. Jacobsen, William H. 1980. Inclusive/exclusive: A diffused pronominal category in native western North America. Chicago Linguistic Society Parassession on Pronouns and Anaphora, 208–227. Jacobson, Steven A. 2012. Yup’ik Eskimo Dictionary. 2nd edn. Fairbanks: Alaska Native Language Center, University of Alaska. Lawyer, Lewis. C. 2015. A description of the Patwin language. Davis: University of California Ph.D. dissertation in linguistics/ Lee, Dorothy Demetracopoulou. 1941. Some Indian texts dealing with the supernatural. The Review of Religion 5(4). 403–411. Malotki, Milo. 1978. Lessons in Hopi. Edited by Ronald W. Langacker. Tucson: University of Arizona Press. McLendon, Sally. 2003. Evidentials in Eastern Pomo with a comparative survey of the category in other Pomoan languages. In Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald & R. M. W. Dixon (eds.), Studies in evidentiality, 101–129. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Mithun, Marianne. 1998. The sequencing of grammaticization effects: A twist from North America. In Monika S. Schmid, Jennifer R. Austin & Dieter Stein (eds.), Historical Linguistics 1997: Selected apers from the 13th International Conference on Historical Linguistics, 291–314. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing. Mithun, Marianne. 2011. Grammaticalization and explanation. In Bernd Heine & Heiko Narrog (eds.), The Oxford handbook of grammaticalization, 177–192. Oxford: UK: Oxford University Press. Mithun, Marianne. 2017. Affix ordering: Motivation and interpretation. In Andrew Hippisley & Gregory Stump (eds.), The Cambridge handbook of morphology. Chapter 7, 149–185. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Mithun, Marianne. 2020. Context and consciousness: Documenting evidentials. In Karolina Brzech, Eva Schultze-Berndt & Henrik Bergqvist (eds.), Knowing in interaction: Empirical approaches to epistemicity and intersubjectivity in language. Special issue of Folia Linguistica Special issue of Folia Linguistica 2020. 54.2.317–342. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/flin-2020-2045  Montler, Timothy. 2012. Klallam dictionary. Seattle: University of Washington Press. Mooney, James. 1898. Calendar history of the Kiowa Indians. Bureau of American Ethnology Annual Report 17. Washington, D.C.: Government Printing Office. Moshinsky, Julius. 1974. A grammar of Southeastern Pomo. University of California Publications in Linguistics 72. Berkeley: University of California Nicolson, Marianne. 2013. Yeχa Uk̓wine’, Yeχa Gukw, Dɬuwida Awin̓agwis ‘The Body, the House, and the Land’: The conceptualization of space in Kwakwaka’wakw language and culture. Victoria: University of Victoria Ph.D. dissertation. O’Connor, Mary Catherine. 1992. Topics in Northern Pomo grammar. New York: Garland Publishing. Oswalt, Robert. 1961. A Kashaya grammar (Southwestern Pomo). Berkeley: University of California Ph.D. dissertation in linguistics. Pitkin, Harvey. 1984. Wintu grammar. (University of California Publications in Linguistics 94). Berkeley & Los Angeles: University of California Press. Pulte, William & Durbin Feeling. 1975. Outline of Cherokee grammar. Cherokee-English dictionary, 235–355. Tahlequah, OK: Cherokee Nation of Oklahoma. Pustet, Regina. 2000. Lakota postpositions. International Journal of American Linguistics 66(2). 157–180. Pustet, Regina. 2008. Discourse frequency and the collapse of the adposition vs. affix distinction in Lakota. In Elena Seoane & María José López-Couso (eds.), Theoretical and empirical issues in grammaticalization, 269–292. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

646 

 Marianne Mithun

Radin, Paul. 1929. A grammar of the Wappo language. (University of California Publications in American Archaeology and Ethnology 27). Berkeley: University of California. Rosenblum, Daisy. 2015. A grammar of space in Kʷak’ʷala. Santa Barbara: University of California Ph.D. dissertation in linguistics. Schlichter, Alice. 1986. The origins and deictic nature of Wintu evidentials. In Wallace Chafe & Johanna Nichols (eds.), Evidentiality: The linguistic coding of epistemology, 46–59. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Walker, Neil Alexander 2020. A grammar of Southern Pomo, an Indigenous language of California. Lincoln, NB: University of Nebraska Press. Watkins, Laurel J. 1984. A grammar of Kiowa. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Whistler, Kenneth. W. 1986. Evidentials in Patwin. In Wallace Chafe & Johanna Nichols (eds.), Evidentiality: The linguistic coding of epistemology, 60–74. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Young, Robert W. & William Morgan, Sr. 1987. The Navajo language: A grammar and colloquial dictionary. Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press.

Sarah Thomason

28 Language contact and linguistic areas Abstract: This chapter surveys the extent and nature of language contacts in North America north of Mexico—that is, in Canada and the continental United States. The chapter begins with an introductory survey of multilingual contacts on the continent— who is (or was) multilingual, where multilingualism exists (or existed), and when contacts occur(red). Next, the focus is on contacts among indigenous peoples, especially in pre-European, pre-reservation/reserve days. The chapter then moves on to a consideration of indigenous people’s contacts with Europeans, in particular immigrants from Spain, France, England, and Russia. The next section describes some of the non-extreme linguistic results of contacts: lexical borrowing and resistance to it, and structural diffusion. A separate section is devoted to a closer look at North American mixed languages and their histories: the pidgins Chinook Jargon, Pidgin Delaware (Lenape), Mobilian Jargon, American Indian Pidgin English, the Plains Indian Sign Language, and a few others; and the bilingual mixed languages Michif and Mednyj Aleut. The final main section covers linguistic areas in North America, in particular the Pacific Northwest, Northern California, and the Southeast.

28.1 Introduction The indigenous groups that are the focus of the chapter are Native Americans in the United States and, in Canada, First Nations, Inuit, and Métis people. A glance at the twelve main cultural regions of North America that were described by Sherzer (1976, and see also Driver 1969) hints at the linguistic diversity of the continent, since each of the culture areas contains multiple languages and therefore multiple potential or actual intergroup contacts: the Arctic, the Western Subarctic, the Eastern Subartic, the Northwest Coast, the Plateau, California, the Southwest, the Great Basin, the Plains, the Southeast Plains, the Northeast, and the Southeast. The first relevant question for the topic of the chapter concerns the range and pervasiveness of multilingualism on the continent. The answer is that multilingualism was very widespread indeed both before and after Europeans arrived, and it was also typically highly valued by the various cultures. Here, for instance, is a representative assessment (Spicer 1961:370, 396): “As befitted their position as traders, Wasco, Wishram, and Cascade [Chinookan] Indians often spoke more than one dialect [= language] of Chinookan and one or more of Sahaptin[…]A few Wascos and Wishrams learned even more distant languages, such as Nez Perce, which were useful for visiting and trading. There was prestige attached to knowing languages and perhaps aesthetic satisfaction; while serving as an army scout, one Wasco learned a certain amount of Delaware from a fellow scout. While this was not a “practical” accomplishment, it provided a basis for pride and prestige.” https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-028

648 

 Sarah Thomason

But although this is a typical view of multilingualism in indigenous North American cultures, it is not universally held. In reservation days early in the 20th century, Victoria Howard, a native speaker of Clackamas Chinook, commented that most people who spoke many languages in the old (pre-contact) days were slaves (Henry Zenk, p.c. 2005, quoting from Melville Jacobs’ fieldnotes): “[T]hey were so often foreigners. Clackamas had no Clackamas slaves. Only slaves could talk all kinds of languages long ago, and a person who went thus from tribe to tribe and knew so many languages was likely to be derided for being a slave, homeless like a tramp or hobo.”

It is not clear, however, that Howard’s opinion was widely shared. Occasions for multilingualism were frequent and varied among Native communities: trading, often over long distances; exogamy; small communities coming together for joint buffalo hunts; gambling games, races, and other contests; and other opportunities as well for social interactions. And as Howard’s quotation indicates, slavery—one common result of wars between nations—provided a different sort of occasion that promoted multilingualism. The chapter begins with a discussion of contacts among North American indigenous peoples (§2) and continues with contacts between Native peoples and Europeans (§3). Section 4 then turns to a survey of various linguistic results of language contact, and §5 focuses on the most extreme results of contact—pidgins, a possible creole, and bilingual mixed languages. Section 6, finally, concerns linguistic areas in North America, and §7 is a brief conclusion.

28.2 Contacts among Native communities There is no documentation of Native-Native contacts in North America before European witnesses arrived in the New World, but at least three kinds of evidence attest to extensive pre-contact multilingualism. First, shared words are found in unrelated languages, which can only be explained as a result of contact—though if two languages share a particular word one cannot necessarily conclude that one of the languages borrowed it from the other language, because it could have been borrowed and re-borrowed from a third or fourth or nth language and transmitted through the whole chain to the ultimate receiving language. It is almost certainly more common to find lexical borrowing between neighboring languages, however, as in Montana Salish séliš and Nez Perce sé:lix ‘(Bitterroot) Salish (formerly known as Flathead) people’ (Aoki 1975:186; Rigsby 1996; Pharris and Thomason 2005:195). The Séliš and Nez Perce tribes used to join together to travel east through the Rocky Mountains to hunt bison in the Plains; they intermarried, and there is evidence that there was considerable bilingualism. Shared words are therefore not surprising, but their languages belong to separate families—Salishan and Molalla-Sahaptian, respectively.



Language contact and linguistic areas 

 649

Even shared words in related languages can provide solid evidence of contact. Berge (2018) reported recently, for instance, that semantic and phonological evidence points to borrowing rather than inheritance for much of the vocabulary shared between Eskimoan and Aleut languages: the languages are certainly related, as shown by cognate grammatical affixes and function words and some genuine cognates, but clearly there has been very extensive mutual or one-way borrowing between the two groups. It is of course possible to borrow words from a language one does not speak fluently; but lexical borrowing attests to at least a modest degree of bilingualism. The linguistic evidence of pre-European multilingualism is not confined to the lexicon, of course. One example of structural transfer is the fact that “Yuki and Wappo, two California languages, borrowed first and second person pronouns from the neighboring but unrelated Pomoan languages’ (Mithun 2010:674, citing Mithun 2008). This example is particularly striking because of the common but mistaken view that pronouns are unborrowable, but other (usually less surprising) examples of structural borrowing are found in many North American languages, for instance in linguistic areas (see §6). A second source of evidence of pre-European contacts and multilingualism comprises reports by European observers once they were on the scene: they often commented on the fact that many Natives understood each other’s languages, as we saw above in the quotation from Spicer (1961). Europeans also reported elders’ comments about multilingualism. And the third source is oral history: even modern elders in many Native communities describe their community’s pre-European cultural history as one involving significant levels of multilingualism. The nature of contacts between Native communities in pre-European days varied as widely as the diverse linguistic results of the varied contacts. Some contacts were peaceful and mutually supportive, like the joint bison hunts of the Seliš and Nez Perce tribes; others were hostile, involving warfare and killing instead of, or in addition to, conversation. Different levels of multilingualism often co-existed side by side. In northern California, for instance, a given language normally “belonged” to a given place, so that only that language could normally be spoken there (Golla 2011:7). But there were groups in the area who became active multilinguals: the “Northeastern Pomo [Pomoan], at Stonyford, who were at the center of a salt trade that extended throughout the Sacramento Valley, had a high degree of bilingualism in the two adjacent Wintuan languages, Nomlaki and Patwin” (Golla 2011:8, citing Kroeber 1925:224). Slaves were typically enemies captured in wars, and sometimes a distinctive slave jargon or even a pidgin arose among a slave population (Splawn 1944:490): “The Nez Perces used two distinct languages, the proper and the Jargon, which differ so much that, knowing one, a stranger could not understand the other. The Jargon is the slave language, originating with the prisoners of war, who are captured in battle from the various neighboring tribes and who were made slaves; their different languages, mixing with that of their masters, formed a jargon[…]The Jargon in this tribe was used in conversing with the servants and the court language on all other occasions.”

650 

 Sarah Thomason

In other cases, even intense contact did not have the kinds of linguistic effects one might have expected, as in the contact situation involving the Arizona Tewa (Tanoan) people, who are surrounded by Hopis and bilingual in the Hopis’ Uto-Aztecan language. According to Kroskrity (1993:65, 74–75), because the Arizona Tewas have a strong cultural bias against language mixing, they have borrowed very few words from Hopi (and they also borrowed very few words from the later-arriving Spaniards). Their language has, however, undergone some structural borrowing, including a passive suffix. Another, and sadder, outcome of contact between indigenous peoples in North America was the partial or total absorption of one community by another, leading to the loss of the absorbed community’s language and other cultural riches. The Alaskan language Eyak (Athabaskan-Eyak) lost territory and speakers to Tlingit (Na-Dené, and thus related to the Athabaskan-Eyak family) as the Tlingits spread northward from British Columbia into Alaska. Tlingit did not completely replace Eyak, however; Eyak did not lose all its speakers until they came into intense contact with Europeans, first Russians and then Anglo-Americans (see §3). Yet another result of contacts among Native communities was the emergence of lingua francas, including a few pidgin languages. Among these were Haida (an isolate), which was apparently used as a lingua franca in the Pacific Northwest (Silverstein 1996:121, citing Scouler 1905), and an Inuit (Eskimo-Aleut)-based pidgin which, according to Stefánsson (1909), was used with Gwich’in (Athabaskan) speakers, and which had more Inuit structure than the distinct Inuit-based pidgin that was used between Inuits and Europeans. The pidgin Mobilian Jargon (lexified by Choctaw and/or Chickasaw; Muskogean) may also have originated and achieved limited lingua franca status before the first Europeans—French speakers—settled near the lower Mississippi River, although Silverstein (1996:124) attributes the origin of the pidgin to French-Muskogean contacts. Similarly, Silverstein argues for a European-mediated origin for Chinook Jargon (Chinookan, possibly Penutian) in the Pacific Northwest (1996:127), while others propose a pre-European origin for this pidgin (e.  g. Thomason 1983). Another contact language that probably arose before European arrivals was the Plains Indian Sign Language, which fulfilled the lingua-franca functions of a pidgin and which stopped spreading westward in the northern Rocky Mountains when it met the easternmost range of Chinook Jargon (Thomason 2001:166; see §5 below for further discussion of this sign language).

28.3 Contacts between Native peoples and Europeans For obvious reasons, information about contacts with Europeans is much more extensive than information about pre-European contacts among Native peoples. Historically, the most widely spoken European languages in North America have been Spanish, English, French, and, in the Northwest, Russian.



Language contact and linguistic areas 

 651

Early commenters sometimes noted difficulties of communication with Natives. One of the earliest was Christopher Columbus, as quoted by Morison (1963:93, cited in Karttunen 2000:215): “I do not know the language, and the people of these lands do not understand me nor I them, nor does anyone on board. And these Indians whom I took along I often misunderstood, taking one thing for the opposite, and I don’t trust them much, for many times they have tried to flee.”

As this quotation suggests, one European strategy for facilitating communication with Natives was to kidnap people and take them back to Europe to be trained in Spanish or English or some other European language (Karttunen 2000). Later explorers and settlers were less likely to assume that all the North American Natives they met spoke the same language. Sometimes interpreting had to proceed through a chain of languages, as in this instance from the Lewis and Clark journals (Jackson 1978:519; also quoted in Silverstein 1996:118, who identifies the tribes): “Sept. 5. 1805. Our convn. with the Tushepaws [Bitterroot Salish] was held thro’ a boy whom we found among them; a boy a Snake (Shoshonee) [Shoshoni; Uto-Aztecan] by birth who had been taken prisoner by some northern band retaken by the Tushepaws whose language he had acquired. I spoke in English to Labieche[…]—he translated it to Chaboneau in French—he to his wife in Minnetar’ee [Hidatsa; Siouan]—she in Shoshon’e to the boy—the boy in Tushepaw to that nation.”

Some Europeans, of course, did try to learn the Natives’ languages. Many missionaries made serious efforts to do so, but they were not always successful. In 1628, for instance, the Dutch missionary Jonas Michaëlius complained that the Delaware (Algonquian) Indians (Jameson 1909:128) “rather design to conceal their language from us than to properly communicate it, except in things which happen in daily trade; saying that it is sufficient for us to understand them in that; and then they speak only half sentences, shortened words[…]; and all things which have only a rude resemblance to each other, they frequently call by the same name.”

What the frustrated missionary had heard was Pidgin Delaware, but he probably would not have been mollified even if he had realized it. Native-European contacts led to the development of several lingua francas and several uncontroversially post-contact pidgins. Silverstein (1996) reports on several of these, among them lingua-franca Cree (Algonquian), which spread across Canada with the fur trade (p.  119), Pidgin Delaware (Algonquian; pp.  122–124, and see also Goddard 1997, contra Thomason 1980), Apalachee-Spanish (Muskogean; p.  124), the Pidgin Inuit that was used with European sailors (pp.  130–131; see also Stefánsson 1909), Trader Navajo (p.131; see also Werner 1963), and American Indian Pidgin English (cf. pp. 135–136). Spanish, English, French, and Russian all contributed loanwords to the languages of indigenous peoples with whom the different European groups came into contact (see

652 

 Sarah Thomason

e.  g. Callaghan and Gamble 1996; Hinton 1994:101, 105). But the major impact of European languages—primarily English but also, in Canada, French—was their speakers’ large-scale destruction of Native cultures and, with the cultures, the living languages. Even people of European descent who found the indigenous languages worthy of attention often did not seem to find them deserving of protection, as this comment by Thomas Jefferson suggests (1782:510): “It is to be lamented, then, very much to be lamented, that we have suffered so many of the Indian tribes already to extinguish, without our having previously collected and deposited in the records of literature, the general rudiments at least of the languages they spoke.”

Government policies aimed at forcing Native peoples to assimilate to the majority culture were powerful in both Canada and the United States. Both governments forced Native children into boarding schools, which implemented government policies to make the children shift to English (or French). Former boarding-school students remember what it was like in those schools. A Tlingit elder commented, “Whenever I speak Tlingit, I can still taste the soap” (Dauenhauer and Dauenhauer 1998:65). And a Montana Salish elder, asked whether she had raised any of her children to speak Seliš, replied, “No. I didn’t want my kids to go through what I went through”. When asked if she would choose otherwise if she could do it all over again, she said, “Yes, of course. But it’s too late now” (Harriet (Alyé) Whitworth, p.c. ca. 1993). Late in her life, Marie Smith Jones (an Eyak speaker) had a similar reaction to the long-term effects of her boarding-school experience (Raymond 1998): “Sometimes I could just kick myself for not teaching my children the language[…]When I was in school we were beaten for speaking our language. They wanted to make us ashamed[…]I have 17- and 18-year-old kids coming to me crying because the elders in their tribes will not teach them their own language.”

Almost every indigenous North American language is gravely endangered, and many languages have vanished entirely, some (as Jefferson observed more than two hundred years ago) without being documented at all. The endangerment process continues all over North America, but vigorous reclamation efforts, both for revitalization of still-spoken languages and for revival of no-longer-spoken languages, have sprung up all over the continent and are offering hope for the future of the languages. Eyak is a case in point. English-speaking Americans in the 19th century (and, to a lesser extent, Russians in the late 18th century) introduced European languages along with alien cultural and economic systems, and an epidemic killed many Eyaks, so that the community shrank drastically. The last fluent native speaker of the language was Marie Smith Jones, who died in 2008. But her relatives and other community members have recently inaugurated the Eyak Language Project to revive the language, in an effort to restore it to its community.



Language contact and linguistic areas 

 653

For some languages, however, it is too late for hope. Susequehannock (Iroquoian), for instance, first lost most of its speakers as a result of a smallpox epidemic and wars with other Native tribes; the language finally died when its few remaining speakers were murdered by a lynch gang in 1763 (Mithun 1981). Although it has a minimal level of documentation and a similarly minimal internet presence, Susquehannock is unlikely to be revived because the tribe vanished along with its language. Nevertheless, the large and increasing number of websites devoted to reclamation of particular Native languages—in North America as elsewhere in the world—is a very positive sign, and in North America, at least, government policies have changed significantly since the boarding-school days. A vital aspect of the current reclamation efforts is that they are led by the Native communities themselves, not by outsiders. When communities ask for their help, linguists provide it in the form of documentation and educational materials, but Native activists emphasize the limits of this help, as in this quotation from the Tlingit oral historians Nora Marks Dauenhauer and Richard Dauenhauer (Lord 1996:68): “Preservation[…]is what we do to berries in jam jars and salmon in cans. Preserved foods are different from thriving berry patches and surging runs of salmon, and dictionaries are not the same as speech. Books and recordings can preserve languages, but only people and communities can keep them alive.”

Still, linguists’ help is often not only welcome but necessary. For instance, consider this comment, from an obituary for the Siouanist Robert Rankin (1939–2014), by Jim Pepper Henry, a member of the Kaw (Siouan) Nation (Wichita Eagle, 2 March 2014): “If it wasn’t for his work, we wouldn’t have our language. He single-handed preserved our [Kansa] language[…]You can’t put a price on something like that”. And further in the same obituary: “Johnnie Ray McCauley, who was Kaw, told The Eagle in 1996: “I just want to hear it again. There has been no one else to talk it with.” McCauley died in his home a few months after receiving the CDs. When he was found, he was wearing headphones and the CD recordings of his late aunt speaking the family’s native Kaw Indian language were still playing, according to his obituary in The Eagle.”

Rankin had recorded Mr. McCauley’s aunt’s stories along with other narratives on reelto-reel tapes in the 1970s, digitized and converted the tapes to CDs in 1996, and presented the Kaw community with a set of the CDs that same year. Not surprisingly, reclamation programs meet with varying degrees of success. Among the most prominent North American programs are the Myaamia Center (Miami-Illinois language, Algonquian) in Ohio, a revival program led by Daryl Baldwin; the Wampanoag (Massachusett language; Algonquian), a revival program in Mashpee, MA, led by Jessie “Little Doe” Baird; and the Yinka Déné Language Institute (the Carrier Nation; Athabaskan), a revitalization program in Stoney Creek, British Columbia.

654 

 Sarah Thomason

28.4 Some linguistic results of contact The most conspicuous linguistic effects of contact are loanwords. Indigenous North American languages have borrowed words from other Native languages as well as from the languages of European colonizers. So, for instance, Central Alaskan Yup’ik has borrowed numerous words from Aleut, e.  g. arliaq ‘albatross’ from aĝligax̂, kalukaq ‘wooden bowl’ from kalukax̂ ‘dish’, and taangaq ‘liquor’ from taangax̂ ‘water’ (Mithun 1999:311, citing Jacobson 1984:688). The direction of borrowing can often, though not always, be determined with the help of evidence from the sounds, the word structure, and/or comparison with related languages. For instance, a word for ‘cedar’ is shared by Muskogean languages (as čuwahla) and the Siouan language Biloxi (as čuwahna, and the directon of borrowing is shown by the fact that Muskogean languages have both l and n while Biloxi has only n (Haas 1978:313). Biloxi must therefore have borrowed the word from Muskogean, because if Muskogean had borrowed it from Biloxi, the Muskogean word would have had n, not l. All the possibilities are instantiated in borrowings between Southern Interior Salishan languages (e.  g. Montana Salish) and languages of the unrelated Molalla-Sahaptian family (e.  g. the Sahaptian language Nez Perce) (Pharris and Thomason 2005). So, for instance, Nez Perce sá:slaqs ‘moose’ must be a borrowing from Montana Salish s-x̣aslqs, both because the word has a well-attested Salishan etymology and because it is morphologically analyzable in Salish (literally ‘prefix-good-nose’) but not in Nez Perce. Similarly, Montana Salish ƛ’iyéɁ must be the source of Nez Perce ƛ’íy’es ‘canoe’, both because the word has a well-established etymology within Salishan and because [ƛ’] is rare and of very limited distribution in Nez Perce. Conversely, the word for ‘wild onion’ was borrowed from Molalla-Sahaptian, where it has a well-established etymology, into Salish, where it is confined to Southern Interior Salishan languages: compare Nez Perce sé:x and Colville-Okanagan saxk. And finally, for some shared words the direction of borrowing cannot be determined. An example is a word that must be reconstructed for both proto-languages (parent languages), Proto-Salish and for Proto-Molalla-Sahaptian: compare Proto-Salish *s-qawc ‘(Indian) potato’ and Molalla qaws ‘edible root’. Often the same words travel from language to language, so we need to keep in mind the difference between direct and indirect routes of borrowing. After Spaniards introduced horses to the New World, for example, the Spanish word for horse, caballo, spread widely among Native languages, appearing in some languages whose speakers were never in direct contact with Spaniards. History and geographical distance might be the only indicator of intermediaries between the ultimate source of a loanword and its eventual farthest reach, but occasionally we find evidence of indirect transmission. One example comes from a French word that entered various languages in the Pacific Northwest from Chinook Jargon, a pidgin that was the main early lingua franca in the region. The French noun phrase la table ‘the table’ appears in Chinook Jargon as the simple word latáb (the original French article la ‘the’ has fused with the noun), and it



Language contact and linguistic areas 

 655

was borrowed in that form into one or more of the several nasalless Northwest Coast languages—languages in which original *m and *n had turned into b and d. But then the word shows up in nearby languages that still have m as latám, showing that speakers of the last borrowing language got it from a nasalless language, not directly from Chinook Jargon (or from French, but Chinook Jargon is by far the most likely indirect source); they then applied a correspondence rule (“their b is our m”) in borrowing the word from a nasalless language. Many Native languages of North America have dozens or even hundreds of loanwords from the languages of European colonizers (see Mithun 1999:312–313 for examples). (It is hardly surprising that European loanwords in Native languages far outnumber Native loanwords in European languages, since Natives were far more likely to learn colonizers’ languages than vice versa.) Interestingly, though, some Native cultures resist lexical borrowing, either from other Native languages or from encroaching European languages, or both. We saw one example above in §2, where Arizona Tewas’ bias against language mixing barred the adoption of loanwords from either their Hopi neighbors or Spanish. A less comprehensive anti-loanword bias exists in the Plateau culture area, including at least Montana Salish and Nez Perce: although both languages have borrowed words from other Native languages, including each other, neither has borrowed words from colonizers’ languages. (There are a few modern exceptions to this generalization, plus some place names and personal names.) The cultures that ban borrowing have of course acquired many new foods, technologies, and other items from the mainstream European-origin culture. To name the new items, some of them construct new words from the native lexical stock, e.  g. Montana Salish p’ip’úyšn ‘automobile’ (lit. ‘it has wrinkled feet’, named from the appearance of the tire tracks). Tewas adopt a similar strategy: “Tewa[…]speakers generally do not employ loanwords to expand their vocabulary to cover introduced material and cultural acquistions. They either coin new words…or else extend the meanings of words already in the language” (Dozier 1967:393–394, cited in Mithun 1999:311). Not all the effects of Native-European contact flow from European to Native languages: English, for example, has borrowed words from Algonquian languages, the first North American languages encountered by British colonists. Among the best-known Algonquian loanwords are moccasin, wigwam, and skunk—words that fall into the typical Native-to-European loanword categories of “native artifacts, flora and fauna, and especially placenames” (Mithun 1999:312). Structural diffusion, the transfer of bits of a language’s structure to another language, also results from language contact, in North America as well as elsewhere in the world. Here are two typical examples of the transfer of sounds. Quileute (Chimakuan) borrowed a nonglottalized lateral affricate [ƛ] from neighboring Wakashan languages (Jacobsen 1979:795, citing Powell 1975); the borrowed affricate fit into the Quileute sound system, because a glottalized lateral affricate [ƛ'] already existed and other glottalized consonants were matched by corresponding nonglottalized consonants. Second, four languages belonging to all three of the major language families of the region shared

656 

 Sarah Thomason

a pair of sound changes from *w > kw and *y > č: these changes occurred in two Salishan languages, Northern Straits and Clallam, Makah (Wakashan), and Chemakum (Chimakuan). In this case the direction of the influence cannot be determined. Examples of diffusion of word structure and syntactic (sentence) structure are also easy to find in situations of contact between indigenous North American languages. For example, Yuki and Wappo, as mentioned in §2, borrowed two personal pronouns from languages of the unrelated Pomoan family: Yuki Ɂa-p, Wappo Ɂah from Pomoan Ɂa: ‘I’ (subject of a transitive verb); Yuki miɁ-s, Wappo miɁ from Eastern Pomo mí ‘you’ (singular, object of a transitive verb)’ (Mithun 2008:308). Silverstein (1977:154) reports that Proto-Chinookan acquired gender categories from neighboring coastal languages; Cupan (Uto-Aztecan) borrowed a prefix m- ‘indefinite’ and a pattern of affixing definite and indefinite prefixes onto verbs meaning ‘to be’ from Diegueño (Yuman) (Campbell 1997:337, citing Elliott 1994); and the Tewa passive-prefix construction is borrowed from Apachean (Athabaskan) (Campbell 1997:339). Mithun (2010:677–678) describes the more complicated picture presented by Tlingit pronoun affixes, which reflect an agent/patient pattern rather than the contrasting nominative/accusative pattern that is characteristic of the Athabaskan-Eyak relatives of Tlingit. But this Tlingit system closely resembles that of the neighboring language Haida, whose speakers were in close contact with Tlingit speakers. In fact, “the Tlingit are known to have absorbed increments of Haidas and Tsimshians” (De Laguna 1990:213, cited by Mithun 2010:678). This suggests that the process by which the Haida agent/patient pattern entered Tlingit was shift-induced influence rather than borrowing by native speakers of Tlingit into Tlingit (see Thomason and Kaufman 1988:37–45 for discussion of this distinction). European languages have had varying amounts of influence on the structures of Native languages of North America, ranging from zero to substantial. Many linguists who have conducted fieldwork on indigenous languages have seen incipient or at least potential diffusion when their bilingual language consultants accommodate their Native speech to the structure of the fieldworker’s language. Accommodation as a route to contact-induced change is extremely common all over the world, including of course in contact situations outside the specialized type of contact in a fieldwork setting; but reports of first-hand experiences with it in fieldwork are especially striking. Here are four illustrations of the fieldwork phenomenon—none of them (as far as I know) actual changes in the Native languages, but all of them potential changes. First, in an elicitation fieldwork session focusing on ditransitive (two-object) constructions, a Montana Salish elder repeatedly provided sentences like (1), when (2) would have been the natural way of expressing the thought: (1)    

Coní naqw’ t  q’ett tl’ Johnny steal particle hide from ‘Johnny stole a [deer] hide from Mary’

Malí Mary



(2)    

Language contact and linguistic areas 

Malí Naqw’-m-ɫ-t-s steal-der.trans-rel-trans-he Mary ‘Johnny stole Mary’s [deer] hide’

q’ett-s hide-her

t  particle

 657

Čoní Johnny

The differences between these two sentences are dramatic: (1) lacks all the usual transitive verb suffixes, and although it is a perfectly grammatical sentence, it would never be uttered without a particular sort of discourse context—which did not exist in the elicitation session. By contrast, (2) is the normal, unemphatic, no-context way of saying ‘Johnny stole a hide from Mary’. When asked whether sentences like (1) were not perhaps a bit Englishy, the elder was surprised: Yes, of course, he said. You were asking in English, so I thought that’s what you wanted. Urged to say it more naturally, he produced sentences like (2). A second example of accommodation was provided by Marie-Lucie Tarpent (1987:157–158), who found that speakers of Nisga’a (Tsimshianic, British Columbia) accommodated to English in elicitation sessions by using object pronouns much more often than they would use them in ordinary speech. In Nisga’a, the object of a verb is normally deleted if it’s identical to the object of a previous verb (as in pseudo-English They heard him but didn’t see); such objects are expressed overtly only for emphasis. Bilingual speakers of Nisga’a express such object pronouns regularly, as is required in English (as in They heard him but didn’t see him). Similarly, Daisy Rosenblum (p.c. 2018) reports that speakers of Kwak’wala (Wakashan, British Columbia) often produce grammatical calques, word by word, in elicitation sessions, accommodating to the English sentences. Moreover, accommodation of this sort is not just a recent practice. David Costa (p.c. 2018) observes that elicited texts recorded by Albert Gatschet in the early 20th century showed that speakers of Peoria (Miami-Illinois; Algonquian) translated sentences with English word order and “completely superfluous pronouns.” These and many comparable examples may not indicate that changes in the direction of English or other colonizers’ languages are under way or even likely in any of the languages; it is quite possible that the speakers would never produce such utterances outside of an elicitation session, in ordinary conversation or story-telling or any other cultural activity. But they do show that deliberate or semi-deliberate contact-induced changes are well within the capabilities of bilingual speakers, and that such changes could occur. And, as noted, above, such changes certainly have occurred in many languages. To take just one North American example, Berez-Kroeker (2016) describes an English-influenced change in the directional system of Ahtna (Athabaskan; Alaska): the traditional elaborate system is oriented to the rivers of the Ahtnas’ homeland (and it varies depending on which river is being referenced), but it is being replaced by the English system (east, west, north, south).

658 

 Sarah Thomason

28.5 Extreme linguistic results of contact: mixed languages Mixed languages are those whose lexicon and structure cannot be traced back to a single ancestral language; they therefore do not fit into the standard historical linguist’s model of genetic relationship. They cannot be classified genetically, and they cannot be used, as whole languages, in the reconstruction of any proto-language. (An argument for this characterization of mixed languages forms the basis of Thomason and Kaufman 1988.) There are two fundamentally different kinds of mixed languages: pidgins and creoles, on the one hand, and bilingual mixed languages on the other. Bilingual mixed languages are created by people who are fluent in both of the input languages; pidgins and creoles, by contrast, resemble an extreme case of imperfect second-language acquisition, where the creators do not speak the lexifier language (the language that provides most of the vocabulary of the mixed language). North America is (or was) home to a number of pidgins, perhaps one creole, and one or two of the three best-understood bilingual mixed languages. Several North American pidgins were mentioned above in §2: Chinook Jargon, Pidgin Delaware (Lenape), Mobilian Jargon, two different Inuit-based pidgins, American Indian Pidgin English, and the Plains Indian Sign Language. Only one of these, Chinook Jargon, has a potential claim to creole status; some authors have suggested that it acquired a few native speakers. But since no one (as far as I know) has provided evidence that there was ever a whole community in which the children’s first language (or one of their first languages) was Chinook Jargon, it is unlikely that it ever achieved true creole status. The pidgin Chinook Jargon (CJ), lexically based primarily on Lower (Shoalwater) Chinook, was once spoken on the Pacific coast from northern California to southern Alaska and eastward into the Idaho panhandle and interior British Columbia. It flourished from before 1850 to about 1950; as late as 1980, monthly sermons were given in the pidgin in at least one church in British Columbia. It also survived on the Grand Ronde reservation in Oregon, where a vigorous revitalization program is now under way. As is typical of pidgins, CJ has a limited vocabulary: more than six hundred words are well attested, one third of them from French or English (but there are fewer European-origin words in the earliest sources). CJ has a sound system typical of indigenous Pacific Northwest languages: more than thirty consonant phonemes (distinctive speech sounds) are reliably attested, among them glottalized stops, lateral fricative and affricates, and both velar and uvular dorsal consonants; and it also has non-European consonant clusters, e.  g. tk’up ‘white, light in color’, ptšix̣. ‘thin’, and ƛ’mən ‘soft’. In these respects CJ is a striking counterexample to the traditional claim that pidgins are maximally simple languages, especially as there is solid evidence that, when speaking CJ, Natives pronounced sounds that were not found in their native languages; for instance, speakers of nasalless languages pronounced



Language contact and linguistic areas 

 659

nasals in CJ words when they spoke CJ. Most non-Natives who spoke CJ did not learn its complex phonology (sound system), but the phonological system is quite regular and systematic in attested utterances from Native peoples (Thomason 1983). CJ has limited morphosyntactic (word and sentence structures) resources, with no bound morphology (suffixes, prefixes)—again, a typical situation for a trade pidgin. Most of its syntactic features, like its phonology, are typical of indigenous Pacific Northwest languages: negative words at the beginning of a sentence, a yes/no question particle, and possessive and imperative constructions. The one apparent exception to this generalization is the basic sentential word order: CJ is mainly Subject-Verb-Object, whereas most Pacific Northwest languages are verb-initial (VSO or VOS). Adjectival predicates in CJ, however, are sentence-initial, as in Hayas ulu tsuq nayka ‘I am very thirsty’, literally ‘much hungry water I’. As noted above (§2), there is controversy about whether CJ arose before or after non-Natives arrived in numbers in the Pacific Northwest. Silverstein’s (1996) argument for a post-contact origin rests primarily on what he considers to be the earliest layer of the CJ lexicon, the small but important word stock from Nuu-chah-nulth (formerly known as Nootka; Wakashan). These words have few of the non-European phonological features that characterize the other Native (primarily Chinook) words in the CJ lexicon, which (since Nuu-chah-nulth does have those non-European features) means that the words were introduced into CJ by non-Natives, not by Natives. That, together with the phonological consistency with which other CJ words are attested among Natives of different Nations, suggests (it seems to me) that the Nuu-chah-nulth words were latecomers to CJ, not part of its foundation. Because there is no documentation of CJ before literate non-Natives arrived in the area, this matter cannot be settled by direct evidence (see Thomason 1983 for a detailed discussion of the origin issue). The lingua franca that was probably a pre-European pidgin was the Plains Indian Sign Language, the main contact language of the U.S. Great Plains (see Wurtzburg and Campbell 1995 for evidence in support of the pre-European origin of this language). The western boundary of its range is just east of the eastern boundary of CJ, and it was used for intertribal communication by a variety of Native tribes—but not (as far as I know) by non-Natives, although a few interested non-Native observers did learn it. Evidence of its usefulness as a lingua franca can be found in various comments from the 19th-century literature. In 1806, for instance, Alexander Henry wrote that “in 1806 the Pawnee [Caddoan] delegation to the Mandan [Siouan] villages used sign language principally” (Silverstein 1996:118, citing Coues 1897, vol. 1:335); and Humreville reported that “[T]he Snakes [Shoshone] were surrounded on every side by Indians of other nations speaking different languages; when they wished to converse with them it was necessary to do so in sign language” (1903:210). In 1930 an event, the Sign Language Council, was held to film fluent sign language, in Browning, Montana (Taylor 1996: 277; Davis 2010:72–73). The conveners brought elders from different tribes (all old men, mostly in their 80s) to the site for the landmark event. A few elders still knew the sign language as late as the 1990s.

660 

 Sarah Thomason

Other pidgins were certainly or almost certainly post-contact. Pidgin Delaware (pidginized Lenape; Algonquian), first attested in 1633, had little Delaware structure and less European (Dutch, Swedish) structure. It was a basic trade pidgin, although it was also used to write religious texts; and to judge by the comment by the missionary Michaëlius (see above, §3), it was used at least in part to keep outsiders from learning real Unami. Mobilian Jargon, a pidgin spoken in Louisiana and beyond, was based on the Muskogean languages Choctaw and Chickasaw (Crawford 1978); it too seems to have been used, and perhaps invented, to keep outsiders from getting too close to the Muskogean cultures: according to Drechsel (1984:160–161), it was used as a “social and cultural barrier against non-Indian outsiders in particular.” Bakker (1989) reports on a Basque-Native pidgin spoken on the East coast of North American in the 16th and 17th centuries, Mishler (2008) discusses a Slavey (Athabaskan) jargon, and Silverstein (1996) describes three other pidgins (or pre-pidgins): the Souriquois Jargon (p.  122), Apalachee-Spanish (p. 124), and Trader Navajo (p. 131). Finally, American Indian Pidgin English is perhaps the only North American pidgin that has a European vocabulary. It was once used by Natives and non-Natives from the eastern U.S. to the West, and in Canada (e.  g. British Columbia) as well. It had a very few non-English structures, such as the transitive verbal suffix preserved in late-20th-century children’s games, as in Me give-um you money. Turning to the other type of mixed language, bilingual mixed languages, we find one or two of the three best-documented languages in this category. The category itself is very small; the number of the world’s known bilingual mixed languages that are (or were) clearly stabilized is tiny, probably fewer than six. Two of the ones that have been studied most intensively involve North American languages: Michif, a French-Cree mixture spoken in Canada and North Dakota (e.  g. Rhodes 1986; Bakker 1997; Bakker and Papen 1997; Gillon and Rosen 2018; Sammons 2019); and Mednyj (or Copper Island) Aleut, an Aleut-Russian mixture spoken on Copper Island, Russia (see e.  g. Thomason and Kaufman 1988:233–238 and Golovko and Vakhtin 1990). Michif, being a continental North American language, clearly belongs in this chapter. A case can also be made for including Mednyj Aleut here, because the homeland of most speakers of Aleut is the chain of Aleutian Islands across the Bering Sea; early in the 19th century, Russian fur traders brought Aleutians to the two Commander Islands, Bering and Copper, which still belong to Russia. Michif is traditionally said to consist of French noun phrases (the nouns themselves, the sound system, word structure, and syntax) and Cree everything else, in particular the verb phrase and the sentential syntax. The Cree parts leak into the French parts, but not vice versa, except for a small amount of lexical borrowing. Recent investigations have shown that the French component is less robust than has previously been thought (Gillon and Rosen 2018), but the mixed status of the language still seems solidly established. The speakers of Michif belong to the Métis community, which arose among the offspring of male French and Scottish European traders and indigenous (mostly Cree, or at least Cree-speaking) women. A strikingly similar split was found by Lynn Drapeau



Language contact and linguistic areas 

 661

(1991) in some Innu (formerly Montagnais; Algonquian) communities in Québec, with French noun phrases inserted into an otherwise Innu grammatical frame. Mednyj Aleut also arose in a mixed population, the outcome of unions between Russian men and Aleut women. Although it has many loanwords from Russian, the language is mostly Aleut, including its elaborate noun structure and non-finite verb structure; but its similarly elaborate finite verb structure is imported wholesale from Russian. Because there are so few stable bilingual mixed languages, generalizing about their origins and their structures is risky. One sociolinguistic feature, however, is clear: these mixtures arise among bilinguals who have formed a separate ethnic group and need or want to have a new language as an in-group symbol. They are exclusively in-group languages, so much so that outsiders sometimes do not know they exist (because their creators, at least—and in some cases perhaps subsequent community members as well—can also speak to outsiders in the two input languages). This sociolinguistic feature distinguishes bilingual mixed languages sharply from pidgins, which arise to serve as a communicative medium used by groups who do not share a common language. Their structural mixes differ greatly, as is also clear from the descriptions of Michif and Mednyj Aleut.

28.6 Linguistic areas in North America Campbell (1997:330–344) lists twelve North American linguistic areas: Northern Northwest Coast, Northwest Coast, Plateau, Northern California, Clear Lake (California), South Coast Range (California), Southern California-Western Arizona, Great Basin, Pueblo, Plains, Northeast, and Southeast. These areas overlap in part (but only in part) with Sherzer’s (1976) twelve North American culture areas, which are listed above in §1. Definitions of “linguistic area” (also known as a Sprachbund) vary; the one adopted here is this: “a linguistic area is a geographical region containing a group of three or more languages that share some structural features as a result of diffusion rather than as a result of accident or inheritance from a common ancestor” (for explanations of this definition, see Thomason 2001:99 and Thomason 2000:311). Linguistic areas have porous boundaries, not sharp lines dividing languages that do have the characteristic areal features from languages that do not; not all the languages in a linguistic area share all the areal features; and the languages need not belong to different families. The most widely studied of the linguistic areas listed by Campbell is the Pacific Northwest—his Northwest Coast—so I’ll discuss this area in some detail, as an illustration of the kinds of difficulties that arise in efforts to trace the histories of shared areal features (see Thomason 2015 for more detail, and also Jacobs 1954; Thompson and Kinkade 1990:42–44; Campbell 1997:332–334; Mithun 1999:314–316; Bach 1997; Poser 1997; Kinkade 1997; Boas 1911; and Beck 2000). The difference between Campbell’s

662 

 Sarah Thomason

name for the area and mine is meant to suggest that not all the relevant languages are or were spoken on the coast. The Pacific Northwest area comprises three main language families: Wakashan (six languages, all coastal), Chimakuan (two languages, both coastal), and Salishan (twenty-three languages, some of which are spoken in the interior of Washington, British Columbia, Idaho, and even as far east as western Montana). The linguistic area is concentrated in the Northwest Coast culture area, but it extends eastward into the Plateau culture area. Some of the areal features are also found in unrelated languages in the general region, including at least Chinookan languages (along the Columbia River), Kutenai (isolate; Montana, Idaho, British Columbia), Molalla-Sahaptian languages (possibly Penutian; Washington, Oregon, Idaho), and Kalapuyan languages (possibly Penutian; Oregon). The structural features that characterize the Pacific Northwest area fall into two categories: those with limited distribution in the area and those that are area-wide. Many of the limited-distribution features probably arose fairly recently, for instance the change from nasal stops *m, *n to oral stops b, d in Quileute (Chimakuan), Twana and Lushootseed (Salishan), and Makah and Ditidaht (Wakashan). Most or all of the area-wide areal features are certainly ancient, because they must be reconstructed for the proto-languages of all three of the main families. Among these features are very large numbers of consonants in each language (38 in Montana Salish, for instance), glottalized nasal consonants and glides, uvular (or back velar) consonants, polysynthetic (extremely elaborate) word structure, a weak noun/verb distinction, many lexical suffixes (with meanings like ‘hand’, ‘water’, and ‘road, trail’), verb-initial basic sentential word order, sentence-initial negation, a distributive plural construction, numeral classifiers, and many others. For most of the areal features of both types, discovering the starting point ranges from difficult to impossible. One might assume, for example, that the change from nasal stops to oral stops began in one of the five languages in which the change occurs, but determining which one is not feasible. It’s even possible that that feature might have developed independently in two or more of the five languages rather than diffusing from language to language, but again, there is no evidence to support this or any other origin hypothesis. One possibility can be ruled out, however: the lack of nasal stops cannot be inherited by the five nasalless languages, because all the other languages in all three families have both m and n. The ancient area-wide features could be inheritances from a remote common ancestor (but there is no evidence that any two or all three of the three main families are related to each other); or they could have arisen in one of the proto-languages and diffused throughout an ancient linguistic area to the other two proto-languages; or they could be “accidentally” shared—that is, shared because of sheer accident or universal tendencies of some kind(s). The third scenario is extremely unlikely, given the cross-linguistic rarity of many of the features, but we would have to flip a coin to decide between the first two scenarios. In other words, we have no support for either



Language contact and linguistic areas 

 663

hypothesis. Because we cannot prove that the ancient features were absent at an earlier stage of any of the proto-languages, or that they were present at an earlier stage of one of the proto-languages, we have in fact no evidence that any of the area-wide features diffused from one language to another. This is unfortunately a fairly common difficulty in studies of linguistic areas: the complexity of multiple contacts among related and unrelated languages too often makes for a murky historical picture. Another especially complex linguistic area is the Northern California area. It contains more language families than the Pacific Northwest: Campbell (1997:335) lists Algic languages (Yurok and Wiyot, distantly related to Algonquian), Athabaskan (Hupa, Mattole, and Kato), Yukian (Yuki and Wappo), Miwokan (Lake Miwok and Southern Sierra Miwok; Utian), Wintuan, Maiduan, Klamath-Modoc (probably Penutian), Pomoan, Chimariko, Achomawi (Shasta-Palaihnihan), Atsugewi, Karuk (isolate), Shasta, and Yana (isolate). (Some linguists posit a Hokan language family that includes some of the unclassified languages in this region, but the proposal remains controversial.) Among the characteristic linguistic features of the area are three series of stop consonants (plain, aspirated, and glottalized), prefixation of verbal subject markers, glottalized resonant consonants (nasals, glides, etc.), a dental/alveolar or plain/retroflex distinction in apical (tongue-tip) stop consonants, and patterns of consonant symbolism (Haas 1970, 1976; Campbell 1997:335–336; Mithun 1999:316–317; see also Balodis 2016 and Jany 2016.). A third robust North American linguistic area is the Southeast, which “correlates well with the Southeast Culture Area” (Campbell 1997:341). According to Campbell (p. 341), the central member languages in this area are Muskogean languages; five isolates (Chitimacha, Atakapa, Tunica, Natchez, and Yuchi); and two Siouan languages, Ofo and Biloxi. There are also less central languages, one further isolate and members of the Siouan, Iroquoian, and Algonquian families (Campbell 1997:341). Campbell lists twenty-three areal traits (pp. 342–343); among them are bilabial or labial fricatives, a lateral fricative /ɫ/, classification of nouns and noun phrases by position, suppletive verb stems based on number (i.  e. singular vs. dual vs. plural stems), retroflex sibilants (s-like sounds), and two different pronoun series marking alienable vs. nonalienable possession. (See also Mithun 1999:319–320; Crawford 1975; and Rankin 1988). Unraveling the historical development of the features of this area is as difficult (or impossible) as with the other areas outlined in this section. Some of the languages are no longer spoken, at least not by native speakers, and lexical and structural information about formerly spoken languages from the more distant past is sketchy at best. Advances in the reconstruction of Southeast languages that are not isolates might help to clarify the historical picture in the future, however.

664 

 Sarah Thomason

28.7 Conclusion Summarizing the whole range of North American contacts and linguistic areas in a few sentences or paragraphs would be impossible. The general conclusions that can be drawn are unexciting: language contact was and is everywhere in North America at all recoverable time periods, past and present. Contact situations were and are diverse in linguistic processes and results, and also in their sociolinguistic aspects. And finally, it is clear that any analysis of the cultures and histories of Native North American peoples must take language (and culture) contact into account.

References Abbott, Clifford. 2016. Contact and change in Oneida. In Andrea L. Berez-Kroeker, Diane M. Hintz & Carmen Jany (eds.), Language contact and change in the Americas: studies in honor of Marianne Mithun, 167–187. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Aoki, Haruo. 1975. The East Plateau linguistic diffusion area. International Journal of American Linguistics 41. 183–199. Bach, Emmon. 1997. Areal grammatical features in Pacific Northwest languages. Paper presented at the symposium “The Pacific Northwest as a Linguistic and Cultural Area”, Annual Meeting of the American Association for the Advancement of Science, Seattle. Bakker, Peter. 1989. ‘The language of the coast tribes is half Basque’: a Basque-American Indian pidgin in use between Europeans and Native Americans in North America, ca. 1540 – ca. 1640. Anthropological Linguistics 31. 117–147. Bakker, Peter. 1997. ‘A language of our own’: the genesis of Michif, the mixed Cree-French language of the Canadian Métis. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Bakker, Peter & Anthony Grant. 1996. Interethnic communication in Canada, Alaska, and adjacent areas. In Stephen Wurm (ed.), Atlas of interethnic communication in the Pacific, 1107–1169. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Bakker, Peter, & Robert A. Papen. 1997. Michif: a mixed language based on Cree and French. In Sarah G. Thomason (ed.), Contact languages: a wider perspective, 295–363. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Balodis, Uldis. 2016. Yuki, Pomoan, Wintun, and Athabaskan: language contact in Round Valley, California. In Andrea L. Berez-Kroeker, Diane M. Hintz & Carmen Jany (eds.), Language contact and change in the Americas: studies in honor of Marianne Mithun, 17–32. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Beck, David. 2000. Grammatical convergence and the genesis of diversity in the Northwest Coast Sprachbund. Anthropological Linguistics 42. 147–213. Berez-Kroeker, Andrea L. 2016. Contact and semantic shift in extreme language endangerment: Ahtna riverine directionals in a cardinal world. In Andrea L. Berez-Kroeker, Diane M. Hintz & Carmen Jany (eds.), Language contact and change in the Americas: studies in honor of Marianne Mithun, 107–138. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Berge, Anna. 2018. Re-evaluating the reconstruction of Proto-Eskimo-Aleut. Journal of Historical Linguistics 8. 230–272. Boas, Franz. 1911. Introduction. In Franz Boas (ed.), The Handbook of American Indian languages, Part 1, 5–83. Washington, D.C.: Government Printing Office. Bright, William. 1973. North American Indian language contact. In Thomas Sebeok (ed.), Linguistics in North America, 713–726. The Hague: Mouton.



Language contact and linguistic areas 

 665

Callaghan, Catherine A. & Geoffrey Gamble. 1996. Borrowing. In Ives Goddard (ed.), Handbook of North American Indian languages, vol. 17, 111–116. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution. Campbell, Lyle. 1997. American Indian languages: the historical linguistics of Native America. New York & Oxford: Oxford University Press. Costa, David J. 2013. Borrowing in southern Great Lakes Algonquian and the history of Potawatomi. Anthropological Linguistics 55. 195–233. Coues, Elliott. 1897. New light on the early history of the greater Northwest: the manuscript journals of Alexander Henry, fur trader of the Northwest Company, and of David Thompson, official geographer and explorer of the same company, 1799–1814. New York: Francis P. Harper. Crawford, James M. 1975. Southeastern Indian languages. In James M. Crawford (ed.), Studies in Southeastern Indian languages, 1–120. Athens: University of Georgia Press. Crawford, James M. 1978. The Mobilian Trade language. Knoxville: University of Tennessee Press. Darnell, Regna, & Joel Sherzer. 1971. Areal linguistic studies in North America: a historical perspective. International Journal of American Linguistics 37. 20–28. Dauenhauer, Nora Marks, & Richard Dauenhauer. 1998. Technical, emotional, and ideological issues in reversing language shift: examples from Southeast Alaska. In Lenore A. Grenoble & Lindsay J. Whaley (eds.), Endangered languages: current issues and future prospects, 57–98. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Davis, Jeffrey E. 2010. Hand talk: sign language among American Indian nations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. De Laguna, Frederica. 1990. Tlingit. In Wayne Suttles (ed.), Northwest coast, 203–228. (Handbook of North American Indians, vol. 7). Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution. Dozier, Edward P. 1967. Linguistic acculturation studies in the Southwest. In Dell H. Hymes & William E. Bittle (eds.), Studies in Southwestern ethnolinguistics, 389–402. The Hague: Mouton. Drapeau, Lynn. 1991. Michif replicated: the emergence of a mixed language in northern Québec. Paper presented at the 10th International Conference on Historical Linguistics, Amsterdam. Drechsel, Emanuel. 1984. Structure and function in Mobilian Jargon: indications for the pre-European existence of an American Indian pidgin. Journal of Historical Linguistics and Philology 1:141–185. Drechsel, Emanuel. 1997. Mobilian Jargon: linguistic and sociohistorical aspects of a Native American pidgin. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Driver, Harold E. 1969. Indians of North America. 2nd edn. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Elliott, Eric. 1994. ‘How’ and ‘thus’ in (Uto-Aztecan) Cupan and Yuman: a case of areal influence. In Margaret Langdon (ed.), Proceedings of the meeting of the Society for the Study of the Indigenous Languages of the Americas and the Hokan-Penutian workshop, 145–169. Berkeley: University of California Department of Linguistics. Gillon, Carrie, & Nicole Rosen, with Verna Demontigny. 2018. Nominal contact in Michif. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Goddard, Ives. 1997. Pidgin Delaware. In Sarah G. Thomason, (ed.), Contact languages: a wider perspective, 43–98. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Golla, Victor. 2011. California Indian languages. Berkeley: University of California Press. Golovko, Evgenij V. 1994. Copper Island Aleut. In Peter Bakker & Maarten Mous (eds.), Mixed languages. 113–121. Amsterdam: IFOTT, University of Amsterdam. Golovko, Evgenij V., & Nikolai B. Vakhtin. 1990. Aleut in contact: the CIA enigma. Acta Linguistica Hafniensia 72. 97–125. Haas, Mary. 1970. Consonant symbolism in northwestern California: a problem in diffusion. In Earl H. Swanson, Jr. (ed.), Languages and cultures of western North America, 86–96. (Reprinted in Language, culture, and history: essays by Mary R. Haas. 1978. Selected and introduced by Anwar S. Dil, 339–352. Stanford: Stanford University Press). Haas, Mary. 1976. The Northern California linguistic area. In Margaret Langdon & Shirley Silver (eds.), Hokan Studies, 347–359. The Hague: Mouton. (Reprinted in Language, culture, and history: essays by

666 

 Sarah Thomason

Mary R. Haas. Selected and introduced by Anwar S. Dil, 353–369. Stanford: Stanford University Press, 1978). Haas, Mary. 1978 [1969]. Prehistory and diffusion. In Anwar S. Dil (ed.), Language, culture, and history: essays by Mary R. Haas, 308–327. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. (Originally from Mary R. Haas. 1969. The prehistory of languages. The Hague: Mouton). Hinton, Leanne. 1994. Flutes of fire: essays on California Indian languages. Berkeley, CA: Heyday Books. Humreville, James Lee. 1903. Twenty years among our hostile Indians. 2nd edn. New York: Hunter & Co. Jackson, Donald. 1978. Letters of the Lewis and Clark expedition, with related documents, 1783–1854. 2nd edn., vol. 2. Urbana: University of Illinois Press. Jacobs, Melville. 1954. The areal spread of sound features in the languages north of California. In Papers from the Symposium on American Indian Languages, 46–56. Berkeley & Los Angeles: University of California Press. Jacobsen, William H., Jr. 1979. Chimakuan comparative studies. In Lyle Campbell & Marianne Mithun (eds.), The languages of Native America: historical and comparative assessment, 792–802. Austin: University of Texas Press. Jacobson, Steven A. 1984. Yup’ik Eskimo dictionary. Fairbanks: Alaska Native Language Center. Jameson, J. F. (ed.). 1909. Narratives of New Netherland 1609–1664. New York: Charles Scribner’s Sons, [Reprinted 1967. New York: Barnes & Noble]. Jany, Carmen. 2016. The role of passives in the formation of hierarchical systems in Northern California. In Andrea L. Berez-Kroeker, Diane M. Hintz & Carmen Jany (eds.), Language contact and change in the Americas: studies in honor of Marianne Mithun, 33–65. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Jefferson, Thomas. 1904 [1782]. Notes on the state of Virginia. The works of Thomas Jefferson. Ed. by Paul Leister Ford, vol. 3. New York & London: Knickerbocker Press & G.P. Putnam’s Sons. Karttunen, Frances. 2000. Interpreters snatched from the shore: the successful and the others. In Edward G. Gray & Norman Fiering (eds.), The language encounter in the Americas, 1492–1800, 215–229. New York & Oxford: Berghahn Books. Kinkade, M. Dale. 1997. The emergence of shared features in languages of the Pacific Northwest. Paper presented at the symposium “The Pacific Northwest as a Linguistic and Cultural Area”, American Association for the Advancement of Science Annual Meeting, Seattle. Kroeber, Alfred L. 1925. Handbook of the Indians of California. (Bureau of American Ethnology Bulletin 76). Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution. Kroskrity, Paul. 1993. Language, history, and identity: ethnolinguistic studies of the Arizona Tewa. Tucson: University of Arizona Press. Lord, Nancy. 1996. Native tongues. Sierra Magazine 81/6. 46–69. www.sierraclub.org/sierra/199611/lord.asp (accessed 11 May 2014). Mishler, Craig. 2008. ‘That’s a Rubbaboo’: Slavey Jargon in a nineteenth century Subarctic speech community. Journal of Pidgin and Creole Languages 23. 264–287. Mithun, Marianne. 1981. Stalking the Susquehannocks. International Journal of American Linguistics 47. 1–26. Mithun, Marianne. 1999. The languages of Native North America. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Mithun, Marianne. 2007. Grammar, contact and time. Journal of Language Contact 1. 144–167. Mithun, Marianne. 2008. The emergence of agentive systems in core argument marking. In Mark Donohue & Søren Wichmann (eds.), The typology of semantic alignment, 297–333. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Mithun, Marianne. 2010. Contact and North American languages. In Raymond Hickey (ed.), The handbook of language contact. 673–694. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell. Mithun, Marianne. 2013. Challenges and benefits of contact among relatives: morphological copying. Journal of Language Contact 6. 243–270. Morison, Samuel Eliot (tr. and ed.). 1963. Journals and other documents on the life and voyages of Christopher Columbus. New York: Heritage Press.



Language contact and linguistic areas 

 667

Papen, Robert A. 2014. Hybrid languages in Canada involving French: the case of Michif and Chiac. Journal of Language Contact 7. 154–183. Perley, Bernard. 2011. Defying Maliseet language death: emergent vitalities of language, culture, and identity in eastern Canada. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Pharris, Nicholas, & Sarah G. Thomason. 2005. Lexical transfer between Southern Interior Salish and Molalla-Sahaptian. In J.C. Brown, Masaru Kiyota & Tyler Peterson (eds.), Papers for the 40th International Conference on Salish and Neighbouring Languages, 184–209. Vancouver: Department of Linguistics, University of British Columbia. Poser, William J. 1997. Areal phonological features in Pacific Northwest languages. Paper presented at the symposium “The Pacific Northwest as a Linguistic and Cultural Area”, Annual Meeting of the American Association for the Advancement of Science, Seattle. Powell, Jay. 1975. Proto-Chimakuan: materials for a reconstruction. University of Hawaii working papers in linguistics 7/2: i–xv, 1–202. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Linguistics Department. Quileute language: Book 1. La Push, WA: Quileute Tribe. Rankin, Robert L. 1988. Quapaw: genetic and areal affiliations. In William Shipley (ed.), In honor of Mary Haas: from the Haas festival conference on Native American linguistics, 629–650. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Raymond, Joan. 1998. Say what? Preserving endangered languages. Newsweek, 14 September 1998. Rhodes, Richard. 1982. Algonquian trade languages. Papers of the Thirteenth Algonquian Conference, 1–10. Ottawa: Carleton University Press. Rhodes, Richard. 1986. Métchif – a second look. In William Cowan (ed.), Papers of the Sixteenth Algonquian Conference. 287–296. Ottawa: Carleton University. Rhodes, Richard. 1992. Language shift in Algonquian. In Allan R. Taylor (ed.), Language obsolescence, shift, and death in several Native American communities. International Journal of the Sociology of Language 93. 87–92. Rigsby, Bruce Joseph. 1996. Some aspects of Plateau linguistic prehistory: Sahaptian/Interior Salishan relations. In Don E. Dumond (ed.), Chin Hills to Chiloquin: papers honoring the versatile career of Theodore Stern, 241–146. Eugene: University of Oregon Department of Anthropology. Sammons, Olivia Nathene. 2019. Nominal classification in Michif. Edmonton, AB: University of Alberta dissertation. Scouler, John. 1905. Dr. John Scouler’s journal of the voyage to N.W. America, 1824–’25–’26, vol. 2: Leaving the Galapagos Islands for the North Pacific. Quarterly of the Oregon Historical Society 6/2. 159–205. Seaburg, William. 1997. Areal features of style in Pacific Northwest folklore. Paper presented at the symposium “The Pacific Northwest as a Linguistic and Cultural Area”, Annual Meeting of the American Association for the Advancement of Science, Seattle. Sherzer, Joel. 1976. An areal-typological study of American Indian languages north of Mexico. Amsterdam: North-Holland. Silverstein, Michael. 1977. Person, number, gender in Chinook: syntactic rule and morphological analogy. Proceedings of the Berkeley Linguistics Society 3. 143–156. Silverstein, Michael. 1996. Dynamics of language contact. In Ives Goddard (ed.), Handbook of North American Indian languages, vol. 17, 117–136. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution. Spicer, Edward. 1961. Perspectives in American Indian culture change. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Splawn, A.J. 1944. Ka-Mi-Akin: last hero of the Yakimas. 2nd edn. Yakima, WA & Caldwell, Idaho: The Caxton Printers (1st edn. 1917). Stefánsson, Vilhjalmur. 1909. The Eskimo Trade Jargon of Herschel Island. American Anthropologist 11. 217–232. Tarpent, Marie-Lucie. 1987. Between ergative and accusative syntax: lessons from Nisgha/English syntactic interference. In John Dunn (ed.), Papers from the 22nd International Conference on Salish and Neighboring Languages, 149–171. Norman: University of Oklahoma Department of Linguistics.

668 

 Sarah Thomason

Taylor, Allan R. 1981. Indian lingua francas. In Charles A. Ferguson & Shirley Brice Heath (eds.), Language in the USA, 79–95. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Taylor, Allan R. 1996. Nonspeech communication systems. In Ives Goddard (ed.), Handbook of North American Indians, vol. 17: Languages, 275–289. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution. Thomason, Sarah G. 1980. On interpreting “The Indian Interpreter”. Language in Society 9. 167–193. Thomason, Sarah G. 1983. Chinook Jargon in areal and historical context. Language 59. 820–870. Thomason, Sarah G. 1997. Mednyj Aleut. In Sarah G. Thomason (ed.), Contact languages: a wider perspective, 449–468. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Thomason, Sarah G. 2000. Linguistic areas and language history. In Dicky Gilbers, John Nerbonne & Jos Schaeken (eds.), Languages in Contact, 311–327. Amsterdam: Rodopi. Thomason, Sarah G. 2001. Language contact: an introduction. Edinburgh & Washington, D.C.: Edinburgh University Press and Georgetown University Press. Thomason, Sarah G. 2015. The Pacific Northwest linguistic area: historical perspectives. In Claire Bowern & Bethwyn Evans (eds.), The Routledge handbook of historical linguistics, 727–737. London: Routledge. Thomason, Sarah G. & Terrence Kaufman. 1988. Language contact, creolization, and genetic linguistics. Berkeley: University of California Press. Thompson, Lawrence C. & M. Dale Kinkade. 1990. Languages. In Wayne Suttles (ed.), Northwest coast, 30–51. (Handbook of North American Indians, vol. 7). Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution. Werner, Oswald. 1963. A typological comparison of four Trader Navajo speakers. Bloomington: Indiana University dissertation. Wurtzburg, Susan & Lyle Campbell. 1995. North American Indian Sign Language: evidence for its existence before European contact. International Journal of American Linguistics 61. 153–167.

Hannah J. Haynie

29 Language classification Abstract: Classification of North American languages has been central to the academic study of indigenous American cultures, their histories, and the more general human history of the Americas since the arrival of European colonists on the continent. Alternative classification proposals have relied on a variety of data types and different standards of evidence for relatedness and have resulted in taxonomies with varying levels of acceptance among linguists. To some extent, disagreement in classifying American languages also reflects the nature of the evidence: how well documented individual languages are, how well analyzed that documentation is, and how effectively and appropriately this data is utilized in various methodologies. This chapter briefly surveys some prominent classification proposals, discusses the data, methods, and theory that inform language classification projects, and takes a more detailed look at specific achievements and controversies in the classification of North American languages. The chapter concludes with commentary on the state of the field and prospects for the future.

29.1 Introduction The classification of languages, a longstanding goal in the field of linguistics, has many potential rewards when applied to the languages of North America. It can highlight the regular patterns of corresponding material in languages that descend from a common ancestor, revealing the processes of language change, diversification, and contact that have shaped the continent’s linguistic diversity. It can help us understand the movements of populations on this continent, including the first migrations of humans into the Americas. It can serve as a framework for understanding the diversity of cultures found in North America, and for examining the development and diffusion of technologies, practices, and beliefs. It can help us answer questions about how humans in general interact with the natural world and with their neighbors. Language classification has impacts outside the annals of academic research as well, providing a window on past relationships between peoples and on the lives and languages of ancestral speaker communities. It can also impact the future by facilitating the use of materials from closely related languages in revitalization efforts and helping communities to make the most of the documentation that exists for sister languages. Classifying the languages of North America has long been recognized as an important goal among those who take a great interest in the history of this continent. Similarities between the continent’s languages were noted by European colonists from the time of their arrival, and this colonial interest in language relationships was formalized through efforts like Thomas Jefferson’s collection of vocabularies and directives to military excursions to collect information about indigenous languages (Fowler & https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-029

670 

 Hannah J. Haynie

Fowler 1969; Shaul 1999). Work on the question of North American language classification has proceeded ever since, kept vital by advances in data and methods. Nevertheless, progress in this pursuit has been punctuated by lurches forward and lulls, leaving many important questions unresolved. How many language families are there in North America? How many linguistically distinct groups were involved in the initial settle­ ment of this continent? What prehistoric relationships might we find evidence for in the words and grammars of attested North American languages? Problems like these have not only attracted interest among linguists and anthropologists, they have also inspired methodological innovation, encouraged interdisciplinary discourse, and generated tremendous and still ongoing debate.

29.2 An abridged history of language classification in North America North American language classification has a history of its own, with changing tides of accepted wisdom redefining our understanding of language relationships over the last 150 years. Following the first attempt at classification in the 19th century, based on limited early colonial documentation (Gallatin 1836), John Wesley Powell and the Bureau of American Ethnology (BAE) amassed an unprecedented collection of vocabularies and proposed one of the earliest comprehensive classifications of North American languages (Powell 1891). Powell and his BAE colleagues grouped the languages of North America into 58 “stocks”, or families. With limited information, primarily from brief word lists, this effort set forth a classification that has served as the starting point for all subsequent work on the classification of North American languages. In spite of its general caution in evaluating evidence, some of the relationships proposed by the BAE classification, such as the grouping of Cayuse and Molala into a Waiilatpuan stock, were eventually rejected (Powell 1891; Rigsby 1966). Nevertheless, Powell’s classification was, in the words of Goddard, “a watershed event” (1996: 299). The BAE team’s focus on identifying sets of related languages without working out the exact internal structure of these families opened the door for subsequent work on internal classification within the families they identified, as well as on the more historically remote relationships that unite some of these groups into a smaller number of stocks. As the 20th century dawned, interest turned toward these deep relationships, and language classification efforts sought to reduce the total number of distinct genealogical lineages of North American languages to a more manageable number than Powell’s conservative fifty-eight. Though the tremendous diversity of the Pacific Coast was a particularly tantalizing target for deep relationship-oriented reclassification, this movement revised down the number of proposed stocks across the entire continent, culminating effectively in Edward Sapir’s classification of North America’s languages into



Language classification 

 671

Map 1: Powell’s 58 language families, adapted from “Map of linguistic stocks of American Indians” in Powell 1891.

just six macro-families, with a further fifteen families in Middle America (Sapir 1929). Where Powell’s classification found only a few large language families in eastern North America contrasting with dozens in the far west, Sapir’s entry in the 1929 Encyclopedia Britannica combined all of the languages in the area surveyed by the BAE effort into just six stocks, or macro-families, that appeared to spread the distribution of language family diversity more evenly (Powell 1891; Sapir 1929). On the west coast, Sapir subsumed more than a dozen of Powell’s families into a single Penutian stock that extended

672 

 Hannah J. Haynie

Map 2: Sapir’s North American macro-families, adapted from Sapir 1921.

through contemporary California, Oregon and through the plateau into Washington State. Another large set of west coast languages, along with languages near the current Texas-Mexico border, were grouped under the Hokan name, and combined with families across the continent including Iroquoian, Caddoan, Siouan, and Muskogean to form a Hokan-Siouan stock. Algonquian was combined with Ritwan (Wiyot and Yurok) as well as Wakashan and Salish to form the Algonkin-Wakashan stock. The Uto-Aztecan languages were clustered with Kiowa-Tanoan into Aztec-Tanoan. Sapir’s Eskimo-Aleut and Nadene families are perhaps the least controversial of the stocks in the 1929 classi-



Language classification 

 673

fication, but even the inclusion of Haida in Sapir’s Nadene has proven to be too poorly supported for widespread acceptance (Krauss 1976; Levine 1979). In the mid-20th century, many of the foremost scholars on North American languages and their classification convened at the University of Indiana and, as a result of their meeting, created a classification of North American languages that represented the general consensus of the group (Voegelin & Voegelin 1964; Voegelin & Voegelin 1965). Upon its publication, this work became the standard reference classification of North American languages within the field of linguistics and across disciplines. The scholarly tradition represented by Sapir’s deep proposals and small number of stocklevel groupings was well represented in the membership of this meeting, as was a more Powell-esque conservatism. The methods described in the published classification involves a marriage of both of these perspectives, relying on traditional comparative methods and reconstruction to establish the makeup of families and a more inspectional approach to identifying possible evidence of relatedness at a deeper stratum of time and phylogenetic (family) hierarchy that the authors refer to as “phylum linguistics”. This work ultimately linked most of the continent’s languages into a small number of remote groupings as Sapir had done previously. However, the prior classification was examined critically, and data collected and comparative work done since Sapir’s classification resulted in differences in the details at the family level and in the sixteen phyla, or macro-families, included in the Voegelin & Voegelin classification. Hokan languages, for example, were decoupled from the Siouan, Caddoan, and Iroquoian families Sapir had linked them to and listed as a standalone Hokan phylum alongside a Macro-Siouan phylum. Macro-Algonquian was pared down to Algonquian, Muskogean, and a list of phylum-internal isolates including Yurok, Wiyot, Natchez, Chitimacha, Atakapa, Tunica, and Tonkawa. Families like Wakashan and Salish that were no longer included in the remote stocks where they had been placed by Sapir were listed by Voegelin & Voegelin as orphan families that could not be determined with any confidence to be related to one of the larger phyla in this classification. The convergence of diverse perspectives in the Voegelin & Voegelin classification demonstrated some level of agreement about the state of the field, but it did not give birth to a unified vision for the future of North American language classification. For some, advancing the field meant working toward establishing better evidence for the remote relationships represented by the Voegelin & Voegelin phyla. In the case of Joseph Greenberg, it meant attempting to identify the ancestral relationships that might exist at an even deeper point in the past. In his 1987 book, Greenberg united all of the languages of North, Middle, and South America, with the exception of Eskimo-Aleut and Na-Dene languages, into a single massive stock he called Amerind (Greenberg 1987a). Eskimo-Aleut, meanwhile, was classified by Greenberg as a daughter of an enormous Eurasiatic stock, along with Indo-European, Uralic, Korean, and other languages and families of that continent. Harkening back to Sapir’s classification (Sapir 1929), Greenberg reincarnated proposed relationships that had largely fallen out of favor in Americanist linguistics, including Algonkin-Wakashan (called “Almosan” by Greenberg) and

674 

 Hannah J. Haynie

Map 3: Phyla and families described in Voegelin & Voegelin 1965, 127–150.

a Siouan-Caddoan-Iroquoian link, which shows up in Greenberg’s Keresiouan with the addition of Keresan. These remote relationships, which had been treated with some skepticism at the Voegelin & Voegelin conference, were only an intermediary level of the hierarchy Greenberg proposed. The proposed Amerind super-stock included not only these phyla, but also familiar hypothesized groups like Hokan, Penutian, Uto-Aztecan, and all languages of Middle and South America. While Sapir and others in the vanguard of identifying deeper relationships tended to couch their hypotheses about deep stocks as speculative proposals in need of further study, Greenberg discussed at length the



Language classification 

 675

reliability of the classification achieved through his inspectional methods (Sapir 1920; Sapir 1925; Dixon & Kroeber 1913; Greenberg 1987a; Greenberg 1987b). Perhaps predictably, this classification was met with swift and compelling criticism from linguists, both on methodological grounds and in response to errors in cited data (see discussion in Campbell & Poser 2008: 266–279). While this classification may never have found much support among linguists, and particularly among historical linguists, it has nevertheless been uncritically taken up by some scholars in other fields either as a shorthand for referring to American languages, or erroneously interpreted as a representation of proven linguistic relationships (see Bolnick et al. 2004). Just as Greenberg’s classification found some uncritical acceptance among non-specialists, the remote genealogical groupings put forth by Sapir and others in the early 20th century as hypotheses for further study had, by the mid-20th century, come into frequent use both within linguistics and outside of it by scholars who trusted that the relationships they represented had been proven. While Greenberg worked to find evidence for even more distant relationships, conversations among another set of linguists at the 1976 Linguistic Institute pointed at the untested nature of distant genealogical classifications that were in common usage. This group argued for an approach based on more careful scrutiny of the evidence for relationships. From this meeting came a reassessment of what was known about American language families and a renewal of a Powell-style conservatism (Campbell & Mithun 1979). The sixty-two families and isolates listed in the resulting volume demonstrate that the granularity at which language relationships can be reliably established was still, nearly a century later, similar to that of Powell’s families (Campbell & Mithun 1979; Powell 1891). Subsequent reference works on North American languages have provided updates on the evidence for individual families and their internal classification, as well as on the outlook for proposed distant relationships (Campbell 1997; Mithun 1999) and though our understanding of language relationships in North America advanced in recent years, it has done so slowly and carefully. At present, Campbell’s classification remains a reasonable representation of the language relationships widely accepted among specialist scholars (Campbell 1997: 86–89). This renewed conservatism is reflected in the North American language classifications implemented in global language catalogs like Ethnologue and Glottolog (Eberhard, Simons & Fennig 2019; Hammarström, Forkel & Haspelmath 2019).

29.3 The empirical basis for classification All language classifications are based on data, though the types of data considered and how that data is used vary from researcher to researcher. North American language classifications are built upon the same types of data that are used to investigate linguistic relationships in any other part of the world, namely systematic correspondences in

676 

 Hannah J. Haynie

the words and grammars of documented languages. Making the best use of this data is particularly important in a region where data availability is often limited.

29.3.1 Lexical data During 19th century scientific exploration of the American west, both the U.S. Geological Survey (USGS) and later the BAE worked to collect information about the cultures and languages of North America, including vocabulary lists from a diverse set of languages. This data collection effort built upon a tradition of collecting word lists that extended from early missionary contact to Thomas Jefferson’s standardized vocabulary lists for indigenous languages (Shaul 1999). By aligning words by meaning across lexical (word list) datasets, John Wesley Powell and the BAE staff were able to make comparisons of word forms that were impressionistic in nature. (Powell 1891; Shaul 1999). This method used by Powell and in other early classifications was called by Alfred L. Kroeber a “simple frontal attack by inspection” (Kroeber 1940: 464), and it was often the method of choice for very early classification work, or in later work that attempted to identify very remote relationships, because it can be undertaken even where insufficient data exists for more rigorous comparative methods. The focus on word lists in some of the earliest scholarly efforts to classify languages of North America was not driven by the same logic that frequently gives lexical data a privileged position in contemporary language classification efforts, however. Powell, like others during and before his time, was driven toward lexical data as the basis for proposing linguistic relationships due to a now-outdated belief that the grammar of a language primarily reflects its general stage of evolutionary development, and not its specific evolutionary history (Powell 1880). This dismissal of grammatical evidence in investigating linguistic prehistory has been colored at times by ethnocentric views regarding what constitutes a highly evolved language, and conversely a so-called ‘primitive’ language. For others, though, ignoring grammar in language classification reflected merely an assumption, based on observation of a small and non-representative sample of languages, that all American grammars are quite similar in their structures and are therefore uninformative for classification (Duponceau 1838). In spite of misguided justifications for the historical prioritization of lexical data in classifying North American languages, sound reasons remain for using lexical data for this work. Perhaps most obviously, documentation of American languages did not keep pace with post-colonial language shift. For some languages, therefore, word lists may be the only available information. Even where documentation of languages is rich in both lexical and grammatical detail, though, words provide important information about the historical relationships between languages. Systematic correspondences in the sounds of cognates, or words that come from a common historical source, can be used to reconstruct the sound changes that have occurred in related languages during the course of their descent from a common ancestor language, thus providing evidence for relatedness (Meillet 1925).



Language classification 

 677

This traditional historical linguistic method was employed famously and successfully by Bloomfield in his reconstructions of Proto-Algonquian (Bloomfield 1925, 1946). Since Bloomfield’s time, work on Algonquian and many other language families has employed these core methods of historical linguistics (see Campbell 1997 for further discussion). Beginning in the mid-20th century, vocabulary was put to work in novel quantitative analyses for inferring relationships. One of the earliest such analytical approaches was “lexicostatistics”, or the use of statistics to infer from the percentage of shared cognates in basic vocabulary lists (in other words, from the lexical “distance” between languages) how closely related languages are. A form of this method called “glottochronology” was used by Morris Swadesh to estimate the historical dates at which the ancestral languages, or proto-languages, for several North American language families might have existed (Swadesh 1954). Although the assumptions of this method, and in particular its reliance on belief in a uniform, universal rate of change in basic vocabulary, have been widely criticized among linguists (Bergsland & Vogt 1962), Swadesh’s work also introduced standard meaning lists for collecting comparative vocabulary known colloquially as Swadesh lists (Swadesh 1952, 1971). The use of Swadesh lists in language documentation since that time has set a precedent for lexical data collection that has helped to ensure that word forms with comparable meanings are available for a wide sample of American languages. Since that time computational researchers have implemented more modern, character-based phylogenetic comparative methods to analyse lexical data. Phylogenies for several language families of the Americas have been created using Bayesian phylogenetic methods since these tools were introduced from biology to linguistics (Gray & Atkinson 2003). These families include Tukanoan, Arawakan, and Tupi-Guarani in South America (Chacon & List 2015; Walker & Ribeiro 2011; Michael et al. 2015). In North America, these methods have been used only to a limited extent to investigate historical relationships between languages. An early application investigated the origin and expansion of the Mayan language family, and more recently phylogeographic methods have been used to infer the structure of the Uto-Aztecan language family (Greenhill et al. 2023). Lexical data is compatible with a range of methodologies for language classification, is widely available, and lends itself to rigorous comparison of forms. However, there are several drawbacks to using lexical data to understand historical relationships between languages. While standardized meaning lists have made it easy to compare words across languages, these comparisons are typically forced to assume that cognate forms retain their original meanings. Although meanings of words can, in fact, change over time, seeking potential cognates across a wide range of meanings can increase the likelihood of finding spurious resemblances. To provide meaningful evidence about genealogical relationships between languages, resemblances in words must also arise through common descent from an ancestor. However, borrowing from neighbors, iconicity of forms (e.  g., onomatopoeia and sound symbolism), and accidental resemblance can also create similarities between words, and must be ruled out before concluding that similar words were inherited from a common ancestor. Finally, we know that the sounds in

678 

 Hannah J. Haynie

words change over time. Over the course of thousands of years, layers of sound change can obscure the common origins of words, creating a time ceiling on the depth of relationship that can be tracked through lexical comparison and reconstruction that may not extend as far into history as some of the relationships of interest in North America (Nichols 1992; Kaufman & Golla 2000; Greenhill et al. 2010).

29.3.2 Grammatical data The other primary source of information about language classification comes from grammatical evidence. ‘Grammatical evidence’ can mean many things (basic grammatical characteristics called typological traits, particular morphosyntactic constructions, forms of grammatical morphemes); the use of grammatical information of various types also has a long history in the classification of North American languages. While some of the earliest work on North American language classification assumed that the grammars found on this continent were not very informative for classification, others engaged in classification in the mid-19th century recognized the utility of comparative syntax and morphology in diagnosing language relationships (Gallatin 1836; Gatschet 1877). While Powell and the BAE team were working with largely lexical datasets to create a complete classification, a competing effort by Daniel Garrison Brinton operated on a principle of emphasizing comparisons of grammatical forms, where possible (Brinton 1891). By the early twentieth century, the notion of a “unity of structure” among North American languages (Gallatin 1836) was falling out of favor, and recognition of grammatical diversity on the continent also generated interest in how this diversity in grammatical structures might aid language classification. Franz Boas adopted a largely areal-typological approach to understanding language relationships, initially using shared morphological characteristics as evidence of genealogical relatedness, and ultimately focusing the bulk of his energies on understanding how grammars are shaped by contact-based relationships (Boas 1911; Boas 1929). An interest in typological diversity was also apparent in the work of Kroeber and Roland B. Dixon, whose early collaboration focused on typological classification of languages in California (Dixon & Kroeber 1903) and whose subsequent work on understanding genealogical relationships between California languages relied considerably on typological features such as the use of case to mark grammatical relations or a lack of plural forms for nouns (Dixon & Kroeber 1913; Dixon & Kroeber 1919). A belief that typological characteristics and grammatical structures may persist in languages even as gradual changes happen in the grammar is also central to the work of Sapir. The deep groupings in Sapir’s “super six” classification of North American languages relied substantially on typological information, but a hallmark of Sapir’s approach is the identification of what he referred to as “submerged features” (Sapir 1925; Sapir 1929). These features involved peculiarities in morphological systems that were recognizable in multiple languages. Because the specific idiosyncrasies involved



Language classification 

 679

in Sapir’s submerged features are unusual, he proposed that their occurrence is an indicator of shared ancestry. This sort of evidence is a particularly convincing source of evidence for relatedness, with a proviso that appropriate care has been taken in identifying features inherited from a common source. Though the structural properties encoded in typological features and shared morphological peculiarities are the sorts of grammatical evidence used most frequently in language classification, other types of grammatical information have been considered. The grammatical evidence in Greenberg’s classification consisted largely of phonetic resemblances between bound morphemes (Greenberg 1987a). Dell Hymes proposed that “positional analysis”, or the comparison of the templates for creating new words out of morphemes could be used to diagnose relationships; however, this practice has not gained wide acceptance (Hymes 1955, 1956; Thomason 1980; but see Vajda 2010a). Whereas layers of sound change are believed to impose a limit on the historical depth at which words can reveal genealogical relationships, grammatical features have been hypothesized to remain stable over longer periods of history, making this sort of evidence an important complement to lexical data (Sapir 1925; Nichols 1992; Nichols 2003; Dunn et al. 2005). Recent work has questioned the extent to which this is true, however, particularly for typological characteristics of language (Dediu & Levinson 2012; Dediu & Cysouw 2013; Greenhill et al. 2017). As with lexical data, care must be taken when working with grammatical data to classify languages. Though some morphological elements may be more resistant to borrowing than words, general typological characteristics are constrained in their variation as well as in the combinations that occur, and some are likely to diffuse through contact (Wichmann & Holman 2009). These considerations cast considerable doubt on the reliability of typological information as evidence of genealogy. Though the striking shared idiosyncrasies used by Sapir are more resistant to the effects of borrowing, chance similarity, and the biases of various analytical traditions than typological features, even these elements of grammar must be treated carefully to avoid misanalysis (Campbell 1997: 221–223).

29.4 Evolving methods in language classification Broadly speaking, language classification involves two primary goals: identifying sets of related languages and establishing the grouping of languages within those sets into better resolved family trees (Voegelin & Voegelin 1965). The methods that have been employed for language classification reflect these two goals, from historical linguistic techniques that target both goals, to modern computational methods that are more specifically tailored to one or the other. Methodological advances and interdisciplinary study have also expanded the set of questions that language classification research can touch upon, from questions about language evolution to understanding prehistoric population movements.

680 

 Hannah J. Haynie

29.4.1 The comparative method The use of basic comparisons for identifying similarities between languages can inform initial hypotheses about relatedness (Powell 1891; Dixon & Kroeber 1913). However, both proving relatedness and working out the subgrouping of families have traditionally been accomplished using a single set of methods, namely the comparative method of historical linguistics (Campbell & Mithun 1979: 49). A brilliant example of the use of this method in the classification of North American languages can be found in the Algic language family. This family includes the Algonquian languages that spread from the Rockies to the Atlantic, as well as the Yurok and Wiyot languages of California (see Oxford, this volume). Similarities among the Algonquian languages, which include some of the first North American languages encountered and documented by Europeans, were quickly identified by colonial language documentarians and assumed to represent a genealogical relationship. Bloomfield’s subsequent use of traditional methods to identify systematic sound correspondences and trace sound changes within Algonquian was an important advance in the classification of North American languages (Bloomfield 1925; Bloomfield 1946). Not only did this work provide concrete, high-quality evidence for the Algonquian relationship and the subgrouping of these languages, it demonstrated that the comparative method could be fruitfully applied to unwritten languages, for which ancient texts cannot be used to inform historical analyses as they are in families like Indo-European. Work on the internal classification of Algonquian and the reconstruction of the parent language has continued ever since (e.  g. Teeter 1965; Goddard 1979; Proulx 2003). The relationships that exist between Yurok, Wiyot, and Algonquian languages were surrounded by controversy from the time that Sapir originally proposed a link between these languages (Sapir 1913). The sound correspondences and morphological evidence that Sapir used to connect Yurok and Wiyot to each other and to Algonquian were not received uncritically. Early debate centered on whether the evidence for the California languages and Algonquian making up a single family could be merely a set of chance resemblances (Michelson 1914). The skepticism with which Sapir’s proposal was received was undoubtedly influenced by the geographic separation between the California Algic languages and their Algonquian relatives. As more documentation of Yurok and Wiyot became available, phonological and morphological evidence for Algic mounted, and ultimately the comparative method triumphed once again in proving the reality of this geographically long-distance relationship (Haas 1958; Goddard 1975).

29.4.2 Evolving perspectives on deep relationships The umbrellas Hokan and Penutian are used to refer to sets of languages thought to share ancestry in the distant past, but both the membership of these stocks and the nature of the ostensible relationships they represent remain uncertain a century after they were



Language classification 

 681

originally introduced (see also Dagostino, this volume). Dixon and Kroeber’s early 20th century hypotheses were expanded and reformulated in the following decades, but at the close of that century, Hokan had contracted back to a California-centric set of languages and ultimately did not find enough traditional comparative evidence to convince most linguists of its status as a family (Dixon & Kroeber 1913; see detailed discussion in Campbell 1997: 290–305). The Penutian hypothesis has faced similar questions about membership and evidence for shared ancestry but fared slightly better with regard to the historical evidence amassed to support relationships within certain subsets of its languages (Dixon & Kroeber 1913; Sapir 1921; DeLancey & Golla 1997; see also discussion in Campbell 1997: 309–322). Callaghan’s work to demonstrate the relationships between Miwok, Ohlone, and perhaps Yokuts languages is representative of the use of traditional methods used to build up from smaller families in the search for evidence of deeper relationships, but at its deepest levels, Penutian remains an unproven hypothesis (Callaghan 1967, 1997, 2013). At the beginning of the 21st century, the evidence for Hokan and Penutian classifications relied nearly as much on inspectional similarity as it had at the beginning of the 20th century. More recently, quantitative refinements of this approach have included the identification of systematic sound correspondence, assessment of statistical significance, and controls for properties of linguistic systems such as phonological inventory size (Kessler 2001; Haynie 2014). An application of such quantitative methods to Hokan and Penutian languages found ample support for the small, well established language families that are included under these two umbrellas, but no significant evidence for systematic sound correspondence patterns among so-called Hokan or Penutian languages at a deeper level (Haynie 2014). Shrouded in controversy but often used carelessly or out of convenience in fields from linguistics to evolutionary biology, the Hokan and Penutian labels are emblematic of the difficulties in proving deep relationships and the perils of proposing distant relationships that may be untestable, given their depth.

29.4.3 Answering questions with new comparative evidence The attraction of scholarly energy to classifying North American languages into deep, stock-level groupings is due in part to the potential for these long-range classifications to help us understand the shared ancestry of human groups and the migration of human populations to North America. Proposals linking North American languages to Eurasia are not new to a field so fascinated by prehistoric origins (Fortescue 1998), but the question of deep prehistoric ancestry and North American migrations from Eurasia has found its most tantalizing clues in recent work on the Dene-Yeniseian hypothesis. Serious inquiry into a possible connection between American Na-Dene languages and the Yeniseian family of central Siberia (of which only the Ket language survives) began with the evidence Merritt Ruhlen put forth for the common ancestry of these families at the end of the 20th century (Ruhlen 1998). The basis for this theoretical deep family

682 

 Hannah J. Haynie

was a set of proposed lexical cognates linking Proto-Yeniseian to Proto-Athabaskan and other nodes of the Na-Dene family tree. Since that time, a leap forward in demonstrating the plausibility of Dene-Yeniseian has been made, largely through the work of Edward Vajda. Combining both a perspective open to the possibility of finding evidence for very ancient ancestral languages, and a classical approach to gathering that evidence, this work put forth a substantial set of data to support an ancient link between these families (Vajda 2010a; Vajda 2010b; Vajda 2013). By pursuing the “gold standard” of historical linguistic evidence, Vajda’s cognate sets, systematic sound correspondences, and shared morphological systems presented a stronger case for this distantly related family than any prior language classification proposal linking American languages to Eurasia. While this evidence has not gone unchallenged (Kari & Potter 2010; Campbell 2011), the research behind the yet-unproven Dene-Yeniseian hypothesis focuses on principles that are more credible than the impressionistic similarities at the heart of prior ancient relationship proposals (Kari & Potter 2010; Rice 2011; Dunn 2012). The temporal and spatial implications of a Dene-Yeniseian family are perhaps the most intriguing, and contentious, part of the proposal. When and where the North American and Eurasian branches split from a common ancestor they may share, and how these distinct linguistic groups may have reached their modern geographies remain the subject of some debate (Sicoli & Holton 2014; Vajda 2018).

29.4.4 Language classification in interdisciplinary research The implication of language classification for human prehistory makes it relevant across disciplines that study the past, such as genetics. For example, Greenberg’s grouping of North American languages into just three families was part of a proposal that sought to correlate linguistic information with genetic and archaeological evidence and was accompanied by explicit claims that its three hypothetical language families reflected corresponding waves of migration from Eurasia (Greenberg et al. 1986; Greenberg 1987a). The three-wave migration model has found both support (Reich et al. 2012) and contradictions (Goebel, Waters & O’Rourke 2008) in genetics and archaeology. However, widespread doubts about the linguistic evidence for a three-family model make the Greenberg classification a dubious yardstick for measuring how well interdisciplinary migration theories stack up against the linguistic evidence (Bolnick et al. 2004). Recent genetic work aimed at understanding how prehistoric migrations may have shaped North American populations has highlighted the importance of involving indigenous communities in research on North American prehistory (Scheib et al. 2018; Posth et al. 2018). At the same time, new research in this area paints a complex picture of movement and admixture, reinforcing the lesson that the distinct histories of genes and languages reflect differences in how biological and cultural evolution are shaped by past human activities.



Language classification 

 683

29.5 Evaluating evidence for language classification Though more empirical data has become available and analytical methods have expanded throughout the history of North American language classification, disagreements and refinements of proposed classifications over the years largely reflect changing attitudes toward two values: laying careful empirical foundations to support hypothesized relationships and advancing bold theories with the intent of breaking new ground. Polarization of viewpoints based on some perceived competition between these values may bear some responsibility for the fact that language classification has not come closer to answering some of the big questions in the field as the available data and methods have evolved. Moving forward, language classification confronts a new set of challenges. Collection of new data has slowed down or become impossible for many North American languages but archiving efforts and digital tools may make the data that exists more accessible and easier to work with for both communities and language classification researchers. Methodological advances in computational phylogenetics may make it possible to rigorously test a wider set of hypotheses but require not only technological expertise but also careful data selection and curation. Interdisciplinary collaborations are likely to help answer some of the big questions in North American language classification, such as origins of language families and Boasian concerns about distinguishing areal relationships from genealogical affinity, but face complex questions about how language, genes, and culture interact in human history. Advancing our understanding of North American language classification in this new landscape will require both open-mindedness toward new ways of evaluating evidence, and careful attention to the linguistic details upon which secure foundations of evidence for language relationships are built.

References Bergsland, Knut & Hans Vogt. On the validity of glottochronology. Current Anthroplogy 3(2). 115–153. Bloomfield, Leonard. 1925. On the sound-system of Central Algonquian. Language 1(4). 130–156. doi:10.2307/409540. Bloomfield, Leonard. 1946. Algonquian. In Harry Hoijer & Cornelius Osgood (eds.), Linguistic Structures of Native America, 85–129. (Viking Fund Publications in Anthropology 6). New York: Viking Fund. Boas, Franz. 1911. Handbook of American Indian Languages. (Bureau of American Ethnology Bulletin 40). Vol. 1. Washington, D.C.: Government Printing Office. Boas, Franz. 1929. Classification of American Indian languages. Language 5(1). 1–7. doi:10.2307/408990. Bolnick, Deborah A. (Weiss), Beth A. (Schultz) Shook, Lyle Campbell & Ives Goddard. 2004. Problematic use of Greenberg’s linguistic classification of the Americas in studies of Native American genetic variation. The American Journal of Human Genetics 75(3). 519–522. doi:10.1086/423452. Brinton, Daniel G. 1891. The American race. New York: D. C. Hodges. Callaghan, Catherine A. 1967. Miwok-Costanoan as a subfamily of Penutian. International Journal of American Linguistics 33(3). 224–227. doi:10.1086/464964.

684 

 Hannah J. Haynie

Callaghan, Catherine A. 1997. Evidence for Yok-Utian. International Journal of American Linguistics 63(1). 18–64. doi:10.1086/466313. Callaghan, Catherine A. 2013. Proto Utian grammar and dictionary: With notes on Yokuts. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. Campbell, Lyle. 1997. American Indian languages: The historical linguistics of Native America. Oxford University Press. Campbell, Lyle. 2011. The Dene–Yeniseian connection (Kari and Potter, eds.). International Journal of American Linguistics 77(3). 445–451. doi:10.1086/660977. Campbell, Lyle & Marianne Mithun. 1979. The languages of Native America: Historical and comparative assessment. Austin: University of Texas Press. Campbell, Lyle & Wlliam J. Poser. 2008. Language classification: History and method. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Chacon, Thiago Costa & Johann-Mattis List. 2015. Improved computational models of sound change shed light on the history of Tukanoan languages. Journal of Language Relationship 13(3). 177–203. Dediu, Dan & Michael Cysouw. 2013. Some structural aspects of language are more stable than others: A comparison of seven methods. PLOS ONE 8(1). e55009. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0055009. Dediu, Dan & Stephen C. Levinson. 2012. Abstract Profiles of Structural Stability Point to Universal Tendencies, Family-Specific Factors, and Ancient Connections between Languages. PLoS ONE 7(9). e45198. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0045198. Dixon, Roland B. & Alfred L. Kroeber. 1903. The native languages of California. American Anthropologist 5(1). 1–26. Dixon, Roland B. & Alfred L. Kroeber. 1913. New linguistic families in California. American Anthropologist 15(4). 647–655. doi:10.1525/aa.1913.15.4.02a00050. Dixon, Roland B. & Alfred L. Kroeber. 1919. Linguistic families of California. (University of California Publications in American Archaeology and Ethnology 16). Berkeley: University of California Press. Dunn, Michael. 2012. Review of Anthropological papers of the University of Alaska: The Dene-Yeniseian connection. Language 88(2). 429–432. Dunn, Michael, Angela Terrill, Ger Reesink, Robert A. Foley & Stephen C. Levinson. 2005. Structural phylogenetics and the reconstruction of ancient language history. Science 309(5743). 2072–2075. doi:10.1126/science.1114615. Duponceau, Peter Stephen. 1838. Mémoire sur le système grammatical des langues de quelques nations indiennes de l’Amérique du Nord; ouvrage qui, à la séance publique annuelle de l’Institut Royal de France, le 2 mai 1835, a remporté le prix fondé par M. le comte de Volney. Paris: Pihan de la Forest. Eberhard, David M., Gary F. Simons & Charles D. Fennig (eds.). 2019. Ethnologue: Languages of the world. Twenty-second edition. Dallas, TX: SIL International. https://www.ethnologue.com/ (4 May, 2019). Fortescue, Michael. 1998. Language relations across the Bering Strait: Reappraising the archaeological and linguistic evidence. London: Cassell. Fowler, Don D. & Catherine S. Fowler. 1969. John Wesley Powell, anthropologist. Utah Historical Quarterly 37(2). 152–172. Gallatin, Albert. 1836. A synopsis of the Indian tribes within the United States east of the Rocky Mountains: and in the British and Russian possessions in North America. New York: AMS Press. Gatschet, Albert S. 1877. Indian languages of the Pacific states and territories. In William Wallace Beach (ed.), The Indian miscellany, 416–451. Albany: Munsell. Goddard, Ives. 1975. Algonquian, Wiyot, and Yurok: Proving a distant genetic relationship. In Marvin Dale Kinkade, Kenneth Locke Hale & Oswald Werner (eds.), Linguistics and Anthropology: In Honor of C. F. Voegelin, 249–262. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing. Goddard, Ives. 1979. Comparative Algonquian. In Lyle Campbell & Marianne Mithun (eds.), The languages of Native America: Historical and comparative assessment, 70–132. Austin: University of Texas Press.



Language classification 

 685

Goddard, Ives. 1996. The classification of the native languages of North America. In Ives Goddard (ed.), Handbook of North American Indians, Volume 17: Languages, 290–323. Washington: Smithsonian Institution. Goebel, T., M. R. Waters & D. H. O’Rourke. 2008. The Late Pleistocene dispersal of modern humans in the Americas. Science 319(5869). 1497–1502. doi:10.1126/science.1153569. Gray, Russell D. & Quentin Atkinson. 2003. Language-tree divergence times support the Anatolian theory of Indo-European origin. Nature 426. 435–439. Greenberg, Joseph H. 1987a. Language in the Americas. Stanford University Press. Greenberg, Joseph H. 1987b. Author’s precis of Language in the Americas. Current Anthropology 28(5). 647–652. Greenberg, Joseph H., Christy G. Turner, Stephen L. Zegura, Lyle Campbell, James A. Fox, W. S. Laughlin, Kenneth M. Weiss & Ellen Woolford. 1986. The settlement of the Americas: A comparison of the linguistic, dental, and genetic evidence [and comments and reply]. Current Anthropology 27(5). 477–497. doi:10.1086/203472. Greenhill, Simon J., Quentin D. Atkinson, Andrew Meade & Russell D. Gray. 2010. The shape and tempo of language evolution. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 277(1693). 2443–2450. doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.0051. Greenhill, Simon J., Chieh-Hsi Wu, Xia Hua, Michael Dunn, Stephen C. Levinson & Russell D. Gray. 2017. Evolutionary dynamics of language systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 201700388. doi:10.1073/pnas.1700388114. Greenhill, Simon J., Hannah J. Haynie, Robert M. Ross, Angela Chira, Johann-Mattis List, Lyle Campbell, Carlos A. Botero & Russell D. Gray. A recent northern origin for the Uto-Aztecan language family. Language 99(1), 1–27. Haas, Mary R. 1958. Algonkian-Ritwan: The end of a controversy. International Journal of American Linguistics 24(3). 159–173. Hammarström, Harald, Robert Forkel & Martin Haspelmath (eds.). 2019. Glottolog 4.0. Jena: Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History. https://glottolog.org/ (2 May, 2019). Haynie, Hannah J. 2014. Deep relationships among California languages. Diachronica 31(3). 407–447. doi:10.1075/dia.31.3.04hay. Hymes, Dell H. 1955. Positional analysis of categories: A frame for reconstruction. Word 11(1). 10–23. Hymes, Dell H. 1956. Na-Déné and positional analysis of categories. American Anthropologist 58(4). 624–638. Kari, James & Ben A. Potter. 2010. The Dene-Yeneseian connection. (Anthropological Papers of the University of Alaska 5). Fairbanks: University of Alaska Department of Anthropology. Kaufman, Terrence & Victor Golla. 2000. Language groupings in the new world: Their reliability and usability in cross-disciplinary studies. In Colin Renfrew (ed.), America past, America present: Genes and languages in the Americas and beyond, 47–57. Cambridge: The McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research. Kessler, Brett. 2001. The significance of word lists. Chicago: CSLI Publications. Krauss, Michael E. 1976. Na-Dene. In Thomas A. Sebeok (ed.), Native languages of the Americas, vol. 1, 283–358. New York: Plenum Press. Kroeber, Alfred L. 1940. Conclusions: the present status of Americanistic problems. In Clarence L. Hay, Ralph L. Linton, Samuel K. Lothrop, Harry L. Shapiro & George C. Vaillant (eds.), The Maya and their neighbors: Essays on Middle American anthropology and archaeology, 460–487. New York: D. Appleton-Century. Levine, Robert D. 1979. Haida and Na-Dene: A new look at the evidence. International Journal of American Linguistics 45(2). 157–170. doi:10.1086/465587. Meillet, Antoine. 1925. La méthode comparative en linguistique historique. Paris: Champion. Michael, Lev, Natalia Chousou-Polydouri, Keith Bartolomei, Erin Donnelly, Sérgio Meira, Vivian Wauters & Zachary O’Hagan. 2015. A Bayesian phylogenetic classification of Tupí-Guaraní. LIAMES: Línguas Indígenas Americanas 15(2). 193–221. doi:10.20396/liames.v15i2.8642301.

686 

 Hannah J. Haynie

Michelson, Truman. 1914. Two alleged Algonquian languages of California. American Anthropologist 16(2). 361–367. doi:10.1525/aa.1914.16.2.02a00150. Mithun, Marianne. 1999. The languages of native North America. Cambridge University Press. Nichols, Johanna. 1992. Linguistic Diversity in Space and Time. University of Chicago Press. Nichols, Johanna. 2003. Diversity and stability in language. In Brian D. Joseph & Richard D. Janda (eds.), Handbook of Historical Linguistics, 283–310. London: Blackwell. Posth, Cosimo, Nathan Nakatsuka, Iosif Lazaridis, Pontus Skoglund, Swapan Mallick, Thiseas C. Lamnidis, Nadin Rohland, et al. 2018. Reconstructing the deep population history of Central and South America. Cell 175(5). 1185–1197.e22. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2018.10.027. Powell, John Wesley. 1880. Introduction to the study of American Indian languages, with words, phrases, and sentences to be collected. 2nd edn. Washington, D.C.: Government Printing Office. Powell, John Wesley. 1891. Indian linguistic families of America: North of Mexico. (Annual Report of the Bureau of Ethnology). Vol. 7. Washington, D.C.: United States Government Printing Office. Proulx, Paul. 2003. The evidence on Algonquian genetic grouping: A matter of relative chronology. Anthropological Linguistics 45(2). 201–225. Reich, David, Nick Patterson, Desmond Campbell, Arti Tandon, Stéphane Mazieres, Nicolas Ray, Maria V. Parra, et al. 2012. Reconstructing Native American population history. Nature 488(7411). 370–374. doi:10.1038/nature11258. Rice, Keren. 2011. Dene-Yeniseian. Diachronica 28(2). 255–271. Rigsby, Bruce J. 1966. On Cayuse-Molala relatability. International Journal of American Linguistics 32(4). 369–378. doi:10.1086/464926. Ruhlen, Merritt. 1998. The origin of the Na-Dene. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 95(23). 13994–13996. doi:10.1073/pnas.95.23.13994. Sapir, Edward. 1913. Wiyot and Yurok, Algonkin languages of California. American Anthropologist 15(4). 617–646. doi:10.1525/aa.1913.15.4.02a00040. Sapir, Edward. 1920. The Hokan and Coahuiltecan languages. International Journal of American Linguistics 1(4). 280–290. doi:10.1086/463726. Sapir, Edward. 1921. Edward Sapir’s map of linguistic groups north of Mexico. Gatineau, Quebec, Canada: Canadian Museum of History. Ms. I-A-264M. Image Number E2006-04688. Sapir, Edward. 1925. The Hokan affinity of Subtiaba in Nicaragua. American Anthropologist 27(3). 402–435. doi:10.1525/aa.1925.27.3.02a00040. Sapir, Edward. 1929. Central and North American languages. Encyclopaedia Britannica. 14th Edition. Scheib, C. L., Hongjie Li, Tariq Desai, Vivian Link, Christopher Kendall, Genevieve Dewar, Peter William Griffith, et al. 2018. Ancient human parallel lineages within North America contributed to a coastal expansion. Science 360(6392). 1024–1027. doi:10.1126/science.aar6851. Shaul, David L. 1999. Linguistic natural history: John Wesley Powell and the classification of American languages. Journal of the Southwest 41(3). 297–310. Sicoli, Mark A. & Gary Holton. 2014. Linguistic phylogenies support back-migration from Beringia to Asia. PLOS ONE 9(3). e91722. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0091722. Swadesh, Morris. 1952. Lexicostatistic dating of prehistoric ethnic contacts. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society 96. 452–463. Swadesh, Morris. 1954. Time depths of American linguistic groupings. American Anthropologist 56(3). 361–377. doi:10.1525/aa.1954.56.3.02a00060. Swadesh, Morris. 1971. The origin and diversification of language. Edited by Joel Sherzer. Chicago: Aldine. Teeter, Karl V. 1965. The Algonquian verb: Notes toward a reconsideration. International Journal of American Linguistics 31(3). 221–225. Thomason, Sarah Grey. 1980. Morphological instability, with and without language contact. In Jacek Fisiak (ed.), Historical morphology, 359–372. The Hague: Mouton.



Language classification 

 687

Vajda, Edward. 2010a. A Siberian link with Na-Dene languages. In James Kari & Ben A. Potter (eds.), The Dene-Yeniseian Connection, 33–99. (Anthropological Papers of the University of Alaska 5). Fairbanks: University of Alaska Department of Anthropology. Vajda, Edward. 2010b. Yeniseian, Na-Dene, and historical linguistics. In James Kari & Ben A. Potter (eds.), The Dene-Yeniseian Connection, 100–118. (Anthropological Papers of the University of Alaska 5). Fairbanks: University of Alaska Department of Anthropology. Vajda, Edward. 2013. Vestigial possessive morphology in Na-Dene and Yeniseian. In Sharon Hargus, Edward Vajda & Daniel Hieber (eds.), Working Papers in Athabaskan (Dene) Languages 2012, 79–91. (Alaska Native Language Center Working Papers 11). Fairbanks: Alaska Native Language Center. Vajda, Edward. 2018. Dene-Yeniseian: Progress and unanswered questions. Diachronica 35(2). 277–295. doi:10.1075/dia.18001.vaj. Voegelin, Carl F. & Florence M. Voegelin. 1964. Languages of the world: Native America fascicle one (Anthropological Linguistics). Vol. 6. Bloomington, IN: Anthropology Department, Indiana University. Voegelin, Carl F. & Florence M. Voegelin. 1965. Languages of the world--Native America fascicle two (Anthropological Linguistics). Vol. 7. Bloomington, IN: Anthropology Department, Indiana University. Walker, Robert S. & Lincoln A. Ribeiro. 2011. Bayesian phylogeography of the Arawak expansion in lowland South America. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 278(1718). 2562–2567. doi:10.1098/ rspb.2010.2579. Wichmann, Søren & Eric Holman. 2009. Temporal stability of linguistic typological features. (LINCOM Studies in Theoretical Linguistics). Munich: LINCOM Europa.

Justin Spence

30 Archival-based sociolinguistic variation Abstract: Dialect and other kinds of sociolinguistic variation in Native American languages can often be understood through careful examination of the cumulative documentary record. Despite superficial differences among sources that often give the appearance of more variation than actually existed in the past, primary sources in archival collections can significantly enrich the information available in published material. This can be especially important for critically endangered and dormant languages, since opportunities to explore variation in the present are otherwise limited. Understanding sociolinguistic variation in this way can support language reclamation efforts, with differences among particular speakers documented in the past offering insights into the rich texture of a language when it was still in everyday use in a community. It can also be relevant for academically oriented linguistic research, shedding light on topics such as language attitudes and ideologies, multilingualism and language contact, and linguistic changes in progress as they unfold in the documentary record across different decades.

30.1 Introduction No two speakers of a language talk exactly the same way, but they may have shared tendencies based on similarities in where they grew up, when they were born, their racial, ethnic, or gender identities, and a host of other factors. For example, we might infer that someone comes from a particular geographic region based on how they talk, and we can do this because we recognize that other people from the same region tend to talk the same way. Across a speech community as a whole, these kinds of similarities – and the corresponding differences they imply – can be conceived of as linguistic resources that people deploy, whether consciously or unconsciously, as they navigate their social environment (Eckert 2008). This kind of sociolinguistic variation, a broad cover term for different ways of talking based on aspects of social organization, has been studied extensively in English and a few other politically dominant languages of the world. However, a growing body of research emphasizes its importance in less-studied minority languages as well (Hill 2001; Stanford and Preston 2009; Hildebrandt, Jany, and Silva 2017; Palakurthy, this volume). These studies make the case that variation is an important feature of all human languages and hence deserves a central place in ongoing documentation efforts aspiring to completeness. This also has implications for language reclamation (in the sense of Leonard [2017]), where understanding the social ramifications of using a language (what ways of talking are typical and appropriate in particular circumstances, and for whom) may be as important as structural considerations like grammar, vocabulary, and the like. Moreover, as discussed by Ahlers (2014), https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-030

690 

 Justin Spence

community attitudes about variation can have a large impact on how the process and outcomes of language reclamation are perceived. However, it can be difficult to study sociolinguistic variation in Native American languages due to the same historical and contemporary disruptions motivating language reclamation work in the first place. As discussed by Lukaniec (this volume), many Native American languages do not currently have fluent first-language speakers. Research on these languages – generally referred to as dormant or sleeping – must initially rely on a finite body of documentation created in the past, one that cannot be independently verified. Even where a language is not dormant, people often turn to legacy documentation as a way to supplement ongoing work with living speakers.1 Unfortunately, items in the documentary record, taken individually, are often ill-suited to sustaining research on variation. Sometimes this is due to practical considerations surrounding the circumstances of their creation – a researcher may have been able to work with only one speaker rather than an entire speech community, for example. Other times it might be due to broader theoretical currents in disciplines such as linguistics. If a language is understood as a uniform system of shared knowledge distributed evenly across an idealized speech community, variation may be overlooked or written off as aberrant, especially in published accounts (Goddard 2010). Instead, in many cases sociolinguistic variation will be represented only indirectly in the sum total of documentation, reflecting the fact that language documentation is cumulative, a term that Kari (2019) has emphasized in recent work. Building on earlier discussions by Goddard (1976) and Conathan (2006), this chapter considers how careful examination of the cumulative documentary record of a language, especially unpublished primary sources held by archives, can shed considerable light on sociolinguistic variation. Many examples illustrating particular points will be drawn from languages of California, especially those in the Dene (Athabaskan) language family, which the author is most familiar with. However, the chapter also highlights general principles that could be adapted to other regions and circumstances. Much of the discussion centers around written documents that were transcribed by outsiders to the speech community in consultation with one or more speakers of a given language. This is not to discount legacy documentation involving other media (audio and video recordings) or materials that were created by community members themselves. Instead, it is a common scenario that presents special problems: transcriptions cannot be verified against a recording, and one cannot assume that the speech community’s interests were taken into account as part of the documentation process.

1 Some of the issues under discussion are problems for older sources whether or not they happen to have been published, especially with regard to access and the interpretation of unreliable transcriptions. Accordingly, I will sometimes conflate older published and archival documentation as “legacy.”



Archival-based sociolinguistic variation 

 691

30.2 Relevance for language reclamation As discussed in detail by Lukaniec (this volume), archival documents and other legacy materials are indispensable resources for the reclamation of dormant Native American languages. Understanding sociolinguistic variation plays an important role in these efforts. This is partly a practical matter: if the same word is written two different ways, for example, it is helpful to know whether the difference is accidental, is conditioned by grammatical context, or reflects something about the speaker that has social significance. This is especially important as raw documentation is transformed into resources intended for a broad audience. Presenting the full range of variability in the documentary record may overwhelm new learners, but dispensing with it entirely runs the risk of promoting a caricature of a language that people might not find satisfying in the long run. Identifying systematic and socially significant variation is a step towards making informed decisions in this regard. For example, Rice and Saxon (2002) discuss the importance of understanding dialect variation when deciding how to spell words in a standard way so that language resources like dictionaries can be shared among different communities. Their discussion centers on Dene (Athabaskan) languages of western Canada where there are still many speakers of each dialect, but similar issues must be considered when the variation is encountered in archival and other legacy documentation. Emphasizing the value of sociolinguistic variation in language reclamation can also counterbalance a tendency in linguistic research to treat languages as decontextualized objects (Leonard 2017). According to such views, languages are abstract systems of knowledge facilitating communication between arbitrary individuals whose identities are largely irrelevant. By contrast, Native American language reclamation is often a much more intimate endeavor (“emotionally saturated” in the sense of Webster [2015]). This work may be motivated, initially at least, by a desire to connect with particular individuals – perhaps a parent or grandparent who dedicated themselves, even in difficult circumstances, to ensuring that their knowledge would be available for future generations (Miranda 2013). From this perspective, individual speakers are not simply interchangeable: who said what, and how they said it, matters a great deal. Coming to grips with variation in legacy documentation thus becomes more than a fact of life that must be endured; rather, it is a way to honor the contributions of speakers by ensuring that their voices will still be heard in the embodied present. For dormant languages especially, it is a way of recovering some measure of the rich texture of a language as it was known and spoken before the community’s shift to another language occurred. This encourages engagement with the sociocultural norms at play in the moment when the documentation was created, supporting broader goals related to decolonization that underpin language reclamation efforts (as defined by Leonard [2017]).

692 

 Justin Spence

30.3 Challenges Researching sociolinguistic variation based on the cumulative documentation of a language presents a number of challenges, some of them related to gaining access to materials in the first place. It can be extremely time-consuming and expensive to track down archival resources, which may be scattered across several repositories in far-flung locations. The organization and description of materials can be misleading, making them hard to find and creating the illusion of either more or less variation than actually exists – if dialects of a single language are classified as separate languages, for example, or vice-versa. Legacy published materials may be out of print and require affiliation with a research library to access them. Institutional policies designed to preserve materials may inadvertently make some potential users feel unwelcome and discourage their engagement with collections. Some of these barriers to access are gradually improving as more institutions come to recognize their responsibilities to the communities whose languages are represented in their collections (Holton 2012), but much remains to be done. The eclectic nature of the cumulative documentary record can create even more daunting challenges. The documentation of most languages will have been collected piecemeal over the course of decades or even centuries by a number of different people with varying degrees of skill as transcribers, diverse research goals, and transcriptional practices that may not align well with expectations of contemporary linguistic scholarship. Thus, while individual items in the documentary record typically don’t include enough information to explore sociolinguistic variation in much detail, the cumulative record presents the opposite extreme: often there are too many potential points of variation to evaluate, rather than too few. This makes it hard to distinguish socially significant differences from superficial ones, such as divergent transcription conventions, transient speech errors produced by speakers, or errors on the part of transcribers. That is, there is so much variability in available sources that patterned sociolinguistic variation becomes hard to discern. The philological methods outlined in the next section offer a way to address this issue.

30.4 Interpreting documents In a general overview of the use of written documents to study sociolinguistic variation, Schneider (2008) draws a useful distinction between validity (the extent to which written documents can be taken as faithful representations of a language) versus representativeness (whether a set of documents can be generalized to draw inferences about a speech community as a whole). When legacy documentation was collected before the advent of modern linguistic field methods, and without the benefit of corresponding audio recordings, validity is a major consideration. The methods required to approach



Archival-based sociolinguistic variation 

 693

these problems are known as philology, defined by Goddard as “that part of the discipline of linguistics that is concerned with getting from texts and other recorded attestations of languages systematic information that is not directly conveyed by such records as they stand” (1976: 73). It is a means to determine “just what the facts of the primary record are, […] the degree to which the record is dependable, and […] exactly what can or cannot be concluded from it” (1976: 74). The application of philological principles can often distinguish patterned sociolinguistic variation from the more accidental kinds of variability due to other factors mentioned in § 2. One of the most important principles is to determine the extent to which observed differences are systematic. Something occurring once in a single source might simply be an error or anomaly, but this is unlikely if it is found multiple times and in multiple sources. Systematic patterns can be discovered through careful comparison of different materials, what Broadbent (1957) called “reconstitution” – the idea being that triangulating a number of potentially inaccurate transcriptions can provide a reasonable approximation of what the correct transcription(s) are likely to have been. By eliminating some of the transcriptional clutter in the data, reconstitution can identify a residue of differences that are candidates for sociolinguistic explanation. It is also important to understand as much as possible about not only the speakers whose languages are transcribed, but about the transcribers as well: this can provide important clues about how to interpret their what they wrote down. Early transcribers often represented unfamiliar sounds in Native American languages using spelling conventions from languages they already knew. Hinton (1979), for example, discusses an anomalous at the end of syllables in a 19th century transcription of words in Mojave, a Yuman language of the Colorado River in southern California.2 Hinton argues that this probably reflects vowel length as rendered by a speaker of an American English dialect where r in words like tar typically is not pronounced. In other words, the appearance of the letter in these documents does not indicate that speakers actually produced an r sound. Instead, it reflects something about the linguistic background of a transcriber who was improvising a way of writing unfamiliar sounds. As Chelliah and de Reuse (2011: 95) point out, perhaps the best way to learn philological methods is by examining a number of studies where they are applied. There are many excellent descriptive materials for Native American languages based on archival sources, most of which discuss how the authors approached philological interpretation of the available documentation. Recent examples from northern California include Chimariko (Jany 2009), Mutsun (Warner, Butler, and Geary 2016), Yuki (Balodis 2016), Wailaki (Begay 2017), Southern Pomo (Walker 2020), and Patwin (Lawyer 2021). Dialect 2 Angle brackets indicate letters as they appear in source documents, whereas square brackets […] are used for the sounds that the letters are intended to represent. To avoid a proliferation of specialized symbols, I am conflating phonetic and phonemic representations and using square brackets for both. Page numbers for unpublished documents refer to digital copies that include covers and, in some cases, microfilm metadata frames.

694 

 Justin Spence

variation in particular is featured in some of these studies. Balodis (2016), for example, identifies a number of systematic differences between Yuki and the related Coast Yuki and Huchnom dialects, and Lawyer (2021) provides detailed information about the Hill, River, and Southern dialects of Patwin. Warner, Butler, and Luna-Costillas (2006) contains an especially clear discussion of the principles guiding their interpretations of Mutsun documentation. These studies also provide excellent models for documenting the source of particular examples, enabling readers to explore points of potential variation on their own and to confirm the accuracy of philological interpretation.

30.5 Enriching the published record For perfectly understandable reasons, many of the messy details typically found in field notes and other archival materials may be backgrounded or omitted entirely when they are prepared for publication (Broadbent 1957). But normalizing published data in this way risks leaving out information that can help identify systematic variation. Examining archival sources is a way to recover some of this information. As an example, consider the following case from Hupa, a Dene language of northwestern California. Hupa is typically described as having at least two main dialects: Hupa proper from the lower Trinity River in Hoopa Valley, and one or more varieties spoken along Redwood Creek and in the region near present-day Blue Lake (Golla 2011: 76–78).3 A specific difference between these dialects is found in a collection of texts transcribed by Pliny Earle Goddard (1914), who discusses a pronunciation difference involving words pronounced with the back of the tongue near the roof of the mouth in Hupa on the lower Trinity (known as a “voiceless velar fricative” [x] in linguistics). In the Redwood Creek dialect, the same words are pronounced with a noisy k sound (an “aspirated velar stop” [kʰ]). Goddard transcribes these sounds with the letters and , respectively. However, close examination of the Redwood Creek texts reveals inconsistencies in this regard: words are sometimes transcribed using or , other times with , as in xōn ta ~ kʻōn ta ‘house’, or a third person possessive prefix (‘his/her’) transcribed as xō or kʻō. There are even cases where both and appear, so-called “interdialect forms,” such as xō kʻōn tau ‘his house’. Goddard’s published statement concerning the ~ variation in Hupa dialects was based on work with just one elderly speaker, a woman identified only as the wife of a man named Molasses. Without further information, the question of representativeness looms large: was this typical of the Redwood Creek dialect in general, or just of this one person? As discussed in Spence (2013, ch. 3), archival documentation illuminates

3 It is important to note that Hupa is not a dormant language, and some people living today remember differences between dialects. There is an ongoing discussion within the community today about how contemporary variation relates to these older dialect differences.



Archival-based sociolinguistic variation 

 695

the situation. Goddard’s (1906–1907) Redwood Creek field notebooks indicate that the variation is even more extreme than what appears in the published edition. On one notebook page, for example, Goddard originally transcribed ‘house’ as xōn ta but then overwrites with (Na20g.2, notebook 4, page 60). This word was published as kʻon ta (1914: 326), omitting the alternative with that was transcribed first. A few lines later on the same notebook page, Goddard transcribes ‘his house’ as xō kon tau. Here, Goddard wrote underneath original in the prefix, a modification in the opposite direction, from to . This also was not reproduced when the word was published as xō kʻōn tau. Transcriptions created by other researchers (Merriam 1850–1974, reel 30; Reichard 1922; Woodward 1953) confirm that the sound transcribed by Goddard was a general feature of the Redwood Creek dialect. Moreover, Merriam’s notes reflect a similar kind of variation as found in Goddard’s texts: the same word is sometimes transcribed with written alongside (equivalent to Goddard’s ), sometimes with corrections made to individual words. That is, the understanding that emerges from the archival data is one in which speakers of the Redwood Creek dialect of Hupa produce [x] or [kʰ] variably. Similar variation is not documented among speakers of the lower Trinity Hupa dialect.

30.6 Language attitudes Identifying parameters of variation like [x] ~ [kʰ] in Hupa dialects is one thing; understanding their social significance is another matter, in many ways much more difficult. This relates to the problem of representativeness mentioned above (Schneider 2008): if only a small number of speakers were documented, it may be difficult to infer which potential social correlates of variation are relevant to explaining their distribution. Moreover, the social correlates themselves may be difficult to discern, since the historical and ethnographic record may be even more fragmentary and subject to inaccuracies than the linguistic record. A major source of historical documentation in many reservation communities in the United States, for example, is the voluminous reports and letters produced by bureaucrats employed by the Bureau of Indian Affairs. Their overriding goals were often hostile to the interests of the communities they were engaged with and therefore cannot be considered entirely reliable depictions of Native American peoples’ social realities. Ethnographers and other academics working in Native American communities in the 20th century tended to be more sympathetic in this regard, but a large body of scholarship emanating from Native American/Indigenous Studies has called into question the accuracy of their accounts as well (see the foundational critiques of Deloria 1969 and Forbes 1982). Thus, while historical and ethnographic documentation contains valuable information, it must be read with a great deal of caution. Nonetheless, variation encountered in the linguistic record can sometimes be connected to statements about its social relevance. Returning to the case of [x] ~ [kʰ] var-

696 

 Justin Spence

iation in Hupa dialects, Goddard also discussed Redwood Creek people who had been relocated to the Hoopa Valley Reservation in the mid-19th century, observing that they “were with the Hupa when young and use the Hupa pronunciation” (1914: 291). His account suggests that differences between dialects were reduced in the reservation context, and in favor of the lower Trinity dialect, where the Redwood Creek population was in the minority. This outcome aligns with descriptions of the relative status of Hupa vs. Redwood Creek in the late 19th and early 20th centuries. Powers (1877: 73) suggests that lower Trinity Hupa was “the French of the reservation, the idiom of diplomacy and of intercourse between tribes,” including the Redwood Creek groups. Powers was prone to exaggeration, but his comments can be compared with statements from Molly and Likaunt, two Redwood Creek speakers from the Blue Lake region interviewed by Gladys Reichard in the 1920s. Reichard paraphrases them as saying that “their dialect is like Hupa but not so ‘fancy’, they consider it more original, more ‘right’” (1922: 18). Although “fancy” here is not used in an entirely positive sense, it does suggest that there was a perception of Hupa as a prestige variety vis-à-vis Redwood Creek dialects. From the other direction, Wallace quotes a Hupa consultant recalling how Redwood Creek people “spoke funny. Their words were a little different from ours,” which Wallace describes as “a constant source of merriment” (1953: 139). Collectively, these statements offer a glimpse of language attitudes that likely influenced the direction of the dialect leveling described by Goddard. Understanding such attitudes, both historically and in contemporary times, can be highly relevant to the design of language reclamation projects (Ahlers 2014). Importantly, comments like these are often found in unpublished field notes and in published sources only tangentially related to language per se.

30.7 Multilingualism and language contact Although many studies of variation have focused on communities with largely monolingual populations, variation in multilingual communities is a topic of interest in sociolinguistic research (Ravindranath 2015). Conathan (2006) considers how archival documents can provide crucial information in this regard. Conathan points out that many speakers of Native American languages in northwestern California worked as linguistic consultants for multiple languages, but individual published accounts might mention a person’s command of other languages only in passing. Individuals’ multilingual abilities are often most apparent in unpublished field notes, which thus provide important clues about the nature and extent of multilingualism within their communities. Multilingualism is also a central part of Drechsel’s (1997) description of Mobilian Jargon, a contact language formerly used in a wide region of the southeastern United States. Drechsel considers archival accounts by European and American observers of where and by whom the Jargon was used, developing what he calls an “ethnohistory of speaking” for the language. Drechsel explains the high degree of variation in Mobilian Jargon as attributable to



Archival-based sociolinguistic variation 

 697

the influence of the primary languages of the diverse Native American groups who spoke it. The dynamics of language contact and multilingualism are also invoked in Scollon’s (1979) examination of Dëne Sųłıné (Chipewyan). Scollon suggests that pervasive variation involving consonants such as [s] and [š] (similar to sh in English), found in more than two centuries of documentation, is due to the influence of the Algonquian language Cree. Scollon’s arguments that variation is a stable and pervasive feature of Dëne Sųłıné are echoed in Spence’s (2016) study of new Hupa words designating technologies introduced since the mid-19th century. The high degree of variation in this domain is interpreted as a continuation of a longstanding tendency in the language rather than a consequence of imminent language shift. In a similar vein, Goddard (2010: 26) reinterprets variation encountered in his own field notes to develop an account of “personal dialects” of Moraviantown Delaware (Munsee), considering a high degree of individual variation a “key element of the sociolinguistic landscape of the community” and “a critical component of any linguistic description as well.” This perspective is especially important for language reclamation, since it implies that differences among speakers cannot be attributed automatically to the existence of normative standards that some speakers fail to live up to. Moreover, in some communities, speaker variation introduced as part of second language acquisition might fall within the bounds of variation encountered among previous generations of speakers, even if the details are different. A solid understanding of sociolinguistic variation in earlier periods, as reflected in older documentation, can contribute to contemporary conversations surrounding the status of variation in reclamation contexts.

30.8 Changes in progress When a language changes in some way, there is typically a transitional period when both the old way of talking and the new way exist alongside one another. The innovation and subsequent spread of such changes are sociolinguistic in nature. Cumulatively, archival documentation collected in different decades can reveal changes in a language as they unfold in real time, a point developed by both Goddard (1976) and Conathan (2006). In some cases, it is possible to connect these changes to aspects of social organization in the communities where the changes have occurred. Hinton (1979, 1981) discusses a particular change in the Yuman languages of southern California and Arizona: an earlier [s] (“alveolar,” produced with the tip of the tongue against the roof of the mouth) changes to [θ] (“interdental,” produced with the tip of the tongue between the teeth), and an earlier [ṣ], produced with the tip of the tongue farther back in the mouth, changes to [s], with the tip of the tongue farther forward. Hinton demonstrates that this sound change occurred between 1870 and 1890: early documentation does not have evidence of it; documentation from between 1870 and 1890 shows variation indicating that the change was occurring during those decades; documentation after 1890 shows that the

698 

 Justin Spence

shift was complete. She moreover suggests that the innovation might have started with Irataba, a particularly influential Mojave leader, possibly due to his exposure to English. Hinton’s analysis may be open to other interpretations: whether or not the change was due to people imitating a single individual with elevated status in the community, or whether contact with English is the ultimate source of the innovation. Nonetheless, this is a compelling example of using the historical record (contact in the reservation setting) to explain the social motivation of a sound change observed in archival language documentation.

30.9 Conclusion This chapter has illustrated some ways that sociolinguistic variation can be recovered from the cumulative documentary record of a language. This can be done productively even with the kinds of language documentation produced in decades and centuries past: eclectic materials that usually were not created with the study of variation in mind. This is an important point, since it provides a justification for the more systematic approaches to language documentation that many people are adopting today. If unanticipated research questions can be answered successfully with a set of legacy materials collected haphazardly in the past, so much the better if today’s documentation is created with a greater forethought and planning. Moreover, although the discussion here has focused on language documentation produced in the pre-digital era, it is worth pausing to consider the methods that will someday be required in order to make meaningful use of the documentation being created today (i.  e., the legacy documentation of tomorrow). Ultimately, understanding sociolinguistic variation in archival language documentation offers insight into how a language was experienced by people who spoke it in the past – offering guidance to those who will continue to speak it in the future.

References Ahlers, Jocelyn C. 2014. Linguistic variation and time travel: Barrier, or border-crossing? Language & Communication 38. 33–43. Balodis, Uldis. 2016. Yuki grammar with sketches of Huchnom and Coast Yuki. (University of California Publications in Linguistics 151). Berkeley: University of California Press. Begay, Kayla Rae. 2017. Wailaki grammar. Berkeley, CA: UC Berkeley dissertation. Broadbent, Sylvia M. 1957. Rumsen I: Methods of reconstitution. International Journal of American Linguistics 23(4). 275–280. Chelliah, Shobhana & Willem J. de Reuse. 2011. Handbook of descriptive linguistic fieldwork. New York: Springer. Conathan, Lisa. 2006. Recovering sociolinguistic context from early sources: The case of northwestern California. Anthropological Linguistics 48(3). 209–232. Deloria, Vine Jr. 1969. Custer died for your sins: An Indian manifesto. New York: Macmillan.



Archival-based sociolinguistic variation 

 699

Drechsel, Emanuel J. 1997. Mobilian Jargon: Linguistic and sociohistorical aspects of a Native American pidgin. Oxford: Clarendon Press. Eckert, Penelope. 2008. Variation and the indexical field. Journal of Sociolinguistics 12(4). 453–476. Forbes, Jack. 1982. Native Americans of California and Nevada. Happy Camp, CA: Naturegraph Publishers. Goddard, Ives. 1976. Philological approaches to the study of North American languages: Documents and documentation. In Thomas A. Sebeok (ed.), Native languages of the Americas, vol. 1, 73–91. New York: Plenum Press. Goddard, Ives. 2010. Linguistic variation in a small speech community: The personal dialects of Moraviantown Delaware. Anthropological Linguistics 52(1). 1–48. Goddard, Pliny Earle. 1906–1907. Chilula field notes. American Council of Learned Societies Committee on Native American Languages, American Philosophical Society, Mss.497.B63c, Na20g.2. Goddard, Pliny Earle. 1914. Chilula texts. University of California Publications in American Archaeology and Ethnology 10(7). 289–379. Golla, Victor. 2011. California Indian languages. Berkeley: University of California Press. Hildebrandt, Kristine A., Carmen Jany & Wilson Silva (eds.). 2017. Documenting variation in endangered languages. (Language Documentation and Conservation Special Publication 13). Honolulu: University of Hawaiʻi Press. Hill, Jane H. 2001. Languages on the land: Toward an anthropological dialectology. In John E. Terrell (ed.), Archaeology, language and history: Essays on culture and identity, 257–282. Westport: Bergin and Garvey. Hinton, Leanne. 1979. Irataba’s gift: A closer look at the ṣ > s > θ sound shift in Mojave and Northern Pai. Journal of California and Great Basin Anthropology – Papers in Linguistics 1. 3–37. Hinton, Leanne. 1981. Upland Yuman sibilant shifts: The beginning of the story. Journal of California and Great Basin Anthropology – Papers in Linguistics 3. 65–76. Holton, Gary P. 2012. Language archives: They’re not just for linguists anymore. In Frank Seifart, Geoffrey Haig, Nikolaus P. Himmelmann, Dagmar Jung, Anna Margetts & Paul Trilsbeek (eds.), Potentials of language documentation: Methods, analysis and utilization. 105–110. Language Documentation and Conservation Special Publication 3. Honolulu: University of Hawaiʻi Press. Kari, James. 2019. Lexware, Dene band labels, and recent Alaska Dene lexicography work. In Justin Spence (ed.), Alaska Native Language Center Working Papers 15, 69–83. Fairbanks: Alaska Native Language Center. Lawyer, Lewis. 2021. A grammar of Patwin. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Leonard, Wesley Y. 2017. Producing language reclamation by decolonising ‘language’. Language Documentation and Description 14. 15–36. Merriam, C. Hart. 1850–1974. C. Hart Merriam Papers. Bancroft Library, University of California, Berkeley, BANC FILM 1022. Miranda, Deborah. Bad Indians: A tribal memoir. Berkeley, CA: Heyday Books. Ravindranath, Maya. 2015. Sociolinguistic variation and language contact. Language and Linguistics Compass 9(6). 243–255. Reichard, Gladys. 1922. Gladys Reichard field notebooks on Wiyot Indians, 1922. Bancroft Library, University of California, Berkeley, BANC FILM 2732. Rice, Keren and Leslie Saxon. 2002. Issues of standardization and community in Aboriginal language lexicography. In William Frawley, Kenneth C. Hill & Pamela Munro (eds.), Making dictionaries: Preserving indigenous languages of the Americas, 125–154. Berkeley: University of California Press. Schneider, Edgar W. 2008. Investigating variation and change in written documents. In J. K. Chambers, Peter Trudgill & Natalie Schilling-Estes (eds.), Handbook of language variation and change, 67–96. Malden, MA: Blackwell. Scollon, Ronald. 1979. 236 years of variability in Chipewyan consonants. International Journal of American Linguistics 45(4). 332–342.

700 

 Justin Spence

Spence, Justin. 2013. Language change, contact, and koineization in Pacific Coast Athabaskan. Berkeley, CA: UC Berkeley dissertation. Spence, Justin. 2016. Lexical innovation and variation in Hupa (Athabaskan). International Journal of American Linguistics 82(1). 72–93. Stanford, James N. & Dennis R. Preston (eds.). 2009. Variation in indigenous and minority languages. Amsterdam: Benjamins. Walker, Neil Alexander. 2020. A grammar of Southern Pomo. Lincoln: University of Nebraska. Wallace, William J. 1953. The role of humor in the Hupa Indian tribe. Journal of American Folklore 66(260). 135–141. Warner, Natasha, Lynnika Butler & Quirina Geary. 2016. Mutsun-English English-Mutsun dictionary: mutsun-inkiS inkiS-mutsun riica pappel. (Language Documentation and Conservation Special Publication 11). Honolulu: University of Hawai‘i Press. Warner, Natasha, Lynnika Butler & Quirina Luna-Costillas. 2006. Making a dictionary for community use in language revitalization: The case of Mutsun. International Journal of Lexicography 19(3). 257–285. Webster, Anthony K. 2015. Intimate grammars: An ethnography of Navajo poetry. Tucson: University of Arizona Press. Woodward, Mary F. 1953. [Hupa texts and vocabulary.] Survey of California and Other Indian Languages, University of California Berkeley, Woodward.001 to Woodward.003.

Kayla Palakurthy

31 Community-based sociolinguistic variation Abstract: Linguistic variation is found in all living languages, and sociolinguistic research reveals how variants can carry social meaning and reflect ongoing linguistic changes. Attitudes towards such linguistic heterogeneity may vary significantly within and across speech communities, with implications for how variants are evaluated and presented in pedagogical contexts. This chapter presents findings from representative studies of sociolinguistic variation in North American languages, alongside those analyzing attitudes towards variation. When considered together, this body of work reveals that the demographic categories of age, region, and gender meaningfully pattern with some language-internal lexical, phonological, and grammatical variation, and research shows that an increased understanding of sociolinguistic variation can inform community-based language projects. Beyond their potential relevance to maintenance and reclamation efforts, these studies expand the typological diversity of the field of sociolinguistics by including communities representing different sociocultural contexts than those that are typically the focus of sociolinguistic research.

31.1 Introduction Linguistic variation—the notion that there are many ways to say the same thing or express the same function—is an important facet of language. Speakers vary in their word choice, pronunciation, and grammar depending on their sociocultural experiences, identities, and interactional goals. In this way, linguistic variation reveals the interconnected relationship between language and social meaning, as speakers may, intentionally or not, produce variants to signal their affiliation with a particular group or community, or to express characteristics of their individual and group identities. Awareness of patterns in the usage of linguistic variants also provides insight into how innovative forms arise and spread within a speech community. While sociolinguistic variation has been extensively studied in many monolingual communities speaking large global languages, it is less often examined at the same level of depth in minority language communities, and relatively little such work has focused on languages indigenous to North America.1 In light of this gap, there has been growing recognition of the need to expand sociolinguistic research on smaller languages (Nagy 2009; Stanford and Preston 2009; Stanford 2016; Hildebrandt, Jany, and Silva 2017).

1 This chapter is largely concerned with work explicitly focused on sociolinguistic variation. Beyond what is discussed here, many grammars, dictionaries, and descriptive studies provide detailed examples of variation. See Table 1 in the appendix for a list of quantitative or variationist sociolinguistic studies. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110600926-031

702 

 Kayla Palakurthy

Increased attention to variation in minority Indigenous languages—including those in North America—has both practical and theoretical benefits. From a language documentation perspective, the ongoing recording and analysis of language-internal variation is essential to accurately representing authentic language usage in different social and linguistic contexts (Goddard 2010; Childs, Good, and Mitchell 2014).2 While astute native speakers and teachers may be aware of patterns related to how people of different backgrounds variably speak, a directed research focus on variation can draw attention to the rich layers of diversity within a language, including the emergence of contemporary varieties and language practices (Woodbury 2005; Leonard 2011). This in turn has the potential to alleviate manifestations of linguistic insecurity that are common in language endangerment scenarios (Ravindranath and Quinn 2017). From a theoretical standpoint, studies of sociolinguistic variation in North American languages provide an opportunity for testing scientific assumptions in under-represented social contexts and for stimulating new questions and approaches. In contrast to more frequently-studied languages, North American languages tend to be spoken by bilinguals, and the social factors and ideologies that influence language variation and change may differ. For instance, while sociolinguistic studies have repeatedly found linguistic differences that correlate with socioeconomic class, other local categories and identities are expected to be more explanatory in smaller societies with different power structures, social networks, and economic practices (Stanford and Preston 2009). Indeed, studies on minority languages have described linguistic distinctions corresponding with clan group (Stanford 2009) and territorial hunting group (Clarke 2009), to name a few. Other work has noted the absence of significant group-level patterns, and instead describes a high degree of individual variation, even within a very small speaker population (Goddard 2010). Given the diversity of North American speech communities, future studies of this nature will likely uncover additional factors, as the field expands in typological breadth. In this chapter, I review findings from studies of community-based sociolinguistic variation in North American languages. Despite a somewhat limited body of scholarly literature, the existing studies demonstrate that research investigating variation correlating with geographic region (§ 2), age (§ 3), and gender (§ 4), has the potential to uncover ethnographic insights, identify changes, and enhance pedagogical materials. In § 5, I briefly discuss pervasive attitudes and ideologies towards variation and in § 6, provide examples of community-based applications of this research. Together, these studies demonstrate how speakers’ use of linguistic variants is not exclusively determined by demographic category membership but rather intersects with community language ideologies in complex, meaningful ways.

2 Mansfield and Stanford (2017) discuss methodological challenges and opportunities in documenting sociolinguistic variation in lesser-studied Indigenous communities.



Community-based sociolinguistic variation 

 703

31.2 Regional variation An intuitive but fundamental sociolinguistic reality is that outside of technology-mediated communication, speakers tend to interact most with those in close proximity. As a result, environmental features, as well as migration and settlement histories, often influence sociolinguistic patterns. In circumstances of prolonged or significant geographic and social separation, speakers of the same language may diverge linguistically to the extent that differences become sufficiently robust to be perceived as distinct dialects by members of the speech community and/or by linguists, with particular words, accents, or grammatical constructions becoming associated with groups from different areas.3 Over time, regional features may take on meaning as markers of identity: a way to express where one comes from and to invoke non-linguistic traits encompassed in those associations. In North America, several studies describe regional variation. For instance, in Diné Bizaad (Navajo), a Dene language spoken in the American Southwest, earlier sources document dialects within the large Navajo Nation territory arising due to geographic barriers, such as mountain ranges, as well as sociocultural, sociohistorical, and economic differences (Weisiger 2009; Jacobsen 2017). Regional differences include the pronunciation of specific sounds (Reichard 1951; Saville-Troike and McCreedy 1980) and the usage of certain words, which tend to be salient to speakers (Webster 2015). In particular, the coining of new words—a longstanding process in the language (Peterson and Webster 2013)—is thought to increase regional variation, as various descriptive terms take hold in different areas (Reichard 1945). As an example, speakers report that the word for ‘soda’ has been variously coined as tółich’íí’í ‘spicy water’ in New Mexico, and tó dilchxoshí ‘bubbly water’, or tó łikani ‘sweet water’ elsewhere (Palakurthy 2019). Though contemporary analyses of phonetic variation have not identified any significant regional patterns, many speakers express a strong perception, and in some cases evaluation, of regional variation (Palakurthy 2019), perhaps due to the aforementioned lexical variation and the strength of non-linguistic regional differences. Another recorded case of regional variation comes from Kumeyaay, a Yuman language spoken in Southern California and Northern Baja (Field 2012). Within the Kumeyaay-speaking region, there are two linguistic and cultural sub-groups associated with different territorial areas: ‘Iipay and Tiipay. Most of the reported differences between these groups are lexical, though they differ in their phoneme inventories and morphological features such as plural marking and verb stem conjugations (Field 2012: 559).

3 With the availability of multimedia mapping tools, communities have developed compelling ways to visually represent the geographic distribution of linguistic variation. Noteworthy examples include the Algonquian Linguistic Atlas: https://www.atlas-ling.ca/# and the Dene Speech Atlas: http://www.sas. rochester.edu/lin/DeneSpeechAtlas/.

704 

 Kayla Palakurthy

However, differences between the groups are not strictly governed by geography, and significantly interact with language ideologies that have been shaped by distinct social networks, sociopolitical organization, and access to resources (Field 2012: 562). On the one hand, what is known as a localist stance is prevalent among the ‘Iipay, whereby they are less accepting of linguistic variation and innovation and overall more protective of their language. On the other hand, the Tiipay adhere to a distributed stance (Hill 2001) and variationist ideology (Kroskrity 2002; Field and Kroskrity 2009): lexical variation is expected, and innovations are more quickly adopted. Based on these findings, Field (2012) encourages increased collaboration within the groups, especially among the Tiipay, whose settlements span the present-day national border, but in fact are ideologically and linguistically similar. Patterns of regional variation have also been documented in the case of highly mobile populations, as illustrated by a quantitative study of Innu-aimun, an Algonquian variety spoken by several formerly nomadic groups now living together in Sheshatshiu, Labrador, Canada (Clarke 2009).4 This study examines how variable phonological and grammatical patterns reflect an interaction between speaker membership in territorial hunting groups, age, and speech style (Clarke 2009: 122). The findings reveal how older residents show a high degree of regional variation, alongside an overall trend of community-wide dialectal convergence in the direction of the prestigious southwestern group, Uashau (Clarke 2009: 127). Younger speakers from lower-status groups were found to be the most advanced in their phonological convergence towards this variety, suggesting that these changes are socially motivated. Accompanying the dialectal leveling, community rather than territory, emerges as more important in discussions of identity among younger speakers.

31.3 Age-based variation Another prominent social factor that shapes variation is age group, or generational cohort (Labov 2001; Tagliamonte and D’Arcy 2007). Age-based variation arises in the process of intergenerational language transmission, and as speakers of different generations amass distinct sociocultural and sociolinguistic experiences (Labov 2007).5 Significant intergenerational differences are often taken as evidence for language change. Though many studies focus on describing changes, this line of research can likewise identify instances of linguistic continuity and maintenance across generations (Palakur-

4 See Crapo and Spykerman (1979) for a discussion of dialect mixing among mobile groups of Shoshoni speakers. 5 Given that speakers of North American languages increasingly speak English, French, and Spanish, age is often highly correlated with degree of exposure to colonial languages. See Thomason (this volume) for more on language contact.



Community-based sociolinguistic variation 

 705

thy 2021). Additionally, analyses of new varieties demonstrate how younger speakers and learners introduce innovative linguistic variants, as they adapt their language in changing sociolinguistic contexts (Schaengold 2004; Morgan 2017). Approaches to the study of age-based variation vary. When similar language samples are available, researchers may compare contemporary recordings with previously-recorded materials; differences are interpreted as real-time changes. Such studies illustrate a valuable application of archived documentation, as previously recorded language samples can serve as a proxy for older generations of speakers.6 Recent phonetic analyses have applied this method to significantly expand research on sound change in North American languages.7 A study of the Washo language, spoken around Lake Tahoe in California and Nevada, compares vowels recorded with speakers in the 1950s with the speech of elders recorded in 2004 and 2005 (Yu 2008). Results show that the contemporary speakers maintain a phonemic vowel length distinction, though it is less robust phonetically than in earlier recordings. Similarly, Babel (2009) examines a lenis/ fortis contrast in Northern Paiute, a Uto-Aztecan language spoken in California and Nevada, using recordings from the 1950s of Carson Desert Northern Paiute and contemporary recordings from speakers of Mono Lake Northern Paiute. Again, speakers maintain the contrast, though the youngest speaker produces it as less distinct phonetically. This speaker substitutes English sounds at times, while maintaining Northern Paiute allophonic rules: English /s/ is used for Northern Paiute /ç/ and English /ʃ/ for a palatalized variant after the vowel /i/ (Babel 2009). Together, these studies reveal phonetic differences in the speech of younger speakers, but also demonstrate how contemporary generations maintain phonological patterns, even in cases of significant language endangerment. Additionally, findings from studies of real-time change in North American languages provide insight into some rarely attested linguistic processes. Through an investigation of the historical record, as well as an acoustic analysis of recordings from the 1970s and 1998–2000, a study of Menominee, an Algonquian language, spoken in Wisconsin, shows how the Proto-Algonquian sounds /s/ and /š/ [ʃ] have merged into a sibilant written (Cudworth 2019). Specifically, results show how two acoustic ranges have been subsumed into one large range, interpreted as a merger by expansion. This type of merger is typologically rare (Labov 1994; Cudworth 2019), underscoring the importance of including studies of this, and other North American languages, in the sociolinguistic literature. Another approach is to compare the contemporary speech of older speakers with that of younger speakers. This method, known as the apparent-time construct, stipulates that differences in linguistic features that pattern with generation may reflect ongoing

6 See Spence (this volume) on archival-based variation and Lukaniec (this volume) on using archival materials for language reclamation. 7 See Gordon and Luna (2004), Chang (2009), Haynes (2010), and Di Paolo (2015) for additional phonetic studies, and Matsumoto (2015) for a morphological study applying a similar method.

706 

 Kayla Palakurthy

linguistic changes (Labov 1994; Bailey 2002). In Kwakʼwala, a Wakashan language spoken in British Columbia, the speech of older, middle-aged, and younger speakers was found to differ grammatically, phonologically, and lexically by generation, as well as from earlier documentation (Goodfellow 2005). Some specific patterns involve younger speakers avoiding less-frequent grammatical and lexical suffixes, substituting some English sounds, and not maintaining certain distinctions, such as phonemic glottalization. In select lexical domains, including kinship and seasonal descriptions, younger speakers displayed a more restricted lexicon. Similar results are found in studies on other languages. Speakers differ by generation in their use of topological spatial relations in Dëne Sųłiné, a Northern Dene language spoken in the Cold Lake Community in Alberta, Canada (Thiering 2009). In this case, younger speakers showed a more limited vocabulary in a task designed to elicit spatial terminology. In Central Yup’ik, an Eskimoan language spoken in Alaska, lexical changes are noted in the demonstratives and evidential markers, as well as in strategies for new word formation (Wyman 2004). Findings from a study of Diné Bizaad likewise show generational differences in some, though not all, examined linguistic features (Palakurthy 2019). Changes include the shortening of the release of the sound /kh/ and the pronunciation of the affricates /tl, tɬ’/ as /kl, kɬ’/. Regardless of linguistic changes, a positive sign for language vitality in several communities is that Indigenous language use remains closely tied to cultural identity, even among younger people who do not necessarily self-identify as speakers of the language (Goodfellow 2005; Davis 2016). Moreover, intergenerational differences are not always due to language change and may be attributed to an effect of age-grading, whereby distinct styles are associated with speakers at different life stages (Eckert 1997; Sankoff and Blondeau 2007). Age-graded differences in North American languages remain under-described, despite the potential value of this topic to language revitalization. The need for including age-graded styles in documentary and descriptive work is succinctly expressed by MENEŦIYE, a teacher of SENĆOŦEN, a Coast Salish language spoken in and around Vancouver and Washington: “I’ve never heard what a teenager or a woman in her early 20s sounds [like], fluent. I’ve heard old people speak, in their 80s, and so I wonder, do I sound like an 80-year-old when I speak?” (Bird and Kell 2017: 553).

31.4 Gender-based variation In sociolinguistic research on larger languages, gender has garnered significant attention (cf. Eckert 1989; Tannen 1994; Bucholtz 2000). Like all broad social categories, studies of gender are most informative when they incorporate ethnography in order to effectively interpret results in a way that is locally meaningful (Eckert and McConnell-Ginet 1992: 462). A caveat is that a frequently-employed sociolinguistic method, whereby a researcher assigns speaker gender based on their perception of an overt



Community-based sociolinguistic variation 

 707

gender presentation rather than a self-identified gender designation, lags behind more nuanced and updated scholarly approaches to gender (cf. Zimman 2018). In research on North American languages, earlier linguistic and anthropological studies describe distinct gendered speech varieties with lexical, grammatical, and phonetic differences associated with “men’s speech” and “women’s speech” in several communities (cf. Sapir 1929 on Yana; Haas 1944 on Koasati; Taylor 1982 on Gros Ventre; Kroskrity 1983 on Puebloan languages of the Southwest).8 Categorical gender dialects are cross-linguistically rare, diachronically unstable, and thought to be found only in relatively small speech communities (Dunn 2014). While these studies identify a connection between gender and linguistic variation, this body of research also reveals the complexity of gender as a category and suggests in some cases that distinctions that align with gender further relate to formality, prestige, and social status. Other work has examined the role of gender in speech genres. In Muskogee, a Muskogean language spoken in the American Southeast, a study finds that the way men and women use different genres reflects a local ideology of gender balance (Innes 2006). Within this context, the genre of women’s gossip is understood as being powerful and important to Muskogee society. While the premise of some research on gender is predicated on the erroneous assumption that gender is a discrete, binary category, other research foregrounds speakers who identify as non-binary, an approach that is necessary for providing a more complete understanding of gender-based language patterns (cf. Zimman et al. 2014). Exemplifying this in North America, Davis (2014) presents a discourse analysis of how Two-Spirit individuals in Indigenous communities position themselves with regards to gender, sexuality, and indigeneity. This analysis of language usage in the context of a pan-Tribal Two-Spirit presentation shows the complex way that Two-Spirit identity is better understood as “potentially overlapping states” (Davis 2014: 62) and derives from “multiple levels of community membership” (Davis 2014: 74).

31.5 Attitudes towards variation An additional dimension to sociolinguistic variation is how speakers and communities vary in their attitudes towards differences. In some cases, such as the aforementioned Kumeyaay example, shared attitudes and ideologies may be observed at a group level. At the same time, within a speech community, different types of variation may simultaneously evoke diverse ideological evaluations (Bird and Kell 2017). As discussed in § 2, pervasive community ideologies have numerous implications ranging from the adoption of new words and language policies, to personal questions regarding language and

8 Mithun (1999: 276–280) provides a thorough overview of this literature, and other work on speech styles in North American languages.

708 

 Kayla Palakurthy

identity. At the risk of oversimplifying the diversity of attitudes towards variation, I present here a few representative studies on these topics. In North America, as in other language communities, an attitude of nostalgia and the related notion of linguistic purism are sometimes attested (Thomas 1991; Dorian 1994). In these circumstances, certain varieties or linguistic features tend to be evaluated as more authentic based on an association with an idealized, often older form of the language (Holton 2009). Such notions of linguistic authenticity may be tied to the speech of sub-groups such as elders or speakers from particular regions (Peterson 2006; Jacobsen 2017). These local associations with authenticity are relevant for understanding patterns of linguistic variation, language change, domains of language usage, and contemporary linguistic practices such as code-switching. Speakers who produce variants that deviate from idealized forms, especially younger speakers, may be subject to criticism, correction, or teasing (Lee 2007). While frequently evaluated negatively by speakers, language change, motivated by a variety of language-internal and external factors, is inevitable and prevalent in all domains of linguistic structure. Negative attitudes towards language change are not unique to endangered languages, though speakers may be more sensitive to intergenerational changes in these contexts (Bird and Kell 2017). In other cases, a variationist ideology—mentioned in §  2—may be more prevalent within a speech community. For instance, in an apparent-time study of phonological variation in Western Cherokee, participants did not evaluate any one variety as more correct than others (Foley 1980). A similar attitude towards regional variation is recorded among some contemporary speakers of Diné Bizaad (Palakurthy 2019), and towards “family accent” in SENĆOŦEN (Bird and Kell 2017: 555). A widespread variationist ideology is more likely to correlate with a greater profusion and acceptance of linguistic variants.

31.6 Community-based applications of ­sociolinguistic research In addition to their relevance to language documentation and sociolinguistic theories, analyses of variation have been applied in community-based language projects. For example, a study of pronunciation in SENĆOŦEN explicitly connects research on variation to speaker goals and language teaching (Bird and Kell 2017). In this study, interviews were first conducted to establish community attitudes towards variable pronunciation and then strategies proposed for addressing pronunciation challenges among second language learners who comprise the majority of the contemporary SENĆOŦEN-speaking population. This study found that pronunciation is very important to speakers and highlights a challenge encountered in many revitalization projects: how do you teach the language in a way that reflects how it was previously spoken, while acknowledging the



Community-based sociolinguistic variation 

 709

inevitable reality of language change and the need for sensitivity towards learners?9 Given this context, this study describes two strategies for addressing pronunciation challenges: raise awareness about the type of variation that is considered acceptable in the community and then target the types of variation that can be corrected with appropriate support. This study proposes pedagogical strategies for improving pronunciation including the targeted teaching of young children with abundant language input to promote correct pronunciation. In the case of adult learners, there is an emphasis on listening to recordings to learn proper pronunciation with daily practice encouraged. As a follow-up to the interviews, word lists were recorded for phonetic analysis, with a focus on sounds that were described as difficult. The study has also led to a community workshop on pronunciation with the goal of future workshops and collaborative projects to help inform decisions about the role of pronunciation in language revitalization efforts.10 Another approach towards incorporating variation into teaching is discussed in the context of Blackfoot, an Algonquian language spoken in Montana (Miyashita and Chatsis 2015). In this case, language instructors did not initially include information on dialects in their curriculum and found that students differed in their background and awareness of linguistic differences. The Blackfoot language has no single standardized variety, but given that some variants are not described, the study identified “a gap between what the students see and what they hear in instruction” (Miyashita and Chatsis 2015: 112). The instructors subsequently addressed this mismatch through an open discussion of regional, generational, and stylistic differences in order to promote awareness and respect for different varieties in the classroom. The representation of sociolinguistic variation in language resources is a related consideration. Along these lines, Rice and Saxon (2002) discuss the complexities of representing phonological, morphological, and lexical variation in dictionary projects for Dogrib, Slave, and Kaska, Northern Dene languages spoken in the Northwest Territories in Canada.11 The dictionaries take different approaches towards sociolinguistic variation. Drawing on these experiences, the authors advise that choosing to standardize forms, rather than including multiple variants, is not always beneficial; dictionaries ultimately must be designed for the intended community of users.

9 Warner, Luna, and Butler (2007) discuss similar questions as they pertain to revitalizing a dormant language. 10 Goodfellow (2003), Holton (2009), and Lokosh (2019) also discuss new varieties and revitalization. 11 See Saxon (this volume) on representing variation in Dene languages.

710 

 Kayla Palakurthy

31.7 Conclusions This chapter discusses work examining sociolinguistic variation in North American languages, with a focus on how variation patterns with region, age, and gender. Collectively, research describes how linguistic differences correlate with broad social factors, uncovers instances of language change, and provides insights into language ideologies. Community-based projects epitomize how a greater understanding of such sociolinguistic variation and attitudes can enhance applied language work. Overall, dedicated sociolinguistic studies of variation in North American languages remain sparse, and such studies present an exciting avenue for future work. This line of research underscores the necessity of in-depth ethnography and collaboration with a diversity of speakers; the use of any general demographic classifications must be contextualized in order for patterns to be meaningfully interpreted and made relevant for community goals. Especially promising are approaches that move beyond analyses of static associations between linguistic variables and fixed social categories, to describe how speakers dynamically use language, along with their other stylistic resources, to express meaning.

References Babel, Molly. 2009. The phonetic and phonological effects of obsolescence in Northern Paiute. In James N. Stanford & Dennis R. Preston (eds.), Variation in Indigenous minority languages, 23–46. Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company. Bailey, Guy. 2002. Real and apparent time. In J.K. Chambers, Peter Trudgill & Natalie Schilling-Estes (eds.), The handbook of language variation and change, 312–332. Malden: Blackwell Publishing. Bird, Sonya & Sarah Kell. 2017. The role of pronunciation in SENĆOŦEN language. The Canadian Modern Language Review 74(4). 538–569. Bucholtz, Mary. 2000. Gender. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 9. 80–83. Chang, Charles. 2009. Convergence and divergence in language obsolescence. In Manghyu Pak (ed.), Current issues in unity and diversity of languages, 933–952. Seoul: Linguistic Society of Korea. Childs, Tucker, Jeff Good & Alice Mitchell. 2014. Beyond the ancestral code: Towards a model for sociolinguistic language documentation. Language Documentation and Conservation 8. 168–191. Clarke, Sandra. 2009. Sociolinguistic stratification and new dialect formation in a Canadian Aboriginal community: Not so different after all? In James N. Stanford & Dennis R. Preston (eds.). Variation in Indigenous minority languages, 109–128. Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company. Crapo, Richley H. & Bryan R. Spykerman. 1979. Social variation in Shoshoni phonology: An ecological interpretation. Human Ecology 7(4). 317–332. Cudworth, Andrea. 2019. Menominee sibilants. Paper presented at the Linguistic Society of America Annual Meeting, January 6, New York, New York. Davis, Jenny L. 2016. Language affiliation and ethnolinguistic identity in Chickasaw language revitalization. Language & Communication 47. 100–111. Davis, Jenny L. 2014. “More than just ‘gay Indians’”: Intersecting articulations of Two-Spirit gender, sexuality, and indigenousness. In Lal Zimman, Jenny Davis & Joshua Raclaw (eds.), Queer excursions: Retheorizing binaries in language, gender, and sexuality, 62–80. Oxford: Oxford University Press.



Community-based sociolinguistic variation 

 711

Di Paolo, Marianna. 2015. A Uto-Aztecan vowel shift: Evidence from Takic, Southern Uto-Aztecan, and Numic. Paper presented at the Society for the Study of Indigenous Languages of America, January 8–11, Portland, Oregon. Dorian, Nancy C. 1994. Purism vs. compromise in language revitalization and language revival. Language in Society 23. 479–94. Dunn, Michael. 2014. Gender determined dialect variation. In Greville G. Corbett (ed.), The expression of gender, 39–68. Berlin, Germany: De Gruyter Mouton. Eckert, Penelope. 1997. Age as a sociolinguistic variable. In Florian Coulmas (ed.), The handbook of sociolinguistics, 151–167. Malden, Massachusetts: Blackwell Publishing. Eckert, Penelope & Sally McConnell-Ginnet 1992. Think practically and look locally: language and gender as community-based practice. Annual Review of Anthropology 21. 461–490. Field, Margaret C. 2009. Changing Navajo language ideologies and changing language use. In Paul V. Kroskrity & Margaret C. Field (eds.), Native American language ideologies: Beliefs, practices, and struggles in Indian country, 31–47. Tucson, Arizona: University of Arizona Press. Field, Margaret. 2012. Kumeyaay variation, group identity, and the land. International Journal of American Linguistics 78(4). 557–573. Field, Margaret & Paul V. Kroskrity. 2009. Introduction: Revealing Native American language ideologies. In Paul V. Kroskrity & Margaret C. Field (eds.), Native American language ideologies: Beliefs, practices, and struggles in Indian country, 3–30. Tucson, Arizona: University of Arizona Press. Fleming, Luke. 2012. Gender indexicality in the Native Americas: Contributions to the typology of social indexicality. Language in Society 41. 295–320. Foley, Lawrence. 1980. Phonological Variation in Western Cherokee. New York: Garland Inc. Goddard, Ives. 2010. Linguistic variation in a small speech community: The personal dialects of Moraviantown Delaware. Anthropological Linguistics 52(1). 1–48. Goodfellow, Anne. 2003. The development of “new” languages in Native American communities. American Indian Culture and Research Journal, 27(2). 41–59. Goodfellow, Anne. 2005. Talking in context: Language and identity in Kwakwaka’wakw society. Montreal, QC: McGill-Queen’s University Press. Gordon, Matthew & Edmundo Luna. 2004. An intergenerational study of Hupa stress. Berkeley Linguistics Society (BLS) 30. 105–117. Haas, Mary R. 1944. Men’s and women’s speech in Koasati. Language 20. 142–149. Haynes, Erin Flynn. 2010. Phonetic and phonological acquisition in endangered languages learned by adults: A case study of Numu (Oregon Northern Paiute). Berkeley, CA: University of California dissertation. Hildebrandt, Kristine A., Carmen Jany & Wilson Silva. 2017. Introduction: Documenting variation in endangered languages. In Kristine A. Hildebrandt, Carmen Jany & Wilson Silva (eds.), Language Documentation and Conservation Special Publication 13. 1–5. Honolulu: University of Hawai’i Press. Hill, Jane. 2001. Languages on the land. In John E. Terrell (ed.), Archaeology, language and history: Essays on culture and ethnicity, 257–82. Westport, Connecticut: Bergin and Garvey. Hill, Jane H. & Kenneth C. Hill. 1986. Speaking Mexicano. Tucson, Arizona: University of Arizona Press. Holton, Gary. 2009. Relearning Athabascan languages in Alaska: Creating sustainable language communities through creolization. In Anne Marie Goodfellow (ed.), Speaking of endangered languages: Issues in revitalization, 238–265. Cambridge Scholars Press. Innes, Pamela. 2006. The interplay of genres, gender, and language ideology among the Muskogee. Language in Society 35. 231—259. Jacobsen, Kristina M. 2017. The sound of Navajo country: Music, language and Diné belonging. Chapel Hill, North Carolina: University of North Carolina Press. Junker, Marie-Odile et al. 2014–2019. The Algonquian linguistic atlas. https://www.atlas-ling.ca/ Kari, James M. 1976. Navajo verb prefix phonology. New York: Garland Publishing Inc.

712 

 Kayla Palakurthy

Kroskrity, Paul V. 1983. On male and female speech in the Pueblo Southwest. International Journal of American Linguistics 49(1). 88–91. Kroskrity, Paul. 2002. Language renewal and the “technologies of literacy and post-literacy.” In William Frawley, Kenneth C. Hill & Pamela Munro (eds.), Making dictionaries: preserving Indigenous languages of the Americas, 171–92. Berkeley: University of California Press. Labov, William. 1994. Principles of linguistic change, volume 1: Internal factors. Malden, Massachusetts: Blackwell Publishing. Labov, William. 2001. Principles of linguistic change, volume 2: Social factors. Malden, Massachusetts: Blackwell Publishing. Labov, William. 2007. Transmission and diffusion. Language 83(2). 344–387. Lee, Tiffany S. 2007. “If they want Navajo to be learned, then they should require it in all schools”: Navajo teenagers’ experiences, choices, and demands regarding Navajo language. Wicazo Sa Review 22(1). 7–33. Leonard, Wesley Y. 2011. Challenging “extinction” through Modern Miami language practices. American Indian Culture and Research Journal 35(2). 135–160. Lokosh (Joshua D. Hinson). 2019. Nanna ittonchololiꞌ ilaliichi (we are cultivating new growth): Twenty years in Chikashshanompaꞌ revitalization. Norman, OK: University of Oklahoma dissertation. Mansfield, John & James Stanford. 2017. Documenting sociolinguistic variation in lesser-studied Indigenous communities: Challenges and practical solutions. In Kristine A. Hildebrandt, Carmen Jany & Wilson Silva (eds.), Language Documentation and Conservation Special Publication 13. 116–136. Honolulu: University of Hawai’i Press. Matsumoto, Katherine. 2015. Language change in Shoshone: Structural consequences of language loss. Salt Lake City, UT: University of Utah dissertation. McDonough, Joyce. The Dene speech atlas. The University of Rochester, the National Science Foundation, and The University of Alberta. http://www.sas.rochester.edu/lin/DeneSpeechAtlas/. Mithun, Marianne. 1999. The languages of Native North America. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Miyashita, Mizuki & Annabelle Chatsis. 2015. Respecting dialectal variations in a Blackfoot language class. In Jon Reyhner, Joseph Martin, Louise Lockard & Willard Sakiestewa Gilbert (eds.), Honoring our Elders: Culturally appropriate approaches for teaching Indigenous students, 109–116. Flagstaff, Arizona: Northern Arizona University. Morgan, Juliet. 2017. The Learner Varieties of the Chikasha Academy: Chickasaw Adult Language Acquisition, Change, and Revitalization. Norman, OK: University of Oklahoma dissertation. Nagy, Naomi. 2009. The challenges of less commonly studied languages: Writing a sociogrammar of Faetar. In James N. Stanford & Dennis R. Preston (eds.), Variation in Indigenous minority languages, 397–417. Philadelphia: John Benjamins. Palakurthy, Kayla. 2019. Many ways to sound Diné: Linguistic variation in Navajo. Santa Barbara, CA: University of California dissertation. Palakurthy, Kayla. 2021. The status of sibilant harmony in Diné Bizaad (Navajo). Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 1–19. doi:10.1017/S0025100321000220 Peterson, Leighton Craig. 2006. Technology, ideology, and emergent communicative practices among the Navajo. Austin, TX: University of Texas dissertation. Peterson, Leighton C. & Anthony K. Webster. 2013. Speech play and language ideologies in Navajo terminology development. Pragmatics 23(1). 93–116. Ravindranath, Maya Abtahian & Conor McDonough Quinn. Language shift and linguistic insecurity. 2017. In Kristine A. Hildebrandt, Carmen Jany, & Wilson Silva (eds.), Language Documentation and Conservation Special Publication 13. 137–151. Honolulu: University of Hawai’i Press. Reichard, Gladys. 1945. Linguistic diversity amongst the Navaho Indians. International Journal of American Linguistics 11(3). 156–168. Reichard, Gladys A. 1951. Navaho grammar. New York: Augustin.



Community-based sociolinguistic variation 

 713

Rice, Keren & Leslie Saxon 2002. Issues of standardization and community in Aboriginal language lexicography. In William Frawley, Kenneth C. Hill & Pamela Munro (eds.), Making dictionaries: Preserving Indigenous languages of the Americas, 125–154. Berkeley: University of California Press. Romero, Sergio Francisco. 2006. Sociolinguistic variation and linguistic history in Mayan: The case of K’ichee. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania dissertation. Sankoff, Gillian & Hélène Blondeau. 2007. Language change across the lifespan: /r/ in Montreal French. Language 83(3). 560–588. Sapir, Edward. 1929. Male and female forms of speech in Yana. In St. W. J. Teeuwen (ed.), Donum Natalicium Schrijnen. 79–85. Nijmegen-Utretcht: Dekker and Van de Vegt. Saville-Troike, Muriel & Lynn A. McCreedy. 1980. Synchronic variation in Navajo: Regional, social, and developmental evidence from child language. Final Project Report National Science Foundation 1979–1980. Washington D.C., Georgtown university. Schaengold, Charlotte C. 2004. Bilingual Navajo: Mixed codes, bilingualism, and language maintenance. Columbus, OH: Ohio State University dissertation. Stanford, James N. 2016. A call for more diverse sources of data: Variationist approaches in non-English contexts. Journal of Sociolinguistics 20(4). 525–541. Stanford, James N. 2009. Clan as a sociolinguistic variable: Three approaches to Sui clans. In James N. Stanford & Dennis R. Preston (eds.), Variation in Indigenous minority languages, 463–484. Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing. Stanford, James N. & Dennis R. Preston. 2009. The lure of a distant horizon: Variation in Indigenous minority languages. In James N. Stanford & Dennis R. Preston (eds.), Variation in Indigenous minority languages. 1–22. Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company. Tagliamonte, Sali A. & Alexandra D’Arcy. 2007. Frequency and variation in the community grammar: Tracking a new change through the generations. Language Variation and Change 19(2). 341–380. Tannen, Deborah 1994. Gender and discourse. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Taylor, Allan R. 1982. “Male” and “female” speech in Gros Ventre. Anthropological Linguistics 24(3). 301–307. Thomas, George. 1991. Linguistic Purism. New York: Longman. Thiering, Martin. 2009. Language loss in spatial semantics: Dene Sųłiné. In James N. Stanford & Dennis R. Preston (eds.), Variation in Indigenous minority languages, 485–516. Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company. Warner, Natasha, Quirina Luna & Lynnika Butler. 2007. Ethics and revitalization of dormant languages. Language Documentation and Conservation 1(1). 56–76. Webster, Anthony K. 2015. Intimate grammars. Tucson, Arizona: University of Arizona Press. Weisiger, Marsha L. 2009. Dreaming of sheep in Navajo country. Seattle: University of Washington. Woodbury, Anthony C. 2005. Ancestral languages and (imagined) creolization. In Peter K. Austin (ed.), Language Documentation and Description, vol. 3, 252–262. London: SOAS. Wyman, Leisy. 2004. Language shift, youth culture, and ideology: A Yup’ik example. Berkeley, CA: University of California dissertation. Yu, Alan. C.L. 2008. The phonetics of quantity alternation in Washo. Journal of Phonetics 36(3). 508–520. Zimman, Lal. 2018. Disambiguating and denaturalizing the voice in sociolinguistics and on Catfish: toward better theory and practice surrounding the phonetics of sex and gender. Presentation at New Ways of Analyzing Variation 27, October 20, New York, New York. Zimman, Lal, Jenny Davis & Joshua Raclaw (eds.), 2014. Queer excursions: Retheorizing binaries in language, gender, and sexuality. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

714 

 Kayla Palakurthy

Other Readings of Interest Boas, Franz & Herman Haeberlin. 1927. Sound shifts in Salishan languages. International Journal of American Linguistics 3. 117–131. Choksi, Nishaant & Barbra A. Meek. 2016. Theorizing salience: Orthographic practice and the enfigurement of minority languages. In Anna M. Babel (ed.), Awareness and control in sociolinguistic research, 228–252. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Dorais, Louis-Jacques. 1986. Inuktitut surface phonology: A trans-dialectal survey. International Journal of American Linguistics 52(1). 20–53. Golla, Victor. 2000. Language histories and communicative strategies in aboriginal California and Oregon. In Osahito Miyaoka (ed.), Languages of the North Pacific Rim, volume 5, 43–64. Suita: Faculty of Informatics, Osaka Gakuin University. Kimball, Geoffrey. 1987. Men’s and women’s speech in Koasati: A reappraisal. International Journal of American Linguistics 53(1). 30–38. MacKenzie, Marguerite E. 1980. Towards a dialectology of Cree-Montagnais-Naskapi. Toronto, ON: University of Toronto dissertation. Miller, Wick R. 1970. Western Shoshoni dialects. In Earl H. Swanson Jr. (ed.), Language and cultures of Western North America: Essays in honor of Sven S. Liljeblad, 17–36. Pocatello: Idaho State University Press. Newmark, Kalina, Nacole Walker, & James Stanford. 2016. ‘The rez accent knows no borders’: Native American ethnic identity expressed through English prosody. Language in Society 45(5). 1–32. Saville-Troike. Muriel 1988. A note on men’s and women’s speech in Koasati. International Journal of American Linguistics 54(2). 241–242. Suslak. Daniel F. 2009. The sociolinguistic problem of generations. Language & Communication 29. 119–209. Tatsch, Sheri Jean. 2006. The Nisenan: Dialects and Districts of a Speech Community. Davis, CA: University of California dissertation. Trechter, Sara 1995. The pragmatic functions of gender deixis in Lakhota. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska dissertation. Trechter, Sara. 1995. Categorical gender myths in Native America: Gender deictics in Lakhota. Issues in Applied Linguistics 6(1). 5–22. Valentine, J. Randolph. 1994. Ojibwe dialect relationships. Austin, TX: University of Texas dissertation. Valentine, J. Randolph. 2002. Variation in body-part verbs in Ojibwe dialects. International Journal of American Linguistics 68(10). 81–119.



Community-based sociolinguistic variation 

 715

Appendix Tab. 1: Quantitative or Variationist Studies of North American Languages Reference

Description

Number of speakers

Foley 1980 Gordon & Luna 2004 Yu 2008 Babel 2009

Study of phonological variation in Western Cherokee. Phonetic/phonological study of stress in Hupa. Phonetic/phonological study of Washo vowels. Phonetic/phonological study of Northern Paiute stops and ­sibilants. Phonetic/phonological study of velar/post-velar fricatives, ­dental/alveolar stops, rhotics, and d-deletion in Southeastern Pomo. Study of 18 phonological variables and 4 grammatical features in Sheshatshiu Innu-aimun. Grammatical study of spatial expressions in Dene Sųłiné. Phonetic/phonological study of several features in Numu. Phonetic/phonological study of Shoshoni vowels. Study of several grammatical features of Shoshone. Phonetic study of Menominee sibilants. Phonetic/phonological study of aspirated stops and lateral affricates; grammatical study of past time marker in Diné Bizaad.

23 3  4  5 

Chang 2009

Clarke 2009 Thiering 2009 Haynes 2010 Di Paolo 2015 Matsumoto 2015 Cudworth 2019 Palakurthy 2019

8 

87 13 25 8  21 18 51

Map of the languages and language families of North America

un

ta

r

ak

a

n

e

a

d

i

a

D

n

c di

d

T E

i t I n u

1

I n u i t E S

N

1

E



E a s t e r n

E

0W

I

ee

A

3

foot

Gros

Assini

an

ok

boine

C

A

D

O

A

N

Tuni ca

ca

ve

(A

sa

xe

)

ia

t n e

r u

39 Coto nam e COMECRU DAN Garz a Qui nigu a Com ecru do 44

C Tus

ch

L 49

C T I N L A A T

a)

(Guale)

0

59

(Chat o

56

t)

Apalache

e

Timuc

N E A C O

ua

O F

Ca

lu

sa

(Xi

(T

é)

ue

xim

ah

)

Scale: 1:7,500,000 43

Te pe ca no

O

OT

15

N 16

0

100

200 200

300

300 400

400

20N

500 Miles

33

KIOWA-TANOAN

3

NADENE

34

KERESAN

4

Haida

35

Zuni

5

WAKASHAN

36

Guaicura

6

CHIMAKUAN

37

OTOMANGUEAN

7

SALISHAN

38

Coahuilteco

8

TSIMSHIANIC

39

COMECRUDAN

9

CHINOOKAN

40

Cotoname

10

ALSEAN

41

Aranama

11

Siuslaw

42

Solano

12

COOSAN

43

Maratino

13

TAKELMAN

44

Quinigua

14

WINTUAN

45

Naolan

15

MAIDUAN

46

Karankawa

16

UTIAN

47

Kootenai

17

YOKUTSAN

48

Cayuse

18

PLATEAU PENUTIAN

49

SIOUANCATAWBA

19

Karok

50

Tonkawa

20

Chimariko

51

CADDOAN

21

SHASTAN

52

Adai

22

PALAIHNIHAN

53

ATAKAPAN

23

POMOAN

54

Chitimacha

24

Yana

55

Tunica

25

SALINAN

56

MUSKOGEAN

26

COCHIMÍ-YUMAN

57

Natchez

27

Seri

58

Yuchi

28

Washoe

59

TIMUCUAN

29

YUKIAN

60

Calusa

30

Esselen

61

IROQUOIAN

31

CHUMASHAN

62

Beothuk

0

Unclassified – data inadequate

500 Kilometers

FAMILIES

17

18

19

80W

14

A M 37 Pa me

100

0

90W

13

110W

32

20N

Mar atin o Nao lan

EAN

100W

U NG

ALGIC

0

Lambert Azimuthal Equal Area Projection

45

2

30N

M E X I C O

(G ua ch ich il) 0

UTO-AZTECAN

Et

I

G

i c an Mah

c

ia Virginnquian Algo way Notto errin) (Meh 2 li n a 61 a C a r o q u ia n or a r occon A lg o n W

70W

e)

So ut he rn Te pe hu an

noy

N

ua

(Z ac at ec )

32

60

40

Mam uliqu e

ke-Co

ESKIMO-ALEUT

40N

A

G U L F

tuset t sach Mas ganset a Narr

2

U

M

0

p

C

0

o

u

h

o

49

23

ipiQuir uachog Unq

i Unam are Delaw

0

U

(Houma) elous a) ( n r 53 Eastern e Tonkawa est Atakapa C h i t i m a c h a W akapa 50 N A P 54 At A (Washa AK ) 46 AT a w Aranama a nk 41 ra a K

2

egan Moh ot u Peq

ee Muns are Delaw

59

Al ab am a

la) agou ( P a sc Bilo xi

Lou

M

C

a

c

(Op

A C re ek

I

a ra

te il

( Grigra)

0

0

(P a k a n

o

22

T

42

yel)

57

K

aqu

ddy

i

So lan o

52

( Avo

S

E

hit

38

Adai

Ch oc taw

55

Yatasi

Nat che z

)

M

U

sam

1

t

49

O Tawas a

Hitc

3

a

L N

A

D

)

G

)

G

0

iou a) (T ( K oro oo) z (Ya

Kitsai

(T ob os o)

56

O SI a b Cataw

26

N

K

Ch ic ka sa w

51

L

mano)

N

N or th er n Te pe hu an

O

I

a

Wichita

32

32

61

s

-

C o m a n c h e

(C on ch o)

T a ra h u m

U

BA W

TA

CA 58

61

o Nantic

49

a

M

I H C

C O

e Shawne

Yuchi

49

2

wk

A

HA

in

tw

120W

Ofo

bile

A

Q

AN

ST

IC

ALG

Pa

ro

(Suma-Ju

61

Pas

2

a

0

a)

Caddo

3

Mayo

(Pericú)

me

0

51

ai Bid

Tubar

í) 0

iga

Qu ap aw

AZTECAN

Lip an

o

0

(

ch Mi

(Mo

ra

36

(

49

ish)

Guaicu

isee

c

ck

Kiowa Apache

0

C

ón

(H u it e , Zoe)

a icm

no

Osage

tern Eas naki e b A

tern Wes naki Abe

an

Mescale

O

C

Miami

3

E

E

Laim

ño Monqu Didiu i-

I

ie

Moha

u eh

33

T

gome

Kickapoo

Er

K

N

K i o w a

(Jova)

Guarijí

L.

I

Senec

(Erie)

0

UTO-

come)

Fo x

)

a

3

Z

Cade

(Mascouten

al)

rio R

Tutelo i Sapon

(Eye

Eudeve

Yaqui

Mal

in

S usq

ua

0

A

ieño

61

(Neutr

Sauk

nta L. O ro) (Wen

Illinois

Missouri

Ji ca ri ll a

K

N

nqu

ida One a dag Onon a g Cayu

E

Algo Old

2

Kansa

-

Ignac

k ame

n

nqui

on Hur

2

n

D

wa

ro

33

A

Ópata

hit

Upper Umpqua Chemakum Sechelt Pentlatch Squamish Halkomelem Nooksack Northern Straits Clallam Twana Lower Chehalis Upper Chehalis Cowlitz Yoncalla Nomlaki Karok Chimariko Konomihu New River Shasta Okwanuchu Southern Pomo Eastern Pomo Southeastern Pomo Northeastern Pomo Kitanemuk Tataviam

Hu

A

P ir o

mi

2

I

(Pecos) (Ubate)

34

A

Otta

2

G

L

B

N SA R E S. 0 E K Tiwa

W

Tano

33

A

35

Zuni

a

T

Lagun

49

Otoe

51

KIOWA-TANOA

Picuris

33

Jemez

Wi nn eb ag o

A

L

Navajo

P o ta w a to

I o w a

Pawnee

Nawathinehen Rio Grand e Tewa Taos

C

CADDOAN

2

32

J

Omaha

e

Rio Grand e Keresan

-

ena

Arapaho t

L.

N

A

49

N

U

e

(Uc

3a 6a 7a 7b 7c 7d 7e 7f 7g 7h 7i 7j 7k 13a 14a 19 20 21a 21b 21c 23a 23b 23c 23d 32a 32b

27

Attik

ern

ern East wa Ojib

SanteeSisseton

Ponca

G A

Algo

Me no mi ne e

Teton

2

B e sa w u n

Aco m a

O

26

Language areas too small to be labeled on map

Lower P im a n

M

2

A YanktonYanktonai

C he ye nn e

Eastern Shosho ne

N

T

Seri

61

n

Sp

TA

U

51

(J a n o -J o

ño

r e e

North

25

24

LANGUAGE FAMILIES

49

U

Borje

C

H

Arikara

C h ir ic a h

32

C

L

2

A

N-

AN

L

P

KE

LM

M ol a

S

D

Mandan

u

K iliwa

y

uperior ke S La

2

O

NA

S

Hidatsa

49

Upp er Pim an

a

p

I

COO

C r o w

AN

Cocop

m

a O j i b w

Saulteaux

49

(C

Q

a

a l C e n t r e r n S o u t h

re

e

Lu

s

Vent

UTS

ai T ip ai P aip

Apach

ue

2

49

la

ALS

EA

OKAN INO CH

N A DENE

G

uk th o Be

G

2

Y OK

P A C I F I C O C E A N

K

w

62

I

UA

W

130

Plains Cree

S

Thomps on

Black

C

M

i s

a

W

2

Yana

12

30N

I

Severn Ojibwa

n

50

G

g t a

o n

n

S A

L I otk 7b 7c S H a A N Niti 7d nah 7d t Nic Mak 47 ola a 7e 3 CHI Quile h 7 f MA u O t k e ana KUA Koot 6a gan N 6 7g enai Qui nau lt 7h Kwa 7 lh Col Low ioqua 7 um i er C bia 7 n Kalis 7j Ca hino pel Cla thlam ok 3 tska et Tua n 7 k i l e a T Cen tin illa tral -Yam mo Sah 3 Coe Kala hil ok apti d 'A le u r n puy l ne an NY Kiks Flath A N 7 ht aq ead 9 T I Alse uina U a N E 10 1 8 7 P 13 Cayu AN S 10 se U Han iuslaw Nez A 48 i s P erce Milu E 1 k 12 1 13 T a A E 3 a 3 E N Tut u App Galice tni Take leg ate lma 18 32 Nor Tolo t 13 Klam wa Sho hern ath sho ne Yur ok 19 Sha sta Wiy 2 2 1 S 1 ot a Nor 21b th H 21 2 Banno 22 Mat upa 20 14 c Achu ck Paiu ern m tole 3 a P te w A W L i i A ntu Cah R Eel Atsu IHNIH Nor to iver gew AN ther i 14a n Po Yuki 24 mo Mai YUK 29 23d Cen d u IAN tral PO Kon 15 Wes kow Lak Pomo MOAN eM 2 N 3 b Sho tern A Kas iwok 23 sho 23c M A I D U 28 h a ne 16 Wap ya Nise p nan Washo Coa o 23a 29 e st M Miwok iwo Plains N U or Cho Karkin k T I A N C thern Sierra Miwok che O T entral Sierra n Ram yo – M B a i w y o M U k iwok aytu A T a mye Awa sh Z n Southern Sierra Miwok T swa 32 16 E 1 Mut s Vall 6 C M s o ey no Rum un Y o Gas kuts sen how Esse C u Kin (Play len 30 halon ano R i v eg s Anto ) 17 r n Pana i a n o min Tule Sout Mig SAL 25 t ue INAN her -Kaw eah Obis leño Paiut n peñ e T u o b atula Bue Puri Pale bal sime wya V i s tn a m CHU Ineseño ño i a ya Kawa MAS m iisu HAN 32a 31 a) Barb 26 areñ 32b o Islan Havasu Vent Walap d Ch pai Hopi ureñ Serran ai uma o o Chemeh sh u evi o n ieli Moha br A N ve M Ga 11 Cahu U il la Luise Y 3 ño Yavapa Cupe – i ño Ipai M a r icopa Í ha Weste rn c ume yaay No

L

e

p

r

7a

swa

e

oet

t

Lillo

ox

Sarc

1

s t E a e e C r

s

Com

7

Shu

t

Western Swampy Cree

2

W o o d s C r e e

a

5

in

ey

ke

utl

49

AN

5

Ston lcot

E

aki

Chi

rier

Revised and enlarged edition, with additions and corrections, 1999.

2

ro

Kw

3

compiled by: Ives Goddard

SE A

OI

N

Be Co lla ola

i

R

F

Pla in Cre s e

he

50

Car

7

BAY

3

u

O

QU

H Oo eilts we ukkya la

9

2

e

n

D

O

5

bin

A

IR

Ba

H a isla

Tsi Coast ms hia n

C

NIC

R

i

p k a s N a

i

HIA

2

t

MS

N

HUDSO

B e a v e r

8

o

ida

ni

Lake Michigan

TSI

ka

C h i p e w y a n

B

W

N Git assksa n

4

Ha

Se

A

C a n a d i a n

3

ut

NATIVE LANGUAGES AND LANGUAGE FAMILIES OF NORTH AMERICA 40

tsa

1

W

1

Tse

c

A

8 4

ic

30

L

S l a v e y

3

170W

L

O

M

I

K

i

3

D

n

180

n

U

D o g r i b

14

ta

32

31

30

an

1

E

hl

100 200 300 400 500 Kilometers

1

a

A

K a s k a

Ta

Tl in gi t

60N

ee Gr

29

28

27

26

a n d i a n C a

n

nd

Proposed Penutian Superfamily

sh

arl

in

N gi

500 Miles

it

Mo

Ta

400

ra

Be

l

St

7

70N

80 N

110W

e

C

N Tu ort tc he ho rn ne

So Tu ut tc he ho rn ne

300

N

n

t

H a r e

3

la st n Ea ree G

n

a

s

AN

17 31 2 5 26 29 54 28 61 2 3 53 1 32 2 1 51 49 14 14 2 49 2 0 24 49 32 10 51 26 55 17 13a 58 29 29 2 0 0 35

K11 Migueleño . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J10-11 Miluk . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K17 Missouri . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L18 (Mobile) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L13 Mohave . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J21 Mohawk . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Mohegan-Pequot . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J21 J11 Molala . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K12 Mono . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M13 Monqui-Didiu . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . G22-23 Montagnais . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . E10-11 Mountain . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Munsee Delaware . . . . . . . . . . . . . J20-21 L18 MUSKOGEAN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K11 Mutsun . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . E9-14, J10, J10-11,L14 NADENE . . . . . . K20-21 Nanticoke-Conoy . . . . . . . . . . . . N16 Naolan . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . F21-22 Naskapi . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . F10 Nass-Gitksan . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L17 Natchez . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . D2 Naukanski Yupik . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K14 Navajo . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K15 Nawathinehena . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J19 (Neutral) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J11 New River Shasta . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H12 Nez Perce . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H11-12 Nicola . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K11 Nisenan . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H10-11 Nitinaht . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J11 Nomlaki . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H11 Nooksack . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H10-11 Nootka . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . North Alaskan Inupiaq . . . . . . . . . . D3-4 Northeastern Pomo . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K11 Northern Algonquin . . . . . . . . . . . . . H20 K11 Northern Pomo. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J12 Northern Paiute . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Northern Shoshone . . . . . . . . . . . . J12-13 Northern Sierra Miwok . . . . . . . . K11-12 Northern Straits . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H11 Northern Tepehuan . . . . . . . . . . . M14-15 Northern Tutchone . . . . . . . . . . . . . . E8-9 K20 Nottoway . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K11 Obispeño . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K18 Ofo . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H12 Okanagan . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J11 Okwanuchu . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H20 Old Algonquin . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J16 Omaha . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J20 Oneida . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J20 Onondaga . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L14 Ópata . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L17 (Opelousa) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K17 Osage . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J16 Otoe . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . N15-16 OTOMANGUEAN . . . . . . . . . . . J19 Ottawa . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . F4-E6 Pacific Yupik . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L12 Paipai . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L18 (Pakana) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J11 PALAIHNIHAN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K12 Palewyami . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . N15 Pame . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K12 Panamint . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L17-18 (Pascagoula) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Passamaquoddy . . . . . . . . . . . . . H22-J22 K11 Patwin . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J16 Pawnee . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K14-15 (Pecos) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H11 Pentlatch . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . N14 (Pericú) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K15 Picuris . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L14 Piro . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . F13-G14 Plains Cree . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K11 Plains Miwok . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J11-H12 PLATEAU PENUTIAN . . . . . . K11 (Playano) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K11 POMOAN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J15-16 Ponca . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J18-19 Potawatomi . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L11 Purisimeño . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L17 Quapaw . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L13 Quechan . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H10-11 Quileute . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H10-11 Quinault . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M15-16 Quinigua . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J21 Quiripi-Unquachog . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K11 Ramaytush . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K14 Rio Grande Keresan . . . . . . . . . . . . . K14 Rio Grande Tewa . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K11 Rumsen . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H11-12 Sahaptin . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K11 SALINAN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . G11-12 SALISHAN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Santee-Sisseton . . . . . . . . . . . . . H17-J17 G12-13 Sarcee . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J19 Sauk . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . G16-H16 Saulteaux . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . G11-H11 Sechelt . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . F10-11 Sekani . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J20 Seneca . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M13 Seri . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L12 Serrano . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . G17-18 Severn Ojibwa . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Seward Peninsula Inupiaq . . . . . . . . D2-3 J11 Shasta . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J11 SHASTAN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J19 Shawnee . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . G11-12 Shuswap . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H16-J17, K19-20 SIOUAN-CATAWBA . . . E1 Sirenikski . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J11 Siuslaw . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . E11-12 Slavey . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M15 Solano . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Southeastern Pomo . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K11 Southern Paiute . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K13 K11 Southern Pomo . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Southern Sierra Miwok . . . . . . . . K11-12 Southern Tepehuan . . . . . . . . . . . N14-15 K14-L14 Southern Tiwa . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Southern Tutchone . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . E8 Spokane . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H12 Squamish . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H11 Stoney . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . G12 (Suma-Jumano) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L14-15 Susquehannock . . . . . . . . . . . . . J20-K20 Tagish . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . F9 Tahltan . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . F9-10 (Tahue) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M14-N14 Takelma . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J11 TAKELMAN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J11 Tamyen . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K11 Tanacross . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . E7 Tanaina . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . E5 Tano . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K14-15 Taos . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K14-15 Tarahumara . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M14 Tataviam . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L12 Tawasa . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L18 Tepecano . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . N15 Teton . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H15 Thompson . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . G11-H11 Tillamook . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H10-11 Timucua . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M19 TIMUCUAN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L19-M19 (Tiou) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L17-18 Tipai . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L12 Tlingit . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . F9 (Toboso) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M15 Tolowa . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J11 Tonkawa . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L16 Tsetsaut . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . F9-10 TSIMSHIANIC . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . G9-10 Tualatin-Yamhill . . . . . . . . . . . . . H10-11 M14 Tubar . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K12 Tubatulabal . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K12 Tule-Kaweah . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L17 Tunica . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K20 Tuscarora . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K20 Tutelo-Saponi . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J11 Tututni . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H11 Twana . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K14 (Ubate) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . N13-14 (Uchití) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J20-K21 Unami Delaware . . . . . . . . . . . . H11 Upper Chehalis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . E4-5 Upper Kuskokwim . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L13 Upper Piman . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . E7 Upper Tanana . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Upper Umpqua . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J11 K14 Ute . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K11 UTIAN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K12-14, UTO-AZTECAN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L13-M14, L15 ........ K11 Valley Yokuts . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L12 Ventureño . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K20 Virginia Algonquian . . . . . . . . . . . . . G10-H10 WAKASHAN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K13 Walapai . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K11 Wappo . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M18 (Washa) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K11-12 Washoe . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J20 (Wenro) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J21 Western Abenaki . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Western Apache . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L13-14 M16-L17 Western Atakapa . . . . . . . . . . . Western Canadian Inuit . . . . . . . . D10-17 Western Shoshone . . . . . . . . . . . . . J12-13 Western Swampy Cree . . . . . . . . . F16-18 West Greenlandic . . . . . . . . . . . C25-E26 L16 Wichita . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J18 Winnebago . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J11 Wintu . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J11-K11 WINTUAN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J11 Wiyot . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K20 Woccon . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . F15-G15 Woods Cree . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . N14 (Xiximé) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J11 Yana . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J16 Yankton-Yanktonai . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M13 Yaqui . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J10-11 Yaquina . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L17 Yatasi . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L13 Yavapai . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L17 (Yazoo) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K11-12 YOKUTSAN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J11 Yoncalla . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K18 Yuchi . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K11 Yuki . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K11 YUKIAN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J11 Yurok . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M14-15 (Zacatec) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M14 (Zoe) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K14 Zuni . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .

WINTU

3 61 1 30 2 32 3 0 7 2 32 3 39 17 0 2 0 36 0 32 0 4 5 7d 3 12 3 26 5 49 56 3 32 0 0 3 61 26 2 31 3 49 26 61 31 0 33 3 32 7 49 46 16 19 23 3 32 34 2 9 26 17 33 3 33 32a 51 18 56 15 21a 47 55 3 26 3 5 3 34 26 16 61 7 3 2 7i 9 32 3 32 7 2 15 15 5 2 39 49 43 26 2 2 3 32 61 2 3 2 0 2

Location

KA

2 23b 32 2 1

25 12 49 56 26 61 2 18 32 26 2 3 2 56 16 3 2 45 2 8 57 1 3 2 61 21b 18 3 15 5 14a 7e 5 1 23d 2 23 32 32 16 7f 32 3 61 31 49 7 21c 2 49 61 61 32 0 49 49 37 2 1 26 0 22 17 37 32 0 2 14 51 33 7b 0 33 33 2 16 18 25 23 49 2 31 49 26 6 7 44 2 16 34 33 16 18 25 7 49 3 2 2 7a 3 61 27 32 2 1 21 21 2 7 49 1 11 3 42 23c 32 23a 16 32 33 3 7 7c 49 0 61 3 3 32 13 13 16 3 3 33 33 32 32b 59 32 49 7 7 59 59 55 26 3 0 3 50 3 8 13 3 32 17 55 61 49 3 7h 0 0 2 7j 3 32 3 3a 32 16 32

s

ak

WA

10 10 25 56 3 41 2 51 49 53 22 2 0 16 3 32 31 16 3 3 7 62 2 0 49 2 26 17 51 51 26 3 32 60 2 3 49 9 61 48 1 13 23 1 16 2 16 0 6a 32 61 2 56 3 6 20 9 3 3 54 16 56 31 7g 3 38 16 8 26 26 7 7 32 39 39 7 0 12 40 7k 56 49 32 31 3 2 2 53 1

M14-N14 (Acaxee) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J11 Achumawi . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K14-L14 Acoma . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L17 Adai . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . E6-7 Ahtna . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L18 Alabama . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . G2-3, inset Aleut . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . G14-16, G20-23, J10, ALGIC . . . . . . . . . . J14-K15, J17-19, J21-22 ....... J10-11 Alsea . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J10 ALSEAN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K11 Antoniano . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L18-19 Apalachee . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J11 Applegate . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M16 Aranama . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K15 Arapaho . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H15-16 Arikara . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H14-15 Assiniboine . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M17 ATAKAPAN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J11 Atsugewi . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H21 Attikamek . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L17 (Avoyel) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K11 Awaswas . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . G10 Babine . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J13 Bannock . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L11 Barbareño . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K11 Bay Miwok . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . D10-11 Bearlake . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . F11-12 Beaver . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . G10 Bella Coola . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H24 Beothuk . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J14-15 Besawunena . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L16-17 (Bidai) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L18 Biloxi . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H13 Blackfoot . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M12-13 Borjeño . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K12 Buena Vista . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L17 Caddo . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J15-16,L16-17 CADDOAN . . . . . . . . . . . M13 Cadegomeño . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K11 Cahto . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L12 Cahuilla . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M19 Calusa . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Carolina Algonquian . . . . . . . . . . . . . K20 G11 Carrier . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K19 Catawba . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H11 Cathlamet . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J20 Cayuga . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H12 Cayuse . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Central Alaskan Yupik . . . . . . . . . . F3-E3 J10-11 Central Kalapuyan . . . . . . . . . . . . K10-11 Central Pomo . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Central Siberian Yupik . . . . . . . . . D1-E1 K11-12 Central Sierra Miwok . . . . . . . . . Central Southern Ojibwa . . . . . . G17-H18 K11 Chalon . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L18 (Chatot) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H11 Chemakum . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L12-13 Chemehuevi . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L19-K19 Cherokee . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J15 Cheyenne . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L18 Chickasaw . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . G11 Chilcotin . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H10-11 CHIMAKUAN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J11 Chimariko . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H11 CHINOOKAN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . F14-15 Chipewyan . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L14 Chiricahua . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M17 Chitimacha . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K11 Chochenyo. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L18 Choctaw . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L11-12 CHUMASHAN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H11 Clallam . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H10-11 Clatskanie . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M15-16 Coahuilteco . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K11 Coast Miwok . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . G10 Coast Tsimshian . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L12-K13 COCHIMÍ-YUMAN . . . . . . . . . L13 Cocopa . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H12 Coeur d'Alene . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H11-12 Columbian . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L15 Comanche . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M15-16 COMECRUDAN . . . . . . . . . . . . M16 Comecrudo . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . G10-11 Comox . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M15 (Concho) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J10 COOSAN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M16 Cotoname . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H11 Cowlitz . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L18-19 Creek . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H14 Crow . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L12 Cupeño . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K11-L12 (Cuyama) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . E11-12 Dogrib . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . G20-21 East Cree . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Eastern Abenaki . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H21-22 L17-M17 Eastern Atakapa . . . . . . . . . . . . Eastern Canadian Inuit . . . . . . . . B19-21, C19-D20, E20-F23 .... H19-20 Eastern Ojibwa . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K11 Eastern Pomo . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Eastern Shoshone . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J14 Eastern Swampy Cree . . . . . . . . G18-H19 East Greenlandic . . . . . . C30-31, D28-29, E27-28 .............. J11-K11 Eel River . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J20 (Erie) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . F3-C7, D16-24 ESKIMO-ALEUT . . . . . . K11 Esselen . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J22 Etchemin . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M14 Eudeve . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . E7 Eyak . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L16-17 (Eyeish) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H13 Flathead . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J19 Fox . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L12 Gabrielino . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J11 Galice . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M16 Garza . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K12 Gashowu . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L17 (Grigra) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . G13-H14 Gros Ventre . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . N15-16 (Guachichil) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M13 Guaicura . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L19 (Guale) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M14 Guarijío . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M14-N14 (Guasave) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . G9 Haida . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . G10 Haisla . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H11 Halkomelem . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . E7-8 Han . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J10-11 Hanis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . D10 Hare . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K13 Havasupai . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Heiltsuk-Oowekyala . . . . . . . . . . . . . G10 H15 Hidatsa . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L18-19 Hitchiti . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . E3-4 Holikachuk . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K13-14 Hopi . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L17 (Houma) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M14 (Huite) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J11 Hupa . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J20 Huron . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M13 Ignacieño . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J17-18 Illinois . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L11-12 Ineseño . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . E4 Ingalik . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J16-17 Iowa . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L12 Ipai . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J20-21, L19-K19 IROQUOIAN . . . . . . . . L11 Island Chumash . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L14 (Jano-Jocome) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K14 Jemez . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K15 Jicarilla . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M14 (Jova) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H12 Kalispel . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K16 Kansa . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M16 Karankawa . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K11 Karkin. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J11 Karok . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K11 Kashaya . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . E9-10 Kaska . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K12 Kawaiisu . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K14 KERESAN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J19 Kickapoo . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H11 Kiksht. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L12 Kiliwa . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K12 Kings River . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K15-16 Kiowa . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K16 Kiowa Apache . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L14-K15, K15-16 KIOWA-TANOAN . . . . K12-L12 Kitanemuk . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L16 Kitsai . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J11 Klamath . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L18-K19 Koasati . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K11 Konkow . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J11 Konomihu . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H12-13 Kootenai . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L17 (Koroa) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . D4-5 Koyukon . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L12 Kumeyaay . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . D6-8 Kutchin . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . G10 Kwakiutl . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H10-11 Kwalhioqua . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K14-L14 Laguna . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M13 Laimón . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K11 Lake Miwok . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H21 Laurentian . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . G11 Lillooet . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M15-16 Lipan . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J21 Loup . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H11 Lower Chehalis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H10-11 Lower Chinook . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M13 Lower Piman . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . E6 Lower Tanana . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L12 Luiseño . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H11 Lushootseed . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J21 Mahican . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J11 Maidu . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K11 MAIDUAN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H10-11 Makah . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H22 Maliseet . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M15 Mamulique . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H15 Mandan . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . N16 Maratino . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L13 Maricopa . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J18-19 (Mascouten) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Massachusett-Narragansett . . . . . . J21-22 J11 Mattole . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M14 Mayo . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K20 (Meherrin) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H18 Menominee . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . L14-15 Mescalero . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . J18 Miami . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K17-18 (Michigamea) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H22-23 Micmac . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .

H

r n t e s a E

e

e

1

C

is

Ey Color Linguistic Unit

os

W

e

1

av

32 22 34 52 3 56 1 2

Location

cr

200

100

170E

U Ta pp na er na

Italicized names appear only as numbers on the map Color Linguistic Unit

na

20

50N

0 0

t

u

e

1

1

n

W

na

k

t

D

i

s

l

A

Proposed Hokan Superfamily

i

a

p

ht

1

50

h

B

AY

e

in

c

F

N FI

1

em

t

n

u

W

1

55N

Scale: 1:15,000,000

r

ai

MAP INDEX

Y

ALEUTIAN ISLANDS Lambert Azimuthal Equal Area Projection

G

n

c

1

1

Ta

1

3

fi

an adi Can it Inu

W

T

TA NO AN

l

a tr n

R

t

Eas

ern

80W

n

Ce

O

3

A

i

W

N

90W

ko

Ta

t

c

50

BA

u

k

1

eu

a

I C

yu K

L Ta ow n e an r a

1

Al

P

W

70W

F

A

T

3

Upper Kuskokwim

E

E

40

B

U

25

24

23

22

21

W

A

13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20

o

li Ho

uk kach

ali

2

S

S

k pi K

K

E

12

60

B

E

11

E 1 Gr ast ee nl

1

i

rd wa nsula q ia ni Pe Inup

or

a

Al

th

T

A

upiaq n In L ska

30W

A

C

E

A

U

Ing

A

la

an

C

10

9

IO W A

Se

nsk uka Na pik Yu

tral

n

O

R

8

120

Cen

ik

eria

17

sk

6

l tra Cen erian Sib pik Yu

1

Sib

0W

Yu

A

0W

M I

5

4

3

2

1

ik s k i

Yup

N G R I B E

A S E

N

7

15



O

Aleutian Islands continue on inset, far right of map

N

100W

70N

S ir e n

60N

180

80 W 160

nla East nd ic

1

Source: Department of Anthropology, National Museum of Natural History, Smithsonian Institution. ­Compiled by Ives Goddard. Revised and enlarged edition, with additions and corrections, 1999. [Note: This is a revised version of the map published in volume 17 of the Handbook of North American Indians and by University of Nebraska Press. It was originally designed by Jeannine Schonta at the Maryland Cartographic Division of GeoSystems (later renamed MapQuest.com), including the later enlarged version published by University of Nebraska Press.]

20

21

Linguistic

Uninhabited

Outside subject area

Linguistic Units Languages, Dialects (Undocumented) Boundaries International

State or Provincial