198 12 3MB
English Pages 164 [168] Year 1986
The Phonology of Cliticization
Egon Berendsen
The Phonology of Cliticization
¥
1986 FORIS PUBLICATIONS Dordrecht - Holland/Riverton - U.S.A.
Published by: Foris Publications Holland P.O. Box 509 3300 AM Dordrecht, The Netherlands Sole distributor for the U.S.A. and Canada: Foris Publications U.S.A. P.O. Box C-50 Riverton N.J. 08077 U.S.A. Cip-data
ISBN 9 0 6 7 6 5 255 5 © 1986 Foris Publications - Dordrecht No part of this publication may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopy, recording, or any information storage and retrieval system, without permission from the copyright owner. Printed in the Netherlands by ICG Printing, Dordrecht.
Contents
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS 1. INTRODUCTION 1.1. Introductory remarks on cliticization 1.2. Background assumptions 1.2.1. The organization of grammar with respect to phonology 1.2.2. Phonological representations
vii 1 1 6 6 6
2. PROLEGOMENA TO A THEORY O F CLITICIZATION 2.1. Types of clitics 2.2. Phonological aspects of cliticization 2.3. A prosodie theory of cliticization 2.3.1. M-incorporation 2.3.2. 0-incorporation 2.A. Summary Notes to Chapter 2.
19 19 23 25 28 29 30 33
3. THE PHONOLOGY OF DUTCH CLITICIZATION 3.1. Introduction 3.2. Dutch clitics 3.3. The syntax of Dutch in a nut-shell 3.4. Dutch clitics and voicing phenomena 3.4.1. Introduction 3.4.2. The behaviour of clitics with respect to voice 3.4.2.1. Initial observations 3.4.2.2. A prosodie account 3.4.2.3. Consonant-initial clitics 3.4.3. Concluding remarks 3.5. Schwa reduction in Dutch cliticization 3.5.1. Introduction 3.5.2. The input structure of CSR 3.5.3. The output structure of CSR 3.5.4. Conclusions 3.6. Ν in hiatus 3.6.1. Introduction 3.6.2. N-deletion 3.6.3. N-insertion 3.6.4. Concluding remarks 3.7. Conclusion Notes to Chapter 3.
35 35 36 Al 45 45 48 48 52 57 74 75 75 75 80 84 84 84 86 91 94 95 97
-
vi
-
4. CLITIC PHONOLOGY IN OTHER LANGUAGES 4.1. Introduction 4.2. Cliticization, lexical and post-lexical processes 4.2.1. Cairene Arabic 4.2.1.1. Hollow verb roots 4.2.1.2. Hollow verb roots and lexical processes 4.3. Prosodie structure and cliticization 4.3.1. Dakota 4.3.1.1. Shaw's analysis 4.3.1.2. A new account of cliticization in Dakota 4.3.2. English 4.3.2.1. Destressing of function words 4.3.2.2. A prosodie account 4.4. Phonological processes and cliticization 4.4.1. Cibaeno Spanish 4.4.1.1. Harris' analysis of Liquid Gliding 4.4.1.2. Clitics and Liquid Gliding revisited 4.4.2. Greek 4.4.2.1. Stressed clitics 4.4.2.2. Some extensions 4.4.3. Palestinian Arabic 4.4.3.1. High Vowel Deletion and Epenthesis 4.4.3.2. Some remarks with respect to cliticization 4.4.4. Margi 4.4.4.1. Tone Polarity 4.4.4.2. A reanalysis of the Margi clitic facts 4.5. Conclusion Notes to Chapter 4.
99 99 101 101 101 102 103 103 103 105 107 107 109 114 114 114 116 117 117 120 123 123 125 128 128 136 139 141
5. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS
143
REFERENCES
149
INDEX
155
Acknowledgements
Many people have contributed to the realization of this thesis. I would like to take
the
opportunity to thank them here, although it is impossible to mention
them all. However, the following people deserve to be mentioned explicitly. First, Wim Zonneveld and Henk Verkuyl who were most intimately involved with this
work. I would like to thank Wim for his many suggestions for improvements
of content and exposition. My gratitude also goes to Henk, who
suggested
some
important changes and supported me in meeting deadlines. Secondly, I would like to thank René Kager and Ellis Visch for their careful reading
of
the prefinal version and for their requests for clarification, and
Marcel van den Broecke for his suggestions and his help with my English. grateful
to
Professor
Cohen
Phonetics at the University of Utrecht for allowing me to finish this am
I
am
and Marcel van den Broecke of the Department of work.
I
also grateful to the members of my dissertation committee, Prof. Bordelois,
Prof. Cohen and Henk Schultink, for the time they spent in reading
the
manus-
cript. Furthermore, I would like to thank Riny Huybregts and Thijs Pollmann for introducing me to linguistics, my former
colleagues of the Section
guistics of the Institute De Vooys and my colleagues of the netics both at the University of Utrecht,
Modern
Lin-
Department of Pho-
for their support,
and Wim de Haas,
Det Paulissen, Mieke Trommelen and Jip Wester for their part in creating such a stimulating phonological environment in Utrecht. Finally, without the non-linguistic support
of
Berendsen this study would not have materialized.
Diana,
Sjoerd
and
Marina
Chapter 1
Introduction
1. Introductory remarks on cliticization
One inevitable task of the modern linguist is the isolation of the morphemes of the language he investigates. When he does this, he finds that they come in two types, bound and unbound. Unbound morphemes, like task, type, hand, and so may
appear
independently,
say,
intuitively
as
'words'.
on,
Bound morphemes ,
however, may not, and we may say that they are the affixes of the
language
in
question, such as -s for plural, un- for negation, and so forth. The latter may occur in plurals such as tasks, types, and hands, and the former in tive
such
an
adjec-
as unbound, but statements such as they are -s and it turned out to
be un- are utterly unacceptable. It will come as no surprise that it is not always easy to this
simple
binary
classification
clearly
maintain
among morphemes. The most recalcitrant of
them are the so-called clitics. Thus, Zwicky (1977, p. 1), one of the most nowned
linguists
working
in
the
languages (...) have morphemes that present analytic difficulties because are
neither
re-
area of cliticization, observes that 'most they
clearly independent words nor clearly affixes. The problem is re-
cognized (...) in traditional language descriptions, where certain elements are set
apart from the ordinary words and affixes of the language by being labeled
clitics.' He then goes on as follows: 'For generative grammarians these
diffi-
culties were obscured for some years, since the traditional domain of morphology was assumed to be apportioned between syntax and phonology. With a return to the
traditional
position that morphological structure and syntactic structure
obey different principles by and large, as
do
morphophonemics
and
phonology
proper, has come the realization that there are borderline cases (...) The generative grammarian's striving for both precision and generalization in linguistic
descriptions
has,
in
fact,
led to the uncovering of a host of analytic
problems (...)'. As an example, consider the Dutch question
is
asked
example
in
(1),
where
a
and answered. If the answer consists of a clitic only, the
answer is ungrammatical. In this respect, the clitic behaves like a bound
mor-
-
2
-
pheme.
(1)
Wie heeft dat gedaan? *Ie Hij
(Who did it?) (He (clitic)) (He (unbound))
On the other hand, the environment does not always seem to govern clitic occurrence:
verbs
(gaf),
nouns (boek), interrogative pronouns (wie), and conjunc-
tions (dat) can occur to the left of ie and articles (een), prepositions (aan), nouns (boeken), and verbs (ging) may occur to its right, as in (2).
(2)a
Gaf ie een boek? (Did he give a book?) Ik vroeg welk boek ie aan Henk gaf. (I asked which book he did give to Henk) Ik vroeg wie ie boeken gaf. (I asked to whom he did give books) Ik dacht dat ie ging. (I thought he went)
b c d
In generative syntax, there has been interest in cliticization-phenomena the
late
sixties.
This
interest
was
order, and, more specifically, by the role of the syntactic language.
In
cycle
(1975)
natural
(1971)
and
on surface filters, and those by Kayne (1969, 1975) on the syn-
tactic cycle. From this, an interest grew in issues selves,
in
these cases, clitics could be used as highly illuminating illus-
trations as demonstrated by a number of studies such as Perlmutter Emonds
since
initially aroused by research on word
concerning
clitics
them-
such as the relationship between weak clitic form and strong form, or,
in other words, the question whether latter,
and
if
so,
the
former
must
be
derived
from
the
whether this must be so for all syntactic positions (cf.
Kayne (1975), Emonds (1975), Strozer (1976), Jaeggli (1982)), and, more recently,
clitic doubling phenomena (cf. Jaeggli (1982), Borer (1984), Aoun (1982)),
and the question theoretical
where
notions
clitics
such
stand
with
respect
to
recently
developed
as the θ-criterion, the case filter, government and
binding, and so on (cf. Jaeggli (1982), Borer (1984), Aoun (1982)). These tactic
investigations
our understanding of this phenomenon, but with almost equal say
that
syn-
into the nature of cliticization have no doubt enlarged certainty
we
can
there is still a great deal about syntactic aspects of cliticization
-
3
-
that is unclear or even unexplored.
As compared to the generative syntactic literature on cliticization, the phonological literature on this topic is much more limited and, apparently relatively more observational. This may be due to the ways syntacticians
and
phonolo-
gists have until recently looked upon this phenomenon. As Kaisse (1985) puts it in the preface of her things,
book:
'(there
are)
syntacticians
interested
in
big
syntacticians interested in little things, arid (...) phonologists. The
first group concerned themselves with movement rules (...);
the
second
group
thought about the generation (or local movement, at most) of single morphemes clitics, auxiliaries, and so forth. Phonologists (...) dealt only segments
within
single
with
single
words.' Even if this view strikes one as an exaggera-
tion, it does contain at least a grain of
truth,
and
so
it
is
really
not
surprising that we do not understand very well the phonological characteristics of cliticization, of which the frequent unstressability of sometimes
clitics
and
logical processes are the most remarkable ones. Cliticization has been of
their
slightly baffling behaviour with respect to otherwise regular phonothought
by phonologists primarily as a sub-area of syntax. Of course, syntacticians
concerned as they are with mining the deeper areas of grammar, were bled
very
not
gated the side-effects of their proposals to the waste-basket of the tive
trou-
much with the surface appearance of their business, and often releinterpre-
components, phonology in this case. Phonologists, if interested, then ti-
died things up with notions such as minor rules, boundary weakening, or just
a
separate clitic boundary. Let me give some brief examples of this situation. For English, Selkirk (1972) postulates a Post Verb Pronoun Clitic mation,
which
Chomsky-adjoins
monosyllabic
personal
pronouns
Transfor-
to preceding
verbs, resulting in structure (3).
V
(3) V
CI
This transformation is assumed solely on the basis of phonological evidence. Of course,
one
wonders why a syntactic operation is thought necessary when there
is no syntactic evidence for such an operation. Specifically, tions
between
syntactic
rela-
elements are disturbed without any convincing syntactic reason.
Furthermore, even if there would be syntactic evidence for this operation,
the
-
4
-
structure in (3) does not provide any information about how it acts in the phonological component of grammar. Clearly, Selkirk wants it to act with
respect
as
one
unit
to phonological operations, but this does not follow immediately
from its syntactic representation. Our second example is taken from Shaw (1980). Following Chambers (1974), she assumes
that
in
Dakota
clitics are separated from the preceding stem by the
clitic-boundary = blocking the rule that assigns primary stress to syllable
from
the
left
in
polysyllabic
words
monosyllabic words. This boundary has the same influence as the on
phonological
stressed
on
processes.
the
clitic-boundary
first
the
second
and to the only syllable in word
boundary
Thus, monosyllabic words followed by a clitic are
syllable.
Of
course,
the
postulation
of
this
is in fact no more than saying that clitics are clitics and as
such relatively uninsightful, and perhaps contrary to fact: if they behave like words,
one's
first
thought is to represent them as such rather than as some-
thing different. We will take the opportunity to come back to Dakota cliticization in Chapter 4.3.1. Finally, OzkaragBz (1981) assumes for Turkish an optional process
of
boun-
dary weakening (from # to =) and a subsequent obligatory rule of vowel-deletion deriving
clitics
vowel-harmony.
from
words
However,
it
in
casual
speech.
These
clitics
undergo
is highly unlikely that vowel-harmony is a casual
speech process. It seems quite clear that such cases reflect a considerable amount of uncertainty with regard to the phonology of cliticization. In my opinion, this situation is essentially due to the confusion of two notions: first, the acceptance that
syntax
and
phonology
are
to
a
large extent mutually autonomous, and
second, the premature conclusion that the phenomenon therefore
have
of
cliticization
as so often, cliticization may have both a syntactic and a the
specific
should
to belong to either. This to the detriment of the option that, phonological
task of the linguist being to reconcile these two to the best of
his abilities. In a broad sense this also describes the actual purpose of study.
The
side,
view
expressed
this
above is not new, and has been worked out for in-
stance in recent work by Klavans (1982, 1985) and Zwicky and Pullum (1983), although
in
this work, without denying it its useful and precise character, the
exact nature of phonological cliticization is not clarified. My decision, embarking
upon
this
study,
standing of the various aspects of cliticization phonology
introduced
seems
have
to
by
several
when
was to try to find links towards a better under-
Liberman advantages.
with
respect
to
non-linear
(1975) and Goldsmith (1976). This approach First,
the
hierarchical
notation
of
-
non-linear,
5
-
especially metrical, phonology has proved itself to be excellently
suited for establishing the Groundbreaking
proper
relation
between
syntax
and
phonology.
work in this respect has been executed by Selkirk (1980b,c) and
Nespor and Vogel (1982). Furthermore, one of the main results of this and other work
was
the discovery that boundary-symbols, such as those postulated by Oz-
karagöz (1981) and Shaw (1980), are not the proper phenomena
mechanism
account
for
of this type. Napoli and Nespor (1979) and Kaisse (1985), among oth-
ers, show that the theory of boundaries proposed by Chomsky and
to
Selkirk
(1972)
and
Halle
(1968)
is inadequate for handling several phonological processes
found in a variety of languages. Furthermore, Selkirk (1980b) argues that
this
theory predicts phonological processes which do not seem to exist. And finally, Napoli and Nespor (1979) argue that the postulation of boundary deletions in
the
(and
same vein: weakenings) overgenerates heavily in ways that seem to defy
constraining.
In Chapter 2. of this study, I will outline my
prosodie
clitic
theory
along
general lines. First, I will briefly go into the various types of clitics known from the literature and will limit the discussion to one type: lexically stored clitics
which cliticize phonologically. Secondly, I will consider more or less
general phonological aspects of cliticization, briefly alluded to above. I will illustrate
these
aspects
by
briefly
comparing
cliticization
in
various
languages, pointing to similarities and differences. Third, a general
prosodie
theory of cliticization will be established, accounting in broad terms for phonological properties of the lexically theory
will
be
parametric,
parameters which make it
stored
clitics.
This
prosodie
clitic
i.e. it will be shown to have certain options or
possible
to
account
for
the
differences
between
languages with respect to cliticization phenomena. The best imaginable test for a general theory of a linguistic phenomenon to
put
it
to
the
test
language. We set ourselves this task in Chapter 3., where the of
is
in a thoroughly executed analysis of one particular prosodie
theory
cliticization is tested against a wide variety of phonological processes in
Dutch which seem to take cliticization into account.
The
following
processes
will be considered: Syllable Final Devoicing and other voicing phenomena, Schwa Reduction in processes
clitics,
N-deletion
and
N-insertion.
The
analyses
of
these
with respect to cliticization provide firm evidence for the prosodie
clitic theory proposed in Chapter 2. Since the second best imaginable test for a linguistic theory is to see it
how
works in a variety of preferably unrelated languages, we will in Chapter 4.
-
6
-
give a survey of cliticization phenomena in Dakota, English, Greek,
Palestinian
surveyed
Spanish,
Arabic, Margi, and Cairene Arabic, basing ourselves on the
literature, but reanalyzing the observations phenomena
Cibaeno
made
there
in
our
terms.
The
are both linear and non-linear in nature, and they have to
do with the proper position of clitics in prosodie
structure.
Again,
I
will
show how they fit into the general theory proposed in Chapter 2. In particular, the different parameters of this theory will be shown to be relevant. Finally, Chapter 5. summarizes our findings. In that chapter, we
will
also
take the opportunity to make some concluding remarks. Since my background assumptions are crucial for an understanding of the proposals
and analyses in the rest of this study, I will first deal with them be-
fore going on with the next chapter.
2. Background assumptions
2.1. The organization of grammar with respect to phonology
In Chomsky and Halle (1968) (=SPE) the phonological rules of grammar were
ori-
ginally considered to apply simply after all syntactic rules, i.e. to syntactic surface structure containing items in their underlying where
surface
form.
In
those
cases
structure failed to be an adequate representation for phonology
to operate on, it was adjusted by so-called readjustment rules
placed
between
syntax and phonology proper. Among the tasks of these rules was to insert boundary-symbols into the
bracketed
syntactic
structure.
Certain
operations took also place here. This SPE-model is given in (4).
morphological
-
w
7
-
SYNTAX
LEXICON lexical items
->
->
SEMANTICS
base rules
1 transformations
i
surface structure
i PHONOLOGY readjustment rules
1 phonological rules
i
phonetic representation
This view of the organization of grammar, since
SPE.
After
the
place in grammar, notably as part of the basket
module
of
however,
has
considerably
changed
publication of Chomsky (1970), morphology took its own lexicon
rather
than
in
the
waste
the omnifunctional readjustment sub-component. This line of
research was taken up by Halle (1973), Siegel (1974), and Aronoff (1976). These studies
investigated
the contents of the morphological component of a genera-
tive grammar, specifically with regards to its rule types and internal
organi-
zation. One of the most relevant consequences of these investigations was the
claim
that some morphological operations were in fact dependent upon information provided by phonological rules, and vice versa. Thus, a certain type of entered
the
phonology
lexicon, leaving the phonological component. Out of this grew the
model of so-called Lexical Phonology which assumes a certain amount of interaction
between
morphology
and
lexical
phonology (cf. Mascaro (1976), Mohanan
(1981), Kiparsky (1982), and Strauss (1982)). Of course, the phonological component came out none the worse and its proper characteristics
may
now be considered the following. Lexical phonology has as
its domain 'the word', and rules apply cyclically after the word's cal
structure.
Post-lexical
(and
post-syntactic)
'sentence'-phonology, and non-cyclic. Its domains, however, ways
morphologi-
phonology are
prosodie,
is in
to be specified below. Boundaries have been recognized as an improper way
of limiting these domains, after Selkirk (1980b), who proposed instead
a
map-
ping of syntactic surface structures into prosodie structures to mark phonolog-
-
8
-
ical domains. Furthermore, she proposed that phonological rules which act
upon
syntactic information, apply before the mapping of syntactic surface structures into prosodie structures and that phonological rules proper
apply
after
this
mapping. Berendsen (1985a) argues for the same division on the basis of the effects of phonologically empty elements upon
a
certain
type
of
phonological
rules. Recently, Kaisse (1985) has augmented this model of post-lexical phonology with a fourth subcomponent to account for rules,
which
are
influenced
so-called
late,
casual
speech
by performance factors such as style and speech
rate. Under these assumptions, the organization of grammar is that of (5).
(5)
SYNTAX
LEXICON lexical items
base rules ->
1 transformations
-> SEMANTICS |
I surface structure
Ï PHONOLOGY phonosyntactic rules
I mapping rules
Ì phonological rules
i casual speech rules
I phonetic representation
It is this model of grammar that I take to be
one
of
the
basic
assumptions
behind the research into the phonology of cliticization reported on below.
-
9
-
2.2. Phonological representations
Another area of
research
representations.
has
concentrated
on
the
nature
of
phonological
Until the raid-seventies a phonological representation in gen-
erative phonology was considered to exist of a linear non-hierarchical sequence of
segments
and
boundaries.
Each of these segments consisted of a bundle of
distinctive features, each with a plus or a minus value. At least two sorts boundaries
were
assumed:
the
affix
of
boundary (+) and the word boundary (#).
Word-boundaries arose by insertion rules in the readjustment component, operating
upon
the labelled bracketing of surface syntax. These boundaries could be
altered by subsequent readjustments in various ways, for instance #,
by
deleting
or by weakening # to +. To give an example: the syntactic surface structure
in (6a) was assumed to be changed into the structure with boundaries in (6b) by insertion
of
these
boundaries at the edge of every lexical and syntactic ca-
tegory (graphemes in fact represent feature bundles
for
brevity).
Subsequent
readjustments altered (6b) into (6c).
(6)a
[ S [ N P [ N John]][ v p [ v [ v meet]ed][ N p [ d e t the][ N boy]]]]
b
[S*[NP#tN# John# I#11 VP#[ V#[ V r a e e t # ] e d # I [ N P # [ d e t t h e Π N # b ° y #
c
t S# [ NP# [ Ν J o h n ] # Π VP# [ V [ V m e e t ] + e d ] [ NP # U e t t h e Η N#b°y I ] # ] # J
Following the lines set out by SPE, representations
such
subsequent
research
W * W
showed
for several types of phonological phenomena. Nowadays phonological tion
is
assumed
that
linear
as those in (6) made it impossible to account adequately representa-
to be three-dimensional in order to account for phonological
phenomena. It is not my purpose to go into details of arguments for the
postu-
lation of all aspects of a three-dimensional phonological representation over a linear one. I will simply indicate here what sort of phenomena made sary
to
broaden
our
view
on
it
neces-
phonological representation and point towards
phenomena which can be dealt with more adequately in a three-dimensional framework (see for a handy summary the first chapter of Van der Hulst (1984)). After that, I will deal with those aspects of three dimensional phonology of particular relevance to our proposals concerning cliticization.
which
are
- 10 -
SPE considered segments to be the building blocks of tion.
phonological
representa-
However, tonal phenomena such as contour tones and tone stability, could
not be adequately accounted for Goldsmith
in
this
approach.
This
led
Leben
should not be represented within the segmental feature bundle, should
(1973),
(1976) and Williams (1976) to the conclusion that the tonal features
appear
on
a
separate
level
but
that
they
of their own (a so-called autosegmental
tier), associated with the segmental representation by association lines. sequent
research
into
the
Sub-
proper characteristics of autosegmental phonology
made it clear that harmony processes could be dealt with more adequately if one assumes
that the harmonizing feature(s) have a position on a separate autoseg-
mental level. Furthermore, ambisyllabicity, vowel length, and affricates
could
be accounted for in a framework which consists of a central core of only consonantal and vocalic elements, while the so-called
segmental
remaining
features
are
placed
on
a
tier. Thus, segments are now considered to be C or V ele-
ments associated with autosegments from several independent planes, rather than as bundles of features, as represented in (7).
C V c
(7)
"i'Yx Y Y Y ζ ζ ζ
Another aspect of SPE's segmental approach was that in stress
phenomena,
the
theory
order
to
account
had to allow for ad hoc elaborations. However,
Liberman (1975) and Liberman and Prince (1977) convincingly showed that did
for
stress
not fit in a segmental approach. Their primary observation with respect to
stress was that it is a relative property, which branching
trees
in
which
the
sister
they
represented
in
binary
nodes were labeled either s(trong) or
w(eak). This approach is called metrical phonology. Subsequent research
showed
that the syllable is represented most adequately within a metrical framework as well, with structure built upon the CV-elements of within
the
CV-tier.
Furthermore,
metrical phonology, certain nodes were assumed to be labelled with pro-
sodie categories, such as syllable and phonological word, which delimit domains for
the
application of phonological rules, thus doing away with boundary sym-
bols. So above CV-elements one may for example prosodie structure.
find
the
following
metrical,
-Ii-
M
is)
Λ ΛΑ
w
s
w
w
s
w
w
s
w
C
V
C
C
V
C
C
V
C
In much work on metrical phonology, metrical trees have a tion,
the
so-called
for rhythmic phenomena and is supposed to be rather Hayes
(1984a)).
Recently,
Prince
(1983)
and
phonetic
Selkirk
grid-only framework, that is, a framework virtually without structure.
However,
derived
because
of
in
(1984)
myself
to
these
(cf.
argued for a prosodie
Hayes (1984a), I will not follow this latter so
I
res-
short remarks. In this study I will be primarily con-
cerned with metrical prosodie structure. The relation between the the
nature
metrical
proposal. Actually, grids are not really dealt with in this study, trict
representa-
metrical grid. This representation is assumed to account
CV-tier
and
segmental tier will play a role in some respects as well. Below, I will go
into some details of these topics.
Syllable structure will not be our primary concern here. However, it will
play
a role in some respects and therefore it is necessary to make the following remarks. The sw-relation of sister nodes in the syllable is not one of prominence but
one
of
sonority.
Furthermore, the syllable is divided in an onset and a
rime, the latter existing of a peak and a coda. Onset, peak, and coda each dominate elements of the CV-tier.
-
12
-
$
(9) /
rime
onset w C
peakg V
coda,w C
The syllable is built according to the so-called sonority hierarchy (cf. Kiparsky
(1979)) in such a way that vowels, the most sonorant segments, are members
of the peak and consonants (less sonorant) are placed around them. less
universal
A
more
or
principle is the Maximal Onset Principle (cf. Selkirk (1983)),
which requires that onsets are filled maximally, whenever possible,
i.e.
with
respect to the sonority hierarchy and language specific constraints on syllable structure. Another relevant part of syllable theory for operation
ble structure does not meet universal syllable
structure
anymore
(for
purposes
is
the
again
and
language-specific
requirements
of
example after deletion or insertion of seg-
ments, or after affixation) automatic 'optimal'
our
of resyllabification. It appears to be the case that whenever sylla-
resyllabification
applies
to
make
it
(cf. Lowenstamm (1979), Clements and Keyser (1983) and Harris
(1983)). The domain in which resyllabification takes place seems to be
univer-
sally determined within the word. Of course, there may be language-specific deviations.
As already mentioned, syllable structure is constructed on the elements of central
CV-tier.
Therefore, this is the place to go into the relation between
syllable, CV-tier and segmental tier. In fact, I assume the onset and the to
the coda
dominate C's of the CV-tier and the peak to dominate V's. Most of the time,
I omit indications of onset, peak and coda in the representations that
follow.
Furthermore, elements of the segmental tier must be associated with elements of the CV-tier. This association is constrained crossing
association
by
a
condition
which
excludes
lines, and by a condition which in general prevents that
consonantal elements on the segmental tier
are
associated
with
V's
on
the
CV-tier, and vice versa. However, this condition leaves open the possibility of having an element on one tier associated with other,
thus
making
two
or
more
elements
on
the
it possible to account for long vowels, consisting of two
vocalic elements on the segmental tier associated with one on the CV-tier,
and
for ambisyllabic consonants, i.e. two C's of the CV-tier associated to one consonantal element on the segmental tier. All this is represented in (10).
- 13 -
/ Χ C V C
A
C V c
m b a b
I Ν b a b
(10)
*$
CM^C
/ χ C V C C V c
Λ
/ X C V c
ΙΛΙ ba ab
I I V b a b
II ab
I II b a b
$
I now turn to the relevance of metrical stress-patterns.
I
assume,
$
structure
and
the
the
repesentation
of
after Liberman (1975), Liberraan and Prince (1977)
and others, that the stress-pattern of a word must trees
for be
derived
from
metrical
way in which the nodes of these trees are labelled: a syllable
only dominated by s's has the main stress; the degree of stress on other syllables
is
inversely
proportional to the depth of embedding of these syllables.
Furthermore, a [stress] feature to achieve a proper interpretation of trees
metrical
is not necessary (cf. Selkirk (1980a) and Hayes (1981)). Thus, the words
órigin, difficùlt, and hâmamelidânthenum are represented as in (11).
Λ Λ A
(11)
M
F
F„
F.
s w difficult
s w w origin
Fι w
F¿ w
Fs
Λ
s w s w s w w hamamelidanthemum
Furthermore, it is observed in particular by Hayes (1981) that
many
languages
have words with a regular foot pattern, but that some of them have an irregular pattern at the edge of some words, while the rest of the word is regular. It is in these latter cases that the notion 'extrametricality' is useful. At the edge of words, elements, i.e. segments, syllables, and morphemes, can trametrical
by
lexical
be
made
ex-
marking or by rule. The extrametrical elements do not
count as relevant for the foot construction rules of the language, and they are adjoined
later
in
the derivation as a weak node. Thus for example, in Latin,
feet are binary with the restriction that a heavy syllable may not be w. Furthermore,
final
syllables
of words are assumed to be extrametrical and thus do
not count for foot-construction in the word in question. After the
foot
construction
application
rules, the extrametrical syllable is adjoined by Stray
Adjunction as a weak node to the foot, in this case to its left. Stray tion
is
a
convention
of
Adjunc-
which applies whenever it can be applied. It is an in-
•In-
stance of a general condition requiring that all elements
should
be
part
of
prosodie structure: i.e. that there are no floating elements. The derivation in (12) shows this. Extrametrical elements are placed within parentheses.
(12)
refec(tus) part.perf.
refee(cit) refi(cit) (to remake) ind.perf. ind.pres. 3 pers.sing. 3 pers.sing.
s w refectus
s w refeecit
Foot Construction
Stray Adjunction
Word tree
M I F
s w w reficit
Adjunction of floating elements will be one of the
leading
ideas
behind
the
prosodie theory of cliticization to be proposed in the next chapter.
At this point we arrive at the third area in which prosodie structure has ven
to
be
relevant:
pro-
domains of phonological rules. Prosodie categories also
happen to function as the domains of phonological rules. This was already known for
the
categories
syllable and phonological word, but is also the case with
respect to other prosodie categories. With respect to domains and prosodie tegories
above
the
ca-
word level, research is still in an early stage. However,
these prosodie categories will be very important in the following chapters,
so
it is worth going into aspects of prosodie structure above the word level here. The most careful and promising study in this area is Nespor and
Vogel
(1982).
They base their assumptions about prosodie categories largely on the domains in which several external sandhi rules apply. Furthermore, they assume these sodie
pro-
categories to be derived by mapping rules (cf. (5)). In particular their
-
15 -
investigation into the first prosodie category above the word level, called
0,
is very interesting, because they claim to have developed a universal mechanism for 0-construction. Their procedure for 0-construction is the following.
(13)a
b
Join into a 0 any lexical head (X) with all items on its non-recursive side within the maximal projection and with any other non-lexical items on'the same side (e.g. prepositions, complementizers, conjunctions, copulas, ...) 0 branches in the same direction as the syntactic trees
As one can see, crucial use is made of syntactic information. is
clear
that
the
lexical
item
non-lexical items are weak. An example of 0 as the rule
Furthermore,
it
in 0 is the strongest element and that the domain
of
a
phonological
can be found in Tuscan Italian. There, the well-known process of Raddopi-
amento Sintattico (RS) is restricted to apply in 0. RS
lengthens
the
initial
consonant of a word that is preceded by a word ending in a stressed vowel. This will be clear when one considers (14) in which tre and cani are in one and
the
same 0, while blu and misteriosi are not. So only the k of cani lengthens.
(14)
0
0
0
/ \ tre cani
I blu
misteriosi
(three mysterious blue dogs)
1 k:
Nespor
and
Vogel
0-construction.
(1982) The
add
first
two
is
proviso's
that
there
to are
their
0-restructuring rules, enlarging the domain 0. Secondly, appear
in
a
syntactically
marked
position,
instruction
language-specific
count
lexical as
for
optional
items
non-lexical
which for
0-construction. The next prosodie category is I, the Intonational Phrase. Nespor also
propose
an
instruction
admit that it is a first guess.
for
and
Vogel
I-construction, although they more or less
-
(15)a
16
-
Any displaced syntactic constituents, parentheticals and nonrestrictive relative clauses obligatorily form at least one I Starting with the first 0 of a sentence, join as many 0's as possible into an I until either 1) the end of the maximal projection of an Ν is reached, or 2) another S 1 begins. Once such an I is formed, proceed in the same way until the end of the main sentence is reached. Join any remaining 0's at the end of a sentence into an I. I is right branching
b
c
By further rules two or more very short I's, constructed by (15b), can be
made
longer, and conversely very long I's can be broken up into smaller ones. Finally, all I's in a root sentence are brought together in a right-branching structure dominated by U (Utterance), the highest prosodie category. Of course, both I and U function as domains for phonological rules. Thus Nespor and serve
that
Flapping
in
American
English
Vogel
ob-
is restricted to apply within the
domain I. This is shown in (16). In (16a) met and Anne are in one I, thus Flapping
is
applied, whereas in (16b) rat and eat are not in one I, thus Flapping
cannot be applied in this case.
(16)a
John met Anne
i
D
0 I
0 ι
0
. I
/ \
Roger, alias the rat, eats only
cheese
.1
*D
These remarks about prosodie structure above the word level end this section on background
assumptions.
The
notions
dealt with here, will play an important
role in the sccount that is going to be given of clitic phonology· In the
rest
of this study, I will concentrate on an account of phonological aspects of eli-
- 17 -
ticization. It is my purpose to establish a cliticization
within
post-lexical
prosodie
theory
of
the framework of non-linear phonology in Chapter 2. This
theory will be tested by closely investigating the phonology of Dutch
clitici-
zation in Chapter 3. In Chapter 4., the testing will be continued more globally for the clitic-phonology of other languages. Finally, in summarize our findings.
Chapter
5.,
I
will
Chapter 2
Prolegomena to a Prosodie Theory of Cliticization 1. Types of clitics
In a generative treatment to the phonology of cliticization, it is hard to nore
Zwicky's
(1977)
oft-cited
overview
of
types
of
clitics
ig-
in natural
languages. He distinguishes three types: 1. 'simple clitics', which are phonologically reduced forms of corresponding strong forms, occupying the same positions as these corresponding strong forms; 2. 'special clitics', which are at least semantically related to a strong form, have a superficially similar phonological shape, but otherwise special syntax and special phonology; 3. 'bound words', which do not correspond to unbound words and show large syntactic freedom. Some examples may clarify these distinctions. As an example of Zwicky
cites
simple
clitics
weak object pronouns in English. According to him, these clitics
are derived from a corresponding unbound word by phonological
reduction
(see,
however, Chapter 4.3.2. for a non-reduction approach to English pronominal clitics) and they appear in the same syntactic positions. This is shown in (1).
(l)a b
He sees hér (unbound word) He sees r (clitic)
An example of the second, special type of clitics can be found that
in
French.
In
language, clitic-pronouns such as le (him) and me (me) are normally used,
whereas their unbound counterparts lui and moi are used if the pronoun must stressed.
be
Furthermore, the clitic pronouns occur in front of the verb, whereas
the full pronouns occur after the verb.
-
(2)a b
20
-
Je vois lúi (unbound word) Je le vois (clitic) (I see him)
One of Zwicky's examples of bound words is Latin - q u e (and) which cally
associated
is
semanti-
with a word, phrase, or clause, though it is attached to the
first word in such a unit. Two examples are given in (3).
(3)a
Duasque ibi legiones conscribit two and there legions (he) enrolls (and (he) enrolls two legions there) arma virumque (arms and the man (acc.))
b
A similar, though more refined, division can be striking
feature
found
in
Klavans
(1982).
of this type of work, one which we had also occasion to com-
ment on in Chapter 1.1., is that in its argumentation it does not in the place
distinguish
A
between
phonological,
first
morphological and syntactic charac-
teristics of clitics. Secondly, this work is relatively pre-theoretical in that the
inventories
themselves appear to be its main aim rather than the possibly
stimulating context of a theory which predicts certain inventories rather others.
than
Precisely the latter track will be taken here. In a modular generative
approach to the phenomenon of cliticization, we expect syntactic properties
to
differ from phonological ones, lexical ones from syntactic ones, and so on, although all may interact in ways that themselves may be tion.
subject
to
investiga-
To be slightly more precise, the various aspects of cliticization may be
approached as follows. Zwicky's division into three types of clitics makes clear that there are two ways
for
clitics
to
arrive at their clitic-status. The first is to become a
clitic by phonological reduction from unbound words as is the case with clitics.
In
the
simple
second option, clitics are not to be derived by phonological
reduction, but rather have a status of their own: they are stored
as
such
in
the lexicon. Thus, special clitics are listed in the lexicon, probably as allomorphs of their strong counterpart, and bound words are listed in without
any
allomorphy-relation.
the
lexicon
Clitics depend on a host, to which they are
adjoined. For clitics which are derived by phonological reduction, this adjunction
can
only take place in the phonological component, because they only ob-
tain their clitic-status there. For lexically
stored
clitics,
there
are
at
-
least
three
components
of
grammar
21
-
in which adjunction can potentially take
place. Since these clitics are stored in the lexicon, they can adjoined
in
potentially
be
the lexicon itself, in the syntactic component, and in the phono-
logical component. However, if the clitic is adjoined to a host in the lexicon, there
is
little
reason to call this adjunction cliticization. It is more ap-
propriate to call it affixation. Thus, I assume here a definition of cliticization
in which this notion is limited to non-lexical adjunction. So the differ-
ence between affixation and cliticization is expressed by their position of application
in the grammar. Notice that a consequence of this definition of cli-
ticization is that lexical phonological rules cannot be applied to sequences of word plus clitic, since these sequences are not encountered as such in the lexicon. As I already remarked in (1982,
Chapter
1.1.,
following
in
particular
1985), cliticization may have a syntactic and a phonological side. This
means that cliticization may be syntactic adjunction, phonological or
both.
adjunction,
The most spectacular examples are those cases in which the clitic is
syntactically adjoined to one side and phonologically to the to
Klavans
Klavans
(1985),
such
a
other.
According
case is found in Kwakwala. In that language, the
function-marking clitics da (deictic), xa (object) and sa (oblique) are syntactically
adjoined
to the Ν to their right. Phonologically, however, these cli-
tics are adjoined to the word to their left. This is exemplified in (4),
where
= means phonological adjunction.
(4)
Ν Cl I I ganamen = xa throw deic child obj house obi rock (the child hit the house with a rock by throwing)
Only clitics belonging to Zwicky's special clitics and bound words have two potential adjunction sides.
From this, the following areas of investigation cliticization,
aspects
arise:
aspects
of
syntactic
of phonological cliticization of lexically stored eli-
-
22
-
tics, aspects of cliticization by phonological reduction, and the lexical relationship
between
special clitic and corresponding bound word. Another area of
investigation is the generation of the lexically stored structure.
In
clitics
base generation, transformational operation, spell-out of other
in
syntactic
the literature, several means to generate clitics can be found: case,
theta
and/or
features, and allomorphy. Furthermore, one could investigate the differ-
ences in generation between those clitics which are only
syntactic
in
nature
and those which are only phonological in nature. Of these possible topics of investigation, I have deals
with
vestigated. I will try to find links towards various
selected
the
aspects
of
cliticization
I
which
a
better
understanding
will
of
cliticiza-
not be concerned about aspects of phonological cliticization by
reduction. The phonological reduction of words to clitics appears to be enced
by
performance
speech
influ-
factors, such as style and speech tempo. This makes the
phonological reduction rules likely candidates for casual
the
with respect to non-linear phonology. This
does not mean that I will deal with all aspects of the phonology of tion.
one
the phonology of cliticization, a subject which is rather underin-
a
position
in
the
late,
module of the post-lexical phonology. However, since the proper
characteristics of this module are rather obscure, it is difficult to
go
into
details about clitic reduction. Furthermore, the obscurity of the casual speech component is the reason that we are not in the position to make insightful servations
about
phonological
reduction
type of cliticization for what it is. It will only be considered when against
such
a
ob-
of clitics. Therefore, I leave this I
argue
reduction analysis and in favour of a lexical storage of cli-
tics. Fortunately, the situation with respect to lexically stored clitics is promising.
As
these
they may play a role as clitic in the post-lexical phonological component the
more
clitics originate in the syntactic component of grammar, from
start. The early sub-components of the post-lexical phonology, in contrast
to the casual speech part, are relatively well-understood. Thus we are in a position to enlarge our understanding of phonological aspects of cliticization on the basis of our existing knowledge of the early parts of the post-lexical phonology. In the next section, I will attempt to deepen our preliminary view upon the object of investigation of this study.
- 23 2. Phonological aspects of cliticization
In the preceding section, I have surveyed types of clitics and limited the discussion to phonological cliticization of lexically stored clitics. In this section I will survey phonological phenomena triggered by these clitics.
One of the initially most striking phonological aspects the
fact
that
of
cliticization^
is
clitics generally appear to lack accent. It is impossible, for
example, for Dutch clitics to receive contrastive stress, as is
shown
by
the
examples in (5).
(5)a b
*Ws eten vlees, maar za eten kip (we (cl) eat meat, but they (cl) eat chicken) Wij eten vlees, maar zij eten kip (we eat meat, but they eat chicken)
Empirically, though, we cannot say that clitics languages
appear
are
always
unstressed.
Some
to have stressed clitics. Thus in Chapter 4.4.2. we will en-
counter Greek cases in which a clitic is stressed,
one
example
of
which
is
property
of
given here.
(6)
fére (bring)
fère mú to (bring to me it)
In such cases, however, stress on the clitic appears not to be the
clitic
a
itself, but rather of clitic plus host. Thus, we can say here that
clitics are inherently unstressed, but may receive
some
degree
of
so-called
derived stress in some languages.
A second phonological characteristic of clitics is their slightly attitude
toward
otherwise regular word-internal phonological processes, which
they undergo in some cases, whereas they fail to do so in 4.4.1.,
we
will
schizofrenie
deal
with of
an
example
from
Liquid
Gliding
(1,
others.
Cibaeno r
->
i)
Spanish. at
the
In
Chapter
There,
a
word-internal
process
righthand
syllable-edge
is applied if a liquid-final word precedes a vowel-initial word,
- 24 as exemplified in (7a). However, this rule is bled
if
a
liquid-final
clitic
stands in front of a vowel-initial word, as is shown in (7b).
(7)a
*el avisa (he advises) ei avisa
b
el aviso (the advice) *ei aviso
It seems that the clitic in (7b) has 'affix'-status and
is
incorporated
into
the following word. This incorporation triggers resyllabification which results in the bleeding of Liquid Gliding. In (7a), however, el
is
an
unbound
word,
thus its final 1 undergoes Liquid Gliding. An example in which clitics do found
in
Dakota,
an
not
Amerindian
influence
word-internal
processes
is
language we will consider in Chapter 4.3.1.
Here, consonant final stems at the end of a word optionally followed by a
cli-
tic, are augmented with the vowel a. Thus in (8) not only the stem i^ap is augmented with a, but also the stem 3uk which is followed by the clitic wa.
(8)
3uk wa - > Suka (dog a (cl))
wa
£hap -> £hapa (beaver)
As will be noted, clitics do not influence this word-internal process. These two types of behaviour processes
may
with
respect
to
word-internal
co-occur in one single language, and in fact in one single cli-
tic. Thus, in Dutch, vowel-initial clitics may, but do not have general
phonological
process
of
Syllable
to
bleed
the
Final Devoicing, if preceded by an underlyingly
voiced word- and syllable-final obstruent: both pronunciations of
heb
in
(9)
are possible. These cases will be dealt with in Chapter 3.4.
(9)a b
He[b]ak dat gedaan (did I do that) He[p]ak dat gedaan
Finally, the vast majority of clitics one finds discussed in the literature monosyllabic
Wiese (to app.)). This clearly strikes one as the unmarked case. On hand,
is
as is also illustrated by the clitics considered so far (cf. also
observations
on
solely
the
other
consonantal, and polysyllabic clitics occur as
- 25 -
well and I will make suggestions about the status of these marked cases
as
we
proceed. Together with the host-dependency, considered in Chapter 1.1., it seems that at least
the following phonological characteristics are typical of the phenomenon
of cliticization as described in this study: (a) clitics 'depend on hosts'; (b) clitics are inherently unstressed; (c) clitics behave schizofrenically with respect to their influence on word-internal phonological processes; (d) clitics are normally monosyllabic. These observations will be the subject of this study, or, in other
words,
our
goal will be to derive a theory, based on the modular approach to grammar, that accounts for them in a natural way and, to the plains
largest
possible
extent,
ex-
them in the usual generative sense of this word. In the next section, I
will establish the initial foundations of such a theory.
3. A prosodie theory of cliticization
Within the framework of this study, a prosodie have
to
account
for
theory
of
cliticization
will
the above-mentioned characteristics of lexically stored
clitics in a natural and straightforward fashion. In establishing this theory I will
follow
the
line
parametric approach.
of current linguistic research which takes a so-called
The
leading
idea
is
that
Universal
Grammar
is
the
characterization of the innate principles which make it possible for a child to learn a language. Universal Grammar consists for example construction
of
of
devices
of
Universal
Grammar
admit
a
certain
language-learning child, the so-called parameters. data,
the
the
the rule system, the system of representations, the system of
conditions on rules and representations, and the components of devices
for
number Based
of on
grammar.
These
choices for the the
experienced
child selects devices of Universal Grammar and fixes the parameters
belonging to these devices. Thus, the child is constructing a core grammar. this
core
grammar
To
a periphery of marked elements and constructions are added
-
26
-
(cf. Chomsky (1982, p. 1-16))· Setting a certain parameter may
have
consider-
able consequences, whereas the differences between the choices seems to be very limited. Consider as an example the following. One of the devices of Grammar
seems
to
be
that
Universal
phonological structure is represented in a binary
branching metrical structure, the direction of branching being a parameter. The language-learning
child
has
to
fix
this
parameter,
either
left-
right-branching, on the basis of the experienced data. Let us assume for ple
or
exam-
that the branching parameter has to be set for the level of the phonologi-
cal word: either structure (10a) or structure (10b) must be chosen.
A consequence of this choice between left- or right-branching will
be
encountered
is
that
stress
word-initially or word-finally respectively. I will show
that this approach is rather fruitful in accounting for phonological aspects of cliticization. Given these preliminaries, let us see what the contours of
a
prosodie
clitic
theory will look like, starting with observations on the inherent unstressability of clitics. In the theory of metrical prosodie phonology, stress is tially
considered
to
vowel or the peak of the syllable, is more prominent (stressed) This
binary
than
stress
seems
to
another.
is expressed by the labels 's1 versus 'w' in a binary
opposition
branching metrical structure, as exemplified in Chapter 1.2.2. This of
essen-
a relative notion: one 'stress-bearer 1 , usually the
be
make
relativity
it impossible to analyze monosyllabic words as in-
herently stressed, that is, stressed in the lexicon. In this case, there are no s-
and
w-labels,
so
one
cannot speak about prominence here. However, it is
well-known that many monosyllabic words are in fact inherently Selkirk
(1980a)
stressed.
Both
and Hayes (1981) account for the stress of monosyllabic words
by stipulating that 'being a (Foot) always implies some degree
of
prominence'
(Selkirk (1980a, p. 565)). This is relevant to the phonology of cliticization in the following way. pointed
out
above,
clitics
are
As
usually monosyllabic and are inherently un-
- 27 -
stressed. If we ask our theory how to treat monosyllables under the
demand
of
unstressability, a type of element surfaces which is left undominated lexically by the prosodie categories foot or word. Although initially this one
may
be
only
alternative out of several, it seems to me highly attractive as a starting
point in the sense that in this way we account for the fact inherently
that
clitics
are
unstressed. If a clitic receives some degree of stress, this stress
must be derived by post-lexical mechanisms. A further consequence appears to be the following. It is likely that the fact that two or more syllables belong together lexically, is also expressed phonologically by putting
these
syllables
together in a foot and/or phonological word, resulting in sw-relations and thus in stressed syllables. These polysyllabic elements in fact lack one of the most stable clitic-characteristics, viz. inherent unstressability, and are therefore highly reluctant to show phonological clitic b e h a v i o u r R e c a p i t u l a t i n g , logical
phono-
clitics are provided in the lexicon only with prosodie information re-
garding syllable structure. They are marked as not undergoing the lexical footand
phonological
word-construction
rules.
This implies that clitics are in-
herently unstressed.
These clitics meet with other elements in syntactic structure. As above,
out
the generation of clitics in syntactic structure may take place by sev-
eral means. Crucial for us here is that the clitic's mains
pointed
lexical
information
re-
unchanged under its insertion in syntactic structure. This holds for the
insertion of other elements in syntactic structure as phonological
clitics
are
still
provided
well.
being a syllable only. As soon as these clitics enter the logical
This
means
that
with their prosodie information of post-lexical
phono-
component, the clitic-syllables are floating. Since floating syllables
have to be incorporated
into
prosodie
structure
both
in
the
lexicon
and
post-lexically, these clitic-syllables have to be incorporated too. The characteristic that clitics may behave schizofrenically with respect to processes
(i.e.
phonological
they behave either more like an affix or more like an unbound
word) suggests that this may happen in at least two ways: (a)
clitics
may
be
incorporated into already existing phonological words to their left or to their right, or (b) clitics may be part of higher prosodie categories. Below, I deal with these two possibilities in this order.
will
-
28
-
3.1. M-incorporation
As remarked above, syntactic surface structure has already been specified the
prosodie
categories
of
and under the word level. Before one proceeds to
construct prosodie structure above the word level, one of away
with
floating
with
clitic-syllables
is
the
options
to
to incorporate them as members of a
neighbouring phonological word by Stray Syllable Adjunction. In that case, clitic
will
do the
automatically occupy a weak position. Together with the plausible
notion 'direction of cliticization', this results in the schematic
representa-
tions in (11).
One further aspect of adjunction of clitics to phonological
words
has
to
be
considered: is there any information necessary to trigger adjunction and if so, what is the nature of this information? If the information is (in
part)
based
on syntax, the adjunction of clitic-syllables to phonological words has to take place in the phonosyntactic module of the post-lexical phonology since
syntac-
tic information is only allowed there. However, if the information is phonological in character or if there is no need for the
adjunction
is
part
of
further
triggering
information,
the mapping of syntactic structure onto prosodie
structure. The fate of the representations in (11) as they are sent through the logical ing of
phono-
component is schematically as follows. First, a subsequent restructurprosodie
structure
may
follow,
depending
upon
the
universal
and
language-specific requirements with respect to this structure. For syllabification, there seems to be a universal resyllabification (cf. Harris (1983),
Cle-
ments and Keyser (1983)), if the syllables in a word do not fit universal principles
(such
as
language-specific
Maximal
Onset
requirements
Principle
and
Sonority
Hierarchy)
and
for syllabification, so as to conform to these
principles and requirements. In Chapter 3. and 4., I will give many examples of resyllabification
triggered
by
cliticization.
Furthermore,
we
claim
that
clitic-syllables must be incorporated into feet if a language has this prosodie
- 29 category
and
the
feet construction rules require such an incorporation. This
will eventually result in feet consisting only of clitic-syllables and thus stressed
clitic-syllables,
as
I
will
in
show in Chapter 4.A.2. and 4.4.3. for
Greek and Palestinian Arabic respectively. Second, post-lexical phonological processes which have the phonological word as their domain, with or without clitics, will be applied on the output of this procedure. Thus, if adjunction of clitic-syllables is a tion,
phonosyntactic,
phonosyntactic
opera-
phonological and casual speech processes are applied to
phonological words with clitics. However, if clitic-syllables are
adjoined
to
phonological words as part of the mapping from syntactic structure onto prosodie structure, only phonological and casual speech processes are applied to phonological words, including clitics. In this case, clitic-syllable and phonological word to which it will be adjoined, are two separate entities for phonosyntactic processes. So if a clitic-syllable is adjoined to a neighbouring phonological word, one has to know the following: - direction of cliticization (to the right or to the left); - the information required for adjunction to apply (the place of application of adjunction is dependent upon this information). Furthermore, a readjustment of prosodie structure ing)
accompanies
the
(resyllabification,
refoot-
adjunction of clitic-syllables. What we see now is that
adjunction of clitic-syllables to phonological words has many things in with
regular
common
(weak) affixation: however, affixation is lexical and cliticiza-
tion to phonological words is not.
3.2. 0-incorporation
If clitics are not to be adjoined to a neighbouring phonological word (the junction
being
optional),
the clitic-syllable will remain floating. So it is
necessary to look for structure of a higher level. Under this option, clitic-syllables
will
become
part
of
rules, which are part of the mapping from structure.
In
this
prosodie
thus
behave
floating
structure by 0-construction
syntactic
structure
onto
prosodie
case, clitics are considered to be non-lexical items. Be-
cause of this, clitics will automatically occupy weak clitics
ad-
positions
in
0.
Since
like other non-lexical items the directional position of
- 30 their host need not be specified: it follows directly from the
formulation
of
the 0-construction rules as such (cf. Chapter 1.2.2.)· This gives the following options.
Notice that in these structures the clitic word-internal
phonological
cannot
exert
any
influence
do not constitute a phonological word. At the same time, clitics are able
within
these
upon
processes, since clitics and neighbouring elements recogniz-
structures as syllables directly dominated by 0 and can be
referred to as such. A number of
Dutch
phonological
processes
discussed
in
Chapter 3. will refer to this property. Of course, the clitic-syllable may also undergo both phonological processes which are limited to o i.e. I, and U, and processes of casual speech .
the
higher
If it is not possible to incorporate floating clitic-syllables
domains,
into
higher
prosodie structure, the resulting representation with floating syllables is excluded because floating elements which cannot
be
incorporated
into
prosodie
structure, are not admitted.
4. Summary
The central aim of this study is to account for phonological characteristics of cliticization. In this chapter, we have pursued this aim along general lines. After having limited our discussion of cliticization to
clitics
which
are
not derived by phonological reduction, i.e. which are stored in the lexicon and which meet with other morphemes in the syntactic component of grammar, I considered
general phonological characteristics of cliticization, of which the fol-
lowing are encountered:
•al-
ia) clitics 'depend on hosts'; (b) clitics are inherently unstressed; (c) clitics behave schizofrenically
with respect to their
influence on word-internal phonological processes; (d) clitics are normally monosyllabic. In Section 3., I have outlined a prosodie theory of cliticization to account for
these
characteristics.
This
theory consists of the following framework.
Clitics are mono-syllables without further prosodie categories in
the
lexicon.
being
provided
Thus, they are inherently unstressed. Since clitics mix with
other morphemes in the syntactic component of grammar, this prohibits them from having any influence upon the application of lexical phonological rules. In the post-lexical phonological component, the floating clitic-syllables have
to
be
incorporated into higher prosodie structure. This is done by fixing the universal clitic parameters: either the clitic is adjoined to a logical
word,
neighbouring
phono-
or the clitic, as a non-lexical category, is directly dominated
by 0. If the clitic-syllable becomes part of a phonological word, the direction of
cliticization
has
to
be
fixed.
The
proper
position
in
grammar
clitic-adjunction depends on the triggering environment: if syntactic
of
informa-
tion is necessary, adjunction is phonosyntactic; if no syntactic information is necessary, adjunction is part of the mapping from syntactic structure onto prosodie
structure. The prosodie structure after adjunction is subject to a read-
justment of prosodie structure, such as resyllabification. A consequence of adjunction of clitic-syllables to phonological words is that the sequence of host and clitic can be subject to word-internal post-lexical processes. If the
cli-
tic is directly dominated by 0, the direction of cliticization follows from the formulation of the 0-construction rules in word-internal
processes
the
language.
they do not constitute a phonological word. The situation depicted in (13).
In
this
case,
no
can be applied to sequences of clitic and host, since described
above
is
- 32 -
ARE CLITICS ADJOINED TO A PHONOLOGICAL WORD?
(13)
Optional
Yes -direction of cliticization -information necessary for adjunction to apply (proper place of application in grammar follows) -restructuring (resyllabification, refooting) -word-internal processes apply to clitic plus host
-dominated by 0 -position in 0 fixed by 0-construction rules -no word-internal processes apply to clitic plus host
-either 'yes' -or 'no'
Notice that under the theory presented here, it is in fact unnecessary sume
the
as-
notions 'phonological clitic' and 'phonological cliticization ' to be
primitives. They can be derived from more primitive notions who
to
(cf.
Neyt
(1985)
makes a similar observation). In our prosodie theory, phonological clitics
are to be recognized as monosyllabic morphemes which are marked as not dergo
the
to
un-
lexical foot and phonological word construction rules and which are
generated in syntactic structure. Cliticization is the adjunction clitic-syllables
of
floating
to higher prosodie structure either by Stray Syllable Adjunc-
tion to a neighbouring phonological word, or by
0-construction
rules
of
the
language. However, I will use the notions 'clitic' and 'cliticization' for sake of clarity. In the next chapters, I will investigate phonological aspects of
cliticiza-
tion in a variety of languages against the background of the prosodie theory of cliticization proposed here. In particular, I will investigate depth
in
Chapter 4.
Chapter
3.,
while
Dutch
to
some
other languages are considered more globally in
Notes to Chapter 2
1. Next to phonological characteristics of cliticization, there are syntactic characteristics of this phenomenon as well. In the literature the following relevant information can be found. Clitics have a preference to be localized next to the word they are dependent upon syntactically or at the periphery of a syntactic constituent. In French for example, object clitic pronouns are located in front of the verb they are dependent upon, and nothing may intervene between these two.
(i)a Le garçon le voit peut-etre (the boy him sees maybe) b *Le garçon le peut-etre voit
Articles in Dutch, however, are left-peripheral in NP.
(ii)a De jongen (the boy) b een erg händige en slimme jongen (a very handsome and smart boy)
Secondly, general syntactic transformations such as topicalization do not have any effects on clitics. So it is not possible to topicalize clitic pronouns in Dutch, while strong pronouns can be topicalized.
(iii)a De jongens zagen em (the boys saw him) b *am zagen de jongens c De jongens zagen hem d Hem zagen de jongens
A third syntactic characteristic is that clitics often occupy a structural position different from real words and constituents with the same syntactic function as the clitic. This is especially clear in the Romance languages, for example in Spanish, where the object position for NP's is to the right of the verb, while object clitic pronouns occupy a position to the left of the verb.
(iv)a b
Vio Maria (I see Mary) La vio (I see her)
Furthermore, when there are two or more clitics grouped together, there is a strict ordering of clitics. The clitics ne and en in French for example must have the order just specified, when grouped together.
- 34 (v)a Elle m'en semble capable (she seems capable of it) b *Elle en me semble capable Finally, clitics cannot be part of a coordination. This is examples in (vi).
clear
from
the
(vi)a Vio Maria y Juan (I see Mary and John) b *La y lo vio
2. Many, and maybe all examples of polysyllabic clitics found in the literature are to be considered syntactic clitics only. Zwicky (1977), for example, mentions bisyllabic clitics in Bikol, a Philippine language, which do not behave phonologically like the monosyllabic clitics: the former bear stress, and do not trigger a rule lengthening certain preceding vowels, while monosyllabic clitics are not stressed and trigger the lengthening rule. Of course, the bisyllabic clitics in Bikol are syntactic clitics only, while the monosyllabic ones are both syntactic and phonological clitics. 3. As is the case with almost all phonologically weak elements, they are likely candidates to undergo rules of casual speech (cf. reduction of unstressed vowels to schwa and deletion of schwa for example). Thus, it comes as no surprise that there seems to be a tendency of clitics as the weakest element of 0 to undergo casual speech rules. It may eventually be the case that clitic syllables dominated by 0 are restructured in processes of casual speech in such a manner as to become incorporated into phonological words. However, I did not investigate the latter processes here, and for that reason I will leave them undiscussed.
Chapter
3
The Phonology of Dutch Cliticization
1. Introduction
Although very interesting in itself, the phonology of Dutch
cliticization
has
not received much attention. Recently, the attention has been growing somewhat. The following papers are relevant in this respect, although (Dutch)
cliticization
is
just
a
in
some
of
them
secondary issue: Neyt and Zonneveld (1980,
1982b), Zonneveld (1982, 1983), Neyt (1985),
Booij
(1985) Booij
and
Rubach
(1985), Gussenhoven (1986), and Berendsen (1983b, 1985b, to app.). The most important drawback of most of these papers is the lack about
of
comprehensive
present an account of several aspects of the phonology of Dutch In
theory
the phonological nature of cliticization itself. In this chapter, I will cliticization.
analyzing the phonology of Dutch cliticization, I will be guided on the one
hand by the prosodie clitic theory developed in Chapter 2. On the however,
Dutch
clitic
phonology
will
other
hand,
function as a testing ground for this
theory. In Section 4., I will analyze voicing phenomena with respect tion,
elaborating
proposals
by
others.
In
Section
5.,
to
cliticiza-
the phenomenon of
Schwa-reduction in clitics is dealt with. This process seems to be unrelated to voicing phenomena, but it is accounted for within the prosodie clitic theory of Chapter 2. as well. The topic of Section 6. is the relation between
N-deletion
and N-insertion, in which clitics and other non-lexical items play a role. Some issues necessary to understand Sections 4. - 6. in full detail are presented in Sections 2. and 3. In Section 2., an overview of Dutch clitics is presented and arguments are provided for their storage in the lexicon. the syntax of Dutch are dealt with in Section 3.
Relevant
aspects
of
- 36 -
2. Dutch Clitics
Dutch has a large amount of 'function' words: pronouns, articles, adverbs, non-main
verbs
and
belong to this class. Many of these function words have phono-
logical clitic characteristics: they only occur in unstressed form, they depend on hosts, they are monosyllabic, and, as we will see below, sometimes they have an unexpected influence on the application of phonological processes. For of
these
clitics,
their
only vowel is schwa. Furthermore, some of these clitics strong
have
a
corresponding
form. As is well-known among Dutch linguists, Dutch in a broad sense is
far from homogeneous as regards phonological clitic data. The form word
most
unstressability is particularly clear because their
may
a
function
have differs from dialect to dialect, and what counts as a clitic in
one dialect need not do so in another. However, here my aim will not be to give a complete overview of the phonology of cliticization of all variants of Dutch. Therefore, I will analyze my own dialect, standard Western Dutch, and extend it at
specific
stages
with data from other dialects, but only cursorily so. The
tables in (1) below give an overview of the function words currently Except
for
some
relevant.
exceptional cases, these tables give only the basic forms of
both strong and weak forms. Other variants will be discussed as we proceed.
(1) PERSONAL PRONOUNS subject strong weak ik ok (I) ja/ie (you) jij u u (you) die/tie/ie (he) hij za (she) zij 1 het at (it) wa (we) wil jullie ja (you) za (they) zij
object strong weak mij ma (me) jou ja (you) u u (you) hem am (him) haar dar/tar/ar (her) het 1 at (it) ons ons (us) jullie ja/ie (you) hun za (them)
- 37 POSSESSIVE PRONOUNS strong mijn jouw uw zijn haar ons/onze jullie hun
weak man/ma (my) ja/ie (your) uw (your) zan/za (his/its) dar/tar/ar (her) ons (our) ja (your) dar/tar (their)
REFLEXIVE PRONOUNS strong mazelf jazelf uzelf zichzelf onszelf jazelf zichzelf
ARTICLES strong
hetl een 1
ADVERBS strong eens daar er
weak ma (myself) ja (yourself) zieh (yourself) zieh (himself/herself) ons (ourselves) ja (yourselves) zieh (themselves)
weak da/ta (the) at (the) an (a)
weak ans/as (once) dar/tar/ar (there) dar/tar/ar (there)
NON MAIN VERBS strong is
weak as (is)
Note that in many cases the alternation between strong and weak form taken
as
be
the result of a purely phonological process reducing a full vowel to
the reduced vowel schwa. This may well be the will
might
correct
diachronic
account.
I
argue, however, that synchronically the clitic forms in Dutch are not the
result of phonological reduction, but must exist independently next to a possibly
existing strong form. This idea is one of the most important initial steps
toward the analysis of phonological cliticization to be developed for Dutch
in
this chapter. The evidence for this approach is presented below. First, not all weak forms have a corresponding strong
form
from
which
it
could be derived by reduction straightforwardly. The tables in (1) show that da simply lacks a corresponding strong form and that the
phonological
outfit
of
- 38 some strong forms is rather different from that of the clitics. Furthermore, in other cases one needs extra rules which are only necessary in a reduction lysis:
one
has
ana-
to change h into d, t or zero in the pairs haar/dar, tar, ar,
hij/die, tie, ie and het/at. In the pair er/dar one has to change zero into The
other
d.
clitics can in principle be derived from corresponding unbound mor-
phemes by reducing their vowels to schwa in most cases and by only
destressing
them in others. However, unbound morphemes which may undergo reduction (and deI
stressing) have to be marked as such, since not morphemes
can
all
monosyllabic
non-lexical
be reduced. Thus, in a phonological reduction analysis, one has
to allow for some lexically stored clitics and the unbound morphemes which undergo
can
reduction, have to be marked as such. However, if we store all clitics
in the lexicon, we do not need a marking of corresponding strong form, but simply
extend
the lexical storage of clitics which is already necessary on inde-
pendent grounds. Second, observe that the clitics za, ja and wa may have
a
deviant
meaning
from the corresponding strong forms zij/hun, jij and hij. For example the third person plural subject clitic za and the second person singular ja
may,
subject
clitic
next to the meaning of simply zij (they) and jij (you), respectively,
also have a more general interpretation, comparable to English people.
(2)a
c
(3)a
c
za zeggen zoveel (i they say a lot) (ii people say a lot) za doen maar (i they just go on) (ii people just go on)
b
d
ja wil snel te veel b (i you quickly want too much) (ii people quickly want too much) js ziet maar d (i you have to do your own thing) (i people have to do their own thing)
zij zeggen zoveel (i they say a lot) (ii*people say a lot) zij doen maar (i they just go on) (ii*people just go on) jij wil snel te (i you quickly (ii*one quickly jij ziet maar (i you have to (ii*people have own thing)
veel want too much) wants too much) do your own thing) to do their
Similarly, the clitic za can be used to denote both persons and things, whereas normally
the
corresponding
Van Haeringen (1951)).
strong forms zij and hun only denote persons (cf.
- 39 (4)a b
Kees zegt dat de fietsen (Kees is saying that the Kees zegt dat de jongens (Kees is saying that the
daar nog bicycles daar nog boys are
staan, raaar zs/*zij zijn weg are still there, but they have gone) staan, maar za/zij zijn weg still there, but they have gone)
The clitic va also has an interpretation which its
corresponding
strong
form
wij lacks. Wij corresponds to English we, including the speaker, whereas wa may also have a meaning comparable to English you (plur), excluding the speaker.
(5)a
b
Wat doen (i what (ii what Wat doen (i what (ii*what
wa hier eigenlijk are we doing here) are you (plur) doing here) wij hier eigenlijk are we doing here) are you (plur) doing here)
A reduction analysis thus needs a phonological rule that has to keep the
track
of
semantics of the function word that is to be reduced. This type of rule is
incompatible with the current model of grammar (see Chapter 1.2.1.). An sis,
however,
which
treats
analy-
corresponding weak and strong forms as separate,
lexically stored elements, does not encounter this problem. A third indication that thougths of a phonological reduction be
abandoned
hold for va, za and ja, but for other clitics as well.
b c
d e f g
must
in favour of a lexical storage is the existence of idiomatic ex-
pressions which allow only a weak, and not a strong form. This
(6)a
analysis
daar kun j9/*jij donder op zeggen (you can bet your bottom dollar) dat haal ja/*jij de koekoek (you got to be kidding) als js/*jij van de duvel spreekt, dan trap ja/*jij op zan/*zijn staart (speaking of the devil, you will step on his tail) daar gaat ie/*hij dan (there you go) laat ma/*mij niet lachen (don't make me laugh) za/*zij achter de elleboog hebben (to act selfishly) daar zit am/*hem de kneep (that is the crux of the matter)
does
not
only
- 40 -
h i j k 1 m η o ρ
sm/*hem ora hebben (to be legless) am/*hem knijpen (to be scared) am/*hem smeren (to make oneself scarce) 3m/*hem op zetten (to do the best one can) am/*hem van katoen geven (to go for it) zo zijn ws/*wij niet getrouwd (that is no way to handle this) 8r/dar/*haar/*daar/*er geweest zijn (to kick the bucket) op zan/*zijn elf en dertigst (very slowly) op z3n/*zijn jan boeren fluitjes (carelessly)
Under the normal assumption that idiomatic expressions are stored in the con,
the
relevant
pronouns
in
lexi-
(6) should contain a rather peculiar feature
[obligatory reduction] in the reduction analysis. However, in an analysis which treats weak and strong forms independently, the account of these expressions is straightforward: the clitic occurs in the idiom from the outset,
that
is,
in
the lexicon. A fourth argument against a reduction analysis for Dutch clitics is provided by
the
behaviour of reflexive pronouns. Inspection of the tables in (1) shows
that a selection of Dutch weak object pronouns plus zieh function as flexives.
They
occur
weak
re-
obligatorily in constructions with inherently reflexive
verbs, as is exemplified in (7).
(7)a b c d
ik (I ik (I ik (I ik (I
vergis m8/?*mij am mistaken) schaam ma/?*mij am ashamed) werk ma/?*mij suf work very hard) herinner ma/?*mij dat remember that)
jij vergisi js/*jou (you are mistaken) jij schaamt jo/*jou (you are ashamed) jij werkt js/*jou suf (you work very hard) jij herinnert ja/*jou dat (you remember that)
Again, the reduction analysis would need some notion of 'obligatory
reduction'
which simply describes the observation. This does not appear to be very attractive, and we therefore conclude that a lexical storage of both weak and
strong
form provides a more straightforward means to account for these constructions.
- 41 -
To take stock, I have argued so far that there are four arguments in of
a
favour
lexical storage of clitics and against a phonological derivation of cli-
tics by reduction of unbound morphemes. 1. Cases in which the clitic differs considerably in phonological shape from a corresponding strong form (da, zieh, ja ( (NP/PP)
NP
V
As is well-known, Dutch moves the finite verb in root sentences Evers
to
INFL
(cf.
(1982)). In subordinate clauses INFL may be filled by a conjunction word
like dat (that) or of (whether) or left empty, i.e. is not filled with material.
COMP
lexical
is filled by NP's (subject formation and topicalization) or by
WH-elements (wh-questions in root and subordinate clauses and relative pronouns in subordinate clauses). With respect to NP and PP the following basic structure for these
syntactic
categories is assumed. NP consists of a noun which may be preceded by a specifier and/or an adjectival complement, and may be followed by a or
a
PP.
normally followed by a NP, together however,
relative
clause
The base rule in (9a) accounts for this situation. A preposition is constituting
a
PP,
as
in
(9b).
Note,
that the so-called R-pronouns such as daar and er (it) always precede
a preposition and that there are also postpositions (cf. Van Riemsdijk (1978)).
(9)a b
NP - > Spec AP PP - > Ρ NP
Ν
(S'/PP)
In structure (11) of the sentence in (10)
(10)
de jongen geeft het meisje dat op de straat staat een boek (the boy gives a book to the girl who is on the street)
many of the above mentioned basic structures and transformations fied.
are
exempli-
- 43 -
(H) COMP
INFL
NP
V
NP
A
Spec Ν Spec
Ν COMP INFL I NP
S
NP
VP PP NP / S Ρ Spec Ν
c de g o n g e n geeft j het meisje dat
c
e | op de straat Staat een boek
The question is now how clitics fit in. With respect to object clitic
pronouns
of V, I argued in Berendsen (1983a) that these clitics are syntactically dependent upon V and must be situated in the left periphery of VP making use of projection
the
of V, as is shown for example in (12a). This object clitic position
is argued for on the basis of syntactic considerations. In Everaert (1986), reflexive clitic pronouns are argued to occupy the same position as object clitic pronouns do. The position of the articles is similar to clitics in VP, left
periphery,
but
in
the
now in NP, as is illustrated in (12b). This position ap-
pears to be undisputed in the literature.
(12)a
NP / \ Spec Ν
I
I
Cl
I
dat de jongen am (that the boy gives him books)
geeft
- 44 -
da aardige (the nice boy)
jongen
Most object clitic pronouns within PP's are situated in phery
of
that
constituent.
The
the
right-hand
peri-
exception is the pronoun clitic a r which is
left-peripheral in PP, and it is even possible to extract this clitic from The
clitic
behaviour of ar is still not accounted every
PP.
ar has puzzled many syntacticians, but in my opinion the syntactic for
satisfactorily.
Furthermore,
not
preposition may take a clitic with it, but to account for that situation
here would be outside the scope of this study. The two clitic positions
in
PP
are represented in (13).
(13)a
PP Ρ met
PP Cl
CI
am (with him)
ar
Ρ
I .1., in (in it)
The syntax of Dutch subject clitics has not been very closely investigated. The fact that these clitics circle around the INFL-node without intervening material suggests that subject clitics in Dutch are part of INFL. The
syntactic
de-
pendence of subject clitics from INFL is furthermore suggested by the fact that these clitics only occur if their inflectional host induces a finite There
seem
to
be
sentence.
no problems with respect to government-binding theory (cf.
Chomsky (1982)) for this point of view, so let us assume it tures in (14) are intended to clarify this.
( 14)a COMP
INFL / \ Conj
(hij dacht) ((he thought) that
CI
J I
dat ak would come)
NP ι e
VP kwam
here.
The
struc-
- 45 b
S' COMP
INFL
/\
V
Cl
S NP
VP NP
V ι e
e
I boeken gaf ie boe (did he give books)
The other clitics are assumed to be
allomorphs
of
the
corresponding
strong
forms and may be inserted freely in place of the strong form. Some syntactically possible insertion positions for these clitics are excluded later on in
the
derivation by prosodie and pragmatic constraints. So much for syntactical remarks. In the rest of this chapter, I will not concerned
with
Dutch
syntactic
structure,
but
structure with respect to cliticization. Of course, prosodie structure in
part
on
syntactic
be
primarily with its prosodie depends
structure (cf. Chapter 1.), and in this respect syntax
will sometimes become relevant. However, I will show that phonological cliticization
in Dutch only depends on prosodie structure (as a derivative of syntax)
and not directly on syntactic structure, showing, like Klavans (1982, 1985) and Zwicky
and
Pullum
(1983)
did, that phonological cliticization and syntactic
cliticization are independent of each other. In this respect, this
study
con-
tributes to a clarification of the relation between syntax and phonology.
4. Dutch clitics and Voicing Phenomena^
4.1. Introduction
Like many related languages, Dutch phonoloy has a process of Final Devoicing of obstruents. The literature formulates this rule either with a word- or a syllable-boundary as a right-hand environment (for example neveld (1981) and Booij (1981), respectively):
in
Trommelen
and
Zon-
- 46 (15)
Final Devoicing a
[-son] - > [-voice] /
b
[-son] - > [-voice] /
#
Both rules account for the data in (16), if syllabification has the
phonologi-
cal word as its domain which causes word- and syllable-boundaries to coincide.
hui[t] (skin) kwa[p] (lobe) hui[s] (house)
hui[t]#arts (dermatologist) kwa[p]#aal (pout eel) hui[s]#nummer (house number)
hui[d]en (skins) kwa[b]en (lobes) hui[z]en (houses)
po[t] (pot) knoo[p] (button) ka[s] (pay desk)
po[t]#huis (cellar shop) knoo[p]#laars (button boot) ka[s]#houder (cashier)
po[t]en (pots) kno[p]en (buttons) ka[s]en (pay desks)
Another well-known phonological phenomenon in Dutch is voicing assimilation
in
obstruent-clusters. Voicing Assimilation manifests itself in two ways depending on the type of rightmost consonant: rightmost fricatives induce PROGRESSIVE ASSIMILATION;
for
other
rightmost
obstruents
assimilation is REGRESSIVE. The
rules are as follows (cf. Trommelen and Zonneveld (1981)).
(17)a
Progressive Assimilation [-son, +cont] - > [-voice] / [-voice] (#)
b
Regressive Assimilation [-son] - > [+voice] /
And these are the data to match.
(#) [-son, +voice]
- 47 -
har[ts]eer (heart-ache) zi[tx]at (bottom) tre[kf]ogel (migrant)
[z]eer (pain) [y]at (hole) [v]ogel (bird)
zi[db]ad (hipbath) ka[zb]oek (account book) knoo[bd]oek (shawl)
zi[t]en (sit) ka[s]en (pay desks) kno[p]en (buttons)
There is an exhaustive and transitive ordering relation between (15)
and
the
rules
in
(17): they apply linearly, in the order given as argued convincingly
in Trommelen and Zonneveld (1981) and Booij (1981). Usually, this analysis of the voicing assimilation phenomena built
on
the
in
Dutch^
behaviour of internal clusters in compounds. In this section, I
will discuss a set of additional data which one should like to fall within descriptive
range
of
the
support
to,
the
the
above analysis, and it will not be surprising that
these data include Dutch clitics. In this analysis, I will give
is
general
make
use
of,
and
prosodie theory of cliticization presented in
Chapter 2.3. Unavoidable side-topics will include the allomorphy
in
the
past
tense verb suffix between - d a and - t a and we will have occasion to go into some properties of Dutch syllable structure.
- 48 -
4.2. The Behaviour of Clitics with respect to Voice
4.2.1. Initial Observations
The most fruitful observations to start out from is the behaviour of verb-forms followed by a schwa-initial clitic. Using first person singular forms, we avoid inflection, and are thus able to inspect Final Devoicing. A fact is that forms
these
have two possible pronunciations, one in which FD apparently applies and
another in which FD fails. Since dialects seem to differ as tion,
I
want
speech, and in rime-related
order pairs
to which
bring vary
this
out
between
most voice
verb-final obstruent.
(19)ai
ii
m
bi
ik (I ik (I ik (I ik (I ik (I ik (I ik (I ik (I ik (I ik (I ik (I ik (I ik (I ik (I ik (I ik (I
to
this
observa-
to emphasize that this is a clear phenomenon of at least my own
he[b] ar met rietjes (cf. he[b]an) have her with straws) le[b]ar met rietjes (cf. le[b]eran) sip through straws) he[p] or met houtjes(cf. he[b]an) have her with bits of wood) kle[p]sr met houtjes (cf. kle[p]eran) clapper with bits of wood) kra[b] ar goed (cf. kra[b]an) scratch her well) zwa[b]er goed (cf. zwa[b]eran) swab well) kra[p] er hard (cf. kra[b]en) scratch her hard) kla[p]er hard (cf. kla[p]eren) clapper hard) schro[b] er flink (cf. schro[b]en) scrub her hard) do[b]er flink (cf. do[b]eren) bob enormously) schro[p] en boek (cf. schro[b]en) scrub a book) klo[p] en boek (cf. klo[p]en) pat a book) laa[d] am uit (cf. la[d]en) unload it) a[d]em uit (cf. a[d]emen) breathe out) laa[t] am uit (cf. la[d]en) unload it) laa[t] em uit (cf. la[t]en) let him out)
clearly, and
lack
(19) of
below voice
gives for the
- 49 ii
ik (I ik (I ik (I ik (I
ha[d] ar veel (cf. had many of them) kla[d]ar veel (cf. often daub) ha[t] an boek (cf. had a book) ja[t] an boek (cf. steal a book)
ha[d]an) kla[d]aran) ha[d]an) ja[t]an)
However clear these facts are, they are somewhat obscured by optionally
voices
a
process
which
t's preceded by a vowel before schwa-initial clitics except
ar. This is exemplified in (20).
(20)a b c d e f
Hoeveel pata(t/d} ak eet (How many chips I eat) Ik ha{t/d) an leuk zaakje (I had a nice shop) Hij moe{t/d) am zien (He must see him) Jan ee(t/d} at niet (John does not eat it) Wa{t/d) as je dat waard (What is that worth to you) Ik wee{t/*d} ar te vinden (I know how to find her)
This latter phenomenon is only triggered by clitics and as such must be dered
a
pure
cliticization
consi-
process. I will return to these cases in section
4.2.2. In addition to the facts given in (19) d-facts
which
are
and
(20)
we
can
add
other
not influenced by clitic-t-voicing, cases in which d and t
are not preceded by a vowel, but by a consonant. The underlyingly sive
some voiced
plo-
shows again two possible pronunciations, while the underlyingly voiceless
one does not,
the
latter
being
an
indication
clitic-t-voicing.
(21)a
b
c
Ston{d/t) ak daar (cf. ston[d]an) (Did I stand there) Welke lon{t/*d) ak daar zag (cf. lon[t]an) (Which fuse I saw there) Welk woor(d/t} ak hoor (cf. woor[d]an) (Which word I hear) Hij hoor{t/*d) a n woord (He hears a word) Welke waar{d/t} as dat (cf. waar[d]an) (Which host is that) Welke vaar{t/*d) as dat (cf. vaar[t]an) (Which canal is that)
of
the
non-application
of
- 50 We may conclude
that
final
underlyingly
voiced
obstruents
followed
by
a
vowel-initial clitic have a voiceless as well as a voiced pronunciation. Furthermore, there is the complicating phenomenon of clitic-t-voicing which may
ob-
scure these two possible pronunciations. So
far,
(19)
vowel-initial
and
(20)
clitics
were
preceded
biased by
toward
plosives.
With
This
to
fricatives, the facts are comparable, but
this is more difficult to demonstrate due to the relative paucity examples.
respect of
relevant
time, the argument goes as follows. Verbs with an underlyingly
voiced final fricative allow both the voiceless and the voiced variant before a vowel-initial
clitic,
but
underlyingly
voiceless fricatives surface only as
voiceless.
(22)ai
ii
bi
ii
ik krij{s/*z) ar twee bij elkaar (cf. krij[s]an) (I shout two together) ik wij{s/z) ar twee bij elkaar (cf. wij[z]an) (I point two together) ik la(s/*z} ar twee bij elkaar (cf. la[s]an) (I weld two together) ik la{s/z} ar twee bij elkaar (cf. la[z]an) (I read two together) pa(f/*v) ak iets (cf. pa[f]an) (Did I shot something) ga{f/v) ak iets (cf. ga[v]an) (Did I give something) ik le(*f/v)ar drie dagen in (cf. le[v]aran) (I hand in three days) ik lee{f/v) ar drie dagen in (cf. le[v]an) (I have lived there for three days)
In so far as distinction in voice can be observed for velar behaviour is the same as for the other fricatives.
(23)ai
ii
dan jui{x/*Y) ak (cf. jui[x]an) (Then I jubilate) dan bui(x/y) ak (cf. bui[y]an) (Then I bow) la(x/*y) ak erg hard (cf. la[x]an) (Did I laugh very loudly) lafx/γ} ak erg hard (cf. la[y]an) (Did I lay very uncomfortably)
fricatives,
their
- 51 -
These data demonstrate also (cf. also Zonneveld (1982)) that the voiced
frica-
tives of wijz ar etc. are not due to a rule of intervocalic voicing of a finally voiceless fricative, as the literature sometimes suggests. So also an
underlyingly
voiced
fricative
in
case
occurs in front of a vowel-initial clitic,
there are two possible pronunciations for these
fricatives:
a
voiced
and
a
voiceless one. So far, the examples consist of a finite verb form plus They
schwa-initial
clitic.
can easily be extended to other vowel-initial clitics (ie, u, uw and ons)
as in (24a) and to other constructions as in (24b) as well. There seems to be a preference distance
1
to
apply
FD in (24b). This is probably due to a 'larger syntactic
between word and clitic. This syntactic distance might
be
expressed
in prosodie structure, but this is an issue that will not be pursued here.
(24)a
b
ga{f/v) ie het boek (cf. ga[v]on) (Did he give a book) wee{s/z) ie naar mij (cf. we[z]an) (Did he point at me) ha{t/d} u eraan gedacht (cf. ha[d]an) (Did you think about it) ik kra{p/b) u niet hard (cf. kra[b]an) (I do not scratch you very hard) ik laa{t/d) uw auto uit (cf. la[d]on) (I unload your car) ik proe{f/v) uw drankje (cf. proe[v]an) (I taste your drink) hij wee{s/z) ons na (cf. we[z]an) (He pointed after us) ik ga{f/v) ons boek weg (cf. ga[v]an) (I gave our book away) ... welke hoe{t/d} am past (cf. hoe[d]an) (which hat suits him) ... welk we{p/b} ie heeft gezien (cf. we[b]an) (which cobweb he has seen) ... welke roo{s/z) ons bevalt (cf. ro[z]an) (which rose pleases us) ... welk hui{s/z} ak koop (cf. hui[z]an) (which house he buys) ... dat de kaa{s/z} ar niet beviel (cf. ka[z]an) (that the cheese did not please her) ... dat de naa{f/v) en beetje kapot was (cf. na[v]an) (that the hub was somewhat broken) ... dat de kra{p/b) u bevalt (cf. kra[b]an) (that the crab pleases you) ... dat het ba{t/d) am te heet is (cf. ba[d]an) (that the bath is too hot)
- 52 -
The data in (19), (21), (22), (23) and (24) clitics
represent
excellent
examples
of
which are 'schizophrenic' with respect to 'affix' or 'word' status. If
the clitic behaves like an affix, the preceding word and clitic are one logical
phono-
word so that FD cannot apply. If the clitic has 'word' status, FD must
make the final voiced obstruent of the verb-form voiceless. voiceless
obstruents
both
Thus,
voiced
and
occur before vowel-initial clitics, and it is this
observation that we will rephrase into an analysis in the next subsection.
4.2.2. A Prosodie Account
Essentially against the background of Chapter 2.3., the data in
the
preceding
section showed clearly that Dutch is a likely candidate of a language which exploits both ways of phonological clitic-adjunction. The first is to optionally adjoin the clitic-syllable to left,
the
word
on
its
resulting in a weak syllable node. This triggers resyllabification, thus
bleeding FD.
(25)
underlying
M
I
λ heb adjoin $ to M
1
am
h
heb resyllabification
$
Λ
$
1
am
$
H bam J
he FD
he [b Jam
Notice that, although the prosodie category Foot plays a crucial description
role
in
the
of Dutch word stress (cf. Neyt and Zonneveld (1982a) and Visch and
Kager (1984)), cliticization does not influence it. So feet are not represented here and in further illustrations in this chapter.
- 53 The second possibility is for the clitic-syllable not to adjoin to the on
its
left.
In
that
case,
word
it is considered to be a non-lexical item with
respect to the syntax-phonology mapping rules of Nespor and Vogel
(1982)
(cf.
Chapter 1.2.). In this way it will be dominated by the prosodie category 0. Although there is one particular situation in Butch Vogel
for
which
the
Nespor
and
proposals appear to be in need of revision, I will adopt their proposals
here and modify them below. Primarily on the basis of the fact that in NP's the recursive
side
is to the right of Ν, I consider Dutch to be a right-recursive
language. So 0's are usually constructed above every lexical category with non-lexical
items
all
to its left. Thus, the underlyingly voiced obstruent of the
relevant words in the examples of the preceding section^ are now word- and syllable-final, and must undergo FD. It does not matter whether the word preceding the clitic is non-lexical as in (26a) or lexical as in (26b). To clarify syntactic structure is indicated in underlying representation.
(26)a
underlying
S* COMP
^
^
INFL NP VP NP
I I
AUX
Cl
V
ik heb am gezien (I have seen him) 0-construction
0
Λ-
ik heb em gezien FD
Ρ he[p]em
this,
- 54
b
underlying
NP / \
V ¿
gav xe een boek (did he give a book) 0-construction
t gav îe een boek
FD
f ga[f]ie However, there are sentences in which a non-lexical item is the last element of the
sentence,
and
hence
cannot
be taken into account by Nespor and Vogel's
0-construction rules. I propose that such a floating non-lexical joined
by
item
is
ad-
convention as a weak member to the phonological phrase on its left,
as shown in (28)· This analysis accounts for the voiceless obstruent variant of the finite verb in (27), as is also shown in (28).
(27) (28)
De jongen za{x/y) am (The boy saw him) underlying
de jongen 0-construction
0
zay
0
d ' de ^ jongen zay adjoin to 0
am
0 0 A Κ d de jongen zay am
FD za[x]am
- 55 As shown before, the adjunction of the clitic-syllable to the finite
verb
can
be held responsible for the fact that the underlyingly voiced obstruent in (27) may also remain voiced. One final complication concerns the optional rule of clitic-t-voicing, which voices
pre-clitic
t between a vowel on its left and a schwa on its right. For
ease of exposition some relevant examples are repeated here.
(29)a b c
Hoeveel pata(t/d} ak eet (How many chips I eat) Ik wee{t/d) an leuk zaakje (I know a nice shop) Hij moe{t/d} am zien (He must see him)
This process has four currently relevant characteristics. First, gered
by
schwa-initial
clitics
it
is
trig-
only^. Second, it thus does not apply before
truly suffixal -an and - a r , as shown in (30)
(30)
wee{t/*d)an (know) maa{t/*d)an (sizes) pata{t/*d)an (chips) moe{t/*d)an (must) ui{t/*d}an (utter)
Third, it can occur
across
ee{t/*d)ar (eater) groo{t/*d}ar (bigger) plo{t/*d}ar (plotter) zoe(t/*d)ar (sweeter) slui{t/*d}ar (shutter)
a
case-marked
trace
left
behind
by
syntactic
WH-movement.
(31)a b c
wat^ denkt hij da{t/dl e^ am is overkomen (What does he think that has happened to him) wieH ee{t/d} eH at op (Who will eat it) ik vraag me af welke staaft/d^ e ± an koning heeft (I ask myself which country has a king)
Within the context of our left-hand
conception
of
the
internal
of
the
side of grammar (cf. Chapter 1.2.1.), this implies that this process
is purely phonological, and will have to be accounted well.
organization
Fourth,
for
in
this
style
as
the examples in (32) show that the schwa-initial clitic must be
preceded by a phonological word ending in t, and not by the syllable-final t of a clitic for clitic-t-voicing to apply.
- 56 -
(32)a b c
geef a{t/*d) as aan hem (Give it to him, please) om a{t/*d) am te geven (To give it to him) dat a{t/*d} an keer gebeurd (That it happens once)
Since ,pre-clitic t differs from pre-suffixal t the
most
in
allowing
clitic-t-voicing,
obvious choice in our parametric theory is to say that t-voicing ap-
plies to 0-joined clitic-syllables, because this is the level where clitics are recognizable as such^. This is taken care of by the following rule.
(33)
Clitic-t-voicing t -> d / ...V —
M
)
($
...
Sample derivations run as follows:
(34)
1
^ I L. I Î Λ I.Í υI A
M
$
M
$
M
(ik) braad am 0-constr.
0
(ik) weet an zaakje
M
braad am adj. to 0
weet an zaakje
M
braad am FD
t
t-voicing
d
vac. d
The variation in application of FD in case a vowel-initial clitic by
is
preceded
an underlyingly voiced obstruent is accounted for in a straightforward way,
couched within the prosodie theory of cliticization as proposed in
Chapter
2.
Depending on whether the clitic has become part of the phonological word to its left or part of 0, the underlyingly voiced obstruent will surface as voiced voiceless
or
respectively. The analysis presented to account for these facts will
provide the basis for our further account of the phonology of Dutch
cliticiza-
- 57 tion. Having thus succesfully dealt with the behaviour of vowel-initial clitics with respect to the voice rules of Dutch phonology, the question arises ally
natur-
how our prosodie analysis accounts for the behaviour of consonant-initial
clitics with respect to these rules. This will be the topic of
the
next
sec-
tion.
A.2.3. Consonant-initial Clitics
In this section, I will deal with consonant-initial clitics within work
which
tics come in two types, obstruent-initial and sonorant-initial deal
with
the
frame-
has been developed above. For our purposes, consonant-initial cliones.
We
will
them in this order. To be more precise: first the clitics za, zieh,
and zan will be analyzed, then I will deal with the vacillation between d and t in
dar/tar
and
die/tie, third I will look at the consequences of the account
for d/t vacillation for the allomorphy in the past tense suffix between -da and -ta, and finally the sonorant consonant-initial clitics will be accounted for.
The obstruent-initial clitics which fit exactly in the above framework are zieh
and
zan.
s's and in other cases they remain z. In responsible
the
former
cases,
FD
and
PA
are
for the voiceless s, while in the latter neither FD nor PA are ap-
plicable. This is shown through the derivations of (35).
(35)a
za,
After obstruent-final words clitic-initial z's are turned into
underlying
M
gav ze had za (gave she) (had she) adjoin to M
vièl za (fell she)
h Jp t¡
FD
gav za
had za
PA
f
t s
ga[fs]a
¥ .
viel za
s ha[ts]a
zou za (would she)
zou[z]a viel[z]a
- 58 b
underlying
M gav Z9
had ζ s
Λ „ M iA$
0 0 1 M $I 1 1 1 gav ΖΘ FD
x
f
I
M
ι
$
hid z i t
PA
s
s ga[fs]s
ha[ts]3
viel[z]a
zou[z]a
The left-most two derivations in (35a) give evidence that FD applies at ble-edge
rather
than
sylla-
at word-edge: if FD were to apply at word-edge only, it
would be inapplicable in (35a), bleeding, incorrectly, PA. In effect, then, for fricative-initial
clitics our two ways of adjunction will always give the same
output, which is a formal representation of the fact that there is indeed one
output
rather
than
two
only
(as with vowel-initial clitics) per word-clitic
pair.
When we consider the d-initial clitics, we find several tions.
It
seems
that
unexpected
pronuncia-
the initial d's of the clitics die, dar and da allow a
voiced as well as a voiceless
pronunciation,
if
they
are
preceded
by
ob-
struents, as in the following examples.
(36)a b c d e f
tro[ft]ie doel (did he strike goal) [vd] ik ga[ft]ar een boek (I gave her a book) [vd] hij hee[st]a kist op (he hoisted the box) [zd] wee[st]ie naar mij (did he point at me) [zd] daar lie[pt]a weg (there was the road) [bd] ik kra[pt]ar niet (I do not scratch her) [bd]
This vacillation of initial d goes beyond the scope of clitics. The same viour
beha-
is exhibited by the non-lexical items dan (than), die (this, which), dat
- 59 (that), daar (there), deze (this) and dit (this). This
vacillation
between
d
and t has already been observed in the older literature (cf. Leenen (1954), Van Haeringen (1955) and Kloeke (1956)). Zonneveld (1982)8 attempts to analyze this phenomenon framework.
within
a
generative
In order to account for the assimilations observed, he assumes that
these non-lexical items begin with underlying é, which may be subject to an optional
'vacillation
rule1 changing é into d. Because Dutch phonetics does not
have é's and 0's, they are changed into the regular dental plosives d and t the
end
of
the
these ideas into our prosodie account, some relevant derivations are (37).
at
derivation by a rule of Absolute Neutralization. Translating those
in
Again, observe that our two types of adjunction trigger derivations with
identical outputs.
(37)a
M
M
/\ /\ lili
$
$
gav èie Vacill. FD
Abs. Neutr.
$
gav èie
0
f
0
0
IΛ IΛ II II
M
$
gav èie
d
PA RA
$
0
M
$
gav èie
d f
f θ
ν t ga[vd]ie ga[ft]ie
f θ
ν
ga[vd]ie
t ga[ft]ie
-
M
M
/\
/\
$I
1$
krab dir Vacill. FD
60
$ ι
-
$ I
krab élr
Ρ
PA b
Abs. Neutr.
—
0
ι r\
M
$
J J krab éar
Ρ θ
RA
0
d
d Ρ
-
Ρ -
θ
-
t
b t
kra[bd]ar M
kra[pt]ar M
$I 1$ 1 1 viel èie Vacill.
0 0 1 A M $ι I 1 1 krab éar
$ 1 1$ 1 1 viel èie
-
kra[bd]ar 0 0 1 Λ M $ι Ι I 1 viel èie
d -
-
PA
-
-
RA
-
-
-
-
d viel[d]ie
0
0
I Λ M $ I t 1 1 viel èie
d
FD
Abs. Neutr.
kra[pt]ar
viel[d]ie
viel[d]ie
d viel[d]ie
The outputs obtained tally perfectly with the observed facts: after there
is
vacillation
There is one snag, however, as Zonneveld (1982) observes, have
in
cases
where
we
a word ending in d or t followed by one of the é-forms or one of the cli-
tics ie and ar which are optional variants of die and dar. In these never
obstruents
between d and t, while after sonorants d is obligatory.
cases
one
finds the d-variant^, as is illustrated in (38), in spite of the predic-
tions of our analysis so far.
-
ston[t]ie von t ar boo t 3 laa t Θ zie t sr maak[t]a
61
-
4> / (j
[-son]
The left-hand syllable-break of rule (62) ensures that the rule applies to clitic-schwa, and prohibits the rule from applying to word-internal syllables with schwa: normally, syllable-initial schwa in Dutch is containing
out,
since
under consideration is adjoined to the word to its left, schwa duced.
any
syllable
a schwa begins with a consonant. Therefore, when one of the clitics
This
cannot
be
re-
is particularly clear in examples with vowel-initial clitics pre-
ceded by underlyingly voiced obstruents.
(63)ai Ga[f] á k ii *Ga[v] i k bi Kra[p] is ii *Kra[b] is ci Ik he[p] ii *Ik he[b]
vier boeken (Did I give four books) vier boeken over mijn rug (Please, scratch my back) over mijn rug it boek hier (I have the book here) it boek hier
We derive example (63ai) in its schwa-less variant by structure
applying
rule
(62)
to
(64a). Conversely, we capture (63aii), because that sentence has the
structure of (64b) where resyllabification blocks both FD (as explained in section 4.) and CSR.
(64)a
0 gaf
1 sk vier boeken
b
ga
vak
We now have a first approximation of how CSR operates. However, there occasions in which it fails, and these must be dealt with as well.
are
two
- 77 First, if the clitic schwa is preceded by a non-lexical item ending in struent,
CSR
is
impossible,
but
if
an
ob-
a preceding non-lexical item ends in a
sonorant, CSR may apply. This is illustrated nicely by the prepositions op (on) in
and
(in). If the former precedes clitic schwa, CSR is prohibited, while if
the latter does, CSR may apply.
(65)a b c d
op *op in in
at it at it
huis (on the house) huis huis (in the house) huis
The same can be illustrated by the Dutch complementizer for t h a t which may have the pronunciation dat as well as daji in casual speech. I assume the t of dat to be victim of a casual speech rule of t-deletion. If dat precedes clitic CSR
may
not
be
schwa,
but if daji precedes the clitic, application of CSR
applied,
seems to be the most natural situation.
(66)a b c d
dat *dat ?dat daji
ak ¿k ak ik
zit (that I sit) zit zit zit
However, if a lexical item precedes clitic schwa, CSR may apply. In it
this
case
does not matter at all whether this lexical item ends in an obstruent as in
(67a) or in a sonorant as in (67b).
(67)ai ii iii bi ii
Ik koop it huis (I buy the house) Praat és een keer (Just talk once) Welke maat ik pas (Which size suits me) Ik win it huis (I win the house) Ik zie it boek (I see the book)
Recalling our discussion of prosodie structure above the word level in
Chapter
1.2.2. and Section 4., it is clear that the crucial distinction between the examples in (67) on the one hand, and the examples in (65) and (66) on the other, hinges
on
a
difference
in
0-structure.
In
(65) and (66) both the element
- 78 -
preceding the clitic and the clitic itself belong to the (67)
they
same
0,
whereas
in
belong to different 0's. So I assume that the ungrammaticalities in
(59) and (60) must be explained in terms of the phonological domain 0. Following this line of thought, it has been suggested in Booij (1981, p. 98) that
in
casual speech the final consonant of a word becomes ambisyllabic with
the initial consonants of a following word. Booij's arguments for this position seem
to me purely intuitive, And not supported by evidence from any phonologi-
cal process. However, it seems that in the process of CSR, we have hand
a
case
at
in which ambisyllabicity plays a highly useful role. If within 0 the last
obstruent of an item is linked with the onset of the initial next
word,
we
syllable
of
the
are now in the position to explain the facts of (65), (66) and
(67) in the following way. The ungrammatical examples in (65) and (66) contain an now
ambisyllabic
with
which
is
a clitic. The clitic is no longer schwa-initial as re-
quired by rule (62) and hence cannot undergo CSR. In the of
obstruent grammatical
examples
(65) and (66), the clitics will still be schwa-initial as the words preced-
ing the clitic end in a sonorant which cannot be ambisyllabic. Finally, in (67) the words preceding the clitic are 0-final and hence their final consonant cannot be ambisyllabic, hence CSR may apply.
The second occasion in which CSR fails, is its overapplication in
cases
where
the clitic is sentence-final. In these case CSR is impossible, as is illustrated in (68).
(68)a b
dat *dat Jan *Jan
doe doe wil wil
ak (I will do that) ék at (John wants it) ét
Precisely because nothing follows the clitics of (68), their cannot
final
obstruents
be ambisyllabic. Thus, if one requires that the obstruent of the clitic
which has to undergo CSR, be ambisyllabic, the facts of (68) are accounted for: in
(68)
the k of ak and the t of at are not ambisyllabic and hence CSR cannot
apply. Notice furthermore that it is no longer necessary to assume nant
in
the
conso-
rule (62) to be an obstruent. Sonorants cannot be ambisyllabic, so in
place of [-son] we had better use the variable 'x' as part of the environment. It is now possible to reformulate rule (62) as a multi-level rule resulting
in
-
the
formulation
79
-
given in (69). It is of crucial importance here that the C of
the left-hand environment is not linked to a segmental
position,
representing
in fact an empty onset.
(69)
CSR (second version) $ $ / X X X c ν c -> c ν c
I
1/
1/
θ X X where 'x' is a variable consonant
The syllabic representations in (70a) of the examples in (65) show that in
the
first example the ambisyllabic ρ blocks application of CSR (69), whereas in the second the η is not ambisyllabic and hence does not block CSR. With respect the
examples
one of its examples shows. Finally, the examples of (68) are the
to
(67), rule (69) can be applied as the representation in (70b) of
clitic-obstruent
is
not
ambisyllabic
excluded
because
as required by rule (69). This is
shown for (68a) in (70c).
(70)a
$
$
$
/ X / X / X cvccvccvc I 1/ aι t Mhu AisI op
M I $ / χ
$ / X
$ $ $ / X / X / X cvccvccvc ! ι aI t Mhu AisI in
$ / X
cvccvccvc I Al I M A ! ko op
Λ
a t hu is $
/ X
c ν c c ν c
ΙΑ
do e
11
a k
We now have a good insight into the optimal input structure of step is to take a look at the output structure.
CSR.
The
next
-
80
-
5.3. The Output Structure of CSR
If we assume that CSR is a deletion process, it seems reasonable that after the application
of
CSR an automatic process of resyllabification takes place, at-
taching the already ambisyllabic consonant to the syllable on deletes
the
after
resyllabification
is
in
between
sentences
First,
accordance
language-specific requirements. Second, it must now be ambiguities
right:
CSR
schwa, and hence the syllable loses its head and as a consequence
its capacity to be a syllable. We now expect two things. structure
its
with
possible
the
syllable
universal to
and
construct
with clitics in which CSR has applied and sen-
tences without clitics. However, both expectations do not turn out to be
true,
as we will show below. First, the examples in (71) show that CSR and the accompanying
resyllabifi-
cation may result in extremely queer consonant clusters in onset position, such as tbl and ksn. These clusters only occur in case of ambisyllabicity.
(71)a b
¿t_bloedt behoorlijk (it is bleeding enormously) ¿k_snoep niet veel (I do not eat many candies)
Second, the data of (72) are selected so as to enforce ambiguities between sentences with a clitic in which CSR has applied and sentences without them.
(72)ai ii bi ii
¿k_aas op een mooi Delfts blauw bord (I lie in wait for a beautiful Delft-blue plate) kaas op een mooi Delfts blauw bord (cheese on a beautiful Delft-blue plate) ik zag ze ¿t_rekken (I saw them stretch it) ik zag ze trekken (I saw them pull)
Upon listening to these sentences, however, even the less trained ear perceives a
difference: they are not ambiguous, although it is not easy to describe pre-
cisely what the perceived difference is.
In order to gain some insight in why the sentences in (72a) and (72b) lack
am-
biguity, and guided by the idea that CSR is a rather late (casual speech) rule,
-
81
-
it turned out to be useful to make oscillograms of these sentences in search of relevant
phonetic
detail.
The oscillograms have been made of three speakers:
since I am primarily concerned with my own dialect, I first from
my
own
with oscillograms from two other speakers. All tendencies^
made
oscillograms
speech and, second, I tested their reliability by comparing them
Relevant
portions
of
some
oscillograms
showed
the
same
of these oscillograms are given in
(73).
(73)ai
fur
ék
a
aa
(73)aii
HÉÉIÌÌ k
aa
s
(73)bi
w ζ
ét
e
(73)bii
M ζ
4 év τ
e
^ e
t
r
e
k
k
e
n
—
^
kk
—
e
η
-
82
-
First, consider the sentences in (72b) and the
corresponding
oscillograms
in
(73b). There are two differences between these two sentences. The first difference is the duration of the silent interval between za and clitic t on the
one
hand, and za and the verb trekken on the other. In the oscillograms represented in (73), the former is two to three times as long as difference
the
latter.
The
second
is that clitic t seems to be rather different from the initial t of
trekken. Sentence-initially, that is in the oscillograms of
(73a),
the
first
difference cannot be observed, of course, but the second difference seems to be even clearer than in sentence-medial position. Phonetically speaking, there appears
to be a rather long silent interval in the position of the deleted schwa
and the remaining clitic-obstruent seems to be onset-position.
different
from
obstruents
in
This is the phonetic raw material. Now what do we make of this
from a phonological perspective? As the oscillograms show, a complete deletion of the clitic
schwa
has
not
occurred, although there does not seem to be a real vowel segment. For a phonological account, we must look for some sort of degenerate segment or
syllable,
i.e. an underspecified segment or syllable which has as one of its consequences a period of phonetic silence. A possible explanation may come from (1983)
Trommelen's
proposals for the underlying shape of schwa, especially with respect to
its behaviour in Dutch syllable structure. On the basis of a number of butional
and
phonological
observations
distri-
Trommelen proposes to consider Dutch
schwa a bimoric (=long) vowel in underlying representation: the peak of a
syl-
lable containing schwa consists of an empty position to the left and a position filled by 'schwa' to the right, as in (74).
(74)
V
/\
This makes the phonological shape of schwa-consonant clitics that of (75).
- 83 -
(75)
$ C
V
c
Λ I
A
s
χ
My tentative proposal here now is, that, adapting the (1983),
it
is
framework
of
Trommelen
indeed the schwa on the segmental tier that is deleted by CSR,
but, at the same time, the empty position remains unaffected. Although here
with
the
I
deal
output-structure of CSR, on the basis of this structure, it is
necessary to reformulate CSR once again, as is done in (76).
(76)
CSR (final version) $ $ / X / X c ν c -> c ν c
Λ1/
IL/
Δ sx Δχ where 'χ' is a variable consonant^
CSR now generates a degenerate syllable with a dummy V on the CV-tier and hence there
is
still
a residue of schwa. I propose that this may be the way to ac-
count for the lack of ambiguity in (72). In (72a) k and aas of are
one
the
noun
kaas
syllable while clitic k belongs both to the degenerate syllable after
application of CSR and to the syllable formed by the verb aas. The
same
holds
for the examples in (72b). (77) intends to make this clear.
$
(77)ai C
V
Λ c1
!
k a a s bi
$
/X C V c Λ Ik I tre
AV c I Δ k
C
c
Ac Λ I a a s V
Λ
c
V
c
$ / Χ C V c
IM I I Δ t r e k
Precisely this difference in prosodie structure can also for
be
held
responsible
the fact that the paired sentences in (72) are not ambiguous. Notice also,
- 84 -
however, that we can now solve our second problem. longer
It
is
not
necessary
any
to account for the awkward syllable-initial strings of clitic-consonant
and initial consonants in (71). One can simply remain silent on them, clitic-consonants
are
ambisyllabic,
a
situation
which
as
both
is rather normal in
Dutch. Notice finally that the empty V-position will be interpreted as a silent interval by the phonetic part of
g r a m m a r
5.4. Conclusions
In this section, I have given an analysis of speech.
the
process
of
CSR
in
It turns out to depend on prosodie structure and the the notion of am-
bisyllabicity, and to fit into the theory of cliticization proposed in 2.,
casual
in
that
only
the relevant clitics which are dominated by 0, may undergo
CSR. The composition of the vowel schwa, with an empty and a position
associated
Chapter
to
V
filled
segmental
on the CV-tier as proposed by Trommelen (1983) may
play an important role in explaining some characteristics
of
CSR.
Under
her
proposal, it is now easy to obtain a degenerate syllable which provides the explanation for some unexpected disambiguities. This degenerate syllable
is
in-
terpreted as a silent interval by the phonetic part of grammar.
6. Ν in Hiatus
6.1. Introduction
A natural tendency in languages is to avoid sequences of two or vowels
tions, many grammars tend to employ some phonological operation them.
more
adjacent
at the phonetic level. If such sequences arise in the course of derivato
annihilate
One of the vowels may be deleted, or a consonant is inserted between the
sequence of vowels. Dutch has both types of processes. For example, within pho-
- 85 -
nological
words
in the lexicon a schwa is deleted if it stands before another
vowel as illustrated in (78) (cf. for example Trommelen and Zonneveld (1981, p. 68)).
(78)
ambassade (embassy) syllaba (syllable) synoda (synod) genada (mercy) boeta (fine)
ambassadá-eur (embassador) syllató-isch (syllabic) synodé-aal (synodal) genad¿-ig (mercyful) boet¿-en (pay)
The other option is exemplified by the process of Homorganic
Glide
Insertion,
inserting j or w between nonlow long vowels including diphthongs, and a following vowel (see for example Trommelen and Zonneveld (1981, p. 69)).
(79)
knie/knie-j-en (knee(s)) ree/ree-j-en (deer(s)) kei/kei-j-en (stone(s))
stratego/stratego-w-en (stratego) judo/judo-w-en (judo) kano/kano-w-en (canoe)
A third hiatus-filling phenomenon inserts an η between a schwa and a word-initial
following
or clitic-initial vowel which bears a very low or no stressl6, as
is shown in (80).
(80)
Met Mieka-n-op de kast Ik wilda-n-at graag zien (With Mieke on top of the cupboard) (I wanted to see it) Bij Joka-n-in de tuin Geloofda-n-ie Jan niet (With Joke in the garden) (Did he not believe John) Door schada-n-en schände Hij gaf ma-n-at boek (By trial and error) (He gave me the book)
The rightmost column of (80) makes quite clear vowel-initial
clitic
that
the
position
between
a
and a preceding schwa is a potential input for this pro-
cess. This motivates us to study this process. A second reason lies in the fact that
this
process also shows a very interesting interaction with a process in
Western dialects of Dutch deleting η in a comparable context, as is clear the
formulation
of
from
this deletion rule by Trommelen and Zonneveld (1981), and
from the accompanying mono- and poly-morphemic examples.
-
(81)a
86
-
N-deletion (T/Z's version) [+nas, +cor] -> i> / 8
b
(# / [+cons, +cont]}
bovafi bovafi-st (above) lov-arf lov-an-d (praise) heidafl heidan-afi (heathen) ochtand ochtand-afl (morning) keukaj! keukarfs (kitchen) tangajis (tangent)
I assume here that rule (81a) is ordered before n-insertion, which will be formulated
in
Section
6.3. Thus the former will feed the latter, giving surface
counterexamples to it, such as those in (82). This will be worked out below.
(82)
wij rekanan at uit (we calculate it)
i
n-deletion n-insertion
η
Further details of the phenomenon of n-deletion badly
understood,
however,
and
n-insertion
seem
to
be
and one of the aims of this section is to enlarge
our understanding of it. In section 6.2., I
will
first
give
an
account
of
n-deletion and in section 6.3., I will discuss n-insertion.
6.2. N-deletion
The first observation relevant to our account was made by
Trommelen
and
Zon-
neveld (1981) who formulated rule (81a), and also attempted to explain why this rule could not apply to first person singular and imperative verb-forms in stem-final -an, as in (83).
(83)a b c
ik opan/*opajl deuren (I open doors) tekon/*tekai< dat uit (draw that) wij opanaft deuren (we open doors) wij tekanafi dat uit (we draw that) de opajl deuren (the open doors) het tekari (the sign)
ending
- 87 -
Their explanation of this fact, based not only on number
of
different
phenomena,
n-deletion
but
also
on
a
runs as follows (see also Zonneveld (1982)).
Dutch weak verbs have a so-called 'theme-vowel1 immediately after the stem. The theme-vowel
is
deleted in the course of the derivations, and if n-deletion is
ordered before theme-vowel deletion, the cases in which η unexpectedly
remains
at word-edge are explained. This is illustrated by the following derivations.
(84)
(ik) opan-V n-del. theme-del
i> open
(wij) opan-V-an j> i opana
This analysis could be carried further when we
opan (adj.) é
opa
consider
compounds
and
words
derived by suffixation. Since Dutch compounds consist of two or more (phonological) words, one expects to find n-deletion compound-internally at This
is
indeed
word-edges.
the case (examples are partly taken from Van Oordt and Reimer
(1983)).
(85)
examarí-vrees (exam fear) wapafi-handel (weapon trade) rekaji-eenheid (nit of measure) dekaji-fabriek (blanker factory) wagarf-dwarsstraat (waggon cross road) buitafl-huis (out house) gararf-winkel (small-ware shop) keuka»4-prinses (kitchen princess)
Words derived by suffixation must be divided into at least two categories: in
which
the suffix behaves like a phonological word itself (for example with
respect to stress assignment and syllabification), and the other in suffix
is
just
part
I
which
the
of a full word, triggering resyllabification in the re-
levant cases. This difference corresponds, respectively, to the Class
one
Class
II
and
type of suffixations as defined by Siegel (1974). For the former, one
expects to find no η before the suffix, and for the latter one expects η to remain, except in front of s. This turns out to be true.
-
(86)
88
-
suffix behaves like phon. word gedegarf-heid (reliability) wetap-schap (science) eigajl-dom (property) herssrf-loos (brainless) suffix does not behave like phon. word tekan-an (draw) opan-ar (opener) volgan-da/volgan-da(next) (cf. mono-morphemic ochtand) bovafí-sta/bovafí-sta ( upper ) Leuvarl-s (from Louvain) (cf. mono-morphemic tangajís)
This seems plausible for the data presented, but clitics raise
one
particular
question: why do the clitics a n s , man and zan undergo the rule of n-deletion at least in Western Dutch, and if this is so, why does the clitic an not. first-mentioned
In
cases the η is normally deleted, although it is not word-final
here, because clitics do not form a phonological word, but only a syllable, proposed
in
the
Chapter
2.
In
as
fact, it seems that this question can be handled
rather easily by a reformulation of the rule:
N-deletion (my version)
(87)
η -> i / a
{ $) / s}
As can be seen, the deletion of η in ans, man and zan is explained by postulating
a
syllable-boundary
instead of a word-boundary as righthand environment.
N-deletion is not applied to an, on the other hand, since syllables and
maybe
in
in
vowel schwa. If η is deleted in an this syllable has schwa as its only and
exactly
Dutch,
other languages as well, can presumably not consist only of the member,
this would be excluded. Another seemingly undesirable consequence
of the reformulation of rule (81a) into (87) is that we do not seem to
account
for the fact that η is not deleted in cases such as volganda in which η is syllable-final, but remains. For an explanation of these cases we
first
have
to
make a slight digression. Berendsen and Zonneveld (1984)
propose
to
consider
consonants
preceding
schwa and following a liquid, to be ambisyllabic. This proposal was made to account for the optional schwa-insertion between liquids and nants at syllable-edge, as is shown in the examples in (88).
non-coronal
conso-
- 89 (88)
werk/werak half/halaf
werkan/werakan (work) halva/halava (half) varkan/varakan (pig) rabarbar/rabarabar (rhubarb)
Let us assume here that ambisyllabic consonants preceding schwa tricted
are
res-
to follow liquids only, but that they are always ambisyllabic^. Under
this assumption, the d of volganda is ambisyllabic, hence bleeding This
not
n-deletion.
is illustrated in (89), where schwa is represented for convenience as one
segment on the segmental tier.
$
$
$
/Χ /X A c v c c v c c v c I IΝ IΝ ι
v o l g a n d a
We now have to consider cases like opanar in which η stays. The of
representation
opanar in (90) makes it quite clear that the η is ambisyllabic. Why is this
η not deleted, although it is syllable-final?
(90)
$
$
$
/Χ A /X c v c c v c c v c I NIp aI Νη aI rI o
The answer to this question is that the non-application of n-deletion due
here
is
to a general condition on geminates, which I already dealt with in Section
4.2.3. This condition can be stated as in (91).
(91)
Geminates, i.e.
χ / \ C
, cannot be split up. C
In case of opanar the η is a geminate belonging to two C's which are each of
different
syllables,
one
syllable-final
part
and the other syllable-initial.
- 90 However, the path from syllable-final C to η does not trigger cause
condition
(91)
demands
that
n-deletion,
be-
also the other part of the n-geminate is
taken into account. In other words, the integrity of the geminate should be ensured.
See
also note 17 with respect to condition (91) and the application of
FD If we now turn to cases such as (little
blanket),
keukantja
(little
kitchen)
and
dekantja
we see that the η is deleted here as well. This is also ac-
counted for in the framework developed above. If we assume that n-deletion plies
ap-
before the merging of tj into c, dealt with in Section 4.2.3., keukantja
is represented as in (92). This representation shows
that
t
does
not
stand
directly in front of schwa, and hence cannot be ambisyllabic. Thus, η is syllable-final and is deleted by n-deletion.
(92)
ke u
k a nt is
I
Finally, we have to consider that part of grammar in which n-deletion is to placed,
i.e.
be
whether it is a lexical or a post-lexical rule. As mentioned be-
fore Trommelen and Zonneveld (1981) made it quite clear
that
n-deletion
must
take place before theme-vowel-deletion. Theme-vowel-deletion simply deletes the theme-vowel from the segmental
string.
Thus,
this
rule
is
morphologically 1 ft governed and therefore a likely candidate to be a lexical rule . Let us assume it is. In that case n-deletion must also be a lexical rule. Otherwise it not
be
ordered before theme-vowel-deletion. Some motivation for this point of
view might come from consonant-initial clitics. If after
a
word
ending
in
-an
(except
these
clitics
are
placed
of course words such as opan (1 pers.
subj.)), η is deleted. Assuming a post-lexical rule of expect
could
n-deletion,
one
would
this state of affairs if the clitic is directly dominated by 0, because
then η is syllable-final. However, if the clitic is adjoined to the word to its left,
it is comparable to "lexical words" of the same shape. So the phonologi-
cal words rekanan da... (calculate the) and represented
in
hindaranda
(hindring)
should
be
the same way, and in both representations n-deletion cannot be
applied. For rekanan da, this is not in conformity with the facts.
91
(93)
$ $ $ / Χ / Χ / Χ Λ c v c c v c c v c c V c I I Ν I NI I Ν , re k η a η d a
/ C I h
$ $ $ $ Χ / Χ / Χ / Χ V C C V C C V C C V C I Ν I Ν I Ν I inds randa
However, if we assume n-deletion to be a lexical rule, this rule is applied before
the
post-lexical operation of clitic-adjunction, resulting in a deletion
of η in rekanan which is in conformity with the facts. We see here that lexical rules can be applied to clitics proper. The η in ans, man and zan is deleted in the lexicon before these clitics become part of syntactic and post-lexical prosodie
structure.
This
is in conformity with our assumptions on cliticization
since only clitic-adjunction is assumed to be post-lexical, but clitics
them-
selves are stored in the lexicon. Now we are only left with examples consisting of words followed
ending
in -an and
by vowel-initial clitics and other non-lexical items in which a η can
be heard. As suggested earlier this is due to a process
of
n-insertion. This
process will be considered in the next section.
6.3. N-insertion
When we reconsider the left-hand examples of n-insertion given in (80) and peated
here
in (94) for convenience, it is quite clear that the domain of the
rule which accounts for this phenomenon must be a prosodie
category
because η is inserted here at the break between two 0's, cf. (96).
(94)
Met Mieka-n-op de kast Bij Joks-n-in de tuin Door schada-n-en schände
(95)
0 Met
re-
A Mieka
op de kast
above 0,
- 92 -
Bij
Joka
Λ
in de tuxn
A
Door schada
en schände
I (the Intonational Phrase) seems to b¿ the proper domain of this process, cause
no
η
is
be-
inserted in between two I's, even if all other conditions are
met. In (96), for example, η is not inserted between Joka and op.
See
Chapter
1.2.2. for the construction of I.
(96)
Ik dacht dat Joka, op wie Jantien erg lijkt, weg was (I thought that Joke, who looks much like Jantien, had gone)
Ik dacht dat Joka op wie Jantien erg lijkt weg was
This gives us the following formulation of n-insertion:
(98)
N-insertion (first version) i - > η / (j ... a 0)
(0
V
... j)
[-str]
When we consider cases in which a non-lexical item is in tion,
we
sentence-final
posi-
do find η in the relevant examples. However, that is not the correct
environment
with
non-lexical
items
respect do
not
to
0
start
for
rule
(98)
to
apply.
Sentence-final
a new 0, but are incorporated into the 0 to
their left.
(99)
ik hoorda-n-am (I heard him) Miep gaat tegen de moda-n-in (Miep goes against fashion) ?Kees vaart tegen de kada-n-op (Kees hits the quay with his boat)
This implies that the 0-boundaries in (98) are not
really
adequate,
and
the
- 93 -
rule had better be formulated as in (100).
N-insertion (final version)
(100)
i -> η / (j ... a
V
... j)
[-str]
One of the characteristics of this analysis is also that schwa-initial may
form
vowel-initial clitics may form part of the right-hand environment (cf. Notice
clitics
part of the left-hand environment of rule (100) (cf. (101)) and that (102)).
that it is irrelevant whether the clitic is dominated by the phonologi-
cal word or by the phonological phrase.
(101)
ik gaf ja-n-an boek (I gave you a book) liepen wa-n-in de tuin (did we walk in the garden) sloegen za-n-am (did they hit him)
(102)
ik gaf Wieka-n-an boek (I gave Wieke a book) wilda-n-ie niet komen (didn't he want to come) heb je de schada-n-ans bekeken (did you look at the damage)
We are now in the position to explain counterexamples in which the quite general
rule
of
n-deletion, as formulated in (87), apparently fails to apply, al-
though η is syllable-final, as in (103). In fact, n-deletion does not apply
here,
but
the
former
to
after the η has been deleted, n-insertion may again fill the
same position with n. This ordering is likely to that
fail
is
a
follow
from
the
assumption
lexical rule, as suggested in Section 6.2., while the
latter is post-lexical, since n-insertion is an I-domain rule. The
derivations
in (104) are illustrations of these assumptions.
(103) wij lagan op het gras (we lay on the grass) wij zagan am niet (we did not see him) zij zien de dekan an huis ingaan (we saw the deacon enter a house)
- 94 -
(104)
lagan op η-del. (lex.) i n-ins. (post-lex.) η
zagen am i η
dekan an έ η
lagan op
zagan am
dekan an
6.4. Concluding Remarks
We have seen in this section that within the proposed prosodie Dutch
clitics,
a
well-founded
motivation
structures
for
can be given for the fact that in
words ending in -an, η sometimes does not seem to be deleted, while it normally is.
I
argued, however, that this η is deleted in the lexicon and another η is
again inserted by a post-lexical rule under this
conclusion
proper
neveld (1981) and by proposing an analysis of the Trommelen
circumstances.
I
reached
by extending the analysis of n-deletion by Trommelen and Zonphenomenon
of
n-insertion.
and Zonneveld's rule was changed in such a way as to apply at sylla-
ble-edge instead of word-edge and it was, furthermore, assumed to be a rule.
With
this
lexical
reformulation, I explained why the η in the clitic-syllables
ans, man and zan can be deleted as well. These cases also provide an example of a lexical rule applied to clitics themselves, i.e. those which are not yet part of syntactic and post-lexical prosodie structure. Furthermore, ambisyllabicity
the
notion
of
of consonants before schwa in the phonological word, was shown
to be of crucial importance.
The
process
of
n-insertion
was
shown
to
be
post-lexical. I argued that the domain of this post-lexical process is I (Intonational Phrase) with no further category information rule
could
apply
both
word-internally
and
required,
externally.
schwa-final and vowel-initial clitics function as part of n-insertion.
The
the
so In
that
the
particular,
environment
of
two adjunction sides for these clitics appear to provide the
input required for this rule.
- 95 7. Conclusion
In this chapter, I argued for two
positions
for
Dutch
clitics
in
prosodie
structure, based on the proposals for phonological clitic-adjunction in Chapter 2. The argumentation was initially built on the phenomenon of of
Final
Devoicing
obstruents. Vowel-initial clitics trigger variation with respect to FD when
preceded by an underlyingly voiced obstruent: this obstruent is
either
voice-
less at phonetic representation, or voiced. An analysis was given in which clitics are either dominated by the phrase.
phonological
word
or
by
the
phonological
In the former case, a process of resyllabification takes place, bleed-
ing FD, while in the latter FD can apply normally. Thus the variation in application of FD has been explained in terms of different prosodie structures. This analysis of FD was extended to other voicing phenomena, i.e. voicing tion
and
clitic-t-voicing.
After
this,
two
other
assimila-
relevant phenomena with
respect to cliticization were accounted for. First, casual speech schwa reduction in at, ak, and as, was shown to be limited to the situations where these clitics are dominated by 0. Furthermore, the need for ambisyllabic obstruents between words within 0 was shown to be sary
in
neces-
casual speech, explaining why Clitic Schwa Reduction (CSR) is blocked
when the clitic is preceded by an obstruent-final
non-lexical
item.
Further-
more, it was argued that the consequence of schwa reduction is not a subsequent resyllabification, but that the clitic-syllable remains unaffected with a dummy vowel
as nucleus. This explains why we cannot enforce ambiguities between sen-
tences with a clitic in which CSR has been applied and sentences without them. Second, apparent counter-examples in clitic constructions to n-deletion have been
explained by showing that n-deletion is a lexical rule, whose application
is undone by a post-lexical rule inserting η in approximately the same environment
where
n-deletion had deleted n-s. With respect to cliticization, two as-
pects of this analysis are relevant. The first aspect concerns the of
lexical
rules
to clitics proper, i.e. clitics which have not yet been in-
serted into syntactic structure. n-deletion
As
has
been
shown,
the
may
lexical
rule
of
deletes η in ans, man and zan. Furthermore, the prosodie structures
assumed for Dutch clitics provide the input for n-insertion: tics
application
occur
environment.
schwa-final
cli-
as left-hand environment and vowel-initial ones as right-hand
- 96 -
Thus, the prosodie structure for Dutch cliticization
is
well-motivated
by
the analyses of several totally different and unrelated processes. This is evidence that we are on the right track in our proposal of the parameters for prosodie
cliticization. Further consequences of this proposal will be examined in
the next chapter.
Notes to Chapter 3
1.
It may be the case that the full forms het and een are just spelling pronunciations (see Van Haeringen (1937)). With respect to the articles, this cannot be tested against the diagnostic tests of chapter 2. However, the pronoun het has some syntactic clitic characteristics. For example, object pronoun het cannot be topicalized, while a corresponding NP can. This is exemplified in (i).
(i)a b c d
Hij gaf het aan de jongen (he gave it to the boy) *Het gaf hij aan de jongen (It gave he to the boy) Hij gaf het boek aan de jongen (he gave the book to the boy) Het boek gaf hij aan de jongen (The book he gave to the boy)
2.
Parts of this section are based on Berendsen (1983b), although the analysis given here deviates crucially in some respects from the one in Berendsen (1983b).
3.
See also note 17 for some effects concerning FD.
4.
Phonetically speaking, voicing does not seem to have a binary value, but rather a gradual one. However, this does not mean that in phonology the voicing feature has to be gradual as well. Over the years, binary features in phonology have proven to be a very useful tool for classification of segments and for the description of phonological processes. As such, the binary phonological feature for voice assumed here is rather well-established. Unfortunately, the precise relation between phonological [voice] and phonetic [voice] has remained unclear, although recently some interesting remarks are made from phonological (cf. Hayes (1984b)) as well as phonetic (cf. Keating (1984)) point of view.
5.
The appearance of underlying d followed by ΘΓ or ie as t and not d considered in the next section.
6.
The impossiblity for ar to trigger clitic-t-voicing will the next section.
7.
In his unpublished M.A.-thesis Wester (1982) assumes clitic-t-voicing to apply word-internally. Furthermore, he assumes that clitics occupy a different adjunction level than suffixes. This separate clitic adjunction level is in fact nothing more than saying that clitics are clitics, and as such less insightful than the analysis presented here.
8.
Zonneveld (1983) changed his position somewhat, arguing now that, whatever the status of the obstruents may be, obstruent-clusters within words must be voiceless. This is stated by 'lexical rule' and presupposes a lexical cliticization rule. However, there is strong evidence that Dutch clitics originate in the syntactic component of grammar (cf. Berendsen (1983a)). Thus
be
will
be
considered
in
- 98 Zonneveld (1983) does not seem to be on the right track. 9. There is an exception to this generalization, i.e. in case there is a pause between the two relevant forms. However, this does not concern us here. 10. People who pronounce the past tense of staan (stand) and vinden (find) ston and von, of course, do not judge these examples as ungrammatical.
as
11. Parts of this section are based on Berendsen (to app.). 12. It seems possible in some dialects to delete schwa if it is clitic-final. However, this seems to be a different process from the one under investigation. It will not be considered here, because the facts are not quite clear to me. 13. I thank Hugo Quené for helping me in matters concerning phonetics. 14. It might be necessary to reformulate rule (76) somewhat because there are some word-internal cases in which syllables containing schwa do not have a filled onset. This is the case if the vowel a is followed by schwa, as for example in sambasn. So rule (76) may apply in ga ak whether or not ak is adjoined to ga. To prohibit word-internal application of CSR we may demand the syllable containing schwa to be directly dominated by 0. 15. Rialland (to app.) independently arrives at a comparable conclusion with respect to deletion of certain a's in French. In that language, there are constructions which are potentially ambiguous after a-deletion, but which do not turn out to be ambiguous. An example of this unexpected lack of ambiguity is provided by the pair in (i). (i)
Le bar trouvé hier (the bar found yesterday) Le basi retrouvé hier (the stocking recovered yesterday)
16. It seems to me that this process occurs more frequently if the element lowing schwa has no stress, i.e. is a clitic.
fol-
17. Notice that this move has some consequences for the formulation of Final Devoicing, which we considered in section 4. There we argued that obstruents at the end of the syllable become voiceless. However, we did not mention ambisyllabic obstruents there. Certainly these obstruents have to remain voiced if they were voiced originally. This has been established by the general condition requiring that geminates (here ambisyllabic obstruents) may not be split up (Steriade (1982)). 18. That is not to say, however, that the proponents of this rule argue in fact for its lexical status. In what follows, I give the most likely analysis, although I admit that alternative analyses are possible.
Chapter 4
Clitic Phonology in other Languages
1. Introduction
This study has as its main theme the development of a prosodie theory ticization
which
of
cli-
accounts for the following more or less general phonological
clitic characteristics: (a) clitics depend on hosts (b) clitics are inherently unstressed (c) clitics behave schizophrenically with respect to their influence on word-internal phonological processes (d) clitics are monosyllabic Under the assumption that clitics are specified in the lexicon with the prosodie
category syllable only, clitics are inherently unstressed because only syl-
lables dominated by the prosodie categories foot and phonological word count as stressed.
As clitics originate in the syntactic component of grammar, they are
'floating syllables' in prosodie structure when entering the phonological ponent.
Since
com-
floating syllables have to be incorporated into prosodie struc-
ture both in the lexicon and post-lexically, these clitic-syllables have to incorporated
as
well.
This
may
be
achieved
in two ways: one in which the
clitic-syllable is adjoined to a neighbouring phonological word, and the in
which and
in
the
latter
it
is
more
like
a
word.
more
like
which
may
itself
result
an
The adjunction of a
clitic-syllable to a phonological word may trigger a restructuring of structure
other
the clitic is considered as a non lexical item which becomes part of
the prosodie category 0. In the former case, the clitic behaves affix,
be
prosodie
in a bleeding or feeding of phonological
processes. A further characteristic of the proposed theory is that there is ence
between
be prosodie clitics while not being syntactic ones, vice versa, or sodie
a
differ-
prosodie cliticization and syntactic cliticization. Elements can both.
Pro-
cliticization is relatively independent of syntax. Syntactic clitics may
be poly-syllabic (see Chapter 2., note 3.), but then they are not
phonological
-
100
-
clitics. As mentioned before, we are only concerned here with prosodie cliticization. In the preceding chapter, I have shown that the proposed theory tion
gives
a
correct
of
cliticiza-
explanation of phonological cliticization phenomena in
Dutch. In this chapter, I will examine the phonological behaviour of clitics in seven
different languages, each with their own phonological phenomena. In this
way, the universal status of my proposals about prosodie structure with respect to clitic phonology is tested, and, as we will shortly see, supported by a wide variety of languages and phonological aspects of cliticization. The aspects of cliticization involved in the test
can
be
roughly
divided
into three categories: 1. lexical and post-lexical processes with respect to cliticization. 2. prosodie structure and cliticization 3. phonological processes and cliticization These categories will be considered in the order given. For the first category, Cairene Arabic is investigated in Section 2. The second category is illustrated in Section 3. with phenomena encountered in Dakota and English. tegory
can
be
subdivided
The
last
ca-
into stress-phenomena (Greek, Palestinian Arabic),
'segmental' processes (Spanish
and
Palestinian
Arabic)
and
'autosegmental'
processes (Margi). These will be considered in Section 4. Each section discussing a particular language is divided
into
two
subsec-
tions. In the first subsection the relevant data are given together with a previous analysis found in the literature on which I base phonological
aspects
of
unclear. Most of the time, side-issues.
However,
I
cliticization have
a
Literature
on
phenomena
are
just
considered
as
attempted to choose for this chapter phenomena
taken from work by native speakers or have
myself.
cliticization is rather scarce, and sometimes rather
well-known
linguists,
which
therefore
relatively solid basis. In the second subsection, difficulties for the
analysis given in the preceding subsection
are
mentioned
and
a
re-analysis
along the lines of the proposals made in this study is presented. My main point will be that the denial of a separate level of phonological cliticization makes it
difficult
to understand cliticization phenomena. Under the prosodie theory
of cliticization advocated here, our understanding of cliticization will and,
what
is
grow,
more, several different clitic-facts are accounted for within a
general theory and in a uniform fashion.
- 101 -
2. Cliticization, lexical and post-lexical processes
2.1. Cairene Arabic
2.1.1. Hollow Verb Roots
A well-known phenomenon in Arabic languages is the appearance of so-called hollow verb roots, in which the exact perfect tense form depends upon the suffixed material. Thus, Kenstowicz and Kisseberth (1979) observe that in bic,
verb
roots
with
Cairene
ferent shapes in the perfect tense: [CiC] and [CuC], respectively, before sonant-initial
Ara-
the underlying shape /CayaC/ and /CawaC/ have two dif-
suffixes,
con-
and [CaaC] before vowel-initial or zero suffixes (or
possibly no suffix at all).
(1)
sil - na saal saal - it
(we carried) (he carried) (she carried)
suf - na saaf saal - it
However, if an object clitic, whether vowel-initial attached
to
(we saw) (he saw) (she saw)
or
consonant-initial,
is
a hollow root, the root is always formed as if a vowel-initial or
zero suffix were attached.
(2)
saal - na saal - u saal - ha
(he carried us) (he carried him) (he carried her)
saaf - na saaf - u saaf - h a
(he saw us) (he saw him) (he saw her)
Of course, this difference in behaviour can be traced back to the difference in status of subject and object pronouns. Subject pronouns in Arabic are suffixes, while object pronouns are clitics. Kenstowicz and Kisseberth (1979) account for these
facts by assuming a +-boundary for subject pronouns and a ^-boundary for
object pronouns, the latter blocking the hollow root process.
102 -
2.1.2. Hollow Verb Roots and Lexical Processes
As remarked in Chapter 1., boundaries are domains.
not
a
(1979) who use boundaries, is in need of revision. fresh
proper
instrument
to
mark
Thus, the analysis of hollow roots given by Kenstowicz and Kisseberth Let
us
therefore
take
a
look at the facts guided by both the theory of lexical phonology and the
prosodie clitic theory proposed in Chapter 2. The difference in behaviour of hollow roots before suffixes and before tics
as
if
cli-
there was a zero suffix, suggests that the allomorphy in the verb
root is a lexical matter. As cliticization is a post-lexical process, the shape of
the
attached
clitic
cannot have any influence on the allomorphy process;
thus the host root assumes a shape as if no suffix were attached. This will
be
clear immediately by comparing the derivation of silna and saalna.
(3) Suffixation (lex.) Allomorphy (lex.) Cliticization (post-lex.)
/sayal/ sayal-na sil-na
/sayal/ saal saal-na
Thus, the account of allomorphy in hollow verb roots fits naturally within proposed
the
prosodie clitic theory, assuming that allomorphy processes take place
in the lexicon.
- 103 -
3. Prosodie Structure and Cliticization
3.1. Dakota
3.1.1. Shaw's analysis
Dakota is an Indian language spoken in some parts States
of
of
Canada
the
United
America. It is a member of the Mississippi sub-family of Siouan and
it is generally considered to have four dialects. Here basic
and
clitic-facts
I
will
only
consider
of the Dakota language and will not go into dialect varia-
tions. I base myself on the facts given by Shaw (1980).
Shaw (1980) works within the linear framework set (1968)
out
by
Chomsky
and
Halle
in SPE. She distinguishes four sorts of boundaries: a morpheme-boundary
(+), a compound-boundary (%), an enclitic-boundary (=) and a word-boundary (#). The
difference
between
enclitic boundary and word boundary is of special re-
levance to us here. The postulation of these boundaries is based on morphological and phonological considerations. The most important phonological consideration for Shaw boundaries
is
the
way
to
postulate
in which main word stress is assigned: generally, the
second syllable from the lefthand edge of polysyllabic words is this
stress-assignment,
morpheme-
and
compound-boundaries
stressed.
observation.
For
are disregarded.
Monosyllabic words are stressed too, with clitics forming an exception to last
these
this
Both clitic boundary and word boundary block stress assign-
ment in Dakota. These observations about Dakota stress assignment are
exempli-
fied in (4), where the secondary stress in (4e) is assumed to be derived from a primary stress by a later rule.
(4)a b c d e
thani (to be old) ma + tháni (I am old) äha Ζ hi (wood skin = bark) SpS = Ini (cooked not = raw) # wak â^ (night holy = a holy night)
As one can see, the clitic-boundary and the
word-boundary
behave
alike
with
- 104 -
respect to stress assignment. However, stress assignment is not the only phonological process in which these boundaries behave alike. Shaw (1980, p. 41) marks
the
re-
following: 'Indeed, the morphophonemic facts of Dakota substantiate
this argument in that the two boundaries = and # function together in any rules which
make
crucial
reference to either of them.' As an example, she mentions
the rule of stem-formation in (5), which is ordered
after
stress
assignment,
and is illustrated by the examples in (6).
(5)
i -> a / C
(6)a b
# Súk = wa # # í^áp #
{# / =)
-> ->
# Súka = wa # (dog 'det') # cfápa # ( wood)
The question arises now whether clitic-boundary and word-boundary are the
in
fact
same. In Shaw's view, indirect evidence for the existence of both boundar-
ies can be obtained from what she calls which
has
syntactic
compounds,
an
example
of
already been given in (4e). Another example of a syntactic compound
is the one in (7).
(7)
# yi = kta # iyè£ h e£a # go fut. end be proper
Shaw's motivation to between
yji
postulate
(he ought to go)
a
clitic-boundary
and
not
a
word-boundary
and the clitic k t a is twofold. Her first argument runs as follows.
According to Shaw, y i k t a originates
syntactically
as
an
embedded
sentence
under the higher predicate iye2^e2a. She assumes that the postulation of a clitic-boundary expresses the syntactic relationship while
between
yi
and
kta
best,
a word-boundary between them does not. For the sake of clarity, the cor-
responding syntactic structure is given in (8).
- 105 -
(8) NP I S yi
kta
iye£ h e&i
Secondly, the postulation of a word-boundary between yjL and kta cannot the
express
fact that this sequence functions as a phonological phrase more or less in
the sense of SPE, i.e. as a string of formatives which is for
phonological
processes,
the
maximal
domain
whereas the postulation of a clitic-boundary can
express this function.
3.1.2. A new account of cliticization in Dakota
Although Shaw can account for the observed facts in the her
analysis
makes
a
rather
way
described
above,
ad hoc and unconvincing impression against the
background of present-day phonological theory. Furthermore, it suffers from the fact that she does not or cannot sharply separate phonology from morpho-syntax. Especially the postulation of a separate clitic-boundary whose behaviour is exactly
like
that of a word-boundary with respect to phonological processes, is
unconvincing. Fortunately, we now have the tools to remedy these will
show
here
that
Dakota
drawbacks.
I
clitic phonology can be handled more adequately
within the prosodie clitic theory as proposed in Chapter 2. Of course, under the proposals of Chapter 2., Dakota with
the
clitics
are
provided
prosodie category syllable only, and not foot and/or word, thus pre-
venting these clitics from being stressed. Under the assumption that in
Dakota
clitics are not adjoined to the word to their left, an assumption which
is con-
trary to the one proposed by Shaw, it can be easily explained why these clitics do
not influence phonological processes at word-edge. In this way, Dakota cli-
tics are
considered
0-structure.
In
non-lexical
Shaw's
items
and
as
such
they
become
part
terms this means that there is always a word-boundary
between a clitic and a preceding word, so that clitics cannot influence logical
processes
phono-
at word-edge. Thus, the situation described here is compar-
able with those cases in Dutch vowel-initial
of
in
which
Final
Devoicing
applies
before
a
clitic. It now immediately becomes clear why verb plus following
clitic function as a phonological phrase. Although Shaw mentions an SPE-type of phonological phrase, this type of phonological phrase seems to me comparable to
-
phonological phrases in
metrical
106
theory
-
as
proposed
by
Nespor
and
Vogel
(1982)1, Shaw (1980) does not discuss many syntactic characteristics of
Dakota,
but
enough to make it possible to construct 0-structure. On p. 10 Shaw remarks that 'Dakota has a basic SOV order 1 . Furthermore, her examples make
it
clear
that
the specifier position of NP is to the right of N. Therefore, we may tentatively conclude that Dakota is a left-recursive language, which implies that a lexical
category
with
all
non-lexical
items to its right forms a phonological
phrase. This is compatible with Shaw's remark that verb
(a
lexical
category)
and following clitic (a non-lexical category) function as a phonological phrase in Dakota. Limiting ourselves to the analysis of clear
clitic
constructions,
it
is
quite
how example (4d) has to be represented in our new account of Dakota cli-
tic phonology. In this example Spá is a lexical category and consequently
head
of 0, while Sni is a clitic syllable dominated by the same 0^·
The examples in (6) can now be accounted for as well, assuming that the rule of Stem Formation has the word edge as its righthand environment^.
Finally, the syntactic structure in (8) which was originally intended
to
evidence for a separate clitic-boundary, obtains a prosodie counterpart.
give
- 107 -
(H)
S NP
V
S yi.
kta
iyeí^eía I
r
Notice that in (11) both considerations to accept a clitic-boundary follow from the above analysis in which no clitic-boundary is postulated. The syntactic relationship between yà and the clitic kta is expressed by the
syntactic
struc-
ture, while the phonological relationship between these two elements (forming a phonological phrase) is expressed by the phonological structure. In this section, I have shown that the basic facts of Dakota clitic phonology
can be explained on the basis of the following assumptions: (a) clitics are
just syllables without any further inherent prosodie information, and (b) tic-syllables
are
dominated
cli-
by 0, because Dakota seems to make this specific
choice with respect to the clitic parameters. Furthermore, in this analysis
no
ad hoc stipulations such as a special clitic boundary are necessary.
3.2. English
3.2.1. Destressing of function words
One of the languages most thoroughly studied by generative lish.
It
is
not
linguists
is
Eng-
surprising that the phenomenon under consideration here has
been given considerable attention in the literature on English generative
pho-
nology, starting with publications by Zwicky (1970) and Selkirk (1972) and ending with recent publications by Selkirk (1984) and Kaisse (1985). However, this has
not
led to a consensus about the way the clitic facts should be accounted
for theoretically. I will mainly follow the relevant portions of the
study
by
Selkirk (1984) which is largely a reanalysis in grid-terms of parts of her doctoral dissertation (Selkirk (1972)). In particular, I will deal with the
beha-
viour and distribution of monosyllabic non-lexical items, which have one strong
-
108
-
and several weak variants, and go into the exceptional
behaviour
of
pronouns
with respect to the above mentioned distribution. With respect to monosyllabic non-lexical items the facts are as mally,
a
non-lexical
follows.
item may appear in its weak form when available if fol-
lowed by a lexical category, and if there is no 'break' between them. when
there
However,
is such a 'break', or when the non-lexical item is sentence-final,
the strong form is required. Furthermore, a strong non-lexical
Nor-
item
bears
pitch
accent
(see
form
is
required
if
the
especially Selkirk (1984) on the
latter topic). The examples in (12) show this behaviour quite clearly ('
indi-
cates weak, and ' strong).
(12)a b c d
Would you sit in/in the car Would you let the car *in/in You càn/càn do nothing (I cannot do anything) But you *càn/càn do the following
Selkirk accounts for these facts by assuming a post-lexical rule of destressing which
destresses
monosyllabic
function
words
and other syllables which are
stressed 'a little'. In case of pitch-accented function words, their stress too
heavy,
is
so they cannot be destressed; and in case the function word occurs
in front of a break, the output of destressing is excluded by a
separate
con-
vention. An exception to the picture given here is the behaviour break.
Pronouns,
although
pronouns
at
a
of the syntactic category NP, are considered to be
function words. So one expects them to remain stressed in environment.
of the
above-mentioned
However, this expectation does not always turn out to be true, as
the examples in (13) show.
(13)a b
She found him They will give it to you
Considering cases like (14), Selkirk accounts for this behaviour by postulating the syntactic encliticization rule in (15).
- 109 -
(14)a b (15)
She put his on the table He showed his mother thêm [Χ] +verb
[Ν
]
v
=>
l#2 i
1 2 Condition: 1 c-commands 2
After rule
(15)
word-internally,
has
applied,
reduces
the
the
rule
of
destressing
which
also
works
stress of the encliticized pronoun. In the next
section, I will argue against such a syntactic analysis of this encliticization and will give a prosodie analysis instead.
3.2.2. A prosodie account As noted, most of the relevant monosyllabic function next
to
a
strong
words
of
English
form, a weak form with several possible pronunciations. In
this section, I will argue, against Selkirk's destressing analysis, forms
that
weak
are not derived from strong ones, but that they are allomorphs stored in
the lexicon. In this way, it is not only possible to give a plausible of
have,
analysis
the distribution of strong and weak non-lexical items, but also of the spe-
cial behaviour of the pronouns.
The first strange thing in a destressing analysis is monosyllabic
function
words
the
their, out, like and through, too, no and on, up and but morph.
With
respect
nonapplication non-lexical
of
items
fact
that
not
all
may destress. For example, the non-lexical items have
no
weak
allo-
to examples such as out and like Selkirk argues that the
destressing composed
here
depends
on
syllable
structure:
most
of a long vowel and a consonant in their rime are
reluctant to undergo destressing. This strikes one as
reasonable,
and
it
is
probably correct from a diachronic point of view. In the other cases, her arguments are less convincing. With respect to examples such as too and no, Selkirk observes
a
contrast in frequency with the items to and do which may destress.
Too and no are less frequent than to and do and this contrast responsible
for
the
lack
should
be
held
of application of destressing in the former class.
Now, frequency may play a role in the application of phonological rules, but see
I
no reason to assume too and no to be infrequent^ and therefore this cannot
be the reason to block destressing in these cases. Finally,
examples
such
as
-
but,
110 -
on and, up are simply listed by Selkirk as exceptions to the general pro-
cess of destressing. In an analysis which does not derive the weak form strong
one,
such a rather vague notion as frequency and on processes.
from
the
corresponding
but stores them both in the lexicon, one does not have to rely on exception
devices
to
general
Some monosyllabic non-lexical items simply have a strong form and a
weak form and others have not. This state of affairs should be learned
by
any
language learning child and stored in his or her lexicon. Further evidence in favour of an analysis which puts both the strong and the weak
form
of a non-lexical item in the lexicon, is provided by Kaisse (1985).
In her analysis of auxiliary reduction^ she remarks that English has no general rule deleting w which should be required for the phonological derivation of the weak forms of will and would. Furthermore, in normal speech h is not deleted in English,
with
the
exception of the weak forms of has, have and had. Finally,
Kaisse mentions an example observed by Carden, in which
it
is
impossible
to
have a strong form, as is the case with the example in (16).
(16)
There 's/*is/*has a new book been written
These arguments to separate strong and weak forms from each other able
to
are
compar-
those given for Dutch in Chapter 3.2. Therefore, we can safely assume
that it is correct to list strong and weak non-lexical allomorphs in the
lexi-
con separately. Recall that the crucial difference between the two allomorphs is that the variant
is
a
clitic
only dominated by the prosodie category syllable, while
strong variants behave as a complete phonological word. respect
to
prosodie
weak
labeling
is
This
difference
with
also the explanation for the difference in
stressability between the two allomorphs. The next question we must consider in this analysis is there
following.
Are
any prohibitions against the insertion of variants of non-lexical items?
I think there are not, except maybe for the marginal leave
the
case
in
(16),
which
I
aside here. In the syntactic component of grammar, it is possible to in-
sert a strong variant or a clitic variant in the relevant non-lexical position. Normally
either
variant will be dominated by 0 as they are non-lexical items.
This is exemplified for the examples of (12a) and (12c) in (17).
- Ill -
0
(17)a would
you sit
in/in
the
car
0 you cán/cán do
nothing
In case there are prohibitions against the distribution of clitics, this
seems
due to the fact that a stressed form is required. Exactly this is an impossible shape for clitics to obtain. Let us examine the two constructions which are some
detail.
The
first
prohibited
for
clitics
(12b). Here, the non-lexical item in must be stressed. We can account lack
in
occurs if a non-lexical item is sentence-final as in for
the
of clitics here if (a) we assume that the grammar of English lacks a rule
like the one in Dutch which adjoins non-lexical items sentence-finally to the 0 at its left and (b) we assume instead that those elements which have phonological word status start a new 0 and occupy a strong position. As clitics are words,
they
may
not
occupy
a
strong
position
in
0.
So
sentence-final, we get a prosodie structure in which the clitic
not
if a clitic is is
not
fully
integrated into prosodie structure. This is rejected because floating syllables may not occur in prosodie structure. Thus only cases with strong forms occur in sentence-final
position.
The
prosodie structure of (12b), given in (18) will
make this clear.
(18)a
Ax A
*
V
would you let
Ψ
th'e
car
i
in
, .. A »
would you let
the
car
iA
In the same vein, no function word occupying a i.e.
starting
prosodically
strong
a new 0, can appear in its weak form. That is in fact what Sel-
kirk (1980c) and Nespor and Scoretti (1986) suggested in a metrical and
position,
framework,
Selkirk (1984) in a grid framework. On the basis of these publications, we
may assume that our analysis makes
the
correct
predictions
as
regards
the
-
112
-
prohibition of weak function words phrase-finally. With respect to function words which bear pitch accent, we may make remarks.
A
pitch accented word must be stressed, and exactly this property of
being stressed cannot be expressed in case the function word is clitics
similar
a
clitic.
As
are only syllables without foot and/or word status, they cannot appear
in a stressed form. The question of how to account for these aspects of prosody in
the
framework
adopted here will be left open for further research. For an
answer to this question in a grid-only framework see especially Selkirk (1984). I have shown here that the phonological clitic theory proposed in Chapter 2. accounts for the basic facts of the distribution of weak and strong variants of English function words in a straightforward way without the need for devices,
special
exception
phonological rules, and reliance on frequency counts. Let us
now turn to some complications.
As already indicated, an exception with respect to the above analysis is formed by
sentence-final
monosyllabic pronouns (cf. (13) and (14)). This exceptional
behaviour of pronouns was already observed by Selkirk accounts
for
this
behaviour
by
(1972).
Selkirk
(1984)
the optional syntactic encliticization rule
given in (15). However, I will argue that it is not a syntactic rule, but rather a phonosyntactic one. On this account, one can explain why only monosyllabic pronouns are attached to the phonological word to their left, whereas Selkirk's syntactic approach cannot explain this fact. A major drawback of the syntactic rule encliticizing the
pronoun,
is
that
there appears to be no truly syntactic evidence for such a restructuring operation, the only evidence being prosodie. Moreover, syntactic relations are
dis-
turbed without any convincing syntactic reason. So it is initially plausible to give a prosodie account for this encliticization within the theory proposed
in
Chapter 2. At the stage where syntactic surface structure enters the phonological ponent
of
grammar,
com-
prosodie structure has already been constructed up to the
phonological word in the lexicon. As clitics are only dominated by
the
sylla-
ble, they must still be incorporated into higher prosodie structure. One of the possibilities to do this is to attach them to phonological words. In that case, not
only
phonological
information must be available for the encliticization,
but also syntactic information. As Selkirk makes clear, only category
[+V]
are
elements
of
c-command the pronoun· Due to the feet that we need syntactic information y encliticization
of
the
suitable hosts for pronouns, and, furthermore, [+V] has to the
pronouns can only take place in the phonosyntactic part of
- 113 -
grammar where phonology and syntax still mix. This adjunction
of
pronouns
is
schematized in (19).
(19)
π
I î
->
t+V] PRO [+V] Condition: [+V] c-commands PRO
PRO
It is important to emphasize that only prosodie structure is altered: the stray syllable is adjoined to the phonological word to its left; the syntactic structure however, is left in tact. Under this analysis, the examples of
(13)
cor-
respond to the structures in (20).
This analysis of encliticization of pronouns in English follows from our theory as
proposed
in Chapter 2. It fits precisely one of the parameters provided by
universal grammar. In comparison with this analysis, the syntactic encliticization
analysis by Selkirk seems ad hoc and syntactically unmotivated. Moreover,
it is only monosyllabic pronouns that encliticize. This fact follows
automati-
- 114 -
cally
from the analysis presented here, but not from Selkirk's. So, I conclude
that the encliticization of pronouns is essentially of
a
phonological
nature
and not a syntactic one. In sum, I have shown in this section that within the
limits
of
the
prosodie
clitic theory of Chapter 2., the behaviour of English function words with their two variants can be captured in a natural way. Both variants of a function word should
be
listed
separately
in
the lexicon: the weak clitic variant is not
derived from its strong counterpart by destressing, as was proposed earlier Selkirk
(1972,
1984).
by
In our analysis, we do not need exception devices, nor
highly specific phonological rules. Neither do we have to rely on a
vague
no-
tion such as frequency, whereas in a destressing analysis we do. Furthermore, I argued against a syntactic encliticization analysis for pronouns. cliticization
en-
should be accounted for prosodically because it is essentially a
prosodie phenomenon and not a syntactic one. Again, this prosodie tion
Pronoun
encliticiza-
is within the limits of the theory proposed in Chapter 2. which allows us
to account for the fact that only monosyllabic pronouns encliticize.
4. Phonological Processes and Cliticization
4.1. Cibaeno Spanish
4.1.1. Harris' analysis of Liquid Gliding
In a study on Spanish syllable structure, Harris (1983) notes, as others before him, that conjunctive environments for phonological rules as in (21)
(21)
—
(C / #}
- 115 should be replaced by an environment making use of the notion syllable
or
its
subconstituent rime, as in (22).
(22)
—
R)
or
—
$)
Harris takes his examples from a variety of Spanish dialects, each containing a phonological
rule
which is earlier argued to have the environment as in (21).
He reanalyses the processes under consideration in such a way that the environment
of
(21)
is
replaced
by
one
of
those given in (22). In one of these
processes, clitics are relevant in a way to be made more precise shortly. is
the
process
of
Liquid
Gliding in Cibaeno Spanish, spoken in El Cibao, a
north central region of the Dominican Republic. In this dialect the and
This
liquids
1
r are optionally changed to ¿ if they are in syllable- or rime-final posi-
tion. The examples in (23) illustrate this.
(23)a
b
$
$
$
$
I I I re vol ver i li é $
$
(revolver)
$
$
M i l re voi ve res i
i
i
*i
$
$
II car ta 1¿ $
$
(letter)
$
II pa pel 1i
(paper)
$
III pa pe les i *i
Harris' rule, slightly adapted here, is that given in (24).
(24)
(1 / r) - > i /
R)
Then consider the examples in (25), where the personal pronoun el (he) is trasted with the article el (the).
(25)
el da - > ei da (he gives) el dia - > ei dia (the day)
el avisa el aviso
- > ei avisa (he advises) -> *ei aviso (the advice)
con-
-
116
-
Here, Liquid Gliding does not apply in el aviso, which is unexpected if el were a
separate
phonological word. However, the specifier el, contrary to the pro-
noun of the same shape, is not clitic-hood.
Harris
assumes
stressed, that
this
and
thus
a
likely
candidate
forms one phonological word together with the following word, and, assumption,
Harris
for
unstressed el is indeed a clitic: it under
this
gives the following derivations for two of the examples in
(25).
(26)
M el syll. str. ass.
stress, ass.
M
Δ avisa
M
M
I $
$ $
el
a vi sa
$I J$ $II $
$
e la
él avisa
vi so
elavíso
Liquid Gliding [àiavisa]
[elavíso]
4.1.2. Clitics and Liquid Gliding revisited
It seems to me that the idea behind this proposal is fully correct,
yet
there
are details which strike me as questionable. Harris considers initial syllabification and stress assignment to be lexical processes. However, as explained in Chapter
2.
of
this
study, the most coherent view of grammar with respect to
cliticization is that clitics and other items are joined in the syntactic ponent
of
com-
grammar and not in the lexicon. Thus el cannot be adjoined to aviso
in the lexicon. So the proposed interaction of these two processes and cliticization is questionable. This objection disappears under the parts
of
view
developed
here:
the
Harris' analysis can be maintained, while the questionable parts can
be strengthened in the following way. Suppose that aviso obtains structure
convincing
and
its
its
syllable
stress in the lexicon, and that clitic el is only provided
there with the prosodie category syllable. These two elements meet
in
syntax,
and during the mapping from syntactic structure to prosodie structure, the clitic-syllable is adjoined to the word to its right by the interpretation of
the
- 117 -
clitic
parameter specific to Cibaeno Spanish. Because the domain of syllabifi-
cation in Spanish is the phonological word (Harris (1983, p. 49)), this adjunction is accompanied by automatic resyllabification if the syllable structure is no longer optimal. In the case of el aviso, this results in a
syllable-initial
1, bleeding Liquid Gliding. This gives the derivation in (27).
(27)
M
$ $ : $I LI
el
a v: so
Adjoin to M $ $ $ $ ' l ' I ' el a vi so Resyll.
M
$•J$ !$ 1 $
e la vi so Liquid Gliding [elavlso]
It will be clear that viewed in this way, the process of Liquid Gliding in baeno
Ci-
Spanish, and more specifically its attitude toward clitics, confirms our
hypothesis about phonological cliticization proposed in Chapter 2.
4.2. Greek 6
4.2.1. Stressed clitics So far, our theory is partly based on the herently
unstressed.
However,
there
are
observation
that
clearly stressed clitics. As explained earlier, this does not are
clitics
are
in-
examples in several languages with mean
that
they
inherently stressed: their stress must be a derived property. In this sec-
-
118 -
tion, I will consider Greek, in which under certain circumstances stressed.
are
I will show that this fact can be explained under the present theory
provided some independently needed language-specific structure
clitics
are
added.
My
sources
are
Kaisse
limitations
(1977)
and,
in
on
metrical
particular,
Malikouti-Drachmann and Drachmann (1981). In Greek, the position of primary stress seems to be a lexical is
to
property.
That
say, one cannot predict which syllable in the word is most prominent on
the basis of some other property than that primary stress always occurs in
the
domain of the last three syllables of the word. This will be clear from the examples in (28).
(28)
Lárisa (a town) Athina (a town) Pireás (a town)
ánthropos (man) skilo (dog) mikrós (boy)
apénandi (opposite) dòlofóni (murdering) efxáristó (thanks)
If clitics are adjoined to words, the stress pattern may be influenced by
this
cliticization in a way exemplified below.
(29)a
ì athina ò mikrós (the boy) to skilo (the dog)
b
ánthropós mu (my man) fère mú to (bring it to me) apénandi mu (opposite me)
Malikouti-Drachmann and Drachmann (1981) account for these facts by postulating the following set of rules, slightly simplified here.
(30)a b c
Form a 'lexical' trochee on the lexically accented and the following syllable, Make trochees within the phonological word, right and left of the lexical trochee, Form a right-dominant word-tree.
Thus, the righthand examples of (28) are handled in the following way. The terisks under the accented syllable indicate lexical accent.
as-
- 119 -
(31)
dolofoni *
efxaristo *
apenandi φ
s w dolofoni *
efxaristo *
apenandi
(30a)
(30b)
A
A I
s w s w dolofoni *
efxaristo
(30c) w
s
Λ A
s w s w dolofoni *
The procliticizations of (29a) are handled in the same way, assuming
proclitic
and following word to be one phonological word.
(32)
i athina *
o mikros *
to skilo *
s w i athina *
o mikros *
/ \ s w to skilo *
/ \ s w s w athina *
s w I o mikros
(30a)
(30b)
(30c) w
s
w
Λ Λ
Α ι
s3 w s w i athina *
s
s ww l o mikros *
The examples of encliticization in (29b) are also assumed
/ s
/ Λ
w W Ss W to skilo *
to
be
phonological
words to which the rules in (30) must be applied. Furthermore, according to the Drachmanns, both the limitation that primary stress never falls to the left the
antepenultimate
syllable
and
the
of
wordtree labelling convention LCPR of
Liberman and Prince (1977) (in AB, Β is strong if it branches) are
responsible
-
for
the
fact
120
-
that enclitics may have primary stress. The derivations in (33)
illustrate this.
(33)
anthropos mu *
fere mu to *
apenandi mu *
anthropos mu *
s w fere mu to *
s w apenandi mu *
w s w anthropos mu
/s s w s w fere mu to *
•N s w s w apenandi mu *
Λ Λ
ΛΛ
(30a)
(30b)
(30c)
S W S W anthropos mu *
w
s
s w s w fere mu to *
w s w sw s'^V apenandi mu *
4.2.2. Some extensions Again observe that the Drachmanns assume stress to be a lexical property: assume
they
lexical accent and lexical trochees. Hence precisely the same objection
can be raised against their analysis as against Harris's above. And as in
that
case, I will show that the prosodie theory of cliticization proposed in Chapter 2. will give a better account, obviating this point. Following first the proposals of the Drachmanns, I feet
are
assume
that
in
Greek,
created from the lexically accented syllable to the right, resulting
in mono-, bi- and trisyllabic feet^ with the stressed syllable as its strongest member.
Foot
is used here as a metrical category label. Furthermore, I assume
that as many bisyllabic sw-feet as possible are constructed to the left of this foot. After that, feet and syllables are combined in a right-branching wordtree (without assuming the LCPR, apparently a language-specific choice Thus,
the
following
column in (28).
structures
are
for
Greek).
created over the words of the righthand
-
121
-
Let us the consider the input structures for cliticization in the (29).
Proclitics
are
examples
in
adjoined to the phonological word to their right, some-
times resulting in a structure which seems clearly untypical of Greek, i.e. two adjacent
syllables
undominated by feet. These sequences of two weak syllables
directly dominated by the phonological word, are thus changed into a foot. This gives
us
exactly
the
prosodie
structure
required
to
stress-patterns of the examples in (28), as is illustrated by in (35).
(35)
A r
s w to skilo * Adjoin to M
w i Restructuring
w athina
Λ
s i
A w s w athina *
Κ Λw
fe
s W ι o mikros *
account the
for
the
derivations
-
122
-
Enclitics are adjoined to the word on their left. Again, some of the prosodie
structures
are
not
allowed
by
Greek
grammar:
resulting
right-peripheral
clitic-syllables not dominated by feet, and stressed syllables to the the
antepenultimate
syllable
left
of
in the phonological word. A slight extension of
the feet-construction rules for Greek will give exactly
the
output
required.
Let
us assume that bisyllabic feet are not only constructed to the left of the I foot containing the lexical stressed syllable, but also to the right of this foot,
here
with
the
additional
option
of
having
a
trisyllabic
word-edge. Under this assumption, the prosodie structures after clitics
are
automatically
foot at
adjunction
of
restructured according to these rules for prosodie
tree construction in Greek, resulting in trees which give
exactly
the
output
required.
(36) r
A
f
S ww I anthropos mu
A îî
S w fere mu to *
apenandi mu *
Adjoin to M
w W anthropos mu * Restr.
w s\i w w apenandi mu * M
r
F
A s S W S W anthropos mu *
/Âw Λ
w s w s ape * nandi
w mu
Notice that under this analysis we do not have to say anything about the possibility
for clitics to become (primary) stressed. This follows from the adjunc-
tion of clitic syllables and the foot-construction rules for Greek, also assumed to apply post-lexically to readjust prosodie structure.
which
are
- 123 -
Thus, the existence of stressed clitics does not phonological
cliticization
undermine
the
theory
of
proposed in Chapter 2. The influence of clitics on
the stress patterns of Greek words follows in fact from
the
assumption
that,
after clitic-adjunction, the foot-construction rules apply lexically as well as post-lexically and readjust foot-structure. In general, at every stage
in
the
derivation, prosodie structure can be readjusted by the rules building prosodie structure. This However,
I
picture
propose
that
is
familiar such
a
with
respect
restructuring
to
may
syllable
also
structure.
occur
above the
syllable-level, viz. at least at the foot-level, as in Greek. Languages, appear
to
choose
which
then,
level of prosodie structure may be readjusted by the
prosodie structure building rules during derivation. In this way, we may
speak
of «syllabification and refooting.
A.3. Palestinian Arabic
4.3.1. High Vowel Deletion and Epenthesis
In this section, I will show how High Vowel Deletion (HVD) Palestinian
and
Epenthesis
in
Arabic can be accounted for in the prosodie clitic theory proposed
in Chapter 2. As remarked in Section 2.1.1., subject
pronouns
in
Arabic
are
suffixes, whereas object pronouns are clitics. With respect to High Vowel Deletion, this difference between subject and object pronouns can
be
demonstrated
in the stem fihim (understand), either with a subject pronoun or an object pronoun attached to it. In the former, HVD applies, but not so in the latter,
cf.
(37).
(37)
fhim-na (we understood) fihim na (he understood us)
HVD deletes unstressed short high vowels if
followed
by
a
consonant
and
a
vowel. Brame (1974) accounted for the data in (37) within a segmental framework by assuming that stress assignment is ordered before HVD and apply
cyclically.
that
both
rules
Recently, Abu-Salim (1982) has given a metrical analysis of
the same facts. In this analysis, an sw-foot is built over the
last
three
or
- 124 -
fewer
syllables
starting with the rightmost heavy syllable or, in the absence
of a heavy syllable, starting with the
antepenultimate,
regarding
the
final
consonant of a word as extrametrical. This procedure applies cyclically, and it is assumed that a foot of an earlier cycle retains its syllables,
shape.
Over
feet
and
a ws-wordtree is constructed. HVD takes place if the vowel is domi-
nated by a weak syllable which itself is not dominated by a foot, at any
stage
at which its structural description is met. Thus, this procedure results in the following derivations.
(38)
first cycle stress ass.
[fihim-na] M
[fihim] na
I Λ M
Λ
WS w fihim-na
F
s w fihim
HVD second cycle
[fihim na]
stress ass.
M
ΓΛ
F
F
I s w fi him na HVD [fhímna]
[fihimna]
Next to HVD, Palestinian Arabic has also a rule of i-epenthesis.
According
to
Abu-Salim (1982), this insertion of i is in fact due to two separate processes: one which makes it possible for unsyllabifiable consonants to
become
syllabi-
fied (Epenthesis I), and another to break up CC-sequences in those coda's which do not meet the requirements of the sonority hierarchy (Epenthesis thesis
I
is
therefore may change the stress pattern of the word. Epenthesis level
rule
to/for
Epen-
II
is
a
low
which only creates a weak syllable and does not alter the existing
stress pattern. Epenthesis I is exemplified wrote
II).
part of the subcomponent which constructs metrical structure and
in
katabtilha