232 66 45MB
English Pages 605 Year 2003
^HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY HANDBOOK
T o my earliest teachers, R o z and Gertrude, and to my family, Michelle, Fred, Hazel, Alan, Lucy, and Penny. L . M . C. T o the cornerstones o f my life. T h a n k you J o d i , Paul, Diane, and Stephanie. D. Ε. M . T o R o b i n and Erin. T h a n k you for sharing me with my w o r k and sharing your life with me. F . L . C.
^ HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY HANDBOOK Practical Issues for the Behavioral Medicine Specialist Editors
Lee M.Cohen Texas T e c h University
Dennis E. McChargue University of Illinois, C h i c a g o
Frank L.Collins, Jr. Oklahonna S t a t e University
/®\ SAGE Publications l^W
I International Educational and Professional T h o u s a n d O a k s • L o n d o n • N e w Delhi
Publisher
Copyright © 2 0 0 3 by Sage Publications, Inc. All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced or utilized in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, or by any information storage and retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publisher. For
information: Sage Publications, Inc. 2 4 5 5 Teller Road Thousand Oaks, California 9 1 3 2 0 E-mail: [email protected] Sage Publications Ltd. 6 Bonhill Street London EC2A 4PU United Kingdom Sage Publications India Pvt. Ltd. B-42, Panchsheel Enclave Post Box 4 1 0 9 New Delhi 110 0 1 7 India
Printed in the United States of America Library
of Congress
Cataloging-in-Publication
data
The health psychology handbook : practical issues for the behavioral medicine specialist / editors, Lee M. Cohen, Dennis E . McChargue, Frank L . Collins, Jr. p. cm. Includes bibliographical references and index. I S B N 0-7619-2614-3 (cloth) 1. Clinical health psychology—Handbooks, manuals, etc. I. Cohen, Lee M. II. McChargue, Dennis Ε . III. Collins, Frank L . , Jr. R726.7.H43357 2003 6 1 3 . 0 1 '9—dc21 2003007165 03
04
05
06
Acquiring Editor: Editorial Assistant: Production Editor: Typesetter: Copy Editor: Indexer: Cover Designer:
07
9
8
7
6
5
4
Jim Brace-Thompson Karen Ehrmann Sanford Robinson C & M Digitals (P) Ltd. D. J . Peck David Luljak Michelle Lee
3
2
1
Contents
Foreword
ix
CYNTHIA D . BELAR
Part I. Practical Issues for the Behavioral Medicine Specialist Introduction to Part I 1.
1
Health Psychology Practice in Medical Settings RICHARD J . SEIME, MATTHEW M .
2.
C L A R K , AND STEPHEN P .
3
WHITESIDE
Psychological Assessment Screening in Medical Settings MICHAEL D .
3.
1
FRANZEN
Working With a Multidisciplinary Staff
28
H E L E N R . W I N E F I E L D AND A N N A C H U R - H A N S E N
4.
Motivational Enhancement Interventions and Health Behaviors 42 THAD R .
5.
LEFFINGWELL
Brief Psychotherapies and Group Treatments in General Health Care Settings 55 D E B O R A H J . W I E B E , L I N D S E Y B L O O R , AND T I M O T H Y W .
Part II. Behaviors That Compromise Overall Health Status Introduction to Part II 6.
75
Alcohol Problems: Causes, Definitions, and Treatments 79 J O E L E R B L I C H AND M I T C H EARLEYWINE
SMITH
75
17
7.
The Etiology and Treatment of Nicotine Dependence: A Biopsychosocial Perspective LEE M .
C O H E N , DENNIS E . M C C H A R G U E ,
CORTEZ-GARLAND,
8.
MONICA
E R I C H . P R E N S K Y , AND SADIE E M E R Y
Obesity and Body Image Disturbance M Y L E S S . F A I T H AND J . K E V I N
9.
101
125
THOMPSON
Physical Inactivity as a Risk Factor for Chronic Disease 146 KRISTA A . BARBOUR, T I M O T H Y T . AND PATRICIA M .
HOULE,
DUBBERT
10. Stress and Health
169
FRANK L . COLLINS, J R . , KRISTEN H .
SOROCCO,
KIMBERLY R . HAALA, BRIAN I. M I L L E R , AND W I L L I A M R .
LOVALLO
11. Management of Inappropriate Medication-Seeking Behavior S U Z Y B I R D G U L L I V E R , BARBARA A . AND A L E X A N D E R
187
WOLFSDORF,
MICHAS
12. Adherence to Medical Recommendations N I C O L E E . B E R L A N T AND SHERI D .
208
PRUITT
Part III. Behavioral Aspects of Medical Problems 225 Introduction to Part III
225
13. Diagnostic and Treatment Considerations in Chronic Pain JENNIFER L . B O O T H B Y , MELISSA C .
229
KUHAJDA,
AND B E V E R L Y E . T H O R N
14. Hypertension
252
M U S T A F A A L ' A B S I AND R I C H A R D G . H O F F M A N
15. Coronary Heart Disease: Behavioral Cardiology in Clinical Practice
279
S T E V E N M . S C H W A R T Z AND M A R K W . K E T T E R E R
16. Behavioral Management of Type 2 Diabetes AHNA L . H O F F , JANELLE L .
WAGNER,
L A R R Y L . M U L L I N S , AND J O H N M .
CHANEY
303
17. Psycho-Oncology
325
SHULAMITH K R E I T L E R
18. Sexual Dysfunctions: Etiology and Treatment
359
SHEILA G A R O S
19. Human Immunodeficiency Virus and Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndrome A N D R E W C . B L A L O C K AND P E T E R E .
CAMPOS
20. Irritable Bowel Syndrome JEFFREY M .
397
LACKNER
2 1 . Insomnia and the Sleep Disorders V A L E R I E A . W O L F E AND SHERI D .
Part IV.
383
PRUITT
Special Issues
Introduction to Part IV
425
441
441
22. Ethical Issues for Clinicians in Behavioral Medicine Settings 443 N I C O L E J . SIEGFRIED AND C H E B O N A . P O R T E R
23. Ethnocultural Issues in Behavioral Medicine
456
H E C T O R F . M Y E R S AND W E I - C H I N H W A N G
24. Women's Health Issues CSILLA T .
469
CSOBOTH
25. Issues With Geriatric Populations B A R R Y A . EDELSTEIN, ANDREA K . A D A M P . SPIRA, AND L E S L E Y P .
485
SHREVE-NEIGER,
KOVEN
26. Public Health Approaches: Finding the Interface With Health Psychology J A L I E A . T U C K E R , JOSHUA C . K L A P O W , AND C A T H Y A .
502 SIMPSON
27. Practical Research in a Medical Setting Is G o o d Medicine 514 KATHLEEN M . PALM, JACK L . M .
MUTNICK,
D A V I D O . A N T O N U C C I O , AND ELIZABETH V .
GIFFORD
28. Evaluating Outcomes in Health Care Settings J O A Q U I N B O R R E G O , J R . , AND W I L L I A M C . F O L L E T T E
525
Author Index
537
Subject Index
567
About the Editors
579
About the Contributors
581
Foreword
W
riting a foreword for The Health
Psychology
Handbook
has stimulated
me to look both backward and forward. Although research and practice in health and behavior has a long history, the year 2 0 0 3 marks the 2 5 t h
anniversary o f the formalization o f health psychology in the United States. It was in 1 9 7 8 that a number o f us obtained sufficient support to establish the Division o f
Health Psychology within the American Psychological Association. During that same year, we met in C h i c a g o to establish the Society o f Behavioral Medicine with the purpose o f bringing together scientists and practitioners from multiple disciplines to advance knowledge o f behavior and health. Shortly thereafter, the Institute o f Medicine ( 1 9 8 2 ) published Health Biobehavioral
Sciences,
and Behavior:
Frontiers
of Research
in the
a landmark study that promoted a surge o f federal funding
for health and behavior research. Research programs expanded, journals grew, and education and training flourished. Recently, within 2 0 years o f that initial report, the Institute o f Medicine ( 2 0 0 1 ) published Health Biological,
Behavioral,
and Societal
Influences.
and Behavior:
The Interplay
of
This report is also a landmark; it
not only updates research on health and behavior but also identifies effective applications o f behavioral interventions and promotes their implementation in our health care delivery system. Although there is much m o r e to do, there is little doubt that health psychology has n o w become mainstream. W h a t has often been missing from the literature in health psychology are those works that provide the kinds o f nuts-and-bolts information that facilitates the translation o f psychological science to practice. In 1 9 8 5 , while writing the first primer for practitioners in clinical health psychology (Belar & Deardorff, 1 9 9 5 , is a revised edition), my postdoctoral fellows and I focused on what I had been teaching at the University o f Florida and Kaiser Permanente Health Care Program about working in tertiary care settings. There was a dearth o f practical advice available at that time. W h e n I began my career during the early 1 9 7 0 s , I had had excellent role models in training (e.g., Doyle Gentry, J o e M a t a r a z z o ) , but there were few written resources; we learned by watching and doing. As I was reading the chapters for this current h a n d b o o k , I found myself wishing that it had been a resource available to me then; there was nothing o f the kind. E D I T O R S ' N O T E : These views are those of the author and not those of the American Psychological Association.
ix
χ
T H E HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY H A N D B O O K W h a t this h a n d b o o k does is facilitate the progression o f the learner from the classroom to the clinical setting by focusing on the translation o f science to practice using practical examples. It does so by reviewing literature on behaviors that compromise overall health status (e.g., smoking, nonadherence) and behavioral aspects o f selected medical problems (e.g., cancer, pain, coronary heart disease). T h e nature o f the health problems is described, related psychological concepts are defined, measurement issues are addressed, and relevant treatments are detailed. There are analyses o f evidence-based clinical assessments and interventions that identify what is k n o w n as well as current gaps in knowledge. However, in addition, experienced authors discuss the application o f those interventions in the real world, noting pitfalls and providing the kind o f practical advice never found in scholarly journals. F o r each problem, a case study is presented that facilitates understanding of the implementation process; the reader can learn what an experienced clinician has actually " s a i d " to a patient and the rationale behind decisions made regarding treatment. It is sure to be an invaluable resource to scientist-practitioners during the education and training process as well as to those continuing their professional development—which includes all o f us. In addition to addressing specific health problems, this h a n d b o o k contains a wealth o f information related to professional issues such as working with a multidisciplinary staff, conducting research in a medical setting, and evaluating clinical outcomes. T h e chapter on public health approaches contextualizes clinical health psychology in our health system, highlighting that it involves more than those services provided in medical settings and by traditional health care providers. T h e public health emphasis represents a major thrust in health care policy that is likely to receive increasing support during the 2 1 s t century given the importance o f population-based approaches to health, the increased awareness o f behavioral components in chronic disease, and the need for attention to issues o f diversity in our health care system. I have always believed that our students should surpass us in knowledge and skills related to our areas o f teaching, research, and practice. Looking backward, it is apparent that those beginning their careers today are at a much different starting point than when we entered the field. This is good. Looking forward, I continue to see great opportunities for the development o f health psychology, which these students have an opportunity to maximize given this different starting point. I f I have an opportunity to write a foreword to a revised Health
Psychology
Handbook
2 0 years from n o w , I anticipate that it will contain many chapters on issues such as technology, prevention, genetic counseling, and health informatics. For example, there are numerous technologies that have been developed during recent years for which we do not have long-term follow-up data to inform our clinical w o r k (e.g., assisted reproductive technologies). Other developments have presented challenges in coping and care for which we are still designing and testing appropriate behavioral interventions (e.g., genetic testing for diabetes, breast cancer) or assessing the impact o f medical interventions (e.g., prophylactic mastectomy). In other cases, interventions that look promising n o w will have been sufficiently tested through widespread randomized controlled studies (e.g., telehealth delivery o f behavioral interventions). Given the explosion in knowledge and information delivery systems,
Foreword there will also be increased focus on h o w to access and evaluate information as well as h o w to facilitate clinical decision making. But in the meantime, with this current handbook, we have a great resource to facilitate what is ready for translation from research to practice now. O u r patients c a n benefit from these services now, and we need a well-trained health care workforce to meet these needs. — C y n t h i a D . Belar, P h . D . , A.B.P.P.
REFERENCES Belar, C. D., & Deardorff, W. W. (1995). Clinical health psychology in medical settings: A practitioner's guidebook. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Institute of Medicine. (1982). Health and behavior: Frontiers of research in the biobehavioral sciences. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. behavioral, Institute of Medicine. (2001). Health and behavior: The interplay of biological, and societal influences. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
\
xi
Parti PRACTICAL ISSUES FOR THE BEHAVIORAL MEDICINE SPECIALIST Introduction
T
to Part I
he first five chapters o f this h a n d b o o k provide an overview and frame o f reference for the clinical health psychologist. T h e s e chapters attempt to build on the broad and general training c o m m o n to all professional psychology
training programs and provide an overview o f some o f the unique skills critical for becoming a behavioral medicine specialist. In Chapter 1, Seime, Clark, and Whiteside provide the reader with a broad overview o f the unique roles played by psychologists working in medical settings. R o l e
identification is critical because many o f the experiences o f psychologists in medical settings are in stark contrast t o experiences in traditional psychology clinics. T h e authors emphasize the unique contributions in the areas o f assessment, treatment, and research that psychology brings to medical settings as well as the unique experiences that c o m e from working in a system dominated by the medical model. In Chapter 2 , Franzen builds on the framework presented in Chapter 1 by providing detailed examples o f the similarities and unique aspects o f psychological assessment in medical settings as c o m p a r e d with traditional psychology clinics. Attention is given t o cognitive, intellectual, and psychiatric screenings, pointing out the unique role o f psychology in medical settings. T h e chapter concludes with
2
PRACTICAL ISSUES specific recommendations for assessment and training needed to w o r k as a clinical health psychologist in a medical setting. In Chapter 3 , Winefield and Chur-Hansen provide a detailed discussion o f the challenges and rewards o f working in a multidisciplinary setting. This is perhaps the aspect o f clinical health psychology that most differentiates it from other professional psychology settings. Medical settings are by their nature multidisciplinary, and psychologists are often not trained to function in this professional arena. T h e authors provide both a historical perspective and detailed information about the specific skills that will facilitate integration into medical settings. In Chapter 4 , Leffingwell describes the importance o f motivational enhancement interventions for use in medical settings. T h e chapter reviews the historical basis o f these procedures and provides a great deal o f detail for h o w these interventions can and should be used for helping clients to change health-related behaviors. T h e author provides specific examples o f dialogue between the therapist and the client to illustrate the conceptual and practical aspects o f these interventions. Finally, in Chapter 5 , Wiebe, Bloor, and Smith present a review o f other brief interventions and group methods used in the practice o f clinical health psychology. T h e chapter illustrates how these interventions fit within a biopsychosocial model, with an emphasis on the unique goals for intervention with health-related problems. T h e authors provide reviews o f the use of psychoeducational, cognitive-behavioral, and interpersonal approaches for reducing the risk of disease, improving disease outcomes, and improving quality o f life. As with the other chapters in this section, a major focus is the unique training necessary to serve as a clinical health psychologist.
1
CHAPTER
Health Psychology Practice in Medical Settings RICHARD J . SEIME, MATTHEW M . CLARK, AND STEPHEN P. WHITESIDE
T
here has been tremendous
growth
and they experience different challenges to
in the number o f psychologists with
their health. Previously, many died from infec-
a primary interest in practicing in
tious
diseases
such
as
tuberculosis
and
medical settings. In this chapter, we c a n n o t
influenza. T h e death rate from life-threatening
provide an exhaustive review o f the literature
infectious diseases declined during the mid-
but instead present a perspective that will
2 0 t h century due to advances in preventive
help put the subsequent chapters into a pro-
measures and medical care. While the AIDS
fessional context. T h e goal o f this chapter is
epidemic has created complex and important
to briefly review the roles o f clinical health
new challenges, most Americans will experi-
psychologists, address some issues o f practice
ence health problems related to cardiovascular
in a medical setting, and focus on specific strate-
disease, cancer, cerebrovascular disease, unin-
gies and recommendations on " h o w t o " func-
tentional injuries, and chronic
tion as a clinical health psychologist in an
pulmonary disease (Kaplan, Sallis, & Patterson,
obstructive
academic health science center. T h e authors
1 9 9 3 ) . Americans are becoming more obese,
o f this chapter represent different levels o f
are being less physically active, and are con-
training and background; therefore, they pro-
suming more dietary fat (Kottke et al., 2 0 0 0 ) .
vide the perspectives o f a psychologist in
It has been estimated that lifestyle behaviors
training (Stephen Whiteside), a health psycho-
account for more than 5 0 % of the mortality
logy researcher/clinician/educator ( M a t t h e w
from these diseases (McGinness &
Clark),
1 9 9 3 ) . Smoking, physical activity level, and
and
a
senior
clinician/educator/
Foege,
administrator (Richard Seime).
nutrition all are lifestyle factors, but so are
BACKGROUND
clinical health psychology has an opportunity
mood, social support, and personality. Thus, to contribute to the health and wellness of our The health issues facing Americans have changed
population. T w o case examples may help to
greatly over the past century. People live longer,
highlight these issues.
4
PRACTICAL ISSUES Case 1: Mr. Smith is a 55-year-old business executive who recently completed his annual physical. His father died at 58 years of age from a myocardial infarction, or heart attack, and Mr. Smith worries frequently about his health. However, he is 40 pounds overweight, has not been a consistent exerciser since college, smokes one pack of cigarettes per day, and has three alcoholic drinks after work to "unwind." He is on antihypertensive and lipid-lowering medications. His physician is recommending numerous lifestyle changes, and Mr. Smith was referred to you, a clinical health psychologist, for consultation.
UNIQUE CONTRIBUTIONS OF PSYCHOLOGISTS Increasingly over the past 2 5 years, the field of medicine has been recognizing the benefits o f the biopsychosocial model. This philosophy adds an understanding and
incorporation
of psychosocial variables to the traditional biomedical approach (Engel, 1 9 7 7 ) . As experts in measuring and altering behavior, psychologists have a unique set of skills to combine with the practice o f our medical colleagues in an effort to apply the biopsychosocial model
Case 2: Ms. Jones is a 65-year-old, recently widowed female who has coronary artery disease and had coronary artery bypass surgery. Her cardiologist referred her to a 12-week cardiac rehabilitation program, but Ms. Jones does not attend on a regular basis. When present, she appears lethargic, does not follow instructions, and reports that she is frequently feeling alone and isolated. Her cardiologist wants your assistance in evaluating her mood and providing assistance in her care.
to patient care. In an environment consisting of multiple health and mental health professions, psychology's most important contributions are its study o f complex behavior and its commitment to critical evaluation of treatment
strategies and outcomes (cf.
Schofield, 1 9 6 9 ) . As a subspecialty o f psychology, clinical health psychology applies assessment
and
learning theories to a unified view o f physical and psychological health. Through this integration, health psychology can evaluate and
These cases highlight different aspects o f
treat many areas that frequently have not been
clinical health psychology. T h e first case may
addressed by the more traditional practices
benefit from cognitive-behavioral therapy for
of clinical psychology and psychiatry. For
lifestyle changes. R e c o r d keeping, stimulus
instance, Belar and Deardorff ( 1 9 9 5 ) identi-
control, enhancing social support, stress man-
fied three areas o f consultation that clinical
goal-setting
health psychologists address more directly
strategies could be beneficial. T h e second
than do other mental health practitioners:
agement
techniques,
and/or
case portrays h o w psychiatric comorbidity
(a) treatment
may affect adherence to recommendations
self-regulation or learning theory to medical
involving psychophysiological
for health behavior changes. Depression in car-
problems, (b) predictions o f response to medi-
diac patients, for example, increases the risk o f
cal-surgical treatments, and (c) reduction o f
reoccurrence (Frasure-Smith, Lesperance, &
health risk behaviors. T h e distinct ability of
Talajic, 1 9 9 5 ) and lowers medication adher-
clinical health psychology to address these
ence (Carney, Freedland, Eisen, Rich, &c Jaffe,
types o f questions stems from
1 9 9 5 ) . Thus, assessment and treatment of
with other mental health fields in four general
differences
comorbid depression would be important for
areas: training, assessment, treatment,
the second case example.
research.
and
Health Psychology
Training
in Medical
Settings
Assessment
Professional training influences the manner
The foundation o f clinical health psychology
in which health care professionals think about
is in standardized assessment, a practice that
clinical issues and influences the tools with
continues to be a hallmark o f the discipline.
which they evaluate and subsequently treat
Consultations often incorporate psychomet-
patients. T h e training of clinical health psycho-
ric assessment, which may be one o f clinical
logists is complementary to the medical train-
health psychology's most unique contribu-
ing o f physicians due to its focus on the
tions to patient care (Belar &
Deardorff,
empirical investigation o f cognition, behavior,
1 9 9 5 ) . Use o f standardized measures, such as
emotions, and
self-report questionnaires, reduces the chance
Specifically,
interpersonal
relationships. in
o f interviewer bias in assessment and adds an
research, program evaluation, and measure-
psychologists are trained
objective piece o f information that can be
ment o f behavior, areas in which psychiatrists
used to standardize the assessment and mon-
and other mental health practitioners are not
itor the success o f the treatment interven-
as thoroughly trained (Belar &c Deardorff,
tions. N o other mental health field has this
1 9 9 5 ) . T h e focus on health behavior change
foundation and expertise in psychometrics.
and prevention in training for the clinical
Another strength o f the discipline is the
health psychology subspecialty equips practi-
type o f information collected by clinical health
tioners with a perspective that has advantages
psychologists. Some disciplines focus primar-
over both traditional clinical psychology and
ily on deriving a diagnostic label; in contrast,
psychiatry training models. Namely, the former
clinical
two disciplines are often viewed as being overly
conduct a functional analysis. A functional
focused on psychopathology (Belar & Deardorff,
analysis o f the symptoms incorporates the
1 9 9 5 ) , thereby limiting services primarily to
antecedents and consequences o f each symp-
individuals with diagnosable mental disorders.
tom or behavior. T h e singular use o f psychi-
This focus on psychopathology
excludes
atric diagnoses has a number o f pitfalls and
patients without psychiatric disorders who,
liabilities. First, D S M - I V diagnoses (American
nonetheless, are exhibiting maladaptive cogni-
Psychiatric Association, 1 9 9 4 ) are not etiology
health
psychologists
frequently
tive or behavioral patterns that affect their
based but rather descriptive in nature. Thus,
health status and quality o f life. Training in
psychiatric diagnoses
clinical health psychology, in contrast, incor-
address or describe the factors that may have
porates the study o f behaviors that promote
precipitated and/or maintained
good physical and emotional health, such as
symptoms and therefore do not provide suffi-
smoking
cessation, weight
do
not
adequately a
patient's
management,
cient information to determine which treat-
development o f adaptive coping mechanisms,
ment interventions are likely to be successful
and adjustment to chronic illness, in addition
for a given patient (Beutler, Wakefield, &
to traditional mental health training. Health
Williams, 1 9 9 4 ) . Second, diagnostic labels,
psychologists are thereby able to assess these
such as hypochondriasis and borderline per-
areas and intervene to promote healthy behav-
sonality disorder, can have pejorative or moral
iors with or without a D S M - I V
(Diagnostic
connotations that lead to negative effects (Van
Disorders,
Egeren & Striepe, 1 9 9 8 ) . For example, these
fourth edition) diagnosis (American Psychiatric
diagnoses can influence health care profession-
Association, 1 9 9 4 ) .
als to inaccurately attribute patients' physical
and
Statistical
Manual
of Mental
5
6
PRACTICAL ISSUES complaints to symptoms of psychopathology
only by the patient but also by caregivers, both
(Belar &
Geisser, 1 9 9 5 ) or can increase
familial and professional. This approach stands
patients' fear that their symptoms will not be
in stark contrast to a purely biological explana-
taken seriously (Van Egeren & Striepe, 1 9 9 8 ) .
tion that locates pathology primarily within the
Finally, diagnostic labeling can to mind-body
dichotomous
contribute
thinking.
For
patient. T h e contribution o f clinical health psychology lies in emphasizing the role that learn-
example, although the D S M - I V diagnosis o f
ing and reinforcement play in
maladaptive
"psychological factors affecting a medical
behaviors while also acknowledging biologically
condition" is an improvement over the previ-
based personality and psychopathology factors.
ous nomenclature o f "psychogenic pain," this new diagnosis still maintains a unidirectional causal link rather than acknowledging
the
interaction between patients' physical symp-
Treatment As in assessment, one o f the strengths of
toms and their behavior and emotional func-
psychologists in a medical center is that they
tioning (Van Egeren & Striepe, 1 9 9 8 ) .
can add a unique set o f skills and options to
A functional analysis has the strengths o f objectively quantifying
the frequency
and
the treatment plan. Although many physicians request that complicated psychophar-
intensity o f a target symptom and locating
macological management
where within the environmental context it
their psychiatric colleagues, an
occurs. This approach
that
physician will frequently employ a first-line
behavioral symptoms interact with emotional,
antidepressant or anxiolytic before requesting
acknowledges
be managed
by
attending
processes
a psychiatry consult. However, many physi-
within the patient. T h e patient is seen as not
cians understandably lack the training exper-
cognitive,
social,
and
physical
merely acting on his or her environment but
tise or comfort level to address many o f the
rather as responding and reacting to behaviors
issues routinely treated by psychologists such
from health professionals and the demands o f
as application o f motivational interviewing to
being in the hospital. Moreover, the patient's
health behavior change (Bellg, 1 9 9 8 ) . Thus,
care
consultation to a clinical health psychologist
factors, so a thorough "assessment requires
can add a novel treatment approach to a
awareness o f life circumstances and an appre-
complex and challenging medical patient.
environment
extends
beyond
health
ciation o f expectancies placed on patients by
M a n y o f the strengths of the treatments
themselves and others" (Rozensky, Sweet, &
offered by clinical health psychologists emerge
Tovian, 1 9 9 7 , p. 6 3 ) . Completing a functional
directly from their training and assessment. For
analysis to understand the environment in
example, because clinical health psychologists
which a target behavior occurs, including the
have expanded from a narrow focus on psy-
precipitating stimuli and reinforcing conse-
chopathology, they can offer treatment options
quences, logically suggests a treatment plan to
for patients w h o are not described by D S M - Γ ν
alter the expression of the symptom. Ultimately,
diagnoses (American Psychiatric Association,
providing information that clearly leads t o a
1 9 9 4 ) . Clinical health psychologists can work
treatment is the goal o f any clinical health
with psychologically well-functioning individ-
psychology consultation.
uals w h o are faced with challenging health
M o r e so than any other discipline, psycho-
problems. This is an important quality because
logy has developed a body o f knowledge
it is not necessary to suffer from psychopatho-
regarding behavioral assessment (through inter-
logy to have difficulties in the hospital setting,
view and direct observation) that can be applied
in coping with illness, or in adhering to medi-
to understanding maladaptive behaviors not
cal recommendations. T o illustrate, adherence
Health Psychology
in Medical
Settings
7
rates in pediatric populations can be as low as
health psychologists can systematically study the
5%,
effectiveness o f a particular treatment within a
and thus many patients will benefit strategies
single patient or can compare separate treat-
(Dickey, Mattar, & Chudzik, 1 9 7 5 ) . Clearly, a
ments within an individual patient. This could
singular focus on psychopathology, defined in
be accomplished by obtaining
part as a deviation from "normal" functioning,
behavioral observations o f target symptoms
from
assistance with
adherence
structured
would not address a problem that occurs in
before and after interventions from health
the majority o f individuals faced with a given
care providers. Psychologists can also apply
medical situation. As a result o f this expanded
their research training to the evaluation o f
focus, clinical health psychology interventions
new treatment protocols. T o date,
with nonpsychiatric patients have facilitated
numerous other accomplishments, psychology
among
health-promoting behavior changes and can
has demonstrated the effectiveness o f psycho-
have direct effects on biological factors that
logical interventions in reducing hospitaliza-
influence the onset and progression of disease
tion rates in asthmatic and diabetic children
(Bellg, 1 9 9 8 ) .
(Christie-Seely &
An additional strength o f the interventions
Crouch,
1 9 8 7 ) and
in
managing chronic pain (Hardin, 1 9 9 8 ) .
enlisted by clinical health psychologists is that they are theory driven, with an understanding of the mechanism o f action. This is particu-
Summary
larly true for interventions based on learning
Clearly, because of the nature of their train-
and behavioral principles. T h e opportunities
ing, clinical health psychologists have a multi-
for clinical health psychologists to apply these
tude of unique skills and techniques that can
skills in medical centers are numerous, includ-
be applied to the assessment and treatment of
ing stimulus control strategies to help patients
patients in medical centers. T h e contribution
with cardiovascular disease manage
of a psychological consultation is being able to
environments, treatment
their
o f adjustment
to
assess the biopsychosocial factors that affect a
medical illness, behavior problems and adher-
patient and then provide a cogent explanation
ence in chronic health problems, cognitive
to the patient for why he or she is experiencing
distortions exacerbating symptoms o f anxiety
difficulties. This empirically based explanation
in patients with medical disease, and family
focuses on the patient as an individual with a
problems exacerbating and resulting
history of experiences interpreted through the
from
health problems faced by the patient (cf.
patient's
Camic & Knight, 1 9 9 8 ) .
processing and logically suggests potential
specific cognitive and
emotional
interventions. However, communicating and applying these skills effectively in the medical
Research T h e fourth general area o f unique contri-
center, an environment that can feel alien to the inexperienced psychologist, can be challenging.
butions by clinical health psychology to medical center consultation is the application o f research to patient care. Training in research design, implementation, and interpretation is emphasized in doctoral training in psychology to a greater degree than in medical training. These research skills can be applied at the individual patient level or at the treatment team level (Malec, 1 9 9 1 ) . For example, clinical
IMPORTANT ISSUES IN MEDICAL SETTINGS THAT AFFECT PSYCHOLOGICAL PRACTICE The Medical Model Clinical health psychologists have clinical and research training and
skills that
are
8
PRACTICAL ISSUES transportable
training
unique skills that psychologists bring to the
arena to the medical setting (cf. Belar, 1 9 8 0 ) .
from
the doctoral
medical setting. It has been our experience
However, psychologists frequently confront a
that these colleagues appreciate the clinical
setting that is steeped in the medical model.
health psychologist who is able to help both
T h e medical model is the cornerstone o f
the patient and the physician to understand
clinical practice in health science centers. T h e
problematic emotions and behavior and to
medical model assumes that a practitioner
intervene effectively.
will diagnose a problem, identify etiological factors, and ultimately correct the underlying issues that result in the overt dysfunction or problem. This is often seen initially by psychologists and psychologists in training who are unfamiliar with practice in a medical set-
Concerns
Related
to the Medical Diagnosis
Model
by Exclusion.
It is essential that
clinical health psychologists avoid making a
ting as antithetical to behavioral or psycho-
diagnosis by exclusion. Often patients
logical formulation. As Shows ( 1 9 7 6 ) pointed
referred when there are no positive physical
are
out, doctoral students often emerge from their
findings but there is dysfunction, and referring
doctoral training with negative attitudes about
colleagues may erroneously conclude that this
the medical model. Mistakenly, psychologists'
equates with the assumption that "there must
initial reaction to the medical model is that
be something psychologically wrong." In such a
physicians may be trying to find a disease
circumstance, we as psychologists must still
where none exists. In fact, historically some o f
identify positive findings to conclude
the tension between psychologists and psychi-
psychological or behavioral factors can account
that
atrists in the medical setting is around the
for a problem. Here is where our skills in func-
medicalization or pathologizing o f behavioral
tional analysis, use o f data gathering,
issues or emotional distress. At its worst, the
psychometric assessment all can play a role in
medical model can "portray the patient as sick
determining what might account for dysfunc-
and dependent and the professional as imperi-
tion in the absence o f physical findings. It is
and
alistic and heroic" (Belar &c Deardorff, 1 9 9 5 ,
important to have a working knowledge o f
p. 3 0 ) . So, psychologists must come to terms
the pathophysiology, behavioral, and psycho-
with the medical model.
logical issues c o m m o n with medical disorders medical
that we are called on to evaluate as a consul-
model, but it is a "fact o f life" in medical
tant. Keep in mind also that some diseases,
Psychologists often malign the
settings. Therefore, it is important to address
such as multiple sclerosis, have elusive or
one's attitude about the medical model so that
equivocal findings. In most cases, it is far too
one can effectively communicate, collaborate,
simpleminded to dismiss as psychological or
and intervene in the medical setting. It may be
functional a patient's presenting problems if
helpful to reframe this model as representing
no definitive physical signs or findings are
an empirical approach to diagnosis and treat-
obtained. In cases such as this, it behooves us
ment. A c o m m o n value held by both clinical
to recognize that we play an important role in
health psychologists and our medical colleagues
ensuring that the patient continues to feel that
is an emphasis on empiricism. Likewise, it is
we will encourage an ongoing consultation
important to get to k n o w colleagues and to
with the referring physician as we also work
ascertain their attitudes toward psychology
to help the patient get well. Somatizing patients
and psychologists. W i t h the increased repre-
in particular can represent a real challenge,
sentation of psychologists in medical settings,
but even patients with somatization disorders
medical colleagues have become aware o f the
have legitimate needs for ongoing medical
Health Psychology
in Medical Settings
|
evaluation and care. W h a t we have to offer
training. F r o m the time a physician graduates
these difficult patients is a different model for
from medical school, he or she is expected to
addressing their dysfunction
that looks at
be caring for patients, making decisions, being
behavioral, social, and psychological factors
on the "front line," and quickly diagnosing
as they interact with biological factors to
and treating medical issues. However, physi-
account for their difficulties and dysfunction.
cians frequently have limited confidence in their ability to counsel patients or deal with
This is the
psychological and behavioral issues due to a
converse in a sense o f diagnosis by exclusion.
lack o f training. Thus, it is important to real-
In this pitfall, a physician may have recently
ize that physicians often feel quite inadequate
diagnosed a physical or biological problem
in addressing the behavioral and emotional
Ignoring
Psychological
Factors.
after having not been able to do so for some
factors that are present in so many o f the
time. Perhaps a patient has been suffering
patients they evaluate and treat (e.g., Kroenke &
from distress or depression, engaging in mal-
Mangelsdorff, 1 9 8 9 ; Philbrick, Connelly, &
adaptive behaviors, or reinforced for sick role
Wofford, 1 9 9 6 ) .
behavior. N o w the patient has received a med-
T h e psychology trainee who is new to the
ical diagnosis, and this new "organic" finding
medical setting, or the psychologist who has
is seen as accounting for all o f his or her diffi-
little experience in the medical setting, can
culties and symptoms. This error in thinking
find the medical environment quite intimi-
can lead to poor patient management in some
dating. Besides often feeling as though his or
cases. For example, assume that an individual
her medical knowledge is lacking, the psycho-
has been having severe anxiety and panic
logist can be intimidated by the pace, the
attacks that have led to agoraphobic behav-
presumed
iors. A recent physical now reveals abnor-
psychological formulations and interventions,
mally high thyroid hormone levels. Does the
and the expectation o f "answers." T o manage
fact that this individual may be more prone to
these feelings and
anxiety as a result n o w account for the behav-
remember that although physicians have had
expectation o f certainty
assumed
about
expectations,
ioral dysfunction? T h e basic principle is sim-
a different " t r a c k " in their training compared
ply that in the process o f serial diagnosis of
with other health professionals, this does not
problems, it is not necessarily the case that a
in any way invalidate the unique knowledge,
particular physical finding accounts for all o f
clinical assessment, and intervention skills
the subsequent or preceding problems. F r o m a
that clinical health psychologists bring to the
biopsychosocial perspective, many factors are
health care arena. In terms o f training, Belar
at work simultaneously. W h a t we are best at
and Deardorff ( 1 9 9 5 ) stated that didactic
doing as psychologists is assessing what is
experiences alone are not sufficient for the
happening
practice o f health psychology. T h e y empha-
in the dimensions o f behavior,
cognition, and emotion as well as in the social
sized the importance o f appropriate
milieu of the patient that provides some avenue
models, supervisors, and mentors. This is
role
for understanding the current dysfunction and
consistent with our experience in
how to ameliorate the dysfunction.
trained and supervised practicum students,
having
predoctoral interns, and postdoctoral fellows.
Medical Background Versus Psychology Background
T h o s e trainees and psychologists w h o have had mentors working in medical settings, who have had physician collaborators and
There are obvious differences between psy-
mentors, and who have had the opportunity
chologists and physicians in the nature o f their
to train side by side with physician trainees
9
10
PRACTICAL ISSUES have an appreciation for physician knowledge
informal aspects of how best to be effective in
without being intimidated.
a consultative role. T h e informal aspects o f effectiveness include the quality o f the collaborative relations with other disciplines (Sweet
Working Within the Organizational and Political Structure of a Medical Setting
&c Rozensky, 1 9 9 1 ) and the psychologist's personal style. For example, Belar and Deardorff ( 1 9 9 5 ) suggested that the most effective clinical
T h e clinical health psychologist with a solid
health psychologists in a hospital setting are
training background in assessment, interven-
those who are "active, open, direct, assertive,
tion, and professional skills enters a challeng-
and energetic" (p. 3 3 ) and w h o have a higher
ing environment in the medical setting. It is an
tolerance for frustration. W e would add that
environment governed
the most effective psychologists are those who
by formalized
rules
(e.g., hospital bylaws, staff privileging) and
have the background, knowledge,
informal "rules," many o f which are not
clinical
familiar to psychology graduate
training,
and
interest,
interpersonal
skills
students.
needed to deal with both complex patient care
Therefore, we emphasize the importance of
issues and a complex, multilayered health care
receiving training in a medical setting from
delivery environment.
mentors who are familiar with the medical
At a minimum, the clinical health psycho-
setting. It has been our experience that physi-
logist who intends to work effectively in a
cians and other allied health professionals are
medical setting needs to understand the formal
welcoming
pitfalls
governance aspects o f psychological practice
abound. Belar and Deardorff ( 1 9 9 5 ) discussed
in a medical setting. This topic is addressed
the implicit and
hierarchy
only briefly here, and the reader is referred to
that affects a psychologist's role function in a
expanded yet succinct coverage of these issues by
medical setting. Psychologists, especially those
others (e.g., Belar & Deardorff, 1 9 9 5 ; Rozensky
o f psychologists—but explicit power
w h o are new to the field or who are still in
et al., 1 9 9 7 ) . T h e medical staff/hospital bylaws,
training, need to be especially sensitive to the
rules, and regulations govern how psycho-
role of the referring physician—the provider
logists are formally recognized in the hospital
who is in charge o f ordering consultations
setting. W h a t a psychologist is able to do pro-
and who ultimately is responsible for the
fessionally in a hospital or medical center is
patient in a hospital setting. A clinical health
governed by staff privileges (i.e., what a psy-
psychologist is invited to see a patient by the
chologist is permitted to do once granted a for-
physician w h o orders a consultation. Although
mal status with
it is important to have excellent relationships
psychologist participates in formally determin-
the hospital). Whether a
with an entire treatment team, ultimately it is
ing rules and in setting standards for practice in
the attending physicians who have the final
the hospital organization is determined by the
say as to w h o sees their patients and what is
category of staff membership (e.g., active staff,
offered to the patients. This process can be
consulting staff, courtesy staff, allied heath
confusing to the clinical health psychology
staff).
trainee or psychologist more familiar with
active staff) permits a formal voice in medical
outpatient practice, where several consultants
staff/hospital staff affairs.
Only voting staff membership
(i.e.,
may be working with the same patient. In our
A clinical health psychologist also needs to
experience, a psychologist needs to consider
be well aware o f the administrative structure
both the formal aspects (i.e., rules and regula-
of psychology in the medical setting where he
tion governing practice) o f h o w to function as
or she works. T h e psychologist needs to know
a psychologist in a consultative role and the
the organized "unit," whether it be a section
Health Psychology
in Medical Settings
\
or division within other clinical departments
psychologist serves the dual role of a consultant
or an independent department o f psychology.
and a liaison psychologist (e.g., being involved
An organized psychology unit facilitates the
with a program, regularly interacting about
individual
psychologist's professional role
psychological and behavioral adjustment of
function in the medical setting. Frank ( 1 9 9 7 )
patients,
and Seime ( 1 9 9 8 ) reviewed some of the issues
patients). As the clinical health psychologist
associated with the organizational structures
gets involved in a liaison role, he or she begins
within health science settings. Connecting
to move away from a more traditional mental
consulting
with
staff,
treating
with the administrative structure for psycho-
health practice and toward a clinical health
logy is required in most institutions. T h e medi-
psychology practice. Liaison roles provide a
cal setting provides such an enticing wealth
rich opportunity for both research and clinical
of professional opportunities that novice psy-
collaboration. T h e physician or other health
chologists can make serious mistakes, without
care professional is not directly
intending to do so, that may jeopardize their
patients "to the psychologist," but the psy-
future practice. Thus, as Rozensky ( 1 9 9 1 )
chologist becomes a regular part o f the
pointed out, it is important to both under-
program's evaluation and service delivery. A
referring
stand and master a specific hospital's "politi-
liaison role also provides an excellent oppor-
cal milieu." Fortunately, we are n o w at a stage
tunity for the psychologist to intervene with
in the development o f clinical health psychol-
the treatment team, to educate, and to affect
ogy throughout many medical settings and
program development.
academic health science centers where seasoned
Typically, consulting roles involve fee-
clinicians/mentors are readily available to
for-service. T h e psychologist interviews the
assist the junior psychologist in how to be
patient and assesses for behavioral, cognitive,
effective in medical settings.
social, and/or mental health difficulties. There may be difficulties in billing for these services, particularly for patients who do not have a
Consulting and Liaison Opportunities
mental health diagnosis under the D S M - I V criteria. However, as of January 2 0 0 2 , there are
The role o f a consultant in a medical setting
now "current procedural codes" (American
can take many forms, and serving as such is
Medical Association, 2 0 0 1 ) that include health
the most c o m m o n role for clinical health psy-
and
chologists. Typically, a consultation involves
codes for patients whose primary diagnosis is
an evaluation whereby the patient is provided
physical. This is a major breakthrough to per-
with a formulation and treatment recommen-
mit appropriate reimbursement for the delivery
dations. Often, the patient is referred
for
of psychological services to the patient with
further services that may require additional
significant physical problems requiring psycho-
medical consultation, mental health services,
logical intervention but without a D S M - I V
and/or other health care services (e.g., occupa-
mental health diagnosis.
tional or physical therapy, nutritional counsel-
behavior assessment and
intervention
Liaison roles for psychologists are satisfying,
ing). This consultation model fits well with a
but practical fiscal issues can limit the psychol-
traditional outpatient practice. There also has
ogist's time in such activities. It is important to
been a recent surge o f interest in psychologists
negotiate with a program to compensate for the
affiliating with primary care clinics or depart-
psychologist's time that does not involve direct
ments of family practice where ongoing con-
billable services. In our experience, it is not
sultation is provided to both patients and
uncommon for programs to greet the involve-
health care professionals. In this capacity, the
ment o f a clinical health psychologist in the
11
12
PRACTICAL ISSUES liaison role but to not be able to pay for the
toward assisting the psychologist in becoming
intensive time involved in such efforts. In aca-
board certified in clinical health psychology.
demic health science settings, one strategy is for
The American Board o f Professional Psycho-
psychologists to pair the ongoing liaison role
logy has recognized clinical health psychology
with a research endeavor funded by a grant that
as a specialty since 1 9 9 0 (Belar &
serves to both purchase time and fulfill research
1995).
interests and goals.
Jeffrey,
As we advance in our careers, we all will need to constantly further our knowledge and develop new skills. For a practicing psycholo-
Training Background and Skills
gist, whether early or late in his or her career, that
there are suggested principles that can guide
graduate students now have a multitude of
self-directed learning to remain competent
Belar and colleagues ( 2 0 0 1 ) noted
opportunities to obtain appropriate prepara-
and to develop new areas o f competency as a
tory
graduate
psychologist providing services to medical-
students, predoctoral interns, or postdoctoral
surgical patients. Belar and colleagues ( 2 0 0 1 )
supervised
experiences
as
fellows. Graduate school training serves as the
developed a template for self-assessment for
foundation by providing training in the biolog-
the practicing clinical health psychologist.
ical, cognitive, affective, social, and psycholog-
T h e template is presented in T a b l e 1.1.
ical bases of behavior. Such training could be integrated with graduate training, but it has been our experience that this training more typically is obtained during the predoctoral internship year or as a postdoctoral fellow.
Translating Psychology Practice and Skills Into a Medical Setting As noted previously, graduate
education
Although postdoctoral fellowship programs
may provide doctoral students with training
have existed for a number of years, only
that will facilitate their functioning as clinical
recently has there been an emphasis on having
health psychologists. These educational experi-
organized, structured, and accredited post-
ences include course work and clinical training
doctoral fellowship training. Recently the
in epidemiology, health psychology, psy-
Committee on Accreditation of the American
chological testing, behavior therapy, family
Psychological Association (ΑΡΑ) has recognized
therapy, geriatrics, and psychopharmacology.
specialty postdoctoral fellowship programs in
Unfortunately, it has been our experience that
clinical health psychology. There are a few
some needed skills are not taught and that
postdoctoral programs that have received ΑΡΑ
some psychology practices do not translate
accreditation, with others soon to be added.
well into medical centers.
Training at the predoctoral internship level in an ΑΡΑ-accredited program within a medical
There are many skills that are crit-
Pitfalls.
setting will ensure supervised experience in the
ical for the successful practice of clinical health
medical environment. T h e opportunity
psychology but that are overlooked during
to
practice under supervision, to consult regularly
graduate
with other health professionals, and to evalu-
to health settings (e.g., managing on-call
ate and treat medically referred patients is an
schedules, learning I C D - 1 0
invaluable training experience. T h e opportu-
Classification
nity to then obtain further depth and breadth
codes), whereas other skills are general prac-
training. Some skills are
of
Diseases,
unique
[International 10th
edition]
of experience and expertise is provided by
tice skills often overlooked in graduate educa-
postdoctoral fellowship training. Postdoctoral
tion (e.g., billing procedures, documentation
fellowship training will also go a long way
requirements and modalities). M a n y clinical
Health Psychology Table 1.1
in Medical
Settings
Template for Self-Assessment of Readiness for Delivery of Services to Patient With Medical-Surgical Problems
1. Do I have the knowledge of the biological bases of health and disease as related to this problem? How is this related to the biological bases of behavior? 2. Do I have knowledge of the cognitive-affective bases of health and disease as related to this problem? How is this related to the cognitive-affective bases of behavior? 3. Do I have the knowledge of the social bases of health and disease as related to this problem? How is this related to the social bases of behavior? 4. Do I have the knowledge of the developmental and individual bases of health and disease as related to this problem? How is this related to the developmental and individual bases of behavior? 5. Do I have the knowledge of the interactions among biological, affective, cognitive, social, and developmental components (e.g., psychophysiological aspects)? Do I understand the relationships between this problem and the patient and his or her environment (including family, health care system, and sociocultural environment)? 6. Do I have the knowledge and skills in the empirically supported clinical assessment methods for patients with this problem and how assessment might be affected by information in areas described by Questions 1 to 5? 7. Do I have knowledge of, and skill in implementing, the empirically supported interventions relevant to patients with this problem? Do I have knowledge of how the proposed psychological interventions affect physiological processes and vice versa? 8. Do I have knowledge of the roles and functions of other health care professionals relevant to this patient's problem? Do I have skills to communicate and collaborate with them? 9. Do I understand the sociopolitical features of the health care delivery system that can affect this problem? 10. Do I understand health policy issues relevant to this problem? 11. Am I aware of distinctive ethical issues related to practice with this problem? 12. Am I aware of the distinctive legal issues related to practice with this problem? 13. Am I aware of special professional issues associated with practice with this problem? S O U R C E : Copyright © 2001 by the American Psychological Association. Adapted with permission.
health psychology trainees are not initially
language easily understood by a number of
prepared for the unique challenges posed by
different health care professionals. In a mental
medical centers. It is not uncommon
for
health care setting, psychologists are provid-
trainees to be asked to make a diagnosis after
ing most (if not all) o f the treatment, whereas
conducting a brief bedside consultation (e.g.,
in a health care setting, psychologists are only
1 5 minutes) with a client in a medical setting,
part o f the treatment team where psychologi-
whereas mental health clinics would
cal interventions must be coordinated with
fre-
quently set aside at least 1 full hour for an
medical treatments. This transition is not
initial intake interview. Likewise, trainees are
always easy for individuals who have little or
often taught to write extensive evaluation
no experience with interdisciplinary teams.
reports primarily intended for other psycho-
In spite o f the challenges and pitfalls, it has
logists, whereas clinical health psychology
been our experience that the transition to a
reports are expected to be brief and to use
medical center is a rewarding yet challenging
14
PRACTICAL ISSUES experience that can be facilitated
through
that may differ from those of other professions.
adequate preparation. A template for effec-
It has been our experience that direct clear
tive practice is described in the next section.
communication can prevent many dilemmas and will be well received by one's medical colleagues. For example, the importance o f
Strategies for Effective Practice
limiting and preventing dual relationships can
First, the psychologist should select an area
be explained in a manner that is well received.
of high interest. T h e patient population needs to
In addition, discussions about how psycho-
be one for which the provider has a "passion."
logical test data and reports are different from
Much time, energy, and devotion will be needed
lab work, and thus have different needs for
to develop expertise, so being passionate about
confidentiality, have been well received by
one's work
our medical institutions.
is important.
T h e psychologist
should also pursue additional training through
Trainees can be wonderful bridge builders.
seminars, workshops, readings, and supervised
Having psychology trainees mentored,
clinical experience as suggested in the self-assess-
cated, or supervised by physicians has built
ment template presented in Table 1.1. Once the
many bonds for us. Similarly, mentoring, edu-
psychologist has the expertise, forming
an
cating, or supervising medical students, resi-
alliance with physicians will ensure continuity
dents, and fellows builds relationships with
of medical care and will facilitate referrals.
potential future colleagues and builds relation-
Completing comprehensive evaluations
with
ships with the medical training faculty. Our
clear recommendations will enhance satisfaction
predoctoral interns and postdoctoral fellows
from referring physicians. Ongoing communica-
have established numerous collaborative clini-
tion with health care providers is essential for
cal and research projects with our medical
edu-
patient care and will promote future referrals
staff. Collaborative research not only improves
(Jowsey, Taylor, Schneekloth, & Clark, 2 0 0 1 ) .
the quality and
Maintaining contact with applied psychologists
research but also fosters collaborative clinical
and other mental health providers will enhance
projects. Struggling through a grant applica-
the psychologist's effectiveness with his or her
tion, preparation
medical colleagues. For example, " N o , I'm not
chart review can be a wonderful team-building
able to assist your patient with couples counsel-
experience (Bock et al., 1 9 9 7 ) .
comprehensiveness
o f the
for a presentation,
or a
ing, but I can offer an excellent referral" is a more useful response than "Sorry, I don't provide couples counseling." W e do not expect physicians to treat all problems, but we do expect them to refer as needed.
T H E F U T U R E IS B R I G H T Although psychologists have been an integral
Finally, developing research projects focus-
part of medical settings for a long time, the
ing on program evaluation will assist in solid-
past quarter century has been a time of rapid
ifying a clinical practice and will allow for
growth.
documentation of effectiveness o f services. As
emerged as a result o f the new and exciting
Many
professional
societies have
noted earlier, one o f the distinctive skills that
opportunities for psychologists in medical set-
psychology brings to the medical setting is
tings. These organizations include the Society
psychologists' extensive training in research.
of Behavioral Medicine and the Association
Psychologists are in the unique position o f
for
continuing to contribute to scholarly endeav-
Education. In addition, divisions o f ΑΡΑ, such
ors as part of their professional practice.
the Behavioral Sciences and
Medical
as Division 3 8 (Health Psychology), Division
ethical
4 0 (Clinical Neuropsychology), Division 5 4
guidelines and standards o f practice to follow
(Society o f Pediatric Psychology), and Division
Psychologists have
professional
Health Psychology 12
in Medical Settings
\
(Society of Clinical Psychology), have
available online, and literature searches are
grown and developed around the interests o f
readily accessible through resources such as
those working in medical settings. There are a
PsycINFO, M E D L I N E , and E B M (Evidence-
host of ΑΡΑ-accredited internships in medical
Based
settings and a growing number of postdoc-
Systematic Reviews. Information relevant to
toral fellowship programs focusing on clinical
psychological practice n o w is at our fingertips.
Medicine)-Cochrane
Database
of
that
Psychologists will continue to play a signifi-
have clinically relevant research specifically
cant role in medical settings through the deliv-
health psychology. Likewise, journals
related to medical settings have emerged (e.g.,
ery o f clinical services, teaching, research, and
Annals
Health
health service administration. Perhaps the next
Medicine,
frontier will be the involvement of clinical
Medical
health psychologists in the genomic revolution
of
Behavioral
Medicine,
Psychology,
Journal
of Behavioral
Journal
Clinical
Psychology
of
in
T h e growth o f information techno-
as advances in the understanding and use of
logy offers incredible opportunities for psy-
genetic data affect how diseases are treated,
chologists to be informed and to practice in an
prevented,
evidence-based fashion. Journal articles are
Guttmacher, &c Collins, 2 0 0 2 ) .
Settings).
and
understood
(Patenaude,
REFERENCES American Medical Association. (2001). Physicians' current procedural terminology 2002. Chicago: Author. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Belar, C. D. (1980). Training the clinical psychology student in behavioral medicine. Professional Psychology, 11, 6 2 0 - 6 2 7 . Belar, C. D., Brown, R. Α., Hersch, L. E., Hornyak, L. M., Rozensky, R. LL, Sheridan, E. P., Brown, R. T., & Reed, G. E. (2001). Self-assessment in clinical Professional health psychology: A model for ethical expansion of practice. Psychology: Research and Practice, 32, 1 3 5 - 1 4 1 . Belar, C. D., & Deardorff, W. W. (1995). Clinical health psychology in medical guidebook (rev. ed.). Washington, DC: American settings: A practitioner's Psychological Association. Belar, C. D., Se Geisser, M . (1995). Roles of the clinical health psychologist in the management of chronic illness. In P. Nicassio &C T. Smith (Eds.), Managing perspective (pp. 3 3 - 5 8 ) . Washington, DC: chronic illness: A biopsychological American Psychological Association. Belar, C. D., &c Jeffrey, T. (1995). Board certification in health psychology. Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings, 2, 1 2 9 - 1 3 2 . Bellg, A. J . (1998). Clinical cardiac psychology. In P. M . Camic & S. J . Knight (Eds.), Clinical handbook of health psychology: A practical guide to effective interventions (pp. 5 3 - 9 8 ) . Seattle, WA: Hogrefe & Huber. Beutler, L., Wakefield, P., &c Williams, R. (1994). Use of psychological tests/instrupsychological ments for treatment planning. In M . Maruish (Ed.), The use of tests for treatment planning and outcome assessment (pp. 5 5 - 7 4 ) . Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Bock, B., Albrecht, Α., Traficante, R., Clark, M., Pinto, B., Tilkemeier, P., & Marcus, B. (1997). Predictors of exercise adherence following participation in a cardiac rehabilitation program. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 4(1), 6 0 - 7 5 . Camic, P. M., & Knight, S. J . (Eds.). (1998). Clinical handbook of health psychology: A practical guide to effective interventions. Seattle, WA: Hogrefe & Huber.
15
PRACTICAL ISSUES Carney, R., Freedland, K., Eisen, S., Rich, M., &c Jaffe, A. (1995). Major depression and medication adherence in elderly patients with coronary artery disease. Health Psychology, 14, 8 8 - 9 0 . Christie-Seely, J . , &C Crouch, M . (1987). The history of the family in medicine. In M. Crouch & L. Roberts (Eds.), The family in medical practice (pp. 1-29). Berlin: Springer-Verlag. Dickey, F., Mattar, M., &C Chudzik, G. (1975). Pharmacist counseling increases drug regimen compliance. Hospitals, 49, 8 5 - 8 6 . Engel, G. L. (1977). The need for a new medical model: A challenge for biomedicine. Science, 196, 1 2 9 - 1 3 6 . Frank, R. G. (1997). Marketing psychology at academic health science centers. Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings, 4, 4 1 - 5 0 . Frasure-Smith, N., Lesperance, F., &c Talajic, M. (1995). The impact of negative emotions on prognosis following myocardial infarction: Is it more than depression? Health Psychology, 14, 3 8 8 - 3 9 8 . Hardin, Κ. N. (1998). Chronic pain management. In P. M . Camic & S. J . Knight (Eds.), Clinical handbook of health psychology: A practical guide to effective interventions (pp. 1 2 3 - 1 6 5 ) . Seattle, WA: Hogrefe & Huber. Jowsey, S., Taylor, M., Schneekloth, T., & Clark, M . (2001). Psychosocial challenge in transplantation. Journal of Psychiatric Practice, 7, 4 0 4 ^ 1 1 4 . Kaplan, R., Sallis, J . , Jr., & Patterson, T. (1993). Health and human behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill. Kottke, T., Brekke, M., Brekke, L., Dale, L., Brandel, C , DeBoer, S., Hayes, S., Hoffman, R., Menzel, P., Nguyen, T., & Thomas, R. (2000). The CardioVision 2020 baseline community report card. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 75, 1 1 5 3 - 1 1 5 9 . Kroenke, K., & Mangelsdorff, A. D. (1989). Common symptoms in ambulatory care: Incidence, evaluation, therapy, and outcome. American Journal of Medicine, 86, 262-266. Malec, J . (1991). Research in the medical setting: Implementing the scientist-practitioner model. In J . Sweet, R. Rozensky, &c S. Tovian (Eds.), Handbook of clinical psychology in medical settings (pp. 2 6 9 - 2 8 4 ) . New York: Plenum. McGinness, J . , &C Foege, W. (1993). Actual causes of death in the United States. Journal of the American Medical Association, 270, 2 2 0 7 - 2 2 1 2 . Patenaude, A. F., Guttmacher, A. E., & Collins, F. S. (2002). Genetic testing and psychology: New roles, new responsibilities. American Psychologist, 57, 2 7 1 - 2 8 2 . Philbrick, J . T., Connelly, J . E., & Wofford, A. B. (1996). The prevalence of mental disorders in rural office practice. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 11, 9 - 1 5 . Rozensky, R. H. (1991). Psychologists, politics, and hospitals. In J . Sweet, R. Rozensky, &c S. M. Tovian (Eds.), Handbook of clinical psychology in medical settings (pp. 5 9 - 7 9 ) . New York: Plenum. Rozensky, R. H., Sweet, J . J . , & Tovian, S. M . (1997). Psychological assessment in medical settings. New York: Plenum. Schofield, W. (1969). The role of psychology in the delivery of health services. American Psychologist, 24, 5 6 5 - 5 8 4 . Seime, R. J . (1998). The section of psychology: Psychology in an academic health sciences center's department of behavioral medicine and psychiatry. Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings, 5, 2 1 5 - 2 3 2 . Shows, W. D. (1976). Problem of training interns in medical schools: A case of trying to change the leopard's spots. Professional Psychology, 7, 3 9 3 - 3 9 5 . Sweet, J . J . , & Rozensky, R. H. (1991). Professional relations. In M. Hersen, A. Kazdin, & A. Bellack (Eds.), The clinical psychology handbook (2nd ed.). Elmsford, N Y : Pergamon. Van Egeren, L., & Striepe, M . I. (1998). Assessment approaches in health psychology: Issues and practical considerations. In P. M. Camic & S. J . Knight (Eds.), Clinical handbook of health psychology: A practical guide to effective interventions (pp. 1 6 - 5 0 ) . Seattle, WA: Hogrefe & Huber.
CHAPTER
2
Psychological Assessment Screening in Medical Settings MICHAEL D . FRANZEN
T
in
psychological/psychiatric diagnosis. Because
specialty medical settings has been
psychological interventions in medical settings
c o m m o n since the late 1 9 6 0 s
and
focus more on enhancing the provision of
1 9 7 0 s . During that era, there was increasing
medical care than on in-depth psychotherapy,
he presence o f a psychologist
attention paid to the use o f psychological
assessment must be tailored to the needs o f the
principles to understand or enhance the treat-
setting. T h e psychologist might be called on to
ment of cancer patients, cardiac patients, and
develop a plan to help manage chronic pain
chronic pain patients. Recently, more atten-
from a psychological perspective or to help
tion has been given t o the potential role o f
manage anxiety related to upcoming surgery.
psychologists in general medical or family
In all o f these cases, the assessment is aimed at
practice and pediatric clinics. These health
elucidating psychological and
care providers, namely pediatricians, primary
factors that could be useful in the provision o f
practice physicians (sometime k n o w n as pri-
adequate medical care. In addition to formu-
environmental
m a r y care physicians [PCPs]), and family prac-
lating interventions, the psychologist might be
tice doctors, are the frontline care providers,
called on to assess the patient for the presence
even in those situations where it might be
o f psychological o r cognitive factors
that
necessary to involve specialists. F o r example,
would negatively affect the provision of medi-
most o f the antidepressant prescriptions in
cal care. These interfering factors may include
the United States are written
cognitive impairment, psychological distress,
by general
practitioners. In addition, even if a diagnosis requires specialized care, the first person to
or substance abuse behaviors. Screening assessment is one type o f psycho-
c o m e into c o n t a c t with the patient is most
logical assessment frequently
likely to be the person's P C P .
medical setting. T h e purpose o f screening is to
used in
the
Psychological interventions begin with a
identify important areas that may require more
psychological assessment. Psychological assess-
detailed assessment and evaluation. Therefore,
ment has various forms, from behavioral func-
the screening target is partially a function of the
tional analysis to personality assessment and
setting, the base rate of potential problem areas,
18
PRACTICAL ISSUES and the population being seen. For example, the
than is general assessment. This is true both
base rate of active psychosis in a family practice
for the administration o f procedures and for
is fairly low, and it would make little sense to
the interpretations
and
recommendations
screen for symptoms o f schizophrenia on a
made on the basis o f the assessment results.
regular basis. Alternatively, there is a fairly
In
high comorbidity between depression
and
appointment may be made for the following
certain medical conditions, and it would be
week and the typed report may be ready a
eminently sensible to screen for depression in an
week after that. In screening, whether for an
endocrinology outpatient clinic.
general
psychological assessment,
an
inpatient setting or an outpatient setting, the person must be screened in the same appointment as the identification of the problem is
PSYCHOLOGY IN THE CLINIC VERSUS PSYCHOLOGY IN THE MEDICAL SETTING
raised and the interpretation must frequently be provided to the referral source outside the consultation room. A second feature that distinguishes screening from general assessment
T h e practice o f psychology in a medical
is the length o f the procedures and the sensi-
setting has characteristics that differentiate it
tivity and completeness with which the target
from the general practice o f psychology. For
constructs are evaluated.
example, the issue o f assessment in a medical
T h e three areas o f most concern in general
setting is complicated by the other variables
medical settings are the possibilities o f neu-
in addition to environmental influences that
rologically based cognitive impairment, o f
may impinge on the patient's behavior. In
psychological or psychiatric disorders, and o f
behavioral assessment, it is assumed
that
substance abuse. Even if a specific psychiatric
the medical and physiological factors have
diagnosis is n o t appropriate, it may be help-
been ruled out. In personality or traditional
ful to screen for the presence o f anxiety or
psychological assessment, it is assumed that
depression, either o f which can significantly
the medical factors are already
affect medical outcome. T h e t w o reasons for
accounted
for. In neuropsychological assessment, it is
screening for these variables are that their
assumed that environmental variables have
presence can negatively affect the medical
been minimized. In screening and evaluating
treatment and that their presence may indi-
the medical patient in a behavioral frame-
cate the need for referral for further evalua-
work, none o f these assumptions
tion or specialized treatment.
can be
reasonably made. In fact, a more accurate assumption would be that all o f these variables are playing a role in the current clinical presentation. Therefore, it would be important for the behavioral medicine clinician to
PSYCHOMETRIC CONSIDERATIONS IN SCREENING
consider medical, cognitive, and psychological
Even though psychological screening might
features. Screening assessment seeks to iden-
not necessarily entail the full range o f psycho-
tify the possibility of an issue such as cognitive
metric complexity that comprehensive or diag-
impairment. Psychological assessment seeks to
nostic assessment might, it is still important to
identify the construct at issue and provide an
pay attention to the relevant psychometric con-
estimate o f the precise level o f that construct.
siderations. T h e several issues of validity and
There are some features o f screening that
accuracy that need to be addressed in assess-
general psychological
ment are simplified somewhat in screening.
assessment. First and most obvious, screening
T h e most relevant aspect of the psychometric
is conducted in a more restricted time frame
properties of the screening instrument is related
distinguish
it from
Psychological
Assessment
Screening
to its accuracy in identifying the presence
symptoms have been noticed or brought to the
of a pathological state and its utility in the
attention of the patient. Other content informa-
decision-making process leading to a referral
tion includes the time course of the symptoms
for comprehensive assessment. T h e construct
and whether there are any consistent changes
underlying the instrument, whether it be mem-
in the level of symptoms. A second important
ory or attention, is less important in screening
source of information comes from the clinical
than whether a score above a certain level is
observations made by the clinician. T h e clinician
indicative o f some form of cognitive problem
can note the quality o f the verbalizations of the
with an organic basis.
patient. Is the articulation understandable? Is
In choosing a screening instrument and in
the diction accurate? Is there any word-finding
setting a cutoff score, it is important to ask
difficulty, paraphasic
error, or paucity o f
what the likelihood is that a certain score would
speech? Are the station and gait normal? What
be associated with a correct decision to pursue
is the appearance of the person? Is the grooming
further evaluation. Similarly, it is important to
and hygiene adequate (inadequate hygiene may
ask what the likelihood is that a certain score
reflect either depression or cognitive impair-
would be associated with a correct decision to
ment)? These factors were addressed in greater
not pursue further evaluation. The first ques-
depth in Berg, Franzen, and Wedding (1994).
tion is an issue of sensitivity. T h e second ques-
In addition to using the observations to
tion is an issue o f specificity. Sensitivity is the
generate hypotheses regarding which areas to
extent to which the assessment
consider for screening purposes, the clinician
instrument
identifies the presence of the target construct.
can
Specificity is the extent to which positive assess-
purpose as well. Any changes in usual functions
ment findings do not occur in the absence o f the
can be a "red flag" that cognitive screening
use the historical information for that
target construct. Positive predictive power is the
might be useful. A history of recent minor auto-
accuracy with which a positive score predicts
mobile accidents might be an indication to
the target. Negative predictive power is the
screen for attention or for visual-spatial skills.
accuracy with which a negative score predicts
The clinician may want to also screen for any
the absence o f the construct. In screening, it
alcohol or substance abuse. If the patient has
may be more important in some cases that an
had to change jobs or was forced into early
instrument be sensitive even when it might not
retirement, cognitive screening may be in order.
be specific. For example, in high-risk situations
A history of multiple sex partners or o f high-
where the incorrect decision to not pursue
risk sexual behavior may also indicate the need
further evaluation could result in missing the
for screening. Occupational history also gives
presence o f a growing brain tumor, the fact that
useful information. If the patient worked in
many cases of no tumor are found with high
an industrial setting with exposure to solvents,
scores (poor specificity) is less important than
heavy metals, or insecticides, cognitive screen-
the fact that cases of tumor are found with high
ing may be indicated.
scores (good sensitivity). COGNITIVE SCREENING THE USE OF T H E INTERVIEW Cognitive screening is not as simple as it The most potent weapon in the armamentarium
may seem. Although discussion o f screening
of the clinician is the interview and history.
frequently involves only a single instrument,
There are two sources o f clinical information
there is no adequate single instrument for
derived from the interview. The first is the
screening all populations in all settings. There
content information such as whether certain
is a range o f decisions that need to be made in
19
PRACTICAL ISSUES the choice o f an appropriate instrument, and
the context of screening because a positive result
because o f the nature o f screening itself, inter-
would be followed by more extensive evaluation
pretations and conclusions are limited. O n e
in which the nature and extent of the impair-
of the first decisions is also one o f the most
ment could be delineated more extensively.
difficult ones to make. T h e clinician needs to
T o guide the psychologist in the screening
decide just which aspects o f cognition are
procedure, there is little available in terms
suspected o f being impaired. Although there
of training at the graduate level where the
is usually considerable correlation
emphasis is on either specialized assessment
different
cognitive skills, this
among
correlation
methods
(e.g., neuropsychological,
marital,
tends to be disrupted under conditions o f
career) or traditional intellectual and person-
impairment. It is quite possible for conversa-
ality methods. T h e psychologist who wishes to
tional speech to be intact while short-term
learn h o w to screen needs to seek out super-
memory is severely impaired and vice versa.
vised experience in a medical setting such as
Choosing a screening instrument that is sensi-
might be available at the internship level and
tive to memory skills when the person is
through continuing education
suspected o f having a cerebral vascular acci-
In addition, there is some published material.
dent with expressive language
impairment
will not be a useful endeavor. Broad-based
screening
requires
Two
excellent b o o k resources are A
Neuropsychology: that
a
range of impairments be considered. There are
opportunities.
Pocket
(Snyder & Nussbaum, 1 9 9 8 ) and for Brain Impairment
Clinical Handbook Screening
(Berg et al., 1 9 9 4 ) .
a few instruments that are sensitive to a broad range of types of impairment. Their sensitivity is derived from the fact that several cognitive skills are required for adequate performance. An example is the Digit Symbol subtest of the
Screening for Dementia Dementia
is
broadly
defined
as
any
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-ΙΠ (WAIS-
acquired cognitive impairment sufficient to dis-
ni). T h e Digit Symbol test requires eye-hand
rupt occupational, social, or adaptive function-
coordination, visual scanning, symbolic transla-
ing. There can be many causes and different
tion, and motor speed as well as learning skill.
manifestations. However, the most frequent
Impairment in any one o f these areas may result
cognitive difficulty evidenced by
in poor performance on the Digit Symbol test.
patients is memory impairment. Therefore, a
dementia
Other examples of such instruments include
brief memory screening procedure can be
Part Β o f the Trail Making test and
the
helpful in uncovering dementia. T h e Digit
Category test of the Halstead-Reitan Neuro-
Span procedure is relatively useless in this
psychological Battery. These instruments tend
situation and has limited utility in screening in
to be sensitive to psychiatric disturbance and
general. T h e maximum span forward in the
also have significant age-related effects. Inter-
digit procedure
pretation o f the results of such an instrument
acquired impairment until either the late stages
is problematic because poor performance can
of a progressive condition or the severe range
is relatively impervious
to
be the result o f any number o f impairments.
of an injury. Suggested procedures for memory
However, this seeming drawback is also the
screening include the Hopkins Verbal Learning
instrument's strength because when the area of
Test (Brandt, 1 9 9 1 ) , the Rey Auditory Verbal
impairment is unknown, it is useful to have a
Learning Test (Schmidt, 2 0 0 0 ) ,
single instrument that can detect impairment
Visual Spatial M e m o r y Test (Benedict, 1 9 9 7 ) ,
in any one of the suspected areas. T h e lack o f
and
specificity is not a tremendous shortcoming in
(Sivan, 1 9 9 2 ) .
the
Benton
Visual
the
Retention
Brief Test
Psychological
Folstein Mini Mental State Exam (MMSE)
Assessment
Screening
\
disorders. These authors concluded that the M M S E was less useful and less reliable when the total scores dip below 1 0 to 1 2 points.
T h e Folstein Mini M e n t a l State E x a m (MMSE)
(Folstein, Folstein, &
This is an important consideration in the
McHugh,
overall use o f the M M S E , but it might not be
1 9 7 5 ) is perhaps the most widely used (and
a substantial problem in a general medical
some say the most widely abused) instrument
setting where cognitive impairment is likely to
for the quick assessment o f cognitive status.
be more subtle and total scores o f 1 0 to 1 2
T h e M M S E is nearly ubiquitous among physi-
may be infrequent. Bidzan and Bidzan ( 2 0 0 2 )
cians, especially those working in neurological
reported on a 5-year follow-up study involv-
or psychiatric settings. T h e M M S E screens
ing 2 0 4 individuals over the age o f 5 5 years.
for different cognitive functions, frequently
Eventually,
using only one item for each construct (e.g.,
diagnosed with Alzheimer's dementia. T h e
1 9 o f these individuals
were
one item taps visual-spatial construction by
Folstein M M S E , the Cognitive subscale o f
asking the individual to copy a drawing o f two
the Alzheimer's Dementia Assessment Scale,
overlapping pentagons). T h e M M S E is heavily
the Instrumental Activity o f Daily Living
weighted toward orientation questions, with
Scale, and the Physical Maintenance Scale
1 0 of a possible 3 0 points being directed at
were found to contribute to predicting even-
orientation to time, place, and date.
tual dementia, although a comparison among
T h e M M S E has been criticized for produc-
the instruments was not conclusive. There
ing too many false negatives, but part o f the
have been reported demographic effects on
problem may exist in the use o f suboptimal
total scores o f the M M S E , making it obvious
cut points. T h e degree o f accuracy depends
that good normative information is necessary.
on the eventual diagnosis o f the individual
Jones and colleagues ( 2 0 0 2 ) presented norma-
(Harper, C h a c k o , Kotik-Harper, & Kirby,
tive data
regarding
the
performance
of
1 9 9 2 ) , something that is not usually k n o w n
community-dwelling
at the time o f the screening. T h e M M S E , the
data are very helpful in interpreting the scores
Mattis Dementia
o f older persons.
Neurobehavioral
R a t i n g Scale, and Cognitive Status
the
elderly persons. Such
Exam
were found to be roughly equivalent in discriminating patients
with
Alzheimer's or
vascular dementia from healthy elderly when
Clock Drawing Test
optimal cut points were used rather than the
T h e C l o c k Drawing Test (Freedman et al.,
cut points suggested in the literature. Because
1 9 9 4 ) is, as its name suggests, a test in which
the M M S E takes less time to administer, it
individuals are asked to draw a clock face
has an advantage over the other t w o tests. T o
with the hands set to " 1 0 after 1 1 . " After
increase the clinical utility o f the M M S E , it
the clock is drawn from c o m m a n d , a line-
would be helpful to use more extensive norms
drawing model is provided
as well as norms that are sensitive to the dif-
task. Adunsky, Flessig, Levenkrohn, Arad,
ferences associated with age and education.
and N o y ( 2 0 0 2 ) reported that the
Grigoletto ( 1 9 9 9 ) presented norms on 9 0 8
Drawing Test is roughly equivalent to the
for the same Clock
healthy Italian elderly persons. M o r e data like
M M S E in identifying impairment. However,
this are needed.
the greater variety o f items in the M M S E may
Barbarotto, Cerri, Acerbi, Molinari, and
provide for a broad-based evaluation. T h e
Capatani ( 2 0 0 0 ) reported data from a study
Clock Drawing Test has its greatest utility
of 2 7 patients with a variety o f cognitive
with the elderly.
21
22
PRACTICAL ISSUES related to medical care. Screening assessment
Other Cognitive Screening Tests T h e r e are several additional screening tests available for the psychologist. Some were designed specifically for screening, whereas others are part o f larger test procedures or shortened from the original versions. T h e Trail M a k i n g test (especially Part B) of the Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Battery is very sensitive to any cognitive impairment. It is easy to administer and takes less than 1 0 minutes to complete. T h e disadvantages are that it is not very specific and that psychiatric conditions such as depression can affect it. T h e Digit Symbol subtest o f the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised is the most sensitive o f all the Wechsler subtests. T h e corresponding research on the WAIS-III has not yet been conducted, but the modification introduced
by the WAISTII will probably
make it more specific as well as more sensitive. Somewhat longer alternatives Neurobehavioral
Cognitive
include
Status
the
Exam
(Northern California Neurobehavioral Group, 1 9 8 8 ) . This test includes subtests o f attention, memory, construction, language skills, and practical problem solving, but the meaning of the various subtest profiles is unclear, and it is best to stick with the total score as an indicator o f cognitive impairment.
is insufficient to answer these questions. H o w ever, screening for intellectual capacity may guide the clinician in deciding whether to refer the person for a m o r e complete evaluation. Although short forms of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale are available (Kulas & Axelrod, 2 0 0 2 ) , and there is even a short form available from the publisher (Axelrod, 2 0 0 2 ) , it appears that a seven-subtest short form is as abbreviated as one can go and still expect reasonable reliability (Axelrod, Ryan, & W a r d , 2 0 0 1 ) . Other options include the use of short tests and procedures that have been shown to correlate acceptably (but not optimally) with
longer, m o r e
comprehensive
intellectual exams such as the WAIS-III and the Stanford-Binet-IV. These short tests include the Slosson Intelligence Test, the Beta-Ill, and the Test o f Nonverbal Intelligence-2. T h e Slosson Intelligence Test is a short test based on the Stanford-Binet. Although short and easy to administer, it may have significant limitations in estimating I Q in the lower ranges (Kunen, Overall, & Salles, 1 9 9 6 ) . T h e Beta-Ill (Kellogg & M o r t o n , 2 0 0 1 ) is a set o f five nonverbal procedures that can be administered in less than 1 5 minutes and give a reasonably culture-fair estimate o f intelligence. T h e Test o f Nonverbal Intelligence-2 (Brown, Sherbenou, & Johnsen, 1 9 9 0 ) also provides a reasonable estimate o f culture-fair intelligence
INTELLECTUAL SCREENING
by assessing visual abstraction skills. It does not have as many different types o f tasks as
T h e utility of intelligence quotient (IQ) scores
the Beta-Ill, and it is somewhat shorter in
is largely related to academic planning and
administration time.
the suitability o f the person for certain services available from the state. In certain cases, a physician may question the intelligence level
PSYCHIATRIC SCREENING
of a patient where the documentation o f mental retardation may make the patient eligible
Affective problems are typically brought to the
for government-reimbursed support services.
attention of primary care providers (PCPs)
In other instances, there may be questions
first. This is not sufficient unless the PCPs
about the capacity of the person to follow a
can accurately recognize emotional disorders.
complicated medical regimen or about the
Wittchen
competency o f the person to make decisions
that in a study o f more than 2 0 , 0 0 0 patients,
and
colleagues ( 2 0 0 2 )
reported
Psychological
Assessment
Screening
\
PCPs were able to reasonably well identify
T h e Beck Anxiety Inventory (Beck &c Steer,
the presence o f serious psychiatric disorders
1 9 9 0 ) has 2 1 items that are endorsed on a
but were not as accurate in determining the
scale from 1 to 3. It correlates well with other
actual diagnoses. T h e role o f psychologists
instruments to measure anxiety and is fairly
here is to act as a resource to w h o m PCPs c a n
accurate in identifying D S M - I I I
turn when emotional disorder is suspected to
and Statistical
provide correct diagnoses and
third edition) (American Psychiatric Associ-
recommend
appropriate treatments.
Manual
(Diagnostic
of Mental
Disorders,
ation, 1 9 8 8 ) anxiety diagnoses. T h e Beck
Derogatis and Dellapietra ( 1 9 9 4 ) discussed
Scale for Suicide Ideation (Beck &
Steer,
screening for psychiatric disorders but did so
1 9 9 1 ) has 1 9 items that can be helpful in
largely from a perspective o f the outpatient
quickly o b t a i n i n g
psychiatric clinic. They reviewed a variety o f
the possibility o f suicide-related thoughts but
screening instruments, including the Symptom
that is not particularly accurate in predicting
Checklist-90 Revised, the brief Psychiatric
actual suicide attempts.
Rating Scale, the Center for Epidemiological
information
An even shorter assessment
regarding
instrument
Studies-Depression Scale (CES-D), the Self-
is the Center for Epidemiological Studies-
Rating Depression Scale, and the Hamilton
Depression Scale, with only 1 0 items. T h e
Anxiety and Depression scales.
C E S - D has been used in multiple settings, including Puerto R i c a n primary care patients (Robison, Gruman, Gaztambide, 8c Blank,
The Beck Scales
2 0 0 2 ) . It has generally been found to have
T h e B e c k Depression
Inventory ( B D I )
adequate sensitivity and specificity.
(Beck & Steer, 1 9 8 7 ) is one o f the most widely
Often, the decision to screen is made after
used self-report instruments for the assessment
some suspicion regarding a general probability
o f depression. Although its authors recom-
that a disorder might be present. For example,
mend the B D I as an instrument suitable for
in cases where individuals complain of cardiac
evaluating the level o f depression as well as
symptoms and describe histories of going to
sensitive to changes in level, the B D I has
emergency rooms because o f fear that myo-
great utility as a screening instrument. It is
cardial infarcts were occurring, the clinicians
brief,
are
might suspect a panic disorder, and screening
endorsed at one o f four levels. There is an
efforts would be directed at this construct
even shorter form that consists o f 1 3 items.
using an instrument such as the Autonomic
T h e r e is considerable agreement
between
Nervous System Questionnaire, a self-report
these two instruments (Reynolds &c Gould,
instrument that contains only five items but
1 9 8 1 ) , and the short form might be prefer-
has been found to have good sensitivity but
able when there are time constraints.
low specificity (Stein et al., 1 9 9 9 ) .
consisting
of
21
items
that
Beck and his associates developed other self-report instruments that have utility in a screening setting. T h e Beck
Hopelessness
Scale (Beck & Steer, 1 9 8 8 ) taps the feelings
PSYCHOLOGICAL ADAPTATION TO ILLNESS
o f negative expectations about the future and global cognitions o f despair. Although it has
Yet another feature o f psychological screening
been found to be helpful in identifying suici-
is particular to the medical setting. T h e con-
dal risk in clinical populations, at least one
struct of interest here can be broadly defined as
study has questioned the utility o f the instru-
psychological reaction to the medical condi-
ment in a m o r e general setting (Steed, 2 0 0 1 ) .
tion. For example, individuals may vary in
23
PRACTICAL ISSUES the degree to which they develop maladaptive
•
behaviors in response to the medical condition. T h e Illness Behaviour Questionnaire is an
•
Have you ever felt guilty about your alcohol use? Did you ever need a drink as an eye opener?
example o f an instrument that can evaluate these responses, defined as the inappropriate
As simple as this assessment method is, it
experience of the state o f health (Pilowsky,
is also fairly effective at identifying people
1 9 9 4 ) . Unfortunately, attempts to shorten the
w h o are likely to have been abusing alcohol
test (cf. Chaturvedi, Bhandari, Beena, &c R a o ,
(Bradley, Boyd-Wickizer, Powell, & Burman,
1 9 9 6 ) have not been successful (Bond & Clark,
1 9 9 8 ; Ewing, Bradley, &
2 0 0 2 ) . Another example o f an
Nadeau, Guyon, & Bourgault, 1 9 9 8 ) . It has
instrument
Burman, 1 9 9 8 ;
that is somewhat shorter is the Health Anxiety
all the prerequisites o f a screening procedure;
Questionnaire (Lucock &c Morley,
it is short, simple, and easy to use and score.
1996).
T h e Illness Behaviour Questionnaire has
In addition, it is n o t generally intrusive and
also been used in patients with chronic pain
is acceptable to patients. It also possesses the
(Pilowsky & Katsikitis, 1 9 9 4 ) . But there are
shortcoming c o m m o n to screening proce-
also instruments
dures, namely fairly high false positives.
that
are
more
directly
focused on pain behaviors and cognitions,
Other procedures for screening for sub-
including the Pain Anxiety Symptoms Scale,
stance abuse behaviors include the Maryland
the Fear Avoidance Beliefs Questionnaire, and
Addiction Questionnaire (O'Donnell, DeSoto,
the Fear o f Pain Questionnaire ( M c C r a c k e n ,
& DeSoto, 1 9 9 7 ) and the Michigan Alcoholism
Gross, Aikens, & Carnike, 1 9 9 6 ) .
Screening Test (MAST) (Seltzer, 1 9 7 1 ) .
SCREENING FOR SUBSTANCE ABUSE BEHAVIORS Individuals w h o abuse alcohol and/or other substances are overrepresented in psychiatric settings and in general medical settings. A large number o f health problems can arise from alcohol abuse, including disorders o f the pancreas, stomach, liver, and intestines as well as hypertension. Obviously, a psychologist working in a medical setting will be exposed to alcoholism continuously. There is a need to screen for substance abuse behaviors on a regular basis. O n e o f the simplest measures that is useful in screening for alcohol abuse is k n o w n as
"CAGE" (cut,
the
annoy, guilty, eye) ques-
tions. These questions are as follows: • •
Have you ever tried to cut down on your alcohol consumption? Does it make you annoyed when people discuss your alcohol use?
REPORT WRITING Report writing for psychological consultation in a medical setting follows the same format of the consult notes written by medical practitioners. These reports are brief, concise, and pointed toward answering the referral question. Unfortunately, sometimes the referral question is not well articulated or thought out. In those instances, it would be helpful t o have a brief conversation with the referral source to clarify the information needed. This conversation can serve to elucidate the current concerns as well as to educate the referral source as to future consultation requests. Psychologists sometimes have a tendency to "show and tell" all that went into an assessment. This is good and well when the recipient o f the report is the patient's psychotherapist or when the psychologist's assessment skills are being assessed, but in a medical setting it is the bottom line that is most important. T h a t is not to say that fine points or subtlety should be ignored. However,
Psychological
Assessment
Screening
the concise report is the report that gets read
to be quick, and the psychologist is often
and used.
called on to think on his or her feet. T h e psychologist clinician should
have
familiarity with a range o f medical disorders
Specialized Training and Skills Needed
and k n o w their basic pathophysiology, etiology, and treatment. F o r example, it would be
T h e psychologist w h o desires to w o r k in a
important for the psychologist to k n o w that
medical setting should first receive supervised
hypertension can be associated with mildly
clinical training in that setting. Any good
impaired attention and memory and
clinical psychology graduate training
pro-
some antihypertensive medications can cause
gram will provide training in psychological
side effects that mimic depression. In partic-
that
assessment, but much o f graduate training in
ular, the psychologist should be familiar with
assessment is conducted in an environment
the types o f patients and disorders seen in
very different from the typical medical set-
that clinic. Cultural sensitivity is a must. I f
ting. Graduate psychology training in assess-
the psychologist is not totally familiar with
ment typically takes place in the university
the particulars o f that clinic, he or she should
clinic, where entire days might be
spent
seek out learning experiences. T h e physician
psychological tests. Reports
is a good resource for medical information
are written over the course o f several weeks
regarding the disorders, and the nursing and
and are lengthy treatises.
support staff are good sources o f information
administering
In contrast, psychological assessment and
regarding the patients and their subculture.
screening in a general health care setting takes place at the bedside in a hospital or in an examining r o o m in the outpatient clinic o f the medical service. T h e psychologist is not on his or her home turf and must be prepared to
OPTIMAL CHARACTERISTICS OF A SCREENING PROCEDURE
conduct the assessment with whatever mate-
It would be useful to briefly reiterate the
rials have been brought to the appointment.
characteristics o f a good screening test. It
T h e report may include a dictated note that
should be accurate. It should be sensitive to
can be up to one page long, but an initial note
the construct under consideration. It should
should be on the chart or given to the referral
possess at least m o d e r a t e
agent immediately on finishing the adminis-
should have moderate
specificity. It
positive predictive
tration o f assessment instruments. For these
power and high negative predictive power. It
reasons, traditional
could be administered by paraprofessional
graduate
training
in
assessment is necessary but insufficient. There
staff or self-administered, thereby meeting
must also be training in the context o f a gen-
the final characteristic o f using a minimal
eral health care setting. Interpretation needs
a m o u n t o f professional time.
REFERENCES Adunsky, Α., Flessig, Y., Levenkrohn, S., Arad, M., & Noy, S. (2002). A comparative study of Mini-Mental test, Clock Drawing, and Cognitive FIM in evaluating functional outcome of elderly hip fracture patients. Clinical Rehabilitation, 16, 414-419.
PRACTICAL ISSUES American Psychiatric Association. (1988). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Axelrod, Β. N. (2002). Validity of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence and other very short forms of estimating intellectual functioning. Assessment, 9, 1 7 - 2 3 . Axelrod, Β. N., Ryan, J . J . , & Ward, C. L. (2001). Evaluation of seven-subtest short forms of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Ill in a referred sample. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 16, 1-8. Barbarotto, R., Cerri, M., Acerbi, Α., Molinari, S., & Capatani, E. (2000). Is the SIB or BNP better than MMSE in discriminating the cognitive performance of severely impaired elderly patients? Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 15, 2 1 - 2 9 . Depression Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1987). Manual for the Revised Beck Inventory. San Antonio, T X : Psychological Corporation. Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1988). Manual for the Beck Hopelessness Scale. San Antonio, T X : Psychological Corporation. Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1990). Beck Scale Anxiety Inventory manual. San Antonio, T X : Psychological Corporation. Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1991). Beck Scale for Suicidal Ideation: Manual. San Antonio, T X : Psychological Corporation. Benedict, R. H. B . (1997). Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Berg, R. Α., Franzen, M . D., & Wedding, D. (1994). Screening for brain impairment. New York: Springer. Bidzan, L., Si Bidzan, M . (2002). The predictive values of MMSE, ADAS-cog, IADL, and PSMS as instruments for the diagnosis of pre-clinical phase of dementia of Alzheimer's type. Archives of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, 4, 2 7 - 3 3 . Bond, M . J . , & Clark, M. S. (2002). A comparison of alternative indices of abnormal illness behavior derived from the Illness Behaviour Questionnaire. Psychology, Health, and Medicine, 7, 2 0 3 - 2 1 3 . Bradley, Κ. Α., Boyd-Wickizer, J . , Powell, S. H., & Burman, M . L. (1998). Alcohol screening questionnaires in women: A critical review. Journal of the American Medical Association, 280, 1 6 6 - 1 7 1 . Brandt, J . (1991). The Hopkins Verbal Learning Test: Development of new memory test with six alternate forms. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 5, 1 2 5 - 1 4 2 . Brown, L., Sherbenou, R. J . , & Johnsen, S. K. (1990). Test of Nonverbal lntelligence-2nd edition. Austin, T X : Pro-Ed. Chaturvedi, S. K., Bhandari, S., Beena, M . B . , & Rao, S. (1996). Screening for abnormal illness behaviour. Psychopathology, 29, 3 2 5 - 3 3 0 . Derogatis, L. R., & Dellapietra, L. (1994). Psychological tests in screening for psychiatric disorder. In M. E. Maruish (Ed.), The use of psychological testing in screening for psychiatric disorder (pp. 2 2 - 5 4 ) . Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Ewing, J . Α., Bradley, Κ. Α., & Burman, M. L. (1998). Screening for alcoholism using CAGE. Journal of the American Medical Association, 280, 1904. Folstein, M . E., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). "Mini Mental State": A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 1 8 9 - 1 9 8 . Freedman, M., Leach, L., Kaplan, E., Winocur, G., Shulman, K., Sc Delis, D. (1994). Clock drawing: A neuropsychological analysis. New York: Oxford University Press. Grigoletto, F. (1999). Norms for the Mini-Mental State Examination in a healthy population. Neurology, 53, 3 1 5 - 3 2 0 . Harper, R. G., Chacko, R. C , Kotik-Harper, D., & Kirby, Η. B . (1992). Comparison of two cognitive screening measures for efficacy in differentiating dementia from depression in a geriatric inpatient population. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 4, 1 7 9 - 1 8 4 .
Psychological
Assessment
Jones, T. G., Schinka, J . Α., Vanderploeg, R. D., Small, B. J . , Graves, A. B . , & Mortimer, J . A. (2002). 3MS normative information for the elderly. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 17, 1 7 1 - 1 7 7 . Kellogg, C. E., & Morton, N. W. (2001). Beta III manual. San Antonio, T X : Psychological Corporation. Kulas, J . F., & Axelrod, Β. N. (2002). Comparison of seven-subtest and Satz-Mogel short forms of the WAIS-III. journal of Clinical Psychology, 58, 7 7 3 - 7 8 2 . Kunen, S., Overall, S., & Salles, C. (1996). Concurrent validity study of the Slosson Intelligence-Revised in mental retardation testing. Mental Retardation, 34, 380-386. Lucock, M . P., & Morley, S. (1996). The Health Anxiety Questionnaire. British Journal of Health Psychology, 1, 1 3 7 - 1 5 0 . McCracken, L. M., Gross, R. T., Aikens, J . , & Carnike, C. L. M., Jr. (1996). The assessment of fear and anxiety in persons with chronic pain: A comparison of instruments. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34, 9 2 7 - 9 3 3 . Nadeau, L., Guyon, L., & Bourgault, C. (1998). Heavy drinkers in the general population: Comparison of two measures. Addiction Research, 6, 1 6 5 - 1 8 7 . Northern California Neurobehavioral Group. (1988). Manual for the Neurobehavioral Cognitive Status Examination. Fairfax, CA: Author. Addictions O'Donnell, W. E., DeSoto, C. B . , & DeSoto, J . L. (1997). Maryland Questionnaire. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. Pilowsky, I. (1994). Abnormal illness behaviour: A 25th anniversary review. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 28, 5 6 6 - 5 7 3 . Pilowsky, L, & Katsikitis, M. (1994). A classification of illness behaviour in pain clinic patients. Pain, 57, 9 1 - 9 4 . Reynolds, W. M., & Gould, J . W. (1981). A psychometric investigation of the standard and short forms of the Beck Depression Inventory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49, 3 0 6 - 3 0 7 . Robison, J . , Gruman, C , Gaztambide, S., & Blank, K. (2002). Screening for depression in middle-aged and older Puerto Rican primary care patients. Journal of Gerontology: Biological Sciences and Medical Science, 57, M 3 0 8 - M 3 1 4 . Schmidt, M . (2000). The Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Asssessment Resources. Seltzer, M . L. (1971). The Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test: The quest for a new diagnostic instrument. American Journal of Psychiatry, 127, \Y7-Y16. Sivan, A. B. (1992). The Benton Visual Retention Test-Revised (5th ed.). New York: Psychological Corporation. Snyder, P. J . , & Nussbaum, P. D. (1998). Clinical neuropsychology: A pocket handbook. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Steed, L. (2001). Further validity and reliability evidence for Beck Hopelessness Scale. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 61, 3 0 3 - 3 1 6 . Stein, M. B., Roy-Byrne, P. P., McQuaid, J . R., Laffaye, C , Russo, J . , McCahill, M. E., Katon, W., Craske, M., Bystritsky, Α., & Sherbourne, C. D. (1999). Development of a brief diagnostic screen for panic disorder in primary care. Psychosomatic Medicine, 61, 3 5 9 - 3 6 4 . Wittchen, H-U., Kessler, R. C , Beesdo, K., Krouse, P., Hoefler, M., & Hoyer, J . (2002). Generalized anxiety and depression in primary care: Prevalence, recognition, and management. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 63, 2 4 - 3 4 .
Screening
3
cHAPTER
Working With a Multidisciplinary Staff HELEN R . WINEFIELD AND ANNA CHUR-HANSEN
T
here are unique problems for clinical
promote interprofessional agendas (Jones &c
multidisci-
Salmon, 2 0 0 1 ) , but this is not the case in
plinary settings compared with those
Australia, even though public sector mental
health psychologists in
faced by psychologists working in more tradi-
health services are most often provided
tional settings such as mental health clinics and
multidisciplinary teams, including
private practice. These include
professional
logists (Australian Department
by
psycho-
of
Health
isolation and the difficulties o f communication
and Aged Care, 2 0 0 0 ; Herrman,
with other professionals trained with different
W a r n o c k , & Professional Liaison Committee
vocabularies and conceptual
[Australia] Project T e a m , 2 0 0 2 ) .
frameworks,
Trauer,
status conflicts, and the risks o f role ambiguity
It is somewhat ironic that many of the inter-
among the treatment team members as well
pretations and analyses o f teamwork, decision
as public confusion over roles affecting the
making, and
expectations o f patients/clients. J o n e s and
based on models from social and cognitive
Salmon ( 2 0 0 1 ) consider "multidisciplinary" as
psychology, yet psychology as a profession is
two or more professional groups with parallel
often not represented. In terms of a scientist-
interprofessional
relations are
but independent goals, whereas "interprofes-
practitioner model, it appears that the science
sional" is the preferred term for situations
of psychology is used in an interdisciplinary
where
back-
way more often than are the practitioner
grounds work together to achieve collabora-
aspects of the discipline. Because o f this dearth
professionals
from
different
tion. In this chapter, however, these two terms
of specific information, this chapter generalizes
are not strictly separated.
principles from studies involving a range of
A
number
o f authors
have
identified
health care professions. T h e traditional sociological wisdom
education at undergraduate and postgraduate levels as the linchpin for successful inter-
that
professions are self-interested groups divided
professional practice. T h e United States, the
by
United Kingdom, and Scandinavia all have in
nomies was rejected by Hudson ( 2 0 0 2 ) as
different
identities, statuses, and
auto-
place a formalized system o f policies that
pessimistic and problematic. H e argued that
Working members o f one profession may have more in
including
common with members o f a different profes-
capabilities.
With a Multidisciplinary
quality o f life and
Staff
functional
sion than with members o f their own profession and that the promotion o f professional values of trust and service to users can form the
HISTORICAL ISSUES
basis of interprofessional partnership. H e further proposed that socialization to an immedi-
T h e various health disciplines likely to be
ate work group can override professional or
working together with psychologists in health
hierarchical differences among staff, that pro-
care settings include physicians, psychiatrists,
fessionals and bureaucracies can join forces
and other medical specialists; nurses; social
in a collective effort to achieve their goals, and
workers; occupational therapists; physiothera-
that effective interprofessional working can
pists; and other allied health professionals such
lead to more effective service delivery and user
as podiatrists,
outcomes. Hudson's vision is an enticing one,
logists, and nutritionists. T h e collaboration
audiologists, speech
patho-
but to consider strategies to move toward his
between psychologists and medical practition-
ideal, we first need to consider the benefits
ers is reported more fully in the psychological
and obstacles of multidisciplinary health care.
literature than is the collaboration between
Advantages that have been identified include
psychologists and other health care profession-
continuity o f care for the patient, a wider range
als. Whatever the reason for that, the sparse
of skills and talents, greater choice for the
literature about nurse-psychologist collabora-
patients in choosing a practitioner from the
tion or social worker-psychologist collabora-
preferred
back-
tion deserves augmentation through careful
ground or sexual orientation (Balon, 1 9 9 9 ) , a
research investigations. T h e collaboration
gender,
cultural/language
more holistic approach to management, and
among the other disciplinary groups, excluding
(for the coworkers) more emotional
and
psychology, must be left for books about inte-
professional support as well as a more satis-
grated care o f those sorts (e.g., medicine with
fying work environment and ethic (Cook,
nursing,
Gerrish, &c Clarke, 2 0 0 1 ) .
Other allied health professions may have a less
social work with
physiotherapy).
W e need to specify relevant outcomes to
ambiguous relationship with medicine than
assess the success o f multidisciplinary staff
does psychology because they are more clearly
teams as opposed to other sorts o f staff
identified as providers o f auxiliary services. In
teams. T h e following seem the most impor-
contrast, psychology defines itself as a science
tant: (a) j o b satisfaction and
as well as a health care profession.
consequent
physical and psychological well-being for the
M u c h o f what follows focuses primarily
clinical health psychologists and the other
on effective collaboration between psycho-
health professionals w h o w o r k together as
logists and medical professionals, whether
well as better staff morale, retention, and
specialists (principally psychiatrists) or those
productivity (Barnes, Carpenter, &c Bailey,
providing primary care. But several authors
2000);
cost-effectiveness o f
have drawn attention to the need for psy-
service delivery such as better use o f the skills
chologists to learn to collaborate with other
(b) improved
and knowledge o f professionals with diverse
nonmedical health professions, pointing out,
backgrounds, higher quality w o r k
perfor-
for example, the key role of nurses, especially
mance, and fewer mistakes and errors in
in hospital care. It is clear that psychologists
diagnosis and
may need to develop effective models for
treatment;
and
(c) greater
satisfaction with services for the clients as
working with mental health-trained
well as improved mental and physical health,
also in primary care. There are many more
nurses
29
PRACTICAL ISSUES nurses than psychologists and will be for the
•
foreseeable future, and nurses seem likely t o have important functions to provide basic •
supportive community care. Consultation-liaison psychiatry is based on the psychosomatic idea o f health and disease arising from
an interaction o f biological,
psychological, and social factors. Lipowski ( 1 9 6 7 ) defined consultation-liaison psychiatry as the subspecialty of psychiatry concerned
• •
with clinical service, teaching, and research in nonpsychiatric health care settings. T h e paral-
•
lel development in psychology has been that o f the subspecialty o f clinical health psychology (Belar & Deardorff, 1 9 9 5 ) . As explained by
•
A concern that psychologists might not have sufficient expertise to deal with serious mental health problems Differences in training between psychiatrists and psychologists, in particular the biological bent of the former against the behavioral bent of the latter (Goldsmith, Paris, & Riba, 1999) A lack of knowledge about each other's roles and capabilities (Neal &c Calarco, 1999) Dilution of psychologists' responsibility for patients The potential for personality clashes, compounded by professional differences The complexity of coordinating and arranging teamwork (Cook et al., 2001)
Home, The clinical psychologist is an expert in the application of clinical assessment and treatment skills to change an individual's maladaptive behavior, thoughts, and emotions. Health psychology provides an expanding knowledge base regarding psychological factors in health and illness within a biological and sociological context. The clinical health psychologist draws on expertise from both clinical and health psychology to work as a practitioner in a medical/health care setting. (Strain & Home, 2 0 0 1 , p. I l l ) H o m e then outlined the key issues particularly well managed by psychologists, including (a) clinician-patient communication, tors in adherence to treatment,
(b) fac-
(c) dealing
with medical anxieties and phobias in patients and clinicians, and (d) preparing patients for invasive and surgical procedures (Strain & Home, 2001). Disadvantages o f interprofessional practice between psychologists and psychiatrists have been identified, including factors such as the following: • •
Perceptions by some members of the medical profession that shared care is unethical An unwillingness by some psychiatrists and psychologists to let go of ideological prejudices
In a report on the roles and relationships of psychiatrists and other service providers, Herrman and colleagues ( 2 0 0 2 ) identified five main obstacles to effective teamwork: (a) ambiguity or conflict over roles, with a common assumption in practice and in the literature that the psychiatrist or medical professional is the team leader who allocates roles and duties, with the other professionals viewed as "physician extenders"
(Schuster, Kern, Kane, 8 t
Nettleman, 1 9 9 4 ) ; (b) conflict and confusion over leadership, whereby the psychiatrist usually assumes leadership on the basis of superior knowledge and training; (c) differing understandings of responsibility and accountability, with psychiatrists sometimes hesitating to work in teams because they are concerned that they may be held responsible for other professionals' errors (which in fact is not the case); (d) interprofessional misperceptions related to differences in skills and training, values, culture, socialization, and cognitive style; and (e) differing rewards among professions, with power, status, and income all playing a role. Gilbert and colleagues ( 2 0 0 0 ) noted that these differences in rewards across professions may also become confounded
with gender
disparities
across professions. Nicholas and Wright ( 2 0 0 1 ) , in discussing collaborative w o r k
by psychologists
and
Working
With a Multidisciplinary
Staff
\
psychiatrists in pain clinics, commented that
undergo the necessary extra training, thereby
mostly the question o f w h o provides which
bolstering the argument for more public access
service should be decided on the grounds o f
to psychologists.
who can do it best, not a priori on the grounds
Other historical (and some continuing)
of discipline. However, as they noted, the
influences include the reimbursement sched-
biomedical background o f psychiatrists makes
ules via government and other third-party
them better able to use psychotropic medica-
payers, insurers, and managed care organiza-
tions and to assess how these may interact
tions. In Australia, the universal public health
with other medical aspects o f the patient's
insurance does not include payment for psy-
condition, whereas psychologists are usually
chologist services; it includes payment only
better able to use cognitive and behavioral
for medically qualified practitioners (includ-
interventions for individuals and groups and
ing, o f course, psychiatrists). Accordingly, pri-
have more expertise in the development and
vate practitioner psychologists are very limited
use o f psychometric measures. This situation
in what proportion o f the population can
is not static. Recently, some U.S. psychologists
afford their services. A recent governmental
have gained
psy-
policy innovation, the Better Outcomes for
chotropic drugs despite intense resistance to
Mental Health initiative, implies more exten-
this innovation by psychiatrists—and some
sive possible roles for psychologists in two
psychologists (Goode, 2 0 0 2 ) .
ways, although their involvement is not stated
the right to prescribe
the
explicitly. O n e is that primary care physicians,
expectations o f each profession regarding its
or general practitioners, will need training in
roles in patient care, and in different amounts
mental health care to qualify for
of emphasis on the scientific evidence base and
rebates for the longer consultations that this
There are historical influences on
higher
how to evaluate the scientific literature and
form o f treatment requires. T h e other is that
whether one is expected to contribute to it.
general practitioners may gain discretionary
Each profession probably has a different view
government
funding
to pay for specialist
of where it stands on various "skills pyramid"
mental health care for their patients, and if
conceptualizations. T h e Australian Psycholo-
they hire psychologists to provide this care,
gical Society ( 2 0 0 0 ) , for example, developed a
they will overcome the financial burden for
model o f the levels o f expertise in mental
patients that seeing a psychologist currently
health care that has specialized clinical psy-
imposes.
chologists at the top level, dealing
with
In
Britain, all clinical psychologists are
complex cases, innovations to treatment, and
employed by the publicly funded
evaluations o f effectiveness. At the bottom
Health Service and work alongside doctors
level are generic counselors w h o , after being
and other health professionals, again at the
trained by psychologists, offer first-line help to
discretion of the general practitioners who
National
those in need and can recognize the need to
direct the spending o f government funds for
refer upward as appropriate. T h e middle level
health care. T h e fact that psychologists are
of skill is characterized by the delivery o f inter-
salaried rather than private practitioners, o f
ventions (after some training) such as assertive
course, increases their accessibility to
therapy, couples therapy,
public enormously.
and
manualized
the
cognitive-behavioral therapy. T h e middle level
In the United States, the managed care
might be where medical practitioners with
movement has created much consternation for
appropriate
best fit,
mental health workers w h o were not accus-
although in normal circumstances many o f
extra training would
tomed to concepts o f accountability and cost-
them will not have the time or interest to
effectiveness (Todd, 1 9 9 4 ) . However, its focus
31
32
PRACTICAL ISSUES problem-focused
health care team to monitor their own practice
treatments sits well with the usual mode o f
and its outcomes in a scientific way, to keep
psychological intervention in medical settings.
useful records of the process and results o f
Conversely, psychologists' familiarity
on brief, evidence-based,
with
care, to design controlled studies of innovative
empirically supported treatments is entirely
treatments, and to communicate with others
compatible with medicine's current emphasis
through the professional literature. O n e recent
on evidence-based practice.
step in this direction has been the acknowledgment that the effort to be scientific about practice need not be abandoned if the "gold
SPECIALIZED SKILLS NEEDED FOR MULTIDISCIPLINARY WORK IN HEALTH SETTINGS
standard" multisite randomized control trial is impractical to undertake. Psychology has a history o f deriving valid and reliable conclusions from the small-sample research study
Although medical patients are likely to show
(Morgan
high rates o f anxiety, depression, and other
Chamberlain, 2 0 0 1 ) . T h i s methodological
&
Morgan,
2 0 0 1 ; Radley
&
affective and cognitive pathologies, psycholo-
expertise is newly valued, particularly when
gists and other clinicians working in medical
managed care demands m a x i m u m account-
settings need a thorough understanding o f
ability and the identification and use o f the
normal psychosocial development, stress and
most cost-effective treatment plan.
coping, and behavioral health issues. In our
Another contribution that psychologists may
society that retains dualistic mind-body con-
be able to offer their professional colleagues is
cepts, the effort to bridge health and mental
their understanding of systemic factors in the
health may always be challenging. As Belar,
workplace that may facilitate or impede effec-
Paoletti, and Jordan ( 2 0 0 1 ) pointed out, psy-
tive delivery of care. Specifically, jobs in health
chologists and psychiatrists in medical settings
care are often high in the demands they make
act as bridges between their core disciplines
on workers (e.g., workload, responsibility, com-
and the rest o f health care, being mainstream
plexity). Therefore, organizational
in neither. They have to accept a consultative
such as the demand-control-support
role, and they also need to become comfort-
(Johnson & Hall, 1 9 8 8 ) predict that the levels
theories model
able in working with patients w h o are sick,
of control and autonomy that workers have,
disabled, disfigured, injured, or even dying.
and the supportiveness of coworkers and super-
This is despite the possibility that a desire to
visors, will have a major impact on worker
avoid contact with illness and death might
satisfaction, retention, and even quality of work
have been a motivating factor for some health
performance (Dollard, Winefield, Winefield, &
professionals to be drawn to psychology or
De Yonge, 2 0 0 0 ; Judge, Thoresen, Bono, &
psychiatry.
Patton, 2 0 0 1 ; Winefield, 2 0 0 3 ) . Although risk
Beyond patient care, there are large areas o f
management in health care is a huge
and
interprofessional collaboration where the spe-
specialized field that must take account of the
cific training o f the psychology graduate may
multiple pathways through which mistakes and
enhance the professional effectiveness and j o b
adverse events may occur, psychologists' educa-
satisfaction o f health coworkers. T h e scientist-
tional background may uniquely fit them to see
practitioner model equips psychologists to
the management and job design issues behind
understand empirical evidence and interpret
these and other work stressors and to suggest
the literature critically; this skill is not empha-
sustainable remedies (Barach 8c Small, 2 0 0 0 ;
sized in the training o f many other health pro-
Griffiths, Randall, Santos, &c C o x , 2 0 0 3 ; Jones
fessionals. T h e model may assist members o f a
etal., 1 9 8 8 ) .
Working
Skills Needed for Hospital Settings
With a Multidisciplinary
Staff
\
mind and to facilitate the resolution o f these conflicts. Mutual respect and understanding
M i l g r o m , Burrows, and Schwartz ( 2 0 0 1 )
among team members is vital for the team to
provided a checklist for new psychologists
function effectively. Dual relationships increase
adapting to w o r k in medical settings. Items
the risk o f exploiting the power differences
include being brief and clear in communica-
between therapists and patients, confidentiality
tion, respecting the organizational
can raise difficulties, and all practitioners have
culture
and unwritten rules o f conduct, becoming
a responsibility to continually update their
familiar with medical procedures and drugs
own professional learning. T e a m approaches
as well as their side effects, being prepared
increase the possibility that responsibility for
to admit ignorance and ask for advice, and
patient care may become diffused (Belar et al.,
adopting a long-term approach to educate
2 0 0 1 ) , so it becomes very important to main-
colleagues over time. F o r example, it will be
tain alertness to follow-up and to appropriate
necessary to attend and participate in team
documentation of cases.
meetings,
however
time-consuming
this
At the m o r e preventive health
promo-
seems, in the interest o f increasing familiarity
tional level, M i c h i e ( 1 9 9 8 ) wrote about con-
and confidence among team members.
sultancy or targeted research undertaken at
Nicholas and Wright ( 2 0 0 1 ) described the
the request of another health professional.
gradual replacement o f "multidisciplinary"
T h e examples she cited c a m e from requests
health care teams (headed by physicians) with
from
"interdisciplinary" teams (led by individuals
occupational health officers, medical educa-
who
tors, and primary health care visitors and
can
coordinate
the
collaboratively
surgeons, physiotherapists,
nurses,
agreed-on treatment plan and manage the
nurses. She commented on the empathy and
team dynamics). These are bound to be tense
tact, in addition to the communication skills
occasionally as people with different concep-
and research design expertise, needed by con-
tual frameworks try to deal effectively with
sultant
highly distressed and complex patients. It is
that psychologists in this role act collectively
psychologists. She also
advocated
essential, according to Nicholas and Wright,
and strive to avoid "the trap o f the individu-
that team members agree on the treatment
alism that has sometimes been associated
model they are using and that mechanisms
with psychologists" (p. 1 6 7 ) . M e m b e r s o f
exist for them to cope with disagreements and
the College o f Health Psychologists o f the
continue supporting each other. Milgrom and colleagues ( 2 0 0 1 ) noted that
Australian trained
Psychological Society are also
in public health psychology
and
nurses often have a unique role to observe
health promotion. Very few physicians, with
patients, their responses to treatment, and their
the possible exception o f epidemiologists
family interactions on a daily basis, making
and public health specialists (who rarely w o r k
nurses key members
multidisciplinary
directly with patients in delivery o f care set-
teams. Doctors may seem pressed for time but
tings), have this perspective, and neither do
should not be excluded from consideration of
nurses or other allied health professionals.
of
the psychosocial aspects o f patient care. Such teams have a complex and largely unspoken hierarchy o f status roles and expectations that are bound to cause some conflict. Psycho-
Skills Needed for Primary Health Care
logists and psychiatrists, with their training in
All o f this is relevant to psychologists
interpersonal relations, should be in a good
working in multidisciplinary primary care
position to keep the systemic perspective in
settings, although these settings have their
PRACTICAL ISSUES o w n special characteristics as well and in fact
to increase the well-being and decrease the
represent one o f the most exciting new direc-
health care use o f a group o f patients generally
tions for applied health psychology at the
regarded as problematic and frustrating in
current time. W i t h the unresolved difficulties
primary care, namely those individuals with
o f providing mental health care through a
undiagnosable physical symptoms and high
separate system that is often parallel but infe-
rates o f consultation, sometimes referred to as
rior in resources to the "physical" health sys-
somatizers. M a n y of these patients are likely
tem, countries such as Britain and Australia
to be suffering from anxiety or depression (cf.
are actively exploring the value t o consumers
M c L e o d , Budd, & McClelland, 1 9 9 7 ) .
of making treatment for psychological diffi-
Bray and Rogers ( 1 9 9 7 ) provided some
culties and dysfunctions available where the
valuable examples o f the differences in profes-
public is—in primary health care.
sional culture and practices between clinical
In Australia, 8 2 % o f the population is
psychology (focused on understanding and
likely to visit a primary health care physician
questioning) and primary health care (focused
(general practitioner) each year. Although
on fixing problems). T h e number of patients
about one o f five adults will experience a
seen per day, speed o f access to
psychological problem sufficient to interfere
sources, and expectations about the confiden-
significantly
tiality o f patient records all vary greatly, as do
with
daily life and
(Andrews, Hall, Teesson, &
function
Henderson,
reimbursement opportunities.
referral
Practical tips
1 9 9 9 ) , only a minority o f those receive any
about h o w a doctor can manage referrals to a
mental health treatment, and when they do
therapist center on demonstrating the collabo-
it is unlikely to be from a mental health
rative nature o f the care, making it clear that
specialist. However, general practitioners are
the therapist will provide specialist help while
p o o r at both detecting and treating high-
the doctor continues to provide other medical
prevalence psychological disorders such as
care, with a shared first consultation in the
anxiety, depression, and substance abuse.
doctor's office for resistant patients. Physical
Untreated anxiety disorders are associated
proximity o f the providers and regular settings
with substantial health care costs, including
for contact seem crucial to the maintenance o f
unnecessary ambulance trips, hospital emer-
the collaboration between practitioners, just
gency department
presentations, diagnostic
as physical sharing o f training experiences is
such as E C G s (electrocardio-
crucial to their initial entry into the collabora-
grams), and frequent use o f primary health
tion. Openness to the emotional impact o f the
procedures
services. Greenberg and colleagues ( 1 9 9 9 ) esti-
work (especially in difficult cases) and the
mated the annual cost o f anxiety disorders,
chance to discuss such issues within the team
adjusted for demographic factors and comor-
may help the professionals to prevent burnout
bid psychiatric conditions, to be U.S. $ 1 , 5 4 2
and secondary trauma. Being able to give each
per sufferer in 1 9 9 0 , with 5 4 % o f the total
other feedback and support creates a cohesive
costs being for nonpsychiatric medical care.
team that is greater than the sum of its parts
T h e human and social costs o f anxiety disor-
(McDaniel & Campbell, 1 9 9 7 ) .
ders
(Mendlowicz &
Stulp deGroot, Price, and Leslie ( 1 9 9 8 )
Stein, 2 0 0 0 ) . Psychological interventions have
are also substantial
reported their experiences in developing a col-
been demonstrated to be successful treatments
laborative primary health care service for
for a range o f conditions, including anxiety
more than 1 0 , 0 0 0 patients using primary care
and depression (Chambless & Hollon, 1 9 9 8 ;
physicians (PCPs) and mental health clinicians
Chambless & Ollendick, 2 0 0 1 ) . In addition,
( M H C s ) . T h e y carefully documented
psychological interventions have been shown
process and were able to derive the following
this
Working
With a Multidisciplinary
Staff
\
conclusions about h o w to make the project
discipline must experience shared
successful. First, the M H C s had to learn to
Here they learn about each other's knowledge
training.
listen and ask questions in a way that helped
bases, conceptual frameworks, and expecta-
the PCPs to sort things out for themselves
tions of both patients and how the health
rather than to give them answers to their ques-
system operates. T h e frequently
tions. Second, c o l l a b o r a t i o n involves the
stereotypes that physical and psychological
negative
whole system—including office staff, family
health professionals have o f each other need to
members, case managers, and so on—and not
be discussed and set in the context o f the dif-
just the PCPs and M H C s . Third, frequent and
ferent professional cultures and working styles.
timely c o m m u n i c a t i o n is essential in
the
In the United States, interprofessional col-
various forms o f face-to-face hallway conver-
laboration is facilitated by the Interdisciplinary
sations, written chart notes, voice messages,
Professional Education Collaborative, a body
e-mails, and responses to pages. Fourth, roles
that stresses the need to incorporate interpro-
should stay flexible and include attention to
fessional expectations and skills into education
the mental health needs o f the staff. Fifth, col-
(Gelmon, White, Carlson, & Norman, 2 0 0 0 ) .
laboration can work best when the process is
The collaborative has identified some o f the
informal. Sixth, not everyone engages in the
obstacles to achieving integration of profes-
collaborative process at the same rate.
sions at the training level. Historically, there has been reluctance by staff from the different health science disciplines to interact with one another in teaching as well as in the pro-
TRAINING TO WORK IN MULTIDISCIPLINARY HEALTH CARE SETTINGS
fessional sense (Gilbert et al., 2 0 0 0 ) . Although psychologists are often employed within medical schools, their role within the teaching o f
In what follows, it is assumed that the specif-
medicine and medical students tends to be in
ically health psychology content o f the health
the role of scientists rather than practitioners.
psychologist's training and education
Psychology students do not, as a rule, train
has
been thoroughly mastered. It must contain,
next to medical students in the clinical compo-
as we have seen, a comprehensive review o f
nents of their courses as fellow apprentices.
the knowledge base in psychology as applied
A further difficulty is the method o f teach-
to health and illness, measurement, research
ing that is commonly employed at universities.
design, and an understanding o f health ser-
Didactic teaching through lectures, where
vice delivery systems. Hopefully but less pre-
students sit passively in the classroom, will
dictably, the other health professionals with
not result in better collaboration between pro-
w h o m the psychologist will end up working
fessionals (Gelmon et al., 2 0 0 0 ) . Hilton and
conjointly will also have received at least
Morris ( 2 0 0 1 ) argued that the ideal learning
basic training in the principles o f human
environment for developing skills for collabo-
behavior and h o w to study it scientifically
rative practice is the clinical setting, where
(cf. Winefield, 1 9 9 8 ) . This section focuses
learning is experiential and in context. They
specifically on methods to facilitate effective
stressed that in addition to students being
collaborative w o r k shared by health profes-
on placements, successful
sionals with multidisciplinary backgrounds.
requires collaboration between clinicians and
implementation
McDaniel and Campbell ( 1 9 9 7 ) described
academics, with the former sharing their prac-
in detail the training experiences that facilitate
tice philosophies and the latter promoting
collaborative care by psychologists and physi-
appropriate teaching and learning principles.
cians. First and foremost, members o f each
Because most clinicians are trained not as
PRACTICAL ISSUES educators but rather as supervisors, academics
institutional
reward
structures
for
faculty
have an essential support role. T h e responsi-
engaged in curriculum improvement,
bility o f students is also stressed; during clini-
tions o f inflexibility and fear o f the innovative,
tradi-
cal placements, more senior students should
and a resistance t o community-based
be responsible for the coordination o f an
project learning. Few interprofessional courses
aspect o f patient care within the team. Some
have been incorporated into curricula, with
educators have attempted to use simulated
time, expense, funding,
placements (Fallsberg 8c H a m m a r , 2 0 0 0 ) or
coordination
2-day workshops (Gilbert et al., 2 0 0 0 ) instead
mentation (Gilbert et al., 2 0 0 0 ) .
of real clinical placements using an existing
cooperation,
difficulties hampering
Multidisciplinary w o r k
and
and
imple-
is highly politi-
authentic team due to the number o f difficul-
cized and complex, so students o f professions
ties involved in arranging such placements.
where teamwork will be required need "pol-
T o date, we have no empirical studies to pro-
icy acumen" (Jones 8c Salmon, 2 0 0 1 , p. 6 7 ) ,
vide us with information about the relative
which should be included as part o f their
merits and weaknesses o f particular teaching
education. Psychologists, like all health care
approaches.
However, Hilton
and
Morris
professionals, need an understanding o f the
(2001)
convinced
encouraging
social, political, and economic frameworks
are
that
students to collaborate with other profession-
around which policies that affect their w o r k
als in a real setting with experiential learning
are structured, and curricula need to address
is educationally beneficial in terms o f out-
this. Such knowledge will encourage proac-
come. Such student experiences are likely to
tive responses to social policy so as to benefit
result in graduates who are able to function as
both the individual practitioner and the wider
team members, with more positive attitudes,
professional body. Without such knowledge,
abilities in collaborative problem solving, and
there may be a tendency to focus on profes-
better professional development.
sion-centered arguments and perceptions that
In situations where didactic lectures are
are antithetical t o interprofessionalism.
the most appropriate, students from across
M c D a n i e l , Hargrove, Belar, Schroeder, and
different disciplines could be taught together,
Freeman (in press) prepared a detailed cur-
where the same base knowledge is required.
riculum for the education o f psychologists to
Problem-based learning curricula in medicine
practice and research in primary health care
work on the premise that there are no depart-
settings. For example, in the
mental
issues" module, they suggested the following
or discipline boundaries
(Harden,
Davis, 8c Crosby, 1 9 9 7 ) . Therefore, the prob-
student exercises that
lem-based philosophy lends itself easily to
some o f the necessary skills:
neatly
"professional demonstrate
the incorporation o f interprofessional collaboration in training. Rather than groups o f eight medical students, groups composed o f students across a range o f health professions would be possible. This ambitious suggestion would require a major shift in university structures. Indeed, the Interdisciplinary Professional Education Collaborative (Gilbert et al., 2 0 0 0 ) suggested
that
higher
education
institu-
tions offer many barriers to implementation of change that would foster interprofessionalism. It listed as major challenges a lack o f
1. Construct a strategy for seeking reimbursement in your community for psychoeducational groups and collaborative sessions (i.e., sessions for which there is more than one clinician present). 2. Write a justification to an insurance company for a child to be treated by a psychologist for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. 3. Write a one-page advocacy statement for inclusion of psychological services in
Working primary care for submission to your state legislature. out
Staff
\
teamwork and interprofessionalism, but there is a lack o f hard evidence to demonstrate the effectiveness
This training needs to be carried
With a Multidisciplinary
o f interprofessional
training
across the board and there is a need for quan-
jointly by members o f the disciplines con-
titative, large-scale, longitudinal
cerned w h o model a respectful partnership,
studies (Jones & Salmon, 2 0 0 1 ) . Hammick,
evaluation
Barr, Freeth, Koppel, and Reeves ( 2 0 0 2 ) have
shared care, and shared inquiry. Students need to learn which sorts of prob-
conducted a substantial systematic review of
lems need which kind o f professional exper-
evaluations o f interprofessional
tise. In their article on teaching health care
seeking evidence linking professional educa-
education
professionals to collaborate, M c D a n i e l and
tion to a change in either professional practice
Campbell ( 1 9 9 7 ) urged primary care doctors
or patient care, but their work has not yet been
to use counseling skills for simple and mild
published. In educating students about inter-
problems and to use collaboration with a ther-
professionalism, the users o f services are also
apist for more complex problems such as non-
stakeholders, so their views must be solicited
adherence, somatization, significant anxiety
and taken into account in any evaluation.
or depression, and coping with terminal ill-
A major methodological consideration o f
ness. In addition, these authors distinguished
any evaluation o f interprofessional training
"red flag" problems that demand collabora-
or practice is that studies conducted in one
tion and referral, including psychosis, physical
location might not necessarily be meaning-
or sexual abuse, previous treatment failure,
fully compared cross-culturally because cul-
and nonresponse to three or four counseling
tural factors can affect the strategies used.
sessions with the primary care physician.
F o r example, Skjorshammer ( 2 0 0 1 ) , in his
In the literature about training health pro-
study on interprofessional negotiations in a
fessionals to enhance their capacity for later
Norwegian hospital, suggested that the high
multidisciplinary work, it is noteworthy that
incidence o f avoidance to allay conflict may
although authors are quite often able to report
reflect a cultural n o r m o f evasion rather than
on long experiences with large numbers of
a generalizable finding. J o n e s and Salmon
students and practitioners, there are very few
( 2 0 0 1 ) also pointed out that different issues
reports of controlled studies. Yet especially to
are pertinent, depending on the setting, even
psychologists with their awareness of method-
within the same culture. T h e results o f
ological issues in discovering new knowledge,
research into interprofessional training and
it is a concern that the powerful nonspecific
practice will be difficult to generalize across
effects o f having enthusiastic, highly commit-
different situations and teams due to the con-
ted faculty offering innovative programs with
siderable variations encountered in educa-
determination to seek student feedback could
tional institutions and team composition as
engender a corresponding student enthusiasm
well as to cross-cultural differences.
for the training method that was independent
T h e r e are
even greater
difficulties o f
of other outcomes. It is only by random allo-
demonstrating better quality o f health care
cation of students to treatment and control
work in multidisciplinary settings compared
groups (including placebo control groups) that
with the other models such as that of parallel
the highest standards of scientific evidence
work by different professions. T h e problems
could be attained. Such standards are probably
hinge mostly on the unsolved task o f measur-
unreachable in most educational settings.
ing quality o f health care work. Beyond
There is a body o f interesting qualitative literature
emerging
into
the
efficacy
of
patient satisfaction, other measures might be the
j o b satisfaction
o f the
health
care
37
PRACTICAL ISSUES providers and the numbers o f patients cured
stereotyping, and jealousy. Nonetheless, there
or returned to work or normal activities. But to
seems to be agreement among experienced
demonstrate the cost-effectiveness in economic
practitioners that these negatives can be over-
terms that are meaningful to accountants and
come by mutual respect and a focus on the
shareholders is more challenging. Certainly,
c o m m o n goal o f improved patient well-being.
one requirement would be a way in which to
This chapter suggested that psychologists' par-
capture the entire health system utilization costs
ticular
for patients treated with the two models of
teams, beyond their specific expertise in
professional care (multidisciplinary and parallel).
understanding patient behaviors, can lie in
contributions
to
multidisciplinary
reducing conflict within mixed groups, translating scientific evidence for colleagues, and
SUMMARY
monitoring the stressfulness o f the
work
environment. This chapter has reviewed some o f the poten-
To
gain acceptance in multidisciplinary
tial hazards and advantages for clinical health
hospital and primary care settings, psycholo-
psychologists o f working with colleagues who
gists need to learn about these settings' pre-
have multidisciplinary backgrounds. T h i s
vailing cultures and acquire the skills needed
challenge is not unique to medical settings,
to negotiate them effectively. There is now a
but in these highly responsible and complex
growing literature about how to do this,
workplaces and in caring for very vulnerable
although it is a descriptive literature rather
patients, the need to develop
high-quality
than an experimental one. A fundamental rec-
work
unarguable.
ommendation about shared training opportu-
Further careful research is still required to
nities to teach such skills has often proved to
practices is acute and
establish the details of the most effective mod-
be too difficult to test in practice. There are
els for health care shared among providers
likely to be many opportunities for psycholo-
from diverse professions, but this chapter
gists to add to the current scientific literature
focused on how psychologists may extract the
and to use this chapter's findings construc-
maximum benefit from this arrangement for
tively to improve the contribution o f psychol-
their clients, their coworkers, and themselves. The history o f interprofessional relationships in health care has included suspicion,
ogist practitioners within
multidisciplinary
health care teams as well as to enhance the functioning o f such teams.
REFERENCES Andrews, G., Hall, W., Teesson, M., & Henderson, S. (1999). The mental health of Australians. Canberra, Australia: Commonwealth Department of Health and Aged Care, Mental Health Branch. Australian Department of Health and Aged Care. (2000). Toward a national approach to information sharing in mental health crisis situations (Expert Advisory Committee on Information Sharing in Mental Health Crisis Situations report). Canberra, Australia: Author. Australian Psychological Society. (2000). The role of psychological treatments in managing high prevalence mental health disorders. Information statement. (Available on request from [email protected]) Balon, R. (1999). Positive aspects of collaborative treatment. In M. B. Riba & R. Balon (Eds.), Psychopharmacology and psychotherapy: A collaborative approach (pp. 1-31). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Working
With a Multidisciplinary
Barach, P., & Small, S. D. (2000). Reporting and preventing medical mishaps: Lessons from non-medical near miss reporting systems. British Medical Journal, 320, 7 5 9 - 7 6 3 . Barnes, D., Carpenter, J . , & Bailey, D. (2000). Partnerships with service users in interprofessional education for community mental health: A case study. Journal of Interprofessional Care, 14, 1 8 9 - 2 0 0 . Belar, C. D., & Deardorff, W. W. (1995). Clinical health psychology in medical settings: A practitioner's guidebook (rev. ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Belar, C. D., Paoletti, N., & Jordan, C. (2001). Assessment and intervention in a medical environment. In J . Milgrom & G. D. Burrows (Eds.), Psychology and psychiatry: Integrating medical practice (pp. 6 5 - 9 2 ) . Chichester, UK: Wiley. Bray, J . H., &C Rogers, J . C. (1997). The Linkages Project: Training behavioral health professionals for collaborative practice with primary care physicians. Families, Systems, and Health, IS, 5 5 - 6 3 . Chambless, D. L., & Hollon, S. D. (1998). Defining empirically supported therapies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 7 - 1 8 . Chambless, D. L., & Ollendick, T. H. (2001). Empirically supported psychological interventions: Controversies and evidence. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 685-716. Cook, G., Gerrish, K., & Clarke, C. (2001). Decision-making in teams: Issues arising from two U.K. evaluations. Journal of Interprofessional Care, 15, 1 4 1 - 1 5 1 . Dollard, M., Winefield, H. R., Winefield, A. H., & De Yonge, J . (2000). Psychosocial job strain and productivity in human service workers: A test of the demand-control-support model. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 73, 5 0 1 - 5 1 0 . Fallsberg, M. B., & Hammar, M. (2000). Strategies and focus at an integrated, interprofessional training ward. Journal of Interprofessional Care, 14, 3 3 7 - 3 5 0 . Gelmon, S. B., White, A. W., Carlson, L., & Norman, L. (2000). Making organizational change to achieve improvement and interprofessional learning: Perspectives from health profession educators. Journal of Interprofessional Care, 14, 1 3 1 - 1 4 6 . Gilbert, J . H. V., Camp, R. D., Cole, C. D., Bruce, C , Fielding, D. W., & Stanton, S. J . (2000). Preparing students for interprofessional teamwork in health care. Journal of Interprofessional Care, 14, 2 2 3 - 2 3 5 . Goldsmith, R. J . , Paris, M., & Riba, M . B. (1999). Negative aspects of collaborative treatment. In M. B. Riba & R. Balon (Eds.), Psychopharmacology and psychotherapy: A collaborative approach (pp. 3 3 - 6 3 ) . Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Goode, E. (2002, March 26). Psychologists get prescription pads and furor erupts. The New York Times. Greenberg, P. E., Sisitsky, T., Kessler, R. C , Finkelstein, S. N., Berndt, E. R., Davidson, J . R., Ballenger, J . C , & Fyer, A. J . (1999). The economic burden of anxiety disorders in the 1990s. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 60, 4 2 7 - 4 3 5 . Griffiths, Α., Randall, R., Santos, Α., &c Cox, T. (2003). Senior nurses: Interventions to reduce work stress. In M. F. Dollard, A. H. Winefield, & H. R. Winefield (Eds.), Occupational stress in the human services professions (pp. 1 6 5 - 1 8 5 ) . London: Taylor & Francis. Hammick, M., Barr, H., Freeth, D., Koppel, I., & Reeves, S. (2002). Systematic reviews of evaluations of interprofessional education: Results and work in progress. Journal of Interprofessional Care, 16, 8 0 - 8 4 . Harden, R. M., Davis, M. H., & Crosby, J . R. (1997). The new Dundee medical curriculum: A whole that is greater than the sum of the parts. Medical Education, 31, 2 6 4 - 2 7 1 .
Staff
PRACTICAL ISSUES Herrman, H., Trauer, T., Warnock, J . , &c Professional Liaison Committee (Australia) Project Team. (2002). The roles and relationships of psychiatrists and other service providers in mental health services. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 36, 7 5 - 8 0 . Hilton, R., & Morris, J . (2001). Student placements: Is there evidence supporting team skill development in clinical practice settings? Journal of Interprofessional Care, IS, 1 7 1 - 1 8 3 . Hudson, B . (2002). Interprofessionality in health and social care: The Achilles' heel of partnership? Journal of Interprofessional Care, 16, 7 - 1 7 . Johnson, J . V., & Hall, Ε. M. (1988). Job strain, workplace social support, and cardiovascular disease: A cross-sectional study of a random sample of Swedish working population. American Journal of Public Health, 78, 1 3 3 6 - 1 3 4 2 . Jones, J . W., Barge, B . N., Steffy, B . D., Fay, L. M., Kunz, L. K., & Wuebker, L. J . (1988). Stress and medical malpractice: Organizational risk assessment and intervention. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73, 7 2 7 - 7 3 5 . Jones, M., & Salmon, D. (2001). The practitioner as policy analyst: A study of student reflections of an interprofessional course in higher education. Journal of Interprofessional Care, 15, 67-77. Judge, T. Α., Thoresen, C. J . , Bono, J . E., & Patton, G. K. (2001). The job satisfaction-job performance relationship: A qualitative and quantitative review. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 3 7 6 - 4 0 7 . Lipowski, Z. J . (1967). Review of consultation psychiatry and psychosomatic medicine: I. General principles. Psychosomatic Medicine, 29, 1 5 3 - 1 7 1 . McDaniel, S. H., &c Campbell, T. L. (1997). Training health professionals to collaborate. Families, Systems, and Health, 15, 3 5 3 - 3 5 9 . McDaniel, S. H , Hargrove, D. S., Belar, C. D., Schroeder, C , & Freeman, E. L. (in press). Recommendations for education and training in primary care psychology. In R. Frank, S. McDaniel, J . Bray, & M . Heldring (Eds.), Primary care psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. McLeod, C. C , Budd, Μ . Α., St McClelland, D. C. (1997). Treatment of somatization in primary care. General Hospital Psychiatry, 19, 2 5 1 - 2 5 8 . Mendlowicz, M . V., St Stein, M . B . (2000). Quality of life in individuals with anxiety disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 6 6 9 - 6 8 2 . Michie, S. (1998). Consultancy. In A. S. Bellack & M. Hersen (Eds.), Comprehensive clinical psychology, Vol. 8: Health psychology (pp.153-169). Oxford, UK: Pergamon. Milgrom, J . , Burrows, G., St Schwartz, S. (2001). The future of psychology and psychiatry in the medical centre. In J . Milgrom St G. D. Burrows (Eds.), Psychology and psychiatry: Integrating medical practice (pp. 2 9 7 - 3 3 2 ) . Chichester, UK: Wiley. Morgan, D. L., St Morgan, R. K. (2001). Single-participant research design. American Psychologist, 56, 1 1 9 - 1 2 7 . Neal, D. L., & Calarco, M. M. (1999). Mental health providers: Role definitions and collaborative practice issues. In M. B. Riba 8t R. Balon (Eds.), Psychopharmacology and psychotherapy: A collaborative approach (pp. 6 5 - 1 0 9 ) . Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Nicholas, M . K., St Wright, M . (2001). Management of acute and chronic pain. In J . Milgrom St G. D. Burrows (Eds.), Psychology and psychiatry: Integrating medical practice (pp. 1 2 7 - 1 5 4 ) . Chichester, UK: Wiley. Radley, Α., Se Chamberlain, Κ. (2001). Health psychology and the study of the case: From method to analytic concern. Social Science and Medicine, 53, 3 2 1 - 3 3 2 . Schuster, J . M., Kern, Ε. E., Kane, V., & Nettleman, L. (1994). Changing roles of mental health clinicians in multidisciplinary teams. Hospital and Community Psychiatry, 45, 1 1 8 7 - 1 1 8 9 .
Working
With a
Multidisciplinary
SkJ0rshammer, M . (2001). Cooperation and conflict in a hospital: Interprofessional differences in perception and management of conflicts. Journal of Interprofessional Care, 15, 7 - 1 8 . Strain, J . J . , & Home, D. J . deL. (2001). Management of medical and surgical patients: Consultation-liaison (C-L) psychiatry and clinical health psychology. In J . Milgrom & G. D. Burrows (Eds.), Psychology and psychiatry: Integrating medical practice (pp. 9 3 - 1 2 6 ) . Chichester, UK: Wiley. Stulp deGroot, C , Price, D. W., & Leslie, B. (1998). Lessons learned: A collaborative care demonstration project. Families, Systems, and Health, 16, 1 2 7 - 1 3 8 . Todd, T. (1994). Surviving and prospering in the managed mental health care environment. Sarasota, FL: Professional Resources Press. Winefield, H. R. (1998). Teaching and training other health disciplines. In A. S. Bellack & M . Hersen (Eds.), Comprehensive clinical psychology, Vol. 8: Health psychology (pp. 1 7 1 - 1 8 7 ) . Oxford, UK: Pergamon. Winefield, H. R. (2003). Work stress and its effects in general practitioners. In M. F. Dollard, A. H. Winefield, & H. R. Winefield (Eds.), Occupational stress in the human services professions (pp. 1 8 7 - 2 0 7 ) . London: Taylor & Francis.
CHAPTER
4
Motivational Enhancement Interventions and Health Behaviors THAD R . LEFFINGWELL
M
ost behavioral health specialists
variety o f interventions that alter the structure
are interested in how to encourage
and setting of the intervention, while retaining
change in health behaviors among
the fundamental principles and spirit of moti-
their patients. These behavior changes might
vational interviewing, have been developed
include reductions or cessations o f behaviors
and tested (Dunn, D e r o o , & Rivara, 2 0 0 1 ;
harmful to health, such as t o b a c c o use and
Rollnick et al., 2 0 0 2 ) .
excessive alcohol use, or adoption/enhance-
Before
considering
interventions
for
ment o f new or infrequent behaviors that may
enhancing motivation, it would be helpful to
improve or protect health, such as adhering
discuss what motivation is. Motivation is a
to a special diet and increasing physical
c o m p l e x concept that has intrigued psychol-
activity. Certainly, an individual's motivation
ogists for years. Motivation has most often
to make a behavior change may determine
been described as a property o f an individual.
whether or not change is attempted or imple-
F r o m this perspective, motivation may either
mented successfully. If motivation for change
be state-like (e.g., driven by transitory states
in important, the question for the behavior
o f deprivation or need) or trait-like (e.g.,
health specialist becomes " H o w can I
moti-
something a client either had or lacked). Both
vate my patients to make important behavior
of these perspectives place the responsibility
changes?" This chapter describes approaches
for modifying motivation in the hands o f the
to enhancing motivation and
patient (if motivation can be modified at all)
encouraging
behavior change based on principles o f moti-
and offer little guidance on h o w to enhance
vational
this critical ingredient for change. T h e guid-
interviewing,
a
patient-centered
(Miller 8c Rollnick,
ing conceptualization o f motivation in this
2 0 0 2 ) . Rather than focusing on "pure" moti-
chapter is more complex and is based on an
counseling approach
vational interviewing alone, this
chapter
focuses on a variety o f similar approaches and
assumption that motivation is dynamic and can
be
modified
by
social
interactions
strategies that are consistent with the princi-
(Miller, 1 9 8 5 , 1 9 9 9 ) . F r o m this perspective,
ples and spirit o f motivational interviewing. A
motivation is conceptualized as a product o f
Motivational an interpersonal
process between
Enhancement
Interventions
43
patient
many health behavior problems, they were
and provider, and certainly the j o b o f the
initially developed in the substance abuse field.
provider is to create conditions that m a x i -
At the time, the prevailing conceptualization o f
mize motivation (Miller & Rollnick, 2 0 0 2 ) .
motivation to change addictive behaviors was
deserve
based on a belief in trait-like motivation, and
mention at the outset. First, the motivational
resistance and denial were seen as common
enhancement interventions described in this
symptoms of substance dependence. T h e prin-
chapter focus on reducing ambivalence about
ciples and techniques o f motivational inter-
behavior change as a means to enhance moti-
viewing with alcoholics were first described
vation. With most health behaviors, the patient
during the early 1 9 8 0 s . Building on social-cog-
typically feels more than one way about chang-
nitive processes and a Rogerian counseling
Two
other fundamental
issues
ing the status quo; part o f the patient wants to
style, Miller ( 1 9 8 3 ) described a new approach
change, and another part does not. It is this
to patient motivation and a counseling style
ambivalence that has the patient stuck in his
that offered a compelling alternative to the pre-
or her current patterns of behavior. T h e practi-
vailing model. Miller described client motiva-
tioner can intervene in certain ways with the
tion as a product o f the interpersonal process
ambivalent patient to "tip the scales" in favor
or as "a product of the way in which coun-
of change. Second, contemporary models of
selors have chosen to interact with problem
behavior change have moved beyond simple
drinkers" (p. 1 5 0 ) . This recasting o f client
doing/not doing conceptualizations of change
motivation and related behaviors of resis-
and now acknowledge several stages in the
tance and denial required a different kind of
change process (Prochaska, DiClemente, &
approach
Norcross, 1 9 9 2 ) . These stages not only include
approach of direct persuasion, confrontation,
various active stages o f attempting change, such
and the "breaking through" of denial (e.g.,
as preparation, action, and maintenance, but
DiCicco, Unterberger, & M a c k , 1 9 7 8 ) . Further-
to counseling from the existing
also include differentiations among individuals
more, this new approach essentially implicated
not actively involved in change, such as precon-
the confrontational interpersonal approaches
templation and contemplation. This transtheo-
typically used by counselors for the frequent
retical model is well known to most behavioral
observation o f resistance and
health specialists and now guides most research
ambivalent individuals. Clearly, motivational
and clinical practice regarding health behavior
interviewing was a radical innovation
change. T h e interventions described in this
encouraging change in patient health behavior.
chapter are consistent with this model in that
The
they provide means for intervening appropri-
quickly gained popularity, perhaps due
ately with patients in nonactive stages and
frustration with then current approaches or the
maximize the probability o f moving patients
attractiveness o f the less confrontational style
along the stages o f change—ultimately toward
of the approach.
successful change.
motivational
denial
interviewing
with for
approach
Since its inception, motivational
to
inter-
viewing has undergone significant refinement and adaptation. T h e first text describing the
HISTORICAL BACKGROUND Clinical Development of Approach
approach in great detail appeared in 1 9 9 1 (Miller & Rollnick, 1 9 9 1 ) . Subtitled People
to Change
Addictive
Preparing
Behaviors,
this
manual focused specifically on how to effec-
Although the principles o f motivational
tively prepare individuals with substance abuse
interviewing are seen today as generalizable to
and dependence problems to make self-directed
PRACTICAL ISSUES or
new
J a c k s o n , W a n g , Dudley, 8c B a r a n o w s k i ,
approach was also described in a Treatment
assisted changes. This innovative
2 0 0 1 ; Resnicow et al., 2 0 0 0 ) , medication
Improvement Protocol (TIP) series manual
compliance (Schmaling, Blume, 8c Afari,
distributed by the U.S. Department of Health
2 0 0 1 ) , H I V infection protective behaviors
and Human Services (Miller, 1 9 9 9 ) , and a
(Carey et al., 2 0 0 0 ) , t o b a c c o use (Butler et al.,
treatment manual was prepared for the moti-
1 9 9 9 ) , mammography
screening (Ludman,
vational enhancement condition of Project
Curry, Meyer, 8c Taplin, 1 9 9 9 ) , engagement
MATCH
in treatment for bulimia nervosa (Treasure
(National Institute
on A l c o h o l
Abuse and Alcoholism, 1 9 9 5 ) . Building on
et
work to develop a brief method o f motiva-
Heckemeyer, Kratt, 8c M a s o n , 1 9 9 7 ) , dia-
tional interviewing for physicians counseling
betes self-care (Trigwell, Grant, 8c House,
patients
(Butler
et
al.,
1 9 9 9 ; Rollnick,
al.,
1999),
weight
control
(Smith,
1 9 9 7 ) , and even water disinfecting practices
Heather, 8c Bell, 1 9 9 2 ) , Rollnick, M a s o n , and
for
Butler ( 1 9 9 9 ) published a text describing a
sources (Thevos, Quick, 8c Yanduli, 2 0 0 0 ) .
model o f brief motivational
enhancement
individuals
living near unsafe
water
Reviews o f the available motivational
intervention for physicians that retains the
interviewing
spirit o f motivational interviewing while sim-
because motivational interviewing is more o f
plifying the technique for applications in more
a counseling style than a set of techniques
brief encounters. Finally, Miller and Rollnick
a collection o f tasks.
(2002)
ventions may vary significantly in
produced
an extensive revision o f
literature
are
challenging or
This means that interform,
the original motivational interviewing text,
including the setting for the intervention (e.g.,
this time reflecting the broad popularity o f
emergency room, therapy office, physician's
the
negotiating
style
by
describing
the
exam room) and structure (e.g., duration of
approach as a more general strategy appli-
interaction), while still retaining the style or
cable to patients who might be ambivalent
spirit
about any behavior change, not just changing
this flexibility almost certainly accounts for
addictive behavior.
some o f the popularity o f the motivational
o f motivational interviewing. Although
interviewing approach as a clinical tool, it creates difficulties for making
Empirical Support
inferences
about exactly which interventions have which
Since the release of the original detailed manual (Miller & Rollnick, 1 9 9 1 ) , numerous
effects. Nonetheless, three reviews o f this literature have been attempted.
clinical trials investigating motivational inter-
Noonan and Moyers ( 1 9 9 7 ) performed a
viewing interventions or related adaptations
qualitative review o f the available clinical trials
have appeared in the literature. A regularly
that
updated
of
substance use problems. They reviewed 11
motivational interviewing literature lists more
randomized trials and concluded that 9 studies
comprehensive
bibliography
applied motivational interviewing
to
than 6 0 reports of clinical outcome studies,
supported the efficacy of motivational inter-
and most o f these are randomized
viewing for substance abuse and dependence
(www.motivationalinterview.org).
trials
Although
problems.
the majority o f these reports focus on appli-
A more comprehensive review was con-
cations with substance use, applications o f
ducted by Dunn and colleagues ( 2 0 0 1 ) . They
interven-
attempted to capture the diverse nature of
tions to other behavioral problems include
brief motivational enhancement
motivational interviewing approaches while
problem
retaining internal validity o f the interventions
gambling
el-Guebaly,
2001),
(Hodgins, nutrition
Currie,
8c
(Resnicow,
by
defining Adaptations
o f Motivational
Motivational Interviewing (AMIs). T o qualify as an A M I ,
Enhancement
Interventions
\
treatment and a stand-alone treatment for
an intervention had to claim to adhere to
patients with alcohol problems. Studies o f
basic principles o f motivational interviewing.
A M I s applied to problems o f tobacco use,
Consistent with a definition o f motivational
illicit drug use, diet/exercise, and eating disor-
interviewing offered by Rollnick and Miller
ders were reported to be encouraging but too
( 1 9 9 5 ) , this meant that the intervention had to
sparse to make strong findings or recommen-
use a client-centered empathie style to reduce
dations. N o support was found for the use o f
resistance, develop motivational
A M I s to reduce H I V risk behaviors. T h e
discrepan-
cies, and support the patient's self-efficacy. A
authors concluded that reasonable evidence
structure most often used in A M I s incor-
supports the use o f A M I s as both a stand-
porates some sort o f review o f assessment
alone intervention and a treatment adjunct at
feedback as the focus o f the interview, while
the onset o f other treatment. Although the
the counselor uses an interpersonal style and
data are fairly consistent that A M I s are effica-
strategies consistent with motivational inter-
cious, the data are very unclear as to how,
viewing to facilitate the processing o f the feed-
why, and for w h o m the interventions work.
back and to elicit self-motivational statements (e.g., Miller, Sovereign, & Krege, 1 9 8 8 ) . Using this definition o f A M I s and other exclusion criteria regarding study design and outcome measurement, Dunn and colleagues ( 2 0 0 1 ) found 2 9 studies for inclusion in their review.
VARIATIONS OF MOTIVATIONAL ENHANCEMENT INTERVENTIONS As noted previously, motivational interviewing
T h e Dunn and colleagues ( 2 0 0 1 ) review
rarely exists in "pure" form in the empirical
included applications o f A M I s in four different
literature, and it seems likely that this is also
behavioral change contexts: substance use,
reflected in clinical practice. For example, the
tobacco use, H I V risk behaviors, and diet/
2 9 studies reviewed by Dunn and colleagues
exercise. These investigators found that 6 0 %
( 2 0 0 1 ) used eight different labels other than
of the studies reviewed had significant effect
"motivational interviewing" to describe their
sizes favoring the A M I . T h e most consistent
interventions. Furthermore, the durations of
evidence for A M I effectiveness was observed
the interventions ranged from 5 to 3 6 0 min-
where the A M I was used as an enhancement
utes. Clinicians in practice, receiving little
to standard treatment for substance use when
direct guidance from the heterogeneous empir-
delivered at the onset o f a treatment episode.
ical literature, are likely to create motivational
The findings for applications for t o b a c c o use,
enhancement motivations customized to their
H I V risk, and diet/exercise were more mixed
patient
but still encouraging, and more research was
encountered, and settings. T h e purpose of this
recommended.
section is to introduce a few prevailing models
T h e third review o f the motivational inter-
populations,
behavioral
of motivational enhancement
problems
interventions
viewing literature was conducted by Burke,
and to introduce key principles from motiva-
Arkowitz, and Dunn ( 2 0 0 2 ) . Using a similar
tional interviewing that would be required
approach to that used by Dunn and colleagues
to capture the spirit o f the
( 2 0 0 1 ) , but with slightly more restrictive
interviewing style.
motivational
inclusion criteria, these investigators identified
Rollnick and colleagues ( 2 0 0 2 ) attempted
2 6 randomized trials o f an A M I for review.
to add clarity to the varieties o f A M I s that
This review reached similar conclusions, with
exist in the literature and in practice. According
the most impressive findings emerging for
to their framework, brief motivational enhance-
A M I s as both a treatment adjunct to standard
ment interventions could be categorized as one
PRACTICAL ISSUES of three general types o f intervention: (a) brief
typically longer in duration and may involve
advice, (b) behavior change counseling, and
more than one problem area. T h e practitioner
(c) motivational interviewing. W h a t
these
and the patient typically share a more equal
share in c o m m o n could be considered the
role in the decision-making and goal-setting
core of effective motivational
process. Using a person-centered
enhancement
approach,
interventions—a nonconfrontational style and
the practitioner often uses open-ended ques-
a goal of eliciting change from within the
tions and reflection to understand the patient's
patient rather than imposing from the outside
perspective and to check for understanding.
via blaming, coercion, or direct persuasion.
Information typically flows in both directions
several
between the practitioner and the patient, in
domains, including the duration o f the consul-
contrast to the one-way flow seen with brief
tation, the role o f the practitioner, the use o f
advice. M o s t often, the goal is to elicit a deci-
The
approaches
may
differ
on
confrontation, and the use o f direct informa-
sion and plan for change in a more pragmatic
tion. Rollnick and colleagues also attempted
sense than in motivational interviewing, where
to describe the skill sets necessary for each o f
one tries to enhance the quality and commit-
these types o f intervention, with more com-
ment of the plans for change as well.
plex and varied skills necessary for the longer
Rollnick and colleagues ( 1 9 9 9 ) described a
and more complex interaction o f motivational
model o f behavior change counseling based on
interviewing.
earlier work trying to develop "brief motivational interviewing" (Rollnick et al., 1 9 9 2 ) . Designed for nonspecialists working in time-
Brief Advice
pressured settings such as primary care clinics,
Rollnick and colleagues ( 2 0 0 2 ) described
this model simplifies the goals and strategies of
brief advice as a typically brief (less than 15
the behavior change consultation. After estab-
minutes) opportunistic intervention delivered
lishing basic rapport and setting an agenda for
by nonspecialists in behavior change counsel-
the consultation, the practitioner's task is to
ing. T h e goals are typically to raise awareness
explore the patient's feelings about a behavior
of a behavioral problem and to initiate at
change using two dimensions: importance and
least contemplation of change.
Information
confidence. In this model, these two dimen-
exchange is largely one-way, from practitioner
sions adequately capture the nature o f ambiva-
to patient. Goals for behavior change are often
lence, and
suggested rather than elicited. Although the
likelihood of both a change attempt and a
enhancing
both
increases
the
with
successful outcome. Throughout the consulta-
motivational interviewing, the practitioner can
tion, the practitioner may exchange informa-
maximize the motivational impact of the infor-
tion (as in brief advice) or use interpersonal
inequality o f roles is not consistent
mation and advice by carefully choosing a good
strategies to reduce resistance (as in motiva-
opportunity for the intervention,
tional interviewing).
presenting
information in a respectful and compassionate
O n e very creative technique recommended
manner, and using at least some open-ended
as part o f this intervention model is the use o f
questions and reflections.
scaled questions (e.g., 1 to 1 0 0 ) for assessing the importance o f a behavior change and con-
Behavior Change Counseling Behavior change counseling, as described by Rollnick and colleagues ( 2 0 0 2 ) is an approach
fidence in making a change. For example, the practitioner might ask " I f 0 is 'not important at all' and 1 0 0 is 'very important,' number
would
you
say represents
what how
somewhere between brief advice and "pure"
important it is to you now to change
motivational interviewing. T h e consultation is
Whatever answers a patient might provide to
?"
Motivational
Enhancement
Interventions
|
these types o f questions, the answers provide
opportunistic planned encounters that normally
great fodder for reflection, amplification, and
last longer than 3 0 minutes and often span
investigation. T h e patient typically will give a
more than one session. Motivational inter-
number somewhere between the extremes.
viewing requires the full complement o f skills
For the question o f importance, this would
and strategies as well as adherence to the core
allow the practitioner
principles o f motivational interviewing (Miller
to follow up
this
answer to identify concerns the patient may
& Rollnick, 2 0 0 2 ) . Confrontational style is
have about his or her behavior (e.g., " Y o u
always avoided, and direct advice is usually
said ' 4 0 ' . W h y not lower? W h a t makes it ' 4 0 '
provided only when directly requested by the
in i m p o r t a n c e ? " ) o r to identify
in
patient. Communication is used more strategi-
knowledge/awareness or other priorities the
cally, with the goal o f creating motivational
patient may have (e.g., " W h y is the number
discrepancies, resolving ambivalence, and elic-
gaps
not higher? W h a t would it take for you to
iting self-motivational statements (or "change
increase the importance o f this change?"). For
talk") from the patient.
the response to a scaled question about confi-
Miller and Rollnick ( 2 0 0 2 ) defined moti-
dence, the practitioner may follow up with
vational interviewing as " a client-centered,
questions or reflections to explore barriers
directive method for enhancing intrinsic moti-
perceived by the patient (e.g., " Y o u rated
vation to change by exploring and resolving
your confidence as ' 6 0 ' . W h y not ' 7 0 ' or
ambivalence"
' 8 0 ' ? " ) or to identify skills or resources the
viewing has also been described as " a coun-
patient
may have available to support a
change effort (e.g., " Y o u rated your confi-
seling style
(p. 2 5 ) . Motivational
inter-
rather than a set o f techniques
applied to or on people" (Rollnick, 2 0 0 1 , p.
dence as ' 4 0 ' . Y o u must feel that there is some
1 7 6 9 , emphasis added). Although a number
chance you could do it if you tried. W h y ? " ) .
of
The Rollnick and colleagues ( 1 9 9 9 ) brief
techniques
and
strategies
are
recom-
mended as consistent with motivational inter-
other
viewing, other strategies could be used so
strategies for exploring importance, building
long as they are consistent with the principles
confidence, assessing readiness for change,
and style o f motivational interviewing. In
and making strategies for change. As with all
fact, adaptation and creative application of
intervention
model includes several
brief behavior change counseling A M I s , prac-
the principles are encouraged. Motivational
titioners are encouraged to capture the spirit
interviewing in practice, unlike other manual-
o f motivational interviewing in their interac-
ized treatments, might not necessarily have a
tions with clients by using a supportive and
structured beginning, middle, and end but
nonconfrontational
and
instead may occur at various times during a
style to minimize
are
treatment relationship between practitioner
encouraged to choose from the menus o f
and patient. It may be left and revisited or
strategies to create an adaptation that fits their
may be integrated with an ongoing treatment
needs and settings rather than adopting
plan. Motivational interviewing is not some-
respond
to
resistance.
Practitioners
a
thing to be done to patients but rather is a
formulaic treatment approach.
way of being with patients (Miller, 2 0 0 2 ) .
Motivational Interviewing Rollnick and colleagues ( 2 0 0 2 ) described "pure" motivational interviewing as the most c o m p l e x and
involved approach
to
brief
Fundamentals Motivational Essential
of Interviewing
Spirit. Miller and Rollnick ( 2 0 0 2 )
motivational enhancement. Applications o f
described the spirit o f motivational interview-
motivational interviewing are typically less
ing as "understanding and experiencing the
PRACTICAL ISSUES human nature that gives rise to that way o f
behavioral strategies to guide the practitioner.
being" and as absolutely fundamental to any
The principles are not necessarily meant to be
intervention that might claim to be motiva-
initiated in order or in equal amounts but
tional interviewing or a reasonable adapta-
rather are intended to be skillfully and ele-
tion. H o w the practitioner thinks about the
gantly woven together during and throughout
patient and the process is as important as any
the process o f the interview.
technique in determining the nature o f the intervention. Miller and Rollnick
further
The first principle is expressing
empathy.
Probably nowhere is it more important than
described the spirit of motivational interview-
here that one believes in the spirit of motiva-
ing as captured by three fundamental con-
tional interviewing. This principle builds on
siderations: (a) a collaborative relationship,
classic work by Rogers ( 1 9 5 1 , 1 9 6 1 ) and
rather than an authoritarian or prescriptive
assumes that acceptance of patients as they are
relationship, between the practitioner and the
paradoxically makes it easier for them to
patient; (b) an eliciting evocative approach
change. As with Rogers's approach, careful
rather than a persuasive or educative stance;
reflective listening is the key to communicating
and (c) a commitment to the ultimate auton-
empathy to the client. T h e practitioner may use
omy o f the patient to make decisions about
a number of familiar listening skills, including
change and to marshal personal resources for
open-ended questions, a variety o f reflective
change. These considerations are mutually
statements, and nonverbal behaviors. Reflec-
consistent in their respect for the patient's free-
tive listening simultaneously accomplishes
dom of choice as well as competence and
several goals, including encouraging elabora-
expertise in his or her own life.
tion by the patient (which aids in understanding by the practitioner by eliciting data) and
Phases
of the Interview.
A comprehensive
communicating to the patient both an effort to
and complete motivational interview would
understand and ultimately greater understand-
include two overlapping phases: increasing
ing for the practitioner o f the patient. This
motivation for change (Phase 1) and strength-
behavior often prevents resistance from the
ening commitment to a decision for change
outset o f the interview because typical behav-
(Phase 2 ) (Miller & Rollnick, 2 0 0 2 ) . Phase 1
iors that would be likely to elicit resistance
typically involves strategies for building rap-
(e.g., direct persuasion, confrontation, appeals
port; increasing problem recognition; and
to authority) are avoided altogether.
identifying, exploring, and resolving the vari-
How
one thinks about the nature o f
ous aspects o f patient ambivalence. Phase 2
ambivalence is a key to demonstrating empa-
typically involves tasks such as goal setting,
thy in motivational interviewing. Ambivalence
making behavioral plans, and negotiating time
about behavior change is normal and natural,
lines while being careful to avoid roadblocks
and it occurs for most behavior changes
to a successful change attempt (e.g., unrealis-
involving habitual behavior patterns, includ-
tic goals, underestimated
ing changing addictive behaviors. Change is
effort,
shallow
commitment).
difficult.
T h e status
quo is comfortable.
Patients have frequently had either direct or Refinements to Miller's
modeled failure experiences involving behav-
( 1 9 8 3 ) original description o f motivational
ior change. If one sees ambivalence about
interviewing have resulted in the distillation
change as normal and natural rather than as a
of four fundamental
sign o f pathology, immorality, or other unde-
Four
Principles.
principles (Miller
&
Rollnick, 2 0 0 2 ) . These principles help to trans-
sirable personality characteristics, it immedi-
late the spirit o f motivational interviewing into
ately becomes much easier to be comfortable
Motivational with
understanding
and
exploring
that
Enhancement
T h e final principle, supporting refers to communicating
experience.
|
Interventions
self-efficacy,
a belief in
the
discrepan-
patient's ability to be an agent o f change
cies, involves understanding and amplifying dif-
on his or her own behalf. This principle is
ferences between the patient's current behavior
sometimes manifest subtly by the
and his or her goals, values, and/or self-image
tioner's implicit belief in the patient's role in
(Miller &c Rollnick, 2 0 0 2 ) . This discrepancy
the change process. W h e n the patient
serves as a motivating force that can be used to
treated as the ultimate decision maker and as
elicit self-motivational statements. If an individ-
a collaborative partner in exploring change,
The second principle, developing
practiis
ual perceives a behavior as inconsistent with
a belief in the patient's ability to change is
other important
assumed.
goals, the probability o f
change increases. Enhancing
motivational
Also,
when
the
practitioner
inquires in a sincere way as to h o w a patient
the
might go about making a change, this implies
behaviors;
a belief in the patient's o w n resources and
increasing the importance of behaviors, values,
ideas. A number o f more explicit strategies
or goals inconsistent with this current behavior;
can also be employed, including reviewing
or both. T h e patient's current
past successes or models, amplifying
discrepancies may
involve decreasing
importance of current unhealthy
behavioral
sonal strengths, brainstorming
attempt among various behaviors, goals, and
and even occasionally giving direct advice
values that are often conflicting. T h e practi-
(Miller & Rollnick, 2 0 0 2 ) .
tioner's goal is to tip that balance in the direction o f change.
interviewing
itself,
most
resis-
investigations o f motivational enhancement
is a hallmark principle that truly sepa-
approaches have used assessment feedback
T h e third principle, responding tance,
ideas,
Although not inherent
Use of Feedback. to motivational
new
per-
patterns are seen as the result o f a balancing
to
rates motivational interviewing from
most
as part o f the process (Dunn et al., 2 0 0 1 ) .
other approaches and is probably the most
Feedback may include information
innovative aspect o f the approach. As men-
health status, presence or absence o f disease
tioned previously, resistance is viewed as the
states, and
result o f the interpersonal process, or the way
data. These data provide a useful start to
in which the practitioner is interacting with
conversations about change. Reviewing per-
the patient at that moment, rather than as a
sonalized feedback with a patient provides
comparisons
with
about
normative
characteristic o f the patient himself or herself.
many opportunities to explore concerns a
resistance; people are not
patient may have about his or her current
Interactions create
resistant. F r o m this perspective, it follows
behavior
or health
that the practitioner holds responsibility for
feedback
happens frequently
creating conditions in the interpersonal inter-
medicine settings, providing
action to reduce resistance. W h e n resistance is
with
encountered in the interview, the practitioner
motivational enhancement strategies. Objec-
sees that as a signal to change strategies rather
tive feedback alone, especially if it contains
than as a signal to press onward as in con-
bad or anxiety-provoking news, is likely to
numerous
status. T h i s type o f in
behavior
practitioners
rich opportunities
to
use
frontational or persuasive approaches. Because
be experienced by the patient as confrontive
of the paramount importance and complexity
and to elicit resistance. Use o f a motivational
of the task o f responding to resistance, Miller
interviewing style and strategies when review-
and Rollnick ( 2 0 0 2 ) offered several strategies
ing the feedback can aid in the patient's ability
for accomplishing this task. These are summa-
to process the information and use it effec-
rized in Table 4 . 1 .
tively to motivate change.
49
PRACTICAL ISSUES Table 4.1
Examples of Various Useful Methods for Responding to Resistance
Method
Description
Example
Reflective methods Simple reflection Amplified reflection
Double-sided reflection
Statement that reflects observed resistance Restatement of what was heard in exaggerated form Restatement of both sides of ambivalence
"You would rather not talk about your weight." "You would rather never talk about your weight. It doesn't concern you at all." "On the one hand, it's embarrassing to talk about your weight, and on the other, you are worried about it and would like to ask for help."
Strategic methods Shift focus
Change in focus of interaction
Reframe
Giving new interpretation to patient's perspective
Agree with a twist
Agreeing with part of patient's message while reframing another part
Emphasize personal control
Reaffirming patient's ultimate freedom of choice
Coming alongside
Taking patient's side of the ambivalence to encourage him or her to voice the other side
"Let's not worry about what I think right now; let's just talk about any concerns you might have." "You say you've tried and failed many times. It sounds like you have tremendous persistence and courage to keep trying." "Food is your favorite form of recreation, and it's important to enjoy life, even if it is causing you health problems." " O f course it's up to you what to do next. No one can make the decision for you." "You're right that it would be difficult to change—maybe impossible."
S O U R C E : Miller and Rollnick (2002). Reprinted with permission.
Training in Motivational Enhancement Interventions
behavior change counseling and motivational interviewing, require more complex skills and strategies that can probably be learned only
The varieties o f motivational enhancement
through training and supervision. Reflective
interventions require a diverse set o f skills that
listening alone is a deceptively complex set
vary in complexity depending on the type of
of skills to master and is made even more
intervention employed (Rollnick et al., 2 0 0 2 ) .
complex by the strategic use o f listening in
For very brief advice encounters, basic knowl-
motivational interviewing. In motivational
edge about risks of the current
interviewing, the practitioner is not simply
behavior,
behavior change strategies, and rudimentary
expected to mindlessly parrot
open-ended
appreciation for the spirit o f motivational
questions and reflections for the sake o f ongo-
interviewing likely are all that are required.
ing dialogue but rather is expected to ask
The more complex interventions, including
particular
questions
and
make
selective
Motivational
Enhancement
Interventions
reflective statements to promote motivation
deal o f thought and energy to training. Both
for change (Rollnick et al., 2 0 0 2 ) .
versions o f the motivational
Limited information
is available in the
empirical literature on appropriate
training
interviewing
text (Miller & Rollnick, 1 9 9 1 , 2 0 0 2 ) included sections on teaching and learning the approach.
or on the effects o f training. O f 2 9 studies
Over the past several years, Miller and
reviewed by Dunn and colleagues ( 2 0 0 1 ) , only
Rollnick have annually provided
1 0 reported the durations of training provided
training for professionals already proficient in
to practitioners in the studies, and the dura-
the method to become trainers in the tech-
intensive
tions o f that training ranged from 2 to 3 1
nique. M e m b e r s o f this group also stay in
hours. In addition, 11 o f the studies reported
regular contact with each other via an e-mail
providing training but offered few details, and
listserv, a regular newsletter, and an annual
8 of the studies did not include any informa-
meeting. A list o f these "trained trainers" is
tion about training. T w o other studies have
available on the motivational interviewing
examined the effects o f training on the knowl-
W e b site (www.motivationalinterview.org).
edge and skills of trainees directly. T h e first
Miller ( 2 0 0 2 ) proposed a tiered system o f
study found that professional participants did
training as shown in T a b l e 4 . 2 . This system
demonstrate
would allow individuals to tailor the neces-
increases in knowledge
about
motivational interviewing and basic listening
sary training for their setting and
skills in a simple pre-post
needs. Individuals interested in applying the
design (Rubel,
patient
Sobell, & Miller, 2 0 0 0 ) . A second study found
approach to a specific behavioral domain and
that a 2-day workshop on motivational inter-
narrowly defined patient population could do
viewing had significant effects on self-reports
with less training than those interested in a
of motivational interviewing by trainees and
more broad application o f the approach and
had modest gains in skill use demonstrated
certainly could do with less training than those
on observed practice samples, but expected
interested in having the expertise to provide
changes in client behavior were not observed
training.
(Miller &c M o u n t , 2 0 0 1 ) .
Unfortunately,
although participants in this study did modestly increase their frequency o f motivational interviewing consistent strategies, they did not necessarily decrease their use of
inconsistent
strategies. This is o f great concern because it may take only very few confrontational interactions to resurrect resistance and spoil the potential gains of motivational strategies. These preliminary results are
encouraging
but insufficient to make recommendations on empirical grounds for a necessary amount o f training
for
competent
practice.
Further
research is warranted and would be aided by the development o f reliable and valid methods for assessing skill acquisition (Barsky
&
Coleman, 2 0 0 1 ) .
At a minimum, any practitioner interested in applying an approach based on the principles o f motivational
interviewing
should
read one or m o r e o f the available manuals described earlier. Further training could be obtained via introductory workshops available at m a n y national and regional professional conferences. M o r e intensive training could be arranged from one o f the network of trainers. T h e best possible training will occur with opportunities for practice with feedback, both with role-play exercises and with actual cases (Miller & Rollnick, 2 0 0 2 ) . Finally, even after formal training in motivational interviewing, a great deal o f learning can occur by carefully observing the effect o f one's counseling approach on clients. Miller
other
( 1 9 9 6 ) described motivational interviewing
empirical treatments, the originators o f moti-
as an approach he "had learned from [his]
vational interviewing have devoted a good
clients" (p. 8 3 5 ) .
Unlike the originators
o f many
51
PRACTICAL ISSUES Table 4.2
Miller's Proposed Tiered System for Levels of Training in Motivational Interviewing
Type of Training
Goals
Approximate
Introduction to motivational interviewing
Learn about the basics of motivational interviewing and decide level of interest in learning more.
2 hours to 1 day
Application of motivational interviewing
Learn about more specific applications of motivational interviewing, including direct practice with a particular application.
1 hour to 1 day
Clinical training
Learn basic style of motivational interviewing, including extended practice. Strengthen empathie listening skills. Learn to recognize client cues for resistance and change talk.
2 to 3 days or several 4- to 8-hour seminars
Advanced training
Learn advanced clinical usefulness of motivational interviewing. Receive individual feedback on intensive practice. Learn methods of evaluating motivational interviewing. Update knowledge of research developments.
2 to 3 days (plus prior minimum proficiency)
Training for trainers
Learn a flexible range of skills for helping others learn motivational interviewing. Learn to assess needs of trainees and adapt accordingly. Update knowledge of research developments.
3 days
Length
S O U R C E : Miller (2002). Reprinted with permission.
SUMMARY Motivational
that the approach is deceptively complex and intricate, and training and supervision are enhancement
interventions
recommended for individuals interested in
based on the style and principles o f motiva-
developing
tional interviewing have enjoyed increasing
Creative adaptations
proficiency
in
the
methods.
popularity over the past decade. Some have
caution is also warranted before implementing
even expressed concern that the clinical pop-
radical departures (e.g., group motivational
ularity o f the approach may be inappropriate
interviewing, computer-guided
given the young nature o f the empirical
interviewing) until further research can evalu-
are encouraged,
but
motivational
literature (Dunn et al., 2 0 0 1 ) . Although the
ate whether these innovative adaptations can
extant empirical literature is encouraging
demonstrate similar efficacy. Practitioners are
and largely supportive o f the effectiveness o f
further cautioned that if aspects o f current
motivational
interventions,
intervention attempts with patients are incon-
much more research is needed to better under-
sistent with motivational interviewing style,
enhancement
stand how, why, when, and for w h o m the
it is unlikely that simply adding new strategies
approach can be effective.
to the repertoire will be effective. For some,
T h e clinical popularity o f this approach is
it may be necessary to abandon old strategies
expected to continue. Practitioners interested
of confrontation, coercion, and
in applying the principles o f motivational
to achieve better outcomes with motivating
persuasion
interviewing with their patients are cautioned
patient health behavior change.
Motivational
Enhancement
Interventions
REFERENCES Barsky, Α., & Coleman, H. (2001). Evaluating skill acquisition in motivational interviewing: The development of an instrument to measure practice skills. Journal of Drug Education, 31, 6 9 - 8 2 . Burke, B . L., Arkowitz, H., & Dunn, C. (2002). The efficacy of motivational interviewing and its adaptations: What we know so far. In W. R. Miller 8t S. Rollnick (Eds.), Motivational interviewing: Preparing people for change (pp. 2 1 7 - 2 5 0 ) . New York: Guilford. Butler, C , Rollnick, S., Cohen, D., Russell, I., Bachmann, M., & Stott, N. (1999). Motivational consulting versus brief advice for smokers in general practice: A randomised trial. British Journal of General Practice, 49, 6 1 1 - 6 1 6 . Carey, M. P., Braaten, L. S., Maisto, S. Α., Gleason, J . R., Forsyth, A. D., Durant, L. E., &c Jaoworski, B . C. (2000). Using information, motivational enhancement, and skills training to reduce the risk of HIV infection for low-income urban women: A second randomized clinical trial. Health Psychology, 19, 3 - 1 1 . DiCicco, L., Unterberger, H., & Mack, J . E. (1978). Confronting denial: An alcoholism intervention strategy. Psychiatric Annals, 8, 5 9 6 - 6 0 6 . Dunn, C , Deroo, L., & Rivara, F. P. (2001). The use of brief interventions adapted from motivational interviewing across behavioral domains: A systematic review. Addiction, 96, 1 7 2 5 - 1 7 4 2 . Hodgins, D. C , Currie, S. R., &c el-Guebaly, N. (2001). Motivational enhancement and self-help treatments for problem gambling. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69, 5 0 - 5 7 . Ludman, E. J . , Curry, S. J . , Meyer, D., & Taplin, S. H. (1999). Implementation of outreach telephone counseling to promote mammography. Health Education and Behavior, 26, 6 8 9 - 7 0 2 . Miller, W . R. ( 1 9 8 3 ) . Motivational interviewing with problem drinkers. Behavioural Psychotherapy, 11, 1 4 7 - 1 7 2 . Miller, W. R. (1985). Motivation for treatment: A review with special emphasis on alcoholism. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 8 4 - 1 0 7 . Miller, W. R. (1996). Motivational interviewing: Research, practice, and puzzles. Addictive Behaviors, 21, 8 3 5 - 8 4 2 . Miller, W. R. (Ed.). (1999). Enhancing motivation for change in substance abuse treatment (Treatment Improvement Protocol Series, No. 3 5 , DHHS Publication No. (SMA) 0 0 - 3 4 6 0 ) . Rockville, M D : Center for Substance Abuse Treatment. Miller, W. R. (2002, January 1). From the desert. Motivational Interviewing Newsletter: Update, Education, and Training, pp. 1-2. (Albuquerque, N M : Motivational Learning Network of Trainers) Miller, W. R., & Mount, K. A. (2001). A small study of training in motivational interviewing: Does one workshop change clinician and client behavior? Behavioral and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 29, 457—471. Miller, W. R., 8c Rollnick, S. (1991). Motivational interviewing: Preparing people to change addictive behaviors. New York: Guilford. Miller, W. R., &C Rollnick, S. (2002). Motivational interviewing: Preparing people for change. New York: Guilford. Miller, W. R., Sovereign, R. G., & Krege, B. (1988). Motivational interviewing with problem drinkers: II. The Drinker's Check-Up as a preventive intervention. Behavioural Psychotherapy, 16, 2 5 1 - 2 6 8 . National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. (1995). Motivational enhancement therapy manual: A clinical research guide for therapists treating individuals with alcohol abuse and dependence (Project M A T C H Monograph
PRACTICAL ISSUES Series, NIH Publication No. 94-3723). Rockville, M D : U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Noonan, W. C , & Moyers, T. B . (1997). Motivational interviewing: A review. Journal of Substance Misuse, 2, 8 - 1 6 . Prochaska, J . O., DiClemente, C. C , & Norcross, J . C. (1992). In search of how 47, people change: Applications to addictive behaviors. American Psychologist, 1102-1114. Resnicow, K., Jackson, Α., Wang, T., Dudley, W., & Baranowski, T. (2001). A motivational interviewing intervention to increase fruit and vegetable intake through black churches: Results of the Eat for Life trial. American Journal of Public Health, 91, 1 6 8 6 - 1 6 9 3 . Resnicow, K., Wallace, D. C , Jackson, Α., Digirolamo, Α., Odom, E., Wang, T., Dudley, W. N., Davis, M., Mitchell, D., & Baranowski, T. (2000). Dietary change through African American churches: Baseline results and program description of the Eat for Life trial. Journal of Cancer Education, IS, 1 5 6 - 1 6 3 . Rogers, C. R. (1951). Client-centered therapy. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Rogers, C. R. (1961). On becoming a person. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Rollnick, S. (2001). Enthusiasm, quick-fixes and premature controlled trials. Addiction, 96, 1 7 6 9 - 1 7 7 0 . Rollnick, S., Allison, J . , Ballasiotes, S., Barth, T., Butler, C , Rose, G. S., & Rosengren, D. B. (2002). In W. R. Miller & S. Rollnick (Eds.), Motivational interviewing: Preparing people for change (pp. 2 7 0 - 2 8 3 ) . New York: Guilford. Rollnick, S., Heather, N., & Bell, A. (1992). Negotiating behaviour change in medical settings: The development of brief motivational interviewing. Journal of Mental Health, 1, 2 5 - 3 7 . Rollnick, S., Mason, P., & Butler, C. (1999). Health behavior change. London: Churchill Livingstone. Rollnick, S., &c Miller, W. R. (1995). What is motivational interviewing? Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 23, 3 2 5 - 3 3 4 . Rubel, E. C , Sobell, L. C , & Miller, W. R. (2000). Do continuing education workshops improve participants' skills? Effects of a motivational interviewing workshop on substance-abuse counselors' skills and knowledge. The Behavior Therapist, 23, 7 3 - 7 7 . Schmaling, K. B., Blume, A. W., & Afari, N. (2001). A randomized controlled pilot study of motivational interviewing to change attitudes to medications for asthma. Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings, 8(3), 1 6 7 - 1 7 2 . Smith, D. E., Heckemeyer, C. M., Kratt, P. P., & Mason, D. A. (1997). Motivational interviewing to improve adherence to a behavioral weight-control program for older obese women with NIDDM: A pilot study. Diabetes Care, 20, 5 3 - 5 4 . Thevos, A. K., Quick, R. E., & Yanduli, V. (2000). Application of motivational interviewing to the adoption of water disinfection practices in Zambia. Health Promotion International, 15, 2 0 7 - 2 1 4 . Treasure, J . L., Katzman, M., Schmidt, U., Troop, N., Todd, G., & de Silva, P. (1999). Engagement and outcome in the treatment of bulimia nervosa: First phase of a sequential design comparing motivation enhancement therapy and cognitive behavioural therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 405—418. Trigwell, P., Grant, P. J . , 8c House, A. (1997). Motivation and glycémie control in diabetes mellitus. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 43, 3 0 7 - 3 1 5 .
CHAPTER
5
Brief Psychotberapies and Group Treatments in General Health Care Settings DEBORAH J . WIEBE, LINDSEY BLOOR, AND TIMOTHY W . SMITH
O
ver the past 3 0 years, evidence has
application of basic research in behavioral
continued
demon-
medicine and health psychology has itself been
strating that medical conditions both
the focus of an expanding body o f research.
affect and are affected by psychosocial fac-
Although the findings are not uniformly sup-
tors. Behavioral habits o f daily life, such as
portive, there is clear evidence that psychother-
to
accumulate
physical activity, smoking, and diet, clearly
apy and other approaches to behavior change
affect the risk of developing the most c o m -
can be useful additions to routine medical care,
m o n sources o f morbidity and mortality (e.g.,
both in the reduction o f unhealthy risky behav-
coronary heart disease, cancer, diabetes) as
ior and in the management of the psychosocial
well as the course of such conditions. Other
impacts of medical conditions (Smith, Kendall,
psychosocial factors, such as stressful
life
& Keefe, 2 0 0 2 ) . In some cases, such interven-
personality
tions are useful as primary or secondary treat-
characteristics, and chronic negative emo-
ments for the underlying medical conditions
tions, affect the development and course o f
themselves.
circumstances, social support,
these same conditions through more direct
This
chapter
psychobiological mechanisms. Finally, most
approach
that
acute and chronic medical conditions influ-
efforts (i.e., the biopsychosocial model [Engel,
ence emotional adjustment, personal relation-
1977]) and provides examples o f the wide vari-
reviews
underlies
the
conceptual
such
intervention
ships, w o r k and other aspects o f functional
ety o f brief and group interventions used across
activity, and overall quality of life.
medical conditions and settings. A comprehen-
One clear implication of the bidirectional
sive review is clearly beyond the current scope,
associations between psychosocial factors and
but the chapter does describe the nature and use
physical health is that psychosocial interventions
of such interventions in general health care
might be useful in the prevention and man-
settings and also reviews evidence of their effi-
agement of medical conditions. This clinical
cacy and effectiveness. T h e skills and training
PRACTICAL ISSUES necessary for the translation of traditional brief
In an influential critique o f the prevailing
psychotherapy and group treatment interven-
biomedical model as t o o simplistic and reduc-
tions into the unique culture and context of
tionistic to accommodate this increasing role
medical care are also discussed. T h e chapter
of psychological and social factors in the
begins with a brief history o f the developments
major sources o f morbidity and mortality,
that have created the opportunity—and even
Engel ( 1 9 7 7 ) argued that a
demand—for the expansion of general health
alternative is more appropriate. In this view,
biopsychosocial
care to include interventions traditionally con-
based in part on the general systems theory o f
ceptualized as mental health services.
V o n Bertalanffy ( 1 9 6 8 ) , health and illness are seen as emerging from the reciprocal interplay of hierarchically arranged levels o f analysis,
HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENTS
ranging from the molecular to the individual to the sociocultural, with several levels in
The growth in the importance o f psychosocial
between. F r o m a biopsychosocial perspective,
interventions for the prevention and manage-
understanding the source o f illness
ment o f medical conditions was set into
designing optimal approaches to medical care
motion by changing patterns o f disease over
requires this multisystem analysis. In this
the last half o f the 2 0 t h century. Until that
expanded
time, acute medical conditions (e.g., infec-
interventions have an obvious place.
and
conceptual model, psychosocial
tious diseases) were the leading cause o f death
Influential predecessors to the current array
in the United States (National Office o f Vital
of psychosocial interventions began to appear
Statistics, 1 9 4 7 ) . By the end o f the 2 0 t h cen-
early in the development o f the fields of behav-
tury, chronic conditions had c o m e to account
ioral medicine and clinical health psychology.
for more than 7 0 % o f all deaths, primarily
Growing epidemiological evidence o f the role
due to the effects o f coronary heart disease,
of smoking, excess body weight, and physical
cancer, and cerebrovascular disease (Centers
inactivity in cardiovascular disease and cancer
for Disease Control and Prevention, 1 9 9 9 ) .
prompted the application o f existing behavior
Certain behaviors (e.g., smoking, activity
change techniques to these health-relevant
levels) contribute to the risk o f these diseases,
targets
and given advances in medical care, patients
Shnidman, 1 9 7 1 ; Stuart, 1 9 6 7 ) . Advances in
(e.g., Shapiro, Tursky, Stuart,
&
suffering from these conditions can expect to
the physiology o f stress and its role in the
live long enough that coping with their many
development o f several medical disorders (e.g.,
impacts will become an important challenge.
hypertension) combined with the available
Finally, the management o f these and other
operant behavioral change methods to form
increasingly prevalent
conditions
the basis o f early biofeedback treatments for
typically involves many behavioral processes
several chronic conditions (e.g., Pickering &
(e.g., adherence
chronic
to prescribed
regimens,
Miller, 1 9 7 7 ) . Early research demonstrating
exercise-based rehabilitation, stress manage-
that the preoperative psychological state (e.g.,
ment interventions). Hence, patterns o f mor-
anxiety, coping behaviors) influenced
bidity and mortality have changed over the
postoperative
past century in such a way as to make psycho-
prompted the development o f brief, structured
social interventions an important component
psychosocial interventions for this population
course o f surgical
the
patients
of current health care. T h e parallel rise in
(Janis, 1 9 5 8 ) . Similarly, operant concepts and
health care expenditures has created additional
behavior change technology were successfully
incentives for effective additions to traditional
applied to the conceptualization and treat-
medical care (Kaplan & Groessl, 2 0 0 2 ) .
ment
o f chronic pain
(Fordyce,
1976).
Brief Psychotherapies Table 5.1
and Group
Treatments
Outline for the Clinical Application of the Biopsychosocial Model
I. Illness Factors A. B. C. D. E.
Pathophysiology Risk factors Prognosis Diagnostic procedures Treatment procedures
II. Patient Factors A. B. C. D. E.
D S M conditions (Axis I and Axis II) Impact of illness on distress, social and occupational functioning, and quality of life Conceptualization of disease and treatment Personality traits and coping styles Educational and vocational status
III. Social, Family, and Cultural Factors A. B. C. D.
Quality of marital and family relationships Use and efficacy of social support Relationships with the health care team Cultural background
IV. Health Care System Factors A. Health care setting and culture B. Insurance coverage and disability benefits for medical condition C. Geographical, social, psychological, and monetary barriers to accessing services S O U R C E : Adapted from Smith and Nicassio (1995). Copyright © 1 9 9 5 by the American Psychological Association. Adapted with permission.
Together, these and other brief interventions
this assessment is presented
in T a b l e 5.1
provided clear evidence o f the promise o f
(Smith &c Nicassio, 1 9 9 5 ) . This outline is not
extending traditional medical care to include
a formal procedural protocol but instead pro-
psychotherapy
vides a general orientation or viewpoint that
and other behavior change
approaches. As reviewed in what follows, the
guides more specific informal and
subsequent 3 0 years o f research has produced
assessment procedures.
formal
many examples o f innovative and valuable
T h e first category involves information
extensions of this prior work (Smith, Nealey, &
about the patient's specific illness or condi-
Hamann, 2 0 0 0 ) .
tion. T h e pathophysiology, relevant risk factors, natural history and prognosis o f the
Clinical Application of the Biopsychosocial Model
condition, and diagnostic and treatment procedures typically comprising its medical management are essential elements o f the patient's
T o be feasible and effective, any brief psy-
context and may help to prioritize specific
chosocial intervention with medical patients
intervention targets that could be usefully
must begin with an assessment based on the
addressed
biopsychosocial model. T h e results of such an
tions. T h r o u g h general experience and spe-
assessment identify not only important targets
cific collaborative discussions with
for intervention but also important moderating
members o f the multidisciplinary health care
factors and contextual issues. An outline for
team, the clinical health psychologist must
through psychosocial intervenother
PRACTICAL ISSUES acquire adequate knowledge o f the general
each o f these four general categories o f the
condition and its typical management as well
patient's "biopsychosocial presentation" can
as o f the patient's specific case.
appropriate interventions be identified and
Similarly, characteristics o f the patient can guide the identification o f specific, potential
implemented in a manner most likely to m a x imize their potential benefits to the patient.
intervention targets (e.g., depression, limitations in functional activity, knowledge o f the disease and its medical/surgical management) or can suggest important moderators o f the likely impact o f the condition (e.g., coping
Intervention Options for Psychologists in Health Care Settings
styles, vocational history and status). This
Clinical health psychologists choose inter-
information, in turn, guides the selection and
ventions from the full range of therapeutic
implementation o f specific interventions.
options available to professional psychology,
Although often overlooked in traditional
but unique features o f the medical setting can
medical assessments, the patient's social,
challenge the psychologist and shape the form
family, and cultural contexts can also identify
that interventions take. T h e diversity of medi-
potential targets for intervention (e.g., social
cal and psychological problems seen across
isolation, serious relationship conflict)
or
health care settings, each o f which is associated
resources to maximize the benefits o f other
with a complex bundle of biopsychosocial
interventions
(e.g., social support).
The
issues, requires the psychologist to be a broadly
strengths and weaknesses o f the patient's rela-
trained generalist. T h e time demands of health
tionships with key members o f the health care
care settings and the cost containment features
team and the patient's skills for managing and
of the health care system push the clinician to
improving those relationships (e.g., assertive-
be increasingly brief, efficient, and account-
ness) are also important considerations in
able. T h e interdisciplinary nature o f care and
selecting targets or methods for intervention.
the psychologist's place in the medical hierar-
T h e patient's cultural/ethnic background is
chy challenge the psychologist to be collabora-
also an important consideration, especially if
tive and resourceful in the delivery o f services.
it is different from that o f key members o f the
These features have resulted in a strikingly het-
health care team given that it can complicate
erogeneous and creative array of treatments.
effective communication and collaboration
As outlined in Table 5 . 2 , this sometimes daunt-
over the long periods
ing set of treatment issues and options can be
o f time typically
involved in the care o f chronic disease.
structured by considering the level and mode
Finally, the specific health care setting (e.g., inpatient vs. outpatient care, brief vs.
of treatment in the context o f one's goals for intervening.
prolonged hospitalization) is likely to make some interventions more feasible than others, as are prevailing attitudes toward psychoso-
Goals
of
Intervention
cial interventions among members o f the
Psychologists working in health care settings
health care team within the culture o f a spe-
intervene to improve patients' health and well-
cific clinic or medical service. Insurance cov-
being across three broad and
erage and a variety o f potential barriers to
domains: (a) reducing the risk of developing dis-
psychosocial intervention (e.g., access to safe,
ease among healthy individuals, (b) improving
interrelated
adequately supervised exercise facilities) are
disease outcomes among those with developed
essential considerations in treatment
plan-
illnesses, and (c) enhancing the quality of life
ning. Only after a thorough consideration of
and emotional health of those experiencing
Brief Psychotherapies Table 5.2 I.
and Group
Treatments
Intervention Options in General Health Care Settings
Reasons for Intervention A. Risk reduction B. Disease outcomes C. Maximizing functioning/quality of life
II.
Levels of Intervention A. B. C. D.
Individual Couples/Family Group Health care team
III. Modes of Intervention A. Psychoeducation B. Cognitive behavioral therapies C. Interpersonal/Social support interventions
illness. Given compelling evidence that behavioral
mortality (e.g., Coyne et al., 2 0 0 1 ) . Hence,
and psychosocial processes are integral to the
interventions that involve families or spouses
development and course of many physical condi-
may be more effective than those that focus
tions, psychologists may intervene to reduce the
solely on the individual (e.g., Anderson, H o ,
incidence or progression of major illnesses.
Brackett, & Laffel, 1 9 9 9 ; Epstein, Valoski,
However, because illness can create profound
Wing, & McCurley, 1 9 9 0 ; Keefe et al., 1 9 9 6 ) .
psychosocial challenges, the goals of clinical
Group psychotherapy is also fairly com-
health psychologists extend beyond attempts to
m o n in medical settings. Group psychothera-
improve physical health to maximize the daily
pies tend to be cost- and time-efficient because
functioning of patients and their families.
m a n y educational, behavioral, and
inter-
personal issues can be addressed readily in a Levels
of
group format (Spira, 1 9 9 7 ) . Groups have the
Intervention
additional benefits o f bestowing a sense o f
These goals can be met by intervening at a
belonging for individuals dealing with the
variety o f levels. Individual therapy remains a
c o m m o n stress of illness and providing impor-
strong option for clinical health psychologists,
tant sources o f support, information,
but interventions involving larger social units
accountability for behavior change (Spira,
and
are increasingly c o m m o n . T h e family unit is
1 9 9 7 ; Spiegel & Diamond, 2 0 0 1 ) . Although
an important focus because illnesses can have
support groups have the potential to be iatro-
adverse effects on families, and risk reduction
genic (e.g., Helgeson, C o h e n , Schulz,
and illness management occur within this
Y a s k o , 2 0 0 0 ) , there is evidence that well-con-
&
broader family context. Family conflict and
ducted, structured groups can be as effective
marital strife appear to be particularly disrup-
as, or more effective than, individual therapy
tive to managing illness and
at
maintaining
quality of life among medical patients (e.g., Schafer, Keith, &
Schafer, 2 0 0 0 ;
promoting
and
maintaining
behavior
change (e.g., W i n g & Jeffery, 1 9 9 9 ) .
Zautra,
A level that may be somewhat unique to
Burleson, M a t t , R o t h , & Burrows, 1 9 9 4 ) and
medical settings involves interventions focus-
may even pose a risk for future morbidity and
ing on the health care team. As we witness a
PRACTICAL ISSUES gradual shift away from an "acute medical
but commonly adds in skill-building features
intervention" model toward a "chronic illness
such as goal setting, self-monitoring, problem
management" model, patients are required to
solving, stimulus control, relaxation and stress
assume new responsibilities for day-to-day ill-
management, and cognitive restructuring. It
ness management, and health care profession-
is also increasingly c o m m o n to include inter-
als are compelled to incorporate behavioral
personal skill acquisition such as assertion
interventions into medical practice (Gonder-
training and developing social support. C B T
Frederick, C o x , &i Ritterband, 2 0 0 2 ) . Health
overlaps with psychoeducation to the extent
care professionals might need the behavior
that both modes teach specific skills (e.g.,
change expertise o f psychologists to do this
relaxation training), but C B T is delivered in a
effectively. Furthermore, because health care
more progressive, individualized, and interac-
providers can be an efficient and powerful
tive manner to help patients not only recon-
source of advice and counseling for patients,
ceptualize their health problems and develop
interventions to promote physicians' commu-
new coping skills but also consolidate (e.g., via
nication skills and enhance the doctor-patient
rehearsal and role-play) and maintain these
relationship can have broad effects. Finally,
skills (e.g., relapse prevention).
the continuing need for pragmatic, cost-effec-
Given compelling evidence on the impor-
tive interventions may result in the training
tance of social relationships for one's physical
of nonpsychologists for the delivery of brief
health and psychological well-being, interper-
psychological interventions.
sonal/social support interventions are also commonly used in medical settings. Aside from
Modes
of
some well-defined interpersonal
Intervention
therapies,
however, many of these interventions have
Given the range o f issues and problems
not been described or studied systematically
likely to be encountered across health care set-
(Hogan, Linden, &c Najarian, 2 0 0 1 ) . Some
tings, psychologists cannot be wedded to any
social support interventions focus on providing
particular therapeutic orientation. This chap-
support during therapy per se. This may occur
ter focuses on three broad modes o f inter-
by direct support provision from the therapist;
vention that can be used across individual,
by simply including family, friends,
group, or family formats: psychoeducation,
peers in therapy; or by including therapeutic
and/or
cognitive-behavior therapy ( C B T ) , and inter-
activities that engage or promote support from
personal/social support. Psychoeducation gen-
important others. Other support interventions
erally provides patients with information (e.g.,
focus on developing patients' social skills,
information about health risks, illness, treat-
which can then
ments, or coping skills) to alter their attitudes
strengthen their naturally occurring social sup-
and behaviors in a direction that will improve
port networks. Support interventions can occur
adjustment.
at any level but most commonly capitalize on
Psychoeducation c a n
provide
patients with a medically accurate under-
be used to nurture and
the supportive features o f group therapy.
standing o f their condition and may be sufficient for some patients to adapt to
the
complex demands o f managing illness. C B T is pervasive in medical settings. These interventions include a myriad o f specific
EXAMPLES OF BRIEF PSYCHOLOGICAL INTERVENTIONS IN HEALTH CARE SETTINGS
behavioral and cognitive techniques that are often combined in a multicomponent fashion.
This section describes h o w these three modes
Multicomponent C B T begins with education
of intervention—psychoeducation, C B T , and
Brief Psychotherapies
and Group Treatments
|
interpersonal/social support—have been used
Health [ N I H ] , 1 9 9 7 ) . This external validation
to achieve the three treatment goals in clinical
of what behavioral scientists have known for
health psychology, namely reducing
risk,
years may increase the demands for integrat-
improving disease outcomes, and enhancing
ing lifestyle change interventions into general
quality o f life. This structure is one o f con-
health care. Psychosocial variables, such as
venience and should not be interpreted
personality, stress, negative emotions, and
as
implying independence across domains. For
impaired social relationships, are also emerg-
example, health behavior interventions
are
ing as important factors in the development
reduction
o f illness. T h u s , although they are not fully
discussed in the context o f risk
but may also improve disease outcomes (e.g.,
established, interventions
smoking cessation to reduce vascular compli-
chosocial risk profiles may become increas-
cations a m o n g
ingly relevant for risk reduction.
individuals
with
diabetes
to improve
psy-
[Gonder-Frederick et al., 2 0 0 2 ] ) and enhance quality o f life (e.g., exercise interventions to reduce depression
with
cancer
[Andersen,
2 0 0 2 ] ) . T h e approach taken for this limited
Psychoeducation Education and self-help information
are
review is to provide general themes from the
mainstays o f most effective risk
most well-developed literatures (e.g., interven-
interventions. In health care settings, psycho-
tions tested with randomized clinical trials) as
education can occur in person or via telephone,
well as salient examples across the range o f
print, or computer-generated
reduction
information
interventions. This section emphasizes brief
delivered to at-risk individuals
interventions but occasionally discusses more
Although generally considered necessary for
intensive therapies if their effects are particu-
risk reduction, education in isolation has fairly
larly impressive. Finally, despite clear advances
modest and short-term effects (Blumenthal,
in research on the efficacy
Sherwood, Gullette, Georgiades, & Tweedy,
o f behavioral
or
groups.
medicine interventions over the past decade,
2 0 0 2 ; Dubbert, 2 0 0 2 ; Niaura
one should note that the interventions dis-
2 0 0 2 ) . There is reason to believe that effec-
cussed here have often not been adequately
tiveness can be improved by including family
tested in the health care settings in which they
members in educational efforts (e.g., Morisky,
are likely to be used. Hence, for all o f these
D e M u t h , Field-Fass, Green, & Levine, 1 9 8 5 ) ,
&
Abrams,
interventions, there is more evidence o f their
and
efficacy in controlled trials than o f their effec-
patients' level o f readiness to change (Dubbert,
tiveness in the conditions and contexts o f their
2 0 0 2 ) . For example, Strecher and colleagues
typical clinical use during medical care.
by tailoring the information
to
the
( 1 9 9 4 ) found that smoking family practice patients reported a doubling o f 6-month quit
Reducing Risk of Developing Disease It is well known that modifiable risk fac-
rates when they received individually tailored smoking cessation letters rather than standard cessation letters (but see Curry,
McBride,
tors, such as smoking, inactivity, obesity, and
Grottos, Louie, & Wagner, 1 9 9 5 , for differing
risky sexual behaviors, play a major role in
results).
the health o f the U.S. population. Behavioral interventions to reduce obesity or stop smok-
A promising educational approach to risk reduction
involves training physicians
and
ing are supported well enough that they are
other health care professionals to
recommended options in the clinical practice
lifestyle change advice and counseling. Health
provide
guidelines emanating from federal agencies
care providers may be particularly persuasive
(e.g., Fiore et al., 1 9 9 6 ; National Institutes o f
messengers for risk reduction
given their
61
62
PRACTICAL ISSUES frequency of contact with high-risk individuals
of
and the importance o f physician advice in moti-
development o f disease was
such interventions
for preventing
the
vating interest to change. Controlled clinical
dramatically in the Diabetes Prevention Trial
demonstrated
trials suggest that physician advice and written
(National Institute o f Diabetes, Digestive,
or telephone follow-up are effective at increas-
and Kidney Disease, 2 0 0 1 ) . Although the
ing physical activity (e.g., Writing Group for
intervention was not brief, this multicenter,
the Activity Counseling Trial, 2 0 0 1 ) and smok-
randomized clinical trial clearly demonstrated
ing cessation (e.g., Ockene et al., 2 0 0 0 ; Pieterse,
that behaviorally based lifestyle interventions
Seydel, DeVries, Mudde, & K o k , 2 0 0 1 ) . There
can be as effective as, or more effective than,
is also evidence that interventions delivered by
medications at preventing the development o f
other health care providers
(e.g.,
nurses,
physician assistants) are effective and additive (Burns, Cohen, Gritz, & Kottke, 1 9 9 4 ) .
diabetes among high-risk individuals. Although impressive, such data must be interpreted and applied cautiously. In most
An area o f emerging interest involves incor-
cases, the interventions represent a bundle of
porating motivational interviewing into risk
educational, behavioral, and cognitive strate-
reduction advice and counseling (see Chapter
gies, making it impossible to evaluate which
4
for more details on motivational inter-
components are most important for reducing
viewing). Originally developed to enhance
risks. At best, this creates an inefficient
motivation to address addiction, motivational
approach to providing therapy in the time
interviewing represents a style o f providing
constraints o f m a n y health care settings
personalized behavior change feedback in an
(cf.
empathie, nonconfrontive, and
although multicomponent C B T appears to be
empowering
Coyne &
R a c i o p p o , 2 0 0 0 ) . Second,
manner (Miller & Rollnick, 1 9 9 1 ) . Although
quite effective at promoting initial behavior
somewhat mixed, preliminary data suggest
change, there is a serious problem
with
that motivational interviewing has the poten-
relapse. M o r e intensive interventions
(e.g.,
tial to be useful across a range o f health
more therapy sessions, multicomponent vs.
behaviors (for reviews, see Dunn, Deroo, &
single-component
Rivara, 2 0 0 1 ; Resnicow et al., 2 0 0 2 ) . If addi-
appear to produce more prolonged changes
therapy,
medication)
tional research supports these promising initial
(e.g., Blumenthal et al., 2 0 0 2 ; Naiura
data, motivational interviewing may be well
Abrams, 2 0 0 2 ; N I H , 1 9 9 7 ; Ockene et al.,
&
suited to medical settings because it is brief and
2 0 0 0 ) . O n the surface, such findings challenge
appears to be transportable across behavioral
the brief therapy
domains and health care professionals.
care. However, Wadden, Brownell, and Foster
environment o f medical
( 2 0 0 2 ) found that monthly 15-minute sessions conducted during patients' regular medication Cognitive-Behavioral
Therapy
Fairly standard group and individual C B T treatment programs are available for a variety
checks maintained weight loss as effectively as did a m o r e traditional
behavioral
group
therapy program, suggesting that prolonged
of health behaviors. Multicomponent C B T
behavioral interventions can be creatively
shows substantial improvement over minimal
incorporated into health care settings.
education interventions for improving H I V risk behaviors (Kelly & Kalichman, 2 0 0 2 ; NIH, 2002),
1 9 9 7 ) , physical inactivity (Dubbert, and
smoking cessation ( C o m p a s ,
Haaga, Keefe, Leitenberg, & Williams, 1 9 9 8 ; Niaura & Abrams, 2 0 0 2 ) . T h e importance
Interpersonal/Social Support
Interventions
Risk reduction interventions often include procedures
to
enhance
social
support.
Brief Psychotherapies
and Group Treatments
\
Although the methodology and resulting data
At a different level, psychobiological processes
are quite varied, there may be benefits to
related to stress, negative emotions, and social
socially based interventions for risk reduction.
relationships also influence disease progres-
For example, obesity treatments that include
sion. Thus, psychologists in medical settings
spouses result in more weight loss for up to 3
may be in a position to alter disease by inter-
months posttreatment (but not beyond) com-
vening to reduce stress, minimize negative
pared with those that do not (Black, Gleser, &c
emotions, or enhance social support.
Kooyers, 1 9 9 0 ) . Similarly, Epstein and colleagues' ( 1 9 9 0 ) family-based behavioral treatment for childhood obesity has
produced
Psychoeducation
remarkably sustained reductions in obesity
Psychoeducation is a necessary component
and improvements in physical activity for
in the comprehensive treatment o f chronic ill-
up to 1 0 years. Such studies demonstrate the
nesses. Such interventions provide information
utility o f embedding risk reduction into the
about the cause, course, and treatment o f dis-
broader
risk
eases as well as stress management (e.g., relax-
social
contexts
in
which
behaviors occur.
ation), coping, and illness management skills.
There also may be benefits to reducing
Although not sufficient for all patients, psy-
risk behaviors by promoting social support
choeducation improves adherence to treatment
more directly. Wing and Jeffery ( 1 9 9 9 ) found
recommendations
that support enhancement procedures (e.g.,
improves health outcomes (for a review, see
recruitment with friends, intragroup activities,
Roter et al., 1 9 9 8 ) . Such interventions have
and, to a lesser extent,
provision and receipt of support, intergroup
been reported to yield clinically meaningful
competitions) improved the effectiveness of
improvement in indexes o f blood glucose
behavioral group therapy for weight loss. In a
control among patients with type 2 diabetes
very different context, Kelly and colleagues
(i.e., at a magnitude
( 1 9 9 3 ) found that depressed patients with
diabetes complications [Norris, Lau, Smith,
that reduces serious
supportive-expressive
Schmid, & Engelgau, 2 0 0 2 ] ) and to decrease
group intervention with reductions in depres-
fatal and nonfatal myocardial infarction over a
H I V responded
to a
sion, maladaptive interpersonal insecurities,
10-year period among those with coronary
and unsafe sex as compared with controls
heart disease (Dusseldorp, van Elderen, M a e s , Meulman, & Kraaij, 1 9 9 9 ) . Dusseldorp and
with no treatment.
colleagues ( 1 9 9 9 ) specifically found that psychoeducation
Improving Disease Outcomes Psychosocial interventions with
improved
disease
outcomes
among coronary heart disease patients primarmedical
ily if it altered the behavioral or psychosocial
patients may also be aimed at improving hard
risk factors hypothesized to mediate interven-
indicators o f disease outcomes (e.g., mortality,
tion effects. Psychoeducation appears equally
cardiac events, blood glucose levels, immune
beneficial across group or individual formats
functioning) by targeting behavioral or psy-
and when delivered by different health care
chosocial risk. Because behaviors are integral
providers (e.g., Linden, Stossel, & Mourice,
to the treatment of many medical conditions,
1 9 9 6 ; Norris et al., 2 0 0 2 ; Roter et al., 1 9 9 8 ) .
psychologists may be called on to improve
These impressive findings are qualified by
adherence to medical interventions or to help
evidence that it is difficult to maintain positive
patients meet the complex self-regulatory and
outcomes over time and that such interven-
lifestyle demands o f chronic illness (e.g., dia-
tions are more effective with more intensive or
betes self-management, home dialysis skills).
prolonged interventions
(cf. Norris et al.,
63
64
PRACTICAL ISSUES 2 0 0 2 ; Roter et al., 1 9 9 8 ) . Innovative methods
(Linden et al., 1 9 9 6 ) . In addition, brief group
to maximize efficiency without limiting effec-
therapy employing multicomponent, cogni-
tiveness are currently being explored. Brief
tive-behavioral stress management
(CBSM)
office-based interventions that can readily be
(e.g., six to eight sessions o f illness education,
incorporated
cognitive restructuring, coping skills training,
appearing,
into routine visits are
now
sometimes taking advantage o f
interactive computer technology. For example,
relaxation or anxiety management,
and/or
provision o f social support) appears to be
Glasgow and colleagues ( 1 9 9 7 ) reported that
quite promising. As examples, group C B S M
having the health care team review individual-
has been found to (a) enhance blood glucose
ized, computer-generated information regard-
control at 1-year follow-up among patients
ing patient goals and motivation for dietary
with type 2 diabetes (Surwit et al., 2 0 0 2 ) ,
behaviors resulted in reduced serum choles-
(b)
terol levels over a 1-year period among patients
those with malignant melanomas (Fawzy et al.,
reduce
6-year mortality rates
among
1 9 9 3 ) , and (c) promote more positive emo-
with type 2 diabetes.
tional and immunological functioning among individuals infected with H I V (e.g., Antoni Cognitive-Behavioral
Therapy
et al., 2 0 0 0 ; Ironson et al., 1 9 9 4 ) . These
Several highly publicized studies
have
demonstrated that intensive, long-term C B T can improve disease processes. For example, Ornish and colleagues ( 1 9 9 0 , 1 9 9 8 ) found that a multicomponent behavioral intervention for C H D patients (i.e., stress management, group therapy, and intensive changes in diet and exercise) resulted in regression o f coronary atherosclerosis and reductions in the recur-
intriguing data are qualified by notable nonreplications and mixed results in the broader literature. Miller and Cohen ( 2 0 0 1 ) suggested that psychotherapy may have stronger effects when patients are highly distressed and when the disease condition is not overwhelming the psychobiological process. This possibility suggests that psychosocial interventions to improve disease outcomes should occur while
rence o f coronary events compared with usual
the biological system is still malleable among
care. Friedman and colleagues' ( 1 9 8 6 ) well-
patients who are seriously distressed.
known Recurrent Coronary Prevention Project
Brief C B T for depression (Beck, Rush, Shaw,
randomized cardiac patients to group C B T
8c Emery, 1 9 7 9 ) is also important for medical
consisting o f relaxation training, stress man-
populations. Although many patients adapt
agement, and cognitive restructuring to reduce
well to disease, depression is fairly common
coronary-prone behavior. T h e
among
intervention
primary
care patients
(Katon
8c
yielded diminished Type A behavior and a
Schulberg, 1 9 9 2 ) and is clearly associated with
4 4 % reduction in the recurrence o f nonfatal
poorer disease progression (e.g., Frasure-Smith,
cardiac events compared with usual care.
Lesperance, 8c Talajic, 1 9 9 5 ) . Brief C B T effec-
The intensity o f these interventions makes
tively reduces
depression
among
medical
it unlikely they can be used on a wide-scale
patients
(Coyne, Thompson, Klinkman,
basis, but their success has spurred attempts to
Nease, 2 0 0 2 ; Schulberg et al., 1 9 9 6 ) , with some
&
identify briefer interventions to improve dis-
evidence that it also improves medical out-
ease outcomes. Although single-component
comes. Lustman, Griffith, Kissel, and Clouse
stress management interventions (e.g., relax-
( 1 9 9 8 ) , for example, found that 1 0 weeks of
ation) are not broadly effective at altering dis-
individual C B T for depression among patients
ease progression (Linden 8c Chambers, 1 9 9 4 ) ,
with diabetes improved blood glucose control
they do appear to enhance the effectiveness o f
over the subsequent 6 months compared with
standard medical rehabilitation
diabetes education. Nevertheless, there are
programs
Brief Psycbotberapies
and Group Treatments
\
nonreplications o f the medical
psychosocial adjustment but did not prolong
effects o f C B T for depression. In a recently
survival (Goodwin et al., 2 0 0 1 ) . Additional
completed multicenter trial, cardiac patients
research will be necessary to détermine whether
disturbing
who met criteria for depression or low social
and when social support interventions in gen-
support were randomized to C B T or usual care
eral, and supportive-expressive group therapy
(National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute,
in particular, improve disease outcomes.
2 0 0 1 ) . T h e intervention lessened depression and improved social support but did not have an overall effect on subsequent cardiac events.
T h e adaptive demands o f medical illnesses
Interpersonal/Social Support
and
Interventions
Interpersonal
Maximizing Functioning and Improving Quality of Life
therapy
treatments
can
be b u r d e n s o m e
for
patients and their families, exacting high depression
costs in their emotional, social, occupational,
(Klerman, Weissman, Rounsaville, & Chevron,
for
and financial well-being. T h e experience o f
1 9 8 4 ) also reduces depression among primary
illness and its treatments may cause pain and
care patients (Schulberg etal., 1 9 9 6 ) . T o the
disability, alter important social and occupa-
extent that such reductions can alter important
tional roles, erode
biobehavioral or psychobiological processes,
resources, and engender hopelessness, fear,
interpersonal therapy may eventually prove to
and depression. In some cases,
be useful for improving the health o f medical
medical management o f illnesses yields c o m -
financial and
coping
improved
patients. At a broader level, provision of social
parable declines in patients' quality o f life. In
support is a common feature o f the multicom-
other cases, illnesses cannot be changed, but
ponent C B S M therapies described earlier and is
suffering and disability c a n be reduced. T h u s ,
hypothesized to be vital to the success of C B S M
psychologists working in health care settings
in improving physical health. This possibility,
are often called on to improve patients' e m o -
however, has not been carefully evaluated; the
tional well-being (e.g., decrease depression
effects o f support enhancement on disease out-
and anxiety), symptom management
comes have primarily been studied in the con-
reduce pain or treatment side effects), and
text of multicomponent therapies designed to
m o r e general quality o f life (e.g., return to
improve psychosocial adjustment. There are, o f
activities o f daily living, decreased isolation).
(e.g.,
course, dramatic demonstrations that support interventions can improve disease. Goodkin and colleagues ( 1 9 9 9 ) recently reported that
Psychoeducation
HIV-positive gay men who randomly received
Educational interventions are commonly
1 0 weeks of group-based bereavement counsel-
used as a first step toward improving the func-
ing were buffered against the increase in H I V
tioning o f patients dealing with chronic or life-
viral load displayed by the normal care control.
threatening illnesses. These brief interventions
Spiegel, Bloom, Kraemer, and Gottheil ( 1 9 8 9 )
seek to improve patients' capacities for coping
found
that women with metastatic breast
by enhancing their understanding o f the cause,
cancer who engaged in 1 year o f supportive-
treatment, and course o f their diseases as well
expressive group therapy had longer survival
as of their coping options. Although the liter-
times than did those in the control group.
ature is difficult to evaluate given heterogene-
Nevertheless, a large randomized clinical trial
ity in method and quality, there is reason to
to replicate this effect recently revealed that sup-
believe that educational interventions
portive-expressive group
useful. Psychoeducation groups have been
therapy
improved
are
66
PRACTICAL ISSUES reported to have positive effects on emotional
For example, techniques such as relaxation,
and functional adjustment and/or pain and
biofeedback, guided imagery, and hypnosis
discomfort
appear to be useful for reducing pain and
among
patients
with
cancer
(Bottomley, 1 9 9 7 ; Helgeson & Cohen, 1 9 9 6 ;
nausea
Meyer & M a r k , 1 9 9 5 ) , diabetes (Clement,
(Compas et al., 1 9 9 8 ) . Specific behavioral
associated with cancer
1 9 9 5 ) , and coronary heart disease (Linden
techniques, such as relaxation, C B T , and
et al., 1 9 9 6 ) as well as potentially many
some
others. At least in the context o f cancer, these
deemed to be efficacious for treating migraine
effective
and tension headache pain (Holroyd, 2 0 0 2 ) ,
interventions appear to be more
forms
treatments
o f b i o f e e d b a c k , have
been
among those who are most in need (e.g.,
and multicomponent C B T (i.e., relaxation,
highly distressed) and when they are delivered
cognitive restructuring, coping skills training,
&
and goal setting) appear to be effective at
1 9 9 6 ; Kiecolt-Glazer, M c G u i r e ,
improving pain, physical activity, and psy-
early in the disease process (Helgeson Cohen,
Robles, & Glaser, 2 0 0 2 ) , suggesting that early
chological
screening to identify and then treat vulnerable
arthritis (for reviews, see Compas et al., 1 9 9 8 ;
distress
among
patients
with
patients may be useful.
Keefe et al., 2 0 0 2 ) .
Psychoeducational approaches have also
In contrast to their qualified success at
been developed to improve patients' reactions
improving disease outcomes, C B T for depres-
to stressful medical procedures and to inter-
sion and the multicomponent C B S M group
acting more generally with the health care
therapies
system. It is well established, for example, that
improve patients' psychosocial well-being.
orienting patients to painful
earlier
consistently
difficult
C B T is effective at treating major depression
(e.g., surgery, c h e m o -
among patients who seek treatment in pri-
therapy) via sensory, procedural, and coping
mary care, regardless o f whether or not there
information results in improved recovery (e.g.,
is a concomitant presenting medical condition
medical procedures
and
described
reduced hospital stays, pain medication, and
(Schulberg et al., 1 9 9 6 ) . Similarly, C B S M
anxiety [Contrada, Leventhal, & Anderson,
appears to enhance emotional functioning,
1 9 9 4 ] ) . Educational interventions to improve
coping abilities, functional abilities, and/or
patients' interactions with health care profes-
quality o f life among patients coping with
sionals have also shown benefits for patient
cancer (Meyer & M a r k , 1 9 9 5 ) and H I V (e.g.,
Brief interventions to improve
Lutgendorf et al., 1 9 9 8 ) . W e are also begin-
physician communication (Rutter, Iconomou,
ning to document the psychosocial processes
&C Quine, 1 9 9 6 ) and to help health care pro-
through which C B S M appears to be effective.
adjustment.
fessionals empower patients (Anderson et al.,
Lutgendorf and colleagues ( 1 9 9 8 ) found that
1 9 9 5 ; Gonder-Frederick et al., 2 0 0 2 )
the effects o f C B S M on depression were medi-
increase patient
satisfaction and
may
minimize
disease-induced functional limitations.
ated by altered coping (i.e., increased cognitive coping and social support) in a sample o f HIV-positive gay men.
Cognitive-Behavioral
Therapy
Medical patients often experience painful or debilitating symptoms
associated
with
Interpersonal/Social Support
Interventions
medical treatments and illness. Behavioral
There is a large enough literature with
and cognitive techniques have been used with
cancer patients to conclude that professionally
success to improve symptom management,
run support groups can improve patient qual-
often more effectively than education alone.
ity o f life (Compas et al., 1 9 9 8 ; Hogan et al.,
Brief Psychotherapies 2001;
Bottomley,
1997).
supportive-expressive group
In
particular,
therapy
(e.g.,
and Group Treatments
\
SPECIALIZED TRAINING AND SKILLS
encouragement o f emotional expression, provision and receipt of emotional support, hyp-
Given the complexity o f issues that psychol-
nosis for pain management) has been found to
ogists encounter when working in general
improve m o o d and pain reports among those
health care settings and the array o f treat-
with metastatic breast cancer (Goodwin et al.,
ment options that may be used to meet these
2 0 0 1 ; Spiegel, Bloom, & Y a l o m , 1 9 8 1 ) , and
challenges, h o w does one learn to translate
to be more effective than C B T at reducing
traditional psychological interventions into
m o o d disturbance among patients with H I V
the unique context o f medical care? Excellent
(Kelly et al., 1 9 9 3 ) . Broadly speaking, how-
guidelines for the training and skills neces-
ever, data on the effectiveness o f support
sary to function effectively as a clinical health
groups are fairly inconsistent. This may be
psychologist are available elsewhere (e.g.,
because o f heterogeneity in h o w
support
groups are conducted. For example, there is
Belar & Deardorff, 1 9 9 5 ; Belar et al., 2 0 0 1 ; M c D a n i e l , Belar, Schroeder, Hargrove,
&
reason to believe that peer-run support groups
Freeman, 2 0 0 2 ) and are described in detail in
can yield negative outcomes. Helgeson and
T a b l e 5 . 3 . T h e knowledge and skills most
colleagues ( 2 0 0 0 ) developed a group educa-
unique to the brief therapy environment o f
tion intervention for cancer patients that was
health care settings are highlighted here.
delivered either with or without peer discussion and opportunities
for peer
support.
Relative to controls, the peer support group displayed impaired psychosocial adjustment (e.g., increased negative affect and conflict with family and friends) that was maintained over 6 months.
A biopsychosocial perspective is fundamental to providing brief psychosocial treatments in medical settings. Although specific clinical assessment and intervention skills are necessary, they are woefully insufficient if delivered without consideration o f this broader context. Patients enter the medical system for a variety
Some treatments have focused on improving
of reasons, some of which may be only tan-
emotional well-being and quality of life by inter-
gentially related to their presenting complaints.
vening more directly with interpersonal pro-
Assessing patients as part o f a biopsychosocial
cesses. For example, interpersonal therapy for
system helps the psychologist to untangle the
depression enhances recovery from postpartum
complex web of issues that often present as
depression (O'Hara, Stuart, Goman, & Wenzel,
medical symptoms. Through this process, the
2 0 0 0 ) , a finding that is particularly important
psychologist can more effectively evaluate the
given that postpartum depression causes great
need and prioritize the goals for psychological
individual and family suffering and impairs
interventions and then implement the most
infant development but often is not treated
appropriate level and mode o f intervention. A
pharmacologically if women are breast-feeding.
biopsychosocial perspective is developed not
Relatedly, interventions that promote spousal
only through didactic knowledge of the bio-
caregiving or couples skills (e.g., communica-
logical,
tion, problem solving, engaging in the caregiving
aspects o f health and illness but also through
role) have the potential to aid in managing pain
mentored experiences in health care settings
and reducing depression (Keefe et al., 1 9 9 6 ) ,
serving various medical populations. Such
and an intervention to promote
experiential learning can be crucial to solidify-
teamwork
psychological,
and
sociocultural
between adolescents with diabetes and their
ing a sophisticated understanding
mothers lessens family conflict and improves
dynamic interrelationships among biomedical,
o f the
diabetes management (Anderson et al., 1 9 9 9 ) .
psychological, and sociocultural processes in
PRACTICAL ISSUES Table 5.3
Specialized Skills and Training for Providing Brief Interventions in Health Care Settings
I. In-Depth Knowledge of the Biopsychosocial Model A. Biological aspects of health and illness B. Psychological aspects of health and illness 1. Cognitive aspects of health and illness (e.g., illness models, irrational beliefs) 2. Affective aspects of health and illness (e.g., bidirectional associations between depression and disease) 3. Behavioral aspects of health and illness 4. Developmental aspects of health and illness C. Sociocultural aspects of health and illness D. Knowledge of the dynamic interrelationships among A, B, and C II. Knowledge of Common Conditions and Issues Seen Across Health Care Settings A. Pathophysiology B. Risk factors C. Presenting signs and symptoms D. Diagnostic and treatment procedures E. Prognosis F. Biopsychosocial issues involved in A to Ε III. Skills in the Biopsychosocial Assessment of Common Medical Conditions A. Traditional psychological assessment skills and knowledge of their limitations B. Skill at detecting mental health problems among medical patients C. Knowledge of specialized instruments (e.g., relevant personality traits, coping styles, patient conceptualizations of illness, disease-specific adjustment) D. Knowledge of medical assessment procedures E. Brief interviewing skills F. Triage skills IV. Clinical Skills for Brief Interventions in Health Care A. Individual, couples, family, and group therapy skills B. Psychoeducation knowledge and skills 1. Social learning theory 2. Motivational interviewing C. Multicomponent cognitive behavioral therapy 1. Relaxation training/stress management 2. Problem solving and coping skills training 3. Assertiveness training 4. Cognitive restructuring 5. Brief motivational interventions 6. Relapse prevention D. Supportive therapy skills E. Interpersonal and family systems theory/therapy F. Crisis management V. Interdisciplinary Collaboration Skills A. B. C. D.
Well-developed and positive professional identity as a psychologist Understanding the training, goals, and perspectives of other disciplines Familiarity with the languages of other disciplines Communication and social skills
VI. Awareness of Sociopolitical Issues Across Health Care Settings S O U R C E : Adapted from McDaniel, Belar, Schroeder, Hargrove, and Freeman (2002).
Brief Psychotherapies
and Group Treatments
\
general as well as their specific instantiation
who have not developed this broader expertise
with a given patient. Mentored experiences
may encounter patient problems that they are
across health care settings also provide invalu-
unequipped to handle (cf. Smith, in press).
able training on how to navigate the numerous
Patients with diabetes who have comorbid
challenges o f functioning as a clinical health
eating disorders require more than illness
psychologist in the culture o f medicine (e.g.,
management
communicating with physicians, getting paid
pathology among family members can com-
for psychological services).
pletely undermine the best that psychotherapy
interventions,
and
serious
also
has to offer if it is not recognized or addressed.
develop a detailed understanding o f the med-
Developing this broad expertise in traditional
ical conditions c o m m o n l y encountered across
areas o f clinical psychology can be difficult
Clinical health psychologists must
health care settings and their associated diag-
due to competing demands and the daunting
nostic and treatment procedures. Such infor-
challenges o f learning a rapidly expanding lit-
mation is integral to the patient's context and
erature and developing skills at the interface o f
allows psychologists to identify
psychology and medicine. T h e importance o f
important
biobehavioral or psychobiological processes
doing so, however, has been recognized in the
that may become targets for intervention, to
training guidelines for health psychology (e.g.,
anticipate stressful transitions in the disease
M c D a n i e l et al., 2 0 0 2 ) .
and treatment process, and to work more collaboratively with the interdisciplinary health care team. Nevertheless, clinical health psy-
CONCLUSION
chologists must embed this knowledge in the broader biopsychosocial context and not
Clinical health
lose sight o f their unique perspective on the
remarkable strides in integrating a biopsy-
psychosocial aspects o f medical care.
chosocial perspective into medical care and in
psychologists have
made
psychosocial
developing novel approaches to meet the
interventions in health care settings requires
unique challenges o f health care settings. As
The
delivery
o f effective
psychologists to work collaboratively with
reviewed in this chapter, brief and group
members of an interdisciplinary health care
psychotherapies hold considerable promise in
team—who themselves have unique and some-
this endeavor. Although the emerging litera-
times opposing perspectives on patient care—in
ture evaluating these interventions is favorable,
the context o f a health care system that might
there are nagging inconsistencies with few
not fully appreciate the systemic approach.
examples o f programmatic support for spe-
Although good social skills and knowledge of
cific interventions and few tests o f their effec-
the training and perspectives of other profes-
tiveness in the medical contexts where they are
sions are important, the development of a solid
typically used. In this climate o f hope and cau-
professional identity as a psychologist may be
tion, the biopsychosocial model provides an
most crucial to accomplishing this difficult task
invaluable framework. This model does not
(cf. McDaniel et al., 2 0 0 2 ) .
provide a prescriptive blueprint for working
In this context, one must remember that
with medical patients. Rather, its strength lies
the specialized skills o f the clinical health psy-
in its flexibility to be useful across patients and
chologist emerge out o f general core training
health care settings, its ability to accommo-
in the broader discipline o f psychology and its
date rapidly changing medical technology and
traditional
treatments, and the often necessarily novel
applications to mental
health.
Mental health problems are c o m m o n in health
and creative clinical interventions that emerge
care settings, and clinical health psychologists
from its application.
69
PRACTICAL ISSUES
REFERENCES Andersen, B. L. (2002). Biobehavioral outcomes following psychological interventions for cancer patients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 590-610. Anderson, B. J . , Ho, J . , Brackett, J . , & Laffel, L. M. (1999). An office-based intervention to maintain parent-adolescent teamwork in diabetes management: Impact on parent involvement, family conflict, and glycémie control. Diabetes Care, 22, 7 1 3 - 7 2 1 . Anderson, R. M., Funnell, M. M., Butler, P. M., Arnold, M. S., Fitzgerald, J . T., & Feste, C. C. (1995). Patient empowerment: Results of a randomized controlled trial. Diabetes Care, 18, 9 4 3 - 9 4 9 . Antoni, M. H., Cruess, D. G., Cruess, S., Lutgendorf, S., Kumar, M., Ironson, G., Klimas, N., Fletcher, Μ. Α., &C Schneiderman, N. (2000). Cognitive-behavioral stress management intervention effects on anxiety, 24-hr urinary norepinephrine output, and T-cytotoxic/suppressor cells over time among symptomatic HIVinfected gay men. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 3 1 - 4 5 . Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J . , Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. New York: Guilford. Belar, C. D., Brown, R. Α., Hersch, L. E., Hornyak, L. M., Rozensky, R. H., Sheridan, E. P., Brown, R. T., &c Reed, G. W. (2001). Self-assessment in clinical health psychology: A model for ethical expansion of practice. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 32(2), 1 3 5 - 1 4 1 . Belar, C. D., &C Deardorff, W. W. (1995). Clinical health psychology in medical settings: A practitioner's guidebook. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Black, D. R., Gleser, L. J . , &C Kooyers, K. J . (1990). A meta-analytic evaluation of couples weight-loss programs. Health Psychology, 9, 3 3 0 - 3 4 7 . Blumenthal, J . Α., Sherwood, Α., Gullette, E. C. D., Georgiades, Α., & Tweedy, D. (2002). Biobehavioral approaches to the treatment of essential hypertension. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 5 6 9 - 5 8 9 . Bottomley, A. (1997). Cancer support groups: Are they effective? European Journal of Cancer Care, 6, 1 1 - 1 7 . Burns, S., Cohen, S., Gritz, E., & Kottke, T. (1994). Tobacco and the clinician: Interventions for medical and dental practice (Vol. 5). Bethesda, JVLD: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (1999). Chronic diseases and their risk factors: The nation's leading causes of death. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Clement, S. (1995). Diabetes self-management education. Diabetes Care, 18,1204—1214. Compas, Β. Ε., Haaga, D. Α., Keefe, F. J . , Leitenberg, H., & Williams, D. A. (1998). Sampling of empirically supported psychological treatments from health psychology: Smoking, chronic pain, cancer, and bulimia nervosa. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 8 9 - 1 1 2 . Contrada, R. J . , Leventhal, Ε. Α., &c Anderson, J . R. (1994). Psychological preparation for surgery: Marshalling individual and social resources to optimize selfregulation. International Review of Health Psychology, 3, 2 1 9 - 2 6 6 . Coyne, J . C , &C Racioppo, M . W. (2000). Never the twain shall meet? Closing the gap between coping research and clinical intervention research. American Psychologist, 55, 6 5 5 - 6 6 4 . Coyne, J . C , Rohrbaugh, M . J . , Shoham, V., Sonnega, J . S., Nicklas, J . M., δί Cranford, J . A. (2001). Prognostic importance of marital quality for survival of congestive heart failure. American Journal of Cardiology, 88, 5 2 6 - 5 2 9 .
Brief Psychotherapies
and Group
Treatments
Coyne, J . C , Thompson, R., Klinkman, M . S., Se Nease, D. E., J r . (2002). Emotional disorders in primary care. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 7 9 8 - 8 0 9 . Curry, S. J . , McBride, C , Grottos, L. C , Louie, D., &c Wagner, Ε. H. (1995). A randomized trial of self-help materials, personalized feedback, and telephone counseling with nonvolunteer smokers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 1 0 0 5 - 1 0 1 4 . Dubbert, P. M. (2002). Physical activity and exercise: Recent advances and current challenges. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 5 2 6 - 5 3 6 . Dunn, C , Deroo, L., &c Rivara, F. (2001). The use of brief interventions adapted from motivational interviewing across behavioral domains: A systematic review. Addiction, 96, 1 7 2 5 - 1 7 4 2 . Dusseldorp, E., van Elderen, T., Maes, S., Meulman, J . , & Kraaij, V. (1999). A meta-analysis of psychoeducational programs for coronary heart disease patients. Health Psychology, 18, 5 0 6 - 5 1 9 . Engel, G. L. (1977, April 8). The need for a new medical model: A challenge for biomedicine. Science, 196, 1 2 9 - 1 3 6 . Epstein, L. H., Valoski, Α., Wing, R. R., & McCurley, I. (1990). Ten-year followup of behavioral, family-based treatment for obese children. Journal of the American Medical Association, 264, 2 5 1 9 - 2 5 2 3 . Fawzy, F. L, Fawzy, N., Hyun, C. S., Elashoff, R., Guthrie, D., Fahey, J . L., & Morton, D. (1993). Malignant melanoma: Effects of a structured psychiatric intervention, coping, affective state, and immune parameters on recurrence and survival six years later. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 6 8 1 - 6 8 9 . Fiore, M., Bailey, W., Cohen, S., Dorfman, S. F., Goldstein, M. G., Gritz, E. R., Heyman, R. G., Holbrook, J . , Jaen, C. R., Kottke, T. E., Lando, Η. Α., Mecklenburg, R., Mullen, P. D., Nett, L. M., Robinson, L., Stitzer, M. L., Tommasello, A. C , Villejo, L., &: Wewers, M. E. (1996). Smoking cessation: Clinical Practice Guideline No. 18 (DHHS Publication No. (ADM) 96-0692). Bethesda, M D : Public Health Service, Agency for Health Case Policy and Research. Fordyce, W. E. (1976). Behavioral methods in chronic pain and illness. St. Louis, M O : C. V. Mosby. Frasure-Smith, N., Lesperance, F., &c Talajic, M. (1995). Depression and 18-month prognosis after myocardial infarction. Circulation, 91, 9 9 9 - 1 0 0 5 . Friedman, M., Thoreson, C. E., Gill, J . J . , Ulmer, D., Powell, L. H., Price, V. Α., Brown, B . , & Thompson, L. (1986). Alteration of Type-Α behavior and its effects on cardiac recurrences in post-myocardial infarction patients: Summary results of the Recurrent Coronary Prevention Project. American Heart Journal, 112,653-665. Glasgow, R. E., La Chance, P. Α., Toobert, D. J . , Brown, J . , Hampson, S. E., 8c Riddle, M. C. (1997). Long-term effects and costs of brief behavioral dietary intervention for patients with diabetes delivered from the medical office. Patient Education & Counseling, 32, 1 7 5 - 1 8 4 . Gonder-Frederick, L. Α., Cox, D. J . , & Ritterband, L. M . (2002). Diabetes and behavioral medicine: The second decade. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 6 1 1 - 6 2 5 . Goodkin, K., Blaney, N. T., Feaster, D. J . , Baldewicz, T., Burkhalter, J . , & Leeds, B. (1999). A randomized controlled clinical trial of a bereavement support group intervention in human immunodeficiency virus type 1-seropositive and -seronegative homosexual men. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 5 2 - 5 9 . Goodwin, P. J . , Leeszcz, M., Ennis, M., Koopmans, J . , Vincent, L., Guther, H., Drysdale, E., Hundleby, M., Chochinor, H. M., Navaro, M., Speca, M., St Hunter, J . (2001). The effect of group psychosocial support on survival in metastatic breast cancer. New England Journal of Medicine, 345, 1 7 1 9 - 1 7 2 6 .
PRACTICAL ISSUES Helgeson, V. S., 8t Cohen, S. (1996). Social support and adjustment to cancer: Reconciling descriptive, correlational, and intervention research. Health Psychology, 15, 1 3 5 - 1 4 8 . Helgeson, V. S., Cohen, S., Schulz, R., 8c Yasko, J . (2000). Group support interventions for women with breast cancer: Who benefits from what? Health Psychology, 19, 1 0 7 - 1 1 4 . Hogan, Β. E., Linden, W., 8t Najarian, B . (2001). Social support interventions: Do they work? Clinical Psychology Review, 22, 3 8 1 - 4 4 0 . Holroyd, K. A. (2002). Assessment and psychological management of recurrent headache disorders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 6 5 6 - 6 7 7 . Ironson, G., Friedman, Α., Klomas, N., Antoni, M. H., Fletcher, Μ . Α., Simoneau, J . , LaPerriere, Α., 8c Schneiderman, N. (1994). Distress, denial, and low adherence to behavioral interventions predict faster disease progression in HIV-1 infected gay men. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, I, 9 0 - 1 0 5 . Janis, I. L. (1958). Psychological stress. New York: John Wiley. Kaplan, R. M., St Groessl, E. J . (2002). Applications of cost-effectiveness methodologies in behavioral medicine. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 4 8 2 - 4 9 3 . Katon, W., 8c Schulberg, H. (1992). Epidemiology of depression in primary care. General Hospital Psychiatry, 14, 2 3 7 - 2 4 7 . Keefe, F. J . , Caldwell, D. S., Baucom, D., Salley, Α., Robinson, E., Timmons, K., Beaupré, P., Weisberg, J . , 8c Helms, M. (1996). Spouse-assisted coping training in the management of osteoarthritic knee pain. Arthritis Care and Research, 9, 2 7 9 - 2 9 1 . Keefe, F. J . , Smith, S. J . , Buffington, A. L. H., Gibson, J . , Studts, J . L., &c Caldwell, D. S. (2002). Recent advances and future directions in the biopsychosocial assessment and treatment of arthritis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 6 4 0 - 6 5 5 . Kelly, J . Α., 8c Kalichman, S. C. (2002). Behavioral research with HIV/AIDS primary and secondary prevention: Recent advances and future directions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 6 2 6 - 6 3 9 . Kelly, J . Α., Murphy, D., Bahr, G., Kalichman, S., Morgan, M., Stevenson, L., Koob, J . J . , Brasfield, T. L., &c Bernstein, Β . M. (1993). Outcome of cognitivebehavioral and support group brief therapies for depressed persons diagnosed with HIV infection. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 1 6 7 9 - 1 6 8 6 . Kiecolt-Glaser, J . K., McGuire, L., Robles, T. F., 8c Glaser, R. (2002). Psychoneuroimmunology: Psychological influences on immune function and health. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 5 3 7 - 5 4 7 . Klerman, G. L., Weissman, M. M., Rounsaville, B . J . , 8c Chevron, E. S. (1984). Interpersonal psychotherapy of depression. New York: Basic Books. Linden, W., 8c Chambers, L. (1994). Clinical effectiveness of non-drug treatment for hypertension: A meta-analysis. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 16, 3 5 - 4 5 . Linden, W., Stossel, C , & Mourice, J . (1996). Psychosocial interventions for patients with coronary artery disease: A meta-analysis. Archives of Internal Medicine, 156, 7 4 5 - 7 5 2 . Lustman, P. J . , Griffith, L. S., Kissel, S. S., 8c Clouse, R. E. (1998). Cognitive behavioral therapy for depression in type 2 diabetes mellitus: A randomized, controlled trial. Annals of Internal Medicine, 129, 6 1 3 - 6 2 1 . Lutgendorf, S. K., Antoni, M. H., Ironson, G., Starr, K., Costello, N., Zuckerman, M., Klimas, N., Fletcher, Μ . Α., & Schneiderman, N. (1998). Changes in cognitive coping skills and social support during cognitive behavioral stress management intervention and distress outcomes in somatic HIV seropositive gay men. Psychosomatic Medicine, 60, 2 0 4 - 2 1 4 . McDaniel, S. H., Belar, C. D., Schroeder, C , Hargrove, D. S., 8c Freeman, E. L. (2002). A training curriculum for professional psychologists in primary care. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 33, 6 5 - 7 2 .
Brief Psychotherapies
and Group
Meyer, T. J . , St Mark, M . M . (1995). Effects of psychosocial interventions with adult cancer patients: A meta-analysis of randomized experiments. Health Psychology, 14, 1 0 1 - 1 0 8 . Miller, G., St Cohen, S. (2001). Psychological interventions and the immune system: A meta-analytical review and critique. Health Psychology, 20, 4 7 - 6 3 . Miller, W., & Rollnick, S. (1991). Motivational interviewing: Preparing people to change addictive behavior. New York: Guilford. Morisky, D. M., DeMuth, N. M., Field-Gass, M., Green, L. W., St Levine, D. M . (1985). Evaluation of family health education to build social support for longterm control of high blood pressure. Health Education Quarterly, 12, 3 5 - 5 0 . National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute. (2001). Study finds no reduction in deaths or heart attacks in heart disease patients treated for depression and low social support. [Online]. Retrieved on January 2 0 , 2 0 0 3 , from www.nhlbi.niv.gov/new/press/ 0 1 - l l - 1 3 . h t m . National Institute of Diabetes, Digestive, and Kidney Disease. (2001). Diet and exercise dramatically delay Type 2 diabetes: Diabetes medication Metformin also effective. [Online]. Retrieved on January 2 0 , 2 0 0 3 , from www.niddk. nih.gov/welcome/ releases/8_8_01 .htm to National Institutes of Health. (1997). NIH consensus statement: Interventions prevent HIV risk behaviors. Bethesda, M D : U.S. Public Health Service. National Office of Vital Statistics. (1947). Deaths and death rates for the 10 leading causes of death by sex. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Niaura, R., St Abrams, D. B . (2002). Smoking cessation: Progress, priorities, and prospectus. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 4 9 4 - 5 0 9 . Norris, S. L., Lau, J . , Smith, S. J . , Schmid, C. H., St Engelgau, M. (2002). Self-management education for adults with type 2 diabetes: A meta-analysis of the effects on metabolic control. Diabetes Care, 25, 1 1 5 9 - 1 1 7 1 . Ockene, J . K., Emmons, K. M., Mermelstein, R. J . , Perkins, Κ. Α., Bonollo, D. S., Voorhees, C. C , St Hollis, J . F. (2000). Relapse and maintenance issues for smoking cessation. Health Psychology, 19, 1 7 - 3 1 . O'Hara, M . W., Stuart, S., Gorman, L. L., 8t Wenzel, A. (2000). Efficacy of interpersonal psychotherapy for postpartum depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 1 0 3 9 - 1 0 4 5 . Ornish, D., Brown, S. E., Scherwitz, L. W., Billings, J . H., Armstrong, W. T., Ports, Τ. Α., Gould, K. L., McLanahan, S. M., Kirekeeide, R. L., St Brand, R. J . (1990). Can lifestyle changes reverse coronary heart disease? The Lifestyle Heart Trial. Lancet, 336, 1 2 9 - 1 3 3 . Ornish, D., Scherwitz, L. W., Billings, J . H., Brown, S. E., Gould, K. L., St Merritt, T. A. (1998). Intensive lifestyle changes for reversal of coronary heart disease. Journal of the American Medical Association, 280, 2 0 0 1 - 2 0 0 7 . Pickering, T. G., St Miller, Ν. E. (1977). Learned voluntary control of heart rate and rhythm in two subjects with premature ventricular contractions. British Heart Journal, 39, 1 5 2 - 1 5 9 . Pieterse, M. E., Seydel, E. R., DeVries, H., Mudde, A. N., St Kok, G. J . (2001). Effectiveness of a minimal contact smoking cessation program for Dutch general practitioners: A randomized controlled trial. Preventive Medicine, 32, 1 8 2 - 1 9 0 . Resnicow, K., Dilorio, C , Soet, J . E., Borelli, B., Hecht, J . , St Ernst, D. (2002). Motivational interviewing in health promotion: It sounds like something is changing. Health Psychology, 21, 4 4 4 - 4 5 1 . Roter, D. L., Hall, J . Α., Merisca, R., Nordstrom, B., Cretin, D., St Svarstad, B . (1998). Effectiveness of interventions to improve patient compliance: A metaanalysis. Medical Care, 36, 1 1 3 8 - 1 1 6 1 . Rutter, D. R., Iconomou, G., St Quine, L. (1996). Doctor-patient communication and outcome in cancer patients: An intervention. Psychology and Health, 12, 5 7 - 7 1 .
Treatments
PRACTICAL ISSUES Schafer, R. B . , Keith, P. M., & Schafer, E. (2000). Marital stress, psychological distress, and healthful dietary behavior: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30, 1 6 3 9 - 1 6 5 6 . Schulberg, H. C , Block, M . R., Madonia, M. J . , Scott, C. P., Rodriguez, E., Imber, S. D., Pere, I. J . , Lave, J . , Houck, P. R., & Loulehan, J . L. (1996). Treating major depression in primary care practice: Eight-month clinical outcomes. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53, 9 1 3 - 9 1 9 . Shapiro, D., Tursky, B . , Schwartz, G. E., & Shnidman, S. R. (1971). Smoking on cue: A behavioral approach to smoking reduction. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 12, 1 0 8 - 1 1 3 . Smith, T. W. (in press). On being careful when you get what you wish for: Commentary on "Self-Assessment in Clinical Health Psychology." Prevention and Treatment. Smith, T. W., Kendall, P. C , & Keefe, F. J . (Eds.). (2002). Behavioral medicine and clinical health psychology [special issue]. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70(3). Smith, T. W., Nealey, J . R., & Hamann, H. A. (2000). Health psychology. In C. R. Snyder & R. E. Ingram (Eds.), Handbook of psychological change: Psychotherapy processes and practices for the 21st century (pp. 5 6 2 - 5 9 0 ) . New York: John Wiley. Smith, T. W., & Nicassio, P. M . (1995). Psychological practice: Clinical application of the biopsychosocial model. In P. M. Nicassio &C T. W. Smith (Eds.), Managing chronic illness: A biopsychosocial perspective (pp. 1 - 3 4 ) . Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Spiegel, D., Bloom, J . R., Kraemer, H. C , & Gottheil, E. (1989). Effect of psychosocial treatment on survival of patients with metastatic breast cancer. Lancet, 2, 8 8 8 - 9 0 1 . Spiegel, D., Bloom, J . R., & Yalom, I. (1981). Group support for patients with metastatic cancer: A randomized outcome study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 38, 5 2 7 - 5 3 3 . Spiegel, D., & Diamond, S. (2001). Psychosocial interventions in cancer group therapy techniques. In A. Baum & B. L. Andersen (Eds.), Psychosocial interventions for cancer (pp. 2 1 5 - 2 3 4 ) . Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Spira, J . L. (1997). Group psychotherapy for medically ill patients. New York: Guilford. Strecher, V. J . , Kreuter, M., DenBoer, D. J . , Kobrin, S., Hospers, H. J . , & Skinner, C. S. (1994). The effects of computer-tailored smoking-cessation messages in family practice settings, journal of family Practice, 39, 2 6 2 - 2 7 0 . Stuart, R. B. (1967). Behavioral control of overeating. Behavioral Research and Therapy, 5, 3 5 7 - 3 6 5 . Surwit, R. S., van Tilburg, M . A. L., Zucker, N., McCaskill, C. C , Parekh, P., Feinglos, M., Edwards, C. L., Williams, P., & Lane, J . D. (2002). Stress management improves long-term glycémie control in type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care, 1, 3 0 - 3 7 . Von Bertalanffy, L. (1968). General systems theory. New York: Braziller. Wadden, T. Α., Brownell, K. D., & Foster, G. D. (2002). Obesity: Responding to the global epidemic. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 5 1 0 - 5 2 5 . Wing, R. R., & Jeffery, R. W. (1999). Benefits of recruiting participants with friends and increasing social support for weight loss and maintenance, journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67, 1 3 2 - 1 3 8 . Writing Group for the Activity Counseling Trial. (2001). Effects of physical activity counseling in primary care. Journal of the American Medical Association, 286, 677-687. Zautra, A. J . , Burleson, M. H., Matt, K. S., Roth, S., & Burrows, L. (1994). Interpersonal stress, depression, and disease activity in rheumatoid arthritis and osteoarthritis patients. Health Psychology, 13,139-148.
Part II BEHAVIORS THAT COMPROMISE OVERALL HEALTH STATUS Introduction
to Part II
T
Ihe next seven chapters switch focus from practical issues that clinical health psychologists face in the workplace to patient behaviors that may impair their
-A.
overall health status. M o r e than 2 5 years o f research, clinical practice, and
community-based interventions have shown that positive behavioral changes help people t o feel better physically and emotionally, improve their health status, increase their self-care skills, and improve their ability to live with chronic illness. Behavioral medicine interventions have been shown to reduce health-compromising behaviors and improve clinical outcomes in patients with a variety o f medical problems. This part o f the h a n d b o o k targets specific health-compromising behaviors, such as alcohol and nicotine use, as well as more general health-compromising behaviors that are important to virtually all service areas, such as dealing with medication-seeking behavior and adherence to medical recommendations. In Chapter 6, Erblich and Earleywine review what is k n o w n about genetic, constitutional, and environmental factors that have been implicated in the development o f alcohol dependence. In addition, they discuss the available yet imperfect treatment options for those suffering from alcohol dependence. It is noted that until recently, progress in this field was impeded by the lack o f transdisciplinary idea sharing and
75
T H E HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY HANDBOOK collaboration among behavioral scientists, molecular geneticists, and neuroscientists. Clearly, for a complete and balanced understanding o f this classic biobehavioral phenomenon, expertise from each o f these fields is required. F r o m the standpoint o f clinicians, working with individuals suffering from alcohol dependence can be quite difficult, yet many have improved their health status by eliminating the problems related to alcohol use. In Chapter 7, Cohen and his colleagues describe and define the construct o f nicotine dependence and provide guidance on intervention strategies for treating individuals addicted to nicotine. T h e y note that nicotine dependence appears to develop via the interaction between neurobiological substrates and cognitive, behavioral, and e m o tional domains. Given this multifaceted presentation, clinicians are encouraged to employ a stepped care approach, which begins with brief interventions and progresses to more intensive interventions. Given the pervasiveness o f tobacco use disorders among various patient populations and the health consequences associated with these disorders, treatment o f nicotine dependence is one area in which clinical health psychologists can have a positive impact on their patients' overall health status. In Chapter 8, Faith and T h o m p s o n explore current etiological models, assessment strategies, and intervention methodologies that inform the behavioral management of obesity. In addition, they highlight the importance o f targeting body image disturbances in the treatment o f obesity given that many obese individuals do not achieve their desired weight loss. T h u s , clinicians are encouraged to be honest about the weight loss their patients can expect, and they need to address goals that seem unachievable. Clinicians are encouraged t o be sensitive to their patients' motivation and t o collaboratively establish attainable weight loss goals that will foster a sense o f mastery and self-efficacy. Ultimately, such treatment can lead to personal satisfaction, self-respect, and healthy interpersonal relationships. In Chapter 9 , Barbour, Houle, and Dubbert review the evidence suggesting that physical activity is beneficial in terms o f the prevention and treatment o f disease. Despite such benefits, physical inactivity continues to be a pervasive problem that requires consideration o f environmental factors (e.g., decreases in activity required on the j o b , consuming larger portions during meals) in addition to individual factors. T h e authors also point out that a weakness in this field is the scarcity o f research examining underrepresented populations. T h i s is a major problem given that certain diseases are more prevalent among ethnic minorities. Clinicians are encouraged to promote physical activity in their practice because this could significantly reduce the prevalence of chronic disease and could improve the quality o f life o f their patients. In Chapter 1 0 , Collins and his colleagues define the c o m p l e x construct o f stress, highlighting the key models addressing this health concern, and incorporate h o w these models can be translated into treatment. Although they note that stress in itself may not cause disease, stress has been established as a significant risk factor in the development o f numerous illnesses and has been implicated in aggravating existing diseases. T h e authors note that stress is a significant problem facing clinical health psychologists given that it has been shown to reduce patient compliance, which directly affects treatment outcomes. T h e y also include an overview o f the available treatments for the stress disorders, including cognitive strategies as well as techniques designed to lower and control physiological arousal, anxiety, and muscular tension.
Behaviors
That Compromise
Health
In Chapter 1 1 , Gulliver, Wolfsdorf, and M i c h a s define the problem o f medication seeking, propose a conceptual framework
for understanding
this construct, and
describe assessments and treatments designed to address this troublesome behavior. T h e y point out that clinical health psychologists are frequently called on to intervene not only with patients but also with providers and health care systems to effectively extinguish the problem behavior. T h e chapter concludes with a synopsis o f what is k n o w n and what still needs to be explored so as to meet the field's broad objective o f optimal clinical care. Finally, in Chapter 1 2 , Berlant and Pruitt expand the traditional conceptualization o f adherence to include multiple levels, including patients, providers, and health care organizations. T h e y note that there are numerous influences on patients' behavior within each o f these levels that could m a k e the challenge o f improving adherence appear to be overwhelming. As a result, the case study in the chapter focuses on a multilevel approach to treatment. T h e authors conclude that although it is impossible to have complete control o f patients' behavior, consistent consideration o f the significance o f events that precede and follow behavior at the three levels mentioned will advance adherence enhancement efforts and ultimately affect health outcomes.
5
CHAPTE R
Alcohol
Problems
Causes, Definitions, and Treatments JOEL ERBLICH AND MITCH EARLEYWINE
W
hat causes alcoholism? O r, put
etiologic model s o f addiction and to review
a n o t h er
what is know n about som e o f the more com -
although
way ,
wh y
is it
that
so many people con-
m o n genetic, constitutional, and
learned/
sum e alcohol on a regular basis, only a small
environmental factors that have been impli-
minority becom e dependent? Thi s seemingl y
cated in the pathogenesis of alcohol depen-
straightforward question has bewitche d clini-
dence. A rapprochement of these diverse factors
cians and researchers fo r centuries. Only
may result in a clinically useful workin g mode l
recently, wit h generous help from the disci-
of understanding the risk fo r alcoholism.
plines o f molecular genetics and neuroscience, have behavioral scientists begun to piece together this age-old puzzle . Part of the prob-
DEFINITION S AN D
lem lies in the way in whic h alcohol depen-
DESCRIPTIO N OFA L C O H O L I S M
dence is defined and conceptualized. Anothe r concern is that w e have ye t to identify the
A nosologic consensus is the outcom e of clari-
proverbial "switch"—tha t functional entity
fyin g an etiologic disease pathway. Unfortu-
(biological, psychological, or otherwise) that
nately, the classification of alcoholism, lik e
"transforms" a nondependent consume r of
many other multisymptomati c behavioral dis-
alcohol into one wh o is alcoholic. Bu t perhaps
orders, has been a matter o f som e debate. T h e
mos t critically, the parallel paths of behavioral
classic medical approach employ s the categorical
scientists, molecular geneticists, and neurosci-
disease mode l in whic h alcoholism is concep-
entists have until recently severely limite d the
tualized as being qualitatively distinct relative
transdisciplinary idea sharing and collabora-
to normal "social" drinking (Meyer , 2 0 0 1 ) .
tions that are essential to gaining a complet e
Theorists espousing a categorical point of vie w
and balanced understanding o f the etiology
woul d consider abstinence/nonuse, use, abuse,
of
this classic biobehavioral
phenomenon .
and dependence as conceptually distinct states.
Thi s chapter attempts to provide som e unify -
Als o consistent wit h this approach is Cloninger
ing theme s that appear to be c o m m o n to all
and colleagues' ( 1 9 8 8 ) classic description of
BEHAVIORS THAT COMPROMISE HEALTH "types" o f alcoholics, an approach that has
meaningful distress and occur within the same
support
year. T h e diagnosis requires judgment on the
(Sannibale & Hall, 1 9 9 8 ) . Concerns regard-
clinician's part, but the symptoms tend to be
ing the limited nature o f such categorical
obvious. Each symptom reflects the idea that
received
only
mixed
empirical
approaches have led many to adopt a quantita-
a person requires the drug to function and
tive approach, which stresses that alcohol use
makes maladaptive sacrifices to use it. T h e
lies on a continuum from nonuse to depen-
current diagnosis focuses on consequences
dence (Meyer, 2 0 0 1 ) . Factors such as quantity
rather than on the amount or frequency of
of alcohol consumed, frequency of consump-
consumption. These consequences are (a) tol-
tion, and variability (i.e., regularity with which
erance and (b) withdrawal, which were once
drinking
considered the hallmarks o f dependence. T h e
occurs) move people along
this
continuum, the extreme of which is alcohol
additional symptoms are (c) use that exceeds
dependence (Streissguth, Martin, & Buffington,
initial intention, (d) persistent desire for the
1 9 7 6 ) . Recent discussions about the existence
drug or failed attempts to decrease consump-
of a "switch" that is responsible for transform-
tion, (e) loss o f time related to use, (f) reduced
ing a "normal drinker" into an "alcoholic"
activities because o f consumption, and (g)
(e.g., Tsuang, Bar, Harley, & Lyons, 2 0 0 1 ) , as
continued use despite problems.
well as data suggesting that there is great
Tolerance serves as a hallmark of physiolog-
individual variability within the subset of alco-
ical dependence. It occurs when repeated use of
holics, have led to a blending o f the two
the same dose no longer produces the same
approaches. By the prevailing view, alcoholism
effect. This symptom often indicates extensive
is seen as a qualitatively distinct state, but there
drinking and may motivate continued con-
is a continuum of symptom severity within
sumption. People do not grow tolerant to a
the subgroup of alcoholics (Meyer, 2 0 0 1 ) . In
drug; rather, they grow tolerant to its effects.
addition, quantitative drinking factors (e.g.,
After repeated use, some of the effects of a drug
quantity, frequency, variability) are necessary
may
predictors o f the development o f alcoholism.
Tolerance to the desired effects o f alcohol may
That being said, it must also be acknowledged
encourage people to drink more, and increased
that the putative switch has not yet been identi-
use may coincide with a greater chance for
fied, and as such, most researchers have relied
problems.
on studying quantitative drinking factors as a reasonable surrogate. Therefore, the preponderance of theoretical grist has aimed at understanding why some would drink more than others rather than at directly addressing why some people become alcoholics. For now, the proverbial lamppost shines down on the quantitative approach.
decrease, whereas
others
may
not.
T h e second symptom o f dependence, withdrawal, refers to the discomfort associated with an absence o f the drug. N o two people experience withdrawal
in the same way.
Hallmark signs can range from mild irritability to full-blown hallucinations. Alcohol withdrawal frequently includes tremor, anxiety, craving, and troubled sleep. A severe, palsylike tremor with frequent perceptual aberrations, k n o w n
Alcohol Dependence T h e D S M - I V (Diagnostic
as delirium tremens,
often
accompanies severe withdrawal. and
Statistical
The
DSM-IV
distinguishes
between
fourth edition)
dependence with physiological aspects and
(American Psychiatric Association, 1 9 9 4 )
dependence without physiological aspects. If
Manual
of Mental
Disorders,
defines drug dependence as a collection o f any
tolerance or withdrawal appears among the
three o f seven symptoms. All must create
three required
symptoms, a diagnosis o f
Alcohol
Problems
81
appropriate.
legal, users might not spend considerable time
Nevertheless, even without the presentation
in search of it. Hence, the number of hours
o f tolerance o r withdrawal, individuals m a y
required to qualify for a meaningful loss of time
still receive a diagnosis o f alcohol depen-
remains unclear, making this symptom quite
dence without the specifier "with physiolog-
subjective. A clear-cut case would be anyone
ical dependence." This change in procedure
whose day is devoted to obtaining alcohol,
physiological dependence
is
has made the diagnosis o f alcohol depen-
drinking to the point o f intoxication, and recov-
dence potentially m o r e c o m m o n .
ering from the effects o f alcohol. An individual
The third symptom o f dependence involves
who spends even a portion o f the day (e.g., a few
use that exceeds initial intention. This symp-
hours) on these activities would also qualify. In
tom suggests that individuals may plan to
contrast, an individual who consumes several
have only a couple o f drinks but then drink
drinks an hour before going to bed each night
markedly more once they become intoxicated.
might argue that he or she has lost little time and
Use that exceeds intention was once known as
should not qualify for this symptom. Thus, sub-
"loss o f control." M a n y people misinterpreted
jective assessment o f a meaningful amount of
the idea o f loss o f control, suggesting that it
time may contribute to problems with the diag-
referred to an unstoppable
nosis o f dependence.
compulsion to
drink everything available. Based on this interpretation, people who drank to the point o f
The
sixth symptom
o f dependence
is
reduced activities because o f drinking. This
blackout but still had liquor in the house the
symptom focuses on work, relationships, and
next morning might have claimed that they did
leisure. T h e presence o f this symptom suggests
not show loss of control. Today, use that
that alcohol has taken over so much of one's
exceeds intention does not imply this dramatic
daily life that the user would qualify as depen-
unconscious consumption. This symptom sim-
dent. Any impairment in j o b performance
ply suggests that dependent users may have
because o f intoxication, hangover, or devoting
trouble drinking only a small amount if that is
work hours to obtaining alcohol would qualify
what they intend to do. Ironically, people who
for this symptom. Individuals missing work
never intend to drink a small amount might
every M o n d a y to recover from weekend binges
not get the opportunity to qualify for this
might also qualify. Sufficient functioning at
symptom.
work, however, does not indicate that one is
Dependence also includes failed attempts to
not dependent. Even with phenomenal j o b
decrease use, or a constant desire for the drug,
performance, impaired social functioning may
as the fourth symptom. An inability to reduce
be indicative of problems. If a drinker's only
drinking despite a wish to do so certainly sug-
friends are drinking buddies and they only
gests that the drug has altered behavior mean-
socialize while intoxicated, the substance has
ingfully. Y e t people with no motivation to quit
obviously had a marked impact on friendships.
would likely never qualify for failed attempts.
Recreational functioning is also important to
Thus, people who have not attempted to quit
the diagnosis. A decrease in leisure activities
may still qualify for this symptom if they show
suggests impaired recreation. A drinker who
a persistent continuous craving. An inability to
formerly enjoyed hiking, reading, and theater
stop drinking or a constant desire to consume
but who now spends all o f his or her free time
alcohol suggests dependence.
intoxicated in front o f the television would
The fifth symptom of dependence involves
qualify for this symptom. This approach to the
loss o f time related to use. T h e time lost can be
diagnosis implies that drinkers who are not
devoted to experiencing intoxication, recovering
experiencing multifaceted lives can improve the
from it, or seeking the drug. Because alcohol is
way in which they function by drinking less.
82
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH The final symptom of dependence requires continued use despite problems. People who
recovery
from
i n t o x i c a t i o n , and/or
time
devoted to searching for liquor. T h e definition
persist in using the drug despite obvious nega-
is necessarily broad so as to include people
tive consequences would qualify for this symp-
with a variety o f responsibilities. Specifically,
tom. Recurrent use regardless of continued
this symptom applies to employees who miss
occupational, social, interpersonal, psychologi-
work because they have hangovers, students
cal, and/or health problems obviously shows
w h o fail tests because they attend classes
dependence. M a n y of these difficulties involve
intoxicated, and parents w h o neglect their
meaningful
children so that they can spend time in bars.
others
in
the
drinker's
life.
Continued consumption despite conflicts with
T h e second symptom requires intoxication
loved ones, employers, and/or family members
in unsafe settings. T h e D S M - I V specifically
might qualify for this symptom. This situation
lists driving a car and operating machinery as
supports the idea that anyone who continues to
hazardous
use despite negative consequences (e.g., stom-
could
situations in which intoxication
create
dangerous
negative
conse-
ach ulcers, feelings of guilt, loss of self-respect)
quences. M a n y experienced drinkers claim
must have a strong commitment to alcohol.
that their intoxicated driving differs little from their sober driving. Such statements
may
reflect poorly on their driving abilities in
Alcohol Abuse
general, but people w h o tremble as a result o f
A subset of individuals may experience neg-
withdrawal might actually drive better after a
ative consequences from alcohol that do not
couple o f drinks. Despite this fact, driving a
qualify for a diagnosis o f dependence but that
car while drunk, even for only a few blocks,
meet criteria for a diagnosis o f abuse. This diag-
qualifies as alcohol abuse.
nosis requires significant impairment or distress
T h e intoxicated performance o f any task
directly related to drinking. A diagnosis of
can lead to a diagnosis o f abuse if impair-
alcohol abuse requires only one of the four
ment may lead to negative consequences.
symptoms that appear in the D S M - I V : (a) inter-
This action need not be as elaborate as
ference with major obligations, (b) intoxication
scaling a skyscraper or handling a firearm.
in unsafe settings, (c) legal problems, and
Driving a forklift or using power tools might
(d) continued use in the face o f problems. Each
qualify. N o t e that no negative consequences
of these signs requires some interpretation on
actually need to occur; their increased likeli-
the part of the individual making a diagnosis;
h o o d alone can qualify for abuse. Thus,
however, most experienced diagnosticians
those w h o drive drunk but never receive tick-
agree on who meets criteria for substance abuse
ets or have accidents would still qualify for
and who does not (Ustiin et al., 1 9 9 7 ) . Abuse
abuse due to the fact that they increase
remains distinctly separate from dependence,
their likelihood o f experiencing negative
which requires different symptoms and more of
consequences.
them. Although a diagnosis of abuse clearly
T h e third symptom included in the diag-
serves as a sign of genuine troubles, many clin-
nosis o f alcohol abuse concerns legal prob-
icians consider dependence to be more severe.
lems. T h i s symptom may say as much about
Thus, those who qualify for dependence would
society's values as it does about an individ-
not receive the diagnosis of abuse.
ual's behavior (Brecher, 1 9 7 2 ; Grilly, 1 9 9 8 ) .
T h e first symptom o f abuse, interference with major obligations, requires
impaired
Any legal troubles related to public intoxication, driving while intoxicated, drunk and
performance at work, home, and/or school.
disorderly behavior, alcohol-related aggres-
Impairments may arise due to intoxication,
sion, or underage drinking would qualify.
Alcohol Finally, the fourth
Problems
83
symptom of alcohol
someone is an addict. Instead, the clinician
abuse concerns consistent use despite problems.
and the client can focus on reducing the harm
Note that recurrent use in the face o f occupa-
that alcohol may cause.
tional, social, interpersonal, psychological, and/or health problems qualifies as abuse. MODELS OF ALCOHOLISM
Alcohol Problems
As with many other topics in psychology, there
Describing alcohol-related difficulties as
are nearly as many theories of the development
addiction, abuse, or dependence creates certain
of alcohol problems as there are theorists. By
misunderstandings. All three words may sound
and large, however, there is agreement that
deprecating (Eddy, Halbach, Isbell, & Seevers,
people drink alcohol because it makes them feel
1 9 6 5 ; Miller, Gold, & Smith, 1 9 9 7 ) , and each
good. Principles o f operant conditioning sug-
lacks clarity; however,
gest that either positive reinforcement, negative
addiction
has
no
accepted definition. As noted previously, abuse
reinforcement, or a combination o f the two
and dependence have formal definitions, but
play a role in drinking behavior. Some data
the specific diagnosis does not reveal an indi-
support the role o f positive reinforcement in
vidual's actual problems. Anyone who qualifies
alcohol consumption. For example, Newlin
for abuse may have one or more o f the four
and Thomson (1990) argued that individuals
symptoms required, meaning that an individual
with a positive family history for alcoholism
with such a diagnosis could be experiencing any
may be more sensitive to the positive/stimulant
one o f more than a dozen combinations of
effects o f alcohol, and several studies support
symptoms. Likewise, dependence requires three
this supposition
of seven symptoms, providing more than 3 0
Erblich, & Bovbjerg, in press). Research in this
(e.g., Erblich, Earleywine,
potential combinations o f symptoms. These
area has also underscored the importance of
terms may also encourage the rmnimization o f
negative reinforcement in understanding alco-
problems that do not qualify for a diagnosis,
hol consumption. Nearly a half century ago,
and this can interfere with treatment.
Conger
( 1 9 5 6 ) advanced the n o w
classic
People experiencing negative consequences
"tension reduction hypothesis," which speaks
from alcohol may prove to be unwilling to
broadly to alcohol's negatively reinforcing
limit consumption if they do not qualify for
properties.
addiction, abuse, or dependence. This limita-
the tension reduction hypothesis have focused
M o r e recent modifications
to
tion has inspired an approach that emphasizes
on alcohol's ability to dampen the human
problems rather than diagnoses or diseases.
stress response (Levenson, Sher, Grossman,
Thus, instead o f worrying about whether a
Newman, &c Newlin, 1 9 8 0 ) , and further mod-
specific user qualifies for a disorder, time
ifications
might be better spent identifying individual
response dampening may be mediated by alco-
have
demonstrated
that
stress
problems related to drinking. For example, a
hol's impairment o f cognitive processes (Erblich
client may report frequent stomach pains. A
& Earleywine, 1 9 9 5 ; Josephs & Steele, 1 9 9 0 ) .
survey o f this person's drinking may reveal
Regardless o f the mechanism, reinforcement
that the pain often follows a binge. Although
appears to play a central role in the initiation
this problem might not interfere enough to
and maintenance of drinking behavior.
qualify for abuse, the client may benefit from
Over the past two decades, Schuckit and
drinking less or quitting. This emphasis on
colleagues (e.g., Schuckit, 1 9 9 4 ; Schuckit,
problems may allow the clinician to avoid
Tsuang, Anthenelli, Tipp, &
pointless arguments about whether or not
1 9 9 6 ) have presented considerable empirical
Nurnberger,
84
BEHAVIORS THAT C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH evidence (both cross-sectional and longitudinal)
account for some o f the observed intergenera-
indicating that drinkers w h o experience lower
tional transmissibility o f alcoholism, the
levels o f response to alcohol consumption are
confluence o f these epidemiological and quan-
more likely to experience problem drinking.
titative genetic studies suggests a preeminent
They have suggested that such
individuals
may need to drink more than others to achieve
role o f genetics in conferring vulnerability to problem drinking.
a desirable level of reinforcement or that such
In 1 9 9 0 , Blum and colleagues became the
individuals' lower interoceptive responses to
first to discover a relationship between a spe-
the substance make it more difficult for them
cific genotype and alcoholism. A long tradition
to regulate intake appropriately. Conversely,
of research in neuroscience has implicated
others have suggested that those w h o experi-
dopamine as the central nervous system (CNS)
ence higher levels of response to alcohol con-
neurotransmitter o f reward, and studies have
sumption are more likely to develop problem
demonstrated that drug use is associated with
drinking
increased C N S dopamine release. Based on this
(e.g., Nagoshi &
Wilson, 1 9 8 7 ) .
These theorists have proposed that the more
research, Blum and colleagues ( 1 9 9 0 ) tested
reinforcing the effects o f alcohol, the more
the possibility that polymorphisms (i.e., geno-
likely one is to consume. Newlin and T h o m s o n
typic variants) in the dopamine D
( 1 9 9 0 ) proposed that both may be the case;
gene ( D R D 2 ) would be related to alcoholism.
that is, lower levels o f response to the aversive
Indeed, they found
effects o f alcohol, coupled with higher levels
were significantly more likely to carry the
of response to its positive effects, create a
D R D 2 " A l " allele compared with controls.
"double w h a m m y " risk factor for problem
They suggested that this locus may be related
drinking. Subsequent empirical studies have
to a lower number o f D
provided some support for their model (e.g.,
in hypodopaminergic function that could be
Erblich et al., in press). T h e prevailing view
alleviated by, among other things, alcohol con-
remains that
the reinforcement
that
2
2
receptor
severe alcoholics
receptors, resulting
value o f
sumption. This suggestion may be consistent
alcohol figures prominently in understanding
with the overall reinforcement model o f risk
problem drinking. extension, would
A critical question,
by
for alcoholism, such that carriers o f this
be the following: W h a t
polymorphism may find consuming alcohol
factors contribute to differential
levels o f
alcohol's reinforcement value?
more rewarding than do noncarriers. Whether or not this is the case remains to be seen. Strikingly, studies o f genetics have typically not included assessments o f perceived levels
Specific Genetic Factors
of reinforcement, so that intuitive relations
Quantitative genetic studies have demon-
between genotype and reinforcement remain
strated in a compelling fashion that alcoholism
largely speculative. Another concern is that
has a substantial, but not an
exhaustive,
molecular biology has, to date, procured only
heritable component.
Troughton,
Cadoret,
sketchy evidence that the D R D 2
polymor-
O ' G o r m a n , and Heywood ( 1 9 8 6 ) estimated
phism is functional; that is, carrying the A l
that up to 6 0 % o f the population's variability
allele does not necessarily translate to fewer D
in alcoholism is attributable to genetic factors.
receptors. Therefore, the mechanism through
Other epidemiological studies have estab-
which D R D 2 - A 1 confers increased risk for
lished that individuals w h o have an alcoholic
alcoholism remains unclear.
parent are three to four times more likely to develop
alcoholism themselves.
exogenous
Although
(i.e., nongenetic) factors
may
2
Nevertheless, Blum and colleagues' ( 1 9 9 0 ) initial findings have spurred an intensive search for other candidate genotypes that may predict
Alcohol problem drinking. Blum and colleagues ( 2 0 0 0 ) have since tested other
dopamine-related
genotypes, including polymorphic loci
Problems
renders some drinkers particularly susceptible to chronic hypodopaminergic
states. This
on
possibility is particularly intriguing as data
D R D 4 and S L C 6 A 3 , a gene that generates the
emerge suggesting that, within the dopamine
protein responsible for regulating presynaptic
system, the D
dopamine reuptake. Other candidate genes
liking, whereas D
(e.g., S L C 6 A 4 , 5 H T - 1 B , GABA-A, m u O R ,
wanting (Berridge & Robinson, 1 9 9 8 ; Self,
t
subsystem is associated with 2
is more associated with
P E N K ) include those related to serotonin func-
1 9 9 8 ) . T h e convergence o f these data may
tion, G A B A function, and opioid release (for a
suggest that psychopharmacological agents
review, see Blum et al., 2 0 0 0 ) . Studies have
with differential affinities to D j and D
provided mixed results, and even the positive
prove to be selectively efficacious depending
2
may
studies account for only a small proportion o f
on the particular need o f the drinker (e.g., a D
variance in alcoholism or drinking, with sub-
genetically "vulnerable" person may
stantial heterogeneity. Findings underscore the
more craving management).
importance o f polygenic or gene-environment interactions
in
better
understanding
this
complex behavioral disorder. Indeed, early biochemical research (Davis & Walsh, 1 9 7 0 ) has suggested that by-products o f alcohol's metabolism (i.e., tetrahydroisoquinolines) may cause a cascade that directly impinges on opioid receptors but that also indirectly affects the breakdown and availability o f synaptic dopamine. Although not yet tested, w o r k by Berridge and Robinson ( 1 9 9 8 ) raised the possibility that genes related to dopamine function may operate by increasing the motivational salience o f the substance (e.g., craving or "wanting"), whereas relevant polymorphisms in opioid genes may operate by increasing the hedonic value o f consumption (e.g., actual reward or "liking"). Although perhaps a way off, possession o f these genotypes may suggest distinct loci o f intervention
(i.e., craving
management therapy for carriers o f dopaminerelated high-risk genotypes vs. opiate antago-
A final set of candidate genotypes that has been examined include those genes responsible for generating alcohol metabolic enzymes (e.g., alcohol dehydrogenase, acetaldehyde dehydrogenase, P 4 5 0 liver enzymes in the cytochrome system) (Higuchi, Muramatsu,
Matsushita,
Murayama, & Hayashida, 1 9 9 6 ) . Polymorphic loci on these genes (e.g., A L D H 2 , A D H 2 , A D H 3 , C Y P 2 E 1 ) are subjects o f continued scrutiny and may also relate to the magnitude of the hedonic response to alcohol consumption. Because stress is a potent antecedent o f alcohol consumption, examination o f genetic factors that relate to the stress response (e.g., Cortisol regulation) may be a promising avenue in the future. Clearly, the preliminary search for candidate genotypes has yielded only modest
results. Genome-wide
microarray
technology may prove to be highly useful in elucidating the roles o f multiple genes in animal models o f alcoholism.
nist therapy or counterconditioning for carriers
Cognitive Factors
o f opioid-related high-risk genotypes). Along similar lines, recent studies charac-
There is currently a large body o f research
terizing the dysregulation o f C N S functional
demonstrating that individuals with a genetic
systems through chronic alcohol use have
predisposition to alcoholism display substan-
demonstrated
of
tial cognitive and neuropsychological deficits.
both the D j and D receptor systems (Self &
Giancola and M o s s ( 1 9 9 8 ) argued that cogni-
striking down-regulation 2
2
need
Nestler, 1 9 9 8 ) . T o the extent that genetics
tive and neuropsychological deficits, especially
may play a role in receptor density, a potential
those related to executive functioning
gene-environment interaction may exist that
predate drinking experiences (e.g., attention,
that
86
BEHAVIORS THAT C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH appropriate
which problem drinking develops. Speculation
inhibition), may somehow be related to the
aside, the precise mechanism through which
development
cognitive and neuropsychological deficits lead
planning,
Alterman,
cognitive flexibility,
o f alcoholism. F o r example,
Gerstley, Goldstein, and
Tarter
to alcoholism remains unclear. In addition,
( 1 9 8 7 ) reported that "children o f alcoholics"
whether these deficits are genetic or environ-
perform more poorly on tasks that putatively
mental in origin is also unclear. Nevertheless,
assess frontal lobe functioning such as the
these factors are important to consider when
Stroop task, the Trail M a k i n g task, and the
developing an etiologic model o f alcoholism.
Wisconsin Card Sort task. Studies of stimulusevoked potentials, especially the P 3 0 0 component (Rodriguez, Porjesz, Chorlian, Polich, & Begleiter, 1 9 9 9 ) , have provided
Characterologic Factors
converging
It is now well established that specific
biological support for the notion that children
personality factors are strongly predictive of
of alcoholics display poorer attentional capac-
drinking behavior. Nearly four decades ago,
ities than do other children. In contrast to the
MacAndrew
predictors mentioned previously, these cogni-
items on the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality
tive predictors
Inventory
do not necessarily directly
(1967)
(MMPI),
identified primarily
clusters o f related
to
operate through differential reinforcement. A
deviance proneness, that significantly differen-
likely explanation is that although
tiated alcoholics from
drinkers
with cognitive deficits experience comparable
nonalcoholics. This
early research was one o f the first systematic
levels o f reinforcement from alcohol to those
investigations of the potential role of personal-
of drinkers without such deficits, the former
ity characteristics in problem drinking. Since
lack the cognitive resources to regulate their
then, the MacAndrew Alcoholism Scale and
intake or to say " n o " when offered a drink.
the Holmes Alcoholism Scale have become
This problem may become particularly pro-
mainstays of risk assessment for alcoholism.
nounced when high-risk drinkers, w h o are
Recent modifications have found that shorter
already mildly cognitively deficient, become
versions of these scales (7 to 13 items) may be
intoxicated, further undermining their ability
even more strongly related to alcoholism
to process information or to attend to internal
(Conley
or external intake regulation cues. T h e possi-
Lumry, Harrison, & Lessard, 1 9 8 4 ) . Problem
bility also exists that cognitive deficits are
drinking has been related to other measures
&
Kammeier,
1980;
Hoffman,
epiphenomenal to a broader relation between
of deviance proneness as well. For example,
chronic hypofrontality (which may, in fact, be
several studies
related to the reinforcement value o f alcohol)
drinkers, alcoholics, and children o f alcoholics
and future drinking behavior. Alternatively,
score significantly more pathologically on the
Erblich and Earleywine ( 1 9 9 9 ) suggested that
Socialization scale of the California Personality
have found
that
problem
such deficits may also stem from the more
Inventory (e.g., Finn, Sharkansky, Brandt, &
general effects o f growing up with an alco-
Turcotte, 2 0 0 0 ) . In addition, symptoms of
holic parent. Poorer nutrition,
antisocial and borderline personality disorders
educational
opportunities, and physical abuse have been
are c o m m o n among problem drinkers, alco-
reported among children of alcoholics ( R a o ,
holics, and children o f alcoholics. Indeed, Sher
Begum, Venkataramana, &
Gangadharappa,
and Trull ( 2 0 0 2 ) reviewed the literature on
2 0 0 1 ) . O n e could speculate that growing up in
personality
such an environment may lead to the observed
although substance abuse is related to many
cognitive deficits and, as indicated previously,
personality symptoms, including those o f para-
may be an important
noid and avoidant personality disorder, the
mechanism
through
disorders
and
concluded
that
Alcohol
Problems
largest consistent set o f findings is in antisocial
a "hungry" brain in dramatically increasing the
and borderline symptoms.
incentive salience and reward value o f alcohol
Problem drinking appears to be related to
consumption.
other personality constructs as well. Studies have demonstrated repeatedly that high scores on Zuckerman's Sensation Seeking Scale (and
Exogenous Factors
other similar scales) predict drinking behavior
Stress is the most consistently reported
(e.g., Finn, Earleywine, & Pihl, 1 9 9 2 ) . Other
antecedent to drinking behavior. Naturalistic
studies o f novelty seeking using similar instru-
studies o f stress have found strong relations
ments provide additional support for such a
between a number o f stressors (e.g., social,
relation (Hesselbrock & Hesselbrock, 1 9 9 2 ) .
medical, trauma) and drinking behaviors. As
A longitudinal study o f children's novelty
one example, Seeman and Seeman ( 1 9 9 2 )
seeking found that those w h o scored highly
found that chronic stress associated with work
were more likely to become alcoholics as
predicted later alcoholism. Indeed, anecdotal
adults (Cloninger et al., 1 9 8 8 ) . Interestingly,
clinical reports consistently support the con-
one o f the relatively few
transdisciplinary
tention that acute stress is a powerful proximal
studies performed (Laine, Ahonen, Rasanen,
determinant of drinking episodes. T o ascertain
& Tiihonen, 2 0 0 1 ) revealed that individuals
a causal relation between stress and drinking,
high in novelty-seeking personality traits also
investigators have employed laboratory-based
have higher densities o f C N S dopamine trans-
studies o f experimental stressors (Stewart,
porter ( D A T ) . This finding is consistent with
2 0 0 0 ) . Findings have demonstrated that social,
genetic hypotheses that high levels o f D A T
cognitive, and physical stressors can induce
(which clears dopamine from the synapse)
alcohol craving, potentiate the hedonic impact
would relate to problem drinking.
of consumption, and increase the amount of
Still other studies have examined the role of
alcohol
consumed
post-stressor
(Stewart,
traits such as disinhibition, reward dependence,
2 0 0 0 ) . Interestingly, the magnitudes o f stress
external locus o f control, and negative self-
reactions also predict drinking behavior, such
concept and have found significant relations
that the previously mentioned drinking param-
with drinking behavior (e.g., Hesselbrock 8 t
eters are more severe for those who have
Hesselbrock, 1 9 9 2 ) . Interestingly, neurophysio-
stronger stress reactions (Sinha & O'Malley,
logical studies have linked many of these per-
1 9 9 9 ) . This finding is important because it
sonality traits, especially sensation seeking,
suggests not only that stress is a predictor o f
disinhibition, and deviance, to chronic hypo-
drinking but also that some who are pre-
perfusion of the orbitofrontal cortex (Friedman,
disposed to more powerful stress reactions
Cycowicz, &c Gaeta, 2 0 0 1 ) . Theorists have sug-
(through some genetic factor or otherwise) are
gested that these personality traits may repre-
at a particularly high risk for problem drink-
sent part of a broader syndrome related to
ing. T h e classic stress vulnerability model may
cortical underarousal
(Brennan 8c R a i n e ,
be particularly appropriate for understanding
1 9 9 7 ) . The localization o f these traits in the
alcoholism. Specifically, constitutional factors,
CNS
is particularly intriguing because the
orbitofrontal
c o r t e x is precisely the
area
involved in the cognitive deficits mentioned pre-
such as genetics, personality characteristics, neuropsychological dysfunction,
and
stress
reactivity, may render some individuals partic-
viously. Furthermore, this region of the brain is
ularly vulnerable to the effects o f stress and
highly dopaminergic. T h e physiological conver-
place them at high risk for dependence.
gence of these biogenetic, cognitive, and per-
If stress predicts drinking behavior, coping
sonality factors speaks to the preeminent role of
skills should moderate the degree to which
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH stress has an impact. Indeed, studies have
friends, work, and the law), the more one will
demonstrated that coping skills can buffer the
drink. Studies have shown repeatedly that the
effects o f stress on drinking behavior (Wills,
Alcohol Expectancy Questionnaire, a classic
8c Yaeger, 2002). Darwin, Freud, and
instrument used to assess positive expectancies,
(most recently) Bandura have underscored the
predicts drinking behavior (e.g., Williams &
Sandy,
importance of coping in adapting to stressful
Ricciardelli,
1996). Similarly, the more recently
situations. T h e Darwinian model o f homeo-
developed
Negative
static maintenance would predict that
an
Questionnaire has been found to negatively
organism would consume alcohol to return
correlate with drinking variables ( M c M a h o n
t o a baseline "pre-stress state"
(Darwin,
8c Jones, 1994). Recent innovations have iden-
1859/1998). Indeed, ethologists have specu-
tified powerful ingrained cognitive schemata
lated that animals may take laborious detours
that
from traditional migratory paths to find psy-
Goldman, Roehrich,
choactive substances. It is thought that this may
these are especially strong among those at risk
serve to maintain homeostasis during the stress-
for
ful process o f migration. Freud
(1901) formu-
underlie
Alcohol E x p e c t a n c y
these expectations (Rather,
alcoholism
8c Brannick, 1992), and
(Erblich, Earleywine,
8c
2001). In an intriguing study, Smith
Erblich,
lated the role o f coping in terms of "defense
(1994) found that expectations o f favorable
mechanisms." He argued that those who are
drinking
consequences predated
drinking
"orally fixated" (i.e., those w h o experienced
experiences, suggesting that such expectancies
some sort o f developmental arrest in early life
may be learned relatively early in life and are
when oral pleasure dominated) might use alco-
not simply a readout o f people's actual experi-
hol to cope with stressors in favor of other
ences with alcohol.
healthier coping mechanisms. Finally, Bandura
(1969) argued in his social learning theory that use o f alcohol as a coping mechanism may stem from imitative learning processes. Drinkers may have observed their parents use alcohol as a method o f "unwinding" after a long day, or they may have observed similar media representations of alcohol (e.g., "Miller time"). All of these theorists share the notion that management o f stress is a critical moderator o f drinking behavior and must be considered when trying to understand the effects o f stress on the development o f alcoholism. Another major predictor of drinking behav-
Modeling is another critical component in the development o f drinking behavior, according to
Bandura's
social learning
theory.
Children and teens often rely on role models when
developing
behavioral
repertoires,
especially regarding health behaviors (Yancey, Siegel,
&
McDaniel,
2002).
Observing
parents, siblings, and other peers consume alcohol may play a powerful role in shaping future behavior (Roski et al.,
1997). Other role
models, including those seen in advertisements, television programs, and movies, can have a profound influence as well. Thompson and Yokota
(2001) found that although the trend
ior is one's expectations of the consequences o f
has been decreasing, a substantial number of
drinking (e.g., Keane, Lisman, & Kreutzer,
G-rated movies depict alcohol and/or drug use.
1980). T h e more one expects alcohol con-
Social support is yet another factor found to
sumption to lead to positive outcomes (e.g.,
be involved in the development of problem
better social performance, better sexual perfor-
drinking. Individuals who report low levels o f
mance, more tension reduction, euphoria), the
social support are more likely to report prob-
more one will drink. Similarly, the less one
lem drinking than are others (Green, Freeborn,
expects alcohol consumption to lead to nega-
8c Polen, 2001). In a longitudinal study, (2001) found that even
tive consequences (e.g., hangover; excessive
Schuckit and Smith
sedation; sluggishness; trouble with family,
among individuals at high risk for alcoholism,
Alcohol
Problems
89
high levels of social support protected against
consequences o f alcohol: cognitive-behavioral
developing alcoholism 15 years later. Marlatt
therapy (CBT), motivational interviewing, and
(1996) discussed numerous "proximal determi-
12-step facilitation. C B T focuses on changing
nants" or factors that contribute to the decision
the thoughts and situations that previously led
to consume alcohol "in the moment." H e sug-
to the use of alcohol. Motivational interviewing
gested that those individuals with poor social
uses assessments and interpersonal interactions
skills, especially those who are uncomfortable
to enhance decisions to alter problem behaviors.
with saying " n o , " are more likely to consume
Finally, 12-step facilitation employs specific
alcohol (see also Smith & McCrady, 1 9 9 1 ) .
techniques to help people make good use of
In addition, those who have lower levels o f
12-step treatment.
self-efficacy, especially regarding the willpower to abstain or moderate drinking
Each treatment has its strengths. An enor-
behavior,
mous project that contrasted the outcomes o f
are more likely to consume alcohol. Taken
these three treatments for alcohol-dependent
together, stress, coping, expectancies, modeling,
individuals found that all three were compa-
social support, social skills, and self-efficacy can
rably effective
be conceptualized as necessary, but not suffi-
Group, 1 9 9 8 ) . T h e treatments share several
cient, moderators o f risk for developing alco-
factors, and this may help to explain their
(Project M A T C H Research
holism, such that the presence o f these factors
similar outcomes. Each emphasizes the client's
may determine whether or not someone who is
responsibility for change, each treats alcohol
vulnerable (by virtue o f genetics, personality, or
use as a phenomenon independent o f the indi-
cognitive functioning) will develop alcoholism.
vidual's value as a person, and each stresses
It should be noted that although these concepts
regular attendance and active participation in
are being presented independently, there is a
treatment.
sizable literature suggesting complex interrelationships between factors that is beyond the scope of this chapter. An illustration of this point is that coping, social skills, and selfefficacy all may be related and may be affected by expectancies (Marlatt & Gordon, 1 9 8 5 ) . Nevertheless, we believe that the current body o f literature on predictors o f drinking behavior points to a classic stress vulnerability model, whereby constitutional factors such as genetics, personality, and cognitive capacities can render an individual vulnerable to the effects of numerous exogenous factors. In sum, the available data
suggest that
the stress
vulnerability
approach provides a clinically useful working model o f the pathogenesis o f alcoholism.
Descriptions of these therapies do not reveal all o f their nuances, and even the best attempt to reduce a treatment to a few pages o f text invariably fails. Academic descriptions of psychotherapy often miss its potential for intimate and curative interactions, whereas stereotypical depictions o f the process often emphasize education,
empathy,
encouragement,
and
occasional insights. Ideally, these descriptions combine to alter actions, diminish problems, and increase happiness. T h e techniques and rationales o f each o f the treatments discussed in what follows provide only a limited picture of the ways in which they actually proceed. Although treatments differ in their methods and strategies, most require a
meaningful
relationship with a therapist. Therapists often believe that techniques create change, but the
PSYCHOLOGICAL TREATMENTS FOR ALCOHOL PROBLEMS
relationship may serve as an equally important
At least three different approaches have shown
strategies may help to explain some of the simi-
considerable promise in minimizing the negative
lar
contributor (Strupp, 1989). T h e idea that the relationship is more important than specific outcomes created by different
therapies
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH (Wampold et al., 1997). Manualized treatments,
The situations that precede drinking often
which clearly delineate specific material for
appear to be diverse. For example, an assess-
each session, can lead to different outcomes
ment might reveal dramatic drinking at a sport-
with different therapists. Although the thera-
ing event, after conflict at home, and every
peutic relationship may account for these
Friday night. T h e commonalities among these
differences, it does not mimic the friendship
situations are obscure. T h e cognitive-behavioral
and coaching c o m m o n outside o f therapy.
model suggests that thoughts about the situa-
D a t a clearly support psychotherapy's efficacy,
tions may contribute more to drinking than do
but the mechanisms that
the circumstances themselves. Thus, each envi-
lead to success
remain unclear (Dawes, 1 9 9 4 ) . Space
limitations
ronment may elicit specific thoughts. A com-
preclude
a
lengthy
mon thought in all of these situations might
description o f all available treatments for alco-
be that "alcohol is the only way in which to
hol-related problems. Given the widespread
enhance this experience." These types o f
familiarity and availability o f 12-step pro-
thoughts are probably easier to alter than are the
grams, this chapter focuses on
C B T and
motivational interviewing. T h e reader
who
is interested in facilitating participation
in
12-step programs is encouraged to read the work o f Nowinski and Baker ( 1 9 9 2 ) .
situations, so the thought rather than the environment becomes the focus of C B T . T h e cognitive-behavioral model
suggests
that people carry a set o f underlying beliefs into each situation. Certain situations activate these beliefs, eliciting specific thoughts
that
subsequently lead to action. F o r example, a
Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy
problem drinker might believe that alcohol
C B T for alcohol problems focuses on alter-
provides the only way in which to relax. T h e
ing environments, thoughts, and actions associ-
drinker may interpret a situation as stressful,
environments
leading to the activation of the belief that he
may trigger undesired problematic consump-
or she needs alcohol to relax. This belief
tion. These triggers involve both external and
would likely lead to thoughts of drinking,
internal factors. External factors include any
which might inspire all o f the actions required
ated with drinking. Different
person, location, or object associated with alco-
to get a drink. In C B T , the client would learn
hol. A beer mug, a rock song, or a swizzle stick
to challenge his or her beliefs in an effort to
may easily trigger a desire to drink. Internal fac-
minimize or eliminate drinking. Thus, the
tors include thoughts and feelings linked to
client may develop skills enabling him or her
alcohol. Some triggers are direct and some are
to see the situation as less stressful, thereby
indirect. Direct factors, such as craving and
altering the belief that drinking is the only
urges, are close to drinking. Indirect factors also
effective way in which to relax (Beck et al.,
increase the chance of drinking,
1 9 9 3 ) . Instead o f drinking, the client might
but
their
import is less obvious. These include frustration, anger, and even delight. C B T suggests
listen to music, meditate, or exercise. Therapists have developed
many
tech-
that problem drinkers learn to use alcohol in
niques for altering these beliefs. M o s t require
reaction to these triggers in much the same way
identifying the underlying
as people learn any behavior. Therefore, they
looking for evidence to support or dispute it.
belief and
then
can learn to engage in new behaviors instead o f
A c o m m o n strategy that cognitive-behavioral
problematic drinking
therapists employ includes Socratic question-
by altering
environ-
ments, thoughts, and actions (Beck, Wright,
ing, a method
Newman, & Liese, 1 9 9 3 ) .
clients through a series o f questions so that
by which therapists
guide
Alcohol they might arrive at their own
answers.
Problems
limits. T h e abstinence violation effect
91 may
Instead of providing information, this strategy
occur when a small thoughtless sip o f beer turns
teaches a process for discovery. Eventually,
into a full weekend binge. It is as if people
clients can learn to ask these sorts o f questions
say, "Well, I wrecked my abstinence, so I might
of themselves so that they can maintain sobri-
as well drink the whole bottle." Minimizing
ety without therapists.
the impact of small slips is essential to relapse
This process also elicits the thoughts and
prevention. Although many believe that the
feelings most important to clients. For example,
pharmacology o f alcohol makes a single dose
those who believe that alcohol provides the
inevitably turn into a relapse, changes in think-
only way in which to relax might respond
ing can actually prevent these slips from creat-
particularly well to questions about alternative
ing further problems. In fact, it has been shown
ways in which to unwind. Questions about
that the interpretation o f the slip appears to
restful recreation in general may prove helpful.
contribute more to relapse than does the
Queries about favorite activities before clients
actual occurrence o f the slip itself (Marlatt &
began drinking may also work. As clients gen-
Gordon, 1 9 8 5 ) .
erate their own list o f preferred ways in which
There is no doubt that intoxicated individu-
to soothe themselves without alcohol, the belief
als can make poor decisions about continued
that alcohol is the sole source o f relaxation
drinking and that the pharmacological effects
weakens. It is important to note that clients find
of
their own examples more compelling than any
Nevertheless, many individuals who relapse
list of relaxation techniques that
report
therapists
might generate. This approach also respects
alcohol contribute abstinence
to these
violation
decisions.
effects
that
occurred at extremely low doses. A single sip
clients' ability to present evidence to alter their
of liquor or smell o f wine often lead to the
beliefs (Overholser, 1 9 8 7 ) . In sum, changing
decision to binge. Pharmacology might not
the thoughts about situations that previously
play a particularly strong role in these relapses.
led to drinking can help to decrease problem-
Marlatt, Demming, and Reid ( 1 9 7 3 ) revealed
atic consumption.
that alcoholics who drank alcohol but were
C B T relies on other techniques that are too
not aware o f doing so did not show the absti-
numerous to list here, but one key set of strate-
nence violation effect and did not continue
gies concerns relapse prevention. M a n y people
drinking after the initial dose. In contrast,
can quit drinking briefly but cannot maintain
alcoholics given a placebo believed to be alco-
abstinence. Thus, many cognitive-behavioral
hol did show the abstinence violation effect
techniques focus not only on quitting
and did consume considerably more alcohol
but
also on avoiding relapse to alcohol. Thoughts
after the placebo. These findings indicate that
and beliefs remain important
thoughts also play an important role in relapse
in preventing
relapse given their relevance to a phenomenon
prevention.
known as the abstinence violation effect. T h e
In sum, C B T relies on the principles o f
abstinence violation effect concerns the way in
learning theory to treat alcohol-related prob-
which people cope with backsliding once they
lems. T h e treatment may w o r k by altering
have committed to altering their alcohol
beliefs about
consumption.
quences. It also focuses on the prevention o f
M o s t people who decide to eliminate or decrease their use of alcohol subsequently make
alcohol use and
relapse by identifying
its conse-
situations that
may
increase the risk o f drinking and then teach-
mistakes. They use alcohol when they intended
ing alternative ways in which to act under
to quit, or they use more than their established
those conditions.
92
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH
Motivational Interviewing
qualities with the stages o f change model to
Motivational interviewing involves brief interactions with a therapist to help the client decrease alcohol-related problems. T h e treatment enhances motivation before attempting any changes in behavior because in the absence of motivation, any efforts to teach techniques for limiting alcohol consumption are typically an inefficient use o f time for both the client and the therapist. Motivational interviewing focuses on identifying clients' own reasons to quit. Once these reasons help to increase desire, clients often develop their own strategies for eliminating alcohol from their lives. M a n y people stop drinking on their own, and motivational interviewing essentially enhances the chances that a client will join this group. (For a more detailed discussion o f motivational interviewing, see Chapter 4.) Motivational interviewing relies on principles designed to help the client decrease alcohol problems. First, the therapist behaves in a manner that will increase the likelihood o f change such as listening attentively without judgment
or blame. Second, the
therapist
employs the "stages o f change" model, which views change as a fluid process that requires a different intervention for each stage o f the client's willingness to act. In motivational interviewing, the behaviors employed by the therapist that are most likely to induce behavior change on the part o f the client (e.g., empathy,
nonpossessive w a r m t h ,
genuineness)
were originally emphasized in client-centered therapy (Rogers, 1 9 5 0 ) .
decrease problem drinking. T h e stages of change model describes specific steps that individuals appear
t o take when
they alter
problem
behaviors (Prochaska & DiClemente, 1983). The researchers proposed six stages: (a) precontemplation, (b) contemplation, (c) determination, (d) action, (e) maintenance, and (f) relapse (Prochaska, Norcross, & DiClemente, 1994). Precontemplation
describes the
period
before individuals consider altering behavior. Drinkers in precontemplation
have
never
considered cutting down or quitting. An adept therapist would not waste time attempting to teach these individuals h o w to quit because they currently lack the motivation to do so. Instead, the therapist assesses clients' quantity and frequency of drinking in an effort to get them
to
contemplate
change.
The
best
approach for this assessment is the time line "followback" (Sobell & Sobell, 1 9 9 5 ) , a calendar
technique that asks drinkers to go
through each day for the previous 3 months and list the number o f drinks consumed. T h e therapist would also ask about any associated consequences such as negative
emotions,
fatigue, hangovers, accidents, and liver troubles. This assessment often leads clients to make the connection between their drinking and the consequences o f their drinking. If these connections are made and they lead clients to consider change in any way, clients have entered the contemplation stage. Contemplation includes the weighing of the pros and cons o f altering actions or continuing the same behavior. T h e motivational interviewer encourages drinkers in this stage to candidly report all o f the positive and neg-
T H E STAGES O F CHANGE M O D E L
ative experiences they attribute to their use o f alcohol. Initial assessments o f pros and cons
As mentioned in the previous section, empathy,
often
warmth, and genuineness lay the foundation for
desires to continue drinking as well as equally
reveal ambivalence, that
is,
strong
any productive therapeutic interaction. M a n y
strong desires to stop. Ambivalence serves as
therapies rely on these aspects of the thera-
a c o m m o n important component o f contem-
peutic relationship to help support growth.
plation. Other approaches t o treatment may
Motivational
see ambivalence as denial. T h e stages o f
interviewing
combines
these
Alcohol change model emphasizes ambivalence as an
Problems
relapse. T h e y identify situations that
93 put
further
the drinkers at high risk for relapse, and they
discussion, the therapist respectfully reflects
plan ways in which to avoid problematic
drinkers' concerns back to them, emphasizing
alcohol
the negative consequences that they generated
example, clients m a y decide to avoid parties
earlier. This process often leads
problem
where alcohol is present. T h e y m a y role-play
drinkers to a decision to change. A firm
refusing drinks if they are offered them. T h e y
inherent
part o f change. During
use in these c i r c u m s t a n c e s . F o r
decision to change qualifies as a step toward
may practice relaxation techniques if tension
determination.
often precedes their drinking. T h e y may call
Determination begins with a clearly stated
a hotline or a friend during times o f tempta-
desire to alter actions. This stage serves as the
tion. It is important to note that these tech-
appropriate time for drinkers to formulate a
niques for preventing relapse are consistent
plan for limiting alcohol consumption. T h e
with 12-step and C B T approaches.
plan often stems from brainstorming between
Occasional backsliding occurs in many
the interviewer and the drinkers and may
efforts to alter maladaptive drinking behav-
include any options that look promising. F o r
ior. T h e stages o f change model considers
example, the strategy for change may rely on
lapses and relapses as another category o f
techniques from C B T such as altering beliefs
change. Discussing this fact with clients
and preventing relapse. In addition, drinkers
may help to normalize the occasional slip.
may decide that membership in a 12-step
Considering lapses as a part o f the change
program sounds appropriate.
process may decrease the chances o f an absti-
O n c e clients regularly limit their drinking
nence violation effect transforming a slip into
or abstain, they have entered the action stage.
a full-blown relapse. T h e key to the lapse
T h e y no longer merely consider change; they
stage parallels the key to the maintenance
actually make the desired change. This stage
stage—preventing
proves to be particularly informative as the
immediate action. Lapsing drinkers can pre-
genuine experience o f new habits and actions
vent relapse by rapidly exiting the situation
reveal valuable information
and removing the chance o f continued drink-
unanticipated
relapse. Lapses
require
during the contemplation and determination
ing. M a n y w h o lapse berate themselves, but
stages. Clients may find
situations
their time and energy m a y be better spent in
to be easier or m o r e difficult than they
identifying the precursors to the slips. A
expected. T h e motivational interviewer will
frank examination m a y reveal a new high-
some
offer reassurance about the process becom-
risk situation, providing the opportunity to
ing less difficult with the passing o f time and
formulate a plan for h o w to handle this
more practice. T h e interviewer helps clients
predicament in the future. F o r example, a
to solve problems related to their alcohol
former drinker may find himself or herself
use and listens attentively to clients' detailed
lapsing after a fight with a family member.
descriptions o f their difficulties and successes.
This situation might not be one that the
After a steady period o f action, clients
drinker had identified as high risk before.
increased confidence in their
N o w the drinker k n o w s that he or she needs
skills. This sense o f efficacy, an optimism in
to plan new ways in which to deal with con-
may report
their o w n ability to continue the new behav-
flict. T h e drinker can turn this lapse into a
iors, serves as a hallmark o f the maintenance
learning experience to prevent future drink-
stage. Self-efficacy and sustained change are
ing. T h u s , lapses remain a part o f the change
the keys to maintenance. T h e therapist and
process, and planning for them may mini-
the clients will n o w w o r k together to prevent
mize problems.
94
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH
CASE S T U D Y "Bob,"
a 54-year-old Caucasian
male t r u c k
driver,
c a m e to
a
Veterans
Administration hospital after falling in his driveway. H e had seriously injured his face and hands. A breath alcohol monitor suggested that his blood alcohol level was approximately . 2 0 . Surgeons removed small rocks from his face and hands and referred him t o the chemical dependency treatment program. Assessment revealed that B o b had been drinking alcohol regularly for 4 1 years, since the age o f 13 years, and had his first drink at age 1 0 years. A time line followback assessment suggested that B o b had consumed between 2 0 and 2 4 beers per day over the past 9 0 days, a pattern he said went b a c k for at least 7 years. H e had been in treatment twice previously, once in his late 2 0 s and once approximately 8 years ago. H e had maintained complete abstinence for approximately a year each time. Both treatments focused on 12-step interventions, but B o b was unwilling to return t o meetings or inpatient treatment. H e did, however, agree to attend a 1-hour outpatient appointment the following week. B o b missed the first outpatient meeting, rescheduled after a telephone call, and missed the second meeting as well. A phone call after his second missed appointment revealed that he was willing to discuss the pros and cons of attending an outpatient appointment. B o b confessed that he thought that the hospital only offered 12-step interventions and that he thought he would be "strong-armed" into going back to " G o d meetings." W i t h the promise that there would be no discussion o f steps or deities, he agreed to attend an outpatient interview the following week. T h e fact that he was willing t o reschedule illustrates the importance o f follow-up calls after missed appointments. B o b would have undoubtedly never returned t o treatment if he had not been phoned after missing appointments. B o b arrived promptly at an afternoon appointment with a breath alcohol concentration of . 0 6 . H e stated that he had consumed 4 beers at lunch but that he was doing much better than he had been doing when he came to the hospital after his fall. He claimed to drink 1 2 beers per day over the previous 3 weeks and again declined inpatient treatment. O n reflection, he admitted that he had cut down to 1 2 beers per day in the past but had eventually increased back to his usual case o f 2 4 per day. He was unwilling to discuss abstinence but agreed to list the pros and cons o f decreasing his drinking to 6 beers per day. This approach is consistent with motivational interviewing interventions for people in the contemplation stage. B o b was surprised when the therapist asked him to first list the disadvantages of drinking only 6 beers per day. T h e most salient disadvantage to him was that he would be forced to drink them all at once on an empty stomach to notice any subjective effects. H e also mentioned that he might receive ribbing from cronies for not "keeping up" when they watched sporting events or went fishing. T h e only advantages to decreasing to 6 beers per day that B o b could generate were financial. With some prompting, he decided that he might also have fewer conflicts with his adult children if he decreased
Alcohol
Problems
his drinking. T h e therapist pointed out several other potential advantages. Specifically, Bob's liver enzymes suggested the potential for medical problems, and these would eventually improve with a decrease (although they would not improve as much as they would with abstinence). In addition, B o b would be less likely to run into problems while driving his truck for work. Mentioning this potential advantage prompted several tales of bravado about his tolerance. With reflection, these eventually turned to a revealing disclosure about a blackout experience. B o b had arrived in a location more than 3 0 0 miles from his home and could not recall any aspect o f the trip. H e feared that he could have had an accident and killed another driver or himself. H e expressed considerable shame, guilt, and fear. Reflection o f these emotions appeared to inspire a willingness to limit consumption to 6 beers per day and to drink these only during the evening when it was unlikely that he would drive. B o b also agreed to three more outpatient visits during the next 3 weeks. The therapist called B o b after 4 days to confirm his next appointment. W h e n asked how he was doing, B o b replied, " I f I can't have a 12-pack, I really don't see the point o f drinking at all." W h e n asked to elaborate, B o b explained that 6 beers provided little change in his state o f mind. H e agreed to stick to the limit but implied that he might experiment with an occasional day o f abstinence. W h e n he arrived for his next appointment, his breath alcohol was . 0 0 . H e had consumed 6 beers per day on each day, but he drank only 1 during the evening that the therapist had called. W h e n asked why, B o b said that he felt "silly" drinking at all after what he had said about requiring 1 2 beers to feel any subjective effects. W h e n asked what subjective effects o f alcohol he preferred, B o b focused on tension reduction. T h e session then turned to standard progressive muscle relaxation training. B o b found relaxing in session to be a bit cumbersome but agreed to listen to a relaxation tape at home. T h e following week, B o b decreased his drinking to an average o f 4 beers per day, with 1 day o f abstinence. W h e n asked h o w he felt about it, he claimed little change in his o w n experience but some tentative changes in those around him. His adult children had commented that it was nice to see him drinking less. H e said that this did n o t matter to him much, but his affect certainly seemed improved. H e had been listening to the relaxation tape daily and agreed to discuss complete abstinence. T h e therapist reviewed some relapse prevention strategies and sent B o b h o m e with a list o f responses he had generated himself for handling difficult situations. H e had focused on drink refusal with friends and stressful situations as his target high-risk situations. B o b generated the expression "I've already had my share" as a response for refusing drinks when offered. H e also agreed to listen to the relaxation tape daily in an effort to reduce stress. H e discussed looking at things differently in an effort to cope, and he agreed to attend a stress management seminar conducted in another area o f the hospital. At 1 month follow-up, B o b had lapsed one time. H e attended a barbecue where an acquaintance handed him an open 4 0 - o u n c e bottle o f beer. B o b stated that he took a drink from the bottle automatically. H e then reported that he excused
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH
himself to go to the bathroom and poured most o f the beer into the sink. H e reported that he then carried the partially empty bottle around the party for a while for reasons he could not explain. B o b exhibited signs o f disappointment regarding this event. T h e therapist emphasized that it was a single slip, that B o b had not turned it into an excuse to start a binge, and that B o b did not drink again. B o b seemed happy with the interpretation. H e agreed to go back to his favorite "I've already had my share" response if a similar experience arose in the future. T h e therapist called once the following week to confirm abstinence. At 3 months, B o b reported no new lapses and was happy to report that his liver enzymes had improved. At 9 months, he had continued his abstinence and reported even more improvement on his liver enzymes. Although 9 months o f follow-up is not a long time, these initial results were encouraging for this combined approach o f motivational interviewing and C B T .
CONCLUSIONS
imperfect but useful treatments have proved to
Alcohol can create numerous problems in the
for many individuals: C B T , motivational inter-
be effective in alleviating alcohol problems lives o f drinkers. Different genetic and environ-
viewing, and 12-step facilitation. These thera-
mental factors interact in the creation o f alco-
pies have many overlapping characteristics
hol abuse, dependence, and problems. A family
but also employ techniques specific to each
history o f alcoholism, a combination o f person-
approach that are designed to decrease alcohol-
ality traits, and a set o f cognitive factors all can
related problems. Although the road to sobriety
combine with various life stressors to lead
is fraught with difficulties, many people have
people to turn to alcohol for relief of stress.
changed their lives by eliminating the problems
Consistent use o f large quantities may lead to
related to their continued alcohol use. Putting
alcohol abuse. It can further lead to alcohol
an end to problem drinking can have a dra-
dependence or to other life problems. Three
matic impact on health and happiness.
REFERENCES Alterman, A. I., Gerstley, L. J . , Goldstein, G., Sc Tarter, R. E. (1987). Comparisons of the cognitive functioning of familial and nonfamilial alcoholics. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 48, 4 2 5 - 4 2 9 . American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Bandura, A. (1969). Social learning of moral judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 11, 2 7 5 - 2 7 9 . Beck, A. T., Wright, F. D., Newman, C. F., & Liese, B. S. (1993). Cognitive therapy of substance abuse. New York: Guilford.
Alcohol Berridge, K. C , & Robinson, T. E. (1998). What is the role of dopamine in reward: Hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? Brain Research Reviews, 28, 3 0 9 - 3 6 9 . Blum, K., Braverman, E. R., Holder, J . M., Lubar, J . F., Monastra, V. J . , Miller, D., Lubar, J . O., Chen, T. J . , & Comings, D. E. (2000). Reward deficiency syndrome: A biogenetic model for the diagnosis and treatment of impulsive, addictive, and compulsive behaviors. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 32(Suppl.), 1-112. Blum, K., Noble, E. P., Sheridan, P. J . , Montgomery, Α., Ritchie, T., Jagadeeswaran, P., Nogami, H., Briggs, A. H., & Cohn, J . B . (1990). Allelic association of human dopamine D receptor gene in alcoholism. Journal of the American Medical Association, 263, 2 0 5 5 - 2 0 6 0 . Brecher, Ε. M . (1972). Licit and illicit drugs. Boston: Little, Brown. Brennan, P. Α., &c Raine, A. (1997). Biosocial bases of antisocial behavior: Psychophysiological, neurological, and cognitive factors. Clinical Psychology Review, 17,589-604. Cadoret, R. J . , Troughton, E., O'Gorman, T. W., 8c Heywood, E. (1986). An adoption study of genetic and environmental factors in drug abuse. Archives of General Psychiatry, 43, 1 1 3 1 - 1 1 3 6 . Cloninger, C. R., Sigvardsson, S., Gilligan, S. B., von Knorring, A. L., Reich, T., & Bohman, M. (1988). Genetic heterogeneity and the classification of alcoholism. Advances in Alcohol and Substance Abuse, 7, 3 - 1 6 . Conger, J . J . (1956). Reinforcement theory and the dynamics of alcoholism. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 17, 2 9 6 - 3 0 5 . Conley, J . J . , 8t Kammeier, M . L. (1980). MMPI item responses of alcoholics in treatment: Comparisons with normal and psychiatric patients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 48, 6 6 8 - 6 6 9 . Darwin, C. R. (1998). The origin of species. New York: Random House. (Original work published 1859) Davis, V. E., 8c Walsh, M . J . (1970). Alcohol addiction and tetrahydropapaveroline. Science, 169, 1 1 0 5 - 1 1 0 6 . Dawes, R. M . (1994). House of cards. New York: Free Press. Eddy, N. B . , Halbach, H., Isbell, H., & Seevers, M . H. (1965). Drug dependence: Organization, Its significance and characteristics. Bulletin of the World Health 32, 7 2 1 - 7 3 3 . Erblich, J . , Sc Earleywine, M . (1995). Distraction does not impair memory during intoxication: Support for the attention-allocation model. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 56, 4 4 4 - 4 4 8 . Erblich, J . , 8c Earleywine, M. (1999). Children of alcoholics exhibit attenuated cognitive impairment during an ethanol challenge. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 23, 4 7 6 - 4 8 2 . Erblich, J . , Earleywine, M., 8t Erblich, B . (2001). Positive and negative associations with alcohol and familial risk for alcoholism. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 15, 2 0 4 - 2 0 9 . Erblich, J . , Earleywine, M., Erblich, B., &C Bovbjerg, D. H. (in press). Biphasic stimulant and sedative effects of ethanol: Are children of alcoholics really different? Addictive Behaviors. Finn, P. R., Earleywine, M., & Pihl, R. O. (1992). Sensation seeking, stress reactivity, and alcohol dampening discriminate the density of family history of alcoholism. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 16, 5 8 5 - 5 9 0 . Finn, P. R., Sharkansky, E. J . , Brandt, K. M., & Turcotte, Ν. (2000). The effects of familial risk, personality, and expectancies on alcohol use and misuse. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 1 2 2 - 1 3 3 . Freud, S. (1901). Psychopathology of everyday life. London: T. Fisher Unwin. 2
Problems
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH Friedman, D., Cycowicz, Y . M., St Gaeta, H. (2001). The novelty of P3: An eventrelated brain potential (ERP) sign of the brain's evaluation of novelty. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 25, 3 5 5 - 3 7 3 . Giancola, P. R., St Moss, H. B . (1998). Executive cognitive functioning in alcohol use disorders. Recent Developments in Alcoholism, 14, 2 2 7 - 2 5 1 . Green, C. Α., Freeborn, D. K., St Polen, M . R. (2001). Gender and alcohol use: The roles of social support, chronic illness, and psychological well-being. Journal of Biobehavioral Medicine, 24, 3 8 3 - 3 9 9 . Grilly, D. M . (1998). Drugs and human behavior. Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Hesselbrock, M. N., St Hesselbrock, V. M . (1992). Relationship of family history, antisocial personality disorder, and personality traits in young men at risk for alcoholism. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 53, 6 1 9 - 6 2 5 . Higuchi, S., Muramatsu, T., Matsushita, S., Murayama, M., St Hayashida, M. (1996). Polymorphisms of ethanol-oxidizing enzymes in alcoholics with inactive ALDH2. Human Genetics, 97, 4 3 1 - 4 3 4 . Hoffman, N. G., Lumry, A. E., Harrison, P. Α., & Lessard, R. J . (1984). Brief Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory scales to screen for substance abuse. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 14, 2 0 9 - 2 1 4 . Josephs, R. Α., 8t Steele, C. M . (1990). The two faces of alcohol myopia: Attentional mediation of psychological stress. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99, 1 1 5 - 1 2 6 . Keane, T. M., Lisman, S. Α., St Kreutzer, J . (1980). Alcoholic beverages and their placebos: An empirical evaluation of expectancies. Addictive Behaviors, 5, 313-328. Laine, T. P., Ahonen, Α., Rasanen, P., St Tiihonen, J . (2001). Dopamine transporter density and novelty seeking among alcoholics. Journal of Addictive Diseases, 20, 9 1 - 9 6 . Levenson, R. W., Sher, K. J . , Grossman, L. M., Newman, J . , St Newlin, D. B. (1980). Alcohol and stress response dampening: Pharmacological effects, expectancy, and tension reduction. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89, 5 2 8 - 5 3 8 . MacAndrew, C. (1967). Self-reports of male alcoholics: A dimensional analysis of certain differences from nonalcoholic male psychiatric outpatients. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 28, 4 3 - 5 1 . Marlatt, G. A. (1996). Taxonomy of high-risk situations for alcohol relapse: Evolution and development of a cognitive-behavioral model. Addiction, 9I(Suppl.), S 3 7 - S 4 9 . Marlatt, G. Α., Demming, B., 8t Reid, J . B. (1973). Loss of control drinking in alcoholics: An experimental analogue. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 81, 233-241. Marlatt, G. Α., & Gordon, J . R. (1985). Relapse prevention: Maintenance strategies in the treatment of addictive disorders. New York: Guilford. McMahon, J . , St Jones, Β. T. (1994). Social drinkers' negative alcohol expectancy relates to their satisfaction with current consumption: Measuring motivation for change with the NAEQ. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 29, 6 8 7 - 6 9 0 . Meyer, R. E. (2001). Finding paradigms for the future of alcoholism research: An interdisciplinary perspective. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 25, 1 3 9 3 - 1 4 0 6 . Miller, N. S., Gold, M . S., St Smith, D. E. (1997). Manual of therapeutics for addictions. New York: John Wiley. Nagoshi, C. T., & Wilson, J . R. (1987). Influence of family alcoholism history on alcohol metabolism, sensitivity, and tolerance. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 11, 3 9 2 - 3 9 8 . Newlin, D. B., St Thomson, J . B. (1990). Alcohol challenge with sons of alcoholics: A critical review and analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 3 8 3 - 4 0 2 .
Alcohol Nowinski, J . , St Baker, S. (1992). The twelve-step facilitation handbook. New York: Lexington Books. Overholser, J . C. (1987). Clinical utility of the Socratic method. In C. Stout (Ed.), Annals of clinical research (pp. 1-7). Des Plaines, IL: Forest Institute. Prochaska, J . O., St DiClemente, C. C. (1983). Stages and processes of self-change in smoking: Toward an integrative model of change. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 5, 3 9 0 - 3 9 5 . Prochaska, J . O., Norcross, J . C , St DiClemente, C. C. (1994). Changing for good. New York: Avon Books. Project M A T C H Research Group. (1998). Matching patients with alcohol disorders to treatments: Clinical implications from Project M A T C H . Journal of Mental Health UK, 7, 5 8 9 - 6 0 2 . Rao, Κ. N., Begum, S., Venkataramana, V., St Gangadharappa, N. (2001). Nutritional neglect and physical abuse in children of alcoholics. Indian Journal of Pediatrics, 68, 8 4 3 - 8 4 5 . Rather, B. C , Goldman, M . S., Roehrich, L., St Brannick, M . (1992). Empirical modeling of an alcohol expectancy memory network using multidimensional scaling. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 1 7 4 - 1 8 3 . Rodriguez, H. S., Porjesz, B., Chorlian, D. B . , Polich, J . , St Begleiter, H. (1999). Visual P3a in male subjects at high risk for alcoholism. Biological Psychiatry, 46, 2 8 1 - 2 9 1 . Rogers, C. (1950). A current formulation of client-centered therapy. Social Service Review, 24, 4 4 2 - 4 5 0 . Roski, J . , Perry, C. L., McGovern, P. G., Williams, C. L., Farbakhsh, K., St VeblenMortenson, S. (1997). School and community influences on adolescent alcohol and drug use. Health Education Research, 12, 2 5 5 - 2 6 6 . Sannibale, C , St Hall, W. (1998). An evaluation of Cloninger's typology of alcohol abuse. Addiction, 93, 1 2 4 1 - 1 2 4 9 . Schuckit, M . A. (1994). Low level of response to alcohol as a predictor of future alcoholism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 1 8 4 - 1 8 9 . Schuckit, Μ. Α., St Smith, T. L. (2001). A comparison of correlates of DSM-IV alcohol abuse or dependence among more than 4 0 0 sons of alcoholics and controls. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 25, 1-18. Schuckit, Μ . Α., Tsuang, J . W., Anthenelli, R. M., Tipp, J . E., St Nurnberger, J . I., Jr. (1996). Alcohol challenges in young men from alcoholic pedigrees and control families: A report from the COGA project. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 57, 3 6 8 - 3 7 7 . Seeman, M., St Seeman, A. Z . (1992). Life strains, alienation, and drinking behavior. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 16, 1 9 9 - 2 0 5 . Self, D. W. (1998). Neural substrates of drug craving and relapse in drug addiction. Annals of Medicine, 30, 3 7 9 - 3 8 9 . Self, D. W., St Nestler, E. J . (1998). Relapse to drug-seeking: Neural and molecular mechanisms. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 51, 4 9 - 6 0 . Sher, K. J . , St Trull, T. J . (2002). Substance use disorder and personality disorder. Current Psychiatry Reports, 4, 2 5 - 2 9 . Sinha, R., St O'Malley, S. S. (1999). Craving for alcohol: Findings from the clinic and the laboratory. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 34, 2 2 3 - 2 3 0 . Smith, D. E., St McCrady, B. S. (1991). Cognitive impairment among alcoholics: Impact on drink refusal skill acquisition and treatment outcome. Addictive Behaviors, 16, 2 6 5 - 2 7 4 . Smith, G. T. (1994). Psychological expectancy as mediator of vulnerability to alcoholism. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 708, 1 6 5 - 1 7 1 . Sobell, L. C , St Sobell, M . B. (1995). Alcohol timeline followback instructional training video. Toronto: Addiction Research Foundation.
Problems
BEHAVIORS THAT C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH Stewart, J . (2000). Pathways to relapse: The neurobiology of drug- and stress-induced relapse to drug taking. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 25, 1 2 5 - 1 3 6 . Streissguth, A. P., Martin, D. C , & Buffington, V. E. (1976). Test-retest reliability of the three scales derived from a quantity-frequency-variability assessment of self-reported alcohol consumption. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 273, 4 5 8 - 4 6 6 . Strupp, H. H. (1989). Psychotherapy: Can the practitioner learn from the researcher? American Psychologist, 44, 7 1 7 - 7 2 4 . Thompson, K. M., &C Yokota, F. (2001). Depiction of alcohol, tobacco, and other substances in G-rated animated feature films. Pediatrics, 107, 1 3 6 9 - 1 3 7 4 . Tsuang, M . T., Bar, J . L., Harley, R. M., & Lyons, M . J . (2001). The Harvard twin study of substance abuse: What we have learned. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 9, 2 6 7 - 2 7 9 . Ustiin, B., Compton, W., Mager, D., Babor, T., Baiyewu, O., Chatterji, S., Cottier, L., Goegues, Α., Mavreas, V., Peters, L., Pull, C , Saunders, J . , Smeets, R., Stipec, M . R., Vrasti, R., Hasin, D., Room, R., Van den Brink, W., Regier, D., Blaine, J . , Grant, B . F., & Sartorius, N. (1997). W H O study on the reliability and validity of the alcohol and drug use disorder instruments: Overview of methods and results. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 47, 1 6 1 - 1 6 9 . Wampold, Β . E., Mondin, G. W., Moody, M., Stich, F., Benson, K., & Ahn, H. (1997). A meta-analysis of outcome studies comparing bona fide psychotherapies: Empirically, "all must have prizes." Psychological Bulletin, 122, 2 0 3 - 2 1 5 . Williams, R. J . , Se Ricciardelli, L. A. (1996). Expectancies relate to symptoms of alcohol dependence in young adults. Addiction, 91, 1 0 3 1 - 1 0 3 9 . Wills, Τ. Α., Sandy, J . M., & Yaeger, A. M . (2002). Moderators of the relation between substance use level and problems: Test of a self-regulation model in middle adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 3 - 2 1 . Yancey, A. K., Siegel, J . M., & McDaniel, K. L. (2002). Role models, ethnic identity, and health-risk behaviors in urban adolescents. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 156, 5 5 - 6 1 .
CHAPTER
5
The Etiology and Treatment of Nicotine Dependence A Biopsychosocial
Perspective
LEE M . COHEN, DENNIS E . MCCHARGUE, MONICA CORTEZ-GARLAND, ERIC H . PRENSKY, AND SADIE EMERY
C
hronic
use
products,
of
tobacco-containing
particularly
cigarettes,
smoke include lung cancer, asthma, respiratory infections, and decreased pulmonary function
remains one of the most avoidable
( D H H S , 1 9 9 9 ) . Despite public health efforts to
causes of death and illness in the United States
reduce tobacco use in the United States, adult
and claims the lives o f more than 4 3 0 , 0 0 0 indi-
prevalence rates have not changed
viduals each year (U.S. Department o f Health
cantly, and
and H u m a n Services [ D H H S ] , 1 9 9 9 ) . T h e
observed during the 1 9 9 0 s ( C D C , 2 0 0 2 ) . For
number
alone
example, the overall rate of adult cigarette
exceeds that o f deaths due to AIDS, murders,
smokers has decreased slightly from 2 5 . 0 % to
o f tobacco-related
deaths
in some cases increases
signifiwere
other drugs, alcohol, car crashes, fires, and sui-
2 3 . 3 % across all age groups except that of 1 8 -
cides combined (Centers for Disease Control
to 24-year-olds ( C D C , 2 0 0 2 ) , whereas the use
and Prevention [ C D C ] , 2 0 0 2 ) . Illnesses associ-
of smokeless (spit) tobacco and cigars has
ated with tobacco use include, but are not
increased substantially (U.S. Department o f
limited t o , laryngeal cancer, oral
cancer,
esophageal cancer, obstructive pulmonary disease, growth
cardiovascular retardation,
disease, and
intrauterine
low birth
weight
Agriculture,
1 9 9 7 ) . T o date, 6 5 . 5 million
Americans continue to use tobacco products on a regular basis ( C D C , 2 0 0 2 ) and appear to be more difficult to treat than their counter-
( D H H S , 1 9 9 9 ) . Evidence o f significant health
parts o f the
risks due to environmental tobacco smoke has
Brandon, 2 0 0 0 ) . As such, tobacco use contin-
1970s
and
1980s
(Irvin
&
also been documented. Adverse health risks
ues to represent an important health behavior
caused by exposure to "secondhand" tobacco
that faces health care professionals.
101
BEHAVIORS THAT COMPROMISE HEALTH
102
DEFINITIONS AND DESCRIPTION OF NICOTINE DEPENDENCE
physiological adaptation
The addictive process associated with tobacco
sea, and vomiting are associated with initial
following
repeated
has been
exposure
to
shown nicotine
(Balfour, 1 9 9 1 ) . F o r example, dizziness, nauuse has been studied primarily with cigarette
exposure t o cigarette smoking;
smoking, but there is a growing body o f litera-
these symptoms disappear rapidly following
however,
ture examining this process in spit tobacco (e.g.,
habitual exposure (Benowitz, 1 9 9 0 ) . For the
Hatsukami & Severson, 1 9 9 9 ; McChargue &
most part, tolerance to nicotine
Collins, 1998) and cigar use (e.g., Henningfield,
quickly, sometimes within
Fant, Radzius, & Frost, 1 9 9 9 ) . T h e D S M - I V -
administration (Porchet, Benowitz, & Sheiner,
TR
(Diagnostic
Mental
and
Disorders,
Statistical
Manual
develops
3 5 minutes o f
of
1 9 8 8 ) . Once tolerance is developed, certain
fourth edition, text revision
effects (e.g., dizziness) are more transient and
2000])
dissipate rapidly following a short period of
classifies chronic tobacco use as a significant
[American Psychiatric Association,
abstinence (Benowitz, 1 9 9 0 ) , whereas toler-
clinical impairment
because of the psycho-
ance to most o f the subjective and behavioral
logical and neurobiological effects caused by
effects appears to be more long term (Perkins
nicotine—the presumed
addictive
ingredient
et al., 2 0 0 1 ) . Researchers have suggested that
found in tobacco products (Henningfield &
the rapid "re-sensitization" o f the more tran-
Heishman,
sient effects, such as the "rush" one experi-
1 9 9 5 ; Robinson &
Pritchard,
1 9 9 2 ) . As a clinical disorder, chronic tobacco
ences from the first cigarette o f the day, may
when
partially explain why tobacco users tend to
use is classified as nicotine
dependence
three of seven criteria are met within the same
show stable use patterns without progressively
12-month period. In particular, the four most
increasing their dose amounts
prominent criteria of nicotine dependence are
(Benowitz, 1 9 9 0 ) .
over
time
(a) developing a tolerance to nicotine, (b) experiencing nicotine withdrawal,
(c) showing a
persistent desire or unsuccessful efforts to quit or cut down the use of nicotine, and (d) continuing to use nicotine despite the development o f physical or psychological problems that are likely to have been caused or exacerbated by tobacco
products
(American
Psychiatric
Association, 2 0 0 0 ) . Other general criteria for nicotine dependence
include
using
larger
amounts over a longer period o f time; spending a great deal of time in activities necessary to obtain, use, or recover from nicotine; and experiencing
impaired
functioning
(American
Psychiatric Association, 2 0 0 0 ) .
Nicotine Withdrawal Nicotine withdrawal is defined as the manifestation o f behavioral, subjective, physiological, and biochemical changes that occur when a person abruptly cuts down or quits using nicotine-containing products (Hughes, Higgins, &c Hatsukami, 1 9 9 0 ) . T h e withdrawal syndrome includes four or more o f the following symptoms: (a) dysphoric or depressed mood; (b) insomnia; (c) irritability, frustration,
or
anger; (d) anxiety; (e) difficulty in concentrating; (f) restlessness; (g) decreased heart rate; and
(h) increased appetite
or weight
gain
(American Psychiatric Association, 2 0 0 0 ) . In
Tolerance Simply stated, tolerance is viewed as a
addition, these symptoms cause clinically significant distress or impairment in social, occupational,
or
other
important
areas
of
diminished response or an adaptation to a
functioning, and the symptoms are not better
given dose after repeated use (Balfour, 1 9 9 1 ;
accounted for by another mental
Benowitz, 1 9 9 0 ) . Subjective, behavioral, and
(American Psychiatric Association, 2 0 0 0 ) .
disorder
Etiology
and Treatment
of Nicotine
Dependence
\
Declining blood levels o f nicotine have been
anxiety is reported, it could be a function of
associated with the onset of nicotine with-
brief lapses in their abstinence (e.g., periodically
drawal; however, it is not clear whether the
smoking one cigarette and then resuming absti-
duration and severity of these symptoms are
nence [West
entirely attributable to the rate at which nico-
sive symptoms may persist beyond 1 month,
tine dissipates from a person's system. For
especially among people who have experienced
8c Hajek, 1997]). Finally, depres-
example, nicotine reaches the brain within 10
a major depressive episode in the past (Borrelli
to 19 seconds after smoking a cigarette, with
et al.,
brain levels o f nicotine declining rapidly over
least a 33% chance that people with a history of
20 to 30 minutes (Benowitz, 1990). O n the
major depression will experience clinically sig-
other hand, nicotine levels in the brain from spit
nificant levels o f depressive symptoms at any
tobacco tend to increase gradually, reaching
time across the first 12 months of nicotine absti-
their peak about 30 minutes after administra-
nence (Borrelli et al.,
1996). In fact, data suggest that there is at
1996; Tsoh et al., 2000).
tion, and decline slowly over 2 hours or more (Benowitz, Porchet,
8c J a c o b , 1990). Despite
the differing rates of nicotine absorption and depletion observed across these two modes o f administration, spit t o b a c c o users consistently report
similar experiences o f
Other Prominent Criteria for Nicotine Dependence Individuals w h o
use nicotine-containing
withdrawal,
products also show signs and symptoms associ-
both in terms o f the types o f symptoms expe-
ated with the remaining criteria for nicotine
rienced (Hatsukami, Gust, 8c Keenan, 1987;
dependence. Specifically, a strong and persis-
McChargue 8c Collins, 1998;
McChargue,
tent desire to use t o b a c c o maintains
use
8c Cohen, 2002) and in terms of the level of severity (McChargue 8c Collins, 1998).
Tiffany, 1990) and contributes to difficulties in
Thus, the severity o f withdrawal
quitting (e.g., Tracy,
Collins,
symptoms
may be dictated by a variety o f individual differences, including tobacco use patterns (Killen,
patterns
(Baker, M o r s e , 8c Sherman,
1986;
1994). Researchers ques-
tion whether nicotine's ability to alter emotions (Baker et al., 1986; Carmody, 1990; Hall,
8c Varady, 1991), psy-
Munoz, Reus, 8c Sees, 1993) drives the motiva-
chiatric comorbidities (Pomerleau, Marks, 8c
tion to use tobacco products or whether this
Fortmann, Newman,
2000), and personality factors (Gilbert 8c Gilbert, 1995; Madden et al., 1997).
Pomerleau,
In general, nicotine withdrawal occurs within
increased
motivation
is
more
automatic
(Tiffany, 1990) and independent from emotion (Robinson
8c Berridge, 2000). Nevertheless,
24 hours of abruptly reducing or quitting nico-
nicotine administration appears to create an
tine use, peaks between 48 hours (Hughes 8c
intense motivation to use tobacco products that
Hatsukami, 1986) and 2 weeks (Shiffman, Paty,
is difficult to break regardless o f the mechanism
Guys, Kassel, Sc Elash, 1995; West, Hajek, 8c
that promotes the powerful desire to continue
Belcher,
1989), and resolves after 1 month of abstinence (Hughes, 1992). However, similar
tobacco use.
to withdrawal severity, the duration o f with-
experience extreme difficulty in quitting, and
Individuals who use tobacco also tend to
drawal patterns are also variable. For example,
unsuccessful efforts usually are made before
increases in hunger and weight gain are the most
they are able to quit permanently. In fact, less
persistent symptoms, lasting as long as 6 months
than 5%
to
1 year (Hughes, 1992; Klesges etal., 1997).
of individuals who meet criteria
for nicotine dependence are able to quit on
1990). This percentage
In addition, individuals who quit using nicotine-
their own (Fiore et al.,
containing products
increases to as high as 30%
do not always
report
increased anxiety; however, in cases where
with assisted
treatment for nicotine dependence (Fiore et al.,
104
BEHAVIORS THAT C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH fewer
the brain (Rose & Levin, 1991) and sensitization
people have quit using tobacco products over
of some neurobiological systems (Robinson &
the past decade ( C D C , 2 0 0 2 ) as compared
Berridge, 2 0 0 0 ; Watkins, K o o b , & Markou,
with previous decades (Emmons, Kawachi, &
2 0 0 0 ) . In general, tobacco use behaviors are
Barclay,
T h e apparent plateau o f
maintained by nicotine's ability to enhance
cigarette smoking rates and the increase of spit
desirable effects (positive reinforcement) and to
2 0 0 0 ) . As stated earlier, fewer and
1997).
tobacco and cigar use may suggest that today's
dispel undesirable effects (negative reinforce-
tobacco users are more resistant to treatment
ment). Over time, frequent and repeated use of
efforts and may even possess underlying vul-
tobacco products in specific situations, environ-
nerabilities that further establish tobacco use
ments, and emotional states may automatically
patterns (Gilbert & Gilbert, 1 9 9 5 ) .
trigger tobacco use (secondary conditioning
Finally, it is not uncommon for nicotine-
and
sensitization)
(Rose &
Levin, 1 9 9 1 ;
use
Shiffman, 1 9 9 1 ) . For example, a person who
tobacco products despite physical or psycho-
typically smokes while talking on the phone
logical problems that may result from chronic
may light another cigarette when the phone
nicotine exposure. Familiar examples include
rings without realizing that he or she already
the patient with emphysema w h o continues
had a cigarette lit.
dependent individuals
to continue to
to smoke while attached to an oxygen tank despite the inherent danger o f doing so and the patient
who
smokes through
a
tra-
cheotomy tube. Overall, there is anecdotal and empirical evidence suggesting that many patients with cardiovascular disease, chronic
Positive Reinforcement and Sensitization: A Story of Rewarding Properties The most widely studied neurobiological
obstructive pulmonary disease, and/or cancer—
substrate
all o f which are related to chronic t o b a c c o use
positive reinforcement is dopamine
associated with
nicotine-related (Wise,
( D H H S , 1 9 9 9 ) — c o n t i n u e their patterns o f
1 9 9 8 ) . T h e mesolimbic dopamine system has
use
long been touted as the reward center o f the
(Gritz,
Kristeller, &
Burns,
1993).
Moreover, continued use is associated with a
brain
heightened mortality rate, whereas cessation
(Olds &
post-disease diagnosis may improve
prog-
noses (Gritz et al., 1 9 9 3 ) .
that
shapes goal-directed Milner,
behavior
1 9 5 4 ; Stein, Belluzzi,
Ritter, & Wise, 1 9 7 4 ) , including drug use behavior
(Di C h i a r a ,
Moal, 1997).
1998; Koob &
Consistent with the
Le
reward
hypothesis of dopamine, nicotine's preferenA BIOPSYCHOSOCIAL
tial binding to nicotinic cholinergic receptors
LEARNING MODEL OF
within
NICOTINE DEPENDENCE
(Clarke & Pert, 1 9 8 5 ) and nicotine's reliable
the m e s o l i m b i c dopamine
system
activation o f dopamine release within
the
Nicotine dependence is a complex biopsy-
same system (Pomerleau & Pomerleau, 1 9 8 4 )
chosocial phenomenon that originates from
suggest that
learning theory. The most parsimonious expla-
rewarding effects for people w h o use t o b a c c o .
nation is that nicotine's effects on neurobiological substrates interact with
behavioral,
nicotine produces
powerful
The rewarding effects of nicotine become more powerful over time due to the biphasic
emotional, and cognitive domains to create
nature o f nicotine's influence on
dependence.
release. During nicotine administration, the
Evidence
also
suggests
that
chronic use patterns may produce secondary
dopaminergic
conditioning o f the pharmacological effects on
rather
than
system
becomes
habituated
(e.g.,
dopamine sensitized tolerance)
Etiology
and Treatment
of Nicotine
j
Dependence
2000; Watkins et al.,
These long-lasting memories may help to
2000). In other words, dopamine release is
explain the incongruent psychosocial findings
enhanced,
related to the reward obtained from nicotine
(Robinson & Berridge, rather
than
diminished,
from
repeated exposure to nicotine. As levels o f
administration.
A standard
assumption
has
abstinence,
been that self-reported pleasure (e.g., positive
dopamine also shows neuroadaptative effects.
affect or euphoria) acts as a substitute for the
nicotine are
during
depleted
progressive
rewarding effects of nicotine. However, empiri-
dopamine
cal evidence has not consistently produced data
(Epping-Jordan, Watkins, K o o b , & M a r k o u ,
to support this assumption. If self-reported plea-
1998). T h e ever-growing disparity between
sure mimicked the neurobiological substrates,
sensitized dopamine
one would expect that pleasure would show
Neuroadaptation blunting
reflects the
o f naturally
occurring
release from
nicotine
administration and blunted naturally occur-
sensitizing effects (i.e., more and more pleasure
ring dopamine release during nicotine absti-
from repeated exposure) after nicotine adminis-
nence
tration and would show acute decreases in
is
hypothesized
to
alter
reward
2000), presumably
pleasure during nicotine abstinence. Although
making it very difficult for tobacco users
research shows the expected decrease in plea-
to experience pleasure without the aid o f
sure following nicotine abstinence (Hughes &
nicotine.
Hatsukami,
thresholds (Watkins et al.,
1986), euphoric effects during nico-
Glutamate functioning also appears to play
tine administration are minimal (Pomerleau &
an important role in the positive reinforcement
Pomerleau, 1992) and may further diminish,
of nicotine via its symbiotic relationship with
rather than increase, with repeated exposure
dopamine. As discussed earlier, dopaminergic
(Robinson & Berridge, 2000). If pleasure dimin-
functioning is regarded as the primary mecha-
ishes with chronic nicotine use and is not linked
nism that accounts for the rewarding properties
with dopamine sensitization, memories about
of nicotine. However, glutamate may actually
the pleasure-enhancing effects of nicotine may
strengthen nicotine's rewarding properties and
be sufficient for continued motivation to self-
permanently implant the effect o f such reward
administer nicotine.
into long-term memory. For instance, nicotine
As noted earlier, the rewarding effects o f
administration has been shown to increase glu-
nicotine are long-lasting in a t o b a c c o user's
tamate release within the mesolimbic dopamine
memory system. A plausible psychosocial
system (Garcia-Munoz, Patino, Y o u n g , &
mechanism that takes into account these
Groves, 1996) as well as within hippocampal
embedded reward effects is positive smoking
neurons associated with memory and learning
expectancies or the belief that smoking will
1999). Given
lead to a positive outcome (e.g., relaxation).
that glutamate is strongly linked to learning and
F o r decades, positive drug expectancies have
(Radcliffe, Fisher, Gray, & Dani, memory (Goda & Stevens,
1996), it has been
been shown to reflect long-term drug use pat-
8c T u c k e r , 1988). In fact, a
hypothesized that the simultaneous activation
terns (Vuchinich
of the hippocampal and dopaminergic systems
recent study showed that smoking outcome
solidifies the rewarding properties of nicotine
expectancies combine with one's tendency
(Mansvelder &c McGehee,
2000). Even after
long periods o f abstinence, the responsiveness
to experience negative affective states
to
predict smoking behavior over time (Cohen,
8c Myers, 2002). These
of these systems to nicotine remains abnormal,
McCarthy, Brown,
suggesting that these neurotransmitters play a
findings indicate that at least part o f the
substantial role in the long-lasting, enduring
c o m m o n l y observed
changes associated with nicotine dependence
negative affect and smoking behavior can be
(Pulvirenti & Diana,
2001).
relationship
explained by smoking expectancies.
between
105
106
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH
Negative Reinforcement: A Story of Emotion Regulation
Specifically, older heavy smokers show dosedependent relief from
stress and
following nicotine administration
anxiety (Gilbert,
When considering negative reinforcement
Robinson, Chamberlin, & Spielberger, 1 9 8 9 ) ,
associated with tobacco use disorders, nicotine
with higher doses o f nicotine producing the
administration is believed to have negative
greatest m o o d relief
mood-alleviating properties via its manipula-
Perkins e t a l . , 1 9 9 3 ) . In addition, nicotine
(Gilbert et al., 1 9 8 9 ;
tion o f neurotransmitters such as serotonin
replacement therapy produces clinically signifi-
(Carmody, 1 9 9 0 ; Hall e t a l . , 1 9 9 3 ) . Specifi-
cant reductions in symptoms of depression
cally, low levels of serotonin have been strongly
among nonsmokers suffering
associated with negative mood states (Maes &
depression
Meltzer, 1 9 9 5 ) , and nicotine administration
Genchi, & Rivera-Meza, 1 9 9 6 ) . Nevertheless,
appears to increase levels o f this neurotrans-
mood responses that are not shown to be
mitter (Kenny, File, & Neal, 2 0 0 0 ) . In fact,
related to nicotine withdrawal
nicotine's ability to elevate serotonin levels
variable. For instance, some evidence actually
from
major
(Salin-Pascual, Rosas, Jimenez-
are highly
may partially explain why people report that
indicates that nicotine creates higher levels of
using nicotine-containing products alleviates
anxiety and stress (Parrott, 1 9 9 9 ; Piasecki &
negative affective states (Carmody, 1 9 9 0 ; Hall
Baker, 2 0 0 0 ) . Similarly, smoking in response to
etal., 1 9 9 3 ) . Consistent with the serotonin
depression may increase, rather than decrease,
hypothesis o f nicotine dependence, when one
symptoms of depression among smokers with
abstains
a ruminative coping style (Richmond, Spring,
from
nicotine, medications
that
improve the efficiency of serotonin (e.g., sero-
Sommerfeld, & McChargue, 2 0 0 1 ) .
tonin reuptake inhibitors such as fluoxetine)
Despite the apparent inconsistencies shown
prolong short-term abstinence (Niaura etal.,
among studies examining negative mood relief
2 0 0 2 ) , particularly among smokers with high
from nicotine administration, the importance
baseline levels o f depression (Hitsman etal.,
of the negative reinforcing properties of nico-
1 9 9 9 ) . Moreover, once people abstaining from
tine should not be minimized. In fact, if only a
nicotine are taken off o f this type of medication,
fraction of individuals achieve negative mood
there is an increased likelihood that they will
relief from the administration of nicotine, nega-
experience a major depressive episode (e.g.,
tive affect's role in the maintenance of tobacco
Borrelli et al., 1 9 9 6 ) . Hence, this depressive vul-
use behaviors
nerability during nicotine abstinence is particu-
example, both baseline and post-quit negative
larly salient for depression-prone individuals.
remains
quite
salient. F o r
affect predict relapse (Pomerleau, Adkins, &
Although many people report using tobacco
Pertschuk, 1 9 7 8 ; Swan, Ward, & Jack, 1 9 9 6 ;
products due to their negative mood-alleviating
West et al., 1 9 8 9 ) . Furthermore, a large portion
properties (Spielberger, Foreyt, Reheiser, 8 t
of tobacco users suffer from psychological
Poston, 1 9 9 8 ) , psychosocial research investigat-
problems that are associated with affective dys-
ing this hypothesis is mixed. It is clear that after
regulation (Breslau, 1 9 9 5 ) . Finally, personality
short-term abstinence, nicotine administration
traits that increase the likelihood of experienc-
will reverse any negative affective symptoms
ing frequent and persistent bouts of negative
associated with the nicotine withdrawal syn-
affect predict tobacco use behaviors and relapse
drome. However, it remains unclear whether
(Gilbert & Gilbert, 1 9 9 5 ) . Although it remains
nicotine has the same effect on negative affect
unclear as to the properties o f nicotine that neg-
that is not associated with nicotine withdrawal.
atively reinforce tobacco use, there is sufficient
In some studies, administration of nicotine exhi-
evidence to implicate the importance o f nega-
bits the expected mood-alleviating properties.
tive reinforcement in nicotine dependence.
Etiology
Classical Conditioning: A Story of Automatic Processes
psychological and
(unconditioned
physiological
response)
that
are
repeatedly paired with neutral stimuli (conditioned stimulus). In other words, chronic nicotine administration elicits many reinforcing properties that eventually become conditioned to environmental and psychological stimuli (Iwamoto, Fudala, Mundy, &
Williamson,
1 9 8 7 ; R o s e & Levin, 1 9 9 1 ) . Over time, the repeated pairings between the once neutral stimuli and nicotine administration
Dependence
\
biologically based predispositions that produce
tine administration (unconditioned stimulus) states
of Nicotine
qualitatively different
Classical conditioning occurs when nicoproduces
and Treatment
produce
conditioned responses that initiate and maintain tobacco use behavior (Rose & Levin, 1 9 9 1 ) . Conditioned responses from emotional and environmental cues reflect the activation of cognitive (Tiffany, 1 9 9 0 ) , emotional (Baker et al., 1 9 8 6 ) , and physiological (Robinson &c
reinforcement
from
nicotine administration (Pomerleau & Kardia, 1 9 9 9 ) . Evidence supporting the notion that genetic factors dictate who is likely to become nicotine dependent comes from a variety of sources. For example, twin studies have shown greater concordance rates in monozygotic twins than in dizygotic twins, with heritability estimates o f 5 3 % for tobacco use (see review by Hughes, 1 9 8 6 ) . In addition, certain individuals may be more sensitive to nicotinic properties than are others. A selective sensitivity to nicotine is hypothesized to produce more rapid tolerance and more extensive self-administration patterns (Pomerleau, 1 9 9 5 ) . As such, genetic factors may help to explain why certain subgroups o f smokers become more dependent at earlier ages (e.g., Madden et al., 1 9 9 9 ) and have extreme difficulties in quitting (e.g., Lerman et al., 1 9 9 9 ) .
Berridge, 2 0 0 0 ) domains. Exposure to such cues evokes strong tobacco use motivation or urges. S o m e researchers hypothesize
that
this increase in motivation reflects the desire to evoke a pleasant feeling or to take away unpleasant states (Baker et al., 1 9 8 6 ) , whereas others view this increased motivation as more automatic (Tiffany, 1 9 9 0 ) , that is, driven by sensitized neurobiological systems (Robinson & Berridge, 2 0 0 0 ) .
Gender and Ethnicity Rates of nicotine dependence appear to differ across gender and ethnic groups. In addition, the proportions of men and women who use tobacco products vary greatly in some countries, such as Japan and Greece, but not in others, such as the United States and the United Kingdom (Grunberg, Winders, 8 t Wewers, 1 9 9 1 ) . Thus, it may be that tobacco use is reinforced differently for women in countries where as many women use tobacco products as do
OTHER IMPORTANT FACTORS IN NICOTINE DEPENDENCE RESEARCH
men. In addition, certain minority populations (e.g., African Americans) within the United States report higher rates of tobacco use than do Caucasians ( C D C , 1 9 9 9 ) , and women and
Genetics
minorities appear to be less successful at quit-
The development o f nicotine dependence cannot result entirely from random actions between neurobiological and
interpsy-
ting (Piper, F o x , Welsch, Fiore, & Baker, 2 0 0 1 ) . Therefore, these individuals
are at
greater risk for contracting smoking-related ill-
chosocial factors. It has been suggested that
nesses, making it very important to consider
individuals who use tobacco and become nico-
how gender and ethnicity influence the recruit-
tine dependent may be different from individu-
ment, retention, and treatment o f nicotine-
als w h o
dependent individuals (Piper et al., 2 0 0 1 ) .
do not use t o b a c c o because o f
107
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH
108
M o r e is known about the etiology and
nicotine dependence
(Hughes,
Hatsukami,
treatment of nicotine dependence for women
Mitchell, & Dahlgren, 1 9 8 6 ) . Comorbid psy-
than for different ethnic groups. The scarcity of
chopathology represents an important issue
research on minorities that use tobacco products
to address in nicotine dependence research
has led many researchers and practitioners to
because these individuals report excessive
examine nicotine dependence among minority
dependence levels and have extreme difficulty
populations. Contemporary knowledge regard-
in quitting (Hughes et al., 1 9 8 6 ; McChargue,
ing what motivates U.S. women to use tobacco
Gulliver, & Hitsman, 2 0 0 2 a , 2 0 0 2 b ) . M o r e -
products, particularly cigarettes, has focused on
over, psychiatric smokers are at a heightened
two primary issues: (a) affect regulation and
risk of smoking and psychiatric-related health
(b) weight control. In general, women are more
problems as compared with nonsmoking psy-
affectively vulnerable than men, and it is
chiatric patients and nonpsychiatric smokers
believed that this vulnerability is well suited for
(Jeste, Gladsjo, Lindamer, & Lacro, 1 9 9 6 ;
nicotine's mood-alleviating effects. As such,
Linkins & Comstock, 1 9 9 0 ) . Prevalence rates
women may receive greater mood regulatory
of smoking among this population range from
benefits from smoking than do men, and this is
3 1 % to 9 0 % , depending on the psychiatric
believed to partially explain why women have
disorder (Beckham et al., 1 9 9 7 ; de Leon et al.,
more difficulty in quitting (Piper et al., 2 0 0 1 ) . In
1 9 9 5 ; Hughes et al., 1 9 8 6 ) .
addition, women frequently express concern
It has been hypothesized
that chronic
about gaining weight after they quit smoking
tobacco use observed among individuals with
(Klesges & Klesges, 1 9 8 8 ) . This concern is not
psychopathological problems reflects self-
surprising given that individuals who are absti-
medicating behaviors. According to the self-
nent for 1 year will gain an average of 13
medication hypothesis, psychiatric patients
pounds (Klesges et al., 1 9 9 7 ) .
smoke in part because nicotine helps to regulate their symptomatology (Gilbert & Gilbert,
Comorbid Personality and Psychopathology The influence o f personality on tobacco use
1995).
F o r example, patients with
major
depression may smoke to improve depressed m o o d states (Hall et al., 1 9 9 3 ) . Similarly, patients with schizophrenia may find
that
is based on the belief that traits predispose
smoking helps to reduce negative symptoms
people to frequent
such as anhedonia, apathy, blunted affect, and
and persistent aversive
mood states (Cloninger, 1 9 8 7 ; Tellegen, 1 9 8 5 ;
emotional withdrawal
Tomkins & McCarter, 1 9 6 4 ) . As such, many
2 0 0 2 a , 2 0 0 2 b ) . Finally, patients with posttrau-
( M c C h a r g u e et al.,
theorize that chronic exposure to mood dysregu-
matic stress disorder and other anxiety disor-
lation provides ample opportunity for people to
ders may smoke to cope with emotional and
learn that tobacco products are an efficient
physiological distress (Beckham et al., 1 9 9 7 ) .
source of relief from these problematic affective
This self-medication process
transforms
states. Traits that are associated with compro-
tobacco use into an extremely
mised affective systems and tobacco use behav-
behavior for psychiatric individuals as com-
ior
pared
include sensation seeking, neuroticism,
with
nonpsychiatric
rewarding
cohorts
who
extroversion, and psychoticism (Gilbert 8 t
report similar tobacco use patterns (Spring,
Gilbert, 1 9 9 5 ; Spielberger & Jacobs, 1 9 8 2 ) .
Pingitore, & McChargue, in press). As such,
Contemporary research has identified an
the goal o f complete abstinence might not
overwhelming proportion o f patients with psy-
be
chiatric mood, anxiety, and psychotic prob-
subgroups (e.g., individuals diagnosed with
lems as possessing high levels o f comorbid
schizophrenia). These individuals may require
initially possible for some psychiatric
Etiology a stepped care approach
and Treatment
of Nicotine
Dependence
\
that focuses on
most comprehensive evaluation possible. The
reducing exposure to tobacco toxins until the
evaluation should include the assessment o f
individuals are able to stabilize lower rates o f
physiological, psychological, and social factors
tobacco use and learn adequate coping skills
that appear to influence the patient's tobacco
(McChargue et al., 2 0 0 2 a , 2 0 0 2 b ) .
use patterns (Ockene, Kristeller, &c Donnelly, 1 9 9 9 ) . Pertinent information may be acquired via a clinical interview, self-report measures, a chart review, and corroboration from behav-
ASSESSMENT AND T R E A T M E N T
ioral medicine staff. Ockene and colleagues As discussed earlier, nicotine dependence is
( 1 9 9 9 ) noted that, at the very least, an assess-
maintained by many factors across diverse
ment of nicotine dependence starts with a
patient populations. Another issue that leads
clinical interview. During this interview, physi-
to difficulties in the treatment and assessment
ological assessment questions should include
of nicotine dependence is the pervasive nature o f
past quit attempts, withdrawal symptoms expe-
tobacco use. Specifically, the widespread use o f
rienced during past quit attempts, and the
tobacco products forces clinical health psychol-
patient's
ogists to address issues related to this construct
Questions within the social domain should
in a variety of settings and situations. Hence,
include the number of friends, family members,
perceived addiction
to nicotine.
clinicians are encouraged to be mindful o f the
and coworkers who use tobacco products; the
settings or situations in which they deliver their
expected amount o f social support or nonsup-
interventions (Collins etal., 1 9 9 9 ) . Overall,
port; and the degree to which the patient can be
it is recommended that a multidisciplinary
assertive at rebuffing pressure from others to
approach to the assessment and treatment o f
smoke (Ockene et al., 1 9 9 9 ) . Included within
nicotine dependence be used if long-term absti-
the psychological assessment should be ques-
nence is to be achieved (Fagerstrôm, 1 9 9 1 ) .
tions related to emotional problems (e.g., stress, depression), behavioral indexes (e.g., extent to which person will go to have a cigarette), and
Assessment of Nicotine Dependence
cognitive factors (e.g., self-efficacy beliefs about
T h e assessment o f nicotine dependence m a y take many forms at differing levels o f intensity within behavioral medicine settings. F o r example, the approach to assessments o f nicotine dependence in an emergency r o o m is likely to differ from the approach to assessments used in outpatient settings. Therefore, the assessment should be tailored to the specific setting. Prior to beginning an assessment,
the clinician should
consider
the
purpose for the assessment, the environment
quitting [Ockene etal., 1999]). Ockene and colleagues ( 1 9 9 9 ) also noted that gathering information about a patient's smoking history, as well as having the patient self-monitor his or her smoking behavior, can provide useful data that may aid in treatment specificity. Finally, health factors that may be a consequence of chronic use patterns should also be assessed, and patients who present with physical complaints (e.g., shortness of breath) should be referred to a physician (Ockene et al., 1 9 9 9 ) .
where the assessment will take place, and the
Pretreatment assessment can be as brief as a
form o f intervention conducive to the setting.
10-minute clinical interview or as long as a 2V2hour structured assessment. Again, the setting
Pretreatment To
provide
essential t h a t
and purpose of the assessment should dictate
Assessments adequate the
treatment,
clinician c o n d u c t
the type o f assessment administered. For a relait is
tively brief assessment or for information that
the
will be incorporated within a larger assessment,
109
110
BEHAVIORS THAT C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH there are a variety o f standardized self-report measures that can be used to assess level o f dependence, self-efficacy, readiness to quit, general reasons for use, and perceptions of what tobacco products do for the person. For more comprehensive interviews, several
structured
and semistructured interviews are available, including the Diagnostic Interview Schedule (Malgady, Rogler, & Tryon, 1 9 9 2 ) and the Structured Clinical Interview for the D S M - I V Axis I Disorders-Clinician Version (First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1 9 9 7 ) . Most self-report measures have adequate psychometric properties and have been used with a
4. The Contemplation Ladder (Biener &c Abrams, 1991): This is a measure of readiness to consider tobacco cessation. It is designed to assess a tobacco user's position on a continuum ranging from having no thoughts of quitting to being engaged in action to change one's tobacco use. The ladder is consistent with Prochaska and DiClemente's (1983) model, which states that tobacco cessation is the culmination of an extended process of behavior change. The measure employs a picture of a ladder, where each rung has an associated number that the patient is instructed to circle representing where he or she is in thinking about quitting.
variety of populations. Although a detailed description of all tobacco-related measures available is beyond the scope of this chapter, the following measures are recommended.
Posttreatment
Assessments
Posttreatment assessment allows the clinician to measure and adjust treatment efforts as
1. The Fagerstrôm Test of Nicotine Dependence (Heatherton, Kozlowski, Frecker, & Fagerstrôm, 1991): This is a 6item, self-report questionnaire designed to assess various components of smoking behavior, including an estimate of daily intake, difficulty in refraining, and other aspects related to the pattern of intake.
necessary. Issues that may arise include treatment compliance, sudden exacerbation o f clinical disorders
(e.g., major
depression),
severe tobacco withdrawal, intense and persistent urges to use nicotine, weight gain, brief smoking lapses, and abstinence status. M a n y o f these issues can be assessed using clinical interviews. A supplementary self-report measure is Scale
2. The Smoking Self-Efficacy Questionnaire (Colletti, Supnick, St Payne, 1985): This is a 17-item questionnaire designed to assess respondents' beliefs about their ability to control their urges to smoke in a variety of situations.
the Minnesota Nicotine Withdrawal
3. The Smoking Consequences Questionnaire (Brandon & Baker, 1991): The Smoking Consequences Questionnaire (SCQ) is a 50item measure designed to assess expectations associated with cigarette smoking. It has four factors: negative consequences (e.g., health risks), positive reinforcement/sensory satisfaction (e.g., taste, relaxation), negative reinforcement/negative affect reduction (e.g., reduction of sadness and anxiety), and appetite/weight control. The SCQ-Adult (Copeland, Brandon, Si Quinn, 1995) is an extension of the SCQ for use with an older population of dependent smokers.
cian may assess tobacco use daily, weekly, or at
(Hughes & Hatsukami, 1 9 8 6 ) , which assesses withdrawal severity. This measure may be used repeatedly to assess withdrawal across time. For purposes of assessing treatment compliance, lapsing, and abstinence status, the clinidesignated follow-up times. Self-reported lapses and relapses in isolation or combined with biochemical verification have been used. A detailed description of the utility of biochemical markers o f tobacco and cessation as well as recommendations for their application in clinical practice is beyond the scope of this chapter. However, the Society for Research on Nicotine and T o b a c c o Subcommittee on Biochemical Verification
( 2 0 0 2 ) recently published
an
overview of this subject. In general, the committee noted that there are currently three
Etiology biomarkers used to assess whether a person has
and Treatment
Brief
of Nicotine
Dependence
|
Interventions
been abstinent from nicotine: thiocyanate (SCN), cotinine, and expired carbon monoxide
Brief interventions are designed to be used
( C O ) . S C N and cotinine are metabolites of
in a variety o f settings and should not take
nicotine that indicate tobacco use over the
more than 3 to 1 0 minutes. Brief interventions
past week, and C O (as obtained via expired air)
include assessing tobacco use patterns and will-
indicates smoked tobacco use within the past
ingness to quit, advising the patient to make a
2 4 hours. T h e committee also noted
quit attempt, assisting the patient in quitting,
that
the standard indication o f tobacco use has
and scheduling follow-up sessions (Fiore et al.,
been cotinine levels above 15 nanograms per
2 0 0 0 ) . Unfortunately, not all tobacco-using
milliliter, carbon monoxide levels above 8 to 1 0
patients are highly motivated to quit. In cases
parts per million, and S C N levels of 7 8 to 8 4
where the patient is not so motivated, it is rec-
micromoles per liter. (For a more in-depth
ommended that the clinician use an empathetic
review o f biochemical verification recommen-
therapeutic
dations, see Society for Research on Nicotine
increases self-efficacy, and encourages adaptive
and T o b a c c o Subcommittee on Biochemical
skills at quitting (Prochaska 8c Goldstein,
style that
avoids
arguments,
1 9 9 1 ) . Confrontational and punitive styles may
Verification, 2 0 0 2 . )
have the opposite effect by further decreasing
Treatment of Nicotine Dependence T h e treatment
o f nicotine dependence
the patient's motivation to quit (Miller
&
Rollnick, 2 0 0 2 ) . Brief assistance that has been shown to
multidisciplinary
increase abstinence rates may be as simple as
stepped care approach. T h e stepped care
providing the patient with self-help material
approach starts with the assessment o f the
combined with recommending approved phar-
patient's motivation to quit and progresses to
macological
the implementation o f brief interventions. For
Newman, 8c Varady, 1 9 9 0 ) . Other brief tech-
should
be viewed as a
treatment
(Killen,
Fortmann,
example, routine screening and brief counsel-
niques include (a) helping to identify upcoming
ing (less than 3 minutes) within emergency
challenges, (b) processing helpful skills from
rooms increase long-term abstinence rates
prior quit attempts, (c) reducing alcohol con-
from 3 % (usual care) to 8 % to 1 1 % (Bernstein
sumption during the first month of abstinence,
& Becker, 2 0 0 2 ) . If the patient is unmotivated
(d) encouraging others w h o use tobacco within
t o quit, the clinician should
incorporate
the same household to quit as well, (e) provid-
motivational enhancing techniques within the
ing social support within the clinical environ-
brief intervention. Only after brief
ment, and (f) helping the patient to find another
inter-
ventions are unsuccessful should the clinician
supportive environment (Fiore et al., 2 0 0 0 ) .
refer the patient to more intensive treatments.
T h e first follow-up session should be sched-
As discussed in the next two subsections, all
uled approximately 1 week after the quit date,
recommendations for brief and intensive inter-
with the second scheduled 1 to 3 weeks later
ventions are consistent with the clinical prac-
(Fiore et al., 2 0 0 0 ) . During the follow-up, the
tice guidelines for treating nicotine dependence
clinician should remain supportive, highlight
(Fiore e t a l . , 2 0 0 0 ) . It should be noted that
successes (no matter h o w small), and encour-
both brief and intensive interventions recom-
age problem solving. T h e patient may have a
mend the inclusion o f pharmacological treat-
tendency to overemphasize an aspect o f the
ment.
However,
pharmacological
agents
quit attempt that is linked to failure (Shiffman
associated with treating nicotine dependence
et a l , 1 9 9 6 ) . If so, the clinician should try to
are discussed in a separate subsection.
reframe the perceived failure as a learning
111
112
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH experience and reengage the patient in prob-
Before a patient attempts to quit, the patient is encouraged to gather as much about his or
lem solving (Fiore et al., 2 0 0 0 ) .
her smoking habit as possible. T h e patient is Intensive
asked to pay attention to specific triggers that
Treatments
he or she believes will challenge the attempt
It is recommended that intensive treat-
at quitting. Tobacco-related triggers include
ments be offered to all tobacco users because,
situations, emotions, thoughts, and places
on average, such interventions are more effec-
that evoke strong urges to use tobacco. Self-
tive than
monitoring smoking behavior prior to a quit
brief interventions
(Fiore et al., however,
attempt often will help the patient to identify
might not be feasible. Thus, for each individ-
tobacco-related triggers that are relevant to his
2000).
This
recommendation,
ual w h o wishes to quit, the clinician is
or her life. However, it is not atypical for many
encouraged to use a stepped care approach
other "unexpected" triggers to arise once the
that starts brief and progressively increases in
patient has achieved abstinence. Thus, it is
intensity. T h e reason for this is that, under
important to continue monitoring triggers long
certain circumstances, brief interventions can
after the quit date. It is also suggested that the
be more effective than intensive interventions
clinician provide the patient with education
(Smith e t a l . , 2 0 0 1 ) , and brief interventions
regarding the withdrawal symptoms that he or
are more practical in a variety o f settings (e.g.,
she may experience as well as the addictive
primary care facilities).
nature o f tobacco because this information can aid the patient in understanding the process o f
Treatment Treatments.
Format
for
Intensive
T o qualify as an intensive treat-
ment, there must be a minimum of four sessions lasting more than 1 0 minutes each (Fiore et al., 2 0 0 0 ) . If feasible, group sessions o f 8 to 10 people are recommended over individual sessions because the group setting fosters social support (Ockene et al., 1 9 9 9 ) . Sessions should be scheduled on a weekly basis during the initial 4 weeks o f treatment and then biweekly for the next 4 weeks (Ockene et al., 1 9 9 9 ) . Finally,
addiction (Fiore et al., 2 0 0 0 ; Kozlowski et al., 2 0 0 1 ) . For example, skills training focused on problem solving and symptom management are helpful (Fiore e t a l . , 2 0 0 0 ) . In addition, there are several nonspecific treatment factors that the clinician should provide during treatment. These treatment factors include discussing and eliciting positive expectancies, being supportive and understanding, and providing a time line for the quit attempt (Fiore et al., 2 0 0 0 ; Kozlowski et al., 2 0 0 1 ) .
posttreatment follow-ups should be scheduled
Once the patient quits, he or she may expe-
6 to 1 2 months after the quit date (Kozlowski,
rience a variety o f nicotine withdrawal symp-
Henningfield, & Brigham, 2 0 0 1 ) .
toms that undermine
quit attempts. It is
important for the clinician to assist the patient M a n y o f the psy-
in coping with these symptoms, particularly
chosocial components used in tobacco cessation
during the first month o f abstinence. T h e clin-
treatment packages are cognitive-behavioral in
ician should encourage the patient to use the
nature. T h e purpose of these components is to
skills he or she learned during the pre-quit ses-
Treatment
Components.
break the association between smoking and
sions. For example, encouraging the use o f
other life activities and to increase the patient's
relaxation techniques (e.g., removing oneself
ability to cope during abstinence. As stated
from
earlier, pharmacological therapies are highly
provides an alternative means by which to
recommended
cope with stressful situations and negative
in
conjunction
psychosocial interventions.
with
these
stressful
situations, deep
affect associated with tobacco
breathing)
withdrawal
Etiology
and Treatment
of Nicotine
Dependence
\
(Dziegielewski & Eater, 2 0 0 0 ; Hatsukami &
have been shown to approximately double
Lando, 1 9 9 9 ) . T h e use o f accessible substi-
abstinence rates when compared with placebo
tutes, such as chewing gum, may also help the
treatments.
patient to cope with withdrawal
symptoms
Second-line treatments
have also been
(Cohen, Britt, Collins, al'Absi, & McChargue,
found to be efficacious, but the use o f these
2 0 0 1 ; Cohen, Britt, Collins, Stott, & Carter,
medications is limited due to the lack o f F D A
1 9 9 9 ; Cohen, Collins, & Britt, 1 9 9 7 ) . In addi-
approval as treatment for nicotine depen-
tion, encouraging the patient to avoid situa-
dence as well as concerns about potential
tions where tobacco use is likely to occur (e.g.,
side effects. Second-line treatments include
bars, bowling alleys) as well as to engage in
fluoxetine, clonidine, nortriptyline, and
healthy alternative behaviors (e.g., exercise)
combination o f nicotine replacement thera-
may help to prolong abstinence (Dziegielewski
pies. M e n t i o n o f second-line therapies is lim-
& Eater, 2 0 0 0 ; Ockene et al., 1 9 9 9 ) .
ited to this paragraph because such therapies
a
Finally, working with the patient to develop
are not viable treatment options at this time.
the requisite skills to elicit social support from
T h u s , this subsection limits further discus-
others outside o f treatment is integral to suc-
sion o f pharmacology to first-line treatments.
cessful tobacco cessation (Fiore et al., 2 0 0 0 ) . O n e type o f social support outside o f therapy
Nicotine
Replacement
Therapy.
Nicotine
that should be suggested is Nicotine Anony-
replacement therapy ( N R T ) is intended to
mous (NicA). These mutual-help groups pro-
break the conditioning o f nicotine with envi-
vide social and emotional support for many
ronmental cues by making nicotine intake
sufferers o f addictive disorders through per-
independent
sonal sharing on a weekly basis (Lichtenstein,
(Glover & Glover, 2 0 0 1 ) . Although N R T
1 9 9 9 ) . It has been suggested that N i c A may be
provides lower doses o f nicotine than
most effective for highly dependent smokers or
other t o b a c c o products, it can be used to
o f events in the environment do
those who also abuse another substance (e.g.,
decrease the severity o f withdrawal
alcohol). At the very least, patients who do not
toms by providing a slow consistent dose o f
have a significant outside support system may
nicotine through an alternate administration
symp-
need more frequent contact from a clinician
route
to support them during their quit attempts
comes in many different forms, including
(Ockene et al., 1 9 9 9 ) .
gums, patches, nasal sprays, and inhalers.
(Jarvis &
Sutherland,
1998).
NRT
Nicotine Polacrilex (gum) was the first Pharmacological
NRT
Interventions
According to the Clinical Practice
approved
by
the
FDA
(Jarvis &
Sutherland, 1 9 9 8 ) . T h e absorption rate is fairly Guidelines
rapid, and peak nicotine levels are reached
(Fiore et al., 2 0 0 0 ) , many first-line medications
within 2 0 to 3 0 minutes
exist for the treatment of nicotine dependence,
Lando, 1 9 9 9 ) . Use is recommended for 3
(Hatsukami &
as do several second-line medications. First-line
months (Hatsukami & Lando, 1 9 9 9 ) . Although
medications have been established as efficacious
nicotine gum can be used on an as-needed basis
through clinical trials and have been approved
to control t o b a c c o urges, a fixed schedule has
by the Food and Drug Administration (FDA)
been shown to be more effective in dealing
for
use with nicotine dependence. First-line
with withdrawal symptoms (Ockene et al.,
medications include nicotine replacement prod-
1 9 9 9 ) . Nicotine gum is dispensed in 2 - or
ucts (e.g., gums, patches, nasal sprays, inhalers)
4-milligram doses, with the 4-milligram dose
and buproprion-SR (sustained release). With the
recommended for heavily dependent smokers
exception of nicotine gum, these interventions
(Fiore et al., 2 0 0 0 ) .
BEHAVIORS THAT C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH
114
The nicotine patch has a passive delivery system. T h e absorption of nicotine is slower than with the gum (Jarvis 8 t Sutherland, 1 9 9 8 ) , resulting in peak levels of nicotine 4 to 9 hours after administration
(Hatsumaki &
Lando,
1 9 9 9 ) . T h e patch is available in either 24-hour (Habitrol, Nicoderm, and Nicoderm C Q ) or 16-hour doses (Nicotrol). Typically, 24-hour patches have 2 1 or 2 2 milligrams of nicotine, whereas patches designed for 16-hour use have 15
milligrams o f nicotine (Hatsukami &
Lando, 1 9 9 9 ; Ockene et al., 1 9 9 9 ) . A nicotine nasal spray is available with a prescription
(Fiore
et al.,
2000)
and
decreases craving within minutes o f use due to rapid
absorption
rates
(Hatsukami &
If All Else Fails.
. .
If all else fails, the clinician is advised to lower tobacco use behavior when abstinence appears to be initially unattainable (McChargue etal., 2 0 0 2 a , 2 0 0 2 b ) and/or pharmacological therapies are not suitable (Ockene etal., 1999). Reducing tobacco use may be accomplished via nicotine fading. Nicotine fading involves switching to a brand with lower nicotine levels as well as gradually decreasing the quantity of tobacco used (Ockene et al., 1 9 9 9 ) . For example, once cigarette consumption has been decreased to 5 to 1 0 cigarettes per day and has been stabilized at this level, a quit date should be reestablished (Ockene etal., 1 9 9 9 ) .
Lando, 1 9 9 9 ; Jarvis & Sutherland, 1 9 9 8 ; O c k e n e et al., 1 9 9 9 ) . Treatment is typically 6 to 8 weeks but can be extended to 3 months
CONCLUSIONS
in severe cases (Hatsukami & Lando, 1 9 9 9 ; O c k e n e et al., 1 9 9 9 ) . It is important to note
Chronic use o f tobacco products has been
that the nasal spray may be more effective in
linked to a number o f serious health problems
situations where instant relief from nicotine
that
craving is a priority (Hurt et al., 1 9 9 8 ) .
world. It appears that nicotine dependence
affect many people throughout
the
Finally, the nicotine inhaler dispenses 1 0 mil-
develops via the interaction between neurobi-
ligrams of nicotine per inhaler cartridge (Eissen-
ological substrates and cognitive, behavioral,
berg, Stitzer, & Henningfield, 1999; Hatsukami
and emotional domains. Although there are a
& Lando, 1999). A unique feature of the inhaler
variety o f factors that contribute to the devel-
is that it provides oral and tactile reinforcement
opment o f nicotine dependence, once people
because it consists of a mouthpiece and a nicotine
are dependent, it is clear that they have
cartridge as well as nicotine (Hatsukami
extreme difficulty in quitting. For the most
&
Lando, 1999; Ockene etal., 1999).
part, treatment o f nicotine dependence takes a stepped care approach, which begins with
Buproprion is
brief interventions and progresses to more
an antidepressant medication that has been
intensive interventions. T h e goal of this chap-
shown to aid in the management o f nicotine
ter was to increase knowledge about nicotine
Non-nicotine
Therapies.
withdrawal symptoms (Johnston, R o b i n s o n ,
dependence and to provide guidance on inter-
Adams, Glassman, & Covey, 1 9 9 9 ) . Although
vention strategies for treating individuals with
the mechanism o f buproprion is not c o m -
nicotine dependence. Given the pervasiveness
pletely clear (Johnston et al., 1 9 9 9 ) , it is pre-
of t o b a c c o use disorders
sumed to block neural reuptake o f dopamine
patient populations and the health conse-
among
various
and/or norepinephrine (Fiore et al., 2 0 0 0 ) .
quences associated with these disorders, treat-
Buproprion remains the only non-nicotine
ment o f nicotine dependence is one area in
medication used in t o b a c c o cessation pro-
which clinical health psychologists can have a
grams that is approved by the F D A (Fiore
positive impact on their patients'
et al., 2 0 0 0 ; J o h n s t o n et al., 1 9 9 9 ) .
health status.
overall
Etiology
and Treatment
of Nicotine
Dependence
CASE STUDY This case study illustrates an intensive smoking treatment. T h e client, " B e t t y , " was a 67-year-old Caucasian female w h o was referred by her primary care physician for individual smoking treatment. Betty presented with complaints o f having " n o c o n t r o l " over her smoking behavior but having a strong desire to quit smoking. She also reported that her health was "failing" and that her physician would not perform " a necessary medical procedure" unless she quit. Specifically, Betty noted that she suffered from numerous medical problems, including chronic bronchitis, asthma, and emphysema. Medical concerns had reduced her independence by causing her to rely on a motorized scooter for community mobility. Betty reported that she lived in an apartment by herself and noted that she had very little local social support. She did indicate, however, that she had several relatives w h o lived "out o f state" with w h o m she talked via phone on a weekly basis. During Betty's intake session, she was asked to exhale into a C O monitor and to complete the Fagerstrôm Test o f Nicotine Dependence. Results revealed a C O reading of 4 8 parts per million (indicating heavy smoking rates) and a test score o f 9 (indicating a high level o f nicotine dependence). In addition to these measures, a detailed account o f her smoking history and quit attempts was obtained via clinical interview. In sum, Betty reported smoking her first cigarette at the age of 1 2 years and progressing to daily cigarette smoking by the age o f 1 4 years. She noted that when she was smoking at her heaviest rate, she smoked two packs ( 4 0 cigarettes) per day, but she was currently smoking 2 5 cigarettes per day. Betty reported that she had tried unsuccessfully to quit smoking many times in her life, noting that she could recall four occasions when she made "serious attempts" to quit by using group smoking cessation programs, using nicotine replacement patches, and stopping "cold turkey." Betty made it clear to the therapist that she did not want to use nicotine replacement patches this time because she had "vivid disturbing dreams" the last time she had used them. She noted that her previous quit attempts resulted in temporary cessation, with her longest period o f abstinence being a little more than Wi years. She also noted, on a scale o f 1 to 1 0 , that she had a strong desire to quit smoking ( 1 0 / 1 0 ) , that it was very important that she quit smoking ( 1 0 / 1 0 ) , but that she was only somewhat confident in her ability to quit (5/10). Betty agreed to attend weekly sessions for the next 8 weeks. Betty and the therapist collaboratively planned to reduce her nicotine intake and to have her learn m o r e about her smoking behavior (e.g., when she smoked, where she smoked, why she smoked) during the first 4 weeks o f treatment. A quit date was set for W e e k 5, and during W e e k s 6 to 8 it was decided that the focus o f treatment would be on issues related to relapse prevention. Betty left the intake session with two " h o m e w o r k " assignments, namely (a) to attempt to reduce smoking intake by 1 0 % during the week and (b) to keep a written record o f her smoking behavior. Specifically, each time she was about to smoke a cigarette, Betty was asked to write down the time o f day, any emotions she was feeling at the time, and the situational circumstances that occurred just prior to her smoking.
115
116
BEHAVIORS THAT C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH
Betty presented
for her second session on time and
indicated that
she
had reduced her smoking intake successfully and kept accurate records o f her smoking behavior. She was eager to report that by the end o f the week, she was able to reduce her smoking t o 1 0 cigarettes per day (a far greater reduction than the agreed-on 1 0 % ) . She also noted that she wanted to smoke more frequently than she actually did; however, she "got s i c k " o f writing down all o f the requested information before each cigarette. She noted that tracking her smoking behavior allowed her to cut out many cigarettes that she did n o t "really need." She also indicated that in reducing her smoking intake, she learned that the "cravings" she had to smoke throughout the day would not "last forever" and in fact passed rather quickly, usually within 1 0 t o 1 5 minutes. After praising Betty on the progress she had made during the past week, the therapist reviewed the "smoking record sheets" that Betty had completed in an attempt to identify commonalities in her smoking behavior. E x a m i n a t i o n o f the smoking record sheets revealed that Betty smoked most often after eating meals and during times o f perceived stress. In an attempt to aid Betty during these difficult times, urge control strategies were discussed. O n e o f the keys t o success in smoking cessation is learning h o w to get through urges or cravings to smoke. Given that Betty had already learned h o w to get through some o f her urges to smoke, the therapist enlisted Betty's help to get a sense o f what worked for her during the past week. Betty noted that if she just waited long enough, her urge to smoke would go away (although she was quick to point out that the urges would return). Building on Betty's success, the therapist noted that delaying smoking might not w o r k in all situations and taught Betty a number o f other urge control strategies that might prove to be useful in situations where delaying smoking was t o o difficult. T h e therapist outlined five basic strategies that Betty could pull from her " t o o l b o x " when faced with difficult urges: (a) delaying smoking, (b) escaping from situations or events that may contribute to the urge, (c) avoiding situations where the temptation t o smoke may be t o o great, (d) ί/istracting herself by thinking about or doing other things that she enjoys doing, and (e) substituting something else for a cigarette such as sugarless gum, candy, or sunflower seeds. (All o f these strategies can be remembered by the simple yet appropriate acronym o f D E A D S . ) T h e therapist encouraged Betty to continue doing what worked for her the previous week and to try some o f the other strategies that were taught when the urge to smoke surfaced. F o r " h o m e w o r k , " Betty was again encouraged to reduce her smoking by 1 0 % . T h e therapist also encouraged Betty to pick a "smoking p l a c e " in her h o m e where she usually did not smoke and did not engage in other activities such as talking on the phone, socializing, eating, watching television, and reading mail. It was suggested that she smoke only in this place, with the idea being that she would not associate smoking in this place with any other kind o f activity. Also, it would mean that she would have t o stop what she was doing so as to smoke a cigarette.
Etiology
During
the third
and
fourth
and Treatment
weeks o f treatment,
of Nicotine
Betty w a s
Dependence
able
to
reduce her smoking to 4 cigarettes per day using the strategies discussed earlier. In anticipation o f the W e e k 5 quit date, most o f the fourth session was centered on preparing Betty for her quit attempt. She was instructed to have her final cigarette no later than before she went to bed on the night before she was to attend her fifth session. She was also encouraged to "seek out and destroy" all o f the cigarettes that remained in her apartment that evening so as to be sure that there would not be any cigarettes readily available to her when she w o k e up the next morning. She was also educated a b o u t w h a t types o f withdrawal symptoms she might expect (e.g., depressed m o o d , irritability, anxiety) so that they would not catch her " o f f guard." In addition, detailed plans were made outlining h o w she would deal with her cravings to smoke so that she had a "plan o f a t t a c k " if a craving surfaced. She was also encouraged to start thinking a b o u t h o w to reward herself once she quit. During the fifth session, Betty was n o t as animated as she had been during the previous 4 weeks o f treatment. She reported that she had n o t s m o k e d a cigarette since before she went to bed the previous evening; however, she noted that she "really wanted o n e . " T h e therapist reinforced Betty for all o f her hard w o r k and reminded her that her cravings would pass and that the intensity and frequency o f the cravings would dissipate over time. T h e remainder o f the session was spent discussing the health benefits that she could expect over the n e x t several weeks (e.g., decrease in coughing and sinus congestion, increase in overall energy level). Finally, Betty was informed that she might " s l i p " and s m o k e a cigarette during the course o f the n e x t w e e k . She was told that this is " n o r m a l " and that if it happens, she should l o o k at it as just a " s l i p , " n o t a " t o t a l relapse." She was encouraged to get b a c k to being " s m o k e free" after the slip rather than to give herself permission to s m o k e as m a n y cigarettes as she w a n t e d and view her efforts as a failure. Sessions 6 to 8 began with Betty exhaling into the C O monitor to s h o w her that her C O levels were decreasing, thereby increasing the amount o f oxygen that was circulating throughout her body. H e r readings were 1 1 , 8, and 7, respectively. In addition, these sessions centered on ways in which Betty could prevent relapse. She had done exceptionally well and did not experience a slip during these 3 weeks. Betty and the therapist worked on anticipating difficult situations and planned ahead as to h o w she would cope with these situations if and when they arose. Betty was particularly concerned about w h a t she would do in stressful situations that were bound to arise in the future. It was discussed h o w she could take a "time out," removing herself from the situation, taking deep breaths, and/or thinking o f something fun she had recently done rather than smoking. At the end o f Session 8, Betty was commended for her hard w o r k , given information about local support groups (in case she desired additional help), and was scheduled for three " b o o s t e r sessions" 1, 3 , and 6 months later.
117
118
\
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH
At 1-month follow-up, Betty reported that she had one slip during the holidays as she was caught off guard by her " e m o t i o n s . " She noted that she recognized what she was doing and immediately put out the cigarette and did not allow herself to smoke again. She noted that she repeatedly reminded herself o f her hard w o r k and told herself that she refused to "go b a c k to Square O n e . " She also noted that this slip was a "reality c h e c k " and reminded her that she should n o t get overconfident about her progress and that she had to continue to w o r k on her addiction to nicotine. Betty admitted that she "would be lying" if she said she did not want a cigarette. But she added that the cravings were not occurring as often and that they were not as severe when they did occur. Betty was praised by the therapist and was encouraged to "keep her guard up." At the 3- and 6-month follow-ups, Betty reported that she had not slipped again and that her energy levels were up. At the 6-month follow-up, she reported that it looked as though her physician was considering performing the medical procedure she needed.
REFERENCES American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washington, DC: Author. Baker, T. B., Morse, E., & Sherman, J . E. (1986). The motivation to use drugs: A psychobiological analysis of urges. In C. Rivers (Ed.), The Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Alcohol Use and Abuse (pp. 2 5 7 - 3 2 3 ) . Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Balfour, D. J . (1991). Neural mechanisms underlying nicotine dependence. Addiction, 89, 1 4 1 9 - 1 4 2 3 . Beckham, J . C , Kirby, A. C , Feldman, M. E., Hertzberg, Μ . Α., Moore, S. D., Crawford, A. L., Davidson, J . R. T., & Fairbank, J . A. (1997). Prevalence and correlates of heavy smoking in Vietnam veterans with chronic posttraumatic stress disorder. Addictive Behaviors, 22, 6 3 7 - 6 4 7 . Benowitz, N. L. (1990). Pharmacokinetic considerations in understanding nicotine dependence. In The biology of nicotine dependence (pp. 1 8 6 - 2 0 9 ) . Chichester, UK: Wiley. Benowitz, N. L., Porchet, H., & Jacob, P., III. (1990). Pharmacokinetics, metabolism, and pharmacodynamics of nicotine. In S. Wonnacott, Μ. A. H. Russell, & I. P. Molecular, cellular, and Stolerman (Eds.), Nicotine psychopharmacology: behavioural aspects (pp. 1 1 2 - 1 5 7 ) . Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Bernstein, S. L., &c Becker, Β. M. (2002). Preventive care in the emergency department: Diagnosis and management of smoking and smoking-related illness in the emergency department—A systematic review. Academic Emergency Medicine, 9, 720-729. Biener, L., & Abrams, D. B. (1991). The contemplation ladder: Validation of a measure of readiness to consider smoking cessation. Health Psychology, 10, 3 6 0 - 3 6 5 .
Etiology
and Treatment
of Nicotine
Dependence
Borrelli, B . , Niaura, R., Keuthen, N., Goldstein, M., Depue, J . , Murphy, C., 8c Abrams, D. B. (1996). Development of major depressive disorder during smoking cessation treatment. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 57, 5 3 4 - 5 3 8 . Brandon, T. H., & Baker, T. B . (1991). The Smoking Consequences Questionnaire: The subjective expected utility of smoking in college students. Psychological Assessment, 3, 4 8 4 - 4 9 1 . Breslau, N. (1995). Psychiatry comorbidity of smoking and nicotine dependence. Behavioral Genetics, 25, 9 5 - 1 0 1 . Carmody, T. P. (1990). Affect regulation, nicotine addiction, and smoking cessation. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 24, 1 1 1 - 1 2 2 . Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (1999). Cigarette smoking among adults: United States, 1 9 9 7 . Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 48, 993-996. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2002). Cigarette smoking among adults: United States, 2000. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 51, 6 4 2 - 6 4 5 . Clark, P. B . , & Pert, A. (1985). Autoradiographic evidence for nicotine receptors on nigrostriatal and mesolimbic dopaminergic neurons. Brain Research, 348, 355-358. Cloninger, C. R. (1987). A systematic method for clinical description and classification of personality variâtes: A proposal. Archives of General Psychiatry, 44, 5 7 3 - 5 8 8 . Cohen, L. M., Britt, D. M., Collins, F. L., Jr., al'Absi, M., & McChargue, D. E. (2001). Multimodal assessment of the effect of chewing gum on nicotine withdrawal. Addictive Behaviors, 26, 2 8 9 - 2 9 5 . Cohen, L. M., Britt, D. M., Collins, F. L., Jr., Stott, H. D., & Carter, L. C. (1999). Chewing gum affects smoking topography. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 7, 4 4 4 - 4 4 7 . Cohen, L. M., Collins, F. L., Jr., & Britt, D. M . (1997). The effect of chewing gum on tobacco withdrawal. Addictive Behaviors, 22, 7 9 6 - 7 6 3 . Cohen, L. M., McCarthy, D. M., Brown, S. Α., & Myers, M . G. (2002). Negative affect combines with smoking outcome expectancies to predict smoking behavior. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 16, 9 1 - 9 7 . Colletti, G., Supnick, J . Α., 8c Payne, T. J . (1985). The Smoking Self-Efficacy Questionnaire (SSEQ): Preliminary scale development and validation. Behavioral Assessment, 7, 2 4 9 - 2 6 0 . Collins, F. L., Jr., Britt, D. M., Cohen, L. M., McChargue, D. E., Larson, Μ . Ε. M., Leftwich, M . J . T., Se Stott, H. D. (1999). Treating nicotine dependence. In L. VandeCreek &c T. L. Jackson (Eds.), Innovations in clinical practice: A source book (Vol. 17, pp. 1 3 5 - 1 5 1 ) . Sarasota, FL: Professional Resources Press. Copeland, A. L., Brandon, T. H., 8c Quinn, E. P. (1995). The Smoking Consequences Questionnaire-Adult: Measurement of smoking outcome expectancies of experienced smokers. Psychological Assessment, 7, 4%4-494. de Leon, J . D., Dadvand, M., Canuso, C , White, A. O., Stanilla, J . K., & Simpson, G. M. (1995). Schizophrenia and smoking: An epidemiological survey in a state hospital. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 453—455. Di Chiara, G. (1998). A motivational learning hypothesis of the role of mesolimbic dopamine in compulsive drug use. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 12, 54-67'. Dziegielewski, S. F., & Eater, J . A. (2000). Smoking cessation: Increasing practice understanding and time-limited intervention strategy. Families in Society, 81, 2 4 6 - 2 5 5 . Eissenberg, T., Stitzer, M. L., 8c Henningfield, J . E. (1999). Current issues in nicotine replacement. In D. F. Seidmean &c L. S. Covey (Eds.), Helping the hardcore smoker: A clinician's guide (pp. 1 3 7 - 1 5 8 ) . Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Emmons, K. M., Kawachi, I., &c Barclay, G. (1997). Tobacco control: A brief review of its history and prospects for the future. Hematological Oncology Clinics of North America, 2, 1 7 7 - 1 9 5 .
\
119
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH Epping-Jordan, M. P., Watkins, S. S., Koob, G. F., &c Markou, A. (1998). Dramatic decreases in brain reward function during nicotine withdrawal. Nature, 393, 6 - 7 9 . Fagerstrôm, Κ. Ο. (1991). Towards better diagnoses and more individual treatment of tobacco dependence. British Journal of Addiction, 86, 5 4 3 - 5 4 7 . Fiore, M . C , Bailey, W. C , Cohen, S. J . , Dorfman, S. F., Goldstein, M. G., Gritz, E. R., Heyman, R. B., Jaén, C. R., Kottke, T. E., Lando, Η. Α., Mecklenburg, R. E., Mullen, P. D., Nett, L. M., Robinson, L., Stitzer, M. L., Tommasello, A. C , Villejo, L., &c Wewers, M . E. (2000). Clinical practice guidelines: Treating tobacco use and dependence. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Public Health Service. Fiore, M. C , Novotny, T. E., Pierce, J . P., Giovino, G. Α., Hatziandreu, E. J . , Newcomb, P. Α., Surawicz, T. S., & Davis, R. M. (1990). Methods used to quit smoking in the United States. Journal of the American Medical Association, 263, 2 7 6 0 - 2 7 6 5 . First, M . B . , Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J . B . W. (1997). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders-Clinician's Version (SCIDCV). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Garcia-Munoz, M., Patino, P., Young, S. J . , &C Groves, P. M. (1996). Effects of nicotine on dopaminergic nigrostriatal axons requires stimulation of presynaptic glutamatergic receptors. Journal of Pharmacology & Experimental Therapeutics, 277, 1 6 8 5 - 1 6 9 3 . Gilbert, D. G., & Gilbert, B. O. (1995). Personality, psychopathology, and nicotine response as mediators of the genetics of smoking. Behavior Genetics, 25(2), 133-147. Gilbert, D. G., Robinson, J . H., Chamberlin, C. L., & Spielberger, C. D. (1989). Effects of smoking/nicotine on anxiety, heart rate, and lateralization of EEG during a stressful movie. Psychophysiology, 26, 3 1 1 - 3 2 0 . Glover, E. D., & Glover, P. N. (2001). Pharmacologic treatments for the nicotine dependent smoker. American Journal of Health Behavior, 25, 1 7 9 - 1 8 2 . Goda, Y . , & Stevens, C. F. (1996). Synaptic plasticity: The basis of particular types of learning. Current Biology, 6, 3 7 5 - 3 7 8 . Gritz, E. R., Kristeller, J . L., & Burns, D. M . (1993). Treating nicotine addiction in high-risk groups and patients with medical co-morbidity. In C. T. Orleans & J . S. Slade (Eds.), Nicotine addiction: Principles and management (pp. 2 7 9 - 3 0 9 ) . New York: Oxford University Press. Grunberg, Ν. E., Winders, S. E., &C Wewers, M . E. (1991). Gender differences in tobacco use. Health Psychology, 10, 1 4 3 - 1 5 3 . Hall, S. M., Munoz, F., Reus, V. I., & Sees, K. L. (1993). Nicotine, negative affect, and depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 761-767. Hatsukami, D. K., Gust, S. W., & Keenan, R. M . (1987). Physiologic and subjective changes from smokeless tobacco withdrawal. Clinical Pharmacology Therapy, 41, 1 0 3 - 1 0 7 . Hatsukami, D. K., & Lando, H. (1999). Smoking cessation. In P. J . Ott, R. E. Tarter, & R. T. Ammerman (Eds.), Sourcebook on substance abuse: Etiology, epidemiology, assessment, and treatment (pp. 399—415). Boston: Allyn 8c Bacon. Hatsukami, D. K., 8t Severson, H. H. (1999). Oral spit tobacco: Addiction, prevention, and treatment. Nicotine and Tobacco Research, 1, 2 1 - 4 4 . Heatherton, T. F., Kozlowski, L. T., Frecker, R. C , Se Fagerstrôm, Κ. Ο. (1991). The Fagerstrôm Test for Nicotine Dependence: A revision of the Fagerstrôm Tolerance Questionnaire. British Journal of Addiction, 86, 1 1 1 9 - 1 1 2 7 . Henningfield, J . E., Fant, R. V., Radzius, Α., δί Frost, S. (1999). Nicotine concentration, smoke pH, and whole tobacco aqueous pH of some cigar brands and types popular in the United States. Nicotine and Tobacco Research, 1, 1 6 3 - 1 6 8 .
Etiology
and Treatment
of Nicotine
Dependence
Henningfield, J . E., &c Heishman, S. J . (1995). The addictive role of nicotine in tobacco use. Psychopharmacology, 117, 1 1 - 1 3 . Hitsman, B., Pingitore, R., Spring, B., Mahableshwarkar, Α., Mizes, J . S., Segraves, Κ. Α., Kristeller, J . L., &c X u , W. (1999). Antidepressant pharmacotherapy helps some cigarette smokers more than others. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67, 5 4 7 - 5 5 4 . Hughes, J . R. (1986). Genetics of smoking: A brief review. Behavior Therapy, 17, 335-345. Hughes, J . R. (1992). Tobacco withdrawal in self-quitters. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 6 8 9 - 6 9 7 . Hughes, J . R., & Hatsukami, D. (1986). Signs and symptoms of tobacco withdrawal. Archives of General Psychiatry, 43, 2 8 9 - 2 9 4 . Hughes, J . R., Hatsukami, D., Mitchell, J . E., & Dahlgren, L. A. (1986). Prevalence of smoking among psychiatric outpatients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 143, 9 9 3 - 9 9 7 . Hughes, J . R., Higgins, S. T., & Hatsukami, D. (1990). Effects of abstinence from tobacco: A critical review. In L. T. Kozlowski, H. Annis, H. D. Cappell, F. Glaser, M. Goodstadt, Y . Israel, H. Kalant, Ε. M . Sellers, & J . Vingilis (Eds.), Research advances in alcohol and drug problems, (pp. 3 1 7 - 3 9 8 ) . New York: Plenum. Hurt, R. D., Offord, K. P., Croghan, I. T., Croghan, G. Α., Gomez-Dahl, L. C., Wolter, T. D., Dale, L. C., & Moyer, T. P. (1998). Temporal effects of nicotine nasal spray and gum on nicotine withdrawal symptoms. Psychopharmacology, 140, 9 8 - 1 0 4 . Irvin, J . E., & Brandon, T. H. (2000). The increasing recalcitrance of smokers in clinical trials. Nicotine and Tobacco Research, 2, 7 9 - 8 4 . Iwamoto, E. T., Fudala, P. J . , Mundy, W. R., &c Williamson, E. C. (1987). Nicotine actions in models of learning/memory and reward. In W. R. Martin, G. R. Van Loon, Ε. T. Iwamoto, &C L. Davis (Eds.), Tobacco smoking and nicotine: A neurobiological approach (pp. 1 0 1 - 1 1 1 ) . New York: Plenum. Jarvis, M., & Sutherland, G. (1998). Tobacco smoking. In D. W. Johnston & M. Johnston (Eds.), Comprehensive clinical psychology (pp. 6 4 5 - 6 7 4 ) . New York: Elsevier Science. Jeste, D. V., Gladsjo, J . Α., Lindamer, L. Α., & Lacro, J . P. (1996). Medical comorbidity in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 22, 4 1 3 - 4 3 0 . Johnston, Α., Robinson, M . D., Adams, D. P., Glassman, A. H., &c Covey, L. S. (1999). Nonnicotine medications for smoking cessation. In D. F. Seidmean & L. S. Covey (Eds.), Helping the hard-core smoker: A clinician's guide (pp. 1 5 9 - 1 7 3 ) . Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Kenny, P. J . , File, S. E., &C Neal, M. J . (2000). Evidence for a complex influence of nicotinic acetylcholine receptors on hippocampal serotonin release. Journal of Neurochemistry, 75, 2 4 0 9 - 2 4 1 4 . Killen, J . D., Fortmann, S. P., Newman, B . , & Varady, A. (1990). Evaluation of a treatment approach combining nicotine gum with self-guided behavioral treatments for smoking relapse prevention. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 58, 8 5 - 9 2 . Killen, J . D., Fortmann, S. P., Newman, B . , & Varady, A. (1991). Prospective study of factors influencing the development of craving associated with smoking cessation. Psychopharmacology, 105, 1 9 1 - 1 9 6 . Klesges, R. C , & Klesges, L. M . (1988). Cigarette smoking as a dieting strategy in a university population. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 7, 4 1 3 - 4 1 9 . Klesges, R. C , Winders, S. E., Meyers, A. W., Eck, L. H., Ward, K. D., Hultquist, C. M., Ray, J . W., &c Shadish, W. R. (1997). How much weight gain occurs following smoking cessation? A comparison of weight gain using both continuous and point prevalence abstinence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 2 8 6 - 2 9 1 .
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH Koob, G. F., 8t Le Moal, M . (1997). Drug abuse: Hedonic homeostatic dysregulation. Science, 278, 5 2 - 5 8 . Kozlowski, L. T., Henningfield, J . E., 8t Brigham, J . (2001). Cigarettes, nicotine, and health: A biobehavioral approach. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Lerman, C , Caporaso, Ν. E., Audrain, J . , Main, D., Bowman, E. D., Lockshin, B., Boyd, N. R., 8t Shields, P. G. (1999). Evidence suggesting the role of specific genetic factors in cigarette smoking. Health Psychology, 18, 1 9 - 3 4 . Lichtenstein, E. (1999). Nicotine Anonymous: Community resource and research implications. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 13, 6 0 - 6 8 . Linkins, R. W., St Comstock, G. W. (1990). Depressed mood and development of cancer. American Journal of Epidemiology, 132, 9 6 2 - 9 7 2 . Madden, P. A. F., Bucholz, K. K., Dinwiddie, S. H., Slutske, W. S., Beirut, L. J . , Statham, D. J . , Dunne, M . P., Martin, N. G., St Heath, A. C. (1997). Nicotine withdrawal in women. Addiction, 92, 8 8 9 - 9 0 2 . Madden, P. A. F., Heath, A. C , Pedersen, N. L., Kaprio, J . , Koskenvuo, M . J . , 8t Martin, N. G. (1999). The genetics of smoking persistence in men and women: A multicultural study. Behavior Genetics, 29, 4 2 3 - 4 3 1 . Maes, M., St Meltzer, H. Y . (1995). The serotonin hypothesis of major depression. The fourth generIn F. E. Bloom St D. J . Kupfer (Eds.), Psychopharmacology: ation of progress (pp. 9 3 3 - 9 4 4 ) . New York: Raven. Malgady, R. G., Rogler, L. H., St Tryon, W. W. (1992). Issues of validity in the Diagnostic Interview Schedule. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 26, 5 9 - 6 7 . Mansvelder, H. D., St McGehee, D. S. (2000). Long-term potentiation of excitatory inputs to brain reward areas by nicotine. Neuron, 27, 3 4 9 - 3 5 7 . McChargue, D. E., St Collins, F. L. (1998). Differentiating withdrawal patterns between smokers and smokeless tobacco users. Experimental and Clinical 6, 2 0 5 - 2 0 8 . Psychopharmacology, McChargue, D. E., Collins, F. L., St Cohen, L. M . (2002). Effect of non-nicotinic moist snuff replacement and lobeline on withdrawal symptoms during 48-hour smokeless tobacco deprivation. Nicotine and Tobacco Research, 4, 1 9 5 - 2 0 0 . McChargue, D. E., Gulliver, S. B . , St Hitsman, B . (2002a). A reply to the commentaries on schizophrenia and smoking treatment: More research is needed. Addiction, 97, 7 8 5 - 7 9 3 . McChargue, D. E., Gulliver, S. B . , St Hitsman, B . (2002b). Would smokers with schizophrenia benefit from a more flexible approach to smoking treatment? Addiction, 97, 7 9 9 - 8 0 0 . Miller, W. R., St Rollnick, S. (2002). Motivational interviewing: Preparing people for change. New York: Guilford. Niaura, R., Spring, B., Borrelli, B., Hedeker, D., Goldstein, M. G., Keuthen, N., DePue, J . , Kristeller, J . , Ockene, J . , Prochazka, Α., Chiles, J . Α., St Abrams, D. B. (2002). Multicenter trial of fluoxetine as an adjunct to behavioral smoking cessation treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 8 8 7 - 8 9 6 . Ockene, J . K., Kristeller, J . L., St Donnelly, G. (1999). Tobacco. In M . Galanter St of substance H. D. Kleber (Eds.), The American Psychiatric Press textbook abuse treatment (2nd ed., pp. 2 1 5 - 2 3 8 ) . Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Olds, J . , St Milner, P. (1954). Positive reinforcement produced by electrical stimuComparative lation of septal area and other regions of rat brain. Journal of Physiological Psychology, 47, 4 1 9 - 4 2 7 . Parrott, A. C. (1999). Does cigarette smoking cause stress? American Psychologist, 54, 8 1 7 - 8 2 0 . Perkins, Κ. Α., Gerlach, D., Broge, M., Sanders, M., Grobe, J . , Fonte, C , Cherry, C , Wilson, Α., St Jacob, R. (2001). Quitting cigarette smoking produces min158, 7 - 1 7 . imal loss of chronic tolerance to nicotine. Psychopharmacology,
Etiology
and Treatment
of Nicotine
Dependence
Perkins, Κ. Α., Grobe, J . E., Epstein, L. H., Caggiula, Α., Stiller, R. L., Sc Jacob, R. G. (1993). Chronic and acute tolerance to subjective effects of nicotine. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 45, 3 7 5 - 3 8 1 . Piasecki, T. M., & Baker, T. (2000). Does smoking amortize negative affect? American Psychologist, 55, 1 1 5 6 - 1 1 5 7 . Piper, M. E., Fox, B. J . , Welsch, S. K., Fiore, M . C , & Baker, T. B . (2001). Gender and racial/ethnic differences in tobacco-dependence treatment: A commentary and research recommendations. Nicotine and Tobacco Research, 3, 2 9 1 - 2 9 7 . Pomerleau, C. S., Marks, J . L., Sc Pomerleau, O. F. (2000). Who gets what symptom? Effects of psychiatric cofactors and nicotine dependence on patterns of smoking withdrawal symptomatology. Nicotine and Tobacco Research, 2, 2 7 5 - 2 8 0 . Pomerleau, C. S., St Pomerleau, O. F. (1992). Euphoriant effects of nicotine in smokers. Psychopharmacology, 108, 4 6 0 - 4 6 5 . Pomerleau, O. F. (1995). Individual differences in sensitivity to nicotine: Implications for genetic research on nicotine dependence. Behavior Genetics, 25, 1 6 1 - 1 7 7 . Pomerleau, O., Adkins, D., Se Pertschuk, M . (1978). Predictors of outcome and recidivism in smoking cessation treatment. Addictive Behaviors, 3, 6 5 - 7 0 . Pomerleau, O. F., Se Kardia, S. L. R. (1999). Introduction to the featured section: Genetic research on smoking. Health Psychology, 18, 3 - 6 . Pomerleau, O. F., Sc Pomerleau, C. S. (1984). Neuroregulators and the reinforcement of smoking: Towards a biobehavioral explanation. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 8, 5 0 3 - 5 1 3 . Porchet, H. C , Benowitz, N. L., Se Sheiner, L. B . (1988). Pharmacodynamic model of tolerance: Application to nicotine. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 244, 2 3 1 - 2 3 5 . Prochaska, J . O., Sc DiClemente, C. C. (1983). Stages and processes of self-change of smoking: Toward an integrative model of change. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 3 9 0 - 3 9 5 . Prochaska, J . , Sc Goldstein, M . G. (1991). Process of smoking cessation. Implications for clinicians. Clinical Chest Medicine, 12, 7 2 7 - 7 3 5 . Pulvirenti, L., Sc Diana, M. (2001). Drug dependence as a disorder of neural plasticity: Focus on dopamine and glutamate. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 12, 1 4 1 - 1 5 8 . Radcliffe, Κ. Α., Fisher, J . L., Gray, R., Sc Dani, J . A. (1999). Nicotinic modulation of glutamate and GABA synaptic transmission of hippocampal neurons. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 868, 5 9 1 - 6 1 0 . Richmond, M., Spring, B . , Sommerfeld, Β. K., Sc McChargue, D. E. (2001). Rumination and cigarette smoking: A bad combination for depressive outcomes? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69, 8 3 6 - 8 4 0 . Robinson, J . H., Se Pritchard, W. S. (1992). The role of nicotine in tobacco use. Psychopharmacology, 108, 3 9 7 - 4 0 7 . Robinson, T. E., Sc Berridge, K. C. (2000). The psychology and neurobiology of addiction: An incentive-sensitization view. Addiction, 95, S 9 1 - S 1 1 7 . Rose, J . E., Sc Levin, E. D. (1991). Inter-relationships between conditioned and primary reinforcement in the maintenance of cigarette smoking. British Journal of Addiction, 86, 6 0 5 - 6 0 9 . Salin-Pascual, R. J . , Rosas, M., Jimenez-Genchi, Α., Se Rivera-Meza, B . L. (1996). Antidepressant effect of transdermal nicotine patches in nonsmoking patients with major depression. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 57, 3 8 7 - 3 8 9 . Shiffman, S. (1991). Refining models of dependence: Variations across persons and situations. British Journal of Addiction, 86, 6 1 1 - 6 1 5 . Shiffman, S., Hickcox, M., Paty, J . Α., Guys, M., Kassel, J . D., Sc Richards, T. J . (1996). Progression from a smoking lapse to relapse: Prediction from abstinence violation effects, nicotine dependence, and lapse characteristics. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 9 9 3 - 1 0 0 2 .
124
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH Shiffman, S., Paty, J . Α., Guys, M., Kassel, J . D., & Elash, C. (1995). Nicotine withdrawal in chippers and regular smokers: Subjective and cognitive effects. Health Psychology, 14, 3 0 1 - 3 0 9 . Smith, S. S., Jorenby, D. E., Fiore, M. C , Anderson, J . E., Mielke, M . M., Beach, K. E., Piasecki, T. M., & Baker, T. B . (2001). Strike while the iron is hot: Can stepped-care treatments resurrect relapsing smokers? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69, 4 2 9 - 4 3 9 . Society for Research on Nicotine and Tobacco Subcommittee on Biochemical Verification. (2002). Biochemical verification of tobacco use and cessation. Nicotine and Tobacco Research, 4, 1 4 9 - 1 5 9 . Spielberger, C. D., Foreyt, J . P., Reheiser, E. C , & Poston, W. S. C. (1998). Motivational, emotional, and personality characteristics of smokeless tobacco users compared with cigarette smokers. Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 8 2 1 - 8 3 2 . Spielberger, C. D., St Jacobs, G. A. (1982). Personality and smoking behavior. Journal of Personality Assessment, 46, 3 9 6 - 4 0 3 . Spring, B., Pingitore, G., & McChargue, D. E. (in press). Reward value of cigarette smoking for comparably heavy smoking schizophrenic, depressed, and nonpatient smokers. American Journal of Psychiatry. Stein, L., Belluzzi, J . D., Ritter, S., St Wise, C. D. (1974). Self-stimulation reward pathways: Norepinephrine vs. dopamine. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 11,115-124. Swan, G. E., Ward, M . M., & Jack, L. M . (1996). Abstinence effects as predictors of 28-day relapse in smokers. Addictive Behaviors, 21, 4 8 1 - 4 9 0 . Tellegen, A. (1985). Structures of mood and personality and their relevance to assessing anxiety, with an emphasis on self-report. In H. Tuma St J . Maser (Eds.), Anxiety and anxiety disorders (pp. 6 8 1 - 7 0 6 ) . Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Tiffany, S. T. (1990). A cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use behavior: Role of automatic and nonautomatic processes. Psychological Reviews, 97, 1 4 7 - 1 6 8 . Tomkins, S., St McCarter, R. (1964). What and where are the primary affects? Some evidence for a theory. Perception and Motor Skills, 18, 1 1 9 - 1 5 6 . Tracy, J . I. (1994). Assessing the relationship between craving and relapse. Drug and Alcohol Review, 13, 7 1 - 7 7 . Tsoh, J . Y., Humfleet, G. L., Munoz, R. F., Reus, V. L, Hartz, D. T., & Hall, S. M. (2000). Development of major depression after treatment for smoking cessation. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 3 6 8 - 3 7 4 . U.S. Department of Agriculture. (1997). Tobacco situation and outlook report (Series TBS, No. 2 3 9 ) . Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Agriculture, Economic Research Service, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (1999). Tobacco Use: United States, 1 9 0 0 - 1 9 9 9 . Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 48, 9 8 6 - 9 9 3 . Vuchinich, R. E., St Tucker, J . A. (1988). Contributions from behavioral theories of choice to an analysis of alcohol abuse. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 181-195. Watkins, S. S., Koob, G. F., & Markou, A. (2000). Neural mechanisms underlying nicotine addiction: Acute positive reinforcement and withdrawal. Nicotine and Tobacco Research, 2, 1 9 - 3 7 . West, R., & Hajek, P. (1997). What happens to anxiety levels on giving up smoking? American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 1 5 8 9 - 1 5 9 2 . West, R., Hajek, P., & Belcher, M. (1989). Severity of withdrawal symptoms as a predictor of outcome of an attempt to quit smoking. Psychological Medicine, 19, 9 8 1 - 9 8 5 . Wise, R. A. (1998). Drug-activation of brain reward pathways. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 51, 1 3 - 2 2 .
CHAPTER
5
Obesity and Body Image Disturbance MYLES S. FAITH AND J . KEVIN THOMPSON
O
besity in the United States and other
divided by the square o f height in meters
countries is a significant and grow-
(w/h ). This method is also referred to as
ing public health problem. T h e U.S.
Quetelet's index (Garrow & Webster, 1 9 8 5 ) .
surgeon general recently released a report
B M I can also be computed from pounds and
referring to the current rates o f obesity as a
inches: weight (in pounds) divided by height
2
"public health epidemic" (Centers for Disease
(in inches) times 7 0 4 . 5 .
Control
weight classification are presented in Table
and
Prevention, 2 0 0 2 ) .
Obesity
B M I cutoffs for
among adults, adolescents, and children is
8 . 1 . N o t e that a B M I between 2 5 . 0 and 2 9 . 9
increasingly encountered in clinical settings,
connotes an overweight status, with
and
greater than 3 0 indicating obesity.
an
awareness
o f current etiological
models, assessment strategies, and interven-
BMIs
Despite its widespread use, there are limits
tion methodologies is necessary for the opti-
to
mal management o f this important
health
(Heymsfield et al., 2 0 0 0 ) . F o r instance, it
problem. This chapter explores these issues,
c a n n o t be used as a specific indicator o f the
with an emphasis on the practical strategies
level o f body fat on the individual, and it is
that may inform the behavioral management
influenced by factors such as age, gender,
B M I as a measure
o f weight
status
of obesity. It also provides a brief discussion
and exercise status (i.e., sedentary vs. active).
o f targeting body image in obesity treatment.
Specifically, w o m e n tend to have a higher percentage o f body fat than do men given the same B M I . In addition,
older
individuals
DEFINITIONS AND DESCRIPTION OF OBESITY
tend to have a higher percentage o f body fat
A consensus has emerged during recent years
cially those w h o lift weights) may have a
that one o f the best methods for clinically
lower percentage o f body fat than do indi-
defining obesity is the body mass index ( B M I ) .
viduals w h o are less athletic for a specific
T h e formula for B M I is weight in kilograms
BMI.
than do younger individuals for a given B M I , and people w h o w o r k out regularly (espe-
226
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH Table 8.1
Classification of Overweight and Obesity by BMI Obesity
Underweight Normal Overweight Obesity
Class
I II III
Extreme obesity
BMI < 18.5 18.5-24.9 25.0-29.9 30.0-34.9 35.0-39.9 >40.0
high blood pressure, high blood cholesterol, gout,
and
Colditz,
gallstones (Field, B a r n o y a , 2002).
(More
information
& on
physical problems relevant to the assessment o f obesity c a n be found later in this chapter.)
Psychological and Psychosocial Problems A wide variety of psychological problems
S O U R C E : N I H / N H L B I (1998).
have been examined as a concomitant of obesity, and perhaps even more research efforts during
Prevalence statistics reported during the past few years reveal an astonishing increase in the level o f overweight and obese individuals in the United States (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2 0 0 2 ;
Mokdad,
B o w m a n , & Ford, 2 0 0 1 ; M o k d a d , Serdula, &
Dietz, 1 9 9 9 , 2 0 0 0 ) . Currently, 6 1 % o f
U.S. adults ages 2 0 to 7 4 years are either overweight or obese. T h e level o f obesity increased from 1 2 % o f the U.S. population in 1 9 9 1 to 1 9 . 8 % in 2 0 0 0 . Thus, an estimated 3 8 . 8 million adults in the United States met the B M I cutoff o f 3 0 . 0 for obesity in the year 2 0 0 0 . T a b l e 8.2 displays these data by gender and ethnicity. These data reflect the particularly high levels o f obesity in
African
American and Hispanic populations.
recent years have focused on the psychosocial "consequences" of obesity. Psychosocial consequences entail the specific interpersonal, social, and occupational problems encountered by the obese individual specifically due to an elevated weight status such as being teased about one's size, facing societal prejudice against obesity (which may have economic consequences), and encountering physical barriers (e.g., plane seats that are too small). Somewhat surprisingly, the common assumption that an elevated weight must necessarily be associated with a plethora of psychological problems is not supported by the literature (Faith &c Allison, 1 9 9 6 ) . Early work in the area did not confirm the expected finding that obese individuals were more depressed than nonobese individuals. Recently, however, a nationally representative sample of more than 4 0 , 0 0 0 people were evaluated via structured interviews for level
PHYSICAL AND PSYCHOLOGICAL PROBLEMS ASSOCIATED WITH OBESITY
of depression (Carpenter, Hasin, Allison, & Faith, 2 0 0 0 ) . Obese women were 3 7 % more likely to have met D S M - I V (Diagnostic Statistical
Physical Problems
edition)
Manual criteria
of Mental
Disorders,
(American
and fourth
Psychiatric
A wide variety o f health problems are
Association, 1 9 9 4 ) for major depression during
associated with obesity. It has been estimated
the previous year compared with women of
that
325,000
deaths could be
attributed
average weight. O n the other hand, obese men
(Allison, F o n t a i n e ,
had a significantly reduced risk for depression.
M a n s o n , Stevens, & Vanltallie, 1 9 9 9 ) . A n
Interestingly, for men, being underweight was
to obesity each year
overweight or obese status has been linked to
associated with greater depression. Research
such seemingly disparate health problems as
into the reasons behind such a dramatic gender
heart disease, cancer, type 2 diabetes, stroke,
difference in the psychosocial consequences of
arthritis, breathing problems (sleep apnea),
obesity is an active area of inquiry.
Obesity and Body Image Disturbance
\
Increasing; Adult Obesity Prevalence: Obesity Trends (percentages)
Table 8.2 Sample
1991
1995
1998
2000
Total Gender Men Women Ethnicity White, non-Hispanic Black, non-Hispanic Hispanic
12.0
15.3
17.9
19.8
11.7 12.2
15.6 15.0
17.7 18.1
20.2 19.4
11.3 19.3 11.6
14.5 22.6 16.8
16.6 26.9 20.8
18.5 29.3 23.4
S O U R C E : M o k d a d , B o w m a n , Ford, Vinicor, M a r k s , and K o p l a n (2001).
disturbances
obese individuals is, o f course, that they live in a
with obesity has been a controversial area.
society that glorifies the antithesis of obesity—
T h e comorbidity o f eating
Obesity is not a diagnosable psychiatric condi-
a slender, nonfat, "ideal" body (Thompson,
tion, and it is not listed as an eating disorder in
Heinberg, Altabe, & Tantleff-Dunn,
the D S M - I V (nor has it been labeled as such in
Pressure and information related by the media,
previous editions o f the D S M ) . However, the
family, and peers may be a constant reminder
association o f a particular type o f eating dis-
of the unacceptable nature of the physicality of
1999).
turbance—binge eating—has been noted as an
obese individuals. For instance, negative verbal
associate of obesity for many years (Stunkard,
commentary in the form o f teasing or criticism
2 0 0 2 ) . During recent years, binge eating disor-
of appearance is a c o m m o n experience o f obese
der has been proposed as a new entry in the
individuals. Some studies have shown
D S M and is currently included in the D S M - I V
more than 9 0 % o f obese individuals have been
that
as a disorder in need of further study. T h e issue
teased about their appearance (Thompson &c
is relevant for obesity because perhaps 3 0 % to
Smolak, 2 0 0 1 ) . Interestingly, teasing may be a
4 0 % o f individuals with binge eating disorder
more direct influence on body dissatisfaction
In
than is weight. In fact, Thompson, Coovert,
addition, individuals with binge eating dis-
Richards, Johnson, and Cattarin ( 1 9 9 5 ) found
order have higher levels of depression than do
that B M I had no direct effect on body image
non-binge eating controls (Wadden, W o m b l e ,
but was mediated through teasing history.
are also obese (Johnson 8 t Torgrud, 1996).
Anderson, 2 0 0 2 ) . Recently,
T h a t is, only those individuals with an elevated
Bulik, Sullivan, and Kendler ( 2 0 0 2 ) examined
B M I who had been teased developed body
the prevalence o f binge eating and obesity in a
dissatisfaction.
Stunkard,
&
population-based
sample o f female twins.
Occupational and
social discrimination
Obese women with binge eating disorder had
against obese individuals is also widely sup-
higher levels o f health dissatisfaction, major
ported
medical disorders, major depression, panic
Gortmaker, Must, Perrin, Sobol, and Dietz
disorder, phobias, and alcohol dependence.
by survey and
laboratory
studies.
( 1 9 9 3 ) evaluated more than 1 0 , 0 0 0 overweight
O n e of the most consistent findings related
and normal-weight adolescents for 7 years. At
to body image dissatisfaction is that obese
the conclusion of the study, overweight females
individuals are more dissatisfied with their
were less likely to be married and also had lower
appearance than are nonobese individuals. One
incomes, whereas overweight males were only
possible reason for the greater dissatisfaction of
less likely to be married. Experimental designs
127
128
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH have also demonstrated that job applicants'
scores. Finally, urban planning and
weight has a powerful effect on factors such as
often do not include sidewalks or bike paths to
sprawl
selection for a particular j o b , promotion, and
encourage exercise, and residents who perceive
dismissal (e.g., Pingitore, Dugoni, Tindale, &
their neighborhoods to be unsafe may be
Spring, 1 9 9 4 ; Roehling, 1 9 9 9 ) .
reluctant to leave their homes. Horgen and Brownell ( 2 0 0 2 ) referred to the "toxic environment" as a primary cause o f the emerging problem of obesity. Support for
MODELS OF OBESITY
this position is enhanced not only by the recent Genetic,
and
epidemic in the United States but also by
offered
the burgeoning problem worldwide, leading
metabolic, environmental,
developmental
models have been
to explain the onset and maintenance o f
the World Health Organization ( 1 9 9 8 )
obesity. T o date, evidence supports each as a
declare that a global epidemic exists. Obesity
potentially relevant factor. T h e r e is, how-
now appears to be increasing in every country
to
ever, a great deal o f variability across indi-
that has been surveyed. Migration, urbani-
viduals, making unitary models limited in
zation,
their explanatory value. Given the tremen-
"Americanization" o f food selections and eat-
dous increase in the prevalence o f obesity
ing habits in other countries, have been sug-
over the past 1 0 years, it is understandable
gested as dominant, large-scale, environmental
and
affluence, coupled
with
the
that environmental causes have received con-
factors (Vanltallie, 1 9 9 4 ) . In the United States,
siderable examination (Hill & Peters, 1 9 9 8 ) .
the pervasiveness o f the "toxic environment" is apparent from even a superficial examination of the strategies of food marketing. Fast-food
Environmental Factors
restaurants offer options to "bigger size" one's
Environmental models address factors that promote the intake of excessive energy (calo-
meals, inducing even greater consumption o f meals already high in fat content.
ries) as well as those variables that promote a reduction of energy expenditure via exercise. The ready availability of energy-dense foods,
Genetic Factors
advent of larger portion sizes at restaurants,
Mapping o f the human genome has gener-
increased use o f fast foods, and decreased
ated interest in studying the genetic factors for
preparation o f meals are environmental factors
a variety o f psychological and physical disor-
that may promote weight gain (French, Story,
ders. Analysis o f the genomic regions with link-
& Jeffery, 2 0 0 1 ) . In addition, there have been
age to obesity is at a relatively early stage of
numerous environmental modifications as a
scientific development (Price, 2 0 0 2 ) , although
result of technological advances that
molecular studies are identifying an ever-
have
enhanced the sedentary nature of many indi-
growing list o f genes that may confer increased
viduals' lifestyles both at home and in the work-
risk for obesity in humans (Rankinen et al.,
place. Computers, video games, and television
2 0 0 2 ) . M o r e traditional work in this area has
are often selected to the exclusion of recre-
focused on the analysis o f familial/genetic
ational activities that burn calories (Dowda,
influence by examining monozygotic and dizy-
Ainsworth, Addy, Saunders, & Riner, 2 0 0 1 ) ,
gotic twins. Specifically, examination of body
whereas physical education is increasingly elim-
composition among twins has revealed that
inated or downsized in schools due to liability
monozygotic twins have correlations ranging
concerns or increased emphasis on classroom
from . 6 0 to . 7 0 , higher than the . 2 0 to . 3 0
activities to
observed among dizygotic twins (Price, 2 0 0 2 ) .
enhance standardized
testing
Obesity
and Body Image Disturbance
\
Recent summaries of the work in this area
7 0 % chance o f becoming overweight or obese
suggest that 6 7 % o f the variation in B M I
adults. Interestingly, the contribution o f the
among monozygotic twins may be attribut-
parents' weight, along with the child's weight,
&
m a y depend on the child. F o r instance,
Ravussin, 2 0 0 2 ) . This genetic influence may
Whitaker, Wright, Pepe, Seidel, and Dietz
manifest
( 1 9 9 7 ) looked at the weight o f both the parents
able to genetics (Price, 2 0 0 2 ; Tataranni in physiological and
behavioral
effects such as different resting metabolic rates,
and the child to determine whether one or both
reduced physical activity (e.g., fidgeting), and
contributed to the development o f obesity in
excessive food consumption.
a
the child. They found that very young children
"thrifty" gene has long been hypothesized to
(ages 1 to 2 years) who were obese and had
Although
lie at the roots of the elevated obesity rates
an overweight parent had a fourfold risk for
(e.g., among the Pima
adult obesity as compared with overweight
Indians), no such single gene has been detected.
children of the same age w h o had average-
Even among such subgroups, however, rates of
weight parents. However, by age 1 0 years, the
obesity have increased along the lines o f other
effect was independent o f the parents' weight.
in some subgroups
population groups over the past 5 0 years, suggesting more than a genetic influence (Price,
Other early developmental factors
that
have received examination include type o f
Charles, Pettitt, & Knowler, 1 9 9 3 ) . Hence,
infant feeding (breast vs. bottle), feeding style,
obesity is most likely a "polygenic trait," influ-
and parental control over feeding. Each o f
enced by multiple genes that interact among
these areas is receiving a great deal o f atten-
themselves as well as by environmental inputs
tion, and the findings are often inconclusive.
(Comuzzie &c Allison, 1 9 9 8 ) .
However, it appears that, despite the widely
An interesting question in this area is
held view to the contrary, there is no differ-
whether genes have a direct biological effect
ence
on obesity and perhaps drive an individually
feeding in terms o f the later development o f
between
breast-feeding
based response to specific environmental fac-
obesity
Feeding style involves the study o f factors
Johnson, & Allison, 1 9 9 7 ; Keller, Pietrobelli,
such as rapid eating and the vigor o f the suck-
Must, & Faith, 2 0 0 2 ) . Put more clearly, are
ing response during infancy. Findings in this
genes partly responsible for those gaining the
area are intriguing. For example, Stunkard,
m o s t weight
Berkowitz, Stallings, and
toxic
environment?
Stunkard,
bottle-
tors (e.g., low activity, excessive eating) (Faith,
in our
(Berkowitz &
and
Schoeller
2002).
(1999)
Bouchard and colleagues ( 2 0 0 0 ) overfed iden-
found that the infant offspring o f overweight
tical twins for 3 months and found
that
mothers had a more vigorous sucking style
although there were large differences in the
than did the infants o f control mothers.
levels o f weight added across pairs, within-
Importantly, the authors also found that they
pair gains were similar. Therefore, it is possi-
could predict level o f fatness at age 1 year by
ble that gene-environment interactions exist
an examination o f the sucking style. Finally,
with respect to weight gain.
parental control is the degree to which a parent, typically the mother, attempts
Developmental Factors There is strong support for the prediction of
to
manage or direct the child's intake. Birch and colleagues have pioneered
work
in
this
area and found intriguing results suggesting
obese status in adulthood from an examina-
that parental control may influence not only
tion of the weight o f the individual during
weight gain but, in certain circumstances,
childhood and adolescence (Thompson S t
restriction o f intake
Smolak, 2 0 0 1 ) . Overweight adolescents have a
Birch, 2 0 0 1 ) .
as well
(Fisher
&
129
BEHAVIORS THAT C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH
130
Assessment
Summary Prevalence rates o f obesity have increased
W i t h approximately half o f the U.S. popu-
drastically during the past few decades in
lation overweight or obese, it is important to
children, adolescents, and adults as well as
determine when treatment is warranted. T w o
among
all ethnicities
measurements that are practical for clinical
both
genders
and
evaluated. This "epidemic" has generated a
settings are recommended as first-step screen-
wealth o f research into associated health
ing instruments: B M I and the waist circumfer-
and physical conditions as well as strategies
ence. As described previously (Table 8.1),
to understand causal variables. T o
date,
B M I can be used to define degree o f obesity.
etiological models are inconclusive, but c o m -
Waist circumference, as determined by a tape
pelling
points
measure, is suggested because B M I alone does
toward a " t o x i c " environment that sets the
not provide an informative index of so-called
stage for the development and
"abdominal fat." Abdominal fat is important
epidemiological
evidence
perpetuates
for
the maintenance o f obesity.
assessment
because higher
levels o f
abdominal fat are an independent predictor of health complications related to obesity ASSESSMENT AND
(Pi-Sunyer, 1 9 9 3 ) . Hence, an individual who
TREATMENT OF OBESITY
is "overweight" but has excess abdominal fat may be at greater risk for various diseases
This section summarizes approaches for the
(e.g., cardiovascular disease, type 2 diabetes)
assessment and
than is another individual who is "obese"
treatment
o f obesity in
adults. General guidelines and strategies are
with lower levels o f abdominal fat.
detailed
Waist circumference is measured by a stan-
descriptions are provided elsewhere (Foreyt
dard protocol (NIH/NHLBI, 1 9 9 8 ) and should
provided &
here;
however,
more
Goodrick, 1 9 9 2 ; Kirschenbaum, 1 9 9 4 ;
be performed by trained staff. Expert guide-
Wadden & Vanltallie, 1 9 9 2 ) . In particular,
lines suggest that waist circumference measure-
clinicians specializing in this area are encour-
ments should be taken among persons with
Clinical
B M I s between 2 5 . 0 and 3 4 . 9 . For individuals
Evaluation,
with B M I s greater than or equal to 3 5 . 0 , or
Obesity
who are "short" in stature, waist circumfer-
(National Institutes o f Health/
ence may be minimally informative beyond
aged to read the seminal report, Guidelines and
on the Identification,
Treatment
in Adults
of Overweight
and
National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute
BMI
[ N I H / N H L B I ] , 1 9 9 8 ) . This report provides
adults with B M I s between 2 5 . 0 and 3 4 . 9 ,
alone ( N I H / N H L B I , 1 9 9 8 ) .
Among
the most authoritative, comprehensive, and
waist circumferences greater than 1 0 2 centi-
empirically based guidelines to date
for
meters ( 4 0 inches) in men and greater than 88
obesity treatment. M o s t o f the guidelines
centimeters (35 inches) in women have been
these
proposed as "high-risk" cutoffs for disease.
This section is broken down into assess-
with increased risk for type 2 diabetes, dyslipi-
provided
herein
are adapted from
Elevated waist circumferences are associated
expert recommendations. ment
and
treatment.
Within
treatment,
demia, hypertension, and cardiovascular dis-
brief
ease. Table 8.3 summarizes the relative risk o f
surgical
disease as a function of B M I and waist circum-
interventions are discussed. This is followed
ference profiles and illustrates how abdominal
behavioral/lifestyle interventions and updates on pharmacological and
up with a discussion o f the role o f body image
fat levels can amplify or attenuate the health
enhancement in obesity treatment.
risks associated with elevated B M I .
Obesity and Body Image Table 8.3
Disturbance
Risk of Type II Diabetes, Hypertension, and Cardiovascular Disease as a Function of Weight Class and Waist Circumference
Weight Class
"Normal" Waist Circumference
"High-Risk" Waist Circumference
Underweight Normal Overweight Obesity-I Obesity-II Extreme obesity
Increased High Very high Extremely high
High Very high Very high Extremely high
S O U R C E : Adapted from N I H / N H L B I (1998).
Beyond
B M I and waist circumference
overweight, or have a high risk waist circum-
assessment, expert guidelines pinpoint addi-
ference in conjunction
tional clinical markers that may
indicate
tors, development of a treatment plan for weight
individuals at a "very high absolute risk" of
control and risk factor reduction is warranted.
with at least two risk fac-
disease. T h e presence of one or more o f these
Individuals who meet these criteria but do not
conditions may suggest the need for more
want to lose weight may be advised to maintain
aggressive obesity intervention. T a b l e 8.4
their current weight or address concurrent risk
summarizes these diseases or target
factors until they are sufficiently motivated to
organ
damages that should be considered when eval-
lose weight. Treatment generally encompasses
uating obese patients ( N I H / N H L B I , 1 9 9 8 ) .
dietary, physical activity, and lifestyle changes, with appropriate assessment of barriers to treatment and periodic weight checks.
Treatment Obesity
Treatment
Algorithm
Intervention
T h e N I H / N H L B I ( 1 9 9 8 ) report provides a concrete algorithm
to guide
the
Components
A well-established literature indicates that
obesity
behavioral/lifestyle approaches t o obesity
evaluation-treatment interface, and is depicted
treatment are effective for inducing weight
in Figure 8 . 1 .
loss, although relapse is very c o m m o n (Faith,
The heuristic begins with assessment o f family
Fontaine, Cheskin, & Allison, 2 0 0 0 ; Wadden,
history of obesity. If there is no history of obesity,
Sternberg, Letizia, Stunkard, & Foster, 1 9 8 9 ) .
experts recommend 2-year intervals for the
Enhanced dietary quality is a hallmark feature
assessment of changes in B M I . At 2-year assess-
of behavioral treatment, as patients are tradi-
ments (or more frequent assessments if obesity is
tionally prescribed a "low-calorie diet" that is
familial), health professionals are advised to cal-
generally 8 0 0 to
1 , 5 0 0 calories per
day.
culate B M I as well as to measure weight, height,
Features o f the American Heart Association's
and waist circumference. For patients who are
Low-Calorie Step I Diet are summarized in
overweight or obese, or who have a high-risk
Table 8.5 ( N I H / N H L B I , 1 9 9 8 ) .
waist circumference (greater than 88 centimeters
Reducing total caloric intake by 5 0 0 to
in females, greater than 1 0 2 centimeters in
1,000 calories per day, assuming no changes in
males), a thorough
physical activity levels, is expected to induce
medical assessment is
warranted that assesses the conditions listed in
weight loss o f approximately 1 to 2 pounds per
Table 8.4. For patients who are obese, are
week. Reducing total caloric intake by 3 0 0 to
[
BEHAVIORS THAT C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH
Table 8.4
Diseases and Target Organ Damages for Potential Assessment in Obese Adults
1. Identification of patients at very high absolute risk: l a . Established CHD History History History History
of of of of
myocardial infraction angina pectoris (stable or unstable) coronary artery surgery coronary artery procedures (angioplasty)
l b . Presence of other atherosclerotic disease Peripheral arterial disease Abdominal aortic aneurysm Symptomatic carotid artery disease Type 2 diabetes Sleep apnea 2. Identification of other obesity-associated diseases: 2a. Gynecological abnormalities 2b. Osteoarthritis 2c. Gallstones and their complications 2d. Stress incontinence 3. Identification of cardiovascular risk factors that impart a high absolute risk (patients can be classified as being at high absolute risk for obesity-related disorders if they have three or more of the multiple risk factors listed below): 3a. Cigarette smoking 3b. Hypertension: A patient is classified as having hypertension if systolic blood pressure is > 140 millimeters of mercury or diastolic blood pressure is > 9 0 millimeters of mercury or if the patient is taking antihypertensive agents. 3c. High-risk LDL cholesterol: A high-risk LDL cholesterol is defined as a serum concentration of > 160 milligrams per deciliter. A borderline high-risk LDL cholesterol (130 to 159 milligrams per deciliter), together with two or more other risk factors, also confers high risk. 3d. Low H D L cholesterol: A low H D L cholesterol is defined as a serum concentration of < 35 milligrams per deciliter. 3e. Impaired fasting glucose: The presence of clinical type 2 diabetes (fasting plasma glucose of > 126 milligrams per deciliter or 2 hours postprandial plasma glucose of > 2 0 0 milligrams per deciliter) is a major risk factor for CVD, and its presence alone places a patient in the category of very high absolute risk. Impaired fasting glucose (fasting plasma glucose 110 to 125 milligrams per deciliter) is considered by many authorities to be a risk factor for cardiovascular disease. 3f.
Family history of premature CHD
3g. Age (males > 45 years or females > 55 years or postmenopausal) 4. Other risk factors: 4a. Physical inactivity 4b. High triglycerides (400 to 1,000 milligrams per deciliter = "high", > 1,000 milligrams per deciliter = "very high") S O U R C E : N I H / N H L B I (1998). N O T E : C H D = coronary heart disease; L D L = low-density lipoprotein; H D L = high-density lipoprotein; C V D = cardiovascular disease.
Obesity and Body Image
Figure 8.1
Disturbance
The Obesity Evaluation-Treatment Interface
S O U R C E : National Institutes of Health/National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (1998).
5 0 0 calories per day (the equivalent of approxi-
everyday living. Examples include parking far-
mately two soft drinks) is expected to induce
ther away in the parking lot so that one needs
weight loss o f Vi to 1 pound per week, assum-
to walk farther to the office/shop, taking daily
ing no changes in physical activity. For these
walks, cleaning the house regularly, moving
reasons, obese adults are typically prescribed
while talking on the phone, and taking stairs
to consume 1,000 to 1,200 calories per day
instead o f elevators whenever one has a choice.
(women) or 1,200 to 1,500 calories per day
A study by Andersen and colleagues ( 1 9 9 9 )
(men) to achieve weight loss.
compared weight loss among 4 0 obese women
Physical activity is another pillar of obesity
who were randomized to a weight loss group
treatment. Physical activity without concurrent
that incorporated a structured aerobic exercise
improvements in diet quality will have a less
regimen versus a lifestyle activity regimen.
potent effect on weight loss than will changes
Although the two groups lost comparable
in diet plus physical activity ( N I H / N H L B I ,
amounts of weight during the 16-week inter-
1 9 9 8 ) . During recent years, research
vention, the aerobic exercise group regained
has
focused on the beneficial effects o f so-called
significantly more weight on average (1.6 kilo-
"lifestyle activity" changes compared
grams) than did the lifestyle intervention group
with
more regimented and structured exercise pro-
(0.08 kilograms). Data from the W o m e n ' s
grams. Lifestyle programs teach individuals
Healthy Lifestyle Project Clinical Trial (Kuller,
how to program physical activity changes into
Simkin-Silverman, Wing, Meilahn, & Ives,
133
134
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH Table 8.5
Low-Calorie Step I Diet
Nutrient
Recommended
Calories
Approximately 5 0 0 to 1,000 calories per day reduction from usual intake < 3 0 % of total calories 8% to 1 0 % of total calories < 1 5 % of total calories < 1 0 % of total calories < 3 0 0 milligrams per day Approximately 1 5 % of total calories 5 5 % or more of total calories No more than 100 millimoles per day (approximately 2.4 grams of sodium or approximately 6 grams of sodium chloride) 1,000 to 1,500 milligrams 2 0 to 30 grams
Total fat Saturated fatty acids Monounsaturated fats Polyunsaturated fats Cholesterol Protein Carbohydrates Sodium chloride Calcium Fiber
Intake
S O U R C E : N I H / N H L B I (1998).
2001)
also confirm that prescriptions
for
Behavioral
Techniques
lifestyle physical activity, in conjunction with the aforementioned dietary prescriptions, can pre-
Descriptions o f the behavioral treatment of
vent excess weight gain and elevations in low-
obesity can be traced back to classical articles
density lipoprotein (LDL) cholesterol among
by Ferster, Nurnberger, and Levitt (1962) and
women as they progress from perimenopause
Stuart ( 1 9 6 7 ) . Between the 1 9 7 0 s and the 1 9 9 0 s , classical experiments tested behavioral
to postmenopause. daily
treatments for obesity among a range o f obese
physical activity can be personally tailored to
patients. As the methodological rigor of these
accommodate individual lives. " M o d e r a t e "
studies
intensity physical activity is conceptualized as
lengths, and follow-up lengths)
activities that translate to an energy deficit of
over time, so did the efficacy o f treatment
1 5 0 calories per day, which would sum to
(Faith et al., 2 0 0 0 ; Wadden & Foster, 2 0 0 0 ) .
On
a practical level, increasing
(including sample sizes, treatment improved
1,000 calories per week (NIH/NHLBI, 1 9 9 8 ) .
Whereas intervention studies published
The amount of time needed to achieve this
1 9 7 4 achieved a mean weight loss o f 3.8 kilo-
in
deficit each day will depend on the nature of
grams (or approximately 8.5 pounds), those
the activity and the individual's weight. Expert
published between 1 9 8 5 and 1 9 8 7 achieved a
guidelines suggest that initially the obese
mean weight loss o f 8.4 kilograms (or approx-
patient should engage in moderate activity lev-
imately 1 8 . 5 pounds). Unfortunately, a cardi-
els for 3 0 to 4 5 minutes per day on 3 to 5 days
nal disappointment of these same weight loss
per week (NIH/NHLBI, 1 9 9 8 ) . According to
studies—and a finding that transcends decades
the surgeon general's report, most adults
of research—is an excessive rate of recidivism
should ultimately strive to accumulate at least
(Wadden et al., 1 9 8 9 ; Wilson, 1 9 9 4 ) .
3 0 minutes of moderate-intensity physical
Specific components o f the
activity on most, if not all, days o f the week.
behavioral
treatment o f obesity have been outlined in
Table 8.6 illustrates the different ways in
detail elsewhere (Foreyt & Goodrick, 1 9 9 2 ;
which an adult can achieve moderate physical
Kirschenbaum, 1 9 9 4 ; Wadden & Vanltallie,
activity levels each day, varying the time spent
1 9 9 2 ) . Critical concepts reviewed by expert
doing the activity and the strenuousness of
guidelines ( N I H / N H L B I , 1 9 9 8 ) include the
the activity.
following.
Obesity and Body Image Disturbance Table 8.6
Examples of Moderate Amounts of Physical Activity
Activity
Requirements
Washing or waxing a car for 4 5 to 60 minutes
Less vigorous/More time
Washing windows or floors for 4 5 to 60 minutes Playing volleyball for 4 5 minutes A
Playing touch football for 30 to 4 5 minutes Gardening for 3 0 to 4 5 minutes Wheeling self in wheelchair for 3 0 to 4 0 minutes Walking 1% miles in 35 minutes Playing basketball for 30 minutes Bicycling 5 miles in 30 minutes Dancing fast (socially) for 3 0 minutes Pushing a stroller V/z miles in 3 0 minutes Raking leaves for 30 minutes Walking 2 miles in 3 0 minutes Engaging in water aerobics for 3 0 minutes Swimming laps for 2 0 minutes Playing a basketball game for 15 to 2 0 minutes Bicycling 4 miles in 15 minutes Jumping rope for 15 minutes
More vigorous/Less time
Running Wi miles in 15 minutes Shoveling snow for 15 minutes Stair walking for 15 minutes
Self-Monitoring
and
(Faith, Allison, & Geliebter, 1 9 9 7 ) . Teaching
Recording in detail what
obese individuals coping strategies to deal
one eats every day can provide revealing
with stress without eating c a n be an impor-
insights into the environmental, emotional,
tant component o f treatment. Exercise can be
and/or interpersonal circumstances that may
an effective stress management technique that
prompt intake o f particular foods. Keeping
also confers benefits for weight management.
Physical
Activity.
of
Eating
such recordings with standard
Habits
paper-and-
pencil methods or with novel palm comput-
Stimulus
Control.
Unhealthy food selec-
ers can bring patterns o f eating and inactivity
tions and/or overeating can be stimulated by
to consciousness and help m a k e them the
foods that are more readily accessible and
target o f treatment.
conveniently ready for eating. For example, homes that do not keep an ample supply o f prompt
fresh fruits and vegetables for snacking are less
overeating in certain obese individuals, espe-
conducive to weight control than are homes in
cially those with
which these foods are readily available.
Stress
Management.
Stress can
binge eating
tendencies
\
135
136
BEHAVIORS THAT C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH Therefore, behavioral interventions focus on
pressure, coronary heart disease, congestive heart
barriers to restructuring the environment in
failure, arrhythmias, or stroke should not take
ways
sibutramine. All patients taking the medication
that
stimulate
healthy
eating
and
physical activity.
are encouraged to have their blood pressure monitored regularly (NIH/NHLBI, 1998). Rewards for
Orlistat exerts its effects by inhibiting
behavioral changes can be an effective c o m -
pancreatic lipase and thereby decreasing fat
ponent o f treatment. Rewards can be tangi-
absorption. T h e effectiveness of orlistat for
Contingency
Management.
ble gifts for oneself or praise from family
inducing long-term (i.e., 2-year) weight loss
members and friends (i.e., social support).
compared with controls was documented in two sets o f multisite studies: one conducted in
Cognitive statements
Positive self-
Restructuring. that
challenge defeating
Europe (Rossner, Sjôstrôm, N o a c k , Meinders,
self-
& Noseda, 2 0 0 0 ) and one conducted in the
statements can be effective for teaching obese
United States (Davidson et al., 1 9 9 9 ) . Orlistat
patients to cope with treatment "failures" (e.g.,
can also have beneficial effects on blood
periods of weight regain) or other unexpected
glucose levels and diabetes-related risk factors
challenges during treatment. These matters can
(Heymsfield et al., 2 0 0 0 ) . T h e fact that orlis-
be especially important
for obese patients
tat's principal mechanism o f action does not
lacking in self-acceptance or self-esteem due to
involve brain neurotransmitters represents a
limited weight loss or weight regain (Wilson,
distinct advantage over sibutramine. O n the
1 9 9 6 ) . Recent advances in treatment are focus-
other hand, orlistat has its own
ing on promotion of more realistic weight loss
effects, including a decrease in absorption o f
expectations to promote better psychological
fat-soluble vitamins, soft stool and anal leak-
well-being and perhaps even weight loss.
age in some individuals, and a possible link to
adverse
breast cancer ( N I H / N H L B I , 1 9 9 8 ) . Pharmacological
Therapy
In sum, there are currently two F D A -
Detailed reviews of the drug treatment literature have been published
elsewhere (e.g.,
approved drug options for obese individuals who may need additional help to achieve weight
Haddock, Poston, Dill, Foreyt, & Ericsson,
loss. However, these pharmacological agents
2 0 0 2 ) . There are currently two prescription med-
can have adverse effects and so require careful
ications that are approved by the Food and Drug
medical supervision if they are to be taken.
Administration (FDA) for obesity treatment:
Expert guidelines propose these alternatives as
sibutramine (Meridia) and orlistat (Xenical).
possible options only after a patient has tried
Sibutramine is an anorexiant (or appetite
6 months o f behavioral intervention, including
suppressant) that exerts its effect by inhibiting
diet and exercise, without success. Even then,
the reuptake of norepinephrine,
these alternatives are recommended only for
dopamine,
and serotinin in the brain. Controlled clinical
patients with BMIs greater than or equal to 3 0
trials document the clinical effectiveness o f
without
sibutramine for weight loss (Berube-Parent,
or for patients with BMIs greater than or equal
Prudhomme, St-Pierre, Doucet, & Tremblay,
to 2 7 with
2 0 0 1 ; Fanghanel, Cortinas, Sanchez-Reyes, &
factors or diseases (NLH/NHLBI, 1 9 9 8 ) .
obesity-related risk factors or diseases concomitant obesity-related risk
Berber, 2 0 0 1 ; Wirth & Krause, 2 0 0 1 ) . O n the other hand, a major concern surrounding sibutramine is the potential for increased heart rate
Surgical
Approaches
and blood pressure (Bray et al., 1996). For these
Surgical approaches to obesity are generally
reasons, individuals with a history of high blood
recommended only for the most severe cases in
Obesity and Body Image Disturbance
\
which other behavioral and pharmacological
numerous prejudices and forms of discrimination
approaches have failed. These approaches are
that
recommended only for individuals with B M I s
Brownell, 2 0 0 1 ) . T o this end, consideration o f
greater than or equal to 4 0 or for individuals
a patient's body image both at the beginning
with B M I s greater than or equal to 3 5 plus
and over the course of treatment is an impor-
comorbid medical conditions (NIH/NHLBI,
tant consideration. There are at least two ways
confront
obese individuals
(Puhl
&
1 9 9 8 ) . Current surgical approaches include
in which body image issues might be built into
so-called "gastric restriction" (vertical gastric
treatment programs: (a) body image as an out-
banding) and "gastric bypass" (Roux-en Y ) ,
come measure index to be targeted in addition
which can achieve sizable weight loss and
to other traditional health risk factors and
improvements in comorbidities. T h e most
(b) body image and size acceptance as a poten-
striking data documenting these effects come
tial mediator o f weight change.
from the Swedish Obesity Study, initiated in 1 9 8 7 , that has prospectively followed 1,000 obese patients who underwent surgical treatment compared with 1,000 obese patients who received conventional nonsurgical
interven-
tion. A decade after the treatment was initiated, long-term results provided compelling data
favoring surgical treatment. Surgical
patients lost an average o f 2 8 kilograms (or approximately 6 1 pounds)
compared
with
0.5 kilograms (or approximately 1 pound) lost by nonsurgical controls. Compared with the control group, surgical patients had a 32-fold reduction in 2-year diabetes incidence and a 5-fold reduction at 8-year follow-up (Torgerson &c Sjostrom, 2 0 0 1 ) . Surgical patients were also more likely to show reduced time on sick leave and disability pension (Narbro et al., 1 9 9 9 ) .
Body
Image
as a Treatment
Outcome.
Assessing body image changes is justified based on the frequency with which obese individuals report dissatisfaction with their bodies. A detailed list o f validated body image measures for obese individuals is available elsewhere (Thompson et al., 1 9 9 9 ) . Results from behavioral interventions indicate that body image is significantly enhanced when
patients
lose
weight (Foster, Wadden, & Vogt, 1 9 9 7 ; Sarwer, Wadden, & Foster, 1 9 9 8 ) . Hence, clinicians can anticipate improvement in body image as an additional benefit of weight loss. Given the challenges o f achieving weight loss, other investigators have attempted to develop cognitivebehavioral interventions that target psychological well-being and body image without neces-
At the same time, surgery-related complica-
sarily inducing weight loss. M a n y of these
tions can be c o m m o n and should be moni-
treatments stem from the so-called "anti-diet"
tored. Side effects noted in one surgical study
framework. Examples include Ciliska's (1990)
(Pories et al., 1 9 9 5 ) included Vitamin Β defi-
program, which attempted to help patients
ciency ( 3 9 . 9 % of patients), depression ( 2 3 . 7 %
"reestablish normal eating, improve self-esteem,
of patients), gastritis ( 1 3 . 2 % of patients), and
and learn to deal with negative messages about
dehydration/malnutrition ( 5 . 8 % o f patients).
our body shape in order to be more accepting of ourselves" (p. 4 9 ) , and Polivy and Herman's (1992) "Undieting" program.
Targeting Body Image in Obesity Treatment
In one o f the most comprehensive programs to date, Rosen and colleagues (Rosen, 1 9 9 6 ;
Body image disparagement, or dissatisfac-
Rosen, Orosan, & Reiter, 1 9 9 5 ) developed a
tion with one's body, is one o f the more com-
treatment that targeted negative body image
mon psychological hardships o f obesity (Faith
among
& Allison, 1 9 9 6 ; Friedman & Brownell, 1 9 9 5 ) .
assigned to either a no-treatment control group
This is readily understandable
or a cognitive-behavioral intervention
given
the
obese
women.
Individuals
were that
137
138
I
BEHAVIORS THAT C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH
Table 8.7
Sample Questions to Probe Patient Weight Loss Expectations
1. What is your target weight loss goal? Why? 2. What would you consider to be an acceptable weight loss goal? Why? 3. What would you consider to be a disappointing but somewhat acceptable weight loss goal? Why? 4. What would you consider to be your dream weight loss goal? Why? 5. How would you feel about a weight loss of [10%]? Why? S O U R C E : These questions were modeled after Foster, Wadden, Vogt, and Brewer (1997).
included exploration of the social consequences
55-pound) weight loss was considered to be an
of obesity, factors that cause and maintain
acceptable but not particularly happy weight
negative body image, stress management sur-
loss, and a 17-kilogram (or approximately
rounding
37.5-pound) weight loss was considered to be a
physical
appearance,
cognitive of
disappointment. At the end of treatment, none
physical appearance, and body image exposure
of the patients had achieved his or her "dream"
restructuring
surrounding
assumptions
in stressful social situations. Results indicated
weight, 9 % had attained a "happy weight,"
that the treatment group showed significant
2 0 % had attained a "disappointing" weight,
improvements on most psychological and body
2 4 % had attained an "acceptable" weight, and
image measures as compared with the control
4 7 % had lost less weight than what they
group. Details of this treatment protocol are
defined as a disappointing weight. T o put these
provided elsewhere (Rosen et al., 1 9 9 5 ) ; how-
numbers into context, behavioral interventions
ever, these data indicate that body image
currently strive to induce weight loss of approx-
enhancement can be a viable treatment goal for
imately 8 % to 1 0 % . Although a 1 0 % weight
certain obese patients.
reduction is less than what patients might desire, it can confer important health benefits
Body
Image
and Size Acceptance
as
Possi-
(NIH/NHLBI, 1 9 9 8 ) .
Studies
Based on these data, Foster, Wadden, Vogt,
by Foster and colleagues revealed that many
and Brewer ( 1 9 9 7 ) concluded, "It seems more
ble
Mediators
of
Weight
Loss.
obese patients hold vastly unrealistic expecta-
appropriate to help patients accept
tions about the amount of weight loss they
est weight
can achieve through current
loss outcomes
more
mod-
rather than attempt
intervention
to devise treatments to increase weight loss"
approaches (Foster, Wadden, Phelan, Sarwer,
(p. 8 5 , emphasis in original). Indeed, Cooper
& Sanderson, 2 0 0 1 ; Foster, Wadden, Vogt, &
and Fairburn ( 2 0 0 1 ) called for novel behav-
enrolling
ioral treatments that assess patients' baseline
in a university-based weight loss program were
treatment expectations and that use cognitive
asked to define the amounts of weight loss they
strategies to promote more realistic treatment
defined as a goal or target weight loss, accept-
goals. T h e underlying hypothesis is that help-
Brewer, 1 9 9 7 ) . Obese individuals
able but not particularly happy weight loss, and
ing patients to adopt more realistic treatment
disappointing weight loss. Results indicated
expectations will ultimately promote better
that most patients brought unrealistic weight
treatment outcomes given that
loss goals to treatment. (See Table 8.7) Patients
associated with modest weight loss will be
defined a 3 2 % reduction in weight as their
reduced. This hypothesis awaits empirical
target goal. A 25-kilogram (or approximately
evaluation.
frustrations
Obesity and Body Image
Disturbance
CASE S T U D Y T h e case o f "Virginia M . " is a useful one that illustrates the many facets o f human obesity and reminds us that obese individuals are no different from nonobese individuals in many respects. Obese individuals lead complicated and busy lives, and they desire professional success and achievement as well as gratifying interpersonal relationships. Obese individuals have a genetic constitution, however, that makes weight gain easier when they engage in behaviors that favor excess energy intake (overeating high-fat foods) and reduced energy expenditure (being sedentary). Virginia's background
fits this description reasonably well. A 37-year-old
divorced attorney, Virginia is a professionally active w o m a n w h o "lives on the run" and perceives there to be limited time to lead a healthier life. Weighing 2 0 0 pounds (or approximately 9 1 kilograms) and measuring 5 feet 5 inches in height, her B M I is 3 3 . 3 , putting her in the Obesity-I category. Virginia has a waist circumference o f 9 2 centimeters, putting her at elevated risk for obesity-related complications (according to the guidelines reviewed herein). Obesity runs on the maternal side o f her family but not on the paternal side. Although Virginia says that she feels fine, a recent medical examination suggested slightly elevated total and L D L ( " b a d " ) cholesterol and blood pressure. Based on the treatment algorithm reviewed herein, Virginia would benefit from weight loss. W h e n Virginia was asked what her "ideal" weight would be, she indicated 1 8 5 pounds. Such a 15-pound weight loss (from 2 0 0 to 1 8 5 pounds) would represent an 8 % weight reduction, which is reasonable given current behavioral interventions. Fortunately, her expectations do not appear to be unrealistically high, and this can be used clinically to enhance her motivation to m a k e changes. If Virginia had defined her ideal weight to be 1 6 0 pounds, it might have been necessary to explore more attainable goals before beginning treatment. Virginia sought behavioral therapy, and a treatment program was developed that targeted healthier eating, increased physical activity, and reduced j o b stress. O n e o f the most critical themes in her treatment was that o f building structure into her life. Structure was operationalized as planning and preparing appropriate meals ahead o f time (something Virginia avoided previously), planning ways in which to increase lifestyle physical activity that would be compatible with her active professional goals, and joining and working out in a gym. T o modify her diet, Virginia began keeping a food record and self-monitored her dietary intake several days each week. Keeping this record revealed important patterns (e.g., snacking on highfat foods during afternoons when she skipped lunch, eating fast foods when she was feeling lonely during the evenings). Based on these insights, she began to structure lunches more regularly, even when she was busy. She also began to find substitute activities during the evening to replace snacking (e.g., movies, walks, social events for singles). In terms o f physical activity, she began 3 0 minutes o f fast-pace walking 3 days per week, and this was gradually increased to 5 days per week
139
240
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH
before she joined a gym. She also began lessons in relaxation training and began reading books on time management to better balance her daily activities. T o date, Virginia has lost nearly 1 0 pounds and continues toward her goal. She recognizes that "treatment" will require lifelong lifestyle modification. Given that Virginia's j o b is central t o her self-image, she will continue to w o r k hard; however, she recognizes the potential health complications o f her weight and will be especially attentive to cardiovascular disease and type 2 diabetes symptoms. These serious medical conditions cannot be discarded. At the same time, Virginia better prioritizes personal happiness and relationships. She is increasingly recognizing that beauty, like body weight, can c o m e in different sizes.
Collectively, these data speak to the utility
daily caloric intake. O n a broader environ-
about
mental level, changes appear to be occurring
weight loss goals. Health care professionals
that put the entire population at increased
should be candid about the types o f weight
risk for obesity. Indeed, the population preva-
loss that patients can expect, given the cur-
lence o f obesity has increased dramatically
rent data, and may wish to address weight
during the past several decades.
of
assessing patients'
expectations
loss goals that seem unattainable. At the
T h e diagnosis and treatment o f obesity
same time, clinicians should be sensitive t o
starts at the simplest level with assessment
patients' motivations and should collabora-
of B M I , although this is hardly the place to
tively establish attainable weight loss goals
stop. Because obesity is associated with many
that will foster a sense o f mastery and self-
health complications, it is important that these
efficacy. T a b l e 8.7 illustrates questions that
be assessed especially among the heaviest indi-
clinicians may wish to ask as part o f an
viduals. High blood pressure, elevated total
assessment o f weight loss expectation.
and L D L cholesterol, and type 2 diabetes are just a few o f the health complications that must be considered. Psychosocial factors, such as poor self-esteem and poor body image, also
CONCLUSIONS
deserve consideration. Because body image Obesity is one o f the most pressing public
and obesity are so strongly intertwined, it is
health disorders confronting Western soci-
important that the overall life satisfaction of
eties. It is a multifaceted problem in terms o f
obese individuals be assessed.
its causes and consequences. Obesity is deter-
Behavioral treatments that target lifestyle
mined by genetic and environmental factors
changes compatible with everyday life, promote
that are poorly understood. It seems appar-
healthier food choices and reduced fat intake,
ent that one's genetic background will affect
and increase physical activity most days of the
weight gain in response t o eating. Certain
week are generally the first intervention strategy.
resist
Pharmacological agents can be used to assist
weight gain, whereas others readily put on
with weight loss, although potential side effects
weight in response to modest elevations in
need to be considered carefully. Each of the two
individuals
inherit a propensity t o
Obesity
and Body Image Disturbance
\
current FDA-approved drugs, sibutramine and
The high recidivism rate for obesity treatment
orlistat, has its own side effects that render it
indicates that many obese individuals will
prohibitive for many obese adults. Surgical
not achieve their desired weight loss. This
treatments have encouraging outcome results
underscores the importance o f helping obese
for severe obesity. For those morbidly obese
individuals to achieve personal satisfaction,
individuals who have obesity-related com-
self-respect, and healthy interpersonal rela-
plications and cannot lose weight through other
tionships through the highs and lows o f
prescribed methods, surgery may be a consider-
weight management.
ation when evaluated judiciously.
REFERENCES Allison, D. B., Fontaine, K. R., Manson, J . E., Stevens, J . , &C Vanltallie, T. B. (1999). Annual deaths attributable to obesity in the United States. Journal of the American Medical Association, 282, 1 5 3 0 - 1 5 3 8 . American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Andersen, R. E., Wadden, Τ. Α., Bartlett, S. J . , Zemel, B . , Verde, T. J . , &c Franckowiak, S. C. (1999). Effects of lifestyle activity vs. structured aerobic exercise in obese women: A randomized trial. Journal of the American Medical Association, 281, 3 3 5 - 3 4 0 . Berkowitz, R. I., &t Stunkard, A. J . (2002). Development of childhood obesity. In T. A. Wadden &C A. J . Stunkard (Eds.), Handbook of obesity treatment (pp. 5 1 5 - 5 3 1 ) . New York: Guilford. Berube-Parent, S., Prudhomme, D., St-Pierre, S., Doucet, E., & Tremblay, A. (2001). Obesity treatment with a progressive clinical tri-therapy combining sibutramine and a supervised diet-exercise intervention. International Journal of Obesity & Related Metabolic Disorders, 25, 1 1 4 4 - 1 1 5 3 . Bouchard, C , Rankinen, T., Chagnon, Y . C , Rice, T., Perusse, L., Gagnon, J . , Borecki, I., An, P., Leon, A. S., Skinner, J . S., Wilmore, H. H., Province, M., & Rao, D. C. (2000). Genomic scan for maximal oxygen uptake and its response to training in the HERITAGE Family Study. Journal of Applied Physiology, 88, 551-559. Bray, G. Α., Ryan, D. H., Gordon, D., Heidingsfelder, S., Cerise, F., & Wilson, K. (1996). A double-blind randomized placebo-controlled trial of sibutramine. Obesity Research, 4, 2 6 3 - 2 7 0 . Bulik, C. M., Sullivan, P. F., 8t Kendler, K. S. (2002). Medical and psychiatric morbidity in obese women with and without binge eating. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 32, 7 2 - 7 8 . Carpenter, K. M., Hasin, D. S., Allison, D. B., & Faith, M. S. (2000). Relationships between obesity and DSM-IV major depressive disorder, suicide ideation, and suicide attempts: Results from a general population study. American Journal of Public Health, 90, 2 5 1 - 2 5 7 . Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2002). Surgeon general's report on obesity. Atlanta, GA: Author. Retrieved on January 2 0 , 2 0 0 3 , from www.cdc.gov Ciliska, D. (1990). Beyond dieting: Psychological interventions for chronically obese women—A non-dieting approach. New York: Brunner/Mazel. Comuzzie, A. G., & Allison, D. B. (1998). The search for human obesity genes. Science, 280, 1 3 7 4 - 1 3 7 7 .
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH Cooper, Z., & Fairburn, C. G. (2001). A new cognitive behavioural approach to the treatment of obesity. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 39, 4 9 9 - 5 1 1 . Davidson, M. H., Hauptman, J . , DiGirolamo, M., Foreyt, J . P., Halsted, C. H., Heber, D., Heimburger, D. C , Lucas, C. P., Robbins, D. C , Chung, J . , & Heymsfield, S. B. (1999). Weight control and risk factor reduction in obese subjects treated for 2 years with orlistat: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Medical Association, 281, 2 3 5 - 2 4 2 . Dowda, M., Ainsworth, B . E., Addy, C. L., Saunders, R., & Riner, W. (2001). Environmental influences, physical activity, and weight status in 8- to 16-yearolds. Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine, 155, 7 1 1 - 7 1 7 . Faith, M. S., & Allison, D. B. (1996). Assessment of psychological status in obese persons. In J . K. Thompson (Ed.), Body image, eating disorders, and obesity: A practical guide for assessment and treatment (pp. 3 6 5 - 3 8 7 ) . Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Faith, M . S., Allison, D. B., & Geliebter, A. (1997). Emotional eating and obesity: Theoretical considerations and practical recommendations. In S. Dalton (Ed.), guide to understanding Obesity and weight control: The health professional's and treatment (pp. 4 3 9 - 4 6 5 ) . Gaithersburg, M D : Aspen. Faith, M. S., Fontaine, K. R., Cheskin, L. J . , & Allison, D. B. (2000). Behavioral 24, 4 5 9 - 4 9 3 . approaches to the problems of obesity. Behavior Modification, Faith, M . S., Johnson, S. L., & Allison, D. B. (1997). Putting the "behavior" into the behavior genetics of obesity research. Behavior Genetics, 27, 4 2 3 - 4 3 9 . Fanghanel, G., Cortinas, L., Sanchez-Reyes, L., & Berber, A. (2001). Second phase of a double-blind study clinical trial on sibutramine for the treatment of patients suffering essential obesity: Six months after treatment cross-over. International Journal of Obesity & Related Metabolic Disorders, 25, 7 4 1 - 7 4 7 . Ferster, C. B., Nurnberger, J . I., & Levitt, Ε. E. (1962). The control of eating. Journal of Mathematics, 1, 8 7 - 1 0 9 . Field, A. E., Barnoya, J . , & Colditz, G. A. (2002). Epidemiology and health and economic consequences of obesity. In T. A. Wadden & A. J . Stunkard (Eds.), Handbook of obesity treatment (pp. 3 - 1 8 ) . New York: Guilford. Fisher, J . Α., & Birch, L. (2001). Early regulation of eating behavior. In J . K. Thompson & L. Smolak (Eds.), Body image, eating disorders, and obesity in youth: Assessment, treatment, and prevention (pp. 2 3 - 3 9 ) . Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Foreyt, J . P., & Goodrick, G. K. (1992). Living without dieting. New York: Time Warner. Foster, G. D., Wadden, T. Α., Phelan S., Sarwer D. B . , & Sanderson, R. S. (2001). Obese patients' perceptions of treatment outcomes and the factors that influence them. Archives of Internal Medicine, 161, 2 1 3 3 - 2 1 3 9 . Foster, G. D., Wadden, Τ. Α., & Vogt, R. A. (1997). Body image in obese women before, during, and after weight loss treatment. Health Psychology, 16, 2 2 6 - 2 2 9 . Foster, G. D., Wadden, Τ. Α., Vogt, R. Α., & Brewer, G. (1997). What is a reasonable weight loss? Patients' expectations and evaluations of obesity treatment outcomes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 7 9 - 8 5 . French, S. Α., Story, M., & Jeffery, R. W. (2001). Environmental influences on eating and physical activity. Annual Review of Public Health, 22, 3 0 9 - 3 3 5 . Friedman, Μ . Α., & Brownell, K. D. (1995). Psychological correlates of obesity: Moving to the next research generation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 3 - 2 0 . Garrow, J . S., & Webster, J . (1985). Quetelet's index (w/h ) as a measure of fatness. International Journal of Obesity, 9, 1 4 7 - 1 5 3 . Gortmaker, S. L., Must, Α., Perrin, J . M., Sobol, A. M., & Dietz, W. H. (1993). Social and economic consequences of overweight in adolescence and young adulthood. New England Journal of Medicine, 329, 1 0 0 8 - 1 1 1 2 . 2
Obesity
and Body Image
Disturbance
Haddock, C. K., Poston, W. S., Dill, P. L., Foreyt, J . P., 8c Ericsson, M. (2002). Pharmacotherapy for obesity: A quantitative analysis of four decades of published randomized clinical trials. International Journal of Obesity, 26, 2 6 2 - 2 7 3 . Heymsfield, S. B . , Segal, K. R., Hauptman, J . , Lucas, C. P., Boldrin, M . N., Rissanen, Α., Wilding, J . P., 8c Sjôstrôm, L. (2000). Effects of weight loss with orlistat on glucose tolerance and progression to Type 2 diabetes in obese adults. Archives of Internal Medicine, 160, 1 3 2 1 - 1 3 2 6 . Hill, J . O., & Peters, J . C. (1998). Environmental contributions to the obesity epidemic. Science, 280, 1 3 7 1 - 1 3 7 4 . Horgen, Κ. B . , 8c Brownell, K. D. (2002). Confronting the toxic environment: Environmental public health actions in a world crisis. In T. A. Wadden 8c A. J . Stunkard (Eds.), Handbook of obesity treatment (pp. 9 5 - 1 0 6 ) . New York: Guilford. Johnson, W. G., Se Torgrud, L. J . (1996). Assessment and treatment of binge eating disorder. In J . K. Thompson (Ed.), Body image, eating disorders, and obesity: An integrative guide for assessment and treatment (pp. 3 2 1 - 3 4 3 ) . Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Keller, K. L., Pietrobelli, Α., Must, S., Sc Faith, M . S. (2002). Genetics of eating and its relation to obesity. Current Atherosclerosis Reports, 4, 1 7 6 - 1 8 2 . Kirschenbaum, D. S. (1994). Weight loss through persistence. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger. Kuller, L. H., Simkin-Silverman, L. R., Wing, R. R., Meilahn, E., Sc Ives, D. G. (2001). Women's Healthy Lifestyle Project: A randomized clinical trial— Results at 5 4 months. Circulation, 103, 3 2 - 3 7 . Mokdad, A. H., Bowman, Β . Α., 8c Ford, E. S. (2001). The continuing of obesity and diabetes in the United States. Journal of the American Medical Association, 286, 1 1 9 5 - 1 2 0 0 . Mokdad, A. H., Bowman, Β. Α., Ford, E. S., Vinicor, F., Marks, J . S., 8c Koplan, J . P. (2001). Journal of the American Medical Association, 286, 1 1 9 5 - 1 2 0 0 . Mokdad, A. H., Serdula, M., 8c Dietz, W. (1999). The spread of the obesity epidemic in the United States. Journal of the American Medical Association, 282, 1519-1522. Mokdad, A. H., Serdula, M., Sc Dietz, W. (2000). The continuing obesity epidemic in the United States. Journal of the American Medical Association, 284, 1650-1651. Narbro, K., Agren, G., Jonsson, E., Larsson, B . , Naslund, I., Wedel, H., 8c Sjôstrôm, L. (1999). Sick leave and disability pension before and after treatment for obesity: A report from the Swedish Obese Subjects (SOS) study. International Journal of Obesity & Related Metabolic Disorders, 23, 6 1 9 - 6 2 4 . National Institutes of Health/National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute. (1998). Clinical guidelines on the identification, evaluation, and treatment of overweight and obesity in adults. Bethesda, M D : Author. Retrieved on January 2 0 , 2 0 0 3 , from www.nhlbi.nih.gov/ guidelines/obesity/practgde.htm Pingitore, R., Dugoni, B . L., Tindale, R. S., 8c Spring, B . (1994). Bias against overweight job applicants in a simulated employment interview. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 9 0 9 - 9 1 7 . Pi-Sunyer, F. X . (1993). Medical hazards of obesity. Annals of Internal Medicine, 119, 655-660. Polivy, P., 8c Herman, C. P. (1992). Undieting: A program to help people stop dieting. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 11, 2 6 1 - 2 6 8 . Pories, W. J . , Swanson, M . S., MacDonald, K. G., Long, S. B . , Morris, P. G., Brown, Β. M., Barakat, Η. Α., deRamon, R. Α., Israel, G., 8c Dolezal, J . M. (1995). Who would have thought it? An operation proves to be the most effective therapy for adult-onset diabetes mellitus. Annals of Surgery, 222, 3 3 9 - 3 5 0 .
144
BEHAVIORS THAT C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH Price, R. A. (2002). Genetics and common obesities: Background, current status, strategies, and future prospects. In T. A. Wadden & A. J . Stunkard (Eds.), Handbook of obesity treatment (pp. 7 3 - 9 4 ) . New York: Guilford. Price, R. Α., Charles, Μ. Α., Pettitt, D. J . , & Knowler, W. C. (1993). Obesity in Pima Indians: Large increases among post-World War II birth cohorts. American Journal of Physiological Anthropology, 92, 4 7 3 - 4 7 9 . Puhl, R., & Brownell, K. D. (2001). Bias, discrimination, and obesity. Obesity Research, 9, 7 8 8 - 8 0 5 . Rankinen, T., Perusse, L., Weisnagel, S. J . , Snyder, E. E., Chagnon, Y . C , & Bouchard, C. (2002). The human obesity gene map: The 2 0 0 1 update. Obesity Research, 10, 1 9 6 - 2 4 9 . Roehling, M . V. (1999). Weight-based discrimination in employment: Psychological and legal aspects. Personnel Psychology, 52, 9 6 9 - 1 0 1 6 . Rosen, J . C. (1996). Improving body image in obesity. In J . K. Thompson (Ed.), Body image, eating disorders, and obesity: An integrative guide for assessment and treatmenr (pp. 4 2 5 - 4 4 0 ) . Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Rosen, J . C , Orosan, P., &c Reiter, J . (1995). Cognitive behavior therapy for negative body image in obese women. Behavior Therapy, 26, 2 5 - 4 2 . Rossner, S., Sjôstrôm, L., Noack, R., Meinders, A. E., & Noseda, G. (2000). Weight loss, weight maintenance, and improved cardiovascular risk factors after 2 years treatment with orlistat for obesity: European Orlistat Obesity Study Group. Obesity Research, 8, 4 9 - 6 1 . Sarwer, D. B., Wadden, T. Α., &C Foster, G. D. (1998). Assessment of body image dissatisfaction in obese women: Specificity, severity, and clinical significance. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 6 5 1 - 6 5 4 . Stuart, R. B . (1967). Behavioral control of overeating. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 5, 3 5 7 - 3 6 5 . Stunkard, A. J . (2002). Binge-eating disorder and night-eating syndrome. In T. A. of obesity treatment Wadden & A. J . Stunkard (Eds.), Handbook (pp. 1 0 7 - 1 2 1 ) . New York: Guilford. Stunkard, A. J . , Berkowitz, R. L, Stallings, V. Α., &c Schoeller, D. A. (1999). Energy intake, not energy output, is a determinant of body size in infants. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 69, 5 2 4 - 5 3 0 . Tataranni, P. Α., & Ravussin, E. (2002). Energy metabolism and obesity. In T. A. Wadden & A. J . Stunkard (Eds.), Handbook of obesity treatment (pp. 4 2 - 7 2 ) . New York: Guilford. Thompson, J . K., Coovert, M., Richards, K. J . , Johnson, S., & Cattarin, J . (1995). Development of body image, eating disturbance, and general psychological functioning in female adolescents: Covariance structure modeling and longitudinal investigations. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 18, 2 2 1 - 2 3 6 . Thompson, J . K., Heinberg, L. J . , Altabe, M . N., &c Tantleff-Dunn, S. (1999). Exacting beauty: Theory, assessment, and treatment of body image disturbance. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Thompson, J . K., & Smolak, L. (2001). Body image, eating disorders, and obesity treatment, and prevention. Washington, DC: American in youth: Assessment, Psychological Association. Torgerson, J . S., &t Sjôstrôm, L. (2001). The Swedish Obese Subjects (SOS) Study: Metabolic Rationale and results. International Journal of Obesity & Related Disorders, 25, S2-S4. Vanltallie, T. B. (1994). Worldwide epidemiology of obesity. Pharmacoeconomics, 5, 1. Wadden, Τ. Α., & Foster, G. D. (2000). Behavioral treatment of obesity. Medical Clinics of North America, 84, 4 4 1 - 4 4 6 .
Obesity
and Body Image
Disturbance
Wadden, Τ. Α., Sternberg, J . Α., Letizia, Κ. Α., Stunkard, A. J . , & Foster, G. D. (1989). Treatment of obesity by very low calorie diet, behavior therapy, and their combination: A 5-year perspective. International Journal of Obesity, 13, 39-46. Wadden, Τ. Α., & Vanltallie, T. B. (1992). Treatment of the severely obese patient. New York: Guilford. Wadden, T. Α., Womble, L. G., Stunkard, A. J . , & Anderson, D. A. (2002). Psychosocial consequences of obesity and weight loss. In T. A. Wadden & A. J . Stunkard (Eds.), Handbook of obesity treatment (pp. 1 4 4 - 1 6 9 ) . New York: Guilford. Whitaker, R. C., Wright, J . Α., Pepe, M . S., Seidel, K. D., & Dietz, W. H. (1997). Predicting obesity in young adulthood from childhood and parental obesity. New England Journal of Medicine, 337, 8 6 9 - 8 7 3 . Wilson, G. T. (1994). Behavioral treatment of obesity: Thirty years and counting. Advances in Behavioural Research and Therapy, 16, 3 3 3 1 - 3 3 7 5 . Wilson, G. T. (1996). Acceptance and change in the treatment of eating disorders and obesity. Behavior Therapy, 27, 4 1 7 - 4 3 9 . Wirth, Α., &c Krause, J . (2001). Long-term weight loss with sibutramine: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Medical Association, 286, 1331-1339. World Health Organization. (1998). Obesity: Preventing and managing the global epidemic (Publication No. WHO/NUT/NCD/98.1). Geneva: Author.
145
CHAPTER
Physical Inactivity as a Risk Factor for Chronic
9
Disease
KRISTA A . BARBOUR, TIMOTHY T . HOULE, AND PATRICIA M . DUBBERT
T
he health benefits of physical activity
activity for good health. T h e report included
have been demonstrated repeatedly over
recommendations for the intensity, duration,
the past several decades, and engaging in
and frequency o f physical activity sufficient for in the
regular physical activity has been recommended
meeting the goal o f disease prevention
as one effective way in which to decrease
general population. A primary conclusion of
both morbidity and mortality (U.S. Department
the report was that a moderate level of activity
of Health and Human Services, 1996). Unfor-
(e.g., 3 0 minutes of walking on most days o f
tunately, current guidelines for leisure time
the week) is an appropriate goal for most
physical activity are not met by the majority of
Americans in terms o f realizing health benefits.
individuals in the United States. Indeed, a signifi-
However, the report also emphasized
cant percentage of the population is considered
6 0 % o f American adults are not physically
that
sedentary or ^sufficiently active (Centers for
active on a regular basis and that 2 5 % are
Disease Control and Prevention [CDC], 2 0 0 1 ) .
sedentary (e.g., report no leisure time physical
Because of its status as a primary risk factor in
activity). A recent, population-based telephone
physical
survey o f American adults found that rates o f
must be addressed in any effort to
physical activity remained stable during the
the development of chronic disease, inactivity
reduce rates of illness and early mortality.
years of 1 9 9 0 to 1 9 9 8 ( C D C , 2 0 0 1 ) , a finding that was disappointing and somewhat surprising given the increased emphasis on public
DEFINITIONS AND DESCRIPTIONS OF PHYSICAL ACTIVITY AND INACTIVITY
health initiatives to increase physical activity rates in the United States. Perhaps even more discouraging are the results o f studies focusing on demographic
In 1 9 9 6 , the U.S. surgeon general issued a
differences
report outlining the importance o f physical
physical activity. These findings indicate that
in the prevalence o f
regular
Physical Inactivity
|
the percentage o f active adults is considerably
becoming active if one is sedentary or by
smaller among ethnic minorities (particularly
increasing one's activity level. For most of the
Hispanic Americans) relative to Caucasians. In
remainder of the chapter, the focus will shift
addition, women are generally less physically
from physical activity as a primary prevention
active than men, and physical activity decreases
strategy to physical activity as it relates to
with age. Other predictors o f an inactive
specific chronic diseases.
lifestyle include lower income, lesser educa-
T o discuss the role that physical activity
tional attainment, and living in the southern or
plays in chronic disease, it is useful to first
midwestern
address the issue o f assessment
United States (Schoenborn
&
Barnes, 2 0 0 2 ) .
o f physical
activity. This includes the methods
com-
According to an American College o f Sports
monly used to measure activity as well as
Medicine ( 1 9 9 8 ) position stand, healthy adults
recent trends in assessment (for a review o f
should engage in 2 0 to 6 0 minutes (which may
the major developments in physical activity
be continuous or accumulated in several shorter
research during the past decade, see Dubbert,
bouts) of aerobic activity 3 to 5 days per week
2 0 0 2 ) . T h e following section addresses the
to improve cardiorespiratory fitness. For most
numerous ways in which physical activity
people, a moderate intensity level o f physical
has been conceptualized by researchers.
activity is recommended to decrease the likelihood of exercise-induced injury. In addition to aerobic exercise, individuals are encouraged to incorporate both resistance (e.g., set o f exercises that are designed to condition major muscle
OVERVIEW OF RESEARCH IN THE ASSESSMENT OF PHYSICAL ACTIVITY
groups two or three times per week) and flexibility (e.g., stretch major muscle groups two or
T h e measurement
three times per week) training into their exercise
received increasing attention during the past several decades. T h e task o f
programs. However, as already noted, most American adults do not meet this level o f physical activity. Given this unfortunate
fact, it is important
to consider the potential negative consequences of physical inactivity.
o f physical activity has
For example, a lack of
physical activity has been clearly linked to an increased risk of all-cause mortality (see, e.g., Blair et al., 1 9 8 9 ; Wei et al., 1 9 9 9 ) . In addition, a sedentary lifestyle has been shown to predict the nation's number one cause of death: cardiovascular disease (CVD) (Farrell et al., 1 9 9 8 ) . Indeed, a large percentage of the population is
quantifying
physical activity across various populations, settings, and purposes
has proved
to
be
particularly daunting. In the subsections that follow, the methods that have been used to measure physical activity in both the laboratory and community settings are summarized, with special emphasis on each method's particular strengths and weaknesses. T h e issues involved with the assessment o f physical activity in specialized populations and settings are also discussed along with future directions o f physical activity assessment.
at increased risk of disease morbidity and mortality because of insufficient physical activity. The surgeon general's report and the body of scientific knowledge it represents suggest that many individuals will acquire illnesses that can
Physical Activity: What to Measure? Physical activity has been characterized as
be prevented by physical activity. In addition, it
"any bodily movement resulting in energy
is possible that the progression of many diseases
expenditure above resting levels" (Freedson &
and conditions can be slowed or halted by
Miller, 2 0 0 0 , p. 2 1 ; see also Caspersen, 1 9 8 9 ) .
147
148
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH This definition logically leads to the goal o f
cost. In a recent review of self-report measures
quantifying energy expenditure as the target o f
used during the 1 9 9 0 s ,
Sallis and Saelens
measurement. Indeed, the methods described
(2000) found that, in general, adult self-report
in this section are often compared with simul-
measures were more valid for reports o f
taneous measures of energy expenditure as
vigorous activity than for reports of moderate
indexes o f validity. However, physical activity
intensity activity. In addition, they found that
is more accurately conceptualized as multidi-
interview measures had stronger psychometric
mensional in nature, with frequency, intensity,
properties than did self-administered measures
duration, and circumstance as relevant vari-
and that self-report measures did not provide
ables (Bassett, 2 0 0 0 ) . This is intuitively the
accurate estimates o f absolute amounts o f
case given that two very different activities,
physical activity; in fact, most self-report mea-
such as swimming and walking, may have the
sures
same net energy expenditure for a given time;
physical activity. In general, simple self-report
even if one is o f greater intensity, the other can
measures, although known to be less than per-
be engaged in for longer duration or greater
fectly accurate, remain extremely valuable in
frequency. Given
overestimated
absolute
amounts o f
many public health surveys and particularly in the
multidimensional
nature o f
clinical applications.
physical activity, no single assessment method provides valid and reliable measurement over
Pedometer.
A pedometer measures vertical
the possible range o f populations, settings, and
acceleration using a spring-loaded lever arm
uses (Wood, 2 0 0 0 ) . These limitations are made
that records motion either mechanically or
more complex when combined with the current
by closing an electrical circuit. Sophisticated
limitations of technology and logistical con-
pedometers have a digital output that can rep-
cerns, which preclude the use o f certain
resent activity either in number of steps or in
methods when assessing certain activities.
mileage estimates. Electronic pedometers are
Thus, when selecting a method for the assess-
extremely portable and are available for less
ment o f physical activity, a researcher must
than $ 2 0 (for a list and review, see Freedson &
clearly define the purpose of measurement and
Miller, 2 0 0 0 ) . Recent studies have shown that
the application for which it is to be used.
pedometers are fairly accurate at counting steps
Methods for Assessing Physical Activity
running (Bassett, 2 0 0 0 ) . Furthermore, pedome-
but cannot distinguish between walking and
Self-Report.
ters have little data storage capabilities and do not allow for the recording o f specific activity
Self-report instruments are the
patterns throughout the day. Despite these lim-
most widely used instruments in the assessment
itations, pedometers are widely used as a low-
of physical activity (Sallis & Saelens, 2 0 0 0 ) .
cost method o f collecting objective physical
These instruments include measures such as
activity data and in interventions that target
activity logs, self-administered questionnaires,
increased walking- and running-type activities.
interview-administered
questionnaires,
and
proxy reports (for detailed lists and reviews, see
Accelerometer.
Accelerometers measure
Kriska & Caspersen, 1 9 9 7 ; Montoye, Kemper,
acceleration in either the vertical (uniaxial) or
Saris, &
three-dimensional (triaxial) plane. T h e funda-
Washburn,
1 9 9 6 ; Sallis, 1 9 9 1 ;
Sallis & Saelens, 2 0 0 0 ) . M a n y o f these mea-
mental assumption behind an accelerometer is
sures have adequate reliability and validity and
that acceleration is directly proportional to
also provide assessment o f multiple activity
muscle forces; the greater the acceleration o f
modalities over a range o f situations at a low
the limb, torso, or the like, the more energy
Physical
Inactivity
|
expended by the organism. Accelerometers are
physical activity. However, recent attention has
extremely portable but can be quite expensive,
been given to the special assessment problems
with triaxial accelerometers costing more than
presented by certain subgroups and especially
$ 5 0 0 (for a list and review, see Freedson &
lifestyle-related activities. For example, the
Miller, 2 0 0 0 ) .
Accelerometers have
large
assessment of physical activity in older adults
capacities for data storage and are able to
using self-report instruments is made more
record the amount and intensity o f activity as
complicated by the fact that this group tends
well as the specific activity patterns over days
to engage in primarily light- and moderate-
or weeks. Although too expensive for many
intensity
clinical applications, accelerometers might be
Washburn, 2 0 0 0 ) . There is as yet a paucity
activity
(Bernstein e t a l . , 1 9 9 8 ;
of value in specialized programs such as for
of age-specific questionnaires to assess this
treatment o f chronic pain and cardiopulmo-
population (Washburn, 2 0 0 0 ) . T h e combina-
nary rehabilitation.
tion o f the types o f activities engaged in by older adults, the measures used to assess them, Heart rate monitors
and the potential unreliability o f recall for these
generally consist o f a chest strap transmitter
types of activities supports the need for more
and
refined assessment tools for older adults.
Heart a
Rate
Monitor.
wristwatch
receiver
for
storage.
Monitors vary in quality but can be purchased
Traditional assessment o f physical activity
for between $ 2 0 0 and $ 5 0 0 (for a list and
has focused on participation in structured,
review, see Freedson & Miller, 2 0 0 0 ) . Under
time-limited bouts o f activity (i.e., "exercise").
normal conditions, heart rate is linearly related
For example, researchers may measure the
to energy expenditure, but many sources o f
number o f miles walked on the treadmill or
error can elevate heart rate even at rest and
the number o f minutes spent on a stationary
can obscure the relationship between energy
bicycle. M o r e recently, however, the definition
expenditure and heart rate (Freedson & Miller,
o f what should constitute physical activity has
2 0 0 0 ) . G o o d heart rate monitors have storage
been broadened. O n e such change has been
capacity to record heart rate over extended
to challenge the notion that physical activity
periods of time and allow for patterns of activ-
must occur in one episode o f long duration
ity over time. These monitors might not be
(i.e., 2 0 to 3 0 minutes) to be beneficial. It is
practical for many clinical applications but
n o w generally agreed that the accumulation o f
could be valuable for cardiac patients.
activity throughout one's day may be sufficient to realize improved health. Such activiand
ties may include walking the stairs instead o f
weaknesses o f each type o f field measure
riding the elevator and walking short distances
described in this section. Recently, researchers
instead o f driving a car.
Table 9.1 highlights the strengths
aware of the limitations o f any single type o f
In addition to the more inclusive definition
assessment modality have focused on combin-
of physical activity, other difficulties remain in
ing the methods to sample more of the quali-
measurement.
ties of physical activity. T h e use o f multiple
activity is often neglected in surveys o f physical
For example,
occupational
modalities and complex calibration techniques
activity. Because most occupations today are
can increase measurement accuracy, and they
primarily sedentary, most physical activity
are often recommended for the assessment o f
questionnaires inquire about leisure time activ-
physical activity. The complex and multidimensional nature
ity only. This is problematic because those individuals most likely to be characterized as
o f various forms o f physical activity is sufficient
sedentary during leisure time are also more
to complicate the accurate assessment o f
likely to engage in job-related physical activity
149
150
BEHAVIORS THAT C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH Table 9.1
Field Methods for the Assessment of Physical Activity
Modality
Cost
Patterns of Activity?
Self-report
Low
Pedometer
Accelerometer Uniaxial
Triaxial Heart rate
Accuracy
Comments
Yes
Good for relative amounts
Many different options available
Low
No
Good for walking
Unable to distinguish instensity of physical activity
Medium
Yes
Measures both amount and intensity
Might not measure light activities in multiple planes
High
Yes
As above
Measures multiple planes of movement
Medium-High
Yes
Good
Multiple sources of measurement error
( C D C , 2 0 0 1 ) . Thus, it may be that some
Traditionally, individuals who had experi-
respondents who are classified as sedentary are
enced heart attacks were prescribed bed rest as
actually active, but only on the j o b . T h e failure
a significant part of their cardiac rehabilita-
of many studies to capture physical activity
tion. However, as the medical and psycholog-
obtained through employment is a weakness
ical benefits of being active were documented
that needs to be addressed in future research.
PHYSICAL ACTIVITY INTERVENTION IN C H R O N I C DISEASE
Cardiovascular Disease and Physical Activity Physical activity has clearly been shown to
for this population (for a review o f this evidence, see Dubbert, Rappaport, &
Martin,
1 9 8 7 ) , physical activity became a
primary
focus o f treatment for those with C V D . T h e recommended
intensity, duration, and
fre-
quency o f physical activity to be prescribed in cardiac rehabilitation programs are described in Leon ( 2 0 0 0 ) . In sum, sessions o f aerobic exercise should include both warm-up and cool-down periods and should be o f an inten-
reduce the risk o f cardiovascular morbidity
sity o f between 4 0 % and 8 5 % o f V 0
and mortality (e.g., Berlin & Colditz, 1 9 9 0 )
widely used measure of maximal oxygen con-
and, as a result, has been recommended in
sumption at a given workload). These sessions
2 m a x
(a
the primary prevention o f C V D . However,
should occur three times per week and last for
because most Americans do not acquire a
at least 2 0 to 6 0 minutes each session. W h e n
level o f physical activity that is sufficient to
health psychologists consider physical activity
decrease the risk o f C V D , research has focused
in C V D rehabilitation, they must keep in mind
prevention following cardiovas-
that participation in a physical activity pro-
cular events (e.g., chest pain, heart attack).
gram should be supervised by a medical expert
T h e purpose of secondary prevention is to
such as a physician or an exercise professional
prevent further cardiac events in individuals
(American Association o f Cardiovascular and
who already manifest some degree o f C V D .
Pulmonary
on secondary
Rehabilitation, 1 9 9 9 ; Dubbert
Physical et al., 1 9 8 7 ) . This is a necessity because
rehabilitation
although events are infrequent, patients with
Caucasian
research
men,
has
future
Inactivity focused
research
on
should
C V D are at increased risk for experiencing
address the effectiveness of physical activity in
cardiac events as a result o f participation in
ethnic minorities and women who have devel-
physical activity relative to their disease-free
oped C V D . (For a more complete discussion
counterparts.
of coronary heart disease, see Chapter 15.)
M a n y observational studies have demonstrated
a significant relationship
between
physical activity and cardiovascular mortality. In
a large-scale prospective
Hypertension and Physical Activity
investigation,
Hypertension, or elevated blood pressure
(2000)
(BP), affects approximately 5 0 million adults
Wannamethee, Shaper, and Walker
examined the association between physical
in the United States (Kaplan, 1 9 9 8 ) . Hyper-
activity and mortality in older men with C V D .
tension is a major health issue in the United
These men were followed up approximately 13
States, representing the most significant risk
years subsequent to a baseline assessment. M e n
factor for the development o f C V D , including
who participated in physical activity experi-
both
enced significant reductions in both all-cause
( M a c M a h o n et al., 1 9 9 0 ) . Current guidelines
coronary
heart
disease and
stroke
and cardiovascular mortality relative to seden-
(Joint National
tary individuals, importantly, several types o f
Evaluation, and Treatment o f High Blood
physical activity (e.g., gardening, walking) were
Pressure Q N C ] , 1 9 9 7 ) dictate that the optimal
shown to have a beneficial effect on mortality
level o f BP should be 1 2 0 / 8 0 m m Hg (milli-
Committee on Detection,
rates. In addition, those men who were seden-
meters of mercury) or lower. Values o f 1 4 0 / 9 0
tary at baseline but who later became active
or higher are considered to be high or in the
demonstrated significantly lower levels of mor-
hypertensive range.
tality than did those men who remained inactive throughout the follow-up period.
Given that m a n y hypertensive patients' BP
Randomized clinical trials of physical activ-
levels are n o t consistently controlled
with
antihypertensive
medication
(most
ity in the treatment o f C V D have generally
likely due to poor compliance with medica-
shown that physical activity is effective
tions
at
reducing cardiovascular mortality (for a review
[ M a n c i a , Sega, M i l e s i ,
C e s a n a , 8c
Zanchetti, 1 9 9 7 ] ) , decades o f research have
Guyatt,
been devoted to the investigation o f the non-
Fischer, & R i m m , 1 9 8 8 ) . In addition, physical
pharmacological treatment o f hypertension.
of
these
studies,
see
Oldridge,
activity interventions have resulted in increased
Examples
exercise endurance, decreased chest pain, and
include relaxation, biofeedback, and stress
o f widely
studied
treatments
reduced progression of atherosclerosis (Leon,
management (for a review o f these treat-
2 0 0 0 ) . Finally, as discussed elsewhere in this
ments, see Linden & Chambers, 1 9 9 4 ) .
chapter, physical activity also improves several
Physical activity has also been examined as a
conditions that are known risk factors for
treatment for hypertension. It has been recom-
C V D such as obesity and hypertension.
mended that for individuals classified as mildly
In conclusion, physical activity has been
hypertensive, initial treatment consisting o f
shown to be effective in the secondary preven-
"lifestyle changes," including physical activity,
tion o f C V D (i.e., reduction o f cardiac events).
should be implemented for the first 6 to 12
Regular physical activity should be recom-
months ( J N C , 1 9 9 7 ) . In this subsection, the
mended for all individuals with C V D who do
efficacy of physical activity for the treatment of
not have conditions that would limit or pro-
hypertension is explored. In general, the desired
hibit their participation. Because most cardiac
outcome of a physical activity intervention
152
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH would be the decreased use of medications used
characteristics such as gender and ethnicity
to control BP or the elimination of the need for
(Lesniak & Dubbert, 2 0 0 1 ) . Regarding gender
antihypertensive medication altogether, along
differences in the effectiveness o f physical
with a possible improvement in other C V D risk
activity in treating hypertension, the data have
factors related to physical activity.
generally shown
no significant
differences
In a recent review o f 1 5 studies o f exercise
between women and men. Studies examining
training in hypertensive individuals, Hagberg,
the effectiveness o f physical activity in lowering
Park, and Brown ( 2 0 0 0 ) found that approxi-
BP in African Americans are
mately
experienced
important because o f the high prevalence of
significant decreases in B P . In addition, the
hypertension within this group. In addition,
hypotensive effect o f physical activity was
hypertension in African Americans is more
shown to occur quickly (within 1 to 1 0 weeks)
severe and less well controlled relative to other
and at low to moderate levels o f intensity.
ethnic groups. Unfortunately, the relationship
Interestingly, the reduction in BP levels was
between hypertension
not related to the amount o f weight lost
in African Americans has not been studied
7 5 % o f participants
particularly
and physical activity
adequately (Lesniak & Dubbert, 2 0 0 1 ) . Encour-
during the various training programs. In a meta-analysis o f 2 9 randomized clin-
agingly, the data that do exist suggest that
ical trials o f aerobic physical activity, it was
physical activity is an effective treatment for
demonstrated that participation in a training
hypertension
program averaging 1 9 weeks resulted in a 4 -
Kokkinos et a l , 1 9 9 5 ) .
point decrease in systolic BP and a 3-point
in African
Americans (e.g.,
In summary, physical activity has been
decrease in diastolic BP ( H a l b e r t et al.,
shown to be an effective treatment for hyper-
1 9 9 7 ) . Although this change in BP was signifi-
tension. However, it should also be noted that
cantly more than that achieved by partici-
the hypotensive effect o f physical activity has
pants in the control groups, it is lower than
been shown to be modest in several studies (for
that reported in other studies.
a review, see Blumenthal, Sherwood, Gullette,
Consistent
colleagues
(2000)
Georgiades, & Tweedy, 2 0 0 2 ) . Although the
review, however, the improvement
in B P
BP reduction achieved by physical activity may
levels occurred in the absence o f weight loss.
appear to be minimal, epidemiological data
with
the Hagberg
and
appears
indicate that small drops in BP are accompa-
to have a beneficial effect on reducing BP
nied by an impressive decrease in the risk of
in individuals with hypertension.
(However,
stroke and C V D . For example, a 5- to 6-point
not all studies have found significant effects
reduction in diastolic BP has been associated
[Blumenthal, Siegal, &
Appelbaum, 1 9 9 1 ;
with a 3 5 % to 4 0 % decrease in stroke and
Nami et al., 2000].) Although the majority o f
with a 2 0 % to 2 5 % decrease in heart disease
Thus, aerobic exercise training
training programs examined have aerobic exer-
(Collins e t a l . , 1 9 9 0 ) . (For a more complete
cise (e.g., walking, cycling), recent evidence sug-
discussion of hypertension, see Chapter 14.)
gests that nonaerobic
forms o f physical activity,
such as resistance training, may also be useful in the treatment of hypertension. A recent metaanalysis (Kelley &
Kelley, 2 0 0 0 )
showed a
small but statistically significant decrease in BP
Chronic Obstructive Pulmonary Disease and Physical Activity Chronic obstructive pulmonary
disease
following resistance training two to five times
(COPD) is a condition in which there is an
per week for an average o f 1 4 weeks.
impaired ability o f the lungs to take in sufficient
The ability of a physical activity program to
air. T h e primary symptoms of the disease con-
decrease BP may depend, in part, on individual
sist o f difficulty in breathing and a long-term
Physical cough. C O P D may result from a variety of lung
Inactivity
Physical activity has been shown to improve
disorders such as asthma and emphysema. A
the cognitive test performance
in C O P D
common corollary o f C O P D is exercise intoler-
patients. For example, Emery, Schein, Hauck,
ance. This often is a result of a vicious cycle in
and Maclntyre ( 1 9 9 8 ) found that, in addition
which the individual, on experiencing breathing
to improvement in exercise endurance, some
difficulties (dyspnea) while exercising, perceives
aspects o f cognitive functioning (e.g., verbal
the dyspnea as threatening and so avoids engag-
fluency) were improved in a sample o f c o m -
ing in physical activity. Eventually, decondi-
munity adults (over age 5 0 years) diagnosed
tioning occurs and leads to exercise intolerance.
with C O P D . In this study, the physical activity
In an effort to target the exercise intolérance
component consisted of 4 5 minutes o f aerobic
that often accompanies the disease, many
activity on 5 days per week for 5 weeks (fol-
treatment
for C O P D include a
lowed by a 5-week period o f lower intensity
physical activity component. T h e physical
activity on 3 days per week). As with the
activity offered is typically in the context of a
improvement
pulmonary
programs
exhibited in physical
health,
(which
these changes in cognitive functioning were
might also include topics such as health educa-
demonstrated after a relatively brief physical
tion and stress management). There is much
activity intervention.
evidence
rehabilitation program
activity
In sum, the evidence clearly supports the use
improves the exercise tolerance o f C O P D
showing
of physical activity in the treatment o f C O P D .
patients. In addition,
inclusion of a physical activity component in
demonstrated
that
after
(Laçasse et al., 1996).
physical
improvement can be 4
weeks
of
training
pulmonary rehabilitation programs has been
Specifically, physical
shown repeatedly to increase exercise endurance
activity decreases dyspnea, decreases leg fatigue,
and may result in fewer complications and
and enhances health-related quality o f life (for
hospitalizations related to the disease. In addi-
a review o f this literature, see Bourjeily&
tion, there is some evidence to suggest that
Rochester, 2 0 0 0 ) .
physical activity improves the cognitive func-
C O P D is a disease that is characterized by intermittent exacerbations (e.g., upper
tioning o f individuals with C O P D . Further
and
research is needed to determine the optimal
lower respiratory tract infections). In general,
dose o f physical activity necessary to produce
the available evidence suggests that participa-
sufficient
tion in a pulmonary rehabilitation program
health and cognitive performance.
improvements
in b o t h
physical
that includes a physical activity component is associated with a reduction in COPD-related exacerbations and hospitalizations
(Berry &
OBESITY AND PHYSICAL ACTIVITY
Walschlager, 1 9 9 8 ) . Unfortunately, it has been difficult to identify the specific components of
Obesity in the United States has achieved the
rehabilitation programs that may lead to this
status o f a public health crisis (for a recent
improvement in health. Further research is nec-
review
essary to determine whether or not physical
Foster, & Brownell, 2 0 0 2 ) . This is due to the
activity alone leads to better health outcomes.
fact that excess weight represents a major risk
of
the
problem,
see
Wadden,
In addition to the physical limitations experi-
factor for chronic disease development (e.g.,
enced by C O P D patients, impaired cognitive
hypertension, C V D , type 2 diabetes mellitus).
functioning is sometimes associated with the
M o s t clinicians are n o w encouraged to evalu-
disease (Prigatano, Parsons, Wright, Levin, &
ate weight status using the body mass index
Hawryluk, 1983) and may result from decreased
( B M I ) . B M I , the most commonly used mea-
blood oxygenation levels (Grant et al., 1 9 8 7 ) .
sure o f healthy versus unhealthy
weight,
154
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH assesses body weight in relation to height and
minimal effect o f physical activity alone on
is defined as weight in kilograms divided by
body weight is the duration o f most exercise
height in meters squared. Current guidelines
programs. It may be that the 4 to 6 months
define "overweight" as B M I values in the
of training typical of physical activity studies
range o f 2 5 . 0 to 2 9 . 9 , with "obesity" consid-
is not sufficient for realizing the benefits o f
ered to be any value o f 3 0 . 0 or higher
physical activity (Wing, 1 9 9 9 ) . Findings from
(National Institutes o f Health, 1 9 9 8 ) . It has
longitudinal investigations of physical activity
been estimated that approximately 5 0 % o f
and weight have suggested that long-term
U.S. adults are overweight or obese (World
physical activity is effective in slowing and
Health Organization, 1 9 9 8 ) , and the preva-
minimizing subsequent weight gain (but might
lence appears to be rising (Bouchard, 2 0 0 0 ) .
not result in weight loss or even prevent
Body weight is determined by energy (food) intake and energy expenditure; however, this is
weight gain [Grundy et al., 1 9 9 9 ] ) . Thus, maintaining
participation in an exer-
a simplified definition o f an extremely complex
cise program is key to successful weight loss. In
set o f biological, behavioral, and environmen-
general, treatment programs for obesity (typi-
tal variables (the complexities of the issue
cally including physical activity accompanied
are addressed in Salbe 8c Ravussin, 2 0 0 0 ) . If
by dietary changes) induce an initially rapid
energy intake and expenditure are in balance,
weight loss followed by a steady reduction in
no significant weight loss or gain should occur.
the amount o f weight lost long term (Jeffery
However, a situation in which food intake is
etal., 2 0 0 0 ) . Perri and colleagues (2001) com-
consistently higher than energy expended will
pared a standard weight loss intervention (20
result in weight gain. T w o primary contribu-
weeks of educational sessions, dietary changes,
tors to the energy imbalance evident in the
and moderate-intensity physical activity) with
United States are a sedentary lifestyle and poor
extended
dietary habits. T h e focus of the remainder o f
occurred for 1 2 months following the comple-
this section is on the relationship between
tion of the standard intervention and included
obesity and the expenditure
the use o f exercise diaries. It was found that
of energy, that is,
physical activity (or exercise).
treatment.
Extended
participants assigned to extended
treatment
treatment
Observational studies have demonstrated a
(after completing the standard program) main-
significant relationship between physical activ-
tained the weight lost during the standard treat-
ity and obesity, with more active individuals
ment, whereas those completing the standard
being less likely to be obese. In addition, there
treatment alone regained approximately half of
exist prospective studies suggesting that
the weight initially lost (Perri et al., 2 0 0 1 ) .
a
lack o f physical activity is a predictor of obe-
There is also evidence that tailoring a
sity (for a review, see J e b b 8c M o o r e , 1 9 9 9 ) .
physical activity program to an individual's
However, isolating the effects o f physical
specific needs may result in higher levels o f
activity interventions for obesity is difficult
exercise maintenance. For example, Bock,
because most studies also include a dietary
Marcus, Pinto, and Forsyth ( 2 0 0 1 ) found that
modification component such as a low-calorie
participants who received individualized feed-
diet. It has been shown that weight loss pro-
back regarding their exercise program were
grams using physical activity alone are effec-
significantly more likely to maintain treatment
tive in producing
modest weight loss (as
levels o f physical activity at follow-up (12
compared with control groups) but do not
months) than were participants who com-
result in as much weight loss as does exercise
pleted a standard physical activity intervention.
combined with dietary changes (Wing, 1 9 9 9 ) .
In conclusion, weight loss programs that
O n e possible explanation for the relatively
include a physical activity component have
Physical generally been shown to lead to a clinically
Mockett, &
Inactivity
\
Fentem, 2 0 0 1 ) . T h e potential
significant weight reduction. However, the
role o f physical activity in the development o f
challenge o f maintaining this weight loss fol-
osteoarthritis has increased in importance
lowing treatment remains. Future research
with the surgeon general's report calling for
should focus on the factors related to contin-
adults to increase their levels o f physical activ-
ued participation in physical activity. F o r
ity (U.S. Department of Health and H u m a n
example, understanding the behavioral vari-
Services, 1 9 9 6 ) .
ables that differentiate individuals w h o main-
Recent research has shown that engaging in
tain physical activity (and weight loss) from
low- to moderate-intensity levels o f physical
those w h o regain the weight initially lost
activity does not increase the risk o f the devel-
during treatment is a high priority (Marcus
opment o f osteoarthritis in the knee or hip
et al., 2 0 0 0 ) . (A more complete discussion o f
(Cheng et al., 2 0 0 0 ; Sutton et al., 2 0 0 1 ) . T h e
obesity and body image disturbances was
risk o f developing osteoarthritis from high-
provided in Chapter 8.)
intensity activities is not so clear, however, with some studies showing increased risk (Cheng e t a l . , 2 0 0 0 ;
OSTEOARTHRITIS AND
Cooper,
McAlindon,
PHYSICAL ACTIVITY
Dieppe,
1 9 9 4 ) and
Cooper et al., 1 9 9 8 ; Coggon, others
Egger, &
not
showing
increased risk (White, Wright, &
Hudson,
Arthritis is one o f the leading causes o f chronic
1 9 9 3 ) . W h a t is clear from the studies involv-
pain
and
ing intense physical activity is that joint injury
decreased quality o f life. Osteoarthritis can be
greatly increases the risk o f osteoarthritis
and
often results in disability
divided into two conceptual types: primary
(Cheng e t a l . , 2 0 0 0 ; Sutton e t a l . , 2 0 0 1 ) and
osteoarthritis, which is thought to be related
that perhaps the higher risk o f developing
to aging and heredity, and secondary osteo-
osteoarthritis associated with intense physical
arthritis, which is caused by conditions such as
activities may be explained by those activities'
obesity, joint trauma, and repetitious joint use
greater association with injury.
(Cheng et al., 2 0 0 0 ) . It is estimated that the
There are several reasons to believe that
prevalence o f arthritis is increasing, with
increased physical activity is beneficial in pre-
1 5 . 0 % o f the population affected in 1 9 9 0 but
venting
an expected prevalence o f 1 8 . 2 % by 2 0 2 0
osteoarthritis (Cheng e t a l . , 2 0 0 0 ) . Activity
(Wang, Helmick, M a c e r a , Zhang, &
and
reducing
the
symptoms
of
Pratt,
strengthens the muscular support surrounding
2 0 0 1 ) . Furthermore, the disability rates pro-
joints and consequently reduces the risk o f
duced from arthritis also appear to be grow-
injury. Furthermore, physical activity improves
ing,
more
and maintains joint mobility. Physical activity
suffering from arthritis (Wang et al., 2 0 0 1 ;
is also effective at reducing many of the other
Yelin, 1 9 9 2 ) .
risk factors o f osteoarthritis such as obesity,
with
those affected reporting
Traditionally, physicians have suggested the
avoidance
o f vigorous
activity
and
hypertension, hypercholesterolemia, and high blood glucose (Hart, Doyle, & Spector, 1 9 9 5 ) .
encouraged physical inactivity for the treat-
Finally, physical activity increases the nourish-
ment o f osteoarthritis, reflecting a belief
ment of joint cartilage through the diffusion of
that joint use exacerbates the condition. This
nutrients via joint fluid (Cheng et al., 2 0 0 0 ;
"wear-and-tear"
Hall, Urban, & Gehl, 1 9 9 1 ) .
hypothesis persists
even
today, as studies examining the role o f physical activity
in producing
osteoarthritis
have
been somewhat inconsistent (Sutton, M u i r ,
In 1 9 9 9 , the Arthritis Foundation, C D C , and
9 0 other organizations
National
Arthritis
Action
released
Plan:
A
the Public
155
156
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH Callahan, &
showing a negative dose-response to increasing
Helmick, 1 9 9 9 ; W a n g e t a l . , 2 0 0 1 ) . Among
levels o f physical activity (Friedenreich, 2 0 0 1 ) .
Health
Strategy
(Meenan,
the recommendations included in this publica-
The
evidence supporting risk
reduction
tion was the need to decrease the rates of
from physical activity in other cancers is
physical inactivity in adults with
not as compelling, but there is substantial
arthritis
( 3 4 . 8 % ) , which are higher than those in adults
evidence
without arthritis ( 2 7 . 7 % ) (Wang et al., 2 0 0 1 ) .
prostate cancer (Friedenreich, 2 0 0 1 ) . Further-
Others have also recommended the use o f
more, preliminary evidence has been gather-
physical
ing to indicate that physical activity may be
activity
in
the
management
of
supporting the association
with
osteoarthritis, and its positive effects have
negatively related to lung cancer, testicular
received empirical support (Ettinger & Afable,
c a n c e r , ovarian
1 9 9 4 ; Ettinger et al., 1 9 9 7 ; Minor, 1 9 9 1 ) .
cancer (Friedenreich, 2 0 0 1 ) . However, there
cancer, and
endometrial
is also compelling evidence to indicate that physical activity is not at all associated with rectal cancer (Friedenreich, 2 0 0 1 ) .
CANCER AND PHYSICAL ACTIVITY
T h e role o f exercise in cancer treatment has not been well researched, yet physical
Recently, Friedenreich ( 2 0 0 1 ) summarized the current
literature
about
the
association
activity can help to reduce the loss o f lean muscle mass during treatment, improve func-
between physical activity and cancer. She con-
tional
cluded that there is growing evidence for a pro-
enhance quality o f life (for a review, see
capacity,
increase
appetite,
1997).
and
Furthermore,
tective effect resulting from physical activity.
Oliveria 8c Christos,
However, the evidence considered was entirely
physical activity has been shown to be help-
dependent on epidemiological research. Frieden-
ful in reducing other forms o f risk in the
reich expounded on the need for randomized,
development o f cancer such as obesity and
controlled intervention trials, which will allow
other lifestyle-related health conditions.
the underlying mechanisms o f the association between physical activity and cancer to be better understood. Y e t even with the lack of controlled research, the negative association between physical activity and certain cancers
DIABETES MELLITUS AND PHYSICAL ACTIVITY
supports the role o f physical activity in the
Diabetes is a heterogeneous group o f disorders
prevention o f many forms of cancer.
characterized
by hyperglycemia
or
higher
The strongest evidence o f the negative asso-
than normal levels o f blood glucose. Type 1
ciation between physical activity and cancer
diabetes, commonly referred to as "insulin-
exists for colon cancer and, to a somewhat
dependent diabetes," occurs as a result of
lesser degree, breast cancer
autoimmune
(Friedenreich,
2 0 0 1 ) . In both forms o f cancer, the risk reduc-
destruction
o f the
pancreas,
leading to a deficiency in insulin production
tion for the most physically active has been
(Peirce,
as high as 7 0 % (for reviews, see Colditz,
accounts for 9 0 % to 9 5 % o f all diabetic cases
Cannuscio, 8c Frazier, 1 9 9 7 ; Friedenreich,
in the United States (Kriska, Blair, 8c Pereira,
2001;
8c
1 9 9 4 ) , occurs as a result o f altered insulin
Britton,
secretion, elevated hepatic glucose production,
Friedenreich,
Thune,
Brinton,
Albanes, 1 9 9 8 ; Gammon, John, &
1999).
Type
2
diabetes,
which
1 9 9 8 ; Lattika, Pukkala, & Vihko, 1 9 9 8 ) . T h e
and/or diminished glucose use in skeletal mus-
average risk reduction in colon cancer is 4 0 %
cle (Wallberg-Henriksson, Rincon, 8c Zierath,
to 5 0 % , with risk for both forms o f cancer
1 9 9 8 ) . Prolonged hyperglycemia leads to the
Physical glycation o f tissues, causing organ
damage
and other negative health effects.
Inactivity
|
can also have a beneficial effect on glucose regulation (Wallberg-Henrikssonetal., 1 9 9 8 ) .
The past decade witnessed an increase in the
Increasing physical activity in
patients
prevalence of diabetes. It is estimated that in
with type 1 diabetes is a complicated issue
1 9 9 5 , 4 . 0 % o f the world's population
was
because o f the necessary self-regulation o f
afflicted, and this prevalence was projected to
insulin levels. Hypoglycemia is a potentially
increase to 5 . 4 % by 2 0 2 5 (Peirce, 1 9 9 9 ) . How-
life-threatening
ever, these reported case estimates may be low
after exercise. T h u s , the use o f physical activ-
given that there may be no symptoms at the
ity in the regulation o f type 1 diabetes must
onset o f the disease, causing many early cases to
be conducted with careful consideration o f
state that can be induced
elude detection (Kriska etal., 1 9 9 4 ) . T h e inci-
the potential
dence o f type 2 diabetes increases with age and
glycemia or ketosis. However, exercise has
for exercise-induced
hypo-
is greatly increased in obese individuals, with a
been shown to decrease the daily insulin
reported 6 0 % to 9 0 % of type 2 diabetic patients
regimens o f type 1 diabetics by increasing
being obese at onset (Kriska et al., 1 9 9 4 ) .
insulin-independent glucose transport and by
Physical inactivity has been shown to affect the physiological mechanisms thought
increasing insulin sensitivity (Peirce, 1 9 9 9 ) .
to
An overwhelming body o f evidence exists to
underlie diabetes. A total o f 3 5 days o f induced
suggest that increased physical activity should
physical inactivity caused a decrease in glucose
be recommended not only as a fundamental
tolerance in eight healthy males (Lipman et al.,
management strategy for physician-diagnosed
1 9 7 2 ) . Furthermore, individuals with spinal
diabetes but also as a strategy for the preven-
cord injury had higher blood glucose levels
tion of type 2 diabetes. (For a more complete
than did age-matched controls (Duckworth
discussion o f type 2 diabetes, see Chapter 16.)
e t a l . , 1 9 8 0 ) . Although a complete review of the physiological effects of physical activity on glucose regulation is beyond the scope o f this chapter, substantial evidence exists supporting the use of physical activity in the
PSYCHOLOGICAL FUNCTIONING AND PHYSICAL ACTIVITY
management and prevention o f type 2 diabetes
In addition to the widely studied effects of
(for
physical activity on physical functioning, partic-
reviews, see Peirce, 1 9 9 9 ; Wallberg-
Henriksson et al., 1 9 9 8 ) .
ipation in physical activity has been shown to
Exercise has been shown to lower blood glu-
play a role in emotional well-being. W h e n dis-
cose levels in diabetics (Hubinger, Franzen, &
cussing the relationship between physical activ-
Gries, 1 9 8 7 ; Peirce, 1 9 9 9 ) . It is estimated that
ity and emotions, it is necessary to consider both
9 0 % of glucose clearance occurs in skeletal
acute and
muscle, and
improvement of mood (e.g., depression) follow-
this process can
be
greatly
chronic effects.
Regarding
the
enhanced with increased energy use created
ing a single bout of exercise, the evidence is
through
mixed, with some studies finding no effect and
physical
Exercise can
activity
enhance
(Peirce,
1999).
insulin-independent
others demonstrating
significantly enhanced
transport o f glucose into cells (Peirce, 1 9 9 9 )
mood. However, the results do indicate that
and can increase insulin sensitivity (Burstein,
dose of exercise (i.e., physical activity o f varying
Epstein, Shapiro, Charuzi, & Karnieli, 1 9 9 0 ) .
intensities) does play a role in subsequent mood
Furthermore, regular exercise programs have
(for a review of these studies, see Rejeski, 1 9 9 4 ) .
been shown to improve metabolic control,
In addition to mood improvement,
one
especially in the young (Wallberg-Henriksson,
must consider the possible inducement o f
1 9 9 2 ) . Exercise-related decrease in body weight
negative m o o d following a session o f physical
157
158
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH activity. Results of several studies have shown
depression than did those receiving medication
that high-intensity exercise bouts may lead
( 3 0 % vs. 5 0 % , respectively). In addition, par-
to an increase in feelings o f anxiety and
ticipants in the physical activity group were sig-
depressed m o o d in some individuals (Rejeski,
nificantly more likely to have recovered from
1 9 9 4 ) . There is some evidence t o suggest that
major depressive disorder (partially or fully)
individuals w h o are predisposed to negative
than were those in the medication group
m o o d states are more likely to experience anx-
(Babyak et al., 2 0 0 0 ) . Regarding maintenance
iety as a result o f engaging in physical activity
of physical activity, 6 4 % o f participants who
(Cameron & Hudson, 1 9 8 6 ) . However, this
received the physical activity treatment contin-
issue is complex, and
further
research is
needed to increase our understanding o f the
ued to exercise following completion of the 1 6 week program.
role that physical activity plays in short-term
In sum, it appears that physical activity has
changes in m o o d . It has been found that in
the potential to improve m o o d both immedi-
women with normal mood at baseline, m o o d
ately following a bout of exercise (i.e., acute
after exercise improves most for those w h o felt
effects) and after participation in a long-term
worse previous to engaging in physical activity
program (i.e., chronic effects). Thus, physical
(Rejeski, Gauvin, H o b s o n , &c Norris, 1 9 9 5 ) .
activity may be an effective means of enhanc-
Given that the U.S. population is aging,
ing mental health as well as physical health. In
the treatment o f depressive disorders in older
individuals experiencing a m o o d disorder (e.g.,
adults is becoming increasingly important.
depression), physical activity has been shown
Blumenthal and colleagues ( 1 9 9 9 ) examined
to be a practical alternative to medication and
the use o f physical activity as a treatment for
should be recommended as either a primary
major depressive disorder in older adults. In
treatment or an adjunctive treatment (assum-
their work, they compared participation in a
ing no physical limitations that would con-
physical activity program with the use o f a
traindicate exercise).
c o m m o n l y prescribed antidepressant medication (sertraline hydrochloride). Participants in the study were randomly
assigned
to
CONCLUSIONS
receive either the medication, a physical activity intervention, or a c o m b i n a t i o n o f
T h e purpose o f this chapter was to review the
physical activity and medication. T h e 1 6 -
evidence that physical activity is beneficial in
week physical activity treatment consisted o f
terms o f the prevention and treatment o f dis-
three 45-minute sessions o f aerobic activity
ease. It was shown that the literature sup-
per week. It was found that both the medica-
ports engaging in physical activity as an
tion group and the physical activity group
effective means to reducing morbidity and
experienced a reduction in their levels o f
mortality. Unfortunately, most
depression
do not achieve an adequate level o f physical
(Blumenthal et al., 1 9 9 9 ) . T h e
individuals
t w o types o f treatment did not differ signifi-
activity. Understanding this pervasive lack of
cantly from one another
physical activity participation requires con-
in effectiveness.
These results suggest that physical activity is
sideration o f the role that environmental fac-
a viable alternative t o medication in the
tors play in our society.
treatment o f depression in older adults. In a 6-month follow-up study o f the same
As indicated in a recent review (Dubbert et al., 2 0 0 2 ) ,
it is clear that the inactive
participants, it was demonstrated that those
lifestyle that characterizes many individuals in
individuals who were assigned to the physical
the United States is due partly to a decrease in
activity treatment experienced lower rates o f
activity required on the j o b as well as to the
Physical
Inactivity
CASE S T U D Y " L . B . " was a 56-year-old married Caucasian male who was referred for evaluation by his primary care physician. At the time o f the evaluation, L . B . was 4 0 pounds overweight and was at the borderline o f requiring medication to control his blood glucose levels. H e was also experiencing low to moderate levels of chronic pain in his lower back, and this pain was reportedly aggravated by exertion. H e was referred for evaluation o f potential behavioral and lifestyle interventions to place his blood sugar levels under better control and help him to lose weight. L. B . was screened for depression, anxiety, and other psychopathology during his initial visit using a standard battery o f self-report assessment instruments in combination with a clinical interview. T h e assessment revealed that L . B . was generally well adjusted but that he was reporting low levels o f dysphoria and poor m o o d . Specifically, he reported a lack o f energy, difficulty in sleeping, and a loss o f interest in pleasurable activities. T h e s e symptoms were severe enough to warrant discussion but appeared to be on the sub-threshold o f a clinical diagnosis. L . B . appeared to be motivated to address his current medical problems, stating that he was quite concerned about developing diabetes. L. B.'s lifestyle was initially assessed during the interview using questions such as "Describe a typical day." F r o m this line o f questioning, it was apparent that his lifestyle largely consisted o f eating meals out, working long hours at his desk, and enjoying his weekend and after-work time by watching sports and being sedentary. W h e n asked about his levels o f physical activity, L . B . reported that he used to enjoy being active but that his lower b a c k pain had forced him to "take it easy" because it usually hurt when he engaged in even moderate levels o f activity. T o further examine his levels o f physical activity, he was given a pedometer to wear throughout the next week and was instructed to engage in his usual schedule. It should be noted that L . B . ' s primary care physician had cleared him medically for all forms o f physical activity and that L . B . had no detectable structural damage in his lower back. In addition, circulation and sensation in his lower extremities and feet were not impaired (walking ability may be limited in those with foot complications secondary to diabetes). H e agreed to record the number o f steps each evening in a log as well as to keep a food diary. Figure 9.1 displays L . B . ' s baseline levels o f physical activity as recorded by a pedometer. These baseline levels o f activity were discussed in the session, and L . B . reported surprise at the low levels o f activity in which he was engaging. His beliefs about increasing his levels o f physical activity were discussed. T h e rationale behind the positive benefits o f physical activity for m o o d , glucose regulation, and chronic pain was explained. L . B . felt that walking would not aggravate his back and stated that he was willing to begin a walking regimen. It was decided that L . B . would attempt to walk during his lunch hour but that if he were unable to do so, he would walk after w o r k with his wife. He also agreed to continue to m o n i t o r his activity but decided to discontinue his food log.
159
160
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH
12000 10000 8000 6000 4000 2000 r
Figure 9.1
L. B.'s Steps as a Function of Time
L. B . called the clinic after his first day o f walking t o report that he had greatly increased pain (which he described as 8 on a 10-point scale) and that he wanted to discontinue the regimen. H e further stated that he was n o t wearing the pedometer (he had called in sick t o w o r k because o f the pain) and that he planned on not moving for the remainder o f the day. After a discussion o f some o f L . B . ' s frustrations, he was persuaded t o continue to wear the pedometer for the remainder o f the week, even if he did n o t continue to implement a walking regimen. At the next clinic visit, L . B . ' s log was reviewed, and as can be seen in Figure 9 . 1 , his increased pain was attributed t o his "overdoing i t " on the first day o f his regimen. M a n y o f his pain beliefs regarding his physical limitations were discussed in the c o n t e x t o f pacing himself. L . B . was persuaded to attempt his walking regimen again, but this time with some restrictions set by the therapist. Specifically, during the first week, he should not walk beyond 4 , 5 0 0 steps (as indicated on the pedometer). H e agreed t o this but felt that this walking regimen was t o o little t o be o f benefit. Throughout the next few weeks, L . B.'s therapist-imposed upper limits o f pedometer readings were allowed to increase (Figure 9 . 1 ) . L . B . reported success with the regimen, stating that it allowed him extra time with his wife, w h o was also enjoying walking with him. T h e goal o f 1 0 , 0 0 0 steps daily was discussed, and L. B . felt that this goal could be accomplished. H e soon reached the goal and discontinued recording his activity, although he purchased his own pedometer. H e was also implementing changes in his diet that greatly facilitated his sense o f efficacy in changing his lifestyle. By the end o f treatment, L . B . had succeeded in losing 1 5 pounds and was extremely proud o f this accomplishment. H e had increased his activity to 1 0 , 0 0 0 steps daily, with no increase in lower back pain. (It should be noted that individuals
Physical
Inactivity
with foot or knee pain might not be able to attain this level o f activity.) In fact, he reported that his back "has not felt better in 1 5 years." Furthermore, his primary care physician believed that if L . B . were able to maintain his lifestyle changes, he would not have to pharmacologically manage L . B.'s blood sugar levels. L . B . was confident that he would be able to maintain his increased levels o f physical activity, stating that he particularly liked the accompanying increased levels o f energy and would like to lose more weight.
availability o f sedentary leisure time activities
availability o f informational
for both children and adults. Americans are
languages other than English).
also dining out more often and are consuming
materials
in
Recent studies have examined physical activ-
larger portions during meals. In addition, the
ity versus physical fitness.
physical environment plays a role in physical
attempted to make this distinction because it is
activity participation. For example, in a sam-
possible, for example, that two
ple o f women age 4 0 years or over, it was
engaging in the same dose (including intensity,
Researchers have individuals
found that the lack o f enjoyable scenery and
duration, and frequency) of physical activity
hills in the neighborhood was associated with
may have different levels o f physical fitness.
less leisure time activity (King et al., 2 0 0 0 ) .
Blair, Cheng, and Holder ( 2 0 0 1 ) sought to
Although more recent studies have included
address the issue o f whether physical activity or
men and women o f color, the majority o f
fitness is more important for good health. After
research has been with Caucasians. Because
reviewing the evidence from 6 7 studies that
certain diseases are more prevalent in ethnic
assessed physical activity, fitness, and a health
minorities
African
outcome, they concluded that it is not possible
Americans), it is crucial to focus attention on
to determine whether activity or fitness is more
(e.g., hypertension
in
these groups. In addition, consideration should
important in terms o f producing health bene-
be given to subgroups within ethnic minority
fits. Instead, they found a consistent dose-
groups. Crespo, Smit, Carter-Pokras,
response relation between physical activity and
and
Andersen ( 2 0 0 1 ) found that degree o f accul-
fitness and health outcomes across studies
turation (i.e., the extent to which an ethnic
included in the review. T h a t is, at both higher
group adopts the customs and traditions o f the
activity and fitness levels, there was a reduction
majority culture) was associated with leisure
in disease morbidity and mortality.
time physical activity in M e x i c a n Americans.
Blair and colleagues ( 2 0 0 1 ) recommended
Specifically, it was shown that inactivity was
that researchers begin to define more specifi-
significantly more likely in those individuals
cally the nature o f the dose-response relation-
who spoke primarily Spanish in the home (a
ship.
sign o f less acculturation to American society).
difference in health outcomes in an individual
In contrast, those who spoke primarily English
who exercises for 1 5 minutes per day versus
For example, what
would
be
the
had physical activity rates that were similar to
an individual who engages in 6 0 minutes o f
those o f the general (majority) population.
physical activity per day? Whether or not such
Thus, it may be useful to tailor physical activ-
a difference in activity duration translates into
ity promotion in such a way as to target less
significantly
acculturated individuals (e.g., by increasing the
unknown. In addition, although the focus has
different
health
outcomes is
162
BEHAVIORS THAT C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH been on cardiorespiratory fitness, other possible
published, further examination of the factors
types o f fitness should be considered
that may increase physician prescription of
studied more widely. These might metabolic, flexibility, cognitive
and
include
physical activity is needed.
functioning,
Finally, more research is needed on effec-
and mental health. As other types o f fitness
tive strategies for promoting physical activity
become better defined, it may be possible to
in adults. As illustrated in the preceding case
tailor physical activity to an
study, cognitive-behavioral strategies such as
individual's
needs. For example, an individual might be
self-monitoring and goal setting can be very
prescribed a specific type o f physical activity
effective
to address his or her particular risk factors.
physical activity behaviors. In
Another
important
area
of
continued
in helping individuals
medicine
settings
with
to
change
behavioral
chronic
disease
research should focus on the maintenance o f
patients, health psychologists work with exer-
physical activity. W h y is it so difficult to main-
cise professionals and other experts who pre-
tain this behavior? W h y is engaging
in
physical activity pleasurable for some individ-
scribe the
appropriate
activity regimens.
Psychologists are often responsible for assist-
uals but not for others? One step in answering
ing patients in developing motivational strate-
the question o f why some individuals are more
gies and self-management programs to build
likely to maintain a program o f physical activ-
physical activity into their lifestyles and sus-
ity might be to examine emotions related to
tain the change over time. Psychologists may
exercise. Although much has been written o f
also assist with assessing mood and cognitive
the emotions that occur as a result o f physical
states that could affect ability to carry out pre-
activity participation, one aspect o f the rela-
scribed physical activity programs.
tionship between physical activity and emo-
Recent reviews (e.g., Blair & Morrow, 1 9 9 8 ;
tion that has received little attention is the
Dubbert, 2 0 0 2 ; Sallis &
importance o f the emotional changes
Department of Health and Human Services,
that
Owen, 1 9 9 8 ; U.S.
occur during physical activity (Rejeski, 1 9 9 4 ) .
1996) have described successful physical activity
Researchers should also continue to investi-
promotion projects and intervention strategies
gate the role of medical professionals in physical
in a variety o f populations
and
settings.
activity participation. In a recent telephone sur-
Participation in physical activity is a powerful
vey of nearly 2 , 0 0 0 U.S. adults, only 2 8 % of the
tool in the prevention of many of the diseases
respondents reported receiving advice from their
covered in this chapter. By increasing the
physicians
to
increase
physical
activity
numbers of individuals who regularly engage in
(Glasgow, Eakin, Fisher, Bacak, & Brownson,
physical activity, we could significantly reduce
2 0 0 1 ) . Although there has been increased
the chronic disease burden and improve the
interest in this area and several studies have been
quality o f life of millions of Americans.
REFERENCES American Association of Cardiovascular and Pulmonary Rehabilitation. (1999). Guidelines for cardiac rehabilitation and secondary prevention programs (3rd ed.). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics Press. American College of Sports Medicine. (1998). The recommended quantity and quality of exercise for developing and maintaining cardiorespiratory and muscular fitness and flexibility in healthy adults. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 30, 9 7 5 - 9 9 1 .
Physical Babyak, M., Blumenthal, J . Α., Herman, S., Khatri, P., Doraiswamy, M., Moore, K., Craighead, W. E., ÔC Baldewicz, T. T. (2000). Exercise treatment for major depression: Maintenance of therapeutic benefit at 10 months. Psychosomatic Medicine, 62, 6 3 3 - 6 3 8 . Bassett, D. R. (2000). Validity and reliability issues in objective monitoring of physical activity. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 71(2), 3 0 - 3 6 . Berlin, J . Α., & Colditz, G. A. (1990). A meta-analysis of physical activity in the prevention of coronary heart disease. American Journal of Epidemiology, 132, 612-628. Bernstein, M., Sloutskis, D., Kumanyika, S., Sparti, Α., Schultz, Y . , & Morbiana, A. (1998). Data-based approach for developing a physical activity frequency questionnaire. American Journal of Epidemiology, 147, 1 4 7 - 1 5 4 . Berry, M . J . , Si Walschlager, S. A. (1998). Exercise training and chronic obstructive pulmonary disease: Past and future research directions. Journal of Cardiopulmonary Rehabilitation, 18, 1 8 1 - 1 9 1 . Blair, S. N., Cheng, Y . , &c Holder, J . S. (2001). Is physical activity or physical fitness more important in defining health benefits? Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 33, S 3 7 9 - S 3 9 9 . Blair, S. N., Kohl, H. W., Paffenbarger, R. S., Clark, D. G., Cooper, Κ. H., & Gibbons, L. W. (1989). Physical fitness and all-cause mortality. Journal of the American Medical Association, 262, 2 3 9 5 - 2 4 0 1 . Blair, S. N., & Morrow, J . R. (1998). Cooper Institute/American College of Sports Medicine 1 9 9 7 Physical Activity Conference. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 15, 2 5 5 - 2 5 6 . Blumenthal, J . Α., Babyak, Μ . Α., Moore, Κ. Α., Craighead, W. E., Herman, S., Khatri, P., Waugh, R., Napolitano, Μ . Α., Forman, L. M., Appelbaum, M., Doraiswamy, M., & Krishnan, R. (1999). Effects of exercise training on older patients with major depression. Archives of Internal Medicine, 159, 2349-2356. Blumenthal, J . Α., Sherwood, Α., Gullette, E. C. D., Georgiades, Α., & Tweedy, D. (2002). Biobehavioral approaches to the treatment of essential hypertension. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 5 6 9 - 5 8 9 . Blumenthal, J . Α., Siegal, W. C , & Appelbaum, M. (1991). Failure of exercise to reduce blood pressure in patients with mild hypertension. Journal of the American Medical Association, 266, 2 0 9 8 - 2 1 0 4 . Bock, B . C., Marcus, Β . H., Pinto, Β. M., & Forsyth, L. H. (2001). Maintenance of physical activity following an individualized motivationally tailored intervention. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 23, 7 9 - 8 7 . Bouchard, C. (2000). Introduction. In C. Bouchard (Ed.), Physical activity and obesity. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics Press. Bourjeily, G., & Rochester, C. L. (2000). Exercise training in chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. Clinics in Chest Medicine, 21, 7 6 3 - 7 8 0 . Burstein, R., Epstein, Y . , Shapiro, Y . , Charuzi, I., & Karnieli, E. (1990). Effect of an acute bout of exercise on glucose disposal in human obesity. Journal of Applied Physiology, 69, 2 9 9 - 3 0 4 . Cameron, O. G., & Hudson, C. J . (1986). Influence of exercise on anxiety level in patients with anxiety disorders. Psychosomatics, 27, 7 2 0 - 7 2 3 . Caspersen, C. J . (1989). Physical activity epidemiology: Concepts, methods, and applications to exercise science. In K. Pandolf (Ed.), Exercise and sports science reviews (Vol. 17, pp. 4 2 3 - 4 7 3 ) . Baltimore, M D : Williams & Wilkins. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2001). Physical activity trends: United States, 1 9 9 0 - 1 9 9 8 . Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 50, 1 6 6 - 1 6 9 . Cheng, Y . , Macéra, C. Α., Davis, D. R., Ainsworth, Β . E., Troped, P. J . , & Blair, S. N. (2000). Physical activity and self-reported, physician-diagnosed
Inactivity
164
BEHAVIORS THAT C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH of Clinical osteoarthritis: Is physical activity a risk factor? Journal 53, 3 1 5 - 3 2 2 . Epidemiology, Colditz, G. Α., Cannuscio, C. C , Sc Frazier, A. L. (1997). Physical activity and reduced risk of colon cancer: Implications for prevention. Cancer Causes Control, 8, 6 4 9 - 6 6 7 . Collins, R., Peto, R., MacMahon, S., Hébert, P., Fiebach, Ν. H., Eberlein, Κ. Α., Godwin, J . , Qizilbash, N., Taylor, J . O., 8c Hennekens, C. H. (1990). Blood pressure, stroke, and coronary heart disease:, II. Short term reductions in blood pressure: Overview of randomised drug trials in their epidemiological context. Lancet, 335, 8 2 7 - 8 3 8 . Cooper, C , Inskip, H., Croft, P., Campbell, L., Smith, G., McLaren, M., 8c Coggon, D. (1998). Individual risk factors for hip osteoarthritis: Obesity, hip injury, and physical activity. American Journal of Epidemiology, 147, 5 1 6 - 5 2 2 . Cooper, C , McAlindon, T., Coggon, D., Egger, P., 8c Dieppe, P. (1994). Occupational activity and osteoarthritis of the knee. Annals of Rheumatic Disease, 53(2), 9 0 - 9 3 . Crespo, C. J . , Smit, E., Carter-Pokras, O., 8c Andersen, R. (2001). Acculturation and leisure-time physical inactivity in Mexican American adults: Results from NHANES III, 1 9 8 8 - 1 9 9 4 . American Journal of Public Health, 91, 1254-1257. Dubbert, P. M. (2002). Physical activity and exercise: Recent advances and current challenges. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 5 2 6 - 5 3 6 . Dubbert, P. M., Carithers, T., Sumner, A. E., Barbour, K., Clark, B., Hall, J . E., 8c Crook, E. (2002). Obesity, physical inactivity, and risk for cardiovascular disease. American Journal of Medical Science, 324, 1 1 6 - 1 2 6 . Dubbert, P. M., Rappaport, Ν. B., Sc Martin, J . E. (1987). Exercise in cardiovascular disease. Behavior Modification, 11, 3 2 9 - 3 4 7 . Duckworth, W. C , Solomon, S. S., Jallepalli, P., Heckemeyer, C , Finnern J . , & Powers, A. (1980). Glucose intolerance due to insulin resistance in patients with spinal cord injuries. Diabetes, 29, 9 0 6 - 9 1 0 . Emery, C. F., Schein, R. L., Hauck, E. R., & Maclntyre, N. R. (1998). Psychological and cognitive outcomes of a randomized trial of exercise among patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. Health Psychology, 17, 2 3 2 - 2 4 0 . Ettinger, W. H., 8c Afable, R. F. (1994). Physical disability from knee osteoarthritis: The role of exercise as an intervention. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 26, 1 4 3 5 - 1 4 4 0 . Ettinger, W. H., Jr., Burns, R., Messier, S. P., Applegate, W., Rejeski, W. J . , Sc Morgan, T. (1997). A randomized trial comparing aerobic exercise and resistance exercise with a health education program in older adults with knee osteoarthritis: The Fitness Arthritis and Seniors Trial (FAST). Journal of the American Medical Association, 277, 2 5 - 3 1 . Farrell, S., Kampert, J . B., Kohl, H. W., Barlow, C. E., Macéra, C. Α., Paffenbarger, R. S., Gibbons, L. W., Sc Blair, S. N. (1998). Influences of cardiorespiratory fitness levels and other predictors on cardiovascular disease mortality in men. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 30, 8 9 9 - 9 0 5 . Freedson, P. S., Sc Miller, Κ. M. (2000). Objective monitoring of physical activity using motion sensors and heart rate. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 71(2), 2 1 - 2 9 . Friedenreich, C. M . (2001). Physical activity and cancer prevention: From observational to interventional research. Cancer, Epidemiology, Biomarkers, & Prevention, 10, 2 8 7 - 3 0 1 . Friedenreich, C. M., Thune, L, Brinton, L. Α., &c Albanes, D. (1998). Epidemiologic issues related to the association between physical activity and breast cancer. Cancer, 83, 6 0 0 - 6 1 0 .
Physical Gammon, M . D., John, E. M., & Britton, J . A. (1998). Recreational and occupational physical activities and risk of breast cancer. Journal of the National Cancer Institute, 90, 1 0 0 - 1 1 7 . Glasgow, R. E., Eakin, E. G., Fisher, E. B., Bacak, S. J . , & Brownson, R. C. (2001). Physician advice and support for physical activity. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 21, 1 8 9 - 1 9 6 . Grant, I., Prigatano, G. P., Heaton, R. K., McSweeney, A. J . , Wright, E. C., & Adams, Κ. M . (1987). Progressive neuropsychologic impairment and hypoxemia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 44, 9 9 9 - 1 0 0 6 . Grundy, S. M., Blackburn, G., Higgins, M., Lauer, R., Perri, M . G., & Ryan, D. (1999). Physical activity in the prevention and treatment of obesity and its comorbidities. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 31, S 5 0 2 - S 5 0 8 . Hagberg, J . M., Park, J . , & Brown, M . D. (2000). The role of exercise training in the treatment of hypertension. Sports Medicine, 30, 1 9 3 - 2 0 6 . Halbert, J . Α., Silagy, C. Α., Finucane, P., Withers, R. T., Hamdorf, P. Α., & Andrews, G. R. (1997). The effectiveness of exercise training in lowering blood pressure: A meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials of 4 weeks or longer. Journal of Human Hypertension, 11, 6 4 1 - 6 4 9 . Hall, A. C., Urban, J . P. G., & Gehl, K. A. (1991). The effects of hydrostatic pressure on matrix synthesis in articular cartilage. Journal of Orthopedic Research, 9, 1-10. Hart, D. J . , Doyle, D. V., & Spector, T. D. (1995). Association between metabolic factors and knee osteoarthritis in women. Journal of Rheumatology, 22, 1118-1123. Hubinger, Α., Franzen, Α., & Gries, F. A. (1987). Hormonal and metabolic response to physical exercise in hyperinsulinemic and non-hyperinsulinemic Type 2 diabetics. Diabetes Research, 4(2), 5 7 - 6 1 . Jebb, S. Α., & Moore, M. S. (1999). Contribution of a sedentary lifestyle and inactivity to the etiology of overweight and obesity: Current evidence and research issues. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 31, S 5 3 4 - S 5 4 1 . Jeffery, R. W., Drewnowski, Α., Epstein, L. H., Stunkard, A. J . , Wilson, G. T., Wing, R. R., & Hill, D. R. (2000). Long-term maintenance of weight loss: Current status. Health Psychology, 19, 5 - 1 6 . Joint National Committee on Detection, Evaluation, and Treatment of High Blood Pressure. (1997). The sixth report of the Joint National Committee on Detection, Evaluation, and Treatment of High Blood Pressure (JNC-VI). Archives of Internal Medicine, 157, 2 4 1 3 - 2 4 4 6 . Kaplan, N. W. (1998). Clinical hypertension (7th ed.). Baltimore, M D : Williams & Wilkins. Kelley, G. Α., & Kelley, K. S. (2000). Progressive resistance exercise and resting blood pressure: A meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Hypertension, 35, 8 3 8 - 8 4 3 . King, A. C , Castro, C , Wilcox, S., Eyler, Α. Α., Sallis, J . F., & Brownson, R. (2000). Personal and environmental factors associated with physical inactivity among different racial-ethnic groups of U.S. middle-aged and older-aged women. Health Psychology, 19, 3 5 4 - 3 6 4 . Kokkinos, P. F., Narayan, P., Coleran, J . Α., Pittaras, M., Notargiacomo, Α., Reda, D., & Papademetriou, V. (1995). Effects of regular exercise on blood pressure and left ventricular hypertrophy in African-American men with severe hypertension. New England Journal of Medicine, 333, 1462-1467'. Kriska, A. M., Blair, S. M., & Pereira, M. A. (1994). The potential role of physical activity in the prevention of non-insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus: The epidemiological evidence. Exercise and Sport Science Review, 22, 1 2 1 - 1 4 3 . Kriska, A. M., & Caspersen, C. J . (Eds.). (1997). A collection of physical activity questionnaires for health related research. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 29, S 1 - S 2 0 5 .
Inactivity
BEHAVIORS THAT C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH Laçasse, Y . , Wong, E., Guyatt, G. H., King, D., Cook, D. J . , & Goldstein, R. S. (1996). Meta-analysis of respiratory rehabilitation in chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. Lancet, 348, 1 1 1 5 - 1 1 1 9 . Lattika, P., Pukkala, E., &c Vihko, V. (1998). Relationship between the risk of breast cancer and physical activity. Sports Medicine, 26, 1 3 3 - 1 4 3 . Leon, A. S. (2000). Exercise following myocardial infarction. Sports Medicine, 29, 301-311. Lesniak, K. T., & Dubbert, P. M . (2001). Exercise and hypertension. Current Opinion in Cardiology, 16, 3 5 6 - 3 5 9 . Linden, W., & Chambers, L. (1994). Clinical effectiveness of non-drug treatment for hypertension: A meta-analysis. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 16, 3 5 - 4 5 . Lipman, R. L., Raskin, P., Love, T., Triebwasser, J . , Lecocq, F. R., & Schnure, J . J . (1972). Glucose intolerance during decreased physical activity in man. Diabetes, 21(2), 1 0 1 - 1 0 7 . MacMahon, S., Peto, R., Cutler, J . , Collins, R., Sorlie, P., Neaton, J . , Abbott, R., Godwin, J . , Dyer, Α., & Stamler, J . (1990). Blood pressure, stroke, and coronary heart disease: I. Prolonged differences in blood pressure: Prospective observational studies corrected for the regression dilution bias. Lancet, 335, 765-774. Mancia, G., Sega, R., Milesi, C , Cesana, G., & Zanchetti, A. (1997). Blood-pressure control in the hypertensive population. Lancet, 349, 4 5 4 - 4 5 7 . Marcus, Β. H., Dubbert, P. M., Forsyth, L. H., McKenzie, T. L., Stone, E. J . , Dunn, A. L., & Blair, S. N. (2000). Physical activity behavior change: Issues in adoption and maintenance. Health Psychology, 19, 3 2 - 4 1 . Meenan, R. F., Callahan, L. F., & Helmick, C. G. (1999). The National Arthritis Action Plan: A public health strategy for a looming epidemic. Arthritis Care Research, 12, 7 9 - 8 1 . Minor, M . A. (1991). Physical activity and management of arthritis. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 13, 1 1 7 - 1 2 4 . Montoye, H. J . , Kemper, H. C. G., Saris, W. Η. M., & Washburn, R. A. (1996). Measuring physical activity and energy expenditure. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics Press. Nami, R., Mondillo, S., Agricola, E., Lenti, S., Ferro, G., Nami, N., Tarantino, M., Glauco, G., Spano, E., & Gennari, C. (2000). Aerobic exercise training fails to reduce blood pressure in non-dipper-type hypertension. American journal of Hypertension, 13, 5 9 3 - 6 0 0 . National Institutes of Health. (1998). Clinical guidelines on the identification, evaluation, and treatment of overweight and obesity in adults: The evidence report. Obesity Research, 6, S 5 1 - S 2 0 9 . Oldridge, Ν. B . , Guyatt, G. H., Fischer, M . E., & Rimm, A. A. (1988). Cardiac rehabilitation after myocardial infarction: Combined experience of randomised clinical trials, journal of the American Medical Association, 260, 9 4 5 - 9 5 0 . Oliveria, S. Α., & Christos, P. J . (1997). The epidemiology of physical activity and cancer. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 833, 7 9 - 9 0 . Peirce, N. S. (1999). Diabetes and exercise. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 33, 161-172. Perri, M. G., Nezu, A. M., McKelvey, W. F., Shermer, R. L., Renjilian, D. Α., & Viegener, B . J . (2001). Relapse prevention training and problem-solving therapy in the long-term management of obesity. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69, 7 2 2 - 7 2 6 . Prigatano, G. P., Parsons, O., Wright, E., Levin, D. C , & Hawryluk, G. (1983). Neuropsychological test performance in mildly hypoxemic patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 1 0 8 - 1 1 6 .
Physical Rejeski, W. J . (1994). Dose-response issues from a psychosocial perspective. In C. Bouchard, R. T. Shephard, & T. Stephens (Eds.), Physical activity, fitness, and health (pp. 1 0 4 0 - 1 0 5 5 ) . Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics Press. Rejeski, W. J . , Gauvin, L., Hobson, M. L., & Norris, J . L. (1995). Effects of baseline responses, in-task feelings, and duration of activity on exercise-induced feeling states in women. Health Psychology, 14, 3 5 0 - 3 5 9 . Salbe, A. D., & Ravussin, E. (2000). The determinants of obesity. In C. Bouchard (Ed.), Physical activity and obesity (pp. 6 9 - 1 0 2 ) . Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics Press. Sallis, J . F. (1991). Self-report measures of children's physical activity. Journal of School Health, 61, 2 1 5 - 2 1 9 . Sallis, J . F., & Owen, N. (1998). Physical activity and behavioral medicine. Thousand Oaks: CA: Sage. Sallis, J . F., 8c Saelens, Β . E. (2000). Assessment of physical activity by self-report: Status, limitations, and future directions. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 71(2), 1-14. Schoenborn, C. Α., & Barnes, P. M. (2002). Leisure-time physical activity among adults: United States, 1997-98 (advance data from Vital and Health Statistics, No. 3 2 5 ) . Hyattsville, M D : National Center for Health Statistics. Sutton, A. J . , Muir, K. R., Mockett, S., & Fentem, P. (2001). A case-control study to investigate the relation between low and moderate levels of physical activity and osteoarthritis of the knee using data collected as part of the Allied Dunbar National Fitness Survey. Annals of Rheumatic Disease, 60, 7 5 6 - 7 6 4 . U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (1996). Physical activity and health: Report of the surgeon general. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion. Wadden, Τ. Α., Foster, G. D., & Brownell, K. D. (2002). Obesity: Responding to the global epidemic. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 510-525. Wallberg-Henriksson, H. (1992). Interaction of exercise and insulin in type 2 diabetes mellitus. Diabetes Care, 15, 1 7 7 7 - 1 7 8 2 . Wallberg-Henriksson, H., Rincon, J . , & Zierath, J . R. (1998). Exercise in the management of non-insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus. Sports Medicine, 25(1), 25-35. Wang, G., Helmick, C. G., Macéra, C , Zhang, P., & Pratt, M. (2001). Inactivityassociated medical costs among U.S. adults with arthritis. Arthritis Care & Research, 45, 4 3 9 - 4 4 5 . Wannamethee, S. G., Shaper, A. G., & Walker, M . (2000). Physical activity and mortality in older men with diagnosed coronary heart disease. Circulation, 102, 1 3 5 8 - 1 3 6 3 . Washburn, R. A. (2000). Assessment of physical activity in older adults. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 71(2), 7 9 - 8 8 . Wei, M., Kampert, J . B . , Barlow, C. E., Nichaman, M . Z., Gibbons, L. W., Paffenbarger, R. S., Gibbons, L. W., & Blair, S. N. (1999). Relationship between low cardiorespiratory fitness and mortality in normal weight, overweight, and obese men. Journal of the American Medical Association, 282, 1547-1553. White, J . Α., Wright, V., & Hudson, A. M. (1993). Relationship between habitual physical activity and osteoarthritis in aging women. Public Health, 107, 459-470. Wing, R. R. (1999). Physical activity in the treatment of the adulthood overweight and obesity: Current evidence and research issues. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 31, S 5 4 7 - S 5 5 2 .
Inactivity
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH Wood, T. M. (2000). Issues and future directions in assessing physical activity: An introduction to the conference proceedings. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 71(2), ii-vii. World Health Organization. (1998). Obesity: Preventing and managing the world epidemic. Geneva: Author. Yelin, E. (1992). The cumulative impact of a common chronic condition. Arthritis Rheum, 33, 7 5 0 - 7 5 5 .
CHAPTER
10
Stress and Health FRANK L . COLLINS, J R . , KRISTEN H . SOROCCO, KIMBERLY R . HAALA, BRIAN I. MILLER, AND WILLIAM R . LOVALLO
A
lthough stress may not cause disease, it has been established that stress m a y be a risk factor for the develop-
ment o f disease, may aggravate an existing disease, and may reduce a compliance and treatment success. As a result, many health professionals frequently refer clients to clinical health psychologists for help in dealing with stress-related issues. Clients referred in this manner frequently have a basic understanding that emotional and psychological factors influence health (often seen as the mind influencing the body); however, they frequently do n o t understand the mechanisms involved in this process. Likewise, clinical
health
psychologists
may
have
an
in-depth understanding o f effective treatment methods for reducing stress while having only an elementary understanding o f the physiological foundations that can help to guide a m o r e sophisticated conceptualization
W H A T E X A C T L Y IS S T R E S S ? T o understand the complexities of the construct we call stress, it is important to have a basic understanding o f how the body is organized. T h e body is a complex machine that functions in a hierarchical manner. At the most basic level, individual organs and tissues have selfregulating properties that allow the body to maintain normal function (homeostasis) when external conditions are constant. However, individual organs and tissues are not able to respond to rapid changes (challenges) in the environment or coordinate their responses with other bodily systems. Therefore, higher levels in the nervous system modulate the self-regulation of tissues and organs when homeostasis is threatened. Modulation in self-regulatory functioning is achieved through the receipt o f sensory inputs, integrated control over target tissues, endocrine outflow, and autonomic function. This process is discussed in further detail later in this chapter.
and treatment for these disorders. T h e major
In its simplest form, stress involves a stres-
goals o f this chapter are to (a) provide health
sor and stress response that challenge the
professionals
with
body's ability to maintain homeostasis. A
clients, (b) review the literature on models o f
with
a model t o use
stressor is any physical or mental challenge to
coping, and (c) provide information assess-
the body that threatens homeostasis. Physical
ment and treatment strategies.
stressors are events that challenge the body to
170
BEHAVIORS THAT C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH function beyond normal capacity ( M c E w e n , 2 0 0 0 ) . Examples o f physical stressors include
Physiology of the Psychological Stress Response
bodily injury, physical exertion, noise, overcrowding, and excessive heat or cold. Physical stressors
are
generated
through
internal
After any external event occurs, sensory information related to the event is processed
mechanisms and are a bottom-up process.
within the corticolimbic system (Figure 1 0 . 1 ) .
Alternatively, psychological stressors are top-
In general, the corticolimbic system is respon-
down processes that challenge an individual's
sible for threat appraisals and the processing
mental
capacity.
Psychological
stressors
of emotions. T h e corticolimbic system is
time-pressured
composed o f multiple brain structures, includ-
tasks, speech tasks, mental arithmetic, inter-
ing the thalamus, sensory cortex, prefrontal
personal
isolation,
cortex, memory system, and amygdala. As an
and traumatic life events. Therefore, both a
individual experiences an external event in the
include challenges such as
conflict, overcrowding,
physical stressor, such as being trapped out-
environment, sensory information is relayed
side in below-freezing temperatures, and a
from the thalamus to the sensory cortex. From
psychological stressor, such as participating in
the sensory cortex, the information is relayed
a public speaking task, can challenge the
to the prefrontal cortex, which is responsible
homeostasis o f the body.
for decision making and planning. Interest-
A stress response may consist of both a
ingly, the prefrontal
cortex is tied to the
physiological
person's memory system, so incoming infor-
response. A behavioral response is any action
mation is evaluated based on his or her prior
taken on the environment, such as quickly
memories. T h e appraisal process is completed
leaving a dangerous situation or implementing
in the prefrontal cortex, which communicates
a coping skill, whereas a physiological response
with the amygdala, the region in the brain
is an alteration in physiological functioning
where emotions are processed.
behavioral
response
and
that serves to restore an imbalance in homeo-
O n e o f the main functions o f the cortico-
static functioning. Examples o f physiological
limbic system during the stress response is the
responses include an increase in blood pres-
appraisal o f potential threats in the environ-
sure, elevated heart rate, impaired memory and
ment. Lazarus and Folkman's ( 1 9 8 4 ) model o f
decision-making abilities, and altered metabo-
coping distinguishes between two types of
lism.
Both
behavioral
and
physiological
appraisals that occur during a stress response.
responses to a stressor may be associated with
A primary appraisal is responsible for deter-
a negative affective state for the individual.
mining the magnitude of the threat such as
M o s t individuals can identify stressors they
immediate danger. If a threat appraisal is
experience in their daily lives as well as how
made, secondary appraisals help an individual
they experience the negative effects o f stress.
to evaluate ways in which to cope with the
However, what goes on between the stressor
stressor. For example, if a threat
and the stress response in terms o f physiology
indicates immediate danger, the corticolimbic
is less clearly understood by clients with
system immediately activates the peripheral
w h o m health professionals come into contact.
nervous system to signal the body to engage in
By educating clients on the psychological and
behaviors that initiate movement.
physiological mechanisms o f stress, health
After primary and secondary been made
appraisal
appraisals
professionals can teach clients the adaptive
have
indicating a threat,
role o f the stress response to episodic stressors
corticolimbic system also sends a message to
the
and the detrimental physical and psychologi-
the hypothalamus in addition to the message
cal effects caused by chronic long-term stress.
that was sent to the peripheral nervous system
Stress and
Health
Cortico-Limbic Threat Appraisals
External Event
Hypothalamus
Brain Stem
Behaviors
ANS
Endocrine
r
ι
Target Tissues
Figure 10.1
Physiology of the Psychological Stress Response
NOTE: ANS = autonomic nervous system.
signaling the body to move. T h e hypothalamus
system in the stress response is to prepare the
is responsible for coordinating the nervous
body to respond to the stressor.
system and controls the autonomic nervous
The hypothalamus, as a coordinator between
system via the brain stem. T h e autonomic
systems, also communicates with the endocrine
nervous system is divided into three main
system during a stress response. There are two
branches: the sympathetic nervous system, the
main endocrine functions
parasympathetic
the
response: the adrenocortical response and the
entric nervous system. For the purposes o f this
adrenomedullary response. The adrenocortical
nervous
system, and
during a stress
chapter, only the sympathetic and parasympa-
response releases Cortisol into the body during a
thetic nervous systems are discussed. Both the
stress response. Cortisol is released through mul-
sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous sys-
tiple steps. First, the paraventricular nucleus of
tems are involved in maintaining homeostasis.
the
hypothalamus
releases
corticotropin-
The sympathetic nervous system is usually
releasing factor (CRF), which travels to the
responsible for increasing the activation of
pituitary gland. C R F causes the secretion of
bodily systems (flight-fight response), whereas
adrenocorticotropin in the anterior pituitary
the parasympathetic nervous system is respon-
gland and the secretion of Cortisol in the adrenal
sible for feeding, energy storage, and repro-
cortex. Cortisol is then released from
duction. T h e brain stem itself can initiate
adrenal cortex. Cortisol as a hormone is always
changes in the autonomic nervous system in
present in the body, indicating its importance in
single organs and tissues but is not efficient
normal functioning, but the levels vary due to
at coordinating across organs or between
both time of day (diurnal pattern) and current
systems. T h e role o f the autonomic nervous
stressors. Cortisol is responsible for increasing
the
BEHAVIORS THAT C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH sympathetic nervous system function, releasing
response, which cannot be easily eliminated by
stored glucose and fats for energy, and suppress-
behavioral and psychological mechanisms. If we reexamine the physiology of the stress
ing immune function. Another stress hormone that is important
response, some o f the negative consequences
also
of chronic stress become evident. T o begin
adrenomedullary
with, Cortisol and epinephrine, the hormones
response releases epinephrine into general cir-
released during the stress response, have detri-
culation in response to the increase in sympa-
mental effects when they are released continu-
to the stress response is epinephrine, known as adrenaline. T h e
thetic nervous system activity caused by the
ously. For example, one o f the roles o f Cortisol
release of Cortisol. Epinephrine increases rela-
is to suppress the immune function, which
tive to the stress response and assists in coor-
episodically is fine but which over a long
dinating
both
period of time leaves an individual susceptible
behavioral and metabolic changes necessary
to illness (Lovallo, 1 9 9 7 ) . Chronic stress also
bodily
systems
to
make
to deal with the stressor. M o r e specifically,
increases the amount o f time it takes for a
epinephrine stimulates heart muscles, increas-
wound to heal. For example, Kiecolt-Glaser,
ing heart rate and oxygen levels to prepare the
Marucha, Malarkey, M e r c a d o , and Glaser ( 1 9 9 5 ) examined the length of time it took for
body to respond to the stressor. After the first three components o f a stress
punch biopsy wounds to heal in individuals
response occur, regulation of the autonomic
caring for a relative with Alzheimer's disease
nervous system and endocrine system is main-
and found that wounds took significantly
tained by negative feedback. Information on
longer to heal in caregivers under chronic
Cortisol and epinephrine output is sent from
stress in comparison with control participants.
tissues and organs to the brain stem, hypotha-
The continuous release of Cortisol also might
lamus, and corticolimbic system, where the
be related to the sleep problems commonly
decision to continue production o f Cortisol
reported
among
individuals
experiencing
and epinephrine is made. If production o f
chronic stressors. As was mentioned earlier,
these hormones is no longer needed, messages
Cortisol is released diurnally two times per
are sent to the pituitary and adrenal glands to
day: once during the morning and then again
discontinue output.
after lunch. Researchers have found that the continuous release o f Cortisol decreases rapid eye movement ( R E M ) sleep, which is essential
Negative Physiological Impacts of Chronic Stress
to normal sleep cycles (Vgontzas, Bixler, & Kales, 2 0 0 0 ) .
Walter Cannon, a physiologist who first introduced
the term
stress,
distinguished
In addition, the release o f epinephrine during the stress response can put
individuals
between short-term and long-term stressors
experiencing chronic stress at risk for cardio-
(Carlson, 1 9 9 9 ) . H e discussed the fact that
vascular disease (Pollard, 2 0 0 0 ) . Epinephrine
physiologically we were built to deal with
stimulates cardiac muscles, resulting in an
episodic stressors rather than chronic stressors.
increased heart rate and potentially hyperten-
The stress response is designed to deal effi-
sion. Epinephrine also is involved in increasing
ciently with episodic stressors; however, there
blood platelet adhesiveness and in reducing
are negative physiological and psychological
clotting time, both of which are risk factors for
consequences when an individual is under
myocardial infarctions and
chronic stress. T h e negative physiological con-
accidents (Markovitz &c Matthews, 1 9 9 1 ) .
sequences resulting from chronic stress are due to the prolonged
activation o f the
stress
cerebrovascular
Chronic stressors also can negatively affect individuals
psychologically.
As
discussed
Stress and
Health
previously, the second stage in the stress
and how these models are translated
response after experiencing a stressor is the
treatment options.
into
appraisal process. During the appraisal process, emotions are generated, and emotions in turn influence an individual's mood. Chronic stress
Stimulus Models
can lead to a negative mood state such as depres-
Stimulus models focus on environmental
sion or anxiety. Negative mood states can influ-
events that produce demand on the organism.
ence how a person appraises situations by
Early research in this area (cf. Brown
serving as a filter through which he or she inter-
Harris, 1 9 7 8 ; Holmes & R a h e , 1 9 6 7 ) led to
prets information from the environment. H o w
a focus on negative life events as a major
&
the person appraises environmental events influ-
source o f stress. Intervention efforts focused
ences the duration of the stress response because
on learning to deal more effectively with these
it can be considered the highest level o f control
demands (coping) or on learning to predict
over homeostatic functioning (Lovallo, 1 9 9 7 ) .
and (where appropriate) minimize exposure
Although the human body is designed to
to negative life events. Cassidy ( 1 9 9 9 ) argued
deal with episodic stressors, chronic stress can
that these models primarily served a classifi-
negatively affect both the physiological and
cation role, providing researchers and clini-
psychological functioning o f an
individual.
cians with necessary tools for identification o f
However, individual differences resulting from
individuals experiencing stress. Intervention,
both genetics and life experiences influence
particularly stress management strategies, has
how individuals respond to stressors. In fact,
moved beyond stimulus models.
two individuals who experience the same traumatic event might react to the stressor completely differently depending on each
Response Models
individual's access to coping resources. By
Stress is frequently identified by the pres-
studying how individuals respond to stress,
ence o f specific symptoms that include overt
researchers have been able to develop coping
behavioral,
models for stress and to identify effective treat-
symptoms. These symptoms are the natural
p h y s i o l o g i c a l , and
cognitive
ment interventions designed to help individuals
consequences o f demand, and although one
develop coping skills.
cannot separate stress responses from stressors, focus on response models has played a major role in the development o f treatment strategies typically labeled "stress manage-
MODELS O F STRESS
m e n t " (cf. Lehrer &c W o o l f o l k , 1 9 9 3 ) . As noted previously, at the most basic level,
These models use a bottom-up explanation
stress involves two processes: an environmen-
rather
tal
physiological
appraisal models. M u c h o f the theory behind
response (stress response). Over the years,
these models comes out o f biofeedback and
models of stress have differentially focused on
relaxation research as well as from Eastern
these two components, with most contempo-
philosophy. T h e basic premise o f these models
event
(stressor)
and
a
than a top-down basis as in
the
rary models focusing on stress as a transactional
is that stress can be influenced by a person's
process (Cassidy, 1 9 9 9 ) . A comprehensive
level o f autonomic arousal. In other words, if
review o f the strengths and weaknesses of each
a person is experiencing high levels of arousal,
model is beyond the scope o f this chapter;
the person will feel as though he or she is
however, clinical health psychologists need to
under more stress. I f the arousal is reduced,
have familiarity with these major approaches
the person will subjectively experience lower
174
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH amounts o f stress. This may occur even if the
event can be perceived as stressful or benign.
demands on the person have not been reduced
Stress appraisals, whether positive or negative,
or changed.
lead to autonomic arousal.
In relation t o this, it is believed that the
Lazarus proposed
three types o f stress
perception o f controllability o f arousal may
appraisals: harm/loss, threat, and challenge.
also influence stress levels. People w h o expe-
Harm/loss appraisals occur in cases where
rience high levels o f arousal and do not feel
damage has already occurred. For instance, a
as though they can control that arousal may
harm/loss appraisal would occur when a per-
report more stress than do people w h o expe-
son has become ill or injured, lost a loved one,
rience the same levels o f arousal but feel as
or suffered a blow to his or her self-esteem.
though they have control over their arousal.
Threat appraisals occur when harm or loss can be anticipated but has not yet occurred. These are different from harm/loss appraisals
Appraisal Models
mainly in that threat appraisals allow the per-
Appraisal models view psychological stress
son to plan and adapt to minimize or alleviate
as a process whereby an individual interprets
some o f the harm before it occurs. T h e third
or views environmental events and perceived
type o f stress appraisal is challenge. Challenge
coping ability that in turn shapes his or her
appraisals are more positive in that the focus
emotional,
physiological
is on opportunity to achieve growth or to gain
responses to events. Appraisal theories suggest
something from the event. These categories
that the person "appraises" an event in an
are not necessarily exclusive. There may be,
behavioral,
and
attempt to discern its meaning in the context of
and often are, situations in which threat and
his or her experiences. These appraisals lead
challenge appraisals are experienced from the
to decisions regarding whether or not the
same event.
event is viewed as threatening or harmful and
Once primary appraisal determines that an
what actions would be appropriate under the
event is stressful, a person must choose what he
circumstances.
or she is going to do about the situation. This is
Appraisal models are a natural outgrowth
where secondary appraisals come in. Secondary
of stimulus and response models. Appraisal o f
appraisals
an event by definition requires that an event
options for coping with a stressful event and the
evaluate the person's
available
occur. Thus, it should be no surprise that many
possible consequences of those actions. These
events that would be labeled as stressors are in
appraisals take into account personal resources
fact appraised as high demand. Likewise, when
and the limitations of the environment.
a person appraises an event as high demand,
Appraisals rarely occur in this linear fash-
the natural consequence o f such an appraisal is
ion. Rather, primary and secondary appraisals
behaviors, emotions, and/or cognitions that fit
may occur almost simultaneously, and changes
the definition of a stress response. Lazarus's model
in the event, new information, and/or changes
(Lazarus, 1 9 6 6 , 1 9 9 9 ;
in the level o f perceived threat may alter a
Lazarus & Folkman, 1 9 8 4 ) is the most widely
person's appraisals. Lazarus referred to these
accepted stress model. As noted previously,
new appraisals as reappraisals. A person reap-
Lazarus divided the appraisal process into two
praises an event when there are changes that
distinct
parts:
primary
and
secondary.
Primary appraisals are intended to determine the threat value of an event. They take into
may affect the stressfulness of the event or his or her ability to cope with it. Coping responses are generated from the
consideration the familiarity of the event, the
appraisal process. According to Lazarus, cop-
beliefs o f the person, and commitments. An
ing refers to constantly changing cognitive and
Stress and Health
175
behavioral efforts to manage specific external
whereas
and/or internal demands that are appraised
troubling to patients. Stress can lead to emo-
others can be quite severe
and
as taxing or exceeding the resources of the
tional impairments
person. Coping can be divided into two types
crying, nervousness, edginess, anger, and irri-
of responses: problem focused and emotion
tability. In addition, patients experiencing
focused. Problem-focused responses operate
chronic stress may feel overwhelmed, power-
directly on the situation itself and may include
less, and isolated. They may report general feel-
behaviors
personal
ings o f unhappiness and dissatisfaction. They
resources, altering the event, and/or generat-
may report feeling that life has no meaning any-
ing alternative solutions. Emotion-focused
more. Some patients may appear cynical or
coping is directed at lessening the emotional
apathetic. They may report that they feel empty
aimed
at
increasing
such as
inappropriate
distress caused by the event. Little or no effort
or directionless, and they may have a strong
is devoted to changing the event that caused
urge to try to prove themselves.
the distress. This type o f coping includes
People experiencing chronic stress often
strategies such as avoidance, reappraising the
exhibit cognitive problems such as foggy think-
situation, distancing, and selective attention.
ing, distractibility, and forgetfulness. They often
As with the division between types of appraisals,
say that they cannot stop worrying. Some
the
problem-focused
patients report that they cannot seem to be cre-
coping and emotion-focused coping is some-
ative anymore or have no sense of humor.
what blurred. People often engage in a coping
Chronic stress can also lead to behavioral prob-
strategy that serves both purposes or use more
lems and trouble in relationships. Stressed
than one strategy at the same time to serve
people may be bossy and critical o f others and
both purposes.
themselves. They may take up alcoholism or
boundary
between
Problem-focused strategies can be costly
smoking, or they may begin to eat or chew gum
early on because o f the energy and time that
compulsively. Spouses or partners of stressed
they require. However, they can lead to alter-
individuals often report that their significant
ations in the event that lessen its stress value
others grind their teeth in their sleep, have a
and so lessen the amount o f coping that con-
lowered sex drive, and have "clammed up."
tinues to be needed. Emotion-focused strate-
Stressed people may also seem resentful or
gies are not as costly early on, but if the
intolerant of others and may isolate themselves.
situation
remains
stressful,
these
coping
In addition to these impairments in a wide
responses can lead to a continued drain on
range o f functioning, people experiencing
resources. O n c e a coping response has been
chronic stress may experience a number o f
used, the situation is reappraised taking into
physical
account changes a person has made to the sit-
report headaches, fatigue, restlessness, and
uation or his or her response to it. In this way,
sleep disturbances. Patients also report unex-
the appraisal process is circular and recurrent.
plained backaches, painful muscle tension,
symptoms.
Patients
commonly
and gastrointestinal discomfort such as stomachaches and indigestion. Patients may also
SYMPTOMS OF CHRONIC STRESS
have an increased heart rate and increased blood pressure.
Chronic exposure to stress can lead to a variety of impairments in many domains. People who are under chronic stress report a number of symptoms. Some o f these symptoms are vague and do not greatly affect their functioning,
Effects of Stress on Health As noted earlier, stress increases the risk of disease. This can occur via the
numerous
176
BEHAVIORS T H A T COMPROMISE HEALTH symptoms just described or through the effects
disorders such as irritable bowel syndrome,
of stress on the immune system. In addition,
Chron's disease, and general gastric discomfort.
stress can exacerbate already existing disease states or can impede recovery from an illness.
As listed previously, some
Sleep Disorders.
Although the effects of stress on health have
of the symptoms o f stress include nervousness
been widely studied, there has been little con-
and constant worrying. These symptoms, as
clusive evidence on the mechanism by which
well as other stress effects, can lead to inter-
these effects occur.
ruptions in sleep patterns. Stress often leads to insomnia and frequent nighttime waking. T h e immune function is
Sleep efficiency is usually lowered, and people
very complex, involving many different anti-
may report feeling tired and worn out even
bodies and activities. One of the acute effects
after receiving a full night's sleep.
Immune
Function.
of stress is to suppress immune functioning and thereby increase infection susceptibility. Stress leads to increased adrenaline secretion,
Anxiety
and Depression.
As can be seen
from the list o f symptoms earlier, stress often
which suppresses the production o f some anti-
leads to feelings o f anxiety and depression, and
bodies, decreases macrophage activity, and
this can become a problem in itself for some
decreases interleukin production.
Reduced
patients.
immune
person's
function
can increase a
chances o f viral and bacterial infection, which
Substance
Abuse.
Some substances have a
may lead to illnesses such as the c o m m o n cold
stress-relieving effect, and some patients may
and mononucleosis. F o r instance,
resort to substance abuse as a way o f coping
Cohen,
Tyrrell, and Smith ( 1 9 9 1 ) placed a cold virus
with their high stress levels.
in the nasal passages o f participants. They found that participants who had reported high levels of stress within the past year were much
ASSESSMENT OF STRESS AND
more likely to become infected than were
T R E A T M E N T O F STRESS
those who reported low levels o f stress.
T h e exclusion o f "stress disorders" from the Cardiovascular
Disease.
Stress may also
exacerbate or lead to cardiovascular disease.
D S M - I V (Diagnostic of Mental
Disorders,
and Statistical
Manual
fourth edition [American
and
Psychiatric Association, 1 9 9 4 ] ) as a distinct
serum cholesterol levels. In addition, some evi-
category has led to a wide variety o f methods
Chronic stress elevates blood pressure
dence suggests that chronic central nervous
used in the measurement o f stress. F r o m the
system hyperactivity reduces the body's sensi-
clinical health psychologist's perspective, the
tivity to insulin, and this also increases blood
most important criterion for diagnosing a
pressure.
given complaint as a clinical stress problem is the judgment that it is amenable to improve-
Gastrointestinal
Disorders.
Stress
also
ment by changing the way in which the per-
affects the gastrointestinal tract. It can increase
son
colonic contractions or can lead to spasms in
transactions with
perceives
or
manages
his
the immediate
or
her
environ-
the colon. There may also be increased muscle
ment. Therefore, the diagnosis o f a clinical
tension in the abdominal area as well as
stress problem has less to do with the etiol-
throughout the body. Stress may also increase
ogy or severity o f the problem itself than
pain sensitivity. Consequently, people
with the prediction o f its responsiveness to
who
experience chronic stress may be seen for
the teaching o f coping skills.
Stress and Health
Assessment
177
interview format called the Bedford College Interview for Life Events and Difficulties, which
O n e o f the primary ways in which the
has been found to be both reliable and poten-
physician assesses preliminary signs o f stress
tially more useful because it allows trained
is the patient's self-report via questionnaires
raters to evaluate contextual factors surround-
and/or a clinical interview. Self-report ques-
ing specific life events. For example, someone
tionnaires typically allow the patient to rate
who lost a parent following a prolonged illness
whether a symptom is present or absent as
that preceded the death may view the "loss of a
well as the severity o f the symptom. Clinical
parent" event quite differently from someone
interviews are often designed to allow the
who lost a parent in an unexpected accident.
physician to use a checklist format for quick and efficient diagnoses.
Symptoms o f stress can be measured using common
emotional
scales, including
the
T w o o f the more frequently used scales to
Spielberger anxiety (Spielberger, Gorsuch,
assess stressors are the Life Events Survey
Lushene, Vagg, & J a c o b s , 1 9 8 3 ) and anger
(Sarason, Johnson, & Siegel, 1 9 7 8 ) and the
(Spielberger, 1 9 9 6 ) scales (for a review, see
Daily Hassles and Uplifts Scale (Kanner,
Spielberger,
Coyne, Schaefer, & Lazarus, 1 9 8 1 ) . T h e Life
1 9 9 9 ) , the Beck inventories (Beck Depression
Events Survey consists o f 5 7 items, and the
Inventory-II [Beck, Steer, &c Brown, 1 9 9 6 ] and
client checks each event that has ever hap-
Beck Anxiety Inventory [Beck, 1 9 9 0 ] ) , and
pened to him or her. T h e n the client rates each
broad-based measures such as the Symptom
of these items positively or negatively in terms
Checklist 90-Revised (Derogatis, 1 9 7 5 ) .
Sydeman, O w e n ,
&
Marsh,
of desirability and impact at the time the event occurred. Each item is rated on a 7-point scale from - 3 to + 3 , with the sum indicating the
Treatment
client's life events experience. Sarason and col-
T h e treatment o f stress disorders often falls
leagues ( 1 9 7 8 ) noted that negative scores are
into one o f two types: treatments aimed at
the best predictor o f health problems.
reducing central nervous system activation
The Daily Hassles and Uplifts Scale was
and treatments designed to address problems
developed to address more minor events in a
in appraisal or coping skills. Treatment tech-
person's life. This 143-item survey has gener-
niques that involve the alteration o f appraisals
ated enormous numbers of research studies
often incorporate cognitive strategies to alter
focusing on the types o f stressors found for
and improve the
competitiveness-readiness
individuals with various disease states (e.g.,
level, whereas central nervous system activa-
VanHoudenhove et al., 2 0 0 2 ) as well as on
tion reduction techniques focus on lowering
differences in stressful experiences of particu-
or controlling physiological arousal, anxiety,
lar high-risk populations
(e.g., M c C a l l u m ,
and muscular tension. It should be noted that is typically used
Arnold, & Bolland, 2 0 0 2 ) . In addition, psycho-
the term stress management
metric research evaluating the questionnaire
to describe a number o f treatment techniques
indicates that for some populations, the order
designed to reduce stress rather than referring
in which items are presented influences ratings.
to any one specific method. Lehrer
Specifically, events are rated as less uplifting
Woolfolk provided detailed chapters on the
when they follow hassle items than when they
methods and approaches that fit this broad
precede hassle items (Mayberry et al., 2 0 0 2 ) .
and
category o f "stress management" (Lehrer &
Life events can also be measured as part of a
Woolfolk, 1 9 9 3 ; W o o l f o l k & Lehrer, 1 9 8 4 ) .
structured or unstructured interview. Brown
In the current chapter, several o f the most
and Harris ( 1 9 7 8 ) developed a
common
structured
methods
are
merely
outlined.
178
BEHAVIORS T H A T COMPROMISE HEALTH Training in these individual techniques should
between tension and relaxation. T o first
be done under close supervision as part o f a
become familiar with the sensation o f tension,
formal training program.
the learner is instructed to lie on his or her back with arms at the sides and to bend the
Treatments Nervous
That System
Focus
on
Central
Reduction
Relaxation methods encompass a range o f
wrist up at a 90-degree angle. T h e learner perceives tension in the forearm. N e x t the learner performs much o f the same task except that this time the wrist is bent at a 45-degree
techniques, each with unique properties; how-
angle, producing less tension. This method is
ever, all share the goal o f creating a relaxation
repeated again at increasingly smaller angles.
response. This response is designed to activate
Using this method of diminishing tensions
the parasympathetic nervous system, resulting
teaches the learner to detect not only high ten-
in a decrease in oxygen consumption, heart
sion but even the most minute tension.
rate, respiration, and skeletal muscle activity
Relaxation
begins with
the
instructor
along with an increase in skin resistance and
explaining the basic physiology o f neuromus-
alpha brain waves. Four factors have been
cular circuits and the nature o f tension and
identified as important for eliciting a relax-
relaxation. T h e learning environment can be
ation response: a mental device (a word, a
quite varied; classrooms, gymnasiums, and
phrase, or an object to shift attention inward),
conference rooms all provide learners with
a passive attitude, decreased muscle tonus,
something soft to lie on such as mats, blan-
and a quiet environment. T h e ultimate goal of
kets, or thick carpets. In clinical treatment,
relaxation training is to evoke the relaxation
individual rooms with cots, pillows, and blan-
response to counter situational stress.
kets are often provided. It should be noted that complete elimination of external distrac-
Progressive
Relaxation.
Modern progres-
tion is not desired because the normal envi-
sive relaxation techniques were derived from
ronment
Edmond Jacobson's work during the early part
relaxing can be quite noisy. Relaxation starts
in which the individual will be
of the 2 0 t h century. T h e main premise of his
with the muscles o f the left arm and proceeds
approach was that it is impossible to be ner-
to the right arm, left and right legs, abdomen,
vous or tense when skeletal muscles are com-
back, chest, and shoulder muscles and then
pletely relaxed. Jacobson noted that relaxation
concludes with the neck and face muscles. T h e
is a fundamental physiological occurrence that
starting position is with the learner lying on
consists of systematically learning to elongate
his or her back, with the arms by the sides.
muscle fibers. For this reason, the use o f sug-
Only one position is practiced each hour. T h e
gestion by the progressive relaxation instructor
control signal, which for the position with
should be abandoned given that the perception
the hand bent back is the vague sensation in
of relaxation is not so important as actual
the upper surface of the forearm, is observed
physiological relaxation (McGuigan, 1 9 9 3 ) .
three times during each period. This is the
Jacobson's full progressive relaxation pro-
critical signal that the individual is to learn
cedure involves systematically tensing and
and recognize. T h e tensed position is held for
relaxing specific muscle groups in a predeter-
a minute or so, and then the "power goes off"
mined order and was described in great
(relaxing the signal away) for a few minutes.
detail by M c G u i g a n ( 1 9 9 3 ) . T h e individual is
During this initial session, the learner will
instructed to tense a muscle before relaxing it
make a few c o m m o n mistakes, and it is the
to help him or her recognize the difference
instructor's crucial j o b to catch and correct
Stress and Health these mistakes. These include misidentifying
Autogenic
Training.
Autogenic
179
training
a control signal as a strain and making an
was developed by Johannes Heinrich Schultz
effort to relax by working the hand down to
following his own observations o f individuals
a "resting" position, which is merely adding
under hypnosis and Oskar Vogt's observa-
m o r e tension, instead o f allowing the hand to
tions in brain research (Linden, 1 9 9 3 ) . Schultz
simply collapse.
noticed that hypnotized patients would report
The amount o f time required to learn pro-
a heaviness o f the limbs and a warmth sensa-
gressive relaxation may seem excessive from a
tion. Hypnosis was believed to be something
naive learner's point of view. Attempts to shorten
that patients allowed to happen to themselves,
the process have not yielded satisfactory results.
not something that the therapist did to them.
Jacobson explained that a body that has been
The objective o f autogenic training is to per-
practicing overtension for decades will not be
mit self-regulation in either direction (deep
able to reverse the process in brief sessions.
relaxation or augmentation o f a physiological
Children, however, have been shown to learn
activity) through
progressive relaxation quite rapidly, probably
also described as "self-hypnosis." Training
because they have not had as many years prac-
can be taught individually or in groups. T h e
ticing maladaptive tension habits.
ideal setting should be one at room temperature,
"passive
concentration,"
slighdy darkened, with a couch or exercise mats Abbreviated Training.
Progressive
Relaxation
and pillows.
Condensed versions o f progressive
relaxation have been linked to the w o r k o f
Biofeedback
Methods.
In general, biofeed-
W o l p e ( 1 9 5 8 ) , w h o developed a "short" ver-
back systems operate by detecting changes in
sion o f progressive relaxation for treating
the biological environment and conveying this
1993).
information to the client in the form o f visual
phobias
(Bernstein
Abbreviated
&
Carlson,
progressive
relaxation
was
and auditory signals (Stoyva &c Budzynski,
standardized and popularized by Bernstein
1 9 9 3 ) . T h e client then synthesizes this infor-
and Borkovec ( 1 9 7 3 ) in their classic text,
mation with a trial-and-error strategy to cause
and is the
the signals to change in the desired direction;
source citation for many clinical intervention
thus, the client learns how to control the bio-
studies using progressive relaxation (Hillenberg
logical response system. It is further intended
& Collins, 1 9 8 2 ) .
that the client will be gradually weaned away
Progressive
Relaxation
Training,
Bernstein and Carson ( 1 9 9 3 ) provided a detailed review of abbreviated which are summarized
procedures,
here. In the initial
from the biofeedback signal, allowing for the transfer o f control into everyday life. The most common form of biofeedback
training session, 1 6 muscle groups are the
used in stress management
focus of tension release procedures. T h e client
graphic ( E M G ) feedback. E M G feedback oper-
is electromyo-
is typically asked to recline during the intro-
ates
duction. T h e order in which the muscle groups
providing visual or auditory signals linked to
by
detecting
biological signals
and
are taught is standardized, and the therapist
this biological system via amplification of the
demonstrates the tensing methods and then
psychophysiological measures. This immediate
paces the client through the series o f tension
feedback is thought to facilitate learning to con-
release procedures. T h e client is encouraged to
trol or reduce arousal through trial-and-error
practice relaxation skills twice a day, 1 5 to 2 0
strategies designed to produce changes in the
minutes each time, with continued training in
signal in a desired
sessions paced by the therapist.
Budzynski, 1 9 9 3 ) .
direction
(Stoyva
&
180
BEHAVIORS THAT C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH E M G feedback offers a number o f specific
Meditation.
Although meditation is often
advantages as compared with traditional non-
viewed as a yoga-based technique, Carrington
biofeedback relaxation therapies in that it
(1978) introduced a "clinically standardized
provides a direct measure o f client learning
meditation procedure" that appears to be useful
through the monitoring o f muscular activity
for reducing stress symptoms in a range of
rather than depending on the client's verbal
health-related disorders (see also Carrington,
report
1993). Meditation can be divided into two dis-
o f relaxation. E M G data
quantify
physiological relationships and operationalize
tinct forms: concentrative and nonconcentrative.
the concept o f relaxation. Some drawbacks
The concentrative forms o f meditation
associated with biofeedback are that the client
simple to learn. The techniques are often prac-
may become dependent on the machine and
ticed in a quiet environment, with the object of
that it provides minimal training in coping
the meditator's attention being a mentally
strategies for reducing tension.
repeated sound, the meditator's breath, or some
are
other appropriate focal sound (e.g., running Yoga
water). If attention is found to be wandering, the
methods have had the most popularity in the
meditator is directed back to the attentional
Yoga-Based
Stress
Management.
treatment of hypertension (Patel, 1993). This
object in an unforceful manner. A nonconcen-
technique appears to reduce stress in part by
trative technique expands the meditator's atten-
helping the client to reframe the stressor in a
tional field to include as much of his or her
nonthreatening fashion. Yoga, which means
conscious mental activity as possible. In this
"union," is an Indian philosophy that presents
sense, the specific techniques of meditation are
various values, techniques, and disciplines to
secondary to the actual experience of meditation
teach ways of establishing harmony by develop-
in bringing about therapeutic change.
ing the mind among the various sides of life. The
In summary, there are many paths to the
sides of human life are both material (body) and
reduction
nonmaterial (mind and soul). Different tech-
activation, producing a shift toward lower
niques or combinations are used depending on
arousal characterized by
whether one's intellect, emotions, or actions dom-
nervous
inate. The main components of yoga include
stress response is characterized by a height-
14 breathing exercises (which facilitate regulation
ened sympathetic tone, shifting autonomic
o f sympathetic nervous
system
parasympathetic
system d o m i n a n c e . Because
the
of the mind and body), 2 0 0 balanced physical
dominance to the parasympathetic nervous
postures (which help to prevent musculoskeletal
system should be effective in
deterioration), and exercises for
awakening
stress and anxiety reactions regardless o f the
"kundalini" (the energy reservoir at the base of
methods used. Schwartz ( 1 9 9 3 ) pointed out
the spinal cord). Yoga is based on a belief that life
that although numerous methods are avail-
can be conceptualized as a gaseous exchange that
able for stress management, client character-
takes place between "inspired" (oxygenated) air
istics may lead the therapist to consider
and the blood circulating in the lungs. Deep
alternative
muscle relaxation, visualization, and meditation
clients' life histories and experiences and
strategies that
moderating
might
best fit
all are components of yoga-based therapies,
that might be more appealing to clients.
further illustrating yoga's emphasis ranging from
Therapists w h o develop skills in multiple
the physical to the spiritual in nature. For a more
methods will be in a better position to pro-
in-depth review of yoga methods, the reader
vide clients with methods that
is referred to Patel (1993), who provides a
evidence based and philosophically consistent
detailed summary of these methods.
with client expectations.
are
both
Stress and Health
|
COGNITIVE
cardiovascular, gastrointestinal). Therefore,
APPROACHES T O STRESS
the ultimate goal o f therapy is to reduce the dominance o f the controlling constellation o f
Cognitive approaches
conceptualize
stress
as the result o f an active cognitive set that includes successive appraisals of environmen-
cognitions and to allow the protective buffers' adaptive functions to take over. An
initial
treatment
approach
often
tal demands and the risks, costs, and gains of
includes directly reducing the exposure o f the
specific coping responses. W h e n an individual's
stressful stimuli to the individual. This serves
vital interests appear to be challenged, cognitive
to reduce the intensity o f the cognitive con-
processes provide a selective conceptualization
stellations, reduce the mobilization o f the
of what is occurring. Beck ( 1 9 9 3 ) provided an
neuromuscular endocrine system, and increase
in-depth review o f cognitive approaches
perspective and objectivity. This sets the stage
to
stress management that are summarized in the
for the individual to n o w reflect on his or her
current chapter.
reactions, test other options, and adopt a
T h e initial appraisal o f an event can be
broader and more realistic view o f the situa-
considered a quick scan to determine whether
tion. T h e specific terms for the previously
it is pleasant, neutral, or noxious. W h e n the
described treatment are as follows: identifying
vital interests (harm or enhancement) o f an
automatic thoughts
individual (egocentric view) are assessed to be
n o w ? " ) , recognizing and correcting cognitive
( " W h a t am I thinking
affected, a critical response ensues. An emer-
distortions ("It does not have to mean this; it
gency critical response is activated when the
is probably that"), and identifying the broad
individual perceives a threat to his or her sur-
beliefs and assumptions
vival, domain, individuality, or status. This
hyperactive constellations ("I'm a bad person
response is critical to the development o f stress
and I don't deserve good things"). T h e process
reactions. At the same time the situation is
described previously
evaluated as a threat, the individual is assess-
known as cognitive restructuring or the A B C
ing his or her resources for dealing with the
approach. T h e premise is that the antecedent
ensuing problem. This assessment is labeled
or event leads to thoughts and feelings that
"secondary appraisal" (Lazarus,
1966).
that underlie
outlines the
the
process
ultimately drive the individual's behavior. By
T h e basic rationale behind the treatment o f stress from a cognitive perspective is that cer-
changing the way in which the
individual
thinks about the events, the person will be able
tain idiosyncratic cognitive patterns become
to change his or her behavior. In respect to this
hyperactive and lead to the overmobilization
chapter, the individual will be able to reduce
of the voluntary nervous system and auto-
the stress response by "seeing the problem in
nomic nervous system. W h e n this occurs, the
another light" using this systematic approach.
protective buffers and adaptive functions (e.g., objectivity, perspective, reality testing)
are
rendered ineffective against the cognitive constellation that has been triggered. This overmobilization
directly
results
in
reactive
Stress Inoculation Training Stress inoculation training is based on the premise that bolstering (inoculating) an indi-
syndromes (e.g., anxiety, hostility) or psycho-
vidual's repertoire o f coping responses
somatic syndromes. In the long term, these
milder stressors can serve to defuse responses
to
physical effects can lead to dysfunction o f spe-
to major life stressors (for a more extensive
cific systems or organs (e.g., musculoskeletal,
review, see M e i c h e n b a u m , 1 9 9 3 ) . This is
182
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH accomplished with the use o f an overlapping
cascade o f behavioral and biological processes
three-phase intervention approach. The goal of
that include autonomic nervous system arousal
the initial phase is to establish a relationship
and endocrine activation. Through the modifi-
between the trainer and the client to help the
cation o f appraisal processes, an
client better understand the nature of stress and
is able t o minimize threat
its effects on emotions and behavior. Similarly to
thereby reducing levels o f the stress response.
the cognitive approach, this initial phase focuses
Treatments that focus on central
on getting the client to appreciate how he or she
system reduction directly can be conceptualized
and
individual demand, nervous
appraises both events and on his or her ability to
as "bottom-up" methods that directly influence
cope with the events. In addition, alternative
behavioral and biological processes. In addi-
explanations and alternatives are explored. At
tion, some central nervous system reduction
the end of the initial phase, a reconceptualiza-
methods have been shown to directly influence
tion of the client's problems is made.
cognitive processes that show up as enhanced
T h e second phase focuses on coping skills acquisition
and
rehearsal
o f these skills.
coping and reduced perception of demand based on the individual's "knowing" that he or
Attention is paid to removing factors that
she now has the skill to reduce arousal directly.
may interfere with adequate coping such as
T h e following case study illustrates this point.
maladaptive beliefs and feelings o f low selfefficacy. T h e skills are practiced initially in the training setting, with gradual
introduc-
CONCLUSIONS
tion in vivo. The final phase of stress inoculation training
This chapter has provided an overview of the
calls for the client to apply the variety of learned
biological and psychosocial factors related to
coping skills across increasing levels ("inocula-
stress. A great deal o f attention was given to the
tion") of stressors on a graduated basis. This is
biological mechanisms involved in the stress
behavioral
response because this is an aspect o f training
rehearsal, modeling, role-playing, and graded in
that is often lacking. As the case study illus-
vivo exposure. Attention is also allocated to
trates, assessment and treatment may at times
relapse prevention. The client is taught how to
focus on behavioral and cognitive factors; how-
handle a lapse and not allow for it to become a
ever, knowledge of the biology of stress may
relapse. In summary, stress inoculation training
help clients to accept psychological treatment.
accomplished
by imagery and
combines client self-monitoring,
cognitive
T h e treatment methods presented in this
restructuring, problem solving, self-instructional
chapter were, by necessity, merely summarized.
and relaxation training, behavioral and imag-
Therapists who have not used relaxation or
ined rehearsal, and environmental change, with
other anxiety reduction methods should seek
the goal of enabling the client to be flexible in
out supervised experiences before using these
his or her coping repertoire and to have the
with clients. W h a t at times seems very easy to
confidence to cope resourcefully.
do can in fact be much more complicated than it appears. As should be noted from both the case study and the early work of Jacobson, it
Summary
takes a great deal of therapist time to success-
As this brief review illustrates, there are many
approaches
to
stress
management.
fully train relaxation skills. Merely sending clients home with a taped session is not suffi-
Cognitive approaches focus on a "top-down"
cient. Likewise, the cognitive
method, with the primary goal being the reduc-
require systematic training and should not be
tion o f cognitions that, if unchecked, begin a
attempted without supervised experiences.
interventions
Stress and
Health
CASE S T U D Y "Jane M . " was referred by her physician for treatment o f stress that was thought to be linked to her frequent headaches. J a n e had recently been promoted to a managerial position with her company, and the new position required that she visit regional offices in nearby states on a frequent basis. She was on the road several days each week, and for the past 6 months she reported constant fatigue and frequent headaches. While fatigued and tired, she found it difficult to relax at night while on the road. Baseline
assessment
included
the
D a i l y Hassles
and
Uplifts
Scale,
the
Spielberger State-Trait Anger Inventory, the Beck Depression Inventory-II, and a clinical interview. Results indicated mild levels o f depression and moderate to severe levels o f anger. T h e anger appeared to be most linked to J a n e ' s dissatisfaction with the staff at m a n y o f the hotels where she stayed and her inability to be "treated with the respect that she deserved" when problems arose. T h e r e were few life events rated as positive, with m a n y m i n o r annoyances receiving very negative ratings. Although it seemed clear that J a n e ' s stress levels were at times generated by her perceptions o f events and expectations that she " s h o u l d " be treated more respectfully, it was also obvious from early sessions that she was physiologically very tense and quite concerned that her physician believed her problems were all "mental." T h e therapist spent a great deal o f time discussing h o w negative events start a chain of physiological processes that lead to heightened arousal that for some people result in headaches or other physical problems. F o r some clients, merely learning to reduce this arousal can be useful. J a n e showed a strong interest in learning h o w to reduce physical arousal, and a treatment plan was developed that initially focused on learning relaxation skills. T h e relaxation method used was a form o f abbreviated progressive relaxation training developed by Charles R . Carlson (Carlson, Collins, Nitz, Sturgis, & Rogers, 1 9 9 0 ; Carlson, Ventralla, & Sturgis, 1 9 8 7 ; K a y & Carlson, 1 9 9 2 ) called stretch-based relaxation. Stretch-based relaxation focuses on stretching o f muscles, facilitating a differentiation o f muscle sensations that contributes to a recognition of muscle tension and fosters relaxation o f the muscles. Training was conducted over an 8-week period, with in-session relaxation training taking between 3 0 and 4 0 minutes o f each session. T h e remaining time was spent discussing the application o f stretch-based relaxation to J a n e ' s daily routine and fine-tuning home practice. H e r initial response was that she could not produce the levels o f relaxation at h o m e that she experienced in sessions, and these concerns and expectations were addressed with a reminder that she would improve with continued practice. At Session 5 , she reported feeling much more effective at home, and in-session training switched from being therapist-led training to client-paced training with the therapist observing J a n e ' s performance.
184
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH
At Session 6 , J a n e reported a critical incident where she was checking into a hotel and the night clerk could not find her reservation. It t o o k some time for the clerk t o c o m e up with a suitable r o o m , and in the past she would have become physically tense and extremely angry and would have developed a headache that would keep her from sleeping well. She remembered thinking t o herself that if she got tense from this "incompetent c l e r k , " she would just use her relaxation skills to c a l m d o w n once she was in her r o o m and could avoid getting a headache. N o t only did she not get a headache that evening, she did not even feel tense while waiting. Discussion with her therapist indicated that the knowledge that she " k n e w what to do if she got t e n s e " seemed to ward off feelings o f tension. She began to entertain the idea that it was n o t merely the situation that " g o t her tense"; rather, it was her reaction to the situation (including the expectancy that a headache would ensue) that contributed to her tension. Sessions 7 and 8 involved a minor review o f stretch-based relaxation, with a great deal o f the sessions devoted to discussion o f h o w J a n e planned to implement this new skill in her life t o keep herself more relaxed. Readministration o f the Daily Hassles and Uplifts Scale indicated that the number o f situations did not change but that the perception o f the situations as negative was greatly improved. Headaches had dropped off significantly with more relaxed sleep and energy. J a n e was encouraged to continue formally practicing relaxation on at least a weekly basis.
REFERENCES American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Beck, A. T . (1990). Manual for the Beck Anxiety Inventory. San Antonio, T X : Psychological Corporation. Beck, A. T. (1993). Cognitive approaches to stress. In P. M . Lehrer & R. L. Woolfolk (Eds.), Principles and practice of stress management (2nd ed., pp. 3 3 3 - 3 7 2 ) . New York: Guilford. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. Α., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-11. San Antonio, T X : Psychological Corporation. Bernstein, D. Α., & Borkovec, T . D. (1973). Progressive relaxation training: A manual for the helping professions. Champaign, IL: Research Press. Bernstein, D. Α., & Carlson, C. R. (1993). Progressive relaxation: Abbreviated methods. In P. M . Lehrer & R. L. Woolfolk (Eds.), Principles and practice of stress management (2nd ed., pp. 5 3 - 8 8 ) . New York: Guilford. Brown, G. W., & Harris, T . O. (1978). Social origins of depression: A study of psychiatric disorder in women. London: Tavistock.
Stress and Health Carlson, C. R., Collins, F. L., Nitz, A. J . , Sturgis, E. T., & Rogers, J . L. (1990). Muscle stretching as an alternative relaxation training procedure. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 21, 2 9 - 3 8 . Carlson, C. R., Ventralla, Μ . Α., Sc Sturgis, Ε. T. (1987). Relaxation training through muscle stretching procedures: A pilot case. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 18, 1 2 1 - 1 2 6 . Carlson, N. R. (1999). Foundations of physiological psychology (4th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Carrington, P. (1978). Clinically standardized meditation (CSM) instructor's kit. Kendall Park, NJ: Pace Educational Systems. Carrington, P. (1993). Modern forms of meditation. In P. M. Lehrer Se R. L. Woolfolk (Eds.), Principles and practice of stress management (2nd ed., pp. 139-168). New York: Guilford. Cassidy, T. (1999). Stress, cognition, and health. New York: Routledge. Cohen, S., Tyrrell, D. A. J . , Sc Smith, A. P. (1991). Psychological stress and susceptibility to the common cold. New England Journal of Medicine, 325, 6 0 6 - 6 1 2 . Derogatis, L. R. (1975). The Symptom Checklist 90-Revised. Minneapolis, M N : NCS Assessments. Hillenberg, J . B., & Collins, F. L. (1982). A procedural analysis and review of relaxation training research. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 20, 2 5 1 - 2 6 0 . Holmes, T. H., Sc Rahe, R. (1967). The Social Readjustment Rating Scale. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 14, 2 1 3 - 2 1 8 . Kanner, A. D., Coyne, J . C , Schaefer, C , Sc Lazarus, R. S. (1981). Comparison of two modes of stress measurement: Daily hassles and uplifts versus major life events. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 4, 1-39. Kay, J . Α., Sc Carlson, C. R. (1992). The role of stretch-based relaxation in the treatment of chronic neck tension. Behavior Therapy, 23, 423—431. Kiecolt-Glaser, J . K., Marucha, P. T., Malarkey, W. B., Mercado, A. M., & Glaser, R. (1995). Slowing of wound healing by psychological stress. Lancet, 346, 1194-1196. Lazarus, R. S. (1966). Psychological stress and the coping process. New York: McGraw-Hill. Lazarus, R. S. (1999). Stress and emotion: A new synthesis. New York: Springer. Lazarus, R. S., Sc Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer. Lehrer, P. M., Sc Woolfolk, R. L. (1993). Principles and practice of stress management (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford. Linden, W. (1993). The autogenic training method of J . H. Schultz. In P. M. Lehrer & R. L. Woolfolk (Eds.), Principles and practice of stress management (2nd ed., pp. 2 0 5 - 2 3 0 ) . New York: Guilford. Lovallo, W. R. (1997). Stress and health: Biological and psychological interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Markovitz, J . H., Sc Matthews, K. A. (1991). Platelets and coronary heart disease: Potential psychophysiologic mechanisms. Psychosomatic Medicine, 53, 643-668. Mayberry, D., Mayberry, M., Bresnan, R., Croft, B . , Graham, R., Macaulay, J . , McQualter, S., Mitchell, E., Sherwell, K., Sc Szakacs, E. (2002). Responding to daily event questionnaires: The influence of the order of hassle and uplift scales. Stress and Health: Journal of the International Society of the Investigation of Stress, 18, 1 9 - 2 6 . McCallum, D. M., Arnold, S. E., Sc Bolland, J . M . (2002) Low-income AfricanAmerican women talk about stress. Journal of Social Distress and the Homeless, 11, 2 4 9 - 2 6 3 . McEwen, B. S. (2000). Stress, definitions and concepts of. In G. Fink (Ed.), The encyclopedia of stress (Vol. 3, pp. 5 0 8 - 5 0 9 ) . San Diego: Academic Press.
185
186
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH McGuigan, F. J . (1993). Progressive relaxation: Origins, principles, and clinical applications. In P. M . Lehrer &C R. L. Woolfolk (Eds.), Principles and practice of stress management (2nd ed., pp. 1 7 - 5 2 ) . New York: Guilford. Meichenbaum, D. (1993). Stress inoculation training: A 20-year update. In P. M. Lehrer & R. L. Woolfolk (Eds.), Principles and practice of stress management (2nd ed., pp. 3 7 3 - 4 0 6 ) . New York: Guilford. Patel, C. (1993). Yoga-based therapy. In P. M. Lehrer & R. L. Woolfolk (Eds.), Principles and practice of stress management (2nd ed., pp. 8 9 - 1 3 8 ) . New York: Guilford. Pollard, T. M . (2000). Adrenaline. In G. Fink (Ed.), The encyclopedia of stress (Vol. 3, pp. 5 2 - 5 8 ) . San Diego: Academic Press. Sarason, I. G., Johnson, J . H., &C Siegel, J . M. (1978). Assessing the impact of life changes: Development of the Life Experiences Survey. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46, 9 3 2 - 9 4 6 . Schwartz, G. E. (1993). Foreword: Biofeedback is not relaxation is not hypnosis. In P. M. Lehrer & R. L. Woolfolk (Eds.), Principles and practice of stress management (2nd ed., pp. vii-viii). New York: Guilford. Spielberger, C. D. (1996). Manual for the State-Trait Anger Expression Scale. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., Sc Jacobs, G. A. (1983). The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Mind Garden. Spielberger, C. D., Sydeman, S. J . , Owen, A. E., & Marsh, B . J . (1999). The StateTrait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) and State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory (STAXI). In M. E. Marush (Ed.), The use of psychological tests for treatment planning and outcome assessment (2nd ed., pp. 9 9 3 - 1 0 2 1 ) . Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Stoyva, J . M., Si Budzynski, T. H. (1993). Biofeedback methods in the treatment of anxiety and stress disorders. In P. M . Lehrer 8c R. L. Woolfolk (Eds.), Principles and practice of stress management (2nd ed., pp. 2 6 3 - 3 0 0 ) . New York: Guilford. VanHoudenhove, B . , Neerincky, E., Onghene, P., Vingerhoets, Α., Roeland, L., Sc Vertommen, H. (2002). Daily hassles reported by chronic fatigue syndrome and fibromyalgia patients in tertiary care: A controlled quantitative and qualitative study. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 71, 2 0 7 - 2 1 3 . Vgontzas, A. N., Bixler, E. O., 8c Kales, A. (2000). Sleep, sleep disorders, and stress. In G. Fink (Ed.), The encyclopedia of stress (Vol. 3, pp. 4 4 9 - 4 5 7 ) . San Diego: Academic Press. Wolpe, J . (1958). Psychotherapy by reciprocal inhibition. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Woolfolk, R. L., 8c Lehrer, P. M. (1984). Principles and practice of stress management. New York: Guilford.
CHAPTER
11
Management of Inappropriate Medication-Seeking Behavior SUZY BIRD GULLIVER, BARBARA A . W O L F S D O R F , AND ALEXANDER
P
MlCHAS
erhaps one o f the largest occupational
challenges
faced
by
clinical
health
medication seeking in a patient or in a group of
patients,
or the psychologist may
be
psychologists is that o f addressing the
required to intervene with a provider or a group
unhealthy and/or inappropriate use o f medi-
of providers whose prescribing practices are
cations. T h e problem o f medication seeking is
suspect. Regardless of how the medication-
made all the more c o m p l e x because medica-
seeking behavior is identified,
tion-seeking behavior is not solely the respon-
sional assessment and treatment plans are the
multidimen-
sibility o f the patient w h o is actively engaged
keys to successful amelioration o f the difficulty.
in the behavior. T h e prescribing providers,
This chapter defines the problem o f medication
the health care system in which the patient
seeking, proposes
seeks treatment, cyber-community
a conceptual
framework
(most recently)
the
for understanding medication seeking, and
for pharmaceuticals
all
describes assessment plans as well as treatment
and
constitute potentially active participants in
development
this high-risk, maladaptive behavior.
descriptive case study is also presented to illus-
to address
this behavior.
A
Clinical health psychologists are frequently
trate these ideas. Finally, the chapter concludes
called on to intervene not only with patients
with a synopsis of what is known and what
but also with providers and the system to effec-
still needs to be explored to meet the field's
tively extinguish the problem behavior. Indeed,
overarching objective o f optimal clinical care.
referral sources for evaluation and treatment of medication-seeking behavior are nearly as c o m m o n as the types o f drugs that patients
DEFINITIONS AND DESCRIPTION
seek. For example, a clinical health psycholo-
OF MEDICATION-SEEKING
gist may become aware of medication seeking
BEHAVIOR
in a patient being treated for another disorder, the psychologist may receive consult requests
Definitions o f medication-seeking
from
(also known as drug-seeking behavior) are often
care providers
who
have
identified
behavior
188
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH incomplete, vague, or lacking in the literature
target psychologists, case managers, nurses,
addressing the phenomenon. For the purposes
pharmacists, and other providers
of this chapter, the term
medication-seeking
"gatekeepers" to prescribing physicians. For
is used to highlight the pursuit of
instance, within the chapter authors' outpatient
behavior
legal prescription medications rather
than
seen as
mental health clinic, the psychiatric Walk-In
illicit "drugs." Medication-seeking behavior is
Clinic (WIC) operates each afternoon of the
defined as a pervasive pattern o f requesting
business week. The W I C is staffed by psychol-
medications that have either little or no thera-
ogy and social work trainees, licensed mental
peutic efficacy for the presenting
problem
health care providers, and supervising clinical
and/or in dosages exceeding therapeutic limits.
health psychologists. Medication evaluations
This definition is consistent with that offered
(with the possibility o f immediate prescriptions)
by Pankratz, Hickam, and Toth ( 1 9 8 9 ) , who
are provided 3 days a week for 1 hour by nurse
defined drug-seeking behavior as "any attempt
practitioners and psychiatrists. O n e of the prin-
to influence a physician to prescribe excessive
cipal tasks in the W I C is to identify medication
medication or to obtain abusable medications
seeking and make appropriate treatment plans.
through illegal activities" (p. 1 1 5 ) .
The multidisciplinary team is ideally suited for
Notably, medication-seeking behavior may
this task, as is described in greater detail later.
occur within several contexts. For example,
The health psychologist, as a senior supervisor,
this maladaptive
manifest
must be aware of the different contexts within
within a somatoform disorder in which the
which medication-seeking behavior may occur
behavior m a y
patient has a genuine conviction of his or her
and must be prepared to work with a multitude
illness and requests medication "appropriate"
of
different
professionals relative to
this
for the believed illness (Singh, 1 9 9 8 ) . Alter-
issue. Although much discussion within this
natively, medication seeking may occur within
chapter centers on the prescribing physician,
the presence of an addiction that began with
the issues raised are equally important for all
appropriate treatment o f a valid medical or
health care providers who come in contact with
psychological illness, in which case the behav-
medication-seeking patients.
ior represents an attempt to prevent with-
The
pervasiveness of medication-seeking
drawal symptoms or loss o f functioning and
behavior, as defined here, is as yet unknown.
may or may not be sincerely driven. Finally,
Very little clean data exist that would yield
this behavior may be largely manipulative in
meaningful conclusions regarding its incidence
nature and be motivated by the desire for a
and prevalence. However, in addition to the
"high," for thrill seeking, or for the street
clinical lore that most practicing health psy-
value and concomitant financial rewards o f
chologists will endorse, some national statistics
possessing desirable medications. Clearly,
are useful. For instance, the American Medical
these contexts for medication-seeking behav-
Association (ΑΜΑ) stated, " T h e abuse of pre-
ior are not mutually exclusive.
scription drugs results in more injuries and
Furthermore, a number o f distinct health
deaths to Americans than all illegal drugs com-
care professionals may be affected by medica-
bined. Prescription drugs are involved in more
tion-seeking patients and subsequently may call
than 6 0 % of all drug-related emergency room
on health psychologists to intervene. Physicians
visits and 7 0 % o f all drug-related
deaths"
are likely the most frequently targeted group
(Weiss &c Greenfield, 1 9 8 6 , cited in Lewis &
(although this assertion has yet to be evaluated
Gaule, 1 9 9 9 , p. 8 3 8 ) . T h e National Institute
empirically), but patients may also look to
on Drug Abuse (NIDA, 2 0 0 1 ) reported that
nurse
to
approximately 9 million Americans over age
obtain medications. In addition, patients may
12 years used prescription medications for
practitioners
in
their
attempts
Inappropriate
Medication-Seeking
Behavior are
\
nonmedical reasons in 1 9 9 9 , and the incidence
addiction). Pain syndromes
of prescription drug abuse seems to be increas-
maintained, at least in part, by the negative
frequently
ing. For example, according to the 1 9 9 9
reinforcement achieved through narcotic medi-
National Household Survey on Drug Abuse,
cations. Although some data argue persuasively
increased
that a cycle of overuse cannot be instated when
for nearly all medication classes from 1 9 9 0
patients are faced with scheduled dosing for a
nonmedical use o f prescriptions
initiation
pain syndrome, certain subgroups of patients
increased by 1 8 1 % , tranquilizer use increased
are at increased risk for negative sequelae from
by 1 3 2 % , sedative use increased by 9 0 % , and
as-needed dosing (Poling 8c Byrne, 2 0 0 0 ) . That
to
1 9 9 8 . Pain relief medication
stimulant use increased by 1 6 5 % . In fact, the
is, patients who have a known history o f prior
street value o f abused (but prescribed) medica-
addictions
tions was second only to cocaine sales between
tobacco use disorder) are more likely to be
1 9 8 7 and 1 9 9 7 ( Α Μ Α , 1 9 8 8 ) .
noncompliant with a fixed dosing schedule and
(perhaps with the exception o f
addressing
are at increased risk for a craving response and
medication seeking are not clear and distinct,
possible relapse to the drug o f choice while on
indirect measures o f the scope and intensity o f
a fixed dosing schedule.
Although the published
data
the problem can be divided into the following categories: challenges for the
patient,
The final negative consequences o f medication-seeking behavior to patients are societal.
challenges for the physician, challenges for the
Patients who become labeled as
medical system, and challenges for the culture.
seeking
medication
suffer the slings and arrows o f social
disapproval,
often inspiring dislike
among
care providers. As described previously, many
Challenges for the Patient
patients began the cycle o f medication seeking
Multiple negative medical consequences
while under care for documented medical or
may result from medication seeking on the
psychological illnesses; therefore, they are in
part o f the patient. First, there is the risk o f
part correct in their assertions that they are ill
medication complications, such as overdose,
and in need o f medication. Y e t the attitude o f
and the ongoing concern o f polypharmacy.
hostility and dislike toward these patients, and
Second, when
physiological
the subsequent conflictual relationships with
addictions are unable to meet their medica-
health care providers, may result in increased
patients with
and/or
increased likeli-
tion needs, the possibility o f triggering an
"doctor shopping"
acute withdrawal
syndrome exists. Third,
hood o f the acquisition o f prescriptions from
patients w h o chronically medicate might not
providers motivated to avoid conflict (e.g.,
observe or report symptoms that need assess-
Longo, Parran, Johnson, & Kinsey, 2 0 0 0 ) .
ment and additional treatment.
These possible consequences in turn decrease
In addition, patients who attain the medica-
the likelihood o f appropriate patient care and
tions of choice begin a cycle of negative rein-
are largely iatrogenic because these patients
forcement that is ultimately likely to increase
are likely in need o f more—not less—clinical
the symptoms for which the medications are
management.
prescribed. For instance, Silberstein
(1992)
noted that medication abuse can result in increased headaches in patients who previously
Challenges for the Physician
had intermittent headaches. T h e tremendous
The drug-seeking patient presents multiple
power o f the negative reinforcement cycle is
challenges for the physician. First, the patient's
well documented in problems that are primarily
behavior places the patient's life in danger,
psychogenic in nature (e.g., anxiety disorders,
and this places attendant liability on
the
189
290
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH physician. Patients suspected o f medication
medication. For instance, much o f the focus o f
seeking are difficult to assess and even more
changing physician prescribing practices has
challenging to treat. In addition, the assess-
been in the area o f decreasing the prescription
ment o f such patients treads very closely to
of "drugs of abuse." However, Parran ( 1 9 9 7 )
accusing them o f dishonesty, and this violates
stated that the systematic undertreatment of
the basic premise o f the physician-patient rela-
acute pain and undertreatment of malignant
tionship. Patients facing an evaluation o f
pain is pervasive and unnecessary. Also of
medication-seeking behavior frequently become
note is the fact that medications with exceed-
hostile and angry. This response is generally
ingly low abuse liability (e.g., antibiotics) are
experienced as punishing to the care provider,
overprescribed, and this creates a new cate-
and it decreases the chances that the provider
gory o f abuse. W i t h the advent o f antibiotic-
will confront similar patients in the future.
resistant bacteria, overprescription of other
Second, in most instances, the physician has
medications has also been called into question.
been trained in a " c u r e " model. Therefore, when a disease that cannot be cured (e.g., chronic pain syndrome) presents at a clinic, the appropriate response within this model o f
Challenges for the System Perhaps the most pervasive systemic prob-
training is to attempt to increase the dose or
lem with medication seeking is the amount of
intensity o f the treatment so as t o better help
time and resources that is consumed by medi-
the patient. I f the patient does not respond to
cation misuse, both in terms o f treating these
the increased care, the physician's confidence
patients
in the mode o f treatment declines, and the
and in terms o f attempting to intervene in
physician is left t o either "blame the healer or
these complex behavioral interactions. Systems
blame the patient."
must
Third, the physician's behavior is shaped by
appropriately
and
establish mechanisms
identify offending
patients
inappropriately
by which and
to
physicians.
the patient's response, and this presents two
They must track both groups for a sufficient
difficulties. T h e patient, w h o may immediately
duration to establish patterns o f behavior and
at
legitimacy o f claims, and they must create a
reinforces
remediation plan. These are costly measures in
become more pleasant and manageable the sight o f a prescription pad, providing
medication.
In
contrast,
the
terms o f both fiscal implications and systemic
unhappy patient can create a rather large dis-
morale. T h a t is, health care providers working
turbance in the course o f a busy schedule. T h e
within systems plagued by medication seeking
physician learns quickly that the provision o f
are likely unhappy with the position of "med-
medication will allow him or her to return to
ication police" and face additional scrutiny by
other patients who are less aversive, so an addi-
a community that may perceive differential
tional negative reinforcement loop emerges.
"fault." Furthermore, reports o f medication
T h e controversy over medication-seeking
mismanagement in the media are likely to
behavior and the concomitant responses o f
send more patients to alternative sources o f
physicians is not complete without acknowl-
health care, and this taxes the system in new
edgment that practices o f overprescribing are
ways (e.g., decreased revenues, increased acute
only half of the story. Inadequate pharmaco-
illness o f unknown etiology due to patient
logical treatment o f legitimate medical and
having nontraditional care that may have only
psychological illnesses presents
placebo effects or even be iatrogenic). Finally,
an
equally
problematic (although less widely discussed)
systems are aware o f bona fide medication
issue. In keeping with the medical code o f
prescriptions for dependents being used by
ethics, it is at least as problematic t o under-
medication-seeking caregivers (e.g.,
prescribe medication as it is to overprescribe
using their children's Ritalin, adult children o f
parents
Inappropriate terminally ill patients using parental
pain
medications to the detriment o f genuine pain management o f the ill parents).
Challenges for the Culture T h e problem o f medication seeking also has implications for the culture. Specifically, problems o f increased mortality o f patients, decreased confidence and hope in medicine (thereby driving health care providers away from patient care), and increased costs to the system all c o m b i n e to create significant threats to public safety. Determining appropriate interventions requires further consideration o f the legal implications for liability, responsibility, and potential prosecution. In sum, the problem o f medication seeking is large and complex and has the potential for widespread harm that far exceeds the dynamics of a small number o f individual patients who greatly t a x a small number o f physicians. A handful o f scientists and practitioners have started to explore a number o f intervention tactics for this problem. This literature is described next.
Medication-Seeking
Behavior
reference sections of the articles mentioned previously. Thus, an extensive review of 2 9 years of published, peer-reviewed literature yielded 5 2 available articles that were directly or peripherally related to this issue (a rate of less than 2 pertinent publications per year). These literature search results are consistent with the one published evaluation that could be located addressing the methodology o f the existing literature (Nichol, Venturini, & Sung, 1 9 9 9 ) . Nichol and colleagues ( 1 9 9 9 ) surveyed a computerized Medline
search from
1 9 8 0 to 1 9 9 6 using "patient compliance" as a major term and "drug therapy" as a subheading term. O f the random subset o f the 7 1 9 identified articles, the majority o f research articles on medication compliance (a behavioral neighbor o f drug seeking) were descriptive in nature ( 6 3 . 9 % of the articles reviewed). M o s t ( 4 1 . 7 % ) used a sample o f convenience, and the overall quality o f the
published
research was evaluated as very poor. Thus, the amount o f empirical literature available to inform this section is quite limited. With that cautionary note, there are three areas in which published data exist: (a) epidemiological data on prescription drug abuse, (b) content and case reviews of physicians, and (c) evaluation reports regarding the usefulness
O V E R V I E W O F RESEARCH IN
of systemic interventions to change the rein-
MEDICATION SEEKING
forcement o f medication seeking by decreasing
Very little empirical work on medication seeking is available in the literature today. A literature search using the Psycblit databases, including publications between 1 9 7 2 and 2 0 0 2 and using the terms "medication seeking" and "drug seeking," yielded 3 8 citations, o f which only 6 were directly relevant to this chapter. A similar Medline search using the same terms and years o f literature yielded 19 citations, o f which only 4 were relevant to this chapter. W h e n the search was expanded to include "prescription drug abuse" and "medication noncompliance" for the same time frame, an additional 4 relevant citations were found using Psych lit and an additional 1 2 relevant citations were found using Medline. Finally, 2 6 additional articles were identified from the
prescriptions, particularly among medications with higher abuse potential. O n e published evaluation o f methods to alter physician skill levels in assessing and responding to medication seeking was available but showed no significant gains as a result o f training (Taverner, Dodding, & White, 2 0 0 0 ) . N o data beyond a handful o f case descriptions were available on the nonmedical treatment o f medication seekers in an individual or group setting.
Epidemiological Data As noted previously, few data from epidemiological studies tracking the use and misuse o f prescription medications are available.
192
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH T h e most recent National Household Survey on Drug Abuse (NIDA, 2 0 0 1 ) indicates that drugs other than caffeine, alcohol, nicotine,
Content and Case Reviews of Physicians
cannabis, stimulants, and heroin were used by
According t o Kofoed, B l o o m , Williams,
about 8 % of the general population during
Rhyne, and Resnick ( 1 9 8 9 ) , the problem o f
the month immediately preceding the trial.
inappropriate prescribing is the largest single
Given the sampling biases o f the National
category o f complaints filed against physi-
Household Survey (e.g., homeless individuals,
cians. Interestingly, only a small percentage
itinerant workers, and school truants are not
of physicians are responsible for most inap-
captured in the sample), actual drug use may
propriate prescriptions. F o r example, results
be greater in the population at large.
from M a r o n d e , Seibert, and Katzoff ( 1 9 7 2 )
Among drugs that are coded as Schedule Π
suggest t h a t
less t h a n
5%
o f the
(high abuse potential with severe dependence
accounted for 5 0 % o f the
liability [e.g., narcotics]) or Schedule ΙΠ (less
prescriptions written in one hospital.
staff
inappropriate
abuse potential than Schedule I and Schedule II
Kofoed and colleagues ( 1 9 8 9 ) reviewed
but containing small amounts of certain narcotic
the cases o f reported inappropriate prescrib-
agents [e.g., acetaminophen with codeine]), no
ing that were deemed substantive and pur-
published estimates of use or abuse could be
sued t o some degree by the O r e g o n B o a r d o f
found. Schedule I V medications (less abuse
Medical Examiners between 1 9 8 1 and 1 9 8 6 .
potential than Schedule ΠΙ medications, includ-
Results indicated that m o r e than half ( 5 1 % )
ing drugs such as benzodiazepines) do have
of the complaints resulting in informal inter-
drug-specific use figures. Specifically, the Task
views with the investigative committee o f the
Force on Benzodiazepine Dependency o f the
board
American Psychiatric Association reported that
writing. (This is in contrast t o the second
involved inappropriate
prescription
1 0 % to 1 2 % of the population use benzodi-
most frequent category o f complaints, habit-
azepines each year but that only 1 % to 2 % of
ual or excessive use o f intoxicants, which
the population take these drugs on a long-term
accounted for only 1 3 % o f such investiga-
basis (Gold, Miller, Stennie, &c Populla-Vardi,
tions.) Furthermore, complaints o f inappro-
1 9 9 5 ; Salzman, 1 9 9 0 ) .
priate prescribing involved an average o f
Perhaps
the most instructive data
drawn from
populations
are
o f hospitalized
approximately
2
drugs,
most
frequently
including opiates and/or benzodiazepines. In
patients. These data demonstrate that addic-
fact, 9 o f the 1 0 most frequently cited inap-
tions affect up to 5 0 % o f this population. In
propriately prescribed medications belonged
addition, 1 5 % t o 3 0 % o f patients seen in
t o one o f these two categories o f drugs.
primary care settings meet criteria for addic-
In addition to the identification of specific
tion (Longo et al., 2 0 0 0 ) . A m o n g psychiatric
types of medications that may be inappropri-
patients, estimates o f c o m o r b i d addictions
ately prescribed, Kofoed and colleagues (1989)
are even higher. Given that
identified types of patients w h o may be pre-
most
abuse
(upward o f 8 0 % in the example o f benzodi-
scribed medications inappropriately.
azepine abuse) o f Schedule I V drugs occurs
8 0 % of patients identified in complaints of
Nearly
within the context o f polysubstance abuse or
inappropriate prescribing were pain patients
dependence (Longo et al., 2 0 0 0 ) , the prob-
( 5 5 % ) or drug-seeking patients ( 2 4 % ) . In addi-
lem o f drug abuse is likely t o be a significant
tion, slightly more than half of the physicians
red flag for identification o f populations at
under investigation for any reason between
increased risk for medication seeking.
1 9 8 1 and 1 9 8 6 had a history of at least one
Inappropriate of
medication-seeking behavior by the disruption
for inappropriate prescription writing.
of
diversion
may
indirectly
\
other investigation during the previous 4 to 9
drug
methods
Behavior
years; of those previous complaints, 7 0 % were Perhaps most important is the assertion by
these
Medication-Seeking
affect
(Cooper, Czechowicz,
Molinari, & Petersen, 1 9 9 3 ) . T h e Automated
Kofoed and colleagues ( 1 9 8 9 ) that inappropri-
Reports and
ate prescribing is unlikely to be a problem o f
( A R C O S ) , Drug Investigational Units (DIU),
intentional deceit. In fact, less than 5 % of
Electronic Point o f Sale Systems ( E P O S ) ,
physicians under investigation appeared to be
Medicaid Fraud and Abuse System (MFAS),
clearly dishonest. Rather, the large propor-
and
tion o f physicians under investigation were
(MCPP) all are available to decrease medication
guilty o f possessing inadequate pharmacological
diversion. An expert panel at N I D A evaluated
knowledge or o f being
"pseudobenevolent
the clinical utility o f these systems and pub-
overprescribers." These last-mentioned over-
lished its findings in 1 9 9 3 . It was found that
prescribers are characterized by a strong need to
although each system had its own advantages
help their patients, grandiose thoughts about
and disadvantages, the differences in applica-
their own importance in treating particular
tions of each method made an empirical evalu-
patients, an orientation toward
immediate
symptom relief rather than long-term
out-
comes, and an inability to handle their own emotions
if the
medication
is
withheld.
Consolidated Orders System
Multiple C o p y Prescription
Program
ation of all systems impossible. Nonetheless, each system did have some impact. M a c L e o d and Swanson ( 1 9 9 6 ) described the systemic intervention made in the emer-
Unfortunately, the phenomena o f inadequate
gency departments o f four Canadian hospitals
knowledge and pseudobenevolent
to better manage chronic pain without inap-
overpre-
scribing may co-occur.
propriate prescribing practices or the reinforce-
With only a general sense o f the categories o f
ment of medication-seeking behavior. These
patients who may be using prescription medi-
hospitals operationally define medication seek-
cations inappropriately, and a similarly general
ing as any patient coming in to any department
idea regarding the health care professionals who
1 0 or more times during the previous 12-month
provide such prescriptions, intervention may
period and requesting opiods for a chronic pain
seem to be a daunting task. O f particular
problem. Once a patient is identified in this
importance is the choice of targets for interven-
manner, treatment is refused unless the patient
tion. Should the target be the patient, the pre-
is registered in the chronic pain registry, where
scriber, the system, or some combination
he or she undergoes a systematic evaluation for
thereof? These options are discussed in greater
pain syndrome and treatment planning. Once
detail within the context o f a biopsychosocial
on the registry, the patient receives only one
model for understanding medication-seeking
care provider. This primary care physician
behavior. However, it may be useful to first
becomes the only recognized prescriber. In
review interventions (all systemic in nature) that
addition, the patient is allowed to visit only one
have been described in the literature.
emergency
department
(to
further
limit
attempts to doctor shop or the "splitting" of
Descriptions of Systemic Interventions According to N I D A, five types o f systemic
treatment providers). Primary care providers see the patient once every 9 weeks for evaluation o f the home care prescribed by the consulting physician. This thoughtfully designed
interventions have been developed to address
system has not yet been evaluated empirically.
medication misuse on a systemic level. Each
T h e results of such an investigation would be
193
194
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH particularly useful to other systems attempting
use of patients in the sole provider program is
to change maladaptive prescribing practices
monitored on an ongoing basis, with decreas-
and medication-seeking behavior as well as to
ing frequency over time. Formal and widely
improve patient care.
accessible computerized files are established
Another example o f an innovative systemic
and continually updated t o reflect any and all
intervention for the problem o f medication
attempts on the part o f these patients to pro-
seeking can be found in the Tripler Army
cure prescriptions (both successful appropriate
Medical
program
attempts and those conceptualized as mal-
Center's
sole provider
(Lewis & Gaule, 1 9 9 9 ) . Similar to the inter-
adaptive medication seeking). This facilitates
vention described by M a c L e o d and Swanson
prescription monitoring and communication
( 1 9 9 6 ) , the sole provider program focuses on
among the health care providers. Hence, when
the systemic monitoring o f prescriptions for
a patient presents to a provider, all potential
controlled substances and access to, as well as
prescribers are armed with the knowledge of
communication among, health care providers
the individual's prescription history and poten-
as important means by which to reduce medi-
tial difficulties associated with that history.
cation-seeking behavior. In this system, a
As in the previous example, such a system
patient can be enrolled in the sole provider
integrates patient care, facilitates easy and
program either through
a referral from a
efficient communication among health care
health care professional or through identifica-
providers, and improves delivery o f service t o
tion during twice-annual reviews o f all pre-
the patient.
scriptions for controlled substances. In the
supports the success o f this program, it also
Although
anecdotal evidence
latter case, a multidisciplinary sole provider
awaits empirical evaluation. W i t h
subcommittee reviews prescriptions for con-
examples in mind, a biopsychosocial model
these
trolled substances written to patients within
of
the Tripler Army Medical Center. Patients
offered next. Based on this model, additional
w h o are identified as having suspicious pat-
targets for intervention may be elucidated.
understanding
medication
seeking is
terns o f prescriptions (e.g., unusual numbers, large quantities, or multiple providers o f prescriptions) are reviewed closely by the sub-
UNDERSTANDING MEDICATION-
committee. If no legitimate explanation for a
SEEKING BEHAVIOR AND
patient's prescriptions can be identified, the
IDENTIFYING TARGETS FOR
patient is provided with one physician (usually
INTERVENTION: A
his or her primary care manager), w h o acts as
BIOPSYCHOSOCIAL MODEL
the patient's point of contact for all prescriptions. T h e patient is also provided with an
Although the paucity o f empirical literature
alternative provider (usually the head o f the
precludes the development o f a data-driven
group within which the sole provider works)
model for understanding and intervening with
for situations in which the sole provider is not
medication-seeking behavior, the existing the-
available.
oretical and clinical literature provides a start-
All providers who have previously written prescriptions
for
identified
patients
are
ing point for the development o f such a model. Specifically,
biological, psychological, and
informed o f these patients' entry into the pro-
social (including environmental) variables all
gram via written communication (i.e., confi-
play important roles in the development and
dential, closed-loop e-mail and letter). Patients
maintenance of medication seeking (e.g., the
are encouraged to address any questions or
biopsychosocial model of behavior [Engel,
concerns to their sole provider. The prescription
1 9 7 7 ] ) . As seen in Figure 1 1 . 1 , these variables
Inappropriate
Medication-Seeking
Behavior
Relevant Factors (Possible Targets for Intervention)
Source of Intervention
r
Biological Factors (-)
x
Physiological addiction Withdrawal symptoms
Health Psychologist
Figure 11.1
Ϋ
Psychological Factors
^
Coping skills Distress (-) Thrill seeking (+) History of substance use disorders Antisocial personality traits Tendencies toward somatization (+)
>v
/
Social/Environmental Factors
\ ] / / V
\
Street value of medications (+) History of drug dealing
/
\ / /
Medication-Seeking Behavior: A Framework for Understanding and Intervention
NOTE: D E A = Drug Enforcement Agency. Minus sign (-) indicates negatively reinforced factors. Plus sign (+) indicates positively reinforced factors.
may be interrelated and form both positive
associated with medication misuse, reversal o f
and negative reinforcement loops for medica-
withdrawal symptoms associated with medica-
tion-seeking behavior.
tion misuse, and/or financial gains associated
Of
greatest
relevance
to
the
health
with black market prescription
medication
psychologist are the psychological factors
sales), and a tendency to manifest psychologi-
associated with (and often reinforced by) the
cal distress as physical symptoms (as in the case
medication-seeking behavior as well as the
of somatoform disorders or subclinical somati-
role o f the prescriber (usually a physician but
zation [Longo et al., 2 0 0 0 ] ) . Interestingly, no
alternatively a nurse practitioner or another
studies that
used psychometrically
sound
prescribing health care professional) in the
assessments of patients characterized as medi-
cycle o f medication seeking.
cation seeking were discovered in the current review o f the literature. Research measures of illicit drug use, abuse, and dependence are
Patient Targets
likely too unwieldy to be clinically useful. be
Future research is needed to establish efficient,
characterized by a number o f psychological
valid, and reliable means o f assessing medica-
characteristics, including ineffective
and/or
tion-seeking behavior. Ideally, such measure-
self-
ment development will allow comparisons to
T h e medication-seeking patient may
maladaptive
coping skills (of which
medication may be just one), high levels o f
be made between baseline measures o f medica-
distress, a tendency toward thrill seeking, a
tion seeking and postintervention assessment
predisposition to addiction based on a history
of such maladaptive behaviors. Currently, the
of substance use disorders, antisocial personal-
description
ity traits (i.e., tendencies toward manipulation
be based on descriptive reports and clinical
o f patient
characteristics
must
for the patient's own gain such as pleasure
experience.
\
195
196
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH Patients m a y different
engage in a number
medication-seeking
of
behaviors.
Specifically, Parran ( 1 9 9 7 ) identified 1 1 such behaviors, including overreporting facturing
symptoms
These
A final patient behavior that is particularly
manu-
difficult to assess and to intervene with is the phenomenon o f "doctor shopping." Patients
or vague
vague
pads and forging prescriptions.
to procure prescription
medication and endorsing complaints
written by physicians or stealing prescription
or
somatic
doctor shop when they use a minimum of two
complaints.
(but often many more) physicians, medical
multiple
somatic
exaggerated
reports
often
centers, or multidisciplinary
clinics, either
include descriptions o f pain (with n o organic
professing dissatisfaction with the previous
origin), anxiety, and/or insomnia. In addition,
prescriber or claiming to have no other care
because the street value o f certain medica-
providers. Patients quickly become aware o f
tions is higher (e.g., Percocet and drugs with
which facilities, as well as which physicians,
known intoxicating effects are worth more
are less likely to identify doctor shopping.
than non-narcotic medications), and because
These psychological factors and forms o f
brand-name medications carry a higher street
medication-seeking behavior suggest numer-
value than do generic medications (perhaps
ous interventions on the part of the health psy-
because brand names are easily verified as
chologist and physician (or other prescribing
such
medication-seeking
clinician). In the chapter authors' own clinical
or
environment, health psychologists often act as
by
the
buyer),
patients may insist on specific refuse
a generic
equivalent.
cation-seeking behaviors arguments demands tions
about
medications
Additional medi-
"gatekeepers" to psychiatric medication man-
include
making
agement. Therefore, they interact
making
with medication-seeking patients and have the
medica-
opportunity to intervene relative to psycho-
pharmacology,
for multiple
prescription
(i.e., polypharmacy) or demands
controlled making tolerance
directly
for
logical factors before the patients even reach
and
the potential prescribers. In instances where
high
the patient is an ongoing patient o f the health
for the medication (and therefore
psychologist, beliefs about prescription medi-
substances assertions
on a first
that patients
visit,
have
a
need a higher or more frequent dosage). Particularly uncomfortable for the health
cation use and misuse can be identified, examined, and challenged. In addition, alternative
medication-seeking
coping strategies can be taught and practiced,
behaviors that involve excessive manipula-
relapse prevention approaches can be imple-
tion and/or dishonesty. Examples o f such
mented,
behaviors include patients w h o seek medica-
placed in the context o f existing distress.
care professional
tion via veiled
are
threats
to the prescriber, clinic
staff, or hospital and those w h o seek medication via excessive
flattery
preceding a pre-
and
somatic complaints c a n
be
Furthermore, with a therapeutic alliance in place,
the
equipped
health
psychologist
is
better
to challenge antisocial behaviors
scription request (Parran, 1 9 9 7 ) . In addition,
(e.g., manipulation) and to set clear limits
Parran and colleagues (Longo et al., 2 0 0 0 ;
regarding appropriate use o f prescribed medi-
Parran,
cation. Finally, and perhaps most important,
1 9 9 7 ) described
typical
scams
prescriptions.
in an ongoing therapeutic relationship, the
Examples o f these scams include claims that
health psychologist can (and should) be suffi-
patients spilled a bottle o f medication or that
ciently familiar with the genuine symptoms
the bottle was somehow lost or stolen. At its
and illnesses o f the patient so that the psycho-
used by patients to procure
most extreme, medication-seeking behavior
logist can identify the appropriate
may take the form o f altering prescriptions
medication in their treatment. Any appropriate
role o f
Inappropriate
Medication-Seeking
Behavior
\
pharmacological intervention may then be
addition, prescribers are encouraged to be
delivered within the context o f psychosocial
meticulous in their prescription-writing practices,
treatment (as recommended in the " B R E N D A "
taking extreme care with legibility so as to
approach, which combines medication and
decrease the likelihood o f a medicine dosage or
psychosocial treatments within the context o f
amount
addiction
adhere to rational and systematic prescribing
treatment
[Volpicelli, Pettinati,
being altered. Prescribers should
practices. In particular, caution with medicines
McLellan & O'Brien, 2 0 0 1 ] ) .
of greater abuse liability should be evident and reflected behaviorally through shorter duration
Prescriber Targets
of prescribing and more careful monitoring of
In instances where the health psychologist
use. Finally, firm limits to prescription pad
has only a brief interaction with the medica-
access must be established (Finch, 1 9 9 3 ; Longo
tion-seeking patient and/or where the health
et al., 2 0 0 0 ) .
psychologist acts as a consultant to the pre-
Early work in the health care field by the
scribing physician, educating the prescriber
Α Μ Α (1990) led to development of a descriptive
about psychological illness and the influences
taxonomy o f the "problem prescriber" known
of psychological factors on physical illness is
as the " 4 D ' s " (Longo et al., 2 0 0 0 ; Parran,
critical to a successful outcome. In addition,
1 9 9 7 ; Wesson &c Smith, 1 9 9 0 ) . According to
the health psychologist will play an important
this view, problem prescribers fall into four cat-
role in working with prescribers to more com-
egories: dated, duped, dishonest, and disabled.
fortably set limits and process conflicts related
In the chapter authors' opinion, the importance
to medication seeking with their patients as
of this system is largely historic. Since the
well as in establishing and coordinating systemic
paradigm shift brought about by Miller and
interventions to reduce medication-seeking
Rollnick's (1991) seminal work on motivational
behavior.
interviewing, the iatrogenic nature o f pejorative
A number o f suggestions for physicians that
labeling processes is understood. T h e qualities
have been published by the medical commu-
subsumed under the 4 D's can be described
nity (Council on Scientific Affairs, 1 9 8 2 ) are
more effectively as barriers to physician success
useful to the health psychologist and
can
in the management of medication seeking. For
easily extend to other prescribing targets. T o
those physicians who have fallen behind the cur-
facilitate the prevention and management o f
rent knowledge base on prescribing practices,
medication-seeking behavior, prescribing care
state-mandated continuing education can facili-
providers must first identify potential mal-
tate change. In response to those individuals
adaptive medication seeking. As noted
by
whose wish to trust their patients sometimes
Parran ( 1 9 9 7 ) , physicians can learn to recog-
overwhelms indications of potential medication
nize c o m m o n scams, including those described
misuse, introduction to and instruction in the
earlier such as the following: " I dropped my
techniques of motivational enhancement will
medicine off the sink and into the toilet. I had
provide the care provider with an empathie
plenty left. I don't know how I can afford to
stance while addressing behaviors that do not
have it refilled, but it's the only thing that
help the patient to receive optimal care. Truly
helps." Prescribers can be empowered to effec-
dishonest care providers with prescription privi-
tively confront the medication seeker, perhaps
leges are atypical based on reviews of state
tech-
registries, and legal interventions with these indi-
niques. Parran described "turning the tables on
viduals appear to be effective (e.g., Kofoed et al.,
the scammer" as an effective intervention. In
1 9 8 9 ) . Disabled prescribers must be assessed
through
motivational
enhancement
197
198
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH and treated as mandated by the ethical codes o f
Gaule, 1 9 9 9 ; M a c L e o d & Swanson, 1 9 9 6 ) .
the governing body. Thus, three interventions
Although outcome data are as yet unavailable,
for problem prescribers are (a) education,
several examples of hospital systems' efforts to
(b) training and feedback in effective interven-
implement these steps to reduce medication-
tions, and (c) support and reinforcement for
seeking behavior have been described in the
adaptive prescribing practices.
literature, and
o u t c o m e measures
should
become available in the future.
Systemwide Interventions Systemwide interventions
actually roll
outward from the individual prescriber to the office setting, where clear procedures are mandated, adhered to, and monitored by objective means. F o r instance, Finch ( 1 9 9 3 )
ASSESSMENT AND T R E A T M E N T S OF MEDICATION-SEEKING BEHAVIOR
Assessment
suggested three office-based policies, namely
In the chapter authors' opinion, the most
that (a) refills should be available through
pressing diagnostic issue is the lack of a system-
only one prescriber, (b) no refills should ever
atic means o f assessment for medication-seeking
be allowed during off-hours, and (c) a rapid
patients. Across what is available in the litera-
accurate method for tracking refills must be
ture, a number of medical and psychological
instated and maintained (Finch, 1 9 9 3 ) .
conditions exist that might prompt careful
Approximately 2 0 years ago, the Florida
assessment of the potential for medication
Medical Association and several county medi-
seeking. Just a few of the conditions with
cal societies took steps to reduce medication-
demonstrated relationships to adaptive and mal-
seeking behavior. These efforts included an
adaptive medication seeking are chronic pain
absence o f certain controlled substances being
disorders, addiction, diabetes, cancer, seizure
routinely stocked in pharmacies and a delay in
disorders, posttraumatic stress disorder, stress,
their delivery (amphetamines or methaqualones),
anxiety disorders, endometriosis, and premen-
personal verification of all prescriptions o f these
strual syndrome. Certainly, a new patient pre-
controlled substances by phone between phar-
senting with any of these concerns should be
macists and prescribers, reminders to physi-
screened for medication seeking. In addition,
cians to safeguard their prescription
patients for whom medications o f choice have
pads
(including avoiding having Drug Enforcement
significant abuse liability should be monitored
Agency numbers printed on the prescription
carefully to guard against dependence.
pads), and assistance by both pharmacy and
Given that a well-developed, psychometri-
law enforcement agencies in these efforts
cally sound assessment o f medication seeking is
(Council on Scientific Affairs, 1 9 8 2 ) .
not available, we suggest that standard clinical
Hospital-based programs are thought to be
interviews
be
augmented
by
self-report
most effective when (a) there is only one pre-
measures and biochemical validation when-
scriber per patient, (b) a systematic yet private
ever possible. First, a mini-mental status exam
marking system for charts is in place to alert
should be conducted. Patients should be asked
other health care providers to the potential for
to list all medicines they take, including over-
medication noncompliance, and (c) communi-
the-counter preparations, herbal supplements,
cation between care providers is fostered by
and teas. Alcohol use, tobacco use, and caffeine
staff meetings and scheduled team supervision
use should also be queried. Information regard-
(Council on Scientific Affairs, 1 9 8 2 ; Lewis &c
ing past use o f medications and medication
Inappropriate
Medication-Seeking
Behavior
\
reactions should be gathered. Illegal drug use,
outcome literature will likely generalize to
abuse, and dependence can be measured by the
the care of the medication-seeking patient. T h e
Drug Abuse Screening Test (Skinner, 1 9 8 2 ) .
compelling data regarding brief interventions
Risky alcohol use can be measured quickly via
delivered in motivational enhancement therapy
the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test
( M E T ) (e.g., Miller & Rollnick, 1 9 9 1 ) argue
(Reinert & Allen, 2 0 0 2 ) or even more quickly
persuasively for the use o f M E T both at the
via the C A G E (cut down, annoyed, guilty, eye
time o f the identification and assessment o f the
opener) questionnaire (Buchsbaum, Buchanan,
problem and during the initial feedback stages.
Centor, Schnoll, & Lawton, 1 9 9 1 ) . If possible,
T h e second active component in the treat-
physiological data, such as liver function tests,
ment o f medication-seeking behavior involves
could be gathered for later use in a motivational
education about the cycle o f negative rein-
enhancement session with medication-seeking
forcement, where the patient is encouraged to
patients. All assessment can be conducted under
identify misuse o f medication as allowing
the umbrella o f ensuring an accurate and thor-
escape or avoidance from unpleasant physio-
ough understanding of the patient.
logical or affective states. T h e patient can then
Such assessment procedures are commonly
be taught to see how escape or avoidance rein-
used in the W I C and frequently inform the
forces the initial state and will likely increase
development of the treatment plan. In addi-
the occurrence of the presenting symptom.
tion, this assessment procedure gathers specific
The next phase of treatment is usually aided
data to be used in the context o f the interven-
by a functional analysis of the antecedents and
tion to the patient as well as in informing the
consequences of the drug-seeking
social support network, the prescribing health
Functional analysis is presented as a means of
care provider, and the health care system.
helping the patient to identify his or her own
behavior.
"choice points for change," in keeping with both M E T and the principles of functional anal-
Treatment
ysis. T h e patient is then encouraged to make a
There are four primary goals for treatment.
plan for change that may, depending on the level
The first goal is to extinguish medication seek-
of physiological dependence to the medications
ing, as defined previously, in patients presenting
in question, involve a supervised taper and/or
for treatment. The second goal is to ensure that
medical detoxification. The patient may also
the bona fide wellness concerns of medication
accept a referral to appropriate individual and/or
seekers are adequately managed as medically
group support at this time.
indicated. The third goal is to ameliorate the
In sum, the treatment provided to the
problems of physicians whose prescribing voli-
medication-seeking patient is truly a brief inter-
tionally or involuntarily contributes to the prob-
vention that may be elaborated or pared back
lem of medication seeking. Finally, interventions
depending on the setting in which the patient
should result in sustained systemic support of
is encountered. Creative application of these
the identification and management of medica-
techniques is easily construed across settings.
tion seeking, as described earlier in the Tripler Army system (Lewis & Gaule, 1 9 9 9 ) . Strategies Strategies
for
Patients
for
Physicians
T h e overarching strategies o f behavior change are perhaps equally effective in conduct-
The chapter authors believe that the most
ing an intervention to aid physicians treating
effective treatments from the addictive behaviors
medication-seeking patients. An M E T approach (Text continues
on page
205)
199
200
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH
CASE S T U D Y "Geoffrey V a n S n e w t " was a busy, 48-year-old, married businessman. H e initially presented to a clinical health psychologist with his wife o f 2 5 years, w h o was having some menopausal difficulties. T h e couple had three children, the oldest o f w h o m was in college. T h e two younger children were in boarding schools two states away from the family h o m e . B o t h parents reported that the children were within normal limits psychosocially and were o f no inordinate concern to the parents. Neither M r . nor M r s . V a n Snewt had living parents. Both reported that the marriage had been solid, without any threats to the longevity o f the relationship. Financial difficulties were absent. T h e husband and wife owned their h o m e and could afford the tuition for all three o f their children's private educations. M r s . V a n Snewt did not w o r k outside the h o m e but volunteered in the community. Aside from the menopausal symptoms and recent depressive symptoms, neither partner complained o f health difficulties, although M r . V a n Snewt mentioned that he was not able to bounce b a c k from athletic injuries as easily and quickly as he had been able to do in the past. Both adults were active and nonsedentary. Neither partner reported smoking cigarettes, and M r . V a n Snewt denied alcohol use beyond the rare social drink while on business. M r s . V a n Snewt reported daily drinking in amounts that were uncomfortable to her and "out o f character." She was not drinking to intoxication but was concerned because she had not been a regular drinker before menopause began. M r s . V a n Snewt presented as the primary patient, although both
parties
reported extreme distress around the perceived changes in her affect and behavior. Specifically, M r s . V a n Snewt reported symptoms o f depression, including loss o f interest in shared activities, decreased libido, and increased alcohol use. T h e couple was advised to seek individual therapy for M r s . V a n Snewt while continuing the conjoint sessions with the clinical health psychologist to increase communication and decrease distress in the relationship. T h e treating clinical health psychologist made a referral for M r s . V a n Snewt to obtain individual treatment and requested and received permission to speak with M r s . V a n Snewt's gynecologist and primary care physician regarding the time course o f her complaints. A brief course o f psychoeducation regarding menopause as well as communication skillsoriented marital therapy followed, with good resolution. After five sessions, the couple felt more able t o cope with fluctuations in M r s . V a n Snewt's m o o d and activity levels, and both partners reported that their communication skills had improved. M r s . V a n Snewt continued with individual therapy and reported that it was going well. N o additional couples treatment was deemed necessary. A b o u t 3 months after treatment concluded, M r s . V a n Snewt called the clinical health psychologist and stated that she thought her spouse was "in trouble" with his pain medication. She stated that he was unwilling t o call for an appointment himself but that he would return t o the clinic if the psychologist agreed to see both parties similar t o the w o r k done when M r s . V a n Snewt had " h e r " difficulties. T h e clinical health psychologist subsequently offered the couple a conjoint assessment
Inappropriate
Medication-Seeking
Behavior
session and asked both parties to bring a list o f all current medications kept in the h o m e as well as a list o f all physicians (including their contact information). W h e n the couple arrived for their session, M r . V a n Snewt exuded
self-
confidence. H e t o o k his seat in the therapist's office and charmingly thanked the therapist for meeting with the couple so quickly. M r . V a n Snewt said, I feel such relief now that it is out on the table. I started on Percocet about a year and a half ago for the plantar fasciitis I developed from running and tennis. As being on the tennis court is imperative for business, I need the medication to keep me on the courts. I'm sure I can get back on track. I've already asked Dr. Galtry to give me a referral to a podiatrist to make me some new orthotics. Runners magazine said they are a sure cure for the problem. I came in today because when Sara found those bottles in my gear, she flew off the handle. She was better for a time, but now she's making a mountain out of a molehill. I just had prescriptions from around the country because of my hectic travel schedule. I know that Sara benefited from individual work, but she really got better when we did it together. She didn't keep seeing her psychologist after our work together stopped, and I don't see why I should have to go to individual therapy either. My plan is to have just one person give me medication, only when I really need it. M r s . V a n Snewt produced lists o f the medications that were currently in the couple's home, most o f which were unremarkable. T h e only medication with significant abuse potential was Percocet. Unfortunately, M r s . V a n Snewt had located more than 1 5 bottles o f the narcotic medication, all prescribed to M r . V a n Snewt over the past 1 8 months. M o s t bottles were empty; however, M r s . V a n Snewt was concerned because these medications came from seven different walk-in centers in the couple's county. In addition to those seven centers, one bottle (with refills) came from the family physician, and the others c a m e from drop-in clinics from cities M r . V a n Snewt had visited while on business. There was also a large bottle from M e x i c o that still contained a number o f pills. M r s . V a n Snewt said,
Geoffrey is so smooth, he can talk a hungry dog off a meat truck. Although he says he knows that this isn't normal, I'm having trouble trusting his plan. He hid this from me all this time, and I thought we had no secrets. I guess I was just too wrapped up in my own problems. I think he needs to see someone, but he is afraid that it will damage his career. I tell him that bringing drugs into the country from Mexico would damage his career more than being unable to play tennis, but he just winks and says it will be okay.
Case Conceptualization M r . V a n Snewt had developed a dependence to Percocet that was maintained by powerful, short-term, positive consequences and a fixed belief that the short-term
202
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH
consequences outweighed any possible long-term negative consequences. T h a t is, he had multiple rapid reinforcers for continued prescription drug use and abuse, including (a) the powerful pain relief offered to him by the narcotic, (b) the immediate success o f "scoring" new prescriptions, (c) the euphoric m o o d associated with narcotic intoxication that aided him during high-intensity business meetings and social interactions, and (d) the avoidance o f loss o f business "meetings" on the tennis courts. In contrast, he believed that stopping Percocet use would (a) decrease his business success, (b) deprive him o f pleasurable activities, and (c) create a perception within his marriage that he had a " p r o b l e m . " A natural strength motivating M r . V a n Snewt toward change was the potential disruption to the marital relationship. Although he did not see this as an overt mediator in his medication-seeking behaviors, the therapist contemplated the utility o f incorporating the marital relationship as a strength to be drawn on in motivating behavioral change. In addition, M r . V a n Snewt valued his status in the business world and had a strong wish to be admired by his children. T h e potential threat to his status inherent in his high-risk, medication-seeking behaviors (e.g., possessing multiple prescriptions from different prescribers, importing large quantities o f medications) was a potential place to build on the discrepancies between the patient's belief that he was not in any danger and the reality that he was. Given M r . V a n Snewt's dedication to athletic pursuits, the health psychologist thought that education about maintenance o f physical integrity might also aid in motivating M r . V a n Snewt to see the genuine potential for long-term physical damage that misusing pain medications might cause. Clearly, the system in which M r . V a n Snewt achieved successful medication procurement needed attention as well. T h e family physician, w h o appeared to be very well meaning but not well informed about the pervasiveness o f M r . V a n Snewt's use, needed t o be involved in the intervention. If necessary, the health maintenance organization ( H M O ) through which M r . V a n Snewt received his care could be called on to assist. T h e therapist completed the model in Figure 1 1 . 2 for the conceptualization o f the case and prepared "pros and c o n s " worksheets for M r . V a n Snewt to complete.
Treatments Used At the outset o f treatment, the clinical health psychologist determined that strategies o f motivational enhancement would be useful. T h e overall therapeutic framework from which the clinical health psychologist worked was similar to that described by Miller and Rollnick ( 1 9 9 1 ) , wherein therapists approach treatment as a collaborative effort with the patient. M r . V a n Snewt's ambivalence regarding changing his Percocet use was to be respected and addressed in an empathie nonjudgmental emphasized.
style. His responsibility and freedom to choose change was
Inappropriate
Medication-Seeking
Behavior
Relevant Factors (Possible Targets for Intervention)
Source of Intervention
Family Physician & Other Prescribers
7
Health Psychologist
Psychological Factors
^ \
/
Beliefs about consequences o f stopping \ P e r c o c e t use Value on athleticism ( + ) Coping skills Euphoria ( + ) Distress ( - ) Pleasure o f "scoring" prescriptions ( + ) Thrill seeking {+ ) History' of substance use disorders ν Antisocial personality traits . ^^Tendencies toward somatization (+
DEA
Figure 11.2
Case Conceptualization of Mr. Van Snewt's Medication-Seeking Behavior
N O T E : D E A = Drug Enforcement Agency; H M O = health maintenance organization. Minus sign (-) indicates negatively reinforced factors. Plus sign (+) indicates positively reinforced factors. Relevant aspects of the model are denoted in roman type and black. Aspects of the model inapplicable to this conceptualization are in italic type and gray.
T h e first session was framed as an assessment session, during which the clinical health psychologist led the couple through a series o f questions regarding perceived use, reviewed the medications listed, and gathered contact information for prescribers. W h i l e the patient and his wife were present, the clinical health psychologist contacted the primary care physician, w h o confirmed that M r . V a n Snewt had told him he was having foot problems, had received one prescription for Percocet (refilled twice), and had recently requested a referral for an assessment with a podiatrist. T h e clinical health psychologist advised the physician that the patient and his wife were present in her office and had given consent for the psychologist to disclose that the patient was overusing his prescription and had received prescriptions from multiple sources. T h e physician then stated that it was likely that the patient could manage a slow taper off the medication and that he would see the patient to begin the taper that afternoon. T h e patient was offered an appointment
but
declined, citing other commitments. T h e physician was thanked and was assured that communication between treatment providers would continue, with the full consent o f M r . V a n Snewt.
204
BEHAVIORS THAT C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH
At the close o f the assessment session, the conceptual model was presented to M r . and M r s . V a n Snewt, with the therapist highlighting the particular choice points for change, including assessment o f actual use, biochemical validation (random and scheduled), continued contact and contracting with the clinical health psychologist, and setting up o f a variety o f contingency contracts designed to encourage change. T h e couple was encouraged to contemplate the feedback delivered regarding its medication-seeking patterns and health status and was encouraged to m a k e decisions from an informed perspective. T h e menu o f alternative change methods from which the V a n Snewts could choose was offered. T h e V a n Snewts were also provided with information about medically supervised inpatient detoxification in case it proved to be necessary. In keeping with M E T and M r . V a n Snewt's stated goals, the therapist's task was to reflect those goals back to the patient by way o f recapitulation, reality test their likelihood, and begin to identify the things that would have t o change for success to occur. The V a n Snewts accepted another appointment within one business week but canceled the appointment several days later, stating that an unexpected visit from one o f their children would preclude their attendance. At the time o f cancellation, M r s . V a n Snewt scheduled a new appointment for the following week. M r s . V a n Snewt attended the rescheduled appointment, reportedly expecting t o meet her husband at the clinical health psychologist's office. M r . V a n Snewt telephoned his wife on her mobile phone 1 0 minutes after the appointment was scheduled to begin, explaining that he was running late at w o r k and that she should "go a h e a d " without him. Because M r . and M r s . V a n Snewt were seeing the clinical health psychologist as a couple, the session was ended at that time. Before leaving the office, M r s . V a n Snewt and the clinical health psychologist scheduled an additional appointment for the couple. T h e morning o f the appointment, M r s . V a n Snewt called the clinical health psychologist, explaining in a voice mail message that the problem had been "solved" and that the couple would no longer need to see her. She continued to say that the family physician had tapered M r . V a n Snewt off the Percocet and had refused to provide any additional prescriptions. M r s . V a n Snewt explained that she was pleased with this outcome and was certain that M r . V a n Snewt was "cured." M r . V a n Snewt did not contact the clinical health psychologist again.
Problems Encountered As can be ascertained from this case example, a substantial systemic challenge exists when treating patients w h o seek and use prescribed controlled substances inappropriately. Although such patients may exist within a relatively integrated system o f care (e.g., a large veterans hospital or other medical center), such individuals often have multiple, relatively disconnected health care providers. W h e n the
Inappropriate
Medication-Seeking
Behavior
j
possibility o f cyber-prescriptions is entered into the equation, systemic intervention becomes even more challenging. In this situation, it is essentially impossible to create a treatment team united in the prevention o f medication seeking and prescription drug abuse. In additional to issues at a m a c r o level, m o r e personal factors often play a significant role in the treatment o f medication-seeking patients. In this case, M r . V a n Snewt's ambivalence regarding the extent to which his use o f Percocet was problematic, as well as M r s . V a n Snewt's investment in believing her husband and avoiding conflict in the marital relationship, intersected in a way that ultimately precluded intervention with the couple directly. Although ambivalence regarding change is a normative characteristic o f most patients early in treatment and could have been managed optimally using an M E T approach, the couple's presence was obviously a prerequisite for this to occur. Finally, individual and systemic challenges intersect in this case, where M r . V a n Snewt's considerable financial and w o r k success, high status in the community, and personal charm disarmed potential prescribers. In thinking o f "medication-seeking patients," M r . V a n Snewt is unlikely to fit the image conjured up by such professionals. Therefore, these prescribers were likely caught off-guard and were easily convinced o f sincere physical pain and the "appropriateness" o f providing prescriptions for controlled substances to this "upstanding responsible" man.
will ensure that the physician does not perceive
controlled drug prescription,
the health psychologist as critical. Education
careful documentation with a firm diagnosis and
teaching
in
about the pattern of negative reinforcement for
the ruling out of chemical dependency,
both the patient and the physician can be con-
practice. This training should be supervised
and
ducted. Specifically, a functional analysis of the
and have ample role-playing
conditions in which prescribers are effective ver-
Physicians are also encouraged to enlist the sup-
opportunities.
sus when they are ineffective will help to clarify
port of all the pharmacists given that pharma-
the behavior for the physician, both in terms o f
cists are liable for filling controlled prescriptions
the patient process of negative reinforcement
along with the physicians who wrote them.
and in terms of the physician process of negative reinforcement. Finally, the physician needs to have easily accessible information
regarding
treatment referrals at his or her disposal. The
Α Μ Α encourages physicians
who
Systemic The
Interventions
systemic interventions, as change.
outlined
are treating medication seekers to have addi-
here, are likely to effect
tional training. Such training should include
research is needed to address the particular
Future
the acquisition and use o f chemical depen-
effects o f each program as well as the effects
dence screening skills, practice in early and
o f the individual components that make up
firm limit setting regarding indications for
each program.
205
206
BEHAVIORS THAT C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH CONCLUSIONS
Biochemical validation o f current use should be considered in the development of such mea-
The most compelling conclusion to be drawn
sures. M a n y interesting questions regarding
from the existing literature and the chapter
this project are possible. Specifically, can med-
authors' clinical experience with the problem
ication seeking be operationally defined and
of medication seeking is that the knowledge
accurately diagnosed in an efficient and effec-
base is extremely
tive manner? Is there a means o f taking the
limited. Indeed, health care
professionals have no true sense o f the prevalence and incidence o f medication seeking
guesswork out o f it? Second, psychometrically sound assess-
itself. Neither are they currently equipped to
ments o f prescribing practices should
describe the financial, physical, and emotional
developed and implemented. W i t h national
be
costs o f this behavior. Health care profession-
databases o f prescription medications easily
als can follow good clinical practice, but they
within the purview o f the established medical
have no means o f establishing the utility o f
community and the experience o f existing
programs designed to deal with this problem.
systems designed to track such measures, this
The state o f the current literature is that they
possibility should not be difficult.
must begin anew. T o that end, this chapter concludes with the following suggestions. First, psychometrically sound assessments of medication seeking need to be developed.
Finally, educational programs to assist in the identification o f inappropriate seeking, as well as inappropriate prescribing, need to be evaluated systematically.
REFERENCES American Medical Association. (1988). Balancing the response to prescription drug abuse: Proceedings from the Second White House ΑΜΑ Symposium on preChicago: American Medical Association, scribing controlled substances. Department of Substance Abuse. American Medical Association. (1990). Balancing the response to prescription drug abuse: Report of a national symposium on medicine and public policy. Chicago: American Medical Association, Department of Substance Abuse. Buchsbaum, D. G., Buchanan, R. G., Centor, R. M., Schnoll, S. H., & Lawton, M. J . (1991). Screening for alcohol use using CAGE scores and likelihood ratios. Annals of Internal Medicine, 115, 7 7 4 - 7 7 7 . Cooper, J . R., Czechowicz, D. J . , Molinari, S. P., & Petersen, R. C. (Eds.). (1993). Impact of prescription drug diversion control systems on medical practice and patient care (NIDA Research Monograph No. 131). Bethesda, M D : National Institute on Drug Abuse. Council on Scientific Affairs. (1982). Drug abuse related to prescribing practices. Journal of the American Medical Association, 247, 8 6 4 - 8 6 6 . Engel, G. L. (1977). The need for a new medical model: A challenge for biomedicine. Science, 196, 1 2 9 - 1 3 6 . Finch, J . (1993). Prescription drug abuse. Primary Care, 20, 2 3 1 - 2 3 9 . Gold, M . S., Miller, N. S., Stennie, K., & Populla-Vardi, C. (1995). Epidemiology of benzodiazepine use and dependence. Psychiatric Annals, 25, 1 4 6 - 1 4 8 . Kofoed, L., Bloom, J . D., Williams, M. H., Rhyne, C , & Resnick, M . (1989). Physicians investigated for inappropriate prescribing by the Oregon Board of Medical Examiners. Western Journal of Medicine, 150, 5 9 7 - 6 0 1 .
Inappropriate
Medication-Seeking
Lewis, P., 8c Gaule, D. (1999). Dealing with drug-seeking patients: The Tripler Army Medical Center experience. Military Medicine, 164, 8 3 8 - 8 4 0 . Longo, J . P., Parran, T., Johnson, B . , & Kinsey, W. (2000). Addiction: II. Identification and management of the drug-seeking patients. American Family Physician, 61, 2 4 0 1 - 2 4 0 8 . MacLeod, B. D., & Swanson, R. (1996). A new approach to chronic pain in the ED. American Journal of Emergency Medicine, 14, 3 2 3 - 3 2 6 . Maronde, R. F., Seibert, S., &c Katzoff, J . (1972). Prescription data processing: Its role in the control of drug abuse. California Medicine, 117, 2 2 - 2 8 . Miller, W. R., & Rollnick, S. (1991). Motivational interviewing. New York: Guilford. National Institute on Drug Abuse. (2001). Prescription drugs: Abuse and addiction (Research Report Series, NIH Publication No. 0 1 - 4 8 8 1 ) . Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, National Institutes of Health, National Institute on Drug Abuse. Nichol, M . B., Venturini, F., & Sung, J . C. (1999). A critical evaluation of the methodology of the literature on medication compliance. Annals of Pharmacotherapy, 33, 5 3 1 - 5 3 5 . Pankratz, L., Hickam, D. H., & Toth, S. (1989). The identification and management of drug-seeking behavior in a medical center. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 24, 1 1 5 - 1 1 8 . Parran, T. P. (1997). Prescription drug abuse: A question of balance. Medical Clinics of North America, 81, 9 6 7 - 9 7 8 . Poling, Α., & Byrne, T. (2000). Introduction to behavioral pharmacology. Reno, NV: Context Press. Reinert, D. F., Sc Allen, J . P. (2002). The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): A review of recent research. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 23, 2 7 2 - 2 7 9 . Salzman, C. (1990). Benzodiazepine dependence, toxicity, and abuse: A task force report of the American Psychiatric Association (Task Force on Benzodiazepine Dependency). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Silberstein, S. D. (1992). Evaluation and emergency treatment of headache. Headache, 32, 3 9 6 - 4 0 7 . Singh, B . S. (1998). Managing somatoform disorders. Medical Journal of Australia, 168, 5 7 2 - 5 7 7 . Skinner, H. A. (1982). The Drug Abuse Screening Test. Addictive Behaviors, 7, 363-371. Taverner, D., Dodding, C. J . , &C White, J . M. (2000). Comparison of methods for teaching clinical skills in assessing and managing drug-seeking patients. Medical Education, 34, 2 8 5 - 2 9 1 . Volpicelli, J . R., Pettinati, H. M., McLellan, A. T., & O'Brien, C. P. (2001). The BRENDA method: Combining medication and psychosocial treatments for addiction treatment. New York: Guilford. Weiss, R. D., &c Greenfield, D. P. (1986). Prescription drug abuse. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 9, 4 7 5 - 4 9 0 . Wesson, D. R., & Smith, D. E. (1990). Prescription drug abuse: Patient, physician, and cultural responsibilities. Western Journal of Medicine, 152, 6 1 3 - 6 1 6 .
Behavior
12 10
CHAPTER
Adherence to Medical Recommendations NICOLE E . BERLANT AND SHERI D . PRUITT
T
he
failure
to
adhere
to
medical
adherence
appear
to
be
insurmountable.
recommendations is a significant and
Fortunately, behavioral science offers valuable
multifaceted
principles, theories, and models that address
health
care
problem.
Estimates are that 3 0 % to 7 0 % o f patients
the determinants
do not fully adhere to the medical advice o f
providers, and organizations (Pruitt, 2 0 0 1 ) .
o f behaviors of patients,
their physicians (National Heart, Lung, and
These same principles, theories, and models
Blood Institute, 1 9 9 8 ) . M o r e o v e r , up to 8 0 %
from behavioral science provide a conceptual
o f patients are unsuccessful in following rec-
framework for organizing current knowledge,
ommendations for behavioral changes such
testing future
as smoking cessation and dietary restrictions.
multilevel interventions for improving patients'
Adherence is a complex behavioral process
hypotheses, and
developing
adherence to medical recommendations.
that is determined largely by environmental influences on the patient. However, the patient's
DEFINITIONS AND
environment is broad, extending beyond his or
DESCRIPTION OF ADHERENCE
her immediate surroundings to encompass associated health care providers and the health care
208
Although adherence has been defined as the
organization in which the patient receives ser-
extent to which patient behavior corresponds
vices. Nonadherence is far more than a patient
with recommendations from a health care
problem, and efforts that focus solely on the
provider (Rand, 1 9 9 3 ; Vitolins, R a n d , Rapp,
patient impede the ability to make meaningful
Ribisl, &c Sevick, 2 0 0 0 ) , this broad definition
advances in the adherence arena.
belies the complexity of the issue. Adherence
The expansion of the conceptualization o f
is better conceptualized as an acceptable fre-
adherence to include multiple levels (i.e., patients,
quency, intensity, and/or accuracy o f specific
providers, and health care organizations), and
behaviors, given a specific circumstance, that
the realization that there are multiple influences
is associated with
on a patient's behavior within each o f these
comes. Adherence is a process—a behavioral
improved
clinical
out-
levels, could make the challenge of improving
means to the end point o f better health status.
Adherence T h e process of adherence is influenced by multiple determinants. These include a variety of factors at the patient, provider, and health
to Medical
Recommendations
209
patients are asked to follow some degree of recommendations for all medical conditions. W i t h the possible exception o f psychiatric
care organization levels. Factors at the patient
disorders,
level consist of a person's knowledge and beliefs
particular condition o r condition regimen
adherence is not specific to a
about illness, degree o f motivation and self-
(Haynes, 1 9 7 9 ) . Basic behavioral principles
efficacy related to illness management behav-
and models o f behavior change cut across all
iors, and expectancies related to the outcomes
medical conditions, rendering a review o f
of adherence or nonadherence. In addition,
each specific condition less important than
disease- and treatment-specific issues influence
a grasp o f the c o m m o n themes. M o r e o v e r ,
adherence at the patient level. These include
increasing the effectiveness o f
(a) the social, physical, psychological, and
interventions may have a far greater impact
adherence
occupational disabilities resulting from symp-
on the health o f the population than would
toms and treatments; (b) the cultural mean-
any improvements in specific medical treat-
ings o f diseases and treatments; (c) the disease
ments (Haynes et al., 2 0 0 2 ) .
severity and prognosis; and (d) the complexity, timing, and
degree o f beneficial and
detrimental effects o f treatments.
Although adherence traditionally is discussed as a patient problem (e.g., " T h e patient is nonadherent"), the time has c o m e to con-
Influential factors at the provider level
sider the adherence issue within the larger
encompass the knowledge, skills, and attitudes
health care organization context. Adherence is
of health care providers as well as the quality
a multifaceted construct that spans patients'
of the patient-provider relationship. Health
medical problems, but providers
care organization factors involve characteris-
health care organization must be considered
tics o f the organization such as access to diag-
as similarly responsible for improving adher-
nostic and treatment services, education to
ence rates. Clarifying our conceptualization o f
manage health problems, coordination and
the construct could serve to forward interven-
and
the
integration o f care, and organizational links to
tion efforts. Better interventions are impera-
community support services (World Health
tive because if significant improvements in
Organization, 2 0 0 1 ) .
adherence are not made, many medical treat-
The need for a clear conceptualization o f the adherence construct becomes even more
ments and providers' therapeutic efforts will continue to be inefficient.
evident when measurement and intervention strategies are taken into consideration. For example, provider recommendations
range
from advice that requires relatively simple and
THE ROLE OF BEHAVIORAL S C I E N C E ΓΝ A D H E R E N C E
familiar behaviors (e.g., requests for patients to return for follow-up appointments or to obtain
T h e influence of behavior on health has never
inoculations) to recommendations that patients
been as apparent as it is today. M o s t people
participate in complex and novel regimens (e.g.,
living in this century will die from "lifestyle
daily alterations in diet, smoking cessation,
illnesses" or medical conditions resulting from
increase in physical activity). Measurement and
daily habits such as diet, exercise, alcohol con-
intervention strategies obviously differ accord-
sumption, tobacco use, and/or health risk
ing to the circumstances and/or intensity of the
behaviors (Kaplan, Sallis, & Patterson, 1 9 9 3 ) .
recommendations. Nevertheless, it is important
In fact, current estimates indicate that daily
to note that adherence is fundamental
for
behavior contributes as much as 5 0 % to an
successful management of health problems, and
individual's overall health status (Institute for
210
BEHAVIORS THAT C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH the Future, 2 0 0 0 ) . Decades o f behavioral
F r o m a theoretical standpoint, it would be
research provide proven strategies for chang-
possible to "control" behavior of patients,
ing behavior. Integrating
providers, and health care organizations if the
this
knowledge
within the medical arena can help patients to
events preceding
and
following a specific
alter their daily patterns and reduce the risks
behavior could be controlled. F r o m a practical
associated with the development o f health
standpoint, behavioral principles can be used
problems and/or negative consequences o f
to design interventions that have the potential
preestablished conditions. Moreover, behavior
to shape behavior at each incremental level o f
change strategies can be applied across diverse
influence (patient, provider, and health care
medical conditions (Dunbar-Jacob, Burke, &
organization) to address adherence problems.
Pyczynski, 1 9 9 5 ; Nessman,
Carnahan,
&
Nugent, 1 9 8 0 ) . In addition, the same learning
Behavioral interventions
based on
the
basic principles o f learning (i.e., antecedents
principles that are effective in changing patient
and
behavior are effective in altering the behavior
changing behavior. N e w behaviors can be
of health care providers ( O x m a n , Thomson,
learned, and established behaviors can be
Davis, & Haynes, 1 9 9 5 ) and health care orga-
increased or decreased using these concepts.
nizations (DeBusk et al., 1 9 9 4 ) .
In fact, a variety o f health behaviors have
c o n s e q u e n c e s ) are
quite
potent
for
Behavioral science is the application of
been altered successfully using some varia-
experimental methods to learn about, predict,
tion o f operant-based techniques (Brownell
and explain the observable actions of humans,
&
including not only observable behaviors but
1 9 7 4 ) . In general, behavioral change strate-
also verbal statements about subjective experi-
gies that focus on what occurs before and
ences and symptoms. M u c h o f human behav-
after targeted behaviors have been substan-
ior has been well understood for decades. T h e
tially more effective than other approaches.
most basic yet most powerful principle is the
Cohen,
1 9 9 5 ; Janis,
1 9 8 3 ; Mahoney,
The most effective methods for changing
influence o f antecedents and consequences on
behavior are those that teach individuals how
behavior, that is, operant learning (Skinner,
to integrate the basic principles o f learning into
1 9 3 8 , 1 9 5 3 ) . Antecedents, or preceding events,
their daily lives (Bandura 8c Simon, 1 9 7 7 ;
are internal (thoughts) or external (environ-
Matarazzo,
1 9 8 0 ) . This practice is called
mental cues) circumstances that elicit a behav-
"self-management,"
ior. Consequences, or expected consequences,
change," or "self-regulation." Specific tech-
"self-directed
behavior
that can be conceptualized as rewards or pun-
niques for changing and maintaining one's
ishments also influence behavior. These princi-
health behavior include self-monitoring, goal
ples translate into the ability to predict the
setting, stimulus control, self-reinforcement,
probability o f a patient, provider, or health
behavioral rehearsal, arranging social support,
care organization initiating or continuing a
behavioral contracting, and relapse prevention.
behavior because such behaviors
partially
The use of behavioral principles in the area of
depend on what happens before and after the
relapse prevention has been particularly well
specific behavior occurs. Although
learning
studied in response to the ubiquitously high
theory historically has been criticized for
rates o f relapse after health behavior changes
explaining behavior in overly simplistic "stim-
such as smoking cessation, reduction in alco-
ulus-response"
relationships,
contemporary
hol consumption, and weight loss. Research in
learning theory integrates environmental cues
relapse prevention has demonstrated the signif-
and contexts, memory, expectancies, and neu-
icance of contextual cues (e.g., physical envi-
rological processes related to learning (Institute
ronment, time of day, emotional status) in the
of Medicine, 2 0 0 1 ) .
maintenance or extinction o f health behaviors.
Adherence Although the fundamental
to Medical
Recommendations
principles o f
behavioral changes are predicated on the belief
learning and behavior change appear to be
that a person can successfully complete a
simple, they are deceptively so. Behavior change
desired behavior. This belief is necessary even
and maintenance continues to be an enigma,
when all other predictors o f behavior would
and even the best behavioral techniques are not
suggest that a person is ready to engage in a
invariably effective. Nevertheless, a century o f
particular behavior. For example, a person can
behavioral science remains the strongest foun-
feel vulnerable to a disease, understand how
dation to guide current efforts in changing
to change his or her behavior to be healthier,
behavior to improve health.
believe that the new behavior will decrease the behavioral
likelihood o f illness, and feel supported by the
science into physical health was recognized
social environment. However, if the person
The
obligation to integrate
2 5 years ago when the interdisciplinary field of
lacks conviction in his or her ability to change,
behavioral medicine was formally
the social cognitive theory predicts that the per-
defined
(Schwartz &c Weiss, 1 9 7 7 ) . T h e adherence
son is unlikely to be successful.
problem is ideally attacked from this interdisci-
The construct regarding a person's belief
plinary perspective. In fact, researchers in the
about ability to change is called "perceived
behavioral medicine arena have
developed
self-efficacy" and is modified by four sources
sophisticated models that envelop the basic
of information: (a) performance attainment or
principles o f learning and apply them to com-
success with previous tasks, (b) vicarious expe-
plex health behaviors.
rience or watching others perform a task, (c) verbal persuasion, and (d) physiological states such as anxiety and relaxation. T h e social
MODELS FOR
cognitive theory predicts that
UNDERSTANDING
influences adherence by mediating behaviors in
self-efficacy
multiple ways, including immediate behavior
ADHERENCE BEHAVIOR
choices, effort expenditure, thoughts, emoTheories and models provide a conceptual
tional reactions, and behavior performance.
framework
about
There is empirical support for the theory as it
adherence and other health behaviors. Over the
relates to adherence and the maintenance of
for organizing thoughts
past 1 0 0 years, numerous theories about behav-
behavior change, but Bandura's theory is
ior have been proposed. This section briefly
limited by the complexity o f quantifying the
reviews some of the more influential models: the
conceptual elements in the model.
social cognitive theory (Bandura & Simon, 1 9 7 7 ) , the theory of reasoned action (Ajzen & Fishbein,
1980),
the
health
belief
model
(Rosenstock, 1 9 7 4 ) , the theory o f interpersonal
Theory of Reasoned Action The
theory
of
reasoned
action
was
behavior (Triandis, 1 9 7 7 ) , the transtheoretical
introduced in an attempt to explain the rela-
model (Prochaska & DiClemente, 1 9 8 2 ) , and
tionships among beliefs (normative and behav-
the information-motivation-behavioral skills
ioral), attitudes, intentions, and
(1MB) model (Fisher & Fisher, 1 9 9 2 ; Fisher,
According to this theory, an individual's intent
Fisher, Miscovich, Kimble, & Malloy, 1 9 9 6 ) .
to adopt a behavior is determined by his or her
behaviors.
attitude about performing the behavior and
Social Cognitive Theory
social factors such as the perception o f attitudes about the behavior held by significant
Bandura's social cognitive theory (Bandura,
others (Ajzen &c Fishbein, 1 9 8 0 ) . Ajzen and
1 9 8 2 ; Bandura & Simon, 1 9 7 7 ) suggests that
Fishbein ( 1 9 8 0 ) suggested that attitude toward
212
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH a behavior is a much better predictor o f that
and cons o f adopting a behavior, an affective
behavior than is attitude toward the disease
analysis o f previous positive and
that is associated with the behavior. F o r
experiences, the social influence o f normative
example,
attitude t o w a r d
negative
mammography
and role beliefs, and personal beliefs about
should be a better predictor o f screening
responsibility for one's health. Habit influ-
behavior than is attitude toward breast cancer.
ences the likelihood o f behavior when regular
Personality and sociocultural variables influ-
behaviors become automatic, at which point
ence the likelihood of adopting a behavior by
the role o f intention is reduced.
mediating the attitudes of the individual and of his or her significant others. Both the degree of influence imposed by the person's attitude and his or her perception o f other's beliefs vary with each behavior.
Transtheoretical Model T h e transtheoretical model (Prochaska & DiClemente, 1 9 8 2 ) proposes the "stages of change" framework as a comprehensive model of behavioral change in both the positive and
Health Belief Model
negative directions (e.g., the acquisition of a 8c
health behavior such as exercise, the reduction
M a i m a n , 1 9 7 5 ; Rosenstock, 1 9 7 4 ) integrates
or cessation o f a risk behavior such as smok-
The
health
belief
model
(Becker
behavioral and cognitive theories to explain
ing). Originally developed as a smoking cessa-
why people fail to adhere to health behaviors
tion tool, the transtheoretical model has been
by considering the impact o f the consequences
applied to psychotherapy and a variety o f
and expectations related to the behaviors.
health behaviors as a way o f matching individ-
Specifically, the probability that a person will
uals to their stage o f preparedness for behavior
adopt or maintain a behavior to prevent or
change. Cross-sectional studies have supported
control a disease depends on four things: (a)
the existence of five stages (precontemplation,
perceived susceptibility, (b) perceived threat,
contemplation, preparation, action, and main-
(c) perceived benefits, and (d) perceived barri-
tenance), but some longitudinal studies have
ers. Perceived susceptibility describes the
not identified these discrete stages. In addition,
perception o f risk to personal health, whereas
a recent investigation has reported the stages to
perceived threat describes the proportion of
be w e a k predictors o f smoking cessation
negative consequences o f disease. Perceived
(Institute of Medicine, 2 0 0 1 ) . Nevertheless, the
benefits and barriers describe the beliefs about
finding that many people have low levels of
the outcomes of recommended behaviors in
motivation for behavior change has led to
reducing the perceived threat. It should be
interventions specifically intended to increase
noted that the health belief model highlights
motivation
the fact that adherence to health behaviors
( 2 0 0 2 ) motivational interviewing strategies.
such as Miller and
Rollnick's
often requires people to act to prevent illness
(A more detailed discussion of motivational
even while they are still healthy.
interviewing was provided in Chapter 4.)
Theory of Interpersonal Behavior
Information-MotivationBehavioral Skills Model
The
theory
o f interpersonal
behavior
(Triandis, 1 9 7 7 ) interrelates a person's inten-
Each o f the theories just discussed has advan-
tion to perform a behavior, facilitating condi-
tages and disadvantages, yet none of them read-
tions, and habit. In this model, intention is
ily translates into a comprehensive intervention
mediated by a cognitive analysis o f the pros
for changing health behavior. The recently
Adherence
to Medical
Recommendations
213
information-motivation-behavioral
support for such behavior, and the patient's
skills model (Fisher 8c Fisher, 1 9 9 2 ; Fisher et al.,
subjective norm or perception o f how others
1996) borrows elements from the earlier theo-
with this medical condition might behave.
developed
ries to construct a conceptually based, generaliz-
"Behavioral skills" includes ensuring that the
able, and parsimonious model to guide thinking
patient has the specific behavioral tools or
about complex health behaviors. Subjected
strategies necessary to perform the adherence
to rigorous empirical investigation, interven-
behavior such as enlisting social support and
tions based on this model have demonstrable
other self-regulation strategies. Finally, infor-
efficacy in effecting behavioral change across
mation, motivation, and behavioral skills must
a
pertain directly to the desired behavioral out-
variety o f clinical applications
(Carey,
Kalichman, Forsyth, Wright, & Johnson, 1 9 9 7 ;
come; that is, they must be specific.
Fisher & Fisher, 1 9 9 2 ; Fisher et al., 1 9 9 6 ) . In
M u c h o f the adherence research and inter-
both prospective and correlational studies, the
ventions applies individual components of the
information, motivation, and behavioral skills
1MB model despite evidence that all three
constructs have accounted for an average of
elements are necessary for complex behavior
3 3 % of the variance in behavior change (Fisher
change. T h e failure to explicitly implement
et al., 1 9 9 6 ) .
information, motivation, and behavioral skills
The 1MB model, similar to what has been
may be partially attributed to the commonsen-
reported previously, demonstrates that infor-
sical nature o f the model. Health care providers
mation is a prerequisite but that information
often assume that they provide information to
in itself is insufficient to alter behavior (see
patients and motivate them, and providers also
Mazzuca, 1 9 8 2 ) . It provides evidence that
recognize the importance of behavioral skills in
motivation and behavioral skills are critical
improving health. However, there is evidence
determinants that are independent of behavior
that providers typically give limited informa-
change (Fisher 8c Fisher, 1 9 9 2 ; Fisher etal.,
tion (Waitzkin 8c Stoeckle, 1 9 7 6 ) , lack motiva-
1 9 9 6 ) . Information
enhancement
abilities
(Botelho
8c
and motivation
work
tional
largely through behavioral skills to
affect
Skinner, 1 9 9 5 ) , and lack the knowledge (often
behavior. However, when the behavioral skills
leading to frustration)
are familiar or uncomplicated, information and
behavioral skills (Alto, 1 9 9 5 ) .
motivation can have direct effects on behavior.
The
expense
of
in teaching patients intensively
educating
In this case, a patient might fill a prescription (a
physicians to improve their information dis-
simple familiar behavior) based on information
semination, motivational interviewing,
given by the provider. T h e relationship between
behavioral skills training may be prohibitive
the information and motivation constructs is
when
weak. In practical terms, a highly motivated
strategies for improving patient
person may have little information, or a highly
However, creative education strategies, such
informed person may have low motivation.
as distance learning techniques, have been
However, in the 1MB model, the presence o f
used successfully to train physicians in basic
both information and motivation increases the
behavioral concepts to influence adherence
likelihood o f complex behavior change.
considering
(Casebeer, K l a p o w ,
efficient
Centor,
and
and
effective adherence.
Stafford, 8c
T o this point, each o f the components of the
Skrinar, 1 9 9 9 ) . Training less expensive or
1MB model has been described. "Information"
more readily available providers in the applica-
consists of basic knowledge about a medical
tion of the 1 M B model may be a more viable
condition and effective strategies for managing
option. Pharmacists, case managers, health
it. "Motivation" encompasses personal atti-
educators, and any persons involved in patient
tudes toward the behavior, perceived social
care should be exposed to these basic concepts.
2U
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH Nonphysician providers have an incredibly
most efficacious patient-focused interventions do
important role and opportunity to improve sig-
not yield substantial effects for adherence behav-
nificantly the health of their patients by specif-
ior over the long term (Haynes, McKibbon, &c
ically targeting patient adherence issues.
Kanani, 1 9 9 6 ) , and few randomized controlled
M o r e structured, thoughtful, and sophisticated provider-patient interactions are essential
trials targeting patient adherence behavior exist (Haynes et al., 2 0 0 2 ) .
if improvements in adherence are to be realized.
A recent review o f the long-term manage-
The generalizable 1MB model can be applied to
ment of obesity (Perri, 1 9 9 8 ) described many
providers to meet this goal. As this empirically
of these techniques by examining the status of
based model predicts, when providers have
research concerning adherence and relapse
adequate information, motivation, and behav-
prevention in weight management. A number
ioral skills, they will integrate new behaviors
of strategies to increase adherence to weight
into their practices. Adapted to an organiza-
control behaviors have been investigated,
tional level, the same 1MB framework can be
including continuing therapist contact, formal
used to change the behavior of decision makers
relapse prevention training, monetary incen-
and administrators toward improved health
tives, low-calorie food provision, and peer
care organization functioning.
support. Intensive behavioral therapist contact (beyond
6 months)
has
been
repeatedly
demonstrated to prolong the maintenance o f STATE-OF-THE-ART
weight loss, although contact does not result
INTERVENTIONS FOR
in greater weight loss (Perri, 1 9 9 8 ) .
IMPROVING ADHERENCE
Relapse prevention training has been used
Adherence intervention research has focused largely on patient behavior and medication regimens as opposed to targeting provider and health care organization variables. According to several published adherence reviews, no single intervention targeting patient behavior is effective, and the most promising methods o f improving adherence behavior use a combination of the following strategies
(Houston-
Miller, Hill, Kottke, & Ockene, 1 9 9 7 ; Haynes et al., 2 0 0 2 ; Roter et al., 1 9 9 8 ) : • • • • •
Patient education (Morisky et a l , 1983) Behavioral skills (Oldridge & Jones, 1983; Swain & Steckel, 1981) Self-rewards (Mahoney, Moura, & Wade, 1973) Social support (Daltroy & Godin, 1989) Telephone follow-up (Taylor, HoustonMiller, Killen, & DeBusk, 1990)
successfully as part of a multicomponent maintenance program
(Perri, Shapiro, Ludwig,
Twentyman, & M c A d o o , 1 9 8 4 ) . Marlatt and Gordon's
(1985)
"relapse prevention"
is
defined as a set of techniques designed to keep people from relapsing to prior health habits after initial successful behavior modification, including training and coping skills for highrisk relapse situations and lifestyle rebalancing (Taylor, 1 9 9 5 ) . In addition, peer support meetings have been associated with greater weight loss maintenance over time. However, neither of these components has been shown to lead to greater behavioral adherence (Perri et al., 1 9 8 7 ) . Multicomponent maintenance programs lead to greater sustained weight loss than does standard care, but within these programs continued therapist contact appears to be the key component. Perri (1998) hypothesized that the improved outcomes seen in extended treatment programs are due to the maintained
Various combinations of these techniques
adherence to behavior changes. This extended
have been shown to increase adherence behavior
adherence is likely secondary to the ongoing
and treatment outcomes. However, even the
effects o f the social pressure o f groups, repeated
Adherence
to Medical
Recommendations
215
eating and exercise,
associations with patient health outcomes have
continued therapist reinforcement and problem
been demonstrated. In a review of randomized
cues for "appropriate"
solving, and sustained motivation and morale
controlled trials, Stewart (1996) reported that
from continued therapeutic support.
providers who share information, build partnerships, and provide emotional support to their patients have better outcomes than do providers
Adherence Interventions at the Patient Level
who do not interact with patients in this manner. Correlational studies reveal a direct
M o s t people have difficulty in adhering
relationship between patient adherence
and
to medical recommendations, especially when
provider communication styles that include pro-
the
care.
viding information, engaging in "positive talk,"
Consequently, patient characteristics have
and asking patients specific questions about
advice entails self-administered
been the focus o f numerous adherence investi-
adherence (Hall, Roter, & Katz, 1 9 8 8 ) . Patient
gations. Efforts to identify stable personality
satisfaction also plays a role in that those who
traits of the "nonadherent patient" have been
are satisfied with their providers and medical
futile. However, mental health problems have
regimens adhere to recommendations more
been examined in recent reports, and there
diligently (Whitcher-Alagna, 1 9 8 3 ) . Finally,
is evidence that depression and anxiety are
patients who view themselves as partners
predictive of adherence to medical recommen-
engaged in their treatment plans have better
dations
adherence
(Chesney, Chrisman,
Luftey, &c
Pescosolido, 1 9 9 9 ; D i M a t t e o , Lepper, Croghan, Ziegelstein
2000; et
Lustman al.,
2000).
et
&
behavior
and
health
outcomes
(Schulman, 1 9 7 9 ) .
al., 1 9 9 5 ;
O n e example of the potential for providers
Interestingly,
to affect health behavior change is the use of
providers historically have attributed
adher-
"minimal contact" interventions in primary
ence problems to patients' personalities (Davis,
care to help patients quit smoking. Advice or
1 9 6 6 ) or attitudes (Stone, 1 9 7 9 ) . It may be that
counseling alone produces increased 6- and 1 2 -
providers are detecting mental health prob-
month quit rates in biochemically validated
lems, such as depression, but are inaccurately
studies (e.g., Ockene et al., 2 0 0 0 ) . T h e most
labeling these problems as "attitudinal"
or
effective primary care interventions include
"personality" faults in their patients. Such mis-
several core elements such as a strong provider-
attribution leads to a failure to treat possible
delivered "quit smoking" message; self-help
underlying mental health disorders that, if
materials covering motivational, behavioral,
treated, can improve patient adherence.
and relapse prevention strategies; a prescription for nicotine replacement therapy; brief
Adherence Interventions at the Provider Level
counseling that includes setting a quit date; and follow-up support (Glasgow & Orleans, 1 9 9 7 ) . T h e American Medical Association has
Because providers play a significant role in
recognized the important influence o f health
adherence, designing interventions to affect their
care providers in reducing smoking rates and
performance seems to be a reasonable strategy,
during the early 1 9 9 0 s created guidelines for
but investigations in this area are few. Providers
the treatment of nicotine addiction (American
prescribe the medical regimen, interpret it, mon-
Medical Association, 1 9 9 3 ) . T h e guidelines
itor clinical outcomes, and provide feedback to
recommend that providers do the following:
patients (Center for the Advancement o f Health, 1 9 9 9 ) . Accordingly, provider communication has been widely examined, and importantly,
• •
Ask about smoking at every opportunity. Advise all smokers to quit.
216
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH •
•
and pharmacies to share information regarding
Assist smokers to quit through the use of self-help materials and nicotine replacement whenever appropriate.
patients' behavior around prescription refills
Arrange follow-up contacts.
possible because the information allows health
has the potential to improve adherence. This is care providers to track patients' use o f medi-
The
ask-advise-assist-arrange model has
cation as a proxy of medication adherence.
been used successfully for inpatient and outpa-
Patients can be contacted if they are using med-
tient settings and has resulted in quit rates sig-
ications at a rate that is too fast or too slow. In
nificantly higher than usual-care approaches
addition, organizations determine the level of
(Glasgow & Orleans, 1 9 9 7 ) .
communication with patients. Ongoing communication efforts (e.g., phone contacts) that help to keep patients engaged in their health
Adherence Interventions at the Health Care Organization Level
care may be the most simple and cost-effective strategy for improving
Health care organizations have the potential to influence patient adherence behavior as
adherence
(Haynes
et al., 1 9 9 6 ) . The
"state-of-the-art"
interventions
in
well given that they control access to care. For
adherence target each level o f the adherence
example,
problem
organizations
direct
providers'
mentioned
previously
(patient,
schedules, appointment lengths, allocation of
provider, and health care organization). Several
resources,
programs
fee
structures,
communication/
have demonstrated
good
results
information systems, and organizational priori-
using a multilevel team approach
(Hyper-
ties. As such, health care organizations ulti-
tension Detection and Follow-up
Program
mately influence patients' behavior in many
Cooperative
ways. Organizations set parameters of care
Factor Intervention Trial Research Group,
(e.g.,
leading
1 9 8 2 ; S H E P Cooperative Research Group,
providers to report that their schedules do not
1 9 9 1 ) . In fact, adequate evidence exists to sup-
allow enough time to address adherence behav-
port the effectiveness o f innovative, modified
ior adequately (Ammerman et al., 1 9 9 3 ) . Fee
health care teams over traditional, independent
structures are determined
physician practice and minimally
appointment
length),
often
by organizations,
Group,
1 9 7 9 ; Multiple
Risk
structured
and many systems (e.g., fee-for-service) lack
organizations (DeBusk et al., 1 9 9 4 ; Peters,
financial reimbursement for patient counseling
Davidson, & Ossorio, 1 9 9 5 ) .
and
education,
substantially
threatening
adherence-focused interventions. T h e allocation o f resources within an organization may
STATE-OF-THE-ART
result in high stress and increased demands
MEASUREMENT OF
on providers that in turn have been associated
ADHERENCE BEHAVIOR
with decreased patient adherence
behavior
(DiMatteo & DiNicola, 1 9 8 2 ) .
Accurate assessment o f adherence behavior is
Furthermore, organizations determine continuity of care. Patients demonstrate
better
necessary for effective and efficient treatment planning and for ensuring that changes in
adherence behavior when they receive care
health outcomes can be attributed
to the
from the same health care provider over time
recommended regimen. In addition, decisions
and when patient information is communi-
to change recommendations,
cated with other providers (Meichenbaum &
and/or communication style so as to invoke
T u r k , 1 9 8 7 ) . For example, the ability o f clinics
patient participation depend
medications, on valid
and
Adherence
to Medical
Recommendations
217
reliable measurement of the adherence construct.
objective strategies may initially appear to be an
Indisputably, there is no "gold standard" for
improvement over subjective approaches, both
measuring adherence behavior (Farmer, 1 9 9 9 ;
approaches have their drawbacks. For example,
Vitolins et al., 2 0 0 0 ) . However, a variety of
remaining dosage units (e.g., tablets) can be counted at clinic visits, but counting inaccura-
strategies have been reported in the literature.
cies are common and typically result in overestimations of adherence behavior (Matsui et al.,
Subjective Measures of Adherence Behavior O n e measurement
1 9 9 4 ) . In addition, important information, such as the timing of dosages and the patterns of
approach
is to
ask
providers and patients to provide their subjec-
missed dosages, is not captured
using this
strategy. Recently, electronic monitoring devices,
tive ratings of adherence behavior. W h e n
such as the Medication Event Monitoring
providers rate the degree to which patients
System ( M E M S ) , have been used to record the
follow their recommendations, providers over-
time and date that a medication container was
estimate adherence behavior (DiMatteo
&
opened, thereby giving a better description of
DiNicola, 1 9 8 2 ; Norell, 1 9 8 1 ) . T h e validity of
the manner in which patients take their medica-
patients' subjective reports has been problem-
tions (Cramer & Mattson, 1 9 9 1 ) . Unfortu-
atic as well. Patients who reveal that they have
nately, the expense o f these devices precludes
not followed advice tend to describe their
their widespread use. Pharmacy databases can
behavior accurately (Cramer &
also be used to check when prescriptions are
Mattson,
1 9 9 1 ) , whereas patients who deny their failure
filled initially, refilled over time, and/or discon-
to follow recommendations tend to report their
tinued prematurely. However, one problem
behavior inaccurately (Spector et al., 1 9 8 6 ) .
with this approach is that complete information
Other subjective rating indicators include stan-
is difficult to obtain given that patients may use
dardized, patient-administered
more than one pharmacy or data might not be
questionnaires
(e.g., Morisky, Green, & Levine, 1 9 8 6 ) . These
routinely captured.
questionnaires have typically been used to assess global patient characteristics or "personality" traits that have proved to be poor predictors o f adherence behavior (Farmer, 1 9 9 9 ) . There are no stable (i.e., trait) factors that reliably predict adherence. However, questionnaires that assess specific behaviors that relate to specific medical recommendations, such as food frequency questionnaires (Freudenheim, 1 9 9 3 ) used for measuring eating behavior to improve management o f obesity, may be reasonable predictors o f adherence
behaviors
(Sumartojo, 1 9 9 3 ) .
Biochemical Measures of Adherence Behavior Biochemical
measurement
is
a
third
approach for assessing adherence behavior. N o n t o x i c biological markers can be added to medications, and their presence in blood or urine c a n provide evidence that a patient recently received a dose o f the medication under examination. This assessment strategy is far from perfect given that findings can be misleading and influenced by a variety o f individual factors, including diet, absorption,
Objective Measures of Adherence Behavior
and rate o f excretion (Vitolins, 2 0 0 0 ) . In sum, adherence measurement provides useful information that outcome monitoring alone
Another approach in assessing adherence
cannot provide, but it remains only an estima-
behaviors is using objective measures. Although
tion of a patient's actual behavior. Several of the
218
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH
CASE S T U D Y Kaiser Permanente, a large health maintenance organization, is addressing the problem o f adherence at the patient, provider, and health care organization levels. M o s t recently, chronic conditions have been the target o f organizational efforts directed toward improving adherence. Specific factors at each level k n o w n t o influence patients' ability to adhere to medical recommendations are addressed, with the ultimate goal o f improving health outcomes in patients with chronic problems. O n e strategy for improving adherence rates is to offer a variety o f appointment formats and time frames (e.g., individual, group, interdisciplinary team led, telephone) so as to increase the amount o f c o n t a c t between patients and providers. Offering a range o f services maximizes opportunities for providers and patients to attend t o adherence issues and incorporates professionals other than physicians into clinical contacts. Clinical health educators, registered dieticians, pharmacists, care managers, and clinical health psychologists are examples o f nonphysician providers at Kaiser Permanente w h o augment patient care and serve to increase the frequency o f patient contacts within the health care organization. Nonphysician providers have different areas o f expertise and different approaches to the adherence problem. M o r e o v e r , they can extend physicians' time and influence by supporting physicians' recommendations through additional education, motivation, coordination, and self-management support. Another strategy for improving adherence at Kaiser Permanente is t o ensure the quality o f the patient-provider relationship. Patients are assigned a consistent primary care provider (PCP) w h o oversees and coordinates all medical care. T h e PCP establishes an ongoing and open relationship with the patient to maintain continuity o f care over time and to enhance two-way communication and shared responsibility for chronic condition
management.
M o s t Kaiser
Permanente
PCPs
participate in group appointments during which adherence issues are routinely addressed. Patients learn about their conditions, undergo motivational enhancement to initiate and maintain new behaviors, and learn new behavioral skills to implement for the daily management o f their health problems. Sophisticated information systems also help to identify patients having difficulty in adhering to recommendations. Kaiser Permanente's electronic systems facilitate communication among primary care, specialty care, inpatient care, and pharmacy services. PCPs and specialists are able to monitor pharmacy data on prescription refills and use this information as a proxy o f adherence to medication regimens. Laboratory results are also readily available to PCPs, and patients' attendance at group appointments or educational classes can be monitored. Treatment protocols specific to different chronic conditions allow for the ongoing monitoring o f a number of biological indexes and o f adherence to screening and prevention activities. Finally, there are special efforts to promote continuity and contact with the organization to maximize patients' adherence behavior and to minimize or delay
Adherence
expensive disease complications. Care is planned;
to Medical
follow-up
Recommendations
|
appointments,
laboratory tests, and health education group appointments are scheduled. W h e n patients do not adhere to the care protocols, they are contacted through a variety o f outreach strategies. These strategies include regular patient newsletters, individualized reminder letters from their PCPs, and telephone calls from care managers. Patient-centered
outcomes continue to improve. Biological indexes
have
improved across conditions such as heart disease, asthma, and diabetes. Screening and prevention activities have increased, and hospital admissions for patients with chronic conditions have declined.
measurement strategies are extremely costly
(Malahey, 1 9 6 6 ; Marlatt & Gordon, 1 9 8 5 ;
(e.g., M E M S ) or depend on information tech-
Zola, 1 9 8 1 ) . Effective strategies that consis-
nology (e.g., pharmacy databases) that is not
tently change and maintain complex behaviors
available in many organizations. Determining
across
the "best" measurement strategy to get an
However, substantial evidence exists in the
time
might
never
be
discovered.
approximation o f adherence behavior requires
behavioral science arena identifying the most
taking all considerations into account. M o s t
effective strategies for changing behavior. This
important, the strategies employed must meet
abundant body o f research should guide future
basic psychometric standards o f acceptable
intervention efforts. Behavioral science also
reliability and validity (Nunnally & Bernstein,
provides fundamental principles o f behavior
1994).
and empirically evaluated models that can
Additional
meeting
the
goals
considerations of
the
include
provider
or
researcher, the accuracy requirements associated with the regimen, the available resources,
serve as a framework for organizing the conceptualization o f the adherence problem. The
fundamental
concepts from behav-
the response burden on the patient, and how
ioral science apply to behavior in general,
the results will be used. Finally, because no
including all medical conditions and the rec-
solitary
measurement
strategy
been
ommendations for their management, health
approach
care provider behavior, and the behavior o f
that combines feasible self-report and reason-
health care organizations. T h e conceptual
able objective measures is the current state o f
models (e.g., 1 M B model) provide a frame-
the art in the measurement
w o r k for these behavioral principles. Given
deemed optimal, a multimethod
has
o f adherence
behavior.
this knowledge, efforts for improving adherence behaviors c a n be focused and intensified. M o r e o v e r , although complete control is
CONCLUSIONS
impossible, consistent consideration o f the significance o f events that precede and follow
Adherence is behavior. Changing it becomes
behavior at the patient, provider, and health
increasingly more difficult as patients are asked
care organization levels will advance adher-
to learn new behaviors, alter their daily rou-
ence e n h a n c e m e n t
tines, and maintain the changes over time
affect health outcomes.
efforts
and
ultimately
219
220
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH
REFERENCES Ajzen, I., Sc Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting social behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Alto, W. A. (1995). Prevention in practice. Primary Care, 22, 5 4 3 - 5 4 4 . American Medical Association. (1993). How to help patients stop smoking. Chicago: Author. Ammerman, A. S., DeVillis, R. F., Carey, T. S., Keyserling, T. C , Strogatz, D. S., Haines, P. S., Simpson, R. J . , Jr., 8c Siskovick, D. S. (1993). Physician-based diet counseling for cholesterol reduction: Current practices, determinants, and strategies for improvement. Preventive Medicine, 22, 9 6 - 1 0 9 . Bandura, A. ( 1 9 8 2 ) . Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. American Psychologist, 37, 1 2 2 - 1 4 7 . Bandura, A. J . , 8c Simon, Κ. M . (1977). The role of proximal intentions in self-regulation of refractory behavior. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 1, 1 7 7 - 1 8 4 . Becker, M. H., 8c Maiman, L. A. (1975). Sociobehavioral determinants of compliance with health care and medical care recommendations. Medical Care, 13, 1 0 - 2 4 . Botelho, R. J . , & Skinner, H. (1995). Motivating change in health behavior: Implications for health promotion and disease prevention. Primary Care, 22, 5 6 5 - 5 8 9 . Brownell, K. D., 8c Cohen, L. S. (1995). Adherence to dietary regimens: II. Components of effective interventions. Behavioral Medicine, 20, 1 5 5 - 1 6 4 . Carey, M. P., Kalichman, S. C , Forsyth, A. D , Wright, Ε. M., 8c Johnson, Β. T. (1997). Enhancing motivation to reduce the risk of HIV infection for economically disadvantaged urban women. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 531-541. Casebeer, L., Klapow, J . , Centor, R., Stafford, M., & Skrinar, A. (1999). Improving physician skills in patient adherence through a combined C M E intervention: Clinical patient outcomes. Academic Medicine, 74, 1 3 3 4 - 1 3 3 9 . Center for the Advancement of Health. (1999). Interventions to improve adherence to medical regimens in the elderly [Online]. Retrieved on January 2 0 , 2 0 0 3 , from www.cfah.org Chesney, M., Chrisman, N. J . , Luftey, K., & Pescosolido, B. A. (1999, April). Not what the doctor ordered: Challenges individuals face in adhering to medical advice/treatment. Paper presented during a symposium conducted at the congressional briefing of the Consortium of Social Science Association, Washington, DC. Cramer, J . Α., 8c Mattson, R. H. (1991). Monitoring compliance with antiepileptic drug therapy. In J . A. Cramer 8c B . Spilker (Eds.), Patient compliance in medical practice and clinical trials (pp. 1 2 3 - 1 3 7 ) . New York: Raven. Daltroy, L. H., 8c Godin, G. (1989). The influence of spousal approval and patient perception of spousal approval on cardiac participation in exercise programs. Journal of Cardiopulmonary Rehabilitation, 9, 3 6 3 - 3 6 7 . Davis, M. S. (1966). Variations in patients' compliance with doctor's advice: Analysis of congruence between survey responses and results of empirical observations. Journal of Medical Education, 41, 1 0 3 7 - 1 0 4 8 . DeBusk, R. F., Miller, N. H., Superko, H. R., Dennis, C. Α., Thomas, R. J . , Lew, H. T., Berger, W. Ε., Ill, Heller, R. S., Rompf, J . , Gee, D., Kraemer, H. C , Bandura, Α., Ghandour, F., Clark, M., Shah, R.V., Fisher, L., 8c Taylor, C. B. (1994). A case-management system for coronary risk factor modification after acute myocardial infarction. Annals of Internal Medicine, 120, 7 2 1 - 7 2 9 . DiMatteo, M. R., 8c DiNicola, D. D. (1982). Achieving patient compliance. New York: Pergamon. DiMatteo, M. R., Lepper, H. S., 8c Croghan, T. W. (2000). Depression is a risk factor for noncompliance with medical treatment. Archives of Internal Medicine, 160, 2 1 0 1 - 2 1 0 7 .
Adherence
to Medical
Recommendations
Dunbar-Jacob, J . , Burke, L. E., & Pyczynski, S. (1995). Clinical assessment and management of adherence to medical regimens. In P. M . Nicassio & T. W. Smith (Eds.), Managing chronic illness: A hiopsychasocial perspective. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Farmer, K. C. (1999). Methods for measuring and monitoring medication regimen adherence in clinical trials and clinical practice. Clinical Therapeutics, 31, 1074-1090. Fisher, J . D., 8c Fisher, W. A. (1992). Changing AIDS-risk behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 4 5 5 - 4 7 4 . Fisher, J . D., Fisher, W. Α., Miscovich, S. J . , Kimble, D. L., 8c Malloy, T. E. (1996). Changing AIDS risk behavior: Effects of an intervention emphasizing AIDS risk reduction information, motivation, and behavioral skills in a college student population. Health Psychology, 15, 1 1 4 - 1 2 3 . Freudenheim, J . L. (1993). A review of study designs and methods of dietary assessment in nutritional epidemiology of chronic disease, journal of Nutrition, 123, 401-405. Glasgow, R. E., & Orleans, C. T. (1997). Adherence to smoking cessation regimens. In D. S. Gochman (Ed.), Handbook of health behavior research II: Provider determinants (pp. 3 5 3 - 3 7 7 ) . New York: Plenum. Hall, J . Α., Roter, D. L., 8c Katz, N. R. (1988). Meta-analysis of correlates of provider behavior in medical encounters. Medical Care, 26, 6 5 7 . Haynes, R. B. (1979). Determinants of compliance: The disease and the mechanics of treatment. In R. B . Haynes, D. W. Taylor, 8c D. L. Sackett (Eds.), Compliance in health care (pp. 4 9 - 6 2 ) . Baltimore, M D : Johns Hopkins University Press. Haynes, R. B., McKibbon, Κ. Α., 8c Kanani, R. (1996). Systematic review of randomised trials of interventions to assist patients to follow prescriptions for medications. Lancet, 348, 3 8 3 - 3 8 6 . Haynes, R. B . , Montague, P., Oliver, T., McKibbon, Κ. Α., Brouwers, M . C , &C Kanani, R. (2002). Interventions for helping patients to follow prescriptions for medications. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 2. Houston-Miller, N., Hill, M., Kottke, T., 8c Ockene, S. (1997). The multilevel compliance challenge: Recommendations for a call to action. Circulation, 95, 1085-1090. Hypertension Detection and Follow-up Program Cooperative Group. (1979). Fiveyear findings of the Hypertension Detection and Follow-up Program: I. Reduction in mortality of persons with high blood pressure, including mild hypertension. Journal of the American Medical Association, 242, 2 5 6 2 - 2 5 7 1 . Institute for the Future. (2000). Health and health care 2010: The forecast, the challenge. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Institute of Medicine. (2001). Health and behavior: The interplay of biological, behavioral, and societal influences. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Janis, I. L. (1983). The role of social support in adherence to stressful decisions. American Psychologist, 38, 1 4 3 . Kaplan, R. M., Sallis, J . F., 8c Patterson, T. L. (1993). Health and human behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill. Lustman, P. J . , Griffith, L. S., Clouse, R. E., Freeland, K. E., Eisen, S. Α., Rubin, E. H., Carney, R. M., Sc McGill, J . B. (1995). Effects of Alprazolam on glucose regulation in diabetes: Results of a double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Psychosomatic Medicine, 59, 2 4 1 - 2 5 0 . Mahoney, M . J . (1974). Self-reward and self-monitoring techniques for weight control. Behavior Therapy, 5, 4 8 - 5 7 . Mahoney, M. J . , Moura, N. G. M., 8c Wade, T. C. (1973). The relative efficacy of self-reward, self-punishment, and self-monitoring techniques for weight loss. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 40, 4 0 4 - 4 0 7 .
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH Malahey, B . (1966). The effects of instructions and labeling in the number of medication errors made by patients at home. American Journal of Hospital Pharmacy, 23, 2 8 3 - 2 9 2 . Marlatt, G. Α., & Gordon, J . R. (1985). Relapse prevention: Maintenance strategies in addictive behavior change. New York: Guilford. Matarazzo, J . D. (1980). Behavioral health and behavioral medicine: Frontiers for a new health psychology. American Psychologist, 35, 8 0 7 - 8 1 7 . Matsui, D., Hermann, C , Klein, J . J . , Berkovitch, M., Olivieri, N., & Koren, G. (1994). Critical comparison of novel and existing methods of compliance assessment during a clinical trial of an oral iron chelator. Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 34, 9 4 4 - 9 4 9 . Mazzuca, S. A. (1982). Does patient education in chronic disease have therapeutic value? Journal of Chronic Diseases, 35, 5 2 1 - 5 2 9 . Meichenbaum, D., & Turk, D. C. (1987). Facilitating treatment adherence: A practitioner's guidebook. New York: Plenum. Miller, W. R., & Rollnick, S. (2002). Motivational interviewing. New York: Guilford. Morisky, D. E., Green, L. W., & Levine, D. M . (1986). Concurrent and predictive validity of a self-reported measure of medication adherence. Medical Care, 24, 67-74. Morisky, D. E., Levine, D. M., Green, L. W., Shapiro, S., Russel, R. P., & Smith, C. R. (1983). Five-year blood pressure control and mortality following health education for hypertensive patients. American Journal of Public Health, 73, 153-162. Multiple Risk Factor Intervention Trial Research Group. (1982). Multiple Risk Factor Intervention Trial: Risk factor changes and mortality results. Journal of the American Medical Association, 248, 1 4 6 5 - 1 4 7 7 . National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute. (1998). Behavioral research in cardiovascular, lung, and blood health and disease. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Nessman, D. G., Carnahan, J . E., & Nugent, C. A. (1980). Increasing compliance: Patient-oriented hypertension groups. Archives of Internal Medicine, 140, 1427-1430. Norell, S. E. ( 1 9 8 1 ) . Accuracy of patient interviews and estimates by clinical staff determining medication compliance. Social Science and Medicine, 15, 57-61. Nunnally, J . C , & Bernstein, I. H. (1994). Psychometric theory (3rd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. Ockene, J . K., Emmons, K. M., Mermelstein, R. J . , Perkins, Κ. Α., Bonollo, D. S., Voorhees, C. C , & Hollis, J . F. (2000). Relapse and maintenance issues for smoking cessation. Health Psychology, 19, 1 7 - 3 1 . Oldridge, Ν. B., & Jones, N. L. (1983). Improving patient compliance in cardiac rehabilitation: Effects of written agreement and self-monitoring. Journal of Cardiopulmonary Rehabilitation, 3, 2 5 7 - 2 6 2 . Oxman, A. D., Thomson, Μ. Α., Davis, D. Α., & Haynes, R. B . (1995). No magic bullets: A systematic review of 102 trials of interventions to improve professional practice. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 153, 1 4 2 3 - 1 4 3 1 . Perri, M . G. (1998). The maintenance of treatment effects in the long-term management of obesity. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 5, 5 2 6 - 5 4 3 . Perri, M. G., McAdoo, W. G., McAllister, D. Α., Lauer, J . B., Jordan, R. C , Yancey, D. Z., & Nezu, A. M. (1987). Effects of peer support and therapist contact on long term weight loss. Journal of Clinical and Consulting Psychology, 65, 2 7 8 - 2 8 5 . Perri, M. G., Shapiro, R. M., Ludwig, W. W., Twentyman, C. T., & McAdoo, W. G. (1984). Maintenance strategies for the treatment of obesity: An evaluation of
Adherence
to Medical
Recommendations
relapse prevention training and posttreatment contact by mail and telephone. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52, 4 0 4 - 4 1 3 . Peters, A. L., Davidson, M . B . , 8c Ossorio, R. C. (1995). Management of patients with diabetes by nurses with support of subspecialists. HMO Practice, 9, 8 - 1 2 . Prochaska, J . O., Se DiClemente, C. C. (1982). Transtheoretical therapy: Toward a Theory, Research, & more integrative model of change. Psychotherapy: Practice, 19, 2 7 6 - 2 8 8 . Pruitt, S. D. (2001). Adherence to medical recommendations (commissioned report to W H O , Adherence Project). Geneva: World Health Organization. Rand, C. (1993). Measuring adherence with therapy for chronic diseases: Implications for the treatment of heterozygous familial hypercholesterolemia. American Journal of Cardiology, 72, D 6 8 - D 7 4 . Rosenstock, I. M . (1974). Historical origins of the health belief model. Health 2, 1-8. Education Monographs, Roter, D. L., Hall, J . Α., Merisca, R., Nordstrom, B . , Cretin, D., & Svarstad, B . (1998). Effectiveness of interventions to improve patient compliance. Medical Care, 36, 1 1 3 8 - 1 1 6 1 . Schulman, B . A. (1979). Active patient orientation and outcomes in hypertensive treatment. Medical Care, 17, 2 6 7 - 2 8 0 . Schwartz, G. E., Sc Weiss, S. M. (1977). Yale Conference on Behavioral Medicine. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare; National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute. SHEP Cooperative Research Group. (1991). Prevention of stroke by antihypertensive drug treatment in older persons with isolated systolic hypertension: Final results of the Systolic Hypertension in the Elderly Program (SHEP). Journal of the American Medical Association, 265, 3 2 5 5 - 3 2 6 4 . Skinner, B . F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton-CenturyCrofts. Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan. Spector, S. L., Kingsman, R., Mawhinney, H., Sigel, S. C. J . , Rachelefsky, G. S., Katz, R. M., Sc Rohr, A. S. (1986). Compliance of patient with asthma with an experimental aerosolized medication: Implications for controlled clinical trials. Journal of Allergy and Clinical Immunology, 77, 6 5 - 7 0 . Stewart, M . A. (1996). Effective physician-patient communication and health outcomes: A review. Canadian Medical Association, 152, 1 4 2 3 . Stone, G. C. (1979). Patient compliance and the role of the expert. Journal of Social Issues, 35, 3 4 - 5 9 . Sumartojo, E. (1993). When tuberculosis treatment fails: A social behavioral account of patient adherence. American Review of Respiratory Disease, 147, 1 3 1 1 - 1 3 2 0 . Swain, M . S., & Steckel, S. B . (1981). Influencing adherence among hypertensives. Research Nursing & Health, 4, 2 1 3 - 2 2 2 . Taylor, C. B., Houston-Miller, N., Killen, J . D., Se DeBusk, R. F. (1990). Smoking cessation after acute myocardial infarction: Effects of a nurse-managed intervention. Annals of Internal Medicine, 113, 1 1 8 - 1 2 3 . Taylor, S. E. (1995). Health psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill. Triandis, H. C. (1977). Interpersonal behavior. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole. Vitolins, M . Z . , Rand, C. S., Rapp, S. R., Ribisl, P. M., Sc Sevick, M . A. (2000). Measuring adherence to behavioral and medical interventions. Controlled Clinical Trials, 21, 1 8 8 5 - 1 9 4 5 . Waitzkin, H., Sc Stoeckle, J . D. (1976). Information control and the micropolitics of health care. Social Science and Medicine, 10, 2 6 3 - 2 7 6 . Whitcher-Alagna, S. (1983). Receiving medical help: A psychosocial perspective on patient reactions. In A. Nadler, J . D. Fisher, Sc Β. M . DePaulo (Eds.), New directions in helping. New York: Academic Press.
224
BEHAVIORS T H A T C O M P R O M I S E HEALTH World Health Organization. (2001). Adherence to long-term therapies: Policy for action (WHO Adherence Project: Toward Policies for Action). Geneva: Author. Ziegelstein, R. C , Fauerback, J . Α., Stevens, S. S., Romanelli, J . , Richter, D. P., &c Bush, D. E. (2000). Patients with depression are less likely to follow recommendations to reduce cardiac risk during recovery from a myocardial infarction. Archives of Internal Medicine, 160, 1 8 1 8 - 1 8 2 3 . Zola, I. K. (1981). Structural constraints on the doctor-patient relationship: The case of non-compliance. In L. Eisenberg 8c A. Kleinman (Eds.), The relevance of social science for medicine (pp. 2 4 1 - 2 5 2 ) . New York: D. Reidel.
Part III BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS Introduction
T Λ.
to Part III
he next nine chapters epitomize the critical role that psychologists play within health treatment teams across the globe. W h e r e a s Part II focused on unhealthy behaviors that deteriorate health status, this section targets the unhealthy out-
comes o f certain behaviors and explores adaptive behavioral approaches that improve, prevent, and/or alleviate such deleterious medical conditions. M o r e o v e r , the chapters in this section also focus on understanding and addressing psychological problems, such as depression, that result from these medical conditions. In Chapter 1 3 , B o o t h b y , Kuhajda, and T h o r n explore the role o f individual difference variables (e.g., biological states, personality), cognitive appraisal, and coping skills in relation t o an individual's adjustment to pain. T h e y address these issues within a biopsychosocial framework and note that chronic pain can have a significant impact on h o w an individual experiences day-to-day life. T h e y also note h o w chronic pain c a n create psychological problems that complicate adequate adjustment to their condition. Clinicians working with this population are encouraged to w o r k as valued members o f multidisciplinary teams and to positively influence the lives o f such patients via cognitive-behavioral techniques. In Chapter 1 4 , al'Absi and Hoffman define hypertension, discuss the etiological factors that contribute t o this health concern, and highlight h o w t o assess and treat this problem. Their underlying message is that hypertension is a silent killer that can be easily overlooked by medical professionals because there are n o specific symptoms. As such, body weight, alcohol consumption, low levels o f physical activity, and the
226
THE HEALTH P S Y C H O L O G Y HANDBOOK experience o f chronic stress are targeted as factors that may independently
and
conjointly contribute t o hypertension. Clinicians are encouraged t o intervene using cognitive-behavioral approaches designed to control weight, reduce or eliminate alcohol consumption, teach more effective ways in which to cope with stress, and foster adherence to medical and psychological treatments. In Chapter 1 5 , Schwartz and Ketterer provide a practical set o f recommendations for working clinically with patients suffering from heart disease. T h e y conceptualize coronary heart disease within a cognitive-behavioral framework and point out the importance o f a comprehensive assessment. T h e y note that the assessment and treatment o f patients' health risk behaviors, such as smoking, obesity, and lack o f exercise, are essential to secondary prevention efforts and are critical for optimal recovery o f function and general health. Given the significant influence o f negative emotions on the development o f heart disease, clinical health psychologists play a vital role in the treatment o f such patients by employing cognitive-behavioral interventions with and without pharmacotherapy. In Chapter 1 6 , H o f f and her colleagues present issues and behavioral strategies associated with the management o f type 2 diabetes as well as assessment and treatment recommendations. T h e y describe and discuss etiological factors, including a host of psychological consequences that co-occur with this health problem. T h e y note that clinical health psychologists intervene with diabetic patients in a variety o f ways, including making patients more aware o f their health-compromising behaviors via self-monitoring and encouraging behavioral changes that lead to a healthier lifestyle. In addition, clinical health psychologists are in a unique position to address the psychological issues that may arise as a result o f this chronic condition. In Chapter 1 7 , Kreitler takes o n the awesome task o f reviewing the significant body of research examining etiological, assessment, and treatment issues related to cancer. She addresses the psychological phenomena
that undermine
annual
preventive
measures, such as screening for cancer, among high-risk groups. She also explores psychological factors that interfere with the adjustment, coping, and preparation o f patients at various stages o f cancer progression. In addition, Kreitler discusses the assessment o f psychological problems that arise from the impact o f cancer on a person's life and the subsequent treatment options for such issues. T h r o u g h o u t the chapter, she highlights the impact o f cancer n o t only on the patient but also on family members and health care providers. In Chapter 1 8 , G a r o s discusses sexual dysfunctions that may arise due to multiple factors. T h e chapter begins with a section reviewing the human sexual response, followed by a discussion about the psychological and medical conditions that may initiate or exacerbate sexual dysfunction. Given that males and females suffer from different types o f sexual dysfunction, Garos opens with an overall approach to treatment and then breaks down the ensuing assessment and treatment section into various male and female sexual problems. In this section o f the chapter, the reader will find prevalence rates and contributing factors for each dysfunction as well as idiosyncratic treatment approaches to each disorder. In Chapter 1 9 , Blalock and Campos present a thought-provoking piece on human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) and acquired immune deficiency syndrome (AIDS). T h e y describe the various expressions o f H I V and the stages at which H I V develops into AIDS.
Behavioral
Aspects of Medical
Next, they highlight the importance o f primary prevention and offer
Problems
prevention
strategies for health psychologists and medical personnel. T h e chapter also includes detailed descriptions o f various assessment tools and recommended
treatment
approaches for psychosocial issues that reduce patients' quality o f life. Both psychological and pharmacological treatments for coexisting psychological disorders are presented. In Chapter 2 0 , Lackner notes the high prevalence o f irritable bowel syndrome (IBS) and describes the clinical and diagnostic features associated with this disorder. N e x t , he points out that this syndrome has historically been viewed as a physical manifestation o f a variety o f psychiatric conditions. As such, he uses a biopsychosocial model to illustrate the interplay between psychological functioning and I B S . Lackner also emphasizes the importance o f comprehensive psychodiagnostic assessments before discussing treatment approaches for patients suffering from I B S . T r e a t m e n t recommendations explore the contribution o f conjunctive therapy that uses pharmacotherapy, cognitive-behavioral, and motivational enhancement approaches. Finally, in Chapter 2 1 , Wolfe and Pruitt discuss the impact that insomnia and the other sleep disorders have on human functioning and the overall quality o f life. T h e y start by describing the basic elements o f sleep before delving into the etiology o f a variety o f sleep disorders. N e x t , they break down assessment and treatment recommendations by type o f sleep disorder. Given the high prevalence and pervasive negative impact o f insomnia, much o f the treatment section in this chapter focuses on this disorder. Clinical health psychologists are urged not to underestimate sleep problems when patients present with such disorders.
227
CHAPTER
13 10
Diagnostic and Treatment Considerations in Chronic Pain JENNIFER L . BOOTHBY, MELISSA C . KUHAJDA, AND BEVERLY E . THORN
P
experience that
site o f the pain problem. LeResche and V o n
includes sensory and emotional compo-
Korff ( 1 9 9 9 ) reviewed prevalence rates for
ain is a perceptual
nents associated with actual or threat-
back
pain
across
several
studies.
They
(Merskey & Bogdale,
reported that approximately 1 1 % to 4 5 % o f
1 9 9 4 ) . Virtually everyone experiences pain at
individuals are affected by persistent back
some time in his or her life, but for most
pain. Other researchers found that 1 7 % o f
ened tissue damage
individuals the pain experience is time limited
men and 2 0 % o f women in Australia reported
and does not warrant clinical intervention.
experiencing a chronic pain problem during
However, for some individuals, the pain prob-
the previous 6 months (Blyth et al., 2 0 0 1 ) .
lem persists and significantly disrupts daily
Approximately 5 0 % o f a sample o f U.S. vet-
functioning. Pain that persists for longer than
erans seeking medical treatment
6 months is referred to as "chronic pain"
suffering from at least one chronic pain com-
reported
(Keefe, 1 9 8 2 ) , and chronic pain is often associ-
plaint
ated with feelings of "demoralization, helpless-
migraine headaches have been reported to
ness, hopelessness, and outright depression"
range from 3 . 4 % to 1 7 . 6 % (Breslau, Davis, &
(Clark, 2 0 0 2 ) . Prevalence rates for
(Turk, 1 9 9 6 , p. 3 ) . T h e experience o f chronic
Andreski, 1 9 9 1 ) ,
pain can be far-reaching, affecting numerous
mandibular joint pain were found to range
and
rates o f t e m p o r o -
areas o f an individual's life. F o r example,
from 3 % to 1 5 % (LeResche & V o n Korff,
limited
1 9 9 9 ) . Overall, w o m e n appear to suffer from
physical functioning, inability or reduced abil-
higher rates o f chronic pain problems than do
ity to work, financial concerns related to their
men, and prevalence rates for many pain com-
work situations and medical costs, frequent
plaints increase with age (LeResche & V o n
medical visits, emotional distress, and/or dis-
Korff, 1 9 9 9 ) .
chronic pain
patients
often
face
rupted interpersonal relationships.
Pain is one o f the most c o m m o n reasons vary
for seeking medical care, and for many med-
depending on the population studied and the
ical disorders pain is a primary complaint.
Prevalence rates for chronic pain
229
230
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS For
example, rheumatological
disorders,
options). T h e cognitive appraisal
processes
cancer, dental problems, gastrointestinal dis-
influence one's emotional, cognitive, behav-
orders, AIDS, and neurological conditions all
ioral, and physiological response to the pain
share pain as a presenting complaint. In addi-
experience. Those responses that
tion, many individuals w h o sustain injuries
attempts to mitigate the pain experience are
represent
experience substantial pain, and for some the
called coping attempts. T h e efficacy of the cop-
pain persists for years. M o r e o v e r , pain is not
ing attempt is not what gives it the label of
entirely a physiological experience. Decades
"coping"; rather, it is the effortful intention to
o f research have shown that pain has sen-
manage the stressful pain experience. Coping
sory, affective, and evaluative components
responses ultimately influence important adap-
(Melzack & Wall, 1 9 6 5 ) . Thus, understand-
tational outcomes such as social and occupa-
ing pain and understanding its treatment are
tional functioning, morale and mood,
important areas in which health psychologists
somatic health. Finally, it is crucial to note that
and
should become involved. In fact, a health psy-
the process of appraisal, coping, and adapta-
chologist would be hard-pressed to work in a
tion is fluid rather than static;
medical setting and not confront patients with
itself changes the nature of the pain experience,
pain problems on a relatively frequent basis.
which affects cognitive appraisal processes,
adaptation
thereby influencing coping and adaptation. T h e transactional model as adapted for pain is not limited to appraisal of, and coping with,
BACKGROUND AND ETIOLOGY
the pain stimulus. Obviously, there are many related aspects o f chronic pain such as feelings
Biopsychosocial Model of Pain
of dependency, dealing with marital strain or
Our current understanding of pain percep-
divorce, and losing one's j o b and income. Each
tion is consistent with the biopsychosocial
of these environmental challenges contributes
model. A useful conceptual heuristic for the
to the "stress" o f a person with chronic pain.
biopsychosocial model
been
T h e multifaceted nature of stressors related to
adapted from Lazarus and Folkman's ( 1 9 8 4 )
pain, and the patient's evolving response to
transactional stress and coping theory (Thorn,
them, is particularly important in the context
Rich, & Boothby, 1 9 9 9 ) and is expanded in
of coping with chronic pain.
o f pain
has
Figure 1 3 . 1 . In the expanded model, the influ-
T h e model just conceptualized is useful
ence of individual variables, cognitive appraisal
from a theoretical perspective but does not
processes, and coping interact to influence
offer a format for treatment conceptualiza-
ongoing adjustment to the pain experience.
tion. T o this end, the chapter authors have
Individual variables include biological state
adopted a cognitive conceptualization through
(e.g., disease process), dispositional factors (e.g.,
which cognitive-behavioral treatment can be
personality), and stable social roles (e.g., pri-
delivered. A cognitive formulation fits well
mary relationship issues). Cognitive appraisal
with a transactional model o f pain because it
processes include both primary appraisal mech-
not only emphasizes the importance o f cogni-
anisms (whether a pain-related experience is
tions but also presumes that cognitive factors
judged to be stressful or benign, and if it is
precede and determine the nature o f coping
stressful, whether it is judged to be a threat, a
attempts. A cognitive formulation also pre-
loss, or a challenge) and secondary appraisal
sumes that
processes (cognitions and beliefs about
results in changes in his or her emotional,
the
altering a person's
cognitions
pain, cognitions and beliefs about the self, and
behavioral, cognitive, and physiological cop-
expectations about the utility of certain coping
ing processes. Put another way, the cognitive
Individual Characteristics : \ biological state personality I \. social roles /
/ /
Primary \ Appraisals:
/
Secondary ^ Appraisals:
Coping: emotion cognition behavior physical
cognitive errors beliefs
threat, harm/loss y challenge
Adaptation: function mood health Figure 13.1
Transactional Model of Adjustment to Chronic Pain
formulation assumes that cognitions are the
important
mechanisms through which certain variables
than to increase coping (Geisser, Robinson, &
have their impact on outcome (i.e., cognitions
Riley,
serve as mediators for adaptive change). Although it is far from established that
to reduce negativistic cognitions
1 9 9 9 ; Newton &
Barbaree, 1 9 8 7 ;
T u r k & Rudy, 1 9 9 2 ) . Thorn, Boothby, and Sullivan ( 2 0 0 2 ) suggested that, especially for
cognitive mechanisms are the mediators for
those individuals w h o engage in catastrophic
adaptive change in pain management, several
thinking about pain and pain-related stressors,
models
it may be important t o target treatment toward
of
cognitive-affective
functioning
ascribe antecedent status to cognitive factors in
reducing cognitive errors prior to attempting to
the development
increase coping skills. These and other authors
o f emotional
dysfunction
( B e c k & Emery, 1 9 8 5 ; Riskind & Williams,
(Chaves & Brown, 1 9 8 7 ) hypothesized that
1 9 9 9 ; Vasey & Burkovec, 1 9 9 2 ) . Vlaeyen and
the change mechanism involved in pain man-
Linton ( 2 0 0 0 ) adopted a cognitive framework
agement is the reduction o f maladaptive think-
to account for pain-related disability, suggest-
ing. There are examples in the literature that
ing that catastrophic thinking might represent
provide support for this formulation.
the cognitive precursor t o pain-related behav-
Pain-related
catastrophizing
is a
well-
ioral avoidance. Several psychosocial pain
researched cognitive factor that is a remarkably
researchers have suggested that it may be more
robust predictor
o f pain
responsivity
and
232
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS adjustment
to
chronic
painful
states.
tendency toward maladaptive cognitions could
Catastrophizing is consistently related to higher
moderate levels o f catastrophic and other nega-
levels of self-reported pain as well as to other
tive pain-related thinking. Negative
measures o f pain maladjustment
(Geisser,
related thoughts could also result from primary
1 9 9 4 ; Jacobsen &
threat and loss/harm appraisals, thereby serving
Butler, 1 9 9 6 ; Robinson et a l , 1 9 9 7 ; Sullivan &
as secondary appraisal processes (Thorn et al.,
Robinson, &
Henson,
pain-
Neish, 1 9 9 8 ; Sullivan, Rouse, Bishop, Sc
1 9 9 9 ) . Finally, there has been some suggestion
Johnston, 1 9 9 7 ) .
that catastrophic responses could represent a
Clearly, individuals
who
catastrophize about their pain are less func-
means of seeking support through emotional
tional than persons who do not catastrophize.
disclosure (Sullivan et al., 2 0 0 0 ) . Because social
M o r e important to the cognitive formulation
support is often used as a means o f coping, it
described previously, catastrophizing has been
might be that negative cognitive, emotional,
shown to mediate certain relations between
and behavioral expressions o f pain and its
individual differences (e.g., sex differences) and
potential consequences may be used to gain
pain responsivity (Sullivan, Tripp, Sc Santor,
social proximity, attention, and/or
2 0 0 0 ) . Relatedly, other negative cognitions
from others. Although the primary goal o f the
empathy
associated with pain have been shown to medi-
communal coper may be to gain empathy and
ate the relation between beliefs about pain and
support, and not necessarily to reduce pain, the
certain outcome measures (Stroud, Thorn,
ultimate outcome o f such coping behavior
Jensen, & Boothby, 2 0 0 0 ) . In a recent path
might be maladaptive in that such negative
analysis of multiple potential predictors o f pain,
thought processes, emotions, and behaviors
catastrophizing predicted pain over and above
may contribute to heightened pain experiences
measures of fear o f pain, trait anxiety, and state
(Sullivan et al., 2 0 0 1 ) .
anxiety (Sullivan, Stanish, Sullivan, Sc Tripp, 2 0 0 2 ) , suggesting that although catastrophizing is related to other variables (in this case emotion), catastrophizing is a separate construct and is more predictive of adjustment to pain debate
regarding whether catastrophic thinking and other maladaptive cognitions are a manifestation o f a personality type (Sullivan et al., 2 0 0 1 ) , an appraisal process (Thorn et al., 1 9 9 9 ) , or a coping strategy (Keefe, Lefebvre, &
Clinical
Description
" M a r y ' s " story, a description o f which
than are these other variables. Although there has been some
Diagnostic and Etiological Issues
Smith,
1 9 9 9 ) , using the transactional model described previously, catastrophizing and other negative thoughts could be accommodated at several locations within the model. For example, the trait-like tendencies toward negative affectivity, neuroticism, and emotional vulnerability all have been associated with catastrophic thinking (Affleck, Tennen, Urrows, Sc Higgins, 1 9 9 2 ; Crombez, Eccleston, V a n den Broek, V a n Doudenhove, & Goubert, 2 0 0 2 ; Ward, Thorn, Sc Clements, 2 0 0 2 ) . Thus, the dispositional
follows and which is actually a compilation o f the stories o f many patients who present for pain management treatment in practice, is not at all dissimilar to what health psychologists see in pain clinics across the United States and abroad. M a r y walked slowly and gingerly, in a self-protective manner, from the waiting r o o m into the therapy office and grimaced as she carefully sat in the chair. She said, No hard feelings intended, but I really don't want to be here today. My doctor, after having sent me to six different specialists who were unable to help, told me yesterday that there is nothing else he can do for me and that I've just got to learn to live with this pain. He suggested that I come to you, but I don't think I need a shrink! I mean, my pain
Chronic Pain is real. I'm not just making it up. I actually had to quit a job I dearly loved, one that I had spent years searching for, but as my pain grew worse, I began to feel fatigued more and more often. I also started having trouble remembering things and would get halfway through a story and forget what it was I was even talking about. It became embarrassing and depressing to think that I might endanger the lives of the people I work with, so I first took a leave of absence but eventually quit. I know I'm depressed, but I wouldn't be if I didn't have this pain.
feel depressed, anxious, and hopeless. These feelings lead to increased muscle tension and the production o f harmful stress hormones, which in turn lead to an increase in pain intensity. Negative and exaggerated thoughts and feelings also influence decisions about activity levels. Inactivity leads to muscle deconditioning, so that when patients attempt activity at a future time, it will be uncomfortable or may be excruciatingly painful, thereby influencing their decision to stay in bed. W h e n patients present for pain treatment, they often have been completely sedentary or perhaps even bedridden for
M a r y also described feelings o f despair and hopelessness. She explained that
6 months prior to their first visit.
her
husband, w h o originally was understanding and supportive, had begun to question the veracity o f her condition by saying things
Diagnostic
Considerations
Historically, the health psychologist has
such as the following: "Everybody gets pain,
looked to the D S M (Diagnostic
especially as they age. Y o u ' r e going to need
Manual
of Mental
and
Statistical
for guidance in
Disorders)
to toughen up and learn to live with it."
accurately diagnosing patients with chronic
M a r y was tearful and angry. N o t only was
pain
her pain negatively affecting her relationship
Psychiatric Association, 1 9 6 8 ) provided no
with her husband, but she had noticed that
specific
some o f her friends had stopped calling—
Therefore, pain patients o f this era were
"probably because I had refused many invi-
typically given a D S M - I I diagnosis o f "psycho-
conditions. T h e D S M - I I diagnosis
pertaining
(American to
pain.
tations to go shopping and to go to the gym
physiological
like I did all the time before this pain."
emotional factors were thought to influence
disorder,"
especially
when
The first several sessions with a health
their painful condition. Psychogenic pain dis-
psychologist are primarily devoted to rapport
order appeared under the rubric o f somato-
building, ensuring pain patients that the psy-
form disorders in the DSM-III (American
chologist believes their pain is real, helping
Psychiatric Association, 1 9 8 0 ) . Three major
patients to understand that feeling depressed
criteria were required for a diagnosis o f psy-
about their pain is actually a normal response
chogenic pain disorder: severe and prolonged
to an abnormal situation, and beginning the
pain, no known organic pathology, and psy-
process of educating patients about the impor-
chological factors (e.g., pain onset is positively
tant body-mind connection. When patients'
correlated with psychological conflict) that are
bodies hurt and they have been diagnosed with
etiologically involved in the pain.
controversial
illnesses (e.g.,
fibromyalgia,
In the DSM-III-R (American Psychiatric
chronic fatigue syndrome), they strive to make
Association, 1 9 8 7 ) , psychogenic pain disorder
sense of this experience. They soon may begin
was
to have exaggerated and distorted
because demonstrating
thoughts
about their current condition, and the follow-
renamed
somatoform that
pain pain
disorder is psy-
chogenic proved to be nearly impossible. T w o
ing may be a common comment: " I ' m never
major criteria were required for a diagnosis of
going to feel better. M y pain is ruining my life."
somatoform pain disorder:
Having catastrophic thoughts leads patients to
with pain" for at least 6 months and no known
"preoccupation
233
234
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS organic pathology (or when organic pathology
100%
is found, the complaint of pain and functional
comorbid depression, according to the studies
[e.g., social, occupational] impairment is not
they reviewed. Fishbain, Goldberg, Meagher,
commensurate with physical findings
o f patients with chronic pain have
but
Steele, and Rosomoff ( 1 9 8 6 ) found depression
rather is grossly exaggerated). T h e D S M - I V
and chronic pain to coexist approximately
subcommittee on pain disorders found that the
5 0 % o f the time. Panic disorder is diagnosed
D S M - M - R somatoform pain disorder diagno-
in 1 6 % to 2 5 % o f patients presenting to
sis was rarely used because there appeared to
emergency rooms with chest pain (Beitman
be no consensus on what "preoccupation with
et al., 1 9 9 1 ; Carney, Freedland, Ludbrook,
pain" meant. In addition, there was difficulty
Saunders,
in determining whether the patient's response
Feinmann, and Harris ( 1 9 9 2 ) reported that
&
Jaffe,
1990).
Aghabeigi,
to pain was being grossly exaggerated. There
2 1 % of fibromyalgia patients in their clinic
was also disagreement on the appropriateness
carried a dual diagnosis o f pain disorder and
of this diagnosis for individuals with disabling
posttraumatic stress disorder. Other studies
pain due to known medical conditions.
purport that anywhere from 5 0 % to 1 0 0 % of
In the D S M - I V
(American Psychiatric
pain patients entering clinical treatment for
Association, 1 9 9 4 ) , somatoform pain disorder
pain management have comorbid posttrau-
was renamed as pain disorder. Diagnostic cri-
matic
teria included pain in at least one specific area
Bertrand, & Maciewicz, 2 0 0 0 ) . It appears to
stress
disorder
(Kulich,
Mencher,
of the body significant enough to warrant clin-
be the case, then, that the D S M - I V pain disor-
ical attention, pain that causes distress or func-
der diagnosis, like its D S M predecessors
tional (e.g., social, occupational) impairment,
(somatoform pain disorder and psychogenic
psychological issues that are thought to play a
pain disorder), remains a diagnosis o f exclu-
significant part in important components o f
sion with ambiguous inclusion criteria.
the pain (onset, frequency, duration, severity, exacerbation, or maintenance), pain that is not intentionally produced or feigned (as in
CO-OCCURRING
factitious disorder or malingering), and pain
PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS
that is not better accounted for by a mood, anxiety, or psychotic disorder and does not
As indicated previously, chronic pain patients
meet criteria for dyspareunia.
often have symptoms consistent with a mood
T h e D S M - I V pain disorder diagnosis has
disorder, namely depression. It is common for
been criticized for a number o f reasons. First,
these patients to argue that depression is their
no guidelines are provided for determining
reaction to the limitations placed on them via
when psychological factors significantly affect
their chronic pain condition. In other words, if
pain or are sufficient to warrant
they did not have chronic pain, they would not
separate
D S M - I V diagnoses in addition to pain disor-
be depressed. Whether or not this is true is of
der. There is a high prevalence of depression
little actual consequence in terms of treatment
and anxiety disorders among chronic pain
considerations. W h a t health psychologists do
patients, yet under the D S M - I V criteria, these
know is that alleviating depressive symptoms in
patients who seemingly would be most appro-
chronic pain patients, regardless of the depres-
priate for this diagnostic category could con-
sive etiology, not only makes their pain more
ceivably be excluded due to the criterion that
tolerable but also lifts their mood, both of
"pain is not better accounted for by a M o o d ,
which serve to improve the quality o f their lives.
Anxiety, . . . Disorder." R o m a n o and Turner
O n the other hand, chronic pain patients may
( 1 9 8 5 ) reported that anywhere from 1 0 % to
view a diagnosis of depression (or anxiety) as a
Chronic Pain
235
sign that their pain is not real—a common
or representative o f a deficit or incapacity.
concern for pain patients. They may perceive
Others believe pain to be the most common
mood and anxiety disorder diagnoses as dis-
conversion symptom
counting their pain and may be prone to ask
Myer, 1 9 6 0 ) . Sullivan ( 2 0 0 1 ) purported that
(Ziegler, Imboden,
&
questions such as the following: "Since I have
although there may be some components of
depression, does that mean that my pain is all in
conversion disorder present in chronic pain
my head?" T h e pain specialist/health profes-
patients (secondary gain), classifying these
sional can typically remedy this misunder-
patients with a conversion disorder is generally
standing by assuring these patients that their
not useful.
depressive or anxious characteristics are actually a normal response to abnormal circumstances.
Although the diagnosis o f somatization disorder is rarely used, it warrants a few brief
Substance-induced mood disorder can also
statements. Several specific criteria are required
occur in chronic pain patients. Due to these
for this diagnosis, and these are detailed in the
patients' compromised health conditions, dif-
D S M - I V (American Psychiatric Association,
ferent physicians frequently prescribe numer-
1 9 9 4 ) under somatoform disorders. Prevalence
ous medications. For this reason, it is a good
estimates for somatization disorder range from
idea to recommend that pain patients main-
0 . 1 3 % to 0 . 4 0 % in the community (Smith,
tain a list o f all current medications, indicating
1 9 9 1 ) , from 0 . 2 % to 5 . 0 % in primary care set-
specific dosages and frequencies with which
tings (Sullivan, 2 0 0 1 ) , and from 8 % to 1 2 % in
they take each o f them, and to make a point
pain clinics (Kouyanou, Pither, Rabe-Hesketh,
of
&
providing
each
o f their
health
care
Wessely, 1 9 9 8 ) . Sullivan ( 2 0 0 1 )
stated,
providers with this list. Medications known
"Somatization as a process, meaning the somatic
to evoke m o o d symptoms include, but are not
experience
limited to, steroids, anticonvulsants, antihy-
Somatization disorder is a rare, chronic, and
pertensives, anti-Parkinsonian
medications,
treatment-resistant condition that characterizes
antiulcer medications, cardiac medications, oral
the most severely and chronically distressed
of
distress,
is
ubiquitous. . . .
individuals" (p. 2 8 4 ) . Thus, although many
contraceptives, and muscle relaxants. Anxiety disorders, such as panic disorder
chronic pain patients might initially appear to
fre-
be highly focused and preoccupied on somatic
quently associated with chronic pain disorders.
functioning, meeting full criteria for a diagnosis
In addition to the studies cited earlier, Stewart,
of somatization disorder is relatively rare.
and posttraumatic
stress disorder, are
Breslau, and Keck (1994) reported that migraine
It is imperative for the health psychologist to
headaches are more strongly correlated with
have at least a basic understanding of common
panic attacks than are any other types of
chronic pain conditions (where pain is the pri-
headache. In a community assessment o f
mary symptom) and other related health condi-
1 0 , 0 0 0 individuals who presented to their pri-
tions (where pain is an associated symptom but
mary care physicians with complaints o f
not the primary one) when patients present for
headache, 1 5 % of females and 1 3 % of males
clinical pain management.
had a history of panic disorder (Sullivan, 2 0 0 1 ).
chronic pain conditions include, but are not
Whether or not pain disorders should be
limited to, fibromyalgia, headache, chronic low
Some c o m m o n
classified as conversion disorders has been, and
back pain, and complex regional pain syn-
continues to be, a point of contention among
drome. Other related health conditions include
health care professionals. Some do not support
chronic fatigue syndrome, multiple chemical
the notion that pain conditions can be classified
sensitivities, and lupus. By and large, the etiol-
as conversion disorder symptoms because most
ogy, nature, and treatment o f these conditions
pain conditions are not neurological in nature
are considered controversial among health care
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS
236
professionals. Before describing specific aspects
approximately 2 % of the population
of each, it may be useful to look at common
fibromyalgia.
challenges that nearly all chronic pain patients
has
According to the International Headache
face regardless o f etiology. Taylor, Friedberg,
Society's
and Jason (2001) identified the following seven
Criteria for Headache Disorders, Cranial
challenges. First, these patients' symptoms and
Neuralgias, and
"Classification
and
Facial P a i n "
Diagnostic (Headache
degree of impairment vary significantly across
Classification Committee o f the International
individuals and chronic pain conditions (e.g.,
Headache
one fibromyalgia patient may be working full-
tension-type headaches are the most com-
time, whereas another is bedridden). Second,
monly diagnosed chronic headache disorders.
treatments (e.g., pharmacological, psychologi-
Pain characteristics o f tension-type headaches
Society,
1988),
migraine
and
cal, physical) for chronic pain conditions are
(previously called tension, muscle contraction,
ameliorative in nature and typically do not cure
stress, or ordinary headaches) include a press-
these disorders. Third, these chronic pain con-
ing and/or tightening sensation o f a mild to
ditions are much more prevalent
among
moderate intensity occurring bilaterally, with
women than among men (e.g., women com-
minimal aggravation by routine physical activ-
prise approximately 7 0 % of chronic fatigue
ity (Rapoport & Sheftell, 1 9 9 6 ) . Typical pain
patients, 9 0 % of fibromyalgia patients, and
characteristics for migraine headaches include
7 5 % o f multiple chemical sensitivities patients).
a throbbing and/or pulsating sensation o f
Fourth, there is high comorbidity
among
moderate to severe intensity occurring unilat-
chronic pain conditions (e.g., many fibromyal-
erally, with aggravation by routine physical
gia patients have chronic fatigue as well). Fifth,
activity. T h e presence o f nausea and/or vomit-
recovery rates are minimal (and that is why
ing along with photophobia and phonophobia
they are referred to as chronic conditions).
is usually characteristic o f migraineurs exclu-
Sixth, patients incur multiple losses of an eco-
sively, although
nomic and social nature (e.g., disability and
experienced to a lesser degree by tension-type
social isolation). Seventh, there is a high level o f
headache sufferers
these symptoms (Rapoport &
may
be
Sheftell,
psychiatric comorbidity (e.g., many patients
1 9 9 6 ) . Although the exact cause o f headache
with fibromyalgia also have depression and
pain remains a mystery, several theories have
anxiety disorders).
been generated. F o r migraine
In 1 9 9 0 , the American College o f Rheumatology officially recognized fibromyalgia as an illness with the following classification criteria: (a)
history o f widespread
muscular
pain
and (b) tenderness in 1 1 of 18 "tender points" located in specific areas o f the body (Wolfe etal., 1 9 9 0 ) . Fatigue, disrupted sleep, headache, irritable bowel, and irritable bladder are also characteristic symptoms o f this disease. The etiology o f fibromyalgia is unknown, but many affected individuals and their health care professionals report that symptoms
often
appear following surgery, following an injury of a traumatic nature, or following an acute
headaches,
Rapoport and Sheftell ( 1 9 9 6 ) reported three major theories, namely that (a) changes occur in cerebral blood flow and electrical cortical activity; (b) neurogenic inflammation, produced by the release o f Substance Ρ and other neurotransmitters, impinges on the site where trigeminal nerve endings meet durai arteries; and (c) the size o f peripheral blood vessels is altered (e.g., constricted, dilated) in response to some unknown noradrenergic and serotonergic disruption. T h e etiology of some tensiontype
increase
in
pericranial muscle activity ( R a p o p o r t
headaches
involves
an
&
Sheftell, 1 9 9 6 ) .
medical illness (Waylonis & Perkins, 1 9 9 4 ) .
The lifetime prevalence of low back pain
Wolfe and colleagues ( 1 9 9 0 ) estimated that
is estimated to be 7 0 % for individuals in
Chronic
Pain
developed countries. Approximately 1 6 % of all
and colleagues ( 1 9 9 4 ) described the current
workmen's compensation claims are low back
U.S. diagnostic criteria as (a) prolonged (at
pain related, and the estimated cost o f lost
least 6 months) overwhelming fatigue that is
productivity is enormous (Amundson, 2 0 0 2 ) .
not ameliorated with rest, where (b) during the
There are many causes o f chronic low back
same 6-month period o f chronic fatigue, the
pain, including muscle spasm and tightness,
individual experiences at least four o f eight
whiplash, osteoarthritis (or spondylosis, a
minor symptoms (sore throat, cognitive dys-
degenerative process affecting the normal func-
function [short-term memory or concentration
tion and structure o f the spine), osteoporosis
problems], cervical or axillary lymph node ten-
(degeneration o f bone mass), herniated disc,
derness, muscle pain, joint pain, development
sciatica (pain [lower back, buttocks, radiating
of new type o f headache, sleep disturbance, or
down the thighs and sometimes into the feet]
malaise) for at least 2 4 hours. T h e etiology o f
that follows the sciatic nerve), compression
chronic
fracture (vertebral body that is crushed by
although medical researchers have speculated
external force to the spine), spinal stenosis (nar-
for years that viruses such as Epstein-Barr,
rowing of small nerve passageway called fora-
cytomegalovirus, and herpes simplex virus
fatigue
syndrome
is
unknown,
men, causing nerve compression, swelling, and
(Types I and II), or an overactive immune sys-
pain), and osteomyelitis (infection in the spine).
tem, may have a role in causing the syndrome.
It is not unusual for individuals with low back pain to have no structural abnormalities that can
be detected on physical examinations,
Multiple chemical sensitivities, a recognized
disorder
by
the
American
Lung
Association, the American Medical Associa-
X rays, or magnetic resonance imaging proce-
tion,
dures (MRIs). In addition, patients who have
Agency, and the U.S. Consumer
undergone repeated back surgeries may have
Safety Commission, remains a controversial
pain from scar tissue that is pressing on nerves.
health condition among many health care
Complex regional pain syndrome, type 1,
professionals. Taylor and colleagues ( 2 0 0 1 )
also known as reflex sympathetic dystrophy,
referred to it as " a chronic condition o f
Sudeck's dystrophy, or hand/shoulder
syn-
irritation or inflammation o f sensory organs,
drome, is a condition that can develop after
gastrointestinal distress, fatigue, and c o m -
traumatic injury to the extremities (Caudill,
promised neurological function,
2 0 0 2 ) but has also been known to develop
learning and memory deficits, hypersensitivity
Harden,
to unpleasant smells, tingling o f nerves, and
2 0 0 1 ) . A primary symptom is autonomic
sensory discomfort" (p. 5 ) . These symptoms
spontaneously
(Bruehl, Steger, &
nervous system dysfunction
that
includes
the
U . S . Environmental
Protection Product
including
appear to be triggered by exposure to certain
swelling, increased sweating, blood vessel
chemical
constriction or dilation, and severe pain in the
reported to be primarily pesticides, detergent
agents,
which
Donnay
(1998)
involved extremity. T h e diagnosis o f complex
residues, and perfumes. In California and
regional pain syndrome with specific criteria
N e w M e x i c o , public health department sur-
agreed on by the International Association for
veys reveal prevalence rates to be anywhere
the Study o f Pain was developed in 1 9 9 4 .
from 2 % to 6 % (Taylor et al., 2 0 0 1 ) .
Chronic fatigue syndrome, a significant
Lupus, or systemic lupus erythematosus, is
public health concern, lowers the quality of all
thought to be an autoimmune disorder that
aspects o f life (e.g., social, occupational, inter-
affects multiple organ systems and that is char-
personal) for approximately 0 . 4 2 % o f the U.S.
acterized by skin lesions (butterfly rash) on sun
population or 8 0 0 , 0 0 0 individuals (Anderson
exposure, arthritis and pain in multiple joints,
& Ferrans, 1 9 9 7 ; Jason et al., 1 9 9 9 ) . Fukuda
chronic kidney infection, and blood vessel
237
238
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS inflammation (causing peripheral neuropathy
related to ease o f use and ability o f patients
in hands and feet or causing pain, ulceration,
to conceptualize their pain in the manner
and infection in lungs, heart, and intestines).
requested (Williams, 1 9 9 6 ) .
T h e etiology may be due at least partially to a genetic defect in Β lymphocytes, which produce autoantibodies that attack cells (F. A.
Pain
Experience.
Although assessing pain
intensity is an important aspect of any pain eval-
Davis, 1 9 9 7 ) . Although some individuals die
uation, other aspects o f the pain experience are
from this disease (i.e., with multiple organ
also critical in fully understanding the patient's
system involvement), medical researchers are
perspective. Measures such as the McGill
helping lupus patients to live much longer
Pain Questionnaire (Melzack, 1987), the West
and higher quality lives than ever before
Haven-Yale Multidimensional Pain Inventory
(Lahita, 2 0 0 1 ) .
(Kerns, Turk, & Rudy, 1 9 8 5 ) , and the Sickness Impact Profile (Roland & Morris, 1 9 8 3 ) provide information regarding sensory and emo-
ASSESSMENT AND TREATMENT
tional components o f pain, physical activity level, spouse responses to pain, and overall psychosocial functioning. Pain behaviors can also
Pain Assessment
be included under the rubric of overall pain
T o effectively treat chronic pain, the health
experience and can be assessed through self-
psychologist must fully assess the individual's
report or observation of the patient. Pain behav-
pain experience and also explore variables
iors include grimacing, moaning, limping, lying
known to affect treatment response. A general
down, and consuming pain medications. Many
strategy for pain assessment is presented here,
pain behaviors are often reinforcing and lead
with specific examples of assessment instru-
to higher levels of disability. For example, if
ments and information about h o w individual
the spouse of a chronic pain patient attempts to
variables affect treatment outcome. Evaluation
comfort the patient whenever he or she appears
strategies for specific pain problems or medical
to be hurting, the patient may learn that certain
disorders are not discussed.
pain behaviors result in attention and sympathy. Keefe and Hill (1985) developed a methodology
Pain Intensity.
Self-reported pain intensity
for
assessing pain
behavior that
requires
is believed to be a good measure o f overall
videotaping, whereas other clinicians have
pain and suffering. Several methods exist for
developed checklists to evaluate behavioral
obtaining this information from patients. For
responses
example, numerical rating
to
pain
(Waddell, M c C u l l o c h ,
scales require
Kummel, & Venner, 1 9 8 0 ) . Pain assessments
patients to rate the intensity o f their pain
that address the broader pain experience have
using a number, for example, from 0 to 1 0 .
the potential of offering more detailed treatment
Verbal rating scales ask patients to describe
recommendations and of providing a standard
their pain with an adjective chosen from a list
of comparison for patients'
that can then be quantified. Visual analog
functioning.
posttreatment
scales are straight lines with verbal intensity descriptors on the end points and require patients to rate their pain by placing a m a r k on the line. Clinicians must then use a ruler to measure where the mark occurs so as to quantify the rating. Each o f these methods has its strengths and weaknesses, primarily
Pain
Beliefs
and
Cognitions.
Cognitive
factors, such as beliefs, appraisals, and cognitive distortions, play an important role in adjustment to pain and the development o f disability ( D e G o o d & Shutty, 1 9 9 2 ) . F o r example, the critical role o f catastrophizing
Chronic Pain in altering the pain experience was highlighted
assessment o f cognitions that are more illness
earlier in this chapter. In addition to catas-
specific.
trophizing, research has shown other negative cognitions to be associated with poorer functioning a m o n g chronic pain patients. For example,
DeGood
and
Shutty
(1992)
Pain Coping.
Coping consists o f cognitive
and behavioral efforts undertaken to manage stressful
situations.
How
chronic
pain
described the importance o f low self-efficacy
patients cope with the pain experience is
and beliefs that pain is mysterious or out o f
related to their physical and psychosocial
the individual's control in predicting poor
functioning. Coping attempts that are pas-
pain treatment response. Fear o f pain and
sive, such as hoping or praying for an end to
reinjury is also associated with poor pain
the pain, are often associated with higher
outcomes, and recent research has revealed
levels o f physical disability and psychological
that
dysfunction than are more active strategies
anxiety sensitivity exacerbates pain-
related
fear
and
leads to
physical activity (Asmundson,
avoidance of
for
1996). Fear o f
Jensen,
pain is associated with selective attentional bias for pain-related Ellery, Hunt,
information (Keogh,
8c Hannent, 2001), potentially
coping
(Boothby, Thorn,
Stroud, &
1999). Catastrophizing is sometimes
conceptualized as a coping strategy, and as described previously, catastrophizing is consistently related to p o o r
pain
outcomes
resulting in more negative pain experiences
(Sullivan et al., 2001). Although active coping
for individuals with elevated fears o f pain.
strategies, such as distraction techniques and
Finally, several studies
that
using positive coping self-statements, are not
(e.g., overgeneralization,
generally associated with negative pain out-
selective abstraction) are related to depressive
comes, they are also not consistently corre-
symptoms in chronic pain patients (Lefebvre,
lated with positive outcomes (Boothby et al.,
cognitive errors
have found
Smith, Christensen, Peck, 8c W a r d ,
1999). As a result, some pain researchers
1994) and that pain treatment that addresses
have suggested that it is more important to
cognitive factors (e.g., cognitive errors) is
decrease passive coping or negative coping
1981;
effective at alleviating depression in this pop-
attempts rather than to only teach more
ulation ( M o r e n o , Cunningham, Gatchel, &
adaptive,
Mayer,
1991). Pain research has also
revealed that changes in negative cognitions and beliefs m o r e generally are also associated with
improvement
following
multidisci-
plinary pain treatment (Jensen, Turner, 8c Romano,
2001).
et al.,
active coping strategies (Geisser
1999; N e w t o n 8c Barbaree, 1987).
Several measures exist for the assessment o f pain coping. T h e Coping Strategies Questionnaire (Rosenstiel 8c Keefe, 1983) was specifically developed to assess coping among pain patients, and it is the most frequently used
M a n y instruments exist for the measure-
instrument o f its kind. T h e Cognitive Coping
ment o f pain-related beliefs and cognitive
Strategies
factors. A few o f these measures include
Beaulieu, Schwebel, & Thorn, 1989) and the
Inventory
the Pain Beliefs and Perceptions Inventory
Vanderbilt
Pain
(Butler,
Management
Damarin, Inventory
1989), the Inventory o f
(Brown 8c Nicassio, 1987) are also inventories
Negative Thoughts in Response to Pain (Gil,
specific to pain coping assessment. Numerous
1990), the Pain
instruments exist for the evaluation o f coping
Scale (Sullivan, Bishop, 8t
strategies more generally. However, it is rec-
1995), and the Pain Anxiety Symptoms 8t Gross, 1992).
ommended that a pain-specific instrument be
(Williams & T h o r n ,
Williams, Keefe, & B e c k h a m , Catastrophizing Pivik,
Scale ( M c C r a c k e n , Zayfert,
used whenever possible because coping is
A variety o f similar instruments exist for the
often
conceptualized as situation specific
239
240
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS (Lazarus, 1 9 9 3 ) and patients might cope with
had higher pretreatment levels o f depression,
chronic pain
anxiety, psychoticism, and overall distress than
differently
from
how
they
approach other stressors.
did patients who returned to work. Gatchel, Polatin, and M a y e r ( 1 9 9 5 ) evaluated a large Individuals with
sample o f acute low back patients and found
chronic pain often have associated emotional
higher levels of self-reported pain and disabil-
difficulties.
ity as well as higher scores on Scale 3 (hysteria)
Emotional
Functioning.
E m o t i o n a l reactions, such
as
depression and anxiety, are c o m m o n respon-
of the M M P I to be associated with a lack o f
ses to persistent pain and can serve to c o m -
return to work after 1 year. Thus, evaluating
pound the original pain problem and lead to
patients for the presence o f these symptoms
more
and disorders is an important piece in poten-
severe
impairment
and
disability.
Research has demonstrated consistently that
tially preventing
certain types o f psychopathology are associ-
planning effective treatment. T h e assessment o f
severe disability and
in
ated with poorer adjustment to chronic pain
psychological symptoms and disorders can be
and poorer treatment response (Block, 1 9 9 6 ;
undertaken using self-report measures that are
Brennan, Barrett, & Garretson, 1 9 8 6 ; Gaskin,
common to clinicians in other areas of psy-
Greene, Robinson, & Geisser, 1 9 9 2 ) .
chological practice (e.g., M M P I ,
Rates o f depression, anxiety, and anger have been shown to be higher among chronic pain patients than in community
Checklist-90-Revised,
Beck
Symptom Depression
Inventory).
samples
(Banks &c Kerns, 1 9 9 6 ; Fernandez & Turk, 1 9 9 5 ; Gaskin et al., 1 9 9 2 ) . Research using the
Acute Pain Management
Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory
Health psychologists are involved in the
( M M P I ) has demonstrated that patients with
management o f acute pain less frequently than
chronic pain tend to have higher elevations on
they are in the management of chronic pain.
scales measuring preoccupation with somatic
Because acute pain is by definition time limited,
complaints, depression, and the development
it is managed primarily with pharmacological
of physical symptoms in response to stressful
agents. However, research has shown
situations (Etscheidt, Steger, & Braverman,
health psychologists can make
1 9 9 5 ) . Personality disorders are also quite
contributions to acute pain
that
important
management.
prevalent among chronic pain patients, with
Chapman and Turner ( 1 9 8 6 ) proposed three
one study finding avoidant, dependent, and
goals for interventions with patients undergo-
obsessive-compulsive personality disorders to
ing painful medical procedures: (a) increasing
be particularly c o m m o n (Elliott, J a c k s o n ,
the patients' knowledge about the procedure,
Layfield, & Kendall, 1 9 9 6 ) . Other researchers
(b) enhancing a sense of control over the proce-
reported that more than 5 0 % of chronic low
dure and the pain experience, and (c) improv-
back pain patients involved in a functional
ing the patients' ability to diminish emotional
restoration program met criteria for a person-
responses to pain that increase stress responses.
ality disorder, with paranoid personality dis-
Research has shown
that
information-
order being the most c o m m o n (Gatchel,
based strategies can be effective at reducing
Polatin, Mayer, & Garcy, 1 9 9 4 ) .
fear and anxiety associated with medical pro-
Higher
levels o f psychopathology
are
cedures as well as at decreasing pain intensity
treatment
during the procedure (Suis & W a n , 1 9 8 9 ) .
response and continued disability. Robbins,
Instructing patients in the use o f relaxation
M o o d y , Hahn, and Weaver ( 1 9 9 6 ) found that
techniques serves to decrease sympathetic
chronic pain patients who did not return to
nervous system arousal and also provides a
work following multidisciplinary
means of enhancing personal control over the
commonly associated with poor
treatment
Chronic Vain
241
pain experience. Relaxation techniques consist
Nedeljkovic, & Katz, 1 9 9 8 ; Moulin e t a l . ,
of deep breathing exercises, progressive mus-
1 9 9 6 ) . Researchers have also suggested that
cle relaxation, guided imagery, biofeedback,
using narcotic analgesics as the sole treatment
and hypnosis. T o date, n o one specific relax-
program without incorporating psychosocial
ation technique has been shown to be signifi-
interventions can undermine overall rehabilita-
cantly more effective than any other for acute
tion and functional restoration
(Turk
&
pain management (Williams, 1 9 9 9 ) . Several
Meichenbaum, 1 9 9 4 ) . Thus, opioids are often
studies have shown that patients who receive
combined with other medical and behavioral
psychological interventions, such as training
treatments to manage chronic pain most effec-
in relaxation methods, prior to painful medi-
tively. Despite strong concerns on the part o f
cal procedures report less pain, use less opioid
medical
analgesics, report less anxiety, and recover
regarding the potential for addiction to opioid
professionals
and
patients
alike
more quickly than do patients who receive
medications, there is a relatively low risk of
standard medical management (Gil, 1 9 8 4 ;
addiction in patients without a history o f sub-
Schultheis, Peterson, & Selby, 1 9 8 7 ) .
stance abuse if the drug is administered orally (Merskey &
Moulin, 1 9 9 9 ) . Nevertheless,
chronic administration o f any drug, particu-
Chronic Pain Management
larly multiple analgesic agents, does
not
Pharmaco-
enhance function and possibly contributes to
logical agents are the first line o f treatment for
pain-related disabilities. It is estimated that 3 %
Pharmacological
Interventions.
acute pain, and these agents are often used
to 1 8 % o f patients seeking treatment
aggressively in an effort to bring rapid relief
chronic pain have substance abuse problems
for
from pain. Centrally acting analgesics are com-
(Fishbain, Rosomoff, 8c Rosomoff, 1 9 9 2 ) .
monly used in the treatment o f acute pain. However, the treatment o f chronic pain tends
Surgical
and Other
Medical
Interventions.
to be more complex, with the use of a wider
A variety o f medical interventions exist for the
variety of pharmacological agents and with the
management o f chronic pain, ranging from
goal of managing the pain condition for a
more radical procedures, such as surgery, to
longer duration. Examples of pharmacological
more benign types o f interventions, such as
agents used to treat chronic pain include nar-
stimulation therapies. Surgery for chronic pain
cotic analgesics (e.g., Lortab, Percodan), non-
conditions is most appropriate when nonsur-
narcotic analgesics (e.g., Ibuprofen, Tylenol),
gical methods have failed to provide pain relief
muscle relaxants (e.g., Flexeril, Robaxin), topi-
and when an individual is severely disabled
cal analgesics (e.g., Z o v a x ) , anticonvulsants
due to the pain. A psychological evaluation is
(e.g., Neurontin,
Tegretol), antidepressants
often a useful precursor to invasive procedures
(e.g., Paxil, Prozac), and antianxiety medica-
given that studies have found a number o f
tions (e.g., Librium, Ativan).
psychosocial risk factors, such as negative
Although narcotic analgesics are commonly
affect, certain personality types, catastrophiz-
used in the treatment of chronic pain, there are
ing, and a history o f medicolegal problems,
mixed findings regarding their utility. Some
to be predictive o f poor surgical outcome
studies have found the long-term use o f opioids
(Block, 1 9 9 6 ) .
to provide significant pain relief (Portenoy &
Stimulation therapies, such as transcuta-
Foley, 1 9 8 6 ) and to increase functional status
neous electrical nerve stimulation ( T E N S ) , are
(Zenz, Strumpf, & Tryba, 1 9 9 2 ) , whereas
another medical intervention for the treatment
others have found opioids to provide only
of pain. Stimulation therapies are based on the
minor pain relief without improvements in
concept o f counter-irritation, in which large-
activity levels (Jamison, Raymond, Slawsby,
diameter afferent fibers are stimulated and
242
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS serve to temper pain transmission by "closing
sensations. One goal of C B T is to reduce
the gate" on the transmission o f pain signals
muscle tension through the use o f one or more
(Melzack, 1 9 7 5 ) . T E N S has been shown to be
relaxation strategies. Biofeedback, progressive
an effective intervention for individuals with
muscle relaxation, and guided imagery all have
low back pain, peripheral nerve damage, and
been shown to be effective at reducing muscle
degenerative musculoskeletal disease, among
tension. Studies that have attempted to com-
other conditions (Melzack, 1 9 7 5 ; Meyler, de
pare the effectiveness of these relaxation tech-
Jongste, & Rolf, 1 9 9 4 ) . However, patients
niques have found no significant differences
who report high levels of psychological dis-
(Holroyd &c Penzien, 1 9 8 5 ; Spence, Sharpe,
tress do not respond as favorably to T E N S
Newton-John, &
(Meyler et al., 1 9 9 4 ) .
choice of which strategy to use with a specific
Champion,
1995).
The
patient will depend largely on patient characBecause
teristics such as age, personality, and overall
chronic pain reflects more than just physical
motivation. For example, the physical tensing
Cognitive-Behavioral
Interventions.
symptomatology, psychosocial treatments are
and releasing of tension called for in progressive
useful in addressing the psychological and
muscle relaxation might fatigue some elderly
environmental difficulties common to pain
individuals. Other individuals might believe
patients. Although cognitive-behavioral ther-
that the active participation and physical exer-
apy (CBT) for pain is a multifaceted approach
tion required by this strategy are required to see
with many goals and objectives, a primary goal
success, and they have difficulty in grasping the
of this treatment approach is to assist chronic
idea that passive interventions, such as guided
pain patients in understanding that pain is
imagery, will result in improvement. Still other
manageable and that disability and depression
patients will be convinced of a treatment's
are not inevitable consequences o f living with
worth only if the strategy appears to be more
a pain condition. Additional treatment goals
"medical" and so will buy into the usefulness of
often focus on developing more adaptive meth-
biofeedback as a treatment approach. W h a t
ods of thinking, feeling, and behaving that will
appears to be more important than
result in increased physical activity, improved
relaxation strategy is incorporated into treat-
interpersonal relationships, and decreased psy-
ment is how patients perceive the intervention
chological distress.
and whether they believe in its utility.
which
W h a t constitutes a C B T program for pain
Another behavioral approach common in
varies from clinician to clinician. Because
the treatment o f pain is operant conditioning
C B T is comprised o f many techniques and
for the reduction of pain behaviors. Operant
interventions, clinicians often pick and choose
conditioning relies on extinction paradigms to
from among those techniques that seem most
reduce overt pain behaviors and uses rein-
fitting for individual clients. For example,
forcement strategies to increase well behaviors.
cognitive and behavioral interventions are
For example, to increase a patient's overall
often quite distinct in their focus but are
activity level, uptime and exercise are gradually
commonly used in conjunction with
one
increased and the patient is reinforced for this
another to treat pain. Frequently used C B T
activity. Similarly, to decrease a patient's ver-
approaches for chronic pain treatment are
bal complaints o f pain, treatment providers
discussed in what follows, but the reader
ignore such instances and instead reinforce the
should bear in mind that many alternative
patient for the absence o f overt pain behaviors.
approaches exist.
The cooperation of a patient's spouse or family
Muscle tension is a c o m m o n
problem
often is enlisted to apply the operant condi-
and
tioning methods within the home. Operant
tensing are often automatic responses to pain
methods have been shown to be particularly
among pain patients because guarding
Chronic Pain
243
effective at increasing chronic pain patients'
coping skills training programs is often to arm
physical functioning and thereby increasing
patients with an arsenal of skills for decreasing
return to work (Fordyce, 1 9 8 8 ) .
pain, increasing functioning, and
Cognitive interventions for pain management
include cognitive restructuring
improving
their overall quality o f life. As mentioned previ-
and
ously, some pain researchers believe that reduc-
cognitive coping skills training. Cognitive
ing maladaptive coping attempts is more critical
restructuring is commonly used in the treat-
to treatment success than is increasing more
ment o f depression and anxiety, and
this
positive coping strategies (Geisser etal., 1 9 9 9 ;
approach can be adapted to address cognitive
Newton & Barbaree, 1 9 8 7 ) . However, research
errors accompanying chronic pain. T h e ratio-
has not been conducted to evaluate this issue
nale o f cognitive interventions for pain is that
specifically. Moreover, even if reducing passive
changing a person's cognitions results in emo-
or negative coping strategies is critical for
tional, behavioral, and physiological changes,
patient improvement, most patients
would
and there is evidence that cognitive mecha-
likely benefit to some degree from also learning
nisms are involved in improvement in adap-
more active and positive methods for coping
tive functioning among chronic pain patients.
with pain.
T h o r n and colleagues ( 2 0 0 2 ) proposed a cognitive treatment framework for specifically
Multidisciplinary
Treatment.
Given that
reducing catastrophizing, a c o m m o n cognitive
the experience o f pain is best understood in a
distortion among pain patients. This treat-
biopsychosocial framework, it follows that
ment approach is based in part on cognitive
pain treatment should also address each o f
therapy for depression (Beck, 1 9 9 5 ) , and it
these areas—the physical, the psychological,
assists patients in first identifying maladaptive
and the environmental. Multidisciplinary pain
thinking patterns and then challenging and
clinics have been established to provide such
replacing those dysfunctional thoughts with
comprehensive treatment and are typically
m o r e adaptive
staffed
thinking
styles.
Although
by health care professionals
from
T h o r n and colleagues focused specifically on
many disciplines, including physicians, nurses,
catastrophizing,
physical therapists, occupational therapists,
other
maladaptive
pain
beliefs and attitudes, such as those described
and psychologists (Turk &c Stacey,
earlier in the chapter, can easily be a focus o f
T h e first pain clinic was established at the
the intervention. In addition, T h o r n and col-
University of Washington in 1 9 6 1 , and it is
leagues expanded their treatment approach to
estimated that more than
address other factors that might
maintain
plinary pain centers exist in the United States
catastrophic thinking. For example, patients
today (Okifuji, Turk, & Kalauokalani, 1 9 9 9 ) .
are introduced to assertiveness skills training as a means o f meeting interpersonal needs rather than relying on catastrophizing as a strategy for garnering social support.
1997).
3 5 0 multidisci-
Although multidisciplinary programs vary in their treatment approaches, most include an assessment process that attempts to identify the specific needs o f a patient so that treatment can
Cognitive coping skills training for pain
be customized to match the patient's needs
focuses on developing new coping skills to better
(Turk & Stacey, 1 9 9 7 ) . T h e objectives o f most
manage pain. Coping skills training
often
pain clinics include reduction o f pain, elimina-
encompasses many strategies for pain manage-
tion (or significant reduction in the use) o f opi-
ment rather than instruction in any one specific
oid medications, reduction in the use o f overall
skill. These coping strategies might include dis-
medical services, improvement in physical
traction techniques, reinterpretation of pain sen-
functioning, and improvement in social sup-
sations, problem
port
solving, positive
coping
self-statements, and goal setting. T h e goal of
(Follick, Ahern, Attanasio, &
Riley,
1 9 8 5 ) . T h e ultimate goal is not to eliminate
the
244
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS pain but rather to rehabilitate patients and improve their quality o f life. T o achieve these ends, patients might be exposed to physical exercise and physical therapy modalities. They are often educated about the nature of pain and its cognitive, affective, and social correlates. They might participate in individual and group psychotherapy where cognitive and behavioral interventions are used for pain management.
Family members are
often
included in the treatment process to provide support and to assist with maintenance o f treatment gains on program completion. Drug
the experience of pain. Because pain has sensory, affective, and evaluative aspects, it is best understood and treated from a biopsychosocial perspective with input from health psychologists. Health psychologists have the training and expertise necessary to positively affect patients with chronic pain, helping them t o manage pain more effectively and to improve their overall quality o f life. Moreover, health psychologists, by nature o f their broadbased training in psychosocial complications of many medical conditions, are in the unique position of being able to offer pain management services in a variety o f medical contexts.
detoxification is sometimes necessary to assist
Multidisciplinary pain clinics are an obvious
patients in reducing their reliance on pain med-
site for health psychologists to be involved in
ications. Finally, work-related issues are often
the treatment of pain. However, other tertiary
addressed,
including
increasing
patients'
care settings, such as neurology clinics and can-
physical capacity to return to w o r k and remov-
cer treatment centers, provide ample opportu-
ing barriers to obtain employment.
nities for health psychologists to play a role in
Meta-analyses have shown that chronic pain
pain management. Although primary care set-
patients w h o participate in multidisciplinary
tings are becoming more popular locations for
pain treatment experience greater pain relief
collaborative treatment relationships between
than do patients who receive either no treat-
psychologists and physicians, they are still a
ment or standard, single-discipline pain treat-
largely untapped source for providing pain
ment (Cutler etal., 1 9 9 4 ; Flor, Fydrich, &
interventions. Because many individuals with
Turk, 1 9 9 2 ) . Multidisciplinary treatment was
pain complaints initially visit primary care
also found to result in significant improvements
providers for treatment, health psychologists in
in mood, activity levels, medication use, pain
these settings would have the opportunity to see
behaviors, return to work, and overall health
patients earlier in the pain experience and could
care use, and treatment gains persisted over an
potentially make important contributions to
extended period o f time. Research has also
preventing chronic pain and disability from
shown that multidisciplinary pain clinic treat-
developing. V o n Korff
ment costs less than standard medical interven-
specific treatment approach for primary care
tions for pain, with an estimated annual savings
patients presenting with pain problems. This
( 1 9 9 9 ) proposed a
of close to $ 2 8 0 million in medical and surgical
approach addresses common challenges to the
costs alone (Okifuji etal., 1 9 9 9 ) . Savings also
treatment o f pain in a primary care setting and
occur when the number of individuals requiring
delineates several foci of treatment that resem-
long-term disability payments is substantially
ble many of the treatment components pre-
reduced.
sented in this chapter. Other psychologists have proposed methods for developing relationships with primary care providers (Bray & Rogers,
CONCLUSIONS
1 9 9 5 ; Holloway, 1 9 9 5 ) , although not specifically for the management o f pain.
Chronic pain clearly affects the totality o f an individual's life experience and often creates additional burdens and stressors that exacerbate
Although our understanding o f the pain experience has grown tremendously during recent decades, research on pain continues
Chronic
Pain
CASE S T U D Y Earlier in the chapter, a case conceptualization o f a hypothetical patient, " M a r y , " was provided. Over a period of several years, M a r y had been diagnosed with fibromyalgia, migraine headaches, and irritable bowel and bladder syndromes. This 42-year-old woman was in her second marriage and had two grown children living out o f the house. Although M a r y had been gainfully employed as a nurse for 1 0 years, she was no longer working and had recently begun receiving disability benefits. She no longer drove because she was concerned that her pain level would cause her to lose control of her car. Thus, she was dependent on her husband for her frequent visits to health care providers. Mary's friendships and other social contacts had been slowly eroding as the amount o f time she spent in bed or on the couch increased. M a r y complained o f an inability to concentrate that had reached the point where she could no longer get enjoyment from reading. Although M a r y complained that her husband was growing weary of her chronic pain and disability, the chapter authors' observation was that her husband was very solicitous and that part of the partners' marital identity was their conjoint attendance to Mary's needs and visits to health care professionals. Mary's medication regimen included more than 1 0 daily prescription drugs, with additional prescribed medications as needed for pain and over-the-counter analgesics as she judged to be necessary. Multiple physicians prescribed the medications, and it was unclear at the time of evaluation whether there was sufficient communication among these physicians. M a r y was admitted into the chapter authors' cognitive-behavioral chronic stress and pain management group described elsewhere ( T h o r n et al., 2 0 0 2 ) . This 10-week group presents pain as a stress-related illness, formats treatment as a stress management approach, and focuses o n both cognitive and behavioral aspects o f increasing coping and function despite the pain. T h e authors believe that group
treatment
offers the advantage o f patient interaction and an opportunity for the patients to learn from one another in addition to modeling appropriate emotional expression and assertive requests regarding pain problems. M a r y successfully completed the group and continues to pursue individual therapy regarding underlying social stressors, including an unfulfilling marriage, which she n o w sees as exacerbating her pain experience and disability. She continues to struggle with issues o f assertive expression o f her needs rather than the aggressive emotional displays that drive away family members. Although she is still in daily pain and continues t o receive disability payments, she is taking fewer medications and her social interactions have improved. T h e next goal for M a r y is t o reduce her fears and anxieties regarding physical activity and to motivate her to engage in a gentle, daily exercise regimen.
and will undoubtedly result in greater refine-
with
ment o f current pain models. T h e role o f
and analyses, those factors m o s t critical for
more
psychosocial processes in the pain experi-
treatment
ence demands continued investigation, and
apparent.
sophisticated
research
designs
success will also b e c o m e
more
245
246
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS
REFERENCES Affleck, G., Tennen, H., Urrows, S., & Higgins, P. (1992). Neuroticism and the pain mood relation in rheumatoid arthritis: Insights from a perspective daily study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 1 1 9 - 1 2 6 . Aghabeigi, B . , Feinmann, C , & Harris, M . (1992). Prevalence of post-traumatic stress disorder in patients with chronic idiopathic facial pain. British Journal of Oral and Maxillofacial Surgery, 30, 3 6 0 - 3 6 4 . American Psychiatric Association. (1968). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Amundson, G. M . (2002). Low back pain. [Online]. Retrieved on June 16, 2 0 0 2 , from www.spineuniverse.com/displayarticle.php/article216.html Anderson, J . S., Sc Ferrans, C. E. (1997). The quality of life of persons with chronic fatigue syndrome. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 185, 3 5 9 - 3 6 7 . Asmundson, G. J . G. (1996). Role of anxiety sensitivity in pain-related fear and avoidance. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 19, 5 7 7 - 5 8 6 . Banks, S. M., 8c Kerns, R. D. (1996). Explaining high rates of depression in chronic pain: A diathesis-stress framework. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 9 5 - 1 1 0 . Beck, A. T., 8c Emery, G. (1985). Anxiety disorders and phobias: A cognitive perspective. New York: Basic Books. Beck, J . S. (1995). Cognitive therapy: Basics and beyond. New York: Guilford. Beitman, B. D., Kushner, M. G., Basha, L, Lamberti, J., Mukerji, V., Sc Bartels, K. (1991). Follow-up status of patients with angiographically normal coronary arteries and panic disorder. Journal of the American Medical Association, 265, 1545-1549. Block, A. R. (1996). Presurgical psychological screening in chronic pain syndromes: A guide for the behavioral health practitioner. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Blyth, F. M., March, L. M., Brnabic, A. J . , Jorm, L. R., Williamson, M., 8c Cousins, M. J . (2001). Chronic pain in Australia: A prevalence study. Pain, 89, 1 2 7 - 1 3 4 . Boothby, J . L , Thorn, Β . E., Stroud, M. W., 8c Jensen, M. P. (1999). Coping with pain. In R. J . Gatchel & D. C. Turk (Eds.), Psychosocial factors in pain: Critical perspectives (pp. 3 4 3 - 3 5 9 ) . New York: Guilford. Bray, J . H., & Rogers, J . C. (1995). Linking psychologists and family physicians for collaborative practice. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 26, 132-138. Brennan, A. F., Barrett, C. L., 8c Garretson, H. D. (1986). The prediction of chronic pain outcome by psychological variables. International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 16, 3 7 3 - 3 8 7 . Breslau, N., Davis, G. C , & Andreski, P. (1991). Migraine, psychiatric disorders, and suicide attempts: An epidemiological study of young adults. Psychiatry Research, 37, 1 1 - 2 3 . Brown, G. K., 8c Nicassio, P. M. (1987). Development of a questionnaire for the assessment of active and passive coping strategies in chronic pain patients. Pain, 31, 53-63. Bruehl, S., Steger, H. G., & Harden, R. N. (2001). Complex regional pain syndrome. In D. C. Turk & R. Melzack (Eds.), Handbook of pain assessment (2nd ed., pp. 2 7 5 - 2 9 1 ) . New York: Guilford. Butler, R. W., Damarin, F. L., Beaulieu, C. L., Schwebel, A. I., 8c Thorn, Β. E. (1989). Assessing cognitive coping strategies for acute pain. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1, 4 1 - 4 5 .
Chronic Carney, R. M., Freedland, Κ. E., Ludbrook, P. Α., Saunders, R. D., & Jaffe, A. S. (1990). Major depression, panic disorder, and mitral valve prolapse in patients who complain of chest pain. American journal of Medicine, 89, 7 5 7 - 7 6 0 . Caudill, M. A. (2002). Managing pain before it manages you (rev. ed.). New York: Guilford. Chapman, C. R., & Turner, J . A. (1986). Psychological control of acute pain in medical settings. Journal of Fain and Symptom Management, 1, 9 - 2 0 . Chaves, J . E , & Brown, J . M . (1987). Spontaneous cognitive strategies for the control of clinical pain and stress. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 10, 2 6 3 - 2 7 6 . Clark, J . D. (2002). Chronic pain prevalence and analgesic prescribing in a general medical population. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management, 23, 1 3 1 - 1 3 7 . Crombez, G., Eccleston, C , Van den Broek, Α., Van Doudenhove, B., & Goubert, L. (2002). The effects of catastrophic thinking about pain on attentional interference by pain: No mediation of negative affectivity in healthy volunteers and in patients with low back pain. Pain Research and Management, 7, 31-33. Cutler, R. B . , Fishbain, D. Α., Rosomoff, H. L., Abdel-Moty, E., Khalil, T. M., & Rosomoff, R. S. (1994). Does nonsurgical pain center treatment of chronic pain return patients to work? A review and meta-analysis of the literature. Spine, 19, 643-652. DeGood, D. E., & Shutty, M . S. (1992). Assessment of pain beliefs, coping, and selfefficacy. In D. C. Turk & R. Melzack (Eds.), Handbook of pain assessment (pp. 2 1 4 - 2 3 4 ) . New York: Guilford. for screening CFS, FMS, and MCS in Donnay, A. (1998, April 2 9 ) . Questionnaire adults. Testimony presented to the U.S. Chronic Fatigue Syndrome Coordinating Committee, Washington, DC. Elliott, T. R., Jackson, W. T., Layfield, M., & Kendall, D. (1996). Personality disorders and response to outpatient treatment of chronic pain. Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings, 3, 2 1 9 - 2 3 4 . Etscheidt, Μ . Α., Steger, H. G., & Braverman, B. (1995). Multidimensional Pain Inventory profile classifications and psychopathology. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51, 2 9 - 3 6 . F. A. Davis. (1997). Taber's cyclopedic medical dictionary (18th ed.). Philadelphia: Author. Fernandez, E., & Turk, D. C. (1995). The scope and significance of anger in the experience of chronic pain. Pain, 61, 1 6 5 - 1 7 5 . Fishbain, D. Α., Goldberg, M., Meagher, B. R., Steele, R., & Rosomoff, H. (1986). Male and female chronic pain patients categorized by DSM-III psychiatric diagnostic criteria. Pain, 26, 1 8 1 - 1 8 7 . Fishbain, D. Α., Rosomoff, H. L., & Rosomoff, R. S. (1992). Drug abuse, dependence, and addiction in chronic pain patients. Clinical Journal of Pain, 8, 77-85. Flor, H., Fydrich, T., & Turk, D. C. (1992). Efficacy of multidisciplinary pain treatment centers: A meta-analytic review. Pain, 49, 2 2 1 - 2 3 0 . Follick, M. J . , Ahern, D. K., Attanasio, V., & Riley, J . F. (1985). Chronic pain programs: Current aims, strategies, and needs. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 7, 17-20. Fordyce, W. E. (1988). Pain and suffering: A reappraisal. American Psychologist, 43, 2 7 6 - 2 8 3 . Fukuda, K., Straus, S. E., Hickie, L, Sharpe, M . C , Dobbins, J . G., & Komaroff, A. (1994). The chronic fatigue syndrome: A comprehensive approach to its definition and study. Annals of Internal Medicine, 121, 9 5 3 - 9 5 9 . Gaskin, M . E., Greene, A. F., Robinson, M . E., & Geisser, M. E. (1992). Negative affect and the experience of chronic pain. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 36, 7 0 7 - 7 1 3 .
fain
247
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS Gatchel, R. J . , Polatin, P. B . , 8c Mayer, T. G. (1995). The dominant role of psychosocial risk factors in the development of chronic low back pain disability. Spine, 20, 2 7 0 2 - 2 7 0 9 . Gatchel, R. J . , Polatin, P. B . , Mayer, T. G., Sc Garcy, P. (1994). Psychopathology and the rehabilitation of patients with chronic low back pain disability. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 75, 6 6 6 - 6 7 0 . Geisser, M . E., Robinson, M . E., 8c Henson, C. D. (1994). The Coping Strategies Questionnaire and chronic pain adjustment: A conceptual and empirical reanalysis. Clinical Journal of Pain, 10, 9 8 - 1 0 6 . Geisser, M . E., Robinson, M . E., Sc Riley, J . L. (1999). Pain beliefs, coping, and adjustment to chronic pain. Pain Forum, 8, 1 6 1 - 1 6 8 . Gil, Κ. M . (1984). Coping with invasive medical procedures: A descriptive model. Clinical Psychology Review, 4, 3 3 9 - 3 6 2 . Gil, Κ. M., Williams, D. Α., Keefe, F. J . , Sc Beckham, J . C. (1990). The relationship of negative thoughts to pain and psychological distress. Behavior Therapy, 21, 349-362. Headache Classification Committee of the International Headache Society. (1988). Classification and diagnostic criteria for headache disorders, cranial neuralgias, and facial pain. Cephalalgia, 8, 9 - 9 2 . Holloway, R. L. (1995). Building a primary care discipline: Notes from a psychologist in family medicine. Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings, 2, 7-20. Holroyd, Κ. Α., 8c Penzien, D. B . (1985). Client variables and the behavioral treatment of recurrent tension headache: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 9, 5 1 5 - 5 3 6 . Jacobsen, P. B., Sc Butler, R. W. (1996). Relation of cognitive coping and catastrophizing to acute pain and analgesic use following breast cancer surgery. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 19, 1 7 - 2 9 . Jamison, R. N., Raymond, S. Α., Slawsby, Ε. Α., Nedeljkovic, S. S., Sc Katz, N. P. (1998). Opioid therapy for chronic noncancer back pain: A randomized prospective study. Spine, 23, 2 5 9 1 - 2 6 0 0 . Jason, L. Α., Richman, J . Α., Rademaker, A. W., Jordan, K. M., Plioplys, Α. V., Taylor, R. R., McCready, W., Huang, C. F., Sc Plioplys, S. (1999). A communitybased study of chronic fatigue syndrome. Archives of Internal Medicine, 159, 2129-2137. Jensen, M. P., Turner, J . Α., 8c Romano, J . M . (2001). Changes in beliefs, catastrophizing, and coping are associated with improvement in multidisciplinary pain treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69, 6 5 5 - 6 6 2 . Keefe, F. J . (1982). Behavioral assessment and treatment of chronic pain: Current status and future directions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 50, 896-911. Keefe, F. J . , Sc Hill, R. W. (1985). An objective approach to quantifying pain behavior and gait patterns in low back pain patients. Pain, 21, 1 5 3 - 1 6 1 . Keefe, F. J . , Lefebvre, J . C , 8c Smith, S. J . (1999). Catastrophizing research: Avoiding conceptual errors and maintaining a balanced perspective. Pain Forum, 8, 1 7 6 - 1 8 0 . Keogh, E., Ellery, D., Hunt, C , Sc Hannent, I. (2001). Selective attentional bias for pain-related stimuli amongst pain fearful individuals. Pain, 91, 9 1 - 1 0 0 . Kerns, R. D., Turk, D., 8c Rudy, T. E. ( 1 9 8 5 ) . The West Haven-Yale Multidimensional Pain Inventory (WHYMPI). Pain, 23, 3 4 5 - 3 5 6 . Kouyanou, K., Pither, C. E., Rabe-Hesketh, S., Se Wessely, S. (1998). A comparative study of iatrogenesis, medication abuse, and psychiatric morbidity in chronic pain patients with and without medically explained symptoms. Pain, 76, 4 1 7 - 4 2 6 .
Chronic Kulich, R. J . , Mencher, P., Bertrand, C , Sc Maciewicz, R. (2000). Comorbidity of post-traumatic stress disorder and pain: Implications for clinical and forensic assessment. Current Review of Pain, 4, 3 6 - 4 8 . Lahita, R. G. (2001). What is lupus? [Online]. Retrieved on July 9, 2 0 0 2 , from www.lupus.org Lazarus, R. S. (1993). Coping theory and research: Past, present, and future. Psychosomatic Medicine, 55, 2 3 4 - 2 4 7 . Lazarus, R. S., 8c Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer. Lefebvre, M . F. (1981). Cognitive distortion and cognitive errors in depressed psychiatric and low back pain patients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49, 5 1 7 - 5 2 5 . LeResche, L., 8c Von Korff, M . (1999). Epidemiology of chronic pain. In A. R. Block, E. F. Kremer, Sc E. Fernandez (Eds.), Handbook of pain syndromes: Biopsychosocial perspectives (pp. 3 - 2 2 ) . Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. McCracken, L. M., Zayfert, C , 8c Gross, R. T. (1992). The Pain Anxiety Scale: Development and validation of a scale to measure fear of pain. Pain, 50, 6 7 - 7 3 . Melzack, R. (1975). Prolonged relief of pain by brief, intense transcutaneous somatic stimulation. Pain, 1, 3 5 7 - 3 7 3 . Melzack, R. (1987). The short form McGill pain questionnaire. Pain, 30, 1 9 1 - 1 9 7 . Melzack, R., 8c Wall, P. D. (1965). Pain mechanisms: A new theory. Science, 50, 971-979. Merskey, H., 8c Bogdale, N. (Eds.). (1994). Classification of chronic pain: Descriptions of chronic pain syndromes and definitions of pain terms. Seattle, WA: IASP Press. Merskey, H., & Moulin, D. (1999). Pharmacological treatment in chronic pain. In A. R. Block, E. F. Kremer, 8c E. Fernandez (Eds.), Handbook of pain syndromes: Biopsychosocial perspectives (pp. 1 4 9 - 1 6 3 ) . Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Meyler, W. J . , de Jongste, M . J . L., 8c Rolf, C. A. M . (1994). Clinical evaluation of pain treatment with electrostimulation: A study of TENS in patients with different pain syndromes. Clinical Journal of Pain, 10, 22-27'. Moreno, R., Cunningham, A. C , Gatchel, R. J . , 8c Mayer, T. G. (1991). Functional restoration for chronic low back pain: Changes in depression, cognitive distortion, and disability. Journal of Occupational Rehabilitation, 1, 2 0 7 - 2 1 6 . Moulin, D. E., Iezzi, Α., Amireah, R., Sharpe, W. K. J . , Boyd, D., 8c Merskey, H. (1996). Randomized trial of oral morphine for chronic non-cancer pain. Lancet, 347, 1 4 3 - 1 4 7 . Newton, C. R., 8c Barbaree, Η. E. (1987). Cognitive changes accompanying headache treatment: The use of a thought-sampling procedure. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11, 6 3 5 - 6 5 1 . Okifuji, Α., Turk, D. C , 8c Kalauokalani, D. (1999). Clinical outcome and economic evaluation of multidisciplinary pain centers. In A. R. Block, E. F. Kremer, 8c E. Fernandez (Eds.), Handbook of pain syndromes: Biopsychosocial perspectives (pp. 7 7 - 9 7 ) . Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Portenoy, R. K., Sc Foley, Κ. M . (1986). Chronic use of opioid analgesics in nonmalignant pain: Report of 38 cases. Pain, 25, 1 7 1 - 1 8 6 . Rapoport, A. M., 8c Sheftell, F. D. (1996). Headache disorders: A management guide for practitioners. Philadelphia: W. B . Saunders. Riskind, J . H., Sc Williams, N. L. (1999). Specific cognitive content of anxiety and catastrophizing: Looming vulnerability and the looming maladaptive style. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 13, 4 1 - 5 4 . Robbins, R. Α., Moody, D. S., Hahn, M . B., 8c Weaver, M . A. (1996). Psychological testing variables as predictors of return to work by chronic pain patients. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 83, 1 3 1 7 - 1 3 1 8 .
Pain
249
250
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS Robinson, M. E., Riley, J . L., Myers, C. D., Sadler, I. J . , Kvaal, S. Α., Geisser, M. E., & Keefe, F. J . (1997). The Coping Strategies Questionnaire: A large sample, item level factor analysis. Clinical Journal of Fain, 13, 4 3 - 4 9 . Roland, M., &c Morris, R. (1983). A study of the natural history of back pain: I. Development of a reliable and sensitive measure of disability in low back pain. Spine, 8, 1 4 1 - 1 4 4 . Romano, J . M., & Turner, J . A. (1985). Chronic pain and depression: Does the evidence support a relationship? Psychological Bulletin, 97, 1 8 - 3 4 . Rosenstiel, A. K., & Keefe, F. J . (1983). The use of coping strategies in chronic low back pain: Relationship to patient characteristics and current adjustment. Pain, 17, 3 3 - 4 4 . Schultheis, K., Peterson, L., & Selby, V. (1987). Preparation for stressful medical procedures and person χ treatment interactions. Clinical Psychology Review, 7, 329-352. Smith, G. R. (1991). Somatization disorder in the medical setting. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Smith, T. W., Christensen, A. J . , Peck, J . R., & Ward, J . R. (1994). Cognitive distortion, helplessness, and depressed mood in rheumatoid arthritis: A 4-year longitudinal analysis. Health Psychology, 13, 2 1 3 - 2 1 7 . Spence, S. H., Sharpe, L., Newton-John, T., & Champion, D. (1995). Effect of E M G biofeedback compared to applied relaxation training with chronic, upper extremity cumulative trauma disorders. Pain, 63, 1 9 9 - 2 0 6 . Stewart, W., Breslau, N., & Keck, P. E., Jr. (1994). Comorbidity of migraine and panic disorder. Neurology, 4 4 ( 1 0 , Suppl. 7), S 2 3 - S 2 7 . Stroud, M. W., Thorn, Β. E., Jensen, M. P., & Boothby, J . L. (2000). The relation between pain beliefs, negative thoughts, and psychosocial functioning in chronic pain patients. Pain, 84, 3 4 7 - 3 5 2 . Sullivan, M . D. (2001). Assessment of psychiatric disorders. In D. C. Turk & R. Melzack (Eds.), Handbook of pain assessment (2nd ed., pp. 2 7 5 - 2 9 1 ) . New York: Guilford. Sullivan, M. J . L., Bishop, S. R., &£ Pivik, J . (1995). The Pain Catastrophizing Scale: Development and validation. Psychological Assessment, 7, 5 2 4 - 5 3 2 . Sullivan, M. J . L., Sc Neish, N. (1998). Catastrophizing, anxiety, and pain during dental hygiene treatment. Communicative Dental Oral Epidemiology, 37, 243-250. Sullivan, M . J . L., Rouse, D., Bishop, S., Sc Johnston, S. (1997). Thought suppression, catastrophizing, and pain. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 21, 5 5 5 - 5 6 8 . Sullivan, M . J . L., Stanish, W., Sullivan, M. E., Sc Tripp, D. (2002). Differential predictors of pain and disability in patients with whiplash injuries. Pain Research and Management, 7, 6 8 - 7 4 . Sullivan, M . J . L., Thorn, B. E., Haythornthwaite, J . , Keefe, F., Martin, M., Bradley, L., 8c Lefebvre, J . C. (2001). Theoretical perspectives on the relation between catastrophizing and pain. Clinical Journal of Pain, 17, 5 2 - 6 4 . Sullivan, M. J . L., Tripp, D. Α., 8c Santor, D. (2000). Gender differences in pain and pain behavior: The role of catastrophizing. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 24, 1 2 1 - 1 3 4 . Suis, J . , 8c Wan, C. K. (1989). Effects of sensory and procedure information on coping with stressful medical procedures and pain: A meta-analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 3 7 2 - 3 7 9 . Taylor, R. R., Friedberg, F., 8c Jason, L. A. (2001). A clinician's guide to controversial illnesses: Chronic fatigue syndrome, fibromyalgia, multiple chemical sensitivities. Sarasota, FL: Professional Resources Press. Thorn, Β. E., Boothby, J . L., 8c Sullivan, M . J . L. (2002). Targeted treatment of catastrophizing for the management of chronic pain. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 9, 1 2 7 - 1 3 8 .
Chronic Thorn, Β. E., Rich, Μ . Α., & Boothby, J . L. (1999). Pain beliefs and coping attempts: Conceptual model building, fain forum, 8, 1 6 9 - 1 7 1 . Turk, D. C. (1996). Biopsychosocial perspective on chronic pain. In R. J . Gatchel &c D. C. Turk (Eds.), Psychological approaches to pain management: A practitioner's handbook (pp. 3 - 3 2 ) . New York: Guilford. Turk, D. C , & Meichenbaum, D. (1994). A cognitive-behavioral approach to pain management. In P. D. Wall & R. Melzack (Eds.), Textbook of pain (pp. 7 8 7 - 7 9 4 ) . London: Churchill Livingstone. Turk, D. C , & Rudy, T. E. (1992). Cognitive factors and persistent pain: A glimpse into Pandora's box. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 16, 9 9 - 1 2 2 . Turk, D . C , & Stacey, B . R. (1997). Multidisciplinary pain centers in the treatment of chronic pain. In J . W. Frymoyer, T. B . Ducker, N. M . Hadler, J . P. Kostuik, J . N. Weinstein, Se T. S. Whitecloud, III (Eds.), The adult spine: Principles and practice (2nd ed., pp. 2 5 3 - 2 7 4 ) . New York: Raven. Vasey, M. W., Se Burkovec, T. D. (1992). A catastrophizing assessment of worrisome thoughts. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 16, 5 0 5 - 5 2 0 . Vlaeyen, J . W. S., Sc Linton, S. J . (2000). Fear-avoidance and its consequences in chronic musculoskeletal pain: A state of the art. Pain, 85, 3 1 7 - 3 3 2 . Von Korff, M . (1999). Pain management in primary care: An individualized stepped-care approach. In R. J . Gatchel Se D. C. Turk (Eds.), Psychosocial factors in pain: Critical perspectives (pp. 3 6 0 - 3 7 3 ) . New York: Guilford. Waddell, G., McCulloch, J . Α., Kummel, E., 8c Venner, R. M . (1980). Nonorganic physical signs in low-back pain. Spine, 5, 1 1 7 - 1 2 5 . Ward, L. C , Thorn, Β . E., & Clements, K. L. (2002). Factor structure of the Personal Attributes Questionnaire: Agency, communion, and emotional vulnerability. Unpublished manuscript, Department of Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Tuscaloosa, AL. Waylonis, G. W., 8c Perkins, R. H. (1994). Post-traumatic fibromyalgia: A longterm follow-up. American Journal of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 73, 403-412. Williams, D. A. (1996). Acute pain management. In R. J . Gatchel & D. C. Turk (Eds.), Psychological approaches to pain management: A practitioner's handbook (pp. 5 5 - 7 7 ) . New York: Guilford. Williams, D. A. (1999). Acute pain (with special emphasis on painful medical procedures). In R. J . Gatchel 8c D. C. Turk (Eds.), Psychosocial factors in pain: Critical perspectives (pp. 1 5 1 - 1 6 3 ) . New York: Guilford. Williams, D. Α., 8c Thorn, Β. E. (1989). An empirical assessment of pain beliefs. Pain, 36, 3 5 1 - 3 5 8 . Wolfe, F., Smythe, Η. Α., Yunus, M . B . , Bennett, R. M., Bombardier, C , Goldenberg, D. L., Tugwell, P., Campbell, S. M., Abeles, M., Clark, P., Fam, A. G., Farber, S. J . , Fiechtner, J . J . , Franklin, C. M., Gatter, R. Α., Hamaty, D., Lessard, J . , Lichtbroun, A. S., Masi, A. T., McCain, G. Α., Reynolds, W. J . , Romano, T . J . , Russell, I. J . , 8c Sheon, R. P. (1990). The American College of Rheumatology 1 9 9 0 criteria for the classification of fibromyalgia. Arthritis and Rheumatology, 33, 1 6 0 - 1 7 2 . Zenz, M., Strumpf, M., 8c Tryba, M. (1992). Long-term oral opioid therapy in patients with chronic non-malignant pain. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management, 7, 6 9 - 7 7 . Ziegler, F. J . , Imboden, J . B . , & Myer, E. (1960). Contemporary conversion reaction: A clinical study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 116, 9 0 1 - 9 0 3 .
fain
CHAPTER
14
Hypertension MUSTAFA AL'ABSI AND RICHARD G . HOFFMAN
H
ypertension is a major risk factor o f
Cultural and psychosocial factors, as well as
heart disease, the leading cause o f
responsiveness to interventions, may also con-
death in the United States. It con-
tribute to differences in hypertension preva-
tributes to stroke, the third leading cause of
lence (Horan & Mockrin, 1 9 9 2 ) .
death, and it also contributes to approximately
Increased risk for hypertension is directly
one fourth o f kidney failures. These devastating
associated with increased risk for premature
diseases exact a high toll in human suffering,
death due to cardiovascular diseases (Lie,
deteriorating quality o f life, and financial cost,
Mundal,
making a strong case for continuous effort to
Wentworth,
&
Erikssen, 1 9 8 5 ; N e a t o n
identify causes and develop means to control
Hypertension increases workload on the heart
hypertension. As is described in more detail
and contributes to myocardial cell enlargement
1 9 9 2 ; Stamler et al.,
left-ventricular
hypertrophy.
oc
1989).
later in this chapter, hypertension is one of the
and
risk factors that can be controlled by available
increases, the pumping action o f the heart
As BP
behavioral and pharmacological interventions.
requires more effort and energy. Under the
However, many challenges face clinicians in
condition of high BP, the arteries carry blood
their efforts to implement and ensure compli-
that
ance with regimens for this disorder.
is
moving
under
greater
pressure.
Chronically, this state eventually leads to vari-
is used to indicate
ous structural changes in the heart and blood
high blood pressure (BP). Although hyperten-
vessels, leading to the hardening o f arteries,
sion can occur at any age, it is more prevalent
and other organs may also get directly affected,
in adults over age 3 5 years. It is particularly
leading to the sequelae of hypertension, includ-
T h e term hypertension
prevalent among African Americans, middle-
ing stroke, congestive heart failure, kidney
aged and elderly people, obese individuals, and
failure, and heart attack. Another important
heavy drinkers ( M a c M a h o n , Cutler, Brittain,
risk for hypertension is that when it is present
Wentworth,
with other risk factors (e.g., high blood choles-
1 9 9 2 ; Stamler et al., 1 9 8 9 ; Whelton, 1 9 8 5 ) .
terol, smoking, diabetes, obesity), the danger
However, there is a high heterogeneity in
of major heart problems or a stroke increases
hypertension prevalence related to heterogene-
in a manner that exceeds the simple addition o f
ity in underlying pathophysiological processes.
the risk weight o f these risk factors.
&
Higgins, 1 9 8 7 ; Neaton &
Hypertension
j
O n e in four adults has high BP, and
systolic 1 4 0 to 1 5 9 m m Hg or diastolic 9 0 to
approximately 3 2 % of those with hypertension
9 9 m m Hg. Fifth, Stage 2 (moderate) hyperten-
are not aware that they have it. Approximately
sion is systolic 1 6 0 to 1 7 9 m m H g or diastolic
1 5 % of those with hypertension are not on any
1 0 0 to 1 0 9 m m Hg. Finally, Stage 3 (higher)
therapeutic regimen, and about 2 6 % are inade-
hypertension is systolic 1 8 0 or higher m m H g
quately treated. Furthermore,
or diastolic 1 1 0 m m H g or higher.
hypertension
affects about one in three African Americans (Burt et al., 1 9 9 5 ) . Hypertension develops earlier in life, and is usually more severe, in blacks
Etiological Issues
than in whites. T h e longer hypertension is left
Hypertension is a highly heterogeneous dis-
untreated, the more serious its complications
order with multiple pathogenic mechanisms.
can become, and this possibly contributes to
T h e causes for 9 0 % to 9 5 % o f hypertension
the prevalence o f the different levels of cardiac
cases (called "essential hypertension") are not
adaptation processes and the greater sequelae
known. T h e remaining 5 % to 1 0 % o f hyper-
seen in African Americans with high BP (Burt
tension (called "secondary hypertension") may
et al., 1 9 9 5 ; Koren, Mensah, Blake, Laragh, &
be caused by other diseases such as kidney
Devereux, 1 9 9 3 ) .
abnormalities, congenital abnormalities in major blood vessels in the body, and abnormalities associated with vasoconstriction of
BACKGROUND AND ETIOLOGY
arteries. Despite no clear identification o f the pathophysiology o f essential hypertension, the role
Diagnosis
of psychological variables in hypertension has
When BP is measured, it is defined as two
occupied a prominent position within the field
numbers: systolic and diastolic. Systolic BP rep-
of behavioral medicine (Alexander, 1 9 3 9 ) . In
resents the force at which blood flows when the
particular, recent evidence suggests that stress
heart beats. Diastolic BP, on the other hand, is
may contribute to the pathophysiology o f
an estimate o f the force o f blood flow when the
hypertension (Henry et al., 1 9 9 3 ) . Researchers
heart relaxes (in between heartbeats). Together,
propose
these numbers (written as the value o f systolic
responses to frequent
that
exaggerated
cardiovascular
and persistent stress
BP divided by the value of diastolic BP, recor-
episodes may be a risk factor and/or a marker
ded in millimeters o f mercury
[mm Hg])
for cardiovascular disease (Everson et al.,
compose a person's BP and are used to deter-
1 9 9 7 ; Light et al., 1 9 9 9 ) . For example, expo-
mine whether or not the pressures are in a
sure to stress has been found to accelerate
healthy range. T h e Joint National Committee
hypertension development in spontaneously
on Detection, Evaluation, and
Treatment
hypertensive rats, whereas unstressed rats had
of High Blood Pressure ( J N C , 1 9 9 7 ) and the
a delayed development and milder hyperten-
American Heart Association have put forth rec-
sion (Henry et al., 1 9 9 3 ; Y a m o r i , Matsumoto,
ommendations on the classification o f BP levels.
Y a m a b e , &c O k a m o t o , 1 9 6 9 ) . Research has
T h e recommendations are as follows. First,
also shown that responses to laboratory stress-
optimal BP is systolic less than 1 2 0 m m H g and
ors predict future BP elevations (Matthews,
diastolic less than 8 0 m m Hg. Second, normal
Woodall, & Allen, 1 9 9 3 ; Menkes et al., 1 9 8 9 ;
BP is systolic less than 1 3 0 m m H g and diastolic
Treiber et al., 1 9 9 6 ) . Individuals who are at
less than 8 5 m m Hg. Third, high normal BP is
high risk for hypertension or who have chron-
systolic 1 3 0 to 1 3 9 m m Hg or diastolic 8 5 to 8 9
ically elevated BP tend to show exaggerated BP
mm Hg. Fourth, Stage 1 (mild) hypertension is
responses to behavioral stressors (al'Absi,
253
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS
254
Buchanan, 8c Lovallo, 1996; al'Absi, Lovallo, M c K e y , & Pincomb, 1994; Everson, Kaplan,
1996). These elevated
Goldberg, & Salonen,
responses, combined with the high baseline BP,
cardiovascular activation on the heart and blood vessels. Thus, they may play a role in the development of hypertension. The relationship among stress, Cortisol, and hypertension development bears some parallel
contribute to a high workload on the heart. in
in humans. Patients with high levels o f Cortisol
Chapter 10, the primary peripheral hormone
due to Cushing's syndrome show about 80%
As
was
discussed
more thoroughly
1990).
produced by the adrenal cortex, Cortisol, is
prevalence o f hypertension (Kaplan,
considered to be the central component o f the
This is often corrected using glucocorticoid
stress response (Kaplan, that
1998). It is proposed
antagonists, which
also reduce BP (Fallo,
stressful
events
Paoletta, T o n a , Boscaro, & Sonino,
one mechanism through
Cortisol response
to
1993).
which
Furthermore, normotensives w h o are at high
hypothesized
risk for hypertension and borderline hyperten-
pathogenic effects on hypertension (Litchfield
sives show enhanced adrenocortical activity
represents prolonged
stress
exerts
its
1998; W a t t et al., 1992). In short, Corti-
(e.g., elevated Cortisol) in response to a variety
sol is essential to the maintenance o f normal
of psychological stressors (al'Absi 8c Arnett,
et a l ,
vascular tone. It has effects on responses to
2000). For example, prior research has evalu-
catecholamines and other vasoactive agents
ated
such as angiotensin II and vasopressin (Vander,
high risk for hypertension during rest and in
8c Luciano, 1994). T h e physical
response to acute stressors. W h e n at rest in a
Sherman,
adrenocortical activity in persons
at
nature o f cardiac output and the resistance o f
novel experimental environment,
the blood vessels to this blood flow determine
hypertensives showed enhanced adrenocorti-
BP. T h e control o f these activities is mediated
cal activation relative to low-risk controls
by
(al'Absi 8c Lovallo, 1993)
neurohumoral
activity, including
nor-
epinephrine, epinephrine, acetylcholine, and
borderline
and had larger
responses during work on mental arithmetic
1994).
their receptors, and these agents are modu-
and psychomotor stress (al'Absi et al.,
lated by Cortisol. Cortisol has ready access to
These tendencies are exaggerated in the pres-
the central nervous system, affecting areas o f
ence o f psychostimulants such as caffeine.
the brain that are involved in the control o f BP
Relative to low-risk controls, caffeine can
(e.g., hypothalamus, limbic system) (Wilson
differentially
8c Foster, 1992). In addition,
unmediated, mildly hypertensive men (al'Absi,
Cortisol
increase Cortisol secretion in
8c Wilson, 1995). This suggests
enhances sympathetic nervous system activity
Lovallo, Sung,
by increasing adrenergic receptor sensitivity to
that early stages o f hypertension
neurotransmitter
especially sensitive to caffeine's
Lefkowitz,
activation
(Davies
8c
1984). T h e bolstering of sympa-
thetic nervous system activity is believed to
adrenocortical effects when under
may
be
pituitarystressful
conditions.
enhance circulating fluid volume by causing
Prior research has also obtained similar
fluid to shift from intracellular to extracellular
results in normotensive persons at high risk
compartments in the kidney (Kaplan,
1990).
for hypertension, defined as having a positive
This results in improved venous return to the
parental history or having mildly elevated B P
heart and increased cardiac output. Cortisol
but not yet medicated (al'Absi, Everson, 8c
also inhibits the production o f prostaglandin
Lovallo,
and arachidonic acid, bradykinin, serotonin,
vated adrenocorticotropin and Cortisol con-
1995). These individuals showed ele-
8c Foster, 1992), lead-
centrations after caffeine ingestion relative to
ing to vasoconstrictive effects. These proper-
placebo. They also showed an additive effect
ties
on adrenocorticotropin and Cortisol increases
and histamine (Wilson of
Cortisol
increase
the
effects
of
Hypertension to the acute stress and caffeine. T h e high-risk
\
ASSESSMENT AND T R E A T M E N T
group showed earlier and more persistent rises throughout the tasks than did the low-risk group (al'Absi et al., 1 9 9 8 ) . Neither
behavioral stress nor caffeine alone produced Cortisol responses
in normotensive
Measurement
the Hypertension is usually called the silent
men,
killer because it has no specific symptoms.
although significant elevations occurred in
Patients may have hypertension for many years
the low-risk men after the tasks in the presence
without knowing it because there is no specific
of caffeine. This line o f work suggests that
perceived sensory information associated with
the previously observed increased activation
high BP. T h e only way in which to determine
of the autonomic nervous system (Julius &c
whether a person has hypertension is by mea-
Nesbitt, 1 9 9 6 ) and the cardiovascular control
suring BP. Measurement o f BP is a quick and
centers o f the hypothalamus and medulla may
a reliable way in which to determine levels
be paralleled by enhanced responses o f the
of risk for hypertension. A screening measure-
adrenocortical system to behavioral stress and
ment can be conducted in a hospital clinic,
to stimulant agents such as caffeine.
doctor's office, nurse's office, company clinic,
T h e increased Cortisol levels and responses
or school or at a health fair. If screening sug-
in hypertension-prone persons may represent
gests high BP, a physician may ask for more
an altered stress response, characterized by
detailed BP measurement, including ambula-
heightened hypothalamic-adrenocortical acti-
tory monitoring over a 24-hour period. This
vation. T h e development o f hypertension is
more detailed assessment is important to have
accompanied by enhanced activation o f the
a reliable conclusion o f BP levels.
cardiovascular control centers o f the hypothalamus and medulla (Bunag &
Takeda,
In a clinical setting, BP is measured using an instrument called a sphygmomanometer.
1 9 7 9 ; Jin & Rockhold, 1 9 9 1 ) as well as by
During this measurement, a rubber cuff is
increased sympathetic nervous system function
wrapped around the patient's upper arm. T h e
and adrenergic activity (Julius &
Nesbitt,
cuff is then inflated, causing the cuff to com-
1 9 9 6 ) . Such activation would predict greater
press a large artery in the arm, thereby stopping
levels of adrenocortical activity, possibly initi-
blood flow in the arm. T h e pressure is then
ated at the hypothalamic level above in the
reduced by releasing the air from the cuff. With
central nervous system. As illustrated in Figure
the reduced cuff pressure, blood starts to pulse
1 4 . 1 , adrenocortical activation may contribute
through the artery, making a sound, while the
independently and in combination with other
clinician listens with a stethoscope. This pulsing
risk factors to increase BP. Genetic vulnerabil-
sound continues until the pressure in the artery
ity may enhance adrenocortical activation both
is higher than the pressure in the cuff. T h e clin-
centrally and peripherally. This adrenocortical
ician records BP using a gauge connected to the
activation may also be caused by other risk
cuff. T w o BPs are recorded. T h e first is when
factors known to increase Cortisol such as
the first sound is heard. This reflects the systolic
obesity
alcohol intake (Keltikangas-
BP and indicates pressure related to the blood
Jarvinen, Raikkonen, Hautanen, & Adlercreutz,
flow when the heart beats. T h e second is when
1 9 9 6 ; Raikkonen, Hautanen, & Keltikangas-
the final sound is heard. This reflects the dia-
Jarvinen, 1 9 9 6 ) . Therefore, it is appealing to
stolic pressure
propose that adrenocortical dysregulation, in
between heartbeats. T h e unit for the assessment
combination with environmental factors such
of BP is millimeters of mercury.
and
as high salt intake and smoking, may represent enhanced risk.
and indicates the
pressure
There are several factors that might introduce error variance into measurement of BP,
255
256
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS
Stress
CRH τ
(")
ACTH&
β-endorphin
ÎCortisol
Τ Vascular resistance
Î Cardiac output
Τ Blood Pressure Figure 14.1
Potential Pathways Among Stress, Cortisol, and Hypertension Risk
N O T E : C R H = corticotropin-releasing hormone; A C T H = adrenocorticotropic hormone.
including behavioral and environmental factors.
hypertension." This occurs when BP mea-
health
sured in the clinic or in the physician's office is
status, prior smoking, consumption o f caffeine-
high enough to warrant diagnosis for hyper-
containing beverages, and time since last meal
tension, but measurement in the field outside
all are factors that could artificially affect accu-
the clinic (e.g., at home) shows a normoten-
For example, prevailing temperature,
racy of BP measurement. Prior to measurement,
sive range of BP (Pickering, Coats, Mallion,
the patient should sit in a comfortable chair with
M a n c i a , & Verdecchia, 1 9 9 9 ; Pickering et al.,
his or her feet on the floor for approximately 5
1 9 8 8 ) . It is estimated that 2 0 % o f hyperten-
minutes. The patient should have emptied his or
sives have white coat hypertension (Pickering
her bladder and should not be engaged in con-
et al., 1 9 9 9 ) . It is still not clear what the clini-
versation during the assessment. Measurement
cal significance o f this discrepancy is or how
should be conducted multiple times to obtain an
to address this phenomenon in treatment.
average, and intervals between measurement
Nevertheless, it should be noted that about
should be more than 1 minute.
one in four of those w h o are considered for antihypertensive
Assessment in Clinical Settings: The "White Coat" Effect
medication may
have white c o a t hypertension
actually
(Myers
Reeves, 1 9 9 1 ; Selenta, Hogan, &
&
Linden,
2 0 0 0 ) . Persons with white coat hypertension
important issue to consider during
differ from true normotensives on several
diagnosis is a phenomenon called "white coat
demographic and lifestyle variables. They tend
One
Hypertension
\
to be male, past smokers, and older and also
strategies that have been used in the control o f
tend to consume more alcohol.
hypertension are described and evaluated.
Some investigators argue that the white c o a t hypertension
is a benign
(Gosse, P r o m a x , Durandet,
&
condition Clementy,
1 9 9 3 ; Julius et al., 1 9 9 0 ; Verdecchia, Schillaci,
Weight Loss,
Control, and Blood
Weight Pressure
Borgioni, Ciucci, & Porcellati, 1 9 9 7 ) . Others argue that it has clinical significance, including increased cardiovascular risk
(Kuwajima,
Suzuki, Fujisawa, & Kuramoto, 1 9 9 3 ; W e b e r , Neutel, Smith, & Graettinger, 1 9 9 4 ) . Because this condition is not associated with sustained BP elevation, it is possible that the harmful effect o f this condition is due to high BP variability across settings (Lantelme, M i l o n , Gharib, Gayet, & Fortrat, 1 9 9 8 ) . An exaggerated BP lability may contribute to greater organ
damage
in
hypertensives
(Parati,
The Role of Weight Control in Primary Prevention. A clear direct relationship exists between body weight and B P . Overweight individuals (body mass index greater than 2 7 . 8 for men and greater than 2 7 . 3 for women) have an increased incidence o f hypertension (Oparil, 2 0 0 0 ) , and this relationship between obesity and hypertension appears to be strongest for younger adults (Stone & Kushner, 2 0 0 0 ) . T h e pathogenesis o f obesity-induced hypertension is not well characterized but is likely
Pomidossi, Albini, Malaspina, &c M a n c i a ,
related to hemodynamic
1 9 8 7 ) and in individuals with white coat hyper-
dysfunction, and an increase in sympathetic
tension (Cavallini et al., 1 9 9 5 ) . Individuals
nervous system activity that may be related to
with white coat hypertension may represent a
insulin resistance (Hall, 1 9 9 7 ) . T h e hemody-
subgroup o f hypertensives w h o require a tar-
namic changes are likely due to increases in
alterations, renal
geted intervention to manage the situational
adipose tissue and expansion o f extracellular
reactivity they exhibit. This is significant in
water and total blood volume, resulting in
light o f the observed negative cardiac abnor-
an augmented stroke volume and increased
malities that have been observed in those with
cardiac output proportional
white coat hypertension (Cardillo, D e Felice,
Abnormal renal sodium and water handling
to body size.
Campia, & Folli, 1 9 9 3 ) . Finally, it has been
(pressure natriuresis) causes the initiation o f
shown that the white coat effect may have a
hypertension
negative prognostic
Ultimately, eccentric left ventricular hyper-
impact, even in
the
(Stone &c Kushner,
2000).
absence o f sustained hypertension (Kuwajima
trophy may develop as an adaptation
et al., 1 9 9 3 ; W e b e r et al., 1 9 9 4 ) .
increased vessel wall stress that results from expanded
intravascular
volumes,
to
venous
return, and left ventricular preload increase
Intervention Strategies to Control Hypertension
(Messerli, 1 9 8 2 ) . Echocardiography studies have demonstrated that a positive correlation
The treatment o f hypertension is a long-term
exists between percentage overweight
and
effort that puts a burden on patients and their
both left ventricular wall thickness and inter-
health care providers. M a n y patients
with
nal dimension, even in the absence o f hyper-
behavioral
tension (Stone & Kushner, 2 0 0 0 ) . Among the
reducing
severely obese, duration o f obesity is associ-
weight, eating properly, and getting the right
ated with higher left ventricular mass, poorer
amount o f exercise. Pharmacotherapy is also
left ventricular systolic function, and greater
hypertension may benefit from modification techniques such as
widely used for treatment o f hypertension. In
impairment o f left ventricular diastolic filling,
the following subsections, specific intervention
and this ultimately may lead to concentric
257
258
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS left
ventricular
hypertrophy
(Alpert
Sc
a direct relationship between weight loss and BP reduction in normotensive individuals, sug-
Hashimi, 1 9 9 3 ) . multiple
gesting that clinically significant long-term
echocardiographic studies suggests that weight
reductions in BP and reduced risk for hyper-
reduction reduces left ventricular mass in both
tension can be achieved with even modest
obese hypertensive and normotensive patients,
weight loss.
The
available evidence from
resulting in a direct hemodynamic effect as a result o f the reductions in blood volume, sys-
The Effect
of Weight
Control
and
Dieting
temic BP, venous return, cardiac output,
on Blood
stroke volume, and
T h e sixth report of the J N C ( 1 9 9 7 ) recom-
oxygen
consumption
Pressure
in Hypertensive
Patients.
(Himeno, Nishino, Nakashima, Kuroiwa, Sc
mended
Ikeda,
hypertension who are above their ideal weight
1 9 9 6 ; Stone Sc Kushner,
2000).
the following: "All patients
with
Because o f findings such as these, prevention
should be prescribed an individualized, moni-
efforts have focused on weight loss. Data from
tored weight reduction program
such efforts demonstrate that weight loss
caloric restriction and increased physical activ-
reduces BP in people at risk for developing
ity" (p. 2 4 2 2 ) . Available guidelines suggest
involving
(Appel et al., 1 9 9 7 ; Stamler
that a target weight loss goal of 1 0 % o f body
et al., 1 9 8 9 ; Stevens et al., 1 9 9 3 , 2 0 0 1 ) . For
weight over 6 months o f therapy is appropri-
example, individuals who were placed on a
ate, with combinatory treatment o f dietary
low-saturated fat diet rich in fruits, vegetables,
therapy, physical activity, and behavior ther-
and low-fat dairy foods showed substantial
apy. Following N H L B I guidelines, treatment
reductions in BP o f 5.5 m m H g relative to
should be initiated with 3 0 to 4 5 minutes o f
control participants (Appel et al., 1 9 9 7 ) .
moderate physical activity 3 to 5 days per
hypertension
As delineated by Stevens and colleagues
week in addition to a low-calorie diet that pro-
( 1 9 9 3 , 2 0 0 1 ) , prevention efforts include group
duces a 5 0 0 - to 1,000-calorie-per-day deficit
and individual counseling focused on dietary
(Stone 8c Kushner, 2 0 0 0 ) . A review o f the
change, physical activity, and social support.
voluminous literature in the behavioral treat-
Dietary interventions target fat, sugar, and alco-
ment o f obesity is beyond the scope of this
hol consumption. Graded increases in physical
chapter, but obvious strategies for weight loss
activity are also planned
monitored.
with this population would include self-moni-
Eventually, physical activity should reflect brisk
toring strategies, weight loss problem solving,
walking at the level of 4 0 % to 5 5 % of heart
urge control, stimulus control interventions,
rate reserve for 3 5 to 4 5 minutes per day, 4
and stress management approaches related to
and
to 5 days per week. Interventions also include
dieting (Bonato Sc Boland, 1 9 8 6 ; Simkin-
weekly group meetings used to facilitate social
Silverman, Wing, Boraz, Meilan, Sc Kuller,
support and enhance nutritional education as
1998;
well as to motivate behavior change. Specific
2 0 0 0 ; Wadden,
behavior change techniques include setting
Wisniewski, Sc Steinberg, 2 0 0 1 ) .
explicit short-term goals and developing spe-
The
Rapoport,
available
Clark,
Sc
Wardle,
Berkowitz, Sarwer, Prusliterature
from
several
cific action plans to achieve goals, developing
weight loss studies that have enrolled hyper-
alternative strategies for situations that trigger
tensive patients suggests that weight reduc-
problem eating, and engaging in self-monitor-
tions o f 3 % to 9 % o f body weight, with
ing via food diaries and graphing of daily
antihypertensive drug regimens held constant,
physical exercise.
are likely to yield reductions in systolic BP in
Evidence from three large-scale clinical
the range o f 3.0 to 6.8 m m H g and reductions
weight loss and dieting trials has demonstrated
in diastolic BP in the range o f 2 . 9 to 5.7 mm Hg
Hypertension
\
(Brand et al., 1 9 9 8 ; Corrigan, Raczynski,
W h i t m o r e , Leenen, &c Larochelle, 1 9 9 9 ) .
Swencionis, &
Jennings, 1 9 9 1 ; Langford
Instead, weight reduction is viewed as a more
et al., 1 9 8 5 ; Reisin et al., 1 9 8 3 ; Stamler et al.,
desirable target for primary prevention efforts.
1 9 8 9 ) . Trials that allowed adjustment o f antihypertensive drug regimens generally found
The
Effect
of Salt
Restriction
on
Blood
that lower doses and/or a fewer number o f
Pressure
antihypertensive drugs were required by suc-
guidelines recommend restrictions o f dietary
in Hypertensive
Current
Patients.
cessful participants in weight reduction pro-
sodium to a range o f 9 0 to 1 3 0 millimoles per
grams versus controls, suggesting that weight
day in hypertensive patients over the age of
loss may enhance the efficacy o f antihyper-
4 4 years (Fodor et al., 1 9 9 9 ) . This corresponds
tensive drugs (Oparil, 2 0 0 0 ) .
to about 3 to 7 grams o f salt per day. Patients should be counseled to avoid foods high in
Sodium
Intake
and Blood
salt (e.g., prepared foods) and to choose foods
Fressure
low in salt (e.g., fresh fruits and vegetables).
Sensitivity.
Combined salt restriction and diet modifica-
Epidemiological literature suggests a direct
tion trials have demonstrated a great deal o f
Primary
Prevention
and Salt
BPs and
promise (Sacks et al., 2 0 0 1 ; Vollmer et al.,
sodium intake, typically expressed as the
2 0 0 1 ) . Such trials report decreases in BP o f
relationship between population
effect o f dietary salt intake (Elliott, 1 9 9 1 ;
between 7 and 1 2 m m Hg in systolic BP and at
Stamler, 1 9 9 7 ) . There is also some support in
least 5 m m H g in diastolic pressure for hyper-
the literature to suggest that the effect o f
tensive patients who were on both a salt
dietary salt intake may vary a m o n g subpop-
restricted diet and the Dietary Approaches to
ulations and that the relationship between
Stop Hypertension
dietary sodium and BP may be pronounced
etal., 2001).
in these subgroups. There have been several studies that suggest that older adults
(DASH) diet (Vollmer
Although there is evidence that salt restric-
and
tion, either alone or in combination with
African Americans may be "salt sensitive"
dietary intervention, is effective in lowering BP
and have a greater BP response to dietary
in short-term clinical trials, there is a clear
sodium intake ( e.g., Ishibashi et al., 1 9 9 4 ;
need for an increased role o f behavioral inter-
Weinberger, 1 9 9 6 ) , and there is at least one
vention in the long-term maintenance o f this
report that suggests an increased salt sensi-
benefit.
tivity in w o m e n (Kojima et al., 1 9 9 2 ) .
reported very little change in BPs at 9 - to 1 8 -
Brunner
and
colleagues
(1997)
a
month follow-up o f hypertensive patients after
restriction in salt intake would have an effect
dietary advice to reduce salt intake was given
on the incidence o f hypertension, even within
by nutritionists or specially trained counselors,
salt-sensitive subpopulations.
and a similar finding was reported by Ebrahim
To
date, it remains unclear whether
Because evi-
dence suggests that the association o f sodium
and Davey ( 1 9 9 8 ) at 6-month
intake with cardiovascular mortality and mor-
although a 2 . 1 - m m H g decrease in diastolic
follow-up,
bidity varies by overweight status (He e t a l . ,
pressure was maintained.
1 9 9 9 ) and that advice to restrict salt intake is less effective in preventing hypertension in overweight people than is advice on weight reduction (He, Whelton, Appel, Charleston,
Alcohol Primary
Intake
and Blood
Prevention
Pressure and
Alcohol.
&C Klag, 2 0 0 0 ) , most current health risk guide-
Epidemiological data indicate a direct rela-
lines do not support a restriction o f salt intake
tionship between excess alcohol consumption
in the
and risk o f hypertension, with several studies
normotensive
population
(Fodor,
259
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS
260
suggesting a linear dose-response relationship
suggest a relatively modest effect on BP (Xin
starting with an alcohol consumption thresh-
et al., 2 0 0 1 ) . For example, high normal to
old o f three drinks per day (roughly 3 0 grams
slightly hypertensive heavy drinkers
of ethanol); more than 2 ounces o f alcohol per
than 2 1 drinks per week), who showed an
day significantly increases the risk o f becom-
average decrease in alcohol consumption o f
(e.g., Klatsky, Friedman,
1.3 drinks per day, reduced their BP by only
ing hypertensive
(more
Siegelaub, & Gerard, 1 9 7 7 ; Treadway, 2 0 0 0 ) ,
1.2/0.7 m m H g following a cognitive-behav-
whereas a daily alcohol intake o f less than 1
ioral outpatient alcohol reduction program
in BP
(Cushman et al., 1 9 9 8 ) . Moreover, meta-
(Gilman, C o o k , Evans,
analytic data from 1 5 alcohol reduction trials
ounce may result in a modest decrease for
m o s t people
Rosner,
&
Hennekens,
1995)
except
involving 2 , 2 3 4 total patients demonstrated
African American males (Fuchs, Chambless,
an overall reduction in BP attributable to alco-
Whelton, Nieto, & Heiss, 2 0 0 1 ) . Recent data
hol reduction of only 3 . 3 1 m m H g systolic
(Thadhani et al., 2 0 0 2 ; Witteman et al., 1 9 9 0 )
and 2 . 0 4 m m H g diastolic ( X i n et al., 2 0 0 1 ) .
also suggests that regular consumption o f
Although these effects are modest, a dose-
more than one and a half drinks per day,
response relationship was observed between
regardless o f the type o f alcoholic beverage
mean percentage o f alcohol reduction and
consumed, substantially increases the risk o f
mean BP reduction. Regardless o f the modest
hypertension for women.
reductions in BP with concomitant alcohol
The mechanism o f alcohol-induced hyper-
reduction, available guidelines suggest that
tension is not clearly delineated. Alcohol is
hypertensive nondrinkers should continue to
hypothesized to affect the renin-angiotensin-
abstain from consuming alcohol. For hyper-
aldosterone axis and Cortisol secretion in
tensive alcohol drinkers, alcohol intake should
addition to contributing to heart rate variabil-
be limited to no more than 1 ounce o f alcohol
system
(2 ounces o f 1 0 0 - p r o o f whiskey, 8 ounces o f
discharge, and affecting insulin sensitivity
wine, or 2 4 ounces o f beer) per day in most
(Oparil, 2 0 0 0 ; Potter & Beevers, 1 9 8 4 ) . As
men and half that amount in women and
such, available guidelines suggest that
smaller men (Oparil, 2 0 0 0 ) .
ity, disrupting
adrenergic nervous
to
reduce the relative risk o f developing hypertension, those healthy adults who choose to drink should limit alcohol consumption to no more than 2 standard drinks per day, with consumption
not
exceeding 1 4
standard
drinks per week for men and 9
Exercise Primary
and Blood Prevention
Pressure and Exercise.
In 1 9 9 5 ,
the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention
standard
and the American College o f Sports Medicine
(Campbell,
jointly issued a recommendation regarding the
Ashley, Carruthers, Lacourciere, & M c K a y ,
primary prevention o f cardiovascular disease,
drinks
per week for w o m e n
1 9 9 9 ) . A standard drink is defined as approx-
stating that "every U.S. adult should accumu-
imately a 12-ounce glass o f beer, a 5-ounce
late 3 0 minutes or more of moderate-intensity
glass o f table wine, or a 1.5-ounce glass o f dis-
physical activity on most, preferably all, days
tilled spirits (Cushman et al., 1 9 9 8 ) .
of the week," with moderate physical activity defined as equivalent to brisk walking at 3 to 4
The Effect
of Alcohol
on Blood
Pressure
in
miles per hour for healthy adults (Pate et al.,
Even though the evi-
1 9 9 5 ) . Current recommendations were based
dence is fairly clear in implicating alcohol con-
on findings that moderate- to high-intensity
sumption as a risk factor for hypertension, the
exercise, if sustained, leads to a lowering o f BP
available data related to the effect o f reducing
in normotensive individuals and an apparent
alcohol consumption in hypertensive patients
reduction in the relative risk o f developing
Hypertensive
Patients.
Hypertension hypertension
(Braith, Pollock, Lowenthal,
(Hagberg, Ferrell, Dengel, & Wilund, 1 9 9 9 ) .
Limacher, 1 9 9 4 ; Paffenbarger,
This may account for some o f the variation in
Jung, Leung, & Hyde, 1 9 9 1 ) . For example,
BP responses to exercise, including the 2 5 % or
regularly scheduled moderate physical activity,
so o f hypertensive patients who show very
such as swimming or brisk walking for 3 0 min-
little response to exercise (Blumenthal, Siegel,
utes, has been reported to decrease both sys-
& Appelbaum, 1 9 9 1 ; Cooper, 2 0 0 0 ) . Although
Graves, &
tolic and diastolic BP from 4 to 8 m m Hg, with
most o f the work in this area has examined the
an average reduction of 6 to 7 m m Hg (Arrol
effect o f moderate-intensity exercise, there is
& Beaglehole, 1 9 9 2 ; Fagard, 1 9 9 5 ) , and this
some evidence to suggest a possible beneficial
level of activity may be more effective in low-
role o f light aerobic exercise in the reduction of
ering BP than more vigorous activity such as
BP in older patients. Young, Appel, Jee, and
jogging (Jennings, 1 9 9 7 ) .
Miller (1999) reported decreases of 7.0 m m Hg systolic BP and 2 . 4 m m H g diastolic BP in a
The Effect
of Exercise
on Blood
Pressure
in
sample of 6 2 sedentary older adults with high
T h e available evidence
normal or Stage 1 hypertension who had com-
suggests that regular rhythmic physical exer-
pleted a 12-week program of t'ai chi, an Eastern
cise of the lower extremities by hypertensive
form of relaxation and exercise.
Hypertensive
Patients.
individuals decreases both systolic and dia-
The sixth report o f the J N C ( 1 9 9 7 ) recom-
stolic BP (in about 7 5 % o f such individuals)
mended that hypertensive patients follow the
by as much as 11 m m H g and 8 m m Hg,
physical activity guidelines outlined in the sur-
respectively, independent
o f weight loss,
geon general's report for lowering resting BP,
alcohol intake, and sodium intake (Hagberg,
which consists o f moderately intense aerobic
Montain, Martin, & Ehsani, 1 9 8 9 ; Kelly &
exercise at 4 0 % to 6 0 % o f m a x i m u m oxygen
McClellan,
&
c o n s u m p t i o n . T h i s c a n be achieved, for
Cushman, 1 9 9 0 ) . Although the mechanism o f
example, by 3 0 to 4 5 minutes o f brisk walk-
1 9 9 4 ; Martin,
Dubbert,
action is not well understood in all cases, there
ing several times per week. T h e most recent
is some evidence that exercise training may
recommendations o f the Canadian Medical
result in regression o f pathological left ventric-
Association are consistent with these guide-
ular hypertrophy in at least some hypertensive
lines, specifically recommending that individu-
patients (Hagberg, Park, & Brown, 2 0 0 0 ) . In
als with mild hypertension should engage in
some cases, a consistent exercise regimen may
5 0 to 6 0 minutes o f moderate rhythmic exer-
also obviate the need for medication in mildly
cise o f the lower limbs, such as brisk walking
hypertensive
or
patients
(Kelemen,
Effron,
Valenti, & Stewart, 1 9 9 0 ) .
cycling, three to four times per week
(Cleroux, Feldman, & Petrella, 1 9 9 9 ) . T h e
The effect o f exercise in this population
Canadian Medical Association guidelines also
appears to be more pronounced in women
suggest that exercise should be prescribed as
than in men, and middle-aged people may
adjunctive therapy for individuals who require
derive more benefit than either younger or
pharmacotherapy for hypertension, especially
older people. Exercise reduces BP more consis-
those who are not receiving beta blockers.
tently in Asian and Pacific Islander patients than in Caucasian patients, especially when systolic BP is examined (Hagberg et al., 2 0 0 0 ) . There is some evidence to suggest that apoE, A C E , and LPL genotypes may identify hyper-
Stress and
Management Blood
Primary
Pressure Prevention
and
Stress
Manage-
tensive patients who are most likely to improve
ment. There is very little evidence in the litera-
BP, lipoprotein lipids, and
cardiovascular
ture to date to suggest that successful stress
disease risk the most with exercise training
management prevents hypertension despite the
262
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS implication that chronic psychosocial stress
Linden, Ramsden, &
may play a role in the etiology o f hypertension
particular, it has been suggested that a stress
Taenzer, 1 9 9 9 ) . In
(e.g., Anderson, Myers, & Pickering, 1 9 8 9 ;
management intervention should include some
Lucini, Norbiato, Clerici, & Pagani, 2 0 0 2 ) . For
or all o f the following components: skills train-
example, Phase 1 of the Trials of Hypertension
ing in adaptive mood management, communi-
Prevention project (Trials o f Hypertension
cation,
Prevention Collaborative Research Group,
reduction of sympathetic arousal as well as
problem
solving, and
relaxation/
1992) examined the effect of a multimodality
training in the réévaluation of negative life
stress management program, which included
events and increased awareness o f stressors
progressive relaxation, on the BP of 5 6 2 indi-
and stress responses.
viduals in the general population with normal
Linden and Chambers ( 1 9 9 4 ) compared
to high normal BP and found no statistically or
the effect sizes o f single-method relaxation,
clinically significant changes in BP following
multimethod relaxation, and
this intervention relative to matched controls
cognitive-behavioral treatment therapies
(Batey et al., 2 0 0 0 ) . However, further investi-
reduce BP. They reported
individualized, to
observed mean
gation in this area is needed to investigate the
decreases o f 9.7 m m Hg in systolic BP and 7.2
efficacy of highly individualized stress manage-
m m H g in diastolic BP with such interven-
ment programs as a preventive measure in
tions. Individualized,
at-risk target populations of normotensive indi-
treatment therapies were found to reduce BP
cognitive-behavioral
viduals with high levels o f chronic psychosocial
at a level comparable in effect size to BP med-
stress.
ications and weight reduction/physical exercise, with observed mean decreases o f 1 5 . 2
The Effect Fressure
of Stress Management
in Hypertensive
Patients.
Blood
m m Hg in systolic BP and 9.2 m m H g in
A variety o f
diastolic BP. T h e individualized, cognitive-
on
meta-analyses have been conducted to test the
behavioral
hypothesis that stress management interven-
studies focusing on marital communication
tions effectively reduce BP among hypertensive patients
(Eisenberg et
al.,
1993; Jacob,
treatment
therapies
included
training for hypertensive patients
(Ewart,
Taylor, Kraemer, & Agras, 1 9 8 4 ) , cognitive
Chesney, Williams, Ding, & Shapiro, 1 9 9 1 ;
restructuring and behavioral intervention pro-
Kaufman et al., 1 9 8 8 ; Linden & Chambers,
grams (Bosley & Allen, 1 9 8 9 ; Chesney, Black,
1 9 9 4 ) . A conservative interpretation o f such
Swan, & W a r d , 1 9 8 7 ; Jorgensen, Houston, &
meta-analytic findings is that individualized,
Zurawski, 1 9 8 1 ) , anger management inter-
multicomponent stress management treatment
ventions (Achmon, Granek, G o l o m b , & Hart,
appears to be more effective in lowering BP
1 9 8 9 ) , and individualized stress management
than do single-component, standardized relax-
that included the recognition of somatic cues
ation
for stress (Richter-Heinrich et al.,
interventions
(Johnston,
Gold,
&
very
encouraging
results
1981).
Kentish, 1 9 9 3 ; Linden & Chambers, 1 9 9 4 ;
Similar,
Ward, Swan, & Chesney, 1 9 8 7 ) . In fact, the
reported by Schneider and colleagues ( 1 9 9 5 )
were
results were negligible in most cases when a
and Linden, Lenz, and Con ( 2 0 0 1 ) .
single stress management technique was used. Therefore, the Canadian Medical Association has tentatively recommended that if stress
Pharmacological Interventions
is viewed as a prominent contributor to a
Hypertension is treated pharmacologically
patient's high BP, an individualized stress man-
by several categories o f medications called
agement intervention that uses multiple com-
antihypertension
ponents should be considered (Spence, Barnett,
Although details into the specific pharmacology
medications (Table
14.1).
Hypertension Table 14.1
Class of
Examples of Categories of Medications Used in the Treatment of High Blood Pressure
Medications
Generic Name Examples
Brand Name
Examples
Captopril Fosinopril sodium Moexipril Ramipril
Capoten Monopril Univasc Altace
Doxazosin mesylate Prazosin hydrochloride Terazosin hydrochloride
Cardura Minipress Hytrin
Candesartan Irbesartan Losartan potassium Valsartan
Candesartan Irbesartan Losartan potassium Valsartan
Acebutolol Atenolol Betaxolol Bisoprolol fumarate Carteolol hydrochloride Metoprolol tartrate Metoprolol succinate Nadolol Penbutolol sulfate Pindolol Propranolol hydrochloride Timolol maleate
Sectral Tenormin Kerlone Zebeta Cartrol Lopressor Toprol-XL Corgard Levatol Visken Inderal Blocadren
Amlodipine besylate Diltiazem hydrochloride
Norvasc Cardizem, Dilacor-XR, Tiazac Plendil DynaCirc Cardene Adalat, Procardia Sular Calan, Covera-HS, Isoptin, Verelan
ACE inhibitors
Alpha blockers
Angiotensin II receptor blockers
Beta blockers
Calcium channel blockers
Felodipine Isradipine Nicardipine Nifedipine Nisoldipine Verapamil hydrochloride Central agonists Alpha methyldopa Clonidine hydrochloride Guanabenz acetate Guanfacine hydrochloride
Aldomet Catapres Wytensin Tenex (Continued)
|
263
264
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS Table 14.1 Class of
(Continued)
Medications
Generic Name
Examples
Brand Name
Examples
Diuretics Chlorthalidone Chlorthalidone Furosemide Hydrochlorothiazide Indapamide Metolazone
of these categories of medications are beyond
Hygroton Hygroton Lasix Esidrix, hydroDIURIL, microzide Lozol Mykrox, Zaroxolyn
Pharmacological interventions
introduce
the scope of this chapter, familiarity with these
another complication, namely side effects.
classes and specific generic and brand names
Clinicians can help to reduce the negative
may be helpful for psychologists who work in
effects o f side effects on compliance by edu-
primary medical care settings. Furthermore,
cating their patients about these side effects,
information about side effects of these medica-
helping them to learn ways in which to min-
tions should enhance the expertise o f behav-
imize side effects or to adjust medication
ioral therapists in their efforts to help other
dosages in coordination with their prescribers
medical professionals and patients, drawing
after achieving normal B P .
better outcomes from pharmacotherapy and increasing compliance. O n e class o f medication that is used to treat
Multicomponent Approach
hypertension is diuretics. These medications
As noted earlier, there are well-docu-
work by helping the body remove excess flu-
mented lifestyle modifications with proven
ids and sodium salt. Beta blockers work by
efficacy (e.g., weight control, diet, exercise,
reducing the heart rate and thereby reducing
alcohol control, sodium restriction, at least
the volume o f blood ejected by the heart (car-
some stress management interventions). A
diac output). Because the sympathetic nervous
significant proportion o f all available health
system innervates the heart, arteries, and other
care resources are expended for the treatment
parts o f the body, activation o f this system can
of hypertension. Hypertension accounts for
increase heart work or constrict arteries, lead-
about 3 0 million physician visits per year in
ing to increased BP. Therefore, a class o f med-
the United States alone and is the second most
ications called sympathetic nerve inhibitors
c o m m o n reason for outpatient
works by inhibiting sympathetic nerves on
visits in the country (Knight et al., 2 0 0 1 ) .
blood vessels. Vasodilators work by causing
Despite this staggering number o f physician
blood vessel walls and muscles to relax, lead-
visits, and despite the impressive array o f
physician
ing to dilation o f the vessels and reducing their
pharmacological and
pressure. T h e angiotensin-converting enzyme
interventions that are available for the man-
nonpharmacological
(ACE) inhibitors work by interfering with
agement o f hypertension,
the production o f angiotensin. Angiotensin
(Berlowitz et a l , 1 9 9 8 ; H y m a n & Pavlik,
it is estimated
is a vasoconstricting chemical. T h e angio-
2 0 0 1 ; M u l r o w , 1 9 9 8 ) that fewer than one
tensin II receptor blockers block the effects o f
third o f U.S. patients with hypertension have
angiotensin. T h e calcium antagonists (calcium
controlled BP (less than 1 4 0 / 9 0 m m Hg). In
channel blockers) reduce BP by reducing heart
one large sample o f 1 , 3 9 4 N e w Y o r k City
rate and relaxing blood vessels.
health care workers, only 1 2 % o f those
Hypertension
\
patients w h o were treated for hypertension
of the American Society o f Hypertension
had BP controlled at less than 1 4 0 / 9 0 m m H g
Ad H o c Panel (Pickering, 1 9 9 5 ) describe the
despite having comprehensive medical insur-
role o f patient self-monitoring o f BP both in
ance and full access to health care (Stockwell,
assessing the response to
Madhaven,
medications more accurately and in poten-
Cohen, Gibson, &
Alerman,
1 9 9 4 ) . O f equal or greater concern, a recent
tially improving
patient
antihypertensive compliance
and
study that employed electronic monitoring o f
involvement in patients' own health care. In a
patients with long-standing serious hyperten-
similar fashion, Gonzalez-Fernandez, Rivera,
sion suggested that fully 6 1 % o f these patients
Torres, Quiles, and J a c k s o n ( 1 9 9 0 ) reported
failed to take their antihypertensive medica-
a 6 0 % increase in compliance and decreases
tion as prescribed (Lee et al., 1 9 9 6 ) .
of 1 4 / 1 1 m m H g systolic/diastolic BP at 8-
Multiple factors have been implicated as
week follow-up after a brief, four-session,
contributing to poor BP control, including the
inpatient educational program administered
lack o f primary care physicians in some pop-
to a sample o f 5 7 middle-aged to elderly
ulations
(Shea, M i s r a , Ehrlich, Field,
&
hypertensive patients.
Francis, 1 9 9 2 ) , the cost o f antihypertensive
There are additional intervention strategies
medication (Ahluwalia, M c N a g n y , & R a s k ,
that may be useful to consider in an effort to
1 9 9 7 ) , medication side effects, presence or
maximize patient compliance and minimize
absence o f comorbid illness, age o f the patient,
adherence problems. T h e use o f reminders
severity o f the disease, health habits, intensity
and prompts, including medication reminder
of care, physician compliance with estab-
calendars or alerts, specialized medication
lished guidelines (Oliveria et al., 2 0 0 2 ) , c o m -
dispensers,
plexity o f the medication regimen, patient
physician or clinic appointments, is very useful
and
reminder
calls
regarding
knowledge base and understanding o f the
for many patients. Behavioral contracting has
seriousness o f uncontrolled hypertension, and
a clear role in the management o f hypertensive
patient behavior related to compliance and
patients, particularly in maintaining adherence
adherence (Knight et al., 2 0 0 1 ; Oliveria et al.,
to nonpharmacological interventions. Stepwise
2 0 0 2 ) . T h e remainder o f this section focuses
introduction of antihypertensive interventions
on the role o f educational
is recommended, that is, using a graduated
interventions,
multicomponent behavioral programs,
and
approach o f introducing more easily attainable
interventions
and
goals early in treatment so as to shape subse-
related
to c o m p l i a n c e
adherence for hypertensive patients given that
quent adherence behavior. Similarly, interven-
patient adherence has been identified as one
tions designed to enhance the medical office
o f the main reasons why BP therapy fails
visit experience have been documented
(Miller, Hill, Kottke, & O c k e n e , 1 9 9 7 ) .
increase adherence. These include using a
to
General guidelines for physicians related to
scheduling system that minimizes waiting time
improving patients' adherence to antihyper-
for patients, providing adequate opportunity
tensive therapy have been published by the
for
JNC
care personnel
(1997)
and
elaborated
by
Kaplan
patients to communicate with
health
(including liberal call-in or
( 1 9 9 8 ) . T a b l e 1 4 . 2 represents a compilation
on-call with clinic staff), and other interven-
of the recommendations from both o f these
tions designed to provide a
sources. M o s t o f these
patient experience in a physically attractive
recommendations
rest on solid behavioral principles from the
personalized
and positive medical office environment.
behavioral medicine adherence literature. For
A recent meta-analysis of the medical adher-
example, self-monitoring is a cornerstone o f
ence literature completed by Roter and col-
assessing adherence, and the recent guidelines
leagues (1998) o f 1 5 3 intervention strategies
265
266
[
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS
Table 14.2
General Guidelines to Improve Patient Adherence to Antihypertensive Therapy
1. Be aware of signs of patient nonadherence to antihypertensive therapy. 2. Establish the goal of therapy: to reduce blood pressure to nonhypertensive levels with minimal or no adverse side effects. 3. Educate patients about the disease, and involve them and their families in its treatment, a. Have patients measure blood pressure at home. 4. Maintain contact with patients, a. Consider telecommunications. 5. Keep care inexpensive and simple. a. Integrate pill taking into routine activities of daily living. 6. Encourage lifestyle modifications. 7. Prescribe medications according to pharmacological principles, favoring long-acting formulations. a. Continue to add effective and tolerated drugs, stepwise, in sufficient doses to achieve the goals of therapy. b. Be willing to stop unsuccessful therapy and to try a different approach. c. Anticipate side effects, and adjust therapy to ameliorate side effects that do not disappear spontaneously. 8. Encourage a positive attitude about achieving therapeutic goals. 9. Consider using nurse case management. S O U R C E : Joint National Committee on Detection, Evaluation, and Treatment of High Blood Pressure (1997). N O T E : Cortisol response is initiated by the release of corticotropin releasing factors (CRF) from neuronal cell bodies of the paraventricular nucleus (PVN). C R F acts on the corticotrope cells of the anterior pituitary, stimulating synthesis of proopiomelanocortin ( P O M S ) , and leading to the subsequent release of A C T H and β - e n d o r p h i n into the systemic circulation. U p o n reaching the adrenal cortex, A C T H leads to the synthesis and release of Cortisol. Cortisol, through direct and indirect cardiovascular mechanisms, contributes to increased blood pressure. These effects, as discussed in the text, seem to be stronger in hypertension-prone individuals.
designed to promote patient adherence
to
that educational interventions plus counseling
medical regimens suggests that adherence strate-
interventions achieved a higher level of hyper-
gies that combine educational, behavioral, and
tension control than did either intervention
affective components may be more effective
alone. T h e best method to increase compliance
than single-focus interventions. For example,
and adherence is likely to involve highly individ-
Clifford, Tan, and Gorsuch (1991) reported sig-
ualized
nificant improvements in weight, percentage
example, patients who characteristically have
body fat, exercise adherence, and systolic and
active styles of coping with illness and treat-
approaches
to
each
patient.
For
diastolic BP in middle-aged overweight patients
ment-related
who were given social support and
control-based strategies such as self-monitoring,
taught
several cognitive-behavioral techniques
issues may benefit from
self-
that
self-evaluation, and self-reinforcement of adher-
applied to exercise adherence, weight reduction/
ence behaviors (Haynes et a l , 1 9 7 6 ) . Patients
maintenance, and stress management relative to
with more passive styles may benefit from more
an assessment-only control group.
structured
adherence interventions such as
Similarly, Boulware and colleagues' (2001)
behavioral contracting, direct instruction by
meta-analysis of 15 behavioral interventions for
health care providers, and external inducements
hypertension involving 4 , 0 7 2 patients noted
or rewards (Christensen & Johnson, 2 0 0 2 ) .
Hypertension
CASE S T U D Y T h e patient, "George R i c h a r d s , " is a 42-year-old married Caucasian male w h o works as a certified public accountant in Minneapolis, M i n n e s o t a . H e had received episodic medical care over the course o f the past 1 5 years, although he has had excellent health benefits through his company. H e had seldom been sick and had not seen any reason to visit a doctor. H e was last seen medically 4 years ago, at age 3 8 years, when he visited his family physician for a physical examination prior to scheduled surgery for repair o f a hernia that he had sustained while helping a friend move some furniture. This had been George's only surgery, and up to that point he had experienced no chronic illnesses o f any sort. At that time, he was 5 feet 7 inches tall and weighed 1 7 0 pounds. H e was a nonsmoker and social drinker, averaging four to six glasses o f wine per week. H e had few hobbies and was a self-professed " w o r k a h o l i c " w h o typically worked 6 0 hours or more per week in a very stressful w o r k environment. At that doctor visit, George's BP was 1 4 5 / 9 5 . H e had n o k n o w n family history o f either hypertension or cardiovascular disease. His cholesterol was within or below the average range at 1 7 2 , and his lipid profile was within normal limits. N o electrocardiogram ( E C G ) was done. H e was told by the family physician to lose 1 0 pounds and to try to get into some sort o f exercise program. H e was also advised to try to do something for relaxation and to cut b a c k his hours at w o r k . H e was advised that his BP would be checked on each subsequent visit back to see the doctor. After George was fully recovered from his noneventful
hernia surgery, he
bought an exercise bicycle but found that he seldom had time to use it and also found this to be a relatively boring activity for him. H e began walking with his wife when he could fit it into his w o r k schedule, which typically was one time per week or less and usually was a mile or so at a time unless the couple was interrupted by a call on George's cell phone. H e tried to watch his weight initially, but he frequently ate in restaurants with business clients and eventually went b a c k to a traditional "meat and potatoes" diet. His alcohol intake increased to t w o to three drinks per day, usually in an attempt to " u n w i n d " after a long day at the office. H e had been promoted to a supervisory position in his office, and this increased his workload and job-related stress; however, with t w o children in college, he felt that he had no other option than to w o r k longer and longer hours. H e developed some occasional sleep onset insomnia but otherwise convinced himself that he "felt just fine" and saw no reason to see his physician. Because o f changes in his company's health maintenance organization insurance plan, George was recently assigned to a different medical practice with a new physician, and an initial physical was recommended
by that medical
plan.
Although he saw no reason to see this new physician either, George reluctantly made an appointment for a physical e x a m . H e had recently become a partner in his firm and had considerable difficulty in fitting a visit to the physician into his busy
267
268
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS
schedule. At this initial visit, George's B P averaged 1 7 5 / 1 0 5 over three separate readings. His weight was 1 8 5 pounds. An E C G done in the office showed a normal rate and rhythm but revealed an axis deviation consistent with mild left ventricular hypertrophy. George was informed that his B P was in the range o f Stage 2 hypertension, which required initial nonpharmacological intervention and BP monitoring every 2 weeks by the clinic nursing staff. A " n o added salt" diet (3 to 4 grams per day) was prescribed that was low in saturated fat and rich in fruits, vegetables, and low-fat dairy products. H e was advised to limit his alcohol intake t o 1 ounce per day. Because his office building had a health club, he was encouraged to purchase a membership and schedule time for exercise at least three times per week. H e was told that if his BP did not normalize within 2 t o 3 months, a full dose o f a first-line antihypertensive medication would be necessary. Subsequent to this office visit, George's BPs averaged 1 6 5 / 1 0 0 in clinic B P checks over the course o f the next 6 weeks. George had lost about 4 pounds but was having difficulty in sticking to his diet. Although he did purchase a health club membership, he found exercising there t o be as boring as it had been using the exercise bicycle, and he had used his membership on only two brief occasions. H e did cut back his alcohol intake to one to two drinks per day but discovered that he had trouble unwinding and had significant problems with insomnia when he had fewer drinks than that. W h e n George saw his physician at 6-week follow-up, the decision was made to refer him t o a local behavioral medicine clinic to attempt to bolster the nonpharmacological interventions prescribed to that point. A psychologist at the behavioral medicine clinic saw George 2 weeks later. At the request o f this psychologist, George was seen with his wife. An individualized exercise plan was crafted for him, including daily walking with his wife or walking on a treadmill placed in front o f the family's large-screen television at increasing rates and distances that he would self-monitor and report b a c k on subsequent weekly visits. George and his wife were given copies o f a modified version o f the D A S H diet that would fit their lifestyle and were asked t o keep dietary intake records. At a subsequent visit 2 weeks later, a behavioral intervention program for George's insomnia was devised. George was enrolled in a stress management course that included relaxation tapes that he could use to help with his sleep onset problems as well as training in life management skills that he could apply in his place o f business. George was seen again by his family physician after attending six weekly sessions at the behavioral medicine clinic. His weight was n o w 1 7 4 pounds, and his BP was 1 5 0 / 9 5 . H e was walking 1 to 2 miles an average o f five times per week, and his alcohol consumption was down to one to two glasses o f wine per week. His physician started him on a low dose o f an A C E inhibitor (Lisinopril, 5 milligrams per day) as an adjunct to the behavioral interventions. At 1 month follow-up, George's BP had stabilized at 1 3 0 / 8 5 , his total cholesterol was 1 9 1 , his weight had stabilized at 1 7 0 pounds, and he was continuing in monthly maintenance sessions at the behavioral medicine clinic.
Hypertension
behavioral medicine research has contributed
CONCLUSIONS
significantly Hypertension is a major risk factor for many devastating
\
illnesses, including
coronary
heart disease, stroke, and kidney
toward
expanding
available
intervention methods. Because o f the longterm
nature o f interventions,
behavioral
failure.
researchers have focused their efforts on devel-
Efforts to develop effective means for diag-
oping diverse behavioral modification tech-
nosing and treating hypertension have been
niques, targeting factors such as weight, eating
in the forefront o f public health research.
habits, physical activity, and substance use. In
These efforts have led to greater recognition
addition to behavioral and psychosocial meth-
o f the importance o f frequent screening o f BP
ods, advancement has been made in available
and for implementing reliable means
pharmacological treatment options.
for
measuring B P , including the use o f ambulatory monitoring for diagnosing this disorder. A strong movement toward promoting pre-
Although advancement has been made in developing effective control strategies, many challenges in implementing them
remain.
ventive efforts has also characterized public
These include compliance with regimens and
health work over the past two decades. T h e
consistency in applying interventions over a
greater awareness o f hypertension has also led
long period o f time. Behavioral medicine
to a more comprehensive view o f the etiologi-
scientists and clinicians are in the forefront
cal
of trying to maximize patients' cooperation
factors o f this disorder,
consequently
leading to a greater emphasis on a menu o f
and
strategies targeting BP control. T o that end,
hypertension.
c h a n c e s for
success in
controlling
REFERENCES Achmon, J . , Granek, M., Golomb, M., &C Hart, J . (1989). Behavioral treatment of essential hypertension: A comparison between cognitive therapy and biofeedback of heart rate. Psychosomatic Medicine, 51, 1 5 2 - 1 6 4 . Ahluwalia, J . S., McNagny, S. E., & Rask, K. J . (1997). Correlates of controlled hypertension in indigent, inner-city hypertensive patients. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 12, 7 - 1 4 . al'Absi, M., & Arnett, D. K. (2000). Adrenocortical responses to psychological 54, 2 3 4 - 2 4 4 . stress and risk for hypertension. Biomedical Pharmacotherapy, al'Absi, M., Buchanan, T., & Lovallo, W. R. (1996). Pain perception and cardiovascular responses in men with positive parental history for hypertension. Psychophysiology, 33, 6 5 5 - 6 6 1 . al'Absi, M., Everson, S., & Lovallo, W. R. (1995). Hypertension risk factors and Journal of cardiovascular reactivity to mental stress in men. International Psychophysiology, 20, 1 5 5 - 1 6 0 . al'Absi, M., & Lovallo, W. R. (1993). Cortisol concentrations in serum of borderline hypertensive men exposed to a novel experimental setting. Psychoneuro18, 3 5 5 - 3 6 3 . endocrinology, al'Absi, M., Lovallo, W. R., McKey, B . , &c Pincomb, G. (1994). Borderline hypertensives produce exaggerated adrenocortical responses to mental stress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 56, 2 4 5 - 2 5 0 . al'Absi, M., Lovallo, W. R., McKey, B., Sung, B. H., Whitsett, T. L., & Wilson, M . F. (1998). Hypothalamic-pituitary adrenocortical responses to psychological Psychosomatic stress and caffeine in men at high and low risk for hypertension. Medicine, 60, 5 2 1 - 5 2 7 .
269
270
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS al'Absi, M., Lovallo, W. R., Pincomb, G. P., Sung, Β. H., & Wilson, M . F. (1995). Adrenocortical effects of caffeine and behavioral stress on adrenocortical responses in hypertension-prone men. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 2, 2 6 3 - 2 7 5 . Alexander, F. (1939). Emotional factor in essential hypertension. Psychosomatic Medicine, 1, 1 7 3 - 1 7 9 . Alpert, Μ . Α., &C Hashimi, M . W. (1993). Obesity and the heart. Journal of Medical Science, 306, 117. Anderson, Ν . B . , Myers, H. F., &C Pickering, T. (1989). Hypertension in blacks: Psychosocial and biological perspectives. Journal of Hypertension, 7, 161-172. Appel, L. J . , Moore, T. J . , Obarzanek, E., Vollmer, W. M., Svetkey, L. P., Sacks, F. M., Bray, G. Α., Vogt, T. M., Cutler, J . Α., Windhauser, M. M., Lin, P. H., & Karanja, N. (1997). A clinical trial of the effects of dietary patterns on blood pressure: DASH Collaborative Research Group. New England Journal of Medicine, 336, 1 1 1 7 - 1 1 2 4 . Arroll, B., & Beaglehole, R. (1992). Does physical activity lower blood pressure? A critical review of the clinical trials. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 45, 439-447. Batey, D. M., Kaufmann, P. G., Raczynski, J . M., Hollis, J . F., Murphy, J . K., Rosner, B., Corrigan, S. Α., Rappaport, Ν. B., Danielson, E. M., Lasser, N. L., Sc Kuhn, C. M. (2000). Stress management intervention for primary prevention of hypertension: Detailed results from Phase I of Trials of Hypertension 10(1), 4 5 - 5 8 . Prevention (TOHP-I). Annals of Epidemiology, Berlowitz, D. R., Ash, A. S., Hickey, E. C , Friedman, R. H., Glickman, M., Kader, B., 8c Moskowitz, M . A. (1998). Inadequate management of blood pressure in a hypertensive population. New England Journal of Medicine, 339, 1957-1963. Blumenthal, J . Α., Siegel, W. C , Sc Appelbaum, M. (1991). Failure of exercise to reduce blood pressure in patients with mild hypertension: Results of a randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Medical Association, 266, 2098-2104. Bonato, D. P., Sc Boland, F. J . (1986). A comparison of specific strategies for longterm maintenance following a behavioural treatment program for obese women. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 5, 9 4 9 - 9 5 8 . Bosley, F., Sc Allen, T. W. (1989). Stress management training for hypertensives: Medicine, 12, Cognitive and physiological effects. Journal of Behavioral 77-89. Boulware, L. E., Daumit, G. L., Frick, K. D., Minkovitz, C. S., Lawrence, R. S., Sc Powe, N. R. (2001). An evidence-based review of patient-centered behavioral interventions for hypertension. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 21, 221-232. Braith, R. W., Pollock, M . L., Lowenthal, D. T., Graves, J . E., & Limacher, M. C. (1994). Moderate- and high-intensity exercise lowers blood pressure in normotensive subjects 6 0 - 7 9 years of age. American Journal of Cardiology, 73, 1124-1128. Brand, M . B . , Mulrow, C. D., Chiquette, E., Angel, L., Cornell, J . E., Summerhill, C , & Grem, R., J r . (1998). Weight-reducing diets for control of hypertension in adults. The Cochrane Library, 4. (Oxford, UK: Update Software) Brunner, E., White, I., Thorogood, M., Bristow, Α., Curie, D., Sc Marmot, M . (1997). Can dietary interventions change diet and cardiovascular risk factors? A meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. American Journal of Public Health, 87, 1 4 1 5 - 1 4 2 2 .
Hypertension Bunag, R. D., & Takeda, K. (1979). Sympathetic hyperresponsiveness to hypothalamic stimulation in young hypertensive rats. American journal of Physiology, 237, R39-R44. Burt, V. L., Whelton, P., Roccella, E. J . , Brown, C , Cutler, J . Α., Higgins, M., Horan, M . J . , & Labarthe, D. (1995). Prevalence of hypertension in the U.S. adult population: Results from the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, 1 9 8 8 - 1 9 9 1 . Hypertension, 25, 3 0 5 - 3 1 3 . Campbell, N. R., Ashley, M . J . , Carruthers, S. G., Lacourciere, Y . , & McKay, D. W. (1999). Lifestyle modifications to prevent and control hypertension: III. Recommendations on alcohol consumption. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 160(9, Suppl.), S 1 3 - S 2 0 . Cardillo, C., De Felice, F., Campia, U., & Folli, G. (1993). Psychophysiological reactivity and cardiac end-organ changes in white coat hypertension. Hypertension, 21, 8 3 6 - 8 4 4 . Cavallini, M. C , Roman, M . J . , Pickering, T. G., Schwartz, J . E., Pini, R., & Devereux, R.B. (1995). Is white coat hypertension associated with arterial disease or left ventricular hypertrophy? Hypertension, 26, 4 1 3 - 4 1 9 . Chesney, Μ. Α., Black, G. W., Swan, G. E., &C Ward, M . M . (1987). Relaxation training for essential hypertension at the worksite: I. The untreated mild hypertensive. Psychosomatic Medicine, 49, 2 5 0 - 2 6 3 . Christensen, A. J . , & Johnson, J . A. (2002). Patient adherence with medical treatment regimens: An interactive approach. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(3), 9 4 - 9 7 . Cleroux, J . , Feldman, R. D., & Petrella, R. J . (1999). Lifestyle modifications to prevent and control hypertension: IV. Recommendations on physical exercise training. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 160(9, Suppl.), S 2 1 - S 2 8 . Clifford, P. Α., Tan, S., & Gorsuch, R. L. (1991). Efficacy of a self-directed behavioral health change program: Weight, body composition, cardiovascular fitness, blood pressure, health risk, and psychosocial mediating variables. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 14, 3 0 3 - 3 2 3 . Cooper, A. R. (2000). What is the magnitude of blood pressure response to a programme of moderate intensity exercise? Randomised controlled trial among sedentary adults with unmedicated hypertension. British Journal of General Practice, 50, 9 5 8 - 9 6 2 . Corrigan, S. Α., Raczynski, J . M., Swencionis, C , &c Jennings, S. G. ( 1 9 9 1 ) . Weight reduction in the prevention and treatment of hypertension: A review of representative clinical trials. American Journal of Health Promotion, 5, 208-214. Cushman, W. C , Cutler, J . Α., Hanna, E., Bingham, S. F., Follmann, D., Harford, T., Dubbert, P., Allender, P. S., Dufour, M., Collins, J . F., Walsh, S. M., Kirk, G. F., Burg, M., Felicetta, J . V., Hamilton, B . P., Katz, L. Α., Perry, Η. M., Willenbring, M . L., Lakshman, R., &c Hamburger, R. J . , for the PATHS Group. (1998). Prevention and Treatment of Hypertension Study (PATH): Effects of an alcohol treatment program on blood pressure. Archives of Internal Medicine, 158, 1 1 9 7 - 1 2 0 7 . Davies, A. O., & Lefkowitz, R. J . (1984). Regulation of beta-adrenergic receptors by steroid hormones. Annual Review of Physiology, 46, 1 1 9 - 1 3 0 . Ebrahim, S., 8c Davey, S. G. (1998). Lowering blood pressure: A systematic review of sustained effects of non-pharmacological interventions. Journal of Public Health, 2, 4 4 1 - 4 4 8 . Eisenberg, D. M., Delbanco, T. L., Berkey, C. S., Kaptchuk, T. J . , Kupelnick, B., Kuhl, J . , &c Chalmers, T. (1993). Cognitive behavioral techniques for hypertension: Are they effective? Annals of Internal Medicine, 118, 9 6 4 - 9 7 2 .
\
271
272
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS Elliott, P. (1991). Observational studies of salt and blood pressure. Hypertension, 17, 3 - 8 . Everson, S. Α., Kaplan, G. Α., Goldberg, D. E., 8c Salonen, J . T. (1996). Anticipatory blood pressure response to exercise predicts future high blood pressure in middle-aged men. Hypertension, 27, 1 0 5 9 - 1 0 6 4 . Everson, S. Α., Lynch, J . W., Chesney, Μ. Α., Kaplan, G. Α., Goldberg, D. E., Shade, S. B., Cohen, R. D., Salonen, R., 8c Salonen, J . T. (1997). Interaction of workplace demands and cardiovascular reactivity in progression of carotid atherosclerosis: Population based study. British Medical Journal, 314, 553-558. Ewart, C. K., Taylor, C. B., Kraemer, H. C , 8c Agras, W. S. (1984). Reducing blood pressure reactivity during interpersonal conflict: Effects of marital communication training. Behavior Therapy, 15, 4 7 3 - 4 8 4 . Fagard, R. H. (1995). The role of exercise in blood pressure control: Supportive evidence. Journal of Hypertension, 13, 1 2 2 3 - 1 2 2 7 . Fallo, F., Paoletta, Α., Tona, F., Boscaro, M., 8c Sonino, N. (1993). Response of hypertension to conventional antihypertensive treatment and/or steroidogenesis inhibitors in Cushing's syndrome. Journal of Internal Medicine, 234, 5 9 5 - 5 9 8 . Fodor, J . G., Whitmore, B., Leenen, F., 8c Larochelle, P. (1999). Lifestyle modifications to prevent and control hypertension: V. Recommendations on dietary salt. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 260(Suppl. 9 ) , S 2 9 - S 3 4 . Fuchs, F. D., Chambless, L. E., Whelton, P. K., Nieto, F. J . , 8c Heiss, G. (2001). Alcohol consumption and the incidence of hypertension: The Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Study. Hypertension, 37, 1 2 4 2 - 1 2 5 0 . Gilman, M . W., Cook, N. R., Evans, D. Α., Rosner, B., Se Hennekens, C. H. (1995). Relationship of alcohol intake with blood pressure in young adults. Hypertension, 25, 1 1 0 6 - 1 1 1 0 . Gonzalez-Fernandez, R. Α., Rivera, M., Torres, D., Quiles, J . , 8c Jackson, A. (1990). Usefulness of a systemic hypertension in-hospital educational program. American Journal of Cardiology, 65, 1 3 8 4 - 1 3 8 6 . Gosse, P., Promax, H., Durandet, P., Sc Clementy, J . (1993). White coat hypertension: No harm for the heart. Hypertension, 22, 7 6 6 - 7 7 0 . Hagberg, J . M., Ferrell, R. E., Dengel, D. R., 8c Wilund, K. R. (1999). Exercise training-induced blood pressure and plasma lipid improvements in hypertensives may be genotype dependent. Hypertension, 34, 1 8 - 2 3 . Hagberg, J . M., Montain, S. J . , Martin, W. H., Sc Ehsani, A. A. (1989). Effect of exercise training in 6 0 - to 69-year-old persons with essential hypertension. American Journal of Cardiology, 64, 3 4 8 - 3 5 3 . Hagberg, J . M., Park, J . J . , 8c Brown, M . D. (2000). The role of exercise training in the treatment of hypertension: An update. Sports Medicine, 30(3), 193-206. Hall, J . E. (1997). Mechanisms of abnormal renal sodium handling in obesity hypertension. American Journal of Hypertension, 10, S49. Haynes, R. B., Sackett, D. L., Gibson, E. S., Taylor, D. W., Hackett, B . C , Roberts, R. S., 8c Johnson, A. L. (1976). Improvement of medication compliance in uncontrolled hypertension. Lancet, 1, 1 2 6 5 - 1 2 6 8 . He, J . , Ogden, L. G., Vupputuri, S., Bazzano, L. Α., Loria, C , 8c Whelton, P. K. (1999). Dietary sodium intake and subsequent risk of cardiovascular disease in overweight adults. Journal of the American Medical Association, 282, 2027-2034. He, J . , Whelton, P. K., Appel, L. J . , Charleston, J . , Se Klag, M. J . (2000). Long-term effects of weight loss and dietary sodium reduction on incidence of hypertension. Hypertension, 35, 5 4 4 - 5 4 9 .
Hypertension Henry, J . P., Liu, Y . Y . , Nadra, W. E., Qian, C , Mormede., P., Lemaire, V., Ely, D., & Hendley, E. D. (1993). Psychosocial stress can induce chronic hypertension in normotensive strains of rats. Hypertension, 21, 7 1 4 - 7 2 3 . Himeno, E., Nishino, K., Nakashima, Y., Kuroiwa, Α., & Ikeda, M. (1996). Weight reduction regresses left ventricular mass regardless of blood pressure level in obese subjects. American Heart Journal, 131, 3 1 3 . Horan, M . J . , & Mockrin, S. C. (1992). Heterogeneity of hypertension. American Journal of Hypertension, 5, SI 10—SI 13. Hyman, D. J . , &C Pavlik, V. N. (2001). Characteristics of patients with uncontrolled hypertension in the United States. New England Journal of Medicine, 345, 479-486. Ishibashi, K., Oshima, T., Matsuura, H., Watanabe, M., Ishida, M., Ishida, T., Ozono, R., Kajiyama, G., & Kanbe, M . (1994). Effects of age and sex on sodium chloride sensitivity: Association with plasma rennin activity. Clinical Nephrology, 42, 3 7 6 - 3 8 0 . Jacob, R. G., Chesney, Μ . Α., Williams, D. M., Ding, Y . , &C Shapiro, A. P. (1991). Relaxation therapy for hypertension: Design effects and treatment effects. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 13, 5 - 1 7 . Jennings, G. L. (1997). Exercise and blood pressure control: Walk, run, or swim? Journal of Hypertension, 15, 5 6 7 - 5 6 9 . Jin, C. B., & Rockhold, R. W. (1991). Sympathoadrenal control by paraventricular hypothalamic beta-endorphin in hypertension. Hypertension, 18, 5 0 3 - 5 1 5 . Johnston, D. W., Gold, Α., & Kentish, J . (1993). Effect of stress management on blood pressure in mild primary hypertension. British Medical Journal, 306, 963-966. Joint National Committee on Detection, Evaluation, and Treatment of High Blood Pressure. (1997). The sixth report of the Joint National Committee on Prevention, Detection, Evaluation, and Treatment of High Blood Pressure. Archives of Internal Medicine, 157, 2 4 1 3 - 2 4 4 6 . Jorgensen, R. S., Houston, Β. K., & Zurawski, R. M . (1981). Anxiety management training in the treatment of essential hypertension. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 19, 4 6 7 ^ 7 4 . Julius, S., Mejia, Α., Jones, K., Krause, L., Schork, N., Van de Ven, C , Johnson, E., Pétrin, J . , Sekkarie, M., Kjedsen, S. E., Schmouder, R., Gupta, R., Ferraro, J . , Nazzaro, P., & Weissfeld, J . (1990). "White coat" versus "sustained" borderline hypertension in Tecumseh, Michigan. Hypertension, 16, 6 1 7 - 6 2 3 . Julius, S., & Nesbitt, S. (1996). Sympathetic overactivity in hypertension: A moving target. American Journal of Hypertension, 9, S 1 1 3 - S 1 2 0 . Kaplan, Ν. M . (1990). Clinical hypertension (5th ed.). Baltimore, M D : Williams Sc Wilkins. Kaplan, N. M . (Ed.). (1998). Clinical hypertension (7th ed.). Baltimore, M D : Williams & Wilkins. Kaufmann, P. G., Jacob, R. G., Ewart, C. K., Chesney, Μ . Α., Muenz, L. R., Doub, N., & Mercer, W. (1988). Hypertension intervention pooling project. Health Psychology, 7(Suppl.), 2 0 9 - 2 2 4 . Kelemen, M. H., Effron, M . B . , Valenti, S. Α., & Stewart, K. J . (1990). Exercise training combined with antihypertensive drug therapy. Journal of the American Medical Association, 263, 2 7 6 6 . Kelly, G., & McClellan, P. (1994). Antihypertensive effects of aerobic exercise: A brief meta-analytic review of randomized controlled trials. American Journal of Hypertension, 7, 1 1 5 - 1 1 9 . Keltikangas-Jarvinen, L., Raikkonen, K., Hautanen, Α., & Adlercreutz, H. (1996). Vital exhaustion, anger expression, and pituitary and adrenocortical hormones:
\
273
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS Implications for the insulin resistance syndrome. Arteriosclerosis, Thrombosis, & Vascular Biology, 16, 2 7 5 - 2 8 0 . Klatsky, A. L., Friedman, G. D., Siegelaub, A. B., & Gerard, M. J . (1977). Alcohol consumption and blood pressure: Kaiser-Permanente Multiphasic Health Examination data. New England Journal of Medicine, 296, 1 1 9 4 - 1 2 0 0 . Knight, E. L., Bohn, R. L., Wang, P. S., Glynn, R. J . , Mogun, H , & Avorn, J . (2001). Predictors of uncontrolled hypertension in ambulatory patients. Hypertension, 38, 8 0 9 - 8 1 4 . Kojima, S., Murakami, K., Kimura, G., Sanai, T., Yoshida, K., Imanishi, M., Abe, H., Kawamura, M., Kawano, Y . , & Ashida, T. (1992). A gender difference in the association between salt sensitivity and family history of hypertension. American Journal of Hypertension, 5, 1-7. Koren, M . J . , Mensah, G. Α., Blake, J . , Laragh, J . F., & Devereux, R. B. (1993). Comparison of left ventricular mass and geometry in black and white patients with essential hypertension. American Journal of Hypertension, 6, 8 1 5 - 8 2 3 . Kuwajima, I., Suzuki, Y . , Fujisawa, Α., & Kuramoto, K. (1993). Is white coat hypertension innocent? Structure and function of the heart in the elderly. Hypertension, 22, 8 2 6 - 8 3 1 . Langford, H. G., Blaufox, M . D., Oberman, Α., Hawkins, C. M., Curb, J . D., Cutter, G. R., Wasserthell-Smoller, S., Pressel, S., Babcock, C , Abernethy, J . D., Hotchkiss, J . , & Tyler, M . (1985). Dietary therapy slows the return of hypertension after stopping prolonged medication. Journal of the American Medical Association, 253, 6 5 7 - 6 6 4 . Lantelme, P., Milon, H., Gharib, C , Gayet, C , & Fortrat, J . O. (1998). White coat effect and reactivity to stress: Cardiovascular and autonomic nervous system responses. Hypertension, 31, 1 0 2 1 - 1 0 2 9 . Lee, J . Y . , Kusek, J . W., Greene, P. G., Bernhard, S., Norris, K., Smith, D., Wilkening, B., & Wright, J . T. (1996). Assessing medication adherence by pill count and electronic monitoring in the African American Study of Kidney Disease and Hypertension (AASK) pilot study. American Journal of Hypertension, 9, 7 1 9 - 7 2 5 . Lie, H., Mundal, R., & Erikssen, J . (1985). Coronary risk factors and incidence of coronary death in relation to physical fitness: Seven-year follow-up study of middle-aged men and elderly men. European Heart Journal, 6, 1 4 7 - 1 5 7 . Light, K. C , Girdler, S. S., Sherwood, Α., Bragdon, Ε. E., Brownley, Κ. Α., West, S. G., & Hinderliter, A. L. (1999). High stress responsivity predicts later blood pressure only in combination with positive family history and high life stress. Hypertension, 33, 1 4 5 8 - 1 4 6 4 . Linden, W., 8i Chambers, L. (1994). Clinical effectiveness of non-drug treatment for hypertension: A meta-analysis. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 16, 3 5 - 4 5 . Linden, W., Lenz, J . W., & Con, A. H. (2001). Individualized stress management for primary hypertension: A randomized trial. Archives of Internal Medicine, 161, 1 0 7 1 - 1 0 8 0 . Litchfield, W. R., Hunt, S. C , Jeunemaitre, X . , Fisher, N. D., Hopkins, P. N., Williams, R. R., Corvol, P., & Williams, G. H. (1998). Increased urinary free Cortisol: A potential intermediate phenotype of essential hypertension. Hypertension, 31, 5 6 9 - 5 7 4 . Lucini, D., Norbiato, G., Clerici, M., & Pagani, M. (2002). Hemodynamic and autonomic adjustments to real life stress conditions in humans. Hypertension, 39, 1 8 4 - 1 8 8 . MacMahon, S., Cutler, J . , Brittain, E., & Higgins, M. (1987). Obesity and hypertension: Epidemiological and clinical issues. European Heart Journal, S(Suppl. B), 5 7 - 7 0 .
Hypertension Martin, J . E., Dubbert, P. M., & Cushman, W. C. (1990). Controlled trial of aerobic exercise in hypertension. Circulation, 81, 1 5 6 0 - 1 5 6 7 . Matthews, Κ. Α., Woodall, K. L., & Allen, M. T. (1993). Cardiovascular reactivity to stress predicts future blood pressure status. Hypertension, 22, 4 7 9 - 4 8 5 . Menkes, M. S., Matthews, Κ. Α., Krantz, D. S., Lundberg, U., Mead, L. Α., Qaqish, B., Liang, K. Y., Thomas, C. B., &c Pearson, T. A. (1989). Cardiovascular reactivity to the cold pressor test as a predictor of hypertension. Hypertension, 14, 524-530. Messerli, F. H. (1982). Cardiovascular effects of obesity and hypertension. Lancet, 1, 1 1 6 5 . Miller, N. EL, Hill, M., Kottke, T., & Ockene, I. S. (1997). The multi-level compliance challenge: Recommendations for a call to action—A statement for healthcare professionals. Circulation, 95, 1 0 8 5 - 1 0 9 0 . Mulrow, P. J . ( 1 9 9 8 ) . Detection and control of hypertension in the population: The United States experience. American Journal of Hypertension, 11, 744-746. Myers, M. G., & Reeves, R. A. ( 1 9 9 1 ) . White coat phenomenon in patients receiving antihypertensive therapy. American Journal of Hypertension, 4, 844-849. Neaton, J . D., & Wentworth, D. (1992). Serum cholesterol, blood pressure, cigarette smoking, and death from coronary heart disease: Overall findings and differences by age for 3 1 6 , 0 9 9 white men. Archives of Internal Medicine, 152, 56-64. Oliveria, S. Α., Lapuerta, P., McCarthy, B. D., L'Italien, G. J . , Berlowitz, D. R., & Asch, S. M. (2002). Physician-related barriers to the effective management of uncontrolled hypertension. Archives of Internal Medicine, 162, 4 1 3 - 4 2 0 . Oparil, S. (2000). Arterial hypertension. In R. L. Cecil, J . C. Bennett, &C L. Goldman (Eds.), Cecil textbook of medicine (21st ed., pp. 2 5 8 - 2 7 3 ) . Philadelphia: W. B . Saunders. Paffenbarger, R. S., Jung, D. L., Leung, R. W., &C Hyde, R. T. (1991). Physical activity and hypertension: An epidemiologic view. Annals of Medicine, 23, 3 1 9 - 3 2 7 . Parati, G., Pomidossi, G., Albini, F., Malaspina, D., ôc Mancia, G. (1987). Relationship of 24-hour blood pressure mean and variability to severity of 5, 9 3 - 9 8 . target-organ damage in hypertension. Journal of Hypertension, Pate, R. R., Pratt, M., Blair, S. N., Haskell, W. L., Macéra, C. Α., Bouchard, C , Buchner, D., Ettinger, W., Heath, G. W., & King, A. C. (1995). Physical activity and public health: A recommendation from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention and the American College of Sports Medicine. Journal of the American Medical Association, 273, 4 0 2 - 4 0 7 . Pickering, T. (1995). Recommendations for the use of home (self) and ambulatory blood pressure monitoring: American Society of Hypertension Ad Hoc Panel. American Journal of Hypertension, 9, 1 - 1 1 . Pickering, T. G., Coats, Α., Mallion, J . M., Mancia, G., ôc Verdecchia, P. (1999). Blood pressure monitoring: Task Force V—White-coat hypertension. Blood Pressure Monitoring, 4, 3 3 3 - 3 4 1 . Pickering, T. G., James, J . D., Boddie, C , Harshfield, G. Α., Blank, S., & Laragh, J . Η (1988). How common is white coat hypertension? Journal of the American Medical Association, 259, 2 2 5 - 2 2 8 . Potter, J . F., 8c Beevers, D. G. (1984). Pressor effect of alcohol in hypertension. Lancet, 1, 1 1 9 - 1 2 2 . Raikkonen, K., Hautanen, Α., & Keltikangas-Jarvinen, L. (1996). Feelings of exhaustion, emotional distress, and pituitary and adrenocortical hormones in 14, 7 1 3 - 7 1 8 . borderline hypertension. Journal of Hypertension,
\
275
276
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS Rapoport, L., Clark, M., 8c Wardle, J . (2000). Evaluation of a modified cognitivebehavioural programme for weight management. International Journal of Obesity, 24, 1 7 2 6 - 1 7 3 7 . Reisin, E., Frolich, E. D., Messerli, F. H., Dreslinski, G. R., Dunn, F. G., Jones, M. M., & Batson, H. M., Jr. (1983). Cardiovascular changes after weight reduction in obesity hypertension. Annals of Internal Medicine, 98, 3 1 5 - 3 1 9 . Richter-Heinrich, E., Homuth, V., Heinrich, B., et al. (1981). Long term application of behavioral treatments in essential hypertensives. Physiology and Behavior, 26, 9 1 5 - 9 2 0 . Roter, D. L., Hall, J . Α., Mersica, R., Nordstrom, B., Cretin, D., Sc Svarstad, B. (1998). Effectiveness of interventions to improve patient compliance: A metaanalysis. Medical Care, 36, 1 1 3 8 - 1 1 6 1 . Sacks, F. M., Svetkey, L. P., Vollmer, W. M., Appel, L. J . , Bray, G. Α., Harsha, D., Obarzanek, E., Conlin, P. R., Miller, E. R., Ill, Simons-Morton, D. G., Karanja, N., Sc Lin, P. H., for DASH-Sodium Collaborative Research Group. (2001). Effects on blood pressure of reduced dietary sodium and the Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension (DASH) diet: DASH-Sodium Collaborative Research Group. New England Journal of Medicine, 344, 3 - 1 0 . Schneider, R. H., Staggers, F., Alexander, C , Sheppard, W., Rainforth, M., Sc Kodwani, K. (1995). A randomized controlled trial of stress reduction for hypertension in older African-Americans. Hypertension, 26, 8 2 0 - 8 2 7 . Selenta, C , Hogan, Β. E., Sc Linden, W. (2000). How often do office blood pressure measurements fail to identify true hypertension? An exploration of whitecoat normotension. Archives of Family Medicine, 9, 5 3 3 - 5 4 0 . Shea, S., Misra, D., Ehrlich, M . H., Field, L., Sc Francis, C. K. (1992). Predisposing factors for severe, uncontrolled hypertension in an inner-city minority population. New England Journal of Medicine, 327, 7 7 6 - 7 8 1 . Simkin-Silverman, L. R., Wing, R. R., Boraz, Μ . Α., Meilan, Ε. N., Se Kuller, L. H. (1998). Maintenance of cardiovascular risk factor changes among middle-aged women in a lifestyle intervention trial. Women's Health: Research on Gender, Behaviour, and Policy, 4, 2 5 5 - 2 7 1 . Spence, J . D., Barnett, P. Α., Linden, W., Ramsden, V., Sc Taenzer, P. (1999). Lifestyle modifications to prevent and control hypertension: VII. Recommendations on stress management. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 160(9, Suppl.), S46-S50. Stamler, J . (1997). The INTERSALT study: Background, methods, findings, and implications. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 65, S 6 2 6 - S 6 4 2 . Stamler, R., Stamler, J . , Gosch, F. C , Civinelli, J . , Fishman, J . , McKeever, P., McDonald, Α., Sc Dyer, A. R. (1989). Primary prevention of hypertension by nutritional-hygienic means: Final report of a randomized, controlled trial. Journal of the American Medical Association, 262, 1 8 0 1 - 1 8 0 7 . Stevens, V. J . , Corrigan, S. Α., Obarzanek, E., Bernauer, E., Cook, N. R., Hebert, P., Mattfeldt-Beman, M., Oberman, Α., Sugars, C , Sc Dalcin, A. T. (1993). Weight loss intervention in Phase 1 of the Trials of Hypertension Prevention: The TOHP Collaborative Research Group. Archives of Internal Medicine, 153, 8 4 9 - 8 5 8 . Stevens, V. J . , Obarzanek, E., Cook, N. R., Lee, L, Appel, L. J . , West, D. S., Milas, N. C , Mattfeldt-Beman, M., Belden, L., Bragg, C , Millstone, M., Raczynski, J . , Brewer, Α., Singh, B . , Sc Cohen, J . , for Trials for the Hypertension Prevention Research Group. (2001). Long-term weight loss and changes in blood pressure: Results of the Trials of Hypertension Prevention, Phase II. Annals of Internal Medicine, 134, 1 - 1 1 . Stockwell, D. H., Madhaven, S., Cohen, H., Gibson, G., Se Alerman, M. H. (1994). The determinants of hypertension awareness, treatment, and control in an insured population. American Journal of Public Health, 84, 1 7 6 8 - 1 7 7 4 .
Hypertension Stone, N. J . , Sc Kushner, R. (2000). Risk factor modification for cardiac disease: Effects of dietary modification and treatment of obesity—Emphasis on improving vascular outcomes. Medical Clinics of North America, 84(1), 9 5 - 1 2 2 . Thadhani, R., Camargo, C. Α., Stampfer, M . J . , Curhan, G. C., Willett, W. C., Sc Rimm, Ε. B . (2002). Prospective study of moderate alcohol consumption and risk of hypertension in young women. Archives of Internal Medicine, 162, 569-574. Treadway, K. (2000). Management of hypertension. In A. H. Goroll, A. G. Mulley, 8c L. A. May (Eds.), Primary care medicine: Office evaluation and management of the adult patient (4th ed., pp. 1 4 9 - 1 6 4 ) . Philadelphia: Lippincott/Williams Sc Wilkins. Treiber, F. Α., Turner, J . R., Davis, H., Thompson, W., Levy, M., Sc Strong, W. B. (1996). Young children's cardiovascular stress responses predict resting cardiovascular functioning 2 A years later. Journal of Cardiovascular Risk, 3, 95-100. Trials of Hypertension Prevention Collaborative Research Group. (1992). The effects of non-pharmacologic interventions on blood pressure of persons with high normal levels: Results of the Trials of Hypertension Prevention, Phase 1. Journal of the American Medical Association, 267, 1 2 1 3 - 1 2 2 0 . Vander, A. J . , Sherman, J . H., 8c Luciano, D. S. (1994). Human physiology (6th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. Verdecchia, P., Schillaci, G., Borgioni, C , Ciucci, Α., & Porcellati, C. (1997). Prognostic significance of the white coat effect. Hypertension, 29, 1 2 1 8 - 1 2 2 4 . Vollmer, W. M., Sacks, F. M., Ard, J . , Appel, L. J . , Bray, G. Α., Simons-Morton, D. G., Conlin, P. R., Svetkey, L. P., Erlinger, T. P., Moore, T. J . , 8c Karanja, N. (2001). Effects of diet and sodium intake on blood pressure: Subgroup analysis of the DASH-Sodium Trial. Annals of Internal Medicine, 135, 1019-1028. Wadden, T. Α., Berkowitz, R. L, Sarwer, D. B., Prus-Wisniewski, R., 8c Steinberg, C. (2001). Benefits of lifestyle modification in the pharmacologic treatment of obesity: A randomized trial. Archives of Internal Medicine, 161, 2 1 8 - 2 2 7 . Ward, M. M., Swan, G. E., 8c Chesney, M . A. (1987). Arousal reduction treatments for mild hypertension: A meta-analysis of recent studies. In S. Julius 8c D. R. Bassett (Eds.), Behavioral factors in hypertension (pp. 2 8 5 - 3 0 2 ) . Amsterdam: Elsevier North-Holland. Watt, G. C , Harrap, S. B . , Foy, C. J . , Holton, D. W., Edwards, Η. V., Davidson, H. R., Connor, J . M., Lever, A. F., Se Fraser, R. (1992). Abnormalities of glucocorticoid metabolism and the renin-angiotensin system: A four-corners approach to the identification of genetic determinants of blood pressure. Journal of Hypertension, 10, 4 7 3 - 4 8 2 . Weber, Μ . Α., Neutel, J . M., Smith, D. H. G., & Graettinger, W. F. (1994). Diagnosis of mild hypertension by ambulatory blood pressure monitoring. Circulation, 90, 2 2 9 1 - 2 2 9 8 . Weinberger, M . H. ( 1 9 9 6 ) . Salt sensitivity of blood pressure in humans. Hypertension, 27, 4 8 1 - 4 9 0 . Whelton, P. K. (1985). Blood pressure in adults and the elderly. In W. H. Birkenhâger Se J . L. Reid (Eds.), Handbook of hypertension (pp. 5 1 - 6 9 ) . Amsterdam: Elsevier Science. Wilson, J . D., & Foster, D. W. (1992). Williams textbook of endocrinology (8th ed.). Philadelphia: W. B . Saunders. Witteman, J . C , Willett, W. C., Stampfer, M . J . , Colditz, G. Α., Kok, F. J . , Sacks, F. M., Speizer, F. E., Rosner, B., Se Hennekens, C. H. (1990). Relation of moderate alcohol consumption and risk of systemic hypertension in women. American Journal of Cardiology, 65, 6 3 3 - 6 3 7 . l
278
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS Xin, X . , He, J . , Frontini, M . G., Ogden, L. G., Motsamai, O. I., & Whelton, P. K. (2001). Effects of alcohol reduction on blood pressure: A meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Hypertension, 38, 1 1 1 2 - 1 1 1 7 . Yamori, Y . , Matsumoto, M., Yamabe, H., & Okamoto, K. (1969). Augmentation of spontaneous hypertension by chronic stress in rats. Japanese Circulation Journal, 33, 3 9 9 - 4 0 9 . Young, D. R., Appel, L. J . , Jee, S., & Miller, E. R. (1999). The effects of aerobic exercise and t'ai chi on blood pressure in older people: Results of a randomized trial. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 47, 2 7 7 - 2 8 4 .
CHAPTER
Coronary Heart
10 15
Disease
Behavioral Cardiology in Clinical Practice STEVEN M . SCHWARTZ AND MARK W . KETTERER
H
eart disease continues to number
be the
o n e killer o f m e n
and
w o m e n in the United States, where
myocardial infarctions ( M i s ) occur at a rate
(e.g., depression, anger, anxiety) is essential to secondary prevention efforts and is critical for optimal recovery o f function and general health.
o f about 1.5 million per year. F o r about 3 0 %
T h e modern evolution o f behavioral cardi-
of these patients, death is sudden and the
ology arguably began during the m i d - 1 9 7 0 s
first "symptom" they experience. Ischemic
with the work o f Friedman and R o s e n m a n
heart disease or coronary heart disease ( C H D )
( 1 9 7 4 ) on " T y p e A behavior." Since that
is predicted to remain the leading cause o f
time, behavioral scientists and clinicians in
death
collaboration with medical colleagues have
worldwide
through
2020.
Despite
these compelling statistics, the survival rate
considerably
from
to
understanding o f the relationship among psy-
improve, and more than 1 million people
chological, emotional, psychophysiological,
survive acute c o r o n a r y events annually in the
and behavioral factors in the development,
United
maintenance, and
acute c o r o n a r y events continues
States. T h u s , issues pertaining
to
psychological functioning, behavioral risk,
expanded
and
refined
our
exacerbation of C H D .
Given the increasing clinical role o f behav-
and quality o f life are increasingly relevant as
ioral clinicians in managing heart patients,
heart patients face m o r e favorable prospects
this chapter provides (a) a brief review o f
for recovery and
the etiology o f heart disease, (b) a cognitive-
rehabilitation. Assessing
and treating these patients' health risk behav-
behavioral case conceptualization o f C H D ,
iors, such as smoking, obesity, fatty diet, lack
and (c) a pragmatic presentation o f the clini-
of exercise, nonadherence to medical regi-
cal issues for assessing and treating patients
men, social isolation, and emotional status
with coronary artery disease.
279
280
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS
PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL AND BIOMEDICAL ASPECTS OF THE CHD PATIENT
much higher prevalence o f "small" lesions
Atherosclerosis
M i s (absolute number) actually occur at sites
trunk. Also o f note, although large blockages are more likely to produce an infarct, the (less than 5 0 % occlusion) means that most with low-level occlusion.
Atherosclerosis is defined as the buildup o f plaques
Atherosclerosis will follow a "stuttering"
(e.g., "blockages," "stenoses,"
course marked by periods o f growth, stasis,
"lesions," "occlusions") typically composed o f
and (sometimes) regression. Plaques may be
lipids, complex carbohydrates,
and
blood
fibrous and stable, or they may be unstable,
products along artery walls. T h e development
containing a pool o f dead cells and lipids cov-
and
ered by a thin membrane or cap and eccentric
progressive unless the condition is vigorously
o f atherosclerosis is multidetermined
in shape. W h e n this cap ruptures, the contents
treated. By the time a cohort reaches middle
of the pool are thought to provoke thrombus
age, half or more will have some amount o f
formation (i.e., clot), sometimes self-resolving
atherosclerosis. However, some people reach
(but perhaps provoking angina) and some-
advanced ages of 8 0 years or more with little
times evolving into a full blockage o f the
or no clinically significant atherosclerosis.
artery ( M I ) . Fully 8 5 % to 9 0 % o f infarctions
Therefore, although a number of etiological
are believed to be caused by this sequence. T h e
risk factors have been identified, our under-
cause of plaque instability is thought to be
standing o f the etiology remains incomplete.
chronic recurring inflammation, perhaps with
However, most o f the currently accepted risk
some causal contribution from genetic factors
factors (aside from family history, sex, and
and/or infection with Chlamydia
age) are modifiable by medication, changes in
(Ridker, Hennekens, Buring, & Rifai, 2 0 0 0 ;
lifestyle, or both.
Ross, 1 9 9 9 ) .
pneumoniae
W h e n patients have lesions at one site
Precisely what triggers a rupture remains a
(e.g., coronary arteries), it is highly probable
matter o f debate (Allen & Scheldt, 1 9 9 6 ) ,
that they will have lesions at other sites (e.g.,
although it has been hypothesized that elevated
carotid, femoral, or renal arteries). Blockages
emotional states may be one triggering mecha-
that
are large enough
blood
nism secondary to emotion-aggravated con-
supply, and thus to threaten oxygen flow to
tractility of the heart, blood pressure changes,
critical areas (e.g., heart, brain, kidney, legs),
increased blood viscosity, vasoconstriction,
produce
transient
to threaten
ischemia (i.e., reversible
and/or localized vasospasm. Thrombogenesis
oxygen supply/demand deficit) or permanent
is certainly encouraged by various psychobio-
damage secondary to infarction (i.e., tissue
chemicophysical events, including via stress
death due to cessation o f blood
supply).
reactivity pathways. For example, elevated cir-
Large plaques ( 5 0 % or greater blockage) are
culating catecholamines and platelet aggrega-
associated with a higher risk o f infarction.
tion are associated with states o f acute fear
Interestingly, although
large plaques
may
or anger (Markovitz, 1 9 9 8 ) . Heart rates and
threaten adequate blood flow locally, chronic
diastolic and systolic blood pressures rise,
deprivation o f an adequate blood supply to
sometimes precipitously, in response to even
the heart can provoke the development o f col-
mild transient and contrived stressors in the
lateral arteries around the occluded sites as a
laboratory (Goldberg et al., 1 9 9 6 ; Ketterer,
remarkable biological compensatory strategy.
Freedland, et al., 2 0 0 0 ) . Daily life stress is typi-
These collateral arteries then provide natural
cally more meaningful and chronic, provoking
bypasses in the event o f infarction of the main
more intense and prolonged emotional burden
Coronary
Heart
Disease
on patients. This has been measured with in
"discomfort" (e.g., pain, pressure, fullness,
vivo monitoring studies (e.g., Polk, Kamarck,
squeezing), angina sometimes manifests as
& Shiffman, 2 0 0 2 ) .
arm
N o t e that even when accounting for all o f the factors in epidemiological studies, only
pain,
jaw
pain,
lower
back
headache, nausea/vomiting/upset
pain,
stomach,
cognitive confusion, dyspnea (i.e., shortness o f
about half o f cases o f ischemic heart disease
breath), dizziness,
can be explained (Farmer & G o t t o , 1 9 9 7 ) .
and/or
Although most infarctions are probably the
delineated triggers (typically physical exertion
syncope/near
syncope,
weakness. Stable angina has well-
result o f unstable plaque rupture, 2 % to
and sometimes stress) and responds well to
1 0 % o f all infarctions in the human heart
nitrates. Unstable angina is far less predictable
occur in the absence o f any atherosclerosis,
and is often accompanied by severe and pro-
and another 5 % to 1 0 % occur in a part o f
longed pain. T h e presence o f unstable angina
the coronary vascular tree without plaque.
is clinically considered an acute
T h u s , acute thrombus formation can occur
event requiring emergent care because it may
independent
o f plaque rupture. It is n o w
be caused by plaque rupture.
believed that the occurrence o f a M I requires something beyond atherosclerosis.
coronary
Angina is generally presumed
to
result
from ischemia, but this "causal" relationship
Depending on location (which détermines
is less than perfect (e.g., Krantz et al., 1 9 9 4 ) .
the amount o f heart muscle affected by cessa-
Importantly,
tion o f blood flow) and the presence o f collat-
shared by a wide range o f other conditions,
eral circulation and/or anatomical variants in
including acute emotional states, psychiatric
the symptoms
o f angina
are
the coronary vascular tree, the size and signifi-
disorders, and other medical conditions, mak-
cance o f an infarction can vary widely. M i s
ing differential diagnosis difficult at times
considered large will generally decrease the
(Richter, 1 9 9 2 ; Schwartz, Trask, & Ketterer,
heart's pumping function (i.e., ejection fraction
1 9 9 9 ) . M o s t episodes o f ischemia captured
[EF]). A normal E F (i.e., the proportion o f
on Holter monitoring in both laboratory test-
blood ejected from the left ventricle during
ing and in vivo settings are not associated with
systole) will average about 6 6 % in the heart o f
chest pain/pressure or other symptoms, typi-
a healthy adult but includes values above 5 0 % .
cally referred to as silent myocardial ischemia.
strenuous
(A Holter monitor is a small portable device
physical activity, EFs above 4 0 % may be unde-
Unless the
patient
engages
in
that collects electrocardiogram [ECG] data on
tectable by the patient. EFs below 4 0 % define
a continuous basis during daily life.) Further-
heart failure, and this generally affects routine
more, most episodes o f chest pain are not
physical function (i.e., dyspnea, fatigue, and/or
associated with ischemia as measured by sur-
dependent edema). W i t h EFs below 2 0 % , the
face E C G ("atypical" or "noncardiac" chest
patient may be considered for a transplant.
pain). In fact, recurrent emergency r o o m visits by cardiac patients for chest discomfort without evidence o f ischemia/infarction are quite
Angina
common.
Angina pectoris refers to transient chest
Angina is strongly associated with depression/
pain or discomfort resulting from ischemia o f
anxiety, and cognitive-behavioral treatment of
occurs sec-
emotional distress has been found to reduce
ondary to an oxygen supply/demand deficit.
episodes o f angina and ischemia (Blumenthal
Hence,
et al., 1 9 9 7 ; Ketterer, Fitzgerald, et al., 2 0 0 0 ;
heart muscle. T h e discomfort strenuous
physical
activity
that
increases the heart's workload is a c o m m o n
Lewin, 1 9 9 7 ) . Pain intolerance
trigger. M o s t commonly experienced as chest
to
depression/anxiety,
or
secondary
ischemia/angina
282
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS provocation secondary to fear/anger, increases
ventricular
chest discomfort (Ketterer, Fitzgerald, et al.,
fibrillation,
2000;
sudden cardiac death. Note that the ventricular
Ketterer
etal.,
1998;
Schwartz
tachycardia the presumed
and
ventricular
causal culprits in
et al., 1 9 9 9 ) , and chest discomfort provokes
portion of the heart is the primary pumping
treatment seeking and aggressive diagnostic/
chamber for the brain and general body. There
treatment decisions by physicians. Thus, if
are a number of factors (e.g., drugs, disease,
not directly affecting disease
progression,
infarction, emotional distress) that can produce
emotional distress will still affect quality o f life,
abnormal rhythms, many o f which can be seri-
illness behavior, and disease management in
ous or even life threatening. In severe instances,
adverse ways.
blood flow may slow sufficiently to cause acute
Because angina is the major driving force
brain failure (syncope). When the heart has been
behind medical system use, treatment of emo-
thrown into inefficient sequencing o f its con-
tional distress may reduce this use (Black,
tractions, cardioversion (transthoracic electrical
Allison, Williams, Rummans, 8c Gau, 1 9 9 8 ;
shock) is used to achieve a normal sinus rhythm.
Davidson, 2 0 0 0 ; Frasure-Smith & Lesperance, 1 9 9 8 ) . Recently, Schwartz and
colleagues
Patients at high risk for future life-threatening arrhythmias will likely receive antiar-
( 1 9 9 9 ) proposed a spectrum approach to chest
rhythmic medications that regulate heartbeat,
pain management (e.g., pain and palpitations)
pacemakers, and (in severe cases) implantable
that
cardioverter defibrillators. This latter device,
includes presentations
consistent
with
pain/anxiety, Syndrome X/nonischemic chest
like the pacemaker, is placed inside the body
pain, Prinzmetal's angina, and angina pectoris.
and can function as an " o n b o a r d " crash cart
Because psychological and behavioral prob-
that greatly improves both sudden cardiac
lems are similar in angina and other forms of
death and
chest discomfort regardless o f the underlying
medication management alone (Gilkson &
cause, disease management strategies should
Friedman, 2 0 0 1 ) . However, there is growing
all-cause mortality relative
to
be multidisciplinary. T h e rise of "chest pain"
evidence that the device itself can produce
clinics for nonemergent chest pain attests to
problems o f behavioral or psychological mal-
greater recognition of this overlapping
and
complex problem.
adjustment
in a significant subgroup o f
patients (for an excellent review, see Sears, T o d a r o , Lewis, Sotile, & Conti, 1 9 9 9 ) . Behavioral clinicians are particularly famil-
Arrhythmias
iar with the sinus tachycardias
The heart contracts and relaxes in a highly
most character-
istic o f panic attacks and other acute anxiety
orchestrated fashion or rhythm to efficiently
and mood states. Importantly, clinicians must
move blood from the venous
compartment
be aware that patients can and do develop anx-
(deoxygenated blood) to the arterial compart-
ious presentations from more dangerous heart
ment (oxygenated blood). This is a two-phase
rhythms as well (Schwartz et al., 2 0 0 2 ) . Also,
process in which the heart fills during the
there is considerable evidence that the subjec-
relaxation phase (diastole) and then the heart
tive symptom of palpitation correlates poorly
forcefully empties during the contraction phase
with underlying cardiac rhythm (Barsky, 2 0 0 1 ;
(systole). This orchestration is electrochemical in
Barsky, Cleary, Barnett, Christiansen,
nature and is maintained by a specialized cluster
Ruskin, 1 9 9 4 ) , as was described previously for
8c
of cells collectively called the sinoatrial node or
chest pain. Although it is generally acknowl-
natural pacemaker. Disruption o f this sequenc-
edged that panic spectrum disorders can and
ing is referred to as an arrhythmia. Arrhythmias
do masquerade
can range from
reverse is also true. T o illustrate the complexity
benign to malignant. O f
particular clinical significance are
sustained
among
as cardiac problems,
anxiety, palpitations,
and
the
cardiac
Coronary rhythm,
Lessmeier and colleagues
(1997)
permanent
tissue
Heart Disease
death
|
("infarction").
found that 6 7 % o f patients presenting for
Likewise, certain patterns on the E C G (ST seg-
treatment
ment
o f paroxysmal
supraventricular
depression)
reflect ischemia or infarc-
tachycardia (PSVT) met criteria for panic and
tion. Table 1 5 . 1 presents some o f the c o m m o n
that P S V T went unrecognized in 5 5 % o f the
cardiac diagnostic and treatment procedures.
patients even after an initial medical evaluation. Equally striking, 8 6 % of patients displayed resolution o f anxiety symptoms with antiarrhythmic therapy.
Cognitive-Behavioral Case Conceptualization of CHD Understanding the factors that contribute
Hemodynamics
to both acute and chronic aspects o f C H D
This chapter has already alluded to the
would be incomplete without consideration o f
notion that the heart can develop compen-
the psychological, emotional, and behavioral
satory strategies in the presence o f heart
characteristics o f the patient or the patient
dysfunction/disease
(e.g., development
of
population. T h e clinical study o f these unique
bypass).
risk factors and their modification is at the
Hemodynamics is the study o f the forces
very "heart" o f behavioral cardiology. Given
involved in the movement of blood through
what is currently supported by the evidentiary
collateral
arteries
as
a
natural
the circulatory system. It is through the study
literature, a general cognitive-behavioral con-
of these processes that one can understand the
ceptualization o f cardiovascular disease devel-
physical and functional changes that occur in
opment and rehabilitation can be constructed
the diseased heart as it attempts to maintain
on the following assumptions:
homeostasis in the body. A simplified version of the process is as follows. As the E F o f the heart declines in heart failure, or with infarctrelated heart muscle damage, the heart begins to struggle to push blood out into the body, creating a variety o f physical changes in the heart's structure
over time. This process,
referred to as remodeling,
can be characterized
by heart enlargement, thickening o f the heart walls, and leaking heart valves that regurgitate blood as the heart becomes more inefficient. This inefficiency is manifest in wall motion abnormalities. W a l l m o t i o n
abnormalities
can be visualized by blood pool radionuclide imaging (technetium sestamibi) or on echocardiogram. Localized perfusion defects can be visualized by uptake o f radioactively labeled glucose (technetium pyrophosphate
or thal-
lium). This type o f diagnostic testing is thought to reflect impairment o f blood flow to an area of heart muscle. This impairment reversible when
the patient
is not
can be being
stressed, or it is "fixed" (present when the patient is both at rest and stressed), implying
1. Many of the biological processes that underlie cardiovascular disease develop over time out of health risk behaviors and lifestyles. These risk factors for CHD are relevant to disease development, maintenance, and exacerbation and also contribute to related and complicating conditions such as emphysema, type 2 diabetes, pulmonary hypertension, and heart failure. 2. Lifestyle and behavioral risks are generally immediately and highly gratifying. The powerful reward value of these risk behaviors causes them to become habitualized as a consequence of frequent and persistent practice, thereby making them particularly resistant to intervention. For example, a pack-a-day smoker receives 7 0 , 0 0 0 infusions of nicotine during the course of a year; that is a number of "hits" unparalleled by any other recreational drug. 3. The negative, debilitating health consequences of these behaviors typically develop slowly over time, allowing the person to accommodate to many of the
283
284
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS Table 15.1
Common Cardiac Diagnostic and Treatment Procedures
ECG:
Monitors the electrical activity in the heart to past or present heart attack, the location of the heart affected, and the rhythm of the heart; can be used with or without exercise (treadmill testing) and in vivo with Holter monitoring (continuous monitoring) or event monitoring (event-specific recordings)
Echocardiogram:
Ultrasound evaluation of the heart allowing for an working heart to be captured, including chambers, muscle walls; can also be done in conjunction with medication (e.g., Dobutamine) that increases heart
Cardiac catheterization:
Invasive procedure that is performed by inserting a catheter (tube-like device) into an artery (typically in the groin) and advancing through the arterial system into the heart, with the catheter able to measure pressures inside the heart chambers and, when equipped with a camera, able to take video of the heart in action
Percutaneous transluminal coronary angioplasty (PTCA)/ directional coronary atherectomy (DCA):
Advancing a catheter up to a blockage in a critical artery and reducing the occlusion by expanding a small balloon or using a plaque-cutting device, with stents (stainless steel mesh tubes) sometimes being put into place to prevent re-occlusion
Coronary artery bypass graft:
Surgical procedure that takes blood vessels from another part of the body (typically in the legs or chest) and then stitches them around the blocked coronary artery
symptoms of physiological degradation and reduced function. 4. The individual's cognitive and emotional status mediates lifestyle risk factors and also contributes more directly to disease development, maintenance, and exacerbation through the autonomic nervous system's stress reactivity pathways associated with fight/flight and psychoneuroimmunological pathways. 5. The biological, psychological, and behavioral factors noted previously, as well as environmental factors, influence one another in a bidirectional or reciprocal fashion.
imaging of the valves, and heart exercise or use of a rate
requires the use o f an objective, psychometrically sound questionnaire to aid clinical staff in recognizing distress. There are a number o f well-validated self-report measures
for
assessing C H D patients, and most focus on emotional status, functionality, and/or quality o f life. T a b l e 1 5 . 2 lists some o f the c o m monly
used
tools
and
the
underlying
constructs they purport to measure. Any o f the psychometric instruments validated as prospective predictors o f cardiac outcomes could be used, but they are clearly redundant in terms o f their predictive utility (Ketterer et al., 2 0 0 2 ) . Because o f brevity, nonstigma-
ASSESSMENT OF T H E CHD PATIENT
Screening Identification and triaging o f emotionally distressed patients from cardiac settings often
tizing content, and ease o f administration and
scoring
(many
become frustrated
CHD
with
patients
lengthy
will
question-
naires or test batteries), the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale ( H A D S ) . with a cutoff o f 13 or greater, is recommended (Herrman, 1 9 9 7 ) . T h e chapter
authors
suspect
that
Coronary Table 15.2
Heart Disease
Self-Report Measures for the Cardiac Patient
Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale Symptom Checklist-90-Revised (Brief Symptom Inventory) Beck Depression Inventory State-Trait Anxiety Inventory Crown-Crisp Cook-Medly Hostility Scale Ketterer Stress Symptom Frequency Checklist Millon Behavioral Health Inventory Toronto Alexithymia Scale SF36 Seattle Angina Questionnaire Minnesota Living With Heart Failure Questionnaire Cardiac Anxiety Questionnaire
screening will eventually require an instrument
model (Goldfried & Sprafkin, 1 9 7 6 ) is one o f
designed for completion by a significant
several c o m m o n models and is used here for
other so as to circumvent denial (Ketterer
illustration purposes.
et al., 1 9 9 6 , 1 9 9 8 , 2 0 0 2 ) , but this remains to be proven in prospective studies.
Clinical Interviewing (Functional Analysis) Evaluation should, as always, include a thorough psychosocial history, mental status e x a m , and review o f cardiovascular risk factors
(Table
1 5 . 3 lists the domains
to
consider in an initial evaluation). Importantly, this evaluation should include identification of barriers to change as well as cardiovascular risk factors. Behavioral assessment models have long been used in the functional analysis of problem behaviors (Barrios, 1 9 8 8 ) , including illness behavior (Schwartz, Gramling, & Mancini,
1994).
Because the
modifiable
risk factors for C H D are deeply embedded in the psychology and behavior o f the patient, approaching
The Stimulus (patient triggers or hot buttons). T h e stimulus portion o f the S O R C model refers to the triggers o r cues that evoke health risk behavior. Stimulus control variables relate to cardiovascular risk in a number o f ways. Overt behaviors such as s u b s t a n c e a b u s e (e.g., n i c o t i n e , a l c o h o l , cocaine), lack o f exercise, and acute emotional reactions can have a broad range o f environmental and cognitive triggers. F o r example, it is not unusual for the mere presence o f a critical supervisor to trigger acute autonomic arousal and c o n c o m i t a n t emotional distress (e.g., anxiety, anger, irritation, w o r r y ) , even when there is no current overt conflict. Therefore, it is imperative to identify the patient's triggers (or buttons), noting that patients m a y vary greatly in their understanding o f or insight into the functional relationships between triggers and emotional or behavioral responses.
the initial assessment o f the
patient can be greatly facilitated by using a
Organismic
Variables.
Organismic variables
functional-analytic framework. T h e Stimulus-
are the dispositional characteristics of the patient
Organismic-Response-Consequence ( S O R C )
(e.g., genetic, physiological, temperamental,
\
285
286
J BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS Table 15.3
An Evaluation Checklist for CHD Patients
Frequency and triggers for chest pain Disease-specific worries (often grief over disease-imposed role changes or limitations) Disease-specific knowledge (what the patient knows about his or her condition and recovery) Physical activity level (including activities such as walking and gardening) Smoking (e.g., number of cigarettes per day, smoking history in years) Eating habits (e.g., high-risk foods with high fat content, salt, sugar; eating behaviors such as binging, rapid eating, stress/emotional eating) Alcohol intake (e.g., number of drinks per day/week, drinking history, CAGE questions) Interpersonal conflicts (particularly focusing on work and marital relationships) Job stress (e.g., deadlines, responsibilities) Sleep habits (e.g., apnea, insomnia, restless leg syndrome) Emotional status (e.g., depression, grief, anxiety, anger/hostility, alexithymia) Comorbid medical conditions (e.g., diabetes, heart failure, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease)
experiential) that are peculiar to the individual.
change. Understanding the ways in which these
The familial aspects of cardiovascular disease
behaviors are reinforced or maintained is criti-
reflect both genetic factors and early learning
cal to the secondary prevention process. Timing
experiences. O f particular interest may be the
is particularly important in health risk behav-
early modeling of cardiovascular risk behaviors
ior. As a general rule, temporal proximity of
such as smoking, angry acting out, and a mal-
behavior and consequence will wield greater
adaptive
and/or
influence over the behavior. As noted previ-
achievement at the expense of other important
ously, many risk behaviors provide immediate
aspects of development.
gratification. In particular, it is important to
emphasis on competition
view the C H D patient's actions as maintained The Response.
T h e response describes the
by relief from negative affective states.
target risk behavior itself (e.g., eating a fatty meal). T h e response can also be operationalized as a nonbehavior (i.e., failure to act) such
Emotional Status
as an inability or unwillingness to engage in
There are several emotional domains that
regular exercise. In addition, behavioral risk
have consistently proven themselves to have
includes not only overt acts or failures to
robust relationships to cardiac outcome and/or
act but also covert behaviors such as certain
are relevant to individual work with the C H D
cognitions. T a b l e 1 5 . 4 lists some o f the c o m -
patient (for an excellent review, see Rozanski,
mon maladaptive cognitions to assess in the
Blumenthal, &c Kaplan, 1 9 9 9 ) . Assessment of depression/dysphoria,
C H D patient.
anger/hostility, stress,
anxiety, and alexithymia is essential in a thorThere are several factors that
ough evaluation of the C H D patient. It is
affect the power o f a consequence to shape
important to note that many patients may pre-
behavior. M a n y of the risk behaviors seen
sent with subclinical levels o f emotional distress
Consequences.
in the C H D patient are highly overlearned,
(relative to cutoffs for traditional psychiatric
habitualized, and/or otherwise resistant
patients), and these levels may still be clinically
to
Coronary Table 15.4
Heart Disease
\
Common Cognitive Errors in CHD Patients
Chronic and excessive worry or anger about uncontrollable events (e.g., "catastrophizing" finances of retirement, allowing kids to sink or swim, confronting boss) is a chronic stressor for many patients. In many cases, patients have difficulty in discriminating the degree of control present in any stressful situation. Inaccurate understanding of "normal" is common, particularly among Type A or assertivenessimpaired patients. Unrealistic expectations regarding their own behavior or others' behavior can cause chronic aggravation or feelings of being unloved (commonly accompanied by the "shoulds" or a set of rigidly held obligations or responsibilities). "Anyone would react as I did" is probably a variant of the misunderstanding of "normal" and can be assessed using the "Rule of 1 0 0 " test." Malevolent attributions are present in many cases. Often, patients will make attributions regarding the motives and intentions of others as malevolent (e.g., a car cuts them off on the highway, thereby provoking rage because the other driver was "obviously" stupid, irresponsible, and/or dangerous). Surreptitious arrogance in interpersonal interactions is often characterized by overt politeness but dismissal of others' desires/opinions and devaluation of their feelings. Inability to trust is frequently present and may have its origins in malevolent attributions or deeper insecurities that patients might not acknowledge or even be aware of fully. Catastrophizing lateness!"unproductive" time is part of the time urgency characteristic of the Type A personality. Often, patients will have a profound intolerance for "downtime." Various forms of selfpacing exercises requiring the patient to go slower are helpful. Confusing "right" with "harmful" might also be called the "just world hypothesis," in which patients will get very angry over perceived injustices, slights, or inequities. They believe that if they can "prove" their case, the world would have to come into compliance. In fact, it often does not matter whether one is right or not, and anger will still kill. Is this worth dying for? Importance of "things" over persons, that is, what is regretted on deathbeds is time not spent with family/friends rather than time not spent at the office. a. The clinician should a s k himself or herself, "If 100 people were in this situation, what proportion would react in this w a y ? " Thus, the clinician's sense of normative behavior can be used to gauge how "normal" the patient's reaction is.
significant, related to important clinical out-
Obtaining two or three adjectives for affect,
comes, and worthy o f treatment even if they do
and then obtaining concrete examples where
not
and
these imply chronic conflict, for each significant
fourth
other (e.g., parent, sibling, spouse/lover, child,
meet
Statistical edition
formal Manual
DSM
of Mental
(Diagnostic Disorders,
[American Psychiatric Association,
boss) facilitates efforts toward mapping the
1 9 9 4 ] ) diagnostic criteria. In many cases,
patient's psychosocial stressors. This can also
patients will "somatize" their distress (i.e.,
provide some clues as to how the patient
report only the physical symptoms and deny or
construes and responds to others and as to the
minimize the affective/cognitive symptoms).
success o f these coping patterns. O f particular
Therefore, emphasizing the concrete benefits of
importance are overlearned/automatized coping
improved sleep or energy, or o f reduced chest
responses (which are presumably learned early
pain, can help the patient to accept treatment.
in life and are often outside the
patient's
287
288
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS awareness) and whether they succeed at merely
care regardless o f the impact on CHD-specific
reducing distress but not stress (maladaptive)
morbidity and mortality.
or at reducing both distress and stress (adaptive). Testing the patient's history against the
Anger.
The
work
of
Friedman
and
"Rule of Three" may be a useful exercise. If the
Rosenman ( 1 9 7 4 ) on the Type A personality
patient has made the same mistake three times
identified a number o f characteristics that
or more (e.g., getting fired, marrying an abusive
tended to cluster together and appeared to be
alcoholic), it is highly likely that this is a deeply
associated with cardiovascular
ingrained pattern that the patient does not
Apart
perceive regarding himself or herself. It should
constant effort, these characteristics include
become a goal o f treatment to raise the patient's
cynicism/mistrust,
from
ambition,
prognosis.
"busyness,"
and
stubbornness/opinionated,
awareness of this pattern, define a face-saving
perfectionism/demanding, alexithymia, control-
way o f construing the pattern, and then develop
ling/overcontrolled, impatient, and
more
obsessional. Importantly, not one of these
adaptive
alternative
responses.
For
worrier/
example, instead of framing the problem as
characteristics c a n
"I'm an angry guy," it may be more acceptable
gnomonic o f the syndrome, nor does any
be argued
as
patho-
and constructive to frame it as " I care too much
appear to have a direct effect on the genesis or
and have too high a set o f expectations, and
exacerbation o f C H D . Rather, these charac-
therefore I get frustrated a lot."
teristics are o f importance because they tend to foster a hostility complex or " A I A I " (aggra-
Depression.
There are now numerous pub-
vation, irritation, anger, and impatience).
lished reports linking depression and depres-
Although the literature has been marred by
sive symptoms to adverse cardiac outcomes
inconsistent operational definitions, psycho-
(e.g., B o o t h - K e w l e y &
metric problems with self-report measures,
Friedman, 1 9 8 7 ; Jaffe,
and a more complex relationship between the
1 9 9 5 ; Cassem & Hackett, 1 9 7 3 ; Frasure-
AIAI construct and C H D than was originally
Carney, Freedland, Eisen, Rich, &
Smith, Lesperance, & Talajic, 1 9 9 3 ) . Based on
conceived, it nevertheless remains prudent to
this evidence, depression should n o w be con-
assess the patient's ability to modulate anger.
sidered an established independent risk factor
AIAI has been proved to be a causal factor for
for C H D outcomes (Glassman & Shapiro,
C H D in a randomly assigned, controlled clin-
1 9 9 8 ) . Unfortunately, there are not yet any
ical trial o f cognitive/behavioral stress man-
randomized
agement that targeted AIAI and
controlled trials o f sufficient
reduced
methodological rigor to demonstrate a reduc-
death/Ml rates by 3 7 % relative to placebo
tion in C H D morbidity or mortality as a
controls (Dusseldorp, van Elderen, M a e s ,
result o f treating depression either psycho-
M e u l m a n , & Kraaij, 1 9 9 9 ; Friedman et al.,
therapeutically or psychopharmacologically.
1986, 1987).
However, S A D H A R T (Zoloft vs. placebo)
A related issue concerns
and E N R I C H D (stress management plus a
avoidance,
confrontation
where conflict with either family
selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor [SSRI]
members or coworkers often leads to "stew-
vs. standard care) are ongoing clinical trials
ing" (anger) or chronic anxiety. This has been
targeting depression in C H D . W h a t can be
conceptualized as the "anger in/anger out"
said with some certainty is that CHD/depres-
issue. This can occur for a variety o f reasons,
sion comorbidity does affect patient function-
including the Type A w h o despises but will
ality, compliance, and quality of life such that
not discuss conflicts with his or her boss and
assessment and aggressive treatment where
the Type A's spouse w h o is afraid o f express-
indicated is an essential part o f comprehensive
ing his or her disagreement over even trivial
Coronary
Heart Disease
\
matters due to the consequent verbal assault
from traditional psychological intervention,
(e.g., "that's so stupid") or cold shoulder.
although they are typically good at complying
O n e c o m m o n example seen is the cardiac
with behavioral assignments.
patient w h o has living with him or her a late adolescent w h o is not working, going to school, or contributing to the workload at home and who may be abusing drugs or even being verbally abusive with the parents.
Substance Use Assessment o f substance use, particularly tobacco products, is particularly important in any evaluation. Smoking is a powerful predic-
T h e data supporting a relationship
tor for many diseases, including C H D . If the
between anxiety and C H D is perhaps more
patient is an active or recent smoker, immedi-
Anxiety.
complex and less compelling than those sup-
ate referral for Zyban or an SSRI and possible
porting depression. As stated previously, acute
nicotine replacement should
anxiety might play an acute role in plaque rup-
(Edwards, Murphy, Downs, Ackerman,
tures,
Rosenthal, 1 9 8 9 ; Ketterer, Pickering, Stoever,
arrhythmias,
and
blood
pressure.
be facilitated &
Anxiety has been implicated as a major risk
& Wansley, 1 9 8 7 ; Murphy, Edwards, Downs,
factor for C H D in multiple prospective risk
Ackerman, &c Rosenthal, 1 9 9 0 ) . Smoking is
factor studies, particularly for death or sudden
such a potent predictor o f outcomes that hesi-
death (Kamarck &c Jennings, 1 9 9 1 ; Ketterer,
tation is not prudent unless the patient insists
&
on thinking it over. T h e patient should be
Sparrow, 1 9 9 8 ) . Unfortunately, no large-scale
taught techniques to avoid smoking cues (e.g.,
randomized control trials exist, nor are any
bars, sitting around after meals) and to disrupt
under way, to examine treatment in this popu-
smoking habits (e.g., smoke with the opposite
1999;
Kubzansky,
Kawachi,
Weiss,
lation and cardiac outcomes. It should be
hand, change brands, smoke menthol), and
noted that acute digoxin toxicity (e.g., tremor,
should be taught relaxation procedures to help
loss of appetite, confusion) may mimic anxiety.
control acute urges prior to setting a contracted quit date (preferably during a psy-
Alexitbymia.
Many
CHD
patients
are
c h o s o c i a l ^ quiescent period). Giving
the
found to display some degree o f alexithymia,
patient 24-hour-per-day access to the clinician
which refers to difficulty in identifying and/or
during the first month o f cessation is a privilege
labeling emotional states in self and others.
that will rarely be used or abused, and it seems
Patients with this disorder
to assist some patients in managing
will display
a
poverty o f vocabulary in reporting their feelings and describing other people's personalities, and
they will typically n o t
explore
their
impulses. Although not a common problem in this population, chemical dependency (e.g., cocaine,
psychosocial circumstances as possible causes
alcohol) should be ruled out. It is a futile exer-
of their problems. T h e y will present as emo-
cise to try to treat emotional distress in some-
tionally concrete (as opposed to metaphorical)
one whose central nervous system is under
in talking about what appear to be emotion-
assault by fluctuating levels o f psychoactive
ally charged situations. W h e n exploring their
substances. Detoxification may be necessary
psychosocial circumstances, it is necessary to
before further treatment can proceed. Note that
gently probe for concrete event/behavioral
prolonged alcohol abuse can result in car-
details because these patients rarely volunteer
diomyopathy.
Prevalence rates for cocaine
information spontaneously because they fre-
abuse in C H D do not exist, but it is clear that
quently view it as irrelevant. Alexithymia can
chronic cocaine use, particularly when smoking
make it difficult for many patients to benefit
is the route o f administration, can lead to early
289
290
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS heart disease. Because cocaine can provoke
early-stage,
coronary vasoconstriction, chest pain, and M I
manifesting on magnetic resonance imaging/
as well as depression, it is important to consider
computerized
this in the history of the patient. Cocaine use is
"periventricular leukomalacia" or "ischemic
probably the most common single factor pre-
white matter changes") is elevated. Although
cipitating M I in young people (e.g., under age
the patient may still be highly functional, irri-
4 0 years), so a drug screen may help to account
tability/frustration over subtle memory-related
for early C H D .
multi-infarct tomography
dementia
(often
[MRI/CT]
as
problems may be an aggravating factor if he or she externalizes causality. Ruling out alternative, potentially arrestable causes (e.g., a
Sleep Apnea Sleep apnea
thyroid-stimulating hormone for thyroid probis a periodic cessation o f
lems, a venereal disease research laboratory or
breathing during sleep that is an underappre-
a rapid plasma regain test for neurosyphilis) is
ciated comorbidity in C H D . Clinicians should
important, but such causes are only rarely
suspect sleep apnea when there are signs o f
found. Patients often fear a large cerebrovascu-
persistent nightly snoring, bed partner c o m -
lar accident more than they fear a heart attack.
plaints or descriptions o f choking/gasping/
Because prophylaxis o f cerebrovascular acci-
cessation o f breathing, and/or chronic fatigue
dents is the same as the regimen followed for
("excessive daytime
M i s , patients can be reassured that all reason-
sleepiness"). Patients
may also manifest subtle signs of cognitive
able steps are being taken, and their concern
dysfunction or m o o d symptoms. Suspected
can be used to further motivate "taking care of
apnea should result in referral for evaluation
themselves."
to a sleep center. It is not clear whether treatment
o f sleep apnea
lowers the risk o f
Ml/death, but this seems likely to be the case (Ketterer, Brymer, Rhoads, Kraft, & Kenyon,
The Importance of Collateral Clinical Data
1 9 9 4 ) . Because many apneics develop a reac-
Because o f the high rate o f denial/mini-
tive depression due to self-attributions regard-
mization/alexithymia present in this patient
ing their "laziness," treatment o f sleep apnea
population, the clinician should be somewhat
can improve m o o d (Dahlof, Ejnell, Hallstrom,
skeptical about the patient's version o f his or
& Hdner, 2 0 0 0 ) .
her life and circumstances. Although it is important
"holes" in narration (e.g., " G e e , I don't k n o w
O f particular importance in this population, particularly given advanced age, are consideration o f whether the patient displays any cogni(often subtle and
patient
attributes meaning to events, psychological
Cognitive Dysfunction
tive impairment
to understand h o w the
why she left m e " ) should be seen as important and indicative o f a chronic coping problem. Significant others should always be used as
in the
collateral sources of information when evaluat-
absence of waxing/waning arousal and atten-
ing a patient or tracking response to treatment.
tion). T h e use o f a cognitive screening tool or
Sometimes, this can be accomplished by simply
procedure
asking the patient,
(e.g., month,
year, three
items
immediate and delayed, counting backward) at
" H o w does your wife
[husband] think you are doing?" Other times, it
initial evaluation is important. Because of the
is necessary to obtain independent contact with
patient's vascular disease and/or arrhythmias
the significant other because o f the patient's
(particularly those patients w h o have atrial
denial/alexithymia or other lack of insight.
fibrillation), the likelihood of mild-moderate,
Often, it is necessary to obtain this data out of
Coronary
Heart
Disease
the patient's hearing range given that many
knows you well" to complete and return
spouses report "walking on eggshells" so as not
independently by mail before
discussions with
to provoke the patient's ire. One prudent tactic
the patients. Although not yet documented, it
is to simply tell the patient that "it's always
will be interesting to see whether this relation-
helpful to have another person's perspective on
ship holds true for female patient/male signifi-
how you're doing." At a later point in time, the
cant other dyads.
clinician can encourage the patient to use an appropriate significant other as a "monitor" under the mutual contract that the spouse should not be punished for providing accurate but unwelcome feedback. As an adjunct to interviewing significant others, the Spouse/Friend Ketterer Stress Symptom
Frequency
PRACTICE
Stress and Coping Framework
(KSSFC)
Stress is a concept that most people, includ-
(Ketterer et al., 1 9 9 6 , 1 9 9 8 , 2 0 0 2 ) is recom-
ing those with C H D , readily accept and for
mended for obtaining a significant other's
which they have some intuitive understanding.
assessments o f the patient's emotional status.
Framing any psychological or behavioral prob-
T h e K S S F C is unique in this feature o f allow-
lems within a diathesis stress model rather than
ing both patient self-ratings and ratings by sig-
using more traditional or formal psychiatric
nificant others. T h e spouse/friend K S S F C can
nomenclature (e.g., "crankiness" rather than
be administered by sending it home with min-
"anger," feeling "down" rather than feeling
imal written
Checklist
T R E A T M E N T AND CLINICAL
stamped
"depressed," "worried" or "stressed" rather
addressed envelope to "someone who knows
instructions and
a
than "anxious") will typically provoke the
you well." T h e patient and significant other
least resistance and help to facilitate a thera-
should be encouraged to not discuss the ques-
peutic alliance. Clinicians working within a
tionnaire until after it has been mailed back. If
traditional psychiatric context and looking to
the clinician is seeking direct input from a
develop cardiac referrals will face resistance
spouse/significant other on the patient's rou-
from patients even before their first visits.
tine emotional functioning, it is critical that
Ideally, treatment should be done as part o f a
the conversation occur without the patient
multidisciplinary cardiac team or consultation
being present. It has been demonstrated that
service rather than removed in a separate psy-
spouse/friend-reported
depression using the
chiatric setting to maximize destigmatization.
K S S F C is a stronger correlate of coronary
Table 1 5 . 5 summarizes some of the treatment
artery disease severity (by angiography) than
studies focusing on stress reduction.
is self-report and that denial of depression
Here the transactional model of Lazarus
(spouse/friend minus self-ratings) is an even
and Folkman ( 1 9 8 4 ) works nicely. In the most
1996).
general sense, coping efforts can be presented
stronger correlate (Ketterer et al.,
Furthermore, spouse/friend ratings (and not
to patients as taking one o f two tacks: prob-
self-ratings) of anxiety were the only predictor
lem-focused coping or emotion-focused cop-
of angina at 5-year follow-up, and it was
ing. For heuristic purposes, these two coping
shown that denial o f distress (particularly
efforts can be discussed as if they are separate,
AIAI) is a very strong predictor o f mortality
independent coping methods, whereas in vivo
(Ketterer et al., 1 9 9 8 ) . F o r this reason, the
they most often interact in dynamic fashion
chapter authors routinely send their male
and over time the patient will need to integrate
patients home with a questionnaire
about
them more effectively. Problem-focused cop-
themselves to be given to "someone who
ing, or efforts aimed at defining the problem,
292
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS Table 15.5
Benefits of Treating "Stress" in Ischemic Heart Disease
Proven (i.e., at least one intervention study published) Improved quality of life Decreased myocardial infarction and death Reduction of ambulatory ischemia Decreased smoking relapse Decreased chest pain/pressure
Dusseldorp et al. (1999) Dusseldorp et al. (1999); Ketterer (1993) Blumenthal et al. (1997); Dusseldorp et al. (1999); Blumenthal et al. (1997); Ketterer, Fitzgerald, et Dusseldorp et al. (1999); Chambers (1994)
Reduction of moderate-level hypertension Probable (i.e., demonstrated to correlate with stress) Greater compliance to medications Greater compliance to exercise recommendations Greater compliance to glucose control Decreased medical system use
Friedman et al. (1996) Hall et al. (1994, 1998) Gallacher et al. (1997); al. (2000); Lewin (1997) Linden and
Carney et al. (1995) Dusseldorp et al. (1999); Stetson et al. (1997) Cox and Gonder-Frederik (1992) Allison et al. (1995); Black et al. (1998); Frasure-Smith & Lesperance (1998)
Possible (i.e., mechanistically plausible) Decreased infection/ulcération/ destabilization of plaque by Chlamydia pneumoniae} Decreased need for revascularization secondary to decreased chest pain Decreased treatment-seeking delay Increased heart rate variability
its solution, and implementation o f instrumen-
tolerate "downtime," and substance use are
tal actions designed to bring about resolution,
indicative o f poor emotion-focused coping.
is often highly developed in C H D patients.
It is usually useful to teach some form of
skills,
relaxation to new patients. Acute reductions in
Despite their good problem-solving
C H D patients may display poor discrimination
heart rate and blood pressure, and the subjec-
skills in situations where problem solving is
tive benefits (e.g., calming, reduced
likely to be ineffective. Consequently, they may
reduced chest pain, reduced headache, easier
worry,
manifest an inability to profit from experience
sleep onset) that accompany these exercises,
(e.g., banging their heads against a brick wall)
provide an immediately perceived sense of con-
conse-
trol over previously "uncontrollable" events
quences. Repeated failures may lead to AIAI.
and so reinforce instructions regarding stress
in apparent defiance o f punishing Consequently, feeling trapped in
stressful
circumstances is a common experience. Reliance on problem-focused coping strate-
and C H D . T h e chapter authors use such a procedure prior to all group sessions because it also seems to foster psychological mindedness,
gies often comes at the expense o f good, con-
permitting possible alteration of pathogenic
structive, emotion-focused coping skills. Issues
cognitive attributions. Helping patients to mas-
such as poor self-pacing, inability to relax or
ter abbreviated relaxation methods, such as a
Coronary
Heart
Disease
"cleansing breath," is important in the transfer
patients, while readily labeling others, seem to
of relaxation skills to in vivo stressors.
have lived unexamined lives. This means that
The core goal o f stress management in coro-
the uniqueness (and maladaptiveness) of one's
nary artery disease patients is to reduce the
own personality often is not part of the way in
frequency/intensity/duration
(or "density") o f
which the patient attributes meaning to events.
emotional distress (or "stress"). T h e clinician
Once the clinician has a firm grasp on the
should consider whether the patient's psy-
patient's habitual ways o f thinking/responding,
stressful
it is critical to find a positive (or humorous) or
(i.e., triggers) and should identify one chronic
pragmatic way in which to frame this trait.
stressor (or a few) and use ongoing life events
Thus, an obsessional person can be described
to alter habitual cognitions/behaviors ("cop-
as "thorough" or "careful," or he or she can
chosocial environment is unusually
ing"). An alcoholic spouse, a demanding or
be described as "picky." T h e former will gen-
abusive boss, and kids w h o are abusing drugs
erally be received by the patient as a positive
and sponging off the patient all require a dif-
trait (perhaps carried too far at times), whereas
ferent nexus of the intervention. For example,
the latter will evoke defensiveness. Once rap-
the chapter authors have seen a half dozen
port is established, teasing/humor can be a
cases where getting an angry spouse on an
powerful tool for getting the patient to cast
SSRI resolved a patient's chest pain.
himself or herself in a new light and for diffus-
Frequently recurring stressors that lead to problematic emotions/behaviors
should
be
ing angry or resentful emotional responses. The locus of the problem then is no longer
identified and reviewed at each session to see
"out there" but at least partly "in here." Well-
whether the patient is attempting suggested
written readings (generally full o f stories/cases)
alternatives and whether the alternatives are
on topics such as Type A behavior (Friedman
"working" (i.e., reducing the density o f emo-
&
tional distress). By attacking the largest stres-
Emmons,
Ulmer, 1 9 8 4 ) , assertiveness (Alberti
sors first, the patient is most likely to succeed
1983),
1 9 7 5 ) , panic attacks and
nurturant
&
(Sheehan,
communication
at having a big effect quickly. T h e only excep-
(Gordon, 1 9 7 0 ) can be helpful in getting the
tion to this strategy is when the patient is
patient to think about his or her life, beliefs,
too overwhelmed (fearful) o f attempting an
and behavior. Without such rich biographical
alternative response or when response-specific
examples, the patient tends to restrict his or her
skills deficits are present and require time to
perspective (e.g., " I ' m right, dammit, and that
remediate. For example, the patient might not
S O B is going to admit it") so as to win the
k n o w h o w to behave assertively and/or might
battle while losing the war. T h e clinician who
not be able to appropriately
can bring a rich body of personal, historical, or
discriminate
between assertive and aggressive behaviors.
clinical vignettes to bear will teach the patient
For such a patient, time must be spent review-
more quickly than will the clinician who deals
ing events and teaching and even role-playing
only in abstractions (Friedman, 1 9 7 9 ) .
skills until the patient begins to perceive the
It is important to review weekly/monthly
unavoidability o f facing the "dragon" (usually
stressful events with the patient to ascertain
a boss, parent, child, or spouse) and
has
frequency o f stressors, the cognitive/behavioral
attained adequate skills. If such attempts are
response of the patient, whether a suggested
unsuccessful, consideration should be given
alternative response was tried, and the success/
to involving the dragon in treatment.
failure o f the response as well as to encourage
The clinician should assess and begin to
alteration o f future events. It is common for
("I'm/He's
patients to not recognize that a " n e w " event is
the kind o f person who . . . " ) . M a n y cardiac
in fact the same as, or at least similar to, a
challenge self/other perceptions
294
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS previous pattern. Thus, helping the patient to
acquired or focus narrowly, in a problem-solving
perceive the similarities is necessary before he
manner, on current stressors with which the
or she will experience familiarity and so the
patient is stuck. The therapy should also secon-
opportunity for a new choice. Some patients
darily serve to support and reinforce adherence
are deliberately evasive or seem to be truly
to secondary prevention efforts given that long-
unable to recall examples of stressful interac-
term adherence rates in cardiac rehabilitation
tions from their own lives. Some such patients
are generally poor (Burke, Dunbar-Jacobs, &
will offer relatively trivial or minor events,
Hill, 1 9 9 7 ) . This is an often neglected aspect of
avoiding discussion o f the major stressors. If a
the coaching role. It is critical to recognize,
patient is reluctant/unable to offer examples
accept, and work within the patient's cognitive,
from daily life, the clinician should use clinical
emotional, behavioral, intellectual, fiscal, and
stories or examples from his or her own life to
life structure limitations so as to help him or her
destigmatize/normalize and provide examples
reengineer a healthier lifestyle. One issue in par-
of situations that the clinician thinks are appli-
ticular pertains to helping the patient to distin-
cable to the patient's situation. T h e chapter
guish between a lapse (i.e., momentary slip in
authors have observed many such patients
behavior) and a relapse (return to baseline level
who, despite no discussion of the significant
of the problem behavior). It is not uncommon
stressors in their own lives, nonetheless bene-
for patients to have lapses, and these should be
fited from hearing the counselors' or others'
normalized for the patient. The intent here is to
stories in group sessions. Indirect feedback
circumvent the patient from allowing lapses to
(usually from a spouse) indicates that
the
evolve into relapses (e.g., "Well, I already
patient is attempting to alter his or her habitual
smoked two cigarettes, so I might as well smoke
way o f thinking/coping.
the whole pack").
Optimizing Self-Management: Using a Family Practice Model
Psychopharmacology Behavioral clinicians are often less familiar
The goal of the therapist is generally to make
with the types o f medications used in treating
himself or herself obsolete as soon as possible,
cardiac diseases. Thus, Table 1 5 . 6 provides a
although the idea of a "cure" in any psycho-
list of the classifications o f the most common
therapeutic relationship is untenable
(Hoyt,
medications along with a summary o f the func-
1 9 9 5 ) . The chapter authors find the family
tions associated with each medication type.
practice model (Hoyt, 1 9 9 5 ; Morrill, 1 9 7 8 )
Importantly, patients can develop tolerance to
attractive for the C H D patient. In this light, they
these drugs, rendering them less effective over
find it helpful
time with frequent use.
to think o f themselves as
"coaches" rather than as "doctors" or "thera-
The
advent
o f the newer
antianxiety/
pists" who serve as periodic resources for reality
antidepressive agents (mostly serotonin and
testing and problem solving. Although the initial
noradrenalin reuptake inhibitors but also atypi-
stages of therapy may follow a more traditional
cals such as Buspar and Wellbutrin) has revo-
pathway, the course of treatment is best con-
lutionized psychopharmacotherapy
ceived of as intermittent but longitudinal, with
patients. Although these new agents are no
in C H D
the ultimate treatment goal of assisting the
more effective at treating anxiety/depression
patient in fitting disease self-management into
than are the older agents, the older agents
the unique circumstances o f his or her own life. Treatment episodes evolve into "tune-up" or "booster" sessions that reinforce skills already
(tricyclic or heterocyclic antidepressants) have anticholinergic properties
(e.g., dry
mouth,
blurry vision, orthostatic hypotension)
that
Coronary Table 15.6
Heart Disease
Common Cardiac Medications Listed by Function
Nitrates (e.g., Sublingual nitroglycerin, Nitropatch, Isordil, Sorbitrate, Ismo, Imdur): Drugs that produce vasodilation, used particularly for the relief of angina ACE inhibitors (e.g., Capoten/Vasotec, Monopril, Destril, Prinivil, Altace, Accupril): Vasodilators that are used as antihypertensives Beta blockers (e.g., Inderal, Lopressor, Toprol, Corgard, Sectral): Drugs that serve as beta-division adrenergic blockers, thereby reducing heart rate, blood pressure, and strength of heart contraction used for hypertension and relief of angina Calcium channel blockers (e.g., Cardizem, Dilacor, Procardia, Norvasc, Isoptin): Central and peripheral vasodilators that serve as antihypertensives and anti-ischemics used to treat chest pain, hypertension, and irregular heartbeats Digoxin (e.g., Lanoxin, Digitoxin): Decreases the strength of heart contractions and allows the heart to keep beating regularly; used for arrhythmias Diuretics (e.g., Lasix, Dyazide, Esidrix): Medications to increase urinary output and decrease fluids in the body and cardiovascular system; particularly used in heart failure Anticoagulants (e.g., asprin, Coumadin, Ticlid, Persantine, Plavix, Lovenox, Ecotrin, Heparin): Change the blood's viscosity by acting as blood "thinners" Antilipidemics (e.g., Lipitor, Zocor, Mevacor, Lopid, Lescol, Niacin): Medications that affect blood lipids by reducing low-density lipoprotein (LDL, bad cholesterol) and total cholesterol and by increasing high-density lipoprotein (HDL, good cholesterol) Antiarrhythmics (e.g., Quinidine, Betapace, Amniodarone, Norpace, Rythmol): Help to prevent both atrial and ventricular arrhythmias
make
adherence
problematic.
In
CHD
and variable, the adage "start low, go slow" is
patients, these agents prolong the " Q R S "
advisable. M a n y patients will stop the medica-
interval that is thought to place some patients
tion if no change is observed during the first
at risk for possibly fatal arrhythmias. H o w -
few days unless the importance o f at least a
ever, in the only (nonrandomized) comparison
1-month trial is emphasized repeatedly. With
o f patients on tricyclic antidepressants (TCAs),
some SSRIs (e.g., Paxil, Remeron), there often
death rates were actually lower (Pratt et al.,
is an immediate soporific effect, and these
1996).
should initially be tried at night. Otherwise, the
T h e use o f SSRIs in the treatment o f stress
medications are taken in the morning to avoid
among C H D patients requires thorough initial
interfering with sleep and with food to mini-
instructions, careful monitoring of side effects,
mize gastrointestinal distress (i.e., abdominal
comparatively low dosing, and consideration
cramping or diarrhea). M o s t o f the improve-
of possible drug-drug complications. Doses
ments will "sneak up" on patients over 1 0 days
typically used in this population are at or
to 6 weeks. Indeed, it is common for patients
therapeutic
to say " I don't feel any different" but for
range in psychiatric patients. Thus, 1 0 to 2 0
below the generally recognized
family members and/or coworkers to notice a
milligrams of Celexa or 2 5 to 5 0 milligrams of
change in the patients. Prepping patients about
Zoloft is adequate for perhaps 9 0 % o f this
the subtle effects of these medications can go a
population. Because the delay in onset is slow
long way toward circumventing unrealistic
\
295
296
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS expectations and potential
nonadherence.
SSRI can result in lowered heart rate and
Tracking a symptom (e.g., chest pain, fatigue,
blood
sleep onset delay, nocturnal awakening) in an
complain o f worsened fatigue or tiredness,
empirical fashion c a n help to
document
pressure.
Clinically, patients
will
dizziness, or light-headedness. Because o f the
improvements. Patients may be inclined to
effects on the liver, hepatic
attribute improvements to other factors. It is
times result from use of an SSRI. This is most
critical that clinicians indicate their belief that
c o m m o n in patients with prior liver injury
the medication is probably the cause.
strain will some-
(e.g., history of alcohol abuse or hepatitis) or
C o m m o n side effects within 2 4 to 4 8 hours
in patients receiving other hepatically difficult
of starting therapy include diarrhea/ abdomi-
medications (e.g., lipid-lowering agents). In
nal cramping (about 1 0 % of patients). Half o f
severe enough cases, weakness, nausea, and
those
loss o f appetite will indicate low-level hepatic
experiencing
diarrhea/abdominal
cramping are only mildly affected, and it
failure and should result in immediate cessa-
should resolve within 1 0 days. T h e remainder
tion o f the drug until liver function tests can be
must be tried on other agents. Headache
obtained. Some SSRIs may be less likely to
occurs in about 3 % o f patients and generally
have this effect.
resolves within 1 0 days. In addition, intense
M a n y patients will resist referral to a psy-
anxiety occurs in about 1 % o f patients. Over
chiatrist but will accept these agents from a
several weeks, about one quarter of patients
cardiologist or a primary care physician. If one
develop loss of libido or erectile problems.
is attempting a first trial, working with the car-
Alternative agents, or referral to a psychiatrist
diologist/primary care physician may be ade-
for additional agents, should be considered.
quate. But if special circumstances occur (e.g.,
Some weight gain (7 to 1 0 pounds) may occur.
nonresponse, intolerance o f two agents, multi-
T h e chapter authors have observed several
ple or problematic side effects such as sexual
adverse effects of SSRIs that are unique to this
dysfunction,
need for larger doses, recent
population. Some patients, usually with a
history o f substance abuse, propensity for som-
history of prior atrial fibrillation, may develop
atization), referral to a psychiatrist should
worsened
atrial fibrillation on SSRIs. T h e
become a goal of psychological treatment.
authors believe that consultation with a psy-
Another option may be to use the herbal St.
chiatrist is necessary to select a safe and effec-
John's wort, which is widely used in Europe
tive agent (e.g., Buspar, Wellbutrin). Likewise,
for the treatment of depression. Several dozen
because o f the effect
on
randomized clinical trials on St. John's wort
cytochrome P 4 5 0 metabolic pathways in the
have demonstrated an effectiveness equivalent
liver, some drugs will be removed from the
to that of T C A s (and therefore presumably
bloodstream more slowly, thereby raising
SSRIs) but with fewer side effects than T C A s
bioavailability.
o f some SSRIs
Management
can
include
(Linde &
Mulrow, 1 9 9 9 ) , although
other
reduced dosing of the affected agent or use o f
reports have called these data into question.
another SSRI that is less likely to have this
Preparations by reputable manufacturers (e.g.,
effect. Celexa and Zoloft are minimally likely
Centrum, Quanterra) should be used because
to interfere with other medications and so are
smaller sources have not carefully standardized
generally first-choice agents. Among the med-
source or dose given that these supplements are
ications potentially affected in cardiac patients
not regulated by the U.S. Food and Drug
are digoxin and beta blockers. Digoxin
toxic-
Administration. Because St. John's wort is not
ity can result in confusion/cognitive impair-
viewed as "artificial" or a "psychiatric medica-
ment, loss o f appetite, and a resting tremor.
tion," patients will sometimes try it (or even
by use o f an
insist on it) rather than the better understood
Potentiation
of beta
blockers
Coronary
Heart
Disease
CASE S T U D Y T h e case o f "Harriet R . " is a useful one that illustrates many o f the psychological aspects of C H D . Harriet was a 46-year-old, white, married mother of three children who was referred for stress management by her cardiologist after she had experienced an M I 6 months earlier. N o risk factors were detected other than her constant stress. Harriet lived with constant tension over avoiding confronting her sometimes outspoken and opinionated husband. O n e o f the early examples o f tension was that her husband frequently "forgot" to do things he was supposed to do, and Harriet was resentful that she had to remind her husband to do things and was often criticized by him for her constant reminding (which he described as "nagging"). F o r example, she reported that she always had to remind him to take his hymnal to choir practice. N o t only did she feel angry over always having to remind him, but she frequently experienced chest pain when thinking about these interactions. Harriet's husband seemed quite concerned but was not able to empathize with her. Rather, he t o o would get angry when she reminded him to do things. Following suggestions from the therapist, Harriet agreed to "deliberately not remind" her husband for a week. H e "spontaneously" began remembering things himself, and it became apparent to Harriet that her worry/resentment was totally wasted energy. N o t only did it seem that she did not need to remind her husband so often, but the number o f negative interactions that the couple experienced went down and the frequency with which Harriet experienced angina was greatly reduced. At this point, discussions with Harriet's therapist shifted her concerns to the belief that her daughter might be gay and that her husband would ban the daughter from the family if he found out. Like her experiences with "reminding her husband," whenever she began to worry about her daughter's sexual orientation, Harriet experienced frequent angina. After reviewing why Harriet thought her daughter was gay, the therapist agreed that it was likely and encouraged her to discuss this with her daughter. Pointing out that Harriet had previously sold her husband short, the therapist coached the patient on h o w to approach the topic with her husband. H e responded with surprise and disappointment, but after pondering the possibility for a few days, he stated that he would still love her and want her in the family. These experiences helped Harriet learn that much o f her concern and worry were generated by her own expectations and that her perceived negative outcomes were not always as predictable as she had once believed. Over the course o f treatment, Harriet became better at recognizing the role o f her beliefs and expectations in her stress levels, and she was able to greatly reduce the frequency with which she became angry at others. This resulted in remarkable reductions in the frequency and severity o f her angina. Harriet reported to her therapist that when she would begin to feel any anger, she could quickly assess the potential "validity" o f what was making her angry, and most o f the time the anger went away immediately when she realized that the situation was not as negative as she had initially thought.
297
298
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS and tested SSRIs. Importantly, St. John's wort
all stripes, have played a significant role in
is known to decrease digoxin levels by about
moving this scientific literature forward. M o r e
2 5 % to 3 3 % .
recent trends have allowed for practitioners to
T h e chapter authors have not yet found it
play more central roles in clinical settings such
necessary to refer a patient for electroconvul-
as Phase II cardiac rehabilitation and consulta-
sive therapy ( E C T ) . But if a patient presents
tion services targeting cardiology patients.
an otherwise intractable depression, special
These trends are likely to continue, thereby
considerations are necessary. Heart failure,
allowing greater and more varied practice
aneurysm, and arrythmias
are considered
contraindications to E C T .
opportunities
with this patient
population.
This chapter has attempted to provide an experienced and practical set o f recommendations for working clinically with the C H D patient.
SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS
There is a continued need for more practicebased writing in this arena that combines evi-
There is mounting evidence that psychological
dence-based literature with clinical experience.
and behavioral factors play an independent and
In addition, novel methods o f integration into
critical role in the development, maintenance,
service delivery behavioral interventions must
and exacerbation o f C H D . T h e clinical behav-
be explored for all manner o f secondary pre-
ioral sciences, including health psychologists of
vention targets (Trask et al., 2 0 0 2 ) .
REFERENCES Alberti, R. E., & Emmons, M. L. (1975). Stand up, speak out, talk back! New York: Pocket Books. Allen, R., & Scheldt, S. (1996). Empirical basis of cardiac psychology. In R. Allen & S. Scheldt (Eds.), Heart and mind: The practice of cardiac psychology (pp. 6 3 - 1 2 3 ) . Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Allison, T. G., Williams, D. E., Miller, T. D., Patten, C. Α., Bailey, K. R., Squires, R. W., & Gau, G. T. (1995). Medical and economic costs of psychologic distress in patients with coronary artery disease. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 70, 7 3 4 - 7 4 2 . American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Barrios, B. (1988). On the changing nature of behavioral assessment. In M. Hersen &c A. S. Bellack (Eds.), Behavioral assessment: A practical handbook (pp. 3 - 4 1 ) . New York: Pergamon. Barsky, A. J . (2001). Palpitations, arrhythmias, and awareness of cardiac activity. Annals of Internal Medicine, 134, 8 3 2 - 8 3 7 . Barsky, A. J . , Cleary, P. D., Barnett, M . C , Christiansen, C. L., & Ruskin, J . N. (1994). The accuracy of symptom reporting by patients complaining of palpitations. American journal of Medicine, 97, 2 1 4 - 2 2 1 . Black, J . L., Allison, T. G., Williams, D. E., Rummans, Τ. Α., & Gau, G. T. (1998). Effect of intervention for psychological distress on rehospitalization rates in 39, 1 3 4 - 1 4 3 . cardiac rehabilitation patients. Psychosomatics, Blumenthal, J . Α., Jiang, W., Babyak, Μ. Α., Krantz, D. S., Frid, D. J . , Coleman, R. E., Waugh, R., Hanson, M., Appelbaum, M., O'Connor, C , & Morris, J . J . (1997). Stress management and exercise training in cardiac patients with myocardial ischemia. Archives of Internal Medicine, 157, 2 2 1 3 - 2 2 2 3 .
Coronary
Heart
Booth-Kewley, S., & Friedman, H. S. (1987). Psychological predictors of heart disease: A quantitative review. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 3 4 3 - 3 6 2 . Burke, L. E., Dunbar-Jacobs, J . M., & Hill, M . N. (1997). Compliance with cardiovascular disease prevention strategies: A review of the research. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 19, 2 3 9 - 2 6 3 . Carney, R. M., Freedland, K. E., Eisen, S. Α., Rich, M. W., & Jaffe, A. S. (1995). Major depression and medication adherence in elderly patients with coronary artery disease. Health Psychology, 14, 8 8 - 9 0 . Cassem, N. H., & Hackett, T. P. (1973). Psychological rehabilitation of myocardial infarction patients in the acute phase. Heart & Lung, 2, 3 8 2 - 3 8 8 . Cox, D., & Gonder-Frederik, L. (1992). Major developments in behavioral diabetes research. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 6 2 8 - 6 3 8 . Dahlof, P., Ejnell, H., Hallstrom, T., & Hdner, J . (2000). Surgical treatment of the sleep apnea syndrome reduces associated major depression. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 7, 7 3 - 8 8 . Davidson, K. W. (2000). Dose-response relations between hostility reductions and cardiac-related hospitalizations. Psychosomatic Medicine, 62, 149 (Abstract 1430). Dusseldorp, E., van Elderen, T., Maes, S., Meulman, J . , & Kraaij, V. (1999). A meta-analysis of psychoeducational programs for coronary heart disease patients. Health Psychology, 18, 5 0 6 - 5 1 9 . Edwards, Ν. B., Murphy, J . K., Downs, A. D., Ackerman, B. J . , & Rosenthal, T. L. (1989). Doxepin as an adjunct to smoking cessation: A double-blind pilot study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 146, 3 7 3 - 3 7 6 . Farmer, J . Α., & Gotto, A. M. (1997). Dyslipidemia and other risk factors for coronary artery disease. In E. Braunwald (Ed.), Heart disease: A textbook of cardiovascular medicine. Philadelphia: W. B . Saunders. Frasure-Smith, N., & Lesperance, F. (1998). Depression and anxiety increase physician costs during the first post-MI year. Psychosomatic Medicine, 60, 9 9 . Frasure-Smith, N., Lesperance, F., 8c Talajic, M. (1993). Depression following 270, myocardial infarction. Journal of the American Medical Association, 1819-1825. Friedman, M. (1979). Qualities of therapist required for successful modification of coronary-prone (Type A) behavior. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 2, 243-248. Friedman, M., Breall, W. S., Goodwin, M . L., Sparagon, B. J . , Ghandour, G., & Fleischman, N. (1996). Effect of Type A behavioral counseling on frequency of episodes of silent myocardial ischemia in coronary patients. American Heart Journal, 132, 3 3 3 - 3 3 7 . Friedman, M., Powell, L. H., Thoreson, C. E., Ulmer, D., Price, V., Gill, J . J . , Thompson, L., Rabin, D. D., Brown, B . , Breall, W. S., Levy, R., & Bourg, Ε. (1987). Effect of discontinuance of Type A behavioral counseling on Type A behavior and cardiac recurrence rate of post myocardial infarction patients. American Heart Journal, 114, 4 8 3 - 4 9 0 . Friedman, M., & Rosenman, R. H. (1974). Type A behavior and your heart. New York: Alfred A. Knopf. Friedman, M., Thoreson, C. E., Gill, J . J . , Ulmer, D., Powell, L. H., Price, V. Α., Brown, B . , Thompson, L., Rabin, D. D., Breall, W.S., Bourg, Ε., Levy, R., & Dixon, T. (1986). Alteration of Type A behavior and its effect on cardiac recurrences in post myocardial infarction patients: Summary results of the Recurrent Coronary Prevention Project. American Heart Journal, 112, 6 5 3 - 6 6 5 . Friedman, M., & Ulmer, D. (1984). Treating Type A behavior and your heart. New York: Alfred A. Knopf. Gallacher, J . E. J . , Hopkinson, C. Α., Bennett, P., Burr, M. L., & Elwood, P. C. (1997). Effect of stress management on angina. Psychology & Health, 12, 5 2 3 - 5 3 2 .
Disease
300
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS Gilkson, M., 8c Friedman, P. A. (2001). The implantable cardioverter defibrillator. Lancet, 357, 1 1 0 7 - 1 1 1 7 . Glassman, A. H., Sc Shapiro, P. A. (1998). Depression and the course of coronary artery disease. American Journal of Psychiatry, 55, 6 8 3 - 6 9 0 . Goldberg, A. D., Becker, L. C , Bonsall, R., Cohen, J . D., Ketterer, M . W., Kaufman, P. G., Krantz, D. S., Light, K. C , McMahon, R. P., Noreuil, T., Pepine, C. J . , Raczysnki, J . , Stone, P. H., Strother, D., Taylor, H., 8c Sheps, D. S., for the PIMI Investigators. (1996). Ischemic, hemodynamic, and neurohormonal responses to mental and exercise stress: Experience from the Psychophysiological Investigations of Myocardial Ischemia study (PIMI). Circulation, 94, 2 4 0 2 - 2 4 0 9 . Goldfried, M . R., 8c Sprafkin, J . N . (1976). Behavioral personality assessment. In J . T. Spence, R. C. Carson, Sc J . W. Thibaut (Eds.), Behavioral approaches to therapy (pp. 2 9 5 - 3 2 1 ) . Morristown, NJ: General Learning. Gordon, T. (1970). P.E.T.: Parent Effectiveness Training. New York: Peter H. Wyden. Hall, S. M., Munoz, R. F., Sc Reus, V. I. (1994). Cognitive-behavioral intervention increases abstinence rates for depressive history smokers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 1 4 1 - 1 4 6 . Hall, S. M., Reus, V. I., Munoz, R. F., Sees, K. L., Humfleet, G., Hartz, D. T., Frederik, S., 8c Triffleman, E. (1998). Nortriptyline and cognitive-behavioral therapy in the treatment of cigarette smoking. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 6 8 3 - 6 9 0 . Herrman, C. (1997). International experiences with the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale: A review of validation data and clinical results. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 42, 1 7 - 4 1 . Hoyt, M . F. (1995). Brief therapy and managed care: Readings for contemporary practice. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Kamarck, T. W., 8c Jennings, J . R. (1991). Biobehavioral factors in sudden cardiac death. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 4 2 - 7 5 . Ketterer, M . W. (1993). Secondary prevention of ischemic heart disease: The case for aggressive behavioral monitoring and intervention. Psychosomatics, 34, 478-484. Ketterer, M . W. (1999). Cognitive/Behavioral therapy of anxiety in the medically ill: Cardiac settings. Seminars in Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 4(2), 148-153. Ketterer, M. W., Brymer, J . , Rhoads, K., Kraft, P., 8c Kenyon, L. W. (1994). Snoring and the severity of coronary artery disease in males. Psychosomatic Medicine, 56, 2 3 2 - 2 3 6 . Ketterer, M . W., Denollet, J . , Goldberg, A. D., McCullough, P. Α., John, S., Farha, A. J . , Clark, V., Keteyian, S., Chapp, J . , Thayer, B . , 8c Deveshwar, S. (2002). The big mush: Psychometric measures are confounded and nonindependent in their association with age at initial diagnosis of I C H D . Journal of Cardiovascular Risk, 9, 4 1 - 4 8 . Ketterer, M. W., Fitzgerald, F., Keteyian, S., Thayer, B., Jordan, M., McGowan, C , Mahr, G., Manganas, Α., 8c Goldberg, A. D. (2000). Chest pain and the treatment of psychosocial/emotional distress in CAD patients. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 23, 4 3 7 - 4 5 0 . Ketterer, M. W., Freedland, Κ. E., Krantz, D. S., Kaufmann, P. G., Forman, S., Greene, Α., Raczynski, J . , Knatterud, G., Light, K., Carney, R. M., Stone, P., Becker, L., 8c Sheps, D., for the PIMI Investigators. (2000). Psychological correlates of mental stress induced ischemia in the laboratory: The Psychophysiological Investigation of Myocardial Ischemia (PIMI) study. Journal of Health Psychology, 5, 7 5 - 8 5 . Ketterer, M. W., Huffman, J . , Lumley, Μ . Α., Wassef, S., Gray, L., Kenyon, L., Kraft, P., Brymer, J . , Rhoads, K., Lovallo, W. R., 8c Goldberg, A. D. (1998). Five year follow-up for adverse outcomes in males with at least minimally
Coronary
Heart
positive angiograms: Importance of "denial" in assessing psychosocial risk factors. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 44, 2 4 1 - 2 5 0 . Ketterer, M . W., Kenyon, L., Foley, Β. Α., Brymer, J . , Rhoads, K., Kraft, P., 8c Lovallo, W. R. (1996). Denial of depression as an independent correlate of coronary artery disease. Journal of Health Psychology, 1, 9 3 - 1 0 5 . Ketterer, M . W., Pickering, E., Stoever, W. W., 8c Wansley, R. A. (1987). Smoking prevention, cessation, and maintenance: A review for the primary care physician. Journal of the American Osteopathic Association, 87, 2 4 8 - 2 5 7 . Krantz, D. S., Hedges, S. M., Gabbay, F. H., Klein, J . , Falconer, J . J . , Merz, C. N., Gottdiener, J . S., Lutz, H., & Rozanski, A. (1994). Triggers of angina and st-segment depression in ambulatory patients with coronary artery disease: Evidence for an uncoupling of angina and ischemia. American Heart Journal, 128, 7 0 3 - 7 1 2 . Kubzansky, L. D., Kawachi, I., Weiss, S. T., 8c Sparrow, D. (1998). Anxiety and coronary heart disease: A synthesis of epidemiological, psychological, and experimental evidence. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 20, 4 7 - 5 8 . Lazarus, R. S., 8c Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer. Lessmeier, T. J . , Gamperling, D., Johnson-Liddon, V., Fromm, B. S., Steinman, R. T., Meissner, M . D., 8c Lehmann, M . H. (1997). Unrecognized paroxysmal supraventricular tachycardia: Potential for misdiagnosis of panic disorder. Archives of Internal Medicine, 157, 5 3 7 - 5 4 3 . Lewin, B. (1997). The psychological and behavioral management of angina. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 43, 4 5 3 - 4 6 2 . Linde, K., 8c Mulrow, C. D. (1999). St. John's wort is more effective than placebo for treating depressive disorders. ACP Journal Club, 130, 6 0 - 8 0 . Linden, W., 8c Chambers, L. (1994). Clinical effectiveness of nondrug treatment of hypertension: A meta-analysis. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 16, 3 5 - 4 5 . Markovitz, J . H. (1998). Hostility is associated with increased platelet activation in Medicine, 60, 5 8 6 - 5 9 1 . coronary heart disease. Psychosomatic Morrill, R. G. (1978). The future of mental health in primary health care programs. American Journal of Psychiatry, 135, 1 3 5 1 - 1 3 5 5 . Murphy, J . K., Edwards, Ν. B., Downs, A. D., Ackerman, B. J . , Sc Rosenthal, T. L. (1990). Effects of doxepin on withdrawal symptoms in smoking cessation. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 1 3 5 3 - 1 3 5 7 . Polk, D. E., Kamarck, T. W., 8c Shiffman, S. (2002). Hostility explains some of the discrepancy between daytime ambulatory and clinic blood pressure. Health Psychology, 21, 2 0 2 - 2 0 6 . Pratt, L. Α., Ford, D. E., Crum, R. M., Armenian, H. K., Gallo, J . J . , Sc Eaton, W. W. (1996). Depression, psychotropic medication, and risk of myocardial infarction: Prospective data from the Baltimore ECA follow-up. Circulation, 94, 3 1 2 3 - 3 1 2 9 . Richter, J . E. (1992). Overview of diagnostic testing for chest pain of unknown origin. Medical Clinics of North America, 92(Suppl. 5A), S 4 1 - S 4 5 . Ridker, P. M., Hennekens, C. H., Buring, J . E., Sc Rifai, N. (2000). C-reactive protein and other markers of inflammation in the prediction of cardiovascular disease in women. New England Journal of Medicine, 342, 8 3 6 - 8 4 3 . Ross, R. (1999). Atherosclerosis: An inflammatory disease. New England Journal of Medicine, 340, 1 1 5 - 1 2 6 . Rozanski, Α., Blumenthal, J . Α., Sc Kaplan, J . (1999). Impact of psychological factors on the pathogenesis of cardiovascular disease and implications for therapy. Circulation, 99, 2 1 9 2 - 2 2 1 7 . Schwartz, S. M., Gramling, S. E., Sc Mancini, T. (1994). The influence of life stress, personality, and learning history on illness behavior. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25, 1 3 5 - 1 4 2 .
Disease
302
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS Schwartz, S. M., Lipman, H., Glasburg, D., Pagano, C , Jones, D., Deaner, S. L., & Lehmann, M. H. (2002). Gender differences in symptom reporting in patients Medicine, 64, 1 3 5 . with palpitations. Psychosomatic Schwartz, S. M., Trask, P. C , & Ketterer, M. W. (1999). Understanding chest pain: What every psychologist should know. Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings, 6, 3 3 3 - 3 5 1 . Sears, S. F., Todaro, J . F., Lewis, T. S., Sotile, W., & Conti, J . B. (1999). Examining the psychosocial impact of implantable cardioverter defibrillators: A literature review. Clinical Cardiology, 22, 4 8 1 - 4 8 9 . Sheehan, D. V. (1983). The anxiety disease. New York: Scribner. Stetson, Β. Α., Rahn, J . M., Dubbert, P. M., Wilner, B. L, & Mecury, M. G. (1997). Prospective evaluation of the effects of stress on exercise adherence in community-residing women. Health Psychology, 16, 5 1 5 - 5 2 0 . Trask, P. C , Schwartz, S. M., Deaner, S. L., Paterson, A. G., Johnson, T., Rubenfire, M., & Pomerleau, O. P. (2002). Behavioral medicine: The challenge of integrating psychological and behavioral approaches into primary care. Effective Clinical Practice, 5(2), 7 5 - 8 3 .
CHAPTER
10 16
Behavioral Management of Type 2 Diabetes AHNA L . HOFF, JANELLE L . WAGNER, LARRY L . MULLINS, AND JOHN M . CHANEY
T
ype 2 diabetes (DM2)
is a chronic
(Anderson, Freeland, Clouse, &
Lustman,
illness characterized by a dysregulation
2001).
of glucose metabolism secondary to
impact o f diabetes is staggering; in 1997,
O n a more global scale, the economic the
an imbalance between insulin sensitivity and
direct and indirect costs associated with dia-
insulin secretion. An estimated 11 million
betes in the United States were an estimated
people in the United States were diagnosed
$98 billion (Ray, Thamer, Gardner, &c Chan,
1998).
2000 (Boyle approximately 90% o f
et al., those
betes in the United States represents a critical
individuals were diagnosed with DM2.
The
public health problem with respect to health
number o f individuals diagnosed with
DM2
care use and resources.
with diabetes in the year
2001),
and
has been rising at an alarming rate over the past several decades (Harris,
1998).
Thus, the increasing prevalence of dia-
Fortunately, DM2
is medically manage-
It has
able, and many o f the serious medical compli-
been estimated that this figure will increase by
cations that are associated with the illness are
165% to 29 million by the
preventable
year
2050, reflect-
through
adequate
control o f
ing a 7.2% prevalence rate. Such an increase
blood glucose levels. Therefore, the primary
is hypothesized to be the result o f changes in
treatment goal for individuals with DM2 is to
demographic composition o f the population,
maintain blood glucose levels within a normal
population growth, and increasing prevalence
range. This requires individuals with diabetes
2001).
rates (Boyle et al.,
to consistently maintain a complicated, life-
Personal and public health consequences
long treatment regimen with few immediate
For
tangible rewards for their efforts. For many,
example, individuals with diabetes experience
maintaining this complex treatment regimen is
associated with
DM2
are profound.
of health complications
difficult, and assistance with disease manage-
(Harris, 1998) and are at higher risk for depres-
ment is often warranted. As such, researchers
sion than are their medically well counterparts
and the medical community have recognized
a greater number
303
304
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS the critical role o f behavioral sciences in optimizing regimen adherence
and
health
ability to produce sufficient amounts o f insulin and so can no longer maintain euglycemia, that
outcomes among patients with D M 2 (Glasgow
is, near-normal
glucose levels (DeFronzo,
et al., 1 9 9 9 ; Wysocki & Buckloh, 2 0 0 2 ) .
Bonadonna, & Ferrannini, 1 9 9 2 ) .
T h e purpose o f this chapter is to review the psychosocial aspects and behavioral management o f D M 2 . First, information
regarding
the nature o f the illness, including etiology and
Short-Term Complications Hypoglycemia and hyperglycemia are two
complications, is reviewed. Then, psychologi-
short-term
cal factors and symptomatology commonly
DM2.
associated with D M 2 are presented. N e x t , the
glucose declines to less than 6 0 milligrams per
complications that result
Hypoglycemia occurs when
from blood
assessment and treatment o f the medical and
deciliter (mg/dl). Symptoms include shakiness,
psychological aspects o f D M 2 are discussed,
perspiration, rapid heartbeat, hunger,
including a review of psychosocial interven-
ache, mood changes, confusion, and attentional
head-
tions focused on improving global adjustment
difficulties. If hypoglycemia is left untreated,
to diabetes. It should be noted that there are
seizures or loss o f consciousness may eventually
a number o f studies reviewed here that used
occur. Conversely, hyperglycemia is defined as
combined samples of individuals with type 1
a blood glucose level greater than 1 4 0 mg/dl.
and type 2 diabetes. Where possible, informa-
Symptoms include increased thirst,
frequent
tion specific to D M 2 is presented and indi-
urination, and glucose in the urine. T h e major-
cated as such.
ity of long-term complications associated with D M 2 are the result of recurrent hyperglycemia (American Diabetes Association [ADA], 1 9 9 8 ) .
BACKGROUND AND ETIOLOGY There are two primary types o f diabetes: type
Long-Term Complications
1 and type 2 . Type 1 diabetes (often referred to
Long-term complications can be classified
in the past as juvenile diabetes or insulin-
into two types: macrovascular and microvas-
dependent diabetes mellitus) occurs when the
cular. Macrovascular diseases include heart
islet cells o f the pancreas are destroyed and
disease, stroke, and
cannot produce insulin. Consequently, exoge-
orders. T h e leading cause o f diabetes-related
nous insulin is required for survival. In con-
deaths is heart disease. Adults with diabetes
trast,
DM2
is characterized
by
other circulatory dis-
chronic
are two to four times more likely to die from
hyperglycemia due to impaired insulin secre-
heart disease than are adults without diabetes
tion and increased insulin resistance in the
(National Institute o f Diabetes and Digestive
body's cells. Insulin resistance refers to a defect
and
Kidney
Diseases
[NIDDK],
2000).
in glucose transport and metabolism. As a
Individuals with D M 2 are also at two to four
result, glucose does not enter the body's cells
times greater risk for stroke than are their
where it can be used as fuel, and subsequently,
medically well counterparts.
a higher level o f glucose remains in the blood-
approximately 73% o f adults with diabetes
stream. Early in the course o f D M 2 , the pan-
have circulatory disorders (e.g., high blood
creas attempts to counteract the high levels o f
pressure), many o f which require prescription
blood glucose by producing increased amounts
medications for hypertension ( N I D D K , 2 0 0 0 ) .
of insulin. However, because of the sustained
Microvascular
Furthermore,
complications
constitute
need for high amounts o f insulin over long
some
periods o f time, the pancreas gradually loses its
tions associated with D M 2 . Microvascular
of the
most
debilitating
complica-
Type 2 Diabetes
\
complications include retinopathy, nephropathy,
DM2
and neuropathy. Diabetic retinopathy is caused
1 9 9 8 ) . Furthermore, women who experience
than do other ethnic groups (Harris,
by changes in the tiny vessels that supply the
gestational diabetes during one or more o f their
retina with blood. It is the leading cause o f
pregnancies are at greater risk for developing
visual impairment among adults ages 2 0 to 7 4
D M 2 later in life than are women who do not
years, resulting in an estimated 1 2 , 0 0 0
to
(NIDDK, 2 0 0 0 ) . Finally, those with a family
2 4 , 0 0 0 new cases o f blindness in the United
history of diabetes are also at heightened risk.
States each year (NIDDK, 2 0 0 0 ) .
Nephro-
Indeed, first-degree relatives of individuals with
pathy, another microvascular complication
early-onset D M 2 are 4 0 % more likely to
associated with diabetes, is the result o f damage
develop diabetes than are individuals with no
to the blood vessels of the kidneys. In severe
family history o f the disease (Owen, Ayers,
cases, it can result in kidney failure, a condition
Corbett, & Hattersley, 2 0 0 2 ) . Thus, there are a
referred to as end stage renal disease (ESRD).
number o f demographic factors that are associ-
E S R D can be a life-threatening complication,
ated with a heightened risk for D M 2 .
and individuals who experience kidney failure must undergo dialysis or a kidney transplant.
Obesity.
T h e previously mentioned risk
The relationship between diabetes and E S R D is
factors can be mitigated by the most critical and
clear; diabetes accounts for 4 3 % o f new cases
modifiable risk factor for D M 2 , that is, obesity.
of E S R D (NIDDK, 2 0 0 0 ) .
It is estimated that up to 7 5 % o f the risk for
Neuropathy (neuronal disease) is a com-
D M 2 is directly attributable to obesity (Manson
m o n microvascular complication affecting
& Spelsberg, 1 9 9 4 ) . For example, Fullier and
approximately 6 0 % to 7 0 % o f individuals
Pedula (2001) demonstrated an inverse linear
with mild to severe forms o f nervous system
relationship between the Body Mass Index
damage ( N I D D K , 2 0 0 0 ) . Characteristic symp-
(weight in kilograms divided by height in
toms include pain, numbing, burning, loss o f
squared meters) and age at diagnosis o f D M 2 .
feeling, and (in more severe cases) paralysis in
In other words, those who are more overweight
the extremities. Neuropathy may also cause
are more likely to be diagnosed with D M 2 at a
digestive problems, impotence, and inconti-
younger age. Although the mechanisms respon-
nence. Severe forms o f nerve damage are a
sible for the relationship between weight and
major cause o f lower extremity amputations.
D M 2 are not entirely clear, recent evidence suggests that body weight is associated with insulin resistance and subsequently glycémie control
Etiological Risk Factors
(Maggio &c Pi-Sunyer, 1 9 9 7 ) . Consequently, the A
majority of D M 2 prevention and intervention
number o f demographic and genetic factors are
programs target weight loss (for a review, see
associated with D M 2 , including age, race, prior
Wing et al., 2 0 0 1 ) . Such weight loss efforts have
history of gestational diabetes, family history of
been shown to be effective in preventing the
diabetes, and obesity. In terms o f age, those
onset of D M 2 (Diabetes Prevention Program
over age 4 5 years are at the highest risk for
Research Group [DPPRG], 2 0 0 2 ) . A primary
developing D M 2 , and approximately 2 0 % of
challenge for behavioral researchers and those at
individuals over age 6 5 years have diabetes
risk for diabetes is to determine effective meth-
Demographic
and
Genetic
Factors.
(NIDDK, 2 0 0 0 ) . Similarly, differential rates of
ods to maintain long-term weight loss given that
D M 2 are found among various ethnic groups.
it is well known that maintenance of weight loss
For
Native
is difficult to achieve (Wing et al., 2 0 0 1 ). In sum-
Americans, and Hispanic/Latino Americans
mary, the most influential risk factor for D M 2 is
have demonstrably higher prevalence rates for
also potentially the most modifiable.
example, African
Americans,
305
306
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS correlates o f sexual dysfunction and visual
PSYCHOLOGICAL
impairment. Sexual dysfunction is a prevalent
FACTORS AND TYPE 2 DIABETES
problem among men with diabetes; approxiIndividuals with D M 2 constitute a population
mately 5 0 % of men with diabetes experience
at risk for experiencing both illness-related and
impotence. Although the prevalence of sexual
general distress. M a n y individuals experience
dysfunction is not known for women, one
subclinical adjustment problems directly related
study
to living with diabetes, including
distress
reported significantly lower levels of sexual
associated with the onset o f diabetes, diabetes
desire or orgasmic capacity, lubrication, and
reported
that
women
with
DM2
complications, and the treatment regimen. Still
sexual satisfaction in their relationships than
others experience clinically significant levels o f
did nondiabetic control participants (Schreiner-
psychological distress, including depression and
Engel, Schiavi, Vietorisz, & Smith, 1 9 8 7 ) . T o
anxiety. This section reviews both diabetes-
date, little research has specifically examined
specific distress, as outlined by Rubin
the treatment o f impotence among those with
and
Peyrot ( 2 0 0 1 ) , and more general distress, such
diabetes
as depression and anxiety.
1 9 8 6 ) . As such, it has been recommended that
(McCulloch, Hosking, &
Tobert,
treatment for impotence proceed in accordance with the standards established for medically
Distress Related to Diabetes Onset T h e diagnosis o f diabetes represents
well individuals (Rubin & Peyrot, 2 0 0 1 ) . a
period o f crisis for many. Unfortunately, few
Visual impairment due to diabetic retinopathy is another complication that
appears
empirical studies have examined levels o f dis-
to have profound psychological consequences
tress immediately following diagnosis of D M 2
for the patient. Individuals diagnosed
among adults. However, increased levels of
progressive
diabetic
retinopathy
with
(PDR)
distress immediately following diagnosis is
reported a greater number o f negative life
common among children diagnosed with type
experiences and psychiatric distress during the
1 diabetes. For most, this distress then dissi-
2 years following diagnosis with P D R (Wulsin
pates over the first year following diagnosis
&
(Kovacs, Brent, Steinberg, Paulauskas, & Reid,
R a n d , 1 9 8 7 ) . Distress related to P D R appears
1 9 8 6 ) . Thus, Rubin and Peyrot ( 1 9 9 4 ) recom-
to be independent o f the severity o f vision loss
mended regular monitoring o f patients' distress
and
levels following diagnosis. Individuals experi-
restored (Wulsin 6c Jacobson, 1 9 8 9 ; Wulsin et
encing clinically significant distress should be
al., 1 9 8 7 ) . Moreover, individuals who experi-
referred for intervention as a means of mini-
enced fluctuating levels of visual impairment
mizing the impact on diabetes management.
experienced more distress than did those with
J a c o b s o n , 1 9 8 9 ; Wulsin, J a c o b s o n ,
remains elevated even after vision is
more stable and severe visual
Distress Related to Medical Complications T h e onset of medical complications can trigger a crisis for many individuals, and health
&
(Bernbaum,
Alpert,
&
impairment
Duckro,
1988).
C o m m o n feelings related to vision loss include failure, uncertainty, and fear. Nevertheless, few
studies
to address onset
have e x a m i n e d
interventions
the emotional consequences of
care providers should not underestimate the
PDR
potential emotional impact of new complica-
Therefore, it is recommended that clinicians
(Bernbaum
et
al.,
1988).
tions. However, research examining the impact
regularly assess psychological distress, espe-
of specific diabetes complications is quite lim-
cially among patients w h o have been diag-
ited and has primarily examined psychological
nosed with P D R .
Type 2 Diabetes Little
is
currently
known
about
psychological sequelae o f other
\
the
Wensloff, Gronsman, & Jaber, 1 9 8 9 ) , other
diabetes-
studies have not (Griffith, Field, & Lustman,
specific complications. However, it has been
1 9 9 0 ) . T h e equivocal findings are likely due
demonstrated that the more complications a
in part to the wide variety o f methodologies
person experiences, the more likely he or she is
employed, including types o f stressors targeted,
to manifest psychological distress (Trief, Grant,
durations o f the stressors, measures of subjec-
Elbert, & Weinstock, 1 9 9 8 ) . Thus, health care
tive stress, interval between the stressors and
professionals should remain cognizant o f the
the blood glucose tests, and baseline blood
psychological consequences of new or accruing
glucose levels. Obviously, the
diabetes complications for their patients.
between stress and blood glucose is a complex
relationship
one that involves multiple variables, including cognitive and physiological factors.
Psychosocial Stress
Indirectly, stress is thought to adversely
Stress, often conceptualized as the interac-
influence metabolic control through changes
individual's
in self-management behaviors. T h e ability to
response to that event, is one o f the most
maintain the demands o f a c o m p l e x treat-
widely studied psychosocial factors associated
ment regimen may be compromised by the
tion between an event and an
with D M 2 (Goetsch & Wiebe, 1 9 9 5 ) . It has
demands o f daily life (e.g., eating fast food
been hypothesized that stress affects metabolic
instead o f taking the time to prepare a proper
control both directly and indirectly (Peyrot &
meal, skipping regular exercise to catch up
M c M u r r y , 1 9 8 5 ) . First, stress is believed to
on work) (Marlatt & G o r d o n , 1 9 8 5 ) . Future
through
research studies are needed to identify psy-
physiological mechanisms. Theoretically, stress
chological and physiological markers o f indi-
triggers the natural physiological responses
viduals w h o are m o r e susceptible to stress
("fight or flight" response), resulting in the
and to shed light on appropriate interven-
release of counterregulatory hormones. T h e
tions. Interventions that directly target stress,
directly affect
metabolic control
release o f these hormones triggers an increase
such as relaxation training and stress manage-
in sympathetic activity to the pancreas, thereby
ment, are discussed later in the chapter.
inhibiting insulin and stimulating the release o f glucagons (Goetsch &
Wiebe,
1995).
Glucagon then stimulates the liver to convert glycogen to glucose and release hepatic glucose
Depression Empirical research indicates that depression
stores into the bloodstream. As a result, there
is quite prevalent among individuals
with
can be increases in blood glucose levels inde-
diabetes. Meta-analytic data indicate
that
pendent
approximately 3 1 % o f individuals with dia-
of
consistent
diabetic
regimen
adherence. A number o f animal models have
betes report clinically elevated
supported the link between acute stressors and
symptoms. Furthermore, the accrued lifetime
metabolic control (Kuhn, Cochrane, Feinglos,
prevalence of major depression among individ-
& Surwit, 1 9 8 7 ; Surwit, Feinglos, Livingston,
uals with diabetes is estimated to be 2 8 . 5 %
depressive
Kuhn, & M c C u b b i n , 1 9 8 4 ; Surwit et a l ,
(Anderson et al., 2 0 0 1 ) .
1 9 8 6 ) . However, generalization of these results
Zheng, and Simpson ( 2 0 0 2 ) found that indi-
Similarly, Egede,
to humans has been difficult. Although some
viduals with diabetes (type 1 or type 2 ) , when
studies have reported significant associations
compared with a healthy control group, are
between life stress and hemoglobin A
twice as likely to be diagnosed with depression.
independent adults
with
l c
of regimen adherence diabetes
(Demmers,
(HbA ) lc
among
Depressive episodes also tend to occur more
Neale,
frequently and last longer among individuals
307
308
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS with D M 2 than among those without D M 2 (Lustman,
Clouse,
Alrakawi,
Rubin,
et al., 1997]). In fact, the prevalence of depressive
&
symptoms among those with diabetes argues
Gelenberg, 1 9 9 7 ) . Finally, consistent with the
for the routine integration o f screening for
general population, the prevalence o f depres-
mood
sion is greater among women with diabetes
providers into their patients' treatment plans
disorders
by primary
health
care
than among men with diabetes (Anderson
as a means o f improving their patients' psy-
etal., 2001).
chological and physical health.
The
demonstrated
association between
depression and diabetes provides the most striking example of the interwoven relationship between psychological functioning and diabetes outcome. Depression is strongly asso-
Anxiety As with depression, rates o f anxiety are significantly higher a m o n g individuals
with
ciated with metabolic control among individu-
diabetes than among the general population.
als with D M 2 (Lustman et al., 2 0 0 0 ) . In their
In fact, rates o f clinically significant anxiety
meta-analysis, Lustman and colleagues ( 2 0 0 0 )
among those with diabetes may be as high as
found that depression accounted for approxi-
4 9 % (Peyrot & Rubin, 1 9 9 7 ) . Gender, age,
mately 3 % o f the variance in glycated hemo-
and education were variables that predicted
globin, a salient finding given that a
greater anxiety symptoms (Peyrot & Rubin,
1%
decrease in glycated hemoglobin results in a
1997).
33%
between ages 4 0 and 4 9 years, and individuals
decrease in the progression rate o f
In particular, females,
individuals
retinopathy (Morisaki et al., 1 9 9 4 ) . Further-
with lower educational backgrounds
more, treatment o f depression could poten-
more likely to report symptoms o f anxiety.
were
tially increase the proportion of individuals in
Individuals who had more diabetes complica-
good glycémie control from 4 1 % to 5 8 % in
tions also reported more anxiety symptoms.
the diabetic population (Lustman et al., 2 0 0 0 ) .
N o t surprisingly, 3 8 % o f those reporting anx-
Thus, psychological functioning has direct
iety
implications for illness outcomes.
(Peyrot & Rubin, 1 9 9 7 ) . Thus, regular screen-
T h e r e also appears to be a relationship
also
reported
comorbid
depression
ing for anxiety among patients with diabetes is
depressive
also recommended, especially among those
symptoms among patients with diabetes. For
with a greater number o f diabetes-related
example, individuals with depression
complications.
between health
care use and
and
diabetes had higher ambulatory care use ( 1 2 vs. 7 visits) and filled more prescriptions ( 4 3 vs. 2 1 ) than did their counterparts without a diagnosis o f depression. A m o n g individuals with diabetes, total health care expenditures
Other Psychosocial Factors Social Support.
Social support has been asso-
ciated with physical and psychological health
for individuals with depression and diabetes
among those with D M 2 . Individuals with D M 2
were four and a half times higher than those
who perceive more social support from friends
for
($247
and family engage in more diabetes self-care
million vs. $ 5 5 million) (Egede et al., 2 0 0 2 ) .
than do those who perceive less social support
individuals without
depression
Despite the influence o f depression
on
(Garay-Sevilla et al., 1 9 9 5 ; Peyrot, McMurry,
D M 2 outcome and health care use, t w o o f
& Davida, 1 9 9 9 ) . In fact, social support may
undiagnosed.
account for as much as 1 7 % of the variance in
Undiagnosed depression is likely due to the
self-reported diabetes self-care behaviors (Wang
fact that many o f the symptoms for depression
8c Fenske, 1 9 9 6 ) . Social support has also
and diabetes overlap (e.g., fatigue [Lustman
been consistently associated with decreased
three cases o f depression go
Type 2 Diabetes
\
psychological distress among medically well and
internal locus of control orientation (i.e., the
diabetes populations (Connell, Davis, Gallant,
belief that a person can manage his or her ill-
&
ness) was significantly associated with fewer
Sharpe, 1 9 9 4 ) . Those who report more
general social support are more likely to have
symptoms
more diabetes-specific social support and subse-
Conversely, a self-blaming internal locus of
quently
symptomatology
control (i.e., the belief that a person is respon-
less
depressive
of
depression
and
anxiety.
(Littlefield, Rodin, Murray, & Craven, 1 9 9 0 ) .
sible for his or her negative illness outcomes
As such, social support appears to serve a pro-
such as poor metabolic control) was associ-
tective function against depressive symptoms in
ated with high blood glucose levels and binge
the context of diabetes.
eating. In addition, participants who thought that diabetes control was a matter of chance Illness intrusiveness,
or fate were more likely to be depressed and
which refers to the perception o f the extent to
anxious and to have lower self-esteem (Peyrot
disrupts
& Rubin, 1 9 9 4 ) . Certainly, more research is
valued activities, has also been associated with
needed to determine how perceived control
depressive symptoms among individuals with
and health outcomes are causally related.
diabetes (Devins, Hunsley, Mandin, T a u b , &c
However,
Paul, 1 9 9 7 ) . T o illustrate, T a l b o t , Nouwen,
perceptions o f control are important cognitive
Gingras, Bélanger, and Audet ( 1 9 9 9 ) exam-
mechanisms to assess and are potentially effec-
ined depressive symptoms in a sample o f 2 3 7
tive targets for treatment.
Illness
Intrusiveness.
which an illness constrains and
it
appears
that
health-related
individuals with D M 2 cross-sectionally and found that illness intrusiveness accounted for
Coping
Style.
T h e inconsistent
findings
6 1 % o f the variance in depressive symptoms.
regarding the relationship between stress and
Such findings suggest that individuals who
glycémie control may be partially due to dif-
tend to perceive their diabetes as restricting
ferences in h o w individuals cope with stress.
activities in valued domains (e.g., family, rela-
Stress has been significantly associated with
tionships, spirituality) are at greater risk for
higher H b A
l c
values among those individuals
depression than are those who do not. T o
who reported "ineffective" coping styles (i.e.,
date, relatively little research has been con-
emotional or angry responses to stress) but
ducted examining cognitive factors such as
not among those w h o reported
intrusiveness and their relationship to depres-
coping styles (i.e., tendency to not respond
sion in the c o n t e x t o f diabetes.
emotionally to stress) (Peyrot & M c M u r r y ,
Further
"effective"
research is needed in this area to determine
1 9 9 2 ) . Coping styles have also been directly
whether these cognitive variables mediate the
related to glycémie control. For example,
relationship between diabetes and depression.
Peyrot and colleagues ( 1 9 9 9 ) found that after controlling for regimen adherence, individuals
Perceived
Control.
Another
cognitive
with D M 2 w h o reported m o r e pragmatic and
appraisal variable, perceived control, has also
stoic coping styles showed better glycémie
been related to levels o f psychological distress
control. Such findings suggest that changing
among individuals with diabetes. Using a mea-
the manner in which individuals respond to
sure o f diabetes-specific control, Peyrot and
stressors may mitigate the impact that stress
Rubin ( 1 9 9 4 ) found that the type o f locus o f
has on glycémie control o f those with D M 2 .
control resulted in either positive or negative health outcomes. Their w o r k suggests that
Quality
of Life.
Quality o f life ( Q O L ) is a
control:
global construct that refers to health-related
autonomous and self-blaming. Autonomous
physical and social functioning as well as
there are t w o types o f internal
309
310
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS perceived physical and
mental
well-being.
among these minority groups than
among
Overall, a number of studies indicate that Q O L
Caucasian populations (Cowie et al., 1 9 8 9 ;
is compromised in individuals with diabetes.
Franklin,
M o r e specifically, those with diabetes reported
Hamman,
reduced
Rowland,
role and
physical functioning
as
Kahn,
Baxter,
1 9 9 0 ; Harris, &
Byrd-Holt,
Marshall, Klein,
&
Cowie,
1 9 9 8 ; Resnick,
well as decreased perceptions o f overall health.
Valsania, &
These findings are congruent with studies indi-
potential explanations have been posited to
Phillips, 1 9 9 9 ) . A number of
cating that the presence of two or more com-
account for these observed differences, includ-
with
ing decreased health care access, physiological
decreased Q O L (Rubin & Peyrot, 1 9 9 9 ; Trief
factors, and psychological factors. As an exem-
et al., 1 9 9 8 ) . As seen with depression, gender
plar, findings from a select number o f studies
differences also exist, as women report lower
with African Americans are presented here.
plications is consistently
associated
Q O L than do men. Accordingly, women tend
A number o f differences have been observed
to be less satisfied with their treatment regi-
in health-related beliefs, attitudes, and coping
men, miss more work, and are involved in
styles. For instance, Samuel-Hodge and col-
fewer leisure activities as compared with men
leagues ( 2 0 0 0 ) found that African American
(Rubin & Peyrot, 1 9 9 8 ) .
women reported that they often feel "nervous
Because o f the required changes in lifestyle
and tired" and worry about diabetes compli-
and rigid adherence to treatment regimens,
cations. Furthermore, many African American
it is not surprising that perceived Q O L is
women tend to respond to the needs of others
affected. In fact, it has been demonstrated that
and neglect their own care in accordance with
patients controlling their diabetes solely with
their caretaking role within the family. Coping
diet and exercise had fewer diabetes quality o f
methods may also substantially differ given
life ( D Q O L ) assessed worries than did those
that African American women tend to rely on
taking oral medications. Furthermore, those
spirituality and their belief in G o d to provide
taking oral medications reported more D Q O L
the strength to cope with diabetes (Samuel-
satisfaction with treatment and less burden o f
Hodge
illness than did those taking insulin (Jacobson,
factors are just a few examples o f potential
deGroot, &c Samson, 1 9 9 4 ) . It appears, then,
ethnic and cultural differences in psychological
that certain components o f an
adjustment and coping style that may exist and
treatment
regimen affect
individual's
Q O L . However,
findings on the relationship between glycémie control and Q O L are less clear. Although
et al., 2 0 0 0 ) . T h e
aforementioned
so should be considered in treatment
plan
development. Unfortunately
only a limited number o f
some studies have found significant relation-
studies have examined the effectiveness of inter-
ships between D Q O L and H b A
ventions designed for specific ethnic groups. The
l c
(Trief et al.,
1 9 9 8 ) , others have not (Peterson, Lee, Young,
few that have been conducted
Newton, & Doran, 1 9 9 8 ) .
improvements in glycémie control, blood pres-
Ethnic and Cultural Considerations
Have, & Adams-Campbell, 1 9 9 7 ) , and weight
demonstrate
sure control (Agurs-Collins, Kumanyika, Ten
As stated previously, rates of D M 2 are clearly higher among some ethnic
loss (Mayer-Davis et al., 2 0 0 1 ) . Such positive outcomes underscore the importance of devel-
groups,
oping culture-specific interventions The rele-
including African Americans, Native Americans,
vance of these findings is also reflected in
and Hispanic Americans (Harris, 2 0 0 1 ) . Further-
culturally centered diabetes outreach programs
more, the frequency and severity o f micro-
(e.g., African American, Native American,
vascular complications are more
Latino) recently developed by the A D A (2002b).
common
Type 2
Diabetes
In the future, research is needed to determine
drug intervention took metformin, which is
the culture-specific factors that contribute to
designed to promote glucose homeostasis, or
diabetes adjustment and effective interventions.
were administered a placebo. Overall, results
A complete review o f the literature on culture
for the lifestyle intervention were promising
and D M 2 deserves far more consideration than
( D P P R G , 2 0 0 0 ) . At follow-up (average 2 . 8
can be achieved within the scope o f this
years), the lifestyle intervention group demon-
chapter. However, it is strongly emphasized
strated a 5 8 % reduction in diabetes develop-
that sociocultural factors must be considered
ment, and the drug intervention demonstrated
when intervening in the context o f D M 2 .
a 3 1 % reduction in diabetes development, relative to the placebo group ( D P P R G , 2 0 0 2 ) . Similar results were found in the Finnish Diabetes Prevention Study (FDPS) (Tuomilehto
ASSESSMENT AND T R E A T M E N T
et al., 2 0 0 1 ) , which randomized 5 2 2 overweight
Primary Prevention: Risk Reduction
individuals
with
impaired
glucose
tolerance to an intensive lifestyle intervention
role o f modifiable risk
(targeting weight loss, food intake, and physical
factors in the etiology o f D M 2 makes preven-
activity) or to a control group (brief diet and
T h e substantial
tion efforts a primary health care objective. T h e
exercise counseling). As in the DPP, Finnish
majority o f prevention efforts involve lifestyle
participants in the lifestyle intervention group
as
showed a 5 8 % reduction in the incidence o f
obesity, overeating, and physical inactivity. F o r
diabetes as compared with the control group.
interventions,
targeting
factors
such
recent large-scale prevention
Clearly, both the DPP and FDPS provide evi-
studies highlight the critical role o f behavioral
dence for the impact o f influential changes in
example, two
change in diabetes care. T h e first, the Diabetes
eating and exercise habits on the development
Prevention Program (DPP), involved a multi-
of diabetes.
center study o f individuals at risk for diabetes
T h e robust findings o f the t w o large-scale
conducted by the National Institutes o f Health
prevention studies just described are reflected
(Diabetes Prevention Program Research Group
in the A D A ' s 2 0 0 2 position statement on
[DPPRG], 1 9 9 9 ) . Participants were randomly
the prevention or delay o f D M 2 . T h e A D A ' s
assigned to one o f three groups, including a
statement consists o f recommendations for
drug intervention (metformin vs. placebo) or an
health care providers to (a) increase patient
intensive lifestyle intervention.
Participants
awareness o f the benefits o f modest weight
receiving the intensive lifestyle intervention
loss and regular exercise, (b) conduct regular
met for at least 1 6 sessions with case managers
screenings o n high-risk populations, and (c)
trained in nutrition, exercise, and behavioral
provide weight loss and exercise counseling
modification over the first 2 4 weeks o f the
for those w h o are found to have impaired
study and monthly thereafter. T h e curriculum
glucose tolerance (ADA, 2 0 0 2 c ) . Although the
consisted o f general information about diet and
A D A ' s recommendations clearly acknowledge
exercise and behavioral interventions, including
the importance o f lifestyle interventions in the
self-monitoring, goal setting, stimulus control,
prevention o f D M 2 , the feasibility o f large-
problem solving, and relapse prevention train-
scale implementation o f these interventions
ing. Participants in the lifestyle intervention also
m a y be problematic within the existing health
attended group courses that focused on exer-
care system. F o r example, use o f an effective
cise, weight loss, and behavioral issues. Group
lifestyle intervention program requires a large
courses lasted 4 to 6 weeks, with additional
number o f well-coordinated resources over an
optional groups offered quarterly. Those in the
extended period o f time. Often, such resources
312
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS do not exist or are not accessible. Thus, future
individuals
research efforts should focus on h o w to trans-
self-monitor blood glucose. For individuals with
late the DPP and FDPS findings into cost-
D M 2 , glucose levels should fall between 8 0 and
effective interventions that can be feasibly
1 2 0 mg/dl before meals and between 1 0 0 and
implemented in real-world settings.
with
diabetes must
frequently
1 4 0 mg/dl at bedtime (ADA, 1 9 9 8 ) . It is also recommended that physicians check H b A ularly. H b A
Medical Interventions
l c
l c
reg-
is an average measure of blood
glucose for the previous 3 months and is an
T o prevent diabetes-related complications,
important
indicator o f metabolic control.
individuals with D M 2 must manage a c o m -
Individuals who do not have diabetes typically
plicated treatment regimen on a daily basis.
have an H b A
l c
percentage of less than 6 %
Such treatment regimens are individualized
(ADA, 1 9 9 8 ) . It is recommended that individu-
according to patients' medical needs
als with D M 2 maintain an H b A
and
l c
of between
resources. They typically consist o f a nutri-
7 % and 8 % . Diabetes-related complications are
tional regimen, an exercise program, and oral
greatly reduced though good metabolic control;
Nutritional
for every 0 . 9 % reduction in H b A , the risk of
plans generally consist o f eating prescribed
developing microvascular diabetic complica-
proportions
tions (eye, kidney, and nerve disease) is reduced
medication
(NIDDK,
2000).
o f protein,
fat,
and
carbo-
hydrates as well as fiber, cholesterol, and
lc
by approximately 2 5 % (ADA, 2 0 0 2 a ) .
sodium. It is also recommended that a regular exercise plan involve aerobic and musclestrengthening exercises. F o r many individuals
Preventive tions.
Screening
of Diabetes
Complica-
Preventive screening and practices is
with diabetes, changes in diet and exercise are
also a vital component o f the diabetes treat-
sufficient to achieve euglycemia (near-normal
ment regimen, particularly as it concerns
glucose levels). However, if diet and exercise
diabetes complications. As stated previously,
cannot adequately sustain euglycemia, oral
hypertension is c o m m o n among individuals
medications that decrease blood glucose are
with diabetes and so must be monitored
incorporated
regimen.
regularly. Adequate blood pressure control
Oral medications lower blood glucose either
can subsequently reduce cardiovascular dis-
into the treatment
by stimulating the pancreas to produce more
ease, such as heart disease and stroke, by
insulin or by increasing the efficiency o f
approximately 3 3 % to 5 0 % and can reduce
insulin use. W h e n oral medications are not
microvascular disease by approximately 3 3 %
sufficient to maintain euglycemia, exogenous
( N I D D K , 2 0 0 0 ) . Cholesterol and lipids must
insulin injections are integrated into the treat-
also be monitored among individuals with
ment regimen. Nearly 4 0 % o f individuals
D M 2 ; cardiovascular complications can be
with D M 2 are treated with insulin to improve
reduced by 2 0 % to 5 0 % through improved
metabolic control. Clearly, the responsibility
control o f cholesterol and lipids. Medications
for diabetes self-management rests on the
are commonly required to control cholesterol
individual with diabetes. Therefore, the pre-
and
ceding treatment regimens require a number
diabetes. In addition, preventive screenings for
o f substantial behavioral and lifestyle changes
diabetic eye disease are important.
for most individuals.
detection and treatment o f diabetic eye disease
blood
pressure
among
those
with Early
with laser therapy can reduce the development T h e fundamental pur-
of severe vision loss by an estimated 5 0 % to
pose of a diabetes treatment regimen is to
6 0 % . Similarly, early detection and treatment
achieve near-normal metabolic control. Thus,
of diabetic kidney disease can reduce the
Metabolic
Control.
Type 2 Diabetes
\
development o f kidney failure by 3 0 % to
It is important to recognize that diabetes
7 0 % . Finally, comprehensive foot care pro-
self-management is a complex, multidimen-
grams can reduce amputation rates by 4 5 % to
sional construct. Typically, individuals
85%
adherent to certain components of their treat-
(NIDDK, 2000).
Given the
multi-
are
systemic nature o f D M 2 , regular screenings
ment regimens but not to others. Indeed, per-
for these difficulties are clearly an essential
fect adherence to every aspect o f the treatment
component o f the treatment regimen.
regimen is extremely rare and (for many) unrealistic (Wysocki & Buckloh, 2 0 0 2 ) . Therefore, Regimen.
clinicians should identify the components of
There are a number o f diabetes-related adjust-
the treatment regimen to which patients are
Difficulties
With
the Medical
ment problems that are directly associated
adhering well and those to which they are not.
with attempts at treatment adherence. O n e
It is c o m m o n for individuals frustrated with
such frustration commonly expressed by indi-
one aspect o f their treatment regimen to gener-
viduals with diabetes is the sense o f being food
alize and conclude that they are "bad patients"
deprived (Rubin & Peyrot, 2 0 0 1 ) . Some indi-
or are "nonadherent" patients. Clinicians can
viduals may subsequently develop poor eating
help such individuals by identifying
small
habits, whereas others report that they do
specific goals to work toward. Subsequently,
not monitor their glucose as often as indi-
the treatment regimen will appear much more
cated. Adhering to monitoring glucose levels is
manageable to individuals with D M 2 .
also problematic. T w o commonly cited reasons for not monitoring blood glucose are the
Diabetes
Self-Management
Education.
pain associated with drawing blood and the
Diabetes self-management education ( D S M E )
inconvenience o f monitoring in certain settings.
equips individuals with the knowledge and
Still other individuals have negative responses
skills to manage their diabetes successfully.
to excessively low or high blood glucose read-
M e t a b o l i c control is potentially optimized,
ings (Rubin & Peyrot, 2 0 0 1 ) . Motivation to
and
adhere to the monitoring portion of the treat-
when the following information and skills are
future
complications are
minimized,
ment regimen is difficult to maintain when the
taught: nutritional
feedback is often negative in nature.
recommendations, self-monitoring o f blood
An additional problem often encountered
information,
exercise
glucose ( S M B G ) , insulin administration, and
by individuals with D M 2 is the frustration
managing
associated with unpredictable blood glucose
glycemic events. Given that patients perform
of
hypoglycemic
and
hyper-
levels, often leading to less active self-care. T o
approximately 9 5 % o f this daily care inde-
help alleviate frustration,
are
pendently (Anderson, 1 9 8 5 ) , D S M E is con-
encouraged to develop realistic standards and
sidered to be the central component o f the
expectations regarding diabetes outcomes.
medical treatment for those with diabetes.
individuals
Finally, the fear o f taking insulin can prevent
Despite the central nature o f D S M E , only
optimal management o f blood glucose levels.
4 0 % o f individuals with diabetes receive for-
Clinicians should attempt to identify specific
mal diabetes management education (U.S.
fears related to taking insulin, including the
Department o f Health and H u m a n Services,
pain associated with the injection, interfer-
1 9 9 8 ) , and 5 0 % to 8 0 % o f individuals have
ence
hypo-
severe deficits in their diabetes self-care
glycemia, and being treated differently by
knowledge (Clement, 1 9 9 5 ) . These estimates
with
lifestyle,
experiencing
others (Rubin & Peyrot, 2 0 0 1 ) . F o r a more
indicate that many individuals do not receive
thorough description o f the fears related to
adequate D S M E and suggest that the method
taking insulin, see Rubin and Peyrot ( 2 0 0 1 ) .
and delivery o f D S M E require improvement.
313
314
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS There is an extensive body o f literature that
psychological outcomes makes it quite difficult
evaluates various methods o f delivery and out-
to determine specific psychological factors
comes related to D S M E . Still, no particular
that
intervention method has been identified as
Furthermore, very few studies have reported
optimally efficacious. Because o f the multidi-
long-term treatment outcomes. This is notewor-
mensionality o f diabetes treatment regimens,
thy given that long-term follow-up studies are
D S M E interventions are differentially effective
less likely to find positive outcomes regarding
depending on the aspects o f the regimens that
glycémie and behavioral outcomes than are
influence
self-management
behaviors.
are assessed (Norris, Engelgau, & Narayan,
those with short-term follow-ups (Norris et al.,
2 0 0 1 ) . In other words,
are
2 0 0 1 ) . It may be that program interventions lead
generally effective in changing certain out-
to short-term behavioral change but that the
interventions
comes but not others. However, a recent
newly acquired behaviors do not necessarily
review article by Norris and colleagues ( 2 0 0 1 )
persist. Consequently, "refresher" sessions over
outlined several consistent findings that have
the course of the illness may be warranted. It is
emerged from the D S M E literature and are
unlikely that one intervention that improves all
summarized here.
self-management behaviors, and that is appro-
Specifically, short-term D S M E interventions
priate throughout the course of the illness, will be
have been found to be fairly effective in yielding
found. Therefore, future research needs to deter-
improvements in knowledge levels, S M B G
mine which types of interventions, or combina-
skills, and dietary habits but not in yielding
tions of interventions, are most effective for
improvements in weight loss and physical activ-
specific
ity levels (Norris et al., 2 0 0 1 ) . Group interven-
research has examined how adherence changes
outcomes. Moreover, virtually
no
improving
over time for adults with diabetes. Thus, longitu-
knowledge and S M B G and may be more effec-
dinal studies are needed to determine whether
tion
formats
are
effective
in
tive than individual formats for lifestyle inter-
self-management behaviors follow a develop-
ventions. However, it is important to note that
mental pattern.
improvements in knowledge or S M B G do not necessarily translate into improvements
in
glycémie control. Notably, interactive, individualized, repetitive interventions
are
more
effective in improving lipid levels than are
Psychological Interventions for Diabetes-Specific Problems Over the past 2 0 years, behavioral interven-
single-session or short-term interventions. In
tions for diabetes-specific problems
addition,
patient
shifted from knowledge- and education-based
participation or take a collaborative approach
interventions to patient-centered interventions
programs
that
promote
have
to instruction appear to be more effective than
emphasizing patient efficacy, esteem, and con-
didactic approaches in affecting outcomes such
trol (Glasgow et al., 1999).
as glycémie control, weight loss, and lipid pro-
interventions appear to be effective in improv-
Patient-centered
files. Finally, Norris and colleagues ( 2 0 0 1 ) con-
ing both physical and psychological health out-
cluded that, by and large, self-management
comes. Three empirically supported examples
interventions that have consistent reinforce-
of patient-centered
ment over long periods o f time are more
specific coping skills training, empowerment,
effective
than single-session or
short-term
interventions.
and
interventions—diabetes-
stress management
interventions—are
discussed here.
Norris and colleagues (2001) also pointed out limitations in the existing D S M E literature. They recognized that the dearth of research examining
Diabetes-Specific
Coping
Skills
Training.
Diabetes-specific coping skills training (DSCST)
Type 2 Diabetes is a cognitive-behavioral, psychoeducational
Stress
\
A number o f stress
Management.
group intervention designed to optimize emo-
management interventions have been devel-
tional functioning, diabetes self-management,
oped based on the strong theoretical link
and metabolic control (Rubin 8c Peyrot, 2 0 0 1 ) .
between stress and metabolic control. A variety
During the intervention, individuals are encour-
of techniques aimed at decreasing stress have
aged to identify patterns of self-care and barri-
been evaluated, including biofeedback (Surwit
ers to self-care and, subsequently, to problem
8c Feinglos, 1 9 8 8 ) , relaxation training (Surwit
solve strategies to address identified problems.
et al., 2 0 0 2 ) , and cognitive-behavioral strate-
A series of studies evaluating D S C S T have
gies (Henry, Wilson, Bruce, Chisholm, 8c
found the program to be effective in improving
Rawling, 1 9 9 7 ) . For instance, studies examin-
psychological outcomes such as depression,
ing the efficacy o f biofeedback-assisted relax-
anxiety, self-esteem, and diabetes self-efficacy.
ation training found significant improvements
Moreover, improvements have been observed
in various measures o f metabolic control,
in diabetes self-care (e.g., diet, exercise, diabetes
including improved glucose tolerance, reduced
knowledge, S M B G ) and metabolic control
long-term hyperglycemia (Lammers, Naliboff,
(Rubin, Peyrot, 8c Saudek, 1 9 8 9 , 1 9 9 1 , 1 9 9 3 ;
8c Straatmeyer, 1 9 8 4 ; Surwit 8c Feinglos,
Rubin, Waller, 8c Ellis, 1 9 9 0 ) . Several key ele-
1 9 8 8 ; Surwit, Ross, McCaskill, 8c Feinglos,
ments of diabetes-specific coping skills training
1 9 8 9 ) , postprandial (i.e., after eating) blood
were outlined by Rubin ( 2 0 0 0 ) , including indi-
glucose levels, and plasma Cortisol, as com-
vidualized treatment plans, problem specificity,
pared with control participants (Surwit 8c
goal setting, reinforcement, problem solving,
Feinglos, 1 9 8 3 ) . Overall, stress management
emotional coping skills, and family involve-
interventions show improvements in metabolic
ment (for a more detailed description, see
control parameters but have not been consis-
Rubin, 2 0 0 0 ) .
tent at reducing psychological distress (Henry et al., 1 9 9 7 ; Surwit et al., 2 0 0 2 ) . Future studies
Empowerment.
Patient empowerment inter-
ventions also appear to be effective in addressing many o f the diabetes-specific difficulties that frequently accompany D M 2 . Empower-
need to determine whether some individuals are more susceptible to the effects o f stress than are others and which types o f interventions effect specific stress-related outcomes.
ment interventions are designed to enhance goal setting, problem-solving, coping, stress management, social support, and self-motivation. Anderson and colleagues ( 1 9 9 5 ) evaluated
Technology-Based Interventions Currently,
greater
attention
has
been
an empowerment intervention that consisted of
directed toward developing cost-effective inter-
six 2-hour group sessions. At a 6-month follow-
ventions that can be easily disseminated. T o this
up, the treatment group demonstrated signifi-
end, Glasgow, Toobert, and Hampson (1996)
cant increases in self-efficacy
(setting goals,
evaluated a brief office-based computer inter-
solving problems, emotional coping, obtaining
vention aimed at improving diabetes self-
support, self-motivation, and decision making)
management. Individuals in the intervention
and positive diabetes-specific attitudes as well
completed a computerized assessment o f self-
as decreases in negative diabetes-specific atti-
management behaviors and then were provided
tudes. Small improvements were also observed
feedback on self-management, participated in
in glycémie control. This study lends further
goal-setting exercises, and selected individual-
support to the value o f patient-centered care
ized interventions. Participants also received a
that addresses the psychological aspects of
one-page feedback form outlining individual-
living with diabetes.
ized
obstacles
to
self-management.
The
315
316
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS computer-assisted
intervention
was
fairly
also
showed
greater
improvements
in
successful in producing dietary improvements
glycémie control than did the self-management
and serum cholesterol levels but did not yield
group. Thus, C B T appears to be an effective
significant improvements in glycémie control
treatment
or Q O L (Glasgow et a l , 1 9 9 6 ) . A later study
diabetes (Lustman et al., 1 9 9 8 ) .
for depression
for those
with
by Glasgow and Toobert ( 2 0 0 0 ) examined the
Although C B T has been shown to be effec-
effects o f a telephone follow-up on providing
tive in treating depression, the combination of
community resource information to the patient
behavioral interventions with
in addition to the computer-assisted interven-
medication may be clinically indicated in some
tion. Neither strategy improved the outcomes
cases. Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors
of the computer-based intervention. Given
such as fluoxitine are effective in treating
these findings, the expanding use of home com-
depressive symptoms and are associated with
puters, and the increasing need for cost-effective
improvements in glycémie control (Rubin &
antidepressant
health care, it is likely that use o f computer-
Peyrot, 1 9 9 4 ) . Furthermore, fewer than 1 0 % of
based interventions will continue to increase
patients with diabetes taking fluoxitine experi-
(Gonder-Frederick, C o x , & Ritterband, 2 0 0 2 ) .
ence significant side effects (Lustman et al., 2000).
Treating Psychological Distress As
Tricyclic
medications
have
also
been shown to decrease depressive symptoms (Turkinton, 1 9 8 0 ) ; however, the use of tricyclic
indicated previously, there is high
medication among those with D M 2 has been
comorbidity between diabetes and psycholog-
associated with adverse side effects, including
ical distress, especially depression and anxiety.
hyperglycemia,
Recommended
hypotension, and other cardiovascular events
treatment
approaches
for
depression and anxiety in the context o f D M 2
weight
gain,
orthostatic
(Lustman, Griffith, Gavard, & Clouse, 1 9 9 2 ) . Although the treatments for depression
are reviewed here.
just described appear to be effective, the conDepression.
Despite the high prevalence o f
clinically significant depressive
symptoms
stellation o f organic and psychosocial factors contributing to depression among those with
among individuals with diabetes, few treat-
diabetes may well be unique. Therefore,
ment outcome studies have been conducted.
researchers and clinicians should not assume
A notable exception is Lustman and col-
that the research findings in medically well
leagues' 1 9 9 8 study, which compared the
populations generalize to those with
efficacy o f cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT)
betes. Future research should focus on h o w
dia-
plus self-management training with that o f
cognitive processing mechanisms interface
self-management training alone in decreasing
with diabetes-specific experiences and subse-
depressive
quently
symptoms
among
individuals
influence
p s y c h o l o g i c a l distress.
with diabetes (Lustman, Griffith, Freeland,
During the interim, it is recommended that
Kissel, & Clouse, 1 9 9 8 ) . Remission o f depres-
clinicians use treatments for depression that
sion was observed at a 10-week follow-up; at
are empirically supported among medically
that time, 8 5 % o f those in the C B T group
well populations
had remitted as compared with 2 5 % o f those
therapy). Such interventions might be tai-
in the self-management group. Moreover,
lored to diabetes by including the identifica-
treatment effects persisted; at the 6-month
tion and modification o f thoughts and beliefs
follow-up, the rates o f depression remission
related to D M 2 (for a review o f empirically
were 7 0 % for the C B T group and 3 3 % for
supported treatments, see also DeRubeis &
the self-management group. T h e C B T group
Crits-Christoph, 1 9 9 8 ) .
(e.g., C B T , interpersonal
Type 2
Diabetes
CASE S T U D Y T o illustrate the potential role o f psychology in optimizing both physical and psychological outcomes, the following case study is presented. In this particular case, the individual has a number o f diabetes-specific problems as well as clinically significant psychological distress. " A . J . , " a 51-year-old male, presented to a diabetes clinic with a 6-year history o f D M 2 . Since being diagnosed with diabetes, he had closely adhered to his medication regimen but inconsistently practiced dietary and exercise recommendations. T h u s , A. J . was still able to maintain an acceptable (but not optimal) level o f glucose control throughout the first few years o f his illness. However, during the past year, A. J . had gained 3 0 pounds and his H b A
l c
value had risen to 1 0 . 5 % . In addi-
tion, a recent examination by his family physician revealed an ulcer on his left foot. Concerned by the rapid decline in A. J . ' s metabolic control, the physician incorporated insulin into his treatment plan and referred him to a health psychologist to facilitate his regimen adherence. During initial therapy sessions, A. J . admitted to having difficulty in adhering to his treatment regimen, citing large Sunday dinners when traditional family recipes high in fat and carbohydrates were served. H e stated that he refused to miss out on this special family time by declining to eat or eating off o f a different menu. Furthermore, he admitted that it was easier to ignore the problem than to change his lifestyle. H e stated that his exercise regimen consisted solely o f playing with his kids on the weekends and doing yard w o r k . In the third session, A. J . revealed that he had been laid off from his position at a local computer company approximately 4 months earlier. H e stated that after he was laid off, he did not feel like playing with his kids anymore, found it difficult to get up in the morning, and felt hopeless about the future. H e also reported that his wife had been nagging him more about "everything," including his diet, completing chores around the house, and being "grouchy." Like many clients that are referred to therapy, A. J . reported both diabetes-specific problems and general psychological distress. After a thorough assessment, he was diagnosed with major depressive disorder. Given A. J . ' s presentation, the focus o f therapy shifted from diabetes-specific problems to treating depression. A. J . was referred to a psychiatrist for a medication evaluation and was subsequently prescribed antidepressant medication. Psychological treatment consisted of C B T in which A. J . explored his beliefs related to himself, losing his j o b , and the difficulty in finding employment. W h e n appropriate, beliefs related to having diabetes were also explored, and where indicated, A. J . ' s beliefs were challenged and modified. Pleasant events scheduling was also conducted. After 3 months o f C B T and medication therapy, A. J . ' s symptoms o f depression had decreased substantially and he reported increased motivation to improve his diabetes management. Thus, a long-term treatment plan was implemented consisting o f monthly meetings with the health psychologist to discuss issues surrounding weight
317
318
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS
loss, exercise, and
behavioral issues as well as ongoing stress
management.
Specifically, A. J . learned ways in which to identify healthier alternatives for his favorite foods and instituted a regular exercise regimen consisting o f running and weightlifting. A. J . and his family also joined the local diabetes support group, where he shares with others what he has learned about coping with diabetes. As a result o f his efforts, A. J . lost 2 0 pounds and did not develop any additional diabetes complications. H e also reported that he felt more confident in his ability to manage his diabetes successfully.
Anxiety.
As mentioned previously, anxiety
symptoms are also c o m m o n among individuals with D M 2 (Peyrot &
Rubin,
effective
treatment
approach
for anxiety
symptoms in individuals with diabetes.
1997).
M o s t individuals with diabetes seeking
Remarkably, no validated randomized con-
psychological services will likely have both
trolled trials have evaluated the efficacy of
general distress (e.g., depression) and diabetes-
behavioral treatment interventions targeting
specific issues. W h e n treating clients with
anxiety among those with diabetes. A number
poor regimen adherence and high levels o f
o f authors have suggested that biofeedback -
psychological distress, it m a y be difficult to
assisted relaxation training (described earlier
determine the initial target o f intervention.
in the chapter), although not yet empirically
In the majority o f cases, it may be advisable
tested to target anxiety symptoms, may be an
to address general distress first (Rubin, 2 0 0 0 )
effective treatment for anxiety among those
because it is unlikely that individuals experi-
with diabetes. At this time, it is recommended
encing high levels o f distress are going to
that clinicians use treatments for anxiety that
have the organizational ability or motivation
have been empirically supported among med-
necessary to effect diabetes-specific behav-
ically well populations (for a review o f empir-
ioral changes. As levels o f general psycholog-
ically supported treatments, see DeRubeis &
ical distress decrease, diabetes-specific issues
Crits-Christoph, 1 9 9 8 ) .
can be addressed more effectively.
Studies examining the effectiveness o f pharmacological
treatments
for
anxiety
among those with D M 2 are limited. H o w -
CONCLUSIONS
ever, initial reports suggest that pharmacological treatments may be potentially effective
D M 2 is a chronic illness characterized by a
(Lustman et al., 1 9 9 5 ) . F o r example, individ-
dysregulation o f blood glucose levels. D M 2
uals treated with alprazolam ( X a n a x ) dis-
requires individuals to manage a complex
played improvements
treatment regimen so as to prevent severe med-
in glycémie control
when taking this medication regardless o f a
ical complications. Fortunately, it is a poten-
formal diagnosis o f anxiety (Lustman et al.,
tially manageable disease, and many of the
1 9 9 5 ) . Although results are promising, fur-
negative physical and psychological health
ther research is needed to determine the most
sequelae are preventable. However, preventing
adverse
diabetes
outcomes
requires
that
individuals with diabetes make multiple behav-
colleagues ( 2 0 0 1 )
Type 2 Diabetes
\
advocated the need
for
public policy and community-based interven-
ioral and lifestyle changes, a task that over-
tions, citing the role o f environmental factors
whelms many. Because o f the unpredictable
in the development and prevention o f obesity
and complex nature o f D M 2 , interventions
and D M 2 . High-fat, high-calorie foods and
may be necessary to facilitate these behavioral
sedentary activities, such as watching television
changes and to promote optimal health out-
and using computers, pervade contemporary
comes. A number o f behavioral interventions
society and shape health-related behaviors and
have proved to be effective in preventing D M 2
outcomes. Thus, studies are needed to further
as well as in improving the psychological and
delineate which environmental variables influ-
physical outcomes o f those w h o have diabetes. Although great progress has been made in understanding the behavioral aspects of D M 2 during the past two decades, a number of areas
ence eating and physical activity and
to
develop community-based interventions that promote positive health behaviors. Importantly, the development
o f more
are in need o f further investigation. First, there
cost-effective interventions will also require
are virtually no longitudinal studies examining
the role o f behavioral scientists to change
the developmental course of self-management
(Gonder-Frederick et al., 2 0 0 2 ) . Rather than
among adults. Furthermore, additional research
providing direct care, behavioral scientists will
should address the cultural-specific impact o f
increasingly provide training and supervision
D M 2 . N o t only are certain cultural groups at
to other health care professionals who directly
high risk for D M 2 , but research suggests that
assess and implement behavioral
there also are potential culture-specific patterns
tions. As a result, behavioral scientists will
of illness appraisal and coping strategies. In
also be required to further examine the role o f
addition, studies that will help to determine the
the provider-patient relationship in influenc-
most effective treatments for clinically signifi-
ing psychological and physical outcomes and
cant psychological distress among individuals
to develop interventions targeting health care
with diabetes, specifically depression and anx-
providers accordingly. In sum, the fundamen-
iety, are warranted.
tal challenge for behavioral scientists will be to
interven-
During the coming decade, there will be an
translate available findings into feasible, cost-
increasing need for cost-effective interventions
effective interventions that can be widely dis-
aimed at preventing and treating obesity and
seminated to psychologists and health care
D M 2 . An exceptional review by Wing and
professionals (Gonder-Frederick et al., 2 0 0 2 ) .
REFERENCES Agurs-Collins, T. D., Kumanyika, S. K., Ten Have, T. R., & Adams-Campbell, L. L. (1997). A randomized controlled trial of weight reduction and exercise for diabetes management in older African-American subjects. Diabetes Care, 20, 1503-1512. American Diabetes Association. (1998). Medical management of type 2 diabetes (4th ed., Clinical Education Series). Alexandria, VA: Author. American Diabetes Association. (2002a). Implications of the United Kingdom Prospective Diabetes Study. Diabetes Care, 25(1), S 2 8 - S 3 2 . American Diabetes Association. (2002b, March). Outreach programs and public [Online]. Retrieved on May 10, 2 0 0 2 , from www. awareness campaigns. diabetes.org/main/application/commercewf?origin=*'jsp&event=link(D57)
319
320
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS American Diabetes Association. (2002c). The prevention or delay of Type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care, 25, 7 4 2 - 7 4 9 . Anderson, R. J . , Freeland, Κ. E., Clouse, R. E., & Lustman, P. J . (2001). The prevalence of comorbid depression in adults with diabetes: A meta-analysis. Diabetes Care, 24, 1 0 6 9 - 1 0 7 8 . Anderson, R. M . (1985). Is the problem of compliance all in our heads? Diabetes Educator, 11, 3 1 - 3 4 . Anderson, R. M., Funnell, M . M., Butler, P. M., Arnold, M . S., Fitzgerald, J . T., & Feste, C. C. (1995). Patient empowerment. Diabetes Care, 18, 9 4 3 - 9 4 9 . Bernbaum, M., Alpert, S. G., & Duckro, P. N. (1988). Psychosocial profiles of patients with visual impairment due to diabetic retinopathy. Diabetes Care, 11, 551-557. Boyle, J . P., Honeycutt, Α. Α., Narayan, Κ. M . V., Hoerger, T. J . , Geiss, L. S., Chen, H., & Thompson, T. J . (2001). Projection of diabetes burden through 2 0 5 0 : Impact of changing demography and disease prevalence in the U.S. Diabetes Care, 24, 1 9 3 6 - 1 9 4 0 . Clement, S. (1995). Diabetes self-management education. Diabetes Care, 18,1204-1214. Connell, C. M., Davis, W. K., Gallant, M. P., & Sharpe, P. A. (1994). Impact of social support, social cognitive variables, and perceived threat on depression among adults with diabetes. Health Psychology, 13, 2 6 3 - 2 7 3 . Cowie, C. C , Port, F. K., Wolfe, R. Α., Savage, P. J . , Moll, P. P., & Hawthorne, V. M. (1989). Disparities in incidence of diabetic end-stage renal disease by race and type of diabetes. New England Journal of Medicine, 321, 1 0 7 4 - 1 0 7 9 . DeFronzo, R. Α., Bonadonna, R. C , & Ferrannini, E. (1992). Pathogenesis of NIDDM. Diabetes Care, 15, 3 1 8 - 3 6 8 . Demmers, R. Y . , Neale, Α. V., Wensloff, N. J . , Gronsman, K. J . , & Jaber, L. A. (1989). Glycosylated hemoglobin levels and self-reported stress in adults with diabetes. Behavioral Medicine, 15, 1 6 7 - 1 7 2 . DeRubeis, R. J . , & Crits-Christoph, P. (1998). Empirically supported individual and group psychological treatments for adult mental disorders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 3 7 - 5 2 . Devins, G. M., Hunsley, J . , Mandin, H., Taub, K. J . , & Paul, L. C. (1997). The marital context of end-stage renal disease: Illness intrusiveness and perceived changes in family environment. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 19, 3 2 5 - 3 3 2 . Diabetes Prevention Program Research Group. (1999). The Diabetes Prevention Program: Design and methods for a clinical trial in the prevention of Type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care, 22, 6 2 3 - 6 3 4 . Diabetes Prevention Program Research Group. (2000). The Diabetes Prevention Program: Baseline characteristics of the randomized cohort. Diabetes Care, 23, 1619-1629. Diabetes Prevention Program Research Group. (2002). Reduction in the evidence of Type 2 diabetes with lifestyle intervention or metformin. New England Journal of Medicine, 346, 3 9 3 - 4 0 3 . Egede, L. E., Zheng, D., & Simpson, K. (2002). Comorbid depression is associated with increased health care use and expenditures in individuals with diabetes. Diabetes Care, 25, 4 6 4 - 4 7 0 . Franklin, G. M., Kahn, L. B., Baxter, J . , Marshall, J . Α., & Hamman, R. F. (1990). Sensory neuropathy in non-insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus: The San Luis Valley Diabetes Study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 131, 6 3 3 - 6 4 3 . Garay-Sevilla, M. E., Nava, L. E., Malacara, J . M., Huerta, R., de Leon, J . D., Mena, Α., & Fajardo, Μ. Ε. (1995). Adherence to treatment and social support in patients with non-insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus. Journal of Diabetes and Its Complications, 9, 8 1 - 8 6 .
Type 2 Glasgow, R. E., Fisher, Ε. B., Anderson, B. J . , LaGreca, Α., Marrero, D., Johnson, S. B., Rubin, R. R., 8c Cox, D. J . (1999). Behavioral sciences in diabetes. Diabetes Care, 22, 8 3 2 - 8 4 3 . Glasgow, R. E., 8c Toobert, D. J . (2000). Brief, computer assisted diabetes dietary self-management counseling: Effects on behavior, physiologic outcomes, and quality of life. Medical Care, 38, 1 0 6 2 - 1 0 7 3 . Glasgow, R. E., Toobert, D. J . , Se Hampson, S. E. (1996). Effects of a brief officebased intervention to facilitate diabetes dietary self-management. Diabetes Care, 19, 8 3 5 - 8 4 2 . Goetsch, V. L., Sc Wiebe, D. J . (1995). Diabetes mellitus: Considerations of the influence of stress. In A. J . Goreczny (Ed.), Handbook of health and rehabilitation psychology (pp. 5 1 3 - 5 3 4 ) . New York: Plenum. Gonder-Frederick, L. Α., Cox, D. J . , Se Ritterband, L. M . (2002). Diabetes and behavioral medicine: The second decade. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 6 1 1 - 6 2 5 . Griffith, L. S., Field, B. J . , Se Lustman, P. J . (1990). Life stress and social support in diabetes: Association with glycémie control. International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 20, 3 6 5 - 3 7 2 . Harris, M . I. (1998). Diabetes in America: Epidemiology and scope of the problem. Diabetes Care, 21(Suppl. 3), C 1 1 - C 1 4 . Harris, M. I. (2001). Racial and ethnic differences in health care access and health outcomes for adults with Type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care, 24, 454—459. Harris, M . I., Klein, R., Cowie, C. C , Rowland, M., Se Byrd-Holt, D. D. (1998). Is the risk of diabetic retinopathy greater in non-Hispanic African-Americans and Mexican-Americans than in non-Hispanic Caucasians with Type 2 diabetes? A U.S. population study. Diabetes Care, 21, 1 2 3 0 - 1 2 3 5 . Henry, J . L., Wilson, P. H , Bruce, D. G., Chisholm, D. J . , Se Rawling, P. J . (1997). Cognitive-behavioral stress management for patients with non-insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus. Psychology, Health, & Medicine, 2, 1 0 9 - 1 1 8 . Hillier, T. Α., Se Pedula, K. L. (2001). Characteristics of an adult population with newly diagnosed Type 2 diabetes: The relation of obesity and age of onset. Diabetes Care, 24, 1 5 2 2 - 1 5 2 7 . Jacobson, A. M., deGroot, M., Sc Samson, J . A. (1994). The evaluation of two measures of quality of life in patients with Type 1 and Type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care, 17, 2 6 7 - 2 7 4 . Kovacs, M., Brent, D., Steinberg, T. F., Paulauskas, S., Sc Reid, J . (1986). Children's self-reports of psychologic adjustment and coping strategies during first year of insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus. Diabetes Care, 9, 4 7 2 - 4 7 9 . Kuhn, C , Cochrane, C , Feinglos, M., Sc Surwit, R. (1987). Exaggerated peripheral responsivity to catecholamine contributes to stress-induced hyperglycemia in the ob/ob mouse. Physiology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 25, 4 9 1 - 4 9 5 . Lammers, C. Α., Naliboff, B. D., 8c Straatmeyer, A. J . (1984). The effects of progressive muscle relaxation on stress and diabetic control. Behavior Research Therapy, 22, 6 4 1 - 6 5 0 . Littlefield, C. H., Rodin, G. M., Murray, Μ. Α., Sc Craven, J . L. (1990). Influence of functional impairment and social support on depressive symptoms in persons with diabetes. Health Psychology, 9, 7 3 7 - 7 4 9 . Lustman, P. J . , Anderson, R. J . , Freeland, Κ. E., De Groot, M., Carney R. M., Sc Clouse, R. E. (2000). Depression and poor glycémie control: A meta-analytic review of the literature. Diabetes Care, 23, 9 3 4 - 9 4 3 . Lustman, P. J . , Clouse, R. E., Alrakawi, Α., Rubin, E. H., Sc Gelenberg, A. J . (1997). Treatment of major depression in adults with diabetes: A primary care perspective. Clinical Diabetes, 15, 1 2 2 - 1 2 6 .
Diabetes
322
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS Lustman, P. J . , Griffith, L. S., Clouse, R. E., Freeland, K. E., Eisen, S. Α., Rubin, Ε. H., Carney, R. M., & McGill, J . B. (1995). Effects of alprazolam on glucose regulation in diabetes. Diabetes Care, 18, 1 1 3 3 - 1 1 3 9 . Lustman, P. J . , Griffith, L. S., Freeland, Κ. E., Kissel, S. S., & Clouse, R. E. (1998). Cognitive behavioral therapy for depression in Type 2 diabetes mellitus: A randomized, controlled trial. Annals of Internal Medicine, 129, 6 1 3 - 6 2 1 . Lustman, P. J . , Griffith, L. S., Gavard, J . Α., & Clouse, R. E. (1992). Depression in adults with diabetes. Diabetes Care, 15, 1 6 3 1 - 1 6 3 9 . Maggio, C. Α., & Pi-Sunyer, F. X . (1997). The prevention and treatment of obesity: Application to Type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care, 20, 1 7 4 4 - 1 7 6 6 . Manson, J . , & Spelsberg, A. (1994). Primary prevention of non-insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 10, 1 7 2 - 1 8 4 . Marlatt, G. Α., & Gordon, J . R. (1985). Relapse prevention: Maintenance strategies and addictive behavior change. New York: Guilford. Mayer-Davis, E. J . , Antonio, A. D., Martin, M., Wandersman, Α., Parra-Medina, D., &c Schulz, R. (2001). Pilot study of strategies for effective weight management in Type 2 diabetes: Pounds Off With Empowerment (POWER). Family and Community Health, 24(2), 2 7 - 3 5 . McCulloch, D. K., Hosking, D. J . , & Tobert, A. (1986). A pragmatic approach to sexual dysfunction in diabetic men: Psychosexual counseling. Diabetes Medicine, 3, 4 8 5 - 4 8 9 . Morisaki, N., Watanabe, S., Kobayashi, J . , Kanzaki, T., Takahashi, K., Yokote, K., Tezuka, M., Tashiro, J . , Inadera, H., & Saito, Y . (1994). Diabetic control and progression of retinopathy in elderly patients: Five year follow-up. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 41, 1 4 2 - 1 4 5 . National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases. (2000). National diabetes statistics fact sheet: General information and national estimates on diabetes in the United States. Bethesda, M D : National Institutes of Health. Norris, S. L., Engelgau, M . M., & Narayan, Κ. M. V. (2001). Effectiveness of selfmanagement training in Type 2 diabetes: A systematic review of randomized controlled trials. Diabetes Care, 24, 5 6 1 - 5 8 7 . Owen, K., Ayers, S., Corbett, S., & Hattersley, A. (2002). Increased risk of diabetes in first-degree relatives of young-onset Type 2 diabetic patients compared with relatives of those diagnosed later. Diabetes Care, 25, 6 3 6 - 6 3 7 . Peterson, T., Lee, P., Young, B., Newton, P., & Doran, T. (1998). Well-being and treatment satisfaction in older people with diabetes. Diabetes Care, 21, 930-935. Peyrot, M., & McMurry, J . F. (1985). Psychosocial factors in diabetes control: Adjustment of insulin treated adults. Psychosomatic Medicine, 47, 5 4 2 - 5 4 7 . Peyrot, M . F., & McMurry, J . F., Jr. (1992). Stress buffering and glycémie control. Diabetes Care, 15, 8 4 2 - 8 4 6 . Peyrot, M . F., McMurry, J . F., & Davida, F. K. (1999). A biopsychosocial model of glycémie control in diabetes: Stress, coping, and regimen adherence. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 40, 1 4 1 - 1 5 8 . Peyrot, M. F., & Rubin, R. R. (1994). Psychosocial problems in diabetes treatment: Impediments to intensive self-care. Practical Diabetology, 13, 8 - 1 4 . Peyrot, M. F., & Rubin, R. R. (1997). Levels and risks of depression and anxiety symptomatology among diabetic adults. Diabetes Care, 20, 5 8 5 - 5 9 0 . Ray, N. F., Thamer, M., Gardner, E., & Chan, J . K. (1998). Economic consequences of diabetes mellitus in the U.S. in 1997. Diabetes Care, 21, 2 9 6 - 3 0 6 . Resnick, H. E., Valsania, P., Sc Phillips, C. L. (1999). Diabetes mellitus and nontraumatic lower extremity amputation in African-American and Caucasian Americans: the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey
Type 2 Diabetes Epidemiologic Follow-up Study, 1 9 7 1 - 1 9 9 2 . Archives of Internal Medicine, 159, 2 4 7 0 - 2 4 7 5 . Rubin, R. R. (2000). Psychotherapy and counseling in diabetes mellitus. In F. J . Snoek Se T. C. Skinner (Eds.), Psychology in diabetes care (pp. 2 3 5 - 2 6 3 ) . New York: John Wiley. Rubin, R. R., Sc Peyrot, M . (1994). Psychosocial problems in diabetes treatment: Impediments to intensive self-care. Practical Diabetology, 13, 8 - 1 4 . Rubin, R. R., Sc Peyrot, M . (1998). Men and diabetes: Psychosocial and behavioral issues. Diabetes Spectrum, 11, 8 1 - 8 7 . Rubin, R. R., Sc Peyrot, M . (1999). Quality of life and diabetes. Diabetes Metabolism Research Reviews, 15, 2 0 5 - 2 1 8 . Rubin, R. R., & Peyrot, M. (2001). Psychological issues and treatments for people with diabetes. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 57, 4 5 7 - 4 7 8 . Rubin, R. R., Peyrot, M., & Saudek, C. D. (1989). Effect of diabetes education on selfcare, metabolic control, and emotional well-being. Diabetes Care, 12, 6 7 3 - 6 7 9 . Rubin, R. R., Peyrot, M., Sc Saudek, C. D. (1991). Differential effect of diabetes education on self-regulation and lifestyle behaviors. Diabetes Care, 14, 3 3 5 - 3 3 8 . Rubin, R. R., Peyrot, M., Sc Saudek, C. D. (1993). The effect of diabetes education program incorporating coping skills training on emotional well-being and diabetes self-efficacy. Diabetes Educator, 19, 2 1 0 - 2 1 4 . Rubin, R. R., Waller, S., & Ellis, A. (1990). Living with diabetes: A rationalemotive therapy perspective. Journal of Rational-Emotive Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy, 8, 2 1 - 3 9 . Samuel-Hodge, C. D., Headen, S. W., Skelly, A. H., Ingram, A. F., Keyserling, T. C , Jackson, E. J . , Ammerman, A. S., & Elasy, T. A. (2000). Influences on day to day management of Type 2 diabetes among African-American women. Diabetes Care, 23, 9 2 8 - 9 3 4 . Schreiner-Engel, P., Schiavi, R. C , Vietorisz, D., 8c Smith, H. (1987). The differential impact of diabetes type on female sexuality. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 31, 2 3 - 3 3 . Surwit, R. S., & Feinglos, M. N. (1983). The effects of relaxation on glucose tolerance in non-insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus. Diabetes Care, 6, 1 7 6 - 1 7 9 . Surwit, R. S., & Feinglos, M . N. (1988). Stress and autonomic nervous system in Type II diabetes: A hypothesis. Diabetes Care, 11, 8 3 - 8 5 . Surwit, R. S., Feinglos, M . N., Livingston, E. G., Kuhn, C. M., & McCubbin, J . A. (1984). Behavioral manipulation of the diabetic phenotype in ob/ob mice. Diabetes, 33, 6 1 6 - 6 1 8 . Surwit, R. S., McCubbin, J . Α., Kuhn, C. M., McGee, D., Gerstenfeld, D. Α., & Feinglos, M . N. (1986). Alprazolam reduces stress hyperglycemia in ob/ob mice. Psychosomatic Medicine, 48, 2 7 8 - 2 8 2 . Surwit, R. S., Ross, S. L., McCaskill, C. C , & Feinglos, M . N. (1989). Does relaxation therapy add to conventional treatment of diabetes mellitus? Diabetes, 3S(Suppl. 1), A9. Surwit, R. S., van Tilburg, M. A. L., Zucker, N., McCaskill, C. C , Parekh, P., Feinglos, M . N., Edwards, C. L., Williams, P., 8c Lane, J . D. (2002). Stress management improves long-term glycémie control in Type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care, 2 5 , 3 0 - 3 4 . Talbot, F., Nouwen, Α., Gingras, J . , Bélanger, Α., & Audet, J . (1999). Relations of diabetes intrusiveness and personal control to symptoms of depression among adults with diabetes. Health Psychology, 18, 5 3 7 - 5 4 2 . Trief, P. M., Grant, W., Elbert, K., & Weinstock, R. S. (1998). Family environment, glycémie control, and the psychosocial adaptation of adults with diabetes. Diabetes Care, 21, 2 4 1 - 2 4 5 .
\
323
324
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS Tuomilehto, J . , Lindstrom, J . , Eriksson, J . G., Valle, T. T., Hamalainen, H., Ilanne-Parikka, P., Keinanen-Kiukaanniemi, S., Laakso, M., Louheranta, Α., Rastas, M., Salminen, V., & Uusitupa, M . (2001). Prevention of type 2 diabetes mellitus by changes in lifestyle among subjects with impaired glucose tolerance. New England Journal of Medicine, 344, 1 3 4 3 - 1 3 5 0 . Turkinton, R. W. (1980). Depression masquerading as diabetic neuropathy, Journal of the American Medical Association, 243, 1 1 4 7 - 1 1 5 0 . U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (1998). Diabetes: A serious public health problem. At a Glance, pp. 1-4. (Washington, DC: Author) Wang, C. Y . , &C Fenske, M. M . (1996). Self-care of adults with non-insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus: Influence of family and friends. The Diabetes Educator, 22, 465-470. Wing, R. R., Goldstein, M . G., Acton, K. J . , Birch, L. L., Jakicic, J . M., Sallis, J . F., & Smith-West, D. (2001). Behavioral science research in diabetes: Lifestyle changes related to obesity, eating behavior, and physical activity. Diabetes Care, 24, 1 1 7 - 1 2 3 . Wulsin, L., &C Jacobson, A. M . (1989). Visual and psychological function in PDR [abstract]. Diabetes, 3S(Suppl. 1), A242. Wulsin, L., Jacobson, A. M., & Rand, L. I. (1987). Psychosocial aspects of diabetes retinopathy. Diabetes Care, 10, 3 6 7 - 3 7 3 . Wysocki, T., & Buckloh, L. M. (2002). Endocrine, metabolic, nutritional, and immune disorders. In S. Bennett Johnson, N. W. Perry, Jr., & R. H. Rozensky (Eds.), Handbook of clinical health psychology (pp. 6 5 - 9 9 ) . Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
CHAPTER
17 10
Psycho-oncology SHULAMITH KREITLER
C
ancer is a c o m m o n l y dreaded disease
of the malignant cells and secondarily on an
that is the second leading cause o f
estimate o f their growth rate). T h e major treat-
death
The
ment modalities are surgery, radiation therapy,
incidence o f cancer, especially o f specific
chemotherapy, immunomodulation, and bone
in the Western world.
diagnoses (e.g., breast, m e l a n o m a , thyroid,
marrow transplantation. Treatment may last
esophagus, liver), has increased during recent
for months and may need to be administered
years, even when improvements in diagnosis
repeatedly. Following the initial diagnosis of the
and age-related trends are considered ( S E E R
disease and its treatment, remission may set in
Program, 2 0 0 2 ) . I f current trends continue,
for differential periods o f time. T h e disease-free
cancer diagnoses
interval ends if the disease recurs. Recurrence
are expected to
double
over the next 5 0 years (Hoyert, K o c h a n e k , &
usually denotes a deterioration but not neces-
M u r p h y , 1 9 9 9 ) , with m o r e than 1.3 million
sarily death.
new cases o f cancer diagnosed annually in the United States alone (Garfinkel, 1 9 9 5 ) .
Cancer affects individuals of both genders and o f all ages and ethnic backgrounds, albeit
Cancer denotes a family o f diagnoses that
to different degrees, so that specific cancers
may affect different body sites, including the
may be more prevalent in individuals o f a par-
breasts, prostate, lungs, brain, gastrointestinal
ticular gender, ethnic background, geographi-
organs, skin, soft tissues, and blood. It mostly
cal area, or age group. T h e pathogen o f cancer
consists of a tumor in a specific site but may
has not been identified, although several risk
spread to other sites (i.e., metastases), and this
factors are known, ranging from genetic back-
is one of the reasons why it is commonly con-
ground to more behavioral factors such as
sidered as a systemic disease. Based on the size
smoking, exposure to the sun, contact with
of the primary lesion and the spread o f the
particular carcinogens, and diets that
disease at diagnosis, different stages (mostly
increase the incidence o f particular cancers
four with subdivisions) are identified, reflecting
(Garfinkel, 1 9 9 5 ) .
may
increasing severity. In distinction from staging,
Psycho-oncology is the discipline o f health
tumor grading indicates the similarity of the
psychology that deals with the psychological
tumor cells to their normal tissue counterparts
aspects of cancer. It is often considered as one
(based primarily on the degree of differentiation
of the most comprehensively studied domains 325
326
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS of health psychology. This is probably due to
security grounded in characteristic attitudes
the high levels of distress bound with cancer.
and beliefs, entrenched coping styles of passivity
Cancer is an anxiety-provoking disease that
and denial, and preexisting emotional difficulties
readily evokes the connotations o f suffering
(Green, Rimer, & Elwood, 1981). As a result,
and death. For patients, cancer is often associ-
cancer is often diagnosed in a later stage than
ated with considerable physical and psycho-
would have been possible and beneficial for the
logical suffering (e.g., pain, fatigue, and diverse
patient. The delayed diagnosis not only may
symptoms that may be debilitating
result in a compromising prognosis but also
either
temporarily or permanently) that may affect
may
most of the domains of their lives and last for
sequelae: sense of guilt and loss o f the sense o f
have
two
significant
psychological
long periods. Cancer also poses difficulties for
physical security and confidence (Kreitler,
health professionals. In particular, physicians
Chaitchik, &
Kreitler, 1 9 9 0 ) . T h e former
often accompany patients for long periods o f
reflects thoughts such as the following: "In ret-
time, sometimes without being able to offer
rospect, I can see that if I had paid more atten-
the expected and desired recovery or even the
tion to myself, I could have avoided
required extent of palliation to moderate the
catastrophe." The latter reflects thoughts such as
patients' pain and suffering. This state of
the following: " H o w come I did not know for so
affairs has enhanced awareness for the poten-
long that something is wrong with my body?"
the
tial contributions o f psychology in oncology
and " H o w could I be sure now that I am not
and has opened many venues for clinical and
terribly sick, although I feel nothing is wrong?"
research psychologists in this domain.
Diagnosis O V E R V I E W O F SPECIAL
Because of the alarming connotations of
CRISES A N D P R O B L E M S
cancer, the diagnosis of cancer often has a shocking effect on the patient. Even the mere
It is o f special importance to identify the
initiation o f a series o f tests in response to the
crisis situations and problems characteristic
patient's complaints may evoke increased anx-
of cancer patients. These form special foci for
iety and a sense of life being set " o n hold" until the tests are concluded. If the diagnosis of
intervention and research.
cancer arrives, the shock may be so great that even the risk o f suicide cannot be ruled out
Prevention
(Campbell, 1 9 9 6 ) . This state of increased stress
Despite knowledge that early
diagnosis
and panic is usually resolved partly through
and treatment of most cancer types facilitates
denial and mainly through speeded-up transfer
treatment and enhances survival rates, many
to the stage o f treatments in which both the
individuals
patient and the physician are interested.
do not undergo
the medically
recommended periodical tests for the early detection of cancer and might even overlook early symptoms if these occur. This is mostly due not to lack o f information on the part o f
Decision Making Following diagnosis, the patient is often
patients or physicians but rather to psycholog-
called on t o participate
ical factors, such as excessive fear of cancer,
about the kind of treatment (surgery, kind o f
in the
decisions
that may even reach the level of the psychiatri-
surgery, chemotherapy, specific protocol of
cally meaningful syndrome o f cancerophobia
chemotherapy,
(Ingelfinger, 1 9 7 5 ) , a false sense of physical
tary/alternative treatment, each singly or in
radiation,
and
complemen-
Psycho-oncology
\
combination), the place o f treatment (which
Chaitchik, Algor, &c Weissler, 1 9 9 3 ; Sneeuw,
hospital or clinic and sometimes in which
Aaronson, & Yarnold, 1 9 9 2 ) .
country),
and
treatment
Chemotherapy may be administered as a
(which doctor). T h e tendency to transfer to
major therapeutic agent for controlling the
patients part o f the responsibility for the deci-
disease and promoting remission, as adjuvant
sions is in accord with the generally increased
treatment subsequent to surgery for eradicat-
empowerment
the
agent
of
in
ing presumptive micrometastases, or as neoad-
for
juvant treatment for reducing the tumor prior
patients because they mostly do not possess
to surgery. Chemotherapy mostly consists o f
treatment
o f patients. Participating
decisions is often
enough expert knowledge
difficult
in the
relevant
a combination o f drugs that are administered
domains. T h e difficulty is further increased by
through infusion (or sometimes tablets), in the
the large amounts of information that need to
hospital or clinic (or sometimes at home),
be mastered, the time pressure, the awareness
according to a certain regimen (e.g., once every
of the momentous importance o f the conse-
2 or 3 weeks) over months.
quences o f the involved decisions, the nearly
may adversely affect the patient's quality of life
Chemotherapy
continuous anxiety, and the overall emotional
( Q O L ) . T h e most salient effects are due to the
stress. Patients who tend to withdraw from the
toxicity o f the drugs themselves. Some o f these
task of deciding are often exposed to the pres-
effects
sure from friends and family members, who
dementia, delirium, lethargy, depression, and
remind the patients o f the importance of taking
even psychosis that may be evoked by pred-
are
neuropsychological,
including
responsibility for their health and may con-
nisone as well as delirium following the use o f
tinue to question the recommendations of their
vincristine or cisplatin. In addition, the drugs
doctors and the patients' initial tentative deci-
may cause nausea and vomiting, alopecia (e.g.,
sions. T h e problems o f decision making may
adriamycin), weight changes (gain or loss),
reemerge during later stages of the disease.
insomnia, gonadal
dysfunction,
difficulties
with concentration and short-term memory, and disorders of fertility and sexuality. T h e
Undergoing Treatments
short-term effects are mostly stronger followdiffi-
ing the administration o f the drugs and get
often
weaker after a few days. T h e side effects
include surgery, chemotherapy, and radiation.
together with the generalized weakness and
Treatments in oncology are both cult
and
mostly
protracted.
They
Surgery m a y be undertaken for
different
fatigue disrupt the patient's
daily
routine
purposes: diagnostic (i.e., biopsy), therapeutic
and render it difficult for the patient to keep
(i.e., removal o f tumor or metastases), recon-
up work or carry out planned commitments
structive (e.g., esophagus, bladder, breast), or
(Clark & Fallowfield, 1 9 8 6 ) .
palliative (e.g., biliary or urethral diversions,
Radiation may be undertaken as a major
pleurodesis or analgesic such as cordotomy).
therapeutic agent, as an adjunct to chemother-
Surgery is often accompanied by intense anxi-
apy designed to minimize the chance of recur-
ety, especially if it is diagnostic or results in
rence, or for palliative and analgesic purposes.
bodily changes such as stoma, loss of an organ
It is often o f shorter duration than chemo-
(e.g., breast, limb), loss o f a function (e.g.,
therapy but may also be accompanied by side
talking, walking), or visible
effects
disfigurement
(e.g., nausea, fatigue, or anorexia,
(e.g., face, neck). Changes of this kind are
depending on the site, dose, and volume of
often accompanied by changes in body image
treatment).
that reflect on the patient's sense o f selfesteem and self-identity (Rapoport, Kreitler,
Other frequent treatments include immunotherapy
(e.g., interferon), with side
effects
327
328
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS similar to those of chemotherapy, and bone
desire t o reestablish their daily
marrow transplantation, which involves isola-
patients may minimize their references to the
tion
disease and put on a cheerful mask o f "busi-
and
possible medical complications
routines,
often accompanied by serious psychological
ness as usual." This may enhance their isola-
responses (e.g., intense anxiety, psychotic
tion and speed up the process o f patients'
symptoms) (Andrykowski, 1 9 9 4 ) .
"burnout" (Kreitler & Chaitchik, 1 9 9 3 ) .
In the course o f treatments, the patient is highly vulnerable and needs a lot o f support and encouragement to overcome the difficulties. However, getting treatment makes the
Disease Recurrence and Deterioration
patient feel that there is hope for recovery,
Disease recurrence is a particularly difficult
thereby contributing to improving the patient's
phase for patients because it represents a shat-
Q O L (Kovner &c Kreitler, 1 9 9 6 ) .
tering of their hopes for complete recovery or
Being a Chronic Cancer Patient in Remission
necessity to undergo a series of treatments,
at least ensured survival. It also indicates the
Although remission is a positive state from
some o f which may be more difficult than those during the initial phase. T h e situation may be exacerbated through the appearance of differ-
the medical point o f view, it is not necessarily
ent physical symptoms signaling deterioration.
so psychologically. During the first stage o f
As a result, patients may find it difficult to
remission, patients may feel anxious because
avoid thinking about death as a real possibility.
of the need to learn to function on their own
Hence, fear o f death is a theme that occurs with
and handle different bodily reactions without
increasing intensity during this phase.
the continuous contact with the medical staff that characterized the phase of treatments. Furthermore, patients w h o have completed a protracted series o f treatments start to recon-
Terminal Stage Physical symptoms
(e.g., pain,
fatigue,
sider whether their lives are satisfying and
motor disabilities) as well as psychological
meaningful enough, especially in view o f the
symptoms
suffering they incurred during treatment. In
may turn this phase into a particularly difficult
(e.g., disorientation,
depression)
view o f the close encounter with death that
one for patients and
patients have undergone, they may also expe-
impending problem is the need to decide
rience the pressure to use advantageously the
whether to continue medical treatments, given
time they have gained by enjoying themselves
that their contribution to prolonging life may
their families. O n e
or doing things they consider relevant and
be small and uncertain and their effect on Q O L
important for themselves. Moreover, anxiety
is negative, or whether to stop treatments alto-
about the possible recurrence o f the disease
gether and enjoy a certain modicum o f Q O L
persists. It may be further enhanced by regular
that may still be possible. Thus, during this
follow-up visits to their doctors and is often
phase, some patients may be getting curative
also maintained by continuous medical treat-
treatments, whereas others may be getting only
ments (e.g., tamoxifen tablets in breast cancer
palliative treatments or none at all. This phase
patients). In addition, patients may feel the
is characteristically marked by the phases of
need to normalize their interpersonal relations
dying, that is, gradual withdrawal from differ-
after a period characterized by getting help
ent preoccupations and interests (e.g., work,
and social support from relatives and friends.
profession, friends, entertainment) that may be
For this reason, as well as because o f their
accompanied by gradual physical weakening.
Psycho-oncology Notably, the different processes described by
\
patient's need to gain a modicum o f control in
Kubler-Ross ( 1 9 6 9 ) , such as anger and despair,
a situation marked by extreme helplessness.
may occur in the course o f these phases, but
These observations suggest that the desire for
they tend to show up concomitantly, and often
hastened death may be a cry for help when
together with other processes, rather than in
basic psychological and physical needs are
consecutive stages. Other themes that
may
unmet. Taking care o f these needs may well
show up during this stage are confronting death
result in a significant decrease in the patient's
in a personal way, taking leave o f beloved ones,
interest in assisted suicide.
and finalizing issues with which patients have dealt in the past (e.g., trying to make up with friends, finishing jobs). However, sometimes
BRIEF OVERVIEW OF
patients may be expected, or even pressured by
RESEARCH IN T H E AREA
their friends and relatives, to deal with such issues even though they might not feel any ten-
As noted earlier, psycho-oncology has stimu-
dency to do so.
lated much research. This section presents
During recent years, the issues o f "physician-
the major domains o f research in psycho-
assisted suicide" and "right to die" have been
oncology. E a c h subsection includes a brief
increasingly discussed in regard to the terminal
description o f issues and major findings.
stage. O n the basis o f questionnaire surveys, it seems that about 8 % to 2 5 % of oncology patients with advanced disease express interest
Cancer Prevention and Screening
in death alternatives (Chochinov et al., 1 9 9 5 ; Emanuel, Fairclough, Daniels, &
A typical assumption is that at least a third
Clarridge,
of the cases o f cancer could be prevented
1 9 9 6 ) , citing mostly reasons such as fear o f
by controlling behaviors such as smoking,
becoming a burden on their families or o f
drinking alcohol, prolonged exposure to the
losing dignity, lack o f social support (Back,
sun, and contact with carcinogens. Another
Wallace, Starks, & Pearlman, 1 9 9 6 ) , disease
assumption is that undergoing regular tests in
status, pain, and other bothersome physical
adherence with the screening regulations for
(Massie, G a g n o n , &
Holland,
the various cancers (e.g., annual PSA test for
1 9 9 4 ) . In general, the desire for hastened death
men over age 5 0 years, repeated self-examina-
is more a function o f psychological and psychi-
tion o f the skin for melanoma or o f the breasts
symptoms
atric factors, such as depression, than o f pain
for
and physical problems (Cherny, 1 9 9 6 ) .
reduced the lethality o f the disease. Despite
suspicious lumps) could have
further
The desire for hastened death is an issue
increased efforts at information dissemination,
that deserves consideration apart from the
prevention and screening have not reached the
operational implication o f aiding the patient
desirable levels. For example, although the
to die. T h e desire is unstable (Chochinov,
contribution o f smoking to cancers of all kinds
Tataryn,
Clinch, &
increases when
Dudgeon,
the patient
1999),
is lonely
it
has been established and is well known, only
and
about half o f the smokers have quit smoking,
depressed (Rosenfeld, 2 0 0 0 ) , it decreases after
and very low percentages persist in not smok-
the patient gets social support and exposure to
ing for longer durations (for 1 year, 1 % to 5 %
empathie listening (Severson, 1 9 9 7 ) , and it is
of smokers after treatment or
not a function o f current pain (Emanuel et al.,
[Cinciripini, 1 9 9 5 ] ) . T h e same is true for
1996;
&
screening. In regard to breast screening, only
Chapman, 1 9 9 7 ) . It is likely that considering
about 1 5 % to 3 0 % o f women get screened,
the
and even individuals at risk do not comply as
Sullivan,
option
Rapp,
o f euthanasia
Fitzgibbon, expresses
the
self-quitters
329
330
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS required. If screening is done once, it is often
increased attendance (Chaitchik & Kreitler,
not repeated. This cannot be accounted for by
1 9 9 1 ) . Second, women of a specific personality
the difficulty of performing
the screening,
type (e.g., higher on negative emotions, with a
given that it is often made very easy, or by for-
restricted self-concept) were found to be more
getfulness, given that health authorities often
likely than other women to undergo screening
remind
for
tests (Kreitler et al., 1 9 9 0 ) . Findings o f this
screening (U.K. Trial o f Early Detection o f
kind suggest that screening may resemble other
people o f their appointments
Breast Cancer Group, 1 9 8 4 ) . Although self-
behaviors in being controlled by motivational
examination is still widely promoted (in regard
dispositions, for example, in line with the cog-
to breast cancer or melanoma [Berwick, Begg,
nitive orientation theory (Kreitler &c Kreitler,
Fine, Roush, Roush, & Barnhill, 1 9 9 6 ; Kreitler
1 9 8 2 ) . This theory assumes that behavior is
et al., 1 9 9 0 ] ) , it is not a reliable method for
guided by motivational vectors defined by four
early detection of cancer and may lead to dan-
types o f beliefs (about oneself, reality, norms,
gerous
delays
in
diagnosis
(Kreitler
&
and goals) reflecting dynamically meaningful themes rather than by conscious, voluntary,
Chaitchik, 1 9 9 5 ) . A great number o f studies have been done
rational decisions reflecting cost-benefit con-
to understand the causes o f noncompliance
siderations. Identifying the motivationally rele-
and to increase compliance in these areas (e.g.,
vant beliefs enabled the successful prediction o f
Cinciripini, 1 9 9 5 ; DiPlacido, Zauber, & Redd,
who
1 9 9 8 ; Rossi, Biais, Redding, & Weinstock,
colorectal, and cervical cancers (Kreitler, 1 9 9 8 ) .
would
undergo
screening for
breast,
1 9 9 5 ) . Studies have shown that information
The prediction for breast cancer held in regard
about risks, benefits, and dangers of doing or
to different populations
avoiding certain behaviors do not suffice for
place) and different kinds o f screening (e.g.,
motivating people to comply with the medical
induced, spontaneous, personal initiative or
(e.g., urban,
work-
recommendations. T h e health beliefs model
not, examination by self or doctor), whereby
assumes that four components play a signifi-
the best results were obtained with repeated,
cant role in screening behavior: beliefs about a
self-initiated screening by a doctor (Kreitler,
person's susceptibility to the disease, beliefs
Chaitchik, Kreitler, Sc Weissler, 1 9 9 4 ) .
about the severity of the disease, beliefs about the barriers and difficulties of the behavior, and beliefs about the benefits of the behavior (Janz & Becker, 1 9 8 4 ) . However, these variables often have proved insufficient for pre-
Coping and Adaptation Coping
is
the
general
concept
that
describes strategies for dealing with threat.
dicting or generating the desired behavior and
Strategies o f coping are triggered whenever
had to be supplemented by various circum-
there is a large gap between the extent o f the
stantial factors (e.g., J a n z & Becker, 1 9 8 4 ;
resources appraised by an individual as nec-
Murray & M c M i l l a n , 1 9 9 3 ) . Some studies
essary for handling a situation and the extent
suggest that noncompliance with
o f resources appraised as available to the
screening
may have deeper dynamic roots. First, because
individual
higher perceived risk among women with a
Coping is evoked when the gap in appraised
(Lazarus
&
Folkman,
1984).
family history o f breast cancer reduces fre-
resources is indeed large but less than in the
quency o f mammography screening (Polednak,
case o f despair, often characterized as hope-
Lane, & Burg, 1 9 9 1 ) , it is likely that increased
lessness/helplessness, and more than in the
fear may deter women from undergoing tests.
case o f mere challenge, often experienced as
Accordingly, reducing the anxiety by framing
" I could handle it if I tried hard enough." A
the medical test in a nonthreatening
large variety o f coping mechanisms
setup
have
Psycho-oncology
\
been identified (e.g., humor, denial, fighting
Concerning the often-discussed mechanism of
spirit), differing
to
denial, the most adequate conclusion is that it
reducing the gap in appraised resources (e.g.,
may be very helpful during specific stages o f
social support increases the amount o f avail-
coping, especially when applied selectively
able resources, humor and denial reduce the
(e.g., to emotions and not to reality appraisal)
in their contributions
a m o u n t o f required resources). T h e coping
(Kreitler, 1 9 9 9 ) . However, it seems warranted
mechanisms used by a cancer patient are not
to assume that the quantity and variety o f cop-
necessarily the same as those used previously
ing mechanisms that a person has at his or
by the individual in other situations. Multiple
her disposal constitute a better guarantee for
determinants affect the use o f one or another
adjustment than does a specific coping mech-
coping mechanism, mainly personal predis-
anism, regardless o f how efficacious it may be.
previous
Some studies have investigated the efficacy
personal experiences, modeling o f others,
positions,
beliefs
and
values,
of coping mechanisms in regard to the course o f
and the severity o f the problems that need to
disease. Efficacy can be assessed in terms of the
be handled (Rowland, 1 9 8 9 ) .
patient's adjustment. For the most part, results
There have been numerous attempts
to
identify the efficacy of coping mechanisms,
show that low adjustment
is significantly
related to recurrence (Rogentine et al., 1 9 7 9 )
in terms o f
and that better adaptation 1 year after diagno-
improving the patient's adjustment and Q O L .
sis is significantly related to fewer recurrences
M o s t prior research suggests that there is no
after 3 years and to longer survival in 5- and
one specific coping mechanism that can be
7-year follow-ups (Kreitler, Kreitler, Chaitchik,
considered as the best because the efficacy o f a
Shaked, & Shaked, 1 9 9 7 ) . Only one study
whereby
efficacy
is defined
strategy depends on (a) the individual (e.g.,
found that adjustment was unrelated to recur-
one patient may find comfort through faith
rence (Temoshok Sc F o x , 1 9 8 4 ) . However,
and religion, another through emotional sup-
studies of specific coping mechanisms did not
port, and another through going back to w o r k
yield clear results about the benefit of any spe-
[e.g., Schonfield, 1 9 7 2 ] ) and (b) the stage o f
cific coping mechanism in regard to survival.
the disease and the problems confronting the
Thus, active behavioral coping was related to
individual. Thus, the individual changes in
survival (Fawzy, Fawzy, & Canada, 2 0 0 1 ) , but
the use o f different coping mechanisms in the
the active means o f distraction and problem
course o f different stages of coping with the
tacking were unrelated to survival (Buddeberg
disease
1 9 9 6 ; Heim,
et al., 1 9 9 6 ) . Again, hopelessness/helplessness
Augustini, Schaffner, & Valach, 1 9 9 3 ) . In
was related to recurrence (Jensen, 1 9 8 7 ) but
general, the more efficacious coping mecha-
also was unrelated to recurrence and survival
nisms are those focused on solving problems,
(Cassileth, Lusk, Miller, Brown, 8c Miller,
(Buddeberg et al.,
confronting real-life issues, actively searching
1 9 8 5 ; Cody et al., 1 9 9 4 ; Ringdall, 1 9 9 5 ) . One
for information (Felton &c Revenson, 1 9 8 4 ;
reason for the lack of clarity o f the findings is
Weisman & Worden, 1 9 7 6 - 1 9 7 7 ) ,
having
that in some studies basic medical prognostic
emotional discharge (e.g., through
humor,
criteria, such as the number of affected lymph
through sharing experiences [Penman, 1 9 8 0 ] ) ,
nodes, were not considered. This was the case
and cultivating hope and optimism (Scheier &
in the famous series o f the Royal Mardsen stud-
Carver, 2 0 0 1 ) .
ies, which reported that fighting spirit and per-
T h e least effective coping
mechanisms are those focused on avoidance,
haps denial had a positive effect on survival,
passivity, pessimism, yielding, blaming, acting
whereas stoic acceptance, anxious preoccupa-
past
tion, and hopelessness/helplessness had a nega-
(O'Malley, Koocher, Foster, & Slavin, 1 9 7 9 ) .
tive effect (Greer, Morris, & Pettingale, 1 9 7 9 ;
out, apathy,
and regrets a b o u t
the
331
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS
332
Greer, Morris, Pettingale, & Haybittle, 1 9 9 0 ;
made to reinstate the optimal level as much as
Morris,
1992;
possible by establishing the new Q O L level on
Pettingale, Morris, Greer, & Haybittle, 1 9 8 5 ) .
improvements and increases in a variety of
Pettingale,
&
Haybittle,
The results were not obtained when medical
other domains such as entertainment, dwelling
variables were adequately controlled in a repli-
conditions, and meaningfulness o f occupation
cation (Watson, Haviland, Greer, Davidson, Sc
(Kreitler, Chaitchik, Rapoport, Kreitler, &
Bliss, 1 9 9 9 ) .
Algor, 1 9 9 3 ) . This homeostatically grounded tendency is probably also responsible for the increases in levels o f Q O L (even over the pre-
Quality of Life
disease levels) that are sometimes observed in
Quality o f life is defined as the subjective evaluation by the individual o f his or her own well-being
and
functioning
in
cancer patients who find new venues for creativity and personal growth.
different
domains o f life. T h e major characteristics of Q O L are that it is subjective (hence, it is assessed by self-reports), based on evaluation,
Effect of Psychological Factors on Disease Incidence
phenomenological (causes not considered),
The major psychological factors investi-
dynamic (changes in line with changes in situ-
gated are stressful life events, psychopathology,
ations and conditions), and multidimensional
and personality tendencies. This subsection
(refers to various domains such as physical
focuses on each in turn. Table 1 7 . 1 presents
functioning, eating, sleeping, sex, emotional
the basic research designs in this domain.
state, cognitive functioning, family life, social relations, meaning, and coherence in life).
Stress. Animal models show that stress has-
Q O L differs from functional indexes that
tens the onset and further growth o f most
assess mainly functioning or ability o f the indi-
virally induced tumors and inhibits the onset
vidual to take care o f himself or herself in
and growth o f nonviral ones (Justice, 1 9 8 5 ) .
daily life
But the results o f studies investigating the rela-
(e.g., Karnofsky's
Performance
Status Rating). Q O L assessments mostly fail
tions o f stressful life events and cancer inci-
to distinguish among groups with different
dence
in
humans
are
equivocal.
Thus,
clinical disease statuses or different levels
case-controlled studies have shown that can-
of
Aaronson,
cer patients, as compared with controls,
Ahmedzai, Bergman, Bullinger, & Cull, 1 9 9 3 ;
had more stressful events in their earlier
M c H o r n e y , W a r e , & Raczek, 1 9 9 3 ) but are
lives (Courtney, Longnecker, Theorell,
performance
ratings
(e.g.,
&
sensitive to effects in different domains o f life,
Gerhardsson de Verdier, 1 9 9 3 ; Geyer, 1 9 9 1 ) ,
for example, in the course of treatments (Ganz
had the same number o f stressful
Sc Coscarelli, 1 9 9 5 ) . T h e assessment of Q O L
(Edwards et al., 1 9 9 0 ) , or had fewer stressful
plays an important role in clinical trials, in
events (Priestman, Priestman, S i Bradshaw,
events
decisions about treatments, and in evaluating
1 9 8 5 ) . Large-cohort studies have yielded sim-
the costs o f treatments in terms o f Q O L as
ilar results; no relation was found between
compared with their contribution to survival
stressful events earlier in life and cancer mor-
(de Haes et al., 2 0 0 0 ) . O n e important finding
bidity in prisoners of war (Joffres, Reed, &
is that Q O L tends to maintain a stable optimal
Nomura,
level based on the appraised status in the
Goldberg, & Beebe, 1 9 7 4 ) or between stress-
major domains o f health, work, and family or
ful events earlier in life and bereaved spouses
social ties. I f status in any o f these domains is
(Helsing, Comstock, Sc Szklo, 1 9 8 2 ; Kaprio,
lowered for a longer duration, attempts are
Koskenvuo, Sc Rita, 1 9 8 7 ) , but more cancer
1 9 8 5 ; Keehn,
1980;
Keehn,
Psycho-oncology
\
Basic Designs Used in Studies on the Effect of Psychological Factors on Incidence and Course of Disease in Cancer
Table 17.1
Design
Description
Advantages
Shortcomings
Retrospective
Psychological variables are assessed after cancer diagnosis
Large samples and immediate availability of all medical information
Cancer diagnosis may affect reports of premorbid personality and events
Quasi-prospective
Psychological variables are assessed after addressing doctor but prior to diagnosis
No biases of recall of the past; medical information available in short time
Psychological assessments affected by the anxiety of waiting for diagnosis and presentiments
Retro-prospective
Psychological variables are assessed long before cancer diagnosis in a cohort studied for another reason/purpose
No biases of recall of the past; medical information available in short time
Psychological assessments may be biased or not quite relevant
Prospective
Psychological variables are assessed before cancer diagnosis; course of disease is checked in follow-ups
Reliability of baseline data; control of selection biases in sample
Difficulty with follow-ups for prolonged periods of time
S O U R C E : For the designs, see T e m o s h o k and Heller (1984).
hematopoietic
Depression is another pathological condi-
malignancies) was found over 2 0 years in
(melanoma, lymphatic
and
tion that has been associated with cancer,
parents o f accident victims and among parents
especially on the evidence o f a
who had lost sons in the Y o m Kippur war in
study at an electric plant in Chicago (Persky,
Israel (Levav et al., 2 0 0 0 ) .
Kempthorne-Rawson
cohort
8c Shekelle, 1 9 8 7 ;
Shekelle, R a y n o r , Ostfeld, Sc Garron, 1 9 8 1 ) . Earlier studies
Psychopathology.
sug-
Y e t other cohort studies did not confirm this
get
finding. T h e combined evidence supported
cancer less often than did others. Y e t later
merely a null or weak relationship between
analyses o f the findings showed that the early
depression and risk for cancer ( F o x , 1 9 8 9 ) .
gested that
studies had
schizophrenics tended
led to erroneous
to
conclusions
because they were based on calculating pro-
Personality.
It has been often claimed that a
portional mortality from cancer instead o f
certain pattern o f personality characteristics,
absolute mortality rates ( F o x , 1 9 7 8 ) . Studies
Type C, presents a risk for cancer. Type C has
based on absolute rates show that schizo-
been described as compliant, unassertive, sub-
phrenics do not have a reduced rate o f cancer
missive, and avoiding the expression of nega-
(Gulbinat et al., 1 9 9 2 ) and that
tive emotions, especially o f anger (Temoshok,
patients
diagnosed with reactive psychosis may even
1 9 8 7 ) . Studies have supported some Type C
have a slightly increased rate o f cancer as
tendencies. For example, one study enabled sig-
compared
nificant identification o f cervix cancer patients,
with
the
general
(Jorgensen & Mortensen, 1 9 9 2 ) .
population
as compared with controls, on the basis of high
333
334
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS defensiveness and extraversion scores (Kreitler, Levavi, & Bornstein, 1 9 9 6 ) . Suppression of
Stress.
There is no definitive evidence that
stress influences the course o f cancer or sur-
emotions is the most widely investigated o f the
vival rates. F o r example, no relationship was
Type C tendencies. T h e studies of this tendency
found between stressful events and breast
(1998b)
cancer survival (Barraclough et al., 1 9 9 2 ) .
reported that five of the main studies sup-
However, bereavement (losing a son in a war
ported the tendency, four did not support it,
or in a traffic accident) increased the risk of
and three had mixed findings. Furthermore, the
death from different types o f cancer, but only
apparent nonemotionality in cancer patients
if the cancer had been diagnosed before the
may be due to their intention to suppress emo-
loss (and not after it) (Levav et al., 2 0 0 0 ) .
have yielded mixed results. F o x
tions as part o f their effort to appease others rather
than to
an
authentic
alexithymia
Psychopathology.
T o date, the evidence
(Servaes, Vingerhoets, Vreugdenhil, Keuning,
concerning the effect o f depression is equivo-
& Broekhuijsen, 1 9 9 6 ) . Moreover, comparing
cal. O n e early study indicated higher rates of
the repressiveness of women before and after a
depression and psychiatric disorders in patients
biopsy for breast cancer and for nonrelevant
with shorter survival periods (Weisman &
control surgery showed that before surgery all
Worden, 1 9 7 5 ) . Another study showed the
women had a comparable level of repressive-
reverse; long-term survivors were in greater
ness (measured by high defensiveness and low
distress,
anxiety) but that after surgery there was a sig-
were well adjusted
nificant rise in the repressiveness o f only those
Melisaratos, 1 9 7 9 ) . Still other studies indicated
women who were given the diagnosis o f cancer
that recurrence and survival in cancer patients
(reflecting a rise in defensiveness to combat
are not related to distress (as assessed by the
whereas
short-term
survivors
(Derogatis, Abeloff,
&
anxiety) (Kreitler, Kreitler, & Chaitchik, 1 9 9 3 ) .
Symptom Checklist-90) (Holland et al., 1 9 8 6 )
Hence, it is likely that suppression of emotions
or to anxiety, depression, and anger (Jamison,
subserves the need to control anxiety. How-
Burish, & Wallston, 1 9 8 7 ) . Indeed, the recent
ever, the theory of cognitive orientation that
replication o f the Royal Mardsen
enables a more extensive approach to measur-
showed that depression was the only psycho-
ing personality tendencies relevant for cancer
logical variable that predicted earlier mortality
studies
showed that limited expression o f emotions
from cancer, but there were too few cases to
may be one o f several tendencies included in the
make the finding reliable (Watson et al., 1 9 9 9 ) .
profile characterizing specific types of cancer patients. For example, it was found to be part of the profile characterizing colon cancer patients as compared with healthy controls, whereby it is probably not a result of the disease because it does not change with disease duration (Figer, Kreitler, Michal, & Inbar, 2 0 0 2 ) .
Personality.
O f the Type C components,
defensiveness was related to shorter survival (Ratcliffe, Dawson, & Walker, 1 9 9 5 ) as well as to a particular personality type whose major characteristics are dealing with loss by despair and retaining closeness to people with whom one's relationship
Effect of Psychological Factors on Disease Course and Survival The main investigated factors were stress,
has
ended
(Grossarth-
Maticek, Kanazir, Schmidt, & Vetter, 1 9 8 5 ) . However, the latter study was severely criticized in regard to reliability (Schueler & F o x , 1 9 9 1 ; V a n der Ploeg, 1 9 9 1 ) . Concerning emotional suppression, the findings are unclear;
psychopathology, and personality (see also
F o x ( 1 9 9 8 b ) found three studies with positive
Table 17.1).
results, three with negative results, and three
|
Psycho-oncology with mixed results. There is also evidence o f no
recurrence was found (Burman & Margolin,
correlation of psychosocial variables to recur-
1992). In regard to survival, studies showed a
rence or survival (Cassileth et al., The
cognitive o r i e n t a t i o n
1985).
small effect o f better survival for married
theory
has
women
(Goodwin, Hunt, Key, &
Samet,
enabled identifying a set o f characteristics
1987), but only when they are young (Neale,
(e.g.,
Tilley, 8c Vernon, 1986) and not for older
readiness
to
expose
a
person's
8c Lusk, 1988).
weaknesses, self-confidence, low seeking o f
women (Cassileth, Walsh,
approval, low obsessiveness and compulsive-
However, there is also evidence o f shorter sur-
ness) that differentiated significantly between
vival for married women (Ell, Nishimoto,
patients with recurrence and those without
Mediansky, Mantell, 8t Hamovitch,
recurrence at 3 years follow-up and survival
and o f no relation between survival and being
1992)
8c Surtees, 1989). T h e
at 5 years onward. During the initial years,
married (e.g., Dean
psychological factors contributed less to pre-
results are inconsistent, possibly because can-
dicting survival than did medical factors, but
cer site, gender, and quality o f marriage have
the former's relative and absolute impacts
not been considered.
increased with time (Kreitler et al.,
1997).
In conclusion, there are two main reasons
In regard to size of social network, studies show no relation to incidence (Reynolds &
unclear. O n e is disregard for the fact that prog-
1990), recurrence (Cassileth et al., 1985), or survival (Vogt, Mullooly, Ernst, Pope, &c Hollis, 1992). In contrast, active social
nosis o f survival should consider conjointedly
participation and active involvement have a
why most of the findings examining the impact of psychological factors on survival have been
Kaplan,
medical and psychological factors. T h e second
positive effect on reduced recurrence (Hislop,
is the tendency to narrow research down to a
Waxier, Coldman, Elwood, 8c Kan, 1987) and
1992), more so
specific set o f psychological variables rather
on longer survival (Vogt et al.,
than to expand the search for new psychologi-
with friends than with relatives (Waxier-
cal variables by applying new methodologies.
Morrison, Hislop, Mears,
8c Kan, 1991).
Cancer patients often refer to their need for emotional support, which is widely believed
Social Relations and Support
to positively affect the course o f disease. In one
It has long been surmised that the social
study, perceived family support did not pre-
environment in which people function affects
dict recurrence (Levy, Herberman, Lippman,
cancer incidence and prognosis. T h e question
D'Angelo, & Lee,
of whether social ties affect cancer incidence
vival was related to feeling isolated and lonely
1991). However, shorter sur-
has been studied by examining two aspects of
(only in women) and to having few contacts (in
social relations: static-descriptive aspects (i.e.,
men) (Reynolds 8c Kaplan, 1990) as well as to
marital status and number o f social ties) and
having a high need for emotional support
8c Stewart, 1988),
active-functional aspects (i.e., extent o f partici-
(Stavraky, Donner, Kincade,
pation and involvement in social relations). In
whereas longer survival was related to per-
regard to cancer incidence, various studies
ceived adequacy of family support (Stavraky
found that married people had a lower inci-
et al., 1988) and to getting adequate emotional
dence o f cancer (Reynolds & Kaplan,
1990),
that they had a higher incidence of cancer
support (only in women) (Ell et al.,
1992). It is
likely that social participation positively affects
Sc M c C r a e , 1989), and
survival and disease progression because it has-
that the findings probably varied with cancer
tens diagnosis (Neale et al., 1986) and pro-
site, gender, and ethnicity (e.g., Swanson, Belle,
motes compliance with treatment (Richardson,
(Zonderman, Costa
& Satariano,
1985). N o effect o f marriage on
Shelton, Krailo, 8c Levine,
1990).
33S
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS
336
There are often cited studies that supposedly
stresses (e.g., children of sick parents, parents
support the effects o f social support inter-
of sick children regardless o f the children's
ventions on survival. T h e best known is the
ages, partners of patients) (Keller, Henrich,
Stanford University study that claimed an
Sellschopp, & Beutel, 1 9 9 5 ; Koch, Hàrter,
additional
advanced
J a k o b , & Siegrist, 1 9 9 5 ) . Fourth, the family is
18 months of life for
breast cancer patients who got social support
exposed to severe stresses due to the patient's
as compared with controls (Spiegel et al.,
sickness and everything else it involves, includ-
1 9 9 9 ) . However, the study was criticized for
ing the changes in daily routine and roles, the
methodological reasons, mainly sample choice
threat o f impending death, and/or conflicts
and differences between the two groups (Fox,
with the medical staff (Jacobs, Ostroff,
1 9 9 8 a ; Kogon, Biswas, Pearl, Carlson,
&
Steinglass, 1 9 9 8 ) . Families vary greatly in the
Spiegel, 1 9 9 7 ) and failed to be replicated
manner in which they cope with the difficulties
&
(Goodwin et al., 2 0 0 1 ; Spiegel et al., 1 9 9 9 ) .
and respond to the stresses. Some mobilize
Methodological criticism was also leveled
resources and are even strengthened, whereas
against other studies in regard to the effects o f
others disintegrate. Some families
social support (Gellert, Maxwell, & Siegel,
support or solve problems, whereas others are
1 9 9 3 ; Morgenstern, Gellert, Walter, Ostfeld,
hostile
&
McKenzie, M c D o w a l l , & Nitzan, 1 9 9 8 ) . T h e
Siegel, 1 9 8 4 ) . In contrast, many studies
or
withdrawn
provide
(Kissane,
Block,
show clearly the beneficial effect o f social sup-
state o f the family is an important factor in
port on patients' Q O L (e.g., Bloom, 1 9 8 6 ;
the patient's well-being, not least because the
Dunkel-Schetter, 1 9 8 4 ; Spiegel et al., 1 9 9 9 ) .
family is a serious provider o f care to the
Future research in social support could
patient. T h e family's coping depends on sev-
benefit from considering the following: (a) the
eral factors such as how it interprets external
differential effects of various types of social
reality and defines its identity (Jacobs et al.,
support (e.g., informational, emotional), (b) the
1 9 9 8 ) as well as the degrees of communication,
source o f support (e.g., relatives, health profes-
emotional expressiveness, and cohesion in the
sionals), and (c) the dependence of the need for
family (Kissane et al., 1 9 9 8 ) . There is a grow-
support
ing awareness in health professional circles
on
personality
and
availability
that family members deserve special psycho-
(Dunkel-Schetter, 1 9 8 4 ) .
logical support so that they can withstand adequately the hard and prolonged stresses of
Family
cancer, provide the patient with an adequate
T h e family is involved in psycho-oncology in different
aspects. First, the family is a
environment, and avoid turning into "secondorder patients" (Jacobs et al., 1 9 9 8 ) .
provider of care for the patient (e.g., providing emotional
support,
getting
information,
offering help in decision making, giving concrete help, sharing financial costs, meeting social needs and costs, maintaining routine)
Getting Information and Truth Telling Information
about
diagnosis, prognosis,
(Lederberg, 1 9 9 8 ) . Second, the family under-
and treatment is one o f the most central issues
goes serious changes due to the patient's dis-
for the cancer patient. Its importance has been
ease and the involved stresses (e.g., changes in
enhanced by the tendency during past genera-
routines, roles, attachment relationships, man-
tions to keep the diagnosis secret and by the
ner o f functioning, structure, and sense o f well-
current tendency to involve the patient in deci-
being) (Weihs & Reiss, 1 9 9 5 ) . Third, specific
sions about
family members may be exposed to special
theme that occurs primarily during the first
treatments.
Information
is a
Psycho-oncology following
different
\
phase o f the disease but that continues to play
remission
an important role in the further phases o f the
Hence, patients are often frustrated. Another
treatments.
disease, mainly whenever there is a recurrence,
complicating factor is the conflict that many
a need to decide about treatment options, or
patients experience among the kind o f infor-
any change in the disease, down to the preter-
mation they want to get (e.g., good news),
minal and terminal phases. T h e kind, amount,
the kind they think they should get (e.g., how
information
long they have to live), and the kind they
change during the different stages o f the
get (e.g., facts about diagnosis and prognosis).
meaning,
and
disclosure
of
disease. Patients often use multiple sources o f
Moreover, patients may be reluctant to ask for
information that vary in completeness and
information they want or think they are enti-
reliability such as physicians (first and second
tled to get because they believe that their doc-
opinions), nurses, social workers, psychol-
tors k n o w best what is good for them, so that
ogists, other patients, relatives, friends, the
if the doctors did not provide that information,
Internet, and the media (Kreitler, Chaitchik,
it is probably not good for the patients to have
Rapoport, Sc Algor, 1 9 9 5 ) . Some patients find
it (Chaitchik et al., 1 9 9 2 ; Kreitler, Chaitchik,
it difficult to understand the information and
Kovner, & Kreitler, 1 9 9 2 ) .
its implications, to evaluate the sources, and to integrate them, especially when
there
After getting the relevant information, the
are
major issue becomes living with the informa-
inconsistencies. Some patients ( 1 3 % during
tion. For example, a study with head and neck
initial phases to 5 7 % during later phases) may
patients showed that those w h o had a large
tend to renounce the effort to deal with the
amount o f information had improved rela-
information and decide to rely instead on their
tions with family members and friends but
primary oncologists. However, this response
functioned less well at work and suffered from
often calls forth pressure o f family members
anxiety about their medical state, those who
and friends on the patient to be empowered
had a little information had tense and poorer
and actively involved in getting information.
relations with family members and friends but
Getting information may turn into a coping
functioned well at work and did not suffer
mechanism for patients (Watson et al., 1 9 8 8 )
from undue anxiety, and those w h o had a
but also for caretakers who feel that they will
medium amount o f information had tense
be best able to help the patient in this way.
relations with others as well as problems with
This is probably due to the fact that dealing
work and anxiety (Kreitler et al., 1 9 9 5 ) .
with information may give rise to the feeling
Another aspect o f information in cancer
or illusion o f having control over the disease.
patients concerns the communication of infor-
Studies show that patients often view the
mation by the patient to others. Patients are
amount of information they got from their
often concerned with issues such as h o w much
doctors as less than the amount the doctors
and which information
estimate they have transmitted
of health to disclose, to whom, and when.
(Chaitchik,
about their states
Kreitler, Schwartz, Shaked, & Rosin, 1 9 9 2 ) .
Considerations include not wanting to bother
There is also a difference in the kind o f infor-
others or to burden them emotionally, pre-
mation expected and that obtained. Patients
serving patients' self-esteem, avoiding the pity
often expect to get information about whether
of others, and maintaining patients' denial o f
their doctors believe the treatment will help
the disease. Notably, one study showed that
them, what their real states are, whether they
even spouses o f cancer patients k n o w rela-
are about to die, and h o w long they have to
tively little about what cancer patients experi-
live. In contrast, the information they get refers
ence and know, indicating a low degree o f
to diagnosis, stage, grade, and percentages o f
communication (Chaitchik et al., 1 9 9 2 ) .
337
338
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS
Pathogen
Disease
Figure 17.1
The Stimulus-Background Model of Disease
Genetic Testing and Counseling
Models of the Problem genetic
Models in psycho-oncology about the etiol-
research have increased the importance o f
ogy and prognosis o f cancer form part o f the
three categories o f genetic
determinants
more encompassing approach of biopsychoso-
related to cancer: (a) cytogenetics, which con-
cial medicine, which has replaced the more
cerns abnormal chromosomes; (b) single-gene
limited approach o f biomedical medicine and
traits, which concern hereditary
mutations
assumes that psychological factors are involved
(c) ecogenetics, which
in physical disorders. Thus, in regard to disease
The
spectacular
(e.g., BRCA1);
and
advances
of
concerns gene and environment interactions
incidence, it is assumed that each disease is
(e.g., Epstein-Barr virus causing lymphoma
caused by some pathogen (e.g., microbe, virus,
in individuals with specific genes). Genetic
material with detrimental effects for the organ-
information may have serious psychological
ism such as radioactivity or carcinogens). As
effects, including anxiety and stress in healthy
illustrated in Figure 1 7 . 1 , the effect of the
and sick individuals as well as in their chil-
pathogen is not automatic; rather, it depends
dren. Studies have shown that individuals at
on background factors such as the organism's
risk might not undergo screening, especially if
genetic tendencies, nutritional state, immune
their fear is high. Furthermore, their readiness
system, comorbidity, psychological factors,
to participate
proce-
and characteristics specific to the particular
treatment,
disease (e.g., lipids and blood pressure in car-
dures,
in health-promoting
including
prophylactic
and
diological disorders). Likewise, the effect of the
beliefs about their likelihood o f getting sick
treatment on recovery is not automatic; rather,
(Kash & Lerman, 1 9 9 8 ) .
it depends on background factors that are o f
depends on their anxiety, education,
Psycho-oncology
Treatment
Disease
Health Figure 17.2
The Stimulus-Background Model of Recovery
similar categories as the factors relevant for disease occurrence (Figure
17.2). Notably,
Garssen &
Goodkin,
1999). The effect of
psychological factors on cancer is assumed to be
background factors always include psycholog-
through the immune
ical factors, but their role and extent o f impact
Valdimarsdottir,
may vary in the case of different diseases and
overall effect of the immune system on tumor
perhaps also across individuals. M o s t of the etiological models in psycho-
system (Bovbjerg Sc
1998). However, because the
growth appears to be moderate on the whole, a variety of pathways are explored to account for
psychoneuroim-
the effects of psychological factors. One such
munology (Bovbjerg & Valdimarsdottir, 1998;
pathway is infections, to which cancer patients
Goodkin & Visser, 2000). According to this dis-
may be particularly vulnerable and
cipline, the causal matrix consists of a multiplic-
form the major cause of cancer-related deaths
ity of factors derived from different domains,
(Bovbjerg Sc Valdimarsdottir,
oncology
are
based
on
1998;
which White,
mainly the neurological, immune, endocrinolog-
1993). T w o other likely pathways are stress,
ical, and psychosocial domains. However, the
which functions through alterations in the
models differ in the interactions assumed among
hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal
the factors, in the nature of the embedded causal
hypothalamic-sympathetic-medullary
axis and the axis
Sc Glaser, 1999), and depression,
links, in the emphasis placed on factors o f dif-
(Kiecolt-Glaser
ferent domains, and in the manner in which the
which functions through alterations in the
different factors are structured and positioned in
hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, the hypo-
relation to one another. M o s t often, the immune
thalamic-pituitary-thyroid
system is placed in a central position and the rest
hypothalamic-growth hormone axis (Mussel-
axis,
and
the
Sc Nemeroff, 1998).
of the factors are considered insofar as they
man, McDaniel, Porter,
affect immunological parameters (Finn, 2001;
Notably, it has been suggested that more weight
340
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS should be attributed to psychological factors in
7 1 % were completely symptom free, whereas
psychoneuroirnmunology to gain a better under-
2 1 % had deteriorated into major depression
standing of the immune effects on cancer and
or alcoholism (Andreasen & Hoenk, 1 9 8 2 ) .
evidence of the full extent of the immunological
Depressive
are the most fre-
symptoms
impact on cancer incidence and progression
quent kind o f mental health problem in can-
(Bovbjerg & Valdimarsdottir, 1 9 9 8 ) .
cer patients. T h e estimates o f their prevalence range from 1 % to 5 3 % in the various studies (DeFlorio & Massie, 1 9 9 5 ) . T h e reasons for
DIAGNOSTIC AND
the variation are differences in the tools of
E T I O L O G I C A L ISSUES
assessment, in the diagnoses (e.g., major depression, unipolar depression, dysthymic
Clinical Description
disorder), in the group o f cancer patients in
A great number of studies have examined
which depression is assessed (e.g., pancreatic,
the prevalence of mental health disorders in
breast), and in the phase o f the disease in
cancer patients. According to a review based
which depression is assessed (e.g., prelimi-
on the findings of the first cooperative group
nary, advanced). T h e tools o f assessment vary
organized for the study of these issues, 5 3 % o f
in the ways in which they solve the problem
cancer patients had "normal responses to can-
o f overlap in symptoms between depression
cer," whereas 4 7 % had different psychiatric
proper and cancer (i.e., the disease and its
diagnoses: 3 2 % adjustment
with
treatments). There are four major approaches
symptoms of depression and anxiety, 6 %
to this issue: (a) the inclusive (counts all
disorders
major depression, 4 % organic mental disor-
symptoms regardless o f origin), (b) the etio-
ders (dementia or delirium), and 5 % preexist-
logical (counts only symptoms due to depres-
( 3 % personality
sion proper), (c) the substitutive (replaces
disorders, 2 % anxiety disorders) (Derogatis
indeterminate symptoms such as fatigue with
et al., 1 9 8 3 ) . O f all psychiatric
disorders
cognitive symptoms such as brooding), and
observed, 8 9 % were related to the disease and
(d) the exclusive (eliminates all symptoms due
the treatments, whereas only 1 1 % represented
to the disease or treatment and uses other
prior psychiatric problems. Other
depression criteria). T h e narrower the defini-
ing psychiatric disorders
studies, and
tion, the lower the observed prevalence o f
criteria, reported 1 4 % to 3 1 % of cancer sur-
depression. O n the whole, depression is also
using various assessment instruments
vivors with psychiatric diagnoses (for a review,
quite frequent in other medically sick people,
see Kornblith, 1 9 9 8 ) . T h e estimates o f the
with its prevalence ranging from 5 % to 5 5 %
reviewed studies resemble the earlier estimate,
(Popkin 8c Tucker, 1 9 9 2 ) .
considering that adjustment disorders are an intermediary state between normal
prevalent in cancer patients is depressed mood
is
followed by unipolar depression. Depression
under stress and pathology. The prevalence o f adjustment
O f the varieties of depression, the most
coping
coupled with a general adjustment disorder, disorders
due to the special stresses o f cancer: the dura-
is higher in some cancer diagnoses than in
tion and difficulties of treatments, the side
others, for example, breast cancer survivors
effects of treatments, the fear of recurrence, the
versus testis cancer survivors (Gritz, Wellisch,
impairment
o f body
image, and
so
on.
&
Landsverk, 1 9 8 8 ; Sneeuw et al., 1 9 9 2 ) .
Adjustment disorders are more frequent during
Advanced disease stage is often correlated
the early phases of cancer and often subside
with more depression. T h e factors contribut-
with time or evolve into another diagnosis. In
ing to depression in cancer patients include
one study with adult patients, after 5 years,
disabling
symptoms,
lower
Karnovsky
Psycho-oncology scores, uncontrolled pain, exhaustion/fatigue,
Anxiety may also be evoked by
metabolic abnormalities (e.g., anemia, hyper-
metabolic states (e.g., hypocalcemia, hypo-
calcemia),
endocrinological
abnormalities
(e.g., hyper- or hypothyroidism),
different
glycemia), hormone-secreting drugs
various
different
tumors,
(e.g., corticosteroids).
Its
chemotherapeutic agents (e.g., vincristine, vin-
manifestations
blastine), diverse medications (e.g., steroids,
tension, fear, dependence, withdrawal, and an
interferon,
enhanced
interleukin-2), and psychosocial
characteristics (e.g., recent loss o f spouse or friend, poor social support, previous Although
the total n u m b e r
o f cancer
tendency
to
become
lability, nauseous
(Noyes, Holt, & Massie, 1 9 9 8 ) .
psy-
chopathology) (Massie & Popkin, 1 9 9 8 ) .
include emotional
and
During recent years, increasing attention has been devoted to posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) in cancer patients
and
survivors.
patients who commit suicide is low, the risk of
Research was spurred particularly by the high
suicide in cancer patients is double that in the
prevalence o f avoidant and intrusive symptoms
general population (Campbell, 1 9 9 6 ) . Factors
in
correlated with the risk for suicide include gen-
Gebhardt, Petersen, & Hirji, 1 9 9 4 ) . In some
this
population
(Greenberg,
Goorin,
der (being male [in adults], being female [in
studies, up to 4 4 % o f patients reported P T S D -
adolescents]), site of cancer (particularly oral,
like symptoms (Cordova et al., 1 9 9 5 ) . Y e t only
pharyngeal, and lung cancers), medical state
4 % to 1 0 % of cancer survivors actually met
(e.g., advanced disease, poor prognosis, suffer-
criteria for a diagnosis o f P T S D (Alter et al.,
ing due to pain and fatigue), and preexisting
1 9 9 6 ) , as compared with 2 5 % to 3 3 % o f indi-
psychopathology (e.g., suicide attempts, psy-
viduals exposed to traumatic events (Yehuda,
chosis). Suicide ideation seems to be much
Resnick, Kahana, & Giller, 1 9 9 3 ) . One impor-
more frequent than suicide intent or actual sui-
tant result of P T S D is that it renders recurrent
cide
exposures to medical tests and treatments more
attempts
(Breitbart, 1 9 9 0 ) .
Suicidal
thoughts expressed during preterminal stages also are often not steady; may subside when patients
get empathy
and
social support.
difficult emotionally for patients. O f particular importance are chiatrie
effects
neuropsy-
due to side effects o f chemo-
Suicidal ideation o f patients may at times
therapeutic
express primarily their desire for control, their
states often observed in cancer patients. F o r
need to test their relatives, and/or attempts to
example, 5-fluorouracil may cause memory
reduce
their
fear
o f death
(Kreitler &c
Merimsky, in press).
agents or abnormal
metabolic
loss and confusional episodes; ifosfamide may cause hallucinations, somnolence, and mutism
in can-
(Tuxen & Hansen, 1 9 9 4 ) ; interferon alpha
cer patients is probably higher than in other
may cause agitation, emotional lability, and
medical conditions but its extent is uncertain
personality change (Quesada, Talpaz, Rios,
due to varying criteria, assessment
instru-
Kurzrock, & Gutterman, 1 9 8 6 ) ; and hypo- or
The prevalence o f anxiety
disorders
ments, and samples. Estimates vary from 1 5 %
hypercortisolism and hypo- or hypercalcemia
to 2 8 % (Carroll, Kathol, Noyes, W a l d , &
may cause depression, delirium, and dementia
Clamon, 1 9 9 3 ) . There are no consistent find-
(Fleishman, Lesko, & Breitbart, 1 9 9 3 ) .
ings about the gender, cancer site, age, and marital status correlates o f anxiety. Anxiety appears to increase in the course o f treatments (surgery and chemotherapy) and particularly
Typical Etiologies In trying to understand the psychological
with the advance o f disease. It may be viewed
state or reactions o f cancer patients, it is always
as a more or less constant accompaniment o f
recommended to apply a broad multidisci-
the
plinary approach and to assume interactions o f
different
vicissitudes
o f the
disease.
342
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS factors of diverse domains. For example, the
their psychological distress because they may
causes of anorexia, a typical symptom in cancer
regard it as a sign of weakness. T h e evaluations
patients (Lesko, 1 9 9 3 ) , may be disease related
are usually based on a structured interview or
(e.g., early stages of pancreatic or gastrointesti-
self-report measures. T h e best known is the
nal cancer, tumor obstruction in advanced
package referred
disease, uremia, anemia) or treatment related
Instruments, which is a structured interview
(e.g., radiation that may cause malabsorption
combined with a self-report measure. It enables
to as O m e g a
Screening
of food, nausea, or changes in taste of nutrients;
constructing the patient's profile on the basis
chemotherapy that may cause stomatitis o f the
of his or her responses to questions about past
alimentary canal or frequent diarrhea; analgesic
history (e.g., mental health, substance abuse)
drugs that may cause constipation and appetite
and social support (e.g., marriage, church),
loss; surgery
[e.g., esophagectomy, gastrec-
answers to the Inventory of Current Concerns,
tomy] that may cause chewing and swallowing
and demographic facts (Weisman, Worden &
difficulties). Further contributors to anorexia
Sobel, 1 9 8 0 ) . T h e four measures that are com-
are psychological factors such as depression,
monly used and are most reliable among
hopelessness/helplessness, withdrawal, anxiety,
the self-report tools are the Brief Symptom
and perhaps patient burnout coupled with an
Inventory (BSI) (Derogatis, 1 9 9 3 ) , which con-
unconscious desire to hasten death. Also to be
sists o f 5 3 items and provides scores on 1 0
considered are preexisting psychological ten-
scales (e.g., hostility, anxiety, paranoia) and 3
dencies, such as expressing noncompliance or
global scores (Global Severity Index, Positive
dissatisfaction by diminished eating, certain
Symptom
food aversions, or phobia of obesity, that may
Symptom Total); the Profile of M o o d States
be evoked by responses to steroids. Finally, it is
(POMS)
not uncommon for patients to believe, on their
1 9 7 1 ) , which consists of 6 5 items and provides
own or following some esoteric "alternative"
scores on 6 scales (e.g., anger-hostility, tension-
or dietary treatment, that they can "starve the
anxiety) and
cancer to death" by avoiding eating. Similarly
Disturbance);
complex etiologies are to be considered in
Depression Scale (HADS) (Johnson et al.,
Distress
Index,
and
Positive
( M c N a i r , Lorr, Sc Droppelman,
1 global score (Total M o o d the
Hospital
Anxiety
and
regard to depression, fatigue, lethargy, anxiety,
1 9 9 5 ) , which consists of 1 4 items and provides
and
scores on 2 scales (anxiety and depression); and
other
common
symptoms
in cancer
the Medical Outcomes Study Short Form
patients.
Health Survey ( M O S SF-36) (McHorney et al., 1 9 9 3 ) , which consists of 3 6 items and provides ASSESSMENT AND T R E A T M E N T
vitality).
Assessment The
major
scores on 8 scales (e.g., physical, pain, social,
assessments
in
psycho-
oncology are focused on evaluating psychological distress, Q O L , physical
symptoms,
and cognitive state.
Quality
of
Life.
Major
assessment
instruments o f Q O L include the European Organization for Research and Treatment of Cancer ( E O R T C ) , which is based largely on particular modules for different cancer diseases
Psychological
Distress. Evaluating or screen-
ing for current or future psychological distress
(Aaronson
et al., 1 9 9 3 ) ;
the
Functional
Assessment o f Cancer Therapy (FACT) scale
is of great importance in view o f the relative
(Cella et al., 1 9 9 3 ) , which is also based on spe-
paucity of professional psycho-oncologists and
cific modules for each cancer disease and
sometimes the reluctance o f patients to admit to
requests evaluation o f extent to which Q O L
j
Psycho-oncology was affected by the disease in each domain o f
Cognitive Capacity Screening Examination
life; and the Cancer Rehabilitation Evaluation
(CCSE)
System (CARES)
(Ganz, Coscarelli Schag,
organic mental syndromes, and the Mini-
Kahn, Petersen, & Hirji, 1 9 9 3 ) and M O S 3 6 -
Mental State E x a m ( M M S E ) (Folstein, Fetting,
item short form health survey (McHorney
L o b o , Niaz, & Capozolli, 1 9 8 4 ) , which is the
(Jacobs, 1 9 7 7 ) , which screens for
et al., 1 9 9 3 ) , both of which include diverse
standard mental state instrument, are based on
scales for the assessment of specific domains in
examining basic processes such as orientation
addition to the more physical one.
in time and place, instantaneous recall, shortterm memory, simple number calculations,
Physical
T h e assessment o f
Symptoms.
and the use o f language. T h e Neurobehavio(NCSE)
physical symptoms may be part o f the assess-
ral Cognitive Status Examination
ment o f Q O L or independent o f it. M o s t often,
(Kiernan, Mueller, Langston, & V a n Dyke,
it is based on self-report symptom checklists
1 9 8 7 ) assesses level o f consciousness, orienta-
that refer to multiple or single symptoms. T w o
tion, attention, and five major ability areas
examples o f multiple symptom
are
(language, constructions, memory, calcula-
the M e m o r i a l Symptom Assessment Scale
tions, and reasoning). M o r e specific delirium
(MSAS), which refers to 3 2 physical and psy-
assessment tools, based on clinicians' ratings,
chological symptoms
tools
(e.g., feeling nervous,
lack o f energy)—each rated in terms o f intensity, frequency, and distress—and
provides
scores on a Global Distress Index and the two
include the Delirium Rating Scale, with 1 0 items based on D S M - I I I (Diagnostic Statistical edition
Manual
of Mental
and third
Disorders,
[American Psychiatric Association,
physical and psychological subscales (Portenoy
1 9 8 8 ] ) criteria scanning diverse aspects such as
et al., 1 9 9 4 ) , and the Rotterdam Symptom
perceptual disturbance, psychomotor behav-
Checklist, which refers to 3 0 physical and psy-
ior, hallucinations, and
chological symptoms, assesses their impact on
(Trzepacz, Baker, & Greenhouse, 1 9 8 8 ) , and
physical activity and function, and provides the
the M e m o r i a l Delirium Assessment
same scores as does the M S A S (de Haes, van
with 1 0 items based on DSM-EV (American
Kippenberg, & Neijt, 1 9 9 0 ) .
Psychiatric Association, 1 9 9 4 ) criteria scan-
Examples o f frequently used tools that measure specific symptoms include the Brief Pain Inventory (BPI) (Daut, Cleeland, & Flanery,
lability o f m o o d Scale,
ning disturbances in arousal and consciousness as well as in cognitive functioning and psychomotor activity (Breitbart et al., 1 9 9 7 ) .
1 9 8 3 ) , which assesses pain history, intensity, location, quality, and interference with overall functioning; the McGill Pain Questionnaire (MPQ)
(Melzack, 1 9 7 5 ) , which
Goals of Treatment
evaluates
There are a great many psycho-oncologi-
through verbal descriptors the sensory, affec-
cal interventions with a diversity o f goals.
tive, and evaluative dimensions o f pain; the
M o s t are tailored to the specific needs and
20-item Multidimensional Fatigue Inventory
benefits o f the patients. T h e main six goals o f
(MFI) (Smets, Garssen, Bonke, 8c de Haes,
interventions are crisis overcoming, problem
1 9 9 5 ) ; and the Visual Analogue Scale (VAS)
solving, patient education, adjustment, med-
for dyspnea (Borg, 1 9 8 2 ) .
ical survival, and prevention. T h e first five deal with treating the patients and sometimes
Cognitive
State.
Some o f the tools for
assessing mental state focus on cognitive deficits, whereas others deal more specifically with delirium and confusional states. T h e
also family members. T h e sixth targets primarily the population at large. Crisis overcoming
is designed to help the
patient through the most difficult physical and
343
344
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS psychological phases
and
happiness beyond the levels attained prior to
treatment process. It is a kind o f "psychologi-
o f the
disease
the disease. Living with cancer and living well
cal first aid" and consists in applying a variety
are the goals o f adjustment.
of procedures geared to resolve the specific
Medical
survival
is focused on using psy-
emergency situation at hand (e.g., breakdown
chological processes to improve the patient's
due to disclosure o f the diagnosis or finding
chances for longer survival or to extend the dis-
out about disease recurrence, suicide intent,
ease-free intervals. Sometimes, this goal is con-
withdrawal from treatment). Stress, emotional
sidered as strengthening the patient's immune
intensity o f reactions, immense fear, and a
system (Finn, 2 0 0 1 ) .
sense o f being unable to go on or o f having been beaten are some of the characteristics o f a
Prevention
is focused on reducing the preva-
lence o f cancer by enhancing compliance with
crisis. Resolving the crisis satisfactorily may
screening guidelines in the population at large,
help to prevent later stress reactions, adjust-
and particularly in individuals at risk, and by
ment disorders, P T S D , and further crises.
modifying behaviors that increase the risk o f
W h e n the crisis is the patient's death, the inter-
cancer (e.g., improper diet, exposure to the sun,
vention may be focused on the family members
use of tobacco) (Cohen & Baum, 2 0 0 1 ) .
and is sometimes called "grief counseling." Problem
solving
is designed to help the
patient solve typical disease- or treatment-
Types of Treatment
related problems such as whether or h o w to
There are great variety of psychological
tell others (e.g., parents, partner, children, col-
treatments applied in the framework o f psycho-
leagues at work) about the disease or its recur-
oncology. Often, more than one are used con-
rence and whether to go on working during
jointly or within the same setup. The major
chemotherapy. Some o f the patient's problems
types of interventions are counseling, dynamic
may have existed before the disease but were
psychotherapy, cognitive therapy, behavioral
exacerbated through it (e.g., family tensions
therapy, existential therapy, psychoeducational
concerning partner behavior or finances).
therapy, group therapy, social support, art ther-
is designed to provide the
apy, and guided imagery. Although the names
patient with information and skills to enable
of most of these interventions are familiar from
optimal use o f medical resources and services
other domains in psychology, their application
Patient education
(e.g., getting services such as psychological
in psycho-oncology is specific in several
help, sex counseling, t a x deductions, and social
respects. First, treatments are focused on the
benefits) while minimizing harassment
and
disease with the more or less implicit assump-
different avoidable difficulties. It also includes
tion that the major goal is to minimize the dis-
skills at getting and evaluating medical infor-
tress occasioned by the disease and
mation, assistance in clarifying misperceptions
treatments. Second, another implicit assump-
and
tion is that if it were not for the cancer, the
misinformation,
and
suggestions
for
improving doctor-patient communication. Adjustment
is designed to reduce as much
the
patient would not be exposed to that particular psychological intervention. Third, interactions
as possible the patient's psychological distress
between psychological and physiological pro-
in the course o f treatment or remission periods
cesses constitute an integral part o f the treat-
by controlling anxiety and depression on a
ment, sometimes as a focal theme (e.g., in the
long-term basis; reducing feelings o f loneli-
life-extending therapies) and other times merely
ness, isolation, and hopelessness; improving
as a fact that is taken into consideration (e.g., in
coping skills; raising the level o f Q O L ; and
treating insomnia, the chemotherapeutic agents
possibly even promoting personal growth and
that the patient is getting are considered).
|
Psycho-oncology are
(Fawzy et al., 2001). Each is based on using the
individual therapy and group therapy. Indivi-
components mentioned previously in a more or
Two
major
modes
of
treatment
dual therapy is more costly in resources but is
less structured manner. Thus, Spiegel's group
tailored better to the needs o f the specific
therapy is based on supporting other patients
patient and shields him or her against the anx-
and receiving support from them, getting family
ieties evoked by the vicissitudes in the state o f
support, getting social support, improving emo-
other patients (i.e., downward comparisons)
tional expression, detoxifying death, reordering
and against the tendency to devote himself
life priorities, facilitating communication with
or herself too much to helping others. For
one's physician, and controlling symptoms (by
objective reasons, individual therapy may be
self-hypnosis, meditation, biofeedback, etc.).
the only option for patients with advanced
Fawzy's psychoeducational
disease. Group therapy is more economical,
based on getting health education
enjoys a good "reputation" in many commu-
informed about cancer), managing stress (iden-
nities, and may be less threatening for patients
tifying sources of stress, identifying one's reac-
(Helgeson, Cohen, Schulz,
8c Y a s c o , 2001).
Each o f the major kinds o f treatment has
intervention
is
(being
tions to stress, and controlling these reactions by hypnosis, relaxation, guided imagery, etc.),
been used for attaining the different goals o f
learning coping skills (using problem-solving
treatments. Thus, group therapy has been used
techniques based on promoting
for life extension, adjustment, and psycho-
approach and weakening avoidance coping),
educational purposes (Fawzy 8c Fawzy, 1998;
and getting psychological support from the staff.
Spira,
1998), and art therapy has been used for
adjustment and vocation o f self-healing potentialities (Luzzatto
8c Gabriel, 1998).
the active
There are four major therapeutic orientations used in the various treatments:
the
dynamic approach, the cognitive-behavioral or
approach, the existential approach, and the
processes used in the various treatments are
cognitive orientation approach. T h e dynamic
T h e major therapeutic
components
getting the support o f others (e.g., patients,
approach is rooted in classical psychotherapy,
health professionals), eliminating mainly neg-
so that it is based on applying transference and
ative affect, sharing one's experiences with
countertransference,
others, learning coping skills, facing one's
past, interpreting dreams and free associations,
fears (including fears o f suffering, pain, and
and using a specific structure o f interplay contents
exploring the
and
process
patient's
death), overcoming despair and strengthening
between
hope, gaining a better understanding o f one-
Massie,
(Sourkes,
self and one's responses, gaining a better
behavioral approach emphasizes the acquisi-
understanding o f the situation (e.g., the dis-
tion o f skills (thoughts, beliefs, or behaviors)
ease, treatments), strengthening one's sense o f
that enable controlling symptoms,
control, and gaining a modicum o f control
stress, pain, and anxiety. T h e most often used
over one's symptoms. Some o f these elements
means are hypnosis, relaxation, systematic
are stronger in some o f the treatments; for
desensitization, guided imagery, and coping
8c Holland, 1998). T h e cognitive-
mainly
8c Hann, 1998). T h e
example, getting support is often stronger in
self-statements (Jacobsen
group therapy,
better
existential approach is based on exploring the
understanding o f oneself and one's responses
function, role, and meaning o f the lives o f
whereas
gaining
a
humans in general and the life o f the patient in
is more salent in individual therapy. There
exist
some
better-known
treat-
particular while examining the authenticity o f
ment protocols, mainly Spiegel's supportive-
assumptions about oneself and the world in
8c Diamond, 2002)
the framework o f one's culture and society
expressive therapy (Spiegel
and Fawzy's psychoeducational
intervention
(Spira,
2000). Finally, the cognitive orientation
345
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS
Dispositio Disposition
Figure 17.3
Motivational Disposition
Formation of the Motivational Disposition ("behavioral intent") According to Cognitive Orientation Theory
approach is based on the assumption that any
However, as noted earlier, their study had
behavior—emotional, motor, cognitive, or
serious methodological shortcomings ( F o x ,
physiological—is the product o f a motivational
1 9 9 8 a ; K o g o n et a l , 1 9 9 7 ) and failed to be
disposition expressing the general direction o f
replicated (Goodwin et al., 2 0 0 1 ; Spiegel
the behavior (formed as a vector based on four
et al., 1 9 9 9 ) . T h e replications showed that
types o f beliefs reflecting underlying meanings)
the
and an operational program implementing the
patients'
actual performance (Figure 1 7 . 3 ) . In the case of
Spiegel et al., 1 9 9 9 ) and did this for only a
physical disorders, the motivational disposi-
short
tion reflects the directionality inherent in spe-
Cunningham, 1 9 9 9 ) .
cific themes unique for each disorder, whereas
intervention
proved
mood
duration
(Goodwin
to
affect
only
et al., 2 0 0 1 ;
(Edmonds, L o c k w o o d , Sc
Fawzy and colleagues ( 2 0 0 1 ) showed that
the operational program is the set o f physio-
their intervention
logical processes underlying the pathological
immune system parameters 6 months after
process (Kreitler & Kreitler, 1 9 9 1 ) . T h e treat-
treatment, but the effects did not differ signif-
ment consists in strengthening
icantly between the intervention and control
the specific
themes expressing health rather
than
the
tendency toward the disorder.
on
groups after 1 year. Furthermore, when the treatment
M o s t treatments report effects in terms o f
had positive effects
and
Breslow depth were
used
together as predictors, treatment did not pre-
improving
dict recurrence of disease but instead predicted
their emotional well-being (Fawzy Sc Fawzy,
survival. T h e results were explained as due to
1 9 9 8 ; Fawzy et al., 2 0 0 1 ; J a c o b s e n S c H a n n ,
better adjustment and coping in the experi-
1998).
on
mental group. However, the number o f par-
survival are less clear. Spiegel and colleagues
ticipants was very small (3 were dead and 3 1
reducing
patients'
T h e effects
distress and
o f the treatments
( 1 9 9 9 ) initially reported a remarkable effect
were alive in the experimental group, and 1 0
o f their intervention on prolonging survival.
were dead and 2 4 were alive in the control
Psycho-oncology
CASE S T U D Y " S . M . " was a 37-year-old w o m a n , married with three daughters, w h o was diagnosed with m e l a n o m a (Stage lib) in the cervix. She underwent surgery and was prescribed a course o f chemotherapy. She kept delaying the beginning o f the treatment with different excuses until she was given up in the hospital files as a case o f "treatment resistance." A year later, she showed up for a regular follow-up, and it turned out that there had been disease recurrence. W h e n the doctor raised the need for chemotherapy, S. M . again declared that she would not undergo any kind o f treatment—"not n o w , not ever." T h e doctor responded by saying, " N o one can be forced to undergo treatment, but I suggest that since you are already here you see the attending psycho-oncologist." S. M . said, " I will do so, but only because you are such a kind doctor w h o has agreed not to prescribe chemotherapy for m e . " At the beginning o f the meeting with the psycho-oncologist, S. M . said that she did not believe in psychology and that all that psychologists had to say was mere "literary fiction." T h e psychologist suggested that S. M . might want to tell the psychologist some "fictional story." S. M . readily agreed and started to tell a story about two sisters, 1 year apart in age, who were so close and inseparable that most of those w h o knew them thought they had a lesbian relationship, which of course was not true. W h e n they were ages 1 6 and 1 7 years, they were invited by their uncle to spend a vacation in Canada. It is there that they went on a boat tour on the St. Lawrence River and one sister accidentally fell off the boat into the water and drowned. T h e other sister did not jump in to try to save her, even though she was a much better swimmer. T h e surviving sister went back to her own country, and because she was now the only child, she was spoiled by her parents and lived happily ever after. T h e psychologist assumed the role o f a "literary critic" and offered the remark that the end o f the story was not convincing from a literary point o f view because it seemed likely that the surviving sister suffered from a sense o f guilt. S. M . rejected the criticism but agreed to explore the possibility that some readers o f such a story might get the " w r o n g " idea that the surviving sister felt guilty. S. M . suggested that the surviving sister m a y have wished the other sister dead for fear that the dead sister would not have allowed her to marry and have children because "they loved t o o m u c h . " T h e psychologist then remarked, " S o m e people w h o feel guilty for actual or imaginary things they have done or felt consider undergoing chemotherapy as a kind o f punishment. W h a t do you t h i n k ? " " F u n n y , " S. M . said, " I thought chemotherapy was quite the contrary—the means for life." S. M . did not c o m e back to the psycho-oncologist, but she underwent the whole course o f chemotherapy without any resistance. At the end o f the treatment, she left the psycho-oncologist a note stating, " F o r me chemotherapy was a punishment, and n o w I have earned my right to live." H e r response to the treatment was good, and after 5 years she was still in full remission. This case illustrates the use o f a brief psychological intervention for resolving noncompliance with treatment and possibly also the contribution o f psychological change to medical remission.
347
348
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS group, at 5- to 6-year follow-up), so that the
for itself a relatively central role in health
stability o f the findings needs to be examined
psychology as well as in oncology. The two
in further replications (Fawzy et al., 2 0 0 1 ) .
main reasons for this are (a) the great need for
As discussed previously, the cognitive ori-
psychological help on the part o f patients and
entation approach has also led to significant
physicians alike (albeit for different reasons)
increases o f survival
and (b) the theoretical and empirical advances
in the
intervention
group that lasted for 1 2 years, which is the
in gaining knowledge and understanding in
follow-up period to date. N o t a b l y , the cogni-
oncology contributed by adding the psycholog-
tive orientation variables predicted survival
ical perspective to the scientific and clinical
together with the medical prognostic vari-
research. Psycho-oncology has become deeply
ables, and the effect o f the psychological fac-
interwoven in the daily practice of and research
tors became more evident the longer the time
in oncology. This has made it possible to detect
since diagnosis (Kreitler, Inbar, & Kreitler,
its contributions in the most varied domains of oncology, ranging from prevention, genetic
1 9 9 9 ; Kreitler et al., 1 9 9 7 ) . Furthermore, there are less conventional
testing, and counseling to decisions about treat-
interventions of the mind-body sphere that
ment, launching of new drugs supported by
mostly share several o f the following compo-
Q O L arguments, and euthanasia. However, if
nents: emphasis on a holistic approach, posi-
psycho-oncology is to maintain its very unique
tive attitude,
mind-body
unity,
personal
responsibility for one's health, belief in the pos-
status in practice and research, three recommendations seem appropriate.
sibility to control bodily processes, and fre-
T h e first recommendation
is increased
quent use o f procedures from the Kabala,
emphasis
Reiki, meditation, and spiritual approaches.
ground variables. This would entail assuming
Results are mostly not evidence based and
an inherently interactional and synergistic
remain equivocal at best. Finally, many cancer patients use psy-
on
considering multiple
back-
approach in every act o f applying psychooncology. T h e second is increased emphasis
chopharmacological drugs for the control o f
on
symptoms in one or another phase o f the dis-
medical variables such as histology, type o f
considering
ease. T h e most frequently prescribed drugs
tumor, and genetic aspects. This would lead
are antidepressants and antianxiety medica-
to widening the scope and generalizability o f
tions as well as analgesics and insomnia-
psycho-oncological findings. T h e third
controlling drugs (Massie & Lesko, 1 9 8 9 ) .
adopting
a
a variety o f specifically
broader
and
more
is
creative
approach to identifying and testing purely psychological variables. This would enable SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS
enriching the host o f familiar variables, such as depression and emotional suppression, that
Psycho-oncology is one of the most active
have contributed to advancing the field up to
domains of health psychology and has earned
the current level.
REFERENCES Aaronson, N. K., Ahmedzai, S., Bergman, B., Bullinger, M., & Cull, A. (1993). The European Organization for the Research and Treatment of Cancer QLQ-C30: A quality of life instrument for use in international clinical trials in oncology. Journal of the National Cancer Institute, 85, 3 6 5 - 3 7 6 .
Psycho-Oncology Alter, C. L., Pelcovitz, D., Axelrod, Α., Goldenberg, B . , Harris, H , Meyers, B . , Grobois, B . , Mandel, F., Septimus, Α., Sc Kaplan, S. (1996). Identification of PTSD in cancer survivors. Psychosomatics, 37, 1 3 7 - 1 4 3 . American Psychiatric Association. (1988). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Andreasen, N. C , 8c Hoenk, P. R. (1982). The predictive value of adjustment disorders: A follow-up study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 139, 5 8 4 - 5 9 0 . Andrykowski, M . A. (1994). Psychiatric and psychosocial aspects of bone marrow transplantation. Psychosomatics, 35, 1 3 - 2 4 . Back, A. L., Wallace, J . I., Starks, Η. E., & Pearlman, R. A. (1996). Physician-assisted suicide and euthanasia in Washington State: Patient requests and physician responses. Journal of the American Medical Association, 275, 9 1 9 - 9 2 5 . Barraclough, J . , Pinder, P., Crudas, M., Osmond, C , Taylor, I., Sc Perry, M. (1992). Life events and breast cancer prognosis. British Medical Journal, 304, 1 0 7 8 - 1 0 8 1 . Berwick, M., Begg, C , Fine, J . Α., Roush, G. C , Se Barnhill, R. L. (1996). Screening for cutaneous melanoma by skin self-examination. Journal of the National Cancer Institute, 88, 1 7 - 2 3 . Bloom, J . R. (1986). Social support and adjustment to breast cancer. In B . L. perspectives. New York: Andersen (Ed.), Women with cancer: Psychological Springer. Borg, G. (1982). Psychophysical bases of perceived exertion. Medical Science of Sports and Exercise, 14, 3 7 7 - 3 8 7 . Bovbjerg, D., 8c Valdimarsdottir, H. B . ( 1 9 9 8 ) . Psychoneuroimmunology: Implications for psycho-oncology. In J . C. Holland (Ed.), Psycho-oncology (pp. 1 2 5 - 1 3 4 ) . New York: Oxford University Press. Breitbart, W. (1990). Cancer pain and suicide. In K. Foley, J . J . Bonica, 8c V. Ventrafridda (Eds.), Advances in pain research and therapy (Vol. 16, pp. 3 9 9 - 4 1 2 ) . New York: Raven. Breitbart, W., Rosenfeld, B . , Roth, Α., Smith, M., Cohen, M . D., 8c Passik, S. D. (1997). The Memorial Delirium Assessment Scale. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management, 13, 1 2 8 - 1 3 7 . Buddeberg, C , Sieber, M., Wolf, C , Landolt-Ritter, C , Richter, D., 8c Steiner, R. (1996). Are coping strategies related to disease outcome in early breast cancer? Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 40, 2 5 5 - 2 6 3 . Burman, B . , 8c Margolin, G. (1992). Analysis of the relationship between marital relationships and health problems: An interactional perspective. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 3 9 - 6 3 . Campbell, P. C. (1996). Suicides among cancer patients. Connecticut Health Bulletin, 80, 2 0 7 - 2 1 2 . Carroll, B. T., Kathol, R. G., Noyés, R., Wald, T. G., 8c Clamon, G. H. (1993). Screening for depression and anxiety in cancer patients using the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. General Hospital Psychiatry, 15, 6 9 - 7 4 . Cassileth, B. R., Lusk, E. J . , Miller, D. S., Brown, L. L., 8c Miller, C. (1985). Psychosocial correlates of survival in advanced malignant disease? New England Journal of Medicine, 312, 1 5 5 1 - 1 5 5 5 . Cassileth, B. R., Walsh, W. P., 8c Lusk, E. J . (1988). Psychosocial correlates of cancer survival: A subsequent report 3 to 8 years after cancer diagnosis. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 6, 1 7 5 3 - 1 7 5 9 . Cella, D. F., Tulsky, D. S., Gray, G., Sarafin, B., Linn, E., Bonomi, Α., Silberman, M., Yellen, S. B . , Winicour, P., Brannon, J . , Eckberg, K., Lloyd, S., Purl, S., Blendowski, C , Goodman, M., Barnicle, M., Stewart, L, McHale, M., Bonomi, P., Kaplan, E., Taylor, S., IV, Thomas, C. R., J r . , 8c Harris, J . (1993). The
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS Functional Assessment of Cancer Therapy (FACT) scale: Development and validation of the general version. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 11, 5 7 0 - 5 7 9 . Chaitchik, S., & Kreitler, S. (1991). Induced versus spontaneous attendance of breast screening tests by women. Journal of Cancer Education, 6, 4 3 - 5 3 . Chaitchik, S., Kreitler, S., Rapoport, Y . , & Algor, R. (1992). What do cancer patients' spouses know about the patients? Cancer Nursing, 15, 3 5 3 - 3 6 2 . Chaitchik, S., Kreitler, S., Schwartz, I., Shaked, S., Sc Rosin, R. (1992). Doctor-patient communication in a cancer ward. Journal of Cancer Education, 7, 4 1 - 5 4 . Cherny, Ν. I. (1996). The problem of inadequately relieved suffering. Journal of Social Issues, 52, 1 3 - 3 0 . Chochinov, H. M., Tataryn, D., Clinch, J . J . , Sc Dudgeon, D. (1999). Will to live in the terminally ill. Lancet, 354, 8 1 6 - 8 1 9 . Chochinov, H. M., Wilson, K. G., Enns, M., Mowchun, M., Lander, S., Levitt, M., & Clinch, J . J . (1995). Desire for death in the terminally ill. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 1 1 8 5 - 1 1 9 1 . Cinciripini, P. (1995). Current trends in smoking cessation research: Psychological therapy, nicotine replacement, and changes in smoking behavior. Cancer Bulletin, 47, 2 5 9 - 2 6 3 . Clark, Α., 8c Fallowfield, L. J . (1986). Quality of life measurements in patients with malignant disease: A review. Journal of Social Medicine, 79, 1 6 5 - 1 6 9 . Cody, M., Nichols, S., Brennan, C , Armes, J . , Wilson, P., & Slevin, M. (1994). Psychosocial factors and lung cancer prognosis. Psycho-Oncology, 3, 1 4 1 . Cohen, L., & Baum, A. (2001). Targets for interventions to reduce cancer morbidity. In A. Baum & B. L. Andersen (Eds.), Psychosocial interventions for cancer (pp. 3 2 1 - 3 4 0 ) . Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Cordova, M . J . , Andrykowski, Μ . Α., Redd, W. H., Kenady, D. E., McGrath, P. C , Sc Sloan, D. A. (1995). Frequency and correlates of posttraumatic stress disorder-like symptoms after treatment for breast cancer. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 9 8 1 - 9 8 6 . Courtney, J . G., Longnecker, M . P., Theorell, T., & Gerhardsson de Verdier, M. (1993). Stressful life events and the risk of colorectal cancer. Epidemiology, 4, 407-414. Daut, R. L., Cleeland, C. S., & Flanery, R. C. (1983). Development of the Wisconsin Brief Pain Questionnaire to assess pain in cancer and other diseases. Pain, 17, 1 9 7 - 2 1 0 . Dean, C , & Surtees, P. G. (1989). Do psychological factors predict survival in Research, 33, 5 6 1 - 5 6 9 . breast cancer? Journal of Psychosomatic DeFlorio, M., Sc Massie, M. J . (1995). Review of depression in cancer: Gender differences. Depression, 3, 6 6 - 8 0 . de Haes, J . , Curran, D., Young, T., Bottomley, Α., Flechtner, H., Aaronson, N., 8c Blazeby, J . (2000). Quality of life evaluation in oncological clinical trials: The E O R T C model. European Journal of Cancer, 36, 8 2 1 - 8 2 5 . de Haes, J . C. J . M., van Kippenberg, F. C. E., & Neijt, J . P. (1990). Measuring psychological and physical distress in cancer patients: Structure and application of the Rotterdam Symptom Checklist. British Journal of Cancer, 62, 1 0 3 4 - 1 0 3 8 . Derogatis, J . R. (1993). The Brief Symptom Inventory: Administration, scoring, and procedures manual. Minneapolis, M N : National Computer Systems. Derogatis, L., Abeloff, M., & Melisaratos, N. (1979). Psychological coping mechanism and survival time in metastatic breast cancer. Journal of the American Medical Association, 242, 1 5 0 4 - 1 5 0 8 . Derogatis, L. R., Morrow, G. R., Fetting, D., Penman, D., Piasetsky, S., Schmale, A. M., Henrichs, M., & Carnicke, C. L., Jr. (1983). The prevalence of psychiatric disorders among cancer patients. Journal of the American Medical Association, 249, 7 5 1 - 7 5 7 .
Psycho-Oncology DiPlacido, J . , Zauber, Α., & Redd, W. H. (1998). Psychosocial issues in cancer screening. In J . C. Holland (Ed.), Psycho-oncology (pp. 1 6 1 - 1 7 2 ) . New York: Oxford University Press. Dunkel-Schetter, C. (1984). Social support and cancer: Findings based on patient interviews and their implications. Journal of Social Issues, 40, 7 7 - 9 8 . Edmonds, C. V. I., Lockwood, G. Α., 8c Cunningham, J . A. (1999). Psychological response to long term group therapy: A randomized trial with metastatic breast cancer patients. Psycho-Oncology, 8, 7 1 - 9 0 . Edwards, J . R., Cooper, C. L., Pearl, G., de Paredes, E. S., O'Leary, T., 8c Wilhelm, M. C. (1990). The relationship between psychosocial factors and breast cancer: Some unexpected results. Behavioral Medicine, 16, 5 - 1 4 . Ell, K., Nishimoto, R., Mediansky, L., Mantell, J . , 8c Hamovitch, M . (1992). Social relations, social support, and survival among patients with cancer. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 36, 5 3 1 - 5 4 1 . Emanuel, E. J . , Fairclough, C. L., Daniels, E. R., & Clarridge, B . R. (1996). Euthanasia and physician-assisted suicide: Attitudes and experiences of oncology patients, oncologists, and the public. Lancet, 347, 1 8 0 5 - 1 8 1 0 . Fawzy, F. I., 8c Fawzy, N. W. (1998). Psychoeducational interventions. In J . C. Holland (Ed.), Psycho-oncology (pp. 6 7 6 - 6 9 3 ) . New York: Oxford University Press. Fawzy, F. I., Fawzy, N. W., 8c Canada, A. L. (2001). Psychoeducational intervention programs for patients with cancer. In A. Baum Se B . L. Andersen (Eds.), Psychosocial interventions for cancer (pp. 2 3 5 - 2 6 7 ) . Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Felton, B. J . , Sc Revenson, T. A. (1984). Coping with chronic illness: A study of illness controllability and the influence of coping strategies on psychological adjustment. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 52, 3 4 3 - 3 5 3 . Figer, Α., Kreitler, S., Michal, M . M., Se Inbar, M . (2002). Personality dispositions of colon cancer patients. Gastrointestinal Oncology, 4, 8 1 - 9 2 . Finn, Ο. J . (2001). Assessing the important effector mechanisms in the immune response against cancer. In A. Baum Se B. L. Andersen (Eds.), Psychosocial interventions for cancer (pp. 1 7 5 - 1 9 1 ) . Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Fleishman, S. B . , Lesko, L. M., Sc Breitbart, W. (1993). Treatment of organic mental disorders in cancer patients. In W. Breitbart Sc J . C. Holland (Eds.), Psychiatric aspects of symptom management in cancer patients (pp. 2 3 - 4 8 ) . Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Folstein, M . F., Fetting, J . H., Lobo, Α., Niaz, U., 8c Capozolli, K. (1984). Cognitive assessment of cancer patients. Cancer, 53(Suppl.), 2 2 5 0 - 2 2 5 5 . Fox, Β. H. (1978). Cancer death risk in hospitalized mental patients. Science, 201, 966-968. Fox, Β . H. (1989). Depressive symptoms and risk of cancer [editorial]. Journal of the American Medical Association, 262, 2 3 1 . Fox, B. H. (1998a). A hypothesis about Spiegel et al.'s 1 9 8 9 paper on psychosocial intervention and breast cancer survival. Psycho-Oncology, 7, 3 6 1 - 3 7 0 . Fox, Β . H. (1998b). Psychosocial factors in cancer incidence and prognosis. In J. C. Holland (Ed.), Psycho-oncology (pp. 110-124). New York: Oxford University Press. Ganz, P., 8c Coscarelli, A. (1995). Quality of life after breast cancer: A decade of research. In J . E. Dimsdale 8c A. Baum (Eds.), Quality of life in behavioral medicine research (pp. 9 7 - 1 1 3 ) . Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Ganz, P., Coscarelli Schag, C. Α., Kahn, B., Petersen, L., Se Hirji, K. (1993). Describing the health-related quality of life impact of HIV infection: Findings of a study using the HIV Overview of Problem Evaluation Systems (HOPES). Quality of Life, 2, 1 0 9 - 1 1 9 .
351
352
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS Garfinkel, J . (1995). Cancer statistics and trends. In G. P. Murphy, J . W. Lawrence, 8c R. E. Lenhard (Eds.), American Cancer Society textbook of clinical oncology (pp. 1-11). Washington, DC: American Cancer Society. Garssen, B . , 8c Goodkin, K. (1999). On the role of immunological factors as mediPsychiatry ators between psychosocial factors and cancer progression. Research, 85, 5 1 - 6 1 . Gellert, G. Α., Maxwell, R. M., 8c Siegel, B. S. (1993). Survival of breast cancer patients receiving adjunctive psychosocial support therapy: A 10-year followup study. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 11, 66-69. Geyer, S. (1991). Life events prior to manifestation of breast cancer: A limited of prospective study covering eight years before diagnosis. Journal Psychosomatic Research, 35, 3 5 5 - 3 6 3 . Goodkin, K., 8c Visser, A. P. (Eds.). (2000). Psychoneuroimmunology: Stress, mental disorders, and health. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Goodwin, J . S., Hunt, W. C , Key, C. R., 8c Samet, J . M . (1987). The effect of marital status on stage, treatment, and survival of cancer patients. Journal of the American Medical Association, 258, 3 1 2 5 - 3 1 3 0 . Goodwin, P. J . , Leszcz, M., Ennis, M., Koopmans, J . , Vincent, L., Guther, H., Drysdale, E., Hundleby, M., Chochinov, H. M., Navarro, M., Speca, M., 8c Hunter, J . (2001). The effect of group psychosocial support on survival in metastatic breast cancer. New England Journal of Medicine, 345, 1 7 1 9 - 1 7 2 6 . Green, L. W., Rimer, E. T., 8c Elwood, J . M. (1981). Behavioral approaches to cancer prevention and detection. In S. Weiss, A. Herd, 8c B. Fox (Eds.), Perspectives on behavioral medicine (pp. 2 1 5 - 2 3 4 ) . New York: Academic Press. Greenberg, D. B . , Goorin, Α., Gebhardt, M . C , Petersen, L., 8c Hirji, K. (1994). Quality of life in osteosarcoma survivors. Oncology, 8, 1 9 - 2 5 . Greer, S., Morris, T., 8c Pettingale K. W. (1979). Psychological response to breast cancer: Effect on outcome. Lancet, 2, 7 8 5 - 7 8 7 . Greer, S., Morris, T., Pettingale K. W., Sc Haybittle, J . L. (1990). Psychological response to breast cancer and 15 years outcome. Lancet, 1, 4 9 - 5 0 . Gritz, E. R., Wellisch, D. K., 8c Landsverk, J . A. (1988). Psychosocial sequelae in longterm survivors of testicular cancer. Journal of Psychosocial Oncology, 6, 4 1 - 6 3 . Grossarth-Maticek, R., Kanazir, D. T., Schmidt, P., 8c Vetter, H. (1985). Psychosocial and organic variables as predictors of lung cancer, cardiac infarct, and Individual and apoplexy: Some differential predictors. Personality Differences, 6, 3 1 3 - 3 2 1 . Gulbinat, W., DuPont, Α., Jablensky, Α., Jensen, Ο. M., Marsella, Α., Nakane, Y . , 8c Sartorius, N. (1992). Cancer incidence of schizophrenic patients: Results of Psychiatry, record linkage studies in three countries. British Journal of 26i(Suppl. 18), 7 5 - 8 3 . Heim, E., Augustini, K. F., Schaffner, L., 8c Valach, L. (1993). Coping with breast cancer over time and situation. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 37, 5 2 3 - 5 4 2 . Helgeson, V. S., Cohen, S., Schulz, R., Sc Yasco, J . (2001). Group support interventions for people with cancer: Benefits and hazards. In A. Baum 8c B . L. interventions for cancer (pp. 2 6 9 - 2 8 6 ) . Andersen (Eds.), Psychosocial Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Helsing, K. J . , Comstock, G. W., 8c Szklo, M . (1982). Causes of death in a widowed 116, 5 2 4 - 5 3 2 . population. American Journal of Epidemiology, Hislop, T. G., Waxier, Ν . E., Coldman, A. J . , Elwood, J . M., Sc Kan, L. (1987). The prognostic significance of psychosocial factors in women with breast cancer. Journal of Chronic Diseases, 40, 7 2 9 - 7 3 5 . Holland, J . C , Korzun, A. H., Tross, S., Cella, D. F., Norton, L., Sc Wood, W. (1986). Psychosocial factors and disease-free survival in stage II breast cancer. Proceedings of the American Society of Clinical Oncology, 5, 2 3 7 .
Psycho-Oncology Hoyert, D. L., Kochanek, K. D., & Murphy, S. L. (1999). Deaths: Final data for 1997. National Vital Statistics Reports, 47, 1-104. Ingelfinger, F. J . (1975). Cancer! Alarm! Cancer! [editorial]. New England Journal of Medicine, 293, 1 3 1 9 - 1 3 2 0 . Jacobs, J . W. (1977). Screening for organic mental syndromes in the medically ill. Annals of Internal Medicine, 86, 4 0 - 4 6 . Jacobs, J . , Ostroff, J . , 8c Steinglass, P. (1998). Family therapy: A systems approach to cancer care. In J . C. Holland (Ed.), Psycho-oncology (pp. 9 9 4 - 1 0 0 3 ) . New York: Oxford University Press. Jacobsen, P. B . , 8c Hann, D. M. (1998). Cognitive-behavioral interventions. In J . C. Holland (Ed.), Psycho-oncology (pp. 7 1 7 - 7 2 9 ) . New York: Oxford University Press. Jamison, R. N., Burish, T. G., & Wallston, K. A. (1987). Psychogenic factors in predicting survival of breast cancer patients. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 5, 768-772. Janz, Ν. K., 8c Becker, M . H. (1984). The health belief model: A decade later. Health Education Quarterly, 11, 1-47. Jensen, M . R. (1987). Psychobiological factors predicting the course of cancer. Journal of Personality, 55, 3 2 9 - 3 4 2 . Joffres, M., Reed, D. M., 8c Nomura, Α. M . Y . (1985). Psychosocial processes and cancer incidence among Japanese men in Hawaii. American Journal of Epidemiology, 121, 4 8 8 - 5 0 0 . Johnson, G., Burvill, J . G., Anderson, C. S., Jamrozik, K., Stewart-Wynne, E. G., 8c Chakera, T. (1995). Screening instruments for depression and anxiety following stroke: Experience in the Perth Community Stroke Study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 91, 2 5 2 - 2 5 7 . J0rgensen, P., 8c Mortensen, P. B. (1992). Cause of death in reactive psychosis. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 85, 3 5 1 - 3 5 3 . Justice, A. (1985). Review of the effects of stress on cancer in laboratory animals: Importance of time of stress application and type of tumor. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 1 0 8 - 1 3 8 . Kaprio, J . , Koskenvuo, M., 8c Rita, H. (1987). Mortality after bereavement: A prospective study of 9 5 , 6 4 7 widowed persons. American Journal of Public Health, 77, 2 8 3 - 2 8 7 . Kash, K. M., 8c Lerman, C. (1998). Psychological, social, and ethical issues in gene testing. In J . C. Holland (Ed.), Psycho-oncology (pp. 1 9 6 - 2 0 7 ) . New York: Oxford University Press. Keehn, R. J . (1980). Follow-up studies of World War II and Korean conflict prisoners of war. American Journal of Epidemiology, 111, 1 9 4 - 2 0 0 . Keehn, R. J . , Goldberg, I. D., 8e Beebe, G. W. (1974). Twenty four year mortality follow-up of army veterans with disability separation for psychoneurosis in 1944. Psychosomatic Medicine, 36, 2 7 - 4 6 . Keller, M., Henrich, G., Sellschopp, Α., Sc Beutel, M . (1995). Between distress and support: Spouses of cancer patients. In L. Baider, C. L. Cooper, Sc A. Kaplan De-Nour (Eds.), Cancer and the family (pp. 1 8 7 - 2 2 4 ) . Chichester, UK: Wiley. Kiecolt-Glaser, J . K., Sc Glaser, R. (1999). Psychoneuroimmunology and immunotoxicology: Implications for carcinogenesis. Psychosomatic Medicine, 61, 2 7 1 - 2 7 2 . Kiernan, R. J . , Mueller, J . , Langston, J . W., 8c Van Dyke, C. (1987). The Neurobehavioral Cognitive Status Examination: A brief but differentiated approach to cognitive assessment. Annals of Internal Medicine, 107, 4 8 1 - 4 8 5 . Kissane, D. W., Block, S., McKenzie, M., McDowall, A. C , Sc Nitzan, R. (1998). Family grief therapy: A preliminary account of a new model to promote healthy family functioning during palliative care and bereavement. Psycho-Oncology, 7, 1 4 - 2 5 .
354
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS Koch, IL, Hàrter, M., Jakob, U., & Siegrist, B. (1995). Parental reactions to cancer in their children. In L. Baider, C. L. Cooper, & A. Kaplan De-Nour (Eds.), Cancer and the family (pp. 1 4 9 - 1 7 0 ) . Chichester, UK: Wiley. Kogon, M . M., Biswas, Α., Pearl, D., Carlson, R. W., & Spiegel, D. (1997). Effects of medical and psychotherapeutic treatment on the survival of women with metastatic breast carcinoma. Cancer, 80, 2 2 5 - 2 3 0 . Kornblith, A. B. (1998). Psychosocial adaptation of cancer survivors. In J . C. (pp. 2 2 3 - 2 5 4 ) . New York: Oxford University Holland (Ed.), Psycho-oncology Press. Kovner, F., & Kreitler, S. (1996). Effect of palliative radiation therapy on quality of life in cancer patients. Radiology, 201 (Suppl.), 2 3 1 . Kreitler, H., & Kreitler, S. (1982). The theory of cognitive orientation: Widening the scope of behavior prediction. In B . Maher & W. B. Maher (Eds.), Progress in experimental personality research (Vol. 1 1 , pp. 1 0 1 - 1 6 9 ) . New York: Academic Press. Kreitler, S. (1998, August-September). Undergoing tests for the early detection of cancer: The cognitive orientation approach. Lecture delivered at the conference of the European Health Psychology Society, Vienna, Austria. Kreitler, S. (1999). Denial in cancer patients. Cancer Investigation, 17, 5 1 4 - 5 3 4 . Kreitler, S., & Chaitchik, S. (1993). Surviving cancer: The psychological problem. Topics on Supportive Care in Oncology, 10, 1 2 - 1 4 . Kreitler, S., & Chaitchik, S. (1995). The paradox of the self-diagnosed lump in the Letters, 6, 3 9 - 5 2 . breast. Psycho-Oncology Kreitler, S., Chaitchik, S., Kovner, F., & Kreitler, H. (1992). Information in breast cancer patients: The good and the bad news [abstract]. Journal of Cancer Education, 7(Suppl.), 2 5 . Kreitler, S., Chaitchik, S., & Kreitler, H. (1990). The psychological profile of women attending breast-screening tests. Social Science and Medicine, 31, 1 1 7 7 - 1 1 8 5 . Kreitler, S., Chaitchik, S., Kreitler, H., & Weissler, K. (1994). Who will attend tests for the early detection of breast cancer? Psychology and Health, 9, 4 6 3 - 4 8 3 . Kreitler, S. Chaitchik, S., Rapoport, Y . , & Algor, R. (1995). Psychosocial effects of information and disease severity in head-and-neck cancer patients. Journal of Cancer Education, 10, 1 4 4 - 1 5 4 . Kreitler, S., Chaitchik, S., Rapoport, Y . , Kreitler, H., & Algor, R. (1993). Life satisfaction and health in cancer patients, orthopedic patients, and healthy individuals. Social Science and Medicine, 36, 5 4 7 - 5 5 6 . Kreitler, S., Inbar, M., & Kreitler, H. (1999, September). Psychosocial factors predicting disease course in cancer patients. Paper presented at the European Cancer Conference, Vienna, Austria. Kreitler, S., & Kreitler, H. (1991). Cognitive orientation and physical disease or health. European Journal of Personality, 5, 1 0 9 - 1 2 9 . Kreitler, S., Kreitler, H., & Chaitchik, S. (1993). Repressiveness: Cause or result of cancer? Psycho-Oncology, 2, 4 3 - 5 4 . Kreitler, S., Kreitler, H., Chaitchik, S., Shaked, S., & Shaked, T. (1997). Psychological and medical predictors of disease course in breast cancer: A prospective study. European Journal of Personality, 11, 3 8 3 - 4 0 0 . Kreitler, S., Levavi, H., & Bornstein, G. (1996). Personality factors and cervical premalignancy. Personality and Individual Differences, 21, 8 8 3 - 8 9 0 . Kreitler, S., & Merimsky, O. (in press). Pain and suffering in cancer patients. In D. Beltrutti, S. Kreitler, A. Lamberto, & D. Niv (Eds.), Handbook of chronic pain. New York: Springer. Kreitler, S., Shahar, Α., & Kreitler, H. (1976). Cognitive orientation, type of smoker, and behavior therapy of smoking. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 49, 1 6 7 - 1 7 5 .
Psycho-Oncology Kubler-Ross, Ε. (1969). On death and dying. New York: Macmillan. Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress appraisal and coping. New York: Springer. Lederberg, M. (1998). The family of the cancer patient. In J . C. Holland (Ed.), Psycho-oncology (pp. 9 8 1 - 9 9 3 ) . New York: Oxford University Press. Lesko, L. M. (1993). Psychiatric management of eating disorders in cancer patients. In W. Breitbart Sc J . C. Holland (Eds.), Psychiatric aspects of symptom management in cancer patients (pp. 8 7 - 1 0 6 ) . Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Levav, L, Kohn, R., Iscovich, J . , Abramson, J . H., Tsai, W. Y., & Vigdorovich, D. (2000). Cancer incidence and survival following bereavement. American Journal of Public Health, 90, 1 6 0 1 - 1 6 0 7 . Levy, S. M., Herberman, R. B., Lippman, M., D'Angelo, T., & Lee, J . (1991). Immunological and psychosocial predictors of disease recurrence in patients with early stage breast cancer. Behavioral Medicine, 17, 6 7 - 7 5 . Luzzatto, P., & Gabriel, B. (1998). Art psychotherapy. In J . C. Holland (Ed.), Psycho-oncology (pp. 7 4 3 - 7 5 7 ) . New York: Oxford University Press. Massie, M., Gagnon, P., &c Holland, J . (1994). Depression and suicide in patients with cancer. Journal of Pain Symptom Management, 9, 3 5 2 - 3 3 1 . Massie, M . J . , & Lesko, L. M. (1989). Psychopharmacological management. In J . C. Holland & J . H. Rowland (Eds.), Handbook of psycho-oncology: Psychological care of the patient with cancer (pp. 4 7 0 - 4 9 1 ) . New York: Oxford University Press. Massie, J . M., & Popkin, M. K. (1998). Depressive disorders. In J . C. Holland (Ed.), Psycho-oncology (pp. 5 1 8 - 5 4 0 ) . New York: Oxford University Press. McHorney, C. Α., Ware, J . E., & Raczek, A. E. (1993). The M O S 36 item short form health survey (SF-36): Psychometric and clinical tests of validity in measuring physical and mental health constructs. Medical Care, 31, 2 4 7 - 2 6 3 . McNair, D. M., Lorr, M., & Droppelman, L. F. (1971). Profile of mood states. San Diego: Educational and Industrial Testing Service. Melzack, R. (1975). The McGill Pain Questionnaire: Major properties and scoring methods. Pain, 1, 2 7 7 - 2 9 9 . Morgenstern, H., Gellert, G. Α., Walter, S. D., Ostfeld, A. M., & Siegel, B. S. (1984). The impact of a psychosocial support program on survival with breast cancer: The importance of selection bias in program evaluation. Journal of Chronic Diseases, 37, 2 7 3 - 2 8 2 . Morris, T., Pettingale, K. W., & Haybittle, J . (1992). Psychological response to cancer diagnosis and disease outcome in patients with breast cancer and lymphoma. Psycho-Oncology, 1, 1 0 5 - 1 1 4 . Murray, M., Sc McMillan, C. (1993). Health beliefs, locus of control, emotional control, and women's cancer screening behavior. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 32, 8 7 - 1 0 0 . Musselman, D. L., McDaniel, J . S., Porter, M. R., 8t Nemeroff, C. B. (1998). Psychoneuroendocrinology and cancer. In J . C. Holland (Ed.), Psychooncology (pp. 1 3 5 - 1 4 3 ) . New York: Oxford University Press. Neale, Α. V., Tilley, B . C , Sc Vernon, S. W. (1986). Marital status, delay in seeking treatment, and survival from breast cancer. Social Science and Medicine, 23, 3 0 5 - 3 1 2 . Noyes, R., Jr., Holt, C. S., & Massie, M . J . (1998). Anxiety disorders. In J . C. Holland (Ed.), Psycho-oncology (pp. 5 4 8 - 5 6 3 ) . New York: Oxford University Press. O'Malley, J . E., Koocher, G., Foster, D., Se Slavin, L. (1979). Psychiatric sequelae of surviving childhood cancer. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 49, 608-616.
356
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS Penman, D. T. (1980). Coping strategies in adaptation to mastectomy. Dissertation 40, B 5 8 2 5 . Abstracts International, Perskey, V. W., Kempthorne-Rawson, J . , & Shekelle, R. B . (1987). Personality and Psychosomatic risk of cancer: 20-year follow-up of the Western Electric Study. Medicine, 49, 4 3 5 - 4 4 9 . Pettingale, K. W., Morris, T., Greer, S., & Haybittle, J . (1985). Mental attitudes to cancer: An additional prognostic factor. Lancet, 1, 7 5 0 . Polednak, A. P., Lane, D. S., & Burg, M . A. (1991). Risk perception, family history, and use of breast cancer screening tests. Cancer Detection and Prevention, 15, 257-263. Popkin, M . K., &C Tucker, G. J . (1992). Secondary and drug-induced mood, anxiety, psychotic, catatonic, and personality disorders. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 4, 3 6 9 - 3 8 5 . Portenoy, R. K., Thaler, H. T., Kornblith A. B . , Lepore, J . M., Friedlander-Klar, H., Kiyasu, E., Sobel, K., Coyle, N., Kemeny, N., & Norton, L. (1994). The Memorial Symptom Assessment Scale: An instrument for the evaluation of symptom prevalence, characteristics, and distress. European Journal of Cancer, 30A, 1 3 2 6 - 1 3 3 6 . Priestman, T. J . , Priestman, S. G., & Bradshaw, C. (1985). Stress and breast cancer. British Journal of Cancer, 51, 4 9 3 - 4 9 8 . Quesada, J . R., Talpaz, M., Rios, Α., Kurzrock, R., & Gutterman, J . (1986). Clinical toxicity of interferon in cancer patients: A review. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 4, 2 3 4 - 2 4 3 . Rapoport, Y . , Kreitler, S., Chaitchik, S., Algor, R., 6c Weissler, K. (1993). Psychosocial problems in head-and-neck cancer patients and their change with disease duration. Annals of Oncology, 4, 6 9 - 7 3 . Ratcliffe, Μ. Α., Dawson, Α. Α., & Walker, L. G. (1995). Eysenck Personality Inventory L-scores in patients with Hodgkin's disease and non-Hodgkin's lymphoma. Psycho-Oncology, 4, 3 9 - 4 5 . Reynolds, P., & Kaplan, G. A. (1990). Social connections and risk for cancer: Prospective evidence from the Alameda County Study. Behavioral Medicine, 16, 1 0 1 - 1 1 0 . Richardson, H., Shelton, D. R., Krailo, M., & Levine, A. M . (1990). The effect of compliance with treatment on survival among patients with hematologic malignancies. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 8, 3 5 6 - 3 6 4 . Ringdal, G. I. (1995). Correlates of hopelessness in cancer patients. Journal of Psychosocial Oncology, 13, 4 7 - 6 6 . Rogentine, G. N., van Kammen, D. P., Fox, B . H., Docherty, J . P., Rosenblatt, J . E., Boyd, S. C , & Bunney, W. E., J r . (1979). Psychological factors in the prognosis of malignant melanoma: A prospective study. Psychosomatic Medicine, 41, 647-655. Rosenfeld, B. (2000). Assisted suicide, depression, and the right to die. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 6, 4 6 7 - 4 8 8 . Rossi, J . S., Biais, L. M., Redding, C. Α., & Weinstock, A. M . (1995). Preventing skin cancer through behavior change. Dermatological Clinics, 13, 6 1 3 - 6 2 2 . Rowland, J . H. (1989). Intrapersonal resources: Coping. In J . C. Holland & J . H. Rowland (Eds.), Handbook of psycho-oncology: Psychological care of the patient with cancer (pp. 4 4 - 5 7 ) . New York: Oxford University Press. Scheier, M. F., &c Carver, C. S. (2001). Adapting to cancer: The importance of hope and purpose. In A. Baum & B . L. Andersen (Eds.), Psychosocial interventions for cancer (pp. 1 5 - 3 6 ) . Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Schonfield, J . (1972). Psychological factors related to delayed return to an earlier life-style in successfully treated cancer patients. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 16, 4 1 - 4 6 .
Psycho-Oncology Schueler, G., 8t Fox, Β. Η. (1991). Questions about Grossarth-Maticek's procedures and results. Psychological Inquiry, 2, 2 5 7 - 2 6 1 . SEER Program. (2002). Cancer incidence in the U.S. Washington, DC: National Cancer Institute. Retrieved on January 2 0 , 2 0 0 3 , from http://seer.cancer.gov/ publications/raterisk Servaes, P., Vingerhoets, Α., Vreugdenhil, G., Keuning, J . , St Broekhuijsen, M. A. ( 1 9 9 6 ) . Breast cancer and nonexpression of emotions. Psychosomatic Medicine, 58, 67. Severson, Κ. T. (1997). Dying cancer patients: Choices at the end of life. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management, 14, 9 4 - 9 8 . Shekelle, R. B., Raynor, W. J . , Ostfeld, A. M., St Garron, L. A. (1981). Psychological depression and 17-year risk of death from cancer. Psychosomatic Medicine, 43, 1 1 7 - 1 2 3 . Smets, Ε. Μ. Α., Garssen, B . , Bonke, B . , St de Haes, J . C. J . M . (1995). The Multidimensional Fatigue Inventory (MFI): Psychometric qualities of an instrument to assess fatigue. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 39, 3 1 5 - 3 2 5 . Sneeuw, K. C. Α., Aaronson, Ν. K., Yarnold, J . R., Broderick, M., Regan, J . , Ross, G., & Goddard, A. (1992). Cosmetic and functional outcomes of breast conserving treatment for early stage breast cancer: II. Relationship with psychosocial functioning. Radiotherapy and Oncology, 25, 1 6 0 - 1 6 6 . Sourkes, Β . M., Massie, M . J . , 8t Holland, J . C. (1998). Psychotherapeutic issues. In J . C. Holland (Ed.), Psycho-oncology (pp. 6 9 4 - 7 0 0 ) . New York: Oxford University Press. Spiegel, D., St Diamond, S. (2002). Psychosocial interventions in cancer: Group therapy techniques. In A. Baum St B . L. Andersen (Eds.), Psychosocial interventions for cancer (pp. 2 1 5 - 2 3 3 ) . Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Spiegel, D., Morrow, G. R., Classen, C , Raubertas, R., Stott, P. B., Mudaliar, N., Pierce, H. I., Flynn, P. J . , Heard, L., St Riggs, G. (1999). Group psychotherapy for recently diagnosed breast cancer patients: A multicenter feasibility study. Psycho-Oncology, 8, 4 8 2 - 4 9 3 . Spira, J . L. (1998). Group therapies. In J . C. Holland (Ed.), Psycho-oncology (pp. 7 0 1 - 7 1 6 ) . New York: Oxford University Press. Spira, J . L. (2000). Existential psychotherapy in palliative care. In H. M. Chochinov St W. Breitbart (Eds.), Handbook of psychiatry in palliative medicine (pp. 1 9 7 - 2 1 4 ) . New York: Oxford University Press. Stavraky, K. M., Donner, A. P., Kincade, J . E., St Stewart, M. A. (1988). The effect of psychosocial factors on lung cancer mortality at one year. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 41, 7 5 - 8 2 . Sullivan, M., Rapp, S., Fitzgibbon, D., St Chapman, C. R. (1997). Pain and the choice to hasten death in patients with painful metastatic cancer. Journal of Palliative Care, 13, 1 8 - 2 8 . Swanson, G. M., Belle, S. H., St Satariano, W. A. (1985). Marital status and cancer incidence: Difference in the black and white populations. Cancer Research, 45, 5883-5889. Temoshok, L. (1987). Personality coping style, emotion, and cancer: Towards an integrative model. Cancer Surveys, 6, 5 4 5 - 5 6 7 . Temoshok, L., St Fox, Β. H. (1984). Coping styles and other psychosocial factors related to medical status and to prognosis in patients with cutaneous malignant melanoma as a predictor of follow-up clinical status. In B . H. Fox 8t Β . H. Newberry (Eds.), Impact of psychoendocrine systems in cancer and immunity (pp. 2 5 8 - 2 8 7 ) . Toronto: Hogrefe. Temoshok, L., St Heller, W. B. (1984). On comparing apples, oranges, and fruit salad: A methodological overview of medical outcome studies in psychosocial
358
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS oncology. In C. L. Cooper (Ed.), Psychosocial stress and cancer (pp. 2 3 1 - 2 6 0 ) . Chichester, UK: Wiley. Trzepacz, P. T., Baker, R. W., & Greenhouse, J . (1988). A symptom rating scale for delirium. Psychiatry Research, 23, 8 9 - 9 7 . Tuxen, M . K., & Hansen, S. W. ((1994). Complications of treatment: Neurotoxicity secondary to antineoplastic drugs. Cancer Treatments Review, 20, 1 9 1 - 2 1 4 . U.K. Trial of Early Detection of Breast Cancer Group. (1984). Trial of Early Detection of Breast Cancer: Description of method. British Journal of Cancer, 44, 6 1 8 - 6 2 7 . Van der Ploeg, H-M. (1991). What a wonderful world it would be: A reanalysis of some of the work of Grossarth-Maticek. Psychological Inquiry, 2, 2 8 0 - 2 8 5 . Vogt, T. M., Mullooly, J . P., Ernst, D., Pope, C. R., & Hollis, J . F. (1992). Social networks as predictors of ischemic heart disease, cancer, stroke and hypertension: Incidence, survival, and mortality. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 45, 659-666. Watson, M., Greer, S., Young, J . , Inayat, Q., Burgess, C , & Robertson, C. (1988). Development of a questionnaire measure of adjustment to cancer: The MAC scale. British Journal of Psychiatry, 18, 2 0 3 - 2 0 9 . Watson, M., Haviland, J . S., Greer, S., Davidson, J . , & Bliss, J . M. (1999). Influence of psychological response on survival in breast cancer: A population-based cohort study. Lancet, 354, 1 3 5 1 - 1 3 5 6 . Waxier-Morrison, N., Hislop, T. G., Mears, B., & Kan, L. (1991). Effects of social relationships on survival for women with breast cancer: A prospective study. Social Science and Medicine, 33, 1 7 7 - 1 8 3 . Weihs, K., & Reiss, D. (1995). Family reorganization in response to cancer: A developmental perspective. In L. Baider, C. L. Cooper, & A. Kaplan De-Nour (Eds.), Cancer and the family (pp. 3 - 3 0 ) . Chichester, UK: Wiley. Weisman, A. D., & Worden, J . W. (1975). Psychological analysis of cancer deaths. Omega, 6, 6 1 - 7 5 . Weisman, A. D., & Worden, J . W. ( 1 9 7 6 - 1 9 7 7 ) . The existential plight of cancer: Significance of the first 100 days. International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 7, 1-15. Weisman, A. D., Worden, J . W., & Sobel, H. J . (1980). Psychosocial screening and intervention with cancer patients. Research report, Harvard Medical School and Massachusetts General Hospital. White, M . H. (1993). Prevention of infection in patients with neoplastic disease: Use of a historical model for developmental strategies. Clinical Infectious Diseases, 17, S 3 5 5 - S 3 5 8 . Yehuda, R., Resnick, H., Kahana, B., & Giller, E. L. (1993). Long lasting hormonal alterations to extreme stress in humans: Normative or maladaptive? Psychosomatic Medicine, 55, 2 8 7 - 2 9 7 . Zonderman, A. B., Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1989). Depression as a risk for cancer morbidity and mortality in a nationally representative sample. Journal of the American Medical Association, 262, 1 1 9 1 - 1 1 9 5 .
CHAPTER
Sexual
10 18
Dysfunctions
Etiology and Treatment SHEILA GAROS
S
tudies indicate that problems in sexual
D S M - I V (Diagnostic
functioning
Mental
may
be
quite
common
(Spector & Carey, 1 9 9 0 ) . As early as
Disorders,
and Statistical
Manual
of
fourth edition [American
Psychiatric Association, 1 9 9 4 ] ) . Sexual dys-
that
functions are further classified into three sub-
5 0 % of couples in the United States suffered
types: pain disorders, arousal disorders, and
from a sexual dysfunction (Masters & Johnson,
orgasmic disorders. T o understand the etiol-
1 9 7 0 , Masters and Johnson estimated
1 9 7 0 ) . Other estimates suggest that up to 2 4 %
ogy and treatment o f sexual disorders, it is
of the U.S. population will experience a sexual
necessary to have some knowledge of the
dysfunction at some point in their lives (Robins
psychobiology o f the human sexual response.
et al., 1 9 8 4 ) . In 1 9 9 2 , Laumann and colleagues conducted a study to assess the prevalence and risk o f experiencing sexual dysfunction across social groups (Laumann, Gagnon, Michael, 8c Michaels, 1 9 9 5 ) . They found that sexual dysfunction was more prevalent in women ( 4 3 % )
BACKGROUND AND ETIOLOGY
The Human Sexual Response
than in men ( 3 1 % ) and was associated with
Biomedical advances and clinical studies
characteristics such as age, educational attain-
suggest that for most people, human sexual
ment, poor physical and emotional health,
functioning proceeds sequentially and rudimen-
experiences in sexual relationships, and overall
tarily involves a biphasic response that is com-
well-being. Having a sexual problem or dys-
posed o f (a) tumescence,
function
fear,
the genitals with blood that leads to erection in
shame, and feelings o f inadequacy. For these
men and vaginal lubrication and swelling in
reasons, the number of individuals who suffer
women, and (b) detumescence,
with a sexual dysfunction is often greater than
of blood from the genitals following orgasm
can invoke embarrassment,
what reported statistics reflect. Table 1 8 . 1 outlines the current classification scheme o f sexual disorders found in the
or the engorgement of
or the outflow
(Bancroft, 1 9 9 5 ; Herbert, 1 9 9 6 ; Schiavi & Segraves, 1 9 9 5 ; Wincze &
Carey,
1991).
However, the psychobiological mechanisms 359
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS
360
Table 18.1
Pain
Disorders
DSM-IV Classifications of Sexual Dysfunctions
Desire
Arousal
Disorders
Disorders
Orgasm
Disorders
Miscellaneous Classifications
Dyspareunia
Hypoactive sexual desire disorder
Female sexual arousal disorder
Female orgasmic disorder
Sexual dysfunction due to a medical condition
Vaginismus
Sexual aversion disorder
Male erectile disorder
Male orgasmic disorder
Substance-induced sexual dysfunction Sexual dysfunction not otherwise specified
Premature ejaculation
that underlie the sexual response are far more
or occur secondarily to, a sexual dysfunction
investigation
(Pollets, Ducharme, & Pauporte, 1 9 9 9 ) such as
(Davis, 2 0 0 1 ; Gaither & Plaud, 1 9 9 7 ; Jupp &
conflict (Metz & Epstein, 2 0 0 2 ) , guilt (Walser
complex and warrant further
M c C a b e , 1 9 8 9 ; Meston & Frohlich, 2 0 0 0 ;
& Kern, 1 9 9 6 ) , depression and other affective
Pfaus, 1 9 9 9 ; Regan, 1 9 9 6 ; Stoleru et al., 1 9 9 9 ) .
states (Seidman & Roose, 2 0 0 1 ) , trauma (van
Masters and J o h n s o n ( 1 9 7 0 ) described a
Berlo &c Ensink, 2 0 0 0 ) , and anxiety (Shires 8 t
physiological model o f the sexual response
Miller, 1 9 9 8 ) . Likewise, principles o f learning
that
phases:
and conditioning in regard to the sexual
excitement, plateau, orgasm, and resolution
response must be considered (Lalumiere &
included
four
physiological
(Table 1 8 . 2 ) . However, this model failed to
Quinsey, 1 9 9 8 ) , as should cognitive appraisals
address those patients w h o reported difficulty
and expectancies about sexual arousal (Palace,
1 9 9 5 ; Weisberg,
and
in becoming aroused or who expressed an
desire
aversion to sex (Kaplan, 1 9 7 7 ) . Subsequently,
Wincze, & Barlow, 2 0 0 1 ) , relationship distress
Brown,
was
(Metz & Epstein, 2 0 0 2 ) , and developmental
believed to precede the "excitement" phase
issues such as age and a person's stage in life
a
"desire stage" was conceived that
described by Masters and J o h n s o n . Desire
(Avina, O'Donohue, & Fisher, 2 0 0 0 ; Bartlik &
involves a patient's "cognitive and affective
Goldstein, 2 0 0 1 ; Dennerstein, Dudley,
readiness for, and interest in, sexual activity"
Burger, 2 0 0 1 ) . M a n y patients develop sexual
(Wincze & Carey, 1 9 9 1 , p. 4 ) .
dysfunctions as the result o f medical conditions
&
such as spinal cord injuries (Sipski, Alexander, & Rosen, 2 0 0 1 ) , kidney disease (Malavaud,
Etiological Factors
Rostaing, Rischmann, Sarramon, & Durand, 2000),
cancer
mance, sexual desire, sexual satisfaction, and
(Merrick, Wallner, Butler, Lief, &
Sutlief,
meaning o f sexual behavior that is constructed
2 0 0 1 ; Shifren et al., 2 0 0 0 ) , and other chronic
Given the complexity o f sexual
perfor-
2000),
diabetes
(Bhugra,
from dominant culture and beliefs, one must
illnesses (Schover, 1 9 8 9 ) . Finally, medications
not rely exclusively on physiological models to
prescribed to treat a variety of medical and
describe or assess sexual functioning (Laqueur,
psychological conditions can often lead to
1 9 9 0 ; Tiefer, 1 9 9 1 ) . Diagnosis and assessment
reduced sexual desire or other interference with
must include an evaluation o f organic causes as
sexual performance (Gelenberg, Delgado, &
well as psychogenic factors that contribute to,
Nurnberg, 2 0 0 0 ; Waldinger et al., 2 0 0 2 ) .
Sexual Table 18.2
Dysfunctions
Sexual Response Phases and Associated Dysfunctions
Phase
Characteristics
Dysfunction
1. Desire
Characterized by subjective feelings of sexual interest, desire, urges, and fantasy; no physiological correlates
Hypoactive sexual desire disorder Sexual aversion disorder
2. Excitement
Characterized by subjective and physiological concomitants of sexual arousal such as penile erection in men and vaginal engorgement and lubrication in women
Female sexual arousal disorder Male erectile disorder
3. Orgasm
Characterized by climax or peaking of sexual tension, with rhythmic contractions of the genital musculature and intense subjective involvement
Female orgasmic disorder Male orgasmic disorder Premature ejaculation
4. Resolution
Characterized by a release of tension and a sense of pleasure or well-being
Dyspareunia Vaginismus
S O U R C E : Adapted from Weiner and Davis ( 1 9 9 9 , p. 4 1 1 ) . In T. Millon, P. H . Blaney, & R . D . Davis (Eds.), O x f o r d T e x t b o o k o f P s y c h o p a t h o l o g y . Copyright © 1 9 9 9 by Oxford University Press, Inc. Used by permission.
ASSESSMENT AND TREATMENT
psychosexual history, a description o f the
Basic Principles of Sex Therapy
tion o f the quality o f their relationship, the
Traditionally, sex therapy is a short-term therapy designed for the special treatment o f sexual dysfunctions. Sex therapy is a behaviorally based, systematic protocol designed to move patients through a series o f "graded experiences, from an avoided, partial, or pleasureless
response
to
response" (Birk, 1 9 9 9 ,
a
fully
p. 5 2 5 ) .
pleasurable
Contemporary
approaches to sex therapy often address issues in the patient as well as in his or her partnership more systemically. Emotional, spiritual, cultural, affective, cognitive, and social factors are addressed and evaluated. Thus, effective treatment o f psychogenic sexual dysfunctions requires knowledge o f family systems and family therapy as well as extensive experience in working with couples in general.
Overall Evaluation and Assessment During an initial evaluation the clinician will typically seek demographic information, the nature and development of the dysfunction, a
patient's current sex life, the partners' percepdegree o f psychopathology of one or both partners, physical health, and the patient's motivation
for
and
commitment
to
treatment
(LoPiccolo & Heiman, 1 9 7 8 ) . T h e initial evaluation can seem quite invasive for the patient, and it is important to inform him or her why the type of information sought is necessary. Questions must be specific to ascertain the nature and degree of dysfunction and to help delineate between possible organic and psychogenic causes o f the problem. Additional information that may be sought includes, but is not limited to, the patient's personality, professional life, education, sexual development, sexual values, experiences with other partners, history o f masturbatory behaviors, and attitudes about pleasure, family life, and religious background.
Overall Treatment Approach Thoughts, attitudes, and feelings play a significant role in mediating
physiological
362
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS responses to sex. T h e goal o f many techniques
In the D S M - I V
(American Psychiatric
1994), sexual desire disorders fall
is to "replace antisexual anxiety with sexual
Association,
pleasure" (Heiman, 1978, p. 123). Thus, facili-
into two categories: hypoactive sexual desire
tation and maintenance of arousal and associ-
disorder ( H S D D ) , defined as a "deficiency or
ated thoughts, feelings, and attitudes are an
absence of sexual fantasies and desire for sex-
important component in treating sexual dys-
ual activity" (p. 496), and sexual aversion dis-
functions. Psychophysiological measures such
order (SAD). Diagnosis o f H S D D generally
as nocturnal penile tumescence and daytime
involves clinical judgment as well as corrobo-
arousal evaluation can be used to assess
rating information from the patient's partner
subjective and objective measures o f arousal.
(Rosen & Leiblum,
In addition, men being evaluated for erectile
keep in mind that often a partner with higher
1989). It is important to
dysfunction (ED) may undergo a penile blood
desire becomes the referent for the partner
pressure examination.
with lower desire, in which case it may be
A number of psychosocial interventions are
indicative o f a desire discrepancy as opposed
available to help with sexual difficulties. Perhaps
to a desire disorder. Clinical judgment must
the most common is sensate focus, which
also take into account interpersonal determi-
involves teaching patients and their partners
nants, frequency and chronicity o f the symp-
to engage in intimate physical and emotional
tom, subjective distress, effect on other areas
closeness in a gradual nonthreatening manner.
of functioning, and the person's current life
Homework is assigned in which couples engage
situation. Some individuals have difficulty in
in various stages of the protocol. Explicit
initiating sexual activity; others are unrespon-
instructions are given to couples as to how
sive to sexual advances from their partners
to approach each stage of treatment. "Rules" of
(i.e., lack o f receptivity). It is important to
engagement are oudined, with some of these
determine whether H S D D is global, with the
rules prohibiting genital contact during the
patient lacking interest in any or all sexual
earlier phases o f the exercise. Modifications
activity, or situational, with the patient's lack
can be made to best address the needs o f
of desire occurring only with a specific partner
the patient and the type of dysfunction being
or type o f activity.
treated. Inclusion o f steps and the duration
H S D D can occur as a secondary condition
of each is left to clinical judgment. An outline
when other sexual dysfunctions are present
of sensate focus is presented in Table 18.3.
(e.g., anorgasmia in women, E D in men). H S D D may also result from a number of physiological or psychological conditions, including other
Female Sexual Dysfunctions Hypoactive
Sexual
Desire
medical conditions (Phillips 8c Slaughter,
2000),
Disorder.
stress, substance use, low self-esteem, anhe-
According to Laumann, Gagnon, Michael, and
donia, hormonal changes, and negative self-
(1994), approximately 33% of women
evaluation
experience a lack of sexual interest at some point
Morokoff,
in their lives. From ages 18 to 24 years, about
Medication side effects are another possible
32.0% of women report some difficulty with
cause of reduced desire (Wincze
sexual desire; at ages 30 to 34 years, this number
Finally, a history of sexual abuse or trauma,
Michaels
29.5%. The largest group affected is women ages 35 to 39 years (37.6%). Among decreases to
(Heiman
Sc
Meston,
1997;
1985; Rosen Sc Leiblum, 1989). Sc Carey, 1991).
abuse, or assault can lead to decreased desire due to "chronic fears of vulnerability of loss of con-
women ages 40 to 54 years, the number of
trol, inability to establish intimate relationships,
women reporting desire disturbances declines,
or a conditioned aversion to all forms of sexual
only to increase once again after that.
contact" (Rosen 8c Leiblum, 1989, p. 27).
Sexual Dysfunctions
Table 18.3
\
Treatment Stages of Sensate Focus
Stage I: Nongenital pleasuring At this stage, each partner will touch one another for at least 2 0 minutes. One partner will initiate and touch for the specified time, and then the two will switch roles and positions. The partner who is touching should be assertive by touching the other in ways and places (minus breasts, buttocks, and genitals) that are pleasing for the one doing the touching. Experimentation with touching each partner in new places and in new ways is encouraged. A partner can use his or her legs, hands, face, arms, and the like when touching the other partner. The partner being touched should concentrate on relaxing his or her whole body and the sensations that touch by the other partner is creating. If spectatoring or anxiety is a problem, the couple should stop for a few moments until relaxed and then start again. If either partner becomes aroused, it is permissible to masturbate to relieve tension so long as the person does it himself or herself. In some cases (e.g., a past history of trauma), the partners can begin this stage with their clothes on. Stage II: Touching for One's Own and One's Partner's Pleasure This stage is similar to Stage I. Each stage has two parts, with one partner caressing and touching the other first and then the partners switching roles. What differs is that at Stage II, each partner indicates to the other what he or she would like the other to do. A hand can be placed over that of the partner to demonstrate how one would like the touch to feel (e.g., faster, harder, softer, slower, more to the right). It is still up to the person who is doing the caressing to decide what he or she will do. Partners are encouraged to discuss their experience after each stage. Stage III: Sensate Focus With Genital Focus The same basic principles apply to this and the remaining stages of treatment. A ban on intercourse remains; however, genital contact with the mouth and/or hands is now permitted. A will caress B , and then Β will caress A. During this stage, change in the pressure, speed, or direction of touch can profoundly affect the sensation received. Thus, communication is of utmost importance. Couples are told not to focus on genital regions exclusively and to spend as much time as before on genital kissing and touching. At this stage, lubricants, oils, and lotions are permitted to enhance pleasure of both partners. The main objective is still to concentrate on and enjoy the bodily sensations being experienced. Should one of the partners become aroused or experience orgasm, the session can continue. It is important to remember that orgasm is not the "goal" of the session. Stage IV: Sensate Focus With Genital Contact and Simultaneous Caressing The focus of this stage is on simultaneous caressing that enables both partners to give and receive pleasure at the same time. Partners are encouraged to communicate to each other when one is doing something that feels particularly nice. Self-assertion and self-protection are also encouraged. Stage V: Vaginal Containment Once the sensate focus is established with consistency, some ejaculatory control is exercised, and erections can remain reasonably firm, the couple is ready for Stage V. This stage is designed to facilitate sensory focus and enjoyment without performance anxiety. Once the partner is ready and the patient is able to maintain a reasonably firm erection, the partner will guide the patient's penis into her vagina. The most accommodating position for this is the "female superior" position, where the patient lies flat on his back with the partner kneeling above him with her knees on either side of his body, roughly at the level of his nipples. The woman will gently insert her partner's penis into her vagina. This allows the partner to have greater control. The goal of this stage is for the partners to reorient themselves with the sensation of the penis and the vagina. The woman should tighten and relax her vaginal muscles on her partner's penis. Genital caressing may resume. The patient is told to concentrate on sensations while the partner keeps control of what is happening. Couples are also to resist the desire to thrust. (Continued)
363
364
I
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS
Table 18.3
(Continued)
Stage VI: Vaginal Containment With Movement It is important to remind the partners at this stage that they are to employ the same principles concerning physical contact that they used at the beginning of treatment. Giving and receiving pleasure, and touching each other in a way that is pleasing to both partners, remains the aim of treatment. Mutual caressing continues and involves both genital and nongenital areas. Each partner should feel aroused and receptive before vaginal entry takes place. After vaginal containment is tried for some time, limited thrusting can be tried to assess each partner's sensations and feelings. Either partner is allowed to tell the other to stop at any time so as to set limits and boundaries without fearing that the partner will become angry. It is also important to remember that responsiveness will vary from stage to stage and from month to month. At this point, the clinician should reiterate that orgasm, particularly mutual orgasm, is not the goal that must be satisfied to meet the expectations of the patient or his partner. Cognitive restructuring techniques, such as challenging negative attitudes and learning to reduce intrusive thoughts, are often incorporated into treatment. Communication training is often a vital element in the treatment of sexual dysfunctions because many couples lack effective communication skills in general and are particularly reluctant to communicate their sexual needs, likes, dislikes, and desires. The use of erotic materials or "toys" is sometimes recommended to patients and should be approached as exposure to a sexual experience with attention paid to mood and setting. Of course, it is essential to assess the patient's or couple's views about the use of such materials to determine whether these approaches are viable and would not be considered offensive or objectionable. Masturbation training with fantasy should be approached with similar caution. It should not be assumed that all patients know how to masturbate or how to do f ι effectively. Masturbation training, when used successfully, can help to build sexual confidence and desire. Other treatments for male sexual dysfunctions include vasoactive therapies, which involve the use of intracorporeal injections of papaverine or transurethral alprostadil suppositories. These agents act as vasodilators. Surgical approaches can be used to correct male erectile dysfunction and include the placement of an implantable penile prosthesis, penile arterial revascularization, and penile venous ligation. Vacuum devices can also be used to draw blood into the corpora cavernosa of the penis. Placing a band around the base of the penis then traps the blood. Among pharmacotherapeutic agents, the most recent, and perhaps most popular, is sildenafil (Viagra).
In the assessment o f H S D D , frequency of
Masturbatory practices vary, as do cognitive
activity should not be considered a reliable indi-
correlates o f desire such as fantasy (Schreiner-
cator of sexual desire. However, initiation is
Engel & Schiavi, 1 9 8 6 ) . Moreover, gender dif-
an important consideration because it serves
ferences must be taken into account in the
as an indicator of female motivation to engage
evaluation o f cognitive descriptors o f desire
in sexual behavior (Wallen, 1 9 9 0 ) . Often, a
(Denney, Field, & Quadagno, 1 9 8 4 ; Jones &
patient will engage in frequent coitus or other
Barlow, 1 9 9 0 ; McCauley & Swann, 1 9 7 8 ,
sexual activities out of a sense o f obligation,
1 9 8 0 ; Person, Terestman, Myers, Goldberg, &
coercion, or an attempt to please or accommo-
Salvadori, 1 9 8 9 ) .
date his or her partner's wishes and preferences.
A
number
o f treatment
strategies
are
Likewise, one must consider that symptoms o f
available t o
low desire may reflect problems of relationship
with sexual desire disorder (for a review, see
address
problems associated
intimacy, power differentials, or territoriality in
O'Donohue, Dopke, &c Swingen, 1 9 9 7 ) . These
the relationship (Verhulst & Heiman, 1 9 8 8 ) .
approaches include (a) psychotherapy (Kaplan,
Sexual Dysfunctions
\
1 9 7 7 ; Scharff, 1 9 8 8 ) , (b) cognitive-behavioral
negative learning experiences (Halvorsen &
approaches (Rosen & Leiblum, 1 9 8 9 ) , (c) cog-
Metz, 1992).
nitive restructuring (LoPiccolo & Friedman,
In treating S A D , it is important to under-
1 9 8 8 ) , (d) analysis o f interactional and c o m -
stand the "approach-avoidance" conflict that
munication patterns (Schwartz & Masters,
exists in many o f these patients (Ponticas,
1 9 8 8 ) , (e) "territorial interactions" (e.g., "When
1 9 9 2 ) . Given that most causes o f S A D are not
you touch my body, I feel like you are
physiological, addressing psychological issues
invading my space"), (f) "rank-order" c o m -
that underlie the disorder is o f particular
munication
(e.g., " I always feel like the
underdog in sexual r e l a t i o n s h i p s " ) ,
and
importance in treatment. In addition to psychotherapy, systematic desensitization
and
(g) "attachment interactions" (e.g., " I find it
vicarious extinction techniques can be used to
hard to trust you after my feelings have been
reduce or minimize the patient's anxiety and
hurt") (Verhulst & Heiman, 1 9 7 9 , 1 9 8 8 ) .
fear response (Wincze, 1 9 7 1 ) .
Other strategies include the use o f pharmacological agents, hormonal treatments, and the
Female
Orgasmic
Disorder.
Anorgasmia is
"coital alignment technique" that can help to
regarded as the most common sexual dysfunc-
increase effective stimulation for women dur-
tion in women (Heiman &c Grafton-Becker,
ing intercourse (Pierce, 2 0 0 0 ) . In some cases,
1 9 8 9 ; Spector & Carey, 1 9 9 0 ) . Moreover,
the use o f sex toys and other stimuli (e.g., fan-
approximately 8 5 % to 9 0 % of women report
tasy, erotic material) and "orgasm consis-
having orgasms without difficulty; however,
tency" training (Hurlbert, White, Powell, &
only one third have had an orgasm during
Apt, 1 9 9 3 ) may be helpful.
intercourse (Seeber &c Gorrell, 2 0 0 1 ) . In addition, the incidence o f orgasmic difficulty
Sexual
Aversion
Disorder.
S A D is a more
severe disruption in desire. S A D is characterized by a "marked aversion to, and active
tends to be higher in single women (Laumann et al., 1 9 9 4 ) . Female orgasmic disorder is characterized
avoidance of, all genital contact with a sexual
by "a persistent or recurrent delay in, or absence
partner" (American Psychiatric Association,
of, orgasm following a normal sexual ex-
1 9 9 4 , p. 4 9 9 ) . T h e aversion to genital contact
citement
"may be focused on a particular aspect o f sex-
Association, 1 9 9 4 , p. 5 0 5 ) . Clinical judgment
ual experience (e.g., genital secretions, vaginal
is an important factor in diagnosing this con-
p e n e t r a t i o n ) . . . [or] revulsion to all sexual
dition given that a woman's orgasmic capac-
stimuli, including
touching"
ity must be determined to be less than would
(p. 4 9 9 ) . This disorder is often accompanied
be "reasonable for her age, sexual experience,
by poor body image and avoidance o f nudity
and the adequacy o f sexual stimulation she
(Katz, Gipson, & Turner, 1 9 9 2 ; Ponticas,
receives" (p. 5 0 5 ) . M a n y w o m e n express con-
1 9 9 2 ) . W o m e n with S A D may experience
cern that something is "wrong with t h e m " if
kissing and
phase"
(American
Psychiatric
reactions such as terror, panic, and nausea.
they do not experience orgasm during inter-
Efforts to cope with the disorder may include
course or if they have multiple or simultane-
avoidance o f sexual contact, substance use,
ous orgasms. Patients need to k n o w
and neglect o f one's personal
appearance.
many women do not reach orgasm during
Although S A D and H S D D are distinct, the
coitus because penile stimulation is often not
two conditions are often related and have
intense or direct enough to produce orgasm.
that
similar causes such as endocrine alterations,
Women's orgasmic potential and type of
medical conditions, psychological distress,
orgasm are variable. Orgasmic capacity has
relationship factors, prior sexual trauma, and
been
associated with
sexual
assertiveness
365
366
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS (Hurlbert, 1 9 9 1 ) , comfort with masturbation
levels of marital adjustment
(Kelly, Strassberg, & Kircher, 1 9 9 0 ) , and rela-
Khalife, & Cohen, 1999).
(Meana, Binik,
tionship and psychological distress (Kaplan,
Pain is a subjective experience. In dyspareu-
1 9 9 2 ; McGovern, Stewart, & LoPiccolo, 1 9 7 5 ) .
nia, the phenomenology o f pain is genital and
M o s t women have clitoral orgasms that result
associated with intercourse. In some cases,
from stimulus to the clitoris and surrounding
pain occurs before or after intercourse as
tissues. Fewer women have pelvic floor or vagi-
well. In women, "the pain may be described as
nal orgasms, and for some women orgasm
superficial during intromission or as deep
involves a combination of the two. It is impor-
during penile thrusting..., [with] symptoms
tant to discern whether the patient's orgasmic
rang[ing] from
disorder is situational (i.e., the patient is able to
pain" (American Psychiatric Association, 1 9 9 4 ,
reach orgasm via masturbation but not by man-
p. 5 1 1 ) . Vulvodynia refers to pain located
ual stimulation or intercourse) or generalized
specifically in the vulva. Dyspareunia can be
(i.e., occurring across all situations and part-
lifelong or acquired as well as generalized or
mild discomfort to
sharp
ners). Female orgasmic disorder typically does
situational. Abarbanel ( 1 9 7 8 ) suggested four
not arise from a physiological condition and
phenomenological categories o f pain associ-
is generally not correlated with vaginal size or
ated with dyspareunia:
pelvic muscle strength. However, some condi-
sharp but momentary pain that varies in inten-
tions (e.g., spinal cord injuries, vaginal excision
sity, (b) repeated
and reconstruction) have been associated with
(c) aching, and (d) intermittent painful pangs
(a) perception o f a
and intense
discomfort,
orgasmic difficulty. Medications such as benzo-
or twinges. A thorough medical examination
diazepines, antihypertensives, neuroleptics, and
must be conducted to rule out physical factors
antidepressants
may contribute to orgasmic
difficulty, as can substance use and abuse.
such as pelvic tumors, hymeneal remnants, prolapsed ovaries, and scarring that occurs as
Often, the source of orgasmic difficulty in
a result o f either an episiotomy or vaginal
women is their own or their partners' lack of
repair (Bancroft, 1 9 9 5 ) . Hormonal changes that
knowledge about the female sexual response and
result from contraceptive use or menopausal
female genitalia. The problem is often resolved
changes can lessen vaginal lubrication and sub-
by helping clients and their partners learn to
sequently cause soreness and irritation during
extend stimulation and lovemaking
beyond
intercourse or penetration. Once organic causes
genitally focused sex. Greater sensate exchange
of pain are ruled out, psychological factors such
between partners, expanding women's arousal
as anxiety, poor body image, religiosity, anger,
pattern, directed masturbation,
and
anxiety
and distrust toward the patient's partner should be investigated.
management also can be helpful.
Psychotherapy is an important element in Dyspareunia
and Vulvodynia.
Estimates of
the treatment of dyspareunia and should be
the prevalence of dyspareunia range from 8 . 0 %
approached in a multimodal framework
(Osborn, Hawton, & Gath, 1 9 8 8 ) to 3 3 . 5 %
examine the patient's (a) behavior (e.g., deficits
(Glatt, Zinner, & McCormack, 1990). Although
and
shortcomings
in sexual
to
techniques),
accurate prevalence rates are difficult to deter-
(b) affect (e.g., feelings of guilt, shame, and
mine, studies have shown that causal attributions
anger), (c) sensation (e.g., assessment o f the
of pain are related to levels of adjustment. For
location, type, frequency, and intensity o f
example, women who cited psychosocial attri-
pain), (d) imagery (e.g., body image, negative
butions indicated greater psychosocial distress,
memories), (e) cognition (e.g., negative self-
more problems with sexual function, and more
statements, dysfunctional beliefs), (f) interper-
frequent reports of sexual assault as well as lower
sonal
functioning
(e.g.,
communication,
Sexual Dysfunctions
\
climate between partners), and (g) biological
response. Psychotherapy can be used to explore
factors (e.g., improper hygiene, medications)
unconscious fears and conflicts that may under-
(Leiblum 8c Rosen, 1 9 8 9 ) .
lie the disorder. Therapy should include progressive relaxation techniques and
Vaginismus.
Vaginismus is a relatively rare
disorder characterized by "recurrent or persis-
fantasy
exercises to help alleviate fears o f gynecological exams as well as intercourse.
tent involuntary contraction of the perineal muscles surrounding the outer third o f the vagina when vaginal penetration with penis, finger, tampon, or speculum is attempted" (American Psychiatric Association, 1 9 9 4 ,
Male Sexual Dysfunctions Male
Erectile
Disorder.
An estimated 3 0
million men suffer from E D in the United
p. 5 1 3 ) . In some women, thoughts o f penetra-
States. E D prevalence rates increase to more
tion alone can create spasms. Contractions
than 5 0 % in men ages 5 0 to 7 0 years, and E D
can be mild, creating tightening and discom-
occurs in approximately 4 0 % of men with
fort, or severe, preventing any penetration.
diabetes
Sexual desire, pleasure, and orgasmic capacity
Hatzichristou, & Krane, 1 9 9 4 ) . In men under
can be impaired as a result o f the disorder.
age 3 5 years, approximately 7 0 % suffer from
The patient should be screened for potential
psychogenic E D , whereas 8 5 % o f men over
organic factors that can contribute to forma-
age 5 0 years have organic E D (Weiss &c
tion o f the disorder such as vaginal hysterec-
Mellinger, 1 9 9 0 ) .
(Feldman, Goldstein,
McKinlay,
tomies or other surgeries, atrophic vaginitis,
E D has been defined as an inability to
endometriosis, painful hymenal tags, and ure-
achieve or sustain an erection o f sufficient
thral caruncle (Lamont, 1 9 7 8 ; Tollison &
rigidity or duration to enable satisfactory
Adams, 1 9 7 9 ) . Although many of these con-
sexual performance
(American Psychiatric
ditions are not directly responsible for vaginis-
Association, 1 9 9 4 ) . Often, E D is associated
mus, they may be associated with the disorder
with older age ( M a r u m o , Nakashima,
indirectly through classical conditioning.
M u r a i , 2 0 0 1 ) . There are different patterns
&
The main objective is to eliminate the "spas-
to E D , with some patients reporting an inabil-
modic reflexive contraction of the muscles con-
ity to obtain an erection from the onset o f
trolling the vaginal entrance typically through a
sexual activity and others reporting having a
series o f gradual approximations with the inser-
satisfactory erection at the onset o f sex but
tion of increasingly larger dilators" (Leiblum,
then losing the erection when
Pervin, & Campbell, 1 9 8 9 , p. 1 1 3 ) . Use o f
penetration or once penetration is complete.
attempting
graduated rubber or plastic catheters helps to
Particularly when E D is psychogenic, patients
extinguish the conditioned spasmodic response
will frequently report having an erection on
via systematic desensitization. T h e patient or
awakening
or
during
self-masturbation.
her partner's fingers can also do insertion. In
Subtypes include lifelong versus acquired and
addition, it is important that the patient feel in
generalized versus situational.
control of what is happening, and this extends
T o obtain a diagnosis of E D and an accurate
to her guiding penile entry during coitus. Use of
understanding of the etiology of the disorder,
the female superior position during intercourse
assessment should include a detailed sexual and
should be suggested because this can help
medical history, physical examination, and psy-
the patient to maintain control o f entry and
chological interview. A medical history and a
movement. Cognitive-behavioral
physical examination are particularly impor-
approaches
are also used to challenge underlying thoughts
tant because a number of physiological factors
and beliefs that drive the conditioned fear
contribute to the pathophysiology o f E D . These
367
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS
368
conditions include, but are not limited to,
Pena, & Mishra, 2 0 0 2 ) . Studies suggest that
(a) cardiovascular disease and
hypertension
sildenafil is particularly effective in cases of
(Burchardt et al., 2 0 0 1 ) , (b) other vascular dis-
arterial insufficiency and psychogenic causes o f
orders and neuropathy associated with diabetes
E D (Basar, Tekdogan, et al., 2 0 0 1 ) and that
(Dey & Shepherd, 2 0 0 2 ; Hecht, Neundorfer,
the drug is well tolerated in men over age
Kiesewetter, & Hiltz, 2 0 0 1 ) , (c) prostate cancer
6 5 years (Tsujimura et al., 2 0 0 2 ) . However,
or prostate cancer treatments (Incrocci, Slob,
even when the patient is treated effectively with
&
sildenafil, it is important
Levendag,
2002;
McCullough, 2 0 0 1 ;
Potters, Torre, Fearn, Leibel, & Kattan, 2 0 0 1 ) ,
to address
the
psychosocial factors that either preceded or
(d) spinal cord lesions (Biering-Sorensen &
developed as a result of E D (Dunn, Croft, &
Sonksen, 2 0 0 1 ) , and (e) hyperparathyroidism
Hackett,
(Chou et al., 2 0 0 1 ) . Depression (Seidman &
Bond, 2 0 0 1 ) , given that sexual satisfaction
1 9 9 9 ; M c D o w e l l , Snellgrove,
&
Roose, 2 0 0 0 ) , cigarette smoking (McVary,
(Shirai, Takimoto, Ishii, & Iwamoto, 2 0 0 1 ) ,
Carrier,
quality o f partnership
&
Wessells,
2001;
Spangler,
(Muller, Ruof, Graf-
Summerson, Bell, & Konan, 2 0 0 2 ) , and medi-
Morgenstern, Porst, & Benkert, 2 0 0 1 ; Paige,
cations
Hays, Litwin, Rajfer, & Shapiro, 2 0 0 1 ) , and
(Gelenberg
et
al.,
2000;
Rizvi,
Hampson, &c Harvey, 2 0 0 2 ) can also affect
attitudes toward interventions have important
erectile function.
consequences for the planning and treatment
Other assessment techniques include measurement
o f voluntary
contractile activity
of sexual problems and partner satisfaction. Intracavernosal
are
another
o f the ischiocavernosus muscle (Kawanishi e t a l . , 2 0 0 1 ) , penile pharmacotesting
Shalev, & Nissenkorn, 2 0 0 1 ) . Intracavernosal
with
option
injections
treatment
(Richter, Vardi, Ringel,
alprostadil (Aversa et al., 2 0 0 2 ) , sexual stim-
injection o f alprostadil (Caverject) has resulted
ulation penograms (Choi et al., 2 0 0 2 ) , and
in reported success rates o f 6 7 % to 8 5 %
measures
(Engelhardt, Plas, Hiibner, & Pfliiger, 1 9 9 8 ) .
o f nocturnal
penile
tumescence
W h e n injected directly into the corpus caver-
(Basar, Atan, & Tekdogan, 2 0 0 1 ) . Several treatment options are available for
nosum,
alprostadil
causes
the
arteriolar
organic causes of E D . There are pharmacolog-
smooth muscle cells to relax. N o more than
ical agents such as apomorphine (Altwein &
three injections per week, with a period o f
Keuler, 2 0 0 1 ; Mulhall, Bukofzer, Edmonds, &
2 4 hours between administrations, is recom-
George, 2 0 0 1 ) , yohimbine (an alpha-adrenore-
mended.
ceptor blocker) (Tam, Worcel, & Wyllie,
alprostadil. O n c e the suppository is inserted, it
2001),
will first diffuse into the corpus spongiosum
hormonal
treatments,
and
(most
the
is
corpus
transurethral
and
has been shown to be efficacious in treating E D
whereby the arteriolar smooth muscle relaxes,
in men who suffer from mild to
resulting
depressive illness (Muller & Benkert, 2 0 0 1 ;
into
option
recently) sildenafil citrate (Viagra). Sildenafil moderate
then
Another
in an erection
cavernosum,
(Viera, Clenney,
Shenenberger, & Green, 1 9 9 9 ) .
Seidman, Roose, Menza, Shabsigh, & Rosen,
A third option in the treatment of organic
2 0 0 1 ) and spinal cord injury (Sanchez et al.,
E D is a vacuum erection device. M o s t devices
2 0 0 1 ) . Sildenafil has also been shown to be
work by creating a vacuum in a cylinder placed
a safe and effective treatment of E D in both
over the penis. T h e vacuum
long- and short-term treatment (Burls, Gold,
into the corpora cavernosa and is trapped by
&
draws blood
&
placing a constricting band at the base of
Shabsigh, 2 0 0 1 ; Steers et al., 2 0 0 1 ) and has
the penis. Another option is the penile pros-
been found to improve the quality of life in
thetic implant. T w o types of implants exist: a
those patients w h o use sildenafil (Giuliano,
semi-rigid silicone implant (Small, Carrion, &
Clark, 2 0 0 1 ; Fagelman, Fagelman,
Sexual Dysfunctions
\
Gordon, 1 9 7 5 ) and a hydrolic inflatable device
exercises and cognitive-behavioral therapy can
(Scott, Bradley, & T i m m , 1 9 7 3 ) . However,
be used to confront performance anxiety, dis-
factors such as poor marital adjustment and
pute irrational
poor coping ability have been associated with
body image issues, and heighten sensuality.
poor postsurgical results (Meisler, Carey, &
Sensate focus is a central aspect of treatment
Krauss, 1 9 8 8 ; Schover, 1 9 8 9 ) .
(Table 1 8 . 3 ) . In addition to using these behav-
In cases o f psychogenic E D , the patient
beliefs, counteract
negative
ioral approaches, the couple needs to be edu-
should be referred for sex therapy, the goal o f
cated about sexual function and
which is to restore the patient's potency to the
Therapy can also address the destructive sex-
best level possible. T h e meaning o f impotence
ual system and
must be explored and transformed into cogni-
dynamics that inevitably develop in these cases.
dysfunctional
anatomy.
relationship
tive and emotional experience because "attentional processes are highly salient in creating disruption o f genital responsivity"
(Beck,
Male disorder
Orgasmic
Disorder.
M a l e orgasmic
is characterized by "persistent
or
1 9 8 6 , p. 2 1 8 ) . M e n with psychogenic impo-
recurrent delay in, or absence of, orgasm fol-
tence often express feelings o f inadequacy,
lowing a normal sexual excitement phase"
confusion, fear, anger, and shame. Perfor-
(American
mance anxiety becomes central in their sexual
p. 5 0 7 ) . Consideration o f this diagnosis must
Psychiatric Association, 1 9 9 4 ,
experience as they take on a "spectator" role,
take into account the patient's age and whether
watching to see whether their penises will
the amount o f stimulation the patient receives
"perform" at will as expected. O n c e an erec-
is adequate in duration and intensity. Delayed
tile "failure" occurs, the cycle o f anxiety, fear,
ejaculation can occur during
and shame repeats itself. Self-generated dis-
and/or during masturbation. M o s t men with
traction techniques that use cognitive interfer-
orgasmic disorder report feeling sufficiently
ence have been used successfully to
help
aroused at the onset o f sex. However, coital
manage anxiety in patients with E D (Beck,
thrusting soon feels like a chore rather than a
1 9 8 6 ; Beck & Barlow, 1 9 8 6 a , 1 9 8 6 b ) .
source o f pleasure. Maintaining an erection is
lovemaking
Partners o f men with psychogenic impo-
not a problem. Soreness and discomfort due to
tence experience their own fears and frustra-
prolonged rubbing can aggravate matters and
tions. It is c o m m o n for a partner to think that
often makes for greater frustration for both the
she is somehow responsible for the patient's
patient and his partner. Certain medical condi-
difficulties. For example, the partner may think
tions (e.g., spinal injuries, nerve damage, dia-
that she is no longer attractive to the patient or
betes), substance abuse, and medications (e.g.,
that the patient is having an affair. Thus, it is
beta blockers, antidepressants) can cause the
important to include the partner in treatment
disorder. In fact, drug therapies are the most
so that the relationship can be treated as well
c o m m o n cause of the dysfunction. A thorough
(Leiblum, 2 0 0 2 ) . M e n with psychogenic E D
examination by a physician is
can overcome the disorder "by understanding
Psychological problems, such as
their responses to their dilemmas, integrating
childhood experiences, extreme anxiety or
previously unacknowledged feelings, seeking
guilt, ridicule from a past partner, and feelings
new solutions to old problems, increasing
of anger, can also contribute to the disorder.
communication, surmounting the barriers to
warranted. traumatic
A distinction must be made between male
intimacy, and restoring sexual confidence"
orgasmic disorder
(Althof, 1 9 8 9 , p. 2 3 9 ) .
tion." Normally, ejaculation is caused by con-
Various interventions can be used in the treatment
o f psychogenic E D . Behavioral
and
"retrograde
ejacula-
traction o f the pelvic muscles, which are behind the penis and expel semen out o f the penis
369
370
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS through the urethra. Retrograde ejaculation is a
age, novelty of the sexual experience or partner,
condition in which semen travels back into the
and frequency of sexual activity. Relationship
bladder instead o f forward through the urethra
stress, anger at one's partner, anxiety about
(Wolf, 2 0 0 1 ) . Although semen is absent, the
intimacy, and low frequency o f intercourse are
sensation of orgasm is still usually pleasant.
other possible causes of PE. Assessment of PE
A third type of orgasmic disorder is called
should include an investigation of neurological
anejaculation. In this condition, the patient is
conditions, acute physical illness, physical
unable to ejaculate at all. Anejaculation can be
injury, and medication side effects (Metz Sc
caused by spinal injury or duct abnormalities
Pryor, 2 0 0 0 ) . It is also important to determine
(Cole, 2 0 0 2 ; Goldstone, 2 0 0 0 ) or by psycho-
whether early ejaculation occurs during mas-
logical factors. A thorough medical examina-
turbation and whether it is partner specific.
tion is necessary to rule out any physiological
It is helpful to understand the physiology
causes, at which point referral to a sex thera-
of the male orgasm to better understand P E .
pist or psychologist is warranted.
Ejaculation occurs as a result o f many differ-
If an orgasmic disorder is caused by medi-
ent physiological events. M a n y young men
cation, symptoms should remit once the med-
think that orgasm and ejaculation are the
ication is discontinued or the dose is adjusted.
same when in fact the two are related but sep-
In the case o f retrograde ejaculation, oral med-
arate processes. It is possible for men to have
ications are available that can help to contract
an orgasm without ejaculating (dry orgasm),
bladder neck muscles. Psychotherapy can help
just as it is possible to experience a partial
by giving the patient "permission" to concen-
ejaculation without the sensation o f orgasm.
trate on his own pleasure as well as by examining underlying psychological factors
that
may be contributing to the problem.
Early in sexual development, masturbation is often practiced in a rapid, intense, and goaloriented fashion. As a result, the "adolescent male focuses only on penis stimulation and is
Despite
intent on reaching orgasm and the associated
its common occurrence, it is difficult to estimate
few seconds o f intense pleasure" (McCarthy,
the frequency of premature ejaculation (PE).
1 9 8 9 , p. 1 4 5 ) , and this is counter to the pro-
Estimates have ranged from as low as 4 % (Metz,
cess o f learning ejaculatory control. Feelings
Early
or Premature
Ejaculation.
Pryor, Abuzzahab, Nesvacil, & Koznar, 1 9 9 7 )
of guilt, anxiety, shame, and/or fear of being
to as high as 3 6 % (Nettelbladt & Uddenberg,
caught
1979). It is estimated that approximately 2 5 % of
Eventually, a combination of high anxiety and
may
contribute
to
the
problem.
men report having an unsuccessful first inter-
sexual excitement can create a pattern of early
course experience, with the most common rea-
ejaculation, which is often made worse by the
son being that ejaculation occurs before vaginal
tendency o f the patient to self-monitor his
penetration (McCarthy, 1989). The D S M - I V
orgasmic response. Thus, an important com-
(American
ponent o f treatment is teaching the patient to
Psychiatric
Association, 1 9 9 4 )
defines PE as the "persistent or recurrent onset o f
experience masturbation and intercourse as a
orgasm and ejaculation with minimal sexual
more sensual, pleasure-focused, and "whole
stimulation before, on, or shortly after penetra-
body" experience.
tion and before the person wishes it" (p. 5 0 9 ) .
Orgasm in males is a two-phase process
Definitions of PE vary, and this is reflected in
consisting of the emission phase and the ejacu-
experimental and clinical research (Rowland,
latory phase. Emission is the movement of
Cooper, Sc Schneider, 2 0 0 1 ) .
semen into the urethra.
Expulsion is the
W h e n considering a diagnosis of PE, it is
propulsion o f semen out of the urethra at
important to take into account the patient's
orgasm. A reflex of pelvic floor muscles that
Sexual
Dysfunctions
CASE S T U D Y "Reggie," age 3 0 years, and "Marsha," age 2 7 years, sought therapy with a presenting complaint o f marital discord. T h e partners stated that their marriage o f 2 years was already in trouble and that therapy was their "last resort." Both Reggie and M a r s h a said that they loved one another and did not want to separate or pursue a divorce. Neither was married before. T h e partners had no children but expressed that having children was something they would like to do in the future. M a r s h a was in the third year o f her doctoral program in education. Reggie was currently working two parttime jobs while searching for employment as a data operations manager. Reggie had his B.S. degree but had "no desire to go back to graduate school." T h e partners agreed that over the past 8 months their relationship had become more and more strained. M a r s h a expressed aggravation with what she termed "Reggie's lack o f motivation in seeking stable employment." Marsha was feeling extremely pressured with graduate school, and although she earned a small income by teaching, the couple was having to rely on school loans as its main source of support. Reggie disagreed with Marsha's assessment of his j o b search efforts. H e maintained that Marsha's anger was due to her desire to start a family and that she interpreted his "lack o f motivation" as an indicator that his desire for children was not as great as hers. As the assessment continued, more information was gained about the partners' respective developmental histories, family lives, family compositions, and medical conditions. Neither had any past psychiatric history. Both were occasional "social" drinkers, with no history o f substance abuse. Neither partner smoked. W h e n asked about previous relationships, Reggie stated that he had been engaged at age 2 2 years but that his fiancé had called off the wedding. Since that time, he dated and had sexual relationships with several w o m e n until he began dating M a r s h a 3 years ago. M a r s h a dated in high school. She had two long-term relationships; one lasting 2Vi years and the other lasting 6 years. T h e 6-year relationship was with her "high school sweetheart" and began when M a r s h a was age 1 7 years. T h e other relationship t o o k place with a m a n she met in college when M a r s h a was age 2 4 years. Reggie and M a r s h a had experience with sex prior to their marriage. Reggie claimed that he found sex pleasurable but that he recently had trouble maintaining an erection. H e stated that he had less interest in sex. H e denied any past erectile difficulties. In contrast, M a r s h a suffered pain with intercourse that worsened over time. She began having pain at age 2 3 years. M a r s h a described the pain as a "sharp stab high up inside m e " that occurred after entry and during intercourse. M a r s h a was recently referred to a urologist, w h o told her that one reason for her pain was that her urethra was situated very close to her vagina, and this could create abdominal pain during intercourse, particularly during orgasm. She was given an antispasmodic medication. M a r s h a complained about using the medication because it had to be taken with a lot o f water several hours prior to intercourse.
372
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS
M a r s h a a n d R e g g i e were seen individually for o n e session. In the session w i t h M a r s h a , she stated t h a t she is able t o lubricate w i t h o u t difficulty a n d c a n experience o r g a s m during o r a l s e x a n d m a s t u r b a t i o n . She a l s o reiterated her suspicion t h a t R e g g i e might n o t w a n t children after all. She expressed feeling angry and betrayed. M a r s h a also described herself as " f o c u s e d a n d i n t e n s e " and stated t h a t R e g g i e tends t o be m u c h m o r e passive in the r e l a t i o n s h i p , causing M a r s h a t o feel as though she has t o be "the responsible o n e . " M a r s h a reported that a n o t h e r source o f discord w a s R e g g i e ' s avoidance o f conflict. M a r s h a insisted t h a t Reggie will "tell me w h a t I w a n t t o h e a r " rather t h a n w h a t he really thinks. In the session with Reggie, he disclosed that he was not ready t o have children and felt great pressure from M a r s h a t o "get his act together" so that the couple will be in a better financial position to start a family. Reggie expressed that he moved across the country and left a good-paying position so that M a r s h a could attend graduate school. H e felt resentful that M a r s h a "seems to forget that." Reggie stated that since his erectile difficulties began, he has been able to maintain an erection on some occasions during masturbation. H e has also awakened with an erection periodically. Reggie stated that he rarely initiates sex. W h e n M a r s h a initiates sex, he tells her that he is tired or not in the m o o d . Reggie and M a r s h a had a number o f relational issues that needed t o be addressed in therapy before sex therapy would become the focus o f treatment. During the first month o f treatment, the couple's relationship was the focus o f therapy. Reggie and M a r s h a were asked not t o engage in sexual activity during this time. T h e y were encouraged to show affection and be close if and when they were comfortable with doing so. As the partners' relationship began t o improve, they began spending more time with each other, reported enjoying each other's c o m pany more, and were expressing more affection toward one another. W o r k i n g on their relationship in general helped t o move them to greater nonsexual intimacy, and this is often an important step in sex therapy. Despite the request not to do so, the couple attempted intercourse twice before sex therapy began. E a c h time, Reggie was unable to maintain his erection. M a r s h a complained that she was still finding the experience painful. It is c o m m o n for couples to " b r e a k the rules" during treatment and t o engage in sexual intercourse or other forms o f genital contact. Thus, it is important t o let the couple k n o w that unsuccessful attempts are a frequent and " n o r m a l " occurrence so as t o avert further setbacks. Reggie had expressed feeling upset with himself. M a r s h a admitted that she would become frustrated and angry when " s e x didn't w o r k . " Reggie was experiencing interfering thoughts prior t o and during sexual relations. H e admitted feeling "like less o f a m a n " since his problem began. H e readily became worried about the quality and duration o f his erection as well as images o f M a r s h a ' s displeasure, disappointment, and anger. These thoughts would lead to greater anxiety and depression.
Sexual
Dysfunctions
It was important to w o r k with Reggie to help him restructure his thoughts and focus on thoughts that would facilitate feeling pleasure rather than those that would inhibit his sexual function. O n c e Reggie was better able to establish a positive sexually facilitating thought process, he was ready to proceed with sensate focus. During this time, M a r s h a ' s fear that she was s o m e h o w responsible for her husband's lack o f sexual interest, and the subsequent feelings o f inadequacy and frustration, were explored. This was an important component in assessing M a r s h a ' s cognitive process. Misunderstandings on the part o f the partner c a n sabotage treatment. Educating the couple about E D helped to alleviate some o f M a r s h a ' s fears and resentment. T h e couple also had to be educated about sensate focus and why certain restrictions were warranted during the intervention. Prior to beginning sensate focus, couples should be told to assert and protect themselves during each session. Self-assertion involves the expression o f phrases such as " I would like you to . . ." and " W h y don't you . . . " E x a m p l e s o f selfprotective phrases would include " I don't find that pleasing" and "Please touch me somewhere else." A gentle removal o f a partner's hand c a n also serve this purpose. O n e reason for this is that partners need to realize that likes and dislikes can be communicated without personalizing one another's statements. A formal agreement is made between the partners to ban attempts at intercourse or other genital contact during early stages o f the program. This agreement removes the pressure to "succeed" or perform. Goals o f sensate focus include (a) learning to touch one's partner for one's
own pleasure, (b) relaxing when being caressed and using a pro-
tective statement or gesture when one finds the touch unpleasant, (c) learning to recognize when one is "spectatoring," (d) recognizing h o w nice it is to touch and be with one's partner, (e) recognizing h o w nice it is to be touched, and (f) becoming more acutely aware o f what one is feeling physically and emotionally during the session. Reggie and M a r s h a began sensate focus treatment that, in their case, lasted approximately 3 months. T h r o u g h o u t the treatment process, it was necessary to monitor the partners' communication with one another regarding both sexual and nonsexual matters. It was also important to check out the couple's comfort levels during the duration o f treatment and to attend to any interfering thoughts o r c o m pliance problems that surfaced during the intervention period. Reggie and M a r s h a also were instructed to conduct their "sessions" in an environment that was free o f distractions and conducive to facilitating an erotic experience. T h i s meant that they also needed to schedule their sessions when they had adequate time to be together. Relational issues and the general quality o f the couple's relationship continued to be an integral part o f therapy. T a b l e 1 8 . 3 outlines the progressive stages o f sensate focus therapy. T h e couple was to spend at least 3 0 minutes together, three times per week. As M a r s h a and Reggie approached the fifth stage o f sensate focus, which involved vaginal containment without thrusting, certain modifications had to be made to try to alleviate
373
374
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS
M a r s h a ' s pain and discomfort. During this stage, the receptive partner controls activity. In this way, the amount o f movement and depth o f penetration can be controlled by the partner. Having M a r s h a assume the " t o p " position further enhanced her control. By doing so, she was better able to angle her pelvis in such a way that she had less discomfort. In the supine position, M a r s h a was encouraged to place pillows under her hips and to experiment with the height and angle that is most comfortable. During the final stage o f sensate focus treatment when intercourse resumed, it was suggested that M a r s h a use a certain lubricant to ensure adequate lubrication and t o heighten arousal. O n c e Reggie's E D resolved, the couple was given videotapes that demonstrated varied sexual positions and techniques. These tapes gave the couple additional information about positions that would be most comfortable for M a r s h a and satisfying to both partners. Eventually, Reggie and M a r s h a were able to resolve their sexual difficulties. Treatment success was due in large part to the partners' commitment to the therapeutic process, their resolve to w o r k through their marital and relational issues, and their compliance with the sensate focus intervention. T h e y were seen approximately 6 months after the termination o f therapy for a follow-up visit. N o problems were noted at that time.
rhythmically contract causes ejaculation. There
stimulation with the partner. T h e couple can
is a point at which men are not able to volun-
institute the technique by having the partner on
tarily control ejaculation. This is called the
top with the patient instructing the partner to
point of "ejaculatory inevitability," which is
stop movement when he senses he is losing
usually a few seconds before the start o f ejacu-
control. Often, the couple can progress to
lation. A central intervention in the treatment
simply slowing down when the patient begins
of PE is to help the patient learn to identify and
to feel close to ejaculating.
control the point of ejaculatory inevitability.
A second method o f intervention is the use
O n e method used to accomplish this involves
of the "squeeze technique," which involves
what is called the "stop-start" technique. T h e
stimulation to the penis until the patient is
patient begins by instituting the "start-stop"
close to ejaculation. Just prior to ejaculation,
technique during masturbation. Usually this
the patient or his partner places his or her
begins without the use o f lubrication. This
hand just below the head o f the penis and
allows the patient more privacy and control.
squeezes hard enough to cause partial loss o f
T h e patient stops self-stimulation until he feels
the patient's
like he has regained control. At that time, he
meant to help the patient become aware
erection. T h i s technique
is
begins stimulating his penis again. This proce-
o f sensations that precede orgasm and to
dure is repeated over time until the patient is
then control and delay orgasm on his own.
able to prolong his engagement in sexual stim-
This technique progresses from manual stim-
ulation while controlling his urge to ejaculate.
ulation to motionless intercourse and eventu-
Eventually, this procedure is transferred
ally to intercourse with movement.
to
Sexual Dysfunctions Drug therapy can also be effective. L o w doses o f antidepressant medications such as
compromises
our
effectiveness in
\
helping
those who struggle with issues pertaining to
Zoloft, Anafronil, and Prozac are often used
sex and sexuality. Treatments have continued
because o f their
that
to be more technologically or pharmacologi-
include the prolongation o f orgasm. M o r e
cally advanced. Granted, sex therapy can be
recently, topical agents such as anesthetics
credited for its ability to treat sexual problems
and herbal medications have been investi-
quickly and effectively; however, the goal
sexual side effects
gated as a possible treatment option (Choi
of most approaches is performance
et al., 1 9 9 9 , 2 0 0 2 ; M o r a l e s , 2 0 0 0 ) .
By shifting the traditional behavioral or cognitive-behavioral
approaches
based.
"to one
that
moves beyond behavior and communication CONCLUSIONS
to personal growth in relationship, we discover new horizons in human sexual poten-
The field o f sexual science has advanced con-
tial" (Kleinplatz, 2 0 0 1 , p. 1 9 0 ) . An important
siderably; however, the conceptual framework
factor in sex therapy is to help guide individu-
that guides the practice o f sex therapy has tra-
als in exercising greater personal agency over
ditionally been, and continues to be, rooted in
their relational needs as well as their erotic
biological science. A failure to broaden our
potential. Interventions
understanding and integration o f individual,
toward treating the individual as a whole,
relational, spiritual, and psychosocial factors
not simply as a malfunction
that may contribute to the problem at hand
equipment.
should
be
geared
in biological
REFERENCES Abarbanel, A. ( 1 9 7 8 ) . Diagnosis and treatment of coital discomfort. In J . LoPiccolo & L. LoPiccolo (Eds.), Handbook of sex therapy (pp. 2 4 1 - 2 5 9 ) . New York: Plenum. Althof, S. E. (1989). Psychogenic impotence: Treatment of men and couples. In S. R. Leiblum &C R. C. Rosen (Eds.), Principles and practice of sex therapy: Update for the 1990's (pp. 2 3 7 - 2 6 5 ) . New York: Guilford. Altwein, J . E., &c Keuler, F. U. (2001). Oral treatment of erectile dysfunction with 67, 2 5 7 - 2 6 3 . apomorphine SL. Urologia Internationalis, American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Aversa, Α., Isidori, A. M., Caprio, M., Cerilli, M., Frajese, V., & Fabbri, A. (2002). Penile pharmacotesting in diagnosing male erectile dysfunction: Evidence for lack of accuracy and specificity. International Journal of Andrology, 25(1), 6-10. Avina, C , O'Donohue, W. T., & Fisher, J . E. (2000). Sexual dysfunction in later life. In S. K. Whitbourne (Ed.), Psychopathology in later life (pp. 1 7 3 - 1 8 7 ) . New York: John Wiley. Bancroft, J . (1995). Human sexuality and its problems (2nd ed.). London: Longman. Bartlik, B., & Goldstein, M. Z. (2001). Men's sexual health after midlife. Psychiatric Services, 52, 2 9 1 - 2 9 3 . Basar, M., Atan, Α., & Tekdogan, U. Y . (2001). New concept parameters of RigiScan in differentiation of vascular erectile dysfunction: Is it a useful test? International Journal of Urology, 8, 6 8 6 - 6 9 1 .
375
376
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS Basar, M., Tekdogan, U. Y . , Yilmaz, E., Basar, H., Atan, Α., &c Batislam, E. (2001). The efficacy of sildenafil in different etiologies of erectile dysfunction. International Urology and Nephrology, 32, 4 0 3 - 4 0 7 . Beck, J . G. (1986). Self-generated distraction in erectile dysfunction: The role of attentional processes. Advances in Behaviour Research & Therapy, 8, 2 0 5 - 2 2 1 . Beck, J . G., &C Barlow, D. H. (1986a). The effects of anxiety and attentional focus on sexual responding: I. Physiological patterns in erectile dysfunction. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 24, 9 - 1 7 . Beck, J . G., &C Barlow, D. H. (1986b). The effects of anxiety and attentional focus on sexual responding: II. Cognitive and affective patterns in erectile dysfunction. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 24, 1 9 - 2 6 . Bhugra, D. (2000). Literature update: A critical review. Sexual and Relationship Therapy, 15, 4 2 1 - 4 2 7 . Biering-Sorensen, F., & Sonksen, J . (2001). Sexual function in spinal cord lesioned men. Spinal Cord, 39, 4 5 5 - 4 7 0 . Birk, L. (1999). Sex therapy. In A. M.. Nicholi (Ed.), The Harvard guide to psychiatry (pp. 5 2 5 - 5 4 2 ) . Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. Burchardt, M., Burchardt, T., Anastasiadis, A. G., Kill, A. J . , Shabsigh, Α., de la Taille, Α., Pawar, R. V., Baer, L., Se Shabsigh, R. (2001). Erectile dysfunction is a marker for cardiovascular complications and psychological functioning in men with hypertension. International Journal of Impotence Research, 13, 2 7 6 - 2 8 1 . Burls, Α., Gold, L., & Clark, W. (2001). Systematic review of randomized controlled trials of sildenafil (Viagra) in the treatment of male erectile dysfunction. British Journal of General Practice, 51, 1 0 0 4 - 1 0 1 2 . Choi, Η. K., Choi, Y . J . , Choi, Y . D., Rha, Κ. H , Kim, J . H , & Kim, D. K. (2002). SS-penogram: A new diagnostic test for erectile dysfunction. Yonsei Medical Journal, 43(1), 1-6. Choi, Η. K., Xin, Z. C , Choi, Y . D., Lee, W. H , Mah, S. Y . , & Kim, D. K. (1999). Safety and efficacy study with various doses of SS-cream in patients with premature ejaculation in a double-blind, randomized, placebo controlled clinical study. International Journal of Impotence Research, 11, 2 6 1 - 2 6 4 . Chou, F. F., Lee, C. H., Shu, K., Yu, T. J . , Hsu, K. T., & Sheen-Chen, S. M. (2001). Improvement of sexual function in male patients after parathyroidectomy for secondary hyperparathyroidism. Journal of the American College of Surgeons, 193, 4 8 6 - 4 9 2 . Cole, J . (2002). Delayed ejaculation. [Online]. Retrieved May 2 2 , 2 0 0 2 , from www.bbc.co.uk/health/mens/rel_sex_delay.shtml Davis, S. (2001). Testosterone and sexual desire in women. [Online]. Retrieved May 2, 2 0 0 1 , from www.aasect.org/jsetarticle.cfm Dennerstein, L., Dudley, E., & Burger, H. (2001). Are changes in sexual functioning during midlife due to aging or menopause? Fertility and Sterility, 76, 456—460. Denney, N. W., Field, J . K., & Quadagno, D. (1984). Sex differences in sexual needs and desires. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 13, 2 3 3 - 2 4 5 . Dey, J . , & Shepherd, M . D. (2002). Evaluation and treatment of erectile dysfunction in men with diabetes mellitus. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 77, 2 7 6 - 2 8 2 . Dunn, Κ. M., Croft, P. R., & Hackett, G. I. (1999). Association of sexual problems with social, psychological, and physical problems in men and women: A cross sectional population. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 53, 144-148. Engelhardt, P., Plas, E., Hiibner, W. Α., & Pfluger, H. (1998). Comparison of intraurethral liposomal and intracavernosal prostaglandin-El in the management of erectile dysfunction. British Journal of Urology, 81, 4 4 1 - 4 4 4 . Fagelman, E., Fagelman, Α., & Shabsigh, R. (2001). Efficacy, safety, and use of sildenafil in urologie practice. Urology, 57, 1 1 4 1 - 1 1 4 4 .
Sexual
Dysfunctions
Feldman, Η. Α., Goldstein, I., McKinlay, J . B., Hatzichristou, D. G., 8c Krane, R. J . (1994). Impotence and its medical and psychosocial correlates: results of the Massachusetts male ageing study. Journal of Urology, 151, 5 4 - 6 1 . Gaither, G. Α., Sc Plaud, J . J . (1997). The effects of secondary stimulus characteristics on men's sexual arousal. Journal of Sex Research, 34, 2 3 1 - 2 3 6 . Gelenberg, A. J . , Delgado, P., 8c Nurnberg, H. G. (2000). The side effects of antidepressant drugs. Current Psychiatry Reports, 2, 2 2 3 - 2 2 7 . Giuliano, F., Pena, B., Sc Mishra, A. (2002). Efficacy results and quality-of-life measures in men receiving sildenafil citrate for the treatment of erectile dysfunction. Quality of Life Research, 11, 6 1 3 . Glatt, A. E., Zinner, S. H., Se McCormack, W. M . (1990). The prevalence of dysparenia. Obstetrics and Gynecology, 75, 4 3 3 - 4 3 6 . Goldstone, S. E. (2000). Retrograde ejaculation. [Online]. Available: www.gayhealth.com/iowa-robot/common/condition.html?record=51 Halvorsen, J . G., Se Metz, Μ . Ε. (1992). Sexual dysfunction: I. Classification, etiology, and pathogenesis. Journal of the American Board of Family Practice, 5(1), 51-61. Hecht, M. J . , Neundorfer, B., Kiesewetter, F., Se Hiltz, M . J . (2001). Neuropathy is a major contributing factor to diabetic erectile dysfunction. Neurological Research, 23, 6 5 1 - 6 5 4 . Heiman, J . R. (1978). Uses of psychophysiology in the assessment and treatment of sexual dysfunction. In J . LoPiccolo Sc L. LoPiccolo (Eds.), Handbook of sex therapy (pp. 1 2 3 - 1 3 7 ) . New York: Plenum. Heiman, J . R., Sc Grafton-Becker, V. (1989). Orgasmic disorders in women. In S. R. Leiblum Se R. C. Rosen (Eds.), Principles and practice of sex therapy: Update for the 1990's (pp. 5 1 - 8 8 ) . New York: Guilford. Heiman, J . R., Se Meston, C. M . (1997). Evaluating sexual dysfunction in women. Clinical Obstetrics and Gynecology, 40, 6 1 6 - 6 2 9 . Herbert, J . (1996). Sexuality, stress, and the chemical architecture of the brain. Annual Review of Sex Research, 7, 1-43. Hurlbert, D. F. (1991). The role of assertiveness in female sexuality. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 17, 1 8 3 - 1 9 0 . Hurlbert, D. F., White, L. C , Powell, R. D., Sc Apt, C. (1993). Orgasm consistency training in the treatment of women reporting hypoactive sexual desire: An outcome comparison of women-only groups and couples-only groups. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 24, 3 - 1 3 . Incrocci, L., Slob, A. K., Sc Levendag, P. C. (2002). Sexual (dys)function after radiotherapy for prostate cancer: A review. International Journal of Radiation Oncology, Biology, Physics, 52, 6 8 1 - 6 9 3 . Jones, J . C , Sc Barlow, D. H. (1990). Self-reported frequency of sexual urges, fantasies, and masturbatory fantasies in heterosexual males and females. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 19, 2 6 9 - 2 7 9 . Jupp, J . J . , Sc McCabe, M . (1989). Sexual desire, general arousability, and sexual dysfunction. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 18, 5 0 9 - 5 1 6 . Kaplan, H. S. (1977). Hypoactive sexual desire. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 3, 3 - 9 . Kaplan, H. S. (1992). Does the CAT technique enhance female orgasm? Journal of Sex and Mental Therapy, 18, 2 8 5 - 2 9 1 . Katz, R. C , Gipson, M., Sc Turner, S. (1992). Brief report: Recent findings on the Sexual Aversion Scale. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 18, 1 4 1 - 1 4 6 . Kawanishi, Y., Kishimoto, T., Kimura, K., Yamaguchi, K., Nakatuji, H., Kojima, K., Yamamoto, Α., Sc Numata, A. (2001). Spring balance evaluation of the ischiocavernosus muscle. International Journal of Impotence Research, 13, 294-297.
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS Kelly, M . P., Strassberg, D. S., & Kircher, J . R. (1990). Attitudinal and experiential correlates of anorgasmia. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 19, 1 6 5 - 1 7 7 . Kleinplatz, P. J . (2001). New direction in sex therapy: Innovations and alternatives. New York: Taylor & Francis. Lalumiere, M . L., & Quinsey, V. L. (1998). Pavlovian conditioning of sexual interests in human males. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 27, 2 4 1 - 2 5 2 . Lamont, J . A. (1978). Vaginismus. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology, 131, 6 3 2 - 6 3 6 . Laqueur, T. (1990). Making sex: Body and gender from the Greeks to Freud. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Laumann, E. O., Gagnon, J . H., Michael, R. T., & Michaels, S. (1994). The social organization of sexuality. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Laumann, E. O., Gagnon, J . H., Michael, R. T., & Michaels, S. (1995). National health and social life survey, 1992 [computer file, ICPSR version]. Ann Arbor, MI: National Opinion Research. (Chicago: University of Chicago Press) Leiblum, S. R. (2002). After sildenafil: Bridging the gap between pharmacologic Psychiatry, treatment and satisfying sexual relationships. Journal of Clinical 63(5), 1 7 - 2 2 . Leiblum, S. R., Pervin, L. Α., & Campbell, Ε. H. (1989). The treatment of vaginismus: Success and failure. In S. R. Leiblum & R. C. Rosen (Eds.), Principles and practice of sex therapy: Update for the 1990's (pp. 1 1 3 - 1 3 8 ) . New York: Guilford. Leiblum, S. R., & Rosen, R. C. (Eds.). (1989). Principles and practice of sex therapy: Update for the 1990's. New York: Guilford. LoPiccolo, J . , & Friedman, J . M . (1988). Broad-spectrum treatment of low sexual desire. In S. R. Leiblum & R. C. Rosen (Eds.), Sexual desire disorders (pp. 1 0 7 - 1 4 4 ) . New York: Guilford. LoPiccolo, J . , & Heiman, J . R. (1978). Sexual assessment and history interview. In J . LoPiccolo &C L. LoPiccolo (Eds.), Handbook of sex therapy (pp. 1 0 3 - 1 1 3 ) . New York: Plenum. Malavaud, B., Rostaing, L., Rischmann, P., Sarramon, J . P., & Durand, D. (2000). High prevalence of erectile dysfunction after renal transplantation. Transplantation, 69, 2 1 2 1 - 2 1 2 4 . Marumo, K., Nakashima, J . , & Murai, M . (2001). Age-related prevalence of erectile dysfunction in Japan: Assessment by the International Index of Erectile Function. International Journal of Urology, 8, 5 3 - 5 9 . Masters, W. H., & Johnson, V. E. (1970). Human sexual inadequacy. Boston: Little, Brown. McCarthy, B. W. (1989). Cognitive and behavioral strategies and techniques in the treatment of early ejaculation. In S. R. Leiblum &c R. C. Rosen (Eds.), Principles and practice of sex therapy: Update for the 1990's (pp. 1 4 3 - 1 6 7 ) . New York: Guilford. McCauley, C , & Swann, C. P. (1978). Male-female differences in sexual fantasy. Journal of Research in Personality, 12, 7 6 - 8 6 . McCauley, C , & Swann, C. P. (1980). Sex differences in the frequency and functions of fantasies during sexual activity. Journal of Research in Personality, 14, 400-411. McCullough, A. R. (2001). Prevention and management of erectile dysfunction following radical prostatectomy. Urologie Clinics of North America, 28, 6 1 3 - 6 2 7 . McDowell, A. J . , Snellgrove, C. Α., & Bond, M . J . (2001). Beyond Viagra: Psychological issues in the assessment and treatment of erectile dysfunction. Australian Family Physician, 30, 8 6 7 - 8 7 3 . McGovern, Κ. B . , Stewart, R. C , &C LoPiccolo, J . (1975). Secondary orgasmic dysfunction: I. Analysis and strategies for treatment. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 4, 2 6 5 - 2 7 5 .
Sexual
Dysfunctions
McVary, K. T., Carrier, S., &C Wessells, H. (2001). Smoking and erectile dysfunction: Evidence based analysis. Journal of Urology, 166, 1 6 2 4 - 1 6 3 2 . Meana, M., Binik, Y . M., Khalife, S., & Cohen, D. (1999). Psychosocial correlates of pain attributions in women with dyspareunia. Journal of Consultation Liaison Psychiatry, 40, 4 9 7 - 5 0 2 . Meisler, A. W., Carey, M . P., & Krauss, D. J . (1988). Success and failure in penile prosthesis surgery: Two cases highlighting the importance of psychosocial factors. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 14, 1 0 8 - 1 1 9 . Merrick, G. S., Wallner, K., Butler, W. M., Lief, J . EL, & Sutlief, S. (2001). Shortterm sexual function after prostate brachytherapy. International Journal of Cancer, 96, 3 1 3 - 3 1 9 . Meston, C. M., & Frohlich, P. F. (2000). The neurobiology of sexual function. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 1 0 1 2 - 1 0 3 0 . Metz, Μ . Ε., & Epstein, Ν. (2002). Assessing the role of relationship conflict in sexual dysfunction. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 28, 1 3 9 - 1 6 4 . Metz, Μ . Ε., & Pryor, J . L. (2000). Premature ejaculation: A psychophysiological approach for assessment and management. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 26, 2 9 3 - 3 2 0 . Metz, Μ . Ε., Pryor, J . L., Abuzzahab, F., Nesvacil, L., & Koznar, J . (1997). Premature ejaculation: A psychophysiological review. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 23, 3 - 2 3 . Morales, A. (2000). Developmental status of topical therapies for erectile and ejaculatory dysfunction. International Journal of Impotence Research, 12(4), S80-S85. Morokoff, P. J . (1985). Effects of sex guilt, repression, sexual "arousability," and sexual experience on female sexual arousal during erotica and fantasy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1 7 7 - 1 8 7 . Mulhall, J . P., Bukofzer, S., Edmonds, A. L., & George, M. (2001). An open-label, uncontrolled dose-optimization study of sublingual apomorphine in erectile dysfunction. Clinical Therapeutics, 23, 1 2 6 0 - 1 2 7 1 . Muller, M . J . , & Benkert, O. (2001). Lower self-reported depression in patients with erectile dysfunction after treatment with sildenafil. Journal of Affective Disorders, 66, 2 5 5 - 2 6 1 . Muller, M . J . , Ruof, J . , Graf-Morgenstern, M., Porst, H., & Benkert, O. (2001). Quality of partnership in patients with erectile dysfunction after sildenafil treatment. Pharmacopsychiatry, 34(3), 9 1 - 9 5 . Nettelbladt, P., & Uddenberg, N. (1979). Sexual dysfunction and sexual satisfaction in 58 married Swedish men. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 23, 1 4 1 - 1 4 7 . O'Donohue, W., Dopke, C. Α., & Swingen, D. N. (1997). Psychotherapy for female sexual dysfunction: A review. Clinical Psychology Review, 17, 5 3 7 - 5 6 6 . Osborn, M., Hawton, K., & Gath, D. (1988). Sexual dysfunction among middle aged women in the community. British Medical Journal, 296, 9 5 9 - 9 6 2 . Paige, Ν. M., Hays, R. D., Litwin, M . S., Rajfer, J . , & Shapiro, M. E. (2001). Improvement in emotional well-being and relationships of users of sildenafil. Journal of Urology, 166, 1 7 4 4 - 1 7 4 8 . Palace, Ε. M. (1995). A cognitive-physiological process model of sexual arousal and response. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 3, 3 7 0 - 3 8 4 . Person, E. S., Terestman, N., Myers, W. Α., Goldberg, E. L., & Salvadori, C. (1989). Gender differences in sexual behaviors and fantasies in a college population. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 15, 1 8 7 - 1 9 8 . Pfaus, J . G. (1999). Neurobiology of sexual behavior. Current Opinion in neurobiology, 9, 7 5 1 - 7 5 8 . Phillips, R. L., & Slaughter, J . R. (2000). Depression and sexual desire. American Family Physician, 62, 7 8 2 - 7 8 6 .
380
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS Pierce, A. P. (2000). The coital alignment technique (CAT): An overview of studies. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 26, 2 5 7 - 2 6 8 . Pollets, D., Ducharme, S., & Pauporte, J . (1999). Psychological considerations in the assessment of erectile dysfunction. Sexuality and Disability, 17(2), 1 2 9 - 1 4 5 . Ponticas, Y . (1992). Sexual aversion versus hypoactive sexual desire: A diagnostic challenge. Psychiatric Medicine, 10, 2 7 4 - 2 8 1 . Potters, L., Torre, T., Fearn, P. Α., Leibel, S. Α., &C Kattan, M. W. (2001). Potency after permanent prostate brachytherapy for localized prostate cancer. International Journal of Radiation Oncology, Biology, Physics, 50, 1 2 3 5 - 1 2 4 2 . Regan, P. C. (1996). Rhythms of desire: The association between menstrual cycle phases and female sexual desire. Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, 5(3), 145-156. Richter, S., Vardi, Y . , Ringel, Α., Shalev, M., & Nissenkorn, I. (2001). Intracavernous injections: Still the gold standard for treatment of erectile dysfunction. International Journal of Impotence Research, 13, 1 7 2 - 1 7 5 . Rizvi, K., Hampson, J . P., &c Harvey, J . N. (2002). Do lipid-lowering drugs cause erectile dysfunction? A systematic review. Family Practice, 19(1), 9 5 - 9 8 . Robins, L. N., Helzer, J . E., Weissman, M. M., Orvaschel, H., Gruenberg, E., Burke, J . D., Jr., & Reiger, D. A. (1984). Lifetime prevalence of specific psychiatric disorders in three sites. Archives of General Psychiatry, 41, 9 4 9 - 9 5 8 . Rosen, E. C , &c Leiblum, S. R. (1989). Assessment and treatment of desire disorders. In S. R. Leiblum &c R. C. Rosen (Eds.), Principles and practice of sex therapy: Update for the 1990's (pp. 1 9 - 5 0 ) . New York: Guilford. Rowland, D. L., Cooper, S. E., & Schneider, M . (2001). Defining premature ejaculation for experimental and clinical investigations. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 30, 2 3 5 - 2 5 3 . Sanchez, R. Α., Vidal, J . , Jauregui, M. L., Barrera, M., Recio, C , Giner, M., Toribio, L., Salvador, S., Sanmartin, Α., de la Fuente, M., Santos, J . F., de Juan, F. J . , Moraleda, S., Mendez, J . L., Ramirez, L., & Casado, R. M . (2001). Efficacy, safety, and predictive factors of therapeutic success with sildenafil for erectile dysfunction in patients with different spinal cord injuries. Spinal Cord, 39, 6 3 7 - 6 4 3 . Scharff, D. E. (1988). An object relations approach to inhibited sexual desire. In S. R. Leiblum &c R. C. Rosen (Eds.), Sexual desire disorders (pp. 4 5 - 7 4 ) . New York: Guilford. Schiavi, R. C , & Segraves, R. T. (1995). The biology of sexual function. Clinical Sexuality, 18(1), 7 - 2 3 . Schover, L. R. (1989). Sexual problems in chronic illness. In S. R. Leiblum, E. Rosen, 8c C. Raymond (Eds.), Principles and practice of sex therapy (3rd ed., pp. 3 9 8 - 4 2 2 ) . New York: Guilford. Schreiner-Engel, P., 8c Schiavi, R. C. (1986). Lifetime psychopathology in individuals with low sexual desire. International Journal of Group Psychotherapy, 17, 211-224. Schwartz, M . F., Sc Masters, W. H. (1988). Inhibited sexual desire: The Masters and Johnson Institute treatment model. In S. R. Leiblum & R. C. Rosen (Eds.), Sexual desire disorders (pp. 2 2 9 - 2 4 2 ) . New York: Guilford. Scott, F. B., Bradley, W. E., Sc Timm, G. W. (1973). Treatment of urinary incontinence by implantable prosthetic sphincter. Urology, 1, 2 5 2 - 2 5 9 . Seeber, M., 8e Gorrell, C. ( 2 0 0 1 , November-December). The science of orgasm. Psychology Today, pp. 4 8 - 5 9 . Seidman, S. N., 8t Roose, S. P. (2000). The relationship between depression and erectile dysfunction. Current Psychiatry Reports, 2, 2 0 1 - 2 0 5 . Seidman, S. N., 8c Roose, S. P. (2001). Sexual dysfunction and depression. Current Psychiatry Reports, 3, 2 0 2 - 2 0 8 .
Sexual
Dysfunctions
Seidman, S. N., Roose, S. P., Menza, Μ . Α., Shabsigh, R., Sc Rosen, R. C. (2001). Treatment of erectile dysfunction in men with depressive symptoms: Results of a placebo-controlled trial with sildenafil citrate. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 1 6 2 3 - 1 6 3 0 . Shifren, J . L., Braunstein, G. D., Simon, J . Α., Casson, P. R., Buster, J . E., Redmond, G. P., Burki, R. E., Ginsburg, E. S., Rosen, R. C., Leiblum, S. R., Caramelli, Κ. E., 8c Mazer, N. A. (2000). Transdermal testosterone treatment in women with impaired sexual function after oophorectomy. New England Journal of Medicine, 343, 6 8 2 - 6 8 8 . Shirai, M., Takimoto, Y . , Ishii, N., 8c Iwamoto, T. (2001). Influence of erectile dysfunction on daily life and general attitudes towards treatment. Japanese Journal of Urology, 92, 6 6 6 - 6 7 3 . Shires, Α., 8c Miller, D. (1998). A preliminary study comparing psychological factors associated with erectile dysfunction in heterosexual and homosexual men. Sexual and Marital Therapy, 13, 3 7 - 4 9 . Sipski, M., Alexander, C. J . , 8c Rosen, R. (2001). Sexual arousal and orgasm in women: Effects of spinal cord injury. Annals of Neurology, 49, 3 5 - 4 4 . Small, M . P., Carrion, H. M., 8c Gordon, J . A. (1975). Small-Carrion penile prosthesis: New implant management of impotence. Journal of Urology, 167, 1 1 9 0 . Spangler, J . G., Summerson, J . H., Bell, R., 8c Konen, J . C. (2002). Cigarette smoking and erectile dysfunction. Journal of Family Practice, 51, 8 1 . Spector, I. P., 8c Carey, M . P. (1990).. Incidence and prevalence of the sexual dysfunctions: A critical review of the empirical literature. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 19, 3 8 9 - 4 0 1 . Steers, W., Guay, A. T., Leriche, Α., Gingell, C , Hargreave, T. B . , Wright, P. J . , Price, D. E., 8c Feldman, R. A. (2001). Assessment of the efficacy and safety of Viagra (sildenafil citrate) in men with erectile dysfunction during long-term treatment. International Journal of Impotence Research, 13, 2 6 1 - 2 6 7 . Stoleru, S., Grégoire, M., Gerard, D., Decety, J., Lafarge, E., Cinotti, L., Lavenne, E , Le Bars, D., Vernet-Maury, E., Rada, H., Collet, C , Mazoyer, B., Forest, M. G., Magnin, F., Spira, Α., 8c Comard, D. (1999). Neuroanatomical correlates of visually evoked sexual arousal in human males. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 28, 1-21. Tam, S. W., Worcel, M., Sc Wyllie, M. (2001). Yohimbine: A clinical review. Pharmacology and Therapeutics, 91, 2 1 5 - 2 4 3 . Tiefer, L. (1991). Commentary on the status of sex research: Feminism, sexuality, and sexology. Journal of Psychology and Human Sexuality, 4, 5 - 4 2 . Tollison, C. D., Sc Adams, Η. E. (1979). Sexual disorders: Treatment, theory, and research. New York: Gardner. Tsujimura, Α., Yamanaka, M., Takahashi, T., Mura, H., Nishmura, K., Koga, M., Iwasa, Α., Takeyama, M., Matsumiya, K., Takahara, S., 8c Okuyama, A. (2002). The clinical studies of sildenafil for the ageing male. International Journal of Andrology, 25(1), 2 8 - 3 3 . van Berlo, W., S i Ensink, B . (2000). Problems with sexuality after sexual assault. Annual Review of Sex Research, 11, 2 3 5 - 2 5 7 . Verhulst, J . , 8c Heiman, J . R. (1979). An interactional approach to sexual dysfunction. American Journal of Family Therapy, 7, 1 9 - 3 6 . Verhulst, J . , Sc Heiman, J . R. (1988). A systems perspective on sexual desire. In S. R. Leiblum Se R. C. Rosen (Eds.), Sexual desire disorders (pp. 2 4 3 - 2 7 0 ) . New York: Guilford. Viera, A. J . , Clenney, T. L., Shenenberger, D. W., 8c Green, G. F. (1999). Newer pharmacologic alternatives for erectile dysfunction. American Family Physician, 60, 1 1 5 9 - 1 1 7 2 . Waldinger, M. D., van De Plas, Α., Pattij, T., van Oorschot, R., Coolen, L. M., Veening, J . G., Sc Oliver, B. (2002). The selective serotonin re-uptake inhibitors
382
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS fluvoxamine and paroxetine differ in sexual inhibitory effects after chronic 160, 2 9 3 - 2 8 9 . treatment. Psychopharmacology, Wallen, K. (1990). Desire and ability: Hormones and the regulation of female sexual Reviews, 14, 2 3 3 - 2 4 1 . behavior. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Walser, R. D., & Kern, J . M . (1996). Relationships among childhood sexual abuse, sex guilt, and sexual behavior in adult clinical samples. Journal of Sex Research, 33, 3 2 1 - 3 2 6 . Weiner, D. W., & Rosen, R. C. (1999). Sexual dysfunctions and disorders. In T. Millon, P. H. Blaney, & R. D. Davis (Eds.), Oxford textbook of psychopathology (pp. 4 1 0 - 4 4 3 ) . New York: Oxford University Press. Weisberg, R. B . , Brown, Τ. Α., Wincze, J . P., & Barlow, D. H. (2001). Causal attributions and male sexual arousal: The impact of attributions for a bogus erectile difficulty on sexual arousal, cognitions, and affect. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 2 3 4 - 3 3 4 . Weiss, J . N., &c Mellinger, B. C. (1990). Sexual dysfunction in elderly men. Clinical Geriatric Medicine, 6, 1 8 5 - 1 9 6 . Wincze, J . P. (1971). A comparison of systematic desensitization and "vicarious extinction" in a case of frigidity. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 2, 2 8 5 - 2 8 9 . Wincze, J . P., & Carey, M . P. (1991). Sexual dysfunction: A guide for assessment and treatment. New York: Guilford. Wolf, S. (2001). Retrograde ejaculation. [Online]. Retrieved on May 2 8 , 2 0 0 2 , from http://health.discovery.com/diseasesandcond/encyclopedia/2477.html
CHAPTER
19
Human Immunodeficiency Virus and Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndrome ANDREW C . BLALOCK AND PETER E . CAMPOS .
A
cquired immune deficiency syndrome
called combination therapy or cocktail therapy)
(AIDS) is characterized by severe
has
immunosuppression
and
ensuing
opportunistic infections that result from infec-
extended
the
lives o f many
living with HIV/AIDS. Called the
patients "Lazarus
syndrome," the extended survival period has
virus
brought hope and new challenges to H I V
(HIV). The disease was first recognized in 1 9 8 1
patients and their caregivers. As the number o f
tion with
h u m a n immunodeficiency
with the unexplained occurrence o f clusters of
persons living with H I V increases, psychologists
cases of Pneumocystis carinii pneumonia and
whose clinical activity once focused on crisis
Kaposi's sarcoma among young homosexual
management and bereavement counseling will
men. As such, it was initially referred to as
be faced with a wider, more diverse spectrum of
gay-related immune disorder. As similar cases
psychosocial issues, including the emotional,
of these and other opportunistic infections asso-
behavioral, cognitive, social, and
ciated with unexplained
aspects o f chronic illness coping and adjustment.
immunosuppression
vocational
were subsequently reported in persons with hemophilia, recipients o f blood transfusions,
and
products/
injecting drug users
and
their heterosexual partners, the disease entity was renamed
acquired immune
BACKGROUND AND EPIDEMIOLOGY
deficiency
syndrome. In 1 9 9 6 , pharmacological treatment advances
In 2 0 0 1 , the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention ( C D C ) estimated that 8 0 0 , 0 0 0 to
introduced a new era to the epidemic and shifted
9 0 0 , 0 0 0 individuals in the United States were
HIV/AIDS from an acute, irnminently terminal
living with H I V , with approximately 4 0 , 0 0 0
medical condition to a more chronic illness. By
new H I V infections occurring in the U.S. every
significantly slowing disease progression, highly
year ( C D C , 2 0 0 1 a ) . By gender, about 7 0 % o f
active antiretroviral treatment (HAART) (also
the new infections are in men, whereas 3 0 %
384
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS
70 70 70 70 70
70 1986
1988
1990
1ΘΘ2
1994
1996
1998
2000
>fearof Report Figure 19.1
Proportion of AIDS Cases, by Race/Ethnicity and Year of Report, 1 9 8 5 - 2 0 0 0 , United States
are in women. In terms of race/ethnicity,
1 9 9 8 , 3 0 0 , 0 0 0 in 1 9 9 9 , and 3 2 3 , 0 0 0 in 2 0 0 0 .
5 4 % o f the new cases are among African
This
Americans, 2 6 % among Caucasians, 1 9 %
increasing need for prevention and health care
growing
population
represents
an
among Hispanics, and 1 % among other ethnic
services ( C D C , 2 0 0 1 c ) .
minority groups (see Figure 1 9 . 1 ) . In terms of the major exposure/risk factor category, approximately 4 2 % o f new H I V cases are
The HTV Disease Spectrum
men w h o have sex with men, 3 3 % are from
Because HTV causes subtle changes in the
heterosexual contact, and 2 5 % are from intra-
immune system long before an infected person
venous drug use (Figure 1 9 . 2 ) . Nearly all o f
feels sick or develops disease symptoms, the
the pediatric AIDS cases reported (more than
term "HTV disease" is used to cover the entire
9 0 % ) resulted from perinatal H I V transmis-
HEV illness spectrum, from initial infection to
sion. In 1 9 9 9 , the annual number o f AIDS
full-blown AIDS (also called advanced HTV
cases appeared to level, while the decline in
disease). T h e time that it takes for each individ-
AIDS deaths slowed considerably. Although
ual person to go through disease stages varies
the rate o f reported AIDS cases has declined
widely. For most people, however, the process
gradually among Caucasian gay/bisexual male
of HEV disease is fairly slow, taking several
adults over age 2 5 years, epidemiological
years from infection to the development o f
trends show increasing rates o f infection in
severe immunodeficiency (Cohen, 1 9 9 8 ) .
women (particularly women o f color), African
Behavioral health and health psychology
American men, and young adults under age
issues are an important part o f comprehensive
2 5 years (Figure 1 9 . 3 ) . Moreover, despite the
health care across the entire H I V disease spec-
decline in deaths and cases, more people are
trum. Although much o f behavioral
living with H I V than ever before: 2 7 5 , 0 0 0 in
care focuses on prevention and psychosocial
health
HIV
385
and AIDS
70
M e n w h o h a v e sex with m e n ( M S M )
60 CO
«
S *
I
50 0
30
Injection d r u g use ( I D U )
MSM & I D U 1986
1988
' 1990
' 1992
1994
1998
1996
' 2000
>fear of Diagnosis Figure 19.2
Proportion of Estimated Adult/Adolescent AIDS Cases, by Exposure Category and Year of Diagnosis, 1 9 8 5 - 2 0 0 0 , United States
N O T E : Data adjusted for reporting delays and proportional redistribution of cases reported without risk.
some
serum at that particular time. M a n y HFV
psychological problems, such as m o o d dis-
patients are knowledgeable about these disease
orders and cognitive changes, are directly
parameters and frequently use them to describe
related to the effect of H I V on the central ner-
their illness status. F r o m a psychological per-
vous system. In medical and behavioral clinical
spective, these numbers may take on particular
care, there are two HIV-specific disease param-
meaning and may be associated with a range of
eters (laboratory test results) that are c o m -
emotional reactions. For example, a patient
monly used to gauge an individual's illness
may be relieved that C D 4 has risen with med-
status or progression. First, C D 4 count is an
ication or frightened that there has been no sig-
index of immunosuppression. C D 4 is a type o f
nificant change in viral load after beginning a
issues o f chronic illness adjustment,
lymphocyte destroyed by HFV. In a nonin-
new regimen. For these reasons, psychologists
fected individual, a C D 4 count of 5 0 0 to 1,200
who work with H I V patients and their families
is considered within normal limits, whereas a
should be familiar with C D 4 and viral load.
C D 4 count o f less than 2 0 0 is considered significantly immunosuppressed and is one o f the criteria for an AIDS diagnosis. Second, viral load or viral burden is an index o f virus concentration in body fluids such as blood or cere-
Transmission Risk Factors and Infection HIV is transmitted in infectious body fluids
brospinal fluid. Current laboratory technology
through unprotected
measures viral load in a range o f undetectable
blood contact (including injection drug nee-
to more than 7 5 0 , 0 0 0 . It is crucial for patients
dles, blood transfusions, accidents in health
and caregivers to remember
that
"unde-
sexual contact, direct
care settings, and certain blood products), and
tectable" does not mean the absence o f virus;
mother-to-baby
rather, it means a very low concentration in the
through breast milk). Infectious body fluids
(prenatally/perinatally
or
386
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS
Women
Exposure Category Men who have sex with men Injection drug use (IDU) MSM/IDU
Hemophilia Other/not identified
Figure 19.3
Estimated AIDS Incidence Among Adults/Adolescents Diagnosed in 2 0 0 0 , by Sex and Exposure Category, United States
N O T E : Data adjusted for reporting delays and estimated proportional redistribution of cases initially reported without risk. Data reported through June 2 0 0 1 .
include blood, semen, vaginal secretions, and
symptoms or look sick to have H I V . In fact,
breast milk, whereas noninfectious body fluids
people may look perfectly healthy for many
include saliva, tears, sweat, urine, and feces.
years despite the fact that they have H I V in
Contact with the virus, called exposure, does
their bodies. T h e only way in which to find out
not always lead to infection. Healthy unbroken
whether a person is infected is to take an H I V
skin is an excellent barrier to H I V infection
antibody test.
because it does not allow viral entry. HEV can only enter through an open cut or sore or through contact with the mucous membranes. The likelihood of infection is a function o f the viral concentration in the particular
fluid;
Primary (or Acute) Infection Primary or acute H I V infection is the first stage o f H I V disease, when the virus estab-
blood contains the highest viral concentration,
lishes itself in the body. Up to 7 0 % o f people
followed by semen and vaginal fluids. Breast
newly infected with H I V will experience some
milk can contain a high concentration o f the
"flu-like" symptoms that usually last no more
virus and so is a high-risk factor for infants
than a few days and include fevers, chills,
because they have developing immune systems
night sweats, and rashes (not cold-like symp-
and consume a high volume o f breast milk
toms). T h e remaining percentage o f people
relative to body weight (American Psychiatric
either do not experience acute infection or
Association, 1 9 9 8 ; Kalichman, 1 9 9 8 ) . People
have symptoms so mild that they might not
with HTV are considered to be infectious
notice them. During acute H I V infection, the
immediately after infection. Although "infec-
virus infiltrates the lymph nodes, a process
tivity" is a function o f viral load and may fluc-
that is believed to take 3 to 5 days. Then H I V
tuate accordingly, individuals are infectious at
actively reproduces and releases new virus
all times. Also, a person does not need to have
particles into the bloodstream. This burst of
HIV and AIDS rapid H I V replication usually lasts about 2 months. People at this stage often have a very high H I V viral load (Cohen, 1 9 9 8 ) .
illness, is the current criterion for an AIDS diagnosis (also called full-blown AIDS or
antibodies
to
the virus. Approximately 9 5 % o f the people infected with H I V will develop antibodies within 3 months after infection. Nearly all them
will develop
T h e concurrent combination of two factors, of an opportunistic infection or AIDS-defining
"Seroconversion" refers to the time when
of
AIDS a C D 4 count o f less than 2 0 0 and the presence
Seroconversion and Asymptomatic HIV Disease the body begins producing
387
antibodies
within
6 months after infection. Given these time frames, individuals w h o seek H I V testing should wait at least 3 months after suspected exposure for the test. If their first result is negative, they should c o m e b a c k for a second
advanced H I V disease). These illnesses are called opportunistic because they are caused by organisms that cannot induce disease in people with normal immune systems but take the "opportunity" to flourish in people with H I V . Common
opportunistic
infections
are
described in Table 1 9 . 1 . Receiving an AIDS diagnosis does not necessarily mean that the person will die soon. In fact, with treatment, many individuals live for many years after their diagnosis (Cohen, 1 9 9 8 ) .
test 3 months later. T h e period o f viral replication is quite variable because the virus slowly damages the immune system for years after
infection. D u r i n g
this period,
the
individual will not experience or exhibit any
PREVENTION AND ADHERENCE
Primary Prevention: Risk Reduction
signs o f AIDS-defining illnesses because the
Over the years, the behavioral health care
level of immunosuppression is not critical.
community has acquired a formidable litera-
However, it is extremely important
ture base that addresses H I V prevention at
that
people with H I V seek appropriate care even
both the individual and community levels. A
if they feel fine at the m o m e n t because the
review o f this literature is well beyond the
virus
scope o f this chapter; however, the C D C ' s
could
immune
already
systems
be
damaging
(American
their
Psychiatric
Association, 1 9 9 8 ; Cohen, 1 9 9 8 ) .
(2001b)
Compendium
provides
a summary
o f this research. In
general, prevention strategies are based on
Symptomatic HTV Disease O n c e the immune system is
o f H I V Prevention
Interventions W i t h Evidence o f Effectiveness
reducing or eliminating behaviors that put an damaged,
individual
at
risk
for
contracting
HIV.
many people will begin to experience some
Consistent with the two largest risk factor
mild symptoms (e.g., skin rashes, fatigue,
categories, prevention efforts are focused on
slight weight loss, night sweats). M o s t will
changing sexual behavior and drug use behav-
have mild symptoms such as these before
ior. Although there are numerous
developing more serious illnesses. Although a
about behavior change, some o f the most pop-
person's prognosis varies greatly depending
ular and widely used ones in H I V prevention
theories
on his or her ability to access support, ser-
are the harm reduction models. Unlike tradi-
vices, and preventive treatment, it is generally
tional models that emphasize abstinence from
believed that it takes the average person 5 to
high-risk behavior, the harm reduction models
7 years to experience the first mild symptom
assess the individual's psychological readiness
(Cohen, 1 9 9 8 ) .
to
change
high-risk
behavior
and
then
388
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS Table 19.1
Common Opportunistic Infections
Opportunistic
Infection
Common
Name
Etiology/Symptoms
Candidiasis
Thrush
Fungal infection of the mouth, throat, or vagina
Cytomegalovirus
CMV
Viral infection that causes eye disease that can lead to blindness
Herpes simplex viruses
Oral herpes or genital herpes
Lesions or "cold sores"
Mycobacterium avium complex
M A C or MAI
Bacterial infection that can cause recurring fevers, general sick feelings, problems with digestion, and serious weight loss
Pneumocystis carinii pneumonia
PCP
Protozoal infection that causes pneumonia
Toxoplasmosis
Toxo
Protozoal infection of the brain
KS
Type of skin cancer
Kaposi's sarcoma
design/negotiate interventions that match that
Project, a synthesized compendium of more
readiness. F o r sexual behavior change, pre-
than 2 0 0 programs with demonstrated positive
vention strategies involve education
about
the range o f transmission risk across various
outcomes, successful prevention
programs
generally have several elements in common.
sexual behaviors, negotiation/refusal skills,
First, they address specific goals and objectives,
appropriate
condom use, and the role o f
target specific audiences, and concentrate on
mood-altering substances in sexual behavior
specific learner priorities and behaviors to meet
decision making. For drug use
behavior,
needs identified by the community. Second,
prevention strategies involve elimination o f
they are developed from existing social and
sharing injection equipment, disinfection o f
behavioral science theory. Third, they provide
" w o r k s " or injection equipment, condom use,
opportunities
and
enrollment in drug abuse
treatment
(American Psychiatric Association, 1 9 9 8 ) .
for patients to develop
and
practice prevention skills and for program providers to learn how to enhance those skills.
Even the most widely used prevention coun-
Fourth, they are appropriate for and accept-
seling models acknowledge that knowledge
able to the targeted audience culturally, develop-
about H I V transmission and prevention is nec-
mentally, and sexually ( C D C , 2 0 0 1 b ) .
essary but not sufficient for behavior change (Ajzen &c Fishbein, 1 9 8 0 ; Fisher &c Fisher, 1 9 9 7 ) . M o r e effective interventions rely on skills training and take into account individual psychosocial and
Secondary Prevention: Medication Adherence
cognitive strategies
Highly active antiretroviral therapy con-
(Kalichman & Hospers, 1 9 9 7 ) . Effective psy-
sists o f a protease inhibitor combined with at
chological interventions help patients to per-
least t w o other drugs. H A A R T has been
ceive themselves as at risk for HFV infection,
shown to suppress H I V viral load, increase
help patients to address motivation to reduce
C D 4 count, improve clinical health,
risk, and ensure that patients have the skills
decrease AIDS-related mortality (Carpenter
and resources to implement risk reduction
et al., 1 9 9 8 ; Catz, Kelly, Bogart, Benotsch, &
strategies. According to the C D C ' s ( 2 0 0 1 b )
McAuliffe, 2 0 0 0 ) . T h e success o f these c o m -
H I V / A I D S Prevention
bination
Research Synthesis
therapies
is patient
and
adherence.
HIV and AIDS Table 19.2
Nucleoside Analog Reverse Transcriptase Inhibitors
Year
Generic
1987 1991 1992 1994 1995 1997
Zidovudine Didanosine Zalcitabine Stavudine Lamivudine Zidovudine/ Lamivudine Abacavir Zidovudine/ Lamivudine/ Abacavir Tenofovir
1998 2000
2001
389
Name
Trade
Name
Retrovir Videx Hivid Zerit Epivir Combivir Ziagen Trizivir
Viread
Also known as AZT, Z D V ddl ddC, dideoxycytidine d4T 3TC Combines Z D V and 3 T C 1592U89 Combines AZT, 3TC, Abacavir bis-poc PMPA
N O T E : These were the first anti-HIV drugs. They block reverse transcription (the creation of viral D N A from R N A ) by providing "decoy" building blocks that interrupt the process.
H A A R T regimens require that
individuals
adherence rates of 7 0 % to 8 0 % , which would
take multiple daily doses o f each medication
be considered favorable in other medical con-
in the prescribed combination. T h e regimens
ditions, have been associated with high rates
may involve as many as 2 0 or more pills, and
(over 7 5 % ) o f treatment failure (Montaner
each medication carries specific dose-spacing
et al., 1 9 9 8 ; Paterson, 1 9 9 9 , 2 0 0 0 ) . Successful
requirements. Depending on the c o m b i n a -
adherence is not only related to
tion, different medications may need to be
treatment outcome but also related to future
taken with food, without food, with water,
treatment outcome. With inconsistent or poor
or in temporal sequence relative to other
adherence, H I V infection can become "resis-
drugs in the c o m b i n a t i o n (Catz et al., 2 0 0 0 ) .
tant" to many current antiretrovirals,
Some
refrigeration.
subsequent treatment efforts often fail. Thus,
medications
require
current
and
Given these requirements, adherence can be a
patients may have a limited number o f combi-
formidable challenge for even the most con-
nation regimens to try. Furthermore, persons
scientious patient. T h e y present even greater
who develop drug-resistant H I V can transmit
barriers to patients w h o have concurrent
these strains to others during high-risk activi-
psychiatric illness or psychoactive substance
ties (Hecht et al., 1 9 9 8 ; Kelly, Otto-Salaj,
abuse/dependence or w h o simply need to
Sikkema, Pinkerton, &c Bloom,
maintain privacy about their illness in their
number o f barriers to adherence have been
social
environments.
described in the prevention literature. These
Because medication and adherence are such
barriers include (a) factors associated with the
and
occupational
1998). A
core issues for patients with ΗΓν illness,
medications themselves such as adverse side
health psychologists should become familiar
effects, large numbers of pills, dosing restric-
with various H A A R T medications (Tables
tions (frequency and
1 9 . 2 , 1 9 . 3 , and 1 9 . 4 ) .
ments), and medication containers that are too
food/water
require-
Treatment success typically requires strict
numerous or too large to carry and (b) factors
adherence rates (i.e., 9 0 % to 9 5 % ) , and
associated with stigma and confidentiality such
some research has shown that even partial
as being reminded o f one's HFV status, not
nonadherence to H A A R T greatly diminishes
wanting other people to know one's H I V
the
status, inability to take medications privately
benefits
o f treatment.
For
example,
390
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS Table 19.3
Non-nucleoside Reverse Transcriptase Inhibitors
Year
Generic
1996 1997 1998
Nevirapine Delavirdine Efavirenz
Name
Trade
Name
Viramune Rescriptor Sustiva
Also known as NVP, BI-RG-587 DLV EFV, DMP-266
N O T E : These also interrupt reverse transcription by binding to the reverse transcriptase enzyme and restricting its activity.
Table 19.4
Protease Inhibitors
Year
Generic
Name
1995 1996 1996 1997 1997 1999 2000
Saquinavir Ritonavir Indinavir Nelfinavir Saquinavir Amprenavir Lopinavir
Trade
Name
Invirase Norvir Crixivan Viracept Fortovase Agenerase Kaletra
Also known as SQV RTV IDV NFV SQV APV, 141W94 ABT-378/r
N O T E : Protease inhibitors block the action of protease, an enzyme that cuts H I V protein chains into specific proteins needed to assemble a new copy of the virus.
while at work or in public, and integrating
self-care, and illness progression. Sadness and
one's medication schedule into daily life (Catz
grief
et al., 2 0 0 0 ) . By considering these barriers,
responses to the multiple stressors and losses
psychologists can help patients to organize and
associated with HFV illness. However, when
are
considered
normal
emotional
manage their medication-taking routines, plan
depressed mood persists for more than 2
and problem solve how they will handle medi-
weeks and is accompanied by social with-
cation taking in the context o f their other life
drawal, lack of motivation/participation in
activities, use strategies to make complex regi-
usual activities, and additional physical and
mens easier to recall, and consult with their
cognitive symptoms, a serious depressive dis-
health care providers when questions about
order requiring professional care may be
regimens arise or when treatment side effects
present (Rabkin, 1 9 9 7 ) . Depression is the most
are encountered.
c o m m o n psychiatric disorder found
among
HIV-infected individuals (Elliot, 1 9 9 7 ) . CrossASSESSMENT AND T R E A T M E N T OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS IN HIV ILLNESS
both
H I V + and at-risk HEV populations estimate that the lifetime prevalence o f depressive disorders ranges from 2 0 % to 6 0 % . Current prevalence (6 to 1 2 months) ranges from 0 %
Depression Introduction
sectional and prospective studies in
to 1 8 % in HEV-positive populations and from and Epidemiology.
Depres-
sion has a significant effect on quality o f life,
0%
to 9 % in HIV-negative populations.
These rates are generally higher than estimates
HIV and AIDS in
community
samples
(Rabkin,
1997;
thoughts and emotions and their underlying assumptions. Group therapy has been used
M c D a n i e l & Blalock, 2 0 0 0 ) .
extensively with HIV-positive individuals in a Assessment/Diagnosis.
Diagnosing depres-
variety of contexts and is highly efficient. It
sion in the context o f H I V disease can be c o m -
provides
plicated because some diagnostic criteria, such
regarding misperceptions about the illness, and
psychoeducation,
confrontation
as loss of sexual desire, loss of appetite,
shared
insomnia, slowed movements, impaired con-
improve these individuals' mood and quality o f
experiences, all o f which help
to
centration, low energy, and fatigue, are also
life (Elliot, 1 9 9 7 ) .
caused by HlV-related illnesses, HlV-related pain, or the side effects o f H I V medications
Pharmacotherapy.
T h e approach to phar-
(Elliot, 1 9 9 7 ) . This diagnostic confounding is
macotherapy for HIV-positive individuals with
partially addressed by making a distinction
a major depressive disorder may be slightly
between the somatic symptoms o f depression
different from that for the general adult popu-
just listed and cognitive or affective symptoms
lation. HIV-positive individuals often respond
such as diminished ability to concentrate and
more sensitively to medications and may need
m a k e decisions, loss o f interest/pleasure, and
a "start low and go slow" approach. In addi-
feelings of worthlessness, guilt, hopelessness,
tion, individuals with advanced H I V infection
and helplessness. These symptoms, occurring
are often on multiple medications, thereby
together, are more clearly indexes o f depres-
increasing the probability o f drug-drug inter-
sive disorder and cannot be accounted for by
actions. Also, side effects may occur differently,
the presence o f medical conditions
with some being helpful and some aggravating
alone
(Rabkin, 1 9 9 7 ) .
current symptoms o f H I V infection itself or HIV-related illnesses (Rabkin, 1 9 9 7 ) . In genPsychotherapy or counsel-
eral, the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors
ing with HIV-positive individuals has been
(SSRIs) (e.g., fluoxetine, paroxetine, sertraline)
Psychotherapy.
approached from several models, including
are more tolerable with fewer side effects and
cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT), supportive
so may lead to an increased overall effective-
psychotherapy, and group therapy. C o m m o n
ness. Testosterone replacement
themes for clients during therapy are loss o f
shown to improve depressive symptoms in with
low
has
testosterone
been
relationships and autonomy, vocational issues,
individuals
physical health and appearance, spirituality, and
especially for those with decreased libido, sexual
levels,
stigma/discrimination. Supportive psychother-
dysfunction, and diminished energy. In addi-
apy examines mood changes in response to
tion, stimulants have been shown to improve
disease-related stressors and role changes.
mood, energy, and alertness and to be effective
Typically, the therapist and patient work on
in medically ill populations
exploring sources of sadness/grief,
McDaniel &
building
social support, and increasing self-care. In contrast, C B T focuses on reality testing
and
(Elliot, 1 9 9 7 ;
Blalock, 2 0 0 0 ) . Psychologists
should be aware o f the potential interaction effects o f some H A A R T medications
and
restructuring distorted, pessimistic, or unrealis-
some H I V medications. Ritonavir, indinavir,
tic thinking patterns that perpetuate a depressive
saquinavir, and nelfinavir all are metabolized
outlook or attributional style. T h e therapist is
in the same hepatic enzyme system as are many
more directive, helping the patient with setting
psychotropic drugs. Consequently, the simul-
goals, defining target symptoms, problem solv-
taneous use o f both classes o f drugs may alter
ing, and investigating relationships
the clearance rate o f one or the other. T h e
between
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS
392
interaction effect may alter the serum levels of
may be directly caused by viral involvement in
either kind o f drug, thereby increasing the risk
the central nervous system, the role o f the
of side effects or potentially decreasing the
AIDS virus in the etiology o f anxiety disorders
therapeutic effect (McDaniel & Blalock, 2 0 0 0 ;
is unclear and remains under investigation
Rabkin, 1 9 9 7 ) .
(Sciolla, Atkinson, 8c Grant, 1 9 9 8 ) . In patients with HEV/AIDS, anxiety symptoms may occur in several HFV-related medical conditions and
Anxiety
with psychoactive substance intoxication or
Introduction
and Epidemiology.
Anxiety
withdrawal (American Psychiatric Association,
disorders may occur at any time during the
1 9 9 8 ) . Similarly, anxiety may also be a mani-
course of H I V illness, particularly at pivotal
festation of side effects from H I V medication
points in disease progression, and are often
regimens, psychotropic medication, or other
considered a normal psychological response
pharmacological agents used to treat HEV-
to stress. In general, most patients respond
related medical problems (American Psychiatric
adequately to the stress of living with H I V and
Association, 1 9 9 8 ) .
limit the impact o f disease-related anxiety on their daily functioning and quality o f life. For
Psychological treatment o f
Psychotherapy.
some patients, however, the severity and dura-
anxiety disorders usually involves a two-phase
tion o f anxiety can cause significant, or even
process: acute symptom reduction in the short
debilitating, impairment o f daily functioning.
term and development o f more adaptive cop-
As with other chronic medical conditions, the
ing skills in the long term. In general, C B T is
entire spectrum of D S M - I V (Diagnostic
particularly useful in the treatment o f anxiety
Statistical
Manual
edition)-defined
of Mental
Disorders,
anxiety disorders
and fourth
may
disorders
because it focuses on
distorted
be
cognitive schémas and maladaptive behaviors.
seen in patients with HIV/AIDS (American
Progressive muscle relaxation and breathing
Psychiatric Association, 1 9 9 4 ; Blalock
exercises are often used to treat the physiolog-
&
McDaniel, in press). Prevalence rates for anxi-
ical manifestations o f anxiety and
ety disorders among patients with H I V disease
impending panic attacks. For specific phobias
range from 5 % to 4 0 % (McDaniel & Blalock,
and situationally determined panic attacks,
2 0 0 0 ) . But despite this wide range in preva-
systematic desensitization is helpful and is
lence estimates, a pattern o f findings
frequently
has
used
prevent
in conjunction with
the
emerged over the past 5 years: T h e prevalence
relaxation techniques. For more generalized
of
anxiety or anxious personality styles, tech-
anxiety disorders
in
HIV-seropositive
patients is not significantly different from that
niques such as
in HIV-seronegative patients. However, life-
therapy
time rates among patients with HLV disease are
designed to identify
generally higher than rates found in the general
pessimistic, and self-defeating thought patterns
and
rational-emotive-behavioral
cognitive
restructuring
and refute
are
irrational,
population or community samples (Dew et al.,
and attributional styles (McDaniel & Blalock,
1997;
2 0 0 0 ) . W i t h all types o f anxiety disorders,
R a b k i n , Ferrando, J a c o b s b e r g ,
&
Fishman, 1 9 9 7 ; Sewell et al., 2 0 0 0 ) .
therapist modeling and therapist-client roleplaying are concrete learning experiences
Assessment/Diagnosis.
A successful treat-
that can be beneficial. M o s t o f the cognitive-
ment plan for anxiety disorders in HEV/AIDS
behavioral techniques can be
relies on a thorough assessment of the patient's
into individual psychotherapy sessions, short-
incorporated
presenting anxiety symptoms, preferred stress
term psychoeducational groups, or ongoing
coping style, and repertoire o f stress coping
interpersonal process therapy groups (Karasic
skills. Unlike mood disorders in H I V , which
& Dilley, 1 9 9 8 ) .
HIV and AIDS Unless contraindicated
Pharmacotherapy. by a history
of psychoactive
substance
393
et al., 1 9 9 8 ) . According to a national cohort study, the incidence rate o f HIV-associated
dependence, benzodiazepines are frequently
dementia over a 5-year period was approxi-
prescribed for short-term treatment o f acute
mately 7 % for those with a C D 4 count o f
anxiety symptoms. Ideally, they are
then
less than 1 0 0 , 3 % for those with a C D 4
learns
count between 1 0 1 and 2 0 0 , and 2 % or less
more psychologically or behaviorally based
for those with a C D 4 count o f more than
gradually tapered as the individual
coping strategies. Antidepressants, particu-
2 0 0 . T h u s , HIV-associated dementia is prin-
larly SSRIs, may also be quite
cipally a disease found in advanced H I V ill-
effective
(Blalock & M c D a n i e l , in press).
ness (Price, 1 9 9 8 ) . Assessment/Diagnosis.
Cognitive Disorders Introduction
and
As with any sus-
pected cognitive impairment, HIV
Epidemiology.
penetrates the blood-brain barrier early in
interview with
a collateral
a caregiver o r significant
other is a crucial part o f assessment. Cognitive
the course o f infection and can be found in
problems may be episodic or subtle and may
the cerebrospinal fluid in nearly half o f
escape detection by short cognitive screening
infected individuals before the development
or brief interviewing techniques. Thorough
of AIDS-defining illnesses. Early or subtle
neuropsychological testing can detect mild
impairment may manifest as HIV-associated
problems across the spectrum o f cognitive and
minor cognitive m o t o r disorder: mild deficits
psychomotor functions. T h e neuropsycholog-
o f attention, information processing speed,
ical evaluation also includes assessment o f
learning and
psychomotor
personality functioning to help identify other
skills. T h e problems may be quite mild such
psychiatric disorders and to distinguish the
as loss o f train o f thought, word-finding dif-
cognitive problems associated with
m e m o r y , and
mood
ficulties, short-term memory problems, and
disturbance and more disease-based cognitive
decreased efficiency in fine m o t o r speed and
problems (Grant & Martin, 1 9 9 4 ; M o o r e ,
dexterity. HIV-associated minor cognitive
van Gorp, Hinken, & Stern, 1 9 9 7 ) .
disorder may be complicated by the presence of depression or anxiety but is not caused by psychiatric problems (American Psychiatric
Psychotherapy.
Depending on the extent o f
cognitive impairment, traditional psychother-
Association, 1 9 9 8 ; G r a n t & M a r t i n , 1 9 9 4 ) .
apies may be o f limited use. For mild to mod-
In more advanced disease stages, neurocogni-
erate impairment, patients often find it helpful
tive impairment may be quite
prominent
to explore fear, anger, and sadness about loss
with HIV-associated dementia, a progressive
of previous level o f functioning. T h e degree o f
disorder that initially presents as
distress is frequently related to the real-life
apathy,
inertia, cognitive slowing, memory loss, and
impact o f the patient's cognitive impairment.
social withdrawal.
For some individuals, the changes interfere
As the dementia
pro-
gresses, multiple cognitive functions become
minimally with daily functioning and
are
increasingly impaired. T h e terminal phases
helped by simple strategies such as using
are characterized by global cognitive impair-
written reminders and simplifying sequential
ment,
psychomotor
tasks. For others, however, the changes may
retardation. Unlike minor cognitive disorder,
necessitate a réévaluation o f vocational/leisure
HIV-associated
abilities or even daily living and self-care skills.
mutism,
and
severe
dementia
rarely
develops
prior to constitutional problems and usually
Conjoint therapy (e.g., family, partner, care-
does not develop prior to other AIDS-defin-
giver) is usually indicated in patients with
ing illnesses (Grant &c M a r t i n , 1 9 9 4 ; Sciolla
HIV-associated cognitive disorders because
394
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS
CASE S T U D Y " R i t a " is a 44-year-old, bisexual Caucasian female with a 12-year history o f H I V disease w h o was referred for a crisis evaluation by her medical provider. T h r e e months prior to consultation, her viral load increased markedly from a previously undetectable level and her C D 4 count decreased from 2 5 0 t o 7 5 , prompting a change in her antiretroviral regimen from zidovudine, didanosine, and indinavir to stavudine, lamivudine, and efavirenz. T h r e e months later during a medical followup visit, Rita became acutely agitated when she learned that her C D 4 and viral load had remained unchanged despite the new regimen. She considered herself a "treatment failure" and was convinced that she would die. Rita described herself as a recovered alcoholic with 7 years o f sobriety. She also reported a long-standing history o f marijuana use and had continued to smoke daily over the past 2 years, finally quitting only recently because she could no longer afford to use marijuana. O n c e Rita was substance free, her underlying psychiatric symptoms emerged. She revealed a long history (preceding her H I V diagnosis) o f obsessive worrying, restlessness/irritability, sleep disturbance, and p o o r stress coping that was consistent with a diagnosis o f generalized anxiety disorder. She also reported a history o f sub-syndromal depressive symptoms and fear o f being open with others about her sexual identity. Although she was eventually able to abstain from alcohol, she continued to use marijuana as a way in which to diminish her chronic anxiety. Rita was finally diagnosed with generalized anxiety disorder after a stable drugfree period, with her differential diagnostic picture also including major depressive episode, depression/anxiety secondary to a general medical condition, and substance/medication-induced depression/anxiety. T h e long-term treatment plan for her anxiety included a combination o f medication and cognitive-behavioral psychotherapy. Benzodiazepines were contraindicated given her history o f substance dependence. Instead, she was prescribed the antidepressant mirtazepine ( 1 5 milligrams at bedtime) to alleviate her anxious/depressive symptoms. Her weekly psychotherapy focused on preventing substance use relapse and strengthening problem-solving and stress-coping skills. During therapy, Rita made progress in several important areas that improved her overall psychological functioning, including disclosing her HEV status and sexual orientation to family members, applying for disability income benefits, and finding a suitable housemate to reduce living expenses. Rita's case illustrates three important issues for treating anxiety disorders in H I V clinical care. First, anxiety disorders or anxious character styles may precede infection and increase vulnerability to problems with chronic illness coping. Second, psychoactive substances are frequently part o f a c o m p l e x history and symptom picture and require careful consideration in assessment and treatment. Third, a thorough differential diagnosis, including medical and medication-induced causes, must be considered when assessing and treating anxiety-related symptoms.
HIV and AIDS these patients may require a range of assistance
CONCLUSION
from others. This becomes particularly crucial for medication adherence due to forgetfulness or confusion.
Although patients
health with
psychologists may
H I V disease
treat
independently,
Pharmacological treatment
health psychologists are typically part o f a
strategies for cognitive disorders can be divided
multidisciplinary treatment team composed
Pharmacotherapy.
therapies,
of infectious disease specialists, psychiatrists,
(b) therapies aimed at immunological measures
nurses, and social service workers. An inte-
or inflammatory mediators, (c) therapies aimed
grated knowledge base helps to bridge exist-
at bolstering the response of the brain to the
ing gaps
onslaught of the infection (e.g., neurotransmitter
health, and it positions health psychologists
manipulation), and
to be uniquely skilled professionals in the
into four types: (a) antiretroviral
(d) nutritional
therapies
(American Psychiatric Association, 1998).
between
medicine and
mental
H I V health care arena.
REFERENCES American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychiatric Association. (1998). Practice guidelines for the treatment of patients with HIV/AIDS. Washington, DC: Author. Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M . (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Blalock, A. C , & McDaniel, S. J . (in press). Anxiety disorders in HIV infection and AIDS. In A. Beckett, K. Citron, & M. J . Brouillette (Eds.), HIV and psychiatry. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. Carpenter, C , Fischl, M., Hammer, S., Hirsch, M., Jacobsen, D., Ketzenstein, D., Montaner, J . , Richman, D., Saag, M., Schooley, R., Thomson, M., Vella, S., Yeni, D., & Volberding, D. A. (1998). Antiretroviral therapy for HIV infection in 1 9 9 8 : Updated recommendations of the International AIDS Society, U.S. Panel. Journal of the American Medical Association, 280, 7 8 - 8 6 . Catz, S. L., Kelly, J . , Bogart, L., Benotsch, E., & McAuliffe, T. (2000). Patterns, correlates, and barriers to medication adherence among persons prescribed new treatments for HIV disease. Health Psychology, 19, 1 2 4 - 1 3 3 . Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2001a). A glance at the HIV epidemic (HIV/AIDS Update). Atlanta, GA: Author. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2001b). HIV/AIDS Prevention Research Synthesis Project: Compendium of HIV prevention interventions with evidence of effectiveness. Atlanta, GA: Author. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2001c, February). HIV/AIDS surveillance report midyear 2001 edition (Vol. 13, No. 1). Atlanta, GA: Author. Cohen, P. T. (1998). Clinical overview of HIV disease. HIV Insite Knowledge Base Chapter. (San Francisco: University of California, San Francisco) Dew, Μ. Α., Becker, J . T., Sanchez, J . , Caldararo, R., Lopez, O. L., Wess, J . , Dorst, S. K., & Banks, G. (1997). Prevalence and predictors of depressive, anxiety, and substance use disorders in HIV-infected and uninfected men: A longitudinal evaluation. Psychological Medicine, 27, 3 9 5 - 4 0 9 . Elliot, A. (1997, May). Depression and HIV: Assessment and treatment. San Francisco: Project Inform.
395
396
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS Fisher, J . D., 8c Fisher, W. A. (1997). Changing AIDS-risk behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 4 5 5 - 4 7 4 . Grant, I., 8c Martin, A. (Eds.). (1994). Neuropsychology of HIV infection. New York: Oxford University Press. Hecht, F. M., Grant, R. M., Detropolus, C. J., Dillon, B., Chesney, Μ. Α., Tian, W., Hellman, N. S., Brandapalli, N. L, Diglio, L., Bronson, B., & Kahn, J . O. (1998). Sexual transmission of an HIV-1 variant resistant to multiple reverse-transcriptase and protease inhibitors. New England Journal of Medicine, 339, 3 0 7 - 3 1 1 . Kalichman, S. (1998). Understanding AIDS: Advances in research and treatment (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Kalichman, S. C , Sc Hospers, H. J . (1997). Efficacy of behavioral-skills enhancement: HIV risk-reduction interventions in community settings. AIDS, 11 (Suppl. A), S 1 9 1 - S 1 9 9 . Karasic, D. H., Sc Dilley, J . W. (1998). Anxiety and depression: Mood and HIV disease. In J . W. Dilley 8c R. Marks (Eds.), The USCF AIDS Health Project guide to counseling (pp. 2 2 7 - 2 4 8 ) . San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Kelly, J . Α., Otto-Salaj, L. L., Sikkema, K. J . , Pinkerton, S. D., 8c Bloom, F. R. (1998). Implications of HIV treatment advances for behavioral research on AIDS: Protease inhibitors and new challenges in HIV secondary prevention. Health Psychology, 17, 3 1 0 - 3 1 9 . McDaniel, S. J . , 8c Blalock, A. C. (2000). Diagnosis and management of HIVrelated mood and anxiety disorders. In New directions in psychiatric services (No. 87, pp. 5 1 - 5 6 ) . San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Montaner, J . S. G., Reiss, P., Cooper, D., Vella, S., Harris, M., Conway, B . , Weinberg, Μ. Α., Smith, D., Robinson, P., Hall, D., Myers, M., 8c Lange, J . M . Α., for the INCAS Study Group. (1998). A randomized, double-blind trial comparing combinations of nevirapine, didanosine, and zidovudine for HIVinfected patients. Journal of the American Medical Association, 279, 9 3 0 - 9 3 7 . Moore, L., van Gorp, W., Hinken, C , 8c Stern, M. (1997). Subjective complaints versus actual cognitive deficits in predominantly symptomatic HIV-1 seropositive individuals. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 9(1), 3 7 - 4 4 . Paterson, D., Swindells, S., Mohr, J., Brester, M., Vergis, R., Squier, C , Wagener, M., 8c Singh, M . (1999, January). How much adherence is enough? A prospective study of adherence to protease inhibitor therapy using MEMS caps. Paper presented at the Sixth Conference on Retroviruses and Opportunistic Infections, Chicago. Paterson, D., Swindells, S., Mohr, J . , Brester, M., Vergis, R., Squier, C , Wagener, M., 8c Singh, M . (2000). Adherence to protease inhibitor therapy and outcomes in patients with HIV infection. Annals of Internal Medicine, 133, 2 1 - 3 0 . Price, R. (1998). AIDS dementia complex. HIV Insite Knowledge Base Chapter. (San Francisco: University of California, San Francisco) Rabkin, J . (1997, October). Meeting the challenge of depression in HIV. GMHC Treatment Issues, 11(10). (New York: Gay Men's Health Crisis) Rabkin, J . G., Ferrando, S. J . , Jacobsberg, L. B., Sc Fishman, B. (1997). Prevalence of Axis I disorders in an AIDS cohort: A cross-sectional, controlled study. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 38(3), 1 4 6 - 1 5 4 . Sciolla, Α., Atkinson, J . , 8c Grant, I. (1998). Neuropsychiatrie features of HIV disease. In W. G. van Gorp 8c S. L. Buckingham (Eds.), Practitioner's guide to the neuropsychiatry of HIV/AIDS (pp. 1 0 6 - 2 0 0 ) . New York: Guilford. Sewell, M. C , Goggin, K. J . , Rabkin, J . G., Ferrando, S. J . , McElhiney, M . C , 8c Evans, S. (2000). Anxiety syndromes and symptoms among men with AIDS: A longitudinal controlled study. Psychosomatics, 41, 2 9 4 - 3 0 0 .
CHAPTER
Irritable Bowel
10 20
Syndrome
JEFFREY M . LACKNER
I
rritable bowel syndrome (IBS) is a chronic
individuals in the United States and Canada
gastrointestinal (GI) disorder character-
experience symptoms
consistent with IBS
symptoms,
(Drossman et al., 1 9 9 3 ) . In the United States,
including abdominal pain/discomfort associ-
approximately 2 0 to 4 0 million individuals—1
ated with altered bowel function (e.g., diar-
in 6 Americans—suffer from IBS (Lynn &
rhea, constipation) that occurs in the absence
Friedman, 1 9 9 3 ) . These figures make IBS not
of organic disease. Because the locus o f the
only one o f the most prevalent chronic pain
ized by a constellation o f
problem is in h o w the gut functions and not in
disorders (Crombie, Croft, Linton, LeResche, &
abnormalities o f its physical structure, IBS is
V o n Korff, 1 9 9 9 ) but also one of the most
considered a functional disorder. There are 2 5
prevalent chronic illnesses in general. T h e
functional G I disorders concentrated in one
prevalence rate o f IBS is at least as common as,
of five anatomic regions: esophagus, gastro-
if not more common than, that of hypertension
duodenal, biliary, intestines, and anorectum
and is much more common than the prevalence
(Drossman, 1 9 9 4 ) . Other functional G I disor-
rates o f asthma, diabetes, and congestive heart
ders include functional dyspepsia, functional
failure (Adams & Benson, 1 9 9 1 ) . Although the
constipation, and chronic functional abdomi-
great majority ( 8 0 % to 9 0 % ) of IBS patients
nal pain. O f the functional G I disorders, IBS is
do not seek medical attention, the 1 0 % to 2 0 %
the most prevalent, costly, and disabling.
who do consult physicians represent 2 8 % o f all visits to GI practices and 1 2 % o f all primary care visits, making IBS one of the most com-
BACKGROUND AND ETIOLOGY
Epidemiology
mon disorders seen by physicians, according to Scott-Levin's Physician
Drug
and
Diagnosis
Audit, a commercial database derived from a monthly survey o f physicians in active office-
Epidemiological studies conducted over the
based practice in the United States. Factors that
past two decades indicate that 1 0 % to 2 0 % of
differentiate treatment-seeking individuals from
A U T H O R ' S N O T E : Preparation of this chapter was supported in part by National Institutes of Health Grant D K - 5 4 2 1 1 .
397
398
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS non-treatment-seeking individuals include the
stools, a sense o f urgency (a strong sensation
severity of abdominal pain, psychological dis-
of the immediate need to move the bowels), a
tress, and sociocultural influences. T o illustrate,
feeling o f incomplete evacuation, flatulence,
IBS is more common among males in rural pop-
and abdominal bloating. Although bloating is
ulations in India and Sri Lanka, where gender
not a diagnostic feature of IBS, it is a particu-
identity strongly influences help-seeking behav-
larly bothersome clinical feature that warrants
ior (Jain, Gupta, Jajoo, & Sidwa, 1 9 9 1 ) . O n the
attention
( M a x t o n , M o r r i s , Sc Whorwell,
other hand, in the United States, where females
1 9 8 9 ) . T h e duration of IBS symptoms is rela-
outnumber males in medical care clinics that
tively short, with the longest set o f symp-
treat IBS, IBS is four times more common
toms—pain/discomfort and bloating—lasting
among females than among males. The finan-
for 5-day periods (Hahn et al., 1 9 9 9 ) . For pur-
cial costs on the U.S. health care system were
pose o f treatment planning, patients are often
estimated in 1 9 9 5 to have been $ 8 billion annu-
classified on the basis o f the relative predomi-
ally (Talley, Gabriel, Harmsen, Zinsmeister, 8 t
nance o f their bowel symptoms
Evans, 1 9 9 5 ) . T h e course of IBS varies greatly
constipation, or pain) at the time of diagnosis.
among people, but episodes can have serious
Diarrhea-prominent
debilitating effects on physical function. IBS
more
has been identified as second only to the
day, loose or watery stools, and
than
three
(diarrhea,
IBS patients experience bowel
movements
per
urgency.
common cold as a cause o f work absenteeism
Constipation-predominant
(Drossman et al., 1 9 9 3 ) . It has been reported
the other hand, are typically characterized by
IBS patients,
on
that approximately 3 0 % of IBS sufferers take
having two or more o f the following symp-
sick leave because o f the disorder, with half of
toms: fewer than three bowel movements per
these individuals being absent from work at
week; hard, pellet-shaped, or lumpy stools;
least 2 weeks per year. IBS affects quality of life
and straining during bowel movements. T h e
(QOL) as much as, or more than, does conges-
predominant classification system is particu-
tive heart failure (Whitehead, Burnett, Cook, Sc
larly useful for patients with one consistent
Taub, 1996).
predominant
symptom.
However,
for
the
majority o f patients, symptoms fluctuate over time, with the predominant symptom alter-
Clinical Features
nating among diarrhea, constipation, and nor-
Although IBS is a multisymptom problem, patients characterize abdominal pain or discomfort as their most frequent and bothersome complaint (Drossman, Whitehead, Sc Camilleri, 1 9 9 7 ; Hahn,
Y a n , Sc Strassels,
1 9 9 9 ) . For this reason, abdominal pain is con-
mal bowel function. Thus, the predominant classification system functions best as a heuristic that helps to conceptualize patients at a single point in time rather than as an empirically grounded
classification scheme
that
reflects the clinical realities of IBS over time.
sidered the cardinal feature of IBS. Pain asso-
O n e factor that complicates accurate diag-
ciated with IBS is experienced as a diffuse
nosis is the comorbidity with and/or temporal
crampy, colicky, and aching sensation concen-
contiguity
trated diffusely (as opposed to localized) in the
Harman, Kaye, Sc Whorwell, 1 9 8 3 ) . There is
o f organic
GI
disease
(Isgar,
abdomen that is often relieved with defecation
increasing evidence that a sizable, albeit minor-
or flatulence. Pain is sometimes associated
ity, group of patients develop IBS symptoms
with defecation and a change in stool fre-
following infectious enteritis (Rodriguez
quency or form (appearance). In addition to
Ruigozmez, 1 9 9 9 ) . Although the hypothesis
changes in bowel habits, IBS pain can be asso-
that acute bacterial infection is causally related
ciated with mucus found around or within the
to IBS awaits confirmation in a controlled
&
Irritable
Bowel Syndrome
\
investigation, 2 0 % to 3 0 % o f patients develop
the apparent age difference may at least partly
IBS after bouts o f acute bacterial infection
reflect the diagnostic practices used to establish
(McKendrick, 1 9 9 6 ; McKendrick 8c Read,
prevalence data (Talley, Gabriel, et al., 1 9 9 5 ) .
1 9 9 4 ) . W h a t is particularly interesting is that
Some researchers argue that correcting for this
individuals who develop IBS after a bout o f
difference would bring the prevalence rate of
gastroenteritis had higher levels of psychologi-
IBS among the elderly in line with the preva-
cal distress at the time of infection than did
lence
individuals who did not develop IBS after bac-
(O'Keefe, Talley, Zinsmeister, 8c Jacobsen,
rates
among
younger
age
groups
terial infection (Gwee, 2 0 0 1 ) . A significant
1 9 9 5 ) . W h a t information is available indicates
o f IBS patients report upper GI
that more than 1 5 % of individuals age 6 5
number
symptoms,
including
reflux,
vomiting,
years or over report IBS-like symptoms (e.g.,
nausea, noncardiac chest pain, and dyspepsia.
pain, constipation) that
Approximately 4 0 % of individuals with IBS
"painful diverticular disease" (O'Keefe et al.,
are classified as
have reflux symptoms and 4 5 % have dyspep-
1 9 9 5 ) . O n the other end o f the age spectrum,
sia. Patients' symptom patterns can transition
the occurrence of IBS in children is similar to
between disorders such that they have symp-
the rate in adults ( 5 % vs. 2 0 % ) . Symptoms
toms of one disorder (e.g., IBS) that in turn are
consistent with a diagnosis o f IBS have been
replaced by symptoms o f another (e.g., reflux).
reported in 6 % of 1 2 - and 13-year-olds and in
There is a higher prevalence o f IBS in patients
1 4 % o f 1 5 - and 16-year-olds (Hyams, Burke,
whose Crohn's disease and ulcerative colitis—
Davis, Rzepski, 8c Andrulonis,
collectively known as inflammatory
bowel
noted previously, there is a strong gender dis-
disease (IBD)—are in remission. In comparison
parity in prevalence of IBS. Community sur-
with
veys indicate that women outnumber
controls, IBS patients
report
more
1 9 9 6 ) . As
men
extraintestinal somatic symptoms, including
with IBS by a two-to-one margin. In clinical
sexual dysfunction, insomnia, fibromyalgia,
settings, the gender difference is even more dra-
facial pain, chronic pelvic pain, and chronic
matic (Drossman et al., 1 9 9 3 ) . Patients with
fatigue (Whitehead, Palsson, Sc Jones, 2 0 0 2 ) .
higher education, high income levels, and a high severity o f pain and stress are more likely to seek medical treatment for IBS. With respect
Natural History
to racial differences, the rate o f IBS is lower in
IBS is most commonly seen in patients during late adolescence and middle age. M o r e than 5 0 % o f patients are diagnosed with IBS before age 3 5 years, whereas another 4 0 % are diagnosed between ages 3 5 and 5 0 years (Dalton
8c
Drossman,
1997).
After
age
6 5 years, the incidence and onset o f IBS decreases among males and females (Maxwell, Mendall, 8c Kumar, 1 9 9 7 ; Sandler, Jordan, 8c Shelton, 1 9 9 0 ) . For these reasons, IBS has been characterized as a "young person's illness." It is unclear whether age differences reflect developmental issues (e.g., age-related differences in gut physiology), differences in health-seeking behaviors, or an alternative factor(s). Data based on epidemiological studies suggest that
Hispanic
samples
than
in
both
African
American and Caucasian samples, with the latter two groups having comparable prevalence rates
(e.g.,
Fasanmade,
Olubuyide, 1995).
Olawuyi,
Aggravating
8c
factors
include psychological stress, dietary triggers, meal consumption, and (for some females) hormonal changes around menses. T h e longterm prognosis o f IBS is favorable, with symptoms generally improving over time. Positive prognostic factors include gender (i.e., male), quality of physician-patient relationship (i.e., patients whose physicians maintain a positive sympathetic relationship with them, address patient concerns and expectations, set limits, and involve the patients in treatment decisions
399
400
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS achieve better outcomes and require fewer
criteria that tend to covary in patients with IBS,
bowel
R o m e Π includes a set o f symptoms with a
habit involving constipation or painless diar-
greater than 2 5 % occurrence that cumulatively
rhea. There is a strong family history o f IBS-
support the diagnosis but are not essential
like symptoms in first-degree relatives but not
for diagnosis. These symptoms consist o f
follow-up
visits), and predominant
spousal relatives o f IBS patients (Morris-Yates,
(a) abnormal stool frequency (may be defined
Talley, Boyce, & Andrews, 1 9 9 5 ) . Although
as more than three bowel movements per day
these data have been interpreted as suggestive
or less than three bowel movements
evidence o f a genetic component that con-
week), (b) abnormal stool form (lumpy/hard or
per
tributes to the development o f IBS, an alterna-
loose/watery), (c) abnormal
tive hypothesis that has not been ruled out is
(straining, urgency, or feeling o f incomplete
that
evacuation; passage of mucus), and (d) bloating
these familial patterns
are
acquired
through learning processes (e.g., parental modeling o f sick behavior).
stool passage
or feeling o f abdominal distension. Confirmation o f an IBS diagnosis using R o m e II criteria is conducted within the context of a thorough medical examination, which
Diagnosis
includes a comprehensive medical history and
The nonspecific nature o f IBS symptoms complicates
accurate
diagnosis.
Drossman
physical examination and the identification o f the predominant symptom. There is a consen-
(1994) identified 3 1 disorders across 13 classes
sus among IBS experts that because R o m e Π
of medical diseases whose symptoms are descrip-
offers a set of "positive" (i.e., symptom-based)
tively similar to IBS symptoms (Table 2 0 . 1 ) .
diagnostic criteria, a strategy that relies on
GI diseases whose symptoms overlap with
excessive diagnostic tests to rule out likely
IBS symptoms include colorectal cancer, I B D ,
organic disease is not only unnecessary but also
endocrine disorders and tumors, enteric infec-
inefficient and potentially harmful. For this
tions, and malabsorption syndrome. Because
reason, diagnostic testing to confirm IBS may
IBS symptoms mimic these and other diseases
be limited to a complete blood count, determi-
with detectable physical pathology,
proper
nation of erythrocyte sedimentation, and a
process:
colonoscopy for patients who are age 5 0 years
(a) excluding possible organic, infectious,
or over and report sudden onset of symptoms
diagnosis
o f IBS is a
two-part
may
and/or those with a family history of colon
account for symptoms and (b) assessing the
polyps or GI cancer (Thompson, Dotevall,
presence of IBS symptoms in accord with diag-
Drossman, Heaton, & Kruis, 1 9 8 9 ) . Because
nostic criteria known as the R o m e Π criteria
symptoms o f IBS mimic those of organic dis-
metabolic, or structural
diseases that
(Drossman, Corazziari, Talley, Thompson, &
ease, it is not prudent to diagnose IBS on the
Whitehead, 2 0 0 0 ) . R o m e Π criteria represent
basis of symptoms per se without the benefit of
the current standard for diagnosing IBS. T o
a sufficiently thorough medical examination.
meet a diagnosis for IBS, R o m e II criteria
Signs and symptoms that are not typically asso-
require that, during the preceding 1 2 months,
ciated with IBS and may signify alternative or
the patient must experience 1 2 weeks (which
coexisting GI disease include visible or occult
need not be consecutive) of abdominal pain or
blood in stool, weight loss, fever, laboratory
discomfort with two of the following three fea-
indicators o f inflammation, frequent nocturnal
tures: (a) relieved with defecation, (b) onset
symptoms, abrupt (i.e., nongradual) onset o f
associated with a change in frequency o f stool,
symptoms, and onset of symptoms in patients
and (c) onset associated with a change in form
who are age 4 0 years or over. Patients with
(appearance) of stool. In addition to delineating
these symptoms or signs may require a more
Irritable Table 2 0 . 1 Physical
Bowel
Syndrome
Differential Diagnoses of Chronic/Recurrent Bowel Dysfunction
Condition
Example
Irritable bowel syndrome Lactase deficiency Drugs
Laxative/cathartics/Mg antacids, diuretics, cholinergic agents, prostaglandins (e.g., misoprostil)
Bacterial infection
Salmonella species, Campylobacter jejuni, enterocolitica, Clostridium difficile
Parasitic infection
Giardia lamblia,
2+
Entameba
histoltica,
Yersinia
cryptosporidiosis
Inflammatory bowel disease
Crohn's disease, ulcerative colitis
Malabsorption
Chronic pancreatitis, celiac sprue, postgastrectomy syndromes
Metabolic disorders
Diabetes mellitus, thyrotoxicosis
Endocrine/ hormone-producing tumors
Gastrinoma, carcinoid, VIPoma, endometriosis
Psychiatric disorders
Depression, anxiety/panic disorders, somatization disorders
Intestinal pseudo-obstruction
Primary visceral myopathy/neuropathy (e.g., scleroderma, diabetes)
Other colonic diseases
Collagenous/lymphocytic colitis, mast-cell disease, villous adenoma
Opportunistic infections in immunocompromised hosts SOURCE: Adapted from Drossman (1994). Reprinted with permission.
extensive medical evaluation to rule out organic
from research showing that types of IBS
disease.
patients
exhibit certain disordered
motor
abnormalities throughout the G I tract, particularly in the small and large intestine. IBS
Pathophysiology
patients whose predominant bowel habit is biomedical
diarrhea may have accelerated whole gut tran-
approach has historically sought to explain IBS
sit times, a greater number o f fast contractions,
Altered
Gut
Motility.
The
symptoms in light o f underlying physiological
and more high-amplitude propagated contrac-
mechanisms within the G I tract. T h e most
tions in the colon as compared with normal
prominent biomedical theory has conceptual-
individuals (Whitehead, Engel, & Schuster,
ized IBS as a disorder of colonic motility
1 9 9 0 ) . Some patients whose
predominant
caused by alterations in the smooth muscle
bowel habit is constipation, on the other hand,
spasms o f the GI tract. T h e nature o f abnormal
may have delays in colonic transit and fewer
patterns o f motility and peristalsis presumably
high-amplitude propagated contractions than
determine the type o f bowel disturbance, with
is the case in normal individuals (Bazzacchi
hypermotility resulting in diarrhea and hypo-
et al., 1 9 9 0 ) . That being said, dysmotility may
motility resulting in constipation. According to
be more appropriate as a mechanistic explana-
the "dysmotility hypothesis," vigorous con-
tion for specific subtypes of IBS than as a
tractions of the colon aggravate pain. Findings
general rule for understanding the pathophysi-
consistent with the notion that IBS symptoms
ology o f IBS across patients. In general, how-
involve disruptions in colonic motility come
ever, clear-cut differences in colonic motor
402
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS responses between IBS patients and normal
balloon is connected to a computerized pump
controls have not been consistently replicated.
(barostat) that inflates the balloon to specific
Although IBS patients exhibit heightened
pressure levels designed to be mildly moderately
reactivity to a variety o f stimuli that stimulate
uncomfortable for a short time to simulate
colonic motility (e.g., meals, rectal distension,
abdominal discomfort. These studies
strong emotions, cholinergic drugs, injection of
demonstrated that a significant proportion o f
have
cholecystokinin fa hormone released in the
IBS patients, particularly those with diarrhea-
duodenum]), healthy controls exhibit a similar
predominant IBS, experience abdominal pain at
albeit less exaggerated motility response pat-
significantly lower levels of pressure and intra-
8c Tulin, 1949). In other
luminal volume than do controls when their
words, IBS patients show a quantitative differ-
rectums, sigmoids, or small intestines are dis-
ence but not a qualitative difference in motor
tended by a balloon (i.e., visceral hyperalgesia).
tern (Almy, Kern,
response as compared with normal individu-
They also experience distensions at a given
als. T h e lack of clear-cut clinical differences has
volume as more intense than do healthy con-
prompted researchers to study motor activity
trols (Mertz, Naliboff, Munakata, Niazi, 8c
in the small bowel. This line of research has
Mayer,
1995). IBS patients may also experience
focused on discrete clustered contractions
discomfort in the GI tract in response to normal
(DCCs), that is, abnormal bursts o f motor
gut stimuli (e.g., gas, stool) that do not typically
activity in the duodenum and ileum. D C C s are
elicit discomfort in non-IBS patients. In IBS
seen more often in IBS patients, but they are
patients, the phenomenon of visceral sensitivity
also exhibited in both healthy controls and
appears to be specific to the GI tract and does
patients with other GI disorders (e.g., pseudo-
not characterize thresholds for somatic pain
obstruction, intestinal obstruction). In other
stimuli (e.g., cold pressor test) that range from
words, D C C s are nonspecific. Perhaps most
normal to above average as compared with
problematic is the relatively weak correspon-
controls (Cook, V a n Eeeden, & Collins,
dence between D C C s and symptoms. D C C s
Visceral hypersensitivity is neither limited to the
correlate with pain in IBS patients only 25% of
colon nor restricted to pain sensation. Visceral
the time (Kellow & Phillips,
1987).
1987). Thus,
hyperalgesia has been demonstrated at different
abnormal motility may well enhance gut reac-
sites in the GI tract (e.g., stomach, colon, small
tivity in some patients, but its role is non-
intestine, esophagus). Beyond pain, visceral
specific and has limited clinical significance
hypersensitivity is reflected in IBS patients'
and explanatory value.
complaints of excess gas, even though they actually show no differences in their composi-
Visceral
Sensitivity and Perception.
Because
of the inconsistent association between the pain
tion or accumulation rate as compared with asymptomatic controls.
and measured colonic motility, there has been
Several peripheral and central neural mech-
an increased focus on the phenomenon of vis-
anisms have been proposed to explain visceral
ceral hypersensitivity, which refers to a state of
sensitivity. In peripheral sensitization, noxious
heightened awareness of and sensitivity to nor-
stimulation (e.g., acute enteric infection, injury
mal intestinal activity (e.g., gas, normal intesti-
to the viscera) reduces the threshold
nal contractions) that arises within the gut
mechanical stimuli (e.g., pinching, cutting,
for
during digestion as well as painful distension o f
stretching) in high-threshold nociceptors and
the colon. Visceral hypersensitivity research has
may recruit "silent" nociceptors that are nor-
been based largely on a series o f balloon disten-
mally unresponsive to stimuli and become
sion studies that involve placing a balloon
activated only in the presence o f inflammation
catheter in the lower large intestine (colon). T h e
(Cervero & Laird,
1999). T h e sensitization of
Irritable these nociceptors may in turn
Bowel Syndrome
\
contribute
system function differently in IBS patients, as
to pain in response to normally innocuous
compared with controls, in response to visceral
stimuli (e.g., colon contractions) that are below
pain. IBS patients, in contrast to controls, fail
the normal perceptual threshold (Cervero &
to activate the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC)
Laird,
on rectal distension (Naliboff et al., 2 0 0 1 ;
1999).
An
alternative—but
not
mutually exclusive—mechanism emphasizes
Silverman et al., 1 9 9 7 ) . Data using f M R I , on
increased excitability of the GI neural system
the other hand, indicate that painful stimuli
(i.e., central sensitization). In central sensitiza-
cause significantly greater activation of the
tion, noxious stimulation may cause structural
A C C in IBS patients than in controls (Mertz
and functional reorganization o f synaptic con-
et al., 2 0 0 0 ) . Notwithstanding disparate find-
nections in dorsal horn neurons, inducing a
ings of P E T and f M R I studies, both lines o f
hypersensitive state that Carr ( 1 9 9 6 ) aptly
research implicate abnormalities in A C C activ-
likened to a "posttraumatic stress phenomenon
ity in visceral pain perception. T h e A C C is a site
in the spinal cord . . . that persists long after
of opiate down-regulation of pain that may,
such stimuli cease"
when activated, inhibit sensory input that con-
(p. 1 1 1 4 ) .
Long-term
changes in the nervous system associated with
tributes to pain production. Based on P E T data,
central sensitization are reflected by "wind-
IBS patients not only fail to activate the A C C in
up" amplification o f pain (i.e., progressively
response to visceral pain but also demonstrate
increasing activity in dorsal
activation of the left prefrontal cortex, whereas
horn
cells),
reduced pain threshold, expansion o f recep-
no such response occurs in normal individuals.
tion fields o f spinal cord dorsal horn neurons,
The prefrontal cortex is a brain structure asso-
and persistence o f pain in the absence o f
ciated with the emotional feelings (e.g., anxiety)
input from the periphery
(Coderre, Katz,
directed partly toward long-term implications
Vaccarino, & Metzack, 1 9 9 3 ) . In effect, pain
of having pain (e.g., "suffering") in relation to
may persist because an ongoing barrage of pain
or in anticipation o f pain. Taken together, P E T
signal imprints in the central nervous system
data suggest that IBS patients may paradoxi-
(CNS) a "memory" of pain whose conscious
cally respond to visceral pain stimuli by activat-
perception no longer requires input from the
ing a brain area
periphery (e.g., inflammation). Neuroplastic
amplifies pain perception rather than a brain
(prefrontal cortex) that
changes may explain why IBS patients, particu-
area (ACC) with important pain modulatory
larly those with more severe symptoms, experi-
properties.
ence an amplification or exaggeration o f pain to a stimulus that is normally painful (e.g., rectal stimulation) and also report pain from a stimulus that does not normally provoke pain (e.g., gas, stool, small meals).
Although the concept o f visceral hypersensitivity addresses some of the limitations of the dysmotility hypothesis, it cannot be definitively characterized as an etiological mechanism for a variety o f reasons. First, the phenomenon characterizes only a subset of IBS patients.
CNS Modulation
of Visceral
Sensations.
A
I B S patients
exhibit
related theory that has grown out of visceral
Diarrhea-prominent
stronger rectal urgency at lower
balloon
sensitivity research suggests that hypersensitiv-
volumes and exhibit greater sensitivity to rectal
ity is due to abnormal central processing
distension than do constipation-predominant
defects. This line of research comes partly from
IBS patients
neuroimaging studies using both position emis-
1 9 9 0 b ) . Furthermore,
(Prior &
Whorwell,
1990a,
non-treatment-seeking
sion tomography (PET) and functional mag-
IBS patients do not exhibit visceral hypersensi-
netic resonance imaging (fMRI). Neuroimaging
tivity. Given the more psychologically dis-
research has determined that areas of the limbic
tressed profiles o f their
treatment-seeking
403
404
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS counterparts (Whitehead et al., 2 0 0 2 ) , this
the absence of peripheral injury at a physiological
suggests that psychological factors may con-
level (Fields, 1 9 9 1 ) . According to Fields (1991),
tribute to visceral hyperalgesia, perhaps by
the response of an individual to damaging or
aggravating nociception stimulation o f vis-
potentially damaging stimuli depends on the
ceral afferent nerves in at least a subset o f IBS
stimulus as well as on the stimulus context. Pain
patients.
perception and the behavioral response to noxious stimuli are controlled by CNS painLike many benign
modulating circuits that can be activated by
chronic painful medical conditions, IBS has been
environmental stimuli. This circuit is activated
Psychological
Factors.
historically conceptualized as a physical mani-
by opioids and controls pain transmission neu-
festation of a variety o f psychiatric conditions,
rons in the spinal cord. The circuit contains sev-
including hypochondriasis, personality distur-
eral opioid and nonopioid neuropeptides, three
bance, somatization, depression, and anxiety
classes o f opioid receptors, and biogenic amine
(e.g., Clouse, 1 9 8 8 ; Hislop, 1 9 7 1 ; Latimer,
neurotransmitters such as norepinephrine and
1 9 8 3 ) . Although there is no evidence to support
serotonin. Furthermore, this circuit exerts bidi-
IBS as a psychiatric disorder, psychological
rectional control of pain transmission through
factors influence onset, expression, and course,
the activity of two functional classes of brain
particularly in more severely affected patients.
stem neurons whose activity is correlated recip-
Research has pointed to three main pathways
rocally with behavioral responses to painful
through which psychological factors influence
stimulation. Neurons of one class (i.e., on cells)
IBS. The first key pathway is directly through
are activated by noxious stimuli, whereas those
physiological systems. Psychological factors
of the other class (i.e., off cells) are inhibited by
(e.g., stress, negative emotional states) can
noxious stimuli. T h e cells activated by noxious
normatively induce changes in gut function
stimuli facilitate nociception, whereas those
(Tache, 1 9 8 9 ) , and their effect is particularly
inhibited by noxious stimuli inhibit nociception.
pronounced in IBS patients (e.g., Welgan,
Fields (1991) argued that psychological pro-
Meshkinpour, 8c Hoehler, 1 9 8 5 ) . For example,
cesses (e.g., attention, expectancy) may con-
psychological factors (e.g., negative mood states,
tribute to pain production at a neurobiological
expectation, attention) can modulate pain sever-
level by "kick[ing] the responses of pain trans-
ity. The mechanism underlying benign abdomi-
mission neurons to non-noxious neurons into
nal pain perception is not well known. M a n y
the noxious range and creat[ing] pain where
IBS researchers have been guided by Melzack
there was no pain" (p. 8 8 ) . If correct, this has
and Wall's (1965) gate control theory of pain as
relevance for understanding benign
an explanatory model for understanding the
medical disorders such as IBS.
painful
relationship between psychological factors and
The second pathway through which psy-
pain. According to Melzack and Wall, afferent
chological factors influence IBS symptoms
(i.e., moving to the spinal cord) stimuli, such as
is the adoption o f illness behaviors that can
nociceptive stimuli entering the spinal cord at
exacerbate symptoms. Health behaviors are
the substantia gelatinosa, are modulated by
strongly influenced by an individual's psycho-
other afferent stimuli (e.g., counterstimulation)
logical health. IBS patients as a group show
and descending spinal pathways (e.g., higher
higher levels of psychosocial distress as mea-
order mental processes, emotions, thoughts,
sured either through standardized questions or
images, attentional focus) in a way that blocks
with structured psychiatric diagnostic inter-
or "gates" the perception of pain signals.
views (Blanchard 8c Scharff, 2 0 0 2 ) . Over the
Psychological factors not only attenuate pain
past 1 5 years, Blanchard ( 2 0 0 1 ) has conducted
but also may produce clinical pain sensations in
a series o f studies that have systematically
Irritable evaluated
levels
of
psychopathology
in
Bowel
Syndrome
seek treatment than are their nondistressed
treatment-seeking IBS patients. Whereas levels
counterparts
(Drossman, 1 9 9 9 ; Whitehead,
of depressive symptoms, as measured by the
Bosmajian, Zonderman, Costa, & Schuster,
Beck Depression Inventory, fall consistently
1988).
within "mild" diagnostic ranges, IBS patients
patients do not differ from healthy controls on
In fact, non-treatment-seeking
IBS
report higher levels o f anxiety, as measured by
measures o f psychopathology
the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. IBS patients
et al., 1 9 8 8 ) . In other words, IBS patients with
had a mean state anxiety score of 4 7 , which
comorbid psychological symptoms are more
(Whitehead
falls at the 4 5 t h percentile for psychiatric
likely to complain o f symptoms and seek treat-
patients, the 61st percentile for general medical
ment than are their nondistressed counterparts.
patients, and the 84th percentile for normal
The precise mechanism underlying the rela-
middle-age females. Trait anxiety scores aver-
tionship between treatment seeking and IBS
aged between 4 7 and 4 8 , which falls at the 5 4 t h
symptoms has not been identified. It is possible
percentile for psychiatric patients, the 72nd per-
that
centile for general medical patients, and the
unpleasant
90th percentile for normal middle-age females.
making threat appraisals o f stimuli (Cohen,
this relationship
may
exist
because
mood states predispose
one to
The notion that IBS is a psychosomatic disorder
Kessler, 8c Gordon, 1 9 9 5 ) , which in turn lead
has also been fueled by research reporting high
to heightened physiological reactivity and the
rates of comorbid psychiatric disorders among
acquisition o f illness behaviors. Treatment-
IBS patents. T h e results of studies using well-
seeking behaviors may also be influenced by
validated semistructured methodologies with
psychological stress. Research has found that
adequate sample size indicate that
between
IBS patients report more stressful events, more
4 0 % and 6 0 % o f IBS patients have a diagnos-
stress-related changes in stool patterns, and
able psychiatric disorder, with anxiety being the
greater reactivity to stress than do controls
most common disorder (followed by mood and
(Bennet, Tennant, Piesse, Badcock, Sc Kellow,
somatization disorders). Among diagnosable
1 9 9 8 ) . Creed, Craig, and Farmer ( 1 9 8 8 ) found
anxiety disorders, the most common is general-
that major life events preceded the onset o f IBS
(Blanchard, Scharff,
in 6 0 % to 6 6 % o f IBS patients. In comparison,
Schwarz, Suis, 8c Barlow, 1 9 9 0 ) . T h e preva-
2 5 % o f controls experienced similarly severe
ized anxiety disorder
lence o f an Axis I psychiatric disorder not only
events at an arbitrary time. Lacking a suffi-
is high in IBS patients but also exceeds the
ciently developed repertoire o f coping skills
prevalence o f a psychiatric disorder
among
(Drossman, Li, et al., 2 0 0 0 ) , IBS patients may
patients with organic GI disorders
(56 vs.
respond physically to psychological stress by
1 8 % ) , whose clinical features are similar to
seeking medical attention that may in turn
those with IBS (e.g., I B D ) .
implicitly reinforce their disease conviction,
Although a select number o f studies have failed to confirm high rates o f psychiatric disorder (Blewett et al., 1 9 9 6 ) , there has emerged
promote a sense o f invalidism, and lead to maladaptive illness behaviors. A third pathway through which psycho-
a general consensus that a sizable number o f
logical factors influence IBS is by increasing
IBS patients have higher levels of distress and
the risk o f developing I B S . In general, IBS
substantial psychiatric comorbidity in propor-
researchers have focused on two types o f
tions that are conservatively at least twice as
psychosocial factors—reinforcement mecha-
high as in patients with organic GI disorders
nisms and childhood abuse—that mediate the
and healthy counterparts. IBS patients with
risk for IBS onset (Drossman, 1 9 9 4 ) . There is
comorbid psychological symptoms are more
a relatively large body o f literature linking a
likely to report more severe symptoms and
history o f early (i.e., preadolescence) abuse to
405
406
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS IBS symptoms in a sizable number o f female
that found that IBS patients were more likely
patients. A history of major traumatic events
than non-IBS respondents to recall that their
(i.e., physical and/or sexual abuse) is present
parents both responded to pain complaints
in patients with IBS more frequently than in
with the provision o f positive reinforcement
healthy controls. A history o f severe emo-
(e.g., gifts, toys) and demonstrated
tional trauma, such as physical and sexual
behaviors that can negatively reinforce illness
abuse
behaviors.
(especially during childhood), also
illness
appears to be associated with an increased risk for IBS. Drossman and colleagues ( 1 9 9 0 ) found that the frequency o f a history o f abuse among IBS patients was 3 0 % greater than in
Biopsychosocial Model Although
psychopathology,
abnormal
w o m e n with an organic GI disease. Female
motility, visceral hypersensitivity, and C N S
IBS patients with a positive history o f abuse
modulation all have been implicated in the
report m o r e pain, more sick days, a higher
expression o f IBS, none o f these processes is
level o f distress and health care use, worse
sufficient t o explain IBS symptoms within
behavioral functioning, and m o r e surgeries.
and across all patients. F o r this reason, IBS is
Although
the
currently conceptualized from the perspec-
relationship between abuse and IBS symp-
tive o f a biopsychosocial model (Drossman,
the m e c h a n i s m underlying
toms is not well defined (Drossman, Talley,
1 9 9 7 ) . T h e biopsychosocial model holds that
Olden, L e s e r m a n n , &
individual biology (e.g., genetic predisposi-
Barreioro,
1995),
abuse likely represents an important
risk
tion, G I physiology), behavior, and higher
factor for I B S symptoms and o u t c o m e in
order cognitive processes (e.g., coping, illness
select patients.
beliefs, abnormal central processing o f gut theory
stimuli) influence IBS through their interac-
(Bandura, 1 9 7 7 ) , Whitehead and colleagues
tion with each other, with early-life factors
( 1 9 8 8 ) approached early-life experiences from
(e.g., trauma, modeling), and with the indi-
a reinforcement perspective. Social learning
vidual's social and physical
theory (SLT) is arguably the prevailing theoret-
(e.g., reinforcement contingencies).
Drawing
from
social
learning
environments
ical framework used in health behavior and
At the heart o f the model is the belief that
health promotion research. In S L T , human
IBS involves a dysregulation in interactions
behavior is explained in terms o f a three-way,
among the C N S ; peripheral sensory, motor,
dynamic, reciprocal theory in which personal
and autonomic nerves; and the enteric nervous
factors, environmental influences, and behav-
system (ENS). This neural network is referred
ior interact continually. A basic premise o f S L T
to as the "brain-gut axis." T h e E N S , the sym-
is that people learn not only through their own
pathetic
experiences but also by observing the actions
parasympathetic nervous system (PNS) com-
of others (i.e., modeling) and the results o f
prise the three divisions o f the autonomic ner-
those actions (consequences). Whitehead and
vous system (ANS), the part of the nervous
colleagues argued
nervous
system
( S N S ) , and
the
that complaints o f IBS
system that regulates involuntary actions,
symptoms are partly acquired in a family
including smooth muscle, cardiac muscle, and
environment, where children learn
glands. T h e A N S ' s enteric division (i.e., ENS)
direct experience and
through
by observing
how
is located in the sheaths o f tissue lining the GI
others respond to IBS. In support o f the princi-
tract from the esophagus to the rectum. T h e
ple of familial transmission are results of a
E N S is composed o f both local sensory neu-
large telephone survey (Whitehead, Winget,
rons, which detect and
Fedoravicius, Wooley, &
regarding changes in the tension o f the gut
Blackwell, 1 9 8 2 )
relay
information
Irritable walls, and
its chemical environment
and
Bowel Syndrome
\
ASSESSMENT AND TREATMENT
motor neurons, which control muscle contractions o f the gut wall and secretion. T h e E N S
Like other chronic illnesses, IBS does not lend
plays a major role in maintaining homeostasis
itself to a " c u r e " or " f i x . " Therefore, the
in the body by controlling GI blood vessel
goals o f treatment are to normalize bowel
tone, motility, gastric secretion, and
function,
fluid
transport. Because o f its heavy concentration of neurotransmitters
(e.g., serotonin,
sub-
decrease
pain/discomfort,
and
improve the Q O L through a combination o f pharmacological
agents,
behavioral
self-
stance P, vasoactive intestinal peptide, calci-
change interventions, and lifestyle modifica-
tonin gene-related peptide), the fact that it is
tion. T h e exact constellation o f treatment
embryologically derived from the same part o f
strategies is not prescriptive but rather based
the neural crest that forms the brain, and its
on the nature (e.g., predominant bowel habit)
ability among other parts o f the
and severity o f symptoms (mild, moderate, or
unique
peripheral nervous system to mediate reflex
severe) o f the individual patient. IBS symptom
behavior (i.e., gut function) in the absence of
severity is a general term that, in the context
input from the brain or spinal cord (Gershon,
o f I B S , reflects not only the intensity and
1 9 9 8 ) , the E N S has been referred to as the
duration o f symptoms but also the emotional
"second brain."
unpleasantness
N o r m a l digestive functions involve communication links between the E N S and the CNS. These links take the form o f parasympathetic and sympathetic fibers that either connect the C N S and E N S or connect the C N S directly with the digestive tract. Through these cross-connections, sensory inputs from
treatment
and illness behaviors
seeking)
associated
with
(e.g., IBS
(Drossman, 1 9 9 9 ; Drossman et al., 1 9 9 7 ) . The
importance o f symptom severity as a
measure o f a patient's symptom experience comes from research showing that psychological functioning is strongly associated with the severity o f symptoms (Drossman, 1 9 9 9 ) .
the gut are relayed to and processed by higher cortical centers where they modulate affect, pain
perception, and
behavioral
response.
Mild Symptoms
Because the neural transmission lines of the
The majority ( 7 0 % ) of individuals who meet
brain-gut axis are bidirectional and reciprocal,
the IBS diagnosis can be classified as mild in that
the C N S receives information from the diges-
the symptoms occur relatively infrequendy (e.g.,
tive tract and modulates the E N S . T h e bidi-
two or three times per month), do not prompt
rectional relationship o f the brain-gut axis
medical care, and are not associated with
means that higher order mental processes
impairment in physical or psychological func-
(e.g., attention, emotion, sensation,
taste,
tion. First-line treatment for mildly affected
thought) can influence GI function, secretion,
patients is corrective information and education
and sensation (Drossman, 1 9 9 4 ) . N o r m a l GI
about the benign nature of IBS and dietary
function is typically characterized by a rela-
advice. For example, patients are encouraged to
tively high degree o f coordination of the brain-
exercise, eat well-balanced regular meals, avoid
gut axis. In IBS patients, however, there is a
fatty and gas-forming foods such as legumes,
persistent disruption in the interaction o f the
increase intake o f dietary fiber, and reduce
neuroenteric
in
intake of caffeine, sorbitol (e.g., sugarless gum,
abnormal motility and visceral hypersensitiv-
system that
is manifested
dietetic candy), and alcohol (Drossman et al.,
ity o f gut stimuli whose sensation/perception
1 9 9 7 ) . Fiber supplementation
is modulated by C N S regions closely linked to
bran) is often emphasized for patients with mild
emotion and pain perception.
IBS, at least for those whose symptoms do not
(e.g., psyllium,
407
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS
408
include bloating. Although most physicians
diarrhea-predominant IBS, a therapeutic trial
recommend increasing fiber intake (e.g., increas-
of
loperamide
(Imodium),
diphenoxylate
ing consumption o f fruits, vegetables, and whole
(Lomotil), psyllium (Metamucil), or methylcel-
grains) for the majority of their patients, its ther-
lulose (Citrucel) or a low-dose tricyclic antide-
apeutic value has not been consistently estab-
pressant (TCA) is often pursued. Patients with
lished in quality cknical trials and is surprisingly
constipation-predominant IBS often undergo a
controversial (Jailwala, Impériale, & Kroenke,
trial o f increased dietary fiber, supplemental
2 0 0 0 ) . It appears that patients with constipation-
fiber, an osmotic laxative (e.g., milk o f magne-
predominant and mixed-type IBS are most likely
sia, lactulose), and/or a stool softener if symp-
to derive maximum benefit from fiber supple-
toms are not relieved by dietary fiber alone.
ments because they act as bulk-forming laxa-
Because o f their anticholinergic side effects,
tives that ease stool passage. However, fiber has
however, T C A s can cause or exacerbate consti-
no
pain.
pation and are often not recommended for con-
demonstrated
positive effect
on
Because bran has a normalizing effect on colonic
stipation-predominant IBS patients (Clauw &
function, it is often recommended for diarrhea-
Chrousos,
prominent patients. Pharmacotherapy is not
bloating-predominant IBS may benefit from a
1997).
Patients with
pain/gas/
typically recommended as a first-line treatment
trial o f an antispasmodic agent, such as dicy-
for mild IBS.
clomine (Bentyl) or hyoscyamine with phénobarbital (Levsin), or a low-dose antidepressant.
Moderate Symptoms In IBS patients with moderate symptoms,
Severe Symptoms
the intensity and duration o f symptoms (e.g.,
In the case o f patients with severe symp-
two or three times per week) typically interferes
toms (daily or near daily symptoms o f severe
with activities o f daily living and is associated
intensity), a tertiary treatment center where
with greater psychological distress than is the
psychotropic medications (e.g., antidepres-
case in mildly affected IBS patients. Treatment
sants), psychiatric management, and psycho-
for IBS patients with moderate symptoms may
logical
require the addition
standard medical care is recommended.
of
pharmacotherapy
interventions
are
integrated
into
directed at the gut to control symptom exacerbations, more extensive lifestyle modification, and behavioral self-management techniques
Psychological Assessment
that often begin with symptom self-monitoring.
Consistent with the biopsychosocial concep-
The goal o f treatment is to increase awareness
tualization of IBS, the protocol o f a psycholog-
and identification o f specific triggers (dietary,
ical testing battery includes measures
stress, and hormonal fluctuations) and condi-
reflect the range of psychosocial factors that
tions under which IBS symptoms are likely to
influence IBS, including psychological distress,
occur, increase patients' sense of predictability,
coping resources, negative cognitions, pain,
that
and foster self-management skills. Moderately
behavioral functioning,
affected patients may also be prescribed behav-
behaviors. T h e first step in the evaluation is
ioral interventions, including hypnosis, stress
assessing the nature and severity o f symptoms;
Q O L , and
illness
management, and relaxation techniques, in an
their natural history; the circumstances, pat-
effort to dampen arousal and increase self-care
terns, determinants,
skills. If pharmacological agents are pursued,
symptoms; and the patient's treatment history
they are structured dominant
around
symptoms.
For
and
consequences o f
the most pre-
(current and lifetime). These goals can be
patients
achieved using a three-part
with
semistructured
Irritable
Bowel Syndrome
\
interview, the Albany GI History, developed by
form used at SUNY-Buffalo was developed by
Edward
State
Blanchard and used in numerous clinical trials
University o f N e w Y o r k - A l b a n y ( S U N Y -
over 15 years. Patients make a series o f daily
Blanchard's
group
at
the
Albany) (Blanchard, 2 0 0 1 ) . T h e Albany GI
severity ratings (beginning 2 to 4 weeks before
History is organized in terms of a functional
treatment begins) for abdominal pain and ten-
analysis o f the clinical problem. Part 1 covers
derness, diarrhea, constipation, flatulence,
history and description o f G I symptoms.
belching, and bloating on a 5-point scale rang-
Symptoms o f pain, bowel disturbance, and
ing from 0 (not a problem)
associated symptoms (e.g., flatulence, bloating)
They also record feelings o f incomplete evacu-
are assessed in terms o f their severity, fre-
ation, whether pain was relieved by bowel
quency, and duration. T h e Albany GI History
movements, avoidance o f foods and activities
is sufficiently detailed to support (but not
associated with symptoms, medication use,
to 4
(debilitating).
derive) a diagnosis using R o m e Π criteria once
and (for women) the first day o f their periods.
physical pathology has been ruled out medi-
There are a number o f advantages with self-
cally, m addition, the interview covers situa-
monitoring data, including accuracy, sensitiv-
tional factors that precede the onset of a
ity to temporal variations, and objectivity
symptom and consequent events. Part 2 is
(Meissner, Blanchard, & M a l a m o o d , 1 9 9 7 ) .
devoted to exploration o f the family history o f
For these reasons, many regard GI symptom
GI disorders in the extended family, a psy-
diaries as the "gold standard" in IBS research.
chosocial history and description of psychoso-
T h e daily symptom diaries are used to calcu-
cial functioning, and related problem areas in
late Composite Primary Symptom Reduction
the patient's life (e.g., relationship with peers,
(CPSR) scores, a previously validated measure
j o b strain, marital relations). Part 3 consists o f
of symptom change that describes clinically
a brief mental status examination. In addition
significant improvements in G I
to the Albany G I History, the R o m e II
(Blanchard & Schwarz, 1 9 8 8 ) . T o calculate
Integrative Questionnaire (Talley, Drossman,
the C P S R score, one must first calculate, for
Whitehead, Thompason, & Corassiari, 2 0 0 0 )
each patient and for each primary symptom, a
symptoms
is a comprehensive assessment tool whose items
Symptom Reduction Score (SRS). T o avoid
reflect R o m e Π criteria for functional GI disor-
the statistical problem o f multiple compar-
ders. Although the R o m e Π questionnaire does
isons between groups inherent in a multi-
not assess psychosocial features o f IBS and was
symptomatic disorder such as IBS, Blanchard
designed for epidemiological research, its struc-
developed a single metric, the C P S R score,
ture, ease of use, and comprehensiveness have
for hypothesis testing. An S R S for each o f
advantages for clinicians and researchers look-
the primary G I symptoms (abdominal pain
ing for a well-designed survey measure o f
and abdominal tenderness combined, diar-
clinical symptoms o f functional GI disorders.
rhea, and constipation) was calculated. For
A major part o f data collection involves self-monitoring o f symptoms using the G I
example, a diarrhea
S R S is calculated as
follows:
symptom diary. IBS symptoms vary considerably over time and across dimensions o f frequency, duration, and intensity. T h e temporal variability makes retrospective recall imprecise
Diarrhea Reduction Score = 1 0 0 χ Baseline Diarrhea Ratings Posttreatment Diarrhea Ratings Baseline Diarrhea Ratings.
and o f questionable validity. Daily recording and daily monitoring are important in both
T h e C P S R score was calculated by averag-
clinical and research settings. Although there
ing the t w o or three symptom rating scores
is no established self-monitoring format, the
relevant to the individual patient:
409
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS
410
C P S R = Pain and Tenderness Reduction Score
Research has shown that patients with persistent
+ Diarrhea Reduction Score
painful
Constipation Reduction Score
medical disorders
develop certain
strategies to cope, tolerate, and deal with pain
+ 2 or 3 (depending on number of symptoms present).
(Keefe et al., 1 9 8 7 a ,
1 9 8 7 b ) . Particularly
important coping responses include efforts to
Blanchard has defined a C P S R score o f
ignore or reinterpret pain sensations as benign,
0 . 5 0 or m o r e (an average reduction o f G I
pacing activities, increasing activity level, pray-
symptoms o f 5 0 % or more) as representing
ing or hoping, and disputing negatively skewed
clinically significant improvement.
thinking patterns using positive self-statements about patients' ability to cope or manage. Pain
Because a majority of IBS patients
coping strategies that are adopted and used
identify pain as their most bothersome symp-
over prolonged time periods may influence
Pain.
tom, clinicians may want to supplement daily
physical and psychological functioning signifi-
measurements o f abdominal pain/discomfort
cantly. In some patients, an adaptive set o f cop-
with data from additional pain instruments.
ing strategies may buffer them from
Whereas daily monitoring provides relatively
adverse effects o f medical illness. In others,
precise
maladaptive
data
regarding
the
intensity
and
coping patterns
the
may become
frequency o f pain, it provides only limited
entrenched, thereby heightening the severity o f
information regarding other important aspects
pain and the impact o f pain on behavior.
of the pain experience. T h e M c G i l l Pain
O n e coping style linked to IBS patients is an
Questionnaire ( M P Q ) provides valuable infor-
overreliance on a negatively skewed thinking
mation regarding the sensory, affective, and
pattern (i.e., catastrophizing) (Drossman, Li,
evaluative dimensions of the pain experience
et al., 2 0 0 0 ) . T h e term
(Melzack, 1 9 7 5 ) . T h e M P Q contains 7 8 pain
refers to an aberrant information processing
"catastrophizing"
words grouped into 2 0 subclasses of 3 to 5
style marked by magnification, helplessness,
descriptive words. Within these subclasses, the
and rumination toward painful stimuli. T h e
patient ranks the 3 to 5 words according to
pattern o f these data is similar to that in
the implied pain intensity. T h e 2 0 subclasses
research findings with osteoarthritis patients
are grouped into four sections: sensory (e.g.,
with persistent knee pain; those who rate
cramping), affective (e.g., agonizing), evaluative
themselves as more effective in managing pain
(e.g., intense), and miscellaneous. In addition to
and who report less catastrophizing cope with
the 7 8 pain words, the temporal pattern of pain
pain more effectively (Keefe et al., 1 9 8 7 b ) .
is assessed with 9 words (e.g., constant, peri-
Pain
odic). The location o f pain is assessed with a
effectively
Coping
Skills
drawing of the body with the words "exter-
Questionnaire ( C S Q ) (Rosenstiel &
Keefe,
coping
responses
with
the
can
Pain
be
assessed
nal/internal" added. T h e M P Q also includes a
1 9 8 3 ) . O n the C S Q , patients are asked to indi-
5-point pain rating scale that requires the
cate how they cope with everyday painful
patient to indicate his or her current level of
experiences. T h e C S Q measures the frequency
pain by choosing a description that most closely
of use o f seven pain coping strategies, six o f
matches his or her pain. Scores range from
which are cognitive (diverting
0 (mild) to 4 (excruciating). Patients' intensity
reinterpreting pain sensations, coping self-
scores reflect the word that best describes their
statements, ignoring pain sensations, praying
attention,
pain level, with higher scores indicating more
and hoping, and catastrophizing) and one o f
intensive levels of reported pain.
which is behavioral (increasing behavioral assessment
activity). T h e C S Q also includes two ratings of
includes the evaluation o f patients' behavioral
coping efficacy: one rating of perceived efficacy
and cognitive response to pain (i.e., coping).
of coping in decreasing pain and one rating of
An important
part o f pain
Irritable
Bowel Syndrome
\
perceived efficacy o f coping in controlling
Life Measure (IBS-QOL) (Patrick, Drossman,
pain. Previous research has shown that coping
Frederick, DiCesare, & Puder, 1 9 9 8 ) , whose 3 4
strategies measured by the C S Q are predictive
items each fall into one of eight domains (dys-
of pain, psychological function, activity level,
phoria, activity interference, body image, health
and physical impairment
with
worry, food avoidance, social relations, sexual
chronic pain problems independent o f disease
activity, or intimate relationships) clinically
severity. T h e C S Q provides a profile o f the use
relevant to IBS. Q O L measures are useful in
o f patients
of a range o f pain coping strategies that appear
assessing the extent to which IBS compromises
to be useful in predicting adjustments to per-
patients' general sense o f happiness and satis-
sistent medical conditions.
faction in important life domains (e.g., health, recreation). In comparison with more tradiAs noted previ-
tional biological measures that focus on infir-
ously, IBS is associated with significant psychi-
mity or disease status, Q O L measures focus on
atric comorbidity that is formally assessed using
both objective functioning and subjective well-
the Structured Clinical Interview for D S M - I V
being. These data sources are usually better for
Axis I Disorders (SCID) (First, Spitzer, Gibbon,
assessing the social and emotional outcomes of
& Williams, 2 0 0 1 ) . The SCID is a semistruc-
the treatment and disease process and for prov-
tured interview for obtaining major D S M - I V
ing an overall picture o f how treatments or ill-
[Diagnostic
Psychiatric
Comorbidity.
Mental
nesses affect patients' ability to function in life.
fourth edition [American Psychiatric
The I B S - Q O L concentrates on the perceived
Association, 1994]) Axis I and Axis Π diagnoses.
well-being of the patient and not more objective
There are other structured diagnostic interviews,
measures of physical functioning (e.g., activities
Disorders,
and Statistical
Manual
including the Anxiety Disorders
of
Interview
of daily living). Because there is general consen-
(ADIS-R) (DiNardo & Barlow, 1 9 8 8 ) , that have
sus that Q O L involves both objective function-
been used in clinical research with IBS patients.
ing and subjective well-being, the I B S - Q O L is
The SCID has some advantages over the ADIS
supplemented
and other semistmctured psychiatric interviews
M O S 36-Item Short Form (SF-36) (Ware &
in that it yields a more thorough assessment of
Sherbourne, 1 9 9 2 ) . T h e SF-36 assesses eight
non-anxiety-related disorders that may accom-
health dimensions (Stewart & Ware, 1 9 9 2 ) that
pany IBS. Several standardized self-report mea-
the I B S - Q O L does not assess directly: (a) limita-
sures provide useful information
regarding
tions in physical activities due to health prob-
psychological functioning. Additional psycho-
lems, (b) limitations in social activities due to
logical self-report measures tap psychopathol-
health or emotional problems, (c) limitations in
ogy. These include the Brief Symptom Inventory
usual role activities due to health problems,
with
information
from
the
(Derogatis, 1993) for psychopathology, the Beck
(d) bodily pain, (e) general medical health (i.e.,
Depression Inventory (Beck, Rush, Shaw, &
psychological distress and well-being), (f) limita-
Emery, 1 9 7 9 ) , the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory
tions in usual role activities due to emotional
(Spielberger, 1983), and the Perm State Worry
problems, (g) vitality (e.g., energy and fatigue),
Questionnaire (PSWQ) (Mayer & Raybould,
and (h) self-evaluation of general health status.
1 9 9 0 ) , a measure of habitual worry due to the
The SF-36 has been used to measure health-
high rate ( 4 0 % ) of generalized anxiety disorder
related Q O L in IBS patients (Hahn et a l , 1 9 9 9 ) .
comorbidity (Blanchard et al., 1 9 9 0 ) .
Treatment Efficacy Quality
of Life.
Because IBS is a chronic
condition that can compromise an patients'
Pharmacotherapy. o f dietary
As noted previously, a
emotional, social, and physical well-being,
number
patients are administered the IBS-Quality o f
agents that are currently used to treat IBS
and
pharmacological
411
412
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS represent the first- and second-line therapies
stool consistency and decrease frequency by
for IBS patients (Table 2 0 . 2 ) . T h e most c o m -
enhancing intestinal water absorption, strength-
mon classes o f medications currently used for
ening anal sphincter tone, and reducing intesti-
IBS are laxatives, antidepressants,
antidiar-
nal transit. Loperamide is preferred over other
rheals, and antispasmodics. There is n o uni-
opioid agonists (e.g., diphenoxylate) due to its
versally agreed-on medical option for IBS. N o
potency, longer duration o f action, and favor-
class o f medications or single medication
able side effect profile (e.g., lower potential for
within a class currently available has been
physical dependence). However,
demonstrated
does not appear to have an analgesic effect.
in well-designed
and
well-
controlled clinical trials to be consistently
loperamide
Bulking agents such as fiber supplements
superior to placebo treatment for the spectrum
(e.g., calcium polycarbophil, methylcellulose,
of IBS symptoms. This conclusion is based
psyllium) are often prescribed for patients with
largely on the findings of an influential 1 9 8 8
constipation-prominent IBS because these sup-
systematic review o f randomly controlled drug
plements facilitate peristalsis and
trials. Klein ( 1 9 8 8 ) concluded that because of
colonic and oral-anal transit. Although fiber
methodological flaws, " n o therapy has been
supplements may decrease constipation in a
shown to be effective in treating I B S " (p. 2 3 2 ) . Since the publication of the Klein ( 1 9 8 8 )
subset of patients, their therapeutic
improve
benefit
may come at the expense o f aggravating some
review, a slightly more positive appraisal of
IBS symptoms
pharmacological agents has emerged.
One
cramping, flatus). Furthermore, they have no
&
established analgesic properties. Dietary fiber
recent
review
(Poynard,
Regimbeau,
Benhamou, 2 0 0 1 ) o f 2 3 randomized
studies
with eight different agents published up to
(e.g., bloating,
distension,
supplements are o f conclusive benefit only in constipation-predominant patients.
2 0 0 0 offered some of the strongest existing evi-
With respect to antidepressants, the tricyclic
dence for direct smooth muscle relaxants (e.g.,
class represents the most commonly prescribed
calcium channel blockers, antispasmodics)
and investigated option. In a meta-analysis of
when pain is the predominant symptom. These
the efficacy o f T C A s on functional GI disor-
medications
ders (vs. IBS per se), J a c k s o n and colleagues
influence
motor
activity
decrease colonic responsiveness.
and
However,
none of the muscle relaxants identified as effec-
(2000)
reviewed
data
from
1 1 placebo-
controlled trials (2 non-ulcer dyspepsia and 9
tive (cimetropium bromide, panivorous bro-
IBS) and concluded that T C A s were effective in
mide, and trimebutine) is available in the
relieving pain. Furthermore, because most of
United States (Poynard et al., 2 0 0 1 ) .
the dosages given were lower than required to
T h e efficacy o f smooth muscle relaxants
achieve psychotropic effects, J a c k s o n and col-
available in the United States has not been well
leagues suggested that T C A s " w o r k " via their
established due to methodological shortcom-
neuromodulatory and analgesic properties (vs.
ings of the clinical trials. In relatively brief clin-
psychotropic effects). T h e intrinsic anticholin-
ical trials, anticholinergics (e.g., dicyclomine,
ergic effects of T C A s (e.g., constipation) may
global
broaden the therapeutic value (e.g., decrease
improvement, but the extent to which they
diarrhea) of T C A s , at least in patients with
improve specific IBS symptoms (e.g., diarrhea,
diarrhea-predominant IBS (Jailwala et al.,
constipation) is not known.
2 0 0 0 ) . Patients with constipation-predominant
hyoscyamine)
are
associated with
Patients
with
diarrhea are often prescribed antidiarrheals,
IBS appear less responsive to T C A s than do
which are designed to reduce colonic transit
those with either pain-predominant o r diar-
through direct action on the smooth muscle o f
rhea-predominant IBS. T h e four most com-
the GI tract. One example is the opioid agonist
m o n l y prescribed and
loperamide, which is designed to increase
nortriptyline (Pamelor), imipramine (Tofranil),
studied T C A s
are
Irritable Table 2 0 . 2 Agent and
Bowel Syndrome
Common Pharmacological Treatment for IBS Examples
Antidiarrheal Agents Diphenoxylate-atropine (Lomotil)
Predominant
Symptom(s)
Diarrhea
Possible
Side
Effects/Comment
Controlled substance, nervousness, drowsiness, dizziness, constipation, headache, urinary retention Preferred over diphenoxylate/atropine due to more favorable side effect profile and no abuse potential; adverse side effects include sedation, drowsiness, fatigue, dizziness, cramping, nausea, rash, and constipation; may improve diarrhea, urgency, borborygmi, frequency of bowel movement, and stool consistency but not abdominal pain or distention
Loperamide (Immodium)
a
Constipation
May worsen abdominal distension, bloating, and flatulence
Abdominal pain, diarrhea
Anticholinergic side effects (urinary retention, decreased sweating, headache, dizziness, congestion, blurred vision, dry mouth, tachycardia, rash)
Abdominal pain, nausea, nonspecific symptoms
May cause or worsen constipation; may stimulate appetite, cause urinary retention, cause blurred vision, and worsen constipation; may be useful for diarrhea-predominant IBS
Comorbid anxiety, depression
May worsen diarrhea
S-HT3 Serotonin Receptor Antagonist Alosetron (Lotronex)
Non-constipationpredominant IBS in females
May worsen constipation and headache
5-HT4 Serotonin Receptor Agonist Tegaserod (Zelnorm/Zelmac)
Constipation-predominant IBS in females
May worsen diarrhea, abdominal pain, nausea, flatulence, and headache
Bulk-Forming Laxatives Methylcellulose (Citrucel) Psyllium (Metamucil) Antispasmodic Agents Dicyclomine (Bentyl)
Hyoscyamine (Levsin) Tricyclic Antidepressants Amitriptyline (Elavil) Desipramine (Norpramin) Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitors Citalopram (Celexa) Fluoxetine (Prozac) Fluvoxamine (Luvox) Paroxetine (Paxil) Sertraline (Zoloft)
N O T E : IBS = irritable bowel syndrome. a. Borborygmi is noisy rumbling of air moving through the intestine.
\
413
414
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS amitriptyline
desipramine
generally modest to disappointing (with a few
(Pertofrane). Because o f their fewer side effects,
(Elavil),
and
exceptions), the track record for psychological
desipramine and nortriptyline are often pre-
interventions can be described as controver-
ferred over amitriptyline and
imipramine.
sial depending on the "eye of the beholder."
Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors have no
Doubts about the track record o f psychological
established therapeutic effect on IBS symptoms
treatments were perhaps expressed most clearly
but are frequently used to manage the psychi-
by Talley, Fett, and Zinsmeister ( 1 9 9 5 ) , whose
atric comorbidity (e.g., anxiety, depression).
influential systematic review concluded that the
Some o f the newer
pharmacotherapies,
efficacy of psychological treatments had not
known as serotonin modulators, are directed at
been established due to methodological flaws of
specific serotonin receptor sites in the gut, which
clinical trials. Talley and colleagues' study was
contains 9 5 % of the body's serotonin. Serotonin
not so much a systematic review as an analysis
receptors are heavily concentrated in the E N S
of the methodological quality of then available
and play an important role in mediating gut
clinical trials. T h e review itself suffered from a
response and sensation. These medications
number o f methodological problems that com-
include alosetron (Lotronex), a serotonergic type
promise the interpretability of its conclusions
3 antagonist agent, and tegaserod (Zelnorm), a
(Lackner,
type 4 agonist. Alosetron is marketed for non-
Hamilton, 2 0 0 3 ) . Notwithstanding its limita-
constipated
tions, Talley and colleagues' study was a land-
females, whereas
tegaserod
is
Morley,
Dowzer,
Mesmer,
&
designed for constipated females. Both medi-
mark one in that it was the first to evaluate
cations are unique among pharmacological
systematically methodological quality of psy-
agents in their ability to target multiple symp-
chologically oriented clinical trials across multi-
toms. Alosetron has been effective in reducing
ple a priori quality criteria. O n the other side of
pain and diarrhea, ostensibly by slowing colonic
the fence are those researchers who point to a
transit time. Tegaserod has been found to be
series o f quality smaller scale studies over the
effective in reducing pain, constipation, and
past 2 0 years as evidence of the clinical effec-
bloating, perhaps by stimulating peristalsis and
tiveness of psychotherapy. A comprehensive
chloride secretion, accelerating colonic transit
review of the outcome research is beyond the
time, and blocking pain signals from the gut.
scope of this chapter but can be found in a
Neither medication has established itself as any
recent article (Blanchard & Scharff, 2 0 0 2 ) .
more effective than placebo for male IBS patients. T h e manufacturer o f Lotronex voluntarily
Four different classes o f psychological treatments—brief psychodynamic
psychotherapy
withdrew it after serious GI events, specifically
with relaxation, hypnotherapy,
ischemic colitis and complications o f constipa-
behavioral therapy, and cognitive therapy—
tion, were reported in association with its use.
have been shown to be superior to symptom
Lotronex has subsequently been reintroduced
monitoring or routine medical care in reducing
to the marketplace with F o o d and
Drug
IBS symptoms (Blanchard, 2 0 0 1 ) . Treatments
restricted
featuring cognitive therapy and hypnotherapy
Administration
approval
under
cognitive-
conditions o f use. In sum, the most positive,
have been replicated and found to be superior
evidence-based appraisal of the efficacy o f
to
attention
placebo control
conditions.
pharmacological agents can be described
According to Blanchard and Sharif's ( 2 0 0 2 )
thusly: Specific agents can be effective for
narrative
some patients with discrete symptoms and/or
strongest empirical support (Whorwell, Prior, &
specific types o f IBS.
Faragher, 1 9 8 4 ) in that its therapeutic benefit
review,
hypnotherapy
has
the
has been replicated independently by two other Psychological
Treatment.
Whereas the over-
all track record for pharmacological agents is
research groups (Galovski & Blanchard, 1 9 9 8 ; Harvey, Hinton, Gunary, 8c Barry, 1 9 8 9 ) and
Irritable is maintained over time (Whorwell, Prior, &
Bowel Syndrome
\
Complications
Colgan, 1 9 8 7 ) . The psychological treatment with the second
Successfully treating IBS is a rewarding but
most consistently positive track record is cogni-
challenging
tive therapy.
achieved and maintained,
Cognitive therapy
for IBS is
endeavor.
F o r change patients
to
be
require
designed to reduce excessive emotional or phys-
motivation and a willingness to invest the req-
iological reactions associated with GI symptoms
uisite effort, time, and commitment in self-care
by modifying or eliminating negatively skewed
skills. This can be particularly difficult among
thinking patterns (e.g., jumping to conclusions)
those patients who are not psychologically
and belief systems (e.g., perfectionism) that
minded, have a strong disease conviction, or see
underlie these reactions. A related goal of cogni-
psychological treatments as reserved for mental
tive therapy is to provide patients with a general
illness. Therefore, it should be emphasized to
set of problem-solving or coping skills to man-
patients that their treatment is really no differ-
age a wide range of situations associated with
ent from other, more widely accepted behav-
IBS. Quality data based on a series o f smaller
ioral treatments
scale clinical trials indicate that 6 0 % to 8 0 % o f
problems. Just as a cardiologist may attempt to
used for physical
health
IBS patients who undergo cognitive therapy
alter cardiac disease by prescribing behavioral
achieve at posttreatment (up to 3 months) a clin-
changes
ically significant ( 5 0 % or more) reduction in IBS
decrease stress, change diet) for a heart patient,
(e.g.,
increase
physical
activity,
symptoms and maintain these gains at 3-month
so too do the cognitive or behavioral interven-
follow-up. Treatment gains associated with cog-
tions, which form the basis o f treatment,
nitive therapy are not limited to a reduction in
attempt to alter gut function
by changing
GI symptoms alone. A significant proportion of
behaviors. By drawing a parallel between the
patients also show substantial reductions in
treatment options o f IBS and those of other,
comorbid psychological distress (e.g., anxiety) at
seemingly more legitimate medical conditions,
posttreatment.
therapists attempt to mobilize patients' change
One o f the most interesting aspects o f the psychological treatment literature is the inconsistent support for multicomponent cognitive-
resources in the direction of a self-management approach. This is not to say that change will necessarily ensue. Patients are reminded about
behavioral protocol used effectively with other
the importance of regular practice and patience.
painful medical conditions (e.g., arthritis, low
There are at least two groups o f patients
back pain, fibromyalgia). Although cognitive-
w h o seem resistant to treatment. T h e first
behavioral therapy has been found to be more
group is patients with strong disease convic-
effective than symptom monitoring, it appears
tion and ongoing investment in pursuing a
to be no more effective than a credible atten-
medical cure for symptom relief. Although
tion placebo condition (Blanchard et al., 1 9 9 2 ;
these patients may attend treatment regularly,
T o n e r et al., 1 9 9 8 ) . Drossman and colleagues
they seem to merely "go through the motions"
( 1 9 9 9 ) conducted a narrative review o f 1 5
of treatment. Their disease conviction limits
psychologically oriented clinical trials
the
derived
conclusions
similar
to
those
and
effort,
commitment,
and
motivation
of
required to complete a treatment successfully,
Blanchard and Scharff ( 2 0 0 2 ) . There was,
which places a premium on behavioral self-
according to Drossman and colleagues ( 1 9 9 9 ) ,
change. O n e approach to these patients is to
sufficient evidence in 1 0 o f 13 studies for the
encourage them to obtain whatever medical
efficacy o f psychological treatments in reduc-
care has been found to be effective and to rein-
ing bowel symptoms at posttreatment (2 stud-
force the value o f simultaneously
ies were dropped from the review because the
management skills for managing a chronic
learning self-
participation rate dropped below 4 0 % ) .
illness that currently defies a medical solution.
415
426
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS
CASE S T U D Y " R a c h e l " is a 35-year-old, married Caucasian female with 1 6 years o f education w h o was referred by her gastroenterologist t o the S U N Y - B u f f a l o M e d i c a l School Behavioral M e d i c i n e Clinic for evaluation and treatment o f a 9-year history o f I B S . H e r assessment was conducted during t w o sessions as part o f the pretreatment phase o f a N a t i o n a l Institutes o f Health-funded clinical trial o f a groupbased cognitive therapy for I B S . R a c h e l ' s primary complaints were abdominal pain associated with constipation, bloating, and gas. O n the M P Q , she described abdominal pain as a cramping sensation associated with a sharp, stabbing, shooting feeling in the rectum. She characterized the affective dimension o f pain as exhausting and cruel. R a c h e l rated the typical abdominal pain intensity as being o f moderate intensity. During the clinical interview, she characterized current pain as "discomforting" and within moderate intensity on the M P Q . T h a t being said, pain occasionally reached severe levels. Pain was relieved when she either had a soft, loosely formed bowel m o v e m e n t o r exercised. Pain tended t o ease on its o w n after approximately 6 0 minutes. In addition t o pain, she experienced periods o f constipation, bloating, nausea, and gas. She characterized constipation as having n o bowel movement for 3 or m o r e days. Several times a week, she experienced a sense after a bowel movement that she had been unable to pass all o f the fecal matter (i.e., incomplete evacuation). Prior to presentation, Rachel had consulted her family physician, obstetrician/ gynecologist, gastroenterologist, and proctologist. Her gastroenterologist had diagnosed her with IBS after her family physician expressed uncertainty about whether her symptoms were symptomatic of colitis. She had undergone a variety of diagnostic tests to detect an underlying organic basis o f her symptoms. These included an upper GI series, gallbladder series, sigmoidoscopy, colonoscopy, and stool tests for ova, parasites, and blood. At the time o f the examination, she was prescribed Librax. She was also taking a number o f over-the-counter agents, including Citrucel, Colace, Triphasil, and Rosewater. Both prescription and over-the-counter agents provided limited relief of pain and bowel disturbance. With the exception o f IBS, Rachel was in generally good physical health. She was not being treated for any extraintestinal health problems. Her only prior operation was a tonsillectomy in 1 9 8 0 . There was no family history of colon polyps, I B D , colon cancer, or liver disease. Although Rachel had never been treated for a psychiatric condition, the clinical picture that emerged from testing was a nervous, tense, shy individual with strong perfectionistic inclinations. She described herself as a "longtime worrier" prone to high levels of general anxiety. M u c h o f her worries focused on interpersonal and evaluative stressors whose resolution necessitated self-assertion, conflict negotiation, and direct communication (e.g. refusing requests). O n the P S W Q , she acknowledged that her worries took on excessive ( " M a n y situations make me worry") and uncontrollable
Irritable
Bowel
Syndrome
("I know I shouldn't worry about things, but I just can't help it") qualities. She was particularly prone to worrying when she felt increased work pressure in her j o b as a social worker. Although she had no history o f a psychiatric condition, she reported a positive history o f sexual abuse that was perpetrated by an older neighborhood boy when she was approximately 9 years old. Rachel participated in a 10-session group-based treatment program that was offered as part o f a current outcome trial. She was randomly assigned to a cognitive therapy condition. T h e cognitive-based treatment for I B S , described m o r e fully in Blanchard ( 2 0 0 1 ) , is designed to teach the patient to learn to identify and correct maladaptive beliefs and information processing errors with the goal o f reducing G I symptoms and related distress. Within this treatment, cognitive interventions consist o f four overlapping phases: (a) educating the patient about IBS and the processes that maintain the disorder, with a focus on its situational, cognitive, and emotional triggers; (b) training the patient in the identification and modification o f his or her cognitive appraisals and interpretations o f situations, thoughts, and behaviors; (c) changing underlying or " c o r e " beliefs supporting negative cognitions; and (d) receiving formal training in problem solving to strengthen the ability to cope m o r e effectively with realistic stressors associated with I B S . Rachel met with four other IBS patients for 1 0 sessions ( 9 0 minutes long) in a small group setting with a single therapist over the course o f 1 0 weeks. T h e provision o f education about the nature and pathophysiology o f IBS (e.g., role o f visceral sensitivity, abnormal gut motility) and corrective information concerning the myths associated with IBS (e.g., " n o t real," "all in your head") was the first objective o f treatment. Rachel and her group members learned that IBS was a benign medical condition whose symptoms are due neither to a detectable organic problem n o r to psychopathology. Instead, IBS was presented as a legitimate illness due to a lack o f coordination in the interaction between the brain and the gut. It was explained that psychological factors (e.g., expectancies, stress, strong emotions) are capable o f disrupting h o w the brain and the gut function with each other in a w a y that aggravates symptoms in both IBS patients and nonclinical individuals. This effect is exaggerated in IBS patients. In R a c h e l ' s case, cognitive factors associated with IBS included her tendency to think the worst in the face o f uncertainty (i.e., overestimate the probability o f negative events), underestimate her ability to cope with adversity, and focus on the emotional distress o f a stressor rather than on her ability to manage it effectively regardless o f the accompanying distress. T o increase awareness o f the link between cognitive processes and symptoms, R a c h e l monitored IBS-related symptoms, their situational antecedents, her
accompanying
thoughts, and her responses (e.g., emotional, physical) using
"dysfunctional
thought records." O n monitoring sheets, she detailed the content o f her worries, their estimated "heat o f the m o m e n t " probability, and evidence to support them. T o the extent that worries could not be substantiated, she was instructed to revise her estimation o f the probability o f worrisome negative events based on realistic
417
418
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS
appraisals o f the evidence if any existed. A major goal o f this technique is to encourage patients t o treat automatic thoughts not necessarily as facts but rather as hypotheses ("dressed up as facts") whose factual basis could be determined both by using evidence-based logic and by generating alternative explanations for negatively skewed appraisals. F o r realistic stressors, R a c h e l was taught a set o f "decatastrophizing"
techniques that
emphasized
acceptance/resignation over
unmodifiable events, the importance o f both adopting a "so-what a p p r o a c h " and attending to the immediate environment, and the limited value o f responding physically to psychological stressors. T h e goals o f decatastrophizing techniques were to strengthen both R a c h e l ' s confidence that she had sufficient coping resources to endure adversity and her understanding o f the limited utility o f worrying about negative life circumstances over which she had limited, if any, control. T h r o u g h problem-solving techniques, R a c h e l learned a more flexible and varied set o f coping strategies for resolving realistic stressors that aggravated symptoms. O n c e basic cognitive restructuring skills were developed, cognitive therapy shifted toward challenging and disputing underlying core beliefs that in R a c h e l ' s case included a strong sense o f perfectionism, that is, the belief that she needed to be perfect in all endeavors and that falling short o f this standard is unacceptable. As her selfmanagement skills improved, her GI symptoms decreased. After about the fourth week, the intensity, frequency, and duration o f both pain and diarrhea decreased in c o m p a r i s o n with
pretreatment
levels. Interestingly, she experienced
no
appreciable change in bloating. W h e n R a c h e l returned for her 2-week posttreatment follow-up, she reported that GI symptoms had decreased by approximately 7 5 % and that pain had decreased by approximately 5 0 % to 6 0 % since pretreatment baseline. Consistent with her global reports o f improvement were data obtained from daily G I sympt o m diaries. Whereas symptoms occurred four or five times weekly at moderate to severe levels at pretreatment and were associated with significant distress and diminished function (e.g., role limitations, reduced Q O L ) , she n o w described onceweekly episodes o f abdominal pain whose intensity she rated as mild and nondisabling at posttreatment.
In addition t o improvement
in GI symptoms, she
completed treatment with an improved Q O L . A board-certified gastroenterologist w h o was blind to her treatment independently
evaluated her symptoms
as
"markedly improved." Beyond improvements in IBS symptoms, she demonstrated improvement in broader health status. Postsession evaluation for Axis I indicated that she no longer met full D S M - I V criteria for generalized anxiety disorder. R a c h e l was seen two additional times at 3 - and 1 2 - m o n t h follow-up to gauge her maintenance o f treatment gains. She experienced no significant exacerbation o f G I symptoms, m o o d disturbance, or functional limitations. T a b l e 2 0 . 3 illustrates the course o f Rachel's symptom severity across the treatment period.
Irritable Table 20.3
Bowel
Syndrome
Symptom Severity at Pretreatment, Posttreatment, and Follow-Up Assessments
Outcome Measure
Pretreatment
Gastrointestinal symptoms
1.27
Posttreatment
3-Month Follow-Up
0.74
12-Month Follow-Up
0.54
0.57
Monitoring index State Anxiety Trait Anxiety* Depression Quality of Life
49 50 11 35
30 30 3 17
35 37 5 15
24 34 4 14
Role limitations Emotional Pain Social functioning Mental health
45 61 70 56
45 51 44 56
45 46 44 51
45 51 44 44
1
b
c
d
d
11
d
a. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (summed scores). b. Beck Depression Inventory (summed score). c. Irritable Bowel Syndrome-Quality of Life Inventory. d. SF-36 (T-scores).
In other words, patients are encouraged to
cognitive-behavioral techniques and learned to
treat their IBS in much the same way that they,
respond more constructively in a way that
their friends, or their family members would
decreased symptom intensity and duration.
approach any other chronic medical illness
Another
such as diabetes, asthma, or heart disease.
compliance with homework assignments. As a
The second group o f treatment-resistant
potential
complication
involves
behavioral self-management treatment, cogni-
patients is patients who are unable to link fluctu-
tive therapy necessitates active commitment to
ations in IBS symptoms to stress or other psy-
attend treatment and complete between-session
chological triggers. These patients are not
homework assignments so as to learn more
necessarily defensive or inclined to "fake good."
adaptive skills (and to unlearn
maladaptive
It is quite possible that their IBS symptoms are
ones). This can be a difficult task, particularly
subject to limited psychological meditation (e.g.,
with patients who are inclined to overschedule
dietary or hormonal fluctuations). Therefore,
their lives. It is emphasized that the degree o f
there does not seem to be much value in attempt-
relief that patients invest in treatment corre-
ing to force the patients to recognize a relation-
sponds with their investment in behavioral
ship between stress and IBS that might not exist.
assignments. N o apologies are made for the
For these patients, one approach is to encourage
behavioral tasks assigned in treatment. I f
them to use their symptoms as situational trig-
patients cannot complete time- and effort-inten-
gers of potentially maladaptive responses that
sive assignments, it is suggested that a behav-
may subsequently aggravate symptoms. T o illustrate, one patient who was unable to
ioral treatment program might not be a good match for them, and they are encouraged to
identify situational triggers discovered that she
seek alternative treatment. Patients seem more
responded
likely to carry out assignments if they have
fearfully and with
catastrophic
thoughts to symptoms that seemed to come
developed a positive expectancy regarding
"out of the blue." Although she was unable to
treatment efficacy, which is reinforced by dis-
reduce the frequency of symptoms, she used
cussing the track record of behaviorally based
\
419
420
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS treatments for IBS. Finally, patients who are
number o f factors, including abnormal motility,
undergoing a high level o f objective chronic
visceral hypersensitivity, abnormal
cerebral
stress (e.g., transportation difficulties, financial
processing of bowel stimuli, and psychological
pressures) seem to have difficulty in carrying
processes. Currently, there are no empirically
out behavioral assignments, although
validated medical options that target the full
this
observation has not been formally evaluated.
spectrum o f symptoms associated with IBS. T h a t being said, a number o f well-defined, short-term psychological treatments have been
CONCLUSIONS
developed that offer hope and symptom relief for the 3 5 million individuals with IBS. This
IBS is a chronic, potentially disabling, painful
chapter has attempted to illustrate the applica-
medical disorder that exacts a heavy toll on
tion o f a cognitive therapy approach
society, employers, and its sufferers. IBS is not
emphasizes
explainable by structural or biochemical mech-
restructuring, and problem-solving training in
anisms. Symptoms are best explained from a
relieving abdominal pain, bowel dysfunction,
biopsychosocial perspective that emphasizes a
and related distress.
patient
education,
that
cognitive
REFERENCES Adams, P. F., 8c Benson, V. (1991). Current estimates from the National Health Interview Survey. In Vital Health Statistics 10 (Volume 8 3 , pp. 9 2 - 1 5 9 0 ) . Hyattsville, M D : U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Almy, T. P., Kern, F. J . , &C Tulin, M . (1949). Alternation in colonic function in man under stress: Experimental production of sigmoid spasm in healthy persons. Gastroenterology, 12, 4 2 5 - 4 3 6 . American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 1 9 1 - 2 1 5 . Bazzacchi, G., Ellis, J . , Villanueava-Meyer, J . , Jing, J . , Reddy, S. N., Mena, I., Sc Snape, W. J . , Jr. (1990). Postprandial colonic transit and motor activity in 98, 6 8 6 - 6 9 3 . chronic constipation. Gastroenterology, Beck, A. T., Rush, Α., Shaw, B., Sc Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. New York: Guilford. Bennet, E. J . , Tennant, C. C , Piesse, C , Badcock, C. Α., Se Kellow, J . E. (1998). Level of chronic life stress predicts clinical outcome in irritable bowel syndrome. Gut, 42, 2 5 6 - 2 6 1 . Blanchard, Ε. Β . (2001). Irritable bowel syndrome: Psychosocial assessment and treatment. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Blanchard, Ε. B., Se Scharff, L. (2002). Psychosocial aspects of assessment and treatment of irritable bowel syndrome in adults and recurrent abdominal pain in children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 7 2 5 - 7 3 8 . Blanchard, Ε. Β . , Scharff, L., Schwarz, S. P., Suis, J . M., Sc Barlow, D. H. (1990). The role of anxiety and depression in the irritable bowel syndrome. Behavior Research and Therapy, 28, 4 0 1 - 4 0 5 . Blanchard, Ε. B . , Se Schwarz, S. P. (1988). Clinically significant changes in behavioral medicine. Behavioral Assessment, 10, 1 7 1 - 1 8 8 .
Irritable
Bowel
Blanchard, Ε. B., Schwarz, S. P., Suis, J . M., Gerardi, M . Α., Scharff, L., Greene, B . , Taylor, A. E., Berreman, C., & Malamood, H. S. (1992). Two controlled evaluations of multicomponent psychological treatment of irritable bowel syndrome. Behavior Research and Therapy, 30, 1 7 5 - 1 8 9 . Blewett, Α., Allison, M., Calerait, B., Moore, R., Jenkins, P., & Sullivan, G. (1996). Psychiatric disorder and outcome in irritable bowel syndrome. Psychosomatics, 37, 1 5 5 - 1 6 0 . Carr, D. B . (1996). Preempting the memory of pain. Journal of the American Medical Association, 279, 1 1 1 4 - 1 1 1 5 . Cervero, E , & Laird, J . M. (1999). Visceral pain. Lancet, 353, 2 1 4 5 . Clauw, D. J . , & Chrousos, G. P. (1997). Chronic pain and fatigue syndromes: Overlapping clinical and neuroendocrine features and potential pathogenic mechanisms. NeuroImmunoModulation, 4, 1 3 4 - 1 5 3 . Clouse, R. (1988). Anxiety and gastrointestinal illness. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 11, 3 9 9 - 4 1 7 . Coderre, T. J . , Katz, J . , Vaccarino, A. L., &c Metzack, R. (1993). Contribution of central neuroplasticity to pathological pain: Review of clinical and experimental evidence. Pain, 52, 2 5 9 - 2 8 5 . Cohen, S., Kessler, R. C , & Gordon, L. K. (1995). Measuring stress. New York: Oxford University Press. Cook, U., Van Eeeden, Α., δί Collins, S. M . (1987). Patients with irritable bowel syndrome have greater pain tolerance than normal subjects. Gastroenterology, 93, 7 2 7 - 7 3 3 . Creed, F., Craig, T., & Farmer, R. (1988). Functional abdominal pain, psychiatric illness, and life events. Gut, 29, 2 3 5 - 2 4 2 . Crombie, I. K., Croft, P. R., Linton, S. J . , LeResche, L., &c Von Korff, M . (1999). Epidemiology of pain. Washington, DC: IASP Press. Dalton, C , Sc Drossman, D. A. (1997). Diagnosis and treatment of irritable bowel syndrome. American Family Physician, 5 5 , 8 8 3 - 8 8 5 . Derogatis, L. R. (1993). Brief Symptom Inventory. Minneapolis, M N : National Computer Systems. DiNardo, P. Α., Sc Barlow, D. H. (1988). Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule— Revised (ADIS-R). Boston: Center for Anxiety and Related Disorders. Drossman, D. A. (1994). Irritable bowel syndrome. The Gastroenterologist, 2, 3 1 5 - 3 2 6 . Drossman, D. A. (1997). Presidential address: Gastrointestinal illness and the biopsychosocial model. Psychosomatic Medicine, 60, 2 5 8 - 2 6 7 . Drossman, D. A. (1999). Do psychosocial factors define symptom severity and patient status in irritable bowel syndrome? American Journal of Medicine, 107(5A), S 4 1 - S 5 0 . Drossman, D. Α., Corazziari, E., Talley, N. J . , Thompson, W. G., Sc Whitehead, W. E. (Eds.). (2000). Rome II: The functional gastrointestinal disorders: Diagnosis, pathophysiology, and treatment—A multinational consensus (2nd ed.). McLean, VA: Degnon Associates. Drossman, D. Α., Creed, F. EL, Olden, K. W., Svedlund, J . , Toner, Β . B . , Sc Whitehead, W. E. (1999). Psychosocial aspects of the functional gastrointestinal disorders. Gut, 45, 1 1 2 5 - 1 1 3 0 . Drossman, D. Α., Li, Z., Leserman, J . , Keefe, F. J . , Hu, Y . J . , 8c Toomey, T. C. (2000). Effects of coping on health outcome among female patients with gastrointestinal disorders. Psychosomatic Medicine, 62, 3 0 9 - 3 1 7 . Drossman, D. Α., Talley, N. J . , Olden, K. W., Lesermann, J . , 8c Barreioro, M . A. (1995). Sexual and physical abuse and gastrointestinal illness: Review and recommendations. Annals of Internal Medicine, 123, 7 8 2 - 7 9 4 . Drossman, D. Α., Thompson, W. G., Talley, N. J . , Funch-Jensen, P., Janssens, J . , Sc Whitehead, W. E. (1990). Identification of subgroups of functional bowel disorders. Gastroenterology, 3, 1 5 9 - 1 7 2 .
Syndrome
422
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS Drossman, D. Α., Whitehead, W. E., &c Camilleri, M. (1997). Irritable bowel syndrome: A technical review for practice guideline development. Gastroenterology, 112, 2120-2137. Drossman, D. Α., Zhiming, L., Andruzzi, E., Temple, M. D., Talley, K. W., & Thompson, D. A. (1993). U.S. householders' survey of functional gastrointestiDigestive nal disorders: Prevalence, sociodemography, and health impact. Diseases and Science, 38, 1 5 6 9 . Fields, H. (1991). Depression and pain: A neurobiological model. Neuropsychiatry, and Behavioral Neurology, 4(1), 8 3 - 9 2 . Neuropsychology, First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J . (2001). Structured Clinical ediInterview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, research version, non-patient tion (SCID-I/NP). New York: New York State Psychiatric Institute. Galovski, T. E., &c Blanchard, Ε. Β. (1998). The treatment of irritable bowel syndrome and Biofeedback, 23, 2 1 9 - 2 3 2 . with hypnotherapy. Applied Psychophysiology Gershon, M . (1998). The second brain. New York: HarperCollins. Gwee, K. A. (2001). Postinfectious irritable bowel syndrome. Gut, 4, 2 8 7 - 2 9 1 . Hahn, Β. Α., Yan, S., & Strassels, S. (1999). Impact of irritable bowel syndrome on quality of life and resource use in the United States and United Kingdom. Digestion, 60, 7 7 - 8 1 . Harvey, R. F., Hinton, R. Α., Gunary, R. M., & Barry, R. E. (1989). Individual and group hypnotherapy in treatment of refractory irritable bowel syndrome. Lancet, 1, 4 2 4 - 4 2 5 . Hislop, I. G. (1971). Psychological significance of the irritable colon syndrome. Gut, 12, 4 5 2 - 4 5 7 . Hyams, J . S., Burke, G., Davis, P. M., Rzepski, B . , & Andrulonis, P. A. (1996). Abdominal pain and irritable bowel syndrome in adolescents: A communitybased study. Journal of Pediatrics, 129, 2 2 0 - 2 2 6 . Isgar, B., Harman, M., Kaye, M . D., & Whorwell, P. J . (1983). Symptoms of irritable bowel syndrome in ulcerative colitis in remission. Gut, 24, 1 9 0 - 1 9 2 . Jackson, J . , O'Malley, P. G., Tomkins, G., Balden, E., Santoro, J . , & Kroenke, K. (2000). Treatment of functional gastrointestinal disorders with antidepressant medications: A meta-analysis. American Journal of Medicine, 108, 6 5 - 7 2 . Jailwala, J . , Impériale, T., & Kroenke, Κ. (2000). Pharmacologic treatment of the irritable bowel syndrome: A systematic review of randomized, controlled trials. Annals of Internal Medicine, 133, 1 3 6 - 1 4 7 . Jain, A. P., Gupta, O. P., Jajoo, U. N., & Sidwa, Η. K. (1991). Clinical profile of irritable bowel syndrome at a rural based teaching hospital in central India. Journal of Associated Physicians India, 39, 3 8 5 - 3 8 6 . Keefe, F. J . , Caldwell, D. S., Queen, K., Gil, Κ. M., Martinez, S., Crisson, J . E., Ogden, W., 8t Nunley, J . (1987a). Osteoarthritic knee pain: A behavioral analysis. Pain, 28, 3 0 9 - 3 2 1 . Keefe, F. J . , Caldwell, D. S., Queen, K. T., Gil, Κ. M., Martinez, S., Crisson, J . E., Ogden, W., &c Nunley, J . (1987b). Pain coping strategies in osteoarthritis patients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 5 5 , 2 0 8 - 2 1 2 . Kellow, J . E., &c Phillips, S. F. (1987). Altered small bowel motility in irritable bowel 92, 1 8 8 5 - 1 8 9 3 . syndrome is correlated with symptoms. Gastroenterology, Klein, K. (1988). Controlled treatment trials in the irritable bowel syndrome: A critique. Gastroenterology, 95, 2 3 2 - 2 4 1 . Lackner, J . M., Morley, S. M., Dowzer, C , Mesmer, C , & Hamilton, S. (2003). of randomized controlled trials of psySystematic review and meta-analysis chological treatments for irritable bowel syndrome. Unpublished manuscript, State University of New York, Buffalo, Medical School. Latimer, P. R. (1983). Functional gastrointestinal disorders: A behavioral medicine approach. New York: Springer. Lynn, R. B., & Friedman, L. S. (1993). Irritable bowel syndrome. New England Journal of Medicine, 329, 1 9 4 0 - 1 9 4 5 .
Irritable Bowel Maxton, D. G., Morris, J . Α., &c Whorwell, P. J . (1989). Ranking of symptoms by patients with the irritable bowel syndrome. British Medical Journal, 299, 1 1 3 8 . Maxwell, P. R., Mendall, Μ. Α., & Kumar, D. (1997). Irritable bowel syndrome. Lancet, 350, 1 6 9 1 - 1 6 9 5 . Mayer, Ε. Α., & Raybould, Η. E. (1990). Role of visceral afferent mechanisms in functional bowel disorders. Gastroenterology, 99, 1 6 8 8 - 1 7 0 4 . McKendrick, M . W. (1996). Post salmonella irritable bowel syndrome: 5 year review. Journal of Infection, 32, 1 7 0 - 1 7 1 . McKendrick, M . W., & Read, N. W. (1994). Irritable bowel syndrome: Post salmonella infection. Journal of Infection, 29, 1-3. Meissner, J . S., Blanchard, Ε., & Malamood, H. S. (1997). Comparison of treatment Psychophysiology outcome measures for irritable bowel syndrome. Applied and Biofeedback, 22(1), 5 5 - 6 2 . Melzack, R. (1975). The McGill Pain Questionnaire: Major properties and scoring methods. Pain, 1, 2 7 7 - 2 9 9 . Melzack, R., δί Wall, P. D. (1965). Pain mechanisms: A new theory. Science, 150, 971-979. Mertz, H., Morgan, V., Tanner, G., Pickens, D., Price, R., Shyr, Y., & Kessler, R. (2000). Regional cerebral activation in irritable bowel syndrome and control subjects with painful and non-painful rectal distention. Gastroenterology, 118, 842-848. Mertz, H., Naliboff, B . , Munakata, J . , Niazi, N., & Mayer, E. A. (1995). Altered rectal perception is a biological marker of patients with irritable bowel syndrome. Gastroenterology, 109, 4 0 - 5 2 . Morris-Yates, A. D., Talley, N. J . , Boyce, P., & Andrews, G. (1995). Evidence of a genetic contribution to self-reported symptoms of irritable syndrome [abstract]. Gastroenterology, 108, A652. Naliboff, B. D., Derbyshire, S. W., Munakata, J . , Berman, S., Mandelkern, M., Chang, L., e t a l . (2001). Cerebral activation in patients with irritable bowel syndrome and control subjects during rectosigmoid stimulation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 63, 3 6 5 - 3 7 5 . O'Keefe, Ε. Α., Talley, N. J . , Zinsmeister, A. R., & Jacobsen, S. J . (1995). Bowel disorders impair functional status and quality of life in the elderly: A populationbased study. Journal of Gerontology, 50(4), M 1 8 4 - M 1 8 9 . Olubuyide, I. O., Olawuyi, F., &c Fasanmade, A. A. (1995). A study of irritable bowel syndrome diagnosed by Manning criteria in an African population. Digestive Diseases and Science, 40, 9 8 3 - 9 8 5 . Patrick, D. L., Drossman, D. Α., Frederick, I. O., DiCesare, J . , & Puder, K. L. (1998). Quality of life in persons with irritable bowel syndrome: Development of a new measure. Digestive Diseases and Science, 43, 400—411. Poynard, T., Regimbeau, C , & Benhamou, Y . (2001). Metaanalysis of smooth muscle relaxants in the treatment of irritable bowel syndrome. Alimentary Pharmacology and Therapeutics, 15, 3 5 5 - 3 6 1 . Prior, A. M. D., & Whorwell, P. J . (1990a). Anorectal manometry in irritable bowel syndrome: Differences between diarrhea and constipation predominant subjects. Gut, 31, 4 5 8 - 4 6 2 . Prior, A. C. S., & Whorwell, P. J . (1990b). Changes in rectal sensitivity after hypnotherapy in patients with irritable bowel syndrome. Gut, 31, 8 9 6 . Rodriguez, L. A. G., & Ruigozmez, A. (1999). Increased risk of irritable bowel syndrome after bacterial gastroenteritis: A cohort study. British Medical Journal, 318, 656-666. Rosenstiel, A. K., & Keefe, F. J . (1983). The use of coping strategies in chronic low back pain patients: Relationship to patient characteristics and current adjustment. Pain, 17, 3 3 - 4 4 . Sandler, R. S., Jordan, M . C , & Shelton, B . J . (1990). Demographic and dietary determinants of constipation in the U.S. population. American Journal of Public Health, 80, 1 8 5 - 1 8 9 .
Syndrome
423
424
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS Silverman, D. H , Munakate, J . Α., Ennes, H., Mandelkern, Μ . Α., Hoh, C. K., Phelps, M . E., & Mayer, E. A. (1997). Regional cerebral activity in normal and pathological perception of visceral pain. Gastroenterology, 112, 6 4 - 7 2 . Spielberger, C. D. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Stewart, A. L., &c Ware, J . E. (1992). Measuring functioning and well-being: The medical outcomes study approach. Durham, NC: Duke University Press. Tache, Y . (1989). Central control of gastrointestinal transit and motility by braingut peptides. In W. J . Snape, J r . (Ed.), Pathogenesis of functional bowel disease (pp. 5 5 - 7 8 ) . New York: Plenum. Talley, N., Drossman, D. Α., Whitehead, W. E., Thompson, W. G., &c Corassiari, E. (2000). Rome II Integrative Questionnaire. In D. A. Drossman, E. Corazziari, N. J . Talley, W. G. Thompson, &C W. E. Whitehead (Eds.), Rome II: The functional gastrointestinal disorders: Diagnosis, pathophysiology, and treatment—A multinational consensus (2nd ed., pp. 6 9 0 - 7 1 4 ) . McLean, VA: Degnon Associates. Talley, N. J . , Fett, S. L., & Zinsmeister, A. R. (1995). Self-reported abuse and gastrointestinal disease in outpatients: Association with irritable bowel-type symptoms. American journal of Gastroenterology, 90, 3 6 6 - 3 7 1 . Talley, N. J . , Gabriel, S. E., Harmsen, W. S., Zinsmeister, A. R., & Evans, R. W. (1995). Medical costs in community subjects with irritable bowel syndrome. Gastroenterology, 109, 1 7 3 6 - 1 7 4 1 . Thompson, W. G., Dotevall, G., Drossman, D. Α., Heaton, K. W., &C Kruis, W. (1989). Irritable bowel syndrome: Guidelines for the diagnosis. Gastroenterology International, 2, 9 2 - 9 5 . Toner, Β. B., Segal, Ζ. V., Emmott, S., Myran, D., Ali, Α., DiGasbarro, I., & Stuckless, N. (1998). Cognitive behavioral group therapy for patients with irritable bowel syndrome. International Journal of Group Psychotherapy, 48, 2 1 5 - 2 4 3 . Ware, J . E., & Sherbourne, C. D. (1992). The M O S 36-Item Short Form Health Survey (SF-36). Medical Care, 30, 4 7 3 - 4 8 3 . Welgan, P., Meshkinpour, H , & Hoehler, F. (1985). The effect of stress on colon motor and electrical activity in irritable bowel syndrome. Psychosomatic Medicine, 47, 1 3 9 - 1 4 9 . Whitehead, W. E., Bosmajian, L., Zonderman, A.B., Costa, P.T., & Schuster, M . M . (1988). Symptoms of psychologic distress associated with irritable bowel syndrome: Comparison of community and medical clinical samples. Gastroenterology, 95, 7 0 9 - 7 1 4 . Whitehead, W. E., Burnett, C. K., Cook, E. W., & Taub, E. (1996). Impact of irritable bowel syndrome on quality of life. Digestive Diseases and Science, 41, 2 2 4 8 - 2 2 5 3 . Whitehead, W. E., Engel, B . T., &c Schuster, M. M . (1990). Perception of rectal distention is necessary to prevent fecal incontinence. Advanced Physiological Science, 17, 2 0 3 - 2 0 9 . Whitehead, W. E., Palsson, O., & Jones, K. R. (2002). Systematic review of the comorbidity of irritable bowel syndrome with other disorders: What are the causes and implications? Gastroenterology, 122, 1 1 4 0 - 1 1 5 6 . Whitehead, W. E., Winget, C , Fedoravicius, A. S., Wooley, S., & Blackwell, B . (1982). Learned illness behavior in patients with irritable bowel syndrome and peptic ulcer. Digestive Diseases and Science, 27, 2 0 2 - 2 0 9 . Whorwell, P. J . , Prior, Α., & Colgan, S. M. (1987). Hypnotherapy in severe irritable bowel syndrome: Further experience. Gut, 28, 4 2 3 - 4 2 5 . Whorwell, P. J . , Prior, Α., &c Faragher, Ε. B . (1984). Controlled trial of hypnotherapy in the treatment of severe refractory irritable bowel syndrome. Lancet, 1232-1234.
CHAPTER
Insomnia and the Sleep
21
Disorders
VALERIE A . WOLFE AND SHERI D . PRUITT
I
nsomnia and the sleep disorders present a
and medical treatments
variety of problems. Some are primarily
treatment o f sleep disorders.
behavioral, whereas others have a dominant
available for
the
Despite recommendations from the National
biological component. Regardless of etiology,
Commission on Sleep Disorders Research,
sleep
common.
physicians rarely receive training in the assess-
Approximately 6 5 million Americans suffer each
ment and treatment o f sleep problems. Medical
problems
are
remarkably
year from transient sleep difficulties, and about
students obtain between 0 and 2 hours o f train-
3 0 million more Americans have chronic insom-
ing specific to sleep disorders
nia (Hauri & Linde, 1996). Moreover, approxi-
Vaughan, 1 9 9 9 ) , and this lack of training is
mately 3 5 % of adults report some type of sleep
reflected in clinical practice. For example, many
disturbance each year, with half of these individ-
physicians do not ask about insomnia during
uals describing their sleep problems as "serious"
office visits (Dement &
(Gallup Organization, 1 9 9 5 ; Mellinger, Baiter,
Moreover, when insomnia is recognized, physi-
& Uhlenhuth, 1 9 8 5 ) .
cians defer to medication treatment rather than
(Dement
Vaughan,
&
1999).
T h e cost o f sleep disorders is exorbitant.
very efficacious behavioral treatments. In fact,
In 1 9 9 0 , the National Commission on Sleep
practice guidelines from the American Medical
Disorders Research, a commission created by
Association, the Canadian Medical Association,
Congress t o investigate sleep disorders and
and many health maintenance organizations
their effects
on the population,
reported
the annual cost o f sleep problems t o be in the tens
o f billions o f dollars
(Dement
indicate that behavioral interventions should be the treatment of choice for insomnia.
&
Vaughan, 1 9 9 9 ) . In addition, estimates indicate that 2 4 , 0 0 0 people die each year from accidents related to falling asleep while driving (Dement & Vaughan, 1 9 9 9 ) . T h e report generated
by the c o m m i s s i o n
advocated
extensive training for primary care doctors, a
BACKGROUND AND ETIOLOGY The Basics of Sleep, Sleep Architecture, and Sleep Cycles
national awareness campaign on insomnia,
Sleep is an exceedingly important activity.
and increased awareness o f the behavioral
Although scientists are not certain how sleep
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS
426
helps daytime functioning, it is fairly easy
wakefulness (Gupta, Gupta, 8c
to assess the difficulties associated with sleep
1 9 8 6 ; Moldofsky, 1 9 8 9 ) .
deprivation. Without enough sleep, individu-
Haberman,
Sleep deprivation can also cause pain. When
als find themselves edgy and irritable. In addi-
individuals
tion, their concentration and ability to learn
interrupted, they complain o f musculoskeletal
new information begin to decline. O n e o f the
tenderness, which resolves after 2 nights o f
first symptoms of sleep deprivation is depres-
noninterrupted
sion. After a few days o f no sleep, some people
1 9 7 3 ) . Sleep is also essential for normal func-
will perform as if they are intoxicated. W h e n
tioning o f the body's immune system and for
people have been denied sleep for about a
healthy cell growth. This explains why sleep-
week, they can experience visual and auditory
deprived individuals are more likely to get sick
have
their
deep
sleep (Hauri
delta
8c
sleep
Hawkins,
hallucinations. Sleep deprivation impairs func-
and tend to heal less rapidly as compared with
tion in nearly everyone. However, the amount
well-rested individuals (Hauri 8c Linde, 1 9 9 6 ) .
of sleep deprivation a single person can tolerate without ill effects varies.
The amount o f sleep that humans need to function maximally varies both by individual
Fatigue also contributes to a host o f psy-
and by age. Whereas newborns need about
chiatric and medical diagnoses. Both anxiety
16 hours of sleep per day, 2-year-olds need
and depression have a strong sleep c o m p o -
about 13 hours. Teenagers require about 9 hours
nent. Among patients with depression, 8 5 %
of sleep per night, and adults need 7 to 8 hours
report insomnia and 1 0 % to 1 5 % complain
on average. Interestingly, the amount of sleep
o f hypersomnia (Ford 8c K a m e r o w , 1 9 8 9 ) .
an individual may require varies from about
Patients with
frequently
5 hours to 1 1 hours. Therefore, 7 or 8 hours
function energetically for days or weeks on a
bipolar
disorder
per night reflects an average. Adults over age
few hours o f sleep and then have periods o f
6 5 years need as much sleep as do younger
hypersomnia associated with major depres-
adults, but their sleep tends to be less deep and
sive episodes. Changes in sleep can predict
they tend to wake more during the night.
a major depressive episode (Perlis, Giles,
Researchers are not sure why this is the case,
Buyesse, T u , 8c Kupfer, 1 9 9 7 ) , and denying
but they believe that it may be a natural out-
sleep t o someone w h o has bipolar disorder
come o f aging or that it may be related to
can
8c
decreased activity, medications, and/or having a
posttrau-
variable schedule. Approximately half of adults
matic stress disorder also report sleep distur-
over age 6 5 years have a chronic sleep disorder.
trigger
Kamerow,
a
manic
1989).
episode
(Ford
Patients with
bance, including nightmares and insomnia (American Psychiatric Association, 1 9 9 4 ) . Insomnia is also associated with chronic
Sleep Cycles
obstructive pulmonary disease, osteoarthritis,
There are five stages o f sleep that occur in
asthma, fibromyalgia, headaches, and chronic
cycles during the night. These are referred to as
pain. Between 5 0 % and 7 0 % o f patients with
Stages 1 to 4 and R E M (rapid eye movement)
a pain diagnosis suffer from a sleep distur-
sleep. Each stage is defined by depth o f sleep,
bance (Moffitt, Kalucy, Kalucy, Baum, 8c
brain wave activity, eye movements, and mus-
Cooke,
8c
cle tone. Stage 1 sleep is a light sleep and is char-
Townley, 1 9 8 5 ) . Patients with fibromyalgia,
acterized by short fast brain waves (called theta
rheumatoid
waves) and slow eye movements. This type o f
1 9 9 1 ; Pilowsky, arthritis,
Crettenden,
back
pain,
and/or
headaches have been found to have alpha
sleep occurs at the beginning of the night or
wave intrusions into their delta sleep, indicat-
during the day with boredom or fatigue. People
ing that their deep sleep is interrupted
are easily aroused from a Stage 1 sleep and on
by
Insomnia wakening can perform cognitive and physical tasks without grogginess. Variable brain wave lengths (called sleep spindles and Κ complexes) characterize Stage 2 sleep. Eye movements stop and brain activity slows down. Stage 3 sleep is a deeper stage of sleep and is characterized by slower delta waves interspersed with small quick waves. Stage 4 sleep is a very deep sleep and is identified by delta wave activity that is not interspersed with shorter waves. Although it is easy to arouse someone from Stage 1 or 2 sleep, it is much more difficult to wake from Stage 3 or 4 sleep. It is during this deep delta sleep (Stage 4) that bed-wetting and sleepwalking occur. R E M sleep is distinct from the other stages of sleep. Humans are very active physiologically during R E M . For example, during R E M , the muscles o f the body stiffen, eyes move rapidly, and heart rate, blood pressure, oxygen use, and respiration become more rapid and
variable. In addition,
during R E M ,
reflexes, kidney function, and hormonal patterns change. T h e body's temperature regulation is affected such that people will not sweat or shiver. During R E M sleep, both genders experience engorgement of the genital region— causing
erections
in
males
and
clitoral
engorgement in females. It is during R E M sleep that humans dream. A typical sleep cycle lasts about 9 0 minutes, and healthy sleepers complete four to six cycles per night on average. After a few minutes in Stage 1 sleep, Stages 2 , 3 , and 4 occur. Stage 2 sleep repeats, followed by R E M sleep, before
and the Sleep Disorders
j
Insomnia Insomnia is the most c o m m o n sleep problem. It is characterized by an inability to fall asleep quickly (generally within 3 0 minutes), waking during the night with difficulty in returning to sleep, waking t o o early in the morning, and/or having nonrestorative sleep. In addition, sleep disruption must occur at least 3 nights per week. Insomnia is typically categorized in terms of chronicity. Transient insomnia typically lasts a few nights to a few weeks. M o s t people will experience transient insomnia sometime during a year. Transient insomnia can be triggered by a variety o f factors, including stress, life changes, illness, a poor sleep environment, shift work, medication changes, jet lag, and poor sleep habits. Transient insomnia can become chronic due to classical conditioning. For example, one patient described insomnia that started after a divorce. H e described waking during the night because of stress and sadness; after smoking a cigarette, he was able to fall back asleep. This patient reported that the divorce was finalized 3 years ago. H e felt like he had moved on emotionally, but he still awakened during the night and could not fall asleep until he smoked a cigarette. Unfortunately, the attempts made to try to improve sleep often worsen the problem. Chronic insomnia can last from a month to many decades, causing both functional depression)
(e.g., decreased and medical
concentration,
(e.g., headaches,
hypertension) complications.
the next 90-minute cycle begins. During the early part o f the night, a greater amount o f time is spent in deep Stages 3 and 4 sleep. Closer to morning, R E M sleep is lengthier.
Restless Leg Syndrome and Periodic Limb Movement Disorder
During the night, normal adults spend about
Restless leg syndrome (RLS) is defined as an
5 % of the night in Stage 1 sleep, 5 0 % in Stage 2
urge to move or shake the lower extremities
sleep, 5 % in Stage 3 sleep, 1 0 % to 1 5 % in
because o f an uncomfortable sensation. M o s t
Stage 4 sleep, and 2 0 % to 2 5 % in R E M sleep.
often, this affects the legs, but it also is experi-
M o s t people can function well if they are able
enced in the arms or in muscles in other parts
to complete four to six sleep cycles during a
of the body. T h e feeling typically is more exag-
night (thereby sleeping about 6 to 9 hours).
gerated in the evening and often can prevent a
427
428
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS person from relaxing enough to fall asleep.
at inopportune times, for example, while driving
Moving the legs relieves the discomfort or
or during meetings. Sleep apnea sufferers also
aching. Diabetes, anemia, chronic renal failure,
tend to snore loudly. T h e consequences o f
and certain medications can cause R L S , but
sleep apnea can be severe because patients not
often the root cause is not apparent. R L S may
only will lack sufficient oxygen in the blood but also will have excess carbon dioxide. They
worsen with age. Periodic leg movement disorder ( P L M D )
frequently have comorbidities, including car-
is an insomnia disorder that is accompanied
diac arrhythmia, headache, malaise, fatigue,
by repetitive episodes o f muscle contractions
weight gain, and night sweats. In addition,
separated
sleep apnea sufferers are frequently awakened
by intervals o f 2 0 to 3 0 seconds. Awakenings
through the night as they try to clear their
(0.5 to 5 . 0 seconds in duration)
may be associated with these movements.
airways; they rarely complete 90-minute sleep
M o s t patients with R L S have P L M D .
cycles. T o clear their airway, sufferers may
T h e prevalence of R L S and P L M D in the
gasp or describe a feeling o f drowning. They
general population is between 9 % and 1 5 %
tend to be the most fatigued patients referred
(Hening et al., 1 9 9 9 ) . T h e number o f cases
to sleep clinics for evaluation.
increases as people age and the symptoms can
become
more
intrusive
with
time.
Approximately 4 3 % o f people with R L S
Narcolepsy
describe the onset of the disorder before age
Narcolepsy is a disorder distinguished by
2 0 years (Hening et al., 1 9 9 9 ) . Prevalence rates
the rapid onset o f a sleep cycle, typically
for P L M D in people age 6 0 years or over range
triggered by periods o f excitement. People
from 2 0 % to 5 8 % (Dickel & M o s k o , 1 9 9 0 ) .
often have their first episode o f narcolepsy between ages 1 0 and 3 0 years (Hauri
&
Linde, 1 9 9 6 ) . Reports indicate that there are
Sleep Apnea
100,000
T h e definition o f a sleep apnea episode is
to
600,000
narcoleptics in
the
United States (Hauri & Linde, 1 9 9 6 ) . Narcolepsy is characterized by cataplexy
the cessation of airflow through the nose or mouth that lasts 1 0 seconds or longer. Sleep
(i.e., lack of muscle tone), hypnagogic halluci-
apnea can be caused by a variety o f conditions
nations (i.e., dreamlike hallucinations), and
and can be exacerbated by alcohol use or aller-
sleep paralysis. During a narcoleptic episode,
gies. There are three types of sleep apnea: cen-
the patient immediately will go into R E M sleep.
tral, obstructive, and mixed. Central sleep
Patients with narcolepsy often report vivid
apnea is the cessation of breathing due to lack
dreams or hallucinations. Obviously, this disor-
of respiratory effort. Obstructive sleep apnea
der is an extremely dangerous condition given
is characterized by sufficient respiratory effort
that people can fall asleep without warning
from the lungs but blockage (obstruction) o f
while driving or operating heavy machinery.
the airway. T h e mixed category refers to apnea
T h e cause o f narcolepsy appears to be
in which the obstructive phase follows a cen-
genetic, and treatment is usually medication.
tral phase, thereby combining both central and
Both stimulants
and antidepressants
have
obstructive sleep apnea. People o f any age can
been prescribed, and some patients find relief
have sleep apnea, although it is more c o m m o n
by
in older adults and in those who are obese.
recently, modafinil has improved
Patients should
be evaluated
for
sleep
taking
naps
during
the
day.
More
wakeful-
ness in patients with this problem. Although
apnea if they are experiencing excessive day-
untreated
time sleepiness. This manifests in falling asleep
rapidly and without warning during the day,
narcoleptic patients
fall
asleep
Insomnia
and the Sleep Disorders
\
they often report difficulty in falling asleep
total hours o f sleep achieved, sleep habits,
at night.
snoring, and level of fatigue during the day. Diet, caffeine, alcohol, tobacco, medications, herbs, medical conditions, and exercise should
Dreams, Nightmares, Sleep Terrors, and Sleepwalking
also be evaluated. It is important to note that patients will frequently underestimate
People dream during R E M sleep; there-
the
amount of sleep they achieve. Therefore, it can
fore, dreams typically occur at the end o f
be useful to interview a spouse to corroborate
the night when R E M sleep is longer. People
the information given. Another method o f
dream for about 2 hours every night and
evaluating sleep patterns is to have the patient
recall dreams
complete
if awakened
quickly
from
a sleep journal.
Unfortunately,
R E M sleep. Dreams can also be remembered
patients often have difficulty in complying with
through specific training strategies designed
this recommendation, or they report inaccu-
to enhance recall.
rate information.
Throughout history, people have looked to
W h e n self-report techniques are insufficient
their dreams for meaning and prophecy. There
for diagnosis and treatment planning for a
are varying theories as to the importance o f
sleep disorder, a patient may benefit from
dreams; some psychologists (e.g., Carl Jung)
a polysomnogram test. A
have spent much o f their careers investigating
involves using electroencephalography (EEG)
the role of dreams as a window to the uncon-
to monitor brain and muscle activity, heart
scious. Other professionals believe that dreams
rate, and respiration. By interpreting brain
are simply people's interpretations o f the phys-
waves and muscle activity, a specialist can
iological arousal that occurs with R E M sleep
identify the type o f sleep disorder.
polysomnogram
(Walsleben & Baron-Faust, 2 0 0 0 ) . Regardless,
If a patient complains o f loud snoring and
many laypeople believe that dreams are mean-
reports periods o f n o breathing followed by
ingful and are disturbed
gasps for air, an evaluation for possible sleep
when they have
anxiety-producing dreams or nightmares. Sleep terrors typically occur in children. Unlike nightmares, sleep terrors occur earlier
apnea should be arranged. A test for blood oxygen levels, called oximetry, is often used to evaluate for sleep apnea.
in the evening during the deep delta sleep (Stages 3 and 4 ) . Nightmares tend to occur toward the end o f the night when R E M sleep
Treatment of Insomnia
is longer. Because children have more delta
Treatment o f insomnia integrates sleep
sleep than do adults, children are more prone
hygiene (i.e., good sleep habits), stimulus con-
to night terrors.
trol, relaxation training, and sleep restriction components. These behavioral strategies are effective for 7 0 % to 8 0 % o f patients with
ASSESSMENT AND T R E A T M E N T
Evaluation/Assessment Phases
primary insomnia (Morin et al., 1 9 9 9 ) . In a review o f the literature on the use o f nonpharmacological treatments for insomnia, M o r i n and colleagues ( 1 9 9 9 ) found that
patients
Because the etiologies o f sleep disorders are
treated with behavioral interventions were
multifaceted, a thorough evaluation is impera-
"better off" (i.e., they fell asleep faster after
tive to ensure appropriate treatment planning.
treatment)
An initial interview should include questions
untreated
regarding sleep onset, number o f awakenings,
taught cognitive-behavioral skills slept longer,
than
approximately
c o n t r o l s . In addition,
8 0 % of patients
429
430
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS shorter
First, they are to use their beds for sex and
time periods, and reported higher sleep quality
awakened less frequently
and for
sleep only; they are not to read, watch televi-
after treatment than did 5 0 % to 7 0 % o f
sion, or do business or other work in bed; and
untreated controls (Morin et al., 1 9 9 9 ) .
they are not to stay in bed if awake for longer
Improvements in sleep were maintained on
than 2 0 minutes at a time. Second, they are to
follow-up. After 3 and 6 months, patients given
go to bed only when sleepy. Third, they are to
cognitive-behavioral intervention
maintain a regular wake-up time. Fourth,
maintained
gains realized in sleep onset and number of
they are to avoid napping during the day.
awakenings (Morin et al., 1 9 9 9 ) . In fact, some studies show that patients actually sleep longer at night 6 months following treatment than during the initial posttreatment
Sleep Restriction.
T h e goal o f sleep restric-
tion is to train patients to sleep more efficiently
assessment
by limiting the amount o f time spent in bed.
(Morin et al., 1 9 9 9 ) . This may be because
This strategy capitalizes on fatigue so as to
patients need a few months to fully integrate
train their bodies to fall asleep quickly and
behavioral strategies into their lives. Additional
to sleep through the night. Patients are first
long-term
benefits o f cognitive-behavioral
instructed to compute their sleep efficiency by
training for insomnia include a reduction in
dividing their time spent sleeping by their time
medication use and increased independence
spent in bed. They estimate the average
from medical intervention (Morin et al., 1 9 9 9 ) .
amount of time they spend sleeping at night.
Reflecting the breadth o f literature support-
N e x t , they choose a wake-up
time
that
ing the use of behavioral treatments for insom-
they will adhere to every day of the week.
nia and the side effect profiles of most hypnotics,
Additional recommendations are to avoid nap-
current
the
ping and to get out o f bed if still awake after
the
2 0 minutes (e.g., from stimulus control train-
best practice guidelines from
American
Medical
Association
and
Canadian Medical Association indicate that
ing). After the assessment and setup phase,
behavioral treatment should be the frontline
patients select one o f the following techniques.
treatment for insomnia. However, because of inadequate provider training and an over-
Significantly
delay going
to bed. This strat-
reliance on the "quick fix" (i.e., medication),
egy is the most difficult because it requires the
physicians rarely use structured behavioral treat-
patient to go to bed much later than usual
ments to treat insomnia. Instead, most people
while keeping his or her wake-up time con-
who suffer from insomnia use over-the-counter
stant. First, the patient determines a wake-up
medications or prescription sleep aids.
time that can be adhered to strictly. T h e initial bedtime is established using the
Core
Components
Behavioral
of
Treatment
Cognitivefor
Insomnia
average
number of hours o f sleep a patient achieves. For example, if a woman typically spends 9 hours in bed but only sleeps 6 hours because
Stimulus con-
of waking, the recommendation is to spend
trol therapy teaches patients to use environ-
only 6 hours in bed. If her wake-up time is
mental cues to augment, rather than inhibit,
6 : 0 0 a.m., she would go to bed at midnight the
sleep. T h e premise o f the therapy is that indi-
first night. She would follow the stimulus con-
viduals will be reconditioned to sleep effi-
trol strategy and get up if she is not asleep
ciently when sleep-incompatible activities are
within 2 0 minutes. She would also get out of
Stimulus
Control
Therapy.
reduced and sleep-associated activities are
bed if she wakes for longer than 2 0 minutes
increased. Patients receive the following basic
during the night. T h e patient then would con-
rules pertaining to the bedroom environment.
tinue to go to bed at midnight until she achieves
Insomnia 90%
and the Sleep Disorders
\
sleep efficiency. When this marker is
to bed too early and by failing to maintain a
reached, she would go to bed 15 minutes
consistent sleep schedule. Typically, patients will
earlier, stopping at the hour at which she can
have one or two bad nights o f sleep because of
maintain a sleep efficiency o f 8 5 % or better and
a transient event and will adjust their bedtime
wake feeling rested and refreshed. The first night
to be earlier because they feel fatigued. This is
of this program is the most difficult because
problematic because the body is not accustomed
patients who are already fatigued have a difficult
to spending additional time in bed, and in an
time staying up later than usual. Napping during
attempt to accommodate the change in bedtime,
the day or falling asleep briefly before bedtime
sleep cycles are altered and sleep becomes less
will usually sabotage this strategy.
efficient. W h e n informed o f this pattern, patients
Delay bedtime
by 15 minutes.
This strategy
advises the patient to go to bed 1 5 minutes
often
identify that
their sleep difficulties
began when they initiated a change in their
later than usual every night until he or she is
bedtime by going to bed earlier due to a
able to fall asleep quickly and sleep through
partner's schedule, an illness, or a stressful
the night. Therefore, the previously mentioned
period.
patient would still have a consistent wake-up
patients learn after a few nights o f poor sleep
time of 6 : 0 0 a.m. and would still follow the
to go to bed a little later. T h e y understand
20-minute rule, but instead o f going to bed at
that attempts to address a few nights o f poor
9:00 p.m., she would go to bed at 9 : 1 5 p.m. If
sleep by going to bed earlier is not effective,
she cannot achieve 8 5 % sleep efficiency after a
even though going to bed earlier makes intu-
few nights, she would go to bed at 9 : 3 0 p.m.
itive sense if a person is fatigued.
With
appropriate
information,
Eventually, this patient may find that if she goes to bed at 1 0 : 4 5 p.m., she is able to fall asleep quickly and sleep through the night.
Sleep
Hygiene
(i.e.,
good
sleep
habits).
Sleep hygiene refers to health practices (e.g., diet, exercise, substance use) and environmen-
Use an intermediate The
delay in going
to bed.
third strategy is an intermediate
one
between the first two strategies. T h e patient
tal factors (e.g., noise, light, a comfortable bed, a m b i e n t temperature)
that
may
be
inhibiting sleep. In terms o f health practices,
first sets a consistent wake-up time, avoids
patients are instructed to reduce caffeine use
napping, and follows the 20-minute rule. T h e
to less than five cups o f coffee or tea a day and
patient is then instructed to go to bed as late as
to avoid using any caffeine after 2 : 0 0 p.m.
possible. This intermediate strategy works well
Caffeine has a half-life o f 4 hours; therefore,
for many patients because they cannot stay up
half o f the caffeine consumed at 4 : 0 0 p.m. will
late enough to use the first strategy. Also, this
still be in the body at 8 : 0 0 p.m., and a quarter
third strategy tends to work more quickly than
of the initial cup o f coffee or tea consumed at
the second strategy. T h e previously mentioned
4 : 0 0 p.m. will be in the body at midnight.
patient would continue to stay up late until
Patients should also limit nicotine intake
she achieves above 8 5 % sleep efficiency. Then,
and never smoke if they wake during the
she would go to bed 15 minutes earlier until she
night. This suggestion will help to eliminate
found the time when she could maintain good
waking to smoke. Frequently, smokers classi-
sleep efficiency and feel rested during the day.
cally condition themselves to wake at the end
These sleep restriction techniques capitalize
o f each 90-minute sleep cycle for a cigarette.
on patients' fatigue to help them learn, adopt,
Finally, patients are told to refrain from using
and maintain good sleep habits. M a n y insomni-
alcohol as a sleep-inducing aid.
acs actually cause their own insomnia by going
alcohol can make people feel sleepy, it inhibits
Although
431
432
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS good sleep cycles and can cause nighttime
modest response to this intervention (Morin
waking.
et al., 1 9 9 9 ) . In general, sleep hygiene is con-
In terms o f sleep hygiene, patients are also educated about the benefits o f daily exercise
sidered a necessary part o f an intervention but insufficient as a stand-alone treatment.
for sleep but are cautioned to avoid vigorous exercise t o o close to bedtime. Exercising intensely before going to bed c a n cause problems with falling asleep because
Relaxation-Based
Relaxa-
Interventions.
tion techniques have also proved to be useful
people
in insomnia treatment. Studies have shown
may find themselves t o o energized (due to
that progressive muscle relaxation, biofeed-
the release of the endorphins) or t o o hot to
back, imagery training, and thought stopping
sleep. T h e body naturally cools as it prepares
all can reduce autonomic arousal and facilitate
to sleep. Therefore, if people are t o o hot due
sleep (Morin et al., 1 9 9 9 ) . Biofeedback and
to exercise or hot summer temperatures, they
progressive muscle relaxation help individuals
often have a hard time sleeping.
to relax the muscles o f the body and activate
In addition, individuals need to make sure
the parasympathetic nervous system or relax-
that their rooms are dark and quiet and that
ation response. Imagery training and thought
their beds are comfortable. Because light
stopping help to retrain the mind to focus on
helps to reset the biological clock, it is essen-
calming thoughts, thereby reducing the sym-
tial to avoid bright lights in the evening and to
pathetic nervous system response and hope-
expose the body to bright light in the morning.
fully activating the parasympathetic nervous
Shift workers can have a difficult time falling
system.
asleep after the "graveyard shift," particularly
interventions have been advocated for insom-
Although
other
relaxation-based
if they drive home in the morning light with-
nia (e.g., diaphragmatic breathing, meditation, hypnosis), these have not been adequately
out sunglasses. Patients sometimes state that their partners
evaluated (Morin et al., 1 9 9 9 ) .
snore or that there is intermittent noise com-
Relaxation-based interventions are impor-
ing from the street that keeps them awake at
tant in facilitating sleep. M a n y insomniacs
night. Unfortunately, intermittent noise is the
become so anxious about falling asleep that
worst type o f noise for sleep. T h e body can
they activate the sympathetic nervous sys-
acclimate to consistent noise (e.g., the sound
tem, thereby releasing endorphins,
of crickets or o f a nearby freeway) or to total
the muscles, and increasing heartbeat and
quiet, but intermittent noise can cause waking.
respiration rate. As one might expect, this
tensing
Suggestions for managing this include using
response is incompatible with falling asleep.
white noise (e.g., turning on a fan or a fish
T h r o u g h classical conditioning (i.e., anxiety
tank) and using earplugs. It is not uncommon
regarding falling asleep that is then paired
for couples to sleep apart if one snores loudly
with one's bed), a transient sleep problem
or has P L M D .
can become chronic. Insomniacs w h o have
M o s t often, sleep hygiene rules are inte-
developed a strong association between a n x -
grated into a treatment program. A few stud-
iety and their beds often say that they can fall
ies have evaluated the effectiveness o f using
asleep easily in a hotel r o o m or in the guest
sleep hygiene education alone and
r o o m but can never fall asleep quickly in
have
found a modest effect (Morin et al., 1 9 9 9 ) .
their own beds. T h e primary goal o f the
O n e study found that approximately 2 7 % of
relaxation therapies is to reassociate the bed
patients improved when given sleep hygiene
with a restful state and help reduce
education
(Schoicket, Bertelson, &
Lacks,
1 9 8 8 ) , but other studies have found a more
anxiety o r a u t o n o m i c arousal with sleep.
the
associated
Insomnia Paradoxical
Paradoxical intention
Intention.
included
and the Sleep Disorders
information
regarding
\
stimulus
is a cognitive technique in which the patient is
control, sleep restriction, and thought stopping.
requested to stay awake. If the patient is
The final section included a review of sleep
instructed to stay awake, falling asleep is no
hygiene, exercise, sleeping pills, herbs, and
longer the anxiety-producing goal. Thus, by
melatonin. T h e information regarding sleep
prescribing the patient's worst fear—staying
cycles and sleep architecture was designed to
awake—the patient paradoxically responds by
provide the supporting rationale for implemen-
falling asleep more quickly. In essence, this inter-
tation of sleep restriction, stimulus control, and
vention helps to reduce the performance anxiety
sleep hygiene. Patients created a personalized
associated with sleeping.
sleep plan during the group and individual appointments (Wolfe &c Helge, 2 0 0 2 ) .
Developing Sleep
a
One
Cognitive-Behavioral
Improvement
month
after
treatment,
patients'
progress was assessed by telephone follow-up.
Program
Results indicated that 9 0 % of the patients
M o s t of the treatment programs in use
who had individual appointments and 7 4 % of
today include multiple sessions and integrate
the patients who attended group appointments
stimulus control, sleep hygiene, relaxation
improved the amount of time spent sleeping at
training, and sleep restriction components.
night (Wolfe &
These programs are often six to eight sessions
gained an average o f 1.78 hours of sleep per
in length and may require additional sessions
night, and group members gained an average of
for screening and paperwork. Evaluations o f
1.90 hours of sleep per night (Wolfe & Helge,
Helge, 2 0 0 2 ) .
Individuals
these programs are often dependent on patient
2 0 0 2 ) . A second cohort o f patients (n = 35) who
self-report through sleep diaries, although a
participated in the same treatment protocol, but
few
with a different health psychologist, had results
studies use p o l y s o m n o g r a m
tests
to
slightly better than participants in the initial
measure outcome. Sacramento,
cohort (Wolfe, Helge, & Jacobs, 2 0 0 2 ) . One
California, a single-session treatment program
month after treatment, 8 7 % of these patients
was developed and evaluated using individual
reported improved sleep by an average of 2 . 3 8
(n = 2 0 ) and group (n = 7 2 ) formats (Wolfe &
hours per night (Wolfe et al., 2 0 0 2 ) . All partici-
Helge,
pants were to be reassessed 1 year following
At
Kaiser Permanente
2002).
Primary
in
care
physicians
referred patients to a health psychologist, who
treatment to determine whether treatment gains
administered an insomnia treatment protocol,
were maintained.
individual sessions were 2 5 minutes, and the
Anecdotal comments from
participants
group sessions were 9 0 minutes. Before treat-
were interesting. A c o m m o n theme was skep-
ment, each patient completed a brief question-
ticism about the techniques. F o r example, a
naire that included queries about sleep onset,
typical remark was " I thought w h a t you sug-
awakenings, daytime fatigue, and sleep habits.
gested was silly, but I decided to try it any-
In addition, there were questions regarding sub-
w a y . " Another typical patient c o m m e n t was
stance use, diet, and exercise. Approximately
" I did not think it would w o r k , but I was
7 5 % o f referred patients attended the program
amazed at the results." Finally, some patients
(Wolfe & Helge, 2 0 0 2 ) .
noted benefits from the information about
The first section o f the intervention was informational, including information
about
insomnia: " I thought I had depression, but n o w I realize I was just sleep deprived."
sleep cycles, sleep architecture, individual vari-
This intervention was developed with the
ations in sleep, and sleep changes over the life
purpose of efficiently and effectively treating
cycle. T h e second section o f the intervention
primary care patients with insomnia. T h e
433
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS
434
groups were not limited to specific populations;
prescribing, a tapering strategy should be in
any patients complaining o f sleep problems
place to minimize risk for potential problems.
were considered appropriate. However, if a
O n e strategy to avoid tolerance is to have the
physician believed that a patient would not
patient use the medication only 3 or 4 nights
do well in a group setting, or if a patient could
a week and to use the lowest effective dose. A
not attend because o f scheduling conflicts, the
patient who has an abuse history or who has
patient was seen individually. T h e findings of
to perform tasks that require alertness and
this uncontrolled clinical trial indicate that a
quick reaction times (e.g., driving) on awak-
single-session brief intervention that integrates
ening should be cautioned when prescribed
evidence-based interventions can be extremely
sleep aids. Finally, hypnotics prescribed to
effective for primary care patients experiencing
elder adults require extra care.
insomnia. However, because this study is lim-
Trazadone is an antidepressant frequently
ited from a methodological perspective, further
prescribed for people w h o have insomnia
research regarding the utility o f brief, behav-
and
iorally based interventions in primary care is
Food
essential.
a depressive disorder. and
However,
D r u g Administration
has
the not
approved the use o f trazadone for people with insomnia. Trazadone should be used
Medications
Used to Treat
with caution because it can cause daytime
Insomnia
sedation, priapism, and hypotension.
In today's "quick f i x " society, patients
Melatonin is a hormone often used for insom-
and providers alike have a penchant for med-
nia. It is released by the pineal gland into the
ications as the frontline treatment for insom-
bloodstream and is produced from tryptophan.
nia. Even before office visits, patients have
Tryptophan is converted to 5-hydroxytrypto-
often
medications
phan, then to serotonin, then to N-acetylsero-
such as diphenhydramine and Tylenol P M
tonin, and finally to melatonin. Melatonin
tried
over-the-counter
(a c o m b i n a t i o n o f diphenhydramine
and
regulates the body's sleep cycle, circadian
Tylenol) for sleep. I f these medications were
rhythm, and endocrine production
as effective as advertised, it is unlikely that
essential for sexual maturation, growth control,
patients would continue to seek assistance
pain control, balance, and regulation of sexual
from their primary care providers.
and is
activity (Natural Medicines Comprehensive
Benzodiazepines are c o m m o n l y prescribed
Database, 2 0 0 2 ) . People produce more mela-
for sleep. These agents replaced barbiturates
tonin when it is dark, thereby stimulating the
and
onset o f sleep. It may be an effective agent to
barbiturate-like substances that
were
associated with addiction, respiratory prob-
help some adults with sleep onset. Moreover,
lems, and occasional deaths. A benefit o f
melatonin may be useful for symptoms of jet
benzodiazepines and other hypnotics used
lag, for shift workers, and for insomniacs with
for
blindness. However, data demonstrating the
insomnia is that
they w o r k
quickly.
However, hypnotics can cause daytime sedation, tolerance, and rebound insomnia. Current
guidelines from
the
National
efficacy of melatonin are inconclusive. Although most consider melatonin relatively safe, it is not for use in children because it may
Institutes o f Health ( 1 9 8 4 , 1 9 9 1 ) suggest that
affect their maturation. There also is concern
short-term use o f hypnotic medications may
that taking melatonin may increase daytime
be indicated for acute insomnia, but these
somnolence.
medications should be used with caution. If
interfere with the effectiveness of cardiac medi-
prescribed for sleep, hypnotics should not be
cation and medications used to reduce immune
taken for more than 2 weeks. At the time o f
system response (e.g., steroids, other cortisone
In
addition,
melatonin
may
Insomnia
and the Sleep
435
Disorders
drugs). There is some indication that melatonin
reports suggest that the selective serotonin
can help depression, particularly when
the
reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) or tricyclic medications
depression comes with insomnia, but other
reduce symptoms o f R L S , whereas other reports
studies indicate that melatonin can
indicate that these medications can aggravate
make
depressive symptoms worse (Natural Medi-
the disorder (Hening et al., 1 9 9 9 ) .
cines Comprehensive Database, 2 0 0 2 ) . Interestingly, it has been documented that patients with depression and patients diagnosed with
Treatment of Sleep Apnea
can have low levels of melatonin
Treatment of sleep apnea is dependent on
(Natural Medicines Comprehensive Database,
the etiology. For example, in children, a com-
2002).
mon cause of sleep apnea is enlarged tonsils.
fibromyalgia
With tonsil removal, the apnea can be relieved. Other surgical techniques, lauded as permanent
Treatment of RLS and PLMD
cures for obstructive sleep apnea, have not been
Behavioral strategies for treating R L S and
as effective as originally expected. With patients
P L M D include moderate amounts of exercise
who are obese, the best treatment is weight
in the evening, hot baths, and
distraction.
reduction because this serves to open adequate
Walking before bedtime has been helpful for
space for airflow. One of the most successful
some patients. However,
treatments for other patients with obstructive
intense exercise
appears to exacerbate the symptoms for many
sleep apnea is the continuous positive airway
sufferers. H o t baths are effective for some
pressure machine (CPAP).
patients, and distraction exercises (i.e., tasks
T h e CPAP provides continuous
positive
that require intense concentration) have sup-
airway pressure through the nostrils to force
port for symptom reduction. In addition, some
the airway clear, enabling the patient
patients find relief from massage or vibrating
breathe. This treatment is effective for many
stimulation before sleep, and there is some evi-
people, but a major complaint is discomfort in
dence that reducing caffeine and
improving
wearing the apparatus. In fact, many o f the
sleep habits are useful. M o s t of the published
patients given a CPAP do not adhere to rec-
literature has focused on medication treatment
ommendations to use it. It is critical that the
for R L S and P L M D , but there is some evidence
facemask fits properly and that the patient
to support integrating behavioral strategies
acclimates to the machine. This is often done
with pharmacological regimens.
by training the patient in relaxation tech-
to
Unfortunately, many of the medications used
niques and having him or her use the machine
in treating R L S and P L M D have limited effec-
while awake but during sedentary activities
tiveness and can cause tolerance and rebound
such as watching television. Having a machine
symptoms. There are five classes o f medications
force air through one's nose can be extremely
typically prescribed: dopaminergic medications,
uncomfortable, especially while trying to initi-
opioids, benzodiazepines, adrenergic medica-
ate sleep onset. Therefore, adequate training
tions, and anticonvulsant medications. A review
and problem solving are essential given that
of the costs and benefits of these agents is
the consequences o f living with
beyond the scope o f this chapter, but Hening
sleep apnea are unfortunate and avoidable.
untreated
and colleagues (1999) provided an excellent review of the treatments for R L S and P L M D . Folic acid and iron supplements have proved
Light Therapy
to be effective for patients with R L S and P L M D
Light therapy has been investigated as a treat-
who are deficient in these minerals. Some
ment for patients with a variety of problems,
436
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS O F MEDICAL PROBLEMS including seasonal affective disorder, shift work
should last between 1 5 and 4 0 minutes or be
sleep, delayed sleep phase syndrome,
2 hours in length. A brief nap can be very
and
advanced sleep phase syndrome. Light therapy
restorative. T h e most favorable time to take a
also has been evaluated as a potential solution
nap is early afternoon. This is when people's
for sleep complaints in the elderly (Chesson
natural biological clock indicates that it may be
et al., 1999).
time for a sleep cycle. Some patients successin
fully time their naps halfway between the time
treating delayed sleep phase syndrome and
they awaken in the morning and the time they
advanced sleep phase syndrome
(Chesson
go to bed, finding a 1 5 - or 20-minute nap to be
et al., 1 9 9 9 ) . With delayed sleep phase syn-
adequate to renew their concentration and
In general, light therapy is effective
drome, the individual has difficulty in initiat-
energy. However, insomniacs should be wary
ing sleep at the appropriate time and awakens
of napping. Napping late in the afternoon can
too late. With advanced sleep phase syndrome,
negatively influence nighttime sleep. If an
the patient falls asleep too early at night and
insomniac has rested during the day, he or she
awakens too early in the morning. With both
might not be fatigued enough to fall asleep
sleep phase disorders, the release o f hormones
quickly at night.
and the level o f body temperature indicate that the person's sleep phase is not oriented correctly. Having individuals expose themselves to bright light in the morning and wear darkened glasses at night appears to be effective in resetting the circadian rhythm
Treatment of Nightmares and Night Terrors Nightmares tend t o be more c o m m o n
and
when patients are under stress. There are two
resuming a regular sleep schedule (Chesson
main treatments to help patients cope with
et al., 1 9 9 9 ) . In addition, light therapy appears
undesirable dreams. T h e first involves reas-
to be useful with seasonal affective disorder
surance that intense dreams are c o m m o n and
(Chesson et al., 1 9 9 9 ) .
is
that dreaming about something is not an
described as a safe intervention when used
indication that one would act in a similar
according to American Academy o f Sleep
manner in real life. Often, patients present
Light therapy
Medicine guidelines. It may be useful for shift
with sexual or violent dreams and express
work problems, jet lag, and
non-24-hour
concern that these dreams are indicative o f
sleep/wake syndrome in some blind patients
underlying desires. Through reassurance and
(Chesson et al., 1 9 9 9 ) .
normalization, their distress can be reduced. Recommendations to avoid the daily news,
Napping and Sleep Deprivation
violent movies, and other negative images before bedtime also are helpful. In addition,
Approximately a quarter o f all Americans
patients can learn to dream about pleasant
are sleep deprived (National Sleep Foundation,
scenes by rehearsing these images before bed.
2 0 0 2 ) . This translates into billions o f dollars in
For recurrent nightmares (e.g., being chased
lost productivity and accidents. In response to
or pummeled by a foe), patients are instructed
this pervasive sleep debt, many businesses have
to return to the dream and change the ending.
created special environments for napping, and
This is particularly helpful with children who
there have been articles in the popular press
are taught to imagine themselves as their
describing the benefit o f quick "power naps."
favorite superhero and then turning on the foe
Sleep "debt" can be "repaid" with a brief
and winning the battle. F o r this strategy to be
nap during waking hours; however, there is an
effective, the patient should create the image
optimal strategy for napping. Ideally, a nap
that makes the most sense to him or her to be
Insomnia
and the Sleep
Disorders
CASE S T U D Y " J o h n D o e , " a 39-year-old male, presented to his primary care physician with symptoms o f anxiety, irritability, decreased concentration, headaches, and insomnia. H e stated that his problems began about 5 years ago when he relocated, married, and changed his j o b . H e reported feeling happy with his current situation and satisfied with his marital relationship and his j o b , but he revealed that he felt "run down." "When queried a b o u t sleep habits, J o h n denied having difficulty in falling asleep but stated that he awakened frequently during the night. H e said that he went t o bed at 9 : 3 0 p.m. because he was " t o o tired t o stay up any later," but then he awakened at 3 : 3 0 a.m. and drifted in and out o f sleep until it was time t o get ready for w o r k at 6 : 3 0 . H e was concerned that he m a y be depressed because he felt " w o r n out and s a d " even though his life was g o o d . J o h n noted that his sleep habits changed following his marriage. W h e n single, he would go to bed at midnight and get up at 6 : 3 0 a.m. H e reported that with this schedule, he felt rested and refreshed. However, when he married, he began to go to bed at 1 0 : 0 0 p.m. because this was his wife's bedtime. During the past year, he altered his bedtime to 9 : 3 0 p.m. in an effort to c o m b a t his ever-present fatigue. H e was concerned that his lack o f energy was beginning to affect his work and relationships. A physical e x a m i n a t i o n determined that J o h n w a s in g o o d health. L a b o r a t o r y results were within n o r m a l limits. T h e patient w a s instructed t o use sleep restriction to retrain his body to sleep well. Specifically, he was t o go t o bed later in the evening until he could fall asleep quickly and sleep through the night. H e was reminded that he felt rested after 6 . 5 hours o f sleep and that his previous sleep schedule o f going t o bed at midnight and waking at 6 : 3 0 a.m. w a s effective for him. After 1 week, J o h n reported feeling much better. H e stated that going t o bed later was very difficult at first because he was so tired, but after 3 nights his sleep had improved and he began t o feel better. In addition, he started walking daily at lunch and found that his energy level and m o o d had improved. During a follow-up visit, his progress was reviewed and recidivism was discussed. J o h n was quite pleased with his improvements because he felt more like himself again. In addition, he reported that his m o o d and concentration were better and that his headaches had decreased. One year later, he denied any ongoing problems with sleep. H e noted that during periods o f transient insomnia, he continued to use sleep restriction strategies that provide him with rapid improvement.
victorious. Nightmares can also be reduced
screaming; children m a y
open their eyes,
through psychotherapy that addresses underly-
begin perspiring, and appear t o be experienc-
ing anxiety.
ing a state o f panic. After a few minutes, a
Night terrors can be particularly alarming
calm state o f sleep returns. Usually, an indi-
to parents. T h e y often involve brief periods o f
vidual experiencing a night terror will not
437
438
BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF MEDICAL PROBLEMS awaken and will not remember the incident in
productivity, health care, and accidents. For
the morning. Night terrors are more frequent
the majority o f sleep problems,
during periods o f stress. They cause no harm
insomnia and nightmares, cognitive-behavioral
including
to the individual, so an optimal coping strat-
intervention is the treatment o f choice. For
egy is reassurance for the parents. Parents are
other sleep disorders, such as sleep apnea, R L S ,
instructed to either gently comfort the child
and P L M D , behavioral interventions play a
or ignore the disruption altogether.
significant role. However, it is far from routine
Three additional strategies can be helpful in
practice for patients with sleep problems to
reducing night terrors. First, psychotherapy for
receive these interventions or learn about them
stress reduction is useful. Second, advising the
during visits to primary care clinics.
patient to sleep for a longer period, thereby
Enhanced training during medical school has
reducing the amount o f deep delta sleep, can
been recommended as one strategy to improve
help. Third, physicians may prescribe a benzo-
assessment and intervention for patients with
diazepine, such as diazepam, for a few days to
sleep problems (Dement & Vaughan, 1 9 9 9 ) .
reduce the delta sleep associated with night ter-
Unfortunately, physician training continues to
rors. This last strategy may be helpful if a child
be limited in this regard, and this deficiency is
is sleeping away from home and is concerned
reflected in clinical practice; medications con-
about arousing others. However,
tinue as the first line of intervention for sleep
benzodi-
azepines should always be used with caution.
problems that would be better treated with
Sleepwalking is also associated with delta
behavioral interventions. This observation is not
sleep and is more c o m m o n with children than
too surprising. Physicians generally report diffi-
with adults. It is also more frequent when
culties in identifying behavioral problems in
people are under stress or sleep deprived,
their patients and frustration with implementing
and it can be a result of certain medications.
behavioral change strategies (Alto, 1 9 9 5 ) .
Sleepwalking
can
be
quite
dangerous.
A realistic practice solution is access to
Treatments include reducing stress, getting
behavioral experts who can provide effective
adequate sleep, ensuring a safe sleeping envi-
cognitive-behavioral interventions in primary
ronment, and taking medication.
care. This strategy is practical because many patients with a sleep disorder will also present with medical and psychiatric complaints. A
CONCLUSIONS
behavioral-medical approach, the integration of medical and behavioral sciences, appears to
Each year, sleep disorders affect millions o f
be an excellent solution for the sizable popula-
people and cost billions of dollars in lost
tion o f those suffering with sleep disorders.
REFERENCES Alto, W.A. (1995). Prevention in practice. Primary Care, 22, 5 4 3 - 5 5 4 . American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Chesson, A. L., Littner, M., Davila, D., Anderson, M., Grigg-Damberger, M., Hartse, K., Johnson, S., & Wise, M. (1999). Practice parameters for the use of light therapy in the treatment of sleep disorders. Sleep, 22, 6 4 1 - 6 6 0 . Dement, W. C , & Vaughan, C. (1999). The promise of sleep. New York: Delacorte.
Insomnia
and the Sleep
Dickel, M . J . , Se Mosko, S. S. (1990). Morbidity cut-offs for sleep apnea and periodic leg movements in predicting subjective complaints in seniors. Sleep, 13, 1 5 5 - 1 6 6 . Ford, D. E., Sc Kamerow, D. B . (1989). Epidemiologic study of sleep disturbance and psychiatric disorders: An opportunity for prevention? Journal of the American Medical Association, 262, 1 4 7 9 - 1 4 8 4 . Gallup Organization. (1995). Sleep in America. Princeton, NJ: Author. (Poll conducted for the National Sleep Foundation) Gupta, Μ . Α., Gupta, A. K., Se Haberman, H. F. (1986). Psychotropic drugs in dermatology: A review and guidelines for use. Journal of the American Academy of Dermatology, 14, 6 3 3 - 6 4 5 . Hauri, P., Sc Hawkins, D. R. (1973). Alpha-delta sleep. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 34, 2 3 3 - 2 3 7 . Hauri, P., Sc Linde, S. (1996). No more sleepless nights. New York: John Wiley. Hening, W., Allen, R., Earley, C. E., Kushida, C., Picchietti, D., Sc Silber, M . (1999). The treatment of restless leg syndrome and periodic limb movement disorder. Sleep, 22, 9 7 0 - 9 9 9 . Mellinger, G. D., Baiter, M . B., Sc Uhlenhuth, Ε. H. (1985). Insomnia and its treatment. Archives of General Psychiatry, 42, 2 2 5 - 2 3 2 . Moffitt, P. F., Kalucy, E. C., Kalucy, R. S., Baum, F. E., Sc Cooke, R. D. (1991). Sleep difficulties, pain, and other correlates. Journal of Internal Medicine, 230, 245-249. Moldofsky, H. (1989). Sleep and fibrositis syndrome. Rheumatic Disease Clinics of North America, 15(1), 9 1 - 1 0 3 . Morin, C. M., Hauri, P. J . , Espie, C. Α., Spielman, A. J . , Buyesse, D. J . , Se Bootzin, R. R. ( 1 9 9 9 ) . Nonpharmacologic treatment of chronic insomnia. Sleep, 22, 1134-1155. National Institutes of Health. (1984). Drugs and insomnia: The use of medication to promote sleep. Journal of the American Medical Association, 18, 2410-2414. National Institutes of Health. (1991). Consensus Development Conference statement: The treatment of sleep disorders of older people. Sleep, 14, 1 6 9 - 1 7 7 . National Sleep Foundation. (2002). Sleep in America poll. [Online]. Retrieved January 2 0 , 2 0 0 3 , from www.sleepfoundation.org Natural Medicines Comprehensive Database. (2002). Melatonin [monograph]. [Online]. Retrieved January 2 0 , 2 0 0 3 , from www.naturaldatabase.com/ monograph.asp?mono_id=9408chilite=l Perlis, M. L., Giles, D. E., Buyesse, D. J . , Tu, X . , Se Kupfer, D. J . (1997). Selfreported sleep disturbance as a prodromal symptom in recurrent depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 42, 2 0 9 - 2 1 2 . Pilowsky, L, Crettenden, L, Sc Townley, M . (1985). Sleep disturbance in pain clinic patients. Pain, 23, 2 7 - 3 3 . Schoicket, S. L., Bertelson, A. D., Sc Lacks, P. (1988). Is sleep hygiene a sufficient treatment for sleep maintenance insomnia? Behavior Therapy, 19, 1 8 3 - 1 9 0 . Walsleben, J . Α., Sc Baron-Faust, R. (2000). A woman's guide to sleep. New York: Crown. Wolfe, V. Α., Sc Helge, T. D. (2002, April). Utilizing a brief, behaviorally based intervention to treat insomnia in primary care. Paper presented at the meeting of the Society of Behavioral Medicine, Washington, DC. Wolfe, V. Α., Helge, T. D., Sc Jacobs, J . R. (2002, November). Effectiveness of a single session group appointment to treat insomnia in primary care. Poster presented at the meeting of the Association for Advancement of Behavior Therapy, Reno, NV.
Disorders
439
Part IV SPECIAL ISSUES Introduction
T
to Part IV
he final seven chapters o f this h a n d b o o k focus on topics that are critical for effective practice o f clinical health psychology; however, the issues are more crosscutting than those in the chapters in Parts II and III, which focused on
specific health behaviors and disease. As in any applied discipline, clinical health psy-
chologists must be sensitive to ethical issues, cultural and individual diversity, and the challenges that are unique t o this multidisciplinary field. T h e editors identified seven important topics and recruited authors to provide an overview o f the issues that are critical to clinical health psychology. Although not exhaustive o f the issues that will challenge the practitioner, the resulting chapters provide a solid foundation for the practice o f clinical health psychology. In Chapter 2 2 , Siegfried and Porter provide an overview o f the "Ethical Principles of Psychologists and C o d e o f C o n d u c t " adopted by the American Psychological Association and illustrate, through case vignettes, aspects o f the code that are o f particular importance t o the practice o f clinical health psychology. F o r each o f the principles o f the ethical code, practical advice is given and cases are used t o illustrate the issues. It is only through the maintenance o f the highest standards that clinical health psychology will have its largest influence. In Chapter 2 3 , M y e r s and H w a n g review the literature on ethnocultural issues in clinical health psychology. T h e chapter begins with a review o f the health disparity issues as they relate to various ethnic minority groups in the United States. T h e s e disparities are then discussed in terms o f a biopsychosocial model that appears t o be useful for understanding the role o f ethnic differences in functional health status. T h e
442
T H E HEALTH P S Y C H O L O G Y H A N D B O O K authors also provide an overview o f various treatment efforts that are tailored to minority communities. In addition, the chapter provides specific recommendations that should enhance the practice o f clinical health psychology. In Chapter 2 4 , C s o b o t h reviews special issues relevant t o w o m e n ' s health. T h e author provides a model that emphasizes the different life course events that w o m e n experience and discusses h o w w o m e n ' s health is differentially related t o the unique physiological and social changes during these life stages. Psychosocial factors such as stress, social support, role identity, and socioeconomic factors appear to differentially influence w o m e n and men. These differences are reviewed. T h e author also briefly reviews selected health problems that occur at greater rates in w o m e n , with an emphasis on the importance o f developing interventions specifically for w o m e n . In Chapter 2 5 , Edelstein and his colleague provide an in-depth review o f issues that are unique to working with geriatric populations. This is clearly an area where there is great demand for services and great need for specialized training. T h e authors begin with an in-depth discussion o f age-related changes, with a focus on those changes that are unique to older adults. Perhaps one o f the more critical sections o f this chapter is the discussion o f psychological manifestations and correlates o f physical disease. Effective practice o f clinical health psychology requires an understanding o f how many physical disease states in the elderly have natural symptoms that are consistent with psychiatric disorders. Recognition o f the cause o f the psychiatric symptoms is critical for working with this population. T h e authors review adverse medication effects and other problem areas that are unique to older adults. Finally, the chapter provides a thoughtful review of end-of-life issues, with a discussion o f h o w such decisions are made. In Chapter 2 6 , T u c k e r , K l a p o w , and Simpson focus their contribution on the public health approach to the treatment and prevention o f disease. T h e y challenge the notion that psychological practice needs t o be focused on the individual, and they encourage clinical health psychologists to consider the role that they can and should play in public health efforts that are m o r e population focused. T h e training program at the University o f A l a b a m a at Birmingham is presented as one model for such training, wherein students are provided with in-depth public health experiences while pursuing training in clinical health psychology. In Chapter 2 7 , Palm and her colleagues focus on the unique challenges and skills that are necessary t o conduct psychological research in medical settings. Using their o w n research as an illustrative model, the authors cover the basic questions o f the development o f a research question, design o f the study, recruitment o f participants, and recruitment o f support from administrators and staff. Problems that are unique t o medical institutions are openly discussed, and practical solutions are offered. T h e chapter concludes with a discussion o f ethical issues and conflict o f interest issues. Finally, in Chapter 2 8 , Borrego and Follette address program evaluation in medical settings. T h e authors provide a step-by-step outline for program evaluation that focuses on the development o f the question(s) to be addressed and proceeds to selection o f populations and assessment methods. A great deal o f attention is given to why program evaluation is needed and t o h o w t o conduct such evaluations. T h e authors make the case that the practice of clinical health psychology is greatly enhanced when practitioners actively evaluate the services they provide and modify services where needed.
CHAPTER
10 22
Ethical Issues for Clinicians in Behavioral Medicine Settings NICOLE J . SIEGFRIED AND CHEBON A . PORTER
T
he continued expansion and integration of health psychology into medical settings raises complex practical, ethical,
and professional issues. M u c h has been written on the logistics of integrating psychology into medicine with ethical issues often overlooked.
T h e "Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct"
(American Psychological
Association [ΑΡΑ], 1 9 9 2 ) is applicable to all psychologists, including those in medical and health settings. It is expected that psychologists guide their practice based on the principles o f competence (Principle A), integrity (Principle B), professional
and
scientific
responsibility
(Principle C ) , respect for people's rights and dignity (Principle D), concern for others' welfare (Principle E ) , and social responsibility (Principle F) (for a complete description of these principles, see ΑΡΑ [1992] guidelines). This chapter outlines the ethical responsibilities of health psychologists based on these principles, with a focus on issues related to health and medical
PRINCIPLE A: COMPETENCE Psychologists strive to maintain high standards of competence in their work. They recognize the boundaries of their particular competencies and the limitations of their expertise. They provide only those services and use only those techniques for which they are qualified by education, training, and experience. Psychologists are cognizant of the fact that the competencies required in serving, teaching, and/or studying groups of people vary with the distinctive characteristics of those groups. In those areas where recognized professional standards do not yet exist, psychologists exercise careful judgment and take appropriate precautions to protect the welfare of those with whom they work. They maintain knowledge of relevant scientific and professional information related to the services they render, and they recognize the need for ongoing education. Psychologists make appropriate use of scientific, professional, technical, and administrative resources (ΑΡΑ, 1992).
settings. C o m m o n scenarios with ethical implications are presented in an attempt to clarify the psychologist's role.
Α Ρ Α ( 1 9 9 2 ) guidelines clearly emphasize the importance o f pursuing and maintaining
443
444
SPECIAL ISSUES competence for psychologists. Several issues
her advertisement changed to read "clinical
related to competence are relevant to health
health psychologist specializing in motiva-
psychology, including achieving and recog-
tional treatment for health problems."
nizing boundaries o f competence, conducting
Dr. Jones failed to act ethically in this
appropriate assessment batteries, and identi-
e x a m p l e . Although
fying appropriate roles in a medical setting.
training in health psychology are not specific
recommendations
for
(e.g., Belar, 1 9 8 0 ; Stone, 1 9 8 3 ) , a weekend seminar is not sufficient to achieve compe-
Achieving Boundaries of Competence
tence. Although it may be within Dr. Jones's boundaries o f competence to treat individuals
T h e initial challenge o f achieving and rec-
with comorbid medical/health issues (so long
ognizing boundaries o f competence requires a
as she seeks consultation in areas where she is
professional commitment from health psy-
not well trained), she cannot ethically promote
chologists. M i n i m u m levels o f competence for
herself as a "clinical health psychologist."
psychologists have been defined (ΑΡΑ, 1 9 8 1 ) . Although these recommendations are guide-
Case 2. " D r . B r o o k e , " a clinical health psy-
lines and not requirements, they can be useful
chologist trained in women's health disorders,
in defining limits o f expertise. Criteria for
is treating " M s . J a c o b s , " who suffers from
competence in specialization areas o f psychol-
depression. As therapy progresses, it becomes
ogy are more ambiguous. General guidelines
clear that M s . Jacobs's sleep problems are
for adequate training in health psychology
beyond those expected for depressive symp-
have been presented and include graduate
tomatology and beyond Dr. Brooke's areas o f
coursework in health psychology and super-
expertise. Dr. Brooke continues to see M s .
vised clinical experience in health/medical set-
J a c o b s for her depression but refers her to a
tings (Stone, 1 9 8 3 ) . In the absence o f specific
sleep disorders specialist for further evaluation
guidelines, conservative estimates o f compe-
of her sleep difficulties.
tence are recommended. It is considered the
Dr. Brooke acted ethically in this example.
responsibility o f the psychologist to recognize
Her evaluation o f her training stands in stark
and practice within his or her areas o f compe-
contrast to Dr. Jones's lack o f regard to her
tence as well as to evaluate the appropriate-
limits o f expertise in the previous example. Dr.
ness o f additional training. Consultation with
Brooke found M s . Jacobs's sleep disturbance
senior colleagues can be helpful in evaluating
to exceed a typical depressive reaction and
the appropriateness o f training endeavors. In
was therefore attending to the "distinctive
addition, it is recommended that further train-
characteristics" (ΑΡΑ, 1 9 9 2 ) o f M s . Jacobs's
ing be sought via forums with
clinical presentation. Furthermore,
appropriate
although
accreditation. Someone w h o has not achieved
Dr. Brooke was trained in health psychology,
adequate training in health psychology cannot
she realized the limits o f her competence and
ethically promote himself or herself as a health
sought outside guidance when appropriate.
psychologist. Consider the following scenario.
Another instance in which health psychologists may inappropriately cross the boundaries
Case 1. " D r . J o n e s , " originally trained as a
of competence is providing medical interven-
psychoanalyst, completed a weekend seminar
tions for which they are not
on motivational therapy with cardiac patients.
trained (Belar & Deardorff, 1 9 9 9 ) . Because
After the seminar, she felt capable o f general-
psychologists in medical settings become very
appropriately
izing this therapy to many patients in health
familiar with medical diagnoses and medica-
settings. She called the yellow pages and had
tions for these illnesses, they may become
Ethical Issues
445
comfortable making unethical recommendations
patients, and it may be inaccurate or even
for medical treatment. Consider the following
inappropriate to compare a patient presenting
example.
in a medical context with psychiatric patients (ΑΡΑ, 1 9 9 2 , Standard 2 . 0 4 ) . It has been rec-
Case 3. " D r . L o g a n " trained " M r . Collins"
ommended (Belar & Deardorff, 1 9 9 9 ) that,
in relaxation training and biofeedback to help
whenever possible, psychological test results o f
control the patient's blood pressure. As M r .
medical patients should be compared with both
Collins became more skilled with his relax-
medical and psychiatric populations, and a dis-
ation training, Dr. Logan encouraged him to
cussion o f the differences should be included in
cut back slowly on his blood pressure medica-
the assessment report. W h e n comparable norm
tion so that the patient could achieve his goal
groups are not available, the clinician must con-
o f behaviorally managing his hypertension. Dr.
Logan behaved unethically in this
sider the lack o f appropriate norms in the interpretation o f the test data. In addition, it is the
example. Medication management is not part
responsibility o f the clinician to maintain exper-
of his role as a clinical health psychologist. A
tise with the instruments used. This includes
more appropriate course of action would have
maintaining knowledge o f current literature
been for either Dr. Logan or M r . Collins to dis-
and published research on the instruments.
cuss the issue with M r . Collins's physician.
Second, assessment results must be c o m -
Although health psychologists are trained for
municated to the patient in plain language. In
medical settings, they must remember that their
fact,
role is to provide psychological interventions.
assessment report also be written in clear
Medical diagnosis and intervention
require
it has been recommended
that
the
simple language given that many reports are
medical training, just as psychological interven-
read
tions require psychological training. Providing
K o o c h e r , 1 9 9 8 ) . T h e psychologist is required
medical interventions as a health psychologist is
to explain to the patient not only the purpose
practicing medicine without a license.
by
the
patient
(Keith-Spiegel &c
o f testing but also the results o f testing, avoiding psychological jargon that is difficult to interpret (ΑΡΑ, 1 9 9 2 , Standard 2 . 0 9 ) .
Conducting Psychological Assessments
Finally, although a health psychologist is often consulted to provide a report to a team
Psychological assessment plays an integral
of medical professionals, he or she is primar-
role in the practice o f health psychology.
ily accountable to the patient. It is recom-
According to Belar and Deardorff
(1999),
mended that the psychologist's allegiance to
" A s s e s s m e n t . . . is inextricably intertwined
the patient be clarified routinely at the outset
with the consultation activity" o f health
o f the consultation with the patient
psychologists (p. 3 9 ) . Multiple ethical issues
medical team.
and
surround competence and psychological assess-
T h e following scenario contains several
ment in health/medical settings. First, the
ethical issues related to psychological assess-
battery o f instruments to which a health psy-
ment in a medical setting.
chologist has access should be generally suffiunique
Case 4. " D r . Stephens" was consulted by
consultative needs and patient variables ser-
ciently broad and sensitive to the
the medical team at a chronic pain clinic to
viced. In this context, norm groups that are
provide a psychological evaluation for " M r .
used must be appropriate to the patient. M a n y
T h o m a s , " who presented with lower leg pain
norm groups for personality, behavioral, and
with
cognitive tests are comprised o f psychiatric
T h o m a s was a first-generation immigrant with
no
apparent
organic etiology. M r .
SPECIAL ISSUES
446
6 years o f formal education w h o did not speak
PRINCIPLE B : I N T E G R I T Y
English as his primary language. Dr. Stephens Psychologists seek to promote integrity in the science, teaching, and practice of psychology. In these activities, psychologists are honest, fair, and respectful of others. In describing or reporting their qualifications, services, products, fees, research, or teaching, they do not make statements that are false, misleading, or deceptive. Psychologists strive to be aware of their own belief systems, values, needs, and limitations and the effect of these on their work. To the extent feasible, they attempt to clarify for relevant parties the roles they are performing and to function appropriately in accordance with those roles. Psychologists avoid improper and potentially harmful dual relationships. (ΑΡΑ, 1992)
conducted an evaluation that consisted of projective and objective personality measures as well as a number o f other symptom checklists. After examining the results, Dr. Stephens concluded that M r . T h o m a s demonstrated personality tendencies consistent with the production of
physical symptoms
for
attention.
Dr.
Stephens concluded the consultation by reporting his findings to the medical team, which promptly
referred
Mr. Thomas
for
psy-
chotherapy targeted at his personality issues. Dr. Stephens demonstrated several ethical violations in this example. First, he should have compared his patient's test results with those o f the appropriate norm group or made adjustments to his interpretations based on differences between his patient and the com-
Psychologists are expected to behave ethi-
parison group. Dr. Stephens should have also
cally and honestly in their profession. T h e y
been more careful not to pathologize physical
present
symptoms. Health psychologists should make
integrity. They conduct their services without
it clear in their written and oral reports that
bias and avoid situations that may impair their
themselves
genuinely
and
with
the presence o f psychological symptoms does
objectivity in their relationships with their
not necessarily negate the presence o f physical
patients. Although all points in the principle o f
symptoms. Results from psychological evalua-
integrity are applicable to health psychologists,
tions are expected to augment—not replace—
perhaps the most relevant is the issue of
physical exams. Persuasive language loosely
relationships with patients and colleagues.
based on test data can be dangerous, not only because it may be inaccurate but also because it may convince medical professionals
to
Relationships With Patients
ignore valid physical symptoms in their search
Dual role relationships with patients is one
for a diagnosis. Second, D r . Stephens should
of
have shared his findings directly with
reported
the
the
most
common
ethical
dilemmas
by psychologists (Pope & Vetter,
patient. Although the ethical standards do not
1 9 9 2 ) . Although health psychologists might
mandate that results be delivered directly by
not be any more vulnerable to potential dual
the psychologist, the psychologist must ensure
relationship violations, relationship
bound-
that the patient receives a clear and accurate
aries in medical settings may be more blurred
explanation o f the evaluation findings. T h e
than in other settings. Psychologists who work
attending physician may have related findings
as part of a medical team interact with other
to the patient. However, most medical per-
medical professionals with differing sets of
sonnel are not trained in interpreting
and
ethical guidelines. For example, the American
conceptualizing psychological assessments.
Medical Association (ΑΜΑ) guidelines for sex-
Therefore, it is recommended that, whenever
ual relationships with former patients are quite
possible, the psychologist w h o conducted the
different
evaluation provide the feedback to the patient.
which clearly discourage sexual relationships
from the ΑΡΑ ethical guidelines,
Ethical
Issues
447
with former patients and mandate a 2-year
from Dr. Pressling, Dr. Goode decides to
period before a sexual relationship can be
attend the golf outing.
initiated with a former patient (for a complete
Although the Α Ρ Α ethical guidelines for
description o f sexual misconduct in medical
nonsexual
practice, see Α Μ Α , 1 9 9 1 ) . Furthermore, non-
specific as those for sexual relationships, the
sexual social relationships with patients are not
guidelines clearly state that
dual relationships
are n o t
as
addressed by the Α Μ Α . Health psychologists need to be aware that although the behavior o f other professionals may be considered ethical based on guidelines for their respective fields, this behavior might not be ethical based on psychological standards.
Relationships With Colleagues Relationships with colleagues also raise ethi-
a psychologist refrains from entering into or promising another personal, scientific, professional, financial, or other relationship with [patients] if it appears likely that such a relationship reasonably might impair the psychologist's objectivity or otherwise interfere with the psychologist's effectively performing his or her functions as a psychologist, or might harm or exploit the other party. (ΑΡΑ, 1992, Standard 1.17)
cal dilemmas for psychologists who work in medical settings. M a n y medical professionals
Although the ethics o f Dr. Goode's behavior
are not well educated as to the role of psychol-
are debatable, his golf outing with the patient
ogy in a medical setting and/or the influence o f
does raise several potential ethical violations.
psychological issues in medical diagnoses. For
First, although Dr. Goode received pressure
instance, an attending physician might unknow-
from his employer to attend the golf outing, the
ingly request inappropriate use of assessment
dual relationship with the patient was avoid-
results or recommend an unsubstantiated psy-
able. Dr. Smith was still a patient in the pro-
chological-behavioral intervention.
gram, and Dr. Goode may be called on to give
Although
this situation may be uncomfortable for the
interpretations/assessments
health psychologist, he or she must educate the
progress that might be biased by his relationship
physician and the treatment team as to his or her
with him as a golf partner. Second, based on the
role in the medical setting and on the appropri-
power differential between Dr. Goode and Dr.
ate use of psychiatric instruments for assessment
Pressling, Dr. Goode entered a professional dual
in medical populations.
relationship
with
Dr.
o f Dr.
Smith's
Pressling when
he
The following case demonstrates ethical
accepted the invitation to play golf. Although it
dilemmas related to relationships with both
can be argued that Dr. Goode may suffer the
patients and colleagues.
potentially negative repercussions of this dual relationship, there is also a potential of harm to
6. "Dr. G o o d e " recently provided a
future patients in that such a dual relationship
psychological evaluation for "Dr. Smith," a
may interfere with Dr. Goode's judgment in
physician in a nearby town and a current
future consultations. This scenario serves as a
Case
patient in a drug rehabilitation program. " D r .
nice example in that the potential for harm to
Pressling," the attending physician for the
the patient is debatable. In addition to the
treatment team, coordinated a golf outing for
responsibility to "avoid improper and poten-
the treatment team and some area physicians,
tially harmful dual relationships," Dr. Goode is
including Dr. Smith. Although Dr. Smith is still
required to "promote integrity" in his behavior
in outpatient treatment at the drug rehabilita-
(ΑΡΑ, 1 9 9 2 ) . W h e n the potential harmfulness
tion center, Dr. Goode has completed his por-
of a dual relationship is debatable, the promo-
tion of the assessment. W i t h encouragement
tion of integrity in science and clinical practice
448
SPECIAL ISSUES may prove to be a clearer guideline. This seems
remain cognizant of their role and
particularly applicable when health psycholo-
patients and colleagues o f this role. Some
gists are forced to interface with colleagues who
patients, who often have several appointments
operate under different ethical guidelines.
throughout
inform
the course of the day, may be
unclear as to the role of each o f their providers. Consider for a moment a patient who has been
PRINCIPLE C: PROFESSIONAL AND SCIENTIFIC RESPONSIBILITY
visited by a speech therapist, a physical therapist, and a physician during the first 3 hours of the morning, only to have a fourth individual, a
Psychologists uphold professional standards of conduct, clarify their professional roles and obligations, accept appropriate responsibility for their behavior, and adapt their methods to the needs of different populations. Psychologists consult with, refer to, or cooperate with other professionals and institutions to the extent needed to serve the best interests of their patients, clients, or other recipients of their services. Psychologists' moral standards and conduct are personal matters to the same degree as is true for any other person except as psychologists' conduct may compromise their professional responsibilities or reduce the public's trust in psychology and psychologists. Psychologists are concerned about the ethical compliance of their colleagues' scientific and professional conduct. When appropriate, they consult with colleagues to prevent or avoid unethical conduct. (ΑΡΑ, 1992) Psychologists define their role for patients and colleagues, and they strive t o perform this role appropriately. Role clarification is particularly important for health psychologists w h o
health psychologist, enter her room with a variety of questions and assessment instruments. From the patient's perspective, the role of the health psychologist may be understandably confused with the services of other providers. The health psychologist is directed to explain his or her role and the purpose of the interventions to the patient at the outset o f the session and must ensure that the patient has a clear understanding of the role that the health psychologist is expected to play. In addition to patients, members o f the treatment team may be unclear as to the health psychologist's role, training, and expertise. An interdisciplinary team consists o f several independent agendas that are coordinated to reach a collective goal. Amid the agendas, a full appreciation and understanding o f the role of fellow providers can become even more ambiguous. Therefore, it is o f utmost importance that the health psychologist define his or her purpose to team members as well as to clarify his or her professional responsibilities.
typically work as part of a multidisciplinary team. In addition, health psychologists are expected to promote the field o f psychology by consulting
with
colleagues,
behaving
responsibly, and participating in scientific and professional endeavors.
Ensuring High Standards of Professional Conduct Ensuring high standards o f professional conduct active
Clarifying Professional Roles Health psychologists typically maneuver in a
and
scientific responsibility is an psychologists
are
encouraged to routinely consult with
process. H e a l t h
col-
leagues as a means o f reinforcement, education,
and
professional
accountability. In
work environment of a variety o f health care
addition, health psychologists are encouraged
providers, including physicians, rehabilitation
to maintain a presence at patient staffings (as
therapists, nurses, and social workers. In this
opposed to sending psychological technicians,
context, it is important that health psychologists
interns, or graduate students)
as
another
Ethical Issues
449
means o f illustrating their role and taking
be established by law, institutional rules, or
responsibility for assessment and treatment o f
profession" (ΑΡΑ, 1 9 9 2 , Standard 5.02). The
their patients. Also, health psychologists are
commitment to confidentiality and the limits
encouraged to make grand rounds presenta-
of this commitment are discussed with the
tions, provide in-service training sessions, and
patient/potential patient at the outset of ther-
remain active in professional societies to facil-
apy/intervention. These limits include (a) harm
itate both public and institutional awareness.
to self, (b) harm to others, (c) child/elderly abuse or neglect, and (d) court order. Ethical dilemmas related to confidentiality are common and can become more complex in medical settings.
PRINCIPLE D: RESPECT F O R PEOPLE'S R I G H T S AND D I G N I T Y
Maintaining Confidentiality
Psychologists accord appropriate respect to the fundamental rights, dignity, and worth of all people. They respect the rights of individuals to privacy, confidentiality, self-determination, and autonomy, always being mindful that legal and other obligations may lead to inconsistency and conflict with the exercise of these rights. Psychologists are aware of cultural, individual, and role differences, including those due to age, gender, race, ethnicity, national origin, religion, sexual orientation, disability, language, and socioeconomic status. Psychologists try to eliminate the effect on their work of biases based on those factors, and they do not knowingly participate in or condone unfair discriminatory practices. (ΑΡΑ, 1992)
Various ethical dilemmas related to the maintenance o f confidentiality are inherent in the medical setting. T h e health psychologist has a primary responsibility to the patient. T h e intricacies o f such a responsibility can at times be easily overlooked amid the shuffle of multiple interventions applied by the medical team. Therefore, it is essential that all limitations and exceptions to confidentiality be openly discussed with the patient at the outset o f assessment and/or treatment regardless of the estimated length o f contact. T h e health psychologist is also required to inform the patient as to who will have access to assessment results/reports and records o f the psychologist-
Multiple issues are pertinent to a discussion of respect o f others for health psychologists in a medical setting, including patient rights to privacy, confidentiality, and respect for diversity. Health psychologists are also sensitive to individual differences o f their patient populations and consider these differences in interactions with patients as well as with
the
treatment team.
Confidentiality
patient interactions. Although not ethically mandated, written documentation o f the clinician's explanation and the patient's understanding of confidentiality is recommended from a legal and practical perspective. The health psychologist not only must thoroughly explain confidentiality to the patient but also must ensure that confidentiality is protected. In medical settings, chart information is typically widely accessed by hospital staff. Therefore, it is recommended that, whenever possible, detailed notes be kept
separately
ΑΡΑ (1992) guidelines clearly describe the
within the psychologist's therapy records and
psychologist's commitment to confidentiality:
only cryptic general notes be included as part
"Psychologists have a primary obligation and
of the medical chart
take reasonable precautions to respect the confi-
1 9 9 9 ) . This practice protects patient data from
dentiality rights of those with whom they work
possible misinterpretation
or consult, recognizing that confidentiality may
staff
and
maintains
(Belar &
Deardorff,
by nonpsychiatric confidentiality
of
450
SPECIAL ISSUES patient
the consequences of breaching/not breaching
Another issue pertinent to maintaining con-
thorough evaluation o f suicidality in a medical
fidentiality is privacy for psychological consul-
setting. Assessments o f potentially suicidal
tations. It is often difficult to secure privacy
patients in a medical setting, particularly in the
for a clinical interview given that families
emergency room, may be rushed. Furthermore,
potentially sensitive, yet irrelevant,
confidentiality. Several issues may complicate a
information.
and/or other patients might be present in the
the clinician does not have the advantage of a
hospital room and the patient might not be
well-established relationship with the patient
ambulatory for the purpose o f moving to a
to facilitate a thorough appraisal of suicidality.
testing/interview office. W h e n possible, it is
If the patient is deemed to be a threat to him-
recommended that the health psychologist
self or herself, the health psychologist must
pre-schedule individual time in the patient's
exhaust non-confidentiality-breaching solu-
room. Coordinating in advance with other
tions prior to breaking confidentiality. When a
members o f the treatment team can often
breach is required, it is recommended that,
facilitate making such arrangements. Also, in
whenever possible, the provider inform the
cases where multiple patients share a room, a
patient o f the pending breach and reasons for
less than ideal but simple alternative is to close
such action. Obviously, the importance of the
the divider curtains around the patient's bed.
initial discussion o f the limits o f confidentiality
Finally, the issue o f confidentiality in medi-
becomes glaringly apparent. T o appropriately
cal settings can often be obscured by the numer-
protect the patient and preserve the therapeutic
ous interventions and assessments conducted
relationship, the health psychologist must be
on a particular patient. Although the patient
thoroughly familiar with guidelines related to
may have consented for some data (e.g., medi-
confidentiality. W h e n a solution to a dilemma
cal data) to be shared with family members or
related to breaching confidentiality is unclear,
with other health professionals, psychological
the psychologist should consult with other
data are considered separate and the patient
colleagues to ensure an appropriate resolution.
must consent for this information to be shared
Another exception to confidentiality occurs
with others. It is also important that the health
when the patient is assessed as a potential
the
harm to others. T h e psychologist's duty to
patient consented to in the original consulta-
warn was well established in the case o f
tion. In a hurried, sometimes chaotic medical
Tarasoffv.
setting, the precise limits o f the patient's origi-
of California
nal consent can be overlooked or misunder-
Supreme Court ruled that it is the psycholo-
stood, even with the best of intentions.
gist's responsibility to assess the threat o f
psychologist remain cognizant of what
Board
of Regents
of the
University
( 1 9 7 6 ) , in which the California
harm in patients and to take measures to pro-
Exceptions to Confidentiality Although the limits o f confidentiality are
tect potential victims. T h e Tarasoff
case has
generated confusion for psychologists as to when it is necessary to preserve or break con-
clearly stated in the ethical guidelines, these
fidentiality
with
potentially
dangerous
boundaries can be somewhat ambiguous in
patients. Moreover, much o f the controversy
medical settings. Confidentiality may need to
surrounds the degree o f intent to commit a
be breached in situations where the patient is a
violent act suggested by patients' statements
threat to himself or herself. It is important
and overt behavior. Obviously, this dilemma
from both a legal and an ethical standpoint
may become even more ambiguous in a medi-
that the health psychologist thoroughly evalu-
cal setting where threat o f harm may relate
ate the suicidal patient and carefully consider
not only to physical violence but also to the
Ethical intentional
and
unintentional
spread
of
Issues
inform others of H I V risk), it is his duty to inform the partner. Dr. Actte calls the partner
contagious diseases. For instance, based on the duty to warn, are psychologists under ethical obligation to inform
and lets him know that M s . Bowen is HIVpositive and that he is at risk.
of H I V -
Dr. Actte committed several ethical viola-
positive patients? As with all "duty to warn"
tions in this example. First, M s . Bowen was
scenarios, careful consideration of each case and
protecting her partner from her infection, so
its particular issues is imperative. Thorough
the risk o f harm was negligible. This case can
sexual or needle-sharing partners
documentation, consultation, and review of
be contrasted to cases in which the patient
specific state regulations concerning H I V
maliciously spreads the virus with the intent to
reporting are necessary before making the deci-
infect others or neglectfully engages in risky
sion to breach or preserve confidentiality. States
virus-spreading behaviors. As mentioned pre-
have differing guidelines regarding the duty to
viously, the ΑΡΑ states that when the patient
warn in infectious disease cases. T h e health psy-
takes precautions to protect his or her partner,
chologist needs to be familiar with state laws in
the clinician is not mandated to warn the
determining whether confidentiality should be
partner o f H I V risk. Second, even if Dr. Actte
breached for a specific case. W h e n state laws
determined that M s . Bowen was placing her
permit or mandate notification o f third parties
partner at risk and that he had a duty to inter-
about risk o f HTV infection, it is recommended
vene, he could have handled the situation
that confidentiality be maintained except in cir-
more responsibly. H e and M s . Bowen could
cumstances where patients are unwilling to
have discussed the risks to her partner and the
reduce the risk o f spreading infection to part-
consequences o f not informing him o f her sta-
ners (ΑΡΑ, 1 9 9 1 ) . W h e n confidentiality is
tus. Then, Dr. Actte could have encouraged
breached in these unusual cases, the practi-
M s . Bowen to tell her partner rather than
tioner must maintain detailed records providing
breaking confidentiality to inform the partner.
a rationale for breaking confidentiality, inform
Finally, if M s . Bowen refused to tell her part-
the patient that the third party will be con-
ner after this discussion and Dr. Actte found it
tacted, and attempt to conceal patient-identify-
necessary to warn the partner, he should have
ing information in the contact (ΑΡΑ, 1 9 9 1 ) .
first informed M s . Bowen o f his intentions
Consider the following scenario.
and preserved her privacy as much as possible in his report to the partner.
Case
6. "Dr. Rey Actte" is treating " M s .
Bowen." M s . Bowen reports to Dr. Actte that she is HIV-positive. She has been dating her partner for about 3 months. Although she has
Recognizing the Influence of Multicultural Issues
not informed her partner that she is HIV-
In addition to maintaining confidentiality,
positive, she has been very cautious in their
health psychologists must respect the rights o f
sexual relationship, using appropriate protec-
others by recognizing and understanding diver-
tion to prevent the exchange o f bodily fluids.
sity. Although all psychologists need to be cog-
She states that she will tell her partner at some
nizant o f cultural influences (ΑΡΑ, 1 9 9 2 ,
point but that the time has not been right. As
Standard 1.08), patient populations continue
M s . Bowen continues to describe her partner, it
to grow more culturally diverse, suggesting
becomes apparent that he is an acquaintance o f
the ever-increasing need for greater cultural
Dr. Actte. After the session concludes, Dr. Actte
awareness (Iwamasa, 1 9 9 7 ) . Clearly, health
determines that, based on the guidelines for his
psychologists should avoid prejudice and dis-
state (which allow a breach o f confidentiality to
crimination in the process o f evaluation and
452
SPECIAL ISSUES treatment. Understanding,
appreciating,
and
implementing cultural sensitivity reaches far
taking medications, to assist her in her treatment decision. Dr. Hayden has so far acted ethically in
beyond political correctness. cultural
this example. Although medication is recom-
diversity, including ethnicity, gender, socio-
mended for non-diet-controlled type 2 diabet-
economic status, geographic region, and reli-
ics, it is M s . Layten's decision as to whether
Multiple factors contribute to
gion. T h e Western medical model adhered to
she wants
in hospitals may represent viewpoints
to take
the
medication.
The
and
referring physician made pejorative assump-
expectations for behavior that stand in stark
tions about M s . Layten's motivation because
contrast to those o f cultures with which many
he ignored her cultural belief system. These
patients identify. For example, cultural belief
assumptions not only pathologized M s . Layten's
systems vary as to perceptions o f illness behav-
belief system but also potentially exacerbated
ior, use o f medication, etiological understand-
her "noncompliance." M s . Layten's case is a
ing o f illness, and family participation. T h e
good example o f multicultural
health psychologist is encouraged to be cog-
Similar to the general population, health care
nizant o f his or her own ethnocentrism and to
professionals are at risk for unintentionally
take steps to appropriately acknowledge and
stigmatizing patients based on cultural igno-
incorporate the patient's cultural belief system
rance and their own ethnocentrism. Health
whenever possible. Even the most politically
psychologists are encouraged to remain aware
correct health psychologist can provide inac-
that multicultural issues are present in nearly
influences.
contraindicated
every patient-therapist relationship and that
treatments by missing the influence o f culture.
cultural diversity surpasses race to include
Consider the following example.
factors such as nationality, disability, socio-
curate interpretations
and
economic status, gender, geographic location, Case
7. " D r . Hayden" is treating " M s .
and religion.
Layten," who has type 2 diabetes. She had not been taking her medication, and she was referred
for psychological and
behavioral
interventions. M s . Layten had been described by the referring physician as "difficult and noncompliant
with
recommended
treat-
ments." T h e referring physician reported trying several medications to no avail, and he is concerned that she may need insulin; however, he is afraid that she would refuse to take it, just like she had the other medications. After Dr. Hayden met with M s . Layten for several sessions, she shared with him that medical interventions were against her spiritual beliefs. Although she had tried to share this with the referring physician, he had told her that many diabetics do not like the thought o f medications and that she would get used to it if she just tried it for a while. Dr. Hayden discussed with M s . Layten alternatives to medication, as well as the perceived consequences o f not
PRINCIPLE E: C O N C E R N FOR OTHERS' WELFARE Psychologists seek to contribute to the welfare of those with whom they interact professionally. In their professional actions, psychologists weigh the welfare and rights of their patients or clients, students, supervisees, human research participants, and other affected persons as well as the welfare of animal subjects of research. When conflicts occur among their obligations or concerns, psychologists attempt to resolve these conflicts and to perform their roles in a responsible fashion that avoids or minimizes harm. Psychologists are sensitive to real and ascribed differences in power between themselves and others, and they do not exploit or mislead other people during or after professional relationships. (ΑΡΑ, 1992)
Ethical Issues Α Ρ Α ( 1 9 9 2 ) guidelines define the psychol-
anhedonia and insomnia. "Dr. Johnson," a
ogist's role in providing informed consent as
clinical health psychologist, is consulted to pro-
follows:
vide an assessment o f B o b . Dr. Johnson is finishing with a staffing when she gets the consult
(a) Psychologists obtain appropriate informed consent to therapy or related procedures, using language that is reasonably understandable to participants. The content of informed consent will vary depending on many circumstances; however, informedconsent generally implies that the person (1) has the capacity to consent, (2) has been informed of significant information concerning the procedure, [and] (3) has freely and without undue influence expressed consent and [that] (4) consent has been appropriately documented. (Standard 4.02a)
and figures that she does not have time to see B o b before his next appointment. Thus, she orders a small battery o f
agenda for the day. O n one o f the instruments, it is later discovered that B o b endorses suicide intent. After ordering the additional following day. Dr. J o h n s o n failed to act ethically in this example. First, she did not obtain B o b ' s consent. Measures were simply left with him, and
T h e health psychologist must be commitensuring that the patient is consenting without pressure or force. Although not mandated by ethical codes, it has been recommended that informed consent be provided in both written
form
(Keith-Spiegel
tests,
Dr. Johnson makes a note to see B o b the
ted to explaining the therapy process and to
oral and
paper-and-pencil
assessment instruments to be added to Bob's
&
Koocher, 1 9 9 8 ) . Providing both written and oral explanations o f therapy decreases confusion for the patient and provides documentation for the health psychologist. T h e consent form should be thorough yet simple and may address the following: expectations for therapy, potential risks and benefits o f treatment, commitment to and limits o f confidentiality, and an agreed-on fee.
he complied with the request to complete them. Second, she did not describe the purpose of the additional assessment procedures to B o b . T h e patient has the choice as to whether to complete these measures, and his options were not explained to him. Third, she did not describe confidentiality and its limits. Because Dr. J o h n s o n is ethically required to protect B o b from harm to himself, which may include breaching confidentiality, she is now in a precarious situation. N o t only has she potentially breached the trust in a patient-psychologist relationship, but she has essentially conducted an assessment on a patient without any permission from the patient. This is clearly a breach of ethics and a generally poor standard o f practice.
Logistics o f informed consent become more complex for the psychologist in a medical setting. In a consultation environment, the psychologist is often asked to provide a brief intervention or assessment. In these situations, informed consent is often inappropriately overlooked. Although failing to provide informed consent in itself is an ethical violation, this ethical infraction can lead to additional ethical dilemmas. Consider the following scenario.
Another situation related to informed consent that can be difficult for health psychologists in medical settings is ensuring consent for medical procedures. Psychologists often work with medical patients in exploring their perceptions and their options for medical treatment. Psychologists in health settings can facilitate informed consent for medical procedures by helping patients to understand the medical procedures, encouraging patients to ask for explanations and options from their physicians,
Case 8. " B o b " is currently in outpatient cardiac rehabilitation. H e reports symptoms of
persuading physicians and medical personnel to
provide
concise
and
understandable
453
454
SPECIAL ISSUES and
level o f suffering and emotional welfare of
assisting patients in planning for and adjust-
patients. When appropriate, it is recommended
explanations of treatment to patients, ing to the medical procedures
(Belar
&
Deardorff, 1 9 9 9 ) .
that health psychologists maintain knowledge of local resources such as low-income hospice care, psychological and psychiatric treatment, chari-
PRINCIPLE F: SOCIAL RESPONSIBILITY Psychologists are aware of their professional and scientific responsibilities to the community and the society in which they work and live. They apply and make public their knowledge of psychology so as to contribute to human welfare. Psychologists are concerned about and work to mitigate the causes of human suffering. When undertaking research, they strive to advance human welfare and the science of psychology. Psychologists try to avoid misuse of their work. They comply with the law and encourage the development of law and social policy that serve the interests of their patients and clients as well as the public. They are encouraged to contribute a portion of their professional time for little or no personal advantage. (ΑΡΑ, 1992)
table organizations, churches, and other services that may be necessary. Such information might not be a part of the consultation question and is typically viewed as a social worker's domain. However, health psychologists should be prepared to at least have reasonable referral information when confronted with inquiring patients and their families. Health psychologists must also be sensitive to the emotional needs of friends and family members of their patients. Health psychologists are obviously encouraged to maintain appropriate boundaries. However, sound professional boundaries do not necessarily exclude a few minutes of supportive contact for patients and their families.
CONCLUSIONS This chapter has provided an overview of ethical guidelines specific to health psycholo-
The principle of social responsibility stands
gists. Although the Α Ρ Α guidelines
may
as a nucleus for any professional endeavor made
appear to be straightforward, these recom-
by a psychologist and is particularly applicable
mendations are often distorted by ambiguous
to the health psychologist. M o r e than other clin-
situations. It is suggested that the health
ical specialties, health psychologists routinely
psychology student not only become familiar
interact with patients who are experiencing
with the Α Ρ Α guidelines but also review
physical suffering that may include pain, deteri-
situations that raise ethical dilemmas. This
oration of motor ability, or even impending
chapter introduced some o f these difficult
death. It is not uncommon for patients to be
situations, but further study is required to
concerned about the emotional and financial
become a health psychologist w h o behaves
welfare of their family members. Likewise,
with integrity to protect patients and pro-
family members are often concerned about the
mote the field o f health psychology.
REFERENCES American Medical Association. (1991). Sexual misconduct in the practice of medicine. Journal of the American Medical Association, 266, 2 7 4 1 - 2 7 4 5 . American Psychological Association. (1981). Specialty guidelines for the delivery of services. American Psychologist, 36, 6 4 0 - 6 8 1 .
Ethical Issues American Psychological Association. (1991, August). Legal liability related to confidentiality and the prevention of HIV transmission. American Psychological Association AIDS-Related Policy Statements. (Washington, DC: Author) American Psychological Association. (1992). Ethical principles of psychologists and code of conduct. American Psychologist, 47, 1 5 9 7 - 1 6 1 1 . Belar, C. D. (1980). Training the clinical psychology student in behavioral medicine. Professional Psychology, 11, 6 2 0 - 6 2 7 . Belar, C. D., & Deardorff, W. W. (1999). Clinical health psychology in medical settings: A practitioner's guidebook. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Iwamasa, G. Y . (1997). Behavior therapy and a culturally diverse society: Forging an alliance. Behavior Therapy, 28, 3 4 7 - 3 5 8 . Keith-Spiegel, P., &C Koocher, G. P. (1998). Ethics in psychology: Professional standards and cases. New York: McGraw-Hill. Pope, K. S., & Vetter, V. A. (1992). Ethical dilemmas encountered by members of the American Psychological Association. American Psychologist, 47, 3 9 7 - 4 1 1 . Stone, G. C. (Ed.). (1983). National Working Conference on Education and Training in Health Psychology. Health Psychology, 2(Suppl. 5), 1-153. Tarasoff v. Board of Regents of the University of California, 551 P. 2d 3 3 4 (Cal. Sup. Ct. 1976).
455
CHAPTER
10 23
Ethnocultural Issues in Behavioral Medicine HECTOR F . MYERS AND WEI-CHIN HWANG
T
here are substantial and
persistent
some of the salient factors that contribute to the
health.
disparities are discussed. Third, examples o f
Compared with Caucasian Americans,
behavioral interventions with these disorders in
ethnic minorities have poorer health (Keppel,
minorities are presented. Fourth, a series of
Pearcy, & Wagener, 2 0 0 2 ; Williams, 2 0 0 0 ) ,
recommendations distilled from
receive poorer quality health care, and have
successful interventions is provided.
racial/ethnic
disparities
in
poorer prognoses and treatment (Smedley,
Stith,
&
Nelson,
the
outcomes
2002;
U.S.
Department o f Health and Human Services,
E T H N I C DISPARITIES IN H E A L T H
1999).
AND H E A L T H CARE
M o r e o v e r , these health
most
disparities
remain even after adjusting for socioeconomic status, severity o f illness, and discrepancies in
Current evidence indicates that,
access to care. T h e continued failure to close
with Caucasian Americans, African Americans
these persistent racial/ethnic disparities in
and Native Americans evidence a significant
health status, combined with the rapid increase
health disadvantage, Hispanic Americans have
in the ethnic and cultural diversity in the
equal or slightly poorer health, and Asian
United States, has serious public health conse-
Americans evidence significant health advan-
quences and results in increased demand on an
tages for some disorders (National Center for
already challenged health care system. If left
Health Statistics, 2 0 0 0 ) . F o r example, African
unattended, racial/ethnic disparities in health
Americans and other minorities are more likely
will eventually lead to a decline in quality o f
to report poorer subjective health and well-
life for all Americans (Smedley et al., 2 0 0 2 ) . This chapter is organized into four main sec-
456
compared
being (Hughes 8 t T h o m a s , 1 9 9 8 ) , poorer functional status, and greater disability than are
tions. First, a brief review of ethnic disparities in
Caucasians, but minorities are more likely to
cardiovascular disease (CVD), diabetes, cancer,
report comparable or better rates o f other indi-
and pain, conditions o f special relevance to
cators of mental health (Kington & Nickens,
behavioral medicine, is presented. Second,
2 0 0 0 ; Williams & Harris-Reid, 1 9 9 9 ) .
Ethnocultural
Issues
457
It is also well known that African Americans
evidence rates approximately twice as high as
have the highest rate of essential hypertension,
that of Caucasian Americans. They also found
develop the disease at an earlier age, develop a
higher incidence rates o f stomach cancer in
more severe form o f the disease earlier, and suf-
Korean Americans, Vietnamese Americans,
fer from more severe complications and death
Japanese Americans, Alaskan Natives, and
from the disease than does any other ethnic
Native Hawaiians.
group in the United States (National Heart,
N o t only are there ethnic differences in
Lung, and Blood Institute, 1 9 9 6 ) . A similar pat-
chronic illnesses, but several clinical and experi-
tern is observed in the prevalence and rates o f
mental studies have also reported ethnic differ-
mortality from C V D and cancer, which are the
ences in the
two leading causes o f death in the United States.
tolerance of pain as well as in pain-related
These rates are significantly higher in African
avoidance o f activity and physical and psy-
Americans and
chosocial disability (Bates 8c Edwards, 1 9 9 2 ;
are lower
Caucasian Americans
in Asian Americans,
and
Hispanic
perception,
sensitivity,
and
Edwards, Doleys, Fillingim, 8c Lowery, 2 0 0 1 ;
Americans, and Native Americans (American
McCracken,
Cancer Society, 2 0 0 2 ; Cooper et al., 2 0 0 0 ) .
2 0 0 1 ) . In their study o f ethnic differences in
However, recent reports offer some good news,
pain perception in chronic pain patients, Bates
Matthews,
Tang,
8c
Cuba,
with significant reductions in heart disease
and Edwards
reported in all ethnic groups.
patients reported significantly greater intensity
( 1 9 9 2 ) found
that
Hispanic
Unfortunately, these positive trends are not
of pain than did the Caucasian subgroups and
observed in all health problems, nor are the
that these ethnic differences were moderated
ethnic
by locus of control; Anglo-Americans and
groups. For example, the rates o f non-insulin
Polish Americans with high internal locus o f
dependent diabetes mellitus ( N I D D M )
are
control reported experiencing more intense
increasing and affect minority groups dispro-
pain, whereas high internal locus o f control
portionately. African Americans, Hispanic
respondents
Americans, Native Americans, and some Asian
reported lower pain ratings.
trends
equally
positive
for
all
from the other ethnic groups
American subgroups suffer from N I D D M at
M c C r a c k e n and colleagues ( 2 0 0 1 ) com-
higher rates than do Caucasian Americans and
pared pain experiences in treatment-seeking
are also more likely to suffer from complica-
African American and Caucasian American
tions secondary to the disease such as blindness,
chronic pain patients and found that although
end-stage renal disease, amputations, and mor-
the groups did not differ on the chronicity o f
tality (Carter, Pugh, &c Monterrosa, 1 9 9 6 ) .
pain, medical diagnosis, work status, or previ-
Also, although there has been a significant reduction in mortality rates from lung and
ous surgeries, African American
patients
reported higher pain severity, more avoidance
breast cancer in the U.S. population overall,
of
Native Americans evidenced increased mortal-
physical symptoms, and greater physical and
activity, m o r e
fearful
thinking,
more
ity rates and Asian Americans evidenced little
psychological disability than did Caucasian
reduction in mortality from these diseases
American patients. These results were also
(National Center for Health Statistics, 2 0 0 0 ) .
confirmed by Faucett, Gordon, and Levine
Miller and colleagues ( 1 9 9 6 ) also reported that
( 1 9 9 4 ) in their study o f ethnic differences in
Vietnamese Americans, Korean Americans,
acute postoperative dental pain in four ethnic
and Chinese Americans have liver cancer inci-
groups. They found that European Americans
dence and mortality rates several times higher
reported less severe pain, whereas African
than that o f the U.S. population and
American and Hispanic American
that
African Americans and Hispanic Americans
patients
reported more severe pain. T h e basis for these
SPECIAL ISSUES
458
ethnie differences in pain are unclear, but
these services, as compared with Caucasian
Bates ( 1 9 8 7 ) argued for a biosocial model o f
Americans. In contrast, Padgett, Patrick, Burns,
pain, suggesting that the observed differences
and Schlesinger ( 1 9 9 4 ) found
in pain are not due to physiological differences
Americans and Hispanic Americans used out-
that
African
but rather are likely due to differences in cul-
patient mental health services less than did
tural experiences, attitudes, and meanings o f
Caucasian Americans, even after controlling for
pain. These, in turn, influence the neurophys-
a number of demographic variables.
iological processes that govern pain percep-
Although the results from studies o f help
tion and tolerance as well as the psychological
seeking among different ethnic groups are
and behavioral responses to pain.
somewhat inconsistent, there is strong evi-
T w o factors that are implicated in the per-
dence that ethnic groups differ in the quality
sistence of many of these ethnic health dispari-
of care received once they enter the health
ties are ethnic differences in help seeking and
care system. In a recent review on racial and
quality of health care received. M a n y studies
ethnic differences in health care, Smedley and
indicate that ethnic minorities typically delay
colleagues ( 2 0 0 2 ) found that ethnic minori-
seeking professional health care for
both
ties are less likely to receive appropriate diag-
physical and mental health needs, and they
nostic tests, treatment, and follow-up care for
attribute this delay to a greater reliance on
diseases such as cancer, diabetes, C V D , and
informal sources of help than is the case with
human immunodeficiency virus (HIV). These
Caucasian Americans (Zhang, Snowden,
differences were evident even after controlling
Sue,
1 9 9 8 ) . Other more apparent
&
reasons
include financial and other barriers to access
for
factors such as delayed help seeking,
health insurance coverage, income, severity o f
such as health insurance, language barriers, and
disease, and differences in use o f services, all
stigma
of which might affect access to care.
against
mental
health
services
(Takeuchi, Leaf, &c Kuo, 1 9 8 8 ; T h o m a s &
Smedley and colleagues ( 2 0 0 2 ) also noted
Snowden, 2 0 0 1 ) . However, results from some
that a variety o f patient, provider, and health
studies on help seeking suggest that pathways
care system variables contribute to these differ-
to care are more complex than thought previ-
ences in health care. Ethnic minorities, espe-
ously. For example, Snowden ( 1 9 9 8 )
found
cially the poor, are more likely to delay seeking
that African Americans tend to rely more on
care, adhere poorly to treatment regimens, and
both formal and informal sources o f help
often refuse recommended services. Several
for
their health needs than do
Americans.
In
addition,
Caucasian
whereas
studies also indicate that physicians tend to
Asian
stereotype minority clients as less intelligent
Americans as a whole tend to seek help at the
and educated, prefer patients of certain ethnic-
lowest rates relative to their representation in
ities, and
the general population (Matsuoka, Breaux, &
patients through the type o f procedures sug-
Ryujin, 1 9 9 7 ; Ying & Hu, 1 9 9 4 ) , Southeast
gested, the amount and type o f medication
Asians evidence higher rates o f use (Ying &c
prescribed, and/or the frequency o f contact. In
Hu, 1 9 9 4 ) .
addition, a number o f health care system vari-
provide
different
treatment
to
Data from inpatient and outpatient mental
ables, such as geographic distribution of health
health services confirm this complexity. For
care facilities, health insurance coverage, avail-
example, Snowden and Cheung (1990) reported
ability
higher use of inpatient services by African
services, and effectiveness o f outreach services,
Americans and
also contribute to the disparities in health care.
Native
Americans/Alaskan
Natives, whereas Hispanic Americans
of
translation
and
interpretation
and
In summary, there is substantial evidence
Asian Americans/Pacific Islanders underuse
indicating that ethnic differences in health
Ethnocultural
Issues
459
status do exist and that there also appear to be
stresses, and personality characteristics (e.g.,
ethnic differences in perceived overall health, in
anger/hostility, neuroticism, pessimism) that
functional status, and in the availability and
serve as psychological vulnerabilities. The nega-
quality of health care provided. Moreover, the
tive effects of these adversities, in turn, are mag-
magnitude of these group differences may be
nified by health-endangering behaviors (e.g.,
underestimated
smoking, alcohol and drug abuse, sedentary
because o f the failure
to
2000;
appreciate the considerable sociocultural and
lifestyle, overeating) (Contrada et al.,
historical heterogeneity among the various
Myers, Kagawa-Singer, Kumanyika, Lex, 8c
racial/ethnic groups. In addition, ethnic minor-
Markides, 1995;
ity overrepresentation in lower socioeconomic
Dominguez,
status levels and the risks associated with low
Myers, Lewis, &
Parker-
2002).
T h e model also hypothesizes that these
socioeconomic status (e.g., environmental and
lifetime adversities exert their effects
occupational hazards, possible differences in
health
through
biobehavioral
on
pathways,
social and behavioral risks) contribute to the
including the chronic triggering o f physiologi-
persistence o f the disparities in health and func-
cal response mechanisms, constitutional pre-
tional status and to the disproportionate bur-
dispositions or vulnerabilities, and overtaxed
den of morbidity and mortality among ethnic
allostatic load (i.e., wear-and-tear
minorities and the poor.
system)
(Geronimus,
Seeman,
1999). In turn, this allostatic load is
1992;
on
McEwen
the 8c
hypothesized to contribute over time to cumulative vulnerability and ultimately to functional
FACTORS THAT CONTRIBUTE
outcomes, including physical and psychologi-
T O T H E H E A L T H DISPARITIES
cal distress and dysfunction (Seeman, Singer, Health outcomes, whether they are chronic
R o w e , Horwitz, & M c E w e n ,
1997).
conditions such as C V D and N I D D M or asso-
However, the model also acknowledges
ciated pain, are clearly the by-product o f the
that a number o f psychosocial and behavioral
complex interaction o f many factors. These
factors
serve
as
psychosocial
assets
or
include individual differences such as biologi-
advantages that can moderate risk. These
cal predispositions, behavioral lifestyle, psy-
include psychological characteristics such as
chological characteristics, and environmental
dispositional optimism and perceived control
and psychosocial factors.
(Eizenman,
Nesselroade,
Featherman,
Sc
of how psychosocial stress and related factors
1997), healthy lifestyles (Myers et al., 1995), flexible stress appraisal and coping strategies (Wong 8c Ujimoto, 1998), and the
might account for ethnic differences in func-
availability and use of adequate social support
tional health status. T h e proposed model makes
resources (Seeman, Lusignolo, Albert,
explicit
Berkman,
In a recent review, Myers and Hwang
(2002)
proposed an integrative biopsychosocial model
that
sociostructural
factors
(e.g.,
Rowe,
8c
2001). Therefore, it is hypothesized
environmental
that it is the balance between cumulative
factors, and biological factors (e.g., genetic
adversities and cumulative advantages over the
vulnerabilities, family medical and psychiatric
life course that ultimately contributes to differ-
histories) interact over time to increase the bur-
ences in functional status and health trajecto-
den of psychosocial adversities experienced over
ries (Singer 8c Ryff,
the life course. These adversities are hypothe-
imbalance between cumulative adversities rela-
sized to be the primary predictors of risk and
tive to assets that is hypothesized as contribut-
include chronic life stresses, major life change
ing to the persistence of health disparities. This
events, ethnicity-related stresses, age-related
model has not been formally tested, but it
race/ethnicity, social class),
1999), and it is the lifetime
460
SPECIAL ISSUES
offers a series of testable hypotheses to guide
improve cardiovascular health in minority
future research on ethnic health disparities.
populations. M a n y C V D risk reduction programs for African Americans have focused on blood pressure reduction and control, smoking
BEHAVIORAL HEALTH INTERVENTIONS
cessation, dietary changes, weight loss, and physical exercise. M a n y o f these programs are faith based, thereby taking advantage of the
Behavioral scientists have made a number o f
historical role o f the African American church
significant contributions toward closing the
as a credible community institution central to
health disparities gap by designing and test-
African American community life (Magnus,
ing theoretically driven, culturally appropri-
1 9 9 1 ) . Reviews o f programs such as Project
ate interventions for a variety o f medical
Joy
conditions, such as C V D , as well as risk
Koffman,
(Yanek, Becker, M o y , Gittelsohn, 2001),
behavior change, such as weight reduction,
Ascanio,
&
smoking cessation, and increased participa-
(Rosamond et al., 2 0 0 0 ) , and the Heart Smart
tion in cancer and diabetes screening.
Program (Johnson et al., 1 9 9 1 ) , as well as weight
CVD Risk Reduction Interventions Behavioral scientists have made some of their
Lighten Up
Egan,
reduction
Obarzanek,
2001),
programs
Stevens, Hebert,
&
(Oexmann, Wisewoman
(Kumanyika, &
Whelton,
1 9 9 1 ) , all indicate that cultural sensitivity, in conjunction with a focus on specific risk
addressing
behaviors that test behavioral principles, yields
ethnic issues in behavioral medicine through the
the best results. However, because many of
design and implementation o f interventions to
these community-based interventions are not
reduce C V D risks in minority
populations.
rigorously evaluated, it is difficult to identify
M o s t of this work has been conducted on
the specific mechanisms and intervention com-
African Americans and Hispanic Americans,
ponents that account for their overall success.
most significant contributions to
with a few studies targeting Asian Americans
Similar conclusions are drawn in reviews
and Native Americans. These interventions have
of C V D risk reduction programs for His-
focused attention on a variety of risk factors,
panic Americans such as the Language for
including blood pressure control, weight loss,
Health
smoking, and increased physical exercise. They
reviewed by Elder and colleagues
have also targeted different age groups (e.g.,
The
children,
adolescents, adults,
program
for
low-literate
same is true for programs
Latinos (2000). designed
the elderly),
specifically for Asians such as the Vietnamese
focused on community and clinic-based patient
Community Health P r o m o t i o n Project, the
populations, and been introduced in a variety of
Chinese Community Cardiac Council, and
settings such as schools, churches, hospitals,
the
community health centers, and social service
reviewed by Chen ( 1 9 9 3 ) . A number o f sim-
Heart
Health
for
Southeast
Asians
agencies (Magnus, 1 9 9 1 ) . These studies have
ilar programs have been designed for Native
used a variety of both quantitative and qualita-
Americans
tive methodologies. However, few of these stud-
Cardiovascular
ies have used well-controlled, large-scale clinical
Indian and Hispanic American children, and
trial methodologies to test and validate theoreti-
the A c o m a - C a n o n c i t o - L a g u n a Adolescent
cally driven interventions, and this limits the
Health Program reviewed by L e M a s t e r and
utility and generalizability o f their results.
Connell ( 1 9 9 4 ) . All o f these C V D risk reduc-
such
as
the
Curriculum
Checkerboard for
American
Nevertheless, there are a number o f success-
tion programs were conducted in community
ful approaches that have been implemented to
settings by members o f the specific ethnic
Ethnocultural
Issues
461
groups w h o understand the cultural norms o f
testing and early identification o f problems.
the groups and w h o are familiar with the
As a result, several studies have been designed
groups' specific needs (e.g., speak the native
to address this problem, and many
language, k n o w the cultural customs, use
shown measurable improvement in screening
have
culturally congruent and effective modes o f
behavior in these populations. For example,
communication and caregiving). T h e curric-
Gotay and colleagues ( 2 0 0 0 ) reported
ula are also designed specifically to address
results o f a controlled trial o f a culturally
the cultural beliefs, behavioral norms, and
tailored breast and cervical cancer screening
language
requirements
of
each
group.
intervention for Native Hawaiian
the
women.
However, as noted previously, many o f these
They used lay educator-led groups, or
Kokua
ethnic-specific programs are not rigorously
groups, to deliver culturally tailored education
evaluated, nor do they use rigorous research
and support for screening. This group-led
methodologies to test their efficacy, and this
intervention produced
tends to reduce confidence in their results.
ments in b o t h knowledge
Also, until their results are replicated, it can-
behaviors in the Native Hawaiian
n o t be determined whether their findings will
participants, and they in turn shared what they
generalize to other ethnic communities.
learned in the program with other women in
significant improveand
screening women
the community, thereby extending the program's impact to the community-at-large.
Interventions to Increase Participation in Cancer and Diabetes Screening
Similar results were obtained by Taylor and colleagues (2002) in their randomized trial to increase cervical cancer screening in Chinese
Another focus o f attention o f behavioral
American women in Seattle, Washington, and
scientists working with ethnic minority popu-
Vancouver, British Columbia. They tested the
lations is increasing participation
in early
screening programs for diseases such as cancer
impact
of a
Chinese-language
education-
entertainment video, a motivational pamphlet,
and diabetes. Both o f these diseases are very
and a fact sheet on Pap smear testing on women
prevalent and are associated with significant
who were randomly assigned to an intensive
ethnic disparities in disability and mortality.
outreach intervention versus direct mail versus
treatment
usual care. They found both higher participa-
reduces both disability and mortality risk,
tion and significant increases in Pap smear test-
ethnic minorities are significantly less likely to
ing in women in the more intensive intervention.
Although
participate
early
in
detection
early
and
programs.
Similar programs have also been developed
Therefore, a number of intervention programs
screening
for urban and rural African American women
have been designed to address this obstacle to
(Earp et al., 2 0 0 2 ; Paskett et al., 1 9 9 9 ) , for
effective treatment, especially in diabetes and
low-income Latinas (Hiatt et al., 2 0 0 1 ;
breast and cervical cancer.
Valdez, Banerjee, Ackerson, &
Fernandez,
Current evidence indicates that minority
2 0 0 2 ) , for Cambodian women (Jackson et al.,
women, especially Chinese Americans (Taylor
2 0 0 0 ) , for Alaska Native women (Lanier,
et al., 2 0 0 2 ) , Vietnamese Americans (McPhee
Kelly, &
et al., 1997), and Native Hawaiians (Gotay et al.,
American w o m e n
Hoick, 1999),
and
for
Native
(Hodge, Fredericks, Sc
2 0 0 0 ) , have some o f the lowest rates o f Pap
Rodriguez, 1 9 9 6 ) . In addition, similar pro-
smears and breast and cervical cancer testing
grams to test for prostate cancer in men have
of all ethnic groups. Deficits in knowledge,
been developed (Myers et al., 1 9 9 9 ) .
lack o f familiarity, embarrassment, and the
In one o f the most comprehensive reviews
inability to afford services pose barriers to
of the effectiveness of interventions to promote
462
SPECIAL ISSUES mammography among women who underuse
integral t o the design and
these services, Legler and colleagues ( 2 0 0 2 )
effective interventions with minority popula-
execution o f
found that the most effective interventions
tions. These include addressing
issues of
used access-enhancing (e.g., aggressive out-
acculturation and acculturative stress, pro-
reach, removal o f barriers to access) and indi-
viding indigenous support resources, identi-
vidual-directed (e.g., tailored interventions to
fying and sensitively addressing group norms
change high-risk behaviors) strategies, which
and beliefs that might undermine treatment
resulted in an estimated 2 7 % increase in
efficacy, and focusing on cultural strengths
mammography use. The combination of access-
such
enhancing and system-directed interventions—
respect for traditional values and practices.
as family support, religiosity, and
for example, interventions that improve the
In addition, behavioral health care systems
cultural competence o f health care providers,
need to learn the lessons o f the more effective
working to establish satellite facilities in ethnic
public health agencies and work to establish
communities
that
provide
ethnic-specific
meaningful working relationships with the
services—also yielded an impressive 2 0 %
ethnic communities they intend to serve. This
increase in screening. Legler and colleagues
can be accomplished by establishing relation-
concluded that access-enhancing strategies are
ships with community centers and groups,
an important complement to individual- and
with educational and vocational settings, and
system-directed interventions for women with
with faith-based organizations in the commu-
low screening rates.
nity. In addition,
meaningful
relationships
should be established with community leaders and gatekeepers w h o have community crediIMPLICATIONS AND
bility, w h o can act as cultural consultants, and
RECOMMENDATIONS FOR
w h o are able to evaluate and make culturally
BEHAVIORAL MEDICINE
congruent
suggestions for modifying
the
design, content, and implementation o f the T h e r e are a number o f ways in which behav-
planned interventions. Special effort should be
ioral health care providers can help to reduce
made to ensure that relationships formed with
the large and persistent racial/ethnic dispari-
the community are truly collaborative and
ties in health. At the systems level, these
long-lasting and are not exploitative.
current
T o increase the efficacy and use o f behav-
risk reduction interventions, reducing sys-
include expanding and improving
ioral services, interventions should be delivered
tematic barriers that limit health care use
by indigenous providers and in the communi-
and efficacy,
that
ties where the participants reside, and every
increases the cultural sensitivity and compe-
effort should be made to remove or reduce
and providing
training
tency of service providers. At the individual
barriers to participation (e.g., stigma, language,
and group levels, more attention needs to be
financial, child care, transportation). These
given to basic research about behavioral and
barriers can be removed by using community
psychosocial factors that enhance risk or
settings that participants are likely to be famil-
serve as buffers or protective factors t o illness
iar and comfortable with as well as by expand-
in racial/ethnic minority populations. Both
ing outreach by advertising information and
comparison and ethnic-specific studies are
referrals for services o f which patients might be
needed. Knowledge about these risk and pro-
unaware. Treatment participation, adherence,
tective factors, and about possible mediators
and retention may also be improved by subtle
or moderators
but important changes in treatment settings
o f risk and protection, is
Etbnoculturai
Issues
\
(e.g., decorating the setting with cultural sym-
and by hiring consultants and cultural competency
bols) and by providing linkages that facilitate
experts to teach about their communities and
patient access to other community resources as
suggest feedback on the modification of inter-
needed such as vocational, welfare, child care,
vention services. In so doing, special care
and other ancillary services.
should be taken to develop effective provider-
T h e efficacy o f behavioral health interven-
patient communication, to establish and main-
tions is also likely to be improved by increased
tain patient comfort and trust, and to establish
understanding o f h o w informal and natural
provider credibility. Assumptions should not be
support networks enhance or interfere with
made that ethnic minority providers are inter-
professional health service seeking and use.
changeable such that members of one minority
Currently, very little is known about how net-
group are culturally competent in working with
works and mechanisms in indigenous cultures
members o f other ethnic minority
identify persons in need o f services or about
Cultural competence, like any other skill, must
groups.
of
be mastered and not assumed by accident of a
services. Understanding and using indigenous
person's birth. In addition, attention should be
services and culturally sanctioned pathways o f
given to understanding possible cultural differ-
how
they
facilitate
access
and
use
and
ences in the expression and reporting of symp-
enhances the effectiveness o f more traditional
toms and illnesses as well as to identifying
behavioral interventions, for example, using
provider biases that may influence perceptions
Latina
and possible differences in treatments given to
care
often
improves
Consejeras,
the
feasibility
African American lay
counselors, American Indian talking circles, and Native Hawaiian Kokua
patients from different ethnic backgrounds.
groups.
In addition, health education and community outreach services not only are integral to
SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS
increasing knowledge about health care issues but also provide information about how to
Ethnic minorities, as compared with Caucasian
obtain those services. O n c e clients c o m e in for
Americans, carry a disproportionate burden of
services, special effort should be made to pro-
morbidity and mortality, receive poorer quality
vide proper education on the importance o f
health care, and have poorer prognoses and
treatment adherence. Obviously, all informa-
treatment outcomes. Although all of the reasons
tion must be provided in the clients' primary
for the racial/ethnic health disparities and the
language, with trained translators being used
disproportionate burden of disease remain to be
when native speakers are not available. This is
identified, the extant literature suggests that bio-
more desirable than using children in the
logical predispositions, behavioral lifestyle,
clients' families as translators
cultural
psychological characteristics, environmental
brokers. This resource is especially important
and psychosocial factors, and health care system
or
for recent immigrants and refugees, who may
factors all contribute to these disparities. The
be unfamiliar with the U.S. health care system
chapter authors (Myers &c Hwang, 2 0 0 2 ) have
or who may be mistrustful or too embarrassed
offered a conceptual framework that provides
to seek services.
testable hypotheses about how these disparate
At the provider level, increasing the cultural
factors might operate to produce these health
competency of behavioral health care providers
outcomes, and they hypothesize that it is the
should be a top priority. This can be accom-
balance between cumulative adversities and
plished by having health care providers take
cumulative advantages over the life course
cultural competency workshops and seminars
that ultimately contributes to differences in func-
463
464
SPECIAL ISSUES tional status and health trajectories. In addition,
services and reduce delayed help seeking and
the literature suggests that the
systematic
poor treatment adherence. Moreover, health
removal of a number o f barriers to help seeking
care providers can also reduce ethnic disparities
(e.g., financial, insurance, language, stigma
in health by improving and expanding on cul-
against mental health services) and a greater
turally sensitive interventions, increasing health
understanding and integration of indigenous
education and outreach efforts, and examining
and informal but culturally congruent services
their own biases when providing services to
may facilitate access and use of professional
culturally different clients.
REFERENCES American Cancer Society. (2002). Cancer facts and figures (surveillance research report). Atlanta, GA: Author. Bates, M . S. (1987). Ethnicity and pain: A biocultural model. Social Science and Medicine, 24(1), 4 7 - 5 0 . Bates, M . S., &C Edwards, W. T. (1992). Ethnic variations in the chronic pain experience. Ethnicity & Disease, 2(1), 6 3 - 8 3 . Carter, J . S., Pugh, J . Α., 6c Monterrosa, A. (1996). Non-insulin dependent diabetes 125, mellitus in minorities in the United States. Annals of Internal Medicine, 221-232. Chen, M . S. (1993). Cardiovascular health among Asian Americans/Pacific Islanders: An examination of health status and intervention approaches. American journal of Health Promotion, 7, 1 9 9 - 2 0 7 . Contrada, R. J . , Ashmore, R. D., Gary, M. L., Coups, E., Egeth, J . D., Sewell, Α., Ewell, K., Goyal, T. M., & Chasse, V. (2000). Ethnicity-related sources of in Psychological stress and their effects on well-being. Current Directions Science, 9(4), 1 3 6 - 1 3 9 . Cooper, R., Cutler, J . , Desvigne-Nickens, P., Fortmann, S. P., Friedman, L., Havlik, R., Hogelin, G., Marier, J . , McGovern, P., Morosco, G., Mosca, L., Pearson, T., Stamler, J . , Stryer, D., & Thorn, T. (2000). Trends and disparities in coronary heart disease, stroke, and other cardiovascular diseases in the United States: Findings of the National Conference on Cardiovascular Disease Prevention. Circulation, 102, 3 1 3 7 - 3 1 4 7 . Earp, J . Α., Eng, E., O'Malley, M . S., Altpeter, M., Rauscher, G., Mayne, L., Mathews, H. F., Lynch, K. S., & Qaqish, B. (2002). Increasing use of mammography among older, rural African American women: Results from a community trial. American Journal of Public Health, 92, 6 4 6 - 6 5 4 . Edwards, R. R., Doleys, D. M., Fillingim, R. B., Sc Lowery, D. (2001). Ethnic differences in pain tolerance: Clinical implications in a chronic pain population. Psychosomatic Medicine, 63, 3 1 6 - 3 2 3 . Eizenman, D. R., Nesselroade, J . R., Featherman, D. L., 8c Rowe, J . W. (1997). Intraindividual variability in perceived control in an older sample: The MacArthur successful aging studies. Psychology & Aging, 12, 4 8 9 - 5 0 2 . Elder, J . P., Candelaria, J . I., Woodruff, S. L, Criqui, M. H., Talavera, G. Α., & Ruff, J . W. (2000). Results of language for health: Cardiovascular disease nutrition education for Latino English-as-a-second-language students. Health Education & Behavior, 27(1), 5 0 - 6 3 .
Ethnocultural Faucett, J . , Gordon, N., & Levine, J . (1994). Differences in postoperative pain severity among four ethnic groups. Journal of Pain & Symptom Management, 9, 3 8 3 - 3 8 9 . Geronimus, A. T. (1992). The weathering hypothesis and the health of AfricanAmerican women and infants: Evidence and speculations. Ethnicity & Disease, 2, 2 0 7 - 2 2 1 . Gotay, C. C., Banner, R. O., Matsunaga, D. S., Hedlund, N., Enos, R., Issell, B . F., &£ DeCambra, H. (2000). Impact of a culturally appropriate intervention on breast and cervical screening among Native Hawaiian women. Preventive Medicine, 31, 5 2 9 - 5 3 7 . Hiatt, R. Α., Pasick, R. J . , Stewart, S., Bloom, J . , Davis, P., Gardiner, P., Johnston, M., Luce, J . , Schorr, K., Brunner, W., & Stroud, F. (2001). Community-based cancer screening for underserved women: Design and baseline findings from the Breast and Cervical Cancer Intervention Study. Preventive Medicine, 33, 190-203. Hodge, F. S., Fredericks, L., & Rodriguez, B . (1996). American Indian women's talking circle: A cervical cancer screening and prevention project. Cancer, 78, 1592-1597. Hughes, M., & Thomas, M . E. (1998). The continuing significance of race revisited: A study of race, class, and quality of life in America, 1 9 7 2 - 1 9 9 6 . American Sociological Review, 63, 7 8 5 - 7 9 5 . Jackson, J . C , Taylor, V. M., Chitnarong, K., Mahloch, J . , Fisher, M., Sam, R., & Seng, R. (2000). Development of a cervical cancer control intervention program for Cambodian American women. Journal of Community Health, 25, 359-375. Johnson, C. C , Nicklas, Τ. Α., Arbeit, M . L., Harsha, D. W., Mott, D. S., Hunter, S. M., Wattigney, W., & Berenson, G. S. (1991). Cardiovascular intervention for high-risk families: The Heart Smart Program. Southern Medical Journal, 84, 1 3 0 5 - 1 3 1 2 . Keppel, K. G., Pearcy, J . N., & Wagener, D. K. (2002, January). Trends in racialand ethnic-specific rates for the health status indicators: United States, 1 9 9 0 - 1 9 9 8 . Statistical Notes, No. 2 3 . (Atlanta, GA: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention) Kington, R. S., & Nickens, H. W. (2000). Racial and ethnic differences in health: Recent trends, current patterns, future directions. In N. Smelser, W. Wilson, &C F. Mitchell (Eds.), America becoming: Racial trends and their consequences (Vol. 2 , pp. 2 5 3 - 3 1 0 ) . Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Kumanyika, S. K., Obarzanek, E., Stevens, V. J . , Hébert, P. R., & Whelton, P. K. (1991). Weight-loss experience of black and white participants in NHLBIsponsored clinical trials. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 53(Suppl.), S1631-S1638. Lanier, A. P., Kelly, J . J . , &c Hoick, P. (1999). Pap prevalence and cervical cancer prevention among Alaska Native women. Health Care Women International, 20, 4 7 1 - 4 8 6 . Legler, J . , Meissner, Η. I., Coyne, C , Breen, N., Chollette, V., & Rimer, Β. K. (2002). The effectiveness of interventions to promote mammography among women with historically lower rates of screening. Cancer Epidemiology, Biomarkers, & Prevention, 11(1), 5 9 - 7 1 . LeMaster, P. L., & Connell, C. M. (1994). Health education interventions among Native Americans: A review and analysis. Health Education Quarterly, 21, 521-538. Magnus, M . H. (1991). Cardiovascular health among African-Americans: A review of the health status, risk reduction, and intervention strategies. American Journal of Health Promotion, 5, 2 8 2 - 2 9 0 .
Issues
465
466
SPECIAL ISSUES Matsuoka, J . K., Breaux, C , Sc Ryujin, D. H. (1997). National utilization of mental health services by Asian Americans/Pacific Islanders. Journal of Community Psychology, 25, 1 4 1 - 1 4 5 . McCracken, L. M., Matthews, A. K., Tang, T. S., Se Cuba, S. L. (2001). A comparison of blacks and whites seeking treatment for chronic pain. Clinical Journal of Pain, 17, 2 4 9 - 2 5 5 . McEwen, B. S., Sc Seeman, T. (1999). Protective and damaging effects of mediators of stress: Elaborating and testing the concepts of allostasis and allostatic load. In N. E. Adler, M . Marmot, B . S. McEwen, Sc J . Stewart (Eds.), Socioeconomic status and health in industrial nations: Social psychological, and biological pathways (pp. 3 0 - 4 7 ) . New York: New York Academy of Sciences. McPhee, S. J . , Stewart, S., Brock, K. C , Bird, J . Α., Jenkins, C. N. H., Se Pham, G. Q. (1997). Factors associated with breast and cervical cancer screening practices among Vietnamese American women. Cancer Detection & Prevention, 21, 510-521. Miller, Β. Α., Kolonel, L. N., Bernstein, L., Young, J . L., Jr., Swanson, G. M., West, D., Key, C. R., Liff, J . M., Glover, C. S., Sc Alexander, G. A. (Eds.). (1996). (NIH Pub. Racial/Ethnic patterns of cancer in the United States 1988-1992 No. 96-4104). Bethesda, M D : National Institutes of Health. Myers, H. F., Sc Hwang, W. C. (2002, March). Cumulative psychosocial risks and resilience: A conceptual perspective on ethnic health disparities in late life. Paper presented at the National Research Council panel on "Ethnic Disparities in Aging Health," Washington, DC. Myers, H. F., Kagawa-Singer, M., Kumanyika, S. K., Lex, B. W., Se Markides, K. S. (1995). Panel III: Behavioral risk factors related to chronic diseases in ethnic populations. Health Psychology, 14, 6 1 3 - 6 2 1 . Myers, H. F., Lewis, T. T., Se Parker-Dominguez, T. (2002). Stress, coping, and minority health: Biopsychosocial perspective on ethnic health disparities. In G. Bernai, J . Trimble, K. Burlew, Se F. Leong (Eds.), Handbook of racial and ethnic minority psychology (pp. 3 7 7 - 4 0 0 ) . Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Myers, R. E., Chodak, G. W., Wolf, Τ. Α., Burgh, D. Y., McGrory, G. T., Marcus, S., Diehl, J . Α., Se Williams, M . (1999). Adherence by African American men to prostate cancer education and early detection. Cancer, 86, 8 8 - 1 0 4 . National Center for Health Statistics. (2000). Health, United States, 2000: Adolescent health chartbook. Hyattsville, M D : Author. National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute. (1996). Chartbook of cardiovascular, lung, and blood diseases: Morbidity and mortality. Washington, DC: National Institutes of Health. Oexmann, M . J . , Ascanio, R., Sc Egan, Β . M . (2001). Efficacy of a church-based intervention on cardiovascular risk reduction. Ethnicity Disease, 11, 817-822. Padgett, D. K., Patrick, C , Burns, B. J . , Se Schlesinger, H. J . (1994). Women and outpatient mental health services: Use by black, Hispanic, and white women in a national insured population. Journal of Mental Health Administration, 21, 347-360. Paskett, E. D., Tatum, C. M., D'Agostino, R., Rushing, J . , Velez, R., Michielutte, R., Se Dignan, M . (1999). Community-based interventions to improve breast and cervical cancer screening: Results of the Forsyth County Cancer Screening (FoCas) Project. Cancer Epidemiology, Biomarkers, & Prevention, 8, 453-459. Rosamond, W. D., Ammerman, A. S., Holliday, J . L., Tawney, K. W., Hunt, K. J . , Keyserling, T. C , Will, J . C , 8c Mokdad, A. H. (2000). Cardiovascular disease
Ethnocultural risk factor intervention in low-income women: The North Carolina WISEWOMAN project. Preventive Medicine, 31, 3 7 0 - 3 7 9 . Seeman, T. E., Lusignolo, T. M., Albert, M., & Berkman, L. (2001). Social relationships, social support, and patterns of cognitive aging in healthy, high functioning older adults: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Health Psychology, 20, 2 4 3 - 2 5 5 . Seeman, T. E., Singer, B . H., Rowe, J . W., Horwitz, R. I., St McEwen, B . S. (1997). Price of adaptation-allostatic load and its health consequences: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Archives of Internal Medicine, 157, 2259-2268. Singer, B., St Ryff, C. D. (1999). Hierarchies of life histories and associated health risks. In N. E. Adler, M . Marmot, B . S. McEwen, St J . Stewart (Eds.), Socioeconomic status and health in industrial nations: Social, psychological, and biological pathways (pp. 9 6 - 1 1 5 ) . New York: New York Academy of Sciences. Smedley, B. D., Stith, A. Y . , St Nelson, A. R. (2002). Unequal treatment: Confronting racial and ethnic disparities in health care. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Snowden, L. R. (1998). Racial differences in informal help seeking for mental health problems. Journal of Community Psychology, 26, 429—438. Snowden, L. R., St Cheung, F. K. ( 1 9 9 0 ) . Use of inpatient mental health services by members of ethnic minority groups. American Psychologist, 45, 347-355. Takeuchi, D. T., Leaf, P. J . , St Kuo, H-S. (1988). Ethnic differences in the perception of barriers to help-seeking. Social Psychiatry & Psychiatric Epidemiology, 23, 2 7 3 - 2 8 0 . Taylor, V. M., Hislop, T. G., Jackson, J . C , Tu, S. P., Yasui, Y . , Schwartz, S. M., The, C , Kuniyuki, Α., Acorda, E., Marchand, Α., St Thompson, B . (2002). A randomized controlled trial of interventions to promote cervical cancer screening among Chinese women in North America. Journal of the National Cancer Institute, 94, 6 7 0 - 6 7 7 . Thomas, K. C , St Snowden, L. R. (2001). Minority response to health insurance coverage for mental health services. Journal of Mental Health Policy and Economics, 4(1), 35—41. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (1999). Mental health: A report of the surgeon general. Rockville, M D : U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, Center for Mental Health Services. Valdez, Α., Banerjee, K., Ackerson , L., 8t Fernandez, M. (2002). A multimedia breast cancer education intervention for low-income Latinas. Journal of Community Health, 27, 3 3 - 5 1 . Williams, D. R. (2000). Racial variations in adult health status: Patterns, paradoxes, and prospects. In N. Smelser, W. Wilson, St F. Mitchell (Eds.), America becoming: Racial trends and their consequences (Vol. 2, pp. 3 7 1 - 4 1 0 ) . Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Williams, D., St Harris-Reid, M . (1999). Race and mental health: Emerging patterns and promising approaches. In A. Horwitz St T. Scheid (Eds.), A handbook for the study of mental health: Social contexts, theories, and systems (pp. 2 9 5 - 3 1 4 ) . New York: Cambridge University Press. Wong, P. T. P., St Ujimoto, V. K. (1998). The elderly: The stress, coping, and mental health. In L. C. Lee St N. W. S. Zane (Eds.), Handbook of Asian American psychology (pp. 1 6 5 - 2 0 9 ) . Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Issues
467
468
SPECIAL ISSUES Yanek, L. R., Becker, D. M., Moy, T. E , Gittelsohn, J . , & Koffman, D. M . (2001). Project Joy: Faith based cardiovascular health promotion for African American women. Public Health Report, 116(Suppl. 1), 6 8 - 8 1 . Ying, Y., &c Hu, L. (1994). Public outpatient mental health services: Use and outcome among Asian Americans. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 64, 4 4 8 ^ 1 5 5 . Zhang, A. Y . , Snowden, L. R., & Sue, S. (1998). Differences between Asian and white Americans' help seeking and utilization patterns in the Los Angeles area. Journal of Community Psychology, 26, 3 1 7 - 3 2 6 .
CHAPTER
24
Women's Health Issues CSILLA T.
I
CSOBOTH
n the developed world, women today have
physiological and social role changes during
a longer life expectancy and lower mortal-
the life stages. Viewed from this life course
ity rates than do men at all ages, but epi-
perspective, gender influences life experience,
demiological studies show that women report
psychological development and functioning,
more chronic disease and disability (Khoury &
and the nature o f psychopathology (Zanardi,
Weisman, 2 0 0 2 ) . During the past two decades,
1 9 9 0 ) and so can result in different psycho-
the science o f behavioral medicine has recog-
logical developmental patterns. Three impor-
nized the importance of women's health in
tant
research and in patient care. Knowledge about
adolescence, pregnancy and childbirth, and
the biological, psychological, and social factors
menopause.
stages are discussed in this section:
influencing women's health has grown considerably, and efforts to change the thinking o f the scientific community and the general public
Adolescence
have led to a better understanding o f women's
T h e period o f adolescence is well known for
health. T h e enhancement of research in this
the biological, social, and cognitive changes
field resulted in more sensitive health care and
in both genders. T h e transition into adulthood
the construction o f specific preventive inter-
is usually a positive experience for young
ventions that serve to improve the health status
women, but in some the biological and psy-
of women worldwide.
chosocial changes increase the risk for certain psychopathologies, for example, the development o f depression or eating disorders. Y o u n g
LIFE C O U R S E PERSPECTIVES
women are at great risk for harmful health behaviors that influence their psychological
Although it is impossible to examine the issue
and physical well-being, including smoking,
of women's health in full detail in this chapter,
binge drinking, illicit drug use, negative conse-
an overview of special issues t o be considered
quences o f sexual behavior, and victimization.
during everyday clinical work with w o m e n is
Gender differences in psychosocial factors of
provided. Specifically, women's physical and
health-damaging behavior are already seen
mental health is influenced substantially by
during adolescence; for example, alcohol abuse 469
SPECIAL ISSUES
470
has a stronger connection to depression in girls
which might result in psychiatric disorders, is
than in boys (Clark et al., 1 9 9 7 ) .
an important task o f mental health profes-
There is evidence that emotional functional
sionals and can be achieved by developing or
are significantly
enhancing health protective factors at an early
more frequent among young females than
age. Y o u n g w o m e n need to be taught how to
among young males. In one study, female uni-
cope with different stressors, for example,
versity students were more likely to see their
functioning in multiple roles and learning to
disorders
and
symptoms
futures as hopeless and reported frequent use
resist peer or social pressure inducing them to
of
participate in harmful health habits that they
emotionally oriented coping strategies,
namely eating, drinking, and/or taking drugs
most likely will experience later on in life.
in difficult life situations. Likewise, a close relationship between different self-destructive behavioral forms was found among women.
Pregnancy and Childbirth
Smoking was closely correlated with other harmful
The transition to parenting for women and
health behaviors, namely alcohol
men is usually a normal biological task, but
consumption, sedentary lifestyle, consumption
women frequently spend a great deal of time
of high-cholesterol foods, and little sleep.
involved in caregiving and so experience many
Women
who
exercised regularly
had
a
biological and psychosocial changes during
reduced risk for suicide attempts and using
this period o f time. A woman's experience of
emotional coping strategies and also had more
pregnancy and childbirth will most likely
goals in life. In contrast, the correlation
affect her role as a mother, her perceptions of
between self-destructive behavior and psycho-
the child and herself, and her relationship with
logical symptoms was not significant among
her partner
young men (Kopp & Skrabski, 1 9 9 5 ) .
between the discrepancy o f a person's actual
Adolescence is one of the most vulnerable
(Shearer, 1 9 8 3 ) . A correlation
self-concept and his or her ideal self-concept
periods o f development. It is also a phase o f
and the amount of post-birth distress has been
constant change, and young women are more
reported (Alexander & Higgins, 1 9 9 3 ) . F o r
sensitive to conflict or change in the family
example, parents with a large discrepancy
environment. Studies have shown that
between h o w they saw themselves and what
the
timing o f pubertal development is influenced
they perceived as an
by
structure.
increases in post-birth distress relative to those
Maturation o f young women has been found
parents with a small discrepancy between
to be closely correlated with the home atmo-
actual and ideal (Alexander & Higgins, 1 9 9 3 ) .
family conflict and
family
ideal self
suffered
sphere, with earlier maturation being predicted
Studies have shown that more negative
by conflict and lack of approval in families
psychosocial outcomes appear to be associated
with both parents living at home (Graber &c
with cesarean section births than with vaginal
Brooks-Gunn, 1 9 9 8 ) . Earlier onset o f menar-
deliveries (DiMatteo et al., 1 9 9 6 ) . Cesarean
che has been reported in young women who
section not only decreases the positive experi-
live in households where there is parental con-
ence of giving birth to a child but also may have
flict or where the mother is the only parent
certain negative psychological outcomes. T h e
(Moffitt, Caspi, Belsky, & Silva, 1 9 9 2 ) . Protective health factors o f adolescence
postponed
encounter between mother
and
child may be a consequence o f breast-feeding or
include tight social network, high social sup-
bonding and may result in less positive mater-
port, development o f positive coping strate-
nal
reactions
to
the
newborn
(Cranley,
gies, resiliency, and hardiness. T h e prevention
Hedahl, & Pegg, 1 9 8 3 ) . W o m e n who experience
of the harm caused by psychological distress,
cesarean section or other interventions need to
Women's
Health
471
be given additional psychological support to
are influenced by personality, coping styles,
cope with difficulties arising after childbirth.
mood history, and exposure to a greater or
Substance use during pregnancy presents
lesser degree o f life stressors (Polit & L a R o c c o ,
Substance-
1 9 8 0 ; Greene & Cooke, 1 9 8 0 ) . Positive or
dependent women experience feelings o f low
negative expectations o f menopause are corre-
self-esteem, lower expectations for themselves,
lated with the number o f symptoms experi-
unique
problems
for w o m e n .
and higher levels o f anxiety than do men
enced; negative expectations were associated
(Jarvis & Schnoll, 1 9 9 5 ) . Pregnant, substance-
with more symptoms, whereas positive expec-
abusing women commonly present with poly-
tations were associated with fewer symptoms
substance abuse (Fischer, Bitschnau, Peternell,
(Matthews, 1 9 9 2 ) . Perception o f menopause is
Eder, & Topitz, 1 9 9 9 ) . They experience spe-
ethnically and culturally related; for example,
cific health problems, such as sexually trans-
Japanese women are far less likely to experi-
mitted diseases, malnutrition, and vitamin and
ence hot flashes than are women living in the
mineral deficiencies, but fail to acquire treat-
Western world (Lock, 1 9 9 3 ) . Research has
ment for their problems, and many do not
also shown that the menopausal experience
present to medical facilities for prenatal care.
may be influenced, either directly or indirectly,
Psychiatric comorbidities, mainly depression
by socioeconomic status. Lower educational
and chronic anxiety, and social problems,
level, lack of employment outside of the home,
chaotic familial backgrounds,
abusive
and lower socioeconomic status are associated
relationships are frequently reported by drug-
with increased severity or longer duration o f
and
abusing women. W o m e n who are convinced
menopausal
to attend prenatal care in multidisciplinary
McKinlay, 1 9 9 7 ) . Occurrence o f mood and
collaboration with
psychiatrists, gynecolo-
sleep disturbances and somatic symptoms has
gists, obstetricians, social workers, and psy-
also been found to be predicted by social class
chotherapists can reduce substance intake and
and employment status.
improve the health situation o f the fetus (Boer, Smit, VanHuis, & Hogerzeil, 1 9 9 4 ) .
symptoms
(Avis, Crawford, &c
Up to 1 0 0 symptoms have been attributed to menopause; depression, anxiety, joint pain, headaches, insomnia, loss o f sexual interest, hot flashes, and vaginal atrophy have been
Menopause
perceived as the
"menopausal
syndrome"
In the developed countries, menopause gen-
(Derry, Gallant, & W o o d s , 1 9 9 7 ) . Research
erally is viewed by women as a negative phase
has suggested that the prevalence o f depres-
of life when symptoms and loss o f well-being
sion in w o m e n is substantially higher during
and function are bound to occur (Hvas, 2 0 0 1 ) .
menopause than during other phases o f life,
Menopause involves biological and psycholog-
possibly due to the changing hormone levels
ical changes and can be an important develop-
(Kaufert, Gilbert, & T a t e , 1 9 9 2 ) . Estrogen is
mental phase that is influenced by specific
capable o f modulating serotonergic function
sociocultural factors
(Dennerstein,
1996).
in the central nervous
system, and
this
Evidence shows that women at age 5 0 years
interaction may explain the increased vulner-
are generally in good health and experience
ability to
emotional stability as well as positive personal
during menopause (Joffe 8c Cohen, 1 9 9 8 ) .
development (Fodor & Franks, 1 9 9 0 ) . Studies
Population-based
during past decades have shown that
the
failed to find a co-occurrence o f symptoms.
duration, severity, and impact of symptoms
M o s t symptoms, other than hot flashes, night
affective disorders studies,
in
women
however,
have
vary by individuals as well as by populations.
sweats, vaginal atrophy, and insomnia, do
Preconceived attitudes concerning menopause
not
increase
in
frequency
during
the
472
SPECIAL ISSUES menopausal years (Ballinger, 1 9 9 0 ) . Recent
with
studies have shown that the reductions in
modeling, learned coping strategies, and sever-
estrogen during menopause do not influence
ity of the stressor have an interlocking effect
mental well-being (Slaven &
on health.
Lee,
1998);
therefore, the occurrence o f depression can-
stress-related
Research
has
disorders.
shown
that,
Personality,
in general,
not be attributed to hormonal deficiency.
w o m e n report higher distress and lower qual-
Epidemiological studies have shown mainly
ity o f life than do men (Gamma &C Angst,
that psychosocial stressors were more likely
2 0 0 1 ) . W o m e n may also experience different
t o predict depression. Such stressors may
types o f stressors than do men. W o m e n are
include existent health problems, responsibil-
more vulnerable to specific life events causing
ity for the care o f relatives, negative attitudes
distress such as physical and sexual abuse, sex-
toward aging, and history o f previous depres-
ual discrimination, distorted body image, and
sion (Woods & Mitchell, 1 9 9 6 ) .
living with multiple roles in life. M e n , on the
In summary, menopause can be regarded as a natural process with symptoms that may vary
other hand,
experience stress from
high-
responsibility jobs or hazardous occupations
individually. Unfortunately, it is often viewed as
( O ' L e a r y S c Helgeson, 1 9 9 7 ) . Historically,
a medical problem, labeling experiences as
w o m e n have suffered more from relationship
symptoms and illness instead o f underlining the
stressors and have been more vulnerable to
positive aspects such as the possibility for inner
partnership or marital dissatisfaction. Parent
growth and the strengthening o f inner wisdom
role stress and the quality o f the parent-child
and power
relationship seem to cause greater distress for
(Hvas, 2 0 0 1 ) . Midlife distress
occurring at the time of menopause can be
women
attributed to personal, familial, or social events
w o m e n generally are more socialized to feel
that occur more frequently around the time o f
responsible for their families (Simon, 1 9 9 2 ) .
menopause. Counseling women is important.
than for men, p r o b a b l y
because
In women, marital stress has been shown to
Counseling must not only address medical risks
have a significant effect
and physical symptoms but also help women to
health. W o m e n admitted to the hospital with
on cardiovascular
overcome life event stressors, through which
an acute coronary event and w h o reported
women's inner resources are strengthened, so as
marital stress at baseline had a worse progno-
to avoid medicalization and disempowerment
sis than did those w h o did not report marital
(Hollnagel & Malterud, 1 9 9 5 ) .
stress. Moreover, women with severe marital stress had a threefold greater risk for a new coronary event than did those not experiencing
PSYCHOSOCIAL FACTORS INFLUENCING WOMEN'S HEALTH Stress and Coping
marital stress (Orth-Gomér e t a l . , 2 0 0 0 ) . In contrast to men, work stress did not have an effect on the recurrence of a new coronary episode among women. T h e experience of stress and the impact it has on health are
Stress is an everyday phenomenon that influ-
shown t o be gender specific; therefore, inter-
ences the lives o f women and men and affects
ventions designed to train women to cope with
each person differendy. Learning to cope adap-
stress must take these differences into account.
tively with everyday stressors is important for
Ways o f coping with specific stressors are
mental health in both genders. T h e amount o f
divided into problem-focused coping (i.e., chang-
stress that one can withstand, and the length o f
ing the actual stressful situation) and emotion-
time that a person experiences a stressor, is an
focused coping (i.e., attempting to palliate the
important factor to take into consideration
reaction to stress) (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984).
Women's Adaptive or maladaptive coping mechanisms are
life events and chronic strain
incorporated into a person's lifestyle during
McLeod,
Health (Kessler &
1 9 8 5 ) . W o m e n more
frequently
youth through learning and modeling from
perceive their spouses as less supportive, and
parents and peers. Strategies for coping with
this can lead to strain in relationships. A
stressful life events depend on personal character,
Swedish prospective study showed that men
relationship characteristics, and the nature o f the
usually perceive their spouses as the primary
stressor. Coping style, whether positive or nega-
providers of social support, whereas women
tive, influences health as well as the outcome of
typically name close female relatives as the per-
an illness. W o m e n have been found to use more
sons giving them support (Karasek, Baker,
expressive ways of coping (e.g., expressing feel-
M a r x e r , Ahlbom, & Theorell, 1 9 8 1 ) . Given
ings, txirning to their social support network),
women's critical need for emotional functional
whereas men use more rational ways of coping
support, the development o f community social
with stress (e.g., exercising). Coping with stress
support systems for women could lead to the
through the use o f social support seems to be
prevention o f psychological distress.
more important in women (Thoits, 1 9 9 1 ) . Ruminating over stressful events also seems to be a more common coping mechanism in women
Multiple Roles
than in men; men usually distract themselves
During the past decade, numerous studies
from the stressful situation (Nolen-Hoeksema,
have shown the positive effects o f multiple-role
1 9 8 7 ) . One can conclude that there are some
involvements on women's physical and mental
specific differences in coping strategies between
well-being (Barnett &C Baruch, 1 9 8 5 ) . T h e
the genders and that prevention of mental disor-
more roles a woman fulfills, the better physical
ders, mainly depression, can be targeted by
health, higher life satisfaction, and less depres-
developing more adaptive ways o f coping
sion she may experience. Multiple roles are
among women.
beneficial, but the quality and combination of roles can sometimes have a negative influence on a woman's life. M a n y roles drain energy,
Social Support
and this may result in conflict and have a neg-
Both the quality and quantity of a person's
ative influence on well-being (Barnett, 1 9 9 3 ) .
social network define perceived social support.
Family roles are regarded as women's core
T w o types o f social support measures are usu-
roles, and success in the roles o f wife and
ally distinguished, namely structural,
charac-
mother has been considered fundamental for
terized by the quantity and structure o f the
psychological well-being and thought to be less
reflecting
stressful than the worker role. Wife roles can
emotional, instrumental, and informational sup-
often be in conflict with mother roles, and this
port and social companionship (Wills, 1 9 9 8 ) .
can lead to distress. In today's modern society,
Social support has been recognized as a major
declaring the distribution o f roles between
social network, and functional,
protective factor for physical illness (e.g.,
both genders, but not depriving either from its
cardiovascular disease) as well as for mental
natural or assumed roles, may actually assist
disorders.
women and men in participating in both
W o m e n are extremely vulnerable to psychological distress if their social support system
family and work lives without experiencing a great degree o f role stress.
is inadequate. Perceived high emotional sup-
Studies show that women sometimes use
port is directly associated with better physical
psychoactive substances to ameliorate anxi-
usually
eties, depression, and feelings o f worthlessness
decreases health-damaging effects of negative
resulting from gender role expectations that
and
psychological health, and
it
474
SPECIAL ISSUES are difficult for them to meet (Bollerud, 1 9 9 0 ;
among women. Socioeconomic inequalities
R o o t , 1 9 8 9 ) . Results o f several studies have
result in disparities in health and determinant
found inconsistencies regarding family roles.
factors o f income inequality; for example, an
Some studies have found that there are advan-
increase in the number
tages for married women (e.g., Verbrugge,
households and an increase in the number of
o f female-headed
1 9 8 2 ) , whereas others have shown that life sat-
women in the j o b market can intensify this
isfaction and well-being are just as high or
effect on women's health. Psychosocial health
higher among single and employed women
determinants, such as j o b characteristics, j o b
(Nadelson &c Notman, 1 9 8 1 ) . Multiple roles
strain, low level o f control at work, high
clearly improve self-esteem, satisfaction with
demands, limited personal resources, frustra-
life, and well-being if the roles complement
tion, and discrimination in the workplace
each other, that is, if the roles result in the feel-
(Moss, 2 0 0 2 ) , all can lead to distress, and this
ing o f being successful both personally and
can be deleterious to health when experienced
financially.
chronically.
T w o hypotheses have been constructed to
Women
commonly work
in jobs
that
explain the ways in which social mechanisms
involve high psychological demands and offer
influence health differently between the gen-
low
ders. T h e differential
exposure
levels o f control (Karasek & Theorell,
hypothesis
1 9 9 0 ) , and this low level of control can lead to
posits that women have higher levels o f
cardiovascular disease, sickness, absence from
demands and obligations in their social roles
work, and psychological distress (Walters
and have fewer coping resources to help them
et al., 2 0 0 2 ) . Nevertheless, women working in
solve arising problems and that this leads
paid jobs generally have better health status
them to complain more about ill health than
than do full-time homemakers (Khlat, Sermet,
do men. T h e differential
vulnerability
hypoth-
& Le Pape, 2 0 0 0 ) . W o m e n working full-time
esis posits that women have stronger reactions
outside their homes are usually responsible for
to life events or stressors that men also experi-
domestic labor, sometimes leading to role
ence and that this increased reactivity and low
strain. In studying the influence of family,
levels o f coping resources lead to ill health
work, and material circumstances on health, it
(Walters, M c D o n o u g h , &c Strohschein, 2 0 0 2 ) .
was found that ill health among women was
Although the mechanisms o f gender differ-
subject not only to employment status, occu-
ences in ill health remain unresolved to this
pational class, and housing tenure (as found
day, it is obvious that social support (from
among men) but also to marital and parental
both family and society) and the development
status (Arber, 1 9 9 1 ) . Recently, a study found
of adaptive coping mechanisms should help
that the most important predictors o f good
women to overcome distress arising from mul-
health among women were being in the high-
tiple roles and that this, in turn, should lead to
est income category, working full-time, caring
better health status o f women.
for a family, and having high levels of social support (Denton & Walters, 1 9 9 9 ) .
Socioeconomic Factors
Health is determined by the coexistence of biologic, genetic, psychological, and social
It is well known that socioeconomic status
factors. Social relations, such as discrimination,
significantly influences physical and mental
exclusion, and exploitation, have an indirect
health. W o m e n around the world live in
influence on the health o f women, especially
greater poverty and have lower education and
those w h o are part o f a minority ethnic group,
employment status than do men; therefore,
through economic and social well-being. In
the impact o f socioeconomic factors is greater
studying ethnic groups, it was found
that
Women's
Health
475
health differences between the genders were
disorders. Secondary and tertiary prevention o f
accentuated, but after controlling for educa-
debilitating disability in life is crucial so as to
tional level and employment status, members
allow women to lead fulfilling lives.
o f minority groups were in equally good health as those not belonging to ethnic minority groups (Cooper, 2 0 0 2 ) .
Depression
Self-ratings of health and diseases are reli-
Depression is the second most c o m m o n
able predictors o f morbidity. A recent study
disease worldwide, with a lifetime prevalence o f
showed that personal income inequalities were
1 5 % and perhaps as high as 2 5 % for women
connected with differences in self-rated morbid-
(Kaplan & Sadock, 1 9 9 8 ) . W o m e n experience
ity in women but not as significantly as in
longer episodes of depression, symptoms can be
men. M e n were more susceptible to income
more severe, and recurrence is more common.
inequality or to the loss o f hierarchy status,
T h e occurrence o f depression during childhood
which coexists with lower socioeconomic status.
in both genders is similar, but the onset o f
Nevertheless, depression was a more important
depression is found to be earlier in women,
predictor o f self-rated morbidity than were
and depression is already found twice as fre-
socioeconomic deprivation factors. This indi-
quently in adolescent girls as in adolescent boys
cates that socioeconomic deprivation predicts
(Angold & Worthman, 1 9 9 3 ) . T h e gap in the
morbidity much more significantly if it is asso-
prevalence o f depression starts to widen during
ciated with depressive symptomatology (Kopp,
adolescence; young w o m e n
Skrabski, & Szedmâk, 2 0 0 0 ) .
already experience depressive symptoms more
It can be concluded that socioeconomic
in their teens
frequently than do their male counterparts.
factors play an important role in w o m e n ' s
Depressive
health but that w o m e n are at greater risk for
among women with low socioeconomic status,
symptoms
are more
common
these effects if they are suffering from depres-
women living in poverty, and women experi-
sive symptoms. Prevention programs should
encing role strain, marital discord, and physical
generally
or psychological abuse. Research studying the
minority women and w o m e n w h o are at risk
relationship between health-damaging behav-
for, or suffering from, depressive symptoms.
ior and depression in women has shown that
include specific target
groups,
binge drinking is closely correlated with depression (Dunne, Galatopoulos, & Schipperheijn, WOMEN'S MENTAL HEALTH
1 9 9 3 ) and that daily or weekend drinking is associated with low self-esteem (Turnbull &
There is a growing awareness o f the increasing burden o f disability associated with mental ill-
Gomberg, 1 9 8 8 ) . W o m e n are often vulnerable to depressive
ness (U.S. Department o f Health and Human
symptoms at certain periods o f the reproduc-
Services, 1 9 9 9 ) , resulting in decreased func-
tive cycle. Depressive symptoms
tioning and well-being. Depressive symptoms
experienced by w o m e n before the onset o f
and anxiety disorder are among the most c o m -
menstruation, after giving birth to a child, or
can
be
mon health problems. Depression and anxiety
at the time o f menopause. T h e changes in
are important women's health issues because
h o r m o n a l levels alone do not explain the true
these disorders occur nearly twice as often in women than in men. T h e early onset, chronic, and recurrent nature o f depression and anxiety impede the lives o f women, who are unable to function as whole humans due to their
cause o f the depressive symptoms.
Most
likely, psychosocial and biological factors both contribute to the whole clinical picture. Premenstrual syndrome (PMS) is a condition that presents during the luteal phase of the
476
SPECIAL ISSUES menstrual cycle, a few days before menstruation.
factors for postpartum depression (O'Hara,
Investigations o f P M S have not been able to
Schlechte, Lewis, & Varner, 1 9 9 1 ) .
find conclusive evidence of hormonal differ-
T h e prevention and treatment o f depres-
ences (i.e., in estrogen and progesterone) or
sion warrants the charting o f biological and
changes in the
psychosocial factors for w o m e n .
neurotransmitter
system's
Women
response to gonadal hormones. Cultural and
are predisposed to unique stressors that can
psychological factors resulting in P M S must
develop into depression, and these factors
also be taken in account when studying the
need to be addressed by screening for individ-
etiology o f the disorder. According to cognitive
uals at risk. School, community, and work-
theory, distress can be induced by the physical
place
symptoms themselves and by the meaning
specifically to prevent depressive symptoms
given to these symptoms by women. M a n y
and enhance protective factors can reduce suf-
times, the symptoms mean a lack of control of
fering from major depression and improve the
mental
health
programs
designed
a woman's own body and may result in depres-
quality o f life o f women predisposed to this
sion and anxiety. A controlled trial of cognitive-
debilitating disorder.
behavioral therapy (Blake, Salkovskis, Gath, Day, & Garrod, 1 9 9 8 ) for premenstrual syndrome was associated with significant improvement of symptoms, depression, and associated impairments.
Anxiety Disorders T h e prevalence o f anxiety disorders is nearly three times higher in w o m e n than in
Postpartum blues and postpartum depres-
men. Anxiety disorders often are
undiag-
sion are also part o f the m o o d disturbances
nosed for years, and w o m e n suffer in all func-
spectrum connected with hormonal changes.
tions o f their lives. T h e symptoms of anxiety
Postpartum blues usually presents 3 to 5 days
affect women's families and close relation-
after delivery and lasts for several days or a few
ships, resulting in overdependency, conflict in
weeks. Symptoms include dysphoria, crying
relationships, substance abuse, and/or domes-
spells, clinging dependence, irritability, and
tic violence. W o m e n with anxiety disorders
emotional lability and occur in 2 0 % to 4 0 % of
are sometimes blamed for their symptoms
women w h o give birth. Postpartum blues can
and regarded as not being strong enough to
be explained by either sudden hormonal with-
control the symptoms, and the reduction o f
drawal occurring after delivery or by the acti-
self-esteem worsens the symptoms o f anxiety.
vation of oxytocin, which is needed for the
Sensitivity to anxiety is thought to influence
development o f mother-infant
attachment
perception o f health. W o m e n with high anxi-
(Miller, 2 0 0 2 ) . W h e n mothers are supported
ety sensitivity reported more severe menstrual
by the environment and experience low levels
symptoms, more preoccupation with
of stress after childbirth, the neurophysiologi-
sensations,
cal changes promote attachment, but depres-
toward illness (Sigmon, Dorhofer, R o h a n , &
and
more
negative
body
attitudes
sion can result when mothers experience low
Boulard, 2 0 0 0 ) . These women, after perform-
levels of support and high levels o f stress
ing a rumination task, also showed greater
(Miller & Rukstailis, 1 9 9 9 ) . Postpartum blues
skin conductance response magnitude than
can lead to postpartum
did those with low anxiety sensitivity. W o m e n
depression, which
occurs in 1 0 % to 2 0 % of women in the United
suffering from panic disorder also reported
States within 6 months o f delivery. A history o f
more severe menstrual symptoms, higher anx-
major depression, a history o f P M S , the pres-
iety sensitivity, state and trait anxiety, fear o f
ence of a psychosocial stressor, and low levels
body sensations, and illness-related concerns
of social support all can act as predisposing
than did controls (Sigmon et al., 2 0 0 0 ) .
Women's
Health
477
W o m e n suffering from anxiety disorders are
was found to be lower in young (15-year-old)
at a greater risk for alcohol or other substance
women suffering from severe primary dysmen-
abuse. Agoraphobia and social phobia have
orrhea, but this loss o f self-esteem is compen-
to frequently precede alcohol
sated at about age 2 5 years by the development
use disorders (Vogeltanz 8c Wilsnack, 1 9 9 7 ) ,
of more achievement-oriented and aggressive
implying that alcohol or other substances are
attitudes (Holmlund, 1 9 9 0 ) .
been found
used as self-medication to alleviate the symp-
These results imply that social support
toms o f anxiety and to restore function to a cer-
systems o f w o m e n
tain extent. Consequences o f anxiety disorders
dysmenorrhea should be evaluated and also
seeking treatment
for
in women highlight the importance o f identify-
should be screened for depression and anxiety
ing women suffering from anxiety early and
when disrupted social support systems are
offering them effective treatment to prevent
found. Treatment interventions include relax-
further functional and health impairment.
ation alone or with imagery, effectively reducing resting time in spasmodic dysmenorrhea (Amodei, Nelson, Jarrett, & Sigmon, 1 9 8 7 ) .
SPECIAL ISSUES
Chronic Pelvic Pain
Dysmenorrhea
The relationship o f primary chronic pain
Dysmenorrhea, or painful menstruation, is the
most
common
gynecologic
disorder
and psychological factors has been studied during the past two decades, but inconclusive data
a m o n g young w o m e n , with a prevalence o f
do not clear the picture. T h e prevalence rates o f
6 0 % to 9 3 % (Banikarim, C h a c k o , & Kelder,
chronic pain range from as low as 5 % to as
2 0 0 0 ) . A variety o f symptoms, such as vomit-
high as 8 7 % (Réthelyi, Berghammer, &c Kopp,
ing, fatigue, back pain, and headaches, occur
2 0 0 1 ) , and depression has been shown to be
during the experience o f pain. Dysmenorrhea
more prevalent among chronic pain patients.
shows a strong correlation with premenstrual
Chronic pelvic pain, or pain experienced in the
symptoms but is not associated with emo-
area o f the pelvis for more than 6 months, is the
tional
(Freeman,
cause o f 1 0 % of all gynecological outpatient
Rickels, &C Sondheimer, 1 9 9 3 ) . Studies mea-
visits, 4 0 % o f all laparoscopics, and 1 0 %
suring
that
to 1 5 % of all hysterectomies (Gelbaya & El-
women with dysmenorrhea show enhanced
Halwagy, 2 0 0 1 ) and is an important women's
pain perception (e.g., Granot, 2 0 0 1 ) .
health issue.
distress pain
in
adolescents
threshold
have
shown
Research studying the psychosocial back-
T h e etiology of chronic pelvic pain, even
ground factors o f dysmenorrhea has found
after laparoscopy, remains unknown in a sig-
that dysmenorrheic women report less social
nificant proportion o f patients. Some connec-
support, characterized by inadequate and geo-
tion has been found between high scores on
graphically distant
(Whittle,
measures o f neuroticism, but other studies
Slade, & Ronalds, 1 9 8 7 ) . A disrupted social
have found no connection between chronic
support system moderates the
pelvic
relationships
relationship
pain
and
anxiety
or
depression
between distress and menstrual pain, and these
(Wilkie & Schmidt, 1 9 9 8 ) . Childhood trau-
women complained o f more symptoms than
matic events are also hypothesized to be
did those whose social network was intact
closely associated with chronic pain, and high
(Alonso & Coe, 2 0 0 1 ) . Depression and anxi-
incidences o f sexual abuse (Toomey, Hernandez,
ety were also found to be strongly associated
Gittelman, S c Hulka,
with menstrual pain. In addition, self-esteem
abuse (Rapkin, Kames, D a r k e , Stampler, Sc
1 9 9 4 ) and physical
SPECIAL ISSUES
478
Naliboff, 1 9 9 0 ) were found in chronic pelvic
life satisfaction, and more self-blame (Greil, 1 9 9 7 ) . In one study, nearly half of the women
pain patients. tremendous
reported that infertility was the worst experi-
impact on the lives o f women who suffer from
ence in their lives (Freeman, Boxer, Rickels,
this disorder. Uncertainty and anxiety con-
Tureck, Se Mastroianni, 1 9 8 5 ) . W o m e n are
Chronic pelvic pain has a
cerned with the pain plays an important role
also known to seek treatment for infertility
in
more often than are men; therefore, women
functional
impairment.
Many
times,
women are confronted with a lack of concern
also experience the distress of medicalization
and understanding or disbelief expressed by
of their problems. Psychological distress is
(Savidge, Slade,
usually caused by undiagnosed (or idiopathic)
Stewart, Sc Li, 1 9 9 8 ) . Verbal reassurance by
infertility; otherwise, the diagnosis seems to be
health care professionals
health care workers that the cause for the pain
unrelated to psychological distress (Shatford,
cannot be found does not alleviate the distress
Hearn, Yuzpe, Brown, Sc Casper,
experienced by women with this disorder.
Pregnancy has been found to decrease the level
Psychological treatment o f chronic pelvic pain of unknown cause should, after careful
1988).
of psychological distress in infertile women (Benazon, Wright, Sc Sabourin, 1 9 9 2 ) .
medical examination, include behavioral ther-
It can be concluded that infertile women
apy techniques, such as operant conditioning,
are clearly affected by their feelings of inade-
to decrease the frequency o f illness behavior
quacy to fulfill a parental role and that mental
and to strengthen health-promoting behaviors.
health professionals often need to identify and
Relaxation techniques are also recommended
treat the distress in these women. Evidence
to decrease muscle tension in the pelvic area.
shows, however, that the level of distress expe-
Cognitive therapy,
rienced by infertile women is not higher than
by treating
depressive
symptoms and anxiety accompanying chronic
that experienced by fertile women.
pain, is also an effective method o f treatment (Wilkie Sc Schmidt, 1 9 9 8 ) .
Victimization of Women
Infertility
A large percentage o f women all over the be
world are victims o f sexual, physical, and/or
attributed to either partner (Guidice, 1 9 9 8 ) .
psychological abuse. Abuse can be perpetrated
About
1 0 % o f infertility
cannot
Researchers hypothesize that infertility has
by parents, close relatives, intimate partners,
psychological causes (known as the
employers, or coworkers. Both physical abuse
psychohas
and sexual abuse have serious effects on
psychological consequences (known as the
physical and mental health, with domestic vio-
genic
hypothesis)
or that
infertility
hypothesis).
lence and rape significantly causing morbidity
Numerous studies have provided evidence
and mortality among women ages 15 to
that infertility does not have significant psy-
4 4 years worldwide, accounting for 6 % of the
chological causes (Greil, 1 9 9 7 ) .
total disability-adjusted life years o f healthy life
psychological
consequences
The experience o f infertility differs between
lost and 9 0 % of the morbidity associated with
the genders. Infertility is a more stressful expe-
disability from injury (World Bank, 1 9 9 3 ) .
rience for women than it is for men, and there
Unfortunately,
abused women rarely seek
are specific psychological measures that are
health care for their health complaints, and
more common among women, namely lower
when they do seek such help, they are secretive
self-esteem, more depressive symptoms, lower
about the causes o f their symptoms.
Women's Violence against women causes significant
Health
479
SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS
psychological distress, which most frequently is a direct consequence o f their experience o f
Health issues concerning w o m e n highlighted
abuse. Across all cultures, battered women
in this chapter underline the necessity o f
who are abused by their partners are charac-
health p r o m o t i o n
terized by low self-esteem, increased insomnia,
interventions designed specifically for women.
hypervigilance, an augmented startle response,
Interventions should take into account health
a sense of disorder, and unhappiness (Fishbach
problems arising in minority groups, in the
and
disease
prevention
& Herbert, 1 9 9 7 ) . Research has shown that
lower socioeconomic strata, among women
battering may subsequently result in longlasting
with lower education, and among women
psychiatric
w h o are victims o f violence.
morbidity
(Mullen,
Romans-
Clarkson, W a l t o n , & Herbison, 1 9 8 8 ) and
Especially important is the primary preven-
that abuse is considered to be a significant
tion of mental health disorders, targeting young
predictor o f the development o f lifelong men-
women and teaching them skills o f adaptive
tal health problems (Kilpatrick, Best, Veronen,
ways of coping, hardiness, and resiliency. Such
Villeponteaux, & Ruff,
Psychiatric
protective factors are important to enable young
morbidity, in association with sexual or physical
women to withstand peer pressure for health-
abuse, includes depression (Goldberg, 1 9 9 4 ) ,
damaging behavior, to prevent the disabling
posttraumatic stress disorder (Roth, Newman,
effect of chronic distress, and to take responsibil-
Pelcovitz, V a n Der Kolk, & Mandel, 1 9 9 7 ) ,
ity for their own health. Special attention needs
substance use disorders (Schafer, Schnack, &
to be given to women who are at high risk
Soyka, 2 0 0 0 ) , dissociative symptomatology
for
(Brunner, Parzer, Schuld, Sc Resch, 2 0 0 0 ) , and
example, women living in homes with parental
dissociative
identity
1985).
disorder
( H o c k e Sc
Schmidtke, 1 9 9 8 ) . A major task for medical professionals is to
developing certain mental disorders, for
conflict, women experiencing sexual or physical abuse, and women who drop out of school early. Improving
social support
systems
for
identify and give proper treatment to abused
women seems to be a crucial issue in prevent-
women. Health care providers need to be edu-
ing psychological distress and disorder as well
cated on h o w to include questions on batter-
as illnesses resulting from chronic stress.
ing and sexual abuse while taking patients'
Hotlines readily available for women in crisis
histories, h o w to react to an emerging prob-
and counseling services in schools, communi-
lem, and h o w to determine the type of treat-
ties, and workplaces are important ways of
ment to which w o m e n should be referred.
giving support to w o m e n in need. But most
Battered women are in great need o f psycho-
important o f all is raising awareness in the
logical interventions, social support, and sup-
community and among health professionals
port from culture and society. Society also has
about not only the physical but also the psy-
to be informed about the problems arising
chosocial aspects o f physical and mental ill-
from battering and needs to adopt an altitude
ness. Raising such awareness may eventually
of not tolerating violence in the culture.
result in better health among women.
REFERENCES Alexander, M . J . , &C Higgins, Ε. T. (1993). Emotional trade-offs of becoming a parent: How social roles influence self-discrepancy effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 1 2 5 9 - 1 2 6 9 .
480
SPECIAL ISSUES Alonso, C , & Coe, C. L. (2001). Disruptions of social relationships accentuate the association between emotional distress and menstrual pain in young women. Health Psychology, 20, 4 1 1 - 4 1 6 . Amodei, N., Nelson, R. O., Jarrett, R. B., S i Sigmon, S. (1987). Psychological treatments of dysmenorrhea: Differential effectiveness for spasmodics and congestives. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 18, 9 5 - 1 0 3 . Angold, Α., Sc Worthman, C. W. (1993). Puberty onset of gender differences in rates of depression: A developmental, epidemiologic, and neuroendocrine perspective. Journal of Affective Disorders, 29, 1 4 5 - 1 5 8 . Arber, S. (1991). Comparing inequalities in women's and men's health: Britain in the 1990s. Social Science & Medicine, 44, 7 7 3 - 7 8 7 . Avis, Ν. E., Crawford, S. L., Sc McKinlay, S. M. (1997). Psychosocial, behavioral, and health factors related to menopause symptomatology. Women's Health, 3,103-120. Ballinger, C. (1990). Psychiatric aspects of the menopause. British Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 7 7 3 - 7 8 7 . Banikarim, C , Chacko, M. R., Sc Kelder, S. H. (2000). Prevalence and impact of dysmenorrhea on Hispanic female adolescents. Archives of Pediatrica and Adolescent Medicine, 154, 1 2 2 6 - 1 2 2 9 . Barnett, R. C. (1993). Multiple roles, gender, and psychological distress. In L. Goldberger Se S. Breznitz (Eds.), Handbook of stress. New York: Free Press. Barnett, R. C , Se Baruch, G. K. (1985). Women's involvement in multiple roles and psychological distress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 135-145. Benazon, N., Wright, J . , 8c Sabourin, S. (1992). Stress, sexual satisfaction, and marital adjustment in infertile couples. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 18, 2 7 3 - 2 8 4 . Blake, F., Salkovskis, P., Gath, D., Day, Α., & Garrod, A. (1998). Cognitive therapy for premenstrual syndrome: A controlled trial. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 45, 3 0 7 - 3 1 8 . Boer, K., Smit, B. J . , VanHuis, A. M., Sc Hogerzeil, Η. V. (1994). Substance use in pregnancy: Do we care? Acta Paediatrica, 404(Suppl.), 6 5 - 7 1 . Bollerud, K. (1990), A model for the treatment of trauma related syndromes among chemically dependent inpatient women. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 7, 8 3 - 8 7 . Brunner, R., Parzer, P., Schuld, V., Sc Resch, F. (2000). Dissociative symptomatology and traumatogenic factors in adolescent psychiatric patients. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 188, 7 1 - 7 7 . Clark, D. B . , Pollock, N., Bukstein, O. G., Mezzich, A. C , Bromberger, J . T., Sc Donovan, J . E. (1997). Gender and comorbid psychopathology in adolescents with alcohol dependence. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 1 1 9 5 - 1 2 0 3 . Cooper, H. (2002). Investigating socio-economic explanations for gender and ethnic inequalities in health. Social Science & Medicine, 54, 6 9 3 - 7 0 6 . Cranley, M. S., Hedahl, K. J . , Sc Pegg, S. H. (1983). Women's perceptions of vaginal and cesarean deliveries. Nursing Research, 32, 1 0 - 1 5 . Dennerstein, L. (1996). Well-being, symptoms, and the menopausal transition. Maturitas, 23, 1 4 7 - 1 5 7 . Denton, M., Sc Walters, V. (1999). Gender differences in structural and behavioural determinants of health: An analysis of the social production of health. Social Science & Medicine, 48, 1 2 2 1 - 1 2 3 5 . Derry, P. S., Gallant, S. J . , & Woods, N. F. (1997). Premenstrual syndrome and menopause. In S. J . Gallant, G. P. Keita, & R. Royak-Schaler (Eds.), Health care for women. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. DiMatteo, M. R., Morton, S., Lepper, H. S., Damush, T., Carney, M., Pearson, M., Sc Kahn, K. L. (1996). Cesarean childbirth and psychosocial outcomes: A meta-analysis. Health Psychology, 15, 2 3 0 - 2 4 1 .
Women's Dunne, F. J . , Galatopoulos, C , Sc Schipperheijn, J . M . (1993). Gender differences in psychiatric morbidity among alcohol misusers. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 34, 9 5 - 1 0 1 . Fischer, G., Bitschnau, M., Peternell, Α., Eder, H., Sc Topitz, A. (1999). Pregnancy and substance abuse. Archives of Women's Mental Health, 2, 5 7 - 6 5 . Fishbach, R. L., Sc Herbert, B . (1997). Domestic violence and mental health: Correlates and conundrums within and across cultures. Social Science & Medicine, 45, 1 1 6 1 - 1 1 7 6 . Fodor, I. G., Sc Franks, V. (1990). Women in midlife and beyond: The new prime of life? Psychology of Women Quarterly, 14, 4 4 5 - 4 4 9 . Freeman, E. W., Boxer, A. W., Rickels, K., Tureck, R., & Mastroianni, L. (1985). Psychological evaluation and support in a program of in vitro fertilization and embryo transfer. Fertility and Sterility, 4, 4 8 - 5 3 . Freeman, E. W., Rickels, K., Sc Sondheimer, S. J . (1993). Premenstrual symptoms and dysmenorrhea in relation to emotional distress factors in adolescents. Journal of Psychosomatic Obstetrics and Gynaecology, 14, 4 1 - 5 0 . Gamma, Α., Sc Angst, J . (2001). Concurrent psychiatric comorbidity and multimorbidity in a community study: Gender differences and quality of life. 251 (Suppl. 2 ) , European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 43-46. Gelbaya, Τ. Α., & El-Halwagy, Η. E. (2001). Focus on primary care: Chronic pelvic pain in women. Obstetrical & Gynecological Survey, 12, 7 5 7 - 7 6 4 . Goldberg, R. T. (1994). Childhood abuse, depression, and chronic pain. Clinical Journal of Pain, 10, 2 7 7 - 2 8 1 . Graber, J . Α., & Brooks-Gunn, J . (1998). Puberty in behavioral medicine and women. In E. A. Blechman & K. D. Brownell (Eds.), Behavioral medicine and women: A comprehensive handbook (pp. 5 1 - 5 8 ) . New York: Guilford. Granot, M . (2001). Pain perception in women with dysmenorrhea. Obstetrics & Gynecology, 98, 4 0 7 - 4 1 1 . Greene, J . G., & Cooke, D. J . (1980). Life stress and symptoms at the climacterium. British Journal of Psychiatry, 136, 4 8 6 - 4 9 1 . Greil, A. L. (1997). Infertility and psychological distress: A critical review of the literature. Social Science & Medicine, 45, 1 6 7 9 - 1 7 0 4 . Guidice, L. (1998). Reproductive technologies. In E. A Blechman Se K. D. Brownell (Eds.), Behavioral medicine and women (pp. 5 1 5 - 5 1 9 ) . New York: Guilford. Hocke, V., Sc Schmidtke, A. (1998). Zeitschrift fur Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie und Psychothérapie, 26, 2 7 3 - 2 8 4 . Hollnagel, H., & Malterud, K. (1995). Shifting attention from objective risk factors to patients self-assessed health resources: A clinical model for general practice. Family Practice, 12, 4 2 3 - 4 2 9 . Holmlund, U. (1990). The experience of dysmenorrhea and its relationship to personality variables. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 82, 1 8 2 - 1 8 7 . Hvas, L. (2001). Positive aspects of menopause: A qualitative study. Maturitas, 39, 11-17. Jarvis, M. A. E., Sc Schnoll, S. H. (1995). Methadone use during pregnancy. NIDA Research Monographs, 149, 5 8 - 7 7 . Joffe, H., Sc Cohen, L. S. (1998). Estrogen, serotonin, and mood disturbance: Where is the therapeutic bridge? Biological Psychiatry, 44, 7 9 8 - 8 1 1 . Kaplan, H. L, & Sadock, B . J . (1998). Kaplan and Sadock's synopsis of psychiatry: Behavioral sciences/clinical psychiatry (8th ed.). Baltimore, M D : Williams & Wilkins. Karasek, R., Baker, D., Marxer, F., Ahlbom, Α., Sc Theorell, T. (1981). Job decision latitude, job demands, and CVD: A prospective study of Swedish men. American Journal of Public Health, 71, 6 9 4 - 7 0 5 .
Health
481
482
SPECIAL ISSUES Karasek, R., Sc Theorell, T. (1990). Healthy work: Stress, productivity, and reconstruction of working life. New York: Basic Books. Kaufert, P. Α., Gilbert, P., & Tate, R. (1992). The Manitoba Project: A re-examination of the link between menopause and depression. Maturitas, 14, 1 4 3 - 1 5 5 . Kessler, R., Sc McLeod, J . (1985). Social support and mental health in community samples. In S. Cohen Se S. L. Syme (Eds.), Social support and health. New York: Academic Press. Khlat, R., Sermet, C , Se Le Pape, Α. (2000). Women's health in relation with their family and work roles: France in the early 1990s. Social Science & Medicine, 50, 1 8 0 7 - 1 8 2 5 . Khoury, A. J . , 8c Weisman, C. S., (2002). Thinking about women's health: The case for gender sensitivity. Women's Health Issues, 12, 6 1 - 6 5 . Kilpatrick, D. G., Best, C. L., Veronen, L. J . , Villeponteaux, L. Α., Se Ruff, G. A. (1985). Mental health correlates of criminal victimization: A random community survey. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 8 6 6 - 8 7 3 . Kopp, M., & Skrabski, A. (1995). Behavioural sciences applied to a changing society. Budapest, Hungary: Bibliotheca Septem Artium Liberalium. Kopp, M. S., Skrabski, Α., & Szedmak, S. (2000). Psychosocial risk factors, inequality, and self-rated morbidity in a changing society. Social Science & Medicine, 51, 1 3 5 1 - 1 3 6 1 . Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer. Lock, M . (1993). Encounters with aging: Mythologies of menopause in Japan and North America. Berkeley: University of California Press. Psychosomatic Matthews, K. (1992). Myths and realities of the menopause. Medicine, 54, 1-9. Miller, L. J . (2002). Postpartum depression. Journal of the American Medical Association, 287, 7 6 2 - 7 6 5 . Miller, L. J . , Sc Rukstailis, M . (1999). Beyond the "blues" hypotheses about postpartum reactivity. In L. J . Miller (Ed.), Postpartum mood disorders (pp. 3 - 1 9 ) . Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, Α., Belsky, J . , & Silva, P. A. (1992). Childhood experience and Development, the onset of menarche: A test of a sociobiological model. Child 63, 4 7 - 5 8 . Moss, Ν. E. (2002). Gender equity and socioeconomic inequality: A framework for the patterning of women's health. Social Science & Medicine, 54, 6 4 9 - 6 6 1 . Mullen, P. E., Romans-Clarkson, S. E., Walton, V. Α., & Herbison, G. P. (1988). Impact of sexual and physical abuse on women's mental health. Lancet, 1, 841-845. Nadelson, C. C , Sc Notman, M . T. (1981). To marry or not to marry: A choice. American Journal of Psychiatry, 138, 1 3 5 2 - 1 3 5 6 . Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1987). Sex difference in unipolar depression: Evidence and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 2 5 9 - 2 8 2 . O'Hara, M. W., Schlechte, J . Α., Lewis, D. Α., Se Varner, M . W. (1991). Controlled prospective study of postpartum mood disorders: Psychological, environmental, and hormonal variables. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 6 3 - 7 3 . O'Leary, Α., & Helgeson, V. S. (1997). Psychosocial factors and Women's health: Integrating mind, heart, and body. In S. J . Gallant, G. P. Keita, & R. RoyakSchaler (Eds.), Health care for women: Psychological, social, and behavioral influences (pp. 2 5 - 4 0 ) . Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Orth-Gomér, K., Wamala, S. P., Horsten, M . , Schenck-Gustafsson, K., Schneiderman, W., Se Mittleman, M . A. (2000). Marital stress worsens progMedical nosis in women with coronary heart disease. Journal of the American Association, 284, 3 0 0 8 - 3 0 1 4 .
Women's Polit, D. F., &c LaRocco, S. A. (1980). Social and psychological correlates of menopausal symptoms. Psychosomatic Medicine, 42, 3 3 5 - 3 4 5 . Rapkin, A. J . , Karnes, L. D., Darke, L. L., Stampler, F. M., & Naliboff, B. D. (1990). History of physical and sexual abuse in women with chronic pelvic pain. Obstetrics & Gynecology, 76, 92-96. Réthelyi, J . M., Berghammer, R., & Kopp, M. S. (2001). Comorbidity of painassociated disability and depressive symptoms in connection with sociodemographic variables: Results from a cross-sectional epidemiological survey in Hungary. Pain, 93, 1 1 5 - 1 2 1 . Root, M . P. P. (1989). Treatment failures: The role of sexual victimization in women's addictive behavior. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 59, 5 4 2 - 5 4 9 . Roth, S., Newman, E., Pelcovitz, D., Van Der Kolk, B . , & Mandel, F. S. (1997). Complex PTSD in victims exposed to sexual and physical abuse: Results from the DSM-IV field trial for postraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 10, 5 3 9 - 5 5 5 . Savidge, C. J . , Slade, P., Stewart, P., & Li, T. C. (1998). Women's perspectives on their experiences of chronic pelvic pain and medical care. Journal of Health Psychology, 3, 1 0 3 - 1 1 6 . Schafer, M., Schnack, B . , & Soyka, M . (2000). Sexual and physical abuse during early childhood or adolescence and later drug addiction. Psychotherapy, Psychosomatic Medicine, Medical Psychology, 50, 3 8 - 5 0 . Shatford, L. Α., Hearn, M. T., Yuzpe, Α. Α., Brown, S. E., & Casper, R. F. (1988). Psychological correlates of differential infertility diagnosis in an in vitro fertilization program. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology, 158, 1099-1107. Shearer, E. C. (1983). How do parents really feel after cesarean birth? Birth, 10, 91-92. Sigmon, S. T., Dorhofer, D. M., Rohan, K. J . , & Boulard, Ν. E. (2000). The impact of anxiety sensitivity, bodily expectations, and cultural beliefs on menstrual symptom reporting: A test of the menstrual reactivity hypothesis. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 14, 6 1 5 - 6 3 3 . Sigmon, S. T., Dorhofer, D. M., Rohan, K. J . , Hotovy, L. Α., Boulard, Ν. E., & Fink, C. M . (2000). Psychophysiological, somatic, and affective changes across the menstrual cycle in women with panic disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 4 2 5 - 4 3 1 . Simon, R. (1992). Parental role strains, salience of parental identity, and gender differences in psychological distress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 33, 25-35. Slaven, L., & Lee, C. (1998). A cross-sectional survey of menopausal status, symptoms, and psychological distress in a community sample of Australian women. Journal of Health Psychology, 3, 1 1 7 - 1 2 3 . Thoits, P. A. (1991). Gender differences in coping with emotional distress. In J . Eckenrole (Ed.), The social context of coping (pp. 1 0 7 - 1 3 8 ) . New York: Plenum. Toomey, T. C , Hernandez, J . T., Gittelman, D. F., & Hulka, J . F. (1994). Relationship of sexual and physical abuse to pain and psychological assessment variables in chronic pelvic pain patients. Pain, 56, 1 0 5 - 1 0 9 . Turnbull, J . E., & Gomberg, E. S. (1988). Impact of depressive symptomatology on alcohol problems in women. Alcoholism, Clinical, and Experimental Research, 12, 3 7 4 - 3 8 1 . U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (1999). Mental health: A report of the surgeon general. Rockville, M D : U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, Center for Mental Health Services.
Health
483
484
SPECIAL ISSUES Verbrugge, L. M. (1982). Women's social roles and health. In P. Berman &c E. Ramey (Eds.), Women: A developmental perspective (Publication No. 82-2298). Washington, DC: Government Printing Office. Vogeltanz, N. D., Se Wilsnack, S. C. (1997). Alcohol problems in women: Risk factors, consequences, and treatment strategies. In S. J . Gallant, G. P. Keita, & R. Royak-Schaler (Eds.), Health care for women (pp. 75-96,). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Walters, V., McDonough, P., & Strohschein, L. (2002). The influence of work, household structure, and social, personal, and material resources on gender differences in health: An analysis of the 1994 Canadian National Population Health Survey. Social Science & Medicine, 54, 6 7 7 - 6 9 2 . Whittle, G. C , Slade, P., 8t Ronalds, C. M. (1987). Social support in women reporting dysmenorrhea. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 31, 7 9 - 8 4 . Wilkie, Α., & Schmidt, U. (1998). Gynecological pain. In E. A. Blechman & K. D. Brownell (Eds.), Behavioral medicine and women (pp. 4 6 3 - 4 6 9 ) . New York: Guilford. Wills, T. A. (1998). Social support. In E. A. Blechman & K. D. Brownell (Eds.), Behavior medicine and women (pp. 1 1 8 - 1 2 3 ) . New York: Guilford. Woods, N., & Mitchell, E. (1996). Patterns of depressed mood among midlife women: Observations from the Seattle Midlife Women's Study. Research in Nursing and Health, 19, 1 1 1 - 1 2 3 . World Bank. (1993). World Development Report 1993: Investing in health. New York: Oxford University Press. Zanardi, C. (1990). Essential papers on the psychology of women. New York: New York University Press.
CHAPTER
Issues With Geriatric BARRY
ADAM
T
A.
P.
EDELSTEIN, ANDREA
he U . S . older adult population
Populations
K.
S P I R A , A N D L E S L E Y P.
10 25
SHREVE-NEIGER,
KOVEN
is
older adults), hypertension
growing at a remarkable rate. There
impairments
were 3 5 million individuals age 6 5
cataracts
years or over in the United States in 2 0 0 0
(30%),
(17%),
heart
(36%),
hearing
disease
(27%),
orthopedic
impairments
( 1 8 % ) , sinusitis ( 1 2 % ) , and diabetes ( 1 0 % )
(Hetzel & Smith, 2 0 0 1 ) . This represents 1 2 . 4 %
(Administration on Aging, 2 0 0 1 ) . In 1 9 9 9 , older
of the U.S. population and a 1 2 % increase from
adults were hospitalized four times as many
1 9 9 0 . In 2 0 0 0 , there were 1 8 . 4 million individ-
days as were those under age 6 5 years (1.6 vs.
uals who were between ages 6 5 and 7 4 years,
0.4 days) and remained hospitalized longer (6.0
and there were 1 2 . 4 million individuals between
vs. 4 . 1 days). Moreover, community-dwelling
ages 7 5 and 8 4 years. Those individuals age 8 5
older adults had more contact with their physi-
years or over (i.e., the oldest old) numbered 4 . 2
cians than did younger individuals ( 6 . 8 % vs.
million and represented the most rapid growth
3 . 5 % ) (Adiriinistration on Aging, 2 0 0 1 ) .
of the older adult population (Hetzel & Smith,
The mental health problems o f older adults
2 0 0 1 ) . The number o f oldest old increased 3 8 %
also invite attention, with estimates o f approxi-
during the
there will be
mately 2 5 % o f older adults meeting criteria for
approximately 7 0 million individuals age 6 5
a mental disorder (Gatz, Kasl-Godly, & Karel,
1990s.
By 2 0 3 0 ,
years or over, twice the number reported in
1 9 9 6 ) . In addition, comorbid health and men-
2 0 0 0 (Administration on Aging, 2 0 0 1 ) .
tal health problems are c o m m o n among older
Although many adults age successfully (cf.
adults, particularly among those seen in medi-
R o w e & Kahn, 1 9 9 8 ) , most older adults have
cal clinics (Lichtenberg, 2 0 0 0 ) and in long-term
at least one chronic health problem, and many
care settings. T h e combination o f health and
have several. In 1 9 9 9 , 2 6 . 1 % o f older adults
mental health problems, coupled with multiple
rated their health as fair or poor as compared
medications and potential adverse effects and
with only 9 . 2 % o f all individuals w h o did so
interactions, yields a challenging array o f diag-
(Administration on Aging, 2 0 0 1 ) . T h e most
nostic and treatment issues for the clinician.
frequently occurring chronic health problems among older adults
are arthritis
( 4 9 % of
This chapter provides a brief review o f agerelated sensory and cognitive changes to help
485
486
SPECIAL ISSUES underscore potential limitations associated
does pigmentation
with normal aging. This is followed by a dis-
reduction in the number o f photoreceptors
o f the vitreous
humor,
cussion of various psychological correlates o f
(rods and cones), and size o f the pupil. T h e
physical disease that are particularly important
lens capsule loses elasticity with age, resulting
in light o f age-related increases in probability
in losses in accommodation and visual acuity
of debilitating diseases and conditions. This
(usually realized
is followed by a related discussion o f adverse
numerous diseases can affect the visual system
as presbyopia). Finally,
effects o f medications that are more likely in
(e.g., diabetes mellitus, cataracts, glaucoma,
older adults. T h e subsequent section discusses
macular degeneration, myotonic
some c o m m o n problem areas that, for the
hypoparathyroidism, Wilson's disease).
dystrophy,
most part, do not fall within the realm o f
Approximately 3 0 % o f adults over age 6 0
diseases but nevertheless constitute significant
years suffer from significant hearing impair-
problems with psychological implications.
ment (Zarit & Zarit, 1 9 8 7 ) . These impair-
Age-related diagnostic issues are then addressed,
ments are due not only to aging but also to
with particular attention given to differences in
drugs used by older adults (e.g., certain antibi-
the presentation of various disorders. Finally, a
otics, certain diuretics) and various diseases
brief discussion of patient decision making and
and organic disorders (e.g., acoustic neuromas,
end-of-life issues is provided.
syphilis, multiple sclerosis, cerebrovascular accidents, circulatory disorders)
(Vernon,
1 9 8 9 ) . As with visual impairment,
hearing
AGE-RELATED CHANGES
impairment
can have psychological conse-
Aging begins at birth. An exhaustive discus-
activities, suspiciousness, hostility, depression).
sion o f age-related changes in the body and
T h e ability to understand
their psychological and behavioral c o n c o m i -
speech decreases with age, due primarily to
quences (e.g., paranoia, reduction of leisure
ability to detect
conversational
tants is beyond the scope o f this chapter.
diminished
Some o f the age-related sensory and cogni-
tones. Thus, older adults may experience diffi-
high-frequency
tive changes are briefly reviewed here (for
culty in understanding the speech o f women
more thorough discussions o f theses changes,
and children in particular. Finally, the ability to
see Edelstein, Martin, & Goodie [ 2 0 0 0 ] and
understand speech in the presence o f back-
Whitbourne [ 1 9 9 6 ] ) .
ground noise can be impaired in older adults.
M a n y older adults have sensory deficits
Older adults may also experience age-
that can influence the assessment and treat-
related declines in memory and cognition,
ment process and may be associated with a
although
variety o f psychological problems (e.g., social
Difficulties with working memory may be
isolation, paranoia). Sight, hearing,
such declines are not inevitable.
touch,
experienced, as may difficulty with inhibiting
smell, and taste all suffer loss to some degree
thoughts or external stimuli, when attempting
over time. Although all o f these declines are
to learn or recall information. Inhibitory con-
c o m m o n in older adults, this chapter focuses
trol tends to be worse for older adults in the
on sight and hearing. Thickness o f the lens o f
afternoon
evening due to
age-related
the eye increases with age, causing increased
changes in circadian rhythms
(Edelstein,
and
light absorption, light scattering within the
Martin, & Koven, 2 0 0 3 ) . Age-related changes
lens, increased susceptibility to glare, and dif-
in cognitive skills include diminished informa-
ficulty in adjusting to abrupt changes in light
tion processing speed, attention, mental flexi-
intensity. T h e increased lens density reduces
bility, abstraction, calculation, and capacity
the amount o f light reaching the retina, as
for inductive reasoning (Park, 2 0 0 0 ) . Overall,
Geriatric
Populations
\
it is important to appreciate that many older
a true depressive disorder is complicated by the
adults never experience many o f the deficits
overlap between symptoms observed in PD and
discussed and that many important cognitive
symptoms o f depression (e.g., reduced appetite,
abilities are maintained.
sleep disturbances, apathy, motor retardation) (Kremer & Starkstein, 2 0 0 0 ) . This point was illustrated by Hoogendijk, Sommer, Tissingh,
PSYCHOLOGICAL
Deeg, and Wolters ( 1 9 9 8 ) , who conducted a
MANIFESTATIONS AND
study in which PD patients were assessed for
C O R R E L A T E S O F PHYSICAL DISEASE
major depressive disorder ( M D D ) using both the American Psychiatric Association's (1987)
As discussed previously, a number of changes
DSM-III-R criteria (Diagnostic
and
occur as part o f the normal aging process. An
Manual
third edition,
of Mental
Disorders,
Statistical
unfortunate factor associated with aging is the
revised) and a clinician-administered depression
increased prevalence of chronic physical dis-
rating scale. After controlling for the presence
eases (Frazer, Leicht, & Baker, 1 9 9 6 ) . In this
of M D D symptoms that could be accounted for
section, the psychological and behavioral mani-
by P D , a reduction in diagnoses o f nearly 5 0 %
festations and correlates of chronic diseases
was observed. Hoogendijk and colleagues rec-
that commonly occur in older adults are
ommended that "depressed m o o d " should be
discussed.
considered mandatory for a diagnosis of M D D for individuals with PD and that depressive symptoms that covary with P D symptoms
Parkinson's Disease
should be diagnosed as "mood disorder due to
Parkinson's disease (PD) is a chronic progressive disorder of the central nervous system involving a deficiency o f dopamine in the basal ganglia (Thompson, 2 0 0 0 ) . This leads to a range of symptoms, including muscular rigidity, difficulty in initiating movement, shuffling gait, resting tremor, and a flat unchanging facial expression. In addition to physical symp-
a general medical condition." Others have determined that most symptoms o f M D D (i.e., those other than motor retardation, anergia, and early-morning awakening) that occur in individuals with PD tend to be legitimate instances o f depression rather than symptoms of PD (Kremer & Starkstein, 2 0 0 0 ) . Psychotic symptoms are also quite c o m -
toms, P D is associated with elevated rates o f
m o n in individuals with P D , although it is
psychiatric
not always clear whether these symptoms are
symptoms,
including
anxiety,
depression, and psychosis. In a review of anxi-
due to pathophysiological changes related to
ety disorders in P D , Marsh ( 2 0 0 0 ) cited preva-
PD, delirium, medications, or other psycholog-
lence rates as high as 2 4 % for panic disorder
ical disorders (Holroyd, Currie, & Wooten,
and from 1 2 % to 3 8 % for generalized anxiety
2 0 0 1 ) . In a recent study, 1 6 % o f patients
disorder. Based on observations that anxiety
with
symptoms commonly occur before m o t o r
delusions (Aarsland, Larsen, Cummings, 8c
P D experienced hallucinations
and
symptoms of P D , elevated rates have been
Laake, 1 9 9 9 ) . R a t e s were higher among indi-
attributed to the neuropathological processes
viduals residing in institutions. Holroyd and
underlying P D (Menza, 2 0 0 2 ) .
colleagues ( 2 0 0 1 )
Elevated rates of depression have also been
found
a prevalence o f
visual hallucinations o f 26.5%
in
outpa-
observed among individuals with P D . Estimates
tients. This rate was negatively associated
suggest that approximately 4 0 % o f individ-
with visual acuity and cognitive ability and
uals with P D report depressive
was positively associated with
symptoms
(Cummings, 1 9 9 2 ) . However, the diagnosis o f
level and disease severity.
depression
487
488
SPECIAL ISSUES
Chronic Obstructive Pulmonary Disease
at least one psychiatric disorder and that the degree o f psychiatric disturbance exhibited by
Chronic obstructive pulmonary
disease
(COPD) refers to a group o f pathological processes affecting the airways (e.g., bronchial tubes,
lungs), including
emphysema
and
chronic bronchitis. C O P D is characterized by dyspnea (perceived shortness of breath), sputum production, and cough (Frazer et al., 1 9 9 6 ) . Depression and anxiety are c o m m o n psychological correlates o f C O P D . A recent meta-analysis found that depression and anxiety symptoms occur in approximately 4 0 % and 3 6 % o f older adults with C O P D , respectively ( Y o h a n n e s , Baldwin, &c Connolly, 2000).
Karajgi,
Rifkin,
Doddi, and
Kolli
(1990) reported that 1 6 % o f a sample o f outpatients with C O P D had an anxiety disorder and that 8 % of the sample had panic disorder. Results from another study indicated that 3 4 % of a sample of respiratory unit patients had an anxiety disorder (Yellowlees, Alpers, Bowden, Bryant, & Ruffin, 1 9 8 7 ) . In addition, significant relations have been shown
to exist
these individuals was inversely related to the degree o f metabolic control that they obtained. C o m m o n psychiatric correlates o f diabetes include depression and anxiety. Lustman and colleagues reported
lifetime prevalences o f
major depressive episode and generalized anxiety disorder in diabetic patients to be 3 3 % and 4 1 % , respectively, hi addition, panic-like symptoms (e.g., dizziness, rapid pulse, sweating) have been reported to occur when blood glucose drops excessively (Morrison,
1997).
Agoraphobia appears to occur more frequently among individuals with type 2 diabetes than among those with type 1 diabetes (Lustman et al., 1 9 8 6 ) . Based on the observation o f higher levels o f functioning in psychogeriatric inpatients with uncontrolled diabetes than in inpatients with hypoglycemia, Hontela and Muller (1975) suggested that the elevated levels of blood glucose might actually maintain higher levels of functioning than those observed in older adults with hypoglycemia.
between the degree of depression and anxiety and the extent o f functional impairment, even after controlling for disease severity (Kim et al.,
Thyroid Disturbances
delirium
Thyroid disturbances comprise another set
attributed to anoxia, has also been observed in
of endocrine diseases that are relatively c o m -
individuals with C O P D (Morrison, 1 9 9 7 ) .
m o n in older adults. T h e thyroid gland's pri-
2000).
Cognitive impairment,
or
mary role is the regulation
o f metabolic
rate. Thus, thyroid dysfunction can have a significant impact on metabolic processes.
Diabetes Mellitus Diabetes
mellitus
Decreased thyroid function can lead to a is a
disease
of
the
pathological condition known as hypothy-
endocrine system that appears in two forms:
roidism,
type 1 (insulin dependent) and type 2 (non-
depressed m o o d , weight gain, low energy,
which
includes
symptoms
of
insulin dependent). The symptoms of diabetes
and loss o f appetite. These symptoms closely
are attributable to pancreatic dysfunction and a
resemble, and can easily be mistaken for,
resultant deficiency in insulin excretion. This
symptoms
deficiency impairs the affected individual's abil-
( 1 9 8 7 ) reported that even minor degrees o f
ity to process glucose, and this in turn produces
thyroid deficiency can be associated with sig-
an accumulation of blood glucose, known as
nificant depressive symptoms, particularly in
hyperglycemia. In one study, Lustman, Griffith,
the elderly. In addition, hypothyroidism has
o f depressive disorders.
Krahn
Clouse, and O y e r (1986) reported that 7 1 % of
been associated with cognitive impairment,
the individuals with diabetes had experienced
even at subclinical levels o f thyroid deficiency
Geriatric (Ganguli, Burmeister, Seaberg, Belle,
&
DeKosky, 1 9 9 6 ) .
agoraphobia, irritability, and
Populations
"pathological
emotionalism" (House et al., 1 9 9 1 ) . T h e psy-
Hyperthyroidism refers to thyroid overac-
chiatric
symptoms
o f these
community-
tivity that produces excessive amounts o f
dwelling patients in this study diminished over
thyroid h o r m o n e
the course o f the year following stroke.
(thyrotoxicosis) and
the
subsequent "speeding u p " o f metabolic processes. In younger adults, behavioral symptoms
of
hyperthyroidism
can
resemble
Cancer
symptoms o f mania and include restlessness
Cancer is associated with a range of psychi-
and irritability. In older adults, however, this
atric conditions, including anxiety, depression,
is far less likely. According to Gregerman (as
and delirium
cited in Frazer et al., 1 9 9 6 ) , less than 5 0 % o f
2 0 0 0 ) . The overlap between symptoms of psy-
older
individuals
with
(Gagnon, Allard, 8c Masse,
hyperthyroidism
chiatric disorders and symptoms secondary to
report these symptoms; older adults tend to
cancer or its treatment has been known to com-
report heart palpitations and a subjective
plicate the study o f these correlates o f cancer
sense o f "racing." W h e n depression is obser-
(Pasacreta, 1 9 9 7 ) . Panic attacks were reported
ved in the context o f hyperthyroidism, it is
in approximately one fifth o f inpatients with
referred
to as apathetic
hyperthyroidism
cancer referred for psychosomatic consulta-
(Morrison, 1 9 9 7 ; T h o m a s , Mazzaferri, 8c
tions (Slaughter et al., 2 0 0 0 ) . A number of
Skillman,
generalized
studies have documented the occurrence o f
anxiety symptoms (e.g., nervousness, tremor,
depression in the context of cancer. Greenberg
tension) have
(1989) reported that information regarding the
1970).
In addition,
been reported
in 4 0 % o f
patients with hyperthyroidism (Hall, 1 9 8 3 ) .
type, course, and treatment of a tumor is important in the diagnosis o f depression in individuals with cancer. In one study, 3 0 % of
Cerebrovascular Accident A
patients referred to an outpatient oncology
cerebrovascular accident, or
stroke,
department were assessed as having a "proba-
refers to brain trauma resulting from vascular
ble"
pathology. A cerebrovascular accident may
Fallowfield, 1 9 9 5 ) ; this dropped to 2 2 % at a
occur due to anoxia caused by atherosclerosis,
6-month follow-up. Using another measure,
psychiatric disorder
(Ford, Lewis, 8c
dysfunc-
Ford and colleagues (1995) found that 2 6 % of
tion. In addition, hemorrhage due to a rup-
patients had significant anxiety problems and
tured aneurysm can cause lesions. Although
that 7 % had significant depressive symptoms;
the location in the brain o f the stroke can
anxiety symptoms decreased significantly 6
thrombosis, embolism, or cardiac
behavioral
months later. T h e treatment of breast cancer in
impact (e.g., strokes resulting in lesions in the
postmenopausal women often requires the ces-
largely
determine
\
its
specific
right hemisphere have been associated with
sation of estrogen replacement therapy to sup-
increased levels of depression more than have
press cancer cell growth (Duffy, Greenberg,
those resulting in lesions in the left hemisphere
Younger, 8c Ferraro, 1 9 9 9 ) . Clinicians working
[Dam, Pedersen, 8c Ahlgren, 1 9 8 9 ] ) , depres-
with such patients should be aware that the
sion is widely observed following strokes. It
withdrawal o f estrogen replacement therapy in
has been reported to occur in 3 0 % o f cases
this population results in a rapid decrease in
within the year following a cerebrovascular
estrogen and has been known to
accident (Wade, Legh-Smith, 8c Hewer, 1 9 8 7 ) .
depressive symptoms
Other symptoms that have been observed
Pasacreta (1997) found that depressive symp-
during the year following stroke
toms occurred in 2 4 % o f a sample o f women
include
(Duffy et al.,
produce 1999).
489
490
SPECIAL ISSUES who had been diagnosed and treated surgically
are more sensitive to some o f the side effects o f
for breast cancer 3 to 7 months prior to assess-
medications. For example, prednisone, a corti-
ment; M D D occurred in 9 % o f the sample.
costeroid used to treat chronic obstructive pul-
Within this study, 3 5 % o f the variance in func-
monary disease, can cause anxiety, euphoria,
tional status was accounted for by physical
depression, and psychosis (Frazer et al., 1 9 9 6 ) .
distress and depressive symptoms.
Beta blockers such as propranolol, taken for chronic heart failure, can cause confusion, depression,
delusions, paranoia,
disorienta-
tion, agitation, and fatigue (Salzman, 1 9 9 8 ) .
ADVERSE MEDICATION EFFECTS
Medications used to treat Parkinson's disease M o s t older adults have at least one chronic
(e.g., levodopa, carbidopa) can lead to confu-
health problem that often requires medication
sion, hallucinations, and nightmares (Smith &
(e.g., arthritis, hypertension,
Reynard, 1 9 9 2 ) . Finally,
heart
disease,
diphenhydramine,
diabetes, sinusitis, esophagitis, constipation)
taken for allergies, can cause cognitive impair-
(Administration
ment in healthy older adults (Morrison
on Aging, 2 0 0 1 ; Knight,
&
Santos, Teri, & Lawton, 1 9 9 5 ) . Older adults
Katz, 1 9 8 9 ; Oslin, 2 0 0 0 ) . Thus, clinicians are
take an average of five prescription drugs each
well advised to examine thoroughly
day (Golden et al., 1 9 9 9 ) and use 4 0 % o f all
client's medical record with an eye to current
nonprescription drugs (Conry, 2 0 0 0 ) . Stoehr,
and past prescription and
Ganguli, Seaberg, Echement, and Belle ( 1 9 9 7 )
medications and their potential adverse effects.
each
over-the-counter
found that 8 7 % o f older adults report using at least one over-the-counter medication. Each of these drugs has the potential for adverse effects, and the potential interactions among multiple drugs can compound this number. Col, Fanale, and Kronholm ( 1 9 9 0 ) found that
C O M M O N P R O B L E M AREAS
Falls
2 8 % o f 8 9 older adult hospital admissions
Falls are a c o m m o n experience among
were due to medication-related problems, and
older adults. Approximately one third o f
6 0 % o f these were attributed to adverse drug
community-dwelling older adults (Studenski
reactions.
et al., 1 9 9 4 ) , 2 0 % o f older adults in acute
Older adults are at greater risk for such
care facilities (Kay & Tideiksaar, 1 9 9 0 ) , and
adverse effects than are younger adults due to
half o f older adults in long-term care facilities
&
(Tinetti et al., 1 9 9 4 ) fall each year. Within a
W o n g , 1 9 9 8 ) . Increases in the likelihood o f
age-related physiological changes (Rho
year, half o f these fallers will have fallen again
adverse anticholinergic effects can result from
(Wolinsky, J o h n s o n , & Fitzgerald, 1 9 9 2 ) .
age-related reduced parasympathetic nervous
Falls can result in a variety o f physical con-
system activity. Reductions in gastric acidity
sequences, ranging from minor injuries (e.g.,
and gastrointestinal motility can slow drug
abrasions, contusions, sprains, lacerations) to
absorption and the drug action. T h e elimina-
serious injuries (e.g., head trauma, spinal cord
tion of drugs is slowed by reduced activity o f
injuries, fractures, internal injuries). Nearly
hepatic enzyme systems and decreased renal
one third ( 3 1 % ) o f falls result in minor injury,
functions. T h e slowed elimination o f drugs can
and
also increase the risk for drug interactions.
(Morse, Tylko, & D i x o n , 1 9 8 7 ) . M o s t deaths
W h a t appear to be symptoms o f psycholog-
another
6%
result in serious
injury
and long-term disability after falls are related
ical disorders can be the result o f medications
to complications o f fall-related fractures, espe-
taken by older adults given that older adults
cially hip fractures (Melton & Riggs, 1 9 8 5 ) .
Geriatric
Populations
\
Psychological consequences o f falls are
Dissatisfaction with sleep is more common
Koski,
among elderly women than among elderly men
common.
Depression
(Luukinen,
Laippala, & Kivela, 1995) and fear o f falling
(Newman et al., 1997) and is higher among
(Drozdick 8c Edelstein, 2001) are often experi-
individuals with lower income and education
enced following a fall. Fear o f falling is associ-
levels (Ohayon,
1996).
ated with higher dependency, greater physical
Increasing age is associated with changes
symptomatology, greater drug consumption
in the nature and duration o f sleep c o m -
8c Andrews, 1990), higher reported
plaints. Problems in getting to sleep tend to
depression and anxiety (Arfken, Lach, Birge,
be most c o m m o n in younger insomniacs,
(Downton & Miller,
1994), restriction of movement and
whereas
problems
staying asleep
become
activities (Howland, Peterson, Levin, & Fried,
increasingly c o m m o n in later life (Maggi
1993), and increased morbidity.
et al.,
Risk factors for falls can be categorized as either intrinsic or extrinsic. Intrinsic risk fac-
1998). Symptoms o f disturbed sleep
are more likely to become chronic in older age groups (Hohagen et al.,
1994).
tors include physical impairment (e.g., muscle
Continuity o f sleep, duration o f sleep, and
weakness, hearing loss, visual acuity loss), the
depth o f sleep also show differences with age.
results o f various physical impairments (e.g.,
Relative to younger adults' sleep, older adults'
ambulation,
sleep is characterized
gait, and
balance problems),
by m o r e
frequent
medical disorders (e.g., Parkinson's disease,
"shifts" from one sleep stage to another and
cardiovascular disease), psychological factors
more frequent
intrasleep arousals
(Boselli,
8c Terzano, 1998). Both
(e.g., denial o f limitations, cognitive status),
Parrino, Smerieri,
and number, types, and amount of medications
events result in sleep that is more broken and
(e.g., antidepressants, sedatives) (Edelstein &
more likely to be rated as poor in quality
Drozdick,
1998). Extrinsic risk factors include
environmental hazards, restraints,
(Oswald,
1980). Similarly, sleep efficiency
improper
(time spent asleep divided by time spent in
footwear, improper use o f a walking aid, the
bed) also tends to decrease with age (Bliwise,
winter
season, and
(Edelstein
difficulty
with
stairs
8c Drozdick, 1998).
1993). Older adults tend to be "lighter" sleep2000) and to wake more easily
ers (Morgan,
with lower levels o f noise than do younger adults (Busby, Mercier,
Sleep
8c Pivik, 1994).
In addition to changes in the structure of
Complaints o f disturbed sleep may be cate-
nighttime sleep, the circadian rhythm itself has
gorized as sleep onset problems (trouble get-
been found to show age-related decay, with
ting to sleep), sleep maintenance
problems
(trouble staying asleep), or early
morning
awakening (Morgan,
2000). These symptoms
may occur alone or in combination and may
and
more
likely to occur during the day (Morgan,
sleep becoming desynchronized
2000).
Controlled-release
melatonin
replacement
therapy has been found to correct some circa-
be transient or long term. Disturbed sleep may
dian rhythm desynchrony, leading to improve-
also present, not as a complaint of sleeplessness
ments in both sleep efficiency and sleep onset
but rather as a report o f excessive daytime
(Haimov et al.,
sleepiness (Morgan,
disturbed sleep increases with age; insomnia has been estimated to affect approximately 5% of those ages 18 to 30 years versus 30% o f those age 65 years or over (Newman, Enright, Manolio,
1995).
2000). T h e prevalence o f
Haponik,
&
Wahl,
1997).
Appetite Approximately 40%
to 70%
o f elderly
patients w h o are hospitalized or institutionalized
are
malnourished
(Brocker,
Vellas,
491
492
SPECIAL ISSUES Albarede, & Poynard, 1 9 9 4 ) . Although social
Subcortical
and financial factors play important causative
as Parkinson's disease, Huntington's disease,
dementias include disorders such
roles in malnutrition, decreased appetite asso-
subcortical vascular disease, white
ciated with aging may also contribute to
disease,
weight
Cummings, 2 0 0 0 ) .
loss and
malnutrition.
Common
physical causes o f decreased appetite in older adults include dental
and
hydrocephalus
matter
(Kaufer
&
Cortical dementias
are
characterized by deficits in elementary intel-
conditions, gastro-
lectual skills, such as language, visuospatial
intestinal disorders, side effects o f medica-
functions, and mathematical abilities, whereas
tions, loss o f taste and smell, and particular
simple sensory and motor functioning is typi-
vitamin or mineral deficiencies. Furthermore,
cally preserved. Subcortical dementias, in con-
cholecystokinin, a hormone known to sup-
trast,
press appetite, has been found in greater lev-
wasting o f executive functions, affective and
els in older adults than in younger adults
personality changes, forgetfulness, and move-
(Baez-Franceshi & Morley, 1 9 9 9 ) .
ment disorders (Cummings & Benson, 1 9 8 2 ) .
are
characterized
by slowing
and
Psychological causes o f decreased appetite
The underlying organic pathology associ-
include depression, anxiety, loneliness, and
ated with various forms o f dementia can pro-
grief 1994).
( A m e r i c a n Psychiatric A s s o c i a t i o n , Psychologists may play
duce a variety o f behavior problems, including
important
physical aggression, screaming, and wander-
roles in treating decreased appetite in older
ing. M a n y o f these behaviors fall within the
adults; however, physiological causes should
category o f "agitation." Agitation has been
be considered before treating low appetite as
defined as "inappropriate verbal, vocal, or
a psychological problem.
motor activity that is not explained by needs or confusion per s e " (Cohen-Mansfield & Billig, 1 9 8 6 , p. 7 1 2 ) and includes behavior
Dementia
that is abusive or aggressive, occurs at an inap-
The dementias refer to a broad range o f dis-
propriate frequency, or is socially inappropri-
orders characterized by memory problems and
ate (Cohen-Mansfield, Werner, Watson, &c
at least one other cognitive disturbance, such
Pasis, 1 9 9 5 ) . Agitation is problematic for the
as aphasia (language disturbance), apraxia
agitated individual, caregivers, and others in
(motor disturbance), agnosia (inability to rec-
the agitated individual's immediate environ-
ognize objects), or a disturbance in executive
ment. Such behavior puts individuals
functioning (American Psychiatric Association,
physical risk and increases caregiver stress
at
1 9 9 4 ) . Although many dementias are progres-
(Bourgeois, Schulz, & Burgio, 1 9 9 6 ) .
sive, some are nondegenerative. However, all are characterized by irreversible declines in cognitive functioning and impairment in social and occupational functioning.
Pain Pain is one of the most c o m m o n concerns
Dementia has many different etiologies,
of older adults and is associated with nearly all
each with its own range o f symptoms and pro-
illnesses and diseases (Grange & Morrison,
gression. But despite the etiologic heterogene-
2 0 0 2 ; Wallace, 2 0 0 1 ) . Pain is reported by
ity of dementias, two basic types of dementia
2 5 % to 5 0 % o f community-dwelling older
exist. Cortical
dementias, such as Alzheimer's
adults (Ferrell, 1 9 9 1 ) and by as many as 8 0 %
disease, involve pathological alterations in the
of nursing home residents (Ferrell, Ferrell, &
cerebral cortical areas o f the brain, although
Osterweil, 1 9 9 0 ) . In general, older adults are
some alterations in subcortical regions are
more likely to experience pain, and are less
also present (Kaufer & Cummings, 2 0 0 0 ) .
likely to report pain, have pain recognized,
Geriatric
Populations
and be treated for the pain, than are younger
agitation, and increased irritability (Blazer
adults (Murray 8c Seely, 2 0 0 2 ) . This is even
et al., 1 9 8 6 ; Fiske, Kasl-Godley, &
Gatz,
more likely to be the case among cognitively
1 9 9 8 ) , and are less likely to report depressed
impaired older adults (Feldt, Ryden, & Miles,
m o o d (Fiske et al., 1 9 9 8 ) , than are younger
1 9 9 8 ; Gibson & Helme, 1 9 9 9 ) . Moreover, a
adults. Patterns o f depression
study by Chakour, Gibson, Bradbeer, and
reflected what has been termed "depletion,"
have
also
Helme ( 1 9 9 6 ) suggested that older adults may
characterized by symptoms o f loneliness, guilt,
experience some forms o f pain differently than
and sleep disturbance (Newmann, Engel, &
do younger adults.
Jensen, 1 9 9 0 ) .
Pain assessment in older adults can be com-
Older adults may differ from
younger
plicated by severe cognitive impairment where
adults with regard to both the prevalence and
traditional self-reports o f pain become unob-
the content of fears and worries. For example,
tainable due to loss o f verbal abilities and sen-
Kirkpatrick
( 1 9 8 4 ) , K o g a n and
Edelstein
sory declines. An alternative to self-reported
( 1 9 9 7 ) , and Liddell, Locker, and
pain is reliance on direct observation o f non-
( 1 9 9 1 ) all found that the number o f fears
verbal pain indicators (e.g., moaning, facial
decreases with age. Although the
grimacing, bracing, restlessness, rubbing)
decreases, Kogan and Edelstein ( 1 9 9 7 ) found
(Feldt, 2 0 0 0 ) . T h e psychological (e.g., depres-
that even low levels o f fears interfered with the
Burman number
sion, diminished cognitive functioning) and
daily lives o f older adults. Finally, Kogan and
behavioral (e.g., agitation, screaming, irritabil-
Edelstein (in press) found that the nature o f
ity) consequences o f pain can pose consider-
fears also changes with age and appears to
able challenges in the assessment o f older
reflect developmentally appropriate themes.
adults. In light o f the decreased likelihood o f
There is mounting evidence that subclinical
pain reports by older adults and the tremen-
or subsyndromal
dous impact o f pain on quality o f life, the pres-
DeBeurs et al., 1 9 9 9 ; Himmelfarb & Murrell,
levels o f anxiety (e.g.,
ence o f pain should always be assessed when
1 9 8 4 ; Palmer, Jeste, & Sheikh, 1 9 9 7 ) and
observing changes in behavior, particularly
depression (e.g., Lewinsohn, Solomon, Seeley,
among cognitively impaired older adults who
& Zeiss, 2 0 0 0 ) are both c o m m o n and clini-
might have trouble reporting
Pain
cally significant among older adults. This
should always be suspected when changes in
research suggests that anxiety and depression
behavior,
particularly
among
pain.
cognitively
impaired older adults, are observed.
among older adults might be best conceptualized along continua rather than in the context of diagnostic thresholds. In addition, these findings should alert clinicians to subthreshold
D I A G N O S T I C ISSUES
levels o f anxiety and depression among older adults that could require intervention.
T h e diagnosis o f mental disorders in older adults can be quite challenging. T h e prevalence, and even the phenomenology (e.g., Blazer, George, Sc Landerman, 1 9 8 6 ) and presentation, o f mental disorders can change with age. In the latter case, for example, depressed older adults are more likely to present somatic symptoms (e.g., decreased energy, chronic pain,
DECISION MAKING AND END-OF-LIFE ISSUES
Competency and Capacity for Decision Making
changes in appetite, changes in sleep patterns,
A number o f older adults who use behav-
gastrointestinal c o m p l a i n t s ) , p s y c h o m o t o r
ioral health services experience various degrees
494
SPECIAL ISSUES of dementia or may exhibit central nervous
otherwise by a court o f law. A capacity
system damage as a result o f cardiovascular
evaluation is one method for assessing an
accident, coronary artery disease, or other
aspect o f competency, but it should not be used
chronic health problems that often result in
as the sole determinant for surrogate or legal
cognitive impairment.
these
guardian assignment. An older adult suffering
older adults' capacity to make decisions and
from dementia may have the capacity for one
overall competency may be called into ques-
type o f decision making but lack capacity in
tion and may need to be assessed.
Competency
another area.
is a legal term referring to an
individual's
Consequently,
Decision-making capacity is
frequently
decision-making abilities for a specific criminal
assessed when issues related to death and
or civil matter and so is generally declared in a
dying
court o f law (Moye, 1 9 9 9 ) . Capacity
refers to
addresses aspects o f death and dying that the
an individual's specific abilities in a number of
behavioral health care provider should be
areas and is frequently assessed by a clinician.
aware o f when treating an older adult, espe-
Capacity is defined as the ability to compre-
are at stake. T h e next
subsection
cially if the adult in question is terminally ill.
hend information relevant to the decision, the ability to deliberate about the choices in accordance with personal values and goals, and
Death, Dying, and Related Issues
the ability to communicate with caregivers
Death is one aspect o f life that is as natu-
(Hastings Center, 1 9 8 7 ) . Clinicians are fre-
ral as birth. As a result o f modern scientific
quently asked to assess decision-making capac-
advances, many medical or
health-related
ity of older adults for a number of areas,
professionals
including financial and medical decision making
death, even at the expense o f the patient's
and independent living skills (Moye, 1 9 9 9 ) .
wishes. Medical professionals are trained to
Although few measures o f capacity with established psychometrics exist, a few that
do all they can to
save lives, and
prevent
society has m a d e
great
advances in medical technology that allow
have recently been developed show promise,
people to live longer than ever before, but
including the Hopemont
Capacity Assess-
there are people for w h o m death remains
ment Interview (Edelstein, Nygren, Northrop,
imminent nonetheless. In these cases, health
Staats, &
care professionals can have a positive impact
P o o l , 1 9 9 3 ) and
the Clinical
Competency Test Interview (Marson, Cody,
on the dying process by making it a more
Ingram, & Harrell, 1 9 9 5 ) . T h e H o p e m o n t
comfortable and dignified experience.
Capacity Assessment Interview presents sce-
Fordyce ( 2 0 0 1 ) listed the health care practi-
narios to the client. Some scenarios involve
tioner's most important contributions to the
medical
involve
dying client as being (a) amelioration of any
financial or money management decisions.
unpleasant physical or mental symptoms the
decisions,
and
others
Following scenario presentations, a number
client is experiencing; (b) meeting the client's
of questions are asked to assess information
emotional needs as much as possible; (c) edu-
comprehension, understanding o f the risks
cating, reassuring, and comforting the client;
and benefits involved, and how the decision
and
(d) preventing
futile interventions
and
or choice is made. T h e same format is fol-
investigations that may result in discomfort for
lowed with the Clinical Competency Test
the client. Aiding a client in the dying process by
Interview, but
nurturing mind, body, and spirit is one o f the
in this case the
scenarios
involve medical decision making only.
most effective ways in which a health care pro-
It should be remembered that all adults
fessional can make the process easier. In addi-
are considered competent unless determined
tion to addressing spiritual or religious issues,
Geriatric
Populations
\
the dying older adult may have a desire to
and should not be discouraged; rather, it
reminisce or write the story o f his or her life in
should be encouraged by health care profes-
a journal, especially for loved ones (Tobin 6c
sionals. It is not necessary to have answers for
Lindsey, 1 9 9 9 ) . Providing favorite music,
dying people, but health care professionals
games, or hobbies for the dying client is also
should let them voice their fears, concerns,
a way to make the final days or months of his
and/or beliefs and should try to offer some
or her life meaningful and ultimately more
kind o f reassurance. It is important not to
enjoyable.
challenge a terminally ill older adult's religious
Tobin and Lindsey (1999) referred to death
or spiritual beliefs but rather to respect them
as a natural part of living that requires prepara-
(Hastings Center, 1 9 8 7 ) and be open to listen-
tion and the expenditure of considerable effort.
ing to the patient discuss aspects of faith or
Health care providers should stress to their older
religion that are especially important or even
adult clients that preparation is an important
troubling to him or her.
part of dying, and as such, these clients should
W h e n death is imminent, it is also important
be considering key decisions that may need to be
for the health care professional to recognize the
made. These decisions may involve life support,
normal physical changes associated with death;
resuscitation, whether they wish to die at home
the health care professional should not try to
or in a hospital, and which family and friends
prevent or stop them but rather should make
they would like involved in the process. In addi-
the client as comfortable as possible. T h e client
tion, older adults should feel free and be encour-
normally does not experience discomfort or
aged to discuss their living wills, who they have
pain during these changes. Loss of appetite,
chosen as proxies, and whether they have docu-
mottling o f the skin, poor circulation, and loss
mented their wishes in writing. These all are
of thirst are typical prior to death (Fordyce,
considerations to be made in advance of the
2 0 0 1 ) . In addition, marked weight loss and loss
terminal stages of illness or death. In addition to
of body functions occur in close proximity to
legal and medical considerations, other issues
death. T h e client should be kept clean, and
such as religiosity and spirituality may emerge as
adult diapers should be changed frequently to
older adults prepare for death.
aid in comfort. None of these changes is cause
R e c e n t surveys indicate that
9 4 % of
for alarm; rather, they all are part of the normal dying process ( T o b i n &
report that religious involvement is a positive
Although further
and enriching experience, and 8 8 % believe
issues is beyond the scope of this chapter, the
that religion is either very important or fairly
interested reader is referred to Benner ( 2 0 0 1 ) ,
important
in their lives (Gallup,
1994).
Religious practice is especially c o m m o n among older adults, with a large number reporting
active participation
activities and prayer
Lindsey,
1999).
Americans believe in G o d , 9 0 % pray, 7 5 %
elaboration on end-of-life
Coppola and T r o t m a n ( 2 0 0 2 ) , and
Corr,
Nabe, and Corr ( 2 0 0 0 ) . End-of-life issues involving death and deci-
in religious
sion making are complex in today's health care
(Koenig, Larson, 8c
setting, as technology allows for life to be sus-
Matthews, 1 9 9 9 ) . Imminent death brings with
tained in circumstances where in the past it
it a period o f spiritual reflection for many
would have ceased. Although every human
people. Questions such as " W h a t is the mean-
eventually must face his or her mortality, the
ing o f life" and " W h a t have I done with my
process of death and dying is unique to each
life?" are frequently asked (Tobin & Lindsey,
individual, and it is the health care professional
1 9 9 9 ) . Questioning one's existence and its
who can grant the older adult a sense of control
relation to a bigger picture or an afterlife is a
and help to make the process o f dying the indi-
normal concomitant o f contemplating death
vidual's own experience.
495
496
SPECIAL ISSUES SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS
Older adults are, by definition, closer to death than are younger adults due to age
and
This chapter has briefly presented information
increased risk of life-threatening diseases. Thus,
that a clinician likely would find helpful when
the health care professional must educate him-
providing psychological services to older adults
self or herself regarding the major medical and
in health care settings. M u c h of the information
financial decisions commonly faced by older
pertains to age-related issues ranging from sen-
adults and how to participate in the determina-
sory processes to the presentation of depres-
tion o f capacity to make these decisions.
sion. It is particularly important that the reader
Finally, because death is experienced more
appreciate the ways in which adults can change
often by older adults than by younger adults, it
as they age and how these changes can affect
is important that the health care professional
their experiences and presentations o f problems
obtain knowledge regarding end-of-life issues
and disorders. These changes, the presence o f
and how to contribute to the care of dying
multiple chronic diseases, the psychological
patients. As has been said before, " W e will not
correlates o f these diseases, and the adverse
get out o f this life alive." However, the health
responses to drugs used to treat them can pre-
care professional can assist others in making
sent an enigmatic collection o f behaviors that
that transition as physically and psychologically
may complicate assessment and intervention.
comfortable as possible.
REFERENCES Aarsland, D., Larsen, J . P., Cummings, J . L., & Laake, K. (1999). Prevalence and clinical correlates of psychotic symptoms in Parkinson's disease. Archives of Neurology, 56, 5 9 5 - 6 0 0 . Administration on Aging. (2001). A profile of older adults: 2001 [Online]. Retrieved January 2 0 , 2 0 0 3 , from www.aoa.gov/aoa/stats/profile/2001 American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Arfken, C. L., Lach, H. W., Birge, S. J . , & Miller, J . P. (1994). The prevalence and correlates of fear of falling in elderly persons living in the community. American Journal of Public Health, 84, 5 6 5 - 5 7 0 . Baez-Franceshi, D., & Morley, J . E. (1999). Causes of malnutrition in the elderly [Online]. Retrieved January 2 0 , 2 0 0 3 , from www.healthandage.com/html/min/ basel/content/publi02 .htm Benner, P. (2001). Death as a human passage: Compassionate care for persons dying in critical care units. American Journal of Critical Care, 10, 3 5 5 - 3 5 9 . Blazer, D., George, L. K., &c Landerman, R. (1986). The phenomenology of late-life depression. In P. E. Bebbington & R. Jacoby (Eds.), Psychiatric disorder in the elderly (pp. 1 4 3 - 1 5 1 ) . London: Springer. Bliwise, D. (1993). Sleep in normal aging and dementia. Sleep, 16, 4 0 - 8 1 . Boselli, M., Parrino, L., Smerieri, Α., & Terzano, M. G. (1998). Effects of age on EEG arousals in normal sleep. Sleep, 21, 3 5 1 - 3 5 7 . Bourgeois, M. S., Schulz, R., & Burgio, L. (1996). Interventions for caregivers of patients with Alzheimer's disease: A review and analysis of content, process, and outcomes. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 43, 35-92.
Geriatric
Populations
Brocker, P., Vellas, B . , Albarede, A. L., & Poynard, T. (1994). A two-centre, randomized double-blind trial of ornithine oxoglutarate in 194 elderly, ambulatory, convalescent subjects. Age and Aging, 23, 3 0 3 - 3 0 6 . Busby, Κ. Α., Mercier, L., èc Pivik, R. T. (1994). Ontogenic variations in auditory arousal threshold during sleep. Psychophysiology, 31, 1 8 2 - 1 8 8 . Chakour, M. C , Gibson, S. J . , Bradbeer, M., & Helme, R. D. (1996). The effect of age on A delta- and C-fibre thermal pain perception. Pain, 64, 1 4 3 - 1 5 2 . Cohen-Mansfield, J . , & Billig, N. (1986). Agitated behaviors in the elderly: I. A conceptual review. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 34, 7 1 1 - 7 2 1 . Cohen-Mansfield, J . , Werner, P., Watson, V., ÔC Pasis, S. (1995). Agitation among elderly persons at adult day-care centers: The experiences of relatives and staff members. International Psychogeriatrics, 7, 4 4 7 - 4 5 8 . Col, N., Fanale, J . E., & Kronholm, P. (1990). The role of medication noncompliance and adverse drug reactions in hospitalizations of the elderly. Archives of Internal Medicine, 150, 8 4 1 - 8 4 5 . Conry, M. (2000). Polypharmacy: Pandora's medicine chest. Geriatric Times, 1, 1-6. Coppola, K., 8c Trotman, F. (2002). Dying and death: Decisions at the end of life. New York: Springer. Corr, C , Nabe, C , 8c Corr, D. (2000). Death and dying, life and living (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Cummings, J . L. (1992). Depression and Parkinson's disease. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 4 4 3 - 4 5 4 . Cummings, J . L., 8c Benson, D. F. (1982). Subcortical dementia. Archives of Neurology, 39, 6 1 6 - 6 2 0 . Dam, H., Pedersen, H. E., 8c Ahlgren, P. (1989). Depression among patients with stroke. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 80, 1 1 8 - 1 2 4 . DeBeurs, E., Beekman, Α., van Balkom, Α., Deeg, D., van Dyck, R., 8c van Tilburg, W. (1999). Consequences of anxiety in older persons: Its effect on disability, well-being, and use of health services. Psychological Medicine, 29, 5 8 3 - 5 9 3 . Downton, J . H., & Andrews, K. (1990). Postural disturbance and psychological symptoms amongst elderly people living at home. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 5, 9 3 - 9 8 . Drozdick, L. W., 8c Edelstein, B. A. (2001). Correlates of fear of falling in older adults who have experienced a fall. Journal of Clinical Geropsychology, 7, 1-13. Duffy, L. S., Greenberg, D. B . , Younger, J . , 8c Ferraro, M . G. (1999). Iatrogenic acute estrogen deficiency and psychiatric syndromes in breast cancer patients. Psychosomatics, 40, 3 0 4 - 3 0 8 . Edelstein, Β. Α., 8c Drozdick, L. W. (1998). Falls among older adults. In B. Edelstein (Ed.), Comprehensive clinical psychology, Vol. 7: Clinical geropsychology (pp. 3 4 9 - 3 7 0 ) . New York: Pergamon. Edelstein, B., Martin, R., Sc Goodie, J . (2000). Considerations for older adults. In M. Hersen 8c M. Biaggio (Eds.), Effective brief therapies: A clinician's guide (pp. 4 3 3 - 4 4 8 ) . San Diego: Academic Press. Edelstein, B . , Martin, R., Sc Koven, L. (2003). Assessment in geriatric settings. In J . R. Graham & J . A. Naglieri (Eds.), Comprehensive handbook of psychology, Vol. 10: Assessment psychology (pp. 389—414). New York: John Wiley. Edelstein, B . , Nygren, M., Northrop, L., Staats, N., & Pool, D. (1993, August). Assessment of capacity to make financial and medical decisions. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Toronto. Feldt, K. S. (2000). The checklist of nonverbal pain indicators. Pain Management Nursing, 1, 1 3 - 2 1 . Feldt, K. S., Ryden, M. B . , 8c Miles, S. (1998). Treatment of pain in cognitively impaired compared with cognitively intact older patients with hip-fracture. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 46, 1 0 7 9 - 1 0 8 5 .
498
SPECIAL ISSUES Ferrell, B. A. (1991). Pain management in elderly people. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 29, 6 4 - 7 3 . Ferrell, Β. Α., Ferrell, B . R., & Osterweil, D. (1990). Pain in the nursing home. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 38, 4 0 9 - 4 1 4 . Fiske, Α., Kasl-Godley, J . E., & Gatz, M. (1998). Mood disorders in late life. In B. Edelstein (Ed.), Comprehensive clinical psychology, Vol. 7: Clinical geropsychology (pp. 1 9 3 - 2 2 9 ) . New York: Pergamon. Ford, S., Lewis, S., & Fallowfield, L. (1995). Psychological morbidity in newly of Psychosomatic Research, 39, referred patients with cancer. Journal 193-202. Fordyce, M. (2001). At the end of life. Annals of Long-Term Care, 9, 8 0 - 8 4 . Frazer, D. W., Leicht, M. L., & Baker, M. D. (1996). Psychological manifestations of physical disease in the elderly. In L. Carstensen, B . A. Edelstein, ÔC L. Dornbrand (Eds.), The practical handbook of clinical gerontology (pp. 2 1 7 - 2 3 5 ) . Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Gagnon, P., Allard, P., Sc Masse, B . (2000). Delirium in terminal cancer: A prospective study using daily screening, early diagnosis, and continuous monitoring. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management, 19, 4 1 2 - 4 2 6 . Gallup, G., J r . (1994). The Gallup Poll: Public opinion 1993. Wilmington, DE: Scholarly Resources. Ganguli, M., Burmeister, L. Α., Seaberg, E. C , Belle, S., & DeKosky, S. T. (1996). Association between dementia and elevated TSH: A community-based study. Biological Psychiatry, 40, 7 1 4 - 7 2 5 . Gatz, M., Kasl-Godley, J . E., Sc Karel, M . J . (1996). Aging and mental disorders. In J . Birren Se K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (4th ed., pp. 3 6 5 - 3 8 2 ) . San Diego: Academic Press. Gibson, S., Sc Helme, R. (1999). Cognitive factors and the experience of pain and suffering in older persons. Pain, 85, 3 7 5 - 3 8 3 . Golden, A. G., Preston, R. Α., Barnett, S. D., Llorente, M., Hamdan, K., Sc Silverman, M. A. (1999). Inappropriate medication prescribing in homebound older adults. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 47, 9 4 8 - 9 5 3 . Grange, T., & Morrison, A. (2002). The relationship between medical and psychological problems in residential care. In R. D. Hill, B. L. Thorn, J . Bowling, Sc A. Morrison (Eds.), Geriatric residential care (pp. 7 7 - 9 8 ) . Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Greenberg, D. B. (1989). Depression and cancer. In R. G. Robinson Se P. V. Rabins (Eds.), Depression and coexisting disease (pp. 103-115). New York: Igaku-Shoin. Haimov, I., Lavie, P., Laudon, M., Herer, P., Vigder, C , 8c Zisapel, N. (1995). Melatonin replacement therapy of elderly insomniacs. Sleep, 18, 5 9 8 - 6 0 3 . Hall, R. C. W. (1983). Psychiatric effects of thyroid hormone disturbances. Psychosomatics, 24, 7 - 1 8 . Hastings Center. (1987). Guidelines on the termination of life sustaining treatment and the care of the dying. Bloomington: Indiana University Press. Hetzel, L., 8c Smith, A. (2001). The 65 years and over population: 2000. Washington, DC: U.S. Bureau of the Census. Retrieved May 2 0 , 2 0 0 2 , from www.census.gov/prod/2001pubs/c2kbr01-10.pdf Himmelfarb, S., 8c Murrell, S. A. (1984). The prevalence and correlates of anxiety symptoms in older adults. Journal of Psychology, 116, 1 5 9 - 1 6 7 . Hohagen, F., Kappler, C , Schramm, E., Rink, K., Weyerer, S., Riemann, D., 8c Berger, M. (1994). Prevalence of insomnia in general practice attenders and the current treatment modalities. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 90, 1 0 2 - 1 0 8 . Holroyd, S., Currie, L., 8c Wooten, G. F. (2001). Prospective study of hallucinations and delusions in Parkinson's disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 70, 7 3 4 - 7 3 8 .
Geriatric
Populations
Hontela, S., & Muller, H. F. (1975). Uncontrolled diabetes in psychogeriatric subjects. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 23, 5 8 - 6 2 . Hoogendijk, W. J . G., Sommer, I. E. C., Tissingh, G., Deeg, D. J . H., & Wolters, E. C. (1998). Depression in Parkinson's disease: The impact of symptom overlap on prevalence. Psychosomatics, 39, 4 1 6 - 4 2 1 . House, Α., Dennis, M., Mogridge, L., Warlow, C., Hawton, K., &C Jones, L. (1991). Mood disorders in the year after first stroke. British Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 83-92. Howland, J . , Peterson, E. W., Levin, W. C., & Fried. L. (1993). Fear of falling among the community dwelling elderly. Journal of Aging and Health, 5, 2 2 9 - 2 4 3 . Karajgi, B., Rifkin, Α., Doddi, S., & Kolli, R. (1990). The prevalence of anxiety disorders in patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 2 0 0 - 2 0 1 . Kaufer, D. I., & Cummings, J . L. (2000). Dementia: An overview. In M . J . Farah & T. E. Feinberg (Eds.), Patient-based approaches to cognitive neuroscience (pp. 3 5 5 - 3 6 8 ) . Cambridge, MA: M I T Press. Kay, A. D., & Tideiksaar, R. (1990). Falls and gait disorders. In W. B. Abrams & R. Berkow (Eds.), Merck manual of geriatrics (pp. 5 2 - 6 8 ) . Rahway, NJ: Merck. Kim, H. F. S., Kunik, M. E., Molinari, V. Α., Hillman, S. L., Lalani, S., Ornego, C. Α., Petersen, N. J . , Nahas, Z., & Goodnight-White, S. (2000). Functional impairment in COPD patients: The impact of anxiety and depression. Psychosomatics, 41, 4 6 5 - 4 7 1 . Kirkpatrick, D. R. (1984). Age, gender, and patterns of common intense fears among adults. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 22, 1 4 1 - 1 5 0 . Knight, B . G., Santos, J . , Teri, L., & Lawton, M. P. (1995). The development of training in clinical geropsychology. In B . G. Knight, L. Teri, P. Wohlford, & J . Santos (Eds.), Mental health services for older adults: Implications for training and practice in geropsychology (pp. 1-8). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Koenig, H. G., Larson, D. B., & Matthews, D. A. (1999). Religion and psychotherapy with older adults. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 29, 1 5 5 - 1 8 4 . Kogan, J . N., & Edelstein, B. A. (1997). Fears of middle-aged and older adults: Relations to daily functioning and life satisfaction. Unpublished manuscript, West Virginia University. Kogan, J . N., &c Edelstein, B. A. (in press). Modification and psychometric examination of self-report measure of fear in older adults. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. Krahn, D. D. (1987). Affective disorder associated with subclinical hypothyroidism. Psychosomatics, 28, 4 4 0 - 4 4 1 . Kremer, J . , & Starkstein, S. E. (2000). Affective disorders in Parkinson's disease. International Review of Psychiatry, 12, 2 9 0 - 2 9 7 . Lewinsohn, P. W., Solomon, Α., Seeley, J . R., & Zeiss, A. (2000). Clinical implications of subthreshold depression symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 3 4 5 - 3 5 1 . Lichtenberg, P. A. (2000). Asssessment of older adults in medical settings. Clinical Geropsychology News, 7, 5. Liddell, Α., Locker, D., & Burman, D. (1991). Self-reported fears (FSS-II) of subjects aged 50 years and over. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 29, 1 0 5 - 1 1 2 . Lustman, P. J . , Griffith, L. S., Clouse, R. E., &c Cryer, P. E. (1986). Psychiatric illness in diabetes mellitus: Relationship to symptoms and glucose control. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 174, 7 3 6 - 7 4 2 . Luukinen, H., Koski, K., Laippala, P., & Kivela, S. L. (1995). Risk factors for recurrent falls in the elderly in long-term institutional care. Public Health, 109, 57-65.
500
SPECIAL ISSUES Maggi, S., Langlois, J . Α., Minicuci, N., Grigoletto, F., Pavan, M., Foley, D. J . , 8c Enzi, G. (1998). Sleep complaints in community-dwelling older persons: Prevalence, associated factors, and reported causes. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 46, 1 6 1 - 1 6 8 . Marsh, L. (2000). Anxiety disorders in Parkinson's disease. International Review of Psychiatry, 12, 3 0 7 - 3 0 8 . Marson, D. C , Cody, Η. Α., Ingram, K. K., & Harrell, L. E. ( 1 9 9 5 ) . Neuropsychologic predictors of competency in Alzheimer's disease using a rational reasons legal standard. Archives of Neurology, 52, 9 5 5 - 9 5 9 . Melton, L. J . , &C Riggs, B . L. (1985). Risk factors for injury after a fall. Clinics in Geriatric Medicine, 1, 5 2 5 - 5 3 9 . Menza, M . A. (2002). Psychiatric aspects of Parkinson's disease. Psychiatric Annals, 32, 9 9 - 1 0 4 . Morgan, K. (2000). Sleep and aging: An overview. In K. L. Lichstein & C. M. Morin (Eds.), Treatment of late life insomnia (pp. 3 - 3 6 ) . New York: Russell Sage. Morrison, J . R. (1997). When psychological problems mask medical disorders: A guide for psychotherapists. New York: Guilford. Morrison, R. L., &C Katz, I. R. (1989). Drug-related cognitive impairment: Current of Gerontology and progress and recurrent problems. Annual Review Geriatrics, 9, 2 3 2 - 2 7 9 . Morse, J . M., Tylko, S. J . , &c Dixon, H. A. (1987). Characteristics of the fall-prone patient. The Gerontologist, 27, 5 1 6 - 5 2 2 . Moye, J . (1999). Assessment of competency and decision making capacity. In P. Lichtenberg (Ed.), Handbook of assessment in clinical gerontology (pp. 4 8 8 - 5 2 8 ) . New York: John Wiley. Murray, Μ. Α., & Seely, J . F. (2002). Strategies to improve cancer pain control in long-term care. Annals of Long-Term Care, 10, 5 5 - 6 0 . Newman, A. B., Enright, P. L., Manolio, T. Α., Haponik, E. F., & Wahl, P. W. (1997). Sleep disturbance, psychosocial correlates, and cardiovascular disease in 5,201 older adults: The Cardiovascular Health Study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 45, 1-7. Newmann, J . P., Engel, R. J . , & Jensen, J . (1990). Depressive symptom patterns among older women. Psychology and Aging, 5, 1 0 1 - 1 1 8 . Ohayon, M. (1996). Epidemiologic study on insomnia in the general population. Sleep, 19(Suppl.), S 7 - S 1 5 . Oslin, D. W. (2000). Prescription and over-the-counter drug misuse among the elderly. Geriatric Times, 1, 1-5. Oswald, I. (1980). Sleep studies in clinical pharmacology. British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 10, 3 1 7 - 3 2 6 . Palmer, B., Jeste, D., & Sheikh, J . (1997). Anxiety disorders in the elderly: DSM-IV and other barriers to diagnosis and treatment. Journal of the Affective Disorders, 46, 1 8 3 - 1 9 0 . Park, D. (2000). The basic mechanisms accounting for age-related decline in cognitive function. In D. C. Park &c N. Schwarz (Eds.), Cognitive aging: A primer (pp. 1-22). New York: Psychology Press. Pasacreta, J . V. (1997). Depressive phenomena, physical symptom distress, and functional status among women with breast cancer. Nursing Research, 46, 214-221. Rho, J . P., Wong, F. S. (1998). Principles of prescribing medications. In T. T. Yoshikawa, E. L. Cobbs, & K. Brummel-Smith (Eds.), Practical ambulatory geriatrics (2nd ed., pp. 1 9 - 2 5 ) . St. Louis, M O : C. V. Mosby. Rowe, J . W., & Kahn, R. L. (1998). Successful aging. New York: Pantheon Books Salzman, C. (1998). Clinical geriatric psychopharmacology (2nd ed.). Baltimore, M D : Williams & Wilkins.
Geriatric
Populations
Slaughter, J . R., Jain, Α., Holmes, S., Reid, J . C , Bobo, W., & Sherrod, Ν. B . (2000). Panic disorder in hospitalized cancer patients. Psycho-Oncology, 9, 2 5 3 - 2 5 8 . Smith, C. M., & Reynard, A. M . (1992). Textbook of pharmacology. Philadelphia: W. B . Saunders. Stoehr, G. P., Ganguli, M., Seaberg, E. C., Echement, D. Α., & Belle, S. (1997). Over-the counter medication use in an older rural community: The Movies Project. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 45, 1 5 8 - 1 6 5 . Studenski, S., Duncan, P. W., Chandler, J . , Samsa, G., Prescott, B . , Hogue, C , & Bearon, L. B . (1994). Predicting falls: The role of mobility and nonphysical factors. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 42, 2 9 7 - 3 0 2 . Thomas, F. B., Mazzaferri, E. L., & Skillman, T. G. (1970). Apathetic thyrotoxicosis: A distinctive clinical and laboratory entity. Annals of Internal Medicine, 72, 6 7 9 - 6 8 5 . Thompson, R. F. (2000). The brain: A neuroscience primer (3rd ed.). New York: Worth. Tinetti, M . E., Baker, D. L, McAvay, G., Claus, Ε. B . , Garrett, P., Gottschalk, M., Koch, M . L., Trainor, K., &C Horwitz, R. I. (1994). A multifactorial intervention to reduce the risk of falling among elderly people living in the community. New England Journal of Medicine, 331, 8 2 1 - 8 2 7 . Tobin, D. R., & Lindsey, K. (1999). Peaceful dying. Cambridge, MA: Perseus Books. Vernon, M . (1989). Assessment of persons with hearing disabilities. In T. Hunt & C. J . Lindley (Eds.), Testing older adults: A reference guide for geropsychological assessments (pp. 1 5 0 - 1 6 2 ) . Austin, T X : Pro-Ed. Wade, D. T., Legh-Smith, J . , & Hewer, R. A. (1987). Depressed mood after stroke. British Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 2 0 0 - 2 0 5 . Wallace, M . (2001). Pain in older adults. Annals of Long-Term Care, 9, 5 0 - 5 8 . Whitbourne, S. (1996). The aging individual: Physical and psychological perspectives. New York: Springer. Wolinsky, F. D., Johnson, R. J . , & Fitzgerald, J . F. (1992). Falling, health status, and the use of health services by older adults: A prospective study. Medical Care, 30, 5 8 7 - 5 9 7 . Yellowlees, P. M., Alpers, J . H , Bowden, J . J . , Bryant, G. D., & Ruffin, R. E. (1987). Psychiatric morbidity in patients with chronic airflow obstruction. Medical Journal of Australia, 146, 3 0 5 - 3 0 7 . Yohannes, A. M., Baldwin, R. C , &C Connolly, M . J . (2000). Mood disorders in elderly patients with chronic pulmonary disease. Reviews in Clinical Gerontology, 10, 1 9 3 - 2 0 2 . Zarit, J . M., 8c Zarit, S. H. (1987). Molar aging: The physiology and psychology of normal aging. In L. Carstensen & B . Edelstein (Eds.), Handbook of clinical gerontology (pp. 1 8 - 3 2 ) . New York: Pergamon.
|
501
CHAPTER
Public Health
Approaches
Finding the Interface With Health JALIE A N D
A.
C A T H Y A.
A of
TUCKER,
10 26
JOSHUA
C.
Psychology
KLAPOW,
SIMPSON
fter decades o f delivering psychologi-
(Curry & Kim, 1 9 9 9 ; Pronk, Boucher, Gehling,
cal services primarily in the form o f
Boyle, & Jeffery, 2 0 0 2 ) . In clinical care, persons
psychotherapy, the settings and scope
w h o have developed problems seek help from
psychological practice have
expanded
professionals and receive treatments that are
into health care, w o r k site, organizational,
relatively intensive and effective but costly.
1999).
Treatment seekers, however, tend to be a small
Health psychology research and practice have
minority o f the larger population with prob-
been vital to this trend, and many o f the inno-
lems, and they tend to have more serious prob-
and
community
settings
(Tucker,
vations have been concerned with understand-
lems. In public health practice, populations or
ing and modifying relations among health,
at-risk groups are actively targeted with brief
disease, and behavior. Services that support
interventions that typically are more preventive
health behavior change n o w span interven-
than therapeutic in focus. Compared with clini-
tions that
cal care, public health interventions do not
vary in scope, intensity, cost-
effectiveness, target audience, and population
involve as much personal contact, can be deliv-
impact. Services range from individual clinical
ered by trained nonprofessionals, and are more
services to focused interventions for select risk
accessible; for example, they may involve writ-
groups to brief, low-intensity interventions for
ten or videotaped materials and can be deliv-
communities or populations.
ered by telephone, television, or via the Internet.
In other words, clinical care is increasingly
Although public health interventions typically
being supplemented by interventions that are
are less efficacious than clinical interventions,
more in line with a public health approach
they are less costly per person and can reach
A U T H O R S ' N O T E : Manuscript preparation was supported in part by National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Aging Grant K 0 2 A A 0 0 0 2 0 9 to Jalie Tucker and by National Institute on Aging Grant K 2 3 A G 0 0 9 3 2 to J o s h u a Klapow.
502
Public Health
503
many more people. Thus, in the aggregate, the
with those o f public health, synergies should
overall impact on the health status of commu-
emerge that enhance the population impact of
nities or populations may be considerably
behavioral interventions to promote health and
greater from public health interventions than
to prevent and manage disease.
from clinical care.
This chapter provides
In combination, the two approaches can
an overview o f
the public health field for psychologists.
contribute to the development o f systems o f
Concepts, methods, and findings from epi-
care that span the conventional end points o f
demiology, behavioral epidemiology,
public health and clinical practice and that
health outcomes assessment are emphasized
and
include both preventive and therapeutic ser-
because they are likely points o f intersection
vices. T h e need for such coordinated systems
with health psychology. T h e chapter summa-
of care is widely recognized (e.g., Galea et al.,
rizes the history and key concepts in these
2 0 0 1 ; U.S. Department o f Health and H u m a n
areas, discusses educational opportunities in
Services, 1 9 9 9 ) , but implementation is chal-
public health generally, and then selectively
lenging and uncommon (e.g., Humphreys &
illustrates education and training in the field o f
Tucker, 2 0 0 2 ) . Psychologists have much to
health outcomes assessment, which is a natu-
contribute to such initiatives. But to partici-
ral connection for psychologists. T h e chapter
pate effectively, they would
ends with consideration o f differences in the
benefit
from
greater public health knowledge and skills,
"worldviews"
which are not routinely covered in graduate
health, discussed in the context o f mental
education
not
health research and practice. These differences
exhaustive, relevant content areas include
must be understood and respected if the disci-
(a) population distributions and dynamics o f
plines are to collaborate effectively.
in
health and
psychology.
Although
o f psychology and
public
behavioral health problems as
well as the nature o f relations between them; (b) organization and economics o f health care
H I S T O R I C A L ISSUES
systems,
AND KEY CONCEPTS
patterns
of
and
influences
on
service utilization, and cost-utility analysis; (c) research methods
suitable for
studying
populations and organizations rather
than
individuals; (d) intervention marketing
and
The
beginnings o f c o n t e m p o r a r y
public
health research and practice lie in the field of epidemiology,
which is the study o f the
dissemination to groups, communities, and
determinants o f disease in populations. T h e
organizations; and (e) health outcomes assess-
origins
ment and health policy research, including
to observational studies o f infectious disease
mental health policy.
transmission patterns during the m i d - 1 8 0 0 s
Psychologists, in turn, can offer
unique
of epidemiology
can
be
traced
such as J o h n Snow's classic analysis o f h o w
knowledge and skills that are fundamental to
cholera was transmitted
an effective merger of clinical and public health
drinking water in L o n d o n (Turnock, 1 9 9 7 ) .
by
contaminated
approaches to health promotion and disease
S n o w collected data on the frequency and
prevention and management. In addition to
distribution o f cholera deaths in L o n d o n as a
their strong research skills, psychologists have
function o f the decedents' source o f water.
comprehensive knowledge and skills concern-
F r o m these data, he was able to identify con-
ing mental health disorders, behavior change,
taminated water sources, thereby preventing
theory and measurement of behavior, and the
further exposure and cholera deaths.
role o f behavior in health and disease. By merg-
As Snow's research exemplifies, epidemiol-
ing the disciplinary strengths of psychology
ogy typically uses nonexperimental correlational
SPECIAL ISSUES
504
methods to assess the strength of association in
Raczynski, in press). Behavioral epidemiology
large samples between possible causes of disease
and health psychology share common goals of
and patterns o f disease onset, transmission,
understanding and modifying behavior and
morbidity, and mortality (Hennekens & Buring,
psychological factors involved in health and
1 9 8 7 ) . Key measures of association are inci-
disease but differ in the scope of application
dence and prevalence,
hicidence is a measure o f
and methods o f study. Research in behavioral
the rate of disease onset and represents the
epidemiology has been instrumental in identi-
number of new cases in a population over a
fying modifiable risk factors for disease that
given period o f time. Prevalence reflects the total
involve behavior and that are appropriate tar-
number of cases in a population either at a
gets for intervention. For example, diet, smok-
single point in time (i.e., "point" prevalence) or
ing, and inactivity have been established as risk
over a given time period (e.g., annual or lifetime
factors for coronary heart disease (National
prevalence). Lncidence and prevalence are inter-
Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute, 1 9 9 4 ) . Ln
related because prevalence is a function o f both
addition, increased risk o f coronary heart dis-
the rate of new cases (i.e., incidence) and the
ease has been associated with specific compo-
duration of disease.
nents o f "Type A " behavior patterns such as
Other central concepts in epidemiology are
hostility, reactivity, and time pressure (e.g.,
for disease (Fletcher,
Houston, Chesney, Black, Cates, & Hecker,
Fletcher, & Wagner, 1 9 9 6 ) . Risk is the likeli-
1 9 9 2 ; Krantz, 1 9 8 8 ) and with the presence o f
risk
and risk
factors
hood that persons who are exposed to a parti-
depressive
cular factor or who have a given characteristic
T y p e A behavior patterns (Frasure-Smith,
symptoms
in conjunction
with
will develop the disease o f interest. Risk factors
Lesperance, J u n e a u ,
are the characteristics, circumstances, or behav-
1 9 9 9 ) . As another example, several modifiable
iors that are associated with an increased risk o f
risk factors for human immunodeficiency virus
Talajic, 8c Bourassa,
disease occurrence. Because relations between
(HIV) infection have been identified, including
exposure to risk factors and disease are evalu-
drug o f choice, injection practices, and sexual
ated at the population level, the observed rela-
practices (Peterson, Dimeff, Tapert, Stern, &
tions do not necessarily hold for individuals
Gorman, 1 9 9 8 ) .
(Kaplan, 1 9 8 4 ) . M a n y persons who have a neg-
Another relevant subspecialty is health
out-
ative health outcome have no known risk fac-
comes
tors, even when a robust relationship between
including health outcomes assessment (Klapow,
research
(Clancy & Eisenberg, 1 9 9 8 ) ,
risk factors and disease has been established at
Kaplan, 6c Doctor, in press). Historically, the
the population level. Moreover, empirical sup-
primary outcome measures in epidemiology
port for risk factors typically comes from non-
were mortality and morbidity as represented
(e.g., case
by biological indicators o f disease. This focus,
experimental research methods
control studies) that fall short of the standards
however, does not represent many important
for causal inference as defined and investigated
functional domains that contribute to health
in experimental psychology.
and disease status. This limitation led to the
A recent development o f relevance to health
development of the "quality-adjusted life year"
psychologists is the emergence o f the subspe-
(QALY)
cialty o f behavioral
which
Weinstein 8c Stason, 1 9 7 7 ) . Q A L Y s reflect in
involves the application o f concepts and meth-
a single measure the benefits o f reduced mor-
ods from epidemiology to investigate the role
bidity and mortality, and it was an important
of behavior and psychological variables in
advance in the assessment o f health outcomes.
health and
epidemiology,
disease at a population
as an alternative measure
(e.g.,
level
The scope o f health outcomes assessment has
(Sexton, 1 9 7 9 ; Tucker, Phillips, Murphy, &
continued to expand to include a range o f
Public Health
505
monetary cost measures and measures of patient
level o f generality that is unattainable
functioning and satisfaction. Economic analyses
psychology. In comparison, psychology offers
in
(e.g., cost-effectiveness, cost-benefit analysis) are
a more intensive individual level o f analysis
becoming a standard part of evaluation research
that often is better grounded in theory and
on health outcomes (Drummond, O'Brien,
sound measurement practices. T o promote
Stoddart, & Torrence, 1 9 9 7 ) . This work has
interdisciplinary synergies, researchers
shown, for example, that including mental
practitioners in each discipline should acquire
health benefits in comprehensive health plans
a working knowledge o f the other's field.
and
tends to reduce the use and cost of medical ser-
Basic training and skills in public health for
vices (Cummings, O'Donohue, &
psychologists are described next.
Ferguson,
2 0 0 2 ) . This medical "cost offset" effect provides strong support for the economic utility of insuring mental health services.
SPECIALIZED T R A I N I N G
Expansion into the assessment o f patient
A N D SKILLS N E E D E D
functioning poses measurement challenges not present in pure economic evaluations. Health
This section provides an overview of graduate
status and health-related quality o f life are
education in public health and pathways for
functional constructs that encompass symp-
acquiring specialized training and skills in public
toms, behavior, and psychological and social
health for psychologists. This material is fol-
functioning. Operationalizing these constructs
lowed by a more detailed discussion of opportu-
and evaluating them are well within the exper-
nities and educational requirements in the area of
tise o f health psychologists. Skills in psycho-
health outcomes assessment, which exemplifies
metrics, behavioral assessment, and instrument
opportunities for psychologists in health services
development and validation are a cornerstone
research. The section ends with a description
o f their training. Given that definitions o f
of the joint clinical psychology-public health
health are shifting from a biomedical process
program
model (e.g., physiological markers of health) to
Birmingham (UAB), which shows how educa-
a patient-focused outcomes model (e.g., symp-
tion and training in the two fields can be inte-
toms, distress, functioning), so too must the
grated effectively at the predoctoral level.
at the University o f Alabama at
measurement o f health shift from a biomedical process to outcomes assessment. T h e process of defining and operationalizing a construct
Public Health Education Programs
(e.g., health, anger, pain, hostility), quantifying
Probably the most direct way for psycholo-
it through instrument development, validating
gists to gain knowledge about public health is
the instrument to ensure reliability and accu-
to enroll in courses or degree programs in
racy o f measurement, and interpreting data
graduate programs in public health. This sub-
obtained from such an instrument are com-
section describes graduate education in public
mon practices in health psychology (Klapow
health, including core content areas required
et al., in press).
for program accreditation by the Council on
In summary, public health and health psychology share many c o m m o n goals, but they
Education for Public Health ( C E P H ) . Much
like the American Psychological
have approached the study o f health, disease,
Association's Committee on Accreditation peri-
and the role o f the environment and behavior
odically reviews doctoral programs in psychol-
in different yet potentially
ogy, the C E P H serves this function for schools
complementary
ways. Public health brings a broad population
of public health and related academic pro-
perspective to bear on the issues and offers a
grams, including community health/preventive
506
SPECIAL ISSUES medicine programs and community
health
that involve direct patient interaction. This is
education programs. As of June 2 0 0 2 , C E P H -
the foundation of training for health psycholo-
accredited programs included 3 2 schools of
gists. T h e meso level consists of the organiza-
public health, 3 6 community health/preventive
tional entities that deliver health care services
medicine
programs,
and
14
community
and
their
associations
with
community
health education programs. T h e current list
resources. T h e macro level consists of policies
may be obtained from the C E P H W e b site
that create the parameters within which health
(www.ceph.org). In addition to serving a gen-
care organizations, communities, and providers
eral quality assurance function, C E P H accredi-
function. As noted in the W H O report, lines of
tation allows programs to be eligible for certain
demarcation between the levels are not always
federal funds such as research awards from the
clear. Nevertheless, when health care policies,
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention
organizations, communities, and interventions
(CDC). Linkages between public health pro-
align, the quality of health care services is likely
grams and the C D C are a unique feature not
to improve (cf. Humphreys & Tucker, 2 0 0 2 ) .
enjoyed by conventional doctoral psychology programs.
As discussed in Frank, Farmer, and Klapow (in press), as a profession o f behavioral scien-
All CEPH-accredited programs offer educa-
tists, health psychologists are potentially capa-
tional experiences leading to the master of
ble o f functioning at any o f these levels. Health
public health (M.P.H.) degree, which is a pro-
psychologists
fessional degree recognizing broad knowledge
education in the scientific method, research
of content and method relevant to the practice
methodology, statistics, psychometric theory,
of public health. Additional generalist and spe-
personality theory, learning theory, and behav-
receive
rigorous
graduate
cialist degrees may be offered, for example, the
ior change interventions. T o date, however, the
academically oriented M.S.P.H. degree and doc-
application o f this knowledge base has been
toral degrees that are professional (Dr.P.H.) or
limited primarily to the micro level of the
academic (Ph.D., Sci.D.) in nature. Five core
health care system. By offering educational
areas o f knowledge are required in M.P.H.
opportunities that convey the necessary exper-
programs: biostatistics, epidemiology, environ-
tise at the meso and macro levels, health psy-
mental health sciences, health services adminis-
chologists will be prepared to contribute across
tration, and social and behavioral sciences.
the multiple levels o f the system o f care.
These core areas often serve as the defining basis
Doing so, however, will require changes in
of departments in schools of public health,
psychology doctoral programs. Graduate pro-
although this arrangement is not a C E P H
grams
requirement per se.
more coursework and expand the curriculum
Educational Opportunities in Health Services Research and Practice
will continue to limit the contributions of health
understandably
hesitate to
require
(Elliott & Klapow, 1 9 9 7 ) , but failure to do so psychologists to the micro level of health care systems. A training model that blends core skills
The delivery o f health care services is a com-
in health psychology with a working knowledge
plex and dynamic process. As discussed in a
of the levels of the health care system will enable
report of the W o r l d Health
future psychologists to apply their skills in the
( W H O , 2 0 0 2 ) titled Innovative Chronic Action,
Conditions:
Building
Organization Care Blocks
for
dynamic health care system and to deal effec-
for
tively with future economic and organizational
health care systems can be divided into
three levels: micro,
and macro.
changes (Elliott & Klapow, 1 9 9 7 ) .
The
Although the possibilities for content are
micro level of health care consists of services
virtually endless when considering the study of
meso,
Public Health
507
health care systems, several core areas are
example, some psychologists (including the first
essential for a basic level of expertise: health
two chapter authors) have obtained knowledge
economics, health insurance, health policy,
and skills in public health through receipt of
quality improvement,
competitive Public Health Service career devel-
patient-based
program
outcomes
evaluation,
evaluation,
cost-
opment awards (i.e., "K-awards").
effectiveness analysis, and design and evaluation of clinical trials. It is important to note that these content areas are in addition to the foundation of training that health psychologists
Health Outcomes Assessment
now
Measurement is a cornerstone of psychol-
receive. Thus, it is assumed that health psychol-
ogy. T h e field o f health psychology has made
ogists who explore these content areas have req-
significant progress in transitioning the princi-
uisite skills in statistics, psychometric theory,
ples of psychometric theory into a variety o f
instrument development and validation, and
assessment applications in the field o f medicine.
research methods. With a health psychology
For the most part, however, measurement in
foundation and these additional content areas,
health psychology has focused on (a) identify-
health psychologists can pursue opportunities
ing psychological processes associated with
across the spectrum of health care. Without
health and illness, (b) evaluating psychological
such knowledge, they are limited in their ability
and social contextual variables and their rela-
to work within the meso (health care organiza-
tionship to biological markers o f health, and
tion) and macro (health policy) levels.
(c) assessing psychological distress as a primary
Although it is necessary for health psychol-
outcome (Klapow et al., in press; Pennebaker,
ogists to have an understanding o f the health
Kiecolt-Glaser, &
care system from the micro level to the macro
these are important contributions, the some-
Glaser, 1 9 8 8 ) .
Although
level, the method o f education can take several
what narrow focus has limited health psychol-
forms. Integration o f coursework and areas o f
ogy's role in health outcomes assessment.
concentration into doctoral programs, contin-
As discussed in Frank and colleagues (in
uing education opportunities, and additional
press), because chronic conditions are increasing
postgraduate degrees all are possible, depend-
as the U.S. population ages, effective medical
ing on the depth o f understanding desired. If
management of chronic conditions is growing in
comprehensive health services and outcomes
importance. Heretofore, biological markers
training programs are not available or feasi-
have been used as primary end points for disease
ble, psychologists can gain some understand-
assessment, but such measures are only mod-
ing
in
the
core
content
areas
through
estly correlated with health outcomes. L o w cor-
individual courses, independent reading, and
relations between biological measures of disease
continuing education classes.
process and
Psychologists also can acquire knowledge of
patient
outcomes
have
been
observed in many areas of medicine (Feinstein,
health policy and the political environment
1 9 9 4 ) . For example, rheumatologists often mea-
through fellowship programs. T h e R o b e r t
sure disease activity by sédimentation rates or
W o o d Johnson Foundation sponsors a health
the number o f swollen joints, but these measures
policy fellowship that supports the participation
are poorly correlated with patient disability and
of psychologists and other health professionals
capacity to function. Thus, biological measures
in the formulation o f national health policy in
serve only a limited role in predicting patient
the U.S. Congress. Similar programs are offered
outcomes. Functional disability and capacity to
by the American Psychological Association and
function (i.e., ability to carry out activities o f
Service (De Leon,
daily living) also are relevant measures, and
the U.S. Public Health
Hagglund, Ragusea, & Sammons, in press). For
measures
o f psychosocial
and
behavioral
508
SPECIAL ISSUES variables (e.g., stress levels, coping responses,
educational initiatives aimed at providing them
health behaviors, social networks) further con-
with the requisite knowledge and skills as part
tribute to the prediction of health outcomes
of their graduate education. One such program
(Kurki, 2 0 0 2 ; Tucker etal., in press). These
exists at UAB, a large urban university that
nonbiological variable classes fall within the
offers undergraduate, graduate, and profes-
domain of psychology and are important lines
sional degrees and is recognized for its excel-
of research for health psychologists to pursue.
lence in health-related research. Three schools at
hi general, definitions of health are shifting
UAB (Social and Behavioral Sciences, Public
that is based
Health, and Medicine) are collaborating to offer
on physiological markers of health and disease to
educational opportunities for psychologists to
a patient-focused
gain expertise in health services and outcomes
from a biomedical
process outcomes
model model
that encom-
passes symptoms, functioning, and
distress
(Kaplan 8c Anderson, 1 9 9 6 ; Kaplan, Anderson,
research at the predoctoral, postdoctoral, and professional levels of training.
8c Ganiats, 1 9 9 3 ) . Medicine is increasingly faced
The joint P L D . / M . S . P . H . (master o f science
with evaluating interventions for chronic condi-
in public health) program in Medical Clinical
tions, rationing health care based on improve-
Psychology and Health Outcomes and Policy
ments in health and quality of life, and viewing
Research is the most comprehensive program.
patients as consumers of care. These changes are
T h e program provides psychology doctoral
propelling the shift in models and measurement
students with coursework and research experi-
of health outcomes, and they pose challenges for
ences in outcomes and policy research that
the medical community, which is not well versed
complement their existing training in clinical
in the assessment of nonbiological outcomes.
health psychology. Students enrolled in the
Thus, psychologists have an opportunity
program complete basic coursework in clinical
to
apply their expertise in measurement across the
health psychology and then are permitted to
health care field in ways that should contribute
enroll in the Outcomes and Policy Research
to better conceptual and operational definitions
M.S.P.H. program in the School o f Public
of health and to sound assessment practices. The
Health. T h e curriculum for the program is pre-
process of defining and operationalizing a con-
sented in Table 2 6 . 1 . Students complete the
struct (e.g., health, anger, pain, hostility), quanti-
core content areas and two elective courses.
fying
T h e électives enable students to concentrate
it through
instrument
development,
validating the instrument to ensure reliability and
their efforts in health policy, advanced analyt-
accuracy, and collecting and interpreting data
ics, or clinical outcomes evaluation. Because of
using the instrument are common practices in
their foundation
health psychology. The requisite skills in psycho-
research design, the biostatistics requirements
metrics, behavioral assessment, and instrument
are waived. O n completion o f the coursework,
o f skills in statistics and
development and validation are cornerstones of
each student conducts an independent research
training in psychology. Thus, health psycholo-
project under the mentorship of a School of
gists are poised to become leaders in the evalua-
Public Health faculty member. T h e program
tion of health outcomes.
typically can be completed in 1 year and so adds an additional year to students' doctoral
Joint Training Programs in Psychology and Public Health: The UAB Example
training in psychology. Graduates of the joint degree program gain knowledge and skills necessary to investigate the effects o f medical and public health interventions on survival, quality
Growing opportunities for psychologists to
of life, and resource utilization. They are pre-
work in select areas of public health are nurturing
pared for careers in public health, health care
Public Health Table 2 6 . 1
Required Courses for the Coordinated Master of Science of Public Health (M.S.P.H.) and Doctor of Philosophy (Ph.D.) in Medical Psychology at the University of Alabama at Birmingham
M.S.P.H. core Biostatistics I (3 hours) Biostatistics II (3 hours) Principles of epidemiologic research (4 hours) Principles of epidemiologic research lab Outcomes research Design of clinical trials Health economics Patient-based outcomes measurement Social and ethical issues in public health Health insurance and managed care Cost-effectiveness analysis for public health and medicine Decision analysis for public health and medicine Approved électives Regression analysis Health program evaluation Applied logistic regression Clinical trials and survival analysis Survey research methods Public health policy Public health law Improving health care quality outcomes Aging policy Policy analysis: Modeling and simulation Special problems in policy analysis Research experience Master's-level research project NOTE: These requirements are in addition to the requirements of the Ph.D. program in psychology.
settings, and health services research such as
research programs. All projects address core
program evaluation, health outcomes assess-
outcomes and health services research topics
ment, and pharmacoeconomics.
such as quality improvement, cost-effectiveness
For those who have already completed
analyses, patient-centered outcomes evalua-
a Ph.D., the U A B Center for
Outcomes
tion, program evaluation, and clinical trials.
and Effectiveness Research and
Education
Fellows attend weekly research discussion
( C O E R E ) offers a 1- to 2-year postdoctoral fel-
groups and journal clubs, attend health ser-
lowship that provides research experiences in
vices methods workshops, and can enroll in
outcomes and health services research. During
the courses offered in the previously described
the fellowship, psychologists are exposed to a
M.S.P.H. program. T h e C O E R E fellowship
variety o f outcomes and
provides opportunities
health
services
to collaborate with
research projects, the content of which varies
other fellows from diverse disciplines, includ-
from year to year depending on
ing medicine, health economics, and health
faculty
509
510
SPECIAL ISSUES administration. Psychology fellows are jointly
health services research; health
supervised by the center directors and by a
psychiatry; public health; and medicine.
clinical psychologist (this chapter's second
education;
An overarching goal o f the meeting was to
author), who serves as a division director in the
explore barriers to collaboration between
COERE.
clinical and public health scientists. T h e following points emerged during the meeting:
CONCLUDING COMMENTS:
•
Basic and public health scientists place different values on many research activities and products, including individual investigator versus collaborative research, hypothesis testing versus applied research, research on interactions that support mediation/moderation of theoretically meaningful effects versus strong main effects with intervention implications, small versus large samples, and internal versus external validity and generalizability.
•
Because of these value differences, the criteria for tenure and promotion, publications, grants, and other career advancement opportunities often differ. Training opportunities in the "two worlds" of basic behavioral science and public health science are lacking. Several psychologists in attendance had acquired knowledge about public health through mid-career development K-awards. Creating educational opportunities at earlier (e.g., predoctoral) levels of training is essential to advancing the disciplinary interface.
DISCIPLINARY DIFFERENCES IN PERSPECTIVE A N D VALUES As the preceding discussion suggests, a fundamental feature o f public health that is new to many psychologists is its interdisciplinary and collaborative nature. In conventional psychology settings, psychologists often labor alone or in small teams with graduate students. This is uncommon in public health because the scale o f problems that are the subject o f research
and
practice
often
cannot
be
addressed unless multiple areas o f expertise are involved.
•
For example, in 1 9 9 9 , the surgeon general released a report
on mental health (U.S.
Department of Health and Human Services, 1 9 9 9 ) that highlighted the need to expand approaches to mental health research and practice beyond dominant clinical approaches to include population-based
public
health
approaches. T h e historical, but diminishing, dominance of psychiatry in the mental health
•
field had promoted a long-standing clinical approach to the neglect of a public health approach.
S o o n thereafter
(in N o v e m b e r
2 0 0 0 ) , the National Institute on Mental Health held a meeting in Rockville, Maryland, titled "Research on Mental Disorders: Overcoming Barriers to Collaborations Between Basic Behavioral
Scientists and
Public
Health
Scientists" that was attended by this chapter's first author. T h e following disciplines were represented at the meeting and exemplify the breadth
o f relevant expertise c o m m o n in
public health research: biostatistics; clinical, experimental, health, and social psychology; community health sciences; epidemiology; health communications; health economics;
•
Even among informed professionals, key terms often convey important and subtle differences in meaning in different disciplines. For example, scientists from disciplines other than epidemiology often do not recognize that incidence is a technical term that refers to a measure of rate with time in the denominator. Similarly, attitude is a wellresearched construct in psychology, but this body of work often is unfamiliar to nonpsychologists, who use the term in the vernacular. Considerable comorbidity exists between health problems and mental health problems. However, the issue is underresearched, and the extent of comorbidity is underdiagnosed and undertreated in practice settings, in part because of historical lines drawn among the pertinent disciplines.
Public
Health
These points are instructive about the chal-
Psychologists have much to contribute in
lenges involved in moving across the disci-
these areas of application, but relatively few
plinary boundaries of psychology and public
have acquired the "hybridized"
health. But many opportunities exist as well.
knowledge needed to work effectively in an
As noted earlier, psychologists have research
interdisciplinary health care or research envi-
skills that would enhance the quality of evalua-
ronment. As discussed in this chapter, there are
tion research on public health behavioral inter-
a number o f paths to acquiring essential knowl-
ventions. They also are knowledgeable about
edge and skills, and joint psychology-public
etiology and conceptual issues in abnormal
health programs are likely to become more
behavior and human change processes. In this
common in response to the need for behavioral
skills and
regard, helping the public health field to move
scientists with the necessary breadth of knowl-
beyond its reliance on the health belief and
edge and skills. Interdisciplinary approaches are
transtheoretical models was noted as a concep-
essential to finding satisfactory solutions to
tual issue that would benefit from input by
many contemporary health-related challenges,
psychologists.
including expanding prevention
In the arena
o f practice and
research, promoting
adherence to
programs,
applied
improving health care access and effectiveness,
health-
reducing health disparities, and
related programs was identified as a critical
containing
costs without reducing the quality o f care.
issue that must be tied more closely to techno-
Psychologists have been underrepresented
logical and pharmaceutical innovations in
in these endeavors, probably due in part to
care. In addition, because primary health care
their historical efforts to practice indepen-
professionals are overburdened,
calling on
dently without medical supervision and to the
them to do more in the behavioral health arena
corresponding applied research agenda that
was viewed as unrealistic. Helping to refine
emphasized comparative outcome evaluations
their focus and increasing the efficiency of
of different types of psychotherapy. These ini-
what
of
tiatives were successful, but the struggle for
"physician extenders," and increasing direct
they
do,
making
greater
use
professional independence may have inadver-
patient access to assessment and intervention
tently deterred the participation o f psycholo-
options were recommended as alternatives. It
gists in interdisciplinary teams that are the
was further recommended that, in nonspecial-
hallmark of public health research and prac-
ized health care settings, mental health services
tice. T h e expertise of psychologists is needed,
should be "bundled" with other behavioral
and they will almost certainly find the colle-
health services to increase efficiency and to
giality and respectfulness that are characteristic
decrease the stigma o f mental illness. T h e tim-
of public health collaborations to be more pro-
ing o f delivery of such services to optimize
ductive and satisfying than the medical hierar-
access, utilization, and outcomes
chy model that dominated the early years o f
remains
poorly understood and underresearched.
psychology's professional development.
REFERENCES Clancy, C. M., & Eisenberg, J . M . (1998). Outcomes research: Measuring the end of health care. Science, 282, 2 4 5 - 2 4 6 . Cummings, Ν. Α., O'Donohue, W. T., & Ferguson, Κ. E. (2002). The impact of medical cost offset on practice and research: Making it work for you (Foundation for Behavioral Health, Healthcare Utilization and Cost Series, Vol. 5). Reno, NV: Context Press.
512
SPECIAL ISSUES Curry, S. J . , & Kim, E. L. (1999). Public health perspective on addictive behavior change interventions: Conceptual frameworks and guiding principles. In J . A. Tucker, D. M . Donovan, 8c G. A. Marlatt (Eds.), Changing addictive behavior: Bridging clinical and public health strategies (pp. 2 2 1 - 2 5 0 ) . New York: Guilford. De Leon, P. EL, Hagglund, K. J . , Ragusea, S. Α., Sc Sammons, M . (in press). Expanding roles for psychologists: The 21st century. In G. Strieker & T. A. Widiger (Eds.), Clinical psychology: Handbook of psychology (Vol. 8). New York: John Wiley. Drummond, M. E , O'Brien, B . , Stoddart, G. L., Sc Torrence, G. W. (1997). Methods for the economic evaluation of health care programmes (2nd ed.). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Elliott, T. E., Sc Klapow, J . C. (1997). Training psychologists for a future in evolving health care delivery systems: Building a better Boulder model. Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings, 4, 2 5 5 - 2 6 9 . Feinstein, A. R. (1994). Art, science, and the doctor-patient relationship. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 729, 1 9 - 2 1 . Fletcher, R. H., Fletcher, S. W., & Wagner, Ε. H. (1996). Clinical epidemiology: The essentials. Baltimore, M D : Williams Sc Wilkins. Frank, R. G., Farmer, J . E., & Klapow, J . C. (in press). The relevance of health policy to the future of health psychology. In R. G. Frank, A. Baum, Sc J . Wallander (Eds.), Handbook of clinical health psychology, Vol. 1: Models and perspectives in healthcare psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Frasure-Smith, N., Lesperance, F., Juneau, M., Talajic, M., & Bourassa, M. G. (1999). Gender, depression, and one-year prognosis after myocardial infarction. Psychosomatic Medicine, 61, 2 6 - 3 7 . Galea, S., Factor, S. EL, Bonner, S., Foley, M., Freudenberg, N., Latka, M., Palermo, A. G., Sc Vlahov, D. (2001). Collaboration among community members, local health service providers, and researchers in an urban research center in Harlem, New York. Public Health Reports, 116, 5 3 0 - 5 3 9 . Hennekens, C. H., & Buring, J . E. (1987). Epidemiology in medicine. Boston: Little, Brown. Houston, Β . K., Chesney, Μ . Α., Black, G. W., Cates, D. S., Se Hecker, M. H. (1992). Behavioral clusters and coronary heart disease risks. Psychosomatic Medicine, 54, 4 4 7 - 4 6 1 . Humphreys, K., Sc Tucker, J . A. (2002). Toward more responsive and effective intervention systems for alcohol-related problems. Addiction, 97, 1 2 6 - 1 3 2 . Kaplan, R. M . (1984). The connection between clinical health promotion and health status: A critical overview. American Psychologist, 39, 755-765. Kaplan, R. M., Sc Anderson, J . P. (1996). The general health policy model: An integrated approach. In B. Spilker (Ed.), Quality of life and pharmacoeconomics in clinical trials (pp. 3 0 9 - 3 2 2 ) . Philapdelphia: Lippincott-Raven. Kaplan, R. M., Anderson, J . P., Se Ganiats, T. G. (1993). The Quality of Well-Being Scale: Rationale for a single quality of life index. In S. R. Walker Se R. M. Rosser (Eds.), Quality of life assessment: Key issues in the 1990s (pp. 6 5 - 9 4 ) . London: Kluwer Academic. Klapow, J . , Kaplan, R., Sc Doctor, J . (in press). Outcomes measurement: The role of health psychology. In T. Boll (Ed.), Health and behavior handbook. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Krantz, D. S. (1988). Environmental stress and biobehavioral antecedents of coronary heart disease. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 3 3 3 - 3 4 1 . Kurki, T. S. (2002). Prediction of outcome in cardiac surgery. Mount Sinai Journal of Medicine, 69, 6 8 - 7 2 .
Public Health National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute. (1994). Report of the Task Force on Research in Epidemiology and Prevention of Cardiovascular Diseases. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Pennebaker, J . W., Kiecolt-Glaser, J . , 8c Glaser, R. (1988). Disclosure of traumas and immune function: Health implications for psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 2 3 9 - 2 4 5 . Peterson, P. L., Dimeff, L. Α., Tapert, S. F., Stern, M., & Gorman, M . (1998). Harm reduction and HIV/AIDS prevention. In G. A. Marlatt (Ed.), Harm reduction: Pragmatic strategies for managing high-risk behaviors (pp. 2 1 8 - 2 9 7 ) . New York: Guilford. Pronk, N. P., Boucher, J . L., Gehling, E., Boyle, R. G., 8e Jeffery, R. W. (2002). A platform for population-based weight management: Description of a health plan-based integrated systems approach. American Journal of Managed Care, 8, 8 4 7 - 8 5 7 . Sexton, M. M . (1979). Behavioral epidemiology. In O. F. Pomerleau Sc J . P. Brady (Eds.), Behavior medicine: Theory and practice (pp. 3 - 2 1 ) . Baltimore, M D : Williams 8c Wilkins. Tucker, J . A. (1999). Changing addictive behavior: Historical and contemporary perspectives. In J . A. Tucker, D. M . Donovan, & G. A. Marlatt (Eds.), Changing addictive behavior: Bridging clinical and public health strategies (pp. 3 - 4 4 ) . New York: Guilford. Tucker, J . Α., Phillips, M. M., Murphy, J . G., & Raczynski, J . M . (in press). Behavioral epidemiology and health psychology. In R. G. Frank, A. Baum, 8c J . Wallander (Eds.), Handbook of clinical health psychology, Vol. 1: Models and perspectives in healthcare psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Turnock, B. J . (1997). Public health: What it is and how it works. Gaithersburg, M D : Aspen. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (1999). Mental health: A report of the surgeon general. Rockville, M D : U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, Center for Mental Health Services. Weinstein, M., 8c Stason, W. (1977). Foundations of cost-effectiveness analysis for health and medical practices. New England Journal of Medicine, 296, ''16-721. World Health Organization. (2002). Innovative care for chronic conditions: Building blocks for action (WHO annual report). Geneva, Switzerland: Author.
513
CHAPTER
10 27
Practical Research in a Medical Setting Is Good Medicine K A T H L E E N
PALM, JACK
M.
A N T O N U C C I O ,
C
L.-M.
A N D E L I Z A B E T H
important
is
becoming
increasingly
in the behavioral
DAVID
O.
V . GlFFORD
onducting clinical research in medical
settings
MUTNICK,
health
There
are m a n y
advantages
to
doing
research in medical clinics. F o r example, researchers can contact more patients, observe
care field. In the w a k e o f managed care, the
a greater range o f complaints, and interact
role o f the behavioral health care provider is
with multidisciplinary teams. Perhaps most
shifting.
the
important, researchers can study treatment
session will eventually
and treatment outcomes as they occur in
S o m e experts speculate that
50-minute therapy
become extinct (Cummings, 2 0 0 0 ;
Hayes,
Barlow, & Nelson-Gray, 1 9 9 9 ) . Behavioral
"real-world"
treatment
settings.
Before
beginning a research program in these set-
health care specialists will increasingly need
tings, however, there are special issues to
to function within different contexts.
consider. T h i s c h a p t e r
describes a
brief
As the practice o f behavioral care special-
history o f research in medical settings and
ists shifts, so must the focus o f research. There
then details institutional and research consid-
is a growing need to assess the effectiveness o f
erations. C o m m o n roadblocks are discussed,
psychological treatments outside o f controlled
and practical advice is provided. A case study
laboratory settings. Controlled studies assess
illustrates h o w
the efficacy o f treatments by testing whether
blocks can be addressed.
different
issues and
road-
interventions w o r k under ideal circumstances. Typical medical settings, however,
include
numerous factors that cannot be controlled
CASE
STUDY
and are hardly ideal. Because this is where many interventions will be delivered, this is
Throughout this chapter, the authors draw
where the research needs to be conducted.
on a specific real-world example of clinical
A U T H O R S ' N O T E : This research was supported by National Institute on Drug Abuse Grant 1 R 0 1 D A 0 1 3 1 0 6 - 0 3 .
514
Practical
Research
in a Medical
Setting
research in a medical setting. T h e investigators
where the focus was more on advancing science
of this study sought to examine the impact on
for the sake o f knowing rather than for the sake
smoking cessation o f bupropion
of healing human suffering. Examples from the
sustained
release (SR) versus bupropion S R plus behav-
19th and early 2 0 t h centuries illustrate how the
ior therapy. Approximately 3 0 0 patients par-
burden o f serving as research participants fell
ticipated and were treated at the local Veterans
largely on poor ward patients, whereas the
Affairs (VA) hospital, a family medicine clinic,
luxury of improved medical care went primarily
or the psychiatry clinic at the University o f
to private and wealthier patients. One example
School o f Medicine. All patients
of maltreatment of research participants is the
received a 10-week course o f bupropion S R
exploitation of unwilling prisoners as partici-
and were given the standard instructions for
pants in Nazi concentration camps (Shuster,
Nevada
taking the medication. Participants were ran-
1 9 9 7 ) . In the United States during the 1940s,
domly assigned to one o f three groups: medi-
the Tuskegee Syphilis Study used disadvan-
cation only, medication plus assessment, or
taged, rural, African American men to study the
medication plus assessment plus
untreated course of a disease that was by no
behavior
therapy. Behavior therapy involved 1 0 weeks
means confined solely to that
of individual and group treatment. Assessment
(Fairchild & Bayer, 1 9 9 9 ) . T o avoid interrup-
was conducted at intake, W e e k 3, W e e k 7,
tions in the study, these individuals
posttreatment,
deprived o f demonstrably effective treatment
6-month
follow-up,
and
population were
12-month follow-up. Although the research
long after such treatment became available to
design and outcomes from this study were
the general public (Fairchild & Bayer, 1 9 9 9 ) .
enlightening, the locations in which this study took place perhaps presented more information and challenging questions than did the initial research hypotheses. As different parts o f this chapter are highlighted, brief examples from this study are provided to illustrate how the investigators addressed the challenges o f working within these medical clinics.
It is against this historical background that the concepts of justice and ethical practice being relevant to research involving human participants (Francis, 2 0 0 1 ) is painfully clear. And it is for these reasons that the selection o f research participants needs to be scrutinized to ensure that particular groups o f people are not being selected simply because o f availability, c o m p r o m i s e d position, or
manipulability.
Rather, participants should be selected for H I S T O R I C A L A N D E T H I C A L ISSUES
reasons related to the problems being studied. Ultimately, when research supported by public
Before commencing a program o f research
funds leads to the development o f new thera-
within a medical setting, it is useful to con-
pies and procedures, ethical guidelines demand
sider the historical context. T w o important
that these treatments not provide advantages
historical angles are the history o f research in
only to those who can afford them and that
medical practice and the history o f the inte-
such research should not unduly involve
gration o f psychological interventions with
persons from groups unlikely to be among the
traditional medical practice. Investigators are
beneficiaries o f subsequent applications of that
also encouraged to become familiar with the
research.
tradition
o f research within
the specific
clinics in which they will be working. One historical issue to consider is the treatment o f research participants
in medical
research. There is a history in medical research
Three basic ethical principles are particularly relevant to the ethics o f research involving human
participants:
"beneficence," and
"respect o f persons," "justice" (U.S. National
Commission for the Protection of Human
515
516
SPECIAL ISSUES Subjects
of
Biomedical
and
Behavioral
alternatives to the proposed
intervention;
Research, 1 9 7 9 ) . Respect for persons incorpo-
(d) an account o f the relevant risks, benefits,
rates at least two ethical convictions, namely,
and uncertainties related to each alternative;
that
(a) individuals
should
be
treated
as
and (e) reassurance that personally identifi-
autonomous agents and (b) persons with dimin-
able private information will be held in strict
ished autonomy are entitled to protection.
confidence (Ashford, Scollay, & Harrington,
Beneficence requires that participants be treated
2 0 0 2 ) . O n e must remember that informed
in an ethical manner not only by respecting their
consent originates from the research partici-
decisions and protecting them from harm but
pants' legal and ethical right to direct what
also by making efforts to secure their well-being.
happens to their bodies and from the ethical
T w o general rules have been devised as corre-
duty o f the research team to involve the
sponding expressions o f beneficent actions,
participants in the decision-making process.
namely, to (a) do no harm and (b) maximize
In the medical industry, there often seems
possible benefits and minimize possible harms
to be a distinction made between research
(Reitsma 8c Moreno, 2 0 0 2 ) . T h e final basic
and practice. Research
ethical principle widely accepted in research is
data, whereas the term practice
is thought to focus o n connotes a
that of justice. In delving into this expansive
focus on healing. O n e o f the biggest distin-
topic, one needs to consider the question of who
guishing
should receive the benefits of research and who
within medical settings is the need to interact
should bear its burdens. This is a question of
effectively with an existing system that typi-
factors
o f research
justice in the sense of "fairness in distribution"
cally
or "what is deserved."
research demands.
prioritizes
clinical
conducted
practice
above
M a n y medical clinics
A very important topic that may be under-
have not experienced the effective integration
emphasized is "informed consent." Informed
o f research and practice. This chapter out-
consent is a process, not just a form to be
lines h o w t o develop a research program that
signed. It is necessary for the researcher to pre-
will w o r k within these settings.
sent all of the pertinent information to the participants to enable these individuals
to
voluntarily decide whether or not to partici-
DEFINING A RESEARCH QUESTION
pate. It is fundamental to ensure respect for persons through provision o f thoughtful con-
W h e n working within a fast-paced setting
sent for a voluntary act (Casari 8c Massimo,
with limited time and staff resources, it is
2 0 0 2 ) . T h e procedures
obtaining
important to pose a question in the most effi-
informed consent should be designed to edu-
used in
cient way possible. In other words, consider
cate the participants in terms that they can
posing the question in a way such that the
comprehend. Therefore, the language and doc-
findings add the most information to the
umentation must be written in terms that are
current state o f knowledge in the field. For
understandable to the people being asked to
example, investigating the question
participate.
Treatment X w o r k ? " may be interesting, but
T h e essential c o m p o n e n t s that be included
when
attempting
to
"Does
should
one can gather more informative data if the
obtain
question is posed in such a way that health
informed consent from research participants
care providers can determine which
are as follows: (a) a description o f the overall
is best for which
treatment
experience that participants can expect; (b) a
tions (Paul, 1 9 6 7 ) . Therefore, studying multi-
people under which
condi-
description o f the benefits that participants
ple factors that may influence the effects of
may
treatment will provide more information than
reasonably
expect;
(c) r e a s o n a b l e
Practical simply inquiring whether or not
Research
in a Medical
Setting
517
treatment
There are several steps researchers can take
works. Furthermore, investigating the mecha-
to facilitate the adoption o f new ideas in med-
nism o f change in addition to outcomes might
ical settings (Strosahl, 2 0 0 1 ) . First, researchers
better inform treatment development (Follette,
should attain the support o f the administra-
1 9 9 5 ) . Researchers can also maximize effi-
tion (e.g., chief executive officers, directors o f
ciency by exploring alternative explanations
units). They should ask these people whether
to the investigated problem. For instance, in
they would be willing to help pitch the idea to
the described case study, the
investigators
the rest o f the clinic. Second, if possible,
hypothesized that treating emotional avoid-
researchers should try to attend staff and/or
ance would
function
to reduce
smoking. They also investigated
cigarette alternative
residents
meetings. Getting to k n o w
the
people within the clinic can be helpful in try-
mechanisms o f change for smoking cessation,
ing
including social support, stage o f readiness,
Having others' support will help to facilitate
and the taste o f cigarettes after taking the
assessment and
medication. By assessing various
increase the likelihood o f obtaining
possible
to
advance
project treatment
implementation. and
may
also more
mediating variables, the investigators could
referrals. A related strategy involves enlisting
identify which mechanisms accounted for the
an employee o f the institution as a collabora-
largest degree o f change in outcomes.
tor or consultant on the study. In the case study, physicians from the various clinic sites were invited to be consultants on the study. T h e investigators
SELLING T H E RESEARCH IDEA
also attended
meetings to educate
residency
the residents
about
Despite the most exciting research ideas, one
bupropion S R , describe the research goals,
o f the main obstacles to confront is selling
and enlist the support o f the medical staff.
the ideas to staff at the medical
facility.
Third, researchers should find out whether
Depending on the size o f the medical setting
there is specific information o f interest to clinic
and the size o f the project, it will be more or
staff. Some information can be easily collected
less feasible to pitch research ideas to the
along with other data. Being open to gathering
administration and staff. Research may be
information relevant to the practice setting
an integral part o f some medical settings,
increases the chance that the clinic will accept
whereas other facilities have never partici-
the research idea. Also, when presenting the
pated in research projects. Before pitching the
idea, researchers should try to have as much
research idea, it would be helpful to assess
detail formulated as possible while maintaining
h o w supportive the clinic or hospital is o f
a degree o f openness to change. Providing a
research. If it has supported research in the
clearly explicated project will bolster confi-
past, it would be ideal to talk to investigators
dence in the competence o f the investigators.
working in that setting so as to anticipate
At the same time, maintaining a degree of flex-
possible institutional barriers that might arise.
ibility will also increase the likelihood o f
F o r example, one practical consideration to
success in these settings.
anticipate may be related to the increased
In addition to information, the medical
security measures that have developed in hos-
clinic or clinic staff may be in need o f services
pitals and clinics during recent years. It may
that the researchers can provide. For instance,
be important to show the staff and adminis-
the investigators in the case study provided
tration that the researchers share their con-
instruction on using the Internet to clinic
cerns about security and will make every
nurses at one site and permitted use o f the
effort to w o r k within their regulations.
assessment computers when these computers
518
SPECIAL ISSUES were not being used by research participants.
nonresponse t o treatment.
In exchange for the use o f these resources, the
medical facilities may insist on additional exclu-
clinic provided
the researchers with
In fact, some
office
sion criteria based on institutional concerns
space and referrals to the smoking cessation
about appropriate candidates for treatment.
program.
Researchers should work collaboratively with
Researchers should try, to the extent pos-
the medical setting staff to develop the safest,
sible, t o incorporate data collection into the
most efficient, and strongest research design
normal routine of treatment. In the case
possible given the requirements of the setting.
study, the investigators simply set up
an
In addition to ethical issues, investigators
adjunctive smoking cessation clinic in the V A
often will need to negotiate more practical
Mental
site.
concerns. T h e issue o f office space is fre-
Participation by veterans in these adjunctive
Health
Clinic
at
another
quently a dilemma. Researchers may have
clinics was counted as use o f V A services.
limited space, if any at all, within which to work. Although having an office for the investigators may be ideal, researchers might
INSTITUTIONAL CONSIDERATIONS
not have any office space available and will have to work quickly in the hallways between
Medical settings use written and unwritten
seeing patients. F o r this reason, it is impor-
policies that include, but are n o t limited to,
tant to have organized assessments that are
regulations,
easily accessible and include clear instruc-
particular guidelines and protocols o f the
tions. Practical issues regarding the everyday
institutional
review
board
medical setting, and other research review
operation o f a research study might seem
procedures. It is important to investigate and
inconsequential at first. However, these prob-
address these institutional policies to facili-
lems can potentially interfere with the flow o f
tate cooperation within the clinic. T h e r e are
research and possibly the eventual completion
two policy matters to which researchers must
of the project. Therefore, it is important to
attend (a) the ethics o f the proposed research
carefully plan h o w treatment and assessments
study and (b) practical concerns related to
will be conducted. In addition to space con-
the logistics o f conducting the study.
cerns, researchers should clearly outline the
Researchers should be aware o f the ethical
format of assessments (e.g., paper and pencil,
guidelines followed by the medical facility.
c o m p u t e r ) , where patients
Although most adhere to standard guidelines,
assessments, where data will be stored, w h o
will complete
some facilities may incorporate additional con-
will have access t o the data, and
siderations. For example, investigators should
personnel will be collecting the data.
which
become familiar with the institutional protocol for adverse events. If none is specified, procedures should be created indicating w h o should
RESEARCH DESIGN
be contacted in an emergency, what reports need to be written in such a case, who should
T h e r e are a variety o f design options to use
receive copies o f the reports, and what needs to
when conducting research in a medical set-
be done in response to specific adverse events.
ting. These designs range from single-case
In addition, some medical settings may have
methodology to large randomized
special policies related to patients' response, or
trials. Although a review o f research designs
lack o f response, to treatment. Patients should
is beyond the scope o f this chapter, the
always be adequately informed
reader is referred to B a r l o w and
of poten-
tial risks, including iatrogenic effects
and
control
Hersen
( 1 9 8 4 ) for single-case methodology and to
Practical Kazdin ( 1 9 9 8 a , 1 9 9 8 b ) for group
design
methodology. The context o f the medical setting introduces special issues that might not apply in
Research
in a Medical
Setting
have large enough variability in
symptom
severity to detect differences? Are the assessments sensitive enough to detect changes over the duration o f the study?
other settings. For example, hospitals may
Another issue that should be addressed is
differ in their views of what types of control
attrition. Some patients might leave the study
conditions are acceptable. Although ethical
before they have completed their participation.
guidelines provide a framework from which to
Although
work, some hospitals or clinics may have more
received the full treatment, information about
stringent policies. Again, researchers need to
these individuals can be very useful. Collecting
these patients
might
not
have
become knowledgeable about local policies and
information about why the patients quit treat-
how to best structure research projects in ways
ment can inform providers
that work within these systems. It is important
patients are more likely to adhere to the treat-
about
which
to find the best balance between workability
ment in the future. If participants do not agree
within the hospital system and strength o f the
to complete the assessment portion o f the
conclusions that can be drawn from the study.
study, using a brief telephone follow-up ques-
Ideally, researchers should identify extraneous
tionnaire or mailing a brief assessment packet
variables and implement procedures that will
can be invaluable. In the case study, the inves-
best control for them. In the case study, the
tigators decided to use a brief, 5-minute tele-
investigators speculated that contact with
phone follow-up format to collect information
assessment staff during the course o f treatment
about smoking status, participation in other
might affect the outcome. Consequently, an
treatments, hypothesized mediating variables
additional condition was added in which par-
(e.g., avoidance, social support), and
ticipants continued to receive medication but
patients were choosing to quit the study at that
why
had limited contact with assessment staff
time. This procedure allowed the researchers
during the treatment phase of the study.
to gather minimal outcome data and prelimi-
In many medical clinics, multiple health care providers see one patient. Within one
nary data about which individuals were not as likely to benefit from the intervention.
clinic, it is not uncommon for a patient to be
It is also important to track and report
involved in an anger management group, indi-
information regarding participants w h o were
vidual therapy for posttraumatic stress disor-
excluded from the study. Often, there are
der, and treatment for diabetes. It is possible
treatment options that are proscribed given
that treatments other than the one under inves-
certain conditions. F o r example, in the smok-
tigation might influence outcomes. In other
ing cessation study, the investigators found
words, outcomes may be due to changes that
that bupropion S R was contraindicated for
are occurring in another treatment domain. It
people w h o , among other things, were taking
is also plausible that an intervention targeted at
selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors or had
another problem is dampening the impact that
a history o f seizures. Given these precau-
the treatment under investigation may have.
tions, many veterans were unable to partici-
Tracking a patient's participation across health
pate in the study due to medical exclusions
services can help to identify the impact o f other
(Kohlenberg, Antonuccio, Hayes, Gifford, &
treatments on patient outcome. Some other
Piasecki, 2 0 0 2 ) . These data provided useful
questions to ask include the following: Has the
information regarding the effectiveness o f
patient already participated in identical or
using this treatment option within the V A
similar treatments such that little improvement
clinic. Identifying
can be expected? Does the patient population
treatments are not suitable is as important as
populations
for
whom
519
520
SPECIAL ISSUES identifying those for w h o m treatments are effective.
In
conducting
good
assessment,
it
is
important t o collect meaningful and efficient measures. Documentation and referrals should be easy (Hollis et al.,
2000), especially if clinic
RECRUITMENT
personnel are collecting data as part o f the
Before recruiting participants for a study, it is
the time demands of busy practice environ-
important to define what the inclusion and
ments. In addition, investigators should care-
research program. It is imperative t o consider
exclusion criteria will be. As described previ-
fully research the most persuasive data. For
ously, patients may have medical or psychi-
example, is self-report o f cigarettes smoked
atric conditions that would preclude their
better than carbon monoxide reading? In addi-
participation. It is useful to have a script and a
tion, researchers should consider the length o f
standardized screening assessment for staff so
time for which data are being collected and
that all participants receive the same informa-
whether it is possible t o see a change in these
tion during the initial phone screen. T h e script
data during that amount o f time.
should include a thorough discussion o f the
Finally, it is important for investigators to
expectations o f study participants. Although
constantly attend to the morale o f the people
patients will be presented with informed con-
involved in the project as well as t o the rela-
sent documents when they enter the study, in
tionship between research assistants and the
the chapter authors' experience, patients seem
medical facility staff. T h e number o f obstacles
t o participate more fully in the program when
that can be encountered during a research
informed consent and commitment-enhancing
study c a n be demoralizing. Barriers may
procedures are implemented during the first
range from dealing with institutional politics
contact with the patients.
to trying to get patients to adhere to treatment and complete assessments. Within this context, treatment and assessment staff may become less committed to the project, drift in
ASSESSMENT
their delivery o f services, and not be as attenbe
tive to maintaining respectful and effective
considered before assessment systems are put
relationships with coworkers. Investigators
There
are several issues that
should
into place. First, investigators should iden-
can plan ongoing training opportunities, hold
tify whether or not reactivity to assessments
regular meetings, and provide a great deal o f
might be a concern. F o r example, researchers
encouragement to everyone involved in the
have found that people can change behavior
study t o maintain healthy relationships and
and their physiology when they have access to
morale. As the investigators found in the case
physiological measures (Abueg, Colletti, 8c
study, food nearly always works.
Kopel, 1 9 8 5 ; M c D o w e l l et al., 1 9 9 9 ; Scharff, Marcus,
8c M a s e k , 2002). In the case study,
researchers were measuring carbon monoxide
TREATMENT
readings from smoking patients and decided to ensure that patients did not see their ratings
Before interventions are implemented, there are
until after the study was completed. Providing
a few steps that researchers can take t o assist in
an attractive graph o f the patients' readings
the flow of the project. T o begin, researchers
only at the end of the study reduced the possi-
should design protocols that explicate the roles
ble effects o f reactivity to this measure and
of the staff that are involved in the study.
gave patients something additional to take
Research and medical staff should be trained on
away from their participation in the study.
what is and is not expected of each. These
Practical
Research
in a Medical Setting
\
protocols should also outline who should be
patients actually received the treatment in
contacted for psychiatric and/or medical emer-
totality. For example, if patients were taking
gencies. Additional
be
an antidepressant, did they take the indicated
addressed in the protocols might include the
number o f pills per day? O r , if they were
training of treatment providers and the moni-
using a nicotine patch, did they use the patch
toring o f treatment adherence.
as indicated? T o measure their adherence,
items that
should
Also, staff roles should fit efficiently into
patients could record whether or not they
office flow (Hollis et al., 2 0 0 0 ) . It is important
receive treatment each day o f the week. T h e
to have a steady flow and good communica-
problem with this procedure, however, is that
tion between health providers and research
self-monitoring has been found to be an effec-
assistants. For example, in the smoking cessa-
tive treatment in and o f itself (Abueg et al.,
tion study, research assistants conducted the
1 9 8 5 ; Gillmore et al., 2 0 0 1 ) . Therefore, it
screening and initial assessments and then pro-
would be difficult to tease apart the effects o f
vided physicians with a summary o f this infor-
the self-monitoring versus the treatment being
mation so as to work more efficiently with the
studied.
doctor. T o avoid losing possible participants,
adherence to treatment might be to use a ver-
investigators should also plan on minimizing
sion o f the time line follow-back procedure
the time between obtaining a referral, conduct-
(Sobell, Brown, L e o , & Sobell, 1 9 9 6 ) . In this
ing the phone screen, and starting treatment.
procedure, the research assistant reviews a
Training o f treatment providers will vary depending on w h o will be delivering treatment. I f the research team is providing the clinicians, investigators often have the luxury o f time and resources to spend on training. However, if the clinicians are current staff
An alternative w a y
o f assessing
calendar with the patients, w h o indicate the days on which they complied with the treatment protocol. Researchers should
always
consider h o w the delivery and monitoring o f treatment affects outcomes. In
the
smoking
cessation
study,
the
from within the medical setting, researchers
researchers specified how many therapy sessions
will need to develop a quick and efficient
a patient had to attend to be considered some-
training program for the clinicians. Providing
one who completed the treatment. Likewise,
manuals, videotapes, C D - R O M s , one-time
they needed to specify the definition of adher-
training meetings, or some combination o f
ence to the study medication. These decisions
these options may be the best approach to
should be made before treatment begins.
training. T o monitor any drift in treatment delivery and to ensure that treatment is being provided as it should be, researchers should
C O N F L I C T O F I N T E R E S T ISSUES
periodically monitor adherence to treatment protocols while the treatment phase is in
Conducting research within medical settings is
progress (Elkin, 1 9 9 9 ) . F o r example, adher-
influenced by factors that lie inside and outside
ence can be assessed through coding tapes o f
of the actual clinics. It is not uncommon for
clinicians w h o are providing the interven-
these factors to interfere with
tion. Periodic monitoring o f treatment deliv-
research
ery during the treatment phase o f the study
researchers have a moral and ethical obligation
can provide opportunities to give feedback to
to address these difficult issues and to make
treatment providers.
sure that important information is dissemi-
Investigators should also define what they
and
clinical practice.
appropriate However,
nated. Some o f these dilemmas include finan-
consider to be a treatment "completer." T o
cial conflicts o f interests and
conclude whether or not treatment was effec-
biases. These factors are especially complicated
publication
tive, researchers must be able to show that
to address when medications are included in
522
SPECIAL ISSUES the study. For example, it is difficult to think o f
results of their studies, no matter whose o x is
any arena involving information about medi-
gored. The integrity of medical research is at
cations that does not have significant industry
stake and is worth fighting for.
financial or marketing influences. Industry financial influences extend to federal regulatory agencies, professional organizations and
CONCLUSIONS
their journals, continuing medical education, scientific researchers, media experts, and con-
Conducting
sumer advocacy organizations (Antonuccio,
presents challenges that are typically not
Burns, & Danton, 2 0 0 2 ) . Respected psychi-
encountered in more traditional
atric researchers such as M a r k s (Marks et al.,
research environments. Investigators
1 9 9 3 ) and Fava ( 1 9 9 8 ) have warned that such
must study politics and persuasion nearly as
research
in medical
settings
behavioral often
widespread corporate interests may result in
much as they study their research question.
self-selecting academic oligarchies influencing
Although these added pressures can be daunt-
clinical and scientific information. In fact,
ing, the rewards of doing this work far surpass
those who produce data contrary to industry
the costs. Within 15 months, the researchers in
interests may find themselves vulnerable to
the case study helped approximately 3 0 0
legal, professional, or even personal attack,
patients to quit smoking. M a n y o f these
either directly or indirectly financed by the
patients would not have had the opportunity to
industry (Antonuccio et al., 2 0 0 2 ) .
join this program if it had been located outside
Furthermore, there are widely acknowledged
of their medical clinics. Although it took time
publication biases, often related to conflicts of
and patience to conduct this project, prelimi-
interest, that favor pharmaceutical industry
nary analyses indicate that 2 7 % o f those
products (Antonuccio et al., 2 0 0 2 ) . In fact, these
patients who could be contacted for assessment
biases have so eroded the credibility o f the med-
6 months after treatment remained smoke free.
ical literature (Quick, 2 0 0 1 ) that new proposals
The ultimate goal in health care is to develop
call for stringent accountability guidelines (e.g.,
and provide the best treatments possible for
Davidoff et al., 2 0 0 1 ; Moses & Martin, 2 0 0 1 )
those individuals who need them, implementing
aimed at ensuring researcher independence in
research programs within medical settings helps
study design, access to data, and right to pub-
to accomplish this goal in several ways. First, this
lish. It remains to be seen whether these new
practice can foster the dissemination of effective
guidelines will have the desired effect of improv-
treatments. Second, it can force researchers to
ing the quality and credibility o f the literature.
develop treatments that are more acceptable
The bottom line is that researchers in a medical
within these environments. Most important, by
setting have a moral and ethical obligation to
moving into medical clinics, patients who need
their human participants to
care will have access to a greater number of
independently
design, implement, analyze, and publish the
opportunities to decrease their suffering.
REFERENCES Abueg, F. R., Colletti, G., & Kopel, S. A. (1985). A study of reactivity: The effects of increased relevance and saliency of self-monitored smoking through enhanced carbon monoxide feedback. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 9, 3 2 1 - 3 3 3 . Antonuccio, D. O., Burns, D. D., & Danton, W. G. (2002). Antidepressants: A triumph of marketing over science? Prevention and Treatment, 5, Article 2 5 . [Online]. Retrieved January 2 0 , 2003, from http://journals.apa.org/prevention/volume5/ pre0050025c.html
Practical
Research
in a Medical
Ashford, R. U., Scollay, J . , 8c Harrington, P. (2002). Obtaining informed consent. Hospital Medicine, 63, 3 7 4 . Barlow, D. H., 8c Hersen, M. (1984). Single case experimental designs: Strategies for studying behavior change. New York: Pergamon. Casari, Ε. E , & Massimo, L. M . (2002). From informed to shared: The developing process of consent. Minerva Pediatrica, 54, 2 1 1 - 2 1 6 . Cummings, N. A. (2000). A psychologist's proactive guide to managed care: New roles and opportunities. In A. J . Kent S i M . Hersen (Eds.), A psychologist's proactive guide to managed mental health care (pp. 1 4 1 - 1 6 1 ) . Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Davidoff, F., DeAngerlis, C. D., Drazen, J . M., Nicfiolls, M. G., Hoey, J . , Hoigaard, L., Horton, R., Kotzin, S., Nicholls, M. G., Nylenna, M., Overbeke, A. J . P. M., Sox, H. C , Van der Weyden, M. B., Sc Wilkes, M. S. (2001). Sponsorship, authorship, and accountability. New England journal of Medicine, 345, 825-827. Elkin, I. ( 1 9 9 9 ) . A major dilemma in psychotherapy outcome research: Disentangling therapists from therapies. Clinical Psychology: Science & Practice, 6, 1 0 - 3 2 . Fairchild, A. L., 8c Bayer, R. (1999). Uses and abuses of Tuskegee. Science, 284, 9 1 9 - 9 2 1 . Fava, G. A. (1998). All our dreams are sold. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 67, 1 9 1 - 1 9 3 . Follette, W. C. (1995). Correcting methodological weaknesses in the knowledge base used to derive practice standards. In S. C. Hayes, V. M . Follette, R. M . Dawes, Sc Κ. E. Grady (Eds.), Scientific standards of psychological practice: Issues and recommendations (pp. 2 2 9 - 2 4 7 ) . Reno, NV: Context Press. Francis, C. K. (2001). The medical ethos and social responsibility in clinical medicine. Journal of the National Medical Association, 93, 5 7 - 6 9 . Gillmore, M . R., Gaylord, J . , Hartway, J . , Hoppe, M. J . , Morrison, D. M., Leigh, B. C , Se Rainey, D. T. (2001). Daily data collection of sexual and other healthrelated behaviors. Journal of Sex Research, 38, 3 5 - 4 2 . Hayes, S. C , Barlow, D. H., Sc Nelson-Gray, R. O. (1999). The scientist practitioner: Research and accountability in the age of managed care (2nd ed.). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn Sc Bacon. Hollis, J . F., Bills, R., Whitlock, E., Stevens, J . J . , Mullooly, J . , Sc Lichtenstein, E. (2000). Implementing tobacco interventions in the real world of managed care. Tobacco Control, 9, 1 8 - 2 1 . Kazdin, A. E. (1998a). Methodological issues and strategies in clinical research (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Kazdin, A. E. (1998b). Research design in clinical psychology (3rd ed.). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn Sc Bacon. Kohlenberg, B. S., Antonuccio, D. O., Hayes, S. C , Gifford, Ε. V., Sc Piasecki, M. P. (2002). Bupropion SR for nicotine dependent smokers: Limited applicability? Unpublished manuscript, University of Nevada School of Medicine. Marks, L M., Swinson, R. P., Basoglu, M., Kuch, K., Noshirvani, H., O'Sullivan, G., Lelliott, P. T., Kirby, M., McNamee, G., Sengun, S., Sc Wickwire, K. (1993). Alprazolam and exposure alone and combined in panic disorder with agoraphobia: A controlled study in London and Toronto. British Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 7 7 6 - 7 8 7 . McDowell, B . J . , Engberg, S., Sereika, S., Donovan, N., Jubeck, M . E., Weber, E., 8c Engberg, R. (1999). Effectiveness of behavioral therapy to treat incontinence in homebound older adults. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 47, 309-318. Moses, H., 8c Martin, J . B . (2001). Academic relationships with industry: A new Association, model for biomedical research. Journal of the American Medical 285, 9 3 3 - 9 3 5 .
Setting
524
SPECIAL ISSUES Paul, G. (1967). Strategy of outcome research in psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 31, 1 0 9 - 1 1 8 . Quick, J . (2001). Maintaining the integrity of the clinical evidence base. Bulletin of the World Health Organization, 79, 1 0 9 3 . Reitsma, A. M., & Moreno, J . D. (2002). Ethical regulations for innovative surgery: The last frontier. Journal of the American College of Surgery, 184, 7 9 2 - 8 0 1 . Scharff, L., Marcus, D. Α., &C Masek, B . J . (2002). A controlled study of minimalcontact thermal biofeedback treatment in children with migraine. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 27, 1 0 9 - 1 1 9 . Shuster, E. (1997). Fifty years later: The significance of the Nuremberg Code. New England Journal of Medicine, 337, 1 4 3 6 - 1 4 4 0 . Sobell, L. C , Brown, J . , Leo, G. I., & Sobell, M . B . (1996). The reliability of the Alcohol Timeline Followback when administered by telephone and by computer. Drug & Alcohol Dependence, 42, 4 9 - 5 4 . Strosahl, K. (2001). The integration of primary care and behavioral health: Type II changes in the era of managed care. In N. A. Cummings, W. O'Donohue, S. C. Hayes, & V. M . Follette (Eds.), Integrated behavioral healthcare: Positioning mental health practice with medical/surgical practice (pp. 4 5 - 6 9 ) . San Diego: Academic Press. U.S. National Commission for the Protection of Human Subjects of Biomedical and Behavioral Research. (1979). The Belmont Report: Ethical principles and guidelines for the protection of human subjects of research. [Online]. Retrieved July 15, 2 0 0 2 , from http://ohsr.od.nih.gov/mpa/belmont.php3
CHAPTER
10 28
Evaluating Outcomes in Health Care Settings JOAQUIN WILLIAM
D
B O R R E G O , JR., C.
A N D
FOLLETTE
uring this era o f managed care and
examining the effectiveness o f a specific
increased consumer awareness, there
treatment for diabetes for a particular individual
is an ever-increasing demand from dif-
to assessing for client satisfaction with the
ferent entities (e.g., federal agencies, third-
services received at a community outpatient
party reimbursements, consumer
advocate
clinic. Along with measuring intervention effec-
groups) to ask for evidence o f professional ser-
tiveness, there is also an increasing focus on
vice effectiveness. This demand is amplified by
evaluating prevention efforts in health-related
the dramatic cost increases in providing quality
problems such as substance abuse (Hansen,
health care (Relman, 1 9 8 8 ) . In turn, public and
2 0 0 2 ) . Evaluation of existing programs can
private organizations are beginning to incorpo-
focus on both process and overall program
rate evaluation components into different ser-
effectiveness (Kelley, V a n Horn, & De M a s o ,
vice delivery programs (Brown & Reed, 2 0 0 2 ) .
2 0 0 1 ) . This flexibility allows professionals to
This change is especially evident in health care
answer a multitude o f questions in different
settings that serve a wide range o f populations
practice settings (Posavac & Carey, 1 9 9 7 ) .
with different health problems (Fink, 1 9 9 3 ) .
Program evaluation is defined as a system-
One way o f directly addressing professional
atic methodology that allows professionals to
accountability is through the systematic evalua-
(a) document whether the treatment was imple-
tion of the services delivered. This systematic
mented as intended, (b) choose appropriate
evaluation of different service components is
measures that are sensitive to behavior change,
called program
T h e goal o f pro-
(c) demonstrate treatment effectiveness, and (d)
gram evaluation is the collection, analysis, and
demonstrate how a particular intervention pro-
evaluation.
interpretation o f data pertaining to intervention
duces change (if desired). T h e ideas set forth in
efforts (Rossi & Freeman, 1 9 9 3 ) . With pro-
this chapter are meant to be broad enough so
gram evaluation, a multitude o f service delivery
that the concepts covered can be implemented
practices can be evaluated, ranging
in multiple settings (e.g., private practice,
from
525
526
SPECIAL ISSUES medical centers) targeting different
services
STEPS IN P R O G R A M EVALUATION
(e.g., individual vs. group treatment, case management, consultation).
There are several steps involved in conducting
Program evaluation can be applied
to
a thorough program evaluation. T h e first step
address a host of physical (e.g., obesity, dia-
in program evaluation is defining the specific
betes) and psychological (e.g., depression,
targets or questions that the clinician is inter-
anxiety) problems encountered in health care
ested in addressing in the evaluation. As an
settings. Questions addressed
example, a clinician in private practice might
by
program
evaluation strategies can range from simple
be interested in evaluating the effectiveness o f
objective questions (e.g., " D o e s Intervention
cognitive-behavioral group treatment with an
X w o r k with Population Y ? " ) to more c o m -
obese population. In contrast, a clinician in a
plex and
"Are
rural community setting might be interested
are
in conducting a systemwide program evalua-
subjective questions
(e.g.,
clients satisfied with the services that
offered in a community clinic and w h y ? " ) . Although it is beyond the scope of the chap-
tion and so would prefer to assess treatment accessibility, treatment
acceptability, and
ter to outline the technical details of all aspects
treatment
of program evaluation, there is one distinction
community catchment area.
satisfaction within
a
particular
to keep in mind: the distinction is between a
Once the question (or set of questions) and
cost-benefit analysis and a cost-effectiveness
hypotheses have been identified, the next step
analysis. Cost-benefit analyses are procedures
is to identify the population to be involved in
used to measure programs and outcomes on the
the program evaluation. T h e target population
same metric, namely dollars. Program costs are
can range from being broad in nature (e.g.,
determined
(e.g., costs per employee in an
assessing for satisfaction in services received in
employee assistance program) and benefits are
a multipurpose clinic) to addressing clinically
measured in terms of dollars saved in medical
related questions with a specific population
services use or dollars saved in employee
(e.g., asking whether Treatment Ζ would work
replacement costs. Often, companies or policy-
with diabetes patients). With the former ques-
makers contracting for or funding services are
tion, clinicians are accessing all clients who use
interested in these kinds of program evaluations.
services that are offered in the clinic, whereas
Individual providers and clients are fre-
the latter question might focus on only a sub-
quently more interested in cost-effectiveness
set o f the clinic population
data. In this type of analysis, costs are still gen-
patients).
(e.g., diabetic
erally computed in dollars, whereas outcomes
O n c e the target population has been identi-
are measured in terms of specific effects. For
fied, the next step involves identifying the
example, it might cost $ 4 0 0 to add an addi-
independent and dependent variables that are
tional harm reduction treatment module to a
going to be used in the program evaluation.
substance abuse intervention to produce a spe-
T h e independent variables that are going to be
cific effect such as a demonstrable increase in
identified are related to the treatment the clin-
employer satisfaction. Details o f the differences
ician is interested in evaluating. T h e key to this
between these two strategies and technical
process is selecting variables that are specific.
details o f how to conduct each can be found
Instead o f asking "Does my antidepression
elsewhere (e.g., Yates, 1 9 9 6 ) . Instead, this
treatment
chapter provides the reader with an overview
clients?," one can develop a more precise
of issues to consider when developing and
question such as " H o w does each component
program
work
with
depressed
implementing a program evaluation compo-
of an antidepressant treatment program con-
nent into one's practice setting.
tribute to the o u t c o m e ? " or "Does the level of
Evaluating
j
Outcomes
training in a particular therapy modality affect
rather than vague. As an example, instructing
the outcome in this clinic?" O n the outcome
a client to "eat better" can be improved by
or dependent variable side o f the evaluation, it
specifying the actual behaviors that the clini-
is again useful to identify maximally useful
cian is interested in modifying: eating two or
measures. Although one might be interested in
three servings o f vegetables per day, decreasing
the question "Are clients satisfied with treat-
fatty food intake by 3 0 fat grams per day,
ment?," it may be much more informative to
drinking 6 4 ounces o f water per day, and so
ask "Does a particular antidepression
pro-
on. As another example, a direction to "exer-
gram lead to a decrease in Beck Depression
cise m o r e " can be improved by making specific
Inventory scores and an improvement in life
recommendations: stretching for 1 0 minutes
satisfaction, and does that lead to increased
per day, walking for 2 0 minutes per day/four
client satisfaction with services, session atten-
times per week, and so on.
dance, and even fee remittance?" This latter
Meeting the criteria for choosing observable
version o f the specification o f the dependent
and specific target behaviors makes it easier for
variable gives a glimpse o f the potential power
clinicians to measure outcomes. It is much
o f program evaluation in that processes that
easier to measure behaviors that are observable
serve the program and the clients alike can be
and specific (e.g., monitoring the frequency
highlighted.
with which a person smokes per day) than to measure events that are unobservable
and
vague (e.g., "thinking patterns" that may or
Identifying and Defining Behaviors of Interest
may not be highly correlated with the actual treatment goals). Appealing to hypothetical
Before a treatment is implemented and eval-
constructs (e.g., the mind, the subconscious)
uated, the clinician should define what behav-
makes it difficult to monitor and document the
iors he or she is interested in changing given the
behaviors, and so they are not as amenable to
target population (e.g., decreasing smoking in
change through standard clinical practices. In
smokers if abstinence is the goal vs. decreasing
contrast, measuring behavior change involving
o f cigarettes smoked if harm
observable and specific behaviors makes it
reduction is the goal). This selection process
easier for clients to follow directions and increa-
and
ses the likelihood o f compliance. It may be
should occur early in the assessment process
helpful to make use o f simple instrumentation
(Hawkins, 1 9 8 6 ) . Three criteria for identify-
in some cases. Walking can be monitored using
the number
involves identifying
target
behaviors
ing, defining, and choosing target behaviors
a pedometer. Medication can be monitored
are that they must be observable, specific, and
with pill counts or with more expensive devices
measurable. Although private events (e.g., cog-
that can record when and how often a pill
nitions) can be measured, it is easier for clini-
bottle is opened.
cians and clients to measure observable events. As an example, although the clinician can assign a homework assignment to monitor the number o f times a person "thinks" about hav-
Other Types of Assessment Methods
ing a cigarette, it might be more beneficial to
Several assessment methods are available to
have the patient monitor the number of times
the clinician in documenting and demonstrat-
he or she smokes during a specified time period
ing behavior change. Some o f these methods
(e.g., each day, each week).
include interviews, self-report measures (e.g.,
T h e second criterion involves choosing a
rating scales), and direct observation in analog
behavior or a set o f behaviors that is specific
(i.e., clinic) and natural settings. Although
527
528
SPECIAL ISSUES interviews are considered to be qualitative in
Given the target behavior in question, some
nature, they are nonetheless important because
instruments will be more applicable than others.
they provide the clinician with information
Choosing instruments that are sensitive to
from the client's perspective about his or her
change is important because not all instruments
history and current environmental situation.
display the same level of sensitivity. As with the
Methods
line follow-back
previous example, some measures might be
(Sobell & Sobell, 1 9 9 6 ) can be reliable and
more applicable in measuring changes in global
useful ways in which to gather baseline data.
distress patterns over a longer period of time
such
as time
Self-report measures are also important
in
(e.g., 4 months), whereas other instruments
helping to choose target behaviors to change.
might be more suitable for detecting subtle
When available, staff trained to a high level of
changes in client behavior on a shorter time
reliability should be used to conduct behav-
basis (e.g., daily, weekly). If clinicians are inter-
ioral observations o f specific client behaviors
ested in tracking subtle changes but choose an
using instruments validated in the literature. A
instrument that is designed to detect global
highly structured environment (e.g., medical
changes, actual changes in client behavior will
center setting) can facilitate the process o f mak-
not be detected.
ing systematic observations but might lack ecological validity. Natural environments can pose problems o f their own, although time sampling using audiotapes may provide useful data if the clinician is evaluating communication training in a family therapy intervention. Institutional settings provide unique data-gathering opportunities o f their own (for a particularly creative reference to program evaluation in institu-
Other Issues Related to the Selection of Assessment Strategies Program evaluation can have a variety of goals and occurs in many types of settings. One goal may be to assess for the presence or absence o f a diagnostic label before and after an intervention. Such assessments will have certain
tional settings, see Paul & Menditto, 1 9 9 2 ) .
characteristics (Silva, 1 9 9 3 a , 1 9 9 3 b ) . If a pro-
Choosing Appropriate and Sensitive Measures
problem, or if the program is designed to
gram assumes a unitary cause for a particular
Choosing appropriate measures will also
deliver a structured, standardized, comprehensive treatment for a problem, reliable standardized assessments o f client problem changes and
assist clinicians in determining what and when
treatment
integrity may be sufficient (cf.
to measure. As stated previously, clinicians
Follette, Naugle, & Linnerooth, 2 0 0 0 ; Haynes,
should choose variables of interest that are clin-
1 9 9 2 ; Haynes, Leisen, & Blaine, 1 9 9 7 ) .
ically meaningful (Biskupiak, 2 0 0 1 ) . Given the
However, some programs and many prac-
question that is asked and the independent and
tices allow treatment staff to vary treatment
dependent variables that are chosen, measures
depending on client characteristics. In this case,
should be chosen that are suitable for the pro-
the service providers in a program ought to
ject. In program evaluation, it is important to
attend to the selection of assessment procedures
choose a measure or a set o f measures that is
that demonstrate
appropriate
and sensitive enough to detect
Nelson, & Jarrett, 1 9 8 7 ; Silva, 1993b). Treat-
changes for the evaluation of targeted behaviors.
ment utility of assessment is the degree to which
When appropriate, clinicians should choose instruments that are suitable for measuring the behavior change being monitored (e.g., changes in distress as a result of decreasing food intake).
treatment
utility
(Hayes,
information derived from an assessment procedure would lead a therapist to alter an intervention in a way that would affect outcome. For example, an assessment of a client's activity level
Evaluating
Outcomes
\
might lead a therapist to include a behavioral
allows for the systematic examination o f
activation component to treatment for depres-
progress (or lack thereof) when dealing with
sion. If decisions based on such an assessment
individual
clients (Hawkins
8c M a t h e w s ,
lead to improved outcomes, the assessment
1 9 9 9 ) . Although statistical analytic procedures
procedure has treatment utility. However, not all
are available for analyzing single-case designs,
programs aEow such flexibility.
visual analysis is frequently appropriate when working with individuals. As recommended by Franklin, Allison, and Gorman ( 1 9 9 6 ) , com-
Levels of Analysis Affecting Design Considerations
parison of the dependent measure across treatment conditions can provide objective data
Program evaluation can occur at many
from which to infer clinical significance.
levels, including the individual client level (i.e., the efficacy o f a type o f intervention for
Group
Designs.
An often-neglected step
a type o f client), the clinic level, or even the
involved in program evaluation is checking to
institutional level. F o r simplicity, the follow-
see whether the treatment was implemented as
ing discussion is limited to the first two cases.
intended. T h e process o f evaluating the degree to which the treatment was delivered as
Single-Case
Designs.
For
single-case
intended is called treatment
(Kazdin,
integrity
designs, multiple baseline data points can be
1 9 9 2 ) . Checking for treatment integrity allows
collected before starting treatment. Gathering
clinicians to check whether the independent
multiple data points during baseline helps to
variable (i.e., the delivered treatment) was suc-
guard against reactivity to measures, lack o f
cessfully manipulated (Follette &
reliability o f a single measurement point, and
1 9 9 9 ) . Figure 2 8 . 2 highlights the four possible
Compton,
regression to the mean artifacts. Multiple data
outcomes involved in conducting a manipula-
points also help in determining the trend and
tion check. In Quadrant I, the independent
level o f the problem being assessed. A single-
variable has been successfully implemented,
case design allows for the use o f repeated
and there was an observed change in the
measurement o f changing client behaviors,
dependent variable. Clinically, this is the best
allowing clinicians (or other evaluators) to
possible outcome because there is a level of cer-
track behavior on an individual basis and to
tainty that the observed change in targeted
tailor the measurement to fit their clinical
behavior is due to the introduction o f treat-
needs (e.g., daily monitoring, weekly therapy
ment. In Quadrant II, the treatment was suc-
session change, within-session change).
cessfully
For individual cases, a strategy that offers
implemented,
but there was
no
observed change in behavior (i.e., no change in
clinical utility is that o f graphing data for
the dependent
clients (Figure 2 8 . 1 ) . Graphing data creates a
research is being conducted, one possible
variable). I f group
design
visual analysis for clinicians that can be shared
explanation is that the treatment did not work
directly with clients. In turn, the graphed data
in accomplishing the clinically relevant goals
(Hawkins,
(assuming that there was sufficient statistical
Mathews, & Hamdan, 1 9 9 9 ) . M o r e impor-
power). In Quadrant III, the treatment was
can lead to therapeutic
gains
tant, if a reliable coding system is in place for
not
the coding of different behaviors, the graphed
was still an observed change in the desired
successfully
implemented,
but
there
data can serve as an objective measure o f client
outcome (e.g., decrease in smoking frequency).
progress during the intervention (Hawkins et
Unfortunately, the change in the dependent
al., 1 9 9 9 ) . This is important information for
variable cannot be attributed to the imple-
both clinicians and clients. For clinicians, it
mented treatment. T h e observed change might
529
530
j
SPECIAL ISSUES Baseline 1
Intervention
•A/V A
.
.
V\ ν
1
1
2
1
3
,
4
NY
4
A
\/V\
V
'
5
ι
6
7
8
9
Intervention
*
.
1
Baseline 2
1
,
,
1
1
1
1
i
\
Λ
1 0 11 12 1 3 1 4 1 5 1 6 17 1 8 1 9 2 0 2 1 2 2 2 3 2 4 2 5 2 6 2 7 2 8 2 9 3 0 3 1 3 2 3 3 3 4 3 5 3 6
Days
Figure 28,1
Individual Monitoring of Behavior Change
be due to non-treatment-related events (e.g.,
checklists that are specific to the treatment o f
new medical information
interest (for a detailed discussion on treat-
received from
a
physician during the course o f treatment) or to
ment integrity checklists, see W a l t z , Addis,
threats to internal validity (e.g., changes due to
Koerner, & J a c o b s o n , 1 9 9 3 ) .
history and maturation) (Follette & Compton, 1 9 9 9 ; Kazdin, 1 9 9 2 ) . In Quadrant IV, the treatment was not successfully implemented,
ANALYSIS O F O U T C O M E
and there was no observed change in the tar-
EVALUATION
geted behavior. From a program evaluation perspective, this result would highlight the
If clinicians are interested in evaluating out-
need to implement better therapy training or to
comes for groups of individuals, there are a
identify another therapy that might be easier to
variety o f analytic techniques that are avail-
deliver for the population being evaluated.
able. Using conventional inferential statistical
In summary, the best clinical condition to
analyses targeting groups, clinicians can com-
have is that in Quadrant I, where clinicians
pare (a) one treatment condition with another
have a level o f certainty that a change in a
or (b) a specific treatment with a placebo or
client's behavior (e.g., decrease in weight) is
"wait list" group or some "treatment as usual"
due to the treatment delivered (e.g., targeting
condition (i.e., control group) (Figure 2 8 . 3 ) .
weight loss). F o r clinicians w h o use treat-
There is a growing literature that provides
ment manuals as a means o f delivering treat-
an alternative to traditional null hypothesis
ment,
testing in the form o f model fitting (Bakeman
treatment
protocol
checklists
are
available in many o f the treatment manuals.
&
If treatment manuals are not available, it
test a priori models o f processes that might
is possible to develop treatment
explain change. T h e notion in model fitting is
integrity
Gottman, 1 9 9 7 ) . Model-fitting strategies
Evaluating Changes in Dependent Variable (DV) Yes
Change in D V ( + )
Change in IV (+) No Change in D V (-)
Yes I
II
No Change in IV (-) No
Change in DV ( + )
No Change in IV (-) No Change in D V (-)
III
Figure 2 8 . 2
\
No
Change in IV ( + )
Manipulation of Independent Variable (IV)
Outcomes
IV
T r e a t m e n t Integrity Q u a d r a n t
N O T E : IV = independent variable; D V = dependent variable.
that clinicians can posit those factors that
First, clinicians can summarize program
account for change and then test the goodness
effects in terms o f Cohen's effect size statistics
of fit o f the proposed model to the actual data.
(Cohen, 1 9 8 8 ) that are often used in meta-
Clinicians can identify sources o f change that
analyses (Rosenthal &
DiMatteo, 2001).
are not modeled to refine their understanding
Rather than showing only that a program
of the change process. These techniques are
produces a change larger than zero, effect size
somewhat more demanding in terms o f soft-
statistics suggest h o w large a program change
ware and numbers o f subjects required for
is compared with similar programs if such
analysis, but they can produce data that guide
summary data exist. Regardless o f the avail-
theory and treatment development in ways
ability o f comparison data, effect sizes and
that conventional statistics cannot.
confidence intervals give some indication o f the magnitude of change a program produces.
Methods of Describing Change Although most conventional group statistical approaches to summarizing outcomes
A second approach gaining acceptance is that
o f normative
comparisons (Kendall,
Marrs-Garcia, Nath,
&
Sheldrick, 1 9 9 9 ;
Rogers, Howard, & Vessey, 1 9 9 3 ) . In this
make use o f reporting the mean change aver-
strategy, if normative data on nonclinical sam-
aged across all participants, many researchers
ples exist, the posttreatment means are tested
looking for ways in which to better describe
to see whether those treated for a problem are
the meaningfulness o f change have proposed
improved to a degree that is equivalent to a
alternatives to merely reporting mean changes
nonclinical normative sample. For example, if
between treatment or program conditions.
a program is designed to address problems o f
Three strategies deserve some mention here
child externalizing behavior (i.e., acting out),
(see also Follette & Callaghan, 2 0 0 1 ) .
two analytic steps would occur. First, the
531
532
SPECIAL ISSUES
Little change observed in control group
High Stress Levels Medium
Low
C h a n g e observed in experimental group
Pre-Treatment
Figure 28.3
Post-Treatment
Traditional Monitoring of Behavior Change
change from pretreatment to
posttreatment
it allows evaluators to categorize program
would be assessed statistically. If the change
results in terms o f what proportion o f individ-
was significant, the posttest level o f externaliz-
ual clients actually significantly improved to
ing behavior would be compared with the
become indistinguishable from normal con-
norms for a nonclinical sample to see whether
trols, what proportion improved but still dif-
the program returned participants to a normal
fered from controls, what proportion did not
level of functioning. These kinds o f data are
improve, and what proportion deteriorated as
useful
a result o f treatment.
in providing
consumers
and
pro-
gram designers with information about the
These three methods o f describing change
practical magnitude o f change an intervention
produced by a program are clinically relevant
produces.
to clients and providers alike, whereas cost-
Although equivalence or normative testing
benefit analyses and even cost-effectiveness
is useful, it assesses average changes at the level
analyses may be more meaningful to policy-
of the group. T h e third alternative for summa-
makers or those w h o contract for services.
rizing data is done at the level of the individual. In an approach similar to equivalence testing, each individual's result is tested to see whether the program both produced a reliable change
Assessment of Social Validity An often-neglected aspect o f program evalu-
and returned the client to a normal level of
ation is assessing what clients find to be accept-
functioning (Jacobson, Follette, & Revenstorf,
able treatment and their satisfaction with the
1 9 8 4 ; J a c o b s o n & T r u a x , 1 9 9 1 ) . Like equiva-
treatment process and outcome. Assessing for
lence testing, this approach requires the exis-
these two components involves assessment of
tence of good normative data on nonclinical
social validity. W o l f
participants
The
important issue o f assessing for social validity in
advantage o f this approach over others is that
the context o f services offered with respect to
for dependent measures.
( 1 9 7 8 ) addressed
the
Evaluating
Outcomes
|
three criteria. T h e first criterion for social
clients what they thought o f or h o w they
validity is assessing for the social significance of
perceived the treatment process and whether
the identified target behaviors and treatment
they were satisfied with the
goals. If clinicians were to identify treatment
Because of the subjective nature of clients'
goals, would clients find the target behaviors
responses, assessing for treatment satisfaction is
and treatment goals to be acceptable? It is very
often omitted in the scientific evaluation o f
important for clients to have input on the target
treatment programs. Although there is a sub-
behaviors given that not agreeing on the same
jective component to assessing for social valid-
target behaviors can interfere with and impede
ity, it is nonetheless an important variable to
treatment progress. As an example, if a clinician
assess for because it can provide valuable clini-
is working with a Mexican American woman
cal information. Information that assesses for
with diabetes, the target goal of decreasing
client acceptability of the treatment process and
intervention.
her intake of fat and sugar and increasing her
client satisfaction with the treatment should be
intake of steamed vegetables (e.g., broccoli)
considered as important as objective indexes o f
would probably fail given that the client has
behavior change (e.g., reported change on a
a history of eating different kinds o f food that
psychometrically sound rating scale). T h e like-
are prepared and cooked differently
(i.e.,
lihood of sustained change observed in treat-
Mexican diet). T h e second criterion to address
ment may be affected by clients' acceptability o f
is the social appropriateness of the treatment
the treatment and their satisfaction with the
procedures. It is important to assess whether
treatment and services offered.
clients find a specific treatment to be acceptable. T h e assumption is that clients who find a treatment more acceptable are more likely to participate in treatment than are those who do not find the treatment to be acceptable. Finally, the third criterion to address is the social importance of the treatment effects. This is the "consumer
satisfaction" c o m p o n e n t
o f social
validity in that it informs clinicians as to whether clients were satisfied with the services offered. Assessing for consumer satisfaction can be tailored to fit the clinician's or agency's needs. As an example, consumer satisfaction can vary from assessing a client's satisfaction with the actual treatment to assessing his or her satisfaction with administrative services and the like. Unfortunately,
CONCLUSIONS This chapter has attempted to make an argument for the systematic method o f conducting program evaluations that can be adapted for different types o f practice settings. Different contingencies
(e.g., third-party
reimburse-
ments) have placed an even greater emphasis on professionals becoming active participants in the continuous evaluation of their services. Implementing a program evaluation component forces professionals to focus on the question of " W h a t should be measured and why?" This measurement question can range from evaluating patients' quality o f life to gauging
social
validity
as
an
their satisfaction with clinical services. Without
assessment component has historically been
outcome
neglected, as is apparent by the low percentage
determine the impact o f the intervention, nor
of published material that assesses for social
can they optimize care (Toscani 8c Pizzi, 2 0 0 1 ) .
validity (Carr, Austin, Britton, Kellum, 8c Bailey, 1 9 9 9 ) . Neglect in the assessment for
measurements,
clinicians cannot
In spite o f noted methodological weaknesses
and
challenges
(e.g., el-Guebaly, 2002;
social validity might be due to the "subjectiv-
Hodgins, Armstrong, 8c Addington,
ity" involved. Social validity is considered to be
Ellingstad, Sobell, Sobell, 8c Planthara, 2 0 0 2 ;
subjective in the sense that clinicians are asking
Follette, 1 9 9 5 ) , professionals can incorporate
533
534
SPECIAL ISSUES evaluation
ranging from individual or group therapy to
component that would enhance the quality o f
case management, can be monitored for their
services offered. Program evaluation is a rela-
efficiency and effectiveness. W h a t may be
into their practice a program
tively easy process, with the numerous benefits
more challenging for clinicians is choosing
including maintaining high ethical and practice
treatments that
standards, monitoring program efficiency and
and that specify the mechanisms o f behavior
effectiveness, and providing important finan-
change. Theoretically, the chosen interven-
cial information (Cone, 2 0 0 0 ) . Although pro-
tion should include a strong rationale for why
are conceptually coherent
gram evaluation seems like a daunting task for
specific treatment components are included
clinicians, it is hoped that the recommenda-
and what they are intended to change.
tions offered in this chapter make clinicians
Continuous evaluation o f one's practice
appreciate the benefits of conducting program
should be as much part of a professional's
evaluations whether in private practice or in a
repertoire as is being ethically responsible.
medical center. T h e data gathered should lead
Program evaluation data should also lead to
clinicians to ask further questions regarding
the dissemination o f more effective standards
what they practice and the way in which they
of practice. In turn, better developed stan-
practice. T h e data obtained from
program
dards o f practice can lead to more effective
evaluations can lead to future needs assess-
ways in which clinicians actually practice
ments and the development, refinement, and
(Dawes, 1 9 9 5 ) . As managed care continues to
implementation o f practice standards.
The
change the landscape and manner in which
results from a well-planned program evalua-
clinicians practice, it is becoming increasingly
tion can place a practitioner or an agency at a
important to demonstrate the effectiveness o f
competitive advantage when asked to justify
their interventions. As the commitment to the
costs, resources, and growth of services. Program evaluation
is
flexible
enough
in its methodology that different services,
systematic evaluation o f programs continues to grow, treatment efficiency and effectiveness will also continue to improve.
REFERENCES Bakeman, R., & Gottman, J . M . (1997). Observing interactions: An introduction to sequential analysis. London: Cambridge University Press. Biskupiak, J . E. (2001). Selecting variables and constructs to measure. In R. Patterson (Ed.), Changing patient behavior: Improving outcomes in health and disease management (pp. 2 0 1 - 2 2 2 ) . San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Brown, R. E., & Reed, C. S. (2002). An integral approach to evaluating outcome evaluation training. American Journal of Evaluation, 23, 1-17. Carr, J . E., Austin, J . L., Britton, L. N., Kellum, Κ. K., & Bailey, J . S. (1999). An assessment of social validity trends in applied behavior analysis. Behavioral Interventions, 14, 2 2 3 - 2 3 1 . Cohen, J . (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Cone, J . D. (2000). Evaluating outcomes: Empirical tools for effective practice. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Dawes, R. M . (1995). Standards of practice. In S. C. Hayes, V. M. Follette, R. M. Dawes, & Κ. E. Grady (Eds.), Scientific standards of psychological practice (pp. 3 1 - 4 3 ) . Reno, NV: Context Press.
Evaluating el-Guebaly, N., Hodgins, D. C , Armstrong, S., & Addington, J . (2002). Methodological and clinical challenges in evaluating treatment outcome of substance-related disorders and comorbidity. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 44, 2 6 4 - 2 7 0 . Ellingstad, T. P., Sobell, L. C , Sobell, M . B . , & Planthara, P. (2002). Drug treatment outcome methodology ( 1 9 9 3 - 1 9 9 7 ) : Strengths, weaknesses, and a comparison to the alcohol field. Addictive Behaviors, 27, 3 1 9 - 3 3 0 . Fink, A. (1993). Evaluation fundamentals: Guiding health programs, research, and policy. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Follette, W. C. (1995). Correcting methodological weaknesses in the knowledge base used to derive practice standards. In S. C. Hayes, V. M . Follette, R. M . Dawes, &C Κ. E. Grady (Eds.), Scientific standards of psychological practice (pp. 2 2 9 - 2 4 7 ) . Reno, NV: Context Press. Follette, W. C , &C Callaghan, G. M . (2001). The evolution of clinical significance. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 8, 4 3 1 - 4 3 5 . Follette, W. C. & Compton, S. (1999). Correcting methodological weaknesses in traditional psychotherapy outcome research. Unpublished manuscript, University of Nevada, Reno. Follette, W. C , Naugle, A. E., & Linnerooth, P. J . (2000). Functional alternatives to traditional assessment and diagnosis. In M. J . Dougher (Ed.), Clinical behavior analysis (pp. 9 9 - 1 2 5 ) . Reno, NV: Context Press. Franklin, R. D., Allison, D., & Gorman, B . S. (1996). Design and analysis of singlecase research. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Hansen, W. B . (2002). Program evaluation strategies for substance abuse prevention. Journal of Primary Prevention, 22, 4 0 9 - 4 3 6 . Hawkins, R. P. (1986). Selection of target behaviors. In R. O. Nelson 8c S. C. Hayes (Eds.), Conceptual foundations of behavioral assessment (pp. 3 3 1 - 3 8 6 ) . New York: Guilford. Hawkins, R. P., & Mathews, J . R. (1999). Frequent monitoring of clinical outcomes: Research and accountability for clinical practice. Education & Treatment of Children, 22, 1 1 7 - 1 3 5 . Hawkins, R. P., Mathews, J . R., δί Hamdan, L. (1999). Measuring behavioral health outcome: A practical guide. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer Academic. Hayes, S. C , Nelson, R. O., & Jarrett, R. B . (1987). The treatment utility of assessment: A functional approach to evaluating assessment quality. American Psychologist, 42, 9 6 3 - 9 7 4 . Haynes, S. N. (1992). Models of causality in psychopathology: Toward synthetic, dynamic, and nonlinear models of causality in psychopathology. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Haynes, S. N., Leisen, M . B . , & Blaine, D. D. (1997). Design of individualized behavioral treatment programs using functional analytic clinical case models. Psychological Assessment, 9, 3 3 4 - 3 4 8 . Jacobson, N. S., Follette, W. C , & Revenstorf, D. (1984). Psychotherapy outcome research: Methods for reporting variability and evaluating clinical significance. Behavior Therapy, 15, 3 3 6 - 3 5 2 . Jacobson, N. S., & Truax, P. (1991). Clinical significance: A statistical approach to defining meaningful change in psychotherapy research. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 1 2 - 1 9 . Kazdin, A. E. (1992). Research design in clinical psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Macmillan. Kelley, S. D., Van Horn, M., &c De Maso, D. R. (2001). Using process evaluation to describe a hospital-based clinic for children coping with medical stressors. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 26, 4 0 7 - 4 1 5 .
Outcomes
536
SPECIAL ISSUES Kendall, P. C , Marrs-Garcia, Α., Nath, S. R., Sc Sheldrick, R. C. (1999). Normative comparisons for the evaluation of clinical significance, journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67, 2 8 5 - 2 9 9 . Paul, G. L., & Menditto, A. A. (1992). Effectiveness of inpatient treatment programs for mentally ill adults in public psychiatric facilities. Applied & Preventive Psychology, 1, 4 1 - 6 3 . Posavac, E. J . , Sc Carey, R. G. (1997). Program evaluation: Methods and case studies (5th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. Relman, A. S. (1988). Assessment and accountability: The third revolution in medical care. New England Journal of Medicine, 319, 1 2 2 0 - 1 2 2 2 . Rogers, L. K., Howard, Κ. I., Sc Vessey, J . T. (1993). Using significance tests to evaluate equivalence between two experimental groups. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 5 5 3 - 5 6 5 . Rosenthal, R., Se DiMatteo, M . R. (2001). Meta-analysis: Recent developments in quantitative methods for literature reviews. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 59-82. Rossi, P. H., Sc Freeman, Η. E. (1993). Evaluation: A systematic approach. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Silva, F. (1993a). Psychometric foundations and behavioral assessment. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Silva, F. ( 1 9 9 3 b ) . Treatment utility: A reappraisal. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 9, 2 2 2 - 2 2 6 . Sobell, L. C , Sc Sobell, M . B. (1996). Alcohol timeline follow-back (TLFB) users' manual. Toronto: Addiction Research Foundation. Toscani, M . R., Se Pizzi, L. T. (2001). Measuring and improving the intervention. outcomes in In R. Patterson (Ed.), Changing patient behavior: Improving health and disease management (pp. 1 7 7 - 2 0 0 ) . San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Waltz, J . , Addis, M . E., Koerner, K., Sc Jacobson, N. S. (1993). Testing the integrity of a psychotherapy protocol: Assessment of adherence and competence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 6 2 0 - 6 3 0 . Wolf, M. (1978). Social validity: The case for subjective measurement or how applied behavior analysis is finding its heart. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 11, 2 0 3 - 2 1 4 . Yates, Β. T. (1996). Analyzing costs, procedures, processes, and outcomes in human services. London: Sage.
Name
Index
Aaronson, Ν. K., 3 3 2 , 3 4 2 Aarsland, D . , 4 8 7 Abarbanel, Α., 366 Abeloff, M . , 3 3 4 Abrams, D. B., 6 1 , 6 2 , 110 Abueg, F. R., 5 2 0 , 521 Abuzzahab, F., 3 6 9 Acerbi, Α., 21 Achmon, J., 2 6 2 Ackerman, B. J., 2 8 9 Ackerson, L . , 4 6 1 Adams-Campbell, L. L . , 3 1 0 A d a m s , D. P., 114 Adams, Η. E., 367 A d a m s , P. F., 3 9 7 Addington, J., 533 Addis, M . E . , 5 3 0 Addy, C . L . , 128 Adkins, D., 106 Adlercreutz, H . , 2 5 5 Administration on Aging, 4 8 5 , 4 9 0 Adunsky, Α., 2 1 Afari, N . , 4 4 Affleck, G., 2 3 2 Aghabeigi, B., 2 3 4 Agras, W. S., 2 6 2 Agurs-Collins, T. D . , 3 1 0 Ahern, D. K., 2 4 3 Ahlbom, Α., 4 7 3 Ahlgren, P., 4 8 9 Ahluwalia, J. S., 2 6 5 Ahmedzai, S., 3 3 2 Ahonen, Α., 87 Aikens, J., 24 Ainsworth, Β. E . , 128 Ajzen, I., 2 1 1 , 388 al'Absi, M . , 113, 2 5 3 , 2 5 4 , 2 5 5 Albanes, D., 156 Albarede, A. L . , 4 9 2 Albert, M . , 4 5 9 Alberti, R. E . , 2 9 3 Albini, F., 2 5 7 Alerman, M . H . , 2 6 5 Alexander, C. J . , 3 6 0
Alexander, F., 253 Alexander, M . J., 4 7 0 Algor, R., 3 2 7 , 3 3 2 , 3 3 7 Allard, P., 4 8 9 Allen, J . P., 199 Allen, M . T., 253 Allen, R . , 2 8 0 Allen, T. W., 2 6 2 Allison, D . , 5 2 9 Allison, D. B., 126, 129, 1 3 1 , 1 3 5 , 1 3 7 Allison, T. G., 2 8 2 , 2 9 2 Almy, T. P., 4 0 2 Aionso, C , 4 7 7 Alpers, J. H . , 4 8 8 Alpert, Μ . Α., 258 Alpert, S. G., 3 0 6 Alrakawi, Α., 308 Altabe, M . N., 1 2 7 Alter, C . L . , 341 Alterman, A. I., 86 Althof, S. E . , 369 Alto, W. Α., 2 1 3 , 438 Altwein, J . E . , 368 American Association of Cardiovascular and Pulmonary Rehabilitation, 150 American Cancer Society, 4 5 7 American College of Sports Medicine, 147 American Diabetes Association (ADA), 3 0 4 , 310, 311, 312 American Medical Association (ΑΜΑ), 1 1 , 189, 197, 2 1 5 , 4 4 7 American Psychiatric Association, 5, 6, 2 3 , 80, 1 0 2 , 126, 176, 2 3 3 , 2 3 4 , 2 3 5 , 2 8 7 , 3 4 3 , 359, 362, 365, 366, 367, 369, 386, 387, 388, 392, 393, 395, 4 1 1 , 426, 487, 492 American Psychological Association (ΑΡΑ), 4 4 3 , 444, 445, 447, 449, 4 5 1 , 453 A m m e r m a n , A. S., 2 1 6 Amodei, N., 4 7 7 Amundson, G. M . , 2 3 7 Andersen, B. L . , 61 Andersen, R., 161 Andersen, R. E . , 133 Anderson, B. J., 5 9 , 6 7
537
538
T H E HEALTH P S Y C H O L O G Y HANDBOOK Anderson, D. Α., 127 Anderson, J . P., 508 Anderson, J . R., 66 Anderson, J . S., 2 3 7 Anderson, Ν. B., 2 6 2 Anderson, R. J . , 3 0 3 , 3 0 7 , 308 Anderson, R. M . , 66, 3 1 3 , 3 1 5 Andreasen, N . C , 3 4 0 Andreski, P., 229 Andrews, G., 34, 4 0 0 Andrews, K., 4 9 1 Andrulonis, P. Α., 3 9 9 Andrykowski, Μ . Α., 328 Angold, Α., 4 7 5 Angst, J . , 4 7 2 Anthenelli, R. M . , 83 Antoni, M . H . , 64 Antonuccio, D. O., 5 1 9 , 5 2 2 Appel, L. J . , 2 5 8 , 2 5 9 , 2 6 1 Appelbaum, M . , 152, 2 6 1 Apt, C , 3 6 5 Arber, S., 4 7 4 Arfken, C. L., 4 9 1 Arkowitz, H . , 4 5 Armstrong, S., 533 Arnett, D. K., 2 5 4 Arnold, S. E . , 1 7 7 Arrol, B., 2 6 1 Ascanio, R., 4 6 0 Ashford, R. U., 516 Ashley, M . J . , 2 6 0 Asmundson, G. J . G., 2 3 9 Atan, Α., 368 Atkinson, J . , 3 9 2 Attanasio, V., 243 Audet, J . , 309 Augustini, K. F., 3 3 1 Austin, J . L . , 533 Australian Department of Health and Aged Care, 28 Australian Psychological Society, 31 Aversa, Α., 368 Avina, C , 360 Avis, Ν. E . , 4 7 1 Axelrod, Β. N . , 2 2 Ayers, S., 3 0 5 Babyak, M . , 158 Bacak, S. J . , 162 B a c k , A. L . , 3 2 9 Badcock, C. Α., 4 0 5 Baez-Franceshi, D . , 4 9 2 Bailey, D., 29 Bailey, J . S., 533 Bakeman, R., 5 3 0 Baker, D., 473 Baker, M . D., 4 8 7 Baker, R. W., 343 Baker, S., 90 Baker, T. B., 103, 106, 107, 110
Baldwin, R. C , 488 Balfour, D . J . , 1 0 2 Ballinger, C , 4 7 2 Balon, R., 2 9 Baiter, M . B., 4 2 5 Bancroft, J . , 359, 366 Bandura, Α., 88 Bandura, A. J . , 2 1 0 , 2 1 1 , 406 Banerjee, K . , 4 6 1 Banikarim, C , 4 7 7 Banks, S. M . , 2 4 0 Bar, J . L . , 80 Barach, P., 32 Baranowski, T., 44 Barbaree, H . E . , 2 3 1 , 2 3 9 , 243 Barbarotto, R., 2 1 Barclay, G., 104 Barlow, D. H . , 3 6 0 , 364, 3 6 9 , 4 0 5 , 4 1 1 , 5 1 4 , 518 Barnes, D., 2 9 Barnes, P. M . , 147 Barnett, M . C , 2 8 2 Barnett, P. Α., 2 6 2 Barnett, R. C , 473 Barnhill, R. L . , 3 3 0 Barnoya, J . , 126 Baron-Faust, R., 4 2 9 Barr, H . , 3 7 Barraclough, J . , 334 Barreioro, Μ . Α., 4 0 6 Barrett, C . L . , 2 4 0 Barrios, B., 2 8 5 Barry, R. E . , 4 1 4 Barsky, Α., 51 Barsky, A. J . , 2 8 2 Bartlik, B., 3 6 0 Baruch, G. K., 4 7 3 Basar, M . , 368 Bassett, D. R., 148 Bates, M . S., 4 5 7 Batey, D. M . , 2 6 2 Baum, Α., 3 4 4 B a u m , F. E . , 4 2 6 Baxter, J . , 3 1 0 Bayer, R., 5 1 5 Bazzacchi, G., 4 0 1 Beaglehole, R., 2 6 1 Beaulieu, C . L . , 2 3 9 Beck, A. T., 2 3 , 64, 90, 177, 1 8 1 , 2 3 1 , 4 1 1 Beck, J . G., 3 6 9 Beck, J . S., 2 4 3 Becker, B. ML, 111 Becker, D. M . , 4 6 0 Becker, M . H . , 2 1 2 , 3 3 0 Beckham, J . C , 108, 2 3 9 Beebe, G. W., 3 3 2 Beena, M . B., 24 Beevers, D. G., 2 6 0 Begg, C , 3 3 0 Begleiter, H . , 86
Begura, S., 86 Beitman, B. D., 2 3 4 Bélanger, Α., 309 Belar, C . D., 4, 5, 6, 8, 9, 10, 12, 3 0 , 3 2 , 3 3 , 36, 6 7 , 6 8 , 4 4 4 , 4 4 5 , 4 4 9 , 4 5 4 Belcher, M . , 103 Bell, Α., 44 Bell, R., 368 Belle, S., 4 9 0 Belle, S. H . , 3 3 5 , 4 8 9 Bellg, A. J . , 6, 7 Belluzzi, J . D., 104 Belsky, J . , 4 7 0 Benazon, N . , 4 7 8 Benedict, R. H . B., 2 0 Benhamou, Y., 4 1 2 Benkert, O., 368 Benner, P., 4 9 5 Bennet, E. J . , 4 0 5 Benotsch, E . , 388 Benowitz, N . L . , 1 0 2 , 103 Benson, D. F., 4 9 2 Benson, V., 3 9 7 Berber, Α., 136 Berg, R. Α., 19, 2 0 Berghammer, R., 4 7 7 Bergman, B., 3 3 2 Berkman, L . , 4 5 9 Berkowitz, R. I., 129, 258 Berlin, J . Α., 150 Berlowitz, D. R., 2 6 4 Bernbaum, M . , 306 Bernstein, D. Α., 179 Bernstein, I. H . , 2 1 9 Bernstein, M . , 149 Bernstein, S. L . , I l l Berridge, K. C , 8 5 , 103, 104, 1 0 5 , 107 Berry, M . J . , 153 Bertelson, A. D., 4 3 2 Bertrand, C , 2 3 4 Berube-Parent, S., 136 Berwick, M . , 330 Best, C . L . , 4 7 9 Beutel, M . , 336 Beutler, L . , 5 Bhandari, S., 24 Bhugra, D., 3 6 0 Bidzan, L . , 2 1 Bidzan, M . , 2 1 Biener, L . , 110 Biering-Sorensen, F., 368 Billig, N., 4 9 2 Binik, Y. M . , 366 Birch, L . , 129 Birge, S. J . , 4 9 1 Birk, L . , 361 Bishop, S., 2 3 2 Bishop, S. R., 2 3 9 Biskupiak, J . E . , 528 Biswas, Α., 3 3 6
Bitschnau, M . , 4 7 1 Bixler, E . O., 172 Black, D. R., 63 Black, G. W., 2 6 2 , 5 0 4 Black, J . L . , 2 8 2 , 2 9 2 Blackwell, B., 4 0 6 Blaine, D. D., 528 Blair, S., 147 Blair, S. M . , 156 Blair, S. N . , 1 6 1 , 1 6 2 Biais, L . M . , 3 3 0 Blake, F., 4 7 6 Blake, J . , 2 5 3 Blalock, A. C . , 3 9 1 , 3 9 2 , 393 Blanchard, Ε. B., 4 0 4 , 4 0 5 , 4 0 9 , 4 1 1 , 4 1 4 Blank, K., 23 Blazer, D., 4 9 3 Blewett, Α., 4 0 5 Bliss, J . M . , 3 3 2 Bliwise, D., 4 9 1 Block, A. R., 2 4 0 , 2 4 1 Block, S., 336 Bloom, F. R., 389 Bloom, J . D., 192 Bloom, J . R., 6 5 , 6 7 , 336 Blum, K., 84, 85 Blume, A. W., 4 4 Blumenthal, J . Α., 6 1 , 6 2 , 1 5 2 , 158, 2 6 1 , 281, 286, 292 Blyth, F. M . , 2 2 9 Bock, B., 14 Bock, B. C . , 154 Boer, K . , 4 7 1 Bogart, L . , 388 Bogdale, N . , 2 2 9 Boland, F. J . , 2 5 8 Bolland, J . M . , 177 Bollerud, K . , 4 7 4 Bonadonna, R. C . , 3 0 4 Bonato, D. P., 258 Bond, M . J . , 2 4 , 368 Bonke, B., 343 Bono, J . E . , 32 Booth-Kewley, S., 2 8 8 Boothby, J . L . , 2 3 0 , 2 3 1 , 2 3 2 , 2 3 9 Boraz, M . Α., 2 5 8 Borg, G., 343 Borgioni, C . , 2 5 7 Borkovec, T. D., 179 Bornstein, G., 3 3 4 Borrelli, B., 103, 106 Boscaro, M . , 2 5 4 Boselli, M . , 4 9 1 Bosley, F., 2 6 2 Bosmajian, L . , 4 0 5 Botelho, R. J . , 213 Bottomley, Α., 66, 6 7 Bouchard, C . , 129, 154 Boucher, J . L . , 5 0 2 Boulard, Ν. E . , 4 7 6
540
T H E HEALTH P S Y C H O L O G Y HANDBOOK Boulware, L. E . , 2 6 6 Bourassa, M . G., 504 Bourgault, C , 24 Bourgeois, M . S., 4 9 2 Bourjeily, G., 153 Bovbjerg, D., 3 3 9 , 340 Bovbjerg, D. H . , 83 Bowden, J . J . , 4 8 8 B o w m a n , Β. Α., 126, 127 Boxer, A. W., 4 7 8 Boyce, P., 4 0 0 Boyd-Wickizer, J . , 24 Boyle, J . P., 303 Boyle, R. G., 502 Brackett, J . , 59 Bradbeer, M . , 4 9 3 Bradley, Κ. Α., 24 Bradley, W. E . , 3 6 9 Bradshaw, C , 3 3 2 Braith, R. W., 2 6 1 Brand, M . B., 2 5 9 Brandon, T. H . , 1 0 1 , 110 Brandt, J . , 2 0 Brandt, Κ. M . , 86 Brannick, M . , 88 Braverman, B., 2 4 0 Bray, G. Α., 136 Bray, J . H . , 34, 2 4 4 Breaux, C , 458 Brecher, Ε. M . , 82 Breitbart, W., 3 4 1 , 343 Brennan, A. F., 2 4 0 Brennan, P. Α., 87 Brent, D., 306 Breslau, N . , 106, 2 2 9 , 2 3 5 Brewer, G., 138 Brigham, J . , 112 Brighton, L. Α., 156 Britt, D. M . , 113 Brittain, E . , 2 5 2 Britton, J . Α., 156 Britton, L. N., 533 Brocker, P., 4 9 1 Broekhuijsen, Μ . Α., 334 Brooks-Gunn, J . , 4 7 0 Brown, G. K . , 177, 2 3 9 Brown, G. W., 173, 177 Brown, J . , 5 2 1 Brown, J . M . , 2 3 1 Brown, L . , 2 2 Brown, L. L . , 331 Brown, M . D., 152, 2 6 1 Brown, R. E . , 5 2 5 Brown, S. Α., 105 Brown, S. E . , 4 7 8 Brown, Τ. Α., 360 Brownell, K. D., 62, 128, 137, 153, 2 1 0 Brownson, R. C . , 162 Bruce, D. G., 3 1 5 Bruehl, S., 2 3 7
Brunner, E . , 2 5 9 Brunner, R., 4 7 9 Bryant, G. D., 4 8 8 Brymer, J . , 2 9 0 Buchanan, R. G., 199 Buchanan, T., 254 Buchsbaum, D. G., 199 Buckloh, L. M . , 304, 313 Budd, Μ . Α., 34 Buddeberg, C , 3 3 1 Budzynski, T. H . , 179 Buffington, V. E . , 80 Bukofzer, S., 368 Bulik, C . M . , 127 Bullinger, M . , 332 Bunag, R. D., 2 5 5 Burchardt, M . , 368 Burg, Μ . Α., 3 3 0 Burger, H., 360 Burgio, L . , 4 9 2 Buring, J . E . , 2 8 0 , 504 Burish, T. G., 334 Burke, B. L . , 4 5 Burke, G., 3 9 9 Burke, L. E . , 2 1 0 , 294 Burkovec, T . D., 2 3 1 Burleson, M . H . , 59 Burls, Α., 368 Burman, B., 3 3 5 Burman, D., 4 9 3 Burman, M . L . , 24 Burmeister, L. Α., 4 8 9 Burnett, C. K., 398 Burns, B. J . , 458 Burns, D. D . , 5 2 2 Burns, D. M . , 104 Burns, S., 62 Burrows, G., 33 Burrows, L . , 59 Burstein, R., 157 Burt, V. L . , 253 Busby, Κ. Α., 4 9 1 Butler, C . , 44 Butler, R. W., 2 3 2 , 2 3 9 Butler, W. M . , 3 6 0 Buyesse, D. J . , 4 2 6 Byrd-Holt, D. D., 310 Byrne, T., 189 Cadoret, R. J . , 84 Calarco, M . M . , 30 Callaghan, G. M . , 5 3 1 Callahan, L. F., 156 Cameron, O. G., 158 Camic, P. M . , 7 Camilleri, M . , 398 Campbell, Ε. H . , 3 6 7 Campbell, N . R., 2 6 0 Campbell, P. C , 326, 341 Campbell, T. L . , 34, 3 5 , 3 7
Name Index C a m p i a , U., 2 5 7 C a n a d a , A. L . , 3 3 1 Cannon, W., 172 Cannuscio, C . C , 156 Capatani, E . , 21 Capozolli, K., 343 Cardillo, C , 2 5 7 Carey, M . P., 4 4 , 2 1 3 , 3 5 9 , 3 6 0 , 3 6 2 , 3 6 5 , 369 Carey, R. G., 5 2 5 Carlson, C. R., 179, 183 Carlson, L . , 3 5 Carlson, N . R., 172 Carlson, R. W., 3 3 6 C a r m o d y , T. P., 103, 106 Carnahan, J . E . , 2 1 0 Carney, R., 4 Carney, R. M . , 2 3 4 , 2 8 8 , 2 9 2 Carnike, C . L. M . , J r . , 24 Carpenter, C , 388 Carpenter, J . , 29 Carpenter, Κ. M . , 126 Carr, D. B., 4 0 3 Carr, J . E . , 533 Carrier, S., 368 Carrington, P., 180 Carrion, H . M . , 368 Carroll, B. T., 3 4 1 Carruthers, S. G., 2 6 0 Carter, J . S., 4 5 7 Carter, L. C , 113 Carter-Pokras, O., 161 Carver, C . S., 331 Casari, E. F., 5 1 6 Casebeer, L . , 2 1 3 Casper, R. F., 478 Caspersen, C . J . , 147, 148 Caspi, Α., 4 7 0 Cassem Ν . H . , 2 8 8 Cassidy, T., 173 Cassileth, B. R., 3 3 1 , 3 3 5 Cates, D. S., 5 0 4 Cattarin, J . , 127 Catz, S. L . , 3 8 8 , 3 8 9 , 3 9 0 Caudill, Μ . Α., 2 3 7 Cavallini, M . C , 2 5 7 Cella, D. F., 3 4 2 Center for the Advancement of Health, 2 1 5 Centers for Disease Control and Prevention ( C D C ) , 56, 1 0 1 , 104, 107, 1 2 5 , 126, 146, 150, 3 8 3 , 384, 3 8 7 , 388 Centor, R., 213 Centor, R. M . , 199 Cerri, M . , 2 1 Cervero, F., 4 0 2 , 4 0 3 Cesana, G., 151 Chacko, M . R., 4 7 7 C h a c k o , R. C , 21 Chaitchik, S., 3 2 6 , 3 2 7 , 3 2 8 , 3 3 0 , 3 3 1 , 3 3 2 , 334, 3 3 7 Chakour, M . C , 4 9 3
Chamberlain, K., 32 Chamberlin, C. L . , 106 Chambers, L . , 64, 1 5 1 , 2 6 2 , 2 9 2 Chambless, D. L . , 34 Chambless, L . E . , 2 6 0 Champion, D., 2 4 2 Chan, J . K . , 303 C h a p m a n , C. R., 2 4 0 , 3 2 9 Charles, Μ . Α., 129 Charleston, J . , 2 5 9 Charuzi, I., 157 Chaturvedi, S. K . , 24 Chaves, J . F., 2 3 1 Chen, M . S., 4 6 0 Cheng, Y., 155, 161 Cherny, Ν. I., 329 Cheskin, L. J . , 131 Chesney, M . , 2 1 5 Chesney, Μ . Α., 2 6 2 , 5 0 4 Chesson, A. L . , 4 3 6 Cheung, F. K., 4 5 8 Chevron, E. S., 65 Chisholm, D. J . , 3 1 5 Chochinov, Η. M . , 3 2 9 Choi, Η. K . , 3 6 8 , 3 7 5 Chorlian, D . B., 86 Chou, F. F., 368 Chrisman, N . J . , 2 1 5 Christensen, A. J . , 2 3 9 , 2 6 6 Christiansen, C. L . , 2 8 2 Christie-Seely, J . , 7 Christos, P. J . , 156 Chrousos, G. P., 4 0 8 Chudzik, G., 7 Ciliska, D., 137 Cinciripini, P., 3 2 9 , 3 3 0 Ciucci, Α., 2 5 7 C l a m o n, G. H . , 3 4 1 Clancy, C. M . , 5 0 4 Clark, Α., 3 2 7 Clark, D . B., 4 7 0 Clark, J . D., 2 2 9 Clark, M . , 14, 258 Clark, M . S., 2 4 Clark, W., 368 Clarke, C , 29 Clarke, P. B., 104 Clarridge, B. R., 329 Clauw, D . J . , 4 0 8 Cleary, P. D., 2 8 2 Cleeland, C . S., 343 Clement, S., 313 Clements, K. L . , 2 3 2 Clementy, J . , 2 5 7 Clenney, T . L . , 368 Clerici, M . , 2 6 2 Cleroux, J . , 2 6 1 Clifford, P. Α., 2 6 6 Clinch, J . J . , 329 Cloninger, C . R., 79, 87, 108
541
542
T H E HEALTH P S Y C H O L O G Y HANDBOOK Clouse, R. E . , 64, 3 0 3 , 3 0 8 , 3 1 6 , 4 0 4 , 4 8 8 Coats, Α., 2 5 6 Cochrane, C , 3 0 7 Coderre, T. J . , 4 0 3 Cody, Η . Α., 4 9 4 Cody, M . , 3 3 1 C o e , C. L . , 4 7 7 C o g g o n , D . , 155 Cohen, D . , 3 6 6 Cohen, H . , 2 6 5 Cohen, J . , 5 3 1 Cohen, L . , 344 Cohen, L. M . , 103, 105, 113 Cohen, L . S., 2 1 0 , 4 7 1 Cohen-Mansfield, J . , 4 9 2 Cohen, P. T., 3 8 4 , 3 8 7 Cohen, S., 5 9 , 6 2 , 64, 66, 176, 3 4 5 , 4 0 5 Col, N., 4 9 0 Colditz, G . Α., 126, 150, 156 Cole, J . , 3 6 9 Coleman, H . , 51 Colletti, G., 110, 5 2 0 Collins, F. L . , 102, 103, 179, 183 Collins, F. L . , J r . , 109, 113 Collins, F. S., 15 Collins, R., 152 Collins, S. M . , 4 0 2 C o m p a s , Β. Ε., 62, 66 C o m p t o n , S., 5 2 9 , 5 3 0 Comstock, G. W., 108, 3 3 2 Comuzzie, A. G., 129 C o n , Α. Η., 2 6 2 Cone, J . D., 5 3 4 Conger, J . J . , 83 C o n l e y . J . J . , 86 Connell, C . M . , 3 0 9 , 4 6 0 Connelly, J . E . , 9 Connolly, M . J . , 4 8 8 Conry, M . , 4 9 0 Conti, J . B., 2 8 2 Contrada, R. J . , 66, 4 5 9 C o o k , E . W., 3 9 8 C o o k , G., 2 9 , 30 C o o k , N . R., 2 6 0 C o o k , U., 4 0 2 C o o k e , D. J . , 4 7 1 C o o k e , R. D., 4 2 6 Cooper, A. R., 261 Cooper, C , 155 Cooper, H . , 4 7 5 Cooper, J . R., 193 Cooper, R., 4 5 7 Cooper, S. E . , 3 6 9 Cooper, Z . , 138 Coovert, M . 127 Copeland, A. L . , 110 Coppola, K., 495 Corazziari, E . , 4 0 0 , 4 0 9 Corbett, S., 3 0 5 C o r d o v a , M . J . , 341 (
Corr, C , 4 9 5 Corr, D . , 4 9 5 Corrigan, S. Α., 2 5 9 Cortinas, L., 136 Coscarelli, Α., 3 3 2 Coscarelli Schag, C. Α., 343 C o s t a , P. T., 3 3 5 , 4 0 5 Council on Scientific Affairs, 197, 198 Courtney, J . G., 3 3 2 Covey, L. S., 114 Cowie, C . C , 3 1 0 C o x , D. J . , 60, 2 9 2 , 316 C o x , T., 32 Coyne, J . C , 59, 6 2 , 64, 177 Craig, T., 4 0 5 Cramer, J . Α., 2 1 7 Cranley, M . S., 4 7 0 Craven, J . L . , 3 0 9 Crawford, S. L . , 4 7 1 Creed, F., 4 0 5 Crespo, C . J . , 161 Crettenden, I., 4 2 6 Crits-Christoph, P., 3 1 6 , 318 Croft, P. R., 3 6 8 , 3 9 7 C r o g h a n , T. W., 2 1 5 Crombez, G., 2 3 2 Crombie, I. K., 3 9 7 Crosby, J . R., 36 Crouch, M . , 7 Cryer, P. E . , 4 8 8 C u b a , S. L . , 4 5 7 Cull, Α., 3 3 2 Cummings, J . L . , 4 8 7 , 4 9 2 Cummings, Ν. Α., 5 0 5 , 514 Cunningham, A. C , 2 3 9 Cunningham, J . Α., 3 4 6 Currie, L . , 4 8 7 Currie, S. R., 44 Curry, S. J . , 4 4 , 6 1 , 5 0 2 Cushman, W. C , 2 6 0 , 2 6 1 Cutler, J . , 2 5 2 Cutler, R. B., 2 4 4 Cycowicz, Y. M . , 87 Czechowicz, D. J . , 193 Dahlgren, L. Α., 108 Dahlof, P., 2 9 0 Dalton, C , 3 9 9 Daltroy, L. H . , 2 1 4 Dam, H., 489 Damarin, F. L . , 2 3 9 D'Angelo, T., 3 3 5 Dani, J . Α., 105 Daniels, E. R., 329 Danton, W. G., 5 2 2 Darke, L. L . , 4 7 7 Darwin, C . R., 88 Daut, R. L . , 343 Davey, S. G., 2 5 9 Davida, F. K., 308
Name Index Davidoff, F., 5 2 2 Davidson, J . , 3 3 2 Davidson, K. W., 2 8 2 Davidson, M . B., 2 1 6 Davidson, M . H . , 136 Davies, A. O., 2 5 4 Davis, D. Α., 2 1 0 Davis, G. C . , 2 2 9 Davis, M . H . , 36 Davis, M . S., 2 1 5 Davis, P. M . , 3 9 9 Davis, R. C , 361 Davis, S., 360 Davis, V. E . , 85 Davis, W. K . , 3 0 9 Dawes, R. M . , 9 0 , 5 3 4 D a w s o n , Α. Α., 334 Day, Α., 4 7 6 D e Felice, F., 2 5 7 de Groot, M . , 3 1 0 de H a e s , J . , 3 3 2 de Haes, J . C . J . M . , 343 de Jongste, M . J . L . , 2 4 2 de Leon, J . D., 108 De Leon, P. H . , 5 0 7 De M a s o , D. R., 5 2 5 De Yonge, J . , 32 Dean, C . , 3 3 5 Deardorff, W. W., 4, 5, 8, 9, 10, 3 0 , 6 7 , 444, 445, 449, 454 DeBeurs, E . , 4 9 3 DeBusk, R. F., 2 1 0 , 2 1 4 , 2 1 6 Deeg, D. J . H . , 4 8 7 DeFlorio, M . , 340 DeFronzo, R. Α., 3 0 4 D e G o o d , D. E . , 2 3 8 , 2 3 9 DeKosky, S. T., 4 8 9 Delgado, P., 360 Dellapietra, L . , 23 Dement, W. C . , 4 2 5 , 4 3 8 Demmers, R. Y., 3 0 7 Demming, B., 91 D e M u t h , N . M . , 61 Dengel, D. R., 2 6 1 Dennerstein, L . , 3 6 0 , 4 7 1 Denney, M . W., 3 6 4 Denton, M . , 4 7 4 Derogatis, J . R., 3 4 2 Derogatis, L. R., 2 3 , 177, 3 3 4 , 3 4 0 , 4 1 1 D e r o o , L . , 4 2 , 62 Derry, P. S., 4 7 1 DeRubeis, R. J . , 3 1 6 , 318 DeSoto, C . B., 24 DeSoto, J . L . , 24 Devereux, R. B., 2 5 3 Devins, G. M . , 3 0 9 DeVries, H . , 6 2 Dew, M . Α., 3 9 2 Dey, J . , 368 Di Chiara, G., 104
Diabetes Prevention Program Research G r o u p (DPPRG), 3 0 5 , 311 D i a m o n d , S., 59, 3 4 5 Diana, M . , 105 DiCesare, J . , 4 1 1 DiCicco, L . , 43 Dickel, M . J . , 428 Dickey, F., 7 DiClemente, C . C , 4 3 , 92, 110, 2 1 1 , 2 1 2 Dieppe, P., 155 Dietz, W., 126 Dietz, W. H., 127, 129 Dill, P. L . , 136 D i ! l e y , J . W., 3 9 2 D i M a t t e o , M . R., 2 1 5 , 2 1 6 , 2 1 7 , 4 7 0 , 5 3 1 Dimeff, L. Α., 5 0 4 D i N a r d o , P. Α., 4 1 1 Ding, Y., 2 6 2 DiNicola, D. D., 2 1 6 , 2 1 7 DiPlacido, J . , 3 3 0 D i x o n , H . Α., 4 9 0 Doctor, J . , 5 0 4 Doddi, S., 4 8 8 Dodding, C . J . , 191 Doleys, D. M . , 4 5 7 Dollard, M . , 32 Donnay, Α., 2 3 7 Donnelly, G., 109 Donner, A. P., 3 3 5 D o r a n , T., 3 1 0 Dorhofer, D. M . , 4 7 6 Doucet, E., 136 Dovetail, G., 4 0 0 D o w d a , M . , 128 D o w n s , A. D., 2 8 9 Downton, J . H . , 4 9 1 Dowzer, C , 4 1 4 Doyle, D. V., 155 Droppelman, L . F., 3 4 2 D r o s s m a n , D. Α., 3 9 7 , 3 9 8 , 3 9 9 , 4 0 0 , 4 0 1 , 4 0 5 , 4 0 6 , 407, 409, 410, 411 Drozdick, L . W., 4 9 1 D r u m m o n d , M . F., 505 Dubbert, P. M . , 6 1 , 6 2 , 147, 150, 1 5 2 , 158, 162, 2 6 1 Ducharme, S., 3 6 0 Duckro, P. N . , 306 Duckworth, W. C , 157 Dudgeon, D . , 3 2 9 Dudley, E . , 3 6 0 Dudley, W., 4 4 Duffy, L. S., 4 8 9 Dugoni, B. L . , 128 Dunbar-Jacob, J . , 210 Dunbar-Jacobs, J . M., 294 Dunkel-Schetter, C , 336 Dunn, C , 4 2 , 4 4 , 4 5 , 4 9 , 5 1 , 52, 62 Dunn, K . M . , 368 Dunne, F. J . , 4 7 5 D u p k e , C. Α., 3 6 4 Durand, D., 3 6 0
543
544
THE HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY HANDBOOK Durandet, P., 2 5 7 Dusseldorp, E . , 63, 2 8 8 , 2 9 2 Dziegielewski, S. F., 113 Eakin, E. G., 162 Earleywine, M . , 83, 86, 87, 88 Earp, J . Α., 4 6 1 Eater, J . Α., 113 Ebrahim, S., 2 5 9 Eccleston, C , 2 3 2 Echement, D. Α., 4 9 0 Eddy, Ν. B., 83 Edelstein, B., 4 8 6 , 4 9 4 Edelstein, Β. Α., 4 9 1 , 493 Eder, H . , 4 7 1 Edmonds, A. L . , 368 Edmonds, C . V. I., 346 Edwards, J . R., 332 Edwards, Ν. B., 2 8 9 Edwards, R. R., 4 5 7 Edwards, W. T., 4 5 7 Effron, M . B., 2 6 1 Egan, Β. M . , 4 6 0 Egede, L. E . , 307, 308 Egger, P , 155 Ehrlich, M . H . , 2 6 5 Ehsani, Α. Α., 2 6 1 Eisen, S., 4 Eisen, S. Α., 288 Eisenberg, D. M . , 2 6 2 Eisenberg, J . M . , 5 0 4 Eissenberg, T., 114 Eizenman, D. R., 4 5 9 Ejnell, H . , 2 9 0 el-Guebaly, N . , 4 4 , 533 EI-Halwagy, H . E . , 4 7 7 Elash, C . , 103 Elbert, K., 3 0 7 Elder, J . P., 4 6 0 Elkin, I., 5 2 1 Ell, K . , 335 Ellery, D., 2 3 9 Ellingstad, T. P., 533 Elliot, Α., 3 9 0 , 391 Elliott, P., 259 Elliott, T. E . , 5 0 6 Elliott, T. R., 2 4 0 Ellis, A, 3 1 5 Elwood, J . M . , 326 Emanuel, E. J . , 3 2 9 Emery, C . F., 153 Emery, G., 64, 2 3 1 , 4 1 1 Emmons, Κ. M . , 104 Emmons, M . L . , 293 Engel, B. T., 4 0 1 Engel, G. L . , 4, 5 5 , 56, 194 Engel, R. J . , 4 9 3 Engelgau, M . , 63 Engelgau, M . M . , 314 Engelhardt, P., 368
Enright, P. L . , 4 9 1 Ensink, B., 3 6 0 Epping-Jordan, M . P., 105 Epstein, L . H . , 5 9 , 63 Epstein, N . , 3 6 0 Epstein, Y., 157 Erblich, B., 83, 88 Erblich, J . , 83, 84, 86, 88 Ericsson, M . , 136 Erikssen, J . , 2 5 2 Ernst, D . , 3 3 5 Etscheidt, Μ . Α., 2 4 0 Ettinger, W. H . , J r . , 156 Evans, D. Α., 2 6 0 Evans, R. W., 398 Everson, S., 2 5 4 Everson, S. Α., 2 5 3 , 2 5 4 Ewart, C . K . , 2 6 2 Ewing, J . Α., 24 F. A. Davis, 2 3 8 Fagard, R. H . , 2 6 1 Fagelman, Α., 368 Fagelman, E . , 368 Fagerstrom, K. O., 109, 110 Fairburn, C. G., 138 Fairchild, A. L . , 5 1 5 Fairclough, C . L . , 3 2 9 Faith, M . S., 126, 129, 1 3 1 , 134, 135, 137 Fallo, F., 2 5 4 Fallowfield, L . , 4 8 9 Fallowfield, L. J . , 3 2 7 Fallsberg, M . B., 36 Fanale, J . E . , 4 9 0 Fanghanel, G., 136 Fant, R. V., 102 Faragher, Ε. B., 4 1 4 Farmer, J . Α., 2 8 1 Farmer, J . E . , 506 Farmer, K. C . , 2 1 7 Farmer, R., 4 0 5 Fasanmade, Α. Α., 399 Faucett, J . , 4 5 7 Fava, G. Α., 5 2 2 Fawzy, F. I., 3 3 1 , 3 4 5 , 3 4 6 , 348 Fawzy, N . W., 3 3 1 , 3 4 5 , 3 4 6 Fearn, P. Α., 368 Featherman, D. L . , 4 5 9 Fedoravicius, A. S., 4 0 6 Feinglos, M . N., 3 0 7 , 3 1 5 Feinmann, C . , 2 3 4 Feinstein, A. R., 5 0 7 Feldman, Η. Α., 3 6 7 Feldman, R. D . , 2 6 1 Feldt, K. S., 4 9 3 Felton, B. J . , 3 3 1 Fenske, M . M . , 308 Fentem, P., 155 Ferell, B. R., 4 9 2 Ferguson, Κ. E . , 5 0 5
Name Index Fernandez, E . , 2 4 0 Fernandez, M . , 4 6 1 Ferrando, S. J . , 3 9 2 Ferrannini, E . , 3 0 4 Ferrans, C . E . , 2 3 7 Ferraro, M . G., 4 8 9 Ferrell, Β. Α., 4 9 2 Ferrell, R. E . , 2 6 1 Ferster, C . B., 134 Fett, S. L . , 4 1 4 Fetting, J . H . , 343 Field, A. E . , 126 Field, B. J . , 3 0 7 Field-Gass, M . , 61 Field, J . K., 3 6 4 Field, L . , 2 6 5 Fields, H . , 4 0 4 Figer, Α., 334 File, S. Ε . , 106 Fillingim, R. Β., 4 5 7 Finch, J . , 197, 198 Fine, J . Α., 330 Fink, Α., 5 2 5 Finn, O. J . , 339, 3 4 4 Finn, P. R., 86, 87 Fiore, M . , 61 Fiore, M . C . , 103, 107, 1 1 1 , 112, 113, 114 First, M . B., 110, 4 1 1 Fischer, G., 4 7 1 Fischer, M . E . , 151 Fishbach, R. L . , 4 7 9 Fishbain, D. Α., 2 3 4 , 2 4 1 Fishbein, M . , 2 1 1 , 388 Fisher, Ε. B., 162 Fisher, J . Α., 129 F i s h e r , } . D., 2 1 1 , 2 1 3 , 388 Fisher, J . E . , 3 6 0 F i s h e r , ] . L . , 105 Fisher, W. Α., 2 1 1 , 2 1 3 , 388 Fishman, B., 392 Fiske, Α., 4 9 3 Fitzgerald, F., 2 8 1 , 2 8 2 , 2 9 2 Fitzgerald, J . F., 4 9 0 Fitzgibbon, D., 329 Flanery, R. C . , 343 Fleishman, S. B., 3 4 1 Flessig, Y., 2 1 Fletcher, R. H . , 5 0 4 Fletcher, S. W., 5 0 4 Flor, H . , 2 4 4 Fodor, I. G., 4 7 1 Fodor, J . G., 2 5 9 Foege, W., 3 Foley, Κ. M . , 2 4 1 Folkman, S., 170, 174, 2 3 0 , 2 9 1 , 3 3 0 , 4 7 2 Follette, W. C . , 5 1 7 , 5 2 8 , 5 2 9 , 5 3 0 , 5 3 1 , 5 3 2 , 533 Folli, G., 2 5 7 Follick, M . J . , 243 Folstein, M . E . , 21
Folstein, M . F., 343 Folstein, S. E . , 2 1 Fontaine, K. R., 1 2 6 , 131 Ford, D. E . , 4 2 6 Ford, E. S., 126, 127 Ford, S., 4 8 9 Fordyce, M . , 4 9 4 , 4 9 5 Fordyce, W. E . , 5 6 , 2 4 3 Foreyt, J . P., 106, 130, 134, 136 Forsyth, A. D., 2 1 3 Forsyth, L. H . , 154 Fortmann, S. P., 103, 111 Fortrat, J . O., 2 5 7 Foster, D., 3 3 1 Foster, D. W., 2 5 4 Foster, G. D., 62, 1 3 1 , 134, 137, 138, 153 Fox, Β. H . , 3 3 1 , 3 3 3 , 334, 3 3 6 , 3 4 6 Fox, B. J . , 107 Francis, C . K., 2 6 5 , 5 1 5 Frank, R. G., 1 1 , 5 0 6 , 5 0 7 Franklin, G. M . , 3 1 0 Franklin, R. D., 5 2 9 Franks, V., 471 Franzen, Α., 157 Franzen, M . D . , 19 Frasure-Smith, N . , 4, 64, 2 8 2 , 2 8 8 , 2 9 2 , 5 0 4 Frazer, D . W., 4 8 7 , 4 8 8 , 4 8 9 , 4 9 0 Frazier, A. L . , 156 Frecker, R. C . , 110 Frederick, I. O., 4 1 1 Fredericks, L . , 4 6 1 Freeborn, D. K . , 88 Freedland, K . , 4 Freedland, Κ. E . , 2 3 4 , 2 8 0 , 2 8 8 Freedman, M . , 2 1 Freedson, P. S., 147, 148, 149 Freeland, Κ. E . , 3 0 3 , 316 Freeman, E . L . , 36, 67, 68 Freeman, E. W., 4 7 7 , 4 7 8 Freeman, Η. E . , 5 2 5 Freeth, D . , 3 7 French, S. Α., 128 Freud, S., 88 Freudenheim, J . L . 2 1 7 Fried, L . , 4 9 1 Friedberg, F., 2 3 6 Friedenreich, C. M . , 156 Friedman, D., 8 7 Friedman, G. D . , 2 6 0 Friedman, H . S., 2 8 8 Friedman, J . M . , 3 6 5 Friedman, L. S., 3 9 7 Friedman, M . , 64, 2 7 9 , 2 8 8 , 2 9 2 , 2 9 3 Friedman, Μ . Α., 137 Friedman, P. Α., 2 8 2 Frohlich, P. F., 3 6 0 Frost, S., 102 Fuchs, F. D., 2 6 0 Fudala, P. J . , 107 Fujisawa, Α., 2 5 7 (
545
546
THE HEALTH P S Y C H O L O G Y HANDBOOK Fukuda, K., 2 3 7 Fydrich, T., 2 4 4 Gabriel, B., 345 Gabriel, S. E . , 398, 399 Gaeta, H . , 87 Gagnon, J . H . , 359, 362 G a g n o n , P., 3 2 9 , 4 8 9 Gaither, G. Α., 360 Galatopoulos, C , 4 7 5 Galea, S., 503 Gallacher, J . E. J . , 2 9 2 Gallant, M . P., 3 0 9 Gallant, S. J . , 471 Gallup, G., J r . , 4 9 5 Gallup Organization, 4 2 5 Galovski, T. E . , 4 1 4 G a m m a , Α., 4 7 2 G a m m o n , M . D . , 156 G a n g a d h a r a p p a , N., 86 Ganguli, M . , 4 8 9 , 4 9 0 Ganiats, T. G., 508 Ganz, P., 3 3 2 , 343 Garay-Sevilla, M . E . , 308 G a r c i a - M u n o z , M . , 105 Garcy, P., 2 4 0 Gardner, E . , 303 Garfinkel, J . , 3 2 5 Garretson, H . D., 2 4 0 Garrod, Α., 4 7 6 Garron, L. Α., 333 Garrow, J . S., 125 Garssen, B., 3 3 9 , 343 Gaskin, M . E . , 2 4 0 Gatchel, R. J . , 2 3 9 , 2 4 0 G a t h , D., 3 6 6 , 4 7 6 Gatz, M . , 4 8 5 , 4 9 3 G a u , G. T., 2 8 2 Gaule, D., 188, 194, 198, 199 Gauvin, L . , 158 Gavard, J . Α., 316 Gayet, C , 2 5 7 Gaztambide, S., 23 Gebhardt, M . C , 3 4 1 Gehl, Κ. Α., 155 Gehling, E . , 502 Geisser, M . , 6 Geisser, M . E . , 2 3 1 , 2 3 2 , 2 3 9 , 2 4 0 , 2 4 3 Gelbaya, Τ. Α., 4 7 7 Gelenberg, A. J . , 3 0 8 , 360, 368 Geliebter, Α., 135 Gellert, G. Α., Maxwell, R. M . , 336 Gelmon, S. B., 3 5 George, L. K., 4 9 3 George, M . , 368 Georgiades, Α., 6 1 , 152 Gerard, M . J . , 2 6 0 Gerhardsson de Verdier, M . , 332 Geronimus, A. T., 4 5 9 Gerrish, K., 2 9
Gershon, M . , 4 0 7 Gerstley, L. J . , 86 Geyer, S., 332 Gharib, C , 2 5 7 Giancola, P. R., 85 Gibbon, M . , 1 1 0 , 4 1 1 Gibson, G., 2 6 5 Gibson, S., 493 Gibson, S. J . , 4 9 3 Gifford, Ε. V., 5 1 9 Gil, Κ. M . , 2 3 9 , 2 4 1 Gilbert, B. O., 103, 104, 106, 108 Gilbert, D . G., 103, 104, 106, 108 Gilbert, J . H . V., 30, 3 5 , 36 Gilbert, P., 4 7 1 Giles, D. E . , 4 2 6 Gilkson, M . , 2 8 2 Giller, E. L . , 341 Gillmore, M . R., 5 2 1 Oilman, M . W., 2 6 0 Gingras, J . , 309 Gipson, M . , 3 6 5 Gittelman, D. F., 4 7 7 Gittelsohn, J . , 4 6 0 Giuliano, F., 368 Gladsjo, J . Α., 108 Glaser, R., 66, 172, 3 3 9 , 5 0 7 Glasgow, R. E . , 64, 162, 2 1 5 , 2 1 6 , 304, 314, 3 1 5 , 316 Glassman, A. H . , 114, 2 8 8 Glatt, A. E . , 3 6 6 Gleser, L . J . , 63 Glover, E. D., 113 Glover, P. N., 113 G o d a , Y., 105 Godin, G., 2 1 4 Goetsch, V. L., 3 0 7 Gold, Α., 2 6 2 Gold, L . , 368 Gold, M . S., 83, 192 Goldberg, A. D . , 2 8 0 Goldberg, D. E . , 2 5 4 Goldberg, E. L . , 364 Goldberg, I. D., 332 Goldberg, M . , 2 3 4 Goldberg, R. T., 4 7 9 Golden, A. G., 4 9 0 Goldfried, M . R., 2 8 5 Goldman, M . S., 88 Goldsmith, R. J . , 30 Goldstein, G., 86 Goldstein, I., 3 6 7 Goldstein, M . G., I l l Goldstein, M . Z . , 3 6 0 Goldstone, S. E . , 3 6 9 Golomb, M., 262 Gomberg, E . S., 4 7 5 Gonder-Frederick, L . , 2 9 2 Gonder-Frederick, L. Α., 6 0 , 6 1 , 66, 316, 3 1 9 Gonzalez-Fernandes, R. Α., 2 6 5
Goode, Ε., 31 Goodie, J . , 4 8 6 Goodkin, K., 6 5 , 339 Goodrick, G. K., 130, 134 Goodwin, J . S., 3 3 5 Goodwin, P. J . , 6 5 , 67, 3 3 6 , 346 Goorin, Α., 341 Gordon, J . Α., 3 6 9 G o r d o n , J . R., 89, 9 1 , 2 1 4 , 2 1 9 , 3 0 7 Gordon, L. K . , 4 0 5 Gordon, N., 4 5 7 Gordon, T., 2 9 3 Gorman, B. S., 529 Gorman, L. L., 67 Gorman, M., 504 Gorrell, C . , 3 6 5 Gorsuch, R. L . , 177, 2 6 6 Gortmaker, S. L., 1 2 7 Gosse, P., 2 5 7 Gotay, C. C . , 4 6 1 Gottheil, E . , 65 Gottman, J . M . , 5 3 0 Gotto, A. M . , 2 8 1 Goubert, L . , 2 3 2 Gould, J . W., 23 Graber, J . Α., 4 7 0 Graettinger, W. F., 2 5 7 Graf-Morgenstern, M . , 368 Grafton-Becker, V., 3 6 5 Gramling, S. E . , 2 8 5 Granek, M . , 2 6 2 Grange, T., 4 9 2 Granot, M . , 4 7 7 Grant, I., 153, 3 9 2 , 393 Grant, P. J . , 44 Grant, W., 3 0 7 Graves, J . E . , 2 6 1 Gray, R., 105 Green, C. Α., 88 Green, G. F., 368 Green, L. W., 6 1 , 2 1 7 , 326 Greenberg, D. B., 3 4 1 , 4 8 9 Greenberg, P. E . , 34 Greene, A. F., 2 4 0 Greene, J . G., 4 7 1 Greenfield, D. P., 188 Greenhouse, J . , 343 Greer, S., 3 3 1 , 3 3 2 Greil, A. L . , 478 Gries, F. Α., 157 Griffith, L. S., 64, 3 0 7 , 3 1 6 , 4 8 8 Griffiths, Α., 32 Grigoletto, F., 21 Grilly, D. M . , 82 Gritz, E . , 62 Gritz, E. R., 104, 3 4 0 Groessl, E . J . , 56 Gronsman, K. J . , 3 0 7 Gross, R. T., 24, 2 3 9 Grossarth-Maticek, R., 3 3 4
G r o s s m a n , L. M . , 83 Grottos, L. C . , 61 Groves, P. M . , 105 G r u m a n , C . , 23 Grunberg, Ν. E . , 107 Grundy, S. M . , 154 Guidice, L . , 4 7 8 Gulbinat, W., 3 3 3 Gullette, E. C . D., 6 1 , 152 Gulliver, S. B., 108 Gunary, R. M . , 4 1 4 G u p t a , A. K., 4 2 6 G u p t a , Μ . Α., 4 2 6 Gupta, O. P., 398 Gust, S. W., 103 Gutterman, J . , 341 Guttmacher, A. E . , 15 Guyatt, G. H . , 151 Guyon, L . , 24 Guys, M . , 103 Gwee, Κ. Α., 3 9 9 H a a g a , D. Α., 62 H a b e r m a n , H . F., 4 2 6 Hackett, G. I., 368 Hackett, T. P., 288 H a d d o c k , C. K., 136 H a g b e r g , J . M . , 152, 2 6 1 Hagglund, K. J . , 5 0 7 H a h n , Β. Α., 3 9 8 , 4 1 1 H a h n , M . B., 2 4 0 H a i m o v , I., 4 9 1 Hajek, P., 103 Halbach, H . , 83 Halbert, J . Α., 152 Hall, A. C . , 155 Hall, Ε. M . , 32 Hall, J . Α., 2 1 5 Hall, J . E . , 2 5 7 Hall, R. C . W., 4 8 9 Hall, S. M . , 103, 106, 108, 2 9 2 Hall, W., 34, 80 Hallstrom, T., 2 9 0 Halvorsen, J . G., 3 6 5 H a m a n n , Η. Α., 5 7 Hamdan, L., 529 Hamilton, S., 4 1 4 H a m m a n , R. F., 3 1 0 H a m m a r , M . , 36 H a m m i c k , M . , 37 Hamovitch, M . , 3 3 5 H a m p s o n , J . P., 368 H a m p s o n , S. E . , 3 1 5 H a n n , D. M . , 3 4 5 , 3 4 6 Hannent, I., 2 3 9 Hansen, S. W., 341 Hansen, W. B., 5 2 5 H a p o n i k , E . F., 4 9 1 Harden, R. M . , 36 Harden, R. N., 2 3 7
548
T H E HEALTH P S Y C H O L O G Y HANDBOOK Hardin, Κ. N . , 7 Hargrove, D. S., 36, 6 7 , 68 Harley, R. M . , 80 H a r m a n , M . , 398 Harmsen, W. S., 398 Harper, R. G., 21 Harrell, L. E . , 4 9 4 Harrington, P., 5 1 6 Harris, M . , 2 3 4 Harris, M . I., 3 0 3 , 3 0 5 , 3 1 0 Harris-Reid, M . , 4 5 6 Harris, T. O., 173, 1 7 7 Harrison, P. Α., 86 Hart, D. J . , 155 Hart, J . , 2 6 2 Hàrter, M . , 336 Harvey, J . N., 368 Harvey, R. F., 4 1 4 Hashimi, M . W., 258 Hasin, D. S., 126 Hastings Center, 4 9 4 , 4 9 5 H a t s u k a m i , D. K., 1 0 2 , 103, 1 0 5 , 108, 110, 113, 114 Hattersley, Α., 3 0 5 Hatzichristou, D. G., 3 6 7 H a u c k , E. R., 153 Hauri, P., 4 2 5 , 4 2 6 , 4 2 8 Hautanen, Α., 2 5 5 Haviland, J . S., 3 3 2 Hawkins, D. R., 4 2 6 Hawkins, R. P., 5 2 7 , 5 2 9 Hawryluk, G., 153 H a w t o n , K., 366 Hayashida, M . , 85 Haybittle, J . L . , 3 3 2 Hayes, S. C , 5 1 4 , 5 1 9 , 528 Haynes, R. B., 2 0 9 , 2 1 0 , 2 1 4 , 2 1 6 , 2 6 6 Haynes, S. N., 528 H a y s , R. D., 368 Hdner, J . , 2 9 0 He, J . , 259 Headache Classification Committee of the International Headache Society, 2 3 6 Hearn, M . T., 4 7 8 Heather, N., 4 4 Heatherton, T. F., 110 Heaton, K . W., 4 0 0 Hébert, P. R., 4 6 0 Hecht, M . J . , 368 Heckemeyer, C . M . , 44 Hecker, M . H . , 5 0 4 Hecth, F. M . , 3 8 9 Hedahl, K. J . , 4 7 0 Heim, E . , 3 3 1 Heiman, J . R., 3 6 1 , 3 6 2 , 3 6 4 , 3 6 5 Heinberg, L . J . , 127 Heishman, S. J . , 102 Heiss, G., 2 6 0 Helge, T. D . , 4 3 3 Helgeson, V. S., 5 9 , 66, 6 7 , 3 4 5 , 4 7 2 Helme, R., 493
Helme, R. D., 4 9 3 Helmick, C . G., 155, 156 Helsing, K . J . , 3 3 2 Henderson, S., 34 Hening, W., 4 2 8 , 4 3 5 Hennekens, C. H . , 2 6 0 , 2 8 0 , 504 Henningfield, J . E . , 102, 1 1 2 , 114 Henrich, G., 3 3 6 Henry, J . L . , 3 1 5 Henry, J . P., 253 Henson, C . D., 2 3 2 Herberman, R. B., 3 3 5 Herbert, B., 4 7 9 Herbert, J . , 3 5 9 Herbison, G. P., 4 7 9 Herman, C . P., 137 Hernandez, J . T., 4 7 7 Herrman, C , 2 8 4 Herrman, H . , 2 8 , 3 0 Hersen, M . , 518 Hesselbrock, M . N . , 87 Hesselbrock, V. M . , 8 7 Hetzel, L . , 4 8 5 Hewer, R. Α., 4 8 9 Heymsfield, S. B., 1 2 5 , 136 H e y w o o d , E . , 84 Hiatt, R. Α., 4 6 1 H i c k a m , D. H . , 188 Higgins, E . T., 4 7 0 Higgins, M . , 2 5 2 Higgins, P., 2 3 2 Higgins, S. T., 102 Higuchi, S., 85 Hill, J . O., 128 Hill, M . , 2 1 4 , 2 6 5 Hill, M . N., 2 9 4 Hill, R. W., 2 3 8 Hillenberg, J . B., 179 Hillier, Τ. Α., 3 0 5 Hilton, R., 3 5 , 36 Hiltz, M . J . , 368 Himeno, E . , 2 5 8 Himmelfarb, S., 493 Hinken, C , 393 Hinton, R. Α., 4 1 4 Hirji, K . , 3 4 1 , 343 Hislop, T. G., 3 3 5 , 4 0 4 Hitsman, B., 106, 108 H o , J . , 59 H o b s o n , M . L . , 158 H o c k e , V., 4 7 9 H o d g e , F. S., 4 6 1 Hodgins, D. C , 4 4 , 533 Hoehler, F., 4 0 4 Hoenk, P. R., 340 Hoffman, N . G., 86 H o g a n , Β. E . , 60, 66, 2 5 6 Hogerzeil, Η . V., 4 7 1 Hohagen, F., 4 9 1 Hoick, P., 4 6 1
Holder, J . S., 161 Holland, J . , 3 2 9 Holland, J . C , 3 3 4 , 3 4 5 Hollis, J . F., 3 3 5 , 5 2 0 , 5 2 1 Hollnagel, H . , 4 7 2 Hollon, S. D., 34 Holloway, R. L . , 2 4 4 Holmes, T. H . , 173 Holmlund, U., 4 7 7 Holroyd, C , 4 8 7 Holroyd, Κ. Α., 66, 2 4 2 Holt, C . S., 341 Hontela, S., 4 8 8 Hoogendijk, W. J . G., 4 8 7 Horan, M . J . , 252 Horgen, Κ. B., 128 H o m e , D. J . d e L . , 30 Horwitz, R. I., 4 5 9 Hosking, D. J . , 3 0 6 Hospers, H . J . , 388 H o u s e , Α., 4 4 , 4 8 9 Houston, Β. K., 2 6 2 , 504 Houston-Miller, N., 2 1 4 H o w a r d , Κ. I., 531 Howland, J . , 491 Hoyert, D. L . , 325 Hoyt, M . F., 2 9 4 H u , L . , 458 Hubinger, Α., 157 Hiibner, W. Α., 368 H u d s o n , A. M . , 155 H u d s o n , B., 2 8 - 2 9 H u d s o n , C . J . , 158 Hughes, J . R., 102, 103, 105, 107, 108, 110 Hughes, M . , 4 5 6 Hulka, J . F., 4 7 7 Humphreys, K., 5 0 3 , 5 0 6 Hunsley, J . , 3 0 9 Hunt, C . , 2 3 9 Hunt, W. C . , 3 3 5 Hurlbert, D. F., 3 6 5 , 366 Hurt, R. D., 114 Hvas, L., 4 7 1 , 472 H w a n g , W. C , 4 5 9 , 4 6 1 H y a m s , J . S., 399 Hyde, R. T., 2 6 1 Hyman, D. J . , 2 6 4 Hypertension Detection and Follow-up Program Cooperative G r o u p , 2 1 6 Iconomou, G., 66 Ikeda, M . , 2 5 8 Imboden, J . B., 2 3 5 Impériale, T., 408 Inbar, M . , 3 3 4 , 348 Incrocci, L . , 368 Ingelfinger, F. J . , 326 Ingram, Κ. K . , 4 9 4 Institute for the Future, 2 0 9 - 2 1 0 Institute of Medicine, 2 1 0 , 2 1 2
Ironson, G., 64 Irvin, J . E . , 101 Isbell, H . , 83 Isgar, B., 398 Ishibashi, K., 2 5 9 Ishii, N., 368 Ives, D. G., 133 I w a m a s a , G. Y., 4 5 1 Iwamoto, E. T., 107 Iwamoto, T . , 368 J a b e r , L. Α., 3 0 7 J a c k , L. M . , 106 J a c k s o n , Α., 44, 2 6 5 Jackson, J . , 412 Jackson, J . C , 461 J a c k s o n , W. T., 2 4 0 J a c o b , P., Ill, 103 J a c o b , R. G., 2 6 2 J a c o b s , G. Α., 108, 177 J a c o b s , J . , 336 J a c o b s , J . R., 4 3 3 J a c o b s , J . W., 343 Jacobsberg, L. B., 3 9 2 Jacobsen, P. B., 2 3 2 , 3 4 5 , 346 Jacobsen, S. J . , 3 9 9 J a c o b s o n , A. M . , 3 0 6 , 3 1 0 J a c o b s o n , E . , 178 J a c o b s o n , N . S., 5 3 0 , 5 3 2 Jaffe, Α., 4 Jaffe, A. S., 2 3 4 , 2 8 8 Jailwala, J . , 4 0 8 , 4 1 2 J a i n , A. P., 398 J a j o o , U. N., 398 J a k o b , U., 336 J a m i s o n , R. N., 2 4 1 , 334 Janis, I. L . , 56, 2 1 0 J a n z , Ν. K . , 3 3 0 Jarrett, R. B., 4 7 7 , 528 Jarvis, M . , 113, 114 Jarvis, Μ . A. E . , 4 7 1 J a s o n , L. Α., 2 3 6 , 2 3 7 J e b b , S. Α., 154 Jee, S., 2 6 1 Jeffery, R. W., 128, 154, 5 0 2 Jeffrey, T., 12 Jeffrey, W. R., 5 9 , 63 Jennings, G. L . , 2 6 1 Jennings, J . R., 2 8 9 Jennings, S. G., 2 5 9 Jensen, J . , 4 9 3 Jensen, M . P., 2 3 2 , 2 3 9 Jensen, M . R., 3 3 1 Jeste, D. V., 108, 4 9 3 Jimenez-Genchi, Α., 106 Jin, C . B., 2 5 5 Joffe, H . , 4 7 1 Joffres, M . , 3 3 2 J o h n , Ε. M . , 156 Johnsen, S. K . , 2 2
550
THE HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY HANDBOOK Johnson, B., 189 Johnson, B. T., 2 1 3 Johnson, C . C , 4 6 0 Johnson, G., 342 Johnson, J . Α., 266 Johnson, J . H., 177 Johnson, J . V., 32 Johnson, R. J . , 4 9 0 Johnson, S., 127 Johnson, S. L . , 129 Johnson, V. E., 359, 360 Johnson, W. G., 127 Johnston, Α., 114 Johnston, D. W., 2 6 2 Johnston, S., 2 3 2 Joint National Committee on Detection, Evaluation, and Treatment of High Blood Pressure ( J N C ) , 1 5 1 , 2 5 3 , 2 5 8 , 2 6 1 , 2 6 5 , 266 Jones, B. T., 88 Jones, J . C , 364 Jones, J . W., 32 Jones, K. R., 399 Jones, M . , 28, 36, 37 Jones, N. L . , 2 1 4 Jones, T. G., 21 J o r d a n , C , 32 J o r d a n , M . C , 399 Jorgensen, P., 333 Jorgensen, R. S., 2 6 2 Josephs, R. Α., 83 Jowsey, S., 14 Judge, Τ. Α., 32 Julius, S., 2 5 5 , 2 5 7 Juneau, M . , 504 J u n g , D. L . , 261 J u p p , J . J , 360 Justice, Α., 332 Kagawa-Singer, M . , 4 5 9 K a h a n a , B., 341 Kahn, B., 343 Kahn, L . B., 310 Kahn, R. L . , 4 8 5 Kalauokalani, D., 243 Kales, Α., 172 Kalichman, S. C , 62, 2 1 3 , 386, 388 Kalucy, E. C , 4 2 6 Kalucy, R. S., 426 Kamarck, T. W., 2 8 1 , 2 8 9 Kamerow, D. B., 4 2 6 Karnes, L. D., 4 7 7 Kammeier, M . L . , 86 Kan, L., 335 Kanani, R., 2 1 4 Kanazir, D. T , 334 Kane, V., 30 Kanner, A. D., 1 7 7 Kaplan, G. Α., 2 5 4 , 3 3 5 Kaplan, Η. I., 4 7 5
Kaplan, H . S., 360, 364, 366 Kaplan, J . , 286 Kaplan, Ν. M . , 254, 2 6 5 Kaplan, N. W., 151 Kaplan, R., 3, 504 Kaplan, R. M . , 56, 2 0 9 , 504, 508 Kaprio, J . , 332 Karajgi, B., 4 8 8 Karasek, R., 4 7 3 , 4 7 4 Karasic, D. H., 392 Kardia, S. L. R., 107 Karel, M . J . , 4 8 5 Karnieli, E., 157 Kash, Κ. M . , 338 Kasl-Godley, J . E., 4 8 5 , 4 9 3 Kassel, J . D., 103 Kathol, R. G., 3 4 1 Katon, W., 64 Katsikitis, M . , 24 Kattan, M . W., 368 Katz, I. R., 4 9 0 Katz, J . , 403 Katz, N . P., 2 4 1 Katz, N . R., 2 1 5 Katz, R. C , 3 6 5 Katzoff, J . , 192 Kaufer, D. I., 4 9 2 Kaufert, P. Α., 4 7 1 Kaufman, P. G., 2 6 2 Kawachi, I., 104, 2 8 9 Kawanishi, Y., 368 Kay, A. D., 4 9 0 Kay, J . Α., 183 Kaye, M . D., 398 Kazdin, A. E., 5 1 9 , 5 2 9 , 530 Keane, T. M . , 88 Keck, P. E., J r . , 2 3 5 Keefe, F. J . , 5 5 , 59, 62, 66, 67, 2 2 9 , 232, 238, 239, 410 Keehn, R. J . , 332 Keenan, R. M . , 103 Keith, P. M . , 59 Keith-Spiegel, P., 4 4 5 , 4 5 3 Kelder, S. H., 4 7 7 Kelemen, M . H., 2 6 1 Keller, K. L . , 129 Keller, M . , 336 Kelley, G. Α., 152 Kelley, K. S., 152 Kelley, S. D., 5 2 5 Kellogg, C. E., 2 2 Kellow, J . E., 4 0 2 , 4 0 5 Kellum, Κ. K., 533 Kelly, G., 261 Kelly, J . Α., 62, 63, 67, 3 8 8 , 389 Kelly, J . J . , 4 6 1 Kelly, M . P., 366 Keltikangas-Jarvinen, L . , 2 5 5 Kemper, H . C. G., 148 Kempthorne-Rawson, J . , 333
Name Index Kendall, D., 2 4 0 Kendall, P. C , 5 5 , 531 Kendler, K. S., 1 2 7 Kenny, P. J . , 106 Kentish, J . , 2 6 2 Kenyon, L. W., 2 9 0 Keogh, E . , 2 3 9 Keppel, K. G., 4 5 6 Kern, Ε. E . , 3 0 Kern, F. J . , 4 0 2 Kern, J . M . , 3 6 0 Kerns, R. D., 2 3 8 , 2 4 0 Kessler, R., 473 Kessler, R. C , 4 0 5 Ketterer, M . W., 2 8 0 , 2 8 1 , 2 8 2 , 284, 2 8 5 , 289, 290, 2 9 1 , 292 Keuler, F. U., 368 Keuning, J . , 3 3 4 Key, C . R., 335 Khalife, S., 366 Khlat, R., 4 7 4 Khoury, A. J . , 4 6 9 Kiecolt-Glaser, J . , 5 0 7 Kiecolt-Glaser, J . K., 66, 172, 3 3 9 Kiernan, R. J . , 343 Kiesewetter, F., 368 Killen, J . D., 103, 1 1 1 , 2 1 4 Kilpatrick, D. G., 4 7 9 Kim, E. L . , 5 0 2 K i m , H . F. S., 488 Kimble, D. L . , 2 1 1 Kincade, J . E . , 3 3 5 Kington, R. S., 4 5 6 Kinsey, W., 189 Kirby, H . B., 21 Kircher, J . R., 3 6 6 Kirkpatrick, D. R., 4 9 3 Kirschenbaum, D. S., 130, 134 Kissane, D. W., 3 3 6 Kissel, S. S., 64, 316 Kivela, S. L . , 4 9 1 Klag, M . J . , 2 5 9 K l a p o w , J . , 2 1 3 , 504, 5 0 5 Klapow, J . C., 506, 507 Klatsky, A. L . , 2 6 0 Klein, K . , 4 1 2 Klein, R., 3 1 0 Kleinplatz, P. J . , 3 7 5 Klerman, G. L . , 65 Klesges, L. M . , 108 Klesges, R. C , 103, 108 Klinkman, M . S., 64 Knight, B. G., 4 9 0 Knight, E. L . , 2 6 4 , 2 6 5 Knight, S. J . , 7 Knowler, W. C . , 129 Koch, U., 3 3 6 Kochanek, K. D., 3 2 5 Koenig, H . G., 4 9 5 Koerner, K., 5 3 0
Koffman, D. M . , 4 6 0 Kofoed, L . , 1 9 2 , 193, 197 K o g a n , J . N., 493 K o g o n , M . M . , 3 3 6 , 346 Kohlenberg, B. S., 5 1 9 Kojima, S., 2 5 9 K o k , G. J . , 62 Kokkinos, P. F., 152 Kolli, R., 4 8 8 Konen, J . C . , 368 K o o b , G. F., 104, 105 Koocher, G., 331 Koocher, G. P., 4 4 5 , 4 5 3 Kooyers, K. J . , 63 Kopel, S. Α., 5 2 0 Koplan, J . P., 127 K o p p , M . S., 4 7 0 , 4 7 5 , 4 7 7 Koppel, I., 3 7 Koren, M . J . , 2 5 3 Kornblith, A. B., 3 4 0 Koskenvuo, M . , 3 3 2 Koski, K . , 4 9 1 Kotik-Harper, D., 2 1 Kottke, T., 3, 62, 2 1 4 , 2 6 5 Kouyanou, K., 2 3 5 Kovacs, M . , 306 Koven, L . , 4 8 6 Kovner, F., 3 2 8 , 3 3 7 Kozlowski, L . T., 110, 112 Koznar, J . , 369 K r a a i j , V., 6 3 , 2 8 8 Kraemer, H. C . , 6 5 , 2 6 2 Kraft, P., 2 9 0 Krahn, D. D., 4 8 8 Krailo, M . , 3 3 5 Krane, R. J . , 3 6 7 Krantz, D. S., 2 8 1 , 504 Kratt, P. P., 44 Krause, J . , 136 K r a u s s , D. J . , 369 Krege, B., 4 5 Kreitler, H . , 3 2 6 , 3 3 0 , 3 3 1 , 3 3 2 , 334, 3 3 7 , 3 4 6 , 348 Kreitler, S., 326, 3 2 7 , 3 2 8 , 3 3 0 , 3 3 1 , 3 3 2 , 334, 3 3 5 , 3 3 7 , 3 4 1 , 3 4 6 , 348 Kremer, J . , 4 8 7 Kreutzer, J . , 88 Kriska, A. M . , 148, 156, 157 Kristeller, J . L . , 104, 109 Kroenke, K . , 9, 4 0 8 Kronholm, P., 4 9 0 Kruis, W., 4 0 0 Kubler-Ross, E . , 329 Kubzansky, L. D., 2 8 9 Kuhn, C . M . , 3 0 7 Kulas, J . F., 2 2 Kulich, R. J . , 2 3 4 Kuller, L. H . , 133, 258 Kumanyika, S. K., 3 1 0 , 4 5 9 , 4 6 0 K u m a r , D., 399 Kummel, E . , 238
551
552
THE HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY HANDBOOK Kunen, S., 2 2 K u o , H-S., 4 5 8 Kupfer, D. J . , 4 2 6 K u r a m o t o , K., 2 5 7 Kurki, T. S., 5 0 8 Kuroiwa, Α., 258 Kurzrock, R., 3 4 1 Kushner, R., 2 5 7 , 258 Kuwajima, I., 2 5 7 Laake, K., 4 8 7 Laçasse, Y., 153 Lach, H . W., 4 9 1 Lackner, J . M . , 4 1 4 Lacks, P., 4 3 2 Lacourciere, Y., 2 6 0 Lacro, J . P., 108 Laffel, L . M . , 59 Lahita, R. G., 238 Laine, T. P., 87 Laippala, P., 4 9 1 Laird, J . M . , 4 0 2 , 403 Lalumiere, M . L . , 3 6 0 Lammers, C. Α., 3 1 5 Lamont, J . Α., 3 6 7 Landerman, R., 493 L a n d o , H . , 113, 114 Landsverk, J . Α., 3 4 0 Lane, D. S., 330 Langford, H . G., 2 5 9 Langston, J . W., 343 Lanier, A. P., 4 6 1 Lantelme, P., 2 5 7 Laqueur, T., 3 6 0 Laragh, J . F., 253 L a R o c c o , S. Α., 4 7 1 Larochelle, P., 2 5 9 Larsen, J . P., 4 8 7 Larson, D . B., 4 9 5 Latimer, P. R., 404 Lattika, P., 156 L a u , J . , 63 L a u m a n n , E. O., 3 5 9 , 3 6 2 , 365 Lawton, M . J . , 199 Lawton, M . P., 4 9 0 Layfield, M . , 2 4 0 Lazarus, R. S., 170, 174, 177, 1 8 1 , 2 3 0 , 2 4 0 , 2 9 1 , 330, 472 Le M o a l , M . , 104 Le Pape, Α., 474 Leaf, P. J . , 4 5 8 Lederberg, M . , 336 Lee, C., 4 7 2 Lee, J . , 335 Lee, J . Y., 2 6 5 Lee, P., 310 Leenen, F., 2 5 9 Lefebvre, J . C., 2 3 2 Lefebvre, M . F., 2 3 9 Lefkowitz, R. J . , 2 5 4
Legh-Smith, J . , 4 8 9 Legler, J . , 4 6 1 Lehrer, P. M . , 173, 177 Leibel, S. Α., 3 6 8 Leiblum, S. R., 3 6 2 , 3 6 5 , 3 6 7 , 3 6 9 Leicht, M . L . , 4 8 7 Leisen, M . B., 528 Leitenberg, FL, 62 LeMaster, P. L . , 4 6 0 Lenz, J . W., 2 6 2 Leo, G. I., 5 2 1 Leon, A. S., 150 Lepper, H . S., 2 1 5 LeResche, L . , 2 2 9 , 3 9 7 Lerman, C., 107, 3 3 8 Lesermann, J . , 4 0 6 L e s k o , L. M . , 3 4 1 , 3 4 2 , 348 Leslie, B., 34 Lesniak, K. T., 152 Lesperance, F., 4, 64, 2 8 2 , 2 8 8 , 2 9 2 , 504 Lessard, R. J . , 86 Lessmeier, T . J . , 283 Letizia, Κ. Α., 131 Leung, R. W., 2 6 1 Levav, I., 3 3 3 , 334 Levavi, H . , 3 3 4 Levendag, P. C., 368 Levenkrohn, S., 21 Levenson, R. W., 83 Leventhal, Ε. Α., 66 Levin, D. C., 153 Levin, E. D., 104, 107 Levin, W. C., 4 9 1 Levine, A. M . , 3 3 5 Levine, D . M . , 6 1 , 2 1 7 Levine, J . , 4 5 7 Levitt, Ε. E . , 134 Levy, S. M . , 335 Lewin, B., 2 8 1 , 2 9 2 Lewinsohn, P. W., 4 9 3 Lewis, D. Α., 4 7 6 Lewis, P., 188, 194, 198, 199 Lewis, S., 4 8 9 Lewis, T. S., 2 8 2 Lewis, T. T., 4 5 9 Lex, B. W., 4 5 9 Li, T. C., 4 7 8 Li, Z . , 4 0 5 , 4 1 0 Lichtenberg, P. Α., 4 8 5 Lichtenstein, E . , 113 Liddell, Α., 4 9 3 Lie, H . , 2 5 2 Lief, J . H . , 3 6 0 Liese, B. S., 90 Light, K. C., 253 Limacher, M . C., 261 Lindamer, L . Α., 108 Linde, S., 4 2 5 , 4 2 6 , 4 2 8 Linden, W., 6 0 , 6 3 , 64, 1 5 1 , 179, 2 5 6 , 2 6 2 , 2 9 2 Lindsey, K., 4 9 5
Name Index Linkins, R. W., 108 Linnerooth, P. J . , 528 Linton, S. J . , 2 3 1 , 3 9 7 Lipman, R. L , , 157 Lipowski, Z . J . , 3 0 Lippman, M . , 3 3 5 Lisman, S. Α., 88 Litchfield, W. R., 2 5 4 Littlefield, C . H . , 309 Litwin, M . S., 368 Livingston, E. G., 3 0 7 L o b o , Α., 343 Lock, M . , 4 7 1 Locker, D., 493 L o c k w o o d , G. Α., 346 Longnecker, M . P., 332 L o n g o , J . P., 189, 192, 195, 196, 197 LoPiccolo, J . , 3 6 1 , 3 6 5 , 366 Lorr, M . , 3 4 2 Louie, D., 61 Lovallo, W. R., 172, 173, 254 Lowenthal, D. T., 2 6 1 Lowery, D., 4 5 7 Luciano, D. S., 254 Lucini, D . , 2 6 2 Lucock, M . P., 24 L u d b r o o k , P. Α., 234 L u d m a n , E. J . , 44 Ludwig, W. W., 2 1 4 Luftey, K . , 2 1 5 Lumry, A. E., 86 Lushene, R., 177 Lusignolo T . M . , 4 5 9 Lusk, E. J . , 3 3 1 , 3 3 5 Lustman, P. J . , 64, 2 1 5 , 3 0 3 , 3 0 7 , 3 0 8 , 316, 3 1 8 , 4 8 8 Lutgendorf, S. K . , 66 Luukinen, H . , 4 9 1 Luzzatto, P., 3 4 5 Lynn, R. B., 3 9 7 Lyons, M . J . , 80 M a c A n d r e w , C . , 86 M a c é r a , C . , 155 Maciewicz, R., 2 3 4 Maclntyre, N . R., 153 M a c k , J . E . , 43 M a c L e o d , B. D., 193, 194, 198 M a c M a h o n , S., 1 5 1 , 2 5 2 M a d d e n , P. A. F., 103, 1 0 7 M a d h a v e n , S., 2 6 5 M a e s , M . , 106 M a e s , S., 6 3 , 2 8 8 M a g g i , S., 4 9 1 M a g g i o , C. Α., 305 Magnus, M . H., 460 Mahoney, M . J . , 2 1 0 , 2 1 4 M a i m a n , L. Α., 2 1 2 Malahey, B., 2 1 9 M a l a m o o d , H . S., 4 0 9
Malarkey, W. B., 172 M a l a s p i n a , D., 2 5 7 M a l a v a u d , B., 3 6 0 Malec, J . , 7 M a l g a d y , R. G., 110 Mallion, J . M . , 2 5 6 Malloy, T. E . , 2 1 1 Malterud, K . , 4 7 2 Mancia, G., 1 5 1 , 256, 2 5 7 Mancini, T., 2 8 5 Mandel, F. S., 4 7 9 Mandin, H . , 3 0 9 Mangelsdorff, A. D . , 9 M a n o l i o , Τ. Α., 4 9 1 Manson, J . , 305 M a n s o n , J . E . , 126 Mansvelder, H. D., 105 Mantell, J . , 335 M a r c u s , Β. H . , 154, 155 M a r c u s , D. Α., 5 2 0 Margolin, G., 3 3 5 M a r k , M . M . , 66 Markides, K. S., 4 5 9 M a r k o u , Α., 104, 105 Markovitz, J . H . , 172, 2 8 0 M a r k s , I. M . , 522 M a r k s , J . L . , 103 M a r k s , J . S., 127 M a r i a i t , G. Α., 89, 9 1 , 2 1 4 , 2 1 9 , 3 0 7 M a r o n d e , R. F., 192 M a r r s - G a r c i a , Α., 5 3 1 Marsh, L., 487 Marshall, J . Α., 3 1 0 M a r s o n , D. C . , 494 Martin, Α., 393 Martin, D . C . , 80 Martin, J . B., 5 2 2 Martin, J . E . , 150, 2 6 1 Martin, R., 4 8 6 Martin, W. H., 2 6 1 M a r u c h a , P. T . , 172 M a r u m o , K., 3 6 7 M a r x e r , F., 473 M a s e k , B. J . , 5 2 0 M a s o n , D. Α., 44 M a s o n , P., 44 M a s s e , B., 4 8 9 Massie, J . J . , 3 4 0 Massie, J . M . , 341 Massie, M . , 3 2 9 Massie, M . J . , 3 4 1 , 3 4 5 , 3 4 8 M a s s i m o , L. M . , 5 1 6 Masters, W. H . , 3 5 9 , 3 6 0 , 3 6 5 Mastroianni, L . , 4 7 8 Matarazzo, J . D., 210 M a t h e w s , J . R., 5 2 9 Matsui, D., 2 1 7 M a t s u m o t o , M., 2 5 3 M a t s u o k a , J . K., 458 Matsushita, S., 85
553
554
T H E HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY HANDBOOK Matt, K . S., 59 Mattar, M . , 7 Matthews, A. K., 4 5 7 Matthews, D. Α., 4 9 5 Matthews, K., 4 7 1 Matthews, Κ. Α., 172, 253 M a t t s o n , R. H . , 2 1 7 M a x t o n , D. G., 398 Maxwell, P. R., 399 Mayberry, D., 177 Mayer-Davis, E. J . 3 1 0 Mayer, Ε. Α., 4 0 2 , 4 1 1 Mayer, T. G., 2 3 9 , 2 4 0 Mazzaferri, E. L . , 4 8 9 Mazzuca, S. Α., 213 M c A d o o , W. G., 2 1 4 McAlindon, T., 155 McAuliffe, T., 388 McBride, C . , 61 McCabe, M., 360 M c C a l l u m , D. M . , 177 McCarter, R., 108 McCarthy, B. W., 3 6 9 McCarthy, D . M . , 105 McCaskill, C . C , 3 1 5 McCauley, C , 3 6 4 (
M c C h a r g u e , D. E . , 102, 103, 106, 108, 109, 113, 114 McClellan, P., 2 6 1 McClelland, D. C , 34 M c C o r m a c k , W. M . , 3 6 6 M c C r a c k e n , L . M . , 24, 2 3 9 , 4 5 7 M c C r a d y , B. S., 89 M c C r a e , R. R., 335 M c C u b b i n , J . Α., 3 0 7 McCulloch, D. K., 306 McCulloch, J . Α., 2 3 8 McCullough, A. R., 368 McCurley, I., 59 McDaniel, J . S., 339 McDaniel, K. L . , 88 McDaniel, S. H . , 34, 3 5 , 36, 3 7 , 67, 6 8 , 69, 3 9 1 , 3 9 2 , 393 M c D o n o u g h , P., 4 7 4 M c D o w a l l , A. C , 3 3 6 McDowell, A. J . , 368 McDowell, B. J . , 5 2 0 M c E w a n , B. S., 170 McEwen, B. S., 4 5 9 McGehee, D. S., 105 McGinness, J . , 3 McGovern, Κ. B., 366 M c G u i g a n , F. J . , 178 McGuire, L . , 66 McHorney, C. Α., 3 3 2 , 3 4 2 , 343 M c H u g h , P. R., 21 M c K a y , D. W., 2 6 0 McKendrick, M . W., 3 9 9 McKenzie, M . , 336 M c K e y , B., 2 5 4
M c K i b b o n , Κ. Α., 2 1 4 McKinlay, J . B., 3 6 7 McKinlay, S. M . , 4 7 1 McLellan, A. T., 197 M c L e o d , C. C , 34 M c L e o d , J . , 473 M c M a h o n , J . , 88 McMillan, C , 3 3 0 M c M u r r y , J . F., 3 0 7 , 308, 309 M c N a g n y , S. E . , 2 6 5 M c N a i r , D. M . , 3 4 2 McPhee, S. J . , 4 6 1 M c V a r y , K. T., 368 Meagher, B. R., 2 3 4 M e a n a , M . , 366 Mears, B., 3 3 5 Mediansky, L . , 3 3 5 Meenan, R. F., 156 Meichenbaum, D., 1 8 1 , 2 1 6 , 241 Meilahn, E., 133 Meilan, Ε. N., 2 5 8 Meinders, A. E . , 136 Meisler, A. W., 3 6 9 Meissner, J . S., 4 0 9 Melisaratos, N., 334 Mellinger, B. C , 3 6 7 Mellinger, G. D., 4 2 5 Melton, L . J . , 4 9 0 Meltzer, H. Y., 106 Melzack, R., 2 3 0 , 2 3 8 , 2 4 2 , 3 4 3 , 4 0 4 , 4 1 0 Mencher, P., 2 3 4 Mendall, Μ . Α., 399 Menditto, Α. Α., 528 Mendlowicz, M . V., 34 Menkes, M . S., 253 Mensah, G. Α., 253 M e n z a , Μ . Α., 3 6 8 , 4 8 7 M e r c a d o , A. M . , 172 Mercier, L . , 4 9 1 Merimsky, O., 3 4 1 Merrick, G. S., 3 6 0 Merskey, H . , 2 2 9 , 2 4 1 Mertz, FL, 4 0 2 , 4 0 3 Meshkinpour, FL, 4 0 4 Mesmer, C , 4 1 4 Messerli, F. FL, 2 5 7 Meston, C . M . , 3 6 0 , 3 6 2 Metz, Μ . Ε . , 3 6 0 , 3 6 5 , 369 Metzack, R., 4 0 3 Meulman, J . , 63, 288 Meyer, D., 44 Meyer, R. E . , 79, 80 Meyer, T. J . , 66 Meyers, H . F., 4 6 1 Meyler, W. J . , 2 4 2 Michael, R. T., 3 5 9 , 3 6 2 Michaels, S., 359, 3 6 2 Michal, M . M . , 334 Michie, S., 33 Miles, S., 493
Name Index Milesi, C , 151 Milgrom, J . , 33 Miller, Β. Α., 4 5 7 Miller, C , 331 Miller, D., 3 6 0 Miller, D. S., 3 3 1 Miller, E. R., 2 6 1 Miller, G., 64 Miller, J . P., 4 9 1 Miller, Κ. M . , 147, 148, 149 Miller, L . J . , 4 7 6 Miller, Ν. E . , 56 Miller, Ν. H . , 2 6 5 Miller, N. S., 83, 192 Miller, W. R., 4 2 - 4 5 , 4 7 - 5 2 , 6 2 , 1 1 1 , 197, 199, 2 0 2 , 2 1 2 Milner, P., 104 Milon, H . , 2 5 7 Minor, Μ . Α., 156 Miscovich, S. J . , 2 1 1 Mishra, Α., 368 Misra, D., 2 6 5 Mitchell, E . , 4 7 2 Mitchell, J . E . , 108 Mockett, S., 155 Mockrin, S. C . , 2 5 2 Moffitt, P. F., 4 2 6 Moffitt, T . E . , 4 7 0 M o k d a d , A. H . , 126, 127 Moldofsky, H . , 4 2 6 Molinari, S., 2 1 Molinari, S. P., 193 Montain, S. J . , 2 6 1 Montaner, J . S. G., 389 Monterrosa, Α., 4 5 7 M o n t o y e , H . J . , 148 M o o d y , D. S., 2 4 0 M o o r e , L . , 393 M o o r e , M . S., 154 Morales, Α., 3 7 5 M o r e n o , J . D., 5 1 6 M o r e n o , R., 2 3 9 M o r g a n , D. L . , 32 M o r g a n , K., 4 9 1 M o r g a n , R. K . , 3 2 Morgenstern, H . , 336 Morin, C. M., 429, 430, 432 M o r i s a k i , N., 308 Morisky, D . E . , 2 1 4 , 2 1 7 Morisky, D. M . , 6 1 Morley, J . E . , 4 9 2 Morley, S., 24 Morley, S. M . , 4 1 4 Morokoff, P. J . , 3 6 2 Morrill, R. G., 2 9 4 Morris, J . , 3 5 , 36 Morris, J . Α., 398 Morris, R., 238 Morris, T., 3 3 1 , 3 3 2 Morris-Yates, A. D., 4 0 0
Morrison, Α., 4 9 2 Morrison, J . R., 4 8 8 , 4 8 9 Morrison, R. L . , 4 9 0 M o r r o w , J . R., 162 M o r s e , E . , 103 Morse, J . M., 490 Mortensen, P. B., 333 M o r t o n , N . W., 22 Moses, H., 522 M o s k o , S. S., 428 M o s s , Η. B., 85 M o s s , Ν. E . , 4 7 4 Moulin, D. E . , 241 M o u n t , Κ. Α., 51 M o u r a , N . G. M . , 2 1 4 Mourice, J . , 63 M o y , T. F., 4 6 0 Moye, J . , 494 M o y e r s , T. B., 4 4 M u d d e , A. N., 62 Mueller, J . , 343 M u i r , K . R., 155 Mulhall, J . P., 368 Mullen, P. E . , 4 7 9 Muller, H . F., 4 8 8 Muller, M . J . , 368 Mullooly, J . P., 3 3 5 M u l r o w , P. J . , 2 6 4 Multiple Risk Factor Intervention Trial Research G r o u p , 2 1 6 Munakata, J . , 402 M u n d a l , R., 2 5 2 M u n d y , W. R., 107 M u n o z , F., 103 Murai, M., 367 M u r a m a t s u , T., 85 M u r a y a m a , M . , 85 Murphy, J . G., 504 Murphy, J . K . , 2 8 9 Murphy, S. L . , 325 Murray, M . , 3 3 0 M u r r a y , Μ . Α., 3 0 9 , 4 9 3 Murrell, S. Α., 493 Musselman, D. L . , 3 3 9 M u s t , Α., 1 2 7 M u s t , S., 129 Myer, E . , 2 3 5 Myers, H . F., 4 5 9 Myers, M . G., 105, 2 5 6 Myers, N . F., 2 6 2 Myers, R. E . , 4 6 1 Myers, W. Α., 3 6 4 Nabe, C., 495 Nadeau, L., 24 Nadelson, C. C . , 4 7 4 N a g o s h i , C . T., 84 N a j a r i a n , B., 6 0 Nakashima, J . , 367 N a k a s h i m a , Y., 2 5 8
555
556
T H E HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY HANDBOOK Naliboff, B. D., 3 1 5 , 4 0 2 , 4 0 3 , 4 7 7 N a m i , R., 152 N a r a y a n , Κ. M . V., 3 1 4 N a r b r o , K . , 137 N a t h , S. R., 5 3 1 National Center for Health Statistics, 4 5 6 , 4 5 7 National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute, 6 5 , 126, 130, 1 3 1 , 132, 133, 134, 136, 137, 138, 2 0 8 , 4 5 7 , 504 National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases ( N I D D K ) , 6 2 , 3 0 4 , 3 0 5 , 3 1 2 National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, 44 National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA), 188, 192 National Institutes of Health (NIH), 6 1 , 6 2 , 126, 130, 1 3 1 , 132, 133, 134, 136, 137, 138, 154, 4 3 4 National Office of Vital Statistics, 56 National Sleep Foundation, 4 3 6 Natural Medicines Comprehensive Database, 434, 435 Naugle, A. E . , 528 Neal, D. L . , 30 Neal, M . J . , 106 Neale, Α. V., 3 0 7 , 335 Nealey, J . R., 5 7 Nease, D. E . , J r . , 64 Neaton, J . D., 2 5 2 Nedeljkovic, S. S., 2 4 1 Neijt, J . P., 343 Neish, N., 2 3 2 Nelson, A. R., 4 5 6 Nelson-Gray, R. O., 514 Nelson, R. O., 4 7 7 , 528 Nemeroff, C. B., 3 3 9 Nesbitt, S., 2 5 5 Nesselroade, J . R., 4 5 9 N e s s m a n , D. G., 2 1 0 Nestler, E. J . , 85 Nesvacil, L . , 369 Nettelbladt, P., 3 6 9 Nettleman, L . , 30 Neundorfer, B., 368 Neutel, J . M . , 2 5 7 Newlin, D. B., 83, 84 N e w m a n , A. B., 4 9 1 N e w m a n , B., 103, 111 N e w m a n , C . F., 90 Newman, E., 479 N e w m a n , J . , 83 N e w m a n n , J . P., 4 9 3 Newton, C. R., 2 3 1 , 2 3 9 , 243 Newton-John, T., 2 4 2 Newton, P., 3 1 0 N i a u r a , R., 6 1 , 6 2 , 106 Niaz, U., 343 Niazi, N., 4 0 2 Nicassio, P. M . , 57, 2 3 9 Nichol, M . B., 191 Nicholas, M . K., 3 0 , 33
Nickens, H . W., 4 5 6 Nieto, F. J . , 2 6 0 Nishimoto, R., 3 3 5 Nishino, K., 2 5 8 Nissenkorn, I., 368 Nitz, A. J . , 183 Nitzan, R., 336 N o a c k , R., 136 Nolen-Hoeksema, S., 473 N o m u r a , Α. M . Y., 3 3 2 N o o n a n , W. C , 44 N o r b i a t o , G., 2 6 2 Norcross, J . C , 4 3 , 92 Norell, S. E . , 2 1 7 N o r m a n , L . , 35 Norris, J . L . , 158 Norris, S. L . , 63, 3 1 4 Northern California Neurobehavioral G r o u p , 2 2 Northrop, L., 494 N o s e d a , G., 136 N o t m a n , M . T., 4 7 4 N o u w e n , Α., 3 0 9 Nowinski, J . , 90 N o y , S., 2 1 Noyes, R., J r . , 3 4 1 Nugent, C. Α., 2 1 0 Nunnally, J . C , 2 1 9 Nurnberg, H . G., 3 6 0 Nurnberger, J . I., 83, 134 N u s s b a u m , P. D . , 2 0 Nygren, M . , 4 9 4 Obarzanek, E . , 4 6 0 O'Brien, B., 505 O'Brien, C. P., 197 Ockene, I. S., 2 6 5 Ockene, J . K., 6 2 , 109, 112, 113, 114, 2 1 5 Ockene, S., 2 1 4 O'Donnell, W. E . , 2 4 O'Donoghue, W. T., 3 6 0 O'Donohue, W., 364 O'Donohue, W. T., 5 0 5 Oexmann, M. J . , 460 O ' G o r m a n , T. W., 84 O ' H a r a , M . W., 67, 4 7 6 Ohayon, M . , 4 9 1 O k a m o t o , K., 2 5 3 O'Keefe, Ε. Α., 3 9 9 Okifuji, Α., 2 4 3 , 2 4 4 Olawuyi, F., 399 Olden, K. W., 4 0 6 Oldridge, Ν. B., 1 5 1 , 2 1 4 Olds, J . , 104 O'Leary, Α., 4 7 2 Oliveria, S. Α., 156, 2 6 5 Ollendick, T. H . , 34 Olubuyide, I. O., 399 O'Malley, J . E . , 3 3 1 O'Malley, S. S., 87 Oparil, S., 2 5 7 , 2 5 9 , 2 6 0
Orleans, C. T., 2 1 5 , 2 1 6 Ornish, D., 64 O r o s a n , P., 137 Orth-Gomér, K., 4 7 2 Osborn, M . , 366 Oslin, D. W., 4 9 0 Ossorio, R. C , 2 1 6 Osterweil, D., 4 9 2 Ostfeld, A. M . , 3 3 3 , 336 Ostroff, J . , 336 Oswald, I., 4 9 1 Otto-Salaj, L. L . , 3 8 9 Overall, S., 2 2 Overholser, J . C , 91 Owen, A. E . , 177 Owen, K., 3 0 5 Owen, N., 162 O x m a n , A. D., 2 1 0 Padgett, D. K., 4 5 8 Paffenbarger, R. S., 2 6 1 Pagani, M . , 2 6 2 Paige, N. M . , 368 Palace, E. M . , 3 6 0 Palmer, B., 4 9 3 Palsson, O., 3 9 9 Pankratz, L . , 188 Paoletta, Α., 2 5 4 Paoletti, N . , 32 Parati, G., 2 5 7 Paris, M . , 30 Park, D., 4 8 6 Park, J . , 152 Park,J. J . , 261 Parker-Dominguez, T., 4 5 9 Parran, T., 189 Parran, T. P., 190, 196, 197 Parrino, L . , 4 9 1 Parrott, A. C . , 106 Parsons, O., 153 Parzer, P., 4 7 9 Pasacreta, J . V., 4 8 9 Pasis, S., 4 9 2 Paskett, E. D., 4 6 1 Pate, R. R., 2 6 0 Patel, C . , 180 Patenaude, A. F., 15 Paterson, D., 389 Patino, P., 105 Patrick, C . , 458 Patrick, D. L . , 4 1 1 Patterson, T., 3 Patterson, T. L . , 2 0 9 Patton, G. K., 32 Paty, J . Α., 103 Paul, G., 5 1 6 Paul, G. L . , 528 Paul, L . C . , 309 Paulauskas, S., 306 Pauporte, J . , 3 6 0
Pavlik, V. N., 2 6 4 Payne, T. J . , 110 Pearcy, J . N., 4 5 6 Pearl, D., 3 3 6 Pearlman, R. Α., 3 2 9 Peck, J . R., 2 3 9 Pederson, H . E . , 4 8 9 Pedula, K. L . , 3 0 5 Pegg, S. H . , 4 7 0 Peirce, N . S., 156, 157 Pelcovitz, D., 4 7 9 Pena, B., 368 Penman, D. T., 3 3 1 Pennebaker, J . W., 5 0 7 Penzien, D. B., 2 4 2 Pepe, M . S., 129 Pereira, M . Α., 156 Perkins, Κ. Α., 1 0 2 , 106 Perkins, R. H . , 2 3 6 Perlis, M . L . , 4 2 6 Perri, M . G., 154, 2 1 4 Perrin, J . M . , 127 Perskey, V. W., 333 Person, E . S., 364 Pert, Α., 104 Pertschuk, M . , 106 Pervin, L. Α., 3 6 7 Pescosolido, Β. Α., 2 1 5 Peternell, Α., 4 7 1 Peters, A. L . , 2 1 6 Peters, J . C . , 128 Petersen, L . , 3 4 1 , 343 Petersen, R. C . , 193 Peterson, E . W., 4 9 1 Peterson, L . , 2 4 1 Peterson, P. L . , 5 0 4 Peterson, T., 3 1 0 Petrella, R. J . , 2 6 1 Pettinati, H . M . , 197 Pettingale, K . W., 3 3 1 , 3 3 2 Pettitt, D . J . , 129 Peyrot, M . , 306 Peyrot, M . F., 3 0 7 , 3 0 8 , 3 0 9 , 3 1 0 , 3 1 3 , 3 1 5 , 316, 318 Pfaus, J . G., 3 6 0 Pfluger, H . , 368 Phelan, S., 138 Philbrick, J . T., 9 Phillips, C. L . , 3 1 0 Phillips, M . M . , 5 0 4 Phillips, R. L . , 3 6 2 Phillips, S. F., 4 0 2 Pi-Sunyer, F. X . , 130, 3 0 5 Piasecki, M . P., 519 Piasecki, T. M . , 106 Pickering, E . , 2 8 9 Pickering, T., 2 6 2 , 2 6 5 Pickering, T. G., 56, 2 5 6 Pierce, A. P., 365 Piesse, C . , 4 0 5
558
T H E HEALTH P S Y C H O L O G Y HANDBOOK Pieterse, M . E . , 62 Pietrobelli, Α., 129 Pihl, R. O., 87 Pilowsky, I., 2 4 , 4 2 6 Pincomb, G., 2 5 4 Pingitore, G., 108, 128 Pinkerton, S. D., 3 8 9 Pinto, Β. M . , 154 Piper, M . E . , 107, 108 Pither, C. E . , 2 3 5 Pivik, J . , 2 3 9 Pivik, R. T., 4 9 1 Pizzi, L. T., 533 Planthara, P., 533 Plas, E . , 368 Plaud, J . J . , 360 Polatin, P. B., 2 4 0 Polednak, A. P., 330 Polen, M . R., 88 Polich, J . , 86 Poling, Α., 189 Polit, D. F., 4 7 1 Polivy, P., 137 Polk, D. E . , 2 8 1 Pollard, T. M . , 172 Pollets, D., 360 Pollock, M . L . , 2 6 1 Pomerleau, C. S., 103, 104, 105 Pomerleau, O., 106 Pomerleau, O. F., 103, 104, 105, 107 Pomidossi, G., 2 5 7 Ponticas, Y., 365 Pool, D., 4 9 4 Pope, C . R., 335 Pope, K. S., 4 4 6 Popkin, M . K., 340, 341 Populla-Vardi, C , 192 Porcellati, C . , 2 5 7 Porcher, H . C . , 102, 103 Pories, W. J . , 137 Porjesz, B., 86 Porst, H . , 368 Portenoy, R. K., 2 4 1 , 343 Porter, M . R., 339 Posavac, E. J . , 5 2 5 Poston, W. S., 136 Poston, W. S. C , 106 Potter, J . F., 2 6 0 Potters, L., 368 Powell, R. D., 3 6 5 Powell, S. H . , 24 Poynard, T., 4 1 2 , 4 9 2 Pratt, L. Α., 2 9 5 Pratt, M . , 155 Price, D. W., 34 Price, R., 393 Price, R. Α., 128, 129 Priestman, S. G., 3 3 2 Priestman, T. J . , 3 3 2 Prigatano, G. P., 153
Prior, Α., 4 1 4 Prior, A. C. S., 403 Pritchard, W. S., 102 Prochaska, J . , 111 Prochaska, J . O., 4 3 , 92, 110, 2 1 1 , 2 1 2 Professional Liaison Committee (Australia) Project T e a m , 28 Project M A T C H Research G r o u p , 89 Promax, H . , 2 5 7 Pronk, N . P., 5 0 2 Prudhomme, D., 136 Pruitt, S. D., 208 Prus-Wisniewski, R., 2 5 8 Pryor, J . L., 3 6 9 Puder, K . L . , 4 1 1 Pugh, J . Α., 4 5 7 Puhl, R., 137 Pukkala, E . , 156 Pulvirenti, L . , 105 Pyczynski, S., 2 1 0 Q u a d a g n o , D., 364 Quesada, J . R., 341 Quick, J . , 5 2 2 Quick, R. E . , 44 Quiles, J . , 2 6 5 Quine, L . , 66 Quinn, E. P., 110 Quinsey, V. L . , 3 6 0 Rabe-Hesketh, S., 2 3 5 Rabkin, J . G., 3 9 0 , 3 9 1 , 3 9 2 R a c i o p p o , M . W., 62 Raczek, A. E . , 3 3 2 Raczynski, J . M . , 2 5 9 , 5 0 4 Radcliffe, Κ. Α., 105 Radley, Α., 32 Radzius, Α., 102 Ragusea, S. Α., 5 0 7 Rahe, R., 173 Raikkonen, K., 2 5 5 Raine, Α., 87 Rajfer, J . , 368 Ramsden, V., 2 6 2 Rand, C , 2 0 8 R a n d , C . S., 208 R a n d , L. I., 306 Randall, R., 32 Rankinen, T., 128 R a o , Κ. N., 86 R a o , S., 24 Rapkin, A. J . , 4 7 7 R a p o p o r t , A. M . , 2 3 6 Rapoport, L., 258 R a p o p o r t , Y., 3 2 7 , 3 3 2 , 3 3 7 R a p p , S., 329 R a p p , S. R., 208 R a p p a p o r t , Ν . B., 150 Rasanen, P., 8 7 Rask, K. J . , 2 6 5
Name Index Ratcliffe, Μ . Α., 334 Rather, B. C , 88 Ravussin, E . , 129, 154 Rawling, P. J . , 315 Ray, N . F., 303 Raybould, Η. E . , 4 1 1 R a y m o n d , S. Α., 2 4 1 Raynor, W. J . , 333 R e a d , N . W., 3 9 9 Redd, W. H . , 3 3 0 Redding, C . Α., 3 3 0 Reed, C. S., 5 2 5 Reed, D . M . , 3 3 2 Reeves, R. Α., 2 5 6 Reeves, S., 3 7 Regan, P. C , 3 6 0 Regimbeau, C , 4 1 2 Reheiser, E. C , 106 Reid, J . , 3 0 6 Reid, J. B., 91 Reinert, D. F., 199 Reisin, E . , 2 5 9 Reiss, D., 3 3 6 Reiter, J . , 137 Reitsma, A. M . , 5 1 6 Rejeski, W. J . , 157, 158, 162 Relman, A. S., 5 2 5 Resch, F., 4 7 9 Resnick, H . , 3 4 1 Resnick, Η . E . , 3 1 0 Resnick, M . , 192 Resnicow, K., 44, 62 Réthelyi, J . M . , 4 7 7 Reus, V. I., 103 Revenson, Τ. Α., 3 3 1 Revenstorf, D., 5 3 2 Reynard, A. M . , 4 9 0 Reynolds, P., 3 3 5 Reynolds, W. M . , 23 Rho, J . P., 4 9 0 Rhoads, K., 290 Rhyne, C , 192 Riba, M . B., 30 Ribisl, P. M . , 2 0 8 Ricciardelli, L . Α., 88 Rich, M . , 4 Rich, Μ . Α., 2 3 0 Rich, M . W., 288 Richards, K. J . , 127 Richardson, H . , 3 3 5 Richmond, M . , 106 Richter-Heinrich, E . , 2 6 2 Richter, J . E . , 2 8 1 Richter, S., 368 Rickels, K . , 4 7 7 , 478 Ridker, P. M . , 2 8 0 Rifai, N., 2 8 0 Rifkin, Α., 4 8 8 Riggs, B. L . , 4 9 0 Riley, J . F., 243
Riley, J . L . , 2 3 1 Rimer, E. T., 3 2 6 R i m m , Α. Α., 151 Rincon, J . , 156 Riner, W., 128 Ringdall, G. I., 3 3 1 Ringel, Α., 368 Rios, Α., 3 4 1 Rischmann, P., 3 6 0 Riskind, J . H . , 2 3 1 Rita, H . , 332 Ritter, S., 104 Ritterband, L. M . , 60, 316 Rivara, F. P., 4 2 , 62 Rivera, M . , 2 6 5 Rivera-Meza, B. L . , 106 Rizvi, K . , 368 Robbins, R. Α., 2 4 0 Robins, L. N . , 3 5 9 Robinson, J. H . , 102, 106 Robinson, M . D., 114 Robinson, M . E . , 2 3 1 , 2 3 2 , 2 4 0 Robinson, T. E . , 8 5 , 103, 104, 1 0 5 , 107 Robison, J . , 23 Robles, T. F., 66 Rochester, C . L . , 153 Rockhold, R. W., 2 5 5 Rodin, G. M . , 309 Rodriguez, B., 4 6 1 Rodriguez, H . S., 86 Rodriguez, L. A. G., 398 Roehling, M . V., 128 Roehrich, L . , 88 Rogentine, G. N . , 3 3 1 Rogers, C . R., 4 8 , 92 Rogers, J . C . , 34, 2 4 4 Rogers, J . L . , 183 Rogers, L. K., 5 3 1 Rogler, L. H., 110 Rohan, K. J . , 4 7 6 Roland, M . , 2 3 8 Rolf, C . A. M . , 2 4 2 Rollnick, S., 4 2 - 5 1 , 62, 1 1 1 , 197, 199, 2 0 2 , 2 1 2 Romano, J . M., 234, 239 R o m a n s - C l a r k s o n , S. E . , 4 7 9 Ronalds, C. M . , 4 7 7 Roose, S., 360 R o o s e , S. P., 368 Root, M . P. P., 4 7 4 R o s a m o n d , W. D., 4 6 0 R o s a s , M . , 106 R o s e , J . E . , 104, 107 Rosen, E. C . , 3 6 2 , 3 6 5 Rosen, J . C . , 137, 138 Rosen, R., 3 6 0 Rosen, R. C , 3 6 7 , 368 Rosenfeld, B., 329 Rosenman, R. H . , 2 7 9 , 2 8 8 Rosenstiel, A. K . , 2 3 9 , 4 1 0 Rosenstock, I. M . , 2 1 1 , 2 1 2
559
560
T H E HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY HANDBOOK Rosenthal, R., 531 Rosenthal, T. L . , 2 8 9 Rosin, R., 3 3 7 Roski, J . , 88 Rosner, B., 2 6 0 Rosomoff, H . , 2 3 4 , 2 4 1 Rosomoff, R. S., 2 4 1 R o s s , S. L . , 315 Rossi, J . S., 3 3 0 Rossi, P. H . , 5 2 5 Rossner, S., 136 Rostaing, L . , 360 Roter, D. L . , 63, 64, 2 1 4 , 2 1 5 , 2 6 5 Roth, S., 5 9 , 4 7 9 Rounsaville, B. J . , 65 Rouse, D., 2 3 2 Rowe, J . W., 4 5 9 , 4 8 5 Rowland, D. L . , 3 6 9 Rowland, J . H . , 331 Rowland, M . , 3 1 0 Rozanski, Α., 2 8 6 Rozensky, R. H . , 6, 10, 11 Rubel, E. C , 51 Rubin, Ε. H . , 308 Rubin, R. R., 306, 3 0 8 , 3 0 9 , 310, 3 1 3 , 3 1 5 , 3 1 6 , 318 Rudy, T. E . , 2 3 1 , 238 Ruff, G. Α., 4 7 9 Ruffin, R. E . , 4 8 8 Ruigozmez, Α., 398 R u m m a n s , Τ. Α., 2 8 2 Ruof, J . , 368 Rush, Α., 4 1 1 Rush, A. J . , 64 Ruskin, J . N., 2 8 2 Rusktailis, M . , 4 7 6 Rutter, D. R., 66 Ryan, J . J , 2 2 Ryden, M . B., 4 9 3 Ryff, C. D., 4 5 9 Ryujin, D . H . , 458 Rzepski, B., 399 Sabourin, S., 4 7 8 Sacks, F. M . , 2 5 9 Sadock, B. J . , 4 7 5 Saelens, Β. E . , 148 Salbe, A. D., 154 Salin-Pascual, R. J . , 106 Salkovskis, P., 4 7 6 Salles, C . , 2 2 Sallis, J . F., 148, 162, 2 0 9 Sallis, J . , J r . , 3 Salmon, D., 2 8 , 36, 37 Salonen, J . T., 2 5 4 Salvadori, C . , 364 Salzman, C . , 192, 4 9 0 Samet, J . M . , 3 3 5 Sammons, M., 507 Samson, J . Α., 310 Samuel-Hodge, C. D., 3 1 0
Sanchez, R. Α., 368 Sanchez-Reyes, L . , 136 Sanderson, R. S., 138 Sandler, R. S., 3 9 9 Sandy, J . M . , 88 Sannibale, C , 80 Santor, D., 2 3 2 Santos, Α., 32 Santos, J . , 4 9 0 Sarason, I. G., 177 Saris, W. Η. M . , 148 Sarramon, J . P., 3 6 0 Sarwer, D. B., 137, 138, 2 5 8 Satariano, W. Α., 3 3 5 Saudek, C . D., 315 Saunders, R., 128 Saunders, R. D., 2 3 4 Savidge, C . J . , 478 Schaefer, C . , 177 Schafer, E . , 59 Schafer, M . , 4 7 9 Schafer, R. B., 59 Schaffner, L . , 331 Scharff, D. E . , 365 Scharff, L . , 4 0 4 , 4 0 5 , 4 1 4 , 5 2 0 Scheidt, S., 2 8 0 Scheier, M . F., 331 Schein, R. L . , 153 Schiavi, R. C . , 3 0 6 , 3 5 9 , 3 6 4 Schillaci, G., 2 5 7 Schipperheijn, J . M . , 4 7 5 Schlecte, J . Α., 4 7 6 Schlesinger, H . J . , 458 Schmaling, Κ. B., 44 Schmid, C . H . , 63 Schmidt, M . , 2 0 Schmidt, P., 334 Schmidt, U., 4 7 7 , 4 7 8 Schmidtke, Α., 4 7 9 Schnack, B., 4 7 9 Schneekloth, T., 14 Schneider, M . , 3 6 9 Schneider, R. H . , 2 6 2 Schnoll, S. H . , 199, 4 7 1 Schoeller, D. Α., 129 Schoenborn, C.A., 147 Schofield, W., 4 Schoicket, S. L . , 4 3 2 Schonfield, J . , 3 3 1 Schover, L. R., 3 6 0 , 369 Schreiner-Engel, P., 3 0 6 , 3 6 4 Schroeder, C , 36, 6 7 , 68 Schuckit, Μ . Α., 83, 88 Schueler, G., 3 3 4 Schulberg, H . , 64 Schulberg, H . C , 64, 6 5 , 66 Schuld, V., 4 7 9 Schulman, Β. Α., 2 1 5 Schultheis, K . , 2 4 1 Schultz, J . H . , 179
Name Index Schulz, R., 59, 3 4 5 , 4 9 2 Schuster, J . M . , 30 Schuster, M . M . , 4 0 1 , 4 0 5 Schwartz, G. E . , 56, 180, 2 1 0 Schwartz, I., 3 3 7 Schwartz, M . F., 3 6 5 Schwartz, S., 33 Schwartz, S. M . , 2 8 1 , 2 8 2 , 2 8 5 Schwarz, S. P., 4 0 5 , 4 0 9 Schwebel, A. I., 2 3 9 Sciolla, Α., 3 9 2 , 393 Scollay, J . , 516 Scott, F. B., 369 Seaberg, E. C . , 4 8 9 , 4 9 0 Sears, S. F., 2 8 2 Seeber, M . , 365 Seeley, J . R., 4 9 3 S e e l y , J . F., 493 Seeman, A. Z . , 87 Seeman, M . , 8 7 Seeman, T., 4 5 9 Seeman, T. E . , 4 5 9 S E E R Program, 3 2 5 Sees, K. L . , 103 Seevers, M . H . , 83 Sega, R., 151 Segraves, R. T., 3 5 9 Seibert, S., 192 Seidel, K. D., 129 Seidman, S. N . , 3 6 0 , 368 Seime, R. J . , 11 Selby, V., 2 4 1 Selenta, C . , 2 5 6 Self, D. W., 85 Sellschopp, Α., 336 Seltzer, M . L . , 24 Semet, C . , 4 7 4 Serdula, M . , 126 Servaes, P., 334 Severson, Η. H . , 102 Severson, K . T., 329 Sevick, Μ . Α., 208 Sewell, M . C . , 3 9 2 Sexton, M . M . , 5 0 4 Seydel, E . R., 62 Shabsigh, R., 368 Shaked, S., 3 3 1 , 3 3 7 Shaked, T., 3 3 1 Shalev, M . , 368 Shaper, A. G., 151 Shapiro, A. P., 2 6 2 Shapiro, D., 56 Shapiro, M . E . , 368 Shapiro, P. Α., 288 Shapiro, R. M . , 2 1 4 Shapiro, Y., 157 Sharkansky, E. J . , 86 Sharpe, L., 2 4 2 Sharpe, P. Α., 3 0 9 Shatford, L. Α., 4 7 8
Shaw, B., 4 1 1 Shaw, B. F., 64 Shea, S., 2 6 5 Shearer, E . C . , 4 7 0 Sheehan, D. V., 2 9 3 Sheftell, F. D., 2 3 6 Sheikh. J . , 4 9 3 Sheiner, L. B., 102 Shekelle, R. B., 333 Sheldrick, R. C . , 5 3 1 Shelton, B. J . , 399 Shelton, D. R., 3 3 5 Shenenberger, D. W., 368 S H E P Cooperative Research G r o u p , 2 1 6 Shepherd, M . D . , 368 Sher, K. J . , 83, 86 Sherbenou, R. J . , 2 2 Sherbourne, C . D., 4 1 1 Sherman, J . E . , 103 Sherman, J . H . , 2 5 4 Sherwood, Α., 6 1 , 152 Shiffman, S., 103, 104, 1 1 1 , 2 8 1 Shifren, J . L . , 3 6 0 Shirai, M . , 368 Shires, Α., 3 6 0 Shnidman, S. R., 56 Shows, W. D., 8 Shuster, E . , 5 1 5 Shutty, M . S., 2 3 8 , 2 3 9 Sidwa, Η. K., 398 Siegal, W. C , 152 Siegel, B. S., 3 3 6 Siegel, J . M . , 88, 177 Siegel, W. C , 2 6 1 Siegelaub, A. B., 2 6 0 Siegrist, B., 336 Sigmon, S., 4 7 7 Sigmon, S. T . , 4 7 6 Sikkema, K. J . , 389 Silberstein, S. D . , 189 Silva, F., 528 Silva, P. Α., 4 7 0 Silverman, D. H . , 4 0 3 Simkin-Silverman, L. R., 133, 2 5 8 Simon, Κ. M . , 2 1 0 , 2 1 1 Simon, R., 4 7 2 Simpson, K., 3 0 7 Singer, B., 4 5 9 Singer, Β. H . , 4 5 9 Singh, B. S., 188 Sinha, R., 87 Sipski, M . , 3 6 0 Sivan, A. B., 2 0 Sjôstrôm, L . , 136, 137 Skillman, T . G., 4 8 9 Skinner, B. F., 2 1 0 Skinner, H . , 2 1 3 Skinner, Η. Α., 199 Skjorshammer, M . , 3 7 Skrabski, Α., 4 7 0 , 4 7 5
561
562
I
T H E HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY HANDBOOK
Skrinar, Α., 2 1 3 Slade, P., 4 7 7 , 4 7 8 Slaughter, J . R., 3 6 2 , 4 8 9 Slaven, L . , 4 7 2 Slavin, L . , 3 3 1 Slawsby, Ε. Α., 2 4 1 Slob, A. K., 368 Small, M . P., 368 Small, S. D., 32 Smedley, B. D., 4 5 6 , 4 5 8 Smerieri, Α., 491 Smets, Ε. Μ . Α., 343 Smit, B. J . , 4 7 1 Smit, E . , 161 Smith, Α., 4 8 5 Smith, A. P., 176 Smith, C . M . , 4 9 0 Smith, D. E . , 4 4 , 83, 89, 197 Smith, D. H . G., 2 5 7 Smith, G. R., 2 3 5 Smith, G. T., 88 Smith, H . , 306 Smith, S. J . , 63, 2 3 2 Smith, S. S., 112 Smith, T. L . , 88 Smith, T. W., 5 5 , 57, 69, 2 3 9 Smolak, L . , 127, 129 Sneeuw, K. C. Α., 3 4 0 Snellgrove, C. Α., 368 Snowden, L. R., 4 5 8 Snyder, P. J . , 2 0 Sobel, H . J . , 3 4 2 Sobell, L. C . , 5 1 , 92, 5 2 1 , 5 2 8 , 533 Sobell, M . B., 92, 5 2 1 , 5 2 8 , 533 Sobol, A. M . , 127 Society for Research on Nicotine and T o b a c c o Subcommittee on Biochemical Verification, 110, 111 Solomon, Α., 493 Sommer, I. E. C . , 4 8 7 Sommerfeld, Β. Κ., 106 Sondheimer, S. J . , 4 7 7 Sonino, N., 2 5 4 Sonksen, J . , 368 Sotile, W., 2 8 2 Sourkes, B. M . , 345 Sovereign, R. G., 4 5 Soyka, M . , 4 7 9 Spangler, J . G., 368 Sparrow, D., 2 8 9 Spector, I. P., 3 5 9 , 3 6 5 Spector, S. L . , 2 1 7 Spector, T. D., 155 Spelsberg, Α., 3 0 5 Spence, J . D., 2 6 2 Spence, S. H . , 2 4 2 Spiegel, D., 5 9 , 6 5 , 67, 3 3 6 , 3 4 5 , 346 Spielberger, C. D., 106, 108, 177, 4 1 1 Spira, J . L . , 5 9 , 3 4 5 Spitzer, R. L . , 110, 4 1 1
Sprafkin, J . N., 2 8 5 Spring, B., 106, 108, 128 St-Pierre, S., 136 Staats, N., 4 9 4 Stacey, B. R., 243 Stafford, M . , 213 Stallings, V. Α., 129 Stamler, J . , 2 5 9 Stamler, R., 2 5 2 , 2 5 8 , 2 5 9 Stampler, F. M . , 4 7 7 Stanish, W., 2 3 2 Starks, H . E . , 329 Starkstein, S. E . , 4 8 7 Stason, W., 5 0 4 Stavraky, K . M . , 3 3 5 Steckel, S. B., 2 1 4 Steed, L . , 23 Steele, C. M . , 83 Steele, R., 2 3 4 Steer, R. Α., 2 3 , 177 Steers, W., 368 Steger, H . G., 2 3 7 , 2 4 0 Stein, L., 104 Stein, M . B., 2 3 , 34 Steinberg, C . , 2 5 8 Steinberg, T. F., 3 0 6 Steinglass, P., 336 Stennie, K., 192 Stern, M . , 393, 504 Sternberg, J . Α., 131 Stetson, Β. Α., 2 9 2 Stevens, C. F., 105 Stevens, J . , 126 Stevens, V. J . , 2 5 8 , 4 6 0 Stewart, A. L . , 4 1 1 Stewart, J . , 87 Stewart, K . J . , 2 6 1 Stewart, Μ . Α., 2 1 5 , 3 3 5 Stewart, P., 478 Stewart, R. C . , 366 Stewart, W., 2 3 5 Stith, A. Y., 4 5 6 Stitzer, M . L . , 114 Stockwell, D. H . , 2 6 5 Stoddart, G. L . , 5 0 5 Stoeckle, J . D., 2 1 3 Stoehr, G. P., 4 9 0 Stoever, W. W., 2 8 9 Stoleru, S., 3 6 0 Stone, G. C . , 2 1 5 , 4 4 4 Stone, N . J . , 2 5 7 , 2 5 8 Story, M . , 128 Stossel, C . , 63 Stott, H . D., 113 Stoyva, J . M . , 179 Straatmeyer, A. J . , 3 1 5 Strain, J . J . , 3 0 Strassberg, D. S., 366 Strassels, S., 398 Strecher, V. J . , 61
Streissguth, A. P., 80 Striepe, M . I., 5, 6 Strohschein, L . , 4 7 4 Strosahl, K., 5 1 7 Stroud, M . W., 2 3 2 , 2 3 9 Strumpf, M . , 2 4 1 Strupp, H . H . , 89 Stuart, R. B., 56, 134 Stuart, S., 6 7 Stulp deGroot, C . , 34 Stunkard, A. J . , 127, 129, 131 Sturgis, E. T., 183 Sue, S., 458 Sullivan, M . , 3 2 9 Sullivan, M . D., 2 3 5 Sullivan, M . E . , 2 3 2 Sullivan, M . J . L . , 2 3 1 , 2 3 2 , 2 3 9 Sullivan, P. F., 127 Suis, J . , 2 4 0 Suis, J . M . , 4 0 5 Sumartojo, E . , 2 1 7 Summerson, J . H . , 368 Sung, Β. H . , 2 5 4 Sung, J . C . , 191 Supnick, J . Α., 110 Surtees, P. G., 3 3 5 Surwit, R. S., 64, 3 0 7 , 3 1 5 Sutherland, G., 113, 114 Sutlief, S., 3 6 0 Sutton, A. J . , 155 Suzuki, Y., 2 5 7 Swain, M . S., 2 1 4 Swan, G. E . , 106, 2 6 2 Swann, C. P., 364 Swanson, G . M . , 3 3 5 Swanson, R., 193, 194, 198 Sweet, J . J , 6, 10 Swencionis, C . , 2 5 9 Swingen, D. N., 3 6 4 Sydeman, S. J . , 177 Szedmâk, S., 4 7 5 Szklo, M . , 332 Tache, Y., 4 0 4 Taenzer, P., 2 6 2 T a k e d a , Κ., 2 5 5 Takeuchi, D. T., 4 5 8 T a k i m o t o , Y., 368 Talajic, M . , 4, 64, 2 8 8 , 5 0 4 Talbot, F., 3 0 9 Talley, N . J . , 3 9 8 , 399, 4 0 0 , 4 0 6 , 4 0 9 , 4 1 4 T a l p a z , M . , 341 Tarn, S. W., 368 T a n , S., 2 6 6 T a n g , T. S., 4 5 7 Tantleff-Dunn, S., 127 Tapert, S. F., 5 0 4 Taplin, S. H., 44 Tarter, R. E . , 86 Tataranni, P. Α., 129
Tataryn, D., 3 2 9 T a t e , R., 4 7 1 T a u b , E . , 398 T a u b , K. J . , 309 Taverner, D., 191 Taylor, C . B., 214, 2 6 2 Taylor, M . , 14 Taylor, R. R., 2 3 6 , 2 3 7 Taylor, S. E . , 2 1 4 Taylor, V. M . , 4 6 1 Teesson, M . , 34 Tekdogan, U. Y., 368 Tellegen, Α., 108 T e m o s h o k , L . , 3 3 1 , 333 Ten Have, T. R., 310 Tennant, C. C . , 4 0 5 Tennen, H . , 2 3 2 Terestman, N., 3 6 4 Teri, L . , 4 9 0 Terzano, M . G., 4 9 1 Thadhani, R., 2 6 0 Thamer, M . , 303 Theorell, T., 3 3 2 , 4 7 3 , 4 7 4 Thevos, A. K., 44 Thoits, P. Α., 4 7 3 T h o m a s , F. B., 4 8 9 T h o m a s , K. C , 4 5 8 Thomas, M. E., 456 T h o m p s o n , J . K., 127, 129, 137 T h o m p s o n , Κ. M . , 88 T h o m p s o n , R., 64 T h o m p s o n , W. G., 4 0 0 , 4 0 9 T h o m s o n , J . B., 8 3 , 84 T h o m s o n , Μ . Α., 2 1 0 Thoresen, C . J . , 32 Thorn, Β. E . , 2 3 0 , 2 3 1 , 2 3 2 , 2 3 9 , 2 4 3 , 2 4 5 Thune, I., 156 Tideiksaar, R., 4 9 0 Tiefer, L . , 3 6 0 Tiffany, S. T., 103, 107 Tiihonen, J . , 8 7 Tilley, B. C . , 3 3 5 Timm, G. W., 3 6 9 Tindale, R. S., 128 Tinetti, M . E . , 4 9 0 Tipp, J . E . , 83 Tissingh, G., 4 8 7 Tobert, Α., 306 Tobin, D. R., 4 9 5 T o d a r o , J . F., 2 8 2 T o d d , T., 31 Tollison, C . D., 3 6 7 Tomkins, S., 108 T o n a , F., 2 5 4 Toobert, D. J . , 3 1 5 , 3 1 6 Toomey, T. C., 477 Topitz, Α., 4 7 1 Torgerson, J . S., 137 T o r g r u d , L . J . , 127 Torre, T., 368
564
T H E HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY HANDBOOK Torrence, G. W., 505 Torres, D., 2 6 5 Toscani, M . R., 533 Toth, S., 188 Tovian, S. M . , 6 Townley, M . , 4 2 6 Tracy, J . I., 103 T r a s k , P. C , 2 8 1 , 298 Trauer, T., 28 Treasure, J . L . , 44 Treiber, F. Α., 253 Tremblay, Α., 136 Trials of Hypertension Prevention Collaborative Research G r o u p , 2 6 2 Triandis, H . C , 2 1 1 , 2 1 2 Trief, P. M . , 307, 310 Trigwell, P., 44 Tripp, D., 2 3 2 Tripp, D. Α., 2 3 2 Trotman, F., 4 9 5 Troughton, E . , 84 T r u a x , P., 5 3 2 Trull, T. J . , 86 Tryba, M . , 2 4 1 Tryon, W. W., 110 Trzepacz, P. T., 343 T s o h , J . Y., 103 Tsuang, J . W., 83 Tsuang, M . T., 80 Tsujimura, Α., 368 Tu, Χ., 426 Tucker, G. J . , 3 4 0 Tucker, J . Α., 105, 5 0 2 , 5 0 3 , 5 0 4 , 5 0 6 , 508 Tulin, M . , 4 0 2 Tuomilehto, J . , 311 Turcotte, Ν., 86 Tureck, R., 4 7 8 Turk, D. C , 2 1 6 , 2 2 9 , 2 3 1 , 2 3 8 , 2 4 0 , 241, 243, 244 Turkinton, R. W., 316 Turnbull, J . E . , 4 7 5 Turner, J . Α., 2 3 4 , 2 3 9 , 2 4 0 Turner, S., 365 Turnock, B. J . , 503 Tursky, B., 56 Tuxen, M . K., 341 Tweedy, D., 6 1 , 152 Twentyman, C. T., 2 1 4 Tylko, S. J . , 4 9 0 Tyrrell, D. A. J . , 176 Uddenberg, N., 369 Uhlenhuth, Ε. H . , 4 2 5 Ujimoto, V. K., 4 5 9 U.K. Trial of Early Detection of Breast Cancer G r o u p , 3 3 0 Ulmer, D., 293 Unterberger, H . , 43 Urban, J . P. G., 155 Urrows, S., 2 3 2
U.S. Department of Agriculture, 101 U.S. Department of Health and H u m a n Services ( D H H S ) , 1 0 1 , 104, 146, 155, 162, 3 1 3 , 4 5 6 , 475, 503, 510 U.S. National Commission for the Protection of H u m a n Subjects of Biomedical and Behavioral Research, 5 1 5 - 5 1 6 Ustiin, B., 82 Vaccarino, A. L . , 4 0 3 V a g g , P. R., 177 Valach, L . , 3 3 1 Valansia, P., 3 1 0 Valdez, Α., 4 6 1 Valdimarsdottir, Η. B., 3 3 9 , 3 4 0 Valenti, S. Α., 2 6 1 Valoski, Α., 59 van Berlo, W., 3 6 0 V a n den Broek, Α., 2 3 2 Van Der Kolk, B., 4 7 9 Van der Ploeg, H - M . , 3 3 4 Van Doudenhove, B., 2 3 2 Van Dyke, C , 343 Van Eeeden, Α., 4 0 2 Van Egeren, L . , 5, 6 van Elderen, T., 63, 288 van G o r p , W., 393 Van Horn, M . , 5 2 5 van Kippenberg, F. C . E . , 343 Vander, A. J . , 2 5 4 VanHoudenhove, B., 177 VanHuis, A. M . , 4 7 1 Vanltallie, T. B., 126, 128, 130, 134 Varady, Α., 103, 111 Vardi, Y., 368 Varner, M . W., 4 7 6 Vasey, M . W., 2 3 1 Vaughan, C , 4 2 5 , 4 3 8 Vellas, B., 4 9 1 Venkataramana, V., 86 Venner, R. M . , 238 Ventralla, Μ . Α., 183 Venturini, F., 191 Verbrugge, L . M . , 4 7 4 Verdecchia, P., 2 5 6 , 2 5 7 Verhulst, J . , 3 6 4 , 3 6 5 Vernon, M . , 4 8 6 Vernon, S. W., 335 Veronen, L. J . , 4 7 9 Vessey, J . T., 5 3 1 Vetter, H . , 3 3 4 Vetter, V. Α., 4 4 6 Vgontzas, A. N . , 172 Viera, A. J . , 368 Vietorisz, D., 306 Vihko, V., 156 Villeponteaux, L . Α., 4 7 9 Vingerhoets, Α., 3 3 4 Vinicor, F., 127 Visser, A. P., 339
Name Index Vitolins, M . Z . , 2 0 8 , 2 1 7 Vlaeyen, J . W . S., 2 3 1 Vogeltanz, N . D., 4 7 7 Vogt, O., 179 Vogt, R. Α., 137, 138 Vogt, T. M . , 3 3 5 Vollmer, W. M . , 2 5 9 Volpicelli, J . R . , 197 Von Bertalanffy, L . , 56 Von Korff, M . , 2 2 9 , 2 4 4 , 3 9 7 Vreugdenhil, G., 3 3 4 Vuchinich, R. E . , 105 Waddell, G., 2 3 8 Wadden, Τ. Α., 6 2 , 127, 130, 1 3 1 , 134, 137, 138, 153, 258 Wade, D . T . , 4 8 9 Wade, T. C , 2 1 4 Wagener, D. K., 4 5 6 Wagner, Ε. H . , 6 1 , 5 0 4 Wahl, P. W., 4 9 1 Waitzkin, H . , 213 Wakefield, P., 5 Wald, T. G., 3 4 1 Waldinger, M . D., 3 6 0 Walker, L . G., 3 3 4 Walker, M . , 151 Wall, P. D., 2 3 0 , 4 0 4 Wallace, J . I., 329 Wallace, M . , 4 9 2 Wallberg-Henriksson, H . , 156, 157 Wallen, K., 3 6 4 Waller, S., 3 1 5 Wanner, K., 3 6 0 Wallston, Κ. Α., 3 3 4 Walschlager, S. Α., 153 Walser, R. D . , 3 6 0 Walsh, M . J . , 85 Walsh, W. P., 3 3 5 Walsleben, J . Α., 4 2 9 Walter, S. D., 3 3 6 Walters, V., 4 7 4 Walton, V. Α., 4 7 9 Waltz, J . , 5 3 0 Wampold, Β. E . , 90 Wan, C. K., 2 4 0 Wang, C . Y., 308 Wang, G., 155, 156 Wang, T., 4 4 Wannamethee, S. G., 151 Wansley, R. Α., 2 8 9 Ward, C. L . , 2 2 Ward, J . R., 2 3 9 Ward, L. C . , 2 3 2 Ward, M . M . , 106, 2 6 2 Wardle, J . , 2 5 8 Ware, J . E . , 3 3 2 , 4 1 1 Warnock, J . , 28 Washburn, R. Α., 148, 149 Watkins, S. S., 104, 105
Watson, M . , 3 3 2 , 3 3 4 , 3 3 7 Watson, V., 4 9 2 Watt, G. C . , 2 5 4 Waxier-Morrison, N . , 3 3 5 Waylonis, G. W., 2 3 6 Weaver, Μ . Α., 2 4 0 Weber, Μ . Α., 2 5 7 Webster, J . , 125 Wedding, D . , 19 Wei, M . , 1 4 7 Weihs, K., 3 3 6 Weinberger, M . H . , 2 5 9 Weiner, D . W., 3 6 1 Weinstein, M . , 5 0 4 Weinstock, A. M . , 3 3 0 Weinstock, R. S., 3 0 7 Weisberg, R. B., 3 6 0 Weisman, A. D., 3 3 1 , 3 3 4 , 3 4 2 Weisman, C . S., 4 6 9 Weiss, J . N . , 3 6 7 Weiss, R. D., 188 Weiss, S. M . , 2 1 0 Weiss, S. T., 2 8 9 Weissler, K . , 327, 3 3 0 Weissman, M . M . , 6 5 Welgan, P., 4 0 4 Wellisch, D . K . , 3 4 0 Welsch, S. K . , 107 Wensloff, N . J . , 3 0 7 Wentworth, D., 2 5 2 Wenzel, Α., 6 7 Werner, P., 4 9 2 Wessells, H., 368 Wessely, S., 2 3 5 Wesson, D. R., 197 West, R . , 103, 106 Wewers, M . E . , 1 0 7 Whelton, P. K . , 2 5 2 , 2 5 9 , 2 6 0 , 4 6 0 Whitaker, R . C . , 129 Whitbourne, S., 4 8 6 Whitcher-Alagna, S., 2 1 5 White, A. W., 35 White, J . Α., 155 White, J . M . , 191 White, L . C . , 3 6 5 White, M . H . , 3 3 9 Whitehead, W. E . , 3 9 8 , 3 9 9 , 4 0 0 , 4 0 1 , 404, 405, 406, 409 Whitmore, B., 2 5 9 Whittle, G. C . , 4 7 7 Whorwell, P. J . , 3 9 8 , 4 0 3 , 4 1 4 Wiebe, D. J . , 3 0 7 Wilkie, Α., 4 7 7 , 4 7 8 Williams, D., 4 5 6 Williams, D. Α., 6 2 , 2 3 8 , 2 3 9 , 2 4 1 Williams, D . E . , 2 8 2 Williams, D. M . , 2 6 2 Williams, D . R., 4 5 6 Williams, J . , 4 1 1 Williams, J . B. W., 110
565
566
T H E HEALTH P S Y C H O L O G Y HANDBOOK Williams, M . H . , 192 Williams, N . L . , 231 Williams, R., 5 Williams, R. J., 88 Williamson, E. C , 107 Wills, Τ. Α., 88, 473 Wilsnack, S. C , 4 7 7 Wilson, G. T . , 134, 136 Wilson, J . D., 2 5 4 Wilson, J. R., 84 Wilson, M . F., 2 5 4 Wilson, P. H . , 3 1 5 Wilund, K. R., 2 6 1 Wincze, J. P., 3 5 9 , 3 6 0 , 3 6 2 Winders, S. E . , 107 Winefield, A. H . , 32 Winefield, H . R., 3 2 , 35 Wing, R. R., 59, 63, 133, 154, 2 5 8 , 3 0 5 , 3 1 9 Winger, C , 4 0 6 Wirth, Α., 136 Wise, C. D., 104 Wise, R. Α., 104 Wittchen, H-U., 22 Witteman, J . C , 2 6 0 Wofford, A. B., 9 Wolf, M . , 5 3 2 Wolf, S., 369 Wolfe, F., 2 3 6 Wolfe, V. Α., 433 Wolinsky, F. D., 4 9 0 Wolpe, J., 179 Wolters, E . C . , 4 8 7 Womble, L. G., 127 Wong, F. S., 4 9 0 Wong, P. T. P., 4 5 9 W o o d , T. M . , 148 Woodall, K. L . , 2 5 3 Woods, N., 472 W o o d s , N . F., 4 7 1 Wooley, S., 4 0 6 Woolfolk, R. L . , 173, 177 Wooten, G. F., 4 8 7 Worcel, M . , 368 Worden, J. W., 3 3 1 , 3 3 4 , 3 4 2 World Bank, 4 7 8 World Health Organization, 128, 154, 2 0 9 , 5 0 6 Worthman, C . W., 4 7 5 Wright, E . , 153 Wright, Ε. M . , 213 Wright, F. D., 90 Wright, J. Α., 129, 478 Wright, M . , 30, 33
Wright, V., 155 Writing G r o u p for the Activity Counseling Trial, 62 Wulsin, L . , 306 Wyllie, M . , 368 Wysocki, T., 304, 313 Xin, X . , 2 6 0 Yaeger, A. M . , 88 Y a l o m , I., 6 7 Y a m a b e , H . , 253 Y a m o r i , Y., 253 Yan, S., 398 Yancey, A. K., 88 Yanduli, V., 44 Yanek, L. R., 4 6 0 Yasco, J., 345 Y a s k o , J . , 59 Yates, B. T., 526 Yehuda, R., 3 4 1 Yelin, E . , 155 Yellowlees, P. M . , 4 8 8 Ying, Y., 4 5 8 Yohannes, A. M . , 4 8 8 Y o k o t a , F., 88 Young, B., 310 Young, D. R., 2 6 1 Young, S. J., 105 Younger, J . , 4 8 9 Yuzpe, Α. Α., 4 7 8 Zanardi, C , 4 6 9 Zanchetti, Α., 151 Zarit, J . M . , 4 8 6 Zarit, S. H . , 4 8 6 Zauber, Α., 3 3 0 Z a u t r a , A. J., 59 Zayfert, C , 2 3 9 Zeiss, Α., 4 9 3 Zenz, M . , 2 4 1 Z h a n g , A. Y., 4 5 8 Z h a n g , P., 155 Zheng, D., 3 0 7 Ziegelstein, R. C , 2 1 5 Ziegler, F. J., 2 3 5 Zierath, J. R., 156 Zinner, S. H . , 3 6 6 Zinsmeister, A. R., 398, 3 9 9 , 4 1 4 Z o l a , 1. K., 2 1 9 Z o n d e r m a n , A. B., 3 3 5 , 4 0 5 Zurawski, R. M . , 2 6 2
Subject
Index
Note: References to tables or figures are indicated by italic type and the addition of "i" or "f," respectively. Abuse alcohol, 2 4 , 8 2 - 8 3 childhood, 4 0 5 - 4 0 6 of women, 4 7 8 - 4 7 9 Accelerometers, 1 4 8 - 1 4 9 A C E inhibitors, 295t Acoma-Canoncito-Laguna Adolescent Health Program, 4 6 0 Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndrome (AIDS). See AIDS Adaptation, to illness, 2 3 - 2 4 Adherence to medical recommendations, 2 0 8 - 2 1 9 behavioral science role in, 2 0 9 - 2 1 1 and cancer, 3 3 0 case study of, 2 1 8 - 2 1 9 definitions and descriptions of, 2 0 8 - 2 0 9 and diabetes, 313 and HIV, 3 8 8 - 3 9 0 and hypertension, 2 6 5 - 2 6 6 , 266t interventions for improving, 2 1 4 - 2 1 6 measurement of, 2 1 6 - 2 1 7 , 2 1 9 models for understanding, 2 1 1 - 2 1 4 in pediatric populations, 6 - 7 Adolescence, in women, 4 6 9 - 4 7 0 Adrenaline, 1 7 2 African Americans. See also Ethnocultural issues and church role in behavior intervention, 4 6 0 and hypertension, 2 5 2 , 2 5 3 , 2 5 9 , 2 6 0 and nicotine dependence, 1 0 7 and physical activity for hypertension, 1 5 2 research exploitation of, 5 1 5 Aged people. See Older adults Aging, 4 8 6 - 4 8 7 AIAI complex, 2 8 8 AIDS, 3 8 7 . See also H u m a n Immunodeficiency Virus cases of, by exposure and year of diagnosis, 385f cases of, by race/ethnicity and year of report, 384f common opportunistic infections of, 3 8 7 , 388t estimated incidence of, by sex and exposure, 386f Albany GI History, 4 0 9 Alcohol abuse, 2 4 , 8 2 - 8 3
Alcohol dependence, 8 0 - 8 2 Alcohol Expectancy Questionnaire, 88 Alcohol problems, 83 Alcohol use and coronary heart disease, 2 8 9 and hypertension, 2 5 9 - 2 6 0 and insomnia, 4 3 1 - 4 3 2 Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test, 199 Alcoholism, 7 9 - 9 6 . See also Alcohol abuse; Alcohol dependence; Alcohol problems case study of, 9 4 - 9 6 characterologic factors in, 8 6 - 8 7 cognitive factors in, 8 5 - 8 6 definitions and descriptions of, 7 9 - 8 3 exogenous factors in, 8 7 - 8 9 genetic factors in, 8 4 - 8 5 psychological treatments for, 8 9 - 9 2 reinforcement and, 8 3 - 8 4 stages of change model and, 9 2 - 9 3 Alexithymia, 2 8 9 Alzheimer's Dementia Assessment Scale, Cognitive subscale, 2 1 Α Μ Α . See American Medical Association American Academy of Sleep Medicine, 4 3 6 American Board of Professional Psychology, 12 American College of Rheumatology, 2 3 6 American College of Sports Medicine, 1 4 7 , 2 6 0 American Diabetes Association, 3 1 1 American Heart Association, 1 3 1 , 2 5 3 American Lung Association, 2 3 7 American Medical Association (ΑΜΑ), 1 8 8 , 2 0 5 , 215, 237, 425, 430, 446 American Psychiatric Association, 1 9 2 American Psychological Association (ΑΡΑ), 12, 14-15, 446, 447, 454, 505, 507 American Society of Hypertension Ad H o c Panel, 2 6 5 Anger, and coronary heart disease, 2 8 8 Angina, 2 8 1 - 2 8 2
Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 15 Anorexia, and cancer, 3 4 2 Antiarrhythmics, 2 9 5 1 Anticoagulants, 2 9 5 i Antidepressant prescriptions, 17 Antilipidemics, 295t
567
568
T H E HEALTH P S Y C H O L O G Y H A N D B O O K Anxiety and arrhythmias, 2 8 2 - 2 8 3 and cancer, 341 and chronic pain, 2 3 5 and coronary heart disease, 2 8 9 costs of untreated, 34 and diabetes, 3 0 8 , 3 1 8 and HIV, 3 9 2 - 3 9 3 stress and, 176 women and, 4 7 6 - 4 7 7 Anxiety Disorders Interview (ADIS-R), 4 1 1 ΑΡΑ. See American Psychological Association Appetite, of older adults, 4 9 1 - 4 9 2 Arrhythmias, 2 8 2 - 2 8 3 Arthritis. See Osteoarthritis Arthritis Foundation, 155 Ask-advise-assist-arrange model, for dependence treatment, 2 1 5 - 2 1 6 Assessment, 5 - 6 of cancer, 3 4 2 - 3 4 3 of chronic pain, 2 3 8 - 2 4 0 of coronary heart disease, 2 8 4 - 2 9 1 of diabetes, 3 1 1 - 3 1 2 ethical issues in, 4 4 5 - 4 4 6 health outcomes, 5 0 4 - 5 0 5 , 5 0 7 - 5 0 8 of hypertension, 2 5 5 - 2 5 7 of IBS, 4 0 7 - 4 1 1 in medical settings, 1 7 - 2 5 of medication-seeking behavior, 1 9 8 - 1 9 9 of nicotine dependence, 1 0 9 - 1 1 1 of obesity, 1 3 0 - 1 3 1 of physical activity, 1 4 7 - 1 5 0 and p r o g r a m evaluation, 5 2 7 - 5 2 8 of psychological disorders in H I V illness, 390-393, 395 of research, 5 2 0 of sexual dysfunctions, 361 of sleep disorders, 4 2 9 of social validity, 5 3 2 - 5 3 3 of stress, 177 Association for the Behavioral Sciences and Medical Education, 14 Atherosclerosis, 2 8 0 - 2 8 1 Australian Psychological Society, 3 1 , 33 Autogenic training, 179 Automated Reports and Consolidated Orders System ( A R C O S ) , 193 Automatic Nervous System, 1 7 1 . See also Central nervous system ( C N S ) Automatic Nervous System Questionnaire, 23 Back BDI. Beck Beck Beck Beck Beck Beck
pain, 2 3 6 - 2 3 7 See Beck Depression Inventory Anxiety Inventory, 23 Depression Inventory, 2 3 , 2 4 0 , 4 0 5 , 4 1 1 Hopelessness Scale, 23 inventories, 177 Scale for Suicide Ideation, 23 scales, 23
Bedford College Interview for Life Events and Difficulties, 177 Behavior change counseling, 4 6 - 4 7 Behavioral science. See also Psychosocial interventions and adherence behavior, 2 0 9 - 2 1 1 and interventions for ethnic disparities, 4 6 0 - 4 6 2 Benton Visual Retention Test, 2 0 Beta blockers, 295t Beta-Ill, 2 2 Better Outcomes for Mental Health (Australia), 31 Billing, for consultations, 11 Biofeedback, 1 7 9 - 1 8 0 Biopsychosocial model of illness, 5 6 - 5 8 and chronic pain, 2 3 0 - 2 3 2 , 2 4 3 - 2 4 4 clinical application of, Sit and ethnic differences, 4 5 9 - 4 6 0 and IBS, 4 0 6 - 4 0 7 and medication-seeking behavior interventions, 1 9 4 - 1 9 8 , 19Sf and nicotine dependence, 1 0 4 - 1 0 7 Blindness. See Visual impairment Blood pressure. See Hypertension BMI. See Body mass index Body, human. See H u m a n body Body image, 127, 1 3 7 - 1 3 8 Body mass index (BMI), 125, 126t, 130, 1 5 3 - 1 5 4 Bowels. See Irritable bowel syndrome B R E N D A approach, 197 Brief advice, 46 Brief Pain Inventory (BPI), 343 Brief psychotherapies. See Psychosocial interventions Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI), 3 4 2 , 4 1 1 Brief Visual Spatial M e m o r y Test, 2 0 Buproprion, 114 Caffeine, 2 5 4 - 2 5 5 , 4 3 1 C A G E (alcohol abuse screening measure), 24, 199 Calcium channel blockers, 295t California Personality Inventory, Socialization scale, 86 Canadian Medical Association, 2 6 1 , 2 6 2 , 4 2 5 , 4 3 0 Cancer, 3 2 5 - 3 4 8 assessment and treatment of, 3 4 2 - 3 4 6 , 348 case study of, 3 4 7 coping with, 3 3 0 - 3 3 2 crises and problems of, 3 2 6 - 3 2 9 , 3 4 3 - 3 4 4 diagnostic and etiological issues of, 3 4 0 - 3 4 2 and information transmission, 3 3 6 - 3 3 7 , 3 4 4 models of, 3 3 8 - 3 4 0 in older adults, 4 8 9 - 4 9 0 and physical activity, 156 prevention and screening of, 3 2 9 - 3 3 0 research in, 3 2 9 - 3 4 0 , 333t screening of ethnic minorities for, 4 6 1 - 4 6 2 treatment of, 3 2 7 - 3 2 8 Cancer Rehabilitation Evaluation System (CARES), 343 Cannon, Walter, 172 Cardiovascular disease and interventions for ethnic disparities, 460—461
Subject Index and physical activity, 1 5 0 - 1 5 1 stress and, 176 Catastrophic thinking, 2 3 1 - 2 3 2 , 4 1 0 C B T . See Cognitive-behavioral therapy C D C . See Centers for Disease Control and Prevention Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression Scale (CES-D), 23 Center for Outcomes and Effectiveness Research and Education ( C O E R E ) fellowship, 5 0 9 Centers for Disease Control and Prevention ( C D C ) , 155, 260, 383, 387, 388, 506 Central nervous system ( C N S ) . See also Automatic Nervous System alcoholism and, 8 4 - 8 5 , 8 7 stress treatment and, 1 7 8 - 1 8 0 Cerebrovascular accident, 4 8 9 Cesarean section, 4 7 0 C H D . See Coronary heart disease Checkerboard Cardiovascular Curriculum for American Indian and Hispanic American children, 4 6 0 Chemotherapy, 3 2 7 Chest pain. See Angina Childbirth, 4 7 0 - 4 7 1 Childhood abuse, and IBS, 4 0 5 - 4 0 6 Chinese Community Cardiac Council, 4 6 0 Chronic fatigue syndrome, 2 3 7 Chronic obstructive pulmonary disease in older adults, 4 8 8 and physical activity, 1 5 2 - 1 5 3 Chronic pain, 2 2 9 - 2 4 5 assessment and treatment of, 2 3 8 - 2 4 4 biopsychosocial model of, 2 3 0 - 2 3 2 , 2 4 3 - 2 4 4 case study of, 2 4 5 diagnostic and etiological issues of, 2 3 2 - 2 3 4 pelvic pain in women, 4 7 7 - 4 7 8 prevalence of, 2 2 9 and psychological disorders, 2 3 4 - 2 3 8 , 2 4 0 transactional model of adjustment to, 2311 Classical conditioning, and nicotine dependence, 1 0 7 Clinical Competency Test Interview, 4 9 4
Clinical Guidelines on the Identification, Evaluation, and Treatment of Overweight and Obesity in Adults, 130 Clinical Neuropsychology: A Pocket Handbook, 2 0 Clock Drawing Test, 2 1 C N S . See Central nervous system Cocaine use, and coronary heart disease, 2 8 9 - 2 9 0 Cocktail therapy. See Highly active antiretroviral treatment Cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT), 4, 6 0 - 6 7 , 4 1 9 - 4 2 0 . See also Information-motivationbehavioral skills model for alcoholism, 9 0 - 9 1 for chronic pain, 2 3 0 - 2 3 1 , 2 4 2 - 2 4 3 and coronary heart disease, 287t and depression in diabetics, 3 1 6 for insomnia, 4 3 0 - 4 3 4 for stress, 1 8 1 - 1 8 2
Cognitive Capacity Screening Examination (CCSE), 343 Cognitive Coping Strategies Inventory, 2 3 9 Cognitive disorders. See also Cognitive screening; Dementia and HIV, 3 9 3 , 3 9 5 in older adults, 4 8 6 ^ 1 8 7 Cognitive orientation theory, 346f Cognitive screening, 1 9 - 2 2 and cancer, 3 4 3 Clock Drawing Test, 2 1 and coronary heart disease, 2 9 0 for dementia, 2 0 Folstein Mini Mental State E x a m ( M M S E ) , 2 1 Colleague relations, psychologists and, 4 4 7 - 4 4 8 College of Health Psychologists of the Australian Psychological Society, 33 Combination therapy. See Highly active antiretroviral treatment Committee on Accreditation of ΑΡΑ, 12, 5 0 5 Competence, of psychologists, 4 4 3 - 4 4 6 and assessment, 4 4 5 - 4 4 6 boundaries of, 4 4 4 - 4 4 5 C o m p l e x regional pain syndrome, 2 3 7 Composite primary Symptom Reduction (CPSR), 409-410 Confidentiality, 4 4 9 - 4 5 1 Conflicts of interest, 5 2 1 - 5 2 2 Consent, informed, 4 5 3 - 4 5 4 , 5 1 6 Consultation, 1 1 - 1 2 , 3 0 Contemplation Ladder, 110 Continuous positive airway pressure machine (CPAP), 4 3 5 Conversion disorders, and chronic pain, 2 3 5 Coping and alcoholism, 8 7 - 8 8 with cancer, 3 3 0 - 3 3 2 with chronic pain, 2 3 9 - 2 4 0 , 2 4 3 and coronary heart disease, 2 9 1 - 2 9 3 and diabetes, 3 0 9 , 3 1 4 - 3 1 5 with pain, 4 1 0 - 4 1 1 . See also Coping: with chronic pain strategies of, 1 7 4 - 1 7 5 and stress inoculation, 1 8 1 - 1 8 2 women and, 4 7 2 - 4 7 3 Coping Strategies Questionnaire, 2 3 9 Coronary heart disease ( C H D ) , 2 7 9 - 2 9 8 assessment of, 2 8 4 - 2 9 1 biopsychosocial model of, 2 8 3 - 2 8 4 case study of, 2 9 7 and c o m m o n cognitive errors in patients, 287t definitions and descriptions of, 2 8 0 - 2 8 4 diagnostic and treatment procedures common for, 2 S 4 r evaluation checklist for, 286t medications common for, by function, 2951 self-report measures concerning, 2851 treatment of, 2 9 1 - 2 9 6 , 2 9 8 Corticolimbic system, 170 Cortisol, 1 7 1 , 1 7 2 , 2 5 4 - 2 5 5 , 256f
\
569
570
T H E HEALTH P S Y C H O L O G Y HANDBOOK Cost-benefit analyses, 5 2 6 , 5 3 2 Cost-effectiveness analyses, 5 2 6 , 5 3 2 Council on Education for Public Health ( C E P H ) , 505-506 Daily Hassles and Uplifts Scale, 1 7 7 Death and dying, 5 6 , 4 9 4 - 4 9 5 Decision-making capacity, of older adults, 4 9 3 - 4 9 4 Defibrillators, 2 8 2 Delirium, 343 Delirium Rating Scale, 3 4 3 Dementia and mental competence, 4 9 4 in older adults, 4 9 2 screening for, 2 0 Dependence. See Alcohol dependence; Nicotine dependence; Substance abuse Depression. See also Antidepressant prescriptions and cancer, 3 3 3 , 3 3 4 , 3 4 0 - 3 4 1 and chronic pain, 2 3 4 - 2 3 5 and coronary heart disease, 2 8 8 and diabetes, 3 0 7 - 3 0 8 , 316 and HIV, 3 9 0 - 3 9 2 Parkinson's disease and, 4 8 7 physical activity and, 158 postpartum, 4 7 6 and sleep disorders, 4 2 6 stress and, 176 women and, 4 7 5 - 4 7 6 Diabetes, 3 0 3 - 3 1 9 assessment and treatment of, 3 1 1 - 3 1 6 , 3 1 8 background and etiology of, 3 0 4 - 3 0 5 case study of, 3 1 7 - 3 1 8 in older adults, 4 8 8 and physical activity, 1 5 6 - 1 5 7 psychological factors and, 3 0 6 - 3 1 1 Diabetes Prevention Program (DPP), 3 1 1 Diagnosis as assessment tool, 5 - 6 by exclusion, 8 - 9 Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. See D S M , various editions Diagnostic Interview Schedule, 110 Diet and IBS, 4 0 7 - 4 0 8 low-calorie Step I, 134t and obesity, 1 3 1 , 133 Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension (DASH) diet, 2 5 9 Digit Symbol test, 2 0 , 2 2 Digoxin, 295f Disease. See Illness Diuretics, 29St Division 12 (Society of Clinical Psychology) of ΑΡΑ, 1 4 - 1 5 Division 38 (Health Psychology) of ΑΡΑ, 14 Division 4 0 (Clinical Neuropsychology) of ΑΡΑ, 14 Division 54 (Society of Pediatric Psychology) of ΑΡΑ, 14 D M 2 (Type 2 diabetes). See Diabetes
Doctor shopping, 196 Dopamine, 8 4 - 8 5 , 87, 1 0 4 - 1 0 5 Dreams, 4 2 9 Drug Abuse Screening Test, 199 D r u g Enforcement Agency (DEA), 198 Drug Investigational Units (DIU), 193 Drug-seeking behavior. See Medication-seeking behavior DSM-II (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, second edition), chronic pain, 2 3 3 DSM-III (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, third edition) anxiety, 23 chronic pain, 2 3 3 delirium, 3 4 3 DSM-III-R (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, third edition, revised) chronic pain, 2 3 3 , 2 3 4 depression, 4 8 7 D S M - I V (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition), 5, 6, 11 alcohol abuse, 8 2 - 8 3 alcohol dependence, 8 0 - 8 2 anxiety, 3 9 2 chronic pain, 2 3 4 delirium, 3 4 3 depression, 126 eating disorders, 1 2 7 emotional distress, 2 8 7 nicotine dependence, 102 psychiatric comorbidity, 4 1 1 sexual dysfunctions, 3 5 9 , 360t, 3 6 2 somatization disorder, 2 3 5 stress, 176 Dysmenorrhea, 4 7 7 Dyspareunia, 3 6 6 - 3 6 7 Eastern philosophy, 173, 1 8 0 Eating disorders, 1 2 7 E B M (Evidence-Based Medicine)-Cochrane D a t a b a s e of Systematic Reviews, 15 Education. See Psychoeducation; Training Effect size statistics, 5 3 1 Ejaculation anejaculation, 3 7 0 premature, 3 7 0 , 3 7 4 - 3 7 5 retrograde, 3 6 9 - 3 7 0 Elderly people. See Older adults Electromyographic ( E M G ) feedback, 1 7 9 - 1 8 0 Electronic Point of Sale Systems (EPOS), 193 E M G (electromyographic) feedback, 1 7 9 - 1 8 0 Emotions and angina, 2 8 1 - 2 8 2 and cancer, 3 3 3 - 3 3 4 , 3 3 4 - 3 3 5 and coronary heart disease, 2 8 6 - 2 8 9 physical activity and, 1 5 7 - 1 5 8 Empowerment, and diabetes, 3 1 5 End-of-life issues. See Death and dying E N R I C H D clinical trial, 2 8 8 Epidemiology
Subject Index history and definition of, 5 0 3 - 5 0 4 and medication-seeking behavior research, 191-192 Epinephrine, 1 7 2 Erectile disorder (ED), 3 6 7 - 3 6 9 Ethical issues, 4 4 3 - 4 5 4 competence, 4 4 3 - 4 4 6 concern for welfare, 4 5 2 - 4 5 4 , 5 1 5 - 5 1 6 conflicts of interest, 5 2 1 - 5 2 2 guidelines and standards, 14 integrity, 4 4 6 - 4 4 8 professional conduct, 4 4 8 - 4 4 9 in research, 5 1 5 - 5 1 6 , 5 1 8 respect for rights and dignity, 4 4 9 - 4 5 2 , 5 1 5 - 5 1 6 social responsibility, 4 5 4 , 5 1 5 - 5 1 6 Ethnocultural issues, 4 5 1 - 4 5 2 , 4 5 6 - 4 6 4 behavioral interventions, 4 6 0 - 4 6 2 diabetes, 3 0 5 , 3 1 0 disparities in health and health care, 456^4-60 HIV, 3 8 4 nicotine dependence, 1 0 7 - 1 0 8 pain, 4 5 7 - 4 5 8 physical activity, 161 recommendations for improved care, 4 6 2 - 4 6 3 European Organization for Research and Treatment of Cancer ( E O R T C ) , 3 4 2 Euthanasia. See Physician-assisted suicide Evaluation of programs. See Program evaluation Evidence-based medicine. See E B M (Evidence-Based Medicine)-Cochrane D a t a b a s e of Systematic Reviews Exercise. See Physical activity Fagerstrôm Test of Nicotine Dependence, 110 Faith-based programs, 4 6 0 Falls, of older adults, 4 9 0 - 4 9 1 Family, and cancer patients, 3 3 6 Family practice model, 2 9 4 Fear Avoidance Beliefs Questionnaire, 2 4 Fear of Pain Questionnaire, 2 4 Fees. See Billing Fellowship p r o g r a m s , 5 0 7 , 5 0 9 - 5 1 0 Fibromylagia, 2 3 6 Finnish Diabetes Prevention Study (FDPS), 3 1 1 Florida Medical Association, 198 Folstein Mini Mental State E x a m ( M M S E ) , 2 1 Food and Drug Administration (FDA), 1 1 3 , 1 3 6 , 414, 434 Functional analysis, in psychological assessment, 5, 6 Functional Assessment of Cancer Therapy (FACT), 3 4 2 Gastrointestinal diseases, 3 9 7 . See also Irritable bowel syndrome similarity to IBS of, 4 0 0 stress and, 176 Gender. See also Women's health and nicotine dependence, 1 0 7 - 1 0 8 and physical activity for hypertension, 1 5 2
Genetics and alcoholism, 8 4 - 8 5 and cancer, 338 and nicotine dependence, 107 and obesity, 1 2 8 - 1 2 9 Geriatric populations. See Older adults Glutamate, 1 0 5 G r o u p psychotherapies, 5 9 . See also Social support H A A R T . See Highly active antiretroviral treatment Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Battery Category test, 2 0 Trail M a k i n g test, Part B, 2 0 , 2 2 Hamilton Anxiety and Depression scales, 23 Headaches, 2 3 6 Health Anxiety Questionnaire, 2 4 Health belief model, a n d adherence behavior, 2 1 2 Health care systems and adherence interventions, 2 1 6 and safeguards against medication-seeking, 1 9 3 - 1 9 4 , 198 workings of, 5 0 6 - 5 0 7 Health outcomes assessment, 5 0 4 - 5 0 5 , 5 0 7 - 5 0 8 Health promotion, psychologist role in, 33
Health Psychology, 15 Heart disease. See Coronary heart disease Heart Health for Southeast Asians, 4 6 0 Heart rate monitors, 149 Heart Smart Program, 4 6 0 Hemodynamics, 2 8 3 High blood pressure. See Hypertension Highly active antiretroviral treatment ( H A A R T ) , 383, 388-389 Histories, patient, 19 HIV. See H u m a n Immunodeficiency Virus HIV/AIDS Prevention Research Synthesis Project (CDC), 388 Holmes Alcoholism Scale, 86 Holter monitors, 2 8 1 Homeostasis, 1 6 9 - 1 7 0 H o p e m o n t Capacity Assessment Interview, 4 9 4 Hopkins Verbal Learning Test, 2 0 Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS), 284, 3 4 2 Hospitals, skills needed for, 33. See also Medical centers H S D D . See Hypoactive sexual desire disorder H u m a n body, organization of, 169 H u m a n Immunodeficiency Virus (HIV), 3 8 3 - 3 9 5 background and epidemiology of, 3 8 3 - 3 8 7 case study of, 3 7 4 non-nucleoside reverse transcriptase inhibitors for, 390t nucleoside analog reverse transcriptase inhibitors
for, 389t prevention and adherence issues with, 3 8 7 - 3 9 0 protease inhibitors for, 390f and psychological disorders, 3 9 0 - 3 9 3 , 3 9 5 Hyperglycemia, 3 0 4 Hypertension, 2 5 2 - 2 6 9
j
571
572
T H E HEALTH P S Y C H O L O G Y HANDBOOK assessment and treatment of, 2 5 5 - 2 6 6 case study of, 2 6 7 - 2 6 8 diagnostic and etiological issues of, 2 5 3 - 2 5 5 health risks of, 2 5 2 and physical activity, 1 5 1 - 1 5 2 stress and Cortisol and, 2S6f white coat, 2 5 6 - 2 5 7 Hypnosis, 179 Hypoactive sexual desire disorder ( H S D D ) , 3 6 2 , 364-365 Hypoglycemia, 304 Hypothalamus, 1 7 0 - 1 7 1 IBS. See Irritable bowel syndrome IBS-Quality of Life Measure (IBS-QOL), 4 1 1 I C D - 1 0 (International Classification of Diseases, 10th edition), 1 2 - 1 4 Illness improving outcomes of, 6 3 - 6 5 physical activity and, 1 5 0 - 1 5 7 psychological adaptation to, 2 3 - 2 4 risk reduction for, 6 1 - 6 3 stimulus-background model of, 338f Illness Behaviour Questionnaire, 24 Immune function, stress and, 176 Impotence, 369 Inactivity. See Physical activity Incidence, of disease, 5 0 4 Indian philosophy, 180 Infections, AIDS and common opportunistic,
387, 388t Infertility, 478 Information-motivation-behavioral skills model, and adherence behavior, 2 1 2 - 2 1 4 . See also Cognitive-behavioral therapy Informed consent, 4 5 3 - 4 5 4 , 5 1 6 Insomnia, 4 2 7 , 4 2 9 - 4 3 5 , 4 3 6 Instrumental Activity of Daily Living Scale, 2 1 Insurance, reimbursements from, 31 Integrity, of psychologists, 4 4 6 - 4 4 8 Interdisciplinary Professional Education Collaborative, 3 5 , 36 International Association for the Study of Pain, 2 3 7 International Headache Society, 2 3 6 Internships, 12 Interpersonal/social support. See Social support Interprofessional settings. See Multidisciplinary settings Interventions. See Motivational enhancement interventions; Psychosocial interventions Interviews, in assessment, 19 Inventory of Current Concerns, 342 Inventory of Negative Thoughts in Response to Pain, 2 3 9 IQ (intelligence quotient), 22 Irritable bowel syndrome (IBS), 397^120 assessment and treatment of, 4 0 7 - 4 1 5 , 4 1 9 - 4 2 0 background and etiology of, 3 9 7 - 4 0 7 biopsychosocial model of, 4 0 6 - 4 0 7 case study of, 4 1 6 - 4 1 8
diseases with symptoms similar to, 4011 psychological factors and, 4 0 4 - 4 0 6 symptom severity development (case study), 419t Jacobson, Edmond, 178-179 Joint National Committee on Detection, Evaluation, and Treatment of High Blood Pressure, 2 5 3
Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 15 Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings, 15 Journals, 15. See also by name Jung, Carl, 4 2 9 K-awards, 5 0 7 Kaiser Permanente, 2 1 8 - 2 1 9 , 4 3 3 Language for Health, 4 6 0 Liaison roles, 1 1 - 1 2 , 3 0 Life Events Survey, 177 Lifestyle behaviors, 3. See also Diet; Physical activity diabetes and, 3 1 0 , 3 1 1 and obesity, 1 3 1 , 1 3 3 - 1 3 4 Light therapy, 4 3 5 - 4 3 6 Lighten Up, 4 6 0 Literature, medical, 5 2 2 Lotronex, 4 1 4 Lupus, 2 3 7 - 2 3 8 M a c A n d r e w Alcoholism Scale, 86 M a n a g e d care, and mental health workers, 3 1 - 3 2 Maryland Addiction Questionnaire, 24 Mattis Dementia Rating Scale, 2 1 McGill Pain Questionnaire ( M P Q ) , 2 3 8 , 3 4 3 , 4 1 0 Measures, selection of, 528 Medicaid Fraud and Abuse System (MFAS), 193 Medical centers. See also Hospitals organizational and political settings of, 10 psychological practices in, 13 Medical literature, 5 2 2 Medical Outcomes Study Short Form Health Survey ( M O S SF-36), 3 4 2 , 4 1 1 Medication Event Monitoring System (MEMS), 217, 219 Medication-seeking behavior, 1 8 7 - 2 0 6 assessment and treatment of, 1 9 8 - 1 9 9 , 2 0 5 biopsychosocial interventions for, 1 9 4 - 1 9 8 , 195f,
203f case study of, 2 0 0 - 2 0 5 definitions and descriptions of, 1 8 7 - 1 9 1 research in, 1 9 1 - 1 9 4 systemic safeguards against, 1 9 3 - 1 9 4 , 198 varieties of, 196 Medications. See also Pharmacology conflicts of interest involving research on, 521-522 problems of older adults with, 4 9 0 Medicine medical model, 7 - 9 medical versus psychological background, 9 - 1 0 organizational and political settings of, 1 0 - 1 1 , 13 profession of, 18
Subject Meditation, and stress management, 180 M E D L I N E , 1 5 , 191 Melatonin, 4 3 4 - 4 3 5 Memorial Delirium Assessment Scale, 343 Memorial Symptom Assessment Scale (MSAS), 3 4 3 Menopause, 4 7 1 - 4 7 2 Mental competence, of older adults, 493-494 Meridia, 136 Mexican Americans, and physical activity, 161 Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test ( M A S T ) , 2 4 Migraine headaches, 2 3 6 Mind-body dichotomy, 6 Mini-Mental State E x a m ( M M S E ) , 3 4 3 Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI), 86, 2 4 0 Minnesota Nicotine Withdrawal Scale, 110 Model-fitting analysis of p r o g r a m evaluation, 530-531 M o o d . See Depression; Emotions Motivational enhancement interventions, 4 2 - 5 2 clinical development of, 4 3 - 4 4 empirical research on, 4 4 - 4 5 for medication-seeking behavior, 1 9 9 varieties of, 4 5 - 5 1 Motivational interviewing, 4 7 - 5 1 , 6 2 . See also Motivational enhancement interventions for alcoholism, 9 2 training in, 52t Multicultural issues. See Ethnocultural issues Multidimensional Fatigue Inventory (MFI), 343 Multidisciplinary settings, 2 8 - 3 8 historical issues of, 2 9 - 3 2 specialized skills for, 3 2 - 3 5 training for, 3 5 - 3 8 Multiple chemical sensitivities, 2 3 7 Multiple Copy Prescription Program (MCPP), 193 Multiple roles, of women, 4 7 3 - 4 7 4 Napping, 4 3 6 Narcolepsy, 4 2 8 - 4 2 9
National Arthritis Action Flan: A Public Health Strategy, 1 5 5 - 1 5 6 National Commission on Sleep Disorders Research, 4 2 5 National Health Service (Britain), 3 1 National Household Survey on Drug Abuse, 1 8 9 , 192 National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA), 193 National Institute on Mental Health, 5 1 0 National Institutes of Health (NIH), 3 1 1 , 4 3 4 Negative Alcohol Expectancy Questionnaire, 88 Nervous system, and IBS, 4 0 3 , 4 0 4 , 4 0 6 - 4 0 7 . See also Automatic Nervous System; Central nervous system Neurobehavioral Cognitive Status Examination ( N C S E ) , 2 1 , 2 2 , 343 Nicotine dependence, 1 0 1 - 1 1 8 assessment and treatment of, 1 0 9 - 1 1 4 , 2 1 5 - 2 1 6 biopsychosocial learning model of, 1 0 4 - 1 0 7 case study of, 1 1 5 - 1 1 8
Index
comorbid personality a n d psychopathology and, 108-109 definitions and descriptions of, 1 0 2 - 1 0 4 gender and ethnicity and, 1 0 7 - 1 0 8 genetic factors in, 1 0 7 Nicotine replacement therapy ( N R T ) , 113 Night terrors, 4 2 9 , 4 3 7 - 4 3 8 Nightmares, 4 2 9 , 4 3 6 - 4 3 7 Nitrates, 2951 Nonpsychiatric patients, psychological needs of, 6 - 7 Normative comparisons, 5 3 1 - 5 3 2 Obesity, 1 2 5 - 1 4 1 assessment and treatment of, 1 3 0 - 1 3 8 , 140 case study of, 1 3 9 - 1 4 0 definitions and descriptions of, 1 2 5 - 1 2 6 developmental factors in, 1 2 9 and diabetes, 3 0 5 and disease risks, 131t, 132t environmental factors in, 128 evaluation-treatment interface for, 133f genetic factors in, 1 2 8 - 1 2 9 and hypertension, 2 5 2 , 2 5 7 - 2 5 9 incidence of, 1 2 5 , 1 2 6 , 127t and physical activity, 1 3 3 - 1 3 4 , 1 5 3 - 1 5 5 physical problems associated with, 126 psychological and psychosocial problems associated with, 1 2 6 - 1 2 8 psychosocial treatments of, 1 3 4 - 1 3 6 treatment algorithm for, 131 and weight loss expectations, 138t Older adults age-related changes in, 4 8 6 - 4 8 7 and cancer, 4 8 9 - 4 9 0 and cerebrovascular accident, 4 8 9 and chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, 4 8 8 common problems of, 490—493 and death-related issues, 4 9 4 ^ 1 9 5 and diabetes, 3 0 5 , 4 8 8 diagnostic issues with, 4 9 3 health issues of, 4 8 5 - 4 9 6 and hypertension, 2 5 2 , 2 5 9 and medication problems, 4 9 0 mental competence of, 493—494 and Parkinson's disease, 4 8 7 and thyroid disturbances, 4 8 8 - 4 8 9 O m e g a Screening Instruments, 3 4 2 Online resources, 1 5 . See also by name Operant conditioning, and pain management, 242-243 Oregon Board of Medical Examiners, 1 9 2 Orgasm female disorders of, 3 6 5 - 3 6 6 male disorders of, 3 6 9 - 3 7 0 Orlistat (Xenical), 136 Osteoarthritis, and physical activity, 1 5 5 - 1 5 6 Outcomes. See Health outcomes assessment; Program evaluation Overweight, 1 2 5 . See also Obesity
574
T H E HEALTH P S Y C H O L O G Y HANDBOOK Pain. See also Chronic pain ethnic differences concerning, 4 5 7 - 4 5 8 and IBS, 4 1 0 - 4 1 1 in older adults, 4 9 2 - 4 9 3 Pain Anxiety Symptoms Scale, 2 4 , 2 3 9 Pain Beliefs and Perceptions Inventory, 2 3 9 Pain Catastrophizing Scale, 2 3 9 Pain clinics, 2 4 3 Pain Coping Skills Questionnaire (CSQ), 4 1 0 - 4 1 1 Pain management, 2 4 0 - 2 4 4 Panic, and arrhythmias, 2 8 2 - 2 8 3 Parkinson's disease, 4 8 7 Patient histories, 19 Patient relations, psychologists and, 4 4 6 - 4 4 7 Pedometers, 148 Pelvic pain, chronic, 4 7 7 - 4 7 8 Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ), 4 1 1 Periodic leg movement disorder ( P L M D ) , 4 2 8 , 4 3 5 Personality alcholism and, 8 6 - 8 7 nicotine dependence and, 1 0 8 - 1 0 9 Pharmaceutical industry, 5 2 1 - 5 2 2 Pharmacology and coronary heart disease, 2 9 4 - 2 9 6 , 2 9 8 and hypertension, 2 6 2 - 2 6 4 , 263-264t and IBS, 4 1 1 - 4 1 4 , 413t and insomnia, 4 3 4 - 4 3 5 and nicotine dependence, 113 and obesity, 136 and pain management, 2 4 1 and psychological disorders accompanying HIV, 391-392, 393, 395 Physical activity assessment of, 1 4 7 - 1 5 0 case study of, 1 5 9 - 1 6 1 and chronic disease risk factors, 1 4 6 - 1 6 2 definitions and descriptions of, 1 4 6 - 1 4 7 and hypertension, 2 6 0 - 2 6 1 and illness, 1 5 0 - 1 5 7 and insomnia, 4 3 2 moderate, 135t and obesity, 1 3 3 - 1 3 4 , 1 5 3 - 1 5 5 and psychological functioning, 1 5 7 - 1 5 8 Physical Maintenance Scale, 2 1 Physician-assisted suicide, 3 2 9
Physician Drug and Diagnosis Audit, 3 9 7 Physicians, and medication-seeking patients, 189-190, 197-198, 199, 2 0 5 P M S (premensrrual syndrome), 4 7 5 - 4 7 6 Postdoctoral fellowships, 12 Postpartum depression, 4 7 6 Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and cancer, 3 4 1 Pregnancy, 4 7 0 - 4 7 1 Premature ejaculation, 3 7 0 , 3 7 4 - 3 7 5 Premenstrual syndrome (PMS), 4 7 5 - 4 7 6 Prescribing, inappropriate, 1 9 2 - 1 9 3 , 1 9 7 - 1 9 8 Prescription drug abuse. See Medication-seeking behavior Prevalence, of disease, 5 0 4
Primary care and detection of psychological difficulties, 34 and pain treatment, 2 4 4 skills needed for, 3 3 - 3 5 Professional conduct, 4 4 8 - 4 4 9 Professional societies, 1 4 - 1 5 . See also individual
organizations Profile of M o o d States ( P O M S ) , 3 4 2 Program evaluation, 5 2 5 - 5 3 4 analysis of, 5 3 0 - 5 3 3 and appropriate measures, 5 2 8 assessment methods for, 5 2 7 - 5 2 8 defining behaviors for, 5 2 7 and individual versus group designs, 5 2 9 - 5 3 0 method of describing change for, 5 3 1 - 5 3 2 and social validity, 5 3 2 - 5 3 3 steps in, 5 2 6 - 5 2 7 Progressive diabetic retinopathy (PDR), 3 0 6
Progressive Relaxation Training, 179 Project J o y , 4 6 0 Project M A T C H , 4 4 Psychiatric comorbidity, 4 Psychiatric Rating Scale, 23 Psychiatrists, and collaboration with psychologists, 3 0 - 3 1 Psychiatry, clinical health psychology versus, 5 PsychLit, 191 Psycho-oncology. See Cancer Psychoeducation, 6 0 - 6 7 Psychological disorders, chronic pain and, 2 3 4 - 2 3 8 , 2 4 0 Psychological factors cancer and, 3 3 2 - 3 3 5 , 333t diabetes and, 3 0 6 - 3 1 1 IBS and, 4 0 4 - 4 0 6 ignoring of, 9 Psychologists assessment by, 5 - 6 . See also Assessment and collaboration with psychiatrists, 3 0 - 3 1 as consultants and liaisons, 1 1 - 1 2 effective, 10, 14 self-assessment concerning medical-surgical problems, 13t treatment approaches of, 6 - 7 unique contributions of, 4 - 7 , 3 2 Psychology, in clinical versus medical settings, 18, 34 Psychometrics, 5, 1 8 - 1 9 Psychoneuroimmunology, 3 3 9 - 3 4 0 Psychopharmacology, 2 9 4 - 2 9 6 , 2 9 8 Psychosocial factors, interaction with medical conditions of, 5 5 , 58 Psychosocial interventions. See also Behavioral science for alcoholism, 8 9 - 9 2 for cancer, 3 4 4 - 3 4 6 , 348 developments in, 5 6 - 6 0 for nicotine dependence, 1 1 1 - 1 1 3 for obesity, 1 3 4 - 1 3 6 options in medical settings, 59f training for, 6 7 - 6 9 and treatment goals, 6 0 - 6 7
Subject Psychotherapies brief, 5 5 - 6 9 group, 59 Psychotropic drugs, 31 PsycINFO, 15 Public health, 5 0 2 - 5 1 1 and clinical care compared, 5 0 2 - 5 0 3 historical issues and key concepts of, 5 0 3 - 5 0 5 interdisciplinary character of, 5 1 0 - 5 1 1 psychologists and, 33, 5 0 3 , 5 0 8 - 5 1 1 training and skills for, 5 0 5 - 5 1 0 Pulmonary disease. See Chronic obstructive pulmonary disease Q R S interval, 2 9 5 Quality-adjusted life year ( Q A L Y ) , 5 0 4 Quality of life cancer and, 3 3 1 , 3 4 2 - 3 4 3 diabetes and, 3 0 9 - 3 1 0 IBS and, 411 improvements for ill patients in, 6 5 - 6 7 Quetelet's index. See Body mass index Race. See Ethnocultural issues Radiation, 3 2 7 Recurrent Coronary Prevention Project, 6 3 - 6 5 Reinforcement alcoholism and, 8 3 - 8 4 nicotine dependence and, 1 0 4 - 1 0 6 Relaxation and insomnia, 4 3 2 and pain management, 2 4 0 - 2 4 1 , 2 4 2 and stress management, 1 7 8 - 1 7 9 Religion and behavioral intervention, 4 6 0 older adults and, 4 9 5 R E M (rapid eye movement) sleep, 4 2 6 - 4 2 7 , 4 2 8 , 4 2 9 Remission, of cancer, 328 Remodeling, of heart, 283 Report writing, 2 4 - 2 5 Research application of, 7 assessment of, 5 2 0 and conflicts of interest, 5 2 1 - 5 2 2 defining questions for, 5 1 6 - 5 1 7 design of, 5 1 8 - 5 2 0 ethical issues in, 5 1 5 - 5 1 6 , 518 institutional considerations for, 518 in medical settings, 5 1 4 - 5 2 2 methodology of, 32 promotion of, 5 1 7 - 5 1 8 recruitment for, 5 2 0 and treatment, 5 2 0 - 5 2 1 Resistance, responses to, 50t Restless leg syndrome (RLS), 4 2 7 - 4 2 8 , 4 3 5 Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, 2 0 "Right to die", 329 Risk factors, for disease, 5 0 4 Risk reduction, for disease, 6 1 - 6 3 Robert W o o d J o h n s o n Foundation, 5 0 7
Index
R o m e II criteria, and IBS, 4 0 0 , 4 0 9 R o m e II Integrative Questionnaire, 4 0 9 Rotterdam Symptom Checklist, 343 Royal M a r s d e n studies, 3 3 1 , 3 3 4 S A D H A R T clinical trial, 288 Salt, and hypertension, 2 5 9 Schizophrenia, and cancer, 333 Schultz, Johannes Heinrich, 179 Screening broad-based, 2 0 for cancer, 3 2 9 - 3 3 0 cognitive. See Cognitive screening ethnic participation in, 4 6 1 - 4 6 2 intellectual, 2 2 in medical settings, 18 psychiatric, 2 2 - 2 3 purposes of, 1 7 - 1 8 for substance abuse, 24 training for, 2 0
Screening for Brain Impairment,
20
Self-assessment, concerning medical-surgical problems, 13t Self-destructive behavior, 4 7 0 Self-management, 2 1 0 and coronary heart disease, 2 9 4 and diabetes, 3 1 3 - 3 1 4 Self-medicating behavior, nicotine dependence as, 108-109 Self-Rating Depression Scale, 23 Self-report measures for coronary heart disease, 28St for IBS, 4 0 9 for physical activity, 148 for psychological functioning, 4 1 1 Sensate focus, 3 6 2 , 363-364t Sensitivity, in psychometrics, 19 Serotonin, 2 9 5 - 2 9 6 Sexual aversion disorder (SAD), 3 6 5 Sexual dysfunctions, 3 5 9 - 3 7 5 assessment and treatment of, 3 6 1 - 3 7 0 , 3 7 4 - 3 7 5 case study of, 3 7 1 - 3 7 4 diabetes and, 3 0 6 female, 3 6 2 , 3 6 4 - 3 6 7 male, 3 6 7 - 3 7 0 , 3 7 4 - 3 7 5 Sexual response, 3 5 9 - 3 6 0 , 3 6 2 ί SF-36. See Medical Outcomes Study Short Form Health Survey Sibutramine (Meridia), 136 Sickness Impact Profile, 2 3 8 Sleep apnea, 4 2 8 , 4 3 5 and coronary heart disease, 2 9 0 Sleep cycles, 426—427 Sleep disorders, 4 2 5 - 4 3 8 assessment and treatment of, 4 2 9 - 4 3 8 background and etiology of, 4 2 5 - 4 2 9 case study of, 4 3 7 effects of, 4 2 5 - 4 2 6 in older adults, 4 9 1 stress and, 176
576
T H E HEALTH P S Y C H O L O G Y HANDBOOK Sleepwalking, 4 3 8 Slosson Intelligence Test, 2 2 Smoking. See Nicotine dependence; T o b a c c o use Smoking Consequences Questionnaire, 110 Smoking Self-Efficacy Questionnaire, 110 Snow, John, 5 0 3 Social cognitive theory, and adherence behavior, 2 1 1 Social learning theory, 4 0 6 Social responsibility, 4 5 4 , 5 1 5 - 5 1 6 Social support. See also G r o u p psychotherapies and alcoholism, 8 8 - 8 9 and cancer, 3 3 5 - 3 3 6 and diabetes, 3 0 8 - 3 0 9 and nicotine dependence, 1 1 2 - 1 1 3 and treatment interventions, 6 0 - 6 7 women and, 4 7 3 Society of Behavioral Medicine, 14 Socioeconomic factors, women and, 4 7 4 - 4 7 5 Sodium, and hypertension, 2 5 9 Somatization disorder, 2 3 5 Specificity, in psychometrics, 19 Spielberger anxiety and anger scales, 1 7 7 Spouse/Friend Ketterer Stress Symptom Frequency Checklist (KSSFC), 2 9 0 SSRIs (selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors), 295-296 St. John's wort, 2 9 6 , 2 9 8 Stages of change model and adherence behavior, 2 1 2 and alcoholism, 9 2 - 9 3 Stanford-Binet-IV, 2 2 Stanford University cancer study, 3 3 6 State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, 4 0 5 , 4 1 1 State University of N e w Y o r k - A l b a n y , 4 0 9 9 Stepped care approach, 111 Stimulation therapies, and pain management, 241-242 Stimulus-background models of disease, 338f of recovery, 339f Stimulus-Organismic-Response-Consequence (SORC), 285 Stress, 1 6 9 - 1 8 4 . See also Posttraumatic stress disorder and alcoholism, 8 7 - 8 8 assessment and treatment of, 1 7 6 - 1 8 2 and cancer, 3 3 2 - 3 3 3 , 3 3 4 case study of, 1 8 3 - 1 8 4 chronic, 1 7 2 - 1 7 3 , 1 7 5 - 1 7 6 and chronic pain, 2 3 0 and coronary heart disease, 2 9 1 - 2 9 4 , 292t definitions a n d descriptions of, 1 6 9 - 1 7 3 and diabetes, 3 0 7 and hypertension, 2 5 3 - 2 5 5 , 2S6f models of, 1 7 3 - 1 7 5 and obesity, 135 physiology of response to, 171 f women and, 4 7 2 Stress inoculation, 1 8 1 - 1 8 2 Stress management
and coronary heart disease, 2 9 3 and diabetes, 3 1 5 and hypertension, 2 6 1 - 2 6 2 techniques for, 1 7 7 - 1 8 2 theory of, 1 7 3 - 1 7 4 Stroke, 4 8 9 Structured Clinical Interview for D S M - I V Axis I Disorders (SCID), 110, 4 1 1 Substance abuse. See also Alcohol dependence; Nicotine dependence and medication-seeking behavior, 192 screening for, 2 4 stress and, 176 Suicide, and cancer, 3 4 1 . See also Physician-assisted suicide Surgery and cancer, 3 2 7 and obesity treatment, 1 3 6 - 1 3 7 and pain management, 2 4 1 Swedish Obesity Study, 137 Symptom Checklist-90, 3 3 4 Symptom Checklist-90—Revised, 2 3 , 177, 2 4 0 0 Symptom Reduction Score, 4 0 9 Systems, health care. See Health care systems
Tarasoff v. Board of Regents of the University of California (1976), 4 5 0 Target behaviors, 5 2 7 T a s k Force on Benzodiazepine Dependency of American Psychiatric Association, 192 Technology, and diabetes interventions, 3 1 5 - 3 1 6 Terminal stage, of cancer, 3 2 8 - 3 2 9 Test of Nonverbal Intelligence-2, 2 2 Theory of interpersonal behavior, and adherence behavior, 2 1 2 Theory of reasoned action, and adherence behavior, 211-212 Thyroid disturbances, in older adults, 4 8 8 - 4 8 9 T o b a c c o use. See also Nicotine dependence and coronary heart disease, 2 8 9 detrimental effect of, 101 incidence of, 101 and insomnia, 4 3 1 Tolerance, of nicotine, 102 Trail M a k i n g test. See under Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Battery Training, 5 curriculum for public health/medical psychology program, 509f in medical settings, 9 - 1 0 , 2 5 in motivational enhancement interventions, 5 0 - 5 1 in motivational interviewing, 52t for multidisciplinary settings, 2 8 , 3 5 - 3 8 in practical skills, 1 2 - 1 3 preprofessional, 12 in public health, 5 0 5 - 5 1 0 in screening, 2 0 Transtheoretical model, and adherence behavior, 2 1 2 Trials of Hypertension Prevention project, 2 6 2 Tripler Army Medical Center, 194
Subject Index Tuskegee Syphilis Study, 5 1 5 Type 2 diabetes ( D M 2 ) . See Diabetes Type A personality, 2 7 9 , 2 8 8 Type C personality, 3 3 3 - 3 3 4 University of A l a b a m a at Birmingham (UAB), 5 0 5 , 508-510 University of N e v a d a School of Medicine, 5 1 5 University of Washington, 2 4 3 U.S. Consumer Product Safety Commission, 2 3 7 U.S. Department of Health and H u m a n Services, 44 U.S. Environmental Protection Agency, 2 3 7 U.S. F o o d and Drug Administration, 2 9 6 U.S. Public Health Service, 5 0 7 Vaginismus, 3 6 7 Vanderbilt Pain M a n a g e m e n t Inventory, 2 3 9 Vicitimization of women, 4 7 8 - 4 7 9 Vietnamese Community Health Promotion Project, 4 6 0 Visual Analogue Scale (VAS), 343 Visual impairment, diabetes and, 3 0 5 , 3 0 6 Vogt, Oskar, 179 Waist circumference measurements, 130 Walk-in clinics, 188 Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Ill (WAIS-III), 20, 22
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale short forms, 22 West Haven-Yale Multidimensional Pain Inventory, 2 3 8 White coat hypertension, 2 5 6 - 2 5 7 Wisewoman, 4 6 0 Withdrawal, nicotine, 1 0 2 - 1 0 3 Women's health, 4 6 9 - 4 7 9 anxiety, 4 7 6 - 4 7 7 chronic pelvic pain, 4 7 7 - 4 7 8 depression, 4 7 5 - 4 7 6 dysmenorrhea, 4 7 7 hypertension, 2 6 0 infertility, 4 7 8 life course perspectives on, 4 6 9 - 4 7 2 psychosocial factors in, 4 7 2 - 4 7 5 sexual dysfunctions, 3 6 2 , 3 6 4 - 3 6 7 vicitimization, 4 7 8 - 4 7 9 Women's Healthy Lifestyle Project Clinical Trial, 133 Workplace issues, 32 Xenical (orlestat), 136 Y o g a , and stress management, 180 Zuckerman's Sensation Seeking Scale, 8 7
\
577
About the Editors
Lee M . C o h e n , P h . D . , is Assistant Professor in the Clinical Division o f the D e p a r t m e n t o f Psychology at T e x a s T e c h University and Assistant Adjunct Professor in the D e p a r t m e n t o f Neuropsychiatry and Behavioral Sciences at the T e x a s T e c h University Health Sciences Center. H e completed his predoctoral clinical internship and a p o s t d o c t o r a l fellowship funded by the N a t i o n a l Institute on D r u g Abuse at the University o f California, San D i e g o , specializing in behavioral medicine. H e completed his graduate training in clinical psychology at O k l a h o m a State University. His research interests involve systematically exploring the behavioral and physiological m e c h a n i s m s that contribute t o nicotine use and dependence. Dennis E . M c C h a r g u e , P h . D . , is R e s e a r c h Assistant Professor in the Clinical Division o f the D e p a r t m e n t o f Psychology at the University o f Illinois at C h i c a g o and H e a l t h R e s e a r c h Scientist in R e s e a r c h Services at E d w a r d H i n e s , J r . , V e t e r a n s Affairs Hospital. H e is currently funded by the N a t i o n a l Institute on D r u g Abuse on a M e n t o r e d Clinical Scientist Development A w a r d . H e completed his predoctoral clinical internship at B o s t o n University/Boston V e t e r a n s Affairs M e d i c a l Center C o n s o r t i u m and completed his p o s t d o c t o r a l training at the University o f Illinois at C h i c a g o . H e completed his graduate training in clinical psychology at O k l a h o m a State University. His research interest revolves a r o u n d testing biobehavioral m e c h a n i s m s that contribute to the development, maintenance, and eventual treatment o f t o b a c c o use disorders, especially a m o n g those individuals vulnerable t o psychopathology. F r a n k L . Collins, J r . , P h . D . , is Professor and D i r e c t o r o f Clinical T r a i n i n g in the D e p a r t m e n t o f Psychology at O k l a h o m a State University and Adjunct Professor in the D e p a r t m e n t o f Psychiatry at the University o f O k l a h o m a H e a l t h Sciences Center, where he serves on the executive c o m m i t t e e for the O k l a h o m a C e n t e r for A l c o h o l and D r u g - R e l a t e d Studies. H e received his d o c t o r a t e from A u b u r n University and completed his clinical psychology internship at the University o f Mississippi M e d i c a l Center. Prior to c o m i n g to O k l a h o m a State University, he was on the faculty at W e s t Virginia University and Rush-Presbyterian-St. L u k e ' s M e d i c a l C e n t e r in C h i c a g o . His research focuses on biobehavioral models o f nicotine dependence.
About the Contributors
Mustafa al'Absi, P h . D . , is Associate Professor o f Behavioral M e d i c i n e at the University o f M i n n e s o t a S c h o o l o f M e d i c i n e . H e directs a research p r o g r a m focusing o n stress, risk for hypertension, a n d t o b a c c o addiction. H i s research has been funded by grants from the N a t i o n a l C a n c e r Institute, N a t i o n a l Institute o n D r u g Abuse, and N a t i o n a l Health, Lung, and B l o o d Institute. H e completed his biological and clinical psychology training at the University o f O k l a h o m a a n d O k l a h o m a State University. H e completed his clinical psychology residency, specializing in behavioral medicine, at the University o f Mississippi M e d i c a l Center. H e has led several collaborative research initiatives focusing o n biobehavioral mechanisms o f hypertension a n d t o b a c c o addictions. David O . Antonuccio, P h . D . , is Professor o f Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences at the University o f N e v a d a School o f M e d i c i n e and D i r e c t o r o f the Stop S m o k i n g P r o g r a m at the R e n o V e t e r a n s Affairs M e d i c a l Center. H e received his d o c t o r a t e in clinical psychology from the University o f O r e g o n in 1 9 8 0 . H e served o n the N e v a d a State B o a r d o f Psychological E x a m i n e r s from 1 9 9 0 t o 1 9 9 8 . H e holds a diplomate in clinical psychology from the American B o a r d o f Professional Psychology a n d is a fellow o f the A m e r i c a n Psychological Association. H i s clinical and research interests include the behavioral treatment o f depression, anxiety, and smoking. Krista A. Barbour, P h . D . , is a Postdoctoral Fellow in Behavioral M e d i c i n e in the D e p a r t m e n t o f Psychiatry a n d Behavioral Sciences at the D u k e University M e d i c a l Center. She completed her predoctoral clinical internship at the University o f Mississippi M e d i c a l C e n t e r / J a c k s o n Veterans Affairs M e d i c a l C e n t e r C o n s o r t i u m , specializing in health psychology. She received her d o c t o r a t e in clinical psychology at the University o f Southern California. H e r research interests involve e x a m i n a t i o n o f physical activity in the t r e a t m e n t o f depression a n d c h r o n i c disease and the relationship between e m o t i o n a l expression a n d health. Cynthia D. Belar, P h . D . , A.B.P.P., is Executive D i r e c t o r o f the American Psychological Association E d u c a t i o n D i r e c t o r a t e and Professor in the D e p a r t m e n t o f Clinical and H e a l t h Psychology at the University o f Florida Health Science Center. She received her d o c t o r a t e from O h i o University in 1 9 7 4 after completing an internship at the D u k e University M e d i c a l Center. Since then, she has been
582
T H E HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY HANDBOOK engaged in education, training, research, practice, and administration in clinical and health psychology. Nicole E. Berlant, P h . D . , is a Behavioral M e d i c i n e C o n s u l t a n t in the Departments o f N e u r o l o g y and Adult M e d i c i n e for the Permanente M e d i c a l G r o u p in S a c r a m e n t o , California. She received her doctorate in clinical health psychology from the University o f Florida in 2 0 0 2 . She received her undergraduate degree from the University o f California, San D i e g o , and completed a clinical internship at the Veterans Affairs M e d i c a l Center in L o n g B e a c h , California. She has authored or coauthored m o r e than 1 0 professional articles, abstracts, and b o o k chapters in the area o f behavioral medicine. Andrew C . Blalock, P h . D . , is a Coordinating Center Liaison for the M e n t a l Health H I V Services C o l l a b o r a t i v e P r o g r a m , a 5-year Substance Abuse and M e n t a l H e a l t h Services Administration-funded service project. H e completed his doctoral training at Georgia State University and completed his predoctoral internship in the D e p a r t m e n t o f Psychiatry at the University o f C h i c a g o . H e was awarded a N a t i o n a l Institute o f M e n t a l H e a l t h H I V / A I D S clinical research postdoctoral fellowship through the D e p a r t m e n t o f Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences at E m o r y University before joining the D e p a r t m e n t o f Behavioral Sciences and Health Education in the Rollins S c h o o l o f Public Health. His clinical and research interests include employment and return-to-work issues for persons with H I V / A I D S and neuropsychological aspects o f H I V disease progression. Lindsey Bloor, M . S . , is a graduate student in the Clinical H e a l t h Psychology P r o g r a m o f the D e p a r t m e n t o f Psychology at the University o f Utah. H e r research interests involve exploring the influence o f psychosocial factors, particularly social support, on physical and mental health o u t c o m e s . H e r clinical interests include facilitating support groups for people living with c a n c e r . Jennifer L. Boothby, P h . D . , is Assistant Professor in the D e p a r t m e n t o f Psychology at Indiana State University. She completed her graduate training in clinical psychology at the University o f A l a b a m a in T u s c a l o o s a and completed her predoctoral clinical internship at the University o f N o r t h Carolina S c h o o l o f M e d i c i n e . H e r research interests involve the application o f forensic issues t o health psychology and chronic pain such as pain and malingering, personal injury assessment, and health issues affecting prisoners. Joaquin Borrego, Jr., P h . D . , is Assistant Professor in the Clinical Division o f the D e p a r t m e n t o f Psychology at T e x a s T e c h University and Clinical Professor in the C o m m u n i c a t i o n Disorders D e p a r t m e n t in the S c h o o l o f Allied Health at the T e x a s T e c h University H e a l t h Sciences Center. H e received his master's and doctorate in clinical psychology from the University o f N e v a d a , R e n o . H e completed his predoctoral clinical internship at the University o f California, San D i e g o . His research interests include the assessment and treatment o f child physical abuse, behavioral observations o f physically abusive parent-child relationships, assessment o f different cultural parenting and discipline practices in the c o n t e x t o f child maltreatment, c o m m u n i t y interventions with ethnic minority populations, and the
About the
Contributors
development, implementation, and evaluation o f psychosocial treatments with Spanish-speaking populations. Peter E . Campos, P h . D . , is D i r e c t o r o f P R N D a t a , a private health research consulting business. H e currently serves as c o o r d i n a t o r o f the clinical core o f the Substance Abuse and M e n t a l Health Services Administration-funded
Mental
H e a l t h H I V Services Collaborative P r o g r a m through the Rollins School o f Public Health at E m o r y University. H e received his doctorate in clinical/community psychology at the University o f H a w a i i and completed his predoctoral internship at the University o f Mississippi M e d i c a l Center. H e was previously a faculty m e m b e r in psychiatry and behavioral sciences at E m o r y University's School o f M e d i c i n e , where he was clinical director o f mental health services at the Grady Infectious Disease P r o g r a m . His interests in H I V w o r k include culturally competent mental health services, psychosocial and psychiatric sequelae o f living with H I V , and the intersection o f Eastern philosophy and cognitive-behavioral therapy. John M. Chaney, P h . D . , is Professor o f Clinical Psychology at O k l a h o m a State University and Clinical Associate Professor o f Psychiatry at the University o f O k l a h o m a H e a l t h Sciences Center. H e completed his internship in pediatric psychology at the O k l a h o m a University Health Sciences Center and received his doctorate from the University o f M i s s o u r i - C o l u m b i a in 1 9 9 1 . H e is the current president o f the N a t i o n a l Society o f Indian Psychologists and serves on the A m e r i c a n Psychological Association's C o m m i t t e e on Ethnic M i n o r i t y Affairs. His research interests focus on children's adjustment to chronic medical illness and on implicit racism affecting academic achievement a m o n g Native Americans. Anna Chur-Hansen, P h . D . , is Senior Lecturer in the D e p a r t m e n t o f Psychiatry at the University o f Adelaide in South Australia. She is a m e m b e r o f the Australian Psychological Society's College o f H e a l t h Psychologists and was the recipient o f the Australian and N e w Z e a l a n d Association for M e d i c a l Education A w a r d for R e s e a r c h in 2 0 0 0 . She has taught medical undergraduate students in the behavioral sciences since 1 9 8 7 . H e r research focuses on evaluations o f teaching initiatives and their assessment for medical and health sciences students, particularly the disciplines o f psychology, anthropology, and psychiatry. Matthew M. Clark, P h . D . , A.B.P.P., is Associate Professor o f Psychology and C o - S e c t i o n H e a d o f Outpatient M e d i c a l Psychiatry and Psychology, as well as D i r e c t o r o f the M e d i c a l Psychology Fellowship Program, in the D e p a r t m e n t o f Psychiatry and Psychology at the M a y o Clinic in R o c h e s t e r , M i n n e s o t a . H e received his doctorate in clinical psychology from F o r d h a m University, was a predoctoral clinical psychology intern at the Syracuse Veterans Affairs M e d i c a l Center, completed a postdoctoral fellowship in behavioral medicine at the B r o w n University School o f M e d i c i n e , and is a board-certified clinical health psychologist. His research focuses on behavioral interventions for weight m a n a g e m e n t , nicotine dependence, and coping with c a n c e r . Currently, his research is funded by the N a t i o n a l C a n c e r Institute.
584
T H E HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY HANDBOOK Monica Cortez-Garland, B . A . , is a doctoral student in the Clinical Division o f the D e p a r t m e n t o f Psychology at T e x a s T e c h University. H e r research interests fall within addictive disorders. H e r current research endeavors include the investigation o f personality characteristics and their relation to smoking behavior. Csilla T . Csoboth, M . D . , is Assistant Professor in the Institute o f Behavioral Sciences at Semmelweis University in Budapest, Hungary. She received her medical diploma at Semmelweis University and specializes in psychiatry. She is currently completing her doctorate on the psychosocial risk factors affecting w o m e n ' s mental health. H e r other research interests include health-damaging behavior (e.g., obesity, nicotine and alcohol abuse), violence against w o m e n , and methods o f prevention a m o n g w o m e n . Patricia M. Dubbert, P h . D . , is Associate Chief o f M e n t a l Health and D i r e c t o r o f Psychology at the G . V . (Sonny) M o n t g o m e r y Veterans Affairs M e d i c a l Center and Professor o f Psychiatry, Preventive M e d i c i n e , and M e d i c i n e at the University o f Mississippi School o f M e d i c i n e . She received her bachelor's degree in nursing in 1 9 6 8 from the University o f M i s s o u r i - C o l u m b i a , her master's in mental health nursing in 1 9 7 2 from N e w Y o r k University, and her doctorate in psychology in 1 9 8 2 from Rutgers University. Mitch Earleywine, P h . D . , is Associate Professor o f Clinical Sciences in the D e p a r t m e n t o f Psychology at the University o f Southern California. H e received his doctorate from Indiana University and completed internship at the University o f Mississippi M e d i c a l School C o n s o r t i u m . H e has m o r e than 4 0 publications in the addictions literature, including the b o o k Understanding
Marijuana.
Barry A . Edelstein, P h . D . , is Professor o f Psychology in the Clinical Psychology Program at W e s t Virginia University. His current research interests include decision-making processes, assessment o f decision-making c o m p e t e n c e , anxiety, and suicidality a m o n g older adults. Sadie Emery, B . S . , is a doctoral student in the Clinical Division o f the Department o f Psychology at T e x a s T e c h University. H e r current research interests include investigating whether Cortisol is a m a r k e r for nicotine withdrawal and investigating the effect o f emotional expressive writing on nicotine withdrawal. Joel Erblich, P h . D . , is Assistant Professor o f Biobehavioral M e d i c i n e in the C a n c e r Prevention and C o n t r o l Program o f the Ruttenberg C a n c e r Center at the M o u n t Sinai School o f M e d i c i n e in N e w Y o r k City. H e completed his predoctoral clinical internship at the University o f California, Los Angeles, and completed a postdoctoral fellowship funded by the N a t i o n a l C a n c e r Institute at the M e m o r i a l Sloan Kettering C a n c e r Center, specializing in behavioral medicine. H e completed his graduate training in clinical psychology at the University o f Southern California. His research interests involve understanding the cognitive, behavioral, and genetic influences on risk for addiction. Myles S. Faith, P h . D . , is Associate R e s e a r c h Scientist at the N e w Y o r k Obesity Research Center ( N Y O R C ) and Assistant Professor o f Psychology in Psychiatry at the C o l u m b i a University College o f Physicians and Surgeons. H e received his
About the
Contributors
doctorate in clinical/school psychology from Hofstra University in 1 9 9 5 , after which he completed a 3-year postdoctoral fellowship at the N Y O R C . During this fellowship, he received training in behavioral-genetic methods for studying h u m a n obesity and focused on determinants o f childhood obesity. H e is funded to study genetic and environmental influences on caloric regulation and body fat in young children and has conducted behavioral interventions for c h i l d h o o d obesity. William C . Follette, P h . D . , is Associate Professor in the Clinical P r o g r a m o f the D e p a r t m e n t o f Psychology, as well as R e s e a r c h Associate Professor in Family and C o m m u n i t y M e d i c i n e , at the University o f N e v a d a , R e n o . H e received his doctorate in clinical psychology from the University o f W a s h i n g t o n . H e has served on a n u m b e r o f review panels for the N a t i o n a l Institutes o f Health and the N a t i o n a l Institute on Drug Abuse. In addition to interests in applied clinical behavior analysis and psychotherapy research design and methodology, he studies psychotherapy treatment development. Michael D . Franzen, P h . D . , is Associate Professor o f Psychiatry in the H a h n e m a n n School o f M e d i c i n e at D r e x e l University and Chief Psychologist at Allegheny General Hospital in Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania. H e completed his doctorate in clinical psychology at Southern Illinois University at C a r b o n d a l e . H e completed an internship and a postdoctoral fellowship in neuropsychology at the University o f N e b r a s k a School o f M e d i c i n e . H e is a fellow o f Division 4 0 o f the A m e r i c a n Psychological Association and a fellow o f the N a t i o n a l A c a d e m y o f Neuropsychology. H e has published extensively in the area o f neuropsychological assessment, test construction, dementia, and head injury. Sheila Garos, P h . D . , is Assistant Professor in the D e p a r t m e n t o f Psychology at T e x a s T e c h University and Assistant Adjunct Professor in Neuropsychiatry in the Division o f Urology at the T e x a s T e c h University Health Sciences Center. She has m o r e than 1 8 years o f experience in mental health. H e r areas o f clinical expertise include the assessment and treatment o f sexual disorders and dysfunctions, marital therapy, addiction, and adult psychopathology. H e r research interests include sexual attraction, compulsive sexual behaviors, w o m e n ' s sexuality, and psychometrics. Elizabeth V . Gifford, B . A . , is a doctoral candidate at the University o f N e v a d a , R e n o , and an intern at the Veterans Affairs (VA) Palo Alto H e a l t h Care System. She is currently working with the Center for Health C a r e Evaluation at Stanford University, the V A Palo Alto Health Care System, and the V A T a s k F o r c e on Violence Prevention. She is the recipient o f a Career D e v e l o p m e n t A w a r d from the N a t i o n a l C a n c e r Institute. H e r interests include addiction treatment, t o b a c c o control, social c o n t e x t and coping, and social and health policy. Suzy Bird Gulliver, P h . D . , is Staff Psychologist and D i r e c t o r o f H e a l t h Psychology at the Veterans Affairs B o s t o n H e a l t h c a r e System Outpatient Clinic as well as Assistant Professor in the B o s t o n University School o f M e d i c i n e ' s D e p a r t m e n t o f Psychiatry and Assistant Clinical Professor in the D e p a r t m e n t o f Psychology at B o s t o n University. She completed her doctoral training in clinical psychology at the University o f V e r m o n t . H e r predoctoral internship was conducted at the W e s t
585
586
T H E HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY HANDBOOK H a v e n Veterans Affairs M e d i c a l Center, followed by a N a t i o n a l Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism postdoctoral fellowship in a l c o h o l treatment o u t c o m e at B r o w n University. H e r research interests are in cross-addictions, c o m o r b i d psychopathology, and the role o f affect (dys)regulation. Kimberly R. Haala, B . S . , is a graduate student in clinical psychology at O k l a h o m a State University. She received her b a c h e l o r ' s degree at M i n n e s o t a State University, M a n k a t o . H e r research interests include investigating behavioral and physiological correlates o f nicotine use. Ahna L . Hoff, M . S . , is a pediatric psychology intern in the Department o f Psychology at Columbus Children's Hospital in Columbus, O h i o . She completed her graduate training in clinical child psychology at O k l a h o m a State University. H e r research interests include the development o f behavioral and cognitive interventions designed to mitigate psychological distress in the c o n t e x t o f chronic illness. Richard G. Hoffman, P h . D . , is Associate Professor o f Behavioral Sciences and Associate D e a n for M e d i c a l E d u c a t i o n and Curriculum at the University o f M i n n e s o t a School o f M e d i c i n e in Duluth. H e completed his predoctoral clinical internship in clinical and pediatric psychology and his postdoctoral fellowship in clinical neuropsychology at the University o f O k l a h o m a Health Sciences Center. H e completed his graduate training in clinical psychology in the B r o o k l y n Center at L o n g Island University. H i s research interests include brain-behavior relationships in closed head injury and the effects o f stress on neuropsychological functioning. Timothy T. Houle, P h . D . , is a postdoctoral fellow in behavioral medicine in the Center for Pain Studies at the R e h a b i l i t a t i o n Institute o f C h i c a g o / N o r t h w e s t e r n M e d i c a l School. H e completed his predoctoral clinical internship at the University of Mississippi M e d i c a l C e n t e r / J a c k s o n Veterans Affairs M e d i c a l Center C o n s o r t i u m , specializing in health psychology. H e completed his graduate training in clinical psychology at the Illinois Institute o f T e c h n o l o g y . His research interests involve the use o f time-series analysis in the clinical m a n a g e m e n t o f chronic pain. Wei-Chin Hwang, C.Phil., is a graduate student in clinical psychology at the University o f California, L o s Angeles. H e is currently completing his predoctoral clinical internship at the R i c h m o n d Area Multi-Services' N a t i o n a l Asian American Psychology Training Center. H i s research interests include cultural influences on the prevalence, etiology, diagnosis, and expression o f mental illness as well as ethnocultural differences in help seeking and treatment progress. Mark W. Ketterer, P h . D . , is a m e m b e r o f the Senior Bioscientific Staff at H e n r y F o r d Hospital and Adjunct Associate Professor o f Psychology at W a y n e State University. H e received his doctorate from the University o f M a r y l a n d , completed his internship at the J o h n s H o p k i n s University School o f M e d i c i n e , and completed a postdoctoral fellowship at the Uniformed Services University o f the Health Sciences. His research interests include behavioral risk factors in c o r o n a r y heart disease, circumventing denial/minimization o f e m o t i o n a l distress in c o r o n a r y heart disease patients, treatment o f anginal chest pain with cognitive-behavioral therapy and selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors, clinical trials, and health care reform.
About the
Contributors
J o s h u a C . K l a p o w , P h . D . , is Associate Professor in the D e p a r t m e n t s o f Psychology and Health Care O r g a n i z a t i o n and Policy at the University o f A l a b a m a at B i r m i n g h a m . H e received his doctorate in clinical psychology from the University o f California, San D i e g o , where he also completed a postdoctoral fellowship in geriatric health services research. His research focuses on the evaluation o f health status and quality o f life in chronic illness, including the use o f multivariate statistical modeling to evaluate change in health status. Lesley P . K o v e n , Μ . Α., is a doctoral student in the Clinical Psychology P r o g r a m at W e s t Virginia University. She earned her b a c h e l o r ' s degree at the University o f M a n i t o b a and earned her master's in clinical psychology at W e s t Virginia University. H e r research interests include geropsychology and behavioral medicine. H e r m o s t current project involves the e x a m i n a t i o n o f etiological variables related to the " s u n d o w n s y n d r o m e " in nursing h o m e residents with dementia. Shulamith Kreitler, P h . D . , is Professor o f Psychology in the D e p a r t m e n t o f Psychology at Tel-Aviv University and H e a d o f the P s y c h o - O n c o l o g y Unit at Tel-Aviv M e d i c a l Center. She completed her doctorate in psychology and psychopathology at Bern University in Switzerland. She also was a postdoctoral fellow at Y a l e University and a research fellow at the Educational Testing Service in Princeton, N e w Jersey. H e r research interests involve exploring the psychological correlates o f disease occurrence and different kinds o f disease course, especially in c a n c e r . Melissa C . Kuhajda, P h . D . , is Assistant Professor in the Department o f C o m m u n i t y and Rural Medicine and the Department o f Psychiatry and Behavioral Medicine in the College o f C o m m u n i t y Health Sciences at the University o f A l a b a m a in T u s c a l o o s a . She is also Assistant Director for Research in the University o f A l a b a m a Institute for R u r a l Health Research and is Adjunct Assistant Professor in the Department o f Psychology. She completed her doctorate in clinical psychology at the University o f A l a b a m a and completed her predoctoral internship at the M e m p h i s Veterans Affairs Hospital. H e r research interests involve the study o f treatments for chronic pain management and rural health issues. Jeffrey M . L a c k n e r , Psy.D., is Assistant Professor in the D e p a r t m e n t o f M e d i c i n e , as well as R e s e a r c h Assistant Professor in the Departments o f Neurosurgery and Anesthesiology, at the State University o f N e w Y o r k , Buffalo, M e d i c a l S c h o o l . H e completed his predoctoral clinical internship at the University o f T e x a s M e d i c a l School in H o u s t o n before receiving a N a t i o n a l Institutes o f Health-funded postdoctoral fellowship in behavioral medicine/pain at the University o f R o c h e s t e r M e d i c a l School. H e completed his graduate training in social psychology at the L o n d o n School o f E c o n o m i c s , in experimental psychology at the College o f W i l l i a m and M a r y , and in clinical psychology at Rutgers University. His primary research interests include o u t c o m e research and cognitive processes underlying painful medical disorders as well as their relationship to physiological mechanisms o f health and illness. T h a d R . Leffingwell, P h . D . , is Clinical Psychologist and Assistant Professor in the D e p a r t m e n t o f Psychology at O k l a h o m a State University. H e completed his
588
T H E HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY HANDBOOK graduate training at the University o f W a s h i n g t o n and his predoctoral internship at the Puget Sound Veterans Affairs Healthcare System-Seattle. His research interests include brief motivational interventions for health behavior change and motivational predictors o f self-directed and assisted behavior change. H e has w o r k e d on five different federal, state, and privately funded intervention projects that investigated adaptations o f motivational interviewing. William R. Lovallo, P h . D . , is Professor o f Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences at the University o f O k l a h o m a Health Sciences Center and D i r e c t o r o f the Behavioral Sciences Laboratories at the O k l a h o m a City Veterans Affairs M e d i c a l Center. His doctorate in biological psychology is from the University o f O k l a h o m a . H e was associate director o f the M a c A r t h u r Foundation's R e s e a r c h N e t w o r k on M i n d - B o d y Interactions and is the author o f Stress and Health: Psychological
Interactions.
Biological
and
H e has received funding from the Veterans
Administration and the N a t i o n a l Institutes o f Health to study stress mechanisms and risk for disease. Alexander Michas, B . A . , is currently pursuing his graduate education in California. H e completed his undergraduate education in psychology with honors at the University o f California, Santa Cruz. After working as a mentor and counselor in California, he joined the Health Psychology Clinic at the Veterans Affairs Boston Healthcare System Outpatient Clinic as a senior research assistant. Brian I . Miller, B . S . , is a doctoral student in clinical psychology at O k l a h o m a State University. H e completed his bachelor's degree at the State University o f N e w Y o r k at B i n g h a m t o n , majoring in psychobiology. His research interests include exploring the role o f the acoustic startle response and emotion-modulated startle in deprived and nondeprived smokers. Larry L. Mullins, P h . D . , is Professor o f Clinical Psychology and Associate D i r e c t o r o f Clinical Training at O k l a h o m a State University as well as Clinical Professor o f Psychiatry at the University o f O k l a h o m a Health Sciences Center. H e completed his internship in pediatric psychology at the O k l a h o m a University Health Sciences Center and received his doctorate from the University o f M i s s o u r i - C o l u m b i a in 1 9 8 3 . His research interests focus on the relationship o f cognitive appraisal mechanisms and children's adjustment to various chronic illnesses. Jack L.-M. Mutnick, B . S . , is a medical student at the University o f Nevada School o f Medicine and will be completing his M . D . in M a y 2 0 0 4 . H e earned his bachelor's degree in biology at the University o f N e v a d a , R e n o . Hector F . Myers, P h . D . , is Professor o f Psychology at the University o f California, Los Angeles ( U C L A ) , and D i r e c t o r o f the R e s e a r c h Center on Ethnicity, Health, and Behavior at the Charles R . D r e w University o f M e d i c i n e and Science. H e received his doctorate in clinical psychology from U C L A and is actively involved in research on psychosocial stress and behavioral contributors to ethnic health disparities in hypertension and cerebrovascular disease, H I V / A I D S , and m o o d disorders. H e has m o r e t h a n 1 0 0 publications and has received several awards for his research and student mentoring.
About the Contributors Kathleen M. Palm, M . A . , is a graduate student in clinical psychology at the University o f N e v a d a , R e n o . She received her master's in clinical psychology from M i n n e s o t a State University, M a n k a t o , in 1 9 9 8 . H e r research and clinical interests involve issues related to t r a u m a and behavioral medicine. Chebon A . Porter, P h . D . , is Staff Psychologist at the B i r m i n g h a m Veterans Affairs M e d i c a l Center's ( B V A M C ) Southeastern Blind R e h a b i l i t a t i o n Center. H e is also Assistant Professor in the D e p a r t m e n t o f Psychology at the University o f A l a b a m a at B i r m i n g h a m (UAB) S c h o o l o f M e d i c i n e and Assistant Adjunct Professor in the D e p a r t m e n t o f Psychology at Samford University. H e completed his postdoctoral fellowship in the U A B D e p a r t m e n t o f Psychiatry and Behavioral N e u r o b i o l o g y , completed his predoctoral clinical internship at the U A B / B V A M C Clinical Psychology T r a i n i n g C o n s o r t i u m , and completed his graduate training in clinical psychology at O k l a h o m a State University. His research interests include mental health issues in A m e r i c a n Indians and Alaska Natives. Eric H . Prensky, M . A . , is a doctoral student in the American Psychological Association-accredited Clinical Psychology Program at T e x a s T e c h University. His research interests involve smokeless t o b a c c o and personality variables. Sheri D . Pruitt, Ph.D., is Director o f Behavioral Medicine for the Permanente Medical Group in Sacramento, California. She received her doctorate in clinical psychology from the University o f N e w M e x i c o in 1 9 9 0 . She has 2 0 years o f health care experience in both the private and public sectors and has been a faculty member at the University o f California, San Diego, School o f Medicine and in the Department o f Psychology at San Diego State University. She has worked as a scientist for the W o r l d Health Organization ( W H O ) , was the principal writer for the recently published W H O global report on innovative care for chronic conditions, and continues to be an ongoing consultant to the W H O ' s Department of Noncommunicable Diseases. She has authored more than 5 0 professional articles, abstracts, and b o o k chapters in the area o f behavioral medicine. Steven M. Schwartz, P h . D . , is D i r e c t o r o f R e s e a r c h for O a k w o o d H e a l t h c a r e System and Clinical Assistant Professor in the Behavioral M e d i c i n e P r o g r a m o f the Department o f Psychiatry at the University o f M i c h i g a n . H e received his doctorate in clinical psychology from Virginia C o m m o n w e a l t h University with a behavioral medicine specialty and completed his predoctoral internship at H e n r y F o r d Hospital in D e t r o i t , specializing in medical psychology. H e went on to a postdoctoral fellowship in the Behavioral M e d i c i n e P r o g r a m at the University o f M i c h i g a n , where he subsequently served as the faculty c o o r d i n a t o r o f behavioral medicine services for University o f M i c h i g a n Hospital until taking over as director o f research for O a k w o o d . His research interests include behavioral cardiology, self-management o f chronic illness, and effectiveness research. Richard J . Seime, P h . D . , A.B.P.P., is Associate Professor o f Psychology, C o - H e a d o f the Section o f Integrated Evaluation and T r e a t m e n t , and C o o r d i n a t o r o f the Adult Behavioral T h e r a p y - C l i n i c a l H e a l t h Psychology T r a c k in the M e d i c a l Psychology Fellowship P r o g r a m o f the D e p a r t m e n t o f Psychiatry and Psychology
\
589
590
THE HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY HANDBOOK at the M a y o Clinic in R o c h e s t e r , M i n n e s o t a . H e received his doctorate at the University o f M i n n e s o t a , completed his internship at the M i n n e a p o l i s Veterans Affairs M e d i c a l Center, and is board certified in clinical psychology. H e previously served as professor and chief in the Section o f Psychology o f the Department o f Behavioral M e d i c i n e and Psychiatry at the W e s t Virginia University School o f M e d i c i n e . His current research and clinical focus is the identification and treatment o f depression. Andrea K . Shreve-Neiger, M . A . , is a doctoral student in the Clinical Psychology P r o g r a m at W e s t Virginia University. She earned her bachelor's degree at the College o f W o o s t e r and earned her master's in clinical psychology at W e s t Virginia University. H e r primary research and clinical interests lie in geropsychology, with a special interest in spiritual and religious issues. Nicole J . Siegfried, Ph.D., is Assistant Professor in the Department o f Psychology at Samford University. She is also Project M a n a g e r in the Department o f Medicine o f the Division o f Preventive Medicine at the University o f A l a b a m a at Birmingham (UAB) School o f M e d i c i n e , where she also completed her postdoctoral fellowship. She completed her predoctoral clinical internship at the U A B / B i r m i n g h a m Veterans Affairs M e d i c a l Center Clinical Psychology Training Consortium. H e r graduate training was completed at O k l a h o m a State University. H e r research interests focus on prevention and treatment o f w o m e n ' s health disorders. C a t h y A . Simpson, P h . D . , is Assistant Professor o f Psychology at J a c k s o n v i l l e State University. She completed her predoctoral clinical internship at the University o f Mississippi M e d i c a l C e n t e r / G . V . (Sonny) M o n t g o m e r y Veterans Affairs C o n s o r t i u m and completed postdoctoral fellowships at the University o f Kentucky and the University o f A l a b a m a at B i r m i n g h a m , specializing in addictive behaviors. She completed her graduate training in clinical psychology at Auburn University. H e r research interests involve behavioral e c o n o m i c models and analyses o f substance use and abuse. T i m o t h y W . Smith, P h . D . , is Professor in the D e p a r t m e n t o f Psychology at the University o f U t a h . H e completed his graduate training in clinical psychology at the University o f K a n s a s . H e completed his predoctoral clinical internship and a postdoctoral fellowship at B r o w n University, specializing in behavioral medicine. H e is a past president o f the Division o f Health Psychology o f the American Psychological Association and is a m e m b e r o f the A c a d e m y o f Behavioral M e d i c i n e R e s e a r c h . His research interests focus on personality and social risk factors for cardiovascular disease, particularly in the c o n t e x t o f close personal relationships such as marriage. Kristen H . S o r o c c o , P h . D . , is a N a t i o n a l Institute on A l c o h o l Abuse and Alcoholism postdoctoral fellow in the Behavioral Sciences L a b s o f the D e p a r t m e n t o f Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences at the University o f O k l a h o m a Health Sciences Center. She completed her graduate training in clinical psychology at O k l a h o m a State University, where she is n o w a visiting assistant professor. She completed her predoctoral clinical internship at the Veterans Affairs Palo Alto Health Care System, specializing in geropsychology. H e r research interests involve
About the
Contributors
examining the relationship between psychological stress and physiological processes a m o n g dementia caregivers. A d a m P. Spira, M . A . , is a doctoral student in the Clinical Psychology P r o g r a m at W e s t Virginia University. H e earned his b a c h e l o r ' s degree at the State University o f N e w Y o r k at Stony B r o o k and earned his master's in clinical psychology at W e s t Virginia University. His research interests include operant conditioning with older adults with dementia and functional neuroimaging. J . Kevin T h o m p s o n , P h . D . , has been affiliated with the D e p a r t m e n t o f Psychology at the University o f South Florida since 1 9 8 5 . H e received his doctoral degree in clinical psychology from the University o f Georgia in 1 9 8 2 . H e has authored, c o a u t h o r e d , edited, or coedited four b o o k s in the area o f eating disorders, body image, and obesity. His current research interests involve the identification o f risk factors for the development o f eating disorders, body image disturbance, and obesity. H e has been on the editorial b o a r d o f the International Disorders
Journal
of
Eating
since 1 9 9 0 and is also on the editorial boards o f four other journals.
Beverly E . T h o r n , P h . D . , is Professor and D i r e c t o r o f Clinical T r a i n i n g in the D e p a r t m e n t o f Psychology at the University o f A l a b a m a . She completed her graduate training in clinical psychology at Southern Illinois University and completed her predoctoral clinical internship at the University o f A l a b a m a at B i r m i n g h a m . H e r research interests involve the assessment and treatment o f c h r o n i c pain and illness. She is particularly interested in c o m p o n e n t analyses o f cognitive-behavioral treatments for c h r o n i c pain. J a l i e A . T u c k e r , P h . D . , M . P . H . , is Professor in the D e p a r t m e n t o f H e a l t h Behavior o f the School o f Public Health at the University o f A l a b a m a at B i r m i n g h a m ( U A B ) . H e r research, funded by the N a t i o n a l Institute on A l c o h o l Abuse and Alcoholism, investigates help seeking for drinking problems and h o w change occurs through different pathways, including natural resolutions. Because o f the relevance o f public health approaches, in 1 9 9 8 she earned a master o f public health degree in health care organization and policy from U A B . J a n e l l e L . W a g n e r , M . S . , is a graduate student in clinical psychology at O k l a h o m a State University. H e r research interests focus on psychosocial adjustment in children with c h r o n i c illnesses, specifically the juvenile rheumatic diseases, and their families. Stephen P. Whiteside, P h . D . , is a postdoctoral fellow in the D e p a r t m e n t o f Psychiatry and Psychology at the M a y o Clinic in R o c h e s t e r , M i n n e s o t a . H e completed his predoctoral clinical internship at Geisinger M e d i c a l Center in Danville, Pennsylvania, specializing in pediatric psychology. H e completed his graduate training in clinical psychology at the University o f K e n t u c k y . His research interests involve investigating the effectiveness o f psychological interventions, including the effects o f cognitive-behavioral therapy on the cognitive misattributions and neural activity in obsessive-compulsive disorder. D e b o r a h J . W i e b e , M . P . H . , P h . D . , is Associate Professor in the D e p a r t m e n t o f Psychology at the University o f U t a h , where she is D i r e c t o r o f Clinical T r a i n i n g .
591
592
T H E HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY H A N D B O O K She also holds an adjunct faculty appointment in the D e p a r t m e n t o f Family and Preventive M e d i c i n e at the University o f U t a h H e a l t h Sciences Center. She completed her graduate training in clinical psychology at the University o f A l a b a m a at B i r m i n g h a m , specializing in medical psychology a n d public health. She completed her predoctoral clinical internship in behavioral medicine at the W e s t Virginia University H e a l t h Sciences Center. H e r research interests involve personality, self-regulation, health, and the interpersonal and developmental aspects o f coping with c h r o n i c illness. Helen R. Winefield, P h . D . , is Associate Professor in the Faculty o f H e a l t h Sciences at the University o f Adelaide in South Australia, where she has j o i n t appointments in the Departments o f Psychology a n d Psychiatry. She is also a fellow o f the Australian Psychological Society; a m e m b e r o f its Colleges o f Clinical, Organizational, a n d H e a l t h Psychologists; a n d a registered psychologist. T h e author o f an early t e x t b o o k in behavioral science for medical students, she currently directs the university o f Adelaide's Clinical Psychology P r o g r a m a n d holds a research grant from the Better O u t c o m e s in M e n t a l Health C a r e initiative in Australia. Valerie A . Wolfe, P h . D . , is Behavioral M e d i c i n e Consultant at Kaiser Permanente in N o r t h e r n California. She received her doctorate in counseling and health psychology from Stanford University. She w a s a teaching fellow at Stanford, an adjunct faculty m e m b e r at Santa C l a r a University, a n d a researcher/clinician at the Palo A l t o , M e n l o P a r k , a n d Albuquerque Veterans Affairs hospitals. Before starting at Kaiser, she w a s the director o f a 3 0 - b e d residential treatment center. She has trained hundreds o f physicians in the use o f cognitive-behavioral strategies to treat insomnia and has presented at national meetings regarding the treatment and evaluation o f insomnia. Barbara A . Wolfsdorf, P h . D . , is Staff Psychologist a n d Associate D i r e c t o r o f Health Psychology at the V e t e r a n s Affairs (VA) B o s t o n H e a l t h c a r e System Outpatient Clinic and Assistant Professor o f Psychiatry at the B o s t o n University School o f M e d i c i n e . She completed her doctoral training in clinical psychology at the University o f M i a m i . She subsequently completed a predoctoral internship at the B o s t o n V A Internship C o n s o r t i u m and a N a t i o n a l Institutes o f H e a l t h postdoctoral fellowship in c o m b i n e d treatment o u t c o m e research at the B r o w n University S c h o o l o f M e d i c i n e . T h e focus o f her research is the process o f e m o t i o n regulation as it applies t o psychopathology (e.g., depression, posttraumatic stress disorder) a n d addictions (e.g., a l c o h o l , nicotine) as well as the treatment o f these difficulties.