Language Change in South American Indian Languages [Reprint 2016 ed.] 9781512803068

South American Indian Languages are a particularly rich field for comparative study, and this book brings together some

128 27 6MB

English Pages 312 Year 2016

Report DMCA / Copyright

DOWNLOAD PDF FILE

Table of contents :
Contents
Foreword
Acknowledgments
I Introduction
A Résumé of Comparative Studies in South American Indian Languages
II Classification and Typological Problems
How to Deal with Unclassified Languages: An Ethnolinguistic View of Comparative Linguistics
Vowel Shift in the Tupi-Guarani Language Family: A Typological Approach
A Spatial Model of Lexical Relationships Among Fourteen Cariban Varieties
III Comparative Linguistics
The Phonology of Ranquel and Phonological Comparisons with Other Mapuche Dialects
Southern Peruvian Quechua Consonant Lenition
IV Grammatical Matters
Variations in Tense-Aspect Markers Among Inga (Quechuan) Dialects
The Minimal Finite Verbal Paradigm in Mapuche or Araucanian at the End of the Sixteenth Century
V Ethnolinguistics
The Talátur: Ceremonial Chant of the Atacama People
VI Distant Relationships
Amazonian Origins and Affiliations of the Timucua Language
Uto-Aztecan Affinities with Panoan of Peru I: Correspondences
Appendix: Language Families
Bibliography of Comparative Studies
Contributors
Index
Recommend Papers

Language Change in South American Indian Languages [Reprint 2016 ed.]
 9781512803068

  • 0 0 0
  • Like this paper and download? You can publish your own PDF file online for free in a few minutes! Sign Up
File loading please wait...
Citation preview

Language Change in South American Indian Languages

Language Change in South American Indian Languages Edited by Mary Ritchie Key

U^l University of Pennsylvania Press Philadelphia

Copyright © 1991 by the University of Pennsylvania Press "Southern Peruvian Quechua Consonant Lcnition" copyright © 1991 bv Bruce Mannheim ALL RIGHTS R E S E R V E D Printed in the United States of America

Library o f Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Language change in South American Indian languages / edited by Marv Ritchie Key. p. cm. Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN 0-8122-3060-4 1. Indians of South America—Languages—Classification. 2. Indians of South A m e r i c a — L a n g u a g e s — G r a m m a r . Linguistic change. 4. Comparative linguistics. I. Key, Mary Ritchie. PM5008.L36 1991 498—dc20

90-26311 CIP

Contents

Foreword Henry M. Hoenißswald and Franklin C. Southworth Acknowledgments

vii ix

I. Introduction A Résumé of Comparative Studies in South American Indian Languages Mary Ritchie Key

II. Classification and Typological Problems How to Deal with Unclassified Languages: An Ethnolinguistic View of Comparative Linguistics Aurore Monod-Becquelin Vowel Shift in the Tupi-Guarani Language Family: A Typological Approach Marilia Facö Soares and Tonne Leite A Spatial Model of Lexical Relationships Among Fourteen Cariban Varieties Maria Eugenia Villalon

23

36

54

III. Comparative Linguistics The Phonology of Ranquel and Phonological Comparisons with Other Mapuche Dialects Ana Fernandez Garay

97

vi

Contents

Southern Peruvian Quechua Consonant Lenition Bruce Mannheim

111

IV. Grammatical Matters Variations in Tense-Aspect Markers Among Inga (Quechuan) Dialects Stephen H. Levinsohn The Minimal Finite Verbal Paradigm in Mapuche or Araucanian at the End of the Sixteenth Century Adalberto Solas

145

166

V. Ethnolinguistics The Talâtur: Ceremonial Chant of the Atacama People Gustavo Rodriguez

181

VI. Distant Relationships Amazonian Origins and Affiliations of the Timucua Language Julian Cranberry

195

Uto-Aztecan Affinities with Panoan of Peru I: Correspondences Lila Wistrand-Robinson

243

Appendix: Language Families

277

Bibliography of Comparative Studies

279

Contributors

287

Index

291

Foreword

Historical and comparative work on South American indigenous languages is a great source of insight. No special plea ought to be needed to justify their study as phenomena in the physical and social world: if anything is worth analyzing and understanding it is human speech in its fabulous diversity—a diversity which has always existed but which in our days is exposed to radical diminution, not unlike the threat faced by living species. In addition, linguists have commonly neglected South American languages, to the detriment of their craft. This is particularly true when it comes to classification and reconstruction and the cthnohistorical and archaeological interpretation which those two activities allow. An understanding of the difference between classification by resemblance or type ("comparative" in the ordinary sense) and by genesis or filiation ("comparative" in the special, professional sense) needs large geographical areas and great diversification to be feasible and convincing; and what better universe to explore than that immense region which is mercifully free of much of the intellectual folklore that must be first combatted in other parts of the world? With regard, in particular, to the genealogical classification by filiation, we are offered an opportunity to satisfy a concrete, technical interest. Some thinkers of the past and present, including some of the most imposing and influential nineteenth-century comparativists, have thought that the methods for the reconstruction of Proto-Indo-European, much admired and indeed much worth admiring in their concreteness and coherence, could be special rather than general; that is, they might not be applicable to all languages. This is certainly not so, as has been shown (insofar as such a thing is possible) both deductively and empirically. But another question does arise: to what extent does the prevalence of given typological traits in given languages and language families in given areas yield more or fewer surprising, chronologically penetrating, and generally speaking, interpretable results?

viii

Foreword

No investigator can remain untouched by these thoughts, doubts, and hopes. This is why we profoundly and gratefully welcome the present undertaking. Henry M. Hoenigswald Franklin C. Southworth

Acknowledgments

The linguistic debt that the academic world owes is first of all to the native speakers of the languages that make up the data basic to our discipline. This volume and all useful linguistic work could not have been produced without the cooperation and dedication of the speakers of these beautiful and intriguing languages. In this volume, we recognize the expertise of Juana Cabral de Carripilón, Veneranda Cabral de Cabral, Marcelina Baigorrita, and Juana Carripí de Lima, speakers of Ranquel; Professor Francisco Tandioy J., José Chasoy Sijindioy, Miguel Cuatindioy C., Jacinto Carreta, Otilia Jamioy Y. de Peña, and Luis Manuel Tandioy T., speakers of Inga (Quechuan); Don Andrés Ramos, speaker of Atacama ( = Cunza); among many others. Comparative linguistic work is also indebted to the scholars who produced the descriptive works that make comparisons possible. Dictionaries and word lists are invaluable sources of data, without which comparative work would not be possible. The selected bibliography at the end of this volume lists only some of the scholars who have given us the foundation for our work. I also express our appreciation to the many readers behind the scenes who have contributed to make this volume more accurate and valuable. In a more public way, the Instituto Caro y Cuervo, Bogotá, Colombia, deserves our thanks. The Director, Ignacio Chaves Cuevas, and Coordinator, María Luisa Rodríguez de Montes, conducted the 1988 seminar, "Estado Actual de la Clasificación de las Lenguas Indígenas Colombianas." They brought together scholars from about eight countries who summarized the knowledge of their respective families. These papers are also listed in the bibliography at the end of the volume. The impact of the seminar will long be felt. My thanks to the authors who contributed to this volume, to Diane Enders of the linguistics office of the University of California at Irvine, and to all who offered help and encouragement along the way.

I Introduction

Mary Ritchie Key A Résumé of Comparative Studies in South American Indian Languages

Comparative studies of South American languages can contribute to our knowledge of language change that results from people distancing themselves in time and space from each other. This collection of articles by authorities on South American Indian languages treats some of the problems of change in sounds and grammar, aspects of change that are the indispensable background for understanding migrations and language classifications.

Background Matters of similarities and differences have intrigued and baffled devotees of language probably since the first times that people gathered around philosophizing about themselves and their thoughts. Every intelligent speaker may observe that "they talk differently down around the bend of the river." The historical world as Westerners know it began recording these observations rather late in human history. Texts on the history of linguistics note that the Classicists remarked about language differences. The first several centuries of the Christian era in Europe give us very few glimpses of the intellectual world making statements about relationships between languages. Percival notes, for example, "It is thought that the first clear statement of the relationship between Hebrew and Arabic was made by Yehuda Ibn Quraysh of Tahort in North Africa, a Jewish physician who is usually assumed to have flourished at the end of the ninth century and the beginning of the tenth" (1987:30). A few centuries later, shortly after the New World was discovered again, this time by Columbus, observations about similarities and relatedness began in serious. What we know about languages in the early history of linguistics comes from people who

4

Introduction

were fascinated with languages. These observers were not trained linguists by any means, and it is to the credit of the remarkable human brain that some of this information displays a brilliance of insight. No matter that their occupations were varied: lawyers, statesmen, naturalists, physicians, and clergymen contributed, along with travelers and adventurers. It is of interest to note that even our more recent linguistics has been enriched by scholars and investigators such as Benjamin Whorf, with primary occupations other than linguistics. Some early works appear to be just listings of languages: Gesner the naturalist organized Mithndates (1555) in much the same way that he organized his specimens. Still others grouped languages in classificatory style: Scaliger (1599) divided the languages of Europe into four major and seven minor classes. These earliest studies did not yet include American languages, though grammars were produced in the sixteenth century for some South American languages (Tovar and Tovar 1984). It would be another century before data from native American languages would be seriously worked into scholarly studies. Then language classifications began to be used in order to answer such questions as "Where did the Indian come from?" (Greene i960:511). A scholar of oriental languages, Adrian Reland (1708), examined various Indian languages: the Brazilian, the Peruvian, the Chilian, the Pocoman, the Carib, the Mexican, the Virginian, the Algonkin, and the Huron, and, to no one's surprise, concluded that if any relation is recognized between the languages of the two hemispheres, it must be sought after among the languages of Asia (Haven 1856:56). Remarkably, Reland's dissertation was written even before the Bering Strait was discovered. Other observers had other ideas, based on their own experiences and perspectives, respectively. The history of comparative linguistics, perhaps especially with regard to the enigmatic American Indian, shows that perspectives have more to do with the experiences and personalities of the observers than with actual facts. In particular, distortions and prejudices of the work often reflected simply the sociology of the times. Edward Brercwood, for example, wrote about the "diversity of languages" in 1614 and affirmed that "the Spanish, Italian, and French tongues are but the latine depraved, and corrupted by the inundation of the Gothes, and Vandals over the southerne partes of Europe" (Brerewood 1614, Preface). Through the eighteenth century, comparisons of languages continued to be made—sometimes between two languages, sometimes among many, and sometimes between families of languages.

Comparative Studies in South American Indian Languages

5

Catherine the Great embarked upon a comparison projcct in a grand style. She determined to compare all the known languages of the world and group them according to their similarities, thus producing the first comparative study on a global scale. She prepared a word list and sent copies around the world through diplomatic channels. As the lists were returned, she analyzed them and grouped the languages together by the inspection method. She commissioned Pallas to edit her research, which was published in 1786—89 under his name (Key 1977,1980). The next stage of comparative studies came into full bloom at the turn of the century. Sound correspondences had been noticed before, but the work of Rask, Grimm, Bopp, and others brought comparative linguistics into its glorious light. Scholars of American Indian languages now benefit from this heritage. Early in the nineteenth century, investigators scurried for word lists of Indian languages, probably stimulated by Catherine's request to George Washington (see Key 1980). Peter Duponceau (1830:76) was proud that the languages he had studied ranged from "Greenland to Cape Horn," the southern extremity of South America. A collection of vocabularies from seventy-three languages is on file at the American Philosophical Society in Philadelphia, with South American languages included (Freeman 1962:537). Benjamin Barton's (1797) comparative list also included South American languages. Duponceau (1830:90—91) later announced that the United States government was preparing to publish a national work "on the model considerably improved of that of the empress Catherine," but I have not found any evidence of it. The matter of orthography immediately presented itself as a problem because of sounds in the New World languages that do not occur in Indo-European languages, so John Pickering wrote a detailed discussion of a "uniform orthography" (1821). Duponceau indeed studied the Indian languages assiduously and influenced the recording and writing grammars of these heretofore unknown tongues, "confident that their development will contribute much to the improvement of the science of universal grammar" (1819:370). He was familiar with the writings of the celebrated mathematician Maupertuis (1750), who wrote on the origins of language and recommended studying the Indian languages "because we may chance to find some that are formed on new plans of ideas" (Duponceau 1819:370; Haven 1856:55—56). Duponceau had so steeped himself in the Indian languages that he recognized that the usual linguistic terminology could not suffice for describing the intricacies of the grammars; he is known for devising terms such as polysyn-

6

Introduction

thetic to handle new constructions (Pickering 1831:599; Edgerton 1943 :2 9)It is still true that the languages of the New World offer much to the Western world in the way of expressing nuances and representations of other world views. It is also true that orthographical problems still exist today and become even more formidable with the application of the computer to comparative studies. To illustrate, I use an example from the Mapuche language of Chile, which has historical documents dating back four hundred years. My files contain three recordings for the word 'ribbon, band' (fajita): guton, nitrohue, and nguchrohue. The letters g, n, and ng were used for the sound rj; u = i; t = chr = tr (the retroflexed stop); 0 = 0; the verbal suffix -n is deleted when another suffix is added. In the recording of this morpheme, then, only one orthographic symbol is the same in all three recordings. At about the same time as the advances of the first part of the nineteenth century, naturalist Alexander Humboldt was gathering information from the Amazon region to send to his brother Wilhelm in Germany, who pored over the linguistic data that changed theoretical discussions. Alexander candidly documented fieldwork difficulties—which even today hamper the flow of good linguistic material into the coffers of theoretical buildings—when he wrote that "it is impossible not to be constantly disturbed by the moschettoes, zancudoes, jejens, and tempraneroes, that. . . pierce the clothes with their long sucker . . . and, getting into the mouth and nostrils, set you coughing and sneezing" (Mclntyre 1985:330). One of the earliest comparative studies attempted in South America, concerning the Arawakan languages, was conducted by Felipe Salvador Gilij (1780-84). Gilij recognized sound correspondences among three of the languages—what he called "una coherencia mayor"—in the following examples (Gilij 1782:173): Espanol

maipure

giiipunave

cdvere

tabaco monte

yema yapa

dema dapa

shema shapa

The significance of Gilij's work is apparent when one realizes that it predated the unfolding of comparative linguistics that was to occur in the following century. It was a time that marked the awakening of the scientific world, where rigorous methodology became requisite. It was also a

Comparative Studies in South American Indian Languages

7

time that saw the compilation of the first encyclopedias, wherein all the knowledge of the world was to be brought together. Don Lorenzo Hcrvas y Panduro, for example, attempted to pack all that goes into the "Idea dell'universo" into six volumes, published in 178487. The languages of South America are discussed in his first volume. Hcrvas y Panduro's insights were remarkable. Though not without mistakes (which could only be recognized after the work of the nineteenth-century philologists), his discussion carries sound judgments and thoughtful advice. He commented about the possibilities of "chance" similarities between languages (Hervas y Panduro 1800:53); he emphasized the criterion of using both sound and meaning when choosing resemblances of vocabulary items (47); and he noted that these heretofore unwritten languages needed to have an alphabet of their own—the letters invented for Latin would not suffice for writing other languages (66). It is likely that his collection formed the major part of Wilhelm Humboldt's resources in American Indian languages. Modern linguistics grew out of the comparative studies of the nineteenth century. That time of scholarly attempts to define races surely had its delimiting strictures on acceptance of observations of language resemblances. It is easy to look back over the two centuries of descriptive and classificatory work and find mistakes. Errors resulted of course from linguistic naivete—after all, the discipline was struggling to be a proper science and to be a respected part of academia. But we also recognize today that errors of classification resulted from the mistaken beliefs about races that dominated the thinking of previous centuries. It is incumbent on us to recognize that such errors may form part of our inherited thinking processes; and that we take a fresh look at language data that give us a better picture of the history of South America, Indian migrations and settlements, and origins. In the last generation we witnessed a surge of interest in native American languages, partly because governments have taken a new look at their human resources and partly because the rapid development of linguistics has invigorated a whole new group of young scholars in the countries of Latin America. The latter resource is all the more precious because many of these nationals arc native speakers of the languages we have come to appreciate and value; thus they enrich the discipline of linguistics with vital information on complex systems (see, for example, Derbyshire and Pullum 1986; Key 1987; Klein and Stark 1985; and Rodriguez de Montes, in press).

8

Introduction

History of Classifications With the Age of Enlightenment came the realization that languages could be grouped into families only if there was comparable material on which to base the groupings. The word lists used through the centuries were products of their times; topological groupings of related words gave order to the universe, reflecting the thinking of those times. Thus, the proper order of the universe began with God and heaven; followed by father and mother (the family); then the body parts (from the head down); natural phenomena, plants, animals, and so on. Comenius (1659), an early scholar who used the topological arrangement, organized his word list vocabulary according to what was "obvious to the senses." The list begins with God and the world, and then moves on to man and the condition of man, including body parts and the soul. Further, Comenius admonished that teaching should be true, full, clear, and solid (Preface). Both Gilij and Catherine the Great, whose works were published in the same decade, used a similar type of listing (Key 1980:76— 87). Such an optimal word list, however, was not practical for dealing with the many languages that had minimal linguistic data. Therefore, Balbi's atlas (1826) grouped languages according to a published word list of twenty-six items. Brinton used an almost identical word list for his classification of The American Race (1891). By the middle of this century, more linguistic material was available, and Swadesh returned to a larger number of lexical items to be compared, that is, a two-hundred-word list he compiled (Swadesh 1955). With remarkable exceptions such as Gilij's work (1782), much of the grouping of languages in the early days was done on the basis of geography. Some of these connections, of course, were correct, because related languages may be spoken in contiguous areas, such as French and Spanish or Tacanan and Panoan (of Bolivia and Peru). The latter arc geographically close across connected political boundaries as well as within the countries. Daniel Brinton (1892) proposed seventy-three groups; Paul Rivet (1924) followed with seventy-seven; but Wilhelm Schmidt (1926) held to a smaller number of thirty-six (see Gray 1939:412-17). Cestmir Loukotka, who spent a lifetime working with South American languages, projected his groupings at ninety-four in 1935 and 117 in 1968. Rivet and Loukotka presented 108 families in Meillet and Cohen's 1952 revision of the languages of the world. Many of the "groups" consisted of a single language, so these numbers cannot really be equated with Antonio Tovar's groups, set forth

Comparative Studies in South American Indian Languages

9

in his 1961 Catdlojjo, which listed single languages as unclassified. Norman McQuown's comprehensive listing in 1955 was a similar type of work. Joseph Greenberg (1956 [i960], 1987) and Morris Swadesh reduced the number into "revolutionary regroupings" (see Key 1979, Chapter 2). For his encyclopedia articlc, Jorge Suarez (1982) conservatively returned to a format that resembled the earlier proposals: he classified eighty-two groups— many of them single languages. Holger Pedersen wrote a few short paragraphs about the American languages in 1924 (trans. 1931) and noted a "dizzying complexity": "It is incredible that there should be no kinship at all among some of these; but a peculiar course of linguistic development may make it difficult to discover what the original relationships were" (137). The difficulties of classifying range from relatively "easy" to complex. "Easy" (or at least well definable) questions have to do with such issues as multiple naming systems—one does not always know exactly which language is being referred to if the term used means something like "people of the north." More complicated difficulties have to do with different sources for material or with library data versus field notes (differences between written and spoken language). I have discussed these and other difficulties in "Methodology and Problems of Research" (1979:15-18). To understand the wide range of proposals of classifications outlined in the preceding paragraphs, it is necessary to take into account varying perspectives, a concept that can be summed up in the useful cliche that differentiates lumpers from splitters. It is likely that people have always had an attraction to observing similarities: note the large numbers of people who choose mates who resemble themselves. But there is also a fascination with differences: it is not uncommon for a person to marry someone who complements—the attraction of opposites. The stances taken by the lumpers and the splitters, of course, are doomed to conflict, and this should be taken as a healthy measure of the situation of dealing with the languages of the Americas. Productive research needs differences of opinion and the unremitting testing of ideas. At times this results in conflicts—difficult to explain to the nonacademic members of the community, who might believe that seeking truth makes for agreement and harmony. On the contrary, too much agreement can be stultifying; it can preclude revision and development. Whatever the total number of enumerated families of the continent of South America, there are still questions to be asked regarding the relatedness, if any, between the families. The symposium held in Colombia in

io

Introduction

February 1988 at the Instituto Caro y Cuervo (Rodriguez de Monies, in press), set some of the questions in order. Investigators of language relationships began to see that the issues are so complicated that it is difficult even to articulate the problems. To some, it was an initiation into the complexities of evaluating one classification over another. I am sure that all serious scholars experience a similar phenomenon when they begin their work: the problem does not appear as complicated at first as it turns out to be. The more we learn about a particular topic we choose to research, the more complexities become evident. If students of native language problems of South America had thought that comparative linguistic analyses would answer the problems and come up with a quick and neat classification of the some 36 to 117 languages and families, they are now aware of some of the realities behind the controversies. Notwithstanding, at the Colombian conference some useful questions were asked that lead the way to bring order to the unclassified mosaic of some 600 languages in South America (Trillos Amaya 1988:42): What urgent tasks face us as we try to make a satisfactory classification of the Indian languages? What are the problems that hinder these tasks? What are the differences of methodology in analyzing "languages" and "dialects"? What criteria should one take into account when choosing a standard variety of language? What direction is comparative linguistics taking in South America?

History of Early Comparative Work The classificatory work summarized in the previous section was the harbinger for work that was to be done after the methodology of comparative linguistics was developed. As is well known, the discipline of comparative linguistics involving sound correspondences was rigorously developed in the nineteenth century, and the systematic nature of sound correspondences was spottily commented on as far back as the seventeenth century (Metcalf 1972:97), but the methodology was not generally available until the twentieth. The bibliography at the end of this volume treats the stages of comparative work on South American languages, including preliminary remarks, word lists, and other bibliographies. As is to be expected of the early stages of theory application, the articles and books are of an uneven quality. This may be due to technique, but it also may be due to availability of material from particular languages that would be crucial to finding sound correspondences and their distributions.

Comparative Studies in South American Indian Languages

n

Systematic comparative studies in South American languages did not, of course, begin full-blown. In the 1940s and 1950s there were many articles that noted some sound correspondences and made comments about genetic relationships (references in Loriot 1964; Hoijer, Hamp, and Bright 1965). These contributions fortified the base of the earlier proposals about relatedness among the languages. The 1960s were important, in that comparative methodology began to be applied to systems as a whole, beginning with Shafer (1959, 1962) on Arawak and Aruakan; followed by Moore (1962) on the reconstruction of Colorado and Cayapa; Key (1963, published in 1968) on Tacanan; Matteson (1964) on Arawakan; Noble (1965) on Arawakan; Shell (1965) on Panoan; Davis (1966) on Je; Orr and Longacre (1968) on Quechuan. The blossoming of comparative linguistics continued in the 1970s, and the 1980s began with great promise with Constenla's comparative Chibchan study (1981). Furthermore the conference in Colombia in 1988 stimulated the systematic reconstruction of the language families of South America (Rodriguez de Montes, in press). As far as distant relationships between North and South America are concerned, we can note the pioneering study of Chipaya and Mayan by Olson (1964, 1965). With hindsight, I feel that it was premature to posit proto sounds, but this does not diminish the importance of the study, which, I predict, will be used again in the future for looking at widespread phenomena. A generation ago, it was felt that actual reconstruction was essential for establishing a relationship. Historical scholars will remember, however, that although Jakob Grimm had not reconstructed proto forms in his early studies that recognized relationships in the Germanic family, this did not detract from the importance of observing the regularities between series of sounds. As a matter of fact, use of the asterisk (*) with a hypothetical reconstructed form was introduced much later (see Hoenigswald 1973:69 for discussions), though the concept of a proto-language goes back much farther—at least as far as Leibniz. Proto forms are a convenience; they are not facts. More recently, I dealt with this question at length and pointed out that it was not the hypothetical form that had substance; rather, it was the regularity of sets of sounds that held the proof of comparisons (Key 1983). If there is great distance between the languages—as, for example, there would appear to be with Mayan and Chipaya—other languages would have come in between, and reconstruction of proto forms should not be attempted. The missing links should be sought; but it is also possible that that part of history is lost, in which case

12

Introduction

it may be impossible to reconstruct the lost languages. Nevertheless, if regularity has been seen, it is worthwhile to note it. It is even possible that the regularities have to do with borrowing, and especially if this is so, the reconstructions should not be attempted. Here I also add a note of caution in using the term "cognate," which should be reserved for those sets of lexical items that are proved to be etymologically related by completion of the tedious and time-consuming labor of reconstruction (Key 1983). Before that proof is laid out, we can refer to lexical "similarities" or "resemblances." Statements about regularity remain important, however, in that there is a common history behind borrowings. A final observation on the bibliography at the end of this volume has to do with the use of glottochronology. Important pioneering studies experimenting with techniques and methodologies were done on the Tupi-Guaraní and Ge/Jé languages by Rodrigues (1958) and Wilbcrt (1962), respectively. As noted in the previous section, there probably have always been those who are able to sec differences as against those who are better able to note similarities. Problems in linguistic classifications and relationships deal with the interpretations of these opposing views. Recent publications on comparative matters bring an invigorating outlook from the point of view of biological systems that show traits parallel with linguistic systems. The term introduced to linguistics is "cladistics" (Hoenigswald and Wiener 1987). This approach distinguishes between characteristics which are more recently derived and those which are relatively earlier—to identify the pattern of branching in the development of a linguistic group. The future is for those scholars who will take recent developments, along with the ideas of the last two centuries, to explore and experiment with the application and modification of our inherited ideas and discoveries to the growing abundance of linguistic data on the native languages of South America.

Present Contributions The focus of this book is on language change, particularly as it is seen in comparative studies. Its contributors include scholars who have done fieldwork and are presently active in research in the native languages of South America. They are or have been in close contact with actual speakers of

Comparative Studies in South American Indian Languages

13

languages discussed here, and they represent seven countries and several major research centers of South America. The first chapters deal with problems of classification. Languages may be classified for various purposes into historical and typological as well as genetic groupings. Monod-Becquelin brings a cross-disciplinary perspective to the study of groupings, with rich insights from the French schools of research. Past comparative studies of the Tupi-Guarani language family, which is among the oldest documented language groups, have been based on reliable descriptive work, and thus form a firm foundation for the work of Soares and Leite, who take the problem of vowel shift and suggest a reformulation of the proto system. Villalon treats the Cariban languages with the modern techniques of multidimensional scaling and then matches the results with information of pre-Columbian trade routes and migrations. Her work is thus relevant to the networks and movements of the people, as well as to the genetic relationships of the languages involved. The next section includes case studies of several language groups. Garay, from Argentina, describes a Mapuchc dialect in that country and compares it with the Mapuche ( = Araucanian) dialects of Chile. Of particular interest is the change in the fricative series; these are illustrated in an accompanying word list. The Quechuan languages/dialects are represented by Mannheim's article, which moves both horizontally and vertically through the dialects, bringing to light illustrations from ancient manuscripts. Grammatical markers are the focus of the next section. The Quechuan and Mapuche languages are again investigated for their grammatical differences, by Levinsohn and Salas respectively. Native Quechua speakers have contributed by providing an analyzed text to illustrate the tenseaspect markers in the Quechua language of Colombia. The next topic dealt with is ethnolinguistics, as represented by an extinct language, Atacama ( = Cunza) spoken until recently by a group of mountain people in northern Chile. Rodriguez compares a ceremonial chant recorded in mid-century with a version of the chant he recorded and analyzed a generation later. The final section in this collection moves across the American continents to project possible distant relationships. Granbcrry plots the dialect boundaries of Timucua in Florida (North America) and traces language resemblances to languages of the northern part of South America. Several word lists are presented, illustrating grammatical markers and lexical simi-

14

Introduction

laritics. T h e last chaptcr is contributed by Wistrand-Robinson, w h o has spent considerable time with the Comanche (Uto-Aztecan) and Panoan languages. She presents an exemplary w o r d list o f one hundred items, with selected representations f r o m the Uto-Aztecan and Panoan families. Investigators will be able to w o r k efficiently with examples that are pertinent to the particular comparisons. I have advocated presenting the data in such a manner so that researchers may decide for themselves whether o r not an example is useful. Interested scholars may also expand the lists according to their o w n experiences. Also, I have suggested that w o r d lists such as these, at the brainstorming stage o f exploring new possibilities, should not be thought o f o r labeled " c o g n a t e " lists, for the reasons I have given above; they are instead intended to stimulate research and to expand on other possibilities. As with all new ideas and "discoveries," it is appropriate to approach the resemblances presented here (and elsewhere) with healthy skepticism. A t the same time, one should not make snap judgments without saturating oneself in the data. Since the first recordings of American Indian languages in the sixteenth century, resemblances have been noted among them and other languages. There is sufficient descriptive material now, in addition to reliable comparative studies, so that establishing connections is an altogether viable possibility. There is plenty o f work to be done in the future by both kinds o f thinkers: those researchers w h o sec "resemblances" and those w h o justify "differences." In whichever camp the contributors o f this collection reside, they have one thing in common: the desire that comparative w o r k will proceed with dispatch. REFERENCES

Balbi, Adrien 1826 Atlas ethnographique du globe. Paris. Barton, Benjamin Smith 1797 New Views of the Origin of the Tribes and Nations of America. Philadelphia: John Bioren. 83 pages. Brerewood, Edward 1614 Enquiries Touching the Diversity of Languages, and Religions through the Chief Parts of the World. London: Gresham College. 198 pages. Brinton, Daniel Garrison 1891 The American Race. New York: N. D. C. Hodges. Reprinted New [1970] York: Johnson Reprint Corporation. 1892 "Studies in South American native languages." Philadelphia: McCalla. Reprinted from Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society 30.

Comparative Studies in South American Indian Languages

15

Comenius, Jan [Johann] Amos 1659 Orbis sensualium pictus (A World of Things Obvious to the Senses: [1970] Drawn in pictures). Reprinted Menston, England: Scolar Press. Constenla Umaña, Adolfo 1981 Comparative Chibchan phonology. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Pennsylvania. University Microfilms 8207943. Davis, Irvine 1966 "Comparative Jé phonology." Estudos Lingüísticos 1.2:10—24. Derbyshire, Desmond C., and Geoffrey K. Pullum, eds. 1986 Handbook of Amazonian Languages, vol. I. Berlin: Moutonde Gruyter. 1990 Handbook of Amazonian Languages, vol. 2. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Duponceau, Peter S. 1819 "A correspondence between The Rev. John Heckewelder, of Bethlehem, and Peter S. Duponceau, Esq." Transactions of the Historical and Literary Committee of the American Philosophical Society 1:351-448. 1830 "The translator's preface." Transactions of the American Philosophical Society 3, n.s.: 65-96. Edgerton, Franklin 1943 "Notes on early American work in linguistics." Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society 87.1 (July): 25-34. Freeman, John Finley 1962 "Manuscript sources on Latin American Indians in the library of the American Philosophical Society." Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society 106:530—40. 1966 A Guide to Manuscripts Relating to the American Indian in the Library of the American Philosophical Society. Philadelphia: American Philosophical Society. Gesner, Conrad 1555 Mithridates. Zurich. Gilij, Felipe Salvador 1782 Ensayo de historia Americana. Trans. Antonio Tovar. Biblioteca de la [1965] Academia Nacional de la Historia, Caracas: Fuentes para la Historia Colonial de Venezuela. Vol. III. Gray, Louis H. 1939 "Antillean and South American." Foundations of Language, pp. 412— 17. New York: Macmillan. Greenberg, Joseph H. 1956 "The general classification of Central and South American lan[1960] guages." In Men and Cultures, 5th International Congress of Anthropological and Ethnological Sciences, ed. Anthony F. C. Wallace, pp. 791-94. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. 1987 Language in the Americas. Stanford, Calif.: Stanford University Press. Greene, John C. i960 "Early scientific interest in the American Indian: comparative linguistics." Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society 104.5 (October): 511-17-

i6

Introduction

Haven, Samuel F. 1856 "Archaeology of the United States; or sketches, historical and bibliographical, of the progress of information and opinion respecting vestiges of antiquity in the United States." Smithsonian Contributions to Knowledge 8. Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Institution. Hervás y Panduro, Don Lorenzo 1784—87 Idea dell'universo. Vol. I: Catalogo delle lingue conosciute e notizia della loro affinità, e diversità. Cesena: 1784. 1800 Catálogo de las lenguas de las naciones conocidas. Vol. I: Lenguas y naciones Americanas. Madrid: Administración del Real Arbitrio de Beneficencia. Hoenigswald, Henry M. 1973 "Linguistics." In Dictionary of the History of Ideas, ed. Philip P. Wiener, Vol. I l l , pp. 61-73. New York: Charles Scribner's Sons. Hoenigswald, Henry M. and Linda F. Wiener, eds. 1987 Biological Metaphor and Cladistic Classification. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. Hoijer, Harry, Eric P. Hamp, and William Bright 1965 "Contributions to a bibliography of comparative Amerindian." International Journal of American Linguistics 31.4 (October): 346-53. Key, Mary Ritchie 1968 Comparative Tacanan Phonology: With Cavineña Phonology and Notes on Pano-Tacanan Relationship. The Hague: Mouton. 1976 "Lingüística comparativa y sincrónica: Método y perspectivas." Estudios Filológicos 11, pp. 103-12. Universidad Austral de Chile, Valdivia, Chile. 1977 "The Linguistic Discoveries of Catherine the Great." In The Third LACUS Forum 1976, ed. R. J. DiPietro and E. L. Blansitt, pp. 39-45. Columbia, S.C.: Hornbeam Press. 1979 The Grouping of South American Indian Languages. Ars Linguistica, 2. Tübingen: Gunter Narr. 1980 Catherine the Great's Linguistic Contribution. Edmonton, Alberta, Canada: Linguistic Research, Inc. 1983 "Comparative methodology for distant relationships in North and South American languages." Language Sciences 5.2 (October) : 133—54. , ed. 1987 Comparative Linguistics of South American Indian Languages. Special Issue, Language Sciences 9.1 (April): 1 - 1 1 7 . Klein, Harriet E. Manelis and Louisa R. Stark, eds. 1985 South American Indian Languages: Retrospect and Prospect. Austin: University of Texas Press. Loriot, James 1964 "A selected bibliographv of comparative American Indian linguistics." International journal of American Linguistics 30.1 (January): 62-80.

Comparative Studies in South American Indian Languages

17

Loukotka, Cestmir 1935 Clasificación de las lenguas sudamericanas. Prague: Josef Bartl. 1968 Classification of South American Indian Languages, ed. Johannes Wilbert. Latín American Center Reference Series, vol. 7. Los Angeles: University of California. Mclntyre, Loren 1985 "Pioneer of modern geography: Humboldt's way." National Geographic 168.3 (September): 318-51. McQuown, Norman A. 1955 "The indigenous languages of Latin America." American Anthropologist 57.3 (June): 501-70. Matteson, Esther 196+ "Algunas afiliaciones de la familia Arawak." In XXXV Congreso Internacional de Americanistas, 1962, vol. 2, pp. 519-23. México, D.F. Maupertuis, Pierre Louis Maureau 1750 Reflections on the Origin of Language. Metcalf, George J. 1972 "Philipp Cliiver and his 'Lingva Céltica'." Deutsche Beiträge zur geistigen Uberlieferung 7:90-109. Moore, Bruce R. 1962 "Correspondences in South Barbacoan Chibcha." In Studies in Ecuadorian Indian Languages 1, ed. Benjamin F. Elson, pp. 270-89. Norman, Okla.: Summer Institute of Linguistics. Noble, G. Kingsley 1965 "Proto-Arawakan and its descendants." InternationalJournal of American Linguistics 31.3 (July), Part II: 1-129 (also pub. 38, Indiana University Research Center in Anthropology, Folklore, and Linguistics, Bloomington). Olson, Ronald D. 1964 "Mayan affinities with Chipava of Bolivia 1: Correspondences." International Journal of American Linguistics 30.4 (October): 313-24. 1965 "Mayan affinities with Chipaya of Bolivia 2: Cognates." International Journal of American Linguistics 31.1 (January): 29-38. Orr, Carolyn and Robert E. Longacre 1968 "Proto-Quechumaran." Language 44.3 (September): 528-55. Pallas, Peter Simon 1786—89 Linguarum totius orbis vocabularia comparativa: Augustissimae cura collecta. St. Petersburg. Pedersen, Holger 1924 The Discovery ofLanguage. Bloomington: Indiana Universitv Press. [i93i] Percival, W. Keith 1987 "Biological analogy in the study of language before the advent of comparative grammar." In Biological Metaphor and Cladistic Classification, ed. Henry M. Hoenigswald and Linda F. Wiener, pp. 3—38. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.

i8

Introduction

Pickering, John 1821 "On the adoption of a uniform orthography for the Indian languages of North America." American Academy ofArts and Sciences, Memoirs 4.2:319-60. 1831 "Indian languages of America." Encyclopedia Americana 6, pp. 581— 600. Reland [Reeland], Adrian 1708 "Vocabularia variarum linguarum Americanarum." In Dissertationum miscellanearum. Utrecht. Rivet, Paul and Cestmir Loukotka 1952 "Langues de PAmérique du Sud et des Antilles." In Les langues du nwnde, ed. A. Meillet and Marcel Cohen, pp. 1099-1160. Vol. 2. Paris: Champion. Rodrigues, Aryon Dall'Igna 1958 "Classification of Tupi-Guarani." International Journal of American Linguistics 24.3 (July): 231-34. Rodríguez de Montes, María Luisa 1988 "Estado actual de la clasificación de las lenguas indígenas Colombianas: seminario-taller reunido en Yerbabuena." Thesaurus 43, Boletin del Instituto Caro y Cuervo. Bogotá, pp. 173-81. In press (ed.), Memorias del seminario: estado actual de la clasificación de las lenguas indígenas Colombianas. Bogotá: Instituto Caro y Cuervo. Ruhlen, Merritt 1987 A Guide to the World's Languages. Vol 1: Classification. Stanford, Calif.: Stanford University Press. Scaliger, Joseph Justus 1599 Diatriba de Europaeorum Unguis. Schmidt, Wilhelm 1926 Die Sprachfamilien und Sprachenkreise derErde. Heidelberg: Winter. Shafer, Robert 1959 "Algumas e q u a t e s fonéticas em Arawakan." Anthropos 5 4 . 3 - 4 : 542—62. 1962 "Aruakan (not Arawakan)." Anthropological Linguistics 4 . 4 : 3 1 - 4 0 . Shell, Olive Alexandra 1965 Pano reconstruction. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Pennsylvania. University Microfilms 66-4648. Suárez, Jorge A. 1982 "South American Indian languages." EncyclopaediaBritannica, Vol. 17, 15th edition, pp. 105-12. Swadesh, Morris 1955 "Towards greater accuracy in lexicostatistic dating." International Journal ofAmerican Linguistics 21.2 (April): 121-37. 1959 Mapas de clasificación lingüística de México y las Americas. Cuadernos del Instituto de Historia, Serie Antropológica 8. México: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México.

Comparative Studies in South American Indian Languages

19

Tovar, Antonio 1961 Catálogo de las lenguas de América del Sur. Buenos Aires: Editorial Sudamericana. Tovar, Antonio and Consuelo Larrucea de Tovar 1984 Catálogo de las lenguas de América del Sur. Madrid: Editorial Gredos. Trillos Amaya, Maria 1988 "Estado actual de la clasificación de las lenguas indígenas de Colombia." Ciotta 3 . 1 : 4 1 - 4 4 . Wilbert, Johannes 1962 "A preliminary glottochronology of Ge." Anthropological Linguistics 4.2:17-25.

II Classification and Typological Problems

Aurore Monod-Becquelin How to Deal with Unclassified Languages: An Ethnolinguistic View of Comparative Linguistics

ABSTRACT Cet article est une réflexion sur la classification possible des langues isolées, dans les cas où font défaut documents et descriptions antérieures et/ou lorsque les langues connexes ont disparu. La question est la suivante: que peut-on substituer au classement génétique? La méthode comparative relue à la lumière des développements récents de l'anthropologie linguistique ouvre des voies de recherche principalement exprimées dans l'oeuvre de Benveniste. La grammaire comparée telle qu'il la pratique montre en effet que le sens et la forme de la grammaire d'une langue viennent du type de réponse qu'elle donne à un certain nombre de questions de nature intralinguistique. E n envisageant la langue comme une des réponses possibles à une structure conceptuelle déterminée de même qu'une expression culturelle n'est qu'une des formes possibles de la structure, on prend en compte le seul niveau auquel se conjoignent langue et culture, sans avoir recours aux catégories dites "universelles." 1

One of the first things mentioned in the description of a language is its assignment to a family along with its relationship with the neighboring languages within the family. Most o f the time nothing is made of the information afterward, except when the research deals with the subject o f language comparison or glottochronological reconstruction. And yet all scholars are aware that it is quite a different thing to study a language related to a known family or a language with no established relationship. Studying unrelated languages may be looked at in two ways. It means, first of all, working on so-called "orphan" languages, lacking a definition that would establish them in the large classification o f the world languages and in the history of human culture save by criteria considered minor (e.g., geographical criteria, which are used in the last resort).

24

Classification and Typological Problems

But it also means trying to discover the unique manifestation of an original linguistic structure, a masterpiece of the human mind to be saved from oblivion. Unrelated Amerindian languages, for example, are often dying out—so studying them means preserving the dialect if not the idiolect, the language, and the family altogether.2 As a matter of fact, for any researcher who has also worked on languages for which the family concept has been essential, one problem is quite clear. How should one approach the study of residual, dying-out, and unrelated languages? What should be included in the description so that this sample of natural language may eventually find its place in a different classification from the one used so far? Finally, is it possible to make up, at least in part, for the lack of the precious contribution brought by the comparison of a language with others of the same family or, if not, should unrelated languages be relegated with the curiosa? My purpose is to take a new look at comparative studies in the light of more recent theories, such as anthropological linguistics or cognitive anthropology, and to present an approach to language isolates that makes up for the lack of related languages or of historical perspectives. My approach is not based on a new theory, but appeals to the methodological combination of two different research traditions. We will consider the matter in two steps. First, we will briefly survey the method used in dealing with "lucky" languages, those that lend themselves to historical, archaeological, and even proto-historical investigations, those that have "true friends" among linguists: the philologists. Then we will review some of the new directions of anthropological linguistics.3 The merging of the two points of view may suggest a few methodological principles so that unrelated languages may cease to be an unlabeled brica-brac just because they cannot be admitted into the museum of History. In so doing, we will have brought to light the common concerns shared by these two theories, however remote from each other: a concern for cultures as well as languages, the emphasis on grammar, and the importance of meaning. Thanks to the clarification imposed by these guidelines, we will better appreciate the most convenient approach to use when dealing with an unclassified language. *

*

*

Comparative-historical linguistics is supported by a gradually developed overall methodological system of such fecundity that, since the

An Ethnolinguistic View of Comparative Linguistics

25

origin of Indo-European comparative studies at the end of the eighteenth century, it has continued to generate or foster a variety of objectives and, accordingly, various disciplines and schools depending on time, place, and people. 4 Antoine Meillet, in 1924, described his own comparative study as a search for "historical clues" as opposed to one leading to the discovery of universal laws (Meillet 1924 [1984]: 1). Hence the diversity of themes in comparative linguistics: genetic classification, language evolution and change, dialectology, and reconstruction of common and proto languages, in addition to further in-depth study of such concepts as state of language, general characteristics of languages, specific features of individual languages, and classes of similar changes. There are actually few themes beyond the scope of historical linguistics, broadly speaking, whether comparative, stricto sensu historical or positivistic, as with the neogrammarians. The concept of structure, for instance, or in any case the closely related one of system, arises: Meillet, a comparatist, but also a disciple of Saussure, emphasized that systems are to be confronted, leading through reconstruction to other systems— which he called structures—and obviously not to languages (Ducrot and Todorov 1972:23—24). The philologists took fascinating trips upstream in time in a meticulous search for the accuracy of form and meaning—from dieu to *deiwos— in order to reconstruct images blurred by the centuries yet probable and perhaps even faithful, since they were based on relations forming systems. "Once apprehended in their past and present, languages appear as systems covering both past and present, phonetics, grammar and signification, for they are systems of signs" (Kristeva 1981:211). The importance of grammatical correspondences is also explicit and, although the place and the very notion of grammar may vary even to the point of being contradictory, grammatical elements, even when typology is not the main focus, remain a privileged material (Ducrot and Todorov 1972:24). All research work, including that of phonetics, is concerned with meaning, if only in selecting or confirming the choice of the units to be compared. Besides, language is perceived as linked to society, and the history of languages is one aspect of the history of civilizations or cultures. Thus for Indo-European, the triumph of historical linguistics, an amazing fresco is drawn over nearly 4,000 years (since the first Hittite inscription, the first recorded token of Indo-European) by Germans, Danes, Italians,

26

Classification and Typological Problems

and many others, impregnated with the ideas of encyclopedists and romanticists, evolutionists and positivists, organicists and typologists. What is, however, Proto Indo-European? It is a family of languages, and a civilization. "A common language implies a common civilization," Meillet asserted bluntly (Meillet 1924 [1984]: 17). It split up step by step into a multiplicity of common languages, generating Illyrian, Germanic, Celtic, Hittite, Indo-Iranian, Greek, Romance, Baltic, and Slavonic; these in turn continued to break up until modern times, when each country formed its own common language at the expense of local languages;5 yet, beyond the amazing diversity of their cultural, historical, and sociological denotata, they maintained a share of their common significatum. One of the tasks of comparative linguists is to relate, despite this split, a major common language with a type of civilization. It was the concern of Emile Benveniste, one of the greatest Indo-Europeanists, whose approach to the language/civilization question had much in common with that of the most modern anthropological linguistics. Orientations, or even directions, for the description of isolated Amerindian languages may thus be found among the subsidiary teachings of comparative linguists on the nature of the relationship between language and society. For this purpose, however, we will have to follow the path of anthropological linguistics and the lessons taught by this quite different theoretical system, which lays a stronger emphasis on the quest for signification. *

*

*

In a lovely, unpretentious text, La mentalité primitive et l'illusion des explorateurs, Paulhan in 1926 does away, in just a few lines, with the "voyageur" ethnocentric prejudices. On the one hand he describes a pompous Westerner: "An English explorer, [the] Rev. A. L. Kitching, returning from a voyage in Africa, wrote about the Luganda tribes: 'Those savages have no notion of love.' The proof, he adds, is that 'many dialects do not have a specific word for love; in the most advanced dialect, loving God and preferring potatoes to meat are expressed by one and the same word, ockwagala.'' Obviously, Kitching is ignorant of the French language and the fact that we too have only one word for loving beef and loving a woman" (Paulhan 1926 [1966]: 143). On the other hand, he quotes one of his Malagasy informants: "The French have a singularly concrete turn of mind. When we use a single word: akoho, they must distinguish between chick, chicken, hen or rooster" (151); or again: "Malagasy expresses things

An Ethnolinguistic View of Comparative Linguistics

27

simply, whereas French is full of detours and details, and its first objective is to depict them" (148). Obviously, the author had meant to counter Lévy-Bruhl's idea that the primitive mind was incapable of abstraction. But his main purpose was to warn against holding as particularly representative of a language those features that are the most striking to the researcher. To avoid this, he recommends not to lose sight of one's own language, but to keep it at a distance and beware of illusion: "When the explorer takes for ideas the primitive's words, he is holding his own words as ideas and things" (153). Structural linguistics is supposed to provide guidelines for resisting illusions of this type. Its brightest fruits—such as phonology, the analysis of genetic relations, or the study of taxonomy, to cite only the most famous—have fed the scientistic mirage of linguists and then of anthropologists. Structures are now apprehended beyond appearances and it has become possible to account for the pressures and consequent changes that affect them. The preeminence of linguistics in the social sciences and the use made by anthropology of some of its methods contribute to reconciling two disciplines that had been separated according to the Cartesian principle. When America was first discovered, language and customs were perceived as recto and verso of one sheet both in the ideology applied to the Savages (Monod-Becquelin 1984:335) and in fieldwork practice (Monod-Becquelin 1984:339—40). Then, at the end of the eighteenth century, the scientific approach came to be applied to the study of peoples, leading to "diviser chacune des difficultés . . . en autant de parcelles qu'il se pourrait et qu'il serait requis pour les mieux résoudre" (Descartes 1637 [1949]). By a pendulum effect well known to epistemologists, Mauss's "phénomène social total," and the ethnological approach of fieldwork, along with the success of structural linguistics at the beginning of the twentieth century arc leading back to an apprehension of things that was anticipated several centuries ago. Ethnographers are less and less working through interpreters, new formulations arc proposed for the language/culture problem, structural linguistics is increasingly thought to provide cognitive organizations more abstract, and therefore more "comprehensive" and meaningful, and all this contributes to the reshuffling of the deck.6 During the second half of the twentieth century, titles are more and more explicit: Façons de dire, façons de faire, written by Y. Verdier, deals with oral tradition as well as anthropology; The Social Use ofMetaphor, by David Sapir and Christopher Crocker, elucidates the common concern of

28

Classification and Typological Problems

a linguist and an anthropologist: "The anthropology of Rhetoric"; to say nothing of John Austin's How To Do Things with Words, which lays the foundations of Pragmatics and the Oxford Analytical School. 7 More recently, L'homme de parole, by Claude Hagège, substitutes for the speaker (abstract and deaf locutor facing an equally abstract and mute hearer) the "homme dialogai" (dialogical man). The same one is found, almost under the same name, by an American school, of which Dennis Tedlock is a good example. One of the chapters in his fine work The Spoken Word and the Work of Interpretation (1983) is entitled "Dialogical Anthropology." The statute of elicitation, the value of the quotation, and the concept of Speech are no longer taken for granted, but arc open to inquiry. The problem of meaning, which is now a common concern of anthropologists and linguists, can thus be considered from three different points of view: 1. The universalist point of view is illustrated by Lévi-Strauss, who, by a prodigious effort of abstraction, writes a topology of myths and narratives—and, whenever possible, a history as well—from the Bororo to the Comtesse de Ségur, which shows, over ranges of successive variations, the unicity of human preoccupations throughout time and space. It is in a way a work of philosophy. By an amusing effect of scholarly trends, the linguistic discipline that derived, as he did, from Jakobson, binarism, and formalism, and whose scope is, as Lévi-Strauss's, universalistic (i.e., generative grammar), is the least apt for dealing with anthropological problems, and is turning resolutely toward cognitive psychology. 2. To the particularist point of view may be ascribed most of the work of the past thirty years, which deals with the review and evaluation of indigenous taxonomies in order to establish as accurately as possible the categories and ways of thought that prevail in any given group. Componential analysis is one of its branches and has given some results (see Hymcs, Tyler); it can be formalized (e.g., Lounsburv's studies on family relationships) and also claims to be of use in a comparative perspective. 3. The intermediate position deals with the system underlying action and speech patterns and considers the structure of a culture at a given time as the possible form of a more general, more abstract structure, of a cognitive type, from which the structure of the language also proceeds. Since, for many reasons—differences in the evolution rate, in malleability and liability to interference and borrowing—language and culture changes do not always correspond, great disparities can be expected in the effects. 8 The process might therefore be figured as two large rhetorical

An Ethnolinguistic View of Comparative Linguistics

29

developments from a common cognitive structure, in which each language state and cultural state of the society under study would be seen as one among many possibilities that could emerge. In a restrictive domain (poetry) but, suggesting a more general application, Ruwet (197$) has shown in the study of poetical figures that every trope is the final stage in a succession of forms: the poet may decide to terminate the process where he chooses, to create a metaphor or be content with the preceding metonymy. What is, in poetry, an individual choice, pertains to the collective and to the unconscious in society; historical events, contacts, and sudden shifts obviously alter both culture and language, each event having a different impact on words and actions. *

*

*

Let us now try to determine in what field items and relationships shared by culture and language may be apprehended. These relationships do not pertain to universal grammar—where anthropological linguistics had nothing to say, being "mostly confined to the superficial aspects of language structure" (Chomsky 1970:114—15); as a matter of fact, universals as a whole "govern language properties that are simply beyond the reach of research in the limited scope that anthropological linguistics, mostly for very good reasons, calls its own." On the other hand, such relationships are found by means of analyses, particularly grammatical, the results of which are applicable, at a very general level, to a whole "group" of languages. As an example, the nature and function of the ergative category existing in Kuikuru, a Carib language, and in several closely related languages, induce us to enquire by what means (whether morphonological, grammatical, or semantic) this category is replaced in the few Carib languages where it is not present. However, the grouping here may be somewhat different from the family proceeding from comparative studies, nor docs it coincide with a typological class or an areal relationship. These points will be briefly reviewed later. The originality of this semantic grouping as opposed to the traditional modes of classifying will then appear clearly. "Since comparative study is the only way to induce the history of languages, so long as a language is unrelated, it remains without history" (Meillet 1924 [1984]: 12). To this statement, Benveniste poses two kinds of arguments. 1. First of all, as illustrated in the above quotation, genetic classification

Classification and Typological Problems has limitations proportionate to its very strength: a language cannot be classified if the family has disappeared (as in the case of Sumerian), or if, in the absence of archaic remains, it is no longer possible to establish relationships with a previous language state, or if the languages under study are better defined, in terms of their mutual relationship, by degrees, than by relating them to a known older structure—"parenté par enchaînement" (Benveniste 1966:105-6). Moreover, and this point will be further developed hereafter, there are other ways, perhaps just as satisfactory, of classifying languages, which have the advantage of allowing to deal with languages without history. 2. The typological classification, which does not coincide with the genetic classification—Benveniste shows that Takelma in Oregon should be counted as an Indo-European language (Benveniste 1966:115)—is tricky, since it rests partly on the linguist's insight and choices. Greenberg, even though he used it himself, underlines the drawbacks of the required cutting up into phonetic, phonological, grammatical, and semantic criteria as well as the arbitrary choice of any number of isoglosses (Greenberg 1968:127). Structural resemblances may arise from what is called "groupement d'affinités" (affinity connections) whereby several languages of different origins, which are only related geographically, undergo identical developments. The converging effect, added to borrowing and chance, may disguise the diversity of origin of languages forming, by a process as yet unelucidated, an "areal group" (Benveniste 1966:101). The above classifications may support each other to some extent, but they all depend on the fact that "the analysis bears upon empirical forms and arrangements," that is, superficial features (Benveniste 1966:114). T o overcome this basic difficulty, it must be recognized, first of all, that form is only a possibility of structure. . . . (115). T h e preliminary condition for a new classification of languages would be to give up the principle . . . that linguistics is concerned only with data, that language is entirely contained in its actual manifestations . . . linguistic facts are a result, and the question is: of what? . . . E v e n ' language has to solve a number of problems which converge on the central question of "signification." Grammatical forms convey the answers to these problems, with a symbolism typical of language; by studying these forms, their selection, their specific connections and organizations, we can induce the nature and the form of the intralinguistic problems they are answering. T h e whole process is essential, though unconscious and difficult to grasp. (1966:115)

This superb text gives the keys to a classification in which unrelated languages are placed on the same level as related ones, or close to it. One

An Ethnolinguistic View of Comparative Linguistics

31

might compare, for instance, different systems of noun classes, or the various expressions of grammatical number that are functionally analogous. Such an in-depth effort on a general theory of linguistic structures, concerned more with meaning than forms—the integration of linguistic descriptions into models rather than a single universal scheme—seems to open to the researcher a bountiful source of directions and learning. It expresses better than many texts more formalized and up-to-date the concern of new anthropological linguistics. I would like to conclude with an example showing the intricate interplay of linguistic and cultural categories: a seven-page article by Benveniste, on the item rex 'king' in the second volume of his work, Le vocabulaire des institutions indo-européennes. It appears to be a purely comparative study, but is really a model of anthropological linguistics. If we chose this example from within the Indo-European domain, it is precisely because it best illustrates the spirit of the author's method and the respective roles played by linguistic and cultural meanings. It should be obvious that the description of a language, if one conceives of it not as a given object, self-contained and similar to a monologue, but as one of the possible answers to a series of conceptual problems, will benefit a great deal from this model. Benveniste studies the concept of roi 'king' at the opposite ends of the Indo-European world: Italoceltic and Indo-Iranian, conservative areas "where institutions and words elsewhere abolished persisted for a long time" (Benveniste 1968:10). His analysis bears upon the Latin rëx and the Sanskrit raj-. The nominal theme shared by the two roots is *reg-. What is the semantic base of the concept? A vestige of another Indo-European language, namely the Greek verb ope'-yw 'spread,' is the beginning of the thread that leads him to the meaning of *reg. Most of the article is meant to demonstrate phonetically the similarity between this term and the *reg domain. We are not directly concerned with this part. The second part establishes the exact meaning of the verb, which is to "draw out a straight line starting from one's position" or "to move forward in a straight line." This leading thread brings Benveniste back easily to Latin to explain rejjto (first "the point reached," then "the area delineated by straight lines"), rectus ("what is physically and morally right"), and régula ("ruler," instrument used to draw a line, and moral rule).9 In the Roman expression "regere fines," the verb regere means "draw the frontiers in straight lines," which is "what the High Priest does for the construction of a temple or of a town when he draws out on the site the consecrated space." The agentive referring to this action is rex, "the one who draws the line, or the guideline." The notion of Indo-European royalty is thus

32

Classification and Typological Problems

brought to light: the Indo-European rex is more a religious than a political figure. He holds no power, but he sets the rules and determines what is right. This notion bears relation to the priest colleges, whose duty was the perpetuation of rites. The accurate contents of the word rex, the king, in terms of what he does and may do in later times and in the various societies is left by Benveniste to the investigation of historians, anthropologists, or sociologists. The demonstration does not rest on any extralinguistic assumption; and what is successively referred to by the word, its denotata, should not be taken into account. And yet, using a comparison between two languages, Latin and Greek, 10 and a commentary on Homer, he describes augury practices, explains a moral constellation, characterizes the founder of towns or temples, talks about magics and customs, and undertakes the study of a social organization. By defining the notions of king and kingdom, and leaving to history the numbering and explicitation of successive denotata, he reaches the deepest level of meaning and understanding. Such an endeavor on grammar and culture and their major common concerns (space, time, number, deixis, aspect, persons, and so forth), even on a different ground and with a different method from the one exemplified above, could only be most enlightening and therefore justify Benveniste's hopes that "any people who follow to the very end the exposition of our work may find in it materials for general reflections, particularly regarding the application of some of the models described herein to the study of those languages or cultures which, having no written documents, lack the historical perspective" (Benveniste 1968:12). Once again associated, the language and culture of a society are seen as realities, not homologous but not heterogenous either. The tenuous intuition that "there is something common" between the two can be developed and tested. Though the past uses of phrases such as "spirit of a people" or "national character" are to be rejected, it is still legitimate to explore the dark and unconscious origin of a human group's language and culture. The apparent paradox of discrepancies is accounted for: "Considered as empirical and historical data, language and culture in their relationships show no sign of having analogical structures" (Benveniste 1974:91); or again: "We have seen that, between an historical language and an historical society, no necessary correlation may be established. However, at the fundamental level,11 homologies can be perceived immediately" (1968:94). Language here is conceived as a system of significant forms interpret-

An Ethnolinguistic View of Comparative Linguistics

33

ing society, capable of reflecting social changes while keeping its structure unscathed. The most difficult task will always be to sort out the manifestation of meaning from the essence of meaning. In any case, avoiding the confusion between the empirical and the conceptual meanings is the true path in the maze of signification, leading to the glorious dawn of a properly semantic typology of languages. "The correlation between the two domains will neither be structural. . . nor typological. . . h i s t o r i c a l . . . or genetic," the common features of social practice and speech will have to be searched for "in this relationship of inter-human communication" (1966:114). NOTES

1. This paper was translated by C. Siegel and revised by P. Menget. 2. My first linguistic fieldwork was on Trumai, a language isolate of central Brazil, spoken by a tribe of fewer than thirty people. The Trumai belong to what the anthropologists call the "Xingu pluriethnic society." This society involves about ten village communities defined by their languages. Four different families are represented: Tupi, Carib, Arawak, Ge, plus the unrelated Trumai. My second fieldwork dealt with a few languages of the Mayan family, Tzeltal in particular. I studied the rhetorical figures found in the oral tradition of rituals (and possibly in the Maya scriptural tradition). Not only is this study comparative, but it rests on an idea of a pan-Maya system which is fairly credible and on which many studies by American scholars have been based. 3. "Anthropological linguistics" does not translate exactly the French word ethnolinguistique, but it comes close. 4. For further details on several early comparatists, see Droixhe (1984), which is devoted to studies carried out in Europe between the sixteenth and the eighteenth centuries. 5. Meillet is aware of the paradox of a diversity of languages expressing one material and intellectual civilization. He calls this an "anomaly" which is bound to disappear. 6. It was mostly the missionaries and the colonizers of North America who, when faced with unexpected linguistic realities difficult to handle in their Latin models, wrote about the discovery of a society together with its language, and on the necessity of living in "huts" rather than in the missions. 7. Pragmatics is understood in various ways in Europe and in the United States. Two major trends might be to consider the action " o f " speech: tradition of the English analytical school; and to consider the action "with" speech: tradition related to sociolinguistics or the ethnography of speech, and the American pragmatic school. In all cases, "the meaning of the language is onlv an indication of the meaning of speech" (Ducrot 1980:32) and this concept is closely related to enunciation theories. 8. The minimal interpretation of the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis consists in comparing specialized vocabularies on the language side and a techno-economical situation on the culture side to deduce that language and culture categories corre-

34

Classification and Typological Problems

spond with each other. It is actually a very special case of coincidence—explained by the necessity for mankind to deal with the environment—and not a general case: most of the facts contradict the direct and mechanical inferences from one field to the other, as do the situations where different languages exist in a homogenous cultural zone (or vice versa) (see Benveniste 1974:91-92). If the Sapir-Whorf intuition is right, it may be that the state of the linguistic theory at the time did not enable them to demonstrate it properly. 9. The passage to the moral sense is attested to by a Persian precept. 10. This is supported by Gothic, Old Persian, Finno-Ugrian, Basque, Armenian, Hittite, Chinese, Gallic, Gaelic, French, Germanic, Japanese, and Polynesian. 11. "As systematic comparisons of language and society were made, discrepancies appeared. It was discovered that the correspondence between the two was constantly disturbed by the major fact of diffusion, both in the language and in the social structure, so that societies with the same culture would have heterogenous languages, or conversely, closely-related languages would express entirely different cultures. Further study showed specific problems inherent in the analysis of either language or culture, as well as in the signification problems which they share. . . . The problem will be to find out the common basis to language and society, the principles that govern both structures, and first of all to determine in each of them the units to be compared in order to establish their interdependence" (Benveniste 1966:15). REFERENCES

Austin, John L. 1965 How To Do Things with Words. Oxford: Clarendon Press. Benveniste, Emile 1966 Problèmes de linguistique générale, Vol. 1. Paris: Gallimard, coll. Bibliothèque des Sciences Humaines. 1968 Le vocabulaire des institutions indo-européennes. 2 vols. Paris: Editions de Minuit. 1974 Problèmes de linguistique générale, Vol. 2. Paris: Gallimard, coll. Bibliothèque des Sciences Humaines. Chomsky, Noam 1970 Le langage et la pensée. Paris: Payot. Cohen, Marcel and Antoine Meillet, compilers. 1952 Les langues du monde. Paris: Champion. Descartes, René 1637 "Le discours de la méthode." In Descartes, œuvres et lettres. Paris: Gal[1949] limard, coll. La Pléiade. Droixhe, Daniel, Pierre Swiggers et al. 1984 "Genèse du comparatisme indo-européen." Histoire, Epistémologie, Langage. Vol. 6, fasc. 2. Lille: Presses Universitaires de Lille. Ducrot, Oswald 1980 Les mots du discours. Paris: Editions de Minuit. Ducrot, Oswald and Tzvetan Todorov 1972 Dictionnaire encyclopédique des sciences du langage. Paris: Le Seuil.

An Ethnolinguistic View of Comparative Linguistics

35

Greenberg, Joseph H. 1968 Anthropological Linguistics: An Introduction. New York: Random House. Hagège, Claude 1985 L'homme de parole. Paris: Fayard. Hymes, Dell, ed. 1964 Language in Culture and Society: A Reader in Linguistics and Anthropology. New York: Harper and Row. Kristeva, Julia 1981 Le langage, cet inconnu: une initiation à la linguistique. Paris: Le Seuil. Lounsbury, Floyd G. 1965 "Another view of the Trobriand kinship categories." American Anthropologist 67.5:142—85. Special publication: Formal Semantic Analysis. Menasha: American Anthropological Association. Meillet, Antoine 1924 La méthode comparative en linguistique historique. Oslo: Aschehoug. [1984] Translated by G. B. Ford as The Comparative Method in Historical Linguistics. Paris: Champion. Monod-Becquelin, Aurore 1984 "La parole et la tradition amérindiennes dans les récits des chroniqueurs aux XVIe et XVIIe siècles." Amerindia 6: Pour une histoire de la linguistique amérindienne en France. Paris: Association d'Etudes Amérindiennes. Paulhan, Jean 1926 La mentalité primitive et l'illusion des explorateurs. Paris: Nouvelle Reti 966] vue Française. Ruwet, Nicolas 1975 "Synecdoques et métonymies." Poétique 23. Paris: Le Seuil. Sapir, J. David and J. Christopher Crocker, eds. 1977 The Social Use ofMetaphor: Essays on the Anthropology of Rhetoric. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. Tedlock, Dennis 1983 The Spoken Word and the Work ofInterpretation. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. Tyler, Stephen 1979 Cognitive Anthropology. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Verdier, Yvonne 1979 Façons de dire, façons de faire. Paris: Gallimard.

Marilia Facó Soares and Tonne Leite Vowel Shift in the Tupi-Guarani Language Family: A Typological Approach

ABSTRACT T h i s chapter deals with the vowel shifts that occur in the Tapirape, Asurini, Guajajara, and Arawete languages o f the Tupi-Guarani family (Brazil). It reviews previous approaches to internal classification such as the tree diagram and degree o f participation in an extrinsically ordered set o f rules. T h e pushchain mechanism is examined and criticized, and a simultaneous changes approach is proposed, with reassignment o f allophonv to resolve categorial conflicts.

Introduction It is common among Brazilian scholars to complain that classifications of Brazilian Indian languages arc based on insufficient material, and that they do not satisfy the requirements of the classical comparative method. In fact, the classifications available for some families sometimes show great divergences among themselves. In general, the classificatory work is the result of a superficial inspection of the lexicon; because of the quality of the descriptive data, the establishment of constant sound-meaning correspondence is not attempted. In addition, there is no continuity of effort, each work being the result of individual interest—sometimes even a sporadic incursion by the investigator into the field of historical linguistics. So there is no natural progression of knowledge built on the revision of previous work in order to enlarge it, confirm it, or invalidate it. Within this general pattern, the study of the Tupi-Guarani family is an exception. It benefits from good descriptive analysis and from the continuous comparative and lexicostatistic studies of Rodrigues (1955, 1964, 1985); the reconstructions of protoforms by Lemle (1971) and Jensen

Vowel Shift in Tupi-Guarani

37

(1984); and the revisions of rules by Leite (1982), Soarcs (1978, 1979), and Viveiros de Castro (1984). Each author has answered a question raised in a previous work. In this paper we will reexamine the vowel shift that occurred in some of the Tupi-Guarani languages, the change subsumed in Lemle's tree diagram as "change of *a"; it is reviewed by Leite (1982), Soares (1978,1979), and Viveiros de Castro (1984).

Lemle's Tree Diagram and Shared Innovations Based on phonemic data from Asurini (As), Guajajara (Gj), Kokama (Ko), Parintintin (Pt), Kamayura (Km), Urubu (Ub), Guarani (Gn), Guarayo (Gy), Siriono (Si), and Tupinamba (Tb), after reconstructing 221 lexical items through the systematic establishment of cognates, Lemle (1971:128) draws the tree diagram in Figure 1. However, as Lemle (1971:129) writes: Tapirape (data from Yonne Leite) has much in common with Asurini, including the loss of contrast between *u and *o, the merging of certain other vowels with *a, and the phonetic manifestation of /yl as [y] in syllable-final position and as [c] in other environments. However, its inclusion in the same section of the diagram as Asurini is difficult, since vowel nasalization remains as a phonemic feature of Tapirape but not of Asurini and Guajajara.

Proto Tupi-Guarani

*PY > c *c > 0

1 *y > y i

*C # >

change of *a

1 As

Figure 1

1 Gj

0

*ti > ci, si

1 Pt

Km

1 Ub

1 Gn

Gy

1 Si

1 Ko

Tb

38

Classification and Typological Problems

It is this difficulty in the tree diagram classification that Leite (1982) tries to solve with an approach using extrinsically ordered rules. The following rules arc formulated by Leite: Rule i V + + —

bk lo rd nas

.-lo]

/ -

C + nas

in As, Tap, Gj

*a goes to a in Asurini, Tapirape, and Guajajara

Rule 2 V

+ +

bk lo rd nas

-

+

[ - lo]

/ i n As, Tap, Gj

*a goes to a in Asurini, Tapirape, and Guajajara

Rule 3 V + L —

bk hi lo nas

" + hi]

/ in Gj except in env.

(Co) #

* o goes to u in Guajajara except in syllable final or in penultimate syllable preceding 0.

Rule 4 V

r

+ bk -1

- hi — lo - rd L_ — nas _

—» [ + hi] in As, Tap

3 goes to i in Asurini and Tapirape

Vowel Shift in Tupi-Guarani Rules

r

_

+ +

bk hi lo rd nas

f + rd] in As [ + hij in Tap

5 goes to o in Asurini and to i in Tapirapé

Rule 6 V + bk + lo - rd

- hi + nas

in Tap

* a goes to a in Tapirape

Rule 7 V r

+ + —

bk - i hi lo rd nas

" + lo " - rd

in Tap (Co) in As

* o goes to a in all environments in Tapirape and only in syllable final or in penultimate syllable preceding o in Asurini

Rule l V + bk + hi

[ — hi]

in As and T a p

+ rd J * u merges to o and * u to o in Asurini and Tapirape

Rule i) V [ + nas] —> [ — nas] in As and Gj Nasality o f vowels is lost in Asurini and Guajajara

40

Classification and Typological Problems

Tapirape and Asurini can be shown to be closely related by their participation in a great number of rules. Leite also observes that the loss of vowel nasality may have occurred independently in Asurini and Guajajara. In Guajajara, the loss is unordered with respect to rules i and 2; in Asurini, it has to be ordered after rule 5 but is unordered with respect to rule 8. Soares (1979) studies the process of loss of vowel nasality in Guajajara, Asurini, and Kokama and the vocalic changes associated with it. She concludes that the loss of vowel nasality is a natural process, explained in acoustic and perceptive terms, so it should not be taken as a criterion for determining affiliation among languages. She also claims that it is necessary—and an urgent task—to isolate the factors that can cause sound change in order to avoid the indiscriminate use of shifts of different nature either to construct tree diagrams or to prove relationship among languages by establishing a relative chronology of rules.

Vowel Shift and Push-Chain The proto system as reconstructed by Lemle has six oral vowels (i e i a u o) and six nasal vowels (i e i a u o). Guajajara (Gj), Asurini of the Xingu (AsX), Asurini of the Tocantins (AsT), Tapirape (Tap), Parakana (Par), and Arawete (Ar) are languages of the Tupi-Guarani language family that have undergone vowel shift and changed that system. The shared innovations of these languages are: • raising of *a to t in Asurini of the Tocantins, Asurini of the Xingu, Parakana, and Tapirape • nasalization of *a to a in Tapirape and Arawete • lowering of *o to a in Asurini of the Xingu, Asurini of the Tocantins, Parakana, Tapirape, and Arawete • neutralization of the contrast between *u and *o in Asurini of the Tocantins, Parakana, and Tapirape • nasality loss in Asurini of the Tocantins, Asurini of the Xingu, Parakana, and Guajajara. Specific changes of each language are : • raising of *a and *a to a and of some *o to u in Guajajara • rounding of *a to 0 in Asurini of the Tocantins • raising of *a to i in Tapirape

Vowel Shift in Tupi-Guarani

41

• raising of *a and *a to i and i, fronting of * i and * i to i and i, centralization and unrounding of *u and *o. According to Leite, these shifts are not language-specific; as we have seen in the first section, they are shared innovations that can be formulated as gradual changes that take place one at a time. So the raising from *a to i in Asurini and Tapirape passed an intermediate step *a > a > i (rules 1 and 4 above). The same graduality occurs in the shift *a to 0 in Asurini: *a > 5 > 0 > 0. The interpretation of the process as a gradual change makes it possible to postulate rules shared by the various languages, even though the final result is quite different in each of them. Rules 1, 2, 4, and 5 say that Tapirape, Asurini, and Guajajara are closely related: the change of *a began at the same time in the three languages and it ended up by differentiating Guajajara from Tapirape and Asurini because Guajajara stopped the process in the intermediate step *9 while Asurini and Tapirape continue their way toward 0 and / (rules 4 and 5). Viveiros de Castro has applied this notion of gradual sound change and participation in rules to relate Arawete intimately to Guajajara, Asurini of the Tocantins, and Tapirape. So the raising of certain *a and *a to i in Arawete has taken place following rules 1, 2, 4, and 5 slightly modified in the context of application. Only after that, Arawete had a change of its own merging * i and *i into derived t and i and change both to 1. Leite's proposal has the merit of focusing on the process of change itself. However, it is connected to chronologically ordered rules and a push-chain result, so that the merger of a phoneme into another opens a hole in the pattern that has to be filled by a sound resulting from another change. This push-chain is exemplified in Tapirape as shown in Figure 2. (The dotted line indicates partial and context-sensitive change.) As can be seen in the diagram after rule 5 there is a hole in the pattern (*a); it causes rule 6 (nasalization of *a), which in turn causes rule 7 (*o —> a), which in turn causes rule 8 (*u 0, *u 0). This sequence accounts for the changes in Tapirape only. The change is unmotivated for Guajajara and Asurini (see Figure 3). In Guajajara, rules 1, 2, and 3 do not open holes in the pattern. Rules 1 and 3 are contextsensitive developments that affect only the distribution of allophones, while rule 2 only changes the pronunciation of the phone. The Asurini shift allows a closer examination of the difficulties inherent in this approach. First, with the exception of rules 5 and 8, no hole in the pattern is opened. The changes accounted for by rules 1, 2 , 4 , and 7 are

42

Classification and Typological Problems

Figure 2

Guajajara

e

9

1 /

Asurini

X 1/

f

7.L

2Ä5 Figure 5

primary splits: the system itself does not change. Second, rules 1 and 2 may be an artifact; it is possible to suppose that the pronunciation of *a in the environment of a nasal consonant as well as of *a was [a] in the proto system. Third, there is no motivation, other than the one mentioned above, for an intermediate step * 3 in the change of *a to 0. Fourth, the segments are merged with no subsequent change. Arawete data confirm this state of affairs. It shares rules 2,5, 6, and 7 with Guajajara, Asurini, and Tapirape and it has as its own, rules 1 0 , 1 1 , 1 2 , and 13 as Figure 4 displays.

Vowel Shift in Tupi-Guarani 11

10

e

43

u

o

10

11

u

6 *

e

'--»•a

'2 o

Figure 4.

The rules that open a hole in the pattern and that are triggered by a push-chain mechanism are 10, 11, 12, and 13; however, rules 11, 12, and 13 are synchronic rules that account for the present fluctuations [u ~ i], [o ~ a] (rules 11 and 12), and [i ~ a] (rule 13). It rather seems that the mechanism proposed by Leite for measuring degrees of genetic affinity is circular: the proof of close relationship lies in the greater participation in a set of extrinsically ordered rules, and the rules are ordered the way they are to prove the relationship. This circularity weakens the push-chain explanation.

The Phonetics of the Phonological Systems Lemle (1971) and Leite (1982) have developed their proposals without taking into consideration the phonetics of the systems under comparison. They worked on a strictly phonemic level. A closer examination of the phonetics of these systems leads to a reformulation of both the proto system and the explanation given to vowel shift using a mechanism of extrinsically ordered rules. The vast majority of Tupi-Guarani Languages (Juma, Kamayura, Kayabi, Kokama, Kayoa, Urubu, Parintintin, Oyampi, and Tupinamba, for instance) reproduce the vocalic proto system: six oral vowels (i e i a u o) and six nasal vowels (1 e i a u o). Oyampi is an exception, since it has five nasal vowels and six oral vowels.

44

Classification and Typological Problems

Asurini of the Tocantins and Tapirape 1 have five oral vowels: i e i a o (Harrison 1 9 6 1 ; Leite 1 9 7 7 ) . Tapirape has the same vowels as nasalized phonemes and Asurini has no phonemic nasal vowels. As for Awarete, which has a five or six vowel system, it is uncertain whether the language has the contrast between [u] and [o], or the contrast between [i] and [a] that fluctuates with [u] and [o]. 2 It has five phonemic nasal vowels. Guajajara is the only language that has a seven vowel system (with no phonemic nasal vowels): i e a a i u o. The differences among the phonemic systems described above can be summed up as (1) presence or lack of the contrast between [u] and [o]; (2) one or two central vowels; (3) presence or lack of nasality as a distinctive feature of vowels. The available data now allow us to go further and to characterize phonetically those systems. Six vowel systems with the opposition [u] and [o] have the phonetic outputs 3 shown in Figure 5. Subsystem la is an archetype based on Kayabi (Weiss and Dobson Oyampi is quite similar (Harrison and Olson n.d.). Subsystem l b is derived from Parintintin (Pease 1 9 6 2 ) ; Urubu (Kakumasu 1 9 7 7 ) and Kamayura (Silva 1981) are linked to it. What is typical of the six vowel system is the possibility of a phonetic output [D] attributed to /o/, and [o] attributed either to /u/ or to /o/. 1975);

Five vowel systems have the phonetic outputs in Figure 6. In these languages there is no [0] as a phone. Another point that should be emphasized is the possibility of having [e] and [A] as outputs of /o/ (Harrison 1 9 6 1 ) . Weiss and Dobson ( 1 9 7 5 ) in

Subsystem la

Figure s. Circles indicate allophonic variation.

Subsystem lb

o

Vowel Shift in Tupi-Guarani

45

Figure 6

0_ Figure 7

their description of Kayabi vowels have labeled back vowels as slightly rounded, and the same description is given by Harrison for Asurini /o/ (Harrison n.d.). This reducing of roundness is increased and systematized in Arawete, ending up as the fluctuation [u ~ i] and [o ~ a]. In Guajajara, the seven vowel system is found in the phonetic outputs in Figure 7 (Gudschinsky 1959; Bendor-Samuel 1961). Once more [3] is present; the contrast between /u/ and lol is also found, as in subsystem la and lb. The correlation between the presence of the /u/:/o/ contrast and the output [0], and the lack of the contrast and non-occurrence of [a], is confirmed if we look at the tree classification of the Tupi branch and bring onto the scene the Macro-Tupian languages. In Aweti (Monserrat 1975) and Satere-Mawe (Graham and Graham 1961) there is the /u/ and lol contrast, and the allophone [0] is a submember

46

Classification and Typological Problems

of lol. According to the distribution of back vowels, Sateré-Mawé belongs to subtype la (Figure 5). Cinta Larga (Sandberg 1977), Gaviáo (Moore 1976), and Surui (Bontkes and Bontkes 1973), languages of the Mondé family, have a five vowel system: there is no contrast between [u] and [o] and the phone [0] does not occur. Munduruku (Braun and Crofts n.d.) has a five-vowel system (i e i a u), without the contrast between [u] and [o] and also without the phone [0]. Preceding a lyl coda, /u/ has an unrounded vowel as an allophone. So the Munduruku and Mondé languages are closely connected to the five vowel systems exemplified in Figure 6. It is important to note that the data that made this mapping of phonemic systems into phonetic feasible were found in tentative phonemic analyses that were in general unpublished. These are the impressions of first surveys, when the analyst had not yet phonemized the material. This freshly heard material was written with a kind of transcription that, according to Abercrombie's distinction (1967), can be labeled impressionist and not systematic.

Reformulation of the Proto System The facts presented thus far call for a reinterpretation of the proto system and of the rules transcribed above. In the languages of five vowel systems, it would be more appropriate to have /u/ instead of lol as a norm of the back vowel. This is not a mere transcription decision, but derives from the fact that Tupi-Guarani languages that have an lol phoneme also have a phone [0] as a phonetic output; this does not happen in languages where the contrast between /u/ and lol is neutralized. The allophony of the back vowel in these languages is very similar to that of the phoneme /u/ in languages with a six vowel system. By the same token, it would be more appropriate to have as norms of the proto system, as well as of the modern systems with six vowels, the symbols e and 3 instead of e and 0. Taking into account the allophony of the modern systems, it is possible to postulate allophonic variations in the proto system. Combining Figures 5, 6, and 7, we can propose the diagram in Figure 8 as phonetic output of the proto system.

0 0

Vowel Shift in Tupi-Guarani

47

Figure S

To conceive of the proto system as already having degrees of roundness for back vowels and a low quality for the non-high back vowel explains why there is in the various languages the change to a lesser or greater extent, more or less systematic, of *o to a. 4 Furthermore, it is possible to discard rules 11 and 12 of Arawete and to interpret the variation of back and central vowels as an extension and systematization of an allophony already existent in the proto system that is maintained in Arawete. This variation is not implemented in the other languages. Thus there is no motivation for a push-chain in Arawete that changes *u to i since the domain of /i/ has never been empty. If the norm of the back phoneme in Tapirape and Asurini is /u/ instead of /o/, rule 8, which merged *u and *o into 0, can also be discarded, and a push-chain mechanism is again unnecessary, since the domain of *o continues open. The concept of a nonhomogeneous proto system leads to a revision of Leite's (1982) explanation of vowel shift. Now we can leave out some of the rules, and these are exactly the ones that were pushing the chain to fill the holes. Furthermore, the push-chain explanation is based on the assumption that the changes occurred to restore the balance of the original system, but it does not explain why this balance was broken leaving an asymmetry. Within a typological framework, there would be no need for such a strong mechanism as extrinsically ordered rules. If asymmetry and allophony are already present in the proto system, we can suppose that each language is just reassigning allophones simultaneously in order to solve the original conflict of uneven categorization.

48

Classification and Typological Problems

Vowel Shift: A Typological Proposal If roundness and backness are not considered independent acoustic and perceptual dimensions—that is, if less roundness is taken as directly connected to less backness and vice versa—Figure 8 can be simplified and the vocalic proto system will have the phonetic configuration of Figure 9. This array is justified also by data from Kamayurâ, a language for which there is a more detailed phonetic study. The display of Kamayurâ vowels (Silva 1981), in comparison to Jones's cardinal vowels (1932:31—38), confirms that the central high unrounded vowel has as its domain of articulation the back area, close to w, as shown in Figure 10. The main difference between the matrix here proposed and the system presented by Lemle is that here it is unbalanced and asymmetric: there is the categorial distinction between the high back rounded vowel and the unrounded one (*u [u]:*i [ui]). This distinction does not hold for the other back vowels, that is, there is no categorial distinction between [*y] and [o] and [-y] and [A]—all of them being linked to a single unit *o. This unbalanced system is maintained in the languages with a six vowel system, while the five vowel systems have tried to restore balance by

i

ui

u

e

y

o

c

a

A

Figure 9

Figure 10

Vowel Shift in Tupi-Guarani

49

the reassignment of allophones in the dimension of height and area of articulation. Guajajara focused on the dimension of height. It was shown above that in Guajajara some *a have been raised to [-y] and with that, the distinction between [y] and [o] begins, since at times [y] becomes a part of the domain of /a/. Furthermore, at times *o was raised to [u] and at other times it went to the domain of hi.5 It is the process of nasal vowel loss that gave rise to a seven vowel system: all *a were raised and lost nasality. Asurini and Tapirape tried to solve the asymmetry by conjoining the dimensions of height and area of articulation. In Asurini some patterned to /a/, some *a raised to HI and at other times *a lined up to [o] and the output [3] was lost. Besides that, every *a leveled to [o] in a joint process of raising and changing the area of articulation. Today, [-y] belongs to the same domain of [u] and [o]. In Tapirape there has been a preference of height over place of articulation. All *d became /a/ and only some *a raised to lull while others became nasalized. All *a were raised to /ml and [7] became a part of the domain of lull. Nowadays there is a fluctuation between [u ~ o], [ui ~ 7], [lii ~ -y], Arawete's choice has been in the dimension of area of articulation, although there have been some changes due to raising. In this way, some lined up to /a/ and *i became HI in a clear process of displacement of area of articulation. The same process happened in the fluctuation [u ~ ui] and [o ~ y]. Figure 11 shows this redistribution of allophony in Guajajara, Asurini, and Tapirape. Depending on its four or five vowel system, Arawete would have the redistribution of allophones shown in Figure 12.

i

UI u

i

UI u

i

UI u

e

y

0

e

y

0

e

y

£

a

D

£

a

£

a

Figure 11

0

50

Classification and Typological Problems

i

UI

u

i

UI

u

e

y

0

e

y

0

e

a

e

a

Figure 12

Conclusions The actualization of the mechanisms of change explained above can be independent of the order in which each one took place. The changes can occur simultaneously. The main concern is the redimension of the phonological domains through the handling of allophones already present in the proto system: thus the original matrix has an inherent asymmetry. This asymmetry favors certain changes, but some languages may choose to remain with the unbalanced system. The idea of simultaneous change is implicit in Rodrigues (1980:3). For him, the shift that occurred in Tapirapé and resulted in *u to 0, *o to a and *a to a can be explained as the phonological increase of one degree of compactness6 in back vowels. This shift is handled by the following rule: V + posterior n compact

•*

[n + i compact]

The potential for change is then circumscribed in the proto system, and universal acoustic, perceptual, and articulatory principles that govern sound change limit the possibilities of shifts. The paths each language can follow, then, are not numerous; and the similarity of vowel shift processes found in different languages should not be taken as evidence for determining closer relationships. In fact, the historical studies of Brazilian Indian languages are still in their infancy. Better descriptive analyses are still needed. And in phonemic studies, an attitude of nonreductionism, with careful phonetic investigation and description of allophones, surely could contribute greatly to the study of sound change and lead to a deeper understanding of the relationship between synchrony and diachrony.

V o w e l S h i f t in T u p i - G u a r a n i

51

NOTES

1. Parakaná (Aberdour 1975) and probably Asurini of the Xingu (Nicholson 1982) could be added to this list. However, the available material does not allow use of the data for the present purposes. 2. Viveiros de Castro, personal communication. 3. Only oral vowels will be dealt with henceforth. The phonetic symbols used by most of the authors are reproduced as is. In the last section, the International Phonetic Alphabet will be used. 4. As was shown above, Tapirapé changed *D to a. In Asurini and Araweté, the rules have restrictions in their environments. In Kayabi (Soares 1978) the change is sporadic, unsystematic, and less frequent. 5. The change of 0 to u has probably had its implementation in vocalic harmony. The change of *o to a in Asurini may also be interpreted as implemented by vowel harmony. This topic deserves more attention in Tupi-Guarani linguistics. 6. According to Rodrigues (1980:3), nasal vowels are one degree more compact than the corresponding oral vowels. REFERENCES

Abercrombie, David 1967 Elements of General Phonetics. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. Aberdour, Catherine 1975 Formulario dos vocabularios padróes para estudos comparativos preliminares nos línguas indígenas brasileiras. R i o de Janeiro: Museu Nacional. Bendor-Samuel, David 1961 "Guajajara phonemics." Manuscript. Bontkes, Willem and Carol Bontkes 1973 "Tentative Phonemic analysis of Suruí." Manuscript. Braun, lise and Marjorie Crofts n.d. "Phonemic statement" (Munduruku). Manuscript. Graham, Albert and Susan Graham 1961 "Phonemic statement of Sataré" (Tentative). Manuscript. Gudschinsky, Sarah 1959 "Notes on Guajajara (Tupi) language (Posto Indígena Gongalves Dias)." Manuscript. Harrison, Carl 1961 "Report of Asurini survey." Manuscript, n.d. "The phonemics of Asurini; a language of Brazil." Manuscript. Harrison, Carl and Gary Olson n.d. "Notas sobre regularidades fonéticas na lingua Wàyàpi." Manuscript. Jensen, Cheryl 1984 O desenvolvimento histórico da lingua Wayampi. M . A . dissertation. Sao Paulo: Universidade Estadual de Campinas. Mimeograph.

52

Classification and Typological Problems

Jones, Daniel 1932 An Outline of English Phonetics. Cambridge: W. Heffer & Sons Ltd. Kakumasu, James 1977 "Formulário-Padrao Tupi" (Urubu). Manuscript. Leite, Yonnc 1977 Aspectos da fonologia e morfofonologia Tapirapé. Sèrie Lingüística V I I , Rio de Janeiro: Museu Nacional. 1982 A classificagäo do Tapirapé na familia Tupi-Guarani. Ensaios de Lingüística 7:25-32, Belo Horizonte: Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais. Lemle, Miriam 1971 "Internal classification of the Tupi-Guarani linguistic family." Tupi Studies I, ed. David Bendor-Samuel, pp. 107-29. Norman, Okla: Summer Institute of Linguistics. Monserrat, Ruth 1975 "Formulàrio dos vocabulários padröes para estudos comparativos ñas línguas indígenas Brasileiras." Rio de Janeiro: Museu Nacional. Manuscript. Moore, Dennis 1976 "Dados preliminares de lingua dos Indios Gaviäo do Territorio de Rondönia (Brazil)." Manuscript. Nicholson, Velda 1982 "Breve estudo da Lingua Asurini do Xingu." Ensaios Lingüísticos 5, Brasilia: Summer Institute of Linguistics. Pease, Helen 1962 "Tentative phonemic statement for Parintintins." Manuscript. Rodrigues, Aryon Dall'Igna 1955 "As línguas 'impuras' da familia Tupi-Guarani." Anais do XXXL Congresso Internacional de Americanistas: 1055-71. Säo Paulo. 1964 "A classificagäo do tronco lingüístico Tupi." Revista de Antropologia 12. 1 - 2 : 9 9 - 1 0 4 . Säo Paulo. 1980 C o n t r i b u y e s das Línguas Brasileiras para a fonética e fonologia. Paper read at the XII Reuniäo Brasileira de Antropologia, Rio de Janeiro. Mimeograph. 1984—85 "Rela^öes internas na familia lingüística Tupi-Guarani." Revista Brasileira de Antropologia 27-28:33-53. Sao Paulo. Sandberg, Peter 1977 "Formulàrio dos vocabulários padröes para estudos comparativos preliminares ñas línguas indígenas Brasileiras." Rio de Janeiro: Museu Nacional. Manuscript. Silva, Marcio 1981 A fonologia segmental Kamavurá. M.A. Dissertation. Säo Paulo: Universidade Estadual de Campinas. Mimeograph. Soares, Marilia Facó 1978 "A classificagäo interna do Kayabi na familia Tupi-Guarani." Manuscript.

Vowel Shift in Tupi-Guarani 1979

53

A perda da nasalidade e outras mutaçôes vocálicas em Kokama, Asurini e Guajajara. M.A. dissertation, Rio de Janeiro: Universidade Federal do R i o de Janeiro. Mimeograph. Viveiros de Castro, Eduardo 1984 Araweté: urna visâo da cosmologia e da pessoa Tupi-Guarani. Appendix i. Ph.D. thesis. R i o de Janeiro: Museu Nacional. Mimeograph. Weiss, Helga and Rose Dobson 1975 "Phonemic statement of Kayabi." Manuscript.

María Eugenia Villalón A Spatial Model of Lexical Relationships Among Fourteen Cariban Varieties

ABSTRACT

A brief summary of the history and distribution of the Cariban speakers and a short review of earlier classificatory work introduce a lexicostatistical study of fourteen Cariban varieties, based on a slightly modified 100-item word list. It was found that the percentages of homosemantic cognates do not conform well to a hierarchical structure, leading to the hypothesis that the pattern of Cariban lexical differentiation may reflect, instead, a cline structure. A spatial tridimensional model generated through the application of the multidimensional scaling technique revealed the existence of both types of structure, with a clear predominance of cline relationships. This model correlates well with the geographical distribution of the sample languages and suggests tentative internal groupings that differ substantially from those postulated in recent works on Cariban classification. The results of the multidimensional scaling show a basic core (east)/periphery (west) differentiation within the family that corresponds to the degree of divergence found among the types tested. It is construed that the greater homogeneity manifested by the core languages probably reflects a combination of later splits and sustained contacts accomplished through participation in regional networks. Finally, the scaling outcome, aided by selected glottochronological measures, becomes the basis for an inferred model of Cariban diffusion.

Introduction1 I n 1955, N o r m a n M c Q u o w n s u g g e s t e d that p r o b a b l y the m o s t u r g e n t task f a c i n g the s t u d e n t o f the C a r i b a n linguistic f a m i l y w a s " t o w o r k o u t the lines o f internal d i f f e r e n t i a t i o n " (19551562). T h e r e are several w a y s o f deali n g w i t h this task. R e v i e w i n g the d i f f e r e n t m e t h o d o l o g i c a l a p p r o a c h e s t o l a n g u a g e g r o u p i n g , K e y ( 1 9 7 9 : 1 5 ) d i s t i n g u i s h e d b e t w e e n the i n s p e c t i o n o f

Lexical Relationships Among Cariban Varieties

55

vocabulary similarities (mass comparison) and the classical comparative method. In this paper I employ preferentially the first without entirely disregarding the second, undertaking a lexicostatistical study of 14 Cariban languages in order to address the priority cited by McQuown. As a first step, the glottochronological technique developed by Swadesh was used to estimate the degree of similarity existing among the varieties. Subsequently, multidimensional scaling, as applied by Black (1974) to linguistics, was used to generate a more sophisticated model of their internal relationships. The object of these lexicostatistical applications was twofold: (1) to outline a provisional classification and (2) to shed light on selected aspects of Cariban prehistory, such as the probable center of origin and dispersal paths of its speakers. The varieties included in this study are the following: Yabarana (YA); Kamarakoto (KA); Panare (PA); Pemon or Arekuna (PE); Bakairi (BA); Makushi (MI); Taurepan (TA); Makiritare or Ye'kwana (MA); Carib, Kari'na, or Galibi (CA); Cumanagoto (CU); Hianacoto-Umaua (HO); Chaima (CH); Oayana or Rucuyen (OA); and Macoita or Yukpa (MT). With the exception of Chaima and Cumanagoto, all the languages are currently spoken. The status of Hianacoto-Umaua is uncertain (cf. Durbin and Seijas 1973a; Durbin 1977).

History and Distribution of the Cariban People Cariban is one of the great linguistic families of South America, encompassing dozens of languages spoken by the Caribs, carriers of a vigorous culture that left a profound and permanent imprint on northern South America. They are manioc cultivators, master canoe builders, and expert navigators; these skills allowed them to engage in successful raiding and to build up extensive trading networks. A historical quirk immortalized these people (caribales) in our English term "cannibal," although the existence of this practice among them has never been definitely proven. SuedBadillo (1978) has recently argued that the term "Carib" was abused by the chroniclers and came to identify any native group that opposed or resisted the process of colonization. Columbus first encountered "Carib" people (i.e., people to whom he attributed man-eating habits) in the Lesser Antilles during his second voyage; however, the real ethnic or linguistic identity of those people was never conclusively ascertained. Recent investigations tend to show that Cariban-speaking groups did not establish permanent settlements in the

56

Classification and Typological Problems

Antilles during pre-conquest times, in spite of the fact that their raids may have taken them as far north as Florida. Allaire (1980), after carefully reviewing the ethnohistorical and archaeological data, found no acceptable evidence of prehistoric Carib settlements and migrations from the mainland into the Windward Islands. As a result, he concluded that the expression "Island Carib" could not be correlated or associated with any particular site, culture, or prehistoric group of people (Allaire 1980:243). Elaborating on his findings, he stated that the term applied most properly and exclusively to a linguistic entity (an Arawakan-based language with heavy Kari'na borrowings): The island population belonged properly to one ethnic group whose distribution included both the islands as far north as Guadeloupe and a section of the Guiana coast between the Corentync and Oyapoc rivers. This ethnic group, despite its cultural uniformity, was essentially bilingual in speech, although perhaps in the process of assimilation to the Cariban language of the mainland. (Allaire 1980:243)

Durbin seemed to hold a similar opinion when he contended "that there was no Carib language in the islands outside of that spoken by invading Carib males for a generation or so after the invasions" (1977:29), a view advanced by Taylor (1954) many years before. Expressing ideas that stand in sharp contrast to those above, Landar (1968) postulated the existence of Carib settlements on the southern Texas coast, claiming that the extinct Karankawa was in fact a member of the Cariban linguistic family, which he believed could be included in the Hokan stock. The evidence he presents for such far-reaching allegations, however, is inconclusive and uneven in quality. Although the likelihood of such a tentative reconstruction is not to be denied out of hand, and stands as a working hypothesis, the facts presently available do not seem to warrant the full acceptance of such greatly enlarged frontiers for Cariban. As the human geography of the newly discovered lands became better known during the centuries following the conquest, Cariban speakers were found on the Atlantic coast of the mainland, from the northern mouth of the Amazon 2 to Cumana (Hoff 1968:9) and the environs of present-day Caracas. In this area, they did not make up a continuous population block, but were interspersed mostly among Arawaks and Waraos. The largest inland concentration of Cariban speakers was centered in the Guiana Highlands, forming enclaves that extended as far as the western shore of Lake Maracaibo and the eastern slopes of the northern tip of

Lexical Relationships Among Cariban Varieties

57

Map 1. Maximum distribution of Cariban languages. Adapted from Rowe (1974b), Loukotka (1968), and Durbin (1977).

Colombia. South of the Lower Amazon were scattered the Arará, Parirí, and the Apiaká. The Palmella were located on the Guaporé River (between Bolivia and Brazil) and the Carijona between the Vaupés and Caquetá rivers (Lathrap 1970; Durbin 1977). On the Upper Xingú and the headwaters of the Tapajós River were settled the Bakairi and Nahukwa, the southernmost Cariban-speaking people. Map 1 gives an approximate picture of the early distribution of Cariban speakers and clearly illustrates their wide scattering. According to many authorities, this widespread arrangement came about as a result of successive and fairly recent migrations:

58

Classification and Typological Problems In the Amazon-Orinoco area, most of the societies with a tropical-forestvillage type of culture belong to a comparatively few, widely distributed, and fairly undifferentiated language groups—Cariban, Arawakan, Tupi-Guarani, and Panoan. The homogeneity of these language groups and the comparative uniformity of their cultures suggest that the wide distribution of these people was brought about by fairly recent and rather rapid migrations. (Steward and Faron 1959:26)

Following a generalized assumption, Lanning has lately suggested that "the last major expansion in the tropical forest was that of the Cariban peoples, which was vigorously carried out when the Europeans arrived in the area" (1974:100, cited in Allaire 1980:238). This belief is shared by most culture historians who frequently claim that the Cariban speakers, supposedly a warlike and expansionist population, fanned out from the heart of lowland South America in a predominantly northerly direction, conquering and displacing the original populations (mostly Arawakan). The evidence supporting this assumption is not decisive however, and allows for more than one interpretation. Durbin apparently concurs with established views when he writes that it would seem almost certain that the Caribs replaced Arawaks, since we see the Guiana Carib languages boxed in among Arawaks on all sides—north and south, east and west. Just as recent Tupi and Yanomamo expansions began to dislodge Caribs from various areas, we would hypothesize that the Caribs pushed Arawaks north into the Antilles, to the west, and deep into southern Brazil. (Durbin 1977:54)

In order to explain how the Caribs might have reached the southern coast of the United States, Landar speculated on the possibility of successive migration waves which, after securing bases in Honduras, Nicaragua, and Panama, finally landed on the Texas coast: The migration resulted from a very early, possibly a late 15th century Kalina naval raid. This raid antedated the loss of the Kalina grammatical core in the Antilles and possibly the Kalina conquests in the Antilles. . . . The migration route was from the homeland north of the Amazon to the coast and Lesser Antilles to parts of Central America to Tamaulipas and Texas (Landar 1968: 257, 258).

On the other land, we have already seen that Allaire (1980) has questioned the historicity of at least some of the Carib migrations. To him the weight of the evidence calls for a radically different interpretation, suggesting that

Lexical Relationships A m o n g Cariban Varieties

59

if one posits Arawak incursions into Carib territories—especially along the continental coastline—rather than Carib invasions, then the interpretation of Carib warfare would change from that of an expansion strategy to one of territorial protection (Allaire 1980:246). In spite of the contrasting views, one fact stands out in the early history of the Cariban people: they constituted one of the dominant elements in the huge and ongoing process of acculturation which, by the time it ended abruptly with the conquest, had given the tropical lowlands its basic cultural homogeneity in the presence of an astounding linguistic and ethnic diversity. Peaceful and progressive acculturation, raids, migrations, the search, structuring and securing of trade networks, occupation of new territories, protection of one's own homeland, and strategic political alliances, must all have played a part in shaping the pattern of Cariban distribution observed in M a p 1. This picture was to be radically altered as the colonization of the mainland proceeded. Today, all the Cariban speakers have disappeared from the Venezuelan coast. According to Humboldt, the Kari'na (Carib) took refuge around 1720 in the lands bordering the Middle Orinoco, 3 where they were evangelized and where they remained after the demise of the missions ( H o f f 1968:11). Their congeners continue to inhabit the coasts of Surinam, the western half of Guyana, and the westernmost coast of French Guiana ( H o f f 1 9 6 8 : 1 1 - 1 2 ) . Other Cariban speakers are presently found in the Sierra de Perija, which divides Venezuela and Colombia, on the Vaupes River, throughout most of the Guiana land mass, and on the Xingu. T h e assumption of prehistoric Carib migrations as the more likely explanation for their widespread distribution leads us to the question concerning their original homeland and center of dispersal. In this respect there has been no lack of alternatives set forth. Rochefort (1681), for example, was unique in identifying the Caribs with the Cofachites and placing their original home on the southeastern shores of the United States, from whence they had supposedly spread wedgelike over South America. Von Steinen (1892) suggested the Upper Xingu as the probable homeland, a belief shared by Rivet and Loukotka (Tovar 1961:134). Landar is ambiguous on the point for, immediately after proposing the area north of the Amazon, he quoted Rivet, w h o thought that the center of dispersal lay between 10° and 12° latitude (between the Upper Xingu and the U p p e r Tapajos) (1968:258). Steward and Faron suggested the general area of Guiana as the probable center of diffusion (1959:289), while Durbin

6o

Classification and Typological Problems

pointed to the Guiana area of Venezuela, Guyana, Surinam, or French Guiana, excluding as improbable Brazilian Guiana ( 1 9 7 7 : 3 5 ) - To Lathrap, on the other hand, "the weight of historical and linguistic evidence suggests that the ancient home of Proto-Cariban was on or adjacent to the Guiana Highlands possibly along the north shore of the Lower Amazon" ( 1 9 7 0 : 8 2 — 8 3 ) . Furthermore, he associated the Carib expansion with a ceramic tradition that spread gradually out of the northeast quadrant of the Amazon Basin into adjacent parts of South America about A.D. 500 (Lathrap 1 9 7 0 : 1 6 5 ) . These conflicting opinions make it evident that many lengthy and comprehensive investigations must be carried out before this question can be elucidated. Indeed, it is in this field that linguistic studies can make a significant contribution to the culture history of some of the most important Southern American ethnic groups. McQuown was quite correct in pointing out that rather urgent and "necessary studies for tracing migration history via language distribution need to be carried out in Carib a n " (1955:564)-

Cariban External Affiliations The essential and original unity of the Cariban stock was recognized in fairly early times. The Capuchin missionary Francisco de Tauste, for example, noted in 1680 mere dialectical differences among Chaima, Cumanagoto, Core, Quaca, Paria, and "Varrigones," reporting that although Carib was understood throughout the Province of Cumana (i.e., from the Gulf of Paria to Caracas) it was more "proper and natural" to the last mentioned groups ( 1 6 8 0 [ 1 8 8 8 ] : 1). Father Gilij, who named the linguistic family, reported in the mid-eighteenth century on the affinities he had observed among the Orinoco tongues, admitting that his knowledge of Tamanaco enhanced his comprehension of other Cariban languages (1782 [ 1 9 6 5 ] : 1 7 1 ) . Proving to be a surprisingly good comparativist, he grouped all the languages spoken along the Orinoco River into nine separate "mother tongues" and distinguished the following Cariban "dialects": "Tamanaco, Pareca, Uokeari, Uaraca-Pachili, Uara-Mucuru, the language of the women without men [Amazons], Payuro, Kikiripa, Mapoye, Oye, Palenco, Maquiritare, Akerecoto, Avaricoto, Pariacoto, Cumanacoto, Guanero, Guakiri, Areveriana," the languages spoken on the Paria coast, and the environs of Caracas (Gilij 1 7 8 2 [ 1 9 6 5 ] : I 7 4 ) - Additionally, Gilij recalled

Lexical Relationships Among Cariban Varieties

61

having perceived very early in his career lexical affinities between the land Caribs and the "cannibals of the Antilles" (Gilij 1782 [1965] :i74-)Thereafter, the autonomous position of the Cariban family has remained unaltered, although suggestions for more inclusive groupings have not been lacking. For example, Schuller (1919-20) proposed including Cariban and Arawakan in a broad linguistic phylum along with Chibchan and Mayan. Mason also believed that Arawakan might prove to be ultimately related to Cariban (Steward and Faron 1959:25), while De Goeje hinted at a possible relationship between Cariban, Tupian, and Arawakan (1909:2). Rodrigues, a contemporary student, has argued for the existence of "a quite close relationship between Tupian and Cariban" (1974:56) although Hoflf, rejecting such a notion, has called attention to the numerous borrowings found between Tupi and Carib (1968:13). Greenberg (1962) tentatively suggested an encompassing Gc-Pano-Carib phylum, and we have already commented on Landar's claims affiliating Cariban to the Hokan stock. Up to the present, however, none of the attempts to establish wider genetic relationships for Cariban has proved entirely successful. Thus it seems most convenient for the purpose at hand to treat Cariban as an independent linguistic entity. This way, the scope of the present discussion is held within manageable bounds and our attention focused on the problems concerning the composition of the Cariban family.

Cariban Internal Subdivisions Until very recently, most of the Cariban internal classifications were based on geographical rather than linguistic considerations. This applies to Mason's (1963), Tovar's (1961), and Simpson's (1940) classifications, which followed essentially the same lines. These attempts have been largely superseded by Greenberg's (1962), Loukotka's (1968), Girard's (1971), Durbin's (1977), and Migliazza's (1980) work. Greenberg divided the MacroCarib group into northern, southern, and northwestern branches. All the languages included in this paper fall within his Northern Branch except Bakairi and Macoita, which are listed as members of the southern and northwestern branches respectively. Key, who based her work on Greenberg's typological classification, skips these subdivisions and instead draws on the work of Durbin and Seijas, who posited, among others, a Western Carib group (Colombia and Venezuela) and a Southern Division embrac-

62

Classification and Typological Problems

ing Hianacoto of southeastern Colombia, Maquiritare of southern Venezuela, and Bakairi of the Upper Xingú in Brazil (1979:103). Loukotka (1968) presented an overly complex and unrealistic picture of the Cariban family, listing 256 languages and dialects arranged in 24 "groups." Yet 153 of these lacked any documentary evidence whatsoever and for many others only a few extant words or phrases were on record (cf. Rowe 1974a). More than half of the subgroups proposed were not supported by adequate data, and represented, in reality, merely the educated guess of this eminent scholar. Girard's (1971) provisional classification, based on languagespecific phonological features, included 14 groups whose members bore a relatively close kinship, plus a miscellaneous category: (1) Cumana, (2) Makiritare, (3) Pemon, (4) Oayana, (5) Waiwai, (6) Carib, (7) Tiriyó, (8) Yabarana, (9) Aparai, (10) Paushiana, (11) Bonari, (12) Arará, (13) Bakairi, (14) Motilon, and (15) Opon-Carare, Panare, Palmella, and Pimenteira. Durbin's (1977), and Migliazza's (1980) partial groupings represent the most satisfactory work on Cariban classification presently available. Since Durbin's groupings constitute a fundamental reference point for the discussion that follows, they are reproduced in Table 1 as originally published. This classification establishes a basic division between Northern and Southern Cariban, reflecting the changes that Proto-Cariban *p has undergone in the latter languages. Migliazza (1980), who recently assessed the status of the languages of the Orinoco-Amazon watershed, agreed with most of Durbin's subdivisions but added some important modifications of his own. He placed Wayumara among the East-West Guiana Carib languages and transferred Waiwai to the Southern Guiana Carib division, arguing that it closely resembles Hishkaryana and exhibits a change from Proto-Cariban *p to h (Migliazza 1980:123). Henley and Mattei-Muller have challenged this assertion, claiming that the available evidence on this point is ambiguous, and thus Waiwai must be considered a borderline case in whichever group it is included (1982:82-83). Additionally, Migliazza pointed out that Sapara, Yawapery, Pauxiana, Wayumara, and Purucoto are all extinct4 and correctly identified Brazil as the former location of the latter two. Lastly, he detected some redundancies in Durbin's classification, observing that "Wabui" is a general name for the people of the Nhamunda River who otherwise speak Hishkaryana, as is likewise Parukoto a common reference term for the aforementioned groups as well as for Waiwai and other tribes of the northern Pará region (Migliazza 1980:122; Gama Malcher 1964:27-28). Similarly, Yawapery, Crichana, and Waimiri seem to be synonymous terms (Migliazza 1980:123; Gama Malcher 1964:

T A B L E I.

D U R B I N ' S CLASSIFICATION OF C A R I B LANGUAGES

I. N O R T H E R N CARIB A. Coastal Carib 1. Venezuelan Coastal a. Chaima* b. Cumanagoto* c. Yao* d. Tamanaco* 2. Sierra de Pcrijâ a. Japreria b. Yukpa c. Yuko 3. Opone-Carare* B. Western Guiana Carib 1. Mapoyo 2. Yabarana 3. Panare 4. Quaca* 5. Pareca* C. Galibi D. East-West Guiana Carib 1. Oayana-Aparai 2. Roucouyene 3. Aracaju* 4. Trio-Rangu 5. Warna (Akuriyo) 6. Urukuyana 7. Triometesen 8. Kumayena 9. Pianakoto 10. Saluma 11. Pauxi* 12. Cachuena 13. Chikena 14. Waiwai 15. Paravilhana* 16. Wabui Source: D u r b i n 1977. •extinct

17. Sapara 18. Yauapery 19. Waimiri 20. Crichana* 21. Pauxiana 22. Bonari* 23. Makusi 24. Purucoto 25. Pemon 26. Patamona 27. Akawaio 28. Arinagoto* E. Northern Brazilian Outliers 1. Palmella* 2. Pimenteira?* 3. Yaruma* 4. Txicao 5. Pariri* 6. Apiaka* 7. Arara* 8. Yuma* II. S O U T H E R N CARIB A. Southeastern Colombian Carib 1. Hianacoto-Umaua* 2. Guaque* 3. Carijona B. Xingu Basin Carib 1. Bakairi 2. Nahukwa C. Southern Guiana Carib 1. Ye'kwana 2. Wayumara-Azumara 3. Parukoto 4. Hishkaryana 5. Warikyana (KashuyanaKahuyana- Ingarune )

64

Classification and Typological Problems

45), although the scant evidence presented by Loukotka (1968:207—8) could easily lead one to think otherwise.

Cariban Lexicostatistics In order to estimate the degree of lexical similarity obtaining among the members of our language sample, percentages of glottochronologic cognates or "sames" (cf. Hymes i960:18) were calculated employing Swadesh's ioo-item word list, to which the following modifications were made: the term claw in the standard list was replaced by nail, and the terms knot and bark were replaced by hammock and manioc, two terms that must be considered part of the basic vocabulary for the geographical area concerned. The criterion employed in selecting the sample was the availability of satisfactory lexical material up to 1976, when the computations were carried out. Given the number of spoken Cariban languages and the geographical distribution of its speakers, this sample cannot be considered an optimal one. Nonetheless, all of the major divisions and subdivisions proposed by Durbin are represented except Opon-Carare and the Northern Brazilian Outliers (see Table 1), all of which are presently extinct. Map 2 shows the location of the sample languages as reported in the published sources employed to compile the test lists.5 The Panare data were gathered from my own field notes. Because of the high number of synonyms encountered, particularly in the older sources, two cognate counts were performed: one which tended to maximize the number of shared cognates among the sample languages and one which tended to minimize it. Tables 2 and 3 show the respective cognate percentages obtained from the two counts, which on the whole do not show great disparities. When differences do occur they usually do not exceed 3 or 4 percentage points. The maximum variation between the two counts was found for the Makushi-Cumanagoto and TaurepanCumanagoto pairs, which show a change of 6 and 7 points, respectively, attributable, in a considerable measure, to the multiplicity of forms recorded by Tauste (1680 [1888]). Unless otherwise specified, the discussion that follows and the ciphers cited therein are all based on the high count. In compiling the measures of shared cognates presented in Tables 2 and 3, due account was taken of sound correspondences and morpheme boundaries. Strict semantic equivalences were adhered to and most forms compared exhibited a V C V or a C V C V structure. Thus the possibility of

BA CA CH CU

Bakairi Carib, Kari'na, Galibi Chaima Cumanagoto

HO KA MA Ml

Hianacoto-Umaua Kamarakoto MakiritareorYe'kwana Makushi

MT OA PA PE

Macoita or Yukpa Oayana or Rucuyen Panare Pemon or Arekuna

TA YA

Taurepan Yabarana

Map 2. Distribution o f the sample languages. Adapted from Rowe (1974b).

TABLE 2.

PERCENTAGES OF SHARED COGNATES AMONG 14 CARIBAN

LANGUAGES (HIGH COUNT)

TA KA PA PE BA MI TA MA CA CU HO CH OA MT

KA

PA

43 76 51

47 51

PE

BA

MI

TA

83 62 60 69 51 69 56 46

60 60 63 51 62 54

MA

CA

CU HO

CH

OA

61 65 50 65 60 48

62 59 64 60 51

53 77 62 42

62 43

48

49 51 52 51 55 54 59 58 59 52 60 58 48

77 76 55 57 56 48 59 55 47

49 47 52 44 56 48 55 48 37

52 84 89 58 58 60 49 60 51 46

54 53 57 52 58 48 60 56 43

47

53 53 43

66

Classification and Typological Problems

T A B L E 3.

P E R C E N T A G E S OF S H A R E D C O G N A T E S A M O N G 1 4 C A R I B A N

L A N G U A G E S (LOW C O U N T )

KA PA PE BA MI TA MA CA cu HO CH OA MT

YA

KA

PA

PE

BA

MI

TA

MA CA

CU

HO

CH OA

46 49 49 51 53 51 55 53 57 51 57 56 45

43 79 49 77 76 53 57 53 48 57 55 44

46 50 47 46 51 43 53 45 55 47 34

48 80 84 53 56 55 48 58 50 42

51 52 57 51 56 48 56 57 40

81 59 58 63 52 64 56 43

59 60 56 51 59 54 44

60 62 51 62 60 46

49 77 59 42

52 54 42

61 43

58 59 63 60 48

45

including spurious matches (due to onomatopoeia, chance, or sound symbolism) was kept to a minimum (cf. Campbell 1973). One of the main applications of glottochronology is the estimation of linguistic time depth. In this regard some claims have been made for Cariban that seem to be in need of revision. Layrisse and Wilbert (1966), after computing strictly preliminary lexicostatistics for Akawaio, Kari'na, Makushi, Panare, Kamarakoto, Taurepan, Ye'kwana, Yabarana, and Macoita, all Cariban languages spoken in Venezuela, concluded that they appear to have been united 4500 years ago, when they formed a single linguistic community. The languages with maximum divergences are Panare and Bacai'ri of the upper Xingu. . . . They seem to have been separated some 4800 years ago, and 7 m.c. earlier than Yukpa (Macoita) separated from Bacai'ri. (106)

Durbin expressed a similar opinion, stating that his data also suggested such a line of development (1977:36). Our results, however, disagree with these estimates. The deepest time span within our language sample was obtained between Macoita and Panare, which apparently began to diverge from a common parent language approximately 3,290 years ago—considerably less than the span propounded by Layrisse and Wilbert. Similarly, the divergence times among Bakairi, Panare, and Macoita appear to be

Lexical Relationships Among Cariban Varieties

67

overestimated, for our calculations suggest that Macoita and Bakairi began to separate from a common ancestor approximately 2,800 years ago, and Panare and Bakairi 2,200 years ago. These latter two languages, moreover, do not appear to be as closely related as Layrisse and Wilbert thought (1966:67). In fact, our data indicate that, lexically at least, both are significantly more distant from one another than each is from Proto-Cariban, 6 showing that they developed independently, as would be expected from their large geographical separation and relative and longstanding isolation from mainstream Cariban (Guiana heartlands and coasts). Other glottochronological statistics have been computed by MatteiMuller and Migliazza (Migliazza 1980; Henley and Mattei-Muller 1982) in an attempt to estimate the degree of linguistic differentiation existing among Panare, Yabarana, and Mapoyo (Wanai). Given the presence of some discrepancies between these two sources, I undertook the reckoning of cognate percentages for the three languages in question. Table 4 summarizes the results obtained and compares them with those furnished by the cited authors. While Henley and Mattei-Muller have suggested that Mapoyo and Yabarana "could be considered dialectical variants of a single language" (1982:81-82), my own calculations imply that both were a single homogeneous speech community approximately 1,090 ( ± 1 8 0 ) years ago (68% confidence level). As far as the differentiation Mapoyo/ Panare is concerned, both Mattei-Muller and Migliazza estimated a time depth of approximately 2,000 years on the basis of a cognate count per-

TABLE 4.

PERCENTAGES OF SHARED COGNATES

FOR THREE CARIBAN VARIETIES

Yabarana Mapoyo

Panare

Mapoyo

51 49 (42)* (45)' (60)'

72 (80)»

"Mattei-Muller's calculation. N o list specified (in Henley and Mattei-Muller 1982:81). 'Mattei-Muller's calculation from the 200-item word list (Migliazza 1980:126). 'Migliazza's calculation from the 200-item word list (Migliazza 1980:126). 'Migliazza's calculation from the 100-item word list (Migliazza 1980:126).

68

Classification and Typological Problems

formed with the 200-item word list, while Migliazza obtained half that amount employing the 100-item word list (Migliazza 1980:126). M y own calculations performed with the latter list suggest that Panare and Mapoyo began to diverge from a common ancestor 2,360 ( ± 320) years ago (68% confidence level). In sum, the data presented so far reflect splits within our Cariban sample ranging from 390 years (Pemon and Taurepan) to 3,290 years (Panare and Macoita). Besides their application to dating language splits and estimating chronological depth, percentages of shared cognates have proved useful as a tool of linguistic classification, with the assumption that this measure of lexical similarity is presumably an indicator of overall affinity. T w o models of mapping or synthesizing linguistic variation have usually been distinguished: the hierarchical or "tree" model and the continuous (spatial) or "wave" model (Anttila 1972). In the pages that follow both will be considered in an effort to determine the most satisfactory model of Cariban interrelationships. Figure 1 depicts, in the classical branching mode commonly employed in comparative linguistics, a gross outline of the internal relationships im-

MT

HO

YA

OA

CA

CU

CH KA

PE

TA Ml

MA

BA

PA

Figure 1. Approximate tree model of lexical relationships among 14 Cariban varieties.

Lcxical Relationships Among Cariban Varieties

69

plied in Table 2. This tree, it must be emphasized, is not intended to be a formal map, but merely a summary of the similarity matrix produced by simple inspection. The lexicostatistics presented therein readily reveal, on the one hand, a close resemblance among Pemon, Taurepan, Kamarakoto, and Makushi, and between Chaima and Cumanagoto on the other, two clusters whose members share "critical percentages" (Dyen 1962:153). Thus, on the basis of cognate percentages alone, it seems reasonable to consider them separate subgroups. Additionally, the comparative extreme positions of Panare and Macoita, and to a lesser extent, of HianacotoUmaua, is clearly observable. However, other linguistic boundaries are not so well delineated, for once the two or three outliers and the two subgroups have been isolated, the picture blurs considerably. For this reason the tree presented in Figure 1 is not regarded as well motivated or particularly informative. In fact, as will be explained next, it conceals serious shortcomings. H o w well a given set of cognate percentages conform to a tree structure and are thus most appropriately described by a hierarchical classification depends, according to Black, on the extent to which they satisfy the constraint of "ultrametric inequality" (1974:6). This condition is minimally satisfied when the two lowest of three percentages in a triad are equal (for a discussion, see Black 1974). A glance at the Cariban percentages reveals triplets that fulfill this condition as well as others that do not. Table 5 reproduces the percentages of cognates shared by Taurepan, Kamarakoto, and Pemon. This information can be adequately and fully depicted by means of a branching structure (Figure 2) because it satisfies the aforementioned condition. Assuming that the percentage differences are significant, the tree shows the closer relationship that Pemon and Taurepan share, as well as the more distant one that both have with Kamarakoto. Just the opposite situation is exemplified in Table 6, where the lexicostatistical percentages for Macoita, Carib, and Cumanagoto are presented.

T A B L E 5.

P E R C E N T A G E S OF S H A R E D COGNATES

AMONG T A U R E P A N , KAMARAKOTO, AND PEMON

Pemon Taurepan

Kamarakoto

Pemon

76 76

89

76%

Figure 2. Cognate percentages among Kamarakoto, Pemon, and Taurepan.

T A B L E 6.

PERCENTAGES OF SHARED COGNATES

AMONG MACOITA, C A R I B , AND CUMANAGOTO

Carib Cumanagoto

Macoita

Carib

51 42

62

51% - 42°«

Figure 3. Cognate percentages among Macoita, Cumanagoto, and Carib.

Lexical Relationships Among Cariban Varieties

71

Rendered graphically, the resulting tree (Figure 3) is capable of displaying the greater similarity existing between Carib and Cumanagoto, but it fails to show that the former is more closely related to Macoita than the latter, assuming the percentage differences to be significant. Cases such as the one detailed above indicate that the Cariban percentages do not conform well to a hierarchical representation or subgrouping. A tree or another type of branching structure, therefore, would not provide a very useful base for interpretation or inference.

Multidimensional Scaling Black (1974) has demonstrated the usefulness of multidimensional scaling in the investigation of nonhierarchical linguistic relationships. This technique allows for the representation of underlying language affinities (as reflected in cognate percentages) in some sort of spatial configuration of varying dimensions. Or, in slightly different terms, it enables the researcher to test for the existence of linguistic clines, "considered to be characterized by potentially continuous variation in some sort of meaningful space" (Black 1974:6). We shall test the hypothesis that Cariban linguistic variation shows a predominantly cline arrangement. If multidimensional scaling can represent the Cariban varieties in a spatial model of n dimensions that correlate highly with their geographical distribution, then it will be considered that the cognate percentages do in fact conform to a cline structure. The aim of multidimensional scaling is to represent linguistic (lexical) differences (proportions of noncognate homosemantic words) as distances among points (languages) in the smallest acceptable number of dimensions (Kirk and Epling 1973 :60). In our particular application of this statistical procedure, Kruskal's M D S C A L multidimensional scaling computer program was employed, the cognate percentages shown in Table 2 having provided the input data. In a most general way, this and other similar computer programs start out with an initial configuration and modify it iteratively until no small change can improve further its fit with the original percentages, within certain preset limitations (Black 1974:21). The resulting configuration is then printed with an index of "stress" or a measure of the "goodness of fit" between the scaling and the original percentages on which it is based. In more precise terms, the stress index is the sum of the squares of the deviations between distances among the points

72

Classification and Typological Problems

in the final scaling and the "ideal distances" for the input percentages. Ideal distances are values that satisfy the constraint of nonmetric scaling (decreasing monotoniaty: i.e., higher percentages have to be represented by smaller ideal distances) and at the same time are closest (as measured by stress) to the actual distances used to represent the percentages in physical space (Black 1974:22—23). As a rule of thumb, a stress measure above .10 is considered an indication of relatively poor fit. Figure 4 shows the "best" bidimensional spatial representation of Cariban lexical relationships obtained after the application of multidimensional scaling. The peripheral positions of Panare, Macoita, and

MT

KA PE '.TA • Ml CA • CH OA

HO

•CU

•MA

• BA

YA,

PA

A

N

Figure 4. Bidimensional representation of lexical relationships among 14 Cariban varieties (stress = .098).

Lexical Relationships Among Cariban Varieties

73

Hianacoto-Umaua stand out clearly, as do two distinct clusters: Taurepan, Pemon, Kamarakoto, and Makushi (Gran Sabana group), on the one hand, and Chaima and Cumanagoto (the coastal group) on the other, close to which now appears Makiritare. Since Figure 4 can be interpreted as a kind of surface map of the northern half of South America, it has been superimposed (with a slight rotation) on Map 2, in order to gauge how well the scaling positions correlate with the actual geographical distribution of the sample languages (see Figure 5). At first glance, it is obvious that the correspondence is very poor, worse indeed than a stress index of .098 would have led us to believe. While Hianacoto-Umaua and Macoita fall very near their true placements and the members of the Gran Sabana group appear not too distant, having preserved their relative positions albeit with a reversed compass orientation (NE to SW instead of NW to SE), all of the remaining points, save Panare, have been "pulled" excessively toward the center of the axes: Chaima and Cumanagoto are aligned inversely and show up quite to the south of their attested locations; Carib and Oayana are veered inordinately to the west; Bakairi appears far to the northwest of its Xingu base, while Panare and Yabarana, in contrast, have undergone a great shift southward, and Makiritare a lesser one. It would have been desirable to test whether the essentially hierarchical positions of Hianacoto-Umaua, Macoita, and Panare were affecting the scaling of the finer relationships, for the possibility exists that the primary split between the latter two (which in most trial configurations surfaced opposite each other) could be interfering with a more sensitive evaluation of the interrelationships obtaining among the remaining variants (Black 1974). This could have been accomplished by means of a new application of multidimensional scaling, utilizing a trimmed sample, but the depletion of the allotted computer time prevented such an undertaking. Nevertheless, several earlier runs performed with an initial sample of 11 languages, in which Panare and Bakairi represented the more extreme varieties, demonstrated that pruning them from the sample enhanced somewhat the overall fit of the new 9-point configuration and lowered the stress index, but it also evidenced that most of the improvement occurred in the areas of least interest, namely, the coastal group and the Gran Sabana cluster. Hence, it is reasonable to suppose that perhaps not much would have been gained by this added manipulation. So far our results indicate that a two-dimensional scaling of linguistic distances within Cariban, measured by the degrees of lexical differentia-

Lexical Relationships Among Cariban Varieties

75

tion that reflect the percentages of homosemantic cognates among 14 varieties, bear little relationship to geographical reality. Therefore, our initial hypothesis stands unproved. The rather poor fit and a stress index (.098) approaching the limits of the acceptable suggest, however, that two dimensions may be insufficient to portray adequately the Cariban interrelationships. If this assumption is correct, then a third dimension must be introduced in order to test for the existence of clines and generate a satisfactory model of internal affiliations and development. Figure 6 depicts a three-dimensional scaling of the percentages presented in Table 2, in the form of a triaxial diagram. Each point (or language) in it should be visualized as "suspended" in the space described by the cube, whose "floor" and "ceiling" can be interpreted as a "flattened" and smaller version of the surface plane shown in Figure 4. This tridimensional model of Cariban lexical relationships has a stress index of .048, a measure equivalent to a 50 percent drop from the previous scaling, which augurs a markedly improved fit.

Figure 6. Tridimensional model of lexical relationships among 14 Cariban varieties (stress = .048).

Figure s• Two-dimensional scaling of 14 Cariban varieties compared to their geographical distribution. Adapted from Rowc (1974b).

76

Classification and Typological Problems

In effect, a comparison with Map 2 reveals an enhanced arrangement of the points in space relative to one another. This time Panare, Yabarana, and Makiritare appear aligned in a manner that reflects their true geographical distribution. Chaima and Cumanagoto are grouped toward the "northernmost" corner, the latter showing up correctly to the "northwest" of the former, and Carib to the "southeast" of both. Oayana also manifests a better placement, figuring to the "southeast" of Makiritare and Carib. Bakairi appears located more accurately toward the "south" of Oayana, as well as Hianacoto-Umaua, which now is situated to the "southwest" of Panare and Yabarana. In contrast, the relative collocation of the Gran Sabana cluster and Macoita seems to have deteriorated. Careful inspection of the configuration, however, reveals that this assessment is not entirely correct, for while Macoita is indeed misplaced, the grouping of the cluster is actually better, figuring to the "south" of the coastal group (Chaima and Cumanagoto) and "west" to "northwest" of Carib and Oayana. Finally, it may be noticed, concerning the internal sorting of the group's members, that our model has generated exactly their mirror image; that is, if Makushi, Taurepan, Pemon, and Kamarakoto were to be reflected on their own axes,7 their arrangement would appear precisely as shown on Map 2. These results indicate that a three-dimensional model of Cariban interrelationships is definitely superior to its two-dimensional counterpart, for in spite of its increased interpretative difficulty, it does correlate highly with the comparative geographical distribution of the sample languages, even if it does not reflect adequately the extent of the physical distances involved. Hence, it suggests strongly the predominance of a cline arrangement in Cariban coexisting, nevertheless, with a few hierarchical relationships. These aside, the outstanding feature of the array is a gradual variation through space without sharp splits. As intimated above, the shift to a higher dimensionality imposes the added task of formulating an acceptable description of the third dimension. Figure 7 shows the graph of its values, corresponding to those registered along the Z axis of Figure 6. It is important to first notice that the interlanguage variation is small toward the center and that all scores, taken together, describe a soft elongated S-curve that jumps sharply at the opposite ends of the distribution, where Panare, Yabarana, Macoita, and Hianacoto-Umaua fall. It is these extreme values (in conjunction with those that determine their position along the X and Y axes) that tend to "push" toward the "ceiling" or the "floor" of the configuration of the

Lexical Relationships Among Cariban Varieties

77

Figure 7. Values of the third (Z) dimension of the scaling configuration in Figure 6.

aforementioned varieties. Three of them, it will be recalled, assumed clearly peripheral positions in the bidimensional scaling, on account of which they were preliminarily identified as outliers. The significance of their spatial location "above" or "below" the others will soon be clarified. We are now in a better position to attempt a more refined interpretation of our three-dimensional model and to draw some tentative historical inferences.

78

Classification and Typological Problems

Conclusions Our results do not corroborate the presence of a basic north-south division in Cariban (cf. Table i). It will be recalled that this dichotomy was formulated after grouping in a southern division all languages whose *p had undergone certain changes, mostly fricativization (Durbin and Seijas 1972,1973a; Durbin 1977). The sharing of a single phonological innovation, however, does not seem sufficient evidence on which to establish such a major split, for commonly a cluster of shared related changes is required in order to discard independent innovation (duplication) or borrowing as alternative explanations (Anttila 1972; Hoenigswald i960). Moreover, it could be argued that a shift from *p to h, , f, or w is a rather trivial marker of language splits, due to the fact that it is a fairly recurrent phenomenon attested under a wide variety of conditions and environments. Indeed, several "northern" languages exhibit a similar tendency. Waiwai, it has been remarked above, furnishes a borderline case as to its affiliation in any one of the two main divisions proposed by Durbin. Similarly, several dialects of Panare, a language in which *p > p is solidly established, exhibit p ~ h in noninitial positions. Until further evidence is forthcoming, these considerations lead me to grant priority to our results based on lexical evidence, which, if correctly interpreted, do not uphold the classification of Hianacoto-Umaua, Makiritare, and Bakairi in a single southern branch. Secondly, our scaling configuration does not accord with the composition of the various subgroups proposed in Table 1. As far as it indicates, the classification of the Sierra de Perija varieties (Macoita) and the Venezuelan Coastal Carib (Chaima and Cumanagoto) in a Coastal Carib unit (Durbin and Seijas 1972; Durbin 1977) doesn't appear to be particularly well motivated, nor does the classification of Oayana and the Gran Sabana cluster in an East-West Guiana Carib group (Table 1). In order to summarize visually the extent of the differences discussed, the classification presented in Table 1 and the tridimensional configuration of 14 Cariban languages are compared in Figure 8. In contrast, our results suggest the existence of a central core of languages surrounded by four outliers, two of which constitute remote varieties, linguistically as well as geographically speaking (Hianacoto-Umaua and Macoita). The situation is portrayed in Figure 9, where the inner core languages 8 have been inscribed within the smaller cube, around which are positioned the four "topmost" and "bottommost" variants: Hianacoto-

Durbin's S o u t h e r n Division and subdivisions Durbin's Northern Division a n d subdivisions

Figure 8. Tridimensional model of Cariban interrelationships and Durbin's 1977 classification.

Figure 9. Core and peripheral Cariban varieties.

8o

Classification and Typological Problems

Umaua, Panare, Macoita, and Yabarana. The first three, in particular, should be regarded as unique sorts for, scaling results apart, they are not significantly closcr to any of their nearest configuration neighbors (see note 6). Each one stands in an essentially ranked relationship vis-à-vis the nuclear languages and the rest of the outliers. Yabarana may constitute a borderline case, while Makiritare seems to represent a transitional type between the core and the periphery. Within the nucleus, contrastingly, greater overall affinity prevails among the constituents. In this manner, our three-dimensional configuration of lexical variation within Cariban reveals a mixture of cline and hierarchical relationships, corresponding to the "core'V'periphery" distinction just proposed. The components of the latter show the greatest divergence, unlike those of the core, among which predominate the more gradual and continuous variation characteristic of linguistic clines. When translated into geographical terms, the core/periphery dichotomy acquires its fullest significance, owing to the fact that all peripherals without exception lie toward the western edge of the Cariban distribution, with Macoita and Hianacoto-Umaua conforming its boundaries (Map 3). It is not without relevance either, that Makiritare, identified tentatively as a transitional variety, holds a similar position in geographical terms (sec Map 3). In sum, the core/periphery distinction coincides with a west/east division within Cariban, this being about the only clearly defined break suggested by the scaling results: toward the periphery ("west"), linguistic divergence is considerable; toward the core ("east"), it is slight. Finally, it remains to be determined whether any subrelationships can be detected among the nuclear specimens, leading to the identification of subgroups. In this regard, our tridimensional scaling has preserved the well-known clusters comprised by Makushi, Taurepan, Pemon, and Kamarakoto, on the one hand, and by Cumanagoto and Chaima on the other. Together they constitute the only two demarcated sets within our sample. For further exploration, several triadic tests (see note 6) were run with the following results: (1) Chaima and Cumanagoto arc significantly closer to each other than to Makiritare or Carib. (2) Oayana and Makiritare arc significantly closer to each other than to Bakairi (i.e., the latter is not significantly closer to either of the two). (3) No pair in the triad Makiritare-Cumanagoto-Carib is signifi-

Lexical Relationships Among Cariban Varieties

81

Map 3. Peripheral and core varieties. Adapted from R o w c (1974b).

cantly closer to each other than to the third (i.e., M A is not significantly closer to C U than to CA; C U is not significantly closer to M A than to CA; and C A is not significantly closer to C U than to MA). (4) N o pair in the triad Carib-Makiritare-Oayana is significantly closer to each other than either is to the third. (5) As expected, Pemon and Taurepan were found to be significantly closer to each other than to Makushi or Kamarakoto, but both were found to be significantly closer to Makushi than to Kamarakoto. These outcomes, in conjunction with those derived from the multidimensional scaling of lexical distances measured by percentages of homosemantic cognates, imply a classification of the fourteen varieties tested similar to that shown in Table 7.

82

Classification and Typological Problems

T A B L E 7.

C L A S S I F I C A T I O N OF 14 C A R I B A N

LANGUAGES DERIVED FROM MULTIDIMENSIONAL SCALING

PERIPHERAL (Outliers or Western Division) I. II. III. IV.

Macoita Hianacoto-Umaua Panare Yabarana

CORE (Eastern Division) V.

Makiritare Carib Oayana Bakairi Makushi 1 J Taurepan j> [ Pemon J Kamarakoto f Cumanagoto I Chaima

Interpretation and Inferences The greater homogeneity discerned among the core or nuclear languages probably reflects a combination of later splits, postscparation contacts, and participation in a dialect chain. It is conceivable that our configuration might have "recovered" the "fossil" evidence of a former state of affairs that stood in sharp contrast to the modern situation characterized by wide linguistic gaps due to the extinction of intermediate forms and diminished intertribal interaction. Having discussed selected aspects of time depth within Cariban in an earlier section of this chapter, we turn next to a review of some of the evidence pointing to the existence of dialect chains, pervasive cultural contacts, and extensive trade networks among the aborigines of northern Venezuela and the Orinoco (Morey 1975; Morales and Arvelo-Jimenez 1981; Lathrap 1973). For example Gilij (1782 [1965]), who was well acquainted with the aboriginal Cariban population occupying the Middle Orinoco in the eighteenth century, observed that, allowing for the "expected small differ-

Lexical Relationships A m o n g C a r i b a n Varieties

83

ences," it all shared practically the same l a n g u a g e as that s p o k e n o n the Paria coast and the environs o f Caracas (174)- H e reported, additionally, that " t h e Makiritare, A r e v e r i a n o s , C u m a n a c o t o s and the inhabitants o f Paria c o u l d be called T a m a n a c o s [extinct speakers o f a M i d d l e O r i n o c o C a r i b a n v a r i e t y ] " (Gilij 1782 [1965]: 172). O n account o f their p r e s u m e d close resemblance, relatively speaking, several current authors have g r o u p e d T a m a n a c o w i t h C h a i m a and C u m a n a g o t o ( L o u k o t k a 1968; Girard 1971; D u r b i n 1977). G i r a r d , f o r example, noticed that Tamanaco's c o n s o n a n t cluster r e d u c t i o n rules seemed t o o c c u p y a position in b e t w e e n the C u m a n a g r o u p , w h i c h retained the clusters intact, and the Makiritare g r o u p , w h i c h r e d u c e d t h e m t o ?S (Girard 1971:206—7). In a n a l o g o u s manner, C i v r i e u x (1980) has remarked o n the manifest affinity b e t w e e n sotto, the M a k i r i t a r e w o r d f o r " p e r s o n , " " t w e n t y , " " h u m a n b e i n g , " and choto (or shoto), generalized w i t h identical m e a n i n g a m o n g the n o r t h e r n C a r i b a n tribes: The central tribes of the Northern Cariban Area [Map 4] (Cumanagoto), and the eastern ones (Chaima) called themselves Choto (people, human being) and spoke dialects of a common tongue (Chotomaimu) which allowed for mutual communication. It is probable, but not proven, that the westernmost Cariban (Caracas, Meregoto) were also Choto and spoke dialects o f the Chotomaimu. (Civrieux 1980:40, my translation) In C i v r i e u x ' s o p i n i o n , this u n i q u e isolexicality bespeaks a degree o f kinship b e t w e e n

Makiritare

and the n o r t h e r n

Cariban

tribes

(Civrieux

1980:38). 9 T h u s , the evidence gathered by a n u m b e r o f researchers emp l o y i n g different m e t h o d s and data provides s o m e s u p p o r t f o r the postulated existence o f a historical dialect area b o u n d e d at the n o r t h by C o a s t a l C a r i b a n a n d t o the s o u t h by G u i a n a C a r i b a n . M i d w a y b e t w e e n the t w o s t o o d as linkages T a m a n a c o and the o t h e r d e f u n c t languages o f the M i d d l e O r i n o c o . N o t e w o r t h y in this respect is the fact that M a k u s h i , another G u i a n a core l a n g u a g e , shares w i t h C u m a n a g o t o and C h a i m a its largest p e r c e n t a g e o f h o m o s e m a n t i c cognates outside its o w n s u b g r o u p ( 6 9 % ) . U p o n application o f the triadic m e t h o d (see n o t e 6), it w a s f o u n d that M a k u s h i is significantly closer t o C u m a n a g o t o than it is t o Makiritare, t h o u g h it is significantly closer t o the latter than t o O a y a n a .

Indeed,

here m a y lie the explanation f o r the inversion that presents the G r a n Sabana q u a r t e t in b o t h configurations, w h e r e M a k u s h i appears repeatedly " p u l l e d " t o w a r d the coastal cluster and C a r i b , reversing the actual g e o g r a p h i c o r i e n t a t i o n o f the chain.

84 68

Classification and Typological Problems 67

66

i CARIBBEAN

65

64

1

r

63

62

SEA

MACUTO CAB CAR CHA COR GUA MAK MAP MAR MAS MER PAC PAL PAR TAG TAM TEQ TES TOM TUC

10

9

8

= = = = = = = = = = = = = = = =

Carib Caraca Chaima Core Guaqueri Makiritari Mapoyo Manche Makushi Meregoto Pareca Palenque Paria Tagare Tamanaco Teque Teserma Tomuza Tucuyo

6

MAS 5

4

A

N

— limit of Northern Cariban area //// Cumanagoto • city

Map 4. Focal area of a major Cariban network. Adapted from Civricux (1980).

One of the most pertinent regions to consider in the examination of sustained interethnic contacts as a significant factor in the evolution of the core or "eastern" languages is the "Northern Cariban Area," which stretched, according to Civrieux (1980), in an easterly direction from Borburata and Lake Valencia to the Paria Peninsula (Map 4). This region was

Lexical Relationships Among Cariban Varieties

85

the home of numerous tribes that were fairly homogeneous in language and culture and clearly distinguishable from the Caribs (Kari'na or Galibi). Halfway through the Northern Cariban belt were located the Palenque and the Cumanagoto, between the Chaima to the east and the Tomuza and its neighbors to the west. The Palenque, who were deft navigators, figured as trade intermediaries between the coastal people, including the Cumanagoto, and the Orinoco tribes. The region comprising the headwaters of the Unare, Ipire, Pao, and Zuata rivers (Map +) constituted a strategic node in the fluvial and trade network linking them. From the Unare Lagoon, where the Palenque and surrounding peoples sought salt and dried and salted fish, flowed these and other items (Civrieux 1980:50— 51) in the Palenque canoes, effectively structuring a socioeconomic web that linked vast territories and set the stage for sustained interethnic contacts. A similar role was played by the Caribs, who, before the advent of the Europeans, exchanged gold and "Amazon stones" throughout Guiana, traveling up and down the Orinoco water system and the Rio Branco as well (Hoff 1968:17, 18—19). When the demand for European items rose among the Indian groups, the Caribs became the key middlemen, bartering foreign goods from the coastal towns deep into the interior country. According to Morey (1975:257), by the seventeenth century they were bringing in large amounts of European goods to the regional markets of the Llanos and exchanging them for slaves. The Caribs also used to participate in the crowded trade fairs that were held annually at the several "trade centers" that developed along key spots on the Orinoco, where highly prized natural resources were gathered at certain times of the year (Morey 1975:269). The turtle beaches of the Middle Orinoco and the fish markets of the Aturcs Rapids were two examples. In the latter, tribes from the east and south met those from the Llanos, especially during the dry season. As early as 1584 Antonio de Berrio was told bv the natives that this area was the center of trade between Guayana and the Llanos (Ojer 1960:54). The Carib made this place one of their regular trading stops on their expeditions up the Orinoco. Here they not only bought provisions for their journey, but met other groups with whom they traded their European articles for slaves. (Morev 1975:271)

It is quite possible (although we lack direct evidence) that the Makiritarc and other Guiana Cariban tribes participated in some of these networks,

86

Classification and Typological Problems

representative of the several regional systems of interaction that were active centuries ago. The idea is not entirely far-fetched, for the Makiritare, who are excellent canoeists and merchants, traveled to Angostura (Ciudad Bolivar) before the end of the eighteenth century in search of iron tools, textiles, and guns. In 1840 the explorer Robert Schomburk met a party of Makiritare from the Cunucunuma River (on the Upper Orinoco) on its way to Georgetown to trade (Coppens 1971:35). The Makiritare oral tradition describes the Indians who inhabited Caicara, a port on the Middle Orinoco (Map 4) and preserves the memory of those who intermarried with these strangers. Similarly, their lore makes reference to those Makiritare who married among the coastal Macuto people (Map 4) and tells how their descendants later dispersed. Lastly, a Makiritare esoteric chant makes numerous allusions to prominent topographical features of the valley of Caracas and surrounding areas. Naturally, the content of an oral tradition cannot be taken in its literal sense or accepted uncritically. Nevertheless, it is not totally devoid of significance for, at the very least, it expresses a point of view about a particular aspect of a distant past epoch (Vansina 1973:20). Altogether, the evidence reviewed confirms the existence of far-flung pre-Columbian regional networks that sprang up in the presence of a vast web of navigable waters capable of sustaining a brisk traffic of peoples and goods. The character of this natural system of communication favored those who, like the Caribs, Cumanagoto, Makiritare, and Pcmon, possessed the required navigational skills, and bypassed or neglected others who, like the Panare, did not. This reconstructed probable course of affairs may provide a partial explanation for the peripheral positions of Panare and Yabarana, neighboring varieties of Makiritare, which bespeak long periods of relative isolation and independent evolution. To conclude the interpretation of the tridimensional scaling results, we will address the problem of determining a likely center of origin and the most plausible dispersal routes for Cariban. In order to do so, it seems convenient to recall briefly some generally accepted principles. The two main postulates of migration theory state that (1) the area of origin of related languages is continuous and (2) the probabilities of different reconstructed migrations are inversely related to the number of reconstructed language movements that each requires (Diebold i960; Dyen 1956). Additionally, it is generally believed that the home of a protolanguage can be safely identified with the area where linguistic differentiation is greatest (Anttila 1972).

Lexical Relationships Among Cariban Varieties

87

In accordance with these axioms, our results suggest the following model of Cariban development: (1) The center of dispersal of the sample languages lay somewhere in the Venezuelan Guiana. Within this general area, the slopes which give birth to the Caura, Cuchivero, and Ventuari watersheds, north of the junction of the sierras Parima and Pacaraima, seem the most likely site of the ancestral home of the Cariban speakers. (2) From this area or thereabout, a very early migration possibly ensued, giving rise to the Yukpa-Macoita enclave. Opon-Carare, farther south, is usually regarded as an extension of this northwesternmost Cariban variety (Durbin and Seijas 1973b). (3) A later cleavage and migration probably gave rise to the Hianacoto-Umaua-Carijona enclave. (4) After these two outward waves were accomplished, or possibly coincident with the second, Yabarana and Panare began to pursue a comparatively independent course of evolution. (5) Concurrently, Cariban initiated a gradual expansion toward the north, south, and east. (6) Among those that followed the southern route (possibly along the Rio Branco) figured, most likely, the ancestors of the Caribs and the Bakairi. The latter, representing the last major split, probably headed straight south, while the Carib branch may have veered northward after its southern sojourn, eventually to wedge between the eastwardly expanding Cariban groups and the coastal population of the Guianas. Or, alternatively, they may have settled on the Atlantic shores and later displaced inland (cf. Allaire 1980). This last reconstruction seeks to incorporate the implications of HofFs analysis of Carib and Tupi borrowings: . . . formerly the area of habitation of the Caribs may well have extended further southwards. An indication of the correctness of this view is perhaps the considerable borrowing that has taken place between Carib and the unrelated Tupi language. . . . Among the loan-words there is a strikingly large proportion of names of plants and animals, far outnumbering words for items of material culture. This supports the assumption that the borrowing was not only the result of long trade travels, but also of a formerly shorter geographical distance between Caribs and Tupi. (Hoff 1968:13)

Map 5 summarizes this reconstructed prehistory of Cariban.

88

Classification and Typological Problems

Map s• Inferred model of Cariban migration.

Acknowledgments A first version of this chaptcr was prepared for a course in quantitative methods in linguistic classification taught by Robert L. Oswalt at the University of California, Berkeley, in the fall of 1975. At that time, he suggested that I revise my paper for publication and to that end made detailed comments and suggestions, most of which I incorporated into the second draft many years later. In spite of the time elapsed, he was kind enough to referee this chaptcr and send his very useful comments. For all this, for his continued interest, and for arranging my use of computer time in 1975, I owe him a debt of gratitude. The Computer Center of the University of California, Berkeley, subsidized the computer time. Finally, Henry E. Corradini deserves special credit and thanks for the beautifully done maps and drawings that accompany the text.

Lexical Relationships Among Cariban Varieties

89

Appendix COORDINATES OF THE THREE-DIMENSIONAL SCALING OF 14 CARIBAN VARIETIES. R

Yabarana Kamarakoto Panare Pemon Bakairi Makushi Taurepan Makiritare Carib Cumanagoto Hianacoto-Umaua Chaima Oayana Macoita

.051 -.176 .879 -.347 1.066 -.159 -.333 .308 -.390 .102 -1.124 .150 .324 -.351

.353 -.501 -.668 -.514 -.059 -.455 -.457 -.101 .324 -.324 .314 -.214 .610 1.691

Z .800 -.774 1.325 -.467 -.156 -.239 -.424 .233 -.128 .198 .514 .048 .103 -1.023

NOTES

1. A shorter and slightly modified Spanish version of this material appeared in Antropológica 68 (1987): 23-47. 2. The evidence for the presence of Caribs in Brazil at the beginning of the seventeenth century is contradictory. For a discussion see Hoff (1968:8-11). 3. For a fuller account of the Kari'ña retreat see Civrieux (1976). 4. It was mentioned earlier that the status of Hianacoto-Umaua, classed as extinct by Durbin (1977), is uncertain. In addition, see Loukotka (1968), Key (1979), Schindler (1977), and Castellvi and Espinosa Perez (1958). 5. Armellada (1943); Goeje (1946); Hildebrandt (1958); Hoff (1968); KochGrünberg (1908,1928); Méndez-Arocha (1959); Ruiz Blanco (1888); Steinen (1892); Simpson (1940); Tauste (1680 [1888]); Williams (1932); Yangues (1683 [1888]); Ahlbrinck (1931). 6. In order to determine whether a particular variety was significantly closer to a second than to a third, the triadic method developed by Oswalt (1973) was applied. This technique of judging relationships involves an analysis of the cognate distribution among a chosen triplet of related languages, followed by a one-tailed test of significance. For example, in considering if A is significantly closer to B than to C, one must determine first the number of cognates which A shares with B but not with C, as well as the number which A shares with C but not with B. Then the probability of getting such a split is determined at a chosen level of significance (0.100 in our case). With the aid of this procedure "probability measures" of the degree of lexical interrelationships are obtained at levels of significance which help in decision-making (Oswalt 1973:330).

90

Classification and Typological Problems

7. Multidimensional scaling plots can be rotated and/or reflected according to need or preference since they merely represent a scaling of points spaced and oriented relative to one another and without reference to any external condition or relationship (see definition, above). 8. These languages were consistently "pulled" toward the center of the distribution in all trial scalings performed to generate two- and three-dimensional configurations. 9. The Chikena speakers, located in the southeast corner of Guyana, also share this morph (cf. Loukotka 1968:205). REFERENCES

Ahlbrinck, W. 1931 Encyclopaedic der Karaiben, Verhandelingen der Koninklijke Akademie van Wetenschappen te Amsterdam, afdeeling letterkunde, n.r. Deel 27.1. Allaire, Louis 1980 "On the historicity of Carib migrations in the Lesser Antilles." American Antiquity 45.2:238-45. Anttila, Raimo 1972 An Introduction to Historical and Comparative Linguistics. New York: Macmillan. Armellada, Cesáreo de 1943 Gramática y diccionario de la lengua Pemón. Vol. 2: Diccionario. Caracas: Artes Gráficas. Black, Paul 1974 Multidimensional Scaling Applied to Linguistic Relationships. Murray Hill, N.J.: Bell Laboratories. Mimeographed. Campbell, Lyle 1973 "Distant genetic relationship and the Maya-Chipaya hypothesis." Anthropological Linguistics 15.3:113-35. Castellvi, Marcelino de and Lucas Espinosa Perez 1958 Propedéutica etnioglotológica y diccionario clasificador de las lenguas indoamericanas. Madrid: Consejo Superior de Investigaciones Científicas, Instituto "Bernardino de Sahagún." Civrieux, Marc de 1976 Los Caribes y la conquista de la Guayana Española (Etnohistoria Kari'ña). Caracas: Universidad Católica "Andrés Bello." 1980 "Los Cumanagotos y sus vecinos." In Los aborígenes de Venezuela Vol. I, Etnología antigua, ed. Walter Coppens, pp. 27-239. Caracas: Fundación La Salle. Coppens, Walter 1971 "Las relaciones comerciales de los Yekuanas del Caura-Paragua." Antropológica 30:28—59. Diebold, Richard A., Jr. i960 "Determining the centers of dispersal of language groups." International Journal of American Linguistics 26.1:1—10.

Lexical Relationships Among Cariban Varieties

91

Durbin, Marshall 1977 "A survey of the Carib language family." In Carib-speaking Indians: Culture, Society and Language, ed. Ellen B. Basso, pp. 23-38. Tucson: University of Arizona Press. Durbin, Marshall and Haydée Seijas 1972 "The phonological structure of the Western Carib languages of the Sierra de Perijá." At ti del XL Congresso Internazionale degli Americanisti (Estratto): 69-77. 1973a "Proto-Hianacoto: Guaque-Carijona-Hianacoto-Umaua." International Journal ofAmerican Linguistics 39.1122-31. 1973b "A note on Opon-Carare." Zeischriftfur ethnologie 98.2:242-45. Dyen, Isidore 1956 "Language distribution and migration theory." Language 32.4: 611-26. 1962 "The lexicostatistically determined relationship of a language group." International Journal of American Linguistics 28.3:153—61. Gama Malcher, José M. 1964 Indios. Grau de integraçâo na comunidade nacional, grupo lingüístico logalizaçâo. Rio de Janeiro: Ministerio da Agricultura, Conselho Nacional de Proteçâo aos Indios. Gilij, Felipe Salvador 1782 Ensayo de historia americana. Vol. 3. Transi. Antonio Tovar. Caracas: [1965] Biblioteca de la Academia Nacional de la Historia, vol. 73. Girard, Victor 1971 Proto-Carib Phonology. Ph.D. dissertation. University of California. Goeje, Claudius Henricus de 1909 Etudes linguistiques Caraïbes. Verhandelingen der Koninklijke Akademie van Wetenschappen te Amsterdam, afdeeling letterkunde, n.r. 10.3. 1946 Etudes linguistiques Caribes, Vol. 2. Verhandelingen der Koninklijke Akademie van Wetenschappen te Amsterdam, afdeeling letterkunde, n.r. 2. Greenberg, Joseph H. 1962 "Provisional classification of aboriginal languages of Latin America." [chart]. Encyclopaedia Britannica, Vol. 12. Chicago: Encyclopaedia Britannica Inc. Henley, Paul and Marie-Claude Mattei-Muller 1982 "Comentarios (Languages of the Orinoco-Amazon watershed: Some comments on Migliazza's classification)." Antropológica 57:79-90. Hildebrandt, Martha 1958 Lenguas indígenas de Venezuela I. Sistema fonémico del Macoita. Caracas: Ministerio de Justicia, Comisión Indigenista. Hoenigswald, Henry M. i960 Language Change and Linguistic Reconstruction. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

92

Classification and Typological Problems

H o f f , B. J. 1968

The Carib Language. Verhandelingen van het Koninkiijk Instituut voorTaal-, Land- en Volkenkunde 55. The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff. Hymes, D. H. i960 "Lexicostatistics so far." Current Anthropology 1 . 1 : 3 - 4 4 . Key, Mary Ritchie 1979 The Grouping of South American Indian Languages. Ars Lingüistica, 2. Tübingen: Gunter Narr. Kirk, Jerome and P. J. Epling 1973 "Taxonomy of the Polynesian languages." Anthropological Linguistics 15.1:42-70. Koch-Grünberg, Theodor 1908 "Die Hianákoto-Umáua." Anthropos 3 : 1 - 1 1 2 . 1928 Vom Roroima zum Orinoko, Vol. IV. Stuttgart. Landar, Herbert 1968 "The Karankawa invasion of Texas." International Journal of American Linguistics 34.4:242—58. Laiming, Edward 1974 "Eastern South America." In Prehispanic America, ed. S. Gorenstein, pp. 87-109. New York: St. Martin's Press. Lathrap, Donald W. 1970 The Upper Amazon. New York: Praeger. 1973 "The antiquity and importance of long-distance trade relationships in the moist tropics of pre-Columbian South America." World Archaeology 5:170-86. Layrisse, Miguel, and Johannes Wilbert 1966 Indian Societies of Venezuela: Their Blood Group Types. Caracas: Fundación La Salle. Loukotka, Cestmir 1968 Classification of South American Indian Languages, ed. Johannes Wilbert. Latin American Center Reference Series, vol. 7. Los Angeles: University of California. McQuown, Norman A. 1955 "The indigenous languages of Latin America." American Anthropologist 57-3(June): 501-70. Mason, John Alden 1963 "The languages of South American Indians." In Handbook of South American Indians, ed. Julian H. Steward, vol. 6, pp. 157-317. New York: Cooper Square. Méndez-Arocha, Alberto 1959 "Yabarana con apuntes fonémicos." Antropológica 7 : 6 3 - 8 4 . Migliazza, Ernest C. 1980 "Languages of the Orinoco-Amazon Basin: Current status." Antropológica 53:95—162. Morales, Filadelfo and Nelly Arvelo-Jiménez 1981 "Hacia un modelo de estructura social Caribe." América Indígena 41.4:603-26.

Lcxical Relationships Among Cariban Varieties

93

Morey, Nancy K. C. 1975 Ethnohistory of the Colombian and Venezuelan Llanos. Ph.D. dissertation. University of Utah. Oswalt, Robert L. 1973 "A triadic method of judging linguistic relationships." Anthropological Linguistics 15.7:328-36. Rochefort, Charles de 1681 Histoire morale des iles Antilles de l'Amerique. Rotterdam. Rodrigues, Aryon Dall'Igna 1974 "Linguistic groups of Amazonia." In Native South America, ed. Patricia J. Lyon, pp. 51-58. Boston: Little, Brown. Rowe, John Howland 1974a "Linguistic classification problems in South America." In Native South America, ed. Patricia J. Lyon, pp. 43-50. Boston: Litde, Brown. 1974b "Indian Tribes of South America" [Map]. In Native South America, ed. Patricia J. Lyon. Boston: Little, Brown. Ruiz Blanco, Matias 1888 Arte y tesoro de la lengua cumanagota [Algunas obras raras sobre la lengua Cumanagota publicadas de nuevo por Julio Platzmann, Vol. III]. Leipzig: B.G. Teubner. Schindler, Helmut 1977 "Carijona and Manakini: An opposition in the mythology of a Carib tribe." In Carib-speaking Indians: Culture, Society and Language, ed. Ellen B. Basso, pp. 66-75. Tucson: University of Arizona Press. Schuller, Rodolfo R. 1919-1920 "Zur sprachlichen Verwandtschaft der Maya-Qu'itse mit der Carib-Aruäc." Anthropos 14—15:465-91. Simpson, George Gaylord 1940 "Los Indios Kamarakotos." Revista de Fomento 3.22-25:201—660. Steinen, Karl von den 1892 Die Bakairi-Sprache. Leipzig: K.F. Koehler's Antiquarium. Steward, Julian H. and Louis C. Faron 1959 Native Peoples of South America. New York: McGraw Hill. Sued-Badillo, Jalil 1978 Los Caribes: Realidad 0 fäbula. Rio Piedras (Puerto Rico): Editorial Antillana. Tauste, Francisco de 1680 Arte bocabulario doctrina Christiana y catecismo de la lengua de Cumana [1888] [Algunas obras raras sobre la lengua Cumanagota publicadas de nuevo por Julio Platzmann, Vol. I], Leipzig: B.G. Teubner. Taylor, Douglas 1954 "Diachronic note on the Carib contribution to Island Carib." International Journal of American Linguistics 20.1:28—33. Tovar, Antonio 1961 Catdlogo de las lenguas de America del Sur. Buenos Aires: Editorial Sudamericana.

94

Classification and Typological Problems

Vansina, Jan 1973

Oral Tradition. A Study in Historical Methodology. London: Penguin Books. Williams, James 1932 "Grammatical notes and vocabulary of the language of the Makuchi Indians of Guiana." Anthropos 8. Yangues, Manuel de 1683 Principios y reglas de la lengua Cumanagota [Algunas obras raras sobre [1888] la lengua Cumanagota publicadas de nuevo por Julio Platzmann, Vol. IIJ. Leipzig: B.G. Teubner.

in Comparative Linguistics

Ana Fernández Garay The Phonology of Ranquel and Phonological Comparisons with Other Mapuche Dialects

ABSTRACT

This chapter describes the phonemes of Ranquel, a Mapuche dialect, and compares the system with other Mapuche dialects spoken in Argentina and Chile. Ranquel was spoken some years ago in the province of La Pampa, Argentina. Nowadays there remain some speakers, but they seldom use their mother tongue. The fricative series of Ranquel is compared with other Mapuche varieties spoken in Argentina.1 The labial and dental fricatives /v, 5/ are generally voiced consonants, in contrast to the other dialects wherein voiceless fricatives prevail. Finally, the chapter shows the relationship between Ranquel and Picunche, the northern Mapuche dialect of Chile.

Introduction2 This chapter was prepared using data collected during fieldwork conducted on October 17—27, 1983, in the province of La Pampa, Argentina. 3 During this period, the following places were visited: Santa Rosa (the capital of La Pampa); Victorica (in the department of Loventue, 150 km from Santa Rosa); Telen (a rural settlement 12 km from Victorica); Santa Isabel (a rural town in the department of Chalileo, in the northwest of the province, 312 km from Santa Rosa); Colonia Emilio Mitre (in the department of Chalileo, 18 km from Santa Isabel and 294 km from Santa Rosa). We concentrated our work in Colonia Emilio Mitre, for it is an exclusively indigenous settlement and the only place where we could find a Ranquel group. In 1983 the colonia had approximately 120 inhabitants, but the population varies frequently because of the periodic or continual migrations of the people who abandon the reservation to look for work. Colonia Emilio Mitre is about 400 km from Chile, at about the latitude of Chilian,

98

Comparative Linguistics

Chile, which coincides with the northernmost area of Mapuche speakers in Chile. Ethnographical information on the Ranquel is given in Fernández Garay (1988).

Background of the Studies The names of the Ranqueles (they call themselves Ranquelinos) come from the words rayktl 'reed-grass' and ce 'people'. They were known from the earliest times from the book written by Lucio V. Mansilla in 1870: Una excursión a los indios ranqueles. He called them an Araucanian group: tribus de indios araucanos que, habiendo emigrado en distintas épocas de la falda occidental de la cordillera de los Andes a la oriental, y pasado los ríos Negro y Colorado, han venido a establecerse entre el río Quinto y el río Colorado, al naciente del río Chalileo. (Mansilla i87o[i966] :66)

According to Rodolfo Casamiquela, the Ranqueles were the transition group of the west area of the Querandies, "Araucanized" by the Mapuche tribes from the north of Neuquén, that is, the Pehuenche group. The old Querandi language, in Casamiquela's view, should be related with the Gününa-küne, that is, the northern austral Tehuelche (Casamiquela 1982:21-22). Cooper classified the Ranqueles as belonging to the Mapuche, who were called Araucanians by the Spanish conquerors; the name comes from Arauco (mi 'clay' and ko 'water'), a region in central Chile. The same position was taken by the missionary Sánchez Labrador, who worked in Pampa and Patagonia about the middle of the eighteenth century. The few Ranqueles who survive nowadays are found in the Colonia Emilio Mitre, in the Pampean desert. According to the data collected in situ, this group could have come from La Blanca, near Luán Toro, in the northeastern area of the province, where they would have arrived from Villa Sarmiento or Sarmiento Nuevo, in Río Cuarto, Córdoba, Argentina. Although it is not the goal of this chapter to search for the mysterious origin of the group, the linguistic analysis could bring forth some evidence about this interesting subject through the phonological comparison of Ranquel with other Mapuche dialects. Ranquel has not been well documented up to the present. We only have the "Vocabulario Rankelce" by A. V. Frich, compiled by Loukotka in "Contribuciones a la lingüística sudamericana" (1929). Frich's vocabulary was collected in General Acha, La Pampa, far from the Ranquel area,

Ranquel and Other Mapuche Dialects

99

and it is quite different phonetically from what I have observed. Today, Ranquel is in an advanced state of extinction, as it is being replaced by Spanish. The present study is based on the corpus obtained in Colonia Emilio Mitre from bilingual speakers: Juana Cabral de Carripilon (75 years old); Veneranda Cabral de Cabral (77 years old); Marcelina Baigorrita (nearly 100 years old) and Juana Carripi de Lima (75 years old). They are considered to be the last Ranquel-speakers of the community. A basic vocabulary of 200 to 600 words and 200 phrases was elicited from the informants, and this forms the corpus for this work.

Phonological System of Ranquel The inventory of Ranquel phonemes is as follows: Consonants

Labial

Stop and affricate Fricative Lateral Nasal Semivowel

Vowels High Low

Dental Alveolar Retroflex Palatal

P v

r

t Ö

s 1 n

m w

t r

Velar

c

k

\ ñ

0

y

Front

Central

Back

i e

i a

u 0

Description of the phonemes: /p/ is articulated as a bilabial, oral, voiceless stop. It/ is articulated as a postdental, oral, voiceless stop. It'/ is articulated as a retroflex, oral, voiceless affricate. In the idiolect of Veneranda Cabral, it alternates with an apico-alveolar, voiceless stop: [ r ] -[%]. IcI

Ik/

is articulated as a palatal, voiceless affricate. In Juana Cabral and Marcelina Baigorrita's speech, it alternates with an apico-alveolar, voiceless stop: [c] ~ [t]. This [t] is a stylistic variation, of affective nature. is articulated as a velar, oral, voiceless stop when it precedes central and back vowels. Contiguous to front vowels, it advances its point of articulation closer to the palatal region.

ioo

Comparative Linguistics

/v/

is articulated as a labiodental, oral fricative, with voiced and voiceless realizations: [v] ~ [f]. The voiced one prevails.

16/

is articulated as an interdental, oral fricative, with voiced and voiceless realizations: [5] ~ [0]. The voiced one prevails.

Is/

is articulated as a predorso-alveolar, oral, grooved, voiceless fricative. Due to its low frequency and to its appearance in loanwords from Spanish, we can assume that this sibilant is a foreign sound lately incorporated into Ranquel because o f continuous contact with the Spanish language. The oldest studies o f Mapuche do not include /s/ in their descriptions o f the pronunciation o f its sounds. Valdivia (1606) says "Finalmente en esta lengua raras vezes se hallan estas sylabas: ga, gue, gui, go, gu, al modo que nosotros las pronunciamos. Ni ga, qc, (ji, [s] / _ $

The syllable-final [s] : [s] distinction has been eliminated entirely in suffixes and stem-finally; north of Cuzco and east of the Vilcanota River, it has also been eliminated in stems (sound change iii). The palatal fricative has a regular aspiration reflex by sound change (ii) in the following stop if the following stop is the first one in the word.8

[STOP] > [ASPIRATE] / Palatal stops o r fricatives induce aspiration o n the following stop, (in) s > s/

$

For examples of (i), (ii), (iii), see Table 2. Notice that in the third example pisqa ~ pisqa does not have an aspirated q because it is not the first stop in the word.

TABLE 2

source

changes

(i)-(iii)

acka

ask h a ~ ask h a

'many'

ucpa

usp h a ~ usp h a

'ash'

picqa

pisqa ~ pisqa

'five'

122

Comparative Linguistics

The fourth change in this group occurs in a rare environment. Jointly with (i) and (iii), it accounts for (as far as I know), two forms, the stem incik > incis 'peanut' and the inclusive plural -cik > -cis. Though the output of (iv) appears to have created new contexts for (i) and (iii) to apply, there are no attested intermediate forms. (iv) k > c /i Juan de Aguilar's Arte de la lengua quichua (1691 [1939]) showed (i) and (iii) to apply simultaneously in suffixes. Thus the enclitic -cuc 'perhaps' also appeared as -cus, but never as *-cus. They must nonetheless be formulated as separate changes in order to account for differential modern dialectal spread. Durative -cka also appears as -sa. But Aguilar lacked (iv) so there is no corresponding -cis inclusive attested. Centeno's auto sacramental, Elpobre mas rico (n.d.) shows the palatal lenition rule jointly with the s > s merger having gone to completion except for the postvocalic alternant of dubitative enclitic -ca, which regularly appears as -c, always in word-final position. The alternation between a postconsonantal CV form and a post-vocalic V form—which was paralleled by -mi —» -m / V for the witness marker of scope of affirmation and -si -s / V for the reportive—allowed the postvocalic -c alternant to be reconstituted by the speaker. In the -cu + c sequence, in which context it never alternated, -c (< -ca) followed the normal course of change to s. The resulting -cus was reanalyzed as a single morpheme. Ultimately, the alternation was dropped, thereby avoiding homophony with the reportive -s in the more frequent, postvocalic environment. The enclitic -ca was reassigned from the class of alternative enclitics (-mi, -si) to the nonalternating set (negative/yes-no question -cu, emphatic -ya, and so forth). Regularization of -ca is attested in the Sahuaraura codex of Usca Paucar. The -cis form of the inclusive plural ( < -cik by [iv], [i], and [ii]) appears in Elpobre mas rico in wordfinal position only, that is, just the condition under which palatalization of k is regular. Word-medially it appears as -cik. In the Sahuaraura Usca Paucar manuscript the alternation was leveled to -cis. The aspiration reflex of the syllable-final palatals is first attested in an unequivocal form in Usca Paucar. By this time it was complete. Syllable-final t did not occur in the suffix system. In stems it has regularly lenited to s ([v]; see examples in Table 3)9 except idiosyncratically in a single place name, Ccatca [q^aGqa] ~ [q'asqa], for which the 0-form appears to be a spelling pronunciation introduced by Spanish speakers.

Southern Peruvian Quechua Consonant Lenition

123

TABLE 3

source

t> s

mutkimitkautqay

muskhimisk'ausqhay

'to smell' 'to trip' 'quickly'

Lenition of syllabic-final t did not become regular in stems until the nineteenth century at the earliest. (V)

t > s/

$

Upon weakening, the syllable-final [grave] stops have gradually been converging upon [x] • The modern-day synchronic reflex of these changes is a rule that realizes all non-strident occlusives as f x ] ; it is widespread in the Department of Cuzco. Sound changes (vi), (vii), and (viii) represent three stages in the lenition and uvularization of syllable-final p. Stage (viii) has regularized in suffixes; all three are dialectally and lexically attested in stems in present-day Cuzco. (vi) p > /

$

(vii) ap > awx / — $ (viii)

4>>x/-$ Stages (vi) and (vii) are in competition in the more conservative dialects. Stage (viii) has been reached in suffixes and is currently diffusing across the lexicon, where its progress slows to avoid homonymy. For example, one of the most resistant words is m [ + CONTINUANT] / _

$

Grave segments—labials, velars, and uvulars—became spirants at the ends of syllables

Nasals Assimilation of syllable-final nasals to the following stop is attested variably in stems throughout the textual sample. Assimilation of n —» m / p, is widespread in El pobre mas rico and likely led to reanalysis of other occurrences of syllable-final m that were not involved in an alternation as underlying Inl. At the time of the Usca Paucar manuscript, the postvocalic

Southern Peruvian Quechua Consonant Lenition

125

TABLE 4

source

(ix)

x >

(vit)

(viii)

-q

-x







-pti

*-0ti





-Xti

maqc'iy" wakca4 *hapq , iy q'apnusqa' rapra* wak

maxc'iy waxca haoq'iy q?a0nusqa ra0ra wax















ha-xq'iy q?a"xnusqa





haxq'iy q'axnusqa raxra

wax







'agentive nominalizer' 'subordinating suffix' 'to wash' 'orphan' 'dig up' 'dented' 'wing' 'several'

"Gonçâlez Holguin (1608:223), macchhini ~ macchini. 'Gonçâlez Holguin (1608:167), buaccha. 'Gonçâlez Holguin (1608:135), kapnuscca.

•'Gonçâlez Holguin (1608:313), rapra as Warna de arbol 0 ramo'.

alternant of the 'scope of affirmation, witness' suffix -mi is also attested as -n. Ultimately, the nasal neutralization rule was generalized to continuant environments. This final expansion occurred by the time the Sahuaraura codex of Oliant ay was written.

r-Spirantization Spirantization of tap Irl began word-finally; this is a regular stage in the lenition process. The rule was generalized to "adjacent to a wordboundary," perhaps under the influence of a parallel rule affecting Spanish /r/, which in much of the Americas, including highland Peru, also undergoes spirantization. The /r/ is currently weakening in preconsonantal position as well. The history of the Irl lenition (which has a reflex in the synchronic grammar) demonstrates the dialectical nature of the evolution of grammatical rules: like the other spirantization processes, it appears to have begun at the ends of words and then of stems. The structural description of the rule was generalized from "before" to "adjacent to" word and stem boundaries under the influence of a formally identical rule in language contact. This obscured the relationship between the form of the rule and its concomitant function. Further generalization of the rule to "before

126

Comparative Linguistics

a syllable boundary"—which was the functionally consistent natural next step for the rule to take—became quite complicated from a formal point of view. With time, however, the functional motivation is coming to override it even at the cost of considerable formal complication.

Simplication of the Vowel-Glide Sequences Three processes have contributed to simplication of vowel-glide sequences: (i) Back glides have fronted at the ends of syllables, following the regular pattern of change from derivational morphology to stems and from the ends of words and stems to stem-medially; (2) /ay/ diphthongs have coalesced to high vowels; (3) /uy/ diphthongs have simplified to [u]. The first of the changes is widespread, although still variable within stems, while the second and third are both changes in progress. (x) w > y/

$

(XI)

ay > e: > i /

$ except after a uvular

(xii) uy > u / _ # The shift of w to y is attested in the derivational suffix -wsi- 'to assist to', the only suffix in which it occurs, in the Sahuaraura manuscript Ollantay. The Justiniani manuscript, on the other hand, shows the change stemfinally as well. It is now complete in suffixes and stem-finally and lexically and dialectally variable stem-medially, as in Table 5. Cuzco-Collao vowels are raised phonetically before a high, front glide, y; for /a/, this is £ —» e. Phonetic raising, like many coarticulatory effects, is normally attributed to the segment that induces the effect, in this

TABLE 5

source

w> y

-wsip?uncaw wawqi

-ysip'uncay wayqi

'to assist to' 'daylight' 'brother of a male'

Southern Peruvian Quechua Consonant Lenition

127

case the y. But many speakers are no longer attributing the effect to the adjacent y, and are treating the raised e as basic and unmodified. In turn, the diphthong is coalescing (monophthongizing) around e. Coalescence does not appear to be taking place in the suffix system. Moreover, in the sequence -sqayki 'I to you, future', the uvular prevents raising from taking place. The sequence is currently in the process of dropping the uvular, but even without the uvular, raising and coalescence do not take place. Rather, there is a competing change in which the y is lost regardless of whether the uvular is then dropped. This change, which is present only in suffixes, also affects the second person nominal form, -yki. But there is no evidence that the second person nominal form is being leveled to *-ki.

Conditions on Sound Change The lenitions and mergers of consonants at the ends of syllables are clearly related to one another, although that relationship is not a formal one. From the standpoint of the structural form of the processes, they reflect separate innovations, that is to say, separate historical events. The textual evidence bears this out. Although many of the correspondences arc quite systematic, and can be collapsed into a very few, very general correspondence statements—for example, that all stops at the ends of syllables correspond to spirants in the modern Cuzco-Collao dialect—the textual record shows that the changes took place piecemeal. Apart from the possibility of (wrongly) collapsing these changes into more general correspondence statements, at a formal level they appear to be unrelated one to another. But the overall organization and sequence of these changes suggests that there is an underlying functional unity to them. They occur in an orderly, implicational sequence. Within the stops, the order of the weakenings is predictable from their acoustic characteristics. The stops which concentrate acoustic energy higher in the spectrum and have a smaller overall energy packet weaken first. Those stops which concentrate acoustic energy lower in the spectrum weaken later. The lenitions have also left a strong mark on the synchronic phonology of the language in the form of a clear, unifying orientation to a number of structurally discrete rules. The syllable-final weakenings also show another kind of unity, which is ignored in a tabulation of sound correspondences but emerges in a historical reconstruction that is also accountable to dialectal and philological

128

Comparative Linguistics

evidence. These changes do not take place equally in all classes of morphological formatives. Rather, they spread across the word, from morpheme class to morpheme class, and from narrower phonological environments to more inclusive ones. A reconstruction that simply tabulates regular correspondences cannot show this. The logical consequence of a reconstruction that is content with simple tabulation of correspondences is that instances of a phonological segment appear to change equally in all nonphonological linguistic contexts. Studies of sound change in progress and of lexical diffusion of phonological changes show that even if this is a good working assumption, it is an oversimplication (Wang 1969; Chen 1972; Chen and Wang 1975; Krishnamurti 1978; Labov 1981). In the case described here, we found that regular principles govern the spread of innovations from environment to environment. These principles constrain the set of possible transitions between any two grammars that are related by a single historical process. The principles bear up equally well in the textual sample (variation in time), dialectological evidence (variation in space), and internal reconstruction (variation in space/time). Here are informal statements of the principles: First, the syllable-final weakenings and mergers begin at the ends of words and word-stems and subsequently also take place word- and stem-medially. This has a straightforward phonological basis. Second, the syllable-final weakenings and mergers begin in the suffix system and spread into word stems. This seems to have a morphosyntactic motivation (cf. Mannheim and Newfield 1982). The principles may be stated as implicational hierarchies that constrain the class of possible language states relative to the output of a particular change. To put it more simply, they only permit sound changes to expand in certain ways. This restricts possible transitions between two historical or dialectal states, (xiii) 1. If a sound change takes place word-medially, then it takes place word-finally. 12 2. If a sound change takes place stem-medially, then it takes place stem-finally.

The first clause of condition (xiii) follows straightforwardly from the nature of the syllable. The changes in question occur at the ends of syllables. A phonological word must contain an integral number of syllables. The environment "word-final" is therefore a proper subset of the environment "syllable-final." Southern Peruvian Quechua does not resyllabify at word boundaries. For the syllable-final changes in Southern Peruvian Quechua, the environment "stem-final" is also a proper subset of "syllablefinal." All verbs and some substantives end in vowels, and the addition of

Southern Peruvian Quechua Consonant Lenition

129

a suffix often adds a final consonant to the last syllable in a stem. Verbs are bound forms. But nouns that act as lexical stems are free, that is, may occur without suffixes. Many of these end in a consonant. Condition (xiii) may be accounted for formally by assigning the #-boundary to stems—it is independently required by other phonotactic contraints—and the # # boundary to words. The ##-boundary also marks the domain of other phonological processes, including stress assignment. The ##-boundary is a proper subset of the $, syllable, boundary. For the second clause of condition (xiii), the lenitions and mergers could be stipulated to spread from / # to / $. (I understand boundaries such as $ and # to be a linear notational shorthand for hierarchically organized constituents.) This clause may be attributed to individual analogical leveling of suffixed stems to their nonsuffixed counterparts. Southern Peruvian Quechua has a strong tendency for morphemes to be phonologically invariable. Very few grammatical formatives and no lexical or stem formatives are involved in regular, phonologically motivated alternations. 13 The second part of the condition contributes to the preservation of stem invariance. The implicational hierarchies in (xiii) are consistent with the states summarized in rows a - d of Table 6, but not consistent with e - h . Here " + " means that the change has taken place in a particular environment and " —" that it has not (" + " includes cases in which the structural description of the change is not met in the environment). The inconsistent cases are marked with an asterisk (*). Here is an example: The shift of syllable-final w to y took place in the

TABLE 6

all syllable-final contexts stem-finally word-finally a b c d *e *f *g *h





-

-

-

+ -

+ + +

+ + + + -



+ + + -

+

I30

Comparative Linguistics

T A B L E

7

Anon,

Anon,

Anon,

Usca OllanPaucar tay

Ollantay

Gonzalez (Sahua- (Sahuaraura) Holguin raura) (X) (X) (X)

w > y (suf) w > y (stem-final) w > y (stem)

-

-

-

-





+ % —

(Justiniani)

+ + %

modern dialects

+ + %

+ + +

(suf) = in suffixes; (stem) = in stems; + = regularly attested; % = variably attested; -

= not attested

derivational suffix -wsi- > -ysi- 'to assist to V E R B ' prior to its spread into stems, where there is still dialectal variation in the modern language (see Table 7). The shift has been completed in final position in stems, as in p9uncaw > p'hmcay (invariably) 'day(light)', but not stem-medially, where the older form wawqi varies with a newer wayqi 'brother of a male'. The two Ollantay codices—which reflect similar but not identical states of the language—provide a useful comparison. In the Sahuaraura text -wsi- > -ysi- is complete, but both puncau and puncai appear for 'day(light)'. In the Justiniani manuscript it has gone to completion stem-finally. Notice that (xiii), the phonologically motivated condition on sound change, accounts for the relationship between the stem-final and stemmedial environments, but does not correctly account for the initiation of the change in suffixes. The -wsi- > -ysi- shift precedes any change in stemfinal position, even though it occurs word-medially. Were the expansion of the sound change motivated entirely on phonological grounds, we would expect rather that the stem-final change (with concomitant leveling of the variant forms to the newer one) precede the word-medial change in the suffix system. Moreover, these data are consistent with others in the textual and dialectological data sets, and with the application of synchronic phonological processes. Consider sound change (viii), in which syllablefinal labials spirantize and merge with the uvular (uvularization; see Table 8). There are three instances in which the appropriate structural descrip-

Southern Peruvian Quechua Consonant Lenition

131

TABLE 8

Anon, Anon, Usca OllanPaucar tay (Sahua- (SahuaCenteno raura) raura) (vi) p > 0 (suf) (vi) p > 0 (stem) (viii) 0 > x (suf) (viii) 0 > x (stem)

% -

% -

-

Anon, Ollantay (Justiniani)

%

+

-

%

%

+

-

-

modern dialects

+ + +

+ + + %

(suf) = in suffixes; (stem) = in stems; + = regularly attested; % = variably attested; -

= not attested

tion is met: in the postvocalic alternant of the genitive, -p-\ in the switchreference subordinator -pti-; and in numerous stems (for example, hapt9ay 'handful' or q^apnuy 'to dent'). The syllable-final bilabial merged with the uvular spirant first in the genitive, then in the switch-reference subordinator (both in the eighteenth century) and is currently shifting in stems. (The extant textual data show uvularization together with spirantization in the suffixes, but as independent of one another in stems. Dialectological evidence supports their separateness in stems.) Uvularization of the genitive is complete in the Sahuaraura codex of Ollantay, but switch-reference uvularization is variable, and there is none in stems. By the time of the Justiniani codex of Ollantay, uvularization was complete in the suffixes. At the present time, uvularization in stems is a major dialectological variable in the Department of Cuzco. The weakening of syllable-final palatals (sound change i) resulted in a /: s contrast in that position. Syllable-final / in turn conditioned aspiration on the following stop, provided that the context met other restrictions on aspiration, namely that it can only occur in stems, and on the first oral stop in a word (sound change ii). The s : s contrast has since been eliminated in suffixes by sound change (iii), in which s> s. The change has spread to stems in Cuzco and the area north and east of the Vilcanota River, where aspiration (from sound change ii) is the only remaining trace of the former contrast between palatal and nonpalatal sibilants. (The only syllable-initial

Comparative Linguistics / is in the durative suffix as a result of other changes. But in the Urubamba valley the s > s merger is in the process of being generalized to eliminate the unique syllable-initial / as well.) To the area south and west of the Vilcanota River, the merger of final sibilants (iii) is restricted to suffixes, leaving the / : s contrast intact in stems. The contrast is reinforced by syllable- initial variable palatalization, s > s, before front vowels and (in loan-words) before palatal glides. The s : s contrast is one of the principle parameters of dialectal variation in modern-day Cuzco-Collao Quechua. In the historical texts, / > i is first attested in Aguilar's Arte, where it had taken place in suffixes. It is complete in stems in Centeno and in the two dramatic poems recorded by Sahuaraura (see Table 9). The dramatic poems evidently belong to the dialect area that includes the city of Cuzco and the area to its north and east. The association of the dramatic poem Ollantay with the village of Tinta (southwest of Cuzco; Markham 1912:90, Lewin 1943), is not supported for either the Sahuaraura or the Justiniani manuscripts of Ollantay. The morpho-syntactic condition on the syllable-final changes is stated in (xiv). It is a condition on the output of changes and is not restricted to sound changes. Thus, the restructuring of preterite -rqa- > -ra-, an inflectional suffix, which is currently a dialectal variable, is a precondition for a comparable restructuring of the "urgency" (derivational) suffix, -rqu— -rqa- > -ru— -ra-.

TABLE 9

Anon, Gonzalez Holguin (iii) s > s (suf) (iii) s > s (stem) ( s u f ) = in suffixes; (stem) = in stems; +

= regularly attested;

% = variably attested; -

= not attested

Usca Anon, Anon, Paucar Ollantay Ollantay Aguilar Centeno

-

+





+ +

(Sahuaraura)

(Sahuaraura)

(Justiniani)

+ +

+ +

+ +

Southern Peruvian Quechua Consonant Lenition

133

TABLE 10

stem

derivation

inflection/ discourse

a



-



b

-

-

+

c

-

+

+

d

+

+

+

e

-

+

*f

+

+

*o

+

*h

+

(xiv) If a change takes place in stems, then it takes place in derivational formatives; if in derivational formatives, then in inflectional and discourse-level formatives, 1 4 provided that the structural description o f the change is met.

The implicational hierarchy in (xiv) is consistent with the cases summarized in rows a—d of Table 10, but not consistent with e—h. Conditions (xiii) and (xiv) are constraints on the expansion of the changes. Thus (xiii) does not stipulate that sound changes must expand from the word-final environment to the syllable-final, but rather that it can only expand consistent with the condition. Simultaneous actualization of the change throughout the word, for example, would be consistent with case d of Table 6. Nor does (xiv) stipulate that a sound change in the suffix system must precede the same change in the stem. Condition (xiii) countenances the cases listed in rows a—d of Table 6 and rules out those starred in e—h, as (xiv) does those in Table 10. But there is a violation of these conditions in the philological corpus. Centeno's drama El pobre más rico shows completed lenition of syllablefinal palatals except for the postvocalic alternant of the dubitative -ca which regularly appears as -c. Worse still, -ca — c is always the last suffix in the word. It contradicts both conditions (xiii) and (xiv) by realizing case f of Tables 6 and 10. The motivation for the retention of the -c form is clear. The postconsonantal long form of the dubitative -ca- provides a model for restoration of the stop in the postvocalic short form -c. The alternation was parallel to -mi ~ -m 'scope of affirmation, witness' and

134

Comparative Linguistics

-si ~ -s 'scope of affirmation, reported'. We must therefore weaken conditions (xiii) and (xiv) with a third condition, (xv), which allows conditions (xiii) and (xiv) to be overridden in the event that a morpheme alternation in a regular paradigm provides a model for restoring the final stop. (xv) T h e expansion o f sound change may be retarded for a formative which otherwise meets the structural description if it is involved in a phonologicallv determined alternation f r o m which the original form can be reconstituted.

Final c was restored in the -c suffix, which alternated with a longer -ca form. In the sequence -cu + c, in which it never alternated, it was not restored; the resulting -cu + s was reanalyzed as a single formative. By the time of the Sahuaraura manuscript of Usca Paucar, the -ca suffix had been reassigned from the class of alternating enclitics to the nonalternating class. It invariably appears there as -ca, just as it does in the modern language.

Motivating the Conditions The first part of condition (xiii) has a clear phonological motivation. The sound system of language is organized into a hierarchy of nested constituents. Below the level of the word there arc features nested into segments, segments nested into syllabic onset and rhyme, onset and rhyme nested into syllables, syllables nested into feet, and feet nested into words. The weakenings and mergers occur at the level of the syllabic rhyme. Rhymes are defined by their role in the organization of the syllable. In Southern Peruvian Quechua, a rhyme consists of a vocalic nucleus followed by a consonantal adjunct. The next larger constituent, the foot, plays a role in the distribution of stress in Southern Peruvian Quechua, but does not seem to be relevant at all to the weakenings and mergers. Finally, there is the word, which is composed of an integral number of syllables and feet. Both "word" and "syllable" are relevant contexts for the changes. Any sound change that takes place at the ends of syllables perforce takes place at the ends of words, but not vice versa. If $, then # (see clause i of xiii). The second clause of condition (xiii) and condition (xv) appear to be consequences of the strong orientation of Southern Peruvian Quechua to agglutinative, transparent morphology, in other words, to a condition in which every formative has one and only one surface manifestation. Southern Peruvian Quechua has few morphological alternations and very

Southern Peruvian Quechua Consonant Lenition

135

few obvious morphophonemic processes. There are exceptions, but they are relatively rare. Thus in the case of the second clause of condition (xiii), word-medial but stem-final consonants are leveled to word-final alternants. Conversely, in the case accounted for by (xv), an alternation was used to restore the older phonetic form and prevent allomorphy, at least until another mechanism for preventing allomorphy won out. Condition (xiv) is another matter; it allows for the spread of sound changes from discourse and inflectional formatives to derivational formatives to word-stems. The neogrammarian position that "sound-laws, inasmuch as they are mechanical, hold without exception," has been so productive (Hockett 1965 is correct in referring to it as one of the few "great discoveries" in linguistics) that we are often loath to accept the possibility of nonphonological conditioning of what are otherwise regular sound changes. This is not to say that all phonological changes are sound laws (lautgcsetze). The neogrammarians recognized analogy and contamination (secondary sound symbolism; sound association) as valid mechanisms of phonological change; Whitney (1875 [1971]: 106) and Meillet (1924 [1967]: 14), among others, recognized primary sound symbolism (onomatopoeia) works in other ways, and Wang, Chen, and Labov (also among others) have shown the importance of phonetically defined word classes to the phonological histories of Middle Chinese and Modern American English. But the syllable-final weakenings and mergers are garden variety "sound-laws," lautgesetze, exactly the kind of change to which the neogrammarian axiom applies. And condition (xiv) docs hold for these changes, as well as other, more properly morphological innovations. Nor is it an isolated set of "co-incidences." Cerron-Palomino (1974) observed similar conditioning of sound change in Wanka, a Central Quechua language. He suggested that (xiv) is a type of conditioning of sound change that is characteristic of the Quechua family. Inflectional lead in sound change has also been observed in Indo-European by Turner (1927, 1937; for Indie) and Malkiel (1968; for Romance); and in Uto-Aztecan by Lastra and Horcasitas (1978; for Mexicano [Nahuatl]). The conditions on phonological change provide a restrictive account of the relationships that may hold between any two successive—dialectal or historical—grammars. As such, they are testable against two kinds of data, contemporary dialectal variation and philologically attested historical systems, which is to say, diachronic variation. Together with the motivated relative chronology of the sound changes (e.g., the role of relative gravity in determining the sequence) they establish both a set of consistent (or

136

Comparative Linguistics

"lawful") variant phonological systems and a partial diachronic ordering (or relative chronology) among them. They draw attention to a significant type of covariation between form and meaning in Southern Peruvian Quechua, in which the relative domains of the synchronic reflexes of the changes reflect the internal structure of the word. And in doing so, they shed light on the nature of form/meaning covariation in language. The observations by Cerrón-Palomino, Lastra and Horcasitas, Malkiel, and Turner demonstrate that the Southern Peruvian Quechua conditions on phonological change are instances of a phenomenon of more general applicability, and demonstrate the need for a general theoretical account of these principles. If we restrict comparative reconstruction to statements of correspondences, the syllable-final lenitions and mergers appear to be a single, uniform change, of local interest—useful as a statement of correspondences between Southern Peruvian Quechua and the other languages in the family. In this chapter, I have demonstrated that it is otherwise: The changes in question took place as individual innovations over several centuries. The itinerary that the changes followed has been more variable than it first appeared, as we saw with the cases in which formal determinants delayed otherwise regular sound changes (the temporary retention of final -c) or left them incomplete (the maintenance of syllable-final

0 spraebbund. 5. González Holguín (1607,1608) devised special symbols to account for the uvulars with a Spanish-based orthography. He used both {cc} and {k} at the ends of syllables. It is unclear whether his orthographic variation can be interpreted as evidence of a variable spirantization process (in which {cc} = [q] and {k} = fx], or is simply an orthographic quirk. But is is worthy of note that orthographic {g} does not appear, and by the early seventeenth century it is the expected representation of a final [x]. 6. Two problematic exceptions are qquiñua [qiñSua] 'cinchona tree' and quinua 'quinoa (a grain)' (both citations, González Holguín 1608:309). Both have etymological syllable-final ñ prior to a labial glide. The former has since undergone metathesis of the ñw sequence, and is currently [qewña] (see Cusihuamán 1976b: 114 and Lira 1944:457). The latter has a number of currently attested variants: In addition to a form that is parallel to the first [kiwña], [kinwa] is common. (It was possibly back-borrowed from Spanish.) Two variant forms have unique epenthetic vowels (raised in the phonetic transcriptions) which do not form syllabic domains for the purposes of stress assignment: [ kiirwa | and | kiwna] (attested by Lira 1944:326 as kiwina). The first might be treated as having an underlying /rW. There is no independent evidence for this analysis, though. The epenthetic vowel in the second is clearly a reflex of the palatal property of the nasal and appears to be a variant of the [kiwña] pronunciation. I am indebted to Peter Landerman for pointing out the problem. 7. For more detailed discussion of the formal properties of these texts see Rivet and Créqui-Montfort (1951) and Mannheim (1988). 8. This is an effect of the word structure constraint that restricts ejectives and aspirates to the first stop in a word. The comparatively few occurrences of the durative immediately following a stem with no stops would presumably have been leveled out. 9. mutki- is attested by González Holguín (1608:608) as mutquini; mitkain Anonymous-Ollantay (Sahuaraura codex), 114 as mitcasccaita; and utqay in

138

Comparative Linguistics

Anonymous-Ollantay (Sahuaraura codex), 114 as utccaitan. The sources of aspiration and glottalization, and the time of their acquisition, are not known. 10. Andahuaylillas was the parish of Juan Pérez Bocanegra, who wrote a Quechua-language priest's manual based on his experiences there in the early seventeenth century. 11. In addition, the accusative case was leveled from -kta (/V ) ~ -ta (/C ) to -ta. The change appeared variably in Aguilar's Arte and was completed by the time of Centeno. 12. Compare Lastra and Horcasitas (1978) on word-final "lead" of sound change over word-medial in Nahuatl. 13. Exceptions to this generalization all involve reduplication. For example, haqay 'there (deictic)' / Ha\-hax-haqay 'over there! (as a warning)'; wic9is 'pig cry' / mc'Hciciy 'to cry sharply' (Cusihuamán 19768:217). 14. By "discourse level formative" I refer to a class of suffixes that notionally indicate the epistemic and evidential status of the utterance as well as question /negation /affirmation and their respective discourse scopes. REFERENCES

Aguilar M., Juan de 1691 Arte de la lengua quichua general de Indios del Perú. Facsimile of holo[1939] graphic manuscript. Tucumán: Instituto de Antropología, Universidad Nacional de Tucumán. Anonymous 1586 Arte y vocabulario en la lengua general del Peru llamada Quichua y en [1951] la lengua Española. Lima: Antonio Ricardo. G. Escobar R. editor. Vocabulario y phrasis en la lengua general de los Indios del Perú, llamada Quichua, Lima: Universidad Nacional de San Marcos. Anonymous—Huarochirí (late 16th, early 17th century) Runayw niscap machoncuna ñaupa pacha . . ., [1942] manuscript 3169, ff. 64R-114R, Biblioteca Nacional, Madrid. Published De priscorum huaruchiriensium origene et institutis, ed. H. Galante (transcription, Latin translation, photocopy of most of the manuscript), Madrid: Instituto Gonzalo Fernández de Oviedo. Anonymous— Usca Paucar n.d. Auto sacramental, El Patrocinio de Nuestra Señora María Santicima en Copacabana. (Sahuaraura codex), Biblioteca Nacional del Perú. Anonymous—Ollantay n.d.a Justiniani: Tragicomedia del Apu Ollantay y Cusi Coyllur. Rigores de [1938] un padre, y generosidad de un Rey. Facsimile edition, Ollantay, ed. H. Galante. Lima: Universidad Nacional de San Marcos. Anonymous—Ollantay n.d.b Sahuaraura: Comedia trágica que intitula los rigores de un Padre y ge[1938] nerosidad de un Rey. Published in Ollantay, ed. H. Galante. Lima: Universidad Nacional de San Marcos.

Southern Peruvian Quechua Consonant Lenition

139

Centeno, Gabriel n.d. El pobre más rico, ed. José M.B. Farfán and Humberto Suárez Alvarez. Lima: (1939) including a facsimile of the manuscript. Cerrón-Palomino, Rodolfo 1974 "Morphologically conditioned changes in Wanka-Quechua." Studies in the Linguistic Sciences 42:40—75. Chen, Matthew Y. 1972 "The time dimension: Contribution toward a theory of sound change." Foundations ofLanguage 8:457—98. Chen, Matthew Y. and William S.-Y. Wang 1975 "Sound change: actuation and implementation." Language 51: 255-81. Cusihuamán Gutiérrez, Antonio 1976a Gramática Quechua: Cuzco-Collao. Lima: Instituto de Estudios Peruanos. 1976b Diccionario Quechua: Cuzco-Collao. Lima: Instituto de Estudios Peruanos. González Holguín, Diego de 1607 Gramática y arte nueva de la lengua general de todo el Perú llamada lenguaQquichua o delinca. Los Reyes: del Canto. 1608 Vocabulario de la lengua general de todo el Perú llamada lengua Qquichua o delinca. Los Reyes: del Canto. Guarnan Poma de Ayala, Felipe c. 1615 El primer nveva coránica i bvégobierno. Facsimile edition, 1936. Paris: Institut d'Ethnologie. Hockett, Charles F. 1965 "Sound change." Language 41:185-204. Hymes, Dell 1973 "Speech and language: On the origins and foundations of inequality among speakers." Daedelus 102. 3:59—86. Krishnamurti, Bh. 1978 "Areal and lexical diffusion of sound change." Language 54:1-20. Labov, William A. 1981 "Resolving the neo-grammarian controversy." Language 57:267-308. Landerman, Peter 1983 "Las sibilantes castellanas, quechua y aimaras en el siglo xvi: Un enigma tridimensional. In Aula quechua, ed. Rodolfo Cerrón-Palomino, pp. 203-34. Lima: Ediciones Signo. Lastra de Suárez, Yolanda and Fernando Horcasitas 1978 "El Náhuatl en el norte y el occidente del Estado de México." Anales de Antropología 15:185—250. Lewin, Boleslow 1943 La rebelión de Túpac Amaru, Third edition, 1967. Buenos Aires: Sociedad Editora Latino Americana. Lira, Jorge A. 1944 Diccionario Kkéchuwa-Español. Tucumán (Argentina): Universidad Nacional de Tucumán.

I40

Comparative Linguistics

Malkiel, Yakov 1968 "The inflectional paradigm as an occasional determinant of sound change." ed. Winfred P. Lehmann and Yakov Malkiel, pp. 21-64. In Directions for Historical Linguistics. Austin: University of Texas Press. Mannheim, Bruce 1982 "Person, number and inclusivity in two Andean languages." Acta Lingüistica Hafniensia 17:138-54. 1984 '"Una nación acorralada': Southern Peruvian Quechua language planning and politics in historical perspective." Language in Society 13.3:291-309. 1985a "Southern Peruvian Quechua." In South American Indian Languages, Retrospect and Prospect, ed. Harriet E. Manelis Klein and Louisa R. Stark, pp. 481—515. Austin: University of Texas Press. 1985b "Contact and Quechua-external genetic relationships." In South American Indian Languages, Retrospect and Prospect, ed. Harriet E. Manelis Klein and Louisa R. Stark, pp. 644—88. 1988 "On the sibilants of Colonial Southern Peruvian Quechua." International Journal ofAmerican Linguistics 54.2:168—208. Mannheim, Bruce and Madeleine Newfield 1982 "Iconicity in phonological change." In Papers from the Fifth International Conference of Historical Linguistics, ed. Anders Ahlqvist, pp. 2H-22. Amsterdam: John Benjamins B.V. Markham, Clements 1912 The Incas ofPeru. London: Triibner. Meillet, Antoine 1924 La méthotk comparative en linguistique historique. Oslo: Aschehoug. [1967] Translated by G. B. Ford as The Comparative Method in Historical Linguistics. Paris: Champion. Molina, Cristóbal de c. 1575 Relación de lasfabulasy ritos de los Tngas. . . . Biblioteca Nacional de Madrid, Manuscript 3169, ff. 2-36. Reprinted in Carlos A. Romero and Horacio Urteaga (1916), Colección de libros y documentos referentes a la historia del Perú, (series 1, volume 1). Lima. Parker, Gary J. 1963 "La clasificación genética de los dialectos quechuas." Revista del Museo Nacional 32:241-52. 1969—71 "Comparative Quechua phonology and grammar." University of Hawaii Working Papers in Linguistics I: 1.1:65-88; II: 1.2:123-47; III: 1.4:1-61; IV: 1.9:149-204; V: 3.3:45-109. 1969b Ayacucho Quechua Grammar and Dictionary. The Hague: Mouton. Parker, Gary J., and A. Chávez Reyes 1976 Diccionario Quechua: Ancash-Huailas. Lima: Ministerio de Educación and Instituto de Estudios Peruanos. Pérez [de] Bocanegra, Juan de 1631 Ritual formulario e institución de Curas para administrar a los naturales de este Reyno los Santos Sacramentos . . . por el Bachiller J. P. B., presbiterio, en la lengua Quechua general. Lima.

Southern Peruvian Quechua Consonant Lenition

141

Rivet, Paul and Georges de Créqui-Montfort 1951 Bibliographie des langues aymará et kicua (vol. 1). Paris: Institut d'Ethnologie. Rowe, John Howland 1950 "Sound patterns in three Inca dialects." International Journal of American Linguistics 16:137-48. Santa Cruz Pachacuti Yamqui, Juan de c. 1615 Relación de antigüedades deste reyno del Pint, manuscript 3169, ff. 132169, Biblioteca Nacional, Madrid. Santo Tomás, Domingo de 1560a Grammatica 0 arte de la lengua generai de los indios de los reynos del [1951] Peru. Valladolid: Fernández de Cordova. Facsimile edition. Lima: Universidad Nacional de San Marcos. 1560b Lexicon o vocabulario de la lengua general. Valladolid: Fernández de [1951] Cordova. Facsimile edition. Lima: Universidad Nacional de San Marcos. Soto Ruiz, Clodoaldo 1976 Diccionario quechua: Ayacucho-Chanca. Lima: Instituto de Estudios Peruanos. Stark, Louisa R. 1975 "A reconsideration of proto-Quechua phonology." Actas del 39 Congreso Internacional de Americanistas 5:209-19. Lima. Tercer Concilio Límense 1584 Doctrina Christiana y catecismo para instrucción de los Indios, y las demás personas que han de ser enseñados en nuestra sancta Fé. Lima: Antonio Ricardo. 1585 Tercer catecismo y exposición de la doctrina Christiana por sermones . . . [1773] Conforme a lo que se proveyó en el Sancto Concilio Provincial de Lima el año pasado de IS83. Lima: Antonio Ricardo. Torero Fernández de Cordova, Alfredo 1964 "Los dialectos quechuas." Anales Científicos de la Universidad Agraria 2:446-78. Trubetzkoy, N. S. 1939 "Gedanken über das Indogermanenproblem." Acta Linguistica Hafniensia 1:81—89. Turner, R. L. 1927 "The phonetic weakness of terminational elements in Indo-Aryan." Journal of the Royal Asiatic Society 2:227-39. 1937 "Anticipation of normal sound changes in Indo-Aryan." Transactions of the Philological Society: 1—14. Wang, William S.-Y. 1969 "Competing changes as a cause of residue." Language 45:9-25. Whitney, William Dwight 1875 "vo-ei or Beerei—Natural of conventional?" Transactions of the [1971] American Philological Association for 1874: 95-116. Reprinted in Whitney on Language, ed. Michael Silverstein, pp. 111-323. Cambridge: MIT Press.

TV Grammatical Matters

Stephen H. Levinsohn Variations in Tense-Aspect Markers Among Inga (Quechuan) Dialects

ABSTRACT Inga storytellers typically employ two or three "past" tense-aspect markers in their narratives to distinguish between foregrounded and types of backgrounded information. The functions of the individual markers van' from dialect to dialect. Correspondences in the functions of two of the markers (rca and morphological 0/n) enable the dialects to be divided into two groups, confirming Inga traditions of at least two distinct migrations into southwest Colombia. Variations in the function of the third marker (sea) may suggest a subdivision of one of the groups. 1

Introduction This chapter concerns variations in the use of the Inga tense-aspect markers rca, sea, and morphological 0/n in the three mountain dialects of Aponte (Department of Naririo, S.W. Colombia), Santiago (Putumayo), including Colon, and San Andres (Putumayo), and in the lowland dialects of Puerto Guayuyaco and of Yunguillo (including Condagua). (See Figure 1.) The next section describes patterns in the way rca and &/n are used, which indicate that the dialects fall into two groups in line with oral traditions concerning migrations of Ingas: Aponte-Santiago and San Andres—Lowland Inga. Variations in the function of sea (shca/shka in other Quechuan dialects) between Puerto Guayuyaco and the remaining communities, identified in the final section, may suggest a further subgrouping of the dialects. The phonemes of Inga are: 2 vowels: /a i u/ consonants: / b c c d g h k l l > m n n p r s s t w y / . (In Yunguillo, the /!>'/ and /y/ phonemes are combined as /y/.)

x

Centres of principal jungle inga communities

o

Centres of principal mountain Inga communities

t

Principal towns

—— Roads

Figure i. Inga (Quechuan) speaking area of Colombia.

0 l

i

20km I

Tcnse-Aspect Markers in Inga (Quechua) Dialects

147

The above set of phonemes closely parallels that given by Orr and Longacre for their representative Ecuadorian Quichua (EcQ) dialects " Q (Quito) and T (Tena)." In particular, Inga manifests with E c Q "certain unique shared innovations . . . : (1) Voiced allophones of stops and affricates, which occur in Proto-Quechua only after nasals, have become phonemic. (2) Velar and postvelar stops have merged." The authors, who refer to Inga as " P (Putumayo, Colombia)," were not aware that b, d, g (and s) are distinct phonemes, not only in EcQ, but also in Inga (Orr and Longacre 1968:530,531,542). Inga oral traditions have the Santiago people arriving in the region by the time of the Spanish conquest in the early sixteenth century and later establishing an outpost in Aponte, several hours' walk across the mountains, by 1700. The San Andres Ingas, who live only two miles from Santiago, are reputed to have come from the lowland region to the south of Mocoa after the Santiago people were well established (Levinsohn and Avendano 1982a: 1—31).3 The Ingas themselves often describe their dialectical differences in terms of the pronunciation of Itl. For example, the Santiago Ingas often wish to write rolled [r] as rr, to distinguish their pronunciation from that of the San Andres Ingas. The latter dialect uses a retroflexed flap with a voiced sibilant on- or off-glide [ • f r - ]. When preceding a voiceless consonant, however, it is often manifested as a voiceless retroflexed alveopalatal stop [c]; see Levinsohn (1976:24f). In turn, speakers from Aponte and the lowlands use a simple flap except that, preceding a voiceless consonant, Aponte speakers employ [s]. Dialectical differences appear in what Muysken (1977:43) calls the "past" conjugation of the verb, the singular of which is as follows (employing the root ni, 'say'):

Person

Aponte

Santiago

San AndrésI Lowlands

Meaning

1 2 3 1 to 2

ni-sha-ni ni-sha-ngui ni-sh(a) ni-sha-iqui

ni-rca-ni ni-rca-ni ni-rsha-ngui ni-rca ni-rca ni-rsha-iqui

ni-rca-ni ni-rca-ngui ni-rca m-rca-iqui

'I said' 'you said' 'he/she said' 'I said to you'

In other words, the "past" marker most commonly has the form rca; however, it is rsha in Santiago when the second person is involved in the ac-

148

Grammatical Matters

tion, and sha in Aponte, the final vowel commonly being dropped before silence. Ingas can also produce a list of words that vary according to the dialect. The following table gives a few of these. (It is common when two forms exist, as in the last item, for the one used in Aponte or in the lowland region to differ from that of the remaining dialects.)

Meaning

Aponte

Santiago

'still' '100' 'walking stick' 'pass' 'bend d o w n ' 'ear' 'Let's g o ! ' 'hole' 'become angry with cach other'

chara patsa tahuina llallicumuriringri acuichi utcu huarbuñi-

chara patsa tahuina valicumuririnri acushi utcu piñachiri-

San Andrés

Yunguillo

Guayuyaco

chaira pátsag taugna vallipageharirinri acuichi utcu piñachiri-

chira pásag tanua vavipagehariringri acuichi/acú juteu piñachiri-

chira pásag tanua vallicumuriringri acuichi/acú juteu piñachiri-

A number of authors have observed that, in Quechua folktales and other stories in which the narrator was not a witness, two if not three basic tense-aspect markers are employed with independent verbs. Muysken, for example, states (1977:43) that the tenses of E c Q are "present or unmarked 0; past or preterite rka; sudden discovery shka." He later comments (p. 60): "In E c Q it [/-shka/] occurs typically in legends, stories, etc., as the suffix used in the first few sentences of narratives, at various intermediate points, and at the end. Often the actions in the story are marked with the /-rka/ past or are unmarked for tense." Ross (1963:79) makes a similar observation: "Some districts favor begining a story with the -shea form for several verbs, then switching over to the present, or the past with -rai." It is significant that Ross speaks of "some districts," because E c Q dialects display considerable variation in the employment of these markers. Chimborazo texts published by Guacho and Burns (1975:27), for example, employ rka ("pretérito narrativo") for the body of the narrative, with shka ("pretérito descriptivo") reserved mainly for the opening sentences. In Cañar texts collected by Howard-Malvcrde (1981:312), the speaker " J M T " from Quinoa Pota consistently uses shka for the body of the narrative. Muysken (1977:60) is faced with the same problem, for, after quoting

Tense-Aspect Markers in Inga (Quechua) Dialects

149

various definitions of the function of shka, he concludes, "The resulting definitions turn out to be contradictory and implausible." An examination of Inga dialects displays a similar phenomenon. The three basic tense-aspect markers discussed by Muysken (see above) are found in all the dialects. However, the functions of rca and the "present or unmarked" morphological 0/n form in the dialects of Aponte and Santiago arc reversed in the dialects of San Andrés and Yunguillo (see discussion below). In all these dialects sea is employed only as an indicator of "sudden discovery," whereas in Guayuyaco it is also used frequently in connection with the events of the narrative. rca and Morphological 0/n in the Mountain Inga Dialects and Yunguillo In his article on aspect and foregrounding, Hopper (i979:2i3ff.) discusses evidence for a distinction between background information and the main events or foreground of a narrative, based on the distribution of independent verb forms in various languages (see also Givón i984:287ff.). Different authors have recognised this distinction as underlying the distribution of the Quechuan tense-aspect markers in narrative material. Thus, Gaucho and Burns (1975:27) talk of a descriptive versus a narrative preterite. Larsen (1974:424) recognises that, in Ancash Quechua, the full form of the verb ending -naq, which characterises legendary narrative, is used in connection with background information. Weber (1983:108) observes that the narrative past marker in Huanuco Quechua is used "to mark these events as of less importance to the text (i.e., push them into background) relative to what follows." Zahn (n.d.: 1) explains the distribution of -rka and -shka in Pastaza Quechua in terms of "background information" and "main line events." In 1978 I used a similar analysis of rca and morphological 0/n in the dialects of Santiago and San Andrés. Rather than repeat the arguments of the works cited above, the present comparative study assumes that the background-foreground distinction underlies the distribution of the tense-aspect markers. In all the Inga dialects except that of Guayuyaco (see below), the background-foreground distinction readily explains the distribution of rca and morphological 0/n. What is remarkable is that, whereas in San Andrés and Yunguillo background information is presented using rea while the main events of the narrative employ the 0/n form, in Aponte and Santiago the functions are reversed.

150

Grammatical Matters

In general terms, the background information expressed in independent clauses in Inga may be divided into three categories: (a) Events and states which are preliminary to the main events of the story, including that which did not take place (when followed by what did) and a rhetorical usage, immediately preceding the climax or "peak" (Longacre 1976: 2i2ff.). (b) Comments on and expansions of a previously stated main event of the story. (c) Events and states which take place away from the scene of the main events of the story. These include that which is perceived, discovered, or otherwise not performed by the character central to that part of the story. (The location of the central character is then treated as the scene of the main events.) These categories are illustrated in the following four stories, one from each dialect. (Only the independent verbs are given in Inga, the relevant marker being italicized. The markers arc not attached to verbs of subordinate clauses. See the appendix for the Inga text and a word-by-word translation of Story 1.) Story 1 is in the Santiago dialect. The main events are marked by rca. Background information (indented) is morphologically unmarked (0) preceding silence, and marked by n preceding other suffixes. A brief commentary follows the story.

STORY I "THE GOSSIP" (CHASOY I 9 8 2 A : 4 - - 6 )

(Preliminary Incident:) 1. A boy who was a gossip used to tell (huillacurasi)4 everyone who arrived, "We are collecting bean pods, to go to heaven." 2. His parents said (niwcunasi), "There will not be enough for everyone. Just a few of us are going to go to heaven. So why are you telling everybody who arrives about this?" 3. The boy said (niwsi), "They arc not serious when they tell me off. We are going to heaven, to heaven." 4. And he used to dance around (bailaricuo) on one leg like a dove. 5. "Be quiet, boy! Don't be telling everybody!" his parents said (niwcunasi). "We have already told you 'No!' You are going to be staying behind for being such a gossip." 6. The boy did not listen (uyao) at all.

Tense-Aspect Markers in Inga (Quechua) Dialects

151

(Main Events:) 7. The father collected (mirarat) bean pods until the house was full of them. 8. When the time arrived to go to heaven, he said (nirca) to the child, "Go and bring water! There is none in heaven." 9. He sent (cacharca) him with an open-weave basket to bring the water. 10. Running, he filled (jundachirai) it in a flash. 11. Like the wind he arrived (chayagrirai), carrying it. (12-17. The child is sent twice more for water, first with a sifter, then with a sieve, rca is attached to each verb.) 18. "You lack water for yourself." He sent (cacharai) him with a woven bag. (Comments on the previous events:) 19. It was not known (yachari0) how to delay him. 20. He used to go around (muyucuo) in no time, like the wind. 21. That trip with the woven bag delayed (unayara?) him twice. (22-27 describe the problems the boy had and how he overcame them; rca is attached to each verb.) 28. He arrived (chayagrira*) running. (Observation by boy [central character]:) 29. But when he looked, there was (tiawcuna) only the water he had brought. 30. "Gracious!" he said (nirarsi). "They were sending me for water, just to leave me behind." 31. He pushed (tangachirai) the bean pods into the midst of the fire. 32. Jumping, he rose (sicarai) upwards in the smoke. (Observation by boy:) 33. His parents were visible (cahuarinacuo) like flies, up on high. (Preliminary event: new setting:) 34. Now he too was nearly reaching (alcansacugrio) heaven. 35. He was lacking (pisicurai) just two lengths (20 feet) to secure himself to heaven. (Preliminary to 37 [climax]: away from and not performed by boy:) 36. Then the bean pod smoke completely failed (pisigri0). 37. He came (samurai) right down from there. 38. He came and fell (urmagsamurai) unconscious. 39. About two hours later, he woke up (rigcharigsamurai), turned into a bird.

152

Grammatical Matters

(Expansion on 39: what sort of bird:) 40. It used to cry (yacha0 caparicunga),5 "Gossip, gossip, gossip" . . . 41. And that is how the gossip, becoming a gossip bird, remained (quidarai) in this world. The first events of the above story, which describe the boy's behaviour prior to the time to go to heaven and explain why his parents would wish to leave him behind (sentences 1—6), are treated as background information. They form a separate incident from the main events of the story (compare Story 3, sentences 1 - 4 ) . The main events themselves consist of the completion of the preparations (sentence 7) plus the events of the great day itself (sentences 8ff.). Once into the body of the story, just a few sentences are treated as background. Sentences 19—20 give background information because they do not describe a new event, but rather explain the problem that the father was trying to solve by sending the boy for water with leaky containers. Sentences 29 and 33 are treated not as events but as observations made by the boy, who has been performing all the actions of this section of the story (21—32). Then, as the climax of the story approaches, the treatment of sentence 34 as background information suggests a change of setting for the main events, away from earth to the proximity of heaven. This is confirmed by the treatment of sentence 36 as background; although clearly a key event of the story, it is neither performed by the boy nor does it take place near him. Finally, sentence 40 does not describe a new event, but rather identifies the bird mentioned in sentence 39. * * *

Story 2 is in the Aponte dialect. As in Santiago, background information (indented) is morphologically unmarked (0) preceding silence, and marked by n preceding other suffixes. The main events are marked by ska, which corresponds to rca in the other dialects. One difference with Santiago is that introductory statements, such as sentences 1 and 2, which employ the construction yacha- 'know' plus a future infinitive (terminating in nga) with the meaning "reputed to . . . ," carrv the sha marker. Contrast sentence 1 with the opening sentence of a Santiago story (Tandiov T. and Levinsohn 1985:14). This sentence also introduces its central character and reads, "Once there was reputed to be (tiangasi yachao) a man called Carlos Tamabioy."

Tcnsc-Aspcct Markers in Inga (Qucchua) Dialects

153

STORY 2 " T H E YOUTH W H O WENT TO BATHE DURING HOLY W E E K " (LEVINSOHN I 9 8 j A : 5 5 )

1. Once there was reputed to be (yacha^asi tianga) a young man. 2. Not at all is he reputed to have feared (yachasha manchanga) God. (Preliminary events to speech 8:) 3. In defiance he went (ri0) to bathe in the big river. 4. While he was bathing, on the other side he saw (cahuarasi) a black mule which was drinking water. 5. He did not become afraid (manchari0). 6. Then a man drew near (caillayagsamuo), saying, "Why are you bathing at such a time as this?" 7. The youth replied (ainiwsi), "Because I want to! Who is there to see what I do?" 8. On hearing this, the man said (nutesi), "As you have that attitude, something may happen to you." 9. Having said this, he went away (rishasi), without saying anything else. 10. The youth remained (quidai^«), thinking, "What was this man trying to say to me? Will something happen to me?" 11. Having thought that, he just continued bathing (armacuiMllasi). (Preliminary to 13 [climax]:) 12. When he had now finished and went to put on his clothing, he didn't find (tari0) it. 13. When he looked to the other side, he hid (pacacur/;«) his face in his hands, exclaiming "Good God!" to sec the wretched mule with his clothes placed on it. In contrast with Story 1, the preliminary events that are treated as background information (sentences 3—7) for the main events of the above story (sentences 8ff.) are part of the same incident as the main events themselves. However, they may be viewed simply as setting the scene for the key speech of sentence 8 and its consequences. For example, the attitude of the youth, as expressed in his speech of sentence 7, may be seen as an illustration of the statement of sentence 2 that he did not fear God. As in Story 1 (sentence 36), the climax (sentence 13) is immediately preceded by a sentence presented as background information. See also Story 3, sentence 7.

154

Grammatical Matters

Story 3 is in the San Andres dialect. In contrast with stories in the other mountain dialects, the main events arc morphologically unmarked (0; n preceding certain suffixes). Background information (indented) is marked by rca. STORY 3 " S U N WATER L A K E " (BY LÁZARO MOJOMBOY P.) (LEVINSOHN AND AVENDAÑO I982B -.7)

(Preliminary Incident [see Story i, sentences i—6]:) 1. In Sun Water Lake, an evil serpent with seven heads and seven tails used to emerge (Ilugsicurai). 2. It used to wait (suyacurai) each Sunday at the edge of the lake for a person to be given to it. 3. They used to send (cachanacurai) someone who didn't go to Mass, to be handed over to the evil serpent. 4. It was eating (micucurai) so many that the people living there became very few. (Main Events:) 5. Then they sent (cachawcuna) a youth to be handed over. 6. He went (rio) with a large machete. (Preliminary to 8 [climax]: not performed by the youth:) 7. It came and churned up (timbugsamurai) the water, to swallow the boy. 8. Then he, cutting off its neck with his machete, killed (huañuchio) it. * * *

Story 4 is in the lowland dialect of Yunguillo. As in San Andres, background information (indented) is marked by rca. The main events are characterised throughout by the presence of n. (On the function of sea in sentences 11 and 19, see below.) STORY4

" T H E OLD BACHELOR" (LEVINSOHN 1983B : 3 6 - 3 7 )

(Preliminary States:) 1. An old bachelor was wanting (munacurca) to marry the woman he loved. 2. For him she was (capúraisi) a good-looking girl. 3. So he told (huilláw) his mother, "I am going to get married this coming festival, when the priest comes."

Tense-Aspect Markers in Inga (Quechua) Dialects

155

4. 5. 6. 7.

His mother would not let (lisinsiaw) him. In those days, he got (tucuw) diarrhoea. He began (cayariw) to take long walks. On one walk he saw (cahuariwsi) (Observation:) 8. His beloved was coming toward him (samupucurai). 9. So he said to himself (niriw), "My coming is a disaster; I stink with the diarrhoea." 10. While he was thinking this, he went and met (tuparigriw) her. 11. She turned out to be (cadusi casca) a devil. 12. She took (apawsi) the man a long way off into a cave. 13. He went and saw (cahuagrmsi) many devils in a fire.

14. Then the one who had brought him said (nin), "Return home again!" (Preliminary Event: new setting:) 15. He arrived (chayarca) at the house. 16. He sat down (tiariwsi) at the table. 17. He asked (manawsi) his mother for some corn-brew. 18. When she went to pass it to him, she saw (cahuarasi). 19. Already he had died (huanurca) just at the table. In the above account, sentences 1—2 set the scene for the events of the story, as does sentence 15 for the climax. As in Story 1, observations and discoveries (sentence 8; see also 11 and 19) are not treated as main events. * * *

To summarise, then, the following distribution of tense-aspect markers is found in the Inga dialects examined to date:

background information main events

Aponte

Santiago

San Andres Tunguillo

0/n sha

0/n rca

rca 0/n

rca n

The dialects of Santiago and Aponte thus employ their tense-aspect markers in basically the same way, the only significant difference being in the marking of introductory "reputed to" statements that employ the "know" verb construction. Similarly, the San Andrés and Yunguillo dialects employ their tense-aspect markers in essentially the same way.

156

Grammatical Matters

It is noteworthy that a speaker is not obliged to use the background information marker in a story. In a particular narrative, he may choose to present all the sentences as main events (e.g., Levinsohn and Avendano 1982b: 15—16 [San Andres]). In such narratives, in the San Andres and Yunguillo dialects, all the independent verbs have the morphological 0/n tense marker, whereas the tense-aspect marker is realsha in Santiago and Aponte.

sea and the Puerto Guayuyaco Dialect In folktales in the Guayuyaco dialect, the morphological 0/« tense-aspect marker is used in connection with the main events of the story, as in San Andres and Yunguillo. However, many background sentences employ not rea but sea. All the Inga dialects employ sea as a nominaliser of past events (see Muysken 1977:65), e.g., chayagmojcunata huillacunsi (those-who-hadarrived he-used-to-tell) " H e used to tell those who had arrived" (Chasoy 1982a: 4). It is used also "like the past participle in Spanish" (Stark et al. 1973:230), for example, yapa cuiyasea carca (very loved he-was) " H e was [the one who was] much loved" (Tandioy T. and Levinsohn 1985:11). sea also occurs verbally as a perfective. Concerning this usage, Ross writes (1963:79), "It specifies events having taken place while the speaker was in a state of ignorance, which has now been remedied by a discovery of the true state of affairs." Consequently, Muysken calls /-shka/ the marker of a "sudden discovery tense" (1977:60). This use of sea is exemplified in Story 4 (Yunguillo), sentence 11 (see also sentence 19):

EXCERPT

FROM STORY 4

10. While he was thinking this, he went and met her. ii. She turned out to be (cadusi casea) a devil (and he hadn't known it until then). This usage of sea is found in Guayuyaco (see excerpt 6 below). However, the vast majority of examples of sea in Guayuyaco narratives present preliminary events, rea is restricted to the presentation of preliminary non-

Tense-Aspect Markers in Inga (Quechua) Dialects

157

events, states that are perceived or discovered by the central character of that part of the story, and comments on a previously stated event of the story. * * *

Story 5 illustrates many of these points, rca is restricted to the opening nonevents of the story (sentences 1 - 3 ) and to the nonevents discovered by the monkey (sentences 39-40). sea is used in connection with events that build up to each set of main events. The main events themselves, as in the dialects of San Andres and Yunguillo, are in the morphologically unmarked form (0; n preceding certain suffixes). STORYs

" T H E T R E E W H I C H DARKENED THIS W O R L D " (jAMIOY Y.

1985:15-19) (Nonevents:) 1. After God made this world, it was (carca) not like it is now: proper days. 2. The sun used not to appear (cahuadiru ca rca). 3. At that time there was (tiaraisi) a very wise medicine man. (Buildup to 5:) 4. He said (niicii), "We are not enjoying proper days, because there is a great tree which is darkening the whole world. If we could cut down that tree, there would be good days, because there is something to give us light, called 'sun.'" 5. Then everyone, meeting together, asked each other (tapunacu0) what could be done to cut down that tree. (Buildup to 8:) 6. Then he answered (ainiica), " U p to now it is not known what to do to cut it down." 7. After that all the medicine men, meeting together, drank (upiaicocuna) (a hallucinatory drug). 8. Then they said (niweunasi), "It will be possible to cut down that tree, just by drinking the drug and chanting." (Buildup to 10:) 9. Then they asked (tapu5c«cuna) which medicine man could chant like that until the tree fell. 10. And he answered (ainiwsi), "The great owl will be able to." (Buildup to i2ff.:)

158

Grammatical Matters

II. Having said that and having asked him, they took (pusajcacuna) him (to the tree). 12. Then, standing beside it, he began (callariwsi) to chant, . . . 13. While he was chanting, in truth the tree began (callari0) to bend. 14. It became (tucu0) as though about to fall. 15. And then breath failed (pisigrio) the chanter. 16. With that, the tree again became upright (dirichario). (17—34. Further attempts to make the tree fall also fail, sea and 0/n are employed. Finally:) (Buildup to 4iff.:) 35. Then the wise men, again meeting together drank (upiaic«cuna). 36. And they said (nircacunasi), "We see that a vine is holding the tree up from above. When someone cuts it, it will fall." 37. Having said that, they asked (ruaftMcunasi) the coto monkey to go and cut the vine. 38. He, having gone up the tree, found (tarigrisea) (The nonevents he discovered:) 39. The tree had (yucara*) ripe fruit. 40. It was (caraisi) good to eat. 41. Then the monkey, filling his stomach, lying down up in the tree, was resting (siricuo), warming himself in the sun. 42. And he didn't remember (yuyario) why he had gone there. 43. Those on the ground were waiting (suyanacuwsi) to see when the tree would fall. (Buildup to 46:) 44. Because it wasn't falling, they asked (ruaicacuna) the squirrel, " G o and see why the tree isn't falling!" 45. Then the squirrel, having gone to see, said (nisea), "The monkey hasn't cut the vine. He's living there without a care in the world." 46. Then they asked (ruawcuna) the squirrel himself to go and cut the vine. (Buildup to 48:) 47. He went and cut (trusagruai) the vine. 48. Then the tree fell (urmao). * * *

The following excerpt from another Guayuyaco text illustrates the use both of rea, in connection with an explanatory comment, and of sea, in

Tcnse-Aspect Markers in Inga (Quechua) Dialects

159

connection with what was perceived, with Muysken's "sudden discovery" meaning. EXCERPT 6 (jAIMOY Y. 1985 : 28)

1. The youth, telling the old man off, ordered (mandao), "Fill the bag quickly, so that we can return!" (Explanation of i:) 2. That wise youth wanted (munaraisi) to sec, when he bent down, whether it really was his father since, if it was a devil, in his neck would be a second mouth with great teeth like a tiger's. (What was perceived by the youth:) 3. When the old man bent down and he looked, it turned out to have been (ciicasi) a devil, with all those terrifying teeth. 4. Then the youth said (niwsi), "Bend down quickly and gather them up!" 5. As soon as the devil bent down, he speared (pasachi0) him with his fish lance, right through to the ribs. The "sudden discovery" function of sea in sentence 3 of the above excerpt is readily distinguishable from its more common usage in narrative, in that the information is a nonevent which the young man observed (usually marked by tea). * * *

In his work on Proto-Quechua, Parker (1969:141) labels *s + qA as an indicator of "quotativc tense," in contrast with *r + qA, which simply indicates "past tense." Certainly in Inga, whether used for "sudden discoveries" or in connection with the preliminary events of a story, sea is restricted to occasions in which the narrator is not a witness to the events. As soon as the narrator is a witness, rea replaces sea in Guayuyaco as the marker of such events. This is illustrated by the following excerpt from a history of how Ingas came to live in Puerto Guayuyaco (Jamioy Y. 1985: 1 - 1 4 ) . The events that took place before the author was born or in her absence are presented using the 0/n or the sea forms (e.g., sentence 1). As soon as the author is a witness to the events, rea is employed instead of sea.

i6o

Grammatical Matters

EXCERPT 7 (jAIMOY Y. 1985 :12—13)

1. The colonists said (niscacuna). "This business of electing an Indian governor is pointless. It's enough to have just a sheriff for everybody." 2. Here also for two years there was (tirarca) no governor. 3. Then the past governors, meeting together, said (nircacuna), "It is because we believed the colonists that we arc now suffering all these calamities. . . . 4. Then, because no-one was willing to agree to be governor, they commanded (mandaraicuna) Baltazar Pena (the author's husband) to be governor. 5. The next year, they reelected (nligiwcuna) him, and again for a third year. The distribution of the tense-aspect markers in narratives in the different Inga dialects may therefore be summarised as follows:

Aponte

San Santiago Andres

Tunguillo

Guayuyaco

rca rca

rca sea

Background information nonevents and comments preliminary events

0/n 0/n

0/n 0/n

rca rca

(rca if witnessed) "sudden discovery"

Main events

sea

sea

sea

sea

sea

sha

rca

0/n

n

0/n

From the above table, it is clear that the distribution of rca and &ln divides the dialects into two groups: Aponte-Santiago and San AndrésYunguillo-Guayuyaco. As for sea, Muysken states (1977:60), "Narrative /-shka/ and sudden discovery /-shka/ have enough in common to be considered one tense; they will be considered the central meaning of /-shka/, and the use of /-shka/ as a generalised past tense marker will be considered a secondary, though significant, development." If Muysken's conclusion is right, then the use of sea in Guayauyaco would correspond with his central meaning, and its function in the other Inga dialects would represent a significant restriction in its use.

Tensc-Aspect Markers in Inga (Quechua) Dialects

161

Alternatively, the variation in the function of sea may suggest that, in spite of the tradition that the San Andres Ingas migrated from the region to the south rather than the north of Mocoa, the San Andres and Yunguillo dialects should be associated together, over against the Guayuyaco dialect. Possible confirmation for this hypothesis is found in the traditional stories common to San Andrés and Yunguillo, but not generally known in Guayuyaco. Asked for a story often told by the old people, for instance, Ingas from both the mountain dialects and Yunguillo responded with the Inga equivalent of "Hansel and Gretel" (Cuatindioy C. 1972; Garreta 1985: 9 - 1 4 ) and the rivalry between the Rabbit and the Bear (Chasoy 1982b: 1 16; Garreta 1985:1-8)!

Appendix INGA TEXT AND WORD-BY-WORD TRANSLATION OF STORY I, CHANCUALA,

" T H E GOSSIP," BY JOSÉ CHASOY DE SINJINDIOY

(SANTIAGO)

1. Chancúala huambra maicán chayagriscacunata Imillacunsi, "Purutu cara mirachinacunehi, nucanchi sug luarma ringapa." Gossip boy anyone to-those-who-went-and arrived 3[rd person]-istelling, "Bean pod we-are-collecting, we other to-place to-go." 2. Chihura atuncuna nincunasi, "Tucuitac manima pusangapa canchu. Nucanchi cuintadúllami ringapa canchi cieluma. Carnean ímata ña maicanpas yaicuscacunata parlacungapaglla?" That-time big-oncs 3-say, "To-all not to-take 3-is. We just-few to-go we-are to-heaven. You-in-contrast what now anyone-whatever tothose-who-arrived to-just-be-telling?" 3. Chi huámbrac ninsi, "Nucanchitayángamipiñanacú. Nucánchic cieluma cieluma ringapa canchi." That child 3-says. "Us in-vain 3-are-rebuking. We to-heaven toheaven to-go we-are." 4. Paluma bailadur ca: chásasi sug chaquillahua huarcuspa, caima y chima bailaricu. Dove dancer 3-is: thus one with-just-leg hanging, to-here and tothere 3-is-dancing-around. 5. "Upalla cay, huambritu. Pítapas manaparlacuy," atuncuna nincunasi. "Ña nucanchi parlangapa 'Mana' ninchi. Parlunmándami quidangapa cangui."

162

Grammatical Matters

"Quiet be, boy. To-anyone-at-all don't be-telling," big-ones 3-say. "Now we to-talk 'don't' we-say. For-gossip to-remain you-are." 6. Ni imapassi mana uyd. Not anything-at-all not 3-hears. 7. Sutipasi huasi junda purutu car a mirarca. Truly house full bean pod 3-collected. 8. Na cieluma ringapa hura chayahurac, chi huambratac nirca, "Apagriy yacu. Cielupi mamma tianchu." Now to-heaven to-go hour when-arrived, that to-child 3-said, "Goand-bring water. In-heaven not there-is." 9. Saparuhua cacharca, yacu apangapa. With-basket 3-sent, water to-take. 10. Calpdspac, man'unailla jundachirca. Running, not-long-at-all 3-filled. 11. Hudiralla aparispa chayagrirca. . . . Just-wind carrying 3-went-and-arrived. . . . 18. "Cam quinmandamipisicu."Quiquinmanda jigrasic cacharca. "You for-self 3-is-lacking." For-self bag 3-sent. 19. Manasi yachari imasa paita unayachinga. Not 3-is-known how him to-delay. 20. Paic man'unailla huairallasi muyucu. He not-long-at-all just-wind 3-is-going-round. 21. Chicar jigrahua parissic unayarca. . . . That with-bag twice 3-delayed. . . . 28. Calpa chayagrirca. Running 3-went-and-arrived. 29. Cahuagrihurac, chasallasi tiancuna astdscac. When-went-and-saw, just-thus there-are what-had-carried. 30. "Caramba!" nircasi. "Caicunac saquihuangamanda, nucata mandahuanacu." "Gracious!" 3-said. "These for-to-leave-me, me 3-are-sending-me." 31. Purutu caracunatac chaugpi ninasi tangachirca. Bean pods middle fire 3-pushed. 32. Saltaspa, huichay cusnipi sicarca. Jumping, upwards in-smoke 3-rose. 33. Atuncunac chuspisinallasi ahuapi cahuarinacu. Big-ones just-like-fly on-high 3-are-appearing. 34. Chihurac nallapaipas alcansacugri. That-time nearly he-also 3-goes-and-is-reaching.

Tense-Aspect Markers in Inga (Quechua) Dialects

163

35. Iscay tupusinalla ptsicurca, cieluma piarigringapa. Two just-like-length 3-was-lacking, to-heaven to-go-and-grasp. 36. Chihúrac purutu cara cúsnic impas pisigrí. That-time bean pod smoke completely 3-goes-and-fails. 37. Chimandata samurca impas. Right-from-there 3-came completely. 38. Na sug rigcha yuyachi umiaksamurca. Now other likeness caused-feeling 3-came-and-fell. 39. Unaipi, sug iscay hurapisina, rigcharigsamurca, piscu tucuspa. After-while, other two like-after-hour, 3-came-and-awoke, bird becoming. 40. Chican, "Chancúala chancúala chancúala". . . yachá caparicunga. That, "Gossip gossip gossip" . . . 3-knows to-be-crying. 41. Chásasi parlunca, chancúala piscu tucuspa, cay mundupi aicha quidarca. Thus gossip, gossip bird becoming, this in-world flesh 3-became.

NOTES

1. The texts were gathered during field trips made between 1968 and 1985. The work itself is based on an analysis of more than seventy narratives, some given orally and recorded, others dictated, and some written by the Inga storyteller. All the narratives quoted in the chapter are published in monolingual (Inga) or bilingual (Inga-Spanish) textbooks for schools supervised by Educación Nacional Contratada (Vicariato Apostólico de Sibundoy). Copies of the texts in microfiche form may be obtained from the author (58 Hithercroft Road, High Wycombe, HP13 5 R H , England). 2. The Spanish alphabet is used for all the Inga examples cited, Is/ being written sh and Id ts. See Levinsohn ( 1 9 7 6 : 1 9 - 2 9 ) and Tandioy J. and Levinsohn (forthcoming) for descriptions of the phonology of Inga. 3. The Guayuyaco people used to live in Puerto Limón (lamioy Y. 1985:1). I have encountered no clear tradition concerning migrations of the Yunguillo or Condagua people from other areas. Personal conversations with Ingas from San Miguel suggest that they speak the same dialect as the Guayuyaco people. They say that they lived close to M o c o a earlier in this century. 4. The reportative enclitic si is optionally attached to the main verb phrase of the sentence, or to any phrase preceding the verb that is given prominence (see Levinsohn 1975: i4ff. for a detailed discussion of this point). The equivalent enclitic for events to which the author is a witness is mi. 5. In the Santiago dialect, this "habitual" construction differs from the "reputed t o " construction (see Story 2), in that the verb yacha- 'know 5 almost always precedes the infinitive and often is not contiguous to it. In the "reputed to" construction, the infinitive immediately precedes yacha-. In Aponte, the distinction

164

Grammatical Matters

between the two constructions appears to have been lost, as yacha- always precedes the infinitive. REFERENCES

Cuatindioy C., Miguel 1972 Iscay Huajchu (Los Dos Huérfanos). Lomalinda: Instituto Lingüístico de Verano. Chasoy Sijindioy, José 1982a Sachucu caugsagcuna tucuscamanda parlocuna (Cuentos en que las personas se convierten en animales). Sibundoy: Educación Nacional Contratada. 1982b Conejomanda y Gurguntillumanda parlocuna (El Conejo y el Oso; Bordoncillo). Sibundoy: Educación Nacional Contratada. Carreta, Jacinto 1985 Condaguamanda antihua parlucuna (Tradiciones de Condagua). Sibundoy: Educación Nacional Contratada. Manuscript. Givón, T. 1984 Syntax: A Functional-Typological Introduction. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Guacho, Juan Naula and Donald H. Burns 1975 Bosquejo gramatical del quichua de Chimborazo. Quito. Hopper, Paul J. 1979 "Aspect and foregrounding in discourse." In Syntax and Semantics Vol. 12, Discourse and Syntax, ed. T. Givón, 213-41. London: Academic Press. Howard-Malverde, Rosaleen 1981 "Dioses y diablos: Tradición oral de Cañar Ecuador." Amerindia número special 1. Paris. Jamioy Y. de Peña, Otilia 1985 Antihua pacay ginticunapa parlucuna (Tradiciones de los inganos pacayes). Sibundoy: Educación Nacional Contratada. Larsen, Helen 1974 "Some grammatical features of legendary narrative in Ancash Quechua." In Advances in Tagmemics, ed. Ruth M. Brend, 419—40. Amsterdam: North-Holland. Levinsohn, Stephen H. 1975 "Functional sentence perspective in Inga." Journal of Linguistics 11:13-37. 1976 The Inga (Quechuan) Language. The Hague: Mouton. 1978 "¿Tiempo pretérito, o acción de fondo? Una diferencia dialectal en inga," Courrier du Sud n: 53-61. Pasto. 1983a (ed.) Nucanchipa Rimay (Lenguaje Inga). Sibundoy: Educación Nacional Contratada. 1983b (ed.) Nucanchipa Simi Rimay (Lenguaje Inga). Sibundoy: Educación Nacional Contratada.

Tense-Aspect Markers in Inga (Quechua) Dialects

165

Levinsohn, Stephen H. and Luis Francisco Avendaño, eds. 1982a San Andresmanda gcntecuna imasa costumbre y parlo yucascacuna (Leyendas y Costumbres de San Andrés). Sibundoy: Educación Nacional Contratada. 1982b Parlocuna leesunchi (Cartilla inga 2). Sibundoy: Educación Nacional Contratada. Longacre, Robert E. 1976 An Anatomy of Speech Notions. Lisse: Peter de Ridder Press. Muysken, Pieter 1977 Syntactic Developments in the Verb Phrase of Ecuadorian Quechua. Lisse: Peter de Ridder Press. Orr, Carolyn and Robert E. Longacre 1968 "Proto-Quechumaran." Language 44.3 (September): 528-55. Parker, Gary J. 1969 "Comparative Quechua phonology and grammar II. Proto-Quechuan phonology and morphology." University ofHawaii Working Papers in Linguistics, Vol. 1, No. 1. Ross, Eileen 1963 Introduction to Ecuador Highland Quichua. Quito: Instituto Lingüístico de Verano. Stark, Louisa R. et al. 1973 El Quichua de Imbabura: una gramática pedagógica. Otavalo: Instituto Interandino de Desarrollo. Tandioy J., Francisco and Stephen H. Levinsohn forthcoming "Fonología comparativa de los dialectos del inga." Tandioy T., Luis Manuel and Stephen H. Levinsohn, eds. 1985 Santiagomandagentecuna imasa parlo yucascacuna (Tradiciones de Santiago). Sibundoy: Educación Nacional Contratada. Manuscript. Weber, David John 1983 A Grammar of Huallaga (Huanuco) Quechua. Ph.D. dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles. Available from University Microfilm, Ann Arbor, Mich. Zahn, Charlotte n.d. Tense Sequence in Pastaza Quechua Text Structure. Lima: Instituto Lingüístico de Verano, Manuscript.

Adalberto Salas The Minimal Finite Verbal Paradigm in Mapuche or Araucanian at the End of the Sixteenth Century

ABSTRACT This chapter presents the core of the Mapuche verbal paradigm (i.e., the system of mode, person, and number markers) toward the end of the sixteenth century, and contrasts it with that of the present-day language. It is discovered that after 400 years, the system remains the same in both the phonological surface and the deep morphological structure. This high degree of resistance to change may be seen as an overall, prominent feature of the Mapuche language.

Introduction The first complete Mapuche or Araucanian grammar was published in Lima in 1606, under the title of Arte y Gmmatica General de la Lengva que corre en todo el Reyno de Chile. . . . It was written by the Jesuit missionary Luis de Valdivia (Valdivia 1606 [1887]).' In this article I present the minimal verbal paradigm known at the end of the sixteenth century, as described by Father Valdivia, and contrast it with that of the modern language in order to gain some insight into change/permanence in this language during the almost four hundred years that have passed since Father Valdivia's Arte. The Mapuche finite verbal forms are those obligatorily inflected by mode and focal person2 or by mode, focal person, and number. They are structurally and functionally opposite to nonfinite verbal forms, grossly corresponding to the infinitive mode of Latin traditional grammars (infinitive, gerund, participle, and so forth).

Minimal Finite Verb Paradigm in Mapuche

167

The Mapuche finite verbal forms consist minimally of a verb stem (in the simplest construction it is a single root) and a finite verbal inflection, manifested by a suffix or suffix cluster carrying the meaning of mode, focal person, and number. When, in a given verbal form, only the stem and the finite inflection are manifested, the verbal form is minimal. When one or more optional suffixes are manifested (such as negation, tense, perfective, locatives, satellite person3), the verbal form is nonminimal or expanded. The total set of forms involving only a given verb stem and its finite inflection is the minimal finite verbal paradigm.4 Thus, the minimal finite verbal paradigm includes, for a given verb stem, forms contrasting only by mode and focal person or by mode, focal person, and number. On the other hand, every verbal form that does not present these contrasts is nonfinite.

Old Mapuche Father Valdivia's Arte is modeled after the traditional grammars of Latin. Therefore, the Mapuche verb is presented by reference to Latin verbal conjugation and its typical categories (voice, mode, aspect, tense, person, and number). In the active voice (Arte, Chapters IIII—V, folios 11—18) there are five modes: (1) indicative, (2) imperative, (3) optative, (4) subjunctive, and (5) infinitive. The last mode does not contain finite forms. The optative is not a true mode because it does not have specific forms; rather, the optative meaning is expressed by means of the so-called mixed tenses of the indicative, by the imperative, or by some tenses of the subjunctive (Chapter IIII, folios 13—14). Both indicative and subjunctive have eight tenses each. Four of them are called "principal": (1) present, (2) imperfect, (3) first future (imperfect future), (4) first mixed tense (future-in-the-past). The other four tenses are: (1) perfect, (2) pluperfect, (3) second future (perfect future), and (4) second mixed tense (perfect-future-in-the-past). The imperative has one tense only, called present (Chapter IIII, folio 13). There are three persons (1st, 2nd, 3rd) in each tense and three numbers in each person (singular, dual, and plural). In the active voice, the minimal present forms are: indicative, imperative, and subjunctive. The other tenses are nonminimal because they are formed by means of optional markers of tense and/or of perfective, placed between the stem and the finite suffixes.

168

Grammatical Matters The forms of the present indicative are:

SINGULAR

I II III

elun

[e-'lun]

I give 5

eluymi

[e-'lui-mi]

you give

eluy

[e-'lui]

he/she gives

eluyu

[e-'lu-yu]

both of us give

DUAL

I II

eluymu

[e-'lui-mu]

both of you give

III

eluygu

[e-'lui-iju]

both o f them give

PLURAL

eluin

[e-'lu-iri]

we give

II

I

eluymn

[e-'lui-man]

you give

III

eluygn

[e-'lui-gan]

they give

(See Chapter IIII, folio 12.) The root elu 'to give' and the following suffixes and suffix clusters may be isolated:

I II III

SINGULAR

DUAL

PLURAL

-n

-yu

-ymi

-ymu

-in -ymn

-y

-ygu

-ygn

The forms of the present imperative are: SINGULAR

I

eluchi

[e-'lu-ci]

may I give!

II

eluge

[e-lu-'ge]

you give!

Ill

elupe

[e-lu-'pe]

may he/she give!

eluyu

[e-'lu-yu]

let us give (both o f us) !

elumu

[e-'lu-mu]

both of you give!

elupe or elupe (e)gu

fe-lu-'pe-qu]

may both of them give!

DUAL

I II Ill

Minimal Finite Verb Paradigm in Mapuche

169

PLURAL

I II Ill

eluyñ elumn elupe or elupe (e)gn

[e-'lu-iñ] [e-lu-'man] [e-lu-'pe-rjan]

let us give! you give! may they give!

(See Chapter IIII, folio 13.) The suffixes and suffix clusters are:

I II III

SINGULAR

DUAL

PLURAL

-chi

-yu -mu -pe(e)gu

-yñ 6 -mn -pe(e)gn

"ge -pe

It may be noted that the inflection of 1st person dual and plural is the same as the corresponding indicative. The forms o f the present subjunctive are: SINGULAR

I II III

eluli elulmi elule

[e-'lu-li] [e'lul-mi] [e-'lu le]

when I give or if I give when you give, etc. when he/she gives, etc.

elulyu elulmu elulgu

[e-'lu-lyu] [e'lul-mu] [e'lul-rju]

without glosses in Valdivia

eluliñ elulmn elulgn

[e-'lu-liñ] [e-'lui-man] [e-'lul-rpn]

without glosses in Valdivia

DUAL

I II III PLURAL

I II III

(See Chapter IIII, folio 14.) The suffixes and suffix clusters are:

I II III

SINGULAR

DUAL

PLURAL

-li -lmi -le

-lyu -lmu -l(e e)gu

-liñ -lmn -l(e e)gn

170

Grammatical Matters

Father Valdivia observes that sometimes 3rd person dual or plural subjects agree with 3rd person singular verbs (Chapter XV, folio 39). This observation permits us to assume that there was only one form of 3rd person, the so-called singular: INDICATIVE

IMPERATIVE

SUBJUNCTIVE

eluy

elupe

elule

and that the apparent forms of 3rd person dual and plural:

DUAL PLURAL

INDICATIVE

IMPERATIVE

SUBJUNCTIVE

eluygu eluygn

elupe (e)gu elupe (e)gn

elul(e e)gu elul(e e)gn

contain optional markers of dual (egu ~ gu) and plural (egn ~ gn). Father Valdivia's examples: (1) aldupuche kupay [al-'Gi'-pu-ce ka-'pai] many Indians come (2) aukapuche vfchiduamlay [aw-'ka-pu-ce uf-ci-Gwam-'lai] the braves do not like peace suggest that the optional markers of number do not occur when duality/ plurality is expressed by lexical means in the subject (in the examples, the segment -pu- in aldupuche and aukapuche is a pluralizer of nouns). Father Valdivia includes two other voices: impersonal voice (in two variants) and passive voice. The first variant of impersonal voice (Chapter VII, folio 20) contains verbal forms of undetermined agent and undetermined patient, as in eluam 'it is given'. This variant is marked by the suffix -am [am], which does not present the typical contrasts of finite verbal forms (mode, focal person, number). The second variant (Chapter VII, folios 21—22) contains forms of undetermined agent and determined patient. It is formed by means of the verbal root ge [ge], meaning 'to be,' directly added to the stem. The verb ge follows the normal inflection of the language (Chapter VI, folios 19—20). In the context of ge the focal person has the semantic value of patient:

Minimal Finite Verb Paradigm in Mapuche eluymi elugeymi

[e-'lui-mi] [e-lu-'rjei-mi]

171

you give you are given

elu is the root for 'to give' and ¿eymi is the 2nd-person singular of the present indicative of ¿e 'to be.' This 2nd person corresponds to patient, and agent remains undetermined. Passive verbal forms are marked by the participial suffix -el [el] which does not present the typical contrasts of finite verbal forms (Chapter VIII, folios 22-23). Since it does not carry personal markers at all, agent and patient are expressed at the clause level. To summarize, in the stage of language at Father Valdivia's time, the minimal finite forms are those of (1) active indicative present, (2) active imperative present, and (3) active subjunctive present. There are three persons in each one of these three series. The 1st and 2nd person are obligatorily marked by number (singular, dual, plural). There is only one form for 3rd person, since it does not present obligatory number contrasts at word level. Imperative 1st person dual and plural are the same as the corresponding indicative. Therefore, the minimal verbal paradigm contains nineteen different forms. Modern Mapuche There are three modes in the modern Mapuche language: (1) real (indicative), (2) volitive (imperative), and (3) hypothetical (subjunctive). There are three focal persons (1st, 2nd, 3rd) and three numbers (singular, dual, and plural). There is only one form in 3rd person, since it does not carry (obligatory at word level) number contrasts. Volitive mode lacks 1st person dual and plural. There are three optional tense suffixes and one optional perfective suffix. A form in real mode without tense or perfective markers has a vague semantic value going from nonremote past to present, depending on the meaning of the verbal stem. A form in hypothetical mode without tense or perfective markers is a present. In volitive mode, tense or perfective markers do not occur.7 Thus, the forms of present/nonremote past of the real mode, those of the present of the hypothetical mode, and those of the volitive mode are minimal finite. Therefore, in the modern language, the minimal finite verbal paradigm contains the following nineteen different forms:

172

Grammatical Matters (A) REAL (INDICATIVE)

SINGULAR

I II

[e-'lun] [e-'lui-mi]

I give 8 you give

I II

[e-'lu-yu] [e-1 lui-mu]

both of us give both of you give

[e-lu-'in] [e-'lui-man]

we give you give

DUAL

PLURAL

I II

W I T H O U T N U M B E R DENOTATION

III

[e-'lui]

he/she gives, they give

(B) VOLITIVE (IMPERATIVE) SINGULAR

I II

[e-'lu-ci] [e-'lu-ge]

may I give! or let me give! you give!

II

[e-'lu-mu]

both of you give!

[e-'lu-man]

you give!

DUAL

PLURAL

II

W I T H O U T N U M B E R DENOTATION

III

[e-'lu-pc]

may he give! or may they give!

(C) HYPOTHETICAL ( S U B J U N C T I V E ) SINGULAR

I II

[e-'lu-li] [e-'lul-mi]

if I give if you give

I II

[e-'lu-lyu] [e-'lul-mu]

if both of us give if both of you give

DUAL

Minimal Finite Verb Paradigm in Mapuche

173

PLURAL

I II

[e-lu-'liñ] [e-'lui-man]

if we give if you give

WITHOUT NUMBER DENOTATION

III

fe-'Iule]

if he/she gives or if they give

The modern language has two free morphemes: {egu} (rju ~ erju) and {erjin} (rpn ~ erpn), meaning, respectively, dual and plural of 3rd person. They are optional and occur at the clause level when the subject does not carry lexical expression of number. When they are contiguous to the verb they tend to occur in the short (enclitic) forms (rju and rpn). 9 The first variant of Father Valdivia's impersonal voice (eluam 'it is given') remains today only in the fossilized form [pi-'am] 'it is said' (from the root [pi] 'to say'), frequently used as a parenthetical expression in folktales. In modern Mapuche, there is a satellite person suffix -[ge] 'undetermined agent' in which context the focal person expresses the patient. For example: (1) lelin {leli 'to look'; -n 'real mode, focal person 1st singular' (agent)} I looked (2) lelirjen {leli 'to look'; -ge 'undetermined agent'; -n 'real mode, focal person 1st singular patient'} I was looked at This suffix accounts for the second variant of Father Valdivia's impersonal voice, that containing verbal forms of undetermined agent and determined patient.

Comparisons Simple inspection shows that the minimal verbal paradigm has not changed for some four hundred years. The system of finite verbal suffixes remains the same, in the phonological surface as well as in the abstract morphological structure. Configuration of finite inflection in terms of its slots and slot fillers is

174

Grammatical Matters

the same in both stages o f language. This system contains four types o f morphemic strings: (i) one slot o f m o d e and focal person: finite inflection mode and person -pe

volitive, 3rd

I6TH C E N T U R Y

2 0 T H CENTURY

MORPHEMIC STRING

[e-lu-'pe]

[e-'lu-pe]

{elu-pe}

(2) t w o slots, one o f m o d e and one o f focal person: finite inflection mode

person

-i real -1 hypothetical

0 3rd -e 3rd

I6TH C E N T U R Y

2 0 T H CENTURY

[e-'lui]

[e-'lui]

MORPHEMIC STRING

{elu-i-0}

[e-'lu-le]

[e-'lu-le]

{elu-l-e}

(3) consisting o f three slots: one o f mode, one o f focal person, and one o f number: finite inflection mode, person, and number -n -ci -lje

real, 1st singular volitive, 1st singular volitive, 2nd singular

Minimal Finite Verb Paradigm in Mapuche I6TH CENTURY

[e-'lun] [e-'lu-ci] [e-lu-'ge]

2OTH CENTURY [e-'lun] [c-'lu-ci] [e-'lu-oe]

175

MORPHEMIC STRING

{elu-n} {clu-ci} {elu-ge}

(4) three slots, one of mode, one of focal person, and one of number: finite inflection mode

person

number

(-i

-i

-u 'dual' -n 'plural'

'real')

'1st'

-i

'real'

-m

'2nd'

-i 'singular' -u 'dual' -an 'plural'

0

'volitive'

-m

'2nd'

-u 'dual' -on 'plural'

-1 'hypothetical'

-1

'1st'

-m

'2nd'

-0 'singular' -u 'dual' -n 'plural' -i 'singular' -u 'dual' -an 'plural'

1 6 T H CENTURY

20TH CENTURY

MORPHEMIC STRING

[e-'lu-yu] [e-'lu-in] [e-'lui-mi] [e-'lui-mu] [e-'lui-msn] [e-'lu-mu] [e-'lu-man] [e-'lu-li] [e-'lu-lyu] [e-'lu-lin] [e-'lul-mi] [e-'lul-mu] [e-'lul-man]

[e-'lu-yu] [e-lu-'in] [e-'lui-mi] [e-'lui-mu] [e-'lui-man] [e-'lu-mu] [e-'lu-man] [e-'lu-li] [e-'lu-lyu] [e-lu-'lin] [e-'lul-mi] [e-'lul-mu] [e-'lul-man]

(elu-(i)-i-u} {elu-(i)-i-n} {elu-i-m-i} {elu-i-m-u} {elu-i-m-an} {elu-0-m-u} {elu-0-m-an} {clu-l-i-0} {elu-l-i-u} {elu-l-i-n} {elu-l-m-i} {elu-l-m-u} {elu-l-m-an}

176

Grammatical Matters

Conclusion Comparison between Mapuche at the end of the sixteenth century and contemporary Mapuche shows very clearly that the nucleus of the verbal conjugation, the system of finite suffixes, has not changed except for the placement of the accent, which is nonphonemic in both stages of the language. After a careful reading of Father Valdivia's Arte, I have the impression that this high degree of resistance to temporal change may be found throughout the core of the grammar and vocabulary of this language. NOTES

1. Luis de Valdivia (born in Granada, Spain, 1561-died in Valladolid, Spain, 1642) wrote his Mapuche grammar in Chile, between March 1605 and April 1606. H e learned the Mapuche language during his first trip to the country, between 1593 and 1602 (Campos Menchaca 1972, chapters V - V I ) . 2. On the notion of focal and satellite person, see Salas (1978). 3. See note 2. An overview of modern Mapuche verbal structure is found in Salas (1984:17—18, 3 2 - 4 2 ) . 4. See Elson and Pickett (i962:76n). 5. Father Valdivia's orthography is given first, followed by a broad phonetic transcription based on the information contained in Chapter I (De la Pronunciación y Ortographia, folios 7 - 8 ) and Chapter 28 (Final del Acento, folio 55). 6. Note that the corresponding indicative form is written -iñ. The orthographic difference i versus y does not have phonological relevance. In Mapuche as well as in Spanish, Father Valdivia vacillates between the two letters, even in the same item; for example, reyno ~ reino. 7. A detailed presentation of tense suffixes is found in Croese (1984). See also Fernández Garay (1981). 8. The verbal root elu 'to give' is marked by agent (person w h o gives) and patient (person who receives). The given thing is expressed beyond the verb, at the clause level. The modern Mapuche items are transcribed in a broad phonetic transcription; details can be found in Salas (1984:14—17). 9. A detailed discussion can be found in Salas (1979). REFERENCES

Campos Menchaca, S.J., Mariano José 1972 Nahuelbuta. Buenos Aires: Editorial Francisco de Aguirre. Croese, Robert A. 1984 "Tiempo verbal en mapudungun." Actas: Jornadas de Lengua y Literatura Mapuche. Temuco, Chile: Universidad de la Frontera-Instituto Lingüístico de Verano, 6 4 - 7 6 . Elson, Benjamin and Velma Pickett 1962 An Introduction to Morphology and Syntax. Santa Ana, Calif.: Summer Institute of Linguistics.

M i n i m a l Finite V e r b Paradigm in M a p u c h e

177

Fernández Garay, Ana V. 1981 "Algunos sufijos verbales mapuches." Revista Latinoamericana de Estudios Etnolingüísticos 1:5-25. Lima. Salas, Adalberto 1978 "Terminaciones y transiciones en el verbo mapuche: Crítica y bases para una nueva interpretación." RLA. Revista de Lingüística Teórica y Aplicada 16:167-79. Concepción, Chile. 1979 "¿Hay marcas de número en la 3a. persona de los verbos mapuches? Una discusión sobre engu y engün." Estudios generales 1 : 2 2 5 - 4 0 . 1984 Textos orales en Mapuche o Araucano del centro-sur de Chile. Concepción, Chile: Editorial de la Universidad de Concepción. Valdivia, P. Luis de 1606 Arte y Gramatica General de la Lengva que corre en todo el Reyno de [1887] Chile, con un Vocabulario y Confessonario, compuestos por el Padre Luys de Valdivia de la Compañía de Iesus en la Prouincia del Piru . . . En Lima por Francisco del Canto. Año 1606. Facsimile edition prepared by Julio Platzmann. Arte, Vocabulario y Confesionario de la Lengua de Chile, compuestos por Luiz de Valdivia. Edición Facsimilar. Leipzig: B.G. Teubner.

V Ethnolinguistics

Gustavo Rodriguez The Talatur: Ceremonial Chant of the Atacama People

ABSTRACT

The purpose of this work is to present two versions of the "Talatur," a ceremonial chant of the Atacama people whose language ceased to be spoken at the end of the last century. This presentation of the texts aims at giving a testimonial to the extinct language of the "Likan Antai" (Atacamenos) rather than to reconstruct the language or to represent historical process, since the evidence is scant, heterogeneous, and multiform. The Talatur is a ceremonial chant sung (even today) during the so-called limpia de canales (cleaning of canals or channels) by the mountain people in northern Chile, in the so-called II Region, w h o inhabit the same geographic area as the Atacama did. In 1976 we collected the last version of this chant and we attempt here to unravel some of its meaning. The notes provide some scant data about a text which is written, supposedly, in "Cunza" ( = Kunza), the language of the Atacamenos, wherein some elements of the grammatical structure of this dead language may be glimpsed.

Introduction T h i s text c o n t a i n s the last remnants o f the extinct C u n z a l a n g u a g e o f the A t a c a m e n o s o r " L i k a n A n t a i . " I have verified f r o m m y t w o field trips (in w h i c h I traveled t o S a n P e d r o de A t a c a m a , T o c o n a o , T a l a b r e , C a m a r , S o caire, a n d P e i n e ) that there are n o speakers o f C u n z a at present. T h i s c e r e m o n i a l s o n g w a s collected in S o c a i r e in n o r t h e r n C h i l e , 85 k m f r o m S a n P e d r o de A t a c a m a . T h e p o p u l a t i o n o f S o c a i r e is a b o u t 345 inhabitants. T h e h o u s e s are m a d e o f s t o n e and straw w i t h small d o o r s a n d w i n d o w s t o m i n i m i z e effects f r o m the cold a n d w i n d y w e a t h e r .

The

w e a t h e r is v e r y harsh d u e t o the altitude: the t e m p e r a t u r e d r o p s b e l o w f r e e z i n g at n i g h t , even in the s u m m e r . A t present, the t o w n has a m o d e r n

I82

Ethnolinguistics

clinic, a school, and one telephone. It also has electric power at certain hours. Goatherders formerly slept near the main arroyo (quebrada) of the town. Circular building remains have Atacameñan names such as sicher, cuno, Toronal, Achal, Quisuna.

Background Documents dealing with the Cunza language such as the "Glosario" by Va'isse, Hoyos, and Echeverría (1895); "Noticias" by Echeverría y Reyes (1890); "La lengua cunza" by San Román (1890); and some notes about Cunza by Mostny (1954) have been used for this work. The mountain range of northern Chile forms a border zone with Peru, Bolivia, and Argentina. There is an exchange of educational, social, and other cultural elements in this part of Chile. Tarapacá and Antofagasta form a vast territory: Antofagasta is about 126,000 km 2 , and Tarapaca is 50,000 km2. These two areas were inhabited by many Indian groups in the past. Testing by the radiocarbon process has demonstrated that settlements can be traced back to ten thousand years in some coastal areas of Tarapacá. Inhabitants of these towns became known as "Changos" during the colonial period. The Puquinas, another group that lived in northern Chile, left their traces in the toponymy. Another group, the Uros, lived in the north coastal region of Tarapacá. They mixed with the Changos, colonies of fishermen brought from Lake Titicaca during the period of Inca conquest. The Aymaras lived, and are still living, in the prerange and interior plateau of Atacama. According to one hypothesis, Aymaras descend from an Arawak family from the continental Carib; they settled later along the coastal Pacific Ocean, where they divided into families and branches that evolved separately, among them the Uros, the Puquinas, the Quechuas, the Chingay, and others. Slow shifts of these groups peopled parts of Peru, southern Bolivia, and northern Chile and Argentina. The Incas conquered them around the fifteenth century and incorporated them into their empire. The conquerors named these regions "Colla." The tracks of these people are recognized by the abundant toponymy found in the range and interior plateau of Tarapaca. The Atacamcños inhabit the Andes range of northern Chile, but their origins are not known exactly. The rich civilization of the Atacameños,

Ceremonial Chant of the Atacama People

183

whose cultural and linguistic features differ from those of the Aymaras, the Quechuas, and other groups of the area, left traces in southern Peru, southwest Bolivia, northwest Argentina, and the coast of the I and II regions of Chile. The core of this culture was located in the region of San Pedro de Atacama, and it expanded from this point to other areas. Atacameña culture reached its maximum development in the ninth century, but a subsequent contraction process led them to live in the hydrographic region of Atacama, where they were conquered by the Incas prior to the arrival of the Spaniards. The Atacameña language, Cunza (also, "Kunza" and "Cunsa," a possessive meaning "ours") was lost at the end of the nineteenth century. Written information about the characteristics of this language is scarce. There is little information on the Inca presence in the area, since little archaeological evidence has been uncovered to date. Some historians such as Garcilaso de la Vega and Bibar, have some data that allow us to calculate that Inca dominance over the Atacameños was brief, amounting to approximately fifty to seventy years (Echeverría y Reyes 1890:93). Spanish reports of 1840 indicate that the region of the plateau and the Chilean prerange was widely populated by farmers, who founded other villages and towns, giving rise to an increasing population. In 1581 Pedro Lozano M., the first person to report information on the Indian population (Lehnert 1978:10), calculated the population of Atacama at about 2,000 persons. D'Orbigny in 1839 reported the following figures: 5,046 Indians and 1,200 mestizos for Tarapacá; 1,942 Indians and 970 mestizos for Atacama. And by 1884 Bertrand found that there were 4,000 Indians for the Puna of Atacama (Echeverría y Reyes 1890:90).

Method of Inquiry The objectives of my field trips were to find out (1) what elements of Cunza are still found in the language spoken today; and (2) what knowledge or awareness the speakers have with regard to these words or forms. With the help of existing literature, a 300-item questionnaire was devised concerning activities typical to the region, for example, activities related to agriculture, cattle breeding, and animals and plants of the zone. In this way, we were able to show that some words are still used by the people of Atacama whereas others are not. The work was carried out at the lexical level only (Rodriguez 1980-82:419—27).

184

Ethnolinguistics

During the interviews we learned that the following words are still known: (1) Three terms related to weaving. (2) Four terms related to the human body. (3) Nine terms for zoology (out of 62 terms in the literature). (4) Six plant names. (5) One term for clothing. (6) Three terms related to colors. (7) Seven terms for other referents. In brief, only a small vocabulary of 33 words bear testimony to the extinct language of the Atacamenos.

Feasts, Rites, and Customs Several celebrations have, for the present inhabitants of the Atacama region, a deep mythical meaning that is manifested in a series of traditional activities performed by the Atacamenos on specific occasions. For instance, certain rites such as el convido, "ofrenda" (offering), which are extremely ancient and penetrating, involve Nature, or Mother Earth, who receives the name pachamama, or simply, la pacha. She represents the source that provides the means of daily subsistence, thus when an individual drinks or eats, he or she takes a little bit of the portion and pours it upon the earth as a sign of an offering. Nonrealization of this ritual implies future family problems or the death and destruction of crops or animals. In the floramiento (or enfloramiento) 'flowering' ceremony, the ears of animals are marked or decorated with flowers, ribbons, or multicolor pom-poms in the estancia or puesto—a stone room and corral located on the slopes of a hill or volcano. These ceremonies are accompanied by music and dancing, after which the flock, la tropa (rebano), is left afloriada 'enflowered'. The festival known as the Talatur, talato, or "cleaning of channels," is performed as a water cult and has also been preserved with great vitality. This festivity is normally observed once a year; its observance is marked by dancing, which proceeds as the community works collectively. The feast ends with a great banquet and much imbibing. The person who directs the Talatur is called capataz or capitdn 'foreman' or 'captain'. The word talatur is of Cunza origin and means "dance"; it is also the name for the final chanting at the end of the process of cleaning the channels. The dance

Ceremonial Chant of the Atacama People

185

related to sowing is called tuscalo, which possibly derives from tusckantur (escarbar 'dig a hole'). The Talatur feasts are traditionally celebrated on 24 October in Socaire.

The Talatur Several factors justify the presentation of this analysis, mainly the fact that documents about this lost language of northern Chile contain serious linguistic limitations that make their use in a rough reconstruction of Cunza problematic. These limitations involve: (1) The heterogeneity of the collected materials. (2) The fact that many of the papers include small vocabularies of doubtful accuracy. (3) The fact that some of the people who gathered information about the Atacamenos were not prepared linguistically. This is not to say that the earlier work was not important for the sketch we have of Cunza today. From the bibliography, it can be seem that few texts exist in Cunza: two versions of the Pater noster given by Tschudi; the Talatur version collected by Grete Mostny; and the version we present in this chapter. Mostny's version was obtained from a local inhabitant from Peine who had it written down in a notebook (Mostny et al., 1954:162): This text was copied from that of Socaire, to which it is identical, except for some expressions. Thus, the Talatur from Peine begins with wilti (or wilte) which is the name for a spring [ = well] in that locality, while the Socaire text begins with muya, the name of the spring from there.

T H E MOSTNY VERSION

The version below is that of Grete Mostny et al. (1954:162-63). The numbering of the stanzas is maintained in accordance with the original. TALATUR

Wilti puri yuyo sai quepe puri pachata awai awai awai.

i86

Ethnolinguistics 2

Solar puri yuyo talu sai tami puri pachata awai

awai

awai. 3

Echar saeta cheresner Saque acta colcaina colcaina awai

awai

awai. 4

Y u r o tucor nace colcoinar sake tucor nace colcoinar awai awai awai 5 Laus saisa carar monte colcoinar Chile saisa carau saire y sairina saire sairina y yentes lulayne yentes carar y yentes illauca saflu islilla. 6 Tumi saisa monte colcoinar Chile sisa carau sare y sairina saire sairina y yentes lulayne y yentes carar y yentes illauca saflu islilla. 7 Quimal sisa carar monte colcoina Chile saira y yentes lulayne y yentes curar y yentes iyauca saflu islilla. ( N O T A : aquí empieza el "zapateo") 8

Calal tanti sayno islamas tanti sayno

Ceremonial Chant of the Atacama People

187

isay pani y ques capana isay San Antonio 9 Ayil tanti sayno katur tanti sayno isay pani y ques caspana isay San Antonio 10 Tarar chusli sayno pauma chusli sayno isay kone islujlina isay San Antonio 11 Lipis chusli isayno koiway chusli sayno y say kone y esluslina y sai San Antonio 12 Uway leyer licau simainuna y pauna licau simanuna y kaper licau simanuna y eya techaynita y eya katalur ya qui y yai y yale y yai iyawe iyawe yolasquita. Textual observations made by Mostny refer to the incorporation of some Hispanic elements (monte, San Antonio, Quimal, etc.). She also points out that this ceremonial chant is no longer understood; it is only repeated by rote by the capataz/capitán (foreman or captain) who directs the ceremony of the cleaning of the channels. Mostny recognizes that her version of the chant may have been modified in relation to the original form because of the oral character of the tradition. In addition to her other conclusions about the supposed meaning of several words or forms, Mostny summarizes (p. 165):

i88

Ethnolinguistics In other words—and making it explicit that this is only an attempt at translation—we believe that the Talatur is danced at the beginning of the precolumbian agricultural cyclc and before the planting process, and that it invokes the different springs so that they may flow abundantly from the rocks to all the surroundings so that these may be covered by clouds and so the maize and potatos may sprout satisfactorily and yield rich harvests. Children and grown-ups are invited to celebrate and to revel.

T H E R O D R J G U E Z VERSION

The text presented here was collected during my fieldwork in 1976 in Socaire; the informant was Don Andres Ramos, who was then seventy-eight years old. This version differs from Mostny's, and it is important to remember that she said her version was a faithful copy of the one obtained in Socaire. We have phonetically transcribed the recording of the text so that some of these differences may be observed. OBSERVATIONS

Comparing the two versions reveals three types of forms (or should we speak of "words"?): Forms of Hispanic origin. Those of marked Hispanic origin, such as San Antonio (patron of these communities); Chile; yentes (gentes) 'people'; montei (monte) 'hill'; and Quintal (perhaps Quechua?), the name of a mountain to the west of San Pedro de Atacama, on whose peak may be found traces of sanctuaries dedicated to the cult of that divinity (some authors indicate that this was the sacred mountain of the Atacamenos). Other forms appearing in the text also seem to be of Hispanic origin: t''alu (.salud) 'health'; solar, nase (nace) 'born'; islilla (isla); kbantei (cantar) 'sing'. Variants of a single form. yuyo, yullo: pure, puri:

pechar, echar:

This form is explained in the "Glosario" as a variety of quinoa. Agua 'water'. Water has alwavs been the fundamental element for the subsistence and development of Atacameño culture. Hence the form pure, -i combines in other terms of Cunza origin: purilari 'agua colorada' (colored water); puritama 'agua caliente' (hot water); purikujter 'agua salobre' (brackish water); punchan 'agua overa' (muddy water); and so forth. Many toponyms include the element puri. Of unknown origin. It is not recorded in any of the glossaries. Possibly, 'echar'?

TftLA TUR. Andres Jarnos

L?Lja.jj thaLo

purt tkaju

purC:

^uäo-

tesLys Linia

2 Laujs

San än-toa^o ^

,

|oKojndr ^

kau/aj

/jesKarpama

' ?L

SAK

/

1

lOTt^) O

saa

k^arauj 7

Tnonlij

K.ko

K.kojn,ar

' U pe cus' i.£ j

cl U

sis£

Agua Dulce Coastal Saturiwa Acuera Oconi (not a political unit?) Tawasa (not a political unit?)

This writer's earlier statements on Timucua dialects (Cranberry 1956) were both incomplete and inaccurate and should not be followed.

Archaeological Correlates The late John Goggin made the first attempt to correlate the then-known linguistic, ethnographic, and archaeological data on the Timucua (Goggin 1953). His insights still hold together remarkably well after a period of thirty-five years of increasingly intensive archaeological work in all the Timucua areas. The most recent summaries of archaeological correlates of Timucua tribal/linguistic units are those of Milanich (1978) for the Western Timucua and Deagan (1978) for the Eastern Timucua. Both are careful, well written accounts which build on Goggin's earlier base. The primary alteration of Goggin's data is the elimination of his Southern Timucua group (Toco-

202

Distant Relationships

baga, Ogita, Mocogo), largely on the basis of data acquired since the time Goggin wrote, which would suggest that these Tampa Bay tribes were likely Muskogean in language and wider cultural affiliation (Bullen 1978). Essentially, the Eastern Timucua (Yui, Icafui/Cascange, Yufera, Tacatacuru, Saturiwa, Agua Dulce, and Acuera) are represented archaeologically by the long St. Johns Tradition in Florida and this tradition, together with the Wilmington-Savannah, on the Georgia coast. The Western Timucua (Yustaga, Utina, Potano, Ocale) participate in the Alachua Tradition and the Fort Walton/Leon-Jefferson Traditions. The picture in the western region is considerably more complex than that in the eastern region, however, inasmuch as the north-central and northwestern parts of Florida were from earliest times a mixing-ground for peoples (?) and traits, both indigenous and foreign, from the west, north, and east. In both the eastern and western regions there is clear cultural continuity from at least a thousand years before the historic period to that timemarker (Milanich 1978:61). In the eastern region, in fact, there is rather clear continuity from Mt. Taylor times (ca. 4000—2000 B.C.) through St. Johns LIE times (A.D. 1513—65). Such material culture continuity, while also implying a considerable degree of continuity in nonmaterial culture traits and possible in situ population as well, does not necessarily imply that the Timucua language was indigenous to either the eastern (especially) or western Timucua regions. We will look at the latter statement in considerable detail later. It may be pointed out, as Gatschet first noted in 1880 (p. 465), that the lexical base of the Timucua language is not genetically related to any language "spoken in the neighborhood of its native soil." A thorough examination of the vocabularies of not only neighboring languages but also of all documented North American languages, stocks, and phyla shows no convincing relationship nor even large-scale borrowing of lexical forms. There are, as would be expected, a small number of Muskogean borrowings, some from probable Proto-Muskogean times (ca. 2000—1500 B.C.), most from later times. There is no broader Gulf, Algonquian, Siouan, Iroquoian, Coahuiltecan, or other affiliation, either lexically or grammatically. From a native North American point of view, Timucua is a linguistic anomaly. It is of some interest to note that there is also a very important archaeological anomaly at approximately the 2500—1500 B.C. time level in what Goggin, Milanich, and Deagan have called the Eastern Timucua region. This is the rather sudden appearance of ceramic wares in a Late

Amazonian Origins of Timucua

203

Archaic context in the St. Johns and Savannah River areas. The wares, the earliest known in North America, while showing some similarity in shape and function to earlier steatite wares, show, as James Ford (1969) has pointed out, difficult-to-explain detailed trait similarities and nearidentities to wares from the northwestern Caribbean coast o f Colombia. As Ford indicates, it is hard to rationalize the independent invention o f such a complex artifactual system on the Archaic base of the southeastern United States, particularly since no gradual developmental sequence from that base to the subsequent ceramic base has so far been discovered. It may not appear completely "full-blown," but the archaeological impression is close to that. I am, obviously, implying that the two anomalous systems—the Timucua language and the archaeologically defined Orange/Stallings Island cultures—may have more in common than meets the eye. Radiocarbon dates and the time of arrival of Timucua speech in North America, as indicated by the few Proto-Muskogean words in the language, both point to the 2000—1500 B.C. time line. Such problems of origin have been studiously avoided in all recent summaries and detailed statements on Florida archaeology, almost as though they were not problems (Milanich and Fairbanks 1980, for example). While we fully agree that origin problems are always difficult of solution, it is nonetheless now possible to form an intelligent, testable hypothesis in the light of our current linguistic and archaeological data. Such a hypothesis will be suggested in the last section of this chapter.

Linguistic Components of Timucua and Their Origins BACKGROUND

Crawford has summarized the history of the linguistic investigation of Timucua from Brinton (1858) to the time of his own writing (Crawford 1979). As he points out, attempts have been made to connect Timucua genetically to a wide range of North and South American languages, all to no avail. Even a cursory examination of Timucua language data makes it abundantly clear that one is dealing with an unusual system. Lexically an isolate, it nonetheless contains a fairly large number of forms stemming from several quite readily discernible sources. Grammatically, it fits a well-known areal model in a single particular part of the Americas and conforms es-

204

Distant Relationships

pecially well to one segment of that model still in existence today. Yet no one source can explain the totality of the system. There is no known dominant contributor. The language has all the expectations of what anthropologists and linguists refer to as a creolized system, and it is probably for this reason that attempts to find the source of Timucua linguistically have been fruitless. To put it another way, the lack of success in assigning Timucua to a single genetic sourcc is a piecc of positive data in itself. The language has no single provenience, in either space or time. One must, rather, talk of the process of ethnic and linguistic formation. The sources of Timucua are clearly multiple, as we shall see. They are partly definable, partly not. Our problem is to identify the sources and estimate the frequency and importance of each component's contribution to the overall system. We may then be able to arrange the components in some kind of logical temporal and spatial framework. If we have difficulty in finding a dominant lexical contributor to Timucua, we have less trouble in finding a primary grammatical contributor. The basic patterns of Timucua grammar conform rather closely to MacroChibchan. The closest similarities are, on the one hand, to the Warao isolate of the Orinoco Delta in far western Venezuela, tentatively assigned by Greenberg to the Paezan stock within Macro-Chibchan (Greenberg i960), and, on the other hand, to Cuna, a member of the Chibchan stock proper within the macrophylum. The specific similarities will be examined in detail later in this chapter. There arc also individual morphemes and lexemes that bear striking resemblances to modern Warao, as well as an even larger number of lexemes with equally striking resemblances to languages of the VaupesCaqueta-Inirida-Guaviare branch of Northern Maipuran Arawakan. The number and close correspondence of nominal and verbal prefix and suffix morphemes with those of Warao is noteworthy—44 percent of Warao noun suffixes and 17 percent of Warao verb suffixes have Timucua parallels or identities. Floyd Lounsbury (Rouse 1986:121) has suggested that the similarities, particularly to Warao, "are neither numerous nor strong enough to eliminate independent invention or the transmission of linguistic norms from one local population to another through trade or other means of intercommunication." While it is certainly the case that the number of resemblances is small, certain peculiarities of the morphemic structure and semological

Amazonian Origins of Timucua

205

usage would, in my opinion, mitigate Lounsbury's suggestion of independent invention. The peculiarities are shared only by seventeenth-century Timucua and modern Warao; they do not occur in other neighboring languages in either the southeast or Venezuela, nor, for that matter, in any other native South or North American language. To attribute such a situation to independent invention is, I think, greatly stretching a point. On the other hand, attribution of such similarities to borrowing as a result of various kinds of intercommunication, particularly if the common elements are not dominant in one of the languages, is not only feasible but quite likely. The picture is not a simple one, but the multiple sources of Timucua lexicon and grammar point to a long proccss of creolization. Such a process can, of course, take many forms. When one culture and language is politically dominant, one usually sees large-scale relexification, as in the case of Anglo-Saxon after the Norman Conquest in 1066. If total military subjugation and fragmentation of the nondominant population is involved, as was the case in the Atlantic slave trade, almost total relexification and large-scale grammatical restructuring of the nondominant language(s) usually occurs, the Caribbean Creoles serving as the classic case. If contact between cultures is largely economic, without extensive military or political domination on the part of one of the participants, as in the case of the development of Swahili, the basic lexicon of the less dominant language tends to remain largely intact but with lexical, morphemic, and syntactic borrowings from the more dominant participant. Timucua would seem to be an example of the last sort—peaceable economic contact over a long period of time leading to incorporation of lexical and grammatical structures from nonnative sources. While the nonnative contact source for the Bantu peoples of East Africa was largely unitary—the Perso-Arabic dialects of southern Arabia—the contact sources for Timucua seem to have been multiple: at least one or more Waroid languages, Northwestern Chibchan, Northern Maipuran Arawakan, and Muskogean, the latter through a 3,000-year time period. The uniqueness of Timucua is that no single dominant source for the language can be identified. A primary lexical source of unknown origin, clearly not Waroid, is countered by a primarily Waroid grammatical source. None of the lexical contributors can be considered dominant in any sense. The only similar case of which I am aware is that of the Mbugu language of northern Tanzania (Goodman 1971). Mbugu, too, has a primary lexical

2o6

Distant Relationships

source of unknown origin, though its grammar is essentially Bantu. There are contributing lexical and grammatical elements from both Sudanic and Hamitic languages, neither of them dominant. The Timucua case is perhaps more amenable to solution than that of Mbugu, however, for we have considerable archaeological data with which to work, both in the Southeastern United States and in the AmazoniaNorthwestern South America regions; some of it is quite enlightening with regard to the possible formation of the Timucua language. STRUCTURE

The most striking accord between Timucua and Warao is that found in the morphemic structure of nouns. In both languages, a lexeme has nominal usage syntactically and semologically if the root morpheme, usually a free-form, is capable of taking the noun-pluralizing suffix -tooma (Timucua): -tuma (Warao). There are six additional affixes which either designate a lexeme as a noun or derive a structural noun of some particular type from another noun or other part of speech. The list here exhausts the noun-designating/deriving machinery of Timucua and represents six of a total of seventeen such affixes for Warao (see Osborn 1967b: 254). There is one additional member of this group of affixes which shows a similarity between the two languages. 'Long' occurs in Warao as the noun-deriving suffix -wari. In Timucua 'long' is indicated by the bimorphemic lexeme ihi-riwa. Metathesis might explain W. -wari: T. -riwa. That is, eight, or 44 percent, of Warao noun-designating/deriving affixes are mirrored by similar or identical forms in Timucua, accounting for 100 percent of the noun-designating/deriving machinery of that language.

Timucua

Warao

na-si 1 -ma 2 -ko -siwa -ma 3

-na -si -ma -ka -sebe -mo

nominalizer reflexive goal-marker definite goal-marker augmentative quantitative plural agent

nominalizer goal-marker goal-marker diminutive quantitative plural agent

The possessive affixes of both languages are also similar. Timucua 3S optionally, but with high frequency, adds the goal-deriving suffix -ma2, essentially a definite article. Homophonous -ma 1 and -ma2 behave accord-

Amazonian Origins of Timucua

207

ing to different rules of phonological alteration and are therefore considered separate morphotactic entities, indicated by the superscript notation. Both Timucua -ma,1 and Warao a- indicate 3S unless otherwise specified, but they may be used with any specified person, as in Timucua heka pahama 'our house' (heka 'we'), Warao oko amoko 'our hand(s)'. They are both general possession markers. As Osborn points out (his N = noun), "unless the person is otherwise specified a- + N may be glossed the-N or third-person's N" (Osborn 1967b: 258), indicating an identical semological function for Timucua -ma 1 (± -ma2) and Warao a-.

IS 2S 3S 1P 2P 3P

Timucua

Warao

-na -ya ~ -ye -0 —ma 1 (+ -ma2 > -mima) -na + ka > -nika -ya + -ka/-ke > -yaka/-yake -ma1 + -ka + -re + -ma2 > -mikarema

mahia- ~ hi0 — akayatu—

The -ka pluralizer of Timucua is also frequently used as a general noun pluralizer. When it is so used, it is normally followed by lexeme-final suffix -re, which has no counterpart in Warao. A -re suffix, however, does occur widely in Arawakan languages to indicate a possessed noun, a noun in "combining form," or a stative when affixed to nouns (Matteson 1972: 164). In Timucua, -re is used as a noun combining-form with both singular and plural possessive affixes on plural nouns only, as in pahana 'my house', pahanikare 'my houses' (-na + -ka > -nika), pahaye 'your (sg.) house', pahayekare 'your (sg.) houses', pahama or pahamima 'his house', pahamikare 'his houses' (-ma 1 + -ka > -mika). With plural possessive affixes, the same principle occurs: pahanika 'our house', pahanikakare 'our houses', pahayake 'your (pi.) house', pahayakekare 'your (pi.) houses', pahamikarema 'their house', pahamikaremakare 'their houses'. In actual practice, however, special stative verbal structures are more usually used to identify a plural possessive plural noun: hekapahamilekare 'our houses' ( = '3P pronoun + they are possessed houses'), pahamitilakare 'their houses' ( = 'they are ^P-posscssed houses'). Morpheme -re is rarely used to indicate morpheme combination except with nouns, always in the plural, the sole noted exception being its use in the 3P pronominal affix group -mikarema 'their'. The structure of lexical verbs in Timucua and Warao is also intrigu-

2O8

Distant Relationships

ingly similar. Of a total of fifty-nine Warao designative and derivational verbal affixes, ten, or 17 percent, occur in identical or very similar form and semological function in Timucua. In both languages, the verb is structurally defined by the presence or potential presence of the verb-pluralizing suffix -wo (Timucua): -bu (Warao). Other forms showing similarity are listed. As in the case of the nominal affixes, the verbal affixes and their semological descriptions have been taken from Osborn (1968:46—47, 62). Timucua

Warao

-hero -he -ku -ke -ta ~ -te

optative potential subjunctive optative durative

-no -ko ya -0

infinitive conditional negative intensive

-mehere -buhu -ku — k u n a -ko -ta -ti -ne -kore -yana -u

desire, command potential subjunctive optative occurrence-momentaneous occurrence-durative gerundive conditional, simultaneous negative intensive

The -si nominal suffix, a goal-marker in both languages, also occurs with lexemic verbs in both languages to indicate "reflexive/reciprocal." Of a total of 32 verb-designative/derivative affixes in Timucua, 11, including -si, are similar to Warao forms—35 percent of the Timucua total. In addition to the morphemic similarities between sixteenth-century Timucua and twentieth-century Warao, there are striking resemblances in patterns of phonological vowel harmony. The concept itself is totally alien to the native languages of the American Southeast, but it is very prevalent in Amazonian languages, regardless of phylum affiliation. The specific patterns seen in Timucua and Warao are so similar both to one another and to those generally found in most Macro-Chibchan languages that they cannot be ignored. I hasten to add that it is of course realized that phonological processes may be similar or even identical in widely divergent languages without the slightest implication of genetic relatedness, and it is also recognized that we are here comparing two languages four hundred years apart in time. The resemblances are still striking. In both Timucua and modern Warao, the process of vowel harmony is morpheme specific. In Warao it affects directional prefixes of C V - shape when they occur immediately before a base. In Timucua it affects a finite

Amazonian Origins of Timucua

209

number of bases, usually kin terms, when they occur with immediately following pronominal suffixes and also with pronominal suffixes when they are followed by any other suffix. In both languages the change is regressive, affecting the first morpheme in the set only. In both languages it is only the last vowel of the first morpheme that changes. The changc is conditioned by the nature of the vowel in the second morpheme of the set, whether or not consonants intervene. These regressive substitutions may be summarized by saying that, in the circumstances just described, a low vowel as the last vowel of the first morpheme may not occur before a low vowel as the first (or frequently only) vowel of the second morpheme. Both Timucua and Warao have five vowel systems, but the arrangement differs in the two languages. In both languages /i/, /a/, and luJ arc high-front, low-ccntral, and high-back vowels, respectively. Vowels Id and /o/ in Warao are phonetically [E] and [o] ~ [a], [o] predominating statistically. In Timucua, the patterns of vowel harmony suggest that /e/ was [c] or [ae] and /o/ was [o] or [w]. The latter allophonic structure of /e/ and /o/ is typical of many Chibchan languages (such as Cuna; cf. Holmer 1946:185). In both languages, morpheme-final /e/ in the first of such a morphemeset is raised to /i/, with the exception that in Timucua /e/ before Id > /a/, as in itaye 'your father' (ite 'father' > ita + -ye 'your [sg.]'). In Warao, this rule means that Ce- directional prefixes become Ci- before bases with /a/ as their first vowel, as in sewiri 'to arrive by canoe' (se- + win + 'to paddle'), sinaka 'to fall down' (se- > si- + naka 'to fall'), temoi 'to blow on' (te- + moi 'to blow'), and tiahi 'to cut on' (te- > ti- + ahi 'to cut'). In Timucua, the process affects only base + pronominal suffix sets, since there are no pronominal suffixes except variant -ye of -ya 'your (sg.)' which contain Id before /e/, /a/, or /o/; for example, itina 'my father' (ite > iti -I- -na 'my'). The latter may be contrasted with a non-kin base, such as paha 'house', as in pahana 'my house'. In the sole instance in which the Id—Id combination occurs in Timucua, itaye 'your father' (ite > ita + -ye 'your [sg.]'), Id > /a/, as indicated earlier. In Timucua, low vowel /a/ is also replaced by /i/ before another low vowel, as in itimile 'it is his father' (ite > iti + -ma1 > -mi 'his, the' + -le 'it is'), pahamkare 'my houses' (paha + -na > -m 'my' + -¿a'plural' -I- -re 'noun combining-form suffix'), and pahamino 'it is his house' (paha + -ma2 > -mi 'his, the' + -no 'it is'). There arc no other kin-term bases nor pronominal suffixes that end in lol so it is impossible to say what /o/—/e/, /o/-/a/, /0/-/0/ would have become in Timucua.

2io

Distant Relationships

It is, for both languages, tempting to postulate an earlier period in which specific kinds of morpheme sets containing a low vowel as the last vowel of the first member of such sets altered before any second morpheme that also contained a low vowel as its initial vowel, regardless of whether there were intervening consonants. The normal substitution was to raise the initial morpheme's final low vowel to /i/ except in combinations of Id—Id, where in Timucua, at least, the first Id becomes /a/. The suggestion is unarguably circular in that we have defined Timucua phonetic vowels to accommodate the system—/e/: [E] ~ [a;], lol: [ O] ~ [OJ). However, such an accommodation yields regular predictions and also has precedent in Cuna and other Chibchan languages. There are also structural similarities between Timucua and non-Waroid Macro-Chibchan languages. By far, the greatest degree of similarity is with Cuna, a Chibchan language of Panama. Noun and verb-pluralizing suffixes containing a bilabial stop, nasal, spirant, or semivowel—/p/, /b/, /m/, /b/, /p/, or Iwl—followed by central /a/ or back lol or lul are quite frequent in Chibchan and Paezan languages (Wheeler 1972). Cuna, for example, uses -(r)pa(a), -ma (with verbal suffix -la), and -pi(i) to indicate plurality in both verbs and nouns, as in uce 'hot' and ucepa(a) 'much heat', uamala 'there are many fish' (ua 'fish' + -ma + -la 'there are'), sanpi(i) 'all meat', penamala 'you all are going' (pe- 'vou' + -na- 'go' + -ma + -la 'verbalizer'). This usage is quite congruent with Timucua -wo and -ma3 verb pluralizers, described earlier. Timucua has a lexeme puk"'a (morphemically pu- + -k"'a 'intensifier') indicating "much, many," similar in usage to the Cuna form -(r)pa(a). Both languages, like Warao, have a -ka plural. In Timucua, this is normally combined with -re, in Cuna with -na, as in Cuna niiskana 'stars', Timucua cuwowokare 'stars'. Cuna uses -la with nouns to indicate the result of a verbal action and -le with verbs to indicate a passive participle. Timucua uses both suffixes with nouns to indicate verbalization, as in itmile 'he is my father' (ite > iti 'father' + -na > -ni 'my' 4- -le 'it is'). With verbs, Timucua -lal-le indicates 'immediate action,' as in bontala 'I am' (ho- 'I' 4- -ini- > -n- 'be' + -ta 'durative' + -la 'immediate action'), paralleling Cuna uamala 'there are many fish' and penamala 'you all are going'. We have earlier referred to the -ma morpheme and indicated that it fills three tactical slots: -ma1 '3S', -ma,2 'definite goal-marker, the', and -ma1 '3P verbal subject'. The semological functions of -ma> and -ma1 arc shared with Warao; the scmological function of -ma 3 is shared with Cuna.

Amazonian Origins of Timucua

211

Cuna -ti derives verbs from nouns, as in soke 'to say', soketi 'the act of saying'. A suffix -tae indicates "habitual action." Timucua uses -ta to derive nouns from verbs, as in hewa 'speak', hewata 'the act of speaking'. It uses -tal-te as an indicator of "durative action" with verbs, as in the example hontala 'I am' used above. Perfect action in Cuna is indicated by -co, in Timucua by -cu. Present time is indicated in Cuna with -na, -la, or -ya; Timucua uses -lal-le, as pointed out earlier, to show 'immediate action.' In addition to the phonological similarity in the allophonic definition of the /e/ and /o/ phonemes in Timucua and Cuna, it is also of interest to note that Cuna form-initial, postjunctural vowels are preaspirated. Pareja's orthography for Timucua shows many pairs of lexeme variants in which an initial /h/ sometimes is used and sometimes is not, as in eka or heka 'wind', hiwa or iwa 'rain'. There are several structural similarities between Timucua and the Misumalpan Chibchan languages of Central America. Twahka, for example, uses pluralizing suffixes -ran and -pak, both meaning "abundance" (Conzemius 1929:79). Timucua uses independent lexemes ara and puk"'a with the same meaning. In Twahka, the morphemes in question are used largely with bases designating plants, while in Timucua ara and puk"'a are generally used only with bases designating plants or animals (but not humans). Timucua has some structural resemblances to Panoan languages. Greenberg (i960) assigned these languages to a Ge-Panoan-Cariban phylum, unrelated to Macro-Chibchan or Andean-Equatorial, but no regular sound correspondences link them to either Ge or Cariban (Rodrigues 1985:397). Key (1968) has shown a definite genetic relationship between Panoan and Tacanan. Though largely limited today to two regions—the Andean foothills and an area in northwestern Bolivia—with Brazilian/ Venezuelan Yanomama as a possible northeastern extension (Migliazza 1978; 1985:29), the Panoan tribes seem not to have had any great migratory tendencies at any time in their history (Lathrap 1970:81). They seem to have occupied their present areas throughout the past, the discontinuities between the two major Panoan-speaking regions representing Arawak migratory intrusion into the pre-Andine regions of far western Amazonia. The system of grammatical affixation of Chacobo (Ch.) shows the following similarities with Timucua (T.): T. -wo '1P/2P verb subject': Ch. -bo 'noun plural'; T. -weta 'for, with': Ch. -bita 'with'; T. oco 'behind' : Ch. -co 'behind'; T. ka 'here' : Ch. -ka(ya) 'here'; T. -ke 'optative' : Ch. -ki 'conditional'; T. -na 2 'durative': Ch. -na- 'to become'; T. -no1 'active': Ch. -no7o

212

Distant Relationships

'locative intransitive'; T. -no + -so 'active transitive' : Ch. -no7so 'locative transitive'; T. -so 'transitive' : Ch. -so 'transitive'. The base morpheme glossed as 'black' in both languages is also similar: T. cuku : Ch. ciki. Chacobo data are from Prost (1962). There arc, lastly, four Timucua affixes with structural parallels in Northern Maipuran Arawakan. All are monosyllabic affixes, and the similarities may therefore be fortuitous. They are: T. ha 'be in the future': Goajiro -he 'future time'; T. na- 'V > N': Island Carib -ni —ne 'V > N'; T. ni-lna- 'first person': Island Carib n-l-na 'first person'; and the form cited earlier, T. -re 'noun combining-form' : general Northern Maipuran -re 'noun combining-form'. We have already noted that Timucua na- 'V > N' is paralleled by Warao -na with the same scmological function. It is also possible that Timucua ha 'be in the future' is paralleled by Warao ha- 'must, have to, will' with the same function as the Timucua form (Osborn 1968: 54). That is, the similarities may simply reflect broadly generalized panAmazonian/pan-Chibchan patterns rather than specific Arawakan sources. There is only one structural similarity between Timucua and any of the Muskogean languages—Timucua ha 'be in the future' and Choctaw -he 'future time'. Again, the monosyllabicity of the form renders comparison rather meaningless in and of itself. VOCABULARY

In contrast to this overwhelmingly Macro-Chibchan/Waroid grammar is the non-Waroid, non-Chibchan basic vocabulary, only a small percentage of which can be traced unambiguously to anv source. Numbers and pronouns are often taken as indicators of the extent of outside influence on a language, since thev are normally resistant to change. Both systems tend to maintain an overall patterning inherited from genetic forebears, and intrusive borrowings are generally very obvious, as in the case of English independent pronoun borrowings from Scandinavian. A determination, therefore, of resemblances between the number or pronominal systems of several languages is likely to tell us something about genetic relatedness and borrowing. The number system of Timucua is particularly informative in this regard. Timucua cardinal numbers are: yaha ~ yakfa '1', yuca ~ yuksa '2', hapu '3', ceketa '4', marua '5', mareka '6', pikica '7', pikinahu '8', pekeceketa '9', tuma '10', cupi '100'. Cupi is certainly a borrowing from Muskogean cokpi- '100' (Alabama, Koasati, Creek), related to co-pi 'large'. Tuma '10' is related to the pluralizing morpheme -tooma, used also as a free lexeme with the meaning "all, total, complete." Hapu '3' is similar to Pre-Andine Mai-

Amazonian Origins of Timucua

213

puran hepu '2' (Canamari), though pa occurs as a component of many numbers in a large range of Arawakan languages. Ceketa '4' bears a resemblance to Muskogean osta-ka '4' (Alabama, Koasati). 'Five' marua is similar to Paezan Choco mare '5' in k"'imare '4' ( = '1 from 5'), though the mare form itself is not presently used for '5' in Choco (Loewen 1963:366). A possible etymology for Timucua marua is -mir- 'all, complete' as in amiro, mirika 'all' plus hue ~ we 'hand' > mir-hue > marua 'complete hand = 5'. The same -mir- > mar- element is found in mareka '6', which might be taken to mean 'all 4- 1'. If this is the case, then one would expect that '7' should be 'all + 2', something like maruca or maruksa, which, however, do not occur. Pikica, the form for '7', though, does have the final syllabic -co, which could well be the -ca of yuca '2'. The possible morpheme piki~ peke- in that case, needs an explanation. It is presumably the same pikithat occurs in pikinahu '8' and pekeceketa '9'. Pekeceketa certainly contains peke- plus ceketa '4', leading one to assume that piki- ~ peke- means '5' or 'hand' or some similar concept. Pikinahu then, should mean '5 + 3' or 'hand + 3', -nahu an aberrant but not impossible variant of hapu. Pikica '7' should mean '5 + 2', which, as suggested earlier, is indeed possible. This somewhat circuitous etymologizing would lead us to assume a meaning of '5' or "hand" for the putative morpheme piki— peke-, occurring as a bound form only in these three numerals. The overall pattern of the Timucua cardinal number system is one in which there are separate morphemes for 1 - 4 , a term for 5 meaning 'complete hand' or something similar, and, for numbers 6 - 9 , words meaning 'hand + 1, 2, 3, 4'. 'Ten' in such systems is often, as in Timucua, a term meaning "total." The 'hand + ' system is not used in Arawakan nor in the native languages of the southeastern United States. It does, however, occur with frequency in the Chibchan and Paezan languages, particularly those of the Colombia-Panama border region. The Cuna cardinal numerals, for example, are: k"ena '1', po(o) '2', pa(a) '3', pakke '4', attale '5', nerk"a (lei > ner 'head' + k"a '1' = 'head + 1') '6', kukle '7', paapakka '8', pakkepakka '9', ampeki '10'. Ordinals are formed by adding -k"'a '1' to the cardinals, as in paak"'a 'third'. The Cuna term for '5', attale, means "hand on head"—atta 'hand' + le(l) 'head'—a typical Chibchan '5'-term, found also in the Misumalpan languages of Central America, as in Miskito matalal!matasip '5': mata 'hand' + lal 'head' or mata + sip 'complete'. In Ulua tin 'hand' = '5'. In the Cuna instance '6' is 'head + 1', as indicated above. 'Seven', kukle, is kuk- '2' + 'head'. In '8' and '9', however, we sec exactly the system used in Timucua: '8' = '3 + pakka\ '9' = '4 + pakka\ Just as in Timucua the

214

Distant Relationships

ordinary form for '5', mama, is not compounded to form higher numbers, so in Cuna attale is not used for that purpose. Instead a morpheme with obscure meaning, pakka, is used in such compounds. Just as we hypothesized a relic morpheme piki- ~ peke- '5/hand' in Timucua, so we can hypothesize a relic morpheme pakka '5/hand' in Cuna. In the Cuna system '8' = '3 + 5/hand' and '9' = '4 + 5/hand'. 'Ten', ampeki, means, like Timucua tuma, "complete, whole." While, in short, the phonological form and content of the Timucua cardinal numbers 1—4 shows no lexical similarity to any other native language of North or South America, the overall system shows remarkable similarities to that of Colombian-Panamanian Chibchan and Paezan, with extensions of that system well into Central America. The two Timucua '5' bases, mar- and piki— peke-, show close phonological correspondence as well as identical semological function and syntactic usage to Paezan Choco mare '5' and Chibchan Cuna pakka, respectively. Timucua independent pronouns show no similarity to any other specific language or language group except for the infrequently used 3S form oke, which is reminiscent of Arawakan, as in Campa oka 'this', and the 2P form yake, which is similar to modern Warao yatu. For comparative purposes, both the independent pronouns and the verbal pronoun affixes of T i m u c u a are given in the table.

IS 2S 3S IP 2P 3P

Independent pronouns

Verbal pronouns

ho-ni-he ho-ci-e 0, oke he-ka, ni-he-ka yake, ci-he-ka 0, oke-ka-re

ho-, nici0ni- + STEM + -wo ci- + STEM + -wo 0- + STEM + -ma 3

The remainder of the Timucua lexicon shows forms similar to Warao (or some earlier Warao-related language), Chibchan-Paezan, Tucanoan, Ge, Panoan, and the Muskogean languages. In a total surviving lexicon of approximately 1,500 roots, only 189 are sufficiently similar to be tentatively considered borrowings. Ninety-two percent of the Timucua lexicon, that is, shows no convincing relationship to any other native American language at the present stage of research. Each of these possible connections will be discussed in turn. It is difficult to assess the resemblances between Timucua and modern

A m a z o n i a n O r i g i n s of T i m u c u a

215

Warao i n a s m u c h as w e are c o m p a r i n g f o r m s f r o m languages almost f o u r h u n d r e d years apart. We are as yet u n f o r t u n a t e l y unable t o reconstruct an earlier stage of Warao, even f r o m internal evidence, a n d t h e equivalences w e are s u g g e s t i n g are o p e n t o criticism. We d o k n o w , however, f r o m t o p o nymic data that Warao-like languages were at o n e time spoken f r o m an i n d e t e r m i n a t e position o n t h e C a r i b b e a n coast of Colombia-Venezuela b e t w e e n Lake M a r a c a i b o and t h e Magdalena, east t h r o u g h t h e O r i n o c o Delta (Wilbert 1957:11—18). Speakers of these languages occupied t h e n o r t h e r n s e g m e n t of S o u t h America as far s o u t h as the A m a z o n i a n rain forest a n d t h e confluence of t h e R i o Vaupes a n d the R i o N e g r o . It is, o f course, clear t h a t T i m u c u a did n o t b o r r o w directly f r o m Warao o r its imm e d i a t e ancestors, t o j u d g e f r o m t h e lack of regular phonological correspondences. Nonetheless, t h e similarities seem close e n o u g h t o w a r r a n t discussion. T h e f o l l o w i n g T i m u c u a - W a r a o resemblances are particularly notew o r t h y . T h e y are given w i t h t h e T i m u c u a first: (I) itori : ruru-ruru ([duru-duru]) 'alligator' yuriko : oriki 'anger' (2) (3) alulu : atabu 'arrow' (4) utasi: ataihase 'attack' (5) aruk'i: araka 'child : younger brother' (6) na- : nao- 'come' w (7) kuk e : kuku- 'cover' hio : hoa 'curse' (8) (9) miso : misi- 'devil' (10) ipa(ru) : hobi 'drink' (») uti: hota 'earth : high land/ mountain' (12) ho : ho- 'eat' (13) eka : eku 'enter/in' (1+) eka : mon-uka 'equal' (15) asu-rupa : so 'excrement' (16) muku : mu 'eye' (17) amara : emoera ' f a t : to be soft' (18) warn : yiwara 'finish' (19) taka : rokia ([dokia]) 'fire : flame' (20) yawi: yaba- 'fishhook : to fish' (2.) hawe : ohia 'fox' (22) awara : ari 'harvest' (see also no. 18 'finish') (23) ha- : ha - 'have'

(24) (25) (26) (27) (28) (29) (3o) (3i) (32) (33) (34) (35) (36) (37) (38) (39) (40) (41) (42) (43) (44) (45)

okoto : noko 'hear' kume : kobe 'heart/breast' bono : noho 'hunger' ho-ni-he : ine 'I' nihi: na- 'kill' nahiawo : nahobo 'know/ understand' karo : o-kera 'light' hani : hahinai 'lose' howa/huwa : obo- 'love' wiro : arao 'man : people' sowa : toma 'meat' aku : waniku 'moon' nariwa : nibora 'old man : man' awi : aba 'put on : put' hono-sta : hunu 'shellfish : shrimp' hewa : ehewere 'sing : sing/cradle song' weni: abani 'slow' yorowa : ni-hara-baka 'snake : cayman' hani: ha- 'stop' tari: taera 'strong' aka/haka : haka 'wind' ak' era : aberu 'name of the southernmost Timucua border province : border/coast'

216

Distant Relationships

There are also lexical resemblances with Chibchan and Paezan languages: (1) (2) (5) (4) (5) (6)

itori : Chocó kore 'alligator' culufi : Timóte, Cuica cu 'bird' marna : Chocó mare 'five' na(ta) : Cuna naa- (< na-) 'go' hue!we : Chocó hua 'hand' ta-pola : Chocó pe, Chibcha aba, Manare epa 'maize' (7) wiro : Yanomamo warolwaru 'man'

(8) aku : Chocó (h)edexo, Sumu waiku, Cacaopera aiku, Matagalpa aiko 'moon' (also Chocó Saixa dialcct axo-nihino 'sun' : Timucua akunihino 'fiery moon') (9) nipita : Chocó itae 'mouth' (10) hiwa : Cuna iya 'rain' (11) cu-wo(wo) : Yaruro boé 'star' (12) itu-k-a : Sumu, Ulua tuke, Brunca -kra, Cuna wa-, Chibcha p-kwa, Manare kna 'tongue' (13) iwi(ne) : Yaruro wi, Esmeralda wivi 'water'

There are six interesting free-base parallels between Timucua and Proto-Tucanoan as reconstructed by Waltz and Wheeler (1972:119—48). Tucanoan languages are neither Macro-Chibchan nor Andean-Equatorial in Greenberg's classification. They were and are centered in the Rio Vaupés—Rio Negro confluence region of Venezuela, Colombia, and Brazil. The forms in question arc: (1) itori : *iSo(-ri) ( S = /s/~ /h/~/v/~ /d/) 'alligator/cavman/crocodile' ( -ri is a frequent Amazonian noundesignating affix) (2) isi : *zie 'blood'

(3) (4) (5) (6)

uku : *ükü 'drink' napona : *poa 'hair' hue/we : *wa 'hand' kecela : *kaserolkatsero 'skin/bark'

There is a single Timucua form—yaraha/hiyamwa 'panther'—that has possible parallels in the Ge languages. In Remokamekran "jaguar" is orobo, in Aponegikran oropa, in Kayapó rop. Eastern Ge (Xavantc) uses an alternate stem hu ~ ru. One is tempted to postulate a Timucua form utilizing both stems, hu-oroplba > (hi)yarahlwa. The very ingenuity of the etymology, however, gives one reason to pause. There is also, it should be pointed out, a Pre-Andine Maipuran *In-yVra-ta form meaning 'kill', which is interestingly similar (Matteson 1972:179), and a Tupinambá form yawar 'jaguar' (Rodrigues 1985:391). Resemblances between Timucua and Arawakan have been noticed for many years. Swadesh (1964:548) has presented a tentative listing of such forms, though unfortunately his data were both inadequate and inaccurate, for he presents a fair number of erroneous forms for both Timucua

Amazonian Origins o f T i m u c u a

217

and the Arawakan languages. Nonetheless, his insight was correct, as the f o l l o w i n g list o f resemblances indicates: (1) kume : Proto-Pre-Andine *tsomi, Canamari tsuma ('back'), Culino tsuhuri 'breast/chest/heart' (2) cokolo : Chamicuro tulu, Lokono óloa 'heart5 (3) ike : Culino tsiki 'earth' (4) paha : Apolista pi-, Yamamadi ube, general Eastern Maipuran pa-, Goajiro pe-, Guahibo po-, Lokono bahi 'house' (5) nekero : ikubuti 'kneel' (6) wiro : Taino -eri, Piapoco -ali 'man' (7) pira : Paumari puru 'red : black (?)' _ (8) cokori : Culino kara- 'strong' (9) aye : Culina awa, Lokono oda 'tree' (10) yuwa : Canamari tsuma (see also no. 1 above) 'back' (11) icikosa : general Pre-Andine Maipuran kaci- 'cold' (12) uku : general Pre-Andine Maipuran -ika 'drink : eat' (13) taka : general Pre-Andine Maipuran titi, tak ('sun'), general Eastern Maipuran tike 'fire' (14) hue/we : Ipurinà. Apolista wa-, Piro we-, Lokono we-, Bare iva-, Achagua -he 'hand' (15) ilaki : Piro ilacinu 'night' (16) -ti : Campa -te 'not' (17) ka/oka : Campa oka 'this' (18) ora : Campa ora 'that' (19) hapu : Canamari bepii '3 : 2' (20) itori : *(iy)akare 'alligator/cayman/crocodile' (21) iciko : Baré nika 'bite : eat' (22) isi : Catapolitani iti, Marawa isa 'blood' (23) yawi : Bare nabi, Achagua yahe, Piapoco api 'bone' (24) tiki : Manao teki, Lokono -dike 'ear'

(25) (26) (27) (28) (29) (30) (31) (32) (33)

(34) (35) (36) (37) (38) (39) (40)

(41) (42) (43) (44) (45) (46) (47)

(48) (49) (50)

uti : Bare rati 'earth' ike : Baniva, Piapoco (h)ipe 'earth' he : Waliperi he, Achagua iha 'eat' ho : Island Carib háu 'eat' muku : Manao uku- 'eve' ite : Lokono if i 'father' huyu : Achagua ku- 'fish' isa : general Northern Maipuran isa 'good' penani : general Northern Maipuran -plbi 'with the hands : hand' kuna : Manao kiiuna 'head' cito : Piapoco, Catapolitani iwita 'head' nu- : Bare nu- 'know' cofa : Carútana cupana 'liver' tapóla : Guaná tsoporo, Terena soporo 'maize' ano : general Northern Maipuran ena- 'man' aku : Bare ki, Yavitero ke, Lokono kaci, Yamamadi maxi ('sun') 'moon' wali : Baniva tali, Lavaná bahalo 'mouth' hiwa : Bare hi-ya, Goajiro huyá 'rain ' ene : Machiguenga nea 'see' yorowa : Lokono òri 'snake' cuwowo : Piapoco, Baniva -wi'star' yowo : Achagua, Goajiro, Piapoco iba, Manao ipa, Bare tiba 'stone' eia : Achagua eri, Tariana, Piapoco -eri ('moon'), Amanzana eri- ('fire'), Arekena ale, Goakiro -ali 'sun/day' api : Tariana, Arekena, Carútana, Bare -ip/bi 'tail' iwi(ne) : Baré uni, Baniva-Yavitero, Maipuré went 'water' nia : Bare, Tariana, Piapoco, Achagua, Baniva ina- 'woman'

218

Distant Relationships

It may be noted that there are three primary Arawakan subgroups to which Timucua lexemes show resemblances: (i) Non-Maipuran (Culino, Chamicuro, Paumari, Yamamadi, Apolista), (2) Pre-Andine Maipuran (Canamari, Ipurina, Piro, Campa), and (3) Northern Maipuran (especially Bare, Piapoco, Baniva-Yavitero, Achagua, Carutana, Tariana—the VaupesInirida-Guaviare branch of Northern Maipuran). The implications of this will be examined in greater detail later in the chapter. Of the 151 Timucua forms showing resemblances to northern South American forms, fifteen show what may be called multiple-resemblance. That is, these forms show similarity to more than one stock or phylum. The greatest degree of similarity is with the Arawakan languages. Inasmuch, however, as all the Amazonian languages, regardless of stock or phylum affiliation, borrowed very heavily from one another beginning in very early times, it is difficult to assign a clear-cut origin to such putative loans into Timucua. The intense riverine trade of the entire Amazon region since time immemorial has tended to blur many language differences, grammatical as well as lexical, within and between phyla (Migliazza 1985:20), and it is accordingly nearly impossible to define with any degree of reliability the exact origin of most of the pan-Amazonian lexemes. In the list below, the Arawakan forms are from Noble (1965), Matteson (1972), or Taylor (1977); Proto-Tucanoan forms are from Waltz and Wheeler (1972); Warao forms are from Osborn (1967a, 1967b, 1968) and Barral (1957). Other forms, largely Macro-Chibchan, are from Loukotka (1968), Rodrigues (1985), and my own notes. Gloss

Timucua Arawakan

Tucanoan Warao

Other

'alligator' 'blood' 'drink' 'earth' 'eat'

*(iy)akare iti/isa -ika ('eat') -atu hau he/ha -uku tak-/tikwe-/wa*tsomi tsoporo -eri/-ali

*iSo(-ri) *zie *ükü

vakare (Tupí)

-axi óri -wiweni/uni

6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11.

'eve' 'fire' 'hand' 'heart' 'maize' 'man'

itori isi uku uti ho he muku taka hue/we kume tapóla wiro

12. 13. 14. 15.

'moon' 'snake' 'star' 'water'

aku yorowa cuwowo iwi(ne)

1. 2. 3. 4. 5.

[duru-|

hota homu [dokia] *wâ

hua (Chocó) kobe arao waniku -hara-

pe (Chocó) waro (Yanomama) oi(Yaruro) axo- (Chocó) boé (Yaruro) wi (Yaruro)

Amazonian Origins of Timucua

219

It may be noted that Timucua resemblances to Arawakan, unlike those to Warao and the Macro-Chibchan languages, seem to cover at least two fairly specific semantic domains: (1) natural phenomena and animal forms; and (2) body parts. One of the most obvious and striking characteristics of lexemes in the Arawakan languages and in many other Amazonian language groups is what Matteson (1972:164) refers to as a "clutter of grammatical markers," lexemes consisting of long strings of predominantly monosyllabic morphemes, many of which do not naturally occur alone with a clear denotative semantic content but which have clear semological connotative meaning (that is, a broad semantic "freight" and consistent grammatical function). Some of the most difficult to analyze of these bound-bases occur at the beginning of lexemes and, as Noble (1965:27—35) pointed out quite accurately, usually have the phonological form t(a)-y k-, £-, m-, n-, x- ( = /h/), a-, i/y- and u-. He unfortunately referred to these forms as "class prefixes," immediately calling to mind a situation similar to that in the Bantu languages and others in which genuine class prefixes are a dominant grammatical fixture. This, one quickly finds, is distinctly not the case in Arawakan. In some cases such "prefixes" do not seem to be independent, separable morphemes at all but simply phonological components of freebase root morphemes. Even in cases in which we clearly are dealing with separable, prefixlike morphemes, they do not—in any instance of which I am aware—define clear-cut or even broad semantic or semological classes, as in the Bantu languages. It seems clearly more realistic, as Taylor and HofF (1966:305) have pointed out, to consider such morphemes "petrified affix" components of frozen compounds in which the first element has become a nonproductive morpheme in the language. Such "petrified affixes" seem to occur not always lexeme-initial, as Noble implies, but also, in complete agreement with the norms of Arawakan lexeme structure, in varying lexeme-internal position (Matteson 1972:163), as morpheme ha 'liquid' in *yi-ha-ki-l"e 'eye', *ka-ha~re 'lake', *ha-po-e-ni-ha 'river', *ka-si-ha 'sap', *w/bini/i-ha'water', *pa-ka-ha-ka'wet', *po-pin-ha-n'dry (Matteson 1972: 176-85). In my view, this in no way invalidates Noble's valuable work; it simply means that much more careful phonological, grammatical, and comparative work is needed before we will be able to iron out the problems of lexeme-initial "petrified" bound-morpheme definition versus ordinary lexeme-initial regular phonological correspondences. Certainly the wide use of morphemes such as pi 'rod-shaped object' in words such as "vine," "finger"; of -pi in words for "furry, fuzzy objects" such as "bat,"

220

Distant Relationships

"bird," "body hair," "feather"; of tsa 'cord' in words for "rope," "vine," "hair"; of ke/ki 'pole or sticklike objects' in words for "crocodile," "fish," "leg," "neck," "spine," "stick" (Matteson 1972:163-64) does seem to point to a former system of word-class definition similar to that of Bantu and many other language groups throughout the world. As Taylor and H o ft point out (1966:304), "In some cases at least, Noble is undoubtedly right as to the one-time morphemic status of some svnchronically immovable segments." To decide which are and which are not "petrified affixes" and then to determine which, if any, of these affixes arc indeed class-designating is the unresolved problem. In any event, such lexeme-initial "entities" also occur in Timucua, but only in those lexical items that show a similarity to one branch or another of Arawakan, such as Timucua ta-pola 'maize', Arawakan (Guana) tso-poro 'maize', Arawakan (Terena) so-poro 'maize', quite possibly containing the Arawakan tsa 'cord' morpheme, widely used in words for "hair" and other objects with long or twisted fibers (such as corn silk?). The fact that there is a widely recurring pe 'maize' morpheme in many languages of Amazonia and the neighboring regions (Choco pe 'maize', for example), perhaps related to or identical with the Arawakan pi 'rod-shaped object' cited above, reinforces the suggestion. The Timucua lexeme-final -lo is quite conceivably related to the Guana and Terena -ro, probably the widely recurring Arawakan -rV "noun-combining form" morpheme. Timucua also shows a small number of lexical similarities to the Muskogean languages of the southeastern United States. These can clearly be called loans. They can be derived from one of two sources: (A) ProtoMuskogean, as reconstructed by Mary Haas (1940, 1941, 1945, 1946, 1947, 1948, 1949, 1950, 1956, i960), or (B) the historically known later Muskogean languages, particularly those of the western section of Eastern Muskogean—Alabama and Koasati. (A) PROTO-MUSKOGEAN

(1) koco : *kac- 'cut' (2) puenlpon- : *(x"u/hu)pun- 'go/ come: go'

(3) (4) (5) (6)

tiri : *akwi 'kill' ukwa : *ik~. . .-«'not' huhe : *umkwairain' neka : *nuci- 'sleep'

(B) LATER MUSKOGEAN

(1) culufi : Koasati kulu-si 'bird' (2) kaya : Alabama aka-ka 'chicken' (3) laka : Alabama, Koasati loca 'black/dirty' (4) efa : Koasati ifa 'dog' (cf. also Proto-Arawakan *ife- 'capvbara')

(5) ukucua : Alabama, Koasati okhica 'door'

(6) ipa-ru : Koasati ipa 'eat'

Amazonian Origins of Timucua

221

(7) muku : Choctaw mucu-(li) 'eye' (8) neha : Koasati niha 'fat' (9) taka : Alabama, Koasati tikba, Creek to-tka 'fire' (10) ceketa : Alabama, Koasati osta-ka 'four' (11) oke/oka : Alabama ak- 'this/that' (12) cupi: Koasati, Hitchiti, Creek cokpi '100' (13) isa : Koasati iski 'mother' (14) aya : Choctaw aiya 'mountain' (15) nipita : Choctaw itopa 'mouth' (16) -ti: Alabama, Koasati -ti 'not' (17) eye : Choctaw ayi- 'road' (18) apt : Alabama, Koasati, Choctaw hapi 'salt' (19) uwa : Alabama, Koasati opahk'swim' (20) aye : Hitchiti ahi 'tree' (21) aliho- : Koasati cayahli 'walk' (22) nali-sono : Alabama, Koasati la-na, Creek la-n-i 'yellow' (23) ho-ei-e : Choctaw cilcia 'you'

It should also be noted that the Timucua form Hake 'night', already compared with Pre-Andine Piro ilacinu 'night', bears a strong resemblance to Proto-Muskogean *niNaki 'night' (*N = Western Muskogean/n/, Eastern Muskogean /I/). Finally, there arc two Timucua items with resemblances to lexical forms in non-Muskogean languages of the southeastern United States: (1) mike : Atakapa mo-k- 'go'; and (2) lyola : Natchez ula 'snake'. The resemblances discussed above, it should be borne in mind, account for only 8 percent of the surviving Timucua lexicon. The remaining 92 percent still resists any effort of comparative treatment.

Interpretation Structural and lexical data, then, indicate that Timucua probably originated as a native language of northwestern Amazonia. While it does not seem in a lexical sense genetically related to any of the languages of Greenberg's Andean-Equatorial or Macro-Chibchan phyla, its grammar is quite

222

Distant Relationships

clearly Waroid-bascd. We know from toponymic data that both Waraoproper as well as Warao-related languages were spoken from an indeterminate time in the past until at least the time of Spanish intervention all along the Caribbean littoral of northern South America, from somewhere to the west of Lake Maracaibo east to and including the Orinoco Delta (Wilbert 1957). Waroid toponyms occur with some frequency as far south in Colombia as the Meta, Caqueta, and Vaupes region and as far south in Venezuela as the northern banks of the Orinoco. The toponymic evidence pointing to a western origin for the Warao is reinforced by the nature of their cult practices, with startling Central and Meso-American parallels, and by their own oral traditions of such an origin (Wilbert 1972,1973). The presence in Timucua of Cuna-like noun- and verb-designating and deriving morphemes would argue for Timucua contact with these Chibchan peoples somewhere in the area of northwestern Colombia or the Colombia-Panama border region. The small number of root morphemes showing resemblances to both Cuna and Choco, on the one hand, and Yaruro, on the other, would stretch the possible contact region from the Colombia-Panama border area southeastward to Yaruro lands in southcentral Venezuela. The presence of even the small number of resemblances to nonMaipuran Arawakan may argue for possible contact with pre-Maipuran Arawak speakers, perhaps as early as 3500 B.C., if we follow Noble and Lathrap (Noble 1965:111, Lathrap 1 9 7 0 : 7 0 - 8 1 ) . This would presumably have taken place somewhere along the upper reaches of the R i o Negro, near its confluence with the Amazon, prior to the time of the development o f M a i p u r a n innovations (Lathrap 1 9 7 0 : 7 0 - 7 9 ) . That the Timucua remained in contact with Arawakan speakers for a long period of time in the R i o Negro region is attested by the number of lexical similarities to Pre-Andine Maipuran and later Northern Maipuran. The time level for the split of Maipuran from general Arawakan can be tentatively placed at about 2000—1500 B.C., perhaps earlier (Noble 1965: hi). Certainly, the Maipuran languages were themselves developing from Proto-Maipuran by 1800 B.C., and Proto-Maipuran had probably developed dialect variants leading ultimately to the present-day Maipuran languages by about 1700 B.C. The remainder of the Arawakan-Timucua resemblances clearly indicate Northern Maipuran contacts. Owing, however, to the imperfect phonological fit with extant Northern Maipuran, it is evident that such putative Timucuan borrowings from these sources came not from the individual

Amazonian Origins of Timucua

223

languages as they exist today, but from a period when Proto-Northern Maipuran was in its early developmental stages, a pre-Proto-Northern Maipuran. This would be approximately 1900—1700 B.C. That is, Timucua speakers seem to have been in relatively close and constant contact with Arawak speakers from Proto-Arawakan times (ca. 3500 B.C.) until approximately 1700 B.C., the time by which the different Maipuran branches had developed essentially their present characteristics. The area of contact must have been the middle Rio Negro, Vaupes, middle Japura-Caqueta region, shown circled on the map in Figure 2. The major possible Arawak donors to the Timucua lexicon are still today located in this region, though in much altered guise after the passage of so many years. The area in question is just on the southern and eastern edges of both Macro-Chibchan speech and the earliest distribution of Waroid speech. All the essential ingredients to explain Timucua lexical and grammatical peculiarities lie within this area. Proto-Tucanoan similarities in Timucua reinforce the Arawakan evidence. Tucano is today the lingua franca of the Vaupes region (Sorensen 1967,1985), and multilingualism along the middle and lower Vaupes is the norm among native speakers of Tucanoan and Arawakan languages, particularly the Tariana, Bare, and Baniva. Most of these peoples speak their own language as well as Tucano and the Tupi lingoa geral Nheengatu, though the latter is less frequent in some parts of the region. The only exception are the Maku, of undetermined language affiliation (Migliazza 1985:52—54). This situation seems to have been in place since pre-European times, though its epicenter was likely the lower Vaupes in aboriginal times (Sorensen 1967). Tribal/language exogamy is the norm in this region, and members of a given longhouse group grow up speaking a mother tongue, a father tongue, and two or more other Indian languages with equal fluency. Nheengatu and/or Spanish and/or Portuguese are usually added to this list. Each language is reserved for use under specific circumstances and with specific individuals for whom the language in question is a native tongue. Ease in language-learning is a cultural trait into which each new member of the society is born. Multilingual, polyglot circumstances are not viewed with the sense of awe (and peculiarity) they engender in Western European and Euro-American societies. Because the use of each language is carefully prescribed, there is little if any lexical or grammatical mixing by the users, and there is no indication of incipient creolization in any of the languages.

Amazonian Origins of Timucua

225

Timucuan resemblances to Proto-Tucanoan rather than to some later form of the language indicates a Timucua presence in the critical area certainly by 2500 B.c. With regard to the possibility that Timucua may have developed in a region in which multilingualism has been a dominant characteristic for millennia, it is of interest to note that there are many lexical pairs or triplets in Timucua in which one variant is of native origin (that is, origin unknown), one showing Waroid, Tucanoan, or Arawakan resemblance, and another of southeastern United States origin: (1) 'snake' (a) elatuwasa—origin unknown, (b) yorowa : Lokono ori, (c) iyola : Natchez ula; (2) 'strong' (a) tari : Warao taera, (b) cokori : Culina kara-; (3) 'eat' (a) ho : Warao ho-, (b) he : Waliperi he, (c) ipa-ru : Koasati ipâ; (4) 'skin' (a) kecela : Proto-Tucanoan *kasero/katsero, (b) pet''a—origin unknown. Multiple- and variant-lexical resemblances rarely occur under conditions of cultural contact in which one member is considered, for whatever reason, "lower" and the other dominant or "upper." The latter situation usually results in a considerable degree of relexification of the nondominant language, the dominant language remaining unscathed. We have in Timucua a situation most closely paralleling what Morris Goodman referred to as linguistic interpénétration in the case of the Mbugu language of Tanzania (Goodman 1971:252). It is the same phenomenon noted by Cestmir Loukotka years ago in his classification of native South American languages, one which he called simply "mixed languages" (Loukotka 1968:13). He found it very characteristic of South American languages in general and of Amazonian tongues in particular. Most professional linguists looked with something akin to horror on Loukotka's pedestrian term, but it is quite apt at a certain descriptive level. There is in Amazonia a very broad common lexical substratum running through all the languages of the equatorial region, regardless of family, stock, or even phylum. This is certainly the result of widespread trade, population movement (temporary or permanent), intermarriage (as in the Vaupés region's preferred language-exogamy), and other factors over an extremely long period of time. The lexical result is a very real "mixing" in which the grammars peculiar to given languages, stocks, and phyla remain surprisingly stable and enduring. Mixing or interpénétration, as distinct from relexification, takes place as the result of particularly intense but peaceful long-term cultural contacts in which no single party is culturally dominant. In the development of pidgins and true, rclexified Creoles, it is universally the norm that morpho-

226

Distant Relationships

logical complexity is drastically reduced and irregularities are regularized. This is usually not so with "mixed" languages. Timucua, while stemming from multiple sources, seems a case of mixing or interpénétration. The primary lexical parent(s), possibly an independent stock within Macro-Chibchan or Pano-Tacanan, perhaps not, has remained intact. If Timucua's closest genetic relatives are the Waroid languages, then its grammatical structure is also largely intact. If its grammatical structures were taken from Waroid neighbors, its own original structures must have been very close indeed to the borrowed ones— perhaps resulting from pan-areal structural similarities (cf. Migliazza 1985:20), though this does not seem to be the case. What relexification has taken place, if that is what it is, is so slight that it is questionable that the term should be used at all with regard to Timucua. A mere 8 percent of the lexicon was affected, and many of the "borrowings" were additions to, not replacements of, native terms. It is difficult to assess the exact nature of the contact between the early Timucua, Tucano, Arawak, Macro-Chibchan, and Panoan peoples that could have led to the situation we see in seventeenth-century Timucua. We have nothing in the way of archaeological data from the Vaupés-Caquetâ region to help us out and very little in the way of carefully done ethnographic descriptions or ethnohistorical documentation. We are, however, fortunate in having considerable valuable ethnohistorical and archaeological data from areas to the north, west, and, to a lesser extent, south of the Timucua epicenter. Of particular value arc the data from the archaeologically defined Intermediate Area, encompassing Ecuador, Colombia from the Eastern Cordillera west and north to the Pacific and Caribbean, and most of Central America south of Guatemala. This important crossroads region between North and South America has recently begun to receive the archaeological attention it deserves, and specialists now realize both the critical nature of its geographic position and the highly civilized energies of its aboriginal inhabitants. Feldman and Moseley have recently pointed out that the peoples and cultures of the Intermediate Area were frequently the donors, not just the half-way-point recipients, of some extremely important cultural innovations (Feldman and Moseley 1978:139—77). It is, however, of equal importance to realize that this region did serve as the point from which Middle American innovations spread into South America and from which South American innovations spread to Middle and North America.

Amazonian Origins of Timucua

227

The Intermediate Area is circumscribed by the Pacific on the west, Honduras to the northwest, the Caribbean on the north, and the Colombian Eastern Cordillera to the east. It is from the flanks of the latter that most of the rivers of eastern Colombia and western Venezuela arise, including the Apure, Casanare, Meta, Guaviare, Inirida, Vaupes, and Caequeta-Japura, emptying their increasing torrents ultimately into the Orinoco, Rio Negro, and the Amazon itself. The rivers served, as they do today, as vast networks for commerce, linking rather than separating regions in northwestern South America and leading directly to the Goajira Peninsula coast in the northwest and the Orinoco Delta in the northeast as logical receiving points from outside and as transporting points to that outside. It has long been known, from archaeological and ethnohistorical data, that movement of peoples in the northern third of the continent has been largely by water. Accounts from early European travelers support this contention. Archaeological data certainly substantiate the notion for Arawakan speakers from the earliest times (Lathrap 1973, Rouse 1986). We know that the distances traveled for trading purposes alone were incredibly vast—Wallace noted in the late 1800s that the Vaupes region peoples regularly traveled back and forth to the Upper Amazon to sell cassava graters (Wallace 1889:336), and Schomburgk, some forty years earlier, had noted that the Waika traveled regularly to Colombia and Brazil to barter for dogs (Schomburgk 1847:198). Journeys from the Guianas to and from southeastern Colombia were not exceptional (Roth 1924:632-37). Feldman and Moseley point out that, at the time of Spanish intervention, there were four major trading networks in the northern third of South America (Feldman and Moseley 1978:142). Two of these were largely limited to the Andes region and moved goods in the north-south direction. The other two, however, centered in east-west movement of goods in Ecuador, Colombia, the southern Andes, and Amazonia. The early Spanish noted these systems, stating that they were largely run and organized by distinct hereditary groups of merchants referred to as mindalaes (Salomon 1977/78:236). In the Colombian region, the mindala formed a group who stood apart from the usual community; they were exempt from taxation and tribute and were of high social rank—similar to the pochteca of Aztec Mexico (Feldman and Moseley 1978:143). The very term mindala is of interest, for it could readily be translated in Timucua as 'The Lords' or 'The Great

228

Distant Relationships

Ones' {mine 'lord, great' > mm- + -ta 'durative time' = [da] by automatic allophonic change after /n/ + -la 'immediate time' = 'Those Who are Lords'). In Amazonia, the Shipibo of Peru traveled as far as 1,600 km downstream to obtain suitable wood for blowguns (Feldman and Moseley 1978:145; Lathrap 1973:171-72). Such trade networks were not only in effect on the river systems, but also involved sea voyages of considerable distances—certainly from Ecuador and Colombia as far as Peru to the south and Guatemala and Mexico to the north (Borhcgyi 1959, Lathrap et al. 1975, Coe I960, Edwards 1965). Lathrap (1973) feels that our data justify an antiquity for such sea-trading of at least 1500 B.C., and it is not impossible that it existed as early as 3500 B.c. (Zeidler 1977/78:26). Trade of the nature described by the Spanish chroniclers and later European travelers, and substantiated by both archaeological data and oral tradition, is often accompanied by the development of special linguistic structures, such as Sabir (the lingua franca of the Mediterranean) or the many pidgins of the world. Normally such pidgins remain just that unless the contact is particularly intensive and long-lasting. In that case, a creolized speech-form usually develops, building on the grammar of the less dominant language with the vocabulary of the dominant one— relexification. If, however, long-term, intensive trade is largely in the hands of a professional trading class, such as the pochteca or mindala, and neither the language of the buyers nor the sellers is considered dominant socially or politically, then the usual result is that phenomenon we have defined as interpénétration, Loukotka's "mixed languages." The classic example is, of course, Swahili: Bantu in grammar and largely Bantu in lexicon, but with large infusions of Arabic and Perso-Arabic lexicon as well. Swahili has also developed grammatical peculiarities atypical of Bantu proper, largely noun-class and concordance phenomena, which have resulted from the new, Arabic lexical material. While much of such new lexical material defines concepts new to the receiving culture (Swahili kitabu 'book'—from Arabic; meza 'table'—from Portuguese, for example), in many cases new, alternate lexemes for familiar concepts are added to account for the use of the concept in a new social situation (Swahili amkia '(to) greet'—native Bantu salimu '(to) greet'—from Arabic)—a familiar situation in Timucua, as we have seen. It is our suggestion that Timucua, as we see it in the late sixteenth and early seventeenth centuries, is such a creolized language. Given the

Amazonian Origins of Timucua

229

lack of full-scale rclcxification, the assumption would be that the language resulted from long-term development and use by a professional trading class, such as the Perso-Arabic traders or the pochteca. Its original genetic base was probably Macro-Chibchan-related, stemming from southeastern Colombia or southwestern Venezuela and far northwestern Brazil. This area could readily have supplied the Waroid, Tucano, Panoan, and Arawak lexical forms seen in Timucua. That the contact was long-lasting is clearly indicated by Proto-Tucanoan, Proto-Arawakan, and Pre-Andine Maipuran resemblances on the one hand (as early as ca. 3500—2500 B.C.) and by early Northern Maipuran resemblances on the other hand (from approximately 2 0 0 0 - 1 8 0 0 B.C.). The long history of multilingualism in the target areas substantiates the assumption. That Timucua was a traders' tongue seems verified by the similarities to Chibchan Cuna and Paezan Choco, far removed from the original putative Timucua homeland, and by clear Proto-Muskogean loans from the southeastern United States. If this picture is even partly valid, the trade network we are suggesting must have stretched from the Vaupes-Caqueta region along the river systems to the northwest, continuing along the Magdalena, Cauca, and Sinu Rivers, and culminating somewhere in or near Cuna-Choco country. Archaeological evidence has indicated for some time now that the Caribbean coast of Colombia, centering around Barlovento and Puerto Hormiga, to the immediate west of the mouth of the Magdalena, was the focus of a trading nexus from at least 2500 B.C. It seems not unlikely that the Timucua trade network articulated with this from early times. The late James Ford has brought together a considerable amount of cogent evidence to support the contention that the appearance of the first ceramic wares in North America, on the Florida-Georgia coast, was a result of direct trade from the Barlovento—Puerto Hormiga area of Colombia's Caribbean coast (Ford 1969). While there are many who will resist the idea, Timucua language data lend additional credence to it, for we know the Timucua ended up in Florida and Georgia, and the presence of Proto-Muskogcan lexical loans in Timucua would date such an arrival, from whatever source, at approximately 2 0 0 0 - 1 5 0 0 B.C. This date is quite in keeping with the radiocarbon dates for fiber-tempered Stallings Island and Orange wares in Late Archaic sites in the Savannah and St. Johns River area of the American southeast. The fact that these North American ceramic wares occur in sites whose other artifactual content is typical of the Late Archaic is, I think, an indication that Timucua movement to North America was not the large-

2}o

Distant Relationships

scalc movement of a people but, rather, the result of trading expeditions. The fact that the earliest Stallings Island radiocarbon dates are ca. 2515 B.C. (Rabbit Mount site, Lab. No. GX0-345) may well indicate that the Timucua were not the first traders from the Colombian coast. The early Northern Maipuran resemblances in Timucua would preclude Timucua arrival much before 1800 B.C. It is not impossible that the differences between Stallings Island wares and later Orange wares (ca. 1625 B.C., Turner site, Lab. No. G-598) arc attributable to the entrance of the Timucua into this trans-Caribbean trade network. As Ford points out (1969:187), the stylistic changes in Orange ware mirror similar changes in Colombian wares, implying a continuation of such trade over a long period of time. Changes in the physical characteristics of the local southeastern population from the more usual Late Archaic dolicocephalic type to a decidedly brachycephalic type in Stallings Island sites also indicates the probable presence of a significant number of alien people (Claflin 1931:43-45). Rouse (1986:121) erroneously attributes to me the hypothesis of "a migration of ancestral Warao speakers northward through the West Indies from Venezuela to Florida." This is a mistaken interpretation of my statement that Timucua has a large Waroid (not Warao) element in its grammar (Granberry 1971:607). In fact, it seemed clear then and even clearer now that the route of such a trade network, as Ford suggests, was through the Yucatán Channel, around western Cuba, through the Florida Straits, and northward by way of the Gulf Stream to the St. Johns and Savannah River areas (Ford 1969:185). Since there is no indication of Antillean-Timucua interaction, linguistically or archaeologically, from any time period, a West Indian route would be quite out of the question. It might, however, be noted that there are many Waroid toponyms throughout the Greater Antilles, with the heaviest concentration in Pinar del Rio and Habana provinces in western Cuba (Granberry 1987b). These, however, are attributable, it would seem from archaeological evidence, to physical Waroid population movement at a time level at least a thousand years earlier, likely associated with the Ortoiroid lithic expansion, which did enter the Antilles by way of Trinidad and the Windward Islands. It is not beyond possibility that the earlier familiarity with the Antilles by Macro-Chibchan speakers was what laid the groundwork for the subsequent Colombia-Florida trade nexus. It may be noted that both Stallings Island and Orange sites are located away from the coast proper, generally on the shores of a river. This

Amazonian Origins of Timucua

231

riverine niche is a salient characteristic of fiber-tempered ware sites and remains so until the advent of horticulture. This environment would, of course, have been the preferred one for the Timucua if our hypothesis of their riverine South American origins is correct. Inasmuch as the Floridian Mt. Taylor Late Archaic people also occupied such an environment, it should come as no surprise to find continuity from the Late Archaic into the ceramic-producing Early Formative in such Floridian locales nor to find Stallings Island sites, such as the Sapelo Island site, also in riverine locations. It might also be added that just as site location is similar on both the Florida/Georgia coast and the Colombian Caribbean coast, so the physical shape and nature of such sites is similar—large shell rings (Willey 1971:268-71). With later ceramic wares in Florida and Georgia, the Deptford in particular, we find quite a different settlement pattern (Milanich 1973). Deptford sites are located either along coastal marshes, on both the Atlantic and Gulf coasts, or, if inland, not in exclusively riverine locales. With the beginning of horticulture, both coastal and riverine patterns begin to weaken, as inland settlement on better agricultural lands takes precedence. In early Deptford times, however, the then-remnant fiber-tempered cultures and the Deptford cultures complement each other rather than compete within the same ecological zones (Milanich 1973:53). The implication is that the Deptford peoples, who show cultural continuity with the succeeding Wilmington and Savannah peoples, were likely Muskogean in both language and material culture. This seems substantiated by archaeological evidence indicating that the Guale (Apalachee wayli 'border'), of known Muskogean affiliation, were the end-product of the Wilmington-Savannah tradition (Milanich 1976, Larson 1978). It seems likely that the same can be said for the Gulf coast Deptford peoples, to judge from the long cultural continuity on that coast from the Late Archaic. Gulf coast fiber-tempered wares, such as that called Norwood (Phelps 1965), are distinctively different from both Orange and Stallings Island wares, giving the strong impression that their presence is due to the spread of an idea and a set of artifactual techniques rather than to the spread of a people. Primary cultural ties on the Gulf coast were always to the north and the south, along the coastal periphery, rather than to the cast. The two broadly defined linguistic-archaeological traditions, native Muskogean and intrusive Timucua, do blend in later times in Florida's geographical middle-ground, the Alachua Prairies and neighboring Co-

232

Distant Relationships

lumbia and Taylor counties, to produce the Alachua Tradition. The two heartlands—the St. Johns River valley and the Gulf coastal plain—remain Timucua and Muskogean, respectively, developing into the long St. Johns Tradition in the first and the Safety Harbor/Fort Walton/Leon-JefFerson Tradition in the second. A final problem that remains is the interesting fact that the bulk of Muskogean loans in Timucua come from Alabama and Koasati, the westernmost of the Eastern Muskogean languages. One would expect Creek and Hitchiti, the more easterly of the Eastern Muskogean tongues, to have been the major contributors, but they are not. There are a few probable loans from Choctaw as well, and also the two possible loans from Natchez and Atakapa, indicating contact considerably farther west than Timucua heartland countrv. j There are two possible explanations for this phenomenon. First, at the time level we are referring to, circa 1800 to 1000 B.C., the Creek and Hitchiti may have been located farther to the north and west than in later times, perhaps on the Georgia-Tennessee border. At the same time, the ancestral Alabama-Koasati may have formed a "layer" of population south of the Creek and Hitchiti, spreading farther cast than we now recognize. This possibility is raised by the use of the Apalachee name Guale (the English spelled it Wali), Apalachee wayli(n) 'border', to refer to this easternmost of the known Muskogean peoples. Apalachee has been shown to have been most closely related to Alabama and Koasati and to have fallen in what we know as the western branch of Eastern Muskogean (Haas 1949, Kimball 1987). If the unknown language of the Guale was indeed most closely related to Apalachee, Alabama, and Koasati, the problem would not be difficult to resolve, and it would be likely that the greatest bulk of Muskogean loans in Timucua had a Guale source. A grammar of Guale was written by Fr. Domingo Augustin between 1565 and 1585, but it has vanished in the maw of Spanish colonial bureaucratic records, either in the Consejo de Indias or perhaps the records of the Jesuit order (Swanton 1946:135). Secondly, if we should find that Guale belonged in the eastern branch of Eastern Muskogean, with Creek and Hitchiti, then we are in trouble, for this would imply that the Timucua were primarily in long-term contact not with the Guale but with their Alabama and Koasati neighbors to the west. This, however, might not be quite as troublesome as it seems on first glance, given the interesting riverine dispersion of the Oconi and Tawasa. These two Timucua groups were distributed along the river systems

234

Distant Relationships

penetrating later Alabama and Koasati lands, and it is not impossible to envision them as the forefront of a Timucua trading wedge into the southeastern heartland, even though their home base was along the Atlantic coast in the Savannah and St. Johns River regions. Either hypothesis could be tested archaeologically, and the presence of fiber-tempered wares at both the undated Fourche Maline site-complex in eastern Oklahoma (Newkumet 1940) and the Bayou La Batre complex near Mobile Bay (Greenwell 1984), dating to around 500—1000 B.C., may be an indication that the second hypothesis is correct. Fiber-tempered wares at both complexes are remarkably similar to Floridian Orange wares, so similar that direct migrations of Orange ware makers have been suggested for the Fourche Maline area (Ford 1969:176). It may in fact be the case that both explanations have some truth in them. The earliest fiber-tempered ware sites in the Savannah River region may have been central dispersion points from which riverine trading expeditions put out. We do not know what the major commodities traded were. One of them, however, may well have been salt—Timucua api, a term borrowed from the western Koasati, Alabama, or Choctaw /hapi/. This vital human commodity is abundant not only on the Atlantic seaboard as ocean salt, but also in extremely large deposits in the central Mississippi Valley region (Kentucky, southern Indiana, southeastern Illinois). Salt was used not only as a food commodity but also increasingly as a vital temper ingredient in the production of good-quality ceramic wares from clays of poor workability (Stimmell, Heimann, and Hancock 1982: 219—26). We know that the gradual growth of ceramic industries in the Southeast led to a salt-trading system of considerable dimensions and sophistication by the Early Mississippian Period (ca. A.D. 500), stimulating development of the great Mississippian cultural centers, such as Cahokia and Angel, which in turn led to the widespread exchange of all types of cultural commodities between these centers and all parts of the Southeast (Stimmell, Heimann, and Hancock 1982:227). It is not at all impossible that the Timucua became the leading salttraders of the Mississippi-Southeast region at a very early date, taking their ceramic wares and ceramic-making techniques with them as they moved on their expeditions. This would certainly help explain the startling identities and similarities between Floridian Orange wares and the fiber-tempered wares of the Fourche Maline site-complex in southeastern Oklahoma. A closer examination of the artifactual inventory of all fiber-tempered

Amazonian Origins of Timucua

235

sites is certainly called for, particularly in the Savannah and Tennessee River regions. The present interpretation of Timucua language data, correlated with archaeological information, tends to support a Chibchan-related ultimate origin for the language and people somewhere in the Vaupes-Caqueta region of Colombia. The people seem to have been traders and to have participated in the north-south network between the northwestern Amazonian jungles and the northwestern Caribbean coast of Colombia. Eventually, they seem to have become one of the prime movers, if not the prime mover, of transoceanic trade between that coast and the southeastern United States. They seem to have established major trading stations on the Savannah and St. Johns Rivers and to have continued to build a Southeastern trading network along the same lines as in northwestern South America, oriented along major river systems of the region. The main trading commodities may have been salt and, to judge from the archaeological distribution of fiber-tempered wares, ceramic wares, or at least ceramicmaking techniques. Such a trade network must have survived for a considerable period of time—at least until the advent of horticulture as the major form of livelihood and the creation of a more settled way of life.

A Note on Timucua Orthography and Phonology It is, of course, difficult to impossible to reconstruct accurate phonetic and phonemic entities for a language for which one has only orthographic evidence. This is particularly the case if the orthography was devised by linguistically unsophisticated users. In the case of the surviving Timucua documents, we are unusually fortunate in that Fr. Pareja was an astute observer of the language. The orthographic system he devised for Timucua is based on sixteenth-century Spanish spelling conventions, with exceptions carefully noted and described (Adam and Vinson 1886). We are able to recover considerable phonetic detail from Pareja's descriptions, and the resulting phonemicization is therefore probably rather accurate. Pareja even provides us with lists of minimal pairs and considerable information on suprasegmental phenomena. For a more detailed description of Timucua reconstructed phonology see Granberry (1987a, 1990). The present description is essentially that given in an earlier article (Granberry 1956) except for the definition of vowel phonemes, which has been considerably

236

Distant Relationships

altered here. T h e table will enable the reader to reconstruct the source orthography f o r the T i m u c u a forms given in this paper: Phoneme

Source spellings

/a/ Id /i/ loi lui Ipl It/ Ikl /k»V HI /{/(= //>) /s/ / h/ /ml /n/ /l/ Irl Iwl lyl

a e i, y + consonant 0 u, v + consonant P t, (n)d ~ (n)t c(a,o,u), q(e,i) qu ch f, h + vowel, b (rare) s, c(e,i) (rare), 5 (rare) h, j (rare), g(e,i) (rare) m n 1 r b, bu, g(a,o,u) (rare), gu(a,o), hu + vowel y, vowel + i + vowel (rare)

REFERENCES Adam, Lucien and Julien Vinson, eds. 1886 "Arte de la lengva Timvquana, compvesto en 1614 por el P': Francisco Pareja, v publicado conforme al ejemplar original único." Bibliothèque Linguistique américaine, vol. 11. Paris: Maisonneuve Frères et Ch. Leclerc, Éditeurs. Barrai, Basilio María de 1957 Diccionario Guarao-Español, Español-Guarao. Monografías N°3. Caracas: Sociedad de ciencias naturales La Salle, Editorial Sucre. Borhegyi, Stephen 1959 "Pre-Colombian cultural connections between Meso-America and Ecuador." Middle American Research Records 2.6:141-55. Brinton, Daniel Garrison 1858 Notes on the Floridian Peninsula. Philadelphia. Bullen, Ripley P. 1978 "Tocobaga Indians and the Safety Harbor culture." In Tacachale, ed. Jerald Milanich and Samuel Proctor, pp. 50—58. Gainesville: University Presses of Florida.

Amazonian Origins of Timucua

237

Claflin, William H., Jr. 1931 "The Stallings Island mound, Columbia Countv, Georgia." Papers of the Peabody Museum of Archaeology and Ethnology 14.1:1-47. Cambridge: Harvard University. Coe, Michael i960 "Archaeological linkages with North and South America at La Victoria, Guatemala." American Anthropologist 62:363-93. Conzemius, Eduard 1929 "Notes on the Miskito and Sumu languages of eastern Nicaragua and Honduras." International Journal ofAmerican Linguistics 5:57-115. Crawford, James M. 1979 "Timucua and Yuchi: Two language isolates of the Southeast." In The Languages of Native America, ed. Lyle Campbell and Marianne Mithun, pp. 327-54. Austin: University of Texas Press. Deagan, Kathleen A. 1978 "Cultures in transition: Fusion and assimilation among the Eastern Timucua." In Tacachale, ed. Jerald Milanich and Samuel Proctor, pp. 89-119. Gainesville: University Presses of Florida. Edwards, Clinton R. 1965 "Aboriginal watercraft on the Pacific coast of South America." IberoAmericana 47. Feldman, Robert A. and Michael E. Moseley 1978 "The northern Andes." In Ancient South America, ed. Jesse D. Jennings, pp. 139-77. San Francisco: W.H. Freeman and Company. Ford, James A. 1969 "A comparison ofFormative cultures in the Americas: Diffusion or the psychic unity of man." Smithsonian Contributions to Anthropology 11. Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Institution Press. Gatschet, Albert Samuel 1880 "The Timucua language." Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society 18:465—502. Philadelphia. Goggin, John M. 1953 "An introductory outline of Timucua archaeology." Laboratory of Anthropology, University of Florida, Gainesville. Paper presented to the 1952 Southeastern Archaeological Conference, 31 Oct-i Nov. 1952, Macon, Georgia. Goodman, Morris 1971 "The strange case of Mbugu." In Pidginization and Creolization of Languages, ed. Dell Hymes, pp. 243—54. London: Cambridge University Press. Granberry, Julian 1956 "Timucua I: Prosodies and phonemics of the Mocama dialect." International Journal of American Linguistics 22. 2:97-105. 1971 "Final collation of texts, vocabulary lists, grammar of Timucua (Penrose Fund Grant No. 4633, 1967)." Tear Book of the American Philosophical Society, 1970, pp. 606-7. Philadelphia.

238

Distant Relationships 1987a

"A grammar and dictionary of the Timucua language." Anthropological Notes 1, Horseshoe Beach, Florida. 1987b "Antillean languages and the aboriginal settlement of the Bahamas: A working hypothesis." Paper presented to the Bahamas 1492 Conference, Freeport, Bahamas, 17 Nov. 1987. 1990 "A grammatical sketch of Timucua." International Journal of American Linguistics 56.1:60-101. Greenberg, Joseph H. i960 "The general classification of Central and South American languages." In Men and Cultures, 5th International Congress of Anthropological and Ethnological Sciences, ed. Anthony F. C. Wallace, pp. 791-94. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. Greenwell, Dale 1984 "The Mississippi Gulf coast." In Perspectives on Gulf Coast Prehistory, ed. Dave D. Davis, pp. 124-55. Gainesville: University Presses of Florida. Haas, Mary R. 1940 "Ablaut and its function in Muskogee." Language 18:141—50. 1941 "The classification of the Muskogean languages." In Language, Culture, and Personality, ed. Leslie Spier, pp. 41-56. Menasha. 1945 "Dialects of the Muskogee language." International Journal of American Linguistics n. 2:69-74. 1946 "A Proto-Muskogean paradigm." Language 22.4:326-32. 1947 "Development of Proto-Muskogean *k"." International Journal of American Linguistics 13.3:135-37. 1948 "Classificatory verbs in Muskogee." International Journal of American Linguistics 14.4:244-46. 1949 "The position of Apalachee in the Muskogean familv." International Journal of American Linguistics 15.2:121-27. 1950 "On the historical development of certain long vowels in Creek." International Journal ofAmerican Linguistics 16.3:122—25. 1956 "Natchez and the Muskogean languages." Language 32.1:61-72. i960 "Some genetic affiliations of Algonkin." Culture in History, pp. 97792. New York: Columbia University Press. 1975 "What is Mobilian?" Studies in Southeastern Indian Languages, ed. James M. Crawford, pp. 257-61. Athens: University of Georgia Press. Holmer, Nils Magnus 1946 "Outline of Cuna grammar." International Journal of American Linguistics 12.4:185-97. Key, Mary Ritchie 1968 Comparative Tacanan Phonology: With Cavinena Phonology and Notes on Pano-Tacanan Relationship. The Hague: Mouton. Kimball, Geoffrey 1987 "A grammatical sketch of Apalachee." International Journal of American Linguistics 53.2:136-74.

Amazonian Origins of Timucua

239

Larson, Lewis H., Jr. 1978 "Historic Guale Indians of the Georgia coast and the impact of the Spanish mission effort." In Tacachale, ed. Jerald Milanich and Samuel Proctor, pp. 120-40. Gainesville: University Presses of Florida. Lathrap, Donald W. 1970 The Upper Amazon. New York: Praeger. 1973 "The antiquity and importance of long-distance trade relationships in the moist tropics of pre-Columbian South America." World Archaeology 5.2:170-86. Lathrap, Donald W., Donald Collier, and Helen Chandra 1975 Ancient Eduador: Culture, Clay and Creativity 3000-300 B.C. Chicago: Field Museum of Natural History. Le Moyne, Jacques 1875 Narrative of Le Moyne, An Artist Who Accompanied the French Expedition to Florida under Laudonniere, 1S64. Translated from the Latin of De Bry. Boston: Osgood Company. Loewen, Jacob A. 1963 "Choco II: Phonological problems." International Journal of American Linguistics 29.4:357-71. Lorant, Stefan, ed. 1946 The New World. New York: Duell, Sloan, and Pearce. Loukotka, Cestmir 1968 Classification of South American Indian Languages, ed. Johannes Wilbert. Latin American Center Reference Series, vol. 7. Los Angeles: University of California. Matteson, Esther 1972 "Proto Arawakan." In Comparative Studies in Amerindian Languages, ed. Esther Matteson, Alva Wheeler, Frances L. Jackson, Nathan Waltz, and Diana R. Christian, pp. 160-85. The Hague: Mouton. Migliazza, Ernest C. 1978 "Some evidences for Panoan-Yanomama genetic relationship." Paper presented at the Linguistic Society of America Summer Meeting, Urbana. 1985 "Language of the Orinoco-Amazon region." In South American Indian Languages: Retrospect and Prospect, ed. Harriet W. Manelis Klein and Louisa R. Stark, pp. 17-139. Austin: University of Texas Press. Milanich, Jerald T. 1973 "The Southeastern Deptford culture: A preliminary definition." Bureau of Historic Sites and Properties, Bulletin 3: pp. 51-63. Tallahassee: Florida Department of State. 1976 "Georgia origins of the Alachua tradition." Bureau of Historic Sites and Properties, Bulletin 5: pp. 47-56. Tallahassee: Florida Department of State. 1978 "The western Timucua: Patterns of acculturation and change." In Tacachale, ed. Jerald Milanich and Samuel Proctor, pp. 59-88. Gainesville: University Presses of Florida.

24-0

Distant Relationships

Milanich, Jerald T. and Charles H. Fairbanks 1980 Florida Archaeology. New York: Academic Press. Milanich, Jerald T., and William C. Sturtevant 1972 Francisco Pareja's 1613 Confessionario: A Documentary Source for Timucuan Ethnography. Tallahassee: Division of Archives, History, and Records Management, Florida Department of State. Newkumet, Phil J. 1940 "Preliminar)' report on excavation of the Williams Mound, Leflore County, Oklahoma." Oklahoma State Archaeological Society 3.2:1—9. Noble, G. Kingsley 1965 "Proto-Arawakan and its descendants." International Journal of American Linguistics 31.3 (July), part II: 1—129 (also pub. 38, Indiana University Research Center in Anthropology, Folklore and Linguistics, Bloomington). Osborn, Henry 1967a "Warao I: Phonology and morphophonemics." International Journal of American Linguistics 32.2:108-23. 1967b "Warao II: Nouns, relationals, and demonstratives." International Journal of American Linguistics 32.3:253-61. 1968 "Warao III: Verbs and suffixes." International Journal of American Linguistics 33.1:46-64. Pareja, Francisco 1627 Cathecismo, y examen para los que comvlgan, en lengua Castellana, y Fimuquana. Mexico. Phelps, David S. 1965 "The Norwood series of fiber-tempered ceramics." Southeastern Archaeological Conference Bulletin 2:65-69. Prost, Gilbert R. 1962 "Signaling of transitive and intransitive in Chacobo (Pano)." International Journal of American Linguistics 28.2:108-18. Rodrigues, Aryon Dall'Igna 1985 "Evidence for Tupi-Carib relationships." In South American Indian Languages: Retrospect and Prospect, ed. Harriet E. Manelis Klein and Louisa R. Stark, pp. 371-404. Austin: University of Texas Press. Roth, Walter E. 1924 "An introductory stud}' of the arts, crafts, and customs of the Guiana Indians." Fhirty-eighth Annual Report of the Bureau ofAmerican Ethnology, 1916-17, pp. 26-745. Washington, D.C. Rouse, Irving 1986 Migrations in prehistory: Inferring Population Movement from Cultural Remains. New Haven: Yale University Press. Salomon, Frank 1977/78 "Pochteca and mindala: A comparison of long-distance traders in Ecuador and Mesoamerica." Journal of the Steward Anthropological Society 9.1—2:231—46.

Amazonian Origins of Timucua

241

Schomburgk, Moritz Richard 1847 Reisen in Britisch-Guiana in den Jahren 1840-44. 3 vols. Leipzig: Verlagsbuchhandlun von J.J. Weber. Sorenson, Arthur P., Jr. 1967 "Multilingualism in the northwest Amazon." American Anthropologist 69:670-684. 1985 "An emerging Tukanoan linguistic regionalitv: Policy pressures." In South American Indian Languages: Retrospect and Prospect, ed. Harriet E. Manelis Klein and Louisa R. Stark, pp. 140-56. Austin: University of Texas Press. Stimmell, Carole, Robert B. Heimann, and R. G. V. Hancock 1982 "Indian potter}' from the Mississippi Vallev: Coping with bad raw materials." In Archaeological Ceramics, ed. Jacqueline S. Olin and Alan D. Franklin, pp. 219-28. Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Institution Press. Swadesh, Morris 1964 "Linguistic overview." In Prehistoric Man in the New World, ed. Jesse D. Jennings and Edward Norbeck, pp. 527-56. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Swanton, John R. 1929 "The Tawasa language." American Anthropologist 31:435-53. 1946 "The Indians of the Southeastern United States." Bureau of American Ethnology, Bulletin 137. Washington, D.C. Taylor, Douglas M. 1977 Languages of the West Indies. Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press. Taylor, Douglas M. and Berend Hoff 1966 '"Proto-Arawakan and its descendants,' by G. Kingsley Noble: A review." International Journal of American Linguistics 32.3:303-8. Wallace, Alfred Russel 1889 A Narrative of Travels on the Amazon and Rio Negro. 2nd edition. London: Ward, Lock & Company. Waltz, Nathan E. and Alva Wheeler 1972 "Proto Tucanoan." In Comparative Studies in Amerindian Langauges, ed. Esther Matteson, Alva Wheeler, Frances L. Jackson, Nathan Waltz, and Diana R. Christian, pp. 119-149. The Hague: Mouton. Wheeler, Alva 1972 "Proto Chibchan." In Comparative Studies in Amerindian Languages, ed. Esther Matteson, Alva Wheeler, Frances L. Jackson, Nathan Waltz, and Diana R. Christian, pp. 93-108. The Hague: Mouton. Wilbert, Johannes 1957 "Prölogo." In Diccionario Guarao-Espanol, Espanol-Guarao, bv Basilio Maria de Barral, pp. 7 - 1 8 . Caracas: Sociedad de ciencias naturales La Salle, Editorial Sucre. 1972 Survivors of Eldorado. New York: Praeger.

242

Distant Relationships 1973

"Eschatologv in a participator)' universe: Destinies of the soul among the Warao Indians of Venezuela." Paper presented to the Dumbarton Oaks Conference on Death and Afterlife in Pre-Columbian America, 27—28 October 1973. Willey, Gordon R. 1971 An Introduction to American Archaeology, Vol. 2: South America. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall. Zeidler, James 1977/78 "Primitive exchange, prehistoric trade and the problem of Mesoamerican-South American connection." Journal of the Steward Anthropological Society 9 . 1 - 2 : 7 - 3 9 .

Lila Wistmnd-Robinson Uto-Aztecan Affinities with Panoan of Peru I: Correspondences

ABSTRACT Allusions have been made to North and South American Indian linguistic relationships but no positive proof has been presented, aside from Ronald Olson's work on Chipaya and Maya, published in 1964-65, which reached only into Central America. I present here a preliminary chart of sound correspondences of Cashibo and Comanche, with corresponding Proto-Panoan, Proto-Uto-Aztecan, Proto-Numic, and Proto-Shoshonean. A list of tentative cognates is also given. Cashibo and Comanche are target living languages with which I have worked linguistically. These actual, present-day languages give more positive and accurate data for comparison than reconstructed artificial forms, which have an enormous timespan that date back to the proto languages. Inclusion of all members of each family group in the reconstructed forms obscures the relationships that are more unique to the older and more isolated Panoan and Shoshonean groups.

The purpose of this chapter is to present: (1) brief phonological sketches of Comanche and Cashibo; 1 (2) a standard ioo-word list, including ProtoUto-Aztecan (*UA), Comanche (Com), Hopi (Ho), *Panoan (*PN), Cashibo (Csh), and Shipibo-Conibo (SC); (3) charts of sound correspondences of * U A and * P N , Com and Csh; and (4) evidence of a tentative relationship between *UA and * P N , thus linking North and South American Indian language families at the proto level. Comanche and Cashibo are chosen as major representatives of the two language families since the author is acquainted with these through work on both languages. Occasionally, a few other representatives of the two language families are given in the comparative list: Mayo El Fuerte (Sinaloa) dialect (Ma); Mayo Jijiri (Sonora) dialect (My), *Numic ( * N U M ) , Aztec (Az), and Papago-Pima (PaPi). The lexicostatistical method has been used to show tentative dates of separation from three ioo-word lists and one 200-word list, though only one ioo-word list is included here. The background for the above studies is also given.

244

Distant Relationships

The ioo-word list by Rea was used, supplemented by the lists of UtoAztecan by Kenneth L. Hale (HAL: 1958); C. F. Voegelin, F. M. Voegelin, and Kenneth L. Hale ( W H : 1962); Lvle Campbell and Ronald W. Langacker (C and L: 1978); David E. Iannucci (IAN 11972); William Bright and Jane Hill (B&H: 1967); Andres Lionnet (LIO: 1985); Burton W. Bascom (BAS: 1965); Wick R. Miller, Kevin J. Hegg, and Cindy High (MIL: 1987); and my work with James L. Armagost in Comanche (ROB: 1990). H o p i items are from Hopi Domains, by Voegelin and Voegelin (IJAL: 1957). Proto Panoan items arc from Olive A. Shell (SHL: 1975),2 and Shipibo-Conibo from James Loriot (LOR). 3 Miller's (1987) reccnt compendium of information, Computerized Data Basefor Uto-Azteean Cognate Sets (CDB), was a great help for checking all information collected prior to its publication and for adding some additional information. I have attempted to coordinate my information with these data to a certain extent. For this reason, the reader is referred to CDB introductory information for further background, which I will not repeat here. The Swadesh method of glottochronology, used on three ioo-word lists, resulted in 60 percent, 66 percent, and 72 percent cognates. The same method, used on a 2oo-word standard list, resulted in 58.5 percent cognates. The latter was chosen as most representative, implying a separation of Panoan from Uto-Aztecan approximately 1,650 years ago, or at A.D. 337, according to the following formula: log 58.5 t = r f — = 2 log 85

-.566 r 1—— = 1,649 vears ago, or A.D. 337 5 2 x - L.63 '

One commonly sees Sanskrit, Latin, Greek, German, and English words compared as cognates, yet we know they span continents and have a common origin in *IE. Meillet (1924 [1967] :29) states that "the agreements are less striking and the rules of correspondences more difficult to determine if we observe languages separated by larger intervals of space and time." The number of phonological and grammatical correspondences are striking in my comparison of Comanche-Cashibo precisely because of the large intervals of space and time, possibly suggesting a migration independent of the fact that both Comanche and Cashibo were isolated for longer periods than their fellow tribesmen, at extreme ends of both language families. The Comanche were the last tribe in the United States to be subdued by government troops. The Cashibo were isolated due to their location in the foothills of the Andes, awav from the more heavily traveled

Uto-Aztecan and Panoan

245

riverine area where contact with other tribes would have been made more easily. Because of their separation, these two tribes conserved some of the same older characteristics of their proposed common ancestor. A later article will present the proposed reconstructions to the ancestor of *UA and *PN. We are faced here with the problem of vocabulary attrition due to the extremely long time span and with extremely differing ecological settings. The Comanche are located in a dry ecological area, lying in the rain shadow of the Wichita Mountains of Oklahoma in the United States in North America whereas the Cashibo are in the tropical rain forest of Central Peru, in the foothills of the Andes Mountains in South America. Meillet has also stated (1924 [1967]: 36) that morphology is "the most stable thing in language." My studies show grammatical correspondences and processes that are equally as important as the phonological ones as a result of movement of sets of morphemes in agglutinative languages, rather than individual phonemes. Though grammatically important morphemes are included with the phonological data, space docs not permit treatment of the grammar in this chapter; that topic will be the focus of a future article.

Panoan The Panoan (PN) languages are Amahuaca, Cashinahua, Capanahua, Sharanahua, Mayoruna, Yaminahua, Conibo-Shipibo, Cashibo, Cacataibo, Chacobo, and Marubo. These tribes are located in Peru, Brazil, Bolivia, and possibly also previously in Colombia. * P N was reconstructed by Shell (1975 version, in Spanish) with augmented data to her 1965 dissertation. This work was added to or modified by Loriot (1965). (See also Key 1968, and Loos 1973a, b.) A reconstruction of Proto Tacanan (*TC) was published by Key (1968, for Tacanan, located only in Bolivia), but not dealt with here. It might be useful to do a parallel study of Tacanan and Aztec, as suggested by Key (1978, 1981b). Relationships of Panoan to Tacanan have been presented by Loriot (1965), Loos (1964), and Key (1968). The Summer Institute of Linguistics series Estudios Panos, written in Spanish, also gives detailed phonological and grammatical background information on the Panoan languages. My work on the Cashibo language took place between 1957 and 1965. In 1978, I was given the opportunity to direct a National Endowment for

246

Distant Relationships

the Humanities project with the goal of editing the late Elliott Canonge's card file of rough lexicon entries so that a dictionary and grammar description could be compiled.4 As I checked entries for the dictionary, apparent cognates with Cashibo turned up. In addition to phonological correspondences, grammatical relationships also became evident. Experience with Cashibo gave insight into Comanche—a kind of depth that had not shown up in the bits and pieces of grammatical descriptions previously published on Comanche or Shoshone. In the years that have followed the initial grant work, Armagost's grammar studies and McLaughlin's (1982) phonological articles have added still more depth to Comanche linguistics.

Comanche Phonemes Comanche phonemes are shown in Tables 1 and 2 in phonemic form of vowels and consonants respectively.5 General stress falls on the first (C) V(C) syllable, with a general rule of alternating stress. Most pro-clitics follow the general stress rules; that is, they take on the initial stress, pulling it away from the main stem, for example: /hani-/ 'do'; /tihani-/ 'cut up, butcher'; /wosa/ 'sack'; /ka:wosa/ 'coyote'. There are also non-stress-changing prefixes that

T A B L E I.

P R E L I M I N A R Y C H A R T OF V O W E L C O R R E S P O N D E N C E S

*UA

Com

Csh

*i

/i i: 1/

Ii i:/

*PN

It i: 1/

Iii:/

*i

*u

/u u: U/

loo:l

*o

*o

loOl

loi

*o

*o

/o:~o:]

h:l

*awa

*a

/a a: AJ

/a a:/

*a

*ai

It e:/

let:/

*ava

Comanche

Cashibo

pühi iritthanitirahhutüa kùvu'ni: möha söni to: [~to:]

pii irintiattirin0toâ köso möka söbi to: -0:-

ha-, -'atapäka kü'c

tapâka k"é:

English be, become leaf burn bright cut, butcher erase, rub off come son; child turkey bitter grass, weeds cane, stick make, build do, make of/with foot bamboo, reed top, upper

T A B L E 2.

*UA Com *p

¥

P R E L I M I N A R Y CHART OF CONSONANT CORRESPONDENCES

Csh Ipl

*PN Comanche *p

päka piirA *b /b/ Ibl *b [ni]bui [tai]bo ba:ba *b /pi Ibl *b pi:!xl *t Ixl *t tatühu-, tutöki*r Irl *r tirahIrl *k /y IkJ *k nokimakuplta 9 *k Ik/ Ik*/ *k kima? kimakwai *k» / w /k[o]/ *k k w i:[pi] *h Ihl PI häni initial hibi*? PI usiPuya'i0 *h Ihl tihäni0 0 medial pühi nähnia* w /w/ /b or w/ * w [äta]bici 9 lyl /i/ *i nühya *y Isl *s kuyu?[ni:] *y lyl töyopl *c Id Id *c tci[?iti] -ci *s Isl Isl *s/ tisu'iusu^yä^i-] si:petf ? *s /[?>/ Id *c [tä] si:tO *s Isl Isl *s tüsikasämohpi? *m Im/ Iml *m möha ~ mühka ma*n InJ *n nokimaIn/ häni *n In/ /n~y/ *y kino'ihäni

Cashibo päka pinyä biro [käi]bo bäba bitatotö: [ki/ka]tirinnökoköki k-ibi k" icima kol ?äno

English

bamboo, reed arm [my] eye people, group grandson, grandchild bring of/with the foot black, dark break in pieces erase, rub off come moving; arrive light; torch lip, edge dull (edge) smoke, cloud beaver; paca drink; swallow 'osänlaugh (at), smile cut, butcher ttapii leaf äniname (vb) other bici noi snake; worm köso turkey tisä neck mäci [be] cold -CO little (dimin.) ? isiadvise, counsel ?osänlaugh, smile sapäN level [on]cis claw, [toejnail ttisuexpectorate, spit säno sibling; woman rnuka bitter mihand (pref.) nökocome moving; arrive ?äno beaver; paca kino ~ kiyo- finish, kill äno ~ äyo what(ever)

248

Distant Relationships

leave the stress on the appropriate stem syllable, for example: /ciwa:'i-/ 'feel around with something sharp" [ci + wa:'i-]. These rules also hold for Cashibo. There is contrast between long and short vowels, even under stress (' = primary stress on the preceding vowel), for example: /ka:'wosA/ 'fox, coyote'; /ku'plta?/ 'light', with the stress overtly marked on both initial vowels (these are not generally marked since they follow the phonemic rule); /usu,'')uya'i-/ 'laugh (at)' (' = secondary stress on the preceding vowel.) In compounds, the noun retains main stress, while the bound modifier has secondary stress, for example: /k'yu?ni'i/ 'turkey'. Comanche consonants group as stops, affricate, fricatives, nasals, flap, resonant, and semivowels. Stops are /p/, /t/, /k/, and /k"/, all nonvoiced, nonaspirated, and nonglottalized. /p/ becomes [b] or phonetic variant [v] from English influence, and III becomes flap [r] in unstressed syllables word-medially. Although this rule holds rather firmly, the /&/ and /r/ are here treated as separate phonemes, since these are in a state of flux. Ik/ has a backed allophone [q] preceding back vowels /u/ and lol. The labialized velar stop Ikwl has the allophone [gw] word-medially when it occurs in unstressed syllables, varying in different dialects of Comanche, for example, /kwahi/ 'back', /kimakwai/ [kimagwai] 'dull edge'. The unaspirated voiceless affricate Id is at times difficult to distinguish from Psl within a verb, for example: /cih-/ 'fill full'; /cahpi?e-/ 'pull out, fish'. The grooved fricative is Isl with [s] only as a pejorative, borrowed variation of Is/ in certain words, for example: /tisu^iri/ 'counsel'; /pisuni'P/ [pisuni'i'/pisuni'i' 1 ] 'skunk'. The use of [s] adds intensification. Nasals arc I ml and /n/, for example, /muhka/ 'bitter'; /kima?/ 'lip, edge'; /nokima-/ 'arrive, come moving'; /tani'yiki-/ 'make sound of footsteps running.' Semivowels are /y/, /w/, /h/, and /?/, for example: /kuyu?ni'i-/ 'turkey'; /nuhya?/ 'worm'; /wobi-/ 'wood'; /ka:wosA/ 'fox'; /ciwa:i-/ 'feel around with something sharp'; /hani/ 'beaver'; /kwahi/ 'back (body part)'; /huhci, hu:ci/ 'grandmother'. Comanche vowels consist of two sets of three-way contrasts: high vowels /i, i, u/, and mid-to-low vowels /e, a, o/. The vowel in an isolated syllable has two morae of time by phonemic rule, for example: /ki/ [ki:] 'corner'; /a/ [a:] 'animal horn'; /no/ [no:] 'sure, definite'; /pa/ [pa:] 'water'; /ma/ [ma:] 'him, her, it'; /mi/ [mi:] 'your (second-pers. pi.)'. There is no contrast of short and long vowels on single-syllable words in isolation.

Uto-Aztecan and Panoan

249

This same rule holds for the Cashibo. Double morae has either mid or high falling tone. Under stress, the vowel has some phonetic length, whether under primary or secondary stress, for example, /amakwo:'^ pa1 'cider, apple juice'; /ana:'/ 'ouch!'; /kahu'u/ 'mouse'. As mentioned above, under phonemic first-syllable basic stress, length versus nonlength shows contrast, for example: /ti:pc/ 'lips, mouth'; /tiranA/ 'long bones of leg', with the first-syllable vowels under stress, also seen in the following: /tua?/ 'son', /tirme'sO/ 'young catfish'. Unstressed vowels following voiceless consonants or in word-final position are voiceless; for example, /habi-klno?/ 'cradle-board, day cradle'; /ka:wosA/ 'fox, jackal'; /nacahkweniitl/ 'hung up, hanging'; /taci:tO/ 'toe-nail'. High vowels are shown in various positions in the following words: i: /hinanahimiti/ 'to exchange'; /hani/ 'beaver' i: /iri-/ 'light a fire'; /masiati/ 'to tend by hand' u: /hu-/ 'come'; /ku?e/ 'top, upper part' /huhci, hu:ci/ 'grandmother' Mid vowels [c] and [o] are historically diphthongs, as corresponding Shoshoni /ai/ and /au/ reveal. Mid back-rounded vowel /o/ has a morphophonemic variant [0:] which has developed from the combination of [o] and [a], lowering the [o] and moving it slightly forward, with length to account for the two morae. The [o:] and [d:] are in free variation when the combination has been stem-forming, but usually occurs as [o: | in the morphophonemic post-position, o: /paro/ 'swollen river'; /wobi/ 'wood'; /to:/ 'cane' e: /eka-e?re'/ 'swamp rabbit'; /e:bi/ 'blue color' a: /hani/ 'beaver'; /nuhya?/ 'woman'; /a/ 'horn' Canonge interpreted the voiceless preconsonantal nonglottal consonant as [h] in all cases. Actually, preceding nasals it is phonetically [M] preceding /m/, [N] preceding /n/, parallel to Miller's geminate clusters /mm/ and /nn/. Even so, the phonemic status of /h/ requires its maintenance as a separate morpheme meaning "made, but belonging to," for example: /huhmara'/ 'falcon'; /hu-pihna/ 'syrup, molasses'. The glottal stop is phonemic, with both phonological and grammatical functions. It occurs word-medially between reduplicated identical vowels, for example /usu'uya^i-/ 'laugh at'. It also occurs between nonidcntical nondiphthongal vowels, for example, /ku9e/ 'top, upper part', and wordfinally as nominalizer for verb stems, for example, /taci:?/ 'louse egg'.

250

Distant Relationships

Cashibo Phonemes Cashibo (or Uni, as speakers call it) consists of four main dialects: (i) Cacataibo (Cac), spoken on the San Alejandro River; (2) Shambuyacu (Cshl), of the Lower Aguaytia River; (3) Pindayo (Csh2), of the Upper Aguaytia River; and (4) Sungaroyacu (Sun), spoken on the Sungaroyacu River. Shell (1950) used a taxonomic approach, basing her description on the Shambuyacu dialect (Cshl). In a later article, following the transformational-generative phonology (Robinson 1976), I dealt with the Pindayo (Csh2) as possibly basic for all. Cashibo (Csh) includes a system of five voiceless stops /p/, /t/, /k/, /kw/ and /?/; two affricates Id and /c/; four fricatives /b/, /s/, /s/, and /§/; three nasals /m/, /n/, and /n/; one resonant flap /r/; and six vowels, three high /i/, /i/, and /o/, and three mid to low /e/, /a/, and hi. Intrinsic stress falls on the first syllable, with a basic alternate syllable stress system. Length may also occur on any vowel. Single syllables in isolation always have phonemiclengthened vowels. Examples of the vowels are: i: /mi/ 'you'; / ? ica/ 'many' i: Pil 'I'; /ima/ 'village, cluster of houses' o: /o/ 'he, she, it (distal)'; /oil 'who' a: /a/ 'that, he, she, it (over there)'; /ana/ 'tongue' e: /me/ 'earth, land'; Pe:t\l 'to swallow' o: /bo/ 'parrot'; /bimio/ 'rubber tree' The two vowels Id and hi have a history of origin similar to those of Comanche, and involve the collapse of clusters of sounds from /*aya/ and /*awa/ respectively, while sister languages have kept the /ai/ and /au/, as in Shipibo-Conibo. All vowels may be either nasalized or lengthened. Stops are all voiceless and unaspirated syllable-initially. Only /n/, /s/, Is/, Is/, and PI occur syllable-finally as interpreted by Shell (1975) and using the work of Loos (1973b) and my work with Comanche, I now see that the syllable-final glottal stop and nasal reflect the Numic geminate consonants discussed by Miller for Shoshoni. Loos also pointed out for the Panoan languages the fact that in such words as Cashibo /kakiti/ 'to be on top o f ' the stem is /kakit-/ plus the suffix l-til 'to be', which is actually a remnant of the geminate consonants as found in the Uto-Aztecan family, specifically seen in Shoshoni, as analyzed by Miller (1973). Cashibo noun suffix /-§/ is a morpheme signifying the subject of an intransitive verb, as /a:s/ 'he, she, it (subject of vi)'; /inis/ 'this (subject of vi)'. As verb suffix, it signifies the subordinate verb link to an intransitive principal verb, as in /kwaans/ 'having gone'; /mikamas/ 'having stolen'.

Uto-Aztecan and Panoan

251

Obstruent /p/ and its fricative counterpart /b/ have been labialized preceding central high vowel /i/ in the speech of Cashibo individuals of advanced age, but this feature is being lost in most of the speech of younger members of the community, as /piti/ [pwiti, piti] 'to undress'; /bici/ [bwfci, fefci] 'other'. Although Comanche /fe/ and /r/ cannot occur word-initially and could be considered word-medial submembers of /p/ and It/, the /b/ and /r/ of Cashibo are separate phonemes from the /p/ and /t/ and occur freely word-initially, for example, /baba/ 'grandchild'; /ban/ 'sun, year'; /rani/ 'body hair'. Voiceless affricates Id and Id occur in word-initial and word-medial positions, but never syllable-final, for example, /caca/ 'fish'; /cici/ 'grandmother'. Cashibo developed the lei whereas many of the other Panoan languages still have only Id as in Comanche. Nasals occur in three-way contrast m, n, and n in the CashiboShambuyaca dialect, but in two-way m and n in all others, as /mapa/ 'Dioscoreaceae'; /nia/ 'pale-winged trumpeter'; /napa/ 'dorado fish'; /?ano/ 'paca'. Older speakers tend to labialize ImJ before central high vowel HI, though this is being lost by the younger generations, for example, /miu/ [mwiu, miu] 'inside, in the middle'. A bilabial stop release that occurs in the speech of older persons is also being lost by younger generations, for example, /miinkoma/ [mbwiinkoma, miiqkoma] 'small crab'. The syllablefinal preconsonantal nasal archiphoneme N has allophones assimilating to the point of articulation of the following stop, but to the Cashibo it is phonemically /n/, as a psychological reality, for example, /kanti/ 'bow'; /sanpa/ [sampa] 'gourd'; /nonki/ [norjki] 'kind of lizard'. Cashibo grooved fricatives are voiceless in a three-way contrast: alveolar Is/, palatal /s/, and retroflex /§/. These have occasional voiced variants /z/, [z], and [z] in Cshi and Csh2, regularized in Cac. In the Cac dialect /§/ is lost altogether word-initially or has the allophone [y], for example, /masi/ [masi, mazi] 'sand'; /si:pi [si:pi, i:pi] 'marmoset monkey'; /sapo/ [sapo, zapo] 'cotton'.

The Comparative List A few explanatory remarks regarding the comparative list are necessary. Comanche and Cashibo phonemic elements are summarized as follows:

252

Distant Relationships

COMANCHE

CASHIBO

P

t

b m w

s n r

p & m

t s n r

C

k

i

kw

u

e

y

h

c s n

k s

o a Length /:/ kw

?

i e

o o

a Length /:/

A hyphen following a single syllable usually indicates a prefix, though the morpheme may also be a single-syllable verb root if the meaning so indicates. Verb roots or stems are generally followed by hyphens. In a few cases, the Shipibo-Conibo (SC) has been left out if it was either identical to the Cashibo or not a cognate at all. The S C has been given to provide a means of showing some other aspects of, and depth for, Panoan, and also to show that S C has /h/ whereas Cashibo does not. Panoan also has /ai/ and /au/ whereas Cashibo has gone to /c:/ and h:l. Unfortunately, Capanahua is not given, which would have shown the V ? V of medial vowels: in Cashibo, simply W . The V ? V occurs in Capanahua and other Panoan languages. To save space, the main given meaning of the heading has sometimes been abbreviated by its first letter in the listings in cases where the meaning has been modified in some way; for example, "seed" = "s". Subscript sounds from W H and others have been capitalized (see a more complete explanation in CDB). Length has in most cases been standardized to V ; in Cashibo, this practice ignores some of the aspects of tone glide, for the glide may be stressed either on the first mora or the second. Cashibo has inherited remnants of tone from its oriental ancestors. For the purposes of this chapter, ignoring the finer points of tone does not affect the comparative aspects. Finally, unmarked meanings have the same sense as the heading. My first wish was to publish a long list of all the words that I consider cognates, but I realized that it would not have a good reception. Thus this standard list, limited in the number of cognates, is presented with the hope that in the future a longer enumeration of the words I consider cognates may be published for the study of other interested linguists.

Uto-Aztecan and Panoan IOO-WORD U T O - A Z T E C A N AND PANOAN COMPARATIVE L I S T

(1) Eng: all, entirely Span: todo *UA Ho W Com *PN SHL-52 Csh ROB SC LOR

so:sok(-t) 'oyeti *ati kamabi hatibi

(2) Eng: ashes Span: ceniza *UA BAS-147 Com ROB *PN SHL-126 Csh ROB SC LOR

*matai 'etfsipf mapo ci:mapo cimapo

(3) Eng: bark of tree Span: corteza *UA MIL-21 BAS-i05a Ho V V Com *PN SHL-395 Csh ROB

*ko *'komi ci:pi po'a *sakata 'bark, skin' saka, bici po'a 'potato with barklike skin'

(4) Eng: belly, abdomen, Span: panza *UA MIL-417 Ma Ho V V Com ROB *PN SHL-341 Csh, SC

stomach *to toppa pono sap! *poko poko

(5) Eng: big, large Span: grande *UA VVH-100 LIO-127 Ho V V Com ROB *PN SHL-I2 Csh ROB SC LOR

*wi* k" iru wi: pa'tall' piapf *?iwa ca: ani

254

Distant Relationships

(6) Eng: bird Span: ave *UA BAS-333 IAN-261 Com ROB Ho W *PN SHL-16 Csh ROB SC L O R

* ? u?u'higi 'birds' *(hu[i][h])ci (huh)cu:? sikaci 'canarylike' *?i'?saka ?isa

(7) Eng: bite, grit teeth Span: morder *UA W H - 4 3 *kiU(?i), *kiU(?i) Ho W ki:ki Com ROB kihcia-, kihkarPi*PN Csh ROB kiruska-; natfsSC L O R nâka, vins (8) Eng: black, dark Span: negro, oscuro *UA IAN-224 WH-23 MIL-45a BAS-232 Ho W Com ROB *PN SHL-464 Csh ROB (9) Eng: blood Span: sangre *UA MIL-47a,b LIO-13 Ho W Com ROB *PN SHL-141 Csh ROB SCLOR (10) Eng: bone Span: hueso *UA VVH-61 MIL-52 LAN-260 Com ROB *PN SHL-398 Csh

*tu(h) *tuU(ku) *tu, *tunu *tuku tôho, to:ki tûhu, tu*tonan tonâ:, to-

*'et, *'ew '«T'a pi:hpl *imi imi himi

* ? oho *?o, *?oho *cuhni cuhnifpi] *sao sâo

Uto-Aztecan and Panoan (11) Eng: burn, hot Span: quemar *UA HoW Com R O B * P N SHL-17 * P N SHL-136 Csh R O B

?iti ?iri?i *?icisa 'hot' *iri- 'burn' irin- 'burn' ? icis 'hot'

(12) Eng: cloud(s), smoke Span: nube(s), humo *UA HAL-19

WH-35 HoW Com R O B * P N SHL-187 Csh ROB SC LOR (13) Eng: cold Span: frío *UA VVH-11 IAN-262 Com ROB *NUM Az * P N SHL-205 Csh, SC

*k"i:ci *k"iSci ki:hi tómo 'cloud' k"i: 'smoke' *ko ? ini kol koin

*siU(pi) *-ci, *si, *sihicPimaci *maci máci

(14) Eng: come Span: venir *UA WH-159

MIL-96 IAN-71 Com R O B PaPi * P N SHL-296 Csh ROB SC LOR (15) Eng: die Span: morir * U A HAL-23 MIL-i28d Co R O B Ho W * P N SHL-233

*kiUma *kim *kihma kíma-, húdáiw *o ó- ~ áihó-

*muki *mek, *me kó:i-, tiyá:i mó:ki *mawa-

256

Distant Relationships

SC L O R (16)

Eng: dog Span: perro *UA VVH-46 MIL-I35 LIO-78 Com ROB *PN SHL-20 Csh ROB SCLOR

(17) Eng: drink Span: tomar, tragar *UA VVH-77 IAN-40 H o VV Com ROB *PN SHL-405 Csh ROB Eng: dry Span: seco *UA VVH-99 LIO-325 Com ROB *PN Csh ROB SC LOR (19) Eng: ear Span: oreja, oído *UA W H - 4 7 MIL-148 HOW Com ROB *PN SHL-310 Csh ROB SC LOR (2D)

Eng: earth, ground Span: tierra *UA MIL-151 VVH-112 LAN Com ROB

teámamawá-

*puNku 'dog, pet' *puku, -137 *cu *kawasi 'fox' sá:ri, pú:kU *?inaka kamó, 'ocíti ? ocíti

*hiS*hipi hí:ko híbí*si ? asía- 'drink' 'é;- 'swallow'

*waki *waki, wak-i 'secarse' pásapl í:ski-, totóánpacá- 'wash' mansán-

*naNka *naka náqvi náki *pabiki pabí pabíki

*k"i*k"iya *soko sokó:bI, sóko-

Uto-Aztecan and Panoan * P N SHL-208 SC L O R Csh R O B

*mai, *mawi mài mé: [cf. kol 'dust, smoke']

(21) Eng: eat

Span: comer *UA W H - 8 8 Com R O B Ho V V * P N SHL-326 Csh R O B SC LOR

*kuUmi, *kuUma tíhka-, tikisówa, pi(kiyi) 'nurse, suckle' *pi koáska- 'drink' pi-

(22) Eng: egg Span: huevo, blanquillo * kawa *UA MIL-156 nó'i, nóovo Com R O B nóhnaa PaPi kábba My *básci * P N SHL-64 bàci ~ waci Csh R O B bàci SC LOR (23) Eng: eye Span: ojo *UA W H - 5 IAN-155

Ho W Com R O B * P N SHL-8oa Csh, S C (24) Eng: fat, grease Span: grasa *UA W H - 1 0 2 Com ROB Ho W PaPi * P N SHL-412 *Csh R O B (2$) Eng: feather, down Span: pluma(s) *UA MIL-168 *NUM Com R O B * P N SHL-325

*puNsi *pu ? i(h) pó:si pui(h) *biro biro

*wi\TihU wfhi nóhna: *sini sini yú ? a- 'render'

*pi' *pihi pt'hi *pi ? i

258

Distant Relationships Csh ROB SC LOR

pici pii, naman

(26) Eng: fire, by means of fire Span: fuego *UA MIL-i7oa *ku, *ko ? *kuhIAN-64 koyohi, koHOW Com ROB kohto:pI, ku*ko*PN ROB Csh ROB ko-, koki kobin- 'boil' (27) Eng: fish Span: pez *UA LAN-146 LIO103Com ROB Ho W *PN SHL-ni Csh ROB SC LOR (28) Eng: fly, make fly Span: volar *UA MIL-183 HOW Com ROB *PN SHL-295 Csh ROB SC LOR (29) Eng: foot Span: pie, pata *UA MIL-187 VVH-28 Com ROB *PN SHL-436 Csh, SC

*per)kwi, *par)k»i *kucu pe:kwi pa:kiw(t) *caca caca 'generic f.' paki, bai, 'f. types' yapa 'small f.'

*hini pooka, piiyala yori*no(ya) noannoya-

*ta, *to *tala ta(h)tásum 'toes' *ta?i, *tatái, ta-

(30) Eng: give; exchange; sell Span: dar; vender *UA W H - 8 3 *maSka Com ROB máka-, ?utU-; hinanahimi- 'exchange' *'?ina; *?inanan*PN SHL-21

Uto-Aztccan and Panoan *PN ROB Csh ROB

SCLOR

*maro- 'sell' '¡nan- 'give' 'inanan- 'exchange' maro- 'sell, buv' mini- 'give' maro- 'exchange'

(31) Eng: good Span: bueno *UA MIL-200 Com ROB Ho W *PN SHL-383 *PN ROB Csh ROB SCLOR

*cam cà: hopi *sara *opi opi hakón

(32) Eng: green Span: verde *UA Com ROB HoW *PN SHL-320 Csh ROB SC L O R

móki *pasa 'v., crudo' pàsa yankón

(33) Eng: hair of head Span: pelo *UA MIL-2i2b CL-240 HoW Com ROB My *PN SHL-106 Csh ROB SC L O R

*po 'body hair' *ku:pa hó:mi papi bówwa 'body h.' *feo: feó: bo:

(34) Eng: hand Span: mano *UA MIL-215 CL-78 CL-242 Com ROB Za *PN SHL-240 Csh, SC

*ma, *mo ? *ma:(v)(V)*ma: ma(h)-, mo?0 màay *mikini, *mibi miki, mi-

26o

Distant Relationships

(35) Eng: head Span: cabeza *UA IAN-138 WH-134

MIL-2i9a Com R O B * P N SHL-221 Csh R O B SC LOR

*pampi 'ino'o pá:pl *mapo, *boskata maská, máposo mápo

(36) Eng: hear, understand Span: oír, entender *kahi, *kaha *UA VVH-126 *kai BAS-p8a (na)ka Com R O B návota Ho VV *nika- 'hear' * P N SHL-148 *k"á' ? i- 'understand' k»iCsh ROB ninkaSC LOR (37) Eng: heart Span: corazon * U A VVH-98 HO VV Com R O B * P N SHL-301 Csh R O B SC L O R

*sula ?inar)\va plcámU, pici *oiti nóito 'heart' oin-'breathe' hointi

(38) Eng: I Span: yo *UA LAN-118 BAS-295

Com R O B * P N SHL-11 Csh R O B SC LOR

nP, nPu

v

ia

(39) Eng: kill Span: matar *UA VVH-45

Com R O B Ho V V * P N SHL-i Csh R O B SC LOR

*kova (pi.) *me péhkamó:ki 'do, kill' ?á-, ríríti

Uto-Aztccan and Panoan (40) Eng: know Span: saber *UA W H - 2 5 Com ROB Ho W * P N SHL-36 Csh R O B

*mati *supana?isúafeetai-, másuabetainanápta * ? oná- 'k., learn' ? onán-

(41) Eng: leaf Span: hoja *UA V V H - 6 4 B&H ROB *NUM Com ROB * P N SHL-325 Csh R O B SC L O R

*sawa *pala*pihi *pihi púhi *pPi píi píi

(42) Eng: lie, lie down Span: acostar(se) *UA W H - 1 3 0 Com Ho V V * P N SHL-356 Csh ROB SC L O R

*po?i, *po?o há:fel, háfeiqá:ci*raka(ra)kátraká-

(43) Eng: liver Span: hígado *UA VVH-89 Ho W Com R O B * P N SHL-438 Csh ROB SC LOR

*niSma níima táni *tak"a ták"a táka

(44) Eng: long Span: largo * U A LIO-294 Com R O B Ho V V *PN Csh R O B SC LOR

*tipi pá ? atí wíiko, wfiyavo ámpako, caskí ninkí 'long fiat' kiyá 'long/tali'

262

Distant Relationships

(45) Eng: louse Span: piojo *UA ROB VVH-24 Ho V V Com *PN SHL-344 Csh ROB

*pusPa '1. of head' *?ati '1 of body' ?âti '1 of body' sâ:bebusia '1. of body' vupusia '1. of head' *?ia '1. of head' ?ia '1. of head' ?upûs 'chigoe'

(46) Eng: man/male Span: hombre, macho *UA BAS 57a *ha'duni'm. relative' *?owi 'male, macho' LIO-24 ténahpi? Com ROB tâaqa Ho W *oni *PN SHL-303 ôni ~ ôn-'i 'man, male relative' Csh ROB (47) Eng: many Span: mucho(s) *UA ROB Ho V V *PN SHL-28 Csh ROB SC L O R (48) Eng: meat, flesh Span: came *UA VVH-22 Com ROB *PN SHL-258 Csh, SC

so:-wiihaq-hiita 'lots of things' *?isca v ica v ica

*tuUku tiihkU, tihkapi *nami

(49) Eng: mountain, hill Span: montana *UA MIL-289a,b LAN-49 WH Com ROB *PN ROB Csh ROB SC L O R

*touno toya:bI *basi bâsi ~ wâsi manâ

(50) Eng: mouth Span: boca *UA MIL-293 WH-19

*teni 'tiUni

*kawi, *kai

Uto-Aztecan and Panoan Ho VV Com ROB * P N SHL-455

Csh R O B

(51) Eng: name Span: nombre *UA IAN-117 Com R O B Ho W * P N SHL-49

Csh R O B SC LOR

mo'a ti:pE *tiso 'throat, neck' tiro 'throat' tisa 'neck' k"ifei 'lips, mouth'

*ni(C)a, *nihnáhni(a)tír)»a *ani áni háni

(52) Eng: neck Span: nuca *UA WH-154

Ho W Ho W Com R O B * P N SHL-455 Csh R O B SC LOR

*kuSta k"a:pi tona 'voice, throat' toyopi, toyo*tiso tiro 'throat' tisa tfso 'neck, trachea, throat'

(53) Eng: new

Span: nuevo * U A IAN-173

Ho W Com R O B *PN ROB * P N SHL-72 Csh R O B SC LOR

*piti(h) 'n., recent' píihi iki *bi ? nä 'young man' i5

binä

(54) Eng: night Span: noche *UA W H - 1 4 4

IAN-228 Ho V V Com R O B * P N SHL-57 Csh R O B

*tuSki~a *tuka mihi, töoki 'last night' tüka(ni) *ba?kisi imi, topifeoki 'sundown, evening'

264

Distant Relationships

(55) Eng: nose Span: nariz *UA W H - n o MIL-308 Ho V V Com ROB *PN ROB Csh ROB

*vaSka *vaka vâqa mu:pl, mubi*rikini rikï

(56) Eng: no, not Span: no *UA VVH-136 MIL-306 Ho V V Com ROB *PN SHL-493 Csh ROB SC L O R

*ka *ka, *kai qâ ké *(-ya)ma -ma vama, ma

(57)

Eng: one Span: uno *UA HAL-33 *NUM Com ROB *PN SHL-480 Csh ROB SC L O R

*se *siin"i simi?, sii *wistita acôsi huistiôra, hâbico

(58) Eng: person, Indian Span: persona *UA IAN-122 *ni(h)mi Ho V V sino Com ROB ni:mt -feu 'people, persons' *PN ROB *honi 'p., man' *PN ROB *-bo 'tribe' Csh ROB oni 'man, persons, people' no: 'enemies' -bo 'tribe, peo.' (59) Eng: rain Span: lluvia *UA LIO-62 HAL-25 *NUM Com ROB Ho V V *PN SHL-33

*horo (vb) *vuki * ? ima ima(h)yo:ki *?oi

Uto-Aztccan and Panoan Csh R O B SC LOR

?

óñe, ? tbe ?ói

(60) Eng: red

Span: rojo *UA VVH-32 MIL-343 Ho W Com R O B * P N SHL-319

*sita *set pála éka-, pási*pasini

(61) Eng: road, path Span: camino *UA V V H - 4 Com R O B * P N SHL-56 Csh ROB SC LOR

*po pú v H *fea ? i, *teai bài ~ wái bài

(62) Eng: root Span: raíz * U A VVH-151 Ho W Com ROB *PN SHL-444 Csh R O B SCLOR

*r)a ná:pi tirahna, k" ítA *tapono tapó tapó

(63) Eng: sand Span: arena * U A IAN-194 Ho W Com R O B * P N SHL-226 Csh R O B SC LOR

*(pa)siwa(h) pisa 'fine sand' pá:si(wapi) *massi mási mási

(64) Eng: say, tell Span: decir * U A BAS-94 MIL-434 IAN-82 HoW Com R O B * P N SHL-62 * P N ROB Csh ROB

*kaiti 'to say' *te 'to tell' kita 'say1 lavayi- 'word' ni-, vik"i- 'say' pana'ai 'praise' *bana- 'talk, say' *ki- 'say'; *ka- 'tell' ki- 'say'; ka- 'tell' bana- 'talk'

266

Distant Relationships

(65) Eng: see Span: ver *UA IAN-159 Ho W Com ROB *PN SHL-33 Csh ROB SC LOR

*puni, *puhyóri puni9, -búni * ? oi-; *isís01

(66) Eng: seed Span: semilla *UA IAN-155 Ho W Com ROB *PN SHL-137 *PN SHL-80 Csh ROB SC LOR

*pu ? i(h) 's., eye' pósimi, pó:si péhe *ïsi *bíro 'eye, s.' biro, ïnsi biro, hísi

(67) Eng: sit Span: sentar *UA VVH-42 WH-76 Com ROB Ho V V *PN SHL-110 Csh ROB SC LOR (68) Eng: skin Span: piel *UA CL-149 Ho W Com ROB *PN SHL-395 Csh ROB *PN SHL-89 (69) Eng: sleep Span: dormir *UA VVH-34 MIL-i2 9 b LIO-91 Com ROB Ho W

*kaSti, *vaNsa há:tel, háfeiqáti; có:va- 'live together, ^ca'ocó:t- 'sit, live' yáka (sg.); sítí (pl.)

*k"itas- 'skin, leather' ci:pi pó^a? (n.) cáhkwe'ya- (v.) *sakata sáka, bíci *bici

*koSci, *koSco *koci, -386 *ku *koco, *koc-i 'áhpi:, íhpii ísuaki- 'snore' tó:ka

Uto-Aztecan and Panoan *PN SHL-39 Csh ROB (70)

Eng: small, little Span: pequeño *UA W H - 1 1 7 IAN-235 Com ROB *PN SHL-336

(7i) Eng: smoke Span: humo *UA W H - 3 5 MIL-392a MIL-329b Ho W Com ROB *PN SHL-187 Csh ROB (72)

?

osaós-

*tima *ti(e)(h) tie-(h)ci 'diminu.' *pi(s)t(i)a

*k"iSci *kwi, *kuhi *k"ici k"i:ci(r)"i) k"i:pi •ko'ini kol

Eng: stand (up) Span: parar, levantar(se) *UA W H - 1 6 1 *wili MIL-412 *ke BAS-I32 *ki'kiva/i Ho W winiCom ROB wini*PN SHL-273 *niCsh ROB ni(ro)SC L O R ni-, winí-

(73) Eng: star Span: estrella *UA W H - 7 1 Az W H - 7 1 Com *PN SHL-489 Csh ROB SC L O R

*su si:(tali:n) tásinüpí, tácinüpi *wistima 'íspa wísti

(74) Eng: stone Span: piedra *UA W H - 1 6 9 BAS-69 Ho W Com ROB *PN SHL-227 Csh ROB SC L O R

*tiUpa *ho'dai 'ówa tí:pl *masás masás makán

268

Distant Relationships

(75) Eng: sun

Span: sol *UA W H - 2 7 *NUM Com R O B Ho V V * P N SHL-63 Csh R O B SC LOR (76) Eng: swim Span: nadar *UA IAN-131 Com R O B Ho V V * P N SHL-291,2 SCLOR Csh R O B (77) Eng: tail Span: cola, rabo * U A VVH-51 HAL-28 BAS-22 Com R O B * P N SHL-142 Csh R O B SCLOR (78) Eng: that (distal) Span: ese, aquel *UA IAN-18 *NUM Com R O B * P N ROB Csh R O B SC LOR (79) Eng: this Span: este (prox.) *UA W H - 1 1 6 BAS-306 Com R O B *PN ROB Csh ROB

*taUca, *tor)a-la *tafec tá:bE tóg-va *bari feári ~ wári bári

*pahapi páhabimómori*nona~onóno míño-

*k"aUsi *k"asi *bahi k"ásl, ínapi 'jerky' *ina ína 'tail, jerky' hína

*u(si[N]) * í> ósi *o 'far'; *a 'near' ó: 'far'; a: 'near' há 'near' '•Viri 'far'

*?i'da, ?i'di?i íni, ? ísi *ini íni

Uto-Aztecan and Panoan (80) Eng: thou, you Span: usted, tú *UABH IAN-22 Com ROB *PN SHL-246 Csh ROB SCLOR (81) Eng: tongue Span: lengua *UA VVH-94 MIL-44ia HAL-50 Com ROB Coa HAL-50 *PN SHL-47 Csh ROB SC L O R (82) Eng: tooth Span: diente *UA VVH-29 MIL-442 Com ROB *PN SHL-414 Csh ROB SC L O R (83) Eng: tree, wood Span: árbol *UA W H - 7 8 MIL-9, 474 Com ROB Ho W *PN SHL-147 Csh ROB SC L O R (84) Eng: two Span: dos *UA W H - 1 0 3 *NUM Com ROB *PN SHL-352 Csh ROB

[cf. *PN*?i *ih ' Í (Sg.), mii (pi.) *mi mi mía

*lir)i *neni *ncn é:kO nánu *ana ána, 'é:- 'swallow' hána

*taSma *tam tá:mA, táma*sita sita sita

*hu *hu hú:hpl, hú:hímicki, hóhi *iwi 'of trees' híwi

*wo*wa:hai'?i wáha(h)*rabita rabí

270

Distant Relationships

(85) Eng: walk Span: caminar, andar * U A IAN-101 *mi?a-, 'go, walk' Com R O B mPa-, k"á- 'go' Ho W níma- 'go' * P N SHL-273 *ni- 'walk, go' Csh R O B ni- ~ nie; k"ân- 'go' -biâ-~kiâ- 'going' (86) Eng: warm, hot Span: caliente * U A IAN-26 Com R O B Ho V V * P N SHL-17 Csh R O B (87) Eng: water Span: agua * U A VVH-123 IAN-127 Com R O B * P N SHL-37 Csh R O B SC L O R (88) Eng: we Span: nosotros * U A BAS-297 ROB Com R O B * P N SHL-283 Csh R O B SC LOR (89) Eng: what Span: que *UA W H - 3 7 VVH-138 IAN-30 Com R O B * P N SHL-43 Csh R O B

*iti(h)-'be h.' ?hPi-

' b e h.'

?

iti 'h. weather' * ? icisa ? icis, irin- 'burn, light fire'

*pa *pa:, *papá: *(' ? om)pasa ( ? om)pás, pa-~ ba( ? om)pás 'w. in a container'

*?a:tPi ~ * ? a:ti ? i *nim nimi *no(-) nó: nóa

*ha- 'what, who' *ha(ki) *hake há *a: á:

Uto-Aztecan and Panoan (90) Eng: white Span: bianco *UA W H - 3 1 MIL-458 IAN-220 Com ROB *PN SHL-306 Csh ROB SCLOR

*toUsa *tosa *tosa tosa *oso ? óso(a) hóso

(91) Eng: who Span: quien *UA WH-138 IAN-30 Com ROB *PN SHL-116 Csh ROB SC L O R

*ha(ki) *hake há-, hákari *co(a) á:, ói coa

(92) Eng: woman Span: mujer *UA W H - 7 9 BAS-319 MIL-472a,b Com ROB *PN SHL-397 Csh ROB SC L O R

*huSpi * ? o'kisi 'w., girl' *nawi, *na sámohpf 'sister' *sano sáno 'aínfeo

(93) Eng: yellow Span: amarillo *UA W H - 6 2 Com ROB *UA BAS-327 *PN SHL-319 *PN SHL-199 Csh ROB SCLOR

*?oha óhapl *uama *pasini *koro 'tan, gray5 kóro, pansía pánsi

(94) Eng: breast Span: pecho, chichi(s) *UA W H - 6 *pi MIL-420 *cun 'suck' HoW pí:hi Com ROB plcámU, píci *PN SHL-429 *soma Csh ROB

272

Distant Relationships

(95) Eng: claw, (toe/finger) nail Span: una *suUtu, *siUtu *UA W H - 2 6 IAN-I93 *situN MIL-298 *sut *NUM *cito Com ROB (ta)si:tO * P N SHL-298 *ocisi Csh R O B '•'on cis (96) Eng: full Span: lleno *UA Com ROB Ho W *PN Csh R O B (97) Eng: horn, antlers Span: cuerno *UA W H - 1 0 4 MIL-235 H o VV Com * P N SHL-207 * P N SHL-8 Csh R O B SCLOR (98) Eng: knee Span: rodilla *UA VVH-30 IAN-108 *NUM Com ROB * P N SHL-359 SC L O R Csh R O B (99) Eng: moon, month Span: luna *UA MIL-286a,b LAN-102 Com ROB H o VV

tihpe?? ó:pokiwta boàkaki pòro- 'fill'

*'awa *'awa ?â:la, -?ala *mâca(n/mV) *?awara 'tapir' manca, '•Y): 'tapir' ? âwa 'tapir'

*torjo *tar)a *tanapi tânapi, tana? *ra-, *-toko rântônko râfeo(soo)

*mea, *meca *mi' ? a(h), *miha(h) mia miiva-wi

U t o - A z t e c a n and P a n o a n *PN SHL-40 Csh R O B

273

«osi ?ósi

(100) Eng: round, circular Span: redondo *UA VVH-148 *cikuri, cikori 'circular' Com R O B tóponi 'round' *PN ROB *toroCsh R O B córoa 'round' tóroki 'circular' topón- 'count, beat of dance in a cir.' NOTES

1. M y fieldwork with the Cashibo was done under the auspices of the Summer Institute of Linguistics, Peru Branch, of which I was a member from 1954 to 1971. The Cashibo/Cacataibo, w h o call themselves oni 'men, people,' arc located in the foothills of the Andes mountains of central Peru, on the Aguaytia, San Alejandro, and Sungaroyacu rivers and their smaller tributaries. 2. This work was published in Spanish in 'Las lenguas paño y su reconstrucción" (Spanish translation by Ezequiel Romero Sanchez-Concha). The Proto Panoan cited here is generally from her listings, though I have used i instead of her ü. 3 . 1 acknowledge professional courtesies from James Loriot, though all mistakes are my own. His list of Shipibo-Conibo was relayed by correspondence. 4. This work was accomplished under National Endowment for the Humanities Research Grant number 2520-8-6006, Project 1366. Permission for working with Canonge's materials was granted by Viola Canonge Frew, S I L Center, Española, N e w Mexico, in 1977. 5. A more detailed discussion of Comanche phonemes is included in Wistrand and Armagost (1990). REFERENCES

Armagost, James L . 1982 "The temporal relationship between telling and happening in Comanche narrative." Anthropological Linguistics 24:193—200. 1983 "Comanche narrative: some general features and a selected text." Kansas Working Papers in Linguistics 8 . 2 : 1 - 2 9 . 1984 "The grammar of personal pronouns in Comanche." Mid-America Linguistics Conference Proceedings, ed. David S. R o o d , pp. 25-35. Boulder: University of Colorado. Bascom, Burton William, Jr. 1965 Proto-Tepiman (Tepehuan-Piman). Ph.D. dissertation, University of Washington. Bright, William and Jane Hill 1967 "The linguistic history of Cupeno." Studies in Southwestern Ethnolinguistics, ed. Dell H . Hymes with William E. Bittle, pp. 351-71. The Hague: Mouton

274

Distant Relationships

Campbell, Lyle and Ronald W. Langacker 1978 "Proto-Aztecan vowels." International Journal of American Linguistics 44:85—102; 197—210; 262—79. [In three parts; last part has the reconstructions.] Canonge, Elliott 1958 Comanche Texts. Norman, Okla.: Summer Institute of Linguistics. [i974]

Freeze, Ray and David E. Iannucci 1979 "Internal classification of the Numic languages of Uto-Aztecan." Amerindia 4:77—92. Hale, Kenneth 1958 "Internal diversity in Uto-Aztecan I." International Journal of American Linguistics 24.2:101-07. 1959 "Internal Diversity in Uto-Aztecan II." International Journal of American Linguistics 25.2:114-21. Heath, Jeffrey 1975 "Major sub-groups in Uto-Aztecan." Paper read at American Anthropological Association meetings, San Francisco, December ;, 1975. Iannucci, David E. 1972 Numic Historical Phonology. Ph.D. dissertation, Cornell University. Kensinger, Kenneth 1981 "Recent publications in Panoan linguistics." International Journal of American Linguistics 47.1 (January 1981): 68-75. 1985 "Panoan linguistic, folkloristic and ethnographic research: retrospect and prospect." South American Indian Languages: Retrospect and Prospea, ed. Harriet E. Manelis Klein and Louisa R. Stark, pp. 224-85. Austin: University of Texas Press. Key, Mary Ritchie 1968 Comparative Tacanan Phonology: With Cavineña Phonology and Notes on Pano-Tacanan Relationship. The Hague: Mouton. 1978 "Araucanian genetic relationships." International Journal of American Linguistics 44.4 (October): 280-93. 1979 The Grouping of South American Indian Languages. Ars Lingüistica, 2. Tubingen: Gunter Narr. 1981a "Intercontinental linguistic connections." Irvine, Ca: Humanities Inaugural Lecture Series, University of California, Irvine. 1981b "North and South American linguistic connections." La Linguistique 17-1:3-18. 1983 "Comparative methodology for distant relationships in North and South American languages." Language Sciences 5.2 (October): 133—54. Kroeber, Alfred L. 1907 Shoshonean Dialects of California. Berkeley: University of California Press. Langacker, Ronald W., ed. 1977 Studies in Uto-Aztecan Grammar Vol. 1. An Overview of Uto-Aztecan Grammar. Dallas: Summer Institute of Linguistics and University of Texas at Arlington.

Uto-Aztccan and Panoan

275

Lehmann, Winfred P. 1962 Historical Linguistics: An Introduction. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston. (Rea one-hundred-word list appears on pp. 112—13.) Lionnet, Andrés 1985 "Relaciones internas de la rama sonorense." Amerindia 10:25-58. Loos, Eugene E. 1964 "Maps of Panoan languages." Unpublished dialect maps showing movement of Panoan languages northward into Peru from Bolivia. 1973a "La señal de transitividad del sustantivo en los idiomas panos." Estudios Panos I, Serie Lingüística Peruana 10, 133-84. Yarinacocha, Peru: Instituto Lingüístico de Verano. 1973b "Algunas implicaciones de la reconstrucción de un fragmento de la gramática del Proto-Pano." Estudios Panos II, Serie Lingüística Peruana 11, 263-82. Yarinacocha, Peru: Instituto Lingüístico de Verano. 1976 Verbos Performativos. Yarinacocha, Peru: Instituto Lingüístico de Verano. Chapter 4: "Datos comparativos de otros idiomas panos," pp. 137-215. Loriot, James 1965 "Shipibo Cognates with Tacanan." Field Notes No. 224, Microfilm Roll 10. Yarinacocha, Peru: Instituto Lingüístico de Verano, 16. McLaughlin, John E. 1982 "From aspect to tense, or, what's -nuh in Comanche." 1982 MidAmerica Linguistics Conference Proceedings, pp. 412-26. Lawrence, Kan.: University of Kansas. 1983 "Gemination and morphologization in Central Numic." Paper read at XXII Conference on American Indian Languages, American Anthropological Association, Chicago, 18 November. Meillet, Antoine 1924 La méthode comparative en linguistique historique. Oslo: Aschehoug. [1967] Translated by G. B. Ford as The Comparative Method in Historical Linguistics. Paris: Champion. Miller, Wick R. 1972 Newe Natekwinappeh: Shoshoni Stories and Dictionary. Salt Lake City: University of Utah Press. 1973 "Some problems in Comanche historical phonology." Paper presented at Linguistic Society of America meetings, San Diego, December 1973. Miller, Wick R., Kevin J. Hegg, and Cindy High 1987 Computerized DataBase for Uto-Aztecan Cognate Sets. Salt Lake City: University of Utah. Rea, John A. 1958 "Concerning the validity of lexico-statistics." International Journal of American Linguistics 24:145-50. Robinson, Dow Frederick 1966 Aztec Studies II: Sierra Nahuat Word Structure. Norman, Okla.: Summer Institute of Linguistics.

276

Distant Relationships

Shell, Olive A. 1950 "Cashibo I: Phonemes." International Journal ofAmerican Linguistics 16.4:198—202. 1975 "Las lenguas paño v su reconstrucción." Estudios Panos III, Serie Lingüística Peruana 12. Yarinacocha, Peru: Instituto Lingüístico de Verano. Swadesh, Morris 1954 "Perspectives and problems of Amerindian comparative linguistics." Word 10.2-3:306-32. Voegelin, Charles F. and Florence M. Voegclin 1957 "Hopi Domains." International Journal of American Linguistics 23.2 (supplement). Voegelin, Charles F., Florence M. Voegelin, and Kenneth L. Hale 1962 "Typological and Comparative Grammar of Uto-Aztecan: I (Phonology)." Indiana University Publications in Anthropology and Linguistics. Memoir 17. International Journal of American Linguistics 28.1:1-144 (supplement). Wistrand [Robinson], Lila 1969 Folkloric and Linguistic Analyses of Cashibo Narrative Prose. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Texas at Austin. Wistrand-Robinson, Lila 1976 "Some generative solutions to problems in Cashibo Phonology." In A. A. Hill Festschrift, Vol. I, 287-95. The Hague: McRitter Publishers. 1983 "Sample cognate list and preliminary sound correspondences toward a North and South American Indian language relationship." Paper read at Southeastern Conference on Linguistics XXVIII, University of Maryland at College Park, 8 April 1983. 1985 "Bi-directional movement in historical change Shoshone: Comanche." Paper read at 1985 Annual Meetings, American Anthropological Association, Washington, D.C. Wistrand-Robinson, Lila and James Armagost 1990 Comanche Dictionary and Grammar. Dallas: Summer Institute of Linguistics, University of Texas at Arlington. Manuscript produced in 1979 by N.E.H. Grant #2520-8-6006, Project 1366, Kansas State University.

Appendix: Language Families

The references cited here are found in the Bibliography, and in chapters of this volume. See Loriot (1964) and Hoijer, Hamp, and Bright (1965) for further references. References marked with asterisks appear in this volume. Aguaruna Gnerre, Larson Arawakan Derbyshire, Gnerre, Heitzman, Matteson, Mosonyi, Noble, Payne (David L.), Shafer, Wise Aruakan Shafer Cari ban Derbyshire, Durbin, Durbin and Seijas, Edwards, Rodrigues, Villalón Chibchan Constenla, González de Pérez, Lévi-Strauss, Levinsohn, Moore, Wheeler Chipaya Olson, Stark Chocó Pardo Chorote Gerzenstein Cinta Larga Crowell Cunza ( = Kunza = Atacama) Lehncrt, Rodríguez Djuka Huttar Gaviáo Crowell Guahiban Christian and Matteson, Kondo, Queixalós

278

Appendix

Guaycuru/Toba Bruno and Najlis Jé/Ge Boswood, Davis, Hamp, Wilbert Juma Pease Mapuche ( = Araucanian) Fernández Garay*, Salas* Mataco Gerzenstein, Najlis Mataguayan Najlis Mundurucu Crofts, Rodrigues Nambiquara Lévi-Strauss, Price Panoan Key, Loos, Migliazza, Shell, Suárez, Wistrand-Robinson* Parintintin Pease Quechuan Cerrón-Palomino, Levinsohn*, Mannheim*, Orr, Orr and Longacre, Parker, Proulx, Stark, Torero Tacanan Key, Suárez Toba Bruno and Najlis, Klein Tucanoan Ardila, Barnes, Gómez-Imbert, Mountain, Waltz and Wheeler Tupi Boswood Tupi-Guaraní Dietrich, Jensen (Allen A.), Jensen (Cheryl), Lemle, Priest, Rodrigues, Soares and Leite* Yagua Payne (Doris L.) Yanomama Migliazza

Bibliography of Comparative Studies

Ardila, Olga 1988 "La sub-familia lingüística Tucano-Oriental: Estado actual y perspectivas de investigación." Seminario-Taller: Estado actual de la clasificación de las lenguas indígenas colombianas. Yerbabuena, Colombia, February 1988. Barnes, Janet 1980 "La reconstrucción de algunas formas del proto Tucano-BarasanoTuyuca." Artículos en Lingüística y Campos Afines 8:37-66. Boswood, Joan 1971 "Phonology and morphology of Rikbaktsa and a tentative comparison with languages of the Tupí and Jé families." Arquivo Lingüístico No. 086. Brasilia: Summer Institute of Linguistics. Bruno, Lidia N. and Elena L. Najlis 1965 Estudio comparativo de vocabularios Tobas y Pilabas. Buenos Aires: Universidad de Buenos Aires. Cerrón-Palomino, Rodolfo 1987 "La flexión de persona y número en el Proto Quechua." In Comparative Linguistics of South American Indian Languages. Special Issue: Language Sciences 9.1:77-89. Christian, Diana R. and Esther Matteson 1972 "Proto Guahiban." In Comparative Studies in Amerindian Languages, ed. Esther Matteson et al., pp. 150-59. The Hague: Mouton. Constenla Umaña, Adolfo 1981 Comparative Chibchan Phonology. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Pennsylvania. 1985 "Clasificación lexicoestadística de las lenguas de la familia Chibcha." Estudios de Lingüística Chibcha 4:1-189. 1988 "Familia lingüística Chibcha." Seminario-Taller: Estado actual de la clasificación de las lenguas indígenas colombianas. Yerbabuena, Colombia, February 1988. Constenla, Umaña Adolfo, director and editor 1983-1986 Estudios de Lingüística Chibcha, Universidad de Costa Rica, San José, Costa Rica. (Contains many articles on comparative problems.) Crofts, Marjorie 1967 "Notas sobre dois dialetos do Munduruku." Atas do Simposio sobre a Biota Amazónica 2:85 - 91. Crowell, Tom 1976 "Comparison of Cinta Larga and Gaviáo." Arquivo Lingüístico, No. 024. Brasilia: Summer Institute of Linguistics.

28o

Bibliography

Davis, Irvine 1966 "Comparative Jé phonology." Estudos Lingüísticos 1.2:10-24. 1968 "Some Macro-Je relationships." International Journal of American Linguistics 34.1142-47. Derbyshire, Desmond 1961 "Notas comparativas sobre tres dialectos Karíbe." Antropológica 14, Bolctim do Museu Paraense Emilio Goeldi, Belem, pp. 1 - 1 0 . 1986 "Comparative survey of morphology and syntax in Brazilian Arawakan." In Handbook ofAmazonian languages: Vol. 1, ed. Desmond C. Derbyshire and Geoffrey K. Pullum, pp. 469-566. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Derbyshire, Desmond C. and Geoffrey K. Pullum, eds. 1986 Handbook of Amazonian Languages, vol. 1. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. 1990 Handbook of Amazonian Languages, vol. 2. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Dietrich, Wolf 1988 "More evidence for an internal classification ofTupi-Guarani." Manuscript. Durbin, Marshall 1977 "A survey of the Carib language family." In Carib-Speaking Lndians: Culture, Society and Language, ed. Ellen B. Basso, pp. 23-38. Tucson: University of Arizona Press. Durbin, Marshall and Havdée Seijas 1972 "The phonological structure of the Western Carib languages of the Sierra de Perijá." del XL Congresso Internazionale degli Americanisti (Estratto): 69-77. 1973 "Proto-Hianacoto: Guaque-Carijona-Hianacoto-Umaua." International Journal ofAmerican Linguistics 39.1:22—31. Edwards, Walter F. 1978 "Some synchronic and diachronic aspects of Akawaio phonology'." Anthropological Linguistics 20.2:77-84. Gerzenstein, Ana 1987 "The Chorote language: Three examples of internal reconstruction." Comparative Linguistics of South American Indian Languages. Special Issue: Language Sciences 9.1 (April): 11—is. Gnerre, Maurizio 1988 "Jivaroan and Maipure-Arawakan." Paper presented at the 46th International Congress of Americanists, Amsterdam. Gómez-Imbert, Elsa 1988 "Problemas en torno a la comparación de las lenguas TukanoOrientales." Seminario-Taller: Estado actual de la clasificación de las lenguas indígenas colombianas. Yerbabuena, Colombia, February 1988. González de Pérez, María Stella 1980 Trayectoria de los estudios sobre la lengua Chibcha o Muisca. Bogotá: Instituto Caro y Cuervo, Series Minor XXII. Greenberg, Joseph H. 1987 Language in the Americas. Stanford, Calif.: Stanford University Press.

Bibliography

281

Hamp, Eric P. 1969 "On Maxakalí, Karajá, and Macro-Jc." International Journal of American Linguistics 35.3: 268-70. Heitzman, Aliene 1975 "Correspondencias entre ciertos grupos de fonemas en varios dialectos Campa." Actas del 39 Congreso Internacional de Americanistas 5:165-79. Lima. Hoijer, Harry, Eric P. Hamp, and William Bright 1965 "Contributions to a bibliography of comparative Amerindian." International Journal ofAmerican Linguistics 31.4 (October): 346—53. Huttar, George L. 1972 "A comparative word list for Djuka." In Languages of the Guianas, ed. Joseph E. Grimes, pp. 12-21. Norman, Okla.: University of Oklahoma Summer Institute of Linguistics. Jakway, Martha 1975 "Listas comparativas de palabras usuales en idiomas vernáculas de la selva." Lima: Instituto Lingüístico de Verano. Datos etnolingüísticos, Microfiche 4. Jensen, Allen A. 1979 "Comparado preliminar das línguas Emerillon e Oiampí no seu desenvolvimento do Proto Tupi-Guaraní." Arquivo Lingüístico No. 135. Brasilia,: Summer Institute of Linguistics and UNICAMP. Jensen, Cheryl 1987 "Object-prefix incorporation in Proto Tupí-Guaraní verbs." Comparative Linguistics ofSouth American Indian Languages. Special Issue: Language Sciences 9.1:45-55. Key, Mary Ritchie 1968 Comparative Tacanan Phonology: With Cavineña Phonology and Notes on Pano-Tacanan Relationship. The Hague: Mouton. 1988 "Situación actual, tareas y problemas de la clasificación de las lenguas indígenas en suramérica." Seminario-Taller: Estado actual de las clasificación de las lenguas indígenas colombianas. Yerbabuena, Columbia, February 1988. Key, Mary Ritchie, ed. 1987 Comparative Linguistics of South American Indian Languages. Special Issue: Language Sciences 9.1. Klein, Harriet E. Manelis 1979 "Comparative Toba-Kadiweu: semantics and morpho/syntax." Paper presented at the Linguistic Society of America, December. Klein, Harriet E. Manelis, and Louisa R. Stark, eds. 1985 South American Indian Languages: Retrospect and Prospect. Austin: University of Texas Press. Kondo, Riena W. 1982 "La familia lingüística Guahibo." Artículos en Lingüística y Campos Afines 11:37-75.

282

Bibliography

Larson, Mildred L. 1957 "Comparación de los vocabularios Aguaruna y Huambisa." Tradición: Revista Peruana de Cultura 7.19-20:147—68. Cuzco, Peru. Lehnert, Roberto 1987 "En torno a la lengua Kunza." Comparative Linguistics ofSouth American Indian Languages. Special Issue: Language Sciences 9.1:103—12. Lemle, Miriam 1971 "Internal classification of the Tupi-Guarani linguistic family." Tupi Studies I, ed. David Bendor-Samuel, pp. 107-29. Norman, Okla.: Summer Institute of Linguistics. Lévi-Strauss, Claude 1948 "Sur certaines similarités structurales des langues Chibcha et Nambikwara." Proceedings of the International Congress of Americanists 28 : 185-92. Levinsohn, Stephen H. 1975 "El Bokotá, el Guaymí, y el Teribe respecto al proto-Chibcha." Lenguas de Panama II. Panama City: Instituto Lingüístico de Verano and Instituto Nacional de Cultura, pp. 3-18. Loos, Eugene 1973 "Algunas implicaciones de la reconstrucción de un fragmento de la gramática del Proto-Pano." Estudios Panos II, Serie Lingüística Peruana 11, 263—82. Yarinacocha, Peru: Instituto Lingüístico de Verano. Loriot, James 1964 "A selected bibliography of comparative American Indian linguistics." International Journal of American Linguistics 30.1 (January): 62-80. Mannheim, Bruce 1983 "Structural change and the structure of change: the linguistic history of Cuzco Quechua in relation to its social history." Ph.D. dissertation, University of Chicago. 1988 "On the sibilants of Colonial Southern Peruvian Quechua." International Journal of American Linguistics 54.2:168-208. Matteson, Esther 1964 "Algunas afiliaciones de la familia Arawak." Actas del 3s Congreso Internacional de Americanistas, 1962, 2:519-25. Mexico, D.F. 1972 "Proto Arawakan." In Comparative Studies in Amerindian Languages, ed. Esther Matteson et al., pp. 160-242. The Hague: Mouton. Matteson, Esther, Alva Wheeler, Frances L. Jackson, Nathan E. Waltz, Diana R. Christian 1972 Comparative Studies in Amerindian Languages. The Hague: Mouton. Migliazza, Ernest C. 1978 "The position of Yanomama and Panoan in South America." Paper presented at the Linguistic Societv of America Summer Meeting, Urbana. 1983 "Lenguas de la región del Orinoco-Amazonas: Estado actual." America Indígena 43.4:703-84.

Bibliography

283

Moore, Bruce R. 1962 "Correspondences in South Barbacoan Chibcha." In Studies in Ecuadorian Indian Languages: I, ed. Benjamin Elson, pp. 270—89. Norman, Okla.: Summer Institute of Linguistics. Mosonyi, Esteban Emilio 1988 "Familia lingüística Arawak: Colombia-Venezuela." Seminar-Taller: Estado actual de la clasificación de las lenguas indígenas colombianas. Yerbabuena, Colombia, February 1988. Mountain, Kathy 1978 "Lista de palabras Swadesh y Rowe." Artículos en Lingüística y Campos Afines 4:1-57. Najlis, Elena L. 1968 "Dialectos del Mataco." Anales No. 4, Universidad del Salvador, Buenos Aires, pp. 232-41. 1971 "Premataco phonology." International Journal of American Linguistics 37.2:128-30. 1984 Fonología de la protolengua Mataguaya. Cuadernos de Lingüística Indígena 9, Buenos Aires: Universidad de Buenos Aires, Facultad de Filosofía y Letras, Instituto de Lingüística. Noble, G. Kingsley 1965 "Proto-Arawakan and its descendants." International Journal of American Linguistics 31.3 (July), part II: 1-129 (also pub. 38, Indiana University Research Center in Anthropology, Folklore and Linguistics, Bloomington). Olson, Ronald D. 1964 "Mayan affinities with Chipava of Bolivia I: Correspondences." International Journal of American Linguistics 30.4:313—24. 1965 "Mayan affinities with Chipaya of Bolivia II: Cognates." International Journal of American Linguistics 31.1:29—38. 1980 "Algunas relaciones del Chipaya de Bolivia con las lenguas Mayanses." Notas Lingüísticas de Bolivia 11, Instituto Lingüístico de Verano. Orr, Carolina D. 1978 "Dialectos Quichuas del Ecuador: Con respecto a lectores principiantes." Cuadernos Etnolingüísticos 2. Quito, Ecuador: Instituto Lingüístico de Verano. Orr, Carolyn and Robert E. Longacre 1968 "Proto-Quechumaran." Language 44.3 (September): 528—55. Pardo, Mauricio 1988 "Grupo Chocó." Seminario-Taller: Estado actual de la clasificación de las lenguas indígenas colombianas." Yerbabuena, Colombia, February 1988. Parker, Gary J. 1969-71 "Comparative Quechua phonology and grammar" University of Hawaii Working Papers in Linguistics I: 1.1:65—88; II: 1.2:123—47; III: 1.4:1-61; IV: 1.9:149-204; V: 3.3:45-109.

284

Bibliography

Payne, David L. 1987 "Some morphological elements of Maipuran Arawakan: Agreement affixes and the genitive construction." Comparative Linguistics ofSouth American Indian Languages, Special Issue: Language Sciences 9.1: 57-751988 "Una vision panorámica de la familia lingüística Arawak." SeminarioTaller: Estado actual de la clasificación de las lenguas indígenas colombianas. Yerbabuena, Colombia, February 1988. Payne, Doris L. 1984 "Evidence for a Yaguan-Zaparoan connection." Workpapers of the Summer Institute of Linguistics, University of North Dakota, pp. 131-56. 1987 "Noun classification in the Western Amazon." Comparative Linguistics of South American Indian Languages, Special Issue: Language Sciences 9.1:21—44. Pease, Helen 1977 "Juma-Parintintin similarities." Arquivo Lingüístico, No. 038. Brasilia, D. F.: Summer Institute of Linguistics. Pottier, Bernard 1988 "Tipología de las lenguas indígenas de América." Seminario-Taller: Estado actual de la clasificación de las lenguas indígenas colombianas. Yerbabuena, Colombia, February 1988. Price, P. David 1976 "Southern Nambiquara phonology." International Journal of American Linguistics 42.4:338-48. 1978 "The Nambiquara linguistic family." Anthropological Linguistics 20.1: 14-37Priest, Perry N. 1987 "A contribution to comparative studies in the Guaraní linguistic family." Comparative Linguistics of South American Indian Languages, Special Issue: Language Sciences 9.1:17-20. Proulx, Paul 1987 "Quechua and Aymara." Comparative Linguistics of South American Indian Languages, Special Issue: Language Sciences 9.1:91-102. Queixalós, Francisco 1988 "La familia Guahibo." Seminario-Taller: Estado actual de la clasificación de las lenguas indígenas colombianas. Yerbabuena, February 1988. Rodrigues, Arvon Dall'Igna 1980 "Tupi-Guarani e Mundurukú: Evidencias lexicais e fonológicas de parentesco genético." Estudos Lingüísticos III, pp. 194—209. Araraquara, Universidade Estadual Paulista. 1985 "Evidence for Tupi-Carib relationships." In South American Indian Languages: Retrospect and Prospect, ed. Harriet E. Manelis Klein and Louisa R. Stark, pp. 371-404. Austin: University of Texas Press. Rodríguez de Montes, María Luisa, ed. In press Memorias del seminario: estado actuel de la clasificación de lenguas indígenas Colombianas. Bogotá: Instituto Caro v Cuervo.

Bibliography

285

Ruhlen, Merritt 1987 A Guide to the World's Languages, Vol. 1: Classification. Stanford, Calif. : Stanford University Press. Shafer, Robert 1959 "Algumas equagóes fonéticas em Arawakan." Anthropos 54.3-4: 542—62. 1962 "Aruakan (not Arawakan)." Anthropological Linguistics 4.4:31-40. Shell, Olive Alexandra 1965 Pano Reconstruction. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Pennsylvania. University Microfilms 66-4648. Stark, Louisa R. 1972 "Maya-Yunga-Chipavan: A new linguistic alignment." International Journal of American Linguistics 38.2:119-35. 1975 "A reconsideration of proto-Quechua phonology." Actas del 39 Congreso Internacional de Americanistas 5:209-19. Lima. Suárez, Jorge A. 1969 "Moseten and Pano-Tacanan." Anthropological Linguistics 11.9 (December): 255-66. 1973 "Macro-Panoan-Tacanan." International Journal of American Linguistics 39.3 (July): 137-54Torero Fernández de Cordova, Alfredo 1964 "Los dialectos Quechuas." Anales Científicos de la Universidad Agraria 2:446-78. Waltz, Nathan E. and Alva Wheeler 1972 "Proto Tucanoan." In Comparative Studies in Amerindian Languages, ed. Esther Matteson et a!., pp. 119-49. The Hague: Mouton. Wheeler, Alva 1972 "Proto-Chibchan." In Comparative Studies in Amerindian Languages, ed. Esther Matteson et al., pp. 93-108. The Hague: Mouton. Wilbert, Johannes 1962 "A preliminary glottochronologv of Ge." Anthropological Linguistics 4.2:17-25. Wise, Mary Ruth 1986 "Grammatical characteristics of Preandine Arawakan languages of Peru." In Handbook ofAmazonian languages, Vol. I, eds. Desmond C. Derbyshire and Geoffrey K. Pullum, pp. 567-642. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

Contributors

ANA FERNÁNDEZ GARAY is Assistant Researcher at the Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (CONICET), Buenos Aires. She earned the degree "Dipióme d'Etudes Approfondies de Linguistique," Paris V, France. Her publications include "Formación de temas verbales araucanos" (1979); "Rogativas Mapuche" (1982); and Relevamiento lingüístico de hablantes mapuches en la Provincia de La Pampa (1988).

JULIAN GRANBERRY is a staff linguist and archaeologist with The Bahamas Archaeological Team, Nassau. His research and published work in these fields has been concentrated on the southeastern United tor for the ongoing Caribbean Handbook Series, published by the Phileas Society (Detroit/Ft. Lauderdale), and volume editor for the Handbook of Caribbean Peoples and Handbook of Caribbean Languages in that series. MARY RITCHIE KEY is Professor of Linguistics at the University of California, Irvine. She has published linguistic studies in comparative American Indian languages, sociolinguistics, phonology, and nonverbal behavior. Most recently she has edited General and Amerindian Ethnolinguistics: In Remembrance of Stanley Newman (1989), with Henry M. Hoenigswald. YONNE DE FREITAS LEITE is Associate Professor, Department of Anthro-

pology, Museu Nacional (Federal University of Rio de Janeiro), with a Ph.D. from the University of Texas. Her specialties are Brazilian Indian languages (Tupi-Guarani) and Portuguese phonology. Her publications include "Lingüística e Antropología" (1975); Aspectos da fonología e morfofonologia Tapirapé (1977); "A classifi^acáo do Tapirapé na familia Tupi-Guarani" (1982); and "As vogais pretónicas na fala carioca " (in collaboration with Dinah Callou). STEPHEN H. LEVINSOHN has worked with the Inga people since 1968 and presently is a linguistic consultant to the Inga organization Musu Runacuna. He is an international linguistic consultant with the Summer Institute of Linguistics. His recent work includes Textual Con-

288

Contributors

nections in Acts (1987), and the editing of Thematic Continuity and Development in Languages of Malaysia. B R U C E M A N N H E I M , P H . D . in anthropology and linguistics from the University of Chicago, has taught at Cornell University, the University of Arizona, and the University of Michigan. He is author of The Language of the Inka Since the European Invasion and numerous articles on Quechua language, culture, and history. He is currently completing a book on verbal art and cultural transmission. A U R O R E M O N O D - B E C Q U E L I N is a Director of Research at the Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Laboratoire d'Ethnologie et de Sociologie Comparative, Université Paris X. Her main fields of research are Mayan linguistics, South American Indian languages and rituals, and oral tradition (rhetorical structures and ethnopoetics). She is the author of La pratique linguistique des Indiens Trumai and numerous articles. G U S T A V O R O D R Í G U E Z is Associate Professor, Institute of Hispanic Philology, Universidad Austral, Valdivia, Chile. His specializations are Hispanic dialectology and general and applied linguistics. His publications include "Linguistic Features of Atacameñan Spanish" (198081); "Substratum Effects in Atacameñan Spanish" (1980—81); and Mother Tongue Methodology (1983). A D A L B E R T O SALAS is Profesor de Lingüística, Universidad de Concepción, Chile, and holds a Ph.D. in linguistics/ethnolinguistics from the State University of New York. His specialties include phonological and morphosyntactical studies on the Chilean languages: Aymara, Rapa Nui (Easter Island), Yamana or Yahgan (Tierra del Fuego), and, especially, Mapuche or Araucanian. M A R I L I A F A C Ó S O A R E S is Professor of Linguistics, Department of Anthropology, Museu Nacional (Federal University of Rio de Janeiro). She has studied Guajajara, Asurini, Kokama, Kayabi, and Tapirapé of the Guarani family, in addition to the unclassified Tikuna, which is the subject of her most important publications. M A R Í A E U G E N I A V I L L A L Ó N , Ph.D. candidate, University of California, Berkeley, has worked on several anthropology projects for government agencies in Venezuela. She has authored Aspeaos de la Organización social y la terminología de parentesco E'ñapa and "Network Organization in E'ñapa Society," and is currently researching Panare narrative. She currently teaches in the Department of Anthropology at the Universidad Central de Venezuela, Caracas.

Contributors

289

LILA WISTRAND-ROBINSON is Professor of Language/Social Sciences,

Southern Baptist College, Arkansas, and holds a Ph.D. in linguistics and anthropology/folklore from the University of Texas. She did fieldwork with the Shapra, Aguaruna, Cashibo (1954—65); and with the Iowa-Otoe (Siouan) of Kansas and Oklahoma (1972—74). She has taught at Kansas State University and Liberty University. Her numerous publications include "A Mandarin Grammar Sketch" (with Ling-Mei Lee-Liang et al.) and Comanche Dictionary and Grammar (with James Armagost).

Index

Abercrombie, David, +6 Aberdour, Catherine, 51 Acuña, Leonor, 102 acute, 118 Abam, Lucien, 235 affective, 102 Aguaruna, 277 Aguilar M., Juan de, 122 Ahlbrinck, W., 89 Allaire, Louis, 56 allophones, 50 "Amazon stones," 85 Anttila, Raimo, 68 Araucanian, 166, 278 Arawak, 33 Arawakan, 6, 11, 212, 277 Araweté, 36 Araya, Angel, 192 Ardila, Olga, 278 Arekuna, 55 Argentina, 97 Armagost, James L., 244, 246 Armelleda, Cesáreo de, 89 Aruakan, 11, 277 Arvelo-Jiménez, Nelly, 82 aspect, 149 aspirate(d) stop, 113 aspiration, 115 asterisk (* = proto form), 11 Asurini, 36 Atacama, 181, 277 Augusta, Fray Félix José de, 109 Austin, John L., 28 Avendaño, Luis Francisco, 147 Aymara, 182 background-foreground, 149 Bakairi, 55 Balbi, Adrien, 8 Barnes, Janet, 278 Barrai, Basilio Maria de, 218 Barton, Benjamin Smith, 5

Bascom, Burton Williams, Jr., 244 Bendor-Samuel, David, 45 Benvenistc, Emile, 26,30 Black, Paul, 55 Bontkes, Carol, 46 Bontkes, Willem, 46 Borhegyi, Stephen, 228 Boswood, Joan, 278 Braun, Use, 46 Brerewood, Edward, 4 Bright, William, 11, 244 Brinton, Daniel Garrison, 8 Bruno, Lidia N., 278 Buchwald, Otto von, 191 Bullen, Ripley P., 199 Burns, Donald H., 148 Campbell, Lyle, 244 Campos Menchaca, S., Mariano José, 176 "cannibal," 55 canoe (builders), 55, 85 Canongc, Elliott, 246, 249 Carib, 29, 33, 55 Cariban, 54, 277 Casamiquela, Rodolfo, 98 Cashibo, 243 Castellvi, Marcelino de, 89 Catherine the Great, 5 Cayapa, 11 Centeno, Gabriel, 120 ceramic wares, 229 ceremonial chant, 181 Cerrón-Palomino, Rodolfo, 135, 278 Chaima, 55 "chance," 7 Chandra, Helen, 239 Changos, 182 Chasoy Sijindioy, José, 150, 161 Chávez Reyes, A., 115 Chen, Matthew Y., 112, 128 Chibchan, 204, 216, 277 Chile, 97

292

Index

Chingay, 182 Chipaya, 11, 277 Chocó, 277 Chomsky, Noam, 29 Chorote, 277 Christian, Diana R., 277 Cinta Larga, 277 Civrieux, Marc de, 83 cladistics, 12 Claflin, William H . , Jr., 230 Clairis, Christos, 108 classification, 21 Coe, Michael, 228 cognate, 12 Cohen, Marccl, 8 Colla, 182 Collier, Donald, 239 Colorado, 11 Columbus, 3,55 Comanche, 243 Comenius, Jan [Johann] Amos, 8 Common Southern Peruvian Quechua, 112 comparative linguistics, 95 consonant lenition, in Constenla Umana, Adolfo, 11, 277 Conzemius, Eduard, 211 Cooper, John M., 98 Coppens, Walter, 86 Corradini, Henry E., 88 Crawford, James M., 203 creolized, 204, 228 Créqui-Montfort, Georges de, 137 Crocker, J. Christopher, 27 Croese, Robert A., 105, 176 Crofts, Marjorie, 46, 278 Crowell, Tom, 277 Cuatindioy C., Miguel, 161 Cumanagoto, 55 Cunza ( = Kunza = Atacama), 181, 277 Cusihuaman Gutierrez, Antonio, 137 d'Orbigny, Alcides, 183 Davis, Irvine, 11, 278 Deagan, Kathleen A., 197 Derbyshire, Desmond C., 7, 277 Descartes, René, 27 dialects, ni, 1 1 2 , 1 1 9 , 1 4 5 , 1 6 0 , 200 Diebold, Richard A., Jr., 86 Dietrich, Wolf, 278 discourse, 135

discourse-level formative, 133 distant relationships, 13, 193 Djuka, 277 Dobson, Rose, 44 Droixhe, Daniel, 33 dual, 168 Ducrot, Oswald, 25 Duponceau, Peter S., 5 Durbin, Marshall, 55, 63, 277 Dyen, Isidore, 69 Echeverría Weasson, Max Sergio, 109 Echeverría y Reyes, Aníbal, 182 Edgerton, Franklin, 6 Edwards, Clinton R . , 228 Edwards, Walter E., 277 Elson, Benjamin, 176 Epling, P. J., 71 ergative, 29 Espinoza Perez, Lucas, 89 ethnolinguistics, 23, 179 exogamy, 223 Fairbanks, Charles H . , 240 families (language), 277 Faron, Louis C., 58 Febrés, Andrés, 103 Feldman, Robert A., 226 Fernández Garay, Ana V., 97,176, 278 Florida, 195 fluctuation, 43 Ford, James A., 203 foregrounding, 149 formative, 133 Freeman, John Finley, 5 Freeze, Ray, 274 fricative, 97 frozen compound, 219 Calibi, 55 Gama Malcher, José M., 62 Carreta, Jacinto, 161 Gatschet, Albert Samuel, 202 Gaviào, 277 G c ( = Jè), 12, 33. See also Jé genetic classification, 29 Gerzenstein, Ana, 277, 278 Gesner, Conrad, 4 Gilij, Felipe Salvador, 6, 60 Girard, Victor, 61

Index Givón, T., 149 glottalization, 113,115 glottal stop, 115 glottochronology, 55, 244 Gncrre, Maurizio, 277 Goeje, Claudius Henricus de, 89 Goggin, John M., 201 Golbert, Perla, 102 gold, 85 Gómez-Imbert, Elsa, 278 González Holguín, Diego de, 113 González de Pérez, María Stella, 277 González, Raúl, 191 Goodman, Morris, 205, 225 Graham, Albert, 45 Graham, Susan, 45 Cranberry, Julian, 195 grave, 11S Gray, Louis H., 8 Greenberg, Joseph H., 9,30, 61, 204, 211 Greene, John C., 4 Greenwell, Dale, 234 Guacho, Juan Naula, 148 Guahiban, 277 Guajajara, 36 Guarnan Poma de Ayala, Felipe, 120 Guarani, 37 Guarayo, 37 Guaycuru, 278 Gudschinsky, Sarah, 45 Gününa-küne, 98 Haas, Mary R., 220, 232 habitual, 163 Hagègc, Claude, 28 Hale, Kenneth, 244 hammock, 64 Hamp, Eric P., 11, 278 Hancock, R. G. V., 234 Harrison, Carl, 44 Haven, Samuel F., 4 Heath, Jeffrey, 274 Hegg, Kevin J., 244 Heimann, Robert B., 234 Heitzman, Aliene, 277 Henley, Paul, 62 Hervás y Panduro, Lorenzo, 7 Hianacoto-Umaua, 55 High, Cindy, 244 Hildebrandt, Martha, 89

Hill, Jane, 244 historical, 3 history of early comparative work, 10 Hittite, 25 Hockett, Charles F., 135 Hoenigswald, Henry M., 12, 78 Hoff, Berend, 56, 219 Hoijer, Harry, 11 Holmer, Nils Magnus, 209 Hopper, Paul J., 149 Horcasitas, Fernando, 135 Howard-Malverde, Rosaleen, 148 Hoyos, Felix, 182 Huarochiri, 120, 138 Humboldt, Alexander, 6,59 Humboldt, Wilhelm, 6 Huttar, George L., 277 Hymes, Dell, 28, 64 hypothetical (mode), 171 Iannucci, David E., 244 Indo-European, 25 Inga Quechua, 145 Instituto Caro y Cuervo, 10 internal reconstruction, HI, 119 "Island Carib," 56 Jakway, Martha, 281 Jamioy Y. de Peria, Otilia, 157 Je ( = Ge), 11, 278. See also Ge Jeldes, Fidel, 191 Jensen, Allen A., 278 Jensen, Cheryl, 278 Jones, Daniel, 48 Juma, 278 Kakumasu, James, 44 Kamarakoto, 55 Kamayura, 37 Kari'na, 55 Kensinger, Kenneth, 274 Key, Mary Ritchie, 3,54, 61, 103, 211, 245, 278 Kimball, Geoffrey, 232 Kirk, Jerome, 71 Klein, Harriet E. Manelis, 7, 278 Koch-Griinberg, Theodor, 89 Kokama/Cocama, 37 Kondo, Riena W., 277 Krishnamurti, Bh., 128

293

294

Index

Kristeva, Julia, 25 Kroeber, Alfred L . , 274 Kuikuru, 29

Mapuche ( = Araucanian), 9 7 , 1 6 6 , 278 Markham, Clements, 122

K u n z a ( = Cunza), 181, 277

mass comparison, 55 Mataco, 278 Mataguayan, 278

L a b o v , William A . , 128,135

Mason, John Alden, 61

Lagos Altamirano, Daniel, 109 Landar, Herbert, 56

Mattei-Muller, Marie-Claude, 62

Landerman, Peter, 116 Langacker, Ronald W., 244

Maupertuis, Pierre Louis Maureau, 5

Matteson, Esther, n , 207, 216, 277

language isolates, 24

Mayan, n, 33 Mclntyre, Loren, 6

Lanning, E d w a r d , 58

McLaughlin, John E., 246

Larsen, Helen, 1+9 Larson, Lewis H . , Jr., 231 Larson, Mildred L . , 277 Lastra de Suârcz, Yolanda, 135

McQuown, Norman A., 9,54 Meillet, Antoine, 8, 25, 118, 135, 244 Méndez-Arocha, Alberto, 89 metathesis, 124, 137

Lathrap, Donald W., 57, 211, 227 Layrisse, Miguel, 66 legendary, 149

Metcalf, George J., 10

Lehnert, Roberto, 191, 277

Milanich, Jerald T., 197 Miller, Wick R . , 244

Leite, Yonne, 36, 278 Lemle, Miriam, 36, 278 L e M o y n e , Jacques, 199 Lenz, R o d o l f o , 104 Lévi-Strauss, Claude, 28, 277, 278 Levinsohn, Stephen H . , 145, 277, 278 L e w i n , Boleslow, 132

Migliazza, Ernest C . , 61, 218, 278 migrations, 58,59, 145, 161, 211, 230

mindala, 227 Mithridates, 4 mixed languages, 225 Moesbach, Ernesto Wilhelm de, 100 M o j o m b o y P., Lázaro, 154

lexicostatistical, 69 Likan Antai, 181 linguistic interpénétration, 225

Molina, Cristóbal de, 119 Monod-Becquelin, Aurore, 23 Monserrat, R u t h , 45 M o o r e , Bruce R . , 11, 277

Lionnet, Andres, 244 Lira, Jorge A . , 137

M o o r e , Dennis, 46 Morales, Filadelfo, 82

Loewen, Jacob A . , 213

Morey, Nancy K . C., 82

Longacre, R o b e r t E., 1 1 , 1 4 7 , 278 L o o s , Eugene E., 1 1 , 1 4 7 , 278

morphophonemic process, 135

Lorant, Stefan, 199

Moseley, Michael E . , 226 Mosonyi, Esteban Emilio, 277

Loriot, James, 11, 244 Loukotka, Cestmir, 8,57, 110, 218, 225

Mostny, Crete, 182, 185 M o t h e r Earth, 184

Lounsbury, Floyd G . , 28, 204 L o z a n o M . , Pedro, 183

Mountain, Kathy, 278 multidimensional scaling technique, 54

lumpers, 9

multilingualism, 223 Mundurucu, 278 Muskogean, 220

Macoita, 55 Maipuran, 212 Makiritare, 55 Makushi, 55 Malkiel, Yakov, 135 manioc, 55, 64 Mannheim, Bruce, h i , 278 Mansilla, L u c i o Victorio, 98

Muysken, Pieter, 147 Najlis, Elena L . , 278 Nambiquara, 278 nasals, 124 Newfield, Madeleine, 115 Newkumet, Phil J., 234

Index Nicholson, Vclda, 51 Noble, G . Kingsley, 11, 218, 277 nonremotc past, 171 numbers, 212 Oayana, 55 Oberhauser, F., 191 Occam's razor, hi Ollantay, 120, 138 Olson, Gary, 44 Olson, Ronald D., 11, 277 Orr, Carolyn (Carolina D.), n, 147, 278 Osborn, Henry, 206 Oswalt, Robert L., 88 pachamama, 184 Paezan, 216 Pallas, Peter Simon, 5 Panare, 55 Panoan, 11, 211, 243, 278 paralinguistic, 103 Pardo, Mauricio, 277 Pareja, Francisco, 199 Parintintin, 37, 278 Parker, Gary J., 112, 159, 278 Paulhan, Jean, 26 Payne, David L., 277 Payne, Doris L., 278 Pease, Helen, 44, 278 Pedersen, Holger, 9 Pemon, 55 Percival, W. Keith, 3 Perez [de] Bocanegra, Juan de, 120 "petrified affix," 219 Phelps, David S., 231 philological, 112 phonetics, 43 Pickering, John, 5 Pickett, Velma, 176 pochteca, 227 Pottier, Bernard, 284 Price, P. David, 278 Priest, Perry N., 278 pronoun, 212, 214 Prost, Gilbert R., 212 proto-language, 11 Proulx, Paul, 278 Pullum, Geoffrey K., 7 Puquina, 182 push-chain, 36

295

Quechua, in, 182 Quechua A, 112 Quechua II, 112 Quechuan, 11, 145, 278 Queixalós, Francisco, 277 quotative tense, 159 r-spirantization, 125 Ranquel, 97 Rea, John A., 244 real (mode), 171 Reland [Reeland], Adrian, 4 reportative, 163 "reputed to," 163 retroflexed flap, 147 Rivet, Paul, 8,137 Rochefort, Charles de, 59 Rodrigues, Aryon (Arion) Dall'Igna, 12, 36, 216, 277, 278 Rodríguez de Montes, María Luisa, 7, 10 Rodríguez, Gustavok, 181, 277 Ross, Eileen, 148 Roth, Walter E., 227 Rouse, Irving, 204, 227 Rowe, John Howland, 57, 113 Rucuyen, 55 Ruiz Blanco, Matias, 89 Ruwet, Nicolas, 29 Salad, Adalberto, no, 116, 278 Salomon, Frank, 227 salt, 234 San Román, Francisco J., 182 Sánchez Labrador, José, no Sandberg, Peter, 46 Santa Cruz Pachacuti Yamqui, Juan de, 120 Santo Tomás, Domingo de, 116 Sapir, J., David, 27 Sapir-Whorf hypothesis, 33 Scaliger, Joseph Justus, 4 Schindler, Helmut, 89 Schmidt, Wilhelm, 8 Schomburgk, Moritz Richard, 227 Schüller, Rodolfo R., 61 Seijas, Haydée, 55, 277 seventeenth century, 195 Shafer, Robert, 11, 277 Shell, Olive Alexandra, 11, 244, 278 Silva, Marcio, 44 Simpson, George Gaylord, 61

296

Index

Siriono, 37 sixteenth century, 166 slaves, 85 Soares, Marilia Faco, 36, 278 Sorenson, Arthur P., Jr., 223 Soto Ruiz, Clodoaldo, 115 sound correspondences, 6, 11 sound symbolism, 135 Southern Peruvian Quechua, 111 spatial model, 54 splitters, 9 Stark, Louisa R., 7, 115, 277, 278 Steinen, Karl von den, 59, 89 Steward, Julian H . , 58 Stimmell, Carole, 234 Sturtevant, William C., 200 Suarez, Jorge A., 9 , 1 0 1 , 278 "sudden discovery," 1 4 8 - 1 4 9 Sued-Badillo, Jalil, 55 Swadesh, Morris, 9, 216, 244 Swanton, John R., 199 Swiggers, Pierre, 34 syllable structure, 117,128 Tacanan, 11, 278 Tandioy J., Francisco, 163 Tandioy T., Luis Manuel, 152 Tapirape, 36 Taurepan, 55 Tauste, Francisco de, 64 Taylor, Douglas M., 56 Tedlock, Dennis, 28 tense-aspect, 145 Tercer Concilio Limense, 120 texts, 119, 161, 185 textual records, 119 Timucua, 195 Toba, 278 Todorov, Tzvetan, 25 Torero Fernandez de Cordova, Alfredo, 112, 278 Tovar, Antonio, 4, 8, 61 Tovar, Consuelo Larrucea de, 4 Trillos Amaya, Maria, 10 Trubetzkoy, N. S., 137 Trumai, 33 Tschudi, Johann Jakob von, 185 Tucanoan, 216, 223, 278 Tupi, 33, 278

Tupi-Guarani, 12, 36, 278 Tupinambä, 37 Turner, R . L., 135 "twenty" ( = "person"), 83 Tyler, Stephen, 28 typological, 21, 30, 36

unclassified languages, 23 uniformitarian, 111 universal laws, 25 universalist, 28 uro,182 Urubu, 37 Usca Paucar, 138 Uto-Aztecan, 243

Vaisse, Emilio, 182 Valdivia, P. Luis de, 103, 166 Vansina, Jan, 86 variation in space, 128 variation in time, 128 Véliz, Orieta, 192 Verdier, Yvonne, 27 Villalón, Maria Eugenia, 54, 277 Vinson, Julien, 235 Viveiros de Castro, Eduardo, 37 Voegelin, Charles F., 244 Voegelin, Florence M . , 244 volitive (mode), 171 vowel harmony, 51, 208 vowel shift, 36 Wallace, Alfred Russel, 227 Waltz, Nathan E., 216, 278 Wang, William S.-Y., 112,128 Warao, 204, 206 Weber, David John, 149 Weiss, Helga, 4 4 Wheeler, Alva, 210, 277, 278 Whitney, William Dwight, 135 Whorf, Benamin, 4 Wiener, Linda F., 12 Wilbert, Johannes, 12, 66, 215, 278 Willey, Gordon R., 231 Williams, James, 89 Wise, Mary Ruth, 277 Wistrand-Robinson, Lila, 243, 278 word lists, 8 written records, HI

Index Yabarana, 55 Yagua, 278 Yangues, Manuel de, 89 Yanomama ( = Yanomami, Yanomamö), 58, 278

Ye'kwana, 55 Yukpa, 55 Zahn, Charlotte, 149 Zeidler, James, 228

297

This book has been set in Linotron Galliard. Galliard was designed for Mergenthaler in 1978 by Matthew Carter. Galliard retains many of the features of a sixteenth century typeface cut by Robert Granjon but has some modifications which gives it a more contemporary look. Printed on acid-free paper.