Handbook of Police Psychology [Revised] 2018060197, 9780367209070, 9781138917057, 9780429264108


430 116 5MB

English Pages [667] Year 2019

Report DMCA / Copyright

DOWNLOAD PDF FILE

Table of contents :
Cover
Half Title
Title
Copyright
Dedication
Contents
Preface to the Revised Edition
About the Editor
1 Introduction and History of Police Psychology
Part 1 General Practice
2 Police Psychological Consultation Services to Public Safety
3 Legal Issues Related to Hiring and Promotion of Police Officers
4 Ethical Issues in Police Psychology
5 Police vs. Probation/Surveillance Officers: Similarities and Differences
Part 2 Pre-employment Psychological Screening
6 Criterion-Related Psychological Evaluations
7 Actuarial vs. Clinical Judgment Prediction Models in Pre-employment Psychological Screening of Police Candidates
8 Appraising and Managing Police Officer Performance
9 Assessments for Selection and Promotion of Police Officers
10 The Integration Section of Forensic Psychological Reports in Law Enforcement: Culturally Responsive Ending Words
11 Challenging the Police De-selection Process
Part 3 Training and Evaluation
12 Couples Counseling/Assessment and Use of the Inwald Relationship Surveys
13 Fitness-for-Duty Evaluations
14 Methods for Real-Time Assessment of Operational Stress During Realistic Police Tactical Training
15 Evolution of Police Leadership Enhancement
16 When Cops Kill: Understanding the Psychology of Deadly Force Encounters
Part 4 Police Procedure
17 Police Use of Force
18 Hostage Negotiations
19 Domestic Violence: An Analysis of the Crime and Punishment of Intimate Partner Abuse
20 Police Interviews With Suspects: International Perspectives
21 Applying Restorative Justice Principles in Law Enforcement
Part 5 Clinical Practice
22 Police Personality: Theoretical Issues and Research
23 Police and Public Safety Complex Trauma and Grief: An Eco-ethological Existential Analysis
24 Suicide in Law Enforcement
Part 6 Treatment and Dysfunction
25 Cops in Trouble: Psychological Strategies for Helping Officers Under Investigation, Criminal Prosecution, or Civil Litigation
26 Evidence-Based Psychological Interventions to Promote Officer Resiliency
27 Critical Incident Reactions and Early Interventions
28 Critical Knowledge for Clinicians Debriefing Critical Incidents in Law Enforcement
29 The Disconnected Values Model: A Brief Intervention of Improving Healthy Habits and Coping With Stress in Law Enforcement
30 Mentally Ill—Crying “Help!” Police and Public Health Prevention, Intervention and Post-vention
Index
Recommend Papers

Handbook of Police Psychology [Revised]
 2018060197, 9780367209070, 9781138917057, 9780429264108

  • 0 0 0
  • Like this paper and download? You can publish your own PDF file online for free in a few minutes! Sign Up
File loading please wait...
Citation preview

HANDBOOK OF POLICE PSYCHOLOGY

The Handbook of Police Psychology features contributions from over 30 leading experts on the core matters of police psychology. The collection surveys everything from the beginnings of police psychology and early influences on the profession; to pre-employment screening, assessment, and evaluation; to clinical interventions. Alongside original chapters first published in 2011, this edition features new content on deadly force encounters, officer resilience training, and police leadership enhancement. Influential figures in the field of police psychology are discussed, including America’s first full-time police psychologist, who served in the Los Angeles Police Department, and the first full-time police officer to earn a doctorate in psychology while still in uniform, who served with the New York Police Department. The Handbook of Police Psychology is an invaluable resource for police legal advisors, policy writers, and police psychologists, as well as for graduates studying police or forensic psychology. Jack Kitaeff, PhD, JD, is a licensed clinical psychologist in the Commonwealth of Virginia specializing in police and forensic psychology.

PRAISE FOR THE FIRST EDITION

“The editor has found THE TOP experts to write the most appropriate chapters. Every practicing police psychologist would buy a book like this. Kitaeff did a fine job of looking at comparable books but none will be able to compete with this book.” —David R. Englert, Adjunct Professor, Department of Human Services, Nova Southeastern University, USA “I would enthusiastically recommend this book to anyone searching for a state of the art reference volume on police psychology.” —Laurence Miller, PhD, Boca Raton, Florida, USA “Jack Kitaeff has compiled an exceptional book on a critical topic in policing. This handbook is the perfect desk reference for police legal advisors, policy writers, and police psychologists who need to create defensible policies or who work with active police officers.” —Jon M. Shane, John Jay College of Criminal Justice, Department of Law, Police Science and Criminal Justice Administration, New York, USA “Jack Kitaeff has brought together a cast of stellar contributors to produce an extremely comprehensive and well organized book. It will surely be of value to students who want to become police psychologists and to individuals who are currently working in the field. An excellent resource, full of valuable information.” —Craig Bennell, Carleton University, Ottawa, Canada “This is a fascinating book and should be read by all psychologists involved in working with the police. It illustrates beautifully the important interface between police practice and applied psychology.” —Gisli Gudjonsson, Psychology Department, King’s College London, UK

HANDBOOK OF POLICE PSYCHOLOGY Revised Edition

Edited by Jack Kitaeff

Revised edition published 2019 by Routledge 52 Vanderbilt Avenue, New York, NY 10017 and by Routledge 2 Park Square, Milton Park, Abingdon, Oxon, OX14 4RN Routledge is an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an informa business © 2019 Taylor & Francis The right of Jack Kitaeff to be identified as the author of the editorial material, and of the authors for their individual chapters, has been asserted in accordance with sections 77 and 78 of the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988. All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reprinted or reproduced or utilised in any form or by any electronic, mechanical, or other means, now known or hereafter invented, including photocopying and recording, or in any information storage or retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publishers. Trademark notice: Product or corporate names may be trademarks or registered trademarks, and are used only for identification and explanation without intent to infringe. First edition published by Routledge, 2011 Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Names: Kitaeff, Jack, editor. Title: Handbook of police psychology / edited by Jack Kitaeff. Description: Revised Edition. | New York, NY : Routledge, 2019. Identifiers: LCCN 2018060197| ISBN 9780367209070 (hardback) | ISBN 9781138917057 (pbk.) | ISBN 9780429264108 (ebook) Subjects: LCSH: Police psychology. Classification: LCC HV7936.P75 H36 2019 | DDC 363.201/9—dc23 LC record available at https://lccn.loc.gov/2018060197 ISBN: 978-0-367-20907-0 (hbk) ISBN: 978-1-138-91705-7 (pbk) ISBN: 978-0-429-26410-8 (ebk) Typeset in Bembo by Apex CoVantage, LLC

To my family, friends, and esteemed colleagues

CONTENTS

Preface to the Revised Edition About the Editor

xi xiii

  1 Introduction and History of Police Psychology Jack Kitaeff

1

PART 1

General Practice

65

  2 Police Psychological Consultation Services to Public Safety Joseph A. Davis

67

  3 Legal Issues Related to Hiring and Promotion of Police Officers Arthur Gutman

72

  4 Ethical Issues in Police Psychology Jeni L. McCutcheon

94

  5 Police vs. Probation/Surveillance Officers: Similarities and Differences D. Scott Herrmann and Barbara Broderick

115

PART 2

Pre-employment Psychological Screening

131

  6 Criterion-Related Psychological Evaluations Peter A. Weiss and William U. Weiss

133

vii

Contents

  7 Actuarial vs. Clinical Judgment Prediction Models in Pre-employment Psychological Screening of Police Candidates Michael J. Cuttler

142

  8 Appraising and Managing Police Officer Performance Rick Jacobs, Christian Thoroughgood, and Katina Sawyer

171

  9 Assessments for Selection and Promotion of Police Officers Rick Jacobs, Lily Cushenbery, and Patricia Grabarek

198

10 The Integration Section of Forensic Psychological Reports in Law Enforcement: Culturally Responsive Ending Words Ronn Johnson 11 Challenging the Police De-selection Process Jose M. Arcaya

217 233

PART 3

Training and Evaluation

245

12 Couples Counseling/Assessment and Use of the Inwald Relationship Surveys Robin Inwald, Elizabeth Willman, and Stephanie Inwald

247

13 Fitness-for-Duty Evaluations David M. Corey

274

14 Methods for Real-Time Assessment of Operational Stress During Realistic Police Tactical Training Donatella Brisinda, Riccardo Fenici, and Anna Rita Sorbo 15 Evolution of Police Leadership Enhancement Adam Park and James S. Herndon

307 333

16 When Cops Kill: Understanding the Psychology of Deadly Force Encounters350 Laurence Miller PART 4

Police Procedure

375

17 Police Use of Force Frank J. Gallo

377

viii

Contents

18 Hostage Negotiations Wayman C. Mullins and Michael J. McMains 19 Domestic Violence: An Analysis of the Crime and Punishment of Intimate Partner Abuse Stephanie L. Brooke and Trisha K. Straus

402

419

20 Police Interviews With Suspects: International Perspectives Karl A. Roberts and Victoria Herrington

438

21 Applying Restorative Justice Principles in Law Enforcement Roslyn Myers

456

PART 5

Clinical Practice

475

22 Police Personality: Theoretical Issues and Research Gwendolyn L. Gerber

477

23 Police and Public Safety Complex Trauma and Grief: An Eco-ethological Existential Analysis Daniel Rudofossi 24 Suicide in Law Enforcement Alice Liang, Alan A. Abrams, Kyleeann Stevens, and Brenda Frechette

494 528

PART 6

Treatment and Dysfunction

535

25 Cops in Trouble: Psychological Strategies for Helping Officers Under Investigation, Criminal Prosecution, or Civil Litigation Laurence Miller

537

26 Evidence-Based Psychological Interventions to Promote Officer Resiliency Stephanie B. Stern and Michele Galietta

550

27 Critical Incident Reactions and Early Interventions Suzanne Best, Ellen Kirschman, and Alexis Artwohl 28 Critical Knowledge for Clinicians Debriefing Critical Incidents in Law Enforcement Jocelyn E. Roland

ix

564

581

Contents

29 The Disconnected Values Model: A Brief Intervention of Improving Healthy Habits and Coping With Stress in Law Enforcement Mark H. Anshel

598

30 Mentally Ill—Crying “Help!” Police and Public Health Prevention, Intervention and Post-vention Daniel Rudofossi

615

Index638

x

PREFACE TO THE REVISED EDITION

In 1995, Martin I. Kurke and Ellen M. Scrivner edited their groundbreaking work, Police Psychology Into the 21st Century. The book revealed what had been taking place at the dawn of the new century in police psychology. The editors described police psychology as “an evolving area in which psychological science is applied in the law enforcement managerial and operational environments.” Police psychology (as envisioned in 1995) was regarded as a “relatively new phenomenon” in the history of psychology. Their book was presented as a “core technology consisting of psychological evaluations, counseling, and training.” The editors of the 1995 version stated that police psychology “must continue to grow in pace, scope, and direction, and that such growth [was] dependent on the development and inclusion of new methods and technologies in support of police officers and their families, police management, and police operations.” The editors saw their book as being a “snapshot of police psychology,” as it turned into the 21st century. They added prophetically, “We can hardly wait to watch and participate in its future.” Jack Kitaeff ’s 2011 edition of the Handbook of Police Psychology posed that the “future is here.” As was the case in the 2011 edition of the Handbook, the present edition is divided into six parts: General Practice, Pre-employment Psychological Screening, Training and Evaluation, Police Procedure, Clinical Practice, and Treatment and Dysfunction. Each category presents chapters that are relevant to the practice and procedures of police psychology as they stand almost 20 years into the 21st century. The first section of the book is preceded by the chapter on the introduction and history of police psychology (Kitaeff ), which presents an overall picture of where we are as a profession, the definitions of the profession, and how we got here. The section General Practice covers police psychological consultation to public safety as a whole (Davis), legal issues involved in the hiring and promotion of police officers (Gutman), ethical issues in police psychology (McCutcheon), and police versus probation and surveillance differences (Hermann and Broderick). The section on Pre-employment Psychological Screening contains chapters on criterion-related psychological evaluations (Weiss & Weiss), clinical judgment prediction models in pre-employment psychological screening of police candidates (Cuttler), appraising and managing police officer performance ( Jacobs, Thoroughgood, & Sawyer), assessments for selection and promotion of police officers ( Jacobs, Pesin, & Grabarek), the integration section of forensic psychological reports in law enforcement: Culturally responsive ending words ( Johnson), and challenging the police de-selection process (Arcaya). xi

Preface to the Revised Edition

The section on Training and Evaluation consists of chapters on couples counseling/assessment and use of the Inwald Relationship Surveys (Inwald, Willman, & Inwald), fitness–for-duty evaluations (Corey), methods for real-time assessment of operational stress during realistic police tactical training (Brisinda, Fenici, & Sorbo), a new chapter on evolution of police leadership enhancement (Park & Herndon), and a new chapter on when cops kill: Understanding the psychology of deadly force encounters (Miller). The section on Police Procedure contains a revised chapter on police use of force (Gallo), a chapter on hostage negotiations (Mullins & McMains), an analysis of the crime and punishment of intimate partner abuse (Brooke & Strauss), police interviews with suspects: International perspectives (Roberts & Herrington), and applying restorative justice principles in law enforcement (Myers). The section on Clinical Practice includes a chapter on police personality: Theoretical issues and research (Gerber), a revised chapter on police and public safety complex trauma and grief: An eco-ethological existential analysis (Rudofossi), and suicide in law enforcement (Liang, Abrams, Stevens, & Frechette). The section on Treatment and Dysfunction contains a chapter on cops in trouble: Psychological strategies for helping officers under investigation, criminal prosecution, or civil litigation (Miller), a new chapter on evidence-based psychological interventions to promote officer resiliency (Stern & Galietta), a chapter on critical incident reactions and early interventions (Best, Kirschman, & Artwohl), a revised chapter on critical knowledge for clinicians debriefing critical incidents in law enforcement (Roland), a chapter on the disconnected values model: A brief intervention of improving healthy habits and coping with stress in law enforcement (Anshel), and a new chapter on Mentally Ill—­ Crying “Help!” Police and Public Health Prevention, Intervention and Post-vention (Rudofossi). Some of the chapters in this latest edition would be barely recognizable when Police Psychology Into the 21st Century was published in 1995. The profession of police psychology continues to grow geometrically, yet there is a familiar thread that continues to run through every decade and every topic. It is the thread of professionalism that continues to highlight our young but growing profession. Hugo Münsterberg advocated for psychology’s involvement in the detection of crime and the presence of psychologists in the criminal process, but he could hardly imagine the extent to which this now takes place on a regular basis. When in 1917, Lewis Terman advocated for intelligence testing in the hiring of police officers, he could barely predict the breath of present-day pre-employment police applicant screening. In 1954, when Martin Reiser became the nation’s first full-time police department psychologist, he could not have foreseen the extent of the influence that present-day police psychologists have in the selection, hiring, and management of present-day police officers at all levels and how police psychology itself would come to be recognized as a true psychological specialty both by the American Psychological Association and the American Board of Professional Psychology. Kurke and Scrivner saw Police Psychology Into the 21st Century as a snapshot of police psychology as it existed in 1995. Kitaeff ’s first edition of the Handbook of Police Psychology presented a panoramic image of how police psychology appeared in 2011. The present edition of the Handbook shows the most up-to-date incarnations in the field of police psychology and a glimpse of how the field is changing as we progress through the first quarter of the 21st century. Jack Kitaeff

xii

ABOUT THE EDITOR

Jack Kitaeff, PhD, JD, is a licensed clinical psychologist in the Commonwealth of Virginia. He received his undergraduate education at Brooklyn College of the City University of New York and his graduate psychology education at the State University of New York and the University of Mississippi. He received his law degree from the Antonin Scalia School of Law at George Mason University and completed a legal clerkship with the United States Attorney’s Office, Eastern District of Virginia. Dr. Kitaeff completed a clinical psychology internship at Walter Reed Army Medical Center and served as a psychologist and major in the U.S. Army Medical Service Corps. He later became the first police psychologist for the Arlington County Police Department, where he established a preemployment psychological screening program for all police applicants, and for officers applying for special units such as SWAT, Hostage Negotiations, and VICE. He has been the consulting police psychologist for numerous law enforcement agencies. He has also served as the Director of Psychology for the Commonwealth of Virginia’s Northern Virginia Mental Health Institute. He is an adjunct professor in the Department of Criminology, Law and Society at George Mason University, and an adjunct professor with the George Washington University Department of Psychology. He is also a contributing faculty member in the School of Psychology at Walden University. He is a Diplomate in Police Psychology from the Society of Police and Criminal Psychology, a Fellow in the American College of Legal Medicine, and a member of the American Psychological Association. He maintains a private practice in clinical psychology in Fairfax, Virginia.

xiii

1 INTRODUCTION AND HISTORY OF POLICE PSYCHOLOGY Jack Kitaeff

The professions of policing, psychology, and police psychology have grown geometrically in the United States during the last 100 years. When American colonist Asser Levy and his brothers-inarms served on the Burgher Guard in New Amsterdam (New York City) in 1657, they in essence became this country’s first policemen. Levy and his brethren could never imagine the current levels of technology and professionalism of modern police work and of the modern application of psychological principles to police work and law enforcement. Today, psychology applied to policing, or police psychology, can broadly be defined as the application of psychological principles and methods to law enforcement. This broad and growing area includes topics such as screening and hiring police officers, conducting screenings for specials quads (e.g., SWAT), fitness-for-duty evaluations, investigations, hostage negotiations, training and consultation, and stress counseling, among others (Kitaeff, 2010).

Police Psychology: The Beginnings Hugo Münsterberg is often called the first forensic psychologist. Yet in many ways he was also the first police psychologist because he wrote, at the beginning of the 20th century, how the law, the criminal investigator, the mechanisms of the courtroom, and the detection of criminals all came together under the watchful eye of the experimental psychologist. In his book On the Witness Stand (1908), Münsterberg talked about many topics such as illusions, the memory of witnesses, the effects of emotions, untrue confessions, the suggestibility of witnesses, hypnosis, and crime prevention. But it was perhaps his work on the detection of crime (and hence the use of primitive “criminal profiling” and “hypnosis”) that stands out as the forerunners of the modern tools of the same names that have been used by police psychologists during the last 100 years. Münsterberg said: The psychologist who seeks to discover the secret connections of ideas may thus, by his association method, not only protect the innocent and unmask the guilty, but bring health and strength to the nervous wreck. Yet our chief interest belongs to the legal aspect of this method [free association and hypnosis]. Carried out with the skill which only long laboratory training can give, it has become, indeed, a magnifying-glass for the most subtle mental mechanism, and by it the secrets of the criminal mind may be unveiled. All this has, of course, no legal standing to-day, and there is probably no one who desires to increase the number of “experts” in our criminal 1

Jack Kitaeff

courts. But justice demands that truth and lies be disentangled. The time will come when the methods of experimental psychology cannot longer be excluded from the court of law. It is well known that the use of stenographers in trials once met with vehement opposition, while now the shorthand record of the court procedure seems a matter of course. The help of the psychologist will become not less indispensable. The vulgar ordeals of the “third degree” in every form belong to the Middle Ages, and much of the wrangling of attorneys about technicalities in admitting the “evidence” appears to not a few somewhat out of date, too: the methods of experimental psychology are working in the spirit of the twentieth century. The “third degree” may brutalize the mind and force either correct or falsified secrets to light; the time-measurement of association is swifter and cleaner, more scientific, more humane, and more reliable in bringing out the truth which justice demands. Of course, we are only at the beginning of its development; the new method is still in many ways imperfect, and if clumsily applied it may be misleading; moreover, there exists no hard and fast rule which fits every case mechanically. But all this indicates only that, just as the bodily facts have to be examined by the chemist or the physiologist, the mental facts must be examined also, not by the layman, but by the scientific psychologist, with the training of a psychological laboratory. (1908, pp. 109–110)

Police Psychology: The Early Years Other early psychologists who influenced the fledgling field of police psychology included William Stern, Alfred Binet, and Lewis Terman. William Stern (1912) decided that “personalistic psychology” or “individuality” was destined to be the main psychological problem of the 20th century. He attempted to classify people according to types, norms, and aberrations. To Stern, it was in the process of investigating individuality that the real essence of personality and intelligence could be discovered. Stern was influenced by the work of Alfred Binet and his studies of intelligence in children. As a result, Stern reviewed the principle findings in the field and developed the idea of expressing intelligence test results in the form of a single number, the intelligence quotient. While Terman’s (1916) study was the first to suggest that testing using the Stanford-Binet was useful for pre-employment evaluations, his results lacked information about the actual criterion-related validity of the Stanford-Binet for the specific purpose of police officer selection. However, Terman did feel that testing would one day be valuable as a selection tool for certain types of occupations and stressed the importance of establishing correlations between test scores and future performance, and in establishing norms of performance for different occupational groups. While Terman’s study was speculative and did not involve the kinds of personality tests used in law enforcement selection today, he was a forerunner for using these evaluations as a means of obtaining the needed research data to make such evaluations viable (Inwald, 1990, 1992). In the early 1920s, a few experimental investigations of psychological testing on police attitude and performance were conducted. For example, in 1922, L. L. Thurstone gave the Army Alpha Intelligence test to a group of officers serving in the Detroit Police Department. He found that, in general, patrolmen scored higher than the lieutenants who commanded them. Thurstone concluded that this may have been due to the fact that the most intelligent law enforcement officers often moved out of police work altogether to other, higher paying occupations rather than waiting for promotions that took too long to come or never came at all. Kates (1950) administered the Rorschach inkblot test (Klopfer scoring system) to a group of New York City police officers and found that some Rorschach variables could be used to predict job satisfaction and motivation for promotion. In the 1950s, psychology became increasingly utilized in many areas beyond testing and classification in the military, and this included increased involvement in police work of various sorts and to 2

Introduction and History

various degrees. During World War II and the Korean War, psychologists were employed by the military service to assist Selective Service Boards in identifying individuals who were psychologically unfit for military service. In addition, they became involved in conducting evaluations for the purposes of selecting spies, saboteurs, and intelligence operatives ( Janik, 1990). Most notable in this area was the work of Henry Murray and colleagues, who used personality assessment instruments to perform personnel selection evaluations for the Office of Strategic Services (OSS), the precursor to the CIA. Success in these areas quickly expanded to postwar civilian life. In 1954, Martin Reiser became the nation’s first full-time police psychologist, when he began screening all applicants to the Los Angeles Police Department using the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI), a group Rorschach, a tree drawing, and a brief psychiatric interview (Blau & Super, 1997). A date that further stands out in importance for police psychology is 1972, when Martin Reiser presented a paper titled The Police Department Psychologist at the Western Psychological Association Convention. Reese (1987a, 1987b), a historian on psychological services to police and law enforcement, has estimated that only six police agencies in the United States had full-time psychologists prior to 1977. The same had not been true in Europe, however, where police departments in Germany were using psychologists in a variety of capacities as early as 1919. In 1966, the Munich police were employing a full-time police psychologist to train officers to deal with various patrol situations, such as crowd control. In 1958, Harvey Schlossberg (shield number 16844) became a patrolman with the New York City Police Department. As he was in college at the time and contemplating attending graduate school, he felt that this might be a job in which he could earn money while he attended to his studies. He didn’t know a lot about what the police job entailed but heard they were recruiting, so he figured he would give it a try. He learned that he would have to attend an academy where he would learn how to shoot a pistol. Most of all, he knew that he would get paid a salary during this time. He could never have imagined back then that he would remain with the police department for 20 years (Kitaeff, 2006a). In his book Psychologist With a Gun, Schlossberg (1974) recounts the first time he wore his police uniform home. His mother didn’t even recognize him. He didn’t even recognize himself when he looked in the mirror. He knew one thing: With that uniform, his badge, and his gun, he felt different. “The first day I wore the uniform of a cop I felt taller than usual” (p. 29). After earning his bachelor’s degree from Brooklyn College, Schlossberg went on for his master’s degree at C.W. Post College and his doctorate at Yeshiva University. In 1971, Harvey Schlossberg became the first known police officer in the United States to earn a doctorate in psychology while still a full-time police officer in uniform. In 1972, when Police Commissioner Patrick Murphy heard that one of his officers (a detective by then) had earned a doctorate in psychology, he asked Harvey Schlossberg to design a police psychological service unit. In many ways, Schlossberg can be considered the father of modern police psychology in the United States. In Psychologist With a Gun, Schlossberg (1974) outlined his thoughts for establishing a psychological services unit within the police department (the second in the country after Martin Reiser established his with the Los Angeles Police Department). But Reiser was a psychologist, not a police officer. Schlossberg was both—a very important distinction. He listed the services this new psychological services unit would provide. They included:   1. The conducting of psychological evaluations, consisting of psychological testing.   2. Psychiatric interviews, diagnosis, and prognosis, all in an attempt to discover if certain policemen sent to [him] by their superiors or coming to [his] office voluntarily were too emotionally upset to be allowed to carry a gun. If they were found to be, then either short-term or long-term therapy would be recommended and an attempt made to find psychiatrists to treat them. 3

Jack Kitaeff

  3. Marriage counseling, where marital problems impaired a policeman’s work.   4. Guidance and counseling to police who had minor problems.   5. Referral of families, wives, or children for treatment if they needed it.   6. Psychological testing and evaluation of candidates for promotion.   7. Psychological testing and evaluation of all recruits.  8. Research and operational consultations to other units within the department for personnel selection and research into function.   9. Instructional services to promotion classes on recognition of psychiatric problems commonly encountered by supervisors. 10. Continued administration of a pilot program for certain officers who needed therapy, which would include referral, payment, and supervision of the therapeutic process. 11. Maintaining of liaison with the honorary psychiatrists who served the Medical Division as consultants. 12. Acting as consultant to the district surgeons on psychological matters concerning members of the force. 13. Conducting of special lectures on psychological problems related to special police functions. 14. Representing the department on psychological matters in lectures before lay and professional organizations. 15. Consultant on behavioral patterns in crime situations. 16. Teaching of specialized courses for department units. 17. Acting as liaison with civilian psychiatrists and psychologists and as advisor to them on special problems relating to the demands of police work. (p. 93) In the psychological screening of police recruits, Schlossberg instituted an assessment battery that included a structured clinical interview along with the administration of certain psychological tests. These tests included the Thorndike Dimensions of Temperament Scale, the Cornell Medical Index (CMI), the Edwards Personal Preference Schedule (EPPS), the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI), and the House-Tree-Person Test (H-T-P) (Kitaeff, 2006b). The Thorndike Dimensions of Temperament Scale was originally published in 1966 and was a selfreport inventory through which the individual describes himself with respect to 10 dimensions of temperament: Sociable, Ascendant, Cheerful, Placid, Accepting, Tough-Minded, Reflective, Impulsive, Active, and Responsible. The Cornell Medical Index (CMI) was created in 1949 and was a self-administered health questionnaire developed to obtain details of the person’s medical history as an adjunct to the medical interview. It consisted of 195 questions divided into 18 sections; the first 12 sections dealt with somatic complaints, and the last six with mood and feeling patterns. The Edwards Personal Preference Schedule (EPPS) was developed in 1959 based on the “manifest need system” theory of H. A. Murray and his associates at the Harvard Psychological Clinic. Beginning with 15 needs drawn from Murray’s list of manifest needs, Edwards prepared sets of items whose content appeared to fit each need. Examples included the need for achievement (to do one’s best and accomplish something difficult), deference (to conform to what is expected), exhibition (to be the center of attention), intraception (to analyze the motives and feelings of oneself and others), dominance (to influence others and to be regarded as a leader), and nurturance (to help others in trouble). The original Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI) was developed in the late 1930s by a psychologist and a psychiatrist at the University of Minnesota. The test had 10 clinical scales and three validity scales plus various supplementary scales. The clinical scales were intended to distinguish groups of people with psychiatric disorders that have somewhat exotic-sounding names such as hysteria, psychopathic deviate, and psychesthenia. The names of these primary clinical scales have remained the same for the last 75 years even though a restandardization process took place in 1989. 4

Introduction and History

The House-Tree-Person Test (H-T-P) is a projective technique developed by John Buck and was originally an outgrowth of the “Goodenough” scale that was used to assess intellectual functioning. In administering the H-T-P, the subject is asked to draw alternately a house, a tree, and a person. Buck felt artistic creativity represented a stream of personality characteristics that flowed onto graphic art. He believed that through drawings, subjects objectified unconscious difficulties by sketching the inner image of primary process. Because it was assumed that the content and quality of the H-T-P were not attributable to the stimulus itself, he believed they had to be rooted in the individual’s basic personality. This screening process was designed to ensure that only the psychologically fit and “normal” person became a police officer. But there was (and is) much to suggest that even the most emotionally stable individual “changes” when he becomes a police officer. Accordingly, Schlossberg knew that it was not enough to psychologically screen police recruits; it would be necessary to provide psychological counseling for them as well. Accordingly, the psychological services unit of the police department offered a full array of individual and group psychotherapy. Schlossberg knew that for almost all police officers, the credibility of anyone offering therapy for officers would be a major issue. Police officers are traditionally very distrustful and wary of outsiders who they perceive as not understanding what the world of a police officer is like. For Schlossberg, this was not an issue because, in addition to being a psychologist, he was also a cop. “I ran after bad guys on the same rooftops as they did,” he would say (Kitaeff, 2006a). Peer counseling, or having cops counsel other cops, was a technique brought to the forefront by Schlossberg. He trained police officers to help other officers in trouble and to be “group leaders” of police group counseling sessions. Interestingly, he found, for example, that these lay therapist-cops who were leading these groups were often more accurate than Schlossberg in spotting the violent-prone officers in the groups. Schlossberg felt strongly that it was best to catch problems before they developed into nightmares. He instituted what he called an “early warning system” in which certain markers or signals in a police officer’s performance would set off a “psychological alarm” that therapeutic intervention was needed. Schlossberg also recommended to the police brass that officers have regular therapy sessions as a means of warding off problems before they grew into bigger ones. A major advantage of such an approach would be to remove the stigma of seeing the “shrink,” which so often occurs in police work. By making such visits mandatory, the officer’s macho self-image could be maintained. Unfortunately, police department higher-ups shot down this idea as taking up too many man-hours. The 1960s witnessed significant legal and cultural changes that led to the expansion of psychologists’ roles and applications in the law enforcement field. In 1963, James Shaw of Washington State was appointed by the King County Sheriff ’s Department as the first “in-house” police psychologist working half-time, conducting research, and providing consultations and pre-employment evaluations for sheriff ’s deputies. In 1967, the release of the Presidential Commission on Law Enforcement and the Administration of Justice (1967) report on law enforcement slowly set in motion a trend that would eventually result in the psychological screening of all law enforcement candidates ( Janik, 1993; Ostrov, 1990). The report emphasized the importance of assessing emotional stability in officer candidates. Specifically, The National Advisory Commission on Annual Justice Standards and Goals (1967) recommended that by 1975 every law enforcement agency employ a trained professional who could administer psychological tests for the purposes of evaluating applicants for characteristics that would be detrimental to police work. As a result of these and other developments, the concept of pre-employment selection evaluations for police candidates became more acceptable to police administrators than in the past. Still, during the late 1960s and 1970s, most chiefs continued to believe that the pre-employment interviews they conducted completely by themselves, along with the medical and physical examination requirements, were sufficient for selecting the best candidates for the job. Some psychologists in the late 5

Jack Kitaeff

1960s conducted research as external consultants to law enforcement agencies, and agencies began to have more direct contact with psychologists and the services they could provide through grant projects. For example, in 1969, Joseph Fabricatore of California began working as a graduate student research associate for the Los Angeles Sheriff ’s Department on a Law Enforcement Assistance Administration (LEAA) study focused on predicting patrol officer behavior from psychological variables (Inwald, 1998, 2006, 2008). It has also been expressed that the beginning of the profession of police psychology officially began at the National Symposium on Police Psychological Services (NSPPS), held at the FBI Academy in 1984 (Bartol, 1991; McKenzie, 1986). What is clear, however, is that since the 1960s, the application of psychological services and research to law enforcement settings has gone from just about nil to almost omnipresent. What is further known for sure is that by 1985, 11 states had passed statutes requiring police departments to psychologically screen their applicants, and by 1990, 64% of state police departments and 73% of municipal police departments required psychological screening. Today, most large and many medium-sized police departments have a full-time police psychologist on staff or utilize part-time police psychologist consultants. In September 1984, James T. Reese (from the FBI) and Harvey Goldstein organized and conducted a five-day National Symposium on Police Psychological Services at the FBI Academy in Quantico, Virginia. At this conference, approximately 150 police psychologists and other professionals working in the field from around the United States began to develop a strong national network. During and after the presentations, many active discussions took place about various testing programs and selection techniques. In 1984, Jack Kitaeff became the first police psychologist for the Arlington County Police Department. In this role, he developed a set of local norms using standardized psychological tests, including the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS), MMPI, and the Sixteen Personality Factor (16PF) test, which he used as a guide in his applicant selection process. He also spent hundreds of hours riding in police cars in an attempt to gain a true awareness of the nature of the job of being a police officer, a knowledge that helped him tremendously in both the assessment and the counseling of officers. In addition to the Arlington County Police Department, Kitaeff applied his police psychological services skills to various departments in the Northern Virginia area, including the Arlington County Sheriff ’s Office, the City of Alexandria Sheriff ’s Office, the Alexandria Police Department, the Stafford County Sheriff ’s Office, the Falls Church Police Department, the Warrenton Police Department, and the Metropolitan Washington Airports Authority Police Department. In 1987, Daniel Rudofossi joined the New York City Police Department at the rank of patrolman. He served as a street officer for nine years, but in 1996 received his doctorate in clinical psychology and subsequently became a uniformed member (sergeant) of the Membership Assistance Program (MAP), which was the first joint Police Department–Police Union employee assistance program created by the Office of the Mayor. Rudofossi was also appointed as the official Medical Division Uniform Psychologist by the agreement of the police union presidents (PBA, SBA, LBA, and CBA) and by order of the Chief of Police Personnel. In this new role, Rudofossi made the clinical decisions regarding all sick, restricted duty, modified duty, restoration of firearms, removal of firearms, and ambulatory assessments and interventions for all affected officers in the city. His work required services to be provided throughout the city on a “24/7” basis for four years. Rudofossi—now a published author, poet, lecturer, and professor—forged a new direction in police psychology, having written his first book on his work with traumatized police officers in the New York City Police Department. “Dr. Dan” as he was known, has had a unique method for the treatment of trauma and grief. Part of this involves making sense of the chaotic experimental traumatic neurosis alive in scientific terms, in the “eco-ethological niches” of police work. Rudofossi’s multi-axial diagnosis and multiple therapeutic approaches are anchored in real clinical illustrations leading to conceptually fleshed-out public safety personality styles. He did research involving 2% of NYPD’s 40,000 officers and applied those findings into his knowledge gained from 6

Introduction and History

his therapy experiences with police and other emergency responders. Working With Traumatized Police Officer-Patients: A Clinician’s Guide to Complex PTSD Syndromes in Public Safety Professionals is a unique and holistic approach about the understanding and treatment of police and public safety personnel. The genesis of this work may have been in the urban intensity of the NYPD, but it is relevant to emergency responders and public safety personnel anywhere. Rudofossi’s supervisors included Dr. Albert Ellis, founder of Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy (REBT); the famed psychoanalyst Dr. Charles Brenner; and Dr. Bob Barnes, who was a supervisee of the famed Dr. Viktor Frankl, founder of Logotherapy. Rudofossi eventually became the Chief Psychologist for the Detective Crime Clinic of New York and New Jersey, a consultant to the Saybrook University Police Clinical Psychology doctoral program, and a professor at New York University, where he received two master’s degrees and another doctoral degree while still an active duty police officer. Rudofossi is currently the Administrative Psychologist and Training and Curriculum Director for the Drug Enforcement Administration’s (DEA) Employee Assistance Program. He sees his learned predecessors as Dr. (Detective) Harvey Schlossberg of the NYPD, San Francisco Police Department Captain Dr. Al Benner, and Dr. James Reese of the FBI’s Behavioral Science Unit, who assisted Rudofossi in developing an initial proposal and content validity analysis when designing his assessment instrument, which would allow him to gain a foothold on police trauma and the public safety officers’ experiences. In 1989, Michael Roberts and Michael Johnson of Johnson Roberts & Associates published their automated Johnson, Roberts Personal History Questionnaire (PHQ), a 300-item questionnaire for evaluating public safety officer applicants. In 1993, Ones and associates published a monograph in the Journal of Applied Psychology that included Inwald Personality Inventory (IPI) validity data previously mentioned in the APA Task Force report. The generalizability and test validities of “integrity” tests were established, which essentially stopped any movement to ban these instruments (Ones et al., 1993). And in 1995, Robin Inwald’s Hilson Safety/Security Risk Inventory (HSRI) was published for use in preconditional offers (see ADA material later in this chapter) for police screening (Inwald, 1995). The HSRI, with its scales focusing on attitudes and behaviors related to safety issues, as well as self-control in other areas, was developed for use in the police/public safety field after police administrators continued to complain about the recurring problem of vehicular accidents in their departments. Inwald’s instruments saw an increased role of police psychologists in the 1990s as evaluators sought to use personality instruments that tapped job-related behavioral characteristics in pre-employment, fitness-for-duty evaluations (FFDE), and the growing special duty assignment evaluations for hostage negotiators, SWAT team members, and so forth. In 1996, Hilson Research published Inwald’s Hilson Management Survey (HMS) for promotional screening. This instrument was used for public safety officer promotions and public safety administrator screening, including some assessments conducted by police psychologists for police chief and assistant police chief positions (Inwald, Resko, & Favuzza, 1996). New instruments for public safety assessment purposes were also developed during the first decade of the 21st century. In 2000, Law Enforcement Services, Inc. (LESI), published Michael Cuttler’s “online PHQ” for evaluating public safety officer applicants. This new instrument grew from research conducted on the validity of individual life history items that was also published in the Journal of Applied Psychology in 1998 (Sarchione, Cuttler, Muchinsky, & Nelson-Gray, 1998). In 2002, Hilson Research published Robin Inwald’s Hilson Trauma Recovery Inventory (HTRI) as a result of discussions at the 1998 FBI Conference on Domestic Violence by Police Officers and the recent influx of officers returning from overseas military duty in the Middle East (Inwald, 2002, 2006). Additional independent IPI validation research was completed during these years (Chibnall & Detrick, 2003; Detrick & Chibnall, 2002), including comparisons of IPI scores with those of newly developed research scales for the MMPI-2 (Ben-Porath, 2006). 7

Jack Kitaeff

In 2005, IntegriQuest, LLC, published Andrew Ryan’s RPIQ, an automated biodata collection instrument for public safety officer candidates. In 2007, Inwald Research, Inc., published the Inwald Couples Compatibility Questionnaire (ICCQ), Inwald Partners Personality Inventory (IPPI), Inwald Personality Survey (IPS), and Inwald Attitude Survey (IPS) for use with police couples (Inwald, 2007, 2008). In 2008, Multi Health Systems, Inc. (MHS), published Robert Davis and Cary Rostow’s “M-PULSE,” a personality test developed for public safety officer screening (Davis & Rostow, 2008). In 2008, Peter Weiss, William Weiss, and Carl Gacono reviewed the use of the Rorschach inkblot method (Exner, 2003) for police psychological assessments (Weiss, 2002; Weiss, Weiss, & Gacono, 2008; Zacker, 1997). This review was completed for several reasons. One was the wealth of personality-related information that can be gleaned from the Rorschach Comprehensive System, and another was the impression management limitations of self-report measures such as the MMPI and other instruments. In 2008, Pearson Assessments, Inc., published the MMPI-2 Restructured Form (MMPI-2RF), a revised form of the MMPI-2 that included a set of law enforcement norms in its technical manual (Tellegen & Ben-Porath, 2008).

The Nature of Policing: What Makes a Good Police Officer? Who are the police, and how are they expected to behave? For one thing, the police are expected to be conscientious, agreeable, emotionally stable, self-controlled, honest, and morally upright (Berry, 2007; Kitaeff, 2006b). Controlled experiments have been designed and conducted to identify the personality traits, or degrees of traits, that are correlated with or significantly predict discrete aspects of future successful police performance. As psychologists trained in research, empiricism, and the scientific method, we appreciate the importance of these studies and try to incorporate their findings into our daily work in the selection of police recruits, the evaluation of present officers, and our consultation with police administration. As any practicing clinician will also disclose, however, years of experience and observation “on the firing line” have allowed us to also develop certain working hypotheses that are perfectly appropriate to be used as one part of our clinical armamentarium. The present author is no different. Some of the personality attributes the author has found important in police work—assessed primarily from the 16PF—include emotional warmth, dominance, conscientiousness, social boldness, self-reliance, organization, and high energy level (Kitaeff, 2010). Of course, complete reliance on these factors without the use of empirically derived personality traits and standardized psychological inventories and tests would be inappropriate. What makes a good police officer? Hogan (1971) reported that supervisors described their best officers as functionally intelligent, sociable, and self-assured. A decade later, using the Inwald Personality Inventory and the MMPI, Inwald and Shusman (1984) found that patrol officers who exhibited heightened awareness and discernment tended to receive better supervisory ratings. Having conventional attitudes and being free of “neurosis” (excessive anxiety, worry, self-doubt, and phobias) have also been shown to predict fewer serious job problems among police officers (Hiatt & Hargrave, 1988b). The pre-DSM-IVTR term neurosis refers to generalized anxiety, phobias, self-doubts, and feelings of helplessness and hopelessness. Yet some degree of guardedness and circumspection appears to be a desirable personality characteristic for effective policing (Detrick & Chibnall, 2002). The police in the United States are the major representatives of the government and the legal system in their transactions with citizens. They are responsible for enforcing the criminal laws, keeping the peace, and responding to calls for service. They are also required to exercise discretion in determining whether or not a violation of the law has taken place and whether someone should be arrested and charged with a particular criminal offense. They are put in the position of mediating disputes between people from all walks of life and making immediate decisions on who might be in the 8

Introduction and History

wrong or the right. Indeed, for better or worse, police officers are possessors of inordinate amounts of power. Furthermore, the average officer is continuously exposed to temptation and opportunities for corruption with only limited likelihood of getting caught (Kitaeff, 2010).

Pre-employment Psychological Selection: Cognitive Assessment The Wonderlic Personnel Test The Wonderlic Personnel Test (WPT) is one of the most frequently used cognitive testing devices in private industry as well as law enforcement. The publishers describe the test as a short-form test of general cognitive ability (Aamodt, 2004). Such cognitive ability is often referred to as general intelligence or “g” and is a measure of the level at which an individual learns, understands instructions, and solves problems. It provides a measurable and quantitative look into how easily individuals can be trained, how well they can adjust and adapt to changing situations and demands, how easily they can solve problems on the job, and how satisfied they are likely to be with the demands of their chosen career. The publishers claim that higher scoring individuals will not only gain more from individualized training but also be more likely to learn effectively from on-the-job experience. Modest scoring individuals will need more detailed supervision, greater hands-on practice, more time and repetition, and closer supervision (Wonderlic, 1999). The WPT consists of 50 questions, including word comparisons, disarranged sentences, sentence parallelism, the need to follow directions, number comparisons, number series, analysis of geometric figures, and story problems requiring either mathematical or logical solutions. Questions are arranged in order of difficulty and allow the test taker 12 minutes to complete the test before being stopped by the proctor. Computerized, large-print, and 11 alternate language versions of the test are available. The WPT correlates highly with the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) (r = .92), which suggests that the Wonderlic taps various intellectual functions and may be a test of global intelligence (Dodrill, 1980, 1981, 1983; Dodrill & Warner, 1988). Research also suggests that scores on the Wonderlic have a test–retest reliability, ranging from .82 to .94 (Dodrill, 1983). Alternate form reliabilities range from .73 to .95, and correlation of odd items on the test to even items on the test ranges from .88 to .94 (McKelvie, 1989). The average score of police applicants taking the Wonderlic as part of pre-employment job screening is 20.91 with a standard deviation of 6.14. Super (1995) has found correlations of .19 between Wonderlic scores and patrol performance. Hankey (1968) has found correlations of .06 with patrol performance and .28 with academy performance.

The Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale The Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) is one of the most common and “global” tests of intelligence in use today. It has been an integral part of clinical and educational psychology practice and research since 1939, starting with the introduction of the original Wechsler-Bellevue Intelligence Scale (Wechsler, 1939). Since the original version, the Wechsler Scale has been restandardized three times: once in 1955 as the WAIS, again in 1981 as the WAIS-R, and most recently in 2000 as the WAIS-3. The tests cannot be proctored for group administration (as the Wonderlic can) but must be individually administered by a qualified and appropriately trained psychologist, and it consists of various verbal and performance measures. The tests are useful in assessing general intelligence as well as more specific components of intelligence such as judgment and common sense, general knowledge and information vocabulary, speed of eye–hand coordination, concentration and attentiveness, and social awareness. Results from these tests can indicate various forms of psychopathology as well as neuropathology and associated problems. The reason for this is that brain damage, psychotic deterioration, and emotional difficulties seem to 9

Jack Kitaeff

affect some intellectual functions more than others. And a pattern analysis of WAIS scores may reveal certain disorders. Indeed, the WAIS is recognized as a valid and reliable test of intelligence but has not been researched to any great extent regarding its ability to predict law enforcement performance. From the limited research available, a correlation of .61 was found between WAIS scores with academy grades, and a correlation of .38 between WAIS scores and performance ratings of police officers after one year on the job was also found (Aamodt, 2004). One of the major drawbacks of the WAIS (in any of its various incarnations) is the time it takes to properly administer the test. The test is designed to be individually administered. The WAIS-R, for example, requires 1 1/2 hours to properly administer. This does not include the time needed for proper scoring and interpretation.

Nelson-Denny Reading Test The Nelson-Denny Reading Test is a 118-item test designed to measure reading comprehension and vocabulary. The test takes about 45 minutes to complete and has a test–retest reliability of .77, internal reliability of .96, and alternate form reliability of .90. Some research suggests that Nelson-Denny scores correlate between (r = .38) and (r = .59) with police academy grades (Rose, 1995; Surrette, Aamodt, & Serafino, 1990).

Shipley Institute of Living Scale The Shipley Institute of Living Scale consists of 60 items, and measures vocabulary and abstract thinking. The test can be administered to a group of individuals and takes about 20 minutes to complete. Most of the uses of the Shipley scale have been in clinical and neuropsychological settings and not for the pre-employment selection of police personnel. Gardner (1994) found a correlation of .16 between Shipley scores and police probationary performance. Other research has found correlations of .39 and .50 (Mullins & McMains, 1995; Scogin, Schumacher, Gardner, & Chaplin, 1995). Davis and Rostow (2003) found a significant negative correlation (r = −.09) between Shipley scores and being terminated for cause.

Personality Assessment Instruments The Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory The Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI) was first published in 1937 and consists of 566 forced-choice true or false questions related to various forms of clinical psychopathology. It is an empirical criterion-keyed test that pretests items used on the various clinical scales. The validity scales consist of scales L (Lie), F (Frequency), and K (Defensiveness). The 10 clinical scales are scales 1 (Hypochondriasis), 2 (Depression), 3 (Hysteria), 4 (Psychopathic Deviate), 5 (Masculinity–­ Femininity), 6 (Paranoia), 7 (Psychesthenia), 8 (Schizophrenia), 9 (Hypomania), and 0 (Social Affiliation). In response to concerns that the original MMPI contained dated, objectionable items (e.g., items that were religious, sexual, and intrusive), it was republished in 1989 to address these issues and increase its face validity as well (Daniels & King, 2002). The new version of the MMPI was a vast improvement over the older one as it satisfied the criticisms leveled against it but maintained its usefulness. The MMPI, and later the MMPI-2, has been and remains the most commonly used psychological test in the assessment of police officer applicants. As Blau. T. (1994) observed, it has been the workhorse of paper-and-pencil personality assessment for more than half a century. Police officers as a group tend to fall in the “normal” MMPI (or MMPI-2) range on clinical scales and generally present an emotionally healthy image (Mills & Stratton, 1982). Studies have found that 10

Introduction and History

police officers tend somewhat to score higher than non-police officers on scales K (Defensiveness), 3 (Hysteria), 4 (Psychopathic Deviate), and 9 (Hypomania) (Bernstein, Schoenfeld, & Costello, 1982; Raza & Carpenter, 1987). A study performed by Hargrave (1987) found that the L scale and the F scale tended to distinguish between satisfactory and unsatisfactory officer criterion groups. Herndon (1998) found that elevations on the L scale of the MMPI-2 appeared related to subsequent poor behavior of police officers after being hired. Boes, Chandler, and Timm (1997) found that corrupt officers were likely to obtain high scores on the L scale. Scores on the L scale also have been found to correlate generally with future problematic police performance. Overall, research has found that elevated L scores are correlated with poor police performance. Weiss, Davis, Rostow, and Kinsman (2003) suggest that the L scale is a valuable predictor of problematic police officer behavior. This is not surprising considering the descriptors of high L scores given by Graham (1993). He describes these individuals as trying to create a favorable impression of themselves by not being honest in their responses to the MMPI-2 test items. These individuals are described as defensive, denying, and repressing. They tend to claim virtues to a greater extent than most people. They manifest little or no insight into their own motivations. They show little awareness of consequences to other people of their own behavior. They overevaluate their own worth. They have poor tolerance for stress and pressure. They are unoriginal in thinking and inflexible in problem solving. It should be noted that the types of police applicants who would obtain high L scores on the MMPI-2, without an actual MMPI-2 administration, may very easily be misidentified as good candidates for police work because they appear so “normal.” The L scale measures a cluster of personality attributes and attitudes that can most certainly negatively impact police performance but are not obvious; rather, they are subtle. These applicants tend to be rigid, moralistic, conventional, and socially conforming. Police department background investigations often fail to discover any criminal behavior or behavioral aberrations with these individuals. However, the very inadequate stress tolerance of these applicants, along with their defensiveness and inflated sense of self-worth, makes them particularly susceptible to impulsive, aggressive, or abusive behavior in their roles as police officers. Scale 4 (Psychopathic Deviate) of the MMPI is generally thought to have particular relevance in the selection of police officers because the scale was developed to assist in identifying persons with psychopathic personality disorders (Weiss, Buehler, & Yates, 1996). A psychopathic personality pattern involves a repeated and flagrant disregard for social customs and mores, an inability to profit from experience, emotional shallowness, and the strongly held belief (whether conscious or not) that “the world is my oyster.” Males who score high on scale 4 are further described as hostile and aggressive in their interpersonal relationships, sarcastic and cynical, ostentatious, exhibitionistic, moody, thrill-seeking, and resentful. Women are described as aggressive, emotionally changeable, and high-strung. Shusman, Inwald, and Knatz (1987) found scale 4 to be important in separating good and poor performance groups of police officers; Weiss, Buehler, and Yates (1995) found that scale 4 correlated with dissatisfaction as a police officer when the “Subtle-Obvious” distinction on scale 4 is more closely examined. Other researchers have found that scale 4 subtle items were associated with termination of probationary officers (Weiss, Serafino, Serafino, Wilson, & Knoll, 1998). Overall, various studies have suggested that officers rated as unsatisfactory by their supervisors tend to score significantly higher on scale 4 (Psychopathic Deviate), scale 6 (Paranoia), and scale 9 (Hypomania) than satisfactory officers (Hargrave, 1987; Hargrave, Hiatt, & Gaffney, 1988). Building on this idea, Bartol (1991) followed 600 police officers from 34 small-town police departments over 13 years to determine which officers were eventually terminated. He concluded that an “immaturity index” consisted of a combination of scales 4 and 9 plus the L scale and was a strong predictor of termination. Other researchers such as Hargrave et al. (1988) have used a composite of MMPI scores (F, 4, 9, and CYN) to identify highly aggressive law enforcement officers and applicants. And Weiss 11

Jack Kitaeff

et al. (1998) found that scale 4 has predictive power for both job retention and supervisory ratings in police officer candidates. Although the research indicates modest success using the validity and clinical scales of the MMPI and MMPI-2 in predicting future police officer functioning, very few studies have addressed the various content scales of tests. The content scales were designed to provide relevant data on certain traits and behaviors not available from the standard MMPI-2 scales (Clark, 1994). Content scales include, but are not limited to, Anxiety (ANX), Obsessiveness (OBS), Health Concerns (HEA), Bizarre Mentation (BIZ), Cynicism (CYN), Family Problems (FAM), and Work Interference (WRK). Daniels and King (2002) examined the predictive ability of the MMPI-2 content scales in differentiating successful and unsuccessful small-town police officers and found that although the scores of police officers differed somewhat from the MMPI-2 normative sample, the content scales were not able to predict police officer performance.

The California Psychological Inventory The California Psychological Inventory (CPI), one of the oldest and most respected of the personality inventories, is similar in format to the MMPI, but its subscales include such personal traits as dominance, sociability, and flexibility. These subscales stand in contrast to the diagnostic categories of the MMPI (e.g., Psychopathic Deviate and Depression). Answers to the 434 items yield scores on 20 dimensions in four primary areas: 1. Measures of Poise—dominance, capacity for status, sociability, social presence, self-acceptance, independence, and empathy. 2. Measures of Normative Orientation and Values—responsibility, socialization, self-control, good impression, communality, well-being, and tolerance. 3. Measures of Cognitive and Intellectual Functioning—achievement via conformance, achievement via independence, and intellectual efficiency. 4. Measures of Role and Interpersonal Style—psychological mindedness, flexibility, and femininity/ masculinity. The CPI also yields scores on several special scales such as managerial potential (Mp), work orientation (Wo), creative temperament (Ct), leadership potential (Lp), amicability (Ami), law enforcement orientation (Leo), anxiety (ANX), narcissism (NAR), and tough-mindedness (Tm). Scale scores on the CPI have been found to be related to police trainees’ academy performance and to supervisors’ ratings (Bartol, 1991). Hargrave and Hiatt (1989) tested 575 police recruits with the CPI and found that CPI profiles distinguished between those suitable and unsuitable for training. The authors concluded that CPI profiles have a more consistent relationship with future job performance than police academy variables. The higher rated officers generally scored higher on the CPI primary areas assessing socialization, responsibility, interpersonal values, and character.

The Inwald Personality Inventory The Inwald Personality Inventory (IPI) was developed with the express purpose of directly questioning public safety/law enforcement candidates and documenting their admitted behaviors, rather than inferring those behaviors from statistically derived personality indicators specifically for the preemployment personality screening of police applicants (Inwald, 1992). The test is a 310-item, true–false questionnaire that usually requires about 45 minutes to complete. It consists of 25 scales and one validity scale (Guardedness). The 25 constructs measured by the IPI are:

12

Introduction and History

• • •



Acting Out Behavior: Alcohol use (AL), Drug use (DG), Driving violation (DV), Job difficulties ( JD), Trouble with society and the law (TL0), and Absence Abuse (AA). Acting Out Attitudes: Substance abuse (SA), Antisocial attitudes (AS), Hyperactivity (HP), Rigid style (RT), and Type A (TA). Internalized Conflict: Illness concerns (IC), Treatment programs (TP), Anxiety (AN), Phobic personality (PH), Obsessive personality (OB), Depression (DE), Loner type (LO), and Unusual experiences and thoughts (EU). Interpersonal Conflict: Lack of assertiveness (LA), Interpersonal difficulties (ID), Undue suspiciousness (US), Family conflicts (FC), Sexual concerns (SC), and Spouse/mate conflicts (SP).

Some studies have found the IPI predicting criterion measures such as retention or termination, absences, lateness, and disciplinary measures more often than the MMPI (Inwald & Shusman, 1984; Shusman & Inwald, 1991). In a cross-validation study, Shusman, Inwald, and Knatz (1987) found the IPI to be superior to the MMPI in predicting police academy success. Several studies have found that a combination of the IPI and MMPI is the most successful approach for predicting maladaptive behavior in police and corrections officers (Inwald, 1992; Scogin, Schumacher, Gardner, & Chaplin, 1995).

The Sixteen Personality Factor Questionnaire The Sixteen Personality Factor Questionnaire (16PF) is a 187-item personality inventory that usually requires about 1 hour to complete. The original test—published in 1949 by R.B. Cattell—yielded 16 scores in such traits as reserved vs. outgoing, humble vs. assertive, shy vs. venturesome, and trusting vs. suspicious (Cattell, Eber, & Tatsuoka, 1970). Since 1949, the 16PF has been revised six times, with the most recent revisions (1993, 1998) used by many law enforcement agencies today. The 16 primary factors of the 16PF consist of “A” (Interpersonal warmth), “B” (Abstract reasoning), “C” (Emotional stability), “E” (Dominance), “F” (Enthusiasm), “G” (Conscientiousness), “H” (Social boldness), “I” (Sensitivity), “L” (Vigilance), “M” (Imagination), “N” (Forthrightness), “O” (Apprehensiveness), “Q1” (Openness), “Q2” (Self-reliance), “Q3” (Perfectionism), and “Q4” (Tension). Results of one meta-analysis suggest that of the 16 scales, only two—Dominance and ­Conscientiousness—were significantly related to police performance (Aamodt, 2004). As indicated earlier, the test is used frequently in pre-employment psychological assessment in law enforcement, and individuals scoring high on certain scales are described alternatively as assertive and competitive, and conscientious and responsible, depending on the scales examined.

The Edwards Personal Preference Schedule The Edwards Personal Preference Schedule (EPPS) was developed in 1959 based on the manifest need system theory of H. A. Murray and his associates at the Harvard Psychological Clinic (Anastasi, 1976). Beginning with 15 needs drawn from Murray’s list of manifest needs, Edwards prepared sets of items whose content appeared to fit each need (Murray, 1938). Examples included the Need for Achievement (to do one’s best and accomplish something difficult), Deference (to conform to what is expected), Exhibition (to be the center of attention), Intraception (to analyze the motives and feelings of oneself and others), Dominance (to influence others and to be regarded as a leader), and Nurturance (to help others in trouble). In its present form, the EPPS consists of 225 pairs of psychological needs and yields a test consistency score and scores on 15 dimensions of manifest needs. These are Achievement, Deference, Order, Exhibition, Autonomy, Affiliation, Intraception, Succorance, Dominance, Abasement, Nurturance, Change, Endurance, Heterosexuality, and Aggression. Law enforcement research using the

13

Jack Kitaeff

EPPS has been rather limited (Aamodt, 2004). But Balch (1977) found that higher scores on Need for Achievement (r = .37) and consistency in completing the test (r = .22) were associated with completing a police academy.

Personality Assessment Inventory The Personality Assessment Inventory (PAI) is an objective inventory of adult personality designed to assess various psychopathological syndromes. This test, introduced in 2004, consists of 344 items and requires about 1 hour to complete (Weiss, Rostow, Davis, & DeCoster-Martin, 2004). The PAI contains the following scales: • •

• •

Validity scales: inconsistency, infrequency, negative impression (NIM), and positive impression. Clinical scales: somatic complaints (SOM), anxiety (ANX), anxiety related disorders (ARD), depression (DEP), mania (MAN), paranoia (PAR), schizophrenia (SCZ), borderline personality (BOR), antisocial personality (ANTA), antisocial-stimulus seeking (ANTS), antisocialegocentricity (ANTE), aggression (AGG), and substance abuse (DRG). Treatment consideration scales: aggression (AGG), suicidal ideation (SUI), stress (STR), nonsupport (NON), and treatment rejection (RXR). Interpersonal scales: dominance (DOM) and warmth (WRM) (Aamodt, 2004)

In a study by Weiss, Zehner, Davis, and Rostow (2005), PAI results of 800 male and female police officer candidates were examined as possible predictors of the criterion variables insubordination, excessive citizen complaints, and neglect of duty. The results indicated highly significant coefficients for antisocial-egocentricity (ANTE) as a predictor of insubordination and excessive citizen complaints. Significant coefficients were also obtained for antisocial-stimulus seeking (ANTS) and negative impression (NIM) as predictors of neglect of duty.

Rorschach Inkblot Test The Rorschach Inkblot Test has been one of the most popular techniques for personality assessment for the last 90 years. Developed by the Swiss psychiatrist Hermann Rorschach, the Rorschach technique utilizes 10 stimulus cards. Five of the stimulus cards are in shades of gray and black only, two contain additional touches of bright red, and the remaining three combine several pastel shades. As the examinee is shown each inkblot, he or she is asked to tell the examiner what the blot could be. Besides keeping a verbatim record of the responses to each card, the examiner notes times of responses, position or positions in which cards are held, spontaneous remarks, emotional expressions, and other incidental behavior of the examinee during the test session. Following the presentation of all 10 cards, the examiner questions the individual systematically regarding the parts and aspects of each blot to which the associations were given. During this inquiry, the respondents also have the opportunity to clarify and elaborate their earlier responses. The Rorschach test has been subjected to several major scoring and interpretive systems through the years. But the most ambitious effort at developing a psychometrically sound system was undertaken by Exner (1974, 1978, 1993), who presented a standardized system for the administration, scoring, and interpretation of Rorschach responses. He used ratios, indices, and combinations of variables in his comprehensive system (Exner, 1978). Some researchers and clinicians believe that, considering the Rorschach’s utility as a diagnostic and assessment devise in other areas, it could be employed effectively in hitherto unused areas that could include law enforcement selection (Meyer, 2004; Meyer, Mihura, & Smith, 2005). Indeed, the

14

Introduction and History

Board of Trustees of the Society for Personality Assessment concluded that “the Rorschach possesses documented reliability and validity similar to other generally accepted test instruments used in the assessment of personality and psychopathology and that its responsible use in personality assessment is appropriate and justified” (2005, p. 221). While no data for the successful use of the Rorschach in selecting police officers currently exist, the test has been used successfully in other settings, including employment-related screenings in other fields (Exner & Weiner, 1994). The Rorschach has also been used for fitness-for-duty evaluations for employees, including airline pilots (Ganellen, 1996). But in order for the Rorschach to be considered acceptable as an assessment tool for the screening of police applicants, considerable research must be undertaken to determine its appropriateness and effectiveness.

Vocational Interest Inventories Kuder Occupational Interest Inventory The Kuder Preference Record was developed in 1948 and then replaced in 1971 by the Kuder Occupational Interest Inventory (Aamodt, 2004). The Kuder is a 100-item interest inventory developed through criterion keying procedures for the purpose of measuring vocational interests (Kuder, 1985). The inventory takes about 45 minutes to complete and yields scores on 119 occupational groups (including police) and 10 occupational areas (outdoor, mechanical, computational, scientific, persuasive, artistic, literary, musical, clerical, and social service). There have been recommendations that an interest inventory be included as part of any testing battery to assess police applicants (Spielberger, 1979). Azen, Snibbe, and Montgomery (1973) found a correlation of .24 between the Mechanical Interest scale and supervisor ratings of performance. The same authors used the Kuder to select police officer candidates in the city of Los Angeles and found that the Kuder Mechanic Scale was also a predictor of success in police work.

Strong Interest Inventory The Strong Interest Inventory (SII) was originally formulated in 1920 by E. K. Strong Jr. and has been revised many times since then. The inventory was based on the work of John Holland, who defined six basic occupational themes (called Holland codes) that can be used to categorize occupations as well as individuals (Anastasi, 1976): Realistic (machines, tools, outdoors), Investigative (science, theories, ideas, data), Artistic (self-expression, art appreciation), Social (people, team work, human welfare, community services), Enterprising (business, politics, leadership, influence), and Conventional (organization, data, finance). The most recent version produces 30 Basic Interest Scales, 244 Occupational Scales, and more than 120 specific jobs. The research supporting the Strong as a predictor of police suitability or performance is meager.

Situational Tests As will be discussed in greater depth later in this volume, besides the administration of paper-andpencil tests, psychologists may administer computerized situational tests designed to measure what applicants might say or what they might do in response to interactive videos that depict police workrelated events. For example, a psychologist can present to an applicant a video that shows a potential interaction with someone. The applicant must quickly make an appraisal ( judgment) as to how dangerous the person’s behavior is (e.g., using a scale ranging from not at all dangerous to very dangerous). Then the applicant is required to make a decision as to how forcefully he or she would respond

15

Jack Kitaeff

(e.g., using a scale ranging from an escapable action to a lethal action). The psychologist can evaluate the applicant’s judgments and decisions, which may reveal aggressive inclinations. Results from situational tests can greatly add to the applicant’s pre-employment screening and evaluation. However, when performing such evaluations, psychologists should not expect police applicants to have the same fund of knowledge as someone who has been a police officer for an appreciable amount of time. What psychologists should assess is the personal experiences and judgment inclinations applicants bring to the job assessment and to the job itself. But it should be noted that situational tests using the computer are very expensive and are not backed up by empirical psychological literature, which would be needed to validate them. In 1989, a major revision of the MMPI was published (Butcher et al., 2001), resulting in the MMPI-2, which in turn caused police psychologists to attempt further validation studies with the new test. While welcoming obsolete item revisions, police psychologists, who were accustomed to interpreting MMPI profiles using the original clinical norms, expressed concern that the new updated norms showed far fewer elevations for the more “normal” and defensive police/public safety officer candidates. This led many psychologists involved in pre-employment assessment to add additional tests (sometimes nonpsychological in nature) to their standard batteries in order to either augment or to replace the MMPI-2. Also, when the Polygraph Act of 1988 banned the use of the pre-employment polygraph for nonfederal police agencies, newly unemployed polygraph examiners became interested in promoting the use of paper-and-pencil honesty tests and “integrity tests.” But integrity tests have not been studied much. In his 1994 book, Theodore Blau indicated that of the 40 integrity tests available at the time, few studies of these tests had been published in peer-reviewed journals. Today, mostly the National Security Agency and the Central Intelligence Agency make significant use of the polygraph for personnel security and pre-employment screening. But it seems to slowly be making a comeback as an adjunct to pre-employment psychological screening for police applicants.

The Polygraph Machine in Historical Context: Early Pioneers The modern polygraph has been used in one form or another for nearly a century, and much cruder versions of its components existed as far back as 300 B.C.E. The Bedouins of Arabia, for example, required the authors of conflicting statements to lick a hot iron. The one whose tongue was not burned was considered truthful. The common principle underlying this primitive test as well as others (e.g., spitting drier rice powder that had been placed in one’s mouth, or being less able to swallow a “trial slice” of bread, as practiced centuries ago in England) is that the tense, nervous person (the one who is lying) has less saliva (dry mouth and tongue) and thus is more likely to have their tongue burned, spit dry rice, or have difficulty swallowing a piece of bread (Smith, 1997). As indicated previously, in modern times, the admissibility of voice identification evidence into court is usually evaluated using the Daubert standard, which allows such evidence into testimony if the judge assesses that such evidence will assist the jury in better understanding the case as a whole. In 1878, Italian physiologist Angelo Mosso used an instrument called a plethysmograph (an instrument for measuring changes in volume within an organ or whole body, usually resulting from fluctuations in the amount of blood or air it contains) in his research on emotion and fear in subjects undergoing questioning. He studied, in particular, the effects of emotions such as fear on subjects’ cardiovascular and respiratory activity. He also studied blood circulation and breathing patterns and how these changed under specified conditions. The use of the plethysmograph revealed periodic waves in a subject’s blood pressure caused by changes in the respiratory cycle in response to certain stimuli (Kitaeff, 2010). Mosso was the first scientist to report on experiments in which he observed that a person’s breathing pattern changed under certain stimuli, and that this change, in turn, caused variations in their blood pressure and pulse rate. Although not for the purpose of detecting deception, 16

Introduction and History

Sir James Mackenzie constructed the first clinical polygraph in 1892, an instrument to be used for medical examinations with the capability to simultaneously record undulated line tracings of the vascular pulses (radial, venous, and arterial) by way of a stylus onto a revolving drum of smoked paper. Until the end of the 19th century, no measuring device for the detection of deception had ever been used. The first use of a scientific instrument designed to measure physiological responses for this purpose came in 1895 when Italian physician, psychiatrist, and pioneer criminologist Cesare Lombroso modified an existing instrument called a hydrosphygmograph and used this modified device in his experiments to measure the physiological changes that occurred in a crime suspect’s blood pressure and pulse rate during a police interrogation. In 1914, Italian psychologist Vittorio Benussi invented a method to calculate the quotient of the inhalation to exhalation time as a means of verifying the truth and detecting deception in a subject. Using a pneumograph—a device that recorded a subject’s breathing patterns—Benussi conducted experiments regarding the respiratory symptoms of lying. He concluded that lying caused an emotional change within a subject that resulted in detectible respiratory changes that were indicative of deception. William Moulton Marston, a U.S. attorney and psychologist, is credited with inventing an early form of the lie detector when, in 1915, he developed the discontinuous systolic blood pressure test that would later become one component of the modern polygraph. Marston’s technique used a standard blood pressure cuff and a stethoscope to take intermittent systolic blood pressure readings of a suspect during questioning for the purpose of detecting deception (Lykken, 1988). The idea of “lie detection” caught on rapidly in the United States during the 1920s and 1930s after John Larson was asked by the chief of police in Berkeley, California, to develop a lie detector to solve a case under investigation (Larson, 1969). This instrument, to many, became the first true polygraph used for lie detection purposes (Bartol & Bartol, 2004). In the 1930s, the increasing demand for polygraph examiners resulted in at least 30 polygraph schools opening across the United States (Barland & Raskin, 1976; Lykken, 1988). A modern polygraph machine is an instrument that simultaneously records changes in physiological processes such as heartbeat, blood pressure, respiration, and electrical resistance (galvanic skin response or GSR). The underlying theory of the polygraph is that when people lie, they also get measurably nervous about lying. The heartbeat increases, blood pressure goes up, breathing rhythms change, perspiration increases, and so on. A baseline for these physiological characteristics is established by asking the subject questions whose answers the investigator knows. Deviations from the baseline for truthfulness are usually taken as signs of lying (Kitaeff, 2010).

Approaches to the Polygraph Test There are three basic approaches to the polygraph test: (1) the Control Question Test (CQT) (also referred to as the relevant–irrelevant technique), (2) the Directed Lie Test (DLT), and (3) the Guilty Knowledge Test (GKT). The details of each of these tests are beyond the scope of this chapter, but suffice it to say, they are all used with varying degrees of success to help ascertain if an individual is telling the truth (Allen, 2002; Allen & Iacono, 2001; Kitaeff, 2010). From a theoretical standpoint, truthful responses are accompanied by relatively flat reaction lines, whereas untruthful responses are hypothesized to cause significant fluctuations in physiological measures such as heart rate, perspiration, and respiration. In addition, some evidence suggests that the accuracy of polygraphs can even be increased through intensive training of the examiner and by using computerized systems. But the majority of the scientific evidence indicates that whatever accuracy the polygraph examination does provide seems to come from the conclusions made by individual examiners of such factors such as the subject’s general demeanor rather than his or her actual chart responses. Most experienced examiners would even concur that the key ingredient for a 17

Jack Kitaeff

competently administered polygraph is the examiner—regardless of whether a computerized scoring procedure is used (Iacono, 2008). Currently, the research conducted under laboratory or controlled conditions indicates that the correct classification of truthful and deceptive examinees ranges between 70% and 80% at best (Kitaeff, 2010).

Criticisms of the Polygraph Test One of the problems with the polygraph is its susceptibility to countermeasures used by examinees to fool the machine and the examiner (Bartol & Bartol, 2004). Countermeasures can be physical or psychological. The most common countermeasures used are either pain or muscle tension. For example, in an effort to deceive the polygrapher, “biting one’s lip or tongue or subtly jabbing oneself with a pin may induce enough pain to promote a physiological response that masks the subject’s response to questions from the polygrapher” (Bartol & Bartol, 2004, p. 86). Mental countermeasures include deliberate attempts at distortion using techniques such as counting backward from 100 or thinking of a peaceful or arousing scene. In this way, the examinee tries to either minimize or maximize the emotional impact of questions. Although physical countermeasures can often be detected by experienced polygraphers, mental countermeasures are far more difficult to detect (BenShakhar & Dolev, 1996). Overall, it is generally recognized that no physiological response is uniquely associated with lying. Moreover, the brain mechanisms involved in lying are unknown, rendering it difficult to develop techniques that can distinguish liars from truth tellers. Indeed, a person’s physiological reactions in response to lying are likely similar in nature to those resulting from the passionate denial that occurs when false charges are leveled against him. The fear of detection is indistinguishable from the fear of false detection, and the physiological reactions recorded by the polygraph under these two circumstances cannot be discriminated from each other. Scientists with no direct involvement in the polygraph profession have repeatedly reviewed the scientific literature and concluded that the accuracy claims of the polygraph profession are exaggerated and indefensible. Carroll (1988) summarized the results of a series of laboratory experiments on the polygraph using mock crimes and blind scoring (where examiners had no knowledge of guilt or innocence). The results suggested a “staggering high rate of false positives and a dearth of contribution to the process of establishing innocence” (p. 27). These opinions are reflected in reviews spanning several decades (Lykken, 1988; Saxe, Dougherty, & Cross, 1985) as well as in more recent evaluations of the literature carried out for texts in polygraphy and forensic psychology (Ben-Shakhar, 2008; Iacono & Patrick, 1999; Oksol & O’Donohue, 2003). In a 1997 survey of 195 psychologists from the Society for Psychophysiological Research, most respondents answered that polygraphic lie detection is not theoretically sound, that the test can be beaten by easily learned countermeasures, and that test results should not be admitted in courts of law (Lacono & Lykken, 1997). The issue was settled in 1998 with the U.S. Supreme Court case of U.S. v. Scheffer, where in an 8-to-1 decision the Court emphasized the poor reliability of polygraph evidence as a whole. Justice Thomas, writing for the majority, stressed that a fundamental premise of the criminal justice system is that jurors are the ultimate and most reliable evaluators of credibility and truthfulness (U.S. v. Scheffer at 313). Since 1998, almost all courts reject polygraph results or only allow them if their admissibility is stipulated by both parties prior to the polygraph examination taking place. Only a few courts leave their admissibility up to the judge (Kitaeff, 2010).

Pre-employment, Psychological Screening, and the Law In general, psychologists have designed pre-employment psychological-screening programs by attempting to “screen out” those applicants found to be psychologically inappropriate for police 18

Introduction and History

work and to “screen in” those with positive characteristics, attitudes, and traits known to be associated with highly functioning officers. The screening-out method is based on the hypothesis that an officer who is “psychologically unstable” is more likely to violate the civil rights of citizens and contribute to community unrest than a “stable” officer. Most psychologists who work with law enforcement agencies employ a screening battery composed of an interview, a cognitive test, and two or more personality tests. There are legal and liability-related reasons why the pre-employment screening of police applicants is necessary. Suing the police is very popular in the United States. It can also be very profitable, as the average jury award is over $2 million. This does not take into account the hundreds of cases settled out of court likely amounting to hundreds of millions of dollars. Lawsuits against the police may be filed in state court as a tort law claim. This is the preferred method because a tort (a “wrong” in the legal sense) can usually only be settled by monetary awards for damages done to the plaintiff. In addition, courts may impose punitive damages against the police department as punishment if the wrong done to the plaintiff was particularly severe in the eyes of the court. Under the law, the failure of a police department to properly select police officers can easily be considered a severe form of neglect. A department may be liable for a charge of negligence by employing an individual who possesses personality attributes or prior conduct that would create an undue risk of harming others in carrying out his or her official responsibilities. Furthermore, the standard of proof in these civil cases is “by a preponderance of the evidence,” which is much easier to meet than the criminal standard of “beyond a reasonable doubt.” Tort liability associated with lawsuits against police departments may also include wrongful death, assault and battery, false arrest, and false imprisonment. Gross negligence may be assumed by a department’s failure to conduct full background investigations of applicants. Police departments can also be charged with negligent retention (keeping employees on the job when they clearly should have been disciplined, demoted, or dismissed) and negligent entrustment (inadequately preparing officers prior to entrusting them with police responsibilities). The risk to municipal government (and to the public) is considerable when a potential employee is a police officer. This increased risk stems from the privileges granted to commissioned police officers to take “life and liberty” under special circumstances. In U.S. society, this privilege to exert lawful, sometimes deadly force and restraint is why the hiring of police applicants known to possess destructive characteristics can make a department so legally liable. Deliberate indifference may be said to apply when a police department should have had the ability to foresee a risk to the public, but for any reason did not take possible steps to minimize that danger. In instances such as these, a lawsuit may be filed in federal court as a violation of Title 42 of the U.S. Code. This is a legal claim that alleges that there has been a violation of someone’s civil rights under the U.S. Constitution. Although individual states cannot be sued in federal court under a civil rights claim, municipalities (and their employees), police departments (and their employees), and sheriff ’s departments (and their employees) can be sued if it is shown that they were acting “under color of law,” and violated a specific amendment of the Constitution. The standards used in federal courts in this regard are custom or policy and deliberate indifference, both of which are loosely defined concepts but similar to totality of the circumstances. Although such suits can result in monetary awards, the amounts are usually not great because the purpose of civil rights suits filed under 42 U.S.C. 1983 is essentially injunctive (i.e., to force the municipality or department to change its procedures or behaviors). A lawsuit against a police department by a present employee or police applicant may also be filed in federal court stemming from a complaint filed with the U.S. Equal Employment Opportunity Commissions (EEOC). Such complaints could be based on race, age, or sex discrimination in hiring, retention, or promotions. During the late 1970s, the personality tests most commonly used with public safety office candidates included the MMPI, the CPI, the 16PF, the EPPS, and various projective tests. Because the 19

Jack Kitaeff

MMPI was the most widely used nonprojective personality assessment instrument available to clinical psychologists at the time, it was a natural choice to be used by psychologists who began working in the police and public safety officer selection field (Murphy, 1972). It was first used in police research in an attempt to predict police academy grades (King, Norrell, & Erlandson, 1959). Articles suggesting the usefulness of the MMPI in police settings, particularly for pre-employment screening, began to appear in the 1970s (Azen, 1974; Azen et al., 1973; Saccuzzo, Higgins, & Lewandowski, 1974; Saxe & Reiser, 1976). However, the MMPI originally had been developed and normed for use with inpatients in psychiatric hospitals and not with more highly functioning and symptom-denying job applicants for law enforcement officer positions. It was noticed by practitioners that the norms for the MMPI often showed “normal” ranges for individual candidates, whereas the job histories and antisocial behavior patterns discovered in background investigations and psychological interviews for these same individuals suggested potential difficulties for high-risk positions (Inwald, 1982). There was growing concern about the inadequacy of available instruments, or of evidence to support the MMPI as a predictor of police performance (Mills & Stratton, 1982). The 1980s represented another decade of change for police psychology. With much growing interest and development in police/public safety personality testing, a larger number of research articles and instrument publications began to appear as more psychologists became involved in conducting pre-employment evaluations for police departments and the need to firmly establish the validity of the instruments used for this application was recognized. In 1981, police psychologists became aware of an important lawsuit in New York City. On December 20, 1976, NYC police officer Blase A. Bonsignore used his off-duty revolver to shoot his wife and then to commit suicide. His wife suffered brain damage and serious motor dysfunction as a result. In the lawsuit that followed, the jury awarded his wife $425,000 in damages on her negligence claim (Bonsignore v. the City of N.Y., 1982). This was a very important case in the development of police psychological-screening programs in that its outcome persuaded many reluctant chiefs that they could have serious difficulties defending hiring decisions if they failed to provide adequate psychological screening at the outset. Meanwhile, the shorter, 310-item IPI soon gained considerable popularity with police psychologists, especially as its validity for pre-employment screening became established during the 1980s. Several IPI research publications and independent studies were completed, including the first longitudinal cross-validation studies and the first publications that directly compared law enforcement candidate test results by gender and ethnicity (Inwald, 1988; Inwald & Shusman, 1984; Scogin & Beutler, 1986). Also at this time, there was some early recognition of the emerging area of police– public safety assessment by professional psychology organizations. For example, in 1982, the New York State Psychological Association’s “Meritorious Research Award” was presented to Inwald for “contribution to test construction in an important new area.” After the IPI computerized report was published in 1982, specifying how test results could be interpreted using public safety officer applicant norms, National Computer Systems (NCS) introduced an MMPI computer-generated narrative report, the “Minnesota Report: Personnel Selection System,” in 1984. The original MMPI clinical norms still were used in the MMPI profile graphs, and no cutoff scores were suggested for law enforcement officer candidates or for other job applicants. In 1985, police psychologists began to expand their practices to include “fitness-for-duty” testing. When some psychologists who were using the IPI for pre-employment screening began to readminister this test as an aid for determining fitness for duty of already hired officers (Rice, 1985), it was clear there was a need for more specialized assessment instruments. In 1986, the first comprehensive personality inventory developed for “fitness-for-duty” evaluations of public safety officers, the Hilson Career Satisfaction Index (HCSI), was developed and validated by Robin Inwald (Inwald & Kaufman, 1989). In 1988, at the IACP annual convention, Inwald introduced the Hilson Personnel Profile/ Success Quotient (HPP/SQ), the first comprehensive personality inventory measuring “emotional 20

Introduction and History

intelligence” (Inwald, 2004; Inwald & Brobst, 1988; Inwald & Brockwell, 1988). Validated for police personnel hiring and promotion, and based on Inwald’s “Success Quotient Theory,” this measure focused on a combination of characteristics such as initiative, “winner’s image,” work ethic, ­popularity/charisma, and social sensitivity. Norms were collected for police chiefs at the 1988 IACP convention and, after validation data were analyzed for law enforcement officer applicants throughout the United States, the HPP/SQ became a standard addition to many police psychologists’ pre-­ employment and promotional test batteries. In 1988, Inwald also added discriminant-function-derived prediction equations for termination, absence, lateness, and disciplinary actions to the IPI’s computerized report. These equations were based on the first five-year longitudinal prediction study, including cross-validation, for public safety officers. The results were published in the Journal of Applied Psychology (Inwald, 1988). Because of complaints about “honesty/integrity” tests received by members of Congress, its Office of Technology Assessment (OTA) launched an investigation into the validity of these tests now flooding the market (though mostly in the retail area). With similar concerns, the American Psychological Association (APA) also organized a special task force to review available research. At this time, practicing police psychologists expressed growing concerns about claims of validity for tests not previously used to screen law enforcement applicants. Because these tests, with little or no published research connected with police performance, were marketed directly to police chiefs and others for use without the input of a psychologist, several articles and research reviews were published on the subject (Inwald, 1984b, 1988, 1992; Sackett & Harris, 1984b; Sackett, Burris, & Callahan, 1989). When the final reports from OTA (1990) and APA (1991) were published, they showed conflicting results. The OTA report concluded that “existing research is insufficient as a basis for supporting the assertion that (honesty tests) can reliably predict dishonest behavior in the workplace.” In any case, the Polygraph Act and subsequent “honesty test debate” of the late 1980s certainly encouraged increased scholarship and validation research in the area of public safety/police assessment. A complaint may also be based on alleged violation of the Americans with Disabilities Act (ADA). The provisions of the ADA state that individuals who believe they have been discriminated against on the basis of their disability can file a charge with the Commission at any of its offices located throughout the United States. The Commission will investigate and initially attempt to resolve the charge through conciliation following the same procedures used to handle charges of discrimination filed under Title VII of the Civil Rights Act of 1964. A charging party may file a lawsuit within 90 days after receiving a notice of a “right to sue” from the EEOC. The ADA also incorporates the remedies contained in Title VII. These remedies can include rehiring, promotion, reinstatement, back pay, and attorney’s fees.

Reasonable Accommodations Reasonable accommodation (making changes in the nature of the employee’s job) is also available as a remedy under the ADA. Under most EEOC-enforced laws, compensatory and punitive damages also may be available where intentional discrimination is found. Damages may be available to compensate for actual monetary losses, future monetary losses, and mental anguish and inconvenience. Punitive damages (used to punish an agency for wrongdoings) also may be available if an employer acted with malice or reckless indifference; however, such damages are not available against the federal, state, or local governments. Certainly there are numerous reasons from legal, psychological, safety, and commonsense standpoints why police departments and other law enforcement agencies should perform some form of pre-employment psychological screening. Perhaps an Indiana appellate court said it best when it opined that a “policeman frequently works alone, wields great authority, carries lethal weapons” and, 21

Jack Kitaeff

“It is not an occupation for . . . a person with questionable emotional stability” (City of Greenwood v. Dowler, 1986 at 1081).

Fitness-for-Duty Evaluations Police employers have a legal duty to ensure that police officers under their command are mentally and emotionally fit to perform their duties, and failure to do so can result in significant civil liability (Bonsignore v. City of New York, 1982) and serious consequences to citizens, the examinee, other officers, an employing agency’s reputation, and the trust of the community. Various courts have interpreted this duty to include the authority to mandate psychological fitness-for-duty (FFD) evaluations of police officers reasonably believed to be impaired in their ability to perform their job functions because of a known or suspected psychological condition (Colon v. City of Newark, 2006; Conte v. Horcher, 1977; Deen v. Darosa, 2005; Kraft v. Police Commissioner of Boston, 1994; McKnight v. Monroe Co. Sheriff’s Dept., 2002; Tingler v. City of Tampa, 1981; Watson v. City of Miami Beach, 1999). In Chapter 13 of this volume, Corey expresses that the circumstances giving rise to fitnessfor-duty evaluations (FFDEs) are many and varied. They may involve suspicion of job-relevant psychopathology associated with on-duty performance (e.g., excessive force, repeated problems of judgment), off-duty conduct (e.g., domestic violence, driving while intoxicated), a suicide attempt, psychiatric hospitalization, or a disability claim. Stone (2000) reported that 26% of the cases from his own practice in the southern region of the United States resulted from suspected psychopathology (i.e., diagnosable mental condition), 19% from excessive force issues, 15% from substance abuse, 13% from behavior implicating poor judgment, and 9% from domestic violence. Dawkins, Griffin, and Dawkins (2006) utilized an alternative classification scheme for their analysis of the FFD referrals in their own Midwestern practice. Similar to Stone, they found that 16.5% of the more than 200 referrals they analyzed involved alcohol use, but they reported more referrals involving domestic violence.They reported behavioral concerns, psychopathology or emotional distress, and 4.7% were for officers being considered for rehire following an employment separation. The right of a police employer to intrude on the medical and personal privacy of its officers derives from two special features of police work: the power of the position and the fact that police officers are public employees. Police officers are members of quasi-military organizations, called upon for duty at all times, armed at almost all times, and exercising the most awesome and dangerous power that a democratic state possesses with respect to its residents—the power to use lawful force to arrest and detain them. (Policemen’s Benevolent Association of New Jersey v. Township of Washington, 1988, at 141) As citizens, police officers retain their constitutional rights (e.g., Garrity v. New Jersey, 1967), but as public employees, they “subordinate their right to privacy as a private citizen to the superior right of the public to have an efficient and credible police department” (Richardson v. City of Pasadena, 1973/1974, at headnote 1). In the words of the U.S. Supreme Court, “The public should not bear the risk that employees who may suffer from impaired perception and judgment will be [in] positions where they may need to employ deadly force” (National Treasury Employees Union v. Von Raab, 1989, at 671). The employer’s duty to ensure a psychologically fit workforce does not, however, extend an unfettered right to require such evaluations of any police officer in any instance (Denhof v. City of Grand Rapids, 2005; Holst v. Veterans Affairs, 2008; Jackson v. Lake County, 2003; McGreal v. Ostrov, 2004). Instead, the employer’s duty is balanced by public interests and the employee’s constitutional, 22

Introduction and History

civil, and property rights and interests. But how do we determine truth telling on the part of an officer-employee? Corey (see Chapter 13 in this volume) points out two primary means for assessing the response style of an officer: third-party information and psychological assessment instruments, or tests, with built-in measures of response style. It is important to gather third-party information. Its utility is tied not only to the improved depth and breadth of information obtained through collateral sources but also to its potential for detecting malingering, defensiveness, dishonesty, and uncooperativeness. Melton et al. (1997) observed that “obtaining information contradicting the client’s version of events is probably the most accurate means of detecting fabrication and may be the only viable one with clients who sabotage interview and testing efforts” (pp. 57–58). This is not to say that any given collateral source should be considered to be more reliable than the examinee, but rather that consistencies and discrepancies across and within data sources are important means of weighing validity. Standardized assessment instruments with validity scales or indices, such as the MMPI-2, MMPI2-RF, PAI, CPI, MCMI-III, and 16PF, also can serve useful roles in evaluating response style, especially when used in conjunction with third-party information. Consideration should always be given, however, to the base rates of these scales in a particular norm group, such that examinees in an FFDE are not judged to be defensive or dissimulating on the basis of validity scales alone when the scale norms were derived from respondents from a decidedly different context (i.e., police applicants in a pre-employment evaluation may not be comparable on validity scales to incumbent employees in an FFD examination) (Heilbrun, 2001). Evidence of frank lying, dissimulation, falsification, or overt concealment should always be reported to the referring party along with any reservations or limitations in the reliability of the examiner’s opinions as a result of the employee’s response style. In general, however, when the examinee’s response style, based on evidence from third-party sources and psychological testing, suggests that his or her responses are unreliable, they should be regarded as less probative than information obtained from other sources with higher reliability.

Treatment If causation is a compelling topic for clinicians, it pales in comparison to the attraction to treatment. Clinicians generally wish to be helpful, and this is especially true when faced with an examinee who is psychologically injured, suffering, or otherwise distressed. Indeed, when an evaluator concludes that an employee is psychologically unfit for duty, the referring party often requests that the examiner address how fitness might be restored. When this is the case, evaluators should still be careful to limit their treatment recommendations, including modalities and duration, to those for which there is adequate evidence of effectiveness. Most importantly, examiners should be careful not to condition a determination of fitness on an employee’s participation in counseling or other therapy. When such a condition is stipulated, it typically is rationalized in one of two ways: either because the examiner believes the employee’s current fitness is unlikely to be sustained without additional or ongoing treatment, or because the employee’s current unfitness results from a minor impairment expected to respond quickly to treatment. In cases where the employee is judged to be currently unfit for duty because of a remediable condition, it is usually more prudent to acknowledge the current unfit status and not address recommendations designed to restore fitness unless requested by the referring party. When an employer requires treatment as an employment condition, some jurisdictions have held that the employer is responsible for the cost of treatment and compensation to the employee for any on-duty time used to engage in treatment (Sehie v. City of Aurora, 2005; see also Todd v. Lexington Fayette Urban County Government, 2009, where the court reached a different conclusion when the mandated conditions are for the benefit of the employee and not the employer). Furthermore, employees required to engage in treatment can easily come to believe (erroneously) that treatment compliance will or should 23

Jack Kitaeff

shield them from consequences for persistent problematic behavior. Unless the referring party has specifically requested the examiner to opine on treatment or restoration of fitness, the report should be silent on this question. When the employer does request treatment recommendations from the examiner, it may be useful to reference the practical guidance of the EEOC: Regardless of whether employers believe they are trying to help employees who have medical conditions, employers should focus instead on addressing unacceptable workplace conduct. Employer comments about the disability and its treatment could lead to potential ADA claims (e.g., the employer “regarded” the employee as having a disability or the employer engaged in disparate treatment). (EEOC, 2008, p. 12)

Accommodation Under the ADA, an employer “must provide a reasonable accommodation to the known physical or mental limitations of a qualified individual with a disability unless it can show that the accommodation would impose an undue hardship” (EEOC, 1997, p. 12). A reasonable accommodation is defined as “any change in the work environment or in the way things are customarily done that enables an individual with a disability to enjoy equal employment opportunities” (29 C.F.R. §1630.9). This may include job restructuring, part-time or modified work schedules, reassignment to a vacant position, and providing additional unpaid leave for necessary treatment. Employers are only responsible, however, for providing a reasonable accommodation to a known limitation of a qualified individual with a disability. Even when an employee has a psychological condition that renders him or her unfit for duty, it is not per se legally equivalent to a disability under the terms of the ADA. Indeed, some conditions that may not meet the ADA’s definition of a disability may nevertheless render an employee unfit for duty. Moreover, except in cases of obvious disabilities, it is generally the case that the employer’s obligation to explore accommodation arises only when the employee tells the employer he is disabled (Hammon v. DHL, 1997). The employee need not use the word disabled or disability to formally initiate the accommodation exploration process (Cannice v. Norwest Bank Iowa N.A., 1999); on the other hand, where the employee openly denies having a disabling condition, even an FFD examiner’s finding to the contrary may not be sufficient to require accommodation efforts on the part of the employer (Larson v. Koch Refining Co., 1997). Once an employee specifically requests an accommodation for a work-impairing condition, the employer has an affirmative duty to engage in the “interactive process” to obtain relevant information about the employee’s condition and the basis for requesting an accommodation (Barnett v. U.S. Air., Inc., 2000). Thus, employers may prefer—and usually are best advised—to separate questions of reasonable accommodation from the FFD question. However, when in the interests of expediency and/or compassion an employer asks the examiner to make recommendations regarding accommodation, the examiner should become knowledgeable about the types of accommodation regarded by the courts as reasonable (e.g., an employer is not required to restructure or reallocate the essential functions of a job as a reasonable accommodation, and the essential functions may include the start and stop time of an employee’s work schedule; see Guice-Mills v. Derwinski, 1992). Unless the examiner is familiar with the statutes, regulations, and case law pertinent to reasonable accommodation, it is best to inform the referring party; police department that questions about accommodations be deferred to a separate stage of the evaluation. Under any circumstances, reasonable accommodations are always an employer’s decision, not an examiner’s. As noted elsewhere, in 1990, the Americans with Disabilities Act was passed by Congress. This law prohibited “pre-conditional offer psychological/medical” testing and became effective in 1992. Passed with the American Psychological Association’s written support, and before APA executives 24

Introduction and History

had notified Division 18 police psychologists of ADA’s ramifications for their assessment work, this new law had a major, mostly negative, impact on police assessment. Police psychologists, prior to 1992, were saving departments large sums of money and resources by inquiring about alcohol/drug use during the early stages of applicant investigations and screening. Robin Inwald and Michael Roberts, who between them at that time had the largest collection of pre-employment test data in the country, pooled these data from various testing instruments and databases. They collaborated on an article about the job-relatedness of such questions (Inwald, Kaufman, & Roberts, 1991) in an attempt to influence the writers of the government’s upcoming ADA Guidance report (guidelines for the new law). Despite the admittedly clear evidence that patterns of alcohol and/or past drug use on written personality/psychological instruments did, in fact, predict poor police officer performance across many test scales and performance variables, Inwald and Roberts’s campaign for an ADA exclusion of public safety organizations on behalf of practicing police psychologists failed. The published ADA guidelines required that police agencies adhere to the requirement that such questions only be asked after an actual “conditional offer of employment” had been made near the end of the screening process. Whereas some psychologists continued to conduct “psychological/medical” screenings using the MMPI or IPI prior to conditional job offers in their departments, most police psychologists changed their pre-employment assessment practices after the passage of the ADA in order to avoid lawsuits and to clearly be within the law. But the choice between administering the MMPI and associated tests, along with inquiring into psychological/mental health issues after the job offer or not at all, was not actually dichotomous. In other words, there was another option available to police psychologists who desired to retain the information of all the hitherto (i.e., pre-ADA) administered psychological tests interview questions yet at the same time not running afoul of the ADA. One of these solutions was developed by this author and consisted of conducting pre-employment psychological screening in two phases. More specifically, a general interview devoid of a mental status examination or questions that could reveal a disability was coupled with a “general personality test” such as the 16PF that only indicated degrees of “normal” personality traits such as interpersonal warmth, dominance, seriousness, conformity vs. nonconformity, being solution oriented, self-assuredness, being open to change, and self-discipline would be administered as part of the “Phase 1” psychological screening. Any other factors that could possibly hint at a disability were not used. If the applicant passed “Phase 1” of the screening, he or she was invited back for “Phase 2,” which consisted of the MMPI-2, the Wonderlic Personnel Test, and a clinical interview combined with a mental status evaluation. This technique for working within the requirements of the ADA was utilized by the author with various police and sheriff ’s departments in the northern Virginia area. The downside of this process was the additional time burden on the applicant, the department, and the psychologist, for scheduling two interviews and for the additional work needed by the psychologist in writing two reports, one based on Phase 1 and the other based on Phase 2. Accordingly, this author, together with the various law enforcements served, decided to administer the entire psychological at the end of the applicant screening process. In this manner, it would be the last appointment for any given applicant. Under such an arrangement, if an applicant had been successful up until the psychological and was then rejected, the reason would be clear, and the need for the cryptic and misleading “other more qualified applicants” letter would be unnecessary. This is not to say, however, that rejected applicants still did not receive this generic form of rejection letter. A second law passed subsequent to the ADA was the Civil Rights Act of 1991, which also had a strong impact on assessment practices and instrument development in the police/public safety field. With concerns that police assessment validity would be decreased should it become illegal to use separate gender norms, APA’s Diane Brown organized a meeting with EEOC representatives and two researchers in the field. Paul Sackett and Robin Inwald presented research data supporting the value of using separate group norms to best predict police and other employee’s performance. 25

Jack Kitaeff

Despite their efforts to preserve the best predictions possible for employee selection, the Civil Right Act of 1991 resulted in responsible selection test publishers having to change available separate norms by gender and ethnicity and merge groups into one overall norm. It became (for all intents and purposes) illegal to consider ethnicity or gender during employee selection, thus making the most accurate prediction equations also illegal. In 1991, the IACP’s Psychological Services Section formally adopted Inwald’s revised “Fitnessfor-Duty Evaluation Guidelines” at its Annual Section Meeting. In a conference program titled “Fitness for Duty: Standards and Practices for the ’90s,” which included presentations by Catherine Flanagan (New York), Stephen Curran (Maryland), and James Janik (Illinois), Robin Inwald formally presented these guidelines to the IACP membership during the 98th Annual IACP Conference in Minneapolis, Minnesota. Rather than alter items and scales of the IPI so that it could continue to be used as a “preconditional offer” test under the new ADA guidelines, Inwald wrote a series of new personality assessment instruments that were normed and validated on public safety officer applicants and did not contain items that would reveal psychopathology or past substance abuse (included in the ADA definition of “medical” diagnoses). In 1992, the Inwald Survey 5 (IS5), which included scales measuring areas of integrity and anger management, among others, was published by Hilson Research (Inwald & Gebbia, 1992). Inwald developed the IS5 in an effort to give police psychologists critical behavioral information at the “pre-conditional offer” phase of screening because the administration of the IPI or MMPI now was illegal under ADA until the end of the screening process. Police agencies began to use this test as part of their “pre-conditional-offer” screening programs and many police psychologists also added it to their “post-conditional-offer” and “fitness-for-duty” batteries. In 1993, the FBI sponsored a conference titled “Law Enforcement Families: Issues & Answers” at the FBI Academy in Quantico, Virginia. Robin Inwald conducted a research study for this conference and developed the Hilson Spouse/Mate Inventory, the first assessment instrument for evaluating relationship issues specific to police personnel and their spouses or partners (Inwald, 1993). In 1995, the FBI sponsored a conference titled “Organizational Issues in Law Enforcement” at the FBI Academy in Quantico. At this conference, Inwald presented summary research on those police/ public safety officers who had been tested during the 1970s or 1980s and later became involved in inappropriate violent behavior as officers (Inwald, 1995). As a direct result of discussions at this FBI conference, the Inwald Survey 2 (IS2) was published by Hilson Research the following year (Inwald et al., 1996). This 110-item inventory, along with its shorter version, the Inwald Survey 8 (IS8), was the first personality inventory developed for public safety officers and applicants that focused specifically on the identification of characteristics associated with violent behavior. Because many police psychologists and public safety administrators, who conducted or organized pre-employment screening programs, often complained about the length of the personality ­inventories being administered, Inwald developed the “Hilson Life Adjustment Profile (HLAP),” a 110-item inventory for identifying psychopathology as well as assessing a police candidate or officer’s actual functioning in personal, social, and family life. Also published in 1996, this inventory was offered as a possible replacement for the much lengthier MMPI, and correlations between these two instruments were included in the HLAP Technical Manual (Inwald et al., 1996). In 1997, Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc., published the “PAI Law Enforcement, Corrections, and Public Safety Selection Report” to be used with Morey’s 1991 Personality Assessment Inventory. Like the IPI and the other Hilson Research test reports, this report relied on law enforcement norms for evaluating public safety applicants. The first comprehensive psychological job analysis, focusing on personality variables found in published psychological tests and documenting their relative importance to subject matter experts such as police chiefs and administrators, was conducted by Michael Cuttler and Robin Inwald in the mid-1990s. In this study, more than 1,500 police administrators, representing every public 26

Introduction and History

safety agency in the state of North Carolina, identified the personality variables they believed most important for police officer performance on the Hilson Job Analysis Questionnaire (HJAQ). Using this and other studies, the HJAQ was published in 1998 and was the first computerized job analysis assessment tool used by administrators to evaluate the relative importance of different personality variables in their agencies (Inwald, 1998). In the fall of 1998, as the result of increasing concerns about domestic violence in police families, the FBI sponsored a “Domestic Violence by Police Officers” conference at Quantico. After the week long presentations and discussions on this topic concluded, Robin Inwald developed a scale for identifying those individuals most likely to become involved in domestic violence. This scale was added to the updated and renormed Inwald Survey 5-Revised (IS5R) in 1999. Additional IPI studies from independent researchers also were published in the 1990s (Mufson et al., 1994), including one linking the IPI and MMPI with the “Big Five” Personality Factors (Cortina et al., 1992). During this time, several researchers continued to examine the validity of the revised MMPI-2. Examples from this period include articles by Brewster (1996); Hargrave, Hiatt, and Gaffney (1986); and Kornfeld (1995). However, research efforts in the field soon were hampered by the fact that police departments, unlike in earlier years, began to take their psychologist’s ratings very seriously. During the 1980s, police administrators continued to hire many officers despite their questionable suitability based on psychological tests. This allowed for validation studies to be conducted where there were a larger number of “failures.” This situation changed during the 1990s, when police psychologists had gained respect in their departments and when the testing came at the end of the screening process (because of the new requirements of the ADA). Now agencies rarely hired officers with “questionable” psychological results. Although beneficial for society, the rejection of nearly all poorly rated candidates restricted the range of officers who could be followed in predictive research studies, limiting a researcher’s ability to directly compare the validities of different tests. In 1999, Deniz Ones began a project for the California Police Officer Selection and Training (POST) organization in its efforts to update psychological-screening guidelines. Over 19,000 validity coefficients from all personality-based tests available at the time were presented at numerous professional conferences. This firmly established the validity of personality tests, such as the MMPI, CPI, and IPI, among others (Ones, 2003).

Negligent Hiring and “Tort” Liability Tort liability associated with lawsuits against police departments also includes wrongful death, assault and battery, false arrest, and false imprisonment (Kitaeff, 2006b). Gross negligence may be assumed by a department’s failure to conduct a full background investigation on a prospective police applicant. Police departments can also be charged with negligent retention (keeping employees on the job when there is significant evidence that they should have been disciplined, demoted, or even dismissed) and negligent entrustment (inadequately preparing officers prior to entrusting them with law enforcement powers and responsibilities) (Kitaeff, 2006b).

Negligent Hiring and Retention, and Duty to Adequately Train and Supervise In Hild v. Bruner, a federal district court in New Jersey upheld a jury finding of municipal liability for excessive force during an arrest (1980). Although the city required its officers to carry guns both on and off duty, the city had no consistent psychological testing in place to help ensure the officers’ fitness to carry weapons. Based on the city’s failure to implement an effective psychological testing program to identify officers unfit to carry guns, the court held the city liable for the injuries its 27

Jack Kitaeff

officer inflicted. In Hardy v. Town of Hayneville, a city was held responsible for the negligent hiring of an officer deemed unfit by later review of his background (1999). In a similar case, Geidel v. City of Bradenton Beach, the court held that the city could be liable for any intentional tort by their police officer if it could be shown that the municipality had been negligent in its duty to select, train, and supervise the officer (1999). In Bonsignore v. City of New York, the failure to adopt a meaningful psychological testing procedure resulted in the award of $300,000 in compensation and $125,000 in punitive damages when an off-duty officer, known generally to have emotional problems, wounded his wife and killed himself (1982). In another case, the court concluded that periodic psychological screening is constitutional, provided that management provides for the privacy of officers’ files and records and respects their right of due process (1998). In Miller v. City of Springfield (1998), a New Jersey court upheld periodic psychological testing of all police officers. Two officers and the union challenged a 12-year-old policy requiring officers of all ranks to be tested every three years. One of the officers was later directed to participate in counseling for anger management. The lawsuit, filed in state court, claimed: 1. Officers were forced to reveal personal and private information which was unrelated to the work performance and their fitness for duty as police officers. 2. The process violates officers’ rights of due process. 3. The screening causes “humiliation, embarrassment, emotional distress, anguish and harm to [officers’] personal reputations.” The judge dismissed the suit. While personality assessment in law enforcement settings came of age in the 1970s and 1980s, reflecting changes in both society and the professional role of psychology, the 1990s were a decade of continued expansion. In 1990, a survey of 72 major law enforcement agencies was conducted in order to determine the tests used for applicant selection. Results of this survey revealed that 51% did not use any psychological tests, and the three most widely used tests were the MMPI, CPI, and IPI (Blau, T. 1994; Strawbridge & Strawbridge, 1990). But then again, the MMPI was (and is) the most commonly used psychological test in the world.

Police Stress, Psychological Dysfunction, and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Police officers regularly deal with the most violent, impulsive, and predatory members of society. They put their lives on the line and confront miseries and horrors the rest of us can’t even imagine. In addition, officers are always in the spotlight and open to complaints and criticism from citizens, the media, and the judiciary. This may skew the officer’s opinions on the character of the average human being and create a cynicism, isolation, and difficulty trusting people in general. Although the stress of viewing many horrific events, violence, and trauma can be substantial in police work, it is exacerbated by its intermittent nature interspersed with periods of boredom and calm. Officers cannot usually control entrance into most traumatic and dangerous situations that they face; they have to “react” to problems and typically do so without sufficient warning or preparation. Officers are also required to always be in emotional control and must show extreme restraint even under highly emotionally charged circumstances. They must wear a social mask that they put on with the uniform. A former president of the International Association of Chiefs of Police has said: What our society expects from police officers is a perfect blend of robot and person. In any confrontation, no matter the potential for violence, we’re not supposed to display emotion or the human characteristics that result from adrenalin. . . . People realize officers are 28

Introduction and History

human, but they don’t understand that we can’t train them to a point where we override every human emotion. (Miller, 1998) One of the most poignant descriptions of the ways police officers are different from the rest of us has been provided by police psychologists Daniel A. Goldfarb and Gary S. Aumiller in their groundbreaking work The Heavy Badge. In it, they express: Law enforcement officers are seen as authority figures. People deal with them differently and treat them differently, even when they are not working. . . . [They are] are isolated. The wearing of a badge, uniform and gun makes a law officer separate from society. Law enforcement officers work in a quasi-military, structured institution. These are [often associated with mental health concerns such as sacrificing] the individual for the good of society. The “rotating shift” schedule is very taxing on an officer’s life. [Their] bodies are adjusted on what is called “circadian schedules” which is a repetitive daily cycle. . . . An officer doing shift work never gets a chance to stay on a schedule. This upsets his physical and mental balance in life. . . . Officers have a different kind of stress in their jobs, called “burst stress.” Burst stress means there is not always a steady stressor, but at times, there is an immediate “burst” from low stress to a high stress state. In other words, officers go from complete calm, to high activity and pressure in one “burst.” The normal stress situation for most of the rest of the work force consists of a stress building process that can be either reduced or adapted to before it gets “out of control.” This is not the case for the officer, because “out of control” can happen in seconds. . . . [Officers] need to be in constant emotional control. [They] have a job that requires extreme restraint under highly emotional circumstances. They are told when they are extremely excited, they have to act calm. They are told when they are nervous, they have to be in charge. They are taught to be stoic when emotional. They are to interact with the world in a role. The emotional constraint of the role takes tremendous mental energy, much more energy than expressing true emotions. When the energy drain is very strong, it may make the officer more prone to exhaustion outside of work, such as not wanting to participate in social or family life. This energy drain can also create a sense of job and social burnout. The “at work” world of the officer is very negative. He sees the bad part of society—the criminal, the abuser of the rules. This may skew the officer’s opinions on the character of the average human being. It creates a cynicism, a critical view of the world. It is hard to adjust to trusting a fellow human being when so much of the day is spent with people who are not trustworthy. It is hard to believe in positive intentions of people, when the day is spent with people who are intending to hurt each other. This lack of trust can show up in the way the officer deals with people on a personal level, with neighbors, with a spouse. It can even show up in the way children are raised, as police parents may tend to be stricter in discipline and more careful with privilege. According to Goldfarb and Aumiller, these “differences” between police officers and other members of the public can lead to increased alcohol use, attitudinal problems, behavioral problems, intimacy and relationship problems, overall increased stress, and even suicide. What is stress? In a very broad sense, stress is a reaction to both internal and external stimuli. Often the external stimuli causing stress are referred to as stressors and include events such as pressure from superiors, excessive traffic, and gunfights. In general terms, stress is the nonspecific “response of the 29

Jack Kitaeff

body to any demand” (Selye, 1979b, p. 3). Stress can assume other meanings. For instance, stress can be a perceived imbalance between what is required of the officer and what he is capable of giving, under conditions where failure may have dire consequences. Implicit in these definitions is the notion that stress is a fundamental part of being alive. That is, any demand placed on an individual to change can produce a physical response to some degree or another, with or without the individual’s knowledge or consent. Thus, just being alive creates a demand on the body. When an individual perceives danger, there is an outpouring of hormones that create alterations in bodily processes and can produce, among other things, an increased heart rate, sweating, trembling, and fatigue (Monat & Lazarus, 1977). In addition, the mere perception of fear can produce fearful consequences in the body. Especially among police officers, finding ways of coping with stress is in both their individual best interest and in the interest of public safety as well. Job stress for police officers or regular civilians can be defined as the harmful physical and emotional responses that occur when the requirements of the job do not match the capabilities, resources, needs, or expectations of the worker (Stevens, 1999c). For police officers, in addition to the daily grind, officers are frequently the target of criticism and complaints by citizens, the media, the judicial system, opportunistic politicians, hostile attorneys, “do-gooder” clinicians and social service personnel, and their own administrators and law enforcement agencies (Blau. T. 1994). Dr. Larry Miller, in his comprehensive and poignant book Shocks to the System, describes stress specifically as it relates to the police officer when he states, “Police officers regularly deal with the most violent, impulsive, and predatory members of society, put their lives on the line, and confront miseries and horrors that the rest of us view from the sanitized distance of our newspapers and TV screens” (1998, p. 216). Miller stresses: Police officers generally carry out their duties and responsibilities with dedication and valor, but that some stresses are too much to bear, and every officer has his breaking point. For some, it may come in the form of a particular dramatic event, such as a gruesome accident or homicide, the killing or wounding of a partner, the mistaken shooting of an innocent civilian, or an especially grisly accident or crime scene. . . . For other officers, there may be no single major trauma, but the identified mental breakdown occurs under the cumulative weight of a number of more moderate stresses over the course of the officer’s career. In either case, all too often the officer feels that the department doesn’t support him and that there is nowhere else to vent his distress. So he bottles up his feelings, acts snappish with coworkers, superiors, civilians, and family members, and becomes hypersensitive to small annoyances on and off the job. (p. 217) The result of prolonged stress may be posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). The DSM-IV-TR (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) defines PTSD as follows: The essential feature of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder is the development of characteristic symptoms following exposure to an extreme traumatic stressor involving direct personal experience of an event that involves actual or threatened death or serious injury, or other threat to one’s physical integrity; or witnessing an event that involves death, injury or a threat to the physical integrity of another person; or learning about unexpected or violent death, serious harm, or threat of death or injury experienced by a family member or other close associate. . . . The person’s response to the event must involve intense fear, helplessness, or horror. . . . The characteristic symptoms include persistent reexperiencing . . . persistent avoidance of stimuli associated with the trauma . . . persistent symptoms of increased 30

Introduction and History

arousal . . . the disturbance must cause clinically significant distress or impairment in social, occupational, or other important areas of functioning. (pp. 463–464) Miller (1998) points out that if no treatment is provided for PTSD, symptoms may persist for many months or even longer, and anger, hostility, irritability, problems with authority, fatigue, inability to concentrate, loss of self-confidence, and increased use of food or mood-altering substances may also be the result. Many of these long-term effects can interfere with a police officer’s job functioning for months, if not years. In some cases, delayed or prolonged stress reactions manifest themselves in the form of psychosomatic physical complaints, or in a tendency to “snap” at minor provocations, irritations, or events that would have not hitherto caused any such reactions in the officer. All this underscores the importance for police officers to receive treatment as soon as possible after traumatic events occur, and certainly as soon as possible after the appearance of symptoms of PTSD.

Psychological Debriefing Police work is stressful and unpredictable. Officers must have the emotional resources to perform multiple tasks without losing control in the face of physical threats. They need to exhibit dominance and assertiveness, and at the same time restraint and empathy. They must be able to complete their tasks despite provocation, ambiguity, and the ever-present threat of psychological or physical injury (Shusman et al., 1987). Although most people do not seek crises in their lives, police officers respond to and immerse themselves daily into the chaos and confusion of other people’s lives, and by doing so they put themselves at risk of becoming victims of traumatic incidents. Yet, they do so willingly and without hesitation. One only needs to stand back and watch officers responding to a call of a “man with a gun” to appreciate their coping abilities. Because officers comfort trauma victims and operate in the wake of traumatic events, it should be expected that they will be exposed to the problematic and undesired effects of stress. The National Organization for Victim Assistance (NOVA) identifies nine categories of trauma workers. Law enforcement personnel and firefighters are listed as immediate responders and trauma workers (NOVA, 1991, as cited in Harris, 1995). In law enforcement, stressful or traumatic incidents are often referred to as critical incidents. A critical incident is any situation faced by an officer that causes him to experience unusually strong emotional or physical reactions. These reactions may have the potential to interfere with the officer’s abilities to function either at the scene or later in life. The reactions are a normal person’s response to an abnormal event, although because of officers’ training, belief, and experience, they may believe the opposite to be true. A critical incident can also be a time when an officer’s expectations about his ability to handle stressful situations are called into question (Mitchell, 1990; Ryan & Brewster, 1994). The officer’s reaction to this event may also interfere with his family life (Hartsough, 1995; Sheehan, 1995). It is important to keep the definition of a critical incident flexible because of the varying effects an incident has on different officers (FBI Bulletin, 1996). The following events are typical of those that may cause unusual distress for emergency personnel (Mitchell & Bray, 1990): • • • •

Death of a fellow officer. Serious injury to a fellow officer. Serious multiple-casualty incident/accident. Suicide of a fellow officer. 31

Jack Kitaeff

• • • • • •

Traumatic deaths involving children. Events that attract “excessive” media interest and public scrutiny. An event involving victims known to the officer. Exposure to infectious diseases. Being the subject of a lawsuit. Any event that has an unusually powerful impact on the officer.

How a person reacts to these events, the meaning he or she attributes to his performance, and the circumstances surrounding the incident can cause a psychological crisis. It is important to remember that a critical incident should not be defined in terms of the event but rather in terms of the impact it has on the individual. An event that is less-than-critical (subcritical) can still have an impact on an officer’s performance and functioning. Psychotraumatologist Pierre Janet wrote that it is how a person thinks about and reacts to a traumatic event that ultimately determines how quickly the person recovers from the experience (Everly, 1995a). Psychological crises violate or contradict the beliefs a person has about the world. A crisis may shatter a person’s assumptions regarding the world as a safe and orderly place. It may also challenge how a person evaluates his competency and gives rise to self-doubt (Everly, 1995a). When a person is victimized, three basic assumptions or beliefs about the self and the world are challenged: the belief in personal invulnerability, the view of oneself in a positive light, and the belief in a meaningful and orderly world. When a person faces a loss, as in the loss of the feeling of invulnerability, there must be some adjustment in order to go on living. The interval between the recognition of the loss and the subsequent adjustment can be difficult with symptoms such as intrusive ideas and numbing of emotions (Horowitz & Kaltreider, 1995). Frankl (1959) felt that the failure to find meaning and a sense of responsibility in one’s life lies at the root of psychopathology. Trauma challenges previously held assumptions, beliefs, and understandings about the world and oneself in the world (Everly, 1995b). In a critical incident, how an officer responds in one moment might serve to define the entire event (FBI Bulletin, 1996). That meaning comes from the socially, and sometimes personally, constructed belief an officer has about the “correct” way to respond. It is the meaning that an officer attributes to an event that determines his or her behavior and reactions after the event. George Everly (1994a) provided another perspective when he stated, “Practically speaking, there is simply no such thing as reality without considering the human perspective” (p. 178). The term critical incident has been defined in a number of ways since it was first used in the early 1990s (Best, Artwohl, & Kirschman, Chapter 27, this volume). As will be seen later in this volume, rather than insisting on a universally agreed upon meaning, law enforcement experts have actually stressed the importance of maintaining a flexible definition because of the wide variation in officer responses to duty-related events (Bohl, 1995; FBI Bulletin, 1996). Therefore, although definitions vary, it is generally understood that line-of-duty events, which for officers are “outside the range of normal activity” (Patton & Violanti, 1996, p. 183) or involve serious threat or loss (Gentz, 1990), are considered critical incidents. Some experts maintain that it is the individual officer’s reactions that make an incident critical. This mirrors the American Psychiatric Association’s criteria of an event eliciting an intense emotional reaction in the exposed individual in order for it to be considered “traumatic” in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSMIV-TR, 2000). Police departments are well advised to educate their personnel when a stressful, high-profile event has occurred involving its officers. As Best et al. in this volume point out, lack of knowledge about the details of the event can fuel rumors and does nothing to allay the anxiety of the personnel who were not directly impacted but may still have concerns. Rumors that begin to circulate may also be harmful to the involved personnel when inaccuracies and speculations about their performance come to their attention (Artwohl & Christensen, 1977). Neither agency nor tactical/operational 32

Introduction and History

debriefings in first responder settings should take the form of a psychological intervention. Rather, such debriefings are forums in which information is shared in order to assist agency personnel in developing a more coherent narrative of the incident from which they can learn and better support affected officers.

When Providers Become Patients Police officers, as will be seen later in this volume, may be in need of critical incident stress debriefing, yet they also serve as providers of such debriefing for others in their role as first responders. There are a variety of models and strategies that can be used to provide mental health support and services to officers. Substantial research supports the indication of two primary targets for on-scene interventions: physiological arousal and emotional distress (Ozer et al., 2003). Miller (1998) describes how in order to accomplish dealing with physiological arousal and emotional distress, many agencies have organizationally formalized critical incident stress debriefing (CISD) teams to deal with the therapeutic work for traumatized officers (Miller, 1998). Such teams have been implemented throughout the United States, Britain, and other parts of the world as well. “Such teams have often been organizationally subsumed under the broader category of critical incident stress management (CISM), which includes a range of crisis intervention strategies in addition to CISD” (Miller, 1998, p. 228).

Formal Models of Police Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Miller (1998) describe CISD intervention as designed to promote the emotional processing of traumatic events through the ventilation and normalization of reactions as well as preparations for possible future experiences. Although CISD is designed for use in groups, other debriefing-type models have been used with individuals, couples, and families. Most CISD takes place two to three days after a suspected trauma-inducing event and is structured as a single group meeting lasting approximately 2 hours. A CISD is run by a psychologist or other trained mental health professional and a member of a peer support team (Mitchell & Everly, 1996). There are a variety of CISD formats (Benner & Quinn, 1993; Blak, 1990; Bohl, 1995; Mitchell & Everly, 1995b). In general, these formats involve taking an officer through a number of stages designed to alleviate symptoms of stress and educate the officer about normal reactions to abnormal events. A detailed discussion of various forms of psychological debriefings is provided below. The San Jose Police Department, for example, reported that between 1972 and 1987, 52 officers were involved in shootings and 17 of those officers subsequently left the department. SJPD did not have a CISD team in place during this time period. Since the inception of their CISD team, 122 officers have been involved in shootings, and none of the involved officers have left the department (Benner, 1994). Officers are by nature suspicious of psychology professionals, who are often seen by them as “the enemy” (Benner, 1982). These professionals are first encountered by officers when they apply for a law enforcement job. Subsequent contacts are at the request of the police department for a “fitnessfor-duty” evaluation. Officers are concerned that mental health professionals who work for a police department will align themselves with the administrators who provide them with a job (Benner, 1982). Further, because the psychologists work for the police department, the holder of privilege is the department administration and not the officers. When officers are sent to a department psychologist, they do not know what can be discussed in confidence. The issue of confidentiality is murky at best and varies from agency to agency (Super & Blau, 1997). Relationships between an officer and a psychologist are also strained when a psychologist is not familiar with police culture (Benner, 1982). 33

Jack Kitaeff

Peer counseling can be described as a process whereby officers can talk about their feelings to another officer. The idea behind peer counseling is that an officer who has experienced a line-ofduty traumatic event can empathize with and validate another officer’s reaction as being normal. Officers tend to trust other officers who have experienced a similar incident. One of the most common types of CISD models in use today consists of several phases (Miller, 1998; Mitchell & Everly, 1996). In the Introduction, the team leader—either a mental health professional or peer debriefer—introduces the CISD process and encourages all members to participate. In the Fact Phase, each member is asked to describe his or her role during the incident; it is basically a process of “What did you do?” The Thought Phase is where the leader asks the group members to discuss what went through their minds as the incident unfolded. This is followed by the Reaction Phase, during which members move from providing cognitive descriptions to relaying their emotional reactions during the event and sharing in so many words, “What was the worst part of the incident for you?” It is during this phase that group leaders (especially if they are trained mental health professionals) look for adverse emotional reactions on the part of participants. In the Symptom Phase, participants are asked to describe cognitive, physical, emotional, and behavioral signs and symptoms of distress that appeared at the scene and at various points after the scene. In essence, members are asked, “What have you been experiencing since the incident?” The Education Phase provides participants with factual information about stress and adverse emotional reactions. This serves to normalize the stress and coping responses and provides a basis for questions and answers. The final stage is the Reentry Phase, which provides a wrap-up of sorts during which participants can ask any final questions, and general group bonding is reinforced. In essence, members are asked, “Is there anything positive that can come out of this experience that can help you grow personally or professionally?” Or “How can you help one another in the future?” It should be noted that to encourage participation and reduce fear of stigmatization, the administrators of any given police department or law enforcement agency should make clear that these kinds of debriefings are 100% confidential. The only exception to this general rule is where there is a clear and present danger to self or others that is revealed during the process. To reduce the stigmatization of individual members, it is sometimes beneficial to have a policy in place that makes postdebriefing participation automatically followed by referral to a mental health professional (Everly & Mitchell, 1997; Reese, Horn, & Dunning, 1990; McMains, 1990). One of the most extensive and comprehensive adaptations of the CISD process for police officers has been developed by Bohl (1995), who explicitly compares and contrasts the steps or phases in her program with the phases of the Mitchell CISD program. In the Bohl model, there is little distinction between the cognitive and emotional phases of the debriefing. Officers are free to express emotional reactions during the cognitive phase, and the other way around. What is important in the Bohl model is not the mere act of venting, but rather the opportunity to validate feelings. Under the Bohl model, the leader does not ask whether anything positive has arisen from the incident. Expecting officers to find a “growth experience” after seeing their partner shot and killed, for example, is seen as ridiculous. Last, under the Bohl model of CISD, is the final round robin in which participants are invited to say anything on their minds, whatever it may be. Remarks can be addressed to anyone, but others cannot respond directly; this is to provide a feeling of safety to participants (Miller, 1998). Although peer counseling services and CISDs in general have been shown to be a very effective way to help officers, getting officers to accept the help has been difficult. Police officers have traditionally avoided seeking therapy or help. Officers tend to believe that other people can’t really understand their problems and that “real officers” shouldn’t have any problems (Linden & Klein, 1988). The issue of requiring an officer or making it optional for an officer to attend a CISD varies from department to department (Super & Blau, 1997). A formalized departmental understanding of 34

Introduction and History

the nature of stress and a departmental order requiring officers to attend a debriefing are preferred to waiting until the officer is calling for help (McMains, 1990). Fay (2000) reports a study by Beijen (1995a), who attempted to determine to whom a veteran police officer is most likely to turn for help. His results showed that 80% of the responding officers would seek help from a friend and fellow officer, but only 35% would seek help from a peer counselor. The majority of officers would, if necessary, seek out a friend for an informal debriefing. Informal discussions of traumatic events among emergency personnel have been going on for many years (Kaufmann & Beehr, 1989). Friends listening to, supporting, and encouraging each other are an important part of reducing stress for officers (Mitchell, 1996). However, the police culture doesn’t easily accept the open expression of feelings and emotions. Further complicating the situation is the internal, organizationally generated stress from the semimilitaristic environment in police departments. Policies, rules, and political alignments within an organization make it important for an officer to choose his confidants wisely (Beijen, 1995b). Finally, unfortunately, despite the best efforts of many dedicated police officers and police psychologists, police officers continue to commit suicide at a rate twice that of the general population (Beijen, 1995b; Gibbs, 1994; McCafferty, McCafferty, & McCafferty, 1992). Psychological services have been developed to try to meet the specific needs of police officers, but officers are still wary of utilizing professional psychological services. Police job-related stress and trauma continue to impact officers, their families, and taxpayers. It is clear that officers need to talk about job stress and trauma, and the people they prefer to talk with are their self-chosen colleagues and friends (Beijen, 1995a). For this reason, further ways of addressing the impact of job stress need to be developed.

Police Officers as First Responders As mentioned previously, the history of police psychology reveals that police officers are not only the recipients of critical incident stress debriefing but also the providers of such debriefing. Certain traumas do much more than injure us physically and even psychologically. They violate our very sense of safety, stability, and what theorists refer to as ontological security. Ontological security is a stable mental state derived from a sense of continuity in regard to the events in one’s life (Giddens, 1991). It is a sense of order and an absence of chaos and anxiety that give meaning to a person’s life. Even being the victim of a violent crime is not consistent with the meaning of an individual’s life, as this tends to threaten that individual’s sense of ontological security. More than traumas such as hurricanes, train accidents, and plane crashes, violence intentionally caused by other people and aimed directly at us robs us of our sense that the world can ever be a safe place again. Victim services are helpful in this regard, are available in all 50 states, and are particularly useful for sexual assault victims, domestic violence victims, and children. One such victim service entity is the National Organization for Victim Assistance (NOVA), which has developed a generic model of victim services that contains three major components: (1) emergency response at the time of the crisis, (2) victim stabilization in the days following the trauma, and (3) resource mobilization in the aftermath of the crime (Young, 1991). Miller (1998) and Clark and Friedman (1992) also offer some very commonsense suggestions for first responders who must deal with the victims of crime. The responder’s first concern is to see that serious injuries get treated. It is vitally important that a first responder (police officer, firefighter, crisis worker, medic, or mental health professional) introduce him- or herself to the victim as well as bystanders. Regardless of any uniform that the first responder may be wearing, it may be necessary to repeat the introduction several times. The victim should be heard by the first responder in an understanding, sympathetic, and nonjudgmental fashion. There is no need to press for details regarding the crime as there will sufficient time for this later by local or federal authorities. Instilling a sense of safety, comfort, and trust is much more important at this point. 35

Jack Kitaeff

The importance of trauma counseling and psychological intervention cannot be overstated. Kilpatrick et al. (1985) reported that 19% of rape victims attempt suicide, 44% report suicide ideation, and 16% say that they had “a nervous breakdown” following the rape. But the degree of susceptibility to developing actual stress-related disorders such as PTSD varies from person to person and seems to depend on psychological, social, demographic, and environmental factors. Demographic factors include ethnic background, religious beliefs, socioeconomic status, gender, age, and, most importantly, the presence of supportive relationships. Psychological factors include coping mechanisms, self-confidence and self-esteem, emotional stability, and resiliency. Environmental factors include the degree of violence involved in the incident and the location of the crime or attack. For example, victims who are attacked in an environment they had formerly perceived as being “safe” tend to experience more negative reactions than those attacked in “unsafe” locations (Markesteyn, 1992). The psychological reactions to violent crime can include minor sleep disturbances, irritability and worry, depression, anxiety disorders, alcohol and drug abuse, suicidal thoughts (and attempts), and PTSD. PTSD symptoms include intense fear; feeling of helplessness, or horror; agitation; persistent reexperiencing of the traumatic event in the form of flashbacks; increased arousal; avoidance of stimuli associated with the trauma; emotional numbing; and clinically significant distress or impairment in social, occupational, or other important areas of functioning (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Most of the research on PTSD indicates that treatment is most effective when begun soon after the traumatic event, and should consist of some combination of pharmacotherapy and cognitive behavioral therapy. But it is important for PTSD survivors to know that recovery is still possible even if treatment is not received immediately (Shalev, 2007). Only in the last 20 years has terrorism become a significant fact of life for Americans. Accordingly, the body of clinical psychological literature on terrorism has lagged behind that of other types of traumatic events. Unfortunately, these events have been increasing in frequency, and many experts maintain that the worst is yet to come. Terrorist attacks, such as those on Oklahoma City and the World Trade Center, combine features of a criminal assault, a disaster, and an act of war (Hills, 2002). Accordingly, many of the treatment approaches combine what we know from treating victims of criminal assault, grief, natural and manmade disasters, riots, war, workplace violence, and school shootings (Miller, 2001a, 2001b, 2002c). A study cited in Miller (2002a) of the devastating 2001 World Trade Center attack found that 11% of all New Yorkers showed symptoms of PTSD two months following the incident, which is almost 3 times the national average. An additional finding was that the degree of PTSD distress was most strongly related to the amount of television coverage watched. This suggests that potentially vulnerable victims may have attempted to use information gathering via television as a coping mechanism but instead ended up retraumatizing themselves. Following a terror attack, many survivors experience a type of “pan phobia” accompanied by a heightened sense of vulnerability and an avoidance of anything related to the trauma. Survivors may have frequent nightmares of the imagined horrifying death of a victim they have known, or wishfulfillment fantasies of rescuing the victim. Their grief may be compounded by guilt if they feel they should have foreseen the attack or done more to keep their loved ones safe (Sprang & McNeil, 1995). The first responder on the scene of a terrorist attack may be a police officer, emergency medical technician, firefighter, or mental health crisis counselor. Miller (2002b) recommends that such people should obtain as much information as possible from the victim about the terrorist crime itself and begin a plan for aiding other potential victims. Collaborative work with other first responders is essential to ensure that investigators obtain valuable data while victims receive optimal care. First responders should avoid empty statements such as, “Everything will be all right,” and instead offer more concrete and realistic information such as, “We’re going to take you to a safe hospital.” A victim’s wishes should be accommodated as much as reasonably possible, if, for example, the victim 36

Introduction and History

wants a family member or friend to remain during treatment or questioning. Victims should be allowed to talk and express their emotions even if they may seem somewhat digressive or rambling. First responders should provide basic, understandable education about the onset and course of possible posttraumatic symptoms and attempt to normalize the traumatic stress experience while discouraging a sense of severe stress-related disability to come. Outreach programs in the community should be identified around which psychological care can be organized. Identifying high-risk groups is one of the most important aspects of any disaster consultation (Ursano, Fullerton, & Norwood, 2003). If possible, help to identify the remains of loved ones. Although this may shatter any hope that the person may still be alive, the actual sight of the deceased often provides a strange sort of reassuring confirmation that the victim’s death agonies may have fallen short of the survivor’s imagined horrors, and even if not, that the physical presence of the body at least means that the victim’s suffering is finally over. Outcome studies of mourners of a death from natural causes report shorter periods of denial and higher total recall of the deceased in those who were able to view the body prior to burial (Sprang & McNeil, 1995). When no definitive remains are found, symbolic remains may serve as a surrogate. For example, an urn of ashes from Ground Zero was offered by the City of New York to each family of a missing person.

Individual Psychotherapy Although it may not always be a comfortable experience, psychotherapy for PTSD often involves facing the memories and images of trauma head on. There is a time in the treatment of the PTSD patient, for example, where the psychologist must take the patient back to the original traumatic event and have him or her discuss it in step-by-step detail. The goal, according to Miller, is to “counteract maladaptive avoidance tendencies and to diminish the chance that they will congeal into longstanding patterns of behavior” (p. 33). Miller cautions, however, that sometimes it is necessary to work through the patient’s other “peripheral issues” before the traumatic event can be adequately explored (Everstine & Everstine, 1993). Therapy should increase adaptive defense mechanisms and allow a patient to reenter normal life and reassume normal social roles with the understanding that problems along the way are not signs of regression but merely necessary bumps in the road to recovery (Miller, 1998). For victims of violent crimes who decide to press charges and testify in court, the recovery process from PTSD can seem even longer than with noncriminal traumatic events. This is because of the time it takes for the criminal justice system to run its course, cross-examinations by defense counsel, and so on. Studies have shown, for example, that the prevalence of PTSD is higher among victims who wade through the criminal justice system than among crime victims in general (Freedy, Resnick, Kilpatrick, Dansky, & Tidwell, 1994). But if the decision has been made to proceed through the criminal justice system, it may be somewhat therapeutic for victims of violent crimes to file a request for restitution as part of the sentence as well as a victim impact statement. The assistance of a mental health professional during these times to help prevent a replay of the original traumatizing effects is highly recommended. Sprang and McNeil (1995) have presented a phased treatment model originally designed for survivors of murder victims that can productively be applied to the treatment of survivors of terrorist homicide. An initial evaluation and debriefing phase occurs immediately following the traumatic event and focuses on crisis intervention and stabilization of the individual’s emotional, social, and physical environment. At this stage, the individual’s defenses should not be challenged. Instead, the intervention should include empathic support, validation, and normalization of the patient’s reaction to the traumatic loss. Survivors should be prepared for the emotional, financial, practical, and social losses 37

Jack Kitaeff

that follow the terrorist killing of a family member. Therapists should encourage a graded and dosed ventilation of emotion and provide necessary support. Then, therapists should gradually begin to educate family members as to what they can expect and try to dispel unrealistic expectations. Other aspects of this educative process include providing concrete information about such victim resources as the Red Cross, National Organization for Victim Assistance, Crime Victims Compensation Fund, employee assistance programs, and so on. At each step, the therapist should monitor patients’ reactions to avoid overwhelming them with too much information too quickly (Sprang & McNeil, 1995). When trust and therapeutic rapport have developed, relaxation training, biofeedback, desensitization, and cognitive behavioral techniques can be applied to symptom management. Opportunities should be provided, arranged, or planned for patients to take back some control of their lives, for example by helping and educating others or running support groups. The patient should be helped to reduce self-blame through the use of cognitive or existential therapeutic approaches. Psychological mastery over the traumatic bereavement can be encouraged by asking patients to describe the future: “If you were not struggling with your grief anymore, what would you be doing?” A related process involves helping the patient say a psychological goodbye to the slain loved one, realizing that there will always be painful memories but that the survivors have a right to continue their own lives (Sprang & McNeil, 1995). A far more productive therapeutic approach involves validating the survivors’ pain while supporting their strengths and helping them to live as normal a life as possible, albeit a life that will be radically different from the one they led before (Spungen, 1998). For such patients, Spungen (1998) recommends they keep a daily diary or journal and write down their thoughts and feelings about the murder and about their deceased loved one. This notebook should be portable enough to carry around so patients can jot down their thoughts as they occur. Another suggestion is to tape-record thoughts into a portable recorder; these can later be transcribed, if desired. Even if the survivor never reads the diary again, the act of writing itself can be therapeutic; clinicians will recognize this as the technique of journaling or narrative therapy. Spungen has found that some covictims may create several volumes of such notes before they realize they have made progress. The only caveat is that this exercise should not become a prolonged obsessive preoccupation to the exclusion of other therapeutic strategies and participation in life generally. By simply asking what the victim saw, heard, felt, touched, or tasted, the clinician opens additional channels of information and facilitates additional narrative working-through. Hanscom (2001) described a treatment model that emerged from her work with survivors of torture and that may be applied to victims of terrorism, especially incidents involving abduction, hostage taking, and abuse. In this model, an essential condition of healing of torture and trauma survivors is the reestablishment of the experience of trust, safety, and the ability to have an effect on the world. This relearning relies less on particular therapeutic techniques and procedures than on the compassionate human interaction and therapeutic alliance between the survivor and a counselor who is willing and able to listen effectively. A HEARTS model to deal with the aftermath of terror has been suggested by Miller (2002c), Hanscom (2001), and others. HEARTS is an acronym for: H = Listening to the history. This includes providing a gentle environment, listening with body language, attending the flow of speech, hearing the voice and tone of the speaker, observing the speaker’s movements and reactions, looking at facial expressions, remaining quietly patient, and listening compassionately. Clinicians will recognize this as a basic description of active listening. E = Focusing on emotions and reactions. This involves using reflective listening, asking gentle questions, and naming the emotions. 38

Introduction and History

A = Asking about symptoms. This involves using one’s personal and therapeutic style to investigate current physical symptoms, current psychological symptoms, and suicidal tendencies. R = Explaining the reason for symptoms. This includes showing how the symptoms fit together, describing how the body reacts to stress and trauma, explaining the interaction between the body and mind, and emphasizing that these are normal symptoms that normal people have to a very abnormal event. T = Teaching relaxation and coping skills. This involves instructing the patient in relaxation skills, such as abdominal breathing, meditation, prayer, imagery, visualization, and others; and discussing coping strategies (e.g., recognizing how they have coped in the past, reinforcing old and healthy strategies, and teaching new coping skills). S = Helping with self-change. This involves discussing the person’s worldview—the original view and any changes, adaptations, or similarities—and recognizing the positive changes in the self.

Family Therapy Whether it is a single family member who is hurt or killed in a terrorist attack, or a mass terrorist casualty incident where hundreds of families are killed, injured, or displaced, family members can act as both exacerbating and mitigating factors to one another in their efforts to cope with trauma. Accordingly, a key therapeutic task often involves turning vicious cycles of recrimination and despair into positive cycles of support and hope. Family therapists have long recognized that the effects of successive traumas are often cumulative (Alarcon, 1999; Catherall, 1995). Accordingly, therapy for terrorist bereavement may have to deal with unresolved traumatic material from the past, which will almost certainly be re-evoked by the more recent trauma of the murder. In addition, other aspects of life cannot automatically be put on hold when the death occurs, so therapy must address coexisting issues such as school and job problems, marital conflict, substance abuse, or other preexisting family stresses. Therapists should inquire about individual family members’ private perceptions of death. Nihilism and despair are common early responses, and helping patients and families to recover or develop sustaining spiritual or philosophical beliefs or actions can buffer the destabilizing and disintegratory effects of the murder. Therapeutic measures may involve exploring the family members’ concepts of life and death, as well as encouraging both private meditative and socially committed activities, such as support groups or political or religious antiterrorism activities (Rynearson, 1996). Many Oklahoma City and World Trade Center survivors started or joined various charitable or social service foundations as a way of memorializing their slain loved ones. Pictures and other mementos of the deceased family member can serve as comforting images for survivors. In looking at family picture albums together, therapists and survivors can bring up positive imagery that may counterbalance the grotesque recollections of the terror attack. Similar memorializing activities include writing about the deceased or creating a scrapbook, but these activities should never be part of an unhealthy obsessive preoccupation (Spungen, 1998).

Community Responses By definition, mass-casualty terrorist disasters are community events, and there is much that community leaders can do to offer support and increase therapeutic and social morale. Commendations and awards to professional first responders, volunteer rescue workers, service providers, and others who have distinguished themselves are important components of the community recovery process. Memorials to the victims of the terrorist disaster are part of the healing process and should be encouraged. Leaders are powerful symbols. Local and regional leaders should be encouraged to set an example of expressing their own grief in a healthy and mature way, to lead 39

Jack Kitaeff

the community in recognizing the appropriateness of constructive mourning (Ursano, Fullerton, & Norwood, 1995).

Emergency Response Unit and Hostage Negotiations: Munich One of the most notable and far-reaching initiatives taken by Harvey Schlossberg was his part in developing the nation’s first true Hostage Negotiation Team in 1972. An eye-opening hostage event took place in the summer of 1972. Simon Eisdorfer, Assistant Chief Inspector at the New York Police Department, watched as a hostage drama unfolded on television. What became known as the “Munich massacre” occurred at the 1972 Summer Olympics in Munich, Germany, when members of the Israeli Olympic team were taken hostage by the Palestinian terrorist organization Black September—a group within Yasser Arafat’s Fatah organization. The attack, miserable and failed attempts at negotiations, and a botched rescue attempt eventually led to the deaths of 11 Israeli athletes and one German police officer. According to news sources, the Israeli athletes had enjoyed a night out on September 4, 1972, watching a performance of Fiddler on the Roof before returning to the Olympic Village. At 4:30 a.m. on September 5, as the athletes slept, eight terrorists clad in tracksuits and carrying guns and grenades in duffel bags scaled a chain-link fence with the help of unsuspecting U.S. athletes who, too, were sneaking into the Olympic Village compound. The Palestinians then used stolen keys to enter two apartments used by the Israeli team. Israeli wrestling referee Yossef Gutfreund heard a faint scratching noise at the door of the first apartment. When he investigated, he saw the door begin to open and masked men with guns on the other side. He shouted, “Hevretistalku!” (Guys, get out of here!) and threw his nearly 300-pound weight against the door to try to stop the Palestinians from forcing their way in. The wrestling coach Moshe Weinberg, age 33, attacked the kidnappers as the hostages were being moved from one apartment to another, allowing one of his wrestlers, Gad Tsobari, to escape. The burly Weinberg knocked one of the intruders unconscious and stabbed another with a fruit knife before being shot to death. Weightlifter Yossef Romano, 31 and the father of three, also attacked and wounded one of the intruders before being killed. The kidnappers were left with nine living hostages: wrestling referee Yossef Gutfreund, age 40; American-born weightlifter David Berger, 28; wrestler Mark Slavin, 18; weightlifting judge Yacov Springer, 51; weightlifter Ze’ev Friedman, 28; track coach Amitzur Shapira, 40; wrestler Eliezer Halfin, 24; shooting coach Kehat Shorr, 53; and fencing coach Andre Spitzer, 27. The terrorists demanded the release and safe passage to Egypt of 234 Palestinians and non-Arabs jailed in Israel. The German authorities, under the leadership of Chancellor Willy Brandt and Minister for the Interior Hans-Dietrich Genscher, rejected Israel’s offer to send an Israeli special forces unit to Germany. Instead, a small squad of German police was dispatched to the Olympic Village. Dressed in Olympic sweatsuits and carrying machine guns, these were members of the German border police, untrained in any sort of counterterrorist response, and without specific tactics in place for the rescue. The police took up positions and awaited orders that never came. In the meantime, camera crews filmed the police actions from German apartments and broadcast the images live on television. With televisions on, the terrorists were able to watch the police as they prepared to attack. Footage shows the terrorists leaning over to look at the police who were in hiding on the roof. In the end, the police simply left. At one point during the crisis, the negotiators demanded direct contact with the hostages in order to satisfy themselves that the Israelis were still alive. Fencing coach Andre Spitzer, who spoke fluent German, and shooting coach Kehat Shorr, the senior member of the Israeli delegation, had a brief conversation with Schreiber and Genscher while standing at the second-floor window of the besieged building, with two kidnappers holding guns on them. When the kidnappers became impatient with 40

Introduction and History

Spitzer’s prolonged answers to the negotiators’ questions, the coach was pistol-whipped in full view of international television cameras and pulled away from the window. The kidnappers demanded transportation to Cairo. The German authorities feigned agreement and at 10:10 p.m., two helicopters transported both the kidnappers and their hostages to nearby Fürstenfeldbruck airbase, where a Boeing 727 aircraft was waiting. The kidnappers believed they were on their way to Riem, the international airport near Munich, but the authorities planned an assault on the kidnappers at the airport. Five German snipers, none of whom had any special training, were chosen to shoot the kidnappers. All had been chosen simply because they “shot competitively on weekends” (Kitaeff, 2006a, p. 87). No tanks or armored personnel carriers were at the scene. A Boeing 727 jet was positioned on the tarmac with five or six armed German police inside, who volunteered to do the job, dressed as the flight crew. The plan was for them to overpower the terrorists with the pretense of inspecting the plane, and give the German snipers a chance to kill the terrorists remaining at the helicopters. But they were ordinary police officers who had not been trained for such a mission. At the last minute, as the helicopters were arriving on the tarmac, the German police aboard the airplane voted on and then abandoned their mission, without contact to or from any central command. The helicopters landed just after 10:30 p.m., and the four pilots and six of the kidnappers emerged. While four of the Black September members held the pilots at gunpoint, Issa and Tony walked over to inspect the jet, only to find it empty. Knowing they had been duped, they jogged hastily back toward the helicopters, and at approximately 11:00 p.m., the German authorities gave the order to the police snipers positioned nearby to open fire. There was instant chaos. The four German members of the chopper crews began sprinting for safety in all directions. In the ensuing frenzy, two kidnappers standing near the pilots were killed, and a third was mortally wounded as he fled the scene. The three remaining exposed kidnappers scrambled to safety, and began to return fire and shoot out as many airport lights as they could from behind the helicopters, out of the snipers’ line of sight. A German police officer in the control tower, Anton Fliegerbauer, was killed by the gunfire. The helicopter pilots fled, but the hostages, who were tied up inside the craft, could not. A stalemate developed. During the gun battle, wrote Groussard, the hostages secretly worked on loosening their bonds. Teeth marks, evidence of the hostages’ determination, were found on some of the ropes after the gunfire had ended. The five German snipers did not have radio contact with each other and were unable to coordinate their fire. None of the snipers were equipped with steel helmets or bulletproof vests, proving an egregious lack of preparation. None of the rifles were equipped with telescopic sights or night vision scopes. Later, it was discovered that one of the snipers never fired a shot because he was positioned directly in the line of friendly fire, without any protective gear. Later in the battle, when kidnapper Khalid Jawad attempted to escape on foot, this sniper shot and killed the fleeing kidnapper, and was in turn wounded by one of his fellow police officers, who was unaware that he was shooting at one of his own men. At 4 minutes past midnight, by now into September 6, one of the kidnappers jumped out of one of the helicopters. He turned and sprayed the helicopter and hostages with gunfire, killing Springer, Halfin, and Friedman, and wounding Berger in the leg. The kidnapper then pulled the pin on a grenade and tossed it back into the cockpit, where it detonated. While the first helicopter was burning, writes Cooley, the surviving kidnappers kept fire trucks at bay by shooting at them. What happened to the remaining hostages is still a matter of dispute. However, it is likely that a third kidnapper stood at the door of the helicopter and riddled the remaining five hostages— Gutfreund, Shorr, Slavin, Spitzer, and Shapira—with fatal gunfire. Jim McKay, who was covering the Olympics that year for ABC, had taken on the job of reporting the events as Roone Arledge fed them into his earpiece. After the botched rescue attempt, he came on the air with this statement: “Our worst fears have been realized tonight. They’ve now said that there were 11 hostages; 2 41

Jack Kitaeff

were killed in their rooms yesterday morning, 9 were killed at the airport tonight. They’re all gone” (Kitaeff, 2006a, p. 89). But almost beyond comprehension, the massacre of 11 Israeli athletes was not considered sufficiently serious to merit canceling or postponing the Olympics. A little over a month later, on October 29, a Lufthansa jet was hijacked by terrorists demanding that the Munich killers be released. The Germans capitulated and the terrorists were let go, but an Israeli assassination squad tracked down the terrorists along with those responsible for planning the massacre. Eight of the 11 men targeted for death were killed. Of the remaining three, one died of natural causes and the other two were assassinated, but it is not known for sure if they were killed by Israeli agents.

The Aftereffects of Munich: The Birth of the ESU Many counterterrorism experts have theorized that the Munich massacre was one of the most significant terror attacks of recent times, one that set the tone for decades of conflict in the Middle East and launched a new era of international terrorism. As the commanding officer of the NYPD’s Special Operations division, Eisdorfer realized that such an event could actually happen in New York City. He also knew that the police department was not prepared to deal with it. Accordingly, he developed the operational plans for the nation’s first Hostage Negotiation Team. The team became reality in the spring of 1973, months after a highprofile standoff in January in which armed robbers seized a dozen hostages at a Brooklyn sporting goods store and one police officer was killed. Eisdorfer knew that by putting fresh cops into a hostage situation, he could wear down hostage takers. He realized that negotiators could subtly turn a siege into a waiting game that played out in their favor. Police officers could change shifts but the suspects could not, and the latter would become tired and hungry and more likely to surrender. His emphasis was saving lives, not ending things quickly as had unfortunately, and tragically, been done in the past. As both a psychologist and a police detective, Harvey Schlossberg provided most of the psychological consultation needed in all aspects of the new team. What resulted was a 48-hour siege involving 11 hostages and four armed men. “In a way,” Schlossberg recalled, “the Brooklyn siege was like a final exam for our hostage negotiations training” (H. Schlossberg, personal communication, February 1, 2006). The gunmen had barricaded themselves with the hostages inside the store, where they were pinned down by police fire. One of the first things Schlossberg did when arriving on the scene was to calm things down. He ordered the cops to stop shooting and wait it out. “I didn’t want to storm the place; I wanted to talk to them,” he recalls (H. Schlossberg, personal communication, February 1, 2006). The gunmen had recently sent out a note stating that one of their comrades was injured. They requested medical aid and food. This made Schlossberg believe that these men were more concerned with staying alive than with suicide. He sent in a police radio and initiated a dialogue. In the end, the hostages escaped by a clever trick: They had persuaded their captors to let them go to the rear of the store in anticipation of a shoot-out. Instead, they broke a plasterboard wall and exited to the roof through a hidden stairway. In his book, Psychologist With a Gun, Schlossberg expressed that by basically “doing nothing” and waiting out the situation, the gunmen’s alertness relaxed and they fell for the hostages’ ruse. Eisdorfer and Schlossberg’s techniques worked and would be studied and emulated by police departments all over the world (Kitaeff, 2006b). A special hostage negotiation team is now a permanent fixture of the police department and is incorporated within the Emergency Service Unit (ESU). The ESU is considered very elite. It is said that when someone needs help they call the police, when the police need help they call the ESU. The unit provides specialized equipment, expertise, and 42

Introduction and History

support to the various units within the NYPD. From auto accidents to building collapses to hostage situations, the ESU is called in when the situation requires advanced equipment and expertise. Also included within the ESU is what is commonly referred to in other agencies as the Special Weapons and Tactics team (SWAT). For possible hostage situation, on-scene psychologists and trained police negotiators profile hostage takers; determine their motivation, vulnerabilities, and dangerousness; suggest dialogue strategies or psychological tactics that would defuse the situation; and spend equal time analyzing which hostages might engage in behaviors that diminish or enhance their chances for survival. A trained hostage negotiator will be able to advise police on whether a hostage taker might be mentally ill (about 50% of them are) and what the extent of their life crisis might be. This is particularly important because some hostage takers may be out of touch with reality or even attempting to commit “suicide by cop.” In contrast to Eisdorfer’s time, crisis/hostage teams now usually consist of at least five people. These include the primary negotiator (who does most of the active negotiations), a secondary negotiator (who monitors negotiations and makes suggestions), an intelligence officer (who seeks and organizes incoming information), a psychologist (who serves as a consultant or advisor), and a tactical liaison (who maintains communications with command). The first 15 to 45 minutes of a hostage situation are the most dangerous. This is because the hostage takers are still going through a panic reaction. This is when most hostages get injured or killed, either because they tried to be a hero, made some remark or suggestion, stood out in some symbolic way, or were just picked at random to make a point. Unless the hostage takers are under the influence of some chemical stimulant, they are likely to calm down after a while, appear to be exhausted, and tell everyone to get some rest. Hostages may be traded for food, drink, and/or toilet facilities, and these released hostages will be interviewed for what they observed (i.e., numbers of hostage takers, weapons present, routine, and chain of command). Sleep may occur, and this is not uncommon, especially when there are a small number of perpetrators or a solo suspect who has handled all the details alone. Authorities use surveillance devices to tell when everyone falls asleep and have at times surprised everyone by ending a hostage situation during this time (Kitaeff, 2006b).

The International Association of Chiefs of Police, Psychological Services Section and Relevant Police Organizations The International Association of Chiefs of Police (IACP) Psychological Services Section has progressed from 1984 to the present time (see Fischler, 2001, 2010; IACP, 2006, 2009). During August 1984, a gathering of police psychologists attending a conference at the FBI Academy discussed how police psychologists could serve IACP membership and agreed to work toward forming an IACP police psychological services committee. This group met again at the October 1984 IACP Annual Conference in Salt Lake City as an ad hoc committee. The ad hoc committee developed an outline of interests and responsibilities and made a formal presentation to IACP Executive Board members, who voted to establish the Police Psychological Services Committee on October 25, 1984. Initial objectives of the committee were to: 1. Provide an immediate and knowledgeable information source on police psychology and related mental health fields to law enforcement in general, and to the IACP in particular. 2. Advance police psychological services in general. 3. Identify police psychology resources, including the development of a directory of law enforcement mental health professionals. 4. Make training available to IACP membership concerning police psychology and related fields. 5. Provide information and training to mental health professionals so they might develop and upgrade skills in the law enforcement mental health area. 43

Jack Kitaeff

The new Police Psychological Services Committee first met on October 13, 1985, at the IACP Annual Conference in Houston, Texas, to further develop initial goals. It immediately began to plan how to advance these goals through presentations at IACP annual conferences and publications in Police Chief magazine. The committee consented that its primary role was to serve IACP member needs. Committee members met again on December 17, 1985, at the FBI Academy to finalize workshops for the 1986 Annual Conference. The committee was elevated to full section status in 1986 at the Annual Conference in Nashville. Since that time, the Psychological Services Section has grown to over 80 police psychologists. The section continues to adhere to the original objectives of the Psychological Services Committee by contributing to Police Chief magazine, presenting training programs at the annual conferences, and scheduling in-service training for police psychologists at each annual conference. The section has also been instrumental in the development of standards relating to police psychology and has developed standards for pre-employment, duty-related shootings and other traumatic incidents, fitness for duty, and use of peer support. Membership in the section is open to licensed psychologists who are members of IACP and who work as psychologists with public law enforcement agencies. The Psychological Services Section has developed, among other things, guidelines for issues relating to: 1. 2. 3. 4.

Peer support. Officer-involved shootings. Pre-employment psychological evaluations. Fitness-for-duty evaluations.

Society of Police and Criminal Psychology The Society for Police and Criminal Psychology (SPCP) is an eclectic professional organization that encourages the scientific study of police and criminal psychology and the application of scientific knowledge to problems in criminal justice. It focuses on law enforcement, judicial, and corrections systems. Members of the SPCP study the full range of human behaviors, motivations, and actions within the framework of the criminal justice system. Consequently, the SPCP encourages input from psychologists, social workers, psychiatrists, lawyers, police officers, corrections personnel, and other professionals concerned with the criminal justice system (SPCP, 2010). The SPCP sponsors an annual conference held during the fall at varying locations. The conference focuses on the interface between criminal justice and the behavioral sciences. It includes presentations on international perspectives in policing, specialized police procedures and techniques, personnel decision-making issues in criminal justice agencies, the law and criminal justice, litigation issues, inmate populations, issues in probation and parole, and other issues affecting those in the criminal justice system. The SPCP invites individuals from all over the world, with expertise from every relevant discipline, to present their work on police and criminal psychology, including topics relating to mental health and the criminal justice system. Proposals are welcomed regarding research, theory, and applications that may be of interest to the members, who are encouraged to share their expertise by making a presentation or conducting a workshop at the conference. The SPCP welcomes presentations from psychologists, psychiatrists, lawyers, police officers, corrections personnel, social workers, and professionals involved in the study of the criminal justice system. The SPCP also publishes the Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology through Springer Publications. The journal is published twice per year and focuses on issues relevant to practitioners and academicians in the field of criminal justice. The journal has a worldwide subscription base, and a subscription is included with membership in the SPCP. 44

Introduction and History

The “Diplomate” designator is designed to recognize the special expertise and contributions made by psychologists who work with law enforcement, as either clinicians or academics. The diploma awarded by the SPCP is the premier board certification in this specialty area of applied psychology. Only psychologists are eligible for the Diplomate certification in police psychology. Police psychology encompasses many specialized services provided to law enforcement agencies by psychologists, such as pre-employment psychological testing, specialty assignment and fitness-forduty evaluations, organization development, training, hostage negotiation team consultation, and critical incident stress debriefing. In order to assess these areas of knowledge, written and oral examinations in person are required of all candidates for the Diplomate. These examinations are designed and administered by currently active Diplomates in police psychology. Attainment of this high and rare distinction signifies that the holder has demonstrated to a board of peers his or her expertise in the history, theory, principles, practices, techniques, and ethics of police psychology. Those who qualify are recognized by their peers as capable and accomplished in police psychology. Diplomate status is a level above graduate training and state licensure requirements. As a minimum, police psychologists hold a doctoral degree in professional psychology and have appropriate credentials to practice psychology within the jurisdictions of their respective states, typically requiring years of supervised experience and postgraduate training. SPCP provides an advanced venue in which police psychologists continue their postgraduate educational experiences. In its more than 30 years as a professional association, SPCP has awarded the Diplomate to only 60 people (the author of this chapter [and editor of this book] is one of these individuals) (SPCP, 2010).

Division 18 of the American Psychological Association: Psychologists in Public Service According to the APA, the Division of Psychologists in Public Service (Division 18) was established in 1946 as a founding division of APA. It was created in response to the needs of the public in such areas as psychological practice, research, training, program development, and outcome evaluation. Among its goals, Division 18 works to protect and advance the profession, foster ethical practice, advocate for persons with mental illness, and promote quality care. Public service psychologists are practitioners, researchers, university professors, legislators, program developers, clinical coordinators, managers, administrators, and more. Their clients include consumers of mental health services, managers, administrators, policy makers, elected officials, and the public. They work in a variety of settings, including state hospitals, community mental health systems, VA medical centers, criminal justice systems, police and public safety settings, state legislatures, and academic institutions. In general, the services they provide are as varied as the persons they serve and the places they work. Members of Division 18 help train more than half the clinical and counseling psychologists in the United States by providing the internship sites and administering the internship programs. Its members develop and implement mental health treatment programs for millions of persons in inpatient and outpatient settings, as well as community support systems. Through the work of its members, Division 18 has the potential to directly or indirectly touch the lives of most people living in the United States.

Police and Public Safety Section of Division 18 Members of this section work with law enforcement, fire departments, nuclear regulatory agencies, emergency medical services, and other public safety entities. They are involved in the selection of employees, fitness-for-duty evaluations, mental health programs, criminal investigative analysis 45

Jack Kitaeff

(profiling), and hostage negotiations. They participate in the development of training, research, and implementation of effective mental health programs, including critical incident stress debriefing.

Criminal Justice Section of Division 18 Members of this section work primarily with incarcerated people and with administrators who operate state or federal correctional facilities and detention centers. They provide professional support to one another through an exchange of information concerning the administration, assessment, treatment, and ethical and training issues involved in this challenging line of work.

Division 41 of the American Psychological Association (The American Psychology-Law Association) According to the APA, Division 41—the American Psychology-Law Society—promotes the contributions of psychology to the understanding of law and legal institutions, the education of psychologists in legal matters and law personnel in psychological matters, and the application of psychology in the legal system. The Division holds a biennial two-and-one-half-day spring meeting that includes paper and plenary sessions. Members receive the bimonthly journal Law and Human Behavior and the American Psychology-Law Society Newsletter three times per year.

The American Society of Criminology The American Society of Criminology (ASC) is an international organization whose members pursue scholarly, scientific, and professional knowledge concerning the measurement, etiology, consequences, prevention, control, and treatment of crime and delinquency. The ASC’s objectives are to encourage the exchange, in a multidisciplinary setting, of those engaged in research, teaching, and practice so as to foster criminological scholarship, and to serve as a forum for the dissemination of criminological knowledge. Members include students, practitioners, and academicians from the many fields of criminal justice and criminology. The ASC conducts an annual meeting devoted to discussions of topics of general interest. The ASC also sponsors an employment exchange at the annual meetings and maintains an active professional employment and position-listing service on the Web. Members receive the journals Criminology and Criminology & Public Policy, and a newsletter, The Criminologist. The ASC has specialized divisions such as Corrections and Sentencing, Critical Criminology, Women and Crime, International Criminology, and People of Color and Crime, which also distribute newsletters, journals, and announcements on a regular basis.

Certification Now aware of recent court cases and the growing potential for publicity and lawsuits in this area, practicing police psychologists have worried about either negligent hiring cases (when officers were hired who initially had “passed” the psychological evaluations and then did something wrong) or charges of discrimination (when candidates were not hired and believed the psychological assessments were unfair). Robin Inwald searched for a credential beyond the Ph.D. and state license to practice psychology that would add credibility in the courtroom for those who conducted police evaluations. In 1986, she became the first full-time practicing police psychologist to be granted Diplomate status by the American Board of Forensic Psychology (ABFP) and the American Psychological Association’s American Board of Professional Psychology (ABPP). But unfortunately ABPP was not the

46

Introduction and History

post-Ph.D. credentialing body that police psychologists so desperately needed. Most police psychologists didn’t even bother pursuing Diplomate status within ABPP as there was simply not a “home” for them there. The closest areas within the organization that had any relevance for police psychologists are Forensic Psychology, Organizational Business and Consulting Psychology, and Clinical Psychology. But none of these covered the material, academic subject matter, or everyday practice activities of the police psychologist. All this changed as of 2011, when the American Board of Professional Psychology began awarding the specialty diploma to police and public safety psychologists. This was due in part to the pioneering and tireless work of police psychologists such as Philip Trompetter, David Corey, Michael Cuttler, and others. As of this writing, Jeni McCutcheon is the National Chair of Examiners and President-Elect of the American Board of Police and Public Safety Psychologists (ABPPSP). She reports that there are currently 70 Board Certified Specialists in Police and Public Safety Psychology. According to the American Board of Professional Psychology, applicants for ABPP candidacy in any of the specialty boards must first meet generic requirements applicable to all ABPP applicants, and only then are applicants subject to additional criteria and examination procedures established by each of the specialty boards. Generic ABPP requirements consist of: 1. A doctoral degree from a program in professional psychology from a graduate program that was accredited by the American Psychological Association (APA) or the Canadian Psychological Association (CPA) at the time the degree was granted, or that offered a curriculum that was the equivalent of APA or CPA requirements. 2. Completion of an appropriate APA- or CPA-accredited internship, or an internship that offered the equivalent of APA or CPA requirements. 3. A minimum of one year of postdoctoral experience, completed through formal postdoctoral training, or a minimum of two years if obtained under supervision other than in a formal training program. [Note: Specialty board requirements are in addition to this minimum requirement.] 4. Licensed at the doctoral level for the independent practice of psychology. Such licensure must be granted by a jurisdiction of the United States, its territories, or Canada. The license must be for independent practice; licensure that is dependent on supervision or is restricted for some reason is not acceptable for admission to candidacy for ABPP board certification. [Individuals who are licensed but have a history of disciplinary action by the governing jurisdiction or of ethical violations (e.g., such as may be determined by the APA) are required to provide details of that history, as well as evidence of acceptable resolution, prior to review of the application.] The ABPP recognizes holders of the Certificate of Professional Qualifications (CPQ) available through the Association of State and Provincial Psychology Boards (ASPPB) as having met the ABPP generic criteria. The ABPP does not accept applications from individuals who are foreigntrained and who practice outside the U.S., its territories, or Canada. To be eligible for board certification, the applicant must satisfy the generic requirements stipulated by the ABPP, as well as the specific requirements of the specialty board (American Board of police and public safety psychology or ABPPSP), described in the next section below. When a psychologist believes he or she meets the generic and ABPPSP eligibility requirements, he or she may apply for candidacy.

Specialty-Specific Requirements Eligibility criteria specific to the specialization in Police &Public Safety Psychology include the following requirements:

47

Jack Kitaeff

A. Specialty Education & Training No fewer than 100 hours of formal education and supervision in police and public safety psychology is required to be eligible for specialty board certification. This criterion can be satisfied by a combination of the following four methods if specifically pertinent to the specialty: graduate coursework or continuing education, formal supervision or peer consultation, peer-reviewed publications, and board certification in another ABPP specialty board or by the Society for Police and Criminal Psychology. CONTINUING OR GRADUATE EDUCATION

The subject matter of graduate education and/or continuing education workshops presented as evidence of full or partial fulfillment of this criterion must be identifiable as directly relevant to the practice of police and public safety psychology (PPSP). A minimum of 50 hours must derive from specialtyspecific continuing education hours, with a minimum of 25 hours that were gathered in the three years preceding application for board certification. These 50 hours must be distinctive to police and public safety psychology (e.g., Hostage Negotiations, Use of the CPI in police and public safety psychology Selection, Sleep Deprivation and its Effects on First Responders, Cognitive Behavioral Therapy with Law Enforcement Personnel) and not simply general continuing education equally applicable to other specialties. An otherwise general workshop may be relevant to PPSP but must be authorized in writing from the NCE (e.g., Threat Assessment and the ADA, Work Fitness Examinations, Issues Surrounding Domestic Violence). Applicants must submit a list of courses taken that meet this criterion. To the extent possible, applicants are required to give a description of the education that includes the title or subject matter, identification of the presenter(s) or faculty, the date and location of the training, and the number of graduate credits or CEU contact hours awarded (e.g., Technology in the Practice of Police Psychology: An Update, Bruce M. Cappo, Ph.D., International Association of Chiefs of Police, Psychological Services Section, Annual Conference. Orlando, Florida, .5 hrs. October 23, 2010). The focus and substantial content of these curriculum-based courses must be directly relevant to police and public safety psychology. Up to 8 hours of independent study (e.g., book-based, examinationbased credits, accredited online courses, DVDs) related to the specialty may be applied if APAapproved. Fifteen hours of credit will be awarded for successful completion of a 3-credit advanced graduate course directly related to police and public safety psychology, up to a maximum of 30 hours. DIRECT SUPERVISION/FORMAL PEER CONSULTATION

Applicants claiming credit for direct supervision and/or formal peer consultation in police and public safety psychology must submit the name of the psychologist(s) providing formal supervision or peer consultation, the specialty-specific qualifications of the psychologist(s), the beginning and ending period of supervision or consultation (months and years), the nature of the supervision or consultation, and the services provided under supervision or reviewed in peer consultation. The supervisor or peer should be a licensed psychologist with at least five years of full-time postdoctoral experience in the practice of police and public safety psychology. A maximum of 30 hours credit can be claimed for direct supervision and/or formal peer consultation. PUBLICATIONS

Ten hours of credit is awarded for each publication of scholarly research in peer-reviewed journals and/or edited texts, up to a maximum of 30 hours of publication credits. In considering these contributions, the following will be taken into account:

48

Introduction and History

a. The subject of the work must be directly relevant to the functional and/or foundational competencies of police and public safety psychology. b. The Applicant must be the primary author or one of two primary authors. Tertiary authors (i.e., third or above) may submit an explanation of their contributions. The ABPPSP may, at its discretion, award credit for 10 or fewer hours. c. Publications in popular, trade, self-published, and/or unedited volumes will not be considered for credit. However, publications in bona fide law enforcement periodicals whose editorial policies include content review by professional subject matter experts in police or public safety psychology may be considered for credit on a case-by-case basis. DISSERTATION

Ten hours of credit is awarded for successful completion of a dissertation submitted in fulfillment of requirements for a doctoral degree in psychology (Ph.D., Ed.D., Psy.D.) from an institution accredited by APA or its equivalent as determined by the ABPP. The subject matter, literature review, theses, and findings must be substantively and specifically related to one or more of the police and public safety psychology domains and activities. BOARD CERTIFICATION

Thirty hours of credit is awarded for current ABPP Board Certified Specialists in another specialty, as well as for current Diplomates in Police Psychology with the Society for Police and Criminal Psychology (SPCP).

B. Specialty Experience Eligibility for specialty board certification in police and public safety psychology requires at least 3,000 hours of direct services or activities in the specialty, accrued over no less than (a) two years of full-time, postdoctoral employment as a psychologist in a police or public safety agency or (b) three years, at least two of which are postdoctoral, if the services were provided outside of full-time employment as a psychologist in a public safety agency (e.g., private practice, part-time agency employment, university employment). If employed full time as a college or university faculty, up to 1,500 of the required hours may be earned through the teaching of courses substantively pertinent to police and public safety psychology. Applicants must submit a description of postdoctoral employment and/or other experience to be considered in fulfillment of this requirement. Note: Full-time employment is defined as no less than 2,000 hours per year. When one or more of these years is performed prior to obtaining independent licensure (i.e., supervised postdoctoral experience), it is expected that at least 1,000 of these hours will be spent in direct service provision in police and public safety psychology and the remainder in supervision. Should additional evidence be requested, this shall consist of letters from colleagues and/or agency administrators familiar with an Applicant’s work, and documents showing contracted services. As indicated earlier in this chapter, the Society of Police and Criminal Psychology (SPCC) offers a Diplomate in Police Psychology for police psychologists who complete an academic review, a rigorous examination, and an oral interview. Although being a “Diplomate in Police Psychology” by the SPCC does not carry the same weight as being “Board Certified in Police and Public Safety Psychology” by the ABPP, it is a very important credential for police psychologists to possess.

49

Jack Kitaeff

Official Recognition of Police Psychology as a Specialty On August 10, 2008, at its annual convention in Boston, APA officially acknowledged “police psychology” as a true psychological specialty.

The Current Status of the Profession of Police Psychology The IACP Police Psychological Services Section, in conjunction with members of the Society for Police and Criminal Psychology and the American Psychological Association (Division 18, Police and Public Safety Section), recently carried out a comprehensive survey of the field of police psychology. The Joint Committee on Police Psychology Competencies identified 57 separate competencies clustered or organized into four distinct domains of practice: assessment-related activities, intervention services, operational support, and organizational/management consultation (Aumiller & Corey, 2007). These core domains of practices include proficiencies such as job analyses; pre-employment psychological evaluations of police candidates; psychological fitness-for-duty evaluations; threat assessments; promotional assessments; psychological autopsies; test development; employee assistance counseling, individual therapy and counseling; group, couple, and family therapy; critical incident intervention; critical incident counseling; substance abuse treatment; wellness programs; and intervention-related consultation (Aumiller & Corey, 2007). These practice domains represent the past in police psychology, the present, and the future. But what is most important is that the American Psychological Association has now recognized police psychology (inclusive of all the listed 57 competencies) as a true psychological specialty. This occurrence, like that for forensic psychology not long ago, has dramatic implications, such as coming graduate programs in police psychology, postgraduate training in police psychology, APA accreditation, and issuance of the ABPP diploma in police and public safety psychology. By establishing these 57 areas of core competencies, gaining APA recognition, and obtaining the specialty designation by the American Board of Professional Psychology, police psychology has truly established itself as a unique and valuable psychological profession with unlimited potential.

References and Further Reading Aamodt, M. G. (2004). Research in law enforcement selection. Boca Raton, FL: Brown Walker. ABAnet. Litigation. Retrieved August 8, 2007 from http://abanet.org/litigation/tips/ Abel, G. G., & Becker, J. V. (1979). The sexual interest card sort. Unpublished manuscript. Abelson, R. P. 1982. The psychological status of the script concept. American Psychologist, 36, 715–712. Abshire, J., & Bornstein, B. H. (2003). Juror sensitivity to the cross-race effect. Law and Human Behavior, 27(5), 471–480. Acton, R. G., & During, S. M. (1992). Preliminary results of aggression management training for aggressive parents. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 7, 410–417. Adelmann, H. (1966). Marcello Malpighi and the evolution of embryology. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. Adorno, T., Frenkel-Brunswik, E., Levinson, D., & Sanford, N. (1950a). The authoritarian personality. New York: Harper. Adorno, T., Frenkel-Brunswik, E., Levinson, D., & Sanford, N. (1950b). The adults: A review of social scientific research. Aggression & Violent Behavior, 12, 300–314. Akhtar, A. (1999). The psychodynamic dimension of terrorism. Psychiatric Annals, 29(6), 350–355. Akiskal, H. S., Kilzieh, N., Maser, J. D., Clayton, P. J., Schettler, P. J., Shea, M. T., et al. (2006). The distinct temperament profiles of bipolar I, bipolar II, and unipolar patients. Journal of Affective Disorders, 92, 19–33. Alarcon, R. D. (1999). The cascade model: An alternative to comorbidity in the pathogenesis of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry, 62, 114–124. Albrecht, S. (1996). Crisis management for corporate self-defense. New York: Amacom. Albrecht, S. (1997). Fear and violence on the job: Prevention solutions for the dangerous workplace. Durham, NC: Carolina Academic Press.

50

Introduction and History Alder, C., & Polk, K. (2001). Child victims of homicide. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Alexander, D. A. (1994a). Police stress: An inside job. In J. T. Reese & R. Solomon (Eds.), Organizational issues (pp. 3–16). Washington, DC: Federal Bureau of Investigation. Alexander, D. A. (1994b). The impact of police work on police officers’ spouses and families. In J. T. Reese & E. Scrivner (Eds.), Law enforcement families (pp. 9–17). Washington, DC: Federal Bureau of Investigation. Alexander, M. A. (1997). Sex offender treatment probed anew. In B. A. Arrigo & S. L. Shipley (Eds.), Forensic psychology (pp. 391–395). Burlington, MA: Elsevier. Alexander, Y., & Brenner, E. H. (Eds.). (2001). Terrorism and law. Ardsley, NY: Transnational. Allen, J. J. B. (2002). The role of psychophysiology in clinical assessment: ERPs in the evaluation of memory. Psychophysiology, 39, 261–280. Allen, J. J. B., & Iacono, W. G. (2001). Assessing the validity of amnesia in dissociative identity disorder: A dilemma for the DSM and the courts. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 7, 311–344. Allen, R. F., & Pilnick, S. (1973). Confronting the shadow organization: How to detect and defeat negative norms. Organizational Dynamics, 1, 2–18. Allen, S. G. (1983). How much does absenteeism cost? Journal of Human Resources, 18, 379–393. Allison, D. B., & Roberts, M. S. (1998). Disordered mother or disordered diagnosis? Munchausen by Proxy Syndrome. Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press. Allison, R. B. (1984). Difficulties diagnosing the multiple personality syndrome. The International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 32(2), 102–117. Alpert, J. (Ed.). (1995). Sexual abuse recalled: Treating trauma in the era of the recovered memory debate. Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson. Altemeyer, R. (1988). Enemies of freedom: Understanding right-wing authoritarianism. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Altemeyer, R. A. (2007). The other authoritarian personality. In J. Crouse & D. Stalker (Eds.), Do right-wing authoritarian beliefs originate from psychological conflict? Psychoanalytic Psychology, 24, 25–4491. American Board of Forensic Psychology (home page). Retrieved February 6, 2008, from www.abpp.org. American Board of Professional Psychology, Specialty Specific Requirements. Retrieved August 15, 2016, from www.abpp.org/i4a/pages/index.cfm?pageid=3612. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychological Association. (1978). Report of the task force on the role of psychology in the criminal justice system. American Psychologist, 33, 1099–1113. American Psychological Association. (2002). Ethical principles of psychologists and code of conduct. Retrieved February 26, 2008, from www.apa.org/ethics/code/index.aspx. American Psychological Association. (n.d.). Elder abuse and neglect: In search of solutions. APA Online. Retrieved March 11, 2008, from www.apa.org/pi/aging/resources/guides/elder-abuse.aspx. Ammerman, R. T., & Hersen, M. (1991). Case studies in family violence. New York: Plenum. Anastasi, A. (1976). Psychological testing (4th ed.). New York: Macmillan. Andersson, L. M., & Pearson, C. M. (1999). Tit for tat? The spiraling effect of incivility in the workplace. Academy of Management Review, 24, 452–471. Andersen, S. M., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1984). On resisting social influence. Cultic Studies Journal, 1(2), 196–219. Anfang, S., & Appelbaum, P. S. (1996). Twenty years after Tarasoff: Reviewing the duty to protect. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 4(2), 67–76. Anson, R. H., & Bloom, M. E. (1988). Police stress in occupational context. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 16, 229–233. Appleman, J. (2007). Successful jury trials. In J. D. Lieberman & B. D. Sales (Eds.), Scientific jury selection. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Arbisi, P. A., & Butcher, J. N. (2004a). Psychometric perspectives on detection of malingering of pain: Use of the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2. Clinical Journal of Pain, 20, 383–391. Archer, J. (1991). The influence of testosterone on human aggression. British Journal of Psychology, 82, 1–28. Arito, H., Sudo, A., & Suzuki, Y. (1981). Aggressive behavior of the rat induced by repeated administration of cadmium. Toxicology Letters, 7, 457–461. Armstrong, K. R., Lund, P. E., McWright, L. T., & Tichenor, V. (1995). Multiple stressor debriefing and the American Red Cross: The East Bay Hills fire experience. Social Work, 40(1), 83–90. Arnson, E. (1995). The social animal. New York: W. H. Freeman. Aronson, E., Wilson, T. D., & Akert, R. M. (2002). Social psychology: The heart and the mind. New York: HarperCollins. Arrigo, B. A., & Shipley, S. L. (2000). Forensic psychology. Burlington, MA: Elsevier.

51

Jack Kitaeff Arrigo, B. A., & Shipley, S. L. (2005). Forensic psychology (2nd ed.). Burlington, MA: Elsevier. Artwohl, A., & Christensen, L. W. (1977). Deadly force encounters: What cops need to know to mentally and physically prepare for and survive a gunfight. Boulder, CO: Paladin Press. Asch, S. E. (1956). Studies of independence and conformity: A minority of one against a unanimous majority. Psychological Monographs, 70(Whole no. 416). Ash, P., Slora, K. B., & Britton, C. F. (1990). Police agency officer selection practices. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 17(4), 258–269. Ashforth, B. E., & Humphrey, R. H. (1993). Emotional labor in service roles: The influence of identity. Academy of Management Review, 18, 88–115. Åström, P. (2007). The study of ancient fingerprints. Journal of Ancient Fingerprints, 1, 2–3. Aumiller, G. S., & Corey, D. (2007). Defining the field of police psychology: Core domains & proficiencies. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 23(1), 23–48. Austin, E. J., & Deary, I. J. (2000). The ‘four As’: A common framework for normal and abnormal personality? Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 977–995. Autopsy shows teen died from “neck compression” (2007, December 12). CTV.ca. Retrieved March 21, 2009, from www.ctv.ca/servlet/ArticleNews/story/CTVNews/20071211/muslim_dad_071212/20071212?hub= TopStories. Avolio, B. J., Bass, B. M., & Jung, D. I. (1995). Multifactor Leadership Questionnaire technical report. Redwood City, CA: Mind Garden. Azen, S. P. (1974). Predictors of resignation and performance of law enforcement officers. American Journal of Community Psychology, 2, 79–86. Azen, S. P., Snibbe, H. M., & Montgomery, H. R. (1973). A longitudinal predictive study of success and performance of law enforcement officers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 57, 190–192. Balch, R. (1977). The police personality: Fact or fiction. In D. B. Kennedy (Ed.), The dysfunctional alliance: Emotion and reason in justice administration (pp. 10–25). Cincinnati, OH: Anderson. Barland, G. H., & Raskin, D. C. (1976). Validity and reliability of polygraph examinations of criminal suspects. In (Contract No. 75-N1–99–0001). Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice, Department of Justice. Bartol, C. R. (1982). Psychological characteristics of small-town police officers. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 10, 58–63. Bartol, C. R. (1991). Predictive validation of the MMPI for small-town police officers who fail. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 22, 127–132. Bartol, C. (1996). Police psychology: Then, now, and beyond. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 23, 70. Bartol, C. (2004). Introduction to forensic psychology. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Bartol, C. R., & Bartol, A. M. (2004). Introduction to forensic psychology. Sage Publication, Thousand Oaks, CA. Beijen, D. J. (1995a). Police seeking counsel. Unpublished manuscript. Beijen, D. J. (1995b). Police suicide: Its reasons, and training to prevent it. Unpublished manuscript. Bell, J. L. (1995). Traumatic event debriefing: Service delivery designs and the role of social work. Social Work, 40(1), 36–43. Ben-Porath, Y. S. (2006). Differentiating normal from abnormal personality with the MMPI. In S. Strack (Ed.), Differentiating normal from abnormal personality (2nd ed., pp. 337–381). New York: Springer. Ben-Porath, Y. S., & Tellegen, A. (2008). The Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2 Restructured Form: Manual for administration, scoring, and interpretation. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. Ben-Shakhar, G. (2008). Effective policing: Understanding how polygraph tests work and are used. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 35, 1295–1308. Ben-Shakhar, G., & Dolev, K. (1996). Psychophysiological detection through the guilty knowledge technique: The effects of mental countermeasures. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 273–281. Benner, A. (1982). Concerns cops have about shrinks. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, San Francisco. Benner, A. (1994). The challenge for police psychology in the twenty first century: Moving beyond efficient to effective. In J. T. Reese & R. Solomon (Eds.), Organizational issues (pp. 363–374). Washington, DC: Federal Bureau of Investigation. Benner, A. (1997). Class lecture on critical incident stress debriefing training. Critical Incident Debriefing Training, San Francisco Police Department. Benner, A., & Quinn, V. (1993). Critical incident stress debriefing manual. Unpublished training manual, San Francisco Police Department. Bernstein, I. H., Schoenfeld, L. S., & Costello, R. M. (1982). Truncated component regression, multicolinearity and the MMPIs use in a police officer selection setting. Multivariate Behavior Research, 17, 99–116. Berry, C. O. (2007). Interpersonal deviance, organizational deviance, and their common correlates: A review and meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 410–424.

52

Introduction and History Beutler, L. E., Storm, A., Kirkish, P., Scogin, F., & Gaines, J. A. (1985). Parameters in the prediction of police officer performance. Professional Psychology Research and Practice, 16, 324–335. Bison, J. I., & Deahl, M. P. (1994). Psychological debriefing and prevention of posttraumatic stress, more research is needed. British Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 717–720. Blak, R. A. (1990). Critical incident debriefing for law enforcement personnel: A model. In J. T. Reese, J. M. Horn, & C. Dunning (Eds.), Critical incidents in policing (pp. 23–30). Washington, DC: Government Printing Office. Blau, P. M. (1994). The organization of academic work (2nd ed.). New York: Transaction. Blau, T. (1994). Psychological services for law enforcement. New York: John Wiley. Blau, T. H., & Super, J. T. (1997). Survey of psychological services general orders in law enforcement. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 12(1), 7–12. Blau, T. H., Super, J. T., & Brady, L. (1993). The MMPI good cop/bad cop profile in identifying dysfunctional law enforcement personnel. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 9, 2–4. Board of Trustees of the Society for Personality Assessment. (2005). The status of the Rorschach in clinical and forensic practice: An official statement by the Board of Trustees of the Society for Personality Assessment. Journal of Personality Assessment, 85(2), 219–237. Boes, J. O., Chandler, C. J., & Timm, H. W. (1997). Police integrity: Use of personality measures to identify corruptionprone officers. Monterey, CA: Defense Personnel Security Research Center. Bohl, N. (1990). The effectiveness of brief psychological interventions in police officers after critical incidents. In J. T. Reese, J. M. Horn, & C. Dunning (Eds.), Critical incidents in policing (pp. 31–38). Washington DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Bohl, N. (1995). Professionally administered critical incident debriefings for police officers. In M. Kurke (Ed.), Police psychology into the 21st century (pp. 169–188). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Bradstreet, R. (1994). Cultural hurdles to healthy police families. In J. T. Reese & E. Scrivner (Eds.), Law enforcement families (pp. 19–26). Washington, DC: Federal Bureau of Investigation. Braverman, M. (1992). Posttrauma crisis intervention in the workplace. In J. Quick & L. Murphy (Eds.), Stress and well being at work (pp. 299–316). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Brewster, J. (1996). Hypervigilance and cynicism in police officers. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 10(4), 7–9. Britt, J. M. (1990). U.S. secret service critical incident peer support team. In J. T. Reese, J. M. Horn, & C. Dunning (Eds.), Critical incidents in policing (pp. 55–62). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Bruner, E. (1986). Ethnography as narrative. In V. Turner & E. Bruner (Eds.), The anthropology of experience (pp. 139–155). Chicago: University of Illinois Press. Bruner, J. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Busch, K. A., & Cavanaugh, J. L. (1986). The study of multiple murder: Preliminary examination of the interface between epistemology and methodology. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 1(1), 5–23. Bush, J. P. (1990). The development of a crisis care unit. In J. T. Reese, J. M. Horn, & C. Dunning (Eds.), Critical incidents in policing (pp. 63–66). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Burgess, A., Douglas, J., & Ressler, R. (1988). Sexual homicide: Patterns and motives. New York: Lexington Books. Butcher, J. N., Graham, J. R., Ben-Porath, Y. S., Tellegen, A., Dahlstom, W. G., & Kaemmer, B. (2001). MMPI-2: Manual for administration, scoring, and interpretation, revised edition. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. Carroll, D. (1988). How accurate is polygraph lie detection? In A. Gale (Ed.), The polygraph test: Lies, truth and science (pp. 19–28). London: Sage. Carter, D., & Radelet, L. (1999). The police and the community (6th ed.). New York: Prentice-Hall. Catherall, D. R. (1995). Preventing institutional secondary traumatic stress disorder. In C. Figley (Ed.), Compassion fatigue: Coping with secondary traumatic stress (pp. 232–247). New York: Brunner Mazel. Cattell, J. M. (1895). Measurements of the accuracy of recollection. Science, 2, 761–766. Cattell, R. B., Eber, H. W., & Tatsuoka, M. M. (1970). Handbook for the 16PF. Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality and Ability Testing. Cattell, R. B., & Jaspers, J. (1967). A general plasmode (No. 30-10-5-2) for factor analytic exercises and research. Multivariate Behavioral Research Monographs, 67(3), 251–259. Cattell, R. B., & Vogelmann, S. (1977). A comprehensive trial of the scree and KG criteria for determining the number of factors. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 12, 289–325. Chantler, L., & Heseltine, K. (2007). Fitness: What is the role of psychometric assessment? Psychiatry, Psychology and Law, 14(2), 350–358. Chibnall, J. T., & Detrick, P. (2003). The NEO PI-R, Inwald Personality Inventory, and MMPI-2 in the prediction of police academy performance: A case of incremental validity. American Journal of Criminal Justice, 27(2), 224–233.

53

Jack Kitaeff Clark, M., & Friedman, D. (1992). Pulling together: Building a community debriefing team. Journal of Psychosocial Nursing, 30(7), 27–32. Clark, M. E. (1994). Interpretive limitations of the MMPI-2 Anger and Cynicism content scales. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 523–527. Cobb, S. (1974). Social support as a moderator of life stress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 38, 300–314. Corey, D. M., & Wolf, G. D. (1992). An integrated approach to reducing stress injuries. In J. Quick & L. Murphy (Eds.), Stress and well being at work (pp. 65–78). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Costello, R. M., Schneider, S. L., & Schoenfeld, L. S. (1996). Validation of a preemployment MMPI index correlated with disciplinary suspension days of police officers. Psychology, Crime and Law, 2, 299–306. Daniels, S., & King, E. (2002). The predictive validity of MMPI-2 content scales for small-town police officer performance. Journal of Police & Criminal Psychology, 17(2), 54. Davis, R. (2003). Relationship between cognitive and background variables and disciplinary problems in law enforcement. Applied H.R.M. Research, 80(2), 77–80. Davis, R. C. (1998). SWAT plots: A practical training manual for tactical units. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Davis, R. D., & Rostow, C. D. (2008). Matrix-Predictive Uniform Law Enforcement Selection Evaluation (MPULSE) inventory: Technical Manual. Toronto, Canada: Multi-Health Systems. Davis, R., Rostow, C. et al. J Police Crim Psych (2003) 18:57. Detrick, P. (2002). Prediction of police officer performance with the Inwald Personality Inventory. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 17(2), 9–17. Detrick, P., & Chibnall, J. T. (2002). Prediction of police officer performance with the inwald personality inventory. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology 17(2). Detrick, P., Chibnall, J. T., & Rosso, M. (2001). Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2 in police officer selection: Normative data and relation to the Inwald Personality Inventory. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 32, 484–490. Dietrich, J., & Smith, J. (1986). The non-medical use of drugs including alcohol among police personnel: A critical literature review. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 14(4), 300–306. Dodrill, C. B. (1981). An economical method for the evaluation of general intelligence in adults. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49, 668–673. Dodrill, C. B. (1983). Long-Term Reliability of the Wonderlic Personnel Test. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, v51 n2 p316–17 Apr 1983. Dodrill, C. B., & Warner, M. H. (1988). Further studies of the Wonderlic Personnel Test as a brief measure of intelligence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 145–147. DuBois, P. H., & Watson, R. I. (1950). The selection of patrolmen. Journal of Applied Psychology, 34, 90–95. Dunne, J. A. (1990). Police counseling unit to deal with critical incidents. In J. T. Reese, J. M. Horn, & C. Dunning (Eds.), Critical incidents in policing (pp. 67–72). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Dunning, C. (1990). Mitigating the impact of work trauma: Administrative issues concerning intervention. In J. T. Reese, J. M. Horn, & C. Dunning (Eds.), Critical incidents in policing (pp. 73–82). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Ellison, K. W., & Genz, J. L. (1983). Stress and the police officer. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas. Endler, N. S., Parker, J. D., & Butcher, J. N. (1993). A factor analytic study of coping styles and the MMPI-2 content scales. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 49, 523–527. Evans, B. J., Coman, G., Stanley, R. O., & Burrows, G. D. (1993). Police officers coping strategies: An Australian police survey. Stress Medicine, 9, 237–246. Everly, G. S. (1994a). Familial psychotraumatology: An analysis of the impact of traumatic stress upon the law enforcement family via destruction of the familial weltanschauung. In J. Y. Reese & E. Scrivner (Eds.), Law enforcement families (pp. 177–184). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Everly, G. S. (1994b). A rapid crisis intervention technique for law enforcement: The SAFE-R model. In J. T. Reese & R. Solomon (Eds.), Organizational issues (pp. 183–191). Washington, DC: Federal Bureau of Investigation. Everly, G. S. (1995a). An integrative two-factor model of post-traumatic stress. In G. S. Everly (Ed.), Psychotraumatology: Key papers and core concepts in post traumatic stress (pp. 27–47). New York: Plenum. Everly, G. S. (1995b). The role of the critical incident stress debriefing process in disaster counseling. Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 17(3), 278–290. Everly, G. S., & Mitchell, J. T. (1997). Critical incident stress management. Ellicott City, MD: Chevron. Everstine, D., & Everstine, L. (1993). The trauma response: Treatment for emotional injury. New York: W.W. Norton. Exner, J. E. (1974). The self focus sentence completion: A study of egocentricity. Journal of Personality Assessment, 37, 437–455.

54

Introduction and History Exner, J. E. (1978). The Rorschach: A comprehensive system: Vol. 2. Current research and advanced interpretation. New York: Wiley. Exner, J. E. (1993). The Rorschach: A comprehensive system: Vol. 1. Basic foundations (3rd ed.). New York: John Wiley. Exner, J. E. (2003). The Rorschach: A comprehensive system (4th ed.). New York: John Wiley. Exner, J. E., & Weiner, I. B. (1994). The Rorschach: A comprehensive system: Vol. 3. Assessment of children and adolescents (2nd ed.). New York: John Wiley. Farr, J. L., & Landy, F. J. (1979). The development and use of supervisory and peer scale for police performance appraisal. In C. D. Spilberger (Ed.), Police selection and evaluation: Issues and techniques (pp. 61–76). Washington, DC: Hemisphere. Fay, J. (2000). A narrative approach to critical and sub-critical incident debriefings. Unpublished dissertation. Retrieved April 14, 2010, from www.narrativeapproaches.com/narrative%20papers%20folder/narrativepolicing2.htm. Federal Bureau of Investigation. (1996). Critical incident stress in law enforcement. Law Enforcement Bulletin, February. Fischler, G. L. (n. d.) Psychological fitness-for-duty examinations: Practical considerations for public safety departments. Retrieved September 29, 2010, from http://edpdlaw.com/FitnessforDuty.pdf. Fischler, G. L. (n. d.) Psychological guidelines for issues in law enforcement. Retrieved August 12, 2010, from www. psycheval.com/issues_in_law_enforcement.shtml. Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1980). An analysis of coping in a middle age community sample. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 21, 219–239. Forbey, J. D., & Ben-Porath, Y. S. (2007). Computerized adaptive personality testing: A review and illustration with the MMPI-2 Computerized Adaptive Version. Psychological Assessment, 19, 14–24. Foreman, C. (1994). Immediate post disaster treatment of trauma. In M. B. Williams & J. F. Sommer (Eds.), Handbook of post traumatic therapy (pp. 268–282). Westport, CT: Greenwood. Frankl, V. (1959). Man’s search for meaning. Boston: Beacon. Freedman, J., & Combs, G. (1996). Narrative therapy: The social construction of reality. New York: W. W. Norton. Freedy, J. R., Resnick, H. S., Kilpatrick, D. G., Dansky, B. S., & Tidwell, R. P. (1994). The psychological adjustment of recent crime victims in the criminal justice system. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 9(4), 450–468. Fuller, R. A. (1990). An overview of the process of peer support team development. In J. T. Reese, J. M. Horn, & C. Dunning (Eds.), Critical incidents in policing (pp. 99–106). Washington DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Fullerton, C. S., McCarroll, J. E., Ursano, R. J., & Wright, K. M. (1992). Psychological responses of rescue workers: Fire fighters and trauma. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 62(3), 371–378. Ganellen, R. J. (1996). Integrating Rorschach and the MMPI-2 in personality assessment. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Gardner, H. (1994). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences (10 anniversary ed.). New York, NY: Basic Books. Garrison, W. E. (1990). Modeling inoculation training for traumatic incident exposure. In J. T. Reese, J. M. Horn, & C. Dunning (Eds.), Critical incidents in policing (pp. 107–117). Washington DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Gentz, D. (1990). The psychological impact of critical incidents on police officers. In J. T. Reese, J. M. Horn, & C. Dunning (Eds.), Critical incidents in policing (pp. 119–122). Washington DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Gergen, K. J. (1985). The social constructionist movement in modern psychology. American Psychologist, 40(3), 266–275. Gersons, B. P. R., & Carlier, I. V. E. (1994). Treatment of work-related trauma in police officers: Post traumatic stress disorder and the post traumatic decline. In M. B. Williams & J. F. Sommer (Eds.), Handbook of post traumatic therapy (pp. 21–33). Westport, CT: Greenwood. Gibbs, N. (1994, September 26). Officers on the edge. Time, 144(13), 62. Goldfarb, D. A., & Aumiller, G. S. The heavy badge. Retrieved August 10, 2010, from www.heavybadge.com. Graff, F. A. (1986). The relationship between social support and occupation stress among police officers. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 14(3), 178–186. Graham, J. R. (1993). MMPI-2 assessing personality and psychopathology (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. Gund, N., & Elliott, B. (1995). Employee assistance programs in police organizations. In M. Kurke (Ed.), Police psychology into the 21st century (pp. 149–167). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Hanscom, K. L. (2001). Treating survivors of war trauma and torture. American Psychologist, 56, 1032–1039. Hargrave, G. (1987). Law enforcement selection with the interview, MMPI, and CPI: A study of reliability and validity. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 15(2), 25–28. Hargrave, G. E., Hiatt, D., & Gaffney, T. W. (1986). A comparison of MMPI and CPI test profiles for traffic officers and deputy sheriffs. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 14, 250–258.

55

Jack Kitaeff Hargrave, G. E., Hiatt, D., & Gaffney, T. W. (1988). F+4 + 9+Cn: An MMPI measure of aggression in law enforcement officers and applicants. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 16(3), 268–273. Harris, C. J. (1995). Sensory based therapy for crisis counselors. In C. Figley (Ed.), Compassion fatigue: Coping with secondary traumatic stress (pp. 101–114). New York: Brunner Mazel. Hart, P. M., Wearing, A. J., & Heady, B. (1994). Police stress and well-being: Integrating personality, coping, and daily work experiences. Journal of Occupational and Organization Psychology, 68(2), 133–157. Hartsough, D. M. (1995). Stresses, spouses and law enforcement: A step beyond. In J. T. Reese, J. M. Horn, & C. Dunning (Eds.), Critical incidents in policing (pp. 131–138). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Hargrave, G. E., & Hiatt, D. (1989). Use of the California Psychological Inventory in law enforcement officer selection. Journal of Personality Assessment, 53(2), 267–277. Havassy, V. (1994). Police stress in the 90’s and its impact on the family. In J. T. Reese & E. Scrivner (Eds.), Law enforcement families (pp. 27–34). Washington, DC: Federal Bureau of Investigation. Hays, G. (1994). Police couples: Breaking the security access code. In J. T. Reese & E. Scrivner (Eds.), Law enforcement families (pp. 337–343). Washington, DC: Federal Bureau of Investigation. Herndon, J. (1998, October). Correlates of MMPI-2 L scale: Elevations in an LEO selection test battery. Paper presented at the 27th annual meeting of the Society of Police and Criminal Psychology, Portland, OR. Hiatt, D., & Hargrave, G. (1988a). Predicting job performance problems with psychological screening. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 16, 122–125. Hiatt, D., & Hargrave, G. (1988b). MMPI profiles of problem peace officers. Journal of Personality Assessment, 52, 722–731. Hogan, R. (1971). Personality characteristics of highly rated policeman. Personnel Psychology, 24, 679–686. Hooper, J. (1996, Fall). Targeting the brain. Time, 47–50. Horn, J. M. (1990). Critical incidents for law enforcement officers. In J. T. Reese, J. M. Horn, & C. Dunning (Eds.), Critical incidents in policing (pp. 143–148). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Horowitz, M. J., & Kaltreider, N. B. (1995). Brief therapy of the stress response syndrome. In G. S. Everly (Ed.), Psychotraumatology: Key papers and core concepts in post traumatic stress (pp. 231–243). New York: Plenum. Iacono, W. G. (2008) Effective Policing: Understanding How Polygraph Tests Work and Are Used. https://doi. org/10.1177/0093854808321529 Iacono, W. G., & Lykken, D. T. (1997). The scientific status of research on polygraph techniques: The case against polygraph tests. In: D. L. Faigman, D. Kaye, M. J. Saks, & J. Sanders (Eds.). Modern scientific evidence: The law and science of expert testimony. St. Paul, MN: West Law. Iacono, W. G., & Patrick, C. J. (1999). Polygraph (“lie detector”) testing: The state of the art. In A. K. Hess & I. B. Weiner (Ed.), The handbook of forensic psychology. New York: John Wiley. Initial Application for ABPP Affiliation of police and public safety psychology as a Specialty Board. Retrieved September 25, 2010, from http://library.constantcontact.com/doc209/1101925593050/doc/hICWPTSiECn MjTtq.pdf. International Association of Chiefs of Police: Officer-involved shooting guidelines. Retrieved September 29, 2010, from http://theiacp.org/psych_services_section/pdfs/Psych-OfficerInvolvedShooting.pdfnver, Colorado, 2009. International Association of Chiefs of Police: Peer support guidelines. Retrieved September 29, 2010, from http:// theiacp.org/psych_services_section/pdfs/Psych-PeerSupportGuidelines.pdf. International Association of Chiefs of Police: Pre-employment psychological evaluation guidelines. Retrieved September 29, 2010, from http://theiacp.org/psych_services_section/pdfs/Psych-PreemploymentPsych Eval.pdf. International Association of Chiefs of Police: Psychological fitness-for-duty evaluation guidelines. Retrieved September 29, 2010, from http://theiacp.org/psych_services_section/pdfs/Psych-FitnessforDuty Evaluation.pdf. Inwald, R. (1984a). Issues & guidelines for mental health professionals conducting pre-employment psychological screening programs in law enforcement agencies. Paper presented at the National Symposium on Police Psychological Services, FBI Academy, Quantico, VA, September 17–21. Inwald, R. (1984b). Law enforcement officer screening: A description of one pre-employment psychological testing program. Submitted to the National Symposium on Police Psychological Services FBI Academy, Quantico, VA. September 17–21. Inwald, R. (1990). Fitness-for-duty evaluation guidelines: A survey for police/public safety administrators and mental health professionals. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Boston. Inwald, R. (1992). Inwald Personality Inventory technical manual (Rev. ed.). Kew Gardens, NY: Hilson Research. Inwald, R. (1993). Hilson Relationships Inventory for public safety personnel and Hilson/Spouse Mate Inventory. New York: Hilson Research. Inwald, R. (1995). Hilson Safety/Security Risk Inventory (HSRI) technical manual. New York: Hilson Research. Inwald, R. (1998). Psychological profiles of police and public safety officers involved with domestic violence. Paper presented at the Domestic Violence by Police Officers Conference, FBI Academy, Quantico, VA, September 17–18.

56

Introduction and History Inwald, R. (2006). Use of a customized Hilson Research test battery to identify stress susceptibility in returning military veterans. Paper presented at the Annual Conference of the International Association of Chiefs of Police, Boston, October 15. Inwald, R. (2007). The Inwald Personality Inventory (IPI) and Hilson Research Inventories: Development and rationale. Journal of Aggression and Violent Behavior, 13, 298–327. Inwald, R., & Brobst, K. E. (1988). Hilson Personnel Profile/Success Quotient (HPP/SQ) technical manual. New York: Hilson Research. Inwald, R., & Brockwell, A. (1988). Success quotient profiles of law enforcement administrators. New York: Hilson Research. Inwald, R., & Gebbia, M. (1997). Hilson Spouse/Mate Inventory and Hilson Relationship Inventory technical manual. Kew Gardens, NJ: Hilson Research. Inwald, R., Gebbia, M., & Resko, J. (1993). Three studies of police/spouse mate relationships using the Hilson Spouse/Mate Inventory. In E. Scrivner (Ed.), Law enforcement families: Issues and answers. Washington DC: U.S. Government Publishing Office. Inwald, R., & Kaufman, J. C. (1989). Hilson Career Satisfaction Index (HCSI) technical manual. New York: Hilson Research. Inwald, R., Kaufman, J., & Roberts, M. (1991). Alcohol use, drug use, and past psychiatric history. Criminal Justice Digest, 10(7), 1–8. Inwald, R., Resko, J. A., & Favuzza, V. (1996a). Hilson Life Adjustment Profile (HLAP) technical manual. New York: Hilson Research. Inwald, R., Resko, J. A., & Favuzza, V. (1996b). Inwald Survey 2 (IS2) & Inwald Survey 8 (IS8) technical manual. New York: Hilson Research. Inwald, R., Traynor, W., & Favuzza, V. (2000). Psychological profiles of police and public safety officers accused of domestic violence. In D. C. Sheehan (Ed.), Domestic violence by police officers (pp. 209–224). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice. Inwald, R. E. (1988). Five year follow up study of departmental terminations as predicted by 16 pre-employment psychological indicators. Journal of Applied Psychology, 4, 703–710. Inwald, R. E. (2004). Personality testing in law enforcement employment settings: A metaanalytic review. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 31, 649–675. Inwald, R. E. (2008). The Inwald Personality Inventory (IPI) and Hilson Research Inventories: Development and rationale. Journal of Aggression and Violent Behavior, 13, 298–327. Inwald, R. E., & Gebbia, M. I. (1992). Inwald Survey 5 (IS5) technical manual. New York: Hilson Research. Inwald, R. E., & Shusman, E. J. (1984). The IPI and MMPI as predictors of academy performance for police recruits. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 12, 1–11. Inwald, R., Resko, J.A., & Favuzza,V. (1996). Hilson Life Adjustment Profile (HLAP) technical manual. New York: Hilson Research. Janik, J. (1990). What value are cognitive defenses in critical incident stress. In J. T. Reese, J. M. Horn, & C. Dunning (Eds.), Critical incidents in policing (pp. 149–158). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Janik, J. (1993, August). Preemployment interviews of law enforcement officer candidates. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Toronto. Janoff-Bulman, R. (1985). The aftermath of victimization: Rebuilding shattered assumptions. In C. R. Figley (Ed.), Trauma and its wake: The study and treatment of post-traumatic stress disorder (pp. 15–35). New York: Brunner/Mazel. Kates, S. (1950). Rorschach responses, strong blank scales, and job satisfaction among policemen. Journal of Applied Psychology, 34(4), 249–254. Kaufmann, G. M., & Beehr, T. A. (1989). Occupational stressors, individual strains, and social support among police officers. Human Relations, 42(2), 185–197. Kilpatrick, D. G., Best, C. L., Veronen, L. J., Amick, A. E., Villeponteaux, L. A., & Ruff, G. A. (1985). Mental health correlates of criminal victimization: A random community survey. Journal of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, 53(6), 866–873. Kilpatrick, D. G., Edmunds, C. N., & Seymour, A. K. (1992). Rape in America: A report to the nation. Arlington, VA, and Charleston, SC: National Victim Center and Medical University of South Carolina. Kilpatrick, D. G., & Resnick, H. S. (1993). PTSD associated with exposure to criminal victimization in clinical and community populations. In J. R. T. Davidson & E. B. Foa (Eds.), PTSD in review: Recent research and future directions (pp. 123–143). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Kilpatrick, D. G., Seymour, A., & Boyle, J. (1991). America speaks out: Citizens’ attitudes about victims’ rights and violence. Arlington, VA: National Victim Center. King, P., Norrell, G., & Erlandson, F. L. (1959). The prediction of academic success in a police administration curriculum. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 19, 649–651.

57

Jack Kitaeff Kirkcaldy, B., Cooper, C. L., & Ruffalo, P. (1995). Work stress and health in a sample of U.S. police. Psychology Reports, 76, 700–702. Kirschman, E., Scrivner, E., Ellison, K., & Marcy, C. (1992). Work and well-being: Lessons from law enforcement. In J. Quick & L. Murphy (Eds.), Stress and well being at work (pp. 178–191). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Kitaeff, J. (2006a). Jews in blue. Youngstown, NY: Cambria Press. Kitaeff, J. (2006b). Selected readings in forensic psychology. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson. Kitaeff, J. (2007). Malingering, lies, and junk science in the courtroom. Youngstown, NY: Cambria Press. Kitaeff, J. (2010). Forensic psychology. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Klein, R. (1990). The utilization of police peer counselors in critical incidents. In J. T. Reese, J. M. Horn, & C. Dunning (Eds.), Critical incidents in policing (pp. 159–168). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Kolbell, R. M. (1995). When relaxation is not enough. In L. Murphy (Ed.), Job stress interventions (pp. 31–43). New York: Plenum. Kornfeld, A. D. (1995). Police officer candidate MMPI-2 performance: Gender, ethnic, and normative factors. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51(4), 536–540. Kuder, G. F. (1985). Kuder occupational interest survey. Chicago: Science Research Associates. Kureczka, A. W. (1996). Critical incident stress in law enforcement. FBI Law Enforcement Magazine, 65, 2–3. Kurke, M. I. (1995). Organizational management of stress and human reliability. In M. I. Kurke (Ed.), Police psychology into the 21st century (pp. 391–415). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Kurke, M. I., & Scrivner, E. M. (1995). Police psychology into the 21st century. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Larson, J. (1969). Lying and its detection: A study of deception and deception tests. Montclair, NJ: Patterson Smith. Lawrence, R. A. (1984). Police stress and personality factors: A conceptual model. Journal of Criminal Justice, 12, 247–263. Linden, J., & Klein, R. (1988). Police peer counseling: An expanded perspective. In J. T. Reese & J. Horn (Eds.), Police psychology: Operation assistance (pp. 241–244). Washington, DC: Federal Bureau of Investigation. Linton, J. C. (1993). Helping the helpers: The development of a critical incident stress management team through university/community cooperation. Annals of Emergency Medicine, 22(4), 663–668. Lippert, W. W. (1990). Police officer suicide or homicide: Treating the affected department. In J. T. Reese, J. M. Horn, & C. Dunning (Eds.), Critical incidents in policing (pp. 179–190). Washington, DC: Government Printing Office. Litz, B. T., & Weathers, F. W. (1994). The diagnosis and assessment of post-traumatic stress disorder in adults. In M. B. Williams & J. F. Sommer (Eds.), Handbook of post-traumatic therapy (pp. 19–37). Westport, CT: Greenwood. Lykken, D. (1988). The case against polygraph testing. In A. Gale (Ed.), The polygraph test: Lies and science. London: Sage. Markesteyn, T. (1992). The psychological impact of nonsexual criminal offenses on victims. Retrieved August 12, 2010, from http://ww2.psepcsppcc.gc.ca/publications/corrections/199221_e.asp. McCafferty, F. L., McCafferty, E., & McCafferty, M. A. (1992). Stress and suicide in police officers. Southern Medical Journal, 85(3), 233–243. McCammon, S., & Allison, E. J. (1995). Debriefing and treating emergency workers. In C. Figley (Ed.), Compassion fatigue: Coping with secondary traumatic stress (pp. 151–130). New York: Brunner Mazel. McCunn, L., & Pearlman, L. A. (1990). Vicarious traumatization: A framework for understanding the psychological effects of working with victims. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 3(1), 131–149. McCutcheon, J. personal communication, August 12, 2016. McKelvie, S. (1989). The Wonderlic Personality test: Reliability and validity in the academic setting. Psychological Reports, 65, 161–162. McKenzie, J. (1986). Preface. In J. T. Reese & H. A. Goldstein (Eds.), Psychological services for law enforcement. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice. McMains, M. J. (1990). The management and treatment of post-shooting trauma: Administration and programs. In J. T. Reese, J. M. Horn, & C. Dunning (Eds.), Critical incidents in policing (pp. 191–198). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Mesnick, S. L. (2001). Genetic relatedness in sperm whales: Evidence and cultural implications. Behavioural and Brain Science, 24, 2346–2347. Mesnick, S. L., Evans, K., Taylor, B. L., Hyde, J., Escorza-Treviño, S., & Dizon, A. E. (2003). Sperm whale social structure: Why it takes a village to raise a child. In B. M. de Waal & P. L. Tyack (Eds.), Animal social complexity: Intelligence, culture and individualized societies (pp. 170–174). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Meyer, G. J. (2004). The reliability and validity of the Rorschach and TAT compared to other psychological and medical procedures: An analysis of systematically gathered evidence. In M. J. Hilsenroth & D. Segal (Eds.), Personality assessment: Vol. 2. Comprehensive handbook of psychological assessment (pp. 315–342). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Meyer, G. J., Mihura, J. L., & Smith, B. L. (2005). The interclinician reliability of Rorschach interpretation in four data sets. Journal of Personality Assessment, 84, 296–314.

58

Introduction and History Miller, L. (1998). Shocks to the system. New York: W. W. Norton. Miller, L. (1999). Workplace violence: Prevention, response, and recovery. Psychotherapy, 36, 160–169. Miller, L. (2001a). Workplace violence and psychological trauma: Clinical disability, legal liability, and corporate policy. Part I. Neurolaw Letter, 11, 1–5. Miller, L. (2001b). Workplace violence and psychological trauma: Clinical disability, legal liability, and corporate policy. Part II. Neurolaw Letter, 11, 7–13. Miller, L. (2002a). How safe is your job? The threat of workplace violence. USA Today Magazine, March, pp. 52–54. Miller, L. (2002b). Posttraumatic stress disorder in school violence: Risk management lessons from the workplace. Neurolaw Letter, 11(33), 36–40. Miller, L. (2002c). Psychological interventions for terroristic trauma: Symptoms, syndromes, and treatment strategies. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 39(4), 283–296. Miller, L. (2006). Practical police psychology: Stress management and crisis intervention for law enforcement. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas. Mills, M. C., & Stratton, J. G. (1982, February). The MMPI and the prediction of police job performance. FBI Law Enforcement Bulletin, 10–15. Mitchell, J., & Bray, G. (1990). Emergency services stress: Guidelines for preserving the health and careers of emergency services personnel. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Mitchell, J., & Everly, G. S. (1995a). Prevention of work-related posttraumatic stress: The critical incident stress debriefing process. In L. Murphy (Ed.), Job stress interventions (pp. 173–183). New York: Plenum. Mitchell, J., & Everly, G. S. (1995b). Of work-related traumatic stress among high risk occupational groups. In G. S. Everly & J. M. Lating (Eds.), Psychotraumatology (pp. 267–280). New York: Plenum. Mitchell, J., & Everly, G. S. (1996). Critical incident stress debriefing: An operations manual for the prevention of PTSD among emergency services and disaster workers. Ellicott City, MD: Chevron. Mitchell, J. T. (1983). When disaster strikes: The critical incident debriefing process. Journal of Emergency Medical Services, 8, 36–39. Mitchell, J. T. (1990). Law enforcement applications of critical incident stress teams. In J. T. Reese, J. M. Horn, & C. Dunning (Eds.), Critical incidents in policing (pp. 201–212). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Mitchell, J. T. (1994a). Critical incident stress interventions with families and significant others. In J. T. Reese & E. Scrivner (Eds.), Law enforcement families: Issues and answers (pp. 195–204). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Mitchell, J. T. (1994b). Systematic approach to critical incident stress management in law enforcement organizations. In J. T. Reese & R. Solomon (Eds.), Organization issues (pp. 215–222). Washington, DC: Federal Bureau of Investigation. Monat, A., & Lazarus, R. S. (1977). Stress and coping: Some current issues and controversies. In A. Monat & R. S. Lazarus (Eds.), Stress and coping: An anthology (pp. 1–15). New York: Columbia University Press. Mufson, L., Moreau, D., Weissman, M. M., Wickramaratne, P., Martin, J., & Samoilov, A. (1994). Modification of interpersonal psychotherapy with depressed adolescents (IPT-A): Phase I and II studies. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 33, 695–705. Mullins, W. C. (1994). Peer support team training and intervention for the police family. In J. T. Reese & E. Scrivner (Eds.), Law enforcement families: Issues and answers (pp. 205–216). Washington, DC: Federal Bureau of Investigation. Mullins, W. C., & McMains, M. J. (1995). Predicting patrol officer performance from a psychological assessment battery: A predictive validity study. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 10(4), 15–25. Munroe, J. F., Shay, J., Fisher, L., Makary, C., Rapperport, K., & Zimering, R. (1995). Preventing compassion fatigue: A team treatment model. In C. Figley (Ed.), Compassion fatigue: Coping with secondary traumatic stress (pp. 209–231). New York: Brunner Mazel. Münsterberg, H. (1908). On the witness stand. Garden City, NY: Doubleday. Murphy, J. J. (1972). Current practices in the use of psychological testing by police agencies. Journal of Criminal Law, Criminology, & Police Science, 63, 570. Murray, H. A. (1938). Explorations in personality (pp. 531–545). New York: Oxford University Press. National Security Institute. (n.d.). An assessment of the Aldrich H. Ames espionage case. Abstract of report of investigation. Retrieved March 2, 2007, from http://nsi.org/Library/Espionage/Hitzreport.html. Nemoto, T., Okiyama, M., & Takahashi, M. (1985). Aspects of the roles of squid in food chains of marine Antarctic ecosystems. In W. R. Seigfried, P. R. Condy, & R. M. Laws (Eds.), Antarctic nutrient cycles and food webs (pp. 415–420). Berlin: Springer-Verlag. Nesis, K. N. (1972). Oceanic cephalopods of the Peru current: Horizontal and vertical distribution. Oceanology, 12, 3426–3437. Nesis, K. N. (1987). Cephalopods of the world: Squids, cuttlefishes, octopuses and allies. Neptune City, FL: T.F.H. Publications.

59

Jack Kitaeff Nielsen, E. (1986). Understanding and assessing traumatic stress reactions. In J. Reese & H. A. Goldstein (Eds.), Psychological services for law enforcement (pp. 369–374). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice. Niles, D. P. (1994). Traumatology: Implications for training, education and development programs. In M. B. Williams (Ed.), Handbook of post-traumatic therapy (pp. 510–519). Westport, CT: Greenwood. Ochberg, F. M. (1995). Post-traumatic therapy. In G. S. Everly (Ed.), Psychotraumatology: Key papers and core concepts in post traumatic stress (pp. 245–264). New York: Plenum. Oksol, E. M., & O’Donohue, W. T. (2003). A critical analysis of the polygraph. In W. T. O’Donohue, & E. R. Levensky (Eds.), Handbook of forensic psychology: Resource for mental health and legal professionals (pp. 601–634). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Ones, D. S. (1993). Comprehensive Meta-Analysis of Integrity Test Validities: Findings and Implications for Personnel Selection and Theories of Job Performance. Journal of Applied Psychology 78(4), 679–703. Ones, D. S. (2003). Comprehensive meta-analysis of integrity test variables: Findings and implications for personnel selection and theories of job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 679–703. Ostrov, E. (1990). Critical incident psychological casualties among police officers: A critical review. In J. T. Reese, J. M. Horn, & C. Dunning (Eds.), Critical incidents in policing (pp. 251–256). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Ozer, E., et al. (2003). Predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder and symptoms in adults: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 129(1), 52–73. Pastorella, R. (1990). Posttraumatic stress disorder and the police experience. In J. T. Reese, J. M. Horn, & C. Dunning (Eds.), Critical incidents in policing (pp. 269–276). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Patton, D., & Violanti, J. M. (1996). Traumatic stress in critical incidents: Recognition, consequences, and treatment. Springfield, IL: Thomas. Poehlman, M. (1988). Peer support programs that deal with traumatic field events in California law enforcement (Command College paper). Sacramento, CA: Peace Officers Standards and Training. Quinn, V. (1994). More about police suicides. San Francisco: Peer Perspective, San Francisco Police Department. Ragaisis, K. M. (1994). Critical incident stress debriefing: A family nursing intervention. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing, 8(1), 38–32. Raza, S. M., & Carpenter, B. N. (1987, August). Piecemeal or through a model: An examination of employment interviews. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, New York. Reese, J. T. (1984). A cop’s best hopes are his family and colleagues. In Behavioral Science in Law Enforcement. Quantico, VA: Federal Bureau of Investigation. Reese, J. T. (1987a). A prescription for burn-out. In Behavioral Science in Law Enforcement. Quantico, VA: Federal Bureau of Investigation. Reese, J. T. (1987b). Coping with stress: It’s your job. In Behavioral Science in Law Enforcement. Quantico, VA: Federal Bureau of Investigation. Reese, J. T. (1991). Justifications for mandating critical incident aftercare. In J. T. Reese, J. M. Horn, & C. Dunning (Eds.), Critical incidents in policing (rev. ed., pp. 213–220). Washington DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Reese, J. T. (1995). A history of police psychological services. In M. Kurke & E. Scrivner (Eds.), Police psychology into the 21st century (pp. 31–44). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Reese, J. T., & Goldstein, H. A. (Eds.). (1986). Psychological services for law enforcement. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Reese, J. T., & Hodinko, B. M. (1990). Police psychological services: A history. In J. T. Reese, J. M. Horn, & C. Dunning (Eds.), Critical incidents in policing (pp. 297–310). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Reese, J. T., Horn, J. M., & Dunning, C. (Eds.). (1990). Critical incidents in policing. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Ressler, R., Burgess, A., & Douglas, J. (1983). Rape and rape murder: One offender and twelve victims. American Journal of Psychiatry, 140(1), 36–40. Rose, J. (1995). Consolidation of law enforcement basic training academies: An evaluation of pilot projects. Flagstaff: Northern Arizona University. Rudofossi, D. (2007). Working with traumatized police officer-patients: A clinician’s guide to complex PTSD syndromes in public safety populations. New York: Baywood Press. Ryan, A. H. (2005). Assessment resources and Interquest, LLC. Retrieved August 10, 2010, from www. rpiq.com/ arpiq/bio.asp. Ryan, A. H., & Brewster, M. E. (1994). PTSD and related symptoms in traumatized police officers and their spouse/mates. In J. T. Reese & E. Scrivner (Eds.), Law enforcement families (pp. 217–226). Washington, DC: Federal Bureau of Investigation.

60

Introduction and History Rynearson, E. K. (1996). Psychotherapy of bereavement after homicide: Be offensive. Winter 1996. https://doi.org/ 10.1002/(SICI)1520-6572(199624)2:43.0.CO;2-5 Saccuzzo, D. P., Higgins, G., & Lewandowski, D. (1974). Program for psychological assessment of law enforcement officers: Initial evaluation. Psychological Reports, 35, 651–654. Sackett, P. R., Burris, L. R., & Callahan, C. (1989). Integrity testing for personnel selection: An update. Personnel Psychology, 42, 491–529. Sackett, P. R., & Harris, M. M. (1984). Honesty testing for personnel selection: A review and critique. Personnel Psychology, 17, 221–245. Sarchione, C. D., Cuttler, M. J., Muchinsky, P. M., & Nelson-Gray, R. O. (1998). Prediction of dysfunctional job behaviors among law enforcement officers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 904–912. Saxe, L., Dougherty, D., & Cross, T. (1985). The validity of polygraph testing: Scientific analysis and public controversy. American Psychologist, 40, 355–366. Saxe, S. J., & Reiser, M. (1976). A comparison of three police applicant groups using the MMPI. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 4, 419–425. Scogin, F., & Beutler, L. (1986). Psychological screening of law enforcement candidates. In P. Keller & L. G. Ritt (Eds.), Innovations in clinical practice: A source book (pp. 317–330). Sarasota, FL: Professional Resource Exchange. Scogin, F., Schumacher, J., Gardner, J., & Chaplin, W. (1995). Predictive validity of psychological testing in law enforcement settings. Professional Psychology, Research and Practice, 26(1), 68–71. Schlossberg, H. (1974). Psychologist with a gun. New York: Coward, McCann & Geoghegan. Schumacher, J., Scogin, F., Howland, K., & McGee, J. (1992). The relation of peer assessment to future law enforcement performance. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 19, 286–293. Sellbom, M., Ben-Porath, Y. S., & Bagby, R. M. (2008). On the hierarchical structure of mood and anxiety disorders: Confirmatory evidence and elaboration of a model of temperament markers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 576–590. Sellbom, M., Ben-Porath, Y. S., & Stafford, K. P. (2007). A comparison of MMPI-2 measures of psychopathic deviance in a forensic setting. Psychological Assessment, 19, 430–436. Seligmann, J. (1994, September 26). Cops who kill themselves. Newsweek, 58. Seligman, M., & Buchanan, G. M. (Eds.). (1995). Explanatory style. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Selye, H. (1979a). Stress, cancer, and the mind. In J. Tache, H. Selye, & S. B. Day (Eds.), Cancer, stress, and death (pp. 11–27). New York: Plenum. Selye, H. (1979b) The stress of my life. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold. Shalev, A. (2007, December 9). Psychotherapy useful in treating post-traumatic stress disorder in early stages. Science Daily. Retrieved May 12, 2009, from www.sciencedaily.com. Sheehan, P. L. (1995). Critical incident trauma and intimacy. In J. T. Reese, J. M. Horn, & C. Dunning (Eds.), Critical incidents in policing (pp. 331–334). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Shusman, E. J., & Inwald, R. E. (1991). A longitudinal validation study of correctional officer job performance as predicted by the IPI and MMPI. Journal of Criminal Justice, 19(2), 173–180. Shusman, E. J., Inwald, R. E., & Knatz, H. F. (1987). A cross-validation study of police recruit performance as predicted by the IPI and MMPI. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 15(2), 162–169. Silva, M. N. (1990). The delivery of mental health services to law enforcement officers. In J. T. Reese, J. M. Horn, & C. Dunning (Eds.), Critical incidents in policing (pp. 335–341). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Sloan, I. H., Rozensky, R. H., Kaplan, L., & Saunders, S. (1994). A shooting incident in an elementary school: Effects of worker stress on public safety, mental health and medical personnel. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 7(4), 565–674. Smith, C. (1997). Comparing traditional therapies with narrative approaches. In C. Smith & D. Nylund (Eds.), In narrative therapy with children and adolescents. New York: Guilford. Smith, C. L., & Chesnay, M. D. (1994). Critical incident stress debriefings for crisis management in posttraumatic stress disorders. Medicine and Law, 13, 185–191. Solomon, R. M. (1990). The dynamics of fear in critical incidents: Implications for training and treatment. In J. T. Reese, J. M. Horn, & C. Dunning (Eds.), Critical incidents in policing (pp. 347–358). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Solomon, R. M. (1996, October). Post shooting trauma. The Police Chief, 40–44. Spielberger, C. D. (1979). Understanding stress and anxiety. New York: Harper & Row. Sprang, G., & McNeil, J. (1995). The many faces of bereavement: The nature and treatment of natural, traumatic, and stigmatized grief. New York: Brunner/Mazel. Spungen, D. (1998). Homicide: The hidden victims. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Stern, W. (1912). The psychological methods of intelligence testing (G. Whipple, Trans.). Baltimore: Warwick and York.

61

Jack Kitaeff Stern, W. (n. d.). Human intelligence. Retrieved October 31, 2010, from www.indiana.edu/~intell/stern.shtml. Stevens, D. J. (1998). What do law enforcement officers think about their work? The Journal, 47(1). Stevens, D. J. (1999a). Do college educated officers provide quality police service? Law and Order, 47(12), 37–41. Stevens, D. J. (1999b). Corruption among narcotic officers: A study of innocence and integrity. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 14(2), 1–11. Stevens, D. J. (1999c). Stress and the American police officer. Police Journal, 72(3), 247–259. Stevens, D. J. (1999d). Police officer stress. Law and Order, 47(9), 77–81. Stevens, D. J. (2001a). Case studies in community policing. New York: Prentice Hall. Stevens, D. J. (2001b). Community policing and managerial techniques: Total quality management techniques. The Police Journal, 74(1), 26–61. Super, J. T., & Blau, T. H. (1997). Survey of psychological services general orders in law enforcement. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 12(1), 7–12. Surrette, M. A., Aamodt, M. G., & Serafino, G. (1990). Validity of the New Mexico police selection battery. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Society of Police and Criminal Critical incident trauma and intimacy psychology, Albuquerque, NM. Swann, G. B., & D’Agostino, C. (1994). Post-shooting trauma and domestic violence: Clinical observation and preliminary data. In J. T. Reese & E. Scrivner (Eds.), Law enforcement families: Issues and answers (pp. 227–232). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Tellegen, A., & Ben-Porath, Y. S. (2008). The Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2 restructured form: Technical manual. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. Terman, L. M. (1916). The measurement of intelligence. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Terman, L. M., et al. (1925). Genetic studies of genius: Vol. 1. Mental and physical traits of a thousand gifted children. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Terman, L. M., et al. (1938). Psychological factors in marital happiness. New York: McGraw-Hill. Thurstone, L. L. (1952). L. L. Thurstone. In E. G. Boring, H. S. Langfeld, H. Werner, & R. M. Yerkes (Eds.), A history of psychology in autobiography (pp. 295–321). Worcester, MA: Clark University Press. Ursano R. J., Fullerton, C. S., & Norwood, A. E. Psychiatric dimensions of disaster: patient care, community consultation, and preventive medicine. Harvard Review of Psychiatry. (1995). Nov-Dec; 3(4):196–209. Ursano, R. J., Fullerton, C. S., & Norwood, A. E. (Eds.). (2003). Terrorism and disaster: Individual and community mental health interventions. New York: Cambridge University Press. U.S. Office of Personnel Management. Alternate dispute resolution resource guide. U.S. Office of Personnel Management. Retrieved July 19, 2007, from www.opm.gov/er/adrguide. Van der Kolk, B. A. (1990). The psychological processing of traumatic events: The personal experience of post-traumatic stress disorder. In J. T. Reese, J. M. Horn, & C. Dunning (Eds.), Critical incidents in policing (pp. 359–364). Washington, DC: Government Printing Office. Violanti, J. M. (1990). Posttrauma vulnerability: A proposed model. In J. T. Reese, J. M. Horn, & C. Dunning (Eds.), Critical incidents in policing (pp. 365–372). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Violanti, J. M. (1995). The mystery within: Understanding police suicides. FBI Law Enforcement Bulletin, 19–23. Violanti, J. M., Marshall, J. R., & Howe, B. (1985). Stress coping and alcohol use: The police connection. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 12(2), 106–110. Wambaugh, J. (1973). The onion field. New York: Delacort. Wambaugh, J. (1987). Echoes in the darkness. New York: Delacort. Wechsler, D. (1939). The measurement of adult intelligence. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins. Weiss, P. A. (2002). Potential uses of the Rorschach in the selection of police officers. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 17(2), 63–70. Weiss, W. U., Buehler, K., & Yates, D. (1995). The Psychopathic Deviate scale of the MMPI in police selection. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 10, 57–60. Weiss, P. A., Weiss, W. U., & Gacono, C. B. (2008). The use of the Rorschach in police psychology: Some preliminary thoughts. In C. B. Gacono & F. B. Evans (Eds.), Handbook of forensic Rorschach assessment (pp. 527– 542). New York: Routledge. Weiss, W., Rostow, C. D., & Davis, R. D. (2004). The Personality Assessment Inventory as a selection device for law enforcement personnel. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 19(2), 23–29. Weiss, W., Zehner, S. N., Davis, R. D., & Rostow, C. D. (2005). Problematic police performance and the Personality Assessment Inventory. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 20(1), 16–21. Weiss, W. U., Buehler, K., & Yates, D. (1996). The psychopathic deviate scale of the MMPI in police selection. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 10(4), 57–60. Weiss, W. U., Davis, R., Rostow, C., & Kinsman, S. (2003). The MMPI-2 L scale as a tool in police selection. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 18(1), 57–60.

62

Introduction and History Weiss, W. U., Serafino, G., Serafino, A., Wilson, W., & Knoll, S. (1998). Performance ratings of recently hired police officers. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 13(1), 40–44. White, M. (1989). Saying hullo again: The incorporation of the lost relationship in the resolution of grief (Selected papers, pp. 29–36). Adelaide, Australia: Dulwich Centre Publications. White, M., & Epston, D. (1990). Narrative means to therapeutic ends. New York: Norton. Wilson, J. P. (1995). The historical evolution of PTSD diagnostic criteria. In G. S. Everly (Ed.), Psychotraumatology (pp. 9–26). New York: Plenum. Wollman, D. (1993). Critical incident stress debriefing and crisis groups: A review of the literature. Group, 17(2), 70–83. Wonderlic. (1999). Wonderlic human resource solutions: Our history. Retrieved August 8, 2010, from http://www. wonderlic.com/hiring-solutions/about-us.aspx. Wygant, D. B., et al. (2007). The relation between symptom validity testing and MMPI-2 scores as a function of forensic evaluation context. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22, 488–499. Yassen, J. (1995). Preventing secondary traumatic stress disorder. In C. Figley (Ed.), Compassion fatigue: Coping with secondary traumatic stress (pp. 178–208). New York: Brunner Mazel. Young, M. (1991). Community crisis response team training manual. Washington, DC: National Organization for Victim Assistance. Zacker, J. (1997). Rorschach responses of police applicants. Psychological Reports, 80, 523–528.

Cases Cited Bonsignore v. City of New York, 521F. Supp. 394, affirmed 683 F.2d 635 (2nd Cir. 1982). Cannice v. Norwest Bank Iowa N.A., 189 F.3d 723 (1999). City of Greenwood v. Dowler, 492 N.E.2d 1081 (Ind. App. 1986). Colon v. Colon, 2006 WL 2318250 (App. Div. 2006). Conte v. Harcher, 365 N.E. 2d.257 (Ill. App. 1977). Davis v. Hennepin County, 559 N.W.2d 117 (MN App. 1997). Deen v. Darosa, #4–2072, 414F.rd, 731 (2005). Denhof v. City of Grand Rapids, 494 F.3d 534,57 n.4 (16th Cir. 2007). Flynn v. Sandahl, 58 F.3d 283 (7th Cir. 1995). Garrity v. New Jersey, 385 U.S. 493 (1987). Geidel v. City Bradenton Beach, 56 F. Supp. 2d 1359 (M.D. FL. 1999). Guice-Mills v. Derwinski, 987 F.2d 794,797 (2nd. Cir. 1992). Hardy Town of Hayneville, 50 Supp. 2d 1176 (M.D. Ala. 1999). Hild v. Bruner, 496 F. Supp. 93 (G=D.N.J. 1980). Holst v. Veterans Affairs, #2008–3012, 2008 U.S. App. Lexis 22998 (Unpub. Fed Cir. 2008). Jackson v. Lake County, 790 N.E.2d. 448,451 (Ind. 2003). Kraft v. Police Commissioner of Boston, 417 Mass. 235 at 242, 629 N.E.2d. 995 at 999 (1994). Larson v. Koch Refining Co., 11 S.W.3d 158 (Texas, 1997). McKnight v. Monroe Co. Sheriff ’s Dept., #IP00–1880-C-B, U.S. Dist. Lexis 18148 (2002). Miller v. City of Springfield, 146 F.3d 612 (1998). National Treasury Employees Union v. Von Raab, 385 U.S.493 (1989). Policemen’s Benevolent Association of New Jersey v. Township of Washington, D.N.J. No. 86–3525 (October 19, 1988). PBA L-319 v. Township of Plainsboro, #C-173–98 Middlesex Co. (NJ Superior Court Unreported 1998). Richardson v. City of Pasadena, 175 Cal. 3666 (1973–1974). Risner v. U.S. Department of Transportation, 677 F.2d 36 (8th Cir. 1982). Sehie v. City of Aurora, 432 F.3d 749 (7th Cir. 2005). Tingler v. City of Tampa, 400 So. Sd,146, 149–150 (Fla. App. 1981). Watson v. City of Miami Beach, 177 F.3d 932 (11th Cir. 1999).

63

PART 1

General Practice

2 POLICE PSYCHOLOGICAL CONSULTATION SERVICES TO PUBLIC SAFETY Joseph A. Davis

Police Psychological Services Behavioral scientists, specifically police psychologists, play an integral role in law enforcement today. Large metropolitan law enforcement agencies like the Dallas, Boston, and Los Angeles Police Departments staff behavioral scientists in the capacity of police psychologists and organizational development and human factors/human performance consultants (Davis, 1995; Reiser, 1972a). Many behavioral scientists who possess specialized training beyond their traditional clinical training can effectively assist law enforcement management and various other personnel, such as directors of police training academies, to enhance their day-to-day operations and performances of each department and staff member. The scope in which police psychologists or behavioral scientists are used in modern law enforcement departments today is diverse. Consultations often range from individual to group consultation, officer candidate assessment and evaluation, police officer selection, hostage negotiation, stress management, and the counseling of police officers and their families (Davis, 1993; Depue, 1979; Reiser, 1972b). Additionally, consultations include police management and supervisory training; police academy teaching and instruction; research and development; police or community critical incident debriefing; urban crime prevention programs; advanced officer training; and diagnosing and solving organizational, managerial, or supervisory problems. Furthermore, employee assistance programs (EAPs) and burn-out prevention programs are fundamentally sound policies, procedures, and administrative decisions in terms of planning for the department’s overall future growth and for the wellness of its personnel who operate within it (Hargrave & Berner, 1984). Typically, police psychologists work with departments in one of two ways: They provide services, consultation, and training as an “in-house” service, that is, as an actual department staff member, or as an outside consultant who provides services under contract departmentwide. There are advantages as well as limitations to both; however, both arrangements have merit and should be set up according to agency size, number of personnel, specialized needs, and budgets of each department and division. Regardless of the arrangement, police psychologists should generally be connected to the highest level of the department’s administrative and managerial structure and, if possible, as close in proximity to the chief of police as possible for the purposes of “direct line” consultation and communication (Lefkowitz, 1977). The issue of confidentiality also has its place in the police psychologist’s office when working with sworn or support personnel, especially when services such as psychotherapy, counseling,

67

Joseph A. Davis

assessment, fitness-for-duty evaluation, and research are conducted (Davis, 1993). Depending on the issue or referral question, range of confidentiality can be complete, limited, restricted, or open and upfront, which is told to the officer or employee seeking or being referred for psychological services once contact with a supervisor or commanding officer is made. An example of a nonrestrictive disclosure or breach of confidentiality is when a question of officer dangerousness or an issue involving fitness for duty is of major concern (Davis, 1995; Reiser, 1972b; Somodeville, 1978). Victimization, misuse, or abuse of police powers or officer misconduct is an area typically open and upfront in terms of disclosure that is not protected by confidentiality. Areas of confidentiality include when (1) the direct counseling and treatment of officers, their families, and various civilian personnel for job-related problems are the issue; and when (2) a critical consideration is given to privileged communications between the police psychologist, the employee, and a third party such as a supervisor or family member (Davis, 1993). When possible, if the police psychologist is a contract provider, the office should be at a location physically outside the department to ensure confidentiality and officer comfort. If the police psychologist is an “in-house” service provider, a department representative, or an extension of the department, an office should be made available geographically within the premises that is secluded enough to promote utilization of services, confidentiality, and employee receptability (Reiser, 1972b; Somodeville, 1978). In a unique “forensic” role utilized by some departments, the expertise of the police psychologist can play an important part as an objective scientist in major case consultation or when a crime investigation is underway, especially when the crime committed by a perpetrator or offender is a “­stranger-to-stranger” crime; in cases where the crime seemed senseless, sexually sadistic, and motiveless; or when the perpetrator is unknown and cannot be identified (Davis, 1995). Requests may come from specific investigative units within the department such as the homicide unit, sex crimes unit, or “violent crimes committed against persons” division for the purposes of “criminal investigative analysis” or “criminal-psychological personality profiling.” Crime scene analysis, criminal investigative analysis, or what is often referred to as “profiling” is derived by carefully detailing the crime scene area identified by officers, detectives, coroners, medical examiners, criminalists, forensic scientists, and evidence technicians (Davis, 1993; Geberth, 1981). Along with talking to forensic crime scene photographers and by examining crime scene or victim photographs afterward, the police psychologist (especially when trained in the forensic sciences) can give many investigators sound, objective scientific support when evaluating and examining the critical elements connecting physical, mental, or circumstantial evidence during intense crime scene analysis. A profile generated at times can yield important psychological, forensic, and behavioral data or associated variables as to the offender’s modus operandi (method of operation, or M.O.) and so-called individual signature in terms of antecedent criminal behavior, or how the individual organized and showed planning (or lack of it) for, carried out, and staged a crime, and possibly even selected the victim in some cases (Davis, 1993; Geberth, 1981). Typically, the criminal does not commit certain types of crimes such as sexually motivated homicide overnight. These types of crimes evolve over time and can involve an organized offender type that plans a very methodical approach to victim selection and abduction such as was seen in the Ted Bundy case. Conversely, a disorganized offender type can carry out the same crime or similar crimes without such meticulous and methodical approaches to detail and victim selection such as was seen in the Richard Trenton Chase case (Davis, 1995). To complicate matters even more, the offender can possess both organized and disorganized approaches to committing violent crimes, suggesting and presenting a mixed offender type to investigators or profilers. It must be recognized even by the most experienced investigator that a criminal’s M.O. can change from one crime to the next; however, the same criminal’s “signature” will remain unchanged and always consistent (Cooke, 1980; Davis, 1994; Geberth, 1981). Additionally, in terms of a homicide case, the crime scene can yield significant clues 68

Police Psychological Consultation Services

regarding pre- and post-crime scene behavior as to whether the perpetrator was an organized, mixed, or otherwise disorganized type of offender, and whether injuries or sexual and physical assaults sustained to the victim’s body were inflicted ante mortem (when the victim was alive) or post mortem (after the victim was already dead). The crime scene, along with additional inspection at the “morgue scene,” typically held at the coroner or medical examiner’s office (some states have either a dedicated medical examiner or coroner system; however, California has a system designated by county jurisdiction as either a ME or coroner system; for example, San Diego County has a ME system and Los Angeles County has a coroner system), can also render information regarding the degree and type of violence exerted by the criminal toward the victim, especially when examining the various types of trauma inflicted to the victim such as blunt force, sharp force, asphyxia, “tight” contact, or close-range gunshot wounds (Davis, 1994; Geberth, 1981). Many crimes have motives; however, some crimes can appear senseless or even motiveless. Evidence of sadistic torture, trauma to the face or personal areas, binding of the victim’s hands and feet (ligature), the type of binding used (duct tape, rope, wire, electrical cord), disposition of the body, location of the body (i.e., in an open area or secluded area, hidden or covered, and buried or dumped in a culvert or alongside a highway or back road), along with specialized positioning of the body (undressed, dressed, in a provocative pose, or otherwise) can all reveal the killer’s organization (or lack thereof ), planning, and thought processes before, during, and after the act of violence was committed. Police psychologists trained in criminal investigative analyses on psychological profiling can also give evidence of connecting elements of the crime as well as the signature and profile of whoever carried out the heinous act of violence, no matter how organized, disorganized, sexually sadistic, or ritualistic, will in most cases typically surface (Cooke, 1980; Davis, 1994). In addition to providing and supporting investigators with valuable antecedent criminal profile information and data, the police psychologist can be a valuable asset to the department directly when the community or even police personnel have witnessed an act of violence, a crime, or a tragedy so bizarre, unusual, brutal, or heinous that critical incident debriefing or consultation and subsequent counseling for post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) occur. An example would be the unforgettable massacre and mass homicide of 21 innocent and unsuspecting men, women, and children (the youngest victim was 8 months old) involving James Oliver Huberty that occurred at a McDonald’s restaurant in San Ysidro, California, in July 1984. Included here would also be the mass homicide of 23 victims, October 16, 1991, at Luby’s Cafeteria in Killeen, Texas, by solo gunman George “Jo Jo” Hennard. His murderous actions at the time remained the largest single incident of mass homicide in the United States until the Virginia Tech mass murder on April 16, 2007, by undergraduate student Seung-Hui Cho, or the brutal serial homicides in 1990 that effectively terrified the residential suburbs of Clairemont and University City areas of San Diego and took the lives of six women, one of which was this author’s former student (Davis, 1995). The San Diego Police Department (SDPD) and Homicide Division received thousands of calls and more than 1,100 leads or tips from the public regarding the six San Diego murder cases. Top police administrators and supervisors subsequently assigned 44 investigators to the case, which eventually turned out to be the largest serial murder manhunt in San Diego Police Department history. The FBI was also involved in the case from a psychological profiling standpoint. Other examples where critical incident debriefing can be useful are when the abduction and murder of a local child is discovered, such as the Laura Arroyo child murder case in Chula Vista, California, a suburb in San Diego County, or in the death following a shooting of a fellow officer or partner in the line of duty such as the one that took the life of SDPD Patrol Officer Ron Davis. Still other countless examples can be cited such as when a bomb explosion occurs and both police and civilians are killed (Davis, 1993). Other examples include the death of a hostage or hostages during negotiating, or in a man-made disaster such as the PSA Flight 182 air traffic fatality in 1978 in the North Park suburb area of San 69

Joseph A. Davis

Diego where approximately 160 victims died traumatically. Yet other examples are the Pan Am 103 disaster over Lockerbie, Scotland, in 1988 where 270 people died, or the USAir Flight 427 where 132 died in 1994. Finally, let’s never forget the tragedies and lessons that the Oklahoma City bombing in 1995 or the World Trade Center event in 2001 left behind. Of the 144 PSA victims, four were never identified even though dental identification teams utilizing forensic odontology (dentistry) were used. The PSA 182 flight utilized the FBI’s Disaster Identification Squad, and hundreds of police, medical, fire, and civilian personnel took weeks to identify bodies from the wreckage and carnage. The identification teams worked 12-hour days. Many of the bodies and remaining carnage were beyond conventional identification and could only be identified by specialized forensic techniques used by highly trained forensic dentists. The plane crash of Reba McEntire’s country and western band also required similar action from forensic and law enforcement personnel. In similar fashion, the State of Pennsylvania mobilized its Dental Identification Team (PADIT) to work the USAir Flight 427 crash. Overall, the police psychologist or behavioral-forensic scientist can effectively provide information and feedback to the chief of police, police public relations officer, or crime prevention specialists in times when a call to action is in order by citizens of the community, especially when an increase in residential burglary takes place or when a series of violent personal crimes occur such as serial rape in the immediate area (Davis, 1993; Depue, 1979; Reiser, 1972b). In times of public outcry or citizen unrest, a police psychologist can be of valuable assistance. Supporting the chief of police or sheriff as an organizational and human factors advisor, the psychologist or behavioral scientists can effectively establish or chair a task force to evaluate specific problems facing police and the community (Davis, 1993; Lefkowitz, 1977; Reiser, 1972b). In general, integrating the expertise and support of behavioral scientists or police psychologists into the structure and daily operation of the police department as service, management, and supervisory personnel can ensure the maintenance of productive functioning; the quality of the day-by-day performances of their sworn and nonsworn personnel; and the overall wellness, mental health, and effectiveness of their entire organization (Reiser, 1972b). Large metropolitan police departments utilize police psychologists and behavioral-forensic scientists in various roles, especially in the areas of counseling, psychotherapy, training, critical incident stress debriefings, consultation, and research. Each department uses police psychologists differently depending on department need. The use of police psychologists has proven to be an extremely valuable asset and is cost-effective (Davis, 1993). Overall, the services provided by the behavioral sciences or psychological services division of any police department or law enforcement agency can ultimately prove to be an excellent police management decision in terms of the overall welfare, function, wellness, happiness, and future of the department, focusing on its key sworn, nonsworn, and support personnel (Davis, 1995; Depue, 1979; Lefkowitz, 1977; Reiser, 1972b; Somodeville, 1978). The time has come when law enforcement departments can no longer afford not to borrow the expertise from the behavioral and forensic sciences (Davis, 1993).

References and Further Reading Cooke, G. (1980). The role of the forensic psychologist. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas. Davis, J. A. (1993, January). The use of behavioral scientists in law enforcement. The Law Enforcement Quarterly, 20–28. Davis, J. A. (1994). The art and science of psychological profiling: Selected readings in forensic profile analysis. Course Workshop Training Manual, San Diego, CA. Davis, J. A. (1995). The police psychologist in today’s law enforcement. The Police Chief Magazine, 62(11), 36–38. Depue, R. (1979). Turning inward: The police counselor. FBI Law Enforcement Bulletin, 8–12. Geberth, V. (1981). Psychological profiling. Law and Order, 46–52.

70

Police Psychological Consultation Services Hargrave, G. E., & Berner, J. G. (1984). POST psychological screening manual. Sacramento: California Commission on Police Officers Standards and Training. Lefkowitz, J. (1977). Industrial-organizational psychology and the police. American Psychologist, 32, 346–364. Reiser, M. (1972a). The police department psychologist. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas. Reiser, M. (1972b). Practical psychology for police officers. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas. Somodeville, S. (1978). The psychologist’s role in the police department. The Police Chief, 21–23.

71

3 LEGAL ISSUES RELATED TO HIRING AND PROMOTION OF POLICE OFFICERS Arthur Gutman

Introduction Allegations of discrimination against police departments span a variety of issues, including refusal to hire minorities and females (i.e., facial discrimination), adverse impact of facially neutral selection criteria on minorities (e.g., cognitive tests) and females (e.g., physical requirements), racial and sexual harassment, diversity as a compelling government interest under the 14th Amendment of the Constitution, age limits for hiring and forced retirement of police officers, illegal medical and psychological inquiries prior to a conditional job offer, and more. Comprehensive coverage of these and related issues is provided by Gutman, Koppes, and Vodanovich (2010). The main focus of this chapter is on what is arguably the most important issue facing police forces today: adverse impact in hiring and promotion related to tests and other selection criteria on minorities and, to some extent, females. As a starting point, adverse impact based on race, sex, religion, or national origin is uniquely associated with Title VII of the Civil Rights Act of 1964 (or simply Title VII).1 There are three phases in the Title VII adverse impact judicial scenario. As depicted in Table 3.1, the plaintiff must identify a selection practice that disproportionately excludes one group (e.g., African Americans) relative to another group (e.g., Caucasians) in Phase 1. If the plaintiff succeeds, the defendant must prove that the challenged practice is job related and consistent with business necessity in Phase 2. Then, if the defendant succeeds, the plaintiff must prove that there is an equally valid alternative practice that produces less or no adverse impact in Phase 3. In general, the most frequent method for assessing knowledge, skills, and abilities (or KSAs) related to police work is written multiple-choice tests because they can be administered to large groups and are easy to score. However, there are other methods of assessing these KSAs, including oral interviews, situational judgment tests, and assessment center methodology, to name a few. These other methods are often used in conjunction with written tests. Indeed, a common claim in adverse impact cases is that nonwritten tests, or different weightings among written versus nonwritten components, are as valid as the actual method(s) used. The discussion below treats all methods of assessing KSAs as a “test” subject to adverse impact analyses. An additional point to note is that there are common issues relating to hiring and promotion of police officers as there are with firefighters. Although the focus of this chapter is on police hiring and promotion, firefighter cases will be discussed when relevant to police issues. For example, as we

72

Legal Issues in Hiring, Promoting Officers Table 3.1 The Title VII Adverse Impact Judicial Scenario Phase 1 Prima facie phase requiring statistical evidence that an identified employment practice disproportionately excludes protected group members. Phase 2 Defense phase requiring proof that the challenged practice is job related and consistent with business necessity. Phase 3 Pretext phase requiring proof there is an alternative equally valid practice (or practices) that are equally job related, but that produce less or no adverse impact.

will witness below, the U.S. Supreme Court’s 2009 ruling in Ricci v. DeStefano relates to promotion of firefighters. Nevertheless, this ruling has equally important implications for police departments and other entities as well. There are six sections below. The first section reviews major Supreme Court rulings on adverse impact between 1971 and 1989, and the second section reviews early lower court rulings that were based on early Supreme Court rulings. The third section features more recent rulings on cutoff scores and related issues, the fourth section features methods of reducing or eliminating adverse impact, the fifth section focuses on the Supreme Court’s recent ruling in Ricci v. DeStefano (2009), and the sixth section offers conclusions and recommendations.

Overview of Supreme Court Adverse Impact Rulings Table 3.2 depicts nine landmark Supreme Court rulings between 1971 (Griggs v. Duke Power) and 2009 (Ricci v. DeStefano). Five of the nine cases feature objectively scored written tests, and four of them feature other causes of adverse impact. These topics will be discussed in turn immediately below. For purposes of exposition, Ricci v. DeStefano (2009) will be reserved for discussion in the fifth section of this chapter.

Adverse Impact and Written Tests Early precedents for written tests were established in Griggs v. Duke Power (1971) and Albemarle v. Moody (1975), and the principal rulings in these two cases served as the basis for the Uniform Guidelines on Employee Selection Procedures (UGESP) in 1978. Washington v. Davis (1976), the first Supreme Court adverse impact ruling involving police officers, addressed the question of whether adverse impact is a valid claim under the 5th or 14th Amendments of the Constitution. Connecticut v. Teal (1982), the first Supreme Court Title VII claim against a government agency, addressed the question of whether a Phase 2 defense to adverse impact is required for individual steps in a multiple-hurdle selection procedure when there is no bottom-line adverse impact for the total selection process.

The Griggs and Albemarle Rulings Griggs and Albemarle featured private sector employers that used cognitive tests and a high school diploma requirement in their selection process. The facts in Griggs were that prior to Title VII, Duke Power facially excluded African Americans from upper-level jobs. The company then initiated the testing and diploma requirements for these upper-level jobs on July 2, 1965, the very day that Title VII took effect. The prima facie case featured statistical evidence that the cognitive tests excluded 94% of African American applicants as compared to only 42% of Caucasian applicants. Additionally,

73

Arthur Gutman Table 3.2 Landmark Supreme Court Adverse Impact Rulings Challenges to Written Exams Griggs v. Duke Power (1971) Albemarle v. Moody (1975) Washington v. Davis (1976) Connecticut v. Teal (1982) Ricci v. DeStefano (2009)

Challenge to use of cognitive tests and high school diploma for upper-level coal-mining jobs in a private company. Challenge to use of cognitive tests and high school diploma for entry-level paper mill jobs in a private company. Challenge to civil service exam for entry-level jobs in the Washington, D.C., police force. Challenge to first hurdle (a written test) in a multiple-hurdle selection procedure for state administrator jobs. Challenge to discarding promotion exams in a fire department after they were administered and scored. Challenges to Other Selection Criteria

Dothard v. Rawlinson (1977) NYC v. Beazer (1979) Watson v. Fort Worth Bank (1988) Wards Cove v. Atonio (1989)

Challenge to height and weight requirements for prison guards in an all-male maximum security prison. Challenge to policy of excluding methadone users for entry-level transit authority police officers. Challenge to subjective selection procedures in promotion of bank tellers. Challenge to disproportionate exclusion of Eskimos and Filipinos from higher-level jobs in a fish packing company.

the high school diploma requirement was deemed to disproportionately “chill” prospective African American applicants because the graduation rate in North Carolina at that time was 34% for Caucasians as compared to only 12% for African Americans. Speaking for a unanimous Supreme Court, Justice Burger ruled that Title VII covers the “consequences of employment practices, not simply the motivation” of employers. He then wrote what arguably are the two most important phrases in adverse impact case law: that if the plaintiff proves adverse impact, the defendant must prove there is a manifest relationship between the challenged practice and the employment in question. There was no evidence to prove the cognitive tests and high school diploma were job related, and Duke Power lost the case. Aware of the Griggs ruling, the Albemarle Paper Company tried to prove that their cognitive tests were significantly and positively correlated with job performance (i.e., using a criterion validity study). However, it was a poor study conducted a month prior to the trial. The Supreme Court struck down the study on four grounds: (1) a lack of quality, or “odd patchwork”; (2) unknown job-performance criteria and subjective supervisory rankings; (3) a focus on high-level jobs rather than the “entering low-level jobs” at issue; and (4) a validation sample that included only “job-­ experienced white workers.” Relying on the then applicable 1970 Guidelines on Employee Selection Procedures by the Equal Employment Opportunity Commission (EEOC), the Supreme Court defined how a manifest relationship should be proven. Accordingly: The message of these Guidelines is the same as that of the Griggs case—that discriminatory tests are impermissible unless shown, by professionally acceptable methods[,] to be predictive of or significantly correlated with important elements of work behavior which comprise or are relevant to the job or jobs for which candidates are evaluated. [emphasis by author]

74

Legal Issues in Hiring, Promoting Officers

The Albemarle Court then created the third phase of the adverse impact scenario, permitting proof of equally valid alternatives with less or no adverse impact.

The Immediate Aftermath of Griggs and Albemarle The Griggs and Albemarle rulings were then codified into regulatory law in the UGESP in 1978. The UGESP are still the most important regulatory authority used by courts in adverse impact cases. However, as noted by several authors,2 the UGESP were based primarily on the first edition of the Standards for Educational and Psychological Testing (or Standards) (1974), and therefore are now outdated. Critically, this was anticipated, and is reflected in Section 1607.5(A) of the UGESP, which states, “New strategies for showing the validity of selection procedures will be evaluated as they become accepted by the psychological profession.” As expected, new strategies in validity research have been developed since 1978, as documented in the fifth edition of the Standards (1999) and the fourth edition of the Principles for the Validation and Use of Personnel Selection Procedures (or Principles) published by the Society for Industrial and Organization Psychology (SIOP) (2003). Courts have cited both of these authorities with increasing frequency in recent years. Therefore, test developers and consumers should be equally as familiar with the Standards and Principles as they are with the UGESP.3

The Davis and Teal Rulings Prior to 1972, Title VII covered only private entities. Coverage of public entities was added in the Equal Employment Act of 1972 (or EEO-72). Consequently, for the plaintiffs in Washington v. Davis (1976), the 5th Amendment was the only basis for challenging Civil Service Test 21, a verbal skills test first used in 1970 by the Washington, D.C., Police Department for entry-level police jobs. The Supreme Court ruled that adverse impact is not a valid claim under the 5th Amendment, or other Constitutional amendments.4 Nevertheless, the Supreme Court evaluated Test 21 and ruled it was valid because it (1) measured minimum skills necessary for completing police training school, and (2) criterion validity was established with proof that scores on Test 21 were significantly and positively correlated with scores on written exams given during police training. Also of interest, a high school diploma requirement, deemed invalid for coal-mining and paper mill jobs in the Griggs and Albemarle cases, was deemed valid for police officer jobs in the Washington v. Davis case. In Connecticut v. Teal (1982), a written test was the first of several hurdles for promotion. After all hurdles were completed, the promotion rate was higher for African Americans (22.9%) than Caucasians (13.5%). However, there was adverse impact for African Americans on the first hurdle. In its defense, the State of Connecticut relied on a provision in Section 1607.4(C) of the UGESP relating to adverse impact of the “total selection process”: If . . . the total selection process does not have an adverse impact, the [EEOC] . . . will not expect a user to evaluate the individual components for adverse impact, or to validate such individual components, and will not take enforcement action based upon adverse impact in any component of that process. [emphasis by author] Rejecting this guidance in a closely divided 5–4 ruling, the Supreme Court favored the plaintiffs on grounds that “Congress never intended to give an employer license to discriminate against some employees on the basis of race or sex merely because he favorably treats other members of the

75

Arthur Gutman

employee’s group.” This remains a critical ruling for police departments because multiple selection criteria are often used in both entry-level and promotion processes.

Adverse Impact and Other Selection Practices Among the four cases in Table 3.2 featuring causes of adverse impact other than written tests, Dothard v. Rawlinson (1977) addressed height and weight requirements for prison guards, and New York City v. Beazer (1979) addressed exclusion of transit authority police officers based on past methadone use. The other two cases, Watson v. Fort Worth Bank (1988) and Wards Cove Packing Co. v. Atonio (1989), addressed subjective cause of adverse impact. As important, Watson and Wards Cove were closely connected to each other and led to a temporary alteration of the Phase 2 defense to adverse impact that was subsequently resolved in the Civil Rights Act of 1991 (or CRA-91).

The Dothard and Beazer Rulings In Dothard v. Rawlinson (1977), minimum height and weight criteria for selection of prison guards adversely impacted female applicants. The State of Alabama argued that these criteria were indicators of strength, an important job-related KSA. However, the Supreme Court struck down the requirement, ruling, “If the job-related quality that the appellants identify is bona fide, their purpose could be achieved by adopting and validating a test for applicants that measures strength directly.”5 The Dothard ruling has directly affected police departments. For example, in Horace v. Pontiac (1980), featuring a height requirement, the city of Pontiac, Michigan, asserted, among other things, that taller police officers meet “less resistance” and gain “greater respect . . . from the general public.” However, the 6th Circuit ruled that “there were acceptable alternative policies which would have accomplished the desired purposes other than the obviously arbitrary 5′ 8″ height standard.” Based on Dothard, municipalities, including the city of Pontiac, subsequently turned to other selection criteria to assess strength and agility. In New York City v. Beazer (1979), the Supreme Court supported exclusion of methadone users from employment as transit authority officers because it is “obvious” that drug addiction threatens the “legitimate employment goals of safety and efficiency.” Coupled with earlier lower court rulings involving public safety concerns (e.g., Spurlock v. United Airlines, 1972), the Beazer ruling made it easier for police departments to defend minimum requirements relating to educational requirements, driving records, and prior drug convictions. For example, in Davis v. Dallas (1985), the 5th Circuit found it was necessary for the Dallas police department to exclude recent drug users from police work on grounds that recent drug use shows a disregard for the law. Additionally, exclusion based on poor driving records was supported based on research indicating that past driving habits predict future driving habits, and a requirement of 45 college credits with C or better grades was supported based on federal commission reports in the 1960s that “a high school education is a bare minimum requirement for successful performance of the policeman’s responsibilities.”

The Watson and Wards Cove Rulings In Watson v. Fort Worth Bank (1988), an African American woman was passed over for promotion four times, each time in favor of a Caucasian applicant, and each time based on subjective ratings by Caucasian supervisors of (1) job performance, (2) interview performance, and (3) past experience. It was not clear how these ratings were scored or combined, but there was clearly “bottom-line” adverse impact for the “total selection process.” Only eight justices heard this case, and they unanimously agreed that adverse impact based on subjective selection criteria is a valid claim.6 However, 76

Legal Issues in Hiring, Promoting Officers

speaking for herself and three other justices, Justice O’Connor proposed changes in the adverse impact scenario. The most important proposal was to weaken the defense burden in Phase 2 so that the defendant need only articulate (or explain) a legitimate business reason for the challenged practices, instead of having to prove that they are job related.7 In Wards Cove v. Atonio (1989), two Pacific Northwest salmon-packing companies had a hiringhall arrangement for selection of unskilled salmon packers, but used word-of-mouth to hire workers for more skilled positions. As a result, Eskimos and Filipinos were overrepresented in the unskilled jobs and underrepresented in the skilled jobs. It is arguable that Wards Cove was more of a pattern or practice case than it was an adverse impact case.8 More important for present purposes, with the addition of Anthony Kennedy to the Supreme Court, there was now a majority of five justices that turned O’Connor’s proposal in the Watson case into case law.

The Aftermath of Wards Cove Congress attempted to overturn Wards Cove (and five other 1989 Supreme Court rulings) in the Civil Rights Restoration Act of 1990 (CRRA-90). However, President George H. W. Bush vetoed CRRA-90, and his veto was nearly overridden. The main reason for the veto was failure by the Democrats and Republicans to agree on the Phase 2 defense burden in Wards Cove. Given the close call in CRRA-90, the two parties compromised in CRA-91, restoring the defense burden originally established in the Griggs and Albemarle cases so that if an identified practice is proven to cause adverse impact in Phase 1, the defendant must prove that the practice is “job related and consistent with business necessity” in Phase 2. Although seemingly a recovery of the original Griggs and Albemarle precedents, as we will witness later in this chapter, at least two circuit courts have parsed the phrases job related and consistent with business necessity so that more is needed than proof of test validity to establish cutoff scores.

Early Lower Court Rulings on Adverse Impact The Griggs ruling was rendered prior to the extension of Title VII coverage to public entities in EEO-72. Having no other avenue for lawsuits prior to 1972, plaintiffs in municipal jobs challenged adverse impact via the Equal Protection Clause of the 14th Amendment. There were several lower court rulings in police and firefighter cases in which Griggs was affirmed under the 14th Amendment. Although the Supreme Court ultimately ruled that adverse impact is not a valid constitutional claim in Washington v. Davis, these pre-Davis rulings established important precedents for adverse impact challenges that influenced post-Davis Title VII rulings. Table 3.3 samples five pre-Davis 14th Amendment rulings and three post-Davis Title VII rulings.

Pre-Davis 14th Amendment Rulings Among the pre-Davis rulings sampled in Table 3.3, two featured fire departments, two featured police departments, and one featured a correctional institution. All but one of these cases (Kirkland) originated prior to EEO-72. The circuit courts supported the plaintiff ’s 14th Amendment claims based on Griggs in each case.

Carter v. Gallagher (1971) Carter v. Gallagher was the first post-Griggs 14th Amendment ruling. The plaintiffs argued that historical discriminatory practices in Minneapolis, Minnesota, created a nearly all-white fire department. Nested within this claim was an adverse impact charge focused on an entrance exam administered to 77

Arthur Gutman Table 3.3 Critical Lower Court Adverse Impact Rulings Between 1971 and 1980 Pre-Davis 14th Amendment Rulings Carter v. Gallagher (1971) Castro v. Beecher (1972) Bridgeport Guardians v. Bridgeport CSC (1973) Vulcan Society v. CSC of New York City (1973) Kirkland v. New York State Correctional Services (1975)

First 14th Amendment challenge to firefighter hiring exam; 8th Circuit upholds challenge, citing absence of job analysis data. First 14th Amendment challenge to police hiring exam; 1st Circuit upholds challenge, citing failure to measure job-related KSAs. 14th Amendment challenge to police entry/promotion exams upheld by 2nd Circuit based on faulty test items. 14th Amendment challenge to entry-level firefighter exam upheld by 2nd Circuit based on then applicable EEOC guidelines. 14th Amendment challenge to civil service exam upheld by 2nd Circuit on “constitutionally acceptable grounds.” Post-Davis Title VII Rulings

NAACP v. Seibels (1980) Guardians v. CSC (1980) Gillespie v. Wisconsin (1985)

Title VII challenge to entry-level police/firefighter exams upheld by 5th Circuit due to faulty criterion validity data. Title VII challenge to entry-level police exam upheld, but important rules for content validity are established. Title VII challenge to state administrator jobs is defeated based on rules established in Guardians v. CSC.

2,404 applicants over a 20-year period. Historically, Caucasians passed at rates varying from 40% to 64.7%. Although there were only 22 identifiable minority applicants over that period (six of whom passed), the district court trial judge ruled that minority applicants did “substantially less well” than Caucasians, and there were many minority applicants who took the exam but could not be identified. The judge struck down the exam, saying, “No effort was ever made prior to the current examination period to analyze the fire fighter examinations to determine whether they were culturally biased or whether they were valid predictive instruments for use in selecting fire fighters.” The 8th Circuit affirmed this ruling, citing a passage from Griggs that states “what is required by Congress is the removal of artificial, arbitrary, and unnecessary barriers to employment when the barriers operate invidiously to discriminate on the basis of racial or other impermissible classification.”

Castro v. Beecher (1972) Castro v. Beecher was the first post-Griggs police case. The case featured a 100-item multiple- choice hiring test measuring word knowledge, numerical sequence, reading comprehension, reasoning, arithmetic, and analogy. The test had a passing score of 70, and the passing rate was 65% for Caucasians as compared to 25% for African Americans and 10% for Hispanics. The trial judge ruled that the passing score was arbitrary and the test’s “emphasis on academic and verbal skills [had] little relation to a policeman’s job.” The judge also ruled: There is almost no effort to test the particular types of observation, memory, statement, and judgment which are useful in connection with the exercise of authority of all kinds, and which are necessary for policemen as guardians, administrators, witnesses, community counselors, social welfare aids, and representatives of a regime of law and order. Consistent with the 8th Circuit ruling in Gallagher, the 1st Circuit supported the trial judge based on Griggs, ruling that a test that causes adverse impact must be “substantially related to job performance.” 78

Legal Issues in Hiring, Promoting Officers

Bridgeport Guardians v. Bridgeport CSC (1973) Bridgeport Guardians v. Bridgeport CSC featured challenges to written exams for hiring and promotion of police officers. The test was developed years earlier by a private company. The trial judge ruled that there was adverse impact for the hiring exam but not the promotion exam. The trial judge struck down the hiring exam based on Griggs and the 8th Circuit’s ruling in Beecher, and the 2nd Circuit affirmed, ruling that the biggest problem with the hiring exam was that “many of the vocabulary and arithmetic questions [were] only superficially or peripherally related to police activity.”

Vulcan Society v. CSC (1973) Vulcan Society v. CSC featured a 100-item entry-level test for firefighters that was deemed flawed by the 2nd Circuit based on the EEOC’s then applicable Guidelines on Employee Selection Procedures. Although there were other issues in this case,9 the primary focus was on failure to establish content validity in accordance with the EEOC Guidelines. The 2nd Circuit ruled that the test was developed in an “unprofessional manner.” Among the criticisms noted was that the CSC failed to do a job analysis, the test contained 20 items on civics-related issues that were deemed unrelated to firefighting, and the CSC eliminated a physical exam that was used in prior selection procedures. As we will witness shortly, the Vulcan ruling was a precursor to Guardians v. Civil Service (1980), in which the 2nd Circuit established important precedents for content validity.

Kirkland v. NY State Corrections (1975) Kirkland v. NY State Corrections featured Test 34–944, used by the New York State Department of Civil Service for promotion of correctional officers to sergeant. There were 1,389 applicants across the state, and the passing rate was 30.8% for Caucasians as compared to 7.7% for African Americans and 12.5% for Hispanics. As in its prior rulings in the Bridgeport Guardians and Vulcan cases, the 2nd Circuit, citing Griggs, ruled: Proof in employment discrimination cases proceeds from effect to cause. Plaintiffs establish the racially disparate consequences of defendants’ employment practices, and defendants must then justify such consequences on constitutionally acceptable grounds. [emphasis added] Thus, the 2nd Circuit treated the Phase 2 defense outlined in Griggs as the “constitutionally acceptable grounds” for defending adverse impact.

Post-Davis Title VII Rulings Among the post-Davis cases featured in Table 3.3, NAACP v. Seibels (1980) established important precedents for criterion validity studies, and Guardians v. CSC (1980) and Gillespie v. Wisconsin (1985) established important precedents for content validity studies.

NAACP v. Seibels (1980) In NAACP v. Seibels, written exams were used for entry-level police and firefighter jobs. The exams were correlated with three criteria, most notably training academy performance, for which there were positive and statistically significant correlation coefficients as in Washington v. Davis. However, the test in Davis was designed to measure minimum skills for succeeding in academy school, whereas 79

Arthur Gutman

the test in Seibel was used for strict rank for actual hiring decisions. The 5th Circuit struck down the Davis defense, ruling: We do not believe the Davis rationale can be extended, as the Board urges, to the general proposition that any test can be validated by showing a relationship to training. More specifically, we reject the Board’s suggested extension of the Davis holding to this case, where the tests were not used to ascertain the minimum skills necessary to complete job-relevant training, but rather were used to rank job applicants according to their test scores and to select only the highest test scorers for job placement. [emphasis added] As important for later cases, the correlations for the other two criteria (efficiency ratings and experimental ratings) were either mixed (i.e., both positive and negative correlations) or not statistically significant. Critically, one of the tests (10-C for firefighters) was rejected even though the correlation was positive (+.21) and statistically significant because it lacked practical significance. Accordingly: With respect to the 10-C test, the court found that there is a statistically significant correlation between test scores and experimental ratings, but that the correlation is of very low magnitude and lacks practical significance. [emphasis added] Other courts have since affirmed the need for practical significance in criterion validity studies, requiring, in many cases, correlation coefficients of +.30 or higher.10

Guardians v. Civil Service Commission (CSC) (1980) Guardians v. CSC was a landmark case that addressed content validity as a basis for strict rank ordering and cutoff scores. In-house personnel in the New York City CSC developed an entry-level police test, and based on test scores, applicants were rank ordered, and a passing score sufficient to generate the required number of potential trainees was established. The plaintiffs challenged content validity as a basis for rank ordering, as well as the cutoff score chosen. The fact that the test was developed in-house ultimately worked to the disadvantage of the CSC. The challenge to content validity and rank ordering was based on Section 1607C(1) and Section 14(C)(9) of the UGESP. Section 1607C(1) contains the following warning with respect to relying “solely or primarily” on content validity to support “mental processes”: A selection procedure based on inferences about mental processes cannot be supported solely or primarily on the basis of content validity. Thus, a content strategy is not appropriate for demonstrating the validity of selection procedures which purport to measure traits or constructs such as intelligence, aptitude, personality, common sense, judgment, leadership and spatial ability. [emphasis added] Additionally, Section 14(C)(9) of the UGESP contains a passage stating that “rank-ordering should be used only if it can be shown that a higher score is likely to result in better job performance.” The 2nd Circuit ruled that content validity is appropriate for validating tests of “mental processes,” and outlined the five requirements for proving job relatedness and using rank ordering based on content validity: 80

Legal Issues in Hiring, Promoting Officers

1. 2. 3. 4. 5.

suitable job analysis reasonable competence in test construction test content related to job content test content representative of job content scoring systems selecting applicants that are better job performers

On the issue of cutoff scores, the 2nd Circuit addressed a passage from Section 1607.5H of the UGESP, stating: Where cutoff scores are used, they should normally be set so as to be reasonable and consistent with normal expectations of acceptable proficiency within the workforce. Where applicants are ranked on the basis of properly validated selection procedures and those applicants scoring below a higher cutoff score than appropriate in light of such expectations have little or no chance of being selected for employment, the higher cutoff score may be appropriate, but the degree of adverse impact should be considered. [emphasis added] In interpreting this passage, the 2nd Circuit emphasized the importance of professional expertise and logical breakpoints to establish cutoff scores. Accordingly: As with rank-ordering, a criterion-related study is not necessarily required; the employer might establish a valid cutoff score by using a professional estimate of the requisite ability levels, or, at the very least, by analyzing the test results to locate a logical “break-point” in the distribution of scores. [emphasis added] Ultimately, the CSC failed on all counts because the in-house test constructors were deemed to lack competence in test construction. However, other defendants profited from the Guardians ruling, most notably the State of Wisconsin.

Gillespie v. Wisconsin (1985) Gillespie v. Wisconsin featured state personnel jobs, but it is as relevant to police selection as any other early lower court ruling. Relying on the 1974 Standards for Educational and Psychological Tests and Anastasi’s (1982) Psychological Testing text, the 7th Circuit opined that criterion validity is often difficult to accomplish because of technical factors.11 Accordingly, criterion-related validation is often impracticable because of the limited numbers of employees available for test development and several measurement errors (APA Standards at 27; Anastasi at 433). Thus, neither the Uniform Guidelines nor the psychological literature expresses a blanket preference for criterion-related validity. Following the lead of the 2nd Circuit, the 7th Circuit supported the cutoff score used by the state of Wisconsin based on professional expertise, as two qualified experts in test construction testified that the cutoff score was chosen so as to “interview as many minority candidates as possible” while “assuring that the candidates possessed the minimal skills” needed to perform the job in question. Subsequently, the Guardians and Gillespie rulings were supported in other cases (e.g., Police Officers v. City of Columbus, 1990; Brunet v. City of Columbus, 1995; and Williams v. Ford Motors, 1999). However, as we will witness shortly, agreement among circuit courts on cutoff scores was disrupted by the 3rd Circuit in Lanning v. Southeastern Pennsylvania Transportation Authority (SEPTA) (1999). 81

Arthur Gutman

Recent Rulings on Cutoff Scores and Related Issues Guardians v. CSC and Gillespie v. Wisconsin established two important rules: (1) if a test is content and/ or criterion valid, employers may hire or promote in strict rank order until positions are exhausted; and (2) if strict rank ordering is justified, professional expertise may be used to determine if cutoff scores are “reasonable and consistent with normal expectations of acceptable proficiency within the workforce” in accordance with Section 1607.5(H) of the UGESP. Rule 1 is still applicable across all circuit courts. However, Rule 2 is currently in question based on the 3rd Circuit’s ruling in Lanning v. SEPTA (1999), particularly as it applies to initial steps in multiple-hurdle selection systems. As depicted in Table 3.4, there were four rulings in the Lanning case. Subsequently, the 3rd Circuit’s opinion in Lanning II (Lanning v. SEPTA, 1999) was rejected by the 7th Circuit in Bew v. City of Chicago (2001) and accepted by the 6th in Isabel v. City of Memphis (2005). Additionally, both the Bew and Isabel rulings addressed important questions relating to the so-called 80% rule for proving adverse impact in Section 1607.4(D) of the UGESP.

The Lanning Rulings The Lanning case featured a 1.5-mile run as the first step in a multiple hurdle for entry-level transit authority officers. Applicants were required to make the run (in full gear) in 12 minutes or less. Those who passed completed additional physical fitness tests and those who failed were excluded. There was no question of adverse impact in this case; the pass rate on the 12-minute criterion was 60% for males and only 12% for females. The only major issue in this case was the validity of the 12-minute criterion. In Lanning I (Lanning v. SEPTA, 1998), the trial judge favored SETPA based on job analysis data relating to the importance of aerobic capacity for both officer safety and public safety, and also on criterion validity data on the relationship between aerobic capacity and both successful criminal arrests and commendations for field work. Consistent with the rulings in Guardians v. CSC (1980) and Gillespie v. Wisconsin (1985), the trial judge supported the 12-minute cutoff. Then, in Lanning II (Lanning v. SEPTA, 1999), the 3rd Circuit opined that CRA-91 established new rules for supporting cutoff scores. As written in statutory language in CRA-91, causes of adverse

Table 3.4 Important Recent Rulings on Cutoff Scores Lanning Rulings Lanning v. SEPTA (Lanning I) Lanning v. SEPTA (Lanning II) Lanning v. SEPTA (Lanning III) Lanning v. SEPTA (Lanning IV)

(1998) (1999) (2000) (2002)

Bew v. City of Chicago (2001) Isabel v. City of Memphis (2005)

District court supports 1.5-mile run in first step of a multiple hurdle for entry-level transit authority police. 3rd Circuit remands to determine if 1.5-mile run reflects a minimum qualification for transit authority jobs. District court rules the 1.5-mile criterion does reflect a minimum qualification for transit authority jobs. 3rd Circuit affirms the district court’s support of the 1.5-mile run in Lanning. Post-Lanning Rulings 7th Circuit disagrees with Lanning II ruling as it applies to full-time appointments for probationary police, and rejects the 80% rule as sole basis for proving adverse impact. 6th Circuit agrees with Lanning II ruling as it applies to promotion to police sergeant, and rejects the 80% rule as sole basis for proving adverse impact.

82

Legal Issues in Hiring, Promoting Officers

impact must be “job related for the job in question” and “consistent with business necessity.” The 3rd Circuit ruled that proof of test validity is sufficient for job relatedness, and that the defendant succeeded in this respect. However, the 3rd Circuit also ruled that to be consistent with business necessity, a cutoff score must reflect “minimum qualifications necessary for successful performance of the job in question.” Accordingly: With respect to a discriminatory cutoff score, the business necessity prong of the Civil Rights Act of 1991, 105 Stat. 1071 (1992) must be read to demand an inquiry into whether the score reflects the minimum qualifications necessary to perform successfully the job in question. [emphasis added] However, the 3rd Circuit did not strike down the 12-minute criterion, but rather remanded to the district court with instruction to evaluate the 12-minute criterion based on the newly adopted Lanning II standard. Thereafter, in Lanning III (Lanning v. SEPTA, 2000), the trial judge supported SETPA on the Lanning II standard based on evidence that (a) a lower cutoff would endanger officer and public safety; (b) officers who passed the 12-minute criterion made more successful arrests; and, overall, (c) aerobic capacity, the main reason for using the 1.5-mile run, was not overrepresented for the job of transit authority officer. The 3rd Circuit then affirmed district court ruling from Lanning III in Lanning IV (Lanning v. SEPTA, 2002).

The Bew and Isabel Rulings Unlike Lanning, where adverse impact was clearly established, there were questions in Bew v. Chicago and Isabel v. Memphis relating to the 80% rule for proving adverse impact. As written in Section 1607.4(D) of the UGESP, the 80% rule states: A selection rate for any race, sex, or ethnic group which is less than four-fifths (4/5 ) (or eighty percent) of the rate for the group with the highest rate will generally be regarded by the Federal enforcement agencies as evidence of adverse impact, while a greater than four-fifths rate will generally not be regarded by Federal enforcement agencies as evidence of adverse impact. For example, a selection rate for Caucasians at 70% (e.g., 70 out of 100), requires a selection rate for minorities or women at 80% × 70% = 56% (or 56 out of 100 or higher). However, in a second part of Section 1607.4(D), the UGESP terms the 80% rule a “rule of thumb,” and warns employers not to use it in lieu of other important factors.12 Accordingly: Smaller differences in selection rate may nevertheless constitute adverse impact, where they are significant in both statistical and practical terms or where a user’s actions have discouraged applicants disproportionately on grounds of race, sex, or ethnic group. Greater differences in selection rate may not constitute adverse impact where the differences are based on small numbers and are not statistically significant. It was the second part of Section 1607.4(D) that was prominently featured in both the Bew and Isabel cases. In Bew v. City of Chicago, 5,181 probationary police officers were required to pass a written test with a 66% score or higher in order to graduate to full-time appointments. Applicants were given three chances to pass. Only 33 applicants failed three times (less than 1%), and the pass rates for African Americans (98.24%) and Caucasians (99.96%) were within the 80% boundary. However, 32 of the 33 failures were African American applicants, and a test of independent proportions yielded a 83

Arthur Gutman

Z-score of more than five standard deviations, prompting the 7th Circuit to rule that adverse impact was established.13 However, as critical for the present discussion, the 7th Circuit supported the 66% cutoff score, ruling that CRA-91 did “not distinguish business necessity and job relatedness as two separate standards.” In Isabel v. City of Memphis, a written test was the first hurdle for promotion for 120 applicants for police lieutenant. The original cutoff score of 70% would have violated the 80% rule, so the city reduced the cutoff score to 66%, yielding pass rates for African Americans (46 of 63 = 74.6%) and Caucasians (51 of 57 = 89.5%) that were within the boundaries of the 80% rule. The city argued that there was no adverse impact, but the plaintiffs prevailed based on (a) a significant difference on mean scores for African Americans (69.17) versus Caucasians (75.59), (b) an effect size of D = .9 (which is very large), and (c) a test of independent proportions that resulted in a Z-test difference of 2.35 standard deviations. The trial judge in this case (Isabel v. City of Memphis, 2003) also rejected the defense of the cutoff score based on Lanning II. Accordingly: The Third Circuit has held that “taken together, Griggs, Albemarle and Dothard teach that in order to show the business necessity of a discriminatory cutoff score an employer must demonstrate that its cutoff measures the minimum qualifications necessary for successful performance of the job in question.” Lanning v. SEPTA, 181 F.3d 478 (3d Cir. 1999). In order to be valid, therefore, the cutoff score of 66 must appropriately measure the minimum qualifications necessary for successful performance of the job of lieutenant in the Memphis Police Department. The 6th Circuit then affirmed the district court ruling, including the portion relating to Lanning II in its 2005 ruling. Interestingly, the Lanning II ruling was not that critical to the Isabel ruling. The expert who designed the test for the City of Memphis admitted in open court that the job knowledge component of the test did not represent the full job domain for police lieutenant. Thus, his content validity study was suspect under requirement 4 of Guardians v. CSC (1980) (that test content must be representative of job content). As critical, the expert admitted he “did not condone the usage of a cutoff score” for the promotion process, that he was pressured to do so by the police union, and that the cutoff score adopted was “totally inappropriate,” a “logical absurdity,” and “ludicrous.”14 In short, it is relatively clear that employers should not rely solely on the 80% rule to determine if there is adverse impact. However, unless and until the Supreme Court decides the issue, it is an open question as to whether CRA-91 established a new rule for assessing the validity of cutoff scores.

Methods for Reducing or Eliminating Adverse Impact Table 3.5 depicts four methods for reducing and/or eliminating adverse impact. Methods 1 and 2 (subgroup norming and banding) use statistical manipulations, Method 3 (alternative tests or combination of tests) was endorsed in two recent district court rulings, and Method 4 (manipulating test content) was endorsed in a controversial ruling in Hayden v. Nassau County (1999).

Subgroup Norming Subgroup norming eliminates adverse impact, but does so by using lower cutoff scores for minorities than Caucasians. Subgroup norming was used routinely in the early 1980s by the U.S. Employment Service (USES) for job referrals based on General Aptitude Test Battery (GATB) test scores.15 At the time, subgroup norming was supported by the National Academy of Sciences, but opposed by the Department of Justice (DOJ). However, the debate was abruptly ended by the race-norming 84

Legal Issues in Hiring, Promoting Officers Table 3.5 Methods for Reducing Adverse Impact 1.  Subgroup Norming 2. Banding

3. Alternative Tests or Combinations 4. Manipulating Test Content

Use of different norms for minority and nonminority groups; outlawed in the race-norming proscription in CRA-91. Arranging scores in bands as an alternative to strict rank ordering. Race-neutral banding has been supported in the courts, but race-conscious banding has been limited at best. Valid alternatives with less adverse impact was supported in two district rulings: Bradley v. City of Lynn (2006) and Johnson v. City of Memphis (2006). Elimination of 17 of 25 components on a test supported by the 2nd Circuit in Hayden v. Nassau County (1999).

provision in Section 106(1) of CRA-91, which makes it unlawful to “adjust the scores of, use different cutoff scores for, or otherwise alter the results of, employment-related tests on the basis of race, color, religion, sex or national origin.”

Banding Bands are ranges (or bandwidths) in which test scores are treated as being statistically equal.16 There are two types of banding: race neutral and race conscious. In race-neutral banding, selections are made randomly within bands. Race-neutral banding is generally legal, even if it increases the percentage of minorities relative to strict rank ordering. This occurred, for example, in Chicago Firefighters Local 2 v. City of Chicago (2001) for promotion of firefighters, where the 7th Circuit ruled that random selection within bands is not a form of race norming. Accordingly: If banding were adopted in order to make lower black scores seem higher, it would indeed be a form of race norming, and therefore forbidden. But it is not race norming per se. In fact it’s a universal and normally an unquestioned method of simplifying scoring by eliminating meaningless gradations. [emphasis added] That said, race-neutral banding rarely results in meaningful increases in minority selection as occurred in the Chicago Firefighters case. Race-conscious banding was originally proposed by Cascio, Outtz, Zedeck, and Goldstein (1991) (see also Cascio, Goldstein, Outtz, & Zedeck, 1995). Proponents of race-conscious banding contend it significantly reduces adverse impact with minimal loss in test utility.17 Opposing viewpoints have been expressed by several authors (e.g., Gottfredson, 1994; Sackett & Roth, 1991, Sackett & Wilk, 1994; Schmidt, 1991). The psychometric issues in this debate are complex. For present purposes, it is critical to note that only limited forms of race-conscious banding have been supported in the courts. Most notably, in Bridgeport Guardians v. City of Bridgeport (1991), the 2nd Circuit supported minority preference within bands, but only as one of nine equally weighted criteria. Then, based on the Bridgeport Guardians ruling, the 9th Circuit supported minority preference within bands as one of four equally weighted criteria in Officers for Justice v. Civil Service Commission (1992).18 Critically, neither court supported minority preference as the sole basis for selection, and in the Officers case, race-conscious banding was limited to the last 15 of 115 promotions. In Boston Police Superior Officers v. City of Boston (1998), another notable case, the 1st Circuit upheld promotion of an African American applicant who scored 1 point less than three higher-scoring 85

Arthur Gutman

Caucasians because it was consistent with the demands of a consent decree. The Caucasian officers challenged the consent decree under the 14th Amendment and lost. In short, there are some instances in which limited forms of race-conscious banding have been supported by the courts, particularly when there were consent decrees to resolve past instances of discrimination. However, race as the sole basis for selection within bands has never been supported in any court (see Henle, 2004), and it is highly doubtful that race can serve as the sole basis for largescale selection in view of the race-norming proscription in CR-91, even under consent decrees.

Alternative Selection Procedures Unlike subgroup norming and race-conscious banding, valid alternative selection procedures with less or no adverse impact have a strong legal basis. As noted earlier in this chapter, the Supreme Court created this standard as Phase 3 of the adverse impact scenario in Albemarle v. Moody (1975). It was then adopted as regulatory law in the UGESP and written into statutory law in CRA-91. As depicted in Table 3.4, plaintiffs in have succeeded with Phase 3 proof in two recent district court rulings. In Bradley v. City of Lynn (2006), a written test was used for entry-level firefighters. The trial judge ruled there was adverse impact and insufficient evidence of job relatedness. The judge also cited two valid alternatives: (1) a combination of cognitive tests and physical abilities; and (2) a combination of cognitive tests, personality tests, and biodata. The judge ruled that “while none of these approaches alone provides the silver bullet, these other non-cognitive tests operate to reduce the disparate impact of the written cognitive examination.” Arguably, the City of Lynn was a likely loser in Phase 2 absent the Phase 3 argument. Not so in Johnson v. City of Memphis (2006), where the trial judge ruled that a promotion exam (for police sergeant) was valid, but the plaintiffs prevailed in Phase 3. The ruling was based on the prior development of a valid promotion test in 1996 that resulted in less adverse impact relative to the current exam. The judge ruled, “It is of considerable significance that the City had achieved a successful promotional program in 1996 and yet failed to build upon that success.” It is unclear how much weight should be placed on district court rulings absent circuit court appeals. However, it is clear that there is sufficient case law, regulatory law, and statutory law to instruct employers to consider alternative selection procedures with less adverse impact as they commission and/or develop selection tests.

Manipulating Test Content The events preceding Hayden v. Nassau County (1996) were that in 1977, the DOJ sued Nassau County for adverse impact in an entry-level police exam, and the end result was a consent decree in 1982 to construct an exam that produces no adverse impact, or is valid “in accordance with Title VII and the Uniform Guidelines.” However, exams developed in 1983 and 1987 again resulted in adverse impact (and two new consent decrees). Then, in 1990, the DOJ and Nassau appointed a Technical Design Advisory Committee (TADC) to develop a new exam. The TADC developed and administered a 25-component test to 25,000 candidates. There was “severe” adverse impact after all 25 components were scored. The TADC attempted to eliminate adverse impact completely, but considered the end result invalid. In the exam ultimately adopted, 16 of the 25 components were eliminated, resulting in less adverse impact. However, the subsequent nine-component test was challenged by 68 unsuccessful candidates alleging that they would have been selected if all 25 components were used. The 2nd Circuit ruled “the intent to remedy the disparate impact of the prior exams is not equivalent to an intent to discriminate against non-minority applicants.” The court acknowledged that the decision to “redesign the exam” was race conscious. However, the court reasoned that the exam was 86

Legal Issues in Hiring, Promoting Officers

“scored in a wholly race-neutral fashion” and ruled that the plaintiffs failed to state a claim under the Equal Protection Clause of the 14th Amendment, Title VII, or the race-norming proscription in CRA-91. The Hayden ruling is difficult to interpret, particularly in view of the Supreme Court’s ruling in Ricci v. DeStefano (2009). At the time, Nassau County had the benefit of a court-sanctioned consent degree with the DOJ to resolve multiple lawsuits over a 20-year period. Therefore, at least in the author’s opinion, it is not advisable to use the Hayden procedure absent court approval.

The Supreme Court’s Ruling in the Ricci Case In Ricci v. DeStefano (2009), the New Haven Civil Service Board (CSB) refused to certify promotion exams for firefighter captain and lieutenant, thereby effectively discarding the exam. The CSB argued it had a good-faith belief that it would lose an adverse impact challenge to minorities. Subsequently, 17 Caucasians and one Hispanic sued and ultimately won at the Supreme Court level on grounds that the CSB decision to not certify constituted illegal disparate treatment under Title VII because there was no strong basis in evidence for believing the minority firefighters would prevail.19 There were 41 applicants and seven vacancies for captain and 77 applicants and eight vacancies for lieutenant. The CSB used a “rule of three” in which any of the three highest scoring applicants could be promoted for a given vacancy. This left nine applicants eligible for captain and 10 applicants eligible for lieutenant. As depicted in Table 3.6, there were racial differences on passing rates (a score of 70 or higher). More important, no African Americans and two Hispanics were eligible for promotion to captain, and no African Americans or Hispanics were eligible for promotion to lieutenant. The exams were developed by Industrial-Organizational Solutions (IOS) using a content validity strategy. The CSB sought input from several sources, but relied primarily on one of them (Dr. Hornick) for their good-faith belief that they would lose an adverse impact challenge. Dr. Hornick, a competitor to IOS, testified by telephone. He never reviewed the actual exams. Furthermore, he opined that the IOS exams were valid. However, he also opined that he generally found less adverse impact as compared with the IOS tests using an assessment center approach. The CSB ultimately discarded the exams without requesting a validity report by IOS. They also hired Dr. Hornick to create and validate new exams. In court, the CSB argued that they “cannot be held liable under Title VII’s disparate-treatment provision for attempting to comply with Title VII’s disparate-impact bar.” The plaintiffs, on the other

Table 3.6 Projected Promotions in the Ricci Case Captain Exam (7 Vacancies)

Applicants Passing Score Top 9 Scores

Caucasians

African Americans

Hispanics

25 16 7

8 3 0

8 3 2

Caucasians

African Americans

Hispanics

43 25 10

19 6 0

15 3 0

Lieutenant Exam (8 Vacancies)

Applicants Passing Score Top 10 Scores

87

Arthur Gutman

hand, argued that a “good-faith belief was not a valid defense to allegations of disparate treatment and unconstitutional discrimination.” Critically, the Supreme Court never evaluated written tests versus assessment centers in reaching its ruling. At the district court level, the trial judge ( Janet Bond Arterton) acknowledged that the CSB’s decision was race conscious. Nevertheless, based on Hayden v. Nassau County (1996), Judge Arterton ruled that the noncertification decision was “race neutral.” She ruled further that the “intent to remedy disparate impact of the prior exams is not equivalent to an intent to discriminate against nonminority applicants.” A three-judge panel of the 2nd Circuit then affirmed Judge Arterton’s ruling in a short per curium ruling. Subsequently, a 13-judge panel of the 2nd Circuit refused to further review the case in a close 7–6 ruling. The Supreme Court then agreed to review the case based on a written opinion by the six dissenters. The Supreme Court’s ruling was 5–4, with Justice Kennedy speaking for Justices Alito, Roberts, Scalia, and Thomas. Kennedy ruled there was a race-conscious motive for discarding the test, and strongly suggested that the CSB would have certified the tests if the results were more favorable for minorities. Accordingly: Whatever the City’s ultimate aim—however well intentioned or benevolent it might have seemed—the City made its employment decision because of race. The City rejected the test results solely because the higher scoring candidates were Caucasian. The question is not whether that conduct was discriminatory but whether the City had a lawful justification for its racebased action. [emphasis added] Nevertheless, this was not the sole basis for the majority ruling, as Kennedy ruled further that it is necessary to balance the “tension” between disparate treatment and adverse impact. He rejected a certainty criterion for losing on adverse impact. Accordingly: Forbidding employers to act unless they know, with certainty, that a practice violates the disparateimpact provision would bring compliance efforts to a near standstill. Even in the limited situations when this restricted standard could be met, employers likely would hesitate before taking voluntary action for fear of later being proven wrong in the course of litigation and then held to account for disparate treatment. [emphasis added] Kennedy also rejected a good-faith belief on grounds that it was too minimal, ruling: Allowing employers to violate the disparate-treatment prohibition based on a mere good-faith fear of disparate-impact liability would encourage race-based action at the slightest hint of disparate impact. A minimal standard could cause employers to discard the results of lawful and beneficial promotional examinations even where there is little if any evidence of disparateimpact discrimination. That would amount to a de facto quota system, in which a “focus on statistics . . . could put undue pressure on employers to adopt inappropriate prophylactic measures. [emphasis added] Borrowing from Title VII and 14th Amendment case law from reverse discrimination rulings (e.g., Wygant v. Jackson, 1986; City of Richmond v. Croson, 1989) and invoking the race-norming provision in CRA-91, Kennedy ruled it would be legal to discard an exam if there is a strong basis in evidence for believing an employer would lose on adverse impact. Accordingly: 88

Legal Issues in Hiring, Promoting Officers

If an employer cannot rescore a test based on the candidates’ race, §2000e-2(l), then it follows a fortiori that it may not take the greater step of discarding the test altogether to achieve a more desirable racial distribution of promotion-eligible candidates—absent a strong basis in evidence that the test was deficient and that discarding the results is necessary to avoid violating the disparate-impact provision. . . . For the foregoing reasons, we adopt the strong-basis-in-evidence standard as a matter of statutory construction to resolve any conflict between the disparate-treatment and disparate-impact provisions of Title VII. [emphasis added] Ultimately, reasoning that the CSB did not pass the strong-basis-in-evidence test, Kennedy overturned both lower courts and granted summary judgment for the plaintiffs, thus ending the case. In the dissenting opinion, Justice Ginsburg, speaking for Justices Breyer, Souter, and Stevens, criticized the strong-basis-in-evidence standard on grounds that it was inappropriately incorporated into Title VII adverse impact law based on 14th Amendment reverse discrimination affirmative action cases (Wygant v. Jackson, 1986; Richmond v. Croson, 1988). Accordingly: The cases from which the Court draws its strong-basis-in-evidence standard are particularly inapt; they concern the constitutionality of absolute racial preferences. See Wygant v. Jackson Bd. of Ed., 476 U. S. 267, 277 (1986) (plurality opinion) (invalidating a school district’s plan to lay off nonminority teachers while retaining minority teachers with less seniority); Croson, 488 U. S., at 499–500 22 (rejecting a set-aside program for minority contractors that operated as “an unyielding racial quota”). An employer’s effort to avoid Title VII liability by repudiating a suspect selection method scarcely resembles those cases. Race was not merely a relevant consideration in Wygant and Croson; it was the decisive factor. Observance of Title VII’s disparate-impact provision, in contrast, calls for no racial preference, absolute or otherwise. [emphasis added] Ginsburg endorsed a lighter reasonableness standard, under which the CSB’s “good faith” belief would be acceptable for discarding the test under disparate treatment rules. It is critical to remember that Kennedy’s majority ruling was based entirely on Title VII. No precedents were established for the Equal Protection Clause of the 14th Amendment. Indeed, Kennedy suggested that a favorable Title VII ruling on strong-basis-in-evidence standard could at a later time fail under constitutional law. Accordingly: Our statutory holding does not address the constitutionality of the measures taken here in purported compliance with Title VII. We also do not hold that meeting the strong-basis-inevidence standard would satisfy the Equal Protection Clause in a future case. As we explain below, because respondents have not met their burden under Title VII, we need not decide whether a legitimate fear of disparate impact is ever sufficient to justify discriminatory treatment under the Constitution. [emphasis added] In short, much uncertainty remains related to the strong-basis-in-evidence standard advocated by the Supreme Court. It should be noted that the Office of Contract Compliance Programs (OFCCP) of the Department of Labor, which monitors compliance with Executive Order 11246 on affirmative action, has released guidance that states, among other things, that Ricci will not alter the affirmative action obligations of contractors, and that the OFCCP will continue to use the UGESP to assess adverse impact cases, particularly as it relates to job analysis and test validation procedures. The OFCCP 89

Arthur Gutman

added, however, that it would examine refusals to use tests in accordance with the strong-basis-inevidence standard.20 An additional point to note is that the Chicago Police Department recently considered scrapping its entry tests. According to an article written on January 6, 2010, in the Chicago Sun Times, this action would “bolster minority hiring, save millions on test preparation and avert costly legal battles that have dogged the exam process for decades.”21 While it is unclear how much weight should be given to this newspaper report, the notion that all legal problems are averted by simply eliminating testing procedures is, for reasons to be discussed shortly, not necessarily true.

Conclusions and Recommendations The case law surveyed in this chapter reveals that municipalities face a variety of Hobson’s choices with respect to development and defense of cognitive tests. By the author’s count, there are four ways for a municipality to be sued relating to adverse impact: 1. Adverse impact is proven by minority plaintiffs, and the municipality fails to prove that a challenged test or other selection criterion is job related. 2. The municipality proves job relatedness, but minority plaintiffs prove that a rank cutoff score for selection decisions is unjustified by the evidence offered. 3. The municipality can successfully defend its cutoff score, but minority plaintiffs prove there are other equally valid methods that produce less or no adverse impact to accomplish the same goals. 4. The municipality uses what it believes is the most valid methods with the least amount of adverse impact, but nonminority plaintiffs challenge these methods on grounds of “reverse discrimination” based on preference for minority candidates. Additionally, there is potential liability if a municipality scraps the testing process altogether or reduces adverse impact by other questionable means, and the selection process used results in lessqualified police officers. For example, if, because of negligence or intent, an unqualified officer injures or kills a citizen, the municipality could be liable for remedies associated with wrongful death based on its failure to select only the most qualified applicants for police work. In short, there are good reasons for relying on objective tests and related procedures for hiring and promoting police officers. They represent the best way to make decisions when there are large numbers of applicants. Additionally, many municipalities require such testing. That said, municipalities must be prepared to prove three things: (1) that the tests or other selection criteria used are job related, (2) that there is sufficient evidence for use of rank ordering and/or a specific cutoff score, and (3) that they have examined potentially valid alternative methods that produce less adverse impact before adopting their test strategy. As discussed by Gutman and Dunleavy (2009), the correct reaction to Ricci is to take a proactive approach when developing selection tests. The Ricci ruling had little to do with whether, for example, written tests are better than assessment centers. That question would have been relevant had the New Haven CSB stood by its tests and offered a content validity defense (which likely would have succeeded), and minority plaintiffs launched a Phase 3 proof on valid alternatives with less or no adverse impact. But that did not happen. However, that is precisely the scenario that municipalities must prepare for regardless of which selection procedures are used. What Gutman and Dunleavy (2009) recommend is to establish an advisory committee that includes experts in job analysis and test validation prior to issuing an RFP. The advisory committee should then play a key role in awarding a contract and work with the contractor(s) to ensure that tests and other selection procedures are administered and scored in accordance with the UGESP, and authorities such as the Standards and the SIOP Principles. That would represent an ideal way to 90

Legal Issues in Hiring, Promoting Officers

establish a strong basis in evidence for discarding a test. Of course, if there is a strong basis for discarding a test, there is no reason to administer and score the test to begin with, and wait to see the results to determine if it is acceptable.

Notes 1. Adverse impact is also a valid claim in the Age Discrimination in Employment Act of 1967 (ADEA) based on Supreme Court rulings in Smith v. City of Jackson (2005) and Meacham v. KAPL (2008). However, the rules for deciding ADEA cases are dramatically different as compared to those for Title VII cases. 2. See, for example, Landy (1986) and Bining and Barrett (1989). 3. Readers interested in test validation developments are referred to a comprehensive discussion of the UGESP, Standards, and Principles by Jeanneret (2005). 4. The Supreme Court subsequently ruled that adverse impact is not a valid claim under the Equal Protection Clause of the 14th Amendment in Arlington Heights v. Metropolitan Housing Corp. (1977) and Personnel Administrator v. Feeney (1979). 5. Interestingly, although the State of Alabama could not defend on height and weight criteria in the adverse impact challenge, it succeeded with a BFOQ (Bona Fide Occupational Qualification) defense in excluding all females on grounds that 20% of the population were sex offenders, and it was reasonably necessary to exclude all females because of potential danger to the prison environment. 6. At the time, Anthony Kennedy’s nomination to the Supreme Court was affirmed, but he was not yet seated for the Watson case. 7. The burden to articulate (or explain) without having to prove is termed a burden of “production” and is characteristic of disparate treatment cases such as McDonnell Douglas v. Green (1973). It is generally easier to explain a legitimate business reason than to prove job relatedness. These are complex issues, and the reader is referred to a more comprehensive discussion of these issues by Gutman et al. (2010). 8. For example, in International Teamsters v. United States (1977), a landmark pattern or practice case, African Americans and Hispanics were congregated in lower paying (short distance) bus-driving jobs and Caucasians were congregated in higher paying (longer distance) bus-driving jobs. Gutman et al. (2010) suggest that pattern or practice rules were also applicable to Wards Cove, because minorities were congregated in the salmon-packing jobs and Caucasians were congregated in the more professional jobs. 9. The CSC argued that content validity was unnecessary because their test predicted training school performance. However, there was no evidence of statistical correlations to prove the relationship, as there was subsequently in Washington v. Davis (1976). 10. The r = +.30 standard for practical significance was actually established in NAACP v. Beecher (1974), a pre-Davis case (see, for example, Landy, Gutman, & Outtz, 2010). However, cases supporting this standard emerged in larger numbers after the Seibels ruling, including Clady v. Los Angeles (1985), Zamien v. City of Cleveland (1988), and Hamer v. Atlanta (1989) in the 1980s, and more recent rulings such as Williams v. Ford Motor Company (1999) and U.S. v. Delaware (2004). 11. Technical difficulties associated with criterion validity studies are also discussed by Landy (1986) and Bining and Barrett (1989). 12. Comprehensive discussions of the 80% rule compared to statistical and practical significance for small and large sample sizes are provided by Morris and Lobsenz (2000) and Siskin and Trippi (2005). 13. In Castaneda v. Partida (1977), plaintiffs charged there was underrepresentation of Mexican Americans in a jury pool where the percentage of Mexican Americans chosen was 39% as compared to 79% in the county as a whole. The Supreme Court, based on testimony from social scientists, ruled that a Z-score difference of two standard deviations or higher constitutes a “gross disparity.” This deviation rule was then applied in Hazelwood v. United States (1977) and was subsequently transported into adverse impact cases as the criterion for proving adverse impact. 14. The city made the mistake of administering the second component of the multiple hurdle, and when both hurdles were complete, one of the African American applicants who finished below the cutoff on hurdle 1 ultimately recorded the second-best score on both hurdles combined. 15. Readers interested in a detailed account of the USES procedures are referred to Hartigan and Wigdor (1989) and Sackett and Wilk (1994). 16. The most common method of computing bandwidth is to compute the Standard Error of measurement (SEM) and multiply it by the square root of 2 to obtain the standard error of difference (SED). Then the SED is multiplied by 1.96 (Z-score associated with 95% of variance in a two-tailed statistical test on the normal curve). For example, assuming an SEM of 3.0, the SED = 4.24 equates to bandwidth of 8.31.

91

Arthur Gutman 17. There are three major methods for race-conscious banding: (1) top-down selection of minorities followed by top-down selection of nonminorities until the band is exhausted; (2) random selection for minorities until either minority candidates are exhausted or the proportion of minorities equals the proportion in the applicant pool, after which nonminority candidates are randomly selected; and (3) the bandwidth slides down after the top-scoring minority and nonminority candidates are chosen. 18. The eight factors other than minority preference in the Bridgeport case were departmental needs, job dependability, assignment history and job experience in the department, departmental or special skills training, participation in law enforcement/management seminars and courses, formal university education in law enforcement/management, ability to interact with persons of diverse backgrounds, and overall job performance. The three factors other than minority preference in the Officers case were professional conduct, education and training, and experience. 19. The facts in this case are very complex and are only summarized here. Readers interested in a more detailed account of the Ricci case are referred to Gutman and Dunleavy (2009). 20. The full OFCCP report is available at www.dol.gov/ofccp/regs/compliance/faqs/Ricci_FAQ.htm. 21. The article may be found at www.policeone.com/patrol-issues/articles/1986463-Chicago-police-mayscrapentrance-exam. See www.suntimes.com/news/steinberg/1979744,C ST-NWS-stein08.article and www.suntimes.com/news/commentary/letters/1990161,CST-EDT-vox14.articl for follow-up articles.

References and Further Reading American Educational Research Association, American Psychological Association, & National Council on Measurement in Education. (1974). Standards for educational and psychological testing. Washington, DC: American Educational Research Association. American Educational Research Association, American Psychological Association, & National Council on Measurement in Education. (1999). Standards for educational and psychological testing. Washington, DC: American Educational Research Association. Anastasi, A. (1982). Psychological testing (5th ed.). New York: Macmillan. Bining, J. F., & Barrett, G. V. (1989). Validity of personnel decisions: A conceptual analysis of T. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74(3) 478–494. Cascio, W. F., Goldstein, I. L., Outtz, J., & Zedeck, S. (1995). Twenty issues and answers about sliding bands. Human Performance, 8, 227–242. Cascio, W. F., Outtz, J., Zedeck, S., & Goldstein, I. L. (1991). Statistical implications of six methods of test score use in personnel selection. Human Performance, 4, 233–264. Gottfredson, L. S. (1994). The science and politics of race norming. American Psychologist, 49, 955–963. Gutman, A. (2005). Adverse impact: Judicial, regulatory, and statutory authority. In F. J. Landy (Ed.), Employment discrimination litigation: Behavioral, quantitative, and legal perspectives (pp. 20–46). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Gutman, A., & Dunleavy, E. (2009). The Supreme Court ruling in Ricci v. DeStefano. The Industrial-Organizational Psychologist, 47(2), 57–71. Gutman, A., Koppes, L. L., & Vodanovich, S. J. (2010). EEO law and personnel practices (3rd ed.). New York: Taylor & Francis. Hartigan, J. A., & Wigdor, A. K. (Eds.) (1989). Fairness in employment testing. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Henle, C. A. (2004). Case review of the legal status of banding. Human Performance, 17(4), 415–432. Jeanneret, A. (2005). Professional and technical authorities and guidelines. In F. J. Landy (Ed.), Employment discrimination litigation: Behavioral, quantitative, and legal perspectives (pp. 47–100). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Landy, F. J. (1986). Stamp collecting versus science: Validation as hypothesis testing. American Psychologist, 41(11), 1183–1192. Landy, F. J., Gutman, A., & Outtz, J. (2010). A sampler of legal principles in employment selection. In J. L. Farr & N. T. Tippins (Eds.), The handbook of employee selection (pp. 627–650). New York: Taylor & Francis. Morris, S. B., & Lobsenz, R. E. (2000). Significance tests and confidence intervals for the adverse impact ratio. Personnel Psychology, 53, 89–111. Sackett, P. R., & Roth, L. (1991). A Monte Carlo examination of banding and rank order methods of test scores used in personnel selection. Human Performance, 4(4), 279–295. Sackett, P. R., & Wilk, S. L. (1994). Within group norming and other forms of score adjustment in preemployment testing. American Psychologist, 49, 929–954. Schmidt, F. L. (1991). Why all banding procedures are logically flawed. Human Performance, 4, 265–278. Schneider, J. R., & Schmidt, N. (1992). An exercise design approach to understanding assessment center dimension and exercise constructs. Journal of Applied Psychology, 77(1), 32–41.

92

Legal Issues in Hiring, Promoting Officers Siskin, B. R., & Trippi, J. (2005). Statistical issues in litigation. In F. J. Landy (Ed.), Employment discrimination litigation: Behavioral, quantitative, and legal perspectives (pp. 132–166). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology. (2003). Principles for the validation and use of personnel selection procedures (4th ed.). Bowling Green, OH: Author.

Cases Cited Albemarle Paper Co. v. Moody (1975) 422 US 405. Arlington Heights v. Metropolitan Housing Corp. (1977) 429 US 252. Bew v. City of Chicago (2001) 252 F.3d 891. Boston Police v. City of Boston (CA1 1998) 147 F.3d 13. Bradley v. City of Lynn (2006) 433 F. Supp. 2d 157. Bridgeport Guardians v. Bridgeport CSC (CA2 1973) 482 F.3d 1333. Bridgeport Guardians, Inc. v. City of Bridgeport (CA2 1991) 933 F.2d 1140. Brunet v. City of Columbus (CA6 1995) 58 F.2d 251. Carter v. Gallagher (CA8 1971) 452 F.2d 315. Castaneda v. Partida (1977) 430 US 482. Castro v. Beecher (CA1 1972) 459 F.2d 725. Chicago Firefighters Local Union 2 v. City of Chicago (CA 7 2001) 49 F.3d 649. City of Richmond v. Croson (1989) 488 US 469. Clady v. Los Angeles (CA 9 1985) 770 F.2d 1421. Connecticut v. Teal (1982) 457 US 440. Davis v. Dallas (CA5 1985) 777 F.2d 205. Dothard v. Rawlinson (1977) 433 US 321. Gillespie v. State of Wisconsin (CA7 1985) 771 F.2d 1035. Griggs v. Duke Power Co. (1971) 401 US 424. Guardians of NY v. Civil Service Commission (CA2 1980) 630 F.2d 79. Hamer v. City of Atlanta (CA11 1989) 872 F.2d 1521. Hayden v. Nassau County (CA2 1999) 180 F.3d 42. Hazelwood School Dist. v. United States (1977) 433 US 299. Horace v. Pontiac (CA6 1980) 624 F.2d 765. International Brotherhood of Teamsters v. United States (1977) 431 US 324. Isabel v. City of Memphis (W.D. Tenn. 2003) U.S. Dist. Lexis. Isabel v. City of Memphis (CA6 2005) 404 F.3d 404. Johnson v. City of Memphis (2006) 355 F. Supp. 2d 911. Kirkland v. New York State CSC (CA2 1975) 520 F.2d 420. Lanning v. Southeastern Trans. Auth. (SEPTA) (E.D.Pa. 1998) WL 341605. Lanning v. Southeastern Trans. Auth. (SEPTA) (1999) 181 F.3d 478. Lanning v. Southeastern Trans. Auth. (SEPTA) (E.D.Pa. 2000) U.S. Dist. Lexis 17612. Lanning v. Southeastern Trans. Auth. (SEPTA) (CA3 2002) 308 F.3d 286. McDonnell Douglas v. Green (1973) 411 US 792. Meacham v. Knolls Atomic Power Laboratory (KAPL) (2008) 128 S. Ct. 2395. NAACP v. Seibels (CA5 1980) 616 F.2d 812. New York City v. Beazer (1979) 440 US 568. Officers for Justice v. Civil Service Commission (CA9 1992) 979 F.2d 721. Personnel Administrator v. Feeney (1979) 442 US 256. Police Officers for Equal Rights v. City of Columbus (CA6 1990) 916 F.2d 1092. Ricci v. DeStefano (2009) 129 S. Ct. 2658. Smith v. City of Jackson (2005) 544 US 228. Spurlock v. United Airlines, Inc. (CA10 1972) 475 F.2d 216. United States v. Delaware (D. Del 2004) Lexis 4560. Vulcan Society v. CSC of New York City (CA2 1973) 490 F.2d 387. Wards Cove Packing Co. v. Atonio (1989) 490 US 642. Washington (Mayor, DC) v. Davis (1976) 426 US 229. Watson v. Fort Worth Bank & Trust (1988) 487 US 977. Williams v. Ford Motor Company (1999) 187 F.3d 533. Wygant v. Jackson Board of Education (1986) 476 US 267. Zamien v. City of Cleveland (CA6 1988) 906 F.2d 209.

93

4 ETHICAL ISSUES IN POLICE PSYCHOLOGY Jeni L. McCutcheon

Ethical issues abound in the specialty area of police psychology. These issues bring challenges if not daily, at least regularly, for psychologists who perform work in this growing area. This chapter will look at select ethical dilemmas police psychologists encounter as well as topics of interest and relevance in resolving such dilemmas. Guidance in how to resolve ethical dilemmas will be covered, including identification of various systematic problem-solving models.

Resources to Guide Ethical Practice Ethical dilemmas arise in the practice of police psychology and there are applicable ethical codes of conduct and practice guidelines relevant to practice in the area of police psychology. These are the APA Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct (2002),1 Specialty Guidelines for Forensic Psychologists (1991), other standards, and various guidelines put forth by the Police Psychology Services Section of the International Association of Chiefs of Police (2004, 2006, 2009). The APA Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct is certainly the most widely used and authoritative reference in recognizing and being aware of expectations for ethical practice as a psychologist. The current version of this code was published in 2002 and came into effect in June 2003. This code appears to be here to stay, though there was an amendment of specific codes (1.02 and 1.03) within Standard 1, Resolving Ethical Issues (APA, 2010). This change was approved in February 2010 by the American Psychological Association Executive Council and became effective June 1, 2010, in order to provide greater clarity for and guidance to psychologists in resolving conflicts between ethics and law, regulations, or other governing legal authority, and conflicts between ethics and organizational demands (APA, 2010). In considering the Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct and the specialty area of police psychology, there are general principles to keep in mind. Although not enforceable, these “aspirational” principles are certainly relevant and important, as they represent the highest standards of thinking and conduct to strive for in providing services. In regard to provision of police psychology services, Principle A, Beneficence and Nonmaleficence, means that psychologists seek to do good, attempt to avoid doing harm, look out for the welfare of those with whom they work, and are aware of their influence. Principle B, Fidelity and Responsibility, speaks to psychologists needing to establish trust, loyalty, and an awareness of their professional responsibilities, and work cooperatively with others. Principle C, Integrity, refers to the need for psychologists to be honest in their dealings, truthful in their duties, accurate in their work product, honorable in their commitments, and considerate in enumerating and taking responsibility for 94

Ethical Issues in Police Psychology

possible harmful effects of their work. Principle D, Justice, seeks to inspire in psychologists the need to be fair, exercise sound judgment, and recognize their own biases and limits of their competence. Last, Principle E, Respect for People’s Rights and Dignity, promotes respect and dignity for others, awareness and positive regard for diversity, and an avoidance of going along with or participating in activities that are based on prejudices and may result in harm to others (APA, 2002). Another source of information and guidance in identifying and resolving ethical issues are the Specialty Guidelines for Forensic Psychologists. These guidelines were created by the Committee on Ethical Guidelines for Forensic Psychologists (1991) and are frequently applicable to this area of practice. These guidelines are currently in revision and have been for several years. The Standards for Educational and Psychological Testing (AERA, APA, NCRE, 1999) also provide guidance about the construction, selection, application, and fair and ethical use of psychological testing in employment situations. Though not ethical codes per se, there are other documents that guide the police psychologist toward ethical practice. There are guidelines developed by the Police Psychological Services Section of the International Chiefs of Police (International Association of Chiefs of Police, 2004a, 2004b, 2006a, 2006b, 2009). After extensive work in creating new guidelines, and revising existing guidelines within the section by section psychologists, they are offered to and approved by the Executive Council of Chiefs of Police. The purpose of the guidelines is to assist law enforcement agencies and their psychologists in handling certain types of occurrences and practices within law enforcement agencies. These guidelines help to establish the current and evolving standard of practice in police psychology service provision; they reflect the end result of police psychologists discussing, often working in collaboration with police chiefs and setting forth desirable courses of action for ethical practice in this area. The following guidelines exist: Fitness for Duty Evaluation Guidelines (2009), Officer-Involved Shooting Guidelines (2004a), Peer Support Guidelines (2006a), Pre-Employment Psychological Evaluation Guidelines (2004b), and Guidelines for Consulting Police Psychologists (2006b). The legal aspects of police psychology practice are extensive and complex. Many federal laws impact police psychology practice. Some of these are the Americans with Disabilities Act (ADA), the Health Insurance Portability and Accountability Act (HIPAA), and the Genetic Information Nondiscrimination Act (GINA), which limits inquiry regarding family medical history and genetic information. Additionally, various tort and case laws such as Pettus v. Cole (1996) and Jaffee v. Redmond (1996) influence police psychology practice. Although employing knowledge of both ethics and legal issues often goes hand-in-hand in choosing a best course of action in handling ethical questions, legal issues relevant to police psychology practice are not specifically addressed in this chapter. Another chapter within this handbook provides thorough coverage of the extensive statutes, laws, and regulations that affect this area of practice. When mentioned in this chapter, laws are referenced only to make ethical examples richer for the reader. Relatedly, while good ethical practice is a step toward risk management, it should be noted that this chapter is aimed specifically at discussing ethical issues in police psychology rather than being a risk management practice “how-to” guide.

Common Ethical Dilemmas Research regarding ethical dilemmas encountered by police psychologists is scarce. Zelig (1988) surveyed police psychologists and found that dilemmas commonly involved six often-overlapping categories: confidentiality, resolving conflicts between professional standards and the needs of the organization, dual relationships, use and administration of psychological tests, problems with the agency overruling the psychologist’s recommendations, and “other” dilemmas. In Zelig’s study, “confidentiality” was the most commonly cited ethical dilemma, with “managing conflicts between 95

Jeni L. McCutcheon

the psychologist’s professional standards and the needs of the organization” and “dual relationships” rounding out the top three most commonly cited dilemmas. Some psychologists reported that they did not encounter any ethical dilemmas. Zelig’s findings are consistent with psychologist-identified ethical dilemmas as a whole. Pope and Vetter (1992) surveyed members of the American Psychological Association. Their survey revealed that the top two ethical dilemmas experienced by psychologists involved “confidentiality” and “dual relationships.” This study was replicated in Sweden with the same findings; the top two ethical dilemmas encountered by Swedish psychologists were confidentiality and blurred, dual, or conflictual relationships (Colnerud, 1997). Slack and Wassenaar (1999) found the same results in surveying South African clinical psychologists. Pettifor and Sawchuk (2006) reviewed other research and similar studies internationally and ascertained that among nine primarily Western countries—the United States, United Kingdom, Norway, Finland, Canada, Sweden, New Zealand, South Africa, and Mexico—confidentiality and dual relationships were among the most often-cited areas of ethical dilemmas encountered by psychologists. Despite the scarcity of research, it is reasonable to believe that a majority of police psychologists would cite ethical dilemmas as an ongoing and routine challenge. Stemming back 25-plus years, articles and texts in the area of police psychology have cited ethical concerns as important to consider. Attention to ethical issues and resolving ethical dilemmas are certainly far from new and novel concepts (Dietz & Reese, 1986; Inwald, 1984; Reese, 1987; Reese & Goldstein, 1986). At police conferences, ethical aspects of police psychology have been addressed through interactive ethical case scenario exercises (Corey & Trompetter, 2009) and at formal conference presentations (Corey & Trompetter, 2009; Gelles, Morgan, & Moorehead, 2006). Also, in the creation and revisions of the guidelines for police psychologists through the International Association of Chiefs of Police, there has been attention paid toward ethical practice. As an example, Section 8.2 of the Fitness for Duty Evaluation Guidelines (2009) reads: As part of the informed consent process, the examiner clarifies who the client is and communicates this to all involved parties at the outset of the evaluation. Regardless of who is identified as the client, the examiner owes an ethical duty to both parties to be fair, impartial, accurate and objective, and to honor the parties’ respective legal rights and interests. (p. 4)

Ethical Dilemmas and Challenges Competence Several areas of the APA Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct (2002), such as Standard 2: Competence, apply to police psychologists. As examples: 2.03 Maintaining Competence Psychologists undertake ongoing efforts to develop and maintain their competence. 2.04 Bases for Scientific and Professional Judgments Psychologists’ work is based on established scientific and professional knowledge of the discipline. 2.05 Delegation of Work to Others Psychologists who delegate work to employees, supervisees, or research or teaching assistants or who use the services of others, such as interpreters, take reasonable steps to (1) avoid 96

Ethical Issues in Police Psychology

delegating such work to persons who have a multiple relationship with those being served that would likely lead to exploitation or loss of objectivity; (2) authorize only those responsibilities that such persons can be expected to perform competently on the basis of their education, training, or experience, either independently or with the level of supervision provided; and (3) see that such persons perform these services competently. These standards speak to the need for police psychologists to receive adequate training in this practice area. Police psychologists need to keep up with advancements in their field, stay apprised of pertinent research and literature, and be aware of revised guidelines specific to the area of police psychology as well as be knowledgeable of federal and state laws and case laws that impact police psychology practice. The ethical codes and laws also speak to the need to assume full responsibility for those working under the police psychologist’s license, such as where allowed for by law, when practicum students and predoctoral interns and postdoctoral residents are volunteers or employees of contract psychologists or of police agencies. Potential ethical dilemmas in this area can arise in a multitude of ways. Consider the self- identified police psychologist who does not belong to or attend any of the annual conferences of the three major police psychologist organizations: the International Association of Chiefs of Police, Police Psychological Services Section (IACP PPSS); the Society for Police and Criminal Psychology (SPCP); and the American Psychological Association (APA), Division 18, Police and Public Safety Section. Consider the psychologist who uses outdated psychological tests or employs assessment measures not typically used in assessment of police populations for pre-employment evaluations (and are not validated or normed based on these populations). Consider the intern who does not keep up in a timely manner on report-writing tasks and updating clinical notes. Consider the psychologist who performs unwarranted fitness-for-duty evaluations on returning veterans simply because the agency thinks this would be a good idea. As with most competence issues, these likely are not purposeful deviations from the standard of practice, but actions taken based on a lack of education and knowledge.

Confidentiality Several areas of the APA Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct (2002), such as Standard 4: Privacy and Confidentiality, are applicable to police psychology: 4.01 Maintaining Confidentiality Psychologists have a primary obligation and take reasonable precautions to protect confidential information obtained through or stored in any medium, recognizing that the extent and limits of confidentiality may be regulated by law or established by institutional rules or professional or scientific relationship. 4.02 Discussing the Limits of Confidentiality (a) Psychologists discuss with persons (including, to the extent feasible, those legally incapable of giving informed consent and their legal representatives) and organizations with whom they establish a scientific or professional relationship (1) the relevant limits of confidentiality and (2) the foreseeable uses of the information generated through their psychological activities. (b) Unless it is not feasible or contraindicated, the discussion of confidentiality occurs at the outset of the relationship and thereafter as new circumstances may warrant. (c) Psychologists who offer services, products, or information via electronic transmission inform clients/patients of the risks to privacy and limits of confidentiality. 97

Jeni L. McCutcheon

4.04 Minimizing Intrusions on Privacy (a) Psychologists include in written and oral reports and consultations, only information germane to the purpose for which the communication is made. (b) Psychologists discuss confidential information obtained in their work only for appropriate scientific or professional purposes and only with persons clearly concerned with such matters. 4.05 Disclosures (a) Psychologists may disclose confidential information with the appropriate consent of the organizational client, the individual client/patient, or another legally authorized person on behalf of the client/patient unless prohibited by law. (b) Psychologists disclose confidential information without the consent of the individual only as mandated by law, or where permitted by law for a valid purpose such as to (1) provide needed professional services; (2) obtain appropriate professional consultations; (3) protect the client/patient, psychologist, or others from harm; or (4) obtain payment for services from a client/patient, in which instance disclosure is limited to the minimum that is necessary to achieve the purpose. These codes point to the need for the police psychologist to keep confidences, define who the client is, and obtain informed consent. It is vital not only for ethical practice but also for the credibility of the police psychologist to keep private (except as otherwise mandated by law) the information and disclosures shared within the context of a relationship. Trouble can arise in this area when the relationship between the psychologist and the client is unclear, never really identified or clarified, and/or not made explicit and transparent to the involved parties. Consider, for example, the applicant who shows up for a pre-employment evaluation thinking he or she already has the job and “because you are a psychologist, I thought what I tell you is private.” Other ethical dilemmas can arise in related areas, when there are questions of who owns the records, who gets the report(s), what is private and what is not as far as what is said in the course of an evaluation, and, more to the point, what sensitive and private disclosures are germane and end up in a report, and which are screened out with only general comments made. To elaborate, ponder that in the process of pre-employment evaluations, applicants are routinely asked highly personal questions about their background, substance abuse, mental health and medical histories, but (unless required by state statute) are not at all privy to how they performed in these evaluations. To psychologists not skilled in police psychology or in forensic evaluative areas, the idea that you do not provide feedback to the very person who spent time and effort taking these tests seems counterintuitive. In this case, the agency holds the confidentiality because they are the client agency and the applicant is the subject of the evaluation. In concert with the obligation to provide relevant information to the agency so that they can make an informed hiring decision, it is also the psychologist’s ethical responsibility to provide only germane, salient data in the report and to the hiring authority. As stated previously, the ethical codes guard against invasion of privacy. As an example, the fact that an applicant engaged in a sex act at work is a relevant detail. Additionally, that he or she engaged in a sex act with a coworker at work is also pertinent. However, the fact that the sex act involved a coworker and that both he or she and the coworker are married but not to each other does not need to be included in the report details. It is becoming more the case that psychologists recognizing that only relevant, job-related behaviors be included in reports include only relevant data, observations, and test results. Typically therefore, long histories such as those routinely included in the psychosocial history section of traditional psychological full-battery reports are not included. “Superfluous data which are not reasonably tied to the purpose for which the evaluation 98

Ethical Issues in Police Psychology

was conducted, such as diagnostic impressions or the inclusion of lengthy personal, social and family histories[,] should not be routinely included in pre-employment psychological evaluation reports” (Super, 1997, p. 4). As the development of laws are often reactive from ethical issues that arise, the introduction of the Genetic Information Nondiscrimination Act (GINA) seems to reinforce that not only is it ethically the trend to include only the most relevant job-related details in reports, but also now specific details about an applicant’s personal family history are not to be included in reports and cannot be part of the inquiry in employment-related testing and evaluations. In regard to how to handle confidentiality in police psychology practice, it is this author’s beginning stance that as far as clinical contacts and services, unless there is a mandated treatment referral situation or an exception to confidentiality by law, all contacts are confidential. As an example, the fact that someone called to inquire about services is confidential even if he or she never follows through and seeks therapy. Similarly, any disclosures made during ride-alongs, facility visits, or briefings are considered confidential. Whenever evaluation, crisis intervention, and formal therapy services are provided, it is good practice to provide and obtain verbal and written informed consent. And, in situations where there is a lack of guaranteed confidentiality such as the psychologist keeping confidentiality but peer-support attendees perhaps having to report a policy-violation disclosure up their chain of command in a critical incident stress management team intervention, proceeding with caution is important. Specifically, because peer support and group debriefing services are not guaranteed to be confidential and considered a privileged communication (in most states), it is prudent for the psychologist to share these limitations, but then to ask attendees to maintain confidentiality. Where there are limited aspects of confidentiality, such as in confirming attendance at a post-officerinvolved shooting one-to-one meeting or in cases of mandated therapy treatment, it is important to be open and transparent about these limitations. Similarly, in the course of evaluations, it is vital to share openly when, and if, there is confidentiality. A point to bear in mind is not just the psychologist’s personal actions, but the environment in which the psychologist works. When one works as an in-house police psychologist, it may be the case that they will encounter past, current, and future clients and evaluation subjects each day. It is vital in situations like these that what is known about these individuals is kept compartmentalized and private. Stating this at the first contact and asking for feedback is often helpful. Additionally, if working within an in-house unit, other nonpsychologists may need encouragement and education to value privacy and confidentiality as well. For instance, when a friendly and sociable lieutenant comes to visit and say hello, he or she may have to be gently reminded to avoid greeting coworkers who are visiting your unit seeking professional services with a loud, “Hey, hello, Sergeant! What are you doing here?!”

Dual Relationships, Role Conflict, and Boundary Issues Multiple areas of the Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct (2002) Standard 3: Human Relations are relevant. These include: 3.04 Avoiding Harm Psychologists take reasonable steps to avoid harming their clients/patients, students, supervisees, research participants, organizational clients, and others with whom they work and to minimize harm where it is foreseeable and unavoidable. 3.05 Multiple Relationships (a) A multiple relationship occurs when a psychologist is in a professional role with a person and (1) at the same time is in another role with the same person, (2) at the same time is in 99

Jeni L. McCutcheon

a relationship with a person closely associated with or related to the person with whom the psychologist has the professional relationship, or (3) promises to enter into another relationship in the future with the person or a person closely associated with or related to the person. A psychologist refrains from entering into a multiple relationship if the multiple relationship could reasonably be expected to impair the psychologist’s objectivity, competence, or effectiveness in performing his or her functions as a psychologist or otherwise risks exploitation or harm to the person with whom the professional relationship exists. Multiple relationships that would not reasonably be expected to cause impairment or risk exploitation or harm are not unethical. (b) If a psychologist finds that, due to unforeseen factors, a potentially harmful multiple relationship has arisen, the psychologist takes reasonable steps to resolve it with due regard for the best interests of the affected person and maximal compliance with the Ethics Code. (c) When psychologists are required by law, institutional policy, or extraordinary circumstances to serve in more than one role in judicial or administrative proceedings, at the outset they clarify role expectations and the extent of confidentiality and thereafter as changes occur. 3.06 Conflict of Interest Psychologists refrain from taking on a professional role when personal, scientific, professional, legal, financial, or other interests or relationships could reasonably be expected to (1) impair their objectivity, competence, or effectiveness in performing their functions as psychologists or (2) expose the person or organization with whom the professional relationship exists to harm or exploitation. 3.07 Third-Party Requests for Services When psychologists agree to provide services to a person or entity at the request of a third party, psychologists attempt to clarify at the outset of the service the nature of the relationship with all individuals or organizations involved. This clarification includes the role of the psychologist (e.g., therapist, consultant, diagnostician, or expert witness), an identification of who is the client, the probable uses of the services provided or the information obtained, and the fact that there may be limits to confidentiality. 3.08 Exploitative Relationships Psychologists do not exploit persons over whom they have supervisory, evaluative, or other authority such as clients/patients, students, supervisees, research participants, and employees. 3.09 Cooperation With Other Professionals Where indicated and professionally appropriate, psychologists cooperate with other professionals in order to serve their clients/patients effectively and appropriately. 3.10 Informed Consent (a) When psychologists conduct research or provide assessment, therapy, counseling, or consulting services in person or via electronic transmission or other forms of communication, they obtain the informed consent of the individual or individuals using language that is reasonably understandable to that person or persons except when conducting such activities without consent is mandated by law or governmental regulation or as otherwise provided in this Ethics Code. (b) For persons who are legally incapable of giving informed consent, psychologists nevertheless (1) provide an appropriate explanation, (2) seek the individual’s assent, (3) consider such persons’ preferences and best interests, and (4) obtain appropriate permission from a 100

Ethical Issues in Police Psychology

legally authorized person, if such substitute consent is permitted or required by law. When consent by a legally authorized person is not permitted or required by law, psychologists take reasonable steps to protect the individual’s rights and welfare. (c) When psychological services are court ordered or otherwise mandated, psychologists inform the individual of the nature of the anticipated services, including whether the services are court ordered or mandated and any limits of confidentiality, before proceeding. (d) Psychologists appropriately document written or oral consent, permission, and assent. 3.11 Psychological Services Delivered to or Through Organizations (a) Psychologists delivering services to or through organizations provide information beforehand to clients and when appropriate those directly affected by the services about (1) the nature and objectives of the services, (2) the intended recipients, (3) which of the individuals are clients, (4) the relationship the psychologist will have with each person and the organization, (5) the probable uses of services provided and information obtained, (6) who will have access to the information, and (7) limits of confidentiality. As soon as feasible, they provide information about the results and conclusions of such services to appropriate persons. (b) If psychologists will be precluded by law or by organizational roles from providing such information to particular individuals or groups, they so inform those individuals or groups at the outset of the service. Entire books have been written on the subject of multiple relationships, often because they have been a controversial topic for psychologists and are often tricky to navigate. Multiple relationships occur when the psychologist has more than one relationship with a client. They can also occur when the psychologist is placed in another role where he or she has a relationship with a client as well as a relationship with someone the client is close to or associated with. Additionally, multiple relationships can occur when the psychologist initially has one role or relationship with a client and then enters into a different or new relationship involving the client. The concern with multiple relationships is that they can become problematic when they risk the possibility of harm or exploitation to the client. Traditionally, in psychology and in past ethical codes for psychologists, there has been a push for psychologists to entirely avoid multiple relationships (previously called dual relationships). Thinking is changing slightly in this area such that not all multiple relationships are automatically viewed as harmful and to be avoided. Instead, the likelihood of psychologists developing multiple relationships is increasingly recognized as commonplace if not inevitable. This evolving perspective is much the case in the area of police psychology. Nonetheless, the burden of proof is always on the psychologist that any multiple relationship is in the best interest of the client and that the client is free from undue influence, exploitation, and harm because of their involvement in this relationship. Multiple relationships in police psychology would be difficult to avoid even if one tried, and typically are commonplace and part and parcel of the daily practice of police psychology. These relationships often naturally evolve in the police agency setting, particularly if one is an in-house police psychologist, because of frequent contact and sometimes even daily contact with their clients and patients. Consider the therapy patient who is selling raffle tickets for an office event, or the past therapy patient whom you work out next to and exchange pleasantries with in the agency gym, or the person you provided crisis intervention services coincidentally being the designated person when you are out of the office on a facility or district tour. Consider the person who was a subject in a fitness-for-duty evaluation you performed who now works in a division and is the person contracting your consultation services about how to handle a personnel action. Though not all police psychologists may share this view, it is this author’s opinion that with careful regard to avoid harm and exploitation, multiple relationships with clients within a police agency 101

Jeni L. McCutcheon

may actually be a positive influence not only for the client involved, but for the psychologist and his or her credibility as well. Word-of-mouth referrals indicate trust. Positive interactions with agency personnel lead to more referrals, not just because people want to send work your way, but also because you have earned their trust. This is not so different from the mindset of the psychologist who works in small or rural communities where your grocer, dentist, and neighbor may all share personal and professional relationships with you, and your lives routinely intersect. Think about the following example. Your unit of police psychologists spends an evening with the K-9 squad for an outreach and education experience. Here you shadow the squad, spend time learning what they do firsthand, and participate in training with them, even donning a bite-suit and being a decoy for the dogs in a training exercise. The psychologist role was that of learner and participant. In the course of spending time with this squad, familiarity ensued, and jokes about the psychologists’ toughness (or lack thereof, really, as the dogs yanked us to the ground) were exchanged. Part of that night involved being a passenger in high-speed driving exercises for pursuit practice for both the officers and the handling of their dogs. As the night progressed, several of the officers opened up about their thoughts and personal issues. Although there was not any sort of patient–psychologist privileged relationship, it could be considered therapeutic, and certainly the details of their disclosures were kept confidential. What happened next is that members of this squad sought out police psychologists for consultation several times over the next year in regard to behavioral and psychological issues. Additionally, when there was an officer-involved shooting incident and many of these officers came for their mandated one-to-one meeting, they felt comfortable and familiar with the psychologists and talked freely. In the context of each relationship, it was important to clarify our role, acknowledge past contact, and invite feedback. To offer more role confusion to ponder, consider that when these officers rise in the ranks, as is typical in law enforcement agencies, it is conceivable that these officers, the past teachers and trainers in one setting who then become seekers of consultation and then akin to clinical patients protected by privilege after a shooting incident, could then become the psychologist’s nonpsychologist commander when they assume command of a division the unit is organized under. Other examples that bring to light the intricacies of multiple relationships in police psychology are as follows. Consider an example where a police psychologist goes on a ride-along with an officer and this officer later becomes a clinical client seeking therapy services. Or, when a police psychologist goes on a ride-along with an officer previously unknown to them, and then the entire squad gets together at a restaurant to eat dinner and the psychologist ends up sitting face-to-face with one of her current or recent clinical therapy patients or someone she performed a fitness-for-duty evaluation on several years ago. In such situations, it is important that the psychologist be adaptable and willing to flex roles. It is also important that the psychologist be aware, look for, and be considerate of what is in the best interest of the client. This means safeguarding and maintaining confidentiality, clarifying roles as much as possible, and finding ways to remove oneself when it appears or it is revealed that a client is uncomfortable. There are a myriad of ways to politely and expediently avoid such multiple relationship situations; it is this author’s stance that such measures should be taken if contact by the psychologist appears at all detrimental to the client. As a counterpoint, an alternative view is presented. In the absence of the multiple relationships causing harm to the client, one might consider it inappropriate in police culture, or possibly viewed as self-serving and disrespectful, to suddenly say in any of these situations something to the effect of “I need to avoid having another sort of professional relationship with you since we already have met in one context or already have just one established relationship and it needs to stay that way.” One could wonder whether it is the existence of multiple relationships in some circumstances that leads to greater comfort, greater collegiality, and greater ease for clients in dealing with a police psychologist. Perhaps such connections may be something to seek out and to nurture, instead of avoid. Gelber (2003) stated this well: 102

Ethical Issues in Police Psychology

Although the traditional professional ethics code for the psychologist discourages “dual relationships,” contemporary revisions have recognized not only the inevitability but also the necessity of psychologists’ working with both line officers and managers. Whether they are participating in supervisor’s meetings or celebrating at promotional dinners, the bureau psychologists are viewed as an integral part of the division and must adapt to the multifaceted nature of their relationships with department personnel. (“LAPD Psychologists Hit the Streets,” para. 16) Additional commentary on out-of-office experiences and multiple relationships is reflected on by Zur (2001). He describes contact with clients out of the office as either carried out as part of a treatment plan or where the experience is “geared to enhance therapeutic effectiveness.” This author’s examples of contact as part of a treatment plan include: (1) having a cup of coffee with a police officer clinical client just prior to his oral advancement board examination to provide lastminute support after a series of therapy sessions aimed at improving confidence and decreasing anxiety related to his board examination; and (2) at the agency’s workout facility, meeting with officers to practice positive self-talk and teach “what-if ” thinking skills prior to law enforcement training academy exercises such as a defensive tactics exercise, or a capsaicin spray/foam exposure exercise to help decrease dropout from the academy. This author’s examples of contact to enhance therapeutic effectiveness include: (1) attending the funeral of a police staff member you did not know, to support the therapy you are doing with his surviving family members; and (2) responding to the scene of an officer-involved shooting incident where one of your patients is having a hard time and upon arrival to the scene you provide support to the employee and his squad members. Zur also details a third type of out-of-office experiences and multiple relationships as encounters that naturally occur as a matter of course of being part of the same community and truly are dual relationships. This author’s examples include: (1) running into a client at the private gym where you both live and work out; and (2) learning that a friend in your circle of friends is related to a past therapy patient. In managing role conflicts, a timeless set of suggestions come from Dietz and Reese (1986). Among their suggestions are to provide their agencies with a copy of the ethical principles of psychologists and code of conduct, always identify your profession and client in your work in police psychology settings, obtain fully informed consent, be mindful of confidentiality as it applies to each situation based on who the client is, and stay grounded in “one’s parent discipline to avoid over-identification with law enforcement” (p. 95). Janik (1994) stressed also the need for the police psychologist to be highly involved with the police agency and to avoid overidentifying, which may lead to a blurring of professional boundaries. In figuring out how to resolve multiple relationship dilemmas, there is often no clear, correct answer about how to proceed. Considering what is in the best interest of the client is surely the foremost concern. This ought to involve considering always, if known or able to be queried, the client’s take on the situation, and whether an honest and open relationship exists or if there is a difference in real or perceived power that will result in a less than forthcoming answer from the client. It may also be helpful to consider the nature and quality of the previous relationship, how the relationship ended or terminated, and whether it is still going on. A best practice approach seems to be to discuss, share openly, and invite feedback and reactions from the client about how the role and relationship are changing. Finally, when able, keeping boundaries as clear as possible is often the best move. It is desirable and wise to try to structure the relationship as early on as possible, preferably at the first contact. When you can refer somebody to another professional and avoid a multiple relationship, this may be something to seriously consider. And, another simple step is to practice what appears to be a commonsense move: to always define and try to state aloud the role you are being asked to assume to those involved. This might look like saying aloud, “I hear you have concerns about Officer A and 103

Jeni L. McCutcheon

you are trying to figure out if you need an evaluation to determine his fitness for duty?” or instead inquiring, “I hear you saying that you are worried about Officer A. Would you like to refer him to me for therapy services?” Or, this might look like saying something to the effect of, “So, Chief, what I hear you saying is that you have such-and-such situation and you would like my input, as far as consultation about how you might consider proceeding regarding your concern about Officer A. Does that sound right?” In doing so, there is room for clarification and questions, so that a role is discussed and agreed on before proceeding. As discussed in previous examples, there also needs to be an awareness and flexibility with the fact that previously designated roles might change.

Identifying the Client Hand in hand with the issues of dual relationships, role conflict, and boundary issues is recognizing who the client is in the course of police psychology practice. This is important in order to figure out where the psychologist’s loyalties lie, who services are being provided to, and how confidentiality is to be held. As a long-ago task force in looking at the role of psychology in the criminal justice system eloquently stated, without likely the prescience that over 40 years later this would still be a pertinent issue: When psychologists do try seriously to articulate who their client is—where their loyalties are to be given—in criminal justice, they sometimes appear to be under the impression that they are constrained to a multiple-choice answer, with the alternatives being (a) the “system” (or “society”) and (b) the offender (or police officer or defendant, as the case may be). It appears to us that there is no need for psychologists to impale themselves on the horns of this dilemma, since “Who is the client?” is not a multiple-choice question. It requires an essay answer. (Task Force on the Role of Psychology in the Criminal Justice System, 1978, p. 1102) Police psychology is not the only area that struggles with multiple pulls regarding “Who is the client?” Kennedy and Johnson (2009) report that ethical dilemmas where there are multiple and sometimes competing obligations are common for psychologists in managed care, educational, governmental, and correctional settings. Because of such competing demands, military psychology has a similar concept that may be applicable to police psychology. They use the term mixed agency. In military psychology, the concept of mixed agency is “the simultaneous commitment to two or more entities” and is described as “a day-to-day matter” that military psychologists grapple (Kennedy & Johnson, 2009, p. 22). Such instances include fitness-for-duty evaluations, working with detainees, and providing consultation in regard to interrogation and counterintelligence issues. A survey of U.S. Air Force psychologists found, consistent with police and general psychology research, that the top ethical dilemmas encountered by these military psychologists involved conflicts between ethics and organizational demands, maintaining confidentiality, and multiple relationships (Orme & Doerman, 2001). Kennedy and Johnson (2009) identify three ways to solve mixed-agency dilemmas. The first involves following procedures exactly such that “one adheres strictly to the letter of the law” (p. 24). The second method involves a stealth approach where the “psychologist attempts to resolve mixedagency quandaries by quietly thwarting ethically problematic legal requirements in favor of full adherence to the APA Ethics Code” (p. 24). The authors credit Johnson and Wilson (1993) for this approach. The third strategy detailed to manage mixed-agency dilemmas is the “best interest approach,” whereby the psychologist attempts to promote the best interests of each person or organization with the idea that this approach “may offer the greatest hope of avoiding harm to any person or organizational entity while balancing obligations to both the Ethics Code and federal regulations” 104

Ethical Issues in Police Psychology

(p. 24). Certainly whether these strategies, particularly the second and third, are applicable to police psychology remains to be seen, but they are offered here as another way one might go about trying to wrap his or her mind around how to negotiate complex ethical dilemmas in police psychology.

Dealing With Organizational Demands Two areas of the APA Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct (2002) are pertinent here: 1.03 Conflicts Between Ethics and Organizational Demands If the demands of an organization with which psychologists are affiliated or for whom they are working are in conflict with this Ethics Code, psychologists clarify the nature of the conflict, make known their commitment to the Ethics Code, and take reasonable steps to resolve the conflict consistent with the General Principles and Ethical Standards of the Ethics Code. Under no circumstances may this standard be used to justify or defend violating human rights. 3.09 Cooperation With Other Professionals When indicated and professionally appropriate, psychologists cooperate with other professionals in order to serve their clients/patients effectively and appropriately. Dealing with organizational demands can be challenging work as a police psychologist. Ethical conflicts can arise when the psychologist runs up against conflicts between the psychologist’s standard of practice, his or her codes of conduct, and the needs of the organization. Often, what the agency asks for or needs collides with something the psychologist feels is outside what is appropriate to provide and in many cases may actually constitute unethical practice if the psychologist acquiesces. Consider the possibility that a police commander first identifies an officer as a problem employee and directs that a fitness-for-duty evaluation be performed on the basis that the officer is prescribed an antidepressant medication (without any personal or workplace behaviors present that might suggest the need for such an evaluation). Consider requests that psychologists sometimes receive to break confidentiality. Or, the idea of perhaps a change in the rating of a pre-employment candidate. Ponder the idea of a new psychologist who has never been trained in or performed a fitness-for-duty evaluation being told that this is now one of his or her new job duties, to start immediately. Consider the reality of being directed to write a letter stating that an officer seen for a one-to-one post-officerinvolved shooting situation is “fit” to return to work (when the standard of practice defined by IACP guidelines is that these are not fitness evaluations but instead supportive interventions). Imagine that in regard to all the previous situations, the psychologist is told, “Well, the last psychologist we used had no problem with these requests.” In each of these situations, competent police psychologists will know what the appropriate response is, namely, resisting and finding a workable solution based on sound police psychology practice. But the matter of how to do this professionally, without being perceived as insubordinate and in trying to be helpful in educating about what you can provide and why you may not be able to meet the request as it currently stands—well, this is a much more complex matter. Though no police psychologist is immune to dealing with organizational demands, there may be slight differences between psychologists who work as employees within police agencies and those who work outside the agency as consulting or contract psychologists. In-house psychologists, by virtue of being there for daily operations, may have more influence on processes and more opportunities to educate and build trust with personnel and command staff, which may in turn diminish the frequency and intensity of these organizational and ethical conflicts. On the other hand, if a 105

Jeni L. McCutcheon

psychologist works in-house and the conflicts are not regarded with sensitivity toward the psychologist needing to maintain ethical practice, life can be very difficult for that police psychologist. Psychologists working outside the police agency may be freer from daily conflict, depending on their level of involvement with the agency, but may also have less exposure to personnel and be afforded less influence in helping shape policies and practice so that psychological services are in line with current standards of practice.

Resolving Ethical Dilemmas There exist multiple ways of identifying, working through, and resolving ethical dilemmas. One should first ask, “Is this really an ethical dilemma, or is it some other (moral, business, or other problemtype) dilemma?” When ethical problems or dilemmas exist, a systematic problem- solving model may be of help. Another problem-solving method is to expand more abstractly the way we approach and solve problems. For instance, there is the idea that we have more than one party we are serving, and that is in contrast to the idea that we have always just one defined client. We may actually have responsibilities to multiple parties. Additionally, there is the idea of considering the culture we are working with and what is normative for that culture as an important factor in resolving ethical dilemmas. Finally, ethical practice can be promoted by attention to other methods that lead toward ethical practice. These include seeking consultation, and being aware of and adhering to professional standards and guidelines by recognizing commonly held standards of practice. These are all methods to employ toward ethical practice in the area of police psychology.

Systematic Ethical Decision-Making Models It seems prudent to have in mind and be able to employ a systematic process through utilization of an ethical decision-making model. Having a systematic method allows for consistency as well as a plan to follow when challenged or genuinely stumped in an ethical dilemma when one needs to weigh options and is unsure how to proceed. It is important to note that even after reviewing relevant codes, guidelines, and seeking consultation, there may be no one clear “right” way to proceed to provide resolution. One method of problem solving when ethical dilemmas exist is to employ a systematic process through utilization of an ethical decision-making model. Many decision-making models exist. The reader with interest in these models may want to refer to Cottone and Claus (2000) for a thorough review of the history of ethical decision-making models as well as a list of popular models. Several select models that this author finds helpful are mentioned here. This author’s preferred model because of familiarity and years of helpful use is Corey, Corey, and Callanan’s (2007) eight-step ethical decision-making model: 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8.

Identify the problem or dilemma. Identify the potential issues involved. Review the relevant ethical codes. Know the applicable laws and regulations. Obtain consultation. Consider possible and probable courses of action. Enumerate the consequences of various decisions. Decide on what appears to be the best course of action. (p. 23)

Pope and Vasquez (2007) propose a lengthier ethical decision-making model, drawing from the Canadian Psychological Associations ethics codes (2000). They caution that not all of the following 106

Ethical Issues in Police Psychology

steps are relevant to each ethical dilemma and that some steps may be omitted or adapted depending on the situation:   1. Identify the situation that requires ethical consideration and decision making.   2. Anticipate who will be affected by your decision.   3. Figure out who, if anyone, is the client.  4. Assess your relevant areas of competence—and of missing knowledge, skills, experience, or expertise—in regard to the relevant aspects of this situation.   5. Review relevant formal ethical standards.   6. Review relevant legal standards.   7. Review the relevant research and theory.   8. Consider how, if at all, your personal feelings, biases, or self-interest might affect your ethical judgment and reasoning.   9. Consider what effects, if any, that social, cultural, religious, or similar factors may have on the situation and in identifying ethical responses. 10. Consider consultation. 11. Develop alternative courses of action. 12. Evaluate the alternative courses of actions. 13. Try to adopt the perspective of each person who will be affected. 14. Decide what to do, and then review or reconsider it. 15. Act on and assume personal responsibility for your decision. 16. Evaluate the results. 17. Assume personal responsibility for the consequences of your actions. 18. Consider implications for preparation, planning, and prevention. (pp. 111–118) Zur (2007) presents a seven-step decision-making model, provided here in abbreviated form: 1. Identify the issues. 2. Identify the relevant moral, ethical, clinical, legal, professional, communal, and other issues and conflicts involved. 3. Develop a series of alternative courses of action. 4. Conduct an analysis of the likely short-term, ongoing, and long-term risks and benefits of each course of action for anyone or anything involved or likely to be affected. 5. First, separately weigh the risks against the benefits within each option, then compare them and choose a course of action. 6. Implement the course of action chosen through the risk-benefit analysis. 7. Develop ways to assess the success or effectiveness of the plan and respond to the results of the assessment by either continuing with it if it has proved successful or modifying or discontinuing it if it has failed to accomplish some or all of its objectives. These models appear to be well suited for clinical and consultation situations, though it is likely that each model could be applied to some or greater extent to assessment, operational, and educational psychology tasks. Bush, Connell, and Denney (2006) offer an eight-step systematic model for decision making in forensic psychology: 1. 2. 3. 4.

Identify the problem. Consider the significance of the context and setting. Identify and use ethics and legal resources. Consider personal beliefs and values. 107

Jeni L. McCutcheon

5. 6. 7. 8.

Develop possible solutions to the problem. Consider the potential consequences of various solutions. Choose and implement a course of action. Assess the outcome and implement changes as needed. (pp. 28–35)

Corey and Trompetter (2009) propose a systematic decision-making model for law enforcement psychologists to resolve ethical dilemmas. Their model involves the acronym C.L.E.A.R: C: Clinical considerations. Will the action I’m taking improve, complicate, or have a neutral effect on my clinical work in this matter (treatment or evaluation)? Note that this can be thought of in the psychologist’s operational or consulting work, too. L: Legal considerations. What does the law say about this issue? Do I know? Are there competing laws? E: Ethical considerations. These involve the ethical principles and standards delineated in the Ethical Principles and Code of Conduct for Psychologists (APA, 2002). A: Administrative considerations. What contractual obligations do I have? What commitments have I made in my disclosures, authorization forms, or informed consent procedures? What agency policy issues or rules need to be considered? What effect will my decision have on my office practices or on my staff? R: Risk management considerations. How might my licensing board view my decision? Will it pass the “sniff ” test or the “headline test”? How would similarly situated colleagues see it? What do the practice guidelines say about it?

Other Ethical Dilemma Resolution Strategies Aside from systematic decision-making models, there are other methods to guide ethical decision making and practice. These may be helpful to psychologists who are still left without clear answers about how to proceed after employing one or more of the systematic ethical decision-making models.

Reconsidering the Definition of the Client One way is to expand more abstractly the way we approach and solve problems. For instance, instead of focusing on just having one single client, as this is often not the case in police psychology where there may be more than one client, there is the idea that we have more than one party we are serving, even with a defined client. Fisher (2009) detailed a method of thinking about “the fact that psychologists have ethical obligations to all parties in every case, regardless of the number or nature of the relationships” (p. 1). She provides examples of how third-party requests, and providing services to or through organizations, are examples where such considerations exist. She encourages that such situations need to be pondered, the relationships need to be clarified, and the psychologist needs to be planful in his or her approach. For police psychologists, there are numerous situations where we may have one such “master” or primary client (typically, the police agency) but actually have many parties to recognize and to be responsible for. Consider, for example, the police officer client who is referred by the police agency for mandated anger management therapy. The primary client could be considered to be the police agency; the agency made the referral and wants information regarding attendance in treatment. But then there is the other client, the person seeking therapy, and as outlined in an informed consent, his or her private disclosures and the details of the treatment are kept confidential. Consider also the work of a police psychologist as part of a crisis negotiation callout. Their client in this case is the police agency. Their client is not the subject on scene, such as a hostage taker. Though not a 108

Ethical Issues in Police Psychology

client per se, an interested party may be the public as a whole, who is expecting the police agency to end the situation and maintain the safety of the environment where the incident is taking place, and the police psychologist is now involved in this as well. Additionally, the idea that we have changing responsibilities to others in evolving situations is relevant to consider. Do we have a duty or responsibility to more than just the client in performing police psychology functions? The answer can sometimes be a surprising “yes.” Consider the case of a pre-employment evaluation where police psychologists may have a responsibility to more than just the client agency. One recent Arizona case could provide food-for-thought in considering different responsibilities in evaluations. Stanley v. McCarver (2003) involved holding negligent a radiologist who interpreted Christine Stanley’s chest X-ray that was administered as part of a pre-employment evaluation. This X-ray revealed lung nodule densities that Stanley believes would have led to a quicker diagnosis of the lung cancer she was eventually diagnosed with 10 months later. In the initial case, the court found that Dr. McCarver had not acted negligently because there was not a physician–patient relationship with Stanley. Upon appeal, this decision was reversed in part and the court found that “a physician has a duty to exercise reasonable care in conducting the examination and this duty includes communicating about the examination directly to the person examined” (para. 21). As an Arizona psychologist, the case has altered this author’s response in several pre- employment situations. As an example, consider the case of a pre-employment evaluation where a client presents as emotionally unstable. Though this is a very rare occurrence in pre-employment evaluations, it does occasionally happen. When an applicant has expressed recent and/or current suicidal ideation for instance and/or is clearly majorly depressed, this psychologist has stopped the evaluation. The focus is then on sharing the concerning findings, expressing a strong recommendation that the person seek prompt or immediate (depending on the level of dangerousness) medical and/or mental health treatment, and, last, identifying referrals for the applicant. It goes without saying that applicants quickly figure out they likely did not receive a “pass,” positive rating, or “hire” recommendation. The incident is documented on a separate form and placed within the applicant’s pre-employment psychological record. The police agency then gets a report with a “no-hire” recommendation. In circumstances such as this case, there are multiple parties the psychologist must exercise responsibility toward.

What About the Culture? Another method to guide ethical decision making and practice is to consider the context and cultural variables of the culture we work in. In this case, this is the law enforcement culture as a whole, and specifically the culture of the police agency where the psychologist works. Traditionally, law enforcement culture values loyalty and collegiality, often regards officers as family, values being helpful, and consists of heroic individuals who value strength. There is also typically respect for rules and policies and structure, and an acquiescence to dealing with issues within a hierarchical setting. At times, there can be rigidity and a black-and-white thinking style (which, of course, helps maintain order and structure within the system). If, as a police psychologist, you are aware and respectful of this culture and work at resolving ethical dilemmas within this framework, then working in a police agency can often become a much easier task. As an example, in this psychologist’s pre-employment evaluation experience, a common situation occurred. This involved processing public safety applicants within a state where 18-year-old applicants commonly reported in their personal life having sexual intercourse with minors, say, typically 17-year-olds. The law in this particular state, by its letter, does not allow consent for such sexual relations. Because of this, in each case, a mandated report of possible child abuse was required by the psychologist. This situation was understandably concerning from a hiring perspective (as well as tedious, but required, for the psychologists). It led to a valid point on the part of the police agency in questioning 109

Jeni L. McCutcheon

along the lines of how we can have you make mandated reports and then we turn around and hire this person for work within our agency. From the psychologist’s point of view, this situation presented the ethical issue of dealing with organizational demands and the legal issue of not being able to ignore or discharge a legal and mandated duty to report. How you approach a dilemma is sometimes everything. What was not successful in this situation was any sort of insistence that this psychologist had an ethical dilemma that needed resolving. It was important to keep in mind that the police agency is not beholden to the APA Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct. Resolution came from discussing with supportive command staff the needs and wants of the agency; the ethical, legal and hiring issues; the conflicts; the pros and cons of taking different courses of actions; and sharing the consultation feedback this psychologist had thus far gathered in looking at this issue. Ultimately, what led to a successful resolution was talking about a mutual respect and regard for following the law, which in this case meant the psychologist making reports as required. A clear statement was made that was the starting point for a resolution. A member of command staff said, “Well, it’s as simple as we can’t have you breaking the law, Doc. You’ve got to follow the law.” A discussion ensued about how while a duty to report exists, with 99% of the applicants evaluated that present with this circumstance, there was not a clinical concern such that this reporting mandate led to a no-hire recommendation in the pre-employment evaluation report. Based on this discussion, new wording was incorporated into the pre-employment report to explain this, a specific reporting system was implemented to keep the law enforcement agency the primary reporting source when possible, and both parties walked away feeling the issue was well resolved. Without an appreciation of resolving this dilemma within and having respect for the police culture, it is unlikely this situation would have been resolved positively.

The Role of Consultation A final way to resolve ethical dilemmas is to have a process in place to seek consultation from other police psychologists. Reliance on previous experience and the opinions of peers may be paramount in solving ethical dilemmas (Osborn, Day, Komesaroff, & Mant, 2009). Seeing ethical behavior of peers is also influential in promoting ethical behavior (Deshpande, Joseph & Prasad, 2006). Having experienced colleagues who can help one to think aloud about the importance of adherence to professional standards, assist in considering standards of practice, and provide a sounding board to contemplate how to proceed are other ways of maintaining ethical police psychology practice. To this end, there are various professional police psychology organizations (previously identified and referred to) to join. There are also committees to call on for consultation such as the American Psychological Association; the Division 18 (Psychologists in Public Service) mentoring program; and the International Association of Chiefs of Police, Police Psychological Services Section Early Career Psychologists Committee, and International Association of Chiefs of Police, Police Psychological Services Section Ethics Committee. Additionally, peers may be identified for individualized consultation through posts on the listservs for the American Psychological Association (APA), Division 18, Police and Public Safety Section; and the International Association of Chiefs of Police, Police Psychological Services Section (IACP PPSS).

Proficiencies and Specialty Ethical Guidelines for Police Psychologists Police psychology is an emerging and evolving practice area. Though there have long been efforts to define the roles and scope of practice of police psychologists (Aamodt, 2000; Blau, 1994; Dietz, 2000; Kurke & Scrivner, 1995; Ostrov, 1986), the greatest effort perhaps toward defining the work police psychologists perform was defined by the Police Psychology Core Domains & Proficiencies Joint Committee on Police Psychology Competencies, which met in the fall of 2007. This 110

Ethical Issues in Police Psychology

committee was a joint effort between members of the International Association of Chiefs of Police, Police Psychological Services Section (IACP PPSS); members of the Society for Police and Criminal Psychology (SPCP); and members of the American Psychological Association (APA), Division 18, Police and Public Safety Section. They identified 57 distinct tasks and proficiencies of police psychologists that were then separated into four core domains: assessment, intervention, operational, and consulting (Aumiller & Corey, 2007). Aumiller and Corey’s extensive document listing and defining these domains and proficiencies are “now the accepted definition of the field of police psychology as defined by members of the profession” (2007, p. 65). Additionally, in 2008, police psychology was recognized as a proficiency area by APA. Police psychology is a compilation of different types of psychology practice. Police psychology is often regarded as part of forensic psychology (Falkenbach, 2008). Certainly many of the assessment tasks of police psychology are forensic in nature. However, many aspects of police psychology practice, such as providing psychotherapy, are more clinical or counseling in nature. Additionally, some police psychologists come from an industrial-organizational background and perspective. Research, personnel selection, and organizational consultation come strongly from this tradition. Provision of police psychological services stems back to psychological tests used in police selection in 1916 (Dietz & Reese, 1986), then it picked up momentum in the 1950s (Zelig, 1987) and really took off in the 1980s, when police psychology started to be recognized as a distinct field and area of practice (Scrivner, 1994). Since that time, it has not stopped progressing at a very steady pace as both an identity for psychologists and in scope of the services provided. Because of this, increasingly, police psychologists run into ethical dilemmas that are not adequately resolved through reliance on current ethical guidelines and codes. This is in part because police psychology practice is not all clinical and counseling in nature, just as it is not all industrial-organizational in nature. It is this psychologist’s opinion that there is a need for specialty ethical guidelines for police psychology. If we rest on the idea that we are a relatively new and emerging field and stay silent, it is likely that our field and scope of practice will be defined for us by others. We then risk having our practice dictated to us and judged by standards that do not fit us well or address some of our unique ways of practicing, based on not only the work we do but also the unique population we serve. Obviously, there are arguments against a set of specialty ethical guidelines for police psychology. First, there is the issue of redundancy and how to manage likely conflicts between a new specialty guideline and the existing APA Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct. There is also a relevant consideration of one more document for police psychologists to consider and try to follow, which some may consider onerous and burdensome. Additionally, there is the idea that perhaps advocating as a group to influence future revisions of the Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct is a preferred course of action instead of creating specialty guidelines. An impetus to consider as a profession developing specialty guidelines may be that we have proficiencies as police psychologists that are currently starting to become out of line or at least risky, within the current ethical standards of our profession as a whole. However, this does not mean that we are unethical psychologists, rather that the codes as they exist may need some fine-tuning to be more applicable to the changing work that we do. Examples of where we may not be currently adequately represented in the current psychological professional culture and Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct are when police psychologists work as consultants with agencies and in crisis negotiation situations. In the last several years, there have been heated debates among psychologists and mental health professionals, with many arguing that psychologists need not be involved in any way with actions that could lead to the harm, serious injury, or death of individuals. Any police psychologist involved in crisis negotiation training or operational support knows that based on the opinions and misunderstandings of what we do by many, police psychologists may soon have mandates placed on us that drastically shape and/or limit our proficiencies and ability to provide such services. We are also somewhat unique as a field in how we engage in multiple relationships, and it is 111

Jeni L. McCutcheon

possible that a stronger stance in recognizing and even in some cases advocating the positive benefits of such relationships is warranted. Examples such as these, and likely there are others, necessitate that we have our practices reflected in ethical codes that are specific to our area of practice. If developed, perhaps a set of specialty guidelines for police psychology practice can be modeled after existing specialty guidelines. For instance, the specialty guidelines for forensic psychologists are aspirational in nature. They do not set a mandate for appropriate professional conduct. Specialty guidelines are meant to be used together with other relevant ethical codes, such as the Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct, and in conjunction with relevant laws and regulations. Perhaps specialty ethical guidelines for police psychologists can be used in much the same spirit and practice. While aspirational, they would provide a sort of set of directions for ideal conduct and actions by police psychologists. Such a guideline would also help establish and expand the literature base of the current standard of practice in police psychology.

Directions for Future Research Further research and study is needed. First, along the lines of Zelig’s (1988) study, there is a need for ongoing research to identify the most prevalent ethical dilemmas that police psychologists encounter. Frequency of these dilemmas’ occurrence would also be enlightening. Surveying about not only police psychology practice but also the wider domain of public safety psychology practice may be helpful as many police psychologists also work with fire, corrections, and detention personnel. It is likely that the ethical dilemmas encountered by police psychologists are similar if not identical to the types of dilemmas encountered by psychologists who work with these other related professions. This is a supposition and therefore research needs to bear out what differences, if any, arise in the types and frequencies of ethical dilemmas psychologists encounter in working with these different professional groups. Research about the methods police psychologists most commonly utilize to resolve ethical dilemmas are also suggested. It would be interesting to learn how police psychologists resolve the ethical dilemmas they encounter as well as to see if there are differences between those psychologists who are longstanding in the fields; newer to this specialized area of work; and clinical, counseling, forensic, academic, or industrial-organizational in training and affiliation; as well as whether there are other relevant variables. Additionally, ethical casebooks and workbooks can be helpful (Kenyon, 1999; Nagy, 2005; Pope, Sonne, & Greene, 2006; Steinman, Richardson, & McEnroe, 1998), and such books aimed specifically at police psychology practice seem warranted. A final area for future study is in arriving at whether there is a need for specialty ethical guidelines for police psychology. Surveying police psychologists would be helpful to answer these questions, as well as then proceed toward creating such a code.

Note 1. © 2002 by the American Psychological Association. Adapted with permission. The official citation that should be used in referencing this material is American Psychological Association. (2002). Ethical principles of psychologists and code of conduct. American Psychologist, 57, 1060–1073.

References and Further Reading Aamodt, M. G. (2000). The role of the I/O psychologist in police psychology. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 15(2), 8–10. American Educational Research Association, American Psychological Association, National Council on Research in Education. (1999). Standards for educational and psychological testing (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: American Educational Research Association.

112

Ethical Issues in Police Psychology American Psychological Association. (2002). Ethical principles of psychologists and code of conduct. American Psychologist, 57, 1060–1073. American Psychological Association. (2010). American Psychological Association amends ethics code to address potential conflicts among professional ethics, legal authority and organizational demands. Retrieved April 3, 2010, from www.apa.org/news/press/releases/2010/02/ethics-code.aspx. Aumiller, G. S., & Corey, D. (2007). Defining the field of police psychology: Core domains & proficiencies. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 22, 65–76. doi:10.1007/s11896-007-9013-4. Blau, T. H. (1994). Psychological services for law enforcement. New York: John Wiley. Bush, S. S., Connell, M. A., & Denney, R. L. (2006). Ethical practice in forensic psychology: A systematic model for decision-making. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Canadian Psychological Association. (2000). Canadian code of ethics for psychologists. Ottawa: Author. Cohen, E. D., & Cohen, G. S. (1999). The virtuous therapist: Ethical practice of counseling and psychotherapy. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Colnerud, G. (1997). Ethical dilemmas of psychologists: A Swedish example in an international perspective. European Psychologist, 2(2), 164–170. Committee on Ethical Guidelines for Forensic Psychologists. (1991). Specialty guidelines for forensic psychologists. Law and Human Behavior, 15, 655–665. Corey, G., Corey, M. S., & Callanan, P. (2007). Issues and ethics in the helping professions. Belmont, CA: Thomson Brooks/Cole. Corey, D., & Trompetter, P. S. (2009, October). Ethical dilemmas for law enforcement psychologists: A systematic model for decision-making. Paper presented at the International Association of Chiefs of Police, Police Psychological Services Section Conference, Denver, CO. Cottone, R. R., & Claus, R. E. (2000). Ethical decision-making models: A review of the literature. Journal of Counseling & Development, 78, 275–283. Deshpande, S. P., Joseph, J., & Prasad, R. (2006). Factors impacting ethical behavior in hospitals. Journal of Business Ethics, 69(2), 207–216. Dietz, A. S. (2000). The role of the I/O psychologist in police psychology. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 15(2), 1–7. Dietz, P. E., & Reese, J. T. (1986). The perils of police psychology: 10 strategies for minimizing role conflicts when providing mental health service and consultation to law enforcement agencies. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 4(4), 385–400. Falkenbach, D. M. (2008). Forensic psychology. In G. Madhavan, B. Oakley, & L. Kun (Eds.), Career development in bioengineering and biotechnology (pp. 214–221). New York: Springer. Fisher, M. A. (2009). Replacing “Who is the client?” with a different ethical question. Professional Psychology Research and Practice, 40(1), 1–7. doi:10.137/a0014011. Gelber, C. (2003). LAPD bureau psychologists hit the streets. Police Chief, September. Retrieved April 3, 2010, from http://policechiefmagazine.org. Gelles, M. G., Morgan, C. A., & Moorehead, O. (2006, October). Ethical challenges for police psychologists in consultation to police operations. Paper presented at the International Association of Chiefs of Police, Police Psychological Services Section Conference, Boston. International Association of Chiefs of Police. (2004a). Officer-involved shooting guidelines. Arlington, VA. Retrieved April 3, 2010, from www.theiacp.org/psych_services_section/ International Association of Chiefs of Police. (2004b). Pre-employment psychological evaluation guidelines. Arlington, VA. Retrieved April 3, 2010, from www.theiacp.org/psych_services_section/ International Association of Chiefs of Police. (2006a). Peer support guidelines. Arlington, VA. Retrieved April 3, 2010, from www.theiacp.org/psych_services_section/ International Association of Chiefs of Police. (2006b). Guidelines for consulting police psychologists. Arlington, VA. Retrieved April 3, 2010, from www.theiacp.org/psych_services_section/ International Association of Chiefs of Police. (2009). Fitness for duty evaluation guidelines. Arlington, VA. Retrieved April 3, 2010, from www.theiacp.org/psych_services_section/ Inwald, R. E. (1984). Psychological screening: Legal, ethical, and administrative questions. Police Chief, January. Janik, J. (1994). Desirable qualifications in a police psychologist. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 10(2), 24–31. Johnson, W. B., & Wilson, K. (1993). The military internship: A retrospective analysis. Professional Psychology Research and Practice, 24, 312–318. Kennedy, C. H., & Johnson, W. B. (2009). Mixed agency in military psychology: Applying the American Psychological Association Ethics Code. Psychological Services, 6(1), 22–31.

113

Jeni L. McCutcheon Kenyon, P. (1999). What would you do? An ethical case workbook for human service professionals. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole. Kurke, M. I., & Scrivner, E. M. (Eds.). (1995). Police psychology into the 21st century. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Nagy, T. F. (2005). Ethics in plain English: An illustrative casebook for psychologists (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Orme, D. R., & Doerman, A. L. (2001). Ethical dilemmas and U.S. Air Force clinical psychologists: A survey. Professional Psychology Research and Practice, 32(3), 305–311. Osborn, M., Day, R., Komesaroff, P., & Mant, A. (2009). Do ethical guidelines make a difference to decisionmaking? Internal Medicine Journal, 39, 800–805. Ostrov, E. (1986). Police/law enforcement psychology. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 4(4), 353–370. Pettifor, J. L., & Sawchuk, T. R. (2006). Psychologists’ perceptions of ethically troubling incidents across international borders. International Journal of Psychology, 41(3), 216–225. Pope, K. S., Sonne, J. L., & Greene, B. (2006). What therapists don’t talk about and why: Understanding taboos that hurt us and our clients. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Pope, K. S., & Vasquez, M. J. (2007). Ethics in psychotherapy and counseling. San Francisco, CA: John Wiley. Pope, K. S., & Vetter, V. A. (1992). Ethical dilemmas encountered by members of the American Psychological Association: A national survey. American Psychologist, 47(3), 387–411. Reese, J. T. (1987). A history of police psychological services. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Reese, J. T., & Goldstein, H. A. (Eds.). (1986). Psychological services for law enforcement: A compilation of papers submitted to the National Symposium on Police Psychological Services (at the FBI Academy, Quantico, VA). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Schank, J. A., & Skovholt, T. M. (1997). Dual-relationship dilemmas of rural and small-community psychologists. Professional Psychology, 28(1), 44–49. doi:0.1037/0735–7028.28.1.44. Scrivner, E. M. (1994). Controlling police use of excessive force: The role of the police psychologist Retrieved April 3, 2010, from www.ncjrs.gov/?txtfiles/ppsyc.txt. Slack, C. M., & Wassenaar, D. R. (1999). Ethical dilemmas of South African clinical psychologists: International comparisons. European Psychologist, 4(3), 179–186. Stanley v. McCarver and Osborn, Nelson & Carr Portable X-Ray, Inc. 1 CA-CV 02–0328 (Superior Court in Maricopa County, AZ 2003). Steinman, S. O., Richardson, N. F., & McEnroe, T. (1998). The ethical decision-making manual for helping professionals. Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole. Super, J. T. (1997). Select legal and ethical aspects of pre-employment psychological evaluations. 12(2), 1–6. Task Force on the Role of Psychology in the Criminal Justice System, American Psychological Association. (1978). Report of the task force on the role of psychology in the criminal justice system. American Psychologist, 33(12), 1099–1113. Zelig, M. (1987). Clinical services and demographic characteristics of police psychologists. Professional Psychology Research and Practice, 18(3), 269–275. Zelig, M. (1988). Ethical dilemmas in police psychology. Professional Psychology Research and Practice, 19(3), 336–338. Zur, O. (2001). Out-of-office experience: When crossing office boundaries and engaging in dual relationships are clinically beneficial and ethically sound. The Independent Practitioner, 21(1), 96–100. Zur, O. (2007). Boundaries in psychotherapy. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Cases Cited Jaffee v. Redmond, 518 U.S. 1 (1996). Pettus v. Cole, 49 Cal. App. 4th 402, 57 Cal. Rptr. 2d 46 (1996).

114

5 POLICE VS. PROBATION/ SURVEILLANCE OFFICERS: SIMILARITIES AND DIFFERENCES D. Scott Herrmann and Barbara Broderick

As discussed in Chapter 1 of this book, the use of psychological tests to aid in the evaluation and selection of police candidates is well established in the United States, with the foundation of such assessment dating back to the turn of the 20th century (Maloney & Ward, 1976). Lewis Terman of Stanford University was one of the field’s earliest pioneers and conducted one of the first documented studies of police selection in the United States (Reese, 1995; Terman & Otis, 1917). However, it was not until the 1967 President’s Commission on Law Enforcement and Administration of Justice that the psychological assessment and selection of law enforcement candidates became solidified in the United States (Benner, 1986; Reese, 1995). In the decades that followed, many changes occurred in the psychological tools that were developed for the selection of law enforcement candidates, the evaluation methods employed, and the laws governing the selection process. Now, as law enforcement psychology (i.e., the application of professional psychology with police and allied law enforcement groups) continues to develop and expand into the 21st century, evolutionary changes continue to punctuate this unique area of psychological practice in new and previously unforeseen ways. One area of change that represents a truly burgeoning area of growth and opportunity for law enforcement psychologists is the provision of psychological services to nontraditional law enforcement agencies. Specifically, these are agencies that fall outside the umbrella cast by the U.S. Department of Justice and state/local law enforcement, but that nevertheless assume quasi-law enforcement functions in communities and neighborhoods. One example of a nontraditional law enforcement agency is that of a probation department, where court-appointed officers function in a hybrid capacity that often involves both law enforcement and social work functions. Currently, there are more than 2,000 separate probation agencies in the United States that fall under the jurisdiction of the courts (Abadinsky, 2009), many of which employ a combination of both probation officers and surveillance officers. While probation and surveillance officers’ job functions tend to be multifaceted and are described in detail later in this chapter, the primary role of a probation officer is to ensure that court-mandated “conditions of probation” are followed and adhered to by convicted offenders, while surveillance officers’ primary duty is to assist the probation officer in ensuring such conditions are met. In addition to probation departments at the federal, state, county, and municipal levels, there are also private companies and nonprofit organizations providing probation services throughout the United States (Burrell, 2005). Indeed, with courts and allied criminal justice agencies growing at a rapid pace, and with the employment of probation officers expected to

115

D. Scott Herrmann and Barbara Broderick

increase by 11% between 2006 and 2016 (U.S. Department of Labor, 2008), practice opportunities are abundant for law enforcement psychologists seeking to expand their services into this adjunctive area of practice. However, while the practice opportunities are plentiful, psychological evaluations of probation/ surveillance candidates should not be construed as simply synonymous with police psychological evaluations because there are a number of important and noteworthy distinctions that exist between these vocational cohorts. For example, there are important differences in essential job functions, in the philosophical orientation of departments, and in background and training requirements of the involved officers. Moreover, in most jurisdictions, police officers are required to be certified under a state’s Police Officer Standards and Training Board (POST), while probation officers may or may not be required to be POST-certified. Additionally in some jurisdictions, probation officers are formally recognized as “peace officers,” while in other jurisdictions they are not. According to a survey by the American Probation and Parole Association (2006), 25 of 56 states and territories have formally recognized adult probation officers with peace officer status, and 17 of 56 states and territories have recognized juvenile probation officers with such status. Other areas of distinction include those related to essential job functions. While a police officer’s primary job function is often defined as “protecting and serving the community,” a probation officers’ job function usually entails providing both enforcement and rehabilitative services to convicted felons under the auspices of the courts. Each probation department is therefore in the unique position of articulating whether “rehabilitation,” “enforcement,” or a combination of both perspectives is to be emphasized in a probation agency’s mission statement and philosophical outlook (see Burton, Latessa, & Barker, 1992; Small & Torres, 2001, for a thorough discussion of this topic). While these are but a few examples that demonstrate how probation departments are not onein-the-same with police agencies, there are nevertheless important areas of similarity and overlap between these entities as well. For example, many probation/surveillance officers in the 21st century carry firearms while performing their duties. They are also empowered with arrest authority, wear body armor in the field, function within “chain-of-command” organizations, and are subject to many of the same occupational hazards and stressors encountered by police officers. One specific example of the overlapping function between probation and police can be found in Arizona, where the Maricopa County Adult Probation Department (MCAPD) received nearly $700,000 in federal stimulus funding in 2009 for its fugitive apprehension unit in order to combat illegal narcotics activity and drug trafficking within the state. Because MCAPD serves more warrants than any other law enforcement agency within the nation’s fourth most populous county, the Department of Justice recognized MCAPD as uniquely positioned to receive and apply such funding to further the DOJ’s law enforcement objectives. Thus, while it cannot be assumed that police and probation are merely synonymous vocational cohorts, there are important areas of overlap regarding police-oriented job functions that should be recognized. Consequently, before stepping into this unique area of practice by providing psychological services to probation departments, law enforcement psychologists must have a good working knowledge of what the specific similarities and differences are between police and probation cohorts. They must know what the different types of probation departments are (e.g., adult versus juvenile, federal versus local, and so forth), what the differences are in terms of agency mission and values, and what the historical factors are that gave rise to the “probation movement” to begin with. The purpose of this chapter is therefore to highlight some of these important distinctions for law enforcement psychologists who may not have been otherwise exposed to this information. It is incumbent on those interested in working in this area to have a solid grounding and knowledge of the important nuances that exist prior to performing psychological evaluations of probation/surveillance officer candidates. Only this way will the most thoughtful and defensible psychological evaluations for probation department organizations and consumers result. 116

Police vs. Probation/Surveillance Officers

Probation/Surveillance Overview History of Probation Movement in the United States The probation movement in the United States can be traced back to John Augustus of Boston, Massachusetts, widely recognized as the “first probation officer” in the United States. In the late 1800s, Augustus was a shoemaker in Boston who took it upon himself to bail out a man charged with being a vagrant and common drunkard. Successful rehabilitation with this first case led Augustus to bail out other defendants in an effort to attempt rehabilitation with them, which commonly included vocational assistance and help establishing a residence. In his capacity as a volunteer, Augustus would also make frequent returns to the court to provide progress updates on his charges and to make recommendations regarding case dispositions. Augustus reportedly worked in this manner for nearly 18 years, and generally received strong support from judges regarding his efforts. Between 1841 and 1859, nearly 2,000 men and women were spared incarceration because of Augustus’s intervention and supervision efforts (Champion, 2008). Interestingly, however, many policemen were said to oppose Augustus’s rehabilitation efforts because they were unable to collect their customary fee for each rehabilitated case that did not result in a commitment to the House of Corrections (Abadinsky, 2009). After Augustus’s death in 1859, supporters successfully lobbied the Massachusetts legislature to enact the first probation statute in 1878, which authorized the Boston mayor to hire a probation officer who would be supervised under the superintendent of police. Thereafter, the second state to adopt an official probation statute was Vermont in 1898, with other states quickly following suit. Although most early probation officers were unpaid, voluntary positions, many of the earliest paid probation officers worked simultaneously as police officers, deputies, or clerks in district attorney’s offices (Champion, 2008). By 1919, the Illinois legislature enacted a law providing that counties pay salaries and expenses of probation officers, and in each police district, a police officer spent part of his time out of uniform performing the duties of a probation officer (Schultz, 1973). In 1925, Congress enacted legislation establishing the Federal Probation System, and in 1929 the first paid United States Probation Officer (USPO) was appointed under the Department of Justice. Thereafter, in 1939, federal probation administration was shifted to the newly created Administrative Office of the United States Courts. In the 21st century, probation departments exist at the federal, state, and county levels. Probation in the United States is administered by more than 2,000 separate agencies supervising in excess of 3 million adult offenders on probation for felonies and misdemeanors (Abadinsky, 2009). With regard to juvenile probation, the first juvenile court was established in Chicago in July 1899 by way of the Juvenile Court Act introduced by the Illinois legislature. Prior to this date, Massachusetts, New York, and certain other states had statutes providing for the separate hearings of children’s cases apart from adult cases, but it was not until 1899 that the first juvenile court in the United States was officially established (Flexner & Baldwin, 1916). Between 1900 and 1920, 20 states passed similar acts to establish juvenile courts, and by the end of World War II, all states had created juvenile court systems (Champion, 2008). The development of juvenile probation followed closely thereafter, although some historical accounts have indicated that some form of juvenile probation was provided by a private society in the central district of Boston as early as 1888 (Flexner & Baldwin, 1916). In short, juvenile courts and juvenile probation agencies emerged as an outgrowth of the notion that juveniles deserve enhanced reform and rehabilitative efforts as compared to adult offenders, and juvenile courts have traditionally had considerable latitude in managing the affairs of juveniles. Another important distinction is that juvenile court and probation records in most states are routinely expunged and/or sealed once the offender reaches a certain age. The purpose of these laws is to allow a minor who has committed delinquent acts to erase his or her record permanently and begin with a “clean slate” upon entering adulthood. 117

D. Scott Herrmann and Barbara Broderick

Essential Functions of Probation/Surveillance Work Among probation officers’ many duties, the most primary job responsibility is to serve as the “eyes and ears of the court.” Beyond this general description, however, probation work may entail a wide array of different responsibilities and duties, and there may be wide fluctuation across probation agencies in terms of the duties assigned to probation officers. Not infrequently, some probation officer duties may appear contradictory, for example, (1) to rehabilitate and treat offenders who are amenable to treatment, and (2) to protect society from those individuals who pose a risk to the community (Colley, Culbertson, & Latessa, 1986). These kinds of multiple role expectations present a true dilemma for probation administrators charged with balancing these often contradictory agendas when there is disagreement about whether a “law enforcement” or “social work” approach should be emphasized. Nevertheless, some common probation officer duties found in most departments often include directing and counseling probationers regarding the terms of their probation, conducting field visits (home, work, treatment facilities, or jail), investigating alleged violations of probation, administering breathalyzer/urinalysis tests, assessing social history and risk to the community, directing probationers to community resources, conducting investigations and preparing a variety of electronic and written reports for the court, coordinating services with other social and law enforcement agencies, testifying at court hearings, and, in some jurisdictions, conducting searches and effecting arrests. Surveillance officers, on the other hand, typically perform “journey-level” probation officer functions and assist probation officers in enforcing the terms and conditions of probation. While in general parlance the word surveillance often conjures up ideas of surreptitious undercover investigations or cloak-and-dagger operations (Lyon, 2007), when used by probation departments, it has an altogether different meaning. Surveillance within probation departments typically involves focused, systematic attention to the personal details and behaviors of those who are under direct supervision of the court. Surveillance officer duties often involve considerable fieldwork and are similar to those of probation officers but are not performed with the same degree of independence or autonomy as that of probation officers. Some essential surveillance officer functions typically include providing ongoing surveillance to a caseload of probationers, assisting in the enforcement of mandated curfews, conducting telephone contacts, performing work-site or school visits during day and evening hours, maintaining chronological logs and records of probationer activity, responding to inquiries by family members or law enforcement officers, assisting with rehabilitation functions by providing counseling, evaluation of living arrangements, administering alcohol and drug tests, appearing in court with a probation officer, and, in some jurisdictions, conducting searches and making arrests. While there is substantial overlap between probation officer and surveillance officer functions, the critical difference between the two is that probation officers function in a more independent and autonomous capacity and are viewed as operating with a higher degree of professionalism. Probation officers are frequently viewed as the eyes and ears of the “court,” but surveillance officers are more often viewed as the eyes and ears of the “probation officer.”

To Arm or Not to Arm: Not All Pos/Sos Carry Firearms Although important distinctions exist between police and probation/surveillance functions, one factor causing these distinctions to blur—especially in the public’s eye—is when probation/surveillance officers carry firearms. In the 21st century there is a growing trend within probation departments to arm their officers (Roscoe, Duffee, Rivera, & Smith, 2005; Small & Torres, 2001), with many probation departments additionally empowering their officers with full arrest authority. In the early 2000s, all but 11 of the 94 federal judicial districts permit U.S. probation officers to carry firearms (Small & Torres, 2001), and similar trends continue to take place at the state and county levels. Accordingly, probation agencies are now providing tactical training to their officers and establishing policies and 118

Police vs. Probation/Surveillance Officers

procedures for firearm certification (and decertification when necessary) for their officers who carry firearms. With the enhanced function of carrying firearms comes increased risks, not only to probation officers but also to probation departments and the community as a whole.

Valued Characteristics of Probation/Surveillance Officers As a rule of thumb, probation departments generally maintain higher educational standards for entrylevel officer positions compared to police agencies. A bachelor’s degree is usually required to work as a probation officer, with some jurisdictions requiring a master’s degree for an entry-level position. Entry-level surveillance officers are usually required to possess only an associate’s degree or a high school diploma/GED. Entry-level probation officers are often recruited into the field from college and university based criminal justice departments as well as from social work, counseling, and psychology departments. Clearly, one of the ways that probation departments differ from police departments is that probation tends to draw officers from a much more diverse educational/vocational background. As Colley et al. (1986) note, “When different professional backgrounds are brought to the same job, friction may arise as to the ‘right way’ of doing the job” (p. 67). This friction is clearly evident in some probation departments and even across certain specialized probation units, simply due to the diversity of backgrounds and perspectives represented within an agency. Regarding a philosophical approach to probation work, some empirical research has begun to emerge that sheds light on how the majority of probation officers tend to view their job functions and what sort of professional roles they most value. An early study on how probation officers perceive their roles was conducted by Van Landingham, Taber, and Dimants (1977). In this study, several hundred probation officers in the state of Ohio were surveyed regarding their perceived essential job functions. Results indicated that officers tended to favor “advice/guidance” services and “referral/court consultation” services the most, but there was considerable disagreement regarding law enforcement-related functions. Some officers embraced law enforcement functions, while others did not. In a related study, Wright (1997) found that probation officers in Delaware were much more inclined toward offender reform than offender control and cited overwhelming support for probation officers embracing a “casework strategy” as opposed to a “law enforcement mentality” (p. 77). While these findings do emphasize a social work over law enforcement orientation, we contend that given the multifaceted functions and duties that currently exist within probation departments, there is adequate room for both philosophical perspectives within probation departments in the 21st century. However, it is incumbent on the law enforcement psychologist and probation department administration to ensure that a probation officer’s unique philosophical and psychological characteristics truly match his or her duty assignment within a department. It is also imperative that probation agencies have a well-articulated set of competencies that clearly spell out the traits and characteristics an agency most values among its officers and that a mission statement has been adopted that supports and reinforces the competencies and characteristics identified. An example of such a competency set can be found in Arizona, where in 2008/2009 the Maricopa County Adult Probation Department (MCAPD), the nation’s fifth largest probation department with more than 1,150 employees, undertook a yearlong effort to identify and define the specific traits and characteristics most valued within its adult probation officers. In collaboration with the consulting agency CPS Human Resource Services, focus groups were established, comprised of probation officer supervisors, division directors, human resource managers, and a psychologist, in order to identify core competencies and traits that best distinguish the department’s top probation officer performers from lesser performers. Thereafter, a second focus group further narrowed the list of core competencies to a list of 14 “must-have” traits and characteristics that define the agency’s top probation officer performers. As a final procedural step, executive-level review and fine tuning was also conducted to ensure the core competencies and traits were congruent and synonymous 119

D. Scott Herrmann and Barbara Broderick

with the agency’s fundamental mission statement. While this process focused specifically on Maricopa County adult probation officers, the characteristics and traits identified through this process have broad generalizability to other probation departments throughout the United States given the rigorous and comprehensive methodology employed. Consequently, the final competency list (see Table 5.1) is reproduced here for use by law enforcement psychologists and other evaluators seeking to identify which core traits and characteristics of standard adult probation officers tend to be most valued within probation agencies in the early 21st century.

Specialized Units Within Probation Departments Beyond these core traits and competencies, however, probation departments in the 21st century are increasingly multifaceted in nature, and frequently contain specialized units that perform highly unique functions with different offender populations. In some instances, it may therefore be necessary to select and screen for probation/surveillance candidates who possess additional sets of specialized characteristics and traits that facilitate the performance of such work. Regarding these specialized functions, some of the most common specialized units within probation departments include intensive probation units, sex offender units, and warrants units, each of which require a specialized set of competencies and attributes from the probation/surveillance officers who perform such work. Consequently, law enforcement psychologists must be aware of the specialized requirements and competencies needed to serve within these specialty areas and must tailor their assessment and selection procedures accordingly. While no comprehensive methodology has yet been employed to identify discrete competencies and traits needed by those who perform work within these specialized units, a brief description of the unique job functions is provided here to help illuminate some of the specific job demands faced by those who work within these positions.

Intensive Probation Units Intensive Supervised Probation (ISP) programs have become increasingly popular since the 1960s for managing high-risk nonincarcerated offender populations. ISP programs function as an alternative to incarceration for both adult and juvenile offenders while providing an acceptable level of public safety (Champion, 2008). While less prevalent than adult ISP programs, juvenile ISP programs are currently operating in about one-third to one-half of all U.S. jurisdictions (Champion, 2008). ISP programs often require more frequent face-to-face officer/client contacts than is normally afforded to probationers. Also probationers in ISP units are often subject to a higher level of scrutiny by way of electronic monitoring devices, offender participation in regular drug screens, and so forth.

Sex Offender Units There is general agreement in the probation field that sex offenders require specialized supervision because they share unique characteristics that often include maintaining secretive and manipulative lives, often planning their crimes with considerable forethought, committing a wide range and large number of offense behaviors, and showing a continued propensity to reoffend (Abadinsky, 2009; English, Pullen, & Jones, 1997). Sex offenders, and especially those who prey on children, represent a significant risk to the communities in which they live and to probation agencies responsible for supervising them (Abadinsky, 2009). Child sex offenders often end up on probation because of prison overcrowding (Lurigio, Jones, & Smith, 1995) or because plea agreements are accepted that stem from weak incriminating evidence or the prosecution’s unwillingness to place victimized children through the trauma of a trial (Stalans, 2004). For these reasons, sex offenders require specialized probation/ surveillance services, and many agencies have developed specialized sex offender units to aggregate 120

Table 5.1 Competency Model for Standard Adult Probation Officer Building Trust

Collaboration

Communications

Conflict Management Continuous Learning and Professional Development Cultural Competency Customer/Client Focus

Decision Making/ Problem Solving Facilitating Change Influence

Planning and Organizing Stress Tolerance Teamwork

Technical/ Professional Knowledge and Skill

Interact with others in a way that gives them confidence in one’s motives and representations and those of the organization; is respectful and seen as positive, direct, and truthful; keeps confidences, promises, and commitments. Builds constructive working relationships with clients/customers, other work units, community organizations, and others to meet mutual goals and objectives; participates as an enthusiastic, active, and contributing member of a team to achieve team goals; works positively and cooperatively with other team members; involves others; shares information as appropriate; shares credit for team accomplishments. Clearly conveys and receives information and ideas through a variety of media to individuals or groups in a manner that engages the listener; helps them understand and retain the messages, and invites response and feedback; keeps others informed as appropriate; demonstrates good writing, verbal, and listening skills. Uses appropriate interpersonal styles and techniques to reduce tension and/or conflict between two or more people; is able to size up situations quickly; is able to identify common interests; facilitates resolution. Is committed to developing professionally; attends professional conferences; focuses on best practices; values cutting edge practices and approaches; takes advantage of a variety of learning activities; introduces newly gained knowledge and skills on the job. Cultivates opportunities through diverse people; respects and relates well to people from varied backgrounds, understands diverse worldviews, and is sensitive to group differences; sees diversity as an opportunity; challenges bias and intolerance. Makes customers/clients/victims and their needs a primary focus of one’s actions; builds appropriate customer/client relationships; shows interest in, empathy for, and understanding of the needs and expectations of internal and external customers; gains customer trust and respect; is caring and compassionate; meets or exceeds customer expectations. Breaks down problems into components and recognizes interrelationships; makes sound, well-informed, and objective decisions; compares data, information, and input from a variety of sources to draw conclusions; takes action that is consistent with available facts, constraints, and probable consequences. Facilitates the implementation and acceptance of change within the workplace; encourages others to seek opportunities for different and innovative approaches to addressing problems and opportunities. Uses appropriate interpersonal skills and techniques to gain acceptance for ideas or solutions; uses influencing strategies to gain genuine agreements; seeks to persuade rather than force solutions or impose decisions or regulations; supports building personal autonomy. Organizes work, sets priorities, and determines resources requirements; determines necessary sequence of activities needed to achieve goals. Maintains effective performance under pressure; handles stress in a manner that is acceptable to others and to the organization. Builds constructive working relationships with interested parties dealing with criminal justice matters, i.e., court, attorneys, treatment providers, police, other work units, community organizations, and others to identify and meet mutual goals and objectives; participates as an active and contributing member of teams with a focus on improving offender outcomes and department goals; works cooperatively with other team members, involves others, shares information as appropriate, and shares credit for team accomplishments. Possesses, acquires, and maintains the technical/professional expertise required to do the job effectively and to create client/customer solutions; technical/professional expertise is demonstrated through problem solving, applying professional judgment, and competent performance.

D. Scott Herrmann and Barbara Broderick

personnel who possess the unique traits and characteristics necessary to work effectively with this specialized population. Research has shown that such specialized sex offender units provide clear advantages for supervision staff and for probation departments as a whole (Gilligan & Talbot, 2000).

Warrants Units Of all the specialized units within probation departments, warrants units are probably the most closely aligned with traditional police functions. The primary purpose of a warrants unit is to investigate, locate, and apprehend fugitive probationers who have had a bench warrant or other arrest warrant issued. Not surprisingly, most departments require probation officers in warrants units to carry firearms, in addition to issuing identification jackets, bullet-proof vests, and other tactical equipment. Warrants units also assist other probation department units with special operations such as executing search orders, effecting home visits of potentially dangerous probationers, and other specialized “law enforcement-oriented” operations.

Empirical Research Findings Regarding the Psychological Assessment of Probation/Surveillance Officer Candidates To date, very little scientific work has been done regarding the psychological evaluation and selection of candidates who occupy probation or surveillance officer positions, and the area has been largely ignored by those who work within the realm of law enforcement psychology. For example, an Advanced EBSCO host search of the PsychARTICLES and PsychINFO databases in April 2009 for the Boolean terms probation, surveillance, psychological, screening, evaluation, and selection yielded zero onpoint hits. While the psychological literature is replete with empirical studies assessing the efficacy of selection tools for predicting police officer performance (e.g., Cortina, Doherty, Schmitt, Kaufman, & Smith, 1992; Inwald & Shusman, 1984; Sarchione, Cuttler, Muchinsky, & Nelson-Gray, 1998; Scogin, Schumacher, Gardner, & Chaplin, 1995), the literature is barren regarding the psychological assessment and selection of probation and surveillance officer candidates. Only one tangentially related dissertation study by Hamill (2001) was found in the literature seeking to determine if cultural sensitivity differences existed among a cohort of probation officer candidates when compared to general community norms. Specifically, in this study probation officer candidates were psychologically assessed by way of the MMPI or MMPI-2 as part of a standard pre-employment screening procedure, and a subset of hired probation officers later completed additional measures after an initial period of employment. While some cultural sensitivity differences related to race, age, and length of employment were found at the pre-employment stage, the differences later diminished as a function of time working within the probation department. Although MMPI and/or MMPI-2 data were systematically collected as part of this exploratory study of probation officer candidates, the data were not systematically analyzed beyond generating supplementary prejudice scale score calculations as a measure of cultural sensitivity (see Duckworth & Anderson, 1995; Dunbar, 1995).

Aggregate Law Enforcement Norms When psychological instruments are used to assess law enforcement candidates, the current preemployment psychological evaluation guidelines of the International Association of Chiefs of Police (IACP) Police Psychological Services Section dictate “tests should have a substantial research base for interpretation with normal range populations in general and public safety applicants in particular” (IACP Police Psychological Services Section, 2009). At present, however, not every psychological instrument that would appear useful for screening police or probation/surveillance candidates

122

Police vs. Probation/Surveillance Officers

has public safety norms available. Moreover, even fewer instruments have “position specific” norms available. Currently, some psychological instruments lump together normative data from different law enforcement occupations under a common rubric of “public safety officers” (e.g., the PAI Law Enforcement, Corrections and Public Safety Selection Report [PAI] (Roberts, Thompson, & Johnson, 2000)), and allow for comparisons to be made between an individual and a pool of literally thousands of police and other public safety applicants (see Roberts et al., 2000). While this practice may allow for comparisons across occupational cohorts where no specific comparison group data are available, we maintain that by doing so important distinctions between vocational cohorts may be minimized. Even though a thread of commonality may run through several related law enforcement occupational groups, aggregating norms in this fashion has a homogenizing effect that may gloss over important distinctions and differences between vocational cohorts. By extension, we also believe it is of utmost importance to know whether psychological screening instruments and procedures historically used and validated with police officer candidates are equally valid and useful for evaluating and selecting probation/surveillance officer candidates. It is sometimes assumed that assessment devices will have equivalent validity with police and probation officer candidates, but predictive validity must be established and cannot be assumed.

Emerging Research Specific to Probation/Surveillance Selection While there is a dearth of published empirical studies on the psychological assessment of probation/ surveillance candidates, the tide is beginning to turn. An in-house study conducted by the Superior Court of Arizona’s Personnel Psychological Services Unit examined the predictive validity of Institute of Personality and Ability Assessment’s (IPAT) PsychEval Personality Questionnaire/Protective Services Report Plus (PEPQ/PSR Plus) in its ability to successfully predict job performance among probation officers. The PEPQ/PSR Plus is a psychological composite intended to provide a global view of protective services candidates (e.g., police officer, fire fighter, security guard, EMT, corrections officer, and other similar occupations) by assessing 16 “normal” personality dimensions as well as 12 “pathology-oriented” dimensions. The PEPQ/PSR Plus specifically contains information about a test taker’s response style, an interpretive section regarding four protective services dimensions, a profile summary of five global factor scales, 16 primary factor scales, 12 pathology-oriented scale scores, and a pathology-oriented index providing a snapshot of a test taker’s psychological health in four critical composite dimensions. In this study, the ability to predict job-related “critical incidents” among probation officers was examined in a group of 203 pre-employment and 60 incumbent probation officer candidates who completed the PEPQ/PSR Plus instrument as part of either a standard pre-employment screening or a pre-arming psychological screening process. Following their evaluation, involvement in subsequent work-related critical incidents (e.g., gross misconduct, demotion, involuntary termination, and so on) was monitored by the employer agency for a period of one year. For pre-employment candidates, this monitoring took place during their initial probationary period of employment, while incumbent officers were not considered on probationary status during this time frame. Through a logistic regression modeling procedure, it was found that (1) a one-unit increase on the PEPQ/PSR Plus Alienation/Perceptual Distortion scale resulted in the odds of a critical incident increasing by 53%, (2) a one-unit increase on the PEPQ/ PSR Plus Interpersonal Relations scale resulted in a decrease in the odds of a critical incident by 3.5 times, and (3) a one-unit increase on the PEPQ/PSR Plus Intellectual Efficiency composite resulted in a decrease in the odds of involvement in a critical incident by 2.87 times, controlling for the other variables (Herrmann & Bidwell, 2009). While these findings should be viewed as preliminary for the time, the PEPQ/PSR Plus is one instrument that has demonstrated some measure of predictive validity when used as an assessment/selection device with probation officer candidates.

123

D. Scott Herrmann and Barbara Broderick

Recommendations for Psychologists Evaluating Probation/Surveillance Candidates For law enforcement psychologists wishing to conduct psychological evaluations on probation/ surveillance candidates, one of the first orders of business is to clearly understand the philosophical outlook of the department for whom the evaluations are conducted. Does the department emphasize a social work perspective, a law enforcement perspective, or a hybrid approach to probation work? Also, what specific duty assignment is the probation/surveillance officer being evaluated for (e.g., standard line officer, specialized unit, and so on)? Is the evaluation for an adult probation department or a juvenile probation department? Once these questions have been answered, the evaluating psychologist must include a number of clinical interview questions to elicit relevant information to determine “goodness of fit” with a specific department and/or duty assignment. Such questions and responses will help determine the degree to which a candidate’s philosophical approach fits within a particular probation department’s culture and the area of assignment a probation/surveillance officer might be best suited for.

Psychological/Personality Variables to Emphasize In addition to clinical interview data, law enforcement psychologists must determine which personality variables and characteristics to emphasize in a written test protocol to screen for “goodness of fit,” and which tests best illuminate the personality variables of interest. As previously noted, the limited research in this area involving probation officers has consistently favored a “rehabilitative perspective” rather than an “enforcement perspective” (Van Landingham et al., 1977; Wright, 1997). Consequently, personality variables such as interpersonal warmth, openness to change, interpersonal sensitivity, and tolerance should be prominently emphasized in most psychological evaluations of probation officer candidates. In calling for such prominence to be placed on personality variables related to the helping/caring aspects of probation work, it is important to note that such is entirely congruent with the personality variable literature involving the selection of traditional police/law enforcement officers. For example, in his extensive meta-analytic review of law enforcement selection studies, Aamodt (2004) found that the personality variable “tolerance” was the single best predictor of performance when compared against all other personality variables. In related research, “conscientiousness” has also emerged as an important personality variable predictor of job performance and work behavior in general (Miller, Griffin, & Hart, 1999; Sarchione et al., 1998; Tett, Jackson, & Rothstein, 1991). Conscientiousness is considered one of the five core dimensions of personality that have recently become known as the “Big Five” dimensions of personality. In short, the Big Five represent broad categories of personality traits that have been empirically shown to largely define human personality. The five overarching dimensions of the Big Five have been identified as extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, neuroticism, and openness. Several instruments have been developed or revised to provide measures of the Big Five personality dimensions among which “conscientiousness is one of the core dimensions” (e.g., the California Psychological Inventory [CPI; Gough, 1995]; the NEO Personality Inventory-Revised [NEO-PI-R; Costa & McCrae, 1992]; the PsychEval Personality Questionnaire/Protective Services Report [PEPQ/ PSR; Cattell, Cattell, Catell, Russel, & Bedwell, 2003]). While some authors have cited a preference for the NEO-PI-R as the most comprehensive and satisfactory measure of conscientiousness (Claussen-Rogers & Arrigo, 2005; Schinka, Kinder, & Kremer, 1997), we maintain that regardless of specific instrumentation choice, law enforcement psychologists should necessarily include at least one validated measure of “conscientiousness” and one validated measure of “tolerance” because both have been consistently linked to performance outcomes in law enforcement groups. 124

Police vs. Probation/Surveillance Officers

Instrumentation Selection In the 21st century, many psychological instruments are available to the law enforcement psychologist wishing to conduct evaluations of police candidates, with many offering specific law enforcement norms and report options. While far fewer options are specifically targeted toward probation/ surveillance candidates, some instrument authors have begun to recognize the important distinctions that exist between police and probation/surveillance cohorts and have recognized the need for specialized instrumentation. Among the instrument options that stand out as most relevant to probation/surveillance screening are (1) the Behavioral Personnel Assessment Device (B-PAD) (The B-PAD Group, 2001), (2) the PsychEval Personality Questionnaire/Protective Services Report Plus (PEPQ/PSR Plus) (Institute of Personality Ability Testing, 2003), and (3) the California Psychological Inventory (CPI) Police and Public Safety Selection Report (Roberts & Johnson, 2001). We feel that each of these instruments should be given serious consideration for inclusion in a comprehensive assessment battery for probation/surveillance officer assessment, since each makes a unique contribution in the ability to evaluate variables and characteristics relevant to probation/ surveillance work.

The B-PAD First, the B-PAD is a video-based behavioral demand test that assesses an applicant’s interpersonal skills and judgments and is based on a rationale and model of test construction called the BehavioralAnalytic Model of Assessing Competence (Goldfried & D’Zurilla, 1969). According to this model, the preferred goal of assessment is the appraisal of competence rather than the presence or absence of global attributes or traits. The B-PAD contains different scenarios portraying difficult interpersonal challenges that are presented to a candidate who is then video recorded as he or she responds to the different scenes. Eight scenarios are presented to ensure that each applicant’s competence is assessed across an appropriate range of situations. What makes this test relevant to probation/surveillance officers is that scene catalogs have been developed for both juvenile and adult probation/ surveillance officer candidates, with job-relevant scenes reflecting the unique challenges that each position might encounter in the field. While validation studies of probation/surveillance versions of the B-PAD have yet to be conducted, multiple outcome studies have assessed the utility of the B-PAD test using police-specific scenes with police officer candidates and have cited support for this instrument’s reliability and validity (e.g., Doerner & Nowell, 1999; Rand, 1987; Stein, 1995; Young, 1992).

The PEPQ/PSR Plus As previously noted, the PEPQ/PSR Plus is one of the few instruments that has been subject to some level of empirical scrutiny regarding its ability to predict performance among probation officers (Herrmann & Bidwell, 2009). It also includes the assessment of a number of normal personality variables thought to be specifically relevant and of interest to probation department employers (e.g., interpersonal warmth, openness to change, interpersonal sensitivity, and rule consciousness [i.e., conscientiousness]). For these reasons, it is also recommended for inclusion in a comprehensive assessment battery, which at a minimum should necessarily include measures of both normal personality variables as well as measures of psychopathologic dysfunction. The Institute for Personality and Ability Testing (IPAT) continues to be involved in active research efforts to provide validity evidence for its public safety screening tools such as the PEPQ/PSR Plus and intends to broaden the scope of its validity efforts to include probation/surveillance officer studies in the years to come (S. Bidwell, IPAT, personal communication, March 24, 2009). 125

D. Scott Herrmann and Barbara Broderick

The California Psychological Inventory (CPI) Police and Public Safety Selection Report The CPI Police and Public Safety Selection Report (CPI; Roberts & Johnson, 2001) is currently the only assessment instrument available to law enforcement psychologists whereby an applicant’s scores can be compared against probation officer norms. Specifically, a probation applicant’s scores can be compared against a sample of 1,174 probation officer applicants, 83 incumbent probation officers, and 6,000 general community member norms. While both the applicant and incumbent probation officer norms are not robust, the instrument is the only psychometric option available to law enforcement psychologists who wish to compare “apples to apples” when screening probation officer candidates as opposed to the more common approach of comparing “apples to oranges” when police officer norms are used as a reference group. However, the downside of this interpretive report is that law enforcement psychologists are forced to choose between norm options that include “Probation Department Officer-Counselor (nonweapon carrying)” and “Weapon Carrying Screening for Nonsworn Position.” No current option is available for “Probation Department Officer-Counselor (weapon carrying).” Nevertheless, by having specific probation officer norms available, the CPI interpretive report by Roberts and Johnson (2001) clearly distinguishes itself as a logical pick among the top-tier choices available for probation/surveillance officer screening. The authors of this interpretive report also allude to their intentions to continue developing probation specific norms in the future. For example, in their CPI Police and Public Safety Selection Report Technical Manual (5th edition), Roberts and Johnson (2001) note that they “intend to provide future updates that will continually expand the value of this report for screening psychologists,” “would like to include larger normative samples of hired, post-probation employees in each job category,” and “intend to add a new profile for probation department juvenile counselors” (p. 39). These intentions were again echoed by report author and statistician Mike Johnson (personal communication, October 14, 2009), who expressed intent to “build this out in the future” by increasing the size and heterogeneity of probation officer norms.

Emerging Trends In addition to the assessment options noted above, the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory2-Revised Format (MMPI-2-RF; Ben-Porath & Tellegen, 2008) is an additional instrument expected to have appeal for screening probation/surveillance officers because of the anticipated ability to generate “local norms” within the next few years. Currently, the MMPI-2-RF allows for scoring options that include separate comparison group data for both law enforcement and corrections officers but currently does not have any direct comparison groups for probation officers. However, as reported by this instrument’s lead author, Yossef Ben-Porath (personal communication, April 29, 2009), the MMPI-2-RF scoring software will soon allow users to create their own comparison group norms by aggregating any MMPI-2-RF data they have accumulated on their individual computer systems, with this functionality expected to be available beginning 2010 or 2011. The ability to easily and efficiently generate local comparison group norms for probation/surveillance cohorts is expected to be a highly useful addition for many users including law enforcement psychologists who routinely work with probation/surveillance candidates where limited normative data currently exist.

Conclusion In summary, the work of probation/surveillance officers is multifaceted and involves a complex blend of both rehabilitative and enforcement functions. Because the area of conducting psychological

126

Police vs. Probation/Surveillance Officers

evaluations and screenings of probation/surveillance officer candidates is evolving rapidly, law enforcement psychologists who perform this work are urged to regularly return to the empirical literature over the next several years to keep abreast of new and emerging trends. Many methodological advancements are currently underway, and more are anticipated, as psychologists become increasingly aware of the abundant practice opportunities that exist within and for probation departments. While the models and templates developed to psychologically evaluate police officer candidates often lend themselves to the task of performing psychological evaluations of probation/surveillance officer candidates, important distinctions between these vocational cohorts require nuanced approaches, methodologies, and perspectives. Probation/surveillance officers should be recognized by the law enforcement psychologist as a distinct but related vocational entity within the law enforcement community—one that possesses its own unique vocational history, mission, culture, and values. As this chapter has attempted to illuminate, it is incumbent on the law enforcement psychologist to be knowledgeable of where the important differences and nuances lie. “Off-the-rack” or “one-sizefits-all” approaches to psychological assessment that too closely approximate standard police psychological evaluation methodologies are likely to miss the mark by failing to recognize important distinctions that do exist.

References and Further Reading Aamodt, M. G. (2004). Research in law enforcement selection. Boca Raton, FL: Brown-Walker. Abadinsky, H. (2009). Probation and parole: Theory and practice (10th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. American Probation and Parole Association. (2006). Adult and juvenile probation and parole national firearm survey (2nd ed.) Retrieved November 12, 2009, from www.appa-net.org/eweb/DynamicPage. aspx?WebCode= VB_SurveyFirearms. Benner, A. W. (1986). Psychological screening of police applicants. In J. T. Reece & H. A. Goldstein (Eds.), Psychological services for law enforcement (pp. 11–19). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice, Federal Bureau of Investigation. Ben-Porath, Y. S., & Tellegen, A. (2008). MMPI-2-RF (Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2-RF): Manual for administration, scoring and interpretation. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. The B-PAD Group. (2001). Behavioral Personnel Assessment Devices: B-PAD. Sonoma, CA: Author. Retrieved from www.bpad.com. Burrell, W. D. (2005). Trends in probation and parole in the states. In K. S. Chi, A. S. Wall, & H. M. Perkins (Eds.), The book of states (37th ed., pp. 595–600). Lexington, KY: Council of State Governments. Burton, V. S., Latessa, E. J., & Barker, T. (1992). The role of probation officers: An examination of statutory requirements. Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice, 8(4), 273–282. Cattell, R. B., Cattell, A. K., Cattell, H. E. P., Russel, M. T., & Bedwell, S. (2003). The PsychEval Personality Questionnaire. Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality and Ability Testing. Champion, D. J. (2008). Probation, parole and community corrections in the United States (6th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. Claussen-Rogers, N. L., & Arrigo, B. A. (2005). Police corruption and psychological testing: A strategy for preemployment screening. Durham, NC: Carolina Academic Press. Colley, L. L, Culbertson, R. G., & Latessa, E. (1986). Probation officer job analysis: Rural-urban differences. Federal Probation, 50(4), 67–71. Cortina, J. M., Doherty, M. I., Schmitt, N., Kaufman, G., and Smith, R. G. (1992). The “Big Five” personality factors in the IPI and MMPI: Predictors of police performance. Personnel Psychology, 45, 119–130. Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Professional manual for the Revised NEO Personality Inventory. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Doerner, W. G., & Nowell, T. M. (1999). The reliability of the behavioral-personnel assessment device (B-PAD) in selecting police recruits. Policing: An International Journal of Police Strategies & Management, 22(3), 343–352. Duckworth, J. C., & Anderson, W. P. (1995). MMPI and MMPI-2: Interpretation for counselors and clinicians (4th ed.). Bristol, PA: Accelerated Development. Dunbar, E. (1995). The prejudiced personality, racism, and anti-Semitism: The PR scale forty years later. Journal of Personality Assessment, 65, 270–277.

127

D. Scott Herrmann and Barbara Broderick English, K., Pullen, S., & Jones, L. (1997). Managing adult sex offenders in the community: A containment approach. Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice. Flexner, B., & Baldwin, R. N. (1916). Juvenile courts and probation. New York: Century Company. Gilligan, L., & Talbot, T. (2000). Community supervision of the sex offender: An overview of current and promising practices. Silver Spring, MD: Center for Sex Offender Management. Goldfried, M., & D’Zurilla, T. (1969). A behavioral-analytic model for assessing competence. In C. D. Spielberger (Ed.), Current topics in clinical community psychology (pp. 151–169). New York: Academic Press. Gough, H. G. (1995). California Psychological Inventory: 434 professional manual. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Hamill, M. A. (2001). Cultural sensitivity of probation officers as a measure for preemployment screening. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, California School of Professional Psychology, Fresno. Herrmann, D. S., & Bidwell, S. (2009). The PEPQ/PSR Plus and the selection of probation officers: A predictive validity analysis. Manuscript in preparation. IACP Police Psychological Services Section (2009). Pre-employment psychological evaluation guidelines. Retrieved January 25, 2010, from www.theiacp.org/psych_services_section/ Institute for Personality and Ability Testing. (2003). PsychEval personality questionnaire/protective services report plus (PEPQ/PSR Plus). Champaign, IL: Author. Retrieved from www.ipat.com. Inwald, R. E., & Shusman, E. J. (1984). The IPI and MMPI as predictors of academy performance for police recruits. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 12, 1–11. Lurigio, A. J., Jones, M., & Smith, B. E. (1995). Child sexual abuse: Its causes, consequences and implications for probation practice. Federal Probation, 69, 69–76. Lyon, D. (2007). Surveillance studies: An overview. Malden, MA: Polity. Maloney, M. P., & Ward, M. P. (1976). Psychological assessment: A conceptual approach. New York: Oxford University Press. Miller, R. L., Griffin, M. A., & Hart, P. M. (1999). Personality and organized health: The role of conscientiousness. Work and Stress, 13(1), 7–19. Rand, R. R. (1987). Behavioral police assessment device: The development and validation of an interactive, pre-employment, job related, video psychological test. Dissertation Abstracts International, 48(3-A), 610–611. (UMI No. 8713282) Reese, J. T. (1995). A history of police psychological services. In M. I. Kurke & E. M. Scrivner (Eds.), Police psychology into the 21st century (pp. 31–44). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Roberts, M. D., & Johnson, M. (2001). CPI police and public safety selection report technical manual. Los Gatos, CA: Law Enforcement Psychological Services. Roberts, M. D., Thompson, J. A., & Johnson, M. (2000). PAI law enforcement, corrections and public safety selection report module manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Roscoe, T., Duffee, D., Rivera, C., & Smith, T. (2005, November). Factors associated with the arming of probation officers: An empirical examination. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Society of Criminology, Royal York, Toronto. Sarchione, C. D., Cuttler, M. J., Muchinsky, P. M., & Nelson-Gray, R. O. (1998). Prediction of dysfunctional job behaviors among law enforcement officers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83(6), 904–912. Schinka, J. A., Kinder, B. N., & Kremer, T. (1997). Research validity scales for the NEO-PI-R: Development and initial validation. Journal of Personality Assessment, 68, 127–138. Schultz, J. L. (1973). The cycle of juvenile court history. Crime and Delinquency, 19(4), 457–476. Scogin, F., Schumacher, J., Gardner, J., & Chaplin, W. (1995). Predictive validity of psychological testing in law enforcement settings. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 26(1), 68–71. Small, S., & Torres, S. (2001). Arming probation officers: Enhancing public confidence and officer safety. Federal Probation, 65(3), 24–28. Stalans, L. J. (2004). Adult sex offenders on community supervision: A review of recent assessment strategies and treatment. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 31, 564–608. Stein, S. (1995). Police officer selection: An inquiry into the scoring criteria of the Behavioral Personnel Assessment Device for police. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 56(3-B), 1741. Terman, L., & Otis, A. (1917). A trial of mental and pedagogical tests in a civil service examination for policemen and firemen. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1, 17–29. Tett, R. P., Jackson, D. N., & Rothstein, M. (1991). Personality measures as predictors of job performance: A meta-analytic review. Personnel Psychology, 44, 703–741. U.S. Department of Labor. (2008). Occupational outlook handbook (2008–2009 ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

128

Police vs. Probation/Surveillance Officers Van Landignham, D. E., Taber, M., & Dimants, E. R. (1977). How probation officers view their job responsibilities. In D. B. Kennedy (Ed.), The dysfunctional alliance: Emotion and reason in justice administration. Cincinnati, OH: Anderson Press. Wright, C. E. (1997). The relationship of case management strategies to probation officer personality types. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Wilmington College (UMI No. 9728590). Young, T. (1992). Use of the Behavioral Police Assessment Device in the selection of law enforcement officers. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, The Professional School of Psychology (UMI No. LD02540).

129

PART 2

Pre-employment Psychological Screening

6 CRITERION-RELATED PSYCHOLOGICAL EVALUATIONS Peter A. Weiss and William U. Weiss

Introduction Use of psychological assessment instruments such as the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2 (MMPI-2), Personality Assessment Inventory (PAI), and California Psychological Inventory (CPI) has become a frequent practice in police psychological evaluations in recent years. Such instruments are often used in conditional pre-employment psychological evaluations as well as for other purposes such as fitness-for-duty (FFDE) evaluations (Weiss, 2010; Weiss, Weiss, & Gacono, 2008). A major issue with such assessment instruments is that many of these tests were originally designed for purposes of clinical diagnosis and treatment planning and are not in fact instruments originally designed for use in employment settings. While certain newer instruments, such as the M-PULSE (Davis & Rostow, 2008), were designed especially for purposes of police psychological evaluations and were originally validated with law enforcement populations, most of these instruments were not, and validity studies had to be performed to make these instruments (particularly personality inventories such as the MMPI-2 and PAI) acceptable for use in law enforcement evaluations. As a result of the current emphasis by the American Psychological Association (APA) on promoting evidence-based practice, it is important for psychologists using such instruments in their practice of police psychology to have an understanding of the validity of these tests for applications in police work. The purpose of this chapter is (1) to better acquaint the practicing police psychologist with the validity issues applicable to the use of such assessment instruments, and then (2) to review the evidence supporting the validity of the most popular instruments.

Test Validity Issues in Police Psychological Evaluations In teaching about the validity of psychological tests, the present authors have divided it into three components. The first may be termed rational validity, which may be further broken down into face validity and content validity. Essentially, rational validity is the degree to which an assessment instrument makes sense during an inspection process—in other words, whether the test items appear to measure what the test developers say they should measure. Face validity is whether or not test items obviously measure the characteristic the test developer is attempting to measure. According to Kazdin (1998), this is not typically part of the psychometric development of a measure but rather an informal evaluation of its usefulness. Content validity is evidence that the items on a test or measure

133

Peter A. Weiss and William U. Weiss

relate to the concept underlying the measure. It asks the question: Does the test appropriately sample the universe of items associated with the concept? Problems with face or content validity are rarely a problem in contemporary psychological testing; tests that are ultimately validated from predictive, construct, and criterion points of view also have good face and content validity. However, this is an important first question to ask in establishing the validity of a measure for any purpose: Does this instrument at least appear to measure what it claims to measure on the surface? The second type of validity is construct validity, which relates to the degree to which a test supposedly measures the construct, or concept, that it claims to measure. For example, does this test actually measure a concept such as depression, locus of control, or self-esteem? The concept of construct validity has been extensively explored by Campbell and Fiske (1959), who proposed the multitraitmultimethod matrix for establishing the construct validity of a measure. The kind of test validity that is perhaps the most important for police psychologists and other mental health practitioners, however, is the third kind, which is commonly referred to as criterionrelated validity. This is the degree to which a particular measure can be used to predict some external criterion. For example, if scores on the MMPI-2 could be used to accurately predict termination for cause within police departments, then the MMPI-2 would be said to have criterion-related validity for that purpose. According to Kazdin (1998) psychologists use both concurrent validity (correlation with a measure at one point in time) and predictive validity (correlation with a measure at some point in the future) as ways of establishing the criterion-related validity of an instrument. Most police psychologists are concerned with the degree to which assessment instruments used as part of a psychological evaluation can be used to predict performance—either good or bad—at some point in the future, so predictive validity is the aspect of criterion-related validity most often explored by police psychologists. However, there are special issues with validating tests used for law enforcement purposes that make establishing their criterion-related (especially predictive) validity difficult.

Problems and Limitations in Law Enforcement Test Research While tests used in psychological evaluations in law enforcement need to be validated for their predictive validity, particularly in the area of police selection, there are a number of limitations to research that must be overcome. A major limitation of such research is that pure predictive validity studies are not practical. A pure predictive validity study in the area of law enforcement would be one in which a wide variety of individuals obtained through random sampling are given a test and then hired as police officers; next, the data would be analyzed to determine if the test predicted either poor or good performance. However, such a procedure would undoubtedly expose police departments to lawsuits because hiring applicants using a random sampling procedure would result in individuals becoming police officers who exhibited performance problems. Given that law enforcement is a sensitive, high-risk profession in which officers have the right, under certain circumstances, to take life and liberty, such an approach to research would be highly impractical. Another related limitation is that psychological evaluations are the last thing considered in making law enforcement hiring decisions. According to the Americans with Disabilities Act (ADA), such evaluations—even if mandated by law—can only be performed after all other factors have been considered and a conditional offer of employment has been extended to the candidate (Weiss, Weiss et al., 2008). While certain kinds of psychological tests can be given on a pre-offer basis (see, for example, Jones, Cunningham, & Dages, 2010), most psychological tests fall into the category of “medical tests” and therefore can only be used postoffer. While this restriction is obviously necessary to prevent discrimination, it also presents a challenge to law enforcement researchers because subjects who find their way into police validity studies tend to be more psychologically healthy than the rest of the general population (Weiss, Hitchcock, Weiss, Rostow, & Davis, 2008). 134

Criterion-Related Psychological Evaluations

These problems, while unavoidable, create a situation in which criterion-related validity studies in police psychology lose power. Most data samples for research on police psychological evaluations consist of assessment data collected during the course of routine evaluations. As stated earlier, this means the sample has been highly preselected, and most individuals with unusual behavioral or personality characteristics are already eliminated from the sample at earlier stages of the process. Therefore, the researcher must work with data samples that have extreme restrictions on predictor variables. As an example, the sample in the Weiss, Hitchcock, et al. (2008) study had mean PAI clinical scale scores that were a half standard deviation below the population mean, and most other criterion-related studies of police evaluations have similar sets of descriptive statistics. In addition, in making these preselections, we also limit the range of the criterion variables because most individuals in the sample wind up being excellent candidates for police work. For example, few candidates who are eventually hired will be terminated for cause, or engage in seriously problematic on-the-job behaviors because of the rigor of modern police-screening procedures. As a result of the truncation of range of both criterion and predictor variables, validation studies in police psychology lose a great deal of power, and effect sizes tend to be small.

The Issue of Effect in Criterion Validity Police Studies The issues described above make it clear that researchers, and consumers of research, must keep an open mind when reading and interpreting validity studies of police psychological evaluations. Moreover, small effect sizes in research can be highly significant when the stakes are high, as they are in police psychological evaluations. Making an error in a pre-employment psychological evaluation can result in serious departmental liability problems—or even loss of life. Therefore, the researcher and consumer must carefully weigh results to determine the true significance of the findings. High correlations are rarely found in this kind of validation research, yet this does not mean that the tests discussed above have no predictive power. Rather, most studies suffer from truncation of range, and small effects can be highly important. A more thorough discussion of these issues can be found in Hitchcock, O’Conner, and Weiss (2010).

Evidence for the Validity of Tests in Police Psychological Evaluations Ethically, psychologists should not be using instruments in their evaluations that have no predictive power. According to APA guidelines, psychologists should follow the basic principles of evidencebased practice (American Psychological Association, 2002). For police psychological evaluations, what this means for the practitioner is that the tests used as part of the evaluation procedure have been proven to work for the purposes for which they are used. This emphasis is particularly important when using tests relating to personality and psychopathology, because in most cases (for example, the MMPI-2 and PAI), these tests were not originally developed for use with police populations. In such cases, being able to present evidence for the usefulness of such tests for police evaluations is crucial. As stated earlier, although effect sizes in research on police psychological evaluations are small, in most cases these effects are small but significant. There are a number of measures for which extensive validation research exists and justifies their use. The remainder of this chapter is devoted to a discussion of the evidence for the validity of some of the most popular measures used in pre-employment police psychological evaluations. The authors hope readers will find this summary useful for justifying their own evidence-based practice. It should be noted that instruments originally developed as measures of personality and psychopathology will be dealt with separately from instruments originally designed for use with, and normed on police populations, as differences exist in how such instruments may be used and interpreted. 135

Peter A. Weiss and William U. Weiss

Tests of Personality and Psychopathology The MMPI-2 The MMPI-2 and its predecessor, the MMPI, have been used for years in the field of police psychology. In 1959, King, Norrell, and Erlandson published an article in which they attempted to use the original MMPI to predict police academy grades. This article represents the first attempt to validate this test for personnel selection. Since that time, numerous validation articles have been published on the MMPI and MMPI-2 in police psychological evaluations (Weiss & Weiss, 2010). The MMPI was restandardized in 1989, and the resultant version, the MMPI-2, is the one in use today. It is similar to the original MMPI, but it has a more representative standardization sample, and item content has been revised so that objectionable items were deleted and archaic language updated. The test is interpreted using standard (T) scores, which have a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10. Any T score greater than 65 on the MMPI-2 is considered clinically significant and therefore noteworthy. The main purpose of the MMPI-2 is the detection of psychopathology. Therefore, in accordance with the Americans with Disabilities Act, it is considered a medical test and is only considered appropriate for use in police selection after all other factors have been considered. However, its criterion validity for use as a postoffer screening measure has been relatively well established, particularly with regard to the validity scales. The use of the L (Lie) scale as a tool in postoffer police selection has been investigated by Weiss, Davis, Rostow, and Kinsman (2003). The hypotheses for the Weiss et al. (2003) study were taken primarily from earlier work by Herndon (1998) and Boes, Chandler, and Timm (1997), which suggested that individuals who obtain elevated L scale scores on the MMPI-2 tend to engage in problem behaviors if hired by police departments. In this study, which used a sample of 1,347 officers, it was discovered that high L scale scores were associated with future performance problems such as termination for cause, knowledge mistakes, failure to complete the requirements for traditional hire, and insubordination. While such candidates appear normal in a clinical interview, their denial, excessive sense of virtue, and lack of a sense of their own shortcomings make them poor candidates for a high-stress occupation such as police work. In addition, their high-L presentation may in part be due to attempts to conceal psychological adjustment problems. Weiss et al. (2003) recommend a raw score cutoff of 8 for psychological screening. A follow-up study (Weiss, Weiss, Davis, & Rostow, 2010) has confirmed the usefulness of this cutoff score. While research on the L scale as a tool in police selection has shown the usefulness of the scale, the clinical scales have presented a more mixed picture. A full review of the evidence for the use of the MMPI-2 Basic Scales, as well as other subscales and scales, can be found in Weiss and Weiss (2010) and is beyond the scope of the present chapter. However, Brewster and Stoloff (1999) have made the argument based on prior MMPI-2 research that any applicant obtaining a T score greater than 65 on any of the MMPI-2 Basic Scales (with the possible exception of scale 5) can be justifiably removed from the applicant pool. In addition, other studies—particularly that of Sellbom, Fischler, and BenPorath (2007)—have suggested the usefulness of the Basic Scales in the psychological screening of law enforcement officers. Aamodt (2004), in his meta-analysis of test results in law enforcement selection, has presented a more modest picture of the ability of the MMPI-2 to discriminate between good and bad applicants. Aamodt states that Scale 9 (Ma) is significantly correlated but at a low level, with poor supervisor ratings of performance and poorer police academy grades as a law enforcement officer. Aamodt states that other Basic Scales produced insignificant results for a number of criterion variables in his metaanalysis. However, it should be noted that the truncation-of-range problems discussed earlier in the chapter are likely to be problems in such a meta-analysis; very few hired applicants actually develop performance problems, and very few applicants with extreme scores on the MMPI-2 even make it to the psychological evaluation phase due to ADA regulations. However, it does seem a relatively safe 136

Criterion-Related Psychological Evaluations

practice to eliminate individuals with extreme MMPI-2 clinical scale scores from the applicant pool, although such individuals are rare because of contemporary assessment practices. As stated earlier, most applicants with personality problems or levels of psychopathology that would cause elevated scores on MMPI-2 clinical scales are eliminated from the applicant pool at earlier stages of the hiring process. Those individuals given a psychological evaluation (the last procedure in the hiring process) typically produce clinical (but not necessarily validity) scale scores that are within the normative range.

The Personality Assessment Inventory (PAI) The Personality Assessment Inventory, or PAI (Morey, 2007), has been the subject of fairly extensive criterion-related validity research by the present authors in recent years. For the last 10 years, we have researched all of the validity scales as well as a number of the clinical scales that appeared related to law enforcement work. The PAI, like the MMPI-2, is a personality inventory oriented around measuring psychopathology, and it has several features that have made it generally appealing to most psychologists doing self-report evaluations. These features have caught the attention of police psychologists as well. It is shorter (344 as opposed to 567 items) and easier to read, and it has a 4-point scale for each item as opposed to a forced-choice true/false format. It also has scales that provide general diagnostic information—such as those measuring certain personality disorders—not generally in use on the MMPI-2. The PAI takes a somewhat different approach to the assessment of profile validity than the MMPI-2. Two of its scales, Inconsistency (ICN) and Infrequency (INF), measure not a response style but rather whether an individual is paying attention to item content when taking the test. These are similar in function to the VRIN and TRIN scales on the MMPI-2. The VRIN scale on the MMPI-2 is a measure of random responding that compares a series of similar item pairs. Individuals who respond randomly to the MMPI-2 typically endorse these similar items in opposite directions due to inattention. The TRIN scale is a measure of all-true and all-false responding that compares items with similar content typically endorsed by subjects in opposite directions. The ICN scale also works by comparing item pairs with similar content, but INF is derived in a different manner from VRIN and TRIN. The INF scale consists of items infrequently endorsed by all subjects regardless of psychopathology. Individuals scoring high on these scales (ICN and INF) have generally produced PAI profiles that are invalid and are not interpretable, because they have answered randomly, or have trouble with attention or reading comprehension. However, the two other validity scales on the PAI, Negative Impression (NIM) and Positive Impression (PIM), have been of much more interest to researchers. Studies by Weiss, Rostow, Davis, and Decoster-Martin (2004), and Weiss, Zehner, Davis, Rostow, and Decoster-Martin (2005) showed that elevated NIM scores are modestly correlated with problem performance as a police officer after hire. These studies discovered that high NIM officers engaged in neglect of duty, made conduct mistakes, and were more likely than other officers to receive reprimands from supervisors. While part of the NIM effect may have been due to associated elevations in clinical scales, these findings are interesting and useful for the police psychologist and suggest that high NIM scorers can be excluded from applicant pools. Unlike the MMPI-2, in which the L-scale, a measure of positive impression management, has been associated with poor performance as a law enforcement officer, such findings have not been confirmed on the PAI with PIM. In fact, in the study by Weiss et al. (2004), higher scores on PIM were very modestly correlated with good performance as a police officer for some outcome variables. This finding is probably because research has shown that PIM measures a different aspect of impression management than the MMPI-2 L scale (Weiss, Serafino, & Serafino, 2000). PIM probably is a good measure of positive impression management; however, L is more a measure of deceptiveness. Some criterion-related research has also been performed on the PAI clinical scales for purposes of law enforcement selection. The Antisocial (ANT) scales on the PAI have been relatively well 137

Peter A. Weiss and William U. Weiss

researched for their ability to predict performance as a police officer. The Weiss et al. (2004) and Weiss et al. (2005) studies showed that the ANT full scale and its subscales (ANT-A/Antisocial Behaviors, ANT-E/Egocentricity, ANT-S/Stimulus Seeking) were associated with problem behaviors such as insubordination, excessive citizen complaints, neglect of duty, conduct mistakes, and termination for cause. Further investigation using a multiple regression format (Weiss et al., 2005) showed that these scales can be used together to predict the performance of law enforcement personnel. This research evidence suggests that a case could be made to eliminate such individuals with high scores (T > 70) from the applicant pool in police selection work. Similarly, support for the criterion-related validity of the Aggression (AGG) scale and its subscales also exists due to the Weiss et al. (2004) study. This study suggested that clinically elevated scores on the AGG scale and its subscales (Physical Aggression/AGG-P, Aggressive Attitude/AGG-A, and Verbal Aggression/AGG-V) are associated with a number of negative performance characteristics. One major study (Weiss, Hitchcock, et al., 2008) has been conducted on the PAI Borderline scales. This study also included the Drug and Alcohol scales as predictor variables as well. A total performance score that summed performance errors across 32 performance variables was used as the criterion variable. This study did not obtain significant results with the entire sample, but when a subsample of the 132 officers who exhibited the most on-the-job performance problems was used, significant correlations with poor performance were found for the Borderline Full Scale (BOR), BOR-N (Negative Relations), and DRG (Drug Use) scales. This study provides further support for the use of the PAI in the pre-employment screening of law enforcement officers. Despite the extensive research on scales measuring validity, problem behaviors, personality disorders, and alcohol/drug use, virtually no peer-reviewed research exists supporting the criterionrelated validity of the PAI scales that measure primarily Axis I psychopathology. However, this is not likely due to problems with the validity of these scales; rather, it is probably due to the issue, discussed earlier, of few individuals exhibiting such psychopathology ever getting to the psychological evaluation part of the hiring process in the first place. Weiss, Hitchcock, et al. (2008) comment on the fact that law enforcement candidates tend to produce profiles on self-report tests of psychopathology that have lower means than those found in normative samples, and very few individuals produce scale elevations. While new research should be conducted, this lack of research evidence speaks to the problems with criterion-related validity research discussed earlier.

The California Psychological Inventory (CPI) The California Psychological Inventory (CPI) has become a popular pre-employment screening measure in recent years because it can be used alongside a test of psychopathology as a measure of more ordinary, commonplace personality characteristics. The CPI has 434 items and measures 20 dimensions of personality over four major areas of personality: measures of poise, measures of normative orientation and values, measures of cognitive and intellectual functioning, and measures of role and interpersonal style. Over the years, the CPI has proven to be one of the more popular measures used in pre-employment screening for police and security personnel. It was the second most widely used test for pre-employment screening in Super’s (2006) survey of pre-employment screening instruments. Although this study was limited geographically (it mainly included departments in the southeastern United States), there is little question that it is popular because it is not a test of psychopathology and has been shown to be useful for pre-employment purposes. It has been frequently used in conjunction with tests of psychopathology so that psychologists will have one test oriented toward psychopathology (such as the PAI or MMPI-2) and one oriented toward more commonplace personality characteristics. There have been multiple validation studies on the CPI. However, Aamodt (2004) ran a metaanalysis of studies of the CPI in predicting the criterion variables of performance ratings, academy 138

Criterion-Related Psychological Evaluations

performance, and discipline problems. This meta-analysis is probably the best overall summary of the efficacy of the CPI for police work. The meta-analysis for performance ratings was a rather large study as it involved between 13 and 17 studies, with overall N’s of between 1,072 and 1,400 participants. While most of the r’s in Aamodt’s meta-analysis were small, as with most criterion-related police research, moderate correlations were found for the Tolerance and Intellectual Efficiency Scales, which produced some correlations above .20, which are fairly large effects in this type of research. According to Aamodt (2004), individuals scoring high in tolerance are tolerant, nonjudgmental, and resourceful; those scoring high in Intellectual Efficiency are intelligent, clear thinking, and capable. According to Aamodt (2004), these are desirable characteristics in law enforcement officers, and low scorers on these scales at times have performance problems; similarly, officers high in these characteristics tend to do well on the job and are well liked by supervisors. Certainly, a case can be made for using the CPI—particularly these two scales—in the pre-employment screening of law enforcement officers.

Tests Designed for Law Enforcement Populations The Inwald Personality Inventory (IPI) The Inwald Personality Inventory (Inwald, 1982) was the first test developed and validated for the psychological screening of applicants for high-risk occupations. Inwald (2008) has extensively documented the process of development and validation of the IPI, so users of this test are strongly encouraged to consult her article for a thorough understanding of the criterion-related validity of the IPI. However, a brief description of the validity of the IPI is presented here. The IPI is a 310-item true/ false self-report inventory that focuses mainly on admitted past behavior patterns in an attempt to predict future job-related behaviors (Inwald, 2008). Particular emphasis was placed on admissions of past antisocial behaviors during the development of this test. When initially published, the test had the advantage of a very large normative sample of more than 2,500 law enforcement candidates, and early research on the IPI showed considerable promise. According to Inwald (2008), an independent meta-analysis conducted using the IPI studies available in 1991 (Ones, Viswesveran, Schmidt, & Schultz, 1991; Ones, Viswesveran, & Schmidt, 1993) revealed a very respectable validity coefficient of .37 for prediction of future job performance. Since the early 1990s, multiple validity studies have been conducted on the IPI. Because of space concerns, we will not list all of these studies here. However, the police psychologist interested in gaining a thorough understanding of the validity behind the IPI is strongly encouraged to consult Inwald’s (2008) article, which contains a thorough list of abstracts related to validity research on the IPI. In addition, Inwald has developed multiple other measures that are often used in police psychology, and a thorough discussion of these can be found in this article as well. The IPI continues to be the most popular of the Inwald–Hilson measures for police psychological assessment (see, for example, Super, 2006).

The Matrix-Predictive Uniform Law Enforcement SELECTION EVALUATION INVENTORY (M-PULSE)

An exciting new addition to the battery of tests available to the police psychologist is the MatrixPredictive Uniform Law Enforcement Selection Evaluation Inventory, commonly known as the M-PULSE (Davis & Rostow, 2008). The M-PULSE is a 455-item inventory scored on a four-point scale that focuses primarily on identifying law enforcement officer candidates at risk for specific liabilities most frequently associated 139

Peter A. Weiss and William U. Weiss

with performance problems as a law enforcement officer. The M-PULSE consists of 18 liability scales that focus on these specific areas such as Potential for Termination and Criminal Conduct. It also has four empirical scales, each with a series of subscales, that assess personality characteristics and attitudes that could negatively influence law enforcement work. These are Negative Self-Issues, Negative Perceptions Related to Law Enforcement, Unethical Behavior, and Unpredictability. The M-PULSE also has two validity scales to assess the degree to which the examinee responds in an open and honest fashion. While the M-PULSE is a new instrument in the field of police selection, the results for correct classification of various future performance problems as a law enforcement officer using the liability scales have been particularly impressive. The classification rates for each performance problem using these scales are presented in detail in the M-PULSE manual (Davis & Rostow, 2008) based on the norming sample of 2,000 officers. While further validation research on the M-PULSE is currently conducted, this test appears to present an actuarial method for identifying officer candidates who are at risk for potential performance problems. OTHER MEASURES

This review has attempted to take into account the most popular and recent measures used for police evaluations and to provide the reader with some understanding of the basis for their criterion validity. However, many measures have been used in the psychological assessment of law enforcement officers. For example, other tests such as the 16PF and Wonderlic Personnel Test have sometimes been used, but probably not with the frequency of the tests reviewed above. In recent years, there has been some discussion in the literature regarding the potential use of the Rorschach Comprehensive System in police psychological evaluations, mainly due to the fact that it is less susceptible to attempts at impression management than most other self-report tests (Brewster, Wickline, & Stoloff, 2010; Weiss, 2002; Weiss, Weiss et al., 2008). While the Rorschach certainly has a great deal of potential, the process of conducting validation research has been slow, and much research needs to be performed to establish its criterion validity in police psychological evaluations. The psychologist wishing to use tests for evaluations should be aware of their established validity for the type of evaluation that he or she is conducting, especially with those instruments encountered less frequently in police work.

Conclusion In this chapter, we discussed the importance of criterion-related validity in police psychological evaluations. Clearly, in making decisions regarding such issues as selection of law enforcement personnel, the psychologist should be aware of the ability of a chosen test to perform a particular function. Fortunately, criterion-related validity studies exist for the vast majority of tests currently used in police psychological evaluations. A brief review of the criterion-related validity data for a number of the most popular and recent tests is presented here. However, we encourage readers to investigate these issues further and to become knowledgeable consumers of research on the tests they utilize in clinical practice.

References and Further Reading Aamodt, M. G. (2004). Research in law enforcement selection. Boca Raton, FL: Brown Walker. American Psychological Association. (2002). Ethical principles of examiners and code of conduct. American Psychologist, 57, 1060–1073. Boes, J. O., Chandler, C. J., & Timm, H. W. (1997). Police integrity: Use of personality measures to identify corruptionprone officers. Monterey, CA: Defense Personnel Security Research Center. Brewster, J., & Stoloff, M. L. (1999). Using the good cop/bad cop profile with the MMPI-2. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 14(2), 29–34.

140

Criterion-Related Psychological Evaluations Brewster, J., Wickline, P. W., & Stoloff, M. L. (2010). Using the Rorschach Comprehensive System in police psychology. In P. A. Weiss (Ed.), Personality assessment in police psychology: A 21st century perspective (pp. 188– 226). Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas. Campbell, D. T., & Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 56, 81–105. Davis, R. D., & Rostow, C. D. (2008). M-PULSE inventory: Matrix-predictive uniform law enforcement selection evaluation inventory technical. Toronto: MHS. Herndon, J. (1998, October). Correlates of the MMPI-2 L Scale: Elevations in an LEO selection test battery. Paper presented at the 27th annual meeting of the Society for Police and Criminal Psychology, Portland, OR. Hitchcock, J. H., O’Conner, R., & Weiss, P. A. (2010). Effect sizes in police psychology personality assessment research: A primer. In P. A. Weiss (Ed.), Personality assessment in police psychology: A 21st century perspective (pp. 250–278). Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas. Inwald, R. E. (1982). Inwald Personality Inventory (IPI) technical manual. New York: Hilson Research, Inc. (Reprinted [2008], Chicago IL: IPAT, Inc., a subsidiary of OPP Ltd.) Inwald, R. E. (2008). The Inwald Personality Inventory (IPI) and Hilson Research inventories: Development & rationale. Journal of Aggression and Violent Behavior, 13, 298–327. Jones, J. W., Cunningham, M. R., & Dages, K. D. (2010). Pre-offer police integrity testing: Scientific foundation and professional issues (pp. 159–187). In P. A. Weiss (Ed.), Personality assessment in police psychology: A 21st century perspective. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas. Kazdin, A. (1998). Research design in clinical psychology (3rd ed.). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon. King, P., Norrell, G., & Erlandson, F. L. (1959). The prediction of academic success in a police administration curriculum. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 19, 649–651. Morey, L. C. (2007). Personality Assessment Inventory: Professional manual (2nd ed.). Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Ones, D. S., Viswesveran, C., & Schmidt, F. L. (1993). Comprehensive meta-analysis of integrity test validation: Findings and implications for personnel selection and theories of job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 679–703. Ones, D. S., Viswesveran, C., Schmidt, F. L., & Schultz, S. D. (1991). Meta-analysis results for criterion-related validity of the Inwald Personality Inventory. Unpublished manuscript, University of Iowa, Department of Management and Organizations. Sellbom, M., Fischler, G. L., & Ben-Porath, Y. S. (2007). Identifying MMPI-2 predictors of police officer integrity and misconduct. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 34, 985–1004. Super, J. T. (2006). A survey of pre-employment psychological evaluation tests and procedures. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 21(2), 83–90. Weiss, P. A. (2002). Potential uses of the Rorschach in the selection of police officers. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 17(2), 63–70. Weiss, P. A., Hitchcock, J. H., Weiss, W. U., Rostow, C., & Davis, R. (2008). The Personality Assessment Inventory borderline, drug, and alcohol scales as predictors of overall performance in police officers: A series of exploratory analyses. Policing and Society, 18, 301–310. Weiss, P. A., & Weiss, W. U. (2010). Using the MMPI-2 in police psychological assessment. In P. A. Weiss (Ed.), Personality assessment in police psychology: A 21st century perspective (pp. 59–71). Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas. Weiss, P. A., Weiss, W. U., & Gacono, C. B. (2008). The use of the Rorschach in police psychology: Some preliminary thoughts. In C. B. Gacono & F. B. Evans (Eds.), The handbook of forensic Rorschach assessment (pp. 527–542). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Weiss, W. U. (2010). Procedural considerations in security personnel selection. In P. A. Weiss (Ed.), Personality assessment in police psychology: A 21st century perspective (pp. 299–316). Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas. Weiss, W. U., Davis, R., Rostow, C., & Kinsman, S. (2003). The MMPI-2 L scale as a tool in police selection. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 18(1), 57–60. Weiss, W. U., Rostow, C., Davis, R., & Decoster-Martin, E. (2004). The Personality Assessment Inventory as a selection device for law enforcement personnel. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 19(2), 23–29. Weiss, W. U., Serafino, G., & Serafino, A. (2000). A study of the interrelationships of several validity scales used in police selection. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 15(1), 41–44. Weiss, W. U., Weiss, P. A., Davis, R. D., & Rostow, C. D. (2010, March). Exploring the MMPI-2 L scale cutoff in police selection. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Personality Assessment, San Jose, CA. Weiss, W. U., Zehner, S. N., Davis, R. D., Rostow, C., & Decoster-Martin, E. (2005). Problematic police performance and the Personality Assessment Inventory. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 20(1), 16–21.

141

7 ACTUARIAL VS. CLINICAL JUDGMENT PREDICTION MODELS IN PRE-EMPLOYMENT PSYCHOLOGICAL SCREENING OF POLICE CANDIDATES Michael J. Cuttler Introduction Psychological assessment of candidates for employment as police officers has become a widespread practice in the United States and most, if not all, police psychologists are either familiar with or directly involved in this practice. In a recent national survey, Cochrane, Tett, and Vandecreek (2003) surveyed a cross-section of small, medium, and large police agencies and found that as many as 90% of the departments surveyed currently use psychological testing in their pre-employment selection. Bartol (1996) surveyed police psychologists in regard to their activities and reported that preemployment assessment accounts for the highest percentage of time expended (34.3%) among this group of practitioners. Scrivner and Kurke (1996) surveyed police psychologists employed by the 50 largest U.S. police agencies and found that 71% of this sample reported performing pre-­ employment psychological evaluations as part of their regular activities. Based on employment figures published by the U.S. Department of Justice (2007), it has been estimated that as many as 100,000 pre-­employment assessments of police officers are performed each year by as many as 4,500 psychologists (Corey, Cuttler, & Moss, 2009). The earliest research in pre-employment assessment/selection of police officers was developed within the disciplines of I/O and clinical psychology, albeit without evidence of significant interdisciplinary interaction. After 1980, a number of professional presentations and publications also appeared in conjunction with the emergence of forensic psychology as a distinct specialty (Monahan, 1981; Shapiro, 1983; Ziskin, 1981). Subsequently, societal, legal, and technological developments (e.g., the Americans With Disabilities Act, 1990; the Civil Rights Act, 1991; and the Equal Employment Opportunity Commission’s Uniform Guidelines on Employee Selection, 1978, as cited in Equal Employment Opportunity Commission, ADA Division, Office of Legal Counsel, 1995) encouraged convergence of research, practice, and perspectives within these disciplines. Convergence of research in this regard is reflected in the literature, particularly through a number of retrospective validity studies and meta-analyses, while convergence in practice is seen through development of specialized test instruments, scales, and other techniques. However, although instrument design and supporting research have converged across clinical, industrial, and forensic disciplines, there has been considerably less discussion in the literature regarding interpretive strategy from the practitioner 142

Actuarial vs. Clinical Judgment Prediction Models

perspective. From this perspective, consideration of the research in regard to actuarial prediction vs. clinical judgment is particularly apropos.1 The actuarial assessment literature can also serve as a conceptual bridge between these disciplines. Such is the purpose, focus, and organization of this chapter.

History* Dawes (2005), in her article “The Ethical Implications of Paul Meehl’s Work on Comparing Clinical Versus Actuarial Prediction Methods,” describes a lake in Massachusetts (Webster) that historically had a long Native American name (Chargoggaggoggmanchargagoggcharbunagungamaug) which, according to Dawes, translates to “I fish on my side, you fish on your side, and no one fishes in the middle” (p. 1245). Assessment of police officer candidates has historical roots in both the clinical and the I/O literature, but until the early 1980s, few (if any) fished in the middle of the lake. Drees, Ones, Cullen, Spilberg, and Viswesvaran (2003) suggest the practice of screening police officers for critical traits may be traced back to 1829 London (see also Chenoweth, 1961; Matarazzo, Allen, Saslow, & Wiens, 1964). Cochrane et al. (2003) cite the President’s Commission on Law Enforcement and the Administration of Justice (1967) which recommended screening of all potential officers at that time. Subsequently, the National Advisory Commission on Criminal Justice Standards and Goals Task Force on Police (1973) recommended a standard screening procedure to include (a) a written test of mental ability or aptitude, (b) an oral interview, (c) a psychological examination, and (d) a background investigation. These reports are identified in the I/O literature as the primary antecedents of a number of studies published during that time frame which focused on operational definition of job success in police work as well as linkages to skill and ability measures (Cohen & Chaiken, 1973; Dunnette & Motowidlo, 1976; Gordon & Kleiman, 1976; Kent & Eisenberg, 1972; Landy, 1976; Smith & Stotland, 1973). In contrast to activity in the I/O area, examination of the clinical literature during the 1960s and 1970s supports the impression that the early state of the art in regard to clinical psychological testing and assessment of police officer candidates was characterized by not only a preponderance of descriptive information regarding the psychological characteristics of officers but also a dearth of objective data and/or testable hypotheses regarding personality and psychological findings. Lefkowitz (1977), in his review of police selection procedures titled “Industrial organizational psychology and the police,” cited evidence that the use of psychological/psychiatric inquiry began to emerge as early as 1938 and had been adopted by a number of large municipalities by the mid-1950s (see also Matarazzo et al., 1964). However, Lefkowitz further noted that although psychological tests and interviews were increasingly included in published investigations of police selection during the 1960s and 1970s, substantive findings in the clinical literature were sparse. Contrasting the clinical literature of the time to the I/O studies noted above, he asserts that the results of psychological tests and clinical interviews were rarely, if ever, directly linked to job performance or designed in such a way as to facilitate cross-validation and/or other forms of scientific inquiry. The published reports of clinical evaluations of police candidates’ emotional fitness usually contain rich descriptive accounts of typical personality and mental functioning. However, they are often devoid of data other than the clinician’s personal impressions. . . . This is often a function of the fact that the clinicians are assessing qualities for which few objective or standardized measuring instruments exist. (Lefkowitz, 1977, p. 354) Similarly, Cuttler and Muchinsky (2006) note that interest in pre-employment testing of police officers was limited primarily to physical ability, mental ability, and aptitude predictors (i.e., I/O psychology) until the work of Oglesby (1957), who suggested that screening of police applicants 143

Michael J. Cuttler

would be enhanced by evaluation of personality factors. Matarazzo et al. (1964) published one of the few early attempts at clinical evaluation of police applicants that contained objective empirical data (10 clinical instruments including the WAIS, MMPI, Edwards Personal Preference Inventory, Rorschach, Taylor Manifest Anxiety Inventory, and five other tests, as well as a clinical interview), derived from 243 police and fire applicants evaluated within a three-year period. The study reported descriptive statistics obtained from this applicant pool and concluded that the police and fire departments in Portland, Oregon, were recruiting “superior young men” defined as those with above average intelligence and excellent social adjustment. Although the authors included the finding that these (test) scores did not differ significantly from scores developed from incumbent officers, the study also did not include longitudinal, comparative, or criterion outcome measures. Similarly, no attempts to cross validate or replicate these findings were attempted or appeared in the early literature. As such, the authors were limited in their conclusions in regard to the broader applicability of these findings, beyond noting that the attrition rate of those hired was similar to that reported in another earlier study in St. Louis (Dubois & Watson, 1950), that is, inasmuch as different measures were used in these studies, not much else could be said. In their review of the history of personality assessment in police psychology, Weiss and Inwald (2010) report increased interest and practice of pre-employment assessment by both police agencies and psychologists during the 1970s and cite a few empirical studies using personality tests in the time period between 1964 and 1979. However, in this regard, the few 1970s studies cited by Weiss and Inwald (Azen, 1974; Azen, Saccuzo, Higgins, & Lewandowski, 1974; Saxe & Reiser, 1976; Azen et al., 1973) were primarily based on the MMPI, and their results were descriptive rather than predictive. The first cross-validation studies for public safety officers, using the MMPI and the Inwald Personality Inventory (IPI) to predict police job performance, were presented at the 1983 American Psychological Association convention and subsequently published (Inwald, 1983, 1988; Shusman, Inwald, & Knatz, 1987; Shusman, Inwald, & Landa, 1984). In addition, from 1979 through 1984, Inwald, with several of her colleagues, reports making over three dozen presentations at state and national conferences that included data from longitudinal prediction research (personal communication, May 14, 2010). In addition to these early efforts, Inwald’s subsequent work has advocated for and supported clear validation guidelines and actuarial prediction techniques as well as increased accountability of police psychologists for their pre-employment evaluations (Inwald, 1980, 1982a, 1984a; Inwald & Sakales, 1982; Sakales & Inwald, 1982). Cochrane et al. (2003) and Varela, Boccaccini, Scogin, Stump, and Caputo (2004) cite two large meta-analytic studies that appeared in the early 1990s (Barrick & Mount, 1991; Tett, Jackson, & Rothstein, 1991), demonstrating modest relationships between personality test scores and police officer performance which included a number of outcome studies published in the late 1980s (Bartol, 1991; Hiatt & Hargrove, 1988; Inwald & Knatz, 1988; Scogin, Schumacher, Howland, & McGee, 1989; Shusman et al., 1987). In their review, Weiss and Inwald (2010) suggest that research results in the 1970s were a function of the design, focus, and standardization of the instruments available at the time that were primarily designed for clinical diagnostic use (e.g., the MMPI and clinical diagnosis based on psychiatric patient norms). The authors further attribute this limitation of existing instruments as the impetus for subsequent development of instruments specifically designed and standardized for use in law enforcement populations (IPI, Inwald, 1982b) as well as normative studies, specialized scales (Inwald, 2008), and adaptations of other tests commonly used for this purpose (CPI, Roberts & Johnson, 2001). While Weiss and Inwald (2010) attributed the lack of empirical findings during the earlier time period to limitations of the instruments, Lefkowitz (1977), in the I/O literature of that time, attributed the absence of empirical work in personality assessment of police officers to a conceptual distinction between screening and prediction, the former characterizing clinical inquiry and the latter attributed to I/O research. According to Lefkowitz (1977), clinical inquiry of the time was focused on 144

Actuarial vs. Clinical Judgment Prediction Models

elimination of those judged to be emotionally unfit . . . [while making] few attempts to ascertain either the validity of those assessments or the degree to which specific clinical attributes may or may not be related to the job performance of those who pass the screening. (p. 354) Fishing on the I/O side of the lake, Lefkowitz (1977) went on to describe this distinction as a fundamental difference between the nature of clinical vs. industrial inquiry, that is, “the traditional psychiatric model practiced by clinical psychologists and psychiatrists” (screening/clinical) vs. “procedures which attempt to predict job success as defined by a variety of operational criteria” (prediction/industrial) (p. 354). However, it is interesting that this clinical vs. industrial distinction occurred within the context of another important discussion in the history of clinical psychology and the science of psychological assessment—clinical vs. actuarial prediction. At roughly the same time that Oglesby (1957), Matarazzo et al. (1964), and others were publishing early descriptive work on psychological attributes of police officers and—on the other side of the lake—Lefkowitz was noting the scarcity of attempts to investigate or document the relationship of personality test findings and/or specific clinical attributes to job performance, Paul Meehl, a clinical psychologist, professor of psychology, and practicing psychotherapist, was defending his “disturbing little book” Clinical Versus Statistical Prediction (Meehl, 1954/1996). Although his work spans a lifetime and his productivity and contribution to the field are legendary, the majority of Meehl’s work in identifying conditions and components of actuarial prediction as well as the consistent, unvarying superiority of this statistical/actuarial approach in virtually all circumstances when compared to clinical judgment, occurred during roughly the same time period that psychological tests were initially used to screen police officer candidates. This occurred while reporting results in clinical rather than performance-linked terms and during the same time period in which this lack of empirical findings was reported in the I/O literature. As noted earlier, prior to the mid-1980s, it was common among police psychologists to gather information about employment candidates using primarily clinical instruments and to interpret this information clinically (i.e., apply observational, theoretical, and experiential perspectives to synthesize predictive hypotheses) while reporting findings and making recommendations in terms that might— or might not—have relevance to future job performance as a law enforcement officer. However, although it is not surprising that industrial psychologists of the time described these clinical evaluations of police officer candidates as “devoid of data other than the clinician’s personal impressions” (Lefkowitz, 1977, p. 354), it should be remembered that this practice was, in fact, characteristic of the entire (broader) field of clinical psychological assessment at the time, that is, police psychologists in the 1970s and early 1980s were performing assessments (and making predictions) following the same manner of practice as most other clinical psychologists performing assessments in other fields. Borrowing from Dawes (2005), the clinicians were fishing on their side of the lake, while I/O psychologists were fishing on their side (and few, if any, clinical or I/O psychologists were “fishing in the middle of the lake”). Within this context, Meehl’s work in actuarial vs. clinical prediction appeared; subsequently, this was also the context into which specialized assessment instruments, practices, and empirical studies of predictive validity in law enforcement selection emerged.

Clinical vs. Actuarial Prediction In 1954, Paul Meehl published his classic book Clinical Versus Statistical Prediction. According to Meehl, clinical prediction referred to the use of an individual—an expert, a clinician—to predict an event. Statistical prediction referred to the use of an actuarial formula to predict the same event (Westen & Weinberger, 2004). 145

Michael J. Cuttler

There is a wealth of research, historical review, and commentary dealing with actuarial vs. clinical prediction and associated areas, for example, Grove and Lloyd (2006), Grove (2005), Grove and Meehl (1996), and Dawes, Faust, and Meehl (1989). Summarizing this entire field is beyond the scope of this chapter. However, it is also well known that Meehl initially examined 20 empirical studies comparing clinical judgment to statistical (actuarial) prediction and found clear-cut and consistent superiority of statistical prediction over clinical judgment. In addition, this finding has been replicated numerous times across multiple studies by Meehl and others over the years, within many contexts, using many instruments with essentially the same findings (Dawes et al., 1989; Grove, Zald, Lebow, Snitz, & Nelson, 2000). In reviewing Meehl’s work, Grove and Lloyd (2006) summarized: Meehl’s (1954/1996) conclusion that statistical prediction consistently outperforms clinical judgment has stood up extremely well for half a century. His conceptual analyses have not been significantly improved since he published them in the 1950s and 1960s. His work in this area contains several citation classics, which are part of the working knowledge of all competent applied psychologists today. (p. 192) It should also be noted, however, that in spite of what amounts to overwhelming evidence in support of actuarial prediction techniques over clinical judgment in virtually all circumstances, Meehl’s findings were not universally accepted in practice when first published nor are they universally accepted and incorporated within the broader field of psychological assessment practice today. Through the years, Dawes et al. (1989), Meehl (1954/1996), Grove and Lloyd (2006), Grove (2005), and others have described and evaluated a host of arguments asserting the equality and/ or superiority of clinical (over actuarial) prediction and have meticulously refuted them. That said, when asked to describe their interpretive strategy, many clinical psychologists respond that they do not routinely make predictions based on actuarial interpretation. Similarly, many police psychologists (a specific subset of “most clinical psychologists”) performing pre-employment assessments report use of actuarially guided or hybrid decision-making practices consisting of routine review, consideration, and aggregation of statistical data (e.g., specialized test scores), followed by intervening expert judgment. Nonetheless, however mainstream, modal, and/or representative of common practice the hybrid assertion may be, it should also be noted that this point of view is neither logically nor scientifically consistent. Statistical and clinical interpretations are mutually exclusive by definition. A decision/prediction can be either actuarial (rule based) or clinical (results modified/ enhanced/ synthesized by professional interpretation); it cannot be both. There are no true hybrids: [C]linical and statistical prediction (are) mutually exclusive. If actuarial predictions are synthesized clinically, this is clinical prediction; if clinical predictions are synthesized statistically, this is statistical prediction. There is no such thing as a true hybrid. This point has been repeatedly misunderstood, and in my opinion, often tendentiously and misleadingly argued by some psychologists. (Grove, 2005, p. 1234) The defining component of actuarial judgment is that actuarial predictions occur mechanically as a function of predetermined rules, that is, there is no intervening professional judgment or mediation of any kind, and no other data are considered. Clinical prediction occurs when data are interpreted and predictions are synthesized by an expert using experience, perspective, and/or other clinically mediated processes. Once predictions based on statistical rules are modified by a clinician, they become clinical predictions. The hybrid or actuarially guided strategy described above (statistical 146

Actuarial vs. Clinical Judgment Prediction Models

aggregation mediated by expert judgment) is, in fact, clinical prediction and has been so described by Meehl (1986) as “clinical data combination” (Grove, 2005, p. 1234). In the case of police psychology, although the clinician may take advantage of information that is statistically aggregated and/or actuarially derived—for example, specialized test scores—once judgment is applied, the final decision is nonetheless clinical. According to more than 50 years of research, these statistically aggregated clinical predictions are not likely to be as accurate as predictions arrived at actuarially. In addition, it also should be noted that this same body of research has found that making additional information and/or experience that is not part of the prediction model available to clinical judges is not likely to alter this finding nor improve clinical accuracy in any way (Dawes et al., 1989; Goldberg, 1991). This confusion regarding hybrid practice may be due to a misperception among some clinicians that actuarial prediction always requires aggregation of quantitative data followed by sophisticated statistical analysis. This is not necessarily the case. According to Meehl (1954/1996), when considering statistical prediction (as opposed to clinical judgment): [The] distinction is that between the source or type of information employed in making predictions and the manner in which this information is combined for predictive purposes. (p. 15) The primary clinical vs. actuarial distinction lies between uniform application of structured rules (actuarial) vs. judgment based decision making (clinical), whatever the form of data gathered. Most objective psychological tests yield statistical data, while interviews yield observational data, and personal history questionnaires yield descriptive data. However, observational and descriptive data also can be aggregated statistically just as statistical data (e.g., scores on psychological tests) can be interpreted clinically. The distinction is in the judgment technique, not in the type of data judged. Actuarial models often include expert rules derived from experience, albeit applied mechanically, just as clinical models often include statistical data, for example, test scores applied or weighted at the discretion of the clinician. In either event, the practice activity of a psychologist utilizing actuarial prediction and the practice activity of a psychologist utilizing clinical prediction are more or less identical, that is, both gather and aggregate the same data protocols (responses to test items and interview questions, review of background information, and so on). The distinction lies in what happens after the data are gathered (the decision/prediction-making process). Objective test and questionnaire items lend themselves reasonably well to statistical prediction, but these same data can be (and most often are) interpreted clinically. In the same way, responses to structured interview questions, such as those dealing with life history and background information, may be coded and scored (aggregated) for use in statistical prediction models, but they may also be (and often are) interpreted clinically. In actuarial models, scores derived from psychological tests and life history inquiries may be inserted into a series of prediction routines and equations derived from comparison of problem officers with controls (nonproblem officers), resulting in a classification prediction (e.g., problem/nonproblem). In a clinical model, this same information (high/low scores, interview responses) is reviewed and interpreted by a clinician who may code it and apply some rules of thumb derived from education, experience, and judgment or from the results of a specialized test. However, unless these rules of thumb and coded information constitute 100% of the judgment equation and are completely, consistently, and/or mechanically applied in all cases—that is, the clinician does not decide which rules apply and/or with what degree of weight—the prediction process is clinical. Once again, and as noted above, based on the results of multiple studies over the past 50 years, the latter prediction (clinical) when aggregated across multiple cases will not be as accurate as the former (actuarial) (Dawes et al., 1989; Grove et al., 2000). With regard to interview data, both Meehl (1959) and Westen and Weinberger (2004) have presented evidence that structured interview and/or structured scoring techniques that convert 147

Michael J. Cuttler

(aggregate) clinically derived nonstandardized data into structured scored form may be incorporated into actuarial models. It is also possible that interview-based judgments at a minimally inferential level—if recorded in standard form (for example, Qsort) and treated statistically—can be made more powerful than such data treated impressionistically as is currently the practice. (Meehl, 1959, p. 124) Similarly, Shedler and Westin (2004) reported development and use of a structured scoring technique similar to Qsort (SWAP-200), completed by clinicians observing psychiatric patients that demonstrated predictive validity superior to psychological tests when the results were calculated actuarially. In this case, clinicians, described as “expert observers,” generated data that were subsequently used actuarially, that is, data created by clinical observation when applied actuarially were found to be superior to statistical data aggregated clinically. On the other hand, responses to semi structured interviews and other nonstandardized, nonobjective instruments of inquiry are more problematic for use in actuarial prediction since the responses are not necessarily uniform across all applicants and the resultant data often require some form of interpretive intervention, for example, scoring, coding, and interpretation. However, semistructured interview formats are reported to be in use in a large percentage of law enforcement agencies using pre-employment assessment (Cochrane et al., 2003), and this technique (semistructured interview) is currently specified as a required component of pre-employment assessment in practice guidelines published by the Police Psychological Services Section of the International Association of Chiefs of Police (2009). As noted earlier, it is estimated that as many as 4,500 psychologists perform as many as 100,000 pre-employment assessments each year (Corey et al., 2009). Because it can be expected that these psychologists are representative of the broader field of clinical practice, it is probably fair to say that, in spite of research evidence to the contrary, a substantial cohort of the psychologists who perform police officer pre-employment assessments each year are making predictions clinically, albeit statistically enhanced by psychological test data. Although many are gathering all the information needed for actuarial assessment, incorporating actuarially derived information (test scores) in their decisionmaking process, and operating well within the mainstream of accepted practice, on the whole, these predictions are ultimately clinical, not as accurate as they could be, and also not strictly consistent with a substantial body of scientific evidence. In this regard, Grove and Lloyd (2006) found that many clinicians were actually unaware of these issues and/or untrained in actuarial prediction: We surveyed a 10% random sample of American Psychological Association Division 12 (clinical) psychologists to learn how familiar they were with the controversy, their views on the matter, and their clinical practices. Of 183 responders (28% response rate), more than 15% had never heard of the controversy or had merely heard that it existed; only 42% had covered the controversy in detail during their training; 10% had not been taught that there were any available statistical prediction methods, let alone what they were or how to use them, and another 6% had only had the existence of such methods mentioned. (p. 194) Dawes et al. (1989) speculated as to why so few practitioners seemed to have changed practice habits in the face of such consistent evidence and concluded:

148

Actuarial vs. Clinical Judgment Prediction Models

Failure to accept a large and consistent body of scientific evidence over unvalidated personal observation may be described as a normal human failing or, in the case of professionals who identify themselves as scientific, plainly irrational. (p. 1673) In a later article, Dawes (2005) goes even further: Providing service that assumes that clinicians “can do better” simply based on selfconfidence or plausibility in the absence of evidence that they can actually do so is simply unethical. (p. 1245) However, although clinical interpretation of pre-employment assessment data may or may not be unethical (Dawes, 2005), clinical rather than (actuarial) interpretation of pre- employment assessment data is arguably not consistent with evidence available in the literature, particularly when predicting training performance and/or overall suitability where screen-out baselines of occurrence exceed 20%. Pre-employment assessment of police officers is a high stakes activity for all involved (applicant, employer, psychologist, and community). In addition to the likelihood that these assessments will have a direct and immediate impact on the future employment prospects of the applicant, these assessments are also typically performed within the civil service/public employment arena where financial loss and/or substantial harm may accrue to an employer as well as to the community at large for wrongful employment, wrongful rejection, and/or failing to properly screen. As such, a higher level of legal scrutiny (and jeopardy) is associated with this activity than may be associated with other assessment activities within clinical psychology. In addition, various legal and professional mandates have emerged since 1978 that have directly impacted the practice of pre-employment assessment (EEOC Uniform Guidelines on Employee Selection, 1978), principles for validation and use of personnel selection procedures (Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 2003), the Civil Rights Act of 1991, and the Americans With Disabilities Act (1990). These developments (increased demand, high-stakes scrutiny, and statutory requirements) have created a practice climate favoring actuarial assessment, or at least statistical documentation of baselines, validity, and outcome that is more pronounced than in many other fields of clinical psychology endeavor. As such, the past several decades have seen a paradigm shift within police psychology, encouraging convergence of clinical, I/O, and forensic inquiry (pushing us into the middle of the lake). As noted by Grove and Lloyd (2006), Meehl’s primary contribution to the field was putting this controversy center stage as well as clarifying the concepts underlying the debate. With regard to police psychology, Meehl’s work occurred within the context of legal and societal changes that were placing increasing demands on the practice of pre-employment psychological assessment with regard to job-related validity. As a result, police psychologists were driven to examine their techniques and practices in a more quantitative and empirical light than previously, thus setting the stage for research, the creation of new instruments, documentation of the predictive validity of pre-employment assessment, and a general convergence of clinical, I/O, and forensic inquiry.

Retrospective Research Starting in the mid-1980s, meta-analytic studies emerged that reviewed the available research linking personality tests to job performance and established consensus in regard to the predictive validity

149

Michael J. Cuttler

of these instruments (Barrick & Mount, 1991; O’Brien, 1996; Schmitt, Gooding, Noe, & Kirsch, 1984; Tett et al., 1991; Tett, Jackson, Rothstein, & Reddon, 1994). During this time, a number of tests and other instruments specifically designed for use in screening police officers also emerged. Studies of the predictive validity of these tests formed the foundation of validation evidence for use of these instruments in pre-employment screening. According to Varela et al. (2004), these earlier studies found evidence of modest but significant validity predicting police officer performance with personality measures. In general, validity coefficients were in the range of .12 to .25 for independent variables related to a number of scales on a number of tests, most commonly MMPI/2 (Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory), CPI (California Psychological Inventory), and IPI (Inwald Personality Inventory), as well as a wide variety of dependent variables, ranging from job performance outcomes (e.g., training performance, turnover, and retention) to subjective ratings on personality constructs. Varela’s study incorporated results from a considerably larger number of studies than previous meta-analyses (78), reported results linked to both objective performance and construct ratings, and found similar validity coefficients (.09–.23) with differential findings grouped by test (MMPI/2, CPI, IPI), study design, and character of the dependent (outcome, criterion) variables. Another interesting and thorough study was performed by Ones, Viswesvaran, and Dilchert (2004) in conjunction with a project initiated by the California Peace Officer Training and Standard Commission (POST) and led by Spilberg (2003). The focus of this project was development of a revised psychological screening manual to guide and inform law enforcement agencies in California in proper use of mandated pre-employment psychological screening. As part of this project, Spilberg (Society for I/O psychology, 2003) performed a comprehensive job analysis, surveying police agency executives and police psychologists (primarily clinical psychologists) yielding a set of 10 contentlinked peace officer psychological attributes. These attributes (constructs) became the psychological screening dimensions that California agencies were advised to incorporate in pre-employment screening. These constructs also became the dependent variables in the meta-analysis performed by Ones et al. (2004). Next, a panel of police psychologists linked these constructs to specific scales on various psychological tests, and these scales became the independent (predictor) variables for the meta-analysis. This study found a somewhat wider range of validity coefficients than reported by previous meta-analyses (0.10–0.40) due, in part, to the careful attention that was paid to development and standardization of constructs and predictor variables. These results also reflected an unusually comprehensive range of police behavior as opposed to earlier meta-analytic studies that aggregated studies with less comprehensive outcome variables. In addition, the study is an excellent example of clinical/IO convergence as I/O techniques such as critical incident review and job analysis were integrated with clinical expertise. These findings further documented the validity and utility of psychological testing in this setting as well. When reviewing this body of research, it is important to note that although the independent predictor variables (psychological test scores, typically, MMPI/2, CPI, IPI, 16PF, and a few others) were quite similar across most studies, the dependent outcome variables varied substantially. Some studies used objective job performance as outcomes (e.g., retention and turnover, attendance, training performance, disciplinary action, and so on), while others used ratings of behavior based on constructs (e.g., Big 5 personality constructs as in Barrick & Mount, 1991; California POST 10 as in Ones et al., 2004) that were subsequently linked to job performance. This distinction was noted by Varela et al. (2004), who further found that validity coefficients for construct ratings, which he called “soft” ratings, were somewhat higher than validity coefficients for objective performance. In as much as the predictor variables in these studies were, in fact, components of test instruments designed to measure psychological constructs in the first place, it would seem that this finding (personality tests are better predictors of construct ratings than actual performance) is not particularly surprising, that is, documenting predictive validity using dependent variables linked to personality may maximize the observed validity coefficient of a test but may not necessarily provide an accurate 150

Actuarial vs. Clinical Judgment Prediction Models

view of the overall utility of the instrument in question since both the predictor variables and the outcome variables are drawn from the same domain (personality) and hence subject to similar sources of error. This is an important item to remember when considering validation evidence supporting specific personality tests for use in a pre-employment assessment protocol.

Practice Guidelines Of the estimated 4,500 psychologists assumed to be performing some kind of police psychology service, about 200–300 have dedicated a substantial proportion of their practice activities primarily to police psychology and are members of national professional organizations (Corey et al., 2009). One such organization, the Police Psychological Services Section of the International Association of Chiefs of Police (IACP_PPSS), is a group of approximately 200 psychologists whose practice is substantially focused on police psychology. Founded in the mid-1980s, this group publishes a number of practice guidelines, including guidelines for practice of pre-employment assessment. These guidelines have come to be viewed as an expression of “best practice” in this area. First published in 1986, they have been periodically reviewed and rewritten (PPSS_IACP, 1992, 1998, 2004, 2009). The comparative content of each version of these practice guidelines represents a series of interesting snapshots of the state of the art of pre-employment assessment in the past 20 years and also reflects the paradigm shift that has occurred regarding validation, performance prediction, and outcome research (and migration to the middle of the lake). The original (pre-employment) guidelines were developed by Inwald (1984a, 1985c, 1986b) and formally adopted by the IACP’s Police Psychological Services Section after discussions and modifications at the 1986 IACP convention. In addition to a number of basic practice considerations (choice of tests, qualification of examiners, confidentiality of results, and so on), Inwald’s original proposed guidelines also contained a number of direct references to program components necessary to support validation and actuarial assessment. These components included surveys to identify critical job performance attributes, development of specific behavioral outcomes to be predicted, identification of base rates of occurrence of outcomes (to assess “cost of selection” and efficiency of tests over chance prediction), and development of behaviorally based structured (scorable) interview protocols. As such, these components were included in the original published guidelines (Inwald, 1986b); however, with the exception of a general reference to base rates, most of these components supporting research and validation were dropped from revised 1992 guidelines (Inwald, personal communication, December 29, 2009). The 1992 guidelines did note that tests should be “validated” and assessment results should be expressed in job-related (rather than medical or psychopathological) terms and linked to a “psychological job analysis.” In 1998, the term psychological job analysis—a term coined by Inwald (1998)— was replaced with more precise terminology: Data on attributes considered most important for effective performance in a particular position should be obtained from job analysis, interview, surveys, or other appropriate sources. (PPSS_IACP, 1998, p. 1, item 4) Although the Americans With Disabilities Act (ADA, 1990) was enacted prior to 1992, the 1992 version of the guidelines was silent on the issue of pre/post-offer assessment, probably because the Act did not go into effect until 1994. In 1998, specific reference to ADA was added to the guidelines without very much elaboration beyond the admonishment that psychological evaluations should be performed subsequent to conditional offers of employment. By 2004, the guidelines had become considerably more detailed on the subject of ADA, particularly in regard to “medical” vs. “nonmedical” evaluations, instruments, and methods of inquiry 151

Michael J. Cuttler

which could be conducted prior to conditional offers of employment. Perhaps reflecting the growing practice of “bifurcation” (nonmedical assessment pre-offer followed by mental health inquiry post-offer): Personality tests and other methods of inquiry that are not medical by the above definition and that do not include specific prohibited topics or inquiries may be conducted at the pre-offer stage. However, these assessments are alone not capable of determining a candidate’s emotional stability and therefore would not constitute an adequate pre-employment psychological evaluation. (PPSS_IACP, 2004, p. 2, item 11) The 2004 guidelines also contained specific reference to ADA regarding post-offer inquiry: A psychological evaluation is considered “medical” if it provides evidence that could lead to identifying a mental or emotional disorder or impairment as listed in the DSM-IV, and therefore must only be conducted after the applicant has been tendered a conditional offer of employment. (PPSS_IACP, 2004, p. 2, item 11) However, beyond asserting that test instruments and the selection processes should be validated in some way, the 2004 version of the guidelines was almost silent about the documentation of validity and/or the process through which this validation should occur. A test battery including objective, job-related, validated psychological instruments should be administered to the applicant. Written tests selected should be validated for use with public safety candidates. Continuing collaborative efforts by the hiring agency and evaluating psychologist should be made to validate final suitability ratings using behavioral criteria measures. (PPSS_IACP, 2004, p. 2, item 11) In this regard, it seems the authors of the 2004 guidelines emphasized legal parameters of the pre- and post-offer distinction over technical requirements. However, as noted earlier, each of these guideline versions represent a snapshot of the professional climate at the time of draft. In 2003–2004, the field was just beginning to come to terms with implications of the ADA, particularly as it impacted practice requirements. More practitioners were expanding practice to include bifurcated pre-offer vs. post-offer assessments, and these developments were reflected in the focus of the 2004 guideline version. As more police psychologists embraced the bifurcated assessment format, a number of new instruments and supporting research emerged. These developments were reflected in the 2009 guidelines, which are considerably more forthcoming in regard to instrument development, validation, and technical issues. As such, this most recent version of the guidelines is another snapshot reflecting the current state of interest and focus in the field. Starting from one side of the lake and moving toward the middle, in some ways, the 2009 guidelines return full circle to the original version drafted more than 20 years previous (Inwald, 1986b). Since 1992, emphasis in the field has transitioned from defining what should be addressed in a report and the type of tests to be used, to revising the process of assessment to conform to ADA, and finally to development of instruments and processes demonstrably linked to job performance and prediction of suitability in the most accurate and efficient manner. Certainly, the early criticism of

152

Actuarial vs. Clinical Judgment Prediction Models

clinical assessment without actuarial evidence by I/O psychologists and early forensic psychologists (e.g., Levy, 1967; Shapiro, 1983; Ziskin, 1981) has been resolved: [They] have not determined what constitutes “emotional suitability” for law enforcement; . . . (and) hence, psychological tests and psychiatric interviews have not demonstrated much predictive value. (Levy, 1967 as quoted in Lefkowitz, 1977, p. 354) The 2009 guidelines reflect specific developments in the field addressing, among other things, research and development of new instruments: Nothing in these guidelines should be construed to discourage scientifically legitimate research, innovation, and/or use of new techniques that show promise for helping hiring agencies identify, screen, and select qualified candidates. (PPSS_IACP, 2009, p. 1, item 2.3) Documentation of Job Relatedness: Information about duties, powers, demands, working conditions, and other job-analytic information relevant to the intended position, should be obtained by the psychologist before beginning the evaluation process. This information should be directed toward identifying behaviors and attributes that underlie effective and counterproductive job performance. (PPSS_IACP, 2009, p. 2, item 5.1) Validation: Tests should have a substantial research base for interpretation with normal range populations in general and public safety applicants in particular. Validation evidence should be consistent with Principles for the Validation and Use of Personnel Selection Procedures (SIOP, 2003). (PPSS_IACP, 2009, p. 3, item 7.2.1) Integration of Predictive Information From Other Domains (e.g., Life History): Information regarding the applicant’s relevant history (e.g., school, work, interpersonal, family, legal, financial, substance use, mental health, and so on) should be collected and integrated with psychological test and interview data. (PPSS_IACP, 2009, p. 4, item 9.1) And Use/Utility of Pre-employment Assessment in Both Nonmedical (Pre-offer) and Medical (Post-offer) Contexts: A pre-employment psychological evaluation may include procedures or tests that are not medical in nature (i.e., designed and used to measure personality traits, behaviors, or characteristics such as judgment, stress resilience, anger management, integrity, conscientiousness, teamwork, and social competence). However, these nonmedical procedures alone would not constitute a complete preemployment psychological evaluation since they do not include the medical element. (PPSS_IACP, 2009, p. 2, item 3.4)

153

Michael J. Cuttler

Specialized Test Instruments In addition to research documenting the validity of psychological tests in pre-employment settings and guidelines reflecting best practices to include research and validation, a number of specialized tests and new scales of existing tests designed for specific use in police pre- employment screening emerged during this period. The Inwald Personality Inventory (IPI), originally constructed in 1979 and published in 1982, was the first comprehensive personality inventory designed and validated specifically for public safety officer selection (Weiss & Inwald, 2010) and, as such, was unique in the field of public safety officer screening at that time. In addition to including standard scales identifying psychopathology, the IPI also included scales containing items related to past job difficulties, trouble with the law, absence, lateness tendencies, and alcohol/drug use, among other behavior patterns. The test also reported scores based on public safety officer norms as opposed to general population norms and included capabilities for generating local agency norms and/or those of related job classifications such as hostage negotiators, dispatchers, and so on (Inwald, 1982b). In addition, the report included actuarial prediction equations derived from systematic follow-up data, providing practitioners with predictions of risk for specific performance difficulties (Inwald, 1982b, 1988, 2008). In this same time frame, the tests most widely in use to screen law enforcement officers were the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI; Hathaway & McKinley, 1940), the California Psychological Inventory (CPI; Gough, 1956, 1987), and the 16PF (Cattell, 1949). Specialized narrative reports for law enforcement selection based on these instruments were also developed and marketed for these instruments during this time, but the scores were based on general population norms and not specifically linked to job performance attributes specific to law enforcement. In 1981, the Law Enforcement Assessment and Development Report (LEADR) was published, a derivative of Raymond Cattell’s 16PF (1949) published by IPAT, with a public-safety-related narrative report (Dee-Burnett, Johns, & Krug, 1981), and in 1984, the Minnesota Report: Personnel Selection System appeared based on the MMPI. For the most part, these tests reported findings in terms of sets of constructs based on conventional clinical interpretations of personality and did not contain direct linkages to specific law enforcement performance attributes. From 1987 to 1997, several specialized tests were developed by Inwald and her colleagues that focused on specific traits and behaviors germane to law enforcement officer job performance. These instruments also reported public safety candidate norms and utilized actuarial prediction equations. The instruments included the Hilson Personnel Profile/Success Quotient (HPP/SQ, social/emotional intelligence issues; Inwald & Brobst, 1988), the Inwald Survey 5 (IS5, integrity, and domestic violence issues), the Hilson Safety/Security Risk Inventory (HSRI, unsafe behavior in the workplace; Inwald, 1995), the Inwald Survey 2 (IS2, violence potential; Inwald, Resko, & Favuzza, 1994), and the Hilson Life Adjustment Profile (HLAP, psychopathology; Inwald, Resko, & Favuzza, 1996). In 1995, a specialized report for the CPI used police applicant norms (Roberts & Johnson, 2001) as a comparison base. This report contained several specialized scales as well as actuarially calculated risk predictions based on comparison of applicant groups (problem officers vs. controls) that were directly germane to law enforcement performance. In most cases, the dependent variables (those that defined problem officers) were based on identification of what the authors described as “selection relevant items” as defined by pre-employment personal history and background findings. Subsequently, however, Roberts and Johnson (2001) enhanced this report to include “true outcome performance measures” such as criterion groups of applicants that lied about illegal drug use during the pre-employment screening process and, subsequently, officers who had experienced “involuntary departures” as well as supervisory issues and other negative job performance events (Roberts, personal communication, December 9, 2009). Roberts, Thompson, and Johnson (2000) created a special report for the Personality Assessment Inventory (Morey, 2007) contrasting current applicants applying for public safety jobs with norms 154

Actuarial vs. Clinical Judgment Prediction Models

from a group of applicants in four job classifications (police officer, communications dispatcher, corrections officer, and firefighter/EMT) who successfully completed the job-screening procedures for the same position, were hired, and completed at least one year in that position. Like the IPI, the report also contains risk statements in regard to ratings of job suitability made by experienced psychologists conducting psychological evaluations of job applicants for specific public safety positions (e.g., police officer or corrections officer), and added specific types of background problems identified on personal history questionnaires. In as much as the primary purpose of pre-employment assessment is prediction of on-the- job behavior, predictor domains (in this case test scores/scales) should be independent, since correlation between predictor domains is known to be a source of error in psychometric tests. Similarly, it has also been noted that multiple predictor models containing correlated scores can be expected to have higher standard error values (Campbell & Fiske, 1959; Cureton,Cronbach, Meehl, Ebel, & Ward, 1996). As noted earlier in the discussion of retrospective research, prediction error can be expected from any personality test regardless of the outcome to which it is linked. However, when a test is linked to construct ratings, both the predictors (scores) and the outcomes (constructs) are open to similar sources of error. When personality constructs are subsequently linked to job outcomes they become vulnerable yet again to the same source of error. As such, the observed validity coefficient between test and construct may be greater than a validity coefficient derived from a direct link between test score and job outcome. Similarly, intercorrelation of personality test scales will inflate the apparent validity of a test and/or a prediction model since the correlated scales are vulnerable to similar sources of error. Apropos to the question of error associated with correlated scales, the Institute for Personality Assessment and Ability Testing (IPAT) has constructed several specialized law enforcement reports based on the 16PF (Cattell, & Cattel, 1993) and the PEPQ (PsychEval Personality Questionnaire, Cattell, Cattell, Cattell, Russell, & Bedwell, 2003). Both the fundamental design and the scales of these instruments were derived from factor analysis; hence, these scores are minimally intercorrelated. Due to their independence, such scales are particularly well suited for construction of prediction models. The most recent edition of the 16PF (2003), which assesses normal personality, was redesigned in light of ADA requirements to be nonmedical and can be administered prior to a conditional offer of employment as well as in bifurcated processes (normal assessment pre-offer, mental health assessment post-offer). The PEPQ assesses both normal personality and psychopathology and is designed for use post-offer, and/or in the second phase of bifurcation. Both instruments report scores on four “Protective Service Dimensions” derived from content analysis of the law enforcement literature as well as linkage to Cattell’s original 16-factor analytically derived personality scales (Cattell, 1949), and the “Big 5 factors of personality” (Goldberg, 1993).2 Scores on these resultant constructs are significantly related to job outcome, with effect sizes ranging from .08 to .87, and intercorrelations of the scores are minimal (Cattell et al., 2003). Similarly, Tellegen et al. (2003), noting that the clinical scales in the MMPI-2 were intercorrelated (and hence a potential source for error in both diagnostic and selection contexts), factor analyzed the MMPI-2 clinical scales and derived a set of nine restructured clinical scales (RC scales) that were not significantly correlated, yet at least as valid in regard to prediction (Tellegen, 2009). Subsequently, Ben-Porath and Tellegen constructed the MMPI-2-RF (restructured form), consisting of a subset of 338 items derived from the original 587 item MMPI-2, incorporating these factor analytic scales along with several higher order scales. Recent outcome research based on objective performance outcomes has been encouraging. Sellbom, Fischler, and Ben-Porath (2007) found that the RC Scales were better at predicting specific behavioral misconduct in peace officers. As such, it would seem the MMPI-2-RF addresses several of the limitations of other personality tests described above (tests linked to constructs not direct outcomes, tests yielding correlated measures). It should also be noted, however, that the MMPI-2-RF contains items that measure emotional stability, and, similar to the 155

Michael J. Cuttler

PEPQ (Cattell et al., 2003), this test is considered medical under the ADA and must be administered after a conditional offer of employment has occurred and where significant range restriction is likely. Unlike other specialized reports (e.g., CPI, IPI, Roberts et al., 2001), the MMPI-2-RF reports scores in terms of general population norms rather than law enforcement applicant norms. As noted earlier, incorporation of applicant norms can be useful in interpreting test results primarily due to the fact that substantial differences are likely between responses of police job applicants vs. the general population (Bartol, 1982; Carpenter & Raza, 1987; Hargrave, Hiatt, & Gaffney, 1986, Varela et al., 2004). However, Ben-Porath (2009) points out that these data can also be misleading in that they can mask genuine differences between peace officer candidates and members of the general population, hence facilitating false negative predictions (passed when not suitable) based on information that may have been identified upon further probing. Therefore, the MMPI-2-RF reports scores in general population terms while providing descriptive information regarding the range of law enforcement applicant population scores for comparative purposes.

Other Predictive Domains and Instruments; Incremental/Convergent Validity and Merged Processes In addition to personality tests, several other classes (domains) of independent variables have been found to be predictive of job performance in police officers. The most common of these measures are cognitive ability and life history. Variables from these predictor domains are commonly incorporated into assessment protocols performed by police psychologists. In addition, there is evidence that each of these predictor domains are independent (noncorrelated) and linked to objective performance outcomes. With regard to general mental ability, Schmitt et al. (1984) conducted a meta-analysis of 99 employees in a variety of occupations, including law enforcement, and reported that mental ability is a significant predictor of success in these occupations. Similarly, in a meta-analytic investigation limited to law enforcement occupations, Aamodt (2004) reported an average correlation of r = .41 between mental ability and various measures of academic performance. Likewise, Hirsch, Northrup, and Schmidt (1986) reported an average correlation of r = .34 between mental ability and academy training. As such, there is general consensus that cognitive ability is a primary predictor of suitability in law enforcement. Use of this predictor, however, is limited by the fact that scores on many tests measuring mental ability also are often questioned in regard to disparate impact3 on specific racial groups (Equal Employment Opportunity Commission, ADA Division, Office of Legal Counsel, 1995); as such, their utility as unilateral predictors can be limited. Life history events are routinely gathered and evaluated by police psychologists during the assessment process as well. In addition to their relationship to personality information, evidence suggests that these data are not particularly subject to disparate impact on racial or gender groups and can also be a robust and independent predictor of police performance. According to Tenopyr (1994), there is substantial evidence for convergence between life history and personality constructs. In similar fashion, Mumford, Snell, and Reiter-Palmon (1994) stated: Perhaps the most straightforward answer to the question we have posed covering the relationship between background data and personality is to argue that background data represent little more than an alternative format for personality assessment. (pp. 584–585) There are basically two ways to report and use life history events in pre-employment prediction. One approach is to identify the occurrence of defined life events in the history of an applicant and measure the degree to which these discrete events (e.g., job terminations) are predictive of a specific 156

Actuarial vs. Clinical Judgment Prediction Models

outcome (e.g., problem performance as a police officer). A second approach is to link defined life events to personality constructs (e.g., job termination linked to conscientiousness), the latter (construct ratings) becoming the outcome variable. Both approaches are convergent valid predictors of outcome and independent of personality measures (Cuttler & Muchinsky, 2006; Sarchione, Cuttler, Muchinsky, & Nelson-Gray, 1998; Tenopyr, 1994). As mentioned earlier, the IPI, first published in 1979, was the first personality assessment instrument that included biographical/behavioral data items such as arrest history, driving record, work history, and substance use as well as scales identifying the presence of psychopathology and antisocial attitudes. These inquiries were presented as true/false test items and were linked to constructs in the context of a personality inventory rather than in an instrument designed to develop discrete descriptive detail about life events such as a personal history questionnaire. As noted, although responses to individual items (e.g., “I received good grades in school”) were qualitatively reported (i.e., in a critical items listing), the responses were quantitatively linked to personality-based construct scales (e.g., conscientiousness) and these construct scores were the primary variables in the subsequent validation studies (Inwald, 1982b, 2008). Similar to the preceding discussion, but taking a more clinical approach to use of life history information, Johnson and Roberts (2006) constructed the PsyQ, which they describe as a selfreport questionnaire that provides life history information pertinent to the evaluation of applicants for public safety positions. The questionnaire contains 340 questions presented in multiple-choice formats, containing a varying number of response categories, as required to reflect the full range of relevant responses to each question. The results (responses by applicant to multiple-choice questions) are organized into problem categories developed by a panel of psychologists. The scores of these items are aggregated into these categories and are reported in relative frequency terms, comparing an applicant’s response to frequencies in a database of previous applicants. According to the authors, the information developed by this questionnaire is primarily intended to help clinicians determine the extent to which the findings from psychological testing are corroborated by actual behavior. As such, although likely to provide utility as a guide to a psychologist in conducting interviews, the questionnaire’s utility and/or potential for convergent validity or as an independent predictor of outcome in an actuarial model has not yet been investigated. With regard to use of discrete life history events as an independent outcome predictor, Sarchione et al. (1998) constructed a list of critical life events identified by subject matter experts (background investigators). These subject matter experts also weighted these items in terms of criticality and predictive value when reviewing police applicant backgrounds, creating quantitative scales for employment, criminal, and substance-related items. The investigators then examined personnel records and internal affairs files and identified groups of problem officers (who experienced discipline or were terminated). After correction for range restriction, the study found effect sizes from 0.40 to 0.74 for specific life event scales as predictors of various on-the-job disciplinary actions. The study also included CPI scores that yielded effect sizes from 0.48 to 0.67 as predictors of these outcomes as well. Cuttler and Muchinsky (2006) cross-validated these findings and added a fourth scale (veracity) corresponding to omissions, errors, and discrepancies in reported information as well as documenting predictive validity for a broader range of outcomes (failure to complete training, termination, selection failure). These studies found consistent evidence that the life history scales were essentially independent of (not correlated with) personality tests and/or cognitive ability measures, and these life history scales (employment, criminal, substance, veracity) were not correlated with each other. As such, the authors suggested that substantial incremental validity may be derived from combining life history and personality test scores. Achievement of incremental validity though the use of scored life events requires gathering and aggregating life history data in a reliable and efficient manner. However, unlike psychological tests to which subjects usually respond in a yes/no, true/false, or multiple-choice manner, a number of 157

Michael J. Cuttler

unique intervening variables impacting the practice and quality of life history assessment are not necessarily present in personality measurement. Cuttler (2007) notes that the accuracy and utility of a life history questionnaire are affected by the way the questions are formulated and presented, the manner in which the responses are scored, and the attitude or bias of the respondent who completes the questionnaire. According to Cuttler (2007), there is also likely to be considerable and understandable (conscious and unconscious) motivation on the part of job applicants to present themselves in as favorable a light as possible. The effect of this motivation in regard to psychological tests is also well known and documented (Anastasi, 1988). However, although it has been suggested that this impression management effect does not necessarily alter the fundamental findings of psychological tests, particularly in normal populations (Ben-Porath, 2009) and/or when interpreting test results actuarially (Grove, 2005; Grove & Meehl, 1996), the transparency of life history questions is such as to render this (bias) effect considerably more critical when aggregating life history information. Direct questions regarding life history are considerably more transparent to an applicant than personality items, and this transparency is likely to affect applicant response. For example, the scoring, interpretation, and/or implication of a true/false item—such as “From time to time I go to the movies by myself ”—is probably not as apparent to a job applicant as “Have you ever been fired from a job”? As a function of this transparency, bias, and/or impression management motivation, the quality of life history data derived through self-report questionnaires is fundamentally dependent on the design of the instrument used to collect this data as well as its ability to minimize response bias. In this regard, the simplest and most straightforward life history questionnaires can also be the most open to bias (Cuttler, 2007). The simplest way to gather life history information is to construct a series of questions to which an applicant responds in a binary (yes/no; true/false) format similar to that of a psychological test. This approach has the advantage of being easily machine scored and aggregated. Some more sophisticated versions of these questionnaires may present these items in conditional branch/stem format (e.g., have you ever been fired from a job? how many times?). However, response to these items will also depend on the applicant’s interpretations of the questions; for example, an applicant might respond no to the question above because “I was not fired, but after I showed up late for the third time, I agreed with my boss that it was best that I resign.” Even in a more sophisticated questionnaire that allows for text entry of an explanation such as this, in order to assign an accurate score, the response must be read and interpreted. Although these omissions, interpretations, and discrepancies will usually be identified and reconciled later in the selection process (i.e., a background investigation and/or upon post-offer interview with a psychologist), these limitations are also likely to affect the reliability and utility of the life history data, particularly if these data are derived from a questionnaire and, in conjunction with psychological test data, placed in an actuarial model for screening purposes. Another way to develop life history information is to create a set of critical items and then manually review a comprehensive personal history questionnaire (similar to those used in most background investigations) that has been previously submitted by the applicant. The life history data are then derived and/or extracted from narrative information rather than directly solicited from the applicant. This approach has the advantage of being passive, that is, not requiring or depending on interaction with the applicant. However, this approach has two major drawbacks. First, it is tedious and time consuming, requires considerable human intervention, is open to error and, as such, is of questionable utility when screening larger applicant pools. Second, although there are fewer direct transparent questions, this approach is still open to the sources of response bias described above (interpretations, omissions, and discrepancies) on the primary document (the personal history questionnaire). Electronic (online) administration of personal history questionnaires can mediate a number of these drawbacks. Specifically, omissions can be easily eliminated by requiring responses. Through 158

Actuarial vs. Clinical Judgment Prediction Models

simple programming, branch questions can be conditionally presented based on applicant responses to stem questions and applicants can be constrained from cruising the questionnaire and/or anticipating questions and changing responses. Similarly, additional and clarifying information can be elicited during online administration that can be stored in a database and/or easily read. However, aggregating and interpreting this information would still require human intervention. In 2001, Cuttler developed an online personal history questionnaire using patented technology that addressed these issues (Cuttler, Cuttler, & Seddon, 2008). The questionnaire also addressed the more fundamental issues of response bias by automating the process by which scored life history information was extracted from applicant-entered personal history information without asking direct questions. In addition to stem/branch conditional question presentation, programming was included that parsed and organized data into defined units (called life events) which were then linked together by logic to create scored items. The application could also intelligently scan text responses as well as report veracity, that is, discrepancies both within the questionnaire and when responses are compared to previous administrations. Inasmuch as this questionnaire reports both quantitative scores based on weighted life history events as well as detailed descriptive information input by applicants and stored in a database, the results are useful for both actuarial decision making and for facilitating background investigations. Cuttler (2000) also developed a series of five actuarial prediction equations using combinations of life history and psychological test data (CPI). This merged process, called the Multi Domain Assessment report, is an actuarially scored report that takes advantage of the opportunities for incremental validity available from combining these noncorrelated predictors derived from multiple domains (life history, personality, and cognitive/educational).4

Practical Considerations for Development and Use of Actuarial Prediction Models Pre-employment assessment of police officer candidates is particularly well suited for development and use of actuarial prediction models. In this regard, the demographic characteristics of the applicant pool, training requirements, duties, and critical performance attributes of police and public safety jobs are reasonably common across employing agencies. Similarly, operational definition of dependent variables (specific negative selection and job outcomes), cross-validation, and follow-up are greatly facilitated within this population since policies and practices across police agencies are often required by statute and/or traditional sense of standard operating procedure. Finally, the emerging practice of bifurcated assessment (nonmedical ability/personality screening performed pre-offer; emotional stability assessment performed post-offer) is also particularly well suited for actuarial prediction since in the pre-offer stage, larger applicant pools must be screened efficiently and inexpensively, and negative outcomes (e.g., failing to complete components of the selection process) occur at substantially higher base rates while smaller groups are screened for mental health issues (which occur at substantially lower base rates) on a post-conditional offer basis. Actuarial assessment is a process by which predictor and outcome variables are identified, rules are applied, predictions are made, outcomes are recorded, and rules are modified based on these observed outcomes. Although identification/development of replicable empirical predictors/models, identification of criterion variables, and valid evaluative research designs are required to incorporate actuarial prediction techniques into practice, psychologists do not necessarily have to create new assessment protocols, that is (and as noted earlier), in many cases the activity of the psychologist need not change—simply the prediction (aggregation) technique would be modified. For identification of variables, the choice of outcome (dependent) variable should be made with reference to the independent (predictor) variables chosen and the manner in which the results and predictions will be reported to the agency. Specifically, when choosing test instruments, if the psychologist chooses to report results and make predictions based on construct ratings (e.g., Psychological 159

Michael J. Cuttler

Performance Dimensions similar to those described such as POST 10 [Spilberg, 2003] or Big 5 [Goldberg, 1993]), then test instruments might be chosen that, according to their technical manuals, have been designed and validated with primary reference to these construct ratings (e.g., 16PF Protective Service Reports, MMPI-2-RF, and PAI). If the psychologist chooses to make predictions and reports based on actual job outcomes, then instruments designed and validated primarily against these criteria should be included in the battery (e.g., IPI, CPI Public Safety Report, Multi Domain Screening Report, and MMPI-2-RF). However, it is also important to note that construct-based predictions can certainly be derived from scores on tests primarily identified with objective outcome. Similarly, direct performance predictions can also be made based on construct-linked test scores. This practice is indeed common, is supported by independent research findings (e.g., Ones et al., 2004), and is even recommended by some training and standards groups (Spilberg, 2004). However, doing so (creating construct-based predictions rather than specific job outcome predictions) from scores on instruments based on general personality constructs also requires that the psychologist then create or adopt rules for converting those scores to job-related construct ratings. Similarly, when used in actuarial prediction, these conversion rules (from general personality constructs to job-specific outcomes) must be mechanical, that is, applied to all cases equally and not modified by discretion or judgment of the psychologist. As stated in the current IACP-PPSS guidelines: 5.1. Information about duties, powers, demands, working conditions, and other job-analytic information relevant to the intended position, should be obtained by the psychologist before beginning the evaluation process. This information should be directed toward identifying behaviors and attributes that underlie effective and counterproductive job performance. 5.2. The psychologist should consult with the hiring agency to establish selection criteria and the agency’s level of acceptable risk for problematic behaviors. (sections 5.1, 5.2, pp. 3–4) And the guidelines further stated: 10.2 Rating and/or recommendation for employment based upon the results of the evaluation should be expressly linked to the job-analytic information referenced in paragraphs 5.1 and 5.2. (section 10.2, p. 5) Many hiring authorities (indeed, many state statutes) also require that the psychologist provide a specific hiring recommendation. Other authorities require an overall score or rating and then set their own hiring policy (the score at which they will hire/reject). In any event, the primary components to be determined for any prediction model are the nature of the outcome (construct vs. specific), the instruments to be used, and the rules to be applied. However, in actuarial decision making, the overall rating will always be the result of a rule set that is uniformly and mechanically applied. As noted earlier, the actuarial rules constructed and applied need not be statistically sophisticated. Rather, they can be expert rules derived by the clinician based on experience, quantitative review of past cases, or both. If applied in an actuarial manner, these predictions are still quite likely superior to clinical predictions (Grove & Meehl, 1996). The critical point is that once established, these rules must be applied uniformly rather than based on the discretion of the clinician. Psychologists applying these rules should carefully gather outcome information. Once outcomes are observed, rules can be modified at reasonable intervals to increase accuracy. Although the evidence shows that actuarial prediction models are superior to clinical predictions in most situations, it is also important to remember that no predictor, rule, or model is perfect. Some level of error is always expected to be associated with any predictor or prediction model, including well-constructed models using independent noncorrelated predictors. In some situations 160

Actuarial vs. Clinical Judgment Prediction Models

(e.g., prediction of events with low baseline of occurrence), no prediction model—neither clinical nor actuarial—will operate efficiently. The validation coefficients and effect sizes reported in the earlier sections of this chapter account for only a fraction (albeit a statistically significant fraction) of the variance observed on outcome, and the acceptance of error is implied when designing any selection process. In accepting the existence of error, it is further implied that the consequences of not screening should be greater than the consequences of screening. This assumption is certainly apropos to law enforcement where the possibility of harm to others is directly apparent; hence, screening is justified. In addition to correlated measures, other sources of error associated with pre-employment prediction are functions of the design of the test or assessment instrument, the nature of the population assessed, and the base rate of occurrence of the outcome to be predicted. Regarding base rate of occurrence, Meehl (1954/1996) and others (Finn, 2009; Westen & Weinberger, 2004) note that actuarial prediction is most accurate when baseline of occurrence approaches 50% (each alternative in the prediction model is equally probable). Unfortunately, this is rarely the case in law enforcement, as the frequency of occurrence of the negative behaviors and outcomes to be predicted usually range from below 5% for serious violations (e.g., integrity, sexual misconduct, and termination for cause) to above 20% for training failure and minor discipline to above 35% for selection process failures (Cuttler & Muchinsky, 2006). In this regard, Streiner (2003), when comparing accuracy of actuarial models that screen out negative outcomes to those that screen in positives, found that the former (screen-out-negatives model) is likely to be more accurate in low-baseline settings (occurrence < 50%) and the latter (screen-in-positives model) is more effective in high-baseline settings (where occurrence > 50%). This finding is consistent with pre-employment screening models in police psychology designed primarily to identify problem officers (i.e., screen out) via actuarial prediction. However, it is also likely that this approach will be less accurate in models that attempt to screen in positive traits (e.g., leadership). In a prediction model used for pre-employment screening, the prediction is usually expressed in terms of a binary classification (suitable/not suitable; problem/nonproblem; pass/fail training, and so forth). The overall utility of the prediction model is a function of the total number of accurate classifications that result as well as number of errors that are made, particularly in regard to screening out otherwise qualified applicants. Accurate classification includes true positives (targeted outcome predicted and occurs), true negatives (target outcome not predicted and does not occur), false positives (target outcome is predicted but does not occur), and false negatives (targeted outcome occurs but is not predicted). The base rate of occurrence of the targeted outcome directly affects the rate at which each of these classifications occurs. In general, the efficiency of a prediction model is a function of the ratio of false positives to true positives as compared to the ratio of targeted occurrence to nonoccurrence (Meehl & Rosen, 1955). In low-baseline situations, a test or set of rules can easily seem accurate in identifying true positives but will nonetheless be worse than, or no better than, chance in terms of overall accuracy. The goal of a screen-out selection model is to maximize the hit rate (prediction of targeted outcome or true positives) while minimizing the rate at which otherwise qualified applicants are screened out (false positive). The rate at which otherwise qualified applicants are screened out due to error is also known as the cost of selection. Given the critical responsibilities of the law enforcement job and the potential harm to society that can accrue in the absence of screening, some level of error (or cost of selection) is usually seen as acceptable. However, this issue is also often the subject of debate when screening decisions are challenged legally, particularly in regard to EEOC requirements. Whether creating a set of actuarial assessment rules or considering information for clinical judgment, the base rate of occurrence of the targeted attribute plays a major mediating role in the overall accuracy of the screening instruments, model, and/or decision. As noted above, predictions in pre-employment assessment are fundamentally classification problems in which one of two conditions is predicted (e.g., problem/no problem). As shown in Table 7.1, 161

Michael J. Cuttler Table 7.1 Classifications of Predictions and Outcomes

Observed Not observed

Predicted

Not Predicted

True positive False positive

False negative True negative

Note: A predicted outcome (e.g., integrity violation) may be observed to be true (true positive) or false (false positive). Similarly, a prediction of no outcome (e.g., not a violator) may be observed as false (false negative) or true (true negative). In general, the efficiency of a prediction model is a function of the ratio of false positives to true positives as compared to the ratio of occurrence to nonoccurrence (Meehl & Rosen, 1955).

two-condition classifications yield four outcomes: correct prediction of problem (true positive), correct prediction of no problem (true negative), incorrect prediction of problem (false positive), and incorrect prediction of no problem (false negative). To appreciate the effects of baseline on classification, one must consider the ratio of true positives (hits) to false positives (cost) in light of base rate of occurrence, and results must be compared to the overall accuracy of what would occur by chance (without screening or testing; Finn, 2009). Table 7.2 illustrates an example of classification in a low-baseline situation: predicting integrity violations whose base rate is thought to be 5% or lower among police officers (Boes, Chandler, & Timm, 1997). In Table 7.2a, 1,200 applicants are randomly assigned to cells based on this base rate; that is, 60 applicants are randomly predicted to be violators. Given the 5% base rate, only three (.25%) will be true positives or actual violators, 57 (4.75%) will be false positives and potentially screened out, while 57 (4.75%) actual violators will be screened in (false negatives). In addition, 1,083 (90.25%) true negatives will be predicted, making the overall accuracy of random selection 90.5% and the cost of selection using this random procedure instrument 4.75% of qualified applicants screened out (false positives). A test or a model can claim to be very accurate in identifying true positives, but identification of true positives alone does not indicate that the test is more accurate than chance (or not screening) in terms of overall accuracy and cost. Table 7.2b represents the results of a hypothetical test (or rule) that claims to be 90% accurate in identifying violators correctly in terms of true positive. As such, 57 of the 60 violators (90%) are identified (true positives); however, this test/rule actually identifies a total of 172 applicants (14.3%) as violators, erroneously classifying 118 nonviolators (10.4%; false positives). The overall accuracy of this test is actually slightly less than chance (89.7%), and the cost of selection represented by this test is 10.4% (118/1,200) or more than twice as high as would be expected by chance (4.75%). Given this rate of false positives (and the low base rate), this would hold true even if the test were 100% accurate in identifying violators, that is, its cost of 10.4% would be twice as high, while its overall accuracy would improve only slightly to 90.2% and still be less than chance (90.5%). In short, given the low base rate of occurrence of integrity violators (5%), a test/rule must produce significantly less than 4.5% false positives to improve over chance Before using a test that claims to have high accuracy in identifying negative performers, psychologists and hiring authorities would be well advised to carefully review complete data in regard to both false positives and false negatives in the test’s technical manual prior to building decision rules based on specific test scores. This is particularly salient given the current legal environment (EEOC, ADA Division, Office of Legal Counsel, 1995) which stresses minimizing cost or false positives (screening out otherwise qualified applicants) since even if the test were perfect in identifying true positives, the hiring authority would also be responsible for minimizing the rate of false positives (cost). When predicting events with higher baseline, tests and models can be considerably more accurate than chance. In this regard, Table 7.3 represents predictions of events with somewhat higher base rates—for example, incidents of on-the-job discipline to include relatively minor reprimands as 162

Actuarial vs. Clinical Judgment Prediction Models Table 7.2 1,200 Officers Integrity Violations (Base Rate = 5%) (a)  Randomly Assign (base rate = 5%)

Actual integrity violators Actual nonviolators

Predicted Integrity Violators

Predicted Nonviolators

3 (true positives) or 0.25% 57 (false positives) or 4.75% 60 (5%)

57 (false negatives) or 4.75% 1083 (true negatives) or 90.25% 1140 (95%)

Chance assignment is 90.5% accurate (1086/1200). (b)  Test for Predicting Integrity Violators (90% Accurate for True Positive) (base rate = 5%)

Actual integrity violators Actual nonviolators

Predicted Integrity Violators

Predicted Nonviolators

54 (true positives) or 4.5% 118 (false positives) or 10.4% 172 (14.3%)

6 (false negatives) or 0.5% 1022 (true negatives) or 85.1% 1028 (85.7%)

Test is 89.7% accurate (1076/1200). Note: In this case, the test is slightly less accurate than chance, but has more than twice as many false positives. Even if the test identified all violators (true positives = 60, false negatives = 0), overall accuracy would be 90.2%—still less than chance with twice as many excluded due to the false positive rate.

Table 7.3 1,200 Officers Minor Discipline (Base Rate = 20%) (a)  Randomly Assign (base rate = 20%)

Actual integrity violators Actual nonviolators

Predicted Integrity Violators

Predicted Nonviolators

48 (true positives) or 4% 192 (false positives) or 16.0% 240 (20%)

192 (false negatives) or 16.0% 768 (true negatives) or 64.0% 960 (80%)

Chance assignment is 68% accurate (816/1200). (b) Test for Predicting Integrity Violators (90% accurate for true positive) (base rate = 20%)

Actual integrity violators Actual nonviolators

Predicted Integrity Violators

Predicted Nonviolators

216 (true positives) or 18% 118 (false positives) or 9.8% 334 (27.8%)

24 (false negatives) or 2% 842 (true negatives) or 70.2% 866 (70.4%)

Test is 88.1% accurate (1058/1200). Note: Due to the higher base rate, the test is now significantly better than chance in terms of both accuracy and false positives. A change in the base rate of occurrence of the dependent (outcome) variable from 5% (integrity violators) to 20% (minor discipline) has a substantial effect on the accuracy of a test that identifies 90% of true positives as well as on the cost of selection (false positives).

well as serious incidents—known to be around 20% (Sarchione et al., 1998). Table 7.3a represents a chance assignment matrix of the same 1,200 officers whereby 240 (20%) are randomly predicted to experience some form of disciplinary action. Given a 20% base rate, 48 of the random assignments will be true positives and 192 (16%) will be false positives. Similarly, 192 disciplined officers (16%) will be randomly assigned as false negatives and the remaining 768 (64%) will be true negatives. The overall accuracy of this (random) classification procedure (68%) is significantly less, and the cost (16% 163

Michael J. Cuttler

false positive) significantly higher than what was observed in the earlier situation when the base rate was 5%. The difference in accuracy of random assignment is a function of the higher base rate and, consequently, the higher number of false positives. Table 7.3b represents the classification results that would be obtained from the same test described earlier that claimed 90% accuracy in true positives when predicting a 20% baseline outcome. Now the test identifies 216 true positives (90% of 240) and the same 118 false positives as were identified in the earlier condition. In this case, the overall accuracy is 88.1%, which is substantially greater than the chance model, while the false positives (cost) represent only 9.8% or slightly more than half the rate expected by chance (16%). These data illustrate the importance of base rate when evaluating instruments as well as when selecting outcomes. In the case of low-base-rate outcomes such as discipline for integrity, violence, sexual misconduct, and so forth, the utility of attempting to screen applicants in order to predict these specific outcomes is questionable. However, accurate prediction of higher base rate occurrences—such as failing background investigation, failing to complete training, recycling in field training, and/or expanding the definition of discipline to include less serious infractions—is considerably more practical, particularly for use in pre-offer screening, given properly designed instruments, uncorrelated predictors, and models. In addition to correlations between measures and base rates of occurrence, a third related issue affecting the validity of questionnaires, tests, and prediction models involves characteristics of the applicant pool when compared to the general population. Although the pool of individuals who apply to law enforcement agencies is in many ways similar to the general population, given the general requirements of the job, a certain degree of range restriction is to be expected within the sample due to educational requirements, credit, and criminal history requirements, illegal drug use, and so on. As noted by Ben-Porath (2009), range restriction may also affect test and questionnaire interpretation when using instruments that report scores in relative frequency terms, for example, some personal history questionnaires and some test instruments that calculate scaled scores from applicant norms without reference to general population norms. Failure to take range restriction into account can cause misleading results, particularly in regard to disregarding or eliminating potentially useful predictors as well as to inflate the apparent validity of a particular test and/or overstate (or understate) the significance of a score that deviates from population norms. Similarly, correction for range restriction is particularly important when considering studies that use concurrent designs (study of applicants who were hired as opposed to all who applied). In this regard, there is likely to be significant differences in range between a group of applicants who have been hired after successfully completing a screening procedure that includes testing, background investigation, interviews, and so on, as well as the larger group of applicants who either failed to successfully complete or dropped out of the hiring process. Given reliable quantitative measures, correcting for range restriction effects is a relatively straightforward statistical procedure that requires comparison of general population descriptive statistics for an instrument or measure to applicant pool statistics (Hunter & Schmidt, 1990). Range restriction within the law enforcement pre-employment assessment literature has been reported and corrected in the work of several researchers in this area (Cuttler & Muchinsky, 2006; Ones et al., 2004; Sarchione et al., 1998; Sellbom et al., 2007; Varela et al., 2004). However, when considering validation evidence and/or research conclusions regarding specific instruments, psychologists and other hiring authorities should carefully review the degree to which range restriction may have affected results.

Summary and Conclusions Research supporting the practice of pre-employment psychological assessment of police officer candidates has its historical roots in clinical, I/O, and forensic psychology. The development of this area

164

Actuarial vs. Clinical Judgment Prediction Models

mirrors the field of assessment in general and is characterized by a convergence of these disciplines as well as the mediating effect of societal conditions and legal developments. In the past two decades, pre-employment assessment of law enforcement officers as an area of interest, inquiry, and professional practice has emerged from its historical roots to become a broadly regarded and relatively popular area of both practice and research. Proficiency in this area calls for a broad understanding of clinical, forensic, and I/O psychology principles. In addition, the work of Paul Meehl and others in regard to actuarial vs. clinical prediction is particularly relevant. The fundamental distinction between actuarial and clinical prediction is not necessarily the data developed or the practice protocol (almost always test scores, life history data, and interview responses). Rather, the primary distinction is that the latter (clinical) involves intervention by professional judgment, while the former (actuarial) involves consistent application of rules in all cases. Actuarial prediction rules may be quantitatively sophisticated, they may be constructed as a set of expert rules derived from professional experience and review of the literature, or they may be a combination of the two. In any event, when using any reasonably developed model, a substantial body of research documents the superior accuracy of this (actuarial) approach over clinical prediction, particularly when base rates of occurrence for targeted outcomes exceed 20% as is the case with background/selection failure, training failure, and disciplinary action (Cuttler & Muchinsky, 2006). Development of an actuarial model does not necessarily require amassing large amounts of data or incorporating sophisticated statistical techniques. Test instruments and personal history questionnaires designed to support actuarial assessment are widely available, and simple models can be built by consistently applying expert rules to actuarially derived data. Practice protocols need not be changed—simply the process by which the data is aggregated. Practitioners need only develop rules, consistently apply them, conscientiously follow up with results, and modify the rules if indicated. No predictor or prediction model is perfect: all are subject to error, and all are sensitive to and affected by error sources such as correlation between predictors, population range restriction, and base rate of occurrence. Certainly, there are some situations—for example, low-base-rate outcomes such as integrity violation, sexual misconduct, and so forth—in which neither actuarial nor clinical models are likely to predict at levels greater than chance. There are also rare situations such as Meehl’s broken leg case (Meehl, 1954/1996), where additional information will trump actuarial models.5 However, these situations are few and far between (Grove & Lloyd, 2004). All things being equal, and in most settings associated with pre-employment assessment, research findings consistently report superiority of actuarial prediction over clinical judgment. In the past two decades, legal regulations and professional guidelines have emerged that have clarified the need for scientifically prudent practice and supported use of new and more efficient assessment protocols (e.g., bifurcated assessment) and, as such, encouraged the emergence of a substantial body of scientific knowledge. Similarly, consensus has emerged in regard to the predictive validity of various psychological tests, and research has been performed documenting the relevance of various outcome criteria both in terms of performance-linked personality constructs and actual job outcome. There have also been several new instruments developed using other predictor domains (e.g., life history and merged processes) that have yielded evidence of convergent validity. All of these developments are consistent with the use of actuarial models. The distinctions between I/O and clinical practice and research have largely evaporated (we are all pretty much fishing in the middle of the lake these days), and the current economic situation within the public sector has created the need to perform these services in an efficient manner. Given the emergence of bifurcated assessment processes and the higher base rates associated with well-defined outcomes, as well as the existing body of knowledge regarding the superior accuracy of rule-based (actuarial) assessment, the state of the art at the present time begs the adoption of actuarial assessment principles into the practice of pre-employment assessment.

165

Michael J. Cuttler

Notes 1. Clinical prediction refers to the use of an individual (an expert, a clinician) to predict an event. Actuarial prediction refers to the use of an actuarial formula to predict the same event (Westen & Weinberger, 2004). 2. The “Big Five factors of personality” are five broad domains or dimensions of personality that have been identified and documented through extensive research. These factors are considered one of the most comprehensive, empirical, data-driven research findings in the history of personality psychology. The Big 5 factors are openness, conscientiousness, extroversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism. (Neuroticism is also referred to as emotional stability.) For detailed information, see Goldberg (1993). 3. In U.S. employment law, adverse impact is also known as disparate impact and is defined as a “substantially different rate of selection in hiring, promotion, or other employment decision which works to the disadvantage of members of a race, sex, or ethnic group” (EEOC Uniform Employee Selection Guidelines; Questions and Answers; Section 16. http://www.uniformguidelines.com/uniformguidelines.html#129). 4. The author (Michael J. Cuttler, PhD) is a principal owner of LESI® (Law Enforcement Services, Inc.), publisher of onlinePHQ® and Multi DomainTM Screening Report, as well as owner and primary named inventor in U.S. Patent 7,346,541 relating to these instruments. 5. Meehl (1954/1996) presented his classic broken leg case as an example of one of the few times clinical prediction would outperform an actuarial model. In this case, Professor A is known to have gone to the movies every Tuesday night for the past year. The actuarial model would therefore predict that Professor A would go to the movies next Tuesday. However, on Monday, Professor A has broken his leg and is forced to wear a cast that does not allow him to sit in a theater seat. The “clinician” observes this fact; the model cannot.

References and Further Reading Aamodt, M. G. (2004). Research in law enforcement selection. Boca Raton, FL: Brown Walker. American Psychological Association. (1985). Standards for educational and psychological testing. Washington, DC: Author. Americans With Disabilities Act, 42 U.S.C.A. § 12101 et seq. (1990). Anastasi, A. (1988). Psychological testing (6th ed.). New York: Macmillan. Azen, S. P. (1974). Predictors of resignation and performance of law enforcement officers. American Journal of Community Psychology, 2, 79–86. Azen, S. P., Snibbe, H. M., & Montgomery, H. R. (1973). A longitudinal predictive study of success and performance of law enforcement officers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 57, 190–192. Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. D. (1991). The big five personality dimensions and job performance: A metaanalysis. Personnel Psychology, 44, 1–26. Bartol, C. R. (1982). Psychological characteristics of small town police officers. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 10, 58–63. Bartol, C. R. (1991). Predictive validation of the MMPI for small-town police officers who fail. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 22, 127–132. Bartol, C. R. (1996). Police psychology: Then, now, and beyond. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 23, 70–89. Ben-Porath, Y. S. (2007, October). Use of the MMPI-2 Restructured Form in assessing law enforcement candidates. Paper presented at the meeting of the Police Psychological Services Section of the International Association of Chiefs of Police, New Orleans, LA. Ben-Porath, Y. S. (2009). The MMPI-2 and the MMPI-2-RF. Manuscript submitted for publication to Spilberg, S., California Peace Officer Standards and Training Commission, Sacramento, CA. Ben-Porath, Y. S., & Tellegen, A. (2008). The Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory—2 restructured form: Manual for administration, scoring, and interpretation. Minneapolis University of Minnesota Press. Boes, J. O., Chandler, C. J., & Timm, H. W. (1997). Police integrity: Use of personality measures to identify corruptionprone officers. Monterey, CA: Defense Personnel Security Research and Education Center. Bolte, M. E., & Smith, E. H. (2001). Psychological screening standards: State-by-state survey. Columbia: South Carolina Criminal Justice Academy, Law Enforcement Assessment and Psychological Services Unit. Campbell, D. T., & Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 56, 81–105. Carpenter, B. N., & Raza, S. M. (1987). Personality characteristics of police applicants: Comparisons across subgroups and with other populations. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 15, 10–17. Cattell, R. B. (1949). 16PF questionnaire. Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality and Ability Testing.

166

Actuarial vs. Clinical Judgment Prediction Models Cattell, A. K., & Cattell, H. E. P. (1993). Sixteen Personality Factor fifth edition questionnaire. Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality and Ability Testing. Cattell, R. B., Cattell, A. K., Cattell, H. E. P., Russell, M. T., & Bedwell, S. (2003). The PsychEval personality questionnaire. Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality and Ability Testing. Chenoweth, J. H. (1961). Situational tests: A new attempt at assessing police candidates. Journal of Criminal Law, Criminology, and Police Science, 52, 232–238. Cochrane, R. E., Tett, R. P., & Vandecreek, L. (2003). Psychological testing and the selection of police officers: A national survey. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 30, 511. Cohen, B., & Chaiken, J. M. (1973). Police background characteristics and performance. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books. Corey, D. M., Cuttler, M. J., & Moss, J. A. (2009, December). Pre-application discussion with the ABPP Board of Trustees regarding a proposed Specialty Board in police and public safety psychology. Presentation made to Board of Trustees American Board of Professional Psychology, Chapel Hill, NC. Cureton, E. E., Cronbach, L. J., Meehl, P. E., Ebel, R. L., & Ward, A. (1996). Validity. In A. W. Ward, H. W. Stoker, & M. Murray-Ward (Eds.), Educational measurement: Origins, theories, and explications, Vol. 1: Basic concepts and theories (pp. 125–243). Lanham, MD: University Press of America. Cuttler, M. J. (2000). Multi domain screening report. Greensboro, NC: Law Enforcement Services. Cuttler, M. J. (2007). Minimizing response bias, enhancing veracity, and improving accuracy of predictions based on biodata [White paper]. IPAT 2007 LESI 2001. Cuttler, M., Cuttler, E., & Seddon, T. (2008). U.S. Patent No. 7,346,541. System, method and computer readable medium for acquiring and analyzing personal history information. Washington, DC: U.S. Patent and Trademark Office. Cuttler, M. J., & Muchinsky, P. M. (2006). Prediction of law enforcement training performance and dysfunctional job performance with general mental ability, personality and life history variables. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 33(1), 3–25. Dawes, R. M. (2005). The ethical implications of Paul Meehl’s work on comparing clinical versus actuarial prediction methods. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 61(10), 1245–1255. Dawes, R. M., Faust, D., & Meehl, P. E. (1989). Clinical versus actuarial judgment. Science, 243, 1668–1674. Dee-Burnett, R., Johns, E. F., & Krug, S. E. (1981). Law enforcement assessment and development report (LEADR)— manual. Chicago: IPAT. Delprino, R. P., & Bahn, C. (1988). National survey of the extent and nature of psychological services in police departments. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 19, 421–425. Drees, S. A., Ones, D. S., Cullen, M. J., Spilberg, S. W., & Viswesvaran, C. (2003, April 11). Personality assessment in police officer screening: Mandates and practices. In S. W. Spilberg & D. S. Ones (Chairs), Personality and work behaviors of police officers. Symposium conducted at the 18th annual meeting of the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, Orlando, FL. DuBois, P. H., & Watson, R. I. (1950). The selection of patrolmen. Journal of Applied Psychology, 34, 90–95. Dunnette, M. D., & Motowidlo, S. J. (1976). Police selection and career assessment. Washington, DC: Law. Enforcement Assistance Administration, U.S. Department of Justice, U.S. Government Printing Office. Equal Employment Opportunity Commission, Uniform Employee Selection Guidelines; Questions and Answers; Section 16. www.uniformguidelines.com/uniformguidelines.html#129. Equal Employment Opportunity Commission, ADA Division, Office of Legal Counsel. (1995). Enforcement guidance: Pre-employment disability-related inquiries and medical examinations under the Americans with Disabilities Act of 1990. Washington, DC: Equal Employment Opportunity Commission. Finn, S. E. (2009). Incorporating base rate information in daily clinical decision making. In J. N. Butcher (Ed.), Oxford handbook of personality assessment (pp. 140–149). New York: Oxford University Press. Goldberg, L. R. (1991). Human mind versus regression equation: Five contrasts. In W. M. Grove & D. Cicchetti (Eds.), Thinking clearly about psychology: Vol. 1. Matters of public interest (pp. 173–184). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. Goldberg, L. R. (1993). The structure of phenotypic personality traits. American Psychologist, 48, 26–34. Gordon, M. E., & Kleiman, L. S. (1976). The prediction of trainability using a work sample test and an aptitude test: A direct comparison. Personnel Psychology, 29, 243–253. Gough, H. G. (1956). California psychological inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Gough, H. G. (1987). California Psychological Inventory administrator’s guide. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Grove, W. M. (2005). Clinical versus statistical prediction: The contribution of Paul E. Meehl. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 61(10), 1233–1243.

167

Michael J. Cuttler Grove, W. M., & Lloyd, M. (2006). Meehl’s contribution to clinical versus statistical prediction. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115(2), 192–194. Grove, W. M., & Meehl, P. E. (1996). Comparative efficiency of informal (subjective, impressionistic) and formal (mechanical algorithmic) prediction procedures: The clinical-statistical controversy. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 2(2), 293–323. Grove, W. M., Zald, D. H., Lebow, B. S., Snitz, B. E., & Nelson, C. (2000). Clinical versus mechanical prediction: A meta-analysis. Psychological Assessment, 12, 19–30. Hargrave, G. E., Hiatt, D., & Gaffney, T. W. (1986). A comparison of MMPI and CPI test profiles for traffic and deputy sheriffs. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 14, 250–258. Harville, L. M. (1991). An NCME instructional module on standard error of measurement; in ITEMS, Instructional Topics in Educational Measurement. Madison, WI: National Council on Measurement in Education. Hathaway, S. R., & McKinley, J. C. (1940). The MMPI manual. New York: Psychological Corporation. Hiatt, D., & Hargrove, G. E. (1988). Predicting job performance problems with psychological screening. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 16, 122–125. Hirsch, H. R., Northrup, L. C., & Schmidt, F. L. (1986). Validity generalization results for law enforcement occupations. Personnel Psychology, 39, 399–420. Inwald, R. (1980, September). Personality characteristics of law enforcement applicants and development of assessment instruments. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Montreal, Canada. Inwald, R. (1982a, August). Conducting testing programs within law enforcement agencies: Ethical/legal issues. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Washington, DC. Inwald, R. (1982b). Inwald Personality Inventory (IPI) technical manual. New York: Hilson Research, Inc. (Reprinted [2008], Chicago: IPAT, Inc., a subsidiary of OPP Ltd.) Inwald, R. (1983, August). Cross-validation of job performance prediction for law enforcement officers. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Anaheim, CA. Inwald, R. (1984a, August). Ethical guidelines for screening for high risk occupations. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Toronto, Canada. Inwald, R. (1984b). Pre-employment psychological testing for law enforcement: Ethical and procedural issues: What should administrators do? Training Aids Digest, 9(6), 1–7. Inwald, R. (1984c). Proposed guidelines for providers of pre-employment psychological testing services to law enforcement agencies, version III. Manuscript submitted to APA Task Force of Division 18, Section of Police and Public Safety Psychology. Inwald, R. (1985a). Professional opinions on a set of proposed guidelines for mental health practitioners conducting pre-employment psychological screening programs in law enforcement agencies. Corrections Digest, 16(7), 1–2. Inwald, R. (1985b). Proposed guidelines for conducting pre-employment psychological screening programs. Crime Control Digest, 19(11), 1–6. Inwald, R. (1985c, December). Establishing standards for psychological screening. Presentation at the FBI-sponsored World Conference on Police Psychology, Quantico, VA. Inwald, R. (1986a). Why include individual interviews for all law enforcement candidates? Criminal Justice Digest, 5(3), 1–3. Inwald, R. (1986b). The development of guidelines for psychological screening in law enforcement agencies. In J. T. Reese & J. M. Horn (Eds.), Police psychology: Operational assistance (pp. 233–240). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice, FBI. (Reprinted in Crime Control Digest, 20(36), 6–9.) Inwald, R. (1988). Five year follow-up study of departmental terminations as predicted by 16 pre-employment psychological indicators. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73(4), 703–710. Inwald, R. (1995). Hilson Safety/Security Risk Inventory (HSRI) technical manual. New York: Hilson Research, Inc. (Reprinted [2008], Chicago: IPAT, Inc., a subsidiary of OPP Ltd.) Inwald, R. (1998). Hilson Job Analysis Questionnaire (HJAQ) technical manual. New York: Hilson Research, Inc. (Reprinted [2008], Chicago: IPAT, Inc., a subsidiary of OPP Ltd.) Inwald, R. (2008). The Inwald Personality Inventory (IPI) and Hilson Research inventories: Development and rationale. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 13(4), 298–327. Inwald, R., & Brobst, K. E. (1988). Hilson Personnel Profile/Success Quotient (HPP/SQ) technical manual. New York: Hilson Research, Inc. (Reprinted [2008], Chicago: IPAT, Inc., a subsidiary of OPP Ltd.) Inwald, R., & Gebbia, M. I. (1992). Inwald Survey 5 (IS5) technical manual. New York: Hilson Research, Inc. (Reprinted [2008], Chicago: IPAT, Inc., a subsidiary of OPP Ltd.) Inwald, R., & Kaufman, J. C. (1989). Hilson Career Satisfaction Index (HCSI) technical manual. New York: Hilson Research, Inc. (Reprinted [2008], Chicago: IPAT, Inc., a subsidiary of OPP Ltd.)

168

Actuarial vs. Clinical Judgment Prediction Models Inwald, R., & Knatz, H. (1988, August). Seven-year follow-up of officer terminations predicted by psychological testing. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, Atlanta, GA. Inwald, R., Resko, J. A., & Favuzza, V. (1994). Inwald Survey 2 (IS2) & Inwald Survey 8 (IS8) technical manual. New York: Hilson Research, Inc. (Reprinted [2008], Chicago: IPAT, Inc., a subsidiary of OPP Ltd.) Inwald, R., Resko, J. A., & Favuzza, V. (1996). Hilson Life Adjustment Profile (HLAP) technical manual. New York: Hilson Research, Inc. (Reprinted [2008], Chicago: IPAT, Inc., a subsidiary of OPP Ltd.) Inwald, R., & Sakales, S. (1982, August). Predicting negative behaviors of officer recruits using the MMPI and IPI. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Washington, DC. Inwald, R., Traynor, B., & Favuzza, V. A. (1998). Hilson Management Survey (HMS) technical manual. New York: Hilson Research, Inc. (Reprinted [2007], Chicago: IPAT, Inc., a subsidiary of OPP Ltd.) Johnson, M., & Roberts, M. (2006, October). PsyQ report and test scoring system. JR&A Users Conference. Kent, D. A., & Eisenberg, T. (1972). The selection and promotion of police officers. The Police Chief, 39(2), 20–29. Landy, F. J. (1976). The validity of the interview in police officer selection. Journal of Applied Psychology, 61, 193–198. Lefkowitz, J. (1977). Industrial-organizational psychology and the police. American Psychologist, 32(5), 346–364. Levy, R. J. (1967). Predicting police failures. Journal of Criminal Law, Criminology and Police Science, 58, 265–275. Levy, R. J. (1973). A method for the identification of the high risk police applicant. In J. R. Snibbe & R. M. Snibbe (Eds.), The urban policeman in transition. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas. Matarazzo, J. D., Allen, B. V., Saslow, G., & Wiens, A. N. (1964). Characteristics of successful policeman and fireman applicants. Journal of Applied Psychology, 48, 123–133. Meehl, P. E. (1959). Some ruminations on the validation of clinical procedures. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 13, 106–128. Meehl, P. E. (1978). Theoretical risks and tabular asterisks: Sir Karl, Sir Ronald, and the slow progress of soft psychology. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46, 806–834. Meehl, P. E. (1986). Causes and effects of my disturbing little book. Journal of Personality Assessment, 50(3), 370–375. Meehl, P. E. (1996). Clinical vs. statistical prediction. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. (Original work published 1954) Meehl, P. E., & Rosen, A. (1955). Antecedent probability and the efficiency of psychometric signs, patterns, or cutting scores. Psychological Bulletin, 52, 194–216. Monahan, J. (1981). The clinical prediction of violent behavior. Washington, DC: National Institute of Mental Health. (Reprinted as Predicting violent behavior: An assessment of clinical techniques. [1981]. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Excerpts reprinted in L. J. Hertzberg, et al., Eds., [1990]. Violent behavior [pp. 125–150, 259–279]. Manhattan Beach, CA: PMA.) Morey, L. C. (2007). Personality Assessment Inventory professional manual (2nd ed.). Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Mumford, M. D., Costanza, D. P., Connelly, M. S., & Johnson, F. F. (1996). Item generation procedures and background data scales: Implications for construct and criterion-related validity. Personnel Psychology, 49, 361–398. National Advisory Commission on Criminal Justice Standards and Goals. (1973). Task force on police. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. O’Brien, S. G. (1996). The predictive validity of personality testing in police selection: A meta-analysis (Unpublished master’s thesis). University of Guelph, Guelph, Canada. Oglesby, T. W. (1957). Use of emotional screening in the selection of police applicants. Public Personnel Review, 18, 228–231. Ones, D. S., Viswesvaran, C., Cullen, M. J., Drees, S. A., & Langkamp, K. (2003, April). Personality and police officer behaviors: A comprehensive meta-analysis. In S. W. Spilberg & D. S. Ones (Chairs), Personality work behaviors of police officers. Symposium conducted at the 18th annual meeting of the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, Orlando, FL. Ones, D. S., Viswesvaran, C., & Dilchert, S. (2004, November). A construct-based, comprehensive meta- analysis and implications for pre-offer screening and psychological evaluations. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the International Association of Chiefs of Police (IACP), Los Angeles, CA. Police Psychological Services Section of the International Association of Chiefs of Police. (1992, 1998, 2004, 2009). Pre-employment psychological evaluation guidelines. Retrieved from http://psych.iacp.org. Roberts, M., & Johnson, M. (2001). CPI police and public safety selection report, technical manual. Los Gatos, CA: Law Enforcement Psychological Services. Roberts, M. D., Thompson, J. A., & Johnson, M. (2000). PAI law enforcement, corrections, and public safety selection report manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

169

Michael J. Cuttler Saccuzzo, D. P., Higgins, G., & Lewandowski, D. (1974). Program for psychological assessment of law enforcement officers: Initial evaluation. Psychological Reports, 35, 651–654. Sakales, S., & Inwald, R. (1982, August). Prediction of police academy performance for transit police officer candidates: Role of two personality screening measures to identify on-the-job behavior problems of law enforcement officer recruits. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Washington, DC. Sarchione, C. D., Cuttler, M. J., Muchinsky, P. M., & Nelson-Gray, R. O. (1998). Prediction of dysfunctional job behaviors among law enforcement officers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 904–912. Saxe, S. J., & Reiser, M. (1976). A comparison of three police applicant groups using the MMPI. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 4, 419–425. Schmitt, N., Gooding, R. Z., Noe, R. A., & Kirsch, M. (1984). Metaanalyses of validity studies published between 1964 and 1982 and the investigation of study characteristics. Personnel Psychology, 37, 407–422. Scogin, F., Schumacher, J., Howland, K., & McGee, J. (1989, August). The predictive validity of psychological testing and peer evaluation in law enforcement settings. Paper presented at the American Psychological Association Convention, New Orleans, LA. Scrivner, E. M., & Kurke, M. I. (1996). Police psychology at the dawn of the 21st century. In M. I. Kurke & E. M. Scrivner (Eds.), Police psychology into the 21st century. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Sellbom, M., Fischler, G. L., & Ben-Porath, Y. S. (2007). Identifying MMPI-2 predictors of police officer integrity and misconduct. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 34, 985–1004. Shapiro, D. L. (1983). Psychological evaluation and expert testimony. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold. Shedler, J., & Westen, D. (2004). Refining personality disorder diagnosis: Integrating science and practice. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 1350–1365. Shusman, E. J., Inwald, R. E., & Knatz, H. F. (1987). A cross-validation study of police recruit performance as predicted by the IPI and MMPI. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 15, 162–168. Shusman, E. J., Inwald, R. E., & Landa, B. (1984). Correction officer job performance as predicted by the IPI and MMPI: A cross-validation study. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 11(3), 309–329. Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology. (2003). Principles for validation and use of personnel selection procedures (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Smith, D. H., & Stotland, E. (1973). A new look at police officer selection. In J. R. Snibbe & H. M. Snibbe (Eds.), The urban policeman in transition. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas. Spilberg, S. W. (2003, April). Development of psychological screening guidelines for police officers: Background and development of essential traits. In S. W. Spilberg & D. S. Ones (Chairs), Personality work behaviors of police officers. Symposium conducted at the 18th annual meeting of the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, Orlando, FL. Streiner, D. L. (2003). Diagnosing tests: Using and misusing diagnostic and screening tests. Journal of Personality Assessment, 81(3), 209–219. Tenopyr, M. L. (1994). Big five, structural modeling, and item response theory. In G. S. Stokes, M. D. Mumford, & W. A. Owens (Eds.), Biodata handbook: Theory, research, and use of biographical information in selection and performance prediction (pp. 519–534). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Tett, R. P., Jackson, D. N., & Rothstein, M. (1991). Personality measures as predictors of job performance: A meta-analytic review. Personnel Psychology, 44, 703–740. Tett, R. P., Jackson, D. N., Rothstein, M., & Reddon, J. R. (1994). Meta-analysis of personality job performance relations: A reply to Ones, Mount, Barrick, and Hunter (1994). Personnel Psychology, 47, 157–172. U.S. Department of Justice. (2007) Bureau of justice statistics. Retrieved November 20, 2009, from www. ojp.usdoj. gov/bjs/lawenf.htm. Varela, J. G., Boccaccini, M. T., Scogin, F., Stump, J., and Caputo, A. (2004). Personality testing in law enforcement employment settings: A meta-analytic review. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 31, 649. Weiss, P. A., & Inwald, R. (2010). A brief history of personality assessment in police psychology. In P. A. Weiss (Ed.) Personality assessment in police psychology: A 21st century perspective (pp. 5–28). Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas. Westen, D. (2002). Clinical diagnostic interview. Unpublished manuscript, Emory University. Retrieved from www. psychsystems.net/lab. Westen, D., & Rosenthal, R. (2005). Improving construct validity: Cronbach, Meehl, and Neurath’s ship. Psychological Assessment, 17(4), 409–412. Westen, D., & Weinberger, J. (2004). When clinical description becomes statistical prediction. American Psychologist, 59(7), 595–613. Westen, D., & Weinberger, J. (in press). In praise of clinical judgment: Meehl’s forgotten legacy. Journal of Clinical Psychology. Ziskin, J. (1981). Coping with psychiatric and psychological testimony. Los Angeles: Law and Psychology Press.

170

8 APPRAISING AND MANAGING POLICE OFFICER PERFORMANCE Rick Jacobs, Christian Thoroughgood, and Katina Sawyer

Overview of Performance Appraisal in Organizations In the classic book Good to Great, Jim Collins notes that successful organizations and teams have one critical thing in common: they get the right people on the bus (Collins, 2001). Truth be told, selecting and promoting the right people in organizations remain critical first steps in creating a highly effective and efficient workforce. In general this is true, but nowhere is it more the case than in a police department, especially when it comes to selection of new officers. Consider the fact that in almost every other type of organization, deficiencies in selection can at least be partially made up by successful introduction of supervisors and other leaders in various positions throughout the company. In police departments, it is rare—in fact, highly unlikely—that supervisors and leaders come from anywhere else except those who joined the force as new officers. The pool of applicants for promotions is almost exclusively made up of individuals who have come up through the ranks. Ineffective selection programs and the inevitable hiring mistakes that occur even with the best selection processes result in limitations in terms of candidates for promotions (these ideas are further developed in Chapter 11). While getting the right people on the bus or, in the present case, into the patrol vehicle is vital to organizational success, it is equally important that organizations possess well-developed performance appraisal systems to (1) make informed administrative decisions regarding promotions, (2) help facilitate training, and (3) ensure we are doing all we can to motivate and improve the performance of those already on the force. Such systems are essential to providing developmental feedback to officers, helping organizations increase the readiness of officers for their current assignments as well as future positions, and understanding where the department’s strengths and developmental opportunities exist. As such, it remains a priority of most departments to seek out and identify those systems that have the capacity to accurately measure officer performance and provide important performancebased information. Needless to say, the ability to make important personnel decisions that benefit the department and its officers hinges on the quality of the performance evaluation system utilized. In many departments, the performance evaluation and management system is a major weakness and a missed opportunity when it comes to improving individual officer performance and the overall effectiveness of the agency. In the following review, we address what role performance appraisal research plays in the evaluation of police officer performance specifically. In so doing, we first summarize key areas of research on performance appraisal in organizations, pointing out related theory, goals, and other traditional

171

Rick Jacobs, et al.

issues found in the research and practice literature. Next, we discuss those system elements that lead to effective performance appraisal and management programs, offer information on where programs can get off track, and document the evidence for performance improvement as a result of performance management. The process of system implementation is also discussed, including best practices for preparing officers for performance evaluations, steps that departments can take to create a positive environment for launching the process, proper training of supervisors who will be the evaluators of performance, departmental requirements for monitoring and evaluating the rating process, and ideas on utilizing performance data effectively. The chapter also addresses issues related to feedback to officers, including who should deliver feedback, what feedback should be given, and how often feedback should be conveyed. We also highlight the critical role of trust in the feedback process and the creation of a climate conducive to feedback. Finally, we conclude the chapter by discussing the future of performance appraisal and management, including the role of online appraisals, performance improvement initiatives, and self-evaluation.

Theory, Goals, and Traditional Issues Defining Performance Industrial-organizational psychologists have long since debated the definition of job performance. Austin and Villanova (1992) famously referred to this issue as the “criterion-problem,” suggesting that traditional measures of job performance fail to conceptualize and measure performance constructs that are multidimensional in nature. Specifically, such measures often suffer from criterion deficiency and contamination. While the former refers to aspects of the performance domain not measured by the appraisal instrument (leaving out of the evaluation critical performance factors), the latter refers to specific measurement error created by including in the assessment performance either measures that are not part of the job requirements or things out of the control of the officer being evaluated (i.e., unrelated elements unintentionally measured as a part of the performance domain). Examples of criterion contamination include inherent rater biases (e.g., halo, leniency, and so forth, which are tied to the assessor—not the assessee), external factors outside the ratee’s control (resource deficiencies such as poor or no training programs, understaffing, and/or inadequate equipment), and/or behaviors not identified through job analysis but that nonetheless influence ratings (e.g., specific indicators of personality that have no obvious link to required job behaviors). As such, work psychologists have underscored the importance of conducting a thorough job analysis to fully define the performance domain, develop a holistic understanding of key job-related behaviors, and thereby develop a valid appraisal system (Viswesvaran & Ones, 2000). Before conducting a job analysis, however, it is important to have a general understanding of what performance entails. Campbell (1999) defined performance as “behavior or action that is relevant for the organization’s goals and that can be scaled (measured) in terms of the level of proficiency (or contribution to goals) that is represented by a particular action or set of actions” (pp. 402–403). Furthermore, Motowidlo (2003) defined performance as “the total expected value to the organization of the discrete behavioral episodes that an individual carries out over a standard period of time” (p. 51). In short, performance is what employers pay employees to do (Campbell, 1999) and, in the context of police performance, it is not only what the department expects from the officer but also what the public demands—and it represents those behaviors associated with preserving life and property. Additionally, scholars have distinguished between task and contextual performance (Arvey & Murphy, 1998). Borman and Motowidlo (1993) suggested that task performance revolves primarily around the organization’s “technical core,” where raw materials are processed and transformed into their final products. As such, they defined task performance as 172

Appraising and Managing Officer Performance

the proficiency with which job incumbents perform activities that are formally recognized as part of their jobs, activities that contribute to the organization’s technical core either directly by implementing a part of its technological process, or indirectly by providing it with needed materials or services. (p. 73) Contextual performance, on the other hand, does not support the technical core itself as much as it supports the “organizational, social, and psychological environment in which the technical core must function” (Borman & Motowidlo, 1993, p. 73). For example, while task performance for police officers might include monthly documentation of traffic stops, creating accurate reports, and listening carefully to citizens while they describe events, contextual performance might include behaviors related to boosting squad morale or being available for additional assignments. In fact, Motowidlo and Van Scotter (1994) found that both factors contribute independently to overall performance ratings, with personality variables predicting contextual performance more than task performance. Thus, it appears that raters take into account both task and contextual elements when evaluating subordinates. This suggests cognitive ability tends to predict task performance, while personality may better predict contextual performance (Arvey & Murphy, 1998). As such, it is possible that different predictors may be required depending on the performance domain in question.

Previous Research Much of the research on performance appraisal prior to 1980 was dominated by measurement issues and the search for a perfect rating format. Such research focused on comparing various rating formats, including graphic ratings scales, behaviorally anchored ratings scales, traditional Likert scales, behavioral checklists, and other formats (Arvey & Murphy, 1998; Jacobs, Kafry, & Zedeck, 1980; Landy & Farr, 1980). However, after 30 years of research on the topic, Landy and Farr (1980) called a moratorium on research related to rating formats, concluding that although raters may have a preference for one format over another, differences in ratings due to format are minimal and overall inconsequential when it comes to the overall performance information provided to the individual and the organization. They did, however, note that behavioral anchors are superior to simple numerical scales or those that rely on common adjectives and that anchoring should be based on methods grounded in firm psychometric theory. In fact, right now any organization—police or other—can find a variety of tools for measuring performance and be ready to launch a program without the burden of creating a site-specific rating form. With respect to police officer performance, the reader of this chapter can contact the senior author and receive a set of scales to be used for officers, supervisors, or command personnel. Suffice it to say, the scales themselves are critical for a successful system, but are available from many sources. Moving beyond issues related to the rating format, Landy and Farr (1980) urged researchers to consider the role of rater cognition in the performance appraisal process, which subsequently led to a flurry of research on the topic during the 1980s. During this time, increased attention was paid toward understanding how raters reach their judgments of employee performance. Such research targeted various cognitive processes of raters including observation and information acquisition, encoding and categorization, storage, retrieval, integration, and evaluation (DeNisi, Cafferty, & Meglino, 1984; DeNisi & Williams, 1988; Murphy & Cleveland, 1995). Moreover, one of several themes to emerge from this body of research was the effects of raters’ implicit theories on ratings, including the tendency for raters to commit various rating errors including halo, leniency, and systematic distortion based on assumed rather than actual ratee behavior (Ostroff & Ilgen, 1992). Among other important findings, it was suggested that characteristics of the rater and ratee (e.g., sex, race, age, tenure, cognitive complexity, leadership style, rater–ratee relationship, and so on) activate inherent rater biases, leading to distorted ratings (Landy & Farr, 1980). Other findings emphasized that raters tend 173

Rick Jacobs, et al.

to utilize more global judgments of employees when they are aware of their need to provide a rating, but must reprocess information from an appraisal perspective (thereby potentially introducing bias) when they are not aware of their need to ultimately provide a rating (Hastie & Park, 1986). Despite such findings, much of the research on rater cognition has not been tested in the field, with the overwhelming majority of studies being experimental in nature and lacking all of the real-world cues that are part of performance assessment in ongoing organizations. In one of the few field studies to date conducted in the field and designed to evaluate processing issues, DeNisi and Peters (1996) found structured diary keeping (a method that allows appraisers to more effectively document employee performance information over time and makes such information more easily accessible to raters) lowered rating elevation (i.e., improved accuracy) and increased raters’ ability to discriminate between and within persons. This finding suggested that structure in the rating process is important for accurate recall of performance information. Unfortunately, the cognitive approach to performance appraisal failed to have lasting effects on informing practice. In fact, such research served to create a major schism between researchers and practitioners during the 1980s. While the cognitive revolution certainly struck a chord in researchers interested in unlocking the mysteries of raters’ thought processes when making performance ratings, those in the field were left wondering how such research informed the practice of performance appraisal in organizations. As a result of this division, the 1990s brought with it an emphasis on performance appraisal as embedded within the larger social-organizational context (Bretz, Milkovich, & Read, 1992; Ilgen, Barnes-Farrell, & McKellin, 1993). Specifically, Ilgen and colleagues (1993) suggested the appraisal process as entrenched within a rating environment or social milieu that raters and ratees occupy during the process. However, it was Murphy and Cleveland (1991, 1995) who brought considerations of the social-organizational context to the forefront of the performance appraisal discussion. In their seminal work, they argued for a social-psychological approach to performance appraisal, emphasizing a variety of contextual influences on performance ratings. Specifically, they suggested that ratings are a function of several different categories of environmental factors, including distal variables (e.g., organizational climate, culture, economic conditions, and so forth), process proximal variables (rater accountability, feedback environment, and so on), structural proximal variables (appraisal goals and purpose, appraisal training, and so on), and rater and ratee behavior (e.g., rater ratee attitudinal reactions, cognitive reactions, perceptions of justice, and so forth). As such, while cognitive research during the 1980s served to shed light on the limits of raters to make accurate ratings, the social-organizational approach expanded our view of the appraisal process by incorporating issues of rating context into the discussion. A brief review of the social-organizational literature suggests that while little systematic research has investigated the role of distal variables, a good deal of work has been done on the influence of proximal (both process and structural) variables (Levy & Williams, 2004). Levy and Williams (2004) refer to process proximal variables as those factors that have a “direct impact on how the appraisal process is conducted” (p. 885). With regard to raters, research demonstrates that rater–ratee similarity is related to performance ratings and specifically that positive affective regard for subordinates is related to higher ratings, a lower penchant to punish, more favorable leader-subordinate relationships, stronger halo effects, and decreased rating accuracy (Lefkowitz, 2000). Other research suggests that certain individual differences, and in particular high rater agreeableness and low conscientiousness, are associated with higher performance ratings (Bernardin, Cooke, & Villanova, 2000). Further, evidence suggests that attributions about ratee behavior (e.g., whether due to ability or effort) may influence raters’ reactions and ratings ( Johnson, Erez, Kiker, & Motowidlo, 2002), and the extent to which raters are held accountable for the ratings they provide have the potential to distort ratings— with more accountability leading to inflated ratings (Klimoski & Inks, 1990). On the ratee side, research has tended to focus on motivation and reactions to appraisal processes (Levy & Williams, 2004). In terms of motivation, while little support has been found for the effectiveness of merit pay systems (e.g., pay for performance, performance pay, etc.) (Campbell, 174

Appraising and Managing Officer Performance

Campbell, & Chia, 1998; Goss, 2001), it appears that participation in the appraisal process is critical to motivating employees, who must perceive the appraisal system as fair and ethical (Pettijohn, Pettijohn, & d’Amico, 2001; Roberts, 2003; Roberts & Reed, 1996; Shah & Murphy, 1995). With regard to ratee reactions, research has tended to focus on system and session satisfaction; perceived utility and accuracy; procedural, distributive, and interactional justice; feedback and acceptability; and due process (Erdogan, Kraimer, & Liden, 2001; Folger, Konovsky, & Cropanzano, 1992; Levy & Williams, 2004; Taylor, Masterson, Renard, & Tracy, 1998). One important piece of work by Folger and colleagues (1992), in particular, suggests that perceptions of fairness are achieved when adequate notice, fair hearing, and judgment based on evidence are present in the appraisal system. In general, it appears that fair appraisal systems lead to a wide array of positive rater and ratee outcomes, including less emotional exhaustion, increased ratee acceptance of feedback, more positive reactions to one’s supervisor and the organization, and more satisfaction with the appraisal system and the job by the rater and ratee (Brown & Benson, 2003; Flint, 1999; Leung, Su, & Morris, 2001; Levy & Williams, 1998; Taylor et al., 1998). Moreover, ratee reactions seem strongly tied to the opportunity to participate in the appraisal process and the amount of information provided about the process (Cawley, Keeping, & Levy, 1998; Levy & Williams, 1998; Williams & Levy, 2000). Research on ratee reactions to various appraisal structures and formats also suggests that ratees (and raters) react most favorably to behavior observation scales (BOS) in comparison to other scales—although these differences are sometimes quite small (Tziner & Kopelman, 2002; Tziner, Kopelman, & Joanis, 1997). Further, DeNisi and Peters (1996) suggest that raters react more positively to appraisal systems that employ performance diaries despite such techniques requiring more work. These positive reactions stem from the ability to better recall performance information and thus better differentiate between ratees. Other research suggests that aspects of the leader-member relationship may impact ratee evaluations. In particular, ratee trust, and specifically trust in one’s supervisor, seem particularly important in predicting both incumbents’ and supervisors’ acceptance of their appraisal and satisfaction with the appraisal system (Hedge & Teachout, 2000; Mani, 2002). Moreover, subordinates who are a part of a leader’s in group may be allocated higher performance ratings than those designated in the leader’s out group (Duarte, Goodson, & Klich, 1993; Kacmar, Witt, Zivnuska, & Gully, 2003). Further, research suggests that group and organizational dynamics may also impact performance ratings. Empirical evidence supports the effects of impression management on the appraisal process. For example, Gendersen and Tinsley (1996) found that assertive impression management techniques (e.g., ingratiation and self-promotion) led to higher performance ratings than defensive techniques (e.g., excuse making and justifications). Additionally, several authors have suggested the importance of a feedback culture (London, 2003; London & Smither, 2002) in which managers and employees feel comfortable both giving and receiving feedback. Feedback culture has been found to be related to satisfaction with feedback, motivation to use feedback, and feedback seeking (Norris-Watts & Levy, 2004; Steelman, Levy, & Snell, 2004). While process proximal variables directly impact both rater and ratee behavior during the appraisal process, structural proximal variables refer to system elements that comprise the organization or design of the performance management process such as the types and number of performance dimensions, the frequency of appraisals, and the purpose of the appraisal (Levy & Williams, 2004). For example, preliminary evidence on multisource feedback systems suggests that rater and ratee acceptance of such feedback and other positive reactions depend on the organizational culture, credibility of raters, high rates of participation, anonymity of ratings, knowledge of and experience with the multisource system, social support, and individual self-efficacy (Maurer, Mitchell, & Barbeite, 2002; Waldman & Bowen, 1998; Williams & Lueke, 1999). Moreover, inconclusive evidence remains as to whether such systems lead to performance improvements (Kluger & DeNisi, 1996; Seifert, Yuki, & McDonald, 2003). 175

Rick Jacobs, et al.

Additionally, many researchers continue to investigate how ratings are influenced by the purpose of the appraisal. Consistent with Taylor and Wherry’s (1951) original hypotheses, Jawahar and Williams (1997), in their review of 22 studies on the effects of rating purpose, found that ratings that were made for administrative purposes were more lenient than those made for research or developmental purposes. Finally, a considerable amount of work has addressed the role of rater training in the appraisal process (Bernardin & Buckley, 1981; Pulakos, 1984). Woehr and Huffcutt (1994) conducted a meta-analysis, which provided support for frame-of-reference (FOR) training (a method that seeks to standardize raters’ perceptions of performance by facilitating agreement on what specific behaviors define the spectrum of performance), and subsequent research has corroborated the efficacy of this form of training (Sulsky & Day, 1992; Sulsky & Keown, 1998; Sulsky, Skarlicki, & Keown, 2002). No doubt the performance appraisal literature has come a long way in the past 60 years. What began with research primarily dedicated to measurement issues and the search for the perfect rating format was accompanied by two subsequent shifts in the research literature, the cognitive approach of the 1980s and the social-organizational perspective of the 1990s. Moreover, this rich research history has provided us with a strong empirical framework for designing performance appraisal systems today. Given this historical backdrop, in the next section, we discuss specifically those system elements that lead to effective programs, where programs can get off track, and evidence for performance improvement as a result of such systems.

Implementing Performance Appraisal Programs Setting the Stage As discussed in the previous section, defining which competencies are required to effectively perform as a police officer is the first step in providing a framework for a useful performance appraisal system. However, once these competencies are in place, the question remains of how to best create a system which accurately measures these competencies. A variety of aspects can be considered when implementing a performance appraisal system. From training the raters to using the resulting data to make administrative decisions, many pieces of the performance appraisal puzzle must be in place to ensure success. Although often overlooked, some of the most important parts of the performance appraisal process take place even before the system is put in motion. Before beginning to use any performance appraisal system, departments need to be sure that officers are aware of the competencies evaluated, how they are linked to the job, and why they are important. As referenced previously, this method of performance appraisal implementation is called due process (Folger et al., 1992). Due process refers to a lifecycle approach to performance appraisal—one in which officers are informed from the start which competencies are important in defining job performance and which behaviors are viewed as effective and which are seen as ineffective. As such, officers are provided with a clear understanding of how performance ratings will be derived (Folger et al., 1992). Ensuring that officers are on board with the way in which performance is defined, as well as the key features of the system, will result in an environment of trust surrounding appraisal, as opposed to suspicion (Farr & Jacobs, 2006). One common finding from both research and practice is that employees do not like being evaluated. Thus, creating clear guidelines for performance from the start allows organizations to avoid the potential for surprise during the evaluation process and enhances the overall perceptions of system fairness. Latham, Almost, Mann, and Moore (2005) also note that performance appraisal should be based on a sound job analysis, along with a behaviorally based criterion to ensure that employees are evaluated on the core tenets of the job in an objective and fair manner. Rosen, Levy, and Hall (2006) reiterate this point by demonstrating that employees who perceive political motives underlying the performance appraisal process were likely to have lower job satisfaction and affective commitment, 176

Appraising and Managing Officer Performance

which were both found to be negatively related to job performance. Therefore, ensuring that the performance appraisal process is perceived as valid and worthwhile by employees is important for maintaining a productive and satisfied workforce. Table 8.1 reflects a list of areas of responsibility for police officers and the basics on which a system can be built for evaluating the performance of police officers. As can be seen, the areas of performance are specific and clearly a part of a police officer’s

Table 8.1 List of Performance Evaluation Dimensions for Police Officer Assessment A. B.

C. D.

E.

F. G. H. I.

J. K.

L.

M.

N. O. P.

Q.

Patrol Preparation and Relief: Activities performed at the beginning and end of a shift during shift changes. This includes safeguarding and accounting for all agency property and equipment. Patrol/Guard Duties and Responsibilities: Activities involving observing assigned area to detect unusual activities or violations of the law and preserving the safety of individuals and security of property in assigned area. Vehicle Enforcement and Control: Activities involving the safe and legal operation of vehicles. This includes enforcing laws, handling accidents, assisting accident victims, and eliminating public safety hazards. Responding to Crimes and/or Disturbances: Activities performed while responding to all types of offenses, including crimes in progress, civil matters and domestic disputes, property crimes, serious crimes, and so on. This includes the initial investigation of crimes and/or disturbances. Apprehension and Control of Suspects, Prisoners/Inmates: Activities involving pursuit, isolation, containment, apprehension, and control of suspects or prisoners/inmates. This also includes activities performed in the process of searching to locate suspects, prisoners/inmates, or missing persons. Search: Activities involving searches of suspects, prisoners/inmates, or crime scene/facility to locate physical evidence or contraband. Arrest and Detention: Activities involving arrest processing procedures, transporting, detention, and lodging of prisoners/inmates or juveniles. Evidence/Property: Activities involving the safeguarding, storing, and accounting for evidence and nonagency property. Emergency Situations: Activities involving responding to emergency situations (e.g., natural disasters, riots, hostage situations, fires, escapes, floods/high water, and so forth), maintaining order, and notifying appropriate agencies to secure assistance. Care of Victim, Prison/Inmate Welfare: Activities involving attending to medical and emotional needs of victims, agency personnel, other citizens, or prisoners/inmates. Inmate Supervision: Activities involving supervising or controlling inmates during daily activities to maintain safety and security of inmates and staff. This includes communicating with inmates, overseeing the visitation of inmates, and maintaining a clean and safe environment during work, meals, showers, and so on. Notifications/Communications/Administration: Activities involving distribution of information regarding incidents and conditions. This also includes communications with superiors, other personnel, and individuals from other agencies to coordinate activities. Reports, Forms, and Memo Books: Activities involving the preparation of written forms, reports, photo/video records, or memo books. Forms and reports may be of a variety of types such as captioned (fill-in-the-blank), written narrative, or a combination of these types. Court Activities: Activities involving providing assistance with court procedures including appearing in court and presenting testimony. Public Interaction: Activities involving interactions with members of the public. This includes answering questions about community conditions and problems. Agency Policies, Procedures, Rules, and Laws: Activities involving keeping current with agency policies and rules, and city, state, and federal laws that govern the activities of officers and applying/ enforcing them correctly. Professional Development: Activities performed to improve one’s skills or to improve the agency. This includes participating in specific training or education and participating in professional organizations or conferences.

177

Rick Jacobs, et al.

job. The performance evaluation process is further enhanced by specific performance assessment scales for each of the areas. Table 8.2 is a similar list but in this case represents a police supervisor/ sergeant. Again, these types of definitions can be used to define scales for rating performance. Scales based on this type of approach are available by contacting the senior author of this chapter.

Table 8.2 Performance Evaluation Dimensions for Police Sergeant I.  Supervision of Field/Case Work A. Incident Supervision: This duty involves assuming responsibility for incidents or special operations to ensure the efficient and safe handling of the incident/operation and the preservation of life and property. B. Case Management: This duty involves assuming responsibility for investigations coming under the sergeant’s supervision to ensure the productive and appropriate handling of all investigative matters and to preserve the integrity of these investigations. II.  Personnel Management C. Personnel Assignment and Coordination: This duty involves assigning or allocating personnel so as to ensure sufficient personnel resources are available to handle the workload. This area also involves planning and preparing for special circumstances such as sporting events and emergencies. D. Personnel Evaluation: This duty involves observing subordinate performance to identify strengths and areas of needed improvement. It also includes conducting formal performance evaluation sessions and informal counseling sessions with subordinates to recognize performance strengths and discuss and resolve performance problems. E. Policy Implementation and Performance: This duty involves interpreting, enforcing, and explaining the rules and regulations that govern the activities of department personnel. F. Training: This duty involves ensuring that personnel are properly trained to carry out their assigned duties. This includes planning, developing, conducting, monitoring, and evaluating formal and informal training programs. The training context can be the academy or the field. The trainees may be department personnel at any level, personnel from other agencies, or civilians. III.  Administrative Activities G. Record and Report Management: This duty involves reviewing, preparing, and/or maintaining logs, records, forms, memos, reports, and other field and administrative documents and correspondence used in the course of performing the job. This includes reviewing documents prepared by subordinate or other personnel for completeness and accuracy, reviewing and authorizing personnel requests, and integrating information from multiple sources into summary documents. H. Internal/External Communication and Coordination: This duty involves communicating or coordinating activities with department personnel and individuals from other agencies to accomplish work objectives and discuss issues of mutual concern. I. General Administration: This duty involves participating in the development, implementation, and evaluation of division/department programs, policies, procedures, and objectives. IV.  Hands-On Field/Case Work J. Traffic Observation, Enforcement, and Control: This duty involves all activities conducted to ensure the safe and legal operation of vehicles on the road. This includes performing or supervising others in performing tasks such as assisting motorists, enforcing traffic laws, handling accidents, assisting accident victims, and identifying and eliminating public safety hazards. K. Responding to Crimes and Disturbances: This duty involves the activities performed while responding to all types of offenses including crimes in progress, civil matters and domestic disputes, property crimes, serious crimes, and so on. L. Investigation: This duty involves all activities performed as part of internal or criminal investigations. This includes performing or supervising others in performing tasks such as obtaining and analyzing information, collecting physical evidence, and participating in the judicial/administrative process.

178

Appraising and Managing Officer Performance

M.

Arrest-Related Activities: This duty involves activities performed for the purpose of apprehending, restraining, arresting, transporting, and detaining suspects to be taken into custody according to the law and department guidelines. It also includes activities performed or supervised in the process of searching vehicles, persons, and/or premises for weapons, fruits of a crime, or contraband in order to effect arrest, protect self and the public, and/or obtain evidence.

V.  Public and Community Relations N. Public and Community Relations: This duty involves activities that have an impact on the department’s image in the community by virtue of the subordinate’s interactions with individual members of the public, community organizations, and the media. VI.  Professional Development O. Professional Development: This duty involves participating in activities to keep apprised of jobrelated developments in laws, technology, policies, and procedures as well as to enhance job-related knowledge, skills, and abilities. This can include reading internal memos and bulletins; reading external publications; participating in training or certification drills and classes; and/or attending outside conferences, seminars, and courses.

In addition to presenting employees with clear and relevant criteria, it should also be noted that it is possible for employees to voice their opinion if a part of the system appears unfair or unclear. The ability to participate in the appraisal process by respectfully questioning the system is also a part of due process (Folger et al., 1992). In line with this idea, Cawley and colleagues (1998) empirically demonstrated the effects of employee participation in the appraisal process. The authors found that employees had more favorable reactions to a performance appraisal system in which they could voice their opinion about the accuracy of their performance ratings and the system in general, regardless of whether or not their feedback would have an impact on organizational practice in the future. Thus, it is clear that employees are more comfortable with appraisal when they feel they have the opportunity to provide input about the system. Kozlowski, Chao, and Morrison (1998) recommend that, in addition to clear and relevant criteria, appraisals should be frequent and appropriate. Therefore, it may be useful to determine up front how often appraisals will take place and to consider how these appraisals align with organizational activities and strategy. Providing employees with a clear idea of what the performance criteria are, why they are important to the job, how often they will be assessed, and how assessment aligns with organizational strategy will all help to set the stage for a smooth and effective performance appraisal process.

Training Raters Once the performance appraisal system has been rolled out, it is important to identify those individuals who will be responsible for providing ratings and to properly train them to use the system successfully. As discussed previously, raters without proper training may fall prey to a variety of rating biases such as halo (employees are either all good or all bad) or leniency (every employee is great) (Ostroff & Ilgen, 1992). Further, raters may pursue personal goals when providing ratings for their officers (e.g., providing low ratings to an exemplary officer as a motivator for even better performance or providing high ratings to a poor performing officer to get them out of a particular unit and under someone else’s command) (Murphy & Cleveland, 1995). Further, Wong and Kwong (2007) tested these assumptions empirically, demonstrating that in a sample of students, those who were given the goals of promoting harmony and fairness in their ratings tended to provide less discriminating ratings between candidates and higher ratings for candidates in general. This provides 179

Rick Jacobs, et al.

support for the notion that when raters have a goal in mind, they can paint a picture of performance in alignment with that goal but not necessarily consistent with what they observe. In the end, a system is only as good as the people who use it. Thus, it is completely possible for an organization to have a fantastic performance appraisal system in place and still achieve mediocre or even disastrous results. As such, it is helpful to view the performance appraisal system as a vehicle: all of the parts may be in place but, without a properly trained driver and periodic servicing, the car does not function properly and is at risk for a crash. However, providing rater training is clearly a key component of a successful performance appraisal process. One type of rater training has been touted as particularly effective in driving an accurate performance system: Frame of Reference (FOR) training (Schleicher, Day, Mayes, & Riggio, 2002; Woehr, 1994). FOR training consists of a variety of components (as outlined in Schleicher et al., 2002), which all help to make certain that raters will be able to accurately assess officer behavior. The first step in FOR training is to communicate the important dimensions of the job as well as the behaviors that comprise effective performance for each role. This is clearly important because raters need to be aware of what performance means, as well as what it looks like when an officer is performing well. What is important to note here is that many systems carry with them an implicit assumption when rater training is minimal or nonexistent (as is often the case) that since a supervisor/sergeant was once a police officer, he or she knows about performance and can appropriately observe and rate the performance of others. Everything we have discussed up to this point shows that, in many cases, this assumption is simply not true. Performance appraisal training helps create a unified definition of performance across raters so that officers will receive similar ratings regardless of their rater. The second step of FOR training is to allow raters to discuss which behaviors are indicative of each level of performance. For example, while it may be useful to know that police officers should call for help when arriving to the scene of an armed robbery, it is also useful to know that high-performing officers call for help on the way to the scene, while mid-range performers would call within 10 seconds of arrival, and low performers would call within 30 seconds of arrival or whenever they find it convenient. This implies that raters should be aware of not only what good performance entails but also what this means when determining whether the behavior of an employee is high, medium, or low for a particular competency. Finally, raters should be able to provide practice evaluations and discuss these evaluations with one another in order to calibrate ratings. This final step gives raters the opportunity to practice assessing behavior and then to compare ratings with other raters in order to provide a consistent and clear basis for which raters should be formulating performance ratings. After completing all of the steps of FOR training, raters should be ready to use the system properly, resulting in ratings as precise and consistent as possible. One important point to take away from this is that training raters can only help. Raters who can effectively evaluate officers will gain additional insights from the training and will find the information reinforcing, while those officers not quite as accomplished as performance evaluators will learn important skills and gain confidence in their ability to do things properly. The effects of FOR training have been empirically demonstrated as well. Woehr and Huffcutt (1994) found an effect size of .83 on rating accuracy for FOR training when compared to either a control group or a no training group. Day and Sulsky (1995) also demonstrated that FOR training had a significant main effect on performance rating accuracy. Further, Schleicher and associates (2002) empirically demonstrated that FOR training improved the reliability, accuracy, and discriminant validity of ratings in an assessment center. In addition, FOR training significantly improved the criterion-related validity of the assessment center for predicting supervisors’ ratings of job performance (Schleicher et al., 2002). Thus, FOR training is an extremely valuable tool for increasing the validity and predictive capabilities of performance ratings. This type of training should, therefore, be considered best practice in preparing raters for providing performance ratings for employees. Systems that fail to provide rater training will be far less effective. 180

Appraising and Managing Officer Performance

Monitoring the Rating Process After raters are trained, they are ready to begin conducting appraisals and the department must be prepared to effectively store, evaluate, and provide feedback on the rating process. As one can see, the performance management process does not begin and end with the rating of officer performance; it is far more involved. Performance appraisal systems must be monitored throughout the lifecycle to ensure they are used properly. Not only must raters be retrained as time passes, but also the system must be evaluated to arrive at correct conclusions about what is needed for the system to function efficiently. In other words, no performance appraisal and management process is perfect from the beginning, and systematic evaluation and updating are critical to meet the needs of the department. For example, even if raters are trained, they might find it difficult to keep track of performance appraisal information over time, particularly if they supervise many employees. Thus, it may be necessary to implement a structured way for raters to keep track of performance information. Structured diary keeping may provide a way for raters to continue making the best use of a performance system by offering a coherent framework within which to organize employee performance data. It has been demonstrated that those who used structured diaries (diaries that keep track of how the employee is performing on a day-to-day basis and are kept for future reference) had more positive reactions to the appraisal system, recalled performance information more effectively, and produced less biased and more discriminating ratings—both between and within employees; DeNisi et al., 1989; DeNisi & Peters, 1996. This may also help to alleviate issues that arise when raters make judgments based on recent performance, discounting prior performance. For example, Reb and Cropanzano (2007) demonstrated in a lab study that raters gave higher ratings to those with increasing performance than those who had consistent or decreasing performance, regardless of whether or not the ending point was the same. The same was true for decreasing performance, which resulted in lower ratings than consistently poor performance. In this case, structured diary keeping may help raters to recall a more accurate portrait of performance—one that is dynamic and takes the entire spectrum of performance into account. Thus, if ratings appear inflated, if there are only fine distinctions made between employees, or if supervisors are having difficulty taking into account the full range of performance, organizations may want to consider implementing a structured diary technique. In addition, it is possible that performance data are biased in other ways, namely, that the data are results in group differences for protected classes. For example, Stauffer and Buckley (2005) reexamined Sackett and Dubois (1991), finding that both black and white raters give higher ratings to white ratees, reporting similar results. However, in both papers, the gap between ratings for white and black ratees was much higher for white raters than for black raters, creating a larger advantage for white ratees when the rater is white than when the rater is black. Thus, it is possible that racial differences may be found in performance appraisal ratings. Similarly, Lyness and Heilman (2006) found that female managers in masculine-stereotyped jobs (line manager) were rated lower than female managers in female-stereotyped jobs (staff jobs) (both groups were rated lower than male managers in both line and staff jobs). Further, women who had been promoted received higher performance ratings than men who had been promoted, demonstrating that women had to be clearly better than their counterparts to make it to the next level. Thus, because the risk for group differences exists, it is in the organization’s best interest to monitor resultant ratings for bias. Although group differences do not necessarily imply foul play, they should be made subject to inquiry and examination. The possibility that ratings could have an adverse impact is another reason to use a valid and well-researched performance appraisal system. If ratings result in bias, but the system is valid and job related, the organization can defend itself. Without a well-founded system, however, the organization is subject to litigation and in the current environment organizations must be cognizant of this possibility. 181

Rick Jacobs, et al.

Utilizing the Performance Evaluation Data Once a system is in place, raters have been trained, and the system is consistently monitored, the next step is to use the ratings properly. Ratings are normally used for two purposes: (1) to make administrative decisions, such as firing or promotion or salary adjustments, and/or (2) for development, through developmental feedback and the potential to create an action plan for the future. In most police agencies, performance assessment is an annual event that has little impact on salary, promotion opportunity, or developmental planning. Most often, it is an event that officers and supervisors are required to conduct and only in rare instances does it lead to anything more than documenting poor performance for a few officers who, after several years of repeated poor performance, might be transferred or terminated. That is an unfortunate lack of use of what can be extremely important information. It is our belief that a culture must be created that focuses on the potential benefits of a strong performance appraisal and management process. Key to creating this culture is a clear statement of how performance evaluation information will be used and what systems will be in place to act on the results. The way in which data is utilized can actually have an effect on the ratings themselves. For example, Greguras, Robie, Schleicher, and Goff (2003) found that subordinate ratings are of better quality when they are used for developmental purposes. This suggests that subordinates may not feel comfortable providing accurate ratings for their supervisor when it is clear that their ratings may have an impact on job outcomes. Further, it suggests that subordinates may fear the consequences of painting an accurate picture of their supervisors’ behavior. In the same study, however, peer ratings were of good quality when used for administrative or for developmental purposes (Greguras et al., 2003). Atkins and Wood (2002) also note that the best predictor of assessment center performance is the average of peer, supervisor, subordinate, and self-ratings. This may demonstrate that different members of the organization may be exposed to different aspects of an employee or, on the other hand, it may point to the unwillingness of various employee contacts to share certain performance information. Thus, it is important to think about the purpose of the rating, as well as the source of the rating, when determining how best to use performance data. These findings may also suggest that organizations may want to keep the two processes separate according to their final use (Murphy & Cleveland, 1995). Murphy and Cleveland (1995) offer three best practices for the use of performance information. The authors note that it may be necessary to change the underlying value of and rewards linked to the performance appraisal system, as opposed to attempting to coerce raters to avoid bias. Thus, it is more important to convince raters that rating is personally instrumental and helpful to the organization at the same time, rather than pushing for accuracy without a clear reason why. In order to do this, Murphy and Cleveland (1995) first suggest that rewards should be tied to rating behavior. In order to increase rating accuracy, organizations should demonstrate that they are willing to reward proper rating behavior. If the organization demonstrates that accurate rating is important through the use of rewards, then raters will be more likely to provide accurate ratings. Further, if raters know that the organization is serious about rating, they may be less likely to treat rating like a chore. Second, Murphy and Cleveland (1995) suggest that making sure the effects of negative ratings are reduced may make it more likely that accurate ratings will be provided. For example, if a rater knows that a poor rating will result in job loss for the ratee, he or she may be unwilling to provide negative information. While meta-analyses have shown that pay-for-performance increases performance, Murphy and Cleveland (1995) suggest that this strategy may take a negative toll on performance appraisal accuracy. This suggestion may be especially helpful when subordinates provide ratings for their supervisors, given that they may fear backlash from supervisors if the rating is negative (Greguras et al., 2003). Thus, it is helpful to create a system in which performance ratings are not directly linked to negative outcomes for ratees. 182

Appraising and Managing Officer Performance

Finally, Murphy and Cleveland (1995) suggest that, while performance ratings should not be directly tied to negative sanction for ratees, raters should see a clear link between rating behavior and rewards. Making the process more visible creates a motivational environment for rating accuracy, similar to the positive effects of pay for performance that Rynes, Gerhart, and Parks (2005) discussed in their meta-analysis. Taken together, organizations should make sure to create a reward structure for raters, with clear links to benefits as the result of accurate rating behavior. Further, organizations should make the links between ratings and negative sanctions for ratees less clear, so that raters do not feel personally responsible for the downfall of fellow employees. What all this points to is the promotion of a culture in which individuals understand how performance information will be used, are motivated to positively participate in the system, and trust the system to benefit the organization and the employees that comprise it. One of the primary ways raters can use performance information, moreover, is to provide officers with feedback about their performance. Feedback is often misused and improperly delivered, creating anxiety and lower levels of motivation in ratees. In the next section, proper feedback delivery and environment will be discussed, in order to ensure that organizations are making the best possible use of performance information. As should be obvious, any rater training process must include information on the proper conduct of the feedback process.

Feedback in Performance Appraisal Reference different fields of study, and you will find various ways in which the term feedback has been conceptualized and defined. Industrial-organizational psychologists generally refer to feedback as information provided to an individual that clarifies expectations of their job and alerts them to how well their current performance meets such expectations (Spector, 2006). Although it was long assumed that employee feedback resulted in positive gains in performance, a meta-analysis by Kluger and DeNisi (1996) exposed the reality that feedback has the potential to produce not only performance gains but also performance declines. Indeed, as Taylor, Fisher, and Ilgen (1984) note, “Feedback may have no impact on the recipient at all, it may cause the individual to lash out angrily, or it may result in a response quite different from that desired by the source” (p. 82). Ilgen, Fisher, and Taylor (1979) suggested that variability in response to feedback might be due to a series of psychological or cognitive processes elicited by the feedback process. As such, many studies have sought to determine what characteristics of the feedback process contribute to its effectiveness or lack thereof (Fedor, Rensvold, & Adams, 1992; Gaddis, Connelly, & Mumford, 2004; Kluger & DeNisi, 1996; Moss, Valenzi, & Taggart, 2003). Before considering such characteristics, it is important to understand that the effectiveness of any feedback system hinges on recipients’ acceptance of such feedback (Ilgen et al., 1979). Ilgen and colleagues (1979) defined feedback acceptance as “the recipient’s belief that the feedback is an accurate portrayal of his or her performance” (Ilgen et al., 1979). Further, research suggests that feedback acceptance acts as a gatekeeper or moderator between feedback delivery and effectiveness (Anseel & Lievens, 2007). Using self-consistency theory, Korman (1970) argued that “individuals will be motivated to perform on a task or job in a manner which is consistent with the self-image with which they approach the task or job situation” (p. 32). Thus, one’s response to performance feedback is expected to be congruent to his or her acceptance of such feedback. As such, positive or negative feedback, which is perceived as accurate and therefore accepted by the recipient, is more likely to motivate one to respond than feedback perceived as inaccurate (Kinicki, Prussia, Wu, & McKee-Ryan, 2004). In support of this proposition, Brett and Atwater (2001) found that perceptions of feedback accuracy from peers and direct reports were positively related to individuals’ reactions to feedback. Moreover, Kinicki and colleagues (2004) tested a path model, which found that perceived accuracy was positively associated with recipients’ desire to respond to feedback. Additionally, other findings 183

Rick Jacobs, et al.

suggest that positive reactions to feedback have significant links with other important organizational outcomes. Jawahar (2006), for instance, found that satisfaction with feedback was positively related to organizational commitment, job satisfaction, and commitment toward and satisfaction with one’s managers, and negatively related to turnover intentions. As such, it seems clear that before any performance changes can occur, feedback recipients must decide whether to lend any credence to the feedback they have been given (Ashford, 1986). Given the crucial role that feedback acceptance appears to play in the feedback delivery–performance relationship, what factors might predispose recipients from perceiving feedback as accurate and acceptable, and what can organizations do to foster such acceptance? In the following sections, we consider the effects of feedback content, specificity, frequency, delivery, and source on individuals’ willingness to accept and utilize performance feedback. Moreover, we explore the role of trust in the feedback process and discuss how organizations can create environments conducive to feedback.

Feedback Content Given the sensitive and anxiety-inducing nature of feedback in organizations (Cleveland et al., 2007), it is no surprise that the content of feedback may have a substantial impact on the likelihood of feedback acceptance. In general, research suggests that individuals are more accepting of positive feedback (Brett & Atwater, 2001), which likely stems from the motivation to preserve one’s self-esteem from the perceived failure resulting from negative feedback (Anseel & Lievens, 2007). More precisely, it has been suggested that individuals are more likely to reject feedback that results in a negative sign (Ilgen et al., 1979), which refers to performance discrepancies in which one’s actual performance, relayed through feedback, is lower than one’s perceived performance (perceived overestimation of performance). In contrast, positive signs refer to discrepancies in which actual performance is higher than perceived performance (perceived underestimation of performance). These findings do not necessarily indicate that individuals wholly reject negative feedback. They do indicate, however, that when performance discrepancies exist, individuals prefer positive feedback signs that portray them in a positive light and confirm their own self-perceptions (Aronson, Wilson, & Akert, 2007; Nease, Mudgett, & Quiñones, 1999). Moreover, they suggest that feedback providers ought to balance their negative feedback with positive feedback in order to reduce perceived performance overestimations as much as possible. By narrowing the perceived gap between one’s actual performance and perceived performance, feedback providers may be more likely to gain feedback acceptance, which in turn may lead to performance improvements on the part of the feedback recipient. Additionally, Smither and Walker (2004) showed that providing a small amount of negative feedback is related to enhanced performance, but that a large amount of negative feedback impairs future performance. Atwater and Brett (2006) further suggest that feedback sessions should begin with positive feedback in order to spur feedback acceptance. Thus, feedback providers should be aware of the amount of negative feedback they provide, making sure to buffer it with positive feedback at the beginning of the feedback session and ensuring that subordinates are not overwhelmed by too much negative feedback during the process. Other research indicates that feedback is perceived as valuable when it helps employees reduce uncertainty, provides recipients information regarding their goal progress, and indicates how one’s performance is evaluated by others (Ashford & Cummings, 1983; Steelman & Rutkowski, 2004). Ilgen and associates (1979) further suggested that feedback is more likely to be accepted and used when it is perceived as valid, accurate, and reliable. Thus, feedback providers should always err on the side of providing higher quality feedback during feedback sessions (Ilgen et al., 1979). Kluger and DeNisi (1996), however, note that such feedback ought to be focused more on the performed task than the person receiving the feedback. In their influential meta-analysis, they found that feedback intervention (FI) effectiveness decreased as feedback became more directed toward the 184

Appraising and Managing Officer Performance

feedback recipient than the task. This is consistent with other research that supports the idea that individuals are more likely to accept feedback that compares them to a neutral standard, rather than to their peers. It is believed that such comparisons can hurt recipients’ self-concept and reduce their likelihood of accepting feedback (Atwater & Brett, 2006) as well as focus attention on recipients’ weaknesses—potentially eliciting negative emotional reactions instead of internalization of feedback (Smither & Walker, 2004). This suggests that recipients may be less likely to accept and ultimately use feedback they perceive as a personal attack rather than an honest assessment of performance on the task. Thus, feedback providers should be careful that their feedback not be directed toward personal characteristics or compare recipients to their peers, but rather should focus on specific performance aspects related to the task.

Feedback Specificity To date, feedback specificity, or the level of information presented in feedback messages, has received quite a bit of attention in the performance appraisal literature (Annett, 1969; Goldstein, Emanuel, & Howell, 1968). Generally speaking, it is believed that effective feedback includes specific information about behaviors performed incorrectly and how to correct them, rather than generalized or ambiguous statements (Bernardin & Beatty, 1984; Goodman, Wood, & Hendickx, 2004; Kirk & MacDonald, 1989). Such feedback provides a rich source of information about explicit behaviors that are appropriate or inappropriate for effective performance, thereby allowing recipients to learn from and correct their behavior (Adams, 1987; Anderson, 1982; Annet, 1969; Ilgen et al., 1979). In so doing, feedback specificity is thought to decrease information-processing activities, such as error diagnosis, encoding, and retrieval (Christina & Bjork, 1991; Schmidt, 1991), which subsequently reduce the cognitive load required to make links between actions and outcomes (Goodman & Wood, 2004). Moreover, specific feedback provides information on how individuals are progressing toward their goals and informs them about how their job performance is evaluated (Ashford & Cummings, 1983). In fact, empirical evidence suggests that specific, objective feedback consistent with actual performance leads to higher performance than less specific, more subjective feedback (Kopelman, 1986). Moreover, feedback interventions that are supplemented with information regarding tasks, strategies, and appropriate behaviors have been found to result in increased short-term performance during practice and training (Goodman & Wood, 2004; Kluger & DeNisi, 1996). In an interesting study conducted by Steelman and Rutkowski (2004), the authors found that feedback quality moderated the relationship between unfavorable feedback and motivation to use feedback. This finding suggests that employees are more highly motivated to use negative feedback when that feedback is perceived to be of high quality and directed toward improving their performance than similar feedback of lower quality. Despite such findings, overly specific narrative feedback may also introduce excessive cognitive load on recipients, hindering their understanding of the feedback (Atkins, Wood, & Rutgers, 2002). For example, Atkins et al. (2002) conducted an experiment that found that participants who received feedback in a graphical format outperformed participants who received feedback in a tabular format on an inventory management task. Findings indicated that the portrayal of relationships between variables visually in the graphical format facilitated quicker decision making than the more detailed derivation of relationships from data provided in the tabular format. Interestingly, however, the tabular format produced the strongest evidence of learning. Atkins et al. (2002) reasoned that while graphical feedback formats may reduce processing and interpretation costs inherent to tabular formats, it is the lack of such processing and detailed analyses that handicap learning—which is critical for long-term performance improvement and development. In a similar vein, Goodman and Wood (2004) further suggested that while feedback specificity is generally advantageous for improving immediate performance, it may undermine certain aspects 185

Rick Jacobs, et al.

of the learning process necessary for independent performance at a later time. In particular, they found that specific feedback improved recipients’ ability to learn from good performance but produced deleterious effects on learning how to respond to poor performance. They reasoned that it is important that feedback providers consider what exactly they want recipients to learn with regard to the task. That is, if the feedback provider wants the recipient to learn how to respond to both good and poor performance, feedback must be supported by sufficient opportunities for the recipient to test and learn the local response rules for different aspects of a task. For example, if we wanted a car mechanic to learn how to respond when a carburetor functions properly and when it malfunctions, feedback should not be so specific that it ensures the carburetor functions properly. Rather, feedback should be less specific so that recipients commit errors and learn which behaviors will lead to proper carburetor functioning. In general, these findings suggest that specific feedback is more beneficial than vague, generalized feedback. Employees must learn which specific behaviors are indicative of good and poor performance, not only so that they can correct such behaviors but also in order to protect against perceptions of arbitrariness in the feedback process. However, it also important that feedback providers consider what they want recipients to learn regarding the task. In order to promote long-term learning, less specific feedback may be more beneficial to promoting experimentation and richer, more effective response patterns on the part of employees. In fact, research on error management training (EMT), a method that involves active exploration and explicit encouragement for trainees to commit errors in order to promote learning, suggests increased training transfer on novel tasks (Keith & Frese, 2008).

Feedback Frequency Feedback frequency has also been explored as a potential factor influencing feedback acceptance and perceived utility (Fedor & Buckley, 1987; Fulk, Brief, & Barr, 1985; Landy, Barnes, & Murphy, 1978). In general, it is thought that frequently delivered feedback is more effective in promoting employee performance improvements than less frequent feedback (Fedor & Buckley, 1987; Ilgen et al., 1979). In a review of the PA literature, Ilgen et al. (1979) concluded that the close temporal pairing of feedback with employee performance behavior has an overall positive effect on future performance. They reasoned that frequently delivered feedback reduces the chances that interference will occur in the periods between behavior and the feedback, which has the potential to distort feedback effectiveness. In fact, research suggests that frequency of feedback is positively related to recipients’ perceived accuracy and fairness of feedback (Fulk et al. 1985; Landy et al., 1978). Because feedback tends to not be delivered as frequently as it should in today’s organizations, recipients may view feedback as a scarce, valuable resource that decreases role ambiguity by continually informing employees about where they stand with regard to their performance levels (Beehr & Love, 1983; Fedor & Buckley, 1987). Thus, increasing the frequency of feedback may facilitate more positive employee reactions and outcomes during the feedback process. However, Fedor and Buckley (1987) warn that more may not always be better when it comes to feedback, and that other factors must be taken into consideration when deciding whether to provide more frequent feedback or not. For example, instituting more frequent feedback sessions may send implicit messages to employees that supervisors do not trust them as competent and capable of carrying out their tasks without increased micromanagement. Moreover, Fedor and Buckley (1987) suggest that because negative feedback is typically perceived as less accurate and more controlling, providing such feedback more regularly may in fact lead to decreased feedback acceptance. In fact, it has been shown that while positive feedback provided immediately promotes a strengthening of previous behavioral responses, negative feedback is more effective after a time lag, which allows for decomposition in the strength of the previous incorrect behavioral response (Buchwals & Meager, 1974; Fedor & Buckley, 1987). As such, it stands 186

Appraising and Managing Officer Performance

to reason that while providing feedback on how to do a job may be perceived positively when done once or twice, such feedback may be reacted to more negatively when delivered in excess (Fedor & Buckley, 1987). Similar to the case of feedback specificity, it may also be the case that more frequent feedback is not necessarily conducive to long-term sustained maintenance of learned behavior (Fedor & Buckley, 1987). Research on error management training (EMT) suggests that individuals must engage in active exploration of a task and make consistent errors in order to promote a long-term structured understanding of how to perform it well (Keith & Frese, 2008). As such, these findings suggest that while supervisors should seek to provide regular feedback to employees, they should also recognize that more is not always better. Specifically, because recipients’ feedback perceptions are crucial to its acceptance, supervisors should be aware that the intent behind providing increased amounts of feedback may not be perceived accurately by those receiving such feedback, that is, what may be intended as supportive may in actuality be perceived as controlling (Fedor & Buckley, 1987). Moreover, too frequent feedback may hinder long-term learning, an outcome all managers want to avoid. As such, Fedor and Buckley (1987) recommend that supervisors find out how often their employees desire feedback, while also minimizing the perceived costs of asking for feedback.

Feedback Delivery It is also important that feedback providers take active steps to preserve and respect recipients’ selfesteem by being sensitive and understanding in order to obtain feedback acceptance, rather than simply viewing their role as a provider of information. Research suggests that the manner in which feedback is delivered may significantly impact employees’ acceptance and perceived utility of the feedback (Steelman & Rutkowski, 2004). For example, it has been found that a supportive, constructive attitude on the part of feedback providers contributes to increased recipient satisfaction, perceptions of fairness, and motivation to improve job performance (Burke et al., 1978). Furthermore, feedback consisting of greater interpersonal fairness has been found to be related to more favorable attributions toward the feedback provider, more acceptance of the feedback provider, and more favorable reactions to the organization (Leung et al., 2001). Steelman and Rutkowski (2004), for example, found that employees were more motivated to improve their job performance following negative feedback when feedback is delivered in a considerate manner.

Feedback Source Not surprisingly, much research has also investigated whether feedback acceptance depends on who provides such feedback. This research has often investigated the feedback source’s credibility, which refers to one’s expertise and trustworthiness in providing feedback (Giffin, 1967; Kinicki et al., 2004). According to Steelman and Rutkowski (2004), expertise includes “knowledge of the recipient’s job requirements, knowledge of the recipient’s actual job performance and the ability to evaluate that performance in an accurate manner,” while trustworthiness represents “whether or not the individual trusts the feedback source to provide accurate performance information” (p. 8). As such, credible feedback providers are seen as possessing expertise relative to the specific tasks evaluated and are trusted to provide objective feedback, free from outside biasing factors (e.g., political considerations, feedback source’s mood at the time of feedback, and so on) (Ilgen et al., 1979; Steelman & Rutkowski, 2004). For example, Bannister (1986) conducted an experimental study in which it was found that source credibility had a significant effect on feedback recipients’ intentions to use suggestions given during feedback. Bannister (1986) concluded that the individual’s perceptions of the feedback provider’s qualifications to provide feedback are critical in determining an individual’s intent to use 187

Rick Jacobs, et al.

feedback or not. Consistent with these findings, Collins and Stukas (2006) found that individuals were more likely to accept self-inconsistent feedback from a high-status therapist than a low-status therapist. Moreover, such findings seem to generalize to the field as well. Steelman and Rutkowski (2004) surveyed 698 employees at two different manufacturing companies and found that employees were more motivated to improve their job performance when they perceived the feedback provider to be credible. Finally, Podsakoff and Farh (1989) found that individuals who received more credible negative feedback set higher goals and performed better than individuals who received less credible negative feedback. In short, these findings provide strong support for the credibility and status of the feedback provider in the feedback process. It appears that feedback recipients make strong attributions regarding the validity of feedback based on the perceived credibility and status of the feedback source. Thus, organizations should make a strong effort to make sure that feedback is delivered by highly qualified individuals with experience on the task evaluated and professional status that is greater than the feedback recipient’s. In so doing, recipients may be more likely to both accept and use feedback in order to improve their future performance.

The Role of Trust in Feedback Further examination of the feedback process highlights the importance of recognizing that feedback does not exist within a vacuum, but rather is embedded within the larger social- organizational context or social milieu, as referred to by Bretz and colleagues (1992). As such, research suggests that context plays a potentially pivotal role in shaping aspects of the appraisal process, including feedback, and the ways in which employees react to such processes (Farr & Jacobs, 2006; Murphy & Cleveland, 1995). Specifically, Folger et al. (1992) discussed performance appraisal as a process in which individuals at all organizational levels (including subordinates, supervisors, and upper management) have a stake, leading to potentially conflicting interests regarding the results of a given performance appraisal or feedback session. Thus, feedback providers, in such contexts, are believed to hold power over subordinates, who inevitably make themselves vulnerable during such sessions (Mayer, Davis, & Schoorman, 1995). Therefore, in order for feedback sessions, and performance appraisal processes in general, to be effective, those engaged in the process must trust in it. For feedback recipients, this means trusting their supervisors and the system used to evaluate them. For supervisors, this means having faith in the quality of their feedback and the appraisal system that informs it. Jablin (1979), for example, suggested that an employee’s trust in their supervisor influences the communication link between the two. As trust fades, it is argued that information fails to make a substantive impact during communication (Ilgen et al., 1979; Jablin, 1979). Thus, it stands to reason that when trust decreases, so does the impact of supervisory feedback on subordinates’ behavior. Providing empirical support for this idea, Herold and Greller (1977) found that supervisors who were more “psychologically close” with their subordinates had a greater impact in terms of feedback than those who were more psychologically distant. In addition, Earley (1986) found that workers’ trust in the feedback source partially mediated the relationship between feedback and workers’ response to and value attached to praise and criticism. In sum, these findings suggest that trust plays a crucial role in individuals’ willingness to accept and respond to feedback. Thus, supervisors should actively try to promote trust among their subordinates by educating subordinates about the performance appraisal and feedback process, showing how the feedback process may be used to promote employee development, demonstrating consideration for employees’ work and well-being, exhibiting dependability on a regular basis, and communicating a sense of honesty and forthrightness in daily activities, among other things. In so doing, it may be possible to promote a climate of trust surrounding the feedback process, which will work to facilitate feedback acceptance and ultimately improved job performance. 188

Appraising and Managing Officer Performance

Feedback Environments and Effort Costs Continuing on with the role of context in the feedback process, increased attention has been paid to creating organizational environments conducive to feedback. While much of the feedback literature has examined feedback through a relatively narrow lens, several researchers have suggested that organizations can facilitate the creation of feedback-oriented cultures by improving feedback quality, emphasizing the importance of feedback, and supporting its use by employees (London, 2003; Rosen et al., 2006). According to Rosen and colleagues (2006), in such cultures, feedback is “easily accessible, salient, and thus likely to influence employee beliefs and behaviors on a day-to-day basis” (p. 212). As such, many researchers have begun to examine what aspects of the organizational environment encourage and support the use of feedback in improving employee performance (Levy, Albright, Cawley, & Williams, 1995; Steelman et al., 2004; Williams, Miller, Steelman, & Levy, 1999). In particular, such research has surrounded contextual elements that contribute to employees’ willingness to seek out feedback from their employer (Ashford & Cummings, 1983; Whitaker, Dahling, & Levy, 2007). Feedback seeking has been defined as the efforts demonstrated by individuals in an organization to reduce uncertainty regarding the acceptability of their performance (Ashford, 1986; Ashford & Cummings, 1983). In particular, the literature has predominantly centered on inquiry, which Ashford and Cummings (1983) referred to as the active request for feedback. Such feedbackseeking behavior, moreover, has been shown to be related to important organizational outcomes, including employee learning, job satisfaction, and motivation (Anseel & Lievens, 2007; Hackman & Oldham, 1976; Mignerey, Rubin, & Gorden, 1995; Murphy & Cleveland, 1995). Further, research on contextual predictors of feedback seeking has primarily focused on perceived effort costs (Ashford & Cummings, 1983) and the feedback environment (Steelman et al., 2004). While effort costs refers to the perceived amount of effort employees believe they must expend to obtain feedback, a feedback environment refers to the extent to which workplace characteristics promote active use of feedback inquiry (Whitaker et al., 2007). The idea of a feedback environment is similar to what other researchers have termed an active learning climate, in which the organizational environment plays an active role in facilitating “new knowledge acquisition by encouraging employees to ask questions, seek feedback, reflect on potential results, explore, and experiment” (KatzNavon, Naveh, & Stern, 2009, p. 1200). Moreover, it is believed that feedback seeking decreases as a function of higher effort costs (Ashford, 1986; Ashford & Cummings, 1983) and increases with the supportiveness of a feedback environment (Steelman et al., 2004). In fact, Whitaker and colleagues (2007) found that among 170 subordinate–supervisor dyads, subordinates who perceived a supportive feedback environment demonstrated increased feedback-seeking behavior, higher role clarity, and higher job performance. Recently, Steelman and colleagues (2004) developed and validated a measure of feedback environment that includes seven factors of the feedback process, including many of the factors previously mentioned such as source credibility, feedback quality, feedback delivery, frequency of favorable feedback, frequency of unfavorable feedback, feedback availability, and support of feedback seeking. Moreover, empirical evidence suggests that higher levels of perceived feedback environment are related to increased affective commitment, job satisfaction, and organizational citizenship behaviors, in addition to decreased absenteeism (Norris-Watts & Levy, 2004; Steelman & Levy, 2001). Given such findings, it is important that supervisors and the broader organization work to decrease effort costs associated with feedback seeking. Research by authors such as Whitaker and colleagues (2007) suggest that employees may not be obtaining feedback even from supportive coworkers if a substantial amount of effort is required to do so. As such, organizations should put forth extra effort to make sure all barriers to the effective exchange of information between feedback recipients and providers are removed. Such research also suggests that by promoting feedback seeking, organizations may indirectly encourage employees to seek development-related feedback more consistently 189

Rick Jacobs, et al.

(London, 2003; London & Smither, 2002), leading to increased role clarity and improved performance (Whitaker et al., 2007). Finally, empirical evidence indicates that organizations that promote environments that are supportive of employee efforts to seek out feedback demonstrate a variety of positive outcomes. As such, organizations can promote such environments by encouraging both supervisors and employees to express openness to the feedback process and by openly rewarding such behavior.

Summary We have provided a lot of information regarding research and practice as it pertains to performance assessment and management. Much has been done in this area, and over the decades of theory building and practical application, we have learned quite a bit. While we believe in the importance of being very specific about what has been done in the past, it is often important to step back and simply summarize what we know and what it means in terms of creating a successful performance management program. Here is our perspective.

Defining Performance Without a doubt, nothing works unless time is spent and effort is expended to accurately define the key responsibilities and expected behaviors of those evaluated. Traditionally, this starts with a job analysis or a very comprehensive job description. We know this step helps rater and ratee when it comes to understanding performance expectations and accepting the accuracy of performance evaluations when they are conducted. We also know that from a fairness and legal perspective, a system of performance management will never be seen as reasonable unless the agency has done its homework and clearly documented what is required by the job. These descriptions should be multidimensional, covering critical areas of job performance, and behavioral, specifying the types of actions that represent poor, average, and strong job performance.

Setting the Stage Performance management is not just an organizational requirement—it is an organizational event. In police agencies and other organizations, the process of evaluating performance is often approached with dread. Evaluators do not enjoy conducting evaluations, those evaluated feel like they are unnecessarily put under the microscope, and those responsible for the process within the HR organization often feel they have to harass and cajole individuals just to do what needs to be done. It is not a happy time of the year for many members of the organization. With this backdrop, it is important that a more positive environment is created by focusing on the process and potential positive outcomes that will result from effective performance management. This requires a campaign initiated by the agency and supported from the top down. Without this kind of effort, the process will wallow in apathy and the benefits will never emerge.

Training, Training, and More Training We make a faulty assumption when it comes to performance management. We often assume that because someone has become a supervisor, they can and will be competent in the evaluation of others. Many supervisors and command personnel are good at reviewing the work of others and may enjoy the process, but not all. The role of training evaluators on the how to’s and why’s of performance evaluation is critical. Even for those positively disposed to the activity, training helps. An essential part of any performance management system is the training of raters on all aspects of the system. 190

Appraising and Managing Officer Performance

It is also critical that those evaluated are informed regarding the process. This is an often neglected area and is part of what we describe above in “Setting the Stage.” While it may be an annual agency requirement that performance reviews will occur, it is not necessarily clear to evaluated individuals exactly how the evaluations will be conducted, what is being measured, and how the data will be used. All these topics should be explained in advance of system implementation.

Keeping Track/Monitoring the System The most effective processes do not stop at the collection of data and the checking off of yet another organizational event required by the agency. The best systems review results, hold raters accountable for doing the evaluations in a timely fashion, analyze data for potential problems (bias, halo, and missing information as examples), and use this information to enhance the future system implementations.

Using the Data Traditionally, data from performance evaluations are used to understand individual performance, to provide feedback to the person evaluated, and/or to trigger personnel actions such as administering rewards or reprimands. While these are clearly important uses for the data, organizations are missing opportunities unless they look across individuals to understand where the agency may have repeated performance decrements indicating needed training programs or where the department is truly excelling and how that information can be leveraged for future motivational programs. Suffice it to say that the best run performance management programs continue beyond the individual level of analysis and look at groups, departments, and agencywide data to better understand performance.

Feedback—“Don’t Ask if You Aren’t Going to Tell” Over the years, we have learned quite a bit about how we can move from performance appraisal to using that information to drive future performance. It begins with a well-developed feedback process that brings supervisor and officer together to talk about the past and to plan for the future. Feedback does not happen automatically, and many involved in the process of delivering and receiving feedback need guidance to effectively turn observations of performance into performance improvements.

The Future of Performance Appraisal and What It Means for Police Departments Thus far, this chapter has outlined best practices for performance appraisal in organizations—from traditional issues regarding instrumentation to how to deliver performance feedback. However, performance appraisal methodology has never been stagnant. We have seen changes in research emphasis and practice over the past five decades. The future will bring improvements and updates to best practice as it stands today. Although it is impossible to predict exactly what the future will hold, there are a few areas that have already begun to show promise and that we believe will allow police departments to enhance the effectiveness of their performance assessment processes. The first of these areas lies in the realm of online assessment. Online assessment is quick and easy, and fits in well with today’s demand for technology-driven solutions. For example, e- learning has become very popular in organizational training because it is easy to use and can be completed by employees at any time. The same is true for online performance appraisal systems. Supervisors can complete surveys about officer behavior at a convenient time and in a quick, easy fashion. As the computerization of police work marches forward, more and more systems will be available for 191

Rick Jacobs, et al.

documenting and evaluating police officer performance. The use of online performance appraisal also facilitates self-evaluation and lends itself to automated comparisons between subordinate and supervisor that can form the basis of performance discussions and goal setting. As such, online assessments are also extremely useful tools for organizations because they save time and energy, and allow for the automated collection and storage of performance assessment data. Making performance appraisal systems more user-friendly and more efficient is undoubtedly in the best interest of the organization, and online appraisals certainly help to propel these goals forward. Second, there has been an upswing in the use of self-assessment for performance appraisal. In this case, employees are able to provide ratings regarding their own performance. However, the literature suggests that self-appraisals are ridden with bias. For example, Atkins and Wood (2002) demonstrated that self-ratings were negatively and nonlinearly related to assessment center performance. In addition, those with the highest self-ratings were those with the lowest actual performance. Overcompensating for poor performance, the worst employees may likely report that they are doing the best. Further, the negative relationship between self-rating and actual performance also indicates that high performers may be hard on themselves, creating a performance profile much lower than reality. It has also been demonstrated that self-ratings tend to be less variable than supervisor, peer, or subordinate ratings, suggesting possible halo or leniency errors (Scullen, Mount, & Judge, 2003). Thus, while selfevaluations may provide interesting information, it is not suggested for use in making administrative decisions. Organizations should be aware of employees’ intent to distort self-ratings and should proceed with caution when determining how to make use of self-report performance data. Finally, organizations falling behind industry standards may choose to implement performance improvement initiatives. In this case, the organization may need to change or increase current performance standards. This may require an overhaul of the existing performance system in order to reflect the new vision of employee performance. However, in terms of validity, this may be dangerous. Using visionary performance standards to create a new appraisal system is possible; however, it is necessary to make sure that employees chosen to fulfill newly envisioned roles are selected into this role based on predictors that indicate high performance using the new appraisal system. For example, selecting an employee into a new role based on old standards and then appraising them according to new standards creates a situation in which employee characteristics may not fit the new tenets of the job. This may lead to a low correlation between predictors, job behaviors, and performance ratings—a recipe for legal disaster. Thus, when attempting to improve performance, organizations should attempt to make changes throughout the entire system, from selection to appraisal to reward structures. In this way, performance appraisal can become an integral part of the organization’s strategy, while still ensuring validity in the process.

References and Further Reading Adams, J. A. (1987). Historical review and appraisal of research on the learning, retention, and transfer of human motor skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 41–74. Anderson, J. R. (1982). Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychological Review, 89, 369–406. Annett, J. (1969). Feedback and human behavior. Hammondsworth, UK: Penguin. Anseel, F., & Lievens, F. (2007). Long-term impact of the feedback environment on job satisfaction: A field study in a Belgian context. Applied Psychology, 56, 254–266. Aronson, E., Wilson, T. D., & Akert, R. M. (2007). Social psychology. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education. Arvey, R. D., & Murphy, K. R. (1998). Performance evaluation in work settings. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 141–168. Ashford, S. J. (1986). Feedback-seeking in individual adaptation: A resource perspective. Academy of Management Journal, 29, 465–487. Ashford, S. J., & Cummings, L. L. (1983). Feedback as an individual resource: Personal strategies of creating information. Organizational Behavior & Human Performance, 32, 370–398.

192

Appraising and Managing Officer Performance Atkins, P. W. B., & Wood, R. E. (2002). Self-versus others’ ratings as predictors of assessment center ratings: Validation evidence for 360-degree feedback programs. Personnel Psychology, 55, 871–904. Atkins, P. W. B., Wood, R. E., & Rutgers, P. J. (2002). The effects of feedback format on dynamic decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 88, 587–604. Atwater, L., & Brett, J. (2006). Feedback format: Does it influence manager’s reactions to feedback? Journal of Occupational Psychology, 79, 517–532. Austin, J. T., & Villanova, P. (1992). The criterion problem: 1917–1992. Journal of Applied Psychology, 77, 836–874. Bannister, B. D. (1986). Performance outcome feedback and attributional feedback: Interactive effects on recipient responses. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71, 203–210. Beehr, T. A., & Love, K. G. (1983). A meta-model of the effects of goal characteristics, feedback, and role characteristics in human organizations. Human Relations, 36, 151–166. Bernardin, H. J., & Beatty, R. W. (1984). Performance appraisal: Assessing human behavior at work. Boston: PWS-Kent. Bernardin, H. J., & Buckley, M. R. (1981). A consideration of strategies in rater training. Academy of Management Review, 6, 205–212. Bernardin, H. J., Cooke, D. K., & Villanova, P. (2000). Conscientiousness and agreeableness as predictors of rating leniency. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 232–236. Borman, W., & Motowidlo, S. (1993). Expanding the criterion domain to include elements of contextual performance. In N. Schmitt & W. C. Borman (Eds.), Personnel selection in organizations (pp. 71–98). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Brett, J. F., & Atwater, L. E. (2001). 360 degree feedback: Accuracy, reactions, and perceptions of usefulness. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 930–942. Bretz, R. D., Milkovich, G. T., & Read, W. (1992). The current state of performance appraisal research and practice: Concerns, directions, and implications. Journal of Management, 18, 321–352. Brown, M., & Benson, J. (2003). Rated to exhaustion? Reactions to performance appraisal processes. Industrial Relations Journal, 34, 67. Buchwals, A. M., & Meager, R. B. (1974). Immediate and delayed outcomes: Learning and the recall of responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 103, 758–767. Burke, R. J., Weitzel, W., & Weir, T. (1978). Characteristics of effective employee performance review and development interviews: Replication and extension. Personnel Psychology, 31, 903–919. Campbell, D. J., Campbell, K. M., & Chia, H.-B. (1998). Merit pay, performance appraisal, and individual motivation: An analysis and alternative. Human Resource Management, 37, 131. Campbell, J. P. (1999). The definition and measurement of performance in the new Age. In D. R. Ilgen & E. Pulakos (Eds.), The changing nature of performance (pp. 399–430). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Cawley, B. D., Keeping, L. M., & Levy, P. E. (1998). Participation in the performance appraisal process and employee reactions: A meta-analytic review of field investigations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 615–633. Christina, R. W., & Bjork, R. A. (1991). Optimizing long-term retention and transfer. In D. Druckman & R. A. Bjork (Eds.), In the mind’s eye: Enhancing human performance (pp. 23–55). Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Cleveland, J. N., Lim, A. S., & Murphy, K. R. (2007). Feedback phobia? Why employees do not want to give or receive performance feedback. In J. Langan-Fox, C. L. Cooper, & R. J. Klimoski (Eds.), Research companion to the dysfunctional workplace: Management challenges and symptoms. Cheltenham, UK: Edward Elgar. Collins, D. R., & Stukas, A. A. (2006). The effects of feedback self-consistency, therapist status, and attitude toward therapy on reaction to personality feedback. The Journal of Social Psychology, 146, 463–483. Collins, J. (2001). Good to great. New York: HarperCollins. Day, D. V., & Sulsky, L. M. (1995). Effects of frame-of-reference training and information configuration on memory organization and rating accuracy. Journal of Applied Psychology, 80, 158–167. DeNisi, A. S., Cafferty, T. P., & Meglino, B. M. (1984). A cognitive view of the performance appraisal process: A model and research propositions. Organizational Behavior & Human Performance, 33, 360–396. DeNisi, A. S., & Peters, L. H. (1996). Organization of information in memory and the performance appraisal process: Evidence from the field. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 717–737. DeNisi, A. S., & Williams, K. J. (1988). Cognitive approaches to performance appraisal. Research in Personnel and Human Resources Management, 6, 109–155. Duarte, N. T., Goodson, J. R., & Klich, N. R. (1993). How do I like thee? Let me appraise the ways. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 14, 239. Earley, P. C. (1986). Trust, perceived importance of praise and criticism, and work performance. An examination of feedback in the United States and England. Journal of Management, 12, 457–473. Erdogan, B., Kraimer, M. L., & Liden, R. C. (2001). Procedural justice as a two-dimensional construct: An examination in the performance appraisal account. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 37, 205–222.

193

Rick Jacobs, et al. Farr, J. L., & Jacobs, R. (2006). Unifying perspectives: The criterion problem today and into the 21st century. In W. Bennett, C. Lance, & D. Woehr (Eds.), Performance measurement: Current perspectives and future challenges (pp. 321–337). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Fedor, D. B., & Buckley, R. M. (1987). Providing feedback to organizational members: A reconsideration. Journal of Business and Psychology, 2, 171–181. Fedor, D. B., Rensvold, R. B., & Adams, S. M. (1992). An investigation of factors expected to affect feedback seeking: A longitudinal field study. Personnel Psychology, 45, 779–805. Flint, D. H. (1999). The role of organizational justice in multi-source performance appraisal: Theory-based applications and directions for research. Human Resource Management Review, 9, 1–20. Folger, R., Konovsky, M. A., & Cropanzano, R. (1992). A due process metaphor for performance appraisal. Research in Organizational Behavior, 14, 129–177. Fulk, J., Brief, A. P., & Barr, S. H. (1985). Trust-in-supervisor and perceived fairness and accuracy of performance evaluations. Journal of Business Research, 13, 301–313. Gaddis, B., Connelly, S., & Mumford, M. D. (2004). Failure feedback as an affective event: Influences of leader affect on subordinate attitudes and performance. The Leadership Quarterly, 15, 663–686. Gendersen, D. E., & Tinsley, D. B. (1996). Empirical assessment of impression management biases: The potential for performance appraisal error. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 11, 57–77. Goldstein, I. L., Emanuel, J. T., & Howell, W. C. (1968). Effect of percentage and specificity of feedback on choice behavior in a probabilistic information-processing task. Journal of Applied Psychology, 52, 163–168. Goodman, J. S., & Wood, R. E. (2004). Feedback specificity, learning opportunities, and learning. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 809–821. Goodman, J. S., Wood, R. E., & Hendickx, M. (2004). Feedback specificity, exploration, and learning. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 248–262. Goss, W. (2001). Managing for results—Appraisals and rewards. Australian Journal of Public Administration, 60, 3. Greguras, G. J., Robie, C., Schleicher, D. J., & Goff, M. (2003). A field study of the effects of rating purpose on the quality of multisource ratings. Personnel Psychology, 56, 1–21. Hackman, R. J., & Oldham, G. R. (1976). Motivation through the design of work: Test of a theory. Organizational Behavior & Human Performance, 16, 250–279. Hastie, R., & Park, B. (1986). The relationship between memory and judgment depends on whether the judgment task is memory-based or on-line. Psychological Review, 93, 258–268. Hedge, J. W., & Teachout, M. S. (2000). Exploring the concept of acceptability as a criterion for evaluating performance measures. Group & Organization Management, 25, 22–44. Herold, D. M., & Greller, M. M. (1977). Feedback: The definition of a construct. Academy of Management Journal, 20, 142–147. Ilgen, D. R., Barnes-Farrell, J. L., & McKellin, D. B. (1993). Performance appraisal process research in the 1980s: What has it contributed to appraisals in use? Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 54, 321–368. Ilgen, D. R., Fisher, C. D., & Taylor, M. S. (1979). Consequences of individual feedback on behavior in organizations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 64, 349–371. Jablin, F. M. (1979). Superior-subordinate communication: The state of the art. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 1201–1222. Jacobs, R. R., Kafry, D., & Zedeck, S. (1980). Expectations of behavioral rating scales. Personnel Psychology, 33, 595–640. Jawahar, I. M. (2006). An investigation of potential consequences of satisfaction with appraisal feedback. Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies, 13, 14–28. Jawahar, I. M., & Williams, C. R. (1997). Where all the children are above average: The performance appraisal purpose effect. Personnel Psychology, 50, 905–925. Johnson, D. E., Erez, A., Kiker, S. D., & Motowidlo, S. J. (2002). Liking and attributions of motives as mediators of the relationships between individuals’ reputations, helpful behaviors, and raters’ reward decisions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 808–815. Kacmar, K. M., Witt, L. A., Zivnuska, S., & Gully, S. M. (2003). The interactive effect of leader—member exchange and communication frequency on performance ratings. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 764–772. Katz-Navon, T., Naveh, E., & Stern, Z. (2009). Active learning: When is more better? The case of resident physicians’ medical errors. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94, 1200–1209. Keith, N., & Frese, M. (2008). Effectiveness of error management training: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 59–69. Kinicki, A. J., Prussia, G. E., Wu, B., & McKee-Ryan, F. M. (2004). A covariance structure analysis of employees’ response to performance feedback. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 1057–1069.

194

Appraising and Managing Officer Performance Kirk, P., & MacDonald, I. (1989). The role of feedback in management learning. Management Education & Development, 20, 9–19. Klimoski, R., & Inks, L. (1990). Accountability forces in performance appraisal. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 45, 194–208. Kluger, A. N., & DeNisi, A. (1996). Effects of feedback intervention on performance: A historical review, a metaanalysis, and a preliminary feedback intervention theory. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 254–284. Kopelman, R. E. (1986). Objective feedback. In E. A. Locke (Ed.), Generalizing from laboratory to field settings (pp. 119–145). Lexington, MA: D. C. Heath. Korman, A. K. (1970). The prediction of managerial performance: A preview. Studies in Personnel Psychology, 2, 4–26. Kozlowski, S. W. J., Chao, G. T., & Morrison, R. F. (1998). Games raters play: Politics strategies, and impression management in performance appraisal. In J. W. Smither (Ed.), Performance appraisal: State of the art in practice (pp. 163–208). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Landy, F. J., Barnes, J. L., & Murphy, K. R. (1978). Correlates of perceived fairness and accuracy of performance evaluation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 63, 751–754. Landy, F. J., & Farr, J. L. (1980). Performance rating. Psychological Bulletin, 87, 72–107. Latham, G. P., Almost, J., Mann, S., & Moore, C. (2005). New developments in performance management. Organizational Dynamics, 34, 77–87. Lefkowitz, J. (2000). The role of interpersonal affective regard in supervisory performance ratings: A literature review and proposed causal model. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 73, 67–85. Leung, K., Su, S., & Morris, M. W. (2001). When is criticism not constructive? The roles of fairness perceptions and dispositional attributions in employee acceptance of critical supervisory feedback. Human Relations, 54, 1155–1187. Levy, P. E., Albright, M. D., Cawley, B. D., & Williams, J. R. (1995). Situational and individual determinants of feedback seeking: A closer look at the process. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 62, 23–37. Levy, P. E., & Williams, J. R. (1998). The role of perceived system knowledge in predicting appraisal reactions, job satisfaction, and organizational commitment. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 19, 53–65. Levy, P. E., & Williams, J. R. (2004). The social context of performance appraisal: A review and framework for the future. Journal of Management, 30, 881–905. London, M. (2003). Job feedback: Giving, seeking, and using feedback for performance improvement. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. London, M., & Smither, J. (2002). Can working with an executive coach improve multisource feedback ratings over time? A quasi-experimental field study. Personnel Psychology, 56, 23–46. Lyness, K. S., & Heilman, M. E. (2006). When fit is fundamental: Performance evaluations and promotions of upper-level female and male managers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 777–785. Mani, B. G. (2002). Performance appraisal systems, productivity, and motivation: A case study. Public Personnel Management, 31, 141–159. Maurer, T. J., Mitchell, D. R. D., & Barbeite, F. G. (2002). Predictors of attitudes toward a 360-degree feedback system and involvement in post-feedback management development activity. Journal of Occupational & Organizational Psychology, 75, 87–107. Mayer, R. C., Davis, J. H., & Schoorman, F. D. (1995). An integrative model of organizational trust. Academy of Management Review, 20, 709–734. Mignerey, J. T., Rubin, R. B., & Gorden, W. I. (1995). Organizational entry: An investigation of newcomer communication behavior and uncertainty. Communication Research, 22, 54–85. Moss, S. E., Valenzi, E. R., & Taggart, W. (2003). Are you hiding from your boss? The development of a taxonomy and instrument to assess the feedback management behaviors of good and bad performers. Journal of Management, 29, 487–510. Motowidlo, S. J. (2003). Job performance. In W. C. Borman, D. R. Ilgen, & R. J. Klimoski (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 39–53). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley. Motowidlo, S. J., & Van Scotter, J. R. (1994). Evidence that task performance should be distinguished from contextual performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 475–480. Murphy, K. R., & Cleveland, J. N. (1991). Performance appraisal: An organizational perspective. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Murphy, K. R., & Cleveland, J. N. (1995). Understanding performance appraisal: Social, organizational, and goal-based perspectives. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Nease, A. A., Mudgett, B. O., & Quiñones, M. A. (1999). Relationships among feedback sign, self-efficacy, and acceptance of performance feedback. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 806–814.

195

Rick Jacobs, et al. Norris-Watts, C., & Levy, P. E. (2004). The mediating role of affective commitment in the relation of the feedback environment to work outcomes. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 65, 351–365. Ostroff, C., & Ilgen, D. R. (1992). Cognitive categories of raters and rating accuracy. Journal of Business and Psychology, 7, 3–26. Pettijohn, C. E., Pettijohn, L. S., & d’Amico, M. (2001). Characteristics of performance appraisals and their impact on sales force satisfaction. Human Resource Development Quarterly, 12, 127–146. Podsakoff, P. M., & Farh, J. (1989). Effects of feedback sign and credibility on goal setting and task performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 44, 45–67. Pulakos, E. D. (1984). A comparison of rater training programs: Error training and accuracy training. Journal of Applied Psychology, 69, 581–588. Reb, J., & Cropanzano, R. (2007). Evaluating dynamic performance: The influence of salient Gestalt characteristics on performance ratings. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 490–499. Roberts, G. E. (2003). Employee performance appraisal system participation: A technique that works. Public Personnel Management, 32, 89. Roberts, G. E., & Reed, T. (1996). Performance appraisal participation, goal setting and feedback. Review of Public Personnel Administration, 16, 29. Rosen, C. C., Levy, P. E., & Hall, R. J. (2006). Placing perceptions of politics in the context of the feedback environment, employee attitudes, and job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 211–220. Rynes, S. L., Gerhart, B., & Parks, L. (2005). Personnel psychology: Performance evaluation and pay for performance. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 571–600. Sackett, P. R., & Dubois, C. L. (1991). Rater-ratee race effects on performance evaluation: Challenging metaanalytic conclusions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 873–877. Schleicher, D. J., Day, D. V., Mayes, B. T., & Riggio, R. E. (2002). A new frame for frame of reference training: Enhancing the construct validity of assessment centers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 735–746. Schmidt, R. A. (1991). Frequent augmented feedback can degrade learning: Evidence and interpretations. In J. Requin & G. E. Steimach (Eds.), Tutorials in motor neuroscience (pp. 59–75). London: Kluwer. Scullen, S. E., Mount, M. K., & Judge, T. A. (2003). Evidence of the construct validity of developmental ratings of managerial performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 50–66. Seifert, C. F., Yuki, G., & McDonald, R. A. (2003). Effects of multisource feedback and a feedback facilitator on the influence behavior of managers toward subordinates. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 561–569. Shah, J. B., & Murphy, J. (1995). Performance appraisals for improved productivity. Journal of Management in Engineering, 11, 26. Smither, J. W., & Walker, A. G. (2004). Are the characteristics of narrative comments related to improvement in multirater feedback ratings over time? Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 575–581. Spector, P. E. (2006). Industrial and organizational psychology: Research and practice. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley. Stauffer, J. M., & Buckley, R. M. (2005). The existence and nature of racial bias in supervisory ratings. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 586–591. Steelman, L. A., & Levy, P. E. (2001, April). The feedback environment and its potential role in 360-degree feedback. In J. R. Williams (Chair), Has 360-degree feedback really gone amok? New empirical data. Symposium conducted at the 16th annual meeting of the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, San Diego, CA. Steelman, L. A., Levy, P. E., & Snell, A. F. (2004). The feedback environment scale: Construct definition, measurement, and validation. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 64, 165–184. Steelman, L. A., & Rutkowski, K. A. (2004). Moderators of employee reactions to negative feedback. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 19, 6–18. Sulsky, L. M., & Day, D. V. (1992). Frame-of-reference training and cognitive categorization: An empirical investigation of rater memory issues. Journal of Applied Psychology, 77, 501–510. Sulsky, L. M., & Keown, J. L. (1998). Performance appraisal in the changing world of work: Implications for the meaning and measurement of work performance. Canadian Psychology, 39, 52–59. Sulsky, L. M., Skarlicki, D. P., & Keown, J. L. (2002). Frame-of-reference-training: Overcoming the effects of organizational citizenship behavior on performance rating accuracy. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32, 1224–1240. Taylor, E. K., & Wherry, R. J. (1951). A study of leniency in two rating systems. Personnel Psychology, 4, 39–47. Taylor, M. S., Fisher, C., & Ilgen, D. (1984). Individual’s reactions to performance feedback in organizations: Control theory perspective. In K. Rowland & G. Ferris (Eds.), Research in personnel and human resource management (pp. 81–124). Greenwich, CT: JAI. Taylor, M. S., Masterson, S. S., Renard, M. K., & Tracy, K. B. (1998). Managers’ reactions to procedurally just performance management systems. Academy of Management Journal, 41, 568–579.

196

Appraising and Managing Officer Performance Tziner, A., & Kopelman, R. E. (2002). Is there a preferred performance rating format? A non-psychometric perspective. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 51, 479–503. Tziner, A., Kopelman, R., & Joanis, C. (1997). Investigation of raters’ and ratees’ reactions to three methods of performance appraisal: BOS, BARS, and GRS. Canadian Journal of Administrative Sciences, 14, 396. Viswesvaran, C., & Ones, D. S. (2000). Perspectives on models of job performance. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 8, 216–226. Waldman, D. A., & Bowen, D. E. (1998). The acceptability of 360 degree appraisals: A customer-supplier relationship perspective. Human Resource Management, 37, 117. Whitaker, B. G., Dahling, J. J., & Levy, P. (2007). The development of a feedback environment and role clarity model of job performance. Journal of Management, 33, 570–591. Williams, J. R., & Levy, P. E. (2000). Investigating some neglected criteria: The influence of organizational level and perceived system knowledge on appraisal reactions. Journal of Business and Psychology, 14, 501–513. Williams, J. R., & Lueke, S. B. (1999). 360 degrees feedback system effectiveness: Test of a model in a field setting. Journal of Quality Management, 4, 23. Williams, J. R., Miller, C. E., Steelman, L. A., & Levy, P. E. (1999). Increasing feedback seeking in public context: It takes two (or more) to tango. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 969–976. Woehr, D. J. (1994). Understanding frame of reference training: The impact of training on the recall of performance information. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 525–534. Woehr, D. J., & Huffcutt, A. I. (1994). Rater training for performance appraisal: A quantitative review. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 67, 189–205. Wong, K. F. E., & Kwong, J. Y. Y. (2007). Effect of rater goals on rating patterns: Evidence from an experimental field study. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 577–585.

197

9 ASSESSMENTS FOR SELECTION AND PROMOTION OF POLICE OFFICERS Rick Jacobs, Lily Cushenbery, and Patricia Grabarek

This chapter provides perspective on assessments used for selecting and advancing/promoting employees. We start with a broad overview of how employees are selected, then focus on what is going on in today’s police departments. The same strategy is used in our presentation on promotion systems before we move on to specific issues confronted when organizations engage in assessment and use the information to make employment decisions. The chapter concludes with a brief overview of legal issues facing police agencies as they attempt to identify talent to join the organization or to move up the rank structure and how we can successfully design and implement programs that encompass best practices and avoid legal complications.

Selecting Employees: A Brief Overview Selecting the best candidates for a job is critical for all organizations, but in few settings is it more important than law enforcement. In valid selection systems, those who do well on the tests and other instruments used in the screening and selection process become the high performers once they are integrated into the organization. Conversely, those who do poorly in the selection process would have likely been poor performers had they been given the job. That is the essence of validity; something we find out about the individual in the pre-employment assessments accurately forecasts subsequent job performance.

Theory and Tools Several important assumptions are made when creating selection systems. Many are obvious, but it helps to make sure we understand the science behind selection. According to Guion (1998), selection systems rely on the following basic assumptions: • • • • •

People have a variety of abilities. People differ in one or more of these abilities. These differences can be measured. These differences remain stable over time. Different jobs require different abilities.

198

Assessments for Selection and Promotion

It is the match between the required abilities of the job and the measured abilities of job applicants that helps us create valid selection systems.

Tests and Test Batteries Employers often use single tests or a combination of multiple tests to predict performance. For many years, civil service testing relied on multiple-choice tests that had one correct answer and three or four incorrect answers. Traditionally, these tests had 100 items, and a candidate’s fitness for the job was based on his or her performance on the test. Those candidates with high scores were selected with the assumption that their high test scores were indicative of future job performance. This logic works, provided a link exists between what is required to perform well on the test and the actual requirements of the job. When an employer creates a selection test or system that includes multiple tests, the key to successful selection is ensuring that the tests and the job have commonality. For years, employers relied solely on tests of cognitive abilities to select employees. The logic here is that those who are smarter and more cognitively complex would become the best performing employees. While many still argue this to be true, the broader reality is that jobs are multifaceted and require a variety of knowledge, skills, abilities, and personal characteristics. Attempting to predict performance with a single test will likely result in partial success at best. Many selection systems are created by assembling a group of tests, often referred to as a test battery. Frequently, these batteries include cognitive ability tests along with one or more distinctive types of tests such as a personality assessment measuring one or more dimensions of personality believed to be related to performance on the job. These systems adopt the perspective that the job in question is a collection of requirements: some involve thinking, some involve how individuals approach tasks, and some involve individuals’ ability to work with others. The goal here is to assemble a group of tests that matches the various requirements of the job. In many of these systems, we see assessments that tap candidates with respect to cognitive ability, personality, background, and more. Hough (2003) and McHenry, Hough, Toquam, Hanson, and Ashworth (1990) provide a nice perspective on the value of test batteries. They concluded that cognitive ability tests are better when predicting certain performance criteria (can-do) and personality scales are better at predicting others (will-do). The accuracy of predicting performance increases when using test batteries because of the combination of different types of tests as well as the more thorough assessment of candidates relative to the critical performance areas required by the job. Many selection systems adopt this test battery approach.

Interviews Interviews are the most frequently used assessment tool and are believed to assess multiple aspects of the applicant, allowing an accurate forecasting of job-relevant skills and abilities. Personality is the most frequently assessed set of characteristics, and 35% of interviews are believed to include some assessment of personality. Other characteristics that interviewers typically assess are applied social skills (28%), mental ability (16%), and specific job-related knowledge and skills (10%) (Huffcutt, Conway, Roth, & Stone, 2001). Interviewers first attempt to assess applicant values and personality before matching them with specific job requirements or organization characteristics (Posthuma, Morgeson, & Campion, 2002). Interviews have historically been one of the most researched selection tools used by organizations. In early studies, interviews were found to be relatively unreliable due to fluctuations in the questions asked and how conclusions were drawn by individual interviewers. Since the early 1960s, interviews have steadily improved with the addition of structure. Structure helped minimize the unwanted variations in what was asked and how interviewers reached conclusions about the applicants.

199

Rick Jacobs, et al.

The research literature has presented mixed findings on the overall utility or usefulness of interviewing during the selection process. Some studies have found no incremental validity, or increased predictability of performance, with the use of interviews above and beyond other information such as test scores. Consequently, some argue that interviews should be eliminated because they require vast resources in terms of personnel and time but do not contribute additional information. Additionally, some concern exists that interviewees can be coached and their performance in the interview is not indicative of what truly defines the candidate (Maurer, Solamon, & Troxtel, 1998). In contrast, other studies have found that tests such as cognitive ability and personality only add 4% of new information beyond what the interview provides, and therefore the interview should be the tool of choice and other selection tools are not required. Modern interview practices treat the interview more like a traditional test rather than an unstructured conversation. Today, selection systems include interviews that tie individual questions to key requirements of the job. In addition, careful attention is paid to how interviewers are trained in things such as the ideal interview environment, interviewer tone of voice, and factors that affect interviewer judgment. Finally, when interviewers use measures that have been developed for the specific purpose of summarizing what was discussed once the interview is completed, accuracy is enhanced. All of these features help increase the reliability of the interview and the ability to predict future performance from the interview evaluations (Chapman & Zweig, 2005). Given that interviews will continue to be used in most selection settings, it is important to understand best practices in creating and conducting interviews.

Work Sample Tests In work sample tests, applicants complete tasks that are very similar to those found in the job. The idea behind this type of test is that performance on job-similar tasks is highly predictive of performance on those same tasks once the applicant is on the job (Bobko, Roth, & Buster, 2005; Schmidt & Hunter, 1998). Many examples of work sample tests are used for selection. For example, in physical ability tests, candidates are asked to engage in a series of tasks very similar to tasks that they would encounter on the job. Such tasks include pushing a stopped vehicle, separating two individuals engaged in an argument, and/or carrying or dragging a dummy over a specified distance. Similarly, written and oral work simulations—such as asking an individual to fill out paperwork, respond to phone inquiries, or deal with an irate citizen—represent real tasks often performed by police officers. The important characteristic of a work sample is that it replicates the job in a meaningful way so that most individuals would conclude that a direct relationship exists between performance on the work sample and performance on the job. Work sample tests are perceived as fair and valid, which results in positive applicant reactions (Bobko et al., 2005). While work sample tests can be a valuable addition to any selection system, they can also be expensive to develop and implement. They require a great deal of specificity and therefore often require full customization for each individual job. Further, work sample tests often require specific information from the job and therefore cannot be used in entry-level testing since candidates will likely gain information and/or skills once they are hired and receive training. Work sample tests can be valuable in selecting employees, but there is a delicate balance between what the test can contain and what candidates come to the test already knowing about the job.

Situational Judgment Tests Situational judgment tests (SJTs) are a series of job-related scenarios presented in written, oral, or visual (video) formats where an applicant is asked to present a written or oral description of how they would react to a hypothetical situation (Clevenger, Pereira, Wiechmann, Schmitt, & Harvey, 2001; 200

Assessments for Selection and Promotion

McDaniel, Morgeson, Finnegan, Campion, & Braverman, 2001). They are designed to measure judgment in work settings. Some tests ask for applicants to choose a response among a set of alternative responses while others ask for a rating of agreement with various responses (McDaniel et al., 2001). There are many advantages of these types of tests. SJTs have good predictive validity and are moderately correlated with cognitive ability. They also have incremental validity above cognitive ability testing (McDaniel et al., 2001). In addition, situational judgment tests have high face validity, meaning they appear to test what they are actually testing (Clevenger et al., 2001). Applicants tend to have positive reactions to these types of tests because of their obvious relationship to the job for which the applicants are competing. Furthermore, these tests are relatively easy and often inexpensive to create, although this is less true for video-based versions. Racial group biases are also smaller in situational judgment tests than in cognitive ability tests. However, the racial group differences for situational judgment tests still exist and are bigger than those found in personality measures (Clevenger et al., 2001).

Integrity Tests Integrity tests are very widely used in selection of employees (Alliger & Dwight, 2000). They are used to identify individual characteristics that increase the likelihood to engage in counterproductive work behaviors such as theft, undermining, and absenteeism. There are two types of integrity tests: overt and personality. Overt integrity tests measure attitudes toward dishonest behavior and ask about previous illegal activity (Sackett, Burris, & Callahan, 1989). Personality integrity tests measure attitudes and dispositions related to integrity, including reliability and conscientiousness (Hogan & Brinkmeyer, 1997; Neuman & Baydoun, 1998). Many employers prefer to use integrity tests to predict applicants who are likely to engage in these negative work behaviors and remove them from the selection pool instead of waiting to discover these behaviors later (Camara & Schneider, 1995; Mikulay & Goffin, 1998). Integrity tests are successful at predicting counterproductive work behaviors because individuals perceive their work environment differently (Murphy, 1993). For example, some individuals do not see a problem with taking some resources home from work (such as pens), while others believe this is wrong. Although integrity tests are often shown to be predictive of subsequent job performance in a broad context, the use of these tests is not without criticism and can be seen as beginning the employment process from a position of distrust.

Personal History Assessment A personal history assessment requires applicants to fill out questions about educational background, prior work experience, and other previous activities. These include what psychologists like to call biodata—historical, external actions or objective and discrete events that people have control over. As the name implies, the focus of personal history assessment is past actions and behaviors and almost always emphasizes specific events (Mael, Connerley, & Morath, 1996). The content of the actual assessment attempts to tie job requirements to indicators from past behaviors that reflect favorably on the candidate’s ability to perform the tasks of the job. For example, personal history data are often collected to select people for overseas assignments. Since this type of work requires interactions with people from different cultures, one indicator of future success is an individual’s interest in other cultures prior to working overseas. Questions on a personal history assessment in this context would include asking about foreign language competency, attending foreign film festivals, and even interest in sampling foods from ethnic restaurants. The logic here is that those who will succeed in the assignment already have an interest in exploring other cultures and these items help define past behaviors reflecting that interest. 201

Rick Jacobs, et al.

Assessment Centers Assessment centers are categorized by multiple assessors, multiple exercises, and multiple competencies (Thornton & Rupp, 2006). They are used to select employees by gathering the opinions of multiple evaluators (assessors) across a variety of work-related tasks (exercises) and simultaneously measuring a series of job-relevant dimensions (competencies). Assessment centers are typically used in selection by diagnosing individuals’ strengths and weaknesses that are relevant to the organization’s requirements for the job. The exercises range from oral presentations to written reports to role-plays of job-relevant situations, each providing opportunities for applicants to display behaviors that showcase their abilities and potential in the different competencies. Several assessors, or raters, observe the applicants completing these exercises and rate the observed behaviors to create competency scores and an overall assessment rating (OAR). Assessment centers have many advantages and disadvantages when it comes to selecting employees. They have criterion-related validity, which means scores from assessment centers predict onthe-job performance (e.g., Haaland & Christiansen, 2002; Thornton & Rupp, 2006). They can also include traditional tests such as cognitive ability and personality inventories (Guion, 1998) in addition to the exercises in the assessment center. Even when these more traditional tests are used, assessment centers still have incremental validity, or provide new information above and beyond the other tests. In addition, many applicants perceive assessment centers positively. Applicants can see how the assessment relates to the job and are more likely to view assessment centers as fair in terms of the employment decision (Thornton & Rupp, 2006). Further, assessment centers have lower levels of racial group differences than tests such as cognitive ability and therefore can enhance the diversity of the selected group. Assessment centers can be seen as the ultimate form of test batteries because they include a wide variety of assessment tools from traditional tests to interviews to more interactive tools where assessors evaluate candidates during the performance of job simulations. Assessment centers provide rich data about each applicant and as such have a correspondingly high cost in terms of both development and implementation. Many organizations lack the resources to create assessment centers, while others simply find it difficult to assemble the finances and people to put them into practice.

Goals of Selection When creating a selection system, the primary goal is to sort candidates in a way that identifies their job-related skills and abilities. When done correctly, the system will allow organizations to select those capable of doing the job and reject those less likely to succeed. This is far from the only goal of selection. A good selection system should have other positive outcomes such as creating favorable impressions among job applicants (Barrick & Zimmerman, 2005), ensuring that those selected are retained by the organization (Hausknecht, Day, & Thomas, 2004), and helping to achieve diversity in the selected group at a rate representative of the diversity seen in the applicant pool (Guion, 1998). However, among these additional goals, achieving racial diversity and avoiding adverse impact have received the most attention in selection. This is particularly true in the police context. Most organizations take a great deal of care to avoid hiring employees using a process that has an adverse impact for a protected class. Title VII of the Civil Rights Act prohibits discrimination by employers on the basis of race, color, religion, sex, or national origin. Actual or implied selection rates are the sources of adverse impact defined by law (Gutman, 2004). If adverse impact exists, an organization must prove job relatedness and business necessity of the selection process via validity evidence that shows that performance on the test is systematically related to performance on the job. Such evidence can be presented by criterion-related validity (test scores are related to job performance 202

Assessments for Selection and Promotion

measures), construct validity (the test is measuring what it should be), and content validity (the content of the test is relevant to the job) (Sackett & Wilk, 1994). Unfortunately, many valid tests commonly used for selection have racial group differences (Campion et al., 2001) that result in disproportionate selection as a function of race. For example, cognitive ability tests have documented validity for a wide variety of jobs, but they also show significant group differences with white candidates outperforming black and Hispanic candidates. Thus, cognitive ability tests are known to create adverse impact in selection systems (Berry, Gruys, & Sackett, 2006). This presents a significant challenge to those designing selection systems where the goals are not only valid selection but also relative equality of outcomes across groups (Sackett, De Corte, & Lievens, 2010). Several potential solutions have been suggested, including alternative tests such as work samples (Bobko et al., 2005) and personality assessments (Sackett, Schmitt, Ellingson, & Kabin, 2001). While both of these have been shown to reduce adverse impact, the decrease is less than anticipated given that many of these indicators have been shown to be related to cognitive ability, at least for the types of job applicants that predominate in police officer selection (Cascio, Jacobs, & Silva, 2010).

Selecting Police Officers Selection systems created for police departments are based on the broader research but also have several unique aspects such as the need to incorporate assessment of physical fitness, the need to ensure high levels of personal integrity, and the need to have a workforce representative of the people it serves. Currently, a variety of selection programs are used to identify police officers who can perform the complicated tasks and assume the heavy responsibilities inherent in the job. The following section discusses the types of assessment tools used specifically in police departments. The majority of police departments use a battery of tests and assessment devices to determine the applicants best suited for the job. This process involves both selection testing and assessments that occur after the initial decision regarding job suitability. It is important to keep the sequence of the selection process in perspective. More than 90% of departments use a combination of an application, job-related ability tests, interviews, medical exams, psychological exams (pathology assessment), and background investigations (Cochrane, Tett, & Vandecreek, 2003). In addition, more than 50% of departments require drug screening, physical fitness assessment, and polygraph testing. Only 8% of police departments use formal assessment centers for officer selection (Cochrane et al., 2003), but the technique is more widely used in police promotional exams. According to Dayan, Kasten, and Fox (2002), it is possible that assessment centers are more prevalent in police officer selection but are underreported.

Ability Testing Virtually every police officer selection process begins with an application, a determination of eligibility (minimum age, limited or no arrest record, driver’s license, and so on), and an ability test. The determination of eligibility is a dichotomous decision that results in either allowing the candidate to move on to the next phase of the process or an early exit from a career in law enforcement with that agency and perhaps with others as well. Those eligible to test are given a time to report for testing, which takes place at a specified location. A majority of these tests are paper and pencil in format with candidates responding to a series of questions and choosing the best response from four or five alternatives. Some agencies have moved from paper-and-pencil to computer-based tests, but the underlying assessment is very much the same and answers the question: Does the candidate possess the cognitive ability to perform as a police officer? The tests include items designed to assess such thinking skills as written comprehension, inductive reasoning, deductive reasoning, information ordering, and problem sensitivity. This type of testing 203

Rick Jacobs, et al.

has been shown to be valid in a variety of settings, including the selection of law enforcement officers. The major drawback is that these types of tests are often associated with adverse impact, resulting in fewer minority group members making it into the final pool of selected candidates. In spite of this undesirable outcome, there is a need for assessing thinking skills as the cognitive demand of the job is substantial, beginning with learning relevant laws, policies, and procedures and culminating with the thought processes required to perform on the street in real time. Many departments have moved to a selection system that uses cognitive abilities to set minimum qualifications based on test results and then moves candidates through to other assessments (personality, biodata, and interview) where those scores are then combined with the cognitive assessment to form a total selection score.

Personality Assessments Personality testing has two distinct purposes in law enforcement selection systems. First, personality testing is often used to identify those candidates likely to be strong performers on the job and to help avoid hiring those who have personality scores/profiles that are predictive of poor performance. In this way, personality tests are used much like cognitive ability tests simply as a means to identify talent. The second way we see personality assessments entering in the employment process is further down the line when departments have identified who to hire and are concerned with avoiding hiring an individual who demonstrates signs of pathology. If the personality test indicates a potential pathology, it is necessary to follow up with a clinical interview. Approximately 90% of police departments use personality assessments somewhere in their selection process (Cochrane et al., 2003) with the dual goals of identifying applicants who are well adjusted and possess the skills necessary to do the job (Detrick, Chibnall, & Luebbert, 2004). A number of different personality inventories have been successfully deployed by police departments, including, but not limited to, the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI), the Inwald Personality Inventory (IPI), the NEO Personality Inventory, and the Sixteen Personality Factor Questionnaire (16PF). The MMPI is the most commonly used inventory in police departments (Detrick et al., 2004), but it is not without criticism. The MMPI seems more of a general measure of psychopathology and is not particularly good at predicting performance in law enforcement (Scogin, Schumacher, Gardner, & Chaplin, 1995). This highlights the point made previously regarding the dual purpose of personality testing. It should always be clear to both the police agency and the candidate the exact purpose of any assessment. Perhaps the MMPI should be restricted to identifying potential pathology and not for identifying performance-related attributes. Much of the MMPI’s widespread use came from its reputation in excluding candidates with problems rather than in selecting those likely to be excellent performers on the job (Scrivner, 2001). The IPI is another popular tool used by police departments and was created specifically for police officer selection. Performance on the job has been shown to be significantly related to the IPI (Detrick & Chibnall, 2002; Inwald, 1988, 1992). The NEO Personality Inventory (Costa & McRae, 1992; Detrick et al., 2004) has also been supported in its use for selecting police officers. Finally, the 16PF is frequently used in police selection (Cochrane et al., 2003) and has been supported in its ability to predict turnover (Drew, Carless, & Thompson, 2008). Recently, Cascio et al. (2010) have made a strong argument for the use of personality testing in conjunction with cognitive ability assessment. These authors have demonstrated that personality information, when added to information about a candidate’s thinking skills, can not only add to the prediction of job performance but can simultaneously reduce the level of adverse impact in the final selection pool when compared to selection systems based solely on cognitive assessment. In many ways this work highlights the importance of assessing many different aspects of the candidate with 204

Assessments for Selection and Promotion

greater validity and lower levels of adverse impact. This approach remains promising as we attempt to build more effective police officer selection programs.

Faking on Personality Tests and Distortion on Other Types of Assessments Despite the extensive research on selection testing and the substantial progress made over the past five decades in creating and implementing valid tests, some issues remain unresolved. There are problems in maximizing the effectiveness of our tools and a clear need for ongoing research. Organizations, especially within law enforcement, tend to fear that applicants fake personality tests to appear more socially desirable or more consistent with the requirements of the job. For example, a person who is chronically late may know that punctuality is a desirable trait and would indicate that they are always on time. If a personality test is used, the candidate would indicate that they have a high awareness of being on time or conscientious. While these statements are not true, the candidate can easily disguise their tendencies by answering in a way that is consistent with their vision of what the organization hopes to hear. Psychologists refer to this specific problem as test transparency, meaning that the candidate can “see the correct or best answer.” In contrast, a cognitive ability test has a right or wrong answer, and the candidate either knows the answer or doesn’t; it is not easy to fake smart. While several researchers have indicated that faking may be less problematic than originally thought (Ellingson, Sackett, & Connelly, 2007; Hogan, Barrett, & Hogan, 2007; Hough, Eaton, Dunnette, Kamp, & McCloy, 1990), in high-stakes testing many individuals will distort their responses. Given that many tests are both potentially useful for selection while simultaneously allowing for faking a selection system, more needs to be done to ensure that those identified via tests are not just those who successfully game the testing process. There are at least four avenues available to help us with the issue. •

• •



Know who is likely to fake—research exists that indicates who is most likely to fake or distort their responses. Applicants with high social skills and/or those who score high on social desirability are associated with improving their scores, while those high on integrity are less likely (Hough et al., 1990). There has been some evidence that faking occurs even on integrity tests; integrity tests are the easiest to fake (McFarland & Ryan, 2000), and they often do not include lie scales (Lilienfeld, Alliger, & Mitchell, 1995). Use validity (lie) scales—these scales indicate whether or not a candidate is likely to engage in response distortions to improve their scores. Verify responses—in the context of police testing, it is possible to ask questions on a test, print out responses for each candidate, and then verify the response during background investigation or another step in the process. Use multiple gates or hurdles in selection—using the polygraph, background investigation, or other hurdles, eliminate those not responding truthfully during earlier stages of selection.

Despite the low base rate of faking, utilizing the research to prevent it is comforting for the organizations that fear its occurrence and can help eliminate the occasional problem candidate.

Interviews One logical perspective pervasive in selection system development is to place the less expensive, less labor-intensive procedures at the beginning of the process and hold those that require more resources for later when we have eliminated candidates and therefore have fewer in the pipeline. The interview can be seen in this context. Law enforcement agencies interview police officer candidates once the pool has been evaluated via the application for minimum qualifications and the absence of 205

Rick Jacobs, et al.

disqualifiers (conviction of a felony as an example), along with certain qualifying information such as sufficiently high scores on entry-level tests including cognitive ability, personality, and/or biodata assessment. With this “partially qualified” group, agencies often create an interview process that will allow them to make additional judgments regarding suitability. Police officer interviews run the range from informal conversations with the chief in smaller departments to formalized processes with multiple assessors in some of the larger and more administratively capable departments. Best practices in police officer interviewing include: • • • • •

Prequalifying those interviewed to minimize spending time with poorer candidates Providing training for interviewers Interviewing in teams of two or preferably three people Developing interview questions around specific dimensions such as empathy, oral communication, interpersonal sensitivity, and the ability to listen Providing interviewers with a structured format for evaluations

These interviews are often the last step in the pretesting selection process, and successful candidates move on to the remaining steps such as psychological screening, medical evaluation, and a background check. Finally, interviews may be useful in decreasing race and gender bias in the selection process. Racial and gender-related group differences are lower in interviews than what is traditionally found in using cognitive ability tests for selection (Huffcutt et al., 2001; Sacco, Scheu, Ryan, & Schmitt, 2003). This is important to consider because including a well-constructed interview in the selection process can enhance validity while simultaneously reducing adverse impact.

Assessment Centers The use of assessment centers for selecting police officers is gaining in popularity, but its rise is limited by the tremendous resource demands of the technique. Assessment center ratings have been found to be valid predictors of training and job performance. These police assessment centers, combined with cognitive ability tests and personality tests, can predict on-the-job police performance better than cognitive ability and personality alone (Dayan et al., 2002). Thus, assessment center performance shows a unique contribution to the prediction of good police performance, above that of cognitive ability. Furthermore, assessment centers have been shown to have less adverse impact than cognitive ability testing in police selection (Pynes & Bernardin, 1992) and therefore may be a viable alternative or addition to cognitive ability testing. In reality, many departments create an assessment center when they piece together the various elements in a series of steps the candidates must follow to gain employment. In addition to having these multiple exercises and multiple assessors evaluating candidates on multiple dimensions, assessment centers generally try to do all steps in a fixed period of time. Many systems exist that have a great deal of overlap with assessment centers and can be considered highly similar in process and tend to have highly similar outcomes.

Physical Fitness Many agencies use physical fitness testing for selection (Cochrane et al., 2003; Lonsway, 2003). This type of work samples testing seems intuitive since police officers often have to engage in strenuous physical activity on the job. However, there is a weak relationship between physical test performance and on-the-job performance in incidents involving physical activity (Wiener, 1994). Because of this and due to the substantial gender differences found in this type of testing, this selection tool is often challenged regarding job relatedness (Lonsway, 2003). Women perform as successfully as men on the 206

Assessments for Selection and Promotion

job (Martin & Jurik, 1996), so the use of physical testing that systematically excludes women from consideration at the beginning of the selection process is often questioned. What is important to know about physical ability testing in police is that there are two categorically different approaches when such tests are deployed. First, some tests actually replicate the activities of the job. These are job simulations, or what we described earlier as work samples. Here, activities such as running a specified distance, negotiating an obstacle course, moving a stalled vehicle, and separating two arguing parties (usually simulated using equipment rather than people) are put together as a course the candidate must follow. Each event is timed, and the set of events is then scored either by awarding points for each event or as a pass/fail for the set of events. Scoring is usually developed by running incumbent officers through the course and determining appropriate minimum performance standards for each event and for the set of events. This type of testing can be contrasted to what is referred to as surrogate testing. In surrogate testing, exercises such as pushups, sit-ups, and step-ups are used to measure the fitness levels of the candidates. Scoring is often referenced to norms created by gender and age so that someone older is not required to do as much as someone younger. This age/gender norming is highly controversial since the job requires all officers regardless of age or gender to perform similarly when they arrive on the scene. Having some form of physical testing in a selection program does provide assurance that the incoming class of officers is ready to assume the duties of the job. In some cases, physical ability/fitness testing occurs after the fact rather than during the selection process. For many departments, the training academy has a fitness program and graduation is predicated on meeting standards (Lonsway, 2003).

Polygraphs Polygraphs are used in more than 50% of police departments (Cochrane et al., 2003). They are intended to add incremental predictive validity to the selection process (Handler, Honts, Krapohl, Nelson, & Griffin, 2009) by providing information that has not hitherto been part of the process. Polygraph tests are intended to be used as a screening tool to identify unacceptable behaviors that are a risk to the integrity of the position of a police officer. However, polygraphs are controversial and not legal in certain contexts. Within law enforcement and due to the safety sensitive nature of the work, police departments continue to use polygraphs but often do not use them as the sole determinant for disqualifying an applicant (Handler et al., 2009). Polygraph tests are frequently used as decision support or as an extra source of information in the selection process. In addition, polygraph tests tend to deter unsuitable applicants from applying because they are uncomfortable with the idea of the test (Handler et al., 2009). However, there are some potential problems with the tool. Polygraphs often result in a false positive, identifying someone as lying when in fact they are telling the truth. To a lesser degree, polygraphs result in false negatives, identifying someone as telling the truth when they are falsifying information. To ensure that the polygraph test is helping the selection system and not creating issues, some have recommended that polygraph examiners be well trained, have a strict protocol, and be observed and evaluated frequently (Handler et al., 2009). Also, only issues relevant to police selection should be discussed in these types of interviews, such as tolerance-related issues, past criminal conduct, illegal drug use, and formal disciplinary actions from previous employers. The information gathered from these types of questions is critical to selecting police officers but is easily falsified using other selection tools. Thus, a polygraph may be useful for gathering this type of specific information. Creating a selection process that will result in the desired outcomes for a police department is a daunting task. It requires a thorough understanding of the job as it is performed in that department, a clear set of tests/exercises/hurdles that candidates must pass, and consistency in the administration of all tests to all candidates. It further requires a great deal of expertise and the use of tools that 207

Rick Jacobs, et al.

have proven effectiveness in terms of validity. In addition, modern police departments seek to be representative of the people they serve so any selection system must result in a diverse set of officers. Over the past several decades, police agencies have created successful systems that incorporate a variety of tests and other assessment procedures. Departments wishing to build a system or revamp the one currently in use have opportunities to learn from the science and practice of police officer selection.

Promotional Testing: A Brief Overview Organizations create structure by moving individuals from lower level jobs into positions of supervision and management. In so doing, they use a variety of strategies including hiring leaders from other organizations and moving individuals within the organization up to positions of greater responsibility. In most settings, although not often in police departments, a combination of external and internal candidates become leaders. Regardless of who makes up the candidate pool, the success of the promotional process will determine the success of the organization as a whole. Promotions represent an opportunity to enhance the skills and abilities of individuals, put people in positions where they can do the same for those reporting to them, and help the organization grow and prosper. Conversely, poor promotional systems can run the risk of identifying the people not capable of performing the job. Worse, these wrongly promoted individuals can poison the well, making capable participants who were not promoted look at the outcome with contempt and cause such unwanted consequences as negative job attitudes, insubordination, lower levels of work motivation, and ultimately higher levels of employee turnover. Formal promotions procedures serve to create a more salient link between employee performance and organizational rewards (Greenberg, 1986). For employees who have long-term plans to be in an organization, the expectation for promotion can have positive effects on performance. For example, one study shows that employees are more willing to forgive instances of inequity in an organization if they believe the long-term rewards of organizational membership outweigh the costs (Kaplan & Ferris, 2001). The opposite can also occur, as individuals who had initially expected promotions become disenchanted with the system within their organization, develop the perception that they are working in a dead-end job, and thus decrease effort (Kaplan & Ferris, 2001). Thus, even how a worker is considered for promotion can have important organizational outcomes. An important point about promotions is their limited quantity. The likelihood of promotion is often less predictable than other outcomes such as annual salary increases and places greater importance on each promotion since the next opportunity will occur at an unspecified future time (Kaplan & Ferris, 2001). For example, in a study of nonacademic employees in a large university, the greater the amount of time between promotion availability, the more organizational politics and job-related stress occurred (Ferris et al., 1996). Moreover, the pay structure in most organizations flattens out after a few years, and individuals may see promotions as a way to increase their incomes. However, employees considered for promotion should be interested in the new job duties and responsibilities that come with the promotion instead of focusing on the rewards (Scarborough, Van Tubergen, Gaines, & Whitlow, 1999). The limited quantity of promotions can sometimes create competition and animosity within an organization. For all of the above reasons, promotions are a key ingredient to the successful operation of any organization. A well-developed system of promotions can add to the capability of the organization not only at the level of the promotion but also for all those who interact with the promoted individuals. On the opposite end of the spectrum, substandard promotion systems can threaten the well-being of an organization in terms of future performance and in the way individuals evaluate the quality of their organization.

208

Assessments for Selection and Promotion

Police Promotions The promotions process in law enforcement has many similarities to selecting new officers in that some individuals receive the new job while others do not. Like selection, there are two desired or correct outcomes when it comes to promotions: those who are qualified get the new, higher level job, and those who are not qualified do not. Similarly, two potential errors exist. We can promote an unqualified person to a position of greater authority or reject someone who would be successful in that new position. The quality of our system is dependent on the degree to which we maximize the first two and minimize the second two. What distinguishes promotional systems from selection systems designed to add new employees to the organization is the following: • • • •

Applicants for promotions already work within the organization (almost always). A great deal more information about each candidate is available. Testing for more of the knowledge, skills, and abilities of the job is possible. Unsuccessful candidates remain part of the organization and require attention.

These differences create unique opportunities and challenges for departments when it comes to the tricky business of making promotions (Scarborough et al., 1992). For police departments, a variety of tools and techniques have been created over the years from such diverse influences as the research literature on promotions to union–management agreements that specify the use of certain types of tests or ban the use of others. The challenge lies in putting tools into practice that result in the identification of those truly ready for advancement, the perception of justice by participating officers, and the fair inclusion of all.

Current Programs in Use Many promotional procedures currently in use in today’s police department start with a detailed understanding of the positions considered via a job analysis, which typically includes (1) a review of the job tasks frequently performed or considered very important even if they are not encountered often, (2) the required abilities that allow individuals to perform these tasks, and (3) the knowledge associated with performance of these tasks. Departments approach the development of promotional systems from a content validity perspective where the goal is to create a system in which the test components match up closely with the actual job requirements. Typically, applicants are given a job knowledge test assessing their expertise on laws, rules, and procedures. This test is often followed by a formalized panel interview in which candidates are asked to respond to situations typical of what they would find should they assume the next-level position. The job knowledge test is usually presented in a written format with multiple-choice questions. The panel interview is conducted by a team of assessors and often involves responding to two or more simulated situations. Scores on the two phases are combined to form the promotion score. The method of combining the information varies from one department to another. Interestingly enough, one piece of potentially valuable information rarely sees its way into the promotional process. Officers considered for promotion are almost always in the rank immediately lower than the one where the competition is taking place. In some instances, departments allow officers to skip ranks but this is the exception and not the rule (Maurer et al., 1998). Departments have a tremendous amount of relevant job performance information on virtually everyone considered for promotion in the form of what those officers have been doing in their current job or role (Harvey, 1999). Since in most police organizations the move from one position (police officer) to the next (police sergeant) often involves many of the same responsibilities and tasks, using past

209

Rick Jacobs, et al.

performance makes sense because it gives a clear indication on how the officer will perform on part of the new job if promoted. It is important to note that this is an untapped information pool for most promotional systems. It remains untapped because of the difficulty in extracting that information in what would be perceived by all as an accurate and fair manner. Still, in some systems performance appraisal does play a role, and it is important to cite not only the usefulness of these types of data but also how we might incorporate them into the promotion process. As an example, one jurisdiction we have worked with for over a decade uses past performance as a qualifier for admission to the promotion process. This system is designed to keep those who the department feels are undeserving of promotion out of the process. At the outset, when promotion opportunities are announced, all interested officers apply. Once the applications are closed, all those who have applied are evaluated by their immediate supervisors, and that rating is further evaluated/approved by a second-level supervisor. If a promotion candidate is seen as problematic based on past performance, the candidate is notified that he or she will not receive further consideration for promotion. This seems to be the best way to incorporate some form of past performance into the process, although it certainly does not take full advantage of all we know about those working for us. Alternatives such as gathering performance scores and entering them into the combination of scores that result in an overall promotion ranking are possible. However, given the suspicion associated with how those performance scores are determined, the bias that may be inherent in them, and issues surrounding supervisor–subordinate interactions all conspire to make such a plan problematic and seldom used.

Applicant Reactions to Promotional Systems Applicant reactions to the promotion process can be categorized into two different types of perceptions of fairness: satisfaction with the fairness of the promotions process and system (procedural justice) and satisfaction with the fairness of the decision or outcome (distributive justice). Of the two types of potential injustice perceptions, the latter is far more frequent than the former. In many departments, the promotions system procedures either have been well accepted over time or have been the result of union–management negotiations and therefore have been created with an eye toward what is acceptable to participants. An example of distributive justice in this context is an officer who believes the promotion system is fair while he or she is going through the promotions process, but no longer believes in the system because his or her name is not on the list of those to be promoted. This is often the case because we hear very few complaints about the process when it is underway and immediately following the actual tests, but many issues are brought to the surface once the list of promotions is published. Perceptions of justice are critical as they can influence future job satisfaction of officers, self-esteem for those unsuccessful in getting promoted, and organizational climate for all involved (Truxillo & Bauer, 1999). These perceptions of promotion system fairness do not occur in a vacuum. Previous encounters with organizational human resource functions, such as performance appraisals, will influence officer reactions to promotion decisions (Kaplan & Ferris, 2001). For example, if an officer received an unfairly harsh evaluation that year, she may have less faith in the organizational leadership. The opposite is also possible; an officer who received a steady stream of positive feedback may have higher expectations of organizational rewards. These perceptions serve as a backdrop when it comes to evaluating how fair the promotion system was in identifying the truly talented officers under consideration. Consequently, it may be very hard to create a promotion program seen as fair when other processes within the department are viewed as lacking in credibility. The importance of these perceptions cannot be overstated. A key difference in promotion—compared to selection—is that the unsuccessful candidate continues to work in the organization. Departments around the world have 210

Assessments for Selection and Promotion

experiences with those who wanted to be promoted and were not. When officers feel the system is fair both in terms of procedure and outcome, there are relatively few issues once promotions are made; however, when the procedure and/or outcomes are seen as unfair, many dysfunctional behaviors follow for officers who were unsuccessful. These include lower levels of job satisfaction, disengagement from the job in the form of poor subsequent performance, increased use of time off, and insubordination toward those who were promoted and seen as unworthy.

Validation One step that is critical from both a logical and a legal perspective is the validation of the individual tests that make up the promotion system and the total system as a decision-making process. As indicated in the preceding discussion, promotion systems in law enforcement almost always involve a content validation strategy. This process of validating the tests and the system requires those associated with creating the system to show the job relatedness of each step in the process by linking what is required on the test to what is required on the job. While using a strong content validity approach helps to enhance the quality of the individual assessment tools and ultimately the quality of the promotion decisions that result from using the system, content validation also can be an important step in building trust in the system by those participating. The validation process is beyond the scope of this chapter, but it is clearly a major consideration in building and evaluating an effective promotion system.

Affirmative Action and Promotions There are many misperceptions about affirmative action, but in its truest form it basically states that when two candidates are equal in every way, an organization will take affirmative steps to hire and promote those from underrepresented groups. This puts quite a burden on a promotion system because it requires a conclusion that two candidates are identical in terms of their qualifications. Most police departments rely on promotion scores that result in very fine discriminations so that they do not have to use such things as race or gender as tie breakers as many perceive such a process unfair or biased to a particular group. In contrast, many departments have been under consent decrees or are currently under such mechanisms designed to create greater minority representation among those in promoted positions. Consent decrees are agreements departments make through the legal process to make up for past unfair practices that resulted in lower levels of diversity by promoting individuals who are qualified but may not be quite as qualified as other participants in the system. While these decrees serve a specific important purpose, they also can have the unwanted consequence of decreasing the perceptions of fairness in the overall promotion process. There have been some suggestions for mitigating the perceptions of unfairness with regard to promotion decisions influenced by affirmative action. Previous work by Heilman and colleagues (Heilman, Lucas, & Kaplow, 1990; Heilman, Simon, & Repper, 1987) has suggested that negative reactions to promotional decisions based on affirmative actions can be mitigated by emphasizing the promoted employee’s merits rather than group identity. This would require a follow-up process to the publishing of a promotion list and should include all those who are promoted and not just those who may have received special consideration. In addition, Bobocel and Farrell (1996) suggest that the way in which information about the system is transmitted to participants and future promotional candidates may be more important than how the decision was made. In a study of people’s perceptions of fairness about affirmative action promotion decisions in police department scenarios, explanations of promotional decisions were perceived as most fair when the leadership provided justifications for promotions decisions. More 211

Rick Jacobs, et al.

specifically, participants rated an explanation that “diversifying the workplace would make it more effective” as less fair than an explanation where the woman was promoted over the man because “historically, qualified women did not have the same access to jobs and promotions and we should attempt to reduce this disparity whenever the opportunity is present.” It is possible to increase perceptions of fairness in affirmative action promotion decisions by providing detailed information about the process. Saal and Moore (1993) studied reactions to affirmative action promotions and concluded that the process is problematic and often results in conclusions of unfairness that are difficult to overcome. Their work looked specifically at gender effects and highlighted potential issues facing police departments as they move to better represent women in leadership positions. While the creation of a fair promotion process is difficult, dealing with the outcomes of the system represents additional challenges as we seek to not only promote the right people but also make sure they can do their jobs once promotions are made.

Considerations After Promotions Changes in roles, while most often seen as a very positive step, can be a source of stress for newly promoted officers. Promoted officers need some guidance and support from their superior officers as they make the transition from officer to sergeant or from lieutenant to captain. The interpersonal dynamics in the department will change when an officer is suddenly in a supervisory or leadership position above his or her previous peers or cohorts during academy days. The promotion system can help identify the most talented, but it does not fully prepare them for the new responsibilities and social situations. In many ways, testing is asked to do too much and we ignore the fact that other opportunities exist to enhance the process of successfully moving people into the promoted position. While the test does supply the right people, providing training for the newly promoted officers is an important part of the promotions process and not always the focus of evaluating its overall success. Gaston and King’s (1995) survey of police officers in the United Kingdom revealed recently promoted officers’ views on their formal promotion training. When asked which topics were most useful for training, 90% answered “developing managerial skills,” which included people management, time management, team building and leadership, interpersonal and communication skills, motivation, and appraisal of subordinates. Two-thirds of respondents believed an external management training program, which would cover these broad topics rather than those specific to police work, would be useful because it would bring a wider perspective for their duties. In addition, 78% of respondents had experienced stress in the process, with role ambiguity cited as the most frequent stressor. Many departments neglect this step in their promotion planning and simply sew on the stripes hoping that the required behaviors for the new position will suddenly appear or have been guaranteed through the testing process. Most departments lack the internal training programs or the financial resources to make them available to newly promoted officers. Mentoring programs may also help newly promoted officers cope with their expanded roles. A mentoring program could consist of a more experienced sergeant reviewing the newly promoted sergeant’s performance on a regular basis or being available to answer questions and provide perspective. Within police work, there is a precedent for such activities as most departments have a Field Training Officer assigned to new officers. Mentoring simply moves the process up the organizational hierarchy. The program can be structured by the department, with formally recognized meetings and evaluations, or it can be a more informal process where new sergeants seek out help from those with greater levels of experience as supervisors and leaders. The department may even wish to implement training programs for mentors to support their progress. There are several positive outcomes that can follow from a well-developed and properly implemented promotion process. Officers who receive promotions may have more positive reactions to their work environment. Their new role will provide challenges and opportunities for personal and 212

Assessments for Selection and Promotion

professional growth. A promotion is also an indication that the officer is no longer entry level within the department, and it is a public acknowledgement that he or she has increased value to the organization as well as an affirmation of their leadership qualities. Well-developed promotional systems can have positive consequences for all participants, even those who do not succeed. When officers are required to study materials critical to successful job performance, they increase their job-related abilities. The very nature of studying for the promotion exam can enhance performance of individuals and raise the level of performance for the entire department. For example, officers studying for exams often identify departmental rules and regulations no longer practiced and accordingly update the documentation of operations. Finally, in the best promotional exams, both successful and unsuccessful candidates receive feedback regarding their performance. When properly done, this feedback improves subsequent job performance and can better prepare candidates who wish to compete next time around. Selecting the best officers for promotion is clearly critical to the organization’s future success, but it should not be the only goal of the promotions process. The perceived fairness of the process can have important outcomes for the department. Since the applicants who were not selected will still be members of the department, they may be just as impactful to the organizations as those selected. Unlike other rewards that can be distributed equally to several employees, the difference between a person who receives a promotion and a person who does not can be substantial (Kaplan & Ferris, 2001). Thus, the department has a responsibility to make the promotions system as fair, transparent, and valid as possible.

Gaining Support From System Participants Applicant reactions to any personnel decision-making system are important to consider for multiple reasons. Applicants who react positively to a promotion system tend to view the organization more positively and are more likely to adapt to the new job more quickly. They are also more likely to speak to others about the department in positive terms (Hausknecht et al., 2004). In addition, candidate reactions may be related to filing legal complaints, such that those who perceive the selection or promotion system as being invalid or negative in some way are more likely to file complaints (Hausknecht et al., 2004). From a more cynical perspective, we have often described the outcomes of a poorly developed and implemented sergeant promotion process as resulting in two groups of individuals, new sergeants and plaintiffs. Clearly this overstates the point, but it should be noted that spending time gaining support for the promotion process can have positive outcomes once the list of promotions is made public. While applicants’ reactions are often determined by their outcome in the process (Ryan & Ployhart, 2000), it is possible for applicants to focus attention away from the exclusive consideration of whether they were promoted to the broader and more inclusive perspective of whether the process was fair to all who participated (Truxillo & Bauer, 1999; Truxillo, Bauer, Campion, & Paronto, 2002).

Concluding Thoughts It is simply impossible to overstate the importance of selection and promotion systems in contributing to the effectiveness of a police department. Unlike many other organizations, most if not all of the talent that comes into a police department enters during the selection process for new officers. This puts a premium on the accurate identification of individuals who can perform the difficult and demanding duties of a police officer and develop the skills and abilities necessary to move up the ranks. The same is true of promotions since most departments rely on some form of successful performance at a lower rank as eligibility to move into the next position in the hierarchy. This chapter discussed the importance of treating selection and promotion as processes that have more 213

Rick Jacobs, et al.

than the single objective of accurate assessment of individuals so that employment decision making is based on sound information. Readers thinking about promotion systems in their organizations should consider that in addition to picking the right people for future performance, selection and promotion systems must be acceptable to the participants, help in building the skills and abilities of all individuals participating, and should result in a pool of selected individuals who reflect the department and more broadly the diverse community it serves. The road to success in human resources decision making is filled with potholes that need to be effectively navigated while balancing desired outcomes with best practices to fulfill the needs of the police department.

References and Further Reading Alliger, G. M., & Dwight, S. A. (2000). A meta-analytic investigation of the susceptibility of integrity tests to faking and coaching. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 60, 59–72. Barrick, M. R., & Zimmerman, R. D. (2005). Reducing voluntary, avoidable turnover through selection. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 159–166. Berry, C. M., Gruys, M. L., & Sackett, P. R. (2006). Educational attainment as proxy for cognitive ability in selection: Effects on levels of cognitive ability and adverse impact. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 696–705. Bobko, P., Roth, P. L., & Buster, M. A. (2005). Work sample selection tests and expected reduction in adverse impact: A cautionary note. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 13, 1–10. Bobocel, D. R., & Farrell, A. C. (1996). Sex-based promotion decisions and interactional fairness: Investigating the influence of managerial accounts. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 22–35. Camara, W. J., & Schneider, D. L. (1995). Questions of construct breadth and openness of research in integrity testing. American Psychologist, 50, 459–460. Campion, M. A., Outtz, J. L., Zedeck, S., Schmidt, F. L., Kehoe, J. F., Murphy, K. R., & Guion, R. M. (2001). The controversy over score banding in personnel selection: Answers to 10 key questions. Personnel Psychology, 54, 149–185. Cascio, W. F., Jacobs, R. R., & Silva, J. (2010). Validity, utility and adverse impact: Practical implications from 30 years of data. In J. Outtz (Ed.), Adverse impact. Hoboken, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Chapman, D. S., & Zweig, D. I. (2005). Developing a nomological network for interview structure: Antecedents and consequences of the structured selection interview. Personnel Psychology, 58, 673–702. Clevenger, J., Pereira, G. M., Wiechmann, D., Schmitt, N., & Harvey, V. S. (2001). Incremental validity of situational judgment tests. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 41–417. Cochrane, R. E., Tett, R. P., & Vandecreek, L. (2003). Psychological testing and the selection of police officers. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 30, 511–537. Costa, P. T., Jr., & McRae, R. R. (1992). NEO-PI-R professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Dayan, K., Kasten, R., & Fox, S. (2002). Entry-level police candidate assessment center: An efficient tool or a hammer to kill a fly? Personnel Psychology, 55, 827–849. Detrick, P., & Chibnall, J. T. (2002). Prediction of police officer performance with the Inwald Personality Inventory. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 17, 9–17. Detrick, P., Chibnall, J. T., & Luebbert, M. C. (2004). The revised NEO personality inventory as predictor of police academy performance. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 31, 676–694. Drew, J., Carless, S. A., & Thompson, B. M. (2008). Predicting turnover of police officers using sixteen personality factor questionnaire. Journal of Criminal Justice, 36, 326–331. Ellingson, J. E., Sackett, P. R., & Connelly, B. S. (2007). Personality assessment across selection and development contexts: Insights into response distortion. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 386–395. Ferris, G. R., Frink, D. D., Galang, M. C., Zhou, J., Kacmar, K. M., & Howard, J. L. (1996). Perceptions of organizational politics: Prediction, stress-related implications, and outcomes. Human Relations, 49, 233–266. Gaston, K., & King, L. (1995). Management development and training in the police: A survey of the promotion process. Journal of European Industrial Training, 19, 20–25. Gilliland, S. W. (1993). The perceived fairness of selection systems: An organizational justice perspective. Academy of Management Review, 18, 694–734. Greenberg, J. (1986). Determinants of perceived fairness of performance evaluations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71, 340–342. Guion, R. M. (1998). Assessment, measurement, and prediction for personnel decisions. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Gutman, A. (2004). Ground rules for adverse impact. The Industrial-Organizational Psychologist, 41, 109–119.

214

Assessments for Selection and Promotion Haaland, S., & Christiansen, N. D. (2002). Implications of trait-activation theory for evaluating the construct validity of assessment centre ratings. Personnel Psychology, 55, 137–163. Handler, M., Honts, C. R., Krapohl, D. J., Nelson, R., & Griffin, S. (2009). Integration of pre-employment polygraph screening into the police selection process. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 24, 69–86. Harvey, A. J. (1999). Promotion time: 10 questions to ask yourself before taking the plunge. The Police Chief, 66, 84–87. Hausknecht, J., Day, D., & Thomas, S. (2004). Applicant reactions to selection procedures: An updated model and meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 57, 1–45. Heilman, M. E., Lucas, J. A., & Kaplow, S. R. (1990). Self-derogating consequences of sex-based preferential selection: The moderating role of initial self-confidence. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 46, 202–216. Heilman, M. E., Simon, M. C., & Repper, D. P. (1987). Intentionally favored, unintentionally harmed? Impact of sex based preferential selection on self-perceptions and self-evaluations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72, 62–68. Hogan, J., Barrett, P., & Hogan, R. (2007). Personality measurement, faking, and employment selection. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 1270–1285. Hogan, J., & Brinkmeyer, K. (1997). Bridging the gap between overt and personality-based integrity tests. Personnel Psychology, 50, 587–599. Hough, L. M. (2003). Emerging trends and needs in personality research and practice beyond main effects. In M. B. Barrick & A. M. Ryan (Eds.), Personality and work: Reconsidering the role of personality in organizations (pp. 289–325). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Hough, L. M., Eaton, N. K., Dunnette, M. D., Kamp, J. D., & McCloy, R. A. (1990). Criterion-related validity of personality constructs and the effect of response distortion on those validities. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 581–895. Huffcutt, A. I., Conway, J. M., Roth, P. L., & Stone, N. J. (2001). Identification and meta-analytic assessment of psychological constructs measured in employment interviews. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 897–913. Inwald, R. (1988). Five-year follow-up study of department terminations as predicted by 16 pre-employment psychological indicators. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73, 703–710. Inwald, R. (1992). Inwald Personality Inventory technical manual (revised). New York: Hilson Research. Kaplan, D. M., & Ferris, G. R. (2001). Fairness perceptions of employee promotion systems: A two-study investigation of antecedents and mediators. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 31, 1204–1222. Lilienfeld, S. O., Alliger, G., & Mitchell, K. (1995). Why integrity testing remains controversial. American Psychologist, 50, 457–458. Lonsway, K. A. (2003). Tearing down the wall: Problems with consistency, validity, and adverse impact of physical agility testing in police selection. Police Quarterly, 6, 237–277. Mael, F., Connerley, M., & Morath, R. A. (1996). None of your business: Parameters of biodata invasiveness. Personnel Psychology, 49, 613–650. Martin, S. E., & Jurik, N. C. (1996). Doing justice, doing gender: Women in law and criminal justice occupations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Maurer, T., Solamon, J., & Troxtel, D. (1998). Relationship of coaching with performance in situational employment interviews. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 128–136. McDaniel, M. A., Morgeson, F. P., Finnegan, E. B., Campion, M. A., & Braverman, E. P. (2001). Use of situational judgment tests to predict job performance: A clarification of the literature. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 730–740. McFarland, L. A., & Ryan, A. M. (2000). Variance in faking across noncognitive measures. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 812–821. McHenry, J. J., Hough, L. M., Toquam, J. L., Hanson, M. A., & Ashworth, S. (1990). Project A validity results: The relationship between predictor and criterion domains. Personnel Psychology, 43, 335–355. Mikulay, S. M., & Goffin, R. D. (1998). Measuring and predicting counterproductivity in the laboratory using integrity and personality testing. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 58, 768–790. Murphy, K. R. (1993). Honesty in the workplace. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole. Neuman, G. A., & Baydoun, R. (1998). An empirical examination of overt and covert integrity tests. Journal of Business and Psychology, 13, 65–79. Posthuma, R. A., Morgeson, F. P., & Campion, M. A. (2002). Beyond employment interview validity: A comprehensive narrative review of recent research and trends over time. Personnel Psychology, 55, 1–81. Pynes, J., & Bernardin, H. J. (1992). Entry-level police selection: The assessment center is an alternative. Journal of Criminal Justice, 20, 41–52. Ryan, A. M., & Ployhart, R. E. (2000). Applicants’ perceptions of selection procedures and decisions: A critical review and agenda for the future. Journal of Management, 26, 565–606.

215

Rick Jacobs, et al. Saal, F. E., & Moore, S. C. (1993). Perceptions of promotion fairness and promotion candidates’ qualifications. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 105–110. Sacco, J. M., Scheu, C. R., Ryan, A. M., & Schmitt, N. (2003). An investigation of race and sex similarity effects in interviews: A multilevel approach to relational demography. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 852–865. Sackett, P. R., Burris, L. R., & Callahan, C. (1989). Integrity testing for personnel selection: An update. Personnel Psychology, 42, 491–529. Sackett, P. R., De Corte, W., & Lievens, F. (2010). Decision aids for addressing the validity-adverse impact tradeoff. In J. L. Outtz (Ed.), Adverse impact: Implications for organizational staffing and high stakes selection (pp. 453–472). New York: Routledge. Sackett, P. R., Schmitt, N., Ellingson, J. E., & Kabin, M. B. (2001). High-stakes testing in employment, credentialing, and higher education: Prospects in a post-affirmative action world. American Psychologist, 56, 302–318. Sackett, P. R., & Wilk, S. L. (1994). Within-group norming and other forms of score adjustment in preemployment testing. American Psychologist, 49, 929–954. Scarborough, K. E., Van Tubergen, G. N., Gaines, L. K., & Whitlow, S. S. (1999). An examination of police officers’ motivation to participate in the promotional process. Police Quarterly, 2, 302–320. Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (1998). The validity and utility of selection methods in personnel psychology: Practical and theoretical implications of 85 years or research findings. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 262–274. Scogin, F., Schumacher, J., Gardner, J., & Chaplin, W. (1995). Predictive validity of psychological testing in law enforcement settings. Professional Psychological Research and Practice, 26, 68–71. Scrivner, E. (2001). Innovations in police recruitment and hiring: Hiring in the spirit of service. Washington, DC: Community of Oriented Police Service, U.S. Department of Justice. Thornton, G. C., III, & Rupp, D. E. (2006). Assessment centers in human resource management: Strategies for prediction, diagnosis, and development. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Truxillo, D. M., & Bauer, T. N. (1999). Applicant reactions to test score banding in entry-level and promotional contexts. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 322–339. Truxillo, D. M., Bauer, T. N., Campion, M. A., & Paronto, M. E. (2002). Selection fairness information and applicant reactions: A longitudinal field study. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 1020–1031. Wiener, J. (1994). Physical abilities test: Follow-up validation study. Sacramento, CA: POST.

216

10 THE INTEGRATION SECTION OF FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGICAL REPORTS IN LAW ENFORCEMENT Culturally Responsive Ending Words Ronn Johnson Introduction The integration section (I-Section) is a critical part of most forensic psychological evaluation reports. This is especially true for reports written for law enforcement personnel. The I-Section refers to that portion of the report in which the examiner analyzes, prioritizes, and synthesizes significant elements of the evaluation data in order to render opinions leading to recommendations (Melton, Petrila, Polythress, and Slobogin, 2007; Sattler, 1988). In general, writing the I-Section involves crafting a conceptual model of the examinee that addresses questions about the individual’s functioning in a number of different domains. The I-Section relies on various sources to support the examiner’s opinions and recommendations (Ackerman, 2006). Multiple data sources are used in the I-Section to support evidence-based explanations for findings that might otherwise appear vague or weakly supported (Cates, 1999). Integrated reports rely on good clinical judgment skills (Groth-Marnat & Horvath, 2006), and the I-Section is a measure of the standard of practice used by forensic psychologists when conducting psychological interviews, for example, pre-employment evaluations, special unit assignments, and fitness-for-duty (FFD) examinations. For instance, did the examiner adhere to the ethical demands of forensic assessments, for example, providing notification to the examinee regarding the purposes of the evaluation and any limitations on confidentiality? The I-Section is not, however, the final word. One important objective of the I-Section is to provide clear and convincing evidence that persuasively communicates the examiner’s “culturally responsive ending words” represented in her or his opinions and recommendations (Melton et al., 2007). This objective is best achieved by establishing an evidence-based (i.e., data-driven) foundation in the report (Goodheart, Kazdin, & Sternberg, 2006). The I-Section is written in a conclusory framework. It is used to help agency/departmental decision makers or legal authorities become more fully informed and knowledgeable consumers of forensic psychological evaluations (International Association of Chiefs of Police [IACP], 1998). Several factors underscore the importance of the I-Section. Agency/departmental decision makers must be able to (1) understand the importance of the psychologist’s opinions and recommendations, (2) understand the basic principles of the forensic psychological evaluation process and the specific

217

Ronn Johnson

role of the I-Section, and (3) evaluate the quality of the written psychological report via the I-Section. The I-Section is thus an extension of applicable laws and codes or relevant departmental policies. Special care must be taken when writing the I-Section. First, the ethical principles and guidelines for forensic psychologists (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) speak to issues relevant to the I-Section. APA codes direct psychologists to base their opinions on information and techniques sufficient to substantiate their findings. Psychologists provide such opinions only after they have conducted a thorough examination of the examinee adequate to support their final conclusions (i.e., ending words) in the I-Section. The ending words of the I-Section must be communicated in ways that promote understanding through a forceful presentation of the data on which the conclusion is based (Melton et al., 2007). The specific content, structure, format, and substance are determined by the relevant psycho-legal issue(s) at hand as well as the laws or rules in the jurisdiction in which the work was completed (American Board of Forensic Psychology [ABFP], 2009). Second, the assessment points for I-Section content are best anchored to a widely recognized system of job-relevant domains, for example, the California Commission on Peace Officer Standards and Training (POST). Thus, the psychological screening dimensions for a patrol officer should include positive and counterproductive behaviors that must be included as the foundation for recommendations in the I-Section. Aamodt (2004) reviewed investigations using personality variables to assess different police performance outcomes and stressed the importance of personal dispositions for predicting performance in these contexts. POST facilitates the psychologist’s use of job-relevant dimensions when crafting the I-Section. Third, concerns about negligent hire or retention issues later manifest themselves through officerinvolved shootings, excessive use-of-force charges, collateral injuries or death secondary to pursuit chases, inattention to detail, off-duty misconduct, or otherwise rude behaviors. For example, the I-Section assumes the added burden of trying to avoid a potential negligent hire or retention; the impact may not surface until five years after the report is written ( Johnson & Scott, 2009). Regardless of the specific standard a forensic psychologist may elect to use, legal consequences to examinees’ behavior can be traced back to the psychological component of the hiring process. Any perceived officer misconduct can serve as a basis for a lawsuit. Legal pitfalls or the threat of liability underscore the pivotal role the I-Section can play (Melton et al., 2007; Davis & Rostow, 2004). The I-Section outlines the basis of psychological decisions and recommendations made by forensic psychologists when evaluating personnel for safety sensitive positions. In the end, an agency/ department relies on the opinions and recommendations made in the I-Section, which can point to aggressive behavior, poor judgment, or other dangerous propensities. An agency/department may be liable for negligent hiring if it knew (or should have known) that the officer posed a threat to others at the post-job offer phase, at the FTO phase, or later. Likewise, an agency/department can be held liable for negligent retention if it continues to employ an officer knowing of his or her problematic behavioral characteristics. In the end, the I-Section should be written as if it is a legal document that will be read in open court (Melton et al., 2007). Forensic psychologists writing I-Sections must keep an eye on the elements that play a role in reducing negligent hiring issues. An awareness of negligent hiring and retention can be enhanced through several ethically based factors. First, there is a duty owed by the agency/department in selecting or retaining an officer; second, a breach of that duty can occur as a consequence of an officer’s on- or off-duty behavior; and third, that breach of duty could result in an injury to a third party, and the agency/department and psychologist could be liable for damages (Knapp & VandeCreek, 2006). The examiner’s opinions, recommendations, suitability ratings, and reservations must be presented in the I-Section in a transparent way. There is cause for some relief when we note that there is only a relatively low percentage of problem officers, as the Christopher Commission found in an investigation of the Los Angeles Police Department (LAPD) after the 1991 beating of Rodney King. The Commission noted that fewer than one-half of 1% of LAPD officers accounted for 15% of 218

I-Section Reports in Law Enforcement

citizen complaints of excessive force or improper tactics. Moreover, the Commission noted that this disproportion could not be explained by assignment or arrest rates, implying these problem officers simply had a proclivity toward deviance (Independent Commission on the Los Angeles Police Department, 1991). Police misconduct is, however, low in comparison to the general population. The controversy over peace officer misconduct is fueled by the high-profile nature of the resulting media coverage due to the perceived breach of public trust. As we will discuss in more detail, forensic psychologists must remain attentive to cultural, ethnic, gender, race, and sexual orientation issues as they conduct evaluations and craft the I-Section. Cross-culturally, the standards for peace officers are much higher than that for civilians (Borum, Super, & Rand, 2003; Rostow & Davis, 2004). The I-Section must be crafted in a way that reflects the advanced clinical judgment of the psychologist and represents a convergence of practice and science. It is the most analytically complex portion of the pre-employment psychological report. The assessment is usually accomplished through an examination of the examinee’s attitudes, emotions, overt behaviors, and personal disposition considered relevant for the position. The I-Section functions like a report (within a report) on risk assessment issues, but it is not a summary of the main report ( Johnson, 2009; Tallent, 1988). General factors are examined to strengthen the psychologist’s conclusions and recommendations in the I-Section. It should be noted that it is wise to never stitch together hunches and speculation, which serve only to weaken the credibility of the examiner’s recommendations. Nietzel and Hartung (1993) stress that an officer’s ability to perform his or her various job duties, as well as handle the interpersonal demands, must be presented through convincing, evidenced-based arguments. The examiner’s skill in synthesizing, analyzing, accepting, and rejecting hypotheses while offering a clear and convincing rationale for opinions offered is a sine qua non of the I-Section. Challenges to the examiners’ opinions can originate from any number of sources. For instance, applicant appeals, internal departmental reviews, and even possible litigation stem from the actions taken by an officer recommended by a forensic psychologist (Borum et al., 2003). An examiner must assume that the I-Section may serve as the basis for a script outline used by an opposing attorney while scrutinizing the work of the examiner (Melton et al., 2007). The format and scope of the I-Section may be the subject of debate and widespread variation, depending on the skill set of the examiner, the purpose of the report, and departmental needs. The forensic psychologist must operate in a system when a nonpsychologist/nonadministrator dictates the scope of work in the report area. The goal of this chapter is to offer guidance to qualified police psychologists on how to approach crafting the I-Section. The clinical judgment of the psychologist working in this context of report writing cannot be ethically abandoned through the clerical expediency of a non-research-based report template. Nor should computerized reports that identify large samples be used in the absence of independent review through exhaustive studies in peer-reviewed journals for those computer-based reports ( Johnson, 2009). Each I-Section must be individualized for the examinee based on the parameters of POST, IACP, relevant code/statutes, culturally responsive assessment techniques, and evidence-based, structured clinical judgment (Melton et al., 2007). This chapter is organized into three sections. The first section discusses the cultural context in which the I-Section is crafted. The second section provides information on the basic principles and a forensic decision-making model (FDM) for the I-Section. Finally, conclusions and implications for training and research are discussed.

Cultural Issues and the I-Section Culture matters in police and peace officer work. Current psychometric practice must be culturally responsive for several reasons. First, APA ethical standards, ethnic minority standards, and ABFP 219

Ronn Johnson

Culture of the examinee

Police culture

Cultures of populations served

Culture of the examiner

Figure 10.1 Overlapping cultures in forensic evaluations

guidelines stress culture as a core examiner competency, and cultural responsiveness should be exercised through the preparation of an I-Section of pre-employment examinations and FFDEs (APA, 2002; Rostow & Davis, 2004). Second, despite efforts to objectify and standardize the evaluation process, examiners are not immune from attitudes or biases in their work with diverse cultural groups. For instance, research has revealed evidence that blacks are misdiagnosed by being evaluated as paranoid and dangerous (Adebimpe, 1981; Levinson & York, 1974). Third, there is a paucity of literature that examines culture and decision making in forensic psychology. Finally, it cannot be assumed that diversity and multicultural training is widespread in forensic psychology. Some examiners confuse their exposure to diverse groups with diversity credibility and cross-cultural competence that could not be impeached when dealing with ethnoracial issues (Ridley, 2005). The I-Section must be written with a firm understanding of at least four overlapping cultures. These cultures include the police, the diverse populations served, and the examinee’s culture as well as that of the examiner (see Figure 10.1).

Police Cultural Factors as a Psychological Filter for Police Work The I-Section highlights the known cultural and racial factors to which the examinee will need to respond in the scope of her or his police work (Covington, 2001; Russell, 2001; Weitzer & Tuch, 2002, 2004). The police—or blue—culture (Woody, 2005) is dominated by a white male heterosexual ethos. A police environment plays an important role in shaping the attitudes of recruits. However, it is probably not rational to expect academy or FTO experiences to somehow program police to remain unaffected by the diversity of the communities where they serve. Figures on diversity suggest noticeable increases in the ethnic/racial, sexual orientation, and number of women in police departments in cities of more than 250,000 people (Doss, 1990; Messerschmidt, 1993; Sklansky, 2006). From a culturally relevant risk assessment standpoint, the questions become, “To what extent does an examinee identify with police culture?” and “To what extent has— or can—an examinee acculturated (or will acculturate) as needed for work in law enforcement?” For example, a pre-employment applicant applying for a highway patrol position with excessive speeding tickets may be identified as someone who has failed to internalize the value of compliance with the law. The I-Section may report that an applicant’s decisions may convert values held into behaviors that may function to the benefit or detriment of individuals of a particular group (Sue & Sue, 2008). The larger issue for the police culture may be the extent to which there could be conflict 220

I-Section Reports in Law Enforcement

between the police culture and an examinee’s gender, sexual orientation, and/or ethnic/racial group (Rostow & Davis, 2004). The latter point is reinforced by high-profile incidents involving police, as reflected in Table 10.1. The net effect of incidents such as those in Table 10.1 reveals problematic behaviors with strong cultural, gender, and racial overtones. Forensic mental health examiners must be able to justify how these cultural matters were assessed.

Cultures of Police Examinees Some police officers might have hidden bias and hostility, which places all officers at risk while undermining public trust (Weitzer & Tuch, 2002; Woody, 2005). For example, how does a young Latino male encountering the police for the first time know if he has met a good officer or a closet racist? It is this uncertainty that creates so many cultural collisions and mishaps (Weitzer & Tuch, 2006). If the high-profile cases presented in Table 10.1 reflect the true feelings of some officers in a department, then it is likely that these biases/prejudices could seep out while officers are on the job.

Table 10.1 High-Profile Cross-Cultural, Race, and Gender Incidents in Police Work • A police chief was ordered held without bail on charges that he tried to cover up the fatal beating of a Mexican immigrant by a group of white teenagers. One of the officers involved in the incident dated the mother of one of the accused and another officer’s son played on the football team. Another officer was indicted separately in a scheme to extort money from illegal gambling operations. • Several female officers were sexually harassed by male police personnel over the course of many years. Four sergeants and an officer made sexually explicit remarks and unwanted sexual advances toward the women, including fondling and rubbing against them. Pornographic videos were allegedly shown in the workplace. Each woman suffered emotional distress and related symptoms. • A lawsuit was filed by a gay police officer who alleged that his peers harassed him, including placing a gay escort ad in his locker, stating that he only handles gay sex crimes, and suggesting that he was infected with HIV. • In a suit, a white police officer asserted that he was the victim of a years-long police department practice of racial set-asides for the position of assistant chief for which he had applied. He went on to state that he was “the most qualified person in the department for the job last year but that the position was given to an officer who is Hispanic.” The plaintiff had been with the department since 1979, three years longer than the Hispanic officer selected. • While in police custody, a suspect of Puerto Rican descent was beaten to death and then hung from the bars of a holding cell to make it appear as if he had committed suicide. The coroner accepted the chief ’s explanation that bruises had occurred when he resisted arrest. An autopsy arranged by the family of the deceased confirmed that he had “suffered extensive, massive injuries consistent with a profound beating. The defendant did not die of hanging,” the civil lawsuit claimed. • Ethnic minority officers complained about being harshly disciplined and receiving fewer promotions. Officers who used racial slurs against blacks were not disciplined by the chief. After lodging their complaints, they were sent for psychological exams. Each officer had previously passed their routine annual psychological evaluations. One of the complaining officers had passed his exam just one month earlier. This time, they all failed the psychological exam and lost their jobs as police officers. The same psychologist who had repeatedly passed them for years now failed them. • A mounting pile of lawsuits alleged there were falsified arrests, harassment, and targeting of LGBT (lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender) drivers. An officer was accused of civil rights violations by falsifying DUI (driving under the influence) charges and other traffic violations. The same officer also allegedly used excessive force and verbally harassed those he reportedly targeted, and the officer allegedly was motivated to falsify charges by the hope to gain more overtime pay for time spent in court. The officer had reportedly made nearly 600 DUI arrests since 2006.

221

Ronn Johnson

In terms of the police culture, the I-Section asks what process is used to assess an officer’s biases and measure its potential impact on the performance of job-relevant duties (Woody, 2005). The I-Section may offer information regarding the question “Do individual from different cultures execute police duties differently?” The culture of the examinee may overlap with the police culture in that it could be an FFDE, a lateral transfer, or the individual might have grown up in a law enforcement family. In sum, an examiner must remain aware of police culture when crafting the I-Section (Rostow & Davis, 2004; Woody, 2005).

Cultures of Diverse Populations Served and the I-Section The suitability criteria of police officers require that they have the ability to effectively communicate with the diverse populations served (Scrivner, 2006). For example, in an investigation involving Boston’s 10-Point Coalition, Winship and Berrien (1999, p. 67) found that the Boston Police Department (BPD) could secure support from African American churches during gang crackdowns. Ethnoracial credibility was established by the BPD through behaviors consistent with fairness and respect previously agreed upon with church elders. The I-Section should contain a projected risk assessment as to how an examinee can be expected to interact with the diverse groups served by the agency/department. The examinee’s cross-cultural experiences, competencies, diversity conflicts, and dispositions must be highlighted (Ridley, 2005; Sue & Sue, 2008). For example, the Cambridge police officer who arrested a college professor at his residence under the assumption that breaking and entering occurred had the perception of a verbally aggressive suspect. How would a psychologist assess such an officer (assuming this same officer would have been referred for an FFDE in the aftermath of such an incident)? High-profile incidents such as the Cambridge case raise concerns about police–community relations and ethnoracial attitudes toward the police and police behavior. These incidents contain a racial flavor due in large part to preexisting tensions between the police and citizens. A history of differential treatment of African Americans has fueled more negative attitudes toward the police among African Americans than whites ( Jordan, Gabbidon, & Higgins, 2009). The psychosocial basis is both historical and recent and has its origins in the role of the police in enforcing the rule of law as well as African Americans’ ethnoracial status. Historically, the relationship between African Americans and the police has been problematic, ranging from tense to openly oppositional (Howell, Perry, & Vile, 2004). The I-Section can offer insights into the ethnoracial window of the examinee as it relates to either issues brought about by the need for an evaluation or factors likely to have an unwanted impact on interracial exchanges (Gabbidon & Higgins, 2009; Woody, 2005). The crosscultural objective of the I-Section is to provide sufficient coverage and support for the ethnoracial or gender-related opinions of the examiner.

Culture of the Examiner and the I-Section Race is an important predictor of perceptions, and it is it implicitly discriminatory to ignore this reality during an evaluation (Weitzer & Tuch, 2004). Each examiner must assume a nondefensive posture and remain receptive to the possibility that ethnoracial factors are operating during the forensic evaluations. For example, findings that suggest that health professionals may hold unconscious preferences and stereotypes that, in turn, sway clinical decisions (Green et al., 2007). These types of factors should prompt the examiner to explore biases or other ethnoracially conscious behaviors that could affect his or her decisions about an examinee (Sue & Sue, 2008). Culturally informed examiners remain aware of the potential for unintentional behaviors that can lead to decisions that could have inequitable outcomes for examinees (Ridley, 2005). Probably the most ill-advised action, on the part of the examiner, would be to discount or marginalize the relevance of these issues in the structured 222

I-Section Reports in Law Enforcement

decision-making process. The examiner may need to confront a range of referral issues with ethnoracial elements. These ethnoracial and job-relevant factors must be communicated in the I-Section in a way that assists decision makers. The strength of the I-Section is contingent on the use of several data sources through a self-imposed ethnoracial filter (Ridley, 2005; Sue & Sue, 2008). The level of detail and amount of attention in the I-Section devoted to this area should be sufficient to cover an applicant’s/examinee’s cultural issues as they would affect her or his anticipated duties. At a minimum, the forensic psychologist must acknowledge—and be able to defend— attention to relevant cultural factors. It is unethical to write a statement such as “The examiner or evaluation did not reveal any cultural or racial factors relevant to this examinee.” This or similarly worded statements leave the examiner open to having to explain why he or she failed to consider cultural factors brought up in any appeal or legal hearing.

Decision-Making Model for Writing the I-Section After an examiner has gathered large amounts of data on an examinee, she or he should ask the question, “What do I do to make sense of this information in order to make the best recommendations?” To answer this question, an examiner must have a precise understanding of the purpose of the evaluation. Moreover, an examiner must combine knowledge of police culture, clinical problem solving, and her or his understanding of several accepted standards in order to craft a useful I-Section (ABFP, 2010; Bersoff, 1995). A few forensic psychology ethical decision-making models help clarify the reasoning process from a forensic psychological context. APA, IACP, and AAFP stress the integration of culture to buttress the defensibility of the decision- making steps when applied to diverse examinees. The following is a description of 10 chief considerations (i.e., interrelated FDM sources) that can be used when writing the I-Section. Use of the FDM allows the examiner to ask the most fundamental I-Section question: What types and sources of data did the examiner use during the process of developing the recommendations? It is worth noting that the majority of these domains allow for the incorporation of cultural factors. This is especially relevant for examiners with limited diversity and cross-cultural training. Figure 10.2 provides an FDM for the I-Section. These components include data that address the significant content and quality of the recommendations made. The decision-making process can be broken down into the above domains. Although not all of these 10 FDM domains are always available, if accessed, they allow for a more structured judgment. It is important to remember that the I-Section is not a summary of the report but sharply

FFDE or other referral questions about the examinee

Relevant laws, codes, and statutes

POST

Specific job tasks

Psychological testing

Psychological interviews

Background history reports

Polygraph, CSVA, or other integrity checks

Conclusary opinions based on all the above info

Recommendations

Figure 10.2 Forensic decision-making model (FDM) for the I-Section

223

Ronn Johnson

focused responses to the reasons for the referral. The FDM is designed to guide the examiner when considering data relevant to the evaluation demands (Melton et al., 2007; Simon & Shuman, 2002).

Step 1. FFDE or Other Referral Questions About the Examinee This first step is included because the approach and organization of the evaluation are largely determined by the nature of the referral question(s) (i.e., references). For pre-employment evaluations, the question is usually based on suitability and stability. Other forensic evaluations and FFDEs have more complicated psycho-legal questions that must be clearly spelled out and understood by the examiner as they are communicated back to authorized stakeholders. An examiner must assess any cultural or gender factors relevant to the referral questions (APA, 2002; Melton et al., 2007; Sattler, 1988).

Step 2. Identify All Relevant POST Standards POST is probably the first reference standard for most police personnel evaluations (POST, 2008). POST is cited by IACP and is widely used by experienced forensic psychologists working in law enforcement. Several areas of work-related psychological functioning should be reviewed in light of several domains in this model. These psychological POST dimensions often include social competence/teamwork, adaptability/flexibility, conscientiousness/dependability, impulse control/attention to safety, integrity/ethics, emotional regulation/stress tolerance, decision making/judgment, assertiveness/persuasiveness, and avoiding substance abuse and other risk-taking behavior (POST, 2008; Trompetter, 1998). For a problematic examinee, usually several POST elements should be considered for their applicability. Police officers encounter job-specific operational stressors such as exposure to danger, threats, and trauma (e.g., accident scenes and dealing with death), and dealing with violence and abusive members of the public (Anshel & Kaissidis, 1997; Thompson, Kirk, & Brown, 2001). Management stressors reflect more common organizational stressors, for example work overload, staff shortages, poor communication, and perceived lack of support (Finn, Talucci, & Wood, 2000). An examiner must assess any cultural or gender factors relevant to the POST issues as well as personal stress and work-related stress boundaries.

Step 3. Identify All Relevant Laws, Codes, and Statutes As previously noted, the evaluation has possible psycho-legal implications and the I-Section must be addressed in the pre-employment, FFDE, or special unit assignment evaluations (Rostow & Davis, 2004). For example, departments operating under some type of POST mandate have the authority to establish minimum selection standards for peace officers employed by agencies that participate in the POST program. It is essential for the psychologist writing the report to know the department’s statutory requirements (Rostow & Davis, 2004). Interpreted properly, the data presented in these codes or statutes usually specify competence domains. Awareness of such codes or statutes can probably increase the frequency with which the components of competence are cited by evaluators in the I-Section (Goldstein, Morse, & Shapiro, 2003).

Step 4. Specific Job Tasks All of the examiner’s opinions and recommendations should be linked to departmental job tasks (Borum et al., 2003). All departments have written job descriptions that outline the specific duties of a particular position (Rostow & Davis, 2004). The human resources department’s written policies may spell out in detail the job tasks. These written job tasks are critical assessment areas for use in the I-Section. The examiner must highlight all job tasks relevant to the evaluation as well as explain why 224

I-Section Reports in Law Enforcement

the actions—or lack thereof—of the examinee have been called into question (as in the case of an FFDE). These job tasks may also be referenced in the I-Section by a psychologist in FFDEs in order to anchor concerns about an officer evaluated as being not fit for duty. For example, police officers regularly work in highly stressful conditions. Therefore, an assessment review would speak to how the examinee anticipates job stress or how job stress could affect the examinee’s performance. Signs of vulnerability to stress may be reflected in a number of data sources, for example, psychological testing or financial stress (Berkun, 2000; Lieberman, Tharion, ShukittHale, Speckman, & Tulley, 2002). In such a case, the examiner could question the extent to which an examinee is able to manage the stress from home and the job or if they can be restored to full functioning with mandatory treatment (in the case of an FFDE). An examiner would highlight the reaction of an examinee to prolonged exposure to stress stemming from police work. For example, an officer was referred for an FFDE because of an incident in which he pointed his service revolver at a sergeant in a glass-enclosed communication center because he thought the dispatcher did not provide him with enough information on a man with a gun call. When asked about this incident during an FFDE, the officer replied, “I yelled at him. ‘You son-of-a-bitch. You could have got me killed.’ ” In this case, the I-Section highlighted the officer’s personal stress (from financial issues) that was the result of him working security at several off-duty events. Sanders, Hughes, and Langworthy (1995) report that police officers must be psychologically fit to cope with various stressors. The I-Section would be further strengthened by citing relevant literature on psychological concerns related to any examinee.

Step 5. Psychological Testing The I-Section must provide a focused discussion of the results of psychological tests used by the examiner (Curran, 1998; IACP, PPS, 1998; Rostow & Davis, 2004). Several tests have been used in police personnel work. An examiner may need to use additional tests to address the unique requirements of some departments or positions, or various FFDE referral issues. The tests most commonly used include MMPI-2, MMPI-2-RF, 16PF, CPI, PAI, and IPI. In practice, the I-Section would highlight relevant elevations and de-elevations to address relevant psycho-legal questions concerning why the examinee was referred. The I-Section requires the examiner to link the psychological testing with corresponding behaviors, interviews, and job tasks (Melton et al., 2007; Sattler, 1988). The discussion of the test results would be strengthened by references to research-based interpretations (i.e., independent peer-reviewed journals). The I-Section also requires some discussion of miss rates for various tests when they are used with various ethnic, gender, and racial groups ( Johnson, 2009). In this case, an examiner would need to be prepared to defend her or his choice of such instruments, to demonstrate that the instruments chosen are valid predictors of job performance and that the instruments chosen do not adversely affect minority groups protected by federal legislation (Inwald, 1988). Each test and its resulting score must be examined for its relevance for use with a particular examinee, for example, use of nongendered norms for the MMPI-2 (Graham, 2006). The I-Section discusses opinions drawn from the psychological testing. Special attention should be devoted to any ethnoracial factor that attenuates a more traditional interpretation of the test results. In sum, psychological testing is a major evidence-based source when linked to other elements in the FDM, such as psychological interviews.

Step 6. Psychological Interviews Interviews are one of the primary in vivo data sources for the I-Section. FFDEs may rely on thirdparty information from coworkers, supervisors, other mental professionals, relatives, or spouses in 225

Ronn Johnson

order to formulate opinions and recommendation (Heilbrun, Rosenfeld, Warren, & Collins, 1994; Melton et al., 2007; Rostow & Davis, 2004). The I-Section should include relevant interview data and observations that pertain to the referral questions or the reasons for the evaluation. Examiners may tailor questions for interview subjects that relate to a specific psychological concern such as anger problems or harsh treatment to certain diverse groups, for example, “Tell me about the times you have observed John lose his temper.” Cultural information that may affect the evaluation must be explained in the I-Section (Weiner, 2006). The examiner should note any significant unsolicited disclosures, comments, or questions from the examinee that are relevant to the evaluation. After the examiner reviews negative history, any details pertinent to the evaluation should be noted in the I-Section. The examinee’s reactions and statements made when confronted with discrepancies or self-serving efforts to soften the impact of negative information should also be noted (Melton et al., 2007). Cross-cultural and other communication issues stemming from questioning techniques affect the examiner’s perceptions of the examinee’s interview style. The I-Section can be used to reveal behavioral characteristics that may point to attitudinal clashes (Scrivner, 2006). And, as noted above, the examiner must have insight into her or his own unexamined cultural assumptions (Ridley, 2005; Sue & Sue, 2008), which can alter the examiner’s perceptions of the examinee. For example, interview behavior noted in the I-Section must be first decoded by the examiner in a culturally responsive way (Matsumoto, 1989; Ridley, 2005). An examiner who is cross-culturally competent in working this domain of the FDM could have advantages over an examiner who is not (Matsumoto, 2002).

Step 7. Background History Reports An accurate and comprehensive background history report usually offers a more longitudinal view of the examinee (Owens, 1976). Studies have demonstrated that background data are effective predictors for a variety of human performance domains (Owens, 1976; Reilly & Chao, 1982). The department may use some type of computerized form for collecting the relevant history, through either a personal history questionnaire (PHQ) or a personal history statement (PHS). This I-Section domain also functions as a foundation for opinions and recommendations. The same background report may also allow the examiner to link the I-Section to psychological testing and POST. For FFDEs, performance reviews and FTO evaluations may also be useful (Rostow & Davis, 2004). Again, the I-Section is not a summary, and the examinee’s history must be analyzed for details that inform the opinions of the examiner. The I-Section is also where history that challenges or contradicts the examiner’s opinions should be reported. This review includes inaccuracies or inconsistencies in the background history report, which must, of course, be discussed as well. In this case, the significant history contained in the I-Section must be communicated in a way that addresses the purpose of the evaluation while offering a justification for giving more or less decision-making weight to some information (Melton et al., 2007).

Step 8. Polygraph, CSVA, or Other Integrity Checks Forensic psychologists encounter referral evaluation questions that require integrity to be addressed (Melton et al., 2007; Simon & Shuman, 2002). Integrity is a critical component in law enforcement personnel selection as well. The polygraph or other integrity measures (e.g., CSVA) are used by departments during the pre-employment phase of the hiring process. There is considerable controversy about the use of the polygraph, which has resulted in some states limiting its use (Employee Polygraph Protection Act of 1988; Lykken, 1981; Sackett & Decker, 1979; Saxe, Dougherty, & Cross, 1985). Although national security concerns have resulted in some federal exemptions for the use of the polygraph, polygraphs do not have strong support based on Daubert and Frye considerations 226

I-Section Reports in Law Enforcement

(Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, 1993). Nevertheless, despite concerns about its use, polygraphs are used in law enforcement and national security situations. In terms of the I-Section, the examiner may use issues identified in the polygraph or CSVA as cautionary points. These issues must, however, be yoked to evidence that substantiates the integrity issue. No examiner should base his or her opinions or recommendations relying primarily on results from these integrity measures.

Step 9. Conclusory Opinions Based on All the Above Information The information gathered during the evaluation process is analyzed and broken down into reasonable conclusions, usually stated as opinions (Kellerman & Burry, 1981). These opinions are not data (Shapiro, 1991): the strength of the opinion(s) is based on psychologically relevant facts that eventually led to the examiner’s recommendation(s). Any weakness in the methods used, such as questionable psychometric properties of tests, will weaken the examiner’s opinions. The examiner’s ending words are not the last word in the process because examinee may hire her or his own experts, or a department-ordered internal second (and third) opinion may come into play. Conclusory opinions must have a traceable connection to the body of the report. In other words, one of the ways of evaluating the quality of the I-Section is the extent to which it explains the reasoning that forms the basis for an examiner’s opinion (Skeem, Golding, Cohen, & Berge, 1998). I-Section opinions must offer full acknowledgement of the original reason(s) for the evaluation such as circumscribed stated intent of FFDEs, pre-employment, or special unit assignments (APA, 2002; Committee on Ethical Guidelines for Forensic Psychologists, 1991). Weiner (2006) offers cogent guidance that is particularly relevant for the I-Section when he recommends the conclusions should be written in a “clear, relevant, informative, and defensible manner.” An examiner should also avoid offering expansive opinions in the I-Section that are not within the scope of the original referral circumstance (Melton et al., 2007). In terms of the format of the conclusory opinions, the I-Section might include a brief statement about the standards (e.g., POST, psycho-legal, or job descriptions) used in pre- employment or the listed concerns resulting in an FFDE. A failure to focus on a narrowly defined scope for the evaluation may result in the report assessed as insufficient (Melton et al., 2007). Another approach could include a breakdown of each referral question followed by the examiner’s opinion regarding that particular issue. The examiner’s analysis of the data contained in the report must offer a cogent description of the relationship between the findings and opinions (Melton et al., 2007). The opinion should contain a brief evidence-based rationale that would be discoverable through an independent report review. Self-imposed questions could include “What were the key single-themed questions about this examinee?” or “Considering the issues resulting in this evaluation and the data you collected, what are your opinions regarding this examinee?” Each opinion should have a risk assessment rating (e.g., low risk, medium risk, or high risk) for problematic issues identified in the opinions. The risk assessment should include a cultural risk assessment rating as appropriate. I-Section conclusions will be strengthened by actual quotes from the research used to support the opinions presented (Ackerman, 2006). It is assumed that the examiner has adequate data on which she or he can base opinions and has addressed the issues emerging from data that are inconsistent with the examiner’s views (Sattler, 1988).

Step 10. Recommendations Recommendations are the final part of the I-Section. Rucker (1967) found that the usefulness of a report is based largely on the strength of the recommendations. Recommendations and interventions must be presented with enough detail that the intended audience is persuaded that the recommendations are relevant and achievable without extraordinary measures. For example, in the case of FFDEs, clear and convincing recommendations for restoring function should not significantly 227

Ronn Johnson

exceed reasonable resources available to assist an examinee (Brenner, 2003; Whitaker, 1994). The recommendations must clarify questions related to findings as they pertain to the evaluation. FFDEs can have at least four broad categories of recommendations based on the opinions rendered by the examiner. These recommendations include: • • • •

Not psychologically fit for duty Not psychologically fit but restorable to minimal level of required functioning (with mandatory treatment) in the current position, other, or modified duty assignment Fit with no significant impairment in psychological functioning Serious reservations based on examinee’s approach to the evaluation process

For pre-employment evaluations, recommendation ratings may be presented in several ways. The pre-employment recommendations may include: • Recommend • Recommend with reservations • Not recommended • Letter grade ratings such as A, B, C, D, or F • “Meets” minimum psychological standards (based on some referenced and applicable code, law, or statute) • “Does not meet” minimum psychological standards (based on some referenced and applicable code, law, or statute) Whatever categories an examiner may use when making recommendations, they must be made in a way that promotes full understanding of how they are to be used by the department or legal authority (Harvey, 1997). Each of the preceding decision-making elements must address issues specific to the reasons (i.e., questions) resulting in the evaluation and issues that emerge during the evaluation that are specific to the person being evaluated.

Conclusions The most challenging demand for a forensic psychologist undertaking an evaluation is integrating data from a variety of sources (Gancono & Hutton, 1994; Meloy, 1991; Sattler, 2001). An effective I-Section is focused on providing information that promotes an understanding of issues or questions raised about the person being evaluated within the context of various law enforcement settings. There are a wide range of referral questions for pre-employment and special unit assignments evaluations and FFDEs. The I-Section should be crafted in a way that allows a diverse group of decision makers to read and understand the recommendations. Crafting the I-Section is a complex task involving multiple comparisons and weighing psychological data in relation to the referral question. The process of writing the report requires a substantial amount of time; the I-Section pulls together what can seem like an unending stream of raw data (Harvey, 1997; Whitaker, 1994), which can be daunting for inexperienced examiners. The I-Section is a triage-oriented process of excluding and including information, keeping only what is relevant to the questions under consideration. There is wide variation in the way reports pull together the information from psychologically relevant sources. The examiner must offer explanations for why (or why not) certain information was handled in formulating the ending words formulated in the I-Section (Kamphaus, 1993; Ownby, 1997; Sattler, 2001; Tallent, 1993). Contradictions and inconsistencies in the findings must be discussed so that the report offers a reasonable basis for the decisions made about the data used or not used. The examiner has a particular burden to explain the meaning of the findings as they pertain to the 228

I-Section Reports in Law Enforcement

opinions rendered (Beutler, Groth-Marnat, & Rosner, 2003; Lewak & Hogan, 2003). Considering the comprehensive amount of data involved in pulling together the I-Section, this must be done in a practical format. Establishing a foundation for the opinions and recommendations offered can be easier said than done. Overreliance on one data source (e.g., testing) can reduce confidence in the recommendations. This is especially true if the testing is not linked to job-specific psychopathology or the absence thereof (Cates, 1999). Each data source used in the I-Section has reliability and validity issues that should be identified and discussed in a way that communicates the examiner’s awareness of how they affected her or his opinions and recommendations. Cross-cultural and international issues are especially relevant. Examiners cannot assume all data will be useful or point in the same direction (Melton et al., 2007; Sattler, 2001). Cultural cautions in the I-Section include being sensitive to an overreliance on data from computerized reporting systems that may identify what appear to be large ethnic/racial groups. Yet, the examiner remains responsible for convincingly articulating his or her awareness of independent, peer-reviewed journal reviews of the data in those computerized reports. Similar concerns are raised by examiners using tightly scripted template reports that have been stripped of cultural factors that could be of particular relevance in formulating I-Section opinions. The purpose of the I-Section is to increase the forensic conceptualization of an examinee through the communication of opinions and recommendations in such a way that personnel action can be taken by department decision makers. Reports written with strong I-Sections weave together multiple relevant data sources as a by-product of forensically relevant judgments.

References and Further Reading Aamodt, M. G. (2004). Research in law enforcement selection. Boca Raton, FL: Brown/Walker Press. Ackerman, M. J. (2006). Forensic report writing. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62(1), 59–72. Adebimpe, V. R. (1981). Overview: White norms and psychiatric diagnostic of black patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 138, 279–285. American Board of Forensic Psychology. (2009). Specialty guidelines for forensic psychology. Committee on the Revision of the Specialty Guidelines for Forensic Psychology. American Board of Forensic Psychology. (2010). Specialty guidelines for forensic psychologists. Chapel Hill, NC: Author. American Educational Research Association (AERA). (1999). Standards for educational and psychological testing. Washington, DC: Author. American Psychiatric Association (APA). (1998). Position statement: Psychiatric treatment and sexual orientation. Washington, DC: Author. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychological Association (APA). (1992). Ethical principles of psychologists and code of conduct. American Psychologist, 47, 1597–1611. American Psychological Association (APA). (1998). Answers to your questions about sexual orientation and homosexuality. Washington, DC: Author. American Psychological Association (APA). (2002). Ethical principles of psychologists and code of conduct. American Psychologist, 58, 650–659. American Psychological Association Committee on Professional Practice & Standards. (1998). Guidelines for psychological evaluations in child protection matters. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Anshel, M. H., & Kaissidis, A. N. (1997). Coping style and situational appraisals as predictors of coping strategies following stressful events in sport as a function of gender and skill level. British Journal of Psychology, 88, 263–276. Berkun, M. (2000). Performance decrement under psychological stress. Performance in Extreme Human Environments, 5, 92–97. Bersoff, D. N. (1995). Ethical conflicts in psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Beutler, L. E., Groth-Marnat, G., & Rosner, R. (2003). Introduction to integrated assessment of adult personality. In L. E. Beutler & G. Marnat (Eds.), Integrative assessment of adult personality (2nd ed., pp. 1–36). New York: Guilford.

229

Ronn Johnson Borum. R., Super, J., & Rand, M. (2003). Forensic assessment for high-risk occupations. In A. Goldstein (Ed.), Handbook of psychology (Vol. 11, Forensic psychology). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley. Brenner, E. (2003). Consumer-focused psychological assessment. Professional Psychology: Research & Practice, 34, 240–247. California Police Officers Standard and Training Commission. (2008). Administrative progress report. Retrieved from www.post.ca.gov/bulletin/APRS/January_2008_apr.pdf. Cates, J. A. (1999). The art of assessment in psychology: Ethics, expertise, and validity. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55(5), 631–641. Committee on Ethical Guidelines for Forensic Psychologists. (1991). Specialty guidelines for forensic psychologists. Law and Human Behavior, 15. Covington, J. (2001). Round up the usual suspects: Racial profiling and the war on drugs. In D. Milovanovic & K. K. Russell (Eds.), Petit apartheid in the U.S. criminal justice system: The dark figure of racism (pp. 27–42). Durham, NC: Carolina Academic Press. Curran, S. F. (1998). Pre-employment psychological evaluation of law enforcement applicants. Police Chief, 65(10), 88–95. Davis, R. D., & Rostow, C. D. (2004). Using the MMPI special scale configurations of predict law enforcement officers fired for cause. Applied HRM Research, 9(2), 57–58. Donders, J. (2001). A survey of report writing by neuropsychologists: II. Test data, report format, and document length. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 15, 150–161. Doss, M. T., Jr. (1990). Police management: Sexual misconduct and the right to privacy. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 17, 194–204. Employee Polygraph Protection Act of 1988, Pub. L. No. 100–347 (H.R. 1212, 29 USC 2001). Finn, P., Talucci, V., & Wood, J. (2000, January). On the job stress in policing: Reducing it, preventing it. National Institute of Justice Journal, 19–24. Gabbidon, S. L., & Marzette, L. N. (2007). Racial profiling and the courts: An empirical analysis of federal litigation, 1991–2006. Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice, 23(3), 226–238. Gagliardi, G. J., & Miller, A. K. (2007). Writing forensic psychological reports. In B. Jackson (Ed.), Learning forensic assessment. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Gancono, C. B., & Hutton, H. E. (1994). The Hare PCL-R: Clinical interview schedule (Adult form). Unpublished manuscript. Goldstein, A. M., Morse, S. J., & Shapiro, D. L. (2003). Evaluation of criminal responsibility. In A. M. Goldstein (Ed.), Forensic psychology (pp. 381–406). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley. Goodheart, C. D., Kazdin, A. E., Sternberg, R. J. (Eds). (2006). Evidence-based psychotherapy: Where practice and research meet. Washington DC: American Psychological Association. Graham, J. R. (2006). MMPI-2: Assessing personality and psychopathology (4th ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. Green, A., Carney, D., Pallin, D., Ngo, L. H., Raymond, K. L., Iezzoni, L. I., et al. (2007). Implicit bias among physicians and its prediction of thrombolysis decisions for black and white patients. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 22(9), 1231–1238. Green, D., Callands, T. A., Radcliffe, A. M., Luebbe, A. M., & Klonoff, E. A. (2009). Clinical psychology students’ perceptions of diversity training: A study of exposure and satisfaction. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65(10), 1056–1070. Groth-Marnat, G. (1999). Current status and future directions of psychological assessment: Introduction. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55, 781–785. Groth-Marnat, G., & Horvath, L. S. (2006). The psychological report: A review of current controversies. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62(1), 73–81. Harvey, V. S. (1997). Improving readability of psychological reports. Professional Psychology: Research & Practice, 28(3), 271–274. Harvey, V. S. (2006). Variables affecting the clarity of psychological reports. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62(1), 5–18. Heilbrun, K., Rosenfeld, B., Warren, J., & Collins, S. (1994). The use of third party information in forensic assessments. Bulletin of the American Academy of Psychiatry and Law, 22, 399–406. Howell, S. E., Perry, H. L., & Vile, M. (2004). Black cities/white cities: Evaluating the police. Political Behavior, 26(1), 45–68. Independent Commission on the Los Angeles Police Department. (1991). Report of the Independent Commission on the Los Angeles Police Department. Los Angeles: Author. International Association of Chiefs of Police, Police Psychological Services. (1998). Fitness-for-duty evaluations. Alexandria, VA: Author.

230

I-Section Reports in Law Enforcement Inwald, R. (1988). Five year follow-up study of departmental terminations as predicted by 16 pre-employment psychological indicators. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73, 703–710. Johnson, R., & Scott, M. (2009). Smelling alcohol in the evaluation of police recruits and fitness for duty evaluations. Paper presented at the IACP 116th Annual Meeting, Law Enforcement and Technology Conference, Police Physicians Section Meeting, Denver, CO. Jordan, K. L., Gabbidon, S. L., & Higgins, G. E. (2009). Exploring the perceived extent of and citizens’ support for consumer racial profiling: Results from a national poll. Journal of Criminal Justice, 37(4), 353–359. Kamphaus, R. W. (1993). Clinical assessment of children’s intelligence. Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Kellerman, H., & Burry, A. (1981). Handbook of psychodiagnostic testing: Personality analysis and report writing. New York: Grune & Stratton. Kellerman, H., & Burry, A. (2007). Handbook of psychodiagnostic testing: Analysis of personality in the psychological report (4th ed.). New York: Springer. Knapp, S., & VandeCreek, L. (2006). Practical ethics for psychologists: A positive approach. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Levinson, R. M., & York, M. Z. (1974). The attribution of dangerousness in mental health. Journal of Health & Social Behavior, 15, 328–335. Lewak, R. W., & Hogan, R. S. (2003). Integrating and applying assessment information: Decisionmaking, patient feedback, and consultation. In L. E. Beutler & G. Groth-Marnat (Eds.), Integrative assessment of adult personality (pp. 356–397). New York: Guilford. Lichtenberger, E. O. (2006). Computer utilization and clinical judgment in psychological assessment reports. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62(1), 19–32. Lieberman, H. R., Tharion, W. J., Shukitt-Hale, B., Speckman, K. L., & Tulley, R. (2002). Effects of caffeine, sleep loss, and stress on cognitive performance and mood during U.S. Navy SEAL training. Psychopharmacology, 164(3), 250–261. Lykken, D. T. (1981). A tremor in the blood. New York: McGraw-Hill. Matsumoto, D. (1989). Cultural influences on the perception of emotion. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 20, 92–105. Matsumoto, D. (2002). Methodological requirements to test a possible in group advantage in judging emotions across cultures: Comments on Elfenbein and Ambady and evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 236–242. Messerschmidt, J. W. (1993). Masculinities and crime: Critique and reconceptualization of theory. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield. Meloy, J. R. (1991). The blurring of ego boundary in projective identification [Letter to the editor]. American Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 1761–1762. Melton, G. B., Petrila, J., Polythress, N. G., & Slobogin, C. (2007). Psychological evaluations for the courts: A handbook for mental health professionals and lawyers (3rd ed.). New York: Guilford. Michaels, M. H. (2006). Ethical considerations in writing psychological assessment reports. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62(1), 47–58. Nietzel, M. T., & Hartung, C. M. (1993). Psychological research on the police: An introduction to a special section on the psychology of law enforcement. Law & Human Behavior, 17(2), 151–155. Owens, W. A. (1976). Background data. In M. D. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology. Chicago: Rand McNally. Ownby, R. L. (1997). Psychological reports: A guide to report writing in professional psychology (3rd ed.). New York: John Wiley. Reilly, R. R., & Chao, G. T. (1982). Validity and fairness of some alternative employee selection procedures. Personnel Psychology, 35(1), 1–62. Ridley, C. R. (2005). Overcoming unintentional racism: A practitioner’s guide to intentional interviewing (2nd ed.). Thousands Oaks, CA: Sage. Rostow, C. D., & Davis, R. D. (2004). A handbook for psychological fitness-for-duty evaluations in law enforcement. Binghampton, NY: Haworth Clinical Practice Press. Rucker, C. M. (1967). Technical language in the school psychologist’s report. Psychology in the Schools, 4, 146–150. Russell, K. K. (1998). The color of crime: Racial hoaxes, white fear, black protectionism, police harassment, and other macroaggressions. New York: New York University Press. Russell, K. K. (2001). Racial profiling: A status report of the legal, legislative, and empirical literature. Rutgers Race and the Law Review, 61, 1–16. Sackett, P. R., & Decker, P. J. (1979). The detection of deception in the employment context: A review and critical analysis. Personnel Psychology, 32, 487–506. Sanders, B., Hughes, T., & Langworthy, R. (1995). Police officer recruitment and selection: A survey of major police departments in the U.S. Police Forum, 5, 1–4.

231

Ronn Johnson Sattler, J. M. (1988). Assessment of children. San Diego, CA: Author. Sattler, J. M. (1991). Assessment of children (3rd ed.). San Diego, CA: Author. Sattler, J. M. (1998). Clinical and forensic interviewing of children and families: Guidelines for mental health, education, pediatric, and child maltreatment fields. San Diego, CA: Author. Sattler, J. M. (2001). Assessment of children: Cognitive applications (4th ed.). San Diego, CA: Author. Saxe, L., Dougherty, D., & Cross, T. (1985). The validity of polygraph testing: Scientific analysis and public controversy. American Psychologist, 40, 355–366. Scrivner, E. (2006). Psychology and law enforcement. In I. B. Weiner & A. K. Hess (Eds.), The handbook of forensic psychology. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley. Shapiro, D. L. (1991). Forensic psychological assessment. Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Simon, R. I., & Shuman, D. W. (Eds.). (2002). Retrospective assessment of mental states in litigation predicting the past. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Skeem, J., Golding, S. L., Cohen, N., & Berge, G. (1998). Logic and reliability of evaluations of competence to stand trial. Law & Human Behavior, 22, 519–547. Sklansky, D. (2006). Sexual orientation and workplace rights: A potential land mine for employers? Employee Relations Law Journal, 18(1), 29–60. Snyder, C. R., Ritschel, L. A., Rand, K. L., & Berg, C. J. (2006). Balancing psychological assessments: Including strengths and hope in client reports. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62(1), 33–46. Sue, D. W., & Sue, D. (2008). Counseling the culturally diverse: Theory and practice (5th ed.). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley. Tallent, N. (1988). Psychological report writing. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Tallent, N. (1993). Psychological report writing (4th ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Tallent, N., & Reiss, R. J. (1959). Multidisciplinary views on the preparation of written psychological reports: III. The trouble with psychological reports. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 15, 444–446. Thompson, B. M., Kirk, A., & Brown, D. F. (2001). Work based support, emotional exhaustion, and spillover of work stress to the family environment: A study of policewomen. Stress Medicine, 21(3), 199–207. Trompetter, P. (1998, October). Fitness-for-duty evaluations: What agencies can expect. Police Chief, 60, 97–105. Weiner, I. B. (2006). Writing forensic reports. In I. B. Weiner & A. K. Hess (Eds.), The handbook of forensic psychology. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley. Weitzer, R., & Tuch, S. (2002). Perceptions of racial profiling: Race, class, and personal experience. Criminology, 40, 435–457. Weitzer, R., & Tuch, S. A. (2004). Race and perceptions of police misconduct. Social Problems, 51, 305–325. Weitzer, R., & Tuch, S. A. (2006). Race and policing in America: Conflict and reform. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Weitzer, R. J., & Tuch, S. A. (2005). Racially biased policing: Determinants of citizen perceptions. Social Forces, 3, 1009–1030. Whitaker, D. (1994). How school psychology trainees learn to communicate through the school psychological report (Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation). University of Washington, Seattle. Winship, C., & Berrien, J. (1999). Boston cops and black churches: New approaches to fighting crime. Public Interest, 136, 52–68. Woody, R. H. (2005). The police culture: Research implications for psychological services. Professional Psychology: Research & Practice, 36(5), 525–529.

Case Cited Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, Inc., 509 US 579, 589 (1993).

232

11 CHALLENGING THE POLICE DE-SELECTION PROCESS Jose M. Arcaya

And I wept both night and day, And he wip’d my tears away, And I wept both day and night, And hid from him my heart’s delight, So he took his wings and fled; Then the morn blushe’d rosy red; I dried my tears, & arm’d my fears With ten thousand shields and spears. William Blake Songs of Innocence

Introduction As central parties responsible for selecting prospective police officers and other law enforcement employees to work in municipal or governmental agencies, psychologists are charged with screening out applicants showing instability, bad judgment, addictive tendencies, and other traits incompatible with successful functioning as peace officers. Apart from administering standardized tests and reviewing applicants’ background records, they must employ face-to-face interviews to assess the demeanor, candor, and intelligences of those potential police officers. That portion of the evaluative process— where the applicant provides unexpected or spontaneous revelations during the oral interview—is this chapter’s principal concern. Specifically, we will argue that police psychologists—those working on behalf of law enforcement departments as evaluators—often use the applicants’ revelations of past indiscretions to disqualify them unfairly. Here, the terms personal information, disclosures, or admissions will refer to adverse data the candidate had not previously mentioned in his or her written, pre-interview application. The applicant tends to provide it trustingly in an attempt to be transparent to the police examiner as well as to demonstrate that the behavior(s) in question (e.g., speeding, excessive drinking, or shoplifting) no longer play a part in the applicant’s life. Despite this open renunciation of that conduct or lifestyle, the admission in question is still used as the basis for disqualifying the candidate as a police officer. Finally, those kinds of revelations are almost always instigated by the police psychologist’s

233

Jose M. Arcaya

mode of questioning and the institutional forces operating in the assessment environment, not by the applicant. In short, admissions tend to be extracted externally rather than by free will. Such confessions, it will be argued, are too quickly incorporated into a reject narrative rather than contextualized in terms of the spirit in which they were made (e.g., “I used to smoke marijuana a lot in high school, but stopped after my first year in college”) because of preexisting biases favoring a suspicious outlook toward all police applicants. They are seen as stable personality characteristics rather than reports of transcended difficulties, cured negative habits, or worked-through issues. Almost always, admissions never receive credit as instances of honesty or integrity but as problems cleverly uncovered by the police psychologist. Moreover, those engaged in police hiring evaluations tend to be influenced by the prevailing job culture, encouraging the commission of false negative errors over false positive ones (i.e., it is better to be safe than sorry). Thus, revelations of impropriety made against one’s own interests are likely to be viewed as free evidence for an easy turn-down, not instances of goodwill behavior. As a professional psychologist who is frequently asked to appeal turn-downs, I have noted repeatedly that admissions not only are not accorded the credit they deserve, but also tend unduly to cancel out or diminish the applicant’s other positive accomplishments. The fact that the candidate had secrets or unrevealed information (because, perhaps, it was no longer relevant to his or her ongoing life) serves to detract from the job seeker’s positive history. Instead, past misdeeds take center stage as a result of being induced during the oral examination and push out current accomplishments from meaningful consideration. Once job applicants admit to something that should have been kept to themselves, police examiners then tend to hold the job applicant’s feet to the fire. This is often done by forcing candidates into acknowledging more serious misbehaviors than the ones admitted, sometimes to the point of extracting false information. Even though the applicant’s intent in disclosing that data in the first place (e.g., having smoked marijuana, stolen a small amount of money, or been drunk behind the wheel) might have been to show a current change in character or behavior by contrasting it to the past, the admission itself now becomes an object of intense interest, close inspection, and skeptical questioning (e.g., “Are you sure? Tell me when you really stopped. How much had you been consuming? What made you stop?”). As is often the case with people trying to explain their way out of misunderstandings, candidates are then likely to make matters worse by trying to clarify what they believe was misrepresented (e.g., “I meant to say that I no longer smoke marijuana, but I have friends who still do”). That sort of reaction sweeps them into an ever deepening whirlpool of disbelief driven by the examiner’s confirmation bias;1 only a guilty individual would act so defensively. As a cautionary note, we assert that not all police candidate selections or determinations are inaccurate or wrong. We only challenge the ones that base their determinations primarily on admissions rather than real information derived from the applicants’ factual histories. We claim that when police candidate evaluators employ browbeating and mental coercion to extract disqualifying evidence, the ensuing rejection is questionable. That information becomes doubly suspect not only because it often refers to matters of the distant past (e.g., “Five years ago or so, I got into a shoving match with my roommate”), but also because it was produced to placate the examiner’s expectations. Admissions are never about the here and now, only about recalled conduct. After exploring this phenomenon further, discussing similarities between its conundrums and the reason for the Fifth Amendment’s presence in the Constitution, we discuss ethical conflicts associated with advising prospective police candidates about the dangers they encounter by making inadvertent admissions. Finally, we make recommendations for improving the police selection procedure with respect to the oral interview.

The Police Hiring Procedure The oral examination is one part within the larger hiring process. While approaches for picking job candidates vary notably among jurisdictions (e.g., some require a college degree, others only a few 234

Challenging the Police De-selection Process

post-high school credits; some demand paramilitary experience, others none; some administer the MMPI-2, others the Inwald Personality Inventory; see Taylor et al., 2006), all demand applicants to submit written applications containing extensive background information, subject themselves to formal psychological testing, and partake in personal interviews with psychological evaluators. Police applicants generally get rejected in one of three ways: by record, by perjury, and/or by admission. The first arises from an objective, problematical record characterized by significant misbehaviors or infractions (e.g., moving violations, firings, bankruptcies, dishonorable military discharges, domestic incidents, and serious juvenile misbehaviors). In this instance, disqualification is directly proportional to the number of identified problems, the severity of the issues concerning those problems, and the vintage of the misconduct in question (i.e., the more recent the problem, the worse its impact on the candidacy). Needless to say, if all three variables are pointing negatively, the police psychologist’s job is made relatively easy and little thinking is involved in making the rejection. On the other hand, if the fact pattern points toward the opposite direction (i.e., the offenses are few in number, relatively minor, and distant in time), the candidate’s chances for being hired improve notably. When that kind of pattern appears, the police psychologist’s job becomes harder because it demands more machinations to justify the candidate’s rejection. The professional must explain how he or she weighed the examinee’s strengths and weaknesses in getting to the disqualification. For example, consider the fact pattern involving an applicant with a checkered past. He has had two arrests; the last one (a disorderly conduct charge for being with a group of people who were fighting others) occurred five years ago and is deemed to be of medium severity. The first arrest was for riding in a stolen car as a juvenile (seven years previously). Today, the applicant works responsibly, has had no other legal issues since the last arrest, is close to graduating from college, and would probably challenge the police psychologist’s decision-making capacity. Because no clear calculus for combining such a group of complex elements exists to justify either a hire or a fire decision, the police psychologist is left with little more than guessing. However, regardless of the decision, at least the situation contains objective evidence. Although the examiner could be accused of being excessively rigid in deciding against the applicant, at least objective information was used in arriving at the determination. The data were transparent, not conjured in the examiner’s mind. Similarly, perjury, the second type of rejection, is also rooted in hard evidence. It emerges when the candidate provides information that contradicts the written record. The opposing data could have come from the written pre-examination or employment questionnaire or because a departmental investigator dug up background facts not previously known to the police psychologist. In either case, the result is that an attempt was made to downplay or omit past bad behavior.2 The lying, of course, is aimed at burnishing an applicant’s job profile, knowing that telling the truth would likely lessen his or her hiring possibilities (e.g., admitting only one juvenile arrest when three was the real number; denying having ever been fired, yet one firing is uncovered when past employers are questioned during a background check). The common issue for the police psychologist to consider is whether the inconsistency was willful lying or the product of simply forgetting (the defense which the candidate almost always invokes). Whatever the eventual decision, the facts under consideration are clear to an external reader of the case as they would be if personality test findings (e.g., MMPI-2, Inwald Personality Inventory, CP) are included to arrive at the final decision (i.e., “Did the applicant also demonstrate propensities toward lying on the objective testing?”). While the disqualification might be debated, the outcome emerged from verifiable information. As already suggested, the validity of an admissions rejection is the most problematic. It is centered on what candidates said, independent of the objective record. It also can be a product of induction by the very circumstances of the oral interview. Because only the candidate really knows the facts of the matter—they cannot be confirmed by a third party—only the police examiner’s conjectures 235

Jose M. Arcaya

remain to determine how much importance to place on the disclosure. At work is not only what the applicant has verbalized, but also the police examiner’s suspicions, biases, and need to play detective. Indeed, it is not unusual for police psychologists to provoke examinees into repudiating or revising sanguine versions of the past because they are deemed unconvincing or questionable, encouraging instead what is believed to be a more realistic (i.e., negative) account of the occurrence. For example, the examiner might remark, “It would not be unusual for a young man in this day and age to have used marijuana on more than one occasion. Was that the case for you?” or “You indicate never having received any moving violation yet you’ve been driving for five years. Didn’t you ever drive recklessly without being caught?” The result would be that an examinee would provide selfdamning data, believing that everyone else going through the hiring process is doing the same thing. Such is the power of suggestion that I have even known candidates who have made up examples of misconduct just to avoid seeming fake by the interviewing psychologist. In short, they did not want to disappoint the examiner’s explicitly stated assumption that “boys will be boys.” On the other hand, police psychologists tend not to delve as deeply into applicants’ positive accomplishments (e.g., that he or she won an academic excellence award or was approved for advanced military training while in the U.S. Marine Corps) as they do the problematic material. The unacknowledged reason is that police psychologists tend to see their primary job as keeping out “bad apples” rather than acquiring good ones. As someone once said, a rejected candidate will never threaten a department, but a hired one can always disappoint. From the trusting viewpoint of the applicant, the selection process is fair, professionally administered, and open-minded. Therefore, they think the open-hearted (i.e., sincere) nature of their revelations will be sensitively understood as a closed episode of their life. They also think they will be given due credit for having voluntarily shared disavowed episodes of fighting, drinking, speeding, and experimental drug use. More often than not, those admissions are about youthful misconduct (e.g., “I had several fights during my first semester in college,” “I was part of a fraternity drinking contest and won,” or “My girlfriends and I raced other cars when we first got our licenses”) or one-time personal problems (“When I graduated from high school, I visited a therapist at my mother’s request because I didn’t feel like doing much,” or “After breaking up with my boyfriend, I was prescribed Elavil for my depression”) that they believe have been transcended. The statements are not intended as reflective of ongoing adjustment issues (e.g., “I have just separated from my wife,” “Although I didn’t want to declare bankruptcy, I just did because of my many pressing debts”). Thus, had the applicants kept such data to themselves, based on their otherwise acceptable backgrounds, they would have probably been hired. At the same time, they want to show their honest side. Thus, admissions are often provided as symbols of good faith—to indicate that the candidate has nothing to hide from the department and can be trusted to fess up if he or she makes mistakes. They are advertisements for sincerity. Yet applicants do not get the expected response for such truthfulness; instead, it is cynically received by the examiner with silent gratitude (i.e., “Thanks, you’ve just made my job easier”). Now, the police psychologist is able to write a fairly straightforward rejection (e.g., “The candidate admitted to . . . getting drunk at several college parties . . . stealing clothes from the shopping mall five years ago . . . driving while intoxicated two weeks after getting her driver’s license”). On the other hand, upon learning of the consequences created by their candor, candidates lament with the typical statement, “Boy! Was I stupid to have trusted the doctor!” They see themselves as having been fools, thinking that had they been clearer in what they acknowledged, the rejection could have been averted. Yet, had they known at the time that their words were interpreted in an opposite manner than intended (which does not normally happen because the police psychologists are experts at hiding their real opinions during hiring interviews), any attempts to revise those misspoken words (e.g., “I now only drink on very few occasions,” “I had financial problems two years ago, but now I have almost paid off my debts”) would still have been 236

Challenging the Police De-selection Process

met by objections and disapproval. Then, the candidates would have been described in the rejection report as uncooperative, defensive, and argumentative.

Fifth Amendment Parallels The impulse toward sincerity and candor in an effort to show good moral character through selftransparency and untempered self-disclosure, but that causes legal difficulties later, is exactly why the Fifth Amendment was included in the Constitution. It provides the right of silence to governmentally detained individuals in order to avoid self-incrimination. The drafters knew that state powers are much greater than the citizen’s ability to defend against imprisonment. Moreover, they were also aware that, without external restraints, government would likely resort to force, coercion, or deception to acquire sought-after confessions. Although criminal interrogations and employment interviews are conducted for very different purposes (i.e., to determine criminal culpability versus job fitness), of relevance for this discussion are the similar psychological dynamics driving both, which explain why examinees admit more negative information than is necessary. Here, we discuss four of these psychological dynamics. First, admissions are produced in the context of social pressure. Both the interrogation and the hiring are conducted in private and in the face of powerful others. Like a police interrogation, the selection interview is open to the use of entrapment, manipulation, or enticement to acquire self-incriminating information. Since police examiners tend to be older than the applicants, they can seem more intimidating when expressing skepticism about the candidate’s degree of candor (e.g., “Are you sure you never got into any fights after turning 18?” “Tell me the truth: how much marijuana have you really smoked?” “Everyone steals. When did you last take something that didn’t belong to you?”). Second, those applying for law enforcement work tend to be young, idealistic, and, besides desiring to help others, committed to the rule of law. They also tend to be respectful of authority—why else would they be seeking employment with a paramilitary organization such as a police department? Thus, lacking the cynicism that they are likely to acquire after becoming seasoned officers, applicants typically come into the hiring situation programmed to tell the truth. Unfortunately, this predilection is very compatible with the police psychologist’s need to acquire rejecting evidence. It propels the applicant to trust and admit. Without such an impulse for honesty, the interviewer would be left with little (assuming there are no skeletons in the closet) to criticize. The police psychologist would be left with the default position of hiring the examinee, not his or her first impulse. As noted earlier, they believe it is safer to reject than to hire. Third, police applicants frequently arrive with the naïve belief that the hiring agency somehow has supernatural powers or uncanny detection capabilities to find out about their personal pasts. Therefore, it would be futile for anyone to lie since it would be detected anyway. Facing the scrutiny of a professional psychologist whom these young and impressionable applicants likely endow with all-knowing powers, they sense themselves as transparent or otherwise psychologically naked. Thus, they are vulnerable to altering their statements at the behest of the interviewer, acceding to his or her authority, or shifting positions if it is so suggested (e.g., “The police examiner claimed that, because I had been stopped four years ago for drunken driving, I had probably drunk more than the five beers I had that night. I went along with his number since he seemed to know what he was talking about”). Last, candidates are not just encouraged but also generally ordered to be honest during every step of the admission process. They continuously hear that truth telling is the hallmark of a good law enforcement officer (e.g., “Honesty is the best policy,” or “Don’t lie—you will be found out if you do”) and lectured that sincerity is compatible with good recruit behavior. Moreover, they are also encouraged to believe—although not explicitly reassured—that they will receive admiration, respect, 237

Jose M. Arcaya

or esteem if they courageously divulge unflattering information that had not already been disclosed in the pre-interview application. Embodying Lerner’s (1980) famous just-world thesis—that most people believe that bad things tend to happen to the bad while good to the just—police candidates think freely provided negative revelations will show they’re good and definable. The candor will protect them from the consequences of what they have disclosed. Kassin (2005) notes this same thinking guides the actions of innocent people who, despite awareness of their Fifth Amendment Miranda warnings, still go on to divulge and even confess false information, believing they will be treated better than if they kept quiet. That travesty occurs because the interrogator fools the suspect into believing that he or she has the suspect’s interests in mind (i.e., is a friend) and can mitigate charges if cooperation is forthcoming. Cooperation, of course, means providing on-the-record statements or admissions, which the investigator can cite to the judge as indications of good-heartedness. Under the stress of detention, the offer is enough to induce the self-damaging revelations (i.e., “Look how innocent I am: I didn’t hide behind silence”). After all, wouldn’t a guilty person remain quiet, knowing that otherwise he or she would worsen a bad situation? Kassin (2005) explains the dynamics of such behavior in the following way: The phenomenology of innocence may be rooted in a generalized and perhaps motivated belief in a just world in which human beings get what they deserve and deserve what they get (Lerner, 1980). It may also stem from of an “illusion of transparency,” a tendency for people to overestimate the extent to which their true [positive] thoughts, emotions, and other inner states can be seen by others (Gilovich et al., 1998; Miller & McFarland, 1987). (Kassin, 2005, p. 218) Negative admissions (e.g., “Yes, I got into a fist fight during a fraternity initiation,” “I did try marijuana in high school”) tend to erase all the good that the examinee has brought to the table, diminishing disproportionately the impact of the applicant’s positive accomplishments. Since it is harder to take back what has been said than never to have said it in the first place, the applicant often then finds himself or herself in the untenable situation of trying to prove that he or she is not guilty. The subsequent rejection that follows is a robotic, canned, or formulaic action—applied in this instance as it has in many other situations. Typically, it questions an applicant’s integrity while implicitly lauding the examiner’s perceptiveness in extracting the negative data (e.g., “The candidate didn’t realize the implications of what he was saying when admitting that he brought a knife to school”). Without any objective “anchor” to determine the context or seriousness of the information, the police examiner is left relying on his or her word or speculations (e.g., “He told me that the knife was in the pocket because he had forgotten to leave it at home after having used it on a fishing trip; the story didn’t sound credible”). The examiner then creates a reject story or narrative in which the admission is placed. Spence (1982) has noted in the psychoanalytic therapy situation a propensity for analysts to “smooth” the patient’s account of his or her life, omitting some details and magnifying others, to establish a coherent—but not necessarily correct—account of the past. That transformation is not done against the patient’s will, but in cooperation with it, providing the latter with a better account of his or her childhood and background than had previously been the case. However, the result is a narrative, not necessarily the truth of what really went on.

Aftermath in the Police Hiring Situation In most cities and jurisdictions, rejected police candidates can appeal their rejections in front of hearing boards or administrative courts. Beyond hiring a lawyer, the appellant or the appellant’s lawyer inevitably contracts a private psychologist to provide an opposing professional opinion regarding the candidate’s mental fitness to accomplish the job in question. 238

Challenging the Police De-selection Process

Opposing the appeal is not only the police psychologist who made the turn-down but also the lawyers representing the department for which the police psychologist works. The legal contest is frequently not about statutory or case law matters but about different psychological theories of the mind, for example, whether a particular test finding really represents the negative behaviors asserted by the police psychologists. Typically, the police psychologist places greater weight on past behavior, not on present conduct. Citation is made to difficulties in such objectively verified spheres of life as the applicant’s work history, psychological test outcomes, driving background, academic attainments, finances, as well as inconsistencies between material contained in the applicant’s written records and words uttered during the oral interview. While the police psychologist is prone to looking at the past as the best predictor of future behavior, the defending psychologist argues that the police psychologist either misunderstood the candidate’s background, misheard the interviewee’s commentary, provided insufficient attention to the applicant’s achievements, and/or magnified more than necessary the candidate’s past mistakes. He or she will assert that current conduct (e.g., stable employment, community involvement) is indicative of compatibility with desired police character makeup. Although technically both are supposedly neutral experts, in reality, they are quasi-advocates who endow the same facts with distinctly different meanings (e.g., police psychologist: “The candidate has had four different jobs in two and a half years, suggesting instability of character”; and private psychologist: “The candidate’s four jobs were in the area of restaurant work, where turnovers are quite frequent”). Thus, not unlike Spence’s thesis (1982), the two psychologists are involved in a kind of storytelling contest, a narrative construction that portrays the disqualified candidate in opposite ways. The administrative officer or judge hearing the appeal obviously must decide the version having the greatest persuasive impact (e.g., police psychologist: “The candidate shows mental instability, poor impulse control, deceptive character, untrustworthiness”; and the applicant’s psychologist: “The applicant is a worthy hire because he or she meritoriously served in the armed forces, has an honorable history of employment, and serves the community through volunteer work”). It is not so much the applicant’s objective history in dispute (because it cannot be challenged) but material gained during the police psychologist’s oral interview, where the greatest degree of contention between the two psychologists occurs. In this matter, the police psychologist is in the more powerful position of the two. He or she has set the mood (e.g., “The applicant was highly guarded . . . flip . . . hostile”) and storyline (e.g., “She admitted to having engaged in two shoplifting incidents, the latest two years ago. That is proof of dishonesty”). The appeal psychologist, on the other hand, must supply a host of credible counterexplanations to contest the damning evaluation (e.g., “The applicant was apparently reactive to police psychologist’s gruffness . . . thought the examiner wanted her to answer quickly . . . she felt disrespected”). If not, what is left uncontested is assumed by the court to be true. Rejections are generally overturned in one or more of three ways: (1) challenging the accuracy of the police psychologist’s claims (i.e., he or she was totally or partially mistaken about the facts used to disqualify); (2) noting the lengthy time span between the last misbehavior and the present date (the longer, the better); and (3) citing laudable conduct contradicting the assertion that the candidate is unsuitable for police work (e.g., being honorably discharged from the armed forces, working adequately as a voluntary peace officer, performing praised community service, and/or attaining advanced educational credentials). It is rare that the rejected candidate has a spotless record. There is always some blemish (if not more) in the background that pushes him or her into the unfit category. The ultimate question for disputation in the appeal process is whether the rejection was based on reasonable assumptions that future work problems would likely arise if the applicant were hired: How much of the decision was driven by the examiner’s cognitive recipes or stereotypes, relegating the candidate into the rejection category (e.g., a drunk, a liar, a hothead), rather than through objective analysis of the facts? Did the 239

Jose M. Arcaya

evaluator excessively simplify the applicant’s history so that a more complicated truth was obscured by a simple-minded conception? While dealing with the objective features of the case is difficult enough (e.g., reframing data and showing evidence of rehabilitation), coping with admissions demands explaining to the court the possibly coercive circumstances in which they were made and the intentions which motivated them in the first place (e.g., an attempt to show candor). It also requires casting into doubt the examiner’s manner of drawing out information from the applicant and why certain implications were drawn from the adduced material and not others. A detailed account of that activity is beyond the scope of this chapter.

Ethical Issues Should candidates be informed of the negative consequences they face by disclosing unflattering information? Must they be warned that the police psychologists are not their friends or that what they put in their pre-interview applications can be used against them in the oral interview? Would it be an implicit encouragement to lie if the consulting psychologist informs a rejected candidate that other jurisdictions would never know that he or she had been rejected by the one in question? What is the consulting psychologist’s responsibility to the larger society? Should he or she give away secrets in which police psychologists disqualify candidates? Should he or she “let the cat out of the bag”? I know of no academic discussion related to those questions. However, they are the ones commonly posed by law enforcement applicants wanting to know how to increase their chances of being hired. As for me, I believe in transparency. Applicants should know the evaluation process is laced with traps to induce the unwary into making admissions contrary to his or her interests. The police psychologist, sometimes appearing warm and personable, will always be on the department’s side, not the applicant’s. By definition, as a forensic expert, he or she advocates or works for the institution that pays his or her salary, not the person being assessed. At least that much should be said. At the same time, applicants should not be encouraged to lie. Instead, the law enforcement job candidates should be educated about the hiring situation that they will be facing. It is not much different than giving them a Miranda warning.

Example Problems arose with this candidate’s application after endorsing two alcohol-related items on a particular police department’s screening questionnaire (#29: “Do you believe social drinking can lead to alcoholism?”—False; #31: “Do you ever get drunk?”—True), suggesting a history of excessive imbibing. He did not have to answer in that fashion since no external record existed that he had taken excessive liberties with alcohol. During the subsequent face-to-face, the following dialogue ensued between the applicant and the examining psychologist. That information is taken directly from her notes: (Number of times drunk in your life?) “. . . my life! . . . wow! (approximate?) I don’t . . . a 100 or 150 somewhere around there . . . (number of drinks for you to get drunk?) “beer probably . . . 4 . . . within an hour . . . 4 won’t drive (total number?) . . . total . . . 10 beers or so . . . Cd indicated that the last time within the last 12 months alone he had been drunk “. . . 20 times maybe.” Mr. Moculski allegedly also confessed to drinking “once a week . . . (each time?) if hanging out maybe a beer . . . if watching football take a beer, if party with friends coming from college 9–10 beers but I won’t drive.” (He also checked “yes” to the question, “Have you ever abused alcohol?” in the field investigator’s record form.)

240

Challenging the Police De-selection Process

The applicant was then examined by a second psychologist, an expert in drug and alcohol addictions. He reported as follows: The candidate admitted to being drunk 200 times and revealed leading an “alcohol centric [life]; that [meaning] getting drunk is an important activity in his life.” [This candidate] also noted, a discrepancy between the amount of drinking per occasion revealed by the candidate (2–3) in that particular interview and what he earlier stated to the other psychologist (9–10). In line with the preceding admissions, [the candidate] is pronounced to be at risk for alcohol related problems. In later conversations, the candidate gave the following explanation for making those statements: Yes, I circled “yes” to the [pre-oral questionnaire asking if I was a binge drinker] because I thought that “binge drinking” meant consuming 3–5 drinks in an hour. I did drink like that while in college, but it lost its appeal. I have not done so in the past six years. I go to the gym to improve my health and also to meet the NYPD academy standards. I have also been mountain biking for about two years which demands great physical stamina. That type of behavior is not consistent with someone who values alcohol more than his wellbeing. My admission was just an attempt to be honest. I had heard on the news that binge drinking was defined as having 3–5 drinks in an hour. I went out every weekend because my friends and I turned 21 around the same time and it was something new and fun to do. But, since we did it almost every weekend, drinking lost its appeal. It is no longer part of my life going out and I am not at all dependent on drinking every time I go out. The only time I drink now is when I watch football on Sundays with my friends. I never claimed to have been drunk 200 times to either police psychologist. Instead, I stated I drank alcohol about 150 to 200 times in my entire life. That means one to three beers in the course of two and a half years. They were all single occasions. I certainly did not get drunk on each occasion or even “high.” I have never done any type of drugs in my life and I certainly didn’t drink every day. During my early undergraduate years I would drink, at most, twice a week and, on rare occasion, three times. Also, I never said that one beer would get me drunk. The [police] psychologist asked me how many drinks I have had in my life not how many times I got drunk. I didn’t think the question was a fair one: “How many drinks have I had in my whole life?” How is someone supposed to know how many drinks they have had in their life? The first also asked me to estimate the amount (as had the second) and I guessed 150 drinks. The psychologist responded with, “Don’t you think that’s a little low” so I revised the number to 200. It was not what I really believed, but what I thought would satisfy her request to give a better response. However, I never intended my answer to mean that I was drunk on each occasion that I tasted alcohol. Yes, the candidate could have refrained from saying all that he revealed. No one would have been the wiser because there was no contradictory data attesting otherwise. However, the candidate made his own bed by putting his foot in his mouth, believing that to tell all would show forthright of character. Now he has contracted me in an appeal action to overturn the rejection that ensued. I do not believe the candidate is or was ever was an alcoholic. Instead, it is likely that he overdrank here and there, like many young college undergraduates. To his credit, he graduated with a B.S. in Criminal Justice in the requisite four years, has worked steadily, and is without untoward incidents since graduating high school eight years earlier. He also moonlights as an emergency medical

241

Jose M. Arcaya

technician in a large metropolitan city. However, those accomplishments were not sufficient to stop the reject label from being applied to his job application.

Concluding Thoughts and Recommendations Certainly, psychologists working for police departments will do what they must to ensure that they hire the most competent applicants. That is why they veer on the false negative rather than false positive side of the decision-making spectrum. Yet, for those of us in the business of working with job rejection appeals and pre-application consultations, the duty is murkier. To repeat the earlier rhetorical questions, should we encourage silence if particular personal facts cannot be verified? If applicants were criminal suspects and we their lawyers, we would certainly advise them to remain quiet and not confess to anything. As I explain to every pre-application candidate, police departments have no universal “eye in the sky.” They cannot check up on matters not in the public record or the candidate’s verifiable history (e.g., schooling, work, military conduct, and criminal history). If they admit the personal, it is at their own peril. With regard to appealing rejections that have the strong taint of “foot-in-mouth” disease (the majority of my cases), the task is much harder than mere prevention. Here, the job is rejecting and revising the storyline now imposed on the candidate by the rejecting psychologist (e.g., he or she is vulnerable to an emotional breakdown because she admitted to taking Prozac five years earlier, or he or she presents the risk of engaging in police brutality because he or she admitted to being a gang member in his or her early junior high school years). It is a labor of “un-smoothing” the narrative. That can only be done by pointing out to the appealing authority that the police examiner overstretched the credibility of his or her rejection argument. It is also helped by piling on the candidate’s positive accomplishments that are contrary to the psychologist’s turn-down. However, the best preventive measure would have been to have said or admitted to nothing. Finally, with respect to the recommendations for the examining police psychologist, he or she should consider the possibility that departmental or institutional biases are at play in their decision making. I also believe that they do not fully appreciate the idealism and naïveté of those whom they encounter. More often than not, when candidates are showing otherwise clean records, they are essentially children who have at times misstepped. They should also acquaint themselves with Freud’s old concepts of transference and countertransference (the references to which are too numerous to detail). How much is a given disqualification a function of the applicant’s shortcomings and limitations? How much of it is a projection of the police psychologist’s own unexamined psychology?

Notes 1. Confirmation bias is selective thinking characterized by a tendency to notice and to look for what confirms one’s beliefs while ignoring or undervaluing the relevance of what contradicts that viewpoint (Nickerson, 1996). 2. I should note that I have never encountered the opposite: a candidate who depicts a problem worse than it really was.

References and Further Reading Blake, W. (1967). Songs of innocence and experience. Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Original work published 1789–1794) Gilovich, T. K., Savitsky, K., & Medev, V. H. (1998). The illusion of transparency: Biased attempts by others to read one’s emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 332–346. Kassin, S. M. (2005). On the psychology of confession: Does innocence put innocents at risk? American Psychologist, 60, 215–228. Lerner, M. (1966). Observer reaction to the “innocent victim”: Compassion or rejection? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4(2), 203–210.

242

Challenging the Police De-selection Process Lerner, M. (1980). The belief in a just world: A fundamental delusion. New York: Plenum. Miller, D. T., & McFarland, C. (1987). Pluralistic ignorance: When similarity is interpreted as dissimilarity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 298–305. Nickerson, R. S. (1996). Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises. Review of General Psychology, 2(2), 175–220. Spence, D. P. (1984). Narrative truth and historical truth: Meaning and interpretation in psychoanalysis. New York: Basic Books. Spence, D. P. (1986). Narrative smoothing and clinical wisdom. In T. R. Sarbin (Ed.), Narrative psychology (pp. 211–232). New York: Praeger.

243

PART 3

Training and Evaluation

12 COUPLES COUNSELING/ ASSESSMENT AND USE OF THE INWALD RELATIONSHIP SURVEYS Robin Inwald, Elizabeth Willman, and Stephanie Inwald During the last three decades, many professional discussions and opinions have been presented at conferences and in the literature about the potential negative effect of police work on couples and family relationships. There appears to be general consensus that training police couples, their families, and/or individual officers in the area of “police stress inoculation,” along with confidential counseling as an early intervention, is appropriate in order to prevent poor work performance, domestic violence, divorce, and even suicide when at least one partner is working in the law enforcement field. However, limited data support this concept, and few research studies focus on police couples (Bartlett, 2004). This chapter includes a literature review of available articles and books written on the subject of couples counseling and couples assessment in the police/public safety field. Survey data describing the frequency of couples counseling in the law enforcement field are presented, and methodological approaches that have been promoted as helpful in working with police couples are described. Finally, two case studies involving a comprehensive assessment of police couples using the Inwald Research Relationship Surveys are presented, one with an apparently successful relationship and one where the couple admitted to substantial relationship difficulties.

Police Couples Literature Review In order to find available studies on this subject, a search through several databases was conducted using the following key words: law enforcement personnel, police marriages, and police couples. An independent follow-up search used the key words police family. In the first search, 78 journal articles and eight books were identified and reviewed from the following databases: PsychInfo, an online database for psychology and behavioral sciences with articles, summaries, and citations from journals, books, and dissertations; ProQuest, an online database containing abstracts and indexing for social and behavioral sciences; Academic Search Premier/ EBSCO, a database that contains indexing and abstracts for more than 8,450 journals; Sage Journals online, a database with more than 560 journals related to health, business, science, and behavioral and social sciences; and SocIndex, an online database for social sciences. Twenty-six articles were found that specifically mentioned work with police couples. However, only three articles (11.5%) contained research-based data obtained from evaluating police couples, each having collected these data using instruments developed by the first author of this chapter. The second search identified two additional articles on ProQuest, four from 247

Robin Inwald, et al.

the PILOTS database, which publishes “international literature on traumatic stress,” and two from Sociological Abstracts. The following review summarizes the findings of these literature searches. During the 1970s, Rogers (1977) wrote that success in police work was best predicted by family stability and Danto (1978) reported that, in police officer suicides, marital problems were evident and may have been the precipitating factors. A number of articles were published about the impact of job stress on the family (Reiser, 1974), including a debate about police officer suicide rates (Rogers, 1977; Terry, 1981) and a call for couples’ communication training and family therapy as intervention techniques (Maynard & Maynard, 1980; Reese, 1982). However, despite related comments in the literature, there were no quantitative studies found that presented research data about police couples in the 1970s or 1980s. Borum and Philpot (1993) noted that there had been a large focus on police stress and its impact on officers but added that the “literature has not equally addressed the effects of police work on officers’ relationships” (p. 122). They also noted: The law enforcement profession brings tremendous pressures to bear on the family relationships of those involved in this line of work. Widespread recognition of this has led to a popular notion that divorce rates for police officers are considerably higher than the national average. While empirical evidence does not support this particular idea, the variety of potential problems for these families is quite evident. (p. 122) In 1993 ( July 27–30), the FBI sponsored the conference “Law Enforcement Families: Issues & Answers” at the FBI Academy in Quantico, Virginia. This conference was organized after the House of Representatives heard testimony regarding the counseling needs of law enforcement families (Bartlett, 2004). Several presentations were generated for this conference, followed by published papers in the FBI conference proceedings, thus beginning an ongoing discussion about police couples and their families by police psychologists and other interested professionals nationwide (Reese & Scrivner, 1994). Despite great interest, and a “setting of the stage” for follow-up studies, few of the presentations at that conference provided evidence-based research on police couples. The first author presented her research at the FBI conference after developing the Hilson Spouse/ Mate Inventory (HSMI) and Hilson Relationship Inventory for Public Safety Personnel (HRI-P), the first assessment instruments known by this author, which are used for evaluating relationship issues specific to police personnel and their spouses/partners (Inwald, Gebbia, & Resko, 1993). These inventories focused on identifying communication/relationship difficulties, attitudes toward police issues, and stress symptoms in police couples. Two additional sets of colleagues also used these inventories to generate research papers for this conference. Kaufmann, Smith, and Palmatier (1994) conducted a police couples’ study using Inwald’s HSMI and HRI-P with undercover officers in two basic narcotics officer training programs. Twenty-four undercover officers and their spouses/mates completed these two inventories. Officers reported a slight to moderate negative impact on their lives because of undercover work. However, contrary to the responses of their spouses/mates, officers reported that “rotating shifts,” “politics of the job,” and “spouses left alone at night” had a significant negative effect on their lives (p. 111). Additionally, spouses and mates of less senior officers reported lower stress levels compared to the spouses/mates of senior officers with longer careers in law enforcement. The researchers concluded that spouse/ mate support programs and officer “transition” (p. 111) programs would be beneficial in the world of undercover policing. Ryan and Brewster (1994) utilized Inwald’s HSMI, HRI-P, a Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder subscale of the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI), and the Beck Depression Inventory to study 12 recently traumatized police officers and their spouse/mate relationships. In this 248

Couples Counseling

preliminary study, both relationship inventory scores and depression scores were similar for officers and their spouses/mates, suggesting that “the marital dyad may share many emotional consequences and symptoms following a traumatic event” (p. 222). Other FBI presentations included some data about divorce, trauma, and suicide in police families. Gentz and Taylor (1994) examined attitudes about divorce among veteran law enforcement officers. The results of this study suggested a strong belief among veteran officers that their career choice and marital problems could lead to divorce. The authors concluded that further investigation regarding the impact of law enforcement careers on marriages is necessary. Bohl and Solomon (1994) examined 30 officers and their spouses regarding postshooting reactions to determine (1) the degree to which spouses, as a group, recognize and acknowledge postshooting psychological and physical symptoms shown by their mates, and (2) the degree of agreement about these symptoms between husband–wife pairs. A 37-item questionnaire was used to identify stress symptoms typically experienced after a critical incident. Results illustrated that spouses often did not recognize and/or acknowledge the symptoms reported by their mates and, as a result, they may have failed to see problems even when the officer admitted that such problems existed. These authors called for more research on spouses and their interactions. Finally, Janik and Kravitz (1994) studied the relationship between marital discord and police suicide rates in a group of 134 police officers referred for fitness-for-duty evaluations, where 55% of them had admitted to previous suicide attempts. Results of this study indicated that complaints of marital problems make an officer 4.8 times more likely to attempt suicide and 6.7 times more likely if they had been suspended; they were 21.7 times more likely if both complaints were present. Interestingly, complaints of being administratively harassed made officers less likely to attempt suicide. (p. 73) Many other FBI conference-generated papers presented the view that the police family “experiences pressures typically not found in other occupations,” stressing the need for additional research and interventions (Reese & Scrivner, 1994). Flater (1994), who described law enforcement family workshops, noted, “It is well documented that working in the field of law enforcement is detrimental to the functioning relationships of family life. However, by the time the law enforcement family seeks counseling, the relationship has usually reached a crisis point” (p. 329). Hays (1994) suggested that “in order to be effective in working with police couples, the therapist must have a solid understanding of the challenges of police work and . . . establish a nonhierarchical relationship” (p. 337). Klein (1994) recommended the use of peer counselors to help officers develop more effective communication skills with their spouses adding, “Communication that works well on the job does not work in (police) relationships” (p. 345). During the mid-1990s, psychologists working with law enforcement agencies acted on the observation that police officers, or “tough guys, are often resistant to psychotherapy in its traditional forms and so special therapeutic approaches are required” (Miller, 1995, p. 592). Two counseling-oriented books were published specifically for law enforcement officers and their spouses/partners about police work, its particular stressors, and potential negative effect on relationships. In Keeping It Simple: Sorting Out What Really Matters in Your Life, Aumiller presented an instructive dialogue between a police officer and his therapist, including a series of therapeutic exercises, to teach the principle of keeping life simple, and free from alcohol/drug abuse and infidelity (Aumiller, 1995). In the first edition of I Love a Cop: What Police Families Need to Know, Kirschman offered information designed to assist officers, families, and their partners in coping with the “everyday challenges facing police officers throughout their careers” (Kirshman, 2007, p. 7). This book’s focus was to aid officers and their partners in creating a resilient and “supportive family environment where everyone 249

Robin Inwald, et al.

can thrive” (Book Jacket). In a revised 2007 edition, chapters were added on substance abuse and domestic violence, along with accompanying statistics. In the fall of 1998 (September 14–18), as the result of increasing concerns about domestic violence in police families, the FBI sponsored a “Domestic Violence by Police Officers” conference at Quantico, Virginia. As with the previous conference on law enforcement families, there were few data-based papers and much theoretical and practical information, including first-person accounts, regarding police couples and domestic violence. After the weeklong presentations and discussions on this topic concluded, the first author developed a new scale for identifying those individuals, including police officer candidates, who were most likely to become involved in domestic violence. This scale was added to the updated and renormed Inwald Survey 5-Revised (IS5-R; Inwald, Traynor, & Favuzza, 2000). While the IS5-R, with its Domestic Violence Scale, now has been used to assess thousands of police officer candidates, as well as officers who had been charged with incidents of domestic violence, no comprehensive studies of police couples or officers have been conducted using this measure prior to the occurrence of violent incidents. During the early 2000s, psychologists continued to comment on police couples and relationships. However, despite the expressed interest in this area, little actuarial research was conducted: “While a few reports give anecdotal accounts of the role of police authoritarian style in marital interaction, a systematic examination of the relationship is virtually absent” (Sgambelluri, 2000). Bartlett (2004) wrote that “in the field of law enforcement occupational stress, the impact of a law enforcement career on the family and intimate relationships has been ignored” (as cited 10 years later in Bartlett, 2004). The present literature search found only two assessment research studies with police couples during the 15-year period from 1995 to 2009, and both were doctoral dissertations. In 2004, Bartlett’s dissertation included a construct validation study for Inwald’s first two police couple’s inventories, the Hilson Spouse/Mate Inventory (HSMI) and the Hilson Relationship Inventory for Public Safety Personnel (HRI-P; Inwald & Gebbia, 1997). With 60 participants, all sworn law enforcement officers and their mates, data were obtained showing a significant −.69 correlation between the communication/relationship difficulties subscale and the Dyadic Adjustment Scale (Spainer, 1976), “indicating divergent validity.” Additionally, Bartlett’s independent “Police Issues Interview” correlated .80 with the police issues subscale, “indicating that the HSMI and HRI-P police issues sub-scale is assessing a common construct pertaining to issues in the lives of law enforcement personnel and their mates” (Bartlett, 2004). In 2005, Hirshfeld conducted a study of 33 police academy trainees and their mates that examined “secondary stress reactions among spouses and co-resident partners of newly recruited police officers, 6 and 12 months into their police service” (p. 1). Several instruments were administered in this study, including the Critical Incident History Questionnaire (CIHQ), PTSD Checklist (PCL), Symptom Checklist 90-Revised (SCL-90-R), and Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), among others. This study’s goal was to “shed light on the conceptual ambiguities surrounding current discussions of secondary stress” (p. 1). Other articles continued to highlight police family issues in the 2000s. In 2001, Roberts and Levenson commented on the connection between work and family life, stating that they are the “two major domains in our lives” (p. 1052). They further noted, “Research suggests that experiences from one domain can spill over into, or impact the other.” These authors commented that police spouses “absorb” (p. 1052) much of the officer’s stress and emotional distress. Kruger and Valltos (2002) added: Ironically, individuals who make good law enforcement officers often share some personality traits with those who batter or abuse their family members, such as the inclination to maintain control in emotional and tense circumstances, the tendency to establish a position 250

Couples Counseling

of power and authority, and the physical presence to use weapons and other methods of physical control when needed. (pp. 2–3) Johnson, Todd, and Subramanian (2005) discussed the theory that individuals who marry law enforcement officers “marry into the police family and are expected to adhere to the values and norms” of police culture. These authors explained: In exchange for the spouse’s loyalty, the department promises to be there (in active duty and in death) for the officer and (his/her) family. This can become problematic when the police spouse calls 911 for protection against an abusive spouse. . . . While a wealth of information and resources exist for domestic violence victims . . . police spouse victims are still as isolated and invisible as victims were 30 years ago. (p. 4) In 2007, Miller stated: Research suggests that if police families can survive for the first three years, they have no greater risk for breaking up than other families. Furthermore, second police marriages tend to be even more stable and to endure as well or better than first or second marriages in the general population. (p. 21) Miller’s therapeutic goals stressed “(1) strengthening the demarcation line between the marriage and the police department demands; (2) reducing divided loyalties, triangulation, and jealousies between the job and the marriage; and (3) helping the couple strive towards bonding and intimacy” (Miller, 2007, p. 24). In 2008, Davidson and Moss found that officers who disclosed insights into their traumatic experiences to their spouses did not reduce their psychological stress symptoms. These authors further noted that mental health providers need to develop new methodologies to assist officers in extracting meaning from the traumatic aspects of their work in order to reduce psychological adjustment difficulties. Finally, Violanti et al. (2008) noted that “marriage has been identified as a protective factor against suicide ideation and suicide” (p. 43).

2009  Survey of Police Psychologists and Couples Counseling In order to gather information about current trends in counseling, research, and assessment of police couples, a survey/interview was conducted by the first author while attending three separate national police psychology conferences in 2009. In-person interviews were conducted with participants at the annual meetings of the Police Psychological Services Section of the International Association for Chiefs of Police (IACP), the Society for Police and Criminal Psychology (SPCP), and the Consortium for Police Psychologists (COPPS). A total of 68 practicing police psychologists were interviewed directly about their work with police couples (where at least one of the partners held a position in law enforcement). Of the 68 police psychologists interviewed, 47 (or 69%) had conducted therapy sessions with police couples during their careers. Together, these psychologists reported that they had counseled a total of 6,285 police couples (median of 24 couples per psychologist) over an average of 17 years in the field (median of 16 years per psychologist). This group of psychologists also represented the most active police psychology professionals in the United States, who regularly attend the specialized 251

Robin Inwald, et al.

police psychology conferences and contribute articles and presentations in their field of specialization. When questioned about their theoretical orientation/approach to counseling police couples, there was little consensus. The most popular methodological approach was cognitive-behavioral therapy (mentioned by 18 psychologists, or 26% of those who conducted therapeutic sessions with police couples), followed by systems approach (mentioned by seven psychologists, or 10%), and John Gottman’s relationship research (mentioned by four psychologists, or 6%). Eleven psychologists (16%) said they were eclectic in their approaches to counseling police couples. Few police psychologists had used standardized assessment tools with their police couples, though 16 (23.5%) mentioned occasional use of instruments such as the HSMI/HRI-P, Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI), Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI), California Psychological Inventory (CPI), Sixteen Personality Factor Questionnaire (16PF), Inwald Personality Inventory (IPI), Symptom Checklist-90 (SCL-90), and Fundamental Interpersonal Relations Orientation (FIRO-B). Other than the research studies previously mentioned that were conducted using Inwald’s HSMI and HRI-P, no additional research on police couples had been conducted by the 68 police psychologists in this survey.

Two Police Couple Case Studies Description of Instruments Used The Inwald Research Relationship Surveys, including the Inwald Couples Compatibility Questionnaire (ICCQ), Inwald Partner’s Personality Inventory (IPPI), Inwald Personality Survey (IPS), Inwald Attitude Survey (IAS), and Inwald Trauma Recovery Inventory (ITRI), are used for the purpose of public safety officer relationship assessment and couples counseling in employee assistance programs (Inwald, 2006a, 2006b, 2006d). The IPS and the IAS, each containing under 110 true/false items, are used for self-assessment with regard to relationship issues. The IPS is a derivative of the Hilson Personnel Profile/Success Quotient (HPP/SQ), the first comprehensive personality inventory measuring “emotional intelligence” (Inwald, 2008; Inwald & Brobst, 1988; Inwald & Brockwell, 1988). The IAS is a derivative of the Inwald Survey 5 (IS5), an instrument that includes scales measuring areas of “integrity” and anger management, among others (Inwald & Gebbia, 1992). The ICCQ and IPPI are surveys completed by individuals about their partners/spouses. The ICCQ, containing 148 true/false items, focuses on relationship compatibility; the IPPI, containing 128 true/false items, focuses on the partner’s behavior patterns that may negatively affect relationships such as antisocial attitudes and substance abuse tendencies (see Appendix A for the individual scale names and descriptions of Inwald Research Relationship Surveys). All four components of the Inwald Research Relationship Surveys were developed after a series of FBI conferences provided information suggesting that relationship problems are often triggers for police officer suicides and violent behavior. The Inwald Trauma Recovery Inventory (ITRI) was developed by the first author as a result of discussions at the 1998 FBI Conference on Domestic Violence by Police Officers and the recent influx of officers returning from overseas military duty in the Middle East (Inwald, 1998; Inwald, 2006c). This instrument, containing 111 true/false items, focuses on the assessment of reactions to traumatic events as well as the identification of an individual’s current feelings of depression, anger, and/or stress. It can be administered to an individual or used as part of a comprehensive battery to evaluate a couple’s coping skills. The Inwald Research Relationship Surveys, used with or without the ITRI, are the first comprehensive partner/spouse-oriented surveys developed for use as a preventative approach, providing actuarial “relationship check-ups” (Inwald, 2010, p. 4) to assist public safety officers. An employee assistance counselor or family psychotherapist can interpret narrative results to couples and review 252

Couples Counseling

comparative profile graphs, showing each individual how his or her admitted behavior patterns and/or personality characteristics may be affecting the relationship. Providing and explaining survey results with the aid of a tangible graph format to officers, who may be defensive about admitting to their shortcomings, can help them better understand how their behavior patterns might be improved. In addition, the therapist can use the visual documentation of the profiles, along with the printed individual item endorsements, to develop a treatment plan with the direct involvement of each couple. Finally, public safety agencies can offer the use of accessible online surveys, such as these relationship surveys, as a way to demonstrate their commitment to maintaining a preventative approach to domestic violence in police families.

Couple Case Study A The Inwald Research Relationship Surveys, including the Inwald Couples Compatibility Questionnaire (ICCQ), Inwald Partners Personality Inventory (IPPI), Inwald Personality Survey (IPS), and the Inwald Attitude Survey (IAS), were administered to a 32-year-old public safety officer, Jim, and his 29-year-old wife, Amy (see Appendix A for each survey’s scale names and descriptions). Jim has been working as a patrol officer for a small Midwestern department for 10 years and has specialized as a K-9 Handler for the past 31/2 years. Amy has been working in a dispatch center for the past four years. The couple has four children together. Jim’s IPS results (see Figure 12.1) showed him to be an academically oriented (AI, Academic Interest), socially sensitive (SC, Social Approval Concerns), and driven/hardworking person (EF, Effort toward Responsibilities) with a strong work ethic (SR, Sensitivity about Responsibility). Liked by his peers (PP, Popularity with Peers), he appeared sensitive to how he is perceived by others. It was reported that, in his department, Jim takes his responsibilities as a public safety officer very seriously and is highly organized when it comes to work. Jim’s high TF score (Limited Tolerance of Frustration) on the IAS (see Figure 12.2) suggests that he may be an impatient person who tends to overreact when frustrated. Jim readily admitted to his shortcomings (AF, Admission of Faults), including having a temper, but also endorsed items showing that he does not trust others easily (TN, Lack of Trusting Nature). Finally, Jim’s high CS (Composure under Stress Difficulties) score suggests a sense of entitlement or belief that he would be able to “beat the system.” Jim’s elevated levels of cynicism and distrust may reflect his personal style or attitudes developed after working in the law enforcement field for over 10 years.

100

T-Score

90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0

AF EM AI OG PP 61 63 70 52 61

SC 70

Figure 12.1 IPS for Jim—Couple Case Study A

253

SB 55

EF 65

TR 50

SR 66

SS 63

AQ 75

Robin Inwald, et al. 100

T-Score

90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0

AW 29

TF 79

TN 68

JA 48

CS 77

II 57

OT 68

Figure 12.2 IAS for Jim—Couple Case Study A 100

T-Score

90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0

CI AF BC FD SN FA HA RE CB JE TE IA BL IB SF RC CQ 35 65 55 45 45 50 60 45 30 10 40 50 70 36 37 14 33

Figure 12.3 ICCQ for Jim—Couple Case Study A

With regard to how he views Amy, Jim’s ICCQ (see Figure 12.3) indicates that, despite several areas of admitted incompatibility, Jim wants to stay in this relationship (high BL, Bottom Line). Jim reported feeling a high level of affection and physical attraction for Amy (AF, Affection/Physical Attraction) as well as compatibility in the area of general habits (HA, Habits/Issues Compatibility). However, Jim also expressed that he feels Amy is often very jealous and controlling of him (low JE score, Lack of Jealous Behavior, and low CB score, Lack of Controlling Behavior). Finally, his ICCQ scores suggest that Jim and his wife do not share the same interests or backgrounds (CI, Interests Compatibility, IB, Interests/Background Compatibility). Jim’s IPPI (see Figure 12.4) about Amy suggests that he believes she is a moderate drinker (AU, Alcohol Use Patterns) with some past work adjustment difficulties (LR, Reliability). Amy’s IPS results (see Figure 12.5) indicated that, like her husband, she was candid on the survey and readily admitted to her flaws and/or shortcomings (AF, Admission of Faults). Somewhat extroverted (OG, “Outgoing” Personality) and liked by her peers (PP, Popularity with Peers), Amy appears to have a strong need for obtaining the approval of others, and she also may tend to be highly 254

Couples Counseling 100

T-Score

90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0

AV 44

AU 65

DU 45

BL 55

EP 58

LR 67

CI 59

OV 60

Figure 12.4 IPPI for Jim—Couple Case Study A

100

T-Score

90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0

AF 75

EM 68

AI 55

OG 62

PP 69

SC 70

SB 55

EF 40

TR 17

SR 53

SS 72

AQ 71

Figure 12.5 IPS for Amy—Couple Case Study A

sensitive to criticism (SC, Social Approval Concerns). However, unlike her husband, she is a procrastinator (TR, Timeliness about Responsibilities) and is not particularly driven or motivated at work (EF, Effort toward Responsibilities). She cares about helping others (EM, Empathy/Helping Others) and may use her social skills to counsel those who come to her for advice/support. Results on the IAS (see Figure 12.6) suggest that Amy has had some difficulties in the area of work, such as a history of absence and/or lateness ( JA, Job/Career Adjustment Difficulties). This was corroborated by Jim’s IPPI about Amy, suggesting past work adjustment difficulties. Amy also appeared to be somewhat suspicious of the motives of others (TN, Lack of Trusting Nature) and showed a tendency to become easily frustrated (TF, Limited Tolerance of Frustration). These characteristics may cause some difficulties in Amy’s marriage because she may tend to be overly sensitive (IPS results above), suspicious, and somewhat rebellious (IAS results) at times. Amy’s admissions on the IAS are corroborated by Jim’s ICCQ results above, where he reported that Amy was both jealous and controlling in their relationship. Amy’s ICCQ (see Figure 12.7) suggests that she, like Jim, feels a strong physical affection/ attraction to her spouse (AF, Affection/Physical Attraction) and wants their relationship to continue (BL, Bottom Line), despite some family differences (FA, Family Compatibility). While generally content 255

Robin Inwald, et al. 100

T-Score

90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0

AW 32

TF 66

TN 72

JA 77

CS 49

II 57

OT 68

Figure 12.6 IAS for Amy—Couple Case Study A 100

T-Score

90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0

CI AF BC FD SN FA HA RE CB JE TE IA BL IB SF RC CQ 60 70 60 25 50 35 60 45 50 20 35 70 70 42 30 26 38

Figure 12.7 ICCQ for Amy—Couple Case Study A

with her marriage, Amy endorsed items indicating that Jim may tend to be a somewhat jealous person ( JE, Lack of Jealousy) who loses his temper under stress (TE, Temper Control) and is very assertive with her (IA, Interpersonal Assertiveness). According to Amy, financial issues have negatively affected this relationship (FD, Financial/Functional Compatibility). Amy’s ICCQ suggests that this couple shares more interests in common than were reported by her husband (CI, Interests Compatibility). It is interesting to note that Amy’s IPPI results (see Figure 12.8) about Jim are very similar to his views about her on the same issues. Amy indicated that she believes Jim has a moderate to high tolerance/use of alcohol (AU, Alcohol Use) and that he also has a history of absence abuse (e.g., taking sick time from work when not really ill: LR, Reliability). In an interview with the second author, Jim reported it was not surprising to him that the survey results suggested a high level of jealousy in the relationship because both he and Amy are working in law enforcement. Jim mentioned that he often hears about police officers from different departments exchanging stories about other officers and their relationships with the dispatchers. He admitted, “It 256

Couples Counseling 100

T-Score

90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0

AV 34

AU 69

DU 45

BL 60

EP 53

LR 71

CI 63

OV 59

Figure 12.8 IPPI for Amy—Couple Case Study A

bothers me from time to time that they are always talking a lot with the dispatchers, especially with Amy.” Jim stated that he feels Amy is often “controlling and unreliable in certain areas” and that he is annoyed when she questions him, from time to time, about the messages he sends to other dispatchers, regardless of their content. Jim admitted that he becomes upset when questioned, causing both partners to struggle with the issue of jealousy. This couple also expressed a strong commitment to police work, mentioning that having different schedules was helpful in allowing individual time with their four children and then time with each other while the children were at school. Jim and Amy felt that this was beneficial in raising their family because at least one parent always was home to supervise the children. Finally, Jim reported that he and Amy found the feedback from the Inwald Research Relationship Surveys to be very helpful and remarked that the information gave them “something to work on.” Despite some differences, Amy and Jim appeared firmly committed to their marriage, with each partner presenting a candid and realistic evaluation of the positive and negative characteristics defining their relationship.

Couple Case Study B The Inwald Research Relationship Surveys, including the Inwald Couples Compatibility Questionnaire (ICCQ), Inwald Partners Personality Inventory (IPPI), Inwald Personality Survey (IPS), and Inwald Attitude Survey (IAS), along with the Inwald Trauma Recovery Inventory (ITRI), were administered to a 27-year-old male public safety officer, Peter, and his 28-year-old wife, Stacy (see Appendices A and B for each survey’s scale names and descriptions). Peter began working in the law enforcement field when he was 19, first as a dispatcher for three years and then as a crime scene technician for two years. He had worked for one year as a part-time patrol officer and had been working for one year as a full-time officer in a small southern police agency when he shot and killed a mentally ill man who was shooting at him. Although later cleared by his department, Peter fired 10 shots at the man, hitting him eight times. Peter began counseling approximately one month after this deadly force incident. After meeting through the Internet, Peter and Stacy were married for eight years and had three children together. They separated one month prior to testing. Peter’s high score on the EM (Empathy/Helping Others) scale of the IPS (see Figure 12.9) suggests that he has a strong interest in coaching or counseling others and that he is the type of person others may seek out for advice. His relatively high EF (Efforts toward Responsibility) score suggests that this officer is a dedicated worker who will put in extra time to meet his responsibilities. Peter recently worked to develop a peer-counseling network in his law enforcement community. 257

Robin Inwald, et al. 100

T-Score

90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0

AF 64

EM 63

AI 55

OG 33

PP 36

SC 51

SB 30

EF 65

TR 56

SR 53

SS 34

AQ 51

Figure 12.9 IPS for Peter—Couple Case Study B

This officer appears to set very high standards for himself and, at times, can be highly self-critical as indicated by his high AF (Admission of Faults) score and low SB (Self-Belief ) score. An introverted person, as indicated by a low OG (Outgoing Personality) score, he may lack self-confidence at times (low SB score) and have some difficulties communicating his needs to others. Peter’s relatively high TF (Limited Tolerance of Frustration) score on the IAS (see Figure 12.10) suggests that he may be a somewhat impatient person who has difficulty tolerating frustration. He may tend to overreact or become argumentative in situations where he feels misunderstood. Peter’s ITRI scores (see Figure 12.11) show evidence of depression, as indicated by his elevated LS (Lack of Satisfaction with Life) score and individual item endorsements, suggesting suicidal thoughts. In addition, elevations on both SN (Lack of Social Network) and FS (Lack of Family Support) reflect his belief that he lacks a significant social network and family support. Finally, Peter’s elevated SW (Lack of Self-Worth) score suggests a low level of self-esteem, in agreement with his IPS results. Several ITRI item endorsements verify that he believes his deadly force incident had a serious negative effect, resulting in increased frustration, temper outbursts, and feelings of being out of control. With regard to how he views Stacy, Peter’s ICCQ (see Figure 12.12) indicates serious difficulties in the overall relationship, as demonstrated by a low BL (Bottom Line) and CQ (Compatibility Quotient). Peter’s low scores on his ICCQ suggest that, other than agreeing that they do not have a problem with jealousy ( JE), Peter feels they do not have compatible interests (CI), habits (HA), or compatible financial attitudes (FD). Coming from different backgrounds (BC), he also feels that Stacy lacks a respectful attitude toward him (RE), loses her temper frequently (TE), and has a drinking habit (individual items). Peter’s IPPI about Stacy (see Figure 12.13) verifies his belief that she is a high risk for alcohol abuse (AU = 83t); has shown a history of work adjustment difficulties, confirmed by an elevated EP (Employment Problems) score; and shows evidence of depression (individual Critical Items—CI). Stacy’s IPS results (see Figure 12.14) were similar to Peter’s in that they were both candid on the test (AF), introverted (OG), lacked self-esteem (SB), and demonstrated a desire to help others (EM). Stacy’s IPS also showed her tendency to procrastinate (TR), seek the approval of others, and be overly sensitive to criticism (SC). Stacy had unusually high scores on the IAS (see Figure 12.15), indicating very poor frustration tolerance and temper control (TF = 82t), distrust of others (TN), job adjustment difficulties ( JA), and integrity concerns (II). Her adjustment difficulties admitted on the IAS corroborated Peter’s view of her on the ICCQ and IPPI. 258

100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0

T-Score

AW 32

TF 61

TN 52

JA 48

CS 49

II 52

OT 48

Figure 12.10 IAS for Peter—Couple Case Study B

100

T-Score

90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0

DA 53

PT 55

SS 39

SN 70

FS 63

CE 31

DP 45

SW 62

LS 88

TC 40

OS 53

Figure 12.11 ITRI for Peter—Couple Case Study B

100

T-Score

90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0

CI AF BC FD SN FA HA RE CB JE TE IA BL IB SF RC CQ 25 45 35 45 55 35 35 30 50 60 25 40 15 66 33 8 17

Figure 12.12 ICCQ for Peter—Couple Case Study B

100

T-Score

90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0

AV 39

AU 83

DU 54

BL 39

EP 63

LR 62

CI 80

OV 70

Figure 12.13 IPPI for Peter—Couple Case Study B

100

T-Score

90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0

AF 77

EM 68

AI 40

OG 43

PP 47

SC 70

SB 20

EF 55

TR 17

SR 66

SS 50

Figure 12.14 IPS for Stacy—Couple Case Study B

100

T-Score

90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0

AW 26

TF 82

TN 76

Figure 12.15 IAS for Stacy—Couple Case Study B

JA 66

CS 55

II 62

OT 73

AQ 55

Couples Counseling

ITRI results for Stacy (see Figure 12.16) indicated that a traumatic event has been significant in her life (PT). Suicidal thoughts and depression (LS = 100t and 28 Critical Items) were admitted along with difficulties with temper control (TC) and low self-worth (SW). Peter reported to his therapist that Stacy had come from an abusive family background and had “cut herself ” in a bid for attention on several occasions. With regard to how Stacy viewed Peter, Stacy’s ICCQ (see Figure 12.17) also indicated serious difficulties in the relationship (low RC and CQ). However, she had higher overall scores and reported that there was much more physical attraction/affection in the relationship than Peter had indicated in his responses (AF). Stacy’s low scores on her ICCQ suggested that she felt Peter lacked a respectful attitude toward her (RE) and lost his temper frequently (TE), and that they had some significant family conflicts/incompatibility (FA). 100

T-Score

90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0

DA 82

PT 71

SS 44

SN 62

FS 53

CE 56

DP 68

SW 70

LS 100

TC 63

OS 83

Figure 12.16 ITRI for Stacy—Couple Case Study B

100

T-Score

90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0

CI AF BC FD SN FA HA RE CB JE TE IA BL IB SF RC CQ 55 70 40 50 50 20 60 20 35 70 40 50 40 42 20 18 29

Figure 12.17 ICCQ for Stacy—Couple Case Study B

261

Robin Inwald, et al. 100

T-Score

90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0

AV 31

AU 69

DU 45

BL 69

EP 62

LR 51

CI 89

OV 72

Figure 12.18 IPPI for Stacy—Couple Case Study B

Stacy’s IPPI about Peter (see Figure 12.18) indicated that she believes he may abuse alcohol (AU), has had several incidents of antisocial behavior (BL), and has shown signs of depression and difficulty controlling his temper (Critical Items). Peter admitted in therapy that he had been drinking more often since the deadly force incident because of his having nightmares and emotional difficulties. He also admitted to putting his fist through a wall. He acknowledged that when Stacy became pregnant, he had decided to become a police officer in order to support the family. It appeared to the therapist that, although Peter took his responsibilities seriously with regard to both the police department and his family, he also felt great stress on his job and was neither fully committed nor well suited for police work as a profession. His interest in counseling and possible goal of obtaining certification in that field was encouraged by the therapist as a future career option. While Stacy did not want to end the marriage, this couple’s unhappiness and incompatibility are evident in their combined survey results. In addition, both endorsed the ICCQ item suggesting that they have “serious differences of opinion related to children.” With therapy, Peter claimed there were improvements in a few of his problem areas. However, the survey results reveal a relationship that will be difficult to salvage due to admitted alcohol use, lack of temper control, impulsive reactions, and expressed incompatibility in several key areas.

Summary After reviewing the available literature and surveying dozens of practicing police psychologists, it is clear that there still is much interest, though relatively little quantitative research, in the area of police couples and police marriages. At a time when public safety departments are cutting back on recruitment because of severe budget restrictions, it is especially important to care for the mental and physical health of working law enforcement officers. Also, with a reduction in pre-employment psychological screening duties, police psychologists may have more available time to attend to new areas of development, such as “police family wellness.” Since it has been established that an officer’s home life, along with a positive significant relationship, is a key factor in maintaining that officer’s sense of well-being, a new focus on helping police couples with their individual relationship challenges may prove valuable for increasing work productivity as well. When presented in a nonjudgmental atmosphere that does not compromise the officer’s

262

Couples Counseling

work status, voluntary “relationship checkups,” with reliable feedback from an experienced police psychologist or trained peer counselor, may become one standard early intervention strategy that will benefit both the individual couples and the police agencies that hire them. It is hoped that the information in this chapter, as well as the development of the Inwald Research Relationship Surveys, will be of use to police psychologists and law enforcement department administrators as they work together to support the physical and emotional health of their public safety officers.

Author Note Inquiries about the Inwald Research Relationship Surveys presented in this chapter, including the Inwald Couples Compatibility Questionnaire (ICCQ), Inwald Partners Personality Inventory (IPPI), Inwald Personality Survey (IPS), Inwald Attitude Survey (IAS), or Inwald Trauma Recovery Inventory (ITRI), should be addressed to: Inwald Research, Inc., P.O. Box 73, Cleverdale, NY 12820; phone: 917–757–9063 or 866–508–2224. There also are free computer administration and test-scoring services available for individuals and/or agencies interested in participating in research using this test battery with police and other couples in the public safety field. Original independent research studies are encouraged, and training will be provided for giving appropriate feedback to couples who complete the surveys. For further information, please contact the authors.

References and Further Reading Aumiller, G. (1995). Keeping it simple: Sorting out what really matters in your life. Avon, MA: Adams Media. Bartlett, K. G. (2004). A validation study of the Hilson Relationship Inventory of Public Safety Personnel (HRI-P) and the Hilson Spouse/Mate Inventory (HSMI). Doctoral dissertation, Wright Institute Graduate School of Psychology, Berkeley, CA. Bohl, N., & Solomon, R. M. (1994). Male law enforcement officers and their spouses’ perceptions to postshooting reactions. In E. Scrivner (Ed.), Law enforcement families: Issues and answers (pp. 101–109). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Borum, R., & Philpot, C. (1993). Therapy with law enforcement couples: Clinical management of the high-risk lifestyle. The American Journal of Family Therapy, 21(2), 122–135. Danto, B. (1978). Police stress. Police Stress, 1(3), 32–36. Davidson, A. C., & Moss, S. A. (2008). Examining the trauma disclosure of police officers to their partners and officers’ subsequent adjustment. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 27(1), 51–70. Flater, L. (1994). Officer/spouse workshops: A prevention and intervention technique. In E. Scrivner (Ed.), Law enforcement families: Issues and answers (pp. 329–336). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Gentz, D., & Taylor, D. (1994). Marital status and attitude about divorce among veteran law enforcement officers. In E. Scrivner (Ed.), Law enforcement families: Issues and answers (pp. 67–71). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Hays, G. (1994). Police couples: Breaking the security code. In E. Scrivner (Ed.), Law enforcement families: Issues and answers (pp. 337–344). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Hirshfeld, A. (2005). Secondary effects of traumatization among spouses and partners of newly recruited police officers. Doctoral dissertation, Alliant International University. Inwald, R. (1993). Hilson Relationships Inventory for public safety personnel and Hilson/Spouse Mate Inventory. New York: Hilson Research. Inwald, R. (1998, September 17–18). Psychological profiles of police and public safety officers involved with domestic violence. Paper prepared and presented at the Domestic Violence by Police Officers Conference, FBI Academy, Quantico, VA. Inwald, R. (2006a, May 19). Assessment of marital issues for law enforcement couples. Annual Conference of the Consortium of Police Psychologists, Fort Lauderdale, FL. Inwald, R. (2006b, October 14). Identifying relationship difficulties in police couples using the Inwald Relationship Assessment Battery. Annual Conference of the International Association of Chiefs of Police, Boston, MA.

263

Robin Inwald, et al. Inwald, R. (2006c, October 15). Use of a customized Hilson Research test battery to identify stress susceptibility in returning military veterans. Annual Conference of the International Association of Chiefs of Police, Boston, MA. Inwald, R. (2006d, October 26). Inwald Couples Evaluation Program (ICEP): An evaluation of the profiles and problems of police couples. Annual Conference of the Society for Police and Criminal Psychology, Washington DC. Inwald, R. (2010). Use of the Inwald Personality Inventory, Hilson tests, and Inwald Surveys for selection, “fitness-for-duty” assessment, and relationship counseling. In P. A. Weiss (Ed.), Personality assessment in police psychology (pp. 91–131). Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas. Inwald, R., & Brobst, K. E. (1988). Hilson Personnel Profile/Success Quotient (HPP/SQ) technical manual. New York: Hilson Research. Inwald, R., & Brockwell, A. (1988). Success quotient profiles of law enforcement administrators. New York: Hilson Research. Inwald, R., & Gebbia, M. (1997). Hilson Spouse/Mate Inventory & Hilson Relationship Inventory technical manual. Kew Gardens, NJ: Hilson Research. Inwald, R., Gebbia, M., & Resko, J. (1993). Three studies of police/spouse mate relationships using the Hilson Spouse/Mate Inventory. In E. Scrivner (Ed.), Law enforcement families: Issues and answers. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Inwald, R., Traynor, W., & Favuzza, V. (2000). Psychological profiles of police and public safety officers accused of domestic violence. In Domestic Violence by Police Officers. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Inwald, R. E. (2008). The Inwald Personality Inventory (IPI) and Hilson Research Inventories: Development & rationale. Journal of Aggression and Violent Behavior, 13, 298–327. Inwald, R. E., & Gebbia, M. I. (1992). Inwald Survey 5 (IS5) technical manual. New York: Hilson Research. Janik, J., & Kravitz, H. (1994). Police suicides: Trouble at home. In E. Scrivner (Ed.), Law enforcement families: Issues and answers (pp. 73–81). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Johnson, L. B., Todd, M., & Subramanian, G. (2005). Violence in police families: Work-family spillover. Journal of Family Violence, 20(1), 3–12. Kaufmann, G., Smith, R., & Palmatier, J. (1994). An assessment of undercover officer spouse/mate relationships. In E. Scrivner (Ed.), Law enforcement families: Issues and answers (pp. 111–116). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Kirshman, E. (1997). I love a cop: What police families need to know. New York: Guilford. Kirshman, E. (2007). I love a cop: What police families need to know (Rev. ed.). New York: Guilford. Klein, R. (1994). Training peer counselors to provide the initial intervention in law enforcement relationship problems. In E. Scrivner (Ed.), Law enforcement families: Issues and answers (pp. 345–351). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Kruger, K. J., & Valltos, N. G. (2002, July). Dealing with domestic violence in law enforcement relationships. FBI Law Enforcement Bulletin. Maynard, P., & Maynard, N. (1980, February). Providing police family stress through couples’ communication training. The Police Chief, pp. 30–31. Miller, L. (1995). Tough guys: Psychotherapeutic strategies with law enforcement and emergency personnel. Psychotherapy, 32(4), 592–599. Miller, L. (2007). Police families: Stresses, syndromes and solutions. The American Journal of Family Therapy, 35, 21–40. Reese, J., & Scrivner, E. (1994). Family issues with no easy answers. In E. Scrivner (Ed.), Law enforcement families: Issues and answers (pp. 3–7). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Reese, J. T. (1982, November). Family therapy in law enforcement: A new approach to an old problem. FBI Law Enforcement Bulletin, pp. 1–5. Reiser, M. (1974). Some organizational stresses on policemen. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 2(2), 156–159. Roberts, N. A., & Levenson, R. W. (2001). The remains of the workday: Impact of job stress and exhaustion on marital interaction in police couples. Journal of Marriage and Family, 63, 1052–1067. Rogers, K. (1977). Marriage and the police officer. The Police College Magazine, 14(1), 10. Ryan, A. H., & Brewster, M. E. (1994). Post-traumatic stress disorder and related symptoms in traumatized police officers and their spouses/mates. In E. Scrivner (Ed.), Law enforcement families: Issues and answers (pp. 217–225). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Sgambelluri, A. V. (2000). Police culture, police training, and police administration: Their impact on violence in police families. In D. Sheehan (Ed.), Domestic violence by law enforcement officers (pp. 309–322). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.

264

Couples Counseling Spainer, G. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: New scales for assessing the quality of marriage and similar dyads. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 38, 15–28. Terry, W. (1981). The empirical evidence. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 9(1) 61–75. Violanti, J. M., Fekedulegn, D., Charles, L. E., Andrew, M. E., Hartley, T. A., Mnatsakanova, A., & Burchfield, C. M. (2008). Suicide in police work: Exploring potential contributing influences. American Journal of Criminal Justice, 34, 41–53.

265

Appendix A INWALD RESEARCH RELATIONSHIP

Surveys—Scale Descriptions Inwald Personality Survey (IPS) Scale Descriptions Admission of Faults (AF) Measures degree of defensive responding on this survey. Low scores suggest a desire to appear unusually virtuous and without fault. Low scores on this scale indicate that the other test scores may be inflated due to “socially-desirable” responses.

Empathy/Helping Others (EM) Individuals with high scores on this scale may be skilled in counseling and coaching others. Such individuals enjoy making suggestions and giving people advice regarding their problems and/ or goals.

Academic Interest/New Skills Development (AI) Measures achievement in past jobs and school. High scores suggest excellence in academic/work history as well as past recognition for special skills and/or talents. General level of academic ability may be reflected in this scale.

SA—Social Ability Content Areas (OG, PP, SC, SB) OUTGOING PERSONALITY (OG)

Measures tendency to be outgoing and talkative. High scores suggest an extroverted, outer-directed individual. POPULARITY WITH PEERS (PP)

Measures degree of popularity this person has enjoyed in the past. High scores suggest a charismatic individual who is liked by others and may have been frequently chosen as a leader or spokesperson.

266

Couples Counseling SOCIAL APPROVAL CONCERNS (SC)

Measures degree of sensitivity regarding social approval. High scorers are particularly aware of how their behavior is being judged by others and strive to gain others’ approval. High scorers may become upset when they feel they have said the wrong thing or hurt someone else’s feelings. SELF-BELIEF (SB)

Measures self-confidence and general sense of mastery over obstacles in the world. High scorers are sure of themselves and feel they can do most things well.

IN—Initiative Content Areas (EF, TR, SR) EFFORT TOWARD RESPONSIBILITIES (EF)

Measures the tendency to go the extra mile and strive for completion of tasks and excellence. High scorers may tend to work harder than their peers and may demonstrate qualities of workaholism. They may also show the ability to focus or concentrate on tasks for long periods of time. TIMELINESS ABOUT RESPONSIBILITIES (TR)

Measures the tendency to avoid procrastination. High scorers tend to complete work on time, to organize work when it is assigned, and to meet responsibilities in a timely fashion. Low scorers may complete their work on or close to schedule, but they tend to put it off until the last minute. SENSITIVITY ABOUT RESPONSIBILITIES (SR)

Measures level of concern regarding completing assigned tasks in a correct and/or timely manner. Anxiety over incomplete or unsatisfactory work is common for high scorers, who try to meet responsibilities so that they will not feel guilty or pressured. SOCIAL SKILLS (SS)

Measures overall social/communication skills by combining three scales: OG + PP + SC.

Adaptability Quotient (AQ) Total Score Measures potential for success and emotional intelligence. High scores on the combination of all IPS scales suggest higher overall potential.

Inwald Attitude Survey (IAS) Scale Descriptions Lack of Self-Awareness (AW) Measures the degree to which the individual has been honest or candid about his/her feelings and behaviors. High scores suggest a denial of minor shortcomings in an effort to appear unusually virtuous and without fault. Elevated scores on this scale also may indicate that other tests scores may be deflated due to socially desirable responses.

267

Robin Inwald, et al.

Limited Tolerance of Frustration (TF) Identifies individuals with impulsive behavior patterns and low frustration tolerance. High scores indicate patterns of impulsive, restless behavior. High scorers on this scale may be outspoken and/or impatient with others. Elevated scores also may identify those who express hostility and anger more frequently than others who face similar situations.

Lack of Trusting Nature (TN) High scorers may tend to distance themselves from others and may show distrust regarding the behavior of others. These individuals may be more content in situations that do not require extensive teamwork.

Job/Career Adjustment Difficulties (JA) High scorers on this scale may tend to have job adjustment difficulties and/or difficulties holding jobs. Elevated scorers may have a history of being disciplined for counterproductive behavior.

Composure Under Stress Difficulties (CS) Elevated scores on this scale suggest antisocial attitudes. High scorers may have more relaxed attitudes towards theft when compared with others. These individuals may feel that taking risks, or bending the rules in order to “beat the system,” is justified.

Integrity Issues (II) Elevated scores on this scale suggest a history of brushes with the law and societal norms, especially with regard to theft from employees and/or other parties. The backgrounds of these individuals may include incidents where personal integrity has been questioned.

Overall Total (Risk Score—OT) A combination of all IAS scales measuring overall conscientiousness/reliability. Low scores suggest lower levels of overall conscientiousness/reliability.

Inwald Couples Compatibility Questionnaire (ICCQ) Scale Descriptions Interests/Background Compatibility (IB = CI + AF + BC + FD) INTERESTS COMPATIBILITY (CI)

Identifies compatibility in the area of common interests such as hobbies, shared activities, time spent together, daily routines, and friends. Elevated scores indicate that, as far as the person answering the questionnaire is concerned, there is compatibility in this area. AFFECTION/PHYSICAL COMPATIBILITY (AF)

Identifies the level of physical attraction the person answering this questionnaire feels towards his or her mate. High scores also measure the level of satisfaction with affection received from the other 268

Couples Counseling

person. Low scores suggest that there may not be as much affection and/or physical attraction in the relationship as the person answering the questionnaire would like. BACKGROUND COMPATIBILITY (BC)

Identifies cultural, political, religious, and age similarities for this couple. High scores suggest that both people in this relationship come from similar backgrounds and age groups. Low scores suggest potential difficulties in the relationship based on different cultural, religious, and/or political backgrounds. FINANCIAL/FUNCTIONAL COMPATIBILITY (FD)

Identifies areas of conflict regarding financial matters, including concerns that the other person in the relationship has had financial difficulties and/or career problems that have caused difficulties for the couple.

Social/Family Compatibility (SF = SN + FA) SOCIAL NETWORK COMPATIBILITY (SN)

Identifies the compatibility of this couple with regard to each member’s friends and social network. Low scores suggest that there are serious concerns about the other person’s friends and/or the amount of time spent socializing with them. FAMILY COMPATIBILITY (FA)

Identifies the compatibility of this couple with regard to family members. Low scores suggest problems getting along with certain relatives and/or concerns about this relationship by relatives that may be a source of conflict for this couple.

Relationship Compatibility (RC = HA + RE + CB + JE + TE + IA) HABITS/ISSUES COMPATIBILITY (HA)

Designed to measure concerns about the other person’s habits such as use of alcohol, drugs, personal care, and overeating. Low scores suggest that some habitual behaviors of this nature may be causing friction in the relationship. RESPECTFUL TREATMENT (RE)

Scale results indicate whether or not the person answering this questionnaire believes that his or her mate is respectful and sensitive. High scores suggest concerns about the way he or she is treated by the other person. LACK OF OVER-CONTROLLING BEHAVIOR (CB)

Identifies the presence of over-controlling behavior on the part of the other person. Low scores suggest problems in the relationship due to the other person’s overly critical, moody, or assertive nature. 269

Robin Inwald, et al. LACK OF JEALOUSY (JE)

Documents a pattern of jealous behavior on the part of the other person. A low score on this scale indicates that jealous behavior is causing some discomfort for the person answering this questionnaire. TEMPER CONTROL (TE)

Measures concerns about the other person’s ability to control his or her temper. Low scores indicate that the other person may tend to overreact, may lose his or her temper, and may be unpleasant and complaining when there are conflicts in the relationship. INTERPERSONAL ASSERTIVENESS (IA)

Documents the respondent’s concerns regarding the other person’s level of assertiveness. Low scores suggest that the respondent believes his or her partner lacks assertiveness and may let others take advantage of situations. BOTTOM LINE SUMMARY (BL)

Identifies doubts or insecurities about the relationship. Serious differences about the level of commitment or staying together may be expressed. Low scores suggest that the person answering the questionnaire is unsure about the other person and the possibility of a future together. INTERESTS/BACKGROUND COMPATIBILITY (IB)

Composite score (IB = CI + AF + BC + FD) SOCIAL/FAMILY COMPATIBILITY (SF)

Composite score (SF = SN + FA) RELATIONSHIP COMPATIBILITY (RC)

Composite score (RC = HA + RE + CB + JE + TE + IA) COMPATIBILITY QUOTIENT (CQ)

The Compatibility Quotient is the total score that indicates overall compatibility in the relationship. High scores suggest high compatibility in most areas for this couple.

Inwald Partners Personality Inventory (IPPI) Scale Descriptions Avoidance of Criticism (AV) A high score on this scale indicates that the test-taker believes his or her partner often is not candid about feelings and behaviors. He or she feels that this partner has limited insight and judgment about personal shortcomings. Individuals with high scores feel their partners may tend to give socially acceptable responses to questions.

270

Couples Counseling

Alcohol Use Patterns (AU) High scores suggest that the test-taker believes his or her partner has been a habitual user of alcohol.

Drug Use Patterns (DU) A positive score indicates that the test-taker believes his or her partner has some admitted use of drugs. A high score indicates the partner may be a habitual drug user.

Brushes With the Law and Society (BL)/Legal Problems (LP—Alternate IPPI Version) A high score on this scale indicates the test-taker believes his or her partner has had a history of brushes with the law and/or with societal norms. This may include arrests and/or convictions.

Employment Problems (EP) A high score on this scale indicates the test-taker believes his or her partner has had difficulties holding a job. The partner may have had a spotty employment record and may have a history of interpersonal difficulties at work.

Reliability (Lack of Reliability—LR) A high score here indicates that the test-taker believes his or her partner has a tendency to contract minor illnesses that may keep the partner from working. The partner also may have difficulty meeting job responsibilities and may have a history of abusing sick leave privileges at work.

Critical Items (CI) A list of critical items for further evaluation and discussion.

Overall Score (OV) A combination of all IPPI scales measuring overall potential for difficulties in relationships due to a lack of conscientiousness and/or the presence of antisocial behavior patterns. High scores are suggestive of such patterns.

271

Appendix B INWALD TRAUMA RECOVERY

Inventory (ITRI) Scale Descriptions Difficulties With Daily Activities (DA) Measures the extent to which tested individuals believe they can take care of themselves and their families. Low scorers express confidence that they have the ability to take care of their basic needs. High scorers may show evidence of an inability to function and to provide daily necessities for themselves and their families.

Past Traumatic Events (PT) High scores on this scale suggest that the individual has perceived a specific event as traumatic. Low scores suggest that the individual has not perceived the specified event as a major life trauma.

Stress Symptoms (SS)/Symptoms of Stress (SY—Alternate ITRI Version) Measures admitted physical symptoms or stress reactions that may adversely affect functioning. High scorers admit to physical symptoms that may be related to stressful events.

Social Network (SN) Measures an individual’s social network and social relationships. Low scorers may be socially active people with a wide circle of friends who can provide them with support in times of stress. High scorers may be isolated individuals who do not socialize often and do not have close friends for social support.

Family Support (FS) Measures a person’s family relationships with attention to the degree of closeness with family members. Low scorers may have close relationships with family members and can depend on those family members for support in times of crisis. High scorers do not express strong family ties, may have negative feelings about immediate family members, and may not be able to depend on family for support in times of crisis.

272

Couples Counseling

Critical Events (CE) Measures the presence of recent events that may have affected the person’s ability to enjoy life. Low scorers have not reported many difficulties that could affect their ability to enjoy daily routines. High scorers report that there have been recent life events that have been difficult for them, such as illness, death of a loved one, physical injuries or illness, serious accidents, or natural disasters.

Depression (DP) High scores on this scale indicate discouragement and depression. High scorers may have difficulty coping with daily stresses and/or may show symptoms of clinical or reactive depression.

Lack of Self-Worth (SW) Measures self-confidence and general sense of mastery over obstacles in the world. High scorers may be unsure of themselves and feel they cannot do things as well as others.

Lack of Satisfaction With Life (LS) Measures individuals’ general satisfaction with their accomplishments in life. High scorers may feel that they have not been able to reach their goals and report dissatisfaction with their accomplishments. High scorers may also express disappointment with how their life has developed. Low scorers express contentment that they have been able to reach most goals and that they are satisfied with their accomplishments.

Lack of Temper Control (TC) Measures a history of difficulty controlling angry impulses. High scorers on this scale may be outspoken and/or impatient with others.

Overall Score (OS) Total score for all scales of the ITRI.

273

13 FITNESS-FOR-DUTY EVALUATIONS David M. Corey

Introduction Police employers have a legal duty to ensure that police officers under their command are mentally and emotionally fit to perform their duties, and failure to do so can result in significant civil liability (Bonsignore v. City of New York, 1982) and serious consequences to citizens, the examinee, other officers, an employing agency’s reputation, and trust in the community (Corey, 1988). Various courts have interpreted this duty to include the authority to mandate psychological fitness-for-duty (FFD) evaluations1 of police officers reasonably believed to be impaired in their ability to perform their job functions because of a known or suspected psychological condition (Colon v. City of Newark, 2006; Conte v. Horcher, 1977; Deen v. Darosa, 2005; Kraft v. Police Commissioner of Boston, 1994; McKnight v. Monroe Co. Sheriff’s Dept., 2002; Tingler v. City of Tampa, 1981; Watson v. City of Miami Beach, 1999). The circumstances giving rise to FFD evaluations of police officers are many and varied. They may involve suspicion of job-relevant psychopathology associated with on-duty performance (e.g., excessive force, repeated problems of judgment), off-duty conduct (e.g., domestic violence, driving while intoxicated), a suicide attempt, psychiatric hospitalization, or a disability claim. Stone (2000) reported that 26% of the cases from his own practice in the southern region of the United States resulted from suspected psychopathology (i.e., diagnosable mental condition), 19% from excessive force issues, 15% from substance abuse, 13% from behavior implicating poor judgment, and 9% from domestic violence. Dawkins, Griffin, and Dawkins (2006) utilized an alternative classification scheme for their analysis of the FFD referrals in their own Midwestern practice. Similar to Stone, they found that 16.5% of the more than 200 referrals they analyzed involved alcohol use, but they reported more than twice as many referrals involving domestic violence (20.5%). They reported that 16.3% involved other behavioral concerns, 36.9% pertained to psychopathology or emotional distress, and 4.7% were for officers being considered for rehire following employment separation. The right of a police employer to intrude on the medical and personal privacy of its officers derives from two special features of police work: the power of the position and the fact that police officers are public employees. Police officers are members of quasi-military organizations, called upon for duty at all times, armed at almost all times, and exercising the most awesome and dangerous power that a democratic state possesses with respect to its residents—the power to use lawful force to arrest and detain them. (Policemen’s Benevolent Association of New Jersey v. Township of Washington, 1988, at 141) 274

Fitness-for-Duty Evaluations

As citizens, police officers retain their constitutional rights (e.g., Garrity v. New Jersey, 1967), but as public employees, they “subordinate their right to privacy as a private citizen to the superior right of the public to an efficient and credible police department” (Richardson v. City of Pasadena, 1973/1974, at headnote 1). In the words of the U.S. Supreme Court, “the public should not bear the risk that employees who may suffer from impaired perception and judgment will be [in] positions where they may need to employ deadly force” (National Treasury Employees Union v. Von Raab, 1989, at 671). The employer’s duty to ensure a psychologically fit workforce does not, however, extend an unfettered right to require such evaluations of any police officer in any instance (Denhof et al. v. City of Grand Rapids, 2005; Holst v. Veterans Affairs, 2007; Jackson v. Lake County, 2003; McGreal v. Ostrov, 2004). Instead, the employer’s duty is balanced by public interests and the employee’s constitutional, civil, and property rights and interests. This chapter will explain how FFD examinations, and the findings and opinions that result from them, are shaped and restrained by six overarching considerations: 1. The threshold for determining when an employer may properly require an officer to submit to an FFD evaluation 2. The definition of unfitness 3. The nature of the examiner’s role and relationship to the various parties 4. The scope of the examination and the sources of information relied upon 5. The limitations on disclosure of private information to the employer 6. The examinee’s statutory procedural rights Each of these considerations will be discussed in this chapter in the context of 15 proposed principles for conducting FFD evaluations of police officers organized under four topics: referral issues (Principles 1–7), examination and procedural issues (Principles 8–11), determining fitness (Principle 12), and communicating the results (Principles 13–15).

Principles of Fitness-for-Duty Evaluations Several excellent texts exist on the topic of FFD evaluations of police officers (e.g., Decker, 2006; Rostow & Davis, 2004; Stone, 2000), but they are aimed at a mixed audience of police employers and examiners. This chapter is focused solely on the practical and conceptual requirements of FFD evaluations for police psychologists, beginning with the initial referral and progressing to the examination procedures, formulating an opinion or determination of fitness, and communicating the results. In his seminal book, Principles of Forensic Mental Health Assessment, Heilbrun (2001), as well as Heilbrun, Grisso, and Goldstein (2009), presented a series of established and emerging principles for conducting psychological evaluations for the courts. I have drawn from this framework to present 15 principles of FFD evaluations for police psychologists derived from the professional literature, case law, federal statutes and regulations, practice guidelines, ethical standards, and my own experience conducting more than 1,000 fitness examinations.

Referral Issues Principle 1: Assess How the Employer Met the Legal Threshold for Mandating a Fitness Examination By law, an employer may require an FFD evaluation of an incumbent police officer only when objective facts pose a reasonable basis for concern about his or her fitness (Equal Employment Opportunity Commission [EEOC], 1997; McDonald, Kulick, & Creighton, 1995). This is a central 275

David M. Corey

distinction between FFD evaluations and pre-employment psychological evaluations of police applicants. Under the Americans with Disabilities Act of 1990, when employers require applicants to undergo pre-employment psychological screening, the evaluation must be given to all entering applicants in that job class. In addition, to the extent that the psychological evaluation constitutes a “disability-related question” (i.e., one or more questions likely to elicit information about a disability; see EEOC, 1995, p. 3) or “medical examination” (e.g., a procedure or test that seeks information about an individual’s physical or mental impairments or health; see EEOC, 1995, p. 11), it may be administered only after the employer has given the applicant a conditional offer of employment (29 C.F.R. §1630.14(c); see also Leonel v. American Airlines, 2005). In contrast, when making a disability inquiry or medical examination of an incumbent employee, the ADA requires the employer to meet a fact-specific, individualized threshold, namely, that the questions or examination are “job-related and consistent with business necessity” (42 U.S.C. §12112(d) (4)(A); 29 C.F.R. §1630.14(c)). In general, the ADA regards this threshold as having been met when an employer “has a reasonable belief, based on objective evidence, that (1) an employee’s ability to perform essential job functions will be impaired by a medical condition, or (2) an employee will pose a direct threat due to a medical condition” (EEOC, 2000, Question 5, p. 7). In other words, legal justification for a compulsory mental health examination of an employee requires objective evidence of job-related performance problems or safety threats and a known or reasonably suspected mental condition. As Gold and Shuman (2009) point out, “One of these in the absence of the other represents an insufficient basis for an FFD” evaluation (p. 244). These threshold conditions also are reflected in the “Psychological Fitness-for-Duty Evaluation Guidelines” published by the International Association of Chiefs of Police (IACP, 2009). (Note: The IACP Guidelines are written and ratified by the members of the IACP Police Psychological Services Section, represent best practices of police psychologists who perform these examinations at the request of police employers, and “should guide the expectations of examiners, examinees, and agencies” [Borum, Super, & Rand, 2003, p. 142].) Sometimes this threshold may be met when an employer knows about an employee’s medical condition, has observed performance problems, and reasonably can attribute the problems to the medical condition. An employer also may be given reliable information by a credible third party that an employee has a medical condition, or the employer may observe symptoms indicating that an employee may have a medical condition that will impair his or her ability to perform essential job functions or will pose a direct threat. Although health problems that have had “a substantial and injurious impact on an employee’s job performance” (Yin v. California, 1996, at 868) can justify an FFD evaluation, these are not the only circumstances that satisfy the business necessity standard. Several courts have held that an employer may preemptively require an FFD examination without showing that an employee’s job performance has suffered as a result of health problems, “particularly when the employer is engaged in dangerous work” (Brownfield v. City of Yakima, 2010, slip op. at 10825; see also Cody v. CIGNA Healthcare of St. Louis, Inc., 1998; Watson v. City of Miami Beach, 1999). Although the business necessity standard “is quite high, and is not to be confused with mere expediency” (Cripe v. City of San Jose, 2001, at 890), this objective test may be met without either known medical problems or observed deterioration in the performance of essential job functions. In Brownfield, the court concluded that an officer’s repeated volatile responses to coworkers and supervisors established business necessity for a series of fitness evaluations. Moreover, the court noted, Our consideration of the FFDEs’ legitimacy is heavily colored by the nature of Brownfield’s employment. Police officers are likely to encounter extremely stressful and dangerous situations during the course of their work. When a police department has good reason to doubt an officer’s ability to respond to these situations in an appropriate manner, an FFDE is consistent with the ADA. (slip op. at 10827) 276

Fitness-for-Duty Evaluations

This threshold analysis is a critically important one for the employer, but it is equally crucial for the examining psychologist who wants to avoid becoming the target of litigation where the employee has successfully shown that a disability-related inquiry or medical examination was not job related and consistent with business necessity (Denhof et al. v. City of Grand Rapids, 2005). Indeed, Borum et al. (2003) regard this as “[t]he most fundamental legal issue in FFDEs” (p. 140). IACP guidelines recommend that the employer and examiner “consult before an FFDE commences in order to ensure that an FFDE is indicated in a particular case” (IACP, 2009, Guideline 4.3). An employer’s initial consultation with the examiner may provide important evidence at trial, buttressing its belief that a mental health condition prevented the employee from performing an essential function of the job (Sullivan v. River Valley School District, 1999). As important as the threshold analysis is for managing litigation risk, it also serves two other valuable purposes. First, it provides an opportunity for the examiner to discuss with the employer less intrusive, nonmedical alternatives (IACP, 2009, Guideline 4.2), given that “[t]he stakes involved in an FFD evaluation for both employees and employers cannot be overstated” (Gold & Shuman, 2009, p. 238). Courts also cite the intrusive characteristics of an FFD evaluation, along with the potential adverse impact of disclosure of information gathered in the examination, when considering whether an evaluation was lawfully ordered (Hill v. Winona, 1990; Stewart v. Pearce, 1973). Thus, prereferral consultation between the employer and examiner helps to ensure that other appropriate alternatives are considered before mandating the FFD examination. As Rostow and Davis (2004) note, “In the end, the most appropriate referrals should be work related and reasonably connected to a suspicion of mental or emotional illness” (p. 149). When a referral involves job-relevant behavior reasonably linked to a possible mental health condition, this means only that the employer may order an FFD evaluation, but nothing in the ADA compels it. Indeed, an array of cases illustrate the appropriateness of an employer’s decision to terminate an employee known to have, or reasonably suspected to have, a mental disorder when the employee’s conduct, or behavioral manifestation of the disorder, was such that it rendered him or her unqualified for the position (e.g., Marino v. U.S. Postal Service et al., 1994; Mazzarella v. U.S. Postal Service, 1994; Palmer v. Circuit Court of Cook County, 1997). In general, when an employer knows in advance that it would be unwilling to return an employee to the job in the event that the employee is found fit for duty, alternatives to an FFD examination should be considered. Thus, in a prereferral conference involving cases of gross misconduct, examiners may wish to discuss with the employer the option of considering termination in lieu of an FFD examination. Employers may benefit from being reminded that the ADA generally does not require an employer to tolerate violations of workplace rules and policies, including those that prohibit workplace violence or threats of it, or to retain employees who engage in such violations. Whereas police employers are permitted to mandate FFD evaluations under the conditions discussed above, they are not required to do so as an alternative or precursor to termination. On the other hand, they also may not take a more severe adverse action against an employee with a disability than against a nondisabled employee who engaged in the same misconduct. A second reason for the importance of a prereferral conference between the prospective examiner and the employer is to better understand the employee’s behavioral history, both across the full term of employment and during the recent episode that spurred the referral. This helps the psychologist begin to think about the types and sources of data that may need to be considered during the datagathering process discussed in Principles 8–11. Examiners should be careful to recognize the special consideration afforded return-to-work certifications when the employee is released from medical leave under the Family & Medical Leave Act (FMLA) of 1993. Under the terms of the FMLA, an employee generally may not be compelled by the employer to submit to an independent evaluation of his or her fitness for duty once certified by the treating health care provider as ready to return to work, although pre-leave or post-return 277

David M. Corey

behavior may justify an FFD evaluation (Albert v. Runyon, 1998; see also Brumbalough v. Camelot Care Centers, 2005). Thus, when the FFD evaluation referral results solely from information obtained from FMLA disclosures, the employer should be cautioned to consider the Runyon decision and to consult with legal counsel before proceeding. On the other hand, when an employer can establish that it would have ordered an FFD examination if the employee had not taken leave, an examination may be permissible (Carrillo v. National Council of Churches of Christ in the USA, 1997). Finally, for referrals involving a federal police agency under the authority of the Office of Personnel Management, federal regulations stipulate that these agencies may not refer an employee for psychiatric or psychological examination unless it has first shown through a physical examination that there is “no physical basis to explain actions or behavior which may affect the safe and efficient performance of the individual or others,” and the position has medical standards that call for a psychiatric or psychological examination (5 C.F.R. §339.301(e)(1)). Particularly when an employee is perceived to pose an imminent risk of serious harm to oneself or others, employers often work hastily to obtain an FFD evaluation in short order, and a prereferral conference helps to ensure that these requirements are satisfied before agreeing to conduct an evaluation that may ultimately be deemed invalid under the law.

Principle 2: Identify the Relevant Clinical and Forensic Questions All mental health assessments are properly driven and constrained by the clinical, forensic, and other questions that prompt the evaluation. Because these questions determine the scope of the examination, the kinds of data that will be gathered, how the data will be analyzed, what judgments or determinations will be made, what information will be disclosed (and to whom), and how it will be communicated, it is essential that the examiner first clarify precisely what the employer wants to know. This is often accomplished by a written referral letter from the employer that specifically lists the questions to be addressed in the course of the evaluation. Alternatively, the examiner can prepare a draft letter of his or her understanding of the employer’s referral questions and provide an opportunity for clarification (IACP, 2009, Guideline 7.2). Even when employers do specify their referral questions in writing, they often simply ask the psychologist to evaluate the employee’s fitness for duty, without elaboration. But what exactly is the clinical meaning of fitness for duty? Alternatively, what does it mean to be unfit? Anfang and Wall (2006) point out that fitness for duty has no consistent clinical definition. They propose a clinically operational definition of unfitness as “the inability to perform required occupational duties with reasonable skill and safety as a result of illness or injury” (pp. 676–677). Rostow and Davis (2004) define unfitness as “mental impairment that may impact upon the ability of the officer to perform his duty in a safe and effective manner” (p. 62). The IACP Guidelines conceptualize unfitness in a police officer as being “unable to safely and/or effectively perform his or her duties due to a psychological condition or impairment” (IACP, 2009, Guideline 3.1). Other authors have emphasized the importance of distinguishing problems of unfitness from those associated with unsuitability or misconduct. Writing about FFD evaluations in the military, Budd and Harvey (2006) observed that there are those individuals whose character structure and the associated attitudes, emotions, and/or behaviors are, in the opinion of the provider, the primary sources of their difficulties in the military. A recommendation for [administrative discharge] should be made when the prognosis for rehabilitation is poor and/or the potential for continued difficulty with occupational demands, misconduct or acting out is high . . . and these recommendations are channeled through the command’s legal department instead of the medical board. (pp. 42–43) 278

Fitness-for-Duty Evaluations

Decker (2006) emphasizes the role of an FFD evaluation in ascertaining whether a law enforcement officer’s behavior is simply misconduct or the result of a mental disorder. “If the set of circumstances or the officer’s behavior precipitating the FFD evaluation is found to be the result of misconduct, then disciplinary action is the appropriate course” (p. 4). She noted that it is “particularly important to differentiate ‘simple misconduct’ from bad behavior that is the result of mental illness” (p. 43). Anfang and Wall (2006) echo the importance of the FFD examiner understanding “the distinction between impairment due to psychiatric illness and inability to perform duties separate from psychiatric illness” (p. 677). There is no more fundamental issue in an FFD evaluation than how the examiner defines unfitness. Some examiners are reluctant to restrict a definition of unfitness to one requiring a psychological impairment or condition, noting that problems such as interpersonal passivity or timidity, which may be personality based rather than caused by a mental health condition, can also lead to ineffective performance (Stone, 2000). But broadening the definition to include normal range, albeit problematic, behavior poses several risks. First, because the ADA requires a reasonable suspicion of a mental health condition to justify an FFD evaluation, it seems disingenuous to conclude from such an examination that no mental health condition exists but that the employee is unfit for duty anyway. Certainly there are instances where an employee’s problematic, disruptive, inefficient, unsafe, or even illegal behavior is caused not by a mental or emotional condition but rather from maladaptive personality traits, character deficits, motivational problems, or other nonpathological factors. These employees may be ill-suited for continued employment, and an administrative decision to terminate their employment may very well be justified. But the fact that an examinee in an FFD examination is a “bad” employee, perhaps even undeserving of continued employment, does not require—legally or ethically—that it is the examining psychologist who should make that determination.2 In general, clinical opinions about fitness for duty should rest on evidence that the layperson is not qualified to assess (e.g., signs and symptoms of psychopathology). When judgments about unfitness are based on behaviors and other evidence that an employer, layperson, and psychologist can assess with equal facility—such as in the case of an insubordinate, disruptive, or dishonest police officer whose conduct does not result from a mental health condition—they fall outside the realm of a professional, clinical or “medical” opinion because they are devoid of the special expertise required for such judgments. As Gold and Shuman (2009) note, “[E]valuators should be certain to limit opinions to questions of psychiatric impairment” (p. 261). If examiners conclude or suspect that problematic personality traits; moral turpitude; or deficits in knowledge, skill, practices, or training, unconnected to an underlying psychological condition or disorder, render an employee potentially ineffective, inefficient, or unsafe, they should report this to the referring party and defer any judgments about disposition to the employer (Gold & Shuman, 2009; see also Anfang, Faulkner, Fromson, & Gendel, 2005). A second risk associated with not linking the definition of unfitness to a psychological condition or impairment is that it increases the potential for the examiner to be misused by an employer with illegitimate motives. Gold and Shuman (2009) point out that “forced FFD evaluations” may be used in an attempt to discredit or even terminate an employee. The authors continue: For example, an FFD referral may be made in an attempt to discharge a chronically underperforming employee or as a substitute for discipline, or as a way to gather information to harm the reputation of the [employee] who has brought a complaint against the employer. . . . Participation in such an evaluation represents a misuse of mental health expertise. (p. 244) As noted earlier, an officer’s inability to perform the essential functions of the position due to a mental or emotional impairment is but one of the two purposes for which FFD evaluations are typically sought by employers and permitted under the ADA. The other purpose is to determine 279

David M. Corey

whether an employee poses a “direct threat” due to a medical condition (EEOC, 1991). Employees may be referred for direct threat evaluations whether or not the employer intends to terminate the employee because questions may exist about the potential for the termination itself triggering violence, and how to minimize or manage that risk (Borum et al., 2003). The standards for determining unfitness on the basis of impairment or direct threat will be discussed under Principle 5 (Identify the legal standard for determining fitness). The employer’s referral may contain other questions that the examiner will be asked to address in the course of the FFD evaluation, and the parameters of each should be fully understood by the examining psychologist before proceeding. These may include questions about disability, industrial versus nonindustrial causation, particular types of impairment (e.g., memory or other neuropsychological functioning), accommodation, violence risk management, treatment, effects of medications, and restricted or limited duty.3

Principle 3: Decline the Referral if It Falls Outside Your Area of Expertise or Competence After clarifying the precise clinical and forensic questions, the examining psychologist is better positioned to judge whether he or she has the expertise (i.e., education, training, and experience) to answer the referral questions. Professional ethical standards require that psychologists provide services “with populations and in areas only within the boundaries of their competence, based on their education, training, supervised experience, consultation, study, or professional experience” (APA, 2002, Standard 2.01(a)). These standards also stipulate that “[w]hen assuming forensic roles, psychologists are or become reasonably familiar with the judicial or administrative rules governing their roles” (Standard 2.01(f )). IACP (2009) Guideline 5.1 contains a similar provision, recommending that examiners have the following minimum qualifications: 1. Be licensed as a psychologist or psychiatrist with education, training, and experience in the diagnostic evaluation of mental and emotional disorders 2. Possess training and experience in the evaluation of law enforcement personnel 3. Be familiar with the police psychology literature and the essential job functions of the employee being evaluated 4. Be familiar with relevant state and federal statutes and case law, as well as other legal requirements related to employment and personnel practices (e.g., disability, privacy, third-party liability) 5. Satisfy any other minimum requirements imposed by local jurisdiction or law 6. Recognize their areas of competence based on their education, training, supervised experience, consultation, study, or professional experience 7. Seek appropriate consultation to address issues outside their areas of competence that may arise during the course of an FFD evaluation Upon fully understanding the clinical and forensic questions pertaining to the FFD evaluation referral, the examining psychologist should carefully and candidly assess whether his or her education, training, and experience are sufficient to adequately address the questions and, if not, whether the shortfalls can be remedied through additional study or consultation with a more experienced colleague. Examples include referral questions that require the psychologist to possess competency in a specialty or proficiency (e.g., forensic psychology, neuropsychology, psychopharmacology), specialized knowledge (e.g., familiarity with ADA case law related to a particular impairment, such as the ability to interact with others), or exceptional experience (e.g., a complex, contested, and/or litigated

280

Fitness-for-Duty Evaluations

case with contradictory evidence). Lacking the requisite qualifications, the examiner should decline the referral. FFD evaluations are often referred to as “high-stakes” or “high-risk” evaluations (Anfang & Wall, 2006; Borum et al., 2003), and the legal liability to both the examiner and employer can be great. In addition, the stakes are also very high for the employee, for whom an “unfit” finding may be career ending. Ethical standards require that psychologists “take reasonable steps to avoid harming their . . . organizational clients, and others with whom they work, and to minimize harm where it is foreseeable and unavoidable” (APA, 2002, Standard 3.04). Meeting this ethical requirement may demand that some examiners decline the FFD evaluation referral, agree to address just a subset of the questions posed in the referral, or proceed only after obtaining appropriate consultation. Anfang and Wall (2006) point out that “[t]he legal concepts, evaluation process, and administrative issues may be complex and unfamiliar to the nonforensic clinician. . . . FFD evaluations can often become the subject of administrative or legal dispute because of the significant personal, legal, and financial consequences” (p. 678), and evaluators may be asked to defend their opinions in deposition and under cross-examination during court testimony. Therefore, when accepting a referral for an FFD evaluation that is known or reasonably anticipated “to be in the context of litigation, arbitration, or another adjudicative process, the examiner should be prepared by training and experience to qualify as an expert in any related adjudicative proceeding” (IACP, 2009, Guideline 5.2). Indeed, Gold and Shuman (2009) advise clinicians performing FFD evaluations to “consider the possibility that litigation or administrative processes may arise from claims requiring mental health assessments. Thus, the specialty guidelines for forensic clinicians may be interpreted to apply to third-party evaluations of all kinds whether litigation has occurred or not” (p. 2).

Principle 4: Decline the Referral if You Are Unable to Be Impartial Standards of professional ethics require that psychologists refrain from taking on a professional role when personal, scientific, professional, legal, financial, or other interests or relationships could reasonably be expected to (1) impair their objectivity, competence, or effectiveness in performing their functions as psychologists, or (2) expose the person or organization with whom the professional relationship exists to harm or exploitation. (APA, 2002, Standard 3.06) Similarly, IACP Guidelines state that a prospective FFD examiner should decline to conduct an FFD evaluation when his or her objectivity may be impaired (IACP, 2009, Guideline 6.1). (See also the draft revision of the Specialty Guidelines for Forensic Psychology (SGFP), American Psychology-Law Society, 2008, Guideline 3.02, Impartiality and Fairness.) The necessity for impartiality in the FFD examiner also requires that the examiner have no treatment relationship with the officer being examined (cf. Gold & Shuman, 2009; IACP, 2009, Guideline 6.1.2). Anfang and Wall (2006) observed: For FFD evaluations, as for most forensic examinations, a comprehensive evaluation often requires review of additional information (i.e., collateral contacts; psychological testing, including validity assessment; and possible third-party or surveillance data) and a neutral objectivity that is different from the typical alliance-based, patient-centered treatment relationship. (pp. 677–678)

281

David M. Corey

This is a view long championed by Greenberg and Shuman (1997, 2007), who argue that, among the salient differences between the testimony of treating psychologists and forensic psychologists, the former “is a care provider and usually supportive, accepting, and empathic; the forensic evaluator is an assessor and usually neutral, objective, and detached as to the forensic issues” (p. 53). Whereas the therapist’s role is to be supportive and to advocate for the client’s interests, the forensic evaluator’s role is to exercise “untainted and unbiased judgment” (Greenberg & Shuman, 2007, p. 131). Indeed, these roles are “irreconcilably mutually exclusive” (p. 132). Heilbrun (2001) defines impartiality as “the evaluator’s freedom from significant interference from factors that can result in bias” (pp. 36–37). Some degree of even implicit bias may be unavoidable, but the “crucial test” involves whether that bias “would keep the evaluator from moving from data to whatever conclusions are best supported by such data” (Heilbrun et al., 2008, p. 102). The consequences of conducting an FFD evaluation when impartiality has been lost or compromised, or even when it appears so to an objective observer, can be significant. In Denhof et al. v. City of Grand Rapids (2007), the examining psychologist, prior to conducting the FFD examination, told the police chief that the relationship between the officer and the department was like a “marriage gone bad” and they were “best off simply separating, for the good of all persons involved” (p. 9). The court concluded that the examining psychologist “was predisposed to finding Denhof unfit for duty” and concluded that it was “hard to see any possibility that [the psychologist] would yield a result other than finding that Denhof should be separated from the police force” (p. 9). This fact, the court decided, prevented the employer from claiming an honest belief in the determination of unfitness from the FFD examination, because “reliance on a doctor who had already made up his mind did not qualify as reasonable reliance” (p. 9). The pressure to affirm the perceived or stated expectations of the referring party can be significant in these high-stakes examinations. Psychologists who receive repeated referrals from employers, labor groups, or other sources may create what Anfang and Wall (2006) call “incentive biases” to provide opinions favorable to the perceived or stated expectations of the referring party, thus requiring a decidedly vigilant awareness of, and defense against, the various sources of bias (Gold & Shuman, 2009). Conversely, Anfang and Wall point out that “when the forensic examiner is retained directly by the examinee or his attorney, high-stakes cases can potentially lead to dramatically partisan and conflicted circumstances” (p. 678). Novice examiners sometimes scoff at the notion of impartiality in FFD evaluations, believing that the employer, as client, is the sole beneficiary of the examiner’s professional duties, that no obligations extend to the officer being evaluated, and therefore there is no requirement for impartiality. Fisher (2009), however, points out that “psychologists have ethical obligations toward every party in a case, no matter how many or how named” (p. 1). Koocher (2007) agrees, noting that “both the entity requesting the service and the person undergoing evaluation hold a kind of client status in such cases” (p. 380). Writing specifically about FFD evaluations of police officers, Stone (2000) observed, While the examination is an independent forensic evaluation conducted on behalf of the employer, the employee also has standing as a client. As such, the employee’s welfare should be given substantial weight in determining whether and how to undertake the evaluation. (p. 130) The IACP Guidelines explicitly acknowledge this as well: Regardless of who is identified as the client, the examiner owes an ethical duty to both parties to be fair, impartial, accurate and objective, and to honor the parties’ respective legal rights and interests. Other legal duties also may be owed to the examinee or agency as a result of statutory or case law unique to an employer’s and/or examiner’s jurisdiction. (IACP, 2009, Guideline 8.2) 282

Fitness-for-Duty Evaluations

Notwithstanding the assertion by many respected FFD examiners that examinees in such evaluations should be advised that they have no “doctor–patient” relationship (Rostow & Davis, 2003; Stone, 2000), several courts and other legal authorities have reached a different conclusion. In Pettus v. Cole (1996), the California Court of Appeals found that an employer’s examining psychiatrist has a doctor–patient relationship with an employee-examinee even when the examination is performed for the benefit of the employer. Similarly, the Nevada Supreme Court held that individuals examined by a psychologist for the purpose of determining suitability for employment were “patients” within the meaning of a statute requiring health care providers to make a patient’s records available on request (Cleghorn v. Hess, 1993). In McGreal v. Ostrov (2004), the court held that McGreal, a police officer who was compelled to submit to an FFD evaluation, was a “recipient of mental health services” and enjoyed the rights of confidentiality associated with personal health information. Other courts and authorities also have held that persons compelled to submit to independent medical evaluations are “patients” under the law (cf. Arkansas Attorney General Opinion, 2001; Crandall v. Michaud, 1992; Elkins v. Syken, 1996; Simmons v. Rehab Xcel, Inc., 1999).

Principle 5: Identify the Legal Standard for Determining Fitness STANDARDS FOR STATE AND MUNICIPAL POLICE OFFICERS

Most states have statutes and/or administrative rules that impose some kind of mental requirements on police officers. The remaining 12 states are silent concerning any psychological or mental health criteria for police officer certification, leaving it to each individual agency to determine what standards to use, if any. More than half of the 38 states with these requirements use language identical, or nearly identical, to a California statute that states that any peace officer shall “[b]e found to be free from any physical, emotional, or mental condition that might adversely affect the exercise of the powers of a peace officer” (California Government Code §1031(f )). The case of Sager v. County of Yuba (2007) provides an instructive illustration of how these qualifying mandates for police officers can be applied in an FFD examination. In response to the argument of Sager, a deputy sheriff who was found psychologically unfit for duty, that the California Government Code §1031 standards apply only to police applicants rather than incumbent deputies, the court wrote: [T]he section 1031 standards must also be maintained throughout a peace officer’s career. . . . At least two of the standards reflect fundamental law enforcement qualifications: good moral character (§ 1031, subd. (d)) and mental fitness (§ 1031, subd. (f )). If Sager’s position is correct, an officer who lost his moral compass would be immune from these standards and only subject to a moral character standard if the applicable job description in that department reiterated that standard as a defined duty of that classification of officers. That absurd result highlights the flaw in Sager’s position. (at 14) Hence, the statutory and regulatory requirements in the employee’s jurisdiction may also provide the examiner with important guidance regarding the standard for psychological fitness. STANDARDS FOR FEDERAL POLICE OFFICERS

When evaluating police officers in the federal system who are subject to the requirements of the Office of Personnel Management (e.g., police officers in the Federal Protective Service, Immigration and Customs Enforcement, U.S. Marshals Service, Department of Veterans Affairs), the Rehabilitation Act of 1973 (29 U.S.C.A. §706), rather than the ADA, establishes the procedural and threshold 283

David M. Corey

standard. Although the ADA and the Rehabilitation Act share common statutory elements, the associated regulations and case law differ substantively from the ADA in three ways pertinent to FFD evaluations: (1) the threshold for determining when a federal employee may be referred for a mental health evaluation, (2) the standard for medical disqualification of a federal employee on the basis of a medical or mental health condition alone, and (3) the standard for determining a direct threat. As noted under Principle 1, a federal police agency under the authority of the OPM regulations may not refer an employee for psychiatric or psychological examination unless certain threshold conditions are met (OPM Medical Qualification Determinations, 1995; 5 C.F.R. §339.301). Once these requirements are satisfied, then an FFD examination conducted by a licensed psychiatrist or licensed psychologist “may only be used to make legitimate inquiry into a person’s mental fitness to successfully perform the duties of his/her position without undue hazard to the individual or others” (5 C.F.R. §339.301(e)(2)). Thus, like the EEOC regulations that implement and enforce the ADA, the OPM regulations contain two prongs to the determination of fitness, either or both of which may be at issue in an FFD evaluation (EEOC, 2000, Question 5, p. 7): inability to perform essential functions or posing a direct threat. When the first prong is at issue and behavioral evidence shows that the employee’s performance is unsafe or inefficient, the OPM regulations rely on 5 C.F.R. §339.301 (OPM Medical Qualification Determinations, 1995) to show simply that the employee’s medical condition is behind the employee’s inability to meet the performance standard. However, when relying on the employee’s medical condition alone, in the absence of inefficient performance, OPM relies on 5 C.F.R. §339.206, which stipulates three elements to the disqualification standard: (1) the medical condition is itself disqualifying with respect to the medical standards of the position, (2) recurrence of the condition cannot medically be ruled out, and (3) the duties of the position are such that a recurrence would pose a reasonable probability of substantial harm (Slater v. Dept. of Homeland Security, 2008). DIRECT THREAT

Under the terms of the ADA, a direct threat means a significant risk of substantial harm that cannot be eliminated or reduced by reasonable accommodation (29 C.F.R. §1630.2(r)). Determinations of direct threat must be based on an individualized assessment of the person’s present ability to safely perform the essential functions of the job. The ADA stipulates that the determination must be based on a reasonable medical judgment relying on the most current medical knowledge and/or best available objective evidence (29 C.F.R. §1630.2(r)). Furthermore, the following factors must be considered when making the determination: (1) the duration of the risk, (2) the nature and severity of the potential harm, (3) the likelihood that potential harm will occur, and (4) the imminence of the potential harm (29 C.F.R. §1603.2(r); Anderson v. Little League Baseball, Inc., 1992). The standard for an employer to show that it has met the burden of proving that an employee poses a significant risk of substantial harm is substantial evidence, defined as “such relevant evidence as a reasonable mind might accept as adequate to support a conclusion” (Universal Camera Corp. v. National Labor Relations Board, 340 U.S. 474 (1951), at 477 [citation omitted]; see also Knill v. Principi, 2001). However, in assessing whether an employee can perform his or her duties without a significant risk to the safety of the individual or others, the examiner “must consider the nature of the position and the consequences should the employee fail to perform his duties properly” (Lassiter v. Reno, 1996/1997, at 1153). In Lassiter, the circuit court decided that the employer was not required to show that a U.S. Deputy Marshal with paranoid personality disorder was reasonably likely to become violent, but rather that he posed a significant risk to the safety of himself or others if he did so. The court wrote: Given the duties of a deputy marshal, a significant risk to the safety of others can arise not only from an inclination to strike out in violence, but also from a tendency to misperceive 284

Fitness-for-Duty Evaluations

the true nature of events. . . . Placed in unfamiliar circumstances that may or may not be hostile, the deputy marshal must have the ability to decide in an instant whether the use of deadly force is warranted. If an innocent person is injured or killed because a deputy marshal “read . . . threatening meanings into benign remarks or events[,]” “it is not difficult to imagine the public outrage, let alone the potential liability” to which the federal government would be subjected. (at 40) Thus, although the EEOC defines direct threat to mean “a significant risk of substantial harm” (29 C.F.R. §1630.2(r)), multiple courts have held that where the employee’s position implicates the safety of others, and the potential harm is severe, even a low probability that the harm will occur will be sufficient to establish a direct threat (Butler v. Thornburgh, 1990; Hogarth v. Thornburgh, 1993; Myers v. Hose, 1995). Conversely, where the potential harm is not fatal or catastrophic, the examiner usually will be required to demonstrate that the risk is highly probable in order to establish that a direct threat exists (Mariani & Avelenda, 2009). It is incumbent on examiners to have a “fundamental and reasonable level of knowledge and understanding of the legal standards, laws, rules, and precedents” that apply to the FFD evaluation within the parties’ jurisdictions (American Psychology-Law Society, 2008, Guideline 4.04). This is especially critical with respect to the legal standard for determining fitness.

Principle 6: Determine the Examinee’s Rights and Limitations to Access to the Report and Other Personal Health Information As discussed under Principle 4, it is neither an ethical truism nor a matter of law in all jurisdictions that the examiner–examinee relationship is devoid of the obligations traditionally and statutorily associated with a doctor–patient relationship. This is no less true in the context of an examinee’s right to access personal health information gathered or created for purposes of an FFD evaluation (McGreal v. Ostrov, 2004; Pettus v. Cole, 1996). The Health Insurance Portability and Accountability Act (HIPAA) Privacy Rule (2000b) gives patients the right to inspect and amend records containing their personal health information (PHI), and psychologists who meet HIPAA’s definition of a health care provider are obligated to comply with the Privacy Rule’s requirements for disclosure of PHI (see 45 C.F.R. §160.524 and §160.526). HIPAA defines PHI as all “individually identifiable health information held or transmitted by a covered entity or its business associates, in any form or medium, whether electronic, paper or oral” (45 C.F.R. §160.103). As noted by Gold and Shuman (2009), this definition “does not distinguish information generated by employment-related mental health evaluations from records of treatment. Nor does the Privacy Rule explicitly make the purpose for which the information was created of any consequence” (p. 37). Some psychologists have argued with only partial accuracy that HIPAA’s Privacy Rule pertaining to an examinee’s right of access to PHI is exempted in an FFD examination under a provision that bars access when the PHI was “compiled in reasonable anticipation of, or for use in, a civil, criminal or administrative action or proceeding” (45 C.F.R. §164.524(a)(ii)). But under this exception, a psychologist may only “deny access to any information that relates specifically to legal preparations but may not deny access to the individual’s underlying health information” (U.S. Department of Health & Human Services, 2000, p. 82554). Consequently, examiners covered under HIPAA should include in their initial disclosures to employers and examinees alike the provisions of the Privacy Rule and their own practices relevant to an examinee’s access to reports and underlying PHI (Gold & Shuman, 2009). Although the Privacy Rule permits denial of an individual’s access to PHI if it is judged by the examiner to be “reasonably likely to endanger the life or physical safety of the 285

David M. Corey

individual or another person” (45 C.F.R. §164.524(a)(3)(i)), it is important to keep in mind—and to disclose—that this decision is reviewable. The HIPAA Privacy Rule establishes a national privacy floor rather than a ceiling, so an examinee may be afforded even more definitive access to PHI by state statutes and administrative rules, related case law, or the terms of a collective-bargaining agreement. Examiners should determine in advance of the examination what jurisdictional or agency rules may pertain to the examinee’s access to PHI, including the final written report. In any event, examinees may be denied access to PHI that “was obtained from someone other than a health care provider under a promise of confidentiality and the access requested would be reasonably likely to reveal the source of the information,” and the grounds for denial on this basis are unreviewable (45 C.F.R. §164.524(a)(2)(v)). A report containing such information should be redacted before giving the examinee access to it.

Principle 7: Provide Appropriate Disclosure to the Referring Party Concerning Fees, Evaluator Role, and Procedures Any medical examination compelled by the employer must be paid entirely by the employer (EEOC, 2000, Question 11, pp. 11–12). Still, the examiner’s fees, including any differences in rate by type of service (e.g., evaluation versus testimony in court or other adjudicative forum), should be disclosed to the referring party in advance of the service. In addition, the examiner should clarify the nature of the services to be provided, the estimated hours and time period, the provisions if the anticipated services cannot be performed within this period, who will be responsible for payment, any special financial considerations (e.g., fees in the event of cancellations, terms of payment, and interest on delinquent balances), and anticipated work products (e.g., verbal consultation, report, and testimony) (Heilbrun, 2001). A useful and simple form of disclosure that meets some, although not all, of these disclosure obligations may be accomplished by providing the referring party with a copy of the IACP FFDE Guidelines (2009), along with a statement that these guidelines are expected to apply to both the referring party and the examiner. Anfang and Wall (2006) advise that it is best to clarify all terms of an FFD evaluation in a letter of engagement, with the letter signed and returned to the examiner before the examination begins. They recommend inclusion of all fees and payment arrangements; cancellation, deposition, and incourt testimony policies; the level of detail that the report will contain; and the precise nature of collateral records and other documents sought in connection with the referral (e.g., job descriptions, performance evaluations, disciplinary records, awards and commendations, complaints and suits, and documentation of previous episodes of impairment and disability). The IACP FFDE Guidelines (2009) also stipulate that the informed consent of the employer, in addition to the examinee, should be obtained (Guideline 8.1).

Examination and Procedural Issues This section describes the principles that pertain to the examination itself and apply only when the psychologist has accepted the evaluation referral and when the referring party and the psychologist have agreed to the terms. It presumes that the examiner has: 1. 2. 3. 4. 5.

Assessed how the employer has met the legal threshold for mandating a fitness examination Identified the relevant clinical and forensic questions Determined that the referral is within his or her area of expertise Determined that he or she is able to conduct the examination with impartiality Identified the legal standard for determining fitness

286

Fitness-for-Duty Evaluations

6. Determined the examinee’s rights and limitations regarding access to the report and other personal health information gathered in the course of the evaluation 7. Provided appropriate disclosure to the referring party concerning fees, the evaluator’s role, and procedures

Principle 8: Provide the Examinee With Appropriate Disclosure and Obtain Informed Consent/Authorization It is a cornerstone of professional ethics in psychology that examiners are to be honest about the nature, purpose, intended uses, and possible outcomes of the evaluation, and this is especially true in FFD evaluations, where the consequence of an employee’s failure to cooperate may be loss of employment (Anfang & Wall, 2006). Some clinicians and attorneys assert that true informed consent in FFD evaluations cannot occur because one of the necessary elements of consent—namely, ­voluntariness— is absent. This view, however, fails to recognize informed consent as a broad ethical obligation “that costs nothing and treats the examinee with respect” (Gold & Shuman, 2009, p. 28). Indeed, the fundamental legal principle underlying the necessity for consent “is now beyond debate” (p. 27). APA ethical standards (APA, 2002) stipulate that even when informed consent may not be legally required, psychologists nevertheless (1) provide an appropriate explanation, (2) seek the individual’s assent, and (3) consider such person’s preferences and best interests (Standard 3.10(b)). Thus, whether conceptualized as consent, informed consent, assent, or disclosure, psychologists should always provide the examinee with clarification concerning important elements of the examination. At a minimum, these include: 1. A description of the nature and scope of the evaluation 2. The limits of confidentiality, including any information that may be disclosed to the employer without the examinee’s authorization 3. The party or parties who will receive the FFDE report of findings, and whether the examinee will receive a report 4. The potential outcomes and probable uses of the examination, including treatment recommendations, if applicable 5. Other provisions consistent with legal and ethical standards for mental health evaluations conducted at the request of third parties (IACP, 2009, Guideline 8.1) Although the importance of obtaining consent or, in the alternative, providing disclosure, is an established ethical and legal principle (Schloendorff v. Society of New York Hospital, 1914), debate surrounds the appropriate actions of the examiner in the event that an employee refuses to consent to the evaluation. Rostow and Davis (2004) contend that the examiner could proceed under some circumstances, whereas Gold and Shuman (2009) assert that the examination should not take place in the absence of written confirmation of consent, noting that “[f]ailing to obtain a consent later determined to be required cannot be remedied” (p. 28). Anfang and Wall (2006) suggest that the consent form be given to the examinee in advance of the examination in order to facilitate dialogue and consultation with other parties who may be involved with the examinee, including attorneys, union representatives, and treating clinicians. Under no circumstances, however, should an employee be required to waive all procedural rights or liability as a condition of the FFD examination ( Jackson v. Wilson, 1979). Even when informed consent is obtained in writing prior to the examination, it should be kept in mind that informed consent is a process, not simply an event. In the course of the examination, the

287

David M. Corey

examiner may need to revisit important aspects of the informed consent document in order to clarify, for example, the limits of confidentiality, the purpose of the examination, or the potential outcomes. Some clinicians request that examinees summarize key elements of the informed consent or disclosure document in their own words both to ensure that consent is given knowingly and intelligently, even if not voluntarily, and to enable documentation of that fact in the event of subsequent litigation. Special attention to an exceptional form of disclosure may be needed in situations where a police officer examinee is asked to discuss or reveal information that could violate the officer’s constitutional right to be free from compulsory self-incrimination (Aitchison, 2000). In Garrity v. New Jersey (1967), the U.S. Supreme Court ruled that the use of a police officer’s statements in criminal proceedings violated the Fifth Amendment guarantee that citizens cannot be forced to be witnesses against themselves. The Court held that “the choice imposed on [the officers] was one between selfincrimination or job forfeiture” (at 497) and ruled that statements which a law enforcement officer is compelled to make under threat of possible forfeiture of his or her job could not subsequently be used against the officer in a criminal prosecution. Under Garrity and its progeny (e.g., Gardner v. Broderick, 1968), before a police employer questioning a police officer can discipline the officer for refusing to answer questions, the employer must (1) order the officer to answer the questions under threat of disciplinary action; (2) ask questions that are specifically, narrowly, and directly related to the officer’s duties or the officer’s fitness for duty; and (3) advise the officer that the answers to the questions will not be used against the officer in criminal proceedings (Lefkowitz v. Turley, 1973). Because an FFD examination is a compulsory examination in which the police officer examinee is usually ordered to participate and cooperate fully under threat of discipline or termination for failure to do so, and because information obtained in the examination about the officer’s self-incriminating statements could be included in an evaluation report, examiners should be careful to ensure that an employer has issued the required Garrity notice before inquiring into matters likely to reveal self-incriminating information.

Principle 9: Decide Whether to Permit Third-Party Observers and/or Recording Devices Into the Interview Otto and Krauss (2009) give a detailed discussion of the ethical, clinical, and legal challenges involved in contemplating the presence of third-party observers in an assessment. In particular, they note the potential impact of the third party’s presence on the examinee’s participation and—in cases in which psychological testing is administered—test standardization, norms, and security. For purposes of their review, they define a third-party observer as “an individual whose sole purpose is to observe (and perhaps document)—but not affect—the psychological evaluation” (pp. 2–3). Otto and Krauss classified concerns about the presence of third-party observers into four categories: (1) negative effects on the examinee’s responses and participation, (2) interruption of the flow of information from the examinee to the examiner, (3) threats to the validity of conclusions that can be drawn from the evaluation, and (4) threats to the security (and future utility) of psychological assessment techniques and tests. With respect to the first of these, they found little empirical evidence to support this concern outside the limited impact of observers on some memory and learning tests. Furthermore, they point out that many other factors common to the psychological evaluation (e.g., examiner and examinee demographic variables, such as race, socioeconomic status, sex; the examiner’s style and expectations of the examinee; the examinee’s anxiety level; the nature and purpose of the evaluation) “can have greater or similar effects on the psychological evaluation process” (p. 6) but are nonetheless well tolerated by examining clinicians. This includes invited thirdparty observers such as students and interns. Concerns over interruption of the flow of information from the examinee to the examiner likewise carry little weight, given the ready availability of the less intrusive alternatives: video- or 288

Fitness-for-Duty Evaluations

audio-recording devices. They also note that ground rules could easily be established that prohibit third-party observers from interrupting the flow of information from the examinee to the examiner. Concerns about the threats to the validity of information that can be drawn from evaluations in which third-party observers were present were also unsupported. Otto and Krauss (2009) point out that a wide array of factors affect an examinee’s performance in an assessment, and in forensic evaluation contexts, threats to validity stemming from the presence of a third party during the assessment are likely to be overshadowed by these other factors—the most important of which being that almost all psychological and neuropsychological instruments have been normed on individuals involved in legal proceedings. (p. 7) They conclude that the presence of third-party observers, “insofar as it constitutes a deviation from standard test administration—is not nearly as well documented, and is likely a lesser threat to the validity of conclusions drawn from psychological test data, than the effects of the litigation” (p. 7). Finally, Otto and Krauss (2009) conclude that the basis for concerns over the threats to the security and future utility of psychological assessment techniques and tests are easily overcome by “requesting that the observer be someone who is bound to protect test security or request that test administration be recorded and only made available to persons obligated to protect test security (i.e., a psychologist)” (p. 8). Although courts have been divided on the question of third-party observers in psychological and psychiatric forensic examinations, the greater weight of opinion appears to fall on the side of not permitting their attendance when, notwithstanding the Otto and Krauss (2009) analysis, the examiner objects to it. In Vinson v. The Superior Court of Alameda County (1987), the court denied the employee’s request to have her attorney present during the FFD examination, noting: We were skeptical that a lawyer, unschooled in the ways of the mental health profession, would be able to discern the psychiatric relevance of the questions. And the examiner should have the freedom to probe deeply into the plaintiff ’s psyche without interference by a third party. . . . Whatever comfort her attorney’s hand-holding might afford was substantially outweighed by the distraction and potential disruption caused by the presence of a third person. (p. 412) In Tomlin v. Holecek et al. (1993), the court concluded that the presence of third parties would lend a degree of artificiality to the interview that would be inconsistent with applicable professional standards and that allowing a tape-recording would be “an undesirable infusion of the adversary process into the examining room” (at 628). In Galieti v. State Farm Mutual Automobile Insurance Co. et al. (1994), the court held that the employee bore the burden of proving the need to record the assessment or have an observer present. In Ragge v. MCA/Universal Studios et al. (1995), the court refused the employee’s request for a third-party observer as meritless because of the observer’s potential to interfere with, or even contaminate, the examination. These court decisions are consistent with the positions of a number of authors on the topic who advise that the presence of third parties unnecessary to the conduct of the evaluation should always be avoided (Gold & Shuman, 2009). These considerations aside, there is at least one circumstance in which an examiner may be obligated either to permit a third party to observe the assessment or to decline the referral, and that is 289

David M. Corey

when the employee is represented by a labor union and is afforded Weingarten rights (NLRB v. Weingarten, Inc., 1975). Under the Weingarten ruling, an employee in a collective-bargaining group who reasonably believes that an interview or examination may result in disciplinary action against the employee has a right to the presence of a union representative, if requested, although the representative may not interfere with the proceedings. Examiners who object to the presence of third-party observers should respectfully postpone the examination until the referring party has been notified and is able to make a determination to allow or oppose the request. In a case involving a federal corrections officer referred for an FFD evaluation, the examiner’s unilateral decision to prevent the employee’s union representative from observing the examination resulted in an adverse finding against the employer when the arbitrator determined that the employee’s Weingarten rights were violated (AFGE Local 596 v. Department of Justice et al., 2007).

Principle 10: Select Multiple Sources of Clinical and Behavioral Information, Using Relevance and Reliability as Guides Reliance on multiple sources of information and corroborating important data whenever feasible is a standard practice in forensic evaluations (American Psychology-Law Society, 2008, Guideline 11.02; Heilbrun et al., 2009). Heilbrun (2001) asserts that the use of multiple sources of information in a forensic mental health assessment is an established principle because it (1) enhances accuracy and (2) allows the examiner to check hypotheses generated by one or more of the data sources or measures. The role of third-party information, primarily consisting of documents and interviews with collateral informants, is especially important given the elevated potential in FFD evaluations for either party to provide an incomplete picture of the relevant facts. But an FFD evaluation is not a fishing expedition wherein the examiner is free to conduct a comprehensive assessment of the employee and scrutinize, measure, and evaluate every aspect of the employee’s functioning. Rather, under the terms of the ADA, “[t]he inquiries or examinations must not exceed the scope of the specific medical condition and its effect on the employee’s ability, with or without reasonable accommodation, to perform essential job functions or to work without posing a direct threat” (EEOC, 1997, Question 14, p. 10). When it is not yet confirmed whether an employee actually has a medical or mental health condition—as when the referral results from a sudden, adverse change in behavior or work performance that leads to the reasonable suspicion of a mental health condition—the scope of the examination should be narrowed to those conditions reasonably linked to the problem behavior or other objective evidence giving rise to the referral. On the other hand, when an employee with a previously diagnosed condition is referred for evaluation to determine his or her readiness to return to work, the scope of the evaluation must be limited to that condition. In any case, an examination for either inability to perform essential functions of the job or possible direct threat must be narrowly tailored to seek only that information necessary to address those referral questions (Gold & Shuman, 2009). The IACP FFDE Guidelines (IACP, 2009) provide a useful starting point for selecting sources of information. These guidelines recommend that they include, but not be limited to: 1. Performance evaluations, previous remediation efforts, commendations, testimonials, internal affairs investigations, formal citizen/public complaints, use-of-force incidents, reports related to officer-involved shootings, civil claims, disciplinary actions, incident reports of any triggering events, medical records, prior psychological evaluations, and other supporting or relevant documentation related to the employee’s psychological fitness for duty (Guideline 7.3) 2. In some cases, medical/psychological treatment records and other data (Guideline 7.3) 3. Psychological testing using assessment instruments (e.g., personality, psychopathology, cognitive, specialized) appropriate to the referral question(s) (Guideline 9.1.2) 290

Fitness-for-Duty Evaluations

4. A comprehensive, face-to-face clinical interview (Guideline 9.1.3) 5. Collateral interviews with relevant third parties if deemed necessary by the examiner (Guideline 9.1.4) 6. Examination by a specialist if deemed necessary by the examiner (Guideline 9.1.5) Heilbrun (2001) cautioned that the information sought and relied on in any forensic assessment should be guided by relevance to the forensic issues and the validity of the different sources. . . . If a given source has little or no accuracy, then it cannot increase the overall accuracy of the evaluation of forensic issues, and will decrease it if given much weight. (pp. 107–108) When evaluating whether third-party information is reliable enough to justify requiring a medical evaluation, the EEOC lists the following factors: (1) the relationship of the person providing the information to the employee about whom it is being provided, (2) the possible motivation of the person providing the information, (3) how the person learned the information (e.g., directly from the employee whose medical condition is in question or from someone else), and (4) other evidence that the employer has that bears on the reliability of the information provided (EEOC, 2000, Question 12, p. 12). These factors may also aid the examining psychologist in evaluating the reliability of third-party information. The use of multiple sources of information can bolster confidence in findings when the data reveal consistency across sources, because some individuals who might be interviewed in connection with an FFD evaluation may be biased against or in favor of the employee (Heilbrun, Warren, & Picarello, 2003). Certainly the most important, but by no means the only, source of information about the employee’s fitness for duty is the clinical interview. In addition to standard examination elements, including a mental status examination, FFD interviews should explore all standard dimensions of history: personal, familial, mental health, developmental, educational, legal, military, marital, occupational, and social. The employee’s history of substance abuse, stress management, interpersonal conflict management, and occupational adaptation should be explored as well. In particular, it is essential that the examinee be provided with a full opportunity to tell his or her side of the story as it pertains to the issues underlying the referral, including alternative explanations and perspectives. Job descriptions and job analyses also are valuable sources of information. Borum et al. (2003) point out: Even if the psychologist is generally familiar with the job or knows specific abilities identified from other agencies, it is often helpful to obtain a job description from the specific requesting agency to ensure that one is providing the most precise assessment of fit between the examinee’s condition and the agency’s requirements. (p. 142) In the previous discussions under Principles 1 and 2, the importance of gathering the employer’s objective evidence of functional impairment, performance deficits, or direct threat to the employee or others was emphasized, along with the necessity for obtaining the specific referral questions. This information, in combination with the job description or job analysis, is critical for determining the breadth and depth of other information that may be required, including collateral interviews of supervisors, coworkers, family members, or treating clinicians. As Gold and Shuman (2009) observed: The quality and quantity of the information upon which an FFD examination should be based is a function of the risk to which third persons may be exposed, the opportunity 291

David M. Corey

of those exposed to the risk to affect it by their own actions, and the examinee’s interests. For example, when serious bodily harm is a potential risk, collateral data to verify possibly partisan information is a necessary component of competent decision-making. (p. 252) The selection of data sources will also be driven in part by the forensic decision-making model used by the examiner (Heilbrun, 2001; see also Principle 12) as well as the legal standard for determining fitness (see Principle 5). In FFD evaluations of police officers, public safety and direct threat are always implicated in the judgment of fitness and must always be a consideration when deciding on the breadth, depth, and type of information and records to gather. This point is underscored in Colon v. City of Newark (2006), in which a police officer, Bazyt Bergus, assaulted Carlos Colon while Colon was detained in a Newark detention facility and Bergus was assigned to detention duties. Bergus’s assignment followed a series of investigations, disciplinary actions, and subsequent appeals related to accusations of domestic assault and other violent conduct. Bergus eventually was referred for an FFD evaluation to be done by an independent psychologist. Neither the employer nor Bergus disclosed to the examiner the details of these accusations and investigations, nor was the psychologist given any information regarding Bergus’s two previous pre-employment psychological evaluations. The first of these concluded that he exhibited marked animus “towards blacks as well as a tendency towards impulsivity and a history of aggressiveness” and that “if appointed as a police officer he could constitute a danger to the community” (p. 5). The second, conducted four years later with the same psychologist, concluded that Bergus was qualified, although with the familiar caveat that he “apparently tried to present a favorable picture by denying many normal, though not socially desirable, characteristics” (p. 6). Colon eventually sued Bergus and the City of Newark, alleging deprivation of his civil rights, partly on the basis of the official departmental policy of withholding from evaluating psychologists complete psychological and disciplinary histories of officers referred for evaluation. The jury found both Bergus and the City of Newark liable. On appeal, the court affirmed, noting: [W]e are satisfied, as was the trial judge, that a reasonable jury could have found . . . that the defendant demonstrated deliberate indifference to the civil rights of the public, including Carlos Colon, in its official policies governing the supervision and discipline of Bergus as well as the transmission of information for “fitness for duty” evaluations, and that these policies were the motivating force behind Colon’s assault. (pp. 12–13) Further, Bergus was cleared as fit for duty after a psychological evaluation that, in accordance with usual departmental practice, was rendered without benefit of a full and complete historical record of disciplinary charges and psychological profiles. (p. 18) Care should be taken to ensure that the examining psychologist requests, and documents the request for, all records and collateral information relevant to a “full and complete historical record” of the employee’s behavior on and off duty. In addition to requesting records from the employer and conducting collateral interviews of coworkers and supervisors, as may be indicated, Borum et al. (2003) advise, “To ensure a fair and balanced process, it may also be probative for the expert to ask the examinee if there are specific individuals he or she thinks should be interviewed or documents that should be reviewed as part of the evaluation” (p. 143).

292

Fitness-for-Duty Evaluations

As in the Colon matter, referrals for FFD examinations most commonly result from on- or offduty conduct that raises reasonable concerns about an employee’s psychological fitness, prompting careful review of the objective facts preceding and surrounding that conduct. Other referrals occur after an officer has requested leave for treatment or recovery from a psychological injury or condition (e.g., Posttraumatic Stress Disorder following an on-duty traumatic incident), often with the agreement of his or her personal health care provider, and subsequently seeks a return to duty. In these “return-to-work” evaluations, the examiner should gather information related to (1) the circumstances that precipitated the medical leave, (2) psychotherapy or treatment records, and (3) information about “what has happened since the declaration of unfitness and what changes have occurred in the symptoms or impairments that initially caused concern” (Borum et al., 2003, p. 144). These authors go on to note: Reliance on third-party information is critical to gauge any changes in thinking, mood, or behavior that may be observable by others and to assess the extent to which they are consistent with the officer’s self-report. If the officer has been referred for treatment, the evaluator ordinarily should contact the treatment provider to request records (with written consent of the officer) and to gather, preferably through discussion, relevant information about specific symptoms or behaviors of concern. The treating professional may also have relevant data and opinions about the officer’s prognosis. When consulting a treating professional, however, the FFDE examiner must always consider that the provider has a primary alliance with the officer, and that the applicability of any information must be considered in light of the known distinctions between therapeutic and forensic roles. (p. 144) The ADA permits an employer to obtain only that medical information necessary to determine whether the employee can do the essential functions of the job or work without posing a direct threat (EEOC, 2000, Question 13, p. 12). “This means that, in most situations, an employer cannot request an employee’s complete medical records because they are likely to contain information unrelated to whether the employee can perform his/her essential functions or work without posing a direct threat” (p. 12). However, when an employer has a reasonable belief that an employee’s present ability to perform essential job functions will be impaired by a medical condition or that he or she will pose a direct threat due to a medical condition, the employer may ask the employee for additional documentation regarding his or her medications or treatment (EEOC, 2000, Question 17, p. 13). In order for an inquiry into the private life of an employee to be upheld by the courts, it must be made as narrowly as possible; broad, sweeping requests for information are not likely to be valid (Aitchison, 2000). But where an order to produce a request for particular private information— including medical or treatment records—has a direct relationship to job performance, it is likely to be upheld as valid (Schuman v. City of Philadelphia, 1979). The authority of the FFD examiner to require a review of treatment records as an element of the examination was affirmed in the case of Thomas v. Corwin (2007). Jana Thomas, a police officer in the Juvenile Unit of the Kansas City Police Department (KCPD), was ordered to submit to an FFD evaluation after she was placed on medical leave by her treating psychologist for work-related stress and anxiety and then was later released for duty. The examining psychologist concluded that Thomas did not appear to have any major psychological disorder, but he could not find her fit for duty in the absence of evidence of effective medical intervention for her anxiety or a change in working conditions to resolve her complaints within the Juvenile Unit. He reported that he could not issue a final report without access to Thomas’s medical records. He opined that Thomas’s reluctance to disclose

293

David M. Corey

her medical records might indicate other reasons for her alleged work-related stress and anxiety, given that her alleged stress reaction appeared to be “disproportional to the problems” (at 524) in the Juvenile Unit. Thomas refused to authorize release of the records, and KCPD fired her. She sued, alleging ADA violations, discrimination, and invasion of privacy. The district court issued summary judgment in favor of KCPD, and the appeals court affirmed, writing, “We agree with the district court that examining Thomas was vital to operating the Juvenile Unit, and the focused request for a limited portion of Thomas’s medical records was no broader or more intrusive than necessary” (at 529). The court went on to conclude: By refusing to provide [the examining psychologist] the opportunity to review her medical records and to discover the root of Thomas’s stress and anxiety, Thomas created a stalemate in which KCPD had little choice but to terminate Thomas rather than return her to the position from which Thomas’s stress and anxiety originated. Thomas’s refusal to cooperate with the reasonable requirements of her FFD evaluation and her violation of KCPD’s rules of conduct provided the defendants with legitimate, nondiscriminatory reasons to terminate Thomas. (at 531) Failure of an examining psychologist to obtain psychiatric treatment records in an FFD evaluation also can lead to error and impede the employer’s ability to make decisions involving officer and public safety. In Thompson v. City of Arlington (1993), the employer sent police officer Ann Thompson for a psychological FFD evaluation once she was released by her treating mental health care providers to return to work following a suicide attempt. The examining psychologist did not review the psychiatric records but determined Thompson fit for duty. In turn, the employer demanded a review of Thompson’s psychiatric treatment records, and she refused, leading the employer to place her on indefinite restricted duty. She sued in federal court, alleging, among other things, a violation of her right to privacy. The court dismissed her suit, citing the inevitable limitations of a treating health care provider’s return-to-work opinion and the need for an objective one: An important, if not the primary, obligation of plaintiff ’s doctors is to serve her needs. If those doctors were to conclude that plaintiff ’s return to regular police officer duty would have a beneficial effect on her mental health, their natural leaning would be to take steps to cause her to return to regular duty. They would be extremely reluctant to report to City that she remains unfit to return to regular duty if, from a medical standpoint, such a report could be a factor in delaying or preventing her recovery. (at 1147) The court went on to distinguish the patient-centered advocacy of the treating health care provider from the independent judgment of the FFD examiner, while noting the critical importance of having access to the records underlying the treating provider’s opinions: And, as to City’s own health care expert, a self-evident fact is that such an expert is highly unlikely to obtain full and candid information if the expert is required to rely on interviews with plaintiff concerning factors that enter into her mental makeup. Common sense says that, if plaintiff has determined that she wants to return to regular police officer duty, there is a serious risk that she will limit the information she provides in an evaluation by City’s expert to facts and circumstances she believes would support her claim that she is fit for such a return. If City’s expert is to be able to provide a meaningful opinion to City, the expert would be required to have full information, certainly at least as much information as plaintiff ’s health care providers acquire. The most effective, and only reasonable, method of 294

Fitness-for-Duty Evaluations

obtaining that degree of information would be to cause all the information in possession of plaintiff ’s regular doctors to be provided to City for evaluation by its own expert. (at 1147)

Principle 11: Assess Response Style One of the core features of a forensic evaluation that distinguishes it from therapeutic assessments is the absence of any presumption that the examinee’s self-report is accurate (Greenberg & Shuman, 1997; Heilbrun, 2001). In an FFD examination, the potential for the examinee to under- or overreport symptoms; deny, minimize, or exaggerate facts; conceal certain information; or otherwise misrepresent the truth must always be considered, due in large part to the incentives associated with the outcome. The candidate’s orientation toward accuracy or inaccuracy in his or her self-report is referred to as response style. Response style is conceived as including four particular styles: (1) reliable/honest (factual inaccuracies attributable to misunderstanding or misperception), (2) malingering (factual inaccuracies derived from conscious fabrication or intentional exaggeration of symptoms), (3) defensive (factual inaccuracies resulting from intentional denial or minimization), and (4) irrelevant/uncooperative (resulting from a failure to become engaged in the evaluation or from a refusal to respond fully or at all) (Heilbrun, 2001; Rogers, 1997). There are two primary means for assessing response style: (1) third-party information and (2) psychological assessment instruments, or tests, with built-in measures of response style. In the previous discussion of Principle 10, emphasis was placed on the importance of gathering third-party information. Its utility is tied not only to the improved depth and breadth of information obtained through collateral sources, but also to its potential for detecting malingering, defensiveness, dishonesty, and uncooperativeness. Melton et al. (1997) observed that “obtaining information contradicting the client’s version of events is probably the most accurate means of detecting fabrication and may be the only viable one with clients who sabotage interview and testing efforts” (pp. 57–58). This is not to say that any given collateral source should be considered more reliable than the examinee, but rather that consistencies and discrepancies across and within data sources are an important means of weighing validity. Standardized assessment instruments with validity scales or indices, such as the MMPI-2, MMPI2RF, PAI, CPI, MCMI-III, and 16PF, also can serve useful roles in evaluating response style, especially when used in conjunction with third-party information. Consideration should always be given, however, to the base rates of these scales in a particular norm group, such that examinees in an FFD evaluation are not judged to be defensive or dissimulating on the basis of validity scales alone when the scale norms were derived from respondents from a decidedly different context (i.e., police applicants in a pre-employment evaluation may not be comparable on validity scales to incumbent employees in an FFD examination) (Heilbrun, 2001). Evidence of frank lying, dissimulation, falsification, or overt concealment should always be reported to the referring party along with any reservations or limitations in the reliability of the examiner’s opinions as a result of the employee’s response style. In general, however, when the examinee’s response style, based on evidence from third-party sources and psychological testing, suggests that his or her responses are unreliable, they should be regarded as less probative than information obtained from other sources with higher reliability.

Determining Fitness Principle 12: Use a Model for Determining Fitness for Duty Heilbrun (2001; see also Heilbrun et al., 2009) argues for the use of a model to help guide the forensic clinician in data gathering, data interpretation, and communication of results. He cites two general 295

David M. Corey

models to consider: Morse (1978) and Grisso (1986, 2003). Both have compelling features and applicability to FFD evaluations, although neither was developed for this application. Morse’s model offers a straightforward application to a wide range of forensic mental health assessments, including the FFD evaluation, with a focus on three broad considerations: (1) the existence of a mental disorder or condition, (2) the functional abilities relevant to the referral questions, and (3) the strength of the causal connection between the first and the second considerations. Gold and Shuman (2009) propose an alternative model (Battista, 1988) for workplace evaluations involving questions of impairment. Like Morse’s model, Battista’s involves the analysis of three elements: (1) work demand, which consists primarily of the relevant work skills and other requirements of a particular job, and can be derived most expediently from the job description and/ or job analysis; (2) work supply, consisting of the employee’s performance and employment history, including the ability to perform work functions with or without a social or interpersonal component, but also including the ability to perform simple and repetitive tasks, perform complex or varied tasks, work under stressful versus routine conditions, and work with and without supervision; and (3) work capacity, meaning the balance or interaction between work demand and work supply, such that adequate work capacity results in the employee having enough work supply (ability) to satisfy work demand. Because reduced work capacity can result from either work supply falling below work demand, or work demand increasing to surpass work supply, Battista’s model provides a useful framework for analyzing and locating the cause(s) for any observed decrease in work capacity. This model also incorporates the procedural advice of Borum et al. (2003) when conducting FFD evaluations for police officers and other high-risk positions: [T]he psychologist must evaluate the degree of fit between the employee’s current capacities or impairments and the essential requirements of the position. The assessment can be done by (a) determining if there are psychological or behavioral problems, and if so, evaluating their potential impact on the employee’s ability to perform the functions of the job; and (b) determining if there are any significant impairments in the employee’s ability to perform essential job functions, and if so, evaluating their cause. (p. 143) In my own approach to determining fitness, I adopt a blended model that incorporates elements of Morse’s and Battista’s models, but which is framed within the context of Grisso’s (2003). It consists of four analytic components: 1. Functional Analysis: Concerned principally with work supply, this analysis asks the question, What is it that the officer is able to do, and not able to do, effectively? It considers the officer’s work and behavioral history, not merely recent or current functioning. It is concerned with both retained and impaired functioning in an effort to obtain as accurate a picture as possible of the officer’s past, current, and reasonably anticipated job-relevant behavior. 2. Contextual Analysis: Not all police work is identical; it varies depending on the nature of the assignment (e.g., patrol versus desk duty, intermittent undercover versus deep and sustained undercover, homicide versus property crime investigations); the volume of calls for service and population-to-officer ratio (i.e., urban, suburban, rural); and other factors affecting the degree of isolation, stress, risk, and other demands associated with the specific position. The contextual analysis considers these and other relevant work demand characteristics, including consideration of what Brodsky (1996) calls “tolerance limits” within which the employee must operate and that are affected by elements outside the employer’s control (e.g., civil service rules, union contracts, and organizational culture). At this stage of analysis, familiarity with the demands and milieu of the position is crucial, and an adequate objective analysis may require a site 296

Fitness-for-Duty Evaluations

visit, interviews with supervisors and/or incumbents, and review of job analyses and position descriptions. 3. Causal Analysis: This component addresses evidence of the existence of a mental or emotional condition that may account for any observed functional deficits. In practical terms, if no mental or emotional condition exists, the analysis of fitness ends here because the minimum requisite condition for “unfitness” is not met. As noted in the discussion of Principle 5, this does not mean the officer should be retained, but only that the officer’s behavioral problems are not attributable to a mental or emotional condition and, therefore, do not implicate fitness. This information permits both the employer and employee to explore alternative means for bringing equipoise to the disparity between work supply and work demand. On the other hand, when a mental condition is found to exist, its nexus to any functional deficits must still be determined. In conducting this analysis, it is important to differentiate between work capacity deficits that (1) derive from the examinee’s mental or emotional condition, and (2) are caused by factors independent of that condition (e.g., attitude, motivation, skill or knowledge deficits, interpersonal conflicts, and general medical conditions). 4. Interactive Analysis: This element is, as Grisso (2003) points out, an assessment of person–context fit. It is concerned with the degree of congruency or balance between work supply and work demand—what Battista (1988) calls work capacity. Naturally, it requires not only an understanding of the examinee’s functional abilities and deficits (derived from the functional analysis), but also an awareness of the work demands, or the particular job and working conditions of the officer. 5. Judgmental Analysis: This component is concerned with the degree of person–context (i.e., work supply–work demand) incongruency required before it can be determined that the employee cannot safely or effectively perform the job. It addresses what Grisso regards as the “ultimate question” (2003, p. 36), which, in the context of an FFD evaluation, is simply, How much incongruency is enough to warrant a finding of unfitness? (Note: Whether this ultimate question is most properly answered by the employer or by the examiner is an issue that warrants serious debate (Heilbrun et al., 2009), although the constraints of this chapter prevent it from further exploration. Examiners who object to opining on the ultimate question should make that position known at the time of the referral (see Principle 1). For these examiners, this model would conclude after the interactive component.) Regardless of what model is used, its usefulness is measured by its ability to bring an organizational structure that constructively guides the selection and organization of data sources and information, promotes effective analysis, and facilitates the communication of opinions and testimony. The use of a poor model, on the other hand, would likely result in a worse outcome than no model at all (Heilbrun, 2001).

Communicating the Results The written product of the FFD evaluation is not merely an opinion or judgment about fitness. It is also an explication of that opinion within the confines of the referring party’s request and/or governing conditions. Melton et al. (2007) discuss several important ways that forensically oriented reports differ from those written for traditional clinical settings. First, the recipients usually will not be other mental health professionals, but employers, human resource professionals, attorneys, labor unions, and other laypersons unfamiliar with clinical language and meaning. For this reason, Heilbrun (2001) and others recommend that examiners avoid the use of clinical jargon when writing these reports. A second difference from traditional clinical reports is the likelihood that they will become more broadly distributed to other unanticipated persons and parties, whether through the employer’s decisions, administrative procedures, or subsequent litigation. Consequently, “special care must be taken 297

David M. Corey

to minimize any infringement on the privacy rights of persons mentioned in the report” (Melton et al., 2007, p. 583). A third difference is the degree of scrutiny that the report and the author are likely to receive in the course of any adjudicative proceedings or negotiations. Examiners should expect to be asked to provide testimony concerning their opinions and the bases for them, and this testimony may take place in the context of adversarial proceedings, under oath, where a well-written report will facilitate testimony and a poorly written one “may become, in the hands of a skillful lawyer, an instrument to discredit and embarrass its author” (Melton et al., 2007, p. 583). To these important considerations must be added an essential final difference, and that is the outcomes or consequences at stake in an FFD evaluation of a police officer. In addition to the impact that the findings may have on the decision to retain or terminate the officer, the nature and content of the report could affect the employee’s reputation, standing, and career even if retained in his or her position. Furthermore, an unclear and ultimately unhelpful report concerning an officer who represents a direct threat to the safety of others could result in the employer or other trier of fact ordering that the employee be returned to duty notwithstanding the findings of the examiner, thereby potentially jeopardizing safety. Thus, reports prepared in connection with FFD evaluations should reflect the seriousness of purpose, the significance of the potential consequences, the scrutiny they are almost certain to receive or should receive, and the breadth and nature of the audience, both intended and reasonably anticipated, that characterize these examinations.

Principle 13: Guard the Legal and Ethical Limitations on Report Content It is a standard of ethical practice that “[p]sychologists include in written and oral reports and consultations only information germane to the purpose for which the communication is made” (APA, 2002, Standard 4.04(a)). This ethical standard is echoed by the federal HIPAA Privacy Rule (U.S. Department of Health & Human Services, 2000), which states that a covered health care provider must make reasonable efforts to limit disclosure of protected health information to the minimum necessary to accomplish the intended purpose of the use, disclosure, or request (45 C.F.R. §164.502(b) (1)). Rostow and Davis (2004) make this point directly when discussing what law enforcement (LE) executives should anticipate receiving in a report from an FFD examiner: The LE executive must not expect a complex examination of the officer’s life outside of the events connected to the reason for referral. . . . In general, the department will be given only the information that it must have to meet its public safety and business necessity obligations in the employment context . . . without revealing protected health information. (pp. 105–106) The ADA also imposes limits on how much private information can be disclosed to an employer, stipulating that “[a]n employer is entitled only to the information necessary to determine whether the employee can do the essential functions of the job or work without posing a direct threat” (EEOC, 2000, Question 13, p. 12). Courts have held that FFD examiners who go too far in disclosing confidential aspects of an employee’s life or health may be subject to tort action for invasion of privacy. In McGreal v. Ostrov et al. (2004), the circuit court ruled that the chief of police and his codefendants “were not entitled to disclosure of anything other than the fitness for duty determination. They were not entitled . . . to force the disclosure of the intimate and irrelevant details of McGreal’s home life” (p. 53). In an earlier California case, Pettus v. Cole (1996), the court reached nearly the identical opinion when the FFD examiners disclosed the employee’s history of an alcohol use disorder when the referring question was limited only to whether Pettus qualified for disability under the company’s benefits plan. The court wrote: 298

Fitness-for-Duty Evaluations

There is no reason in law or policy why an employer should be allowed access to detailed family or medical histories of its employees, or to the intricacies of its employees’ mental processes, except with the individual’s freely given consent to the particular disclosure or some other substantial justification. (at 99) Although it is important for examiners to strike a balance between offering too much detail and too little (see Principle 14), there also are particular facts about an employee that should not be communicated at all. One of these involves the employee’s genetic information, defined under the Genetic Information Nondiscrimination Act (GINA, 2008) as including the manifested medical conditions of family members (29 C.F.R. §1635.3(b)). Thus, for an FFD examiner who is performing the examination as an agent of the employer, both acquiring information about the employee’s family medical history and disclosing that information are prohibited under GINA. Nonforensic clinicians are generally not trained in preparing reports for the lay audience, so particular care must be taken to ensure that the FFD evaluation report does not contain information that exceeds either ethical or legal boundaries. It also is important to be vigilant to the possibility of organizational or administrative limits on the content of a fitness-for-duty report, inasmuch as many police agencies have written policies that specify the constraints on report content. In Pettus (1996), the court noted that, in the absence of a specific authorization from the employee to disclose personal health information, the examining doctor must limit disclosure to a description of the “functional limitations” that may result from the employee’s medical condition. As stated by Gold and Shuman (2009), “Advancing the credibility of the examination by providing all information disclosed to an employer risks unnecessary breaches of confidentiality and psychological harm to the examinee” (p. 33).

Principle 14: Avoid Mere Conclusory Opinions Unless Otherwise Instructed by the Referring Party In an effort to avoid reporting too much private information and facing possible legal, civil, regulatory, or professional sanctions, psychologists sometimes choose to limit their reports simply to a “fit versus unfit” statement without further detail or explanation. This strategy might even be requested by the referring party, in which case it is incumbent on the examiner who accepts such a referral to comply with it. But doing so may very well undermine the usefulness of the report, and examiners should consider the consequences of this approach, as well as the alternatives, and discuss both with the retaining party at the outset of the evaluation. In the federal environment, the Merit Systems Protection Board (Lassiter v. Department of Justice, 1993) held that the proper standard when assessing the probative weight of medical opinion in an FFD evaluation is (1) whether the opinion was based on a medical examination, (2) whether the opinion provided a “reasoned explanation for its findings as distinct from mere conclusory assertions” (p. 4), (3) the qualifications of the expert rendering the opinion, and (4) the extent and duration of the expert’s familiarity with the condition of the employee. In Slater v. Dept. of Homeland Security (2008), the Board concluded that the FFD reports that were “entirely conclusory, devoid of any medical documentation or explanation in support of their conclusions” carried less “credibility and reliability” than one that was “a thorough, detailed, and relevant medical opinion addressing the medical issues of the agency’s removal action” (at paragraph 16). Gold and Shuman (2009) argue in favor of a “reliability” standard for reports and testimony regarding workplace mental health evaluations, and they assert that reliable expert opinion in this context is characterized by four considerations. It should (1) rest on an adequate basis (e.g., dates and details of interviews and examinations; results of psychological testing; school, military, and work 299

David M. Corey

records); (2) clearly articulate what opinion(s) or conclusion(s) the examiner draws from the raw data; (3) clearly explain how the examiner reasoned from the raw data to the opinion offered, including the relevant science and its limits; and (4) fairly address these issues from the perspective of alternative explanations (Gilbert v. Daimler Chrysler Corp., 2004; Shuman, 2005; see also American PsychologyLaw Society, 2008, SGFP Guideline 13.01, Accuracy, Fairness, and Avoidance of Deception). Preparing a report that contains these elements of reliability may also facilitate the due process rights of the employee. In the landmark case of Cleveland Board of Education v. Loudermill (1985), the Court held that a represented public employee may not be terminated without first giving the employee “oral or written notice of the charges against him, an explanation of the employer’s evidence, and an opportunity to present his side of the story” (at 546). Subsequent cases have extended Loudermill due process rights to circumstances in which an officer has been dismissed as a result of an FFD evaluation, noting that the terminated employee has a right to understand the psychological “charges” against him, the evidence underlying the charges, and to be given an opportunity to refute them (cf. Bass v. City of Albany, 1992; Bauschard v. Martin, 1993; Nuss v. Township of Falls, et al., 1985). When the retaining party requires a mere conclusory opinion in spite of the inherent limitations of that approach, examiners may consider an alternative posited by Stone (2000). He suggests under such circumstances that the examiner prepare two reports: one for the referring party that conforms to the required limitations and a second for the file that explicates the rationale for the opinions and conclusions and is available for use in litigation should it later be required. Whatever approach is used, examiners should make every effort to prepare a report that conforms to the limits of the law, the explicit terms of the referring party’s request and policies, and ethical considerations, while also striving to provide sufficient probative value to be useful both in the immediate employment-related decisions and any reasonably anticipated adjudicative or administrative proceeding.

Principle 15: Address Causation, Treatment, or Restoration of Fitness, and/or Accommodation Only if Requested by the Referring Party The importance of clarifying the relevant clinical and forensic questions in advance of the examination was emphasized in the discussion under Principle 2. It does little to benefit the examiner or any of the involved parties if this task is postponed until after the examination is finished and the report writing begins. But even at this stage, obtaining such clarification is better done late than not at all. As previously discussed, it is best to obtain the referral source’s questions in writing. Evaluators should write their opinions framed as responses to these questions, organized by listing each question, followed by the response. “When specific questions are asked, evaluators should limit themselves to providing opinions and supporting data responsive only to these questions unless otherwise specified” (Gold & Shuman, 2009, p. 153). This is especially true on matters of causation, treatment or restoration of fitness, and accommodation. Offering opinions beyond the limits of the referral questions inappropriately extends into the disposition stage (cf. Grisso, 2003) of decision making, which is a matter properly left to the employer. CAUSATION

Examining clinicians are often tempted in FFD examination reports to address causation, if only because it is a central element of most clinical training programs and a compelling topic of interest. Clinicians generally are drawn to questions of causation, and in an effort to help explain the employee’s condition or to facilitate treatment, evaluators may be inclined to discuss or opine on causation in the report, notwithstanding the lack of empirical evidence underlying many such opinions. Evaluators should be mindful that causation is not merely a clinical concept, but a legal one, as well, and the

300

Fitness-for-Duty Evaluations

definitions and implications associated with each are quite different from one another. Furthermore, for important reasons having to do with risk management, liability exposure, collective-bargaining procedures, and other legal and administrative considerations, the referring party may want the issue of causation to be addressed at another time, by a separate examiner, or not at all. Causation in particular is a concept that carries implications of potential liability both for the employee and the employer, and each party may be differentially helped or harmed by addressing this question prematurely. (Note: Causation in this context refers to the etiology—proximate or remote—of the disorder. In contrast, the causal analysis discussed as a decision-making component under Principle 12 is concerned with the underlying cause of the employee’s functional impairments (i.e., whether the impairment is due to a mental health condition or some other factor). It is not concerned with the cause of the condition itself.) TREATMENT

If causation is a compelling topic for clinicians, it pales in comparison to the attraction to treatment. Clinicians generally wish to be helpful, and this is especially true when faced with an examinee that is psychologically injured, suffering, or otherwise distressed. Indeed, when an evaluator concludes that an employee is psychologically unfit for duty, the referring party often requests that the examiner address how fitness might be restored. When this is the case, evaluators should still be careful to limit their treatment recommendations, including modalities and duration, to those for which there is adequate evidence of effectiveness. Most importantly, examiners should be careful not to condition a determination of fitness on an employee’s participation in counseling or other therapy. When such a condition is stipulated, it typically is rationalized in one of two ways: either because the examiner believes the employee’s current fitness is unlikely to be sustained without additional or ongoing treatment, or because the employee’s current unfitness results from a minor impairment expected to respond quickly to treatment. In cases of the former, if the employee’s current fitness is so fragile or unstable as to be undone in the foreseeable future without the benefit of ongoing treatment, the examiner should reconsider whether the interactive analysis (see Principle 12) justifies the fit-for-duty conclusion. Examiners need not, nor should they, be constrained in their analyses only to the employee’s present symptoms and adaptation; they should also consider the known or reasonably anticipated course of the examinee’s condition and fitness in light of the individualized assessment. In cases where the employee is judged currently unfit for duty due to a remediable condition, it is usually more prudent to acknowledge the current unfit status and not address recommendations designed to restore fitness unless requested by the referring party. When an employer requires treatment as an employment condition, some jurisdictions have held that the employer is responsible for the cost of treatment and compensation to the employee for any on-duty time used to engage in treatment (Sehie v. City of Aurora, 2005; see also Todd v. Lexington Fayette Urban County Government, 2009, where the court reached a different conclusion when the mandated conditions are for the benefit of the employee and not the employer). Furthermore, employees required to engage in treatment can easily come to believe (erroneously) that treatment compliance will or should shield them from consequences for persistent problematic behavior. Unless the referring party has specifically requested the examiner to opine on treatment or restoration of fitness, the report should be silent on this question. When the employer does request treatment recommendations from the examiner, it may be useful to reference the practical guidance of the EEOC: Regardless of whether employers believe they are trying to help employees who have medical conditions, employers should focus instead on addressing unacceptable workplace

301

David M. Corey

conduct. Employer comments about the disability and its treatment could lead to potential ADA claims (e.g., the employer “regarded” the employee as having a disability or the employer engaged in disparate treatment). (EEOC, 2008, Question 12, p. 12)

ACCOMMODATION

Under the ADA, an employer “must provide a reasonable accommodation to the known physical or mental limitations of a qualified individual with a disability unless it can show that the accommodation would impose an undue hardship” (EEOC, 1997, p. 12). A reasonable accommodation is defined as “any change in the work environment or in the way things are customarily done that enables an individual with a disability to enjoy equal employment opportunities” (29 C.F.R. §1630.9). This may include job restructuring, part-time or modified work schedules, reassignment to a vacant position, and providing additional unpaid leave for necessary treatment. Employers are only responsible, however, for providing a reasonable accommodation to a known limitation of a qualified individual with a disability. Even when an employee has a psychological condition that renders him or her unfit for duty, it is not per se legally equivalent to a disability under the terms of the ADA. Indeed, some conditions that may not meet the ADA’s definition of a disability may nevertheless render an employee unfit for duty. Moreover, except in cases of obvious disabilities, it is generally the case that the employer’s obligation to explore accommodation arises only “when the employee tells the employer he is disabled” (Hammon v. DHL Airways, 1997/1999, at 445). The employee need not use the word disabled or disability to formally initiate the accommodation exploration process (Cannice v. Norwest Bank Iowa N.A., 1999); on the other hand, where the employee openly denies having a disabling condition, even an FFD examiner’s finding to the contrary may not be sufficient to require accommodation efforts on the part of the employer (Larson v. Koch Refi ning Co., 1996). Once an employee specifically requests an accommodation for a work-impairing condition, the employer has an affirmative duty to engage in the “interactive process” to obtain relevant information about the employee’s condition and the basis for requesting an accommodation (Barnett v. U.S. Air., Inc., 2000). Thus, employers may prefer, and usually are best advised, to separate questions of reasonable accommodation from the FFD question. However, when in the interests of expediency and/or compassion an employer asks the examiner to make recommendations regarding accommodation, the examiner should become knowledgeable about the types of accommodation that are regarded by the courts as reasonable (e.g., an employer is not required to restructure or reallocate the essential functions of a job as a reasonable accommodation, and the essential functions may include the start and stop time of an employee’s work schedule; see Guice-Mills v. Derwinski, 1992). Unless the examiner is familiar with the statutes, regulations, and case law pertinent to reasonable accommodation, it is best to inform the referring party that questions about accommodation will be deferred to a separate stage and examiner, even if requested. Under any circumstances, reasonable accommodation is always an employer’s decision, not an examiner’s.

Concluding Remarks These 15 principles represent established practices informed by law, professional standards, the literature, and experience. In selecting these, I undoubtedly have left out other fundamental practices that seem obvious, are universally accepted, or have little consequence if not carried out. To the extent that I have overlooked a principle of importance, future editions of this Handbook may provide a remedy. 302

Fitness-for-Duty Evaluations

As a reflection of law, standards, and experience, these principles will evolve over time. They also will, and should, provoke discussion and debate. But in the interim it is my hope that they also will provide some measure of guidance to psychologists who seek to approach these high-stakes evaluations with the exacting attention to law, ethics, and practice standards that they demand and deserve. Nothing short of human lives is at stake when evaluating the psychological fitness of a police officer, and nothing less than principled work is required from those who do them.

Author Note The author is grateful to Casey O. Stewart for his review of an earlier draft of this chapter and for his substantive advice. Thanks also go to Kirk Heilbrun for generously permitting his principles of forensic mental health assessment (Heilbrun, 2001) to be used as a foundation for this material. Finally, gratitude is extended to Yossef S. Ben-Porath, Herbert M. Gupton, Michael D. Roberts, Jim Tracy, Philip S. Trompetter, and Richard C. Wihera, whose ongoing peer consultation provided the impetus for this chapter.

Notes 1. The term FFD evaluation is used throughout this chapter to avoid the more awkward abbreviation FFDEs in its plural form. Some authors and publications use the latter abbreviation, and they are used here when quoting from them. These terms and abbreviations are equivalent in meaning. 2. Under most circumstances, the examiner’s fitness determination should not directly decide the employee’s disposition (i.e., return to duty or not). According to the EEOC,   A doctor who conducts medical examinations for an employer should not be responsible for making employment decisions or deciding whether or not it is possible to make a reasonable accommodation for a person with a disability. That responsibility lies with the employer. The doctor’s role should be limited to advising the employer about an individual’s functional abilities and limitations in relation to job functions, and about whether the individual meets the employer’s health and safety requirements. (EEOC, 2002, at 6.4)   In the U.S. military, ultimate judgments about fitness are made by the service member’s commanding officer, and the examiner’s role is limited to providing findings and a recommendation as to whether the service member is deemed unsuitable for continued military service due to a mental health disorder (U.S. Department of Defense, 1997). Likewise, police employers retain the ultimate responsibility for making retention decisions, with proper consideration—not deference—given to the results and recommendations of the FFD evaluation. As noted by the court in Thompson v. City of Arlington (1993):   City and its officials, who have special knowledge of the factors that enter into whether a particular person should serve as a police officer, are better equipped than the health care providers, or other health care experts, to determine whether plaintiff should return to regular duty. (at 1144) 3. Restrictions generally pertain to what an employee should not do as a result of a mental or emotional condition, whereas limitations refer to what an employee cannot do (Anfang & Wall, 2006, p. 677).

References and Further Reading Aitchison, W. (2000). The rights of law enforcement officers (4th ed.). Portland, OR: Labor Relations Information System. American Psychology-Law Society. (2008). Specialty guidelines for forensic psychology (4th draft). Retrieved December 15, 2009, from http://ap-ls.org/links/92908sgfp.pdf. Americans with Disabilities Act of 1990, 42 U.S.C. § 12101 et seq. (1990). American Psychological Association. (2002). Ethical principles for psychologists and code of conduct. Washington, DC: Author. Anfang, S. A., Faulkner, L. R., Fromson, J. A., & Gendel, M. H. (2005). American Psychiatric Association resource document on guidelines for psychiatric fitness-for-duty evaluations of physicians. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 33, 85–88. Anfang, S. A., & Wall, B. W. (2006). Psychiatric fitness-for-duty evaluations. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 29, 675–693.

303

David M. Corey Battista, M. E. (1988). Assessing work capacity. Journal of Insurance Medicine, 20(3), 16–22. Borum, R., Super, J., & Rand, M. (2003). Forensic assessment for high-risk occupations. In A. M. Goldstein & I. B. Weiner (Eds.), Forensic psychology (Vol. 11, pp. 133–147). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley. Brodsky, C. M. (1996). Psychiatric aspects of fitness for duty. Occupational Medicine, 11(4), 719–726. Budd, F. C., & Harvey, S. (2006). Military fitness-for-duty evaluations. In C. H. Kennedy & E. A. Zillmer (Eds.), Military psychology (pp. 35–60). New York: Guilford. California Government Code §1031 (2005). Corey, D. M. (1988). The psychological suitability of police officer candidates. Ph.D. dissertation, Fielding Graduate University, Santa Barbara, CA. Retrieved August 26, 2010, from Dissertations and theses: Full text (Publication No. AAT8821055). Dawkins, M., Griffin, S., & Dawkins, M. (2006, October). Psychological fitness-for-duty examinations (FFD): Making effective use of FFD outcome data. Paper presented at the 113th Annual Conference of the International Association of Chiefs of Police, Police Psychological Services Section, Boston, MA. Decker, K. P. (2006). Fit, unfit or misfit. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas. EEOC regulations to implement the equal employment provisions of the Americans With Disabilities Act, Title 29, Subtitle B, Chapter XIV, Part 1630 (1991). Equal Employment Opportunity Commission. (1995). ADA enforcement guidance: Preemployment disability-related questions and medical examinations (Compliance manual, Volume II, Section 902, No. 915.002). Washington, DC: Author. Equal Employment Opportunity Commission. (1997). EEOC enforcement guidance on the Americans with Disabilities Act and Psychiatric Disabilities (Compliance Manual, Volume II, Section 902, No. 915.002). Washington, DC: Author. Equal Employment Opportunity Commission. (2000). Enforcement guidance on disability-related inquiries and medical examinations of employees under the Americans with Disabilities Act (Compliance Manual, Volume II, Section 902, No. 915.002). Washington, DC: Author. Equal Employment Opportunity Commission. (2002). ADA technical assistance manual, January 1992. Publication EEOC-M-1A (10/29/2002 Addendum). Washington, DC: Author. Equal Employment Opportunity Commission. (2008). The Americans with Disabilities Act: Applying performance and conduct standards to employees with disabilities. Retrieved January 4, 2010, from http://www.eeoc.gov/facts/ performance-conduct.html. Family & Medical Leave Act, 29 U.S.C. §2601 (1993). Fisher, M. A. (2009). Replacing “Who is the client?” with a different ethical question. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 40(1), 1–7. Genetic Information Nondiscrimination Act of 2008 (GINA). Pub.L. 110–223, 122 Stat. 881 (2008). Gold, L. H., & Shuman, D. W. (2009). Evaluating mental health disability in the workplace. New York: Springer Science & Business Media. Greenberg, S. A., & Shuman, D. W. (1997). Irreconcilable conflict between therapeutic and forensic roles. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 28, 50–57. Greenberg, S. A., & Shuman, D. W. (2007). When worlds collide: Therapeutic and forensic roles. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 38(2), 129–132. Grisso, T. (1986). Evaluating competencies: Forensic assessments and instruments. New York: Plenum. Grisso, T. (2003). Evaluating competencies: Forensic assessments and instruments (2nd ed.). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Press. Health Insurance Portability and Accountability Act (HIPAA). Title 45, Subtitle A, Subchapter C, Part 160, General Administrative Requirements (2000a). Health Insurance Portability and Accountability Act (HIPAA) Privacy Rule. Title 45, Subtitle A, Subchapter C, Part 164, Subpart E, Privacy of Individually Identifiable Health Information (2000b). Heilbrun, K. (2001). Principles of forensic mental health assessment. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum. Heilbrun, K., Grisso, T., & Goldstein, A. M. (2009). Foundations of forensic mental health assessment. New York: Oxford University Press. Heilbrun, K., Warren, J., & Picarello, K. (2003). Third party information in forensic assessment. In A. M. Goldstein & I. B. Weiner (Eds.), Forensic psychology (Vol. 11, pp. 69–86). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley. International Association of Chiefs of Police. (2009). Psychological fitness-for-duty evaluation guidelines. Arlington, VA: International Association of Chiefs of Police. Retrieved August 25, 2010, from http://theiacp.org/ psych_services_section/pdfs/Psych-FitnessforDutyEvaluation.pdf. Koocher, G. P. (2007). Twenty-first century ethical challenges for psychology. American Psychologist, 62, 375–384. Mariani, R. C., & Avelenda, S. M. (2009). Mental health issues under the ADA. Retrieved December 15, 2009, from www.cnapro.com/docs/ada_mental_health_issues.doc.

304

Fitness-for-Duty Evaluations McDonald, J. J., Kulick, F. B., & Creighton, M. K. (1995). Mental disabilities under the ADA: A management rights approach. Employee Relations Law Journal, 20(4), 541–569. Melton, G. B., Petrila, J., Poythress, N. G., Slobogin, C., Lyons, P. M., & Otto, R. K. (2007). Psychological evaluations for the courts. New York: Guilford. Morse, S. (1978). Law and mental health professionals: The limits of expertise. Professional Psychology, 9, 389–399. OPM Medical Qualification Determinations. 5 C.F.R. §339 (1995). Otto, R. K., & Krauss, D. A. (2009, August 10). Contemplating the presence of third party observers and facilitators in psychological evaluations. Assessment Online First. doi:10.1177/1073191109336267. Rehabilitation Act of 1973, 29 U.S.C.A. §706 (1973). Rogers, R. (Ed.). (1997). Clinical assessment of malingering and deception (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford. Rostow, C. D., & Davis, R. D. (2004). A handbook for psychological fitness-for-duty evaluations in law enforcement. Binghamton, NY: Haworth Press. Shuman, D. (2005). Psychiatric and psychological evidence. St. Paul, MN: Thomson-West. Stone, A. V. (2000). Fitness for duty: Principles, methods and legal issues. New York: CRC Press. U.S. Department of Defense. (1997). Directive 6490.1, Mental Health Evaluations of Members of the Armed Forces, October 1, 1997. U.S. Department of Health & Human Services. (2000, December 28). Standards for Privacy of Individually Identifiable Health Information, Federal Register, 65(250), 82554.

Cases Cited AFGE Local 596 v. Department of Justice et al. (Sherman, 2007). Albert v. Runyon, 6 F. Supp. 2d 57, 1998 U.S. Dist. Lexis 7505 (D. Mass. 1998). Anderson v. Little League Baseball, Inc., 794 F. Supp. 342, 61 USLW 2050 (D. Ariz. 1992). Arkansas Attorney General Opinion No. 2000–338 (1–21–2001). Barnett v. U.S. Air., Inc., 228 F.3d 1105 (9th Cir. 2000). Bass v. City of Albany, 968 F.2d 1067 (11th Cir. 1992). Bauschard v. Martin, 1993 WL 79259 (N.D. Ill. 1993) (not reported). Bonsignore v. City of New York, 683 F.2d 635 (2d Cir. 1982). Brownfield v. City of Yakima, No. 09–35628 (9th Cir. July 27, 2010). Brumbalough v. Camelot Care Centers, Inc., 427 F.3d 996 (6th Cir. 2005). Butler v. Thornburgh, 900 F.2d 871, 876 (5th Cir. 1990), cert. denied, 498 U.S. 998 (1990). Cannice v. Norwest Bank Iowa N.A., 189 F.3d 723 (8th Cir. 1999). Carrillo v. National Council of Churches of Christ in the USA, 976 F. Supp. 254 (SDNY 1997). Cleghorn v. Hess, 853 P.2d 1260 (Nev. 1993). Cleveland Board of Education v. Loudermill, 470 U.S. 532, 105 S.Ct. 1487, 84 L.Ed.2d 494 (1985). Cody v. CIGNA Healthcare of St. Louis, Inc.139 F.3d 595 (8th Cir. 1998). Colon v. City of Newark, 909 A.2d 725 (NJAD 2006), 2006 WL 1194230 (unpublished decision). Conte v. Horcher, 365 N.E.2d 567 (Ill. App. 1977). Crandall v. Michaud, 603 So.2d 637, 637 (Fla. 1992). Cripe v. City of San Jose, 261 F.3d 877 (9th Cir. 2001). Deen v. Darosa, 414 F.3d 731 (7th Cir. 2005). Denhof et al. v. City of Grand Rapids, 494 F.3d 534 (6th Cir. 2007). Elkins v. Syken, 672 So.2d 517, 519 (Fla. 1996). Galieti v. State Farm Mutual Automobile Insurance Co. et al., 154 FRD 262; 1994 U.S. Dist. LEXIS 8835; 29 Fed. R. Serv. 3d (Callaghan) 183. Gardner v. Broderick, 392 U.S. 273 (1968). Garrity v. New Jersey, 385 U.S. 493 (1967). Gilbert v. Daimler Chrysler Corp., 685 N.W.2d 391 (Mich. 2004). Guice-Mills v. Derwinski, 967 F.2d 794 (2nd Cir. 1992). Hammon v. DHL Airways, Inc., 980 F. Supp. 919 (S.D. Ohio 1997), aff ’d, 165 F.3d 441, 450 (6th Cir. 1999). Hill v. Winona, 454 N.W.2d 659 (Minn. App. 1990). Hogarth v. Thornburgh, 833 F. Supp. 1077 (SDNY 1993). Holst v. Veterans Affairs, 106 MSPR 499 (2007). Jackson v. Lake County, 2003 WL 22127743 (N.D. Ill. 2003) (not reported). Jackson v. Wilson, 262 S.E.2d 547, 152 Ga. App. 250 (Ga. App. 1979). Knill v. Principi, EEOC DOC 01984220, 2001 WL 683335 (EEOC), citing Universal Camera Corp. v. NLRB, 340 US 474, 477 (1951).

305

David M. Corey Kraft v. Police Commissioner of Boston, 417 Mass. 235, 629 N.E.2d 995, 1994 Mass. Lexis 93 (1994). Larson v. Koch Refining Co., 920 F. Supp. 1000 (D. Minn. 1996). Lassiter v. Department of Justice, 60 M.S.P.R. 138 (December 21, 1993). Lassiter v. Reno, 86 F.3d 1151 (4th Cir. 1996), cert. denied, 519 U.S. 1091 (1997). Lefkowitz v. Turley, 414 U.S. 70 (1973). Leonel v. American Airlines, Inc., 400 F.3d 702 (9th Cir. 2005). Marino v. U.S. Postal Service et al., 25 F.3d 1037, 3 A.D. Cases 704 (1st Cir. 1994). Mazzarella v. U.S. Postal Service, 849 F. Supp. 89 (D. Mass. 1994). McGreal v. Ostrov, 368 F.3d 657 (7th Cir. 2004). McKnight v. Monroe Co. Sheriff ’s Dept. (S.D. Ind. 2002). Myers v. Hose, 50 F.3d 278 (4th Cir. 1995). National Treasury Employees Union v. Von Raab, 489 U.S. 656, 109 S.Ct. 1384, 103 L.Ed.2d 685 (1989). NLRB v. Weingarten, Inc., 420 US 251 (1975). Nuss v. Township of Falls, et al., 89 Pa. Commw. 97; 491 A.2d 971, 1985 Pa. Commw. LEXIS 1029. Palmer v. Circuit Court of Cook County, 117 F.3d 351 (7th Cir. 1997). Pettus v. Cole, 57 Cal. Rptr. 2d 46, 49 Cal. App. 4th 402 (Cal. App. 1 Dist. 1996). Policemen’s Benevolent Association of New Jersey v. Township of Washington, 850 F.2d 133 (3rd Cir. 1988). Ragge v. MCA/Universal Studios et al., 165 FRD 601 (CD Cal. 1995); 1995 U.S. Dist. LEXIS 20669. Richardson v. City of Pasadena, 500 S.W.2d 175 (Tex. Civ. App.—Houston [14th Dist.] 1973), rev’d on procedural ground, 513 S.W.2d 1 (Tex. 1974). Sager v. County of Yuba, 68 Cal. Rptr. 3d 1, 156 Cal. App. 4th 1049 (Cal. App. 2007). Schloendorff v. Society of New York Hospital, 211 NY 125, 105 N.E. 92 (N.Y. 1914). Schuman v. City of Philadelphia, 470 F. Supp. 449 (E.D. Pa. 1979). Sehie v. City of Aurora, 432 F.3d 749 (7th Cir. 2005). Simmons v. Rehab Xcel, Inc., 731 So.2d 529, 531–532 (La. App. 1999). Slater v. Dept. of Homeland Security, 108 MSPR 419, 2008 MSPB 73. Stewart v. Pearce, 484 F.2d 1031 (9th Cir. 1973). Sullivan v. River Valley School District, 197 F.3d 804, 9 AD 1711(6th Cir. 1999). Thomas v. Corwin, 483 F.3d 516 (8th Cir. 2007), U.S. App. Lexis 7601, 100 FEP Cases (BNA) 297. Thompson v. City of Arlington, 838 F. Supp. 1137; 1993 U.S. Dist. LEXIS 17093; 2 Am. Disabilities Cas. (BNA) 1756. Tingler v. City of Tampa, 400 So.2d 146 (Fla. App. 1981). Todd v. Lexington Fayette Urban County Government, 2009 U.S. Dist. LEXIS 115183 (C.D. KY 2009). Tomlin v. Holecek et al., 150 FRD 628 (D. Minn. 1993); 1993 U.S. Dist. LEXIS 16851; 27 Fed. R. Serv. 3d (Callaghan) 977. Universal Camera Corp. v. National Labor Relations Board, 340 U.S. 474 (1951). Vinson v. The Superior Court of Alameda County, 43 Cal. 3d 833, 239 Cal. Rptr. 292 (1987). Watson v. City of Miami Beach, 177 F.3d 932 (11th Cir. 1999). Yin v. California, 95 F.3d 864 (9th Cir. 1996).

306

14 METHODS FOR REAL-TIME ASSESSMENT OF OPERATIONAL STRESS DURING REALISTIC POLICE TACTICAL TRAINING Donatella Brisinda, Riccardo Fenici, and Anna Rita Sorbo Introduction Numerous studies have shown that factors related to organizational structure, inappropriate authoritarian management, poor interpersonal relationships with supervisors, lack of adequate participation in planning and of autonomy in performing duties, and lack of recognition for work accomplishments are usually reported as the major source of stress for police officers. On the other hand, there are other risk factors such as alteration of normal sleep patterns because of shift schedules, daily exposure to interpersonal violence, personal endangerment with fear of revenge from criminals, and interactions with an ambivalent public, which induce chronic emotional effects that jeopardize the physical and psychological balance of police officers with their families. A chronic burden of negative emotions such as anger, anxiety, or depression can lead to psychological burnout, whereas continuous activation of the stress response systems (i.e., the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis and the autonomic nervous system [ANS]) can induce alterations of the neuroautonomic and endocrine balance, leading to higher incidence of hypertension, cardiovascular disease, metabolic syndrome, and even cancer. Under such unfavorable basal conditions, law enforcement officers may be suddenly called to respond to critical situations implying an appropriate use of tactics and force to protect their lives and the lives of others. In front of a potentially lethal threat, although in the grip of fight or flight— induced psychophysiological alterations, the police officer is required to maintain vigilance, dynamic threat assessment, sound judgment, and appropriate tactical decision making. Instead, uncontrolled stress-induced emotions (i.e., fear and anger), perceptual anomalies (tunnel vision, auditory exclusion, and altered sense of space and time), memory loss, and intrusive distractive thoughts may induce behavioral alterations (automatic pilot, dissociation, and freezing) with tragic consequences. In spite of the high risk of stress-induced errors, accidents, or overreaction that can compromise performance, jeopardize public safety, and determine liability, the training of police officers on tactical stress management is still more empirical than based on scientific knowledge. From real-life experience, we have to acknowledge that empirical police training is often insufficient to guarantee operational success and officers’ survival. Thus, an unquestionable mandatory commitment of any modern police institution should be to favor research in the psychophysiology of officers’ behavior under critical tactical stress and to develop more efficient scientifically based training to improve officers’ survival on the street.

307

Donatella Brisinda, et al.

During the last decade, only a few attempts have been reported in such direction; however, there could be others that have not been disclosed to the public (as in our case) for institutional classification of the information. Available data are still limited and in most cases rather empirical. The purpose of this chapter is to summarize present experience and to provide suggestions about how to design more comprehensive investigational protocols to evaluate police tactical stress, using scientific methods and available technologies gathered from experimental and clinical psychophysiology.

Tactical Stress It is well known by police officers that when something is going to be wrong for an unexpected sudden threat occurring during a boring night shift or the best-planned SWAT operation, there are immediate, uncontrollable physical reactions (one for all, a marked increase of the heart rate), which alarm the mind about the impending danger and trigger the Oh shit! statement (Solomon, 1991). At that point, the officer may do the right thing, react and survive, or lose control and be defeated. The empirical experience of survivors is an inestimable wealth that must be transferred to other officers, to prepare them for the danger before they run into troubles themselves (Solomon, 1991; Solomon & Horn, 1986). However, besides fate, common sense suggests that there have to be critical differences in the functioning of officers who performed well and those who failed. In order to inoculate this precious experience into operational efficiency with more efficient training, it is necessary to study and understand such psychophysiological differences. The problem is that acute psychophysiological stress occurring during high-risk police operations is difficult to study and may elude an accurate quantification because it consists of several (interrelated) mechanisms, including psychological, endocrine, immunologic, and physical involvement, which reciprocal interaction is characterized by a pronounced interindividual and situational variability. To achieve a comprehensive understanding, high competence in very different fields and an appropriate interdisciplinary approach are required. Scientists and police officers have to work together to understand the weak points of present knowledge and to design a better training, based on scientific evidence of efficacy. On the contrary, at the moment, the majority of police instructors are highly experienced street veterans but usually have not had much knowledge about how to use scientific methods in training. On the other hand, scientists skilled in research useful to studying police tactical stress are usually closed in psychophysiology laboratories and very far away from the street and from the real-life stress occurring in highly demanding police tactical tasks. Thus, a first mandatory step is to create an appropriate communication between such different professionals, by overcoming the diffidence of police officers who usually don’t like to deal with doctors (especially “shrinks”) and to have such people telling them theories about what works (or not) on the street and by pulling the “scientist” out of the laboratory and into the police reality, at least in the training arena where such a realm can be realistically reconstructed and studied. An excellent example of such fruitful interaction and integration can be found in Matthew J. Sharp’s recent book Processing Under Pressure (2010), where a lot of scientific knowledge has been provided in an understandable style and has been used to interpret real operational situations.

Emotions and Cognitive Function Under Tactical Stress Indeed, the relationship between emotion and cognition has been a matter of scientific debate for decades, and a systematic treatment of such a complex matter is obviously beyond the scope of this chapter. Here, we will briefly summarize only basic information useful to understand what happens to police officers when a tactical scenario turns into violence and needs force-on-force action. In such a situation, major emotional reactions to perceived danger trigger the officer’s defensive response, mobilizing strength for survival through ANS adaptations, but inducing significant body 308

Real-Time Assessment in Tactical Training

dysfunction (e.g., perceptual distortion and modification of cardiorespiratory function) (Grossman & Siddle, 1998; Honig & Roland, 1998; Solomon & Horn, 1986) with possible derangement of cognitive function and rational control. Initially, cognitive and emotional functions were viewed as largely separated, with belief that emotion was primary to and independent of cognition (Kunst-Wilson & Zajonc, 1980). Modern research in neuroscience and psychophysiology has significantly modified this interpretation. An important progress in understanding the relationship between cognition and emotion in humans has become possible after the advent of modern neuroimaging, especially the functional MRI, which confirmed that cognitive processes occur in cortical regions of the brain (Gazzaniga, Ivry, & Mangun, 2008) and that brain structures linked to emotion are mostly subcortical. However, there is a more complex network, including the anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex, which provides the mechanism for self-regulation of cognition and emotion (Allman, Hakeem, Erwin, Nimchinsky, & Hof, 2001; Bush, Luu, & Posner, 2000; Posner & Rothbart, 2007; Posner, Rothbart, Rueda, & Tang, 2009). The anterior cingulate cortex also (1) regulates the processing of information from other brain areas, (2) is sensitive to reward and pain (Eisenberger, Lieberman, & Williams, 2003; Hampton & O’Doherty, 2007), and (3) serves as part of a controlling network in coupling cognitive and emotional areas during task performance (Crottaz-Herbette & Menon, 2006; Etkin, Egner, Peraza, Kandel, & Hirsch, 2006; Posner, Rothbart, Sheese, & Tang, 2007). In addition, subcortical structures (e.g., the amygdala, the ventral striatum, and the hypothalamus), which are considered part of the primitive brain and capable of operating fast and in automatic fashion to evoke survival responses (Whalen et al., 2004), are widely networked to integrate emotion and cognition. In this way, typical cognitive functions, such as perception, attention, and memory, are largely dependent on emotional stimuli. In summary, it is now evident that in the real world, there are situations in which cognition and emotion are acting simultaneously, and it is difficult to separate their reciprocal interaction; therefore, they are now considered as interdependent and integrated functions in controlling human thought and behavior (Duncan & Barrett, 2007; Gray, Braver, & Raichle, 2002; Pessoa, 2008; Sergerie, Lepage, & Armony, 2006), especially under high-stress situations.

The Amygdala and the Response to Threat and Fear The key brain structure that coordinates behavioral, immunological, and neuroendocrine responses to environmental threats and fear reaction is the amygdala, a multinuclear structure located deep within the temporal lobes, medial to the hypothalamus, and adjacent to the hippocampus, well situated to integrate and distribute information through widespread projections with the rest of the brain. The amygdala serves in assessing the environment, stores emotional memories within the brain, and compares incoming emotional signals with previous emotional memories in order to make instantaneous decisions about the threat level of new incoming sensory information. The emotional content can change the formation and recollection of a memory event. In animals, the enhancement of memory owing to an emotion is due more to the induced arousal level than to the positive or negative valence of the emotion per se (McGaugh, 2004; Phelps, Ling, & Carrasco, 2006). In humans, the amygdala is known to be a critical structure for the enhancement of emotional memory (Adolphs, Cahill, Schul, & Babinsky, 1997; Phelps, 2004) and to identify new items as opposed to old (Sergerie et al., 2007). The right amygdala is more involved in the formation of emotional memory, whereas the left amygdala is activated by the retrieval of those memories (Sergerie et al., 2006). The amygdala and the associated basal forebrain system play a major role in emotional memory storage (McGaugh, 2004) by modulating activation in a network of brain regions, including the hippocampus, which is centrally involved in memory formation, and in other brain structures (e.g., the nucleus accumbens, caudate nucleus, entorhinal cortex) and cortical regions (McGaugh, 2002). Experiments on fear conditioning have shown that the amygdala participates in the acquisition, storage, and expression of the conditioned fear response (i.e., when an animal learns that a 309

Donatella Brisinda, et al.

neutral stimulus predicts an aversive event). In humans, integrity of the left amygdala is necessary for physiological reaction to the threat stimulus (Olsson & Phelps, 2007) also in the case of instructed fear (Funayama, Grillon, Davis, & Phelps, 2001; Hugdahl & Ohman, 1977; Phelps et al., 2001) and observational fear (Ohman & Mineka, 2001). Moreover, the link between perception, attention, and emotion is mediated by the amygdala. In fact, visual responses are stronger when subjects view emotional scenes. It appears that the amygdala may provide a form of emotional attention that enhances visual information under emotional stress (Pessoa, Kastner, & Ungerleider, 2002; Vuilleumier, 2005) and responds to emotional faces of which subjects are not conscious (Whalen et al., 2004). Through its extensive connections to the hypothalamus and other autonomic nervous system centers, the amygdala is able to shortcut neural signals activating the ANS and emotional response before the higher brain centers receive sensory information. In this way, it provides fast and automatic—fight or flight—behavioral responses important for survival, following the “low route” suggested by LeDoux (1996). To date, little knowledge exists on the molecular basis of stress-induced defense (fight or flight) response underlying the simultaneous coordinated changes in the cardiovascular, respiratory, sensory, and behavioral parameters. To explore neural mechanisms of stress-related adjustments of central autonomic regulation, research has recently focused on several neurotransmitters possibly involved in modulation of the efferent pathways during defense responses. Among them, the orexin system can possibly serve as one essential modulator among many for coordinating circuits controlling autonomic function and behavior. Orexin-containing cells are widely distributed in the hypothalamus, thalamus, cerebral cortex, brain stem, and spinal cord, with widespread connections with other brain regions to control multiple physiological functions, including motivation and regulation of autonomic and neuroendocrine systems (Kuwaki, Zhang, Nakamura, & Deng, 2008). At the perception of a life-threatening attack, the effects of fear, anxiety, and anger are so automatic and rapid to preclude analytical thinking of what objectively occurs. In front of a real-life operational danger, the information-processing situation, the officer might unconsciously switch to the experiential thinking mode that occurs when a perceived emergency requires a quick response (Artwohl, 2008; Epstein, 1994). As opposed to the deliberative, analytical rational thinking mode, experiential thinking is seized by emotions and oriented toward immediate action. This is confirmed by the fact that 74% of the police officers studied by Artwohl (2002, 2008), under a sudden, life-threatening attack, responded “automatically to the perceived threat, giving little or no conscious thought to their actions,” in a way very consistent with the experiential thinking mode. The same author reports also that about 20% of the officers involved in a shooting incident “feel” that the information provided by “self-evidently valid experiential thinking” was more real than what actually happened during the incident, even when confronted with a videotape proving that they saw things that didn’t happen. Another cognitive process relevant to police work is the so-called behavioral (or response) inhibition—­ the process required to cancel an intended action. Behavioral inhibition involves several areas of the prefrontal cortex (e.g., dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, and inferior frontal cortex) (Aron, Robbins, & Poldrack, 2004; Rubia, Smith, Brammer, & Taylor, 2003) and is usually studied in the laboratory by using the so-called GO/NO-GO tasks in which subjects are asked to execute a motor response when shown the GO stimulus (e.g., press a key as fast as possible when you see a GO stimulus), but to withhold the response to the NO-GO stimulus. An equivalent situation in police work typically occurs in a SHOOT/NO-SHOOT scenario. A recent study investigated the interaction between the processing of emotional words and response inhibition. Response inhibition following negative words engaged in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. However, this region was not recruited by negative valence or inhibitory task demands per se while it was sensitive to the explicit interaction between behavioral inhibition and the processing of words with a negative valence (Goldstein et al., 2007). This might suggest a possible mechanism for emotional interference on the officer’s decision-making capability when he is in the empirical thinking mode piloted by 310

Real-Time Assessment in Tactical Training

the fear and/or anger emotional state. Thus, the experiential thinking mode has obvious advantages in life-threatening situations demanding an immediate response because it rapidly processes information to pilot almost automatically a survival response; however, it doesn’t guarantee that such automatic responses will always be the appropriate ones, from both tactical and legal points of view. This fact, in association with stress-induced memory distortion and fragmentation (Grossman & Siddle, 1998), might have serious implications in the postshooting aftermath, especially if officers have to provide justification for the use of deadly force. On the other hand, when officers have to make split-second decisions about the use of force, the automatic processing of the experiential system is dominant over the rational system and becomes the default option. As experiential thinking mode is based on “past experiences” (Epstein, 1994), it seems evident that in order to improve officers’ survival in a force-on-force confrontation, appropriate training should provide “past experiences” of proven efficacy. This cannot be achieved by theoretical teaching of police tactics and skills only, but also requires providing coached repetition under realistic stress, to verify the achievement of emotional control by the trainees and that their “automatic behaviors” under stress will be adaptive and efficient to solve tactical problems, within the law (Artwohl, 2002, 2008; Artwohl & Christensen, 1997; Humes, 1992). Moreover, police training must be redesigned, taking into account modern knowledge in psychophysiology to provide officers with the capability to keep the highest possible degree of emotional and situational control. This is obviously a challenging task because it implies filling the gap between scientific knowledge on psychophysiology gathered in the laboratory and the lack of methods to quantify what extent emotions affect individual tactical behavior in operational scenarios, in order to apply this knowledge in the demanding practice of police work.

Autonomic Reaction to Threat, Fear, and Anger In a life-threatening situation, emotional responses in the brain orchestrate bodily resources in an integrated fashion to secure specific functional adaptations to different and complex demands, finalizing them to survival; however, the debate of whether fear and anger drive specific physiological responses, useful to differentiate them, is still controversial. A first meta-analysis of 22 studies on somatovisceral responses sampled during a variety of emotional states indicated that discrete emotions couldn’t be fully differentiated by visceral activity alone (Cacioppo, Berntson, Larsen, Poehlmann, & Ito, 2000). Instead, a meta-analysis of previous research, focusing on the possibility to separate the effects of anger from those of fear, has demonstrated that the simultaneous effects of such emotions and of different behavioral responses (e.g., approach and withdrawal) (Christie & Friedman, 2004) can be at least in part be differentiated by measuring several physiological parameters related to bodily adaptation (Stemmler, 2004). A recent work of the same author has confirmed that anger could be somehow differentiated from fear by measuring bodily reactions (Stemmler, Aue, & Wacker, 2007). In extreme synthesis, the results confirmed that somatovisceral response to anger is characterized by a mixture of adrenaline and noradrenaline effects (Breggin, 1964; Funkenstein, 1955, 1956; Funkenstein, King, & Drolette, 1954; Wagner, 1989) with a relatively larger noradrenergic influence, some vagal withdrawal, and a slight α-adrenergic activation (Weiss, del-Bo, Reichek, & Engelman, 1980), with resultant bradycardia and periferal vasoconstriction, without blood pressure increase. Instead, somatovisceral response to fear is characterized by a relatively larger adrenergic influence and comprises vagal withdrawal, slight α-adrenergic activation, and diverse signs for β1-adrenergic activation, which are consistent with the action of adrenaline (Fahrenberg & Foerster, 1989; Funkenstein, 1955) (see Figure 14.1). The dominant autonomic response to threat is characterized by the prevalence of the sympathetic nervous activity over the parasympathetic tone. The enhancement of sympathetic activity generates energy, diverts blood flow away from the gastrointestinal tract and skin via vasoconstriction, enhances blood flow to skeletal muscles (by as much as 1200%) and to the lungs, dilates bronchioles for greater 311

Donatella Brisinda, et al.

Anger • Dominant noradrenaline effects mixed with adrenergic effects • Bradycardia • Stroke volume increase • Vagal withdrawal

Fear • Dominant adrenaline-like pattern • Increased LV contractility • Smaller T-wave, larger P-wave amplitudes • Stronger vagal withdrawal

ADRENALINE Stimulates alpha-, beta1, and beta2- adrenergic receptors. ⇓ Finger temperature, diastolic blood pressure, total peripheral resistance ⇑ Heart rate, systolic blood pressure, stroke volume, left-ventricular contractility, cardiac output, number of skin conductance responses, and respiration rate NORADRENALINE Stimulates alpha- and beta1 adrenergic receptors ⇓ Heart rate and finger temperature ⇑ Systolic and diastolic blood pressure, total peripheral resistance

Figure 14.1 Autonomic response to anger and fear, according to the meta-analysis of Stemmler (2004). Relatively stronger noradrenergic effects characterize anger, whereas fear is characterized by relatively dominant adrenergic response. Major physiological effects of adrenaline and noradrenaline are also summarized.

alveolar oxygen exchange, increases heart rate (HR) and contractility, dilates pupils, and relaxes the lens, allowing more light to enter the eye. The prevalence of sympathetic effects and concomitant adrenergic neuroendocrine response are finalized to increase strength, resistance, and attention for survival; however, they induce functional alterations (visual, auditory, cardiac, and so on), which largely affect the officer’s capability to move and act with the same flexibility available during training, in the absence of life-threatening stress (Siddle, 1999). Now, the question is whether it is possible to objectively quantify the kind and the amount of individual emotional reaction of a police officer involved in a critical operation, by measuring physiological parameters affected by dominant autonomic reactions, and how to do that. Thus, the challenge is to develop reliable, scientifically validated methods to evaluate such psychophysiological response during tactical tasks.

Methods to Measure Stress Reactions in the Laboratory In the psychophysiology laboratory, somatovisceral responses to emotions can be investigated under controlled conditions by monitoring a variety of physiologic signals such as electroencephalogram, electrocardiogram (ECG), electromyogram, impedance cardiogram, blood pressure, skin conductance, skin temperature, pulse volume, and respiration (Kreibig, Wilhelm, Roth, & Gross, 2007; Stemmler et al., 2007). Interestingly, it has also been recently suggested that the amplitude of resting ECG waveform contains information related to emotional personality and brain activity in the amygdala and hippocampus (Koelsch et al., 2007). Most of such recordings are feasible and reliable only when the investigated subject is sitting quietly to prevent movement artifacts, although even in the 312

Real-Time Assessment in Tactical Training

laboratory the quality of the recordings can be altered by several factors, which might jeopardize the reliability of the result. Such technical difficulty is obviously higher when attempting physiological recordings in the realm of police operational scenarios. For this reason, most parameters usually monitored to measure stress reactions in experimental psychology are not reliably obtainable on the field. Therefore, it is difficult to transfer scientific investigations from the laboratory in the training range and even less in the operational realm. In fact, although the wide experience developed in sports medicine and experimental psychology might help to find some technical solutions, the complexity of the dynamic variables that have to be monitored during realistic police scenarios implying the use of force is too wide to be covered with presently available technology. It turns out that there is very limited experience on direct measurement of human physiology during force-on-force encounters by law enforcement and that the only parameter that has been studied so far is the HR obtained with ECG recording or with HR monitoring devices used for sports activity, both methods having pros and cons. In addition to physiological signals, several humoral stress markers can be also measured. Salivary Dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA), Cortisol, and Secretory Immunoglobulin A (S-IgA) are the most frequently used, because they can be easily sampled without any subject discomfort, even in an outdoor situation. DHEA and its sulfated metabolite DHEA-S are hormones secreted by the adrenal cortex in response to pituitary adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) production. DHEA-S is the most abundant circulating steroid hormone in humans and its measurement has been of interest, especially because reduced levels of both DHEA and DHEA-S are associated with aging. DHEA is also the precursor to the human sex hormones (estrogen and testosterone) and has been found to be deficient in people suffering from many diseases including obesity, diabetes, hypertension, cancer, Alzheimer’s, immune deficiency, coronary artery disease, and various autoimmune disorders. Cortisol, a glucocorticoid hormone, is involved in protein, carbohydrate, and fat metabolism, and it is widely known as the stress hormone because it is secreted in excessive amounts when people are under stress. Cortisol is tightly regulated by feedback mechanisms in both the hypothalamus and the pituitary glands, where the original hormonal signals trigger its production. As in other systems, the hypothalamus begins the process by secreting corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) in response to a variety of stressors. Then, CRF triggers the anterior pituitary to release ACTH that increases the adrenal cortex secretion of cortisol. Salivary cortisol levels have been compared to serum cortisol levels in a variety of patients, founding a very reliable measurement (Aardal-Eriksson, Karlberg, & Holm, 1998). The advantages of using salivary measurements include noninvasive sample collecting anytime and anywhere without inducing cortisol/stress due to venipuncture for blood sampling. Salivary cortisol levels have been used to measure acute stress induced by consecutive parachute jumps (Deinzer, Kirschbaum, Gresele, & Hellhammer, 1996) or by a psychosocial stress test involving free speech and mental arithmetic in front of an audience for 15 minutes (Kirschbaum, Wust, & Hellhammer, 1992). DHEA/cortisol ratio has been proposed as an important marker of stress and aging. When individuals are under prolonged stress, a divergence in this ratio results, because cortisol levels continue to rise while DHEA levels decrease significantly. The effects of DHEA/cortisol imbalance can be severe and may include elevated blood sugar levels, increased bone loss, compromised immune function, decreased skin repair and regeneration, increased fat accumulation, and brain cell destruction. A significant increase in DHEA/cortisol ratio was found in volunteers who showed a significant reduction in stress, burnout, and negative emotion as a result of stress management intervention. Reduced stress diminishes the system’s cortisol demand and can result in the diversion of pregnenolone, a common precursor of DHEA and cortisol, from cortisol production into DHEA synthesis (McCraty, Barrios-Choplin, Rozman, Atkinson, & Watkins, 1998). S-IgA is produced by B-lymphocytes, a major component of the immune system, and it is viewed as the first defending line against pathogens in the upper respiratory tract, the gastrointestinal system, and the urinary tract. Salivary S-IgA levels were inversely correlated to perceived stress in emergency department nurses (Yang et al., 2002). In healthy volunteers, salivary S-IgA levels were measured before and 313

Donatella Brisinda, et al.

after experiencing the emotional states of either positive feeling of care and compassion or negative feeling of anger. Self-induced positive feeling states produced a significant increase in S-IgA levels, while 5 minutes of self-induced anger feeling produced a significant inhibition of S-IgA lasting from 1 to 5 hours after the emotional experience (Rein, Atkinson, & McCraty, 1995). Thus, salivary S-IgA level can provide information about stress-induced depression of immune defense. Stress-induced activation of mononuclear interleukin 1β (IL-1β) is a mechanism potentially linking stress with immune and endocrine status and with heart disease. Regardless of the nature of the stress, the effects of IL-1β include the stimulation of ACTH secretion, with a consequent increase in glucorticoid levels and activation of the sympathetic nervous system, followed by a release of catecholamines. Salivary IL-1β concentration is another easily measurable marker of psychological stress (Brydon et al., 2005; Ilardo et al., 2001). However, costs and organization might be the only limitations in testing the above biomarkers of stress on a large scale. Finally, the degree of individual emotional involvement is usually rated on the basis of self-reports of emotions and motivational approach (Kreibig et al., 2007; Stemmler et al., 2007).

History of Tactical Stress and Heart Rate In 1984, Massad F. Ayoob, in his book Stress Fire, described how survival stress reactions, induced by a deadly force confrontation, could affect defensive efficacy and provided fundamental suggestions for coping with stress, to control physiological symptoms, and to be efficient in an armed confrontation. Since that reading, we started to figure out how to find a way to objectively quantify individual stress in police officers exposed to acute operational pressure implying the use of force and shootings. As cardiologists, we thought the easiest way was to monitor the HR continuously; however, wearable Holter recorders were bulky at that time and very vulnerable to mechanical shock. Thus, we had to wait for the first solid-state recorders to initiate in 1990, the first pilot study evaluating the feasibility to capture the heart rates of law enforcement officers during actual force-on-force scenarios. Bruce Siddle, in Sharpening the Warrior’s Edge: The Psychology & Science of Training and in a subsequent publication (Siddle, 1995, 1999), reviewed previous research and discussed how survival stress reactions might affect the law enforcement officer when placed in a life or death situation, taking into account the neural basis of survival motor programs, motor skills classification, reaction time, and the psychology of survival training. Most important, he attempted an integration of available information about HR levels, motor skills, and associated cognitive performance, in a model correlating different levels of survival stress reaction, based on the elevation of the HR. In his model, fine motor skills (such as finger dexterity and eye–hand coordination) deteriorate above 115 beats per minute (bpm), whereas complex motor skills (i.e., ability to track and shoot a moving target, which involves eye–hand coordination, timing, and balance) begin to deteriorate when the HR reaches 145 bpm. With HR above 145 bpm, perception begins to be altered, leading to auditory exclusion and narrowing of the visual system (tunnel vision). With further increase of the HR, a progressive deterioration of cognitive control of action can occur, with a sort of functional shut-off of prefrontal cortical areas, and irrational behavior prevails when HR exceeds 175 bpm. The gross motor skill controlling the actions of the large muscle group such as the thighs, chest, back, and arms is enhanced and maximized with increasing level of stress, although excessive blood reduces flexibility and capability to run well. Siddle as well as other authors (Grossman & Christensen, 2004) suggest that, as in athletic competitions, the optimal tactical HR “zone” should be between 115 and 145 bpm. When HR raises above 175 bpm, tactical efficiency may be lost because of a progressive trend toward irrationality, favored by perceptual and memory distortion (Klinger, 2001), reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex, and prevalence of amygdala activation under uncontrolled fight-or-flight response (Sharps, 2010). Bruce Siddle’s research about the effects of stress on HR and the physiological consequences on the human body were paramount for police instructors who started to figure out that officers have to receive 314

Real-Time Assessment in Tactical Training

and practice realistic force-on-force training, because only in that way can they be prepared to deal with the real-life operational stress (Olson, 1998). However, in spite of the unquestionable merit of Siddle’s intuition and attempt to correlate the intensity of operational stress with the HR response, the significance of HR increase is still questionable. In particular, experimental evidence that the absolute value of HR achieved can be an index of individual stress and predictive of operational behavior is lacking at the moment. In 1999, Fenici et al. reported about ECG and blood pressure monitoring in six healthy adult athletes (five were police officers) to evaluate if there was indeed an elevation in the HR and blood pressure of law enforcement officers under the stress of competitive pistol shooting. Four shooters’ heartbeats reached above 180 bpm and in two cases the HR exceeded 200 bpm, for the occurrence of paroxysmal arrhythmias. The study demonstrated that under competition stress, healthy athletes had elevated heart rates but reported different subjective stress perception. The highest level of stress was achieved during “man-versus-man” shoot-offs, which better mimic a defensive situation, affecting shooting precision and the outcome of the competition. The authors recommended that further studies were needed to evaluate cardiovascular stress and the coping capabilities of the law enforcement officer in operational scenarios. However, it was also evident that the pure measure of HR increase could be misleading, if not interpreted in the context of the event dynamics. In fact, in action shooting, as in the realm of a police tactical scenario, an overlap can occur of physical effort and psychological strain, both contributing to increase HR. It is possible that the same 175 bpm might have a very different effect on behavioral appropriateness if predominantly generated by physical effort and not by psychological stress. Moreover, the results of a few studies, carried out by monitoring the HR during realistic tactical training, seem partially in disagreement with Siddle’s model and suggest the need for more sophisticated methods to quantify the individual psychological stress during police tactical tasks. At the Federal Law Enforcement Training Centers, Meyerhoff et al. (2004) found a significant elevation in HR and blood pressure during very stressful realistic scenarios simulating highly dangerous situations, created to evaluate the performance of law enforcement personnel with a protocol measuring indices of stress and impact on performance. Differences in HR responsivity were observed between officers who achieved passing scores and those who failed. However, successful officers displayed additional HR acceleration while in the passenger role during a stressful driving episode as well as during the gunfight, whereas officers who received failing scores on those elements had lower HR than the successful ones. Unfortunately, absolute HR values were not reported in this study. Another study, conducted at the Texas State University at San Marcos in connection with the Advanced Law Enforcement Rapid Response Training (ALERRT), was presented at the Annual Conference of the Society of Police and Criminal Psychology (SPCP) in 2008. The authors concluded that in the 42 investigated officers, (1) there was no significant correlation between the amount of experience and training of the officers and average or maximum HR achieved during force-on-force scenarios, (2) training doesn’t affect management aspect, and (3) there is no relationship between HR and Body Mass Index. However, they also correctly suggested that, as the HR was not measured with continuous ECG recording, some measurements could have been affected by technical limitation of the HR monitor used (Kemp & Diez, 2008). A larger prospective research project, carried out in Italy since 1992 by monitoring ECG and blood pressure of police officers undergoing realistic training in medium- to high-stress tactical scenarios, including force-on-force and gunfight with Simunitions®, has provided evidence that the resting HR and blood pressure values of the police officers tended to be significantly enhanced already before action. Their HR suddenly increased (even above 150% of the basal state) during life-threatening confrontations. However, the absolute value of HR and the percent of HR increase did not predict the tactical behavior of individual officers or the outcome of their action. In fact, about half of the officers acted properly in spite of very high HRs, whereas half of those who failed had a lower HR increase and their HR was sometime still within the “ideal combat range” (Fenici, 2008; Fenici & Brisinda, 2008b). In summary, the results of previous research could raise doubts about the information that 315

Donatella Brisinda, et al.

can be gathered with measurements of the HR when the intention is to evaluate the amount of individual stress induced by police tactical operation and/or to predict appropriate tactical behavior as a function of individual HR reaction. Moreover, when tactical tasks implied an amount of physical effort, it was difficult to distinguish to what extent the HR acceleration was due to emotional involvement or to physical activity. In order to separate these two effects, knowledge of mechanisms underlying HR regulation in different physiological and emotional conditions is required.

Heart Rate Regulation Daily life is characterized by alternation of rest and activity, requiring adjustments of the ANS to adapt cardiorespiratory function for bodily demands. The ANS controls the body’s visceral functions, including heart activity, gastrointestinal tract movement, and endocrine secretion. The normal sinus rhythm is generated by the intrinsic automaticity of the physiological pacemaker of the heart, which is largely modulated by the ANS through the interplay of sympathetic and vagal outflows (see Figure 14.2). In most physiological conditions, the efferent sympathetic and parasympathetic branches regulate HR, influencing the activity of ion channels involved in the regulation of depolarization of the cardiac pacemaker cells (Piot, Copie, Guize, Lavergne, & Le Heuzey, 1997). The sympathetic system enhances automaticity by increasing the rate of pacemaker depolarization, whereas vagal stimulation causes hyperpolarization and reduces the rate of autodepolarization, with consequent reduction of HR.

Heart Rate Variability Although the heart beating at rest was once believed to be regular, it is actually known that the sinus rhythm of a healthy heart is slightly irregular because of three major physiological originating factors: (1) quasi-oscillatory fluctuations in blood pressure control, (2) respiration, and (3) oscillations due to thermal regulation. This phenomenon is named HR variability (HRV). The dynamic modulation of HR is provided by the interaction of the sympathetic system (which has a response time in the order of a few seconds) and of parasympathetic activity (which works much faster: response time 0.2–0.6 seconds) (Berntson et al., 1997). Such continuous modulation by ANS results in HR fluctuation or variability. Whereas the measure of HR is a static index of autonomic input to the sinus node, which doesn’t provide direct information on sympathetic or parasympathetic function in a given state, HRV analysis provides a quantitative assessment of cardiac autonomic regulation (ESC and NASPE Task Force on Heart Rate Variability, 1996; Lahiri, Kannankeril, & Goldberger, 2008; Perini & Veicsteinas, 2003). Thus, HRV analysis may be a useful clinical tool to assess the dynamics of sympathovagal balance in a given situation. Even though HRV has been extensively studied during the last decades, its clinical application has reached general consensus (ESC and NASPE Task Force on Heart Rate Variability, 1996) only as a predictor of risk after myocardial infarction (Fox et al., 2007, 2008) and as an early warning sign of diabetic neuropathy (Braune & Geisenörfer, 1995; Pagani, 2000). However, HRV analysis is nowadays also increasingly used as a research tool to quantify emotional response in social and psychopathological processes, since theoretical and empirical rationale for its use as an index of individual differences in emotional response has been given by Appelhans and Luecken (2006).

Quantitative Assessment of HRV HRV is usually measured from changes in heartbeat interval, which is the reciprocal of HR. The starting point for HRV analysis is the ECG recording from which the HRV time series are extracted. The sinus interval is generally defined as the time difference between two successive P-waves. However, 316

Real-Time Assessment in Tactical Training

Rational thinking mode

Cognition Emotion

Empirical thinking mode

Amygdala Reflex

Nodose ganglion

Brain stem

Sympathetic

Extrinsic cardiac ganglia

Parasympathetic

Dorsal root ganglion

Fight or flight • •

Respiration Baroreceptors



Reninangiotensin Muscle neural activity



> HR Intrinsic cardiac sensors

< HR

HRV LF

HF

Cardiac emotional feedback

Figure 14.2 Neural interconnection between brain, cardiac autonomic innervation, and other visceral inputs to the brain stem modulating heart rate (HR) and its variability (HRV). Sympathetic (max effect in 4 s) and parasympathetic (max effect in 0.6 s) “efferent” (solid arrows) and “afferent” (dashed arrows) limbs are shown. The afferent signals from the heart reach the medulla, then travel to the subcortical and cortical areas of the brain, affecting feeling states, mental processes and emotional balance.

as the P-wave is usually a low-amplitude signal, in order to improve the accuracy of detection of the heart rate, the heartbeat period is usually evaluated as the time difference between two consecutive QRS complexes, which are signals 10 times larger in amplitude. After QRS peaks have been properly detected (with a time accuracy of 1–2 ms), the HRV time series (or tachogram) can be derived. Any technical (e.g., errors in QRS detection) or physiological (e.g., arrhythmic events) artifacts in the RR interval (i.e., the interval between two consecutive R waves) time series, which may affect HRV analysis, must be manually removed, and only artifact-free sections should be included in the analysis (ESC and NASPE Task Force on Heart Rate Variability, 1996). An example of an HRV tachogram in different physiological conditions is shown in Figure 14.3a. Quantitative assessment of HRV is performed with time-domain, frequency-domain, and nonlinear methods. For a comprehensive description of HRV analysis, the interested reader is addressed 317

Donatella Brisinda, et al. Sleep

Awake and upright

Peak exercise

(a)

(b) HF

VLF

VLF LF

LF

HF

LF

HF

Figure 14.3 HRV analysis in different physiological conditions. (a) Tachograms (time variation of RR interval). (b) Power spectral analysis (5′ intervals—ARMA model) shows a prevalence of vagal tone (HF) during sleep, and a clear-cut increase in LF power (and LF/HF ratio) occurs when awake and upright, predominantly due to an increase in direct neural stimulation of sympathetic tone. At the peak of exercise, the HRV total power decreases, with dominance of the VLF component.

to more specific literature (ESC and NASPE Task Force on Heart Rate Variability, 1996; Lahiri et al., 2008; Sztajzel, 2004; Tarvainen, Georgiadis, Ranta-aho, & Karjalainen, 2006). This chapter will mainly describe the fundamentals of frequency domain analysis, because it is the most suitable for short-time analysis and useful to study stress-induced transient fluctuations of autonomic balance potentially affecting tactical behavior of police officers. The time-domain parameters (see Table 14.1) are statistical calculations directly applied to the series of successive RR interval values. Nonlinear methods are increasingly used in clinical studies, but the physiological interpretation of their results is still difficult (Carrasco, Caitan, Gonzàlez, & Yànez, 2001; Zbilut, Thomasson, & Webber, 2002). Frequency domain analysis describes the periodic oscillations of the HR signal decomposed into different frequencies and amplitudes. It can be performed with nonparametric methods, such as the Fast Fourier Transform (FFT), which is characterized by discrete peaks for the several frequency components, or with parametric methods, such as the autoregressive model estimation (ARMA), resulting in a continuous and smoother spectrum of activity, more suitable for very shortterm HRV changes evaluation. The spectral components (see Table 14.2) are evaluated in frequency (Hertz: Hz) and amplitude, the latter assessed by the area below each component (power spectral density). Short-term (5 minutes) spectral recordings are mainly characterized by two components: the low frequency (LF: 0.04–0.15 Hz) and the high frequency (HF: 0.15–0.4 Hz). The most relevant periodic determinant of HRV is the respiratory sinus arrhythmia due to the physiological influence of breathing, which is measured by the HF component and generally believed to be of parasympathetic origin. The frequency components within the LF band are considered of both sympathetic and parasympathetic origin (Berntson et al., 1997) even though some researchers have suggested them to be mainly of sympathetic genesis (Malliani, Pagani, Lombardi, & Cerutti, 1991). Furthermore, there are also feedback mechanisms providing quick reflexes. The most relevant is the arterial baroreflex, based on specialized stretching sensors (baroreceptors) located on the walls of some 318

Real-Time Assessment in Tactical Training Table 14.1 Time-Domain HRV Parameters Variable

Units

Description

SDNN SDANN

msec msec

rMSSD

msec

SDNN index

msec

Standard deviation of all NN intervals Standard deviation of average of NN intervals in all 5-minute segments of entire recording Square root of the mean of the sum of the squares of differences between adjacent NN intervals Mean of the standard deviations of all NN intervals for all 5-minute segments of the entire recording Numbers of pairs of adjacent NN intervals differing by more than 50 msec in the entire recording NN50 count divided by the total number of all NN intervals

NN50 pNN50

%

Table 14.2 Frequency Domain HR Parameters Absolute values (expressed in ms2) • Total power = variance of all NN intervals