310 54 8MB
English Pages 227 Year 1989
Linguistische Arbeiten
207
Herausgegeben von Hans Altmann. Herbert E. Brekle, Hane Jurgen Heringer, Christian Rohrer, Heinz Vater und Otmar Werner
LiloMoessner
Early Middle English Syntax
Max Niemeyer Verlag Ti.ibingen 1989
CIP- Titelaurnahme: der Deu1i;chenBibliochek
Moessner.LIia: Early Middle English syntax/ Lilo Moessner. -Tilbingcn: Niemeyer.1989 (LinguisliS,the Arbeiten~ 207) Freibura (Brcis,au). Univ.. Habil.-Schr.• 1985
NE:GT [SBN 3484-30207•0 0 Max Niemeyer Verlag, Tllbingen 1989 Das Werk einsclllicSlic:haller seincr Teile isl urhebene(htlichgescht.ltzt.Jede Verwcnufti: au6erhalb der mgen Grenzcn des Urheberrcchlsge:setus isl ohne Zustimmungdes Verlagc~
uniulassig und srrafbar. Das gilt insbesondere rur Vervielf"lltigungen. Obetsctzungen, Mikroverfilm1mgcnund die Einipeicherung 11ndVcrarbeitung in clcktronischcn Syslemcn. Prinled in Omnany. Druck: Wtillcn.Druck GmbH, Da.nnstadl
CONTENTS
Preface
Vii
Abbrev.iations I.
ix
Introduction
1
1. Research on Middle English Syntax 2. Theoretical Framework 3. Axiomatic Functionalism 4. The Place of Syntax in Axiomatic Functionalism
1 4
11
S. Syntactic
13
6. Data Base
Relations
7. Descriptive
II.
20
Method
22
Compulsory Constituents
of Early Middle English
Clauses
24
1. The Active
Verbal Syntagm
2. The Copulative 3. The Passive
III.
IV.
v,
6
24 42
Syntagrn
Syntagm
Nominal Syntagms 1. The Substantival
47 53 53
Syntagm
2. The Adjectival
Syntagm
3. The Pronominal
Syntagm
73 78
Functional Syntagms 1. The Prepositional
84
Syntagm 2. The Conjunctional Syntagrn 3. The Comparative Syntagm 4. The Genitival Syntagm
84 86 90 94
The Predicative
98
Syntagm
1. Syntactic Properties of Predicative Syntagms 1.1. Passive vs. Active Verbal vs. Copulative 1.2. Intransitive vs. Non-intransitive 1.3. one Predicative Complement vs. more than one Predicative Complement 1.4. Transitive vs. Non-tra.nsitive
2. ICs of Predicative 2.1.
Syntagrns
Nucleus
Complements 2.2.1. Copulative Complement 2.2.2. Objects 2.2.3. Verbal Complement Syntagms 3. Types of Predicative 1. Type (1):
100 104
105 108 108
2.2.
copulat1ver
Intransitive
3. 4. Type (4):
Copulative,
Non-intransitive,
3, 5. Type (5):
Copulative, Non-intransitive, more than one Predicative
3.
98
98
3. 2. Type (2): Active verbal, lntrane1t1ve 3. 3. Type {3): Passive, Intransitive
one Predicative Complement
Complement
108 108 109 115 116 117
118 118
179 119
Vi 3. 6. Type ( 6 l : Passive,
Non-1ntrans1t1ve, one Predicative Complement
3. 7. Type ( 7 1: Passive, Non-intransitive, than one Predicative
more
Complement
3. 8. Type ( 8) : Active Verbal, Non-intransitive, one Predicative Complement, Transitive 3. 9. Type { 9) = Active Verbal, Non-intransitive, one Predicative Complement, Nontransitive 3. 10. Type ( 10): Active Verbal, Non-intransitive, more than one Predicative Complement, Transitive ( 1 1 ) ; 3.11 • Type Active Verbal, Non-intrans1tive, more than one Predicative Complement, Non-transitive
VI.
121
122 123
125 128
132
The Clause
137
1. Nucleus 2. Expansions
137 137
2.1.
2.2.
2.3.
Subject
Adverbial
137 147
Complement
Agent Complement
150
Functional Constructions 1. Infinitive Constructions 1.1. Infinitive Constructions without Subject 1.2* Infinitive Construction with Subject 2. Relative Constructions Con&tructions 3. Participial
168
VIII.
Conjunctive
172
IX.
Syntagms with Coordinated
x.
syntagms with Interord1nated ICs 1. The Passive Syntagm 2. The Syntactic Type ure iwil us is lod to lete 3. The Syntactic Type pe bet pe bet-
VII.
Appendix
Ii
Appendix II:
Constructions
ICs
'tense'
Position
'tense'
Position
Descriptive
157
161
184 184 184
186
188
1. verbs which only Combine with 'habben•
Ill:
155 155
179
Data Base
2. verbs which only Combine with 'ben' Appendix
155
Models
in
in
191 193
195
Bibliography
200
Name Index
209
Key Word Index
211
PREFACE
In the early 70s, Professor H. Pilch drew my attention to the lack of a comprehensive description of the syntactic structures of Middle English. After several years of collecting data, at a
point when I was about to be overwhelmed by the sheer mass of material, I met, by a very lucky coincidence, Professor Jan Mulder, then chairman of the Linguistics Department of St. Andrews university. He introduced me to his theory of Axiomatic Functionalism. During my repeated visits to St. Andrews, he took a lively interest in the progress of my work and spent many an hour with me on the discussion of individual problems. I owe him special thanks for his unfailing support over the years and also for letting me use a draft version of his latest book. Unfortunately, Foundations
of Axiomatic Functionalism appeared only after I had finished the typescript of my own book, so that the most recent developments of this theory could not be incorporated. A German version of my study was accepted as a Habilitationsschrlft at the University of Freiburg in 1985. The plan of translating it into English was shaping in my mind, when Professor w. D. Bald offered to recommend my study for publication - in EngArbeiten. As it is, a thorlish - to the editors of Linguistische ough revision of the original has resulted, requiring a reconsideration of already completed descriptions and the reorganisation
of the whole subject matter. I gratefully acknowledge the generosity and patience with which Max Niemeyer Verlag put up with more and more delays. The final version of my text profited from the scrutiny of Dr. Janet Harkness and Lorna Walker, who pointed out inconsistencies and made valuable suggestions concerning the finer points of English. The present format would not have been possible without the expertise of Hans-Gunther Borrm.ann of Freiburg Univer-
sity Computing Centre. I wish to express my gratitude
to all those mentioned above,
Viii
but also to many others on whose professional and personal support I was able to count from the f 1rst draft version to the final product. Needle!iiis to say, I alone am responsible for all inaccuracies, omissions and rnisto.kes which remain despite all precautions taken. May my shortcomings inspire others to do better and more, thus furthering our knowledge of the structure of Middle English syntax.
Freiburg,
July
1989
ABBREVIATIONS
act.verb,
act.verb.synt. adj. adj.synt. adv.compl. 'AF
active
verbal
active verbal syntagm adjective, adjectival adjectival syntagm adverbial complement Axiomatic f'Unctionalism, ist
agent comp!. app.suppl.
agent complement
compl. comp. compl. comp.conj. conj. conj .compl.
cop.
compl. comparative complement comparative conjunction conjunction# conjunctional conjunctional complement copula, copulative
dir.obj.
direct
EME
Early Middle English
cop.compl.
empha.s.
gen.comp!. gen ..synt. IC( s ~
intens. 1ntrans.
copulative
complement
object
Grammar of Contemporary genitival complement genitival syntagm
GCE
(~intr.)
ME mod.aux.
non-intrans.•
non-intr.
non-tr. obj.
Irmiediate intensifier
Constituent(s)
intransitive Middle English modal auxiliary non-intransitive non-transitive object
OE
Old English
part. part.compl.
participle, participial
part.past (=p.p.} part.pres.
functional-
appos1t1ve supplement
emphasizer Extended Predicative-Based
EPBS(s)
axiomatic
participial complement
past participle present participle
Syntagm(s)
English
X
pass. pass.cop. PBS{s) PDE
pred. pred.nucl. pred.synt.
prep. prep.obj. prep.suppl. prep.synt. pron. pron.synt. relat.
rel. comp!.
passive
passive copula Predicative-Based
prepositional prepositional
relativizer
relative
relative relative
subj.
subst.
subject substantive,
suppl.
supplement
tr ans • ( =tr • )
transitive
verbal compl.= verb.comp!.
object supplement
prepostional syntagm pronoun, pronominal pronominal syntagrn
rel.constr. rel.suppl.
subst.synt.
Syntagm(s)
Present-Day English p.redicate, predicative predicative nucleus predicative syntagm preposition, prepositional
complement
construction supplement
substantival
verbal
substantival
syntagrn
complement
I. Itfl'RODUCTION
1. Research
on ME Syntax
ME syntax is usually guistics. The designation
considered 'historical'
a branch of Historical Linis ambiguous here. It may
refer to a previous stage of a language or to its development in time. In the latter sense it is synonymous with 'diachronic', and this is the prevalling approach of traditional linguistics towards stages of a language. Traditional Historical Linguistics earlier was mainly concerned with the development of the distinctive elements of languages. Therefore many so-called historical grammars are nothing :but diachronic phonological studies. Under these conditions it is not surprising that most ME grammars and course-books do not contain a chapter on syntax at all (Bihr, Berndt, Brunner, Fisiak 1964, Fisiak 1968, Jordan, wardale, Weinstock, Wright). Chapter 14 of M. Roseborough's grammar (1938) is entitled "Accidence:Syntax", and there we find scat-
tered remarks about the obligatory ( sic 1 • status of the subject in ME, the frequency of impersonal constructions, special features of number concord, etc. Hosse apart, we have to wait until the 1970s before more extensive and systematic treatments of ME syntax appear (Dilrmilller/Utz 1974, Jones 1972, Moessner/Schaefer 1974; 21987). But they, too, are rather specialized, covering primarily synt.actic structures attested in the texts which these books include. Individual problems of MEsyntax were investigated in detail at the beginning of our century (BOhrne 1903:Temporal especially Clauses; v.d.Gaaf 1904:Impersonal Constructions; Gebhardt 1922: Subject Omission). More recent studies deal with wordorder (Harris 1964; McLeish 1969~ Swieczkowski 1962), pronominal syntax (Rennhard 196 2; Suter 1955) , relative clauses {Ki v imaa 1966 ) , modal verbs (Wallum 1973), and the syntactic functions of the elements
2
~,also,~
(Nummenmaa 1973) and~ (Rissanen 1967). Most syntactic descriptions of individual ME texts are also outdated. Only very few texts have been investigated and described
syntactically with modern linguistic methods: •Ancrene Riwle • (Humbert 1944), •ormulwnt (Palmatier 1969), 'Peterborough Chronicle' (Shores 1971), 'Cursor Mundi' ~Snouffer 1966). Due to their outstanding literary quality Chaucer' s works have also received special attention from linguists {a.o+ Fries 1985: Kerk.hot 21902; Sandved 1985).
one book only promises a survey of the whole field syntax:
Hustanoja'
checks
s
A Hi.ddle
any rash expectation,
"It
[=the book] deals
difficult otherwise."
to
English
toning
Syntax.
But
the
of ME preface
down the promise of the title,
only with constructions
the literary student {Mustanoja 1960:5)
or
of
likely
particular
to prove interest
The Table of Contents
shows that Mustanoja's approach is traditional, and rather morphological than syntactic. His subject matter is arranged according to the traditional word-classes: substantive1
tion,
verb.
pronoun,
article,
Syntactic
numeral,
adjective,
constructions
are treated
adv~rb,
preposi-
under morphologi-
cal categories, e.g. the group genitive under 'case', verbal tag1ns of the type mai curnen under the heading 'moo~', etc. explains why we look in vain for a chapter on clause types or plex sentence constructions. The second volume, which will
synThis comdeal
with these, has unfortunately not yet appeared+ The most exhaustive· diachronic treatment of English syntax is without doubt Visser•s monumenta.l work An Historical Syntax of the English Language. One of the most valuable aspects of the book is the vast anount of data which it provides. Many authors of recent studies on historical English syntax gratefully acknowledge their indebtedness to this inexhaustible data source (Traugott 1972; Faill 1977). Unfortunately it takes quite a bit of effort to
find the data relevant ment of his material
construction
contains.
to a given topic,
because Visser's
arrange-
is based on the number of verb forms which a
Thus the first
part of the book deals with
"syntactical units with one verb", the second part with "syntactical units with two verbs" and the third with "syntactical units with two and more verbs''. The author freely admits the npurely
3
formal and rnechan1stic character of this method. It has the disconcerting effect that syntactically similar constructions like he is rich and you are being naughty are not treated together, but 11
in different sections of the book (§241:§1834), whilst syntactically very different constructions like the old woman knew well to babble and dol my dear love, write to me are treated in the same volwue (§§1185, 1428), because both contain two verb forms (knew, babble; do, write) the second of which is an infinitive.
diachronic of different
study presupposes a series of synchronic studies stages of the language under consideration. Visser describes the ideal working conditions for a diachronic grammarian as follows: Any
11
Ideally
his
speaking,
disposal
the
a complete
scriptions of the state another. ·• (Visser I: v)
historical
granmarian
should
have
series of exhaustive synchronic of the language from one generation
at
deto
He is well aware that we are still far from this goal with regard to English. An exhaustive description of the syntactic structure
of PDE is provided in A Grammarof Contemporary English and A Comprehensive
Grammar of Contemporary
English.
OE at
the
other
end
of the scale has also received increased attention. Pilch included a chapter on syntax in his Al tengllsche Grammatik ( 1970) , McLaughlin published a booklet with the title Old English Synt4X. A Handbook t1983), and 1985 saw the publication of what will remain the standard reference work on the subject for many years to come, Bruce Mitchell's Old English Syntax. However, apart from Mustano-
ja's book there is a huge gap in the description of English syntax between OE on the one 4nd PDE on the other hand. As it
is a well-known
fact
that
every synchronic
description
implies a certain amount of diaehrony - the more so when a historical state of a language is concerned - it was necessary to decide remark was my which time span should be covered. Barbara Strang's guide-line here: ..Some developments
will
not
be clearly
established
within
a
century. Doubling this span gives a meaningful stretch of time, but one not too complex to account for in a reasonably unified way." (Strang 1974:21£.) Thus I restricted
my
investigation
to texts
of the 12th and 13th
4
centuries,
the period
usually
referred
to as Early Middle English
(DIE)•
2. Theoretical
Framework
Language data can be interpreted in a variety of ways by different scientific disciplines, e.g. history, literary criticism, psychology, sociology, a.nd last but not least linguistics. We do not expect that such diverse investigations will yield the same results, al though they may be based on the same data. Each discipline is interested in different aspects of the same evidence; different $olutions are sought to different problems. These solutions have the status of scientific hypotheses, which can be corroborated or falsified by the data. These hypotheses are necessarily different because of the different underlying theories. Linguistic descriptions presuppose a linguistic theory. As competitive linguistic theories, a careful there are several choice must be made, because the quality of a description depends partly upon the quality of the theory behind it. When choosing a theory, two considerations are especially important. Does it meet with the epistemological requirements of consistency, adequacy and simplicity and, is it appropriate for the set of objects which we want to describe? If a theory allows descriptive statements of the form "a is identical with b" and "a is not identical with b", it has to be rejected because it is inconsistent. If a theory excludes the description of a subset of our data, it has to be rejected because it is not adequate for our purpose. If two competitive theories rank equally high in consistency and adequacy, the simpler theory is to be preferred. of course, it is possible to modify theories .. The temptation to do so is particularly great when they lack adequacy. Modifications of one part of a theory may, however, entail undesirable consequences for other parts, consequences which may
easily be overlooked. In extreme cases such modifications sult
in a completely
useless
theory:
"Many, however, if not most are too imptttient
proper
experience,
may re-
or are not self-critical
to acquire the enough to see that
s their skill as a descriptivist is still underdeveloped. The latter ones are the most dangerous, as it 1s they that ultimately may cause the decline and downf al 1 of the theory in question. When they come across a problem which they are unable to solve, or they notice inconsistencies in their descriptions, their inclination is to blame it on the theory, rather than on their own incompetence. Then they start tinkering with the theory - but someone who is not even a good descriptivist can hardly be expected to be a good theory-builder. Even less than those persons are able to oversee all the consequences of their decisions, are they capable of foreseeing the dedescriptive scriptive consequences of changes in, or additions to, the theory. This is how theories usually die." (Mulder 1988:124 [typescript]) are Mulder touches upon a basic problem here. Very few linguists as well. Linguists with a theoretical bias trained epistemologists very often lack experience in handling empirical data. Descript1claim a theory's supev1sts on the other hand all too willingly riority over other theories withou~ attempting to demonstrate this 1 Others simply state that for their descripalleged superiority. tive purposes no linguistic theory is available, thus justifying a highly eclectic combination of different theories:
"With regard to the present study then, the conclusion 1s that no descriptive framework currently available could adequately accomodate the different grammatical aspects which appear to be crucial features of the grammar of subject less predicates. " (Elmer 1981:11}
Such eclecticism reflects yet another predicament. One theory may be perfectly adequate for the description of one set of linguistic data or for one structural level, but rather clumsy for the description of another set of data or another structural level, whereas the reverse may be true for a second theory. Which theory is then better? These considerations show how dangerous it is at the present stage of development in linguistics to make statements about the of one linguistic theory over another, let alone over superiority all others. This does not mean that descriptiv1sts are right in adopting one theory for one purpose and a different theory for an1cf. Fowler, R. An Introduction to Transformational Syntax, London 1971, p.viiz "its [=TG's] general framework has become
accepted by the majority of western linguists most reliable and revealing version of linguistic
as providing analysis. 1•
the
6
other,
or even combining
theories
at will.
In this
way we will
never arrive at a consistent description of all structural levels of one or more languages or of one structural level at different of one or more langua9es. It is quite legitimate that descrlptivlsts should choose a theory which they consider appropriate for their purpose. As long as they stick to one consistent theory, it is likely that they will produce a consistent description. Such descriptions may be challenged by others, on the basis of different theories. If they periods
are more adequate and/or simpler, they are to be preferred. But even then this would not mean that the first theory must be discarded, because it might still prove better at the end of the day when all structural levels of one or more languages have been described. It is therefore wlth no claim as to the superiority of the theory of A:xiomatlc Functionalism (AF) that I chose this the-
although I believe that this theory meets the epistemological requirements mentioned above and allows a consistent, adequate and relatively simple description of EMEsyntax. If there are shortcomings in the description which must be blamed on the theory, competing descriptions based on other theories will certainly be put forward. Even if these are ory
as
the
bettel:",
it
framework
must
still
of my description,
be proved
that
their
underlying
theories
also allow better descriptions of other sets of linguistic data and/or other structural levels. To help in the evaluation of the following description I shall outline the structure of M, concentrating on those parts of the theory which are relevant to syntax.
3. Axiomatic Functionalism theory which was developed by Jan Mulder, now professor emeritus of the University of St. Andrews, Scotland. It is in the tradition of two well-known European linguistic schools: Prague School Linguistics and GlosseAxiomatic
matics.
Functionalism
is a linguistic
Mulder acknowledges his indebtedness
to both schools ex-
plicitly1
The theory of functionalism has always struck me as extremely sound in pr 1nc1ple, scoring higher than any other school of 11
7
thought
i.n those points [==consistency, adequacy, simplicity]. may, perhaps, be Hjelmlev's theory of Glossematics, which is very well organised, very explicit, reasonably simple, and agreeably consistent." (Mulder 1980:28)
An exception
was mainly
It
Prague
School
through
person
influenced
Linguistics
after which Martinet's municative function;
the
linguistic
of
Jan
approach
Andr~ Martinet that Mulder. 2 The function
was named is the com-
essentiel le de cet instrument qu • est de eommun1cat1on... (Martinet 1980:§1.4).
une langue
"La f onct ion
est eelle
other
Although functionalists are well aware of the existence of functions of language as well, they consider them to be of
secondary importance.~
funct1onal1st
linguistic
description
con-
tains only those elements which are relevant for the act of communication. The functional aspect is thus the point of view from which the object of l1nguistics is investigated. This object itself 1s defined as "those conununication systems that have a vocal character and ar-e in general use among all hwna.n communities"'
(Martinet
1975:12).
Hjelmslev 1 s influence on AF can be seen in the terminology: The Ceneme, plereme, etc. have been taken. ove.r from Glossematics.
Glossematic relations stellation
correspond
ordination and similarities is thodology makes entation of AF AF requirement
of interdependency, determination and conto the AF relatlons of interord1nat1on, .sub-
coordination. More important than terminological the adoption of Glossematic methodology. This mepossible a high degree of stringency in the prestheory in the form of an algebraic calculus.3 The that a theory must be arbitrary and appropriate
also goes back to Hjelmslev•s Glossemat1cs. 2naoman Jakob$on und Andre Martinet kann man in der internationalen Sprachwissenschaft als die be1den bedeutendsten Erben der Prager Schule ansehen. Martinet betont ausdrilcklich die 1 funk-
tionale'
Orientierung
se1ner
eigenen
vorstellungen."
(Lepschy
1969: 85) {In international 1 inguistics Roman Jakobson and Andre Martinet can be called the most important heirs of Prague School
Linguistics. Andre Martinet explicitly point of view of his own approach.) 3cf.
Mulder 1980:chapt.4
stresses
the
1
functiona1•
8
In his theory of AF Mulder distinguishes a signum-theory (origin.ally called theories:
three parts
or sub-
sign-theory),
which status of linguistic objectsJ a system, which covers phonology and grammar; a
defines the ontological theory ( or systemology) theory of semantics. 4 Phonology is phonotactics, and paraphonotact1cs,
(:morphology),
subdivided into phonematics, grammar into pleremat1cs (-syntax), and paraplerotactics (=pa-
plerotactics
rasyntax). This means that in the present context only the systemtheory is relevant. Before
describing
the
structure
of the theory I would like to stress a feature which makes AF unique among the various kinds of present-day linguistics. I am referring to the rigorous separThis may seem trivial, but none ation of theory and description. of the other current approaches to linguistics seem to bother about such trivialities. Consequently their descriptions cannot be properly evaluated, because the criteria on which an evaluation could be based are not specified. Descriptions which are based on against the theory and against the theory of AF can be evaluated the data, because they are viewed as "the application of a particular linguistic theory to a selected field of linguistic phenom-
ena11 (Mulder 1980:17) .. The theory of AF contains terms and statements. The statements are of three different kinds. AXioms, that kind of statements which gave AF its name, are neither true nor false; by definition they are not subject to refutation. They have to be accepted as starting-points of deductive theories, cf. The initial justification of these statements is that they seem reasonable and acceptable to others, and their further justification is that they are assumed, in the absence of refutation, to be appropriate ... (Mulder 1980:23) •
1
Axioms imply theorems, the second kind of statements. The terms which are used in axioms are variables, i.e. terms which have no meanings at all at the outset, but which receive meanings through definitions, the third kind of statements. Then they a.re no longer variables, but defined terms. While attributing meanings to var14This part of the theory was developed by Mulder's collegue Sandor Hervey, cf. Hervey, Sandor. Axiomatic Semantics, Edinburgh 1979.
.
9
ables, definitions may introduce new variables into the theory. The process of defining goes on until all variables have become defined terms. Without circularity this is only possible in a theory which also contains primitive terms, i.e. terms whlch need not be defined, because they are understood intuitively. AF may indeed find a weak point of the theory here. of primitive terms as a third kind of terms {beside
Critics
of
The inclusion
defined terms and variables) introduces a certain amount of subjectivity into the theory. Mulder is aware of this problem, and that is why he makea every effort to keep the subj ect1 ve, and there£ ore intuitive, component of his theory as small as possible, cf.
"By pushing this intuitive interpretation as far back as possible, we are removing much of its harmfulness. • (Mulder 1980: 1
25).
Nevertheless he considers a subjective theory which is not circular:
residue unavoidable
in any
"We may still take this state of affairs to be, scientifically speaking, deplorable, but we have to live with it. our system of definitions will either be circular, or it will contain this intuitive residue of primitive terms, just as a theory that does not contain primitive, i.e. intuitive statements, namely the axioms, will always be a circular theory.n ,Mulder 1980125) The structure of AF as sketched lowing diagram:
above is represented
in the fol-
AF
terms---------
/
primitive
terms
""
-----statements
/
~
defined terms axioms tneorems variables f- - - - meanings - - -.clef initions
Some definitions assign meanings to variables which denote processes, others to variables which denote entities. The latter correspond to descriptive entities which stand in a relation of isomorphism to classes of speech phenomena. The descriptive entities are called models; the corresponding theoretical terms are called theoretical models or meta-models.
10
Like any scientific theory AF lays claim to the attributes consistency, adequacy, and simplicity. Being hypotheses, these requirements cannot be part of the theory itself. They have the status of meta-hypotheses because they are hypotheses about the theory. Descriptions which are based on the theory of AF neither contain axioms nor definitions. The statements they contain have the status of scientific hypotheses. One of their most important properties is that they must be refutable by the speech phenomena they are meant to describe. Descriptive statements are about descriptive models1 their constituents, and their relations. They, too, must meet the requirements of consistency, adequacy, and simplicity. Descriptions are inconsistent not only when their hypotheses contradict each other, but also when they are not interrelated. Inconsistency of a description may imply inadequacy, because speech phenomena cannot adequately be described by contradictory
hypotheses.
Irrespective
of
other
qualities,
hypotheses
have to be rejected or at least mod1f 1ed if they are refuted by the speech phenomena. Hypotheses about the grammatical structure of the data must also be checked against their meaning, et.
"grammatical
know to
be
statements
. . . the
.•• have to be consistent
meaning
of
the
construction
with what we in question"
(Mulder 1980:13). Some statements of descriptions resemble the definitions in the theory. This similarity is only superficial; it does not mean that these descriptive statements are definitions. They introduce useful mnemonic labels (e.g. 'subject', 'transitive', 'prepositional syntAgm•, etc.) for language-specific constructions.
The methodology used in AF can be characterized as hypothetico-deductive. AF descriptions are deductive insofar as their statements are derived from the theory (of AF) on the one, and from the data on the other hand. They are hypothetical because their statements are presented as hypotheses. It is especially the deductive component of the methodology He rejects any kind of speculawhich Mulder considers important. tion and inductivism, speculation being non-scientific and inductivism leading to circularity. A.s inductiv1sm exclusively relies on the observation of data, its statements - however general and
11
abstract they may be - must remain part of the description. only by an arbitrary act can they be made statements of the theory. Such a theory is useless because its application to a field of
speech phenomena yields exactly the descriptive statements which the theory started with.5 also be based on a deThe underlying theory must therefore ductive methodology. It follows that it will contain a limited set of primitive terms and statements from which all other statements can be derived by logical operations. It is independent of any specific language, but it provides the framework for the description of any set of language data.
4. The Place of Syntax in Axiomatic Functionalism The structure of the system-theory of AF is based on Martinet's tenet of the double articulation of language. That part which deals with the distinctive elements is called phonology, the other part, which deals with the significative elements is grammar. For Martinet it is this double articulation which accounts for the economy and the efficiency of language. Mulder,
however, argues that these qualities also depend on the fact that on both levels the arrangement of the elements may be relevant, i.e. they ma.y stand in ordering relations. The English word -sick, for example, can be analysed on the second level of articulation as a sequence of the phonemes /s/, Ii/,. /k/. That the arrangement of these phonemes is relevant can be dem,onstrated by reversing their order. The language sign~ (/kts/J has a very different meaning from the sign sick (/s1k/). On the first level of articulation the relevance of the arrangement of elements can be demonstrated by the English syntagrn dog show. The permutation of its constituents yields a syntagm with a completely different meaning, show dog. The possibility of ordering relations leads Mulder to a redefinition of the principle of the double articulation of language: '
5cf.
Mulder 1980:9f.
12
"As, however, the possibility
of ordered combinations, as opi.e. unordered, ones, exhibits the ultimate level of efficiency, I have defined articulation so as to refer to syntax and phonotactics only ••, posed to the possibility
tMulder
of only simultaneous,
1980~147)
Consequently, both the phonology and the grammar part of AF contain a component which deals with the respective elements as simultaneous bundles of constituents, and c1 component which deals with the combination of the respective elements into bigger units. Phonematics treat~ phonemes as simultaneous bundles of distinctive features, phonotactics deals with their combination into phonotagms (=ordered sets of phonemes}. Pleremat1cs is a.bout plerernes as simultaneous bundles of monemes, 6 plerotactics is concerned with the combination of pleremes into syntagrns. 7 The notions 'plerematics' and 'plerotactics' roughly correspond to the traditional terms 'morphology' and 'syntax'. As a matter of fact Mulder himself very often uses the traditional terminology, althouqh his definition excludes for example the treatment of compounds in morphology/plerematics. I am going to use the traditional term 'syntax' with the definition given for 'plerotactics'. The relation between the • -atics • and the •-tactics' components is that the biggest entities of the first components are at the same time the smallest entities of the second components. Phonemes are on the border-line between phonemat1es and phonotactlcs, pleremes on that between plerematics and plerotactics. Phonology and grammar contain a third component, the "para• component. Paraphonotactics accounts for the co-existence of phonotagms and suprasegmental features, paraplerotactics (or parasyntax) for the co-existence of syntagms and suprasegmental features .. The relation
between
the
'tactic'
and
the
1
paratactic
1
levels is different
from that between the 'atic" and the 'tactic' because the output of the 'tactic' levels, phonotagms and
levels syntagms,
furnishes
only part
of the input of the paratactic
lev-
61n functionalist terminology monemes are the smallest significative elements, cf. Martinet 1960:§1.9. 70ne would expect the combinations of pleremes to be called plerotagms to keep up the terminological parallelism between phonology
and grammar.
not necessary.
Mulder
avoids
coining
new terms
when it
is
13
els. The place of syntax in the system-theory in the following diagram:
of AF is represented
system-theory phonology/ phonLcs
~rammar
phitactics
plerema!ics lple~ctics paraplerotactics
paraphonotactics
5. Syntactic
{ syntax)
Relations
In AF syntagms are analysed into their immediate constituents (=ICs) step by step. The relations which hold between !Cs are tactic relations. 8 The syntagm many very old boo.ks contains the ICs many, very old and books. They stand in tactic relations to each other. The IC books can be substituted by elements like houses,
hats,
syntactic
dogs,
structure
etc. of
These the
substitutions
syntagm.
do not The substitutable
change
the
elements
stand in the paradigmatic relation of cornmutation.9 The descriptive models of AF are defined by their ICs and the relations which hold hE!tween them. Syntactic relations are therefore very important terms of the syntactic component of the theory. relations. Syntactic relations are a subset of grammatical Some of them hold between elements of the same syntagm (=syntagmatic relations), others between elements of different syntagms (=paradigmatic relations). The only relevant paradigmatic relation for is commutation. It holds between ICs which are substitutable each other in a syntagm. In the syntagm he killed the lion the IC he commutes with syntagms like my grandfather, Charles~ or the
extravagant
fellow who told everybod~ he was a vegetarian.
Syntagmatic syntactic relations are either tactic or nontactic, either direct or indirect. Tactic relations hold between 8cf.
Mulder 1980:45,
def.7c3
9cf.
Mulder 1980:44,
de£.
7a2
14
ICs; syntagmatic relations are non-tactic.
Direct
between constituents
relations
that are not ICs
are those which are not med.iated
by other constituents .. 1O The features tact 1c or non-tactic and direct or indirect are properties of all syntagmatic relations. Therefore syntagmatic relations can be tactic and direct, tacti.c
and indirect, The following
non-tactic and direct, or non-tactic diagram maps the system of syntactic
syntactic
relations
" / " \ I\ syntagmatic
paradi~
direct
commutation
and indirect. relations:
I
tactic
non-tactic
indirect
tactic
non-tactic
In the syntagm man~ very old books direct tactic relations hold between many and books, very old and books, verr and old. There is one direct, non-tactic relation, namely between old and books. It 19 direct because 1t is not established via another constituent, and it is non-tactic because the constituents old and books are not ICs of the same syntagm; books is an IC of the syntagm many very old books, old is not, whereas old is an IC of the syntagm very old to which books does not belong at all. The relation between many and very old is indirect and tactic~ many and very old are ICs of the syntagm many very old books, their relation is mediated by the constituent books. Indirect, non-tactic relations hold between man:k: and old, man~ and very, very and books. In the first two examples the relation is via books, in the last via old. The constituents are not ICs of the same syntagm. It is quite obvious that indirect, non-tactic relations are irrelevant for syntactic analysis. The structure of a syntagm is best revealed by its tactie relations, especially by its direct tactic relations. They are either symmetrical or asymm.etrical. Symmetrical relations are called relations of simultaneity, 11 asymmetrical ones ordering relations. Subordination and superor10cf. 11cf.
Mulder 1980:52,
def,15
Mulder 1980:44,
defs.
6a, 6b.
15
dinat!on
are asymmetrical,
coordination
and interordination symmetrical relations. Subordination holds between ICs of unilateral functional dependency, i.e. the function of one IC is dependent on that of the other, but not vice ve.rsa. 12 The functionally dependent IC is subordinated to the functionally independent IC. This implies, of course, that the functionally independent IC is superordinated to the dependent IC. Usually the relation is stated in term.s of subordination. In the syntagm the books the definite article the is subordinated to the substantive books. The function of books is independent of that of the definite article; the function of the definite article, however, depends on the function of books. In the environment ••• are expensive the substantive books has the function of a subject whether it is accompanied by the definite article or not, cf. the books are expensive, books are expensive. The function of the can be described as subject modi-
fier. In the environment he bought ... the substantive books has the function of an object whether it is accompanied by~ or not, cf. he bought books~ he bought the books. The function of the definite article can be described as object modifier in this context. terms
In AF the function of the ICs of a syntagm is described in of positions in a descriptive model. Functionally independ-
ent rcs are said to stand 1n nuclear position, they are called dependent ICs stand in peripheral posi'nucle1·. 13 Functionally tions. If they commute with zero, they are called •expansions•, 14 In otherwise they are called 'bound entities' or 'actualisers•. the syntagm the books the definite article is an expansion, books
is the nucleus. In AF the relation subordination is symbolised by an arrow with its head pointing towards the nucleus, e.g. the-> books Interordination is a symmetrical relation between ICs which
mutually depend on each other for their 12cf. Mulder 1980!48,
def.
11a
13cf.
Mulder 1980:50,
def.
73a
14cf. Mulder 1980:50,
defs.
15cf. Mulder 1980;49,
def.
13b-d 11c
function. 15 In the syntagm
16
the more the better the ICs the more and the better are interordinated. The function of the more depends on the function of the better and vice versa. Syntagms with ICs i.n interordination are also known as correlative constructions. The relation of interor-
dination
is symbolised by a double-headed
arrrow,
e.g.
the more the better As none of the ICs is functionally independent, both occupy aperipheral position. The relation of coordination holds between ICs which are functionally independent of each other.16 In the syntagm old expensive books both adjectives modify the nucleus books. The func-
tion of old does not depend on the function of expensive, and vice versa. Therefore old and expensive are coordinated, The relation of coordination is symbolized by a crossed double-headed arrow, e.g. The terms
old subordination
expensive and coordination
in particular
are
'1iide-spread in modern linguistics. Therefore it is important to stress that in AF these notions are unambiguously defined and consistently applied. Their meanings do not necessarily coincide with the meanings which other linguistic schools give them. A striking example of possible terminological confusion and
of inadequate
and inconsistent
description
at the same time is the
analysis of syntagms like boys and girls as coordinated constructions. The ICs of the syntagm are said to be boys, and, girls with of coordination existing between the ICs boys and the relation gJ.rls. This relation is established by the so-called coordinator and. This analys1s is not only inadequate, but also inconsistent with the underlying definition of coordination which requires that have the same distribution the ICs of a coordinated construction 17 as the construction itself. The IC and has certainly not the
16cf. 1 7ef.
Mulder 1980:48,
def.llb
Bloomfield, L. Language, New York 1933, repr. London 1969, chapt. 12. 1O: "Endocentr 1c constructions are of two kinds, co-ordinative ( ••• ) and subordinative (.T.). In the former type the resultant phrase belongs to the same form-class as two or more of the constituents. Thus, the phrase boys and ~lrls belongs to the same form-class as the constituent~, boys, girls; these con-
17
same distribution as boys and girls. Two considerations are important for the analysis of the Firstly, the relation between syntagm according to AF principles. bo~s and girls is not direct, but via and. Secondly, boys has the same distribution as the whole syntagrn~ the ICs of the syntagm are therefore boys and and girls. 18 This means that the relation between bo~s and girls is not tactic either. The function of the IC boys is independent of that of and girls, but not vice versa. The direct, tactic relation between the ICs of the syntagm boys and girls is therefore not coordination, but subordination with boys as nucleus and and girls as expansion. All linguists will probably agree that the ICs the and books of the syntagm the books stand in a relation of subordination. holds between the ICs the and According to AF the same relation book of the syntagm the book. In both syntagms there is unilateral functional dependency with the substantive in nuclear and the definite article in peripheral position. If syntactic relations are 19 the analysis of the defined, however, via paradigmatic classes, singular ayntagm as a subordination construction is no longer justified. Whereas the elements books and the books belong to the same distributionclass, book and the book do not, cf. the book.a
are expensive, books;
the
books are expensive;
book is
expens1 ve,
I
bought the books,
1rbook is
expens1 ve;
I
bought bought the I
book~ ~I bought book. Bloomfield himself was aware of this
prob-
sti tuents are the members of the co-ordination, and the other constituent is the co-ordinator.~ Lyons, J. Introductlon to Theoretical Linguistics, Cambridge 1971, chapt. 6. 4. 2. : "Endocentric constructions fall into main types: co-ordinating and subordinating. Co-ordinating constructions have the same distribution as each of their constituents taken sepa-
rately."
Pilch, H. Altengli:;c;he Gca11111U!tik,Mlinchen 1970, §39.3; "GehOren a, b und ab alle drei 2u einer gegebenen paradigrna.tischen Klasse, io ne113t ale Relation zwischen ihnen eine Koord1nat1on." [If a, b, and ab belong to the same paradigmatic class, the relation between tfiem is called coordination.) 18At first sight the grouping boys and and pirls seems equally adequate. For arguments against this analysis c . Mulder 1980!168.
19cf.
Bloomfield
1933!cha.pt.12.10: In subordinative endocentr1c constructions, the resultant phrase belongs to the same form-class as one of the constituents, which we call the head ... 11
18
lem, but he dismissed
it as "minoru.20 His followers
do not discuss it at all, but choose their examples with great cAre so that problems do not arise. In AF problems of this kind cannot occur because synta.ctlc relations are not defined in terms of occurrence dependency, but in terms of functional dependency. Occurrence dependency provides, however, a useful test in syntactic analysis, because there are regular correspondences between certain types of occurrence dependency and certain types of
functional
dependency. As with functional
of occurrence
dependency three
types
dependency
can be distinguished: bilateral occurbilateral occurrence dependency, and unilater-
rence independency, al occurrence dependency. 21 Uni.lateral occurrence dependency is exemplified by the ICs of the syntagm the books; the occurrence of books is independent of the occurrence of the definite article, 22 but not vice versa. Bilateral occurrence dependency holds between the ICs of the syntagm the book~ neither of them has the same distribution as the whole syntagm.23 The ICs the more and !h!:.
better of the syntagm the more the better stand also ln a relation of mutual occurrence dependency; neither of them can occur in the same environment as the whole syntagm. In the syntagm old expensive books the ICs old and expensive are mutually occurrence-independent. They belong to the same distribution class as the IC old expensive of the syntagm olq e>g2ensive books. It follows that there is a one-to-one relation between bilateral occurrence independency and coord1nat1on, and a one-to-one relation between uni11 20cf. Bloomfield 1933:chapt.12.10: There may be minor differences of form-class between the resultant phrase and the members... 21 cf. Mulder 1980:49£., defs. 12 a-c
fining for
22For Bloomfield unilateral criterion for the relation
23 silateral occurrence Bloomfield• s exocentric
belong to a form-class stance,
John
ran
is
occurrence dependency is the desubordination, cf. fn. 19.
dependency is the defining relation:
..The resultant
criterion
other than that of any constituent.
neither
a nominative
express.ion
phrase
( like
may
For inJohn)
nor a finite verb expression ( like ran). Therefore we say thattne English actor-action construction is exocentric: the resultant phrase belongs to the form-class of no immediate constituent. (Bloomfield 1933:chapt.12.10) ently applied in descriptions
That this definition is not consistwas shown before, cf. p.19£.
It
19
lateral occurrence dependency and subordination. If two ICs are mutually occurrence-dependent, the relation between them is either subordination or interordinatlon. In the syntagrn the book there 1s mutual occurrence dependency. The statement that the definite article is subordinated to the 5ubstantive is based on the hypothesis that the relation between the and book is the same as that between the and books, which is a clear case of subordination. This hypothesis is valid until it is refuted. one nucleus can be expanded by more than one peripheral element. The direct tactic relations in such syntagms are complex. TWOcomplex relations are to be distinguished: disjunctive or diverse determination and conjunctive or parallel determination. The complex relation in a subordinative syntagm with more than one peripheral element is called diverse if the peripheral elements determine the nucleus 1n significantly different ways.24 In the syntagm the hunter killed the lion the subject the hunter and the object the lion determine the verb1 and they do so in significantways, cf. the hunter killed the lion i the lion killly different ed the hwiter. The complex relation in this syntagm is therefore diverse determination. If a nucleus c 1s expanded by the peripheral elements! and~ and the complex relation between these ICs
is diverse determination,
it 1s symbolized as
H->c If the elements a and b determine a nucleus c and it ascertained
that
comple.x relation
they do so in significantly is called
parallel
determination.
different 25 It
cannot be way$,
the
is symbol-
ized as
In the syntagm many very old books the nucleus books is expanded by manyand by very old. It may be - and indeed it is very proba24cf.
Mulder 1980:50,
25ct.
Mulder 1980:50!.,
def. def.
14a 14b
20
ble - that the relation between many and books is not exactly the same as that between very old and books1 but the difference cannot
be established on functional grounds~ Therefore the hypothesis is that the complex tactic relation in the syntagm many very old books is parallel
many and very old
determination. The expansions peripheral positions, cf.
are in different
many}very old If two elements
exclude
> books
each other
as determiners
of the same nu-
they realize the same position, if not they realize different positions. The plereme many excludes the plereme several as a determiner of books; they realize the same position, which JM.Ybe called 'quantifier'. On the other hand, many does not exclude elements like his or my uncle"s, cf. his many very old books, !!!l uncle's many very old books. Therefore his and my uncle's (which cleus,
exclude each other) realize a different peripheral position from man~. The only possibility for two syntagms not to exclude each other and yet to be in the same position is if they are coordinated, e.g. many very old extremely expensive books. The syntagms very old and extremely expensive are in the same position because they are coordinated, cf.
(very->
old)
(extremely-->
man}-
expen$ive)
> books
6. Data Base
Early Middle English Syntax implies that the descriptive models cover the syntactic structure of all EMEtexts which have come down to usf even of those which are not yet known, but may be discovered in the future. The material adequacy of this claim may be tested by applying the models to randomly chosen syntagms from randomly chosen texts of the period. The 11\0dels, howeThe title
ver, do not presuppose an exhaustive analysis of all extant texts.
21
a living lanbeing that the amount of available guage are, the only difference data against which descriptive models can be checked 1s limited in a dead language, whereas it is unlimited 1n a living language. Prior Mtalysis of all data is not only unnecessary, but it would also be very time-conswning. In his Manual of the Writings in Middle English Wells lists more than 80 texts of the EME period. They a.re generative
to
the
same degree
as models
of
I chose a little more than one third of these (33 texts) as the data base for my study. This main body of data is supplemented by references
from ME dictionaries
and relevant
special
studies.
Heterogeneity was my main concern in the selection of the texts. They vary in literary genre, dialect, size, source and form. The data base includes romances (e.g. King Hom, Havelok) and chronicles (e.g. Peterborou9h Chronicle), sermons (e.g. Old Kentlsh Sermons) and debates (e.g. The Owl and the Nightingale). The shortest text consists of 8 lines only (Wlll and Wit}, the longest runs to over 30000 lines (Layamon"s Brut). Some texts &re are translations or adaptations of foreign originals which they may or may not acknowledge. Layamon, for example, indicates his sources at the very beginning of his text1 ''he nom pa Englisca boc pa makede Seint Bed.a an oper he nom on pe makede Seinte Albin & the feire Austin l)e fulluht Latin broute hider in boc he nom pe pridde leide per am.idden pa makede a Frenches clerk Wace wes ihoten" (LB 31ff.)
The books he mentions can be identified as the Latin original and the OE translation of Bede's Historla Ecclesiastica Gentis Anglorum as well as Wacets Brut. The ME Bestlary 1s an English version of the well-known Latin text by Theobaldus. other texts are original works for which no sources a.re known (e.g. The Proverbs of Alfred, Poellld Morale). Reflecting the proportions in which verse and prose texts have come down to us, the data base contains fewer examples of the latter {e.g. The Peterborough Chronicle, Old Kentlsh
Sermons,
The Bodley
Homllies,
The Shires
and Hundreds
of Eng-
land) Regional variation is more amply illustrated on the eastwest axis than on the north-south axis; most texts belong to the Midlands and the South. For all texts I have used editions of the w
earliest
MS available.
22
7. Descriptive
Method
The models of a linguistic description form a system with complex relations between its members. It is difficult to present the models in a convenient arrangement because the description of the realization possibilities of each model presupposes some knowledge about the structure of other models. The description of the model of the substant1val syntagm, for example, requires some understanding of the prepositional syntagm and vice versa, because a substant1val syntagm. like those very old books on the green shelf contains the prepositional syntagm on the green shelf as one of its ICs, and the prepositional syntagm on the green shelf contains the substantival syntagm the green shelf as one of its ICs. Although references to later chapters are unavoidable, the arrangement chosen here restricts these to a necessary min1mwn. The presentation begins with the models of those syntagms which realize
compulsory
constituents
of EME clauses.
The chapter
which
follows deals with nominal syntagms, Le. syntagms with substantival, adjectival, and pronominal nuclei. The common feature of Syntagms') is the syntagms described in Chapter IV. ('Functional that their nuclei belong to closed classes. The models in Chapter v. ('The Predicative Syntagm') are a necessary prerequisite for the description of EME clause types which fol lows. The heading •Functional Constructions' for Chapter VII. is motivated by the fact that the nuclei of these syntagms resemble those of 'funcpositions are tional syntagrns • , but the I Cs in the peripheral either realized by or contain predicative syntagms. In contrast Chapters VIII. and IX. introduce no new modto earlier chapters, els and instead present syntagms characterized either by special syntactic properties of their ICs ('Conjunctive Constructions 1 ) or by special syntactic relations between their ICs ( 'Syntagms 'Syntagms with coordinated ICs' ). The last chapter with the title with Interordinated ICs takes up the passive syntagm again and treats two constructions corresponding to the PDE types he is easy to please and the more - the better. The descriptions of all the models presented are accompanied by several examples which illustrate some of the realization possibilities. Examples from verse texts are referenced by line numI
23
ber,
those from prose texts
by page and line
number. All examples are translated into PDE {preceded by ":: Where helpful to understand the syntactic structure of the EMEexamples, a word-for-word translation was added, sometimes accompanied by morphological information ( in italics) • The syntactic structure of the relevant 11
).
part of each example is analysed in terms of the appropriate el.
mod-
II.
COMPULSORY CONSTITUENTS OF EARLYMIDDLEENGLISHCLAUSES
1. The Active Verbal syntagm In its
an inflected Ex~les;
simplest
form the active
verbal
syntagm consists
of
verb form. beorkep his hu.ndes (LB:21340) bark his hounds ch1s hounds are barking pu fli st ni tes euer mo (ON238) you fly [at] night ever more
=you fly always at night
More complex realizations of the syntagm contain a verbal nucleus and one or more peripheral elements. As the latter belong to different paradigmatic classes, several peripheral positions have to be set up in the model of the active verbal syntagm. An action which took place in the past can be expressed by an inflected past verb form (=morphological past) or by a conbinaation of an inflected form of habben 'have' and the past participle of a lexical verb ( •syntactic past) . As in OE there 1s no complementary distribution between the morphological and the syntacby the following syntagms.: 1 tic past. This can be illustrated Ex~les:
past
And alle po pet anie wise po diefle er ikuemde po bed nu mid him in helle, vordon and vordemde CPM271f. t and all those who in any wise the devil before pleased, they are now with him in hell, ruined and damned •and all those who pleased the devil before in any way are now with him in hell, ruined and damned Blipe ma.1 he panne ben ~et god haued iquemed (PM174) glad may he then be who God has pleased ~he who pleased God may then be glad
1cf. Mitchell 1985:291: tense and the periphrasis
lel or connected
sentences."
"The well-known overlap between the is attested by their use in paral-
25
Both passages
this
point
out the consequences
world will have on his life
after
that
death.
man's behaviour
In the first
ple the morphological, The constituents
in the second the syntactic past haued and iguemed in PM174 are
occurrence dependency.
Therefore they are definitely
nated.
The data
in
exam-
is used. in mutual
not coordi-
show that
the number and type of the constituents which are governed by EMEactive verbal syntagms depend on the lexical verb and not on the tense marker. In our examples the objects ~o diefle and god are governed by forms of the verb cwemen. properThe form haued in PM174 has no bearing on the syntactical ties of the active verbal syntagm. This establishes the form of cwemen as nucleus and haued as an actualiser. The peripheral poby haueo will be called 'tense', and the sition which is filled model of the active verbal ayntagm can provisionally be represented as
verb.
tense->
The peripheral
position
a form of the auxiliary
iary hen 'be'.
'tense'
can be realized not only by but also by a form of the auxil-
habben. Usually the lexical
verb determines
the choice of
one or the other auxiliary.2 The auxiliary habben is used significantly more often than ben. out of the 163 verbs of my data which position, 116 combine only are expanded by an element in •tenset with habben, 40 only with ben. Bearing in mind the PDE situation,
we may infer
from this
distribution that the substitution of ben by habben is well under way in EME. This pro-
in ttense' position cess had started in OE already. According to Mustanoja the distribution of the auxiliaries in OE was such that beon/wesan was used with intransitive, habban with transitive verbs. The latter was "occasionally also found
with true
intransitive
OE it is developing fect and pluperfect
sents
verbs
( ••• ), which suggests
that
even in
into a kind of general auxiliary of the pertenses" (Mustanoja 1960:500). But he misrepre-
the EMEsituation tically all transitive
when he maintains that "by early MEpracverbs use have for the formation
2Appendices II.1 and II,2 contain lists bine only with one or the other auxiliary.
of their
of verbs which com-
26
perfect and pluperfect tenses'' (Mustano ja 1960 ! 500) . The following examples show that in EME intransitive verbs can be combined with habben and transitive verbs with hen. Examples: ~a men hafden
iuast
=the men had fasted
him ~at hit
(LB22310)
ischapen
he who it created is =he who created
Mustanoja.
himself
admits
is
(PA143)
it
that
the
syntactic
properties
of a
verh did not exclusively determine the choice of the auxiliary. He points out that o~iginally both auxilia.i:-ies denoted a state, then they came to denote the effect or the result of an action. But only beon/wesan kept the former possibility beside the newly acquired one. Thus the pragmatic/textual meaning can be made to explain the use of ben in contexts where we would expect habben, i.e. in the presence of an object. Another feature which is considered relevant for the choice of ben as auxiliary 1& the semantic structure of the verb itself. Mustanoja mentions the semantic component 'mutative', which prevents intransitive verbs from being combined with habben. The same semantic component may explain the use of ben with transitive verbs, too. The following syntagm is a case in point. Example: po he was iknowe pat Rimenhild
was his 03e (KH983f.)
The verb cnawen which usually means 'to know• is used here as a mutative verb, which expresses the transition from one state to another. The passage should be translated learnt that Rimenhild was his own [ =had
as 'when he (=Horn] remained faithful to
Horn had been forced by an intrigue to leave the country, King Modi had proposed to Rimenhild. As the wooer was supported by Rimenhild's father, the wedding was imminent despite her refusal. In her di:stress :she sent a messenger to Horn. The passage quoted above introduces Hornts reaction. It expresses his transition from the state of ignorance or uncertainty to the state of certa.in knowledge. him)'.
After
Examples of intransitive frequent
than transitive
verbs combined with habben are more verbs combined with ben. They fore8hadow
27
the later generalization of have as a tense aux1liary. 3 The following syntagms illustrate the use of habben as an expansion of intransitive verbs irrespective of whether they express
the result
Examples:
of an action
or a state
(of limited
duration):
Jhc habbe wa.lke wide (KH953~
I have walked wide =I travelled far pe saulen ... pat so hedden isped pat leyen hedden in peouene bed (VP255f.) the souls ... that so had behaved that [they] lain had in thieves' bed =those who had behaved in such a way that they had been thieves Hit halJ ileie l)ere Fulle seue 3ere (KH1139) it has lain there full seven years
lay there seven clear years pos laste on ure habbe~ i-travailed (KS34,3) those last [ones] one hour have worked =those last ones worked for one hour =it
The choice of habben with intransitive tivated by the context: Example: Al to lome ic habbe igelt
verbs may also be mo-
on worke and on worde (PM11}
all too often I have sinned in works and in words =all too often have I sinned in deeds and in words
The verbs in the immediate neighbourhood
of gelten are transitive (habbe iguede {9), ha.bbe idon ( 10}, habbe ispent ( 12), habbe ileid (12)). Therefore the use of habben needs no justification here. This series of active verbal syntagms with habben in 'tense• position may have had a levelling influence on the construction of gelten. Some verbs
of my data
combine
with
habben
as well
as wit.h
ben: bipenchen:
To late ic habbe me bipoht (PM8)4 too late I have me bethought ;too late did I think [about it} To bidden his milce to late we beod b1-pohte {D055f.} 5
3This development has to be seen as complementary to another process, namely the generalization of be as a passive auxiliary. 4cf.
(LB16745)
5cf .. (LB2510)
28
to ask his mercy too late we are bethought breken:
~too late did we consider to ask for his mercy If 6u hauest is broken (B137)6 if you have them broken •if you have broken them [~the laws of the church] ~u ert of pyne ibroke (VP10)
you are of pain broken-away •you overcame the pain
don:
what ha.uelJ pes mon ido (PL336) what has this man done =what did this man do mine dagis arren nei done (PA582) my days are nearly done =my days have nearly
faren:
he ...
tolde
passed (=come to an end] him ful 3are Hu he hadde 1fare
(KH465f,
)
he told
him full
•he readily
how he had fared
readily
told him how he had fared
Julius pe k~1sere was from ~issen londe 1faren (LBBOOJf.) Julius the emperor was from this land travelled =ElTiperorJulius had left this country
Ihc habbe go man1 mile
(KH1176)
I have gone many miles
=I travelled
many a mile
a 33 wass pe 33 re
efenn (OR4352f.} always
was their
=their
(=the Jews'}
evening
evening of that leggen:
week.e gan
week gone
day
All
all
at
out that
day at
week had always finished
in the
Al to muchel ic habbe ispent, to litel ileid on horde (PM12) all too much I have spent, too little laid on hoard
=too much have I spent,
too little
laid aside
ure lord was i-leid him don to $lepe our Lord was lain him down to sleep =our Lord had lain down to sleep
setten:
di 3 att
tatt
1cc hafe sett hero
f0Rd41ff.) I have set
~is boc ...
here on this
=I have written
po vre louerd
man13 word
book ••• many words
many a word into this
wes isethe
(KS32, 1Sf.)
to his supere
book
(PL90)
when our Lord was sat [down] to his supper =when our Lord had sat down to his supper
With some of the 6c f.
( HK1238)
verbs
the
difference
in construction
is
29
f don, f aren , gan ) • The verbs leggen and setten are combined with ben, when they govern a pronominal object which refers back to the subject. This construction is still current in Modern High German, cf. setzen Sie sich 'take a seat', leg' dich h1n und sei still 'lie down and be quiet'. In PDE, however, the construction 'causative verb+ reflexive pronoun object' no longer exists. The causative verbs have been replaced in this construction by their corresponding pr 1maries: ~ lay + reflexive pronoun' by lie, 'set + reflexive pronoun• by sit. The nucleus of an active verbal syntagm can also be expanded by a class of elements which correspond in form and function to the PDE modal auxiliaries. This class has the following members: ah,7 can, dear, mz1, ~, ~~ schal, ~1 uten, willen8. They require the nucleus in its infinitive form. matched
by a d 1 ff erence
in
meaning
Examples: ich hire wulle teche
a5 ic con (LR8) I her will teach as I can =I will teach her as well as I can
Summeme may per iseon (VP95) some one mAy there see ·
zone can see some there Eade hi mu3en bi offerd
pet sullen hine bihialde (PM286) easily they may be afraid that shall him behold =they may well be afraid who shall see hi.m
In PDE modal auxiliaries
cannot manifest
active
verbal
syntagms
on their own, lexical verbs can. It is t.berefore correct to say that there is unilateral occurrence de~endency between modal auxiliaries and lexical verbs in PDE, although there are environments in which - due to concord restrictions - the form of the lexical
verb cannot replace
the whole syntagrn,
cf.
7The
auxiliary ah occurs in two variants: ah and ah to. They are in free variation"; cf. ne og ur non oder tosunen {B28S} "not ought of-us none (the] other to shun' ~Tnone of us should shun the other one'; Her-of we owe penche (B217) 'of-this we ought think =•this we should consider' 8GCE §3. 20 in.eludes also the elements need and used to for PDE; they do not exist in EME. The EME forms man, ~irf, uten do no longer exist in POE. Traces of man survive 1n,-rna1n y nortfiern) dialects of PDE. -
30
he
I will come I to wcome
the meeting
In EME- as indeed up to the Early Modern English 'auxiliaries'' can manifest nuclei of active verbal
period - modal synta.gms.
Example: We solden us bi~enche bet ofte and wel ilame Hwet we bied,
(PM327f.)
and of wham we come
to hwam we sulle,
us bethink better, often and well readily, what we are, to whom/what we shall and of whom/what we come =we should consider more, often and readily, what we are, ~here we go to, and where we come from we should
Thus it seems that there modal verbs and lexical refuted by the following PM32S 1s the
nucleus
is mutual occurrence independence between verbs in EME. This asswnption i.s easily considerations. It is true that sulle in
of
an
active
verbal
syntagm. It governs a
prepositional object {=to hwam). Sullen in PM286, however, cannot be the nucleus of sullen bihialde, because this active verbal syntagm governs the direct object h1ne, which 1s a syntactic pro~
perty of the lexical fore,
i.s that
verb bihialde.
The correct
PM328 and PM286 represent
analysis,
different
there-
construcions.
occurrence independence, however, holds only between elements of the same construction, As the syntactic properties of a syntagm are determined by its nucleus, it follows that in EMEactive verbal syntagms of the form 'modal auxiliary + lexical verb' the
latter
1s the nucleus.
The peripheral
ized by modal auxiliaries
representation
of this
will
be called part of the verbal
modal -->
Modal auxiliaries
pos1t1on which may be real•modal•t and the formal syntagm is
verb.
and tense
markers
do not
exclude
each
other in front of the same verb.
Example: Ic mi3te habbe bet idon (PM15} I migfit have better done :I
Therefore
might have done better
two peripheral
positions
have to be set up in the model
of the active verbal syntagm. The elements in the two peripheral positions determine the verb in the same way. The complex syntactic relation involved 1s therefore parallel determination, and the enlarged
version
of the model is
31
moda}verb >
tense The above example is to be analysed
modal: mi3t}tense:
as
> verb:
idon
habbe
shown before, some verbs combine only with habben, others only with ben in 'tense' position, some combine with either, but none with both at the same time. Interestingly enough, can occur before the we find that more than one modal auxiliary same verb in the same syntagrn. As was
Ex!!!llPle: he sel him cunnen silde wel (PM334) he shall him can shield well =he will be able to protect himself well pat mannk.inn shollde mu3henn wel upp cumenn inntill heoffne (OR3944f.} that mankind should may well up come into heaven =that
mankind
up into heaven
should
have the
opportunity
of getting
The first modal auxiliary shows person and number concord with the subject, the second and the lexical verb are in the infinitive. Theoretically the syntagm may represent four different con-
structions: a) The two modal verbs realize each determining the lex.ical
modal
1}->
verb
modal 2
different verb, i.e.
------>
modal 2
b) The two modal verbs a.re coordinated determining the lexical verb, i.e.
1 -->
modal
2} ->
verb
in the same position,
each
verb
c) The two modal verbs realize one peripheral being sUbordinated to modal 2, i.e. (modal
positions,
modal 1
or
(modal 1 modal 2} -->
peripheral
verb
position,
modal 1
32
d) The first
sisting
modal verb determines
a complex verbal
of the second modal verb and the lexical
modal 1 -->
(modal 2
~~>
nucleus,
con-
verb, i.e.
verb).
The first two solutions can be ruled out on the grounds that there is no direct relation between the first modal and the nucleus. This statement presupposes that we know exactly what the syntagm means. The nearest equivalent in PDEare constructions of the type must be allowed to come. In German the isomorphic construction er muB kommen kOnnen exists. It can be shown that the permutation of the two modals changes the meaning of the syntagm, cf. er mu.13 kommenkOnnen ~ er kann kommenmQssen 'he must be allowed to come' ,(t 'he may have to come• • This means that the two modal verbs do not determine the nucleus in the same way. Moreoover, the syntagrn er muBkommenkOnnen is significantly different from er muBkommen the must come'. This means that there is no direct relation between~ and kommen. Accordingly we assume that there is no direct relation between shollde and upp cumenn either. This in turn implies that the second modal cannot form a complex nucleus with the lexical verb, because this would establish a direct relation between the first modal verb and the lexical verb. This was ruled out before. The correct analysis of the construction is therefore c) with the two modal verbs in the same peripheral position, the first subordinated to the second. OR3944f. should be analysed as (shollde
mu3henn) -> upp comenn. This analysis is borne out by tests with Modern English informants. Asked to answer questions with complex verbal syntagms, m.y informants included all elements in peripheral positions in
their
->
answers, e.g.
Could the dog be lying under the table in a-floor sitting-room? - Yes, he could be. Should parents be allowed to forbid their children to go to the cinema? - Yes, they should be allowed.
Visser not analyse He mentions
lists the construction (§1645, §2134), but he does the syntagmatic relations between the constituents. that
died out after
the construction
that
period,
was frequent in ME, but that it surviving only in Scottish dialects.
33
In nearly all his examples the first modal 1s shall or should. As the construction is not attested for OE at all, but is con1110nin Dutch
and German,
Visser
conjectures
that
it
may have
been
bor-
rowed from these languages. As this construction shows, the 'modal' position may also be realized by more complex syntactic entities. Different constructions are possible. Fairly frequent is a realization by two modal auxiliaries, which are connected by a conjunction, in particular by and. 9
pat
kunne & wille ri 3t us deme (ON186) who can and will rigfit us judge =who can and will judge us competently
se t,et mai and nele l)ider come (PM372) the [manJ that may and will-not thither come =he who can, but does not want to go there The active
verbal
syntagms of the examples are analysed
& wille modal: mai and nele
modal: kunne
->
->
as
verb: deme verb: come
active verbal syntagm may contain a form of ben which requires the following nucleus in present participle form, As the construction 1s isomorphic with the PDE progressive aspect, the by ben will be called •aspect'. position which can be realized Expansion by an element in 'aspect' position does not, however, mean that the syntagm expresses simultaneity, incompletion, an action of limited duration, etc. 10 Some verbs, e.g. waxen, walden, wunien occur more often in this construction than others. An
Exam_..e,_les : nys no wrt uexynde a wude ne a velde
pas feye furp vp-holde (P~168ff,J
pat
euer mvwe
not-is no herb growing in [the) wood nor in [the] field which ever may the doomed further uphold =no plant grows in the wood or in the field which could ever save those who are doomed to die po were in iherusalem •.• Men wunyinde of alle kunnes londe (PL665f.) then were in Jerusalem ••• men living of all kinds of countries •people of different countries were living in Jerusalem then 9For the description 10cf.
GCE §3.29ff.
of this
construction
cf.
Chapt.VIII.
34
preo dzies wes pe king wuniende l)ere & pan feorde c:la!ie to Tintaieol he wende (LB19216ff.) three days was the king- living there and [on the] fourth day to Tintaieol he went =the king stayed there for three days, and on the fourth day he went to Tintaieol The verbal syntagrn of the first example expresses an action of unlimited duration, which corresponds to the simple form in PDE. Out of context PL665f. is ambiguousz it may either express an action of limited duration ( =were living) or a completed action (=lived). LB19216ff. is a c1ear case of enumeration of actions completed. in the past.
Mustanoja (1960;585) assumes that the construction which in OE is restricted to prose was associated in HE with vivid narration. Although ben is by far the most frequent 'aspect' expansion, it is not the only possible one. The following syntagm shows that 1n this function. When ben commutes with verbs like sitten 'sit' these verbs are used as peripheral elements in an active verbal syntagm, their meaning is nearly equivalent to that of ben.
EXamEle; he murninge sat
he mourning sat
(FB36)
=he was unhappy
expansion in 'aspect' position does not exclude an element in 'mod.al' position, but data are lacking for the simultaAn
neous
occurrence
earliest
of
'aspect
'
and
•tense
I
expansions.
's
examples (§2148) date from the 14th century. The lack of
co-occurrence of the two expansions may be due to the and the 1 ~ the 'have+ past participle periphrasis' participle periphrasis' had not yet well-established functions. Therefore it is not necessary to set up two
positions verbal
Visser
in the model of the active
different verbal syntagm. The active
syntagms of the examples are analysed ys
were wes
sat
-> -> -> ->
The same position pansion or by a 'tense'
fact that + present different
as
uexynde
wunyinde wuniende murninge
may be realized expansion.
either by an •aspect' exDepending on the type of expan-
35 sion,
the nucleus
is either
a present
participle
parof the model
or a past
ticiple of a lexical verb. The formal representation of the active verbal syntagm is now11
~> verb
modal tense l {aspect) The nucleus
of an active
verbal
syntagm may also
be expanded
by a negative particle. In EMEthe most frequent negative particle is ne, and there is unilateral occurrence dependency between the negative particle and the verb. As the verb determines the syntactic properties of the active verbal syntagm, there is also unilateral functional dependency between these two elements. The verb is
the
nucleus,
peripheral
the
position
negative
p&rticle an expansion which will be called 'negation•.
Exameles: he ne hedden per before ine him beliaue they not had there before in him belief
realizing
a
(KS30, 4f.)
=there they did not believe in him before Hwi ne serue we crist (SC1) why not serve we Christ =why do we not serve Christ
The negative particle
ne, which precedes the nucleus has two var-
iants: ne and the proclitic form!!:.· The latter is a positional va.riant which occurs with the verbs habben 'have', witen 'know', 1 bileven 'believe', !!2!! 'see•. 2
Examples: he pe bet nise 3e (ON382) he the better not-see =he does not see the better hy nvten hwat hi dop {PL442) they not-know what they do =they do not know what they do The position
this
element
11eraces
'negation'
occurs
ean
also
much less
indicate
the same position. 12That the variant
frequently.
alternative n- is
can be seen from syntagmsltke
not saw• =•we did not see you·.
be realized
Its
realization
not obligatory
by !!2J!,
usual
although
position
possibilities in these
is
of
contexts
we pe ne yseyen (BE305) •we you
36
after the verb, nucleus.
but occasionally
Example: Neded de de deuel
no3t also precedes
the verbal
no 3t (8164)
urges you the devil not =the devil
does not urge you
The frequency of no3~ must have increased rapidly after the EME period , because according to Mustano ja ( 1960 : 34 0 ) .it had become
the ordinary negative particle in the 14th century. A third possibility is the co-occurrence of ne and no3t in 'negation• position. Examples:
ich ne singe nopt whan ich teme {ON1470) I not sing not when I breed ~I do not sing when I breed ich bi dai no3t ne flo (ON372) I by day not not fly
.r do not fly during the day
As both elements also occur independently of each other, one might be tempted to interpret their co-occurrence as a sign of emphasis. This interpretation is refuted, however, by para.l lel syntagms where the combination of the particles commutes with one of them.
Exam~les~ we{n)st pu ~at ich ne cunne singe (ON47) think
you that
I not can sing
=do you think that I cannot sing
pu .••
telst
pat ich ne can no3t singe
you say that I not can not sing =you say that I cannot sing
fON309f.)
Thus ne-no3.!:, is a (discontinuous) constituent which commutes with the elements~ and ~3t in 'negation• position. The analysis of the active verbal syntagms of the above examples is
ne
ne ne ne
no3t
ne-no 3t ne ne-no3t 13As
positional
-> -> ->
-->
-> ->
-> ->
hedden
~erue 13
1.se3e wvten 14
nede flo
singe 15 s1nge 16
variants
are irrelevant in syntactic analyis, pleremes in a •normalized• form.
neglects
the elements in 'modal'
it is customary ( in AF) to represent
14cf.
fn.13
15This analysis
position.
37 They realize that part of the model of the active which may be represented as negation->
syntagm
verb.
element in 'negation• the other expansions.
position
An
Examples:
verbal
No men ne may herre
iseon
does not exclude any of (VP 34)
no man not may higher [trees]
see
~nobody can see higher (trees) Nabbed he no ping vor3ete of pan ~et he ise 3en (PM98) not-have they no thing forgntten of this that they saw =they have not forgotten
anything of what they saw nys no wrt uexynde a wude (PA168) not-is n.o herb growing in [the] wood =no herb grows in the wood
The examples illustrate
the following
combinations
of expansions.
negation+ modal, negation+ tense, negation+ aspect. stellation requires th8t we set up a sep&r&te 'negation' and the model of the active verbal syntagm 1s now
This conposition,
negation modal > verb
ten. se } { aspect
The nucleus of an active verbal syntagm may also be exp~ded by an inflected form of the verb ginnen; the nucleus follows in its infinitive form. Past forms of ginnen are much more frequent than present tense forms, the ratio being about 20: 1. The inflected forms of the past are gan/g_on, gunne, gunnen. The phonological similarity of the singular forms gan/ gon explains why they are by the forms£!!!!/~ of the modal auxiliasometimes substituted ry.17 17cf. Mustanoja 9.an are phonological
1960:610. variants
Visser even maintains that can and of the same verb: "both 2 and c
represent allophones of the same phoneme /g/, differing not in their being voiceless or voiced, but in the energy of articulation'' {Visser §14 77). He supports his argwnent by quoting cases of reverse spelling.
38
£?5!mple: t,e see him con ded prowe Vnder hire chambre wowe
(KH970af.) the sea him did dead throw under her chamber window =the sea threw him dead under her chamber window
This syntagn. shows that the original meaning 'begin 1 became weakened during the EME period, and ginnen could be used as an empty periphrastic verb. Mustanoja's statement that this holds only for the past forms of 91nnen should be modified so as to include all
forms. 18 The following two syntagms illustrate the range of possible meanings of the present tense forms of ginnen. Examples: pe blostme ginnep springe & sprede (ON437) =the blossoms begin (to] spring and spread clerkes ginneb songes wirche (ON722) clerics do songs make =clerics compose hymns The first
example occurs
in the context
of a conventional
picture
of spring: the snow has melted, early flowers begin to blossom, the birds come back. ON437 describes the beginning of a new season. The context of the second example is a simile in which the nightingale likens church services on earth to the state of everlasting bliss in heaven. Earthly and heavenly hymns are one aspect of the comparison. The form ginnep in ON722 has no ingressive meaning. The different shades of meaning are irrelevant, however, for syntactic analysis, because they have no bearing on the syntactic relation between the forms of ginnen and the following infinitive .. It is therefore not adequate to set up two different signs ginnan_ - as
Visser
does
{§1269,
§1477).
He explicitly
rejects
earlier
descriptions (Funke 1922, Homann 1954, Mustanoja 1960) which set up one verb ginnen with a range of different meanings. When gtnnan expresses the beginning of an action, state, etc., Visser calls it a 'verb of aspect•; when this meaning is not present, it is an
auxiliary (subclass 'periphrastic it is very hard to discriminate 11
(81269).
A valuable
verb'). He ad:mits, however, that between these two kinds of gan,.
piece of new information
18 1'Through frequent
which he contributes
use 1t loses 1ts ingressi ve colour more more and occurs mainly in the intensive-descriptive functions.11 (Mustanoja 19601611) and
39
is that the ginnen-expansion means that among the usages
Mittel
zur
Perfekt1vierung,
is
restricted to verse texts. This of ginnen enumerated by Funke - als 0
als
deskriptive
Ausdrucksform,
als
farbloses Flickwort .. (Funke 1922; 15) - the last one is perhaps even more frequent than was assumed before 1n the analysis of individual syntagm.s. Another element which may realize a peripheral position in an active verbal syntagm 1s the verb don. Here, too, the nucleus follows in its infinitive form. The meaning of 9.2.!lin this function is as opaque as that of ginnen. Very often an expansion by don does not change the meaning of the nucleus at all. When don is used in texts with end rhyme, it may safely be assumed that it serves to place the infinitive into end (=rhyme) position.
EXample: Apulf hit dude write
pat Horn ne luuede no 3t lite (KH931f.) Athulf it did write who Horn not loved not little =Athulf wrote it, who loved Horn very much
There are other
instances,
however, where don clearly
serves
the
purpose of emphasis. Example: se pet e3hte wile hialde wel pe hwile pe hi mot wealde, / 3ieue hi for godes loue, panne ded he hi wel ih1alde {PM55f.) the [man) who [his) possession will hold well as long
as he may dispose (of it], give it (away] for God's love, then does he it well hold •he who wants to keep well his possession while he may dispose of it, should give it away for God's sakei then does he keep it well
The four active verbal syntagms wile ihialde, mot wealde, 31eue, ded ihialde form two pairs of semantic contrast in a crosswise arrangement. In the pair mot wealde: 3ieue the antonymy is realof the verbs wealden 'keep' : 3~ ized by the semantic structure 'give away•. In the pair wile ihialde: ded 1hialde the verbal nucleus 1s the same. The semantic contrast is realized by the pewille expresses ripheral elements, wille and ded. The actualiser ded stresses the realization of this intention. The an intention, obviously deliberate rhetorical structure of the passage supports is not a neutral statement, but an the argument that de6 ihialde
40
early use of emphatic do. 19 It thus refutes Mustanoja"s hypothesis that "unambiguous cases of emphatic periphrastic do are not recorded until the 15th century" (Mustanoja 1960:606). The verb don as an actualise£ in an active verbal syntagm
has a similarly wide range of meanings as ginnen. Funke describes them as follows: "Doch begegnen uns einige Beispiele, 1n denen don entweder bereits an der Grenze zwa farblosen T!tigkeitswort steht, vielleicht vOllig pleonastisch geworden 1st oder aber zum Intensivum periphrastic
neigtn tFunke 1922!15). Both elements are either mere verbs which do not modify the meaning of the follow-
ing infinitive their meaning
at all, or they are aspect in an individual syntagm, it
markers. 20 Whatever seems safe to assume
with Visser that they exclude each other in the same active verbal posisyntagm.2 1 This means that they occupy the same peripheral tion. Expansions by don or 9innen w-il1 be called •periphrasis• expansions. Nearly all active verbal syntagtns of rny data which have a form of ginnen or don in peripheral position are not expanded by any other element. Only one syntagrn illustrates the combination of don and an element in 'modal' position.
Example: pat schall
don vs tene 22 (KH683)
that shall do us harm will harm us
=that
Vtsser quotes one syntagm with a combination of a negative ticle
par-
and don;
19The EMEtext is an allusion
ment where Jesus
to a passage in the NewTesta-
salvation to those who follow him. The Authorized Version preserves the same rhetorical structure: ..For whosoeuer will saue his life shall lose it, but whosoeuer shall lose his life for my sake and the Gospels, the same shall saue it.
11
promises
(Mk 8:35)
20 oo.rmOller/Utz (1974) analyse
..be+ present participle" and as alternative expressions of the progressive aspect. Mosst {1952:8134) considers ~1nnen as an aspect marker: 11 To express the idea of a 'beginning action (inchoative as;eect) HE normally used the verb gin(ne) ... 2 1visser goes even one step further, maintaining that don and "do + inf initive
11
tinnen are substitutable hesls by the observation
for each other.
and ginnen in another.
22tene is a verb here.
that
the
He supports thisli'ypohas don in one MS
same text
41
Example: we ne doz nou3t ore ordre breke (quot. we not do not our order break .:we do not break our order
Visser 11438)
In the EMEtexts investigated so far the combination of a 'periphrasis• expansion and either an 'aspect • or a t tense• expansion has not been found. Consequently, all three expansions realize the same position
and the model of the active
its most elaborate
form is
verbal
syntagm in
negation
moda~ense
}
> verb
aspect
{ periphrasis Applied ses.
to
Analysis1
the last
two examples,
yields
O negation; modal: schall
tense aspect
)
{periphrasis negation:
moda~!n~e aspect
{ periphrasis usually
it
the nuclea.r
: don
the following
>
verb: tene
>
verb: breke
analy-
ne-nou3t
} : doz
position
is realized
by one lexical
verb
"411hose form is determined by the immediately preceding expansion. One more complex realization is the combination of two (or more} lexical verbs which are connected by a conjunction, mostly by and.23 Examples: hi wolden heore louerdes
dom iseon and ihere
(PL228)
they would their Lord's judgement see and hear
=they wanted to see and hear their Lord•s pu mi3t, Hule, 3itte & clinge (ON743) you may, Owl, sit and wither-away =OWl, you may stay there and decay 23For the description
of this
construction
cf.
judgement
Chapt.VIII.
42
Analysis:
negation:
o
modal: wolden tense } aspect :0
> verbz iseon and 1here
negation: O moda~~n=!3t aspect ;0 {periphrasis
> verb:
fperiphrasis
J
2. The Copulative
sitte
&
clinge
Syntagm
Like the active verbal syntagm the EMEcopulative ayntagm can be realized by a finite verb form. It may therefore seem superfluous to nevertheless tive syntagm tation on the
set up two separate models. The two main reasons for doing so are the internal structure of the copulaon the one hand and the possible types of complemenother.24
Examples: Stille beo pu penne (KA24, still be you then =be quiet
1}
now
co£ he waxep lB124) bold he grows =he grows bold
Ho stondep pustrur
pane pe nyht (VP22S)
they stand darker than the night =they [=the devils] are blacker than the night
The first example illustrates the most frequent realization of the nucleus of a copulati.ve syntagm by a form of ben tbe•. The verbs which col1U'ltutewith ben in this function have next to no lexical
meaning. They serve to establish tion
between
•copulative
the
subject
a syntactic
and another
and a semantic rela-
IC ( which
will
be called
complement' - cf. Chapt.V.2.2.1.).25
24The complementation types are described
in Chapt.v.2.2.
25 rn my data the following verbs form nuclei of copulative syntagms: ben, bicumen, bll2fen, biliven, biseme, faren, elden, hoven, laeven"; II:9gen:, 11v Ien, sit ten, stanoen, llflnchen, waxen, wurpen.
43
The internal
structural differences between the active verbal syntagm and the copulative syntagm concern not only the realization possibilities of the nucleus, but also the number of peripheral positions and their realization. A 'negation• position has to be set up for the copulative syntagm, too. It is realized by the particle ne or by the discontinuous constituent ne-no3!• The particle ~3! alone is not recorded in this peripheral position of the copulative syntogm. As in the active verbal syntagm, the negative particle ne has two positional variants,~ and the procl1tic form!!=· The latter negates the forms~, art, is, were, weren, resulting in the forms !!!!!!, !!!.!:!.,!!!!, nere, neren. The particle ne precedes the nucleus of the copulative syntagm (alone or as part of the discontinuous constituent), the element !!23~ follows it.
Examples: pat land nis god (ON999) that land not-is good •that land is not good Ne beop nopt ones alle sunne (ON1395) not are not the-same all sins =all
sins
are not alike
Adopting the terminology
English
(GCE), I shall
call
used in the Grammarof Contemporary
the nuclear
position
model of the copulative syntagm which is required aa of the exam.plea above can be represented
negation--> The copulative
'copula".
for the analysis
copula.
syntagms of the examples. are analysed
ne ne-no 3t
-> -->
The
as
1s26 beot:,27
In the copulative syntagm the modal position cannot be realized by the elements man and uten. A possible explanation for this is that these modal auxiliaries were no longer well integrat1
26The pleremes
27cf. fn.3
are given in their
1
•normalized'
form, cf. fn.13.
44
ed in the language
system of the EMEperiod.28
Examples: syker he may sitte pe hyne hauep to 1-vere (PA217f.) safe he may sit who him has as friend =whoever possesses
it
[=reason]
can be safe
se man pet wile siker bien (PM41} the man who will safe be =the man who wants to be safe
position of a copulative syntsgm ah to and ah for to. They are free variants.
The element ah in •modal' has the
variants
ExamEles: mi wit oh to bi more (PM2)
my wit ought to be more ~1 ought to be wiser
Englond auhte forto ben Youres (HK2800f.) :England ought to be you4s I The modal ' position can also be realized by more complex syntactic entities, e.g. by two modal auxiliaries which are connected by a conjunction.
he one ma1 and sel bien engles and mannes blisce (PM378)
he alone may and shall be angels' and man's bli~s -he alone can and will be the bliss of angels and
mankind As in the active sition
i11re a.ctualisers, The copulati.ve syntagms
verbal syntagm, the elements because
they
do not
of the examples
in 'modal' po-
commute with
are therefore
zero.
to be ana-
lysed as may
oh to auhte forto
The formal
->
bi ben
->
->
representation
model of the copulative
modal-> Element.s
sltte bien
->
wille
the respective
part
of the
syntagm is
copula,
in 'negation'
not mutually exclusive
of
position
and in
in the same copulative
'modal
I
position
are
syntagm.
ExarnEles. ne sel no mer3de bi swo muchel swo is godes s1hte (PM367) 28This assumption is supported by the observation that also in the 'modal" position of the active verbal syntagm man and uten occur very rarely (4 examples for man, 7 for uten).
45
not shall no mirth be so big as Godts sight ~no delight is so big as the sight of God pu ne mi 3t mid us ho bllpe (ON418) you not can With us be merry =you cannot be merry wlth us
The representation which allows
the analysis
negatio}modal Analysis:
of the model of the copulative
syntagm
of the examples above is
> copula
negation:
n}->
copula:
bi
copula:
bo
modal: sel
negation: n}-> modal: mi3t The •tense' position in the by forms of habben •have• or ben in complententary distribution such ed by forms of habben, the nuclei by forms of ben.2 9 The combination the nuclei waxen and wurt,en may nuclei express occurrence of
the ~
transit ion in 'tense'
1
copulative syntagm is reali:zed be The two auxiliaries stand that the nucleus ben is expandwaxen and wurpen are expanded of ben in •tense' position with 1
•
be due to the fa.et that these from one state to another. 30 The position needs no justification.
It seems quite natural that an element on its way to being generalized in a particular peripheral position will also combine readily with the most frequent nucleus of the construction. Ex!!$>les:
Wel longe
ic habbe child
ibien
on worde and on dede
(PMJ)
well long I have child been 1n words and in deeds =a very long time have I been a child and in my deeds
29other nuclei are not recorded position in my data.
with an element
in my words
in 'tense•
30The semantic component 1 mutative' is also considered responsible [or the combination of particular verbal nuclei with ben in 'tense' position, cf. chapt.II.1.
46
Godess enngless w.eerennpa Well swipe glade wurpenn Off ~at (OR3914ff.)
God's angels were then very glad become of that =then God's angels had become very glad about it That part
of the model of the copulative
in these examples can be represented syntagms
of the
from elements 'modal' and 'tense',
examples
->
habbe
wcerenn Apart
as
copula.
tense-> The copulative
syntagm which is realized
are
analy3ed
as
ibien
wurl)erm .
-->
positions 'negano other expansions occur in the copu-
which can occupy the
tion', lative syntagrn. Constituents in the peripheral positions and 'tense' determine the form of the following element. ment in
'modal' An eleconstituent to
'modal'
position requires the following have infinitive form, an element in tense position requires the following constituent to have past participle form. The very first 1
element
of
a copulat.ive
syntagm
1
shows
person
and number
concord
with the subject. An element in •negation t position cannot be marked for person or number and it has no influence on the form of any of the other constituents of the syntagm. The elements in the peripheral positions do not determine way. The complex tactic rethe nucleus in a noticeably different determilation in the copulative syntagm is, therefore, parellel nation.
As
there
is
unlimited
combination
between
the
different
expansion types, three peripheral positions have to be set the model. Its generalized form can be represented as
negatio}modal tense
up in
> copula.
The following syntagms illustrate 'tense' and 'negation' + 'tense'.
combinations
of
Examples: hit mi3te han iben wel his Wille (FW87) it migfit have been well his will =it may well have been his will
nedde lust I-ben of mine moupe (FW100) not-had lust been of my mouth =if there had not been my gluttony
•modal•
+
47
Analysis:
negation:
03-
modal; m13te tense: han negation:
n}-
modal: o tense: hadde
> copula:
tben
> copula;
I-ben
3. The Passive syntagm Within
y,
the
the
passive
theoretical
syntagm
framework
needs
special
of AF chosen
for
this
stud-
justification.
In 1968 we find Mulder maintaining that there is no structural difference between a copulative predicative syntagm (e.g. was ill) and the
verbal part of a passive construction (e.g. was beaten).3 1 This 111 the position still taken by him today: 0ne sees that, unlike most other linguists, I regard the so-called tpassive' as a copulative predicative" (Mulder 1980 t 158). It 1s only consistent therefore that he does not set up a model of the passive syntagm for his description of PDE. In EMEthe passive voice is expressed by a form of ben 'be' or wurpen 'become' plus the past participle of a lexical verby Both elements are in mutual occurrence dependency. The syntactic relation between them is therefore either subordination or interordination. Both constituents can have other functions as well. Forms of ben can realize the nucleus of a copulative syntagm or the peripheral position 'tense• in an active verbal or in a copulative syntagm. Past participles can be nuclei of active verbal syntagrns. Whether a past participle has the function 'constituent of a passive syntagm' depends on the function of the other con0
stituent, relationship
and
interordinated.
vice holds
versa, between
since a mutual functional dependency the two constituents, i.e. they a.re
It could be argued that the situation
was the same
3 1·•we also see that it is not necessary at all to distinguish on the higher levels of analysis ... between ~assive and copulative (Mulder 968:86)
48
when ben had the function of a tense marker in an active verbal is easily refuted, since in an syntagm. However, thls ob)ection 1 active verbal syntagm of the form 'hen + past participle an , inflected verb commutes with this construction, thus establishing the form of the lexical verb as the nucleus of the syntagm. A second consideration supports the hypothesis of interordinat1on in the passive syntagm. If two isomorphic constructions (e.g. ben + past
have two different grammatical meanings {e.g. active, past 1passive), it fo1 lows that the syntactic relations between their constituents must be different, too. Otherwise the different grammatical meanings would be unaccounted for. The relation in the passive syntagm would also be different from that in the active verbal syntagm, if we assumed - as Mulder doesthat the past participle was subordinate to the form of ben. This would mean that the copula was the identity element of the syntagm, which determines its syntactic properties. This is not the case. The syntactic properties of the passive syntagm are partly partly by the other. Neidetermined by one of its constituents, ther of them can be called the identity element of the constru.ct1on. The past participle determines whether the passive syntagm can be expanded by an object ( cf. PDE I was g1 ven an apple {1 objects an apple)), the copula restricts the number of possible object expansions by one (cf. PDE he gave me an apple [2 objects; me, an apple]). The model of the EME passive syntagm contains therefore two equivalent positions; they will be called 'passive copula'
participle)
and
tpast
model which accounts passive
participle
1
•
The
formal
representation
for very simple realizations copula
past
of
a
is
participle.
In the discussion earlier of the relation between the constituents 'passive copula' and 'past participle,' only the element ben was mentionedT The reason for this was nwnerlcal: ben simply occurs much more frequently in 'passive copula' position than wurpen does. The two also involve somewhat different event-notions: ben expresses primarily a state, wurpen a process, although
49
this
is of secondary importance in syntactic analysis.32 The model developed so far accounts for passive
syntagrns
like the following. Examples:
hanged wurpe he on a hok (HK1102)
hanged be he on a hook
•may he be hanged on a hook parmide heop men acwalde (ON1370)
therewith are men killed =men are killed
Analysis:
with it
passive copulat wurpe
passive
copula:
beo~
hanged
past participle: past participle~
acwalde
Usually the nuclear position 'past pa~ticiple' is realized by just one plereme. Occazionally the position is occupied by two past participles which are connected by a conjunctlon.33
P?!"Ples1
hw he weren born and fedde (HK2984)
=how they were born and fed if pu art iworpe oper ishote (ON1121) =if you are hit or shot dead hit was swo i-seid and be-hote hwilem bi ~o profetes (KS26, 21 f. ) it was so said and promised whilom by the prophets =thus it was once said and promised by the prophets
The complex nucleus of the passive syntagm can be expanded by a number of peripheral elements. While the peripheral positions and
their
realizations
the
copulative
syntagm,
resemble they
those
are
not
of
the
active
completely
verbal
identical
and with
them.
The particles which are used for the negation of passive syntagrns are ne (with the positional variant n-, cf.Chapt.II.2.), !!231 and ne-no3l• Ne is considerably more frequent than the other two.
32The possibility of distinguishing between a static passive and a dynamic passive was lost in English when wurpen dropped out of use as a passive copula, 1.e. at the end of the ME period (cf. Mustanoja 1960:439). It was not until several centuries later that this distinction was reintroduced, when the aspect expansion was extended to the passive syntagm: "The passive was being_ built by X with overt progressive auxiliary •.. did not develop until the end of the eighteenth centuryu {Traugott 1972:144).
33syntagms of this type are called 'conjunctive tions'~ for their description cf. Chapt.VIII.
construc-
50
Examples: he nis parof bireued (ON120) he not-is thereof bereft =he is not bereaved of it [=his head] neren hi nouht ihud (PL645) not-were they not hidden
=they were not hidden
The 'modal• position can be realized by the auxiliaries schal a.nd willen. They require the following constituent
mot,
!!!!!J to
h~ve the
infinitive form. Comparing the possible elements in this peripheral position of the active verbal, the copulative and the passive syntagm, we note that their number decreases from syntagm to syntagm. Those with the highest frequencies in the active verbal syntagm are also recorded in the two other syntagms ~ those with a lower frequency in the active verbal syntagm also occur in the respective position in the copulative syntagm, but not in the passive syntagm: those with the lowest frequency in the active verbal syntagm are restricted to this construction.
EX!ffl!!les: ech ••• sal panne ben idemed (PM173) each ... shall then be judged =then everybody will be judged til his sone mohte •.. king ben maked of Denemark (HK378ff.) until his son might ••• king be made of Denmark =until his son could be made kin.g of Denmark The only auxiliary which can occupy the 'tense• position in a passive syntagm is habben *have•.3 4 It requires the past par-
ticiple
of the passive copula.
Examples: twien ~u hafuest
ibeon ouer-cwnmen (LB8325) you have been overcome ;you were overcome twice panne he hauede ben ofte swngen (HK226~ when he had been often beaten =when he had often been beaten twice
than
The nucleus of the passive syntagm can be expanded by more one peripheral element at the same time. As it cannot be de-
monstrated
that
the expansions
determine
the nucleus
in differ-
34.a.lthough it must be admitted that passive syntagms with a.n element in 'tense' position are not very frequent in EME, Mustas11.ghtly by quoting just two exnoja misrepresents the situation amples (Mustanoja 1960: 440). The Havelok text alone provides
fourteen.
51
ent
ways,
the complex tactic relation in the passive syntagm is determination.. The combination of a 'negation 1 and a
parallel 'modal' expansion ls quite
frequent.
Examples: hit it =it pis
ne moste ifulled bean (VP142) not must baptized be could not be baptized selkouth mihte nouht ben hyd (HK1059) this wonder might not be hidden =this
wonder could not be concealed
Thus the model of the passive syntagm must contain the two peripheral positions •negation' and 'modal'. An element in •tense• po:si tion, however, excludes any other peripheral element. This striking difference between the EME and the PDE passive syntagm can be explained by the observation that 1n EME 'tense' expansions
of passive syntagrns were not yet well integrated in the language system, as is also suggested by their low fcequency. A model of the passive syntagm which accounts two alternatives in its peripheral
for this situation must provide part; one alternative containing the two positions 'negation' and 'modal', the other only one position, ·tense'. This model can be represented as3 5
negation/ modal The examples
tense }-
with
> (passive
elements
copula
in peripheral
past participle)
positions
are analysed
as
negation: modal:
0
neJ / tense:
negation: modal: 0
n1tense:
35The
tpass.cop.;
t-,
(pass.eop.:
is
weren
p.p.:
p.p.:
indicates alternat1 ve poss1bil1 ties slash braces are not available when one alternative contains
one position.
bireued)
ihud)
here,
as
more than
52
negation: modal: sal negation:
o/tense: 0
modal; mohte negationl
modal: o negation:
0
f
ense:
(pass .cop.:
3-->
(pass.cop.:
/tense, J hafuesL>
o /
modal: moste
tense:
ben
p.p.:
idemed)
ben
p.p.:
maked)
(pas.s.cop.1
3--> 3--
(pass.cop.,
negation: nouht/ tense: modal.: mihte
0
> (pass.cop.:
ibeon
P•P•,
ifulled)
ouer-cwnmen)
beon
p.p.,
ben
P·P~= hyd)
Elements in peripheral positions of the passive syntagm do not restrict the realization possibilities of the nuclear positions. The following syntagm illustrates a combination of two participles connected by a. conjunction in the position 'past participle', and an element in the peripheral position 'tense'. Example: unto pis (HK1433f.)
unto this
[ilke]
day haue ich ben fed and fostred
very day have I been fed and fostered
ay
ever
=to this very day I have always been fed and fostered
Analysis,
negation:
modal: O
0/ tense:
3---
haue
> (pass.cop.:
p.p.:
ben fed and fostred)
III.
NOMINAL SYNTAGMS
1. The Substantival
Syntagm
The simplest
realization of this construction is a substanis why I call it 'substantival syntagm'. Names like
tive, and this 'noun phrase', 'nominal group' t 'nominal syntagm', etc. are misbecause by evoking the dichotomy verb; noun they imply leading, that they also cover constructions with other than substantival nuclei (e.g. pronominal_ 4dject1val). The structure of the substant1val syntagm 1s much more complex than that of the syntagms described earlier. A separate study would be needed to develop an exhaustive model of this construction. The model presented here can account only for the most frequent realizations of the syntagm in my d4t4. 1 One of the most striking differences between the EME subis that in DIE the occurstant1val syntagm and its PDE equivalent rence of singular forms of so-called 'count nouns' as unexpanded
nuclei
1s not restricted
to special
env1ronments. 2
Ex!:ffl.Ples; hwa swa sloge
heart odde hlnde (PC1087,119) whosoever slew hart or hind ;;the person who killed a hart or a hind one frogge [pat sit at mulne vnder coggeJ (ON85f.) a frog that sits at [the) mill under {the) cog-wheel •a frog that sits under the mill-wheel
Substantival nuclei can be expanded by various peripheral elements. The following passage• illustrate s\lbstantival syntagm.s which are expanded by adjectives. 1Mulder's model of the
'nominal syntagm', which he developed for the description of PDE, is not powerful enough to handle all realizations of the construction either (Mulder 1980z1S4f.)1 it does not even cover all those substantival nuclei with only premodification (e.g. the other important contribution, his only important
contributionJ,
2For these
let
environments
alone postmodlfied cf.
nuclei.
GCE §4.14 NOte(a}
54 wil,de dor (0N1012) ::::wild animals
mid mycelan here (PC1090,13f.) =with a big army
position may also be realized by more complex syntagms which have an adjective as one of their nuclear constituents, and for this reason the position will be called 'adjectival syntagm' .3 The generalized form of this part of the substan-
The same peripheral
tival
syntagm
is
adjectival Among the
syntagm ->
more complex
substantive.
realizations
of the
position
I
a.djecti val
syntagm' are coordinated adjectives, adjectives which themselves are expanded by peripheral elements, and combinations of adjectives which are linked by a conjunction.
Examples: patt lape riche flocc (OR3990) =that hateful rich flock wel bli~e mon (FB698) =a very joyful man weder ful god (FB70t
=very nice weather
swa swide ungemetlice
mycel wind (Pc:1118,16)
[a] so very terribly big wind ;such a terribly strong wind
Seftes ..• leide & lodlike (B356£.) =hateful and loathsome creatures sut,e fair and hende mon (FB156) =a very handsome and friendly man There is a tendency
to place the adjectival
the substantival nucleus. to a reverse arrangement also
be realized
presentation
of may, however, lead
syntagrn in front
Metrical constraints of the ICs. The adjectival
as a discontinuous
of the analysis
constituent.
syntagm The formal
may re-
of the above examples is
adj.synt.: (lat,e riche) --> subst.: flocc4 adj. synt. ; (wel --> blipe) --> subst. ; mon adj. synt. adj.synt.:
: ( ful --> god) --> (swa swi6e ungemetlice
subst. -->
3For
the
4 The
plereme ~att is not included
r11.2.
description
of
the
: weder mycel)
adjectival
in this
-->
syntagm
analysis.
subst.: wind cf.
Chapt.
55 adj.synt.: adj.synt.:
(le1de ((supe
& lodlike)) fair)
subst.: (and hende))
seftes -> subst.:
mon
Elements 1n the 'adjectival syntagm' position co-occur with a number of other peripheral constituents. These include possessive pronouns, demonstrative pronouns, articles, numerals, and indefinite pronouns. EXamples1 possessive
pronouns=S
hore wode wise (ON1029) =their crazy habits leuene mine fadre ~LB3549)
dear my father =mybeloved father
his lemman hende (FB820} his darling pretty =:his pretty love demonstrative
pronouns:
pis hali3 godspel (BH4,1) =this holy gospel l)at rnaide hende (FB478) =that pretty girl articles: 6
a gud mascun (FB326)
;a competent mason pe gret parel (VP12)
=the great peril numerals;
twa gildene scrinen =two gold shrines
fiftene
mycele roden
=15 big crosses
(PC1070,31) tPC1070,32)
5Possessive pronouns usually precede the substantival nucleus, but occasionally- follow it, e.g. wordes his (FB7S7) 'his words' 6Inflected forms of the definite And the indefinite article are quite frequent, e.g. in per sa: (PM83) 'in the sea', l!l'lne bro6er (LBSS6) •a brother•. 6
56
indefinite
pronouns;7
non .•. sturne mon (FB701) •no stern man sum ha11 3 biscop (BH16,6)
=a certain
holy bishop
This evidence suggests that the constituents with an adjecelements either realize at tival nucleus and the other peripheral le~st two different peripheral positions, or they are coordinated in one position. Coordination implies, however, that the coordinated elements belong to the same d1str1but1on class. This 1s not
the case for adJecttval
syntagms and all
the other constituents,
and consequently at least two peripheral pos1t1ons have to be distinguished in the model ·Of the substantival syntagm. The com-
plex
tactic
pansions
relation is parallel determination, because the exdo not determine the nucleus in noticeably different
ways. If none of the expansions illustrated above excluded any of the others, the nwnber of peripheral positions would be identical with the number of expansions. This is certa.inly not the case. As the structure of the PDE substantival syntagm differs from that of the corresponding OE construction, we expect that the EMEsubstantival
.syntagm
will
share
some
properties
with
the
OE con-
struction, some with the PDE. OE possessive and demonatratlve pronouns do not belong to the same paradigmatic class; they CAfl expand the same substantival nucleus. 8 In PDE they exclude each other as expansions of a substantival syntagm. 9 In my EME data
elements of these two classes of pronouns do not occur together, i.e. they realize the same peripheral position. The same position by a definite or indefinite artiele. A8 the8e can also be realized elements serve to pick out a closed set of the objects denoted by the substantive, I shall call this peripheral position 'deicticum • • The generalized form of the model of the substanti val
syntagrn developed so far 1s 7cenerally
indefinite pronouns precede the nucleusJ the neno(n) sometimes follows it, e.g. ioie none (FB106)
8cf.
Pilch
1970:219
9cf.
GCE §4.27
gation particle 'no joy'.
57
deictic}adj.synt. Its
application
> substantive. .
to the examples yields deicticum:
hor}-
adj .synt.:
wode
de1cticwn:
mine}leuene
a.dj. synt.:
the following
> subst.:
>
adj,synt.:
his hend~>
deictic:um:
t,is }-
wise
su.bst.:
7-~-
deicticwn:
subst.
fadre
: lemrnan
subst.;
godspel
hali
>
deicticum:
l:)at }-
adj.synt.:
hende
> subst .. : maide
adj. synt.
deicticum:
adj .synt., deicticwn: adj. synt.:
z
L>
a gu~
pe
gret
subst.:
analyses:
mascun
J->
subst. , parel
As is the case in other periods, in EMEthe semantic relation •possess.ive• can be expressed not only by a possessive pronoun, but also by a substantive in genitive case. This substantive may or may not be expanded. Constructions of this type are called here •genitival syntagm•. 10 This means that a genitival syntagm can also realize the position 'deicticum'.
1°For the description
of this syntactic
type cf. Chapt.IV,4.
58
Exameies:
Godes rice
(BH2,13)
=God's realm
blauncheflures
chaumbre (FB479)
=Blancheflure's
Analysis:
dei.cticum:
chamber
~de},
adj .synt.; deicticumt
subst.,
rice
J
bolaunc:hef lureL>
adj. synt.
1
subst.:
chaumbre
As a rule,
the genitive case is marked by an inflectional ending, although occasionally this ending is lacking (the so-called zero genitive). Such substantival syntagms in 'deicticum' position usually precede the substantival nucleus, but they may also follow it.
f;Xamples: his fader chawnberlein (FB18) =hls father's chamberlain his leue moder luue (PL704) =his dear mother's love blisse daies preo (LLJS) bliss three days =three days• bliss
Analysist
deicticumt adj.synt
•• 0
fade}, subst.:
deicticwn:
his leue mode}
~dj. synt.:
0
deicticwn:
daies
adj.
The function
his
synt.
1
O
of a genitival
postponed possessive
J>
preL
EXample1 pe king his fader
=the king's
father
> subst.:
subst.:
syntagm may also
pronoun. (FB797)
chaumberlein
luue
bliss be indicated
by a
59
Analysis:
deicticum:
pe king
adj.synt.:
0
The positions
occupied
hiL j>
subst.:
fader
pronouns in EMEdiffer somewhat from those possible in POE. In PDE they realize the same peripheral position as possessive pronouns, articles, demonstrative pronouns and genitival syntagms, cf. expensive
my the
by indefinite
books
these the old man's some
In EME, in contrast,
in 'deicticum'
indefinite
pronouns
co-occur
with elements
position.
Examples; sume pa munecas (PC1083,2S) some the monks
=some monks/some of the monks heore ineward vych del
(VP151)
their inwards every part =every part of their inwards
example illustrates a definite article/demonstrative pronoun, the second a genitival syntagrn (zero genitive) in 'deicticwn' position. The nuclei munecas and~ are not marked for case. In EMEsingular case endings had already been dropped with the exception of genitive. Case opposition in the plural was rare, the !-ending having been extended to all plural forms. This resulted 1n the confusion of two formerly distinct constructions. In OE the indefinite pronouns could either realize a peri.pheral position in a substantival syntagrn (e.g. hilderinc sum (Bwf3124) •some/a warrior') or the nuclear position of a pronominal syntagm. In this latter construction the pronominal nucleus could be exsyntagm (e.g. gumena sum (Bwf1499, 'one of panded by a genitival the men'}. OE pronominal syntagms of this type had a partitive meaning. When the case endings were dropped, the two constructions became isomorphic. Only the context al lows us to disambiguate constructions like sume his seferan. 11 It may mean 'some of his The
first
11cf.
the title
of T. Heltve1t*s
article
(cf.
Bibliography)
60
friends', implying that he has more than these, or 'some friends of his• , without further information about the number of this person's friends. As these PDE paraphrases show, one of the meanings was expressed differently after the end of the ME period,
when the construction
'some friends of his' became available. In contexts where the partitive meaning was important, EME had the alternative of an unambiguous pronominal syntagm followed by a prepositional syntagm which was introduced by partitive£! (e.g. sume of dam cnihten (PC1083,17) 'some of the warriors'). The isomorphism of the two constructions explains why in EME indefinite pronouns could co-occur with elements in 'deicticum•
position, Syntagms of the form 'indefinite pronoun + possessive pronoun, article, demonstrative pronoun, or gen1t1val syntagm + ••• + substantive' are syntactically ambiguous in EME. They ma.y rea112e a substantival or a pronominal syntagrn. If they realize a substantival syntagm, the indefinite pronouns are in a different peripheral
position
from 'deicticwn'.
be called 'quantifier', because the elements which realize it denote a certain quantity. After its integration into the model of the substantival syntagm, its generalized form is
quantifie}-
It will
> substantive.
deicticum adj.synt.
The analysis of the examples with indefinite eral position is
quantifier: deicticwn: adj.synt.: quantifier:
non}
0 sturne
sum}-
de1cticum; O adj.synt.2
hali3
>
pronouns
> subst.:
mon
subst.:
biscop
The following syntagms illustrate combinations nouns with elements in the position 'deicticwn'.
in a periph-
of indefinite
Examples: znig his foregenga (PC1087,64) any hi5 predecessor =one of his predecessors/a predecessor
of his
pro-
61
swu his pe3ena {BH18,5) some his servants =one of his servants/a servant
Analysis:
quantifier: deicticurni
ami}his
quantifier: deicticum:
s1his 0
adj.synt.:
adj.synt.s
>
0
>
of his
subst •• foregenga
subst.:
pe 3enz
The examples with numerals in a peripheral position have not yet been analysed. Whereas numerals do not exclude constituents in the position 'adjectival syntagm' and also co-occur with elements in the position 'deicticum', they exclude indefinite pronouns in front of the same nucleus. This means that cardinal numbers and indefinite pronouns realize the same position, namely 'quantifier'. EXamples: hi.re armes tweie
(KH301)
=her two arms
pat on cupe (FB438} =that on.e flower-pot pa twegen kyngas {PC1O7O,45)
•the two kings
The model of the substantival ified to allow the analysis in a peripheral position. Analysis:
quantifier: deicticum: adj.synt.:
quantifier: deicticum:
adj.synt.:
quantifier=
de~cticwn: adJ .synt.:
syntagm does not require to be modof the examples with cardinal numbers
twa
o gildene
0
J-
fiften}-
mycele twei}
hire O
> .!lub.st.:1 serinen
> subst.:
> subst.:
roden
armes
62 quantifier: de~cticum: adJ.synt.:
o}-
pat
quantifier1
twegej-
deicticum: pa adj.synt.:
>
0
There are also substantival three peripheral positions. Example:
Analysis:
kyngas
syntagms with elements in all
tw}-
quantifier: deicticum:
> subst.:
his adj. synt. : gode
from spelling
forms which still
in PDE, it
subst.;
h1s gode kn13tes two (KH49) ;his two brave knights
Since apart flected
cupe
> subst.:
0
kni3tes
differences
and occasional
in-
occur in EME, the forms are the same as
is not necessary
to enwnerate
the individual
items
which belong to the morphological cla.sses demonstrative pronoun, article, and possessive pronoun. The situation is slightly difpronouns. This class comprises the eleferent for the indefinite ments no (variant non), al(le}, an1, nan13, swu(e~, ech, fela, mani, Unlike in PDE, this last element combines with singular and plural forms of substantives. When it expands singular substan12 it has the variant man! a. tives, Examples~ moni wunrnon (ON1393t amany a woman
rnani wisdom ION1756~ :many a wise judgement song mani elne (ON759}
=manya song
selcudes
rnanie (8441)
=manywonders
Mustanoja
(1960:216)
12soth variants
also
includes
are still
oper in his list
current
of indefinite
1n Modern High German. The
EME syntagm moni wummon could be translated ma.nch eine Frau. In PDE only many a survives singular su6stantives.
as manche Frau as an expansion
or of
63
may be justified from a morphological point of view, but the syntactic properties of ober differ from those of the elements mentioned before. It cannot realize the peripheral position 'quantifier•, because it does not exclude an expansion by another element in this position. pronouns.
This
Examples: non oper heuene (FB553) =no other heaven eni oper note (ON557) =any other usefulness sumne oderne mon (BHJ0,27) =some other man fif 7 twenti o6re cnihtes
(PC1124,11f.)
=25 other warriors
As oper also combines with elements in the positions and 'adjectival syntagm', we must conclude that it
separate
peripheral
position
of its
'deicticum'
realizes
a
own.
Ex!!!!eles: deo oder acennednysse {BH4, 15£.) =the other birth pat oper ping (ON784) •that other thing oper wi3te
gente & smale (ON204) =other smart and graceful creatures
Adopting the terminology 'ordinal'
• 13 The enlarged
used in GCE, I shall mode 1 of
the
call
substanti
this val
position
synta.gm
is
now
quantifier deicticum
ordinal
> substantive.
adj.synt.
This version of the model is powerful the examples quoted above.
13cf. GCE§4.22. As in PDE, this ized by ordinal .numbers, e.g. day' ;'on the third day'.
enough for the analysis
position
pe pridde
of
can a1so be real-
day (FB4JO) 'the
third
64
Analysis:
quantifier: deicticum:
ordinal:
O deo
> subst. ; acennednysse
o6er
ad j • synt • : O
quantifier:
0
de1ct1cum.; pat
> subst.;
ordinal: ot;,er adj.synt.: O quantifier: deict.icwn:
ordinal:
ping
non O
> subst.
oper
: heuene
adj • synt. : O
quantifier:
deicticum: ordinal: adj.synt.: quantifier: deicticum1
eni
o
> subst. : note
oper O sumne 0
ordinal: adj,synt,:
oderne O
quantifier: deicticumz
0
fif
> subst. ; mon
1 twenti > subst.:
ordinal: odre adj .synt.: o quantifier: deicticum:
o
ordinal: adj.synt.:
ot,er gente
cnihtes
0
&
smale
It can also be used for the analysis
of more complex construc-
tions.
Examples: an oper clene stede (ON590) =another clean place
65
his an oder castel
(PC1124,6)
•his one other castle mani an oi>er d1rewert,e ston (FB289) many an other precious
stone
~manyother precious stones fela odre godre cnihtes (PC1124,11f.) =many other
Analysis:
valiant
quantifier:
an
deicticum:
O
ordinal.
ot,er .. : clene
adj.synt
quantifier:
his
stede
> subst. : castle
oder .. : 0
quantifier: de1cticum: ordinal: adj.synt.:
> subst.1
an
deicticum:
ordinal: adj.synt
warr.i.ors
mani an 0
> subst.:
ston
oper direwerpe
quantifier; deicticwn:
0
fela:
ordinal : odre adj.synt.z godre
> subst.=
cnihtes
The elements which realize the peripheral positions of the model of the substantival synt.agm developed so far usually precede the nuclus of the construction. They do not exclude expansions
which follow the nucleus. Examples,
po prie kinges of he~enesse {KS26,7) the three kings of the orient
.the three Magi insi 3t in eche songe (ON194) insight into each song =insight into every song one pipe of one smale wode unripe
(ON319f.)
a pipe of a thin wood unripe ;a pipe made of a piece of thin green wood
The relation
between
the
substa.ntiva.l
nuclei
and
the
syntagms
66
which follow
them is easily
established. As there between the two constituents,
is
unilateral
occurrence dependency the tactic relation between them is subordination. The characteristic element of this kind of expansion is a preposition. l 4 The constituents which realize it need not always follow the nucleus; the metrical structure of verse texts, for example, may require a different
arrangement
of the ICs.
Example: of opere[5] songe ••. plaiding supe stronge (ON11f.) of [the) other's song ••. plea very fierce •a very fierce debate about each other's singing Post-nuclear expansions of substantival syntag:ms can also be realized by other constructions. The following syntagms illustrate some of the possibilities.
Examples: pe louerd pat ous hauep wrout (DS112) the Lord that us has wrought =the Lord who made us
o da1 pat he ferde to wde (KH938f.} a day that he went into [the) wood =a day when he went into the wood time for to gonge (HK1739) =time to go ~a adas ... his dohter pamcasere
to gifene
(PC1109,4f.)
the oaths ••• his daughter to-the emperor to give =the oaths to give his daughter to the emperor pi wile ••• pat pu me kni3ti woldest {KH643f.) your will .•. that you me knight would
=your intention
to knight
me
bi-liaue pet·he was diadlich pet diath solde 5Uffr1 for man-ken ~KS27,27f.) belief that he was mortal that death should suffer for mankind =the belief that he waa mortal, that he should suffer death for mankind ore uaste picke hegge Imeind mid spire & grene segge (ON17)
•an impenetrable and green sedge trou wel grete (ON615ff.)
th1c.k hedge interspersed ••• Mid iui
with reeds
grene al bigrowe
tree very big ••. with ivy green all overgrown =a very big tree all overgrown with ivy 14For the description
of the •prepositional
syntagm' cf. Cllapt.IV.1.
67
Considering
the structure of the POE substantival syntagm.1 we expect expansions with finite and others with non-finite verb forms. Among the expansions with finite verb forms, relative 15 clauses are the most frequent 1n English of all perlods. In EHE relative clause expansions do not exclude expansions by preposi-
tional
syntagms.
Examples: se king of gyus pet was i-bore
the king of Jews that was born
(KS26,14f.)
=the king of the Jews who was born
si glorius tel~
seywinge of ure lordes beringe
t>et hol1
godespel
t::;~us
of te day (KS2.7, 16f.) the glorious showing of our Lordts birth that tells the holy gospel of the day
=the glorious heralding of our Lord•s birth the holy gospel of today tells us
us
which
The co-occurrence of these two kinds of expansions makes us enlarge the model of the substant1val syntagm by two more positions. They
will
be
supplement';
'prepositional
la.belled
the generalized
supplement•
and
1
relative
form of the model is now
quantifier
deict1cum ordinal adjectival
> substantive.
syntagm
prepositional supplement relative supplement It allows
the analysis
Analysis:
quantifier: deicticum:
ordinal:
adj.synt.:
O
t,o
o
> subst.:
of het,enesse
of this
16The syntagm is ambiguous; as an expansion
us ber.1.nge.
kinges
O
15For the description terpre~ed
quoted above.
prie
prep ..suppl.:
rel.suppl.:
of some of the examples
syntactic
type cf.
the relative
of the nucleus
se~inge
Chapt.VII.2.
clause can be inor of the nucle-
68
quantifier: deicticum:
ordinal:
O
O
o
>
o
adj.synt.:
subst.:
1nsi3t
prep. suppl . ; in eche songe rel. suppl.: 0
o
quantifier: deicticumt
ordinal:
o
adj. synt.:
one
> subst. : pipe 0
prep.suppl.~ of one smale wode unripe rel.suppl.; O quantifier: 0 deicticum: 0 ordinalt o adj.synt.:
prep.suppl.;
su~e stronge
of ot,ere[s]
rel.suppl.:
0
quantifier; deicticum;
O pe
ordinal:
o
> subst.:
plaiding
> subst.
: l ouerd
songe
adj .synt.: 0 prep. suppl . : 0
pat ous hauet, wrout
rel.euppl.:
quantifier= deicticum:
ordinal:
o
O O > subst • t dai
ad j • s yn t . : 0
prep.suppl.: rel.suppl.,
0
pat he ferde to wude
quantifier: o deicticum! se ordinal: o
adj.synt.: o prep.suppl.: of gyus rel.suppl.: l)et was i-bore
> subst.
: king
69
quantifier: O deicticum: si o ordinal:
adj.synt.:
glorius
prep.suppl.:
of ure lordes beringe ~et us telp ~et
rel.suppl.;
holi
The PDE equivalent
> subst.1
godespel
of the other expansion in GCE. 17 Whereas
is called •appos1t1ve clause' tory ~ is obligatory in appositi
conjunctions
seywinge
ve clauses,
with a finite
verb
in PDE introducEMB allows other
as well, such as hu 'how•. red pat Riwald kinge iwerd dead {LB3910f.) =a hard fate that king Riwald died tidende •.. hu ~eking Rodric his rzflac makede (LB9936ff.) =tidings that King Rodric was ravaging
EXamples: a strong
Appositive clauses commute with appositive infinitive tions And do not exclude expansions by pI"epositional relative clauses.
Examples: tidende
construcsyntAgms or
pat him wzs ser pat icwnen wes Julius
Cezar
(LB7418f.}
tidings that him were unpleasant that come was Julius Caesar =ne~s which was un~leasant to him, that Julius Caesar had arrived paet word .•• paetwas widene cud paet be king Latin 3ef Lauine his douter Eneam to are brude (LB160ff.} that word •.. that was widely known that the king Latin gave Lavine his daughter to-Eneas as a bride =the news which was widely known that King Latin betrothed his daughter Lavine to Eneas The consequence of this situation
is that a new peripheral
tion, which will be labelled 'appos1 ti ve supplement' integrated into the model of the substantival syntagrn. alized form is then
17c£. §13.16£.
posi, must be Its gener-
70
quantifier
deicticum ordinal adjectival syntagm prepositional supplement relative supplement appositive supplement
> substantive.
This now allows us to analyse yet more examples used for the illustration of syntagms 1n ·supplement• pos1t1ons. 18 Analysis:
The 'appositive
infinitive
for to gong~> pa } his dohter pam Casere to gifene
Th.e 'apposi t.i ve supplement' si ti ve clause:
deicticum: app.suppl.:
pi
pat
}
pu me
kn13ti woldest deicticum:
app.suppl.:
is realized
by
subst.= time > suhst.:
1s realized
> subst.
adas
by an appo-
: wile
a
adj . synt. : strong app.suppl.;
position
constructions:
app.suppl.i deicticum: app.suppl.:
supplement•
> subst.
bat Riwald
: rzd
kinge iwerd dead
hu pe king } Rodric his raflac makede
> subst.:
2}-
rel. suppl. 1 t,at him we. app. suppl. ; bat icwnen wes
Julius
Cezar
18From now on empty positions
> subst.
tidende
: tidende
are omitted in th.e analyses.
71 deicticumt
rel.suppl.: app.suppl.:
p.zt
pa:t was widene cud
pzt pe king Latin 3ef
> subst.
Lauine his douter
:word
Eneam to are b:r:ude
The 'appositive supplement• is realized coordinated appositive clauses: app.suppl.:
pet
he was diadlich >
pet diath solde suffri for man-ken
by two
subst.:bl-liaue
In PDE relative clause expansions commute with two kinds of participial constructions: the characteristic element of one is a present participle, that of the other, the past participle of a lexical verb, e.g. a famous poet living in Paris, a famous poem written after the Second World War. Substantival syntagms with past participle constructions as postmodifiers are recorded from 19 the OE period onwards. It is therefore not surprising that my EMEdata provide a substantial number of syntagms which illustrate this postmodifier, Like relative expansions they combine freely supplement' position. 20 with eXPansions in •prepositional an god f locc of p.restess, Sprungenn strenedd ~urrh himm (OR510f.)
=a good flock of priests, ten by him
off
himm,
descended from him, begot-
Visser (1963-73:§1010) also quotes examples ple constructions in peripheral positions
with present particiof substantival syn-
tagms. However, most of his examples illustrate present participles rather than present participle constructions. Both types of expansions
are
recorded
for
EME, too - neither
very
frequently.
Present participles realize the same position as adjectives, namely that of 'adjectival syntagmt. Like adjectives they precede the constructions, on the substanti val nucleus. Present participle 19et. Visser
(1963-73z§1141)
20For the description
of participial
constructions
cf. Cha.pt.VII .J.
72
other hand, fol low the relative expans1ons.21
substanti
val
nucleus;
conunute with
they
EXamples, man! wepinde eie (FB742) =many a weeping eye Lazarus ••• fule pa stincende (BH26,4f.) •Lazarus stinking then foully pe children ••• Wringinde here hende {KH111f.) -the children wringing their hands Apart from ordinary relative clauses the 'relative supplement ' position can thus also be re&lized by participial construct.ions. A fourth syntactic type which realizes the same peripheral
position
is the infinitive
without subject. 22
construction
Example; faire gomes Wip him for to pleie {KH22f.) =noble youths to keep him company This construction occurs also nominal constituents deleted.
in a reduced
form, with one of its
EX!!!!Ple: scheld TO fi3te Wi~ vpon ~ feld (KH513f.) =a shield to fight with on the battle-field
Consequently, for the analysis of the substantival syntagms not yet presented the model need not be modified. It has simply to be born in mind that the label 'relative supplement' must not be identified with 'relative clause in peripheral position•, but that this position can be realized by a variety of constructions. The application of the following analysesi deicticwns
adj.synt.:
rel.suppl.:
ore
uaste
the
picke
Imeind mid spire
adj • synt . : wel -->
rel.suppl.:
model to
&
remaining
examples
}>
grene
grete
segge
L>
Mid iui grene al bigrow:r-
subst.:hegge
subst.:
21cf.fn.20
22This syntactic
type is described
yields
in Chapt.VII.1.
trou
the
73
de1ct1cum: an adj.synt.1
god
of prestess
prep.suppl.: rel. suppl.
1
quantifieri
Sprungen of him
}->
fule pa stlncende
deicticum:
~e Wringinde
rel.suppl.:
To fi3te
rel.suppl.s
eie
1
-->
here hende
> subst.;
wi~ vpon pe feld
L>
faire Wip him for to plei:_i
2. The Adjectival
subst.:
}-
rel.suppl.i
synt.
subst.:
wepinde
rel.suppl.:
adj.
flocc
strenedd pur:rh himrn mani
adj.synt.:
> subst.:
-->
subst.:
Lazarus
children subst.:
scheld
gomes
Syntagm
The label tadjectival syntagm' is motivated by the nucleus which is reali2ed by an adjective. Adjectives of the construction, may be expanded in various ways. The most frequent expansion is hy adverbs.
Examplesi to long (ON343) =too long swide fagen (8415) •very glad ful brihte
-very bright
(PM75)
Most of these adverbs have an intensifying meaning, e.g. to ttoo 1 swide, wel 'very', ~, al •completely', ~, eus 'so', iliche 'equally• , and for this reason the peripheral position they realize in the model of the ad)ectival syntagm will be called '1ntens1f ler • . The adverbs in this pos1 tion can themselves be 1
expanded.
74
Example: al to wlonc (ON489)
:all
too wanton
on principle, adverbs in any position are expandable by other adverbs. This recursiveness would theoretically allow an unlimited sequence of adverbs in the '1ntensif ier' position. In the most is expanded complex syntagm of this type in my data, the adjective by an IC which consists of three adverbs. Example! swa swide ungemetlice rnycel wind (PC1118,16) =a so very terribly strong wind
The generalized form of the model of the adjectival syntagrn which accounts for the syntactic structur-e of the above examples is
intensifier--> Its application to the the following analyses:
intensifier: intensifier; intensifier:
adjective.
adjectival
syntagms
quoted
so far
yields
to-> adjective: long swioe -> adjective: fagen ful --> adjective: brihte
intensifier: (al--> in tens. = ( swa -->
very often
to}-->
{ swi6e -->
adjectives
adjective: wlonc ungemetl ice) ) -->
are nuclei
adj. : mycel
of comparative
construct1ons, in which case the adjective occurs in its comparative form, i.e. its stem is followed by the comparative affix :.!E· There is unilateral occurrence dependency between this affix as nucleus
and several other ICs, e.g. the particle~ 'the' {as in PDE the more, the better) and constructions which are introduced by panne than• (as in PDE purer than gold). 23 The affix -er itself is 1
unilaterally occurrence-dependent on an adjective, and the comparative affix is therefore to be analysed as an expansion of the adjectival nucleus as well as the nucleus of expansions like~ and ~nne-conatruc:tiona.24 23 constructions
of
this
('The Comparative Syntagm• ).
type
are
described
in
Chapt. IV. 3.
24My treatment of comparative-constructions differs from that Mulder proposed for PDE. He considers the comparative affix (or its equivalent more) and the particle than together as one, discontinuous, IC.Tli'Is analysis is basedonthe fallacious assurnp-
75
E>camplesz wisure
pane he (ON1250}
=wiser than he
... panne atter irnaingd mid wine (PM144) =better than poison mixed with wine pe gladur (ON19) =the gladder betere
As
the same adjective
can be preceded by an adverb and folaffix, 1t might seem as if the two con-
lowed by the comparative stituents realized different peripheral positions in the adjectival syntagm. This hypothesis has to be refuted, however; because in constructions of this type the adverb is not an expansion of the adjective, but of the comparative affix.
Examples: hundredfealde
hotter panne is vre {PM249) hundredfold hotter than is ours =a hundred times hotter than ours muchele more and betere panne alle opre pinges· (PM388)
=much bigger
and better
than all
other
things
Thus the model of the adjectival syntagm is also able to account for the analysis of the constructions with adjectival nuclei which are expanded by comparative constructions. Analysis:
adj. : glad
hundred£ ealdJ ( panne
hot
-~>
is vre
J
tion of a bilateral occurrence dependency between the two element:11 "if we take the item more as basic, the item than is compulsory, and, therefore, nottimctional, and if we take than as basic:, more is not functional (Mulder 1980:169t. In PDE, just as in EME,"tli'"e occurrence of the comparative affix (or more) does 0
not depend on the occurrence of a than/paine-construction, cf. ich am J?e blipur euer more (ON1108) ;'""!am t e gayer ever more•. 25The comparative
affix
is
represented
in
a
'no.rmalized'
form. The arrangement of the ICs in the analyses 1s irrelevant as far as possible, it mirrors the arrangement in the data.
J
76
intena.
J->
a ( muchele panne alle opre pin9es
-> Adjectives
adj.:
(muchel
no ~ing -->
adj.
t
unorne
adj.:
long
When an intensifying adverb and a negation particle co-occur in front of the same adjective, the negation particle either modifies only the adverb or a complex nucleus consisting of ~he adverb and ita adjectival nucleus, cf. PDE not very long. Depending on its meaning. this syntagrn has to be analysed as ((not--> very t --> long) or (not --> ( very --> long) } . Similarly, a negation particle which precedes the comparative form of an adjective either expands the comparative affix alone or it expands the complex nucleus which consists of the adjectival nucleus and the comparative affix. The first alternative seems to be appropriate in non-contrastive (=neutral) environments. Example: nowiht lesse
•not less
Analysis:
intens.:
Adjectives
(PM214)
{nowiht ->-er)->
adj.:
can also be expa.nded by prepositional
lytel syntagms. 2 7
Such expansions do not exclude an element in 'intensifier' position and the model of the adjectival syntagm must be enlarged by 26The comparatives~ and betere belong to suppletive paradigms; the corresponding positive forms are muchel and god. 27This syntactic type is described in Chapt.IV.1.
77
one peripheral position, called here •supplement'. 'Prepositional supplement would be too specific a label for this, suggesting that only prepositional syntagms could occupy this position, which i
is not the case. P~onominal syntagrns, substantival synta~, niti val syntagms, in£ initi ve constructions and ~-clauses commute with prepositional syntagms in this position.
geall
Examples; icundur to one frogge (ON85) :more natural to a frog to gredi
of selvere
and of golde
=too greedy for silver oper vniliche
(PM266)
and gold
(PM358)
=unlike the other a meiden ilike (B442)
=resembling a girl eches godes ful (PH369) =full of every good thing 'ir11.1rd1to hauen same (8346) worthy
to have shame
=deserving shame
iwar ~at ho song hire
a bisemar
(ON147f.)
aware that she sang her a mockery f!AWAre of her singing to mock her The elements
position and in •supplement' position do not determine the adjectival nucleus in noticeably different ways. Consequently, the complex tactic relation in the adjecti
in *intensifierr
vAl .synta.gm
construction
i.s
pAral lel
can be represented
1ntens1f1eJsupplement This model accounts presented so far. Analysis:
intens.:
for
determination
suppl.z
the
mode 1 of
> adjective.
the syntactic
structure
of all
examples
-er > adj . : icunde
to of selvere
the
as
suppl . : to one frogge }-
intens.:
and
and of golde }-
> adj.;
gredi
78
}->
intens,; o suppl.: ol;)er intens.:
suppl.:
0
}-
1ntens.: suppl.:
3. The Pronominal
ilike
0
supp l . : eches
suppl.:
vniliche
> adj.:
a meiden
intens.:
intens.
adj.:
=
godes }-
0
to hauen same}-
> adj.s
ful
> adj.:
O
wurdi
}-
~at ho song hire a bisemar
> adj.:
lwar
Syntagm
In the description of the substantival syntagrn it became evident that some pronouns (e.g. possessive, demonstrative, indefinite pronouns) can function as expansions of substa.nti va.l nuclei. In this function the pronouns can themselves be expanded. Al- though these expansions have an intensifying meaning comparable to that of the elements in the peripheral position 'intensifier•
of
the
adjectival
syntagm,
the
peripheral
position
in
the
pronominal syntagrn requires a different label, because there is only partial over lap between the elements which realize the two functions. Moreover, in the pronominal syntagm the choice of posclass of the sible expansions is governed by the m.orphological nucleus. Possessive pronouns can be expanded by o e 'own', demonstrative pronouns by ilke 'same', and indefinite pronouns by elements like to 'too', !,2 'so'. Examples:
pino3e dep (FB200}
=your own death heora agene men (PC1070,9f.) =their own men
79
on pamilcan geare (PC1070,3) =in that same year pat 1lke mon (FB251) =that same man so manie £lures (FB442) •so many flowers to monie tale (ON257) =too many tales
The label •emphasizer' has been chosen for the peripheral position in the model of the pronominal syntagm on the basis of its mnemonic value. Its nuclear position is called 'pronoun', because it can be realized by a number of different pronouns. The general form of the model is then emphasizer->
pronoun.
This model accounts for the syntactic syntagms of the above examples. Analysis:
emphasizer: emphasizer: emphasizer:
of the pronominal
structure
03e --> pronoun: ~in agene --> pronoun: ilcan --> pronoun: ~am ilke -> pronoun: pat so--> pronoun: manie
emphasizer: emphasizer: emphasizer, to-->
heo59
pronoun: monie
If pronominal syntagms are not expansions of substantival to •emsyntagms, their potential expansions are not restricted phasizer• elements; hence the model requires an additional peripheral position. The following examples illustrate some of the realization possibilities. Examples: nucleus=
personal pronoun we •.. sylfe {PC1106,18) =we ourselves vvip hom pat ne mu e from pe schilde a.gainst
=against you
them that
(ON62)
not may from you protect those who cannot protect themselves
heo sat on pe sunne,
Wlp tieres
=completely covered with tears
28This form may either demonstrative pronoun
al blrunne
from
(KH653f.)
she sat in the sun
be analysed as an inflected form of the p~t or of the definite article. The nuclear position of the mode of the pronominal syntagm can also be real-ized by the definite article, e.g. bilke day (FB371) s'on the same day' •
80
nucleus=
demonstrative pronoun se of France (PC1120,2) that of France ;the king of France se ••• pet sent hi to heueriche (PM42) that ••• that sends it to heaven =the person who sends it to heaven nucleus~ indefinite pronoun eall pet he mihte (PC1070,15) all that he might =all he could to feawe of pam folce (BH28,21) •too few of that people feole to fordeme in schynynde wede (SC18) many to condemn in shining weeds =many who are going to be condemned in shining garments
In addition to expansions by means of 'emphasizer' elements, pronouns can be expanded by relative clauses and by participial constructions which con1nute with relative clauses. The syntactic structure of these expansions is identical with that of the corresponding expansions in the 'relative supplement• position of to use the the substantival syntagm and thus it 1s appropriate same label
for t:he peripheral
position
syntagm, too. 29 As 'emphasizer' expans.ions and 'relative supplement' expansions do not exclude each other, the model of the pronominal syntagm with personal pronouns in nuclear position can be represented in
t.he pronominal
as
emphasizer relative It allows
}
supplement
the following
29strictly
speaking,
> pronoun.
analysest
the
same label
should
only be used if
exactly the same realizations "'ere possible in the two syntagms. There is indeed reason to believe that the investigation of further texts wi 11 provide the missing examples, i.e. participial constructions and infinitive constructions without subject in the 'relative supplement' position of pronominal syntagms.
81
emphas.: sylf}-> rel .suppl.: emphas.
i
pron.:
we
O
o
rel.suppl.: emphas. = 0
1__, pron.,
rel. suppl.
emphas.:
: Wip tieres
]-
rel.suppl.:
o
rel.suppl.1
heo
pron.:
hi to heueriche
~et sent
horn
al birunn~
0
emphas.:
pron.:
~at ne mu3e from j)e sohild}-,
>
~et he mihte ]-
pron.
se
>
t
eall
Unlike in PDE, relative expansions are not restricted to pronominal nuclei which are not expansions, A case in point are possessive pronouns which realize the 'deicticum• position in substantival
syntagms.
Examples: pat spek in chirche pat nes no god To his mupe takep pe flod (VP109f.) that spoke in church that not-was good to his mouth goes the flood ~the flodd mounts to the mouths of those who spoke ill in church
and his soule worpe I-schend ~at pe to me
his lif
pis herende haue~ send (DS213f.) his life and his soul be put-to-shame errand =that
has sent per.son I s life
and soul
shame, who sent me this
The pronominal sions
in
DS(213f.)
the his
nuclei
are~
substantival lit and his
errand
ought
in both examples;
who to me this to be put to
they are expan-
syntagms his mupe (VP109f,) soule, respectively.
and
Analysis: emphas.:
rel.suppl.t
O
pat spek in chirche
pat nes god ]-
> pron.:
his
1n
B2
emphas.
=
o
> pron.:
pat pe to me p1s
rel.suppl.:
his .•• his
herende hauep send
A second, more elaborate model has to be developed to account for the syntactic structure of pronominal syntagms with a demonstrative or an indefinite pronoun in nuclear position and which are not expansions ln substantlval syntagms. These nuclei can be expanded not only by syntagms which realize the •relative supplement'
position
(e.g.
relative clauses, infinitive construcbut also by prepositional syntagms.-Thus
tions without subject), the model must also contain a 'prepositional and its generalized form is emphasizer relative supplement
application
to
> pronoun.
supplement
prepositional Its
}
the
relevant
supplement' position,
examples
yields
the
following
analyses: emphas.:
O
rel.suppl.: prep. suppl.: emphas.:
L>
o
of f'ranc:..r--
pron.,
.l ·
to
rel.suppl .. : o prep.suppl.: of pamfolc:..r--
>
emphas.:
L>
rel.suppl.s
0
to fordeme
se
pron.:
prep.suppl •• in schynynde wed:..r--
feawe
pron.,
feole
The early prose texts Old Kent1sh Sermons and The Peterborough Chronicle show traces of the OE construction with an indefinite
pronoun in nuclear
tive of a personal
position
which 1s expanded
or a deictic/demonstrative
Examples: here eurich
(KS34,1) =each of them heora ~le (PC1087,15) =each of them
by the geni-
pronoun.
83
sum para (PC1097,12f.) eone of them These syntagms
cannot
be analysed
above. Their expAnsions realize which might be called •partitive'. disappearing
in EME; it
with any of the models presented a separate peripheral position
The construction
was replaced
either
by the
was already coordination
of two pronouns (e.g. hi sume (PC1101,8) ;;'some of them'; pet ... eall (PC1070,19f.) ='all th1s or by pronominal syntagrns with an 1
)
expansion in 'prepositional supplement• position. It is, therefore, simpler and more appropriate to describe this archaic construction separately rather than to burden the model of the pronominal syntagm with an additional peripheral position. A further simplification of the description of the pronominal syntagm is to be expected from a closer investigation of the syntax of the personal and possessive pronouns. As soon as exam-
ples appear in which these pronouns are expanded by prepositional syntagms, er' ,
the model with the three
•relative
supplement'
, and
peripheral
positions
•prepositional
account for all occurrences of the construction, of its nuclear position. the realization
'emphasiz-
supplement•
irrespective
w11 l
of
IV. FUNCTIONAL SYNTAGMS
The syntactic function of a linguistic entity can be indicated by several means, such as wordorder or function.al markers. For markers which indicate the syntactic function of constituents of an utterance, Martinet coined the term monemes fonctionnels Mulder uses the more general label 1 functionalf for such syntactic entities. Functionals are the nuclei of ' functional syntagms • • The label 1 functional syntagm' is thus a cover-term for a whole class of syntagms with the common property that their syntactic 1
function
is indicated
the
functional nature of this
the potential
1.
by a special
1
plereme. The classification
syntagm.s of EME:presented
The prepositional
of
here
is based on the plereme which determines
plereme. It is also this realizations of the IC in the peripheral
The Prepositional
•
position.
Syntagm syntagm consists
of two ICs. One of them 1s a preposition, while the other may be realized by different constructions, the most frequent being that of e. substantlval syntagm. The two ICs are mutually occurrence-dependent and hence the syntactic relation between them is either that of subordination or of interordination. The following syntagrn is used here to demonstrate that it is the preposition which indicates the syntactic function of the prepositional syntagm.
of to day us telp i>et a bred.ale was i-maked ine po londe of ierusalem (KS29,Bf.)
Exampler l)et holi godspel
=the Holy Gospel of today tells
celebrated
us that a wedding was
in the land of Jerusalem
The first prepositional syntagm of the example is of to day. The adverb to day •today• can have different syntactic functions; very frequently it is used as an adverbial complement (of a clause).
85
Here the preposition g,! indicates that the prepositional syntagm of to da~ is an expansion of the substant1val nucleus godsEel. Similarly, the preposition 1ne indicates that the prepositional syntagm ine po londe of ierusalem is an adverbial complement of the clause a bredale was i-maked. Thus the preposition is the syntactically dominant part of the construct.ion, and the relation between its ICs is subordination, with the preposition as nucleus
and the other IC as actualiser. The general form of the model of the prepositional syntagm may be symbolized as preposition verb
Syntagm
negatio}modal tense
> copula.
3. The Passive Syntagm negation/ modal
tense}
4. The Substantival
> (passive
Syntagm
quantifier deicticum ordinal adjectival syntagm prepositional supplement relative supplement appositive supplement 5. The Adjectival
1ntensif1e}supplement
copula
Syntagm
>
adjective.
> substantive.
past
participle)
196
6. The Pronominal Syntagm emphasizer } relative supplement prepositional supplement 7. The Prepositional
> pronoun.
Syntagm
preposition