138 58 20MB
English Pages 424 [431] Year 2019
APPROACHES TO CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHIATRY
APPROACHES TO
CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHIATRY Edited by
Jane M.
Murphy
and
Alexander H. Leighton
Cornell University Press ITHACA,
NEW YORK
Copyright
©
1965
by Cornell University
Press
All rights reserved
CORNELL UNIVERSITY PRESS First published
1965
Library of Congress Catalog Card Number: 65-13631
PRINTED IN THE UNITED STATES OF AMERICA BY VAIL-BALLOU PRESS, INC.
Preface
THIS
work, reporting on methods and concepts for comparamental
tive studies of
outgrowth of
efforts
illness in different cultural
groups,
is
the
sponsored by the Cornell Program in Social
Psychiatry to explore and understand the nature of some of the
problems confronting research in have approached
this topic
this field.
The
contributors
through reviews of the
literature,
The exploratory field among studies were carried out Eskimos, Navahos, and Mexicans, as well as British and Acadian French in Nova Scotia. In the early 1950's, two epidemiological studies were undertaken in northeastern America. One study was concerned with seminar discussions, and
a rural
county
in
trial investigations.
Maritime Canada. This, the Stirling County
Study, was formulated and directed
The
by Alexander H. Leighton.*
research was conducted through Cornell's Department of
Sociology and Anthropology on the campus
York.
The
other epidemiological study, the
Study, concerned an area in metropolitan
by Thomas A.
C. Rennie,t
who
Ithaca,
New
Midtown Manhattan
New
directed
at
it
York. Instituted
until his death in
Other chief investigators in the Stirling Study were B. P. Dohrenwend, J. S. Harding, C. C. Hughes, D. C. Leighton, D. B. Macklin, the late A. M. Macmillan, the late G. E. McCreary, J. M. Murphy, A. L. Nangeroni, R. N. Rapoport, and M.-A. Tremblay. f For the Midtown Study, the other main investigators were P. Kirkpatrick, T. S. Langner, S. T. Michael, M. K. Opler, V. Rubin, L. Srole, and A. Weider. *
v
Preface
vi
it was based at the Cornell University Medical College in York City. The main results of the two studies have been reported in a series of volumes which give detailed presentations
1956,
New
of theoretical orientations, methods employed, and analysis of findings.* After Rennie's death, the Stirling
and Midtown Studies
were joined under the directorship of Alexander H. Leighton and the name "Cornell Program in Social Psychiatry" was adopted.
The two
investigations of populations in
were conceived with somewhat
Western culture
similar frames of reference.
Although each faced unique problems and pursued
a separate
course of data gathering and analysis, the exchange of ideas in defining concepts
and developing methods of research gave
a measure of comparability to the
was
interest in
the full range
two
studies. In
both there
of psychiatric disorders from
psychoses and mental deficiency to psychoneuroses and behavior disturbances. it
From
the theoretical viewpoint, however,
seems probable that psychoneurotic and psychophysiologic
symptoms
are the kinds
most directly under the influence of
different sociocultural experiences
with significant distribution tionalized groups.
pay
The
and most
likely to be
in nonhospitalized
found
and noninstitu-
investigators determined, therefore, to
special attention to discovering the mild as well as the
severe forms of mental
illness.
To
accomplish
this,
questionnaire
interviewing was employed as one means of case-finding in each of the studies. In Stirling County, a fishing, lumbering, and subsistence-
County Study: A. H. Leighton, My Name Is Legion York: Basic Books, 1959); C. C. Hughes, M.-A. Tremblay, R. N. port, and A. H. Leighton, People of Cove and Woodlot (New Basic Books, i960); D. C. Leighton, J. S. Harding, D. B. Macklin, Macmillan, and A. H. Leighton, The Character of Danger (New Basic Books, 1963). Midtown Study: L. Srole, T. S. Langner, S. T. Michael, M. K. and T. A. C. Rennie, Mental Health in the Metropolis (New * Stirling
(New RapoYork: A. M. York: Opler,
York:
McGraw-Hill, 1962); T. S. Langner and S. T. Michael, Life Stress and Mental Health (New York: The Free Press of Glencoe, 1963).
Frejace
vii
farming region inhabited by two main ethnic groups, English
and French Acadian, the research program was concerned with studying community
The
life in
its
analysis of sociocultural
background
in cultural
total
impact upon individuals.
environment focused on contrasts
well as on differences in degree of
as
and "disintegration." The concepts of
"social integration"
tegration and disintegration provided a
framework for
in-
selecting
communities displaying both adequate and inadequate functioning, the features of
which were predicted
to have differen-
on mental health and mental illness. Hence social disintegration was considered to be a pattern or combination of a number of noxious conditions. The criteria by which this
tial
effect
process was identified included such factors as poverty, secularization,
cultural confusion, fractured social relationships, and
rapid sociocultural change. Other components of the process are inadequacies in
leadership,
recreation,
com-
associations,
munication, and control of crime and delinquency.
The aim was tions
to find out
whether there are important correla-
between the prevalence of psychiatric symptoms and
residence in integrated or disintegrated communities. Other aspects of
were
life
experience, such as age, sex, and ethnic identity,
also investigated.
surveyed to
elicit
A probability sample
of 1,010 adults was
sociocultural information and data
of both a physical and psychiatric nature.
The
physicians were also interviewed about the
on health
county's local
members
of the
sample, and mental and other hospital rec-
statistically selected
ords were considered
when they
pertained to the sample
mem-
bers.
The Midtown
Study,
already noted, dealt with a section
as
of Manhattan in the city of
study area,
Midtown
is
a
New
York. In contrast to the rural
much more complex and
society with several ethnic groups represented:
heterogeneous Irish,
German-Austrian, Hungarian, Czechoslovakian, Polish,
Old American, and tions
others.
Broad diversity
exists in the
Italian,
British,
occupa-
and professions present, and the span of economic
levels
Preface
viii
extremely wide. In
is
status"
this
was employed
as a
order to uncover
in
study the concept of "socioeconomic
way
of quartering the environment
sociocultural
patterns
which
with the distribution of psychiatric symptoms.
A
correlate
number of
the elements of social disintegration used in the Stirling Study
were given attention stress"
was analyzed
in the
Midtown
in terms of
homes, economic deprivation, alence survey in
"life
broken
as
and so on. The prev-
health,
ill
Midtown was
Study. For example,
components such
carried out with a probability
sample of 1,660 adults.
Allowing for the many eralization
some
studies,
The
limitations to
comparison and gen-
imposed by the differences of procedure
conclusions are nevertheless
tentative
differences
in the
environment encompassed by
in
two
possible.
the
terms
"urban" and "rural" and by the divisions of Western society into ethnic groups such as French, English, Irish, and Italian
did not parallel major differences in the prevalence of psychiat-
disorder in the
ric
two sample
populations.
Psychoneurotic
symptomatology formed an overwhelming part of the ture of disorders
among
total pic-
these nonhospitalized people, and
it
was much more prevalent than had been suspected. In the Stirling
Study the factors of advancing
and of low occupational living in a disintegrated rates of
symptoms.
age, of being a
woman,
position, as well as the experiences of
environment were correlated with high
Among
the
Midtown
adults,
advancing age
and low economic position had similar relationships. This platform of trends,
Western and developing
as well as general interest in
non-
countries, raised several questions for
exploration in contrasting groups: 1.
is
What
kinds of psychiatric disorders are there and what
the actual prevalence in groups that practice life-ways very
different
from those of the West? That
cultural contrasts than those
found
in
is
to say, will greater
Midtown and
Stirling
bring to light significant qualitative and quantitative differences?
Preface
Do
2.
the apparent influences of age, sex, and occupational
position hold
up
residence have
cross-culturally?
when looked
How may
3.
ix
rural or
urban
sociocultural disintegration be defined cross-
and does
culturally,
What effects do
at in cross-cultural perspective?
it
continue to have the same association
with prevalence of psychiatric disorders?
What
4.
tensive
all
the effect of cultural change, so rapid and ex-
is
around the world, on these interrelated factors? In
other words,
do the patterns of
acculturation,
and low socioeconomic position have
social
rapid
disintegration,
similar
relevance for the epidemiology of disorders in non- Western populations?
As
a start
toward investigating cross-cultural problems of
type,
many
questions of concepts, theory, methods, and feasi-
bility
had to be reviewed by members of the Cornell Program.
According
to interests
this
and opportunities, the contributors chose
particular topics
and decided on various ways of exploring them.
The approaches
entailed field studies as well as
and
libraries;
and
this
book
is
one
work
in clinics
result.
The volume has been organized so common theme are grouped together.
that
chapters with a
In order to link the
chapters to the overall purpose of the book, the editors have
prepared an introductory the chapters.
comment preceding
These two chapters, the
general orientations written first
by one
first
all
but two of
and the eighth, are
or both of the editors.
The
chapter, "Cross-cultural Psychiatry," concerns history of
the field, definition of terms, and a
be approached.
It
map
of the problems to
opens Part One, which focuses predominantly
on questions of identifying and surveying for psychiatric orders.
cultural jMethods for Psychiatric Research," begins Part
which
dis-
Chapter VIII, "Social Science Concepts and Cross-
deals
Two,
mainly with sociocultural approaches useful to cross-
cultural psychiatric research.
Following the
first
chapter in Part
One
are
two
chapters
x
Preface
concerning identification of disorders in societies with cultures
very different from that of the West. other in that Chapter
II
They complement
devoted to disorder
is
Western psychiatric terms, while Chapter indigenous conceptions of
illness
III
each
defined in
as
addressed to
is
and deviance. The next two
chapters take up questions about certain indicators that might
be employed to estimate the prevalence of disorder in different cultures.
Chapter
IV
discusses a questionnaire,
which
with the possibility of employing physiological
The
indicate psychiatric disorder. also
deal with a
common
problems of establishing
and children. Their of the
work done
Two
Part
is
They concern among
VII,
the special adolescents
heightened by the fact that most
consists of four chapters.
The
sideration to the socially shared
Chapter VIII,
first,
IX concerns
an instrument for intensive studies of
with a focus on analysis of cultural
ill
roles.
the
life
story
and well individuals
Chapter
X
gives con-
sentiments characteristic of
and discusses what meaning they
different groups of people
may
might
so far has dealt with adults.
has been mentioned above. Chapter as
deals
VI and
criteria of disorder
interest
V
tests that
next chapters,
theme.
largely
is
concerned with psychophysiologic symptoms. Chapter
have for mental health and mental
illness.
Chapter XI
is
the report of an exploratory field study in which certain of
were given trial and in up between some kinds of psy-
the previously described techniques
which
correlations are pointed
symptoms and some kinds of social experiences. described in this volume was carried out between 1956 and i960. In 1961 some of the investigators began a com-
chiatric
The work
prehensive epidemiological study of a rural area in Nigeria. Dr.
T. Adeoye Lambo, tendent of the
who was
Aro Hospital
for
at that
time Medical Superin-
Nervous Diseases
in the
West-
ern Region of Nigeria, was co-director of this study, which
was
called the Cornell-Aro
Many in this
Mental Health Research Project.
of the methods of data gathering and analysis discussed
book were applied
in the Nigerian study,
and further
Preface effort
was made to
xi
substantiate their validity and reliability.*
Our coverage of the field in this volume is, of course, partial selective. Taken together, however, the chapters deal with number of technical questions both conceptual and meth-
and a
odological that are germane to a wide variety of problems re-
garding the relationship of sociocultural environment to psychiatric disorder.
Our hope
is
that they will be useful to others
approaching cross-cultural psychiatry.
Jane M. Murphy Neiv York,
New
York
April, 196$
* A. H. Leighton, T. A. Lambo, C. C. Hughes, D. C. Leighton, J. M. Murphy, and D. B. Macklin, Psychiatric Disorder among the Yoruba: A Report from the Cornell- Aro Mental Health Research Project (Ithaca,
N.Y.: Cornell University Press, 1963).
Acknowledgments
ALTHOUGH
this
volume comprises the individual work of
fourteen contributors,
it
represents a
common
enterprise largely
undertaken within one framework. This general acknowledg-
ment therefore supplements the specific acknowledgments appear in the notes appended to the separate chapters.
The
book has been sponsored by
research reported in this
the Cornell
Program
in
that
Social Psychiatry, which, in turn,
administratively attached to the
Department of Psychiatry
is
in
Department
the Cornell University Medical College and to the
of Sociology of the College of Arts and Sciences in Cornell
when
University. During most of the time
this
volume was
being developed and written, the Program was also attached to
Department of Anthropology.
the
The cial
Cornell Program in Social Psychiatry has received finan-
aid
from
a
number
The work
of institutions.
been mainly supported by
a
Foundation's Behavioral Science Division.
The
Cornell Program
however, an outgrowth of the Stirling County Study and
is,
the a
herein has
program grant from the Ford
Midtown Manhattan Study. Both
these Studies constitute
fundamental background for the projects described in
book.
The
Stirling
County Study was
derived primarily from four sources:
Fund, the Carnegie Corporation of xiii
this
carried out with funds
the Milbank Memorial
New
York, the Depart-
Acknowledgments
xiv
ment of National Health and Welfare of Canada, and partment of Public Health of the Province of
The Midtown Manhattan Studv was
the
Nova
De-
Scotia.
given financial aid from
the National Institute of Mental Health in the United States
Public Health Service, the Milbank Memorial Fund, the Grant
Foundation, the Rockefeller Brothers Fund, and the Corpora-
Company. These foundations
tion Trust
of course,
are not,
book and
the authors, owners, publishers, or proprietors of this are not to be understood as approving,
bv
virtue of their grants,
any of the statements made or views expressed
therein.
In addition to this general support, the following
The work
noted.
by
represented in Chapter
II
to be
is
was made
possible
fellowships granted to Charles Savage and Alexander
H.
Leighton from the Center for Advanced Study in the Behavioral Sciences.
Alaska,
A
study of Eskimos on
referred to throughout this
is
in Chapters
II,
III,
IV, VII, and IX.
St.
Lawrence
book and
Island,
specifically
The 1954-195 5
investiga-
was conducted by Charles C. Hughes and Jane M. Murphv from funds supplied by the Social Science Research Center of Cornell University and by Dr. and
tion of this population
Mrs. Rex Murphv.
The
1940 studv was carried out by Alex-
ander H. Leighton and Dorothea C. Leighton on a joint fellow-
from the Social Science Research Council. The study of
ship
VI was supported
adolescent
svmptoms reported
by
from the Health Research Council of the City of
a grant
New
York and
Council
is
in
The Health Research
backing to the study of child-
Chapter VII. Chapter
X
reports
comparative analvsis of materials gathered in the Stirling
County Study and Funds for the poration of
the
New
statistical
Program
Cornell-Navaho Southwest Project.
latter project
were given by the Carnegie Cor-
York.
Several chapters of this
of
Chapter
the Foundations Fund.
also giving financial
hood svmptoms described a
in
book have been written
in the light
guidance given by consultants to the Cornell
in Social Psychiatry. Special
acknowledgment
is
there-
Acknowledgments
xv
fore given to Melvin S. Schwartz, David B. Macklin, and John S.
Harding.
This book has been reviewed in whole or in part by a number of colleagues.
Some
of these reviews have been
by authors
of one chapter for authors of other chapters. Outside the
list
of
contributors and statistical consultants, grateful acknowledg-
ment
is
made
especially to Dr.
William T. Lhamon, Professor
and Chairman, Department of Psvchiatry, Cornell University Medical College, and Dr. Robin M. Williams, Sociology, Cornell University, editorial
who
serve as
Jr.,
Professor of
members of an
board for the Cornell Program in Social Psychiatry.
Dr. Allan R. Holmberg, Professor and Chairman of the De-
partment of Anthropology, and Dr. Gordon Streib, Professor
and Chairman of the Department of Sociology, both of Cornell University, have also reviewed the manuscript. For reading and
commenting on to
particular chapters,
appreciation
is
expressed
Robert Ascher, Charlotte Babcock, David V. Becker, Laurel
H. Hodgden, Alice L. Nangeroni, and Peter Stokes. Technical assistance has been given by Veronica A. Shaw and Marlene Mandel. Administrative and secretarial help has come from Amorita Suarez, Dee Watt, Norma Bain, and Donna Hamilton.
For
all
this aid
and
express sincere thanks.
assistance, the editors
and the authors
Biographical Notes on Contributors
Gloria
J.
tice. A4rs.
Berk
is
presently a psychotherapist in private prac-
Berk received
lege School for Social
a master's degree
Work. Before
from the Smith Col-
joining the staff of the
Payne Whitney Adolescent Psychiatric Out-Patient Clinic of the
New
York
Hospital, she participated in the
Bureau of Mental Hygiene Co-ordinated
work
in Hartford, Connecticut,
Community Mental Health
of the
and the
Clinics of Brooklyn.
Charles C. Hughes, Professor of Anthropology and Director of the African Studies Center at Michigan State University, received his Ph.D. degree in anthropology from Cornell University. Dr. Hughes has been a Fellow at the Center for Advanced Study in the Behavioral Sciences and has done research in
Nova
publications include
Scotia,
Alaska,
An Eskimo
(Cornell University Press,
Liberia,
and Nigeria. His
Village in the
i960)
field
Modern World
and co-authorship of Peo-
Cove and Woodlot (Basic Books, i960) and Psychiatric Disorder among the Yoruba (Cornell University Press, 1963). He has published articles on Eskimo culture, cultural change and social psychiatry in such journals as the American Anthropolople of
gist,
Current Anthropology
Thomas
S.
Langner,
ciology)
at
the
New
,
and the Journal of Social
Issues.
Assistant Professor of Psychiatry
York University School xvii
(So-
of Medicine,
Biographical Notes on Contributors
xviii
received
his
Ph.D. in sociology from Columbia University.
Dr. Langner has done anthropological research in Mexico and
American Southwest; race relations research for the AntiDefamation League; propaganda analvsis for the Voice of the
He
America, and studies of the aged. a
program of
sity,
is
currentlv engaged in
interdisciplinarv research at
New
York Univer-
Department of Psvchiatry. His publications include con-
tributions to
many
scholarlv journals as well as co-authorship
Hundred Over
of Five
Sixty
(Russell
Sage,
1956), Mental
Health in the Metropolis (McGraw-Hill, 1962), and Life and Mental Health (The Free Press of Glencoe, 1963).
Alexander H. Leighton, Director Social Psychiatry,
is
of the Cornell
Stress
Program
in
Professor of Psvchiatry (Social Psychia-
try) at the Cornell University Medical College and Professor
of Sociologv and Professor of Anthropology at the College of
Arts and Sciences, Cornell University. Dr. Leighton received his
medical degree from Johns Hopkins Medical School.
Advanced Study
has been a Fellow at the Center for
Behavioral Sciences and the holder of
from the Carnegie Corporation of
The Governing
include 1945),
1949),
Men
of
Human Relations in My Name Is Legion
tion to Social Psychiatry
a
New
a
He
in the
Reflective Fellowship
York. His publications
(Princeton University Press,
Changing World (E.
(Basic Books, 1959),
P.
An
Dutton,
Introduc-
Thomas, i960), and coWoodlot (Basic Books, i960),
(Charles C.
authorship of People of Cove and
The Character of Danger (Basic Books, 1963), and Psychiatric among the Yoruba (Cornell University Press, 1963).
Disorder
Dorothea
C.
Leighton
is
Clinical Associate Professor of Psy-
chiatry (Social Psychiatry) at the Cornell University Medical
College and Senior Research Associate and Lecturer in the De-
partment of Sociology and in the Department of Anthropology of the College of Arts and Sciences, Cornell University. Dr.
Leighton received her medical degree from Johns Hopkins Medical
School and has done
field
research
among
the Xavaho, Zuni,
Biographical Notes on Contributors St.
xix
Lawrence Eskimos, and Yoruba. She has been doing epidemioCounty Study, now a part of the
logical research in the Stirling
Her publications include The Navaho Door (Harvard University Press, 1944), The Navaho (Harvard University Press, 1946), Children of the People (Harvard University Press, 1947), The
Cornell Program in Social Psychiatry.
co-authorship of
Character of Danger (Basic Books, 1963), and Psychiatric Disorder among the Yoruba (Cornell University Press, 1963).
Edward Llewellyn Thomas macology
at the
is
Associate Professor of Phar-
University of Toronto and Medical Research
Associate at Ontario Hospital,
New
Toronto.
He
received his
medical training at McGill University. Before the war, he trained as an engineer at the University of
London and
is
at
present carrying on his research at the Institute of Bio-medical Electronics, a joint project
between the Faculty of Medicine
and the Faculty of Engineering
Thomas
is
to
University of Toronto. Dr.
also a part-time Professor of
versity of Waterloo,
man
at the
Psychology
where he has been teaching
Factors Engineering in Canada.
He
at the
Uni-
a course in
Hu-
has contributed articles
Science, Canadian Medical Association Journal,
Canadian
Psychiatric Association Journal, Radiology, Canadian Journal of
Psychology
,
Journal of Aerospace Medicine, and the Journal
of Projective Techniques.
Masterson,
James
F.
at the
Cornell University Medical College,
Clinical Associate Professor of Psychiatry is
Director of the
Research Project on Psychiatric Disorder in Adolescents and is
in charge of the Adolescent Division of the
Payne Whitney
Out-Patient Department. Dr. Masterson received
his
degree from Jefferson Medical College and combines search interests with part-time practice in psychiatrv.
medical his
re-
He
has
contributed to journals such as Psychiatry, Journal of Nervous
and Mental Disease, American Journal of Psychiatry and The Medical Clinics of North America. ,
Biographical Notes on Contributors
xx
Jane M. Murphy is Assistant Professor of Anthropology in the Department of Psychiatry of the Cornell University Medical College and Research Associate in the Department of Sociology and in the Department of Anthropology, College of Arts and Sciences in Cornell University. Dr.
Murphy
received
her Ph.D. degree in anthropology from Cornell University and has done field
work
in Alaska,
Nova
Scotia,
and Nigeria. She
contributed to the research of the Stirling County Study and is
currently with the Cornell Program in Social Psychiatry.
Her
An
publications include collaboration on
in the
Modern World
Eskimo Village
(Cornell University Press,
authorship of Psychiatric Disorder
among
the
i960), co-
Yoruba (Cornell
University Press, 1963), and articles on cross-cultural psychiatry and psychotherapy in
World Mental Health,
Memorial Fund Quarterly, and Magic,
by Ari Kiev (The Free
Seymour Parker,
Faith,
the Mil bank
and Healing, edited
Press at Glencoe, 1964).
Associate Professor of Anthropology and
Associate Professor of Social Science at Michigan State University,
was formerly
a cultural anthropologist in the
ment of Psychiatry of the Jefferson Medical
Depart-
College.
He
received his Ph.D. degree in anthropology from Cornell University,
and
his field
work
Mexico, and Alaska. social structure of a
working on
a
includes research in
He
Nova
Scotia,
New
has also participated in studying the
mental hospital in England, and
is
currently
study of mental health among Negroes in Phila-
delphia. Dr. Parker has contributed to
many
journals such as the
American Anthropologist, American Sociological Review, Psy-
Human
chiatry, Ethnology,
and
Tom
Associate Professor in the Department of
T. Sasaki
is
Sociology, University of
New
Relations.
Mexico. His Ph.D. degree in
sociology was received from Cornell University. field
work among
the
Navaho and
has served as a social science consultant to the
He
has done
Apache and Navaho Indian
the Jicarilla
Biographical Notes on Contributors
Tribe
xxi
well as to various training and extension programs in
as
American Southwest. Dr. Sasaki was formerly Field Di-
the
rector of the Cornell-Navaho Project in Technological Change.
He
the author of Fridtland,
is
munity
in
Transition
has published
articles
New
Mexico:
New
Business,
Charles Savage
is
Navaho Com-
(Cornell University Press,
i960)
and other
Issues,
Rural Sociology,
journals.
Director of Research, Spring Grove State
Hospital, Baltimore, Maryland. Dr. Savage received his ical
and
about the American Indians in the Ameri-
can Anthropologist, Journal of Social
Mexico
A
degree from the University of Chicago and
is
med-
a graduate
Washington School of Psychiatry and a member of the American Psychoanalytic Association. He has been a Fellow at the Center for Advanced Study in the Behavioral Sciences; earlier he was Acting Chief of the Adult Psychiatric Branch of the
of the National Institute of Mental Health.
experience
among
the
Navahos
in
New
He
has had field
Mexico and the Yoruba
of Nigeria and has contributed papers to numerous journals
such
as
American Journal of Psychiatry, Journal of Nervous
and Mental Disease, Archives of Psychiatry and Neurology Psychoanalytic Review, Diseases of the Nervous System, and Psychiatry.
Marie-Louise Schoelly
is
Clinical Assistant Professor of Psy-
chiatry in the Division of Child Psychiatry of the Cornell University Medical College. She received her medical education at
the University of Zurich, Switzerland, and took her psychiatric
residency at the Psychiatric University Clinic in Basel, Switzerland.
She
is
currently engaged in research concerning psychiat-
symptoms in childhood and also practices psychiatry in New York City. Dr. Schoelly has written on the use of curare in electroshock therapy and on psychosis associated with tetany in such journals as the Journal of American Physical Medicine and the Monthly Review of Psychiatry and Neurology. ric
Biographical Notes on Contributors
xxii
Albert
Sherwin
C.
is
Clinical
Associate Professor of Psy-
chiatry in the Department of Psychiatry and Director of the
Division of Child Psychiatry in the Cornell University Medical College.
He
received his medical degree from Columbia Uni-
versity Medical College of Physicians and Surgeons. In addition
to a part-time practice, Dr.
Sherwin
doing research on child-
is
hood symptoms. He has published articles on the relationship between psychopathology and music as well as papers on various psychiatric disorders found in children in journals such as
American Journal of Psychiatry, Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, Bulletin of the New York Academy of Medithe
cine,
and the Journal of Chronic Diseases.
Kenneth
F.
Tucker, who received
Cornell University Medical College,
his is
medical degree from
a Clinical Instructor in
Psychiatry at that institution. Currently he devotes the major part of his time to the private practice of psychiatry.
He
has
participated in the research on adolescence conducted at the
Payne Whitney
Clinic.
Journal of Psychiatry
He
has contributed to the
American
Contents
Preface,
by Jane M. Murphy
v
Acknowledgments
xiii
Biographical Notes on Contributors
xvii
Part One. Identifying and Surveying Psychiatric Disorders I
Cross-cultural Psychiatry
BY ALEXANDER II
The Problem
H.
LEIGHTON AND JANE M.
MURPHY
3
of Cross-cultural Identification of
Psychiatric Disorders
BY CHARLES SAVAGE, ALEXANDER DOROTHEA III
C.
H.
LEIGHTON, AND
LEIGHTON
2
Native Conceptions of Psychiatric Disorder
BY JANE M. MURPHY AND ALEXANDER
IV
1
The Use
H.
LEIGHTON
Symptoms as Indicators among Eskimos BY JANE M. MURPHY AND CHARLES C. HUGHES
64
of Psychophysiological
of Disorder
V
The
Possibility of
108
Using Physiological Indicators
for Detecting Psychiatric Disorder
BY EDWARD LLEWELLYN THOMAS xxiii
l6l
Contents
xxiv
VI
Some
Criteria of Psychiatric Disorder in Adolescents
BY JAMES
MASTERSON, KENNETH TUCKER, AND
F.
GLORIA BERK
VII
87
Criteria of Psychiatric Disorder in Children
BY ALBERT
Part VIII
I
Two.
SHERWIN AND MARIE-LOUISE SCHOELLV
C.
219
Assessing the Socio cultural Environment
Social Science Concepts
and Cross-cultural Methods for
Psychiatric Research
BY JANE M. MURPHY N
IX
The
I
Life History in Cross-cultural Psychiatric Research
BY CHARLES
X
25
HUGHES
C.
Society and
Sentiments in
285
Two
Contrasting Socially
Disturbed Areas
BY SEYMOUR PARKER AND TOM
XI
Psychophysiological in
Two
BY THOMAS
T.
SASAKI
Symptoms and
329
the Status of
Women
Mexican Communities S.
LANGNER
360
Concluding Note
BY ALEXANDER Index
H.
LEIGHTON AND JANE M.
MURPHY
393
398
Charts
IV- 1
IV-2
Age-sex distribution of the Bristol Ecological Area population (Ninth Census of Canada, 1951)
151
Age-sex distribution of Sivokak village for 1955
152
Figures IV- 1
Frequency diagram based on raw data of the
Stirling
weighted scores for three samples IV-2
Frequency
distributions of
villagers, Stirling
HOS
community
127
scores for residents,
Eskimo
and
Stirling
neurotics with best-fitting normal curves
IV-3
Comparison of Eskimo and
IX- 1
Schematic representation of data-gathering techniques
IX-2
Role
"nesting"
as
Stirling response patterns
illustrated
130
286
by Eskimo apprentice
hunter, hunter, and boat captain
XXV
128
298
Charts
IV- 1
IV-2
Age-sex distribution of the Bristol Ecological Area population (Ninth Census of Canada, 1951)
151
Age-sex distribution of Sivokak village for 1955
152
Figures IV- 1
Frequency diagram based on raw data of the
Stirling
weighted scores for three samples IV-2
Frequency
distributions of
villagers, Stirling
HOS
community
127
scores for residents,
Eskimo
and
Stirling
neurotics with best-fitting normal curves
IV-3
Comparison of Eskimo and
IX- 1
Schematic representation of data-gathering techniques
IX-2
Role
"nesting"
as
Stirling response patterns
illustrated
130
286
by Eskimo apprentice
hunter, hunter, and boat captain
XXV
128
298
Tables
IV-
1
IV-2
Eskimo Health Opinion Survey questionnaire Eskimo
HOS
scores, psychiatric
ABCD
1 1
ratings,
and
independent evidence of svmptom patterns
XI- 1
Twenty-two Item Score
XI-2
Number
142
373
of interviews in Mexico City
by
and
district
social class
XI- 3
Sample
376
age and education
characteristics:
come, and residence
XI-4
Average
number
XI-5
sex,
in-
(in per cent)
symptoms
of
Score) according to
by
378
(Twenty-two
sex, residence,
Item
and income
Complaints reported on Twenty-two Item Score by sex
and city (in per cent) XI-6
379
384
Attitudes toward female equality
and sex
(in per cent)
by
residence, income,
387
xxvi
Part
One
IDENTIFYING
AND
SURVEYING PSYCHIATRIC DISORDERS
Cross-cultural Psychiatry
I:
By Alexander H. Leighton and Jane M. Murphy
IF one takes a broad definition of social psychiatry, then
it
is
appropriate to regard cross-cultural psychiatry as occupying a
on certain aspects in the relationship between
position within this field and as focusing certain ways.
two
1
The
point of attention
is
orders of phenomena: psychiatric disorders and sociocul-
tural environment.
As
areas of interest, the fields of social
cross-cultural psychiatry rest
on
the assumption that there
and is
an
interdependence between psychiatric and sociocultural processes that
is
in
some measure
distinct
from the
relationships
between
psychiatric processes and organic experiences or heredity. It is
well recognized, however, that such compartmentaliza-
tions are as false to fields
of
human
phenomena
science.
versus environment and
in psychiatry as
The
body
old arguments about heredity
versus
mind have been
not settled in the twentieth century, and that
human
they are in other
it is
set aside if
generally accepted
functioning cannot be adequately comprehended
viewed from one orientation to the exclusion of theless, a division
others.
if
None-
of the total problem into subfields such as
cross-cultural psychiatry facilitates
the organization of ideas
along lines that are amenable to research and that utilize the con3
Approaches
^ cepts and
skills
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
of different disciplines. Current specialization of
by the intention of achieving an ultimate convergence in which fuller understanding of psychiatric phenomena will be reached through a number of perspectives. Within the broad framework of interest in causes of psychiatric disorder it is not only permissible but useful to ask what is more and what less important. The goal may be heuristic unfocus
thus justified
is
derstanding or
it
may
be a desire to modify and control. In the
latter case the investigator
is
concerned with the major obstacles
overcome and opportune points
to be
in the process. effort
Those
at
that promise the
which most
to initiate
change
effect for the least
can be considered the most important whether the phe-
nomenon
in question be considered part of genetics, physiology,
psychology, cultural processes, or some division or overlapping area in one or
more of
these.
The purposes of this chapter are, therefore, to explain what we mean by cross-cultural psychiatry, trace briefly its history as a
body of thought and
investigation, define terms,
and
list
some
of the important problem areas.
Nature and History of Cross-cultural Psychiatry Cross-cultural psychiatry, as conceived here,
is
a
way
of ob-
serving and ordering facts about the processes of psychiatric
by studying people in numbers rather than as individual patients and by comparing the sociocultural processes of different groups of people. There are, as it happens, many disorders
groups that can be isolated for comparative analysis within
Western
society.
Thus comparisons can be made between
ethnic
enclaves that have been transplanted into complex societies. 2
Nonetheless, the full scope of interest in cross-cultural psychiatry
is
worldwide.
It
includes concern with people in their
native habitats and deals with maximal cultural contrasts, identi-
(though probably misnamed) by such differentiating terms "Western" and "non- Western"; "underdeveloped," "develop-
fied as
ing," and "industrialized"; "primitive"
and
"civilized."
Cross-cultural Psychiatry
One problem
of basic research
is
5
to discover
what
factors or
combinations of factors in the environment produce, encourage, or perpetuate what kinds of psychiatric disorders.
toward
this
end
is
A
first
step
How
to sort apart universals and variables.
do
the patterning, frequency, and duration of psychiatric disorders
vary in the light of sociocultural differences? appear in more or
disorders
the
less
What
psychiatric
same form in many
—perhaps —regardless
all of culture? In view of known, conditions such as mental deficiency, chronic brain syndrome, and some types of schizophrenia appear
populations
what
already
is
to exist in
A
all
human groups.
further interest
is
to discover
kinds of psychiatric disorders bear
what relationships various (when looked at in cross-
cultural perspective) to the universal categories of the
—being male, being female, or being
situation
in the life-arc of childhood, adolescence, maturity,
cence. Since there
is
employ age and sex
wide variation in the
may
way
and senes-
cultural groups
and obligations, the pat-
in defining rights
terning of cultural roles
exert a major influence
development and distribution of psychiatric symptoms. lows, therefore, that questions about
human
at different points
human
universals
on the It fol-
and cul-
tural differences are fundamental.
Although by no means is
still
a
new
in a formative stage.
defining
cross-cultural psychiatry
field,
There
is
considerable fluidity in
problems and a great deal of variability in the terms used to denote its character. In an early phase it was referred to its
as "primitive psychiatry,"
and it has also been called "anthropoand "ethnopsychiatry." These designations have usually been employed in connection with studies of vari-
logical psychiatry"
ous exotic types of psychopathology found in specific cultural groups.,
Among
anthropologists
the
"psychiatric anthropology," but
field
is
sometimes
more commonly
it is
known
as
subsumed
under "culture and personality." These titles tend to emphasize an interest in what psychiatry can offer to an understanding of
Approaches
6
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
on the
cultural processes and focus attention
relatively
normal
development and functioning of personality in different cultural
on psychiatric
contexts rather than
By
disorders per
among
the welfare-minded and
se.
those involved in the ad-
ministration of psychiatric services, the field has been included
under "public health psychiatry" and "psychiatry for developing countries." These labels clearly indicate a major interest in treatment, control, and Finally and
more
psychiatry,"
tural
—where
possible
—prevention.
specifically the field has
been called "cul-
psychiatry,"
"transcultural
and,
here,
as
"cross-cultural psychiatry."
This multiplicity of names makes nication and choice of words.
difficult the task
The problem
disciplinary nature of the topic talk in several languages at once
of
commu-
indicates the inter-
and the consequent necessity to
—
at a
minimum
psychiatry, social science, and public health.
the languages of
It also
indicates that
phenomena similar ideas by
the people interested in this subject see the pertinent
from many points of view and may
refer to
several different terms.
We two
have selected the term "cross-cultural psychiatry" for
reasons:
(i) the phrase adequately conveys the fact that
in this field the
inquiry search
is
is
main conceptual and methodological tool for
comparative analysis, and
who
"cross-cultural" re-
a familiar idea to social scientists in the
use allows us to identify with and
its
(2)
draw on the
United
States;
efforts of those
have concerned themselves with cross-cultural research
methods.*
Modern to have
scientific
endeavor in cross-cultural psychiatry
begun with Kraepelin.
He
is
said
traveled extensively to see
whether the "disease" types he had specified could be found in places other than * It
has
Europe. His attention was primarily, though
should be noted here that our approach to cross-cultural psychiatry
much
in
common
at a general level
research being carried out
Canada.
with the transcultural psychiatric
by Wittkower,
Fried,
and their colleagues
in
Cross-cultural Psychiatry
7
not exclusively, directed toward heredity and "racial" tendencies.
Freud
3
was
also interested in culture
and cultural differences,
by such anthropological contemporaries and Lang as well as by the Lamarckian tradition
influenced in this area as
Tylor, Frazer,
Europe regarding the inheritance of acquired
in
He
characteristics.
racial experience
concerned himself with the possibility that
transmitted through the unconscious to succeeding gener-
is
At
ations.
root, his ideas
were primarily within the framework
of evolutional biology as this was conceived at the turn of the
century.
Meyer's psychobiological approach
4
included attention to the
probable effects of social and cultural factors in personality de-
velopment and to subsequent patterns of adaptation course of
life.
Impelled
by
this interest,
himself familiar with the ideas of such
Mead,
W.
later to
I.
all
along the
he sought out and made
men
as
George Herbert
Thomas, and William McDougall.
exchanges with anthropologists such
He went
Edward
as
on
Sapir,
Ruth Benedict, Bronislaw Malinowski, and others. As the names above reflect, there was considerable expansion of interest in the relationships between personality patterns and cultural milieu
World War spread
II
from some time
—especially
concern for
this
in the field
up through
in the twenties
United
States.
had tapered
By
off
1950 wide-
somewhat
probably because of the ending of the war. At that time the urgency for seeking the causes of human conflict, especially as they might hinge on cultural differences, began to sixties,
lessen. In the
however, there are noticeable evidences of some
revival.
Before and during the war period, the widened scope of interest
was perhaps due more
to
anthropologists'
psychiatric ideas than to psychiatrists'
becoming
delving into interested in
cultural anthropology. 5
sake of inquiry
is
The union of the two disciplines for the marked by a number of collaborative efforts
beginning with the Kardiner and Linton seminars at Columbia University which started in the mid-thirties. 6 Today there is a
Approaches
8
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
sizable roster of joint anthropological
and psychiatric studies
as
number of books specifically on this topic. 7 When at its height the combined approach had a stimulating influence which suffused much of American anthropology. This was not limited exclusively to an interest in mental and emowell as an expanding
tional
problems but appeared in a broadened view of the "whole
man" and
of
human
relations in cultural situations.
During
this
period the subfield of "culture and personality" achieved considerable popularity. It acquired academic standing as an area of specialization in the graduate
A
ments.
number of
programs of anthropology depart-
field investigations
were designed for the
and dream materials. The Oedipal theme and various hypotheses from psychoanalytic theory were of Rorschach
collection
taken into cultures.
Up
field
work
One major
until
for examination in different nonliterate
effort involved national character studies.
mid-century two trends of thought had been evident
Much
in cross-cultural psychiatry.
and
of the early psychiatric inter-
non- Western groups had been dominated by biological
est in
racial determinism.
and with
ality,"
was
order,
it
Later the
much
pervaded
field
of "culture and person-
of the thinking about psychiatric dis-
by
determinism.
cultural
Although
acknowledgment was often made to "biological factors," the written and even more the spoken words of many anthropological investigators did this,
there
was often
not seem to treat these as a feeling that there
is
real.
More than
something reprehensi-
ble about giving credence to hereditary factors as controlling
human
behavior in any important way.
for this are manifold, but
No
doubt the reasons
beyond the myopia common to most
specialized interests and resistance to anything that
from the feeling of working on
the central
might detract
complex of
causes,
At
a time
there
were
when
racist ideas were a focus of world struggle, liberal-minded
also matters of
moral and
political belief.
people were reluctant to do anything that might appear to strengthen such a framework for injustice and crime.
The
rational basis for cultural determinism rested largely
on
Cross-cultural Psychiatry
9
assumptions of psychological determinism, and this in turn drew
on dynamic psychiatry, particularly psychoanalysis. This reliance was, however, somewhat selective, overlooking heavily
Freud's fundamental ideas about constitutional factors as well as
more diffuse conceptions of multiple biological influences which permeate much of the rest of psychiatry. Since World War II there has been a dropping away of insist-
the
ence on cultural determinism, owing in part to "culture and personality" losing ground as an area of interest. This in turn
followed on the realization that a great deal more
work was
needed in developing and validating research methods.
It also
awareness that quick answers to
stemmed from the growing world problems were not yet on
The
the horizon.
influence of cultural determinism has also diminished
through the advance of knowledge on
work similarities. The
Expanding anthropological variation
and cultural
of the relationship
field
many
relevant fronts.
has clarified both cultural early spectacular theories
between culturally defined child-rearing
practices
and basic personality patterns have been progressively
modified
as
more
solid information
about peoples of different
backgrounds has accumulated. Equally, demonstrated progress in the fields of genetics is
more
and biological determinants
difficult for students
is
such that
of personality to ignore.
these accounts, a fairly balanced cognizance
is
now
On
it
all
given both
to cultural and biological considerations and to the interplay
between them. It is
obvious, of course, that anthropology and psychiatry
have not been the sole contributors to the development of ideas regarding the relationships between sociocultural and psychiatric
phenomena. From other branches of behavioral science
have come general semantics, communication theory, the concept of role, the concept of class structure, field theory, and reference group concepts.
Gregory Bateson, Leonard
Names such S.
Cottrell,
Ruesch, Robert Merton, and Lloyd
as
Alfred Korzybski,
Kurt Lewin, Jurgen
Warner
help illustrate the
Approaches
10
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
ence in studies
of
culture
through Dollard and Miller. 8 tend to emphasize situations
been
reflex theory have long
and conditioned
point. Learning
potentially important but have had, in fact, relatively
little influ-
and psychiatric disorder except
The as
from these various
ideas
fields
of etiological importance and,
hence, at least indirectly refer to culture.
We
article to present
have attempted in a recent
ideas that have
emerged over the
on psychiatric
by saying 1.
as
disorders. 9
Malaya), koro
as:
such
specific disorders,
China), and loitiko
(in
cul-
These can be summarized
that culture has been conceived
(in
how
exert causal or determining
Determining the pattern of certain
Utah
years as to
last fifty
ture and cultural situations might
influences
some of the
(in the
Indian cultures of Northeast America) 2.
Producing basic personality types, some of which are espe-
cially vulnerable to psychiatric disorder; 3.
Producing psychiatric disorders (usually considered
latent
for a time) through certain child-rearing practices; 4.
Having
a selective influence
on
a population's potential for
psychiatric disorder as well as the pattern of disorder through
types of sanctions and whether
"shame" or "guilt"
is
en-
gendered; 5.
Precipitating disorder in an otherwise adequately function-
ing personality by confrontation with stressful roles;
Perpetuating disorder by rewarding
6.
such
as
7.
it
in prestigeful roles
holy man, witch doctor, or shaman;
Precipitating disorder
by changing more
rapidly than per-
sonality systems are able to tolerate; 8.
Producing disorder through the inculcation of sentiments
(beliefs
and values) that produce emotional
personality, such as fears, jealousies, 9.
Affecting
the
distribution
of
and
states
damaging to
unrealistic aspirations;
some kinds of disorder
through breeding patterns; 10.
Influencing
the
amount and
distribution
through patterns of poor hygiene and nutrition.
of
disorder
n
Cross-cultural Psychiatry
The above
are
all
couched in terms of possible variations be-
tween sociocultural groups such
as
would
quencies and kinds of disorder. There
namely that culture per
idea,
se
—that
result in different fre-
is,
one overarching
also
is
any and
cultures
all
produces a certain amount of psychiatric disorder. This
arises
because regulation of the basic natural urges in the growing-
human organism
is
logically injurious.
conceived to be both universal and psycho-
Although sociocultural factors may vary
the degree and extent of their effects
in
from group to group, they
have been thought to produce some damage and some disorder in
all.
apparent that the point in current cross-cultural psy-
It is
chiatry
is
no longer to prove that culture
rather to ask
what kind of
it is.
So far
as the total process
development of psychiatric disorder
in the
would seem best
to
for one, or for
theory,
is is
is
concerned,
it
assume that hereditary, biological, and
psychological factors are
which
a cause
major cause, but
a
is
all
three engaged.
any subarea within one,
To
as a
claim dominance
matter of general
and
to express a linear conception of cause
out of keeping with what
we know
about
effect all
the
More germane is an approach discover and map out the interrelated
processes in the world around us. to the topic that aims to
factors
and the nature of their interrelationships. Definitioji of
It is
Terms
appropriate at this point to define "psychiatric disorders"
and "sociocultural environment" more precisely. I.
By
we mean all those behaviors, emocommonly regarded as proper for the 10 psychiatrist. More specifically the term includes
psychiatric disorders
tions, attitudes,
attention of a
and
beliefs
brain syndromes, mental deficiency, the functional psychoses,
psychophysiological disorders, psychoneuroses, personality
trait
disturbances, sociopathic behavior, and acute situational reactions. In
our
use, then, "psychiatric disorder" has a
broader connotation than "mental
illness,"
which
is
somewhat often re-
Approaches to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
12
and severe psychoneuroses.
stricted to the functional psychoses
All of these patterns of psychiatric disorder are conceived as
phenomena having
process
origin, course,
and outcome which
can be fully understood only in relation to the total of the individual.
We
life
history
hazard the guess that the disorders most
thoroughly affected by sociocultural experiences and therefore
most are
vary according to different sociocultural factors
likely to
symptoms of
a
psychoneurotic and psychophysiologic na-
ture.
On
these
symptoms can be
part of a person's genetic and constitu-
may
be precipitated, maintained, or inhibited
the other hand,
and
tional heritage
by such
it is
apparent that tendencies toward
physiological events as a long illness or sustained malnu-
trition.
In the
predominantly hereditary or organic disturbances,
sociocultural factors
may
be related to cause (and hence influ-
ence the prevalence of such disorders) through breeding patterns or patterns of physical hygiene.
Furthermore the function-
ing of people with psychiatric disorders which are chiefly of hereditary or physiological origin the sociocultural setting in
may
which they
relate significantly to
find themselves. If the
sociocultural environment offers protection,
makes fewer de-
mands, or has the resources for therapy, the course and out-
come
of the process
may
be very different from that in an en-
vironment which lacks these properties.
What
has been said thus far about psychiatric disorders refers
to the province of psychiatry in
points in this
volume
Western
culture.
At
several
(especially Chapters II and III) the prob-
lems of cultural relativity and variable conceptions of disorder
and deviance will be taken up. For the most part
comparing Western views with those found
this involves
in other groups in
order to discover the areas of overlap and divergence.
underlying principle regards just as
we
we do
As an
take an attitude of inclusiveness in these
in dealing
with the range of psychiatric
phenomena as defined in Western thought. While systematic studies are needed to
reveal the kinds of
Cross-cultural Psychiatry
13
behavior that are considered normal and abnormal in different cultural groups,
seems unnecessary to waver in the face of
it
though
cultural relativism as
As
ards of functioning.
a
we
completely lacked valid stand-
beginning there
society no matter
We
what
its
evidence that some
is
command
disorders are so impairing as to
recognition in any
cultural patterning.
do not mean to imply that
all
beliefs
and knowledge
about the causes and cures of psychiatric disorders can be put on the same plane.
The
distinction
between magical and
scientific
explanations obviously remains a necessary and fruitful one. But the fact that they are quite different orders of explanation, and
involve contrasting
ways of dealing with
for ignoring either one.
through witchcraft
is
The
belief that
clearly apposite as a
some kinds of psychiatric disorders oriented therapy
Our concern
is
just as
events,
not reason
is
one has been "hexed"
background feature is
in
the fact that magic-
apparently effective in some instances.
for the
moment, however,
is
less
with ideas of
cause and methods of cure than with the question of
what forms
of emotion, belief, and behavior are considered psychiatric in other cultural streams of thought. For basic research
we
believe, to allow the definition of the
it is
phenomena
unwise,
to be col-
ored by etiological assumptions and views of treatment emerging
from our
own
tradition or other traditions.
To
discover further
information about causal factors, which can then be employed in
developing methods of prevention and therapy,
is
the major rea-
son for conducting the kinds of research that constitute the topic of this book. This very fact argues strongly against build-
ing preconceptions of cause into the definition of the
about which
we
are
situation in
which
correlations
we
II.
phenomena
making inquiry in order to avoid a later would be obscured by dazzling
real findings
ourselves inserted.
In using the phrase socio cultural environment
we employ
a combination of concepts thought to be advantageous for the
kinds of studies discussed here. a
way
It is a
combination that provides
of talking about the total configuration of
what
is
going
Approaches
14
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
on outside the individual organism whose psychiatric characteristics
are of concern.
Certain important distinctions between "society" and "culture" need to be emphasized in studies focusing strictly on social structure or strictly
on
cultural processes. In a recent paper
Kroeber and Parsons point to a confusion about what and what cultural that has not yet been
by
frequently glossed over
with
full
tion."
11
is
social
and that
clarified
"is
the use of the term socio-cultural
awareness that the hyphen
is
no symbol of integra-
equally to be recognized, however, that in any
It is
empirical situation the
two
are intertwined and that
many
times
the differences cannot be fully specified. For the purpose of investigating the relationships
between psychiatric phenomena
characteristic of individual personalities
and the
mental factors that impinge upon them,
we
Following customary usage, "society"
ships.
beings
who
Except for
ber of hermits, this definition
it more two components.
defined as a group of
together in a system of social relation-
live
minuscule and presumably diminishing num-
a
all
is
human
beings live in social groups. Although
and generalization are widely accepted,
immediately evident
just
of groups should be studied for characteristics that
There
as
There
are artificial
natural groups (a mental hospital as
And as
there are
compared
and
may
have
compared to
town). There are small groups and large (families to national units).
not
aspects
are short-lived groups
committee or task force
(a
it is
what kinds of groups or what
significance in psychiatric disorder.
and long-lived
as
environ-
have found
appropriate to combine than to separate the
human
many
as
a
compared
institutional as well as
compared
to a
community).
homogeneous and heterogeneous groups (a tribe These different characteristics influence
to a city).
the degree and kind of system of relationships
group operates, but
all,
at
by which
the
one or another level of generalization,
can be said to exhibit the structural and functional qualities of a social system.
Psychiatric studies are currently going on in nearly
all
these
Cross-cultural Psychiatry contexts. In cross-cultural psychiatry
we
/y
have elected to focus
on perduring and culturally cohesive groups localized in natural geographic habitats. There are, however, several questions about choosing a group and setting analytic boundaries around it for study purposes, which together with the problem of population mobility will be taken
"Culture" of
is
expresses
its
in Chapter VIII.
an abstraction which encompasses the total
of a society.
life
up
It is
way
a precipitate of the group's history
adaptation to the physical environment.
It is
and
charac-
by what A. I. Hallowell has called a "psycho12 logical reality." That is, it refers to the shared patterns of belief, feeling, and knowledge the basic values, axioms, and assumptions that members of the group carry in their minds as guides for conduct and the definition of reality. Along these lines C. P. Snow has observed: "Without thinking about it, they respond alike. That is what a culture means." 13 And Ruth terized especially
—
—
Benedict has spoken of "the unconscious canons of choice" that characterize one group in contrast to another. 14
Besides social relationships, technology, economics, religion,
and other aspects of human
life,
culture refers to the inter-
connections and interdependencies that bind part to part to
make
whole. Culture
a
is
constantly changing;
it is
transmitted from one generation to the next; and
have
it.
When
The
learned; all
it is
societies
however, varies from one group to another.
style,
the variation between groups
is
marked
it is
appropriate
to speak of different cultures.
Combining "sociocultural" with "environment" emphasizes any given individual is composed of other
that the milieu for
individuals
who
share similar or reciprocal sociocultural experi-
ences. This usage of terms also underscores the fact that the
group
as a
whole
exists in particular physical
circumstances of
climate, altitude, natural resources,
and the presence or absence
of noxious agents, such as the tsetse
fly,
disease.
That
chiatry
is
these factors
may
which produce endemic
be relevant to cross-cultural psy-
demonstrated in localizations of psychiatrically impor-
1
Approaches
6
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
tant diseases such as trypanosomiasis in Africa.
way, one must
also consider theories
such
1
"'
In the same
as those that
propose a
relationship, for example, between the inclemency of the arctic
environment, and the various manifestations called "arctic hyste18
ria."
Thus, similar to the definition of psychiatric disorders,
our view of environment
is
made broad enough
ple types of sociocultural factors that
may
to include multi-
be causally related to
different kinds of disorders.
Problem Areas
in Cross-cultural Psychiatry
This volume makes no attempt ever, underlying the
areas
1.
at
being comprehensive.
How-
various approaches three main problem
may be noted.
COMPARATIVE STUDY OF DIAGNOSTIC ENTITIES Within Western culture we
tients
with each other.
The
are
accustomed to comparing pa-
results of psychiatric examinations,
history-taking, and continuing observation of various cases are laid side larities
by
side in clinical analysis.
may
and differences
comparative analysis and
Groups of
cases
thus be assembled with further
tests, all
aimed
at elucidating the char-
acter of the underlying psychological processes. this
method of
with simi-
essentially qualitative analysis that
It is
through
most of what
we understand about psychiatric disorder has been derived. What happens when this procedure is extended between tures?
Can
it
be rendered more objective and susceptible to
quantitative treatment? ties
What
can be learned
as to the potentiali-
and limitations of cultural influence? These problems of
diagnostic entities are taken
2.
cul-
up mainly
in Chapters II
and
III.
COMPARATIVE EPIDEMIOLOGY
As
applied to psychiatry, comparative epidemiology refers to
the study of prevalence or incidence of disorder in different populations. Such study has several uses.
Cross-cultural Psychiatry First,
it
ij
helps in the planning of services, both as to kind and as
to distribution.
Second,
we
studies
are at the edge of a virtually
hence there collecting.
helps add to scientific knowledge. In cross-cultural
it
a
is
first
two or more
cultures,
what the ultimate aim may
ploring causes.
way
of doing
this. If
from which it
deals
with
questions that has to be asked in comparing
may
Third, epidemiology
we may
one systematic
studies can be carried out, for
psychiatric disorder in gardless of
is
will lay a quantitative baseline
it
manner of other
one of the
continent; and
primary need for observation and information
Epidemiology
done successfully, all
unknown
By
be used
more or
less re-
be. as
an instrument for ex-
studying populations with different cultures
find associations
between certain cultures or subcultures
on the one hand and the distribution of certain types of psychion the
atric disorder
tions
is
other.
The
demonstration of such associa-
then a basis for additional comparative studies, designed
to elucidate reasons, together with longitudinal and experimental investigations.
We
can clarify theories of cause by making them
and then checking prediction by means of epidemiological surveys.
a basis for predicting findings
Discussions
of the
problems connected with comparative
epidemiology thread throughout
this
book but
are especially
prominent in Chapters IV, V, VI, VII, and XI. 3.
COMPARATIVE STUDIES OF PERSONALITY AND PERSONALITY FORMATION
While the
first
two problem
areas
concern the frankly patho-
logical or at least deviant, comparative study of personality in
different cultural settings implies an interest in
What
normal processes.
can be learned about personality from comparative studies
of "nature's experiments" through data collecting and analysis?
Attacks on ideas
this question
have led in the past to a wealth of
and the development of theory, but,
as
noted
earlier, effort
1
Approaches
8
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
has dwindled since the war.
of making
One
reason
is
probably the
difficulty
studies in a variety of cultural contexts that are suffi-
ciently explicit and factual to permit a satisfying comparative analysis for checking theories. It
is
plain that
we
need operative
more easily converted into research practice, by which to identify and classify the phenomena
theories that can be
better systems
of interest, and better instruments with which to detect and count. Dealing with these needs
would seem
beyond present work with projective
tests,
to be the next steps
symbolic interpreta-
and the reporting
tions, descriptions of child-rearing practices,
of isolated
life histories.
These topics
will be discussed chiefly in Chapters
IX and X,
although the chapters on children and adolescents (VI and VII) are indirectly pertinent.
Approaches to
all
the problem areas just outlined are closely
related. Quantitative information
orders and their trends in time
is
about the distribution of
necessary for an understanding
of the meaning of personality studies case analysis.
One may
clinical case studies as
as
well as for comparative
regard the personality studies and the
being
end of
at the intensive
a series of
related problems that has epidemiology at the extensive end.
intensive studies leave
can generalize.
The
unanswered questions
set in the
as to
how
When
one
the intensive can
context of the extensive, and both in a knowledge
of the relevant sociocultural systems, areas of doubt
reduced and
The
far
extensive studies, on the other hand, leave us
unsatisfied about underlying meaning.
be
dis-
sets
may
be
of findings mutually illuminated.
Notes 1.
A. H. Leighton,
in Social Psychiatry
Leighton,
An
J. S.
N. Wilson, eds., Explorations York: Basic Books, 1957); see also A. H.
Clausen, and R.
(New
Introduction to Social Psychiatry (Springfield,
111.:
Charles
Thomas, i960); and "Culture and Mental Health," in Trends in Modern American Society, C. Morris, ed. (Philadelphia: University of PennsylC.
vania Press, 1962). 2.
E. D. Wittkower, and
J.
Fried,
"Some Problems
of Transcultural
Cross-cultural Psychiatry Psychiatry," in Culture and Mental Health,
'9
M. K. Opler,
ed.
(New
York:
Macmillan, 1959). 3. S. Freud, Totem and Taboo (New York: New Republic, Inc., 193 1 ). 4. A. Meyer, Psychobiology: A Science of Man, compiled and edited by E. E. Winters and A. M. Bowers (Springfield, 111.: Charles C. Thomas, 1957)5. C. Kluckhohn, "The Influence of Psychiatry on Anthropology in America during the Past One Hundred Years," in Personal Character and Cultural Milieu, D. G. Haring, ed. (Syracuse: Syracuse University Press,
1956) , pp. 485-5336.
(New
A. Kardiner, The Individual and His Society
York: Columbia
University Press, 1939); see also Kardiner, R. Linton, C. Du Bois, and Columbia J. West, The Psychological Frontiers of Society (New York:
University Press, 1945). 7.
The
a
on psychiatric problems
literature
flected in
number
numerous of books.
articles
The
in different cultures
is
re-
published in professional journals as well as
following
is
a selection of
books that indicate part
of the coverage of world areas or that deal with problems in crosscultural
psychiatry
and
have
bibliographies
that
might be
R. Linton, Culture and Me?ital Disorders (Springfield,
111.:
useful:
Charles C.
J. W. Eaton, and R. J. Weil, Culture and Mental DisComparative Study of the Hutterites and Other Populations (New York: The Free Press of Glencoe, 1955); M. K. Opler, ed., Culture and Mental Health (New York: Macmillan, 1959); J. C. Carothers, The African Mind in Health and Disease [Monograph Series No. 17 (Geneva: World Health Organization, 1953)]; M. K. Opler, Culture, Psychiatry, and Human Values: The Methods and Values of a Social Psychiatry
Thomas, 1956); orders:
A
(Springfield, 111.: Charles C. Thomas, 1956); N. Kline, ed., Psychiatry in the Underdeveloped Countries, Report of Roundtable Meetings, Atlantic
City, N.J.,
1 1
6th Annual Meeting of the
tion (Washington, D.C.:
American Psychiatric AssociaAmerican Psychiatric Association, i960); E. A.
Weinstein, Cultural Aspects of Delusio?i: A Psychiatric Study of the Virgin Islands (New York: The Free Press of Glencoe, 1962); M. J.
An Etlmo-Psy chiatric Study of Rural Ghana Northwestern University Press, i960); P. M. Yap, Suicide in Hong Kong, with special reference to attempted suicide (New York: Oxford University Press, 1958); G. M. Carstairs, Twice Bom: A Study of a Cormnunity of High-Caste Hindus (London: Hogarth Press, Ltd., Search for Security:
Field,
(Evanston,
1957)
111.:
-
J. Dollard, and N. E. Miller, Personality and Psychotherapy (New York: McGraw-Hill, 1950). 9. A. H. Leighton, and J. M. Murphy (formerly Hughes), "Cultures as Causative of Mental Disorder," Milbank Memorial Fund Quarterly, vol. 8.
39 (July 1961).
Approaches
20 10.
Man
A. H. Leighton, in Relation to
chiatric
Books,
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
My Name Is
Culture [Vol.
Legion: Foundations for a Theory of The Stirling County Study of Psy-
I,
(New
Disorder and Sociocultural Environment
York: Basic
1959)].
A. L. Kroeber, and T. Parsons, "The Concepts of Culture and of Social System," American Sociological Review, vol. 23, no. 5 (Oct. 1958). 12. A. I. Hallowell, Culture and Experience (Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press, 1955). 13. CP. Snow, The Two Cultures a?id the Scientific Revolution [The Rede Lecture, 1959 (New York: Cambridge University Press, 1959)], 11.
p. 11. 14.
R. Benedict, Vatterns of Culture (Boston: C. Carothers, op. cit.
Houghton
A4ifflin, 1934).
15. J. 16.
S.
Novakovsky, "Arctic or Siberian Hysteria
graphic Environment," Ecology, vol.
5
as a
(1924), pp.
1
Reflex of the Geo-
13-127.
II:
The Problem of
Cross-cultural Identification
of Psychiatric Disorders By
Charles Savage, Alexander
H. Leighton,
and Dorothea C. Leighton
EDITORIAL NOTES
The
questions posed in the present chapter arise
from the
relationship of psychiatric disorder to culture. Psychiatric activities
and ideas form a subpattern within the family of Western
cultures,
and the definitions of psychiatric disorders have
their
Western views about what human nature is or ought to As phenomena, the disorders are patterns of behavior and
base in be.
feeling that are out of keeping with cultural expectations
that bother the person
who
acts
and
feels
and
them, or bother others
around him, or both. Since, however, different cultures are by definition different systems of standards
lows that what garded
may
activities
is
it
fol-
may be rewoman who in
be disturbing in one culture
as desirable in another.
America
and expectation,
Thus
the
man
or
hospitalized for hearing voices or jailed for his sexual
might have
this
behavior ignored, accepted, or even
venerated in some other cultural group.
An
extreme proponent of cultural
relativity,
using this line
of argument, could deny the possibility of identifying and
enumerating the same kinds of psychiatric disorder in markedlv 21
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
Approaches
22
different cultures.
might run, defined
Any
pattern of behavior, so the argument
healthy or sick only to the extent that
is
by
defined as psychiatric disorder in the West, but
it
is
so
may
be
it
a given culture. Certain patterns of behavior
does not
follow that they will be so defined in another culture. Con-
we
versely, behavior patterns that
may
be defined in other cultures
between
contrast, furthermore,
discrepancies are apt to be.
disorder
atric
two
in
To
regard as normal or admirable as
abnormal.
cultures, the
The
more
greater the
radical these
speak, then, of "the same psychi-
different
cultures,"
is
virtually
self-
contradictory.
Such an extreme view with regard
two
bodies bilities
which
for variation is
more or
less
among
cultures
necessary to the
infinitely plastic, that all of us as
human
every form of first,
to cultural relativity
rather doubtful assumptions.
we
is
One
is
limitless.
first, is
begin
em-
that the possi-
The
second,
that personality life
is
are capable of
With regard to the evident among known
behavior and feeling.
despite the considerable variation
cultures the range does not appear to be boundless.
On
the con-
trary, the evidence suggests that there are denominators and
limitations
common
to
all
sociocultural systems. This applies not
only to certain specific patterns such to larger functional attributes. tural
groups
known
as the incest taboo,
There
are, for instance,
is
no
cul-
that lack patterns of leadership and fol-
lowership, or that have no set of rules regarding
and what
but also
what
is
right
wrong.
In a similar
way one may
argue that the evidence available
against a theory of infinite plasticity in personality.
It
seem more probable that there are some biological factors
work found
that influence the in
at
norms and deviations of personality
any sociocultural group. Hence, from the point of
view of psychiatry we there are
is
would
some
also universal
may
characteristics
say that although cultures
differ,
common to all cultures and hence The mentally deficient, the per-
forms of deviance.
Cross-cultural Identification of Disorders
son
who
kills
and the person
indiscriminately
who
23
exhibits un-
controlled excitement are possible examples. If,
however, one rejects an extreme view with regard to cul-
tural relativity, he
must
also
on the
basis of available anthro-
pological evidence reject a corresponding extreme of biological
determinism. culture,
Some behavior
pertinent to psychiatry
is
relative to
and cultural differences must enter to some extent into
the definitions and perceptions of psychiatric disorders.
problem then
If
is:
far can
start
and the
tions of disorder
how
we
we go
with our traditional Western
criteria
The
defini-
by which we recognize them,
with these into other cultures with some
hope of being able to identify comparable phenomena?
THIS
chapter attempts to point out a lattice of criteria whereby
psychiatric disorders as they occur
among
adults
nized outside the European family of cultures. first
chapter of this book, a reason for this interest
discovering something about etiology. lence and incidence studies as a
what kinds of course, and the
As soon ever, he
ent,
as
way
We
be recog-
want
is
in the
the hope of
to use preva-
of gathering evidence about
sociocultural factors influence the origin, the
outcome of psychiatric
one begins to define the
disorders. criteria of disorder,
becomes aware of an enormous number of
questions. sion,
may
As noted
They buzz about
in the
mind and
how-
difficult
in the air of discus-
coming and going from here and there, disturbing, insistand unconnected or perhaps connected only dimly by un-
—
clear premises.
In part the problem arises from the language of psychiatry itself,
which, although technical,
consistent.
Moreover,
many
is
often neither precise nor
of the terms for disorders and even
symptoms imply
theories of cause, so that
rate reference to
phenomena from reference
it is
difficult to sepa-
to etiological ideas.
Approaches
2j.
This distinction
you wish check the
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
of course, of fundamental importance
is,
examine the phenomena
to
in order to develop
Another
set of questions
common
hovers around the matter of deviance
most behavior recognized
to
psychiatric disorder, and in
any
it
would seem
ment of Sapir
is 1
in our society as
By
how-
itself,
from being an adequate guide. The com-
far
Two Crows, who denied that two "We suspect that he is crazy." On the
on the Indian,
and two make four was:
Two
other hand, perhaps
Crows was
had transcended the ordinary be considered
as a result
a charac-
is
desirable as one criterion
definition of disorder in another culture.
ever, deviance
also
and
ideas.
and conformity. Deviance from cultural expectation teristic
when
deviant
as
a
mathematical genius
rules of counting.
by members
A
person
who may
of his cultural group
of accidental events; in some tribes, for instance, a
man who had
two wives and
the bad luck to lose
through sickness might be regarded
six children
finally as a witch,
and hence
deviant.
Turning the coin
we must
over,
deviance from cultural standards
is
also
observe that just as
not sufficient evidence of
psychiatric disorder, so conformity and adaptation are not sufficient evidence of
by persons
absence.
its
whom we
It is
possible for roles to be filled
would say were psychoneurotic or schizophrenic. In talking with Navahos we have noted that impairment from senility was not perceived as either illness
as
clinicians
or deviance, but rather a part of the expected behavior
(role) of older people.
A
similar
problem
arises
from the
fact that in the diagnosis of
psychiatric cases there are sociological, biological, and psychological parameters,
makes
it
any one of which may predominate. This
impossible to describe psychiatric disorder in unitary
terms and leads the diagnostician to jump from one level of discourse to another. ourselves with
or
less
two
traditional
For example,
different in
in defining illness
we
find
and divergent models, the one more
clinical
medicine,
the
other
statistical.
Cross-cultural Identification of Disorders
According
the medical view,
to
if
person has treponema
a
pallidum in his system he has syphilis and he
not contagious, and
is
Furthermore,
syphilis.
not in any
if
everyone
way
is ill.
Even
impaired, he
recognized
agent
causal
specific
ditions of this sort
some organ or growth
is
factors are at
Cause
in this sense,
replaced
is
—blood sugar we
cancer the disorder is
may
by
these malfunctions,
even be fairly well-founded
however,
criteria
is
not part of the diagnosis as a critical
factor
for recognizing the pathology cell
and so on.
say with Knight of syphilis," or,
if
we must
in the
is
thought probable, of course,
work producing
carry this model over into psychiatry then 2
this
no
diabetes,
in
electrocardiogram reading, microscopic
level,
characteristics,
instance
is
center of disorder in con-
and tuberculosis. The agent
in syphilis
in diagnosis
If
it
con-
considered to be the malfunctioning of
and in some instances there theories.
—for
The
set of organs. In
characteristics of cells. It
unknown
is
has
is still
which there
also applies to conditions in
coronary thrombosis, and cancer.
as it
is
has
ill.
This viewpoint
that
he
if
still
community
else in the
so that deviance consists in not having syphilis, he
sidered
25
that a "touch of schizophrenia
not syphilis,
have
is
we would
like a
at least like diabetes.
criteria that point infallibly to
touch
But to do pathology;
they can be more complicated than the blood sugar or electro-
cardiogram but they must have specificity.
At
times
it
at least a
almost seems that
comparable degree of
we
have such
criteria,
but on closer scrutiny they tend to melt away.
A
clinical
example will serve
as illustration.
A
young woman
calls a psychiatrist,
asking for an appointment. She complains of
backache. She
again and breaks the appointment, saying she
is
calls
going south to see her doctor. She shows up for the broken
appointment
fifteen
minutes early. She enters the
office
fur-
glasses. Her hair is slicked back. She sits on the couch and then moves over to a chair by the window. A dark flush creeps up over her neck and face. Even
tively.
tensely
She wears dark
Approaches
26
before she has said a psychiatrist's
She then
word
that she
the conviction has is
a
formed
in the
an acute paranoid schizophrenic.
relates that she has violated a taboo.
mate with nist,
mind
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
She has been
piano salesman. She complains that he was a
inti-
Commu-
watching her, and that the doctors in a where she was examined for ulcer symptoms have
that the police are
hospital
planted a radio in her stomach.
Even
if
she vomited a radio,
many
to cling to our original diagnosis.
of us
Are there not
points evident so that one can say: this less?
Yet
all
would
is
still
be inclined
sufficient critical
schizophrenia, regard-
symptoms which we inAs a two weeks of tranquilizers
the immediate signs and
terpret as schizophrenia could point just as well to panic.
matter of
fact, in this patient after
and psychotherapy
all
the schizophrenic indicators disappeared
and have not returned over still
recur,
a period of several years.
sion in
symptoms.
We
also possible that the
with periods of remis-
know, however, from other cases that it symptoms will never return, or if they do
they will again be of brief duration and related to
We
could
and the natural history of the case might eventually
establish the patient as schizophrenic, but
is
They
stressful events.
can go further and say that in some situations her be-
havior would not be regarded rather as realistic fear
—
if,
as
symptomatic even of panic, but
for example, she lived in a police state
in the midst of a stringent
campaign against communism. The
"radio in her stomach" might turn out to be an uneducated person's version of
some medical maneuver involving the use of
radioactive isotopes.
This example demonstrates be.
The same problem
how
illusive the clinical
model can
occurs, of course, with diagnosis in other
branches of medicine, and the difference the degree in psychiatry
is
is
one of degree
—but
considerable. If psychoneurosis rather
than schizophrenia were chosen for
illustration,
the problem
would appear even greater. The idea of disease as a type of malfunction works well so long as the agent causing the malfunction, or a central aspect of the malfunction, or both, can be
detected with a high degree of accuracy and reliability. In
Cross-cultural Identification of Disorders
where there deal with
have difficulty meeting these criteria except an organic condition. Usually what we have to
we
psychiatry
27
is
malfunctional behavior. This immediately raises the
is
whom?" There
question: "Malfunctional according to
are
two
broad types of possible answers: those according to cultural expectations,
we
atry. If
and those according to dynamic theories of psychilean heavily on the first of these, we are back again to
we lean on the other, we we wish to investigate.
the problem of deviance. If
answers into the questions
The
statistical definition
of normal and deviance
plained and easily applied, but
if
of difficulties. Strictly applied,
possibility of finding the majority of
neurotic. If
medicine,
it
brings
it
would
it
any
cultural
own
its
out the
rule
group to be
were utilized in this manner in other branches of and tooth decay would be ob-
trouble,
tonsilitis, sinus
most people have them. In
literated as types of illness because
some
easily ex-
is
employed independently of
concepts about function and malfunction, share
are building
parts of the
world malaria, hookworm, and vitamin
defi-
ciency would be similarly eliminated.
We
have given here only a bare indication of the questions
that can be raised.
They
they are also slippery premises. It
is
numerous, complex, and entangling;
are as
to foundations
Now,
find that
there are
.
.
.
many
not exist in nature, and
it
,"
for
the
"But
as
"How
if
.
.
.
,"
about a case such
"Suppose
as
.
.
?" .
possible "supposes" and "ifs" that
do
occurred to us in due time that perhaps
some of these were wrong consequently,
of hidden
full
notable, too, that they cut across each other
with introductory expressions such
you should
and
questions.
We
turned from them,
being and took up a
time
different
approach.
What we
did was to select one
as a psychiatric disorder
tions: this
(1)
By what
and then
criteria
of these criteria
most other cultures
would
as well?
entity recognized
we
answer two ques-
would
condition in a person in our
Which
by one each
I
tried to
I as
own
a psychiatrist diagnose
society and culture?
(2)
expect to remain employable in
We
did not seek general criteria
Approaches
28
would
that
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
distinguish
all
disorder
criteria for each recognized
psychoneurosis,
deficiency,
from nondisorder, but only
clinical
such
entity
as
mental
and obsessive-compulsive person-
Attention was concentrated on observable phenomena
ality.
and, to the greatest extent possible, theories of psychological
all
definitions based
on
and sociocultural cause were avoided.
The review at the
of criteria was conducted in the autumn of 1957 Center for Advanced Studies in the Behavioral Sciences in
a series of
weekly seminars. Four
psychiatrists participated: the
Hamburg. The guide employed in the 1952 Diagnostic and Statistical Manual issued by the American Psychiatric As-
authors and Dr. David
was the
deliberations
of Mental Disorders, sociation. By January 1958 a set of criteria had been developed upon which the four psychiatrists could agree. Hamburg
moved on
the Center at this time, but the authors
left
vite a
group of anthropologists to participate
to in-
in a continua-
tion of the seminar, reviewing the criteria in the light of the cultural
fa-
were Ethel Albert,
Ward Goodenough,
Hymes, David Mandelbaum, Milton
Singer, and Charles
miliar.
Dell
group with which each anthropologist was most
The
participants
Wagley. The
cultural groups that served as the
main points of
reference were the Rundi of Burundi (Africa), the Eskimos of
Lawrence Island (Alaska), the Gilbert Islanders (MicroNavahos (American Southwest), the Tapirepe (Bra-
St.
nesia), the zil),
and the peoples of
In June
Leighton to
India.
were taken by Savage and A. Farms, Arizona, and reviewed during a two-
1958 the criteria
Many
Walsh McDermott and his staff, who had run an experimental clinic for Navahos during the preceding three years. Although the Many Farms Clinic was not conday
session with Dr.
cerned with psychiatric disorder countered some
cases.
as such, it
The Navaho
had inevitably en-
assistants, particularly the
"health visitors," were a valuable source of criteria offered in the
several steps.
comment. 3 The
following pages are the upshot of these
Cross-cultural Identification of Disorders
29
Criteria for Identifying Psychiatric Disorder
Non-Western Cultures
in
BRAIN SYNDROME The
APA Manual
divides psychiatric disorder into
two major
groups: the brain syndromes, which compose mental disturbances resulting from or precipitated
everything
by
lesions of the brain;
and
including mental disturbances in which brain
else,
damage is secondary, absent, or never yet demonstrated. The number of categories in each major group is about the same, but the number of cases occurring in nature is far different. The
much
brain syndromes are
having so
many
subdivisions
ciated organic malfunctions esting,
So
it
and
asso-
therapeutically useful, inter-
differentiations.
syndrome has the following
are seen in a variety of organic con-
and judgment, and manifest
The
latter
unwarranted list all
may
lability
and shallowness
agitation, irritability,
and "corny" joking.
syndrome and to discuss would be lengthy, repetitive, and
the subcategories of brain
concerned with only a small proportion of the psychiatric disorder likely to be found in
We
abil-
take the form of easily provoked cry-
their cross-cultural criteria
tion.
reason for their
impairment of orientation, memory, comprehension,
of affect.
To
The main
that the causal agents
and easy to frame numerous
ity to learn,
ing,
is
make
far as behavior goes, the brain
main components, which texts:
fewer.
shall therefore
monest subcategory
handle the matter
—
senile disorders
total instances of
any general popula-
by taking
—for more or
discussion and treat selected others in a
summary
the
com-
less detailed
fashion.
The
remainder will be omitted. I.
SENILE DISORDERS
Within
the
senile
disorders
we
include
the
cerebral-
cardiovascular difficulties as well as disturbances from the direct
aging of the brain. Below are given the items that require con-
Approaches
jo
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
making a diagnosis of senility. The first two are matters of background or contextual evidence. The remaining three are components of senile behavior itself. Since each of sideration in
these can also occur as a part of other patterns of disturbed
behavior, and no single one disorder
is
called a
essential, it
is
syndrome.
warrant stating that the condition
whether these are is
The
least fifty
why
this
sufficient to
present.
A. Age. For operational purposes, should be at
evident
diagnosis consists in eliciting
Its
plural evidences and then judging
is
we
suggest that a person
years old to be placed in this classification.
degree to which determination of age
is
a
problem
varies
With some peoples, such as the Gilbert Lawrence Island Eskimo, birth dates are known and so there is no difficulty. Among others such as the Rundi, the Navaho, and the Tapirepe, nobody is apt to know when he or anyone else in his group was born. In this case approximations can be achieved by finding out the dates of well-
from culture to Islanders
known
culture.
and the
St.
events (great storms, famines, accession of a chief, etc.)
and then asking which of these was
closest to the birth of the
person in question. This method has been used considerably by anthropologists and others in compiling
life histories.
In the interpretations of responses about age, one must also be
aware that
in
some
cultural groups answers of "young," "old,"
"middle-aged," or even answers in terms of years such thirty," refer to appearance
and
as
"about
status rather than chronological
We were once told by a Navaho, for instance, that a certain
age.
individual five years previously had been "about the same age
he
is
now."
B. Signs of age. Usually with senility there
is
indication of
physical wearing out: muscular wasting, general feebleness, dryness
and
and atrophy of the
skin,
and tortuous, pipe-stem systemic
retinal arteries.
Evidence of ticular
this sort is
not likely to be
difficult to ascertain.
may
require taking into account par-
characteristics of the
population under study due to
Its evaluation,
however,
Cross-cultural Identification of Disorders
5/
prevailing conditions of diet, disease, climate, and type of work.
As
show
well known, some groups
is
physical changes earlier
than others, and within any group there vidual variation. In other words,
is
concomitant of itself
may
or
parallel.
C. Loss of recent
This
also considerable indi-
and the syndrome
senility varies as to age of onset,
may not be
is
this suggestive
retention of remote
memory with
memory.
an exceedingly characteristic and early manifestation of
the senile syndrome.
Standards of
remote
The
recall,
of checking both recent and
memory and ways
must, however, be
crucial items to
employ
customarily interested.
worked out
are those things in
is
which people
are
A common practice in our own society from
for the psychiatrist to learn privately
what the person he
for each culture.
is
member
a family
examining had for lunch that day and then
ask him. This procedure loses
usefulness in a culture
its
meals are the same or in which lunch
is
not taken.
where
We
all
believe,
however, that substitute questions can be devised that will reveal this
type of
memory impairment.
D. Habit deterioration. This
refers particularly to matters of
personal care, neatness, and cleanliness.
It
may
manifest
itself in
lack of sphincter control, in sloppiness of dress, care of hair and fingernails,
While
and in
eating.
the standards of proper care must be
known
for a given
culture in order to detect this kind of personal deterioration, there are
few
if
any cultures
in the
world that do not have such
The Tapirepe, for instance, go about nearly naked, yet if a man exposes his glans penis, he is considered to have something wrong with him, and women must be attentive to standards.
maintaining proper bodily postures. E. Transient confusion. Here
we
are
ception, misidentification of people,
the environment. brief.
The
condition
During the daytime
ing the night
is
and misinterpretation of
frequently intermittent and
senile persons
wander about,
fail
concerned with misper-
may
be normal yet dur-
to recognize the people they
Approaches
%2
encounter and
confuse them
Thurber's grandfather thing
was his
when during
a deserter
grandson:
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
may
with those
known
have been an example of
youth.
in
this sort of
the night he shot a policeman, thinking he
from Meade's army, and then the next day asked
"What was
around the house
There can be
last
the idee of
night?"
little
all
them cops taryhootin'
4
doubt that transient confusion would be
noted by the people of virtually any culture, and
it
seems proba-
ble that a history-taking psychiatrist could learn of
formants were willing to
tell
his in-
it if
him.
Let us pause a moment to take stock, after
example,
this first
of criteria that might be employed cross-culturally. Within our
own society it is perhaps evident that senility is easy to recognize when the condition is advanced and information is available. The criteria are clear enough for that. The diagnosis can be difficult
and uncertain in cases where impairment
which information
situations in
slight
is
withheld, or in which
is
and
features conspire to obscure the picture. This latter occurs
time to time with people
who
through remote
memory
in
special
from
and habit
continue to perform roles they have been carrying out the greater part of their
For
lives.
a
may
long time no one
that they are disoriented for time, place, and person.
mentions
this sort
of thing
among
recalls the case of a professor
to a
home. His
The the
new
lectures,
that
it
was
many
is
said,
5
lectured so well to his classes
was not discovered
house and thereafter could never find
existence of
odd one
Thomas
older fishermen, and one of us
who
that an advanced state of senility
moved
suspect
remained for
a
until he
way
his
time unimpaired.
borderline cases and the occurrence of
obscured by special circumstances do not
require that the senile category be written off as too nebulous. If
one were to
on the
insist
on such standards, he would
also
have to
uselessness of virtually every disease entity
demiology.
The
point
is
and
insist
all
epi-
rather to recognize the limits of preci-
sion that are attainable in trying to "count every case" and to
focus on research problems that are workable within this level of
approximation.
Cross-cultural Identificatioji of Disorders
When we
turn to the application of the senile criteria to an-
other culture, our expectations must not be higher than the attainable in our
bound
to occur.
there a
much
own. The same
new
The
question
difficulties is:
and
risks of error are
Are there more of them, or
and different order of difficulty such
as to
is
suggest
greater, or a particular kind of, error?
We cannot generalize to
all
the cultures of the world, but
we
can say that for the cultural groups reviewed by our seminar, the answer
We
was no.
do not
absent, or that differences in the total different groups
count obtained in two
might not be due to cultural factors rather than
to differences of true prevalence. a
However, the
criteria
measure of cross-cultural applicability, and
if
do have
one wished
and count cases in two or more different cultural
to identify
groups,
problems are
assert that cultural
it is
reasonable to believe that through careful study of
made such One would not,
the particular conditions in each, adaptations could be
degree of comparability.
as to yield a useful
furthermore, need to be in the dark as to the likely sources of
and ways could be found for evaluating their extent. It would remain true that minor differences could be due to cul-
error,
tural factors just as
technique.
Major
they could to chance and to variation in
differences,
however, that stood up under
peated surveys and cross-checks on methods and sources of
would command serious attention as genuine tween the populations with respect to senility.
To
summarize,
it
bias,
differences be-
seems that although cultural difference adds
from an already complex
to rather than subtracts it
re-
does not appear to present insuperable
set of
difficulties,
problems,
and
many
of
the theoretical problems noted in the introduction to this chapter lose their
importance
when
actual groups are considered.
have not been seriously bothered with questions of statistical
concepts
of
normal,
of
deviance,
We
clinical versus
or
of
cultural
relativity.
must be admitted, however, that we have begun with one of the easiest syndromes for which to propose cross-cultural criIt
teria.
As progress
is
made
to other categories, especially those
Approaches
$4 that have
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
no known organic etiology,
it
can be expected that the
outlook will not remain so comfortable. 2.
SOME ADDITIONAL TYPES OF BRAIN SYNDROMES conditions to be mentioned here are those due to toxic
The
agents, infections, trauma, nutritional deficiency, disorders of
metabolism, and tumor. In
all
of these, diagnosis depends on
discovering and noting the characteristics of behavioral dis-
turbance and identifying the underlying organic condition or agent.
Acute disorders of the brain such as from high fever or the ingestion of some poisons may produce delirium, a fairly clearcut pattern of behavior.
It
involves rapid onset, disorientation
for place, time, and people, delusions, hallucinations and, outstandingly, fear.
When
this
occurs in the setting of an infection,
especially with a high fever, the presumptive evidence of a brain
syndrome
so strong that
is
could hardly have
any instances of mistaken diagnosis
statistical significance. It is
reasonable to ex-
pect that the same situation would obtain in another culture, the
symptoms being what they are and presenting no particular masked by cultural differences.
the physical signs of acute
infection
characteristics
apt
Delirium without evidence of a concurrent infection different matter diagnostically. possible organic causes,
such
as
There
are a
to
is
be
a far
wide variety of
and there are nonorganic syndromes
psychoneurotic dissociation and the acute onset of
schizophrenia that
may
for a time at least be indistinguishable
from delirium. The brain syndrome diagnosis then depends on finding the organic cause, or some concomitant neurological evidence of brain damage. Accomplishing
among
patients of our
exceedingly doubtful. there
is little
better.
The
The
own
culture,
With people
ground for expecting
best one can say
is
it
that the
this
and the
is
very
difficult
results are often
of other cultural groups
would be
either
problem
rare.
is
worse or
behavioral manifestations of other brain disorders, par-
Cross-cultural Identification of Disorders
$j
ticularly those that are chronic, are almost infinitely manifold little syndrome islands of more symptoms such as the Korsakow, which presents memory defect combined with a remarkable capacity to
and varied. Here and there occur or
less clear
gross fill
this
gap with plausible invention.
diagnosis depends not primarily
on the
On
the whole, however,
identification of particu-
of behavior, but on finding neurological signs of
lar patterns
disturbance and evidence of having taken a
having a disease that affects the brain, such
had
a serious
blow on the head, with some
known
as syphilis;
poison; of
of having
indication of cranial
injury; of having physical appearance and history of nutritional
deficiency;
of having endocrine disturbance; and of having
X-ray or electroencephalographic indication of the behavioral aspects do not raise
any questions
tumor. Since
a
that have not al-
ready been mentioned under senile disorder, or that will not be taken up
The
later,
we may pass them by.
identification of organic trouble within the is
far-advanced cases.
On
is
the other hand, the procedures involved
and the interpretation of
questions because of cultural factors.
results
There
should not raise is,
practical question of getting people to submit to
volved, and this might be affected
it
however, the all
that
is
in-
culture. The matter is not own society, and one could much more difficult among
by
always easy among persons of our readily anticipate that
that
usually difficult, except in extreme and
affecting the brain
are objective,
body
would be
people unacquainted with the European brand of medicine.
The
identification of possible toxic agents that
body from
To
come
into the
the outside presents a comparable set of problems.
deal with these
it
would obviously be
desirable to learn
one could about the use of alcohol and drugs and the bility of poisons in
all
availa-
whatever cultural groups he intended to sur-
vey. Thus, the fact that Navahos take Jimson weed (Datura) means they consume stramonium alkaloids. The Many Farms Clinic had seen a case of one severely disturbed girl with a his-
tory of eating
this plant.
She tried to disrobe and run out of the
Approaches
36 clinic
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
and the doctors had
difficulty restraining her.
Behavior of
type has been linked with Datura intoxication ever since
this
Captain John Smith's crew was was founded.
affected at the time
Peyote, which contains mescaline, usual clinical picture
Jamestown
another example.
is
The
of people with widely dilated pupils,
is
perspiring freely, in a trancelike state, living in an internal world
from which they can be aroused but prefer not
The main
to
perform ordinary functions,
to.
problem, however,
about the identification of
toxic agents in peoples of other cultures
some
that are
is
the possible use of
unknown to us, thus leaving the matter exceedThe behavioral aspects of many brain syn-
ingly open-ended.
dromes are nonspecific, and knowledge varieties of agents that
presence
incomplete
as to the
can produce them. Furthermore, even in
the case of those agents that are tests for their
is
known, few
reliable
and valid
exist.
These considerations do not appear very important in surveys of psychiatric disorders in general populations of North ica,
but
we
might be in
have to recognize that they
Amerspecial
groups. In approaching other cultures the possibility does exist that brain
syndromes of various obscure types are
significant.
This
target
if
a
is
a matter to
number of
because they do not
have in mind
cases are discovered
fit
statistically
as a possible research
which
alert suspicion
well into the other categories to be de-
scribed presently, or because the prevalence of one or
more
gories of these other disorders seems unusually high
and might
cate-
contain unrecognized brain syndromes. If
we
look back
now
sidering senile disorder,
at the conclusions it is
evident that
reached after con-
by
contrast
we
have
here the likelihood of some major problems as a result of cultural factors.
The problem
is
not,
however, a general theoretical
stumbling block and does not involve
much
puzzling over
how
to recognize abnormal behavior in groups with different stand-
ards and systems of sentiment.
The problem
is
much more
Cross-cultural Identification of Disorders specific: it
may
it
consists in the possibility that in
cultural groups
be customary to consume foods, herbs, and extracts that
affect the brain but are
and physiology. The
some of
that
some
37
unknown
difficulties are
our own modern
pharmacology
to the fields of
not diminished by the fact
drugs, such as iproniazid (for
tuberculosis), have turned out to be able to affect brain
ways not at first coming into use and
new
and be-
drugs are continu-
havior in
realized. Since
ally
are being distributed over the world,
there
is
some of these may
the possibility that
set
going one or
another kind of brain syndrome.
MENTAL DEFICIENCY Following the Manual's usage, all
grades of retardation.
it
has
no subcategories
The
we
include in this classification
category
as
we
propose to employ
that parallel those in brain syndrome,
only groupings according to degree of impairment. In our culture and society degree
is
defined
norms of performance, and
by
this has
statistical
deviation
from
been highly standardized
through
tests. It is often expressed as I.Q., with less than 70 being considered an indication of marked handicap, 70 to 85 borderline, and over 85 as reaching into the normal range and
above. Intelligence testing
is
a highly specialized field,
and the prob-
lems of comparing persons brought up with different kinds of
background, especially cultural, are well recognized. tions of
more genuinely disorder.
The
defini-
normal and subnormal (and supernormal) are much
For
statistical
this
than with other types of psychiatric
reason and because of the influence of up-
bringing and experience, each sociocultural group does have to
be a measure of
itself,
concerned. This
is
where the milder types of deficiency
not,
dition approximates an I.Q.
we
—that
where the conof 70 or under. In almost any cul-
grossly impairing kinds of defect
ture,
are
however, necessarily true for more is,
those
think, behavioral standards could be established that
would enable one
to single out the grossly defective.
The
fol-
Approaches
38
lowing
criteria
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
might be employed for identifying the more ob-
vious degrees of impairment.
A. History.
A
story of growth and development that shows
marked delay compared to other children of the same group. We have in mind particularly such matters as age of sitting up, walking, talking, recognizing people, developing commonplace manual
One
skills,
and grasping simple
difficulty
ways of handling the age
when
instructions.
that culture could involve radically different
is
children, so that cross-cultural comparison of
walking, talking, and so on occurred would be
inappropriate. This
is
certainly a matter to consider, especially
under some conditions, but for the kind of behavior that stands out in the history of persons issue does
who
have an I.Q. of 70 or
view. In the cultural groups reviewed
some
less,
the
not seem significant from the epidemiological point of
by the seminar
there
were
differences in standard expectations regarding child devel-
opment, but none of these was great enough to create problems of any serious proportion.
length with a
number
of
The
matter was also discussed
Navaho informants and
it
at
was evident
that they considered slowness in walking and talking as
indicators of retardation ("his
some
mind not strong enough
good
to take
care of himself") and that their standards were close to our
own. B. Current performance. If tests exist that are standard in the culture, they
may
help.
The
degree of retardation being con-
by very simple questions of memory, counting, informa-
sidered here, however, can be detected
aimed tion,
at
checking ordinary
levels
and puzzle solving. Data can
close to the person as to
how
also
be gathered from those
he performs in the daily round of
work, feeding, dressing, and understanding what people say to him.
One must be
alert to the fact that in
some
cultural groups
there are roles for the mentally deficient that might cult to find such persons.
make
it diffi-
Thus, the Buddhist Monastery
in
Cross-cultural Identification of Disorders
Burma may be India could
We
and being
a refuge for retarded boys,
on occasion help conceal
a
Sadhu
in
retardation.
conclude that gross manifestations of mental retardation
can be identified and difficult to detect
mild forms
that, conversely,
may
and distinguish from behavior that
The
rather than the result of intellectual disability. is
compared
cross-culturally, but rather,
how
cultural
problem,
upward from
far
gross toward the mild this identification can proceed. is
is
real
be very
not whether mental retardation can be identified and
therefore,
answer
39
likely to be forthcoming, but
it
No
the
general
does seem probable
that standards could be set, at least after field
trial,
for any
two
or three particular cultural groups one desired to survey.
WITHOUT DEMONSTRABLE ORGANIC DISEASE
PSYCHOSIS
I.
SCHIZOPHRENIA
The
is
vexatious because of the
among psychiatrists At the Second
International Congress of
diagnosis of this condition
range of opinion
as to
cluded and excluded.
what should be
much
Psychiatry in 1957 the focus was on schizophrenia, and
disagreement was in evidence.
The range
with intractable, incurable, irreversible
in-
stretched from people to
illness
those
who
manifested only some autistic thinking. It
is,
however, possible to select and specify
the range, and
we
have chosen what
tom complex
that
most American
The
we
a section within
believe to be the
psychiatrists
would
sympaccept.
criteria are:
A. Delusions.
A
delusion
may
be defined
as a belief at vari-
ance with those beliefs or sentiments accepted in the cultural
system to which the individual belongs. 6 the beliefs characteristic of a culture
most circumstances the presence of
is
An
understanding of
therefore mandatory. In
a delusion
evidence of psychosis, and where there
is
is
presumptive
lack of indication
Approaches to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
40
pointing to other psychoses, the most likely possibility
common
phrenia. Further, a
feature
is
is
schizo-
that schizophrenic delu-
sions are directed against cultural sentiments
and
at the
same
time are strongly influenced by them.
As
evident, the concept of delusion
is
cultural definition,
is
primarily a matter of
and hence confronts us with the problem of
While there may conceivably be some deluwould be false in all cultures of the world (for in-
cultural relativity. sions that
stance, that
human
beings have wings),
effort to catalogue these
it
is
apparent that an
would not get us very
far. It
must be
accepted that the content of delusions cannot be the basis of epidemiological comparisons between cultures. delusion
if
a
wandered up
man
in our society
It
would be
into his head and caused his brains to deteriorate.
In parts of India, however, this idea
is
culturally acceptable, and
apparently has considerable history in the East, being the
a
were to believe that semen had
Ming Dynasty
in China.
One
is
known
reminded of the
formerly held in Europe that a woman's
womb
could
in
belief
move up
her throat and choke her. Delusions can, however, be
into
detected against each culture's network of sentiments for defining reality.
This means that
as instances
of sentiments that are out of
keeping with culture, they can be counted in two or more groups that have different cultures. It
remains possible, even probable, that the character of the
sentiment system in various cultures
may
exert a differential
influence toward obscuring or revealing delusional behavior.
Cultures vary not only as to topics, but also
as to
sharpness with which they define credibility.
lem
this
amounts to
is
the degree of
How
big a prob-
something for investigation in particular
cultures selected for study.
B. Schizophrenic thinking. Like dreaming, schizophrenic think-
ing
is
symbolic and wish-fulfilling. There
is
persistent misinter-
pretation and preoccupation with inner fantasies, so that the
person seems to be out of touch with the world.
and the symbolic are characteristically confused,
The
as in
concrete
the patient
Cross-cultural Identification of Disorders
who
said
and
literally
meant he was "going up
41
in an airplane to
Although the schizophrenic himself often
get perspective."
makes extensive use of symbols, he has difficulty with other people's symbols and is apt to misunderstand metaphor. This kind of thinking, so characteristic of schizophrenia, is hard to describe, yet recognizable after one has become acquainted with
it.
The
quality of thought
is
very different from
shown by the person who is senile, mentally deficient, or slowed up by depression. At the same time, it can be expected to that
present grave problems for an American or European psychiatrist
attempting
ground not
his
detection in a language and a cultural back-
its
own. For these reasons we do not regard
we
strong criterion, and yet
because of
its
are disinclined to omit
frequency in cases of schizophrenia.
thermore, turn out in some cultures to be
than
as a
altogether
it
It
it
could, fur-
less difficult to
detect
we now imagine.
C. Hallucinations. hallucinations hallucinations
there
—
Whereas
consist in
—for
sensory perceptions. Auditory
when nobody
instance, a voice speaking
are probably the
vision, smell, taste,
delusions consist in false beliefs,
false
body
commonest, but feelings,
and touch
also occur.
Cultural sentiments and definitions present the distinguishing of this
some problems
hallucinations are sanctioned or "believed in" except
case.
in
phenomenon. In our own society few
the smaller religious sects.
may not be the
is
parallel disturbances of
Among
by some
of
other societies, however, this
Hearing voices and having
visions
may be
an
expected part of experience and take place on a basis that has nothing to do with schizophrenia or any other psychiatric disorder.
One must
distinguish, then, in part
on the
basis of the cultural
acceptability and appropriateness of the hallucination. Duration
provides another indicator. are
more or
less
Many
schizophrenic hallucinations
continuous for years, whereas those that
arise
purely on a cultural basis are apt to have short duration or occur
only from time to time
So far
as the situation
as cross-cultural
comparison
demands. is
concerned, the remarks
Approaches
42
made
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
in regard to delusions
may
be repeated here.
not the
It is
reported voice or vision as such that can be counted and com-
pared cross-culturally but the instances in which the voice or vision
is
out of keeping with the cultural framework.
D. Schizophrenic
Schizophrenia
actions.
primarily a dis-
is
order of thinking, but certain individuals develop related characbehaviors that are very striking: withdrawal, gesturing,
teristic
posturing, or lying in a stuporous state. In time there habit deterioration and
scattered,
silly,
manic features but without the manic's tivity
may
occur such
also
superficial
wit.
may
be
with
talk,
Unusual sexual ac-
chronic and open exposure and
as
masturbation.
The
cross-cultural
applicability
of this
criterion
needs comment. Although exceptional instances and
can be pointed out, behavior of
this
type
is
with which
as disordered in all the cultures
scarcely
possibilities
usually recognizable
we
are familiar.
E. Inappropriate affect. This means showing emotion that unsuitable for a given cultural context or failing to tion that
is
called for, or
is
or no outward appearance of emotion. This
little
latter is called "flattened affect,"
and although
a necessary part of the schizophrenic
very marked and
it is
syndrome,
across cultural lines. In India, life is
to
by no means
it is
sometimes
arrests attention.
This criterion in general should not give too
goal of
show emo-
may be laughing when sadness there may be a chronic state of
expected. Thus, there
or solemnity exhibiting
is
downgrade
much
difficulty
where for many people the whole affect, there
might be some
special
problems in distinguishing schizophrenic flattening from other kinds.
This
is
not,
however, something to be expected
fre-
quently in other cultures; and in any event, since the flattening
only one criterion in a syndrome,
it
with others in reaching a conclusion
is
could be evaluated along as to
whether
a schizo-
phrenic complex of symptoms was present or not. F. Personal
under
this
history.
heading.
One
Several is
may
characteristics
that the age of onset
is
be
noted
generally from
Cross-cultural Identification of Disorders
A
the middle teens to the middle thirties.
schizophrenic symptoms after forty
The mode
of onset has
one, a normal
youth
first
43
appearance of
exceptional.
is
two common
alternative patterns. In
change of personality over
suffers a
a
period of a year or so. Although the particulars of this change
vary a good deal,
usually so striking that everyone around
it is
the person takes note of
somehow
strange.
that year he
was
it.
He was
"The
always one of the bunch before, but
different.
seemed to be doing a
year in high school Jim got
last
He
kept more to himself, and he
He
lot of thinking.
wasn't the same guy at
all."
The
other
a bit rigid,
mode
is
one in which
a
who
person
has always been
withdrawn, proud, and uncompromising gradually
beyond the bounds of nor-
accentuates these characteristics
mal and then begins to show delusions and other frank manifestations of the disorder.
A in
its
third possibility
the sudden appearance of schizophrenia
full-blown form, perhaps
From common.
excitement and
much
is
is
less
fear.
There do not appear
first
clinical
to be
being manifest in a state of
experience one would say this
any major and general
cultural
obstacles to our getting personal history data of this type. are always chance
and
situational factors, in
our
There
own society and
in others, that cause the completeness of history to
vary from
case to case. This does raise questions of case comparability, but it
does not promise to be significantly greater cross-culturally
than intraculturally.
Considering the schizophrenic criteria calling again that they constitute a
as
a
whole, and re-
syndrome, so that one looks
for a plurality and evaluates each item in relation to the others,
seems that in most cultures one could detect level of is
impairment that
confirmed by the fact
we
associate
in
very
it
least the
with hospitalization. This
that, so far as
has been found in every cultural
at the
group
we know,
schizophrenia
which people trained European psychiatry have worked. The problem lies in the in
Approaches
44
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
gradation from obvious schizophrenia to conditions that are bet-
There
ter described as eccentric or maladjusted.
lem that most
if
not
is
with some of the milder forms of schizophrenia can
which the symptoms of the disorder
diagnosing any
symbolic
artist as
own
We
society.
are
at times
is
not hereby
we derive from psychiwe take the matter from we have known schizophrenic
because of meanings
style, or
work. Rather,
the opposite side and say that
who and who did
in
schizophrenic because of his abstract or
atric interpretation of his
patients
and
fit
are part of a role. Schizo-
phrenic thinking expressed in poetry and painting acceptable as art in our
prob-
which persons
cultures provide niches in
all
also the
did find a market for their schizophrenic thinking circulate in fashionable
bohemian
tient in particular,
known
without
circles
their disorder being at first evident in this subculture.
One
pa-
was an outstanding and
to one of us,
successful artist and just as outstandingly schizophrenic. This
was eventually obvious to his colleagues when he went to live on top of a pillar like St. Simon Stylites and then became mute. It
has often been suggested that
might well be an
what we
call
medicine
asset in the role of
schizophrenia
man
or Sadhu.
Delusions, hallucinations, posturing, and symbolic utterances of
unclear meaning might render the incumbent
more
rather than
From both clinical and anthropological experience we are inclined to doubt very seriously that this often happens. The healer or priest role has its requirements and regulations,
less effective.
and the unusual behavior that goes with particular context.
The
role
is,
it
in short,
integrated node in a social system.
The
nearly always has
most often
characteristics of the
we commonly
syndrome
is
highly
a
schizophrenic's behavior
and thinking are not that well controlled or adaptable, not in the kind of patients
see. In fact
the tendency to
at least
one of the
wander
off
the established pathways of society and to break through fences,
no matter
even where
how
self-defeating this
this countersocial
may
appear to be.
or asocial behavior
the oddness of the schizophrenic
may
its
still
is
all
its
And
not marked,
be apparent to others
Cross-cultural Identification of Disorders
may
of his social group even though he
some of
utilizes
his
45
be performing a role that
symptoms. Thus, one of the seminar mem-
bers said that in India people will
tell
you
that there are three
kinds of Sadhus: the quacks, the real thing, and those
who
are
touched in the head.
We
may
say again, therefore, that between the grossly evi-
dent and the almost normal
lem
is
a considerable range,
and the prob-
any two
to hit a similar cutting point for the people in
is
cultures to be compared. This does not
seem impossible, but
it
does require working out standards for particular cultures, and at present
it is
probably not possible for cultures
in general.
PARANOID STATES
2.
We
suggest
two
criteria
taken together as definitive of
this
condition.
A. Delusions of persecution and/or grandeur. The meaning of delusion has already been discussed, and the meaning of "persecution"
is
"Grandeur"
obvious.
refers to
a role
more
far
splendid, powerful, and important than one's actual role; the per-
who
son
thinks he
Napoleon
is
is
the conventional example.
This criterion, then, involves delusions with
a particular
kind
of content. B. Hostility
toward
others.
This
is
expressed in a tendency on
the part of the subject to interpret the entire world in terms of hostility
tion
—
toward himself. Projection
at least in the
is
a part of this interpreta-
simpler sense of the word. That
is
to say, he
reads into the acts of others hostile intentions against himself
which
are actually his
Paranoid behavior
own is
hostility
schizophrenic picture, and yet orders such as brain
toward them.*
thought of most often it
as part of the
occurs widely in other
syndrome and depression. That
it
dis-
occurs
alone as a chronic disorder with tremendous logical elaborations
on the *
As
basis of a is
well
few
false
premises
known, Freud employed
tion of projection.
a
paranoia vera
—
is
a matter
much more complicated
defini-
Approaches
j6
open to question.
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
Many
not most psychiatrists believe that
if
who seem
does not happen, but that people
at first to
this
have such
symptoms of some
a condition sooner or later develop additional
disorder, usually schizophrenia. Occasionally they turn out not
to be delusional at
but to have justification for their views.
all
Transient paranoid behavior time in almost everyone. for
what we do not
like
It
is
probably found from time to
seems human nature to blame others
but find going on within ourselves.
few
things are so painful as self-reproach and
Few
are so relieving as
righteous indignation.
With
regard to cross-cultural identification,
it
has to be recog-
nized that societies exist in which hostility, suspicion, and what
we would
call false beliefs
of persecution
(e.g.,
by witches)
are
exceedingly prevalent. Several familiar questions immediately
Are we to regard patterns which normal arise.
the society, or do
mass scale?
And
we
these sentiments and behaviors as cultural individuals learn through
have instead a
if it is
clinical
growing up
in
paranoid state on a
a cultural pattern,
how
can one ever
identify the truly paranoid in such a context? It is
easy to puzzle oneself into a state of paralysis
ing further questions along this
key
The
line.
by
issue often
elaborat-
seems the
obstacle to identification and counting of psychiatric dis-
orders in any culture except our own.
But this
us note several things. First, the question
let
comes up
in
major form with only one complex of psychiatric symp-
toms,
and not with psychiatric disorder generally. Second,
symptom complex number of different syn-
paranoid behavior as a clinical entity
which appears dromes, most in people
who
a delusion
is
as a
component
commonly
in a
schizophrenia.
are not psychiatrically
that
it is
ill.
is
a
It is also
Third, our definition of
a false belief according to the standards of
the believers culture.
That
a belief
is
false
according to the
standards of some other culture including our therefore
make
clinically
observed delusion
it
seen transiently
a delusion. Indeed, is
its
own
does not
one of the features of the
resistance to the
conforming
Cross-cultural Identification of Disorders
On
pressures of the social group.
47
we
the basis of these points,
think that culturally patterned sentiments and behavior reflecting
and what persons of our culture would
hostility, suspicion,
call
of persecution, are not to be equated with a mass
false beliefs
paranoid state in any clinical sense of the word.
be considered
as
more
They can
rather
closely related to a normal capacity for
paranoid reaction together with the normal process of culture acquisition. It
remains possible that masses of people in a
still
become
society could in a short time
terms of their
happening regarded
One can
cultural standards.
Whether
visualize this as it
should be
normal or abnormal would depend on the
results of
at a
as
own
and delusional in
hostile
time of great
stress.
or not
examining the actual situation and on standards
with
this particular
one that
is
tural pattern loses
down
this
into
more
a special case,
is
We
general in cross-cultural work.
problem of mass paranoid
therefore, that the
it
phenomenon. But
some of
its
set for dealing
may
and
is
conclude,
state versus cul-
formidable character
specific parts
not
if
one breaks
careful about definitions.
Let us return to the question of identifying paranoid symp-
toms in an individual where the cultural background gives
prominence to cuted.
It is
hostility, suspicion,
symptoms comprise only one of dromes
we
several
are trying to distinguish.
the schizophrenic syndrome, stances
and
beliefs of
being perse-
important again to keep in mind that the paranoid
to
make an
hallucinations,
it
would
identification
components
Thus still
if
be possible in
through
other
is
further reduced
by recognizing
terms of the culture in which is
bewitched
is
something about
is
many
in-
delusions,
schizophrenic thinking, withdrawal, posturing,
inappropriate affect, or history of personality change.
lem
in the syn-
they were part of
it
occurs.
The
that a delusion
The
is
prob-
such in
conviction that one
not a delusion in a witchcraft culture unless there it
that does not
fit
within the range of the
accepted pattern. It is
cases,
here, of course, that
one begins to get into borderline
and competent knowledge regarding the cultural range
Approaches
48
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
becomes important. Very often the question
is
not one of
specific items as such, but of appreciation of oddity in fit
together. This
may
how
they
be illustrated by a conversation one of us
had with Dr. T. A. Baasher of the Clinic for Nervous Disorders in
Khartoum North, Sudan. A. Leighton asked Baasher
delusion from accepted belief
how
he would distinguish religious
among people coming
to his clinic.
Baasher replied that he could give no general rules, but he could illustrate it
with a case he had seen that involved a murder. Some
two religious leaders in the neighborhood of Khartoum who were great rivals. A follower of one of the men began to hear voices telling him to kill his master's rival. It seemed to him that the voices came from a divine source and he set out to execute their command. Since he did not know his intended victim by sight, he wandered from place to place relying on the voices to let him know when he had come upon the man. Several times he saw someone he thought was his target, but the voices said, "No." Eventually one evening he entered a village where a number of men were sitting under a tree talking. One was on a chair, playing with some little sticks. The voices said that this was the man. The religious follower protested that surely not, he was just a poor individual, not a great leader. The voices, however, said, "Don't you see? It's a disguise. Kill." So the follower speared him. Later the follower was arrested for murder and then sent to Baasher for examination. years ago there had been
"Now," ture for a is it
is
not out of keeping with
man,
and of course he could make
as this disciple did. It
religious
to
fitted
man
is
things.
on
and
his master's
kill
the
wrong
within the cultural range for a
him
But the
together in this case did not
two
kill
a mistake
to hear voices that tell
go and do those
so since the
this cul-
to be extremely devoted to a religious leader, nor
out of keeping for him to undertake to
behalf,
him
said Baasher, "it
man
make
to do things, and for
way
all
sense.
religious leaders in question
these elements
This
is
especially
had made up
their
differences and been friends for over a year prior to the murder.
Cross-cultural Identification of Disorders
Item by item, there
is
nothing that was counter to the culture,
but the whole reflected odd thinking, like the Knight's
move
in
thought he was a paranoid schizophrenic from the story,
chess. I
and examination brought
3.
49
this out."
DEPRESSION AND MANIC STATES
The prototype tion
it is
of depression
characterized
by
grief.
is
As
a pathological reac-
excessiveness and inappropriateness for
the situation and for the standards of the culture.
appear where there sufficient,
is
no external
Thus
where the cause
cause,
or where the gloom in relation to the cause
The
cessively prolonged.
very strong. This
is
sense that
it
inexplicable
is
may
it
is
perhaps what Shakespeare had in mind
is
is
in-
ex-
often
when
he had Antonio say: In sooth It
I
know not why I am so
wearies me;
you say
it
sad;
wearies you;
how I caught it, found it, or came by it, What stuff 'tis made of, whereof it is born, I am to learn; And such a want- wit sadness makes of me, That I have much ado to know myself. But
Criteria cially
may
be stated
as:
weight
loss;
sleep disturbance, espe-
early-morning awakening; constipation; poor appetite;
amenorrhea; the subjective feelings of gloom,
and
tration,
behavior.
difficulty in thinking;
guilt,
poor concen-
and an objective slowing up of
Throughout the occurrence of such symptoms there is in common-sense terms or by the standards of
no adequate cause the
culture.
There
is
Suicidal
preoccupations and
acts
are
common.
often a twenty-four-hour variation, with the worst
feelings being in the early
morning and improvement occurring
toward evening. In some cases negativism and as well as to self
may
hostility to others
be very marked. In a few there are delu-
committed some unforgivable act or delusions of somatic type such as having no intestines or blood.
sions of having a
Some
depressions are cyclical, so that the patient has a
life
Approaches
jo
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
story of disabling periods with normal performance between, or occasional episodes of manic excitement. Such a history of alter-
nating mania and depression
is,
or course, indicative of the classic
manic-depressive psychosis. This pattern, however, even sic, is
A
if clas-
not by any means common.
number of other
subdivisions are also
we may
thinking, but
employed
in clinical
consider these as too detailed or special-
ized for the purpose of the present chapter.
The manic
state
sometimes not too easy to distinguish from
is
a schizophrenic excitement. Euphoria,
humor, and joking are the
hallmarks, and like depression the state of excitement episodic.
Sometimes
it is
set off
by
a situation,
may
but then
it
be be-
comes detached from the situation and goes on and on. Behavior is unconventional and some of the most strict taboos of the society
may
be broken.
The
expression of the emotions and of
energy takes precedence over convention. Outstandingly there
The
person
busy
the time, and his busyness
is
overactivity.
is
disorganized and distractable like that of a monkey. There
is
all
is
great deal of playfulness with bursts of anger and hostility frustrated; difficulty
and
with
as
with the depression there
is,
a if
as a rule, a lot of
sleep.
In their full-blown form, both depression and manic states can
probably be recognized in any culture. Again, however, protec-
may
tive coloration
How
do you detect depression among people of
gloomy
culture
in India?
is
—
as
Or manic
excitement, as
lem
be afforded by certain cultural backgrounds.
is
is
a generally
by some writers to be the situation where the culture has a high level of
alleged
states
said to be the case in parts of Africa?
The
prob-
the one discussed previously as the borderline case. Culture
adds to the difficulty of differentiation, but not necessarily in an
overwhelming manner. As with
delusions,
the standards for
"excessively" depressed or elated behavior have to be
within each cultural framework. The number of the other hand, parison.
may be open
worked out
instances,
to cross-cultural counting
on
and com-
Cross-cultural Identification of Disorders
As
for the
seminar,
it
was
cultures familiar to felt that
bly did occur in
all
j/
those participating in
the
depression and manic conditions proba-
On
of them.
the other hand,
that several clinical studies in Africa reported
exceedingly rare and excitement
as
very
we
took note
depression as
common by
comparison
with Europe and North America.
PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGIC CONDITIONS These are disorders that are primarily organic in their manifestations and consequences, but in which there are important psychological causes. Clinical studies in Europe and America point to certain
On
nature.
such
symptom complexes
we
a basis
as
being frequently of
this
suggest that the following types of
deserve attention: skin eruptions of the eczematous type,
illness
asthma, hay fever, hypertension, hypotension, peptic ulcer, and colitis.
No
interferences
from
cultural factors are to be anticipated in
distinguishing these symptoms.
The
necessary information can
be obtained by physical examination and fairly simple questions about personal history.
On one
the other hand, major difficulties are likely to appear
tries to establish
This matter
is
in every case,
the role of psychological events as cause.
always
difficult,
requiring exhaustive investigation
and even then one
strong evidence one
way
may
not be able to produce
or the other.
Cultural differences
would surely make the problem worse. Added possibility that a high exists in
many
morbidity rate of chronic
to this
diseases,
is
the
such
as
mask those major psychological component in their the open question as to which organic
parts of the world, could well serve to
disturbances that have a etiology.
when
There
is
also
syndromes commonly involve psychological factors to
a signifi-
cant degree. Clinical opinion ranges from implicating them in practically
all
disorders (through such mechanisms as lowering
resistance) to a
more
limited
cultures other than our
own
it
list
such
as that
given above. In
could well be that a different set
p
Approaches to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
would have major psychological
of syndromes
factors in their
origin and perpetuation. It
remains true, however, that despite
culties,
all
these serious
diffi-
the manifestations of psychophysiologic disorders are
easy to detect cross-culturally.
With
this
one advantage
made by counting
gest that a start can be
we
sug-
the syndromes in a
sample and seeing whether or not their prevalence, incidence,
demographic
features,
contrast markedly
and so on turn out to be the same or
from one
cultural
group to another. With
such a procedure, one could go beyond the conservative
we
syndromes
have given and note the distribution of
toms according to the systems of the body
all
list
of
symp-
as outlined in the
Manual. Oriented by knowledge of such comparative distributions,
one might then carry out intensive physical and psycho-
logical investigation of a selected sample of cases in order to
adduce evidence for or against the presence of compelling psychological influences.
PSYCHONEUROSIS we shall outline two symptom complexes that common among people of our own culture, and
In this section are clinically
then treat in a more Finally
I.
shall discuss
fashion several rarer types.
cross-cultural application of criteria.
ANXIETY REACTION
We in
we
summary
begin with the anxiety reaction or
some form
depression
is
is
state,
because anxiety
the chief characteristic of psychoneurosis. Just as
like
mourning, so anxiety
is
like fear:
it is
excessive
and has either no object or an inappropriate object. The person so afflicted has the subjective sensation of fear, and at times the
outward physiological manifestations, but the and
diffuse. Either
he does not
know why
he
feelings are feels this
the causes he alleges appear improbable to others. fort
vague
way, or
The discom-
and suffering, on the other hand, are apparent enough.
The
criteria
proposed are both objective and subjective. Ob-
Cross-cultural Identification of Disorders jective criteria
may
be stated
as: fast
53
pulse, rapid respiration, pale
skin, sweating, dilated pupils, facial expression of apprehension,
poor
sleep,
and
would
restlessness. Subjective criteria
include:
apprehension, worry, fear of dying, palpitations, cold sweats,
and nightmares. Since
it is
rare to see a person in such an acute or
severe state of anxiety that the objective criteria are immediately
we must usually
evident,
rely
on the
patient's story.
DEPRESSION
2.
The Manual,
following
attempts to
trists,
chotic
make
a
the
thinking
many
of
fundamental distinction between psy-
We
and psychoneurotic depression.
depression
admit, however, that
we
psychia-
have not found the points of
The
entiation sufficiently specific to be workable.
must differ-
distinctions
proposed are too elusive for standardization and the develop-
ment of
reliability
we
consequence
in
judgment between
psychiatrists.
As
a
suggest that the matter be considered primarily
one of degree. This means that the
criteria for identifying
psychoneurotic
depression are the same as those already outlined for psychotic depression.
County Study 7 contains very few cases, all of which
Applying these
yielded one category that
in
the
Stirling
have a history of being severely disturbed, whether episodically
many cases, most of which show mild yet exceedingly chronic disturbance. Both clinical experience and survey work suggest that depression, mixed with anxiety and other symptoms, is a very com-
or chronically, and another category with
mon
type of psychoneurotic disorder. Frequently, especially in
the mixed reaction, there the
functioning
of
the
is
a focusing
body,
and overconcern with
constituting
hypochondriacal
symptoms. 3.
SOME ADDITIONAL TYPES OF PSYCHONEUROSIS
Under
this
heading the following conditions
A. Conversion reactions. These consist
may
be noted.
in anesthesias, paraly-
Approaches ses,
and sensory
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
losses that are recognizable as psychological be-
cause the disturbances distribution
be consonant with the actual
to
and functioning of nerves.
B. Dissociative
fugue
fail
and multiple
states,
Grouped
reactions.
here
are
personalities.
These conditions appear to be exceedingly tures of
Europe and America at when they do occur
so dramatic
tion and remain long
amnesias,
the
rare in the cul-
present, although they
may
be
as to attract considerable atten-
remembered.
C. Phobias. People with this condition
show unreasonable and
incapacitating fears of specific objects or situations, such as elevators, trains, crowds, or
open
spaces.
D. Obsessive-co?npulsive reactions. intrusion of
unwanted
ideas, or the
Here
the trouble
the
is
need to do things repeatedly,
despite the feeling that they are foolish.
Referring again to the Stirling County Study,
show
its
findings
that psychoneurotic syndromes exclusively of one or an-
other of the six types mentioned above are rare. Although anxiety or depression
may dominate
the picture, there are often
mixtures of symptoms pertinent to the other subcategories. Indeed, the type of psychoneurotic
monly found mixed that
by
defined gories,
it
symptom complex most com-
one in which the distribution of symptoms
is
has to be given the label "Other"
—
that
is is,
so as
the Manual, not specifically in any of the subcate-
although comprising symptoms referable to a plurality of
them.
Many
studies
have
psychophysiologic
symptoms
purely psychoneurotic. neurosis
that
are
a
high
associated
proportion
with
the
of
more
It is
unusual to find a case of psycho-
unaccompanied by
gastrointestinal, cardiovascular, or
musculoskeletal
To
indicated
this
difficulties.
matter of range and amorphousness in psychoneu-
symptoms must be added by item the symptoms comprise
feelings
time or another are experienced
by almost everyone. This means
rotic
still
another consideration. Item
and behavior that
at
one
that the definition of psychoneurosis and the standardization of
Cross-cultural Identification of Disorders
procedures for
recognition are particularly
its
55
difficult.
A
solu-
tion has been to define the disorder in theoretical terms rather
than in terms of phenomena that can be observed.
One such
view,
for instance, gives emphasis to the dominance of unconscious
motivation. This, however, cannot be done in the approach
represented here, for reasons given earlier (p. 27).
Our
inclination, therefore,
is
to identify the
symptoms, and
then estimate the degree to which they produce impairment.
These may be considered
as
two
separate steps.
each individual could be surveyed
how
toms he shows and
first
That
to say,
is
to see what, if any,
these cluster into patterns.
symp-
Both number
and type of symptoms are relevant to the question of whether a psychoneurosis
is
present or not. Multiple diffuse
symptoms
of anxiety could be considered presumptive evidence, but so also
could the presence of one clear-cut phobia without any other
symptoms at all. With symptoms
second step would be to judge
identified, the
impairment. Criteria for
of course, important in
this are,
all
psy-
chiatric disorders, but in the brain syndromes, mental deficiency,
and the psychoses they are implied in the very nature of the
symptoms. This
is
not necessarily so in the case of psycho-
physiologic and psychoneurotic disorders;
person to have
many symptoms
function fairly well.
We
family relationships,
is
possible for a still
suggest in estimating impairment that
attention can be given to four
The
it
of the latter character and
community
main
areas of activity:
relationships,
work,
and recreation.
categories for rating impairment, as proposed in the Manual,
would appear
No
to be serviceable.
impairment.
Minimal. Perceptible incapacity, but not exceeding 10 per cent. Mild. Impairment in social and occupational adjustment, such as
20 to 30 per cent disability.
Moderate. Impairment that seriously but not totally interferes
with the
patient's ability to carry
on
his social
and voca-
tional adjustment, such as 30 to 50 per cent disability.
Severe.
Anything over the preceding.
Approaches
$6
On
such a
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
scale, the division
between
nificant impairment can be considered
as
and
significant
insig-
lying above "minimal"
and below "mild." "Moderate" and "severe" could probably
mark
the line between unhospitalized patients and the majority
of those
who are hospitalized.
Turning now to the problem of our point of departure
is
cross-cultural identification,
to note again that the psychoneuroses
present particular difficulties because of their protean character,
the lack of congruence in psychiatric definition, and the large
number of marginal
To jump
cases.
from
directly
study of people in one or more other cultures epidemiological study
hand,
it
criteria.
as
is
to
compound
—particularly an
confusion.
On
the other
does seem possible to establish operating definitions and It
cannot be hoped that these will
body's support, but
they
—
this into a
it
may
command
be possible to make evident
every-
just
agreement and disagreement are
are, so that points of
what clear,
well as the extent and limits of the ground covered by the
study.
The symptoms and impairment
criteria that
have been
already mentioned in this section are a sketch of operating defi-
North America. The question now is: Could they be employed in a cross-cultural study? The answer sums up what has already been said about other categories of psychiatric disorder. While it is to be expected that nitions as these
might apply
in
attempts to apply criteria in another culture will increase the
width of the margins of uncertainty, there assuming a priori that the cultures that
this
is
not ground for
renders the problem unmanageable. In
were used for reference
in our seminar there
appeared to be no sweeping barriers to the identification of psychoneurotic symptoms and degrees of impairment. remains, of course, that here and there a particular
might be
The fact symptom
difficult to detect.
For instance, anxiety might be hard to distinguish from tional fear
under certain circumstances. One
features of anxiety ever, if great
is its
free-floating nature.
enough may develop
a
ra-
of the identifying
Genuine
fear,
how-
free-floating character,
Cross-cultural Identification of Disorders
may
while on the other hand anxiety lose
its
condense into a phobia and
free-floating quality. In cultures
where witches
are per-
may
arise in
ceived as a constant threat, very real difficulties separating psychoneurotic anxiety
termined
fear.
from
rational, culturally de-
Obviously, fear of witches should not automat-
be labeled a phobia; nor should a preoccupation with
ically
witches and witchcraft be seen hand,
57
it is
as
an obsession.
On
the other
possible to take one's respect for culture too far
and
assume that fear of witches could not be a phobia. This could lead to finding
no phobias
in a cultural
group
in
which they were
actually present.
What we
are saying, in effect,
is
that the distinction
on the
fear (rational) and anxiety (nonrational) depends tural standards of the
between cul-
group to which the individual belongs. The
content will vary more or
less
from culture
to culture, but the
principle of relationships remains the same.
The
test of
whether the object of fear
concern needs to be son's
mode
of reaction,
tional within the
is
a legitimate source of
One must consider, too, the perwhich may also be rational or nonra-
qualified.
framework provided by the
cultural system of
sentiments. Poorly controlled handling of emotion a clue as to
may
provide
something psychologically wrong in situations where
legitimate witchcraft fear
may
otherwise be hard to separate
from anxiety and phobia. Although
concerned
with
(p.
48)
illustrates
both points
hand there
is
prehension
itself as
there
is
we
are trying to make.
On
the one
measured by cultural standards, and then
symptom
manifestations in the
way the emotion is managed or mismanaged. Finally we should like to suggest that even
if
anxiety were
cultural factors in particular individuals, this
not necessarily prevent our identifying these persons neurotic.
anxiety,
who committed murder
the detection of oddity or abnormality in the ap-
the question of further
masked by
than
rather
hostility
Baasher's story of the religious follower
The very
as
would
psycho-
protean, or multisymptomatic, character of
Approaches
j8
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
psychoneurosis argues against
this.
connection with schizophrenia,
hang on one symptom.
it
As was mentioned
If this or that
earlier in
unusual to have decision
is
symptom
particular context, other elements of the
is
sufficiently in evidence to permit identification.
own
various situational contexts within our
obscured by a
syndrome
are apt to be
This
culture
is
times obscure one or another aspect of a disorder, and it
true in
which some-
we
think
probable that the same will apply in issues of cross-cultural
identification.
PERSONALITY DISORDER Personality disorder refers to a condition that
and in the
similar to psychoneurosis;
is
outwardly
clinical assessment of cases
often particularly difficult to decide between the two. In
it is
general, personality disorder
is
conceived
as
being more diffuse
than psychoneurosis, spreading through virtually every aspect of personality.
It is also
thought to be
life-long,
beginning
early age and persisting ever afterward, unmodified
Although there
ence.
three of the
by
an
experi-
varieties of this disorder,
more common types occurring
be mentioned for
in our culture
may
illustration.
A. General emotional fly apart
numerous
are
at
under ordinary
instability. tests
of
life
This means a tendency to with symptoms of crying,
screaming, rage, or general confusion. B. Passivity.
A person so afflicted gives in too easily,
lets
others
push him around, and seems to bend without spine to every wind that blows.
C. Hostility
symptoms
and aggression. In personality disorder these
are chronic, or frequently
provoked by very
slight
stimulation, and they are extreme in degree.
We
shall
not discuss here the problem of cross-cultural identi-
fication, since
been tial
said
we would
only be repeating
much
of
about psychoneurotic and other symptoms.
points of concept and
what has
The
essen-
method have already been covered.
Cross-cultural Identification of Disorders
59
SOCIOPATHIC DISORDERS The Manual
considers sociopathic disorders to be a subgroup
under personality disorder, but separately.
The
reason
is
we
find
it
convenient to
list
them
that the central criterion of this cate-
is deviance from cultural expectation. The underlying syndrome may be personality disorder, but it may also be mental
gory
and even brain syn-
retardation, psychoneurosis, schizophrenia,
drome.
As implied above In
group
in the reference to deviance, this
the most culturally defined of
all
is
those reviewed in this chapter.
Western society the category presents major problems be-
cause
it is
so essentially a matter of relationship to norms,
norms vary enormously from one part of the another.
The
question
may
the behavior represented
The
three
main
is
system to
social
be raised, furthermore,
and the
as to
whether
a disorder.
divisions of sociopathic disorder are listed
below. 1.
Dyssocial. This represents amoral disregard for the con-
ventions of society. 2.
Antisocial.
Such persons act against other people with rob-
bery and violence. In the
first
above there
pursuit of one's
own
desire to harm. In the
enjoyment of such
is
disregard for the feelings of others in
hedonistic ends, but there
second there
acts
may
be
a
is
is
direct attack
no
on
deliberate
others,
and
prime reason for carrying them
out. 3.
Addiction. This
commonly means
alcohol,
but also in-
cludes overuse and dependence on a wide variety of drugs.
A that to
feature of is
all
three of these sociopathic patterns, and one
often used as a confirming criterion,
learn
from experience.
No
defeating the behavior proves
unable to relinquish
it.
matter
itself
is
how
a curious inability
costly
and
self-
to be, the sociopath appears
Approaches
60
The
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
seminar members saw no great difficulty in the identifica-
Most were sure they had
tion of sociopaths in other cultures.
encountered
The
just
such persons in the course of their
seminar members also
felt that in the cultural
field
work.
groups they
knew, there was common recognition of the existence of the sociopathic phenomenon. This might be put in terms such
"Occasionally you find people
who
are
against everything and they cannot help
made
way
way." Or, "There are people
that
regardless,
and there
is
as:
born outlaws; they are it
who
because they are just
go
their
own
nothing that can be done to change
them."
SOME NOTES ON DIAGNOSIS
We
have discussed the possibility of identifying symptom
patterns and impairment cross-culturally, and
cluded that
if
one
we
have con-
selects particular cultures instead of trying to
deal with cultures in general, he will probably be able to establish
operational criteria that will permit the desired comparison
at a useful
and meaningful
We also think
level of approximation.
that operational steps are facilitated
if
the detection of
symptom
patterns and the rating of impairment are used as a primary
focus in cross-cultural estimates.
At
the end of the seminar
it
was
the opinion of most participants, anthropologists and psychiatrists
alike,
culturally.
there
that
When
syndromes
as a
whole
are manageable cross-
one takes up individual symptoms one by one,
plenty of opportunity for postulating
is
and ground for arguments regarding considers the
symptoms
as
difficult situations
feasibility,
but
when one
components of syndromes, and takes
the latter as the unit for counting, the task does not look so difficult.
we do
Although problematic
cases will
undoubtedly occur,
not expect these to be so numerous
as to
cause serious
statistical difficulty.
When sider full
one moves beyond symptoms and impairment to condiagnosis,
especially with regard to the functional
psychoses, the psychoneuroses, and the personality disorders, he
Cross-cultural Identification of Disorders
61
must be prepared to meet considerable challenge. There are
many
bound
subtleties in the diagnostic process that are
by
affected
subtleties
cultural differences.
necessary in any
is
consistency, and
it
A
to be
constant awareness of these
work
that strives for accuracy and
seems appropriate, therefore, to outline a
few. In the psychiatric interview, the patient influences the doctor
and the doctor influences the patient; diagnosis
complex
resultants of this interaction.
The
is
one of the
diagnosis depends not
only on the symptoms and manifestations of the patient, but also
on the doctor's and
ture,
training, familiarity
his attitudes
influenced
by
with the patient and
toward the
patient,
which
the patient's attitudes toward him.
whether he
diagnosis reflects
his cul-
are in turn
Very
often the
and
likes or dislikes the patient,
whether he approves or disapproves of him. Unfortunately the hierarchy of values differs from doctor to doctor, from time to time,
and place to place. "Sociopathic disorder," for instance,
often implies dislike and disapproval. "Depression"
more
flattering than "schizophrenia,"
is
usually
which sometimes simply
implies craziness.
The
may
diagnosis
at times
be
a projection of the physician's
personality onto the patient. This
is
apt to happen in rather
obscure cases where the diagnosis
is
achieved empathetically,
that
is,
by
transient trial identification with the patient.
identification self
one runs the
useful, but
is
with the patient.
To employ
it
risk of
skillfully
one must have a
thorough knowledge not only of the patient and also of one's self
The
and of one's
psychiatrist
will be influenced also
toward
who
by
own
The
his culture,
his attitude
culture.
not only toward
and
his patient
his
toward Western
culture,
toward the
psychiatrist,
but
anthropolo-
by
the atti-
own
culture,
diagnosis will also be influenced
tudes of the informants and patients toward their
mental disease in general.
but
attempts diagnoses in another culture
his informants, his interpreters,
gist colleagues.
Such
confusing him-
and toward
Approaches
62
A diagnosis then
is
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
the result of a relationship, whether
it
be a
one-one relationship between psychiatrist and patient, or a
more complex one between
psychiatrist-interpreter-informant-
anthropologist-patient. Spiegel establishing relationships
8
has described his difficulties in
and doing interviews with the parents
of disturbed children from American-Irish Catholic families of
He
the lower income bracket in the Boston area.
new One
set of values,
and
new modes
and those found in dealing with psychotic
culties
other
were not psychotic, not
patients,
and people of other cultures
essentially
patients.
diffi-
Yet
and not from an-
These comparable problems of dealing with
culture.
psychotics
a
of behavior.
forcibly struck with the similarities between these
is
these people
as
new
language, a
had to learn
communication
difficulties.
can be described
The psychiatristown culture, in
anthropologist must maintain an orientation in his
the culture under study, and in the private world of the patient
under
discussion.
Our
interest in culture,
important, and possibly
from endemic at
illness
Many Farms
however, should not obscure the very
common, masking
such
as tuberculosis.
effects that
From
can
arise
the discussions
one would suspect that the problem
is
not so
much one
of disentangling neurotic and psychotic complaints from native superstition and folklore as it is of disentangling
such complaints from the organic background and perhaps from the tendency of unlettered peoples to express emotional dis-
comfort
in
terms of aches and pains.
Notes 1. E. Sapir, "Culture and Personality," in Selected Writings of Edward Sapir in Language, Culture and Personality, D. G. Mandelbaum, ed.
(Berkeley: University of California Press, 195 1 ) 2. R. P. Knight, "Borderline States," Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, vol. 17 (1953), pp. 3.
W.
1-12.
McDermott, K. Deuschle,
"Introducing 131, nos. 3395
Modern Medicine
J.
in a
Adair, H. Fulmer, and B. Loughlin,
Navaho Community,"
and 3396 (January 22-29,
I
9 5o), pp. 197-205 (
Science, vol.
and 280-287.
Cross-cultural Identification of Disorders
*3
Thurber, "The Night the Ghost Got In," My Life and Hard Times York: Harper & Row, 1933). 5. E. Llewellyn-Thomas, "The Prevalence of Psychiatric Symptoms in an Island Fishing Village," Canadian Mental Association Journal, vol. 83 4. J.
(New
(July i960), pp. 197-204.
A. H. Leighton,
A. Clausen, and R. N. Wilson, eds., Explorations York: Basic Books, 1957), Editorial Comment to ch. 2 ("Paranoid Patterns" by J. S. Tyhurst), p. 69. 7. D. C. Leighton, J. S. Harding, D. B. Macklin, A. M. Macmillan, and A. H. Leighton, The Character of Danger: Psychiatric Symptoms in Selected Communities [Vol. Ill, The Stirling County Study of Psychiatric Disorder and Sociocultural Environment (New York: Basic Books, 6.
in Social
Psychiatry
J.
(New
1963)]. 8.
J.
P. Spiegel,
"Some
(New
Cultural Aspects of Transference and Counter-
and Familial Dynamics, York: Grune & Stratton, 1959).
transference," Individual
J.
H. Masserman,
ed.
Native Conceptions of
Ill:
Psychiatric Disorder By Jane M. Murphy and Alexander H. Leighton
EDITORIAL NOTES
An
obvious fact about cross-cultural psychiatry
knowledge and techniques
is
that the
that have been found useful
in
our
society are not necessarily appropriate for investigating a non-
Western group. This chapter attempts clinical definitions
examined in the
last
to
move on from
the
chapter and to delve into
problems involved in obtaining the native outlook on such matters. Its
orientation entertains the possibility that the indigenous
views of another culture might be wholly different from those of Europe and America, or they might be surprisingly the same. It
is
theoretically
of
possible,
course,
that
groups have no idea of psychiatric disorder
happen
if
there were, in fact, no disorders
of that society.
It
could happen
if
all.
among
cultural
This could
the
members
the sociocultural system pro-
vided roles that could accommodate individuals,
at
some
as functional
members
all
equipped with no matter what inadequacies, disturb-
ances, or disorders.
ignore disorders
them "natural"
Or
it
could happen
—to overlook them
if
if
if
the group chose to
they were there, or to
they could not be overlooked. 64
call
Native Conceptions of Psychiatric Disorder probably
It is
safe,
however, to dismiss these
6$
possibilities as so
unlikely as not to warrant serious attention. There appears to be
no culture known today nor
at
any period
so startling an omission in awareness of
its
lack a concept of mental
in history that exhib-
human
behavior
would seem equally
illness. It
as to
foolish to
expect that any people anywhere consider such physical events as a
broken toe or an eclipse of the sun
as psychiatric
phenomena
even though they might figure in the preoccupations or delusions of an individual suffering a psychiatric disorder. Neither
does
seem possible that any people would
it
human being nor
because he thinks of himself as a
crazy
that
any
even a druid-worshipping group, could make very good
society,
man who
Alfred Kroeber once remarked, of a
use, as
man
call a
mute
himself to be a tree and stands
believes
with arms
in the forest
outstretched.
Hence we can assume variability.
may
probability
is
that
way they conceptualize psychiatric disorder. patterns we recognize may not be so regarded, as
differ in the
Some for
The
some bounds to cultural within these bounds cultures
that there are
of the
instance
changes with
psychophysiologic disturbances. havior
is
likely to
age
old
The
or
psychoneurotic
and
content of psychiatric be-
vary in some regards. For example,
it is
incon-
view of what is known about cultures that any group would not have standards of crime and sociopathy; but what behavior is considered criminal obviously differs from culture to ceivable in
culture.
Among many
hunting,
gathering,
groups, an abuse of animals and land
is
and
agricultural
thought of
as
a far
greater offense against the sanctity and viability of the society
than beating one's wife or killing an enfeebled old man.
It is also
certain that the explanatory systems for psychiatric disorder will
be different in other cultures. In approaching cross-cultural studies of psychiatric disorder,
we
are confronted immediately
by
a situation that calls for a
balance between skepticism and confidence. There
confidence that what
we know
already
is
is
reason for
applicable in
some
Approaches
66
ways
to
human
all
indicated that
it is
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
beings everywhere.
The
previous chapter
not altogether hopeless to attempt identifying
disorders, as conceived in clinical psychiatry,
among
peoples of
very different cultural background. Our current methods of
dis-
covering and evaluating symptoms also seem to provide starting points.
But there
is
need for skepticism that any analysis limited
to these resources alone
would be complete. At
the very
important items of content, norms, and specific deviance might be missed. will, that
If
we
find, as
of
evidence suggests
we
most cultural groups have a word
we
least,
criteria
in their language for
to the view suggested in the last what people in a particular group call "crazy," then opportunity is at hand for exploring what these people mean by "crazy" and how well it actually does relate to what we define as schizophrenia. Beyond this there is
"crazy" and
if
hold at
chapter that schizophrenia
the possibility of picking as latah,
a
koro, and arctic
all
is
up information about syndromes such hysteria which appear to be specific to
given culture, and of gaining illuminating suggestions
as to
causal relationships.
All of this argues for securing the native viewpoint, or range
of viewpoints, and
it
points to questions that should be recog-
nized concerning the use of data-gathering methods that are predicated on Western psychiatric criteria of disorder.
commonly
accepted,
employed thus
we
far in cross-cultural psychiatry have
quirements of freedom from cultural bias only
and
It
is
and methods
believe, that the criteria
met
the re-
at certain points
in limited degree. Until the last seven or eight years,
investigations in non- Western areas have been
most
conducted in
terms of one or another of the following: hospital admission rates,
penal records, vital
statistics
veying with the Rorschach
test,
on homicide and
suicide, sur-
the T.A.T., the I.Q., or other
and observations by itinerant psychiatrists. some respects culturebound, although it has long been thought that the Rorschach was less contaminated
psychological
tests,
All of these are in
than most
tests;
and when the T.A.T. has been used
in cross-
Native Conceptions of Psychiatric Disorder cultural research
it
has usually been adapted to
show
67
situational
stimuli appropriate for the culture being studied. In view of the
unknowns
that confront cross-cultural psychiatry, however,
seems wiser to begin with a search for intelligence, for example,
bly but
The
by no means
than to
with
start
a test that
presuma-
definitely discloses these.
present chapter
is
an effort toward the goal of discover-
ing such indigenous criteria of psychiatric disorders.
It is
ex-
ploratory in nature, and the information was gathered mainly
Murphy
as a part of a general
as a
by
anthropological investigation with
special attention given to health
The study
it
criteria of anxiety or
problems
an Eskimo
in
village.
whole has drawn on the observations and
assess-
ments made by two psychiatrists and one other anthropologist.
THIS
report deals with native conceptualization about psychiat-
ric disorder. In
approaching
background
exists as
this topic, it
a large literature
tendencies of non-Western peoples
Les Fonctions mentales dans
The Mind
of Primitive
tive
apparent that there
on the conceptualizing
for example, Levy-Bruhl's
les societes inferieures,
Franz Boas'
Man, and Paul Radin's Primitive Man As
Philosopher. 1 Although versial, there is
as,
is
much
of this literature
is
now
contro-
no doubt that the magical philosophies of primi-
man have been carefully
In order to illustrate
described and discussed.
some of the
issues
involved in the under-
taking of this chapter, an example of a similar conceptual prob-
lem will be used suggested
by
as
an introduction.
W. H. R. Rivers'
tion of Death."
2
report on
The example "The
has been
Primitive Concep-
Certain liberties have been taken for the sake of
conciseness and clarity, but they do not do injustice to
known
generally about non- Western peoples.
what
is
The example
is
this:
In Melanesia there
is
a
group of people whose language has
two words, "X" and "Y," which,
in
view of what people say
Approaches
68
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
and do, you soon equate with
and "death." You draw
"life"
conclusion because corpses at the time of burial are called
this
"Y"
and are contrasted to healthy, young individuals called "X."
you
stay with this group long enough, however,
you
If
discover
when they are not dead. person "Y" and bury him if
that sometimes people are buried even
You
learn that the Melanesians call a
he
very sickly or very old
is
who, according
—
bury people
in other words, they
to society, ought to be dead even
if
they are not.
Before long you learn further that the state of "Y-ness"
is
some-
by somebody in the group getting on the person now designated as "Y." further inquiry enhances confusion, because you find that
times thought to be caused
angry and casting Still
a spell
after the person called
now say
he
discover,
On
isn't
"Y"
"Y"
means something
taking thought
has been buried for a while, others
but rather
at all
"Y x ," which, you
gradually
like eternal-life-after-death.
you decide
that the Melanesians have a
very different conception of death from that given in the medical textbooks and collected clinical judgments of the Western world; and yet you have the uneasy feeling that some
as-
pects of this Melanesian life-and-death business have a decidedly familiar ring
and that perhaps your
own
obstructing a nicer division between is
concepts of analysis are
what
is
and what
different
the same.
As
a beginning
a concept
you
ask yourself about the difference between
and a word. Following dictionary usage
to call a concept "a mental
zation be."
3
from
by
a
particulars, an idea of
a thing
what
we
can agree
formed by
generali-
a thing in general
should
impossible to communicate a concept without using
It is
words, but held
image of
it
does not necessarily follow that
group of people
all
the concepts
will each have a specific label in the
language of that group. Nevertheless, in view of the close connection between language and culture, you suspect that this
important area to explore. that
what they
distinguish
Among by
the Melanesians,
the term "Y,"
related to their conception of death,
is
which
is
is
an
you note somehow
the basis of the cultural
Native Conceptions of Psychiatric Disorder
69
practice of burying both dead and nearly dead people.
word "Y"
about
also linked to the cultural beliefs
is
"Y x ." But
in
The
after-life, as
the significance of the difference in the Melanesian
meaning of "Y" and our meaning of the word "dead" does not altogether convince you that the Melanesians cannot tell a dead man from a live one or that they are unaware of the difference between
a state
of rigor mortis and a state in which the heart
beats and breathing goes on, even
if
these functions are impaired
as in a sickly old person.
In an effort to clarify the matter
you put yourself in the body of
Melanesian's position and look at some comparable
thought from one of the Western traditions,
way we hood
a
as for
example the
distinguish people in our kinship systems. In
all likeli-
Melanesian has separate words for male cousin, female
cousin, elder jnale cousin
on the
father's side,
and so on, and he
would be somewhat befuddled by the compression of all these useful distinguishing words into our blanket English term, cousin. He would be correct, we would say, in assuming nevertheless that this evidence does
unable to cousins.
language
men from women
tell
But he would is
important
also
mean
not
that civilized
cousins or old
man
is
from young
be right in noting that our kinship
as a reflection
of behavioral tendencies.
Most English-speaking groups do not have as many rules for distinguishing between people in the cousin category as the Melanesians have. In addition to the value derived from sorting out the conceptual from the linguistic
dimensions of the problem, the
Melanesian example indicates that
illness
to quite different causal mechanisms. cal explanations
such
the
phenomena
cal
and nonmagical
as
At
and death are attributed least
we
note that magi-
the casting of spells account for part of
of illness and death in the Melanesian mind. Magilevels of analysis
should be distinguished. Al-
though magical thinking has been given most attention literature, there
is
in the
evidence that no groups of people are entirely
ignorant of empirical relationships between cause and effect.
Approaches
jo
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
more thorough investigation of our hypothetical Melanesian group would very probably disclose that while they
For example,
a
believe that the burning of effigies kills people
has an after-life, they also
know
and that the soul
that eternal life does not exist
for physical beings, that old age eventually leads to physical death, and that life
may
be cut off by such nonmagical acts
as
the thrust of a knife into a man's heart.
In beginning to explore the interplay between magical and
nonmagical concepts of the origin, course, and treatment of diseases,
it is
possible to refer to a rather impressive literature
on the topic of primitive medicine. 4 Although
specifically
this
has bearing mainly for theories and classifications of physical
studying psychiatric disorders.
disease, there are guidelines for
One
of these
is
the indication given
by numerous
observers that
magical and nonmagical theories have a compatible coexistence insofar as natural causes and processes explain honjo illness and
death occur
—the nature of
while magical ideas explain afflicted. Also, there is ties
these
why
phenomena,
in other
words
that a particular person
it is
no evidence so
some other
pertain exclusively to physical illnesses and that
order of explanation pertains to psychiatric manifestations.
matter of
fact, primitive
is
far that witchcraft activi-
As
a
medicine has been said to represent an
uncompromisingly psychosomatic attitude toward both causal explanations and treatment. 5 Various psychosocial experiences
such
as the
perception of
ill-will in
interpersonal relations and
the belief in the potency of "hexing" activities are thought to be the veritable causes of disease. Correspondingly, treatment in-
cludes other types of psychosocial experiences such as confession,
group pressure, or acting out the
belief that the disease has
been destroyed or rendered harmless. In view of
this
mentally psychosomatic orientation, one of the
questions to
first
funda-
be asked in studying native concepts of mental disorder
whether and what
a distinction is
is
drawn
at all
between what
is
is
physical
psychical.
Here again we may
find a small but
growing
literature
on
Native Conceptions of Psychiatric Disorder
7/
primitive psychiatry and further bits of information scattered
throughout numerous ethnographic accounts of non-Western peoples.
6
Most of
this material focuses
on various kinds of
easily
observable hysterias and schizophrenias, including the culture-
We
syndromes.
specific
can learn, for example, the kinds of
behaviors that are called Utah or koro? Further, that the
Navahos
believe incest
insanity and that the
mechanism whereby the mind
thought to be a moth that
cifically linked to insanity
nized in non- Western groups.
The purpose It is
of this chapter
is
to
not a comprehensive
ways one might approach the problem,
any claim made for having achieved generalizations about
primitive psychiatry as a whole. Rather, an effort has been to gauge the parameters of the sions based
on the study of
during a given Site
The a
spe-
is
whole range of disorders recog-
progress in that direction.
analysis of all the various is
of Eskimos a dog spirit
is
We
9
ceptualizations pertaining to the
nor
affected
however, few systematic analyses of the con-
are,
make some
is
and that particular kinds of offenses
are part of the causal matrix.
There
can learn
around in the brain. 8
flutters
among some groups
learn that
we
specifically responsible for
is
slice
made
problem and to present impres-
a particular
non- Western group
of time.
and Kelevant Aspects of Eskimo Culture
questions outlined above
group of Siberian Eskimos
Island in the Bering Sea.
in
The
were
raised during the study of
Sivokak village on island
is
St.
Lawrence
situated at a transitional
point between the Alaskan, Canadian, and Greenland
Eskimo
cultures to the east and the Siberian cultures of the Chuckchee,
Koryak, and Kamchadal to the west. Until recently the
St.
Lawrence Eskimos displayed most of the characteristics typical of traditional Eskimo cultures: habitat is usually coastal or islanddwelling; hunting of sea
mammals
is
the main source of food,
clothing, and material equipment; wife-exchange
and religious
beliefs center
on the shaman
is
practiced;
as a medico-religious
Approaches
72
The
practitioner.
St.
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
Lawrence Eskimos
also shared
many
features of traditional Siberian cultures. This affinity
of the
was espe-
form of shamanism practiced on Lawrence and among the neighboring Chuckchee. 10 cially noticeable in the
It
is
St.
impossible to broach the topic of the psychiatric and
medical views of these Eskimos without reference to the beliefs
and practices of shamanism. Some background on shamanizing
is
shamanize in past
were
11
therefore imperative.
this
Both men and
on the
women
Lawrence shamans enter
basis of a "call."
carried out in private but they follow
The initiation rites are a common pattern. The
novitiate goes through a five-day period of isolation, in the cold
Lawrence
group but the most powerful shamans of the
transvestite homosexuals. St.
the profession
St.
and subjecting himself to
wandering
a variety of hardships.
Dur-
ing this time the prospective shaman enacts death and resurrection in a performance of breaking the bones of a bird and,
presumably, bringing
it
back to
candidates "go something like crazy."
At
that — crazy" "out of
life. It is
the end of this period
better and has "straightened
up
when
in his
him," he acquires a spirit-familiar
him
said
at this
time the
mind but not
the initiate begins to feel
mind what was bothering
who
will subsequently assist
in the healing rites.
As These
shaman does his curing in a seance. usually prefaced by a series of "tricks" of
a practitioner, the
sessions are
ventriloquism or sleight-of-hand.
shaman goes
into a seizure; he
The room
falls,
darkened, and the
is
seemingly unconscious, on
the floor, and then he rises as though possessed familiar. In this guise
by
his spirit-
he carries out various rational and magical
treatments.
There
is
shamanism
a controversial literature itself is
on the question whether
psychopathological
—whether,
that
is,
it
is
necessary for a person to be severely disturbed before he can be recruited to this role. 12
behavior
is
The
contesting view
simply theatrical
is
—demonstrating
that shamanistic
the learned and
highly controlled behavior appropriate to a certain role in
Native Conceptions of Psychiatric Disorder society.
What
13
shaman
zation of
Lawrence Eskimos think about the
the St.
normal or abnormal
73
qualities of
seizure
shamanism
as
and homosexuality
well as their as points
utili-
of refer-
ence in discussing psychiatric disorder will be taken up
later.
obvious that shamanism rests on magical beliefs about the
It is
and cures of
causes
The background
disease.
literature
on
magical disease concepts must be reviewed briefly in order to give context to the St.
Lawrence Eskimo
situation. Several use-
ways of categorizing such beliefs have been developed, first by Clements in a world survey of available ethnographic litera14 ture in then in a reassessment of his categories by 93 2, Hallowell in 193 5, 15 by Rogers in 1942 and 1944, 16 and finally in a somewhat different approach by Whiting and Child in ful
1
1
95 3.
17
Building on earlier work, Rogers in 1944 proposed a topi-
cal outline to I.
which the following explanations have been added:
Proximate Disease Causes A. Object intrusion: Sickness
is
by
believed to be caused
the
intrusion of a physical object into the patient's body.
Sickness
intrusion:
B. Spirit
to be caused
by
C. Sotd loss: Sickness soul
Remote
is
believed to be caused
is
believed
by
the loss of the
by
spirits
during sleep, sneezing, or fright.
Disease Causes
A. Black magic: Sickness activities
burning B.
—
insanity
—the soul being thought to depart from the body or be
stolen II.
—especially
the intrusion of a spirit into the patient.
of
human
is
thought to be caused by the sorcery
beings
—
casting spells, saying evil prayers,
effigies.
Dreaming: Sickness is believed to be caused by an experience in a dream such as the ingesting of an object or the loss of the soul while dreaming.
C.
Violatio?i of taboo: Sickness
is
believed to be caused
transgression against cultural rules
by
a
committed by the patient
or someone else in his family or group.
D. Divine wrath: Sickness
is
believed to be caused
by
the anger
of the gods due to some act of omission or commission relevant to the
way people
are supposed to treat gods.
Approaches
74 It is
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
evident from this categorization that proximate and re-
mote causes often work hand-in-hand and
may
emerge from these
beliefs involve
that
The
any one
disease
rites
which
such varied phenomena
as the
require both kinds of explanations.
curing
magical extraction of an object from the patient's body; exor-
cism of the possessing
spirit;
by
the recovery of the lost soul
the
medicine man's supernatural powers; discovery by the medicine
man
of the nature of the taboo breach, the eliciting of con-
fession,
and the prescribing of
acts of expiation;
and the medi-
cine man's counteractant sorcery in order to mitigate the force
of the
initial
black magic.
In classifying the same
body
of ethnographic literature,
Whit-
ing and Child focused on another kind of variation in explana-
Their topical headings are
tions of illness. 1.
Agent: The illness-producing agent
may
as follows:
be a living person with
supernatural power, a ghost of an ancestor, a
or an animal 2.
Responsibility: illness,
Act:
The
The
Materials:
etc.
The
perform
may
be the breaking of a taboo,
rituals, etc.
illness-producing materials
strual blood, spirits, 5.
god,
may be solely responsible for his may be entirely in other spheres, with
illness-producing act
sacrilege, the failure to 4.
spirit, a
patient
or the responsibility
other people, 3.
human
spirit.
magical weapons,
may
be poisons, men-
etc.
Means: The means by which the materials have
effect
may
be
ingestion, introjection (material being magically taken or thrust
into the body), removal or loss of the materials, their being used in rituals, etc.
Where do
the St.
Lawrence Eskimos
Interestingly enough, almost
rency in
this
fit
into these schema?
of these ideas have some cur-
ideas were elaborated some were more prevalent than others. In
group although some of the
rather differently and his
all
world survey, Clements noted that the "soul
especially
common
borne out
as
in the Siberian arctic.
well in the
St.
Lawrence
loss" idea
is
This dominance was
data,
but there were also
Native Conceptions of Psychiatric Disorder cases of black magic, taboo violation, divine wrath,
75
and dream
experiences.
Clements observed that
Spirit intrusion presents a special case.
usually the only
form of
sickness attributed to spirit intrusion
insanity (the utterances of the
mad man
is
often being taken as the
may
voice of a resident spirit), although madness
also
be ex-
by soul loss. Clements found that the concept of spirit intrusion was notably absent from Siberian cultures; its absence he believed to be the logical outcome of the high development of the soul-loss idea. It is in the Siberian and Siberian Eskimo plained
groups,
however,
shamanism.
He
that
spirit
possession
the
is
hallmark
of
explained shamanism, therefore, as an atrophied
form of the idea that spirit intrusion causes disease and implied that shamans were sick people. The St. Lawrence Eskimos recognize that
spirit possession
a
is
phenomenon of shamanism,
but they do not therefore regard shamanism to insanity.
any of the
Nor
being equivalent
cases of psychiatric disorders recognized
scribed as such in this study. This fact transitional character of the St.
intrusion
as
did they consider spirit intrusion as relevant to
may
Lawrence
and de-
well relate to the
culture, since spirit
better developed as an explanation of mental disorder
is
among
the central Eskimo groups. (For example, a schizophrenic Eskimo woman from Southampton Island who believed herself to be host to a fox spirit has been well described
Another aspect of the this
literature
by Teicher. 18 )
on Eskimo culture relevant to
chapter concerns the culture-specific disorders that have
been observed and described, often under the general heading of "arctic hysteria."
To
the east of St.
Lawrence
Island
—among —
Greenland, Canadian, and to some extent Alaskan groups
syndrome
called piblokto existed. 19 It
widespread
it
may
historical records
ness.
Some
impossible to
evidence on seventeen cases, mainly
which he summarized temporary
is
tell
how
have been. Gussow recently collected from
states of
as follows:
women,
piblokto involved episodic and
excitement with disturbance of conscious-
seizures lasted a
few minutes;
others nearly an hour.
Approaches
j6
The concomitant
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
behavior included such things
as
disrobing or
tearing off one's clothes, running away, rolling in the
jumping into
or
(mimicking animal or bird
glossolalia
lake,
a
snow
sounds or jabbering in meaningless neologisms), unusual but harmless acts such as trying to walk on the ceiling, throwing things around, grimacing, mimetic acts, choreiform movements,
and coprophagia. 20 Characteristic psychopathology
Siberian groups.
21
"copying mania"
in
some places
found among the
also
women,
Especially with
—
was
called
this
appeared to be
amurakh.
It
a
included
endless and exhaustive imitative behavior (dancing, running, ob-
scene posturing) which could
somehow be
words or gestures known by the such an
effect.
A
similar disorder
"triggered off"
rest of the
by
group to produce
(though not
as specifically a
female reaction and usually wilder and more paroxysmal) was called menerik. This included periods of screaming and dancing
which culminated
in an epileptoid seizure.
designated as "sickness"
when observed
in
This was natively
some people,
but, as
already indicated, this kind of behavior was also a routine and integral part of shamanizing
on both
sides of the Strait.
The
between these disorders and what was Lawrence Eskimos will be presented later. By the time of our investigations of the St. Lawrence culture
differences and similarities
discussed
by
(1940 and
1
St.
954- 1 95 5) the island had been
outside world for nearly a century. For
was mainly
in contact
in contact
much
with the
of that period
it
with Russian and American whalers, then
with missionaries, with people in the governmental school system, and more recently with
armed
forces.
The
these contacts has Island
members of
the United States
by Lawrence
progressive modernization brought about
meant
that the 1955 culture of St.
was no longer "primitive"
in the classical sense. It
was
a
culture in transition, with a fairly large segment of the population
displaying
various
degrees
of
bilingual
facility.
Public
shamanism had declined, and there was increased mixing of the ideas of
Western and shamanistic medicine. In the process of
Native Conceptions of Psychiatric Disorder
77
Eskimo views of psychiatric disorder, like everything have very probably undergone some modification. These
transition else,
Eskimos exemplify, therefore, what Redfield has described mixture of a "great tradition" and
a "small tradition."
as the
22
Questions raised by the influence of contact in the case of the
Eskimos have been given considerable attention in the following analysis. The effects of cultural contact add another dimension of complexity and represent a problem of almost insurmountable difficulty, because there are
Western groups
left that
Although
outside contacts.
remarkably few isolated non-
have not been somewhat influenced by
from early ethnographies
a certain
amount of reconstruction
possible, the goals of
is
research are inevitably plagued
by
contemporary
the implications of culture
change.
Method The
field work on which this report is based was carried out summer of 1940 by Alexander Leighton and Dorothea Leighton and for the year of 1954— 1955 by Jane Murphy and
in the
The
Charles Hughes.
data on native viewpoints were part of an
epidemiological study of psychiatric disorders in Sivokak village.
Of 1 1
3
were found
falling in the 1 1
3
between 1940 and 1955, have symptoms that A. Leighton identified as
the 495 Eskimos living in the village to
province of psychiatry.
The
case histories
on these
Eskimos combine information from various sources. The
main source was
a series of interviews carried out
by Murphy
with a bilingual key informant, during which the informant was asked to describe the
Eskimos
who formed
cal study.
life
and health experiences of each of the
the base population for the epidemiologi-
Since this was the major data-gathering operation
from which the methodology of
analysis of native conceptualization stems, the this aspect
of the research
is
described in an
extended chapter note. 23 Additional sources included comments
by other
native
informants,
the
available
medical
records,
records of the meetings of the village council (the native govern-
Approaches
78
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
ing body), information regarding mainland hospital admissions
and federal court
actions, life histories of individual
comments made by
local teachers
and public health nurses, and
by
observations recorded in field notes
two
anthropologists.
It is
Eskimos,
two
the
psychiatrists
and
24
obvious that the case materials consist of a welter of infor-
mation given and recorded in English and derived from both
Western conceptualization. Also, they include
native and
evi-
dence that has varying degrees of reliability and validity. In some cases as
many
as
seven different sources would indicate that one
individual had something psychiatrically wrong with him. In
other instances the information was fragmentary and far from
congruent. (The method used for dealing with
this
problem of
different levels of confidence in an epidemiological study
is
presented elsewhere. 25 )
Having
From
by
natives
we
could then review
on the comments made by Eskimos. of
the specific instances
scribed
we
available this collection of data,
the case materials focusing
pathological
have inferred
a
functioning de-
general
orientation and specific psychiatric ideas which,
psychiatric
we
believe,
represent the conceptualizations of this group of Eskimos.
This method
is
obviously oriented primarily to concepts
contrast, for example, to the
method used by Frake
—
in
in studying
among the Subanun Our focus on concept does not mean a disregard None of the investigators in this study was able to
the linguistic categories of physical disease
of Mindanao. 20 of language.
speak or understand the Eskimo language.
we
achieved proficiency in the language,
been
much
lieu of this,
virtue of a necessity. In order to
were forced to explain
more
we
communicate
their ideas
of us
would, of course, have
better equipped to understand
mo's psychiatric concepts. In
Had any one
at
fully the Eski-
tried to all,
make
a
the Eskimos
through the medium of
a
limited vocabulary in English. Their
terms used in English was sparse. quently, they
knew
knowledge of psychiatric As will become evident subse-
several lay terms for psychiatric
phenomena,
Native Conceptions of Psychiatric Disorder but they
knew none
of the technical terms such as "depres-
sion," "anxiety," "schizophrenia," or "mental deficiency." Their
lack of a psychiatrically sophisticated vocabulary in English and
our lack of proficiency in the Eskimo language meant that
we
had to ferret out their psychiatric views uncommitted to the semantic restrictions of either language.
The
inability to speak
Eskimo did not preclude gathering data
about the meaning of particular Eskimo words. Hughes was especially interested in language, and he contributed
most of the
information that will be presented about the psychiatric concepts that do and those that do not have a label in the
Eskimo
language. Although this part of the methodology was not ex-
on St. Lawrence Island, we incorporated on native words in a later study of the Yorubas
haustively carried out greater emphasis
of Nigeria. 27
Eskimo Psychiatric Orientation
We are quite certain that the Eskimos referred to in this study do not have to
a
word
in their language that directly corresponds
our word "psychiatry."
The
first
question,
therefore,
is
whether they have the idea of psychiatry. At no point in the field
work was
it
stated that
we were
interested in psychiatric
was a definition of psychiatry ever offered for up and elaborate or compare with their own. From materials it was possible to determine that they have
disorders, nor
them
to pick
the case
notions about a variety of behaviors, emotional states, and think-
ing patterns which they recognize as "something being
wrong"
and which they clearly distinguish from purely organic orders.
The most
succinct statement of this referred to a
who by several accounts the widow of a powerful
exhibited psychotic behavior. She
shaman. After
were trying to times to run away
his
to think that people
kill
She tried several
in the
dis-
woman was
death she had begun
her through witchcraft.
middle of the winter, and her son would find her wandering on the bank of the lake. This was summarized by the key informant as, "her sickness is
Approaches
80
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
getting wild and out of mind, but she might have sickness in her
body
too."
The
fact that the
body-mind dichotomy was
expressed in this statement does not
Eskimos
failed to subscribe to the
previously
tioned
mean
Lawrence
psychosomatic attitude men-
being characteristic
as
the St.
clearly
of
most primitive
groups. Nevertheless, this statement was one indicator
among
many that the two elements were at times conceived separately. The next important question is whether or not our subjects recognize the alliance between
body and mind. Do they have
the
idea of psychosomatic interdependencies? If they
do conceptual-
we
infer as being
ize this
interdependence, what criteria can
employed
to
mark
off
what
reviewing the case materials recognized by us
is
we
as psychiatric
described as either
psychiatric and
what not? In
observed that the disorders
were the ones that the Eskimos
(i) psychologically expressed, or
(2)
psy-
chologically derived.
The
psychologically expressed disorders were those having to
do with personality
characteristics as reflected in patterns of
thought, perception, emotional cluded, for example,
"not right mind," habits," "too
Although
all
much
states,
and behavior. These
what the Eskimos
"ignorant mind,"
in-
called "out of mind,"
"slow to learn,"
"bad
nervous," "easy to get afraid," and so on.
of these
phenomena were manifest
at the
psycho-
logical level, the Eskimos had a variety of explanations about what caused them. As indicated in the first example, one type of explanation concerned magic and witchcraft. In these instances a
psychological experience since witchcraft depends
is
considered to be the causative factor,
on the psychological readiness to be-
lieve in the effectiveness of magic. Surprisingly, this
type ac-
counted for relatively few of the cases about which explanation
was
offered, perhaps because the
Eskimos nowadays are some-
what inhibited on this topic. The other types of explanation had more in common with Western psychiatry, but we do not believe they were imported. They appear to have emerged from rational observation of
human
behavior, and several times they were
Native Conceptions of Psychiatric Disorder
couched
in terms of "this
just
is
what
81
think myself." There
I
were comments, for example, that indicated some awareness of heredity.
The "slow
ning in that family." ness to
wc
(which
by
to learn"
A
syndrome was described
interpreted as personality disorder)
to explain psychological manifestations. It states of
as
—
it's
their
also called in
seemed to be well
unconsciousness or delirium might be
associated with severe physical illness.
once pointed to
"run-
was referred
the informant as "all those brothers are the same
way." Various kinds of physiological events were understood that
as
pattern of eccentricity and habitual odd-
A
blow on
what must have caused
the head
a child
was
(who had
appeared to be developing normally) to change suddenly in her
mind
—she talked or sang nonsense
incessantly. In addition to the
magical, hereditary, and physical explanations, the Eskimos have the notion that
life situations
may
cause psychiatric
illnesses.
For
example, a death in the family might precipitate such an episode.
A
man who
lost a
much-beloved daughter went on an unsanc-
tioned shamanizing binge following her death. Like the Eskimo
shamans, he claimed that he would bring the dead back to
He became
life.
wild and unrestrainable, carrying out various kinds
of quasi-shamanizing activities, until finally a bona fide
shaman
cured him, and after that nobody "paid any attention to
his
shaman."
The
psychologically
physical sensations or
derived
mood
disorders,
disturbances,
usually
involving
were those which the
Eskimos linked to one or another psychological experience. As with the psychologically expressed disorders, even though the
Eskimos do not have the technical terms for explaining phe-
we
nomena
as
inferred
from the
do, a psychiatric frame of reference could be specific case descriptions.
instance of "heart beating too hard"
"too
was
much worrying," one woman was
For example: an
said to be caused
by
said to faint at the sight
of blood and another at being in crowds, a temporary facial paralysis
was
attributed to "too
much
brain work," and another
conversion reaction producing a temporary inability to walk
Approaches
82
was
said to
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
have been caused because
woman's husband
this
abused her. It
a
appears from this that the Eskimos in our investigation have
way
of identifying and understanding certain kinds of
behavior which can be called a psychiatric orientation.
is
divergent in
With
bears
to
among
the particular content of magical theorizing found
kimos of
It
Western psychiatric thought, especially as evidenced in Western lay attitudes. It is markedly that segment of the conceptual map which involves
some consonance the latter
human
Es-
this area. this
background on orientation
it is
now
possible to turn
to the specific concepts that constitute the psychiatric thinking of
the St.
Lawrence Eskimos.
Eskimo Concepts
Specific
For purposes of continuity,
this section
of the major categories of
symptom
in the previous chapter.
Taking
is
organized in terms
patterns that
drome" or "psychoneurosis," we then review
the
order to see what corresponds and what not. phasized here that
we
view, and our attempt in the
look is
were discussed
a topic such as
at these data
to construct
from
It
"brain syn-
Eskimo data
a clinical point of
from multiple
minds of Eskimos would compare with
in
should be em-
what model of
cases
a clinical
a particular kind of psychiatric entity.
I.
BRAIN SYNDROME
One of the commonest brain syndromes is senility. The St. Lawrence people have a word for the condition when "a person's brain no longer controls him, when he is 'out of mind' as in old age." It is impossible to tell from our data whether this is considered an
illness
or simply one of the natural courses of
life.
was not thought of as an illness for which shaman treatment would be sought. The kinds of aberrant thinking, feelCertainly
ing,
it
and behaving that were noticed by the Eskimos
emplifying
this
condition are very similar to what
we
as
ex-
think of as
Native Conceptions of Psychiatric Disorder
and consistently these manifestations were related to old included "becoming like a child," "crying a lot," "hav-
senility,
age.
83
They
ing to be taken care of," "partly crazy, doesn't
walks to the beach instead, but sometimes
know way home,
in right
mind," "not
up from the ground and eating them,"
eating," "picking things
"making odd sounds
is
at night,"
"trying to
kill
himself," and so
forth.
Regarding other types of brain syndromes there are
two words
in the
Eskimo language for convulsions
at least
—one
of
them especially denotes a state of unconsciousness. Unconsciousness was clearly described, as for example in a brain concussion following a hunting accident. Also, a small child, thought by the public
health
meningitis
Eskimo
nurse
—leading
to
have
brain
a
ultimately to death
in the following
tumor or tubercular described by an
—was
manner. "She was kind of paralyzed,
and then she got very sick before she died. She was unconscious, and she was sounding
any
like a little
direction, shaping to this side
pup, and her face moving in
and that
side."
Epilepsy or epileptoid seizures were described several times. In view of the manifest similarities between some aspects of
psychopathological seizures and the seizures enacted in shamanism,
it
seemed to us that
find out
how
similarities,
this
might be an area
in
which we could
the Eskimos conceptualize the differences and
pathological
the
and
the
nonpathological,
and
whether the one might be used to help define the other. It
appeared that our subjects clearly conceived the difference
between shamanistic seizure and
a variety of kinds of seizures
The distinguishing characteristic of shaman seizure was possession. The other types of seizures or quasi-seizures, involving "falling down unconscious," ranged from epileptoid seizures to fainting episodes. The Eskimos' disthat
were defined
tinction
tween
as illness.
between the
latter
was phrased
as the difference
be-
and "fainting." (Fainting will be discussed with psychoneurotic reactions.)
The
"fits"
kinds of
phenomena described
as "fits" can,
with some
Approaches
84
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
what Western psychiatry looks upon as evidence of epilepsy. What occurred in these episodes was "stretching out himself," "foaming coming out of mouth,"
assurance, be equated with
"shaping or twitching the face," "snoring very loud," "peeing in her bloomers,"
"when
pee in bloomers."
had
in
mind
It
well again, crying because ashamed of
seems evident from
a certain recognizable
this that the Eskimos syndrome of symptoms which
had been observed often enough to become a pattern of
illness in
their conceptualization.
was
It
said that "fits" of this nature
were caused by the
spirit
of a fox. For example, the father of a child with "fits" was said to
have been hunting one day fox.
When
the fox died,
was
to have
catching foxes.
from from
him wear
wounded but
did not
kill a
this trouble for the
as a coat the net
and there were
their lives.
all
he
began to make
then used for
We heard reports of children who had recovered
this illness, it
when
one of the treatments for a child with
child. In earlier days, "fits"
it
also adults
who had
Apparently the recovery
suffered
(based
rate
perhaps on spontaneous remission) was impressive enough for a specific shamanistic treatment to be accepted as effective. is
Also
it
interesting to note that "fits" are the only psychiatric phe-
nomena
which the Eskimos gave such
for
a specific
magico-
causal explanation.
MENTAL DEFICIENCY
2.
From our
interviewing
Eskimos do not have
it
appeared that the
a general
St.
Lawrence
term for mental deficiency.
suspect therefore that mental deficiency
We
was not conceived
as
being responsive to shamanistic treatments. Although there was
no
overall
term for the condition, there were
many
phrases in
English used to denote a conception of mental deficiency: "slow to
learn,"
quent." follows:
"ignorant mind,"
The
and
—graphically— "not
an elo-
behaviors that clustered in this syndrome are as
"slow of speech," "looking in some direction, some-
times even without blinking," "very
dumb
at school;
he stayed
Native Conceptions of Psychiatric Disorder for years and years in second grade, so he
was
tall
8$
and he
just
shake his head side to side; and he doesn't hunt; and he couldn't find himself a wife,
much
somebody had
to help him," "didn't learn
and she has been going too
in school
with most of the
far
boys around here," "never learned to talk until he was doesn't talk back to
anybody but once she beat up somebody."
This pattern was summarized by one informant
something
person whose mind
like ignorant, a
anything, thick and clumsy, and
he do
even
it
anything about
As can be
five, just
drowned," and "that dull one
sort of grunted, then later he
if it is
when
told to
as,
"We
call it
is
not thinking
do
a thing, then
know
harmful, just because he doesn't
it."
seen, there
were
performance was related to
number of
which mental the formal school system which had a
cases in
recently been introduced into the culture. This fact, together
with the lack of a specific term, might suggest that the conception of mental deficiency had developed as a response to the testing
tainly
bold
ground provided seemed
it
relief.
as
in a
Western
style of education. Cer-
though school brought these phenomena into
It is interesting,
however, that the same kinds of
retarded behaviors were described for certain elderly people
who had
never been exposed to anything except routine Eskimo
culture: "She doesn't
can't say
words";
know how
some of the words
"He
doesn't
to fix things at home, and she
in the right
know much
way
up when he was young and he never hunts,
The
Eskimo
just stays
by
was the
the various particulars of this
tionally thought to be the
it
home."
best evidence that the mental deficiency concept had
origin in the indigenous culture
scribed
either,
about trapping so he gave
its
fact that people de-
syndrome were
handmaidens of sorcerers.
It
tradi-
was
this
kind of person who would be sought out by a black-magic worker and used for errand running. A mentally defective person might be approached by a sorcerer and coerced into collecting nail parings or scraps of hair
whom
evil
was
to be
worked.
from the victim
against
Approaches
86
3.
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
psychosis
There
is
Eskimo term
a specific
that can be translated as
"crazy" or "insane," and the English word "crazy" was em-
ployed with several variations crazy," "a
little bit
in the case materials
crazy," "out of
—"sort
mind but not crazy," and
of so
on. Generally the term pointed to patterns of behavior and
thought that are indicators of functional psychosis. Sometimes
it
was used for other syndromes, such as a senile person doing things that were "a little bit crazy"; and a person who appeared in
both Eskimo definition and our
personality disorder
was described
own as
observation to have a
sometimes acting "sort of
(This will be more fully discussed under personality
crazy."
disorder.)
Focusing
now on
the delusional,
hallucinatory,
states of unreality involving the cognitive
we
ness,"
in part, sis
face an area for
was
that there
which our data
were
relatively
few
dimension of "crazi-
are spotty.
as delusion
scribe than overt
is
is
phenomena such
not going to do
delusional pattern such its
as,
it
it
if a
as "fits."
was
that covert
St.
However we have
Lawrence Eskimo goes
in a culturally particularistic
In other words there did not appear to be a
with
reason,
more difficult to observe and de-
concluded from our data that crazy he
The
cases of frank psycho-
in this small population. In part, too,
behavior such
and other
common and
way.
specific
for example, the witiko psychosis
systematized beliefs and cannibalistic impulses, which
has been
found among the Northeast Woodland Indians. 28
Lawrence Eskimos provided us with some insights about their view of certain unusual mental states. One of these conditions was called "thinness." "Thinness" enabled people to "see things which other people don't see," to look into the future, to prophesy, to inspect a world of reality Nevertheless the
that
is
St.
normally not observable to other people and thereby to
find lost articles.
This
is
considered a highly valuable attribute
Native Conceptions of Psychiatric Disorder
among Eskimos, and many people their ability.
All shamans were
cultivated
"thin," as
8j
to the best of
it
were
also
numerous
they were called in this culture peculiar gurgling or crackmaking because divination involved ing sounds in the back of the throat). None of the people who were described as "sick in mind" and "crazy" were also called "thin." This is one hint that the Eskimos distinguish between a
minor diviners ("yawners"
as
and culturally patterned delusion
socially useful
—"thinness"
and the cognitive aberrations of being "crazy."
"Out of mind," on
the other hand,
is
a ubiquitous concept,
varying in meaning from unconsciousness and possession to
When
shaman went into possession, he was said to be "out of mind" but not crazy. Presumably this was a way of saying that the shaman was, for the duration of the psychotic reactions.
the
seance, not in possession of himself but rather possessed and
controlled
by
his spirit-familiar.
As was
the case with "thinness,"
the socially useful aspect of "out-of-mindness,"
was not conceived
as
i.e.,
genuine insanity. What, then,
of true insanity in the Eskimos' eyes? Within the limited cases, possibly,
we
we
the
way
the stamp
framework of
could not find the one perfect criterion
suspect, because there simply
insanity in the
is
possession,
Eskimo view,
different elements
is
no
—
quite
mark of
single
insanity being conceived instead as
go together. The psychotic
woman
example believed that somebody was try-
mentioned
as the first
ing to
her through witchcraft. Since witchcraft fear
kill
we
ceedingly widespread,
paranoid belief of
this
is
ex-
could not conclude that a seeming
nature was a criterion of insanity; the
point was, rather, that this belief featured in an
illness also in-
volving wildness and running away.
The
closest
we
could come to
by
experience designated ness"
was the case of
a
a specification of a subjective
the Eskimos as an attribute of "crazi-
woman who
recurrently experienced
periods of nervousness, "fits," and being "crazy and out of mind."
The
report of
what she believed had happened
to her
is
as fol-
Approaches
88
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
was married as a teenager to an elderly blind man from was greatly mistreated and starved, and she decided that she didn't want to live. She went out in the night during winter, stripped off her clothes, and lay down in the
lows. She
the Siberian side. She
snow so that she would freeze to death. It seemed to her that she went to sleep and then woke up. She saw a lumber house and she went in and found a stove and food, and she ate and got warm. It was like a "white people's" house. She remembered this experience as being similar to a dream. When she woke up she was still in the snow naked and her body had melted the snow clear
down
to the earth.
She did not
but her stomach was
lumber house any more,
see the
She got up and put on her clothes, and
full.
there
was nothing wrong with her body;
it
Then
she started to take a journey, and she
was
There was always
Some
a light in the
of the things this
acquired.
The
a
frozen.
by
a light.
led
sky to show her where to go.
woman
believed happened to her are
actually the motifs in several folk tales
happening onto
was not
—being
led
by
difference
is
that outside of shamanistic possession
no other Eskimo to our knowledge experienced events such
as these.
said or believed that he
The
a
had
improbability that these
experiences were real was enhanced in the Eskimo
oddity of including
a light or
house where food and shelter could be
mind by the
"white people's" house rather than the
Eskimo house found in Siberia. As compared to these inner, subjective
traditional
perception, describe
was apparently much
behavioral
mindness," eating
it
as,
wrong
ting wild"
states of belief
easier for the
associated
characteristics
with
for instance, running away, hiding in
and
Eskimos to "out-of-
odd
places,
things such as feces, and the manic states of "get-
—impulsive,
recalcitrant,
and sometimes homicidal or
suicidal.
Although no this
was used
specific magical theory
order of psychiatric phenomena,
dent that insanity, though variable
major anchor point
in the
Eskimo
it
in
to account for
seems abundantly eviits
manifestations,
psychiatric framework.
is
a
Native Conceptions of Psychiatric Disorder
4.
89
DISORDERS
SOCIOPATHIC
A. Sex deviance. The Eskimos
in this
study recognize several
The concept
kinds of sexual deviance.
of homosexuality
is
especially well formulated, and there are Eskimo words for
"womanlike man" and "manlike woman." Homosexuals who became transvestite shamans were thought to be exceptionally powerful, but homosexuality in
There were no
man was
one
be "a
to the
Eskimos for
many
this
.
years ago
.
?
.
In the States do they have like
young boys
—no moustache, baby-skin
the time.
manner, "Didn't you
some men acting
are pretty worse, they are after
long ago
studied, but
type of behavior, but "anasik"
in the following hesitant
ever hear in the States here
we
There was apparently no English word
anasik."
was described
Up
was severely disapproved.
homosexual, and one or two others were said to
a
little bit
known
itself
transvestites in the population
He is a pretty fatty man,
.
.
?
women, and some One died not too
too.
and he keeps sewing
face,
but he
.
isn't
all
always a real anasik
because later he got a wife."
The St. Lawrence culture recognized wife-exchange between men who had entered into a special kind of brotherhood bond. In addition, the culture was generally permissive regarding occasional
nonformalized heterosexual
fairly easily
affairs,
and divorce could
be achieved. Nevertheless, the Eskimos appear to
have standards of sexual activity that allow them to judge heterosexual
excesses
of a pathological nature.
voyeurism, described
as
"he
is
girls
undressing or sitting on the pot
it's
his
Although
brought in from the outside
of
these
was
always showing us funny pictures
of naked people or looking into people's
habit."
One
this
—
he's
pattern
windows trying
always been that way, involved
—pictures—the
conceived censoriously, and the young
to see
an
artifact
overall behavior
man
in question
was
was
brought before the village fathers for punishment and supervision.
Similar village action
was brought
to bear in controlling one
Approaches
yo
—
two people
or
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
defined as promiscuous
a girl "going too far
many boys," or "not controllable by her parents." A young man was sent by the Eskimos to a federal jail because, "among other things, he stays home from hunting and 'forces' all
with too
the
women, any woman from
The only
B. Addiction.
recent history of
St.
16 to 60."
intoxicant that could be abused in the
Lawrence culture was
alcohol. Fly-agaric
was used aboriginally on the Siberian side, but there was no mention of it or other local intoxicants by our informants. For many years, however, alcohol was available from American and Russian whalers. In 1878 the St. Lawrence people are reputed to have gone on a
rum debauch during
the hunting season. Failing
to lay in an adequate supply of walrus
population was decimated
by
meat for the winter, the
starvation. Later, a self-imposed
prohibition against the importation of alcohol
was enacted.
Throughout the was not
in our study,
lifetime of
most of the Eskimos
was changing by 1955 when beer could sometimes be obtained privately from soldiers at an installation near the village). This background explains the alcohol
easily accessible
relative lack of experience
(this
with alcohol on the part of the recent
population. Nonetheless the concept of overuse or misuse of alcohol was quite clear, on the basis not only of the 1878 debacle,
but also of observation of specific individuals.
grown up
in the
and
his sensations
me
he
felt
as
whaling era was
known
One man who had
as a
"big drunkard,"
during inebriation were described:
though he were walking two
feet
"He
told
above the
ground." C. Antisocial and dyssocial behavior. There were numerous acts against the rights of others or against
that the
moral prescriptions
Eskimos considered to be punishable or reprehensible.
who
Some
of the individuals
learn
from experience or punishment were thought of
carried out these acts and did not
permanently "bad characters." These
acts
as quite
included stealing,
physical violence against others, and repeated swindling. tain
A
amount of maltreatment of women was not considered
cer-
to be
Native Conceptions of Psychiatric Disorder
91
— abnormal "women used to be pushed low around here" —but even
this
could be carried too
to the village
far, as
when one man was brought
governing body because he beat up
his
wife "too
much." Another kind of sociopathy in the conceptions of these Eskimos is black magic. Although the practice of sorcery and witchcraft, like shamanism,
witching
as
is
on the
an antisocial behavior
is
decline, the still
"You
can't trust that kind of person, he
with
the devil,
and
lots of
concept of
viable; for example:
knows
songs,
is
in
touch
people believe that he killed a
man
recently with his ways."
5.
PERSONALITY DISORDERS
The St. Lawrence language has a term for a person "who knows what he should do but has no sense." By one informant, this was not considered a sickness, nor was it mental deficiency; it meant lack of judgment and often making a nuisance of one's self. As such it seems to parallel rather well our concept of personality disorder. A young man who fitted this pattern was said to be "sort of crazy."
For example, "He goes around saying
a false voice, 'I'm the
United States of America Legion on
island' ";
but
all
the time he knows, of course, that he
informants said that he
is
is
not.
in
this
The
always fooling, and he doesn't take care
of himself.
A
dimension of personality disorders pertinent to Western
psychiatric thinking
is
the passive-aggressive continuum.
It
is
comments under sociopathy that aggressive were recognized as such in this group. There were other
evident from the acts
instances,
however, when aggression seemed to be thought of
a personality feature
even though
the level of intensity that
would
its
expression did not reach
incline
Eskimos to
institute
severe restrictions and restraints against the person. "Getting
very easily" was their native store
was
fired
way
of depicting
from
this post
by
this.
A
when he was
mad
manager of the
the village council be-
cause "he has got the 'educational' for the job but he gets
too quick, the same as
as
left in
mad
charge of the mission
Approaches
$2
when
the missionaries
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
went on
leave,
it
got into a mess with
everybody's feelings hurt." This kind of response was noticeable to Eskimos and was easily discussed, partially because it required some kind of action or adjustment on their part. The same was
not true of passivity.
We
have
likely to interfere
a less definite picture of its
con-
was thought deviant or with normal functioning. There were assess-
ceptualization, especially as to
ments of people, however,
whether
it
"very humble," "doesn't talk
as
rough to anybody," "very quiet."
It is
evident then that Eskimos
recognize differences in personality and that some of these differences approximate the aggressive-passive polarization. 6.
PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGIC REACTIONS
As
indicated earlier, the Eskimos conceive a relationship be-
tween psychological events and physiological reactions beating hard because of too
much worry,"
"headaches due to
brain work," and "vomiting but not sick, just throwing
she hears a bad story."
of words such
Eskimos,
if
It is
lence, but
there
up when
was no use
"asthma," or "hypertension."
To
the
they suffered from any of these syndromes, they
were described aches."
As might be expected,
as "ulcer,"
—"heart
"trouble breathing," or "head-
as "belly ache,"
not our purpose in
it is
this
chapter to discuss preva-
our general impression from the epidemiological
uncommon. It may be, concepts of these more complex
investigation that asthma, for example, therefore, that the
Eskimos lack
is
psychosomatic reactions because they lack experience with them.
It
may
also
be that the heavy burden of tuberculosis and
other physical diseases in this population masks the prevalence
and reduces the likelihood of conceptualization. (The topic of physiological reactions
7.
is
the focus of the next chapter.)
PSYCHONEUROTIC REACTIONS
The
native vocabulary for neurotic traits
is
quite rich. St.
Lawrence Eskimos have a general term for "worrying too much until it makes a person sick," and others for "easy to get afraid"
Native Conceptions of Psychiatric Disorder
and "too
much
nervous, sitting with head
93
down and
rocking."
—
As can be seen, English terms were also used "nervous," "fear," and "worry." The word "anxiety" was never used, but it
We
appeared that the idea of anxiety was in their minds.
mean
"worry" and "fear" were always qualified as excessive or inappropriate when they were remarked upon as indicators of sickness or malfunction; for example: "You know, he
by
this that
sometimes worried and afraid too much. 'Alingatuk,' that
what we
call the
man who
is
easy to get afraid
when something
happens like bad weather or sickness or out of food. the chairman of this village once, but he that time, he hates to rule the people
is
He became
was worried too much
around here, and so he
refused later on."
There were cases described by Eskimos that correspond to several of the more specific varieties of psychoneurotic reactions known to Western psychiatry. One of these was phobic with a claustrophobic focus: to stay indoors, always
"He
worrying and wants to stay outdoors; he
worrying too much, but is."
don't
I
know what
kind of sickness that
Several appeared to be conversion reactions.
for example,
walk and
was described
can't
as,
how
she
his fingernail
later a
Coast Guard cutter
along the sole of her foot;
and jerked and the doctor told her to try walking time and
of these,
she was beaten so bad by her became paralyzed, but I guess she
thought she was sick because
doctor ran
One
"She was paralyzed once, can't
move around much;
husband, that was just
has been very sick once, even hates
it
jumped
a little at a
now she is okay."
Some
kinds of fainting, as said earlier, were definitely linked in Eskimo minds with psychological events. These types of fainting seemed to be associated with the functioning of the heart, which served as a premonition that a faint would be experienced: "She is become frightened all of a sudden and get the
nervous, heart beating fast," "so nervous she almost collapsed."
These sensations were thought of
as
"heart attacks,"
which
cul-
minated in a swoon or sometimes in the feeling of choking, and
Approaches
94
were concomitants
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
coming
to such experiences as seeing blood,
near a sick child, or being in a
crowd.
Other symptoms of anxiety that the Eskimos meant when they talked of nervousness were trembling, shaking.
One
sickness"
—
and
restlessness,
was actually called "shaking was one feature of the claustrophobic syndrome mentioned above, and the onset of the illness was coincident case of "nervousness"
this
with the patient's conversion to Christianity. In addition to the equation of "nervous" with "trembling," there was also a meaning that referred to "lack of feeling" in
much
way we
the same
would refer to anesthesias: "During that time this side of my was nervous no feeling." There were two or three cases in which a reaction focusing on the facial muscles was noted: "Her face getting loose right after marriage it went loose or
—
face
something 8.
—then she got
—
all
right."
DEPRESSION
This
is
not
a separate
described in the
last
symptom
pattern category in the system
chapter because depressive symptoms can
feature in several kinds of psychiatric disorders. Since depression is
an important psychiatric concept,
look
at the
Eskimo view of
The concept Lawrence
we wanted
to take a special
it.
of suicide has had a long history in the
culture. In the past, ritual suicide
St.
was practiced
the person seemed to be suffering an interminable
usually
if
ness, if
he thought he could safeguard the
taking his own, or
if
life
ill-
of a sick child
he were excessively despondent.
29
by
The
pattern of such suicides was highly formalized, and the suicidal act took place in a designated public area.
of people
who had
living Eskimos,
died this
no one
in
way
Although we learned
during the
memory
spans of
our study population had committed
ritual suicide.
The
idea that a person might contemplate a private suicide in
moods of
hopelessness or in despair at misfortunes
was
recognized, as indicated in the psychotic episode of the
also
woman
Native Conceptions of Psychiatric Disorder
who
95
Another instance was a crippled girl unsuccessful love affairs, drank part of a
tried to freeze herself.
who,
after several
bottle of Clorox in the
hope that she would
She did not die
die.
but continued to have periods of depression which she described as I
involving disinterest in what happened to her; but she
do die
I
hope they put
reach up and get out of this self
door or window in
a
my
said, "If
can
coffin so I
in case I'm not dead." In addition to
it
by unhappy
report of depressive feelings, there were descriptions
Eskimos that they had observed
person to be sad or
a
even though these emotions did not reach the level of preoccupation with suicidal thoughts.
Thus
the
Eskimo conceptualizations
of mood, at least as portrayed through English, indicate that
they identify patterns that are closely allied to depression.
CONCEPTS FOR SPECIAL PSYCHIATRIC REACTIONS
9.
There were two Eskimo concepts of psychopathology diverge from the Western psychiatric outline
lowing thus observed in
far.
even though death
of these
a sufficient
Western
the
One
is
"voodoo" death,
number of
it is
name
to
it
rarely seen in our culture.
well developed on
St.
activities
Lawrence
that
have been a
fol-
phenomenon
primitive tribes that people in
tradition have put a
through witchcraft
we
—
—thanatomania
30
The concept
psychological
Island.
That
despite the gradual dwindling of sorcery and
it
death
of
—
is
remains viable
shamanism
is
indi-
by the fact that a recent death in the village was explained by numerous Eskimos as being caused by a particular witch and the belief of the deceased that he would die this way. Interestcated
ingly enough, the public health nurse in this instance could offer
no
alternate or
One
other
more kind
plausible cause of death.
of
psychopathology recognized
Eskimos differed from what
is
usual in
specific pattern of piblokto in the eastern arctic;
know whether
the St.
term of reference for
it.
the
Western psychiatry. This
followed very closely what has been reported not
by
as the culture-
however,
we do
Lawrence language includes a specific Only one case was reported in this
Approaches
y6 study, and
it
was described
shaman but
she was partly
way once
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry as follows:
later
in a while, out of
"I thought at first that
on I found out that she does that mind a little bit, I think." The
were sudden, brief in duration, and not manner of shamanism. During these periods of
episodes
patient's
stylized in the
disturbed consciousness she shouted and grimaced.
mindness" lasted for only IO.
The
"out-of-
few minutes.
a
THE CONCEPT OF IMPAIRMENT
The
foregoing makes clear that beside these specific concepts
of patterns of disorder, the Eskimos have the conceptual equip-
ment
for
making judgments about impairment of functioning
produced by psychiatric symptoms and cases
were partly explained
disorders.
Many
of the
with normal
in terms of interference
functioning: not hunting, not trapping, not taking care of the house, having to give
up
a job or leadership role.
Conclusion This chapter has centered on views of psychopathology
Eskimo
found
in an
to the
model of
was used
at the
death
way
as a
village. In
order to summarize,
let
beginning of
this
us return
This model
a hypothetical culture in Melanesia.
report to explore ideas about
of illustrating some of the facets involved in cross-
cultural comparisons of concepts.
In the Melanesian group,
tween
their orientations
were marked
we
noted
first
the differences be-
and those of the West. The differences
in that they
bury not only dead people but
also
near-to-dead people, that they do not have separate words for
dead people and buriable people believe death effigies.
The
is
who
are not dead,
caused by the magic of casting
St.
Lawrence Eskimos
exhibit
and that they
spells or
many
kinds of differences regarding psychiatric disorders.
We
be struck by the differences in cultural practices were a child
wearing
burning
of the same
we
would to see
a fox-trapping net as a cure for "fits"; or
Eskimo sorcerer picking on
a
an
mentally defective person to run
Native Conceptions of Psychiatric Disorder his
a psychotic patient being treated
ominous errands; or
shaman who, patient.
97
in seance, looks
and
much
acts
by
a
"crazier" than his
a study of the Eskimo words on the topic of
Then
phenomena would disclose the lack of an Eskimo counterpart for some of the general terms, such as mental deficiency and senility, which we consider eminently useful; it psychiatric
would show
that
many
of their designations for psychiatric syn-
dromes are descriptive where ours are diagnostic, and that while some of the patterns of thought, feeling, and behavior which we consider to be psychiatric illnesses they also call "illness," others are looked
upon simply
as
"oddness," "badness," or "unhappi-
ness." In the realm of etiological explanation, differences are also
remarkable. Asking Eskimos what they think causes various kinds of psychiatric patterns,
we would
hear of a patient's father
breaking the law of the hunt in wounding but not killing a fox,
enemy
of an
and of
another patient's soul having been captured by
still
marauding
using witchcraft to put a hex on another patient,
spirits
from
whom
the
Eskimo shaman would seek
the soul's release.
A us,
longer acquaintance with the
however, to discover that
St.
Lawrence people permits
conjunction with these basic
in
differences there are equally basic similarities. In the Melanesian
example, further exploration of views about death brought us to
word and the concept. Melanesians have only one word for people they
the value of distinguishing between the
Although the bury
—dead or
fuddlement level
by
alive
as to
—
who
terminology
this
is
dead and
who
is
not a measure of be-
is alive.
At
they distinguish the physical properties of
virtually the
same
criteria
study
we found much
atric
vocabulary varied in
the conceptual
life
from death
used in the West. In the Eskimo
the same situation. Although their psychi-
many
respects,
our subjects recog-
nized disabilities that correspond to the whole range of major
types of disorder identified in psychiatry. labeled
categories
were
different
elaborations, they described the
in
Even though
their
terms of omissions and
symptoms we commonly
associ-
Approaches
p8 ate
with
senility,
we
behaviors control
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
neurotic reactions, and so on. Since some of the
think of as psychiatric
—figure
—
seizures
and
spiritualistic
seances as well as in psycho-
in shamanistic
pathology and to some extent the same words were used to describe both types of phenomena,
it
was of
interest to learn that
conceptual distinctions are clearly made. In other words, there
was no
difficulty in differentiating
between genuine pathology
and socially useful behaviors that bear some resemblance to psychopathology. At share with us
many
criteria
for telling pathology
share
common
this level
we
can say that the Eskimos
regarding impairment in functioning
from nonpathology,
just as the
Melanesians
physical indicators of the difference between
life
and death.
The Melanesian
illustration
ences and similarities
gave further insight into differ-
by demonstrating
that magical theories
about the causes of death coexist with rational theories. These rational theories are similar to ours in several
ways
—for
instance,
in the awareness that people die of old age or as a result of
physical
illness,
accidents,
and violence.
An
interplay between
magical and natural explanations was also characteristic of the psychiatric orientation of Eskimos in this study. Thus, along
with the ideas about soul
loss,
taboo-breaking, and witchcraft, the
Eskimos pointed to further determining influences such ity,
brain
damage through accidents or
fortunes. Their observations of
to
illness,
as
hered-
and personal mis-
human behavior seem
to have led
concepts about body-mind, nature-nurture, disorder-well-
being that in the gross sense are rather comparable to ours.
From disorder
this exercise in
we
studying native concepts of psychiatric
have concluded that there are noteworthy
differ-
ences between the stream of thought of the St. Lawrence
Eskimos and that of psychiatry. There parallels
which strongly suggest
can reasonably be made.
are,
however, underlying
that cross-cultural comparisons
Native Conceptions of Psychiatric Disorder
99
Notes 1.
L. Levy-Bruhl, Les Fonctions mentales dans les societes inferieures
(Paris: Librairie Felix Alcan, 1922); F. Boas,
The Mind
of Primitive
Man,
York: Macmillan, 1938); and P. Radin, Primitive Man As Philosopher, rev. ed. (New York: Dover, 1957). 2. W. H. R. Rivers, "The Primitive Conception of Death," in Psychology and Ethnology (New York: Harcourt, Brace & World, 1926), pp.
rev. ed.
(New
36-50. 3. Webster's New Collegiate Dictionary, 2nd ed. (Springfield, Mass.: G. & C. Merriam Co., 1953), p. 171. 4. For example, "A Bibliography on American Indian Medicine and Health," compiled by William C. Sturtevant for the Smithsonian Institute, Bureau of American Ethnology (mimeographed) contains approx-
imately 400 references. 5.
H. Ackerknecht, "Psychopathology, Primitive Medicine and
E.
Primitive Culture," Bulletin of the History of Medicine, vol.
14,
no.
1
(1943), pp. 30-67. 6. The most comprehensive study of primitive psychiatry to date is: G. Devereux, Mohave Ethnopsy chiatry and Suicide: The Psychiatric Knowledge and the Psychic Disturbances of an Indian Tribe, Bulletin 175, Smithsonian Institute, Bureau of American Ethnology (Washington, D.C.: United States Government Printing Office, 1961). A useful bibliography on this topic is: M. I. Teicher, "Comparative Psychiatry: Some Refer-
Ethnopsy chiatry," Revue Internationale d? Ethnopsy chologie Pathologique, vol. 1, nos. 1 and 2. " 'Arctic Hysteria' and 'Latah' in Mongolia," Transac7. D. F. Aberle, tions of the Neiv York Academy of Sciences, vol. 14, no. 7 (May 1952), " 'Latah' and 'Amok,' " The British pp. 291-297; see also J. J. Abraham, Medical Journal, February 24, 191 2, pp. 438-439; F. H. G. van Loon, "Amok and Latah," The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, vol. 21 (Jan.-Mar. 1927), pp. 434-444; P. M. Van Wulfften Palthe, "Psychiatry and Neurology in the Tropics," in A Clinical Textbook of Tropical Medicine, C. D. de Langen and A. Lichtenstein, eds. (Amsterdam: G. Kolff, 1936); and P. M. Yap, "The Latah Reaction: Its Psychodynamics and Nosological Position," The Journal of Mental Science, vol. ences
in
Normale
et
98 (Oct. 1952), pp. 515-564. 8. K. Spencer, Mythology and Values:
way Myths
(Philadelphia:
H. Ackerknecht,
An
Analysis of
American Folklore Society,
Navaho Chant1957), P-
3-
K. Rasmussen, "Intellectual Culture of the Igulik Eskimos," Reports of the Fifth Thule Expedition, Vol. VII, no. 1 (Copenhagen, 1920). 10. W. Bogoras, "Chukchee Mythology," in Memoirs of the American 9.
E.
op.
cit.;
wo
Approaches
Museum
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
of Natural History, Vol. XII, Pt.
[Publications of the Jesup
I
(New
York: G. E. Stechert, 1910)]. 11. J. M. Murphy, "Psychotherapeutic Aspects of Shamanism on St. Lawrence Island, Alaska," in Magic, Faith, and Healing: Studies in Primitive Psychiatry Today, Ari Kiev, ed. (New York: The Free Press of
North
Pacific Expedition, Vol. VIII
Glencoe, 1964), pp. 53-83. 12. G. Devereux, "Normal and Abnormal: The Key Problem of Psychiatric Anthropology," in Some Uses of Anthropology: Theoretical and Applied (Washington, D.C.: The Anthropological Society of Washington, 1956); see also A. L. Kroeber, "Psychosis or Social Sanction," in The Nature of Culture (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1952), pp.
310-319. 13.
E.
14. F.
H. Ackerknecht,
fornia Publications in no.
2
15.
op.
cit.
E. Clements, "Primitive Concepts of Disease," University of Cali-
American Archeology and Ethnology,
C/932), pp. 185-252. A. I. Hallowell, "Primitive Concepts of Disease,"
vol.
32,
American Anthro-
pologist, vol. 37, no. 2 (1935), pp. 365-368. 16. S. L. Rogers, "Primitive Theories of Disease," Ciba Symposia, vol. 4, 1 (1942), pp. 1190-1201; and "Disease Concepts in North America," American Anthropologist, vol. 46, no. 4 (1944), pp. 559-564. 17. J. W. M. Whiting, and I. L. Child, Child Training and Personality: A Cross-Ctdtural Study (New Haven: Yale University Press, 1953), pp.
no.
122-123. 18. M. I. Teicher, "Three Cases of Psychosis Among the Eskimos," Journal of Mental Science, vol. 100 (1954), pp. 527-535. 19. E. H. Ackerknecht, "Medicine and Disease Among Eskimos," Ciba Symposia, July-August 1948, pp. 916-921; see also A. A. Brill, "Piblokto
or Hysteria
Among
Peary's Eskimos,"
The
Journal of Nervous and
Disease, vol. 40 (August 1 9 1 3 ), pp. 514-520. 20. Z. Gussow, "'Pibloktoq (Hysteria) Among Polar Eskimos:
Men-
tal
An Eth-
nopsychiatric Study," in Psychoanalysis and the Social Sciences, sterberger and Axelrad, eds., Vol. sities Press,
21.
M. A.
VI (New York:
Muen-
International Univer-
i960).
Czaplicka, Aboriginal Siberia:
ogy (Oxford: Clarendon
A
Study
in Social
W.
Jochelson,
Anthropol-
"The KorAmerican Museum of Natural History, Vol. X, Pt. II [Publications of the Jesup North Pacific Expedition, Vol. VI (New York: G. E. Stechert, 1908)]; and S. Novakovsky, "Arctic or Siberian Hysteria as a Reflex of the Geographic Environment," Ecology, vol.
yak," in
5
Memoirs
Press, 1914); see also
of the
(April 1924), pp. 1 13-127. 22. R. Redfield, Peasant Society and Culture:
An Anthropological Approach to Civilization (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1956); see pp. 70-71 for a description of the mixing of disease concepts in Latin
Native Conceptions of Psychiatric Disorder
101
American villages where contact with the Spanish brought knowledge of the humoral pathology of Hippocrates and Galen. See also R. Redfield and M. P. Redfield, "Disease and Its Treatment in Dzitas, Yucatan," Contributions to American Anthropology and History, no. 32 (June 1940).
an explanation of the concepts and methods employed by Murphy in the key-informant interviewing: One must take a number of concepts into account when interviewing about psychiatric disorder. An inventory of these ideas includes such terms as: "normal-abnormal," "dominant-deviant," "functional-malfunc23.
The
following
is
tional," "natural-unnatural,"
The inventory
itself is far
ports to catalogue
is
which concept or
full
concepts
is
like the
phenomena
pur-
It is
which end of the continuum of health
offers the best starting point.
it
not immediately obvious most appropriate for this kind of
of interconnections.
set of
exploration, nor even
and "well-sick." precise, and
from
One way
to illness
of looking at the matter suggests
markedly different cultures the initial step should be to construct \ paradigms of normality and from these to characterize abnormality. ) Another way suggests that the firmest step is to adhere as closely as possible to our own concepts of disorder and to seek counterparts in native
that in
belief.
Decision in this study to work mainly through the apertures of deviance and pathology rather than trying to construct ideas of normality involved a number of considerations. One was that it is doubtful if a person of any cultural background can conceptualize and verbalize normality as well as deviance and pathology. To ask an Eskimo the dimensions of wellness and normalcy is comparable to asking the average man in our society what human nature is. If the question is understood at all, the answer is so axiomatic in the informant's system of thought and feeling that he has difficulty communicating it and is reduced to describing it as "self-evident." "Self-evidence" under such circumstances has a way of being extremely recondite. The concept of deviance has limitations, too. A deviant mode of thinking, feeling, and behaving is as likely to fall on the superior end of the scale and indicate optimal functioning as it is to indicate malfunctioning. It is usually easier, however, to spot evidences of incapacity and handicap than of exceptional ability. Historically many geniuses have gone unrecognized in their time, but few idiots have, even though the levels and shades of incapacity are extremely difficult to judge without testing and
measurement.
Although some understanding of the value and limitations of each of is essential to the researcher, none is the kind of idea that
these concepts
can readily be explained to a native informant. In this study, psychiatric data were obtained in response to the question: "What sickness has
Approaches
102
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
such and such a person had?" or sometimes even more simply: "Tell me about such and such a person." This orientation appeared to help the key informant on two accounts: ( i ) questions about one person specifically are easier to answer than questions about people in general; and (2) questions about sickness require less generalization than questions about health and normality. Further, the concept of "illness" can be communicated fairly easily, and it can be anticipated that all people have a near equivalent in their
own
language.
After a conceptual approach has been chosen to guide the interviewing, a number of points about key informant selection and qualification remain to be spelled out in order to make clear both the limitations and the extent of data that can be gathered through this means.
One
factor to bear in
common knowledge
is
mind concerns whether
special
knowledge or
sought. For the kind of special knowledge that
of interest in psychiatric epidemiology, an experienced shaman
seem an obvious choice. His knowledge of symptoms and of a large portion of the population during illness
his
is
would
treatment
would have given
a fund of directly relevant information. None of the Sivokak shamans, however, was sufficiently bilingual to permit extensive and systematic interviewing in English. Since we were interested in generally held
him
views of disorder, it seemed appropriate to seek someone convey common knowledge.
who
could
Not all cultures lend themselves to the use of common knowledge as an entre. In some cultures there are areas of knowledge to which only special novitiates have access, and there are cultural segregations that limit the expanse of any one individual's information. In India, for exlife into man's world and woman's world informant most unlikely as a candidate for an epidemiological study except of women. In Sivokak there are no barriers to a free flow of knowledge about people, and a woman key informant was chosen. The decision to ask her assistance rested on a number of factors such as language skill, willingness, and motivation demonstrated in earlier work. Also intimate health information was more easily conveyed in this
ample, the purdah division of
would make
a female
culture between
The
two women,
investigator and informant.
interviews were conducted in twenty-four sessions over the five-
month period of this aspect of the year's study. They were composed of the informant's comments on a limited number of questions, systematically presented, regarding each individual in the 1940-1954 census. The project was explained as an attempt to find out different kinds of things that
had happened to people in the village. The census was described as the means by which we would organize our work. The first question for each person was: "Can you tell me about X.? " followed by: "Did he or she marry?" "Who are their children?" "Has X been sick?" "What jobs
Native Conceptions of Psychiatric Disorder
103
does he or she do?" "Has he or she been to the mainland?" "How far did he or she go to school?" The questions were chosen in the hope of getting as full a record as possible of general life experience. It became evident early in the procedure, however, that the questions were not equally appropriate for keyinformant interviewing. I soon realized that for a culture where all men are hunters and all women are homemakers, the question about jobs was not giving any new information except about those people who held
governmental positions, and those
I
knew
already.
Much
the same applied
regard to schooling, I found that the informant could give accurate data about whether a person had or had not finished the eight-grade school on the island and about those few who had gone be-
to migration.
With
yond. But to ask for grade levels was quite unrealistic. Throughout the series I tried to minimize situations that required the self-deprecation of too many responses such as: "I don't know" or "I didn't quite remember that."
informant had sufficiently which I was interested that she usually gave all the information she knew on the basis of my asking simply: "Can you tell me about X now?" It was clear that the questions regarding illness fell on fertile soil, as did also those about marriages and children. For these I consistently pressed to make sure that I had recorded everything she knew, and that I had given her time to remember. For the more expendable questions I was satisfied with what she gave on her After the
first
two or three
sessions the
internalized the aspects of experience in
own accord. Key informants
have been employed in collecting
many
different kinds
of data such as kinship terms, diet habits, witchcraft, and religion. But to talk of practices
The
is
quite a different matter
mind
question inevitably comes to
how much
as to
from
talking about people.
how much
information about
The opportunity be an open sesame for the expression of personal animosities. On the other hand, if there is no evidence of the informant's personal likes and dislikes, the question of hypocrisy arises. Especially in a psychiatric study this aspect of key-informant interviewing has to be judged in the light of cultural feelings of stigma regarding mental disorder. Can an accusation of mental symptoms stem
people
is
gossip and
is
report of actual events.
to talk about one's acquaintances
may
from malicious
intent against a disliked person? Is mental disorder conceived as something to be ashamed of and therefore repressed from information-giving?
The amount
of data
obtained from the key informant and other
native informants suggests that there
was little reluctance to communiThere was certainly less hesitancy to talk than about some of the old customs and beliefs.
cate psychiatric information.
about mental
illness
Approaches
104
With
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
no stigma appeared to be attached homosexuality was concerned, if it did not take the form of transvestitism, it is as likely that native informants lacked knowledge of it as that they withheld information. It would be hushed up in the in-
to
it.
the exception of homosexuality
Where
group
as
much
as to outsiders.
Psychoses and mental deficiency did not appear to be something to hide. Mothers were known to be relatively free with their comments that
one or another child had difficulty learning. "Sickness of mind" that reached psychotic dimension was viewed with the same sympathy as were other sicknesses. Likewise psychoneurotic and psychophysiologic symptoms did not seem to be stigmatized by the Eskimos, and it was more a matter of extent of knowledge than of prejudice. Quite aside from cultural stigma, there are questions about reliability that relate to the internal standards by which a key informant herself judges the accuracy of information. How does she sort items into events versus rumors, facts versus fictions? This is by no means an insignificant problem when the informant has grown up relying on magic to explain
many
things.
between magical "truth" and "truth" and scientific sense, it seemed to me that the informant's world was starkly dichotomized between the Eskimo truths of her childhood and the new truths of the white world which she accepted in adulthood. The new criteria did not invalidate past history; they were means of judging current affairs, but they simply did not apply to verification of her past experiences and those of her parents' generation. To her it was absolutely true that people used to die because evil prayers were said against them. "It is a good thing we put away all those old stuffs," she said. Back in those times, people did get sick because they broke the law of the hunt and stole someone else's whale; shamans did see things and hear things of that other reality. Even in terms of the old beliefs, however, there seemed to be standards of accuracv what might be called measures of the reliability of magic! Once, in the presence of her sister-in-law, who was secular to both the old and new faiths, the informant recounted a story concerning an entrancing half-woman, half-seal creature who had been seen on an ice floe. Her sister-in-law said that the story was nonsense. The informant insisted: "It's true; two people saw her!" Thus, like most Eskimos at mid-century, she believed both Eskimo and "white" at the same time. I believe that she gave as accurate data as possible from whichever of her two premises was foremost in her mind at the time, and I conceived of it as my job as an anthropologist to decide which premise was operative at any given moment. For example, it was clear that in both of her intellectual worlds she was aware of the difference between rumor and opinion on one hand and events and history on the other. She In terms of the broad division
in the historical
—
Native Conceptions of Psychiatric Disorder sensed that
I
105
valued her discriminations about the criteria and validity it was possible to divide her
of judgments, and at the end of the study
information into the categories of what she had actually seen herself; what she had heard from the person in question; what she considered her
own
opinion ("That's just what
I
what she conwhat she remembered
thinks myself");
sidered rumors, enlightening and unenlightening;
clearly; what she did not remember; and what she did not recall but thought she could find out, and did find out, from other Eskimos. Motivation is a complex aspect of this kind of interviewing both too much and too little being disadvantageous. This informant appeared to be highly motivated, but I do not believe she therefore gave me false impressions based on a preconceived notion of what kind of information would please me. Since our work was never described as a psychiatric
—
study, she never
knew
fully of this special interest.
In addition to assessing her
own
standards of accuracy
it
was
also
her data about general health by comparing what she said with the health records. There were only 107 health records available for the population of 495, but in 85 per cent
possible to
measure the
reliability of
of the overlay cases she correctly designated the
was described
in the
symptom system that mean that the
record. This obviously does not
nomenclature was the same. It was possible, however, to see that her dethough crude, accurately portrayed a disability of the pul-
scriptions,
monary system,
the cardiovascular system, or the musculoskeletal system.
For example: Health record of an old man ivho died in 1942: "1940: Impetigo contageosa. Lesions about healed. Edema of feet, at times has bloody urine. Urine neg. for protein. Ointment for lesions. 1941: Complaint gastric, vomits dark fluid, pain sometimes, used to drink much whiskey. 1942: Man has jaundice, eyes and entire skin very yellow. Is losing weight rapidly. Has dyspnea, ascites, icterus. Bloody stool and sputum. Died in 1942. Informant's report of his symptoms:
"During his boyhood when he was a young man he used to be a big drunkard. But he quit when he became older. When he getting older he became unhealthy. Sweating most of the time, always sweating, change his clothing about twice a day, especially in the evening he sweating too much and he doesn't feeling good with his belly too. Later on during his sickness his skin became yellowish. I don't know why it became like that, green or yellow pardy, like that all over his skin. And then he died from that, and he had T.B. and he has been vomiting some big chunks of blood, something like that, awful smells too. T.B. that is what he vomits I think. He died in 1944, oh no, 1943. He has been sick and don't feeling good for a long time ago with his urine too. Sometimes he couldn't pass urine so easily I think. It was long before he died that his skin became yellowish too." [Unpublished field notes: J.M.M., 26 June 1955, p. 2.] In addition to asking about reliability,
much information one person can
it
is
also
important to ask
how
give concerning 494 other individuals.
Approaches
io6
To some
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
extent this depends on the stability of the population, the
amount
of face-to-face contact between them, and the proximity of living arrangements. Certainly such a method would not be appropriate for a study of urban areas where affiliations are based on
class,
occupation, or associ-
and where population mobility makes many acquaintanceships temporary and superficial. Where Sivokak village is concerned, the first indication of the usefulness of this technique was the fact that the 494 questions drew no blanks. There was considerable variation in how much the informant knew about each person, but she had something to say about each name. One of the variations regarding her knowledge of the village population concerned the age of its members. She had less to sav about children than about adults. The age group of five years and under received scanty treatment. This made sense, however, for the length of contact with children ations that cross-cut residence
They have
is less.
lived so
of their lives that there
little
is
not
much
to
note unless they are severely abnormal, do not respond to the shaping
As
processes of culture, or do not survive.
far as the epidemiological
concerned there was no reason to doubt her coverage. She described childhood symptoms that we would call tantrums, mental deficiency, epilepsy, and behavior problems. For other children her descriptions tended to be: "He goes to school. He is beginning to help his father. He is a healthy." Apparently there were no variations relevant to sex. She knew things of psychiatric interest regarding both men and women. Of the 68 cases she contributed, 38 were women and 30 men. Thus her distribution on the basis of sex was similar to that in the overall total from all sources which includes 60 women and 53 men. 113 cases There were differences in the amount of information (and therefore evidence of
a psychiatric
nature
is
—
the likelihood of disclosing the less obvious psychiatric related to kinship ties and proximity.
The most
symptoms)
that
detailed account of
medical and psychiatric history was of herself, then of her immediate family, and from there the degree of detail went out in waves of diminishing description to the extended family, tribe, and village levels. Detail
was
also greater for her
next-door neighbors and, since she lives in the fanned out to less particular information regarding people who live at the peripheries. Nevertheless her coverage of the population in terms of psychiatric descriptions was sufficiently ample to have made this type of key-informant interviewing useful to a study center of the village,
it
of native concepts. 24.
Grateful acknowledgment
by our 25. J. j^T
is
collaborators, Charles C.
given for the use of field notes gathered
Hughes and Dorothea
M. Murphy (formerly Hughes),
Psychopathology
in an
Eskimo Village" (Ph.D.
University, i960), pp. 224-231.
C. Leighton.
"x\n Epidemiological Study of Dissertation,
Cornell
Native Conceptions of Psychiatric Disorder 26. C.
C. Frake,
"The Diagnosis of Disease Among
!0J
the Subanun of Min-
danao," America?! Anthropologist, vol. 63, no. 1 (Feb. 1961), pp. 11 3-1 32. 27. A. H. Leighton, T. A. Lambo, C. C. Hughes, D. C. Leighton, J. M.
Murphy, and D. B. Macklin, Psychiatric Disorder a?nong the Yoruba: A Report from the Cornell-Aro Mental Health Research Project in the Western Region, Nigeria (Ithaca, N.Y.: Cornell University Press, 1963). 28. J. M. Cooper, "The Cree Witiko Psychosis," Primitive Man, vol. 6 (Jan.
1933), pp. 20-24. See also R. Landes,
Ojibwa,"
The
"The Abnormal Among
the
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, vol. 33 (Jan.
1938), pp. 14-33. 29.
A. H. Leighton, and C. C. Hughes, "Notes on Eskimo Patterns of
Suicide," Southwestern Journal of Anthropology, vol. 11, no. 4 (1955), pp. 327-338. 30.
no.
2
W.
B.
Cannon, "Voodoo Death," American Anthropologist,
(1942), pp.
1
69— 181.
vol. 44,
IV The Use of Psychophysiological :
Symptoms
as Indicators of Disorder
among Eskimos By Jane M. Murphy and Charles
C.
Hughes
EDITORIAL NOTES Although
interest in "culture-personality" studies declined in
the postwar era, serious attention was directed during this period to epidemiological research regarding the distribution of psychiatric
symptoms
in nonhospitalized populations. It
that one source of guidance for developing
of previous as these
work
was obvious
methods was
a
study
with psychological screening tests, particularly
had emerged during the war.
Study got under way
When
the Stirling
in the fifties, the late Allister
County
Macmillan
undertook to examine these screening instruments and to
select
would be appropriate for among adults in rural communities. The approach found to be most closely allied to the needs of the Stirling Study was that taken by the armed forces in a or construct and standardize a test that
use
program of screening for neurotic and related symptoms. Nevertheless the questionnaire had to be remodeled to suit the different situation in
which
it
would be 108
used. Unlike the
young
Psy chophy siological Indicators among Eskimos males tested for military service, a rural community "captive audience."
It
/op
is
not
a
comprises, moreover, a broader age range
men and women.
and includes both
With advance knowledge of the community and in consultation with physicians who knew the area, Macmillan devised a questionnaire which he named the Health Opinion Survey (hereafter called the HOS). For purposes of validation and was tried out in communities County but at a distance from it. 1 In
standardization, the questionnaire similar to those of Stirling its
original pretesting form, the instrument consisted of seventy-
five items dealing
questions
nature
such
centering on
—that
as
broadly with health but with a majority of
is,
complaints of a psychophysiological
queries mainly about physiological sensations
numbness, fatigue, or weak
feelings. Items
were taken
from the Army's Neuropsychiatric Screening Adjunct with additional questions
from other instruments such
as the
Minne-
sota Multiphasic Personality Inventory and from suggestions
made by
local physicians.
private, using mobile
The
questionnaire
trailers fitted
out
was administered
as offices,
in
and the ques-
were asked by an interviewer. The whole procedure required about twenty minutes, and the questions as well as the presentation of the project were given with as much care as
tions
possible to avoid causing anger, suspicion, or embarrassment.
During the stages of standardizing this technique and reducing its size to a manageable number of items for surveying larger populations,
two measures
to the same questionnaire
of validity were instituted. Responses
were obtained from
both in-patient and out-patient, neurotic.
By means
achieve a smaller
of
list
statistical
who had procedures
local patients,
been diagnosed it
was
as
possible to
of items (twenty questions) and a con-
comitant scoring system which reached an adequate level of discrimination between the group of
group composed
as fully as possible
of
known "ill" and another people who were presum-
ably "well." Another measure as to whether a given pattern of responses indicated the presence or absence of psychiatric dis-
i
Approaches
io
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
order was an independent clinical appraisal by a psychiatrist of a
subsample of the pretested group.
This work
as
well as the efforts of others to develop screening
instruments has produced series of survey questions for which
high frequency of positive responses
is
what
correlated with
psychiatrists in a clinical context discover to be psychiatric dis-
order, especially psychoneurosis.
The
fact that the survey ques-
tions tend to
concern psychophysiologic sensations rather than,
for example,
dynamic or interpersonal questions
empirical demonstration. Correlation ent
matter from
a
clinical
is,
is
a
matter of
however, quite of
definition
a differ-
psychoneurosis.
It
should be emphasized that frequent feelings of weakness, upset
stomach, and palpitations do not themselves necessarily constitute psychiatric illness.
Midtown Study,
In the
a rather similar
ployed in developing a screening
test.
2
Called the
Item Screening Score (hereafter referred to Item Score),
this list of questions also
psychophysiologic symptoms, and there
is
a
is
Mexico, which
Midtown
use of the
is
the
last
A
chapter of this volume,
the topic of the present chapter.
when not tells little
buttressed
West-
be noted
as the
or nothing about the type of psychiatric disorder,
duration, or the kind of impairment. ently, register rather
On
The
mental retardation, and sociopathic be-
the other hand, the question
of psychoneurotic
—that
is,
its
questions also, appar-
poorly some of the more severe reactions
as schizophrenia,
indicators
may
HOS. The score on the test by information drawn from other sources
regarding instruments such
ances
made
screening instrument, while the Stirling
the basis of trying to assess populations of people in
havior.
that
study of social
ern society, several drawbacks and advantages
such
is
considerable overlap in the twenty questions selected for
status in
is
Twenty-two Twenty-two
concerned mainly with
and the twenty-two for Midtown.
On
as the
noteworthy aspect
Stirling
HOS
procedure was em-
lists
serve as very
good
and psychophysiologic disturb-
those types of disorder
which
are prevalent in
Psychophysiological Indicators
communities
The
at large.
m
among Eskimos
brevity and rapid scoring
make
possi-
ble the screening of large samples, and the content of the questions
is
not likely to offend or incline people into giving evasive
or misleading answers.
For cross-cultural
studies instruments such as the
Twenty-two Item Score have
HOS
or the
the further asset that the questions,
being mostly concerned with fairly specific sensations, are some-
what
resistant to distortion
when
translated
from one language
to another. Before assuming, however, that such a screening
device can be employed in cross-cultural epidemiology, several questions of feasibility and validity need to be explored in a
circumscribed cultures
trial situation.
other
than
It
could be, for example, that in
urban and rural North America these
physiological indicators are not highly correlated with anxiety, depression, or other psychoneurotic patterns. In such a case, the
questionnaire
The
would
fail as a
present chapter
is
screening instrument.
a report of an exploratory study
made
954- 1 95 5 by two anthropologists, Murphy and Hughes, employing the HOS with a sample of Eskimos from the larger
in
1
study of psychiatric epidemiology described in the
Their Eskimo work followed the
initial
Study during which they collaborated
The
goal of the
to scrutinize the test for
trial its
HOS
chapter.
period of the Stirling in using the
gather data about mental health and mental
Canada.
last
illness
HOS
to
in rural
survey among Eskimos was
usefulness as a
method of surveying
psychoneurotic symptoms in a non-Western population. 3
THIS
chapter reports a
trial
experiment in using and analyzing
the results of a structured questionnaire for gathering data about
mental health in a non-Western culture. For ple of
Eskimos from Sivokak
this
purpose, a sam-
village on St. Lawrence Island in were asked to respond to the Health Opinion Survey questionnaire. A copy of the questionnaire is incorpo-
the Bering Sea
Approaches
ii2
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
some notes concerning its adminEach question is then reviewed as to its suitability for use in the Eskimo culture. Alternative or additional questions are recommended where knowledge of the culture indicates the possibility of some misunderstanding of the concepts involved, and a quantitative comparison of the Eskimo responses with responses from sample members in the Stirling County Study is rated in the text, together with istration.
presented. In the last section of the chapter, attention
methods for scoring the
HOS
results
so
that
is
given to
the
"illness-
by each Eskimo's responses can be compared with other evidence on the mental health status of the wellness" rating provided
sample members.
The
corollary information used in these final
comparisons consists of key-informant materials and observational data
which have been evaluated by two
psychiatrists.
Questionnaire and Preparatory Steps for Its Administration
The Health Opinion Survey questionnaire used in this study is reproduced in Table IV- 1. The arrangement of items follows Table IV-i. Eskimo Health Opinion Survey questionnaire
Name i.
Sex
Age
*
Highest School Grade Completed
How
has your health been, on the whole? good 2. medium 3. poor Do you have any particular physical or health trouble at present? 1. no 2. yes What sorts of serious illnesses have you had? Illness Age How sick were you? Have you ever had one of the following? (Check any already mentioned in previous question, and omit from mention in this i.
*
2.
3.
4.
question. Illness
Age(s) at
in years
time(s) of
occurrence (s)
Eye
trouble
Ear trouble Sinus trouble
Throat trouble
How sick were you?
Remarks
Psychophysiological Indicators
among Eskimos
113
Bronchitis
Pneumonia Pleurisy
T.B. Boils
&
abscesses
Low blood
(anemia)
Heart trouble High blood pressure Low blood pressure Stomach trouble Diabetes
Bowel trouble Piles
Kidney trouble Bodily injury
Operations
(list)
Rupture (hernia)
Rheumatism Arthritis Sciatica
Neuralgia
Tooth trouble For women: female trouble Other (specify)
5.
Have you 1.
ever had to go easy on your
2.
What
7.
Have you ever had 1.
9.
2.
often
2.
often
*i4.
this last?
you?
How
long did
it last?
never
3.
2.
sometimes felt that
sometimes
3.
feet sweating so that they
never
you were going
to have a nervous break-
once or twice 4. never by your heart beating hard? 1. often 2. sometimes 3. never Do you tend to feel tired in the mornings? 1. often 2. sometimes 3. never Do you have any trouble in getting to sleep and staying asleep? 1. often 2. sometimes 3. never 1.
*i3.
long did
your work because of poor health?
ever tremble enough to bother you?
sometimes
*n. Have you ever down? *i2.
how
to change
Are you ever troubled by your hands or feel damp and clammy? 1.
of poor health?
yes
What age were Do your hands 1.
*io.
age were you? For
no
work because
yes
6.
8. *
no
often
Have you
2.
3.
ever been bothered
Approaches
//^
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
Table IV-i. Eskimo Health Opinion Survey questionnaire (cont.)
How
you bothered by having an upset stomach?
the time
all
2.
prettv often
3.
not very
much
never
4.
*i6.
often are
nearly
i.
Are you ever bothered by nightmares? (dreams which frighten or upset you?
)
many
*2
2.
times 2. a few times 3. never your arms or legs go to sleep rather easily? 1. often 2. sometimes 3. never Have you ever been troubled by "cold sweats"? 1. often 2. a few times 3. never Do you feel that you are bothered by all sorts (different kinds) of ailments in different parts of your body? 1. often 2. sometimes 3. never Do you smoke? 1. a lot 2. some 3. not at all Are you ever troubled by sick headaches? 1. often 2. sometimes 3. never Do you ever have loss of appetite?
*2
3.
Do you
1.
Do
*i8.
*io.
*2o.
*2i.
often
1.
often
1.
*24.
sometimes
3.
2.
sometimes
3.
Does your food ever seem often
1.
*25.
2.
Do you
never
ever have a bad taste in your mouth?
2.
feel
sometimes
it is
never
tasteless
and hard to swallow?
never necessary to take vitamin 3.
pills
for your health?
sometimes 3. never *i6. Do you depend on patent medicines? 2. sometimes 1. often 3. never often
1.
*2 7.
Do you
2.
feel that
you
are
more
apt to catch contagious diseases than
most people? 1.
yes
2.
no
3.
undecided
How
would you say your health was this past year? 2. good 1. excellent 4. poor 3. fair 5. very poor 29. Do you feel in good spirits? 2. sometimes 1. most of the time 3. very few times Now I'd like to ask some questions about the health of your 28.
close
relatives: 30.
How 1.
31.
has the health of your mother and father been, on the whole?
excellent
2.
medium
3.
How
about your brothers and the whole? 1.
excellent
2.
medium
3.
poor sisters,
how
has their health been, on
poor
A portion of the material has been used in earlier studies and is reproduced here by permission of the author's wife, Mrs. Allister M. Mac*
Psychophysiological Indicators
among Eskimos
115
County Study. The twenty crucial questions are indicated by an asterisk. A few additional questions were asked in both the Stirling and Eskimo studies to provide a rough estimate of the degree to which illness interfered with routine activities and to obtain information on family
the format
employed
health history. takes the
A
form of
a
in the Stirling
further inquiry, not part of the
medical checklist.
HOS
itself,
4
HOS was conducted in the last month of a year's visit on Lawrence Island. By that time most of the study of social change the central purpose of the field trip had been comThe
St.
—
—
and much had been learned about Eskimo culture and about concepts and prevalence of illness. Thus one of the main
pleted,
preparatory steps for the
HOS
trial
hunches and reservations about the
was
to formulate a priori
suitability of
each individual
question in studying psychiatric disorder in this culture.
For the survey, a census of the population in Sivokak village was prepared from government records compiled by the school teacher. Resident in the village during the survey period and
were 146 adults over systematic sample of twenty names was
available for the first-hand interviewing
eighteen years of age.
drawn by
A
selecting the first appropriately aged person in the
census and every subsequent seventh thereafter. 5 Although
it
was not adjusted for equal representation of men and women, the sampling produced ten male
names and ten female names.
Five additional questionnaires were obtained from
and three men. These people had previously given data and
were included
in the
HOS
two women life
history
survey for supplementation
and the publisher from A. M. Macmillan, "The Health Opinion Survey: Technique for Estimating Prevalence of Psychoneurotic and Related Types of Disorder in Communities," Psychological Reports, vol.
millan,
3, Monograph Supplement no. 7 (1957), and by permission of the authors and publisher from C. C. Hughes, M.-A. Tremblay, R. N. Rapoport, and A. H. Leighton, People of Cove and Woodlot: Communities from the Viewpoint of Social Psychiatry [Vol. II, The Stirling County Study of Psychiatric Disorder and Sociocultural Environment (New York: Basic
Books, i960)].
n6
Approaches
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
and cross-checking. Since there is no reason to think that the Eskimos were either especially ill or especially
additional five
well, the remainder of this chapter focuses
on the twenty-five
questionnaires in order to reflect as large a
number of response
patterns as possible. Although the sample
small,
is
it
represents
population. 1 7 per cent of the adult Prior to the survey, we interviewed key informants to find
out whether the symptoms and sensations discussed in the questions were
known
HOS
Eskimos and whether they could
to the
be translated into the Eskimo language.
We
discovered that
all
the health items qualified in this minimal way. This does not
mean
that
all
items
were equally relevant
as
characteristic
expressions of psychiatric disorder in this culture. But whether
or not the questions on vitamin
pills
and patent medicines, for
example, applied to the Eskimos in terms of psychiatric malfunc-
them and could be language or conveyed by English words that were
tioning, the concepts
stated in their
commonly
were understandable
understood.
The
to
questionnaire
was administered
Eskimos or when necesEskimo language using experienced interpreters with the questions were carefully reviewed before the survey
verbally, either in English to bilingual
sary in the
whom was
instigated.
By
1955 tne Eskimos in Sivokak village were well accustomed
to the procedures of public health programs. People ally
informed of these by the
village leaders,
house to house telling them to appear examinations
of
one kind
tuberculosis, sickness,
or
school or clinic for
at the
another.
were usu-
who went from
Apprehension about
and death was so vivid and so widespread
that cooperation could usually be counted on. Also the
had previously experienced "home nurses or itinerant doctors
who
asked
visits"
many
by
Eskimos
public
questions.
health
With
this
background, there appeared to be a favorable climate for a survey such as the HOS. This was confirmed by the fact that we as
ran into
few problems of
concluded that
it
was
rapport. 6
At
the end of the survey,
feasible to administer
we
such a questionnaire
among Eskimos and
nj
among Eskimos
Psychophysiological Indicators
that the individual queries
were
sufficiently-
meaningful to warrant the further analysis described in the
fol-
lowing pages.
Twenty Questions
Analysis of
The
ideal
way
and standardize an
to design
naire for cross-cultural
work would be
HOS
question-
to start with a large
list
of questions administered in each of several different cultural
groups, so that
we
could be sure the
full
spectrum of significant
psychological reactions and psychophysiological sensations was
being tapped.
The
next step would be to select the questions that
most effectively sort the known psychiatrically these cultures
combine the
from the known
well.
ill
people in
The hope would
then be to
results of the several studies
set of universally appropriate
and determine a core
questions so that cross-cultural
comparisons could be made directly from responses to the same questions. This forecasts an extremely long
and expensive opera-
tion of pretesting, and
we know
what
in reality
we
presupposes that
in
advance
are seeking to discover, namely, the
ways of
it
identifying psychiatrically
ill
people in a foreign culture.
Thus for the purposes of gathering data that this
problem,
let
assumption in view of
(i) the
This
is
not an unreasonable
denominators of the
condition such as need for sleep, fatigue after exertion,
occasional palpitations as well as
and (2) the fact that variations
other physiological events,
in these functions are
significant to the psychiatric status of people
ent cultural origins. For the mental.
We decided to
moment,
then, our attitude
studies, bearing in
variations in psychoneurotic
reactions could be reflected responses.
For example,
probably
from widely
go ahead and ask the questions
been found useful in other specific
illuminate
common
tions have universal applicability.
human
may
us assume at the beginning that certain ques-
mind
is
differ-
experi-
that have
that culture-
and psychophysiological
by weighting and scoring of
questions
about
generalized
chondriacal preoccupations or specific bodily rhythms
the
hypo-
may
be
u8
Approaches
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
strong indicators in culture A, while sensations of fear
more important
may
be
though one would not want
in culture B, even-
to leave out questions of either kind in cross-cultural studies. It
seems worthwhile, therefore, to review the twenty ques-
what we know about Eskimos and Eskimo Eskimo response patterns with those of Stirling County residents. Although subsequent sections of this chapter build on the results of this review, the purpose here is mainly to systematize a series of recommenda-
tions in the light of
culture and through comparison of the
tions
how
on
the
HOS
might be improved
large-scale study of Eskimos. It
is
in preparing for a
not intended
as a
demonstra-
tion of the relevance or irrelevance of individual items
questionnaire. This is
is
from the
to be especially underscored because there
Midtown
reason to believe from the Stirling and
Studies that
the questions taken separately are not nearly as crucial in indicat-
ing disorder If
we
as is the totality
start
of responses.
with the question:
Eskimo say
'yes'
answer
in several factors,
lies
idiosyncratic,
"What makes
or 'no' to a given item?"
some
some of which
a particular
obvious that the are
more or less some to
related to physical considerations,
and
cultural practices
it is
beliefs,
some
tion or the interview situation, and
nomena. Because of our
to the
wording of the ques-
some
to psychological phe-
interest in cross-cultural comparison,
one aspect of the twenty-question review
"Does an Eskimo respond
in a particular
is
way
to ask ourselves,
because he under-
by the question the same thing that a resident of Stirling County understands?" The second aspect concerns whether
stands
there
is
a similar psychiatric implication in the questions
asked of Eskimos, that tiality for either
is,
when
do the questions have the same poten-
being correlated with or conveying information
we have not operated on the and feelings queried in the twenty
about emotional upsets. Although principle that the sensations
items define psychiatric disorder in either Stirling
among Eskimos, random
it is
selection.
County or
clear that the questions are not an entirely
"Does your food seem
tasteless
and hard to
Psychophysiological Indicators
up
among Eskimos
swallow?" bears a relationship to psychological disturbance in Western culture that would not be true of physical sensations such
experiencing pain
as
we want
when sunburned
or frostbitten.
to look at the questions further to see
if it is
Thus
likely that
a psychological dimension will be dominant and the influence of
cultural
and physical factors minimal.
From
this
viewpoint, the twenty questions seem to
fall
into
five categories.
Questions centering on views, practices, and patterns of
i.
physical
illness.
Present health trouble?
2.
*
27.
Susceptible to contagious disease?
25.
Vitamin
26.
Patent medicines?
pills?
Attention was immediately drawn to questions relating to physical illness because the Eskimos, like
groups, experience a
much
own
than do members of our these
Eskimos
is
many underdeveloped
greater prevalence of physical disease culture. Further, the history of
typical of that of other arctic inhabitants re-
garding the introduction of tuberculosis and periodically devastating epidemics of measles
Eskimos
as a
diseases that
flu,
giving evidence that the
group do not yet have immunity to many of the have
less drastic effects in
of a psychiatric
bility
and
our culture.
component being
related
The to
possi-
positive
responses about present health trouble or susceptibility to contagious disease might therefore be vitiated. It sion,
however, that
the St.
illness
Lawrence Eskimos
was
so
much
was our impres-
a focus of anxieties for
that these questions
might be highly
correlated with psychological disturbance though not in themselves a direct
on
outcome of such disturbance. Also, the question is appropriately worded: "Do you feel more
susceptibility
* Although the questions have been rearranged in order, the numbering system has been retained for cross-reference to pp. 1 12 to 114.
Approaches
120
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
apt to catch contagious diseases than most people?"
seemed an adequate
The
such,
it
reflector of preoccupation with health.
questions about need for vitamin
were
cines
As
and patent medi-
pills
the only ones not consistently asked.
Such
pills
and
medicines are not part of the existing culture and are not readily
Thus we asked about them only
available.
in situations
understanding was certain. In looking back on our
where
field notes
and reviewing the affirmative responses to these questions when they were asked,
The
overcome by had
also
by
schooling. Mail ordering
a great deal to bridge the gap.
was one of the
Western medicine
and most enduring attractions the white
first
world offered to "cure-alls"
we had
mainland for hospitalization, by Na-
training camps, and
done
these matters than
physical isolation of the Island had been largely
trips to the
Guard
Eskimos seemed
that the acculturated
more knowledgeable about
to be far
supposed.
tional
we found
this
physically
population,
debilitated
and "wonder drugs" had great significance
and
as objects
about which to center, however unrealistically, the hopes and fears
our
concerning the health situation of the Island. Thus, despite
initial
misgivings about the four questions involving physical
knowledge of the Eskimo
health, fuller
situation suggested that
these items are particularly suitable. 2.
Questions involving food and the gastrointestinal tract ivhere diet habits might be relevant.
A
15.
Upset stomach?
22.
Loss of appetite?
23.
Bad
24.
Food
first
as a
tasteless?
reaction to the
to think of
taken
taste?
it
HOS
in connection
whole,
it
questionnaire
Four out of the twenty items concern
some aspect of the alimentary canal Although
No
this
—stomach
upset, food, taste,
other bodily system receives so
focus
that,
seems to emphasize questions involving the
gastrointestinal tract.
and appetite.
when we began
with the Eskimo survey was
may
much
attention.
be appropriate in Northeast America,
Psychophysiological Indicators
among Eskimos
121
Lawrence Island. Our reservations, therefore, mainly concern the number of items on this theme and the correspondingly fewer items on other themes that might be more pertinent to the Eskimos. Also, however, there were
it is
not necessarily so on
diet patterns that
We
St.
might influence responses to these questions.
doubted that "upset stomach" would have much psy-
chiatric significance in the
Eskimo population.
complaints were extremely
common
poor
health, mild epidemics of flu,
the times of poor hunting
Gastrointestinal
as a reflection of generally
and
diet variations.
when food was
During
scarce people ate meat
was rotting and which in better times would have been consumed only by the dogs. They did not ordinarily eat "high meat," although as in other Eskimo groups this was sometimes a necessity. The public health nurse for the Island held the view that
that lean periods
were correlated with outbreaks of stomach
trouble and attributed this to periodic waves of food poisoning.
Although lean periods were not continuous, food was rarely In addition, diet habits were being revolutionized.
plentiful.
Many
people preferred white man's food to Eskimo food, and
some children refused altogether to eat native meat because they found the odor offensive. Although responses about taste and appetite might be
somewhat ambiguous
population undergoing such change in
in such an underfed
diet,
our skepticism about
was somewhat allayed by the fact that outside Eskimos said they recognize a link between emotional disturbance and at least one of these items loss of these questions
the
HOS
situation,
—
appetite.
Of
these four questions, then,
tions about "upset
we have
greatest reserva-
stomach" and greatest confidence in
"loss of
appetite." 3.
Questions in "which wording might influence response.
We
13.
Tired in morning?
19.
Ailments
all
over?
did not believe that the "morning tiredness" question
would apply
to the
Eskimos
— not because they lack the idea and
sensations of tiredness but because they have
different sleep
Approaches
122 habits.
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
work
People sleep during the daytime and
necessary in view of hunting
possibilities.
We
at night if
were consistently
impressed by the irregularity of sleep patterns and believed that
was increased by the nearly continuous darkness or continuous light depending on the season of the year. Since the Eskimos do understand fatigue, we would recommend that questions about this sensation be recast in more understandable terms such as, "Do you tend to feel tired a lot?" or "Are you tired even
it
when you wake up?" The question on ailments is bothered by all sorts (different parts of
your body?" This
is
stated:
"Do you
feel
you
are
kinds) of ailments in different
the most abstract of
all
the items
and requires the greatest generalization from the respondent.
We anticipated that questions about specific sensations would better understood.
It
was intended
be
that this question elicit re-
sponses indicative of hypochondriacal concern and of those
psychologically induced organic discomforts that have the distinguishing feature of shifting
all
around the systems of the
body. This intent was probably not conveyed
in the
complicated
wording of the question. Since there are Eskimo words for overconcern with illness and malingering, these phenomena are
known
them and would probably have been more profitably explored by two or three more concrete questions.
clearly
4.
It
to
Questions in which cultural practices might be pertinent. 17.
Arms
20.
Smoke?
or legs asleep?
seemed to us that the item on "arms and
would lack
legs
going to sleep"
psychiatric significance because of a cultural practice
of the Eskimos. Customarily they
sit
on the
floor
without back
support and with their legs extended straight out in front of
them. This posture almost invariably produces numbness in peo-
who do not habitually sit that way, and even Eskimos say it becomes uncomfortable and that they frequently have numb feelings. This is one of the few questions that was stated simply
ple
Psychophysiological Indicators
123
experience this sensation?" rather than "Are
"Do you bothered
among Eskimos
or
recommend
troubled
by
sensation?"
this
you Thus we would
a restatement of the question in the latter
form
in
order to reduce the likelihood that a cultural practice will mask the psychophysiological import.
The nate
other question in which cultural practice might predomi-
"smoking." In studying
is
a
group that does not have
to-
would obviously be meaningless. bacco As it happens, the St. Lawrence Eskimos, both men and women, have long made use of tobacco in many forms. There were no available, this
question
restraints of propriety to control the use of
the presence of missionaries or latterly
adopted the missionary view.
Nor were
by some of
hibited Stirling
ill
who
effects of
common
the factors that relate to
have
smoking.
practice, unin-
smoking patterns
we would recommend that worded: "Do you feel you smoke
County,
Eskimos be 5.
a
those
there restraints in 1955
based on medical awareness of the possible
Because smoking appeared to be such
tobacco except in
among
in
the question for
too
much?"
Questions that do not appear to he distorted by cultural or physical considerations. 9.
10. 11.
Hands tremble? Hands or feet sweat? Nervous breakdown?
21.
Sick headaches?
14.
Trouble sleeping?
12.
Heart beating hard?
18.
Cold sweats?
16.
Nightmares?
The judgment
of the adequacy of the "hands tremble" item
was supported by descriptions we heard of trembling and shakworry and depression ("shaking sickness"
ing as expressions of
has already been referred to, p. 94). Variations in tendency to perspire a
were recognized; for
"big sweater."
We
instance,
one
man was
described as
did not hear of this sensation in association
Approaches
124
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
we
with emotional disturbance but neither did dence that
it
might not be
have any
a psychiatric variable.
The
evi-
phrase
"nervous breakdown" was used by some of the acculturated Eskimos, and, as indicated in the last chapter, the concept was
approximated by a number of Eskimo words. headaches" has
people in Stirling County.
by
tone that
a colloquial
It is
phrase "sick
readily understandable to
doubtful that
we presume
the Eskimos, and
is
The
this
was
assimilated
that their responses refer to
ordinary headaches. This did not seem to be a distortion of significance.
The
question on "trouble sleeping" avoids the
"morning tiredness" and seemed to be an
culties described for
adequate
way
diffi-
to ask about sleep disturbance.
During the year of investigation
we were
impressed
by
the
degree to which fear was generated and expressed in the Eskimo
group
—fear of witchcraft,
fear of
hunting on the ice or being this
was undoubtedly
becoming
sick
with tubercu-
and fear of the physical dangers of
fear of going hungry,
losis,
lost in the arctic storms.
fear,
realistic
and
we
Much
of
realized that the
problems of distinguishing between fear and anxiety are some-
what
similar to those of differentiating
between organic symp-
toms that stem from physical malfunction and organic commainly of psychological derivation. Nonetheless
plaints that are it
seemed to us that any symptoms such as palpitations and "cold which might be a physiological expression of fear, de-
sweats,"
served special attention.
We
heard
many
descriptive accounts of people having the
sensation of heart pounding. reactions to "scary" events.
Eskimos conceive of situations
this
and "normal"
of "worrying too
considering
it
a
Some were simply
We
also learned,
symptom
in others, that u
much" and
as is,
the reporting of
however, that the
"psychiatric" in
they linked
it
some
to states
too easy to get afraid" as well as
normal reaction to psychological trauma. Also
the Eskimos look
upon palpitations as a premonition of a "faint," and some of them viewed a "faint" as chiefly characterized by
the cessation of the heart beat rather than
by unconsciousness.
A
Psychophysiological Indicators
among Eskimos
"faint" was, in fact, described as "dying a
marked
among
in the last
little
125
bit."
As
re-
chapter, fainting seemed to be quite frequent
the Eskimos.* Thus, besides noting the appropriateness
we would recommend further exploration of this area through questions such as, "Do you take weak turns?" "Do you get dizzy or faint often?" and "Do you ever of the palpitation item,
lose consciousness?"
7
Other questions that could be asked
as
ways of investigating the psychology and would refer to "choking sensations," "hair-raising experiences,"
physiology of fear
and "goose
We
flesh."
expected that the item on nightmares might be espe-
also
cially appropriate to a
group that practices witchcraft and other
forms of spiritualism. During the year there were a number of
which Eskimos elaborated in dreams. For example, two saw a big animal prowling around the village at night. This was so vivid a threat that for several
"scares"
or three people thought they
young men took turns standing watch to guard the vilSome people had "bad dreams" about this eerie creature.
nights lage.
Nor should "voodoo"
Among
witchcraft. to
death be ignored in a group that believes in
were imputed would be pertinent to ask about example, "Have you ever felt that you
these Eskimos, several deaths
"voodoo" sorcery, and
"voodoo" experiences; for
it
were being hexed and might die?" This concludes the statement of recommendations and conjectures based
on knowledge of Eskimo
summarized
as
culture.
The review can be
an attempt to separate those questions which
seem to be equally good indicators for the Eskimos from those questions
which might not
be.
We
judged ten questions to be
appropriate for the Eskimos in their present form
("present
health trouble," "susceptibility to disease," "vitamin pills," "pat-
ent medicine," "loss of appetite," "hands tremble," "hands or * Also, as remarked in the last chapter, conversion reactions seemed to be more prevalent than anticipated. Although conversion is not related to the point in hand nor directly relevant to other points in these five categories, we do want to observe that questioning in this area would
seem
desirable.
Approaches
126
sweat,"
feet
"nervous
to Cross-cultural Psychiatry
breakdown,"
"sick
we
"trouble sleeping"). Eight questions,
headaches,"
and
would have
thought,
different meaning, either being unintelligible because of wording
("tired
mornings" and "ailments
all
over") or referring to be-
havior and sensations that are especially
common
for cultural
and physical reasons and for which the enhanced or reduced psychiatric implications would have to be established through
work ("smoking," "arms and
further
stomach," mares").
We
and "food
The detail
were undecided about two questions ("bad
taste,"
tasteless").
credence of these ideas can
by comparing
now
be examined in greater
the areas of convergence and divergence in
response patterns of the Eskimo sample and
One
"upset
asleep,"
legs
"cold sweats," and "night-
"heart beating hard,"
of the Stirling groups
two
subjects living in rural communities.
The
Stirling samples.
sample of 1,003
a probability
is
other
a
is
group of
ninety-three diagnosed neurotic subjects from in-patient and outpatient services in the general area of Stirling County.
swers from
this latter
group were those mentioned
The
an-
earlier as
HOS scores em-
having been used in selecting and weighting the
ployed in the Stirling Study.
We
know, then, that the two Stirling samples are different: a group identified as "symptom-carriers"; the other is a typical community population in which, we can presume, the one
is
symptom-carriers are
made
much
less
concentrated.
An
assumption
for the twenty-question review as well as for this compara-
tive analysis like that
is
that the sample
from the Eskimo
village
is
more
of the Stirling communities than like the population of
neurotic patients in terms of the prevalence of
ill
people. This
view rests on the strong unlikelihood that the Eskimo sample would include as great a number of symptom-carriers as a group purposefully defined as neurotic
—even though, one could
few more psychiatrically the Eskimo village owing to the dearth
there might be a in
services. Figures
IV- 1 and IV- 2 present
ill
guess,
people maintained
of hospital and clinic
HOS
data on the three
Psychophysiological Indicators
among
Eski?nos
127
populations showing that the Eskimo village sample and the sample of Stirling
community respondents
to each other than either
Figure IV- 1 constructed
is
a
are indeed
more
similar
to the sample of neurotic patients.
diagram of each of the three populations,
by scoring
sample respondent. Stirling
is
HOS
the
The
information given
scoring system
Study has been employed, and
it
by each
worked out
in
the
has been assumed that
the samples are an adequate reflection of the populations as
r
35%t
0
28
Stirling
Stirling
neurotic sample
community sample
_
Eskimo village
sample
on raw data of the Stirling weighted scores for three samples. Ordinate = percentage of sample population. Abscissa = Stirling weighted HOS scores. The range is from o to 28, low scores meaning illness and high scores "wellness." Figure IV- 1. Frequency diagram based
wholes.
The
lations are
similarities
and differences between the three popu-
brought out more coherently by the application of
shown
probit analysis as
in
Figure IV- 2. This analysis was
undertaken in order to examine the consistency of the raw data with the normal distribution of frequencies. probit technique,
it
By means
of this
has been possible to project the presumed
normal population from the evidence provided
in the sample
study. 8 This figure appears to verify the similarity of the
two
16%
14% -
12%
-
10% -
10% Stirling
B%-
M-
-
(P
-
community residents
20 5.7
6%-
Well
-=»
4%-
2%0% 12% Stirling neurotics
10%
Figure Stirling
ft
=10.8
cr
-
4.9
IV -2. Frequency community
normal curves
distributions of
residents,
and
HOS
Stirling
scores for
Eskimo
villagers,
neurotics with best-fitting
Psychophysiological Indicators
community populations and
among Eskimos
their difference
from
129
that of the
neurotics.
The
next task
is
to look at the
for the individual questions.
Eskimo and
Stirling responses
These materials are given
in Figure
IV-3. Each lined bar represents two standard deviations around the
mean per cent of
the Eskimos responding "often," "some-
times," or "never" to each question.
same meaning for the for the neurotics.
The checked
Stirling respondents,
The
use of
bars have the
and the dotted bars
two standard deviations conby showing the range
tributes to the understanding of these data
which two-thirds of the responses
in
fall,
assuming normal
distributions.
Inspection of this chart indicates that there
response pattern for the Stirling patients that of the Stirling
is
which
a characteristic
is
different
from
community members.* The general paradigm
of the patients' response pattern (based on averaging)
is
for 25
per cent to respond "often," 44 per cent "sometimes," and 31 per cent "never." The paradigm for the community residents is 9 per cent "often," 24 per cent "sometimes," and 67 per cent "never." Because the Eskimo sample is small, the range of 2
standard deviations
is
considerably broader than the ranges for
the Stirling samples. Nevertheless the
HOS
it is
immediately apparent that
questions produced a unique distribution of responses
among Eskimos.
It is
theoretically possible that
all
the Eskimos
would have responded "never" to every item, in which case we would have concluded that these questions were probably meaningless
and certainly incompetent for differentiating psychi-
ill Eskimos from those in good mental health. Also the Eskimo pattern is, for some questions, similar to that of the
atrically
*
Nineteen of the questions provide a trichotomous response pattern; one requires the choice of "yes" or "no." This gives a total of 59 possible comparisons. For the Stirling community residents and the Stirling patients there are only nine instances where the standard deviation ranges overlap. This includes the questions on vitamin pills and patent medicines, which are omitted from Figure IV-3 because not every Eskimo was asked these questions.
Figure IV-3. Comparison of Eskimo and Stirling response patterns* Eskimo
I.
villagers
Stirling
community residents
Stirling neurotics
Present health trouble?
1
^5
0%
40%
20%
60%
60%
100%
2. Susceptible to diseases?
Often
Sometimes
Never
100%
* The diagram for each question is arranged so that the figures at the bottom represent the percentage of the population responding Often, Sometimes, or Never. The bars represent two standard deviations around the mean for the
respective population, and the
middle of the bar. the facts that:
(1)
The
mean
for each
(the Stirling residents
tions
drew
indicated
the three samples are different in
range for the smaller sample (the Eskimos)
two
is
by
a line
difference in the lengths of the bars
and the
is
through the explained by
and, hence, the
larger than that for the other
Stirling neurotics),
a particularly large or small
size,
is
number
and
(2)
where ques-
of responses in the Often,
Sometimes, or Never category, the range is smaller than if the mean had more nearly approached 50 per cent. It should be further noted that where a question elicited no responses in a given category (and this occurred only for the Eskimos in the Often category for questions 6, 8, 9, 11, and 17, and in the
Never category for question 13) the two standard-deviation range is from o to 13.7 per cent.
in this instance,
130
for zero,
IV -3
Figure Loss of appetite?
3.
0% 4.
(cont.)
100%
20°/c
Bad taste?
*