ABC of Clinical Reasoning (ABC Series) [2 ed.] 1119871514, 9781119871514

ABC of Clinical Reasoning Being a good clinician is not only about knowledge ― how doctors and other healthcare professi

134 75 5MB

English Pages 80 [82] Year 2022

Report DMCA / Copyright

DOWNLOAD PDF FILE

Table of contents :
ABC of Clinical Reasoning
Contents
Contributors
Preface (Second Edition)
1 Introduction to Clinical Reasoning
2 Evidence-based History and Examination
3 Choosing and Interpreting Diagnostic Tests
4 Problem Identification and Management
5 Shared Decision-making
6 Models of Clinical Reasoning
7 Cognitive Biases
8 Situativity and Human Factors
9 Metacognition and Cognitive Strategies
10 Teaching Clinical Reasoning
Index
EULA
Recommend Papers

ABC of Clinical Reasoning (ABC Series) [2 ed.]
 1119871514, 9781119871514

  • 0 0 0
  • Like this paper and download? You can publish your own PDF file online for free in a few minutes! Sign Up
File loading please wait...
Citation preview

Clinical Reasoning

Clinical Reasoning 2nd Edition EDITED BY

Nicola Cooper

Consultant Physician & Clinical Associate Professor in Medical Education Medical Education Centre University of Nottingham, UK

John Frain

General Practitioner & Clinical Associate Professor Director of Clinical Skills Division of Medical Sciences & Graduate Entry Medicine University of Nottingham, UK

This edition first published 2023 © 2023 John Wiley & Sons Ltd Edition History 1e © 2016 John Wiley & Sons Ltd All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording or otherwise, except as permitted by law. Advice on how to obtain permission to reuse material from this title is available at http://www.wiley.com/go/permissions. The right of Nicola Cooper and John Frain to be identified as the authors of the editorial material in this work has been asserted in accordance with law. Registered Office(s) John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 111 River Street, Hoboken, NJ 07030, USA John Wiley & Sons Ltd, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford, OX4 2DQ, UK Editorial Office 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford, OX4 2DQ, UK For details of our global editorial offices, customer services, and more information about Wiley products visit us at www.wiley.com. Wiley also publishes its books in a variety of electronic formats and by print-on-demand. Some content that appears in standard print versions of this book may not be available in other formats. Trademarks: Wiley and the Wiley logo are trademarks or registered trademarks of John Wiley & Sons, Inc. and/or its affiliates in the United States and other countries and may not be used without written permission. All other trademarks are the property of their respective owners. John Wiley & Sons, Inc. is not associated with any product or vendor mentioned in this book. Limit of Liability/Disclaimer of Warranty The contents of this work are intended to further general scientific research, understanding, and discussion only and are not intended and should not be relied upon as recommending or promoting scientific method, diagnosis, or treatment by physicians for any particular patient. In view of ongoing research, equipment modifications, changes in governmental regulations, and the constant flow of information relating to the use of medicines, equipment, and devices, the reader is urged to review and evaluate the information provided in the package insert or instructions for each medicine, equipment, or device for, among other things, any changes in the instructions or indication of usage and for added warnings and precautions. While the publisher and authors have used their best efforts in preparing this work, they make no representations or warranties with respect to the accuracy or completeness of the contents of this work and specifically disclaim all warranties, including without limitation any implied warranties of merchantability or fitness for a particular purpose. No warranty may be created or extended by sales representatives, written sales materials or promotional statements for this work. The fact that an organization, website, or product is referred to in this work as a citation and/or potential source of further information does not mean that the publisher and authors endorse the information or services the organization, website, or product may provide or recommendations it may make. This work is sold with the understanding that the publisher is not engaged in rendering professional services. The advice and strategies contained herein may not be suitable for your situation. You should consult with a specialist where appropriate. Further, readers should be aware that websites listed in this work may have changed or disappeared between when this work was written and when it is read. Neither the publisher nor authors shall be liable for any loss of profit or any other commercial damages, including but not limited to special, incidental, consequential, or other damages. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Names: Cooper, Nicola, editor. | Frain, John (John Patrick James), editor. Title: ABC of clinical reasoning / edited by Nicola Cooper, Consultant Physician & Clinical Associate Professor in Medical Education, Medical Education Centre, University of Nottingham, UK, John Frain, General Practitioner & Clinical Associate Professor, Director of Clinical Skills, Division of Medical Sciences & Graduate Entry Medicine, University of Nottingham, UK. Description: 2nd edition. | Hoboken, NJ, USA : John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 2023. | Includes bibliographical references and index. Identifiers: LCCN 2022047017 (print) | LCCN 2022047018 (ebook) | ISBN 9781119871514 (paperback) | ISBN 9781119871521 (pdf) | ISBN 9781119871538 (epub) Subjects: LCSH: Medical care--Decision making. | Medical care--Quality control. | Diagnostic errors. | Evidence-based medicine. Classification: LCC RA399.A1 A215 2023 (print) | LCC RA399.A1 (ebook) | DDC 362.1--dc23/eng/20221007 LC record available at https://lccn.loc.gov/2022047017 LC ebook record available at https://lccn.loc.gov/2022047018 Cover image: © agsandrew/Shutterstock Cover design: Wiley Set in 9.5/12pt Minion by Integra Software Services Pvt. Ltd, Pondicherry, India

Contents

Contributors, vi Preface (Second Edition), vii 1 Introduction to Clinical Reasoning, 1

Nicola Cooper and John Frain

2 Evidence-based History and Examination, 7

John Frain

3 Choosing and Interpreting Diagnostic Tests, 17

Nicola Cooper

4 Problem Identification and Management, 23

Nicola Cooper and John Frain

5 Shared Decision-making, 29

Anna Hammond and Simon Gay

6 Models of Clinical Reasoning, 35

Nicola Cooper

7 Cognitive Biases, 41

Nicola Cooper

8 Situativity and Human Factors, 47

Nicola Cooper

9 Metacognition and Cognitive Strategies, 53

Pat Croskerry

10 Teaching Clinical Reasoning, 61

Nicola Cooper and Mini Singh Index, 71

Contributors

Nicola Cooper

Simon Gay

MBChB MMedSci FRCPE FRACP FAcadMEd SFHEA Consultant Physician and Clinical Associate Professor in Medical Education Medical Education Centre, University of Nottingham, UK

MBBS MSc MMedEd FRCGP SFHEA Professor of Medical Education (Primary Care) University of Leicester School of Medicine, UK

Pat Croskerry

Anna Hammond

MD PhD FRCP(Edin) Director, Critical Thinking Program Division of Medical Education, Dalhousie University, Canada

MBChB DRCOG FRCGP MClinEd SFHEA General Practitioner and Academic Lead for Clinical Skills and Reasoning Hull York Medical School, UK

John Frain

Mini Singh

MBChB MSc FRCGP DGM DCH DRCOG PGDipCard SFHEA General Practitioner and Clinical Associate Professor Director of Clinical Skills Division of Medical Sciences and Graduate Entry Medicine University of Nottingham, UK

vi

MBChB MMEd FRCP PFHEA Professor of Medical Education and Honorary Consultant Dermatologist University of Manchester, UK

Preface (Second Edition)

Excellence in clinical practice is not just about good knowledge, skills, and behaviours. As fellow author Pat Croskerry points out, how doctors think, reason, and make decisions is arguably their most critical skill. While medical schools and postgraduate training programmes teach and assess the knowledge and skills required to practice as a doctor, few currently offer comprehensive training in clinical reasoning and decision making. This matters because studies show that diagnostic error is common and results in significant harm to patients, and the majority of the root causes of diagnostic error involve errors in clinical reasoning. Clinical reasoning is complex and takes years to learn. Most of the time it is learned implicitly and in an ad hoc fashion. In this book, we have made it explicit, broken down into its core components. This book is designed to be an introduction for individuals

and also an up-to-date resource for teachers and curriculum planners. Each chapter describes a component of clinical reasoning and its applications for clinical practice, teaching, and learning. This second edition has been extensively re-written and updated, and key references and further resources have been included for readers who want to explore topics in more detail. Clinical reasoning is relevant to every clinical specialty in every setting, and it is not confined to medical students and doctors – we have written this book with advanced clinical practitioners and other clinicians in mind as well. We hope you enjoy reading it as much as we enjoyed re-writing and editing it. Nicola Cooper John Frain

CHAPTER 1

Introduction to Clinical Reasoning Nicola Cooper and John Frain

OVERVIEW • Clinical reasoning describes the application of knowledge to collect and integrate information from various sources to arrive at a diagnosis and/or management plan • A lack of clinical reasoning ability has been shown to be a major cause of diagnostic error • Several components of clinical reasoning have been identified • Expertise in clinical reasoning develops as a result of different types of knowledge plus some other important factors • Clinical reasoning can be viewed from different perspectives that each give insights into how it can be taught and learned and why it goes wrong

Introduction Fellow author, Pat Croskerry, argues that although there are several qualities we would look for in a good clinician, the two absolute basic requirements for someone who is going to give you the best chance of being correctly diagnosed and appropriately managed are these: someone who is both knowledgeable and a good decision maker. At the time of writing, medical schools and postgraduate training programmes teach and assess the knowledge and skills required to practice as a doctor, but few offer a comprehensive curriculum in decision-making. This is a problem because how doctors think, reason, and make decisions is arguably their most critical skill [1]. This book covers the core components of clinical decisionmaking – or clinical reasoning. It is designed for individuals but also for teachers and learners as part of a curriculum in clinical reasoning. Chapter 10 specifically covers teaching clinical reasoning in undergraduate and postgraduate settings. In this chapter we define clinical reasoning, explain why it is important, and introduce some of the different components of clinical reasoning that are explored in this book. We will consider how expertise in clinical reasoning develops, and also look at clinical reasoning through different lenses. ABC of Clinical Reasoning, Second Edition. Edited by Nicola Cooper and John Frain. © 2023 John Wiley & Sons Ltd. Published 2023 by John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

Definitions Clinical reasoning describes the application of knowledge to collect and integrate information from various sources to arrive at a diagnosis and/or management plan for patients [2]. It is a complex cognitive process involving clinical skills, memory, problemsolving, and decision-making. A definition of clinical reasoning is given in Box 1.1. As the definition in Box 1.1 states, clinical reasoning can be defined as a skill, process, or outcome and multiple components of clinical reasoning have been identified. However, for teachers and learners, it can be useful to think of clinical reasoning as a process made up of different components, each of which requires specific knowledge, skills, and behaviours. The UK Clinical Reasoning in Medical Education group has defined five broad areas of clinical reasoning education [3]: 1. History and physical examination 2. Choosing and interpreting diagnostic tests 3. Problem identification and management

Box 1.1  A definition of clinical reasoning ‘Clinical reasoning can be defined as a skill, process, or outcome wherein clinicians observe, collect, and interpret data to diagnose and treat patients. Clinical reasoning entails both conscious and unconscious cognitive operations interacting with contextual factors. Contextual factors include, but are not limited to, the patient’s unique circumstances and preferences and the characteristics of the practice environment. Multiple components of clinical reasoning can be identified: information gathering, hypothesis generation, forming a problem representation, generating a differential diagnosis, selecting a leading or working diagnosis, providing a diagnostic justification, and developing a management or treatment plan. A number of theories (e.g., script, dual process, and cognitive load theories) from diverse fields (e.g., cognitive psychology, sociology, education) inform research on clinical reasoning.’ From Daniel et al. (2019). Acad Med; 94(6): 902–12.

2

ABC of Clinical Reasoning

Figure 1.1  Five broad areas of clinical reasoning education. Clinical reasoning concepts include key theories (e.g., script, dual process), how clinical reasoning ability develops, the problem of diagnostic error, the role of clinical reasoning in safe and effective care for patients, cognitive errors, and other factors that may impair the clinical reasoning process or outcome.

4. Shared decision-making 5. Clinical reasoning concepts In this second edition, we have used this framework and explore each of these areas (see Figure 1.1) in more detail.

Why Is Clinical Reasoning Important? Diagnostic errors tend to occur in common diseases and are a significant cause of preventable harm to patients worldwide [4]. It has been estimated that diagnosis is wrong 10–15% of the time [5]. Post-mortem studies consistently find undiagnosed disease as the cause of death in 10–20% of patients, of which half could have been successfully treated [6]. Diagnostic error is by far the leading source of paid malpractice claims in the UK, and diagnostic error has been identified as a high-priority patient safety problem by the World Health Organization. A lack of clinical reasoning ability has been shown to be a major cause of diagnostic errors which can result in unnecessary pain, treatments, or procedures, and increase the costs of healthcare [2]. There are many reasons why diagnostic errors occur. A comprehensive review of studies of misdiagnosis assigned three main categories, shown in Box 1.2. However, faulty synthesis of the available information was found to be the most common reason for diagnostic errors leading to death and serious harm [7]. In other words, the clinician had all the information to make the right diagnosis but made the wrong diagnosis. There is a growing consensus that medical schools and postgraduate training programmes need to do more to teach clinical reasoning in an explicit and systematic way. The National Academy of Medicine’s report Improving Diagnosis in Health Care [8] found that diagnosis and diagnostic errors have been largely unappreciated in efforts to improve the quality and safety of healthcare and called for curricula to explicitly address teaching the diagnostic process, using

educational approaches that are aligned with evidence from the learning sciences. Diagnostic error definitely causes harm, but increasing attention is also being paid to another problem which can be caused by faulty clinical reasoning – the harm caused by unnecessary tests and overdiagnosis. A study of over one million Medicare patients in the USA looked at how often people received one of 26 tests or treatments deemed by scientific and professional organisations to be of no benefit [9]. These included things like brain imaging in syncope, screening for carotid artery disease in asymptomatic patients, and imaging of the spine in low back pain with no red flags. In one year, at least 25% of patients received at least one of these tests or treatments. It has been estimated that at least 20% of healthcare spending is waste [10]. This waste has a huge impact on patients and the wider healthcare economy. Overdiagnosis occurs when people without relevant symptoms are diagnosed with a disease that ultimately will not cause them to experience symptoms or early death. There are many factors contributing to overdiagnosis (see Box 1.3), but one of them is the increasing availability of increasingly sensitive tests. The growing recognition of the problem of diagnostic error, unnecessary tests, and overdiagnosis is why clinical reasoning is Box 1.2  Categories of misdiagnosis Error category

Examples

No fault

Unusual presentation of a disease Missing information

System errors

Technical, e.g. unavailable tests/results Organisational, e.g. poor supervision of junior staff, error-prone processes, impossible workload

Human cognitive error

Faulty data gathering Inadequate reasoning

Adapted from Graber ML et al., 2005 [7].

Box 1.3  Factors contributing to overdiagnosis • Screening programmes that detect ‘pseudodisease’ – disease in a person without symptoms in a form that will never cause symptoms or early death • Increasingly sensitive tests • Greater access to scanning – diagnostic scanning of the head and body reveals incidental findings in up to 40% of those being scanned for other reasons, often leading to anxiety and further testing for an abnormality that would never have caused harm • Widening definitions of disease and lower treatment thresholds, for example: • Chronic kidney disease • High cholesterol • Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder • Cultural considerations – medicalisation, commission bias (better to do something than nothing), fear of litigation • Individual clinicians’ lack of understanding of statistics relevant to the disease, diagnostic test, and intervention in question Adapted from Moynihan R. Preventing overdiagnosis: how to stop harming the healthy. BMJ 2012; 344: e3502.



of such interest to researchers, medical educators, and policy makers. Improving clinical reasoning outcomes is a patient safety and healthcare economy priority.

Components of Clinical Reasoning Several components of clinical reasoning have been identified. A fundamental one is the application of knowledge to gather and interpret data in the patient’s history and physical examination. The purpose is to establish the clinical probability of disease – a judgement based on the clinician’s knowledge of epidemiology and what we call evidence-based history and physical examination, a topic which we explore in Chapter 2. The clinical probability of disease is a prerequisite for choosing and interpreting diagnostic tests. Interpreting diagnostic tests is something even qualified health professionals find difficult [11]. This is because tests lie; very often, tests give us test probabilities, not real probabilities, which is why test results have to be interpreted by knowledgeable clinicians, a topic which we explore in Chapter 3. Problem representation is something that is neglected in traditional ‘history–examination–differential diagnosis’ teaching methods, but studies show that being able to represent the problem before attempting to solve it (i.e., think of a diagnosis) is a key step in problem-solving, and dramatically increases diagnostic accuracy, especially in more complex cases [12]. This is a skill that can be learned, and a topic which we explore in Chapter 4. Clinical reasoning often takes place within teams. Clinicians also make use of guidelines, scores and decision aids, and co-produce decisions with patients and carers. The important topic of shared decision-making is explored further in Chapter 5. Simply knowing about clinical reasoning concepts does not help people reason better. But it is important for clinicians, teachers, and learners to have a shared definition, vocabulary and understanding of clinical reasoning in order to facilitate meaningful discussion and learning. Models of clinical reasoning can be useful to help us understand the processes underpinning our decision-making – as clinicians, teachers, and learners. Chapter 6 explores dual process theories which are widely accepted as a framework with which to understand diagnostic reasoning and diagnostic error. Some common misunderstandings are identified, and we explore critical thinking, rationality, the different types of knowledge used by Type 1 and Type 2 processing, and thinking about one’s own thinking (metacognition). The topic of cognitive biases in clinical reasoning is controversial. This is partly because there are several key fallacies in the received view of dual process theories. There is definitely agreement that cognitive biases exist in medicine, but disagreement as to whether they are a significant source of diagnostic errors compared with knowledge deficits. Chapter 7 explores this topic further using a case history and analysis. Clinical reasoning does not exist solely inside a clinician’s organised cognitive structures but is entangled in the activity of providing care for the patient [13]. Chapter 8 explores ‘situativity’ and human factors (the science of the limitations of human performance). ‘To err is human’, therefore in order to minimise

Introduction to Clinical Reasoning

3

errors, we need to focus on improving processes, systems, and technology, as well as education and training in cognitive strategies. Pat Croskerry explores metacognition and cognitive strategies further in Chapter 9. Finally, we look at teaching clinical reasoning in Chapter 10. There is no evidence that teaching clinical reasoning concepts alone, or short courses, improves clinical reasoning ability. The most effective way to teach clinical reasoning is to use strategies that build knowledge and understanding, and to practice with as many different cases as possible in as many different contexts as possible with coaching and feedback. We explore key concepts in teaching clinical reasoning, specific evidence-based strategies that teachers can use, and describe one approach to introducing a clinical reasoning curriculum at undergraduate level.

How Does Expertise in Clinical Reasoning Develop? If how clinicians think, reason, and make decisions is arguably their most critical skill, it is useful to consider how expertise in clinical reasoning develops. In the 1970s, expertise in medicine was thought to be related to superior general thinking skills. However, when researchers observed experts and novices, they found there was no difference in the processes or thinking strategies used – both quickly came up with one or more diagnostic hypotheses which guided the search for further information. Experts were more accurate because they knew more, and because the knowledge of experts varied from case to case, their performance varied from case to case as well [14]. This led to researchers changing direction and examining the role of knowledge in medical expertise. One of the next questions for researchers was, do experts have bigger, better memories? The answer was no – given unlimited time, novices can remember as much as experts about a clinical case on paper. But experts appear to acquire information more efficiently and pay attention to more critical information (you have probably seen this in action). In a series of well-known experiments, Chase and Simon showed chess players of varying strength – from master to novice – chessboards set up as if in the middle of a game for only 5 seconds and then asked them to reconstruct the position of 28 pieces on a blank chessboard immediately afterwards. What they found was the chess masters showed a remarkable ability to reconstruct the board almost perfectly, whereas the novices could only recall the position of four or five pieces. However, when the experiment was repeated with the chess pieces arranged randomly, chess masters performed no better than anyone else [15]. Chase and Simon concluded that chess masters had stored in memory a large number of recognisable ‘chunks’, or meaningful patterns (see Box 1.4). Similar results have been found in other fields – experts can reconstruct a briefly examined scene provided it portrays a realistic (as opposed to random or meaningless) pattern. But pattern recognition by itself does not explain expertise. Non-chess players can be trained to memorise chess patterns. Experts recognise patterns of high significance because of their formal as well as experiential knowledge of chess – in other words, they study [16].

4

ABC of Clinical Reasoning

We know that knowledge is fundamental to expertise in clinical reasoning. (As we will see in Chapter 6, other things matter as well.) But by knowledge, we do not mean only facts. That is like saying the raw ingredients are the same as the cake. Figure 1.2 refers to different types of knowledge; all these types of knowledge matter in clinical reasoning. With learning, the process of chunking and automating, as described in Box 1.4, frees up mental resources. The difference

Metacognitive • Knowledge of cognition in general as well as

awareness of one’s own cognition

Procedural • How to do something, methods of inquiry, and

criteria for using skills, algorithms, techniques, and methods

Conceptual • The inter-relationships among the basic elements

within a larger structure that enable them to function together

Factual • The basic elements learners must know to be

acquainted with a discipline or solve problems in it

Figure 1.2  Different types of knowledge. Adapted from: A model of learning objectives–based on: a taxonomy for learning, teaching, and assessing: a revision of Bloom’s Taxonomy of educational objectives by Rex Heer, Center for Excellence in Learning and Teaching, Iowa State University. https://www.celt.iastate.edu/teaching/effective-teaching-practices/ revised-blooms-taxonomy (accessed April 2022).

Box 1.4  Chunking and automating: how we can think more complex thoughts Human working memory can only process a limited number of elements at a time. But not all elements are created equal. The elements of information stored in our long-term memory increase in complexity over time, with smaller elements combining to form larger ones. This process is called ‘chunking’ and it is what allows us to think more complex thoughts. For example, a child who has not yet learned the alphabet will see the letter ‘H’ as three straight lines. Reproducing these three straight lines correctly could be a cognitively demanding task. Over time, they will chunk and automate drawing the letter ‘H’ until it becomes effortless. Once they have mastered individual letters, whole words will still be new to them. The word ‘H, o, u, s, e’ will consist of five separate elements. But over time, this will be chunked as the word ‘House’ in long-term memory which can then be read and written effortlessly. Chunking and automation is a result of learning. Experts store knowledge in long-term memory as rich chunks called schemas which allows them to overcome the limitations of working memory when solving problems. (See Chapter 10 for information on how teachers can facilitate the process of schema formation.) Adapted from Lovell O. Sweller’s cognitive load theory in action. A John Catt Publication, 2020. pp. 20.

between normal learning and expert learning is what people do with those freed up resources. People who become experts reinvest their mental resources in further learning. They seek out more difficult problems. They tackle more complex representations of common problems. They continue to work at the edge of their competence [17]. In summary, we know that expertise in clinical reasoning is highly dependent on knowledge, but that is not the whole story. We will explore this further in Chapters 6 and 10.

Clinical Reasoning through Different Lenses At its most basic level, clinical reasoning is to do with knowledge, how knowledge is organised in long-term memory as mental representations, and the cognitive processes responsible for storing, transforming, and retrieving these. This view of clinical reasoning is important for learners, who must have a) a deep foundation of factual knowledge, b) understand facts and ideas in a conceptual framework, and c) organise their knowledge in a way that facilitates retrieval and application. (The latter two points are why we need expert teachers.) However, clinical reasoning is also something that is ‘situated’ in the environment. Thinking and learning is context-dependent, the result of multiple dynamic interactions between individuals and the environment. Cognition is also distributed in team members and in non-humans (e.g., computers). It can be adversely affected by poorly designed technology and systems, as well as sleep deprivation, fatigue, and excessive workload. Understanding clinical reasoning through this lens is important too. But sometimes we need to be able to go beyond the ‘content’ and make sense of the patient’s illness (and our own response to it) and understand the practice of medicine in its wider socio-cultural context. This is what we sometimes call the ‘art’ of medicine, which involves crafting a wise and deliberate course of action appropriate for the circumstances and may not involve any technical decision-making at all. Clinical reasoning through this lens is often what postgraduates are able to start practicing once their mental resources are freed up from focusing on the technical aspects of medicine. Many studies demonstrate a correlation between effective clinician–patient communication and ‘whole person care’ with improved health outcomes. An example of this is given in Box 1.5.

Listen to Me; I Am Telling You My Diagnosis Whatever their level of health literacy, no patient is an empty vessel devoid of knowledge or insight about their own condition. By presenting to a clinician, patients have often engaged in their own reasoning and formulated ideas about a diagnosis or even several diagnoses. The patient’s perspective is not simply a psychosocial one but may include relevant insights into anatomical and physiological changes and pathology (see Box 1.6). This does not mean they are always correct – that is why they attend a clinician – but neither is their perspective irrelevant. While the clinician arrives at a differential diagnosis (and/or problem list) following the history, or patient interview, the patient arrives with their own differential diagnosis (and/or problem list).



Introduction to Clinical Reasoning

Box 1.5  The importance of whole person care Two patients had similar symptoms. They were experiencing transient numbness of different parts of the body – one side of the face or the other, sometimes the arm or hand. These symptoms were causing a great deal of anxiety. The patients went to see two different physicians. The first patient told his story. At the end of the consultation the physician said, ‘Well you’ve either got migraine or multiple sclerosis so we’ll do an MRI scan and I’ll let you know the results.’ He was not given a further appointment. While waiting for his MRI scan, his anxiety and symptoms increased significantly. The second patient told her story. Recognising that these symptoms are common in stress and did not fit any neurological pattern, the physician said, ‘I see lots of people with these symptoms and very often it’s because they are working too hard, not sleeping, or under stress. Even though they might not realise they are stressed, their body is telling them they’re stressed. Tell me about your schedule and what’s going on in your life.’ The patient’s husband looked at her knowingly and sure enough there were lots of stressors related to work and home that had been an issue. An MRI scan was arranged, but the patient was advised to make changes to her lifestyle and her symptoms resolved. Both patients had normal MRI scans. Explanation and good communication lead to better outcomes, greater compliance with recommended treatments, and less re-attendances.

Box 1.6  A patient-centred approach to differential diagnosis Agreeing the differential diagnosis Patient’s differential

Doctor’s differential

• Leading hypothesis • Alternative 1 • Alternative 2 • …… • ‘Must-not-miss’

• Leading hypothesis • Alternative 1 • Alternative 2 • …… • ‘Must-not-miss’

Where is the common understanding? ‘Let us think about this together’ Eventual diagnosis must explain both parties’ data

The history, or patient interview, is a discussion of the patient’s ideas and insights into their symptoms and diagnosis. The clinician’s role is crucial to the correct synthesis of this information, but this must be done with a complete dataset elicited from the patient.

Patients describe their own insight with differing levels of articulacy, but all patients do this. Clinical reasoning should from the outset be a collaborative and dynamic process between patient and clinician (see Box 1.7). The benefits include identifying the patient’s priorities to the eventual goal of shared decision-making. Yet, how often do we hear, either in a media story, complaint, or adverse event, that the clinician ‘wouldn’t listen to what we were saying’? This feeling among patients of dissonance between clinicians and themselves is reflected by the evidence base [18–20]. Assessing the patient’s own starting point by defining their own understanding and experience of their symptoms at the beginning of the interview is crucial to avoiding diagnostic error.

5

Box 1.7  The patient presents after reasoning through their symptoms ‘Thank you for speaking to me doctor. I’ve been feeling unwell for several days. My hay fever is usually bad at this time of year. I know the pollen count is high at the moment, but my usual medication is not working. My nose is blocked, and I’ve been sneezing. My chest is tight as well and I’ve been coughing more at night. My blue inhaler has helped but I’m using it more than usual and it’s not as effective. My peak flow is down. I was wondering if I need some steroids as well. I was shielding during the first lockdown, and I know COVID is coming back again. Do I need another jab to boost my immunity?’ Patient’s problem list: 1. My hay fever is not controlled 2. My asthma is deteriorating 3. The prevalence of COVID-19 is increasing. Do I need further immunisation? There is a lot of data to unpack here to make the diagnosis and answer the patient’s concerns.

The different lenses through which we can view clinical reasoning, whether from the clinician or the patient’s perspective, are not mutually exclusive. There are situations that call for rapid technical responses, for example, emergencies. Then there are others that call for time, wisdom, and care. Clinical reasoning is complex and takes years to learn. The challenge for educators is to provide clinical environments with multiple tasks and strategies in order to equip learners over time to be able to reason through a variety of clinical problems as effectively as possible [21].

Summary At the time of writing, most medical schools and postgraduate training programmes provide instruction in the basic elements of the diagnostic process. However, students and trainees largely learn the knowledge, skills, and behaviours required for effective clinical reasoning implicitly and ad hoc, through experience and apprenticeship. Yet a lack of clinical reasoning ability has been shown to be a major cause of diagnostic errors resulting in significant preventable harm to patients worldwide. There is a growing consensus that medical schools and postgraduate training programmes should teach clinical reasoning in a way that is explicitly integrated into courses throughout each year of the programme, adopting a systematic approach consistent with current evidence. For teachers and learners, it can be useful to think of clinical reasoning as a process made up of different components, each of which requires specific knowledge, skills, and behaviours. These components are what the following chapters explore in more detail. If we can start with an understanding of what clinical reasoning is, why it is important, what the key components are, and how it develops, we are in a better position to create clinicians who are good decision makers and who ultimately provide better care for patients.

6

ABC of Clinical Reasoning

References 1. Croskerry P. Clinical decision making. In: Barach P, Jacobs J, Lipshultz S, and Laussen P (Eds). Pediatric and Congenital Cardiac Care. London: Springer, 2015. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4471-6566-8_33 (accessed April 2022). 2. DID-ACT Project. Developing, implementing, and disseminating an adaptive clinical reasoning curriculum for healthcare students and educators. An EU funded project, 2020. https://did-act.eu/home/project (accessed April 2022). 3. Cooper N, Bartlett M, Gay S et al. On behalf of the UK Clinical Reasoning in Medical Education (CReME) consensus statement group. (2021). Consensus statement on the content of clinical reasoning curricula in undergraduate medical education. Medical Teacher; 43(2): 152–159. 4. Tehrani ASS, Lee HW, Mathews SC et al. (2013). 25-year summary of US malpractice claims for diagnostic errors 1986-2010: an analysis from the National Practitioner Data Bank. BMJ Quality & Safety; 22(8): 672–680. 5. Graber ML. (2013). The incidence of diagnostic error in medicine. BMJ Quality & Safety; 22(Suppl 2): ii21–ii27. 6. Leape LL, Berwick DM and Bates DW. (2002). Counting deaths due to medical errors. [Letters]. JAMA; 288(19): 2405. 7. Graber ML, Franklin N and Gordon R. (2005). Diagnostic error in internal medicine. Archives of Internal Medicine; 165: 1493–1499. 8. National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. Improving diagnosis in health care. Washington (DC): The National Academies Press, 2015. doi:10.17226/21794 (accessed April 2022). 9. Shwarz A, Landon B, Elshaug A et al. (2014). Measuring low value care in Medicare. JAMA Internal Medicine; 174(7): 1067–1076. 10. Berwick D and Hackbarth A. (2012). Eliminating waste in US healthcare. JAMA; 307(14): 1513–1516. 11. Whiting PF, Davenport C, Jameson C et al. (2015). How well do health professionals interpret diagnostic information? A systematic review. BMJ Open; 5(7): e008155.

12. Bordage G. (1994). Elaborated knowledge: a key to successful diagnostic thinking. Academic Medicine; 69(11): 883–885. 13. Koufidis C, Manninen K, Nieminen J et al. (2022). Representation, interaction, and interpretation. Making sense of the context in clinical reasoning. Medical Education; 566: 98–109. 14. Elstein AS, Shulman LS and Sprafka SA. Medical problem solving. An analysis of clinical reasoning. Harvard University Press, 1978. 15. Chase WG and Simon HA. (1973). Perception in chess. Cognitive Psychology; 4: 55–81. 16. Bereiter C and Scardamalia M. Experts are different from us: they have more knowledge. In: Surpassing ourselves: an inquiry into the nature and implications of expertise. Open Court Publishing, 1993. pp. 25–42. 17. Bereiter C and Scardamalia M. Expertise as a process. In: Surpassing ourselves: an inquiry into the nature and implications of expertise. Open Court Publishing, 1993. pp. 77–120. 18. Fransen GAJ, Janssen MJR, Muris JWM et al. (2007). Measuring the severity of upper gastrointestinal complaints: does GP assessment correspond with patients’ self- assessment? Family Practice; 24: 252–258. 19. Dorval E, Rey JF, Soufflet C et al. (2011). Perspectives on gastroesophageal reflux disease in primary care: the REFLEX study of patient-physician agreement. BMC Gastroenterology; 11: 25–33. 20. Humphrys E, Walter FM, Rubin G et al. (2020). Patient symptom experience prior to a diagnosis of oesophageal or gastric cancer: a multi-methods study. BJGP Open; 4(1): bjgpopen20X101001. 21. Eva K. (2004). What every teacher needs to know about clinical reasoning. Medical Education; 39: 98–106.

Further Resource 1. Bereiter C and Scardamalia M. Surpassing ourselves: an inquiry into the nature and implications of expertise. Open Court Publishing, 1993.

CHAPTER 2

Evidence-based History and Examination John Frain

OVERVIEW • The history and physical examination are essential diagnostic tools

will introduce the idea of evidence-based history and physical examination, an important component of clinical reasoning.

• To formulate a differential diagnosis, the history should identify the patient’s key presenting symptoms and associated symptoms

The Evidence Base

• The patient’s background and the natural history of a condition are essential contextual factors in making a correct diagnosis

Most of our knowledge of history and physical examination predates the development of evidence-based medicine, and from a time when patients presented later in the course of their illness, usually with more ‘classic’ features. Nowadays, especially in developed countries, clinical problems present earlier and are often less well differentiated from one another. We know that many single features in the history and physical examination are limited in their usefulness (e.g., whether someone’s chest discomfort is ‘central and crushing’ or not; or whether Kernig’s sign is present). Nonetheless, good quality evidence now exists, including systematic reviews of symptoms and diagnostic accuracy studies of physical signs. This is being assimilated into textbooks (see further resources) that provide epidemiological and qualitative data as well as diagnostic accuracy studies including sensitivities, specificities, and likelihood ratios. Where statistical evidence is lacking, authors provide diagnostic guides based on the best available epidemiological evidence. Evidence-based principles and knowledge can be reinforced for learners through reflective coursework (see Box 2.1) which can further enable reflection on clinical reasoning during workplace training.

• An incomplete physical examination increases the risk of diagnostic error • If available, likelihood ratios are a measure of diagnostic accuracy which are easy to understand and apply at the bedside • Clinical teachers should be familiar with evidence-based history and examination

Introduction Evidence-based history and examination surveys all data from the clinical encounter, compares it to available evidence, including recognised diagnostic standards, and quickly identifies those variables with the greatest diagnostic accuracy. Around 80% of diagnoses are made from the history alone [1]. Its importance is such that Hampton et al. recommended more emphasis should be placed on teaching students accurate historytaking and more emphasis should be placed on research into communication between patients and clinicians [1]. The purpose of the history is the generation of a differential diagnosis broad enough to include the actual diagnosis but focussed enough to be tested by an appropriate physical examination, and investigations when necessary. The differential diagnosis consists of a leading hypothesis and two or three other hypotheses including any ‘must not miss’ diagnoses, all justifiable by the data gathered, and taking into account the patient’s own concerns. Generating a differential diagnosis guards against premature closure (see Chapter 9). For the learner, it aids in developing their clinical reasoning ability. Generating a differential diagnosis needs to be taught alongside the history and physical examination, not separately. This chapter

ABC of Clinical Reasoning, Second Edition. Edited by Nicola Cooper and John Frain. © 2023 John Wiley & Sons Ltd. Published 2023 by John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

Exploring Symptoms: Combining Process and Content The process of history-taking cannot be separated from knowledge of epidemiology, anatomy, and physiology. Nor can it be separated from effective communication with patients [1]. Irrespective of the cause, each patient seen by a clinician is essentially saying: ‘I’ve noticed some changes in my anatomy and physiology. Can you tell me, is this normal, part of getting older or is it pathology? If it’s pathology, will I return to my previous health, will it leave me with some loss of function or could it even shorten

8

ABC of Clinical Reasoning

Box 2.1  Reflective coursework: the presentation of chest pain in women

Box 2.2  The Calgary-Cambridge model: the process required for accurate history-taking

‘I encountered a female patient in her 50s presenting with a possible acute coronary syndrome (ACS). We had recently been discussing in clinical skills about possible differences in the presentation of acute coronary syndrome in women and men. I decided to examine the evidence for this. I undertook a systematic search of the literature using the 6S evidence pyramid we had been taught. I identified 16 relevant studies using the SORT criteria.i There was heterogeneity of data recording and analysis across the studies. However, I was able to draw the following conclusions:

• The patient’s opening statement (usually 30–120 seconds) • Identifying a problem list • Agreeing an agenda for the interview • Exploring each symptom experienced by the patient • Patient and clinician agreeing on definition of each symptom present • Gathering data for each symptom using open questions • Completing details using closed question • Establishing a sequence of events • Attentive listening • Picking up cues • Exploring the patient’s relevant background information • Relevant systems review • Ensuring all the patients concerns have been addressed • Explanation and planning • Shared decision-making

• Women are more likely to present with ACS atypically compared to men • Men are more likely to present with chest pain than women • Chest pain is the most common symptom presentation for both sexes • Younger women are more likely than older women to present with typical symptoms • No difference exists between sexes for prevalence of chest pain and/or other typical symptoms of ACS • Women reported more associated (non-chest pain) ACS symptoms than men

Adapted from Silverman J, Kurtz SM, Draper J. Skills for Communicating with Patients, 3rd edn. CRC Press, 2013.

Continued development as a clinician requires three things: reflective practice, unwavering curiosity, and maintaining an open mind to new evidence and ideas. Writing this essay has highlighted the importance of looking to the highest level of evidence for guidance, while maintaining a healthy level of scepticism for the recommendations by analysing the primary literature and systematic reviews behind them.’

Box 2.3  Summary of key symptoms by body system General Fatigue/malaise Fevers/rigors/night sweats Weight/appetite Sleep disturbance Rashes/bruising

Cardiovascular Pain Breathlessness Palpitations Swelling

i. Ebell MH, Siwek J, Weiss BD et al. (2004). Strength of recommendation taxonomy (SORT): a patient-centered approach to grading evidence in the medical literature. Am Fam Physician; 69(3): 548–56.

Respiratory Pain Breathlessness Wheeze Cough Sputum/haemoptysis

Alimentary Difficulty swallowing Nausea/vomiting/haematemesis Indigestion/heartburn Pain/distension Change in bowel habit Bleeding

Genitourinary Frequency Dysuria Incontinence Change in urinary volume Prostatic symptoms Menstrual symptoms

Nervous system Headache Loss of consciousness Dizziness Visual disturbance Hearing Weakness Numbness/tingling Memory or personality change Anxiety/depression

This is an excerpt from a patient-based piece of coursework undertaken by graduate entry medicine students at the University of Nottingham, UK. Students examine the evidence underlying a clinical feature encountered in practice.

my life?’ Clinical reasoning and decision-making is required to assess all these concerns. Models for clinical communication have been developed, most notably the Calgary-Cambridge model [2] consisting of 70 skills which facilitate accurate history-taking (see Box 2.2). Teaching this model involves deliberate practice with detailed, specific feedback by observers. It is used either one-to-one with a tutor and patient, with patient actors, or at the bedside with real patients. An adapted form of the observation guide includes feedback on elements of clinical reasoning [3, 4]. When taking a history, key symptoms emerge within the relevant system (see Box 2.3). While symptoms may overlap different systems (e.g., chest pain could be cardiac, musculoskeletal, or respiratory in origin) or be challenging for both patient and doctor to define (e.g., dizziness), many diseases present with varying configurations of key symptoms within the relevant system. Provided the clinical setting of an individual symptom is clearly defined (e.g., nausea and vomiting in patients with suspected intestinal obstruction, or chest pain in patients with suspected myocardial infarction in the emergency department) it is possible

Musculoskeletal Pain Stiffness Swelling Loss of function or activities of daily living Adapted from the Calgary-Cambridge Guide. In: Silverman J, Kurtz S and Draper J. Skills for Communicating with Patients, 3rd edn. CRC Press, 2013.

to reason its significance and thus its usefulness as evidence of the presence of the target condition. Course content can be developed to teach students the evidence-base for each symptom alongside the process of exploring symptoms with the patient (Box 2.4).



Evidence-based History and Examination

Box 2.4  Summary evidence box for cardiovascular pain Definition Impairment of the blood supply to the heart. Pain is precipitated by exertion and relieved by rest. Anatomical site Narrowing of the coronary arteries, usually by atherosclerosis but occasionally by spasm. Pathophysiology Primarily, smoking, high cholesterol, high blood pressure, and diabetes damage the endothelial lining of the coronary arteries. What does the patient say? ‘I keep getting pains in my chest whenever I do things.’ ‘My angina is getting worse.’ Evidence base The table shows the European Society of Cardiology clinical classification of angina:i Typical angina

Meets all three of the following characteristics: 1. C  haracteristic retrosternal chest discomfort – typical quality and duration 2. Provoked by exertion and emotion 3. Relieved by rest or GTN (glyceryl trinitrate) or both

Atypical angina Non-cardiac chest pain

Meets two of the above characteristics Meets one or none of the above characteristics

Up to 95% of chest pain presents in primary care.ii Of patients presenting with chest pain at the emergency department, almost a third have serious cardiovascular disease. Only 11–44% of patients referred to the cardiology out-patients have coronary artery disease.iii The presence of atypical symptoms such as pain worse on inspiration, stabbing pain, positional, reproducible on palpation of the chest wall and no radiation is more useful in identifying patients who do not require investigation (positive predictive value = 0.94) than are the features of typical angina in identifying patients who do require further investigation (positive predictive value = 0.21). In stable chest pain, the patient’s own description of the pain is the most important predictor of coronary heart disease, and in acute pain, the history of chest pain should be judged alongside the ECG findings.iv Chest pain in a young person is unlikely to be coronary artery disease but you should ask about associated dizziness, syncope, and a family history of sudden death as well as screen for risk factors. Women present more often with atypical angina. They are more likely than men to have pain at rest, during sleep or precipitated by stress. References i. Talley NJ, O’Connor S. Clinical Examination, 7th edition. Sydney: Churchill Livingstone, 2014. ii. Polmear A. Evidence-Based Diagnosis in Primary Care. Edinburgh: Butterworth Heinemann, 2008. iii. Buntinx F, Knockaert D, Bruyninckx R et al. Chest pain in general practice or in the hospital emergency department: is it the same? Fam Pract (2001) 18:586–589. iv. Chun AA, McGee S. Bedside diagnosis of coronary artery disease: a systematic disease. Am J Med (2004) 117:334–343. Adapted from Frain J. Module 3 Clinical Skills Workbook: Cardiovascular. Graduate Entry Medicine, University of Nottingham, UK.

9

Defining Symptoms Patients and clinicians do not always agree on how a symptom is experienced. Learners benefit from realising and exploring this at the outset. Misunderstandings can occur more readily when symptoms appear to overlap body systems and/or when there are multiple causes. An example is dizziness, where one classification lists 33 possible causes. Students, in particular, worry they, ‘Don’t know what questions to ask’. Providing knowledge and categorising the underlying causes of symptoms can enable students to explore and define the symptoms further: Patient:     ‘I feel dizzy.’ Student:    ‘What do you mean by that?’ Patient:     ‘I feel I’m going to pass out sometimes’ – pre-syncope ‘I feel as though I’m (or the room is) moving’ – vertigo ‘I feel as though I’m falling’ – disequilibrium (often neurological) ‘I feel light-headed all the time’ – possible anxiety or depression

Similarly, misunderstandings regarding ‘indigestion’ can be clarified by using the patient’s own definition as the starting point (see Box 2.5). In the case of upper alimentary symptoms, agreement between patient and clinician on the patient’s experience is critical to the diagnosis, including malignancy.

Associated Symptoms Many students are proficient at obtaining a detailed description of a presenting symptom but fail to explore associated symptoms. By associated symptoms, we mean those symptoms which, though subsidiary to the patient’s main concern (e.g., pain), are nonetheless present and may contribute to the patient’s overall concern about the diagnosis. The main associated symptoms for any presenting symptom most commonly arise from the same organ system as the presenting one (see Box 2.3). A patient presenting with a cough may be concerned about the inconvenience of the cough and the impact on their ability to function. However, the diagnosis is not apparent from this symptom alone. For example, diagnosing pneumonia requires enquiry about sputum, fever, breathlessness, chest pain, and haemoptysis. Having to present the history at the end of the interview and getting the student to list the problems and formulate a differential diagnosis forces students to use associated symptoms in their reasoning [5] (see Box 2.6). Interpreting Features in the History Studies of the history have assessed which features might be more diagnostic than others for a particular condition. One looked at what features of the history in chest pain are most helpful to clinicians in differentiating cardiac from non-cardiac causes [6]. The authors found no single element of the chest pain history was a powerful enough predictor of non-cardiac pain to allow a clinician to decide on history alone, but researchers have attempted to combine features in the history which can be of use in clinical practice (see Box 2.7). Pain that is stabbing, pleuritic, positional, or reproducible by palpation has likelihood ratios

10

ABC of Clinical Reasoning

Box 2.5  Patient perspectives on upper alimentary tract symptoms

Under CC BY 4.0. Humphrys E, Walter FM, Rubin G et al. (2020). Patient symptom experience prior to a diagnosis of oesophageal or gastric cancer: a multi-methods study. BJGP Open; 4 (1): bjgpopen20X101001. https://doi.org/10.3399/bjgpopen20X101001.

near zero, meaning the likelihood of this kind of pain being cardiac is very low. Conversely, chest pain that radiates to one or both shoulders or arms or is precipitated by exertion has higher likelihood ratios (LR  =  2.3 − 4.7) meaning this kind of pain is more likely to be cardiac. Likelihood ratios are discussed in more detail later.

Natural History and Context The natural history of a disease is the sequence of changes occurring within the body from the beginning of the illness until its resolution. The disease resolves to either complete restoration of health, to loss of function, which may also be progressive, or the patient’s death. Familiarity with the symptoms and signs of a



Evidence-based History and Examination

Box 2.6  The importance of associated symptoms is formulating a reasoned diagnosis ‘Mr. Jackson had a car crash yesterday when his car was hit from behind. The pain (presenting symptom) is worse today and he has been taking paracetamol and ibuprofen. A hot water bottle has helped to relieve his symptoms, but he has been unable to use his keyboard at work. I think he has whiplash.’ Exploring the mechanism of injury, the radiation of pain and the presence of sensorimotor deficit clarifies the diagnosis: ‘Mr. Jackson was hit from the left yesterday just after he had got into his car. Unfortunately, he had not put his seatbelt on. He was thrown against the door and then rebounded back again into his passenger. He felt a sudden pain on the right side of his neck. His pain is worse this morning. Mr. Jackson describes pain radiating to his right shoulder. His arm feels heavy and weak (associated symptom). He has tingling down his arm to his right thumb (associated symptom), in the C6 dermatome. I am concerned he has a hyperextension injury and right C6 nerve root irritation.’ Adapted from Bokshan SL, DePasse JM, Eltorai AE et al. (2016). An evidence-based approach to differentiating the cause of shoulder and cervical spine pain. Am J Med; 129(9): 913–8.

Box 2.7  Differentiating cardiac from non-cardiac chest pain by history • Low risk of cardiac pain: the 3 P’s – pain that is pleuritic or stabbing, positional, and reproduced by palpation • Probably low risk: pain not related to exertion that occurs in a small inframammary area of the chest wall • Probably high risk: pain described as pressure, similar to a previous heart attack/angina or accompanied by nausea, vomiting, and diaphoresis • High risk: pain that radiates to one or both shoulders or arms or is related to exertion When combined with the patient’s age, sex, and past medical history, it should be possible to accurately identify those at low risk of cardiac pain, although these are a minority of patients who present to hospital with chest pain. Other serious causes of the chest pain still need to be considered. Adapted from Swap CJ and Nagurney JT. (2005). Value and limitations of chest pain history in the evaluation of patients with suspected acute coronary syndromes. JAMA; 294 (20): 2623–9.

condition facilitates its diagnosis. The classical presentation of disease as described in textbooks is quite uncommon. Some symptoms and signs occur early in the disease while others occur later. For example, in early heart failure, the patient may become breathless only on severe exertion. Later, breathlessness occurs on more moderate exertion (e.g., climbing the stairs), while later the patient has a problem lying flat in bed (orthopnoea) and may be breathless at rest. If a learner interviewing a patient with breathlessness believes the symptoms of orthopnoea must be present for heart failure to be diagnosed, they will miss patients in the earlier stages of disease who may benefit from proven treatment. This combination

11

of the patient’s symptoms and the natural history of the condition is reflected in the New York Heart Association’s functional classification of heart failure (see Box 2.8). The correlation of each class with echocardiographic findings can be used to guide evidence-based therapy. This concept can be demonstrated to students with a simple class exercise of constructing the natural history of a peritonsillar abscess (quinsy). All will have experienced a viral sore throat, many will have experienced bacterial tonsillitis, and a few suffered a quinsy. Discussion of the symptoms from onset to recovery provides the group with appreciation of the natural history of a condition and its relevance to clinical reasoning (see Box 2.9). Similarly, students who are taught lists of features of various diseases are not as accurate at diagnosis compared with those taught probabilities of symptoms being present [7]. For example, nearly all adult patients with meningitis in the UK have headache and a fever at presentation, the next most common symptom is nausea and vomiting (70%), but only around half complain of photophobia and a stiff neck [8]. Severe, or late bacterial, meningitis, on the other hand, is associated with prototypal symptoms and signs as described in textbooks. Simply teaching prototypes and ‘medical triads’ is likely to encourage superficial pattern recognition that results in overconfidence, premature closure, and incorrect diagnosis. The patient’s context, including age and gender, should be emphasised when considering the clinical probability of a disease. Epidemiological studies provide learners with an evidence base to assist in their clinical reasoning. For example, the Interheart Study [9] found over 90% of all patients suffering their first myocardial infarctions had the risk factors of smoking, hypertension, diabetes, abdominal obesity, low physical activity, and poor diet. Box 2.10 shows the percentage pre-test probability for American Box 2.8  New York Heart Association Functional Classification of Heart Failure – correlation of symptoms, echocardiographic findings, and therapeutic options Class

I

II

III

IV

Symptoms None

On moderate activity

On mild activity

At rest and on minimal activity

Ejection fraction

40–50%

30–40%