233 51 10MB
English Pages [490] Year 1986
RHYTHM AND METRE
Studies in Greek and Latin Linguistics edited by H. Pinkster, University of Amsterdam C.M.J. Sicking, University of Leiden 1. A.M. Bolkestein, Problems in the Description of Modal Verbs: an
investigation of Latin 2. E. Vester, Instrument and Manner Expressions in Latin 3. M. van Raalte, Rhythm and Metre. Towards a Systematic Description of Greek Stichic Verse
MARLEIN VAN RAAL TE
RHYTHM AND
METRE Towards a Systematic Description of Greek Stichic Verse
VAN GORCUM 1986 Assen/Maastricht, The Netherlands Wolfeboro, New Hampshire 03894-2069, U.S.A.
©
1986 by Van Gorcum & Comp. B.V., P.O. Box 43, 9400 AA Assen, Th( Netherland.-. Van Gorcum & Comp. B.V., 27 South Main Street, Wolfeboro, New Hampshire 03894-2069, U.S.A.
No parts or this book may be reproduced in any form by print. photoprint. microfilm. or any other means without written permission by the publisher. The publication o( this book has been made possible through a grant from the Netherlands Organisation for the Advancement of Pure Research (Z.W.O.).
UIJnry al Concn- Catalopls-ln-PubllcaUon Data
Raalte, Marlein van, 1952Rythm and metre. (Studies in Greek and Latin linguistics: 3) Bibliography: p. Includes indexes. I. Greek language- Metrics and rhythmics. 2. Greek poetry- History and criticism. I. TitJe. II. Series. PA417.R3 1986 881'.01'09 86-19030
ISBN 90 232 2229 6 (pbk.)
Printed in The Netherlands by Van Gorcum, Assen
PREFACE
I
The research ory
study
for
Professor
to
he
Verslehre
orip.in has
will
1t
Handbuch
be
ar,reenent
about
involve«JPl;~
should
of
to
uu:
-----uv-uu-vu-41. Now this
the
The selection
dactylic
by
a
of a hexameter
symbols
representation
Accordingly,
diverge
also
which constitute
- thereby
below -:
by
same set
first
schematic
effect
blurred
the
clear
real-
attention.
In
of
things
Greek dactylic
3.2.2.
of a concrete
the
whether
both
syllables
an example
the
be represented
(largely)
u,
-,
represent
make
being
(e.g.
and the
JTPtaauc,'HQUVOV't1.,:b
b
special
order
of
-vu,-vu,-uu,-uu,-uu,-t,-V->t,-V->t,-V-U-r,Jl.
rhythmical
movement of the beginning 3.2.2.
trochaic
which would complete
groups)
as to produce
the
do
(-uu,-uu, is
a kind
as a whole,
movecient of
colon showing a falling,
movement. Now, these
two rhythmical
factors (internal structuring and the modulation of the movement of the verse) may apply both at the rhythmic and at the metric level. At the rhythmic level tion will be ascribed
caesura is realized to some particular
by word-end: caesura funcword-end in a particular
INTRODUCTION verse-position 'caesura'
19
near refers
the
to
a word-boundary
middle
that
with
of
the
boundary
caesura
verse;
between
function
to a concrete
trimeter,
therefore,
trimeter
will
show t110 caesurae,
indicate
the
positions
ln short,
in the
at
metric
namely
where word-end
metric
level
two verse-elements
is usually the
the found.
profile
those
where
(In contrast of the
iambic
two boundaries
which
with caesura
function
co1m10nly
appears.) word-, ric
c.q.
metric
present
boundary
or
sub-phrases
study which
31
cola.
caesura
will
seperates
be conceived
two rhythmic,
The following
c.q.
uy
trimeters
as the met-
serve
as
examplea:
d:U.11 I i:flc6E:~ YVV!',I(Sea. 7.30) t\v iu'I n~ f\ ~ If\ ~ ~I (Sem. 7.69).
OUt 6nw Now since
the
trimeter
iambic
aa
one non-structured
whole,
in
the
of this
metric
profile
that
word-boundary
near
the
middle
justified
to
belonging
at
(at
of the
seems to be too
long
it
to
of
to the metric
desirable
type
of verse.
least
verse
conceive
is
one of)
occurs them
profile
as
be perceived
include
a caeaura
In virtue
these
quite
to
fact
two verse-positions
commonly,
alternative
of the iambic
of the aeeaa
it
caesura
to be
positions
trimeter:
x-u-,x :-u:-,x-u-fl. Accordingly ceived turn is
• .. tric
as a
actually
the
the case As far
of
in three
at
may be argued
- the
would
seems
likely
however
anced
whole the
proportion
verse-elements account.
which
that really of
- which
in other
types
in
its
of verse
(as
verse). balance
with
of the
verse
colon
a large
preferred
seem to
not
trimeter
is
concerned,
seems to be rel-
least.
in trimeters
events
iambic
of composition
For instance,
is
the
phenomenon of metric
respects
mical
that
of
in asynartetic
the
of the form x-u-x may be con-
e.g.
as the rhythmical
recognition
evant
colon'
as a unit
may serve
3.2.2.1.
it
the group of elements
proportion
be disturbed in
these
or
constitute
7:5
in still the
of
at
cases
disturbed 5:7
number of resolutions the the
5: 7 or rhythmic of
applies metric
7: 5 rhyth-
impression
virtue
cola
the is
level;
it
of a balcircumatance
the
if
-
number taken
of
into
RHYTHMAND METRE
20
As regards
the
distinguishing obvious.
between
The iambic
falling
second
the
of
amples
cited
is
necessarily
begins
the
a long
as
profile, does
a rising of
not
the
rhythmic
of
here,
the
the
rhythmic
realization
extent,
without
uy
diverge
thereby
losing
the
anceps
by
of
in the
colon
-
the
case
of
which
is
of a falling
uuu-...
second
degree,
not-
• So, obviously,
the
its
of
perception
second
from
ex-
actually
virtue
to apply
of
subthis
verses
to an objectional
realization
the
initial
the
a to
Semonides
in
perception
with
realization
of the
af feet
seem to be disturbed
not
withstanding
-:
the
Apparently
The same seems element
over
of these
realization
more
correlative
rhythmic
neither
easentially
first
frequent
the
syllable.
sequence.
the
from
appears
of
even verse
of couree,
as
profit
is
a rising
events
of
the
colon
is
is,
since the
verse
rhythmical
level
case,
metric
does
some
the
non-marked
comparatively colon
the
the
rhythmic
situation
However, at
of
example,
of
a
resolution
the
verse.
syllable
and for
This
above,
with
underlying
metric
distribution
phrases not
movement
trimeter,
colon.
preferred
verse
rhythmical
metric
profile
character
of
a
to
iambic
trimeter. Finally, phenomenon
the of
notion
caesura
of
metric in
media
explained
in
terms
of
compared
with
the
metric
colon
the
an overlap
serves
elucidate
trimeter,
iambic
of the
to
actual
which
(rhythmic)
cola
of
the
trimeter:
see
from
but
complementary
the can
be
colon
as
Ch. III,
3.2.1.
below. 3.2.3.
Clausula.
An effect produced
different
by the
caesura
tioning
of
indices
of verse-end
optional the
word-end.
device
incidence
somewhat
before
necessity
for
indicating
for of the its
verse-end
is
the
movement produced
Clausular
movement
announcing word-end end the
verse-end, in
of the
occurrence
is
to sub 3.2.1.2.
referred
to
in
a
structuring
by clausular
one
of
the
above.As
posi-
rhythmical
such it
accomplished
rhythmically
verse.
the
preferred
position
no rhythmical
there
is
view of the
other
possibilities
which
constitutes
an
by means of
Since
boundary
is
the
clausula
of
INTRODUCTION does
not,
type
of verse
21
unlike
the
with
rhythmical Often to
its
of the
will
of
to
the
metric
profile
word-boundary(-ies) device
cola
in
movement
of the
related
of
such
(Le.
the
will
in some manner
type
of
verse
movement
to the
in question.
a way as to make the
the
be in ac-
verse
return
characteristic
of
its
profile).
The most obvious in
the
be chosen
initial
metric
clausular
some rhythmical
movement
it
belong
concerned.
The position cordance
caesura,
the
close
tragic in
a
internal
example
rising
as
returns
at
the recurrence
which
manner.
structuring
movement,
40
trimeter,
is
internally
narks
The effect
produced
is
by the
verse-end
to
of word-end the
that
the
caesura
the
at pos.10
verse
near
trimeter,
produces
initial
its
whose
a falling
('basic')
rising
movement.
A similar dihaeresis' with
in
its
the
rising
movement the
device second
which
(by its
very
rhythmical
nature)
the
3.4.
P• 348 below.)
returns
different
of
recurrent the
in
pos.
that,
(in
type
of
virtue
trochaic
return
to
to
the
contrast
both
verse
falling
B. This in
of word-
the
'Bucolic
characteristic
boundary,
clausula
so-called
falling
at
implies
metron
situation
This
word-end
movement
the
to its
show a coincidence
verse-beginning
of this
particular
and metron-boundary. tetrameter
see
Ch. IV,
Bridge.
The distinction pertains
to
'bridge'
between
the
or
speaking) ticular
base
hexameter.
and the
option)
the
colon
overlaps
(For
3.2.4.
of
rhythmical
caesura
at
dactylic
by virtue
initial
lies
interpretation
'zeugma'.
This
significantly low
in
file.
Such
tions
may be
accounted
will
be argued
below,
upon
purely
metric
of
is
the
incidence for the
factors
verses
and a metric the
of
of
word-end
in
two
phenomenon (concerning
that
word-end
one and
of
at
the
of bridge
also
phenomenon
of
a (statistically occurs
at
a par-
same metric
particular
essentially the
verse-level
rhythmical
phenomenon
low incidence
verse-position a
a rhythmic
distinct
verse-posiways.
may be based
identification
pro-
As
either
of rhyth-
22
RHYTHMAND METRE
mic events ric
as being
ele•nt: the
modulation
In
study,
i.e.
under
certain
both
realized
above).
It
This
applies
ing metric
iambic
resolution
that
("split of the
- which, with
may occur
in
two such
resolution")
is
of
other
the
metric
is
the
short
3.1.1.
syllables
generally
avoided.
of the
usually
underly-
marked as to
initial
anceps
profile
see
value
this
tetrameter,
('resolution':
1a an
the
of the
comedy,
as
in
trochaic
between
exception
well
discussed
the
quantitative
(which
as
41
element
outside
the
trimeter
verse
metfactors
rb9tblli.cal
verse
syllables
observed
irrespective element
the
and in
a single
of word-end
(long),
the
trimeter
two short
been
of
single-short
iambic
by
has
incidence
of
underlying
generally
movement (3.2.4.2).
circumstances
may be
more
structuring
types
the
of a particular
upon
rhythmical
in
the
or
internal
of its
3.2.4.1.
in
realization
3.2.4.1)
concerning
quantity
the
verse-position
element).
In
comedy,
non""'lllarked verse-positions
as
well.
seems very
Now it two
such
syllables
was
as
the
syllables
being
may be supposed different
that
linguistic
tity
as being
very
irrelevance
metric
likely felt
in this
endanger
realization if
these
which
element.
resolution
belonged
same metric
(+/-
and markedness
particular
metric
seem to lose
of one and the the
of
events
would
occurs
explanation
for
between
perception
of a single
they
position
of word-end
the
two rhythmic
entities, of
the occurrence
to
a realization
element
corroborating
that
It
to two
their
iden-
element.
quantity)
can
these
The
of
the
be conceived
as
the avoidance
of split
resolution. The phenomenon is concerns
the
element,
i.e.
metric
level
phenomenon enon
of
called
preservation of of
of
its
character
the
verse.
regards
bridge
the
the
bridge
identity
of
as a single (Therefore
because
a particular
rhythmical does
it
not
event imply
level
of the
verse
only:
the
avoidance
of
word-end
in
- being -
of
course
its
is
a metric
appears one,
at
as here,
the
motivation
its
metric
verse-position motivation
a metric
rhythmic
or concerns
the
verseat that
the the
phenom-
a particular
level,
whether
the rhythmical
INTRODUCTION
23
of the
aoveMnt
verse.
discussed
below.)
3.2.4.2.
In other
at
a certain
of
fact,
neither nor
general
as is
generally lack
of
a priori
- the
syllable
infrequently•
caesura
at
syllable
are pos.
hardly 5.
explanation one
word-
that
(long)
syllable
rhythmical
a
of
the
exposed
to this
undesirable
effect.)
is
avoided
istics
we must
throw
le88 for a
internal
an explana-
indeed
and to the tenth
realized
realizations
in
the
metron
about
the
either is
question
upon the
realized
see
dactylic
would
at
coincidence
effect:
the
of the
The most likely
which
verse-final
sixth
a long final
occurs
12). of
the
in virtue
hexameter
(pos.
by a long
of
preferably
first
to be avoided
has been realized
verse-position
more light
the
not
of -(UV)
hexameter, be likewise
why this
rhythmical
ef feet
character-
of the verse. The metrical
( the by
be more precise
of
as
by word-end
verse-end
(Naturally
type
fourth
dactylic
a pendant,
that
preference
this
by virtue
above. to
is
verse-end
produces,
index
to the
in the
concerned.
likewise
appears
if
followed
3.2.1.2
In order
word-end
verse-position
and metron-boundary,
the
split
to.
long
a non-marked
is
to be
with
indicate
some likelihood
while
2) or at
case
authors
special
verse-element
In short:
(pos. is
have
referred
ever
or bridge
a
of which
in a non-marked
verse-beginning
for
hexameter,
latter
the
Therefore
The same applies
verse-element: verse-element
to
of
As a matter
to appears
seems to
kind
and
eighth
syllable.
genres
nature.
bridges
non-marked
as
this
movement
'optional'
all
universality
for
after
by
in
bridges
of word-end
explained.
same strictneH
as
very
so easily
the
In the dactylic
by a long
low incidence
with
3.2.4.2.1. the
not
'rhythmical'
referred
seeu the
the
verse-positions
rhythmical for
in
the
and of a more subtle
structuring tion
is
responsible
special
case
at
comparative
motivation
the
a significantly
verse-position
resolution This
is
cases.
word-end
avoided
as
dactylic an
internal
groups metra)
which
have
structuring
been into
constitute integrated
the
rhythm
into
two non-identical
of
the
a rhythmical component
verse whole parts
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
24 (rhythmical of
sub-phrases);
one or
caesura
two syllables,
with
ond metron ing
the
and the
indicated
by
the
the
the
end
word-end
of
of
a word-end
at
flow of
verse
metron
is
gives
rise
position
which
echoes
the
movement
within
this
type
of verse,
if
short
the
preceding
syllables
beginning pos.8,
of
the
where
the
favoured
as
occurrence
of
short
realization
there
is
the verse
a
the
to
verse:
metron-boundary desired)
verse-end
is only
which
by a bisyllabic
of
word-
kind
(see
is
within
3.2 •.4.2.2.
In
the
rhythmical
bridge,
can
verse,
the
on the condition
that
which
trimeter is
somewhat
and which,
the
at
case
the
the
there
of
is avoided. as
being
internal
mo-
struc-
of word- and dihaeresis,
rhythmical is
so that
beginning
Bucolic
coincidence
the
by a double-
coincidence
the
even
and where
effect
the
the at
is
verse-element,
of the preceding
iambic
movement
by two
preceded
at
of
realized
above),
regarding
pre-
incidence
be understood
(or,
realization
-
illustrated
movement of
by this
of
- unobjectionable
clearly
eighth
unobjectionable produced
element
characteristic
never
is
3.2.3.
factors the
to
verse
been
invariably
bridge
likely
and metron-boundary
rhythmical
of
is
rhythmical
at the same time a verse-final this
of the
the
least
most
non-marked
the
since
has
say,
is
almost
is
the
mid-verse
syllable is
the
in
metric
of the
element
This
as
which
falling
beginning
effect
by two interdependent
turing even
41
a
to
interpreted
long
more so,
- to say
coincidence
return
In short, tivated
is
verse).
of
while
of the
needless
word-end
effect
produces
clausular
a
the
non-marked
final
one
is
last•
boundary
the
by
to the
non-marked
(which,
if
a fall-
verse-end
Consequently
a metrical
realized
hexameter;
this
only
the
be
the sec-
into
above,
the
to
extent by the
(i.e.
structured of
element.
the
constituted
Aa was said
syllable,
to
groups
is
realization
single
at
phrase
verse
double-short
the
'overlap',
two metrical
a
it
an
rhythmic
successively.
one
onward
because
of
the
invariable
double-short
cisely of
of
occurrence
impair
one),
colon by
realization
the
between
third
the
verse-polition
of
boundary
and a rising
the
by means
likeness
not
to
intensified
metron. is
another
more complicated
example in virtue
of
a of
25
INTRODUCTION the
more complex
manner of metrical
grouping
operative
in this
type
of verse. 'Porson'e end
Law' refers
is
highly
exceptional
ization
of
eight,
such
'false
caesura'.
the
the
ninth
after
(an
of word-end analysis
of
which
objectionable
a long
the
is
the
ele.mentu.m ancep.s).
an explanation
after
comedy, word-
rhythmical
At first
as creating
does
syllable
real-
not
a
account
exclusively.
for Here
characteristics
of
the
of
one
seems to be desired.
verse In
the
iambic
marked
and
one non-marked
metrical
v-.v-.u
first
of
of
long
the
namely
by groups
ternal
structuring
coincide the plies trasts the
recurrent so
the
boundary
( 1)
with verse
or
the
the
consequence
to
the
which
overlap
within
the
movement.
As
has
fect
is
achieved
the
bridge, clausular
movement it
the
will effect
said
of be since
the
of
there
is
second (2)
of
the
alternation
word-end
It
Now, at
no coincidence
not
(whether
of
- which
im-
is
of
some
effects:
metrical
groups
of
the
rhythmical
where
the
successive
movement,
near
does
of
concurrent
rhythmical
or
a partition
are
verse
fifth
con-
these
in
is
colon
avoided.
a return,
that
the
been
verse-beginning.
clear
of
integration an
nature,
events
caesura
(metron))
oc-
The in-
the
groups
one and
has
before,
of
type
first that
implies
after
two metrical
movement the
grouped the
dis-
element
a more complex
cases,
both
in the
x--v--, x--v--, etc.
'complex'
parts
by means
a free
an anceps
of
each:
in
the
show an alternation
there
of word-end
effects
cola
since
is
the
('feet')
However,
incidence
more,
been
(i.e., fact
event,
two elements
of metrically
once
simply
of
sequence
rhythmical
note
verse
in
between
two equal
rhythmical
verse-positions is
movement of
into
alternation
P• 12 above).
that,
of
the
groups
elements
of this
(foot)
that
in
syllables
grouping four
of
single-short)
2.3,
short
element,
with
'basic'
(i.e.
(see
of
by the
virtue
basically
and
metrical
seventh
in
two non-marked
curs),
produced
is etc.
every
tribution
the
trimeter,
grouping
each:
to
such
outside
syllable
may be considered
However,
further
that,
a long
verse-element
word-end
avoidance
again,
to the circumstance
the
clausular
end of the
concerning pos.9
verse,
Porson 's
frustrates
of word-
ef-
this
and metron-
26
RHYTHMAND METRE
boundary
and
syllabic
thus
quantity
answer
in
iambic
verse.
As
the
indicated
dactylic
ing
two ('basic')
element verse,
the
more
is
often
comedy)
becomes
apparent
mical
index
mical less the
realized
which to
objectionable anceps
if
element
confirmed
In
by the
in
nature
this
use
of at
other
single-short
it
as
hand the
rhythm rhyth-
the
preferred to be
realization rhythm
basic
situation
exceptions
a rhyth-
be felt
rhythmic
an-
again,
is,
the
two
the
may be argued,
movement might with
first
another
with
of
by means
hand,
basic
conceived
actual
in
type
the
(that
conjectural
of the
e.g.
as cover-
syllable
the
accordance
fact,
that
this
the
accordance
other
which
of
on the
verse-initial
on the is
sequence.
rather,
the
in
of
and a marked
Consequently, not
events
than
occurring
may be
its
grouping
can be conceived
by a short
is
find
has been accomplished
of which
why
of a non-marked
metron,
(which
metrical
rhythmical
metron
syllables
third
question
must
more complex
elements
of verse-end). return
iambic
the
again
is
of
history
- by virtue
movement
clausular
the
of long
in
outside
grouping iambic
by four
proportion
of the
sub-groups
Throughout
positions;
ceps
the
The
circumstances
nature
trimeter
metrical
grouping
a large
anceps
the
iambic
movement.
these
complex
hexameter:
each.
rising
in
above,
the
the
to
matters
specific,
characterizes
of
no return
of of to
seems
to
this
the
'law',
be or
43
tendency.
4. Conclusion. From these cussed
in greater
of concrete to
prove
concept provides
similar detail
verses
us
rhythmical observed
the
the
strictnesa between
the
stichic
and verse.
to
type of verse
periods, occurs.
course
that
the
profile
be disconception is
interpretation
the
preferences genres,
likely of
the
same time,
the
norm (see
make explicit
in rhythmical several
of
At the
as a rhythmical
means (and
will
of a metric
description
profile
with
which a particular
realization
of Greek
a metric
(which
we may conclude
below)
to
properties of
examples
as the
beneficial
rhythmical
be
and
.sub 2 above)
differences
in
in general)
to
and
contexts
in
INTRODUCTION
27
AJJ appears
be
met with
atively
in
the
simple,
its
internal
the
most
shall
namely
corpus
since
attention the
about
Greek
poetry,
the
are of
quite
two
rhythmical
verse
identity
(rhythmic
as a rhythmical
coherence
and the movement of a rhythmical
three that
typea is,
of
Greek
simple
how each statement proposed of ric
same time, above
rhythmical level
relates
of
of
conceptual
to
benefit
(sc.
if (I)
organization, the
verse)
transparency
our understanding
of with
the
between and proves will
is of
grouping
special
attention
( II) to
of
metric
the
be-
profile, of the
description
to
and
a rhythm1c
the
question
distinction other
kinds
and a met-
a certain
amount
which may turn
of verse
the
involved.
double
be convincing,
(from
-
more complic-
to
the
of
manner
subjects
grouping
between
types
contrast
verse-levels
have been gained
of other
particular,
systematic
metrical
metrical
we
sequence.
ordering
application
in
interrelatedness
study
to the different
and
as examples,
and its
the
rel-
is
Now, taking
potential
in a fairly
a particular
phenomena),
(2)
present verse
phenomenon
rhythmical At the
the
stichic
by following
relatively ated
aim of
the
to
verse-elements
verse
sequence)
conceived
The principal
verse
principles
of
and
its
of verse
recognized.
stichic
relevance
types
stichic
of
rhythmical
norm,
other
easily
metrical
to
concrete
remarks
of
both
co111111on types
( 1) the
above
structuring
pay
tween
from the
as well.
out
CHAPTER II: THE DACTYLIC HEXAMETER
Editions
28
Used
Homer Iliu
edd. D.B. MONRO & T.W. ALLEN,Oxford
Odyssea
ed. T.W. ALLEN, Oxford
2
3
1920 (OCT)
1917/9 (OCT)
Homeric HIJlltllS*ed. T.W. ALLEN, Oxford 1911 {OCT)
Hesiod 'l'heogonia ed. M.L. WEST, Oxford 1966 Opera et Dies ed. H.L. WEST, Oxford 1978
Parmenides edd. H. DIELS & W. KRANZ,Berlin
Empedocles ibid. Archestratus
'1951 (Vol.I,
228ff.)
308ff.
Hedyphagetica
ed. P. BRANDT,Leipzig
1888
Aratus Phaenomena ed. J. HARTIN, Firenze 1956 Apollonius Rhodius edd. F. VIAN& E. DELAGE,Paris 1974-8l(Bud~) Callimachus ed. R. PFEIFFER, Vol. II. Oxford 1953 Theocritus ed. A.S.F. GOW,Vol.I. Cambridge 2 1952 Nonnus Dionysiaca
ed. F. VIAN, Paris
1976 (Bud~)
* It may be noted that the poems known to us as the 'Homeric Hymns' have been treated in this study - with some occasional exceptions as if they formed a homogeneous corpus. This, intrinsically questionable, approach has been adopted because (1) the most comprehensive quantifications of the material which are available fail to distinguish between the different Hymns, and (2) the data of those authors who distinguish between individual Hymns are often unreliable (see e.g. N.B. ad Table VII, pp. 80/1 below). In recent years, monographs have appeared which throw some light upon (esp. linguistic and/or 'formulaic') characteristics of the hexameters of individual Hymns (c.q. parts of them: cp. the bipartition of the HIJlltllto Apollo argued for by, e.g., JANKO(1982) 99f.), but an adequate survey of rhythmical characteristics of the Hymns considered as individual poems is still wanting. (The study by TEBBEN (1971), whose quantifications indicate "a noticeable departure from the consistency of the corpus both metrically and lexically" in the Hymn to Ares (see DA XXXII(1972) 6400A) is not available to me.)
THE DACTYLIC HEXAMETER hexameter
1. The dactylic
by means of
tending ingly
The sixth,
syllables
prolongation
Ch.I,
Therefore
the metric
above). profile
of
close.
invariably
is
realized
be represented
index
ex-
Accord-
two elements
as a rhythmical
,.,.
•Y
sequence,
a pendant
by sets
metron
be considered
3.2.1.2.
double-short
and with
grouping
verse-final
- which uy
end (see
a falling
is
1s a metrical
there
( -uu ).
29
each by two
of verse-
as follows:
-vu, -vu, -vu, -vu, -vu, -IIThe interpretation of the dactylic hexameter as a catalectic verse ( ••• -u ..l\) - see, e.g., SNELL ( 4 1982) 13 - seems to be incorrect. i.a., by the modulation of the rhythmical movement This is suggested, of the verse, whose second colon is a rising one in contrast to the beginning of the verse (see 3.2. below), a phenomenon which seems characteristic of acatelectic types of verse especially: compare the situation in the iambic trimeter (Ch.111, 3.1.2.; 3.1.3.2) and contrast the situation in the trochaic tetrameter (Ch. IV .3.1); the same holds for the type of clausular rhythm involved (i .c. the bucolic dihaeresis: 3.3. below), where there is a return to the rhythmical movement of the beginning of the verse - as is, again, the case in the (acatalectic) iambic trimeter but not in the (catalectic) trochaic tetrameter: see Ch.lV.3.4. below. Furthermore it may be noted that the acatalectic interpretation of the dactylic hexameter plays a role in the explanation of its rhythmical bridges, which are not easy to account for on a catalectic interpretation: see 4.2. below. 2. The possibility long
syllable
file:
this
visible
of
realizing
produces
a
rhythmic
the
non-marked
rhythmical
divergence
variation
(seeCh.
1.3.1.
verse-elements
by a
of
pro-
the
metric
above)
can
the
dactylic
be made
schema:
in the following
~-11. The frequency
of
bisyllabic
( 'spondees')
varies
such
tendencies,
of
general spondees
genres
can
be
with
the
realizations several
differences seen
to
and between the individual
exist
of
verse-positions; in
frequency
between
the
metra
apart and
from
localization
several
literary
poets.
The irresolvability of the longa in the dactylic hexameter does not require an explanation, as follows from the very nature of resolution aa a rhythmic divergence (See Ch.V, 2.2.1. below). Speculations about the absence of resolution in these elements appear to be
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
30
inapired by a paHage in Dionysius of HalicarnaHus, CV XVII (• UR likely, however, that Dionysiua merely refers to 71.lOff.). It seeu hexameters of a special rhythmical a particular manner of reciting shape (i.e., 'holodactylic' verses), in which the length (or rather, occupying the marked verse-elements 18 the tempo) of the syllables 41 shorter (faster?) than a "full syllable". The main difficulty arises from his equation of the phen011enon - an equawith the denotation of '1-\ayoc; as used by the rhythmiciane Aa Ariatoxenus BR II.20 tion which, however, aeema to be erroneous. (W.) show■, in the theory of the rhytbmician■ (or at least of this 4M,voc; apparently refers to an irrational particular rhythmician) verse-elements, not between sylproportion which exi ■ t• between value of the lables ( - which 6.M,yCa. appears to arise from a specific ■eema to be to nan-,urked element, too); and finally, the reference there is no evidence that eilyric ver1e e ■ pecially. Consequently, ther Dionyaiua or the rhythmiciane to which he refers ascribed an hexaaeter; exceptional value to the elementa loaga of the dactylic so that there are no historical reasons to call in question the of Greek binary opposition of syllabic quantity •• conatituti'Vlt verse rhythm either. The literature on the subject is vast as well as complicated, as is eaaily understood in view of the extreme complexity of the testiaoniea. 3 See KOSTER ( 1962) 82-5, who gives a survey of the discussion on the subject; also HAENDEL (1963) 76 n. 21; 342-4; ROSSI (1963); IRIGOIN(1965); KORZENIEWSKI(1968) 40-4. argue that the differences DEVINE & STEPHENS (1976) convincingly between the phonological shape of long syllables in marked- and nonnaturally marked verse-positions as observed by IRIGOIN 11 (result) and predictably from the interaction of the metrical structure of the hexameter with one type of Greek ('l'hucydidean Greek)." (163); cp. also (1984) 23. Consequently, these durational differences are not to be considered as having independent rhythmical significance. 2.1.
In practically
or 3.6 dactyls In Callimachua, the
average
meters,
41
all
authors
studied,
per verae (see Table Apollonius
number of
the average
and
dactyls
there
is
an average
of 3.7
I below).
the epic
hexameters
1a somewhat higher;
is as high as 4.25 dactyls
of
Theocritus
in Nonnus'
41
hexa-
per verse.
The large amount of dactylic realizations in Nonnus' verse 1a only one aspect of a general tendency towards rhythmical uniformity it 1a produced e.g. by the use of archaic, (see 2.4.3.1.2. below); sugnon-contracted forms: see KEYDELL(1959) 431\ff •• Thie situation gests that a large proportion of dactyls is obtained only with some effort; cp. the comparatively high incidence of epondees in verse with a non-poetical subject matter: see below. The (only relatively) high percentage of dactyls in Callimachus, Apolloniue and the epic hexameters of 'nleocritue reflects a comparatively large proportion
THEDACTYLIC HEXAMETER
31
of verses with one spondaic realization and a s11111llproportion of verses with three spondees rather than a particularly large proportion of verses without a apondee: see 2.2. with Table II, p.36 below. The hexameters ably
of Archestratua,
low average
on the other
number of dactyls
(3.06
It is true that the terminology of ably be expected to adapt itself to very smooth manner. Nevertheless, the percentage of apondees appears to be large number of correptions.
hand,
show a remark-
per verse).
a Hedypll4getica cannot reasonany rhythmical subtleties in a occurrence of an even higher avoided by means of a notably
All a matter of fact, in poetry dealing with 'technical• (or, 'cognitive') subjects it is obviously more difficult to keep the number of spondees within certain rhythmical limits 41 , as may be increase of verses with two sponconcluded e.g. from the remarkable dees in Parmenides. In the light of this, Empedocles' conformity to the rhythmical practice of the Homeric hexameter is remarkable indeed (see e.g. 2.4.3.2.2. below).
• In all
tables
a precision
below,
the
percentages
of two decimal
and ratio's
are
expressed
to
places.
e N.B. The quantifications dactylic the
and
several
presented
spondaic
cles,
1899,
Archestratus
as
regards
tends
to
increase
adopted
here
(as
well
the
subject:
1900).
questions the
as being
the
and
distribution
their based
of
textual of
are
the
most
most generally
used
see JONES & GRAY(1972)
189f.).
over by
Eapedo-
from JAECKEL (1902).
criticism.
diphtongs.
of
upon studies
presuppose
of dactyls
proportion
Parmenides,
for
derived
phenomena
proportion they
the
primarily are
interpretation because
concerning
The figures
of rhythmical
tions
bisyllabic
are
and Theocritus
Quantifications
of
realizations
verse-positions
LA ROCHE (1898,
below
i.a. His
particular
LA ROCHE's work by his data
comprehensive in recent
op-
practice have
been
available
literature
on
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
32 Other quantifications
available
DROBISCH(1873)
(comprising
are:
a
comparison
of
Greek and
Latin
hexameters) LUDWICH 11(1885) 301-46 O'NEILL (1942) 159; also
CUNNINGHAM (1977)
(hexameters
istic
of the
'fragmentary
Hellen-
poets')
SCHAMP(1981) (Orphic hexameters).
TABLEI MEANFREQUENCY OF DACTYLS ANDSPONDEES dactyls per verse
spondees per verse•
ratio da: sp•
sample size
Hom.//. Od. Hes. Hom. Hymns
3.72 3.68 3.64 3.69•
1.28 1.32 1.36 l.25•
2.9 2.8 2.68 2.94
15693 12110 2331 2328
Parm. Emp.
3.6 3.75
1.4 1.25
2.57 2.99
148 405
Archestr.
3.06
1.94
1.57
307
Arat.
3.67 3.85 3.91 3.62 3.79
1.33 1.15 1.09 1.38 1.21
2.77 3.34 3.59 2.63 3.12
5836 936 876 l058
4.25
0.75
5.69
2774
Apoll. Call. Theocr. buc.
ep. Nonn.
* The
sixth metron, which is invariably has been excluded from these figures.
* The average number of dactyls/apondees Hymns does not total five as a result figures
include
some incomplete
realized
1154
by two syllables,
per verse in the Homeric of the fact that LA ROCHE's hexameters.
TilE DACTYLICHEXAMETER
33
2.2.
Of course,
the average number of
not
by itself
provide
to
be observed
verses.
It
thors the
the
appears
studied
relevant
in in
metra
infol'llllltion
about
distribution
of
however 70% of
are
3.7/3.6
that
the
in
verses
realized
dactyls
the
measure
dactyls
the
over
case
either
of
verse
(see
regularity
the
individual
two of
Table
does
of
virtually
one or
by a spondee
per
11).
all
au-
the
five
Only in
hexameters of Nonnus - who see11& to be the only one actually
keep
the
occurrence
of spondees
as low as possible
to
- the percentage
is somewhat lower. More specifically, roughly 40-50% of the hexameters have one metron with a spondaic realization (Archestratus excepted, who has 31.6% of verses with one spondee), while + 25-35% of the hexameters have two spondees. 41 Verses with two spondees are frequent especially in Parmenides (37 .84%: see 2.4.2) and in Archestratus (38. 76%) - in whose hexameters the total percentage of spondees is notably higher. Of Nonnus' hexameters, only 13.27% show two spondaic realizations; of the other authors with a high average number of dactyls per verse (see 2.1) only Callimachus has a rather low percentage of verses with two spondees (24.15%) - but a particularly high incidence of verses with one spondee (50%). In 1110st of the authors have
three
spondees
(none
Callimachus
and
Apollonius
verses
four
spondees
with
discussed,
As was to be expected spondees in his verse, percentage both of verses and 2.28% respectively.
at
all
between in
respectively), does not exceed
5 and 10% of the verses
Nonnus;
3.53% and
while
the
4.52% in
proportion
one percent.
proportion of in view of the large general Archestratus has an exceptionally high with three and with four spondees: 22.8%
E.g. (3 spondees): Archestr.
fp(nlJt'w.ra:nd.
IV.4 ~WCI.~~
1s A£UNbc ~c, V. l -3
bt., ~ .. rqn S&'Cm.ov t.oi:rA> ~ 001. .
TDVTOlQ(;
IX.8/9
of
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
34 (4 spondees):
X£L\OI, 'HPI.\NL"tTIV,ol 6' ctuo1. x6\16p1,vov~
IV.13
a0-t6c,6:,o\., tv 6' AC'Vf.l)>Gt ~ IJov'tlf)Ul'Vw 6.'v4DC£
XIX.2 --tCN~ Tliv1xalY y6yy()ltN ~TEL
XXI1.1
XXIII .15
IWX'hv 'KtxtllVmL 8vrrtav
XLV.6 6:U.' OUf. ~ y~ Only among the the metra
Homeric
~v
XXIII.221
Od.
xv. 334 oli:ou l«1l ~ 6'
XXII .19 2 In
most
five
of
the
bisyllabic
tv
CJEL~
metra
realization
are 10
some verses
in which all
:
6£1.M)LO
11:xt~
KL~
MP£WN ft>' oCvou a®l~x,..v
6UMo1.•.:v 6t TtA£K't'IW tf alrroO r;e; ~
~\IIJ
authors•
relevant
there
realization
Il.
XXI.15
to,v 't' 6rr6rw,,cto1.
'Ntx't1'1VltU 6.M)i.qt,\,
hexameters
have a bisyllabic
£
TtE+>
~'tTIV
17-20% of
the
( 'holodactylic' of the final
verses
have
verses metron
only
dactyls
in
the
inasmuch
as
the
is not optional).
lbe percentage is somewhat lower in Parmenides (14.19%) and in the bucolic poetry of lbeocritus (13.93%); they are especially frequent in Nonnus (38.5%) and, to a leHer degree, in Callimachus (:!: 22%) as well. and Apollonius For example: Hom. Il.
XIII.2
't0US;~ fa. TQn. $1.
M' &>' 6 111aa6'tl)CQ0'
15
~TM
y'
tE
~
n6vav
't
1
ft£-r' t~,
txtu£v 'HO.l6•.:c;uv ~P£6'
~)O:l.1.0UC
te ~ )(O.TI$ficEwm1.T'CDA6f;vror;
n.cx,l nix:,e1.~·
't~
6'
~ ~
kQl Cw,
6ptEat' tc:.,.,,-rost 'tt't~ tX£'t'O~ 0d. XI 11.1 ~ 6saf)' , ot 6' &:xJ.rdvttt &dtv tytvavto o~ 8 6:n:>1. tvl IJEYdpot,01, YE+>OOOLOV a.Caoru o[vov 20 'HOl.m µtv ro wa"tt8rix' l£POVµtvc>r; 'A>.x1.v6o1.0 24 "t0to1. 61; ac,0\, ttt::,e:uo•te:pbv µtvc>r; •MK1.v6o1.o Apoll. 111.1/2 £t 6' dy£ vOv, 'F.,p::r:tw, mc:xi&' tcmxx>kQl µ01, fv1.on£ 20 -rptc ~
~ ~
63-5
tc ·~
~.. , 6:::xx,v
6.vfryay£x&xJt; ·1~ twto1.v tvl a6tvot fTt.AE"tO yulo1.c,
6AJ(l.1.di tyy£.\aaollEAlnt HCDCbv o[mv ~, ~ i,.' ~ 6utwv 6.ytixx,rov far,xe.
nlE DACTYLIC HEXAMETER ea11. II.4/5
35
o6x.~;
~
b Al\A.LCC~ n qx>tvLE
tEi:Jnlvrte, b 61::~ l:v fiti>L w:i).bl} 6£l6£L • 1OI 1 0s; w.v C&;),1Jfyac; ou-tOG, 0s; aM. C6£, AL-roe txd\,q;.
~·
'
~
•E>at+IYE, >at ~·
62/3 6££u:no l,E\I
tK ~, Nonn.
I. l 18
K~I.V
x~
oOnat& AL,:ol. to&8ALO.,Ttf'IE£ E£ ~ 61::ntpLf ~,:o wlxour;•
dnt, 8Exi, KQov!oao ot.dx"tqXN aC8onocEOvrk; , (a')LKU
~ llia'CCEW q:O.\a I"LYQVlXAJV
28 6l,L y6vou 'tPLtd:toLo KuBr\Al6a.1.111-cti:a. Bdxxou
32
~
35 vq3p£6a. 41
•bcllpto1.o, TtOLKLAOWtOV
rq:n a.DSL ~ tafil,.OVC)C ~l XL~
~
eotec,vtlJ6v· ~ Y6i:> &.a!VEm1.
46 t1.6avlns; 1TUtt ~ bt'
~
fi,atvoav
W,l~ etc.
tix.w
ZE'bc etc.
36
RHYTIIM AND METRE
TABLE II
THE NUMBER OF SPONDEES PER VERSE* 0 sp.
1 sp.
2 spp.
19.19% 18.62% 17.29% 19.97%
42.57% 40.61% 40.58% 41.71%
29.7 % 31.87% 32 % 28.48%
7.95% 8.35% 9.22% 8.03%
14.19% 17.53%
39.86% 47.65%
37.84% 26.67%
8.11% 8.15%
4.56%
31.6 %
38.76%
Arat. Apoll. Call. Theocr. buc. ep.
18.02% 21.98% 22.33% 13.93% 19.19%
40.29% 45.65% 50 % 43.95% 46.31%
33.1 % 27.72% 24.15% 33.1 % 29.02%
Noon.
38.5 %
48.23%
13.27%
Hom.//. Od.
Hes. Hom. Hymns
Parm. Emp.
Archestr.
of verses
*•occurrence percentage
2. 3.
of the sum total
Some of
the
spondees
occur second ency
in
beginning shows
put
the
metron that
together
beginning - such
of the
machua occur
(the the
from
0.23% 0.38%
spondees
verse,
the
percentage
(See
verse-final
expressed
metric of
in Nonnua; metron).
51
show a
Table
metron
i.e.
with
sequence.
in the second
0.01/ 0.03%
hexameter
others.
in accordance
spondees
0.58% 0.51% 0.9% 0.64%
0.52% 0.12%
4.52% 3.53% 8.79% 5.1%
dactylic
than
a rhythmical
highest
5 spp.
2.28%
8.06%
O, 1, 2, etc.
the
of
divergences
the
52.16% of
of
metra
22.8%
4 spp.
in a
of verses.
more frequently
realization all
with
3 spp.
in
first
the general
or occur
metron
realizations
44.47% of the
spondees
Of
50-70%
rhythmical
norm preferably
spondaic
below.)
excepted),
the
The second
Ill
spondaic
in
the
tendat
the
invariably (as
many as in Calli-
TilE DACTYLICHEXAMETER
37
Theocritus' hexameters have a comparatively high percentage of spondaic realizations of the first metron, both in his bucolic and in his epic poetry. In Nonnus, there is a relatively low occurrence of apondees in the first metron, more than half of the - comparatively small - total number of spondees being found in the second metron. Parmenides comparatively favours a dactylic realization of the first metron: see 2.4.3.2.2. below. In than
all
the
20% of
lenistic
authors
the
spondees
hexameters
a smaller
Table
meters
of
ron
the
in
quency
"bucolic
riore
strongly
Table
VIII, In
third
hexameters
of
the in
Nonnus,
of
this
see 3.2.3.
earlier
Hel-
in the
is
hexameter
the
(under
epic
hexa-
fourth
met-
with
following
syllable
rhythmical
Empedocles, the
the the
avoided
poetry
frefourth
somewhat
(see
3.3.
and
and
apondees
Callimachus is the
occur
and
somewhat frequency
in
in the
lower.
the epic
(For
the
of a 'trochaic'
74 below.) where
over
the
first
is
not
easily
above),
the
percentage
phenomenon
seep.
subtlety 2.1.
12-19% of
verse,
spondees
(cp.
long
In
the
connected
word-end
more
position
of
of spondees
poets,
& Theocritua)
in this
exception
i.e.,
authors,
In Archestratus'
ial
in
of Theocritus
interrelation
of
occur
obviously
is
metron.
Callimachus,
notable
a preceding
than
Hellenistic
p. 86 below).
metron;
tion
fourth
the
decrease
poets
case
even
most
caesura,
the
dihaeresis",
in which
in
spondees
This
Hellenistic
the
Apollonius,
the
with
Theocritus.
of
metron,
of
III),
except
occur
(Aratus,
percentage
18%: see
studied
22.45%
the
exceptionally
four
metra
the
in this
spondees
distribu-
to indicate
seems
achieved
of
even
that
technical
occur
in
materthe
third
metron. The fifth
metron
tions:
the
verse')
is
to have
avoided
meters high
phenomenon especially
occurrence
of
characteristic
of 52
(called rare
this
of Theocritus
qualification.
shows the lowest
The highest
with
the
of spondaic
spond1acus'
poetry percentage
in
cannot is
'spondaic appears
and in the bucolic
common opinion
a spondee
or
realiza-
(who accordingly
phenomenon)
- so that Hellenistic
'versus
in Archestratus
particular verses
percentage
the
hexa-
that
a relatively
fifth
metron
be maintained
found in Aratus
is
a
without (10.83%).
53
38
RHYTHMAND METRE
Some examples of spondaic verses
from the Iliad
are:
(one spondee):
' tb.~
~lCj,N
(four
XXIV.49 'tN'IUN 281 "tWlJi;v
~
yap >t>t~1.
~
c~
tv
~'Co1,
8tcnv
~l.OLV
&:ux,1.v ~to1.
below).
THE DACTYLICHEXAMETER (four
consecutive IX.137
39
spondees):
vfp t'U.1.cXPUCC0 kal
xcwco0~
oo6' &t-£ 4>ol v1.M01;~ XVII.670 \IOv TLC tvndl'lC Ila;~
XIV. 321
In
the
great
tetrasyllabic. lable
6&1.~to
XXIII.65,
105).
lOff ••
majority 14
279)
'tTlA£)CA£ 1.1:o'Co
(cp. See LA ROCHE(1898)
(cp.
of
these
instance&
As LA ROCHE suggests,
words decreases
when there
the
the
verse-final
proportion
word 1s
of four-syl-
are more than two consecutive
spon-
dees. See e.g. (1898) 62: of the verse-type dssds(s) 83.33% ends with a four-ayllable word; in the verse-type sddss(s) the corresponding percentage is 43.33%. 1s of course easily accounted for by the rhythmical Thie situation tendency of realizing a 'bucolic' clauaula: see 4.2.3. below.
More specifically it may be noted that the preference for 1----,1 1--·=Ucannot be advanced to make it plausible that "there must be a prosodic difference between the initial syllable of the one of the other." (DEVINE & STEPHENS word shape and the medial syllable (1982) 41; (1984) 51). This phenomenon is adequately accounted for by the rhythmical preference for a clausular return to the falling double-short rhythm of the hexameter, on the underatandin~ that there 18 either a return to a ('basically') falling movement (1----,1) or a return to a double-short movement (luu-41) or I preferably, both {1-uu-~fl}. (See alao 3.3. and 4.2.3. below.) over
40
RHYTHM ANDMETRE TABLElll DISTRIBUTION OF SPONDEES OVERTHE SEVERAL METRA*
1st
2nd
3rd
4th
Hom. Hes. Hom. Hymns
29.79% 28.58% 28.27%
31.6 % 31.99% 30.74%
12 % 13.06% 14.19%
22.78% 21.18% 20.87%
3.82%• 5.18%• 5.93%
Parm. Emp.
21.16% 29.33%
32.37% 37.2 %
18.84% 6.89%
24.64% 21.06%
2.9 % 5.51%
Arcbestr.
24.29%
26.63%
22.45%
26.47%
0.71%
28.57%
15.33% 13.45% 7.74% 17.18% 8.11%
14.42% 15 % 17.73% 11.45% 20.5 %
10.83%• 7.57% 6.27%• 0.92% 5.61%
5th
Arat. Apoll. Call. Theocr. buc. t!p.
23.8 % 34.11% 32.66%
30.85% 37.71% 44.47% 36.35% 33.13%
Nonn.
20.36%
52.16%
5.02%
22.46%
-
average
27.26%
35.43%
12.85%
19.88%
4.6 %
* Percentage
* In
26.26%
of the spondees.
these cases, the total number of spondees on which these percentages are based is somewhat different from the figures presented in Table II. JAECKEL, who notices some of the irregularities apparent in LA ROCHE's figures (but disregards others), adds to the chaotic state of affairs by adding various miscounts (and/ or misprints) of his own.
THE DACTYLICHEXAMETER 2.4.
In virtue
dactylic
of the
hexameter
41
bisyllabic
nature
of the
final
may take any one of 32 different
metron,
shapes
the
('verse-
types'). From the sixties of the present century onwards the study of verse-types has derived fresh impulses from the rapid development of calculating systems and of statistical methods. Research on the subject - which chiefly concerns the Latin hexameter - serves, purposes. roughly speaking, two distinct In the first place, the production of programmes of automatic scansion is thought to be helpful for the editing and correction of according to the metrical texts (based upon a reduction of decisions number of syllables occurring in each verse). In the second place, the possibility of comparing rhythmical characteristics of different texts statistically and in a more or less objective manner may help to settle questions of authorship and chronology where other methods fail. Profit may be gained from the statistical coordination of rhythmical and (other) stylistic properties of the verse of the several authors, which may help to avoid impressionism. (Compare JONES & GRAY(1972) 188 & note 8.) In the present study no independent research of this nature has been undertaken. The following studies refer to automatic scansion and/or the relative frequency of verse-types of the Greek hexameter especially: DYER (1967); JONES (1968); JONES & GRAY (1972); JONES (1966); EVRARD( 1972). Some studies of more general interest may also be referred to: OTT (1973) - with which compare EVRARD-GILLES(1975) -; THRAEDE (1978), with bibliography; GROTJAHN(1979), id.; (1981); CANCIKe.a. (1979). As the interpretation dactylic
present of
hexaMeter,
in the first
study
is
concerned
thf" principal
with the recognition
rhythr.-ical
the most current
characteristics
verse-types
not ~ore than one of the Metra is realized the pl11ce of the spondees in verse-types the !'letra are realized
(2.4.3)
the
relative
the several • The figures
of the
be considered
place.
we will presently discuss Accordingly, (2.4.1) the place of the spondee in verse-types
(2.4.2)
will
and the
frequency authors
in which one anct by a spondee; in which two of
by a spondee; of the
most common verse-types
studied.
have been derived
from JAECKEL(1902) Tabula VI.
in
42
RHYTHMAND METk.E
2.4.1.
In the authors
types
in
apondee one
which
one
outnumber
of
the
studied of
those
(Archestratus
the in
metra has a spondaic
the five
me,ra has been
relevant which
excepted)
not
a single
verse-
as a
realized
metron,
or more than
realization.
(See Table
distribution
of
II,
col.2,
P• 36 above.)
2.4.1.1. the
In
several
metra
verse-types metron
accordance the
occurs
in
one
with
the
general in
which
(see
Table
Ill),
the
apondee
is
the
apondees
of the aituated
five
over
possible
in the
aecond
moat frequently: --vu---uu-uu-vu-~I.
(See Table IV, P• 46 below).
Strictly speaking, this 1a not the case in Archestratus (8.15% of his verses have a single spondee which is situated in the second metron, as against 8.79% with a single spondee which is situated in the first metron). In Nonnus and in Callimachus, whose hexameters show the largest proportion of verses with a single spondee, the verse-type with the spondee in the second metron is particularly frequent. (In Nonnus and Callimachus respectively, 22.9% and 27.8% of the sum total of verses is of this type, whereas the other verae-typea with a single make· up 25.4% and 22.3% of the verses spondee all put together respectively.)
E.g. Hom. Il.
m.t'&:l 6' t).iot AUCQ.t.T£q>().nv,
1.20 26 33
l.FI0£, Y~, lCOLA~PLV tyW ~ 6'D:t', ll>E:1.0EV6' b y~
ta 6'
1QX1
4:rtoi.va.c5tx~1,
""1UC7l 'KI.XELW
Hat btEC&t'O 1Jib.>
Hom.Hymn.111.3/4
wa.Ct,a. t"' m.a.fc:xnx,1.v tnt ax.£6bv~1.0 rdvu:r; &i>' ~, &tt qnC61.1,11 t"6fp. nmlw1.. Hes. Th- 15/6 fl6t nax1.6&.MJ.n1.nox.ov twcolya.1.ov wat etiu.v a.t6olriv tA1.~ t"' •"'A:)06C t'1lV Parm. 2. 7 oO't£loP dv yvolnr; 't'6 YE ui'\ tbv (oo loP dvoo'tav) twuxlour;
Emp. 6 2. 2
MPLv6µ£vov
nf¥>
~
'tUt01.xeovbc; tfavtt"&Uov o0T' t:vordtvol6v t"' tn1.xt:P1.ovdvS(xk,1, yutov
8
Arat.
utv
~er;
4
Archestr.
~ ~ya.y£
VIIl.4
645/6
~
T1.&Lr; ro1.6v6£ 6u\~,:oc;
TTA£0V£'Kt'£LC
611wa.l qao1, Ttf+X11,68E::v tpx01Jtvo1.o &lcq:Jnlou•Qpl«»cbvtca:-ca.1. c!t\.yoc;"F4:Xa>'t1. 123 & Inv Riv, £[T' bx'Jt >«l't 1 4,txJ. ).mcp&.!UJHQUIJ 7 4 noUa.l
TE
148 t'.t&:tav, Ttn1.yoc; tn:£t nmi qioi-£POV 46£1.c 152 a., µii o,up,:aaf\n:, µii b TJnYO(; °'411.v &vcx,tfJ As a matter a particularly
of fact there is in the bucolic hexameters of Theocritus high incidence of those verse-types in which the two spondees are consecutive:
«11.~
6' oob
48
RHYTIIM AND METRE
In the hexameter•
of Parmenidea and Archeatratus
types with tvo spondeea the type in which dactyl
of the verse-
and spondee alter-
nate occurs most frequently: -vu---uu- - -vv--11. Parm. 1.4 tji ~tji Y4>l.1£ ~L (f/:pov tmoL 9 • m_1.~ ~L, npo,\LTICCicnL ~ NuK"C6,; 32 ~ 6oH.C~ E['VCJ.L61.0.1'llVroCTDV'IXl ~ 2.3 fa utv &IAlC;lDTLV n: IGJ. ~ CO( lDTL ufl Et'VCJ.L 8.16 lDTLv 1\ cue.lDTLv· kbcpLta.L 6' chv, iix:Jn£p c1\dyxri Archestr.
-rov6' de
~,nv.
IV.15
6:fc,plJv ~" ~
VIII• 7 >al 'tO rdx.o{; ~. X.2
~
-cfi-,uvov ~tcnv
XII.4 E[&' ~ 61tc~ XIII.5/6 tivlx' 4v Eli~
6' o[l,ICU. tu,1.A.£UE L
olCOJCXIV drio.lP£1. >dv
~C,
wal
&,tov
fi 6cNdnri~
~ nivta ~,
>CPCecwov tc ~ ~ cu:1. ta >a'tOTIO. This is also
the case in Nonnus' hexameters,
where two consecut-
ive spondeea occur only in the second and third metron - perhaps because the rhythmical boundary of the penthemimeral caesura would seem to diminish
Nonn. II.14
the effect
yN:11.JHbv 6x~LOLV
06c.p
~
33 nl~
51
of 'consecutiveness'.
tpf:"LlJ0tt
A£001.V£V
b.uu:r.lvavt0
~t
38 YElTCM:C ~ ~ ~l6£c
6x{nL
73/4 >al OM0n'£;\o1.~tn:rovm· ~(OLC
6t t>&t~1.~ ~
1'£P6eE:v 1tCTCtOVt'Et tv~o 2.4.3.
The data put forward above add up to the overall
sented
in Table VI (pp.
quency of the several the ten verse-types studied 2.4.3.1.
and their
which shows (1)
56/7),
verse-types
occurring divergence
picture
the order
pre-
of fre-
in Homer (down to 0.3%), and (2)
moat frequently
in some of the authors
from the Homeric order.
In the Homeric hexameter
of all
verse-types
the one with a
Of the dactyl in all five relevant metra occurs moat frequently. other poets, this is not the case only in Empedocles, Archeatratus,
a " a.... >
TABLEV
~
VERSE-TYPES WITHTWOSPONDEES*
n
~ ssddd(s) dsdsd(s) sddsd(s) dssdd(s) sdsdd(s) ddssd(s) sddds(s) dsdds(s) dddss(s) Hom. Hes. Hom. Hymns
8.1 % 9.1 % 6.3 %
Parm. Emp.
6.1 % 9.37%
Archestr.
5.87%
Arat. Apoll. Call. Theocr. buc. ep.
8 % 7.7 % 6.8 % 13.2 % 7 %
-
Nonn.
• percentage
of
6.2 % 5.4 % 5.6 %
3.3 % 2.8 % 4.1 %
2.8 % 2.9 % 2.4 %
1.3 % 1.7 % 1.4 %
0.9 % 1.04% 1.05%
0.8 % 1.26% 1.3 %
0.4 % 0.5 % 0.5 %
6.1 % 3.4 %
8.1 % 1.7 %
3.4 % 12 %
2 % 0.72%
-
1.35% 0.91%
0.7 %
1.2 %
9.12%
7.82%
6.51%
4.23%
4.89%
-
0.32%
4.3 5.5 6.1 3.7 7.3
3.9 2.9 3.9 3.6 5.9
% % % o/o %
5 % . 4.1 % 2.7 % 7% 1.8 %
3.6 2.9 1.2 4.8 2.9
0.8 0.3 0.1 3.5 0.2
3% 1.33%
3.1 % 2.26% 1.6 % 0.22% 1.3 %
3.5 o/o
0.4 o/o
0.5 %
6.4 % 6.9 % 5.7 % 10.1% 7.4%
% % % % %
8.8 %
the sum total
of verses
that
% o/o % % %
constitute
% % % % %
1%
0.11% 1.8 %
the different
ddsds(s) 0.25% 0.59% 0.2 %
-
0.6 %
0.43% 0.02%
-
0.7 % 0.69% 0.65%
0.3 %
0.22%
&
~
tlj ::ii,
samples studied.
.po \0
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
50 Callimachus
and in
stratus
there
is
other
poets
with
a single
the
the
show a
order
of Theocritus.
preference
of for
Only in Arche-
dactyls
one
in general;
particular
the
verse-type
realization.
of frequency
with
with
poetry
a low percentage
spondaic
types
types
bucolic
also
just
As the
of
the
of the several
two spondees
a single
occur
verse-types
shows,
more frequently
than
some
some of
spondee.
(In Archestratus, there are as many as two spondees in the versetype which most frequently occurs: dsdsds; see Table VI below.) 2.4.3.1.1. third
In
the
metron
either dee
the
occur
first
in two of
was indicated with of
the the
one
less
of
of the
metra
occurs
tendency
preference
for
a caesura
(see
3. 2.3., to
p.
realize
symmetrical
shape:
see
a spondee
in
often
than
the
type
the
a spondee
in
or with a spon-
2.4.1.1.-3.
above).
1s obviously
after
also
it
short
precludes colon.
(In
disfavoured
2.4.1.4. first,
with
first
second
apparently
As
connected
the
since
rising
the
in
above). second
on Like-
and fourth
two spondees
of
which
metron.
when there in
any
metron
see
a is
with
with
74 below),
dds,dds
in the third
than
which
verse-type
more
occur
(for
fourth
this
Furthermore, spondees
the
(2.4.1.4),
verse-type
one occurs
or
a spondee
verses
above
its
wise,
than
with
positions
the
account
second
possibilities
addition,
verses
these
metron
the
hexameter frequently
or the
rhythmical third
quently
Homeric
is
first
in the fourth:
a spondee and/or
so,
in
in the the
third
second
metron,
metron
other
more fre-
e.g.
-uu---l--uu-uu--11 and ( ll) ---uu-1--uu-uu--H occur -uu-uu-!-- - -uu--il: ( lO)
more frequently
than
likewise,
(12.5)-----J--uu-uu--ll
E6Ttl'ix -a+>
61
'11IE DACTYLIC HEXAMETER
See CHANTRAINE ( 3 1958) 116ff.
2.5.2.1.
If a word does not fit
into the metric
profile
of the hexa-
meter at all, poets may choose a different word-form or adapt a particular word to suit the demands of dactylic poetry. is sometimes adopted
Such a procedure which have a limited For a recent with
applicability:
reference
extensive
to
in the case of words
see SCOTT(1909).
somewhat chaotic)
(if
also
the
survey
of
literature
these
on the
phenomena,
subject,
see
CRESPO(1977) - but compare FUEHRER(1978); see also WYATT (1969). 2.5.2.1.1.
In order
may opt
for
that
a word can be used in dactylic
a different
manner of scansion
or for
different word-form; for instance - with synizesis, the use of ttUX.8.IJV, ~ V
v-
_,,_,,._,
VV-
See MEISTER (1921) (1968). In the case of other
-
148ff.;
-
aar,1.Af'\Ocinstead
CHANTRAINE ( 3 1958) 68ff.;
words a suitable
-
,_,
- TT0AEUL~instead
of
-
RUIJGH
form seems to have been coined in either
the same or
,.,.._
of ~-
See THOUVENIN (1905); 2.5.2.1.2.
...~;..,
as a
w-
of ·Ax1.>.tc.lc;.
(often in analogy with a word-form occurring paradigm) e.g. a different V
functioning
W._,V
8ao1.>.tc.lc;;·Ax1.>.fiocinstead
V
one
a prosodically
V
bisyllable; contrast the trisyllabic ~, - with "metathesis quant1tat1s", the use of
poetry
SHIPP (1953) 104ff.;
CHANTRAINE ( 3 1958) 94ff.
Sometimes a word only conforms with
owing to a lengthening
or shortening
the metric
profile
. (c. q. absence of lengthening)
of the vowel involved: vowel lengthening:
mdla~,
-
6.novtooeru.; la,;l n,
See WITTE (1915); 97ff.;
>OKOEPYi:nv.
MEISTER (1921) 34ff.
WYAn' (1969); RUIJGH (1971);
STRA(1978).
11
3
CHANTRAINE ( 1958)
MCLENNAN (1978);
ll>EK-
62
RHYTHM ANDMETRE shortening: boundary
correptio epica: ~ov; ... ~ bi££; aee SJ6LUND (1938);
oaiaaion of lengthening ... V#ZOKUv8oc.
Finally,
2.5.3.
with regard
the verae at the linguistic
to the metric
profile,
Such
concern,
the verae
lable
occupying a verae-poaition
abort
syllable
uy
11.
ahow 'defects' an explana-
above.
sub 2.5.2.1.2.
for
sequence"
level
which aeem to require
from thoae indicated
defects
WEST (1982)
VIE~
by position:
tion different
within
or, at a word-
e.g.,
17 :
example,
the occurrence
of hiatus
or of either
a (word-final)
long syl-
which would be properly
or a (word-final)
position
where one would expect
question
aa such would easily
short
syllable
occupying a verae-
a long syllable fit
taken by a
- while the word in
in with the profile
of the hexa-
meter. Such defects
seem to be connected
with the formulaic
of the epic diction especially; that ia to aay either (a) an epic formula ia not sufficiently integrated into lar
(rhythmical)
context
in which it
which enda in -VCI is followed requires
a long syllable:
e.g.
XIII.587
Il.
(cp. V.99 Od.
~xoe
(cp. 11.261 VI.294
e.g.,
a formula
by IV whereas the metric
profile
.......... 'YUJ)Dv, mtb 6' btto:to m.~
.........
X£L~ X£L~
\11.~, \11.~
X.182 X£t'Pl(; Qd.
the particu-
6i.:~
frnu-ro TU.~ 6.LO't~) • 68-' tnl ~ fjv 6.\ltl,101.0
'YUJ)Dv" 6u\ 6'
~'KoC
XII .336
occurs;
character
1,0.XJOV
£UX£T
TIXM.1.nc ~,
vuldi.1£VOI.
T£1J)(OVT
.........
1
1
'Aal'tV{l
&I.ha.) •
~1.w.uota
&Tt>'l't"t6Au:,c &icov T£ ytyCJNE ~.
u
(cp. IX.473 6.U' 6T£ 1,6:xx,v 611:f\v 6::icov T£ yl;yt,JVE ~); or (b)
a formula
tion)
is
(e.g.
by means of declension
in a manner which makes it
the underlying e.g.
modified
Il.
metric
XVUI.288
(cp. Od.
prof1le
70
deviate
or conjuga-
from the quantity
:
.........
rrclv 1Jtv "10P[t)I.Ol,DI.O rro>.1.v~
I.250 ,Q 6' 1'ia) 6oo XI.402 ~ Tt£PI.~
Y£VEXJ.l~ ...utv ., -
(cp. XXIV.112 6oOcTt£PI.~
fr5'
ot6'v
Ip'
of
~ ..
~)
•
11WEX1 MOM oU',v
TU:a.lGNi).
63
THE DACTYLICHEXAMETER
See PARRY(1928)
• (1971)
197ff.;
HOEKSTRA(1965).
For the Homeric Hymns: JANICO(1982)
33-5;
EDWARDS(1971)
90-3.
2.6.
Theories,
current
from antiquity
types
of
particular
shape
("ethos")
a are
realization posed of
neceBSarily
of the
to
produce
rhythmical
the
several a certain
impreHioniam
for
onwards,
have
a
speculative. metra
wh~ch hold
Only
stylistic
versevalue
occasionally
or spondeea not
that
emotional
very
effect; does
HOEKSTRA(1957)
specific
by dactyls
however
Hesiod:
in
the
may be sup-
general
disengage
this itself
kind from
realm of mere hypothesis. For
the
hexameter
various in
theories
antiquity,
concerning see
the
£
C6r\ of the dactylic
RAUSCHER (1886);
GROSSMANN(1887);
VOLTZ (1893). As SCOTT (1914/5) points out "Homer was just as willing to picture a lively scene with spondees as with dactyls, and a solemn one with dactyls as freely as with spondees" (330). SCOTT cites, as an example of the supposed rapidity of both action and i .a., rhythm Il.
VI.511
~llllD, t yoO\.a. qipe:1. \,1£"CQT'
~
klCll~ tT'O'l,.1y,
whereas in Od. XII.12
~ ~I.,
~ ~ ~
xtovtt:~
the dactylic rhythm proves to be compatible with the description of great solemnity. 71 Anyway it should be realized that in view of the co11aon occurrence of holodactylic verses it is not very plausible a to each priori that a significant emotional value ahould be attached of them. On the other hand, one may confront the particular verse comonly cited as an example of the solemn tone of the spondee: Il.
XXIII.221
Od.
XV.334
\WXflv'KL~ OL'tOU
Ih~
&1.~to
with,
e.g.,
>Gt 'HPEi:aat:tv Tt£1.d,."> Mat -coO-rov, hatpe:. '1XJlrm.6t UIOm ~ ~ Hal rri:,bc ot., !Uta.l.\lE. 940/1 f\ 61.0UU11 fCi&DE 61.c.\ut'YtlV~ [p1.c·
Arat.
f\ 'HCI.L TTOU nc Apoll.
~
UEhl.1.~
fx.£1.&:m'v.,.
III.531-3
rota1. Mat 6:Mai.dro1.oTQX)C;'IJELALCXJE't'6u'tl.lW, 'HCl.l ~ tanp1, V &iai:> X£Aa6E: L\Q. ~, OO'tP1-r£ 'HCl.l~vr,c t~ ~ x~. Nonn. V.619-21
fkt.Tt£, ¼oC 6' <£LTt£f.lM,ITlC 1 Ml00c t>tlkv ~, eti.i.1.vQJUYE,kd.U..t.Tt£ An~, ~ 6' de CJut\o.1.ovtaf>.y£ro ~lnc. •~
3.1.1.
6'
The situation -
metric
phrase
realizing
O[l'ot YE I; 287 6.vt£o. 6 1 a.L£1.I ; 306 M nC1l.lii:N I ; 222 olx:il. "COL aa"J-cb; 231 tix' nEP 4,qxaA, I . 260 ~ u.iA.' ~ I ; 288 o0n: )(£\I tiot I ; 294 6Ua >C0.1. ~ ( ; 298 ot 6 1 fn n6::x:x.i> I ; 328 ~ l«l"t' at.rttav I ; 329 Ii 61. ot ~ I ; 330 0c tx,. l,IU.t.ata I ; 336 ot 6t 6'i 6Uo1. I ; 339 ~ &t' a.t£L I ; 344 o'fa >at aCJtat I ; 371 ot6. 't£ no.\M I ; etc.
6l'i'; 51 ;
70
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
ea11. HIJlln. 1.16 ~ X& 't0c01.o I ; 37 ~ w x&OIA I · 44 f\ie&v t>u:tw 10 a() au YE vrPN n.36 ro101:E 4>ol~ \, 45 b. 6t vu 4>oltk-.1~ ; 50 a()6t 'K£V a(y&(; I ; 51 6cn,v •AITti>J.liN ' ; 52 066' ~'t~ I ; 91 1'x1.'>.tovm.I ; 95 o66t utv al':rrol ; 103 £006 OE ~'tl'v:> 108 ~ m no.Ua.I ; III.22 6,l JJ£ )t)'Cl:xM,I ; 31 &x,• t~ I ; 38 JGl. ul:v dvu&.at,I ; 39 ~ b JJtv £(TUI I ; etc. etc •• rbythaical, preference would appear to be If this, eaaentially strong enough, it uy, at least partly, account on the one band for the proportional increase of verses with a strong enjambement and on the other band for the proportional decrease of veraes in which a weak fora of enjambe•nt or none at all occurs. The connection in the Sophoclean triaeter between a strong enjambe•nt and the treatment of the clausula described in Chapter Ill sub 3. below would provide a parallel to this practice. This in itself might be considered as one more argument for not preHing the point of the presence or absence of fonmlae; alternatively, this form kind of enjambement may be connected with a 110re 'accomplished' of (non-oral) verse-composition.
I;
I;
I;
3. 2. The dactylic phrases
(cola),
perceived
hexameter presumably
by a single
because
it
into
is
two rhythmical
sub-
too long a sequence
to be
act of attention.
The colon-boundary before
structured
is
(caesura)
occurs at one of two positions
just
the middle of the verse: -uu,-uu,-:u!u,-uu,-vu,,uu)II,
so that
the cola are characterized
(1) the second colon is slightly
by three longer
(interrelated)
features:•
than the first;
(2) the colon-boundary does not coincide with a metron-boundary; (3) the second colon of the falling rhythmical sequence begins rising
manner.
Considering the
verse
the length is
ceptually
optimal the
the hexameter
of the hexameter
structured
with the conception of fact
in a
into
more than
extent
tendencies
(see
Ch. I
observed
as consisting
sub
incompatible
sub-phrase above).
that
of a perAs
a matter
to a theory
of
of more than two cola can be satisfact-
for
by the
preferences
turing
described
sub
3. 3.
4. ff.
a
3.2.2.
is
by those who adhere
accounted e.g.,
which holds
two cola
of a colon as a rhythmical
orily
designating,
any theory
&
strong
for
colon-internal
below. Accordingly,
syntactical
boundary
struc-
the habit at
pos.3
of
as a
THE DACTYLICHEXAMETER (secondary) (cp.
caesura
is
not
PP• 8lff.
3.2.4.l.,
71 only
confusing
but
theoretically
unsound
below).
FRAENICEL's theory of the hexameter as (potentially) consisting 3 of four cola (FRAENKEL (1926; 1968), which has been modified by PORTER (1951) and ROSSI (1965), has been rejected by DALE (1957) 302, KIRK (1966) 76-104 (eliciting a reappraisal from INGALLS (1970)), and BEEKES (1972). 3.2.1.
At
the
boundary
-
various
rhythmic
which,
degrees
strictly
of
hearer, it
realize E.g.,
strength
the
a heavier an expected
with
occurring (ranging
expectancy
fulfilled
a stronger
is
there
is
that
caesura
is
a full
or
from
syntactical (or stop
of one.
at
full
a
a rhythmical - the
It
is
stop
may to
sequence
syntactical
also
have a
not
see
Ch.
with
the
boundary
unimportant
elsewhere, BEEKES loc.
colon."
the caesura
verse-end,
3.
boundary
po88ible)
by a word-
word-boundary:
by a signal
a weaker
realized
at
Compare BEEK.ES(1972)
is
which
the
potentially-structural
below).
"Essential be
like
speaking
III.3.1.l.
level,
when it
-
whether does
not
cit • •
position:
I ~ G'xi,f::>.E{; a!rto&' 6.\t:oea.1. Od. III.444 ~ 6' 6u,vCov &[x&· I yt;:xiN 6' traa,>am. Nta'mp Hom.Hymn. V.109 oO 't'LC 't'OI. 8£6'; dµ.1. • I 't'L µ.' 6a:x\d.~1.v tfOK£1.{;; Hes. Th. 412 Zd~ 1 Emp. 17 .14 6.U' dy& uu&,v 'NA0&1. • I J.03r1Y6':>'t'OI. q:ptwc; a0E£t. • Archestr. xxn.6 q:)1.l,IIJ61.dc 6Eoc;" I >4l'' ~· ro Hat trt£Cyou Arat. 5 TOOY6':>Hat yi:vo{; dµ.tv. I b 6' frnu:,c 6.vep;rco1.01. Apoll. 111.1008/9 ~ c,d.w K'lXXlL\.tlJ\1°I ti 6'~1.obv ~ 6a,\oOoa. V£)(T4:,cov UEC6rioE· I xu8rl6t ot ~1. ~ Hom.Il.
III.428
"~{;
b(
~1.'
...
Theocr. Id.
I 11. l 5 vOv fY\.tlJV,:ov -~ XXIV. 35 c1voi:a8', •~L
Call.
HJIIIIIJ• III
't'Pll,N.
0
I ~ ~. fi tn. MXJ.L~ I tut Y6':>6to{; CoX£L 6KVl'¥)6v"
8/9
toucHat
l'6EaI - m mn+>,cLJOE (W. oo yap RA> i. Lov t>crt4x:nvt'I ro'te:l xe:ov d:rr.c:,vtEXXni. 299 't>Ji'tE, ut-r~, wal. h:>l.ou,a.C k£ 6wTlcnL, ~ 4v XPl"ILUA1J wal. tc 6:nEPOVb1.ov ~0
iii) )G(Q >U+)01LVEL'V.wmaJCtp5m.
to• dn;pLv.
't0V 1.>.tovm 1.A.Etv wal. -a;,rrpcx,1.6vn np:x:,Etw.1., wal. 6ouf;v 6{; X£'V Ei;), wal. l8l 6ouf;v 6{; KE'V l8I Ei;). Cp. PORTER(1951) 30: "It is reasonable to assume, I think, that in the first half of the llorks and Days, where most of the material is presented in highly organuea paragraphs, this processing of traditional material was so thorough that the metrical statistics which we compile from the text represent the poet's own free choice. In the second half, however, where the text reads like a string of proverbs, the traditional element plays such an important role that the statistics of choice of line-type show the influence of the metrical characteristics of earlier folk literature." More specifically, the correspondence of the colon following the penthemimeral caesura with the rhythmical shape of various proverbial expression• (the paroemiac: see KORZENIEWSKI (1968) 90/1) is likely
76
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
to have some influence particularly in certain
3.2.3.1.
In the
case
to be a tendency (which of
is,
at
realizing
remarkably
on the specified
of a penthemimeral
to start the
rhythmic
a rising large
frequency contexts.
the
second
level, of
penthemiaeral
caesura colon
the
colon-beginning),
proportion
of
dactylic
also
caesura
there
appears
in a double-short
most
strongly
manner
marked
as can be deduced realizations
manner
from
of
the
as compared
to
the
third
metron,
-1.1""'0--w-luu... being
favoured
---Ou-u7i-l-- • . . . Within a limited sample of Homeric hexameters - comprising the first 100 verses of Il. Ill, VI and IX each - the following situation can be observed: trochaic caesura 156 • 52% penthem. caesura 138 • 46% with consecutive uu 84 • 60.87% with consecutive - 54 • 39.13% without a caesura 6 • 2%. Compare the figures derived from a sample 150 verses of both Th. and hexameters(• the first trochaic caesura 191 • 63.67% penthem. caesura 105 • 35% with consecutive uv with consecutive without a caesura 4 • 1.33%. This
tendency
other ter.
properties
also
appears
also
to
to decrease be of
in poetry
a rhythmically
300 Erg.):
of
Hesiodean
69 • 65.71% 36 • 34.29% which
less
iB shown by
strict
cbarac-
'0
Compare, for example, the situation in the last 100 verses of the Brga: of the comparatively frequent occurrences of penthem. caesura (51.51%), 46.34% is followed by a long syllable, 53.66% by two short syllables. E.g.:
trochaic
Il.111.13-8
caesura:
~ &:n Ti1Ntlftb TTDCJOi I KOVL~
tpx~·
4,vu"t' ~ ~ 6' 00 I 61.trcpwµx,v nEE,£01.0.
ol 6' lh£ estox.r.fbJ ficnvI bt' ~1.01.v t6'm:{;, Tpc&XJlv utv TTQOl.lix1.C£V I •A>.1,~ &0€1.~, wal )QJl"CUN.l~ ~v ~1.01. v I fX(.i7v wat ftqJOC·a{n:ap 6oCix I Sui> ~ xcwcii>
ntE DACTYLIC HEXAMETER
77
c5r:>' f11.EXPV£ I edlv 6.~ 4t.~ T£uepxvla,v, ~ev I tuKn]Jtv tv .Ap'of3TJ dq,vEt.bi;6t.6'tot.o, I Q>lAOC 6' liv 6.v8J:xmot.ot. • 'l'l.ivmc~ (l)l.AiWK£V I b&i>fut. O(Kla. \Q.LC&JV. :rh. 63-6 fv6d ott)t.V At.TQX>li:clxq:x,t' lGL 6r4lna 'HOMi, rap 6' alrctk; lfdpt.Ttc i:cl>Gt ·1UE+>OC; otKl' ~ooot.v tv &.\luc; • tp:,.rliv 61:I6t.a aros.n &::aiv tctaa.t. ~, raivaiJVi:c I ~ KOHbv>GL6f"ip1. V 6q,t.U£ t., OX.£1:'Aln· o(j nc -n',v YE I tw vtq,m >WJVfovtO(;. i:ov ~ wvovli:~ otKm& vb:a(u" 732-4 fi' 6 YE TIPbcrotxov I~ ~ a.(Mflc. un6' a.t6ota. yovij I ~ NSo&t. oC'KOU LCTCLTJ ~ 1 ~1,.V&µEV, W.' ~
+ consecutive Il.
'tEUXEa. ~ I cpqx-a,, lCOLMJC; tnL 'Vfbc, a14n 61: oC>G6' tiibv I~ ro:Ut.v, 6iP1 T0.¥)6c U£
spondee:
a·
VI.58/9 XE'C~
f\U£T~,
I un6' ~ nva Yoott.:>t. l,a\'t1'¥) I uno'acqi,yot., w. · ai.o.TDVTEC
tovm. q,tQot,, IX.25/6 M' fn KOiTOI;tcrr:l l,ltyt.arov. 'H00pov
W.' Compare Archestr.V.1-3
dyt:a', &c dv tYWI £Cm...>,rr.£1.~fu fotw 61'1oot. c.1vi'iP I totvt.E fi lwl!!b;,
nrivtcc·
tv oCw.>,
6:rnc btt.o-n',1,1.17\1 I t.cna.t. ol 1:01.0 lO.,:' ~ mvcolac; t6tac; I TEUXE t.v, &c dv ouK£AEUJJC.
78
RHYTHMAND METRE
IX. 6-9 dv 6t nr:,u llJ£l~
Erg.
I a{rn:lO Y&\'n...An.,, 6'.l00~
KVLOCIC ~tv, I tfc ~~ ~ &tc ·talrc6v µ£foe 6' I aO-tac; tnl 'tllyd;vou Ortto., ~ ,:PLllac; I dv6ri ~)C.QVt&JV tv ~699-701 1Q)8Ev1.Kfrv 6t Yda.1£1.v, I~ K' t\8m. )(€0\Q. 61.odft';1.c· i:fiv 6t lli\.i.ata ~tv, I frnc atar:vhYu61.\CLLE:1., ravm.lli\.' 6a.1Plct&:,.,, I ldi ye:l't001.x.dPl.ai:a. Yl'wQc. 783-7 ~6voc 6' ~•I~ 6' oo~ tanv, oOtt y&'Vix::J6a.c. dt' I o(h·' ~ -.d1Jcuc.tvt1.~. o(,6t l,ll;v ti ~ I h'tll ~ YE: ye:vtacku. ~, ~• tp£qJOU(;I -cduve:1.v ,a1. 111m ~ Cll'l'HOV 't' ~eaA&tv I tt01.~1.ov ~1.ov ~-
lllE DACTYLIC HEXAMETER
79
TABLEVII CAESURA
penthem.
troch.
-caesura
sample size
Hom.
42
%
56.8 %
1.2 %
27803
Parm. Emp.
46 % 29.6 %
51.3 % 67.9 %
2.7 % 2.5 %
150• 436*
Archestr.
62.4 %
32.5 %
5.1 %
311*
Arat. Apoll. Call. Theocr. buc.
47.8 % 36.75% 28.2 % 49 % 28.3 %
51 .5 % 63.22% 71.8 o/oII 50.7 % 71.5 %
0. 7 % 0.03%
18.9 %
81.1 %
tp. Nonn.
* For
0.3 % 0.2 %
1154 5836 2109 1319• 1058 1119
reasons obscure to me, the number of the hexameters counted by JAECKELin this case differs somewhat from the number used in his quantifications of the proportion of dactyls and spondees, so that here also the sample size is somewhat different from that in Table Iff. above. The most remarkable difference concerns Theocritus' bucolica, since here, in contrast to the earlier quantifications, they include the mimes as well.
80
RHYTHMAND METRE
• N.B.
The data
JAECKEL (1902) able.
presented rabulaXII
Unfortunately,
For some reason
he does
Homeric
Hymns;
the
relevant
data
Od.
Table
- as being
VII
have
for
derived
not
include
the
sake of
been
in his
Th. Erg.
table
completeneaa
from PORTER (1951)
Th. & Erg.
Hymn.Dem. Apo/. Hema. Aphr.
-----------
Hom. Hym.n.12
With
either
Hesiod
troch.
-caesura
sample size
44% 46.95%
54.9% 51.75%
1.1% 1.3%
1000 1000
40.3% 54.6%
57.6% 42.9%
2.1% 2.5%
47.45%
50.25%
2.3%
43.6% 48.7% 30.6% 47.2%
54.6% 50.3% 69.2% 52.4%
1.8% 1% 0.2% 0.4%
42.525%
56.625%
0.85%
-------------------------calculation
the
or
one may compare
penth.
some additional
rived
avail-
59:
--------------------------
-----------
from
to elision.
-----------------------------------53.325% 1.2% 45.475% II. & Od. Hes.
derived
the most comprehensive
JAECKEL pays no attention
the
Hom. II.
in
corresponding
----------2000
1000 800•
----------1800 495• 500• 500•
250•.
i-----------1745
data
can be de-
from O'NEILL (1942):
Hom. II. Od.
42.3% 42.35%
56.2% 57.55%
1.5% 0.1%
1000
II. & Od.
42.325%
56.875%
0.8%
2000
52.5%
45.8%
1.7%
1000
-------------------------------------~---------Hes.
* "raised to * "multiplied
the scale
of 1000",
1000
PORTERo.c. 50.
by two", ibid •• For the figures concerning the RIJlllll to Demeter he adds the warning that they are "not strictly comparable to those for the other texts, being based on a scale of 990".
**
"the
first
250 lines
multiplied
by four"
(ibid.).
TiiE DACTYLICHEXAMETER
81
The figures presented by both PORTER and O'NEILL are based upon quantification of word-types. Somewhat arbitrarily in our computations of the caesura has the lllOnosyllabic words occurring in pos. 6a have been reckoned as prepositives, the other half as postpositives. As a matter of fact, an indiscriminate counting of word-types is bound to produce a distorted picture as far as the incidence of caesura is concerned; in particular, the percentage of trochaic caesurae may be somewhat lower than would be the case with figures based upon a careful assessment of the word-boundaries involved, since it is likely that at pos. 6a there is a high occurrence of postpositive boundaries (TE, at, etc.). The nuaber of monosyllables involved amounts, for PORTER, to Il. 118, Od. 119, Th. 184, Brg. 134, H!lfllll to Dea. 132, Apol. 122, Berm. 104, Aphr. 112 (the last five being raised to a scale of 116, Od. 109, Hes. 134. 1000); for O'NEILL, to Il. According to SCHAMP (1981) 85 in the Orph. Li th. hexameters has a penthemimeral, 62% a trochaic caesura.
3.2.4.
Archestratus'
without the p.
a
sa•
verse
caesura
shows
(5.1%);
characteristic
here
to
the
highest
a lesser
percentage
Parmenides'
again,
38% of
degree:
of
the
verses
show
hexameters
2. 7%. (See
Table
VII,
79 above.) However,
the
far
as
prae-Hellenistic
whole:
Emp.
fall
in with
ness
to
stance, dee
as
2.5%,
Hom. 1.2%
a general
fifth
in
the
metron
general
BEEKES (1972)
(Hes.
towards
Hellenistic
less
This
strict
on
situation
increased
seems to
rhythmical
hexameter
the
strict-
(compare,
for
in-
'normalization'
(3.3.
with
1.5%).
be
concerned,
(2.4.3.1.2.
caesura
is
also
should
the
be
unobjectionable
regard
3: "perhaps
above);
below)).
of
which
regularity
to
are
with a spon-
absence
licence
a caesura
on the number of verse-types
dihaeresis
The incidental occasional
without appears
tendency
the restrictions
of the bucolic
verses
hexameter
be observed
in the
have
the
to
admitted
considered
only
the
observance
only
because
all
a
caesura
a
in of
as
virtue
of
caesura.
other
an a
(Cp.
verses
do
the caesura".)
3.2.4.1. shows
Of word-end
infrequently element
the
(at
hexameters after
there pos.3).
is
without
the
fourth
also
a word-boundary
So in these
marked cases
too
substantial
element after the
(at the
falling
part
pos. 7); second type
not
marked of verse
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
82 is
(internally)
structured
in a rising
manner:
-vu-luu-\.lu-luu-\.lu--41For
this
reason
word-end
caesura
(the
at
3 sometimes
pos.
so-called
above),
caesura
function
at
a verse-position
significance
therefore,
according
hexameters
without
ant
factor
in
end in verses Not which,
in
types
the
of See,
the
present
position for
of
their
this
- or, On the
length.
(O'NEILL (1942)
word-type 100%:
as
being
group
instance,
pos. 5 pos. 7 pos. 9
of
however
ascribed
the is
cases
do not by preference for
be
middle
(see
theory, the
of
the
verse,
verses
are
5 7 9 11
word-type 100% :
;
simply
does,
produces
a
may be a relev-
positioning
of
the
occur
relative
involve not not other
either
quite
optional,
surprisingly, hand,
in this
proper
word-
such
particular
frequency
of the
names
(other) long
position. following
146,148): Call.
Apoll.
41.7% 6.7% 51.7%
63.5% 1.6% 34.9'1/o
63.3%
45.3%
36.7%
54.7%
60
79
49
64
44.4% 22.2%
16.7%
75%
66.7% 16.7%
25%
6
4
word-
word-types
Hes.
33.3%
-
may be con-
II.
9
4
the circumstance
word-boundaries
particular
the
established•
hand,
the caesura
3.2.2.
a word-boundary
these
other
Ch.I,
luu-vv-1 pos. pos. pos. pos.
e.g.
):
to
statistically
On the
ts
these
a greater
certainly
13
a caesura.
separate
a
which
option
without
virtue
as
of
movement just
infrequently
sidered
types
the
only near
a caesura.
rhythmical
and even word-end
("trithemimeres"
of a caesura
can
to
also,
here
rising
as a caesura
conception
structural
that,
caesura"),
as a third
31.
present
occurring
7 is sometimes conceived
pos.
''hephthemimeral counts
KORZENIEWSKI(1968) In the
at
2
word-
THE DACTYLICHEXAMETER The
supposition
caesura
that
and with
rhythmical tionable words
the
relative
a word-end
tendency
but
exception which
83
merely
pos.
the
third
of
7 does
not
constitutes
in accordance
is
span
at
frequency
to
without
reflect
the be
increasing observed
a
a positive
a comparatively
with
metron
verses
unobjecscarcity
in
of
Hellenistic
hexameters. without a caesura which show a See LA ROCHE (1899) 3: verses (both proper names), word-end at pos. 7 are found twice in Apollonius eight times in Aratus (three proper names); never at all in Callimachus.
Verses with a ''hephthemimeral by LEHRS (1865) 396-403. Some examples of verses
caesura"
in Homer have been listed
a caesura are:
without
Hom. Il • I II. 71 atnO'tl+X)f; 6t KE 'VI.KTlOI;) kPE lc::x:Mv TE ytvrrm.1.
to tot v u n
80
ME:c,o-1."t• fea,Uov •
~1.
VI. 3 ~ l~ Parm.
"tTIVO'~
1• 2 7
~fipm.
(Ii Y6':> 6n'
8. 21 ~ ytvro1.,
9•2
bn6'
nriTOUtatl
v)
~ ..
8vrrciiN, a£J:.
TI.{; ~ ~"COLO
1\ MO."t'Q ~
fiypoTttx,Jv Y~
17.5 t) 6t TlOAI.V01.~
"tE:A£\J"tTI
1\ MO."t'Q~
8p;;q,6£Cai
01.tmn
60.\1.xa.luJVE.{; n 'l,djo1. OP'\~1. ~"~ ~ VIU.85 c.U~ 6' avtrn\:ro, ~ 6' de; ~ 00 XI.274 'Vl'lUOlV fm y~i.v ~• 1\x.&roy6p Kf¥.). 278 quAOn.1.v~v, t:rtE:l aJK tlJt 1J11nt-mz~
v .214 at,·tlx'
bt£1. T'
(cp.
E£t\lOV6rl6&~""'
Od.I.120
Ues. 'l'b.
~-
520, 904,
914; Erg.
tY)'U&L 6t:
amc
~ L\.OC, t:rtE: l
l"ICMAOl Coa.vc1vti:>£c;fn.itTE+>OVf:CJ 310 OcAx&. ~ tt tt~ tt q,l~ K¥tP 324 at,TlJO. 6i: ~ trr.,x£ro to6&oc; ~Hes. Th. 726 -rovntp1. 'X,O.\xEov~ ~m1. • 6;a.lpl 6t µ1.v ~ 884 rulnc ~1.v ·0AU1,1t1.ov£q:,uorn Zflv Erg. 795 i&tc,t,n" ~ 6t tt ~N:l )(Q.l ECAlTTDEnc; fA.Ll«JC ~ 176
Parm.
1. 25 [ra,:o1.c;ml
v lka\.Wv flµfn:pov
OE ~I.
8. 1o u:nE+>OVf\ n;x5a6£v, 'tOO ~ ~, 56 )(.(lt)lc;an' ~, 14 .1
~
&;,
qilv:
utv q>,\oybc;atat+>1.ov ~
'WK'tl.qm;c;11:E+)l 'YQ'i:av6.:¼J,£vov ~1.ov
~
Cp. Em.p. 45.1:
KUHAO"tE+)tc; TtEPl 'YQtav tAlCXJEm1.~1.ov
~
104.).
89
THE DACTYLICHEXAMETER Emp. 62.2 tvwx.louc; ~ 6.vi'r;aye HP'-~
&'le; at i:6-t' tv ~v1.yf;1.v
84.7
nOp
tE:PYlJfvov ~1.av
nOp
88. 1 ( ••• )ii.la. ylyve:m.1. ~~ 61,. Arat.
408 eaKOV I tofl>.f+)6.vnc; ~
ti 6t x1.l,lllP11C,I ti 6'
ouo'Ca.y1'iPul;
6,pu:){; lftxl-CE+X>to~
96
RHY'IUM AND METRE
Arat.
I 6.y~
11'>GlO.~
Emp. 9 .2
43 1J£t.Ol'tp,J
y6p
I r6::a
11'>GlD.~
Yt\oc
01:pcxix.U1.yy1.
lU+)t.~i:a.t.
also
(cp. Id. I.19
Theocr.
A similar
4.2.1.2.
ro Y6o 6t'i, I ~L,
cUM
explanation
of word-end at pos.
rence
total trl - monos. Here
seems to apply
to
the
6£l6£,
~y&'
limited
occur-
4a:
II.
Od.
Hes.
Arat.
Call.
Apoll.
Theocr.
18.1% 8.9%
14.7% 8.2%
12.9% 6.3%
9.29% 4.43%
8.4% 3.4%
9.6% 6.5%
15.2% 9 %
identical
with
also
sequence
m ta;a,vt.~
80)
the
preceding
which
rhythmic
may fill
the
sequence
hexameter
is
from
the
the
dihaeresis
bucolic
onwards. In fallowing account
this
case word
for
the
however does
not
fact
avoidance
of
word-end
(4.2.1.1);
see below.
no metron
dihaeresis
begin
a
that
thia
after
a
in
falling
tendency long
is
syllable
occurs
(so
manner)
-
which
strong
than
less
occurring
that
in
the may the
pos.
4
As far as may be deduced from O'NEILL'• tables, in the case of word-end at pos. 4a in most of the samples studied the proportion of trochaic caesurae is even larger than it is in general (cp. Table most clearly by the proportion of VII above). This is illustrated word-types beginning with a single short syllable which reach from pos. 4a to the caesura; it may be expected that the proportion of trochaic caesurae is yet a little larger if monosyllables (preceded or) followed by a bisyllabic word are taken into account ( lul-t I or u- ltf I").
u.l v-t,
JI.
Od.
Hes.
Arat.
Call.
Apoll.
Theocr.
45.45% 54.55%
36.94% 63.06%
51.82% 48.18%
37.5% 62.5%
20.59% 79.41%
27.14% 72.86%
48.15% 51.85%
63.22%
50.7%
of trochaic (Compare the percentage 57.55% 45.8% 56.2% tro.c.
caesurae: 51.5% 71.8%
I
71.5%.)
Therefore, in some of the samples studied, a trochaic caesura seems to be considered as a 'mitigating factor' as far as the occurrence of word-end at pos. 4a is concerned: it appears that the clausular may be considsequence, disfavoured at the beginning of the verse, ered less objectionable when the following word (or word-group) falls in with the rhythmical movement produced by the caesura.
97
nIE DACTYLIC HEXAMETER
Of the words ending 60% is bisyllabic: II. Od. lu
71.95%
60.67%
pos.
at
excluded),
4a (monosyllables
over
Hes.
Arat.
Call.
Apoll.
Theocr.
66.67%
80.39%
91.18%
67.69%
74.44%
- in other words, over 60% follow a dihaereais after the first metron. Therefore the exceptional occurrences of word-end at poa. 4a may partly be accounted for by the tendency to avoid two consecutive 107 the proportion of the sevinstances of matron dihaeresis ; compare eral word-types following such a dihaeresis after the first metron (monosyllables excluded) 10 •: l.luul.lu 20. 38%
.luu
9.81%
..l_
.luvJ .l _J. ..l
:
--e
2.26% 24.53% 22.26%
(see
4.2.1.1)
11.32%
.luu-t7
9. 43%.
For extensive quantifications concerning the incidence of word-end in this part of the verse in the Hellenistic hexameter, see BONARIA (1977) - whose figures are, unfortunately, not very useful for our purpose owing to a considerable difference in approach to the subject. Examples
of
this
comparatively
infrequent
positioning
of word-end
are:
l,li'iv1.v&:uSE:, I &d, nrwit~ •Ax.1.~ Hes. Th. 30 JO.L}JOI. OKflrcrpc,v I t.oov ~ tt:>1.~ 6Cov Parm. 6.1 XPilTO Atye:1.vTE I VOEtv 't 1 tov ~. fun yap e:t'VC11. 8. 2 7 fun v dvapxov I&nucnov, bt£ t y~q: >0.t 6.\£~ Ar at • 113 ~.:a, ndvta. I T'QX tx.e: AL'H.Tl , &.Jre: 1.~ 61.>0.Cwv Apoll. 1.9 XEl.l.lEPLOI.O I lxe:~ Kl.llv 61.a T'tOClOl'V ·A~ Hom. Il.
4.2.2. 8a)
1.1
Word-end after occurs
very
the
first
infrequently
short
of the
('Hermann's
fourth
Bridge':
metron see
(at
MAAS(1962)
§87).
totaJ "I - monos.
pos.
II.
Od.
Hcs.
Arat.
Call.
Apoll.
Thcocr.
4.2% 1.4%
3.3% 0.6%
6% 2.5%
6.25% 1.56%
3.4% 0.3%
2.7% 0.7%
5.2% 1.1%
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
98 This
seems to be motivated
tendency
unless
the
second
short
word such a word-end sis
(which
occurs
86 above),
are
comparatively
(see
metron
following
not at
3.2.3,
more marked pos.
8a the
u6u-u/ -1-v apply
•
pp. the
caesura
v-u
1a not
at pos.
Od.
Hes.
45.45%
45.45%
13.33% 13.33%
caesura:) 45.45% 9.09%
45.45%
13.33%
9.09%
( trochaic caesura:) tf'-fp 60% (penthemimeral
Table
Vlll,
p.
functional
by means of which
favoured
in the third
to be avoided hand,
occurs: the
this
of the
notably
monosyllables
ending
is
other
dihaere-
above.)
- appears
On the
(lit
reckoning
see
a rhythmically
- which
above)
caesura.
word-type
II.
tfv--b
linked
when a trochaic
Not
of the bucolic
(See 3.4.
that
by a monosyllabic
of a word-boundary
74/5
to the word-types
.1--fp
rare.
circumstance
occupied
preceding
most strongly
metron
is
a realization
the occurrence
are
by the
+ 60% of the hexameters:
in
monosyllables
In addition,
metron
precludes
while
word-boundary two metra
of the
mainly
less
in
the
tendency
is
words ending frequent
following
than
percentages
8a:
Arat.
Call.
Apoll.
Theocr.
55.55%
33.33%
50%
40% 20%
22.22%
16.67% 50%
12.5%
40%
22.22%
(rest)
.l"
Therefore
the
similarity
pressed
too much; at
ceding
sequence
apparently preferred
clausula,
is
one of
ron
( ... -vl ... ),
4a,
with
because
which
of the
the
the
the
(1)
it
and
(2) it
at
main point
clausula; would it
characteristics
where
bridges
has
4a and 8a should
is
at
the the
produce favoured
structural
identity
not
is
infrequent
realization
the in
function
of
rhythmical the
be
of the pre-
word-end
8a
frustrate would
37.5%
effect
preceding as
the
the met-
trochaic
caesura. In Nonnus, word-end at 8a never occurs (KEYDELL(1959) 35*). Ac.cording to LUCIC (1973) the phenomenon is practically absent from Hellenistic poetry, and from later Greek poetry as a whole. (Cp. MAAS (1962) S92.) This fact however cannot be deduced from O'NEILL's tables.
THE DACTYLIC HEXAMETER
99
Exceptions are, e.g. Hom. Il. VI. 2 ~ 6' 6p'
~ MQLf\18' C8uaEI id.XTITtE:6lo1.o
Tt£TtOV, ~ ~, -cln 6t I oo xfi6E:o.1.oCr~ Hom.Hymn.II • 1 7 Noo1.ov llu Tt£6lov -cfl~ [ &of TT.OA.UE,tYl,l,JV 55
~
I 1,.1£~1.ac; WVEI.6t I )(Q"tQ ~1.V
186 alrtq> anb ~ o>..lyav
Arat.
572 OOVE:I. utv E-ctqavoc;,
Theocr.
KE
'Ix.~
oOrtao. v1.>eaa£i:Cll', ou5' EC n ! no8o1.c -cuy' 6£l&..Jv) IX. 5 }Jl'l6tv ~n J.,aYEAru\ftEC• tii.Lv 6t I w ~1.o.C£u
Id. (VIII.10
Word-end after a long realization of the fourth metron (at pos. 8) occurs 4.2.3.
of the double-short element infrequently, especially in
the
see HAAS (1962)
Hellenistic Bridge': hexameter ('Naeke's also 93ff.); see also Table VIII above. II. 12.3% total LI - monos. 5.8%
192;
Od.
Hes.
Arat.
Call.
Apoll.
Theocr.1oe
9.6% 4.6%
10.1% 3.3%
6.08% 2.6%
1.1% 0.2%
2% 1%
5.2% 1.5%
This tendency may be interpreted as the avoidance of the rhythmical effect characteristic of verse-end, namely of dihaeresis after a 110 See also 4.2.1.1. above. metron which is realized by two syllables. the majority of the exceptions Not reckoning monosyllables, this tendency belong to one of two word-types:
.u ~
.....
II.
Od.
Hes.
Arat.
Call.
Apoll.
Theocr.
27.59% 62.07%
39.13% 60.87%
51.52% 30.3%
43.33% 56.67%
50% 50%
20% 70%
53.33% 40%
10%
6.67%
to
(contrast:) uvl .fl_.l
8.62% 1.n%
(Sc. percentage
12.12% 6.06%
of word-end after
Thus one may note that begin at the trochaic meral caesura data,
(and,
more frequently
L.)
words which end on a long syllable caesura
more frequently
in so far as this
in pos. 8
than at the penthemi-
can be deduced from O'NEILL's
than may be accounted
for by the frequency
the trochaic caesura in general). This might indicate that way of integrating the metrical groups at the caesura makes the quasi-verse-final word-boundary less objectionable.
of
the best position
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
100 E.g. Hom. Il.I.2
f\ IJl.¥)l' ·Axcu.oCclc!V.vE' f6rpc.£
~v,
~6u£vov
III.22
npcrQX>1,&v
vn·.::ov b.,ai.MJOLV,
~A.CUI~
BLaavto.
6' I~~ 160 l,Ol6' tn.u:vTE:kUa7L T'6rtloa.>j T1fh,n .\l1101. ro 62
t~ 1VlYOOL~ oonouc:x:,c 611Tl'P006Ev 4PE'n I xdNc£ovfvxoc ot 6' tn.Et bcd;cq:,8£.v ~lAOUI&pfixOncuv ~ .6L6c; Stmi"tl'IP KU6lomI'l'PL -roytvE; LO.
197 dpvt:i.4)
311 340 IV. 515 Hes.
More surprisingly which -
is
the
would also
of -
tent
word-type frequent.
these so
that
cases
It also
would be a realization
of
occurrences
involve
here
I Srw.,~
Y\J\O.L~
consisting
two successive
relatively
prepositive boundary
the
produce
majority
he,,(£
µLV
b. -cficy6p y~
Th. 590
6qt.\.).£1,
two long
a bisyllabic
of the
however,
word preceded
rhythmically (trochaic)
-
of metron-dihaeresis
may be suspected, the
syllables
relevant
that by a word-
caesura.
E.g.
6'rl, tatvE:A.oc 6t HCl8'tTD"liJV I &.\TO')(Cll.&1C£ 176 1:p'.ix'q;, bt£l ~ TE HCl.l b::J&>i1N I YOU\O.T'~ 249 6U.' dvt: 611~e• b.i>'tTD"liJV, I 1,.1116tl,IOL oem,,, Emp. 100 .1 i&sE:6' ~ t mvttl HCl.lbmvE'C• I n&:1LAL(IXU.l,IOL V.111 G:cc!tp'
Hom. Il.
or,
with
penthemimeral IV. 384 M'
Hom. Il.
caesura: aiJT' 6:yy£.\lnv tnl 'l\JE,f\ I an:t>av
v.11a eq; at. -et µ' Parm.
dv6p:J.
t>.E:tvHO.ltat. T' 6rt' atcilvof; >OC.bv~ I 6.vnqJE:plCEL
V.183
Hom. Il.
Hes. Th.
More exceptional Hom. Il. Apoll.
types
are,
1.a.
fi6t l,O')(TK; kC.ll.lat'&LPl'leI avnl3o).fl(XlL 510 '>.pyEloL~, bt£l oO oPL .\l80(; ~ I o(£,t ol~
IV. 342 ~ III.415 758
~L
'tEU)(Tp't'ijoL
c5tioc· n:,uc;6' I aiJT6L
fit TTCUt:v ~ xtxumL,
oat~
ti 6' I~ HO.l ~
THE DACTYLIC HEXAMETER Some of
the
101
(comparatively
metic hexameters
frequent)
examples
(see note 61 ad 2.4.3.2.1.
from Theocritus'
mi-
above) are:
I XPftln H0.\6v "CL (also Id. XV.23 ~ -tbv •Aail,JLv• 6xouc..>
26)
6J'ltl3o."COL •1m0"tO M.£V £[TID.LC -rovlLL)O(.QVI naro5£ kJaotcn 62 >d;ULO,W, na.Uxo1JV TtEL~ Sriv I Tldvta. TEA£i:mL 1 ~, 74 xns; ~ ~ ...m, Q)LA I tv ~ £[~ (compare 73:( ••• ) Mi:xxL, 'YU\QL• I tv kaA4> d]Jtc;.)
38 ~ WO."tQ y'4,av 42 ~,. tp.,yt:ci,
0
etc.
etc.
See HAAS(1962) 94: "the mime section (1-95) [sc. of Id. XV) actually affects the break which is usually avoided". Archestratus, as was to be expected, shows the largest proportion of exceptions XI.4
to this
tendency (see Table VIII above);
t\ n:cpl U1V•Ao'TlV
TTOU a.)4
e.g.
yflv 11\TtEPl ~TT'lV
,:b ydp W't'LV I vbnt¥X>s; dv8os; XXII. 4 t\ou:Ju', 01\,\' tc; 0&.p UOVOV tvaECs; I 'HOl.8tq,n )(L 'VG':Jv XXIIl.5 d un yNJ'JJ'HbvUru.ov. bt£Loav 6 1 I 6rrni ytvrrta.L XV.4 6£Lvi} 6n' ~twV·
11 61J£6' tn, a.v3p:xKL1's; ~ "COU"tOLc I un T1POOEVEYHtiN 20 -rova:xiov ~~ U£-c' txe,voo I ro.fuvop,CELV etc. etc. (Apparent) exceptions to this tendency in Callimachus discussed by MORELLI(1964); see also MINEUR(1984) 38.
have
According to KEYDELL(1959) 35* Nonnus' hexameters of word-end after a long syllable in pos. 8.
no example
4.2.3.1.
The theory brought forward by Wernicke (see MAAS(1962)1125)
which holds position
contain
been
that
a word-final
syllable
has been convincingly
refuted
in pos. 8 may not be long by by DEVINE& STEPHENS(1976).
This "law", which is held by various scholars to apply to other verse-positions as well (HAAS, l.c.) seems to derive support from the quantifications of syllable types published by IRIGOIN (1965). These data indicate that the syllable structure of long syllables differs according to their occurrence in a marked or in a non-marked verse-position. (IRIGOIN's data are presented as corroborative evidence for the quantitative conception of dAovos;: see sub 2. above.) taking into account (rhythmical) DEVINE & STEPHENSshow that, preferences such as the avoidance of word-end after a long syllable in pos. 8, such differences with regard to the structure of long
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
102
eyllablee in the several veree-po■ itione are completely accounted for by other (non-durational) factor ■: namely pronn.tnee, i.e. "the frequencies with which the syllables of the longum and the biceps respectively come from varioue position• in different word shapes", and poait:ional frequency, i.e. "the frequencies of the ayllable types ( ••• ) in thoee positions of those word-shapes 0 (1976) 156. (It may be noted that DEVINE& STEPHENS'calculations have the aerit of avoiding the comon fallacy of circularity: whilst deriving the quantifications of word-types from a poetic text (Hoaer), a prose sample (Thucydides) is called upon to provide the concrete words, and thus the types of heavy syllables involved. Compare also STIFLER (1924). 4.2.4.
Word-end after
a long syllable
in pos. 10 (virtually)
never'''
occurs. Because of the general rhythm of the verse the fifth
tendency
towards
metron is in itself
its
to preserve
close,
the identity
a bisyllabic
exceptional
(see 2.3.
of the
realization
of
& Table III,
p.
40 above). Not surprisingly
dihaeresis
after
a metron realized
lables - which is characteristic of verse-end - is in the position preceding the verse-final metron.
by two syl-
strongly
avoided
Consequently, in view of the comparative scarcity of a verse-final monosyllabic word, a 'spondaic' verse practically always ends on a word which consists of three long syllables at least: ( ... }---41; in virtue of the preference for the bucolic dihaeresis, the 'spon1a realized in most cases by a word of four long daic' clausula syllables: see 2.3. above.
TIIE DACTYLICHEXAMETER 5. 1beories ily
concerning
remain
has or
of
which
developed that
is
verses
(or
of
verse-f
oru).
Moreover,
as
we know it
one might
a compound consisting cola
'originally'
some of
its
type of theory.
the
dark.
nor
can
into
doubt
whether
( ••• ), left
any
in
being, by
it
necessar-
provide
argue
either
it
developed
the
Greeks
seems certain traces
information that
the
type
of verse,
isosyllabic two originally
deriving
from
appear
to
by verse
independent
other,
i.e.
lyric
be incompatible
with
(See VAN RAALTE& SICKING (1980).)
completely
borrowed
hexameter
not
of
features
33-53.
caae
does
convincingly
Cp. HOEKSTRA(1981) are
of the dactylic
from a Proto-Indo-European
it
either
the origin
speculative.
The hexameter means
103
We do not it
be
from
origin
some
of the
hexameter
we
know how, when and where
established
from cola
that
in
"About the
beyond
of Indo-European mediterranean
a
reasonable
origin people.
the
earliest
narrative
poetry
Homer was
already
composed
in
it
or was However that
has
hexameters."
( 53). On
(1980) period
linguistic grounds suggest that there onwards.
especially, PAVESE {1980) and has been a continuity from the
The theory of the hexameter as developed has been advocated by e.g. O'NOLAN {1969); VIGORITA (1977); BERG (1978); TICHY (1981). 112 For the theory that independent) verses or GENTILI (1981). VON WEBER (1955) concerned, lyric ver.sa. is
from isosyllabic NAGY (1974),
HORROCKS Kycenean verse (1979);
the hexameter is a compound of (originally GENTILI & GIANNINI (1977); cola, see i.a.
points out that, as far as literary poets are dependent on epic verse,
imitation not vice
CHAPTER III: THE IAMBIC TRIMETER
Editions Except
104
used where indicated
occurring
in the iambic
upon computations
otherwise, trimeter
made for
the
the quantifications presented occasion.
in this
of phenomena study are based
The material
taken
into
account cocprises: Archilochus
ed. M.L. WEST(Iambi et elegi graeci Vol. I) Oxford 1971 & ed. D.L. PAGE (SupplementWI lyricis graecis fr. 478) Oxford 1974 (• SLC)
Ser.ionides
ed. M.L. WEST ( Iambi et 1972
elegi
graeci
Vol.
II)
Oxford
Solon
ed. M.L. WEST ( Iambi et 1972
elegi
graeci
Vol.
II)
Oxford
Aeschylus Septem, Agamemnon ed. D. PAGE, Oxford 1972 (OCT) Sophocles oedipus Rex ed. R.D. DAWE,Leipzig Philoctetes id., 1979 Euripides
lledea
Orestes
Aristophanes
ed. A. ELLIOTT, Oxford 1969 ed. W. BIEHL, Leipzig 1975 (BT)
Nubes
Bquites
Henander Dyscolus Aspis Slmia
Lycophron Alexandra Anthologia
1975 (BT)
ed. K.J. DOVER,Oxford 1968 ed. V. COULON,Paris 1 1964 (Bud~)
ed. E.W. HANDLEY,London 1965 ed. F. SISTI, Edizioni dell'Ateneo id., 1974 ed. L. MASCIALINO,Leipzig
Graeca ed. H. BECKBY MUnchen 1957-58
1971
1964 (BT)
THE IAMBICTRIMETER 1. As to its rising
basic
rhythm the iambic trimeter
single-short
with a blunt
105
sequence
close.
are recurrent
as a
by means of prolongation
and
Within the sequence,
marked verse-positions there
extending
is to be regarded
can be occupied
the first
by a long syllable,
groups of four elements X--i,-
t
X--i,-
t
of every two non-
(three
so that
metra):
X--iJ-11,
The metron, then, is a structural unit and as such has a rhyththat "metron mical function. SCHEIN ( (1979) 2-3) is wrong in stating boundaries are arbitrary"; "(metra) are inventions of ancient metricians"; or that "the metron is a name given to a sequence of syllables, but this sequence comprises a unit neither of composition nor of appreciation". On the contrary, the iambic metron is constitutive of the metric profile of the trimeter, whose organization is perceived as underlying each concrete verse. The fact that rhythmic boundaries preferably span metron-boundaries, far from indicating the fictitiousness of the metron, surely testifies to its reality and its significance. The conception of ancipitia outlined above may have the advantage of il!lparting transparency to the metaphorical notion of "verseweight" (see HARRISON(1914)). No doubt, the idea that a verse 'gains weight' according to the number of anceps elements realized as long syllables is due to the habit of referring to short and long syllables as "light" and ''heavy" respect! vely. The use of this metaphor gives rise to all kinds of implausible calculations. For instance, in the comic trimeter the large proportion of long syllables in pos. 9 would "seem to serve as a make-weight against the trisyllable feet" (HARRISON(1914) 209). A more realistic interpretation seems to be that the large proportion of long syllables in this position makes it easier to perceive the structure of the verse by metrical groups of four elements. In comedy, the perception of the as a result metrical grouping is likely to be endangered especially of the high incidence of deviations from the underlying metric profile (i.e. resolutions: see 2.1. below). Likewise a verse which shows short syllables in all three anceps positions is perceptually different from a verse with three long ancipitia not because it is "less heavy", but rather in virtue of a high de~ree of 'iambicity', or conformity to the extending singleshort rhythm which basically constitutes the metric profile of the verse. (See Ch.I, 2.3. p. 12 above.) 1.1.
The ancipi tia
iambic syllable applies
trimeter than
all as
in
the
put
together
a short
in an increasing
as compared to the early
three one.
relevant
verse-posi
are lllore often (See Table
proportion
I,
to trimeters
iambographers,
Uons
realized p.
of
the
as a long
108 below.)
of the tragic
This poets
and to comedy as compared to
106
RHYTHH ANDMETRE
tragedy; anceps the
in Lycophron the highest positions
found (67%).
is
comparatively
high
percentage
percentage (It of
of long
syllables
may be noted
that
resolutions
in
in
in comedy the
anceps
positions is not arrived at at the expense of the large proportion of long syllables.) In Solon and in the Hellenistic epigram, there is
a relatively
positions;
large
see 1.1.1.
amount of short and 3.4.1.
syllables
occupying
anceps
below.
In the case of a random distribution of syllables one would expect to find a proportion of, roughly, 60% long and 40% short syllables. This proportion is based upon a computation of the number of long and short syllables occurring in a specimen of Greek prose, chosen with a view to a minimum of artificiality, viz. Lysias Or. Ill ( A9aln.st S1aon). The text comprises 4844 syllables, 2862 (or 59.08%) of which are long and 1982 (or 40.92%) are short. 113 See Appendix, p. 394 below. In so far as this specimen may be considered representative, the proportion of 3/5: 2/5 can be taken as a starting-point for the calculation of binominal distribution in a random sample. If we take a sample of 12, i.e. the number of elements that constitute a iambic of long/short trimeter (as we must do in order to get the probability syllables in the anceps positions, given the number of three brevla) the arithmetic mean of short syllables within thh sequence of 12 will be 2/5 x 12 • 24/5 • 4.8. So with random distribution the amount of short syllables would exceed the number of three, and we should expect to find short syllables at 1.8 of the other places as well (which corresponds to 60% of the anceps positions, or a mean of 0.6 short syllable per anceps position). Leaving aside the occurrence of resolution - which introduces another complication in that it adds to the number of syllables in the verse - the percentage of short syllables in the anceps positions appears to be rather low, even in the trimeter of the iambographers, Aeschylus and Sophocles. This justifies the assucption that the proportion of long/short syllables in the anceps positions is not simply predictable on the ground of the nature of the linguistic material involved.
lllE IAMBIC TRIMETER
107
e N.B. As far
as
the
between
the
samples
from
realization
presentation the
to the following totals
I)
under 2) within
are
ancipitia total
is
concerned,
numbers
studied
should
and the
the
choice
observation
be made with
of
due regard
circumstances: available
only
in
the
case
of
some of the
authors
discussion; the
different
Therefore,
for
which,
course,
the
quantifications
upon
observations
III
of
material
found whose grounds of
of
the
quantifications are
not apparent.
of
completeness
sake
crucial
is
of
available
in
ancipitia
made for
the
divergences
are
and maximum consistency
questions
of
relative
presented
in
this
occasion
(but
frequency
study
compare
-
are
note
-
baaed
ad Table
below).
Totals
are
provided
SCHEIN (1979): WHITE (1912)
by, or can be gathered
iambographers 11 nt. 15 Aeschylus 19 nt. 7 + 78 Table Sophocles 36 nt. 4 + 78 Table : Aristophanes 38 + 44 Henander 58 + 60
The Tables
provided
pose since
trimeters
by DESCR0IX (1931) including
In view of the tween
Euripides'
from:
evident
earlier
resolutions difference
and his
the Hedea and to the Orestes
are
later given
46/9 are
XXX XXXI
useless
for
this
pur-
in some respects
be-
are disregarded. apparent plays,
the
data
relating
to
separately.
In the case of Archilochus trimeters occurring in epodic alternahave been included; compare SCHEIN (1979) 6 nt. 3. (Sept., ~g.), Sophocles Samples are used in the case of Aeschylus (Phil.,OR), Euripides (Hed.,or.) and Aristophanes (Nub., Bq.); the sample counted for Menander comprises the trimeters in his Asp., sam. and Dl)SC. tion
With the exception of some verses by the early iambographers mutilated in the three final syllables, only complete verses have been taken into account (i.e. non-mutilated verses, or verses that have been completed by the editor of the text concerned to his own satisfaction).
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
108
TABLEI REALIZATION OF ANCIPITIA* long syllables
shon syllables
two shon syllables
ArchiJ. Sem. Solon
53.03% 51.15% 45%
46.97% 48.85% 55%
-
Aesch.
43.55% 41.39% 42% 38.75%
0.48% 0.49% 0.2% 3.25%
1324 2251
Or.
55.97% 58.12% 57.8o/s 58%
Aristoph. Men.
61.61% 59.61%
31.13% 34.1%
7.26% 6.29%
1436 1615
67.01% 49.69%
32.97% 49.49%
0.02% 0.82%
1474 163
Soph. Eur. Med.
sample size 66
174 40
1019 1107
-
Lye. Epigr.
* i.e.
the percentage of ancipitia in poss. 1, 5 & 9 all put together, which are realized as a long syllable, a short syllable or two short syllables.
THE IAMBICTRIMETER 1.1.1.
The distribution
anceps Table
positions 11
In
in
for
centual
long
the
ranje
long
a
over
remarkable
at
of
syllables
the
several
regularity.
outnumber
in Semonides,
syllables
the
long
of
epigram,
See
and
the
short
ones
who shows a relative
beginning short
Solon)
of
the
syllables
frequently what
in
see
choliambic
verse,
also
the
pref-
verse,
in
the
per-
5 is
position
in
1.1.
Chapter
IIIA
syllables
lables
is
decreasing.
anceps
is more often
So in addition
pying
anceps
different
1.1.1.1.
ring
in
the
phrase that
anceps
grounds.
As indicated
urked)
verse-positions
syllables,
can iambic
early
iambographers
the than
anceps
large
total
cp.
also
and
1.1.1.
outnumber
the
long
though,
the
as 'feet').
frequency
general,
the (see the
is
the
Ch.I,
positions
the
situation
sub
structured Obviously,
In trag-
in
this
posi-
number
of
short
syl-
in pos.
9 also,
syllables
occu-
distribution
over
for.
a linguistic above),
example seems reas-
it
of long
syllables
is
explained
on
to
be
ancipitia,
interpreted
distribution
serving
to
into
groups
the
of
prevent
marking
the
syllable.
percentage
free as
1.2.1.
in
ones
unequal
as
(This
of _short
below.)
of long
verse
sub 1. above, with
a
(esp.
one.
amount
to be accounted
iambic
high
the
much more
as a long
1.1.
sub
needs
be conceived
verse-profile
( u- :
the
1 above;
to the relative in
in
within
varies
as a long than as a short
realized
verse-positions
to suppose
syllables
syllable
plays,
When we consider
onable
comedy and
In comedy and in Lycophron
positions
of a rhythmical
in
for - the
Table
later
long
epigram
a short
and
Euripides'
tragedy,
the amount is even larger.
hand,
accounts
short
of
Hellenistic
as
principally
ancipitia:
tion;
other
the
realized
amount
in
in Lycophron
9, on the and
Archilochus,
the
of 10% (58-68%); In pos.
the
5 the
trimeter
Hellenistic
the
shows
syllables
negligible.)
In
edy
verse
(Though
difference
almost
the
1 and
exception.
erence
is
of long and short
P• 113 below.
poss.
without
in
109
long
occur-
rhythmical as
(non-
and
short
a perception
of -two elements off
in
sets
of
of each four
110
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
elements
each
realized
as a long
realization
is
most
syllable,
as being
of the anceps
The largest two
accomplished
proportion
of
positions;
the
iambography
(+80%),
where
observed
apply
istic
epigram,
because
the
most
clearly
long
syllables.
them
in
occurs the
situation
first
than
in
pos.
basic
9)
single-short
in
tragedy
tions to
in
the
different
accomplish
the the
some extent short
in
the
in the Hellenistic In comedy, comedy
also
realizations
in
pos.
relatively epigra~:
however,
a
larger
is
this
found
in long
positions,
the
atively
large
proportion)
in a far
tendency
proportion: 9 and high 3.4.1
the
does the
9 than distributed
more even manner.
occur
close.
trimeters
the
115
of early
of long with
realiza-
the
at
tendency
poa.
the
10 (see
seem to large
be
of word-end
pos.
to
proportion at
9) in Solon and
below. not
apply.
Although
(comparatively in
9 on the
movement preferably
at
, p. 207 f.
the
9 to be observed
two devices
reaiizations are
pos.
low incidence
situation
also
in pos.
word-end
incidence
of
(cp.
of
its
compare
the
and fewer
to emphasize
sum total
the
long
towards
with
of
two
of the
realizations
short
Hellen-
with
the
single-short
fact,
pos.
proportion
may be connected
proportion
anceps positions
of
begins
in
the
positioning
an inverse
syllables
10 (c.q.
pos.
recurrent
level
are
accomplished
syllables
as compared
to rising
As a matter
below).
3.4.
return
the
is
see below.
syllables
authors)
also
1, apparently
positions,
of the sequence
15-18% of
early
poets,
short
of long
in
position
a rhythmical
va.
(23-26.5%
by means of
of
proportion
large
anceps
44-55% of
reflect
and in Lycophron
iambography
of
f1 rst
in pos.
two anceps
For the collie
character
The increasing
a smaller
second
the
as in
elements
poets
first
comedy,
seems to
by four rhythmic
frequency
(outside
occurs
its
in the
in Lycophron).
hand
at
tragic
in the
The comparative other
proportion
in
tendencies
as well
is
114
especially
Here,
anceps
more salient
occurs
rhythmical
strictly.
verse
In the
realizations
is
of grouping
when the
rhythmically
realizations
other
largest
perception
long
when the
verse-position.
proportion
fairly
the
the
as a non1arked
anceps to
effectively
either
of
(of which there over
the
few) the
in
short
other
two
is a comparthree
anceps
THE IAMBIC TRIMETER
( vid.
4.1.
sub
subject
to
other
4.4.
of
iambic
intact
ondly,
diverse
the
nature
four
elements.
its
long
realizations
as
the
predominance
by metrical ize
groups
a clausular For
anceps
the
1.1.1.2. tice
a
tion
on
the
several
positions.
realization
is
syntactical
phrasing:
Semonides'
trimeters
AB Tables ively
high at
these
5 (or
circumstances realize
the )
to real-
rhythm. in the different
from
the
common prac-
non-significant
of
rhythmical
requires
devia-
preferences
more detailed
with
a
favourite
realization
of
the
anceps
informa-
pos.
Semonides' at pos. only', to
(2)
5 preferably
word-end
p. 114 below).
1.1.1.
below.
below).
be a fairly
at
general
by a
namely
IIA,
of 5 in
show a relat-
3.1.3.2.
circumstances,
(( ••• ) .l 1-J- ( ••• )) or
pos.
7 not accompanied
than a short
5 only
the
manner in
trimeters
more often
in other
in
as an example.
appears
in
of
a particular
connected
anceps
Ch. IIIA,
tendency
for
occurs
number
patterning
preference
( ...
choliambus,
the
the
the
'hephthemimeres
(( ••• ) .lj-JI ): see Table
clear
some cases
of word-end
rhythmic
over
of
can be interpreted
occurring
anceps
~-o
large
word-types
there
amount and sec-
of
(i.e.,
the
terms
XIV and XVI show,
pos.
below)
realizations
however
may serve
percentage
as a sequence
frequency
the
serve,
comparatively
statistically in
this
obviously
the
of groups
divergences
of for
In
in
p. 138 f. below.
margin
comparative
likewise
as consisting
single-short
individual
in question;
2.
sub
to keep each
not
by the large
positions
of double-short
may be accounted author
elements
'basic'
to
In addition,
the
end
motivation
see 2.1.3.3,
(exceeding
tions)
of
proportion
anceps
word-end
observed
verse
endangered
the
of
features
of the
structure
the
of four
is
those
(see
in all
return
positions,
indeed
therefore,
the
as
profile
metrical
trimeter
characteristic
resolutions
In comedy,
of
its
is
positioning
comic such
metric
of
the
the
poetry:
which
to preserve
to
refinements
12 verse-elements, the
regard below),
rhythmical
to keep
and
to
with
and
genres
first, of
be seen
will
As
111
(1)
both
Cp. also
short
realization word-end pos.
by wordNow in tendency syllable of the at
pos.
5 and pos.
the situation
7 in
RHYTHMAND HF.TRE
112
This tendency applies without exception as compared to the other word-types adjacent to (penthemimeral or hephthemimeral) caesura. d--ill is also absolutely more frequent than -'--ol in all of the authors, Sophocles and Lycophron excepted. (Exceptions which, by the way, suggest that this tendency does not just reflect the situation that, in the Greek language, words shaped u-u outnumber those shaped • As a matter of fact, in the trimeter the rhythmic sequence v--u may be positioned in twice as many positions as --v.)
So the in
relatively
Semonides
preted
as
partly
caesura; deviate
from
5 not
appears
More the
Semonides
inclines
the
tragic
poets
both
pos.
5 and
ence, is
likely
lables
towards
to
in this
The high
percentage
other
hand,
seems to
of
'phrase-initial',
esp.
word-groups II
3.4.
above;
the
ad Table
frequency
tion
of
e.g.
by the
a high syllables.)
the
verse.
percentage
rather (That
situation
syllables this
than
of
long
syllable•
caesura
too,
more strongly
than
As
word-end
anceps
percentage
at
preferin
pos.
of short
5
syl-
degree. in pos. with
1 in
the
note
- which are often
(-rov6'
)
•:
may reflect a particular where but
is
in
long,
cp.
3.5.
not this
words 99
universal position
a low percentage
or
ad Ch.
either
by
below.
So
a preference rhythmical
on
occurrence
(1 .e.,
see 11
Semonides,
frequent
'monosyllables'
connexion
in Lycophron,
the anceps So it
the
verse-position:
or by position
at
Semonides'
of
high
be connected
XXI below)
of long
syntactical
particular
realization
elided
word-end one.
do.
infrequent
the
to
as a short
iambographers)
to a lesser
a single
(066' , 'ffl\l 6')
nature here
occupying
of
neces-
appears
7, to realize
colon-end
of long ancipitia
the
case
penthemimeral
single
5
be internot
trimeter
of
for
only
does
than
a short
position
pos.
relatively
be responsible
reverse the
pos.
hephthemimeral
more often
other
7 is
for
at
single-short
the
pos.
again,
here
case
in
in
could
single
Semonides'
by word-end
(and
epigram the
operating
syllable
syllables
for
shows,
significantly,
the
short
a preference
tendency,
in
of
Hellenistic
trimeter
accompanied
that,
the
reflecting
5 as a long
pos.
percentage
as in
as Lycophron's apply.
in
well
as
sarily pos.
high
for
a
realizais
shown
there
is
of mono-
J.I\OL&...
.i. J.
REALIZATIONOF ANCEPSPOSITIONS
pos.l long
2 shorts•
short
66.66%
% %% % %% % %%
•44.44%
33.33% 23.65% 28.16% 19.22% 40% 24.24%
% %% % Soph. %% Eur. Med. % %% Or. % %%
60.95% 36.3% •65.04% 37.3 % 63.4% 36.56% 59.71% 34.32%
37.69% •28.84% 33.59% 27.05% 31.01% •28.58% 31.35% 26.96%
% %% % %%
60.03% 32.48% 57.96% 32.41%
26.04% 27.89% 30.03% 29.36%
%
71.51% 35.57% 58.9 % 39.51%
28.49% •28.81% 38.65% 26.03%
Archil. Sem. Solon
Aesch.
Aristoph. Men.
Lye.
%% Epigr.
% %%
41.9 % 71.84% 46.82% 60%
% • percentRge %
0
of
pos. 5 long
short
63.64% 40% 50.57% 32.96% 55% 40.74%
36.36% 25.81% 49.43% 33.72% 45% 27.27%
64.65% 38.51% 66.37% 38.06% 70.07% 40.41% 68.02% 39.l %
35.35% 27.05% 33.59% 27.05% 29.93% 23.75% 31.71 % 27.27%
13.93% (13.58) 68.24% 63.9 % 36.92% 12.01% (11.76) 64.21% 35.91% 63.61%
27.79% 29.75% 32.76% 32.02%
-
1.36% 94.74% 1.38% 93.94% 0.59% 100% 8.94% 91.9 %
(0.6) (1.2) (0.29) (5.15)
2.45% (2.45) 100%
lEuvfl ~" I qx:>wlac~ 1392 YavELorTQJrrt6'; ~ I tv ~~Lv 1029 adal,tx,'~ i:bv I o(Jo' a.tax.w01,1:11. .... .., 568 ~ c:l',v -roe•OO"toC b c,cq:DCI OUKnOEo.i:dSE:; 612 'HCl.l u,v Tilt>' au-a;, el~, I ~ rw;totOV ~, X'(lt,x, I Zmq; ~,:pov >QXJ.. Tt4X>I; "'C0\6' txrrm.L, I yfpJc; ~ 'tb o6v ......
303 OLU!JVl'
Aristoph.
.....
1
4,efp.t.
1
"Al•lnc 6af;AlOH.OV,I e:tm OL 1.1'\vT' 6&:~v lJ£t'Q"tQY£ ... ... 238/9 tylVET' ~L rdv&' trol~, tb 61: tdxcc 153/4
lWV rrp:J.~ occurs
&voolouc ...... navta. 1a1..rrtl«ltwD::lu ...... Y£ >Gt ~L ......
~ 61; ~ YE lPL~1.6n:WJ.L
verse:
the
not
1156
of
that
rhythmic
IV A) and does
~, a«1m muw)(Qt Tbv ~
syllable the
2
by CHRIST (
w yt~;
~
preceding
of the anceps
The most
is
preceding
instance
V
928
as
verse-profile
occurs. Outside comedy, ,,o , except for poss.
in comedy are listed TACCONE(1904) 67.
Aristoph.
ized
occurs
seems to occur.
resolutions
pides,
the
of the
resolution
single
verse-positions
of
which
observation
108-11.
successive
The
that
where
SUTTON(1980)
XXIVff.;
they this
l,l&\E:I.
~I.
wtc
prefer
xtporo-1.v to avoid
suggestion
is
a falling in keeping
THE IAMBICTRIMETER with in
the
absence
other
low
should
be noted
syllables
pursued
poss.
apply
at
again
(see
anceps
both
is
pos.
5 and
4.4.
Law':
2.1.5.4,
in general,
where,
(Compare
verse-beginning
or)
7
below.) to be
155 below).
percentage
as compared
(preceded
the
pos.
seens
p.
the lower
1 and 5 in Henander
when the
VII)
degree.
of 'Porson's
double-short
Table
a rising/falling/rising
lesser
word-end
by Menander
that,
(see
of
to a far
of
especially in
not
tion:
It
of long
to Aristophanes
followed
by resolu-
compare 2.1.4.3. The relaxation
for
of this
a different
between
earlier
regulating
the
does
not
be attractive ber
of
of
a change
a pr1or1.
Indeed
the
resolution;
this
in
the
amount
of words
too'"
, which
likely
to
(poss.
5/6
and
that at
poss.
bridges
the
penthemimeral
is
in
accordance
because
various
realization
-
frustrates
2.1.4.4.
below.)
in
this the
being
to
position preferred
likely
to imply
an
increase
(or
involve
num-
may be a proportional consist
of their
tendency see
anceps
- which
is
realization
matter,
towards
in
because
it
since
it
pre-resolutional XXII);
preceding of
most
possibilities
former
that
mostly
are
objectionable
the
Table
a dactylic
shape
other
comparatively for
of)
very
(Both
- or,
the
of the
to
occurrence
93-100%; of
seeu
the
tendencies:
the
an explanation
increase
which
practice
practice
is
calls
difference
in his
verse,
the
verse-beginning.
with
long
observed
plays
with
caesura
amounts
resolution
begin
rhythmical
(which a
later
plays
a similar
significant
there
9/10)
later
rhythmical
in consequence
of
word-end
of
of word-types
consequence not
the
means that
be situated
not
of his
Euripides'
variety
that
is
movement
involve
of
sequence
plays
rhythmical
in
in Euripides'
Considering
and later
resolutions
extension
tendency
explanation.
his
which
end
alternation
of a falling
after
of
in comedy
XIV) and the non-observance
sought
of
the
percentage
T~e effect
does
tendency
also,
movement is
relatively (Table
of this
respects
rhythmical
141
the preceded
the
the
latter
occurrence by word-
clausula:
see
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
142
TABLE VII
REALIZATION OF ANCIPITIAPRECEDING RESOLUTION IN POS. 2 (as compared with the realization of the anceps irrespective of resolution*)
J
uu
.l
uu
Aesch.
85.71% 37.69%
14.29% 60.95%
Soph.
75.64% 33.59%
24.36% 65.04%
Eur.
39.96%
60.04%
M~d.*
Or.•
100% 36.01% 31.58% 31.35%
63.4%
68.42% 59.71%
Aristoph.
31.18% .. 26.04%
68.82% ... 60.03%
(cp. Nub. Eq. •
30.35%
69.81%)
Men.
31.52% JO.OJ%
68.48% 57.96%
* The general percentage of short/long ancipitia (as derived from the sample studied: see Table II above) has been added in italics. • (All) figures derived from sample studied. ••Proper names included.
TIIE IAMBICTRIMETER 2.1.4.3. is
In the
realized
eral;
this
seems
to
end at quent
applies
to
be connected
than
5 - which otherwise:
syllable
in pos. even
with
the
with
syllable
the
XXII,
more often
preceding
than
it
as to tragedy.
tendency
resolution
Table
6,
more often
comedy as well
see
as a long
II
Table
of resolution
by a long
pos.
realized
case
143
that
in
in pos. p.
6 is
case
it
is
situation of word-
even more fre-
230 below - the than
in gen-
is
This
the
anceps
anceps
in general:
is
see
A above.
TABLEVIII REALIZATION OF ANCIPITIAPRECEDING RESOLUTION IN POS. 6*
J
Aesch.
UV
76.74% 64.65%
Soph.
18.45% 33.59%
81.54% 66.37%
Eur. M~d. •
15.78% 29.93%
84.21% 70.07%
27.37% 31.71%
72.62% 68.02%
26.62% 27.79%
73.38% 68.02%
26.6% 32.76%
73.39% 64.21%
Aristoph. Men.
Vid.
~
23.25% 35.35%
Or.*
*,•,••
IJU
**
ad Table VII above.
Observation of a sample consisting of the first 400 trimeters of Euripides' Ned. and or. each shows that the anceps in pos. 5 is realin 74.84% and 74.41% of the occurrences of ized as a long syllable word-end at pos. 5 followed by a long syllable in the Ned. and or. respectively, whereas in the case of a resolution in pos. 6 (preis realized as a long syllable in ceded by word-end) the anceps
RHYTHMAND METRE
144
78.57% and 64.94% (of the occurrences of word-end at pos. 5 followed by resolution) respectively. The explanation of this difference would seem to require a more detailed observation of the word-types (and the caesura situation) involved, which in the case of these two plays however does not take us very far. This situation in itself is in accordance with the supposition that the difference between Euripides' earlier and later plays concern the frequency of resolution rather than the rhythmical movement of the verse (realization of the ancipitia, incidence of word-end); - contrast the situation in the comic trimeter, where the differences (i.e. from the tragic trimeter) concern both the frequency of resolution and the rhythmical movement of the verse. Study of a larger sample and of the vocabulary involved remains as a desideratwa. 2.1.4.4. of
In tragedy
resolution
lable.
in
the anceps preceding pos.
In comedy this
10 is
the infrequent
invariably
realized
occurrences
as a short
is not the case. TABLEIX
REALIZATION OF ANCIPITIA PRECEDING RESOLUTION IN POS. 10* ~
uu
JOO% 57.63%
Aesch.
57% Eur. Or.•
Men.
*,•,**
Vid.
uu
42.3%
1()()%
Soph.
Aristoph.
~
42.96%
100%
**
53.21%
46.25%
30.47% 39.55%
69.53% 56.55%
26.92% 39.5%
56.66%
ad Table VII above.
73.07%
syl-
145
'IltE IAH.BIC TRIMETER
This for
situation
word-end
after
before
a long
tragedy E.g.
than
in
thus
is
even
the
view
at
the
allowed
9 is
, P• 248ff.
in
to
realize
keep
absence
of
the
distinct
stronger
time
word-end
comedy but
below).
not
in
.....
...
&.q:n ~ov
in
anceps
the
the
in
of
this
9 as
pos.
metrical
case
resolution
in
grouping
resolution
by four
in
position
a long
(esp.
pos. in
10 Men-
- ......
Men. Dysc.
2.1.4.5.
In studied
enough
to
i:bv Iii.\Q." Hal 't'OOt'£\J8uc alrti;>lJE~I.
41
~
55
all'
Orestes, the
of
X),
particularly
ated
in one,
is
which
the
long
and
reap.
28
is
anceps
the
places
in several
this
other
only,
from position are apt
whereas
places
in
4 is
the large
resolutions
syllable.
uu-1 or uu- ...
shaped
two other
these
divergent in
tragedy
in pos.
than as a short ancipitia
~
only
succeeding
significantly
short
as words
( ... ) can be located 4
not
I J 61.6rt£p ~
number of resolutions
informative,
situation
- ... ..
6at1.'Hbv 'tij 6t.a-rp1.'3ij
as a long more often
proportion
or.
~,
OUKmtt.au'>.
in which
be
realized This
12
Euripides'
sample
E • g.
preference
\.£V Y\J\,QL~ Q\.IOJL0Ur;
1,U.d.;)
to
the
same
ander).
is
of
.. .. ...
tyw ~
tendency
(and
elements)
poe.
in
in
1302 o6t dv lJ£8€u,v. I] ci,e0· 'tL ll'
Eur. or. 518
syllable
while
's Law": see 4.4
("Porson
comedy the
understood
resolution,
syllable
Soph. Phil.
In
easily
is
the
general Table
(see
to be situ-
words shaped
uuu
in the verse.
6vE1.6LCw~ tolac,u 6' a&.xlav l,L£'VTL 6£i: MO.TilYQ:>Ei:v;
b "VOP l,llkOPU:>r; -
-
'KO(m
.,._
tvtEOSEv UYPLQ.ouvmxdc 'VCXlt> \IOO£L 3 7 iavlruaw. 6vot.de£1.v "VOP atooOi.n1. 6roc 34
In Aristophanes' proportion is
of
approximately
occurrence
Nubes & Equites
the divergence
long/short
ancipitia
following
as
as
proportion
of resolution.
large
the
is even smaller: resolution irrespective
in
pos. of
the 4 the
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
146
TABLEX REALIZATION OF ANCIPITIAFOLLOWING RESOLUTION IN POS. 4* uu
Aesch.
*
Vid.
( 1=) 33.33%
uu
J
( 2=) 66.67% •
64.65%
35.35%
Soph.
(10=) 66.67% 66.37%
( 5=) 33.33% 33.59%
Eur. Med.
( 4=) 57.14% 70.07%
( 3=) 42.86%· • 29.93%
Or.
76.19% 68.02%
23.81% 31.71%
Aristoph.
69.23% 68.24%
30.07% • • • 27.79%
Men.
78.24% 64.21%
21.76% 32.76%
ad Table
VII above.
• The two instances which account for are Sept.
.l
of short anceps following resolution in pos. 4 the divergent percentage in Aeschylus' trimeter
OMi>JJYlhv te+:>bv £UU£Vr\ na1.c:rvux,v .........
268
59 3 ea&: i:av cUoka 61.a. ~ ~ - which are also exceptional in that they are two of a total three trimeters with double resolution occurring in the septem.
of
**In the Nedea the instances of short anceps following resolution in pos. 4 are 375, 483, 1046, all three verses containing the word
m-rtt,n. ***One occurrence
• All figures observation
4
5
of uu uu ( • O. 7%).
in this (and in the following) table are based of the samples used elsewhere in this study.
upon
TiiE IAMBICTRIMETER In
comedy
words
of
the
possibility mical the
course
mark
metrical
resolution
compare first
grouping
in
the
often
the
four
so
verse-positions
a
accommodating
an extension
of
relaxation
here
also
to be even
appears
high
to
that
verse-position
relatively
to
in
of
the
rhyth-
9; see 4.4. below). In Menander, in pos. 4 is realized as a long
pos. -
both
and
a resolution
comparatively the
at
difficulty
owing
divergence
(word-end
succeeding
no
is
( ••• ),
rhythmic
tendencies anceps
there
uu-
shape of
syllable of
of
147
preceding
percentage
of
in Menander's
the
tendency
stronger the
words
in
the
anceps. which
trimeters:
to case
(One may occupy
see Table
the
XX, p.
214 below.) E.g.
<x.CUJ.L ...... t,:x'i'Jv,n OWI.OEL'Vtar1. 1:l 1l0'tE ~ .. ... ... -
U£8uc.>, kO.takW, AO)'OV~ o(M
60
Dysc.
77
TP(£1.s;au 1:l ~;
110 2.1.4.6.
In
tragedy
the
realized
by a short
tions.
the
first
beginning
clausular
of
cnl"lstances.
since
word
at
(See Table
as the effect (see
pos.
(For exceptions
Table
XI, p.
or. 81
Eur.
&:t
896 ~•
Word-end gether,
at will
tidlv
pos. occur
short
realization
sample
is
irrespect-
150 below.) motiva-
often the
be realized
in
occurs
at
preferred these
cir-
p. 240 below.)
Ch. II.3.4. word-boundary
n.99) at
... ... ... ... ... ... otx6'JE;a&l" ot y6p fxw l,l6vn'V ... ... K~s;· 06E:...6'...cw-rote CQl,lJfl"tPl 61.~ ........
75
or.
anceps
vcn.> MC11XK7tdCO\ft'Q
7
Soph. Phil.
the
in lied.
rising
short
(2)
movement (see
single-short
CN
in pos.
majority
75% and
In most cases
10,
a double-short
Soph.,
the
(1) the preferred
cases
of resolution
concerned,
100%; in
and 64.29% respectively. in these
MClLTtq)
m'CSac, ~ ~1. ........ fat:vto iaOta ruttQE, ot TOAQI.
word-types
Aesch.,
~ e,t ~
y6'.a,1., .........
O£
18.18%, 6.25%, 1.92% of all cases Eur. Ned. and Eur. Or. respectively).
(i.e., Soph.,
that
npoc:,rtL T\M
597 qiJcJcu. ~
Eur. Ned.
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
.....
...
6' •~,
fxci.;
lJl'l"tPbc;&voou.nd:nls:
oo MJUlP' ~ XPaJ"tL.~1.6u..,v ....... 6' f.aA> 37 3/ 4 ~ rowx.oOv~, fKAUOV&\1.TUrlAJV ...... n \Jbc ~ 'l'Uv6at:>E:lac rui.obc 6;vcx,1.ovcs,avov. 42
the
Of
realization po&. at
word-types of
10,
pos.
the
beginning
anceps
also
at the
here
also
there
10;
but
here
the
beginning
that
the
comparative
type
almost
long syllables
still
a
popularity
completely following
of
rising) (or
accounts resolution
word itself
movement.
in pos.
of this
64
TD.:JP6tvov tufl
T£
w
proportion
--
lJl'l"tPt 1'1D(Xai.J y6p
260 ¼ELV Men. Dysc. Aristoph.
oro• tv•
~~L
10 c65tvl. , TT(XX)TlYCIPC\JC.E TQ::)6-r~ 6' Nub.
ao6tva.
485
- ... ~ bpe:l>..w c,xt-r-".1.QC, bt1.~
495
>d:n£1. 't' bt1.c,xcwOALYOVbtL~L
14 xfpxv
-
....
v~-
JO.l 'HQM)L
TICU.TIDNl
YEVYPf;L "tPLlJUl,~
644 d:6tv A.tyELC ~-
Men. Dysc.
Etol
raivu
--
-
I) _..,__ Tt£PLoaJ vuv
Y\J\,Qt'x' fY1"llJ£, t'E'tEA£V'tTlx6roc
tiiol
150
RHYTHMAND METRE
TABLEXI REALIZATION OF ANCIPITIA FOLLOWING RESOLUTION IN POS. 8* vu
.2
Aesch.
23.08% 42.3%
76.92% 57.63%
Soph.
18.18% 42.96%
81.82% 57%
6.25% 39.94%
93.75%
34.62% 46.25%
65.38% 53.21%
Aristoph.
64.91% 56.55%
35.09% 39.55%
Men.
66.88%
33.12% 39.5%
Eur.
Med.
Or.
60.06%
56.66%
*
ad Table
Vid.
2.1.5.
Parallel
tive)
frequency
pose
a list
resolutions terial
the
the
verse-types
likewise
tabulating
ization
by the purpose,
habit
of it
10 whether
the
they
in verses
the
with
resolution.
the
preceding
or as a short
ancipitia
subject provide
by the case
could
syllable;
in the verse
of
are
com-
not
the
ma-
adequate
information
on the
As a consequence
various
of
the
(rela-
number of verse-types,
do not
in the
one
quantifications
on the
resolutions
anceps
which show the
resolution,
complete
known only
as a long
of the other
A above,
without
literature since
naming
is
and Ill
Unfortunately,
of ancipitia
realized
Ill
of
this
'foot',
6 and is
to Tables
presented
realization of
VII Above.
included.
to serve
tl
uu
forms
resolution
occurrence
of
taken
by
in poss.
2,
resolution
but even so the realcannot
be ascertained.
THE IAMBIC TIUMETER Obviously
151
the relative
be more informative higher
frequency
trimeter ever
will
in
relative
and a greater of
the
and Menander
should
be borne
is
rather
small
indicated
text
in
above
question
of resolutions.
suited
to give
preferences
will
shows a The comic
an impression,
which may play
how-
a role
in
of verse-types. of
132
sample
our the
in mind,
from
following
however,
in relation
The average incidence
the
diversity
rhythmical
limits
phanes
as
be best
frequency
Within
of verse-types
proportion
therefore
defective,
the
order
the
trimeters
Aristo-
observations
can be made. It
the
quantity
of the material
questions
investigated.
that
to the detailed
per 100 verses
of resolution
of
is:
& pr. names - pr. names for Aristoph. (Nub.Eq.: for Men. (Dysc.Asp.Sam.: Compare Table IV above.
In our sample of comic
2.1.5.1. ent
verse-types
amounts
to
98.12 87.49
1436 trim.) 1615 trim.)
trimeters,
315 (in
the
91.57 85.76
sum total
Aristophanes,
235;
of differin Henander,
245). These shape
So
at
verse-types the beginning
-- ...
103 types•
v- ...
80 types
UV-
67 types
...
can
be
distinguished
of the verse 133
with
in the following
respect
to
manner:
Aristoph. Hen.
34.89% 82 types• 82 types - 33.47%
• 25.4%
Aristoph. Hen.
63 types• 63 types•
26.81% 25.71%
• 21.27%
Aristoph. Hen.
48 types• 47 types•
20.43% 19.18% 10.21% 13.88%
32.7%
-vu ...
40 types•
12.7%
Aristoph. Men.
24 types• 34 types•
uuu ...
25 types•
7.94%
Aristoph. Men.
7.66% 18 types• 19 types • 7.76%
the most remarkable
authors,
the
ginning
is
figures
above,
only)
- the
diversity
very
observation of
much similar several
greater
is
verse-types (although,
verse-types diversity
that,
of
are
in the
with
trimeters
a particular
verse-types
by one of with
I :14 135
of both verse-be-
as may be inferred used
their
from the the
authors
a (double-short)
152
RHY'niMAND METRE
falling
verse-beginning
tion.
(See also
in
2.1.4.2.
Henander
with
without
any
three
three
positions
containing
have
and do not contain
in
long
the
of verse-types
Aristoph.
syllables
-
fE
since
Aristophanes,
the
his
verse-types
proportion
of his
by those the
anceps
in the Aristophanes sample from Menander,
lo~g ancipitia
a~e:
mv ot>,~ -
de;
-
Vf1ll«1l
Whereas in Menander there in
(and
-
hw,
ToCc; T' ru"'CU)(O(b1.v Toi:c; T£ n,:d:n:ouo1.v ~ 'MQYW. TL 00V &;; TDV1Xl taOT' QJYlC~1. -
Sam. 185
than
two of
VOIJ.lCt.i> Tml.V ~1.c;
£[\.0.1.
180
Asp.
the
positions
followed in
oo Y d~ -
-
271/2
Dysc.
included
a.U.' ~ ldx1.0T' t\Jn1.~1. Tl'iv o(Klav ~ 6a:iA£Dx,{;;,J. 6£C¥x> Ed.p', wIbvala,
Eq. 4/5
Men.
excep-
resolution.
wi~h three
1484/5
Nub.
are
anceps
These four verse-types are represented, sample, by 298 trimeters (or 20.75%); in the by 385 trimeters (or 23.83%). Examples
notable
below.)
moat frequently,
occurs
which
one
resolution
syllables
long
resolution)
verse-types
the
above and 2.1.5.4.
Even when the verse-types verse-type
being
au y', wldv. a greater
is
verse
is
occurring
at
most
uvrn.1ov£ut:1.c;,dnt
diversity
the
of verse-types
same time
frequently
1.101.
more uniform,
constitute
a larger
trimeters.
The first ten verse-types in the order of frequency are represented, in Aristophanes, by 504 instances {or 35.1% of the trimeters studied); in Menander, by 643 instances (or 39.81% of the trimeters). In
Aristophanes,
verse-types
contain
two of these In double
the
four one
of
resolution
(comprising 42
verse-types
of
the
725
trimeters).
contain
or
Menander This
of
of
preceded
instances
10 most
frequently
resolution;
in
occurring
Menander
only
a resolution.
frequency
is in
these
occurrence
verse-types order
of
by
verse-types, 22
verse-types
50.49% of (comprising
situation
is
the
the 1111
first
type
in
trimeters
Aristophanes counted),
instances,
in accordance
with
with
by
or
68. 79%
the
higher
153
THE IAMBIC TRIMETER
percenta~~
of
resolutions
in
the
sa~ple
from
Aristoph~nes
in
rt-nPral. 2. l .5.2.
Both
verse-type
in
Aristophanes
containing
and long syllables
one resolution
Henander
has the
the
E.g.
resolution
... ... ... ...
l
-
-
-rovKTPbva!rtflc w ~ -
500 >«X'COBou. ,: l A1P£ tc; 11 £ tnt &'i w
w
-
-
w
-
VtN
1,.101. 't06C
uovoc;,
1398/9 btt mtc nu.\a.l.c 6Mavt~t. w
-
"tU K\JVEl.(1 l,1£1.Y'VU(; i:otc 6vclo1.c nr:xm.a:n,'V. Sam. 71 i:l 13cx'i:>; yE:AOtov. ~• e:['\nL 't~ y4,iouc w
Hen.
w
327
Asp.
This verse-type resolution.
w
w
-
-
'tfk; ~lac;
a.\6vLO"toC_fatal.
in fact
occurs
lCPL"tTK;
more frequently
than one type without
namely that with three short ancipitia: u-v-v-v-v-v(Aristoph. 28 instances• (Men. 31 instances•
E.g.
1.95%) 1.92%)
...
Aristoph.
Nub. 1178/9
Hen.
205
Asp.
579/80
Dysc.
In fewer
Aristophanes, short
a resolution short
in pos.
131 - 2.3%) 2.11%)
Ari stop h • 33 i nstancea (Hen. 34 instances•
150 tvtec.J.£v Etc
Nub.
Eq.
most frequent
in the other anceps positions:
uv-v---v---v-
Aristoph.
and in
ancipitia,
... ... ~t ... 6t 6fl i:l;... (] Tl'Jvfvr1'V ... 'tE let~. l I lV!1ydp b:rc1. >«it vm 't1.c truti:n; ... ... ... 6c::lci&> 6t. aol 1:1., ~ eo;,.,, ~"td'V£1.v; ... ... ~ Til'V6lk£Ma'V &.::76E:VEt nvi. ... ... ...
who has there
proportionally are three
which have a higher
more resolutions
other
frequency
verse-types than
this
containing pure single-
sequence: - -v- --vuu- - -v-
(32 instances 22 instances•
- -vu- - -v-- - -v--
(32 instances• 24 instances•
- -v- ---vuv- -v-
(29 instances• 20 instances•
and
• 2.23%, cp Hen.: 1.36%.) 2.23%; cp. Men.: 1.49%.) 2.02%; cp. Men.: 1.24%.)
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
154 E.g.
791
Nub.
t\v t, °'""''VlA'1 1,101.IJl'iTIPC>a+i t64; Unl&I(;
1500/ 1
-
w
-
-
-
f~ OE T1'X)CTbv eiil.lOV,t\.O.a,ic 1,101.6lxnv. 11 >GY6> 6t. o' ~ Ha.t 6~ 1lA£LO\.Q.
--
710-1
Eq •
Aristophanes
In
2.1.5.3.
resolutions
has
the
resolution
moat
in
frequent
poss.
verse-type
1 and
3 and
with
long
two
ancipitia
otherwise:
a type
which
does
not -
E.g.
90
Nub.
occur
-
in Menander.
w
w
¼£ &'i,
TL xd£u£1.c;
-
w
-
• 0.91%),
(13 instances
ulHJu---v---u-
-
-
seems
to
11 Ha.L n
11 Tt£LOOUJ.I.
ll£LOEI.;
1111 ~ .. , xouu:C 'tOO"rov cxxp1.0Tt,v6£E1.6v This
relative
frequency
resolution
in
notably
two
strong
at
successive the
be part
of a general
non-marked
beginning
of the
preference
verse-positions,
t~
which
is
verse.
In
Aristoph11neR 14 verse-types represPnted by 36 instances (or of the trimeters counted) have a rising double-short versebeginning ( uu-uu- ... ) ; in rienander there are 6 of these verse-types, represented by 11 instances (or 0.68~).
2.s1,:
Resolution types
in
0.84%),
in
the
poss.
3 and
Aristophanes
whereas
in
the
5 ( u-ulHJu-
sample sample
••• ) occurs
(represented from
5 verse-
12 instances
by
Menander
in
there
is
or
only
one
occurrence.
E.g. Aristoph.
(each
Eq.
183
0[1,101., -rl TTOT'
actually only)
which
has
featuring
verse-positions
phenomenon
two verse-types
OOH.de
occurrence
marked
ru 't4) 61.~;
871
Aristophanes one
..w4xJkac; .. >«l~
Nub.
- namely
is altogether
6:18' 6n cm.,Tbvoo dJc 4f1.ov;
a resolution poss. absent
3,
5,
in the
and
7;
sample
Eq.
1228
Nub.
50
by
successive
in three
E.g. Aristoph.
represented
5,
7,
and
non9 - a
from Menander.
- ..
auTbv TtE'.PI.~. 11 Ha.'tdaou mx.tc..,;,l,DOTI.YLQ
--
,,,,,..
""'
THE IAIIBIC TRIMETER
155
2.1.5.4.
As may be inferred
in pos.
2 in
centage tophanes ander's
Henander
in the preceding
and Menander is
the rhythmic
level,
the high percentage
on the one hand, and the fact
of long syllables trimeters
froc
very much similar
more frequently that
anceps
of resolutions that
position
on the other
show a falling
the perin Arishand,
Men-
verse-beginning
(on
I 2
uu · · · •
is):
In Menander, there are 34 verse-types represented by 126 instances (or 7 .8:); in Aristophanes, 24 verse-types have a falling verse-beginning, represented by 83 instances (or 5.78%). (Sc. of the samples concerned.) (Men., 24 inst.;
-vuu---v---v-
Hen. Asp. Aristoph.
Aristoph.,
- t>aeev ...... auTfivoCa de
74 ~
Nub. 659
1Xi.iv 1:'E"tTXJlr.o&N dn' (Men.,
-vuu-v--u-v--u-
10 inst.;
16 inst.)
tiiJv~
b:n1.v ~ Aristoph.,
~
5 inst.)
- ...... 00X 'tO n:i:xiYl.11 6' £le uto:,v ~ - CRtovo{;), ...... 213 cqJl")C
does not occur in the Aristophanes &Ample.)
tl«l'VOVE'On.JX.TlU' tim,"tQ YEYovt\0.1.
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
156
2.2.
The first
ceptional rence
metron
phenomenon
of resolution
E •8• Aesch.
occurs
mainly
in non-marked
Choeph.
6 57
Aristophanes
l"lil'lianhi
-
(cp.
the
ex-
occur-
in tragedy).
oxf\\.Ul«1l ~ tun0C
E
•kJNd(;. b 6t 't'OL6o6'6;vt'p
ttv, ~.
-
1'T.06aJtbc; b [;t'\.q;: rr6a£v;
b Ku5a81,\aL£UC;K1.'.x...,.,:
veap. 902 noO 6' 6 6L~, and
Accordingly, (cp.
names
This
Pax ~63);
interjections
volver.137
....
.... .., 547 ~'VOl"Dtoc;
e.g.
Aristoph. where
w
-
Cboeph.
proper
verse-positions
488 • I~
Sept.
(compare
in
occasionally.
qmaxl-«Mc; MO.l1't£'rW:K,:avraJtva.L
1049 -
But also,
as -vu- very
is realized
(other) the
colloquial
phenomenon
VON WII..AMOWITZ (
2
189'l)
is
eYpressions slightly
111.166):
arP
in-
more frequent see
Ch.
III
in
A sub
2.2, p. 276 below. of
n-e
fact
this
phenomenon
long
and
types
of verse:
that
short
the
beginning
reminds
syllables compare
of
the
triineter
one of
that
of
in
the
Ch. v.2.1.2.
the
verse-initial E,
with
note
occasionally free
admits
distribution
positions
196 below.
in
of other
157
THC IAMBIC TRIMETER 3.
The rhythmic
verse
as a rhythmically It
the
should
verse as
types
the
of
structured,
utterance
dactylic
of verse
concerned.
the
constitutes
whole.
rhythmical
completeness
necessarily - as
the
(and
rel!;ards
its
not
of
usually)
meaning,
being
UI
(see
Ch.II,
utterance
filling
3.1.1.
above),
according
to
the
metric
several
the
degree
profile
of
the
instance:
7 .27-9
Sern.
profile
may be distinguished
of
for
the
not
is
hexameter
enjambement
Compare,
while
linguistic
completeness
type
that,
of a poem.
metric meaningful
it
part
As in
the
axiomatic, a
an integral
of
be realized
is
complete
of
realization
-div 6' b. ~, f'i 00 1 tv qpoolv 'VOEt:· ytY'l)8Ev tn.1tp1w• -div lJ£VyE).4 u: 'MCl.l tm.1-VWEL
96-8
~Et:voc; tv 66\Jot.!; (ai,.,
l,LLV
ZEUCy6p µtyLarov -roOt-' btolflCEVWCDC6v, Y\J\QL>«l(; • t\v n >«1l oat~L v ~kt:v V fxovn , tii>lJi.\Lota. y l ve:mL WCDC6v
37/8
l-l£Va~
cbi:Ep ~ rroUcot1.c
fo-rr)>C.',
6ntr~.l~,x«iro1.6e:vfi~
ot6'
Tbv K1.a:it.p:i;voc;'t6rcov (cp. Phil. 549)
6t1.
159
THE IAfIBH.: TRIHETER
1138/9
X.£1.l,ii'NJ.6'
ftal "t6ui, 't'1 ElC ~'
ty&,
ci>.oc T' etc i:a. tiafoo ~ecu.ov 0' fvEp&v te 6':>1.0TE,:xic; mx'
tV,au\ov, 2011
Phil.
dv
C001.crro't'OV~w.tov, &CTT.EP tent (Ml,). 64/5 .~. ~
Even here
of course
it
at stake,
the rhythmical
tA.tyx£1.c; (compare oc 1164).
is not the context
See PRATO(1970 (Studia SCHEIN clearly illustrates of enjambement in Sophocles (1906).
Tl "t'CJOT'
rhythmical
14 2
completeness
which is
being unambiguous.
Florentina)); the rhetorical (o.c. 46-50);
SCHEIN (1979) 31 + note 42. effect of different types for Euripides, see HUMBORG
THE IAMBICTRIMETER 3. 1. Recurrent near
the
incidence
middle
rhythmical
161
of
of word-end
the
balanced
constituent
In comedy, as will movement of
words over This
the several
reduced
rhythmical another
verse
tendency
verse-positions for
movement seems
to
which
is
a
into
See Ch.I,
3.2.2.
to modify the rhyth-
the
distribution
verse-internal preserve
the
trimeter
is considerably
be connected to
modifies
the '43
(or cola).
by means of
preference
tendency,
the
verse-positions
internally
and structures
parts
be seen,
the
one of the
(caesura)
movement of the verse
two evenly above. mical
verse
at
less
variation
with the
the
of the strong. of
the
prevalence
of
distinctness
of
the
metric profile underlying the verse (identity of verse-elements; above; 4.1, 4.4. below. metrical structure): see e.g. 1.1.1.1. Here again, are rhythmical
one should sub-phrases)
beware of interpreting as syntactic
the
cola
(which
entities.
The facts do not by any means bear out SCHEIN's claim ((1979) 32) that an adjective is not usually placed in a different colon from corresits corresponding noun, unless there is another adjective ponding to the noun in question and in the same colon with it. Thus, according to SCHEIN, Ag. 4 would be exceptional: &.,-r'217vk0.l'Ol.6a.WH.1:~
bur'Jyq,1. V •
Compare, however, e.g. Aesch. Ag.
330 ~ 6 1 345 8E:C>Cco' 347 YML ,:'
499
®1:£WK"tLTWXYK't'OC b.~ n6vor; Et ~1. ~ dv, d rrix,c,ru1.a JJnWX,01. )G(Q
~,:°'
wv {iv,:lov 151:,:oi:a6'
arcantp-yw ¼ov
504 ODCQ't'OOOE ql£YY£L 'tii£, 1 &,)1.MqJJ'lVf,:cq; 528
563 xe:1.~ 621
x~
'MO.L antpJ,JJ. Tmt'lCt~,n1. o'
Et ¼01.
nc
ow,vc:JKrovov
tc wv TTt:M1Jv q:,0,.01.01. ~ XPOVCJV
641 ~ 6l: ~ t~y1.o6tvtat;
6 52 q&lpc,vre:
-tbv ~'VOV
~
• ICY£lwv
etc.
c,-qxn-6v
etc.
Stylistically speaking a more significant difference seems to be whether adjective and noun are adjacent or not; 14 4 the incidence of caesura in between is quite a different matter. (Compare, for instance, the following examples with those cited above:
162
kliYTH.t1 AND METRE
1209 "'6r) "t~Ot.'V
Ag.
tvetc1.c; '(p,\.ltvt,;
.\uHr+l, Atovroe; cuy£'VC:JO(; mux,lai.
1259
m"COVO(;. td6 • c}6 •
14 06 t.pyav, 61.>C1la(;
fx£
1. •
>
Often, the question whether the adjective is situated within same colon as the noun or not depends solely on the option for of the two caesurae (see 3.1.3.1 , p. 172 helow); e.r. y6p
1284 ~I.
Ag.
tx
~
8£,G;.,
which complies with the tendency formulated decides in favour of a penthemimeral caesura. short
In Lycophron the of a stereotyped
the corresponding 17
to
seems
one
if
little
be
t:niiN
utv a.(TlU'V&,n .,YLOO Tliyov
16 •~
with
by SCHEIN only
(123) &t-£ >eal"'ol,q; c34>
particles
• words followed
by enclitics
(particles,
pronouns,
verbs)
lCT:£1.nt ..... 1J01. (224)
n, (
edKvEC° µt .....
I utor;"'b:rnv I
£("cx,("nc • words followed
3 7)
by postpositive
(and non-enclitic)
(795) particles:
TtECL·rcx,vo •.o:t0:' l!it C6 2 ) It'\
T\UELC \Jtv(794) • negatives - except
,...•wu"""L-• """'"' ~~1,
(56)
in the case ofiota
deaon•tratiwmor
ooxtI W0ft1XtJ • nouns immediately
followed y~
• words in formulae/stock
an added particle
(132)
by a corresponding
"'(;)J
( 129)
phrases vl-,"'1:tr,rA1.6vu:1ov ( 91 )
aau•""£lc'"'~ ~•~1.
n,.rri:xi,n.,.'
(
1290) (823).
(133)
participle
fol-
166
RHYTHMAND HETRE
3.1.2.
In the
the
early
trimeters
~reat
have
majority
caesura
iambographers
of
word-end
which
penthemimeres);
Euripides
the percentage
lenistic
period,
Henander
position.
See Table
So, in contrast being
and
proportion
is
of
half
as
70% or
verse-element,
the
the
caesura
p.
176f.
3.1.3.1.2., the
epigrams
iambic
over
the
(called below.
In
60%, and
trimeters
of
even
have
the
Hel-
in the
case
word-end
at
this
XII below. to the rising
two elements
fifth
111orP.than
80%.
Lycophron than
tragedy
the
however
is over
the
more
after
in
to be considered
see
In Aristophanes, of
is
and
longer
than
verse-beginning, the
first
the second
- has a falling
colon
-
rhythmical
movement. This
variation
produced in
this
ducing in
by the a rising
the
in
iambic
the
in its
sequence
there
only are
level,
boundary
involved
(see
izations
to
stop.
caesura
ized caesura
according full
is
however
just
there
is
the
that
3.1.1.
hexameter.
However,
two possibilities the
middle
one way to may account
caesura
for
(see
(stronger
above), of
phenomenon
141
whereas
the
-
proverse,
the
pre-
fact
that',
an alternatbelow).
caesura type
at
the
of word-
from sub-optimal
significant
antilabe is
the
or weaker)
ranging
the
3.1.3.2.
ways of realizing the
of
is
effect for
realize
position
degree
(A dramatically
occurrence this
only
to a lesser
to
rhythmical
before
second
various
the
are
- which the
rhythn.ic
the
there
structuring
own right
resembles
dactylic
colon
trimeter trimeter,
Of course
rhythm
in the
second
iambic internal
the
caesura
double-short
ferred ive
of
real-
realization it
should
by no means restricted
of
be realto
the
positions.)
The following epigram by Leonidas of Tarentum may be quoted here as a poem consisting solely of trimeters with a penthemimeral caesura (AG XVl.307):
•15•, &JC b ~U(;lb.
\.lt{nc;'A~
UtEOK£A1.ata.t. I'HQ.Lt6 ~ Uw.Em1. tx:nxP1. yulEUyELV x86w.(cp. 943) 1124 ,:C 6' t1fLOVUOLI Tf'ic6£l W'fx.avE L l o' ~
2 6. 6 ra'nnL ve: xal c,qac;I6.Uo'
ll:x:me+:> ~LC
bpa.i:ctv' tx:rtl!x£tvocl~ raiyor; SLC l 5 ~E -rb 8e:tov!~UJ. • 1-cwv "CLCbtoEL • 7 .23 oO-r' ta8)bv o66tvjol6£1 "tOLCJU"tTIYuvi'i 176.1
Seci.
1. 114 Corrv6' fxOVTEC !l-DC'PC1VI ooYL~ So 1 on 3 7. 7 OUKdv xa-rtax£ Ia'iJJOV, Ioo5'
Aesch.
Ag.
351 630
928 Soph.
OR
271 361
410 Phil.
Eur.
Ned.
ammm
Y\J\W.,kat'' d\4:n.lCLXAXN' I~
¼£LC
TIO"tE+U yap al>'t'OO ICwvroc;11'TE~ a£00 utl'Latovj&rpov. l~law. 61; XPfl lJl'I,:'cLNyuwiwwv lna.Ua~, l110,:ir,> GIUX l:x:rc£y' £t1t£tv l'('\Kal'.TtUV" li 6oOAOC, I 6.ua /toElQ.
242
CX.JV u' ~~ l 6Mo I n:.Vtv~ ¼£Lv; ~ (l)LAm:tOU na.Cj nr.xt'POC, I~ (l)LAl'lS; xaov6c
49
~bv
100 TL
oCwwvIKi:flUJ.l6£0rrol'Vl'IC ~
356
oo ydp
798
tlkl· i:l l-DL Cfiv l~;I
n
at
of metric
below;
22.1
19.2
is
of an enjambe-
I C~, looo' 6.ya.Lcu:11. ooyap "CL ~!xiiix>!;loo5' tq>l~
Archil.
impression
word-boundary
speak
5 (for
the
appears
remarkably
rence
pos.
6 and 7, parallel
tragedy
the
has
one could
level
positioning.'
in
at
is,
stronger
Ch.V.2.3.2.
positioning
than
structuring
so that ending
it
As
syntactically
position,
word :In poss.
the
hesitation.
internal
by the
metric
rhythmically
the
of
caesura
186 below).
upon
without
outruled
ment of the P•
verse
possibility
were
colon,
177
~LC
iOEL'VOVl@V~ ll'
o0-r£ l-DL
fXEL
TD'tPlc
7
178
RHYTIUI MD METRE
~16E:qx); I~ 6' ~1.M4Jnv; ~ TlA£\.POL(;!~, IlCCJlJ)OW)'HCJIJJ'lV ~ mu"lla¥) I 6.>L~; \ Et 6t oot f:au: t au i:01.qui:EUXJC; l8uvattP', lyt(x:Jv, JCDdrv TOU\a\lTLOV 61.:l~i:• 11\or:xm.L ~1
n6ecv nat
215
Or.
2 23 31 0
585 938
Arietoph.
117 d \m 6q,e:lAti>j61.Qot, I,:oou.:,vT:iJNXPOiN
Nub.
660 6>i.\' o[cS' t.Yt,JYE i~',
Id l6l 1,11L'VOl,D.L ta1.haclf\.oov, IL\.O.c:iJl6l ~;
1393 ~E
Eq.
Men. Sam. 4276>i.\'
tx:n' fpyov
21
Asp.
t.a"t' bc.&t\lOf;l~ouc•IOUM. (4)YLCEi:o ~i:'v
irrp6.ixx,1.v, I6At~
102 >CQI.-div ~lncpnvl~
Lye. Alex.
6'
£tffl.QX7V
AUH.0f;
306 u{rvr1(; ULCD.iJV !KtwlOV,I~to.I. 366 -~
at~EL!ra:nhrui;
KEVOO(;~
6 7 9 l,WJ.J(; cx::wEL!~Lea.I )(Qt K~ qxxvcl(;
a.li.nl~ ~ 1115 MrPELY£\JOVtO.! auuovl ayplac x~ 1 2 l 5- 7 ,:.u:il\,OUJ"'t()AW\I V4 .455.2-5 ~ KELttlL! y,::,,~, I~ untp 'tQpOU ~ TTPO(EL to.1. ITD:1Lvl•ATILK~ xi>Ai.~. at£\IE:L 61.:~ Yt%l vtp8Ev, Iaix. urrtp W(\(JV fcna.L a-c~mvci.'Jv! o[KO(;.
VII
Anth.Gr.
Compare also Aesch.
Sept.
48
n Y'iivaxv6vtE(;i Trl'VOEI ~¥DJEt.V
qxS~
X..t:tv oCMOU !-roOo£I~ CTC£\A:&JV dv dvop;J. !i:6voe:l -ct7Jv ~ xwa.
1048 oO, TTPLVYE Ag.
18 ~lw 896
Soph.
OR
Phil. Eur.
i;o,:'
AtloLµ'
1164 'tL'VO(; T'IDAL'tWVli:wv6E:ll(Q)( nolac cniYT)(;;
rear' t(; Y'iiv1Trl'VOEl'VOUi:l,¾> nMTIJ cot yap rqx,v yfiv !Trlv&I)(Qt ~ fx.ELV O t 1,11L 6t na.,:ti;n l "tOV ti,civ, IEl wa,: ' 6uu:im
220 TL~
Ned.
448
Or.
288
JJ£"t0t5oi;; «JJJ(;: Ta'.io6e:I )(Q.T~fu
THE IAMBICTRIMETER
179
TABLEXV PERCENTAGE OF VERSESWITHPENTHEHIMERAL c.q. HEPHTHEMIHERAL CAESURA WITHCONCOMITANT WORD-END AT POS. 7 & POS. 5 RESPECTIVELY*
Archil.
Sem.
* Percentage
Penth. + 7
Hephth. + 5
13.64%
21.21%
7.47%
13.79%
Solon
17.5%
12.5%
Aesch.
12.84%
11.48%
Soph.
17.15%
9.64%
Eur. Med.
Or.
19.33% 16.44%
7.95% 9.49%
Aristoph.
12.12%
4.46%
Men.
13.81%
4.58%
Lye.
14.93%
6.65%
Epigr.
12.27%
5.52%
of sum total
of verses
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
180 3.1.3.2.
In
comedy,
the
conconitant
the
trimeters
of
percentage word-end
verses
of at
the
iambographers,
of
tragedy
with
at
pos.
word-end
22%. (See Table
5 1s ahout
pos.
and
of
7 without XVI, p.
184
below.) The exceptions are Semonides, of whose verse single hephthem.1meral caesura (i.e., hephthemimeral caesura unaccompanied by wordend at pos. 5) seems to be a rhythmical characteristic (28.16%, cp. 1.1.1.2) and Euripides, in whose verse the percentage is 15-16, penthemimeral caesura (with or without concomitant word-end at pos. 7) being relatively frequent. In the Hellenistic poetry of Lycophron and the Anthologia Graeca (where the average number of words per verse is somewhat lower) single hephthemimeral caesura occurs in more than 30% of the trimeters. Here indeed the hephthemimeral caesura seems to be a caesura in its own right. In accordance with the general average word length (see Table XXVI A), in Lycophron and in the Anthologia Graeca the words preceding a single hephthemimeral caesura begin at the third verseposition more frequently than at the fourth (contrast the situation in Semonides: See Table XVI A). Just
as with
hemimeral in
caesura
this
than
case,
the
words
has
a
however,
first.
in
All
the
verse
is
conspicuous
(as,
preceding
first
second
a verse and
colon
4,
is
e.g.,
in
if the
single
hepht-
second
colon;
two elements equilibrium
produced
especially
with
a falling
same, a satisfying frequently
3 or
at
caesura,
rising the
the
pos.
word-end ically
a penthemimeral
by means of this
case
word-end of
shorter
between
the
concomitant is
enjambement
syntactof
the
verse).
For those instances within the present hephthemimeral caesura occurs most frequently phron, Anth. Gr.) the (combinations of) word caesura have been given below (Table XVI A). that the verses themselves are more informative: Archil.
sample in which single (viz. Semonides, Lycolengths preceding this One may feel, however,
177.3 (epod.): ~ HO.L&1.u.cnd.,Ioot 6'; 8rlPL).IU.LOtO.IYL'VE't'Q.L>«JH6v
~Lqq.D'l'V
16.1/2
Id lONl{; 6.v1tP
l4x:>LOL I >Ct 8'.4.a,L 'V
Y6':>nclfJmOPC)(; ,q:iiv.
Ma.t amo«iPL • >Ct
Aesch. sept.
18/9
mnvta. ~,TnL&lac; O(Kl'lfflPQC;l&:ntL~
~T'
37
202
6TAOV,
frtE:iaJa,, ~ ntnoL& I1Jilin-tav bl::i;> ~
1' o()K
11'~ AiYt&>;
~,
'taX1.crnil)CQ.t -aix' £ C~L. 't'QAQ&.\.Q. Qt6£L. 1 hd, q>l~
2 61 / 2 AiyoLs; &, ~ ~
olyrpov, Ag.
dv, d npocn1La.li.dl 'tUXOL >CCDCO. 5 5 7 o'd:vOV'tEC, oo>.ax.6vt£C t,\1,.D"tOC ~; 347
Soph. OR 319
YMLT
1
TL 6 1 WTL'V; ~ ~
,El~-
~: tVV£1"1A> at l"tii>>C1VJUY1.nn 384 &.p,T6v, o()K al't1'1T6v, IELO£XELPLOEV 350
Phil.
Eur.
Ned.
221
>CTO:,XET' o0T' ~
loOT'ot~v;
335
TOfE:uT6c, ~ Atycu:JL 'V,
ltx laouSau£lc
345
.\tvOVtEC, ECT' ~CIECT'
403
fx.OV"tEC, ci.'C foLJu;, ~ ~
482
£le avTAlav, Etc ~,
I
de 1lPOUVl1V, ~
ooxt OUYYE:\lli 257 oo 1Jl')T£P', OUK~,, 1 00 rrpd.a:>t. TE• 1,:nv6£ 6t xa6\Q. , 313 ~T
402 Or.
c!lp' 00V iatn'V
Mn6£La, ~ >Ct IT£~ 0
6' 26 f>CL TOv ,f\TtO\Q.
6.>.A.'ECOELJ.U., ICXIO6' oo ()
tnt
TL OTtqnvov;
Q¥XJV't LG">
I0C1,.01.,~TE(;
6>.£KTPIJ6va.. I) ~ YE. I 'tCW'tllVL 6t ,:l; 259 bi>lAJ"PE, TtEPLTl'V£Yl4£V. I~ 6' trm'.xnro 38 7 ci.'C 0Cqn1. II T l nodlcnv; 111c:iJ6tv. 6.U' fxE 1.c
848
tie n.
OOTLCtoTl,I
182
RHYTHMAND METRE Dysc.
I 6:U.' C11JtoC JJOVC>C
oCJxlve:,mv',
331 1,U,o&nov,
Lye. Alex. 151 •Ert£1.6v, aJK •Apye:tov ~q:,vf\
yO\Qi:C
373-6 ·OqJU.m wa.l 1A'X,Ol¥XI xo1.~
14:xJf
lwa.t. "Cp:ixO(; Nt&N
OltLAOI. TC )(QT Tpuxavm
>Ill £\1.QkPUt.N
kCLlmvta. £\1.~tol
yCih;1.&>Ill~ lot'KTl"CnP'-OV 614 ·0rwx,µi.ac, ~01.V lf1(m::,m~ 6 71 At-«MLCf\ IIll 'tfic JJtv ~, YJiYDVl6TPEJJtc OKtAO{; ~ 6ux:m'p1. CEvi,\tl:\.aln A1.adc;. fxou:,1,
( 10)
~"'
6'
nilm£I, V
1,U,V
ad,c:q:x,v·j Ql 6' tnr.lVEXJaV ~I.K'tOV Itic,8Etoa.1. 66aE1..
f}.£1,TT£
TI{E IAMBIC TRIMETER Sometimes
it
occurrence verses
of
with
functional fate
is
tempting
to consider
as
a single
hephthemimeral
penthemimeral
caesura,
at
of women
sequence
183
of
pentherrl.meral
the
level
rhythmically caesura
that
of content;
after
is,
e.g.,
significant
if
a sequence
this
in Hedea's
seems speech
(" Y\J\Qtxtc; b:,uE;v MA...c:na.'tOV qJU"COV ", 231), fifteen
verses,
all
of
them
but
one
the
showing
to
of be
on the after
a
single
caesura:
Ned. 244/5 ~ 6'&tav
ro'Cc; fv&:,vldxarrca.1. ~,
f~ ~ fraJoE IkQP6lav cb-ic• -
but
better
as
such
impressions
apparently
cannot
be verified,
they
had
be disregarded.
As is illustrated by the examples quoted above, verses with a single hephthemimeral caesura (like those with a single penthemimeral caesura) naturally include many of the verses consisting of three words only. The phenomenon is especially frequent in Lycophron - in accordance with the low average number of words per verse in his trimeters in general -; "the obscure, oracular style of Lycowas just calling for such massive words and phron 's Alexandra emphatic lines, reaching the highest TWT (Three-Word Trimeter) frequency recorded in antiquity (1/24)", MARCOVICH(1984) 163 (with extensive quantifications).
184
RHYTHM AND METRI.
TABLEXVI
& HEPHTHEHlMERAL CAESURAE* PF..NTHEMIHERAL
p
P-7•
H
H-5•
Archil.
56.06%
42.42%
43.94%
22.73%
Sem.
58.05%
50.57%
41.95%
28.16%
Solon
65%
47.5%
35%
22.5%
Aesch.
63.97%
51.13%
33.08%
21.6%
Soph.
64.5%
47.36%
31.19%
21.55%
Eur. Med.
Or.
74.58% 71.91%
55.25% 55.41%
23.75% 24.93%
15.8% 15.45%
Aristoph.
55.71%
43.59%
26.04%
21.59%
Men.
50.46%
36.66%
27.37%
22.79%
Lye.
59.36%
44.44%
40.43%
33.79%
Epigr.
63.19%
50.92%
36.81%
31.29~10
• Penthemimeral caesura without concomitant word-end at pos. hephthemimeral caesura without concomitant word-end at respectively. • For trimeters without word-end at either pos. 5 or pos. and 3.3. with Tables XVIII and XIX below.
7, and pos. 5
7, see 3.2.
THE IAMBICTRIMETER
185
TABLEXVI A LENGTH OF WORDS PRECEDING SINGLEHEPHTHEMIMERAL CAESURA IN SEMONIDES, LYCOPHRON ANDTHEAN'l'HOLOGIA GRAECA: NUMBERS ANDPERCENTAGES* Lye.
Sem.
o-v--o--vl
1-
2.04%
9=
ol-v-o-vl
2-
4.08%
I
o-1v--0--v I
3 = 6.12%
88
ol-1v--0--v
-
1
17 = 34.69%
o--vl-o-v ol--vl-o-vl
1 = 2.04%
o-v-lo--vl
14 = 28.57%
ol-v-lo--vl
4=
o-lv-lo--vl
6 = 12.24%
o--vl-lo--v/
1
=
223
Epigr.
=
1.81%
2 "" 3.92%
0.2%
-
= 17.67%
8
=
-
0.2%
= 44.78%
23 = 45.1%
-
0.8%
4=
131 = 26.31%
8.16%
6=
2.04%* •
= 15.69%
14 = 27.45%
1.2%
1 = 1.96%
34
=
6.83%
2=
3.92%
I
=
0.2%
I =
1.96%
H-5 (totals) % in trim.
49 28.16%
498 33.79%
51 31.29%
word-end at 3 • • word-end at 4 • •
18 = 36.73% 24 ==48.98%
227 ==45.58% 171 = 34.34%
23==45.1% 17 = 33.33%
* **
*
Percentage
of single
Total pf>rcentage dee irr.al places.
r:onosyl1'tbles
hephthemirneral
• 99 .98% as
in pos.
a result
4 excluded.
caesurae. of
the
rounding
off
to
two
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
186 3.1.4. is
Outside
avoided.
(see
comedy word-end The one exception
Ch.II,3.4.
elision.
n.
falling
movement
either
the
preceded most 2.3.2.
to
the
emphasis
effect
of
scribed
sub
the
less
felt
to the
in
which
monosyllable
first
colon.
a ry at In
has
with
the is
monosyllable,
is
by far
often
one
the
of
above,
of
of
be compared
hephthemimeral
caesura
an
Ch.V.
essential
a kind
is
to
employed:
is
Ch.I.3.2.2.
there
a
hephthemimeres
some sort
a
effecting
been
The former
(compare
of
tension
precisely at
the
more salient verse
in
owing
would
between
the
caesura
expectation
caesura
comrhetor-
with
the
as
de-
middle
position.
to
the
that
the
have
been
(accompanied
the of
the
This
elision, word will
(syntacticand
effect
may be
which r,ives
rise
end at
interpreted
by a less
verse
as
prominent
pos.
7 -
having
a
word-bound-
pos • 5) • other
cases
the
enjambement
of
a monosyllable
monosyllable,
of
second
following
metric
colon,
colon
(to
be compared
both
attention Archil. SLG
rather
the
a penthemimeral
Common to special
or
being
1s a kind
(unfulfilled)
following
the
7, often
above.
one
the
verse
a monosyllable
pos.
verse
54
Frequently
the
word-end
case
in
The effect
preceding
there
hephthemimeral
E.g.
the
be even
of
of the
two means for
the
both.'
colon
prominent
to
or
metric
prominent
th,:
followed,
the
3.1.3.1.2.
middle
6 and/or
of
phenomena.
to
the
occurrence
of
middle is
bisyllable
Presumably
pos.
one
the
added
a
the
two
of
below),
plement
of
least
by a monosyllable, the
in
191 f.elow.
penthemimeres
enjambement
ally)
at
near
common of
ical
in
XVII, p.
cases,
these
is
99 above)
See Table
In
precisely
23.16 478.12
kinds
caesura: of
to be paid
~e.
in
pos.
than
to
6,
be
constitutes with
interpreted
effect
see 3.1.3.1.1.
l1i;l6~ln Y'UVCll.kOC, f\v vwlY'fll)(Ql''~· WV
1
as
an
beginning
of a bisyllable above).
positioning,
to the monosyllable
¼lklLt;1«1-ccx::naintc
llv1-ro a'naov wxEtv 6ta.v 6t "t'E'UX1l IZEUr;I 6n' ~~ TtLMP5c; ~ TEA£LOU lai:41'1 bt~ ow tvrtpo&.c tt bnMvxv',ItnolxTLO'tOV ~ dv aC8r,)~lbi>' tx:nlac; ~ no.\£&. utv Uko(;
Y£}..IOC
(403 above); 585
Ag.
(Et y6p
m.1+>lc TE ya.ta ~; IUl'tb ona,etk;
Eq:,t
h-aiu'l mtcX7TPolnL~ ~LC, loo,I~L, YSXNOV 'RAtw
850
Tt£L~
1299
OUK~·
160 2
cx'rn-3C 6>.t.oea.L ~ Tb
IIA£ &.aetvc:q; yi:voc;
110 tv -rij6' fqxx,)(.£lyij"ITO6t 130 263 364
Cn"'COU1J£\IOV f\ TTOI.XL~li:.£~~
Lye.
2 28 00K av 556
o 6'
x0u • ItrtEKMXJEV HCJH&Jv
~!
al, OL~l'f'CA£UP' ,~~ ecb.;
l'iut'vlroiu'ltT~
787 ui,LatOV
110n:
1373 ,a ADln' ~ li:a08',Id v0v compare also
the following
475 ~
examples:
aa>..ruaJvlrdvl lPLKUULq.
485
ac;w,v,6LKt.Ur,) lriivll.1£~
491
o 6'
1244 Anth.cr.
nt6ov
Yvu8oc
alrcb.; 6py4,IT'O{;l~LCJv
~
W.VOC, TW:iva.LOL Irdvr' , ~ lJUXbv Vl.90.7 &\.b.; TUPClW£, laol,l11a:7EuSov, ·Aox.L~ 9 4. 6
A£OVT66u,PE:, Iool, I ·Pb),
In the case of a monosyllable p.ramc.atically found esp.
ut.uw8pc)Et'v
with
the last
frequently
1UoToo8tvrlC
in pos. 6 also, word of the verse,
in Euripides'
Hedea (cp.
this P
word often situation
3.1.3.1.1,
which is P•
above). Solon 36.25 ~ dv Aesch.
Ag.
Eur. Ned.
258
~Kt.)
6;vq::wvl1'6' I~ nnl.LC CJEBltM,,loov, Ilydp I Ui,v npl V 6ncw: 1.«:J.t roto6t l,L1 t\.ea,lvov8'l ~ro1.v ace,.., 833
955 ~, 1049 1254 1256 1270 1360/
13 79
1394 1420 1599
Sept.
252 385
410 5 38
799 ~
OR
,I tuot ~o
T1£l8o1. • 4v, et TI£l8o1.'·1mu:1.aolrac 6' c~ >«:J.tiu'lv dyav y' "EMrtv' ltnlaroin1. qxi,:1.v TUJUt· olov Tb Tdl:>·I tnt?xc"tW. 6t IJOL »:Jl1(mlp(av ~T' ,I ~ 6t \.L£ 1 (way&, t'Ol.o0't6c £(1,L' ,I £TTElaJOJ.lllxa\ii> ) ¼01.01. mv ~• l&vLO'ttX\.O.L TKU.1.v. ~ 6' fva' fm.1.0' Ibt' tEeL~Le xalpo1.T' dv, e:i: xal,x,1.i:' , Ity&> 6' bu:ux.cui1. l,Ll.a:Jl.,U~ &-ro1.v';I ba'i'HCIOC 6' tliiN ~cv, {:q,utlrrcc1. 6' 16:nb c,qx:,.yfiv tr:i1N oC1Ktc cp6cpov 01.yi;x,' ,~ "t'OO£; OEi.£1.,kPCivouc xal"'tt41',I un' a:ml6oc; 6t "t4> 1 TI.JLJVUJ. 'H0.tOTUY00"6I~ ¼a.>e oouhv ~ y' Itci>lcna:m.1.T1UACL1.e
635 ~I.J,JOV
Soph.
a-cpo.,:o0 ~l,L'
m.1.dv' Itn£E1.mc.~
fx.£1."t6.1lAELOT1 ltv EE ~I.V 1005 6oMoOvta.MC1t ~• lmuyytll£1.v \.L£ xdl 1046 c.W.' 6v T10A1.e OTUY£i: , Ioo n 1JJPE Le ~; 46 [6', w~:xra';Jv t1pl.(J't', I~ T'IOALV 250 tv Tote tuote ytvo1.T' Itiw £;W£1.66-toc; 326 iv'l,nix,i;ace,..,,~ y' I~, tn£t 328 ~,:q; y6p 00 ~i:,:' .1tyw 6' 00 lri TID't£ 7 38 wZEU, TL I.ICU ~1. I~>..Eu:nl. nip1.; 744 0Cuo1.nUac;· fo1.x' I~ £Le~ 77 9 ~ y6p tv &:l nvo1.e \.L' I~ le J.L£~
THE IAtIBIC TRIMETER
197
K£i:VO(;y' Ib ~VO(; nat£ )O.Tbc:m.v', 6M.' ClllroCITQX>1.8£v (}h-.ro 1150 OOH. t:vvtrri.rv-rovna.i:o'IOv ~ tatCY)€t 1 15 5 Eu:1t"nvoc, ~ t TOO;1-c.: l"fPOOXPl5Cu,., 1,Ct8E; tv ; 1163 t1,bv JJl;v oiJK £Yt 6' Mc Su-1a~
oCK"tOLol ye:
trtlqpcqx,v 6tPQ
Aristoph. Nub.
16 6vEi.~t
&'
tnncur;.It-v&6'
2s ~ ~ ~
~L
I ta. rro.\.£Ul,O't"l'll:>I.Cl;
50 / 1 ~ -cpuy6(;, ·q::xx:11.~,Iti::>"'1v,llEPLOOOlo..lyov ydp 1,.01. utAEL v0v 00V ~ ct:x:,cLC lJ.' , I trt£ i. ~ "t l ex>L OOK£t ~ LV1 I~ Lv OOL 6oK£ t ; 'tOU"tl au VLMTIOEL(;;11 JTCDA.u Y£ cl lxlowc.-
1142 vOv 117 7 124 6
1335 Eq.
79
-cwxetp' tv At~tc,I
b ~ 61
tv
I 6' 4>' t.vf:cv Va,lf3(x,vr' I~vvuc fnn 670-3 ot 6' tE ~ O"t~"toel &uvte:c &vbpayov" "vuvt Tt£Pl ~; I bt£L61i y', ~ )..l.tA.E:, 131
THE IAMBICTRIHETER
199
i5aSovTo l6'; ~ITIDP' tiutv 6ELac. ou &:c,µcaaarrD\Jfi:i1,J"Ib ~ t,:,ntw." tyw 6t tQ
676-8
I btp1,c~.11w ~ 'tE Yn'tEI, Ioo' fiv tv ,:ayq:x,:
>«¥)Law'
dnavra la I fn£1.ta tatc; ~1.c;I t6Loouv tieiiol.nm 1023/4 tyw 1,.IEV e:tµ' b KUiJV'lntx> cx,O )'OP 6nui> oot 6' e:t:ra:a.,Cex:1~:11. 'u' I b \)C)i:Bot:toV KU\O.. 1 1204 tyw 6' buv&:Ne:00 • I I J tyw 6' ye:. Men. Sam.
70
111 180
fiv f:vta08' •I~ a'i I.I£ OU am• wm ,m,ay)O.t' Itnt~ -rot, tKe:t tyW 'tOP00K £ l~ llxovta 'tOUWV_L lCt ~Le,
2 26 b
2 35
-rauc; ~
m ,:o0
TtaAJV, I b SullV "tote; aootc;
ro llP'iYU'
YQIJ0UTT.Pi't't£1.v, I~
~
tv, ntne: 1.v, I ~ex:.xn1. KaVOOv 284 e:C't' - ou ).tyw 6' , 4',qxe,, I npq; ~ 'tOO't' tyw 321 tyw OE ioati. YCAN, in 't0UC,~ ~ 388/9 'tb rm6Lov, rliv yp;s{Jv I~L~ noi:t. I J 6n ,:oO,:' cive:1.¼.a,v;I11 61.a 'toO-roHat iJ "CLlCQ.L;
2 37 ~
Tf0e:
633 "tt.J08' tl«J.'Jbve:uTUX.TJ1J.' I tu:wii;> ye:yovtw.1. Asp.
Dysc.
~~ ~ptq)ci.v.
Ill
fX.N6/) YLVE"'ttll.; 11 tyw utv t~lf.wae:v· ~ 0' tyw ~ tn.itP:ii. , I~e:c. 't0UC,~ 48 ~Cov ol Tt\&Catoi.. I 11 ~ ye: ~ 203 Nicv dye:T' e:tc. iatoov·1 -ea-ciZlv~ 2 2 3-5 e.:xiXJ.iiN 'tPI.C&JV Maovl61.' fl~ 6twa wvov ~ • i;u,v Ifxe:1.v • -cau-mc; ~ v..&:lV nc. tK AuKL~ l~rm.1. BLa1. 2 t.l>u.Viv, 'tb WJ.KP:lLOV 6' I 08£v ~ot,111. 39-41
62
96 163 229
2 60
'tL
ro utv ~ ..v Y6? ItoV ~· aOfe:1.TTDMJ 6.U.' ou ~1. ¼e:1.v,I T1µCXJWtr1'K£V&t. 1,1:>1. e:Cuin 61.a-iplBe:1.v,I~ 066' ~1. "6n 6t 'tOO't' ~ ITt01P£1.V 1,1:)1, 00k&> ut.u.axn 6' ti utrnlP lae:t;> 8u£ 1.v n vL e:u8£vroOv"t' 16£ L -eatoO Bloo, &,c,v av yp!,vov qitpe:1.vloovrita.1.mv wxnv
2 75I 6 t-6.llPtina't'
200
RHYTHM AND METRE
348 dJ6tv 66t.w.£tc n).i:i(;, lidlTlV 6t lOCCJl"D8Etc 373/4
·ro(h:6v T£
Ul'V~,~Eave'
c:\1.n
tvox~•
mitv lni:xxn6vm 1:• ~. 386/7 ~ ~ ur#v lurrb 'tT,8C&:,cTt.\,O(; oe6t.Eautvi, 1,11LQ(;T' ,I fA£U8£pl~ 6t TI.ill; 400 6.U.' ttrttv rlrru~ -ro~tov 1:o6C 466/7 1:i: -cfic ~ &rt£t. ,ITPt.ad6At.',£tnt l.lOt., ~; 11 id! Sc»a.K;.I 11 OE, vll ~.:a 544/5 ot.6. -r.:JJl:voCnt txwl¼£t.v, Jil. TOb(; 8E:ouc;, fAK£t. 6t u • au,:6-.a:rov It0 rq::dY1J.• £ tc u,v -r6nov. a 3112 cuAA£~ o' aurov.ll I qw.QJEtc, rt¥>Yla. oCnt'5ft.ov kPLV£t.(;IOEOl1t6v ,:a) vdioJ; n::un:,6'
tvw
Lye.
Alex.
154 &:a::Jp,.a Ut.anwx1' lt~£00t.V 41 7 UJV JJl:v~
~
•Ht.00\/IE{JUl..10\q; Bt.CUA T t.ei
THE IAHBIC TRUIETER
201
TABLEXVIII
CAESURA MEDIA
Archil. Sem. Solon
-et.•
word-end at 6
monosyll. in 617
CM
4.54% 7.47% 5%
4.54% 7.47% 5%
-
-
0.68%
-
+el."
-
-
Aesch.
11.4%
8.61%
2.79%
Soph.
12.97%
9.6%
3.38%
Eur. M~d.
11.29%
10.01%
1.28%
2.11% 0.32 1.64% 1.73% 0.95 0.39% 0.88%
11.65%
9.21%
2.44%
0.99%
0.44
Or.
1.45% 0.69
Aristoph.
15.04%
3.69%
11.35%
9.33%
2.02% 4.62
Men.
19.94%
4.83%
12.69%
15.11%
2.41% 5.26
Lye.
1.56%
1.42%
0.14%
0.07%
0.07%
I Epigr.
* The ratio
2.45%
2.45%
of the occurrences has been added in italics.
-
-
of caesura
nedia
-
without/with
elision
202
RHYTHM AND t1ETRE
3. 3.
caesura
If
media
verses
in
meters
complying
boundary
which
with the
the
preferred
case
sent
-
ples
of verses
in
the
conceived
occurs
it
'near'
this
is
rhythmical
middle
of
demand
of
trimeter
also
a caesura
(cp.
proper
caesura,
be reckoned
verse
the
variation
without
a
may therefore
the
tragic
as
for
a structural
rhythmical
there 3.2.
are
the
among the
- although,
the
and
triword-
of course, movement
only
very
is
in ab-
few exam-
above).
Even in Sophocles, where the percentage of such verses without any caesura is the highest, this amounts to only 0.93%; in the Orestes there are more verses without a caesura than in either of the two Aeschylean plays studied or in the Nedea. (See Tahle XIX hPlow.) In comedy, rir,id has
in
where
other
ceived
there as
positions
while
usually
structural
tially-structural infrequently
Also
being
this
overlapped
in at
a very
in disregard given is
verse
3 .4. I,
tragedy
occurs
word-end' even
the
not
sub
the
by one single
appears
.:!:n.
4 .4),
form of quite
in
only
of
(see
The linguistic
remarkable:
positions
rhythm
respects
no caesura.
equally
the
to of
least
the
3.1.l. case
less verses
a number of verses one of
weak word-boundary of
the
be
definition above),
-
the
in
potential
is
the caesura (to
be con-
of
'poten-
comedy not caesura
word.
For suggestions about the function of such verses devoid of caesura in Henander see SISTI (1968) 126: indicating a change of tone, or ch~racterizing the speaker. On the other hand, in Menander verses without a caesura often simply illustrate the integration of colloquial speech into the metric schema of the trimeter (compare the comparatively free use of resolution in the comic trimeter). In those cases, no particular regulation of the rhythmical movement seems to be aimed at.' 51 The rhythmical function of a weak word-boundary at a caesura position is not necessarily to be explained by a hypothesis of 'phonostylistic variation' as brought forward by DEVINE & STEPHENS (1978), i.e., stylistic variation at the phonological level within a particular dialect. Such variation would range "from lento and formal that those preand to allegro and informal" (323). "It is clear postpositives that stand both at Porson's bridge and at the caesura in tragedy do so because they are treated, for the sake of the metrically relevant phonological rule, as separate phonological words in lento and as part of the following (or preceding) phonological word whether the hypoin allegro 0 (325). However, it seems to be doubtful thesis of phonostylistic variation necessarily implies a differentiation of speech tempo: cp. DEVINE & STEPHENS (1981 (TAPA)); see Ch.I n. 25,and 3.1.1. above. The linguistic embodiment of different de-
THE IAMBICTRIMETER
203
grees of 'formality of speech' might in itself for a phenomenon like 'phonostylistic variation'.
sufficiently
account
However this may be, such verses as have a sub-optimal wordboundary at a caesura position should not be regarded as rhythmically deficient, but rather as testifying to a different conception of which word-boundaries may serve as a caesura and which may not. It is doubtful whether a prefix-boundary too can be conceived as a realization of the caesura, as e.g. TACCONE(1904) 101 suggests. ( For example, 244 'HO.l ~ rt6:U.' tnl£L'HOXLL 6l'HO.LO\I~
Aesch. Suppl. Soph.
969 ,:l oii,:a. 1:006' tnl £YY£>Ji'E;vdv Ha"'t'O.; )
Ai.
See also BURY (1886) who regards even a verse such as ~ ~I 6t.a ~ Aes eh. Pers • 501 : OTPC1TOC,
nnya,
as an instance of hephthemimeral caesura. In principle, it is just possible that one could perceive a difference as coapared to a verse in which none of the caesura positions has been realized even by a prefix-boundary. One should not go so far, however, as to suggest that such a prefix-boundary would prevail over an incidence of caesura media; e.g.
I mlrlYPLc..:utvov
Soph. Phi 1 • 2 26 & Lal\1T£C bowxvfi,:' - and not 1.c.,
6:nlnYPLc..:utvov,
as TACCONE,
has it. Some examples
realization
of verses
of the caesura)
Aesch. Ag. 326
without
94
(or with a sub-optimal
are:
ot \.£VY6':> ~!CX:UX,Lv TtETI:ttAJ40T£C
628 ~ i:rrt£!~ Soph. OR
a caesura
&cpoc OKOl'tOO
-rorctv8ot1\ 'HO.l!ti\(;
302 n6ALV 1,1£\1,d l«ll
tuf\c \J.Ux.Tit rrtpL
iiu'i~L(;,
(AXM:L(; 6' ~
455 'HClll'tt'WXbc avd i TtAOlOLOJ, etvrw fn1. I
5 38
1\ -ca,pyov &Jc00 !~LOLlJ.L
oou -ro6E:
tv 1filJ4xl.ywlll(; d;Uoc, i:rrt£jlJJ'i 'Kin8£tv
615 MCl>C0V 6t ldv 1085 ll0t
1
416
571 573 991
~ Y£'VOI;
6>.A' Et!tiit ~ f,:' ~ oCl,.IOL't0ACIC;.6>.A'Iooxb ~ Y6voc tar£.UOV, frvlK' i t~y6'.a,v tvcl> liv &'I nc _ 6.Ua[ ,:6v6£ lJOL ~ ~ ~ l,Ll:cx,c, o?aj ~LOK£LC A£Y£LV
1386 f'i>u.(:rtdy'. Phil.
UL~
1288 1'Tl7,c: Et~;
6(:n[OEU'tt+)OV~3:1;
204
RHYTHMAND METRE
Eur. Ned.
1151 114
~
6 90
or.
tc ~I
tot-£!001. ~ At yd), )«DU,Ot'6(; Itot-l I lJOI, nrivuiJv TT0CJLC ¼"1v ldo'. oo 161 I~ t.cr.lq)LAOq;
453
4 6'
6' QI.IPL !Tbv IUu'm.1.ll'lotpr~ ,:QpOV I
1. "tOOC otxt"CO(; 13 unb -enc &:nivnc MO.l !tile q:Q'tVrlClGL Ti1N 'I.Pf:liN 4
&CrtEP
MO.lltil
mya8ij
Yo\QLKl
I J Tl,
~
m-c£P;
µ' ~ q,1.>..&tc
t:>t 'tficl,cap5l~
'Ul'V~ btl U1V~ uiv lF...&0,nic l41cc61.rrtoc ~ ~ ~ -.6p t'oOll'llL~
5
TWlY6c 6£t npxn;pl6£mL -coi:i; otKt,:ac
45
1'11\0.¥))'6mt'OVMall61.aaohJttlt'6v t'L\0.
100
ea.,4\.U1"C
112
&t.601. X.'
129
~
Lwc.1.61.MOV 'ITDtll)LOVi 481 tvwa& vil -rov l -~ ~ Men. 5am. 8 &Ivm1.6lov, µ&µvrn.atvoi;~ tC> 170 ~ -coot'OVl'HCll I~ ~ aCrrij ~ 228/9 c:J.,o•e;t ~, Ji,.l'Ul'V 'Aan\.dv, o[En wv ~ ft''· r:»K,a:JlCAJV l«ll I 'tiilu ~ inc;
Dysc.
16 UOUTE lC'tQA£A£1.µµtvou
28/9 In
Lycophron 's
caesura
(or
~
n\.C OUVTETPI. uutVf1V 6 ', qJC>t &::nu:: t
tnJ£T~
urrh>i:-hv!t,A.1.xlavTbv voOv ~tTPC,aYEI. YotJ fi -cGiv~ ta.in£1.pla.
b TD.it
trimeters
rather,
l,Ll.l(poO
Tt£TPCfC i:6tt
there with
a
is
one example of a verse
sub-optimal
realization
sura): 1o12
Aa86vm >ca.lTbv i b. .l\.UH.C.R,Olwv TtO"tWV
of
etc.
etc.
without the
a
cae-
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
206 TABLE XIX
VERSES WITHOUT PENTHEMll1ERAL, HEPHTHEMIMERAL OR MIDDLE CAESURA
-P or H
% of -P.
CM
tt•
-P.H. CM
Archil. Sem. Solon
-
-
-
Aesch. Soph. Eur. M~d.
2.95% 4.31% 1.67% 3.16%
2.79% 3.38% 1.28% 2.44%
94.87% 78.35% 76.47% 77.14%
0.15%* • 0.93% 0.39% 0.72%
18.25% 22.17%
11.35% 15.11%
62.21% 68.16%
6.89%· • 7.06%
66.67%
0.07%· •
Or
Aristoph. Men.
Lye.
0.2%
0.14%
Epigr.
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
• Trimeters with caesura media expressed as a percentage without either penthemimeral or hephthemimeral caesura • .. Some of the percentages added slightly divergent from those the rounding off to two decimal
of
verses
up to those in the CM column in the left column, as a result places.
are of
THE IAMBIC TRIMETER 3.4.
In the
iambic
movement which by
recurrent
after
is
or
the end returns
of
only
in
rhythmical
circumstances:
3.4.1.
In
the
607: of
the
trimeters
of
by the
(see
XX, P• 214 below).
have
Table
incidence
come down to
word-end
both
convincing
at
us
pos.
one:
authors
see
verse
profile
colon the
8.
by means
So
of
the
trimeter
near
(3.4.1).
within rising
stressed
pos.
iambic
rhythm
studied
early
the
first
colon
of
movement may be obor
only
iambographers, rising
of word-end
under
in
clausular
at
either
certain
are
10 and at
two instances pos.
see
8 (one
(more
effect pos.
Only among the verses
helow);
is
rising
below.
the
there
the
10 and/or
second
a (basically)
complished
pos.
of the
3.4.2.
tritr1eter
at
rising
stressing
the
metric
of word-end
the
of
caesura,
(basically)
some of
the
a falling
hephthemimeral)
a similar
end
of
the positioning
trimeter
the
word-end
into
to its
Concerning
near
characteristic
modified
(penthemimeral
served
trimeter,
incidence
being
the
207
ac-
is
10 or pos.
8
of Semonides
that
a trimeter
with
of
of which
is
not
a very
(p.
256f .)
XX A, and 4.5.2.
Table
than)
below. frequency of word-end at In the case of Solon, the comparative pos. 8 seems to reinforce the effect of his preference for short realization of the anceps in pos. 9 (see 1.1.1. above), producing a single-short rising effect at the rhythmic level as well (11.'i •.. rather than 1.i... ). 37 .5% of the short ancipitia being preceded by word-end against 25% of the long realizations: see also the connection between the high frequency of short syllables in pos. 9 and the high incidence of word-end at pos. 9 to be observed in Solon' s verse: 1.1.1. above. It should be realized, however, that the total number of trimeters involved is very small. (The two examples of a long syllable in pos. 9 preceded by word-end are:
3 6. 2 6 -tGN oCM:K •
6Ndlv ndvto8Ev
I no1.e:Ou£voc; u
37. 5 Q(Wi:£\1 dv µ£ lGt Q>LAOV I 1t01.0LQ't0 no (independent) reason to assume correption In 75T. of
tragedy the
a clausular
trimeters,
pos.
8 or
with
increasing
Or.;
this
Pnd only
such
at
both
implies at pos.
by the pos.
effect
frequency,
up to
a proportional 8 especially:
8.
17% of
of
realized, word-end
The last the
reduction see Table
there
is
(d'1.}.
is
incidence
10 and pos.
(in which cases in
(more
at
pos.
possibility
trimeters of
trimeters
than) 10, at occurs
in Euripides' with
XX A, p. 215 J.elow.
word-
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
208 In the early
Hellenistic
iambography,
occurrences
except
of verses
Lycophron 10;
epigram
there
is
comparatively
for
the
situation
the
fact
with word-end
a remark.ably
few of the
is
that
there
at both pos.
large
proportion
occurrences
more like are
that
in
some more
10 and pos.
8; in
of word-end at pos.
are
accompanied
by word-
end at pos. 8 as well. It may be noted here that the supposed general rhythmical kinship of Lycophron's trimeter with the verse of early iambography has reality only in so far as they have in common a scarceness of resolution and a comparative infrequency of word-end at both pos. 10 and pos. 8 (while as regards the high occurrence of single hephthemimeral caesura Lycophron 's trimeter resembles the verse of Semonides but As far as their clausular pracnot that of the other iambographers). tice is concerned the differences outweigh the similarities: in Lycophron, the anceps in pos. 9 is realized by a long syllable far more often (53%, against 29%, 31% and 20% in the iambographers: Table 11 above); the occurrence of word-end at pos. 10 in Lycophron's trimeters is higher even than in tragedy; accordingly, in Lycophron 20: of the occurrences of word-end at pos. 8 have concomitant word-end at pos. 10, against only 3.6% in Semonides (while in Archilochus and Solon there is not a single instance: see above). All things put together Lycophron's objective seems rather to be to fit an extremely formal vocabulary into the metric profile of the iambic trimeter, without thereby falling short of the principal de51 (Contra HAAS (1962) mands of verse rhythm (caesura, clausula).' 5102; SCHEIN (1979) 60: Lycophron as an 'imitator' ot the trimeter of the iambographers.) In comedy the distribution positions end at
is pos.
without This
10 and/or
situation of
hemimeral subject return 1.1.1.1.
tallies the
to tendencies the
to basic above.
with Parson's
verse
with
at the
10 occurs
observation of
caesura,
that
the
(variation
movement near This
pos.
frequency
concerning
relatively
brought
trimeters
forward
the comic trimeter
is less
of course
at pos.
in the a pent-
alternation
of the rhythmical
10:
the
caesura,
see 3.1,
essentially
8
often.
without
below.
iambographers,
showing word-
of the movement at is
verse-
word-end at pos.
the end of the verse):
situation
Law: see 4.4.
in the early
8; in Aristophanes
word-end
comparative
three
50-54% of the trimeters
at pos.
or hephthemimeral
movement of
E.g.,
much more even,
accompanying
context
of word-end over the last
also
connected
209
THE IAMBIC TRIMETER Ar eh 11.
19. l oO l,K:>1. i:a. f\jye:w i:oO ~ IU£A£ I. V
\.ltv "fflV~ ~l>«»dlv o0-to1. nat' ~c; tE~,I ao ~ vOv e:i:AE:c;at Xl.d!i l«ll l,lty' tfrp&> I~.
yuva.i., q0.'tl.V
23.8
23.18/9
I ae:6c &Le; ~ ~ w6rt1.TtE 1.&ln I~ 1. 1.9/10 vtx.Jta 6' o6&:lc; 6:rnc; oo OOH.Et!~ 6cwcbo:nov
Sem. 1.5/6
mou-a., "tE
b.'t&AEUTftcxi.
>a)'Q8oto1.v tEEX:lfu.1. Il~
£!J8Etav e:tc; f'Har:n'ov~l6ll(l)V
Solon 36.19
36.23 d rote; t\.avrlo1.01.v ~vcavE:Vlt0'tE
at pos. 8: Archil.
21.2 25.2 3 5. 1
Sem. 1.1
W'tl'll(£'V Cw'tc; '1yplnc;ltn.1.0"tEXirK:; 6.U.' ~ dUt,> lQX>lrivltalve:mi. l3oOc; ton v ~ui. v ~'tl'IC Itv ot>,l T.l
mt, T£AO(;1,11:vZe:uc; fxe:1.l~wrcoc
l • 13 / 4 qi8e: lpou:,1. 'VOCn> 1.,
TT.turtEL lJ£Adrc.£1. T' • ATPE1.&'.i'jv tc t06E !OKT1ITTE1. IO"ttyoc; 497 Cwlc; qx l~
orn.a.Lve:CI~
lru.p6c;.
505 ~ p;s.y£1.d1N UnlcxJv luu%; lruxea.twaJ«&JV lx.\uELI~
~ be. ~ 11T£.CW I"tJXXCi>
ba:noc at.rri'i1.J ~ ~ldYOL l¼ov ~ nv' tiutv, a; Cq:XJ.LC::
TTQOUrtE}.WE:\,I tc;
224 &i,£>.E ~-
I
I
2 33/ 4 1j >OJtL ya.i.Q(;(~J, .•~XJ.L n6cof; atA€ 1.c;,
C')('VC)c; & i.c;; U£i:a8oMInrivtwv Iy AUKU. 281 / 2 citO){\NO\.ni.OE, U£TaOl.6ouc;ITW,N I~ ~LOV
~lvcxx,1.c;I tintc;. 688 fil«A> Ya,:) ~ ouuuixwvIkE'VOVI 54:,., 891 abv 6' CXJH. bnL viiN auyyovov,I~ I~ 1202 oo-c' QAX1.uocrrtq,u,«:. ~v6' lnµtvltxw 6x,'Aov"CEJq)EX~
1244 TPl.ax>t'c; q,i.>..oi.c;y6p de ay~,l6i.xn!µi.ci 1669
6Aac,-rcpw c56Ea1,µ1, anv IMUE:I.v IOrn
RHYTHMAND METtu..
212
Aristoph.
vuxa•I~; 6M.' oCM.btElac-to rote tiJotc I Af\c C1Ca8c:tvroa'IftcS:r'l I't"OTTOU; 1494
Asp.
59
lllPO!; 'toO't"'lchti> lJtv 'f1DtXLi::4\'tPCXPLJ.Kt>I "COUtoJ I't"O't"E
MXPLOLOV fiv tvtaOO'
106 ~ b
u~·
111 w.q)LO{;~-
4 64 Dysc.
l-ri. oov; 111't".:v6P:
l-calJ1XJ(;I~ TVlALlCOC;IJ tiJot lJtv ~ I6oKc CcIvttxiJv. - ... COi.~ o-ttva1., 1•A1P£1.ex'.i'Jv dY)(08£v, xuvc!>c 6l>L'tDC.ldP; II to-nv, ITl 6' o(Jx.1.; 1-di~ SEoO• ., ...
1J natf+XJ,6' l6vt:l!Xi7vqxbn't' 351-3 Y'U\O.L, j>«l't 1 ~ ~•
ML8f\ otee:1.s;; dxp:xM,JC ).t;ye:1.,·
ty& 6' I ~ TtLcnd oou 'tEklJTIPl.(1 8E:ouc ITCIXXX LTt£ tv ro ~I. • 530-2
dwfl •A'tPElai, f\KEL.
ITL tdnt.
~UC
cC&.ll,.l&JVOORP
6' ~ Lwta.~
8Po'tc7Jv
'tc7Jvv0v· lnap1.s;v6'.>dlte: ouv'te:M~ rr.6>..1.~
RHYTHH ANDMETRE
216 3.5. I,
6,
and,
to
most cases The istics
ura rent:
Greek
factors
are
especially
the
already
in
addition
(again,
case, of
pos.
initial
the
both
greater
Monosyllable Soph.
part
pos.
in
these
Phil.
appear
to
of caes-
be concur-
beginning
1.1.1.2.
of a phrase
of a colon. above):
to
and
This
occurrence
cases
is
pos.
1
,
the
only
the
emphatic
and
-
occurrence
positions
1
¼ rq::q;or>COV, bC6t£r;; 880 tv', I f)vlK' av k6rror; 1,1 ~ TTOTt ToL
J.DX~
1
of
in which
has no elision.
AAELC 6
which
phrase-
l:
ou 6'jeC
this
proportion
smaller
elision Cp.
a mono-
In
the
rel'larkahly
below). 12 -
of
rhythmically).
between
3.5.1.
of
that
is
58
480
is
ot 6
e.g.
enjambement;
relationship
5 and
the
alternatively
of the monosyllables
49
show that
as in the case
beginning
l implies
(see
pos.
in pos.
In
character-
differences
emphatic
regard
effect
in
monosyllables in
syntactic
effects
syntactically
supposed
monosyllables
the
(cp.
with
the
word-ends
confirms
the
6,
a monosyllable elided
in
Here,
emphatic
Semonides
XXI, P• 223 below).
individual
as well.
often
(poss.
U\V6'.
ou6', oO't', 't'OV6', syllable
inherent but
rhythmically
As indicated
Table
rhythmical
is
at colon-beginning
elision.
partly
and
common in
see
involves
relevant
monosyllable as
8:
language,
linguistic
the well
degree,
may be
the
itself,
most frequently
~onosyllable
situation of
occur
a lesser
the
stylistic
as
151
MonoRyllables
THE IAMBICTRIMETER
217
with enjambement: ~
94/5
~I
t~i:ctv
l,4Uov
f\ v1.MCiv ~. ~
464/5
brawlx' 4v
~
TWJ0vInut v &C>a,J, Tf'lVl.'MCIOe'~,=1&E8a. ~ OW -~ ~ ,:do' CU< WtaYYEAOC
568/9
wtvl~
tmt~: YML"CO6t
779/80 ~lo(jpt.e~
'tE
~,6no1.
TtOTt
flyct"Ca.l. lll.fflP
1442/3
ZE:0C • 100y6p e;vc,te£ LO. ~ I. l:3Po"COLC • Monosyllables Phil.
in pos. 8 (with forward pendency):
130
l:3Poi:otc&:n1.mq;lTOC;, oo 'K£L'VOt.Ot.v,lo0.).
1 27 OU"t' OUJ.L' tTDLIX)UC:1 00-t'
~,~
lro
241 'Hdv lJ£V-me'tn.i.tv ~t.Ot.V
o'I £?i.
613 Et\.101.cTE ntun:£1.v CJIUl,.lf3aA', ot ~l 764 & Z£0 t.lxn i:E
znvoc"IU.lou
"t£1~
Id>
874 JGt OlJOIJ£VC1L\(&) i:ot:01. ~t.V 955
or.
TUlJX>C TD-rnP 6l6c.iD1.vbc.y6vo1.01.vjolc.
au
73 T'IQ;, ir>'1Xl.M1t.VC1,"'[£ HaOLYVrrroe "'[£
104
au \IUV
lobe
x.ap1.v1.101.'tOV qx5f3ovAUD::nl&:Sc (compare 302)
287 i:otc
utv ¼01.c nt'.xA:xxvE, Tote
6' 6:>Yo1.01.vlo0-
~I~
292 Et l-1\"t' be:Cvc:,c ~tv
4 31 ,: l V£C TtOAt. tii1v t~A.;~i,lv-ta.l OE I~;
T'DUlPutv bpj"tEOOtv l.lE, on6 • t nxi:e: 1TDtc 603 1,llkOPt.O{; alwv" olc 6t J.11'1 nln:tOUO't.Vlro 622 MEvtAO.E, oot 6t 'm.5E:iif.yw- ~ -ce:l~ 659 ti;, o' fXEt.V taij8' • ·F.cuu.M,v J.11'1 K"r£LV£ ,au.
ss2
920 alJ'tOI..PYQC - otTtEOHat l,DVC>t. a.,C0t01. lyf\v -
M col YE KOt.vff ·ta!rtn wat TIXJX.£ t.v l.lEI6E:C. 1088 f:>.£~ "tOUUOV ano\lTTDt.'IJ,t. lot 1600 1i yap 6lwa.1.ov Ctivoe:; I lwat JCt:Xl"tEtv YEIyfk;. 1 Aristoph. Nub. 79 ~ oti"t' dv f\6t.O"t ao-rbvat£YELtx11.1J,t.; Iniili;,; 201 ~la utv ai.rcn.:. II "tou"tt 6t 1-rl; 107 4
{compare 848, 132
ex>-roe, >«JaEu6£.., ;
u in
1'0V •An:6A,\w •
1186)
vwµ£'V Ioe,.
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
220
aCrr' tKEt&:v lalJ 732 bnti o' ,& M\lJ..',~ T' e:tlJ..lla6c• 949 Et ur'tlJ..' biof:1., bt1.Tpat.f;UE:1.v, fTEQO(;lalJ 4 7 1 >CQLfwNP01;00cn v d\q:)E'
Eq.
(compare 1351)
TOU-ro1., b l,1.1.~
-roe, 6kinEKlo1.01. ot:oe• c:htJ6 &xxx,v; 11 e:Co:lu', t\
1018 1158
lnoO; ~
ye
I ou.
lrpJotc ~ tv"tatla'. ~r, &'i 1.1£lou 271 rdvta
41 9
-caO-m.,J:l,1.1.>CPi vri.
35, 464,
D!fSC•
II .\t)'E
..,
&44)
IT L;
6t
Atye:1.c; ll6m.Uaviiu£v, tKETE:UW OE -1()not': 12 5 ti~. 11wx.ovcOlllC ~utvoc i: 1. I v0v 342 1.1£LlldxLOV; l)ou6' ffEati l,101., 8tATLOTE- I1Jnii>;; 86
Dysc.
(compare 630) 377
tyw. n:orrctov 6t cl
453
tn1.etv-ce:calJrol 't6Ma.
Illooc. >CJ."CCIJ'tL'VOOOL. Iy~,
TOOT' b::Ttl •
(compare 587) Lye. 448 cl
l:atPCJ.XOV ~c,
•yM:roo "te:lvflv
724 nttPXV 6x:i',:,£1. f:D+,a,J, ~ ~ I "Ic 769 >CQLNr\Pi-rou~-
6'1£-m1.stlldv 1209 6f£L lC;t~
968 'H£USE:1. )GUr.) 1391
pos.
Uo> K1.aJ.1.~,
.. Sa.1.l,J0\K.l,JV, Ii:d6' Itv XE+X>CV &'i vt,c, ty& 6' IM' Itvaio£ TL
u.' t5tx,ou;
ITL
u.'loo ~
10: PhJ. 1.
111
l.a'i001. yevtetb-1. nDAUl'tOV' au-cu I'1J116' I~
978 OC\,101..~ OR
63 98s
1111 1290
IM' I~ &:X1 ~ J«Ja' ab't6v" w:ll1tv' c.UAOV, In 6' 1M et \.di •JruP£..CPD' n ttKOUCa• 1v0v 6 • 1tne:t k£L\OJ yt. -ro1. &'I me, bc>J.lte&'" It, 6' IfcL>
l\orlIndvonAoc; •/>CYELuN 01JXlTb.:; 'X.(&¥Xi:, kOVL£t., lrt£6Lo.5' 6':>~, ~ ...... 495 6D£.«1t-rplcl~tv 6&iw."COV l6'xrliv fox.ov, ~ rraptc,6" 61:Xlv
....
21 ~ l,l#;v~,
l~t .....
fi;
cSE:Eui~
mm'1I~-~
>emd; 224 TtL~ ~,,:a.Lcltattv 6a.D8lac ...... Ono 479 CEUY'),zpL >Gl ~l{n\a,L)JOV 'YU1V 51 ~,
&.Jn:v0c (,,., ~ un.£PTtA.Aovta. 0£Llll,L~ tt£'tPOV ., ... 1,IXVLQLOLV j6\.qaC£LV 'YoPcdooC'4a.L 8Eac
481 an:£LP1LC b,)C£ ~LC ... ., Or.
6
37 42
ou >.ourp' llxi>c.£~,. tc ncl (irrespective of rPsolution) hAs bP.en itnlfcs. (HPre, ;i,:; els£•wlien•, resolutions nccurrinp, 1n propf'r nAmPR
p)(r]111h•1l.)
50% 34.97%
100% 68.71%
1()()% 49.08%
77.77%
! i ~ ~
1-i
~
THE IAMBICTRIMETER 4. Complementary cial at
tendency
effects
particular
verse-positions,
about
rhythmic
positions positive
and the metric
For
side
comedy at
pos.
5 (as
tion
is
as
with
this
verse
which
the is
word-boundary
rhythmic
and
the
repetition
of
comedy
mically
the
on the the
expense
posi-
the
aim of between
with
level
the
at
infrequency
is
avoided
into
groups
movement
of
4 (out-
percentage
of
as word-boundary
at
monosyllabic at
produces
of verse,
of caes-
pos.
high
verse-
of certain
the incidence
evidently
type
reverse
of word-end
Just
the
the
posi-
a rhythmical
so the coincidence 1
10
,
the
structuring
of
to metra,
corresponding
verse-beginning
of
being
dis-
effects.
avoided; clear
other
some of
the
e.g.
relative
this
the
at
relation
at
merely
affecting
and metron-boundary
on the
spe-
of word-end
towards
satisfying
in conformity
in
verse
In
being
metron-boundary
the
favoured
as
the
produce
of the verse.
situation).
sought
avoided
contribute
low percentage
least)
well
following
effect
the
to
incidence
of word-end
tendencies
example,
in
level
be understood
ura.
above
is
speaking
occurrence
rhythmical
words
seem to
a rhythmically
can
to
word-end
also
The infrequent
4. l.
referred
by means of recurrent
Such tendencies
bringing the
to the
rhythmical
tions.
231
contrary,
metrical
of
the
balanced
Aristoph.
it
structure internal cola:
Nub.
Men. Dysc.
coincidence
of
word-
seems to serve of
the
structuring see 3.1.
and metron-boundary
verse of
the (not
the
purpose
is
of keeping
infrequently
verse
into
not
at
the
two rhyth-
above).
13
tmb tf'ic;; Eanavric;;!kQ.t tf'ic;; q.d.4M1c;;lkQ.tti3v 'I.P£{iJv
23
&t' tT'rpLOlJJlVj -rov XOl"IJU"tLa'V. IOC)JOL ~
22s
~wl
686 91.
~AOE~, IME:MlO~, 1 ·~LVL~ U£Yc&>, a(, ai'xxN, 1029 Mat
invariably
excluded,
should
XXIII A shows,
WEST (1982)
is
phrase-initial
Monosyllables
elision"
even
All of phrase-initial
as
one
Table
Menander
monosyllable
that
amongst
As
the far
above).
frequent,
contra
sample,
wv
in our sample
...
froQ Sophocles
involve
...
loull'I t'J..bpav, £~
TL l1' dv£T£; ITl l1'! 61-liyw6£; ToO x.6':)1.v;
...
1247 'Hal.~ OLl«ll.OV,Id y'
...
1 348 ~ CTCUY"Vbi; aCC.:,.,, ITL u'
... I ~,
! tn
eou;\atr;
a'iT'
tio.Cr;
fx.£1.r;d\tl>
a
IAMBICTRI.METER
THE
936 t>t 'tfk; Ia£,noi: obv
632
to
the
general
the
tragic
tendency
poets
there
is
only
most
fre-
resolution:
n68'Itnl split
~
ouwolQ. KUNA£i:c
resolution
1s the
to avoid
the
occurrence
Aristoph. Nub.
t\.lt lJl;v c,b rr.o.UaJc;rov m'ttt::>' ltNJUVE:1.s:; ~ 62 Tt£Pl ~ &'i ·~ ltAOI.~ 10 &oru:p !£~, Eu:rrl6'l~ - tvw 0' 6'rlv 2 14 6,;U,' ti hJxE&ll l,IA7\/ noO 'at Cv; 111 6nou 'atl v; airtnl. 29
-
-
of
234
..
RHYTHMAND METRE
...
737 aO-dicl&tL
~
~L
°''td&.,;,_I ... ... ~
t~
>.ty£
&.o.ti:>t«Ec.-cbvMt)£LTT0Va 23 a&J-lO I qxi&L TOO~11 a!rto. 11 Tlivu ~.
884 Eq •
~ ~I~
a0T~. II ~, (where the rhythmical peculiarity may serve to reinforce 67 •yMIN 6L' t1Jt l&XJ't'LYCUIJ£\IOV; the 'joke'); 26
~~l'tbv
651
ol 6' ~ >Gl nix,c; ~•
728
Tlve:c ot ~;
OOH. dnL;'
~ l6rrb ~LOC..:N
tl,lt
1159
Men. Sam. 262 417
658
T£
lbt£xT(V£0].V
l6nb~ ~;
>Gl TOJTOVL
autbvltTL&,VOUl,ITV d.yarw:n" vOv 6' tn&t OTt»'i~lutvav, 0J »xla. 'aTt roi:c'OAWJ'CloLc ~ l66L'N&lvf6E:Laa.WC1li:ov 6ronon1\I ooxt l~'tOAEL"-tt.> 't#lvMv 6c.(¥JflXXU.
Asp.
183
Dysc.
~l6~CJK00aa.c. ~ov tn.iic;. II ~Lav 82 in.:~a• lb61.~, in.:ve:mc.. II Tl rouro, rut; 14 3/ 4 i6asLMCJWttl.; II >Gl rq:>EOTt Y' o(rccx,l. 70
oooiv
II au"t6(;; I () ...'&.avw, ~ I. • ... etl "CLOT£. au 6l; "'COU-a.> 257 b U\'VXMINLO'I~ oo-roc tanv 0v >.ty£c.c; ... ... 296 1t"CLJX.~ I661.>crt& le taTc. Sucoo::w:m:nov "' ... CJ66tvI66LK£tc tnA'!c;,uinw i:c ... ... 6l; NaKOntJ8E 4 15 (Js.&£ tc TID£ t T' ; II oOcSt IKOXllav t:yc,;rytOE • 348
"'
According labic tion
to
...
l\k£"'ITd>..L...v. IJ
807
b IJoo£1.Swv OE - II NO.l AOAELCfn; alrt6v, I bt1.KQX>£ tv n&:,1.v, ~ TIDEtv
the
theory
504
here
word which constitutes should be considered
In Aristophanes
the
second
as highly
a phrase-initial short
of the split
monosylresolu-
objectionable.
Nub. one instance 1192
- where there
presentPd,
occurs: t\0. f;li TL 't#lvf\Ai'VT'l'P(XJt&,K£v;I I! Cv', !?:, ut.l.£
is in addition
a salient
enjambement.
THE IAMBICTRIMETER
235 TABLEXXIII
SPI.IT RESOLUTION: PERCENTAGES* Soph. & mono- - monosyll. syll.
Eur. Or. & mono- - monosyll. syll.
pos. I - el.
0
0
0
0
1.03% 0
0.51% 0
1.05% 0.53%
0.53% 0.53%
pos. 2 • el.
0
0
2.63% 1.32%
1.32% 1.32%
4.46% 3.57%
4.46% 3.57%
10.87% 9.24%
10.87% 9.24%
pos. 3 . el.
-
-
-
-
0.46% 0.46%
0.46% 0.46%
1.38% 0.69%
0.69% 0.69%
pos. 4 • eL
0
0
0
0
2.1% 0.7%
2.1% 0.7%
1.18% 0.59%
1.18% 0.59%
pos. 5 • el.
-
-
-
-
2.04% 2.04%
2.04% 2.04%
0
0
pos. 6 • el.
6.52% 0
0
1.65% 0
0.55% 0
0
0
0.49% 0.49%
0.49% 0.49%
pos. 7 . el.
-
-
-
-
2.07% 1.38%
2.07% 1.38%
0
0
pos. 8 . el.
0
0
0
0
2.92% 0
2.92%
()
()
0.64% 0
pos. 9 • el.
-
-
-
-
1.89% 1.89%
1.89%. 1.89%
0
()
pos. 10 • el.
0
0
0
0
0
()
0
0
tot. . el.
3.17% 0
0 0
1.1% 0.22%
0.44% 0.22%
].6% 0.76%
1.52% 0.76%
2.02% 1.52%
1.81% 1.52%
*
Percentage of the sum total poeitions. • Second short • monosyllable
N.B. No occurrences Epigr.
in Archil.,
Aristoph. & mono- - monosyll. syll.
of resolutions ( ...
in
the
Men. & mono- - monosyll. syll.
several
verse-
ulu/). Solon,
Aesch.,
Eur.
/tied.,
Lye. and
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
236
TABLEXXIll A SPLIT RESOLUTION: TOTALNUMBERS (&ratio-/+
Eur. Or.
Soph.
pos. 1
0
0
0
0
elision)
Aristoph.
0
2
Men. 1
I
0 pos. 2
0
0
1
1
pos. 3
pos. 4
-
-
0
0
I
4
I
J
-
-
1
0
0
I
17
0
pos. 6
0
-
-
6
0
0 pos. 7
-
I
2
-
1
3
0
-
1
I
J
0
0
0
I
0
0
-
0
-
I
1
0.5
pos. 5
3
5.67
4
-
2
1
0
5
0
0
2 pos. 8
0
0
0
0
0
0
-
-
-
-
pos. JO
0
0
0
0
0
tot.
0
6
I
4 0.25
10
3
0
pos. 9
0 monosyll.
0
6
• monosyll.
0
0
0
0
0
0
J
I
10
0
3
0 10
J
7
21 J
0 I
1
11
0.9
0 0
0
-
0
0
1
I
0
21 4 5.25
THE IAHBICTRIMETER 4.3.
Word-end after
verse-element rhythm
two shorts
is
restricted
quent
2 it
than
word-end
of
would
produce
cessive
short
without
the
before
to
relatively
is
word-end
distinctness
which occupy
the
certain
position
of a single
conditions
regarding
the
112
of the verse.
At pos.
237
frequent
(in
resolution).
rhythm
as
resolution
Here,
an alternative
(cp.
a monosyllabic
tragedy
3.6.
word in pos.
it to
above),
even
more fre-
may enhance the
which
1 followed
the
incidence in
of
this
case
by three
suc-
syllables.
That word-end after resolution in this position is indeed the is suggested also by alternative to word-end before the resolution is the situation in Aristophanes, where word-end after resolution relatively infrequent, whereas word-end before resolution occurs frequently. Word-end effect of
of
following
resolution
reinforcing
the
rising
of the verse
the beginning
in
this
position
rhythmical
(see
has
the
positive
movement characteristic
3.4.2).
6cnt,:Jac,I&iav 1:00' ~I. lJCAEI. ..... •~, 388 ~1.ovl~v, 6:rnc tv rote 'Htpof;oi.v ..... 7 21 qovm.l y~1. m.T~ oOTEldt.ov ... 9 20 txtnc; I&,,cnn1. roi:o6£ d.Jv waidona,1. v ..... 934 &yaa\l{q.toi.c; TE wat 110X1.~ oQ, Y\NCLI. Phil. 636 nt~oelbc::>lCTJi:n, ·~ ~
Aes
eh. Ag.
7
....
-
....
.....
932 943
6no5c::>c,I ..... t'KVOOl,,.aio', 6n65or;, l~J~ ToO znvbs;·~cue
981 6n65oc;, l&i,e;c \.K)I., Tl:lL, m
....
l O18
Eur.
Hed.
~l.!D'>~ tpi\l,DV, 6no.\t.~ tv
10 TU"tqXll~tl. ......
"Cllv6E: yfjv
....lnac:dtat. • wal 61+>1. .... n,xnlmu:LJ'.Xt)
896 TUTtc:xih,.1£8' ft~,
or.
7
25 ~
v ......
27 0:, ~-,
fxti. 't6fa..l) "to0"tO lJ,tv V
vt:JCP6v;
Kopt.v&Lav
>«J.1. 61.0.Uaxa.,8' ~ 'tLV£t. TCIUTI'IV 6LK11V
n£Pl.ecw,(b'
tf;) To0T'
~I.
llC.£T£W, Tbnbw.Jv; b,01,.1£V.1&mv et 'tdi.L'~· [&Jc (ll£L~8' ... ., • Ib ea.£\IAN6' tc tl&I; nA0\i:n.or;WCN7N ,,rrt,::a. • 1-ro~ T' l>.aacv&v'tl ~ ... w & ytpov, Itvw toL t1)bc ot 6£ Ll&J.L'VCI)¼t LV ... w
296 394 502 544
b \q.lCC la\1Etm1., 'HOU qedvo1.~ w w
941
nc
dv
956
In comedy the positive 2 is
observed
with
the
to a far
circumstance
movement of the (cp.
avoided too,
lesser that
trimeter
below).
4.4.
tendency
On
the
is
to word-final
degree the
in general
other
of the
,
word-end
in accordance
to a lesser after
with the fact is
in pos.
rising/falling/rising
stressed
hand,
resolutions
resolution
apparently
consecutive
even in comedy, in accordance
the majority
113
preceded
degree
resolution that,
is
in comedy
by word-end
(3.6.
and Table XXII above). Aristoph. Nub.
... ...
k&cQl£PE 1-roy,:rqqo't'CLOV, tv' w ... -
19
~ ~
32
&a.ye.., I't'bv tnnov t~~ oC>OfiE w
55
nix>lCX:h v
6 71 / 2
.....I6'mkov· & VU\W.,A.Lav ~ ~ tc 8'\A£1.av oi'x:av. ll -a;,'tP6rlt>; .....l1GA£
11 ~,~ 'yw )QXX)ITDV;11 l,d-\.1.ow.vc ...... Men. Sam. l 0 l
~JnEVl')lX&.lV 6.yd8' • •Aafl\GL ~
/fed. 31
Eiiol~ &.¥::,niam ~ 'tQ Tpo.:C.C .....Tt£6l'I ~ 814 U6vln w.toc .... I(Jntpmwv .>.aa£tv
a/Jvi
1 ~ ai.rtfiv T'D't£t) lmt0Li.J;t;> .tv,,v, 6 6t IIU.u'ta.Ll,flCTC~ >.tx.o;.
Or.
--
u'rcr¥:>m.pl IMa3r'!Y\ILO't0.L 6tiac ...... f'iv yap WC1-r'oC~ ,P.LlavfnAtt. 4-ta.vcmootxw,utV£ Atxouc ., ...6' Itnt o-cpw'tCI() be. ~Ui7v 6t -ro.6£..... IVOCEt, nolc.i,v Ono; oOrta>"Tb J,LtMov..6 1 ...Coovl~lq. At'YW
40 8avoOca. 63 313
4 o7 426
549 Tb ~ 1'l1L\I 'tb 624 658
oov, ..... 16l1'
672 ~
Occurrences
of
otov ~-
'tL 6t; l-m>.at.Tl4Xi:VlJ£ &i:
.....
6' I ~ 66uc.u.;.
resolution
6 followed
in
pos.
word are more exceptional;
or. 439 'tL q::xjv'tc(;; 6 n Nat"'~, 1575
~ ~
oo~ • m.pt
a bisyllabic
Eur.
~OU
t""
1585 cn66oi; fdi.QJ-ros; vtxw,1~ 1594
~L
o-c6'n .....lvtl.1£L cx:s~.cna:rov iii 'ti;D' 6'Juv£Lv qt,vov ltvavtlov 6£ot, ..... d 6' A6AL, t.\a6£ cqd;yL' I ~
592 w"'1v 8'xnoi:oL
involving
lCCl'tQ O-C£)'0(;
117 (I)(; -ro (36£~E+)Of; ......
!'tE+)Of;
o' te ~ o-c4a't0tl&-ravcc,~ov TO t, ~L(; m l-lt'tP~ ' Im:JPN£tv t6u\.d:\.a,v .....
610 ot Men• Sam • 16
~
4o OUVl'lYJ,£\,OC ~ llllXlC ti~ l,1£w. n\t\JV ...... 360 -cobc6.:Uo-rplouc Et, ......tul:l-roLoOtovYEYOVC\QL Asp.
Dysc.
0
105 brv..LC6iJEvc>L -rb 1'QX,\.Ilbocrros; TW')OLOV
......
I
595 /:t&Jv JCaA£i:(;,6.v6:11.' 6:vELP'l)(\)LQ
Ytmh
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
240 At pos.
8 there
resolution,
though
word-end special
at
some occurrences
rather
this
few compared
position.
rhythmical
(basically)
are
These
effect,
rising
namely
of
word-end
to the general
occurrences that
of
following
incidence
seem
to
of
produce
a reinforcement
of
a the
movement near the end of the verse.
E.g. Aesch.
547 (involving
Sept.
amples of this
type,
see 4.5.1.1.
1022 Soph.
OR
a bisyllabic
word;
for
other
ex-
below)
sub fin.
"' ... ~O(; 'Apdc;"!b 6tlroLCXJ6' ...... lCl Ul'a8'~"tELV ...... ~IXEL~'tQ
~
826 lJJl'tlX>(;~uyfh.a.L>Gt TDttcXll>«J."IXJXmvELV
Eu r • Hed. 5 05
6t~L
......
oCMOLC &\.ITll.ttc:Xl I>«J."tbnuvov
vro l1'
- ..
tc 0C4f.'til.lt"tf+>OV. ...... IlarLv 6' hr.> 4 8 7 >«J.t ti'i1v ~ YE l.a'irri:xhqx,v IE[ \aL 8tA£ Lv ...... 671 taU"tl'lC t~i. a' -tr> l,1£AEC>Cjtl,IZN HllMWV ...... 1092 Mv yap cwmv,r'ic;YE }..txO(;ltn(ivroa
or. 60
~
A similar verse-final effect may apply as regards word-end after resolution in pos. 10 in Sophocles' trimeters: 2 out of 6 resolutions, or 33.33%. This percentage is proportionally high compared to the occurrence of word-end after resolution in the other verse-positions - but not in comparison with the general incidence of word-end at pos. 10. The sample, of course, is rather small • oR 719 fA:>1.~ ~
x£+JOt v E tc
1496 TL yap )(O)(.(J\I dn.Eat1.; i:ov In comedy too, word-end after ively
often
(especially
in
a resolution Aristophanes);
seems to be somewhat different,
fact
the
resolution serve is
in
phenomenon is pos.
the preservation
observed
4 as
I~ TU"tt(n I na"dlP in pos. 8 occurs
motivation that
... ...
4aarov
comparatively
well:
both
of the metrical
but here
comparat-
the
rhythmical
as may appear common in
the
from the case
of a
phenomena may be supposed structure
to be one of the main objectives
in general: compare e.g. 3.1. above ... ...• Aristoph. Nub. 7 ~,:• cx'5t ~•lf~ex:nC
of the verse
which
of the comic trimeter \.IOL "tOU(;otKtlXJC ~
100
Etatv 6t. i:CVEC;IU o6K o[6'
179
tK 'tfk; Tll.\alcrcJXIC 8oClJii:1.ovlUPELAETO
1e4
& ·~1.,,
......aHCL~
- ...
"tOU"ttrrooaral in
arpca.;
to
THE IAMBICTRIMETER
241
...... ......
233 l>Jc£1.TtPbc:; au-cnv -cnvt>Q..dfia.1 i;fls; qpovtlOOC 238
t\O. l.1£ 61.fdEr.lc~ fv£x I Iv.rv.ua:i
...-
663 ~'tPUO\,QINClm ,xx!rtb >Gt -rbv ~ (but compare 849 below) ... 736 JT£Pt"COO;ou yap \JOI. To0To ~. I er,
-
.....
816
~n:,
er,Ea.1.1.DV1.E,ITl XPftl,a Tl.iox.E1.C, & n.in:p; ... 61.6.~to. eJiNC1taotl,dnCNI~; ~
Men•
856 168 b:mcu6cucoto.;
S'd m •
Il,al6tv
~;
....
NCl"tt'l'\101:3
306 uutv llCCl'\10{;, Et 't'OU't'tdv1.ovl NCluiatEyCN
.....
oox~
f i:uxe:c. IlloC ir,.,CE• I) >«1Lau· 'tCino YE ... 668/9 oretv. TL dJv ~ fquye:s;,l6etAT£PE ...... >Gt 6E:1.AO'taTE;Iyclotov. nTt£lAl)Clt l.1£ 309
In tragedy
word-end after
-
a resolution
pos.
1, which would produce a verse-beginning
after
which the verse would continue
Compare the following
occurrences ... ...
with a 'monosyllable'
with a falling
movement.
in comedy:
..... ltilV cnpa:tT1yG.;v~ tx Il\Ma> 1205 .....&u&r looylap dUa -co0 TQDBtvT°' ft ~LC
Eq.
Men.
112 ytyov'I ......
cu.~•
Cfj 6t >GL~'tQI.
402 ~ lt\aT.EoEtv. OKTlTt'tOI;nc de
-cnvoCKlav
Some of the rarer cases are: Soph. Phil • 1314 ~ TntttXlITOV~ e:(w)yoOvtd. Eur. or. 483
kEL'VOU
ai.rt6f; d>e' b
- - ld"tla
(pos.4)
(pos. 5)
b ~
(pos.7)
lJ.LMCO.~ (pos .5) ~; 11)nii'),;; ~~ ~~- (pos. 3)
630 l>crnc ~n' 664 nii'),; &'I;
Ini.iti:n yEVl'to£m1,;
-CunoeloOa'
c65tno8'
3 ~ov.
4)
(pos.4)
unb 6' lfTEL\1£ ~ ¼ouc
---
Aristoph. 122 o(h'
OE ( pos.
-yap 65EITt£q'JUH.£, ro1.oO""COC YEY~;
915 13a).,\ovr~· Nub.
avoided at
742 6 n;l
Aristoph. Asp.
is positively
I ...... 849 ~TPUOV' -Ill~
749 yV\,Q.L>GQXJPI.JJKLO' Et TtPLQ1J£\IOC 8ET10A1iv taUTO; NCllaY~
Et.
(pos. 5) (pos.3)
(pos • 5) Men. Sam. 100 &ill v&LOV, TtLxinlrdvt' , •Ano').).ov. Ta(h:a. 6t Asp. 343 Tl:6vrpcac;, t:E"Pl~ ~I "oCx.EmL (pos.9)
242
RHYTHM AND MI::TRE
It should assume
be noted,
that
the
short
syllables
occur
in
a
avoidance that
18,
which
certain
under as
both
of
21):
above,
case
in
metron-dihaeresis
they
see
e.g.
rather,
relaxed.
pose.
and
two
if
avoidance
may be
to
the
more strict
be a general
the
after
anapaests',
('split
is
be no reason
or)
is
circumstances
this
produce
(between
a resolution seems to
there
seems to
(In
4 and
8 in
accordingly
are
of them marked verse-positions.) As can
final
be observed
resolution
of ten;
only
in Table
elided
in
the
case
labic
occurrences
are excluded
is even smaller
(notably
whole,
within
the
points
out,
words
involved
the
of
frequent
On the
than
in
average
sample)
Euripides'
does
resolution (cp.
in
in the
case
not
occur
pos.
3 in
Table
XXVI A).
the proportion
of elided
of
word-
particularly comedy If
the
is
it
bisyl-
word-boundar-
so in Menander).
word-final
present
XXIV A below,
word-boundary
more ies
word-end
rhythmical
which
there
verse-position
indicated
particular,
that
constitute
non-marked
SNELL (1982) comedy,
however,
resolution in
occurs
Euripides'
later
plays
Or ••
the
most As
frequently
(sc.
WEST (1982)
proportion
87
of bisyllabic
increases.
See Table XXIV A: the proportion of non-'monosyllabic' vs. 'monosyllabic' resolutions is 5 in Aesch., 2.67 in Soph., J in Eur. Hed., •l.07 in the or.! J.96 in Aristoph., 1.94 in Hen •• This situation is even more significant in view of the fact that in Euripides' later plays the percentage of words occupying a single verse-position does not likewise increase (see Tables XXI and XXI A above). Consequently
this
motivations
for
lose
their
(ad Table
*
situation the
seems
allowance
of
to
indicate
word-final
that
the
resolution
rhythmical gradually
influence.
XXIV)
Uord-end following resolution, expressed as a percentage of the resolutions in that particular verse-position; the total incidence of word-end at the verse-position concerned (i rrespect 1 ve of resolution) has been added in italics.
TABLEXXIV WORD-END AFTERRESOLUTION:PERCENTAGES* Aesch. & mono• - monosyll. syll.
Soph. & mono- - monosyll. syll.
Eur. Med. Or. Aristoph. & mono- - mono- & mono- - mono- & mono• - monosyll. syll. syll. syll. syll. syll.
Men. & mono- - monosyll. syll.
i
!.... 0
pos. I w.e. at I
5.29%
pos. 2 w.e. at 2
50'3/o 45.24%
()
0 9.15%
()
0 6.68%
8.73%
11.28% 7.73%
0
6.84% 5.94%
0
;J .... Ea ~
pos. 3 w.e. at 3 pos. 4 w.e. at 4 pos. 5 w.e. at 5 pos. 6 w.e. at 6 pos. 7 w.e. at 7 pos. 8 w.e. at 8
50%
6.67% 15./9%
0 10.42%
9.78% 12.97%
0 11.4%
pos. 10 w.e. at JO
-
total
7.23%
0
-
-
-
6.67%
-
-
pos. 9 w.e. at 9
52.63%
-
-
15.38% 33.84%
52.63% 37.67%
7.69%
9.09% 32.25%
50% 35.82%
50%
18.75% 39.42%
18.75%
29.35% 38.02%
29.35%
-
6.39% 28.2%
4.11%
0.69% 29.29%
0.69%
0 7.16%
2.38% 8.22%
15.38% 24.37%
11.89%
8.24% 25.57%
7.65%
-
-
8.16% 60.17%
8.16%
4.44% 55.05%
2.22%
7.1% 15.04%
2.19%
12.32% 19.94%
3.45%
2.07% 38.16%
2.07%
0.79% 41.18%
0
22.81%
19.88%
12.74% 27.74%
8.91%
1.89%
4.92% 37.21%
1.64%
2.63% 11.29%
6.25% 36.31%
-
-
48.68%
-
0
9.09%
51.32% 41.37%
20.33% 11.65%
2.2%
6.25%
17.31% 39.48%
7.69%
36%
-
3.TI% 29.25%
to, ~
N
,1:--
-
6.02%
33.33% 55.98%
33.33%
-
17.46%
12.7%
5.88%
0 55.19% 4.41% 19.21%
9.93%
4.44% 24.65%
2.22%
4.81% 30.15%
0.96%
10.8%
7.~%
9.96%
6.57%
w
TABLEXXIVA WORD-END AFTERRESOLUTION: TOTALNUMBERS (&ratio-/+
elision) N
Aesch. tot. pos. I
0
pos. 2
4
+
Soph. tot.
- +
0
3
I
20
3
pos. 3
-
-
pos. 4
0
1
Eur. Med. tot. - + 0
18 2 9
1 0
2
Or. tot. 0
2 0
39
-
-
0
2
pos. 5
-
-
pos. 6
0
9
pos. 7
-
-
1
35 4 8.75
0
l
37
0
-
-
1 1 I
-
tot.
-
+
14 8 l.75
13
21
17 4 4.25
54
48 6 8
1
0 l 0
14
13 1 /3
2
2 0
25
16 9
22
2 12 0./7 20 2 JO
4 31 6 5.17
l:1:-
Men.
22
14
-
5 4 1.25
- +
Aristoph. tot. +
13
0 4 0 9
4
7 6 1.17
-
2.25
3
2 l
l.78 1
0
2
pos. 8
2
2 0
I
1 0
pos. 9
l
-
-
0
l
9
-
-
8
l
39
8
-
-
2
33 6 5.5
20
1
3
l
20 0
3 0
-
I
pos. 10
2
0
2 0
0
0
2
2 0
-
total
6
1
5
33
5
monosyll.
1
1 0
9
-
- monosyll.
5
4
5 4
I
24
4 ---
22 2
II
3
I
87
75 12 6.25
1
42
35 7
3
1.25
I
4
27 6 4.5
0
3 0
-
45
40 5 H
94
5
138 114 24 4.75
47
25 2.7'1
91
n9
5 0
-
17 I.H2
31
5
0
3
142 100 42 2.38 48
l 0
32
15
2.13 82 9 9,/1
~
i
~ ~
~
THE IAMBICTRIMETER 4.3.1.
245
As has been argued above,
described
as the rhythmic
t rimeter less
which is
confined
to
of
preferred
verse-positions, This
from the metric
tolerated
particular
occurrence
above).
divergence
increasingly
preceding ( 4.3.
the phenomenon of resolution
rhythmical word-end
increasing
tolerance
appears
not only from the proportional
resolution
mical
but also
occurring
present
definition
above), type).
involve
In
our
percentages
fulfilment
in verse-positions
motivation,
olutions
the
within of a
a single
sample
or,
gradually
such
as
the
resolution
rhythmical
conditions
of word-end followis no positive
increasing
proportion
rhythof res-
word (some of which, owing to the of
of
word-boundary' a
resolution
(3.1.1.
non-potentially-structural
trimeter
poetry
the
following
apply:
Aristoph. 28.21% 30.47% Men.
Aesch. 7.23% 12.7% Soph. Eur. Med. 7.35% Or. 13.25%
Lye.
Epigr.
10% (see 3.6.
above)
~ome examples are: Aesch.
Ag.
539 )(.Q.LP-&>·uf"t~L
6' ouuh'
ave~
&or,
630 tt6-rE+XJ,"y&, a!rro0 ~ 1\ TE3vr)>c6i:c,c ...... 1590 a!rroo· ......EtvLa.-Pot To06E 6vc6EO(; m'rliP Sept •
2 7 2 TtEOLov6uoL,TE 'Hliy~
tnum6rtoL (;
.....
6 53 ~~, TE l«lL 8Eii'J\I}.&tya aroy~
......
Soph.
OR
301 6'::Prcd T' ~LCif'TE
.. 1372 mTt+xfrrot' 703 ~l)t,
Phil.
qwJ:JL
the
in rhythmically
to
increase
from the
and is
of the
the phenomenon of res-
of these
'potentially-structural
of word-internal
Archil. Sem. Solon
for
where there
word-boundary
present
above)
subsequent
even without
profile
circumstances
(3.6.
word-end
olution ing
as such,
can be
ta.fou
lGL x_fuvcx:rnt3f'i ~L
&, ~C6ov
d(;"ALOOU
1401 tnlET£...... TUT~, &:d.io, 309 ~ otxTLpavt£(; 662 OOLQC,\T£ qx.JVEL(;rot'L T 1
JJCMWV
lJClM'l08'fn t\ n \Q crroMv ,
i:'>T£KVOV, atuL,'IU'tQXL
-~
't£
weTQ:)£06' ~
>Gl 'H4:lirlv 1l0al.'V
378 ~~~~Nb,.)
't'
ha6v
~,
t\ abv ~ ¼ot; ... 4 b ~ LD>GPLCC-ko6N 6v£1.6l ~ roxac;697 nat~,.~lc;
or.
... "'I'
54 ALl,1£\.Qet Naut.\l£LOV ~ Tt>d'TIJ 86 ob
a\_,cr~Za.
'IJ»(QpLC~ 8' b (XI(; ~Lt
we
i:>~ • tt 't'DCOCkJa a· , 6L..tvr,v 6:y~
~L~ b(
't£LX,t.wv
OUO)C..\£ft 't' &I' •E.Udea • 11 auvw 6~ -w,.,~· fEWTL 'A). 294 )(Qt v0v ~•, & MJOtyvr)lOV )QX1 ... 313 dyav 6ncotx.ou,iitv£ 6' tnt 01+X,JtOO¼out 444 ~ ~ El.\u:l:'6~££&:l ~Lt &w:,q; 250/ 1 Yt'vot 6u"1a,'t~
-
......
472-4
x.~ x.~ f\MOL
hloov
~
tt
Naut.\lav
TtO.\uc'tftt ~. TTPb{;~ cS£Eu~v a!rroO atA
OUV~
dv·r:rl' lJ£ •
~~6' dl,1.1. lJJl'tQ:Xl K~ 5 55 v.;,y ~c:diol'V CXJV "CQY~ XTlYf'TIJ ... 630 at£LX' , we ~ eutL&N ftuCv ¼oc; 722/3 0C1,J01., ~, K01JKtr' etatv Unlc5£c;, 546 ~·.
-
~ ~ aiw.Tov •>eye
~ ur'ivl~ ¼Cf) 1211 clm.i>6toano.L1 II et nqn y'i eoa•I ~ >.t.yw. 593 6u::-.1om1.'M£0UOLV
Phil.
(The
four
examples
in
the
are
Anth.cr.
those
referred
to sub
3.5.
above.)
fin.
In comedy,
Porson's
Law is
9 follow
a long
syllable.
pos. rather
small
short
syllables
-
as
is
in this
that
of
not observed:
60% of the word-ends
The proportion those
of
word-boundaries
elided
which
position.
E.g. Aristoph. Nub.
! TOOiru>.tyc1. 1212 fru:1.3ov cix:ltv' oCotv, I~ too' it\~ 553
examples
254
RHYTHMAND METRE
(5&9:) ...
~ ~,l~latatp1.
6
m 6m110LGNY6':>ldi1"tEXXNtXll~1. 39 lo6o0a1.v ~lxoO JD6o0al. l>Jreos.m 3so ~ "10P t,;,&>.ii,,.,Iff)v OvrJ,1. v I£t~ 584 6£ l '10Pt.eq.h·otc; vt,xu:n. v I£61.a8£tv 32
617
ao 6'
671
kCl.lvOv bu:l\.!C&1.ll£C -c1.c;tcnlvlt~
l!£vtAEXIJV 6t I~
nt, ~,
£C
"°""
881
6.Ua. ~ v "t£ lkCl.t~ t1-co1.>etavr~ If\ -c£lJQVt~ I~ E"tPO(ploc; 6 ~, '6wt:A£Km hdanm
923
tv
').i.veu. m o'
844 849
9 50/ l
lavn-rov 6vro. b.iueo1.v WU"ttAJV 1-ltvOO'lX&>, ITl'av Etvriv 6t ~ TI'iv6't.oK6t.i.1.C£ • l-rovMiXJ:toOvm~ TIDI.KLAOl.c; 6t
(penth.&7&9:)
520 EC rr.oulGAO.I.,Iqa.1.~tot.l -coLOlo'I~1. -rMl,la\l 8uo:mlC I\.IOLP:lVITM)E;"t I~
1588 4.5.1.1.
According
stances verse
in
i.e.
this
substantial
the
by
of
of it
relatively
in
certain
Knox and
resolution
in
two following
special
Haas applies the
second
circumalso
half
in
of the
movement at the
Law appears
to be no more
Law. statisticaly
of word-end
in poss.
8 and
at
speaking,
pos.
9 and the
10 may account
for
that limited
the
phe-
to. in
the
rhythmical
preceding there
so-called
is not improbable,
of resolution
More precisely,
above),
the
low percentage
nomenon referred
with
of
the original
face
occurrence
that
case
extension
than
On the
(1)
IRIGOIN (1959),
(''hmistiche").
However,
the
to
'Law' formulated
the
tragedy,
v
1
clausular
be
of resolution
situations
word-end
will pos.
case
is
(for
no occasion
8 unle88 effect
likely
which
see
to
realize
a monosyllable will
in pos.
8 one of the
to occur: 3.6.
and
a clausular
is involved
be accomplished
Table
most
XXII rising
(luul'D--u--11). so often
by the
THE IAMBIC TRIMETER incidence
255
of word-end
frequent
situation
at
10 (which
pos.
in the
case
in
fact
of resolution
is
by far
the
luuv-lu-ll,
8):
in pos.
most
or the
(2)
resolution
actually
are
rising
in
some
clausular
Table
is
followed
by word-end:
instances,
of this
presumably
movement in
intended
8 referred
pos.
situation to
to above
there
produce
(see
4.3.
the with
XXIV) • For
similar
reasons,
pos.
10 are
sometimes
be no realization volved.
It
described
the
exceptional
followed
of clausular
will
the
then
situation
that,
resolution
not,
there
a monosyllable
given
described
of
if
by word-end;
movement unless
be obvious
above
occurrences
the
is in-
clausular
by IRIGOIN is
will
practice unlikely
to
the
in-
occur. In
addition,
the
cidence
of word-end
of
falling
the
rhythmical at
not
be realized
pos.
8 or 10:
at
9, namely
pos.
rhythmical the
circuutances
movement rhythmic
that
by the
if
is
.a 5 1t£ £aer,'t 1.• ~,
-
1'M
S97 q,Cc:nl, ~
Ned.
512 ~
cases
profile
seems
to
will
a resolution
be
the by the
stronger
w
'1W
in
...,
mTti::ul-rovti.iav;lb 6t ...... OE
1302 oCJKc1v 1JE8e:lrrv.1 Jqn,"
metric
there
caesura,
are:
Ph 11 •
- in which
a reinforcement
produced
level
Soph. OR 967 K't£VELV ~
or.
is
there
luuul-lJ-11 or 1-uluu u-JI.
Some exceptions
Eur.
conditioning
~
Iy6vao1. , Il«ll Tt£P ... ...(1,o}
l'L µ' l&.qnlno>.tµ1.ov
- ...
mtoac, It.pus,nl~1.
- ...
f&vro -caOmlTD'tt+>ECIol rdhJ.1. tendency
to keep
occurrence than
of
the
clear
the
word-end
tendency
distinctness
before
to
the
realize
of the resolution
a
clausular
movement. As indicated olution
in pos.
Aesch.
Ag.
above, 8 is, 119S
the most common situation however,
the
in the case
following:
......
t\ ~u66t.aavtlc £LUI. I~
1265 Mal. OKflyn~
I Cli>,i;&Jv; ... "MO.L invt£i:'Qlrt£PL c5tar;)I ~tc~; I ......
I
1276 "MO.lvOv rt0>..li:w.c -cdtJ6€ 6ulf:ip61.JOVCq,uyQ(;
1445 ~ l.l.01.c,>..uou::n I TT.CM.£µlwv I~
of res-
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
256
I~ 270 ~ 6t n.\£x-aiw.1..0Ll Tt£PLQ)OIJCNI KU"t~ -416 ara'ivar' tc ro1..a:J6£ I 8avaal1,.10US; I~ .... 495 -rovoom"tOV 1.1£~ I ac:iwo1..uovI voov -- I n6Ea. 7 v6at>J«l~W. I 6t.aa6P:.> .. 257 a.\.\' ot ~ ~EC I avoo~ I tut 809 6.\.\., avtL' I~ or. 81 'EA.tvr,, "tL OOL A£YOLU av I d. YE 1TQ:X)UO' I~; - ... 305 xti'pr;i ·t1..v', otx~· I OE y6') i fx.w I ~ ...... 896 rcn&;;o' a£t KTIPUK.E:c· I &>£ 6' I a&toi:c I q,lAO(; 547 ~i:~
Sept.
For occurrences
·~·
of word-end
after
resolution
in
poss.
8 and
10, see
above.
4.5.2.
has
As
iambographers
been
alternatives:
rhythm
is the
form
stating
8 and
In the of
rather
clausular
words
towards strong
XX above);
proportionally
stronger.
of
pos.
shaped
of
rhyth-
to
rising
f0-1J-11 or
... 'Law'
negative
concurrent
however
such
by a
associated
is
, Knox and Haas, in its tragedy
the
10 are
returning
iambographers, In
this
situation
verse
in
the
underlining the
I 70
early
at
trimeter
word-end word-end
is
XX and XX A, P• 214/5 ahove).
with than
the
11.J-11).This
( ...
avoided.
Table
per
effect
by a word(group)
the
place,
conforms less
in
in
8 and word-end
word
10 is
first
situation
Table
that,
common (see
tendency
pos.
names of von Wilamowitz
pose.
average
sub3.4.l.above,
either
bisyllabic
with
rather
at the
produced
verse-final
stated
word-end
mical
at
with
1
Eur.
4.3.
instances,
it
tendency
reflects
early
iambography.
observation
that,
the is
obviously
rising
in tragedy to
do
movement (see
so
in
at
3.4.1. pos.
the
lower
Rhythmically
the
iambography,
the
at
pos.
and 3.4.2. 8 appears
10 is with to
be
257
THE IAMBIC TR.IMETER
and Solon respect! vely in the case of In Archilochue, Semonidee, single word-end at poe. 8 (ll>-U--11) 84.21%, 78.85% and 85. 71% of the 71 ancipitia are realized as a short syllable.' It
should
be
in
pos.
ancipitia
9 tends
final
iambic metron
tion
in tragedy
While
on
pos.
the
9 at
(being see
one
hand
movement
which
of
streSBed
by the
the
risk
of
characteristic
the
of word-end
situa72
ancipitia metric
of
level
basic
up the one.'
long
'basic'
rhythmic the
incidence
of
by means
of
short
of breaking
'sophisticated'
the
extending the
of
'nlue viewed,
'feet'.
clarity
hand at
recurrent
percentage
an increase
is
the
other
high
to be the more
rhythm
is
the
increase
there
expense
on the
l),
that
two iambic
single-short
sub
pos.
into
to
can be said
the
a
also,
realized
in
profile
prolongation, the
rhythmical
metric
profile
at
pos.
both
is 8 and
10.
For graphere,
examples of the clausular see 3.4.1. p. 209 above.
4.5.2.1.
'nle
GOIN (1959) pos.10
in
'Law'
formulated
to verses
with
tragedy
4.5.2.
sub
seems to
preference
for
word-initial in
pos.
early
iambo-
extended
of resolution
accounted
resolution
a monosyllable
the
by IRI-
in pos.
8 or
uulv-l1r1I: luuul1r1I. However ,this
...
be sufficiently
of
has been
an occurrence
(cp. 4. 5 .1. 1. above):
phenomenon involving
practice
(see 8,
for
3.6.
are
by the
above).
cited
general
Exceptions,
4.5.1.1.
sub
fin.
above. 4.5.3.
MORELLI (1961)&(1962)
cies
observed
ther
restrictions
colon
of
13/4.
According
graphers
by Poreon, on
iambographic only
to
reduced
to one general
von Wilamowitz,
the
incidence
verse Morelli,
one word-boundary
as in
of
word-end e.g.
trimeter
may occur
the
tenden-
Knox and Maas and the fur-
formulated the
'Law' within
the
second
by PERROTTA (1938) of
between
the
early
caesura
iambo-
and verse-
end. 'nle
possible
combinations
of word-shapes,
ing: with
penthemimeral
caesura:
1-ul-v--v--1 I l-v-lv-1r1I
then,
are
the
follow-
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
258
l-u-v1-v-J I /--u-v-1 \J-11 with
caesura:
hephthemimeral
1-ul-v-JI l-v-l\J-11 - a monosyllable
verse-end
at
the
undeairability
ated
by Perrotta)
of
being
infrequent.
a monosyllable
following
which
of penthemimeral
caesura
of monosyllables
to
implied
is
(see
the
here
3.1.4.
sum-total
the
does
not
above).
speaking,
Strictly
caesura
apply
However,
of word-shapes
in
(indic-
case
the
the proportion
is of course
rather
small.
So,
in
practice,
for1111lated 4.5.2.
Porson,
above)
the
von
at
all
infrequent,
preted
as
pos.
even
5,
in
amples
having the
the
but
Law merely
Wilamowitz,
restriction
(l-ul-v-l\J-11). Now
10
at
by
's
Morelli
this
sequence
since
part
case
of
does a
of such a sequence
the
these with
apply
penthemimeral
(see caesura
of word-shapes
;
173
verses
4.5.1,
at
poss.
7 and
as
such
is not
of word-shapes of
tendencies
Maas (4.4,
word-end
caesura
still
to
Knox and
on concurrent
a hephthemimeral tendency
adds
will
concomitant 3.1.3.1.1. there
are
be interword-end above).
But
clear
ex-
e.g.
t]Jot '1,1£ATPEL IS\JtJbvlthlov!TC&o V 19 wv d~ Qt )().ij I>ea.l}.lty' It~I Mt~ 177 • 1 ~ Z£0, lUTQ:) Z£0, l00V 1Jl:v I~ I>CPi"COC sLc 45 XM1l'Vll ~, lauxtv'I~ IfYJ,11\/ Sem. 1.12 nplv T®' [Krrto,L, ITc:iJ(; EtlounTIVOL I~ 2.2 EC n qp::,voCJ.L£V, ln.\.£Covltn.at,:i11c li,u.fic
Archil.
0
23.10
6.2
6'.&eLvovlciJetlblyLovl~c td!)}.bv" 6(:,mv 6' ldUoT'ldUolTJV lfX£L
~,
7 .11 Tb 6'
mc.o0ca&. , IT'livtu 6' IElot\O.L latN: L 27 U1V6' b. ~, II\ 001 ltv qpi;c,1,v IVOEC 13 ~ nivr'
38
Wtl'l'K',~,
lx.dA,nI\QlrtT,JOL v liatYQ
55 ~ 6' ~ L
,~
IYEL 't'O\OCl}G(Q
74 £to1.v 61.' ci:n£~ jTD,1. v I~LC V Compare also, Archil.
with
42.2:
~£•,~
a monosyllable
f\ tp(JE
in pos.
Iv~ 8:
6'! ~ ~ u
THE INCIDENCE OI-' WORD-1::ND:GENERAL SURVEY
Archil.
Sem.
Solon
Aesch.
Soph.
Eur. Med.
Or.
Aristoph.
Men.
Lye.
Epigr.
~ tll
pos. I • cl.
4.55%
-
12.64% 1.72%
5%
-
5.29% 0.53%
9.15% 1.82%
8.73% 1.08°/o
6.68% 1.26%
7.73% 2.37%
5.94% 1.36%
2.99% 0.2%
4.29% 0.61%
t--4
?c bi:!
t--4 (")
pos. 2 • cl.
pos. 3 • cl.
34.85% 27.27%
25.29% 16.09%
27.5% 20%
45.24% 34.()6%
37.67% 26.21%
35.82% 26.2%
41.37% 30%
39.42% 31.27%
38.02% 30.4%
37.79% 32.23%
32.52% 26.99%
40.91% 36.26%
45.4%
40.8%
45% 37.5%
32.7% 28.63%
34.52% 28.52%
40.14% 33.37%
36.95% 31.44%
28.2% 23.26%
29.29% 25.57%
32.43% 30.94%
34.97% 32.52%
pos. 4 - et.
6.06% 3.03%
14.94% 13.22%
l0% 10%
10.42% 9.29%
15.19% 11.99%
7.16% 6.28%
8.22% 7.32%
24.37% 19.36%
25.57% 21.73%
12.62% 12.21%
14.11% 13.5%
pos. 5
77.27% 75.76%
71.84% 68.97%
77.5% 75%
75.45% 71.15%
74.14% 67.93%
82.53% 75.47%
81.39% 71.09%
60.17% 55.01%
55.05% 48.98%
66.01% 64.52%
68.71% 66.26%
4.54% 1.51%
7.47% 1.72%
5%
11.4% 4.08%
12.97% 3.91%
11.29% 3.43%
11.65% 5.6%
15.04% 11.07%
19.94% 14.43%
1.56% 0.47%
2.45% 1.84%
pos. 7 - el.
57.58% 51.52%
49.43% 46.55%
52.5% 42.5%
45.92% 42.82%
48.33% 42.51%
43.08% 37.88%
41.37% 34.6%
38.16% 33.56%
41.18% 35.85%
55.36% 54%
49.08% 48.47%
pos. 8
28.79% 22.73%
31.61% 25.29%
35% 32.5%
38.84% 29.15%
32.25% 24.57%
36.31% 30.42%
39.48% 32.7%
36% 30.92%
27.74% 22.85%
23.13% 21.57%
30.67% 24.54%
9.09%
6.06%
9.77% 9.77%
15% 12.5%
4.53% 3.7%
13.24% 10.66%
8.24% 7.36%
10.75% 8.49%
29.25% 25.84%
37.21% 33.31%
4.68% 4.41%
18.4% 18.4%
pos. 10 • el.
34.85% 33.33%
47.13% 45.98%
57.85% 55.74%
55.98% 49%
55.05% 51.72%
55.19% 50.05%
24.65% 22.49%
30.15% 24.33%
62.82% 62.01%
33.74% 33.74%
pos. 11 - el.
1.52% 1.52%
0.57% 0.57%
1.57% 1.47%
2.44% 2.08%
1.81% 1.74%
3.9% 3.65%
0.27% 0.27%
. el.
pos. 6 - el.
. el. pos. 9 . el.
-
25% 25%
;I t--4
~
t-i t'IS :,:,
N
pos. ii-
1 100%
* Irresoective
100%
2.5% 2.5% 100%
of the sub-optimAl
0.76% 0.6% 100%
realizations
2% 2% 100%
100%
of verse-end.
100%
100%
100%
100%
100%
V,
'°
TABLEXXVIA THE INCIDE~CEOF WORD-END: TOTALNUMBERS (&ratio-/+ Sem.
Archil.
tot. pos. 1
3
+
-
3
0
tot. 22
Solon -
3 19
0 pos. 2
23
18
+
27
24
5
44
pos. 5
4 51
3
79
71
pos. 7
pos. 8
3 38 19
I
26
23
125 120
u
n
34 4 8.5
H6
pos. 9
6
4
55
4
2
pos. 11
23 1
22 I 22
1
0
pos. 12
66
66
& split res.
18
17
5
4 31 2 21
44 11 4
14
80
()
2
15
0
264 2.37 27 8.78 (4)
174 174
-
3
2
17 13
IO
5 5 HI
I
1
433 379 138 123 999
151
724 645 79 (4.16) 8.16
40
40
-
41
4
608
54 15
I
848
386
97 41 62
6.23
I
60
49
590
766 738
342 270 1669 1529
1()
292
11
160 143 17 8.41 (4)
28
140
204 0.43
69
34()
4.93
73
841 769
72 10.08
115
0
35
370 310
I IIJ
157
2251 2250
60 5./7
84
0 1
8308 1500 5 ..54
34
561 521 16
15
1
15
9802 8308 1494 (4.35) 5.56 (4.J6)
I} 15 8.33
15.5
22.5
'>8()8
53
7.28
7.03
45
80
0.44
726 553 173 3.2
45
9
64
439 386
1260 1103
98
7.11
-
5606 SOUi 578 8.7(
409
131 7.31
(4.23)
365 267
4./4
8 2
1324 1324
72
88
298 240
80
2 72
1088 957
4
0
9
/0.92
26.36 0
258
13.5 4.76
4.45
0
89
+ 0./J
777 642
13.83 448
-
3.75
51 16.53
567
165
tot.
2.29
942 54
+ 0.25
0.56
0
206
3.05
/3
6
-
8.2
30 I JO (I
tot.
7.02 0
4
63
599 451 148
0
l
3
4.25
4()
I
7
+
0.JJ
-
5 /6.2
-
82
70
-
81
17
-
5
-
tot.
3
3 IO
2 pos. 10
8
0.3
3.75
2
tot.
2.67
7.67
I 2 0.5
15
8
8.87
50
pos. 6
II
l.75
2 2 I 50
0
+
0
28 16
8 pos. 4
2
0./6
3.6 pos . .3
-
N a,. 0
Eur. Med.
Soph.
Aesch.
tot.
elision)
1019 1019
-
0
4381 3816 565 (4.3) 6.75
I t
t:,
E5
a
(TABLEXXVIA cont.) Or. tot.
74
(I)
Mc:n
Anstoph.
-
+
14
60
IOI.
-
Ill
34
+
0.23
(2)
458 409
332 126 348
566
91
81
495
901
350
787 114
864
129
62
27K 72 790
(7)
458
J83
67
216
159
75
548
437
J62
(9)
119
75
517 420
3.76 ( Ill)
611
554
27
23
57
354
1107
1107
4
26
(&
spl.r)
323 25
0
1436
1436
413
t,(I
3
41
665 448
351
62
791
475
478
456
186
6()1
538
973
951
23
7
487
393
86
Xl6
796
63
79
341
0
1615
63
69 926
44
318
65 914
4
4
4
22
57
6
1474
5813 (4.05)
5125 688
6686
7.45
(4.14)
7.04
4X26 4152 674 (4.Y..) 6.16
5834
5135 699
5X76 83K
(4.(16)
7.35
6714 (4.16)
1474
4
53
13.25
23
22
i t-1 (")
~
~
rrJ ::ii,
I
22
22
112
16
4
l08 27
4
3
I
.1
20
80
79
I
79
23
50
40
4 12 0
IO
4
30
30
0
55
55
0
(}
0
9
4.89
76./7
0
-
5854 832 5891
5643 248
(4)
22.75
7.01
53
16.25
94
59 1615
82
/3.83
/4. 75
-
6
0./1
39.8
4,/8
I
I
0.44
8.54
31
+
43.23
X9
579
180
4.67
49
7
-
30
98
233
.'\69
tol.
20.73
4147 674 6./5
4821 (4.35)
+
5.79
673
73
371
551
2.6/
25
(tot.)
322
/0.42
5. 75
( 12)
57
4H2 66 444
491 123
8.07
7.57
9.72
( 11)
889
6.0H
25
94
74
7.3
4.83
-
rrJ
0.07
5.M
2.79
5.11
(8)
413
JO68
0.93
44
6 X8
3.Hf>
6.9
(6)
473
4.7 Ill
74
22
IOI
3.'lCOU8cvl o(f;,t -cf\c~ 17 ~ dYnlJO"t~ qplOCOV1X1 h«:1LTOV 1')(1.arov 32 l«:ll Tfl y&'Vij ~£(; lcb TL 001. ~lncav la.t(l)Lku. fitJOca.1. 11 / 2 cb< dv -mx~ .:\~f;£1.£ • 1Tn\l Y£ id,v TDlCTtPllV, 0HCMl£P otxlCoooi.vloi: T£ TQXJU\1£1.KOL 94 &a:,1.
N
c,,, 00
TABLEII REALIZATION OF ANCEPSPOSITIONS
Hippon. %
long
short
2 shorts•
long
short
57.39%
40.87% 26.55%
1.74% (1.74%) 66.67%
66.96%
33.04% 21.47%
-
31.52%
-
%% 40% Call.
% %%
Herod.% %%
pos. 9
pos.5
pos. 1
62.42% 47.69%
64.54% 46.49%
37.58% 22.22% 33.94% 21.28%
46.67%
-
68.48% 52.31%
1.52% (0.91%) 90.91%
2 shorts
short
19.13% 13.33%
51.98% 33.33%
80%
-
100% 59./4%
18.64%
70.02% 50.44%
29.98%
-
18.8%
2 shorts•
long
0.87% (0.87%)
-
4.26%
95.59%
0.15% (-)
3.07%
59.92%
9.09%
~
% p~rcer,tngP
%% percentngc
"
The been
of of
long/short/rloublt>-~hort the
smn
total
pt"rcentar,e of double-short added in brackets.
rPalizations of
long, realizations
short
occurrir,g or
which
in
double-short applies
the
anr.eps
position
concerned
proper
naf!les are
i ~
realizatiors if
i
>.tyw 001.; la>Ji ,IKu61..UO., -rb MXoS; CJU'ffl 6nb aaui:fi,.lui,l.\l8oc; -r1.c, ou 6ouAr\
21
VII.118
vw·I iu,I )(QAOV n 6rw't" l~l6 NJKTL~
TP&.TTllJUWJ
~
6:p,1,.D.
~
TABLEVIII WORD-END AT POS. 6: MONOSYLLABLES*
Hippon. 0.87%
w.e. at 6 (tot.)
4.24% 2.42% 1.82%
5.63% 3.()4% 2.59%
1.82% 2.42% 4.24% 100%
1.98% 2.89% 4.87% 86.49%
+ cl. - cl.
0.87%
,1..01 1..01 tot. 1~II
0.87% 0.87% 100%
of w.e. at 6
Herod.
Call.
IJl1 JJI
0.15% 0.15% 5.41%
of w.e. at 6 monosyll. in 6&7ofw.e.at6
100%
100%
91.89%
* Here also,
the notion of a monosyllable covers and bisyllabic words (or word-groups) occupying position: see Ch.II, 3.4. with note 99 above.
3.2.
In choliambic
hemimeral
or
occurrence
of caesura
verse,
hephthemimeral media:
the
percentage
caesura see Table
is
of
verses
scarcely
IX below.
all monosyllabic a single verse-
without diminished
a pentby the
THE CHOLIAKBUS There
are
ura
media
this
is
few examples
in
Herodas,
of
remarkable
less
tragedy,
to
likeness:
trimeters
(and
those
not
to
only
amounting
considerably
t rimeters the
287
even
than
which
compare
of Aeschylus
convincing)
0.46%
the
Herodas'
for
very
of
his
percentage choliambi
instance
the
and of Euripides'
of caescholiambi:
found
in
otherwise 2.79%
and
the
show a 1.28% of
Hedea respectively.
~lv' c'.:x:,-r£~1JTlO,ibS6vta. KLvrioa,L IV. 7 2 6-.\n&i. \O.L, ~0.,n, [y6p a! •a,,oolou Xt i:PEC
Herod.
111.49
- which
but
for
the
vocative
a verse
with
hephthemimeral
inserted
would have been considered
as
caesura;
vI • 3 .at.\ICl, TTO i.ncnC 3.3.
The proportion
of
than
it
regular
is
smaller
in
than
especially
the
that of
verses
in the
the
circumstance
of
verses
without
caesura
is
hardly
at
that
in
at
verses
one of
lacking
although verse
or
8.2
MO.t~,
olovitoeloooL
(see
~l
!~"°'
26.3
l::oTt£p tu\,OOXO(; OOLVUIJE:'VO!; 5 7 cndCoooLV i:orc£p l0Lt£ lO.t l.dil Tq:n in ML~ au't6v 7 4 f\ «P£Ufou' b< 'tf\r; i ot KlTK:. ~ "tOOTov
a rather
Ch.111,
3.3.
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
288
TABLEIX CAESURA MEDIA;VERSESWITHOUT A CAESURA
without P or H word-end at 6 monosyll. in 6/7
Hippon.
Call.
Herod.
2.61% 0.87% 0.87%
2.42% 4.24% 4.24%
l.67% 5.63% 5.18%
CM • el. + el. ratio
0.46% 0.3% 0.15%
2
without P. H. or CM
2.61%
3.4.
poets
Of the
choliambic
rhythmical
movement
pos.
10.
Or
speak
of a 'rising
underlying this
rather,
marked 8 occurs
situation
incidence
of
both
to
the
with
Table
X A below,
8 in
fact
occurs
as
in Aeschylus'
of
word-end
at
in
is
word-end
in
9% of
trimeters, pos.
tragic
the
one
to is
it
only
at
pos.
incidence
of
to
of the
word-end
(cp.
at
3.
since
at
3.1.
low, at
in
pos.
10. As appears
from
pos.
pos.
about
comparatively
to be produced
chol-
in Herodas'
comparatively
pos.
in
and
where word-end
choliambi, the
possible
practice
of word-end
so that
at
of Herodas'
8 is
the
word-end
features
in tragedy word-end
stress
in virtue
more remarkable
Herodas'
8 appears
position other
situation
coincidence
only verse
frequent
the
and without the
this
like
of
the
choliambic
be,
rather
is
1.22%
by means of recurrent
the
to
This
contrast
in
profile,
reminiscent the
verse
movement'
appears
especially). verse
the
Herodas
since
metric
position
iambi,
of
2.42%
10 and at
as frequently low incidence
by a lower
occurrence
THE CHOLIAMBUS of word-end ticular.
at
pos.
The
sence,
289 8 unaccompanied
situation
in choliambic
ence
of
word-end
of tragedy:
at
In Hipponax over
the
however
meter
the
early
far
35.65% of his
as
Archilochus;
rences
of a choliambic
than
quent
of
to
incidence
the
trimeters
ponax with
at it
high
even
that
in tfenander:
for
of a single
of word-end
choliambic 37 .21%), were,
8 is
here
the
practice
At the
pos.
variety
10 is
8 is
con-
at
somewhat
The few occur-
both
pos.
10 and pos.
in Archilochus'
percentage pos.
occurs,
10 word-end is
trialthough
occurs
of Hipponax.
9 however
the
much
{The fre-
of course
contrast
pos.
foreign
30.43% in Hip-
(35. 76%, only
mainly
accounted
accompanied
observed
the
incidence
the
Hellenistic
observed
in
of the anceps beginning
movement of
of the
his
poet
the
verse
verse
to
be compared
for
by the
word-end
in
the
at
choliambi
with
frequency at
pos.
9
both
pos.
5
of Hippo-
of word-end
both
at
than
the
case
of Hipponax,
to
carry
in
seems
acknowledged
in pos.
remarkably
by word-end
for also
even more exceptional
also
9 in Callimachus,
as an alternative
In Callimachus
that
mical
it
at
caesura
7 - a phenomenon
realization
is
penthemimeral
and
pos.
small
verse
as
at
at
unparalleled
iambography:
(serving, nax).
word-end
pos.
pos.
tri-
to 34.85% of the trimeters
in the choliambi at
regular
the 9.09% in Archilochus.)
The percentage
pos.
at
Archilochus).
8 and at
does
the
Hipponax'
Archilochean
word-end
word-end
are
pos.
early
incid-
are distributed
to
the
compared
with
word-end
of
ab-
trimeters
more evenly:
to
of
a comparably
both
more frequently
{or
28. 79% in
Hipponax
Semonides
regular
par-
the
on the
the
similarity
of
verse
in
trimeters
to
in
10 in with
restrictions
rather
incidence
compared
be observed
his
harsh
the word-boundaries
incidence
(23.48%,
pos.
below.
choliambi,
the
at
unconnected
9 to be observed 302f.
the
lower
in
the
iambographers
cerned:
in
of
show an apparent
as
meters;
verse,
not
verse-positions
anyway)
8 to
course
p.
three
choliambi
of
of
and Callimachus,
last
of
is pos.
see 4.3.,
by word-end
pos.
to
predecessor
the
10 and so
extreme
{compare
the
9). a tendency
by means of recurrent
to
stress word-end
the
rhyth-
at pos.
2
290 is
RHYTIIH AND METRE
obviously
absent
machus
are
On the
whole,
the
aa far
concerned,
early
the
as
the
word-end
situation
iambographera;
at
choliambi
Hipponax
3 being
pos.
in Hipponax Callimachus
of
far
more
can be compared
may rather
and
have
Calli-
frequent.
with
that
in
followed
Hip-
to reinforce
the
ponax. Herodas, rhythnical end at less E.g.,
like
the tra~ic
111ovement more 4 is
pos.
frequent:
poets,
here
strongly,
again
on the
somewhat more frequent,
see also
4.2.
tends
undera tanding
word-end
at
that
pos.
2 somewhat
below.
10: Hip pon. 4 KlkWV6' t>1"IXV00.Al\ToC ~ I'HOlJl'lC at pos.
20 ~ tx&tvov -cfl6c1"'CTITPLTJ IKO,lo.1. u 26 • 2 ~ T£ >Gt ~ f\lJkxl(;, ~ 4-6 'HO.TbsaY£ fii TOV KM1POV. c'btE xPl'I IOHQ1TT£1,\I
ruca1J£TP1.0.I ~ >Gt >e0L&1. vov x6.\A.1.l0.,6ou.\1.ovIxcxrn,v.
nt't'IXJ(; ~lac;,
39.112 >Cak0L01. &::aa>mv ruwx:rtOVOVIIWXY'lv,
t\v uhu ~ wetdxund 'µ01, l>e01.~ u 44 HO.LmL y' £6.iJvov atnbv £t at>.£1.cl&::aa>. 47.1/2
me' ot,
~
n1J£P11V!U£Cvac
TCP0f;utv Kuvr'iaE 1.c Tbv tbMJl"IOWN I·~v. Ca 11.
194. 1 de - oovdP: - "'~, Tnt' 38 oo6' 0[6 1 OMOlnv~1~1. 52
Herod.
tyw 6': k\Mnv
fl\lLK'
I
70 TLC mv t>alnv, TLC 6': UIV eatM"IVInl-4; 78 Tb 5ruTQX)\I Tl811u1.ti5 ~ I~ V.7 lO.t uh T6 )J£U a.rin 'VUKta.KTl)JtP1wlntVE
uh ~ YMI. 't'O Trl1J£Pll l'KELvr;) fine a' f:.cmya:f' U:£.. 1~·
22/3 wi;
"""''n,
26-8 BlnV\Cl, ~ -µo1.'IT!VJ"" y6p ~AOV I'MQ.~1. ~ uil 61.' 6.yopfk; yw;vbc; ~ I3E(,,pn-a:u.
49 >Cl XLALOMETRE
292
TABLEX WORD-END ANDWORD-SHAPES AT THE BEGINNING ANDTHE ENDOF THE VERSE Hippon. beginning: monosyll. w.e. at 2
•-I w.e. at 3
I•--vl w.e. at 4
I•--v-1
Call.
Herod.
5.22%
9.09%
6.7%
23.48% 23.48%
24.85% 23.64%
29.07% 28.77%
45.22% 42.61%
43.64% 38.18%
37.6% 33.33%
8.7% 7.83%
8.48%
4.85%
18.11% 10.96%
20% 23.48%
24.85% 26.06%
14.31% 23.9%
29.57% 30.43%
35.76% 35.76%
26.33% 27.7%
35.65% 35.65%
26.06% 26.06%
50.99% 50.99%
end:
1----11 w.e. at 8
1---ll w.e. at 9
1--ll w.e. at 10
monosyll.
-
-
-
TABLEX A WORD-END BOTHAT POS. 10 ANDPOS. 8 Herod.
Hippon.
Call.
w.e. at 10 and 8
1.74%
0.61%
8.98%
of w.e. at 10
4.AA%
2.33%
17.61%
of w.e. at 8
7.41%
2.33%
37.58%
THE CHOLIAMBUS 3.5.
In accordance
in
Hipponax'
in
with
Solon,
see
most
of
Table
pos.
6 being
the
proportion
rence
and
of
(cp.
appears
elision.
majority
of
and
word-boundaries E.g. , pos.
is
in
8, and the
is
quite
24.141.
and In
Hipponax
1 (the
occurrence
the
choliambus
also
above);
in
of
proportion
the
and
so that the
Callimachus'
occurs
19.7~
positioning
3.5.1. in
verse
in
above).
irrespective
example,
remarkably
15,:,
in pos.
3.1.1.
per
there
a large
this
tend-
general
choliambi
occurthe
verse-
colon-initial
of non-elided
far
vs.
elided
small.
1:
Hippon.
7
6€[
6'
lain-ovtc
~ ~t.
79.17
6 6' looi:lx' ~ ciJv
'tPt.OCot. l,.Q)nlOt.V
104.47
6 6'ltEoAu:1&:,-, txtn:U£
rliv 'HtXiuarrv
191. 7 2
Call.
hold
words
respectively).
phrase-initial
monosyllables
1, 6,
positions
see
Ch.III,
to
For
the
occur
of
monosyllables
coopare
XI B below,
between
of elision
of
Semonides
low:
from Table
actually
percentage
monosyllables
a correspondence
low average
XI A below;
comparatively
As appears
general
the
Archilochus
also,
ency
the
choliamb1,
{10.43%:
er.all
is
293
82 194.24/5
au6' IEt ~ unAO)'Ot.C 6rt£1.~1.c o 6' ltEon1.o6E~toe tY){(XJ)(E1. i:lc 6' lotMoCd1n.Ep OUKty& ~ cp,\1.ij; i:lc 6'
loOU£ f\ -rlc; ooau-rr.c fA:H.£1.;
77 ~ 6'
lti ~ f\v ~ x~ 8rpE:uc I.26 )(£L 6' ltoi:tv otMoC"tfk; 8£00° m ~ navta. 52 6tc 6' ltv IJJµnlTJutti.i,v
Calli-
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
294 (103 104
Z£~1l'OO'ro· wa.Ly6p
For monosyllables Call.
in pos.
•••
~ µ£
YEL'tav&Ca'
6, see 3.1.1.
above;
TIDL~)
611:atvLYELC
pos.
8:
ft utv "tdo', OUuotlov, or~, c1yaBo.I nav8' I oo' dv XP(,Cr;i ty~ 6t ~-caOto.I 000c I ru'Cv' f~ELC
Herodas' lables
verse in
pos.
percentage 3.4.
a
word-end Since
being
in
large
proportion
comparatively
5 (which
of
above).
occurrences
shows
in
at
high
accordance
percentage with
pos.
4 to
be observed
words
will
seldom
verse-po&!
of the
in
is
these
a colon-initial
I Cp' I QYLVECx:nL
aLnc ulCov'
IV.87 fA80LlJ£V
tion),
monosyllables trimeters
Soph.
monosyl-
relatively
in his
there
is
elision.
Phil.
cited
(Compare
Ch.III,
E.g., I. 6 2 dl.DP"tLfl'V I ~ I,:f.\ 8£:i;>. WCl.1:0Q'tT)CX)V 11.52
II I. 36
I 'tPELC I tvxt.aai roO 6:kon-rou 6.A.Anv Tlil..LI uv1w· I ~ 6t -ro. otxi:' ~ >cw npoon,xwv I 41puf. I ~ 6t 6n i: L 'KOl ulCov aU' 6 K£1:XQ.IOC I TU%;I OOTtEP Ci:pLa. 8M\"CO.L bt' tin,~ I auIxcL~, w6.\la£;, 'tEL\,104; or' fpya. KEL I ·vtw· I -roOi:' toE:i:c•A3nva.Cnv ~ 1.Yl-0- ~, l id, I un U£ 8f,'i{;Y\.M1LX' etva.L tv i:ij otxlr;i I xEi:o' • I 6».a. 1:6.NpLi:' ~ lJ£'ti:xw
44 IV. 18
57 V .13
VI. 5
11
see (not
no particularly
above.)
so xfl}tm~
high
verse:
be phrase-initial
involving of
the
of
the 3.5.
THE CHOLIAMBUS
295
Compare also Hippon.
36.3
(and 38 It
may be noted
currence the
of
regular
syllable immediately
at
I uve:ac;I apy~ w Ze:O, TU"tEP I I(KTA.) 6l&i:4,Ll TOI.
that
Tp1.nwovm
V
in choliambic
a monosyllable
in
verse
pos.
trimeter,
Ch.III,
3.5.
verse-end
would detract
cited
there
is
12 (contrast above),
3.1.1.
above).
not
a single
oc-
the
situation
in
presumably
from the
effect
since of the
preceding.
TABLEXI MONOSYLLABLES
pos. 1 - el.
Hippon.
OO 191.11
.. ..
ytt;Mv ~l&L>«J.
.....eLf:U.la ~XEL
54 )(Qt 't'f\c •AIAfrlcIutvero Herod.
I.9
ruu,,,
~
.. ..
~L
tn.itac;ITL au&6'
~
~;
only
three
to
be too
298 28
rw:,O-roc,~,
30-2 ~ ~,
......
- ... l6u\oa.u.~,
e ~ w 56 ta&Jv a&l>CJ86tx,> i:fk; M£ai1; b.wrivE etc.
TABLEXII WORD-END PRECEDING RESOLUTION* Hippon. pos. 1 W.t. al 0
pos. 2 w.t. al I
Call.
100% 100%
100% 100%
0 5.22%
Herod.
0 9.09%
11.11% 6.7%
pos. 3 W,t. al 2
pos. 4 W.t. al J
pos.
HXl% 45.22%
50% 37.6%
s
W.t. 014
pos. 6 W.t. al 5
100% 71.3%
100% 75.15%
70.59% 70.78%
W.t. at 6
0 5.63%
pos. 8 W.t. at 7
100% 51.21%
pos. 7
pos. 9 W.t. 018
100% 23.48%
pos. 10 W.t. at 9
total
* Percentage
66.67%
66.67%
50%
of the resolutions in a particular verse-position prethe total incidence of word-end (irrespective ceded by word-end; of resolution) has been ~dded in italics.
THE CHOLIAMBUS
299
4. As could be expected positive
rhythmical
word-initial verses tice
(cp.
proportion
of
2.2.
the prac-
is less
than to
in
the
verse
strictly
shows sig-
easily
as is
of the final
of Herodas
under-
metron.
the number of split-resolutrimeter,
in the iambic
that
of
be one more respect
in which there
rhythmic
of
realization
the
metric
above).
.. ..
OU't'tl\q.JOV oO-ct nµ:x,cdnrv o0T' &:,xovco. 56 o0Ttln6>..LVoO-ct nC:c;TtOALtOLOLXEtto.L 79 ~ CAJ lJtvl~ 1'1.ptdAn{;; CXJ6tv0£L'VaV I I I .1 9 a.t 6cpca).i:6E:t 6t IALl'Q)(JtQXl,L ~ II.40
w
-
w
(The one occurrence early
practice,
realization
seems
tolerance
verse-positions
iambic
poetry
This
in the choliambus
rhythm of choliambic
higher
E.g. Herod.
and the somewhat larger
from the
significantly
a certain
profile
of
the
iambic trimeter.
the clausular
differences
is
proportion
as
above)),
to than in the regular
comedy included. is
(3.6)
In the choliambic
tions
smaller
(such
in view of the divergent
4.1.
to
word-end at certain
Furthermore, stood
with regard
a cRe&ura (3.3.
of avoiding
nificant
liberty
tendencies
resolution
without
adhered
in view of the greater
iambography
in Hipponax - is fr.
..
- unparalleled
in the
trimeters
of
25:
"6:no 0 1 !6UDELEV "'Ap"tf;llLt."-"CJt 6t ~." which
is
Hipponax' 4.2.
verse,
verse-position the
see 2.1.2.
t rimeter;
it
Menander.
See also
is
resolution
in
above.) a resolution
often;
compare
in pos. the
4 is
frequency
followed
of word-end
by at
in general.
following
general
the only example of double
choliambi
comparatively
Word-end with
the same time
In Herodas'
word-end this
at
resolution
situation, rather 3.4.
not as frequent
similar above.
in pos.
2 is,
again
as it
to the incidence
is
in accordance in the
in the
tragic
trimeter
of
300
RHYTIIM AND METRE
E.g. Herod.
VI.93 Vll.60
- -
Vlll.11 The
reduced
UL'IQ)£0V,l .....6 6' 4.MJ£o6k 4v dn£i:v \JOL ~1.0.,l >m4.LVLC1, ~18a).' "Apyctci ou in lei.a OE ITPUXoucnv· 6-Ua \ilv att\4D
strictness
divergences
also
resolutions
occurring
with
regard
from
within
a single
word
to
perfectly
comparable
meter.
Word-internal
resolution
ively:
resolutions
occurrence
remarkably
is
the
the
the
appears
again,
of
to
in
the
occurs
Hipponax,
large (or
in
22.22%,
Callimachus
and
rhythmic
proportion
word-group),
situation in
of
the
which,
comic
Call. He rod.
26.3/4 191.36
i:xJnw~vb(;
60.L~
-)G,:bp:iyc ty~w,
6n U,V
Herodas
respect-
TUVtQ
6'
rovoOx°'-
'1S7\OMmttELV ctxcv oto1.v~" ~•
w:TtE
I. 2 9 8tclL, (1)1.~0CXXIX>I. , XPUOLOV,'VETl\lLOKOL
no.\1.°',4vf.-c::>cc, >an' ~ CJl£}..lVUVE03£, ,:m,~LTIV ~ 8l,\fac Ala: L• Kn, ""eacavov cit Ti), TtP006la.,11.lQ.lll.)i:6v
I I. 2 6/ 7 ~,.
88
ur.44 IV.19 91 93 V.46
- ... i:bvTtEkxvbv
6.U,..6 H.qnuoc ~ CxmEp C,:p1.ci~'ttlL .., ... tK 6£~1.fic -rbvnl\.aNO., KOKKQ.\n, Ot;\cov
~°' £UP'!~ 6a.uX~J£~, ...... M0l tnl Iii M8r.lolVLK£C 5 7/8 t:v TOO 6L~oC I TOV YQ)OV't'QI ~ Euovi:a. urvvftv I >«1t v~ I To ax.fh.n. a2 o 6' tfortLo6& 1 ~oelt:vxdoK&L 65 ~ 6n' ~
Ca 11.
192.611
6l>«1Loe
I tc I~
qot~ I ~l\WJ
~
V
o zeuc,1 oo6l>«1La. 6'
1 a.tOlJl,CVt.ciN
I tf;6caJ£ I TO ~\.11 I.I 8otLc.n, 6oao:::JEL I -drv~ nc· I OUK 6JJr;i 2 5 6.U.' bAtArPto-L I HOL ~ I t:K >«11.vf1c 79/80 OEti:a.L• eotu:xn, I -drv lJ£).o.1.vl6' I flUC Tpe;tc I t:vxtaa:,.I -rooaHPnTou "'['WVtt:,ltE"t"WV ~
Herod.
Word-end
at
7 and 9:
I t:v 6t [ ~ ~ 13.1 fK Tt£AAUx~nCvovt&c· I o«1L t1k 8".pns; -cov c:ll:bv I tx{3p6vI fryetcft:11. 74 ~r=ou:n x4'£1. iw8ov" I 6v ot I YPfY.lOL r 1 • 2 tn..iwvKPLi:a.t 6nKOl.>8e:vI ooot l Tf'lc 66Enc 33 1'tPbc~ VOlJOU(;f3Atrtooo1. I Kft}Jt ! TOV f;&tvov 89 ~, ~n, cn-pta>.ouU£ liJOOvov I fi TLUYl III.3 -roOtov Ka-c' ~u 6£tpov, I dXPL,iA, µn l,1£ % I YU\Al,LK • I e:r\0.1.• VI.12 ClLvtouc ~vlKTK; 6 1 tv 8£oto1. TtEl,::n:m 119. 1 >Gl TtEOELVe:pfpcqv bt' 6oH6v, uant YaJLPL yaartcri Aesch. Pers. 709 c!>6':x:ni.i:lv nav-cwv Ut.£00XWV &Jx,v EU'"LUX.EL n.6~ 713 navtQ yap, l!apE',C', aKOUC7f;l u00ov t:v ai:nxet XSXMt>
E.g. Archil.111
-
Soph.
-
OR 1517
oroa• bi>' OL!; OC1V Erl,J.L; Eur. Or
775/6 11 d
is the most frequent situation
trimeter;
only
from this
picture
the
1.1.1.1.
8 and a long
one in
therefore sample
in that
those
ancipitia
>V.U.ilV
ELOOUQ.L
~
fq:xxnc ~v51.kCl; a' 6ou£vo1..
V
realized
as a long syl-
one.
small
of
similar
to that
Solon's
tetrameters
the verse-type
with
is the most frequent
be expected
above,
three
is
at the anceps positions As might
!I
tu:w,;ou; 11 unM&ul
In comedy the type with all The general
RDMEEL!;;, wat 't6't'
Atyo1.µ' 6crroto1.V t>JM.J •••
11 mi:pt n~ lable
V
in
view of
verse-types pos.
the
three
deviates
short
syllables
one (see below). tendencies
described
which have a short
12 are
in the iambic
comparatively
anceps
rare
sub
in pos.
- being
the
THE TROCHAIC TETRAMETER verse-types
with
positions
a
319
disfavoured
in which rhythmical
realization
tendencies
of
those
two
anceps
are strongest.
Some examples are: Aesch. Pers.
244
1'UlUm, xt.l..Euaov -
748 TIEPL~
one compares
If
authors a
studied,
particular the
be expected by Solon that
low.
Of
rather
of
the
the
Sophocles'
excepted
A below;
coapare
other
tragic
poets,
correlation
only
comic poets:
(.92);
the
a manner
comparable
to
meter
(eh.III,
three
long
which
is
due to the
Euripides'
tetrameters
the
1.1.2.
syllable
Accordingly, that
it
is
Ch.III,
1.1.2.
nature
of those
the
correlation
in
the
authors
within
in
iambic
the
above.
and
to
the
of his
verse
extremely
is
Euripides
show a nature
between
between
to
As was
singular
correlation
of
few tetrameters
included)
Aeschylus
than that samples
with
the
the
the tragic
of
tragic and the
three
does
short
syllables
ancipitia
observed not
in pos.
the
iambic
in
with
in triwith
proportion
of this
in
verse-type
the
4 observed
In fact,
the
decreases
of the verse-type
(tallying
II above).
in
increase
infrequency
especially
which occur
Solon and Sophocles,
The frequency
comparative
and Table
of
situation
however
of short
see 1.1.1.
verse-types
appears
verse-types
- which show a correlation
average
above).
ancipitia
percentage
a short
than
owing
small
of the verse-type
meters:
of
.76 and .71 respectively.
frequency
high
only
the
&, EVC'P(Oi:l,L'tyw
(Archilochus
somewhat higher
we disregard
If
poets
-
the several
to be greater
poets
tetrameters,
is
between
have come down to us,
high
poets
III
~
,
frequency
in view of the singular that
with
appear
comic
see Table
-
relative
the differences
genre
trimeter, .97:
the
("8£i:p::iL O"tp:l't6v
~ ~ O"tp:ll:4>
'IT1V Mv \lAJxti'V l(Q~'
151 7
>«1tw::J).bv
oooe;lou TTO..\w't£
~£
relatively
in his tetra-
two out of three
most commonly in Euripides'
of
verse
have
Euripides,
it
predominate
in
in pos. 4. and
again
verse-types
with in
which
the
exception
long
ancipitia
of
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
320
general
tend
to become more frequent
in which a greater order (total (total order (total
nuaber
of short
at
the
ancipitia
expense
of
verse-types
occur.
Verse-types with two or three short ancipitia occupy, 2, 4.5, 4.5 of frequency in Archilochus, the poss. 17 ; in the Aeschylean order, the poss. 3, 5.5, 5.5 IT); in Euripides, poss. 2, 5, 6 and 7 (total 20); 3.5, ~ 7 of verse-types in Aristophanes, the poss. 24.5). In the
ization
case
of
of the
the
last
also,
tetrameter anceps
verse-types
position
(pos.
12)
with
a long
become
in the and 6 and 7 in the and 8 real-
increasingly
more frequent. In the order of frequency in Archilochus, those verse-types 3, 6, 7 and which have a long syllable in pos. 12 occupy the poss. 12 (total 28 ); in the Aeschylean order, poss. 2, 4, 7 and 8 (total 21 ); in the Aristophanean order, pose. 1 1 3.5, 5 and 8 17.5). (total of a tetrameter
Some examples anceps
positions
with
short
in all
three
are:
... o00' W~~ ... 122. 1 ~1,.IJ,u.,,, ~ d:6tv tan v oOO' ~ .. ... ... 32. 2 TD-COlOOC, "t'l-lXlWLOOC 6t l«:l.l ~L~ ~I.ALXOIJ ... ... ... 3 3. 2 toeM ~ &oO 61.e,6vi:q; at,~ OUKtot~-«> - y6p Erm., ... ... 34.6/7 oo~- d )Jtv c:bv 8Eoto1.v t\\M:Xl, ... ... ... ~ 6' oo1,1.i"CT'IV f£P{:Dv, a(J6t UC)I."CQXlWLOOC ., ... ... Ag. 1666 CPlVE:1.v tl,ll (pos.8)
......
uiv x~v...... t.)IStN AfY£t.V (pos.10) Vesp. 496 t\v 6£ 'fflT£1.0V~ ..i:0 mCc &pucu" ~ n (pos .10) ...... 1 Men. Per. 340 o6 y6p ~ CW.MrrptLV ~t&lL,
f3lQ."t'dyEL~;
~,
4 oo l'OO,nOOL f3f:nxta. M>,oc:mL >«1l ~ >«1l ~LQ
2.4.2.
In
pos.
9 the
all
selves
authors
colon-final
more frequently In
the
Euripides, unique
than
studied,
anceps
is
in
the
realized
case as
of
a long
resolution
in
syllable
even
usually. the
within
resolutions
occurring
the
of
corpus
the
in tragic
pos.
7 -
tetrameter
in
them-
are
332
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
exceptional of
the
also
ancpes
occurrence:
in five
that
they
times
out
are of
followed 14 (or
by a short
realization
35. 71%; compare
the
general
29.94%).
E.g. IA 884
b 6t ~ 'tlv'
£l'X£ nr.:xiqa:nv, ~
......
1382 'tbv ·E>.tvt,c 't'E~
or.
1524
also
or.
ro>.ty£1.c;• a:,,t£1. OE 736, IA 346 cited
2. 4. 3. The tendency anceps
in pos.
not
evidence
in
syllables. from
limited
(word-initial) realization For
the
Here again
so
ducing
( to be observed
if
a
one should
of
the
one in
anceps
a pseudo-rising
the
preceding
resolution
this
less
movement:
two occurrences
in
or. 738 IA 863
1354
yap, f\nc;
comedy)
rather
element
is
be cautious
might
realized
objectionable
µ£
to
(as
it
one
is
conclusions
however,
tends
the
as two short
in drawing
that
the
make the
long
were
by pro-
l--vlw---v-11). tetrameters,
•~•
see
sub
2. 3.
alrtoCc; 6' U8t oC
realize
as a long
than
suggest,
position
to
.., .... fl,.to1.y£ 'tali'tbv <a.M£V ..,.., ~
~ oOK t~
1515 o6
outside
Archilochus'
above. Eur.
above.
syllable
sample;
bc4u,c,Ev bt 66i.rAJV;
6M:~, "'V~ Ildp1.c; auvt:a1.c;. ~ f3(:ltv' b.) 66u::.,v.
~ub 2.3.
12 as a short
1
l,l.
......
acx,1.klu:lv .......
66i.rAJV
'tbv YQl.l.iJV ~ f'p:x,\.a.( )6n£,cpC\K.i)cSt Tl;
As a matter of fact, this observation may be considered to corroborate the rejection of both Aesch. Pers. 171 and Eur. IA 356 as showing a case of resolution in pos. 12: while neither of them i::. 111 word-initial, both are followed by a long syllable. (See DALE (1958)• (1969) 133). Aesch. Eur.
Pers. IA
356
171 roOot \.101.ytvE:oe&, 114xn1., ~ .., n1.at4n,'ta .., ~ ~1.c;• 'tl ~; 't'L\.0. cSt nQ:>OV £\¥X4)tt66e:v; (DALE: 't'C\.0. TQXJV 8')
THE TROCHAIC TETRAMETER
333
TABLE VII
REALIZATION OF ANCIPITIAFOLLOWING RESOLUTION IN POS. 3*
Ju.! Archil.
-
Aesch.
1
Ju"
= 50% 55.3%
Soph.
-
Eur.
4
1 = 50% 44.7%
= 44.44%
5 = 55.56%
46.6% Aristoph.
6
= 54.55%
53.4%
5
Men.
17
= 68%
= 45.45% 31%
69% 8
= 32% 38.87%
61.13%
TABLE VIII
REALIZATION OF ANCIPITIAPRECEDING RESOLUTION IN POS. 5*
"u~
.!Ju 1 = 100% 56.25%
-
Aesch.
-
-
Soph.
-
-
Eur.
2=
Archil.
25% 46.6%
Aristoph.
Men.
* The general
4=
80% 69%
43.75%
6"" 75% 53.4% 1
15 = 78.95% 4 61.13%
percentage of short/long above) has been added in italics.
= 20% 31%
= 21.05% 38.87%
ancipitia
(see
Table
II
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
334
TABLEIX REALIZATION OF ANCIPITIA FOLLOWING RESOLUTION IN POS. 7
Ju a
u'L.I Archil.
-
-
Aesch.
-
-
Soph.
-
Eur.
9=
Aristoph.
2
64.29% 5 = 35.71% 29.94% 70.06%
= 100%
27%
73% Men.
31
=
86.11% 67.4%
5 = 13.89% 32.6%
TABLEX REALIZATION OF ANCIPITIAPRECEDING RESOLUTION IN POS. 9 .I
Archil.
-' iu
Ju 1
= 100%
31.25%
68.75% Aesch.
Soph.
Eur.
Aristoph.
Men.
7 = 100% 71.21%
-
1 = 100% 70.59%
-
45
14
41
= =
=
28.79%
29.41%
77.59% 13 70.06%
= 22.41%
82.35% 73%
3
= 17.65%
68.33% 67.4%
19
29.94%
27%
= 31.67% 32.6%
TiiE
TROCHAIC TETRAMETER
335 TABLEXI
REALIZATION OF A.NCIPITIA FOLLOWING RESOLUTION IN POS. 11 JL JJ 2 = 100% 33.33%
-
1 = JOO% 43.18%
-
-
I
Archil.
Aesch.
Soph.
JL U
66.67%
56.82%
= 100%
47.06% Eur.
Aristoph.
5=
52.94%
4=
55.56% 41.98%
11 = 68.75%
44.44% 58.02%
5 = 31.25%
53.33% Men.
18 = 62.07% 55.8%
46.67°/o
11 = 37.93% 43.89%
TABLEXII
REALIZATION OF A.NCIPITIA PRECEDING RESOLUTION IN POS. 13 J;
JJ
uJb
Archil.
-
-
Aesch.
-
-
Soph.
-
-
Eur.
-
-
Aristoph.
2
= 66.67% 53.33%
Men.
10 ==66.67% 55.8%
1 = 33.33% 46.67%
5
= 33.33% 43.89%
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
336 3.
In the
trochaic
produced
by the
rhythmical in
between
the
Here at
are
of
structuring devices
as
to
the
effect
to
those
contrast
basic
movement
(falling
different
metrical
(1)
responsion)
produced
the
the
their
internal
verse
regulating
similar
as both
their
(2)
of the
for
seemingly
However,
effect
in
would
the
lead
trochaic
observed
in
strucus to
tetrameter
the
iambic
tri-
p. 348f. below. the
rhythmic
realization
complete
a
verse-end
Menander's
verse,
complete
a rhythmically
ences:
other
and
opposite
again,
the
trimeter.
rhythmical the
internal
of verse
vs.
see 3.4,
tuting
the
verse-beginning)
exactly
meter:
of
two types
the
the
and
iambic
(catalectic
expect, ia
the
rising
ture
caesura,
movement
observed vs.
tetrameter
tetrameters
whole
weak (just
of
profile
as consti-
shows marked individual
rhythmic as
the
boundary
in his
notably
trimeters:
see
differoccurs
Ch.III
in
sub
3.
above). E.g •
.sam. 502/3
( ••• ) I) ~ Ze:O, 'toO ~.
ono' 511 /2
~lov
~ -cii,v ~-
O(olrr.ov -re: Ha.l eut1.6'IEtol l,1.1¥)Lq. ~ 880 rrot'; -aUn1.v't'tili, l~\O. 6'~v ra~ \JofAA.€1. KTEVEtv 913 ~ >«J.lidvTOA.U' •j&ptnn1. 6', l 1iOU, rux:,O\.D.1.o 'Iu6v, d T' fx.ei:,v TU~ )G~
339
THE TROCHAICTETRAMETER This situation described
above,
conform type
shows that,
to their
of the
the
in contrast
tetrameters
respective
elided
to the statistical
of both
literary
boundaries
Archilochus
genres
occurring
divergence and Henander
as far as the linguistic
at
the caesura
position
is
concerned. TABLEXIII CAESURA totals
93.75%
Archil. Solon
89.58%
4.17%
21.5
6.25%
98.15%
-
-
1.51%
-
-
91.36%
6.79%
13.45
73.67%
68%
5.67%
12
Men.
95.3%
91.85%
3.45%
26.64
In most of the authors
bi syllable the lU
quite
often;
phenomenon is tends
to
reinforce
Ch.111,
the
the caesura
tetrameters frequent.
falling
is
followed
by a
of Solon and Aeschylus The word-boundary
movement of
in the iambic
4.7%
trimeter,
the
Ch.III,
at pos.
second
colon;
Table XIV and
above.
This (such
the
particularly
compare the situation
3.1.3.1.
in
studied,
1.85% 26.33%
Aristoph.
3.1.2.
3.79%
62.5
-
100%
100%
Eur.
without caesura
94.7%
96.21%
Soph.
ratio
..
100%
100%
Aesch.
+ elision
• elision
in
tendency marked
3.1.3.1.
does
not
contrast above)
apply to
the
in
the tetrameters situation
and Sophocles
in
his
of Archilochus trimeter:
- which can be understood
see in
340
RHYTHM AND tlETRE
view of ance
their
with
rhythm
the
is
ively,
practice.
tendency
produced
there
is
Sophocles
by word-end a
word-end
which case In which
certain
word-end
pos.
at
pos.
such
reflect
not
is
lacking
the
comparative
the Aristophanean
at
early
preference the
11 as
reverse
situation
10 is not likely
comparatively
often.
frequency
tetrameter
of
in general:
13 alternat-
l-v-l"u--v--11 applies);
remarkably
in
often
- in
to occur.)
tendency with
accord-
clausular
the sequence
such occurs
unconnected
in
iambography,
for
this
where,
11 or at pos.
pos.
tetrameters
apparently
Archilochus
in
in Solon
at
Aristophanes' is
(In
prevailing
l~l-v--lu-11(while
over
as
clausular
the These
long
is
absent,
fact
that
too
the
caesura
observations
words
see Table
to
-
seem to
be observed
in
XXI A below.
E.g. Solon 33. 2 td!J>,/J.y6p
{£00 81.&6v-roc; lal.rc6';loCJKbStEato
5/6 ~ ydp
wtn"tnea(;,ITWJOmvl&peovov~ HO.l~ •Aarivb.iN Ii.dlvov Iti~ ula.v u 34.1/2 ot 8' t«it i:do' tfal.d'tcn1. ln:a.Ud,I &xmivov atpoc. (For
the difference
and 3.1.3.l.2. Pers.
(p.
in rhetorical 175ff.)
phrasing,
cp.
Ch.III,
3.1.3.1.1.
above.)
170 ~ i:do', ~ OU~
fXOV'D:IN I 1.ii:'lv6EJ ~1.
').f,yo.,
£"W,&V£t, y6p ~ ti1.15c; I1.ii:'1v6£ I~ 'HCV\.ELC 101 n.o.>.MlJtv v&:>tx ~, ln:oUao~tx x~1GH0. 109 l:i ~ T"dvruN~ I6>43ov Iru-ruxEt no"tllt> 1 1s 't l v1. T"6rf+,; A.01. \,dj nc fi>.&IOKT1m6d 1\ cn:do1.c TTOA£ 1. ; 728 \ICW'tL>«X; O'tlX1.'t6c ~t,1Tt£Cbvl~ OTIXl"COV fvt. C xSo\.of; cUA', tnet otoi; T'llMJ.L6vlaoL~ &velatnmL wat '006' t~trri:XJE£V c:x:n-£ le6ouc~x,v>CA(icuL~; µ0\0a1 flt Btpfri-vtpfiµ6v kixx,L V 00 no.Uli)v µim
342
RHYTHMAND METRE
Eur.
or.
779 o6K txw. II
IA
389
1,llL~L~ ~'t"L~,
6' tAnl,lt.an
~L
>«»cbv¼°'
OO'TL,1~
CA) i6Uav,
>CD«i:N;
~L
nMv t, at >0.t ativlTDtoo: 'tOO'to 6'o6 avrtxouauya,:~. II Cal(;"tOOo'1£tVEX' oo ~'ta'-.
877
1367
cbc;ldVDC.' 006t, NC1nklv£tv
Ar1stoph. l't6A.Ls;JGt
ofnl', up' oWvlarv:>UAJV-ya:nplCotnL 283 JGt 't"fl.uxoc;,~ IIQ)L~ ouclfi;~ TUt0't£ 5 71 d 6t. 1T0U Ttt.ooLEV de -rov lq.iovtv \dxQ n vl 622/3 t'ivlx' 4v ~ I\ 1-rov Mtit.wv'I\ ~,
i
273
Eq.
Nub.
416
artb&a' blJEte l«1L ye:M't"'• m.e1 ! 0.J )a.yjA7IJ·~ 4Ud. u' E'WX,£i:vfroLl.4 lt.ar•, M 6' o6 ~ vti Ill', de -rov~ y' .11~ l"t0\10t-vwou U£~1.
4 34
>Gt ~ 'tOU-COUl'NCll. llJl'iU£atic6£1,llOEVL
341
Vesp.
1
etc.
etc.
t.yc,xa rdvm.l)noto.
lnivm;lllJl'il,OL 6i.a.\ty,,u. o6v lt.arLv·0(6', 6:Ja'i>c00,
Sam. 481 ·ro(h' •
tvw. -roTIJ.L6lov ouTlXX\&Lvov, ~IU\.PLOL &'inou, nan:() O't'q:,yw. 1)-roAOLTIOVItatL AO.)'t"P6, U£'t"LML.
492 tU.\' 501 tatLv 744 ~,
f\).DP'toV,OOTLChi;,v~ tn1.U,£AOO'toU'tOU.-roAOLTIOVlt.anv t'iutv fv 6' C~
Dysc • 713
759
(2)
Verses
occur
only
in
These
tetrameters
verse-positions 5 or
case
the
words
in
of
except
almost is On
verses
at
caesura'
metre
very
the at
only
pos.
not
other
iambic
trimeter.
that
the
circumstance
hand
all
the
7, 6, or,
less
greater
the
same,
of
the at
the
frequently,
here
wordthat
in
(61.36%)
of
a caesura
have
of word-end
pos.
1
even
without
percentage
•
10. However,
be considered
incidence
word-end
one
part
end at pos.
1t cannot the
that
at
NY be relevant
it
1
1n tragedy.
word-end
tetrameters
to
such verses
1n view of the average
positions
9% of
All
(poss.
a caesura
these
analogous
have
tyY1,4v
caesura;
two occurrences
remarkable
10 - so that
the
for
invariably
without
beginning
Aristophanes
word-end ative
comedy,
involved.
the
so much as a sub-optimal
of the second
4) - which
length
without
~V
of
as an 'alternat 61.36
10 produces
pos.
7 in
suggests a falling
343
THE TROCHAICTETRAMETER novement
(as
the caesura
(compare 3.1.2. poss.
E.g.,
7
above);
does)
does have some rhythmical
relevance
see also WEST(1982) 91.
& 10 (-v--0-u-lv-ul--v-u--jl):
Aristoph. 275
Eq.
6.:U.' tyw
OE "Cf!i aoflIta{rnJ YE,~
lqip..ot. hat m Ot."tLQ 1307/8 11 ·A110q.,6.u1.',o6 fiii;' tlJC)(; y' lcipE£t.not'' ,ldU• Mv brtb ,;~ CXDt£to'\tvmoeo. hca:n:,~." 620 ,46' &tav 8u£t.v oi:Q, lc:,,:i:xaA.o(h£ heat 6t.Md.C£T£ 5 75 vOv 6' tbv
Nub.
~t.
625
iifl ~lav
-rovoi;tqavc,v
~ai,
•
427 ~ fyCJYt'aiTd1,.J Wit,J
Vesp.
h6;u.ov VC¥>1 ~ £Caei:a.t.
\6toot.)(Q I-me tYKCVtPleac
432 ot ~ ~ XUWf)lw.£Vt£[T£1)(Qt 't00c Sak~ 495
OOtOC ~tv
fot.x' l~lbtt 'tqJQWL6t. nucv6v, I VEOOVtE1.IADX'Cim, ~ov o6x.~ 't05£. (lcta~,IElqx:,cl~ rl1c; 'tL(; E:lrqxn1.C,tUI). 1660/1
oa.luovoc;Ixn>.ij I txrpE;lq. ~ lt£TWlvl&twL. ~• Pers.
fxEt. lAOYO(;IY\Ml-t.kOC, EC TL(; ~1.o'C lOBE'Cv.
168/9
tar 1. lQC I~ 6'4.11 ~l~l\q.l.let,.)
Y•
I~,
6'.IPt 0' ~ ~ •
6ExmO'tOU lQX)lOlav.
712/3
vuv"tt Tlivta. 7 26 ~
OEICra-\@I~
yap, llJiJIXt',I~
rrplv )G(WV l&tv ~· lJ08ov tv ~X.EL ~-
l&tv 1-ctAor. Im,xan v olov ~ >OCOV 728 wun1«lcni:n:tb{;l~t, Tt£Cov~ mp:u6v 131 ~ TTOrTOt. lxc~ I~(; Haltt.Ka.Plac; cnp::J."'COO. 736 ~IUOA&tv!Y~, lv ouotv C,£UK'tfip1.ov
347
THE TROCHAIC TETRAMETER Eur. Or. 789-93 ~ laLydv l&.1£1.\IOV. 11-ui>'1IXMt> 6t Kt+XDVEi:r;. IJKEL\.O
l,K)L
111-l'a am£
11,.0VOV lnix,ax.vttr;•••
()Tl
'tax
KlL'\.OV
al,
¼EL{;1
1)6.Ua ~ a' t-yw.
\.L' loCcrt:Pt>IKJ.~L.
IJEuJxe+>tr;l'1au£1.\d~ ~()oCJK ~LYE aoO. IJE~ l>.ux,nr;h.1£-ca:,x.Etv t1k; Mc. IIT66' oovCt"W. 1528 OO'tE)'a,:) IY\M'l lntqucac OO't1 tv dvixmLV au y' er 1530 IA
310
6fo
)'a,:) lectic
ldkotcav •AcYo,;.t~EY£lixm1.
·E.Uaooc ll.dA.1.0't' lfla.1.TW4JOU O"t~
873 TDC!Xl ar\v lnc:t~ lb ~ alrt6x.ELP ~L
KttVEtv
87 5 ~ IA£VKnv lq0V£\XAJV tile m>a.LTU4)0l) c5tcriv
1110:JEL a.>,k.CJ6x~ OO"ttl> ~ 914 \OUnJCOV lcnpc1't'EUIJ.• Id\.opxov JGnl mtc kOHOCC~ ••• 899
~L
,~
Aristoph. Vesp.
337 ot.m:x,l l~vlwa8£UO(l.)v.
6U'
6:P£.CJ6E 't0U-c6vo.J
340 o6K t4, u' ,lw 'vope:c,loudCcLv cx:,e,l;f(:dv a(££v
lGM6v
435-7
EC 6t ur'i,I•v nt&:iq; lrnxECa1.ca(££v
6':>1.ati'ic:,t'tE,
~ hw ITTOUCJv I~ or!n ~~ -cov ~I )EC 6t 1-l'a l'toChovllJ£8"iaE1.c,~ 'tL 001. fl1YY'lc,E:m1.. Men.
S'dm.
559-62
TIDP'.1-rEVEtr; I""NXbv" !AtYtAJV ).K)L mvta. I )'t0U1X&JV aCn6' du' ty~. II hl ll )4nLO'tOV rri::dnn l,K)L 6oK£t'c ¼£LV. lid.Un lJl',vIE[6ov.l II~. II OO'tOf;.o6K t.onv ~o,;.· rnJOV, autJoOa· ~ 'HQ.l~ I&a:op.w; ,, 128.l
Solon
~,
~·,
13):
(pos.
Archil.
Tt6v,;or;, ~l 0 1 ~ ~ ~ tcnata1.I~, V oftin X£1.~, Kl.)(.QV£1. 6' tE 6V.m'C1'1(;1 ~116 6:J.n; 246-8 &u.' flJot 6ok£tv 'td.x' £COJJmvm.\,Ql.l.£0~ IA6vov· TOOo£v6':>~in qxiJU>cnq:x,1.x6v TTPtne:1. lio9£tv, Ha.t qqx 1. CDPtcn rrp5:yq; tJ::Ja)bv f\ waxbvIXAUE 1.v. (poss.
>axv6vm nptv ~lt&:tvledaoc 723 wat T65' tf;bti:xifcv i:m-£ B0:ntc:¥X>V IxMp:11. IJJtyav1 744 na.tc 6' tl,lbc tdo' cl.J waTe:1.~ tiVlDEvlw.>1~1. 712 vOv T£ OE
351
THE TROCHAIC TETRAMETER 746
~LOE
O')(nc,El.'V ~,
Ba:JIIOP()vlr,!x,vl8£00
Tt.£()1.~ rroUnv xt>.£uaov ti'VUO£'VlrroUii>IOTIXl"t9 751 Etxe: rai:6' f\.16v;6tooLwa.l.a'iTIDAi.,clTWJU't'OUln6voc 757 wt.do' te ~ 6v£C011 no;udx1.elw>.wvl~ 748
In Soph. (poss. 11 & 13): OR 1516 TT£ LOTOOV, KE t 1518-21
Eur. (poss.
*
UJ1c5tv tt,o. IJ Tvivtu yapI)((11.~
[ )(Q,,\Q,.
JJ.' ~ ~1.e d:no1.KOV. 11 i:oO8£00 JJ.1 la.Ci:e:tcl66cnv. dW,1.v te noCav TTPCealw.,v;'t00lO TlivT' Ifx.I ~() cx5,:oeMa' , b -me;6':,COTO(;auyo.-r~ Iart£ C~ Ira"tflp. 764 TIPbc 1:lvoc1 "'C'ah'' d'V rq:xx,e:Cn rote tiaotel lO.tye:w. II otaea. Eti-r' tJJ.', oo-rLeCAIcot wa.t Tbcvo-.cl e:Ovouc;I txi,Jv; 1394 de v' d'Vl'iP MQ£l~ la.lu.'lfx0001. no1.J,atw,v 784 ~ yq:, a0roucl&¥:>'!fxOV'tCIC; tv Xqx)LV
Sem. 20. 2 Eur.
Med.
or Aesch.
Pers.
736 ~
Sept.
6
~rv
l~l6uot'vCEUK-rrp1.ov
Y~,
with
dvle:lclTtC>,.\uc ,au\ ITTOA.1.v n6.\£1.lJtv VJcoi; I !;v Iro Siiu.1.ov TUXEI.v
·E't~
Ag. 640
The with
'mannerism'
the
meter,
of
verse-final
too;
Aesch.
word
compare, 739 ~
or.
Eur.
Ned.
colon-initial
frequently
monosyllables
occurs
in
the
responding
trochaic
tetra-
e.g.,
227 m1.6t ,at
Pers.
Eur.
using
yap tanv
t1-Dta1. It'l'rv6'Itx.•~
~LC
~
~lmvo'
qu:nv
!&i>1.Yl,lhq; xam.d.;
with
and
the
examples
l 86f f.
the
constitutes
fiiab:xxv
~1.,lt1'rv6'!~H.£V 1237 rutoac K1X1VOOOJJ Itf'io6' I~1.
other
3. 1 • 4 , p.
In
373 ~
of
this
type
of
xaov6(;
trimeter
cited
in
Ch. I II,
a hove.
tetrameter
also,
the
an emphatic
addition
monosyllable to the
first
in colon
9 sometimes
pos. instead:
e.g. 122. 3
Archil. Eur.
IA 401
tx U£o:xi.~Cncf8rlKEI~'t',
d 6t lJ1'i~ qpove:tvlro,Iu\J.'
Compare the following Sem. 6.1 Aesch.
Ag.
I~
QXX)(;
tvw~ )(Q,)J;,;
trimeters:
Y\}\,(11.XOC ~I xi::tiu.' I~ Mit.Ce:m1. 1221 ciJv tvtt,x,1.c Te:I onMyxv',ltnolKncrtov vt~
Sept.
375 .).tyoLll'
c1v e:(~
Irolm -tG',v t'\QVt'l' olt oov£tu1.; (••• ) vfit u' I~ ~ .., &Toi.w.ov.c••• >
88 9 TOOo'Ibtu:rcd:tt;i Kcw.,Jvo(j;
The sample however these observations.)
seems
to
be too
( ••• )
small
to
draw conclusions
from
In Menander also, monosyllables are elided less frequently than they are in his trimeters: a proportion of 1.18, against 0.83 in his trimeters, where a larger proportion of them occurs in a coloninitial verse-position. Here, the remarkably large proportion of nonelided monosyllables is produced principally by those monosyllables which occur in non-colon-initial verse-positions (and which accordingly comparatively seldom are phrase-initial words: see Ch.III, 3.5. and 3.5.1. above). This phenomenon seems to be due to a relaxation of the rhythmical norm in order to incorporate everyday speech.''" Sam.
loe,~ d~ fpx~1.. 11-n'\vXp..oLln 453 £ho. noO 'an l wv;I IJTDP' tiutv t:veDv. I Jw5cvvbv >.ty~ 462 "tOOC vdlJou!;, ~t, JC1nm.wvTtiv xOAJ'rv. 'tTIP£1. 6t laul 482 6M' mdYKT1, ln1i:t~. IIJmdYKT1;~ tl,ii7v d., MWL~ 4 86 "tOUt vdlJou!;In lJ£ TTOLd \I, 4v fxTJt I\IOUV.llJ6M' t:w. 495 6.>.M.Tlc; lau;IIJTC ~ t>u:CvnytyOVEV atTCa.11)TC ~; 500 6u\ TC, I J5i.a TC,I~; I~ 6' df 1.ott: i h-6 TtPiYln yao 598 ci:tx.£ 61tOEaUT6v.IJ~LK£tt, Arn.it.a,1\.1£, ,~ Et 450 -~
..,·ITCt;
356
RHYTHM AND METRE
722 ~ ~
Dysc.
129 o(;J:,'
llC)L.
ot
~LC.
tvwCX>Llvw.1't,
0'
Tl£L011v
tat,,
de.
~lwv
UELPo>tLov;tav
hw
T'
741 n>..EC.ov'OUK~ \IOULUA>, rvJtv b.Etvo Y' Ca6L,
lnat
In Aristophanes the situation is the opposite: 0.525 in the against 0.84 in his trimeters. This is the more noticetetrameter, able since the incidence of elision in the case of non-monosyllables is not much higher than that in Menander. This situation apparently constitutes an exception to the tendency for a large proportion of elision to be concurrent with a high incidence of phrase-initiality: the majority of the elided monosyllables which occur at the beginning either. Here again deicof a colon appear to be non-phrase-initial tic words are involved remarkably frequently (cp. note 194 above). ~
314 oto'
Eq.
w
~ I't'OO&'I66Ev rd.\C1L cTroE-a:tL
319 ""' t.la. ~ 1-roOT' I~ 596 '1E1.01.
1311 ~ 6'
6' IcCo' I~ctan1.
6'xc»caI'1XIOt'I .A8twcLLOLC.,
580 1,.D'l6E:vl Ew ~,
Nub.
607 614
ix:x6-c6v,"tti>~ 514 &.a61.Edf;e:1.v oCCUJ.C. a' wi; Tdv,nl 1X108' l~'tXiVE1.c. 111 7 -roOto 6'
I6:n' I&y 1.0TOVfn.ttv,
t\v nc. 6c,-q:xiTruix>c;~
but also Eq.
5 7 2 "t'CO-c' ~-r•
Nub.
584 fl c:,e;Mvri6'
Ves p •
Iet -r'IfpvoOv-ro 1'l ~L tEt.At:1.T1£V -rocb8ouc, lb 6' IM1.oc. dv,
480 -oC6t um,IoC.10' Itv OEAL \Kf>oou:n LVI oC.10' Itv TUlY~> 524 ctrtt llOL, l'tC.0'
1~-ro
6Et\O.-,:f;I 01.0.,~ idt
'141tvt\c;
THETROCHAIC TETRAMETER
357 TABLEXVI MONOSYLLABLES
Archil.
Solon
Aesch.
pos. I - el.
4.17%
5.56%
-
-
9.85% 3.79%
pos. 2
-
-
-
- el.
-
-
-
pos. 3
2.08%
-
0.76%
- et.
-
-
-
pos. 4
-
5.56%
1.52%
- el.
-
-
-
Soph.
Eur.
Aristoph.
Men.
7.72% 3.09%
4.33% 0.33%
11.91% 5.02%
0.31%
-
-
-
-
-
-
1.85% 0.62%
2% 1%
2.19% 0.63%
-
2.47% 0.93%
2% 0.33%
1.88% 0.94%
35.29% 17.65%
-
-
pos. 5
2.08%
-
- el.
-
-
-
-
2.16% 0.31%
2.67% 1%
4.39% 2.82%
pos. 6 - el.
2.08%
-
3.03%
-
-
-
-
1% 0.33%
1.25% 0.63%
-
-
-
0.31%
-
0.93% 0.31%
1.33%
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
4.63% 4.01%
0.33% 0.33%
3.45% 2.51%
11.42% 4.63%
2.67% 1.33%
6.58% 2.82%
pos. 7
- el. pos. 8 - el. pos. 9
- el.
14.58% 4.17%
-
14.39% 7.58%
-
-
-
pos. 10 - el.
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
0.31% 0.31%
pos. 11
2.08% 2.08%
-
2.27%
-
-
-
4.32% 1.85%
2.33% 1.33%
1.57% 0.63%
-
-
-
-
1.23%
0.33%
- el.
-
-
-
-
1.57% 1.25%
pos. 13
-
-
-
1.85% 1.23%
1.25%
-
1.52% 0.76%
0.33%
- el.
-
pos. 14 - el.
-
-
-
-
0.62%
-
-
-
-
pos. 15
-
-
-
0.76% 0.76%
-
- el.
3.7% 3.7%
1% 1%
4.39% 4.39%
43.21% 20.06%
20.33% 7%
41.07% 21.94%
- el. pos. 12
tot. - el.
-
28.89% 8.89%
-
11.11% 0
34.09% 11.36%
35.29% 17.65%
-
358
RHYTIIM AND HETRE
TABLEXVI A
pos. 1 pos. 2
TOTALNUMBER OF MONOSYLLABLES (&ratio-/+
elision)
Archil.
Solon
Aristoph.
0 2 0
0
-
- -
-
1
Aesch.
5
8 0.625
0
-
-
Soph.
Eur.
3 3 I
10
-
0
-
15
0.67
1
1 12 0.08
Men.
16 22
0.73
-
- -
-
3
2
0 pos. 3 pos. 4
pos. 5
pos. 6
pos. 7 pos. 8 pos. 9
0 I 0
- -
- -
0 1 0
0 2 0
0 1 0
- -
-
0 1 0
- -
- -
- -
0 0
1
- -
0.5
-
-
- -
- -
3 4
-
0.6
-
-
-
-
0 4 0
- -
-
- -
-
- 9
- -
10
- -
0.9
pos. 10
-
-
- -
-
pos. 11
1 0
- -
0 3 0
- -
pos. 12
- -
- -
- -
- -
pos. 14
-
pos. 15
- -
I
4 9 0.44
6
- -
-
-
-
- 1 2 0.5
4
5
9
3
-
-
1
-
-
-
- -
- -
I
0
- -
0 2 0
0.55
0 4 0
0
15 22 0.68
4
- -
-
8
4
J
4
9
-
I
3
2
1
0
67
73
3
0.67 4
1
0
4
0
- 3
0
4
0
0
0.92
-
1.33
0 0
12
12 0.75
I
4
0 2 0
1 8 3 2.67
-
1
2
2
0
0 0
0
3 3
2
0 4 0 2
16 29
0.5
0.75
1 I
5
/.8
2 I
I
6
2
2
0.6 1
5
0.4
1 5 0.2
13 2 6.5
-
tot.
5
3
3
I
0./7
0.75
pos. 13
4
2
21 40
0.525
-
14
0
0 71
60
1./8
359
THE TROCHAIC TETRAMETER TABLEXVI B MONOSYLLABLES ANDELISION monosyll. in l and 9
general prop. of elision
monosyll.
- monosyll.
Archil. Solon
9.87 14.33
0.44 0
15.93 21.5
69.23% 50%
Aesch. Soph. Eur.
9.72 6.42 7.64
0.55 1
16.54 8.22
0.92
11.33
71.11% 100% 44.29%
Aristoph. Men.
8.22 7.47
0.525 1.18
10.97 10.14
3.6.
Most of the resolutions
above).
However,
this
does
are word-initial not
hold
for
34.43% 45.04'1/o
(compare Ch.III,
each author
3.6.
and for every
to the same degree.
verse-position
As in the iambic trimeter, the tendency does not likewise apply in Aristophanes' verse (61.33% of the resolutions being word-initial) - although here again word-end preceding resolution is almost invariably more frequent than the general incidence of word-end in the verse-position in question. More remarkably, in Menander's tetrameters also the great majority of the resolutions is word-initial (80.39%), whereas in his trimeters the percentage is similar to that in Aristophanes (61.16%, cp. 63.12% in Aristophanes). Therefore in this respect also, Menander's tetrameters show a rhythmic strictness which may be considered atypical for comic verse (compare, e.g., the incidence of caesura: 3.1.1. P• 337 above). Most cases of resolution which diverge from this tendency either are
without
adjacent
4.3.1.
below)
mical
tendency
see 3.3.
above;
word-end
(esp.
or to be understood (esp.
4.3.
in
the
below).
case
in
the
as falling of resolution
comic tetrameter: in with another in
poss.
see rhyth-
3 and 5:
360
RHYTHM ANDMETRE Archil.
....,
128.5 ur,6t tv oCkCf>lwa~ ., .,v1.~li; Solon 33. 3 TtEP1.Sa,M,v 5' dyprw ~ti; Aesch. Ag. Pers •
6f4xo o(M tTttoru::,e;vut"ttl
...... 6ai.o1.JJOO nc fiA8£ OKTll"t'WC 1\ oroo1.c n6>.£1.; Soph. Phil.
l~U~/6
"CLydp; t:h.v nq:xxio1. x.4xxvmv tia'iv; I Jtvw ~ II "CCVI. T~; A.01.1,W'tLC ~ ~,, ata:71.(; rr6M:1.; Eur.
765-7 t'tfJOq)I.CJ{; ~
or.
IA
6:rt' oCHWVlq,uyd,So. ~le w w I IC51.ov 1\ K01.-vbv T'IDAL m1.c I l:n1.~ f)')Wll.D "tl; I )6n ~v f,lO\ICIV 001. 1JJ1"CJ.)6r;, 6:va:,1.ov A/:.yu11J. w
w
W
V
1363
}!
1
V
alrtoe c»t(J{;.IIJC51.0. ~. ....
1382 "tbv ·~
TU~.
W
1\ atiXX:tOOi:a.xect,Orro;
"te:lcnvtael6M:~,
f\v ~ IJdp1.c
Aristoph. Vesp.
492
C:0-CE MO.t6ti "C'OUVOU' ainf\i;I tv 6.yq::,{Jw.UAC'JO£m1.
.....
Hen. Sam. 457 ~ 1,IXYELP+);lrravu YQ4:>tanv
6f1.ov, vfi "tbv6LQ, ...... l:n1.&:l)cpxn1. • 1utvay&J uutv t;,x.ET'tH. 'ti'ic olKLQC ......
a:yat;6v. (••• ) Dysc.
770/1 l:y~
0COE1.. 5£&.MQCTT£LfXXVI tMaVfiv"to0 "tp6nou.
V¥
-
51.autvo1., UOVO'V "tOl.OOT()C. j( etc. (For resolution
¥
I 'llOAUutv
OW
~,TILN
etc. at the beginning
of a colon see also
2.3.
above.)
f't'L.
361
THE TROCHAIC TETRAMETER TABLE XVII
WORD-END PRECEDING RESOLUTION*
Archil.
Solon
Aesch.
Soph.
Eur.
Aristoph.
Men.
pos. I w.e. at 0
l00% /00%
100% /00%
100% /00'%
100°1,,
/(}()%
l00% UXJ''-';,
100% /00%
HX)'!r,, /00%
pos. 2 w.e. ar I
-
-
-
-
pos. 3 1Le. ar 2
-
-
pos. 4 w.e. at 3
-
-
-
-
pos. 5 w.e. at 4
0 6.25%
-
-
-
pos. 6 w.e. at 5
-
-
-
-
pos. 7 w.e. at 6
-
-
-
-
pos. 8 w.e. al 7
-
-
-
-
pos. 9 K'.t'. at 8 pos. JO w.e. at 9
l00% 93.75%
-
-
0 2!(03%
100%, 96.2/ 0/r,
-
-
-
100% /00%
-
44.44% 25.31%
50% 14.81%
71.43% 36.42%
98.28% 98.15%
-
9.(19% 24.67''/o
60°/4, 28.33%
50% 32%
82.35% 73.67% 0
68% 29.69%
94.74% 25.71%
27.78% 28.21%
100% 95.3%
-
8.33%
pos. 11 100% w.e. al JO 25%
-
pos. 12 w.e. at II
-
pos. 13
-
-
80%
l(Xl%
0 4(),9/%
-
-
!00% 23.53%
-
88.89'3/o 37.96%
-
31.25% 28%
-
62.07% 3/.66'fo 0
43.89%
-
-
-
w.e. at 12 tot.
-
* See explanation
84.21%
ad Ch.III,
l(Xl%
89.21%
Table XXII above.
66.67% 24%
80% 30.41%
61.33%
80.93%
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
362 4.
In the
the
tetrameter,
again,
verse-positions
complementary adjoining)
to
the
comparative
bridges
which applying
In
infrequency
obviously
high
to
incidence
of word-end
be
accounted
of word-end
at
some of
for
as
in other
being
(i.e.
the
verse-positions.
The
their
is
the
the
avoiding
universality
regard
the
in the
to
rhythmical
effect
metric
of
those
and in the
produced
in is
(i.e.,
iambic
essentially
from
alike. occur
movement)
the
those
appears
trimeter
bridges
rhythmical
observed
"bridges
verse-elements)
some rhythr.lical
disfavoured
correspond
the
identity
tetrameter
tetrameter, some
of
(sc.
which
trimeter,
bridges seemingly
whereas
different
(compare
the 3.4.
above). For example, in both types of verse word-end at pos. 4 occurs infrequently, dihaeresis after the first metron being avoided; the rhythmical effect in both cases however is quite different: in the iambic trimeter, the phenomenon occurs comparatively seldom because it is in disagreement with the preferred variation of the rhythmical movement near the middle of the verse (penthemimeral caesura); in the trochaic tetrameter, because the repetition of the rhythmical movement which is characteristic of the caesura in this type of verse, is avoided within the colon. 4.1.
was to be expected,
As
adjoining
the
exceptions lable:
see
to
caesura this
3.5 with
word-end
(sc. tendency
Table
at
both
7 & 9)
poss. almost
of
the
occurs
invariably
verse-positions
infrequently. involve
a
The monosyl-
XVI above.
Only in Aristophanes a number of tetrameters show word-end at poss. 7 or 9 without at the same time having a caesura (90.91% and 68% of the occurrences of word-end at these verse-positions respectively) - as may be deduced from the comparative frequency of verses without a caesura (Table XIII) and the comparative scarceness of monosyllabic words (3.5. above). 4.2. is
In the the
case
trochaic in the
tetrameter iambic
trimeter:
split
resolution
see Ch.III,
is 4.2.
even
rarer
than
above.
Only in Aristophanes' tetrameters the percentage is somewhat higher (2.67% as against 1.6% in the trimeter); note, however, that the tendency does apply even in the case of Aristophanes if monois a notable scarceness in his tetrasyllabic words (of which there meters) are excluded: 1.33% and 1.52% respectively.
THE TROCHAIC TETRAMETER Except for split (i.e.
one instance
resolution articles
(sc.
in Aristophanes,
within
or pronouns
The one exception
all
the occurrences
sample)
are
monosyllabic
by elided
6t),
our
followed
in Aristophanes
of words
is
... ...
-rovnaTtp'
504:
vesp.
363
l6n ~1.
-couu.,vdm.Udx&:vm
w
- which occurs in a context where the author is making a mockery of rhythm in various ways: see verse 505 cited sub 3.2. above. Monosyllabic Aesch. Eur.
occurrences... are:... Pers.
897
IA
218 in 6' I c1yda' b.lav
fMuov
OE,
T£
HO.tl'~
Tb0 1 flDV ex,~~
ot&:v
1
Aristoph. Vesp.
Hen.
Sdm.
Dysc.
451
~£
...
OE
..,
cr\0.1.;d_,o•I &x_dp1.cnoe"°6' &xl
Cn~
592 OU6' tn'
t~
13aKTI¥>lav .., 7 27 naTpl \.iM.1.cn1.ato8' , 6:>cc1.6Cl -div lJJlT~ y\J\,'Cl(l«l ~I.C
..,
fxwv
On the
whole,
the
very
small;
still,
total
numbers have been listed
for
number of occurrences the
sake
of
in Tables
of split
completeness,
resolution percentages
XVIII and XVIII A below.
is and
364
RHYTHM ANDMETRE TABLEXVIII
SPLIT RESOLUTION: PERCENTAGES(&/-monosyllabic
pos. 1· - el.
pos. 3
words)•
Aesch. & mono- - monosyll. syll.
Eur. & mono- - monosyll. syll.
Aristoph. Men. & mono- - mono- & mono- - monosyll. syll. syll. syll.
11.11% 0
0
0
()
0
0
2.86% 0
0
0
()
0
9.()9%
9.09% 0
0
()
()
0
• el.
pos. 5 • el.
-
-
()
()
()
0
0
0
pos. 7
-
-
0
0
()
0
0
0
pos. 9 • el.
0
0
1.72% 0
0
5.88% 0
0
0
0
pos. 10
-
-
-
-
0
(I
-
-
pos. 11 . el.
0
0
()
0
0
0
6.9%
0
pos. 12
-
-
-
-
-
-
0
()
pos. 13
-
-
-
-
0
0
0
0
tot.•• • el.
5.26% 0
()
0.72% 0
0
2.67% 0
1.33%
1.57% 0
0
0
• el.
0
* Percentage ** Percentage •
of resolutions in the relevant verse-position of the sum total of resolutions ; monosyllable in the sense adopted in the present study, since the words (or combinations of words) in this case do not occupy a full verse-position.
N.B. No occurrences
in Archilochus,
Solon,
& Sophocles.
THE TROCHAIC TETRAMETER
365 TABLE XVIII A
SPLIT RESOLUTION: TOTALNUMBERS (&ratio-/+
elision)
Aesch.
Eur.
Aristoph.
Men.
po!>.1
0 1 0
0 0
0 0
0 2 0
pos. 3
0 0
()
0
0 1 0
()
()
pos. 5
()
0
()
()
0 0
()
0
pos. 7
0
0
0 0
0 0
0 0
pos. 9
0
0
0
1
()
0
1
0
()
0
pos. 10
- -
- -
()
()
0
()
pos. 11
()
0
0 0
0
0
()
2
0 pos. 12
- -
- -
0
pos. 13
0
0
0 0
0 0
0 0
tot.
0 1 0
0 1 0
0 2 0
0 4 0
monosyll.
0 I 0
0 1 0
0
0 4 0
0
()
- monosyll.
0
0
0
I
0
0 1 0
0
()
()
()
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
366
4. 3.
In
early
contrast
history
word-end same
time
trochaic
(see
general
4.3.
a
resolution a
720
~Q:Xl,•
733
~ ~~,I
-
i:l
such
is is
of the
appear
to fall
tendency
of marking
the
in
particular:
word-end
at
one
more
may judge
favoured
in
rhythmical
that
occurrences
in
frequently
from
so
small
the
case
than
is
a sample,
of the that
6>..\.' Ca1. cntvnc; b
~-
invariably
so
of four
i:nv Mv: I Jty ~
x~ following found
at
order
to
movement,
a resolution
(sc.
verse-positions bring
i.e.
at
where
about poss.
monosyl-
the
word-
preferred
3 & 5.
3 64
387
l«J.t,:6 ~,
886
1349
ou6t
~
,:~
IfxE1.v. TTOVl1POU ~
~x~I
A.£AE)(,:a.1.HO.tc,aqii
f)6c:M:it
l«KlL
HO.tbQ.61.a.
OO'ta;~' I~1.,
tc ~I
conformity in
involved
1s
word-end
400 -caO-ai 001. .......
meter
excluded).
":!16' tc fp:v~tEa,t., t£vt~, ~lq. ,:' dy£1.c: ~1. roeItai> 'tE ~ anriv1.oe. &.q:n 6' oo xs:,d.i,v .., ... &i; q;ov€U{;OUH.t,: 1.I8uya.,:p6c oneOO'J.id,\.und YE
319 345
In
the
(the
monosyllables,
vo.:>, I ti:J.v~t.
also,
excepted)
IA
cases
of a total
..
In Euripides
regulation
if
two out
a;\.1.c ...ltv' ... tfl'lx£1.c
1405
as
words
at
Iltp:n1., ~ln1.cm:ui"t0. l6t.TtA00v utU1110V ~ CU:,(V c U:1p:10£-ca6rov, i:x:n:£ -~-cdy~ ou5' dv El,
v0v 5' ttr.J ut("~Q:>lav ~ .. MnL m ......01.1""1 '-"I Al ' fi iio1. HP£ Cnov ~.. l"O,... TQ:d:rrav To0 nnTPQC
4 9 6 / 7 frv 6t yfiTE LOV TTPXXlL Tf.l me,~
fl
- ...
AO.X.~LC
~ 't1.l,101..'tl ., .,
462 "COUC vduouwdvapE:c, ., ., >0.i:an~
~I.
-div xCMnv.ll'p:1. ot.
558
l~ t:v&:Yvk(UtN:Jf3ov ., ...rliv fuxu'tou fuya'tfp:J. &toe:v, t~l(l)Vr'K; >0.ttTtE:OEV. I huxbv Ccu,i;ll,oft
565
llXlXUC ~,
au
5 56 6'),:
....
O)(Q"tOqnyO{;,~
... ""
622 rr,::xS.n&1Xl.L • ~ Tt&PLTD't&t ~6 98
1Jl'iEtrii:' , dvapE:c,>0.-routve .....
740 &tx&e, Dysc.
7 36
wruii:e:p, q>1.~ ....,
i:d.Ua rrocin'
alrtoc.,~•
-a,,'t~
OUKdv ~ ..
u,aELctq. &>n.l ht4JIXlY& oh au lJOL.
1.v, 6.U.' 6ncpy
'VOOvfX&I.C d.JVt'OLC &ot'c
....
But compare also Sam.
001. •
uµ.tv b...~ 6~ -c' ta"ta1. YC+XiN >Cl-CO. ~( Jt;v&:,v 'llEPLU&V&LC, oo YoQ;() OUK&~6:>x~L fv6o8Ev. () l,U.lqXN 6LWl.1.~ ~ TOLVUV a' t:vw
747 tK~ 782/3
..
t:vwMPL\.ti) -ea.'toi.aOa'
529 ((1!Jvov
...
-
6:rnc,.tT'llVCIJ"'t'OC TtOEC
rliv.... ~'tEP' &>n rl)v Mv w TD1.6l 734 of'p:,v, t'H.ET&ui.>.( I qi.\uJpe:tcnp6c l-1' f'IJJN. o. Cl)
0 C
0
< z :..l
...l =:i
lov 499 t\ voulCELCIme;•Aar'iW(; oot .6u::Jou' t~· I"CL~ fxo1.u'dv 4Mo TTD1.£tv; ~lwv vesp.
448
456-8
Dysc.
~ ... !"CLCb ~;
o(; OLCXKOVr'Jc,ETE
-tQ J,.D.YEL~;
lmwYoPOO"'CL\I dct.ov,
btt.~I.
• 11,.1£ya~ WL\I
vfi W'V6La,
1
d'VO.l.l>l.a'i(lli)"tt CTlTOUV'ttiJV btl n.\tov. -rotnlo ~ ~ 01Mdve:u61-~ nvq;. Theophr. Net. 8 b 10-17. tv£cva,C01Jtvr,,6t. ' ~,
1. In Chapters scription them
of
the
conceived
expression. the
II-IV
verse, general
chapter
2. Rhythmically Considering with
sense
some
understanding
the
the
of
though
tain
that
full
oore
than
oere
tors some
specific
of
rhythmical
by summarizing
types
different form
verse
each
of
study
these
Greek
of
1t
surprising
is
not
the
subject
a more
of
is
light
the
of
different at
the
base
is
some
rhythm
of
of
the
made in
the
essentially
that
involves
a and
no denying
nature
the
rhythm
spiritually
appreciation
are
of
verse,
means
this
more
phenomenon
there
which
stichic
de-
Contrast
involved,
contrdst
a
nature
perception
starting-point
conclude cotm:1on to
sense
upon
Greek
coherent
(Ch.I).
perception
Even
to
in
Functional that
of
a maximally
satisfying
characteristics
on
offer
aesthetically
recapitulating
introductory
to
common types
an
statements
trast.
tried
may be useful
salient thus
most as
It
most
we have
verse types of all
the
attempt
several
kinds
inclined poetic
that rhythm of
critic
text
analysis
connected
of
anyone does
main-
something
who wants well
con-
dii ferent
to take
rhythmically
rhythmical
of
might
requires the
to gain
experience.
fac-
to
shed
as his
functiondl
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
380
Contrast
2.1.
Constitutive
As may be
inferred
Chapter
1.2.
I,
of contra&t,
order
vs.
absence
the
the
definition
nature
(2.1.1)
namely
Sequence of
As Such
rhythc
formulated
in
involves
two
of
rhythm
itself
marked
vs.
non-marked
and (2.2.2)
of order.
Marked Vs. Non-Marked
A rhythoical
sequence
non-marked evant
events.
2.1.2.
event
absence contrast
recurrence
of classical
Greek,
syllable, versus
(in
non-marked
this minus
order.
between
varyin~
events
of a gradual of
the
of plus
involved
and
time
rhythm
by the
of marked and where
contrast
the
rel-
consists
marked for
in a
quantity.
Vs. Absence of Order
regularity
this
constituted
is
opposition
Order
marked
is
In the
rhythmical
disjunctive
The
from
above,
types
2 .1.1.
of Rhythoical
implies
nature-,
As far
degrees)
in
a second
which is that
as
prose
a prosodically
rhythm
occasional
occurrence
of
ally
preferred
sequences
(usually
of
between
is
quantitatively
recurrence
kind
order
end
of
considered
is
a
one hand and
well-shaped,
the
-
and the
there
on the
at
of
contrast
concerned
random sequence
the
the
tradition-
the
sentence)
on
the other. In other
metrical end of
verse, the
scale
contrast
exists
relevant
verse-profile
marked of
thus
rhytht1
that
half
indifferent; consist with
runs
from randomness
between
as
in
the
a potential its
as
lyrics
in a general
fixed) while
kind 1
where
the
than
there
of
degrees concerned prose
in the
lat-
(comparatively) order
seems to
number of successive is
is
a
and the groups
is
only is
there
such
in common with
half
on the 19 7
of
contrast order
former
restriction
random sequence
of
the
to order,
intermediate
has
''
of Greek draca,
verse-elements
are
rhythmical the
constitute
recurrence
There
this
to
random sequence
regular
Wilamowitzianus
sequence,
and non-marked
a potential
far
has a (Le.
the
mainly
marked
which
so-called
is
of
he
verse-elements.
order
the
can
with
and non-marked
rhythmical
too: ter
rather
which
less
of a contrast
in metrical
verse
in
381
CONCLUSIOti
virtue
of the
2.2.
fact
Rhythmic
Considering degree the
that
Realization that
of
the
a rhythmical
on
part
r.1ore specific
according
rhythnic
this
will
level
be,
be
event
conceived
sucietiI:K?s ticular
as
only
its
to
the
degree
an
tially
with
the
involved
(2.2.1), the
the
of the
single-short
chaic by
markedness
type
one
non-narked
of
verse
Harked
the
is
now and
traction')."'
It
seems
contrast short
occurs, vs.
In
namely
single
·short
trochaic
tetraoeter
marked
verse-position
the
rhythm,
position
as
the
would
do;
contrast an
paras
is
an
essen-
expectation,
it
tension.'''
which
the
in
the of
contrast the
contrast
events
only
re-
and
tro-
(2.2.2).
of
the
iambic is
hexameter realized
(1) marked
vs.
trimeter
occasionally in
realized
the
double-short
a non-marked, by one marked
to be significant
that
in both
non-marked
and
double-short syllable
('con-
cases a double
(2) one/single-
200
like
occurrence does
occurrence in
of
('resolution');
sequence the
appears
markedness
which
verse-element
then
two/double-short.
a
in
verse
dactylic
verse-element
is
which
Vs. Non-Marked
of
marked
norm,
verse
in
the may
naMe of rhythmical
the
the
Such a contrast
but
of
also
At
between
rhythm,
of
kind
cases
cases
syllables
of
this
those
events
types
tetrameter two
mostly
those
One Vs. 1\.lo plus
2.2.1. In
and
and
type
to by the
between
number,
the
(non-)fulfilment
be referred
concerns
of
be
frequently
a contrast
the rhythmical
Since
the
One may distinguish
gards
from
less
of
will
involved.
language.
a particular
divergence.
connected
in
expectation
order
may be
a certain
expectation
and more or
variation
in
institutionalized
may conveniently
there
occasional
verse-positions
of
may be,
divergence
a
This
realization
manner.
involves
a certain
listener.
is,
linear
be always
the
that
and
invariably
of
expectation
thwarted:
expected
will
in a core
Element
sequence
there
the
proceeds
Vs. Metric
regularity,
pat tern
order
not
of appear
of one
a double-short
the
iambic
trimeter
two, non-marked to
affect
non-marked sequence
the
syllables
the at a
perception
syllable like
and
in the
such
dactylic
of a
382
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
hexameter
the
occurrence
verse-position the
is
basic
short
rhythm,
as
verse-position In
the
that
to
the
(•-w)
one
would
be
tion
are
why,
ning
of
degree
that
is:
of
see vs.
marked
nature). the
in
the
no
since
and
is on
this
as
an abandoning
as
regards
dactylic and
sarily
imply
the
tetrameters
20I
the
present tolerance
of
restriction
the
the
the
the
occurs
high
iambic
trimeter
(outside
comedy,
we would
have
a contrast
marked
and
non-marked
syllables
for
being
the
majority
analysis
in
of
there
a non-
as,
rhythmic
occadiver-
of)
the
non-marked
terms
of
rhythmical
may be conveniently that
begin-
a rhythmical
of comedy as well
(the
ex-
should
characterized be a contrast
as well.
from
marked
This
at
of
of
since
comparatively
of
toler-ance
to
se-
contrac-
concerned.
beginning
the
of
nature and
element
between and
increased
the
hexameter,
double
contrast
alternative,
divergence,
the
resolution
extended
the
markedness
As appears
with
basic
recognizability
is
resolution
verse-positions
resolution
contrast
no
choliambus
verse-positions;
the
tetrameter
trimeters
sionally, of
verse-positions
the
rhythmic
the
fact
the
double-short
anceps
at
the
in
resolution
(exemplifying
be observed
but
the
the
the
in a double-short
in markedness of
then,
in a single-short
the
the
of
as
(a)
of
non-marked
one,
(both
forms
and
consist encumbers
(b)
with
phenomena
trimeter
below),
besides
gence
the
to
syllable
variance
double-
contraction,
with
of
syllable.
other
and
perception
non-marked,
in as nuch
one,
exception
to
the
variance
the
of
a non-marked
the
the
seems
for
harmless
the
trochaic
two
In
be at
iambic
in
uu
non-marked at
if
and
degree,
number
tolerance
sequence), the
with
the
of
and
in
case
and
in
with
by one short
a marked
comparatively both
the
one
Accordingly,
contrast
plains
as would
rhythm.
the
of
verse-position
quence
-
possible
occurrence
interfere
resolution of
least
(•J.!)
be
syllable
marked
realized
of
of
the
sequence
the
to
'equivalence'
rhythm
this
likely would
both
substitution
sc.
one,
were
case
supposed
a
not
of
vs. actual
irresolvability the
of
double
non-marked occurrence
the
contrast
does of
not the
marked
elements
one/single
on the potential
other
vs. hand
of two/
neces-
divergence
in
383
CONCLUSION
question. gence it
As was as
such
does
may be argued
element the
in
clarity
of
compensated
of
metron
Or,
of
(one
long, seems
to
be the
the
phenomenon -
resolution
described
As far cerned
guished: other
the
above,
explanation
a marked
syllable
to
any
to which
be
restriction the
contrast
the
dactylic
remark-
which,
the
two
the
putative
ancient
in
the
all
in
the the
accords
of
more
the
fact verse-
contraction to
and
be allowed
restriction
pure
the
relevant
principle,
only
rhythm
with
the
phenooena
seem,
in a non-marked
rhythmically
case
allowed
keeping
types
of
the
one
Cholosis
(cholosis)
choliambus, realized
discussed cases
single-short
being
towards
the
in
which as
a
this
the
study
a marked
the
end of
of
the
of rhythmic
the lonr,
last syllable,
are
syllable may be
institutionalized
feature status
of
in
verse-position
an
a structural
2.2.2.1.
always
verse
different
non-marked,
transcending
nearly
of
of
below).
one of
that
it
:J
(anceps)
tlic
fact
opposite
verse-positions,
actually
In
in
not
towards
as
a
only
profile
an
bound
contrast
two essentially at
of is
is
in
relevant 20
colon.
ring
this
-
the
if
observance
2.3.1.
again,
the
only
the
also
to
endanger
Only
supposition
positions,
see
metric
very
occurrence
This
tendency
the
tolerable as
marked
the
of 6Aoy,a..
significant.
the
with
Non-Marked
then,
with
of
Still
would
(such
divergence
double-short)
in terms
this
rhythmic
given
is
tendency
connected
verse-position
the
which
contrast
single-short
general
a degree
diver-
explanation.
contraction
rhythmical
terms
Marked vs.
all
of
of
interpretation
in
resolution
in
enough,
that
of
verse:
already
Apparently
to
a rhythmic
an
anapaestic
is
2.2.2.
occurrence
regulating
1!14Y be
hexar1eter
the
of
require
in
actualization
one/two
possibility
rhythm
absence
principle
the
by some
possibilities
ably
to
the
dihaeresis
speaking the
in
that
the
°2
above,2
not
addition
is
of
said
conoccur-
distin-
divergence, metric
profile
the itself,
divergence.
non-marked the
verse-elenent tension
between
is the
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
384 rhythmic
realization
and
the
tionalized
and
to
serve
is
seems
apparently
just
strengthened
before
general
the
end
tendency
a comparatively 2.2.2.2. In
pure
10 5
as
every
this
the
basic
Consequently
()(-u-
if
an
is
a rhythmic
f ile
which
involves
hand
and
the
syllable,
ment,
not
does form;
in
marked
contrast
verse
is
effect occurs to
the
characterized
is
anceps
element,
In
sense
by
of
a
by
the
metric
name of
kind
-
that
anceps with
its
of
contrast
by
makes
in
on the
on
manifest these
simple
grouping.
an
other. ele-
in
com-
its
in an anceps
iambic
of
the
the
structuring of
verse.
trimeter
a structural
from
one
anceps
types
and
feature,
phenomenon
of
possibility'
non'"11!arked verse-element involves
the
the
'institutionalized
itself
syllable
realization
as
such
-u, -u).
syllable
profile
the
four
verse-pro-
occurring
of
so
'simple'
of
metric
syllable
the
of
a long
the
as
although
groups
as
basic,
in
be perceived
realized
occurring
Group and Hetric
to
verse
u- resp.
characteristic
the
tends
realization
it
non-marked
u-.
may be conceived
of
the
characteristic
distinguished
realization
Rhythmic
most
of
verse-element
from
a long
is
tetrameter
profile
Another
diverge
tro-
As was maintained
the marked
a proper
ancipitia
divergence
long
between
the
the
than
the
single-short
the
which
carefully
from
contrary,
the
trochaic
rhythmic
is
and
of
structured
elet1ent
a tension
being
which
rather
considering
elements
is
second
profile
-v-x
divergence
actually
by four
be
anceps
the
the
each
trimeter
syllable.
sequence
resp.
however
on
position
2.3.
phenomenon
every
affects
non-marked
The marked
to
the
iambic
as a long
elements
there
the
This
that
the
the
c.q.
single-short
two
rather
this
first,
possibility
of
elements
of
may be realized
groups
plex
of
institu-
realization.
sequences
tetrameter
verse-elements
in
this
clausula
rhythAic
single-short
chaic
far
the
verse,
104
purpose.
fact
is
Anceps
the
above
the
verse-element
a stylistic
by the
of
that
underlying
a divergence
referred from
the
to basic
Group
operative
in most
types
of
Greek
verse
is
385
CONCLUSION
that
between
(group two
of
a
rhythmic
verse-elements).
essentially
of composition
2.3.l
Rhythmic
one
hand.
In
metra, and
others
cidence or
the
these
coincidence
units
and
involved,
namely
i.a.,
on the
depends.
of
composition the
and a
boundary
seems
is
to have
to
on the other.
between
two
evidently
favoured,
speaking,
the
effect
the
absence
an
of
on the
the
produce
the
concerned)
boundary
Generally
clarity');
seems
is
phrases)
of
(i.e.,
building-material
study
(words
avoided.
('rhythmical
tendencies
the
iambic
complete each the
as
regards
regular
this
near
units
lapping apart
of
coin-
separation of
such
integration
of
sequence wards
does
the
a the
continues
(caesura)
coincide
with
where
coincidence
of
trimeter
also
coincidence
a return
group: to the
not initial
is
word-boundary
only
the
of
is
ends.
and
movement.
fact
of this
also
two over-
falling
the
rhythmic
other
hand,
by means
the
that
from
metron-boundary with
posi-
between
that the
of
a manner
boundary
On
word-end 207
such
accomplished
li:i-v--llbut
preferred
prevented
the
are
by virtue
The effect
of
unit
effect
regular
cetric
virtue
a metric
a clausular
by the
metra).
two in
(i.e.,
sequences
parts of
206
sequence
in
These
one
verse
(or
this
at
hexameter
sequences
two constituent
rhythmical
two halves,
verse-end
not
composition
that
acatalectic
word-end the
dactylic
structure).
into
of
by the
are
metrical
of
middle
of
is into
their
occurrence the
illustrated
which
structured
word-boundary
metric
may be
trimeter,
internally
tions
is
the
rather. These
basic
groups
as
coincidence
be
('overlap')
group
be made between
verse
present
a rhythmic
to
a metric
of Cocposition
units
the
boundaries
distinctness
Unit
as
and
metric
be regarded
the
appears
1t
of
verse
far
and
should
of
metric
rhythmic
Kroupa
phrase)
of perception.
to
circumstances
metric
in
the
as
or
an individual
verse-elements
some
such
of
between
verse:
units
and
quality
relationship
the
kinds
Group and Metric
The rhythmical
of
(word
A distinction
different
units
groups
group
of (in
boundary
v-lu-JI).so
to-
that
the the of a
there
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
386
In
the
plete
trochaic
internal
reverse
responsion
seems
to
metron-boundary same
apply. does
rhythmical
in
this
('fixed' to
between
its
ated
by
both
the
initial
-
and
so
that
non-marked
of
the
with
movement of both In non-metrical
ceived complete
the
between here metric tion
the
verse
as is
for
of
the
boundary
tendency
of
of
the
be
only
to
type
of
one
initial shows
an
to
observed
towards
the
from
a rhythmically
after
is
the
the
boundary 1
e •8• --uu--v-v--luu-u--u--) between
rhythmic
very
is
fact
that
named after
0
'
•
and
situa-
the more significant
the
that
may be con-
an integration
element
an overlap
verse
the
contrasts
into
such
be rhythmically
inferred
word-end
colon-internal
which
verse
groups ('dovetailing':
seems
a falling
of
to
may be
verse-elements
such
movement:
201
cola. a
to
compens-
tetrameter
which
fact
integration
have
a
the
be used
incidence
clausula
the
were,
of which the
the
position
rhythmical
producing
word-end
of
it
returning
also
of
absence
might the
(both
to
of
as
the
One way of accomplishing
boundary -
of
be,
and
have
caesura
lack
of
building-t:taterial
two such metric also
to
the
between
could
Now this seems
cola
extends one
incom-
2 o1
colon-
integration
instead
groups
occurrence
the
'asynartete'
colon
of the
metric
whole.
(recurrent)
term
metric
verse
the
as
the
is
catalexis)
of
only
is
verse-element,
verse,
rhythmic
integrating
tetrameter
there
between
both
the
there
Consequently,
the
of
that
of
of
second
of
a contrast
structuring.
the
which
effect
show a preference
movement.
initial
verse
in
absence
variation
a
overlap
The
colon-internal
first
movement
so
parts
movement)
rising
occur,
two constituent
a
preceding
case
of
internal
the
In this
or cola
type
caesura)
refer
the
not
parts
however,
(with
movement.
two constituent that
tetrameter,
asynartetic
its
colometric
structure. Even which
more
there
ary,
as
tion
seems
freedom structure
is
there
of
to
significantly, a is
regular
find
a
safeguarded
other
coincidence
in anapaests
responsion is
in
specific demands in
types of metric
and dochmiacs, explanation that another
of
the
metrical and
this in
rhythmic lack
that
distinctness way.
verse
Presumably,
of
their of
their the
in
boundintegraextreme metric combina-
CONCLUSION tion
of
the
387
frequent
markedness
of
tion
the
with
obscure 2.3.2.
the
the
clarity
the
rhythm
with
Unit
two
the
rhythmic
of
the
the
metric
realiza-
~roups
would
degree.
unit
constitute perception
be
in
(colon):
for
being
considered
described
groups
those
rhythmical
--uu-uu-)
may
units
also
the
sequence
hexameter to
quantity/
rhythmic
as a metric
a unit (i.e.
the
of Perception
usually
dactylic
between
of their
and
we may consider
In contrast
boundary
that
between
to an intolerable
which
the
tension
metric
essentially
is
of
colon'.
coincident
the
"penthemimer"
colon
'metric
2 I I
above,
which
instance,
the
of the
verse-elements
sub-phrase
first
of
a
and
Group and Metric
As was argued of
of
verse-elements
overlap
Rhythmic
groups
occurrence
2.3.1.
sub
this
sense
is
the as
a
above, usually
a word-boundary.
It should be stressed however that the situation is complicated somewhat by the fact that in other rhythmical genres such metric groups tend to serve as units of composition, as the penthemimer does e.g. in dactylo-epitritic verse, and as many of the aeolic cola do in the choral lyrics of Greek drama. In this case these groups fall under 2.3.l. above; here we are concerned only with those stretches of the metric profile which usually serve as units of perception. Here
again
two such mic
tl~re
metric
cola
realization
rence
of
of
of
contrast
position(s)
and
between
the
expected
and the
and the
which
Contrast
Another
of
is
not
study, trast
consist
(see
boundary
2I 2
trimeter, in
2.2.
positions):
contrast strictly
is
one
based
example
the
above)) one
that
actual
rhyth-
is
occur-
the
the
rhythmical
rhythmically between
position is,
between
func-
the
further
a tension
caesura on (or,
between
the
caesura.
however, what
the most obvious
to
rhythmic
boundary
by the
seems
the
the
indicated
iambic
tension
3. Rhetorical
boundary
the
(or:
actual
between
in
two caesura
type
the
verse;
media'
'causura
significance tional
may be an overlap
operative speaking,
may call
on word order.
in
Greek
within
rhetorical
rhythmical the
contrast,
scope
texts, of
which
the is
which present a con-
RHYTHU AND METRE
388 The phenomenon criterion
for
sense
from
confusing which
that
and devised
such
a
based
by
the
text
(there
mical'
is
not
which
order used
auditory
stimuli
The
phenomenon speaking
relevant
to
the
was argued
as a kind
of
One of of
ferred
'rhythmical
syntactical the
completeness
of
incompleteness
to
ferring
trimeter
the
a
essential
a
term
'rhyth-
the
level
of
the
appears
to
all
same:
phenomena
not
the
be
may be conceived
214
of
investigation
situation
that
one
in
there
the
is,
the of
the
rhetorical to
the
the
it
one
2
a
colon
cl
de-
between
the
and
the
'!
syntactically/
penthemimeral metric
phenom-
were,
hand
other.
enphasis next
as
a contrast
of
the
a word with
is
localization
after
is
which
verse
on the
word
such, rhythm
utterance
word
in
as
linguistic
of
ii
although
on
kind
that
I
however,
these
( 'enjambement
create
word
- a designation
at
of
with
is
'random'
be a contrast
the
word
sequence
significant
iambic
2.1),
the
in
operative
verse
the
so
or
same time
rhythmical
phenomenon
semantically
seems
the of the
A similar
the
verse,
of
somewhat texts
however
Greek
connection
next
different
'conscious'
of
objects is
the
theory
stylistics'.
that
a
contrast
contrast
study
more obvious
in
the
common
213
rhythmical
the
case,
of a contrast
interpretation above,
the
to
of
a
in
be noted
sense
as
non-poetical
to above,
strict
I
'natural'
any
at
rhetorical
part
as
only
course
occurrence
of
enjambement,
(strong)
the
to be conditional.
strictly
enon
the
especially
1
in
referred
the
(but
should
is
as
in
we take
or, It
may of
prose
a supposedly
1
contrast
instance
prose
between
author.
for
I
2.1)
rhythmical
'artificial'
on syllabic
for
of
rhetorical
particular
above,
occurs an
serves
rhythmical
defined
a contrast
order
to
identifying
conception
phrasing
as
referred
caesura 2I I
colon') by as
it
in
, which were
( 'rhythmical
de-
focus-
ing I)• Finally
such
I
grammatically the
the
to
localization
11 & 12 and iambic
devices fill of
pos(s). trimeter,
as
the
separation
rhythmically a noun 6 (& creating
and 7)
effective the
of
words
corresponding
verse-positions
corresponding
respectively
which
a contrast
between
pronoun
(e.g. in
poss.
may be observed rhythmical
1
conti-
in
389
CONCLUSION nuity
and
to the
syntactic
domain
217
discontinuity)
be reckoned
may also
to
belong
stylistics.
of rhythmical
4. Bridge The
notion
of
classify the
the
different
present
study;
fundamental one
4.1.
and
Metric
bridge
lative
two
short
in
avoided
single
verse-element
refers
bridge
is
a
'metric'
class
of
the
also
to
course
of
above
the
bridge
most
on
'rhythmical'
the
bridges
the
- as in in
linked, name
or
the
by
that
preservation
a single
of
is
between
the
belonging
The
term
particular the
being
limits,
(syllables
this
is
,
which,
contrast
word-groups).
characteristic
Bridge:
metric type
identity
of
of
the
bridge
and
4.1.
that
case
whereas split
occurs.
the
between to
two
avoidance resolution
Another
rhythmic rhythmic
be the
However, the
two kinds
between
found
above.
this
involves
that
was also
of
Complex
i.e.
(2.2)
cussed
(words
between
rhythmical
additional
•
of a cumu-
contrast
syllables,
realization
21
verse-element
tension
two
of word-
concerned.
contrast,
element
its
by
a
certain an
avoidance
of a double
by
is
the one
involves
there
groups
rhythmical
tional
terms
within
and
motivated
Rhythmical
Complex
i.e.,
realization
when
to
verse-element
actually
3.2.4.
filling
in
itself
rhythmic
bridge
syllables
its
strictly
the
in
Ch.I,
between
resolution,
avoidance
and
unobjectionable
two
split
the
verse-element
that
met with
in
comprehensive
expressed
nature:
(2.3)
that
is
of
moBt conveniently
4.2.
argued
more
'bridge'
may serve
Bridge
between
to)
was
contrast
other.
The cetric end
of
as
the
functional
types
distinction
hand
on the
rhythmically
realization
in
a fundamental of
of
the
applies that
-
seems not
disto
be
essentially
referred
only
group
bridge
difference
word-boundary
is
metric
metric
are
func-
and metric
and
the
contrast
naturally difference
rhythmically
group
case
kinds
of
if
depending
to
by
resolution on
the
RHYTHM Alm METRE
390 rhythmical
circumstances
may be just
as relevant
We shall bridge,
involved as the
distinguish
namely
those
(1)
two
bridges
in
is
only
on
that
well
(in
which
situation
case
interferes
bridges self
condition
in
which
the
situations,
as
resolution: 4.2.1.
category
bridges
which
consist
after
a
marked
which
is
is
avoided
is
rhythmical
situation
or
even
the
in
the
fact ) and
occurrence at
in
not
the
4.2.1
the
of
the
of
metric
but
apply
as
that
one
(2)
those
two,
same time
effect
cumulative
also
favoured,
should
consists
situations
case
trimeter
and
(i.e.,
the
the
there
is
and
of
not
the
at
seems
syllable
jectionability and metric of
the
sequence.
it-
in (so
that
two rhythmical
bridge
of
split
case
the
the
of,
or
of
situation
the
there 220
of the
is
tension
the
element) for,
because
and
therefore
avoided
pos.
4 in the
trochaic
similar,
but,in
of
the
is
the
(long) this
only
if
double-short
syllable. kind
The
of contrast
with
situation the
met-
but
-,
a coincidence
at
iambic
unobjectionable
221
interferes
this
of
(constituent)
between one
a variation
metron-dihaeresis
),
verse
occurrence
hexameter of word-end
(like
favoured
by
preference
presumably
is
incidence
position
actually the
those
4 element, in poss. l n t hi s type o f verse,
which
within
realization
double-short
the
boundaries
same time
vs.
place,
tetrameter
is
to be that
verse-end
In the
the
of
caesura
groups)
its
boundary,
istic
the
8 it
the
in
trochaic of
pos.
and
motivation
at
first
a double-short .,.""VV-7 _JI) • 2 II ...
sequence
rhythmic of
the
avoidance
"' ' ' ( -uu---:u:u-
unlike
case
in
the
movement
verse-element
-v-/1) 222
in
coincidence
groups in
belong,
an acatalectic
rhythmical
(one
situation:
realization
an d 8 espec i a 11 y
-
one
Interfering first
ric
rhythmical
4.2.2).
To the
the
another
in
a coincidence
complex
situation
with
the
was
which
complexity
consists
of
unobjectionable
rhythmical
complexity
types
another
of
of contrast.
the
what
non-preferred
occurrence
occurrence
between
(coincidence/contrast) the
- the
the of
is
other
unob-
rhythmic
character-
positions
tetrameter
(-v-~l-v-u
one respect
at
least,
in
re-
CONCLUSION versed:
391
the
marked
tension
verse-element
establishes
the
sequence, same metron
coincidence
between
the
quite
in
order,
So as
within
(basic)
does not
realization
unobjectionable tetrameter a
is
or the
characteristic
and
cola
(and,
the
is
rhythmic
it
of
the
at
the
not
boundary
of
its
non-
since
beginning
there
if
themselves. and
the
(1 .e.,
the
caesura,
So here
the
rhythmic
and
tension
realization
is
the
coincidence
of word-boundary
the
interfering
factor
reverse:
in
of
the
the
the
verse-element
even
word
quite
is
role
of
of
and
interfere.
the
two bridges
at
but only
verse-element
regards
1 n these
elements
metric
that
realization
unobjectionable
of
the
provided
metron-boundary
gent
which
avoided
is
favoured),
dihaeresis),
accordingly
marked
by four
slightly
the
the
(anceps)
grouping
even
time
with
between
favoured
boundary,
unobjectionable
hexameter,
interferes
or
preferred even
in
in
situation the
with
word-boundary;
not
the
favoured
a
the
itself,
diverquite
trochaic interferes
realization
of
the
verse-element. 4.2.2.
Cumulative
The clausular iambic
bridge
trimeter
named after
and in
the
--v-v--v--Ol-v--"'-u--ll ) may in
both
cases,
avoided,
non-marked
(basic)
syllable
-
which,
rhythmical quantity of
the
metric
in
cidence
of
preferred the
rhythmic
the
the coincidence
the
group:
with
- and,
at
with the
between
an overlap (where of these
two
and
non-preferred
as
tendency
is
the
same time,
the
regard
to the
relation
trimeter
metric
a contrast groups.
(where
and rhythmic and is
towards
occurrence between the
groups
rhythmic
preferred:
a long
non-preferred
the
metric
between
realization
sequence
in the
category:
a tension
of
general
iambic
of
both
0--V--U:-v:--"-u--ll and
equally
its
the
occurs
cumulative
occurrence
the
in
(
second, of
end of
situation
boundaries tetrameter
the
accordance
position
situation),
trochaic
above),
this
the
namely
near
non-preferred and
exemplify
which
tetrameter
verse-element
in
purity
trochaic
co-occurrence
is
situations the
the
22:S
Porson
coinis
the
group;
in
see
2. 3.1.
392
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
4.3.
Rhythmical
Simple.
remains a category
There nature,
involving
avoidance short In
Bridge:
of
these
ment groups
of
cases
and
the
virtue
which
the
cola
the
evidently,
is
devices
of
by
between
(syllables
sequence
as being
the
as
( the
(-v~:u:u---uu--u\r-il)
2 24
one double-short
itself
to)
(in
characteristic
two
nature
of
caesura),
-
of one of verse
near
the
somewhat less
and,
of verse-end
split
element
characteristic the
ele-
to
this
•
rhythmic
contrast
structuring
trochaic
the
double-
being
internally
is
the
the
twofold
avoided
for
of some of
belonging
in
example
An
short
hexameter
occurs
of
seem to be of a more simple
contrast.
first
unobjectionable
in
preferred
the
dactylic
realization
- completely
middle
of
kind
after
resolution),
the
one
a contrast
its
within
which
only
word-end
elements
of bridges
225
a well.
5. Conclusion We may conclude, chapter
between
as
(conceived essential
to
tional in
Greek
poetry.
The Greek
the
standing
of
its
actual
of
in
its
are
of
the
interplay
of
appears
the
rhythmical the
for
the
in
study.
verse
metric
further
'rhythmical considered
the
verse
a
prominent
of
into
aspects
of
and
which
A better and
the
research
a
of side
have under-
rhythmic into
stylistics', as
is func-
rhythmical
nature
for
of
characteristics
inquiry
the
first
play
stylistic
between
the
respectively)
to
this
in
of rhythmically
purely
further
occasionally
be conditional
level
shape
turn,
a more
called
221
phenomenon
analyse
scope
only
to
the
of to
what we have
phrasing
ques-
concerning
meaningful
linguistic
217
stylistic
s tylistic
and
upon
As was argued such
expected
may provide
of
utterance.
rhythmic
effort
tions the
the
understanding
may seem
introduced
and
which,
levels
distinction
metric
which
touched
the
description
present
material
been
its
our
poetry
that
the
contrast
part
the
then,
above, inquiries,
approach,
which
it
1s to be doubted there assumes
is
room that
whether,
for (types
the of)
in addition so-called
to
phono-
word-boundaries
393
CONCLUSION have
phonological
in
particular
any
case
provide
may be doubted
it
a method
should
be noted
whether,
stichic
of
responsion. languages it
rhythm, verse
that
whether
the
of
for
should
facts
approach
the
Greek
metrical verse
far
as
verse
concerned, not
rhythm as such.
rhythm
a careful
lead
may prevent
a less
to
premature :ut
are
of this
approach
to
same time
the
to for
external
in other,
esp.
of Greek the
and
under-
internal
non-quantitative
of Greek verse
understanding
to
be answered
analysis
informative
regular
to lead
it
UI
observed.
of
generalizations
In
to a rhythm-
the
waiting
words
speech.
infrequently
still
study
a better
at
the facts
results
grouping
with
while
with
of
by appealing
seems not
or to what extent,
As
is
the
claim
be substantiated,
the
positioning styles
question
with
the
particular
more fundamental
verse
standing
can
to
in
which are at variance
Another,
if
to account
factor
predictions
relevant
verse-positions
independent
is
properties
rhythm,
of other about
the
types nature
even of of
394
RHYTHM AND METRE
APPENDIX WORD-LENGTH IN LYSIAS, or. III
number of syll ./words 1 2 3 4 5 6
number of words
% of words
25 289 382 .302
1.87% 21.63% 28.59% 22.6% 15.42% 6.59% 2.69% 0.22% 0.37%
206 88 36 3 5
7 8 9 total
1336
number of syllables 25
578 1146 1208 1030 528 252 24
45 4836*
* The slightly different number of syllables presented in this table as compared to our quantification of syllabic quantity in Ch.111, 1. 2.1. above arises from occasional differences in the treatment of hiatus and contraction.
NOTES
l DE GROOT (1932) esp. 83-9; VOS (1973) 22-3; ALLEN (1973) 102; LOOTS (1979) 1-18; GROTJAHN(1979) 23-33; 197-201.
395
esp.
96-
2 Supposedly falling in with the rhythm innate in human nature, such as the pulse or muscular tension ('motor activity'); see e.g. CH.Ant.AN(1965) 28. It May he pointed out that tl•e discussion on the subject has been carried on in approxi~ately the sane terms for nuch longer than is commonly realized: for a survey of the research in the preceding century, see HEUMANN(1894); for further reference, see RUCYJ-1ICH (1913), (1915), (1918), (1924). 3 See \JOODROY(1951) 1232: "By rhythm, in the psychological sense, is meant the perception of a series of sti111Jli as a series of groups of stimuli. The successive groups are ordinarily of similar pattern and experienced as repetitive. Each group is perceived as a whole and therefore has a length lying within the psycholo&ical present." Compare LOOTS (1979) 4: ''We define rhythm as the phenomenon that in a series of (auditory) stimuli we perceive these sti111.1li as grouped, each group containing one stinulus that is more prominent than others." On the other hand, a concept ion such as that advocated by MALMBERG(1971): "Jede gesprochene J\usserung ist rhythmisch. Schon die filr die Rildung der Sprachlaute erforderliche Ausatmung setzt eine ziemlich recelmassige Aktivitit der Rippenmuskeln voraus" (21) does not seem to be very illuminating either. 4 CHATMAN(1965), Chapter 11; for a survey of comparatively early acoustic experiments, see HARRELL (1937) 63-5; ~LL's own experiments include such fascinating topics as "Preferred Rates for Singing 'The Star Spangled Banner', Walzing, and Hearing 'Turkey in the Straw'", and "Effect of Fatigue and Whiskey on Preferred Rate". 5 DE GROOT (1932) 242; (1946) 19. His examples include phrases "De Wondere Wereld", "over en such as "The Han of Property", weer". DE GROOT fixes the ideal interval at 3/4 of a second. Quite inappropriately, he refers to such Hebungsabstinde as "Perioden", a term which is hard to dissociate from the very the theory different connotations which adhere to it in, e.g., of rhetoric or in musicology.
396
RHYTIDI AND METRE
6 Cp. tlf.P.TlH (1972) 488: "Thf' constrair.t on speech souncls, or on any other real-time sequence of behavioral elements, that is directly implied by the concept of rhythm is relative timing, which rieans that the locus of each (sound) element along the time dimension is determined relative to the locus of all other elements in the sequence, adjacent and nonadjacent. ( ••• ) The alternative to a rhythmic sequence, which is less restrict! ve, is that the loci of the elements in the sequence are only successive (concatenated) in time. Sound sequences like these cannot have a structured, internal organization, although entities that they might represent can, of course, have such an organization, albeit one that is relatively abstract." of e.g. Greek lyric poetry, it seel?lS evidIn view of the nature ent that the so-called concatenated type should also be included 2 in the notion of rhythm. For 'free verse', see HRUSHOVSKI ( 1964). A comparatively early example of the idea that repetition not a necessary condition for constituting a rhythmical sequence is JACOB (1918) 99ff. 7 VOS (1973) 10-1 likewise uses the ten, 'metrical' to denote rhythm with l"'easured tone-sequences (which he calls "periodic", to isochronous ti~e-intervals: cp. note 5 above). Since referring he regards the phenonenon as beini not yet sufficiently clear, he cioes not venture to provide a definition of rhythm. Compare also CHATMAN (1965) 29: "Let us ASSlll'll' that 111eter is basically linguistically determined •secondary rhytha• - linguistic events grouped rep,ularly in tirie, such that each group has unity in its internnl conposition and in its external relations." SMITH (1968) distin~uishf's 'metrical poetry' frol'I 'free verse' (e.g. p. 94); similarly GROTJAHN (1979) referred to in note 19 helow. See also 3 \TF.Uf.Y f. \.JARRF.N( 1962) 163, who "distin~bh between theories requirinr, 'periodicity' as the 8ine qua non of rhythm and theories which, conceiving of rhythm more widely, include in it even non-recurrent cnnfigurationR of movements. The first view definitely identifies rhythm with metre, and thus may require the rejection of the concept of 'prose rhythm' as a contrAdiction or a nere metaphor." VEP.\.IEY (1931) 20, quoted in note 20 below is also relevant here. 8 FAY (1966) 28. The experiments referred to concern clicks produced by (i .a.) Savart 's Wheel. By varying the distance between two (hence: "paired") clicks, it is found that in the case of an interval under 2-10 msec. (sc. in the different experiments) a fusion of two clicks takes place (sc. in human perception). It should be noted, however, that this limit of fusion varies according to the quality of the stimulus involved: "The acoustic event, its intensity, and its paired partner all play a role in human resolving time." (l.c.). FAY's own investigati~ns concern the phenomenon of 'tel"'J)oral resolution' in the case of different pairs of phonemes; as conparerl with the limit of 2-10 nsec. for the awareness of successiveness of clicks he considers his "results with phoner,es ( ••• ) particu-
397
NOTES
larly impressive since the minimUDI resolution time was not established. Just how lll.lch below 10 msec resolution would continue to be accurate is a question unanswered." (o.c. 86). 9 See for example LOTZ (1960). 10 Compare DE GROOT(1932) 11
97.
I.a. CHATMAN (1965) 21; compare VAN NOORDEN (1975) 8; 85: "(The experiment) suggests that the perception of temporal coherence is based on some kind of 'echoic memory'. In order to make it possible for a listener to hear temporal coherence between successive tones, the first tone must as it were continue to echo in the memory until the second tone ls presented."
12 LEHISTE (1977) 258; cp. also
CHATMAN (1965) 21-2.
13 DE GROOT(1932) 251. 14 Cor,parP \!EST (1982)
24: "It 1s precisely the asymmetrical distribution of the longs and shorts that gives many metres their charthe acteristic quality - the fact that in -uu-1.r- , for instance, It should he noted, however, that principes are not equidistant." between in the present interpretation the idea of a 'distance' marked verse-positions c.q. syllables refers to the number of interjacent non-marked positions/syllables exclusively. Since it seems likely that the idea of 'bars' should be discarded from the theory of Greek verse rhytrun altogether, the absence of equidiatance should not be connected with "changing bar-lengths" (WEST, l.c. ). The non-applicability of the notion of bars in Greek poetry has been argued most convincingly by GEORGIADES (1949) 47/8; 90ff.
15 Cp. SMITH (1968) 86, referring to free verse: ''What the reader expects here, from line to line, is not a fixed number of syllables or pattern of stresses, but that certain limits of variability will not be exceeded." The supposition that the comparatively random character of these types of verse does not endanger the coherence of the sequence might be supported by research on coherence in the perception of tone sequences (VAN NOORDEN (197 5) 55): "previous knowledge of the tone sequence does not influence the perception of temporal coherence. The temporal coherence boundary for random sequences is found at roughly the same tone intervals as that for regularly alternating sequences." And, ibid. 46: "listening for temporal coherence is probably not a question of moving the attentional "filter" to and fro actively, but of following the tone sequence passively." lb DF. GROOT(1946) 111: "Als algemene regel zal men wel mogen poneren, dat een oMvang van 5 tot 12 lettergrepen voor de voort-
398
RHYTHMAND METRE brenging en waarneming van e~n reeks woorden als eenheid rle neest gunatige is; in het algemeen schommelt dan ook d~ lengte van de versregels in de West-Europese talen binnen deze grenzen." Recent research on auditory tone sequences may prove fruitful in these matters, that is, if the preferred extension of the colon may be connected with a notion like 'time window': see e.g. TEN HOOPEN a group of tones as having certain (1982) 13: "one might define physical or stimulus characteristics, hut it becomes a perceptual group only and only if it is "spanned" by the til'le window." Also 140: "we should study the short t.erm UOOTtBOOM& COHF.N (1975) storaRe capacity for melodic and rhythAic aspects of speech, how this relates to short term storage of words and word groups, and how the latter may be enhanced by the first."
17 E.g. tone phrasing) historical
(musical phrasing), or similarity to context (referential
the incidence other phrases phrasing).
of word-end (lexical in the immediate or
SMITH (1968) 12-3: "until the series is announced as conwe cannot be sure of the pattern", i.e., "retrospective patterning". Ibid. 94: "( ••• ) although the distinction between free and fixed forms can often be expressed as the difference between degrees of variability, this difference is often radical, and its effect on closure must be acknowledged. While our expectations, in a free-verse poem, are controlled by probabilities and confined by limits of variability, we obviously cannot predict the specific occurrence of formal elements with the same degree of confidence as in fixed forms." For our understanding of such processes, recent research on speech perception may be relevant. Compare, for instance, the notion of 'anticipation' used by NOOTEBOOM & COHEN (1975), by which they mean "cases where an event coming later in the speech act exerts overt influence on a preceding event. ( ••• ) the anticipation itself may often enhance temporal integration in production in a way which makes the task of the listener easier, by providing clues on how the incoming flow of speech can be divided into coherent auditory structures suitable for further processing." (125/6). Compare also FAY (1966) 102/3: "forward dependency".
18 Cp.
cluded
19 See 3.1. 3. below. For a fine illustration of the blending of these notions see e.g. HRUSHOVSKI( 2 1964). The partial overlap of these two applications of both 'rhythm' seems to underlie, for example, the confused and unand 'metre' convincing criticism addressed to WIMSATT& BEARDSLEYby HENDREN (in HENDREN, WIMSATT & BEARDSLEY(1961) 300): ''We cannot ( ••• ) it. Timing is separate meter from the performance which defines a fundamental character of the poem in the basic metrical sense we are concerned with. It accounts for the measured movement, the rhythm, of verse as well tts for the durational variations of different readings. Properly understood, the two fit together.
NOTES
399
One is foundatinn scheme, the other the flexibility of syllabic overlay." Compare also PACE (1961) 415, who introduces an historical aspect: ''Heter ( ••• ) I would regard as the domain of traditional metrics. The determining of gross stress contrasts, the analysis of these into recurrent units called feet, and the identification of the units as iambic, trochaic, etc. has always been the central task of traditional metrics. It 1s only when the older 1s extended to include the spoken poem that difficulty system occurs. For there is another domain, the poem as read aloud. I will call the metrical aspect of this domain rhythm. Here (degrees of stress count); here pitch is involved; here the unit is not the foot but the juncture group - or, as I would prefer to name it, the rhythmic phrase. There was a time when the older system could properly concern itself with this domain. But the time is long passed." (For "the day has come when no one should attempt work in metrics without linguistic knowledge of the linguistic features involved." ibid. 419.) GROTJAHN's application of the term 'Metrum' shows a similar ambiguity. One the one hand, 'Metrum' is defined as the "funktionales Konstruktionsprinzip einer rhythmischen Silbenfolge" (1979) 27, by which he apparently means: the principles in virtue of which a linguistic utterance is (produced/perceived as) a verse. Accordingly, it seems that 'metrisch' in this sense covers our notion of verse rhythm: "Als metrisch sind somit solche Texte anzusehen, die erstens eine rhythmische Struktur aufweisen und denen zweitens ein Met rum zugrunde lie8t. Nicht-metrische Texte sollen jetzt als Prosa(texte) bezeichnet werden." (28). On the other hand, it is also a kind of abstraction, similar to the notion of the 'metric profile' of a verse: "Das Met rum ist als theoretisches Konstrukt selbst nicht direkt beobachtbar, sondern kann lediglich indirekt anhand seiner sprachlichen Realisation in Form des Rhythmus erfasst werden." (30). So, GROTJAHN's definition of 'Metrum' as the structural principle of verse rhythm can be considered as a sensible extension compared to the use of 'metre' to denote grouped rhythm only; unfortunately, he too fails to distinguish between a principle of rhythmical order and a particular kind of order to be abstracted from a particular rhythmical text. (-Asa matter of fact, in this case the parallelism between the two notions is even more salient: if the principle of 'metre', for example, would be formulated as "between two marked syllables no more than two unmarked syllables may occur", the metric profile of any verse would show this situation (no more than two consecutive non-marked verseelements), whereas the rhythmic realization, i.e. an individual verse may diverge from it in various manners.) 20 URMUNSKIJ (1966) 17. To VERWEY(1931) this is apparently the primary meaning: "Ritmen zijn werkelijkheden, metra zijn schemata." (22). But, according to VERWEYalso, at the same time it is true that ''het metrum, voor zoover het samenvalt met het ritme
400
RHYTHMAND METRE due als regelmatige weerkeer • wel degelijk beetaat. Hen hoeft het geen met rum te noemen, men blijft bij de benaming ritme; maar ale men dat doet is men toch genoodzaakt nadruk erop te leggen dat men nu ritme niet ale etroom maar ale regelmatige weerkeer bedoelt. Bij echerpe begripsbepaling krijgt dus het woord metrum al een grond in de werkelijkheid: het beduidt ritme voor zoover di t een bepaalde vorm van weerkeer is." (ibid.). A similar ambiguity in SECKEL (1937). Cp. BROADHEAD(1933) 115/6, who distinguishes between a 'rhythmical' and a 'metrical' point of view, referring to the grouping of syllables and the grouping of scansion respectively. SCHULTZ (1972) 28: "die rhythmische Realisation der metrischen Schemata"; KIPARSKY (1975) 580: "(the tension between) the abstract metrical pattern and the actual rhvthm of the verse". Somewhat 3 confusingly, FOWLER(1966), after WELLEK& WARREN( 1962), refers to this verse-level by means of the term 'prose rhythm' (82ff.). 4 Th~ question has been sensibly discussed by ASMUTH( 1976) 58-61.
21 Ancient terminology is too diverse to be a real objection against the way of expression advocated here.•~, of course, is current as denoting all kinds of rhythmical material: e.g. o'ati ~dva, Plato Leg. 669 E 1: (6«.acru:nv ot 1101.TlUU)~ ~(i.e •• non-linguistic rhythm). The term ut"Q:10V denoting grouped rhythm is attested in Ar is toph. Nub. 641 / 2: ( ••• ) 6n >aUL.O'tOV 1,lttPOVI flyEt, n6tqx,v U> "CP'\.1£"Q:10V ~ lb 't~1. Sometimes, a distinction between verse-levels seems to be va~uely implicit in ancient ex11Et'101.1'l\Jt\lCI. f1VI) pressions (e.g. Xen. Hem. 1. 2. 21: ,:0, tv \Jtl:P,.> but of course, the idea of a formal principle is still far remote from the actual distinction of verse-levels. 22 Compare also meter, cited
PORTER (1951) on the in note 27 below.
pattern
of
the
Greek
hexa-
23 See DE GROOT (1932) 179. ALLEN's theory of a - rhythmically relevant - dynamic accent operative in Greek verse ((1966); (1973) 260 ff.) has been proved to be implausible by NEWTON(1969), whose "examination of the practice of Sophocles and Homer provides no evidence of the existence of dynamic stress in classical Greek. If such a stress existed it is easier to regard it as an automatic accompaniment of the pitch accent; the subsequent development of a dynamic stress would then be not so much a radical change in the phonetic characteristics of the accent as a readjustment of the phonological status of its ingredients." (370/1); compare PARKER (1970) 62; (1977); BARRY (1974): "the unconscious exploitation of a secondary stress system for the resolution of verse structure while making only limited use of the primary system, and the assumption that Greek alone among the lndo-European languages possessed such a secondary stress system fail to satisfy." (240); even WEST (1976) fails to see the relevance of this assumption: "That ancient Greek had some system of mild stress-
NOTES
401
is likely enough. But if the grammarians say nothing variation about it, if it has no discoverable phonological effect, and if suggests no simple and coherent principles, there versification (6); also RUIJGH is little hope of finding out how it operated." (1981) 399.
24 Roughly speaking, a syllable 1s long if it contains either a long vowel or a short vowel followed by more than one consonant; otherwise it is short. to realize that, so far as prosody It is of some consequence and thus the markedness of syllables - is concerned, wordseems as yet to boundaries are irrelevant. This circumstance shut out classical Greek verse from the practice, if not also from the theory of generative metrics. Most advocates of the 'generative' approach are mainly concerned with poetry in which the distinctive contrast is based on dynamic accent (with English poetry especially) - without, therefore, eschewing the pretension of universality. However, in classical Greek, both the essentially binary nature of the quantitative opposition and the irrelevance of word-boundaries to quantity are at variance with, e.g., the application of 'tree structures' which is basic to this approach. (Compare for instance LIBERMAN & PRINCE (1977) 273: "The essential generalization rendered by the (English Stress Rule) is that location of stress is calculated from the end of the word, and hinges on certain features also KIPARSKY (1977) 194, on the of the syllables counted."; relative/hierarchical nature of stress.) their See also DEVINE & STEPHENS (1984) 107 where they explain option for "the simple comb model in preference to the currently popular (binarily branching, rooted) tree model. ( ••• ) The latter model would have led us to make claims about the grouping of syllables into contours and about the hierarchical relationships between contours for which independent evidence is not 11 Furthermore, the current application of correspondavailable. ence rules in order to account for deviations from the metric pattern and in order to determine the 'metricality' of a verse is likely to lead to conclu(see e. g. HALLE & KAYSER (1971)) sions which are either trivial or faulty (see DEVINE & STEPHENS (A (1975)) - even if only stichic verse 1s taken into account. striking example of the latter category is provided by KIPARSKY (197 5), who interprets the anceps in the iambic trimeter as a "u-- after quantitative neutralization according to the rule verse-boundary" (• metron-boundary). The implausibility of this interpretation is concealed only in virtue of his misleading is a characteristic of statement that "(this neutralization) iambic verse only, and has no counterpart in trochaic verse, where /-/ in the corresponding positions cannot be replaced by /u/." (614). For criticism of the 'generative' approach as such, see STANDOP (1972), KLEIN (1974). DEVINE & STEPHENS (1975) argue that a "metrical grammar" may be applied to Greek poetry which "involves both structural and transformational techniques of analysis" and which therefore may be justly called 'generative' -
RHYTHMAND METRE
402
although the approach advocated by them shows more affinity to "traditional metrical analysis" than to the theory of metrics marked with that label (428). In this alternwhich is usually ative 'generative metrics', however, the designation to a coherent & hierarchical set of 'grammar rules' of essentially divergent phenomena such as the syllabic realization of verseelements, and the regulation of word-boundaries seems to be still quite problematic. (For example, the appointment to one and the same set of transformation rules of catalexis, brevis in longo, contraction and caesura ( ibid. 420, 428) is very unconvincing.) In short, the relation between the two main factors which operate in linguistically realizing a metric profile (viz. syllabic quantity and the localization of words) seeu to be too complex to fit into a systematized set of transformation rules. Apparently grallllll8r proposed by DEVINE& STEPHENS even the transformational does not escape the cri ticiam which they themselves address to current generative metrics: ''When one considers metrical rules in greater detail and takes account of bridges and coda restrictions, etc., it becomes clear that in principle the (generative) theory has to permit a potentially different mapping rule for every foot in each different metre." (1975) 415. - More fundamentally, it is obvious that a careful rhythmical/ stylistic interpretation of the phenomena involved is preliminary to any attempt to compose a "metrical grammar" - which seems to justify the conclusion that a generative approach, possibly useful as is devoid of any providing a generalized descriptive system, heuristic value whatsoever. 25 The multi-valued
scale of syllabic quantity postulated by WEST not indispensable to the understanding of Greek verse rhythm and should therefore be discarded. (In his recent monograph on Greek metre (1982) WEST does not explicitly invoke this idea of multiple quantities; on the other hand, it is apparently implied in his interpretation of C"'rtain particular phenomena: PP• 20, 24 (see note 14 above), 87 (compare our Ch. III sub e.g. 3.6. below).) In quantitative rhythm the actual duration of the syllables 1s irrelevant, just as in verse based on the recurrence of syllables with a dynamic accent, the actual duration of the interstress intervals is irrelevant to the perception of a regular recurrence. This point of view is most convincingly expressed by (1958) 17; CHATMAN (1965) 115: ''we GEORGIADES (1949) 20; also conceptualize metrical time in terms of rough equalities; we treat syllables of varying durations as if they were identical in duration. That we should do so is not remarkable from the psychological point of view; it is simply one more instance of our propensity to categorize experience, to consider things 1n terms of structural groups rather than fragmentarily, as legions of disparates. The motive is a simple principle of human economy 11 ( ••• ) • For the corresponding phenomenon in dynamic rhythm: DE
(1970) is
GROOT(1946) 19; LEHISTE (1977).
403
NOTES
Nevertheless, especially in Greek lyric poetry of the Hellenistic period there may have been a considerable variation in the realization of long (and short) syllables according to the accompanying music; however, there is no reason to suppose that this variety anyhow interferes with the binary nature of the rhythmical events as such. We may compare Longinus, Proleg. in Reph. 83.10-6 C.:
'tO lJt"tPOV,ij 'tO utv l,lt"tpov Tt£TD"IY6~ fx.£1. 1:'0UC ~, l,.O'HQ6vTE MOL&xtxuv MOL'tOV~ "COUlXIJV 'tOVNOi. '\}CV ~, c%:MOL~ ~ ~ ta't1. v 1\ 8',xxxuc;. b ot ~ ~ ~ fAM£1. 1:'0UC ~l"IO.UaKl.tycO., MOL'tOVecaxw ~ no1.Et ~•En
ml'VVV 01.~1.
~
The above does not necessarily affect theories of phonostyle such as that brought forward by DEVINE & STEHPENS (1978); (1984). seem to concern differences Possible differences in phonostyle in the performance of the rhythmical sequence rather than the binary opposition of syllabic quantity characteristic of Greek verse rhythm. As a matter of fact, it seems advisable to reserve the term 'duration' for rhythmically distinctive quantity in terms of the binary opposition (contra DEVINE & STEPHENS (1984) 106, 108); phonostylistic variation can be adequately expressed in terms of 'tempo of performance' or 'speech tempo'. See also Ch. III 2.1.1. note 124 and 3.1.1. for aeolic verse - in which 26 We must however make an exception case it seems to be essential that this is a rhythmical genre not originating in Greece. The underlying metric profile thus postulated is made visible by (• (1969) 6lff.), in the notation suggested by DALE (1951) 20ff. which the basic units are -u- (s: single-short) and -vu- (d: double-short). These units may be extended by prolongation (-v-u-: ss; -1.1v-vu-: dd) or by means of blunt junction (-v--v-e s 's; -vu--u.r : d 'd). Rhythmical events which di verge from the situation described in this manner are 'translated' into these units ( --• i or d; uuu• rs) or are written in (--v• -s; u- • ,.s). This notation surely has the merit of providing an economic way of describinr GreP.k verse within the limits of the definition as formulated (Ch. I.3.) above. It should be realized, however, that the notation does not necessarily provide information about the actual rhythmical effect (or even, 'nature') of the verse; sometimes it may in fact give rise to false suggestions, as e.g. by describing ionics of the form uv--vu-as ...d 'd-, whereas the metrical grouping clearly is uu-- , uu-- • In addition, the possibility of descrihin~, e.g., u---uas ...sis, uis or ...s-s alternatively shows that subjectivity of int~rpretation can not always be avoided. 21 See LOOTS (1979), referring to CREEK (1920): .. the expectation of movement on the part of the the recurrence of a particular listener" (22); also FRAISSE ( 2 1967) 301; (1974) 200; CHAntAN (1965) 207/8; VOS (1973) 103 (tthogere-orde reeks").
404
RHYTHMAND METRE The ability to do so may be connected with the human tendency to interpret a sequence of rhythmical events in a subjective manner ( 'subjective rhythm', see page 2 above) - whether ultimately based upon a 'motor readiness' of human perception (compare note 2 above) or not. DE GROOT (1932) 92/3: "Es kann ein Rhythmuserlebnis, einen subjektiven Rhythmus, ohne gleichzeitige Wahrnehmung einer objektiv rhythmischen Reihe geben"; "(es ist moglich), dass zwei kurze Silben infolge der Erwartung einer langen Silbe oder jedenfalls einer langen Grundeinheit an einer bestiamten Stelle, infolge der Einstellung auf die Wahrnehmung e1ner Grundeinheit, als e1ne akustische oder motorisch-akustische Einheit wahrgenommen werden." Compare also PORTER (1951) 8/9: "The hexameter may be defined as the sum total of all metrical usages of all the poets who composed in the form. However, in this mass of disparate metrical phenomena we can, by statistical analysis, perceive a norm, or rather a system of norms. This system of norms is no mere scholar's abstraction but is rather a pattern of expectancy present in the mind of the listener or reader. Indeed, a metrical system can hardly be said to exist until it is present in the mind of the listener or reader as an abstraction with which he compares the words of the poet as they come. The poet sometimes satisfies the demands of this ideal form, though constantly varying his method of doing so, and sometimes by distorting the form creates for his own purposes tension between what is expected and what is actually spoken." The expectancy of regularity and the human ability to interpret deviations from the metric profile plays a role also in recent discussions of the metricality of a linguistic utterance. See e.g. BERNHART(1974) 114/5: "The presence of ( ••• ) an expectancy of rPgularly recurring stressed syllables in the listener makes verse 'metrical'. So the metricality of a verse-line is formed by the guarantee of the expected sequence of stressed and unstressed syllables. This guarantee is given when a larger portion of the line affirms the pattern; if a greater part of the line deviates from the pattern the expectation of the pattern is lost and the line thereby becomes 'unmetrical'. So it can be stated that the criterion for determining the metricaU ty of lines is the predominance of pattern-affirriin& parts over pattern-contradicting parts within a line." (The criterion of 'metricality' in this sense has been (duly) criticized by STAND0P(l972) 16/7.) In the present context, we are concerned with the identification of the metric pattern rather than with nort'l8tive valuation of 'metricality'. However, BERNHART's modification of the above statement to the purport that "in some cases ( ••• ) it is not the degree of complex1 ty that becomes relevant for metricali ty but rather the individual distribution of deviations in the line" (o.c. 132) may be mentioned here in view of such restrictions as are those on the occurrence of consecutive resolutions in Greek iambic verse: see Ch. III 2.1.4.1. below.
28 For anceps as a structural see note 36, p. 406 below.
feature of iambic and trochaic rhythm, The same holds for the link-anceps of
NOTES
405
dactylo-epitritic poetry: in virtue of our awareness of the recurrence of the groups -\n,HJVand -v- (preceded by or) linked by means of single verse-elements realized by either a long or one short syllable we will interpret the second of the three consecutive long syllables in a sequence like -vv-uv---,u••• as the realization of a non-marked (anceps) element. The third possibility of a single-short interpretation concerns cholosis, i.e. the (occasional or institutionalized) realization of a nonmarked, single-short element which is the final one of a rhythnical (suh-)phrase - as in the choliambus, for which see 2.3. above - or, exceptionally, of a rhythmical group (as in the dochmius, where u---- responds to u --u-). 29 That is, in virtue of the recurrence of the same metric sequence in one and the same poem. The most common types are stichic responsion, in which all verses are the realization of one and the same metric sequence, and strophic responsion, i.e. the occurrence of two or more sets of verses corresponding to one and the same set of metric sequences. So, in the case of several occurrences of rhythmic realization of one and the same metric profile, the value of a verse-element may become clear in virtue of the occurrence of the 'proper' realization of the verseelement in question; e.g. -vu-uu-v- in responsion to -vu-vu---. For the terminology, see MAAS(1962) § 28. see nlso SICKING (1981) 30 Compare P• 5 w1 th note 18 above; "De beoogde rhythmische ken111erken zi jn met de opbouw van kolon uit lange en korte lettergrepen gegeven, en het zijn onderlinge betrekkingen tussen de kola van een vers en tussen verzen van een lied die verzen en liederen tot rhythmisch volle gehelen maken ". The more complex types of verse may, of course, exploit the biguity of a particular sequence within a particular context is often the case in the choral lyric of Greek drama, where is constituted by (variants of) the infrequently a strophe , -vu--, -vu-v-- ) • A similar kind of ambiguity quences -vv-uuoperative in music: see MEYER(1956) 37; 51/2.
243: het de de zinam(as not seis
31 Sometimes, the identity of the rhythm seems to consist only in the recurrence of a special 'motive', the remainder of the verse being apparently random. (But even here, one must assume that the underlying pattern which is in accordance with the definition of verse rhythm has been somehow indicated, e.g. by music or dance.) 32 Compare also the situation in dactylo-epitrite verse (SICKING & VAN RAALTE (1981)) and the "gleitende Ueberginge" in the choral lyric of Greek drama (SNELL (1982) 58ff.). Here, a special 'linkis achieved by an 'overlap' of the rhythmic phrase ing' effect compared with the boundary of the metric phrase by one or more verse-elements.
406
RHYTHMAND METRE
33 The term 'verse' is slightly preferable to the collllllOnname of associated with questions of 'line', which is unavoidably writing and printing. In stichic poetry, line and verse will usually be concurrent; however, the situation in, e.g., the odes of Pindar and Bacchylides is rather more complicated. 34 The phenomenon is brevis
in
(eleaento)
dactylic hexameter; be a more appropriate
therefore
not appropriately named (syllaba) as is most obvious in the case of the brevis pro (syllaba) longa would way of referring to it.
longo (syllab~)
35 However obvious this may be in stichic or (otherwise) distinctly phrasal types of verse, in verse in which the rhythmical phrasing is less distinct this way of defining '"brevis 1n longo n tends towards circularity. If there is no other index of verse-end (as hiatus or an argument to be derived from clausular characteristics or from observation of the rhythmical context), and, therefore, one cannot be certain about the metric profile of the verse in question, it is in fact impossible to be positive about the occurrence of the phenomenon. 36 The 'x' indicates an (eleraentum) anceps, i.e. an element of the metric profile in which a long or a short syllable may occur. In tetrameter, it is a non-marked versethe case of the trochaic position which may be realized indifferently either•~ a long or as a short syllable. In both the iambic trimeter and the trochaic tetrameter, the indifference to syllabic quantity is essential to the rhythm of the verse: the metrical grouping by four elements exists in virtue of the fact that the first, c.q. the last of every four verse-elements is not simply a short like the second, c.q. the third element of the metron. Otherwise, the rhythm would be perceived 1n groups ("feet") of two elements each: see Ch. III and IV sub 1. below. In view of this fundamental indifference to syllabic quantity the habit of notating an anceps element as O if it is more frequently realized as a short syllable and as Y if it is realized more of ten as a long is awkward (contra SCHEIN (1979) 4 note 19). syllable 37 It should be noted that a verse of such length as the types of verse discussed in this study is adequately structured into two cola (one 'functional' caesura): see 1.2.1. p. 5 above. '!'bis means that the assignment of more than one actual caesura in e.g. a concrete hexameter is not in accordance with the theory adopted here (see also Ch. II, 3.2. P• 70 below). There may, on the other hand, be more than one caesura position: see below. 38 'Rising', i.e. beginning with a non-marked event (element at the metric, syllable at the rhythmic level of the verse); 'falling', i.e. beginning with a marked event. The counterparts for the ending of a rhythmical sequence are 'pendant' (ending with a non-marked event) and 'blunt' (ending with a marked event).
NOTES
407
39 The description of the phenomenon of caesura may stop at this verse is concerned. point as far as stichic In verse without stichic or strophic responsion (or if only a single or few verses of a particular type have come down to us) the situation is rather more complicated. In such cases one can only choose one out of the existent word-ends as a hypothetical caesura; the relevant arguments may be derived from the (innediate or historical) rhythmical context. A conclusive factor will often be the position of word-end in relation to rhythmical groups (metra, phrases) to be distinguished at the metric level. Conversely, a boundary between two such groups should not be regarded as a caesura if there is not a potentially structural word-boundary at that position. In those verses where there is some sort of external responsion versebut no recurrence of word-end in a rhythmically plausible position one can only suppose that - if the verse extends beyond the length likely to be perceived as a non-structured whole - the structuring into rhythmical sub-phrases will have been brought to light by means of the accompanying music and/or dance. As a matter of fact, it may be no coincidence that this situation also occurs in Pindaric verse in particular: compare 3.1.2, p. 15 (with note 31) above. 40 In the present study, syllables or verse-elements have invariably been referred to by mentioning the number of the verse-position in which they occur. I am aware that this manner of numbering implies the debatable option of conceiving the two short rhythmical events which characterize double-short verse as one (sc. non-marked) verse-position. The expression 'word-end occurring at a certain verse-position' means that the final syllable of the word in question occurs in the verse-position mentioned. 41 It will be noted that we do not recognize the phenomenon of prosodic bridge. This is not meant to say that prosodic properties are altogether irrelevant to the study of bridges; on the contrary, they do play an important role with regard to limits set to the type of word-boundary which may occur in a particular verse-position - which may vary according to period, genre, or context. These are, however, phono-stylistic refinements and as such without explanatory value as far as the occurrence of bridge qua rhythmical phenomenon is concerned. Compare DEVINE & STEPHENS (1984) 134: "The term 'bridge' suggests a direct constraint against (phonological) boundary, which is just what the rhythmic bridge represents. The 'prosodic bridge', on the other hand, is a constraint not against a boundary per se, but against phonological properties which occur on syllables contiguous to a boundary, but which need not be uniquely distributed on such syllables. Thus, the term 'prosodic bridge' does not provide a direct characterization of the nature of the constraint, which is why it can lead to loss of descriptive generality. It is a
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
408 term that philological
belongs properly data collection."
to
the
pre-explanatory
stage
of
is perceived as pre42 It goes without saying that no syllable ceding the first syllable of a verse - which is an independent rhythmical sequence. Therefore, the word 'echoes' is essential to the present theory: there is no danger of misinterpretation (which is improbable also in virtue of the stichic recurrence of the sequence), but only of a disfavoured rhythmical association.
43 See Ch. III
sub 4.4.
below.
44 Also HARDIE (1920) 3: ible by decreeing that lables".
"In the hexameter the end is made perceptthe last foot shall always be of two syl-
45 It may be noted that Dionysius' testimony does not, to say the least, corroborate the theory to the effect that (long) final syllables are somewhat longer than non-final ones (for which see e.g. DEVINE & STEPHENS (1984) 25); for if Dionysius would be right in positing that the syllable occupying the marked element of the dactylic metron takes less time than a 'full' syllable, one would expect, according to the theory of lengthening of final syllables, that word-final syllables were avoided in marked verse-positions rather than (as they actually are) in nonmarked ones (see 4.2.ff. below). The theory has in fact been advanced to account for the restrictions on word-end in the fourth and the fifth metron of the hexameter by PORTER (1951) 20: "The easiest assumption is that long final syllables are somehow more effectively long than other long syllables." For further evaluation of the theory of final syllable lengthening, see Ch.III,3.1.1. below. i.e. the rhythmic (• linguistic: see Ch.I, 2.1. above) 46 'Dactyl', realization of the dactylic metron by three syllables (- uu ); opp. 'spondee', i.e. a bisyllabic realization of the dactylic metron (--).
JAfCKEL's assignment of 47 I am aware that adopted throughout this study - either to bucolic genre could be called in question. rhythmical characterization of the inrlividual (1898) 329ff.; see also KUNST (1887) 5-61.
Theocritus' poems the epic or to the For a more detailed poems, see LEGRAND
48 The question to what extent the dactyl 'comes natural' to the Greek language cannot be answered without entangling oneself in all kinds of circularity. Still, PAULSON 's observation that (l) a Greek prose text comprises more dactylic sequences than a Latin prose text and that (2) the Greek language has a greater (lexical and prosodic) adaptability (cp. 2.5. below) is apparently not unconnected with the larger proportion of dactyls found in the Greek hexameter. (See PAULSON(1897) 34-7.)
NOTES
409
49 Compare CUNNINGHAM( 1977) 96/7: in the hexameters of the "fragmentary Hellenistic poets" discussed in his study there is an average percentage of 42. 7% of verses with one spondee, and of 32.2% of verses with two spondees. In Eratosthenes' hexameters the percentages diverge from the average most clearly: one spondee 25%, two spondees 46.9%. 50 See LA ROCHE (1898) 68. The hexameter counted by him as the verse is printed with a doublefifth example of a 'holospondaic' short jirst metron in the MUNRO/ALLENedition ( Il. XI.130): •A1:P£ ~ 6' OOT' tx 6l-GILLES, AC XLIV (1975) 686--9.4. FARINA, A. lpponl.lle. Napoli 1963. 430 n. 175. FAY,w.H. Temporal Seq~nce in the Peruption of Spuch. Den Haag 1966. 396f. n. 8; 398 n. 18. FJNNEGAN, R. "What is Oral Literature Anyway? Comments in the Light of Some African and R.s. (edd.), Oral LiteraJUreand tltt Other Comparative Material." in: STOLZ, A. a SHANNON, Formula. Ann Arbor 1976, 127-76.413 n. 83. FOURNIER, H. "Formules homeriques de reference avec verbe 'dire"', RPh XX (1946) 29-68. 73. FOWLER, R. '"Prose Rhythm' and Meter", in FOWLER, R. (ed.), Essays on Style and Language. New York 1966, 82-99. 400 n. 20; 423 n. 139. FRAENI.EL, E. Aeschylus Agamemnon. Oxford 21962. 423 n. 144. FRAENI.EL, H. "Der kallimachische und der homerischc Hexameter", Nachr. Gesellsch. d. Wissensch. zu Gottingtn, Phil.-Hist. Kuwe 1926, 197-229.71; 417 n. 105. -, "Der homerische und der kallimachische Hexameter", in: Wege und Formen fruhgriechischtn Denuns, Munchen 31968, 100-56. 71; 417 n. 105. FRAISSE, P. Psychologie du temps. Paris 2 1967. 403 n. 27. -. Psychologie du rythme. Paris 1974. 403 n. 27. FUEHRER, R. Ree. CRESPO (19n), Kratylos XXIII (1978) 181-3. 61. 1. ·• Prcistoria e formazionc dell' esametro". in: I poemi epici rapsodici non omerici e la GENTILI, M. CANTILENA, c.o. PAVESE, Padova 1981, 75-106. 103 tradizione orate, a cura di c. BRILLANTE, - & GIANNINI,,."Prcistoria e formazione dell' esametro", QUCC XXVI (19n) 7-51. 103. GE0RGlADES, t. Der griechische Rhythmus. Hamburg 1949. 397 n. 14; 402 n. 25. -, Musik und Rhythmus bei den Griechen. Hamburg 1958. 402 n. 25. GOODELL, TH.D. "Bisected Trimeters in Attic Tragedy", CPh I (1906) 145-66. 425 n. 155. GR0NINGEN, I.A. VAN,La polsie verbale gr«q~. Essai de mist au point. Amsterdam 1953. 409 n. 53; n. 56; 410 n. 65; 411 n. 71; 416 n. 103. GROOT, A.w. DE,"Der Rhythmus", NPh XVII (1932)81-100; ln-97; 241-64. 2; 226f.; 395 n. I; 397 n. 10; n. 13; 400 n. 23; 404 n. 27. -. "Wesco und Gesctze der Caesur. Ein Kapitcl der allgemeinen Versbaulehre. ", Mn Ill.ii (1935) 81-154. 425 n. 155. -, Algemene versleer. Den Haag 1946. 395 n. 5; 397 f. n. 16; 402 n. 25. GROSSMANN, H. De doctrinae metricae reliquiis ab Eustathio servatis. Strassburg 1887. 63. GROTJAHN, R. Linguistische und statistische Methoden in Metrik und Textwissenschaft. Bochum 1979. 41; 395 n. l; 396 n. 7; 399 n. 19. -. HexameterStudies, Bochum 1981. 41. HANEDEL, P. Formen und Darstellungsweisen in deraristophanischen Komodie. Heidelberg 1963. 30. DREW-BEAR,TH.
REFERENCES ANDAUTHOR INDEX
437
HAJNswoam, J.B. "A Note on Elision in Homer", BICS XIV (1967) 17-21. 72 -, The Flexibilily of the Homeric Formula. Oxford 1968. 73; 413 n. 84. HALLE,M. A ltEYSEJt,s.J" EnglishStras: Its Form, usGrowth and usRole in verse. New York 1971. 401 n. 25. HARDIE, w.a. Res metrica. Oxford 1920. 408 n. 44. HAU.ELL,r.w. "Factors ln0uencing Preference and Memory for Auditory Rhythm". Journal of General psychology XVII (1937) 6.J..104.395 n. 44. HARJl.lSON, E. "Verse-Weight''. CQ VIII (1914) 206-11. 105. -, "Aristophanes, Frogs, 1203", CR XXXVII (1923) 10-4. 139. -, "Interlinear Hiatus in Greek Tragic Trimeters", CR LV (1941) 22-5; LVII (1943) 61-3. 423 n. 141. HENDltEN, J.W., WIMSATTJl .• W.K ... BEAIDSLEY, M.C."A Word for Rhythm and a Word for Meter", PMLA LXXVI (1961) 300-8. 398 f. n. 19. HERINGTON, c.,. The AUlhor of the Prometheus Bound. Austin 1970. 158; 422 n. 129. HESCHER, H. Metrische UnJersuchungen am epischen Hexameter der Alexandriner. Giessen 1914. 416 n. 104. HOEKSTRA, A. "H~siode et la tradition orale", Mn X (1957) 193-225. 63. -, Homeric Modifica1ions of Formulaic Protorypes: Studies in lhe Developmenl of Grttk Epic Diction. Amsterdam 1965. 63. -, The Sub-Epic Stage of the Formulaic Tradition. Studies in the Homeric Hymns to Apollo, to Aphrodi1e and to Demeter. Amsterdam/London 1969. 73. -, "Metrical Lengthening and Epic Diction", Mn XXXI (1978) 1-26. 61. -. Epic Verse Before Homer. Three Studies. Amsterdam/Oxford/New York 1981. 103. (1979, 273-90. 413 n. 83. H0ENIOSWALD, H.M. "Summary Commentary" in STOLZ a SHANNON H0EJtSCHELMANN, w. a causms, o. "Die Betonung des Choliambus", Ph N.F. VII (1894) 214-27. 430 n. 174. H0LOltA,J.P. "Homeric Originality: A Survey.", CW LXVI (1973) 257-93. 73. HOOPEN, G. TEN,The Perceptual Organizalion of Alternoling Tone ~quences. Diss. Leiden 1982. 398 n. 16. HORROCKS, o.c. "The Antiquity or the Greek Epic Tradition. Some New Evidence.", PCPhS XXVI (1980) 1-11. 103. HlUSH0VSKJ, B. "On Free Rhythms in Modem Poetry. Preliminary Remarks Toward a Critical Theory or Their Structures and Functions." In: SEBE0K,T.A. (ed.), Style in Language. Cambridge, Mass. 2 1964, 173-90. 396 n. 6; 398 n. 19. HUMBORG, F. Quaationes euripideoe.Qua rolione in Euripi.dis trimt'tris iambicis senJentioe cum versi.spugnenl. Hannover 1906. 160. HUMPHREYS, M.w. "On Cenain Effects of Elision", TAPA X (1879) 32-60. 195. IMH0F,M. "Tetrameterszenen in der Tragodie", MH XIII (1956) 124-43. 324. INGALLS, w.e. "The Structure of the Homeric Hexameter. A Review.", Phoenix XXIV (1970) 1-12. 71. -, "Another Dimension or the Homeric Formula", Phoenix XXVI (1972) 111-22.73; 413 n. 84. -. "The Analogical Formula in Homer", TAPA CVI (1976) 211-26. 413 n. 84. IRIGOIN, J. "Lois et r~gles dans le trim~tre iambique et le tetram~tre trochaique··. REG LXXII (1959) 67-80. 192: 254 r.; 257; 432 n. 195. -. Ree. aoss1, (1963). GGA CCXVII (1965) 224-31. 30; 101. JACOB,c.F. Foundations and Nature of Verse. New York 1918. 396 n. 6. JAECKEL, FR. De poetarum Sicu/orum hexametro. Diss. Leipzig 1902. 31; 40; 41; 79; 80; 408 n. 47. JANKO,R. Homer, Hesiod and the Hymns. Diachronic Developmenl in Epic Diction. Cambridge 1982. 28 nr.; 63; 68: 73; 409 n. 52; 412 n. 78. JONES,F.P. "A Binary-Octal Code for AnaJyzing Hexameters", TAPA XCVI (1966) 275-80. 41. -. "Notes on the Input for an Automatic Scansion Program", RELO (1968.4) 5-11. 41. -, & GRAY,F.E. "Hexameter Patterns, Statistical Inference, and the Homeric Question: an Analysis of the la Roche data." TAPA CIII (1972) 187-209. 31; 41: 410 n. 63.
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
438
Hipponax rtdivivus. Diss. Giessen 1929. 263. Dt lttramttro trochaico. Diss. Giessen, Darmstadt 1913. 431 n 192. KEYDELL, R. Nonni Panopolitani Dionysiaca. Vol. I: Berlin 1959. 30; 90; 98; 101; 416 n. 102. KIPARSKY, P. Str.ss. Syntax, and Meter.", Languagt LI ( 1975) 576-616. 400 no 20; 401 n. 24.
JUNG, F. KANZ, J.
0
"Oral Poetry: Some Linguistic and Typological Considerations." in: STOLZ, a.z. A SHANNON, R.s. (edd.) Oral Uttraturt and tht Formula. Ann Arbor 1976. 73-125. 413 n. 83. The Rhythmic Structure of English Verse'', Linguistic Inquiry VIII (19n) 189-248. 40 n. 24. KIRK, G.s. Studies in Some Technical Aspects of Homeric Style". YCS XX ( 1966) 73-152. 64; 65; 71;417n. 107. KITTO, H.D.F. "Sophocles, Statistics, and the Trachiniae.", AJPh XL (1939) 178-93. 122. KLAUER, R. Dt Aristophanis trimttrorum compositiont artificiosiort. Diss. Marburg 1905. 421 n. 125. KLEIN, w. "Critical Remarks on Generative Metrics". Potties XII (1974) 29-48. 401 n. 24; 432 n. 198. KNOX, A.D. "Herodes and Callimachus", Ph LXXXI (1926) 241-55. 430 n. 183. The Early Iambus''. Ph LXXXVII (1932) 18-39. KORZENIEWSKJ, o. Gritchischt Mttrik. Darmstadt 1968. 30; 75; 82; 248; 263; 418 n. 117;422 n. 130; 423 n. 146. KOSTER, w.1.w. Traite dt metriqut grtcqut. Leiden 1 1962. KRAFFT, F. Vtrgltichtndt Untersuchungen zu Homtr und Htsiod. G0ttingen 1863. 73. DJEG, w. "Der trochiische Tetrameter bci Euripides", Ph XCI (1936) 42-51. 324; 349. KUNST, c. Dt Thtocriti vtrsu htroico. Leipzig 1887; Amsterdam 2 1970. 408 n. 47. LATACZ, J. (ed.) Homer. Tradition und Ntutrung. Darmstadt 1979. 73. LEE, K.H. "The Influence of Metre on Tragic Vocabulary". Glotta XLVI ( 1968) 137-43. 122. BAAAQ Compounds in the Tragedians", AJPh XCII (1971) 312-15. 122. LEGRAND, PH.E. Etudt sur Theocritt. Paris 1898. 408 n. 47; 410 n. 61. LEHISTE, 1. "lsochrony Reconsidered", Journal of Phonetics V (19n) 253-63. 397 n. 12; 402 n. 25. LEHRS, K. De Aristarchi studiis Homtricis. Leipzig 1865. 83. LIBERMAN. M. a PRINCE, A. "On Stress and Linguistic Rhythm". Linguistic Inquiry VIII ( 19n) 249336. 401 n. 24. LOOTS, M.E. Mttrical Myths. An Experimmtal-Phonttic /nvtstigation into tht Production and Ptrception of Metrical Spetch. Utrecht 1979. 395 n. l; n. 3; 403 n. 27. LORD, A.B. "Homer and Huso Ill: Enjambement in Greek and Southslavic Heroic Song.". TAPA LXXIX (1948) 113-24. 65. LOTZ, J. "Metric Typology", in: SEBEOK (ed.), Stylt in Languagt. Cambridge, Mass. 1960, 135-48. 397 n. 9. LUCK, G. "Parergon metricum", Journal of lndo-European studies I (1973) 352-3. 98. LUDWICH, A. Dt hexamttris poetarum Graecorum spondiacis. Halle 1866. 409 n. 54. -. Aristarchs homerischt Textk.ritik..2 Vols. Leipzig 184.85. 32. MAAS, P. "Kolometrie in den Daktyloepitriten des Bakchylides", Ph N.F. XVII (1904) 297-309. 427 n. 164. -, "Zur Verskunst des Nonnos". Byzan1inische Ztitschrift XXVII (1927) 17-8. 90. -. "Hephthemimeres im Hexameter des Kallimachos", in: Ftstschrift Bruno SM/I, Munchen 1956, 23-4. 415 n. 94. -. Grtek Metre. (transl. H. I.LOYD-JONES) Oxford 1962. 94; 97; 98; 99; IOl; 208; 405 n. 29; 418 n. -.
0
-.
0
0
-.
0
-.
109. MCLENNAN, G.a.
"Enjambcment
in the HymnsofCallimachus",
Hemu-sCII (1974) 200-6. 69; 416
n. IOI. -.
"The Longum and Bictps of the Greek Hexameter", Mn XXXI (1978) 68-70. 61. "Die expressiven und isthetischen Ausdrucksm6glichkeiten der Sprache. lhre strukturale und quantitative Beschreibung. ", Zeitschrift fur Lileratu.rwissenschaftund Linguistik. 111(1971) 9-38. 395 n. 3.
MALMBERG, B.
REFERENCES ANDAUTHOR INDEX
439
MARCOVICH, M. Thrtt-Word Trimeter in Grttk Tragedy. KOnigstein/Ts. 1984. 183. MAmN, J.G. "Rhythmic (Hierarchical) Versus Serial Structure in Speech and Other Behavior". Psychological Review LXXIX (1972) 487-509. 396 n. 6. JC. Die Homerische Kunstsprache. Leipzig 1921. 60; 61. MEISTER, MEUMANN, E. "Untersuchungen zur Psychologie und Aesthetik des Rhythmus". Philosophical Studies X (1894) 249-322; 393430. 395 n. 2. MEYER, L.B. Emotion and Meaning in Music. Chicago/London 1956. 405 nr. 30. s. (et al.) "Sentence Length Distributions in Greek Hexameters and Homer". MICHAELSON, Assoc. for Liltrary and Linguistic Compuling BulletinVl.3 (1978) 254-67. 411f. n. 74. MINEUR, w.H. Callimachus. Hymn to Delos. Leiden 1984. 92; 101;409n. 52; 410n. 60;415 n. 90; n. 91. MINTON, w.w. "The Fallacy of the Structural Formula", TAPA XCVI (1965) 241-53. 413 n. 83. -. "The Frequency and Structuring of Traditional Formulas in Hesiod's Thtogony", HSCPh LXXIX (1975) 25-54. 73. MORELLI, G. "Studi sul trimetro giambico" I: Maia XIII (1961) 14J.6I; II: Maia XIV (1962) 149~1. 257. -. "Callimacho e la legge di Nacke", RCCM VI (1964) 140-55. 101 NAGLER, M.N."Towards a Generative View of the Oral Formula", TAPA XCVIII (1967) 269-311. 73. -. Spontaneily and Tradilion. A Study in tht Oral Art of Homer. Berkeley 1974.411 n. 70; 413 n.
83. NAGY, G. Comparative St~s in Grtek and lndic Meter. Cambridge, Mass. 1974. 103. R.S. (edd.) Oral Literature and tht -. "formula and Meter", in: STOLZ,B.A. & SHANNON, Formula. Ann Arbor 1976, 239-72. 413 n. 83. -. "On the Origins of the Greek Hexameter: Syncbronic and Diachronic Perspectives.", in: Studies in Diachronic, Synchronic and Typological Linguistics. (festschrift o. SZEMER!NYI) Amsterdam 1979, 611-31. 103 NEUBOURG, L. DE"Le nom de ladsure aprb le 3- demi-pied de l'hexametre", Pallas XXV (1978) 3-7. 415 n. 93. -, "Mots longs apres les ditr~ mtdianes dans l'hexametre grec", AC XLVII (1978) 403-37. 416 n. 100; 417 n. 106. NEWTON, B.E. "Metre and Stress in Greek", Phoenix XXIII (1969) 359-71. 400 n. 23. NOOlDEN, L.P.A.S.VANTemporal Coherence in Tone Sequences. Diss. Eindhoven 1975. 397 n. 11; n. 15. NOOTEBOOM, s.G. & COHEN,A. "Anticipation in Speech Production and Its Implications for Perception", in:NOOTEBOOM & COHEN(edd.), Structure and Process in Spttch Perceptwn. Berlin 1975, 12445. 398 n. 16; n. 18. T. VANTht Homeric Hymn to Herma. A Study in Early Grttk Hexamettr Style. Diss. NORTWICK, California 1975. 73. NOTOPOULOS, J.A. "The Homeric Hymns as Oral Poetry", AJPh LXXXIII (1962) 337-68. 73. OLCOTT, M.D.Mnrical Varialions in tht Iambic Trime1tr as a Function of Dramatic Technique in Sophocles' Philoctetes and Ajax. Ann Arbor/London 1974. 122; 157; 158; 164; 232; 423 f.n. 146; 424 n. 148. o'NEILLJr., E.G."The Importance of Final Syllables in Greek Verse", TAPA LXX (1939) 256-94. 418 n. 113. -, "The Localization of Metrical Word-Types in the Greek Hexameter. Homer, Hesiod and the Alexandrians.", YCS VIII (1942) 105-78.32; 44; 80; 81; 82; 86f.; 90; 91; 93 ff.; 98 f.; 409 n. 52; 415 n. 98. o'NOLAN,K. "Homer and the Irish Heroic Narrative", CQ XIX (1969) 1-19. 103. OTT, w. "Metrical Analysis of Latin Hexameter- The Automatation of a Philological Research Project.", in: Linguistica Mattmatica t Calcolatori. Firenze 1973, 379-90. 41. PACE,G.B."The Two Domains: Meter and Rhythm.", PMLA LXXVI (1961) 413-9. 399 n. 19.
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
440
o.w. "Metrical and Grammatical Patterns in the Greek Hexameter", in JONES. A. & (edd.), TM CompUllr in Literary and Lingwtic Studia. Cardiff 1976, 85-91. 73; 413 n. 83. PAUEI, L.P.E. "Porson's Law Extended". CQ XVI (1966) 1-26. 427 n. 164. -. "Greek Metric 1957-1970. ", Lusmun XV (1970) 37-98. 400 n. 23; 429 n. 173. -, Ree. ALLEN (1973). JHS XCVII (1977) 191-2. 400 n. 23. PAUY, M. L •Epithlte tradilionnelle dans Homen; Euai sur WI problmu! de style homlrique. Paris 1928. (Also in PADY, M. TM Making of Homeric Ver.se. Oxford 1971, 1-190: "lbeTraditionaJ Epithet in Homer".) 73; 211 n. 68. -, La Formuks et la mltriqu.e d'Homln. Paris 1928. (Also in PADY (1971), 191-239: "Homeric Formulae and Homeric Metre".) 63. -, "The Distinctive Character of Enjambment in Homeric Verse", TAPA LX (1929) 200-20. (Also in PAIJlY (1971), 251-65.) 65; 68. -, "Studies in the Epic Technique of Oral Verse-Making. I: Homer and Homeric Style:•. HSCPh XLI (1930) 73-147. (Also in PAIJlY (1971), 266-324.) 73; 414 n. 85. PAULSON, 1. Lukrezstudit'n. /: Die lussere Form des Lu.lcntianischen Huamner.s. Goteborg 1897. PACKARD,
CHUICHHOUSE, I.I'.
408 n. 48. c.o. "L'origine micenea della tradizione epica rapsodica", SMEA 1980, 341-52. 103.
PAVESE,
TM Winged Word. A Study in the Technique of Ancient Gllt'k Oral Composilion as Sun Principally through Haiod's Works and Days. New York 1975. 68; 411 n. 73. PELCKMANN, 1. Venus choliambi apud Grattos et Romanos historia. Diss. Greifswald 1908. 431 n. 184. PEIJlO'ITA, G. "II poeta degli epodi di Strasburgo", S/FC 1938, 3-41. 257. PEIUSINO, F. "Tecnica e stile nel tetrametro trocaico di Menandro", RCCM IV (1962) 1-22. 329. PICKAID-CAMBltlDGE, A. Dithyramb, Tragedy, and CorMdy. (2 1111 rev.ed. by T.B.L. WUSTEJt). Oxford 2 1966. 325. PLATNAUEI, M. "Dactyls in Comic Trochaics", CR LXV (1951) 132-3. 329. PLEW, E. "Eine Eigentumlichkeit des Nonnischen Versbaus", Jahrbucher fur Klassische Plulolog~ XIII (1867) 847-52. 416 n. 102. PONTE, A. DEL "Lycophronis Alexandra: Versificazione e mezzo espressivo.", SIFC N.S.UII (1981) 101-33. 423 n. 145; 425 n. 157. POISON, a. Euripidis H«uba. Cambridge 1802. 248. PORTEI, H.N. "Hesiod and Aratus", TAPA LXXVII (1946) 158-70. 91. -, "The Early Greek Hexameter", YCSXII (1951)3-63. 44; 71; 75;80;81; 86f.;400n. 22;404 n. 27; 408 n. 45; 414 n. 88; 415 f.n. 98. POSTLETIIWArrE, N. "Formula and Formulaic: Some Evidence from the Homeric Hymns.", Phoenit XXXIII (1979) 1-18. 73. POWELL, 8.8. "Word Patterns in the Catalogue of Ships (B 494-709): A Structural Analysis of Homeric Language.". Herma CVI ( 1978) 255-64. 73. PRATO, c. "L' "enjambement" nei tragici greci", in: Studia Rorentina, A. IONCONI oblata, Roma 1970, 349-55. 160. -, "Lingua e ritmo nel verso recitativo di Euripide", in: Siu.diLinguutici Salenlini Ill, Scritti in memoria di 0. PAILANGEU, Lecce 1970, 237-44. 122. -, FILIPPO (t>tal.) Riurcht sul lrimetro dei trogici gnci: Metro t' vtr.so. Roma 1975. 121; 122. -, (et al.) Ricerche sul trimttro di Menandro: Metro e verso. Roma 1983. 422 n. 132. l'IESC01T, u.w. "The Position of 'Deferred' Nouns and Adjectives in Epic and Dramatic Verse", CPh VII (1912) 35-58. 67. PREZIOSI, P.O. "The Homeric Hymn to Aphrodite. An Oral Analysis.", HSCPh LXXI (1966) 171204. 73. PULORAM,E. "Attic Shortening or Metrical Lengthening?", GlottaLIX (1981)75-93. 410n. 66;418 n. 112. PYE, o.w. "Wholly Spondaic Lines in Homer", G&:R XI (1964) 2-6. 63. RAALTE, M. VAN & SICK.ING,C.M.J. Ree. NAGY (1974). Bibliotheca Orit'ntalis XXXVII (1980) 38617. 103. PEABODY, 8.
REFERENCES ANDAUTHOR INDEX
441
IAUSCHEJt, G. In lllCHAJtDSON, N.J.
sclwliis Homericis ad ~m metricom sp«tanlibus. Diss. Strassburg 1886. 63. The Homeric Hymn to lnmettr. Oxford 1974. 409 n. 52; 412 n. 78. am:HIE, w. The AlllMnticily of lM Rhesus of Euripides. Cambridge 1964. 418 n. 117; 419 n. 121. ROCHE, J. LA "Die Stellung des attributiven und appositiven Adjcctivs bci Homer", WS XIX (1897) 161-88. 67. -, "Zahlenverhlltnisse im homerischen Vers", WS XX (1898) 1-69. 31; 39; 40; 409 n. 50. -, "Untersucbungen ubcr den Vers bci Hesiod und in den homeriscben Hymnen", WS XX (1898) 70-99. 31; 32; 40. -, "Der Hexameter bci Apollonios, Aratos und Kallimachos. ", WS XXI (1899) 161-97. 31; 40; 83. -. "Zur Verstechnik des Nonnos", WS XXII (1900) 194--221.31. ROSE, H.J. A Handbook of G~lc Liltratun. London 7 1964. 430 n. 180; n. 181. ROSSI, L.E. Mttrica t crilicastilistica:II krmino 'ciclico't l'a'Y'JTYTI ritmica. Roma 1963. 30. -, "Estensione e vaJore del 'colon' nell'csametro omerico", Studi Urbinali XXXIX (1965) 23973. 71. RUCKMICH, c .A. "A Bibliography of Rhythm", American Journal of Psychology XXIV (1913) 50819; XXVI (1915) 457-9; XXIX (1918) 214-8; XXXV (1924) 407-13.395 n. 2. RUUGH, c.1. L'l/lme,u achltn dans la langut lpiqut. Assen 1957. 412 n. 76. -, "Observations sur la ·metath~se de quantite' ", Lingua XXI (1968) 382-99.61. -. Ree. WYA1T (1969), Lingua XXVII (1971) 263-76. 61. -, Ree. ALLEN (1973), Mn XXXIV (1981) 399-409. 401 n. 23. RUSSO, J.A. "A Ooser Look at Homeric Formulas", TAPA XCIV (1963) 235-47. 73. -. "The StructuraJ Formula in Homeric Verse", YCS XX (1966) 217-40. 73; 413 n. 83. -, "Is 'Oral' or• Aural' Composition the Cause of Homer's Formulaic Style?", in: srou, a.A. & SHANNON, R.s.(edd.). Oral Liltratw"tand tM Fol'1ffUUI, Ann Arbor 1976, 31-71. 413 n. 82; n.
83. "L'hexa~tre du lapidaire orphique: pour une etude metrique des 'Orphica'.", RPh LV (1981) 73-90. 32; 81; 87. SCHEIN, S.L. The Iambic Trimeter in Aeschylus and Sophocles. A Study in Mttrical Form. Leiden 1979. 105; 107; 121; 122; 123; 134; 160; 161f.; 163; 194; 208; 248; 270; 406 D. 36; 419 n. 123; 422 n. 138; 425 n. 153; 426 n. 160; 428 n. 167; 430 n. 182. SCHULZ, H. "Methoden und Aufgaben einer zukunftigen Metrik", in: Spracht im technischtn Ztilalttr XLI ( 1972) 27-51. 400 n. 20. scon, J.A. "The Influence of Meter on the Homeric Choice of DisyUables", CPh IV (1909) 24855. 61. -, "The Ethos of Dactylic and Spondaic Verses in Homer", CJ X (1914/5) 326-30. 63. SECKEL, D. H6lderlins Sprachrhydunus. Mil tintr Einltilung abtr das Problem da Rhythmu.s und tintr Bibliographit zur Rhythnws-Forsclumg. Leipzig 1937. 400 n. 20. SEVEllYNS, A. Homirt, II: Lt p.nov) 157; 221; and clausula 158ff.; and monosyllable in pos. I 216 Trochaic Tetrameter: 336 Enjambcmcnt of the Metric Colon 177; 186; 285; 388 Epidlannus 422 n. 129
Eplrrhematk Scenes and low incidence of resolution 325; and verses without a ceasura 344f. 'Equjvalenc:e'of- and vv (29f. ); 382{. Eubulus SEE Comedy, Middle Eptanm, Hellenlstic(A""°'°Jia Grotta) comparatively large proportion of short syllables in the anccps positions 106; in pos. 5, and single hephthemimeral caesura 112: in pos. 9 109, and low incidence of word-end at pos. 10 110 proportion of resolutions comparable with the Sophoclean trimeter 122; comparatively high incidence of resolution in pos. I 130; resolutions listed 229 comparatively large proportion of hephthemimeral caesurae without concomitant word-end at pos. 5 180; clausula not unlike that of early iambography 208 EntOldlenes SEE Hellenlltk Poets, (FnlllllfJll&ary)
·~·
of verse-types in the dactylic hexameter, hypothetical 63f.; in the iambic trimeter. ,dem418f. n. 117
EupborionSEE Helleultk Poets, (Fnpneatary) Euripides SEE also Tnaedy Iambic Trirneter: remarkable difference in frequency of resolution in different (earlier/later) plays 122 comparatively high incidence of word-end at pos. 5 166; 169; id. of penthemimeral caesurae 180; low incidence of word-end al pos.7 169; comparatively few occurrences of caesura media 194 later plays: proportion of short realizalions of the anceps in pos. 9 decreasing 109; remarkable increase of resolutions 122; 125; of resolutions followed by word-end 242 increasing frequency of 'falling double-short' verse-beginning 142; larger proportion of elision at caesura 167; comparatively high incidence of verses with word-end at both pos. 10 and pos. 8 207
452
RHYTHM AND METRE
Trochaic Tetrameter: relatively large proponion of shon realizations of anceps in pos. 4 314; 31 'l; comparative infrequency of verse-types with three long ancipitia 319; increase of resolution 324 comparatively small proponion of monsyllables in colon-initial verse-positions 353 Orates shows word-end at poss. 3 and 5 more often than IA 348; IA higher incidence of wordend at pos. 4 3 I 7; 348: greaterrhythmical strictness in Or. ofthe second colon as well 349: 352 Effllt, Rllythmical SEE Stlmul• Es:pedadonSEE also Reaulartty;Predktablllt)';T_.. anticipation of rhythm based on awareness of metric profile 13; 403f. n. 27; and colometry 5; 387; and rhythmic divergence 13; 14; 381 Expnsslveaet11SEE also Stylilda, Rll:,tlunbl of caesura media 194
Extemlon, Rllytlunkal by means of prolongation 29; 105
F...... Rll)'tbmSEE Movemenl, Rb)'thmical Foot SEE Metre I FormulaicDiction 28 n. and rhythmic defects 62f.; verse-end 64; enjambement 67ft.; and monosyllables at verse-end 90; and caesura 72f. FreeVerN 2; 5; 396 n. 6; 397 n. 15; 398 n. 18
Genendve Metrics pretension of universality not legitimate 40lf. n. 24; tree structures inapplicable to Greek verse rhythm 401 n. 24; and formulaic diction 413 n. 83 Grid, Metric SEE also Vene--Prollle;Metre D 7;8
Hellodorus 276 Hellenistk Heumeter SEE DaclyllcHeumeter (Hellffllsdc) Helleutk Poets, (Frasmmtary) 32; 92; 409 n. 53; trochaic caesura more frequent than penthemimeral caesura 414 n. 87 Antagoras 409 n. 53 Eratosthenes 409 n. 53; large proponion of verses with two spondees 409 n. 49 Euphorion 409 n. 53; n. 56; 416 n. 103; occurrence of holospondaic verse 409 n. 50; bucolic dihaeresis comparatively infrequent 87 Hegesianax 409 n. 53 Theopompus 409 n. 53
Herodu rhythmical similarity of his choliambi to the trimeters of tragedy 2n; 288; follows Hipponax only occasionally 278 realization of ancipitia more like Hipponax than like Callimachus 266; comparatively large proportion of long syllables in pos. 5 265; effect of pseudo-cholosis 265; percentage of resolution similar to that in the trimeter of Aeschylus· earlier plays 270; resolutions located at the beginning of the verse especially 272; resolution in pos. 2 generally preceded by a long syllable 274 practice of internal structuring similar to ( esp. Aeschylean) trimeter 278: word-end at pos. 10 288; at both pos. 10 and pos. 8 309; beginning of verse 290; penthemimeral caesura with concomitant
GENERALINDF.X
453
word-end at pos. 9 only an alternative for id. with word-end at pos. 7 .303; monosyllable in pos. 6 less frequent 284; infrequency of caesura media 287; low incidence of word-end in pos. 8 288; evident breach of Porson's Law 302; resolution in pos. 4 followed by word-end, and general high frequency of word-end at this verse-position 299
Heslod rhythmical similarity to the Homeric hexameter (diversity of verse-types) 53; 54; more spondaic verses 5l ; penthemimeral caesura in gnomic passages 75f. Thtogonia relatively high percentage of unperiodic enjambement 69; increase of penthemimeral caesurac in second part of the Erga 14; 15 Hladua prosodic index of verse-end 16f.; rhythmic 'defect' 62
Hippoaax small average of words per verse 279; comparatively large proportion of long realizations of the anceps in pos. S 264; and pseudo-cholosis 265; comparatively large proportion of short syllables in pos. I 265; incidence of resolution similar to that in Aeschylus' earlier plays 270; located in the beginning of the verse comparatively often 272; one occurrence of double resolution 271; resolu• tion in non-marked verse-positions 272, and subject-matter 272; notorious for rhythmic divergen• cies other than resolution 276 low incidence of bisyllables in poss. 6 and 7 278f.; virtual absence of penthemimeres with concomitant word-end at pos. 7 279. and frequency of words reaching from S to 9 279; 289; 302; .305:large proportion of single hephthemimeral caesura 279; scarceness of monosyllables in pos. 6 284; wordend at pos. 10 comparable to the siruation in Archilochus 289; incidence of word-end at pos. 8 somewhat lower 289; observance of Porson ·s Law only apparent 302; .303;no clausular refinements .305ff.; some occurrences of word-end at both pos. 10 and pos. 8 289; 308f. Holodadyllc SEE Dactylic Hexameter HololpondaJcSEE Dactylic Heumettt; Homer Homer hexameters characterized by diversity 53; all possible verse-types are actually found 52 (all possible spondaic verse-types 5 I); holospondaic verses 34; 409 n. 50; greatest variety of word-types involved 91 Hymas.Homeric treated as a homogeneous corpus 28 n.; rhythmical similarity to the Homeric hexameter (diversity ofverse-types) 53; 54; more spondaic verses 51 Hymn to Apollo alleged bipartition 28 n. Hymn to Arts 28 n. Hymn to Dtmtttr large proportion of spondees 409 n. 52; stronger types of enjambement relatively frequent 69 Hymn to Htrmts unperiodic enjambement relatively frequent 69
Iambic Metroa SEE Metre I Iambic Trtmeter metric profile 12; 19; 20; 105; metron structural unit 105; 384: 'basic' metric profile 12; schema of comic trimeter vs. schema of tragic trimeter 9f.; compared with trochaic tetrameter 336; 345; 348f.; 431 n. 192 realization of ancipitia IOSff.: verse-types 115ff., and 'ethos' 418{. n. 117; resolution 14; 120ff.; increase of total number 122; incidence of resolution per verse (multiple resolution) 126f.; distribution over verse-positions. becomes more even 129; rhythmical context 140ft. rising verse-beginning 20; and long realization of anceps in pos. I 20; and resolution in pos. 1138. in pos. 2 140; caesura 19: 20; 161ff. falling second colon 20, and resolution in pos. 6 20; caesura preceded/followed by bisyllabic 17lff.; 175ff.; 179; penthemimeres followed by a monosyllable 186; caesura media 193ff.; clausula 207f.: clausular return to (rising) single-short rhythm 21; 110; 149; rising double-short 149; bridge 23l ff.
454
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
lambognplty, Early SEE also ArdlBodll•, SemDllldea,Solon
Iambic Trimeter: high frequency of short syllables in pos. 9 109; 248; 257; low incidence of resolution 122; always at the beginning of a word 226 remarkably small proportion of elision at pos. 5 167; comparatively high incidence of word-end at pos. 7 169; no verses without either penthemimeral or hephthemimeral caesura (accordingly no caesura media) 193; clausular effect by means of word-end at pos. 10or at pos. 8 alternatively 207; 256f.; further refinements 252ff.; clausular rhythm and the comparatively low average of words per verse 256 Trochaic Tetrameter: word-end at pos. 11 and pos. 13 alternatives 349; 375 lmltadoa, Rbydunlcal of Homeric practice. or shared preference? 54; Aratus and Hesiod 91 1111111 SEE also 'Etllol' 183 lndo-Europeu Oripl (of Dactylic Hexameter) cannot be established 103 latep'adoa of metrical groups: and realization of anceps 315; and caesura 18f.; 23f.; 70 (trochaic caesura in dactylic hexameter maximum of integration 75); 231; 385; absence of integration in trochaic tetrameter 386, integration of metrical groups within cola 386
•••llm
Latin
the Latin hexameter and statistical research 41; smaJJer proportion of dactylic sequences in Latin prose 408 n. 48; smaller proportion of dactyls in the Latin hexameter 408 n. 48 l..ea&tbmJnl SEE also PbOROltylt lengthening by position 60f.; 418 n. 112: omission of lengthening by position 61; 62 vowel lengthening 61
Lycophroa trimeters characterized (rhythmical monotonousness) 123; alleged kinship with trimeters of early iambography 208 largest proportion of long syllables in the anceps positions l 06; I09; anceps in pos. 9 also realized as a long syllable more frequently than as a short one 109; small proportion of resolutions 123: resolution preferably in pos. 6 129f.; listed 229; invariably preceded by word-end 226 general scarceness of elision 167; 221; not specifically in the case of monosyllables 221; comparatively large proportion of hephthemimeral caesurae without concomitant word-end at pos. 5 180: high incidence of word-end at pos. 10 208; comparatively few trimeters with word-end at both pos. 10 and pos. 8 208; comparative frequency of three-word trimeters 183; hyperbaton 162 Lyric Poetry (Greek) 2; 5; 12; 396 n. 6; and the origin of the hexameter 103; til.oyia 30 drama: linear variation 15; 380; 405 n. 30 unpredictability of rhythm 15f.; rhythmic phrase vs. metric phrase ("gleitende Uebergiinge") 405 n. 32, 'dovetailing' 386
Markednea SEE also Prominence
rhythm of ancient Greek based on syllable length plus/minus marked for quantity 12f.; 380 Meander Iambic Trimeter: greater diversity of verse-types than in Aristophanes 152; but his verse more uniform 152 resolution in pos. 10 less rare 138 (preceded by word-end comparatively often 226); comparative preference for falling double-short verse-beginning 141; 15lf.; 155; 427 n. 163 larger proportion of elision at caesura 167; comparatively free realization of verse-end 138; 160
GENERAL INDEX
455
Trochaic Tetrameter: somewhat lower incidence of resolution than in his trimeters 325; frequently preceded by word-end 359; word-internal resolution in pos. 7 comparatively frequent 368; small proportion of elision at caesura 337; no increase of verses without a caesura 337;comparatively small proportion of monosyllables in colon-initial verse-positions 353; comparatively large proportion of monosyllables in colon-final verse-positions 353; monosyllables found without elision comparatively often 355. connected with non-colon-initial monosyllables 355 'Metathesis Quand1at1s• 61 Mtttt I (MetricalRhytbm)SEE also l■tesntion defined 3 (see also 5f. ); lff.; 4; 27; 380; vs. Metre II ('metrical' vs. ·metric') 6ff.: lerminology used by modem authors 396 n. 7; metric profile easily recognized 13f. basic or simple metrical group ('foot'): dactylic metron 18; 23f.; 29: constitutive of iambic & trochaic metron 12; 109f.; 384; 385; 406 n. 36; 'pseudo-feel' 375 complex metrical group (iambic. trochaic metron) 12; 25f.; 105. 109f.; 312; 385 Mtttt II (Vene.Prollle: Metric Lenl) introduced: sequence of verse-elements 6ff.; 13; 26f.; 381; terminology used by modem authors 8; 399f. n. 20; rhythmical norm 6; }Off.;26; and rhythmic divergence 13ff.; 381ff.; and metric bridge 21f.; 389; explained in terms of rhythmical awareness 13; easily recognized (only) in the case of internal or external responsion 13ff., in the case of metrical verse especially 15 and rhythmical movement 16ff.; caesura included in metric profile 19; metric group and rhythmic group 385ff. Basic (Simple) Metric Profile. defined 12; and Porson's Law 25; 248: 302f.; 391; and clausular refinements 257; 309 Metric Colon 19f.; 130; lTI; 186: 285; 388; metric unit of perception 387. may serve elsewhere as unit of composition 19; 387 Metric Proflle SEE Mtttt II MetrlcalRhythmSEE M~tt I 'Metrlcal1ty• 401 n. 24; 404 n. 27 µhQOV SEE also Metre I 400 n. 21 xata µhQOV 8 MlmlambusSEE Choliambus Monosyllable defined 416 n. 99 phrase-initial: at the beginning of a syntactical phrase 87; 216: and elision 216; 221: in Semonides 112; and split resolution 232ff.; and Porson's Law 249 frequency at the beginning of a rhythmical (sub-)phrase 87; 293; 353; and elision 216; 221; 225; 293ff.; 297; 355 increasing proportion of monosyllables without elision. and decreasing proportion of phraseinitial monosyllables 216; 355 at verse-end. mannerism 88f.; 218ff.; 353; comparatively low incidence of elision 216; emphasis 2l8ff. Dactylic Hexameter 87ff. Iambic Trimeter 216ff.; in poss. 6 or 7 186ff.; 191; and Perrotta's Bridge 258; in pos. 1. wilh enjambemc!nt 216, proportion of elided words smaller 216; in pos. 5, pos. 12 216; 218ff., comparatively low incidence of elision 216; in pos. 9, and Porson's Law 249 Chol iambus 293ft.; in pos. 6 284ft .. comparalively rarely with enjambement of the metric colon 2X5; in pos. 5 294; not a single occurrence in pos. 12 295 Trochaic Tetrameter 352ff.; in pos. 8, in pos. 15 353
456
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
MecorT'beory 3; 395 n. 2; 404 n. 27 Mofflllell(,, Rlaydunkal 16ff.; and rhythmical coherence 27: rising/falling, pendant/blunt 18; 29; single-short/double-short 14; 403 n. 26; applies at both verse-levels 7; 18; contrast metric vs. rhythmic level 19;130 caesura 18ff.; alternation of rhythmical movement, and integration of metrical groups 18; 25; 70; 75; 166; 385 dihaeresis, and absence of alternation of rhythmical movement, in catalectic verse 336, 386 clausula 20f.; return to basidinitiaJ movement, and coincidence of word- and metron boundary 21; 25f.; St; 53; 83f.: 2fJ7ff. alternation of rhythmical movement, in catalectic verse 349; 386 bridge,rhythmical: concerns internal structuring and modulation of rhythmical movement 22 23ff.; 92ff.; 248ff.: 30Sff.; 37lff.; 389ff. Dactylic Hexameter: long final syllable in non-marked verse-position produces pendant. verse• finaJ effect 23f.; 29; 94; 99; 102; 390 Iambic Trimeter: ·underlining' of rhythmical movement of second colon by bisyllable in poss. 6 and 7 17lff.; 175ff.; 'underlining' of rhythmical movement characteristic of metric profice by word-end at pos.2 213; word-end afterresolution with reinforcement of preferred rhythmical movement 237f.; 240f. Trochaic Tetrameter: rhythmical movement opposite to that in iambic trimeter 21: 29; 348f. Matk musical phrasing 398 n. 17; possible role in identification of metric profile in Pindaric odes 15. of caesura in Pindaric odes 407 n. 39 recitative 324f.; 344f. ambiguity in music, compared with ambiguisty in choral lyric 405 n. 30
NaaOI dactylic hexameters show tendency to rhythmical uniformity 30; 52; 53; SS incidence of spondees as low as possible 33, and use of uncontracted word-forms 30; high average of dactyls per verse 30; no verses with more than two spondees 33; most of spondees in the second metron 36; 37; 42; comparatively few in the first metron 37; comparative frequency of verses with a single spondee which occupies the fourth metron 44; avoidance of two consecutive spondees 48; 55 large proportion of trochaic caesurae 73
Oral C4Nllpoadoa SEE FormulaicDidion
OrderSEEReplartty 'Orphic' Hexameters 32; caesura 81; bucolic dihaeresis 87 Overlap rhythmic group and metric unit of composition 385ff.; in metrical verse this implies an alternation of the rhythmical movement 25; 'dovetailing' 386 rhythmic group and metric unit of perception 193; 387 Enjambement: completeness of rhythmical sequence, incompleteness of linguistic utterance 388: SEE also Styllltlcs. Rhytlunkal
Paratragoedia
412 n. 125; and realization of ancipitia 115; and resolution 122f.; and the alleged observance of Porson's Law in Middle Comedy 250
GENERAL INDEX
457
Panaenklea poverty of verse-types, variety of distribution 53; comparatively great divergence from Homeric order of verse-types 55; cognitive subject-matter 31 comparatively low percentage of holodactylic verses 34; high frequency of verses with two spondees 31; 33: relative frequency of symmetrical and alternating verse-types 45; 48; dactylic realization of first metron comparatively favoured 37; spondaic verses relatively rare 52 clausular rhythm neglected 44; 84 Parody, Rhytlunlcal 139 Paroemlac and penthemimeral caesura in dactylic hexameter 75f. Par1tclesSEE also Moneayllablt; Appolklfftl elided particle and 'artificial' lengthening of syllable 60 >«llfollowing trochaic caesura 411 n. 67; 417 n. 106 Pmdant SEE Movemeat~Rhythmical PerceptionSEE also Rhythm I; T~ perceptual theory 8, and rhythmic divergence 13 linear perception of rhythmical phrase 5; 15 PboaOllyle phonostylistic variation 403 n. 25; rhythm-independent nature doubted 163 lengthening of final syllables not corroborated by Dionysius of Halicamassus 408 n. 45; relevancy to the rhythm of classical Greek questionable if on purely phonological grounds 163 and ·prosodic bridge' 407 n. 41; and resolution 419f. n. 124: 325; and split resolution 426f. n. 161; and elision 424 n. 148; and caesura 202f.; and Porson's Law 419f. n. 124; 427f. n. 166 initial/medial syllables 39 Pllra1e, RhythmJc:al SEE Vaw; Colon Pllnsln1 SEE also Movement, Rhytlunlcal manners of rhythmical phrasing 398 n. 17 rhythmic phrasing SEE R.llythm II: Rhetorical Phrasing Plndar metric profile odes often unclear 15: 405 n. 31; internal structuring idem 401 n. 39 ......
Dtstrll,atlea
and multiple resolution 126ft.; 271; 328 Ponon'sLaw SEE Bridp, R.llytlunbl Pndk:tability SEE Replarlty; Expect.adon Prefb-Boundaries and caesura 203 Pnposidves SEE Appoeidffll Prolonptioa SEE Emndoa, R.llytlunbl ProminenceSEE also Manedlle• 3; optimal time interval between two prominent stimuli 4 Proper Names and resolution 120f.; 135; 329; and formulae 90; 414 n. 85; n. 86; and verses without a caesura 82 PnaRhytbm 2; 56; 380; not rhythmical in the strict sense of the word if based on rhetorical contrast only 388 Proee-Sample quantification of syllable-types 102: proportion of long and short syllables 106: length of words 17lf.: 394 PrmodySEE also Quadty; Pllonoltyle; Bridp, Prmodk; ............. Sllor1ealna; ElllcNa 59ff.; prosodic indices ofvene-end 16f.; 423 n. 141; prosodic neutralityofverse-final syllable 17; rhythmic divergence not always to be distinguished from prosodic peculiarities 276 diphtongs 3 I : 229; 276
458
RHYTHM Ahl> HETRE
Quudfk:adem SEE also Aulytk Table olConteau: TABLES; ........s.mp1e and textual criticism 31 ; and prosodic options 31 ; and types of word-boundaries 162ft. Dactylic Hexameter: dactyls and spondees 3 If.; verse-types 41; caesura 79ff.; bucolic dihaeresis 86f.; monosyllables 87; 90; bridge 93; 97 Iambic Trimeter: 104; ancipitia 107; verse-types 117: resolution 123; 127; 135; l43f.; 146: wordend 162ft. Choliambus: 262 Trochaic Tetrameter: 312; verse-types 322 Quality (Duration) 3; binary opposition of syllabic quantity distinctive of Greek verse rhythm 12: 380: 401 n. 24; not affected by cdoyw 30; multi-valued scale criticized 402f. n. 25; 426 n. 159 (SEE also Bridge. Prosodic); to be distinguished from speech tempo. pbonostylistic variation 403 n. 25; no bars in classical Greek poetry 397 n. 14: proportion of long and short syllables in prose-sample 106 Maximum Time Interval SEE c..-e.e Optimum Time Interval. no conditio sittt qua non 4
Redtative SEE Music llKurnace of stimuli: maximum time limit 3; vs. successiveness 4 of prominent stimuli 3; 10; rhythm involving only a single recurrence of a prominent stimulus 2; 4; 5 of groups of stimuli 2; simple/complex 2; invariable recurrence of identical groups (metrical rhythm) 3; 5; 7; no recurrence of identical groups 8; vs. linear variation 15 Stichic Recurrence 17 Replarlty involves predictability 3; often based upon perception of temporal equivalence 4 degrees of regularity 3: 4; 380; in the verse rhythm of classical Greek 10; 13
R..audon defined 14; of marked clement, double contrast 38lff.; of non-marked element, single contrast 382; outside comedy mostly of marked elements 22: 382 and phonostyle 324f.; 419ff. n. 124; and colometry 19; 20; 192; and proper names 120f.; 135; 329: and vocabulary 122f.: 144; and subject-matter 122 preferably at the beginning of a word 145; 213; 226ff.; 297; 359; split resolution avoided 22 (SEE also Bridp, Metric): followed by word-end with rhythmical motivations 237ff.: 299f.; 366f. absence of resolution of the longa in the dactylic hexameter 29f.; 382f. Iambic Trimeter: 120ft.; multiple resolution 126ft.. no general avoidance of double resolution 126; in non-marked verse-positions 22; 120; l 38f.; 382, in pos. 1 22; 138; distribution over the several verse-positions becomes more even 129; in stricter genres most frequent in colon-initial versepositions 20; 129; avoided in two successive verse-positions 127; 140; rarely in pos. 10 137f. followed by word-end: gradual decrease of rhythmical motivations 242; not particularly often with elision 242; no avoidance of 'split anapaests' 242; increasing proportion of word-internal resolutions 245; no tendency to equality of colon-length 192 in pos. 2, pos. 3. and word-end at pos. 2 213; 237f.; in pos. 6, bisyllabic words 238f.; in pos. 8. pos. 10. and lrigoin's extension of Porson's Law 254f.; pos. 4. pos. 8 in comedy 240f. Choliambus: 270ff.; occasionally also in non-marked verse-positions 270; 272; no avoidance of double resolution 27 l; especially frequent at the beginning of the verse 272. and cholosis 272 majority of resolutions word-initial, but less frequently than in iambic trimeter 297; in pos. 4. followed by word-end (Herodas) 299f.; word-internal resolution comparatively frequent 300 Trochaic Tetrameter: 324ff.: in non-marked verse-positions only in comedy 324; 329; 382: increase of proportion. Aristophanes excepted 324f.; no avoidance of multiple resolution 326; especially frequent at the beginning of the verse 327. and catalexis 327; in pos. 3. pos. 5, pos. 11, followed by word-end 366f.; proportion of word-internal resolutions smaller than in iambic trimeter 367f.
459
GENERAL INDEX
Respouion and expectation of rhythmical shape 5 Internal Responsion 13; complete (acatalectic verse) 14; 29; 385; 390: incomplete SEE Catalnls, Metrlal External Responsion 15; 405 n. 29; srichic 17; 405 n. 29: strophic 16: 405 n. 29; and identification of caesura 407 n. 39 Rhytlun I (General Notion) defined 3; lff.; 379; 380; 388; "rhythmical", vs. "rhythmic" (Rhythm II) 6ff. rhythmical groups (metrical and non-metrical) lff.; 6f.; 14: absence of groups 14f. characteristics of Greek verse rhythm 12f.: 380; 401f. n. 24; 402 f. n. 25; pitch accent irrelevant 12 primary and secondary rhythm 2; objective and subjective rhythm 2: 404 n. 27; metrical and nonmetrical rhythm 3; SEE also Metre I rhythmical movement SEE Movement, Rhythmical rhythmical stylistics SEE Styllsdcs, Rhytlunkal Vent: RhythmicLevel) SEE also Word-Boundary;Word-Type;Moaosyllable; Rhythm II (COIK'l"ete
Bisylable introduced: sequence of syllables 6ff.; 26f.; 381; terminology used by modem authors 8; 399f. n. 20 rhythmic divergences from metric profile 11; 13f.; l 9f.; do not affect perception of rhythmical movement 19f.; SEE also Di'ffflflltt, Rhythmic rhythmic defects as measured against the metric norm 62f.; 276 rhetorical phrasing 175f.; 176f.; 189; 387ft.; SEE also Styllsdcs, Rhythmical Rhytlunkal Groaps SEE Rhytlun I
Rhylhmldans on Q).oy(a Qu6µ~ 400 n. 21
Risina Rhythm SEE Movement, Rhythmical
S.ndllt-PhenommaSEE also ~I series interrupted at verse-end 17; frequency of a particular .. liaison syllabique" at bucolic dihaeresis no independent significance 86; 101
Satyr Drama excluded from quantifications 123: 421 n. 126 Scanston, Automalk SEE Stadsdcs
Schema Introduced: sequence of symbols 9f.; schema of metric profile 9. normative 10; "the" schema of a type of verse includes all possibilities 9 SemomdesSEE also lambography, Early comparatively large proportion of long syllables in pos. I 109. and phrase-initial monosyllables 112; 216; 418 n. 116; comparatively large proportion of short syllables in pos. S 109; Ill. and 'hephthemimeres only' 111: large proportion of hephthemimeral caesura without concomitant word-end at pos. S 111; 180 Shortenlna, Vowel 61; 62: correption 229; in Archestratus 31 cn«i~wv SEE ChollamlHls Solon SEE also lamboanplly, Early Iambic Trimeter: comparatively large proportion of short realizations of ancipitia 106; especially in pos. 9109. and low incidence of word-end at pos. 10110; comparative frequency of word-end at pos. 8 207
460
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
Trochaic Tetrameter: sample very small 313, et passim comparatively large proportion of short realizations of ancipitia 313. in pos. 4 314; verse-types low correlation with those of other tetrameter poets 319 SophoclesSEE also Tnapd:, Iambic Trimeter: no chronological development in frequency of resolution 122; 419 n. 123; low incidence of resolution in pos. 8 130; comparatively high incidence of resolution in pos. 6 not preceded by caesura 131; resolution in pos. 10, followed by word-end 240 preference for deferring syntactically significant word-boundary over metric boundary 131; violent enjambement(dbo(; Ioq>inc.Anov)l57ff.;enjambement of metric boundary and positioning of phrase-initial monosyllables 221; rhythmical strictness of second colon 130.contrast situation in first colon 213; high incidence of elision 167; 221, produced by monosyllables with elision 221 Trochaic Tetrameter: sample very small 312, et passim high incidence of resolution 324; low incidence of a bisyllable in poss. 9 and 10 339f.; absence of colon-internal variation of movement in the first colon 345. strict realization of the second colon 345: word-end at both pos. 13 and pos. 11 remarkably frequent 349; monosyllables in pos. 1 without elision relatively often 355 Span of Atteadoa SEE also Time Interval
1 'Split Ana,-u' SEE ReNladoa SpoocWc Vent, versus spondiacus SEE Dactylic Hexameter Spoaclft SEE also Dact:,lk Hexameter defined 408 n. 46; 29ff. in third metron, and caesura 44; 74 Statillks automatic scansion 41 of rhythmical phenomena 41; verse-types (dact. hex.) 41; random incidence of multiple resolution
126 statistical aspect of caesura 82, bridge 21 Sddlk Vent SEE also Rec:wrence;llapa•h•• Edenaal identity of verse-elements, internal structuring relatively distinct 16; 27 O"dXO(;lax~OQQWYuc.6; 264 Stimall, Audkery events constitutive of rhythmical sequence 2ff. conditions for plurality 3; identification (metric bridge) 2lf.; prominence 3 non for rhythmical experience 2f.; recurrence recurrence of identical groups not a condilio siM qU1.1 of identical groups ( = metrical rhythm) 3; 13 Stroplle
5
StructuriaaSEE
Colon;MMement, Rh)'lluDk:al
St:,lildcs, Rhydunkal defined 162; 388; 392; and rhetorical contrast 388f. Dactylic Hexameter: 9lf. Iambic Trimeter: l6lf.; 175f.; 186f.; 218 Trochaic Tetrameter: 339f.; 353{. Subject-Matter adaptation oftechnical ('cognitive') terminology, and average number of dactyls 30f.; and distribution of spondees 37: and spondaic verses 52, symmetrical verse-types 44f.; and resolution 122 Succetlllveoas SEE also Stimuli, Audkery 3; vs. recurrence 4; concatenation 396 n. 6 Sarface-Structaft SEE R.laytlunII
Syllable marked vs. non-marked 5; in classical Greek: plw vs. minus marked for quantity (long vs. short)
GENP.RAL INDEX
461
12; 380; variations in performance rhythmically not distinctive 12; 30; differences in phonological
shape no rhythmical significance 30 possible coherence with semantic level 226f. quantity of verse-final syllable irrelevant 17 Symbols, RhytbmlcalSEE also Sdlelu XVlllff.; 9f. Symmetry, RllytbmlcalSEE also VetW-Types symmetry of verse-types. instrument of emphasis? 45
s,..._. 61
TempoSEE aJso Pllonoltyle "Vortragstempo" 4; vs. quantity 402f. n. 25: of holodactylic verses, and a).oyla 30 Telllioa,Rbytbmlcal expectation not fulfilled 38 l; 383f.; 432 n. 198; prosodic continuity vs. rhythmical discontinuity (elision at caesura) 169; enjambement of metric colon 186; 388; caesura media 193; 195; 387; split resolution 232; 381
Tbeoc:ritm comparative frequency of hexameters with a spondee in the first metron 37: 43; 56; frequency of bucolic dihaercsis 84; distinction epic and bucolic hexameters Q n. 47 Epic Hexameters: comparatively great resemblance to the Homeric hexameter 55; greater diversity of verse-types than bucolic hexameters 53; comparatively high average of dactyls per verse 30; comparatively large proportion of verses with a single spondee 30f.; relatively large proportion of trochaic caesurae 73 Bucolic Hexameters: remarkable divergence from the Homeric order of verse-types 54; comparatively low percentage of holodactylic verses 34; 55; spondaic verses rare 37; 52; frequency of verses with a spondee in the first two metra 47; 55; of verses with two consecutive spondees in general 47; frequency of verses with a spondee in the third metron 54, and low incidence of trochaic caesura 54f. ; 73 (Mimic Hexameters: compara1ively many exceptions 10 Naeke's Bridge IOI) Tll.ree-WordTrtmeten SEE Word-Types
Time lntenal maximum time-limit ('span of attention') I; 3; 4; interval between two prominent stimuli, with 'optimal' length 2. no conditio sin~ q1111 non for the perception of rhythm 4; 5; impression of equivalence (undercstimalion and overestimation of time intervals) 4
Tone,.Sequences auditory research 2
Trapdy Iambic Trimeter: first two ancipitia more frequently realized as a long syllable. the third one as a short 109; virtually complete correlation of verse-types of the tragic poets 115; increase of resolutions 122; distribution over different verse-positions becomes more even 129; highest percentage of resolution in pos. 6 130 clausular effect by means of word-end ar pos.10. pos.8. or both 207 Trochaic Tetrameter: correlation of verse-types of tragic poets comparatively low (in virtue of Sophocles· practice especially) 319
•Tramladon', Rhythmical rhythmic sequence 'translated' into metric profile 11
Trocbak MetronSEE Metre I Trocbak Tetrameter metric profile 312; catalexis 18; 312; 386, and the definition of ·metrical rhythm' 6; compared with iambic trimeter 336; 345; 348f.; 391; 431 n. 192
462
RHYTHM ANDMETRE
nature of passages involved 300f. realization of ancipitia 3l2ff.; large proponion of long syllables in pos. 8 314. and caesura 314; large proportion of shon syllables in pos. 12 314f .• and clausula 315; anceps in pos. 4. and integration of metrical group 315; .371; verse-types 318ff.; resolution 324fL resolution more frequent than in iambic trimeter 324; increase of incidence. Aristophanes excepted 324f.; resolution in anceps position exceptional even in comedy 313. in non-marked versc-positions in general 324; 329; resolution mostly colon-initial (esp. at the beginning of the versc) 327 caesura 336ft.; •fixed' caesura 336; 386; involves metron dihaeresis 336; 349; 386; ·asynartete' 386; bisyllable in poss. 9 and lO 339f .• and clausula 340; tetrameters without a caesura 341ff.; coloninternal rising movement involves variation of the basic rhythm 345; 349; 391; bridge 362ft.
Variadoa. Linear vs. recurrence of rhythmical groups or phrases IS Vene (RhythmicalPbrale) S; 11; 16ff.; terminology 406 n. 33 beginning of verse tolerant of rhythmic divergence 36; 129; 382; 409 n. 51; conservatism towards end 137 structuring into two identical parts usually avoided 18 Vent-Element SEE EleaNllt Vene-Ead SEE also E"6ambemeat 16; 64ft.; prosodic indices of verse-end 16L; rhythmical indices l 7f.; 20L and bridge 26; 94; 99; 102; ·voice pause· irrelevant 157; 423 n. 141 Vene-LeYelSEE also Metn II; RlaytlulllII 6ff. Vene-PruftleSEE also Metre U 6ff.
Vene-Type1 Dactylic Hexameter 41ff.; statistical research 41; symmetrical and 'alternating· 44; 50. comparatively frequent in verses with less 'literary· subject-matter 44f.; 'ethos· of verse-types 63f. relative frequency 53ft.; all possible verse-types only in Horner 52 Iambic Trimeter (without resolution) l lSff.; in comic trimeter (resolution included) 150ff. Trochaic Tetrameter 318ft. Vent RhythmSEE also Rh:,tlamI species of rhythm 3; verse rhythm of classical Greek IO; 11; 12f.; 380 'Vene-W"sht' 105
Wilamowitzlan• 380; 432 n. 196 Word BoundarySEE also Phoaoltyle irrelevant to quantity of syllable 163; 401 n. 24 differentiation of word-boundaries 162ft .. potentially-structural 162; 164ft.; questionable effects of distinction 162; 196 at verse-end 16; 64; caesura 18(.; lexical phrasing 398 n. 17; clausula 18; 20f.; bridge (low incidence of word-boundary) 21ff. supposed concentration on beginning of word. and resolution 226(.; alleged phonological properties of word-beginning 39; 428 n. 170; of word-end 408 n. 45 contrast/coincidence with metric boundary 384ft. Word-EndSEE Word-Bouadary 0
GENERALINDEX
463
Word-Fomu option for particular word-form and preference for daclylic or spondaic realization 59f.; restrictions on use of word-forms enforced by metric profile 59f.; word-forms coined 61 Word-Types 60: 171: vs. words 163 Dactylic Hexameter: variety of word-types 91f.; increasingly limited 91: long word-types. and verses without a caesura 82; word-types and bridge 93ff. Iambic Trimeter: extension of variety in Euripides' later plays 141; word-types adjacent to caesura l 71f.; 174; preceding single hephthemimeral caesura 185; three-word trimeters 183 Choliambus: word-types and realization of the anceps in pos. 5 269 Trochaic Tetrameter: word-types and verses without a caesura 341ff.
uugma SEE Brldp