172 32 6MB
English Pages [257] Year 2019
Haunted Data
Also available from Bloomsbury Metanoia, Armen Avanessian and Anke Hennig Medialogies, David R. Castillo and William Egginton A History of Light, Theresa Mikuriya Against Transmission, Timothy Barker Retroactivity and Contemporary Art, Craig Staff Enduring Time, Lisa Baraitser Reparative Aesthetics, Susan Best
Haunted Data Affect, Transmedia, Weird Science Lisa Blackman
BLOOMSBURY ACADEMIC Bloomsbury Publishing Plc 50 Bedford Square, London, WC1B 3DP, UK 1385 Broadway, New York, NY 10018, USA BLOOMSBURY, BLOOMSBURY ACADEMIC and the Diana logo are trademarks of Bloomsbury Publishing Plc First published in Great Britain 2019 Copyright © Lisa Blackman, 2019 Lisa Blackman has asserted her right under the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act, 1988, to be identified as Author of this work. Cover image © Andreas Töpfer All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, or any information storage or retrieval system, without prior permission in writing from the publishers. Bloomsbury Publishing Plc does not have any control over, or responsibility for, any third-party websites referred to or in this book. All internet addresses given in this book were correct at the time of going to press. The author and publisher regret any inconvenience caused if addresses have changed or sites have ceased to exist, but can accept no responsibility for any such changes. A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library. A catalog record for this book is available from the Library of Congress. ISBN: HB: 978-1-3500-4704-4 PB: 978-1-3500-4705-1 ePDF: 978-1-3500-4703-7 eBook: 978-1-3500-4706-8 Typeset by Deanta Global Publishing Services, Chennai, India To find out more about our authors and books visit www.bloomsbury.com and sign up for our newsletters.
Contents Acknowledgments Preface: Affect, Transmedia, Weird Science Part One Priming and Networked Affect: Data Mediation and Media Contagions 1 2 3
Transmedial Storytelling, Weird Science and Archives of the Future Social Media Contagion(s): An analysis of Priming Controversies within Cognitive Science Data-Mediation and Hauntological Analysis: The ‘Clever Hans Charge’
vi viii
1 3 28 53
Part Two Feeling Futures: Mediating Futures
79
4 5 6 7
87
Feeling the Future Pornception and Big Data Open Science and Quantum Matters Conclusion: Affect and Archives of the Future
Notes Bibliography Index
106 123 166 183 209 223
Acknowledgments The idea for this book came out of a short paper I gave at the Max Planck Institute in Berlin at an event, “Experimental Entanglements”, organized by Felicity Callard and Des Fitzgerald in October 2012. Thank you to the both of them for inviting me. The event brought together humanities scholars and neuroscientists to discuss collaboration and interdisciplinarity through foregrounding and questioning experiment-as-practice. It became very clear that although there was an appetite for new methods and ways of working this was thwarted by huge chasms that were preventing the emergence of something genuinely new and unexpected within the innovation of experimentation. Drawing on my own training in science and the humanities I wanted to pursue this problem further, particularly at the intersection of science studies, affect studies, weird science, debates on datafication and feminist and queer theories. The focus on data within the context of weird science was developed initially at the Compromised Data Colloquium held at the Social Innovation Centre, Toronto Canada in 2013. Thank you to Greg Elmer for the invitation. The first foray into affect studies at this conjuncture came from an invitation from Greg Seigworth to give a keynote at the inauguration of the first Affect WTF conference in 2015. Thank you to Greg for his on-going support and enthusiasm for my work. Greg is a rare breed of academic who thinks collectively and with the utmost generosity. Since then I have presented the research to anomalistic psychologists at Goldsmiths. Thanks to Chris French for the invitation and for going some of the ground with me. Thank you also to Samantha Frost for inviting me to contribute to the “Experiments in Thinking the Human” symposium at the University of Illinois in 2017, which further convinced me of the value of engaging with new materialism within this context. Thanks also to the Wellcome Trust and particularly Emily Wiles for the invitation to the “Out of Control” symposium in 2016, which brought the weird and wonderful into science in such a productive and engaging way. Thank you also to the numerous invites I have had to Copenhagen to present different parts of the research. This includes invites by Christian Borch and
Acknowledgments
vii
Justine Grø nbæ k Pors at the Copenhagen Business School, and Carsten Stage and Britta Tim Knudsen at Aarhus University. Thank you to Jan Slaby for the invite to present material as part of the Affective Relationalities conference at the Freire University Berlin; Gary Hall for the invite to present as part of the PG Meccsa conference on “Transformative Practice and Theory: Where we Stand Today”. Thanks also to my Finnish collaborators including Tuula Juvonen for the invite to the University of Tampere, Leena Rouhiainen and Esa Kirkopelto for the invitation to give a keynote at the “Perilous Experience? Extending Experience through Artistic Research” at the University of the Arts, Helsinki (and our ongoing collaborations). Also thanks to fellow ghost-hunters and artists Sarah Sparkes, Kjersti Sundland, Stephen Fortune, Sarah Wood, Birgitta Hosea, Outi Condit and Shona Illingworth. Thanks also to Frankie Mace, Liz Thompson and Monica Sukumar at Bloomsbury. During the finalizing of this book I have been co-Head of the Department of Media, Communications and Cultural Studies at Goldsmiths. This has curtailed my research time and ability to travel but has incorporated a renewed focus on action plans, strategic documents and cascading: thanks particularly to my co-Head Joanna Zylinska who has made a brilliant ally, colleague and friend (with much good humour) and to Sean Cubitt, Julian Henriques and Hugh Macnicol for the “DMT experience” while I was Deputy and to Sean for the year when we shared the Headship; also thanks to all my colleagues in the department who make the department the world renowned place that it is. Lastly a huge thank you to my partner Isabel Waidner who through her own creative writing makes the weird into techniques for world-making. She is just a brilliant person whom I love dearly.
Preface: Affect, Transmedia, Weird Science Things, places, people, objects, music, memories, buildings can all be haunted. Haunting evokes images of visitations by ‘things’ not of this world; this might include the dead and aliens. One iconic image of an alien visitation that evokes images of alternate imaginaries and virtualities is the image of the late David Bowie appearing in the film The Man Who Fell to Earth. Bowie falls to earth on a mission to save his own species dying from a lack of water as a result of a catastrophic drought. Throughout the film, as well as being out-of-space (extra-terrestrial), Bowie’s character, Jerome Newton, is also presented as outof-time, represented perhaps by his androgyny and enviable fashion sense. Although the alien in this context is aligned to extra-sensory perceptions, superior intelligences and technological prowess the ending is all too human. Through the exploitation of the alien by the human, Jerome Newton is exposed, cheated and incarcerated such that his mission is thwarted by alcoholism and depression. He is made ‘thing-like’, outside of human connection, and as a hybrid human-alien life form discloses the intimate cultural connection made between the alien and psychopathology. Newton becomes haunted by persistent telepathic images of his own family dying and his failure to return home and save his species. The film explores the etymological connection between haunting and home,1 and what it might feel like to not feel at home in one’s surroundings, milieu, country, planet or even body, a familiar theme to many who experience their own embodiment as ‘thing-like’. This haunting persists in his own torment and anguish made worse by Newton’s addiction to alcohol, which does little to quell his troubles and anxiety. This book explores the themes of haunting and being haunted within the context of alien phenomenologies. I am using the term ‘alien phenomenology’ as it has been specified within psychiatry and cognitive science (the field of automaticity research more specifically) to describe experiences where people feel as if they are being directed, moved, possessed or haunted by someone or something else. The body is often experienced as ‘thing-like’.
Preface: Affect, Transmedia, Weird Science
ix
Automaticity research is, broadly speaking, concerned with processes that exist below the threshold of conscious awareness and attention. In the more conventional sense of the term, ‘automaticity’ explores processes that feel automatic, that might become habitual and that do not demand our attention. However, automaticity research also focuses on experiences that explore how we can be made to do things without being consciously aware (so-called unwilled action), and to that extent has a much more controversial side. Automaticity research brings together all kinds of enigmatic behaviours and puzzling phenomena facing modern psychology and often falls under the rubric of weird science. This includes hypnotic suggestion, trance states, voice hearing, motor automatisms (including involuntary muscular movements), various contagious phenomena and ‘actions that are so remarkably divorced from a feeling of doing’ (Ansfield and Wegner, 1996: 483) that they are often attributed to supernatural forces. The focus on ‘alien phenomenologies’ includes experiences of suggestion, contagion and imitative processes that are shared and distributed across the human and more-than-human. They invite a renewed focus on registers and modalities of attending to the world that exceed conscious rational thought, or that exist at the edges of consciousness. They are ‘alien’ if one subscribes to psychological individualism and clear and distinct borders and boundaries between self and other, human and technical, material and immaterial, affect and cognition, and body and society. As will become clear, this assumption is perhaps untenable, unwise and even in many cases does harm. The term ‘alien phenomenologies’ has also been used in speculative philosophy to describe the phenomenology of what it might feel like to be ‘thing-like’ within the context of objects and object-oriented ontologies. Ian Bogost’s (2012) book, Alien Phenomenology, or What It Is Like to Be a Thing argues for a phenomenology of things beyond the human. Also see Steven Shaviro’s provocative book Discognition (2015), and particularly the chapter ‘Thinking like an Alien’, which considers the question of alien consciousness through Peter Watts’s novel Blindsight (2006). This book extends this work beyond the human by exploring alien phenomenologies within the context of software cultures and digital archives. It does this by taking two science controversies, which cross cognitive science,
x
Preface: Affect, Transmedia, Weird Science
speculative philosophies, anomalistic psychology and the field of affect studies. These controversies focus on alien phenomenologies, which at the same time take shape within digital archives that are haunted by submerged narratives and displaced actors and agents. These archives re-move (that is put back into circulation) what haunts contemporary science within this area. The book mines the potential of this ghost-data thus returning the hauntological dimensions to studies of the media and mediation. This is done primarily within the context of computational culture and software media and to the question of the difference that digital media are making to the evolution of science. The hauntological dimensions of analogue media have been written about before, not least in Jeffrey Sconce’s book, Haunted Media: Electronic Presence from Telegraphy to Television (2000), and John Durham Peter’s evocative book, Speaking into the Air: The History of the Idea of Communication (1999). These books, as well as the writings of the media archaeologist Stefan Andriopolous, including his two seminal books, Possessed: Hypnotic Crimes, Corporate Fiction, and the Invention of Cinema (2008) and Ghostly Apparitions: German Idealism, the Gothic Novel, and Optical Media (2013), have all been sources of inspiration for the book’s themes. In different ways, all of the aforementioned books draw explicitly from the relations between spiritualism, psychic research (entities, practices and imaginaries), forms of ghosted consciousness and the material-technical shaping of media such as telegraphy, television, film, radio and so forth. Media have always been haunted and haunting and the themes of haunted media live on in discussions of twenty-first-century media and computational culture. This includes Mark Hanson’s book Feed-forward: On the Future of 21st Century Media (2015), which seeks inspiration from the writings of the early twentieth-century philosopher Albert North Whitehead to explore the ghostly qualities of mediation as it takes form within software and computational practices. This book explicitly explores the challenges data and software media pose to theories of mediation, developed within the context of older forms of media, and extends this within studies of the cultural and affective politics of data. The term ‘data’ is of course a vexed term referring to different practices (software, informational, statistical, etc.); different theoretical and disciplinary traditions (software studies, sociology, science, etc.); different platforms (Facebook, Twitter); and different ways of imagining the relations between
Preface: Affect, Transmedia, Weird Science
xi
people, things, entities and power relationships. What we mean by data, what counts as data, and how to study the cultural politics of data is a pressing concern and one that this book attempts to tackle. I am, however, not approaching data as code although the psychological and psychic imaginaries that shape code are part of the story. I am not approaching data as monetized or mathematized although some of these conceptions enter the story I will tell. The story of data I will tell is one which primarily explores data hauntologically and analyses how software-driven encounters, transactions, traces and practices, which take place within media environments understood as digital or data-driven, return media to its ghostly dimensions. Some have argued that data is now a ubiquitous presence in our lives and that in different ways we live with data, imagine with data, feel with data and even anticipate futures, which have yet to come. This includes that we are living in the midst of what Patricia Clough and others (2015) have called the ‘datafication’ of society. The term ‘datafication’ is an important attempt to move beyond some of the utopian and dystopian fantasies associated with the ubiquity of data – that is that data either changes everything, or repeats and extends forms of power that are part and parcel of older forms of surveillance and regulation. Datafication draws attention to what exceeds human capacities of measurement and meaning – to the ‘noise’ in the system, to the incomputable, which sometimes allows for novelty, creativity and the generation of the new (also see Clough, 2018). This book is situated within these debates that explicitly concern the challenges of twenty-first-century media and computational cultures to our understandings of the media, mediation, representation, affect, power and subjectivity. The book specifically looks at how software-driven transactions are changing scientific innovation, progress, discussion, review, debate and the nature of consensus and controversy. It explores how both computation and science share a hauntological dimension. In order to explore this, the book takes a field of scientific experimentation that has caught the interest of many humanities scholars interested in contagion, imitation, suggestion and processes and registers of experience more broadly associated with the field of affect study. The ‘turn to affect’, as it is sometimes known, will be discussed throughout the book, particularly as it intersects with debates on networked affect and the question of how fads, fashions, beliefs, emotions, moods and so
xii
Preface: Affect, Transmedia, Weird Science
on might spread across social media and networked culture in ways that invite analyses of ‘contagious relationality’ (Sampson, 2012).
Affect We can see in writings about affect2 by media theorists, philosophers and cultural theorists that there is a renewed focus on registers that exceed conscious rational thought or that exist at the edges of consciousness. This includes recognition that normative conceptions of self-determination and psychological autonomy occlude questions of how power works in registers that are never simply conscious or rational. As many scholars across the social sciences and humanities have argued, ‘philosophers and critics have largely neglected the important role our corporeal-affective dispositions play in thinking, reasoning and reflection, then it seems to follow that an account of affect and its place in our lives and institutions is called for’.3 We encounter affect in descriptions of architecture (in terms of atmosphere and as immersive, immaterial), in discussions of objects as enchanted and captivating, in discussions of social media and networked affect4 and the question of what gains a reach and traction and why, and in relation to political and governmental practices and policies. This includes the relationship between post-truth politics and the registers of emotion and feeling. Across a broad rubric of disciplines, which cross the arts, humanities, social, human and natural sciences, there is a renewed interest in how our experiences might be understood, targeted and modulated via processes understood to exist below the threshold of conscious attention. These processes open the subject to modalities of power and mediation understood to be suggestive or operating with the potential for contagion or imitation, for example. They invite consideration of what it might mean to govern through affect or what I term processes and practices of ‘psychomediation’.5 The logics underpinning these strategies of governance draw from the psychological sciences and particularly theories, concepts and understandings which have attempted to understand the suggestive capacities of human subjects – turning attention to processes that are assumed to not be accessible to conscious awareness or control. It is at this shared interchange and intersection between the psychological sciences and affect studies that the book is situated.
Preface: Affect, Transmedia, Weird Science
xiii
Data and affect The book specifically develops an affective approach to data, which attends to what exceeds more quantitative and often instrumental approaches to big and small data (see Chapter 2). I argue that data bear the traces of human, material, technical, symbolic and imaginary histories that are often displaced and occluded in data metrics. The case studies analysed throughout the book and the affective methodology that puts them into circulation foregrounds the relationship between science and storytelling.6 Although storytelling might not usually be associated with either science or computational culture, the book makes an argument that storytelling, or what Donna Haraway (1990) has called the relationship between science and fiction (or science-fiction), is central to understanding what counts as science and how it takes form. This relationship is further illuminated by Haraway’s more recent shaping of the feminist practice of ‘speculative fabulation’ (Haraway, 2013) as a transdisciplinary strategy for intervening within science and world-making. My particular approach to the relationship between science and storytelling will be framed through the concept of transmedial storytelling (see Chapter 1). This is a form of distributed storytelling, which specifically allows a consideration of the difference digital media make to the potential evolution of science, and our capacity to make sense of controversies. This book will make an argument that suggests that both science and computational culture are haunted by both the histories and excesses of their own storytelling and that these excesses surface in ‘queer aggregations’ or haunted data to be mined, poached and put to work in newly emergent contexts and settings.7 The book points to the propensity of straight or legitimate science to sanitize, excise or even exorcise narratives, actors, agents and entities, which ‘contaminate’ it with queerness. The ‘datalogical turn’ also invites a reformulation and reconfiguring of important questions about the nature of subjectivity, of the human, and what counts as a digital subject within the context of data and computational culture.8 A current trend addressing some of the distinctiveness of computational media is to turn to older theorists of media and communications. These were primarily developed and shaped prior to the internet and social media. This might include Erving Goffman’s dramaturgical approach to the self, or
xiv
Preface: Affect, Transmedia, Weird Science
nineteenth-century philosophers and theorists who were concerned more with early media technologies, such as Gabriel Tarde, for example.9 At the same time the canons and historical a prioris10 of established disciplines are being challenged by new configurations of cross-disciplinary knowledge formation. These configurations include tactical alignments across different disciplines and forms of analysis, which aim to unsettle the boundaries of knowledgepractices and disciplines in order to open to the new and unexpected. Mark Hansen, for example, moves through and across nineteenth-century process and vitalist philosophies, phenomenology and post-phenomenology, affect theories, the non-human turn and contemporary media theory. This allows him to reread Albert North Whitehead, an early twentieth-century philosopher as a contemporary media theorist. One question explored throughout this book is what other kinds of unorthodox or tactical alignments of disciplines, sub-disciplines, theories, perspectives, figures, archives, entities and practices would help us probe into the complexity of twenty-first-century media, the cultural politics of data, and the question of what counts as a digital subject within the context of what I am calling haunted data. My contention is that there is much to be gleaned from bringing together the fields of science studies, affect studies, the non-human turn, with queer theories, feminist approaches to automation, new materialisms, hauntologies, and some of the diverse and differing genealogies of subjectivity that exist on the margins of many disciplines and philosophical perspectives. The book specifically takes its cue from a number of feminist and radical philosophers of science and from science studies scholars who have developed innovative approaches to science, which cross philosophy, science and culture. They have explored the histories and genealogies of science within specific historical, cultural, political, technical, psychological and symbolic conjunctures. This includes attention to the historical a prioris, which have shaped the philosophies and practice of science, including what counts as an experiment, experimentation and evidence, as well as the close interdependence and interrelationship between cultural configurations of matter-meaning and the materialities of scientific cultures. Specifically, I have been inspired by the writings of Isabelle Stengers, Vinciane Despret, Donna Haraway, Karen Barad and Hans-Jö rg Rheinberger. The work of the philosophers Stengers and Despret have been of particular
Preface: Affect, Transmedia, Weird Science
xv
importance given their focus on experiences and phenomena that are often considered strange, weird, outside of reason, or as presenting challenges to established scientific orthodoxies. In their book Women Who Make a Fuss: The Unfaithful Daughters of Virginia Woolf (Stengers and Despret, 2014) they recount how the subject matter they have focused on, which includes ‘hypnosis, addicts, witches, the Arabian babbler, peasants, the uneasy dead’ (p. 15), are topics that are not considered serious or worthy of interest for most conventional philosophers. I would add that they are also considered anomalous within psychology, where the designation ‘anomalous’ works to mark out what is understood as falling outside of conventional psychological understandings and explanations. The definition of ‘anomalous’, that which deviates from what is standard, normal or expected – the aberrant, freakish, odd, bizarre, peculiar and unusual, captures the experience and phenomena that are under investigation in this book. ‘Anomalous’ practices, phenomena, experiences and entities haunt two contemporary science controversies within psychology, which have crossed into the mainstream primarily via social and digital media. The content of the controversies and their ‘alien phenomenologies’ will be introduced in Chapter 1. They both allow new stories to be told about affect, emotion and the psychological, which question the borders and boundaries between the psychic and the psychological, the rational and the irrational, the self and other, truth and falsehood, the material and immaterial and the corporeal and incorporeal. Vinciane Despret’s engagement with the psychological sciences and the ambiguities, puzzles and anomalies that can be found historically and in the present imaginatively show how we need approaches that unsettle the polarizing logic that often frames debate and scientific investigation in this area. This includes explanations which focus either on proving the existence of phenomena or undermining them as evidence of so-called false belief: is it real or unreal, true or false? In the area of anomalous psychology the scientist is often cast in the role of judge and juror attempting to close down on the ambiguity, hesitations, puzzling curiosities, and what continually resists current scientific explanations. As she has argued in relation to the case of ‘Clever Hans’,11 a talented horse who challenged psychological ideas, theories and practices at the turn of the nineteenth century, there are much more
xvi
Preface: Affect, Transmedia, Weird Science
interesting questions to be asked and speculative propositions to be explored. Hans reappears in one of the controversies staged in the first half of this book, raising questions about the policing, shaping and emergence of concepts of psychological individualism and subject–object bifurcation, which are challenged throughout the book. My companions in thought (Ahmed, 2014) allow me to situate and speak to the problem of the ‘psychological’ and what counts as psychological processes, entities and matters when the psychological is not considered separate from contiguous processes, including the historical, technical, ecological, symbolic and affective. The conversation and dialogue that these authors make possible throughout the book is unorthodox and tactical. It disrupts many of the border wars that police the boundaries of different intellectual heritages and philosophical perspectives. Some of these perspectives aren’t usually brought into conversation, or if they are it is often through antagonisms, suspicion, refusal, or by simply ignoring dissent through specific citational practices. Rather than being interdisciplinary in nature the book is therefore rather undisciplined and to that extent is aligned to many queer theorists and queer theories, which refuse the straight path.12 I approach data primarily as a non-expert (particularly in software studies and computational culture), as a non-philosopher (although the Feeling the Future controversy explored in Chapters 4, 5 and 6 engages with the philosophies of statistics and speculative philosophies), and as a scholar who has spent the last twenty-five years working in sociology and media and cultural studies within a very interdisciplinary context. The value of the undisciplinarity that I inhabit with its queer and feminist orientations, and my previous contributions to critical genealogies of psychology and affect, are important for the stories I am able to tell.
Questions and challenges The book is centred around and responds to four main questions which relate to the broad areas that the book is situated within: data, affect, weird science and transmedia. These areas connect up timely questions related to science, governance, subjectivity, data and the question of what difference digital media
Preface: Affect, Transmedia, Weird Science
xvii
and computational cultures are making to our lives. The first question comes out of a much earlier tradition of work, which has explored and examined the role of the human sciences in the government and management of human and even non-human life. This work, which has been indebted to the thinking of the radical philosopher Michel Foucault, has analysed how scientific concepts, theories, practices and experimental forms of life have shaped the conditions under which specific human subjects have emerged. This work, as we will go on to consider in Chapter 1, has been shaped in the context of disciplinary forms of power, which have formed the basis of technologies of the social, such as the prison, hospital, asylum and school. These technologies for governing life have circulated the norms through which conduct, behaviour, thought and feeling have been judged, evaluated, categorized, measured and circulated. They have also circulated in different ways as techniques of selfinspection and self-production. The book raises the question of whether and how science and governance are changing in the context of digital media and digital forms of communication. What are the consequences thereof for how we might understand and examine new technologies of power, and specifically psychological and affective forms of governance, as they extend and are extended within software and computational cultures?
Engaging with science These two questions are posed against a backdrop of calls for humanities scholars to take the sciences more seriously and to see them as potential allies or ‘critical friends’ rather than as opponents or antagonistic partners. It is being argued that there are too many schisms between and across the sciences and the humanities, and that there are now new opportunities for rapprochement and for interdisciplinary and collaborative forms of inquiry. Some of these arguments suggest that there are emergent ontologies, which can be found in the sciences and humanities, which destabilize well-rehearsed arguments that have positioned the sciences as deterministic, reductionist and as reinforcing already existing inequalities and oppressions. These new opportunities are captured by new fields of study, which cross the humanities and sciences, including the biosocial and the biohumanities. Some of the scholars who are
xviii
Preface: Affect, Transmedia, Weird Science
making these arguments have also come out of traditions of research that in the past have been more critical of scientific ontologies and experimental forms of life. This includes the work of the sociologist Nikolas Rose, and of Felicity Callard and Des Fitzgerald in the context of the neurosciences;13 Elizabeth Wilson in the context of the cognitive and psychological sciences;14 and arguments made by sociologists such as Maurizio Meloni who argue that sociology is now more open to biological suggestions just at a time when biology is becoming more social.15 As the philosopher and software studies scholar Tiziana Terranova (2004) also suggests, what are viewed as non-deterministic approaches to materiality are providing renewed opportunities for humanities scholars to forge alliances with the natural and human sciences. She asks, ‘Is it possible to draw on scientific concepts to further our understanding of cultural processes?’ (2004: 51). There are also now a slew of books and special issues of journals devoted to ‘biosocial alliances’ (see Meloni et al., 2016). This includes the coining of terms such as the ‘biocultural’ (Frost, 2016), ‘political biology’ (Meloni, 2016), or ‘New Biologies’ (Blackman, 2016), which are taken to indicate something explicitly new about the present conjuncture. The question of whether and how to forge alliances between the humanities and the human and life sciences is, of course, not new, even if there is apparently something distinctive about the present conjuncture, which brings the potentiality of possible collaborations sharply into focus. This shift in the positioning of science relates to the identification of common ontologies emerging across the sciences and humanities, which emphasize the complex, processual, indeterminate, contingent, non-linear, relational nature of phenomena constantly open to effects from contiguous processes. These arguments are being advanced in relation to the fields of genetics and the biological sciences (including epigenetics and the microbiome), mathematics, quantum physics and the physics of small particles, the neurosciences (particularly the social and critical neurosciences), affect theories across media and cultural theory (see Gregg and Seigworth, 2010, Clough, 2008), new materialisms (Coole and Frost, 2010), as well as the neurosciences of affect and emotion (see Wetherell, 2012). These common ontologies are grounded in concepts such as biosocialities (Rabinow, 1996), naturecultures (Haraway, 2003), entanglement (Barad, 2007; Wilson, 2015), assemblage, flow,
Preface: Affect, Transmedia, Weird Science
xix
turbulence, emergence, becoming, relationality, intra-action, co-evolution, co-emergence, the machinic, to name just some of the heuristics and new biosocial languages being deployed. In their wake, relationships between the social and the natural, the mind and body, the cognitive and the affective, the human and the technical and biology and identity are being reformed. In some ways the engagement with science and specifically psychology and the neurosciences (and particularly the neuroscience of affect and emotion) by humanities scholars is not new. In a book I wrote with Valerie Walkerdine in 2001, Mass Hysteria: Critical Psychology and Media Studies, we were intrigued with how theorists who staked a claim for their radical thinking and were critiquing psychologism often drew on psychological concepts and theories through the back door and I might add in rather conservative ways. Then the debates were framed around postmodernity and the postmodern (turns which had a similar feel to the turn to affect), and we remarked on how psychological concepts were being taken up within postmodernist cultural theory, in the writings of Baudrillard, Lyotard and Frederik Jameson, to theorize conditions of experience under postmodernity. Some of the concepts used were schizophrenia, autism (a subject trapped within their own silence), flattened affect and the invocation of silent and frozen masses that we find in Baudrillard’s work, for example. What was curious about these theorists was that, on the one hand, they disavowed modernity and psychology (as grand narratives based on universalizing experiences and depth models, for example), while, on the other, they used psychological and even psychiatric concepts and theories to describe experiences within postmodernity. We remarked how much these theorists were dependent upon psychological terminology and concepts despite their disavowals and how this led to wild overgeneralizations and prohibit the study of precisely what subjectivities were formed within any specific historical and cultural location. I think we can see similar problems within the field of affect study, which is not helped by the arguments made by many scholars that subjectivity is now obsolete and redundant as a concept and field of study. The second controversy analysed in the book Feeling the Future suggests that this is not the case and that the issue of how to investigate mind–matter relations remains a problem for affect studies, new materialisms, speculative philosophies and for the psychological sciences.
xx
Preface: Affect, Transmedia, Weird Science
As we argued back in 2001 (making reference to trends in the 1990s), cultural theorists are, on the one hand, all too ready to dismiss psychology, while, on the other hand, making completely questionable psychological assumptions in their own work. We argued that these moves displayed a wearisome disinterest in the critical work that had been going on within and outside psychology for a long time, as well as freely adapting terms from psychology to give their work a spurious authenticity. I think these arguments could be made in relation to some of the debates within the field of affect study and particularly those that dismiss subjectivity, while at the same time replacing subjectivity or critical approaches to subjectivity with a neurophysiological body, or sometimes with more quantum approaches to matter. As a trained psychologist who left the discipline to work in sociology and then media and cultural theory, I have retained an interest in what experiences, phenomena, entities, processes and practices have been marginalized within psychology. I want to bring these interests and experiences more directly to bear on these issues and to specifically address what a critical reconfiguration of psychological matters might add to the fields of affect studies and the emergent field of the biohumanities (see Frost, 2016). This book is therefore also an attempt to open up what I call ‘speculative psychologies’, which might draw from new materialisms, affect studies and critical theorizations of subjectivity at the intersection of the non-human, more-than-human and post-human.16 My focus will be on what is excluded from straight psychology and from the historialities of psychology’s disqualified and submerged pasts – what remain as outliers, anomalies and puzzles, which sometimes register as abnormal perceptions or signs of psychopathology. Within the book these moments appear as haunted or ghostly data to be followed, re-moved and put back into circulation within the context of contemporary philosophy and cultural theory. This diffraction of psychology through the weird, strange, ridiculous and ludicrous is offered as a strategy for the playful contamination of science. This strategy has more in common with what psychology might have become if it had constituted psychological processes as fundamentally entangled, indeterminate, processual phenomena. These archives of the future exist in psychology's disavowed past and return in the book as insistent traces to be mined, poached and put to work in new and newly emergent conjunctures and context.
Preface: Affect, Transmedia, Weird Science
xxi
The politics of data The third question asks what kinds of critical research can be done in the context of software and computational cultures. In recent years the politics of data, its social and cultural life, and the new methods cultural theorists might need to analyse this have been posed as central issues for the humanities and social sciences (see Clough et al., 2015; Ruppert, Law and Savage, 2013; Manovich, 2013; Gitelman, 2013). The politics of data open up the question of exactly what counts as data, especially in the context of the multiple media transactions that register our presences, both in work and play, or as many have argued, those which blur the boundary between work and play (Gregg, 2011). These transactions leave traces, which potentially accrue ‘after-lives’ (see Fuller, 2009). As Beer and Burrows (2013) suggest, data acquire their own social lives, becoming lively in ways that are difficult to see, comprehend and analyse using conventional qualitative methods of inquiry. I argue that data can be extracted, mapped, aggregated, condensed, measured and translated, acquiring autonomies and agencies that extend and travel beyond the original event or transaction. Dystopian arguments present what is seen as the increasing metrics of life as the final stage in technology acquiring its own agencies and taking over. Reminiscent of nineteenthcentury and early twentieth-century dystopian anxieties, machines, and in this context, machine learning, are now governing humans in ways that are impossible to see, comprehend, understand or predict. The so-called back-end of social media, for example, provides data that is conjoined with automated practices and analyses in complex ways. These recursive relations defy calls for transparency and raise ethical questions about ownership of data and corporate agendas. Data repositories potentially create surplus value, including revenue and profit for businesses, governments, science and related actors. Particular data banks and archives are mined, often using proprietary forms of software, which can aggregate vast amounts of data in order to shape and anticipate the future – or this is at least the data-driven dream. There are many debates related to the politics of data that we will explore throughout the book. This includes the emergence of a new interdisciplinary object of study, ‘big data’, which is providing a new focus for scholars across many disciplines who are repurposing usually rather positivist methods to
xxii
Preface: Affect, Transmedia, Weird Science
make certain claims. Although scholars who share vastly different approaches to the epistemology and ontology of knowledge claims are now speaking to each other, at conferences and symposiums, for example, the axiom that governs these debates is that ‘big data’ require new forms of processing – new approaches and methods that challenge the well-rehearsed qualitative/ quantitative divide.17 Funding councils support these axioms, as much as they are invested in by venture capitalists keen to harness new automated forms of power. Positivist methods are also being remediated within digital environments by humanities scholars, often using advanced software techniques. These are pro-offered as potentially transforming research across the arts, humanities and social sciences. For some, this will potentially result in the ‘end of theory’ replacing critical inquiry with data analytics (Anderson, 2008). Techniques based on quantification have always been the mainstay of more positivist methods. However, they are now regularly offered as the solution to a range of questions that have been more central to the humanities: how to gain a purchase on questions of power, agency, subjectivity, technology and embodiment, for example. Those of us who have remained suspicious of the claims of positivism (mine due to my own training within positivist science) must however recognize that as David Beer (2013) has argued, the ‘doing of culture has changed’ (12: author’s emphasis). Beer suggests that there are four aspects to the performance and circulation of what in the past have been termed cultural processes that require consideration: archives, algorithms, data-play and the body. These are important concerns and concepts and the approach to data that I develop in this book takes these suggestions seriously. As with any interdisciplinary field of study this will require a movement across debates that are taking place in many disciplines, and which importantly are far from settled. One important question that is at the centre of this book therefore is what counts as data. What does one analyse when one takes data as a central focus of study and analysis? This question is far from clear, particularly when one moves away from instrumentalist, mathematized and monetized definitions of data and pause to reflect on what exactly can be translated into data. If data is considered a process of translation, which creates a use or surplus value from digital traces, then what exceeds these data analytics and strategies of pre-emption and anticipation? Are there other strategies open to cultural
Preface: Affect, Transmedia, Weird Science
xxiii
theorists who might not want to engage software techniques as the sole way of understanding some of what inevitably are new forms of power? Against the popular rally that big data represents the ‘end of theory’ and the capacity for humanities scholars to engage in critique, there are many adjectives being used to describe data that introduce nuanced critique and qualification into such arguments. This includes the description of data as beautiful, small, smart, anticipatory, aggregated, false, raw, cooked, compromised, lively, inert and so on. My own chosen adjective to approach data in the context of postpublication-peer-review (PPPR) is haunted. This adjective is inspired by the work of Matt Fuller (2009) in software studies. He has used the concept of the ‘after-lives’ of data, to explore the agency and autonomy of data as it moves on from the particular event that originated it and becomes active. The agency or what I call aliveness of data allows for a consideration of the social and cultural life of data, which exists beyond more instrumentalist notions of data. The concept of ‘haunted data’ is designed to disrupt the distinction between big and small data and to explore what leaves the frame if we focus solely on metrics, quantification and digital methods based on counting, measuring, aggregating and visualizing numbers. The book develops a data ethnography that is attentive to historicity and to what Hans-Jö rg Rheinberger (1994) has called historiality (i.e. the intimate connection between science and storytelling). It explores two science controversies in the area of weird science, which took form across social media and in the context of a corpus of digital communication produced by scientists. The data refers to science writing appearing in blogs, tweets, comments posted on websites, in comments offered by open-access journals linked to journal articles, in Google+ communities, Reddit bulletin boards, emails and responses to science journalists writing about the controversies and bringing them to the attention of broadcast media. As well as a data ethnography the book is also offered as a resource for affect theorists and those interested in suggestion, contagion, imitation, automaticity, the non-conscious, pre-conscious and related processes and practices. The book is part allegory of the turn to speculative forecasting and futurisms of all kinds, and the historical, ontological, technical, economic, symbolic and psychic conditions under which such imaginaries have taken form. It also develops a methodological strategy for engaging with science
xxiv
Preface: Affect, Transmedia, Weird Science
and bringing hauntology into digital environments. It is an attempt to think through networked virality or networked affect in the context of social media and the politics of small data. It is also an attempt to debate what we might inherit or actively refuse in the ways digital communication is transforming science and measures of academic value. It also provides reflection on some very unconventional psychologies of time, will, intentionality and experiences gathered together under the designation of the subliminal or non-conscious. This is taken from the perspective of what is actively disqualified from cognitive science. These dispersed interests and influences are all brought together in a single question: what does following science controversies as they take form in digital communication add to contemporary calls for more rapprochement between the humanities and the sciences?
Conclusion: The politics of change, truth and indeterminacy In this age of post-truth politics, and questions about the emotional and affective nature of change and transformation, this book situates these concerns within a novel context of scientific controversy and debate. This is linked to new forms of post-publication-peer-review, which are driven by the forms of comment, review, evaluation and discussion made possible by the digital disruption of the publishing industry. PPPR refers to a particular context of data production and circulation that has the potential to transform academic practices of writing, publishing, debate and impact. It focuses on the afterlives that academic articles and books might accrue after publication, and the ways in which the PPPR found on blogs, internet forums, social networks and other social media might enter into, intervene within and change the settings and parameters of what counts as legitimate and illegitimate debate. PPPR is a corpus of distributed data which some academic journal articles and books might accrue post publication and particularly as they might be blogged about, tweeted, discussed on websites, in comments boxes attached to science trade journals and in digital versions of newspaper and broadcast media. Journal editors, book publishers and authors in different ways see PPPR as an important measure of impact, as well as a resource to harness and extend the reach of an author’s work.
Preface: Affect, Transmedia, Weird Science
xxv
Post-publication-peer-review entangles multiple actors and agents, including social media and open-access platforms, websites, communities of academic scholars and researchers, journal editors, science journalists, media broadcasters and particular interested publics. The book explores what kind of scientific data PPPR is and how it contributes to the dynamism of science and its potential to innovate and shape new entities, practices, subjects and objects. I argue that PPPR does matter to the humanities and sciences but these extend beyond some of the current arguments being made about its significance. For example, there are arguments which suggest that PPPR is potentially changing what counts as the proper object of science and science-in-themaking, and what counts as legitimate and illegitimate entities, practices and processes. Some scientists feel the data produced by PPPR is threatening the integrity of science, whereas others argue that it has the potential to transform science, making it more open, democratic and participatory. In other contexts this might be considered part of how science is popularized, or publically communicated. I argue that PPPR provides a corpus of data that can be mined, poached and put to work in newly emergent contexts and conjunctures. This data provides the humanities scholar interested in science an entry point into some of the controversies, submerged narratives, displaced actors and disqualifications that are often covered over, edited out, discarded or exist as minor agencies within legitimate science. As we will see in Parts 1 and 2 of the book, the radical potential of PPPR has the tendency to be straightened and replaced with a form of representationalism or storytelling that is more in keeping with positivist science writing. The book will reveal this ‘politics of straightening’ or ‘business as usual’ allowing a discussion of the relationship between power, status, hierarchy and the dynamics of change – of processes of movement, indeterminacy, stabilization and actualization. Digital media may well afford the potential for change and transformation but this potential is often closed down and thwarted. Understanding the complex historical, economic, political, affective and governmental reasons for these processes of opening and closing will help us understand some of the wider dynamics of change, transformation and foreclosed possibility and epistemic uncertainties. This book provides a novel context to explore these issues, which cross the field of affect studies, affective science, new materialisms and the
xxvi
Preface: Affect, Transmedia, Weird Science
biohumanities, anomalous psychology, the digital disruption of publishing within science, and the cultural politics of data. It argues that the sciences need the arts, philosophy and the humanities in order to develop the possibility of more open, creative, adventurous and inventive science(s). It is also driven by the concern that it is important for cultural theorists to invent new ways of engaging with science, in all its ambiguities, contradictions, uncertainties, fracture-lines, hesitancies, erasures and displacements. It is a call for a Queer or even QWeird Science that can follow those data-trails or haunted trails, which point towards new visions for the future.
Part One
Priming and Networked Affect: Data Mediation and Media Contagions
2
1
Transmedial Storytelling, Weird Science and Archives of the Future Introduction: Weird science Is it possible to see into the future or even for the future to retroactively influence the past? Can experimental subjects be primed to walk to a lift more slowly after being shown words associated with ageing on a scrambled language task?1 How do we make sense of the experimental evidence, which suggests that both of these questions can be answered affirmatively? These questions are related to experiments written up in two journal articles, considered classic studies, which cross cognitive science, anomalistic psychology and psychic research (Bargh, Chen and Burrows, 1996; Bem, 2011). In different ways the studies also remediate debates within affect theories, new materialisms and speculative philosophies in the humanities. They invite a refractive method, which reads and stages texts, events, human and non-human actors and agencies, objects, entities and practices through one another (Barad, 2010). One of the studies and the controversies that ensued speak to current debates about data and computation and the question of what one analyses when data is a central focus of study and analysis. Last but not least, they both provide a springboard for addressing a topical question: how can we perform critical research in the context of the computational turn and what implications does this have for studies of the media and mediation? Both of the articles provide starting points for the argument of this book. They have both been highly cited and have captured the attention of the broadcast media in different ways. They have both had impact and have afterlives on the internet and across social media. Readers will be able to find hyperlinks to these articles, which extend across space and time, and lead to an
4
Haunted Data
encounter with a range of strange related entities: horses that can type or tell the time, clairvoyant computers, entangled minds (human and non-human), non-local consciousness, so-called mad scientists, and entities and practices, which are deemed impossible, improbable or the product of irrational belief systems. Both articles have become part of unfolding controversies across social and broadcast media and have in different ways become ‘media events’. They have left contagious trails composed of montages of hyperlinks, some of which have been assembled into accepted versions of events, and others that have been rendered insufficient, nonsensical and have been redacted or exist below the radar. These ghostly links sometimes open to detours and dead ends and often to submerged and displaced actors and agents. The articles and the controversies surrounding them concern puzzling phenomena and connect to some of the most vexed questions concerning life, matter, nature, the universe and sometimes to everything! Both of the controversies challenge some of the inherited beliefs readers might have about what it means to be human, an organism, a subject and to have and be a body. We might think of ourselves as primarily, or striving to be, unified, bounded and whole with clear and distinct boundaries between self and other. We might recognize of course that others influence us in a myriad of ways, and that relationality is perhaps a better concept for describing the richness and potentiality of what it means to be embodied. But what when relationality extends to forms of experience, practices, entities and phenomena that suggest more of a radical indeterminacy and contingency that questions any distinct sense of boundary between self and other, inside and outside, mind and body, material and immaterial, human and technical, past and present, psychic and somatic and the affective and cognitive? The experiences and phenomena that are the subject of the controversies are often described as having an ‘alien phenomenology’. What I am calling the ‘John Bargh priming controversy’ opens up the question of where mind should be located when taken out of a distinctly human bounded subject. The ‘Feeling the Future controversy’ for how to approach modes of perception and sensation that are ‘extra-sensory’, or that challenge the limits of current modes of sensing and relating. These include modes of perception and sensing that appear to be distributed and extend across time and space, and which break down any clear causal and linear relationship between past, present
Transmedial Storytelling, Weird Science and Archives of the Future
5
and future – what we might call ‘alien time’. They involve non-linear and entangled practices of memory and forgetting, which challenge any sense of psychological autonomy and bounded consciousness. This includes challenges to any assumed bifurcation of the human and technical, present and absent, mind and matter, the corporeal and the incorporeal, and the popular and the scientific. In different ways both controversies challenge a key set of colonial cultural beliefs and theories of mind, which have assumed that psychological capacities are properties of clearly bounded, autonomous subjects that are subject to bifurcations between subject and object, material and immaterial, and biological and cultural. The phenomena that are the subject of the different controversies bypass reason or rationality or disclose how limited these concepts are for describing what it means to have and be a body. Both controversies also raise important questions about the limits of the scientific method as it operates in its most positivist mode. They introduce the ‘wonder’ back into what it might mean to conduct experiments with experiences that are considered odd, strange, anomalous, uncanny and unsettling, and which regularly appear as the subject matter of psychology as it intersects with weird science. ‘Weird science’ is a broad term, which captures all manner of sciences of oddities, exceptions and anomalies. It is a term often used to refer to phenomena, practices, experiences and entities, which have been associated or linked with the paranormal or supernatural. As a field it refers to science, which concerns itself with unexplained mysteries, oddities, ‘strange stuff ’ or challenges to established thinking. This might include the area of anomalous psychology, or the ‘psychology of anomalous experience’, formerly known as parapsychology. This sub-discipline of psychology aligns a diverse range of phenomena and experiences, including mediumship, electronic voice phenomena, magical beliefs, lucid dreaming, deathbed visions, miracle cures, paranormal beliefs, false memory, telepathy, near-death states, haunted experiences, hypnosis, the placebo effect and so forth. It is framed as a study of extraordinary or exceptional phenomena, but is not restricted to those experiences which might be delineated as paranormal. These phenomena are often framed and constituted through the cognitive and neuropsychology of perception and belief. Both controversies speak to the
6
Haunted Data
vexed question of what it means to enter into suggestive relations with another human or non-human. They both disclose how little we understand processes, practices and registers of experience, which challenge rationality, control, will and autonomous thought. Mark Fisher (2017) invites us to consider the affective pull or intensity of the ‘weird’ in his book, The Weird and the Eerie. He asks, ‘What is the weird? When we say something is weird, what kind of feeling are we pointing to?’ He goes on to say, I want to argue the weird is a particular kind of perturbation. It involves a sensation of wrongness: a weird entity or object is so strange that it makes us feel that it should not exist, or at least that it should not exist here. Yet if the entity or object is here, then the categories, which we have up until now used to make sense of the world cannot be valid. The weird thing is not wrong, after all: it is our conceptions that must be inadequate. (Fisher 2017: 15)
Weird is often linked to the supernatural but he suggests that this does not exhaust the kinds of phenomena and experiences that might be designated weird. Fisher’s focus is on weird fiction or what he also calls ‘writers of the weird’ (particularly exploring the fiction of H. P. Lovecraft and George Orwell). He also suggests that the affect of the weird is linked to a fascination with the unknown, where ‘the weird cannot only repel, it must also compel our attention’ (p. 17). The weird involves an interplay between this world and others and evokes ‘a flavour of the beyond’ (p. 21) or invokes a break with something. This might include normality, the past, Euclidian time and space for example. Within legitimate or straight science ‘weird’ phenomena, such as ‘alien phenomenologies’ retain such a fascination and evocation. They primarily appear as abnormal perceptions, signs of psychopathology or puzzling curiosities that challenge foundational assumptions and normative values and expectations. They include a diverse range of suggestive, contagious and imitative phenomena that suggest we can be moved to action, to feeling, to thought, to belief in ways we little understand or comprehend. They relate to other unusual experiences, such as possession, thought control, altered states of mind and body, and the sense of futures speaking in the present. They are sometimes associated with the paranormal and the occult but have always retained a fascination in popular culture, film, art, literature, psychoanalysis and entertainment. They are the subject of psychology, psychiatry, cognitive
Transmedial Storytelling, Weird Science and Archives of the Future
7
science, neuroscience, anthropology and sociology, as well as providing a range of concepts, explanatory structures, heuristics and imaginaries for exploring the ontological indeterminacy of what it means to be human.
Governing through the affective Both controversies also involve two distinct ways of imagining and governing conduct, behaviour, thought and feeling. The first, which will form the subject of Part 1 of the book, is linked to the concept of priming. Priming relates to a range of strategies and techniques of psychological governance, or psychomediation, which have been taken up in nudge behavioural economics, popularized in the book Nudge: Improving Decisions about Health, Wealth and Happiness by Taylor and Sunstein (2009). Priming refers to the management and control of conduct, behaviour, thought and feeling, which can be shaped and produced via techniques taken to work primarily through non-conscious registers of experience. It relates to the use of techniques of indirect suggestion to influence conduct and induce behaviour and to stimulate change and transformation. Nudge has been an important dimension of how citizens in neo-liberal countries have been moved to action by governments attempting to shape behaviour beyond a subject’s conscious reflection and control. According to the philosopher Gary Gutting (2015), although priming is part of a scientific tradition that crosses cognitive science, behavioural economics and political science, the approach popularized by the authors of Nudge chimes more with common sense than established science in this area. Perhaps the invocation of common sense to describe nudge is another example of how little we understand priming, although we might be able to identify moments where subjects might be said to have been primed. Priming is already controversial as it unseats the rational cogito from its Cartesian throne and draws limits around the concept of free will. It discloses how much of our thinking, action and reaction occur in registers which exist below or beyond conscious, cognitive control. As a mode of power or governance it is more akin to post-hypnotic suggestion than to power operating in registers that are conscious, cognitive and rational; the latter is usually associated with the neo-liberal subject of agency and choice. Priming
8
Haunted Data
covers a broad range of techniques used by advertisers and marketers to shape consumer behaviour, but also appears as a set of psychologized explanations of how practices, such as racisms, are taken to be produced, maintained, shaped and rehearsed. The following example will illustrate what is at stake for the reader. In an article published in The Washington Post2 titled ‘Racial Prejudice Is Driving Opposition to Paying College Athletes. Here’s the Evidence’, the article recounts the controversies surrounding how college athletes are treated within the American university system. The revenue that athletes bring to colleges through merchandise, subscription fees for broadcasting sports events, concessions and licensing fees adds up to a very lucrative business. However this far outweighs the grants that are given to athletes, where money only usually very minimally covers tuition and maintenance. Why is this the case? The article suggests that this is primarily due to racial prejudice and that most blacks want college athletes to be paid properly while most whites don’t. In order to authenticate this statement the article validates a survey carried out that links this to pre-existing racial prejudice by conducting an experiment. The experiment is a typical priming experiment where white respondents were asked their view on whether college athletes should be properly paid, while showing ‘one group pictures of young black men with stereotypical African American first and last names. We showed another group no pictures at all.’ They go on to demonstrate within the parameters of the experiment that ‘whites who were primed by seeing pictures of young black men were significantly more likely to say they opposed paying college athletes’. As readers can see from this experiment, priming is a broad term, which covers processes and practices, which are taken to emanate from nonconscious registers of experience that can be triggered, stimulated, modulated, amplified, extended and shaped through particular technologies of materialsemiotic-affective association – in this case, images of black men, taken to have stereotypical African American first and last names. However, this is ultimately a very psychologized explanation rooted in the concept of racial prejudice, which is not adequate for exploring and examining how and why racism exists as an institutional structure of inequality and oppression. Ultimately, as we will see in Chapter 3, priming is a limited concept due to the assumptions and
Transmedial Storytelling, Weird Science and Archives of the Future
9
ontologies surrounding mind, cognition, will and affect, which have shaped modern psychology. It is haunted by what is disavowed and returns in the priming controversy. This includes already existing controversies surrounding how to understand the basis of these psychological forms of governance, management and control. This includes the need for more innovative propositions, which bring the humanities more into the frame.3 Part 2 of the book, ‘Feeling Futures/Mediating Futures’, turns to a rather different set of strategies and techniques for governing through the affective materialized within the second controversy, ‘Feeling the Future’. This controversy is part of a broader set of cultural imaginaries and discourses, which are entangled with new strategies of power based on future shaping and anticipation, which attempt to govern through rather non-linear and distributed psychologies of time. This includes techniques and practices of preemption, foresight, foreseeing and premediation. These techniques are what I call ‘strategic imaginaries’, which are manifest and becoming instantiated in computing (particularly programming and software development), biology and the neurosciences, practices of mediation within the context of communication technologies (see Grusin, 2010; Hansen, 2015), business strategy, finance capitalism, and in the conduct of war, terrorism, politics and public health responses to global threats (such as the Ebola crisis of 2014). They underpin and are shaping the development of future technologies, some of which are based on quantum mechanics and theories of quantum entanglement. Examples of these include quantum teleportation and quantum cryptology, and algorithms which attempt to change the past within open systems, sometimes called programming in the subjunctive (including retroactive update). These are algorithms which attempt to change computational pasts and are therefore seen to step sideways in time. One might also add that all of these imaginaries and the strategies that they are entangled and produced by are rather queer. Algorithms that can bend, telescope or subvert linear conceptions of time and technologies that create parallel times that bend into and beyond past, present and future certainly represent ‘a “queer” adjustment in the way in which we think about time; in fact, (they) require(s) and produce(s) new conceptions of space’ (Halberstam, 2005: 6). Quantum teleportation might be more recognizable as the stuff of science fiction, conjuring up the image of Star Trek and people being made to
10
Haunted Data
disappear from the Starship by entering the transporter, which converts people to energy replicas to reappear somewhere else. These more recognizable sci-fi fantasies are invested in by large corporations and scientists employed by IBM (for example), who have been experimenting with teleportation since 1993.4 Quantum cryptology is a strategy of encoding and decoding messages, which converts photon light waves into signs. This is a process of encryption, which depends on quantum physics rather than mathematics and is attempting to create processes of encoding and decoding, which will be difficult or even impossible to decipher. This includes an encryption machine using quantum physics, developed by a Swiss manufacturer, Id Quantique, which was used in the October 2007 parliamentary elections in Geneva to prevent voting fraud and to safeguard votes.5 Alongside these more futuristic and yet increasingly realizable fantasies, we should include business strategies such as strategic foresight, often marketed as ‘leading from the future’, and forms of venture capital based on probabilistic thinking about future risk and profit wound together into the derivative (see Seigworth and Tiessen, 2012). As the reader will see, some of these strategies and practices are quite commonplace, others less so, but certainly they are part of an entangled set of objects, entities, atmospheres and practices, which are based on anticipating and shaping future actions. They disrupt linear and Euclidian conceptions of time and space and often work in micro or even nano scales. These are considered difficult to see in the conventional methodological sense. As we will see, these attempts to govern through the affective draw more on concepts, understandings and techniques of psychological life, which are to be found in psychic research and particularly forms of quantum mechanics, which draw from theories of the paranormal and supernatural. These alien imaginaries disrupt any clear and distinct division between past, present and future, and displace from its privileged position the clearly bounded agentive psychological subject, as the subject of choice, control and reason. Within the current conjuncture the rational, choosing subject is celebrated as the epitome of democracy, civilization, liberalism and scientific truth, while at the same time suggestive processes and practices operate as part of the curious vertigo of neo-liberal forms of power. Suggestion is both feared and to be avoided while at the same time we are increasingly targeted through practices, which attempt to work in registers beyond, below or at the edges
Transmedial Storytelling, Weird Science and Archives of the Future
11
of conscious awareness and attention. Isabelle Stengers, the Belgian science studies philosopher, has cogently highlighted the paradoxes of suggestion, when she asks, ‘But above all, what do we really know about this suggestion that we are supposed to avoid?’ (Stengers, 1997: 103). As she goes on to argue, It is logical, in particular to ask oneself what hypnosis would be if it was rid of the illusion whereby the hypnotist is situated as an external observer of his patient; what is more, it is logical to again raise the question of knowing what suggestion can do in its many diverse modalities from the moment it is stripped of the illusion that the one who suggests knows what he is doing and can control the meaning and consequences of his suggestions with regard to the one he is addressing. (Stengers, 1997: 105)
Stengers’s arguments raise the important question of how our understandings of suggestion and contagious phenomena have been framed by historical discourses, which have primarily associated suggestion, contagion and imitation with a lack of will or loss of self-control, as the intrusion of the irrational, or evidence that the primitive and animal have not been successfully renounced. Contagion, suggestion, imitation and related concepts, strategies and phenomena have been making something of a comeback or return within contemporary theorizing across the humanities and social sciences, not least within what has come to be known as the turn to affect. They are also foregrounded in some of the popularized language and imaginaries, which address why and how things, processes, objects, entities and phenomena spread in ways, which appear to defy the actions of rational conscious control. This includes the concept of networked virality or networked affect – how and why trends, fashions, fads, feelings, moods and emotions spread across social media in ways which appear to defy the actions of rational logic and understanding.
Psychomediation/psychology as a science of population management This book situates both forms of governance or psychomediation within the context of a large body of scholarship, which has developed critical approaches to the psychological sciences. It also extends arguments developed in my previous book, Immaterial Bodies, Affect, Embodiment, Mediation (2012). The approaches that inform my orientation to psychology include those
12
Haunted Data
that have drawn primarily from the writings of the French philosopher Michel Foucault. This includes a reconfiguration of psychology and its subject matter as intervening within a science of population management, rather than being part of a science of the individual. Within this broad tradition of work, psychological knowledge, practices, techniques and concepts have been analysed as part of processes and practices of governance and regulation. These traditions of scholarship are often subsumed under the mantle of studies of neoliberalism and its detrimental effects and affects. This includes what has come to be known as ‘governmentality studies’, which takes the relationship between technologies of governance and techniques of selfhood as its focus. A more popularized version of these arguments can be found in a book written by the sociologist Will Davies, The Happiness Industry: How the Government and Big Business Sold Us Well-Being.6 This book in many ways contributes to over fifty years of scholarship, which has examined the place of psychology in the government of human lives. This includes the important work of the British sociologist Nikolas Rose (1985, 1989, 1990) whose genealogical approach to the psychological sciences has cogently demonstrated how the psychological sciences emerged during the nineteenth century as key knowledge practices to constitute, frame and offer resolutions to the management of conduct across a range of settings and surfaces of emergence.7 Psychology was never simply a science of the individual with unquestionable claims to neutrality, fact and truth. It was rather a set of veridical practices, which contributed to and helped to shape those historical truths, or ‘fictionswhich-function-in-truth’, which have come to make up what it means to be human within contemporary neo-liberal societies. Rose’s writing is indebted to the work of Michel Foucault and his archaeological and genealogical analyses of the human sciences as part of a history of the present. The aim of these approaches is to explore the complex power–knowledge–subjectivity relationships, which shape particular forms of regulation and particular kinds of subjects. Within this broad tradition of work, power is taken to work on and through subject’s actions and desires. This work has been extended and can be found across a range of disciplines including sociology, media and cultural studies, education, philosophy, organization studies and feminism and queer theory, for example. This includes studies of the consensual and conformist nature of popular fictions and fantasies as
Transmedial Storytelling, Weird Science and Archives of the Future
13
they might circulate and become mediated within media and popular cultures, as well as studies of key practices, such as schooling, education, healthcare, social work, mental health, work and the management of austerity. Within these settings it has been demonstrated that psychological norms become the arbiters of what is considered desirable, normal, natural, aspirational, entrepreneurial and what is ultimately considered to fall within or outside the parameters of the human. This interdisciplinary approach to psychology was also shaped by feminist psychologists entering the discipline, the turn to discourse within social psychology,8 and the work of philosophers and historians of psychology such as Graham Richards.9 The approach I have developed in my previous writing10 is associated with the book Changing the Subject: Psychology, Power, Social Regulation, published in 1984, which was written by a multiauthored collective of psychologists who had gone on to leave the discipline of psychology to work in neighbouring disciplines, such as sociology, education, psychosocial studies and media and cultural studies. The collective had begun developing what now might be described as ‘Foucauldian critiques of psychology’ taking Michel Foucault’s theorizations of power-knowledge as the basis of their approach (Henriques et al., 1984). The authors had previously worked together on the journal Ideology and Consciousness in the 1970s, developing a post-Althusserian approach to psychology and the production of the human subject. The following is a quote taken from the founding editorial of the journal and sums up the collective’s position nicely: This position conceives of the social formation as a complex, overdetermined and contradictory nexus of discursive practices, in which the human subject is constituted and lives in a relation of absolute interiority. No region or level of the social formation is contemplated which stands outside the discursive practices in which the material activities of concrete subjects consist; the social formation is equivalent to the non-unified totality of these practices. The human subject is not seen as occupying a given ‘place’ within a ‘social structure’, but as constituted in the intersections of a determinate set of discursive practices, which take their particularity from the totality of practices in which they are articulated. The concept of discursive practice thus theorises the internal relation between the constitution and existence of human subjects in the totality and the always-on going processes of production and reproduction of that totality. (Adlam et al., 1976: 46)
14
Haunted Data
The collective11 developed an ‘Althusserian-Lacanian axis’,12 which increasingly moved away from the structuralism of Althusser to the post-structuralism of Michel Foucault and towards the necessity of developing a ‘theory of the subject’. I will quote from the founding editorial of the journal Subjectivity as my own work has a direct lineage to this collective and to the approach to weird science that implicitly frames my approach. As Valerie Walkerdine recounts, This shift was seen to be one that took us towards an acknowledgement of plurality and the historical specificity of structures. This move signalled a sense that Foucault’s work on power/knowledge understood subject positions as formed within the apparatuses of power/knowledge, the discursive practices and technologies of the social through which subjectification occurred. That this was historically specific and plural was crucial. We moved away from a singular theory and a singular pathway understood through psychoanalysis. This also implied moving away from ideology. This is because Foucault understood the human and social sciences, for example, as creating knowledge that itself was a ‘fiction functioning in truth’ or a ‘regime of truth’. He therefore followed Althusser but went beyond him, in claiming not that science was ideological but that all knowledge was itself fictional and productive of subjects. This placed a great deal of emphasis on the historical emergence or genealogy of the present ‘truths’, and on the multiple sites through which these historically contingent truths are productive of positions for subjects to be formed. (Blackman et al., 2008: 6)
Readers who are not familiar with this work might recognize some similar ways of thinking in the work of other seminal figures within cultural and queer theory, including Judith Butler, Angela McRobbie and Lauren Berlant, who have all developed a discourse-fantasy axis combining Foucault with psychoanalysis (Lacanian and otherwise). Their writing has particularly explored subject-formation in the context of what Berlant describes as ‘cruel optimism’. Butler’s work (see 1990, 1993 for example) has developed this in the context of gender performativity exploring how gendered subject positions are materialized through a subject’s own actions rather than as the expression of an inherent gendered binary. McRobbie (2008) has explored this particularly within the context of new forms of femininity and post-feminism and their cultural symptoms, manifestations and consequences. All in different ways are interested in the question of how and why we might invest or subjectively commit to particular normalized fictions and fantasies,
Transmedial Storytelling, Weird Science and Archives of the Future
15
even when they do us harm, what Berlant calls ‘cruel optimism’ (see Berlant 2011). Or to the question of what happens psychically and socially when subjects who are excluded from such norms attempt to live liveable lives. They echo the view contained within Foucault’s writings that all knowledge is fictional and productive of subjects, and attend to the multiple sites (science, popular, war, conflict as well as the mundane and everyday) through which such historically contingent truths and fictions are produced and lived (also see Ahmed, 2010). The emphasis has been on some of the enduring and recurring positions (in all their sexed, gendered, raced and classed dimensions), through which subjects become and are sometimes, and even often, undone by processes of subjectification.
Speculative psychologies and ghost-hunting This book draws from these traditions but focuses instead on what became excluded from the psychological sciences as it professionalized as a discipline and separated from its previous close relationship to psychic research throughout the nineteenth century and at the turn of the twentieth century. I pay particular attention to those entities, agents, actors, practices and objects, which became disqualified and disavowed and which exist in a submerged and displaced form. I argue that what became excluded, including entities, processes and practices associated with psychic research13 was of interest to nineteenth-century process psychologies and philosophies. They approached mind, consciousness, perception and so forth, as a set of transitive processes contiguous with the technical, symbolic, psychic, affective, historical, political, cultural and so forth. In other words prior to the instantiation of the singularly bounded psychological subject as the normative position, early psychology worked more with the radical indeterminacy of the human and with more speculative and proto-performative approaches to experimentation linked to research with psychic phenomena (also see Blackman, 2012, 2014a and b). These can be found in the writings of William James, Henri Bergson, Frederick Myers, Gabriel Tarde, Boris Sidis, William McDougall, Gertrude Stein, Leo Solomons and Edward Ross. They all took ‘threshold phenomena’ seriously for illuminating questions about body–world–consciousness relations. As phenomena they were mapped onto a more relational ontology – one which emphasized process, indeterminacy, non-linearity and contiguity and the
16
Haunted Data
co-emergence of the ‘psychological’ with the technical, symbolic, biological psychic, affective, historical, political, cultural and so forth. One of the questions explored in Immaterial Bodies, which returns in this book, is why this nineteenth-century conceptual apparatus and radical relational ontology, although banished from psychology, comes back within the humanities and philosophy in the present. The practice of rescuing ‘lost figures’ that exist within a historical archive, and restaging their theories within the context of contemporary problems and questions is not a new enterprise. Philosophers have engaged this practice in order to create wonder, and to enable the present to be seen as a process of becoming rather than the natural and inevitable outcome of historical processes. Deleuze (1992) has restaged Spinoza’s philosophical writings on ethics in order to refigure the body as a process, rather than a substance or essence. Latour (2004) has restaged the work of Gabriel Tarde in order to inject psychic energy into social processes, and Massumi (2002) has restaged the writings of William James (1890), in order to make visible the limit of science’s ability to theorize affect, passion and emotion. Grusin’s (2015) inspiration for his concept of radical mediation is the radical empiricism of William James’s psychology, also a source of inspiration for the process philosophy of Brian Massumi (see Massumi, 2016) and the innovative writings of Vinciane Despret (2016). Isabelle Stengers (1997: 49) has advocated a ‘going back’ in order to resurrect figures that have seemingly been forgotten. She cogently shows how reversing the logic of scientific invention enables one to see, in a contemporary light, how, ‘questions that have been abandoned or repudiated by one discipline have moved silently into another, reappearing in a new theoretical context’. She argues that it is never simply the case that questions have been definitively abandoned or refused. What we might be more likely to see is the way in which questions are slightly modified or translated, or particular theories exist in a dynamic relationship with those that elide or disavow the claims they might make. Avery Gordon (2008) describes ghost-hunting as a practice, which makes ‘a contact that changes you and refashions the social relations in which you are located. … It is often [about] inarticulate experiences, of symptoms and screen memories, of spiraling affects, of more than one story at a time, of the traffic in domains of experience that are anything but transparent and referential’ (Gordon 2008: 25).
Transmedial Storytelling, Weird Science and Archives of the Future
17
This is the ‘background context’ that Vinciane Despret (2004b) argues is what makes practices of science-making so creative and inventive. They exist in relations of disequilibrium, disqualification, coexistence, conflict and continuation with those versions which are kept in the background. This relates to what Stengers (1997: 49) refers to as the ‘deep communications beyond the proliferation of disciplines’. In the context of haunted data rather than ‘going back’ to a historical archive in order to understand present formations, the book explores the removal and circulation of a submerged historical archive, which returns and haunts online science discussion within the context of both controversies in the present. As we will see the direction psychology took in the twentieth century occluded the more radical potential these practices presented to the shaping of a more open, inventive and creative science.14 Contemporary psychology took a very different path leaving behind a ‘Future Psychology’, which is more compatible with contemporary affect theories and processual accounts of what it means to be human, a subject and so on. I argue that weird science’s close alliance with this disavowed archive of experimentation offers up a productive set of resources for engaging with contemporary science. The kinds of data (biological, cognitive, psychological, affective) that are shaped and refracted through the post-publication-peerreview surrounding both controversies provide some new and innovative ways for contributing to the dynamism of science and to interdisciplinary and collaborative forms of inquiry. This book opens the field of affect studies, new materialism and the biohumanities to new productive possibilities and future directions. It invites a speculative consideration of what it might mean to experiment with the impossible or the improbable. It is also a call for the creation of more inventive propositions that can provide openings to what Derrida (1995) has termed ‘archives of the future’ and for the proliferation of more speculative sciences.
Methodological reflections Affective methodologies and data hauntology The book develops an affective methodology informed by hauntology and genealogy to analyse the post-publication-peer-review associated with the
18
Haunted Data
two science controversies. In the context of the data that form the basis of this book, the reader will encounter not just texts or statements or practices, but spectres, displacements, disjointed times, submerged events and multiple temporalities. As Derrida (1994) argues in his reflections on hauntology in Specters of Marx: The State of the Debt, The Work of Mourning and the New International, hauntologies raise the important ethical and methodological questions of how one can follow ghosts, or be followed by ghosts, interpret ghosts, interrogate ghosts, and listen to ghosts. Derrida argues that hauntologies are ‘performative methodologies’, an ‘interpretation that transforms the thing it interprets’ (p. 63). The data that form the basis of this book entangle the somatic, historical, technical and digital in complex ways. It requires a method of ‘interfering’ in order to make visible what cannot be easily seen in the conventional methodological sense. As Karen Barad has argued, hauntology is a methodological orientation that requires a diffractive reading (a term she borrows from Donna Haraway), so that the displaced event or narrative can be interfered with. She terms this ‘diffraction as method’ (2010: 243), in which texts, events, actors and agencies are read ‘intra-actively’ through one another. The use of the term ‘intra-action’, rather than ‘interaction’, signals that texts are not separate and then brought together, but rather that texts, or data (or statements, events, actors, agencies), are always-already entangled in complex ways. This is what lies outside of the frame of more quantitative instrumentalist approaches to data.
The after-lives of data One of the methodological questions addressed in the book is, how does one study the after-lives that data attract as they move on from the original event and accrue their own agencies? This book is an attempt to situate this question within the context of two case studies of ‘small data’. One answer to the how of this question is that it requires some tenacity and training. Following the lead of others who have had a keen interest in the positivity and proto-performativity of science, I have constituted myself as an embodied instrument (see Despret, 2008; Solomon and Stein, 1896). I have had to attune myself to a new practice of research, which I consider rhizomatic, and that has entailed the development of new habits of academic attention. Deleuze and Guattari (1980) evoke the
Transmedial Storytelling, Weird Science and Archives of the Future
19
concept of the rhizome, which they relate to botany and the rhizomatic quality of botanical roots. They argue that it is a philosophical concept or heuristic that allows for the apprehension of multiplicities. Rhizomatic inquiry looks for distributed and non-hierarchical entry and exit points. They are seen to have no origin, genesis, beginning or end. Rather the connections are considered to be ceaselessly moving. They grow and propagate new links, directions and connections, as an image of thought the data, research and analysis in this book has rhizomatic qualities. Unlike practices of research located within particular archives and technologies of inscription, including the (paper) book and journal article (as well as newspaper reports and cuttings; scientific reports held by particular institutional bodies; ethnographic research and interviews with research subjects, etc.), my research data consists of a dizzying array of hyperlinks. These links extend across blogs, tweets, online science discussion forums, online science journalism, comments on websites and open-access science journals. The links are related to specific URLs and their after-lives. It is what some media theorists have called cross-platform data, as the data is not generated and bound by particular application programming interfaces. All of the data is digital, in the sense that I am following the fate of particular journal articles as they are transformed post-publication within and across different digital platforms. I liken my role to a ghost-hunter with an obsessive compulsion who focuses on what sometimes appear as insignificant or minor details to the plots, which take form. I attend to outliers, gaps or links, which insistently return, while at the same time being subject to processes of redaction or recoding. They set in motion trails, which sometimes end at dead ends and which are often obscured by particular regimes of remembering and forgetting. These trails are difficult to account for in terms of graphs, data visualizations, index cards, overviews of the data sample, taxonomies of research materials, categorizations of methodological protocols, or as an account of the dispersion of texts as they relate to each other in an archive delimited by particular conditions of possibility and existence. The method is perhaps closest to an example of embodied hauntology, where the data is shaped and reshaped by my own actions. I have often experienced this reshaping and re-moving as akin to a form of daydreaming or reverie. It is the closest account I can give for the absorption I have experienced as I move through and experience the logic
20
Haunted Data
of what Bolter and Grusin (2000) have termed hypermediation – that is acts of mediation, which draw attention to their construction. Bolter and Grusin explore this logic in relation to the hyperlinked design of the internet and the remediation of graphic design within its aesthetic construction. This aesthetic is perhaps closest to practices of montage and collage found in modernist and postmodernist art, and is a visual logic that they argue emphasizes process, fragmentation, indeterminacy and heterogeneity (in that it does not emphasize one unified point of view). This hypertextual style means that lots of things compete for attention and reverie is perhaps one affective style that is suited to the remediated research environment that digital hauntologies engage. This feeling of syncopation and compulsion is as much about paying attention to absences, gaps, silences, contradictions and places where data trails coalesce and become attractors. Attractors relate to statements, texts, objects, events or entities that become entangled through discord, discontinuity, a temporal clash or collision. These collisions often create moments of affective intensity – anger, incredulousness, disbelief or an insistent belief that there is something more to say. These entanglements might set in motion a genealogical trail that resurrects the spectre of past controversies. These ghosts might undo the present and open to those lost-futures, which are still very much with us, albeit as repressions, displacements and movements in submerged forms. The book will illustrate the challenges of working affectively with particular archives when genealogies explicitly confront hauntologies – where the researcher encounters not just texts or statements or practices (in the Foucauldian sense), but spectres, displacements, disjointed times, submerged events and multiple temporalities. Avery Gordon (2008) describes ghost-hunting as a practice, which makes ‘a contact that changes you and refashions the social relations in which you are located. … It is often [about] inarticulate experiences, of symptoms and screen memories, of spiraling affects, of more than one story at a time, of the traffic in domains of experience that are anything but transparent and referential’ (p. 25). She goes on to argue: Being haunted draws us affectively, sometimes against our will and always a bit magically, into the structure of feeling of a reality we come to experience, not as cold knowledge, but as a transformative recognition. (p. 8)
Transmedial Storytelling, Weird Science and Archives of the Future
21
How might one provide the means to make visible such a transformative recognition? One technique that has been used by Jackie Orr (2006) is collage, which she describes as a performative strategy that allows for the telling of more than one story at a time. She brings together different fragments, including fiction, autobiography, history, dreams, and an ethnography of antianxiety medication in order to question linear time and to disrupt patterns of connection. Coleman (2009) describes how collage or collaging are not just aesthetic practices but can become methodological. The practices of moving, cutting, looping, tearing, juxtaposition and so forth are all techniques developed within this book as a form of transmedial storytelling. The technique is moved into the digital realm to develop a performative strategy that tells more than one story at a time and where events and controversies are always more than one.
Transmedial storytelling The concept of transmediality is borrowed from the work of the critical race studies scholar Rey Chow (2012). Transmediality is usually associated with strategies of storytelling, which are coordinated and orchestrated across multiple media platforms. Transmediality is often framed as a form of multiplatform storytelling that has emerged and is situated within practices of media convergence and the emergence of networked media (Jenkins, 2006). One might on this basis find reference to transmedia narratives and texts (Leavenworth, 2011), to transmedia television (Evans, 2011), to transmedia technologies, performances and even transmedial worlds. The book will explore how the focus on fiction and the construction and orchestration of transmedia (fictional) worlds can be extended within the context of hauntology and controversy analysis. As Evans (2011) argues, the association of transmediality with new media platforms obscures the way in which stories and myths, which blur fact and fiction (for example), might be considered transmedial in a way that is anachronistic to the rise of (new) media technologies. She argues, for example, that ‘the narrative of Jesus Christ might be considered multi-platform’ (p. 19). It does not exist in one place, is distributed across time and space and is enacted by multiple agents, actors, agencies, entities and objects, for
22
Haunted Data
example. She argues that the history of storytelling is one that is transmedial and to that extent there are historical precedents for transmediality and its remediation within and across networked media. As she argues, it is important to differentiate historical and more contemporary enactments (or what she terms uses) of transmediality. This question will be considered by turning attention to the hauntological forces that are transmitted within and across different media, and by extending the question of what counts as media within transmediality.
Transmediality and hauntologies As we have seen in the preceding discussion transmediality is primarily concerned with fictional worlds, with the transmission and circulation of narratives, stories, myths and texts across time, space and different ‘platforms’, which contribute to a particular kind of experience (of a historical figure, an event or a television programme, for example). The etymology of the prefix ‘trans’ in transmediality comes from the Latin ‘trans’ and refers to processes that are crossed, gone beyond or through. It introduces a particular kind of temporality into discussions of mediation, which blur distinctions between past and present and space and time. This leads, Chow suggests, to an experience of immediacy, where everything appears connected yet experienced as part of a perpetual present. This is a familiar account of media time and one that is associated with the entangled relations of the web, for example, which underpin Chow’s analysis. Chow suggests that this un-fixes the past and the present and she offers a variety of performative strategies, which might mine and work with this potentiality. This includes the concept of montage, which she presents as a practice which both separates and reconnects; she argues that montage is akin to a process of scattering a ‘(purported) previous continuum into fragments, which are then soldered or sutured together and distributed anew. We perform montage whenever we move things around from one context into another in the realm of thought, producing unanticipated, unsuspected relations – oftentimes triggering a crisis and a new situation – through the very gesture of juxtaposition’ (Chow 2012: 3). Montage as a critical practice is reliant on the critical theorist or artist able to separate and reconnect entangled relations to produce something new or
Transmedial Storytelling, Weird Science and Archives of the Future
23
unanticipated.15 The artist or theorist is implicated in the cut and creates what might be equated to a ‘self-conscious staging of mediality’ akin to a post-human form of reflexivity (Chow, 2012: 28). The critical theorist or artist becomes part of the assemblage or what Chow (Chow, 2012) terms the ‘event of capture’ intervening in order to open up the potential to think otherwise. Chow connects this transmedial strategy to critical and creative strategies that have gone before, including Brecht’s strategies of de-familiarization. Chow suggests that ‘shadow media’ or social media, which are both atomized and increase capacities for connectivity and interactivity, allow new realities to happen. She equates this to the setting in motion of different times and temporalities – no longer fugitive, fossilized and anachronistic. The events of capture made possible by such time-shifting and their radical potential should not be judged for truth-value or veracity (i.e. as the capture of reality). Rather Chow ties the event of capture to the concept of captivation inviting the reader to consider their own investments and entanglement within particular events. Chow defines captivation as the capacity to be ‘lured or held by an unusual person, event or spectacle’ (Chow, 2012: 47) and which underlies the extent to which we might be drawn into particular (imaginary) worlds. She prefers the term ‘captivation’ over interpellation suggesting that our ability to be drawn beside ourselves involves registers, which might be termed ‘affective’ and open up to theories of attachment, desire, imitation, mimetic violence, embodiment, victimization and forgiveness. Chow’s approach to transmediality turns our attention to ‘scenes of entanglement’ and to the potential transmedial relations open up for radical politics. Although Chow’s examples are mainly from earlier media forms (films and literature, for example), her development of transmediality in the concept of social media is useful for the digital hauntology that underpins the book and the concept of haunted data. Each ‘scene of entanglement’ that I stage in Parts 1 and 2 of the book splice and enact different temporalities and media times. In this sense, the staging has affinities with Karen Barad’s (2010) aligning of her own performative approach to science experimentation as hauntological. Barad’s approach is developed in an article written in the journal Derrida Today and draws an analogy between the quantum activity of electrons and what it means to be a host – to be receptive and open to entanglements. Barad (2010) argues that an
24
Haunted Data
electron is ‘an interesting body to inhabit’ because electrons are intra-active – entangled phenomena ‘differentiating and entangling, within and across, the fields of spacetimemattering’ (p. 244). Quantum movement is discontinuous rather than linear, undoing strict boundaries between here and there, past and present, dead and alive, material and immaterial and so forth. Barad uses this analogy to upset the linear progression of science and to introduce multiple temporalities into histories of quantum mechanics. Quantum activities extend perception and, Barad argues, act as a ‘queer’ lens through which to enact hauntologies of science. Barad aligns the quantum to queerness and to queer readings which are diffractive – reading and staging texts, events, human and non-human actors and agencies, objects, entities and practices through one another. This performative methodology is particularly suited to the social media science controversies that are staged in the book. In this respect I will be working with the concept of haunted data to follow those traces, deferrals, absences, gaps and their movements within a particular corpus of data and to re-move and keep alive what becomes submerged or hidden by particular regimes of visibility and remembering. These movements are simultaneously technical, affective, historical, social, political and ethical as will be illustrated throughout the book.
Ethos and queer aggregations One of the questions explored in the following chapters is, what does it mean to be a ‘host’ to alien phenomena as they appear within these controversies and particularly those appearances that are themselves ghostly – that is they appear as multiple present-absences (as data traces more specifically) that often end up in dead ends, detours, backtracks and loopings? What does it mean to commune with the haunted data associated with these controversies that do not become assembled into established or accepted narratives of the event – indeed they do not register on Google PageRank as important or significant? What kind of host would allow this data to take form and become re-moved – that is put back into circulation within the present? This hauntological approach to data draws from queer approaches to ethos that recognize ethos less as character and more as dwelling or habitual haunt. Ethos in this context relates to a habitual haunt or site of dwelling (as in an
Transmedial Storytelling, Weird Science and Archives of the Future
25
animal’s lair), but has an uncanny dimension in relation to the human. In this context the locale or site of dwelling is always haunted by what cannot easily be seen or spoken: ethos-as-haunt. Thus ethos is also the cultivation or attunement to one’s site or dwelling that incorporates manners, customs, shared experiences and memories, ambiences, atmospheres, displaced narratives, structures of feeling, traditions and so forth.16 To that extent responsiveness or attunement is not simply a human affair or activity, and requires mediated forms of perception to be ‘seen’. Although these arguments have largely been made in relation to analogue media, to memories of place, or to situated writing practices, the focus on ethos understood in this way will be extended within the context of digital archives and data ethnography. Within this context a host can be human, non-human or, more usually within the context of computational entities and practices, an entanglement of human and non-human actors and agents. These entangled relations, practices and processes come together in non-linear, indeterminate and sometimes rather queer ways – what I am calling ‘queer aggregations’.
Conclusion: Digital archive fever and archives of the future What is at issue here is nothing less than the future, if there is such a thing: the future of psychoanalysis in its relationship to the future of science. As techno-science, science, in its very movement, can only consist in a transformation of the techniques of archivization, of printing, of inscription, of reproduction, of formalization, of ciphering, and of translating marks. (Derrida, 1995: 16)
Derrida (1995) in Archive Fever: A Freudian Impression considers the nature of the archive and what he terms the archive of a familiar word. His subject is both Freudian psychoanalysis and the techniques of archivization that organized and produced the very archive that might be recognized as Freudian psychoanalysis (and therefore the ‘proper’ object(s) of psychoanalysis). This archive was consigned at the time of Derrida’s writing through a process of institutionalization linked to the inauguration of Freud’s house in Finchley (North West London) as the site of the Freud Museum. As Derrida argues, this is the archive located within a place, literally a house or residence,
26
Haunted Data
which becomes ‘a privileged topology’ or what is also described as a logic of conservation (p. 10). The process of institutionalization of such an archive becomes a process of gathering together signs, which are given order, place, and status allowing regimes of visibility and knowing to be enacted as part of the ‘violence of the archive’ (p. 11). As he argues, the politics of the archive understood in this way draws attention to what materials and techniques allow the taking form of the archive – what counts as public/private, theory/personal feeling, significant/insignificant, human/technical, real/imagined and so forth. When reading these reflections on archivization in the present they feel prescient in relation to the issues, questions and debates at the heart of this book. These concern the future of science in the context of (social media) controversies; the extent to which digital media, open-access and postpublication-peer-review are changing science communication and have the potential to contribute to a more open science. What new objects, entities, processes and practices might be possible if this corpus of data were mined to disclose the potential dynamism of science and science’s lost futures and futures-yet-to-come? One of the questions that Derrida asks, for example, is the extent to which archival machines or techniques change the object. At the time Derrida reflects on how the traces of his own thinking were recorded on his portable MAC (which travelled with him to Naples) and the extent to which psychoanalysis might have been or indeed have become a different object (in terms of its archivization) if it had been shaped and produced by archival machines of the future. The future in 1995 referred primarily to portable computers, printers, faxes, televisions, teleconferences and email, for example. As Derrida asks, ‘Do these new archival machines change anything?’ (p. 15). He replies that indeed psychoanalysis would have been something quite different if email had existed. Technical mediation of communication in this context is not supplementary or ancillary to what takes form but plays a constitutive role. This of course is a familiar argument within contemporary media and cultural theory where technologies are not simply carriers of pre-existing objects, processes, desires and information, but enter into their very formation. One must of course be wary of technological determinism and take into consideration the extent to which mediation is simultaneously technical, political, somatic, social, psychic and so forth. In this respect Derrida recognizes some of what is
Transmedial Storytelling, Weird Science and Archives of the Future
27
changed by electronic communication (with possible changes to what counts as public and private, and to the increased speed and instantaneous nature of communication), but also focuses on the important issue of what we are already primed to both recognize and misrecognize as part of an archive’s subject matter. In order to expose this process, part of what Derrida terms the Freudian impression; he works against the logic of conservation in order to uncover the half-hidden and submerged statements that have the potential to radically transform psychoanalysis. He finds the traces of one such statement in a private inscription, specifically in a dedication from Freud’s grandfather to be found in a bible given to Freud as an act of paternal love. This trace sets in motion a trail, which takes him to a text written by the Jewish scholar Yerushalmi (1991).17 This text poses a question to the specter of Freud and the extent to which Freud might have characterized psychoanalysis as a Jewish Science. This question posed hauntologically sets in motion a trail, which is followed by Derrida. This takes him to those gaps, traces, contradictions and moments, when Freud enacted this characterization (in private correspondence, for example). This strategy allows Derrida to performatively re-stage or re-imagine an archive of the future or at least a future-yet-to-come. These are examples of what he terms ‘half-private, half public conjurations’ (p. 57). These issues, I will argue are important for understanding processes of change and transformation, and what becomes resistant to alternative directions and possibilities. This is despite the virtual potential of what Derrida termed ‘archives of the future’, as we will go on to explore in the next two chapters.
2
Social Media Contagion(s): An analysis of Priming Controversies within Cognitive Science Introduction: From thinking without thinking to psychic animals – The case of Clever Hans and the strange coupling of Mr von Osten and Professor John Bargh In August 2014 John Bargh, the Yale cognitive scientist, considered by some to be the ‘Father’ of priming, received the APA (American Psychological Association) Award for Distinguished Scientific Contributions. On 1 January 2014 he published a notable article in Scientific American with the title ‘How Unconscious Thought and Perception Affect Our Every Waking Moment’. On 5 March 2012 John Bargh resurrected his blog for Psychology Today, which had lain dormant for two years prior. He did this in order to respond to a failed replication study of one of his studies on priming, one of the most highly cited studies in the field (1996). To date it has been cited over 3,634 times. He wrote another subsequent blog post responding to a comparison that had been made between himself and Mr von Osten. Mr von Osten was the owner of a horse and equine celebrity known as Clever Hans, who courted controversy and publicity in the late nineteenth and early twentieth centuries. This response was made by a science journalist Ed Yong, commenting upon his first blog entry and contributing to its contagious affects. These posts were ephemeral and elusive, subject to recodings, redaction and deleting.1 This chapter will primarily consider why this post and subsequent blog posts gained a reach and traction and became part of what was described as an ‘Internet drama bomb’. In the next chapter I will amplify some of its contagious and hauntological potential by assembling two further scenes
Social Media Contagion(s)
29
of entanglement, which will re-move some of the displaced and submerged actors, traditions, agents and narratives, which haunt this controversy. These ghosts open contemporary psychology to some rather curious and interesting pasts.2 These are put back into circulation by the strange coupling of Mr von Osten and Professor John Bargh made in Ed Yong’s interpretation of the significance of the controversy, as we will go on to explore. What comes back and is re-moved by this coupling are displaced psychologies, which were attempting to grapple with the ontological indeterminacy of the human. They approached psychological processes as transitive and contiguous with the ecological, biological, symbolic, technical and historical, rather than as discrete properties of mind, which was later to become stabilized within psychology. In this chapter I will consider the micropolitics of the postpublication-peer-review that shaped this controversy, and consider what we might learn about media contagions and network viralities from this novel case study.
Scientific communication is down at the moment, please check later3 The ‘John Bargh priming controversy’ is a good example of a controversy which largely took shape across digital and social media, and is one which might be described as having a contagious virality, or ‘networked affect’. The question of how to understand why this controversy became so contagious and what this raises for analyses of social media is a vexed issue, and one that takes us beyond some of the key debates and concepts often found in social media scholarship. These will be discussed throughout the chapter. The controversy involved a number of protagonists, some alive, some dead, and involved both human and non-human actors. The controversy revolved around a tradition of research known as priming, which crosses psychology and cognitive science as discussed in the previous chapter. Readers might be more familiar with priming as popularized by Malcolm Gladwell (2007) in his book Blink: A Study of Thinking without Thinking. The Nobel Prize-winning cognitive scientist Daniel Kahnman, in his best-selling book Thinking Fast and Slow, 2011, has also popularized priming. This outlines
30
Haunted Data
what is considered a fast, intuitive form of thinking, which is seen to register below the threshold of conscious attention. To that extent the controversy perhaps has a receptive audience given many readers have been primed to think about priming and its implications for the limits of will, consciousness and intentional thinking. As we saw in the Preface and Chapter 1, it has also been taken up within nudge behavioural economics. One area of scientific experimentation linked to priming that Gladwell discusses is the field of automaticity research. Automaticity’s subject matter is related to a kinship of experiences, which disclose that a subject’s decisions are often made in ways that exceed conscious, rational thought – or what is often understood as free will or intentionality. Gladwell brings together a range of experiences which are taken to exceed conscious volition, these include snap judgements, rapid cognition, gut feeling and intuition. He also exposes the entanglement of the human and the technical in such processes. Priming, the subject of the controversy, is part of an imaginary, which connects hypnosis, brainwashing, advertising, mediumship, voice hearing and related phenomena. All of these phenomena in curious ways disrupt the borders and boundaries between the self and other, past and present, material and immaterial, human and technical and inside and outside. They relate to an ‘alien phenomenology’ – that is the sense that we can be haunted or moved by someone, or even something, else. They also articulate a range of fears and fascinations which surround what it means to be human, and the limits of conceptions of the unified, self-determining subject for understanding processes of subject-formation. This chapter will respond to a key question raised by social and digital media scholars who have asked how we might perform critical research within the context of the ‘computational turn’ and what many see as the reshaping of the social through data practices (see Langlois, Redden and Elmer, 2015; Terrenova, 2012). The data explored in this chapter reveal how data-mediations are entanglements of the personal and the political, the human and the technical, the affective and the cognitive, past and present, fact and fiction, and forms of communication, which challenge the rationality of science. This extended and distributed commentary carried by postpublication-peer-review (post-publication-peer-review) leave behind living archives, which open possibilities for reinterpreting the ‘proper object’ of
Social Media Contagion(s)
31
scientific traditions, as well as observing science in the making (Latour, 1987). These new practices, which involve scientists and non-scientists, and human and non-human actors and agencies (specifically algorithms and application programming interfaces), are challenging the conventions of positivist science communication (see Nosek and Bar-Anan, 2012). They underpin some scientists’ calls for the development of an Open Science in the context of the affordances of digital publishing and social media.
Open Science ‘Open Science’ is a term, which is explicitly linked to the digital transformation of publishing and the argument that the published journal article is anachronistic in the context of digital communications. Open scientists argue that the published static unmodifiable journal article is rooted in seventeenth-century technologies and practices of publishing, which should now be abandoned. Open Science is a call for a more democratic, inclusive, participatory, public science, which has the capacity to transform itself. These debates are happening across the sciences and are linked to different trends and themes. This might include the embracing of digital communication and social media platforms, the call for open-access publishing, new forms of evaluation including continuous peer review and the publishing of peer review, and new outlets for disseminating research, which allow new publics to contribute to the evolution of science. For some scientists, the barriers to change are not technical or even financial, but rather to be found in the different cultures of science, which are linked to the status quo and to already existing relations of prestige, status, hierarchy and power. There are many fears and anxieties surrounding the new publishing platforms and forms of dissemination made possible by digital and social media. As with similar debates across the humanities, the transformation of publishing made possible by digital communication is shaping new forms of peer review, evaluation and opportunities for academic research to be read, used and appropriated.4 It is also being linked to distinct calls by some feminist academics for the publishing of research and forms of evaluation, which are fairer, less sexist and more humane.5 However, as we will see in this chapter, despite the politics of open access and open science, it is often business as usual which ensues.
32
Haunted Data
A key question examined in this chapter is, to what extent the archives associated with this controversy might open to what Derrida (1995) termed ‘archives of the future’? I am particularly interested in those archives, which relate to the possibility of a ‘Future Psychology’. That is to a psychology that is attentive and oriented to psychological processes as fundamentally relational, indeterminate and entangled in complex ways with material, immaterial, symbolic, technical, historical, cultural and political practices, objects and entities. This ontology of psychological processes is more compatible with affect studies, and is one that I argue can be found in traces of psychology’s many pasts – pasts, which resurface and return within science controversies when followed across time. As Emily Martin (2013) has argued in relation to the affective turn across the humanities, many affect theorists have turned to the cognitive and neurosciences for inspiration, and on that basis are in danger of working with stripped-down models of human subjectivity – what are often described as forms of biomediation (see Clough, 2008a). Indeed, as she argues, the genealogy of the models of subjectivity that are being brought into the humanities have separated mind from brain and subjectivity from the social, seeing the brain or the supposed ‘gap’ between brain and mind as the site of (critical) potentiality. In her article she returns to a rather different site of psychological experimentation, The Cambridge Anthropological Expedition to the Torres Straits Islands in 1898, which haunts contemporary cognitive neuroscience – one where she argues that ‘even the most raw, “natural” perceptual inputs from eyes, ears, nose and skin were only graspable as products of specific human social environments’ (p. S153). She argues in comparison that many affect theorists ‘depict the social as stopping well before we get down to the bottom’ (Martin, 2013: S154). The ‘bottom’ here refers to the brain and to non-conscious processes which are taken to exceed or exist below conscious perception and attention and sometimes even outside culture and social relations. The controversy that is the subject of the next two chapters also re-engages a more anthropological or ethnographic imaginary that haunts priming and automaticity research across the cognitive neurosciences. It is one that is brought back in the present and to that extent will be of interest to contemporary affect theorists and humanities scholars interested in science and the problematic of subjectivity. It represents what I will go on
Social Media Contagion(s)
33
to describe as a more ecological approach to affect and the question of what makes us human. Derrida’s method as outlined in the last chapter is particularly useful when considering how archives might be transforming within digital environments with the potential for re-interpretations, rewritings, reposings and restagings.6 However, the contrary logic of archives that Derrida discusses, with both the drive towards conservation of the past and the potential for future rewritings (what he describes as the conditional nature of all archives), is one that is further constrained by the performative nature of algorithms and the human and non-human agencies which shape and anticipate future decodings and recodings (see Rieder, 2012).7 In this respect, a data hauntology also needs to consider the processes of automation enacted by algorithms that shape particular regimes of remembering and forgetting (also see Parisi, 2013). In previous writing I have considered this in the context of the Google PageRank algorithm and the particular relations of prestige, status, hierarchy and power relations which shape what can and cannot be easily seen.8 These rankings are time-sensitive and constantly changing. The labour, human and non-human, which supports the rankings is further hidden and obscured by the algorithm’s proprietary nature, automated practices, which ‘clean-up’ or edit comments for professionalism, as well as the immaterial labour that users might engage in order to manage their own social media reputational economy (see Gerlitz and Lury, 2014). All of these issues constrain the conditional nature of the archives, but do not close down on the critical potential and the places that the data become compromised and open to possibilities for future rewritings. The half-hidden and submerged statements within the controversy, that form the subject matter of this chapter and Part 1 of the book, are set in motion by three blog posts, which were subject to processes of removal, redaction and partial erasure. Written by the Yale cognitive scientist John Bargh and posted in his blog The Natural Unconscious, they were published in the trade magazine Psychology Today. This chapter will set the scene (scene 1), before I move in Chapter 3 to assemble a distributed form of perception (a series of decodings and recodings), which works across a rhizomatic or machinic form of non-conscious9 (see Cho, 2008). This will allow the reader to see why these removals were qualitatively significant, even if quantitatively they only represent ‘small data’.10
34
Haunted Data
Scene 1. Priming horses and new media worlds: The case of Clever Hans Clever Hans was a horse in the early twentieth century who was apparently able to tell the time and solve complex multiplication puzzles by stamping his hooves. The comparison between John Bargh and the owner of Clever Hans, Mr von Osten, was made in a blog post for the online science journal Discover by a respected science journalist, Ed Yong.11 Yong was commenting on the same ‘failed replication study’ of a classic priming experiment by John Bargh, Chen and Burrows (1996). Bargh’s study claimed that experimental subjects walked more slowly to a lift after being shown words associated with ageing on a scrambled language task. A Belgian post-doctoral researcher Stephane Doyen carried out the failed replication study. Doyen is a psychologist interested in priming, who led a team intent on replicating Bargh’s classic study. They changed some of the parameters of the study and did not replicate the results. The only parameter within which they did replicate the results were when experimenters believed that subjects would walk more slowly to the lift. As they suggest: Experimenters’ expectations seem to provide a favorable context to the behavioral expression of the prime. Obviously, this interpretation remains tentative, as we do not know how this process operates. However, it is likely that experimenters who expect their participants to walk slower behave differently than those who expect their participants to walk faster and that such behavioral cues are picked up by participants. (Doyen et al., 2012: 1)
They argued that this suggests ‘that both priming and experimenters expectations are instrumental in explaining the walking speed effect’ (p. 1). This non-replication led to what Yong described as a scathing personal attack on Doyen, written in Bargh’s blog, The Natural Unconscious, for Psychology Today, which had lain dormant for two years previously. The URL to this blog entry – https://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/the-natural-unconscious/201203/ nothing-in-their-heads – was later removed.This scathing attack, represented by Yong with an image of toys being thrown out of a pram was taken down and removed from the scene leaving behind ghostly traces. Interested readers could find hyperlinks to Bargh’s response – commentaries on the response, responses to the response – but the actual response remained elusive.
Social Media Contagion(s)
35
Figure 1 Clever Hans with his owner Mr von Osten. Copyright Wikimedia Commons.
As one commentator argued, In response to that non-replication, Bargh published two blog posts called ‘Nothing In Their Heads’ and ‘Angry Birds’, criticizing both the study itself and the journal it appeared in, PLoS ONE. It was a combative defence of his work. However, Bargh seems to have since decided it was a bit too defensive, because the posts have been deleted. They just vanished: to my knowledge Bargh hasn’t announced this, nor explained why.12
Although the blog post was removed, one could gather clues as to its emotional valence and tone through a set of hyperlinked blog posts. One was a commentary by Daniel Simons, a psychology professor at the University of Illinois with 22,920 Google+_ followers and with 708,922 views at the time of writing on this particular post: https://plus.google.com/+DanielSimons/posts/ VJH8wXxxc3fHe provides a commentary on Bargh’s removed blog post with the title ‘A Primer for How Not to Respond when Someone Fails to Replicate Your Work.’ As he goes on to comment, In the linked post, John Bargh responds to a paper published in PLoS ONE that failed to replicate his finding that priming people with terms related to aging led them to walk more slowly to the elevator afterward. His post is a case study of what NOT to do when someone fails to replicate one of your findings.
36
Haunted Data
One could also find some reflections made by Sanjay Srivastava, an American psychologist who writes a blog, The Hardest Science: A Psychology Blog: Thoughts about the Mind, Science, Society and Whatever Else. The blog entry is titled ‘Some Reflections on the Bargh-Doyen Elderly Walking Priming Brohaha’.13 He reflects on the inflammatory nature of Bargh’s response, and Yong’s comparison of Bargh to Mr von Osten. Srivastava frames his discussion by calling for a discussion of replication and its continuing importance to scientific discovery and innovation. As we will go on to see this became the way in which the controversy was framed as it started to settle. Interested readers can also read a summary of Bargh’s removed post with the author’s annotated responses posted in a Reddit community, which calls itself the Reddit Society for Psychological Research.14 It was submitted by a user called ViscountPrawn with the title ‘Failed replication of famous elderly-priming walk speed experiment prompts icy response from original author, internet drama-bomb in comments’. It drew twenty-nine comments, two of which were later deleted or perhaps edited for professionalism. The ‘internet drama-bomb’ relates to the tone or sentiment of Bargh’s PPPR, which was considered unscientific by many, and as exceeding scientific debate and dialogue usually linked to the norms of rationality, reason and reflection. Bargh claimed to be offering the expert review and editorial scrutiny, which he suggested the Doyen failed replication study did not receive as part of the official review process. Bargh tried to persuade readers that the experiment lacked integrity, using sarcasm, righteous indignation and hostility. He expressed hurt and upset creating a sentiment that cast doubt on the findings of the study. Sentiment analysis reflects a key area in social media analysis that relates to the nature of communication within digital environments. These debates often challenge or contest idealized norms of rational, detached and deliberative communication, viewed as essential to democracy and effective public spheres. The use of social media certainly represents an issue for many scientists now finding their research and its evaluation post-publication challenging the norms of science writing and its cherished ideals of objectivity, detachment and neutrality. Many theorists have argued that social media communicative strategies challenge so-called rational conventions of communication. To that extent they are often argued to be more affective. Papacharissi (2012) suggests that many communication strategies on social media are improvisational and
Social Media Contagion(s)
37
primarily affective in nature. In other words, the performance of what she calls ‘affective statements’ are those that combine ‘opinion, emotion and fact’ (Papacharissi, 2012: p. 2000) and are designed to provoke.15 In her article Without You I’m Nothing16 (Papacharissi 2012), she foregrounds play as an important performative strategy. That is, play is part of the performance of selfhood, to use the dramaturgical metaphor that underpins her work. She identifies this as part and parcel of self-presentation within social media environments. The ‘storytelling project of the self ’, which she analyses on Twitter, for example, does not reveal or disclose a pre-existing self. Rather it is comprised of performative strategies designed to do certain things – persuade, provoke, impress and upset, for example. This often includes the use of irony, humour, profanity, incivility and the broaching of taboo and risque subjects. That is, as she argues, the ‘playing out of the fantasy of saying shocking or potentially shocking things in public’ (Papacharissi, 2012: p. 1994). These forms of play, she suggests, operate within ‘as if ’ registers. They are virtual rather than actual, and ascribe an improvisational character to the performance of selfhood across social media platforms. She describes such performative strategies as forms of anticipation rather than actualization, which allow for fantasy to be integrated into the everyday giving voice to personal sentiment and feeling. Such practices understood as performative strategies also blur fact and fiction, the real and imaginary and the private and the public creating contradictions and tensions that require management through engagement in particular storytelling projects of self-management. As we will see in this controversy, this required Bargh to engage in a series of redactions, erasures and ongoing attempts to manage the after-lives that the blog posts and their removals and re-movals accrued. These storytelling projects of the self almost always exceed the intentions and volition of individual social media users, revealing or disclosing the hauntological potential of individual social media transactions, as we will see in this and the next chapter. In the following paragraph I reproduce the opening paragraphs of the first blog post ‘Nothing in Their Heads’ (https://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/th e-natural-unconscious/201203/nothing-in-their-heads) – later removed – so that the reader can get a sense of the tone and framing of Bargh’s commentary: Scientific integrity in the era of pay-as-you-go publications and superficial online science journalism. What prompts the return of the
38
Haunted Data
blog is a recent article titled ‘Behavioral Priming: It’s All in the Mind, but Whose Mind?’ by Stéphane Doyen, Olivier Klein, Cora-Lise Pichon, and Axel Cleeremans. The researchers reported that they could not replicate our lab’s 1996 finding that priming (subtly activating in the minds of our college-age experimental participants, without their awareness) the stereotype of the elderly caused participants to walk more slowly when leaving the experiment. We had predicted this effect based on emerging theory and evidence that perceptual mental representations were intimately linked with behavioral representations, a finding that is very well established now in the field (see below). Following their failure to replicate, Doyen et al. went on to show that if the experimenter knew the hypothesis of the study, they were able to then find the effect. Their conclusion was that experimenter expectancies or awareness of the research hypotheses had therefore produced the effect in our original 1996 study as well – in other words, that there was no actual unconscious stereotype effect on the participants’ behavior. The Doyen et al. article appeared in an online journal, PLoS ONE, which quite obviously does not receive the usual high scientific journal standards of peer-review scrutiny (keep reading for the evidence of this); instead, the journal follows a ‘business model’ in which authors pay to have their articles published (at a hefty $1,350 per article). The journal promises a ‘rigorous peer review’ for technical soundness but not as to the importance of the finding. On their website PLoS dismisses the use of knowledgable editors to oversee what gets published and what does not, claiming this adds only a subjective element to the acceptance decision that can be biased against new research directions. But knowledgeable editors also can prevent articles from being published based on faulty peer reviews, such as by inexpert, lazy, or biased reviewers. Expert editors also know the relevant theory and past research in a given domain, and also know of common methodological pitfalls that inexpert researchers in the domain – such as, apparently, Doyen et al. (keep reading) – can fall prey to. The lack of rigorous expert editorial scrutiny by PLoS in the Doyen et al. case means that I must supply it here, only after it has been published. If I’d been asked to review it (oddly for an article that purported to fail to replicate one of my past studies, I wasn’t) I could have pointed out at that time the technical flaws, though these might not have mattered to PLoS ONE – as a for-profit enterprise, PLoS published 14,000 articles in the year 2011 alone. Fourteen thousand. Something tells me they don’t turn down many $1,350 checks. ...
Social Media Contagion(s)
39
Although the essentially self-published nature of the Doyen et al. article is bad enough, the misleading conclusions it drew were made even worse by the publicity given to them by some online science-journalism blogs, one of which posted about the Doyen et al. failure with the title ‘Primed by expectations: Why a classic psychology experiment isn’t what it seemed’. So as you have read this far, and are therefore a reader interested in psychological science, I’d like (and apparently need) to set the record straight.
This blog entry had many after-lives moving on from the original event of ‘setting the record straight’ and becoming linked to an assemblage of actors and agencies, and academic and non-academic publics across various social media platforms and APIs (application programming interfaces). The multiple media transactions provide a corpus of responses to what was framed as Bargh’s personal attack on open-access publishing (and particularly the journal PLoS One), science journalism (and the trustworthiness of online media sources), and the problems with replication studies. I have partially reconstructed the trails of the lives that this blog entry accrued in previous writing.17 I will commit here to discussing more of the hauntological implications of this re-moving.
Hans-Jörg Rheinberger The concept of re-moving is taken from the science studies scholar HansJörg Rheinberger’s work. I argue that although Rheinberger’s focus is science and specifically scientific controversies, his insights have much to add to social media analysis and the digital humanities. Rheinberger is a significant German science studies scholar, who until his retirement was based at the Max Planck Institute in Berlin. His work, like those of many feminist science studies scholars (Haraway, Barad, Franklin, for example), has produced new objects, entities, methods and ways of thinking at the intersection of science and philosophy. His work was very influenced by Derrida, Haraway, Bachelard, Foucault and Canguilhelm, for example. He has been described as a leading historian and philosopher of the biological and life sciences (Lenoir, 2010). His philosophy of experimental practice is one that has many shared ontologies with those taken up within anthropology, sociology and literary studies (those which foreground process, enaction and relationality, for example). It is what Lenoir (Lenoir, 2010: xii) refers to as an ‘exercise in
40
Haunted Data
historical epistemology’. His work and historical method present a critique of scientific positivism and explore the entanglement of science, the technical and cultural in the production of scientific objects and entities – or what we might term, following Karen Barad (2007), ‘phenomena’. The term that Rheinberger uses is ‘epistemic things’. Rheinberger’s approach foregrounds recursion, or patterns of repetition and difference, which underpin the invention of new scientific objects. They also contribute to the epistemological foreclosure of specific materialized interpretations. Scientific objects are always mediated and become an agent in ‘the process of making knowledge’ (Lenoir, 2010: xiii) and are part of ‘experimental systems’ or apparatuses which are performative. That is, they invent rather than discover. However, the processes of what becomes stabilized are always haunted for Rheinberger in terms of displaced and suppressed narratives, which always threaten to surface and come back; they exist as traces or deferrals in the Derridean sense. The concept of ‘epistemic things’ captures the patterns of difference and repetition that are characteristic of scientific objects and entities. Although science controversies might be considered settled at particular times, Rheinberger (1994) shows how they have the tendency to resurface in new ways and forms. This is something he cogently shows when following the controversies surrounding chicken tumour agents within oncology across time. This is what Rheinberger (1994), following Derrida, refers to as the historical movement of a trace (its haunting perhaps), the tension between persistence and transformation. He argues that this process is not captured by Thomas Kuhn’s more totalizing notion of a paradigm, and a paradigm shift, to understand change and transformation within science. It would seem that experimental systems are haunted by traces of the past, and that this haunting increasingly materializes in new forms of animation and automation, off-line and most visibly in online science discussion and its often volatile displays of affect, emotion and feeling. I argue that the time lags, time shifts and multiple media times and temporalities re-moved within this controversy, allow one to orient attention to the uncertainty and indeterminacy that characterizes experimental systems. The distributed data of post-publication-peer-review, as presented in the John Bargh priming controversy, present opportunities to analyse the hauntological dimensions of social and digital media and science more generally.
Social Media Contagion(s)
41
Rheinberger’s influence by Derrida is most telling in the neologisms that he constructs to undergird his approach to science and scientific forms of experimentation. This includes the concept of historiality, which draws attention to the multiplicity of times that intrude within experimental systems. The concept also draws attention to science as a storytelling machine, where as he argues, ‘An experimental system has more stories to tell than the experimenter at any given moment is trying to tell with it’ (Rheinberger, 1994: 77). He equates this dynamic potential to older narratives that persist in the future, as well as ‘fragments of narratives that have not yet been told’ (Rheinberger, 1994: 77). As is apparent from this discussion, controversies are scenes of entanglement where the past and future crisscross, intervene, intrude and open up the potential for something new to emerge. Rheinberger also characterizes this potential dynamism as an excess that escapes definition. It has different momentums, and allows for a potential tinkering, or what he also characterizes as a form of ‘re-moving’ (Rheinberger, 1994: 78). The concept of re-moving expresses what I take to be the hauntological vision of this thesis – the re-moving or animation of frozen time, or temporalities which haunt what has come to pass (as scientific truth or certainty, for example). Controversies are potential ‘scenes of entanglement’ which re-move, or have the potential to set traces in motion – traces which perhaps are not visible or knowable in relation to the intelligibility of particular experimental systems. Re-movings have the potential to perform retroactive re-shapings, but importantly such dynamism is not reducible to scientific research and practice itself. Re-movings are not internal to science or to the internal times and practices of formal scientific laboratories. As we will go on to see, the concept of re-moval allows a researcher to move beyond the immediate display of sentiment and emotion within online discussions, and to also explore what traces or deferrals are also potentially being set in motion or kept alive by such practices of selfperformance and self-curation of data. I term these ‘queer aggregations’. As an example of the haunting persistence of a trace or deferral, although Bargh’s blog posts ‘Nothing in Their Heads’ and his subsequent post ‘Angry Birds’ were taken down from his Psychology Today blog, the posts are hyperlinked in complex ways to a distributed network of comments, responses, counter-responses and links to actors, agencies, sites and practices, which have become part of the controversy’s extended life and after-lives. This assemblage
42
Haunted Data
of relations is performative, extending the controversy in unforeseen directions and having unanticipated consequences for the debate that ensues. The data that carry these relations are visible and invisible, material and immaterial, covering the internet like a spider’s web of present and ghostly traces. This is a corpus of haunted data to be mined, poached and put to work in newly emergent and emerging conjunctures and contexts. These traces point towards another scene, the ghosts of Clever Hans and his owner Wilhem von Osten from the nineteenth and early twentieth centuries, which this controversy re-animates in the present. These ghosts follow John Bargh. The area of priming within psychology has continually to be policed in order to prevent these historical associations from surfacing. The hauntological question is, why is this policing necessary? What is being displaced, submerged and occluded by this policing? This hauntological dimension is confirmed in the first comment made by Bargh in his subsequent erased blog entry ‘Angry Birds’ posted in Psychology Today.18 He begins with the plea: The discussion sparked by my previous post has now far transcended the remarks I made in the post itself, in defense of our lab in the face of the ‘Clever Hans’ charge. That was a slur on our lab that had to be responded to in order to set the record straight. Insults like that typically make people angry, and so a lot of heat was generated, but too much heat produces smoke, and smoke obscures clear vision. Let’s see if we can continue the discussion without anger and hostility clouding the real issues.
The blog goes on to engage with some of the issues that have been raised in relation to the study and to put the record straight, again. The comments for this entry have not been recovered and no subsequent entries by Bargh have been made in Psychology Today since this post (and at the time of writing). As a cultural theorist interested in affect and phenomena that disrupt borders and boundaries between the inside and outside, material and immaterial, past and present, public and private and self and other, for example, I have been fascinated by the unfolding of this controversy and what has taken form. I was led to this controversy by my own research into the phenomenology of will and what has come to be known as the ‘half second delay’ within affect studies (Massumi, 2002; Thrift, 2007).19 I knew that the neuroscientific evidence underpinning this statement was controversial and through my own searches I was directed towards and became captivated by this particular social media controversy.
Social Media Contagion(s)
43
I argue that this controversy is a good example of Rheinberger’s arguments, which suggest that science controversies, although considered settled at particular times, have the tendency to resurface in new ways and forms. In his recent work (2010) he has extended this insight to consider what he terms the ‘economy of the scribble’ – those traces of practices which exceed the parameters of recognized scientific practice. Although Rheinberger in this instance focuses on what gets left out of studies once they are written up for publication – scribbles on pieces of paper, workings out, ponderings and so on, I argue that digital archives represent opportunities to explore such economies within distributed, extended networks of actors, agencies and practices. These exist as traces, which can be followed, mapped, listened to and re-animated. Some of these traces are deleted or removed and exist as ghostly presences, requiring ingenuity, tenacity and some knowledge of software and data practices in order to recover and re-perform. The traces are lively and disclose some of what is carried by the social and cultural life of data. Data traces can be moved and re-moved, redacted as well as re-mixed, and require the work of articulation, translation and staging in order to be made visible. This is a different strategy to predictive analytics and is one that mines and focuses upon the potential of compromised data. These traces often become hidden or covered over by representationalism – by what narratives and representations come to stand in for the event or which take on the status of animations (see Kember and Zylinska, 2012). These obscure the more temporal aspects of events – the historicity of time and the different temporalities that disclose the more-than-one nature of controversies and events. The strategy of data hauntology as a mode of inquiry reveals the dynamic nature of archives and their unexcavated potential. It also opens up questions of ethics and ethical entanglements – about how our attempts to repurpose and re-imagine data are highly situated engagements. The focus here is much more on the performing and re-performing of archives, rather than their conservation as monuments.
Page not found ‘Page not found’ has been a recurring theme within this scene of entanglement;20 and after the removal of Bargh’s post ‘Nothing in Their Heads’ it was replaced
Haunted Data
44
with various animations to carry the traces of Bargh’s irate response, that were later removed. One meme, which was later removed, went by the name of the ‘Barghinator’ and for some time captured something of the uncivil discourse that for many within this debate crossed the boundaries of acceptable science debate and communication. The ‘Barghinator’, enacted a trace of Bargh’s outburst and was represented by a cartoon image of a face crumpled in anger with furrowed brows and cross-eyes set against a brown background. Although I have a copy of this meme I am unable to reproduce it due to copyright permissions. The removal of Bargh’s post was often commented upon by social media users, where interested parties attempted to reconstruct and familiarize themselves with why Bargh was so angry. This includes questions such as the following: August 31, 2012 @ 7:52 am I cannot find the rebuttal (of the Doyen et al. study) by Bargh anymore. All the different links to it appear to be broken (perhaps Psychology Today removed it?). Can anyone tell me where I can find this rebuttal? Thanks. JP
What was it about this particular failed replication study that incensed Bargh and motivated an intense outpouring of emotion on his blog for Psychology Today, now removed? What is all the fuss about? Why the cover up? Why was Bargh’s response(s) removed from the scene? Surely the results of Doyen et al.’s study (2012), which has already been cited over 426 times21 at the time of writing, are significant, even if they failed to replicate Bargh’s study. What sparked an outpouring of anger and a scathing personal attack by Bargh in relation to Doyen, an ambitious researcher who tinkered with the experimental apparatus and interpreted the results by drawing links between studies of priming and an archive of earlier psychological experimentation that Hans the Horse is part of. What kind of imaginary or dispositive22 did Doyen’s experimental apparatus enact, such that the specter of Hans returned to haunt Bargh and the field of automaticity research more generally? What made Doyen et al.’s (2012) failed replication study into a media event? Most psychological studies do not become media events. They do not become the subject of journalistic scrutiny, audience speculation and more general public interest. This failed study was the subject of a blog in the Wall Street Journal with the headline ‘Failure to Replicate Famous Study Causes Furore’.23 It was picked up by various bloggers and published in different science magazines,
Social Media Contagion(s)
45
including Nature, Discover Magazine, and websites such as Live Science, with its headline ‘Psychologists Confront Rash of Invalid Studies’.24 It is the subject of numerous Google+ posts and personal blog entries. It has been tweeted and re-tweeted and been discussed in emails that have found their way onto the internet for public consumption. It has travelled and in its travels it has accrued its own agencies, moving from the original scene of experimentation and has become an actor within a mutable scene of entanglement; it opens to multiple leads, crisscrossings, loopings, backtracks, movings and re-movings. It has come to stand in for a history of experiments within psychology that have been tainted with the mark of scandal. Indeed Bargh’s priming study is considered by some to be in the top five most controversial psychology studies ever published.25 It continues to attract attention in the present, variously articulating a perceived crisis within contemporary psychology, including with replication, which is considered the cornerstone of the scientific method.26
It’s all in the mind but whose mind? Doyen et al.’s non-replication study bears the title ‘Behavioural Priming: It’s All in the Mind but Whose Mind?’ – a provocative title no less as it suggests that Bargh and his associate experimenters were unaware that they were influencing the experimental set-up. Within the scientific method, ideally any form of experimental bias or experimental expectation should be eliminated from the experimental scene. Experimental bias is usually related to the beliefs of experimental subjects rather than the experimenter. It is understood to be a form of bias where the subject conforms because they think they know what the experimenter wants. In this context what Doyen et al.’s non-replication revealed was the influence of the experimental apparatus itself and how this might re-qualify what counts as an experimental effect or bias. Doyen’s study challenges the assumption that experimental bias should necessarily be removed or eliminated by its translation into various technical devices for removing suggestion so as to seek objectivity. This is what is known as the ‘experimenter effect’ (Rosenthal, 1966). The need to remove what is understood as experimental bias is often linked to a discovery made within an experiment that has become known as the ‘Hawthorne Effect’.
46
Haunted Data
The Hawthorne Effect The Hawthorne Effect takes its name from a study carried out in the late 1920s which was exploring whether there were any physical characteristics of a factory setting (the Hawthorne factory), such as the level of illumination, that would increase or decrease productivity. The study set out to explore the hypothesis that increased levels of illumination by electric light would increase productivity. The experimenters created a factory setting within a factory (through the use of partition walls) and after establishing a baseline measure of productivity, increased the level of illumination twice and measured productivity (which was seen to rise). By mistake they then decreased the level of illumination and found that productivity still rose. They concluded that the increase of productivity was not due to the level of light, but rather because of the way in which the experimental subjects had been singled out for study and given special attention (Coombs and Smith, 2003). Doyen et al.’s conclusions are interesting because he suggests that experimental bias within the context of Bargh’s study does not mean that priming does not exist. He rather suggests that priming involves mechanisms that are still little understood and that might challenge those typically assumed to underpin priming. Rather than eliminating the experimental bias, he suggests rather that this is a productive process that reveals something very interesting about modes of psychological experimentation in this area. In other words, the experiment introduces epistemic uncertainty into the area of priming, and raises questions about what priming is and how scientists might experiment with priming effects. It does not suggest that experimental bias or the experimental effect should be removed or eliminated, but rather that within the conditions of the experiment, or what we might call following Despret (2015) the dispositive, the implications cannot be adequately investigated. As Despret has argued, what psychology has failed to do is examine and acknowledge ‘the influence of the dispositive itself ’ (p. 81). As the title of Doyen et al.’s article suggests, ‘Behavioural Priming: It’s all in the Mind but Whose Mind?’ It is a scholarly article, which fulfils the criteria expected of a scientific journal article. The citations in the article are mainly from the field of cognitive neuroscience and high-ranking social psychology journals, such as the Journal for Personality and Social Psychology. It is written in the manner of a considered journal article, with clear descriptions of the parameters of
Social Media Contagion(s)
47
the experiments. Its aim is to open up scientific debate about priming and the question of how to understand and analyse priming effects. So what then is all the fuss about?
#conceptual replication The article was eventually published in an open-access science journal with a (mythological) reputation for taking articles which carry a history of rejection from proprietary journals. The journal, PLoS One, has however been lampooned for publishing the Doyen study. John Bargh considers this action a result of their questionable ethics and business model.27 When I initially followed this controversy in May 2012 one was taken to Psychology Today and met with the automated response: ‘Page Not Found’.28 However, while attempting a temporary denouement to the controversy during April 2018, if the user followed the link https://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/the -natural-unconscious/201203/nothing-in-their-headsthey will be led not to Page Not Found (its previous after-life29) but rather to a page which has a link to a third reinstated blog post, posted initially on 11 May 2012 (https://ww w.psycholog ytoday.com/blog/the-natural-unconscious/201205/priming-effe cts-replicate-just-fine-thanks). It now bears the date, ‘Post published by John A Bargh PhD on May 11th, 2012 in The Natural Unconscious’. This version has no comments and does not allow comments to be made by users and interested publics. The prefix post is suggestive of the curious after-lives this URL now carries, where it has come to stand in for the two previously removed blog posts (https://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/the-natural-unconscious/2012 03/nothing-in-their-heads and https://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/the -natural-unconscious/201203/ angry-birds). This blog entry represents an attempt by Bargh to repair what many saw as his damaged reputation, and the blog becomes an actor and agent, which enters into and attempts to change the setting of the controversy. That is to link the controversy to issues and problems surrounding replication, seen as the cornerstone of the scientific method. This included that the controversy was an example of the importance of having strict protocols so that subsequent researchers can replicate scientific experiments. This is considered an important part of how the validity and reliability of statistical results and effects are assessed. One of the key debates on Twitter, for example, became
48
Haunted Data
about ‘conceptual replication’, something that the science journalist Ed Yong picked up, which became part of a discussion associated with the hashtag #conceptual replication.30 One might speculate that the reappearance of Bargh’s blog post https://ww w.psycholog ytoday.com/blog/the-natural-unconscious/201203/nothing-in-t heir-headsis because it corresponds more to the conventions of positivist science writing and might be considered part of a narrative and practice of redemption. It exists in a rather static, unmodifiable form. This was the last blog entry that Bargh wrote which tries to set things straight (again) by responding to an article published in Science News debating priming (https://www.science news.org/article/hot-and-cold-priming), and to a second blog post by Ed Yong, the science journalist, written on the 16 May 2012 (http://www.nature.com/ne ws/replication-studies-bad-copy-1.10634), which discusses the controversies surrounding replication within psychology and the cognitive sciences. It includes a short paragraph, which revisits the Bargh/Doyen controversy: Stéphane Doyen, a cognitive psychologist at the Free University of Brussels, encountered similar issues when he and his colleagues failed to replicate a classic experiment by John Bargh from Yale University in New Haven, Connecticut, showing that people walk more slowly if they have been unconsciously primed with age-related words. After several rejections, Doyen’s paper was also eventually published in PLoS ONE, and drew an irate blog post from Bargh. Bargh described Doyen’s team as ‘inexpert researchers’ and later took issue with the writer of this story for a blog post about the exchange. Bargh says that he responded so strongly partly because he saw growing scepticism of the idea that unconscious thought processes are important, and felt that damage was being done to the field.
At the time I followed this link31 there were no Facebook likes and the post appeared in an un-modifiable form. The second removed blog post, known as ‘Angry Birds’ (https://www.psy chologytoday.com/blog/the-natural-unconscious/201203/angry-birds), was removed and has not reappeared at the time of writing. For some time, if one followed the link one was taken to a dead end, a blank white page. However, at the time of writing the link now takes an interested reader to Bargh’s blog for Psychology Today, The Natural Unconscious, which details three posts, including a link to the recovered post, https://www.psychologytoday.com/blo
Social Media Contagion(s)
49
g/the-natural-unconscious/201205/priming-effects-replicate-just-fi ne-thanks As the reader will be aware, this blog post has come to stand in for the two redacted blog posts, ‘Angry Birds’ and ‘Nothing in Their Heads’. The full text related to ‘Angry Birds’ could be recovered in a Google Doc by following this link: https://docs.google.com/document/d/1wuu8URArgZusJELXF5j4xpM2 6ESkFfOveYoGKBf0CHo/edit?pli=1 Why these patterns of disappearance, removal and reappearance? What archives of the future are put into motion and what is being remembered and forgotten by these data practices of redaction, removal and re-moval? What does this investigation open up for a future psychology – one that can listen to the traces of its past in the present and open to possible futures? As the controversy opened up to traces of the past and the possible futures of priming, very specific entanglements of human and non-human actors also closed it down. It is these narratives which will come to the reader’s attention within Google PageRank algorithms. This includes an interview with John Bargh in The Chronicle for Higher Education some twelve months after the controversy erupted where he narrates the events as a particular redemption narrative.32 In the interview he regrets taking down the blog posts and attempts to settle the controversy and set things straight, again. As is probably clear from my discussion so far this controversy attracted the attention of many scientists in the field. The following extract appears in a blog written by Cedar Riener known as Cedar’s Digest, which provides a remediated story about the John Bargh priming controversy.33 He opens the review of the post-publicationpeer-review commentary with the following spoiler: Attention conservation notice: This post dives into some inside baseball stuff on social psychology, how the science of psychology is practiced, and how science is communicated online. It is kind of long, but I intend it as sort of a reference for this event, but also something to point to for my colleagues who either aren’t aware of, or doubt whether real scientific discussion can occur online.’
He goes on to say, In the past I have been skeptical of online post-publication peer review this serving as a bona fide replacement of peer review. Comparing my own comments when I have served as a reviewer (and received for my submitted manuscripts to those that I leave as a web commenter), the formal peer review
50
Haunted Data
process (anonymous or otherwise) has always come out on top. But this latest episode, and some related conversations, have convinced me that online post-publication peer review has made an amazing amount of progress.
If any readers are having trouble following the detail and time scale of the controversy as it was initially shaped within the context of PPPR, Cedar Riener provides a useful overview and remediation of events in his blog post, some of which I reproduce below:34 Then, this is all within a week: 1. Science writer extraordinaire Ed Yong writes up this failed replication attempt on his blog at Discover magazine: ‘Not Exactly Rocket Science’. 2. John Bargh responds on his blog at Psychology Today. 3. Ed Yong responds to the response on his blog. 4. The psychology blogosphere erupts with conversation and reaction. I am going to annotate a little here. a) The comments on Ed Yong’s first post include someone who is a priming researcher (‘Joe’), another research psychologist (‘Chris’) as well as someone who I would assume is Bobbie Spellman (‘Bobbie’), a well-respected social psychologist and one of the founders of a site devoted to rectifying the fact that unsuccessful studies often never see the light of day: PsychFileDrawer.org b) The initial response by Bargh (62 comments) includes comments by Ed Yong, a neuroscientist who goes by Neuroskeptic, Publisher of PLoS One Peter Binfield, another founder of PsychFileDrawer.org Alex Holcombe, one Peter C who says he is an editor of a major journal in this field, in addition to a number of anonymous commenters. c) Ed Yong’s response to the response has mostly named commenters, including: Well known cognitive (gorilla) psychologist Dan Simons, social psychologist Dave Nussbaum, social psychologist Michael Krauss, neuroscientist Chris Chambers, neuroscientist and editor at Nature Noah Gray, cognitive neuroscientist Matt Craddock, little old me, and social psychologist (and someone who published with Bargh) Gordon Moskowitz. d) Dan Simons google+ post entitled ‘A primer for how not to respond when someone fails to replicate your work’. This includes a long
Social Media Contagion(s)
51
conversation between Dan and another well-known cognitive psychologist, and one of the few reasons I would still visit Psychology TodayArt Markman. e) Friend and social psychologist par excellence Sanjay Srinistava posts reflections on his blog. f) Conversations on twitter about the difference between conceptual replication and direct replication involving Ed Yong and quantitative molecular biologist, Professor of genomics and evolutionary biology at Princeton, and guy you should totally follow on twitter Leonid Kruglyak. And me. Here’s my first attempt at a storify of this convo. The above storify and remediation of this event illustrates how scientific debate, review and assessment are potentially being transformed by digital media and PPPR. Journal articles and books can accrue after-lives as they move on from the event of their publication and enter into new scenes of entanglement. The impact virality of these practices can of course increase the chances of an academic’s work becoming read, cited, used and appropriated. They can extend the reach and traction of an academic’s research, open it to new publics and readerships, and potentially increase the academic’s standing and status. However, as academic work becomes controversial and operates with the potential for contagion it can also become an attractor for agencies, actors, entities, submerged and displaced narratives, which disclose its hauntological potential. If, as Rheinberger has argued, science is a storytelling machine that always has more stories to tell than the experimenter at any given moment is trying to tell with it, what opportunities does this open up for transmedial storytelling? If transmedial storytelling is a form of storytelling that sets in motion fugitive and fossilized times and that allows new visibilities to be shaped, then what might these insights bring to our understanding of scientific progress, innovation and creativity? To what extent can this data be moved around into new contexts, to be separated and reconnected, in order to create something new or unexpected? These questions will form the subject of the next chapter where I will explore the hauntological force of this science controversy and how it unexpectedly re-moves or sets in motion some very interesting and innovative propositions. These connect to key contemporary debates within the field of affect studies, science studies and related perspectives.
52
Haunted Data
Conclusion The ethics and integrity of science communication within these new media worlds and the image and reputation of the field of priming is at the heart of this controversy. However, in order to explore the contagious and hauntological potential of this controversy I will turn in the next chapter to the comparison made between John Bargh and Mr von Osten, the owner of Hans the Horse or Clever Hans, that was made by Ed Yong. Why did it create what some media theorists have termed ‘emotional flaming’, impassioned and emotional responses, which exceed what is often assumed to be the basis of scientific debate – calculative and deliberative rationality, for example. Why did the association between Bargh and Mr von Osten challenge the field of priming studies and what exceeds what has started to settle following this event? Why would the comparison with Mr von Osten upset Bargh? In other contexts the case of Clever Hans has been revitalized in contemporary cultural theory to ask questions about what it means to affect and be affected and what issues this controversy raises about what it means to be open to the other, human and non-human (see Despret, 2004a, 2015). In order to re-examine some of the hauntological potential of this controversy I will expand, extend and mediate the splicing of two different historical moments and temporalities that have become entangled and refracted through one another. This will involve reading what haunts this controversy genealogically, by revisiting an earlier related controversy, which, as we will, see was and remains far from settled. It is this controversy which is re-moved by Ed Yong’s comparison, and becomes spliced with John Bargh’s attempts to manage his reputation as a prestigious cognitive scientist and one of the primary authors of the most highly cited journal article in the field of priming. This ghostly corpus of data extends the controversy across space and time, disrupting the boundaries between past and present, the rational and the emotional, fiction and fact, the human and the technical, and science and media. It is the practices of remembering and forgetting that visualize certain of these data-trails, while others become lifeless or inert. My focus will be on those marginal agencies, which remain as a seething absent-presence, allowing a rather playful investigation of psychology’s many pasts, presents and possible futures.
3
Data-Mediation and Hauntological Analysis: The ‘Clever Hans Charge’ Introduction: Haunting as a form of mediation This chapter will analyse the contagious and hauntological potential of what became known within the John Bargh priming controversy as the ‘Clever Hans charge’ or the ‘Clever Hans slur’. As we saw in the last chapter the association made between John Bargh, and the owner and trainer of Hans the Horse, Mr Wilhelm von Osten, was experienced by Bargh as a slur on his reputation. This slur became a half-hidden statement partially occluded by the redaction of Bargh’s blog post, ‘Nothing in Their Head’, which accrued agencies in its absence. The aliveness of this statement and its generative potential will form the basis of the hauntological analysis performed within this chapter. The focus will be on the splicing of two different historical controversies put back into circulation by this statement, which produced a corpus of ghost or haunted data. I will argue throughout the chapter that hauntological analyses of ghost-data allow an interesting and novel way of analysing the affective potential of forms of data-mediation beyond the actions of individual social media users. It also provides a novel way of engaging with science, particularly with scientific traditions, that have been and are being engaged with by affect scholars. The chapter will engage with arguments that suggest that media can be hauntological, and haunting is a form of mediation. As Mark Fisher (2012) has suggested, certain media forms, practices and genres have allowed us to anticipate a world radically different to the one we now live in. Fisher defines haunting as the mediation of possible (lost) futures in the present – what he terms ‘a virtuality that already impinges on the present, conditioning
54
Haunted Data
expectations and motivating cultural production’ (p. 16). Fisher focuses on music and television as carriers of hauntological forces. He suggests that digital platforms in the present do not present innovation if the potential for hauntology is constrained or erased. He argues that the public broadcasting of the BBC and its potential for hauntology (within what he describes as its visionary public broadcasting ethos from the 1950s to the 1970s) has now diminished, ‘made obsolete by neoliberalism’ (p. 24). This lament is one that places the hauntological potential of media as something that is potentially lost, and places this loss primarily within standardized media content and genres, which are repeated across platforms with little difference. In contrast, Fisher describes hauntological media as visionary and views digitalization as closing down the possibilities of public or social dreaming so necessary to imagine different realities. He laments the BBC broadcasting ethos of the 1950s to 1970s as now firmly in the past, made obsolete by the rise of neoliberalism in the 1980s in Britain and further afield of course. The forms of ‘popular modernism’, the term he also uses to describe hauntological media, refer and come out of a time that is ‘no longer’ (p. 19). Popular forms of modernism are contrasted with the media of now, the media futures, which he suggests reproduce ‘the same thing, seen and/or heard on a new platform’ (18). The future of media therefore returned to a linear historical development tied to broadcast technology and its innovation. Younger generations reading this chapter might feel that this is a form of generational nostalgia – a defensive reaction to so-called digital natives who experience newness as the inevitable forward of progress and innovation. What I am interested in doing is extending this concept of hauntological media in the context of the transmedial controversies followed in this book. The broadcast model of media production and consumption that underpins Fisher’s model does seem rather dated when we consider modern media ecologies with all their recursive entanglements and remediations. Although Fisher’s approach is associated with the work of Jacques Derrida, we will explore how the principles of hauntological inquiry have been developed by many feminist, queer and critical race scholars. I will consider what this adds to current discussions relating to the hauntological potential of social media. Fisher’s approach to mediation is also framed within a broadcast model of media and communications. That is it assumes that media are discrete objects
Data-Mediation and Hauntological Analysis: The ‘Clever Hans Charge’
55
(film, television, music) made up of content which is transmitted to audiences and potentially constrained or enabled by particular institutional structures and organizations: the BBC or Fox news, for example. It follows what many have described as the triangle of perspectives which have been brought to bear upon the study of media and communications: political economy, textual analysis and audience reception specifically within the field of media and communications research.1 Where Fisher laments social and digital media as evidence of the loss of media’s hauntological potential, I want to ask whether social and digital media can be considered hauntological and if so what questions this might open up for data analysis.
Data and the traces of software-driven media One assumption that seems to unite researchers within the field of social media analysis is that one is studying traces, usually digital marks that are left behind by transactions that are made using software-driven media or what are usually referred to as data. The concept of data derives from mathematical and computational approaches to information, which assumes that information can be given some kind of numerical value. This is taken to enable aggregation, comparison, cross-referencing and searching according to common factors and indices. However, this raises the question of what is and is not available to be quantified and what exceeds the instrumentalization of data as metrics. These are critical questions as the concept of traces might not equate to data and might exceed attempts to search and aggregate. These anomalies are particularly intensified given the restrictions on what is searchable using proprietary software shaped by APIs owned by conglomerates such as Google, Facebook and Twitter for example. In that sense, critical methodological issues are raised regarding what it means to engage in ‘data ethnographies’ which take software media as an object of analysis (see Hochman and Manovich, 2013 for example; Langlois, 2015). These questions are recognized as important aspects of a cultural politics and analysis of the social life of data. This issue has been raised as an important aspect of contemporary sociological inquiry (see Beer and Burrows, 2013). As we saw in the last chapter, data have a life, which exceeds attempts to measure and quantify behaviour, thought and existence. Data have
56
Haunted Data
hauntological force as they move from the original event, accruing what Matt Fuller (2009) has termed ‘after-lives’. These after-lives can be approached hauntologically, directing our attention to what already exists in the present as a form of anticipation, to a time in the present that has not passed, and which operates as an attractor for possible futures. The after-lives of data attract, collect and channel fragments, gaps, anomalies, puzzles and parallel temporalities producing the potential for ‘queer aggregations’. They can flow and arrest, stopping us briefly in our tracks before they disappear from view, or at least from those narratives which quickly frame and interpret data according to particular authorized stories. As boyd and Crawford (2012) remind us, all data require interpretation. Some interpretations are staged and authorized through relations of prestige, status and hierarchy and are embedded within the ontologies of the network enacted by computational objects, such as Google PageRank. These almost always remove wonder from the world. Gitelman and Jackson (2013) have argued that there is no such thing as ‘raw data’; data has to be imagined as data and such imaginings are materialized through technical forms that generate, aggregate, code, classify, pattern and sort information into specific data-forms. These forms of management are largely obscured once data is scraped, visualized and made to speak in relation to certain issues, debates and matters of concern. As they argue, data is cooked and has to be ‘imagined as data to exist and function as such, and the imagination of data entails an interpretive base’ (2013: 3). In that sense, data is not simply a function of machines (algorithms, software, programmes, etc.), but is imagined as data in different ways and through different interpretive and often disciplinary imaginaries. Data, as with statistical forms of analysis, never speak for themselves. Data assume rhetorical forms, functions and strategies. One dominant form is the speculative, anticipative relations shaped from the aggregation and enactment of data patterns, put to work in order to generate future value and capital. Data are assembled, reassembled and re-performed. One thing that is an axiom is that data within digital and software media environments accumulate, leave traces and also disappear. Despite the myth or fiction of the universal database, where it is assumed that every action and transaction leave a trace, this dystopian myth of complete dataveillance does not stack up. Data disappear, are removed, become submerged or displaced, are lost, overlooked, deemed irrelevant, make accidental connections (rather
Data-Mediation and Hauntological Analysis: The ‘Clever Hans Charge’
57
than aggregated patterns), can remain alert or lifeless (Gitelman and Jackson, 2013). This often depends on the kinds of imaginaries at work. As Gitelman and Jackson (2013: 6) argue, ‘Data require participation. Data need us.’ Data can obscure ‘ambiguity, conflict and contradiction’ (Gitelman and Jackson, 2013: 8), or work against such tendencies. This requires different acts of data mobilization, interpretation and narrativization. As much as ghosts might be considered material transmutations ‘in time’, which travel with us, I stage my archive of data as a haunted archive which materializes the past in the present as particular forms of ‘time travel’. Time is stretched in these archives; it becomes disjointed, emotional, bent and otherworldly. The data are enacted as agents of temporal disruption. The data look backwards and project forwards to what has been left behind after particular narratives and sorting processes have taken place, as well as to those traces and absences, which operate as a double to this process. I am looking for something more than now and this refrain will be repeated in myriad ways throughout this chapter. To that extent the concept of haunted data also points towards the hauntological forces at work within and across social and digital media. I therefore do not agree with Mark Fisher (2012), where he suggests that new media forms have lost their hauntological potential and repeat the same with little difference, closing down on the potential of staging futures different to the one now. This offers a bleak horizon in my view that is too tied to technical form. It is an argument that overlooks the imaginaries that are as much at work in so-called new media, as they have always been in media sometimes considered old or even obsolete. Queer archives and queer imaginings have been described in these ways long before and provide an important reference point for analysing software media’s hauntological potential.2 Within this context, my focus throughout this and the next three chapters will be on the haunted media times and temporalities, which return and might be captured and dramatized through specific scenes of entanglement. Although my focus is on science controversies, we will see they have much broader repercussions for the politics and ethics of mediation. Rather than seeing the individual user as the site of such entanglements, I will argue that a data hauntology provides a method for exploring, analysing and representing controversies as particular scenes of entanglement. The scenes of entanglement
58
Haunted Data
I will perform and analyse stage multiple (media) times and temporalities that open up to queer, feminist and even non-human and otherworldly voices. This chapter will stage two related scenes of entanglement, which will allow the reader to orient their attention to the hauntological potential of the ‘Clever Hans charge’ that Bargh experienced as a slur on his reputation. They overlap, criss-cross, intersect and backtrack to the scene of entanglement developed in the previous chapter. The controversy brings back the ghost of Clever Hans. It stages a re-imagining of what ‘Clever Hans’ might be trying to tell us if only we could and would listen. The method in different ways emphasizes the non-linear times that hauntologies apprehend and the importance of developing a distributed and mediated form of perception (many eyes and ears – human and non-human) in order to create the possibility of ‘seeing’ what often remains foreclosed, disavowed, fugitive and yet which seethes as an absent-presence. This chapter more explicitly brings science studies into conversation with these approaches and opens up a novel imaginary for mining, poaching, re-moving, assembling and mediating the extended and distributed perception that software media in some instances might make possible.
Scene 2. Clever Hans: The horse that won’t go away Clever Hans was a horse with apparently remarkable and prodigious talents. He captivated audiences in the early twentieth century keen to witness his abilities and offer explanations for his seeming mathematical acuity: stamping his hooves it was claimed he was able to tell the time and solve simple arithmetical problems. It was also claimed that he could spell, recognize people from photographs and differentiate pieces of music and different colours. Was he psychic or a genius? This question was central to how he was judged by enthralling psychologists and other experts with his demonstrations and capturing the attention of newspapers, including The New York Times which published an article on Clever Hans on 4 September 1904, with the headline, ‘Berlin’s Wonderful Horse – He Can Do Almost Everything But Talk – How He Was Taught’. The article informs the reader that Hans’s equine abilities were not to be found in imagination but rather in fact. These facts, we are told had
Data-Mediation and Hauntological Analysis: The ‘Clever Hans Charge’
59
Figure 2 Hans the Horse with Mr von Osten. Copyright Wikimedia.commons.
been witnessed by many, including the scientific, military and sporting worlds. His credentials were to be found in his performances of such amazing feats, which had even been taken seriously and witnessed by the Prussian royalty, including the young Duke of Sachse-Coburg-Gotha (Hans was apparently able to spell his name correctly!). We are told in the article that Hans’s owner, Mr von Osten, is a retired schoolteacher who lives in a tenement building. Since his retirement and for the last four years he has been teaching Hans, much like he had taught schoolchildren, to read, count and tell the time. He used various devices to enlist Hans’s attention, including a blackboard and a counting machine comprised of balls (an abacus); modulation of his own voice; vegetables and minerals, including sugar and carrots; the mechanics of the horse’s foot; and various human and non-human objects. These objects included gold, silver and copper coins, alphabet letters and various members of the public who took part in identity recognition parades. Hans’s acute sensitivities were also demonstrated in his ability to attune to musical scales and to be able to differentiate tones. The article contains some notable comments by Professor Karl Moebius, an eminent Prussian zoologist (who at the time was the director of the Prussian Natural History Museum). He suggests that wild horses exhibit
60
Haunted Data
some of these potentialities providing evidence that horses could indeed be taught such skills. Moebius’s own expertise was in relation to marine animals and he is famous for coining the term ‘biocenose’, which refers to the relations and interactions between organisms, which make up an ecosystem. His own studies focused specifically on oysters and mussels. This approach became known as ‘synecology’, the ecological study of communities of plants and animals. Moebius’s accreditation of Hans’s abilities (and von Osten’s teaching prowess) will be important in what surfaces later as the traces of the controversy have travelled across time. I am equating this time-travelling to what Rheinberger (1994) terms ‘historiality’ or the historical movement of a trace. The article finishes by saying that Mr von Osten indeed claimed that any horse of fair intelligence could be taught. It finishes by pre-empting that by the time the reader will have seen this article the German Kaiser will have seen and witnessed the prodigy known as Hans. By 12 September 1904 a special commission of experts brought together to judge Hans’s abilities had been assembled – this became known as the Hans Commission. The Hans Commission was a response to a request to the German Board of Education by Mr von Osten who was keen for experts to witness Hans’s talents. It was comprised of psychologists (notably Professor Carl Stumpf a philosopher and psychologist at Berlin University), a retired military person, zoologists, teachers, a circus manager, a veterinarian and physiologist, among others. The conclusion of this commission was that there was no fraud. It was not a hoax and Hans’s abilities defied various explanations brought to bear upon him. Here was a horse that could genuinely think and reason and not simply learn tricks. The commission was reported on in The London Standard on 13 September 1904 with the headline, ‘‘Clever Hans’ again. Expert Commission Decides That the Horse Actually Reasons’. The article cites a statement from the commission ‘that it is of opinion that there is no trickery whatsoever in the performances of the horse and that the methods employed by the owner Mr Herr von Osten, in teaching Hans essentially differ from those used by trainers, and correspond with those teaching children in elementary school’. This article was reprinted in The New York Times on 2 October 1904. Although Prof Stumpf was satisfied with the conclusions of the commission, of which he was a participant, his doctoral student Oscar Pfungst saw the
Data-Mediation and Hauntological Analysis: The ‘Clever Hans Charge’
61
occasion for further investigations. Pfungst is credited with ‘solving the mystery’ within contemporary historiographies of psychology and with the associated narratives of the controversy, which appear in many places and related sciences. Pfungst conducted a series of imaginative experiments, which requalified the proposition that Hans was either psychic or a genius. These experiments were clearly of interest to Hans and extended and disclosed the nature of his intelligences.3 The various experiments that Pfungst conducted with Hans and Mr von Osten were written up as a book length analysis based on a psychological assessment of what was considered a performance of unconscious deceit (Pfungst, 1911). Mr von Osten was accused of unwitting fraud and the book is written in the manner of a legal report and judgement on what had become a national controversy and scandal. Here was a controversy that even at the time the book was written was far from settled. Pfungst’s report was framed in the preface to the book, written by James Angell an American psychologist, as a final verdict to settle the matter once and for all. Angell was perhaps a good person to write the preface having a reputation for resolving disputes and intervening within controversial issues within psychology (see Angell and Moore, 1896). The riddle was solvable. Unconscious unintentional movements made by Mr von Osten, his owner and trainer, were influencing Hans, it was argued. Without von Osten, Hans was impotent, although not everybody agreed with this statement. Despite Pfungst’s intervention and the judgements that were made, which brought Mr von Osten into disrepute, the many after-lives of Hans lived on. He had many fans due to the international sensation, which surrounded his performances. He had already become an attractor for many different interests, actors and agencies. This included vets, animal trainers, physiologists, psychic investigators, psychologists, philosophers and medical doctors, for example. He was compared to talented circus animals by the American director of the Bronx Park Zoological Gardens, Dr William T. Hornaday in his 1922 book, The Mind and Manners of Wild Animals. Rudolf Steiner, the Austrian philosopher, social reformer, architect and spiritualist enthusiastically introduced Hans as an interesting case study for exploring human–animal relations. In his Nine Lectures on Bees, comprised of lectures and interviews, first published in 1923 and translated and published into English in 1998, Steiner suggests that Pfungst was wrong. Where Pfungst
62
Haunted Data
claimed that Hans could read minimal, almost imperceptible unintentional movements (facial expressions for example) expressed by von Osten, Steiner suggests a more spiritual reading of Hans through his training in anthroposophic science. Hans had the ability to be able to ‘read von Osten’s brain’ – to get inside his head – and this ability Steiner suggests had been cultivated through Hans’s love of sugar, fed to him by von Osten during his education and training. This sugar-related bond had enabled Hans to develop an acute sensitivity such that he could anticipate and sense von Osten’s thoughts – a taste for what Steiner termed ‘truly invisible things’ (p. 59). This taste was shared with bees, other animals and even plants, who he argued were able to attune, affect and be affected by human and non-human others. Hans therefore travelled and was taken up by many people keen to make him speak for their own particular theories and philosophies. The figure of Hans retained celebrity status even if many psychologists became less and less entranced with his seemingly miraculous actions. He also lived on in the interest more generally in ‘wonder animals’ that Sofie Lachapelle documents in her 2010 article, ‘On Hans, Zou and the Others’. Hans also shares a queer kinship with a range of psychic animals, including Lady Wonder the Typing Horse, a supposed ‘mind-reading’ horse who was investigated by J. B. Rhine and Louisa E. Rhine in 1929. They used experimental techniques and methods developed in their parapsychology laboratory at Duke University.
Psychic animals Rhine and Rhine had an interest in the reporting of telepathic sensitivities in animals and rather than adopting an attitude of ‘credulous skepticism’ (Rhine and Rhine, 1929a: 449), they took the investigation of animal telepathy into the scientific laboratory. Rhine and Rhine drew a lineage between a range of cases where owners testify to the psychic abilities of their pets; this includes Dodgerfield and Roger, two unrelated psychic dogs, Clever Hans, telepathic circus dogs, the Elberfeld horses,4 as well as human psychics including Van Dam, who could be ‘willed’ ‘to move to a given square on the board’ and ‘would tap twice on the board when he felt a ‘conviction’ that he had the right square’ (ibid: 451).5 Lady Wonder the Typing Horse was the specific animal subject of
Data-Mediation and Hauntological Analysis: The ‘Clever Hans Charge’
63
Figure 3 Lady Wonder the Typing Horse with Mrs Fonda (the owner). Copyright Wikimedia.commons.
their telepathy experiments. She was a three-year-old filly, ‘black with white face and feet’ (ibid: 451; see Figure 3). Lady Wonder’s owner Mrs Fonda’s claims that Lady Wonder could ‘mind-read’ were subjected to a battery of tests to determine whether these claims were rather dependent on hyperesthesia. Hyperesthesia is the argument that rather than telepathy, the animal was responding to signals made by the owner, either conscious or unconscious. Professor William McDougall (1910), an eminent British social psychologist, had moved to Duke University due to a dislike of the English climate. He had acted as a mentor to Rhine and Rhine, who were the first to coin the term ‘parapsychology’ and to set up the first parapsychology laboratory under his tutelage. McDougall had an interest in telepathy, hypnosis, suggestion and other psychic phenomena. He had been influenced by the writings of Gabriel Tarde and saw contagious phenomena as central aspects of understanding what it means to be human (see Blackman, 2013). Rhine and Rhine were protégés of McDougall; J. B. Rhine was initially trained as a botanist but had
64
Haunted Data
been galvanised into action in May 1922 when he went to a lecture given in Chicago by Sir Arthur Conan Doyle at the time almost as well known for his dabbling in spiritualism as for his famous detective, Sherlock Holmes (Rhine, 1937: 50). This changed the course of J. B. Rhine’s career where the investigation of psychic phenomena as part of psychology became his and his wife’s life’s work. On the basis of their investigations of Lady Wonder, the experimental team, which included McDougall (referred to as M), concluded that the telepathic explanation was the only one that held sway. They acknowledged that telepathy was an ‘unknown process’ (ibid: 465). They also argued that telepathy also seemed to involve hypnosis. Both were facilitated by an equally unknown process, called ‘willing’, which was also described as ‘telepathy under hypnosis’ (p. 451). In a later postscript, the Rhines described a further study of Lady Wonder, which took place in December 1928, where they did not replicate earlier results. This led them to observe that her owner, Mrs Fonda, and specifically her voice and whip seemed to ‘coordinate very significantly with the body movements’ of Lady Wonder (1929b: 288). Horse, whip, voice and the proximity of horse to the particular human who stood at her head seemed to account for Lady Wonder’s superior intelligences. This led the Rhines to conclude not that telepathy did not exist or had not been observed in the previous experiments, rather ‘that the telepathic ability we earlier found the horse to possess has been now almost if not entirely lost and that Lady has become merely a trained animal conditioned to a system of signals made up of indicative body movements, voice inflections, whip movements’ (p. 291). This they argued is not unique to Lady, as telepathy in humans can be weakened or even lost. This might be due to a change in her passivity or sleepy state, which they linked to telepathy under hypnosis. This was seen to provide one of the clues as to her subsequent loss of sensitivities. Lest the reader think that this is an outmoded statement that science would now dispel, one only has to confront contemporary neuroscientific experiments on hypnosis to see how little is understood about hypnotic suggestion, and the question of what it means to enter into suggestive relations with another, human and non-human. In an experiment which caught the attention of BBC Radio 4 All In the Mind series,6 the episode, ‘Hypnoticism; Automatic Writing, Magic and Memory’ broadcast on 23 December 2014 discussed a
Data-Mediation and Hauntological Analysis: The ‘Clever Hans Charge’
65
contemporary neuroscientific experiment using hypnosis and the technique of automatic writing (see Walsh et al., 2014). The assumption of the experiments carried out by a team of neuroscientists at Kings College, London, is that the normative psychological subject is aware and has ownership and control of their thoughts and movements. Using a particular experimental apparatus, involving the technique of automatic writing, a specially constructed writing frame, the technique of hypnotic induction and a mock MRI scanner, they modulate this sense of control and automaticity, such that subjects experience their hand moving as if was being directed by someone or something else, what they also describe as the phenomenology of alien control. However, the authors also highlight an important tension in the experiment that challenges and exceeds the assumption they are working with, but which is left hanging as an anomaly and puzzle. They assume that ‘hypnotic phenomena must be experienced as involuntary and effortless’ by hypnotized subjects (p. 33), while recognizing that this actually relates to a set of cultural beliefs or ‘expectancy effects’ about hypnotic suggestion, which are tied to ‘explicit learning’. This paradox has also been captured by the term ‘cultural invitation’, developed by the anthropologist Tanya Luhrmann (2011) within the context of voice hearing and suggestion. ‘Cultural invitation’ identifies how local theories of mind shape perception, sensation and attention. This term is useful within the context of the biohumanities and affect studies because it suggests that the biological or materiality is cultural right the way down, all the way to the bottom, and even to what is often experienced as automatic, visceral, raw and so forth. It reveals what might be described as the body’s ‘deep history’ (Smail, 2007). However, I would also argue that the concept of ‘cultural invitation’ is overly cognitivist and rationalist. It foregrounds beliefs and theories of mind, rather than opening up to mechanisms and processes of becoming that exceed learning, conditioning and cultivation understood in this way. The cognitivism of ‘cultural invitation’ will form the subject of Part 2 of the book, where we will find some interesting propositions to explore this within the context of the Feeling the Future controversy. These propositions are less cognitivist and approach psychological processes as transitive and distributed across a range of human and non-human actors and agents. The precursors to these propositions can be found in psychology’s past, and
66
Haunted Data
particularly at the nexus of psychic and psychological research. This was at a historical moment where I argue that psychological processes were considered more transitive and contiguous with the technical, ecological, non-human, historical and cultural. This was a transdisciplinary moment, which occurred at a point before the professionalization of disciplines and where researchers were experimenting in more speculative and proto-performative ways with the question of what it means to be human. The examples we will explore were more open to the radical indeterminacy of the human than we might now find in the biohumanities and related perspectives. They offer up some interesting ways of exploring cultural invitation in the context of affect studies. The puzzle of ‘cultural invitation’ is however left unresolved in this particular neuroscientific experiment, and demonstrates how phenomena such as suggestion, contagion, imitation, telepathy, automatic writing and so forth still present challenges to the contemporary psychological, cognitive and neurosciences. They are part of ‘weird science’, which challenges the psychological individualism of the psychology such examples constantly exceed, displace and subvert. To repeat a refrain presented in Chapter 1, made by Isabelle Stengers: ‘But above all, what do we really know about this suggestion that we are supposed to avoid?’ (Stengers, 1997: 103). Clever Hans, Lady Wonder the Typing Horse, as well as experiments on automatic writing, mediumship, human and animal telepathy, clairvoyance, precognition and related phenomena are all part of this science of oddities, exceptions and anomalies. This strange or queer kinship are re-moved or put back into circulation by Clever Hans’s association with John Bargh in the present. These associations have to be continually policed by the cultivated scientific attitude of credulous scepticism, which at the same time helps to keep the association alive but fixed as part of science’s past. They represent an archive of ‘unreason’ which haunts contemporary psychology. As a host for this association I want to offer a temporary home, which can open up to other positions which are not exhausted by this sceptical attitude. As a very queer coupling, the Bargh/Clever Hans association is one that has not gone unnoticed by cultural theorists and philosophers who have asked critical questions about the kinds of stories that we might tell about human/animal relations that disrupt the anthropocentrism of the masterful human exerting control over the animal ‘other’ (Despret, 2004a, 2015; also see Haraway, 2007; Massumi, 2015).
Data-Mediation and Hauntological Analysis: The ‘Clever Hans Charge’
67
These critical questions and more inventive stories also raise important questions about what it means to enter into suggestive relations with another. The excess to this storytelling and the inventive stories yet to be told points towards ‘archives of the future’. They might help open up the potential of cultural theory to shape a future psychology and future visions for worldmaking within the context of psychology. This excess will form the subject matter of Scene 3. What happened to Hans, the reader might ask? Did Hans, along with Mr von Osten, fall into disrepute allowing psychology to relieve itself of its close connection and proximity to psychic research? What disturbed psychologists? If Hans was a ‘good imitator’, as Dr Strumpf claimed, then what did this disclose about the nature of suggestion and imitation? What further debates, problems, questions and issues were specified on the basis of Hans’s capacity to imitate? Who was imitating whom in this story (see Despret, 2004a)? Is this a story about Hans’s capacities or the story of a relational connection that extended and distributed mind as a collective, shared process, even if Mr von Osten was seemingly oblivious to his unwitting participation? If priming alters thought, action and behaviour, where in this case a horse can be made to add or subtract or tell the time through its connection with another, then who is mediating whom? In whose mind should the capacity to imitate be located or is this the wrong question to be asking?
Scene 3. Berlin’s wonderful horse, synecology and feminist/queer diffraction ‘He can do everything but talk – how he was taught’. New York Times, 4 September 1904. ‘During all studies of animal behavior, any face-to-face contact between the examiner and the experimental animal should be strictly avoided’ (Samhita and Gross, 2013: e27122.2). ‘The horse was simply a channel through which the information the questioner unwittingly put into the situation was fed back to the questioner. The fallacy involved treating the horse as the source of the message rather
68
Haunted Data
than as a channel through which the questioner’s own message is reflected back’ (http://skepdic.com/cleverhans.html).
In a reconsideration of the ‘Clever Hans Phenomenon’, the lessons of Clever Hans are linked to the problem of controlling for face-to-face contact between experimenter and experimental subject, human and animal in this context (Samhita and Gross, 2013). As with experimental psychology, the problem of ‘experimental expectation’ and ‘experimenter bias’ (Rosenthal, 1966) has come to be seen as an issue that might confound experiments exploring animal intelligence, and ideally should be controlled, removed or eliminated. One identified problem is how to modulate facial expressiveness and its potential to (inadvertently) shape the experimental apparatus. The authors go on to argue that ‘professional poker players know about the importance of unwitting cues and present a “pokerface,” even going so far as to wear dark sunglasses’ (Rosenthal, 1966: e21722.3). Experimental bias and expectation is considered a design problem, which ideally should be eliminated or controlled for in order to produce valid and reliable evidence. This, it would seem, is one of the lessons of Clever Hans that lives on within positivist experimental practices. Experimental bias is carried by the slur that this is nothing but ‘the Clever Hans Effect’ that helped to orchestrate John Bargh’s umbrage. Let’s return to a rather different statement made by an ethologist of the early twentieth century who had something to say about Clever Hans, which reveals a trace of what experimental psychology might have become if it had taken a more ecological turn. This more ecological way of specifying human/ animal, organism/environment relations does not start with a bifurcation between nature and culture and the subject and object. Arguably, it is this trace and its movement within and across cultural and feminist theory, including feminist science studies, which opens up to an archive of the future (Derrida, 1995). Let’s return to Professor Karl Moebius, the eminent Prussian zoologist who endorsed Hans’s abilities in a New York Times article written in 1904. In this article Moebius has the following to say about what we might term the synecology of Hans-von-Osten. Synecology brings together the prefix syn of Greek origin to mean, together with, and ecology – studies of the interaction between organisms and their environments. Moebius coined the term biocenosis in 1877,7 a key concept within synecology, which refers to
Data-Mediation and Hauntological Analysis: The ‘Clever Hans Charge’
69
the interactions between organisms within a particular habitat. This perhaps gives us a clue to the basis of his enthusiastic endorsement of the Clever Hans phenomenon.
(Extract from The New York Times, 1904, ‘Berlin’s Wonderful Horse – He Can Do Everything but Talk – How He Was Taught’.)
Moebius draws attention to Hans’s ‘hoof language’ and his ‘desire for delicacies’, which are modulated by a variety of experimental set-ups (or his surroundings). This apparatus or setting is taken to retain Hans’s interest and curiosity, or at least his want for food. The potentialities of Hans and von Osten become entangled and take form as shared sensitivities, which allow them both to be moved by a material-semiotic apparatus which disappears through training, habit and discipline. At least, this is another interpretation, which is compatible with what we know about Moebius and his interest in co-evolution and co-enactment within marine animal ecologies, or what he might rather have termed the close interdependence of one with the other. These traces are re-moved (Rheinberger, 1994) by Vinciane Despret, the Belgian science studies scholar who introduced Clever Hans to body studies and cultural theory within the journal Body & Society in 2004. This article was in a special issue on science
70
Haunted Data
and technology studies devoted to the important question of what counts as a body when the human and technical are not viewed as separate pre-existing entities. It also contains Bruno Latour’s (2004) oft-cited article ‘How to Talk about the Body? The Normative Dimensions of Science Studies’.8 Despret (2004a) reconsiders Pfungst’s interpretation of Clever Hans and argues that it opens up more questions than it answers. Although Pfungst aimed to settle and solve the mystery once and for all, Despret (2004a: 113) argues that the most interesting aspect of this story is the way Pfungst decided to construct the problem. Yes, it was a beautiful case of influence, but it was more over a wonderful opportunity to explore a fascinating question. Indeed, the horse could not count, but he could do something more interesting: not only could he read bodies, but he could make human bodies be moved and be affected, and move and affect other beings and perform things without their owners’ knowledge. And this could be experimentally studied. Hans could become a living apparatus that enabled the exploration of very complicated links between consciousness, affects and bodies. Hans could play the role of a device that induced new articulations between consciousness, affects, muscles, will, events ‘at the fringe’ of consciousness (Pfungst, 1911: 203); he could be a device that, furthermore, made these articulations visible. Hans, in other words, could become a device that enabled humans to learn more about their bodies and their affects. Hans embodied the chance to explore other ways by which human and non-human bodies become more sensitive to each other.
She goes on to suggest that the practices and training enabling the Clever Hans phenomena are not just human practices or on the side of the human. As she argues, ‘Hans was teaching them what made him move. Hans the horse was as much leading them as the humans were leading him. Their human bodies were not only sensitive to their own desire to make the horse succeed, they were also translating the horse’s desire to help them to lead him successfully. Let us not miss that last point: Hans wouldn’t have done so well if he had not been interested in the game, sometimes for different reasons than some of the humans’ (p. 116). Importantly, she suggests that these sensitivities and their modulation would need to occur without intention (implying consciously directed thought) and more through anticipation, an embodied sense that might be considered more non-conscious or even
Data-Mediation and Hauntological Analysis: The ‘Clever Hans Charge’
71
affective (also see Game, 2001). However, despite opening up these questions and riddles Hans’s after-lives are not so exciting it seems. As Despret argues, the potential of this controversy is reduced to and replaced by ‘the most impoverished version of Hans’s marvellous story’ (p. 121).9
The ‘Clever Hans charge’ Despret illustrates this by turning to contemporary experimental psychology and the Clever Hans phenomena or effect as it is now termed. Disappointingly (from a science studies scholar’s perspective), Clever Hans is reduced to the problem of experimental bias and the importance of eradicating this from the experimental scene (see Rosenthal, 1966). As we have seen, this denouement can be found in many places across past and contemporary historiographies of psychology. When Hans’s more interesting after-lives are opened up these are simultaneously closed down and seen as a legacy of both how gullible science can be (according to critics and sceptics) and of its ‘queer’ kinship with other controversial experiences and phenomena that cross cognitive science and psychic research. Importantly, it is argued that these need to be policed and expelled. These phenomena include facilitated communication, psychic animals and pets (including Lady Wonder the Typing Horse) and mental telepathy, or what were also termed ‘Wonder Animals’ (see Lachapelle, 2010). Clever Hans is entangled closely with psychic and paranormal phenomena, all of which often appear and are framed as hoaxes and scandals, tainting the area of priming and automaticity within cognitive science. There are traces of Hans’s more curious after-lives, which appear in veterinary science for example. This includes a special issue of Vetlearn (2012) devoted to the Clever Hans phenomena. In this issue, the guest editorial written by James R Coffman, with the title ‘The Clever Hans Effect’10 reconsiders Clever Hans as an example of the extraordinary sensitivities of equine perception that practitioners should attune to, understand and enter into as part of their practice. This interestingly is not framed as something that should be suppressed or eliminated from encounters but rather focused upon and even increased and improved as part of ‘his or her awareness of the subtlety of the human-horse interface’. The notion of an interface also conjures up an image of a shared boundary through which an exchange takes place, an exchange where what emerges does not pre-exist the relating perhaps.
72
Haunted Data
These traces appear in odd places and are not part of the narratives or forms of ‘bundled time’ (see Ruppert et al., 2013), which stand in for the potential after-lives. They are perhaps traces of what Derrida (1995) termed an ‘archive of the future’. They exist as outliers, rarely if ever becoming objects, entities or sources, which are performative. They are rather non-performative (Ahmed, 2010; Butler, 2010), never or rarely transforming the relationship of cognitive science to its own rather queer archive. These outliers suggest some rather different ways of understanding suggestive capacities and bodies’ capacities to affect and be affected. In order to understand what might be at stake I will discuss some ways of conceiving of suggestion, which amplify and extend more radical understandings of the extraordinary capacities of equine perception. I will do this by turning to more marginal ways of thinking about the suggestive capacities of the human in the context of hypnotic suggestion. This reveals the importance of approaching suggestion technically and has resonances with Vinciane Despret’s approach, as I will go on to illustrate. In order to situate the discussion I will turn to the practices of Milton Erikson, a medical hypnotist, who discloses the importance of approaching suggestion technically and as a potentiality distributed across a range of actors and agencies. Milton Erikson (see Erikson and Rossi, 1981) spent years developing techniques to induce hypnotic trance, and subjected the phenomenon of hypnotic trance to particular kinds of experimental staging. He published numerously in a range of different experimental psychological journals during the 1940s, 1950s, 1960s and 1970s. He also edited the Journal of Clinical Hypnosis until his death in 1980 and is a seminal figure in the establishment of medical hypnosis exploring the role of hypnotic trance in the induction of anaesthesia and pain relief, for example. Importantly, Erikson did not frame suggestion as a problem of will. Rather, he saw hypnosis as a different way of engaging and knowing. Hypnosis was approached as a more incorporeal form of knowing and feeling that positions the successful hypnotist as somebody who can read very subtle minimal bodily communications. Rather than suspending ‘will’ this was about developing the capacity for attunement that would allow the hypnotist to engage the subject through non-conscious forms of knowing and awareness. This would enable the subtle redistribution of ways of feeling, thinking and knowing that might result in changes in physiology, neurology, the nervous system and
Data-Mediation and Hauntological Analysis: The ‘Clever Hans Charge’
73
so forth. However, attunement was not simply a human affair. Milton’s life story is particularly interesting in the context of thinking about the technicity of suggestion, and the role of training and practice to the orchestration of what might take form. This is what I call a more developmental aspect to the modulation of this capacity, which takes into account Erikson’s life-history, as well as the milieu and setting which shaped his practices. Due to his history of polio and congenital sensory problems this resulted in Erikson being tonedeaf and colour blind (see Rosen, 2010). As Rosen (2010) argues, these problems encouraged Erikson to pay attention to minute muscular movements in his attempts to rehabilitate his own partial paralysis and overcome a limp due to the polio. He also developed an acute sensitivity to minimal bodily communications in order to develop ways of knowing that did not rely on cognition – famously he said he could tell a good piano player by the way their hands moved across the keyboard. However, what also characterized being able to enter into hypnotic relations and bring about change was persistence and perspicacity. In other words, suggestion was highly technical and could not be located within a psychological capacity such as will. What was important in this process was the setting that would allow various psycho-physiological processes to be aroused ‘into activity by indirect forces’ (Rossi, 1980). This was not about the will of the hypnotist and the compliance of the subject, but rather the capacity or affordance of the experimental apparatus to be sensitive and enact attunement in meaningful or even surprising ways. This is a point that Vinciane Despret (2008) makes in her discussion of psychologist’s attempts to communicate with animals. She argues that the co-production of meaningful communication across species-borders is dependent upon the success of the apparatus in redistributing the parameters and terms of what is meaningful for the species in question. Despret recounts issues that emerge from experiments designed to enable communication between humans and animals – that is talking with parrots. She asks the question: What makes an experimental apparatus successful within this context? Rather than just focus on the objects – the devices or non-human actors – she also focuses her attention on how to think and approach subjectivity in relation to this question. She banishes objectivity and control from the scene, showing how impoverished these organizational concepts are in creating ‘interested’
74
Haunted Data
experimental subjects. Drawing on the work of the psychologist Irene Pepperberg, who is credited with success in speaking with parrots, she considers why the experimental apparatus that Pepperberg created was efficacious in facilitating communication. Objectivity and control are replaced with the concepts of interest and attunement, which fundamentally redistribute control and agency within the apparatus. Pepperberg recognized for example that parrots’ use of language is pragmatic rather than referential and that the apparatus would have to be sensitive to this in order to keep the parrot interested, to remain attuned to what matters to the parrot and adjust accordingly. This also challenges one of the other axioms of contemporary positivist experimental analytics – generalization. Rather than approaching generalization as what might be possible or probable to predict from a specific case, ‘generalization is something that might be accomplished ‘bit by bit’ ‘in terms of the possibilities that the apparatus could actualize’ (p. 128). Generalization qualifies the apparatus rather than the subject (the parrot and the human as essential categories for example). As she argues, The question is now about the effectiveness of the apparatus, the researcher’s desire no longer being anything but one of the modes of this efficacity. The apparatus reveals nothing, it testifies instead to the power of the transformations themselves. As a corollary of this resistance, the question of the subjectivity of the parrot no longer has much sense, short of restricting it and rendering it very concrete. If the parrot can talk, we do not know what it is, nor what parrotness is, nor anything about the point of view of parrots on the world. But we do learn in a viable manner about its point of view on the apparatus. We learn something about its point of view on the new materials with which it will make a world: colour boxes, numbers, words, a grammar, forms, humans and abstractions. In the same manner that the refusal to talk, in other apparatuses, constitutes an expression of the parrot’s opinion in relation to the relevance of what it is asked, the fact that it engages with, accepts and actively transforms what becomes a part of its world, translates an extension of this world and therefore an extension of its subjectivity as ‘parrot-with-human’. (p. 128)
The human, animal and apparatus work together to accomplish what is possible, she suggests, through attributing intentions and acting accordingly – human and animal (also see Game, 2001). This she suggests is about the extension of subjectivities rather than the banishment of subjectivity from the scene. In a development of this thesis in an article in Theory, Culture & Society (2013),
Data-Mediation and Hauntological Analysis: The ‘Clever Hans Charge’
75
Despret turns her attention to the different ways in which scientists’ bodies are involved with the animals they observe in the field. She equates this to a form of ‘embodied empathy’. This is defined as ‘a concept, which describes feeling/ seeing/thinking bodies that undo and redo each other, reciprocally though not symmetrically, as partial perspectives that attune themselves to each other’ (p. 51). So as well as the importance of the experimental apparatus – that is the devices – in actualizing what might be possible, the subjectivity or transsubjectivity of the experimental subject, and how to think this are also an important part of the success or efficacy of inventing or constructing particular experimental scenes. This is a more inventive and creative approach to experimentation and human/animal relations. It does away with the scientific method and the social technologies that underpin how experimental arrangements are conducted within positivist science (also see Chapter 4). Psychological processes, such as will, intentionality and anticipation are approached as indeterminate, relational processes that are contiguous with the technical, symbolic, material, immaterial, historical and political forces that shape the milieu and setting.11 This is what Hans the Horse, Lady Wonder the Typing Horse, and all the strange oddities and puzzles encountered in this chapter re-move in the present. This is the hauntological force of the Hans the Horse charge, which was disclosed in the affective intensities surrounding the comparison between John Bargh and Mr von Osten – or what Bargh called the ‘Hans the Horse slur’. This perceived slur led to various attempts by Bargh to close down on forces that are part of the historiality of priming and reveal the more-than-one nature of the controversy. The slur opens to a future psychology that approaches the affective as relational, indeterminate, contingent and contiguous with the setting and milieu. This future psychology is one that might open to more speculative modes of experimentation. This represents what I am calling an ecological approach to affect that approaches affect as historical, all the way down, right to the bottom. We can see how this alternative genealogy of affect is disclosed by all those marginal and displaced agencies, actors and entities re-moved and put back into circulation by this controversy. I am suggesting that this way of approaching affect and the biohuman has its precursors in ecological approaches that were mobilized to explore psychological phenomena, as can be found in synecology and the concept of biocenosis (Moebius12). These have a lineage to epigenetics and related fields in the present that similarly attempt to break down nature/culture separations and bifurcations (see Blackman,
76
Haunted Data
2016b). However, the newness of contemporary approaches such as these are questioned when placed within genealogical approaches to psychology informed by hauntology and historialities. This chapter reopens a case that, although consigned to history within psychology, lives on as a reminder of how little we understand the suggestive capacities of subjects, human and non-human. As Despret (2015) has argued ‘Clever Hans’, and the entry of Clever Hans into the field of psychology, has closed down on relevant, interesting and important propositions about how to understand and explore Clever Hans’s intelligences. In her reopening of the case of Clever Hans, she argues that Pfungst’s experiments and interpretations close down on the ‘undecidables’ – those anomalies, which put his explanations into doubt, revealing that the case is ‘far more complicated than Pfungst wants to think’ (p. 82). She also suggests that what Pfungst’s experiments reveal is the entry of Hans into psychology (rather than ethology), transforming Hans and his capacities to be affected and affect, within the first beginnings of a behaviourist technology.13 As she argues, the Hans before Pfungt’s experiments and the Hans afterwards was not the same horse, where he becomes a ‘well and truly mechanized horse’ (p. 80). This dispositive or experimental apparatus put in place particular conditions which transformed Hans into ‘a circus horse, a clown horse’ (2015: 80), and left behind a residue of possible explanations and propositions that have become disqualified, disallowed and left unnoticed. It is these which are re-moved by the John Bargh priming controversy. It also shows how the movement of these traces and their transformation across time continues, albeit in a controversy, which is again closed down and consigned to history. The haunted data in this controversy return, affectively and hauntologically, reminding us why Hans truly is a horse who refuses to go away.
Conclusion: Affect and future psychology Software media, specifically in the context of post-publication-peer-review, make visible the extent to which the micro-dynamics of individual user’s social media transactions are entangled with different temporalities, actors and agencies. These material and immaterial traces entangle the past, present and future in contrary ways. These antagonistic forces open up to archives of the
Data-Mediation and Hauntological Analysis: The ‘Clever Hans Charge’
77
future while often closing down such potentialities. The histories of citation, which are carried by the historical movement of a trace, or historiality, are revealed in those submerged narratives and displaced actors which haunt such a movement. I have called this transmedial time following the work of the critical race scholar Rey Chow (2012). This approach enriches studies of media-time beyond the perpetual instantaneous time of the present (marked by flow and duration for example), to traces of the power relations, which govern specific regimes of visibility and invisibility. This is often referred to as the back-end of social media but perhaps more aptly might be considered hauntologically as what haunts automation and cannot easily be converted into particular kinds of capital (financial, social, cultural, etc.). These processes are captured by the concept of haunted data. This book invites scholars to pay attention to what is rendered immaterial, invisible, to what becomes displaced or submerged. It is one attempt to continue long traditions of critical research across the humanities, science studies, feminist work and queer theory in the context of the ‘computational turn’. The conclusion to this chapter is that we can ‘read against the grain’ in digital environments, and that working with ‘small data’ can allow a purchase on what might be at stake in mapping the cultural and social life of data and the politics of automation. It can also open up to and mine the gaps, contradictions, silences and anomalies within science and science controversies as inventive ways of working across and between the sciences and humanities. This reveals the traces of a future psychology, which is re-moved and haunts the present. It speaks through the affective intensities of individual user’s software transactions, while at the same time exceeding attempts to set things straight. This chapter is perhaps one example of how digital communication and data analytics have the potential to make a difference and anticipate futures yet-to-be-realized. However, what has settled following this controversy is ‘business as usual’, a process of setting the record straight, which endorses some of the most conservative understandings of what it means to have and be a body. Despite radical indeterminacy and the potentiality of those historialities removed by this controversy, the excess of stories are closed down, repealed, displaced and erased from what has taken form. A moment of potential is lost leaving traces to what might have been and could be if psychology were able and willing to listen to the traces of its own radical pasts.
78
Part Two
Feeling Futures: Mediating Futures Introduction: Mediating futures The introduction to Part 2 of the book will situate the second controversy, Feeling the Future, within the context of the current political conjuncture. This includes the rise of futurology and futurism, alongside for some, the inability to imagine alternative futures within the context of the anthropocene, global finance capitalism, the rise of nationalisms and reactionary populisms, and a concomitant loss of hope. The introduction will contextualize the Feeling the Future controversy for the reader within the context of broader cultural imaginaries and practices. As we will see, the capacity to feel multiple futures in the present and even futures, which can rewrite the past, and alter the present is a potent cultural imaginary. It connects regimes of anticipation that can be found within and across software and computational culture with strange psychic and extra-sensory capacities, such as precognition. The Feeling the Future controversy invites a consideration of what these displaced and submerged relationships might bring to our understandings of the present; to the relationships between affect and governance, science and the popular; to post-human understandings of the subject; and to the need for speculative experimental philosophies within science that can return the wonder to the world. I am writing the introduction to Part 2 of the book at a historical conjuncture where for many imagining alternative futures is politically imperative, but seems at the same time replete with failure and a (lost) longing for the hope that things could be otherwise.1 At the time of writing, the United Kingdom is moving towards a post-Brexit landscape and President Trump is establishing himself as the ‘people’s president’ within the United States. People are being
80
Haunted Data
further displaced due to wars, terrorism, persecution and poverty, while at the same time borders and boundaries are being drawn and redrawn amplifying racisms, xenophobia and a fear of the ‘other’. This book was begun at the first signs of economic recovery across the globe and following the financial crash in 2008. However, what we have witnessed since 2012 are worsening inequities between rich and poor, the entrenchment of nationalisms and fundamentalisms across the globe, the rise of populist nationalist politics and the emergence of ‘post-truth’, alongside discussions of the anthropocene and the end of the human and even the planet. These latter narratives abound in literature, film, philosophy, science and the humanities. The future is a hot topic, which continually encounters its own uneven pasts and potential catastrophic futures. Futurism abounds in the United States, fuelled perhaps by a therapy culture where self-proclaimed futurists not only advise on what individuals can do to maximize their own potentials, recovery, successes and health, but also turn their gaze to what consumption will become, what shopping will look and feel like in the future. As David Houle, futurist to Oprah Winfrey and the communities she enacts, asks, People like to shop. Americans like to shop. American women in particular like to shop. According to futurist David Houle, in the past 40 years, shopping went from something one did when something was needed to a leisure activity in and of itself. With changes in economics and technology, what will shopping look like in the next 10 years?2
Of course the future of shopping is not a banal concern. Consumption has been placed as a central driver of economic recovery across many neoliberal countries, where we are urged, encouraged or incited to buy property, goods, services or lifestyles to help develop and strengthen failing systems of governance. The prediction of how and what we will shop for has therefore been a concern for politicians, governments, economists and even our own perceived well-being. Trendspotting – predicting the future – is big business. But of course the trendspotting that is taking form is no crystal ball gazing and neither is it a solely human activity. Non-human agencies are at the forefront of not simply predicting, but in a recursive relationship also shaping what we will come to want and, importantly, buy. One of David Houle’s futurist predictions,
Feeling Futures: Mediating Futures
81
for example, is that the ubiquity of online shopping in our lives will not destroy one of the main non-places, to use Mark Auge’s (2009) term – shopping malls. Shopping malls are generic places of transit where the experience of shopping, he argues, as an embodied, sensory and sensual practice is crucial. The irony of giving a talk to business executives in a carefully and hermetically sealed air-conditioned shopping mall in Dubai is not lost on Houle. He reflects on how one of his predictions was made in a mall in the desert, which had a ski slope for skiing and snowboarding (despite the 40+ degree desert temperatures outside)! However, experiential shopping omits or overlooks the role nonhuman agencies play in shaping our desires. As Luciana Parisi (2013) among many others have argued, algorithms, that is practices of machine-learning, which seek to pre-empt and shape what we desire, are already at work in shaping possible futures out of the patterns, anomalies, accidents and multiple media transactions that we make throughout our lives. As she argues, algorithms are no longer to be thought of as instructions or rules to perform tasks at the level of computation, but are ‘performing entities: actualities that select, evaluate, transform and produce data’ (p. ix). She argues that increasingly computation and computational entities have pervaded culture and to that extent are our co-creating partners in what might come to be. She argues that we live increasingly in computational cultures and, as Adrian Mackenzie (2013) has argued, what defines computation and the practices of software programmers, for example, are attempts to anticipate and shape the future rather than predict timeless truths, regularities and laws. Drawing on a special issue of the journal Subjectivity, devoted to Technoscience, he cites the feminist science studies scholars Adams et al. (2009). They argue that governance has increasingly moved from regimes of truth to regimes of anticipation. As they suggest, Anticipation … emerges at a moment of actuarial saturation, when one realises that the sciences of the actual can be abandoned or ignored to be replaced by a knowledge that the truth about the future can be known by way of speculative forecast, itself relying on proliferating modes of prediction. (p. 247)
Rather than sciences of the actual and knowledges that seek to confirm truth, we have moved into what many people now describe as a post-truth landscape.
82
Haunted Data
The knowledges that condition these post-truths are those which are able to mobilize longings, desires, habits, beliefs, fears, anxieties, defences, prejudices and processes and practices more associated with psychological registers of experience – in other words, with how people feel about the conditions of their lives. Placing feeling within a broader political context therefore speaks directly to the role the psychological knowledges play in strategies of governance and regulation. Out of this conjuncture practices, such as speculative forecast, anticipation, post-truth, pre-emption and human and non-human agencies arise, which all seek to shape the future at a time where the future of the planet, the human, and for some ‘life itself ’ is under threat. Millennial fantasies abound in film, literature and TV – they contemplate the end of the planet and of a possible time of past, present and future. These scenarios stage possible futures that defy forecast or even comprehension. Future studies is a recognizable discipline with its own journals, monographs, magazines, think tanks, methods, strategies, indexes, organizations and forms of intervention. Rather than see the future as an inevitable unfolding of the present, framed within a linear narrative of progress, it is rather recognized that there are possible futures that could be engineered, planned and orchestrated as a way of solving conflicts in the present. One focus of future studies is trend analysis and forecasting, a particular form of human and non-human pattern recognition that attempts to anticipate trends in order to shape them in the future. These strategies and regimes are taking form in the context of increasing anxiety about the future. As we have seen, this includes talk of the anthropocene and the end of the human and even the extinction of the planet3 – and in the context of the impotence of human problem solving and capacity to understand the financial entities, objects and practices enacted by global finance capitalism (see Seigworth and Tiessen, 2012). These objects and entities appear to have their own unanticipated and autonomous agencies and effects. Thus futurology, with its hope for engineering possible futures different to now, is situated within melancholic and sometimes bleak predictions for the legacies (environmental, economic and otherwise) that current generations will bequeath to others. Futurology assumes therefore that the future is not fixed, and therefore the inevitable outcome of the present. Neither does it have to be an extended present, which repeats the problems of the past. Speculative
Feeling Futures: Mediating Futures
83
forecast and regimes of anticipation have therefore become the new forms of quantification and thought-style marking out present concerns.4 I argue that post-truth politics should be situated within these broader shifts and landscapes of production and consumption. The trend analysis and forecasting which are part of futurology combine machinic forms of pattern recognition with human consensual vision – what kind of future do we want; and on the basis of this vision, how can we get there? The invitation to ‘plan backwards’ enacts a form of ‘global foresight’, which remediates the capacity to anticipate the future once associated with psychic phenomena, such as clairvoyance and precognition, within a technical apparatus. This includes a range of actors and agencies, which include software and data analytics, business consultants, economists, policy makers, NGOs, speculative narratives and different modalities of visioning (e.g. blurring the distinction between science and science fiction). These help to create a distributed and mediated form of networked collective intelligence5 – or at least the hope is that ‘Tomorrow Can Be Built Today’.
Feeling futures It is in the context of futurology and futurisms, media and otherwise, that the second controversy analysed in this book is situated. Chapters 4, 5 and 6 will focus on the Feeling the Future controversy, which is associated with a beguiling series of experiments carried out by an eminent Cornell American scientist, Daryl Bem (2011).6 Bem attempted to prove or disprove precognition – that is the proposition that it is possible to forecast the future. Within the realms of statistical probability Bem argued that he had demonstrated the existence of premediation – the capacity to anticipate and shape the future, or even for the future to retroactively reshape the past. This controversy, as with the John Bargh Priming Controversy, gained traction across social media, and was also brought to the attention of the broadcast media and even an American comedy show. In the context of futurology it is an interesting controversy to follow, and one that re-situates attempts to anticipate and shape the future, within a much longer history of concern with registers of attention, remembering, sensation and thought, which exceed current conceptions of cognitive processes. The question of time and of foresensing the future in the present, or of
84
Haunted Data
time-travelling to the future and the future retroactively influencing the past, is an evocative cultural fantasy. It is perhaps not surprising that this experiment became part of a much bigger controversy, and one which as we will see, raises interesting questions about the re-imagination of concepts of time, duration and temporality that govern digital archives, with their capacity for remixing and remediation. Feeling the Future is linked to broader cultural imaginaries, which enact foresight, anticipation, pre-emption and premediation such that temporalities are queered (see Freeman, 2010; Halberstam, 2005; Love, 2007). Queer temporalities question what it means to be human, turning this upside down, inside out, back to front and sideways, often at the same time. This controversy also connects to a dispersed set of surfaces of emergence, where the quantum scale of matter is enacted, imagined, performed, materialized and dematerialized. This includes discussions which range from the quantum brain and biology and quantum entanglement in physics, through to controversies regarding queer sexualities and pre-trans-feminisms (see Bem and Lipsitz, 1981). Queer sexualities and queer times intrude within this particular scene of entanglement, and prefigure in important ways current trans-feminisms with their focus on molecular becomings (see Preciado, 2013). The ‘scenes of entanglement’ that I enact in the next three chapters will stage a queer montage of relations based on the experiments and their afterand even future-lives. The experiments became something of a media event, creating a media flurry of attention and becoming the butt of many jokes, academic and non-academic. As well as remediating this event in the context of the submerged and displaced narratives the controversy revitalizes, I will also turn my attention to the politics of data that was disclosed and brought to the surface by this controversy. This will allow me to advance an argument that considers the performativity of the different software analytics that allow particular claims to be made within science. I will consider what these debates and controversies reveal about the social and cultural life of data that are often covered over when data is reduced to metrics. This will also enable a discussion of post-truth politics, and how we might understand the parameters and conditions of what might be believed or believable, and what is excluded as fake, hoax and as an object of ridicule. As we will see these differentiations have little to do with truth per se and more to do with historical narratives,
Feeling Futures: Mediating Futures
85
practices, modes of remembering and forgetting, and the specific conditions under which truth-claims can be made and advanced. It is perhaps no surprise that the Feeling the Future experiments have taken on the status of weird science, linked to so-called mad scientists conjuring up statistical probabilities that defy belief. Many commentators believe that the experiments should be consigned to the fraudulent, and aligned to experiments, which with hindsight have been exposed as hoaxes and scientific jokes. They are a good example of what Judith Butler (2009) and Sara Ahmed (2010) have termed non-performative(s) – that is they do not bring about the objects and entities of which they speak. In other words, despite their particular strategy of speculative forecast (or even post-truth-claims) it is not possible for the future to change the past, or at least this question remains unsettled. I will use the concept of the non-performative to explore the limits of techniques and technologies of futurity, which are based on particular software analytics and historical truth-claims. These can create statistically significant effects (within the parameters of the scientific method and positivist science), while failing to bring about the objects, entities and effects they perform. The discussion will show how mathematical probabilities and the cultural and social imaginaries they perform (particularly at the level of algorithms) are actualities, which often enact impoverished versions of past, present and possible futures. On this basis I will argue our concerns about data and the potential of data to shape worlds is perhaps overstated. Rather what the controversy discloses is questions about what we are already primed to believe (technically, culturally, historically, affectively) and what therefore the limits and reach of post-truth politics are. I will argue that the future-sight that this enables shows how limited mathematical probabilities (and particularly Bayesian probabilistic statistics) and the techniques of anticipation they might enact are. I call this paradox the paradox of the mathematical non-future(s). I will argue that imagining and realizing possible future(s) is still within the grasp of arts, humanities and science scholars, who can work in the gaps, silences, absent-presences, hesitations, submerged narratives and displaced actors surrounding scientific controversies. I will argue that this playful way of contaminating legitimate science represents the basis of an important rapprochement between the humanities and sciences, which does not simply confirm positivist science
86
Haunted Data
by way of endorsement, speculation or confirmation. It represents a form of undisciplinarity that is compatible with queer theories and with critical approaches to science that remain committed to the possibility of more inventive, creative and open science(s). In this era of post-truth finding new ways of judging and authenticating truth-claims is ever more urgent. The analysis of the Feeling the Future controversy in the second half of the book will go some way to identify what might be at stake.
4
Feeling the Future
‘Daryl Bem Proved ESP Is Real: Which Means Science Is Broken’1 To have an idea of the impact of this paper, it is instructive to take note of the number of times this paper was cited in other scientific journals: 342 times as of 3 July 2015. Furthermore if you search ‘Feeling the future’ with Google you can find approximately two million pages that includes a dedicated page on Wikipedia. Additionally Bem was interviewed by many radio and TV stations, from the Colbert Report to the David Letterman show and Through The Wormhole with Morgan Freeman. Such widespread and generally positive media coverage is unprecedented for any kind of parapsychological type finding. Bem deserves plaudits for breaking new ground in this way.2
Introduction This chapter sets the scene for the unfolding controversy explored in the next two chapters. It will provide the reader with the means to ‘see’ what might usually remain occluded by forms of representationalism, or what has come to stand in for the controversy. It shapes a form of mediated perception that opens to epistemological uncertainties and foreclosures. This allows the haunted data within the controversy to speak back to some of the usual commentary that can be found about it on the internet (particularly commentary which engages either in endorsement or scepticism). The commentary is a good example of the polarizing logic that Vinciane Despret (2015) suggests characterizes science within this area and that needs unsettling. The scenes that shape the chapters all commune with ghost-data and provide a home and hospitable
88
Haunted Data
atmosphere or ethos for ‘queer aggregations’ that usually remain submerged, disguised or displaced. These ‘queer aggregations’ are put back into circulation. The implications of this for the shaping of biological and psychological data will be discussed in Chapter 6 and in the conclusion of the book. In this and the next two chapters the reader will encounter very complex arguments that bring back or re-move unresolved debates and issues within statistics, truth and post-truth-claims, scientific experimentation, studies of consciousness and the status of the human within the context of the molecular and more-than-human. The storytelling discloses how complex this more-thanone event is. It raises questions about media contagions and what we might learn about the contagious potential of (science) controversies as they spread across digital and social media. As the reader will see, the contagious potential of the controversy is due to the layered and yet displaced actors and agents who intrude in the present. In different ways they all invite consideration of what it might mean to learn to listen, commune and speak with ghosts or haunted data.
Scene 1: The “Buzz on Bem” In 2010 the Cornell scientist Daryl Bem conducted a series of nine experiments, which demonstrated, within the realms of statistical probability that the future could retroactively reshape the past. The experiments were framed as an exploration, demonstration and possible verification of the existence of precognition defined as the ability to anticipate the future. This ability could extend not just from the past to the future but also from the future to the past. Controversially Bem argued that the future could retroactively anticipate and shape events that had already happened. He has also suggested that whether one believes in precognition or not, it is important to engage in what we might call more speculative modes of experimentation, that is experimenting with the impossible or the improbable. Bem (2011) recounts that the inspiration for one of the dramatic hooks of the experiments was based on the beguiling proposition that ‘time-reversal’ is possible. This he says was influenced by the character of the White Queen in Lewis Carroll’s Through the Looking Glass. The concept of ‘retroactive facilitation of recall’ is aligned to the White Queen’s view that memory that
Feeling the Future
89
only works backwards (from present to past) is a poor memory. As he suggests, ‘In Lewis Carroll’s Through the Looking Glass, the White Queen explains to Alice that the citizens of her country have precognitive ability, or, as she puts it, “memory works both ways” in her land and she herself remembers best “things that happened the week after next”. When Alice says, “I’m sure mine only works one way. … I can’t remember things before they happen.” the Queen disparagingly remarks, “It’s a poor sort of memory that only works backwards’” (Carroll, 2006, p. 164). Although many scientists have responded to Bem’s invitation to experiment with the impossible or extraordinary as ridiculous, absurd and not to be taken seriously, I will argue throughout this and the next two chapters that this invitation is one that should be taken seriously. As I will argue this invitation connects to broader issues in the humanities and the field of affect studies. This includes how to analyse the relationships between anticipation, pre-emption, subliminal and non-conscious registers of experience, and their modulation and orchestration within systems of power and regulation. So for the purposes
Figure 4 Alice with the White Queen from Lewis Carroll’s Through the Looking Glass. Copyright Wikimedia commons.
90
Haunted Data
of this chapter I invite the reader to suspend their own disbelief and to entertain the White Queen’s retort that memory that only works backwards is a poor sort of memory. On this basis I will approach the experiments as engaging in rather queer forms of time-travelling. They are certainly analogous to the way Karen Barad (2010) uses the queerness of quantum ontologies to disrupt any notion of fixed time in nature. As she says, ‘quantum weirdness’ is actually ‘quantum queerness’ (Barad 2010: 246–7) – not queer as in strange, but queer as undoing, queer as ‘trans/formation’ (p. 247). She also uses the figuration of quantum queerness to refer to asynchronous time, where time is out of joint and where stable identities are undone. The experiments also show the entanglement of literature and science in the imaginaries that are enacted and come to matter within scientific forms of experimentation (see Haraway, 1997). Fantasies of time-travelling are of course the stuff and staple of science fiction and popular culture. We can think of the Hollywood blockbuster Back to the Future or the film adaptation of the Philip K Dick Sci-Fi novel Minority Report, for example. The concept of the seer, the mystic who has second-sight enabling visionary experience, also appears as a figure within many ecstatic religions and ancient Middle Eastern traditions. The concept of foresight and the capacity to anticipate the future also underpins new strategies of governance, such as premediation and pre-emption, which increasingly shape automated media worlds. Not just the stuff of fantasy or science fiction, the concept of artificial precognition is a new form of data analytics developed by IBM, which provides a data service that claims to ‘provide you leads when you don’t know the question to ask, and for when you want to uncover and discover in the data new insights and patterns’.3 This service, known as the Watson Discovery Advisor, identifies trends, patterns and correlations in large amounts of data in order to generate new insights. Alongside this, there is a rush to design, implement and patent artificial precognition technology, which claims to provide ‘knowledge of possible events’4 before they happen. This is linked to desires to intervene and shape possible futures. These data-driven dreams and fantasies bring together big data analytics with forms of decisionmaking based on probabilistic statistics, often to extract value and revenue from data. The hope is that operating costs can be reduced and efficiencies for business increased.
Feeling the Future
91
In some senses these technologies extend cybernetics and communication science into a psychology of time that has always haunted cognitive science, neuroscience and social psychology. This includes studies of the paranormal and those psychic entities, concepts, practices and phenomena that remain as a ‘nagging residue’ to psychophysical understandings and measurements of sense perception and attention (Halpern, 2015: 66). Parapsychology has always represented a form of inquiry ‘on the margins’ of experimental psychology. Although often dismissed by contemporary scientists, the historian Ian Hacking has argued that early psychic research shaped the parameters of contemporary positivist scientific practices as they operated in a more speculative mode. This speculative mode and the more radical implications of understanding what it might mean to experiment with the impossible or extraordinary have been displaced, occluded and under-explored (Hacking, 1988: 434). This chapter will begin to explore the implications of this for the field of affect studies and those interested in the biohumanities, that is in developing new theories of the human that draw from the sciences and humanities. The analysis as it extends across this and the next two chapters will show how we need more creative and inventive approaches to experimentation and the shaping of more speculative sciences. I will specifically outline how the experiments were preempted within the scientific community showing how the criteria for arbitrating truth are never simply statistical or mathematical. What one is willing or able to believe directly shapes the post-commentary and post-publication-peer-review associated with this ongoing controversy, including a gamut of non-replication and replication studies.5 The chapter opens to some new ways of assessing and analysing the scientific method and scientific experimentation that draw from literature, art and theatre. This analysis will be extended in the subsequent two chapters allowing for a novel analysis of the significance of these experiments for the field of affect studies, speculative philosophies and the biohumanities.
Pre-empting the experiments Before the experiments were written up and published in the peer-reviewed journal International Journal of Social Psychology in 2011 (cited 670 times to date), the scientific community and science bloggers had picked up on a prepublication version that appeared on Daryl Bem’s website.6 This was circulated
92
Haunted Data
in blogs and across Twitter pre-empting the outrage that many in the scientific community felt. The article quickly started to create a stir and subsequent media flurry of attention. In a blog written by two psychologists, Andrew D Wilson and Sabrina Golonka, called Notes from Two Scientific Psychologists, they summarize their basic take on events as follows: I don’t believe a word of it because a) let’s face it, it’s about precognition and b) there’s simply no effort to propose a mechanism that might support such an outrageous claim (and no, ‘quantum mechanics’ is not a mechanism). Bem explicitly states that coming up with a mechanism isn’t his job and he’s just ‘reporting the data’. But this is precisely the problem with psychology right now – not enough theory – and the links below that talk about the analysis problems with this paper (and all statistical testing in the social sciences) make good points about the fact that statistical testing in the absence of a clear theory which includes mechanisms is effectively a fishing trip. There’s also a nice discussion here about why it’s healthy to be immediately sceptical of a study that claims to have found something inconsistent with the rest of science.7
In another blog, Practical Ethics, published by Oxford University some two days prior to this (and hyperlinked to the blog above), it is stated that ‘the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, the most influential journal in social psychology, is about to publish a study that presents evidence for the existence of psychic phenomena’.8 Over the next twenty-four months and beyond, commentators from science, media and even the American comedian Steven Colbert weighed in to ridicule and lambaste the experiments. The commentary included a revitalization of Bem’s already existing status as a controversial figure both within and outside psychology. Bem was invited on to The Colbert Report, a late-night American political comedy show on 26 January 2011, with the headline, ‘Time-Traveling Porn’.9 As we will see in Chapter 5, the experiments provided ample material for such an attentiongrabbing headline. He appeared in Season 2, Episode 5 of the American documentary series Through the Wormhole presented by Morgan Freeman, in an episode asking ‘Do We Have a Sixth Sense?’10 He appeared on the CNN and the MS NBC news, and various newspapers and bloggers picked up on the experiments and amplified their apparent comedic and entertainment value. On 7 January 2011, Daryl Bem appeared on the NBC news, with the headline, ‘Psychic Ability Doesn’t Fly in New Experiments’.
Feeling the Future
93
The experiments were reported on in the New York Times three times, on the 5th, 7th and 11th of January, 2011. They were reported on in the Spanish TVC Channel News on 25 April 2013, and in the United Kingdom in the broadsheet The Telegraph (three times) and particularly on 19 November 2010, with the headline, ‘Frankly the Future Is All Too Predictable’.11 Bem received a sympathetic response, which was accompanied by a lively discussion linked to 352 posted comments in the British Daily Mail on-line (18 November 2010), with the headline, ‘Are Humans Psychic? Startling New Study “Proves” That We Can See into the Future’.12 His experiments were also the subject of an article published on 17 December 2010, in the blog for the Healthy Living Section of the HuffPost. This was written by the CEO and president of the Institute for Noetic Studies (and author of Mindful Motherhood), Cassandra Hieton. It was published with the title ‘It’s About Time: The Scientific Evidence for Psi’.13 Bem became the subject of more measured scrutiny in the British broadsheet The Guardian, as well as in the arts and culture magazine Dazed, with the headline, ‘Can We See into the Future?’.14 Bem was also interviewed by Al-Jeezera, ABC and Fox News. As The Institute of Noetic Sciences has argued, there was a ‘Buzz on Bem’, with numerous blogs reporting on the experiments, enough that the traction registered on the Science Commons.15 This is a web application that organizes and curates information about the impact and reach of scientific articles on social media.16 The ‘Buzz on Bem’ brought the realm of anomalistic psychology and weird science into the mainstream press and extended post-publication-peer-review across a number of actors and agencies. The experiments became ‘the thing’ (Latour, 2005) that brought together and re-assembled a number of longstanding and unsettled issues that connect psychology to its submerged and displaced pasts. As with the last chapter, this chapter will focus on how the experiments travelled, and in their travels and curious forms of time reversal have accrued their own agencies. They have moved from the original scene of experimentation and become an actor within a mutable scene of entanglement. The experiments open to multiple leads, criss-crossings, loopings, back tracks, movings and re-movings. As I tease apart the entangled relations set in motion by this controversy, we will see how different temporalities and media times are knotted, spliced and enacted. In this respect and as with the last two chapters, I will be following those traces, deferrals, absences, gaps and their movements
94
Haunted Data
within a particular corpus of data. I will attempt to re-move and keep alive what becomes submerged or hidden by particular regimes of visibility and remembering. These movements are simultaneously technical, affective, historical, social, political and ethical and are distributed across a variety of social media platforms, actors, publics, agencies, bodies and practices. The approach I take exceeds the usual critical commentaries that surround anomalistic psychology and weird science, which include the role of professionalized sceptics, who police the boundaries between the psychological and the paranormal. This is an established tradition of critique within anomalistic psychology, and is often where critical commentaries are located. This usually includes an assessment of the experimental claims and the status, reputation and worth of the person making them. It is perhaps not a surprise therefore that Bem’s personal life also became the subject of scrutiny. We are told he is a gay Cornell professor with an unusual gender non-conforming past. He had married and had children with Sandra Bem, the late women’s studies professor.17 He was a former stage magician, had a history of civil rights activism and perhaps had a wry sense of humour. Was he being taken too seriously and what was his intent? Does Bem really believe in extra-sensory perception (ESP) or is he a Mad Scientist?18 Is Bem simply reproducing the traditions of dramaturgy and stagecraft, which have been recognized by some as integral to some traditions of social psychological experimentation? (see Millard, 2014). Is he extending the deception of psychological experiments into a mediatized realm, such that the joke is distributed across different publics? As Ray Hyman, an emeritus professor of psychology at the University of Oregon argued in The New York Times on 7 January 2011, ‘He’s got a great sense of humor.’ ‘I wouldn’t rule out that this is an elaborate joke.’ What can we read into Bem’s apparent poker face and what exactly is the nature of the deception? Or rather what exactly is motivating the ‘blather on Bem’ or ‘hissy fit’ as Larry Dossey, the executive editor of Explore asks?19 Is this an assault on rationality? This was a quote from the New York Times article ‘Journal’s Paper on ESP Expected to Prompt Outrage’ (5 January 2011). Do the experiments reveal something more endemic about the problem with statistical probability and the mathematization of chance that underpins the scientific method and positivist science? Are these experiments an enactment of quantum entanglement (see Hameroff, 2012)? Do these experiments enact
Feeling the Future
95
quantum scales of matter at molecular levels that might turn on our heads the already vexed and complex questions concerning the nature of consciousness, matter, free will and intentionality? What might these experiments and the statistical software analytics they use reveal about the problems and possibilities of data analytics and the politics of data? Do they have anything to say to media and cultural theorists who might be attempting to wrestle with the challenges of computational cultures to analyses of mediation? All of these questions are carried by the controversy and extend the controversy into areas of debate, research, dialogue, curiosity and reflection, which disturb boundaries between fact and fiction, past and present, science and humanities, private and public, the material and immaterial and the theoretical and the technical. The denouement that Bem leaves puzzled or sceptical readers with in the article relates to the question, ‘But how can it be like that?’ (p. 17). How is it possible to believe impossible things? Pre-empting the scepticism of many psychologists (and non-psychologists) he defers again to the White Queen who in response to Alice’s protests that ‘one can’t believe impossible things’ retorts, ‘I daresay you haven’t had much practice. When I was your age, I always did it for half-an-hour a day. Why, sometimes I’ve believed as many as six impossible things before breakfast’ (Carroll, 2006: p. 166; see Figure 4).
Future forecasting and post-truth politics As we explored in the introduction to Part 2 of the book, the capacity of the future to retroactively reshape the past is part of a potent cultural imaginary. Futurism(s) of all kinds are performed, imagined, practised and enacted across a variety of practices, locations, strategies and technologies, revealing some of the current ways in which conceptions of life and its parameters are being shaped. These ‘imagined worlds’ (Franklin, Lury and Stacey, 2000) are sometimes considered extraordinary or anomalous. They are sometimes projected as part of science(s) possible futures. They particularly appear in scientific and increasingly humanities traditions, which take the ‘quantum’ as the basis of imagination, intervention and experimentation. Foresight and the capacity of anticipated futures to shape the present (and sometimes the past) are also and often an ordinary and barely perceptible part of how business,
96
Haunted Data
commerce, governments, the military, politics and related actors and agencies attempt to intervene and shape what comes to matter. As the feminist science studies scholars, Adams, Murphy and Clarke (2009) have argued, one of the defining qualities of the current moment ‘is its characteristic state of anticipation, of thinking and living toward the future’ (p. 246). As the authors go on to argue, this state of anticipation is displacing science(s) based on actuality to science(s) based on speculative forecast. As they argue, these future-oriented logics enact a particular politics of temporality and affect that pervades how current problems might be thought, felt and addressed. Anticipation and pre-emption of possible future(s)-acting-in-thepresent displace static and linear conceptions of time. They argue that time is stretched, bent, mobilized, reversed, telescoped, inverted, turned upside down, inside out, entangled, disjointed, disoriented, bundled, expanded and multiplied. It is perhaps therefore not a surprise that a series of experiments claiming that precognition (the capacity to anticipate and feel the future) is possible should attract attention, both inside and outside science. The New York Times pre-empted the publication of the experiments on 5 January 2011, with the headline, ‘Journal’s Paper on ESP Expected to Prompt Outrage’. Part 2 of the book will consider why these experiments were met with scorn, amusement, embarrassment, anger, hostility and scepticism, particularly when they are read and made to collide with the myriad of sites and practices, which attempt to mobilize time, ‘turning the ever-moving horizon of the future into that which determines the present’ (Adams, Murphy and Clarke, 2009: 251). Why did these experiments become a media event that demanded attention and become the subject of debate, scandal, controversy, scrutiny, gossip and disbelief? What did this event become an attractor for, and how was it extended, intensified, multiplied and potentially made more indeterminate and messy, when we consider the distributed and pervasive effects of social and digital media on the enactment and shaping of such a controversy? What are some of the conditions of possibility for precognition, as an assumed psychic capacity or form of perception, to be modulated, simulated and materialized through a particular material-semiotic apparatus? This apparatus was composed of a computer, a screen, a software package, erotic images, a chair and an experimental subject. How is precognition reworked within these experiments and how does it differ from psychic experiments on
Feeling the Future
97
precognition in the nineteenth and early twentieth centuries? What do these experiments and their reception suggest about historical forms of attention and perception and their productive limits and constraints? What do they suggest about those points of ‘epistemic anxiety or instability’ (Daston and Galison cited in Halpern, 2015: 27), which characterize the ontologies underpinning future-oriented modes of ‘seeing’? Computers or software data analytics might be able to ‘see into the future’ but they of course do not employ clairvoyants, or at least not as far as I can tell! In order to start to explore some of these questions, scene 1 will consider philosophies of science, and the different histories and assumed a prioris of what counts as an experiment, evidence and experimentation that are enacted and incorporated. In the Preface I stated that the book specifically takes its cue from a number of feminist and radical philosophers of science and science studies scholars who have developed innovative approaches to science, which cross philosophy, science and culture. They have explored the histories and genealogies of science within specific historical, cultural, political, technical, psychological and symbolic conjunctures. This includes attention to the historical a prioris, which have shaped the philosophies and practice of science, as well as the close interdependence and interrelationship between cultural configurations of matter-meaning and the materialities of scientific cultures. The next section will specifically develop this through the work of Steve Brown (2012). Brown is an interesting ally as he was also trained as a psychologist and has come through the critical traditions and approaches to psychological matters that have characterized my own trajectory. He has also opened his thinking to sociological approaches to science studies (including science and technology studies), and more speculative philosophies, including the thinking of nineteenth and early-twentieth-century process philosophers such as William James, Albert North Whitehead, Henry Bergson, as well as contemporary philosophers of science who have been influenced by this thinking (see Brown and Stenner, 2009). This includes Isabelle Stengers, Vinciane Despret and Bruno Latour. This reconfiguration of psychological matters allows for a consideration of science experimentation as an entanglement of art, culture, media, philosophy and science. Psychological experimentation is always situated within specific milieu and sociopolitical
98
Haunted Data
understandings of what counts as an experimental subject and experimental rules and procedures. As we will see, once the historicity of psychological experimentation is opened to its own submerged and displaced narratives, actors and agents, more speculative modes and philosophies of experimentation can start to speak back to the scientific method. This is particularly as it has stabilized and operates in its more positivist mode in the present.
The experimental device Most psychology experiments are conducted within the auspices of the scientific method, which operate according to a particular ‘social technology’ (Brown, 2012). The scientific method relates to a set of procedures, rules and a prioris, which assume that scientific experiments are oriented to arbitrating truth and establishing the statistical probability of truth-claims. Within the context of psychology this would position psychology as a science of mind or science of the individual, rather than as a science of population management (see Chapter 1). Steve Brown uses the concept of social technology to rather explore how experiments are shaped according to a particular arrangement of forces, or what he terms the experimental device.20 The experimental device consolidates and enacts specific contingent histories of what counts as a proper psychological experiment. It sets up the rules governing the status of evidence and how this might be authenticated, legitimated, verified and evidenced. The scientific method is shorthand for such a contingent history of experimental practice within psychology. This method is meant to guard against spurious claims, unfounded evidence, statistical error and to contribute to the accumulation of scientific fact and knowledge. On this basis how might it be possible to enact a series of experiments shaped by the scientific method, which ‘prove’ within the realms of statistical probability that it is possible for the future to retroactively shape the past? Usually psychology experiments do not make such dramatic claims or contribute to the invention of the new or unexpected. As we explored in the previous chapter, the explanation of anomalous psychological phenomena (such as a horse telling the time and counting with his hooves) are usually framed through the most impoverished version of what might be possible and probable (see Despret, 2004a, 2015). Psychology is largely viewed as reductionist
Feeling the Future
99
and deterministic, based on establishing connections and relationships, which close down the invention and enactment of what the Belgian science studies philosopher terms ‘innovative propositions’ (Stengers, 2000). However, there is much more to say about the experimental method and its close links to more creative forms of experimentation associated with the arts, entertainment and theatre. For example social psychology experiments have a history of courting controversy and turning on a dramatic hook or proposition. Brown considers a number of social psychology experiments, including those undertaken by Stanley Milgram and Philip Zimbardo, which disclose psychology’s close relationship to media, artistic and theatrical practices. These experiments in different ways all attempt to simulate social phenomena. Most readers will have heard of Stanley Milgram and his studies of conformity and disobedience carried out in the wake of what Hannah Arendt during 1961 termed the ‘banality of evil’.21 These experiments are usually framed through a long tradition of research in social psychology, which aims to analyse ‘what happens to the individual in the presence of other people’? (Gough and McFadden, 2001). As Brown argues, this tradition of research has much in common with Brechtian theatre and art practices such as abstract expressionism, which draw attention to artifice and a moment of dramatic tension, or a dramatic hook (Millard, 2014). In the case of Milgrim’s obedience experiments, the dramatic hook revolved around a particular question: will the experimental subject deliver what they believe to be an electric shock, in order to help the ‘learner’ improve their success on recognizing word-pairs? This moment of dramatic tension is staged as a miniature drama, ‘which turns on a single moment’ (Brown, 2012). The drama is amplified and enacted by the unwitting insertion of the experimental subject into an arrangement of forces. These include deception, cover stories, props, pranks, knowing confederates and an experimenter who plays an executive role in directing and animating proceedings (Brown, 2012). To that extent the ‘social technology’, which shapes this tradition of psychological experimentation, has much in common with reality TV. Many commentators have drawn attention to the influence of early reality TV on the shaping of experimental apparatuses within psychology (Brown, 2012, McCarthy, 2008). This includes an awareness of how audiences might be enrolled into the drama when it might subsequently be watched on television and film.
100
Haunted Data
The dramaturgy and stagecraft of psychological experimentation has been extended by recent work, which has explored the role of media in the production of scientific fact, or at least those scientific theories, concepts and experiments, which take on the status of truth and veridicality (Foucault, 1980). The Australian film studies professor Kathryn Milliard (2014) has re-examined the role of photography and film in the shaping and production of Milgram’s famous obedience experiments, and what has become the accepted, although highly contestable, explanation of what took form. In her analysis of the discarded film footage of the experiments, which were edited into the educational documentary Obedience (first shown in educational settings from 1965), she has re-examined the archives from the Stanley Milgram papers held at Yale University. Her focus is on what did not make it into the final cut. As she argues, Milgram was very interested in the role of media in the production and establishment of scientific fact (or consensus and fact-building). He designed the experiments with a camera and audience in mind. The initial prototypes of the experiment were watched through a two-way mirror reproducing a ‘peep-show’ aesthetic. Filming the experiments with concealed cameras later extended the aesthetic. This created some of the footage which was used in the original cut of the documentary, and also incorporated ‘cut away’ shots of the main prop involved (the electric shock machine) filmed at a later date in what was conceived as a ‘film-noir’ style. This was thought to further heighten the potential for dramatic tension. Milliard also illustrates how the documentary and the design of the experiments remediates the Eichmann trial; this was the court case in Israel which put one of the Nazi high ranking officers on trial for crimes committed during the Holocaust. This led Hannah Arendt to describe him as ‘frighteningly normal’. The case was filmed and shown on television and took the form of a ‘season-length morality play’ (Milliard, 2014: 441). It was a ‘television event’, which interrupted normal schedules, was shown every night on television in many countries (including the United States and Europe), and used close-ups and concealed cameras to capture the supposed ordinariness of Eichmann. Milliard shows how Milgram remediates this event in the design of his experimental apparatus (specifically the peep-show aesthetic and construction of the experimental subjects as ordinary, as ‘every man’); how audiences brought this event to their reading and experience of the Obedience
Feeling the Future
101
documentary; and how at a later point, when Obedience had been released for general distribution, it was then assumed to have been filmed before the trial. This pre-empted perhaps its growing status as a cautionary tale of what ordinary people might do when they cede responsibility to others.
Mediatization and science The complex processes of remediation involved in these experiments illustrate the entanglement of art, media and science in the construction of experimental apparatuses. As well as the dramatic hook and the stagecraft involved in creating the tension, the experiments are perhaps a good example of how some psychologists have engaged with mediatization or ‘media life’ – what Mark Deuze defines as a critical awareness of ‘the scripted and broadcasted nature of everything one does’ (2012: 255). Although one could argue that Milgram simply saw the footage as a scientific record, the editing of the footage clearly shows how attuned he was to the structuring role of media in our everyday lives and how the editing process was used to construct a version of events that was compatible with an already accepted consensus – that ordinary people are capable of doing extraordinary things (see Milliard, 2014). This belief was particularly apparent in the public appetite for screenings of the Obedience documentary in 1970 in the wake of the Vietnam War and the My Lai massacre. The tension between seeing the film footage as scientific record and approaching it as part of the performativity of the experimental apparatus comes through in the misgivings of the Nobel laureate psychologist Daniel Kahneman in relation to the John Bargh priming controversy (see Chapter 3). In an open email sent to students of priming studies,22 Kahneman positions himself as a general believer in priming, while at the same time acknowledging the stagecraft involved in psychological experimentation. As he documents, ‘I am not a member of your community, and all I have personally at stake is that I recently wrote a book that emphasizes priming research as a new approach to the study of associative memory – the core of what dual-system theorists call System 1. Count me as a general believer. I also believe in a point that John Bargh made in his response to Cleeremans, that priming effects are subtle and that their design requires high-level skills. I am sceptical about replications by investigators new to priming research, who may not be attuned to the subtlety
102
Haunted Data
of the conditions under which priming effects are observed, or to the ease with which these effects can be undermined’. Kahneman argues that the conduct of psychological experiments is akin to the direction of a theatre performance, where all the subtleties and contingencies, which modulate experimental processes (or what we might call the performativity of the material-semiotic apparatus) are obscured, suppressed and written out of the published study. This important insight is partially acknowledged but reduced to ‘confirmation bias’, which is considered a tendency for psychologists to confirm their preconceptions. It is assumed that preconceptions should be removed by the development of tighter protocols governing replication, and Kahneman suggests that this strategy is important to rescue, what he considers to be the tarnished nature of the field. As we will see the Feeling the Future controversy has also been settled in a similar way, coming to stand in for the problems with replication in the field and with replication as the cornerstone of scientific experimentation.23 We saw at the end of the previous chapter how this closes down on more inventive propositions and approaching scientific experimentation in a more open, creative and speculative way. As Milliard has argued, the rushes that are left on the cutting room floor, the editing notes written on scraps of paper, the writes and rewrites of the scripts and scenes, the editions and subtractions and the nuances and influences on the filming methodology all demonstrate the time, effort, labour and direction that was integral to the success and efficacy of the experimental apparatus and its media after-lives. The discarded potential lives of the documentary might also be related to Hans-Jörg Rheinberger’s (2010) concept of the ‘economy of the scribble’, those traces of practices which exceed the parameters of recognized scientific practice. Although Rheinberger in this instance focuses on what gets left out of studies once they are written up for publication – scribbles on pieces of paper, workings out, ponderings, and so on – I argue that digital archives also represent opportunities to explore such economies within distributed, extended networks of actors, agencies and practices. These exist as traces, which can be followed, mapped, listened to and re-animated. However, as we saw in Chapters 2 and 3, there is an ‘economy of the trace’, where certain links and associations are subject to practices of redaction, disavowal, disqualification, and exist in a submerged form. The links to these traces are often broken or fail, one comes to dead ends but often also to uncivil discourse
Feeling the Future
103
or to an intensification of dialogue, a ‘blather’, which discloses through the affective dynamics on display that there is something more than meets the eye.
Queering Weird Science There is a buzz, an outrage, incredulity, concern, a sensationalist gaze cast across a person and their reputation. They might become the subject of interrogation, the butt of a joke, of journalistic scrutiny and media broadcast attention. The person might come to stand in for a complex set of entangled processes, practices, human and non-human entities and devices. This creates the possibility for the experimental apparatus to attune and shape what takes form and travels beyond the laboratory. What kind of networked virality or media contagion is therefore on show when a series of experiments can channel public(s) hopes, fears, anxieties, fantasies and curiosities in this way? What queer forms of time-travelling can a series of experiments in the present enact such that multiple leads are laid through dead ends, failures, subterfuge, detours, turns, twists, temporary denouements and through what might appear irrelevant to the plot that takes form? If, as Halberstam (2010) has argued, failure is a queer art that can be mined for critical potential, what insights can be brought from queer studies to weird science with its tendency to operate according to a polarizing logic? This includes explanations, which focus either on proving the existence of phenomena, or undermining them as evidence of so-called false belief or even the musings of a ‘mad scientist’. I will argue that we need more inventive propositions that do not operate according to such logic; is it real or unreal, true or false? In the area of anomalous psychology the scientist is often cast in the role of judge and juror attempting to close down on the ambiguity, hesitations, puzzling curiosities and what continually resists current scientific explanations. Rather than taking the position of judge and juror, what does approaching science experimentation as a particular genre of production and consumption add to our understandings of the production of truth, and even post-truth within the context of scientific knowledges? What does queer studies bring to weird science and those experiments which achieve statistical probability but which are considered aberrant, freakish, odd, bizarre, peculiar and unusual?
104
Haunted Data
Drawing on Foucault’s (2003) concept of ‘subjugated knowledges’ Halberstam argues that we might turn to what gets rendered nonsensical, irrelevant, insufficient, inferior and buried below to refuse and resist normalization. As Halberstam cogently shows, Foucault’s tactic of ‘antidisciplinarity’ was directed to those knowledges ‘that are below the required level of erudition or scientificity’ (Halberstam, 2010:11: Foucault, 2003: 11). Conversation, gossip, rumour, raised eyebrows, righteous indignation, in-fighting, anger, surprise, ridicule, humour, as well as well as what I call the practice of sceptical correction, are all remediated in and across the mechanisms and practices of post-publication-peer-review. As many scientists have lamented, post-publication-peer-review is opening science up to publics who can now engage with science in mediatized environments and in ways that can exceed either endorsement or scepticism. The ‘shadow media’ of blogs, websites, Twitter, Google+ posts, comments on articles in open-access journals and so forth (Chow, 2012) are potential carriers of ‘low theory’ (Halberstam, 2010). Low theory can take us to ‘the unplanned, the unexpected, the improvised, and the surprising’ (p. 16). As Halberstam goes on to argue, Here we can think about a kind of theoretical model that flies below the radar, that is assembled from eccentric texts and examples and that refuses to confirm the hierarchies of knowing that maintain the high in high theory. (Halberstam, 2010: 16)
Conclusion If we consider science experimentation as part of what Halberstam terms ‘high theory’, we can also turn our attention to what flies below the radar and is a carrier of ‘low theory’, of those subjugated knowledges that trouble the parameters of positivist science experimentation. In the next chapter and Scene 2 ‘The Experiments: Time-Traveling Porn, the Political Economy of Pornception and Big Data’. I will explore this by focusing particularly on what was considered absurd and nonsensical within and about Bem’s experiments (despite his use of the scientific method). Rather than confirm some of the scepticism, which accompanies these experiments as they have travelled across
Feeling the Future
105
broadcast and social media, I will mine the potential of the buzz and blather in order to extend and make visible the historical a prioris that have shaped science in this area. They continue to haunt the scientific method. Chapter 5 will explore how the comedic satire of the experiments that was condensed into the image of pornception or ‘time-travelling porn’ disclose the underexplored social technology of these experiments. This will extend Brown’s (2012) analysis of the social technologies and social devices of contemporary psychological experimentation and re-move those displaced narratives, actors and agents who gesture towards a future psychology. This is a psychology that is more compatible with the field of affect studies, speculative philosophies and the biohumanities and with a reconfiguration of psychological matters as transitive, indeterminate and primarily ecological, that is contingent within specific historical, technical, symbolic, biological and historical milieu – all the way down and right to the bottom. The analysis in the next two chapters will reveal or disclose some of the more interesting and inventive propositions, which revive the scientific methods’ more curious and creative pasts.
5
Pornception and Big Data Scene 2. The Experiments: Time-Travelling Porn, the Political Economy of Pornception and Big Data The media descended on the story. Stephen Colbert brought Bem on air and made quips about ‘time-traveling porn’. The New York Times interviewed Bem, as did Al-Jazeera, ABC, and Fox News. And by the time I finally reached him – in early February, at his modest condo in Ithaca – it seemed like he was returning from a long and comical voyage. The egghead scholar, who’s spent the last 54 years of his life tucked away on college campuses, had just journeyed through the bowels of celebrity culture – and emerged sporting a bemused grin. ‘My partner is always talking about who should play me in the film version,’ Bem said of his long-time companion, Ithaca College communications professor Bruce Henderson. ‘He’s thinking Dustin Hoffman.’1
Professor Daryl Bem appeared on The Colbert Report on 27 January 2011,2 where Stephen Colbert mined the comedic potential of ‘time-reversal’ and retroactive facilitation to predict the 2012 presidential election. The title of the segment was ‘time-traveling porn’ and referred to experiment 1, one of the nine experiments, which Bem constructed in order to investigate his proposition that time reversal is possible. By all accounts the other eight experiments did not have much dramatic tension despite Bem’s experience and recounted gift for stagecraft, which was demonstrated to audiences in his former life as a stage magician. The other eight experiments were rather boring and certainly did not capture the imagination of commentators in the way that this one did. Colbert amplified Bem’s apparent proposition that erotic images were likely to be more attuned to experimental subjects’ propensities for precognition, such that anticipating and feeling the future was transposed to the future anticipation of porn – or, in a curious time reversal, porn’s propensity to anticipate subject’s
Pornception and Big Data
107
capacities for precognition. The comedic value of erotic images being used to turn on (no pun intended) this capacity became a humorous tag-line for broadcasting the experiments to a wider public and the joke was certainly not lost on Colbert. To understand what captured Colbert’s attention I will turn to the specific experiment, which captured and captivated public attention. The experiment, which became the butt of the joke, used erotic imagery to shape experimental subject’s future propensity for porn.
Time-travelling porn and pornception As with most psychology experiments, the experimental subjects were psychology undergraduates (fifty men and fifty women) who were briefed as to the experiment’s aims in the manner of a prime designed perhaps to arouse and stimulate their potential desire for erotic imagery. The experimental brief is reproduced below: This is an experiment that tests for ESP. It takes about 20 minutes and is run completely by computer. First you will answer a couple of brief questions. Then, on each trial of the experiment, pictures of two curtains will appear on the screen side by side. One of them has a picture behind it; the other has a blank wall behind it. Your task is to click on the curtain that you feel has the picture behind it. The curtain will then open, permitting you to see if you selected the correct curtain. There will be 36 trials in all. Several of the pictures contain explicit erotic images (e.g. couples engaged in nonviolent but explicit consensual sexual acts). If you object to seeing such images, you should not participate in this experiment (Bem, 2011: 3).
The main hypothesis for the experiment is that experimental subjects would be able to anticipate the position of erotic imagery more accurately than might be expected by chance. Bem situates this hypothesis within studies of psi phenomena, which include telepathy, clairvoyance, precognition and premonition. Although such phenomena have a long history of being associated with the paranormal, Bem suggests that the term psi is descriptive – it refers to phenomena that cannot be explained through current conceptions of what it means to sense, anticipate and predict. Bem argues that psi phenomena present two challenges: empirical and theoretical. The empirical challenges are related to the need to design experiments that are ‘simple and
108
Haunted Data
transparent’ (p. 408), so that they can be replicated by other researchers and laboratories. The theoretical challenges relate to most psychologists’ beliefs that such phenomena are impossible or improbable. In relation to the empirical challenges of Bem’s particular experiments, experiment 1 is presented as a simple and transparent experiment, which will be easy to replicate by others. Indeed, Bem has made available all the materials needed to carry out the experiments in order to meet the protocols for replicability (also see Bem, 2015).3 This includes detailed instruction manuals, the computer software for running the experiments and the dataanalytic packages to translate the results. They are presented as needing only a desktop computer and some statistical packages to collect and analyse the data and are available for a general public to participate in.4 A simulation of the ESP test that Bem used was published in the New York Times on 27 February 2011. Interested readers can follow the link5 to take a version of the test for themselves. Some of the experimental instructions as reported in the New York Times are reproduced below: Click Anywhere to Continue After a ten-second relaxation period, during which you should try to clear your mind, you will see two curtains. Click the curtain that you believe has an image hidden behind it. After you choose, the curtain will open, and you’ll see if you were correct. After 24 guesses, you’ll see your results. Daryl Bem’s original experiment included ‘neutral’ images and ‘erotic’ images; so will ours, although our erotic images are much more SFW than his.
The eleven comments on this article represent some of the microdynamics surrounding the reception of his study, which have formed some of the extended commentary found across different media platforms. This includes that Bem is demonstrating what a good sense of humour he has and that in the ‘ProfessorRetirement Phase of his life’ he is having a bit of fun. More importantly, he is showing how easy it is to fool people. Brianj is more concerned, suggesting that the experiment is ‘fake’, and as he goes on to suggest: I suspect this is a Randi sting – he has designed a fake experiment which will nearly always produce a big effect, and after a lot of people have written
Pornception and Big Data
109
in saying this proves psi exists he will announce that it is a fake, and say triumphantly that this shows how easily people can be fooled! I’ve not been fooled anyway.
The sceptical view that the experiment is fake and a hoax is entangled with endorsements of the experiments by parapsychologists who indeed believe that psi phenomena exist. The reader may recall in Chapter 3 that the first parapsychology laboratory was set up at Duke University in the 1930s by JB and Louisa Rhine. The Rhine Research Centre, as it is now called, exists to this day, although it is now independent from Duke University. In the blog associated with the centre, a post published on Wednesday 2 February 2011 leads with the headline, ‘Daryl Bem: Precognition in the Mainstream’.6 As the author, John J Kruth argues, On Thursday, January 27, 2011, the well-respected psychologist Daryl Bem, PhD appeared on the Comedy Central program the Colbert Report to discuss a topic that has set the media ablaze and has given the world of psychology a new perspective on how to test theories that have been the subject of parapsychology for over a century. Retroactive habituation, or time-travelling porn (as Stephen Colbert called it), is an example of precognition (receiving information about future events) which can be demonstrated under laboratory conditions using traditional psychological testing methods. This top notch research has been recognized by the New York Times, New Scientist, Psychology Today and ABC News, and it has been published by the respected peer-reviewed scientific Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.
Bem of course was very aware how controversial this and the other eight experiments were likely to be. His appearance across the media, in what Kruth terms the ‘media blaze’ fuelled by these experiments, is tempered by Bem’s more considered call for other researchers to replicate his study. The challenge of replication has been taken up by at least ninety laboratories to date, and Bem (2015) has presented a meta-analysis of this data, which still fails to convince his sceptics.7 The debate and stack of proliferating non-replication and replication studies piles up.8 As with all of the experiments conducted by Bem they are presented as ‘time-reversal’ experiments. They are framed as reversing ‘known psychological effects’. Experiment 1 is said to reverse a known psychological effect, which Bem terms presentiment, which has a lineage to studies of priming (see Chapters 2
110
Haunted Data
and 3). This psychological effect relates to studies by Dean Radin (1997), which found that subjects viewing erotic imagery were more physiologically aroused than when viewing so-called neutral imagery, and that the physiological arousal occurred a few seconds before the imagery was presented. Readers familiar with debates on affect theory in the humanities might recognize this as a version of what Massumi (2002) and many others have termed the ‘half-second delay between stimulus and response’. This refers to studies, such as those by neuroscientist Benjamin Libet on readiness potential, where measures of physiological arousal or brain activity apparently occur prior to cognitive awareness.9 There are many critiques and counter-critiques in the affect literature of this position. Despite the contestation surrounding this experimental artefact, the ontological assumption of a delay between matter and mind (and affect and meaning) has become something of a blackbox across affect theories. The more interesting proposition within the affect literature is that processes that are seen to take place subliminally or outside conscious awareness or attention have not been given enough consideration in accounts of the media and mediation. This is what Richard Grusin has described as a more ‘embodied, affective experience’ of mediation’, which foregrounds bodily, sensory, haptic extensions of media into the everyday ontological immediacies of our lives (2015: 131). This trend within media studies towards studying processes variously described as subliminal, non-conscious, automatic, pre-conscious and so forth, is also mirrored in social psychology with more attention, according to Bem, being given to ‘processes that are not accessible to conscious awareness or control’ (2011: 2). Bem cites studies on priming by John Bargh to evidence this tradition, who in his 2008 book, The Unconscious Mind has argued that psychology has suffered from a ‘conscious-centric bias’ and a ‘mind first’ cosmology, which he links to the influence of Descartes and Locke on the discipline (p. 73; also see Chapter 3; and experiment 3 in Bem’s study). As we saw in the last two chapters, studies of priming have a long history of courting controversy and exactly what priming is and how it might be understood is far from settled. It is certainly far from a known psychological effect although it might be considered a demonstrable psychological effect that often exhausts the interpretations brought to bear upon it. For that reason priming shares a kinship with other phenomena, such as suggestion, imitation,
Pornception and Big Data
111
emotional contagion, automaticity, and related processes, which have been of interest to many affect theorists. As I have argued in previous work, engaging in creative mis-readings or speculative what if readings of mainstream psychology and neuroscience, often obscures the dynamism of scientific debate, contestation and controversy. On that basis just what might count as ‘known psychological effects’ and how then these might be reversed relate to more fundamental ontological issues that are not adequately acknowledged or discussed by most psychologists. Despite these important issues, which for the most part fall outside the established parameters of critique in this area, I will invite the reader to now suspend these problems and return to experiment 1 and its experimental design. In its ‘simple’ formulation it is designed to reverse the anticipation of erotic imagery and the physiological arousal that pre-empts it. It is important to state that measures of physiological arousal were not undertaken in these studies, but variations of the experiment did explore gendered differences in precognition scores, which were related to male-identified subjects’ supposed higher threshold for erotic imagery – in other words that the imagery used would need to be more explicit to produce arousal. In short, the argument made is that subjects will have a better-than-chance capacity to anticipate erotic imagery, because of erotic imagery’s ability to increase physiological arousal. This would be the case even when erotic imagery was presented after the subjects had already made their prediction. This according to Bem is confirmation of the White Queen’s view that memory works both ways. The experiments might therefore be described as engaging in very queer forms of correlation! This was framed by Bem as the potential of erotic imagery to retroactively influence the past, providing the comedian Stephen Colbert with his headline and punch line ‘Time-Traveling Porn’. Within the realms of statistical probability Bem demonstrated that precognition, the capacity to feel the future, is possible and that the hypotheses in most of the experiments were supported (53.1%).10 Bem’s extended discussion considers the problems with the automated logic of the experimental design and the extent to which the ‘Araneus Alea 1 hardware-based random number generator’ can be considered truly random. He concludes that the supposed simple and transparent design of the experiment and their time-reversal logic makes the experiments easy to
112
Haunted Data
replicate, and that these are well-established psychological effects that would be familiar to most readers of the journal (see p. 14). As he argues, ‘Simplicity and familiarity become essential tools of persuasion’ (2011: 14). As we have seen, one of the discourses, which emerged in relation to the experiments and spread across social and broadcast media related to the use of erotic imagery to shape the past and therefore the assumed capacities of experimental subjects to feel and anticipate the future. This was picked up in headlines on blogs and articles, including an article on a website for Fastcompany.com with the title ‘Predicting the Future with Porn’,11 or in a similar article published on the site mentalfloss.com with the headline, ‘Predicting the Future (or at least predicting where Naked people are)’.12 It was also carried by an article in the Scientific American on 19 April 2011, with the headline, ‘Extrasensory Pornception’,13 making links to Bem’s appearance on The Colbert Report and written by the publisher of the Skeptic Magazine, Michael Shermer.14 The article outlines a position endorsed across social and broadcast media by many sceptics, and draws on a riposte to Bem’s study written by Ray Hyman, which has gained much attention and coverage. This response appeared in the New York Magazine in March 201115 with the headline ‘53.1% of You Already Know What This Story’s About. Or Do You? Need a Hint?’ Although using an attention-grabbing headline, which made links to The Colbert Report, the article starts with a slideshow ridiculing another of the experiments based on reversing the ‘retroactive facilitation of recall’. This time reversal is based on the assumption that practice improves memory recall. In this case, the practice occurred after the recall demonstrating The White Queen’s proclamation that good memory works both ways (forwards and backwards in time).
Panspectric surveillance and pornception I will consider the parameters of the sceptical positions in Chapter 6, scene 3. What I want to do in the next section is consider why erotic imagery condensed into the image of ‘pornception’ is so contagious and what this might tell us about the political economy of propensity. The surveillance scholar Karl Palmas (2011: 343) identifies the political economy of propensity as a field, which takes new forms of surveillance and regulation as its object. He turns his attention to those techniques and forms of surveillance which attempt
Pornception and Big Data
113
to ‘control markets through techniques of predictive surveillance’. These techniques are reliant on ‘computer-assisted data-mining’ and corporations such as Walmart and Google are good examples of such practices. Drawing on the work of Deleuze on ‘control societies’ (1992) and Delanda (1991) on the panspectron, he argues that panspectric surveillance works through data-mining, data analytics and forms of regulation, management and control aligned to ‘computer analyses of large sets of data’ (343). The concept of panspectric surveillance is often seen to mark a decisive break with more panoptic forms of power analysed by Michel Foucault, specifically in Discipline and Punish. Palmas rather explores how these diagrams of power co-exist and indeed how predictive analytics extends probabilistic statistics, taking it into new thresholds of analysis and attempts by corporations, government and the military to anticipate, govern and shape behaviour. These new diagrams of power are also associated with new ways of specifying and acting upon human propensities, based on ‘preemptive modes of control’ (343), which work in and through what Palmas describes as ‘sub-individual mimetic contagions’ (350). This is one version of what Deleuze (1992) termed ‘abstract machines’, which brings together new data-mining techniques, the increasing digitalization of cultures, with new ways of acting upon (human) subjects. This includes targeting what many refer to as precognitive, or non-cognitive registers of experience, also described as affective and immaterial forms of labour. Thus, data analytics and strategies of pre-emption and anticipation, which increasingly organize software cultures, are being taken up by business and consumer organizations using ‘sophisticated techniques that anticipate the propensities of customers to act in certain ways’ (Palmas, 2011: 339). Palmas calls for a ‘political economy of propensity’ (p. 352), which can contribute to an excess or ‘something else’ to these normative anticipatory strategies. In relation to this I want to consider the iconic image of porn and pornception, and particularly the capacity of porn to retroactively reshape the past. This icon speaks to current regimes of anticipation and pre-emption, which as we have seen work with such a conceptual a priori. These relate to the more ordinary aspects of preemption, foresight, foresensing and premediation, which shape the cultural politics of data and which disclose the gendered, classed, sexed and raced politics of automation (also see Chun, 2006, 2016).
114
Haunted Data
The political economy of pornception This section will ask why the anticipation and perception of porn, or what commentators referred to as ‘pornception’ became so contagious? How did pornception condense Bem’s studies into a particular iconic image that was shared across broadcast and social media? We might ask, why is porn so contagious? What imagined futures was the capacity of pornception valorizing, creating and making perceptible? What does this example of media contagion suggest about networked affect and how to understand processes of change, transformation and transmission within and across digital and social media? In order to address these questions let’s first return to the ‘simple and transparent’ experimental design that Bem (2011) argues will enable easy replication. This design of course will be familiar to most readers as it deploys a techno-bodily interface, which inserts the experimental subject (or user) into a computer-assisted mode of response and attention. The subject is sedentary (in a chair), facing a screen, where they are asked to anticipate and respond to the possibility of erotic imagery appearing from behind one of two curtains by pressing keys. The keys correspond to two possibilities (yes or no), which reproduce a ‘forced-choice’ design that has been used in psi research since at least the beginning of the nineteenth century. Bem suggests that one of the differences between this forced choice design and the current experiment, is that the experiment is assumed to be working through processes that are not available to conscious awareness or control – in this case preemptive physiological arousal. On this basis we might consider the experiment an interesting laboratory for isolating those micro-processes that are the subject of regimes of pre-emption and anticipation discussed in the previous section (see Palmas, 2011) – that is the subject’s propensities for arousal in the anticipation of erotic imagery and the capacity of algorithmic entities to respond and shape preemptive arousal. The experimental subject is also participating in the creation of a particular databank, where their responses will be aggregated with others and patterns of statistical significance will be determined. The analysis will use a data analytics shaped by specific algorithms – Markov Chain algorithms – , which underpin Bayesian statistical analysis, for example (see Chapter 6). In an interesting twist to the problem of experimental bias, or experimenter expectation, the experimenter and their own intentions, anticipations and
Pornception and Big Data
115
actions have assumed to have been excised from the material-semiotic apparatus. Despite some images which have circulated across social and broadcast media of Darly Bem as a magician conjuring effects from the screen,16 the decisions as to what kinds of imagery (neutral or erotic) go behind which curtain are fully automated. The automated decisions are based on a set of algorithms, which compute and enact particular mathematized conceptions of randomness, with the aim of taming and beating chance.17 In this sense they also foreground the capacity of algorithms to shape anticipation, introducing non-human actors and their computational actions into the culture and design of the experimental apparatus. The experiment crystallizes many of the arguments made about software cultures and the new forms of mediation that are being materialized within contemporary forms of governance and control. As we explored in the introduction to Part 2 of the book, Luciana Parisi (2013), among many others, has argued increasingly that so-called intelligent algorithms, which seek to pre-empt and shape what we desire, are already at work in shaping possible futures out of the patterns, anomalies, accidents and multiple media transactions that we make throughout our lives. As she argues, algorithms are no longer to be thought of as instructions or rules to perform tasks at the level of computation, but are ‘performing entities: actualities that select, evaluate, transform and produce data’ (p. ix). She argues that increasingly computation and computational entities have pervaded culture and to that extent are our co-creating partners in what might come to be. To this extent the experiment simulates an arrangement of forces that are part and parcel of everyday media transactions. In the specific case of pornception, such practices are part and parcel of media transactions seeking, anticipating and consuming pornographic images on computer screens, while clicking or ‘clicking through’ (Paasonen, 2011). The human subject is sedentary and browsing the internet for porn, or engaging in practices of distraction as part of a technology of divided attention (combining work and play perhaps). One example of this latter practice of divided attention relates to a CNN report, which suggests that when the United States was in the middle of one of its worst financial crises, in 2007, employees of the Security and Exchange Commission (the SEC) were visiting porn sites and consuming sexually explicit images on government computers.18 The switching from financial trading to porn and
116
Haunted Data
the temporality of porns’ (and financial markets’) endless cycles of repetition, arousal, open-ended gratification, deferral and waiting is perhaps one clue as to why traders switched screens from one ecology to another (related) ecology in the midst of a financial meltdown. This is perhaps not so surprising when read in the context of what it means to be plugged into a social technology and milieu, which disappears or recedes from conscious awareness while at the same time returning in curious and sometimes not so curious forms of anticipation and feeling the future. In this sense practices of divided attention are shaped through the affordances and embodied immediacy of particular technological apparatuses. Users are ‘plugged in’ to life forms that have become ubiquitous and computer screens embody imaginaries that disappear from view only to reappear as automatic reactions or physiological responses (also see Venn, 2010). In order to understand the significance of the way subjects might be primed in their habits, dispositions, affective tendencies and apparent capacities for pornception, let’s consider insights from science and technology studies and body studies in relation to the under-explored social technology or dispositive of this experiment. In a seminal article based on an ethnography of market traders, KnorrCetina and Bruegger (2002: 180) explore market traders’ apparent propensities for ‘knowing’ the financial market and when to act – what some describe as ‘developing a feeling for the market’. They argue that the kinds of automatic bonds and capacities that traders experience as gut reactions or being on automatic pilot come from training, choreography, discipline, habit and a form of ‘binding’ where their insertion into a complex set of human and nonhuman relations disappears from view. This might be described as an ecological analysis that underscores the reliance, interdependence and relationality of organism with environment. As the authors describe, the trader appears to be ‘viscerally plugged into the screen reality’ (Knorr-Cetina and Bruegger, 2002: 176), often from 7 am to 6 pm, enjoying the adrenalin buzz, which is described by some traders as literally eating, sleeping, working and relaxing with the market. The complex actors and agents, which compose the market and its speed and liquidity, or even illiquidity (see Seigworth and Tiessen, 2012), are condensed into a screen, which the trader becomes plugged into. They experience the ‘hooking power’ of the market via the screen as a life form,
Pornception and Big Data
117
which they enter and become part of (Knorr-Cetina and Bruegger, 2002: 164). As Knorr-Cetina and Bruegger (2002: 164) describe, When traders arrive in the morning they strap themselves to their seats, figuratively speaking, they bring up their screens, and from then on their eyes will be glued to that screen, their visual regard captured by it even when they talk or shout to each other, their bodies and the screen world melting together in what appears to be a total immersion in the action in which they are taking part. The market composes itself in these produced-and-analysed displays to which traders are attached.
This ethnographic or ‘anthropological attitude’ to the visceral and affective is one that we encountered in Chapter 3 in relation to the writings of the American anthropologist Emily Martin, and the work of the Belgian science studies scholar, Vinciane Despret. As Martin (2013) argues, it is important to develop an understanding of how the social goes all the way down, right to the bottom, and even to those behaviours which are experienced as automatic, visceral, raw, natural and so forth. This is important, she argues, if we are to avoid a neuro-reductionism in our understandings of the non-conscious and what is often gathered together under the designation of the subliminal. The question of precognition might be recast differently in this respect: how are subjects primed historically through attachments to particular technical assemblages of matter and meaning that are brought to the fore within these experiments? In other words, how do these experiments make visible those practices of anticipation, which work on the edges of consciousness, reduced in this experiment to an apparent capacity to anticipate erotic imagery? How have capacities of anticipation and attention been shaped through historical assemblages of matter-meaning such that dispositions are always-already part and parcel of what becomes experienced as automatic or involuntary? These questions are left under-explored. As is the question, why did pornception become such a contagious meme for spreading the experiments to a wider public? The experience, memory and potential histories of such practices of anticipation form the unexplored social technology of this experimental apparatus. They arguably come to the foreground in the comedic and contagious potential of what has come to be known as ‘time-traveling porn’ or extrasensory pornception. This is perhaps just one example of how twentyfirst-century media work in what Mark Hanson (2015 has termed a shift from
118
Haunted Data
a past-directed recording platform to a data-driven anticipation of the future. He argues that twenty-first-century media largely work outside the realm of perceptual consciousness and through these new future-oriented modalities of ‘seeing’. These of course are complex and vexed genealogical questions, which foreground how specific imaginaries are entangled with technologies. The critical question is how to understand mediation without instating a technological determinism. Approaches to mediation must also account for the entanglement of particular imaginaries that are not only necessary preconditions for technological forms, but also shape and are shaped by the affordances of particular technologies. It is the imaginary of pornception that grabbed public attention as well as the potent cultural fantasy of the future time-travelling to the past. What therefore does the contagious potential of pornception disclose about networked affect or the affective dynamics of pornception? Susanna Paasonen has recognized the affective potential of porn and developed an analysis informed by affect studies in her book Carnal Resonance; Affect and Online Pornography (2011) and in an article in the journal Porn Studies, ‘Between Meaning and Mattering: On Affect and Online Porn’ (2014). As she argues, although analysing the representational dynamics of porn are important, there is also a need for ‘more complex theorizations of how these cultural images, texts and sounds work and what they may do, and how these depictions of bodies work with and in relation to the bodies of the audience. This necessitates explorations into the visceral and the affective’ (p. 138). The capacity of porn to move subjects beyond ideological effects, as Paasonen argues, is one that affect theories foreground. What she calls ‘carnal resonance’ relates to ‘practices of sensing and making sense’ (p. 138) that following Stephen Shaviro (2010) she suggests, open up to a conceptualization of affect ‘as a non-personal and precognitive intensity that animates encounters with images’ (Shaviro 2010). What I have hoped to show is that the question of affect and non-personal and precognitive intensities cannot be divorced from the complex entangled relations (economic, psychic, political, cultural, historical, technical, symbolic, imaginary) that go all the way down, literally right to the bottom!19 Porn travels and, as the feminist Sheila Jeffereys (2009) has argued, is a multibillion-dollar global market that is outsourced to search engines such
Pornception and Big Data
119
as Google. Its ‘intelligent’ algorithms anticipate and shape pornception aiding and extending the ‘commercialisation of women’s subordination’ (Jeffereys 2009: 1).20 Pornception is an iconic image that articulates the enactment, performance, techniques, and imaginaries that shape practices of future forecasting. Franklin, Lury and Stacey (2000) use the concept of the icon to capture those complex relationalities, which become figuratively condensed into particular recognizable images. In the context of theories of globalization in the 1990s, they identify three recurring icons – the blue planet, the seed and the cell. As they argue these icons perform new ways of imagining life and its parameters. Franklin et al. were writing in 2000, where in the interim the open-ended and future temporality of such icons has been extended and supplemented by icons, which condense future forecasting, computation and new ways in which populations are being specified, governed and managed. Feeling the future is perhaps one icon, as is its more bleak counterpart, the anthropocene. I would also suggest there are much ‘older’ icons that have become barely perceptible, which perform future-oriented imaginaries. This might include Preciado’s (2013) description of the current phase of capitalism as one, which is primarily ‘pharmacopornographic’. The enthusiastic reception across the arts and humanities of Paul, formally Beatriz Preciados (2013) book, Testo Junkie: Sex, Drugs, and Biopolitics in the Pharmacopornographic era, cogently describes the pharmacopornographic era as one in which sex, sexuality, gender and the body have become the target and object of biopolitical rationalities (pharmaceuticals, hormones, psychotropics), which trouble any notion of there being any truth or secret to the body. Pornception is therefore an iconic image, which captures such complex entangled relations, and articulates the range of interests (commercial, economic, political) with those labours (immaterial, emotional, sexual, affective) that produce porn as an object to be consumed via a computer screen. Who consumes whom, whose interests and investments are furthered, and how do computational entities enact, shape and anticipate specific subject’s desires and propensities for pornception are all-important questions. These are often missed by analyses of big data and analyses of those feedforward processes seen to characterize media and mediation. Porn is a dirty matter and the future-oriented temporalities of the internet only serve to shape propensities in ways that repeat some of the most enduring
120
Haunted Data
raced, classed, gendered and sexed inequalities and oppressions. In that sense, the future ability of porn to anticipate and retroactively shape the past (its capacity to time-travel), its contagious potential and the complex entangled relations, which are condensed in the image of porn, make pornception an interesting subject for analyses of mediation and computational cultures. This displaced narrative is also carried by Bem’s studies, albeit if it only surfaces in the comedic satire made of his attempts to conduct experiments on precognition. Porn is big (data), it is anticipatory and provides interesting subject matter for examining the political economy of propensity and the engineering of affective contagion. This is an important feminist issue and has yet to be noticed and written about within the context of big data and analyses of the data-driven anticipation of the future. Time-travelling porn and pornception highlight the importance of a feminist politics of automation that challenges the number crunching of many big-data analyses. Perhaps in this sense Bem’s use of erotic imagery is an invitation to lead from the future and consider why pornoception has created such a stir!
Conclusion In this chapter I hope to have illustrated how the comedic satire of the experiments condensed into the image of pornception or ‘time-travelling porn’ disclose the under-explored social technology of these experiments. Although the experiments might meet the criteria for statistical probability – that is the probability that the effects were not due to chance – what is missed are the complex affective, technical, symbolic, historical and material elements that shape the arrangement of forces that circumscribe the experiments. What subjects are ‘plugged into’ and how these forces and relations might circumscribe the experiment and may or may not work through pre-emptive physiological arousal is left under-acknowledged. As is what subjects are primed to believe or disbelieve, revealing the politics of truth, and even posttruth, in terms of our investments, desires and subjective commitments. These issues intersect with an important issue raised within the context of science and technology studies and what Bruno Latour (2004) has termed the conditions under which a body can learn to become affected by others,
Pornception and Big Data
121
human and non-human. Latour uses the term articulation to refer to those histories, practices, training, choreography and disciplining which allow a body to become more sensitive to finer and finer differentiations (in this context learning to become a ‘sensitive nose’ within the perfume industry). What is important is the setting and milieu, which circumscribes what it might mean to learn to become affected within this context. This rather different way of approaching what it might mean to become affected by an experimental apparatus also allows a more interesting way of approaching one of the gendered issues revealed by the experiments exploring pre-emptive physiological arousal.21 This is reported as a male-identified subject’s supposed higher threshold for erotic imagery – or, in other words that the imagery used would need to be more explicit to produce arousal. The long histories of how different subjects might learn to become affected by porn and how to understand the intersections of class, gender, sexuality, age, race and so forth are an important yet under-acknowledged issue.22 This is what I term the ‘historicity of psychological matters’ and is an approach, which I argue invites a more ecological approach to affect studies. It is one that requires a complex psycho–social–material–affective account that displaces psychological individualism. Pornception as a contagious icon therefore condenses a range of issues, which point towards how little we understand experiences, which register or are experienced as visceral, anticipatory or preemptive, particularly as they are experienced through bodily forms of arousal and attachment. These ‘known psychological effects’ are actually little understood and reveal at best how important it is to not assume that psychological processes are outside of history, milieu, setting and the material-semiotic apparatus of the experiment(s). As Emily Martin (2013) has argued in the context of affect theory the visceral and anticipatory are historical all the way down. In the next chapter this ethnographic and ecological approach to affect will be developed by returning to the social technologies of earlier psychic experiments in the nineteenth century. These have much to offer contemporary affect theorists and those interested in exploring the modulation of processes taken to exist outside of conscious awareness. Scene 3 takes these issues into the histories of statistics and statistical imaginaries in relation to truth, and even what we might now call post-truth – and to past histories of psychic
122
Haunted Data
research which have attempted to experiment with impossible or improbable things. We will explore how they intersect and are revived by contemporary discussions across the sciences and humanities of quantum and non-local consciousness, process philosophies and new materialisms. We will return to a beguiling question posed by Bem: what does it mean to experiment with impossible or improbable things? As the reader might anticipate, this question returns us to some very vexed questions about the nature of the human, subjectivity, and those processes, phenomena and registers of experience currently gathered together under the designation of the affective, the quantum and the weird.
6
Open Science and Quantum Matters Scene 3. Statistical Imaginaries; Telepathy; #replication; The Chorus of Sceptics; Open Science; Quantum and Non-local Consciousness; Quantum Retrocausality; New Materialisms; Clairvoyance; Moralizing the Future; Conspiracy theories; OOO and Orch OR Introduction This chapter splices together two scenes: current debates about conceptual replication within the philosophy of science and statistics, with nineteenthcentury debates as they played out in the wider social technologies and imaginaries governing experimentation into impossible or improbable things – this was specifically within the context of early psychic research. The epistemological hesitancies and uncertainties provide a data-trail to some of the displaced and submerged narratives, which are re-moved and carried by this controversy but which appear primarily as haunted data. The corpus of data that is re-moved by this storytelling provides openings to quantum approaches to precognition or retrocausality as they intersect with discussions of matter shaped within new materialisms and speculative philosophies across the humanities. The extended scene foregrounds the invention and creativity of experimenting with the improbable, impossible and the counterfactual as a key focus of an open science. It points towards collaborative opportunities for scientists, artists and humanities scholars to work together in new ways.
124
Haunted Data
A Year of Horrors Eric-Jan Wagenmakers For social psychologists, the year 2011 can go in the books as a true annus horribilis. First, the flagship journal in the field, the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, decided to publish an article claiming that people can look into the future. Going from silly to bizarre, this ability was reported to be strongest for extravert women confronted with erotic pictures. The resulting media frenzy centered on questions such as ‘should JPSP ever have accepted such an article?’ and, more to the point, ‘is there something wrong with the way social psychologists conduct their experiments and analyze their data?’ The author of the infamous article, Dr. Daryl Bem, was a guest on the Colbert Report, where the host mocked the effect as ‘extrasensory pornception’. And then, as if the reputation of JPSP had not yet been tarnished quite enough, the journal rejected (without external review) all manuscripts that reported failures to replicate the Bem results. As it turns out, JPSP has a long-standing policy not to publish ‘mere’ replication studies. A terrible policy to espouse, of course – apparently, JPSP believes it can pollute the field and then leave the clean-up effort to the lesser journals.1
Statistical imaginaries Eric-Jan Wagenmakers is a psychologist based in Amsterdam with an interest in Bayesian statistics, models of decision-making, philosophy of science and the interaction between quantitative modelling and cognitive neuroscience.2 The ‘year of horrors’ that Wagenmakers depicts is one that saw Bem’s precognition study reaching a ‘media frenzy’, with its claim that ‘people can look into the future’. As he looks forward to the beginning of 2012 he reflects on the fact that the year has not got off to a good start either with Doyen’s non-replication of John Bargh’s priming study (see Chapter 2). He focuses particularly on Bargh’s response in a blog post for Psychology Today, which is described as a scathing personal attack.3 As readers will recall, this post was later removed and the user will be taken to a broken and repaired link. Wagenmakers’s piece published in De Psychonoom, (27 pages 12–13) reflects on the problems in social psychology from the position of a psychologist engaged in the philosophy of statistics, or at least in drawing distinctions between psychological research which is exploratory (he cites that at least 99 per cent of experiments would
Open Science and Quantum Matters
125
meet this criteria) and those that are confirmatory (the remaining few), which he says statistics have been designed for. Wagenmakers was also a co-author of an article published subsequently to Bem’s ‘Feeling the Future’ article in the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, responding to Bem’s claims (Wagenmakers et al., 2011). This article, which Bem also responded to in a further publication in the journal (Bem, 2011b), is titled ‘Why Psychologists Must Change The Way they Analyse their Data: The Case of Psi: Comment On Bem (2011)’. The article reports on a non-replication study of Bem’s experiments and a change made by the authors to Bem’s statistical analysis, where they use a Bayesian T test rather than a one-sided P value. These technical changes in statistical analysis lead to what is described as a more conservative analysis (rather than the ‘liberal’ charge made against statistical analyses of the P value, for example). They do not replicate Bem’s effects. Rather than orient the discussion to disproving the existence of psi phenomena (a recognized sceptics position), they rather argue that parapsychology is an interesting field of study because it demonstrates how statistics and probabilistic thinking can be used to mislead people. Later in this section I will relate this to the data-driven fantasy or dystopia of data increasingly exerting agencies over our lives, representing a hidden and increasingly omnipotent form of dataveillance. I am calling this the ‘magic of statistics, or predictive analytics as non-performatives’, to draw attention to the difference(s) between the automated practices of anticipation that users experience (often hovering at the edges or fringes of perception) and what is brought into consciousness or materialized as part of these attempts to shape action. They are not homologous or the same thing. As Giraud (2015) has argued, accounts of digital subjectivity which assume that users are captured and their desires shaped to the needs of capitalism represents a ‘deterministic picture of media-use and depicts on the one hand, a subject who is passive and politically dis-engaged and, on the other hand, the technologies of capital as operating smoothly to commodify subjectivity, with no scope for resistance’ (p. 125). Although this is a familiar argument to media studies scholars versed in active audience traditions, the attempt to move beyond the concept of ‘digital capture’ to open up more vexed questions of subjective entanglement with technology is welcomed. However, the shift to media-use is also rather voluntarist and separates technology from subjectivity
126
Haunted Data
in ways that map onto other dichotomous forms of thinking: virtual/actual, performed/authentic, technology/consciousness and so forth.4 The critical point of Giraud’s discussion, as it bears on the calls for scientists to change the way they analyse data, relates to the problems with how different statistical tests and practices aggregate data in different ways. The concept of aggregation is central to statistics and the kinds of probabilistic thinking, which underpin future-oriented data analytics. Aggregation is a strategy of taking data from different (often numerous) sources and measures, which are replaced with what are called ‘summary statistics’. Summary statistics are then used to model probable or possible explanations to shape possible futures. It is an automated competence within the context of big data analytics and remediates a strategy, which is central to probabilistic statistics. Debates and discussions about the problems with aggregation are numerous in the literature, and relate to issues of ‘ecological validity’ and what gets obscured and lost when data are taken and combined and put to work in relation to other variables (see Clark and Avery, 1976, for example). Later in the chapter we will consider the value of ‘queer aggregations’ to some of these arguments. Aggregation has perhaps overtaken randomization, which as Ian Hacking (1988) has argued was one of the principal methodological practices of early experimental design to assess validity and reliability of statistical results.5 This was primarily shaped within early psychic research on telepathy particularly through the Society for Psychical Research (established in the United Kingdom in 1882). Hacking suggests that the philosophy and practice of statistics was first developed within psychic experiments which, he argues, were marked primarily by ‘compete ignorance’ of possible explanations of psi phenomena, for example (p. 427). That is they were not concerned with explaining psychic phenomena but rather what it might mean to experiment with improbable, impossible or ‘counterfactual phenomena’. I use the term counterfactual in the dictionary sense of the definition – that is as representing what has not happened or is not the case, or, more curiously, as ‘representing what has not happened but could, would or might under differing conditions’.6 Hacking shows how one of the key concepts to assess the truth and validity of scientific experiments, randomization, emerges in this context and has a much more curious genealogy that complicates its existence in the present primarily as an arbiter of truth and reliability.
Open Science and Quantum Matters
127
Hacking shows how in early psychic experiments randomization was interwoven with the challenges of the ‘Bayesian’ school of statistics, ‘modeled by the calculus of probability and a theory of personal utility’ (p. 429). The Bayesians argued that one was never simply confirming or disconfirming hypotheses through randomization. Rather, that personal belief entered into the dispersion of probabilities, or probable explanations. These are often referred to as priors. Beliefs or priors needed to be acknowledged, factored into the design and modified in light of experimental evidence, rather than statistical tests confirming or disconfirming hypotheses. In other words, experimentation was speculative and future oriented in its probabilistic thinking. Although randomization as an arbiter of statistical truth eventually won out in the 1930s as a key practice of the scientific method, according to Hacking, the more speculative and future oriented assumptions of Bayesian statistics were interwoven with what took form. In other words, statistical practices are never confirming or disconfirming truths, according to these arguments, but rather are always involved in anticipating and mapping possible futures. In contemporary data analytics, Bayesian models of statistical inference are primarily used in futureoriented thinking (based on a dispersion of probabilities). The theory of personal utility (of the usefulness of different possible avenues for thought) has become more aligned with the commercial and corporate utility of datamining and aggregation services. Wagenmakers’s critique of Bem is primarily one which raises the spectre of these earlier disagreements and moments of epistemic uncertainty in statistical reasoning in the nineteenth century. As we have seen, it is usually assumed that randomization is used primarily to confirm or disconfirm a hypothesis – does precognition exist? However, an early debate in psychic research, which also introduced probabilistic thinking into studies of psychic phenomena, further undermined this distinction. One question related to this is whether asking if precognition exists is primarily an empirical question (does it exist) or is it rather what Hacking calls ‘non-theoretical’ (Hacking, 1988: 434)? That is it relates to what can’t be explained or to ‘a residue of inexplicable well-attested facts’ (p. 436). As the reader can probably see, these earlier historical problems are re-moved by Bem’s experiments and particularly his distinction between the theoretical and the empirical.
128
Haunted Data
Telepathy An example of what Hacking means by the distinction between the empirical and the (non)-theoretical relates to his discussion of the coining of the term telepathy by Frederick Myers in 1882. Myers argued that telepathy was a ‘mere designation’ and implies no hypothesis (p. 436). It was what he also called a ‘neutral term’ (Hacking, 1988:). Hacking suggests in this early research on psychic phenomena that the questions were taken to be technical or empirical rather than theoretical representing a more proto-performative and speculative approach to scientific experimentation. The experiments were exploring what it might be possible to produce, invent and shape if one opened up to the possibility of the improbable or impossible (or the odd and the strange). This of course was met with critical commentary and discussions on how to explain particular experimental results (their modelling perhaps) which was lively and unsettled. The interest by the Society for Psychic Research in what Hacking terms the odd and strange was embodied by the experiments into clairvoyance and thought transference by Charles Richet.7 Hacking shows how these experiments were not simply confirming or disconfirming hypotheses. Rather through a ‘calculus of probabilities’ the results were assessed against measures and calculations of results being obtained by chance. An interesting related issue in early research on psychic phenomena involved the capacity of scientific forms of experimentation to simulate phenomena rather than validate, assess or arbitrate truth-claims. One example of this relates to attempts by a Russian doctor, M. Gomalez to replicate some of Charles Richet’s experiments on ‘thought transference’ or telepathy. These results were published in a Russian medical journal Vratch and were reported on in the Journal of the Society for Psychical Research in 1885. As with debates on priming and Clever Hans, as well as experiments with psychic animals, such as the Rhine’s experiments with Lady Wonder the Typing Horse discussed in chapter 3, Gomalez argues that people receiving thoughts (percipients) could learn by observing the slight respiratory motions of the operator. He suggests that these are not consciously perceived, but rather perceived in a state of rapport or hyperanaesthesia (considered a form of hypnotic rapport). It was possible therefore, according to Gomalez, to simulate Richet’s results with telepathy in two ways.
Open Science and Quantum Matters
129
First, telepathy can be simulated through a curious form of hypnotic rapport (sometimes called muscle memory), which produces effects, which transfer through states still little understood. This argument is similar to the one we encountered in chapter three in relation to psychic animals and how to understand their apparently prodigious talents. As we saw the replacing of telepathy with processes of hypnotic suggestion and what it means to enter into suggestive relations with another, human and non-human is still little understood within the contemporary sciences (see Chapter 3; Walsh et al., 2014). The productive potential of suggestive phenomena has been closed down, due in part to suggestion’s close association with Hitlerism, propaganda, fascism, dictatorships, crowd psychology and the image of an evil Svengali figure manipulating others (see Stengers, 1997; Blackman, 2012). Hacking also argues that Gomalez simulates telepathy in another way by drawing attention to the material-semiotic apparatus, or measuring apparatuses, which produce particular phenomena. Gomalez used what he called ‘chance devices’ (Hacking, 1988: 441) such as the National Lottery to gain effects that could be ‘read’ as telepathic. These practices of simulation, which were designed to discredit Richet show how particular practices of probabilistic reason can be used to obtain results, which are nonperformatives; that is they do not bring about the objects of which they speak but do produce particular objects and entities. They tell us something very interesting about the productive and constitutive possibilities of particular measuring devices and act as an interesting precursor to more performative approaches to scientific experimentation that we find within new materialisms and science and technology studies for example. Although some of these early debates have been inherited by contemporary science, as we will go on to see, the question of what exactly measuring devices and experimental apparatuses do returns only on the margins of the Feeling the Future controversy, and in those narratives, which speak of the quantum and quantum ontologies. This submerged controversy speaks to very contemporary debates across science studies and the humanities in relation to the performativity of scientific experimentation and particularly in relation to quantum physics (see Barad, 2007). This will be developed in the next section. What also return more centrally in this controversy are earlier debates in the philosophy and practice of statistics as they were developed and played
130
Haunted Data
out in early psychic research. These still haunt the contemporary field of parapsychology as it leaks into cognitive science and anomalous psychology. This includes the field of debate, contestation and argumentation concerning randomization and probabilistic thinking, which as we have seen have their roots in psychic research. It is not surprising therefore that these issues return in relation to experiments with contemporary psi phenomena. As Wagenmakers et al. (2011: 1) argue, Instead of revising our beliefs regarding psi, Bem’s research should instead cause us to revise our beliefs on methodology: The field of psychology currently uses methodological and statistical strategies that are too weak, too malleable, and offer far too many opportunities for researchers to befuddle themselves and their peers.
If Bem’s experiments were therefore elaborate practices of simulation, what exactly is being simulated by these curious practices of anticipation and feeling the future? What does simulation do as a particular material-semiotic apparatus? What exactly is pornception if approached as a practice of simulation? What kind of experimental apparatus is simulation and in what ways is simulation performative? These are all complex questions, which would reconfigure how scientists working in the area of psi research and anomalous psychology approach experimentation.8 This is one area where science and technology studies, philosophies of experimentation, and the field of affect studies has much to offer contemporary scientific debate and formulation in this area. This argument will be returned to later in the chapter, when we consider the relationship of this controversy to the quantum and particularly to retrocausation, a concept that is derived from quantum mechanics. One of the key issues that is foregrounded in contemporary debates on psi phenomena are practices of statistical reason themselves, and the performative and nonperformative nature of probabilistic thinking. If as Wagenmakers, as well as many others, have argued, certain experimental results might be thought of as practices of statistical simulation, which do not bring about the results they claim (that people can see into the future), then what does this suggest about the kinds of data imaginaries that are shaping what Adams, Murphy and Clarke (2009) have termed sciences of speculative forecast? The gaps or lacunae between future-oriented forms of anticipation and the kinds of practices that data/experimental subjects might exhibit show
Open Science and Quantum Matters
131
how little we understand simulation as a productive practice that produces effects as at the same time these effects might be rendered counterfactual – as representing what has not happened or is not the case, or, more curiously, as ‘representing what has not happened but could, would or might under differing conditions’. These questions, as Hacking cogently shows, go right back to early psychic research and the shaping of positivist scientific methodology. They demonstrate the relationship of positivist scientific methodologies to their rather queer and submerged historical archives. A related issue according to Hacking is that psychic researchers ‘were not able to think very carefully about how to analyse their data’ (1988: 437). If we think that automated data practices of speculative forecast are more ‘intelligent’ we might want to reflect on the histories of these problems and how they are central to the conditions of possibility under which statistical reason was shaped as a form of probabilistic thinking. We inherit these problems as they mutate within and across computational culture and practices of artificial precognition. Perhaps, this is one lesson that Bem teaches us, whether as a joke, a hoax or even by simply showing what can be produced that confounds what we take the empirical and theoretical to be. These are of course complex questions that most scientists struggle with and which re-surface in these controversies in new ways in the present.
#Replication and the chorus of sceptics The complexity of these issues is missed by most contemporary scientific researchers who condense and reduce these vexed questions into the importance of replication, considered the cornerstone of scientific debate and progress.9 This became one of the key ways in which the controversy became represented across social media and ‘the blogosphere’. It was carried by the machinations of sceptics keen to disprove Bem’s claims. As Ritchie, Wiseman and French (2012) argue for example, If these results are true, the implications for psychology – and society – are huge. In principle, experimental results could be confounded by participants obtaining information from the future, and studying for an exam after it has finished could improve your grade!
132
Haunted Data
The sceptical position is usually accompanied by a call for the importance of replication. I will only touch upon this narrative briefly because it is the dominant narrative or form of representationalism, which comes to stand in for what becomes submerged, displaced, overlooked and discredited within the controversy. There are numerous blog entries that an interested reader can follow,10 which articulate such sceptical positions, which aim to discredit Bem. I will however give some examples of these positions as they coalesce in relation to the call for a more open science. As we saw in Chapter 3, ‘open science’ is constituted as a science fit for the twenty-first century and as an appropriate response to how digital communication is potentially transforming the theory and practice of scientific experimentation. As an example, a blog written some two years after the Bem study by Daniel Lakens11 on Friday 23 May 2014 outlines the practice of pre-publication-peer-review and the ethics and problems with replication and statistical claims and practices (including correcting statistics). It includes the claim that parapsychology is an area of research, which is responsive to improvements in methodology and statistical calculations and the evidential claims that can be made. The blog includes a link to a site run by Koestler Parapsychology Unit at the University of Edinburgh where parapsychologists are encouraged to pre-register their experiments for the purposes of pre-publication peer review.12 The author of the blog, Daniel Lakens, is in the School of Innovation Studies at Eindhoven University of Technology and works in the Human–Technology Interaction Group. He is a keen advocate for open science and open research and lectures, writes and blogs about methods and statistics. His view on open science has been influenced, he says in the blog post, by an article published in Psychological Inquiry by Nosek and Bar-Anan (2012) called ‘Scientific Utopia: 1: Opening Scientific Communication’.13
Open science Nosek and Bar-Anan (2012: 1) have written what might be considered a manifesto for how they believe science ought to be responding to the challenges of digital communication. Their specific focus is on the need for new practices of scientific authentication and legitimation and for a debate on how to use the
Open Science and Quantum Matters
133
new practices of science communication made possible by digital platforms. They call for six changes, which I will reproduce for the reader: (1) full embrace of digital communication, (2) open access to all published research, (3) disentangling publication from evaluation, (4) breaking the ‘one article, one journal’ model with a grading system for evaluation and diversified dissemination outlets, (5) publishing peer review, and, (6) allowing open, continuous peer review.
They go on to argue that the constraints on this happening are not financial but social. As they ask, what would it mean to ‘replace paper with the internet as the primary mechanism of scientific communication’? (p. 12). They specifically challenge the hierarchical model of influence and prestige, which they associate with scientific publishing based on the learned journal with its high impact factor, committed to publishing the leading researchers and research in the field. They argue that scientists in the context of digital communication are more akin to ‘a distributed system of agents operating with minimal hierarchical influence’. This more swarm-like analogy is used as a basis for expanding what peer review should, could and might become, in the context of post-publication-peer-review. It might be considered a more rhizomatic14 approach to scientific progress and innovation, where the concepts of reliability and validity are replaced with transparency and accessibility. One of the insights of the article commensurate with some discussions of post-publication-peer-review within the context of humanities research is that published journal articles are no longer static non-modifiable entities. As they argue, ‘Web-based publishing enables improved search and linking capabilities such as adding hyperlinks to citations for immediate article retrieval’ (p. 15). There is also a useful discussion on open access, which mirrors debates that the humanities are inheriting from the sciences. These practices are being shaped in relation to discussions about new forms of digital publishing and potential new measures of post-publication-peer-review linked to judgements of academic worth and value by funding bodies and related institutions (see Broekman et al., 2014; Blackman, 2016). As I have argued, controversies are interesting because they draw attention to some of the factors which govern networked virality or the reach and traction of particular articles, which impact factors, citations and networks of relation, prestige, academic value and hierarchy obscure.
134
Haunted Data
As we have seen throughout the book, journal articles in the context of post-publication-peer-review and digital communication can be blogged about, and commented upon in websites, in the comments sections of open access journals; they can be tweeted and posted on Facebook, and they sometimes capture the attention of broadcast journalism and other more entertainment-based media cultures. However, as we have seen with this and the Bargh controversy, certain animations come to represent or stand in for the messy and indeterminate nature of digital communication and those traces, which open up to archives of the future. They are subjected to a politics of straightening becoming part of practices that work to confirm ‘business as usual’ or the reproduction of the same. As Wendy Chun (2016) has argued in her book, Updating to Remain the Same: Habitual New Media, we need to understand the habitual nature of new media if we are to grasp the potential for change and transformation. This includes addressing questions such as: ‘Why and how are so many things seemingly ignored? Why and how do things linger?’ (p. 15). Within this context the question of how and why the potential of PPPR is often simultaneously closed down at the point at which it opens to change and indeterminacy is pertinent. Nosek and Bar-Anan (2012) make a similar argument, referring to a case study linked to two blogs discussing fMRI research and the problem of corrected statistics by Neurocritic and Bor15 Although there was a substantial discussion among neuroscientists ‘using this as a case example to decide’, they ask, ‘How much of the neuroimaging literature should we discard’? After all that, the original article remains in print, unmodified. Notably, the critique, response, and subsequent debate all occurred within a matter of days through science blogs operated by scientists online. In the present system, articles with known deficiencies continue to influence future research because there is little incentive or opportunity to reexamine them.
This was also the temporary denouement to the ‘priming controversy’ staged in Chapters 2 and 3, where John Bargh’s attempts to ‘set the record straight’, overshadow the controversy that took form and the status of what remains as haunted data. In the next section I will turn to discussions of the quantum and non-local consciousness. This brings a submerged narrative within the Feeling the Future
Open Science and Quantum Matters
135
controversy into dialogue with contemporary debates across the humanities. These debates concern the nature of the human in the context of data and materiality framed within the context of the post and more-than-human. These debates currently appear within vitalist and speculative philosophies, some affect theories, new materialisms and science studies. I will attempt to show how productive this controversy is for approaching some of the most vexed and contested questions concerning the human, consciousness, subjectivity, time, and calls for a more inventive, creative and speculative science. It also raises the difficulties of analysing mind-matter relations, particularly within the context of theories of consciousness, which defy linear laws of time and space, and disrupt and displace psychological individualisms. It shows the need within affect studies, the biohumanities and speculative philosophies to theorize the incorporeal (see Grosz, 2017). It recognizes that the turn to materiality does not adequately exhaust our understanding of those experiences that have become the subject of psychology, and which are often consigned to weird science. In previous work I have described these relations as immaterial rather than material (Blackman, 2012).
Quantum and non-local consciousness16 Some readers might have felt on firmer terrain in the previous discussion of statistics, non-performatives, practices of simulation, histories of statistics and discussions of replication and its possible remediation within digital forms of communication. However, I want to begin to provide a temporary denouement to this controversy by re-moving a more marginal discussion, which hovers on the edge of this controversy. This relates to an alien psychology of time, which is rather queer (and noted as such by Karen Barad, for example). It has extended discussions of Feeling the Future into the realm of the quantum with its bending of time and nonlinear conceptions of past, present and future. This is perhaps the more submerged and displaced narrative, which hovers at the edge of scientific consciousness and which is carried by some of the affective dynamics on display. It certainly represents a very ‘queer aggregation’. More ironically perhaps it is the narrative and set of concentrated hyperlinks, which relate to contemporary discussions across the humanities within some
136
Haunted Data
affect theories, new materialisms, queer and speculative philosophies and related perspectives. It specifically relates to those perspectives which take the quantum and the molecular as objects of inquiry, as the basis of thoughtexperiments (what if the world were like this?), as ontology, as a methodological strategy, and sometimes as a quasi-positivist rendering of the world and its virtual potential. The quantum is also a design problem for some contemporary technologies, which cross the military, business and commerce in their emerging computational forms (see introduction to Part 2). As Larry Dossey, the executive director of the journal Explore asks in relation to the Feeling the Future controversy, why is the concept of non-local consciousness, or non-local knowing, that is consciousness ‘manifesting non-locally in ways that defy the limitations of space and time, why this concept is so offensive to many scientists, and why is it likely to become, at long last, a part of the scientific worldview’.17 Dossey goes on to argue, One of the unique features of these phenomena is their capacity to elicit overheated, hysterical responses from scientists. Many scientists, who are willing to entertain hypotheses in other areas of science that are so breathtakingly bizarre they can hardly be imagined – for example, an infinite number of alternate or parallel universes; string theory, which many scientists consider to be unproven and unprovable, requiring eight extra dimensions that have no basis whatever in human experience, and which cannot be experimentally verified in any way; or a Big Bang, out of which an entire universe arose from nothingness – lapse into fevered frenzy when confronted with so-called paranormal events. They simply ignore the research validating these phenomena and resort to the ‘everybody knows’ argument – because ‘everybody knows’ these things cannot happen, they do not happen.
As well as invoking prejudice as an explanation for such responses, he also suggests a form of ‘herd immunity’, which dampens any curiosity, wonder or inkling to consider whether extraordinary or even impossible things might exist (as the White Queen would retort). Dossey backs up this view with a quote from the vitalist philosopher, Albert North Whitehead, from his 1948 book, Essays in Science and Philosophy; The Universe is vast. Nothing is more curious than the self-satisfied dogmatism with which mankind at each period of its history cherishes the delusion of the finality of its existing modes of knowledge. Sceptics and
Open Science and Quantum Matters
137
believers are all alike. At this moment scientists and sceptics are the leading dogmatists. Advance in detail is admitted: fundamental novelty is barred. This dogmatic common sense is the death of philosophical adventure. The Universe is vast.
Humanities readers may be familiar with the burgeoning scholarship on Whitehead and the revitalization of process philosophy and the re-invention of some of his concepts and neologisms to address a range of subjects, areas and topics; this includes but is not exhausted by Whiteheadian-inspired analyses of medicine (Michaels and Rosengarten, 2012), psychology (Brown and Stenner, 2009), media and mediation (Hansen, 2015; Shaviro, 2015), sociology and social theory (Halewood, 2011), contemporary philosophy (see Faber and Goffey, 2014; Savransky, 2016) and science studies to name just some avenues (see Stengers, 2014). The orientation of this work is on invention and creative experimentation, particularly using Whitehead’s concepts and neologisms as the basis for adventurous thought. What is perhaps less considered are the relations more generally between vitalist or process philosophy and psychic research.18 Although Whitehead was not explicitly attached to programmes of psychic research as his contemporaries William James and Henry Bergson were, he did study at Trinity College, Cambridge, in the 1870s (where the Society for Psychic Research – SPR – was founded), he was a close friend of Henri Sedgwick who was the president of the SPR during his time there, and as Papanicolaou and Gunter (1987: 355) have argued, Whitehead might be considered a ‘pioneering citizen’ of the same philosophical world as Bergson (with the close links between the psychic and the philosophical) given their shared emphases on contingency, indeterminacy, duration, prehension, intuition, becoming and related concepts. It is perhaps not a surprise therefore that Whitehead’s process philosophy might be invoked as an example of a science that is more open, inventive, adventurous and creative. Nor that this orientation has migrated primarily from mainstream science to humanities, philosophy and social science disciplines, as well as to those researchers in the sciences still pursuing the pull and lure of psi phenomena. This migration can be seen most visibly in those areas influenced by philosophical traditions keen to reopen questions that have become disqualified and discredited by legitimate or straight science and to invent, fabricate and form new concepts for thinking. This includes
138
Haunted Data
the work and writing of Deleuze and Guattari and process philosophers for example, sometimes aligned through a form of ‘speculative philosophy’ (see Faber and Goffey, 2014). To return to Whitehead in the context of the Feeling the Future controversy, Papanicolaou (1987) suggests that Whitehead and Bergson were interested and influenced by physics and were perhaps prophets of quantum mechanics. The link between quantum philosophies of time (nonlocal knowing) and physics (specifically quantum mechanics) is reopened in this submerged narrative. As Dossey argues, there is no consensus in quantum mechanics so controversy is a resource for thinking, not an indication that an area should be closed off or closed down. As he suggests, It would be wrong to suggest there is agreement on what these experiments mean. But the fact that there is controversy and that fundamental issues in physics remain unsettled suggests that the presumptuous, full-bore criticisms of Bem’s findings are inappropriate. (page 30)
Quantum retrocausality A different history is always possible, at any time, here and now. (Schrader, 2012: 125) The past is never closed, never finished once and for all, but there is no taking it back, setting time aright, putting the world back on its axis. There is no erasure finally. The trace of all reconfigurings are written into the enfolded materialisations of what was/ is/ to-come. Time can’t be fixed. To address the past (and future), to speak with ghosts, is not to entertain or reconstruct some narrative of the way it was, but to respond, to be responsible, to take responsibility for that which we inherit (from the past and the future), for the entangled relationalities of inheritance that ‘we’ are, to acknowledge and be responsive to the noncontemporaneity of the present, to put oneself at risk, to risk oneself (which is never one or self), to open oneself up to indeterminacy in moving towards what is to come (Barad, 2010: 25). As the quantum eraser experiment shows, it is not the case that the past (a past that is given) can be changed (contrary to what some physicists have said), or that the effects of past actions can be fully mended, but rather that the ‘past’ is always already open to change’ (Schrader, 2012: 27, my emphasis).
Open Science and Quantum Matters
139
Quantum retrocausality is a queer phenomenon indeed. Part thought experiment and inventive concept, retrocausality is a term dispersed across quantum mechanics. It is also a placeholder and name given to curious events which defy linear conceptions of time – what is often described as the fixed arrow of time from past, present to future. These events relate to the sub-atomic behaviour of entities encountered by physicists at the molecular level – electrons, particles, atoms and so forth. Physicist and queer theorist Karen Barad has brought the queer potential of quantum ontologies to the humanities attention. For readers not familiar with her 2007 book, Meeting the Universe Halfway: Quantum Physics and the Entanglement of Matter and Meaning, considers how queer theory, and specifically Judith Butler’s theories of performativity, might be developed through quantum insights relating to the performativity of matter (at the sub-atomic level). She extends Butler’s concept of performativity into the realm of non-human agencies. Within what has become known as new materialisms or agential realism, it is argued that her concept of materiality offers a way out of human-centred, biologically reductionist and deterministic views of (human) vital processes. Her work is seen to replace the psyche (often equated to subjectivity) with a more entangled and distributed conception of materiality, which introduces nonhuman agency into being and becoming.19 Barad’s work was very influenced by Judith Butler’s (1993) concept of performativity, but also claims to offer a critique of this approach when refracted through particle physics. Barad situates her interventions within the context of science studies and how to understand the nature of scientific practices of experimentation. The approach to scientific forms of experimentation is informed by particle physics and quantum mechanics primarily through the work of Niels Bohr, the quantum physicist. Performativity has also been used in different ways to understand social media practices as vehicles for self-mediation. The question then often becomes framed through the need to judge these forms of self-mediation, where the questions often presuppose a series of (false) oppositions and polarizations; are they staged or authentic, individual or collective, private or public, expressing or managing the self; do they represent practices of anticipation rather than actualization; are they real or fantasy, deliberately motivated or automatic – often linked to contagious processes? So in other words, as much as the performative is central to
140
Haunted Data
discussions and analyses of identity and subjectivity within and across social media platforms, the status of the performative is reintroduced as a matter of concern.
New materialisms Barad (2007) engages in a reworking of Butler’s (1993) concept of performativity through a diffractive reading of Butler and Bohr’s ‘insights through one another for the patterns of resonance and dissonance’ (p. 195). Barad suggests that Niels Bohr is an interesting scientist as he developed a ‘proto-performative account of scientific practices’ (p. 31). That is, he recognized that scientific apparatuses do not simply describe or measure pre-existing objects and entities, but rather bring about the objects of which they purport to measure, inscribe and speak. Words, or scientific concepts are considered material enactments, ‘that contribute to, and are a part of, the phenomena we describe’ (p. 31). This is framed as a critique of representationalism. In this sense, Barad argues that scientific practices are performative and productive, and on this basis Barad seeks to make a ‘sympathetic but critical reading of Judith Butler’s provocative theory of performativity’ (p. 34). This is to displace what she suggests is an overly human-centred approach. This is framed as a ‘posthumanist performative understanding of the materialization of bodies’ (p. 34). Barad’s work, like Butler’s, has been influential across the humanities and has become staged as part of a series of turns towards the non-human, more-thanhuman, post-human and the ‘new materialisms’. It is perhaps easy to see why her provocations might be useful to media and cultural theorists who recognize that we come into being in and through mediation and that media forms and practices are entangled with such processes of being and becoming. Throughout the history of media studies and the long history of work on mediation and mediatization the critique of media forms and practices as representational has been a central unifying epistemology (also see Kember and Zylinska, 2012).
Performativity and post-human performativity Butler was influenced by Michel Foucault’s theory of power and the question of subjective commitment or investment found in psychoanalysis: why do we
Open Science and Quantum Matters
141
repeatedly invest in certain social norms, even when and if we recognize that these norms fail to deliver their promises or even do us harm – what Lauren Berlant has termed a form of ‘cruel optimism’ (2011). One of Butler’s (1990, 1993) main focuses has been on gendered norms or heteronormativity and how gendered binaries are repeated across a range of practices such that they come to appear as natural instantiations of human nature rather than reiterative statements which enter into our own self-formation in complex ways – or what Foucault termed practices of subjectification (see Chapter 1). Barad suggests that this approach relies on some kind of mechanism of psychic identification (however conscious or unconscious) in order for power to act on and through a subject’s actions – a psychic mechanism which places identification within the realm of ideation and is in danger of reducing the materiality of the body to some kind of dumb or brute materiality. These critiques have been made by many and are related to the kind of ‘docile body’ that can be found in Foucault’s account of how power works. The concept of iteration presents for many an overly semiotic way of approaching subjectification, which primarily approaches statements as forms of signification (as sites of compulsion as well as conflict and ambivalence, for example), rather than perhaps the more ambivalent and complex status of statements that can be found in Foucault’s archaeologies of knowledge, such as The Order of Things. For Barad, iteration and reiteration are primarily linguistic concepts and in her argument they fail to adequately account for material processes or in her words, how matter comes to matter. On the basis of this critique Barad suggests that performativity needs to be revised and extended beyond the realm of the human body and its primary focus on citationality within (social) practices. One of the conditions of possibility for this move are some of the new ontologies, within and across the biological and life sciences, including quantum physics. Quantum physics as well as the ‘New Biologies’ (see Blackman, 2016) or the biocultural (see Frost, 2016) present bodily materialities as lively, agentive, temporal, and always already entangled within material-semiotic apparatuses, rather than the object and instrument of power in a Foucauldian sense. As Barad (ibid: 65) argues, reflecting on Foucault’s contributions, ‘He fails to offer an account of the body’s historicity in which its very materiality plays an active role in the workings of power.’ This shift from ideation and
142
Haunted Data
psychic identification (subjectivity) to the role of material processes or the liveliness of materiality (ontology) has paved the way for humanities scholars to turn towards the neuro and biological sciences to furnish their own accounts of mediation. This move towards shaping a biohumanities or the biosocial is often framed as a move from epistemological to ontological issues, which Barad argues have not been given sufficient attention within accounts of performativity. Barad suggests that quantum theories of the liveliness of matter unsettle any distinction between matter and meaning, which she argues can be found in Butler’s writing. Barad’s approach displaces the view of scientific knowing or rationality, which sees the human subject as either potentially mastering the world or until that time existing in a state of unknowing-until-the-future – that is enacting a form of anticipative epistemological certainty. Barad has shaped a conceptual cosmology that adds materiality to the assumptions that ‘there is one further anthropocentric attitude that needs to go: the idea that the universe is as “in the dark” about the future as we are ourselves’.20 At a quantum level this appears not to be the case. Barad’s writing has been extended across the humanities and includes Vicki Kirby’s (2011) quantum anthropology; quantum approaches to (new) media studies (Kember and Zylinska, 2012; Coleman, 2011); and a branch of science studies, known as new materialisms (Coole and Frost, 2010; also see Frost, 2016), which takes seriously the liveliness of matter in the context of the human and life sciences. Barad’s own account of quantum mechanics engages with a particular psychology of intentionality that she suggests quantum mechanics inherits. She attempts to upset its human-centred ontology by extending it through distributed approaches to cognition, or what are often referred to as ‘extended mind approaches’. These assume that cognition is extended beyond the body, distributed among human and non-human actors, embodied and embedded such that cognition should not be approached as a psychological property of mind (see Clark, 2008). These are refracted through quantum ontologies of time, which further upset notions of free will, human mastery and control. This is a productive interchange and one that reconfigures intentionality as ‘attributable to a complex network of human and non-human agents, including historically specific sets of material conditions that exceed the traditional notion of the individual’ (p. 23). This, as many people have
Open Science and Quantum Matters
143
argued, develops a post-human or more-than-human approach to subjectivity. It is an approach, which appears to leave psychology and its human-wedded individualistic conceptions of the subject behind.
Speculative psychologies However, psychology like any knowledge-practice is far from unified and as we have seen throughout the book has its own histories of displaced traditions, ontologies, experimental practices, objects and entities, hesitations, anomalies and controversies. In different ways what reappear as haunted data within the book also decentre psychological individualism and point to alternative directions that psychology might have travelled. These ‘archives of the future’ reconfigure psychological matters as transitive, indeterminate and contiguous with the biological, symbolic, technical, material, immaterial and historical. My argument is that although we have very good ontological arguments, which support post-humanism, these are arguably closed down by rather impoverished engagements with subjectivity and how to analyse the complexity of mind-matter relations and the reconfiguring of psychological matters.21 This is even in perspectives such as new materialism, which are trying to move beyond psychologically individualistic accounts but do not provide a rethinking of the psychosocial within this context (see Blackman, 2017). It is these problems which are put back into circulation by the Feeling the Future controversy, which re-moves earlier psychological ideas about personhood which did not subscribe to a strict separation between self and other, human and non-human, material and immaterial and human and technical. As we have seen these displaced histories haunt the Feeling the Future controversy in the present and return as traces to a past, which open to archives of the future. One issue related to this, which was explored in the last chapter is what I termed the ‘historicity of psychological matter’ characterized as a more ecological approach to affect; or how psychological matters are always already shaped and reshaped through milieu, history, context and setting. As some scholars have argued, work within affect studies often does not view affective processes as historical, all the way down and right to the bottom. Instead separations are made between matter and meaning, which can lead
144
Haunted Data
to rather neuroreductionist and even behaviourist accounts of the suggestive capacities of subjects. A good example of what is at stake can be found in the work of Hannah Landecker (2016) and the analytic attention she gives to the materiality of history as well as the historicity of matter, or what she calls the biology of history and the history of biology (Landecker, 2016). Landecker makes these arguments in relation to the historicity of bacteria showing how the very human and technical practices, which have taken bacteria as their object have fundamentally shaped and reshaped what bacteria is, as an entity. In other words matter is always already entangled and contiguous with other processes and practices and bears the imprint of historicities and even historialities before it enters into future relational refigurings. Matter is not simply lively but historical and this historicity already displaces strict separations between past, present and future conditionings and reconditionings. These arguments provide an opening to future research directions that are carried by the Feeling the Future Controversy. However, what we find in Barad’s account is a refutation of a particular psychological account and theory known as a ‘psychology of intentionality’. This inadvertently reproduces the role of the scientist as an arbiter and judge of which theories are true and relevant and which are not. What are missed are the histories of hesitations, gaps, anomalies and ‘versions’ of science (to use a term coined by Despret), which become backgrounded, yet exist in relations of disqualification, disequilibrium and co-existence. It is these trails which haunt Feeling the Future and which trouble the historical distinction and policing of the boundaries between the psychic and the psychological, which have become part of psychology’s emergence and professionalization as a discipline. As an example, a very particular psychology of intentionality and will entered quantum mechanics via the work of Niels Bohr. This was primarily in his consideration of how experimenters influenced measurement interactions. Bohr assumed a particular human-centred psychology, where the experimenters’ intentionality is taken to influence the experimental system. Barad rejects this human-centred psychology and as we have seen replaces it with a post-human account of performativity. However, Bohr is recruiting one possible account of experimenter subjectivity, which does not exhaust other productive possibilities and interchanges. There are other starting
Open Science and Quantum Matters
145
points in the past and present of psychology – particularly as it intersects with ‘weird science’ – which also displace traditional individualistic notions of the individual and the human. The question might then be reframed – it is not whether the human is over, post or obsolete, but rather it is, what other kinds of experimental arrangements or forces might attune to submerged or displaced potentialities or historialities? What kind of human is possible when experimental practices that actively recruit or attune to particular potentialities (improbable, impossible, extraordinary or even counterfactual) are staged? One obvious starting point, which already intrudes on the contemporary cognitive neuroscience of will and intention, is to engage with a physics and psychology of time that is re-moved in the Feeling the Future controversy. It is expressed in and through the ontological indeterminacy of all those strange psychic entities, processes and objects, which confound scientific consensus. These enter the controversy via the work of particular physicists developing quantum theories of consciousness or mind-matter relations to explain psi phenomena and particularly clairvoyance and precognition (Hamerof, 2012; Stapp, 2011). These strange mysterious relations align the quantum activities of particles and atoms with psychic capacities considered impossible, improbable or otherwise inexplicable by rational cognitive means. This rather weird or queer science is not given any attention by Barad and I want to imagine in the next section what such a queer coupling might produce in terms of shaping an archive of the future.
Strange causalities, queer critters and nature’s queer performativity Although Niels Bohr engaged with a particular psychology of intentionality and will (predicated on a bounded human agential subject) he perhaps did not recognize approaches to will and intentionality within weird science that have more in common with his performative approach to quantum experimentation. One example is in those border-crossings between and across parapsychology, automaticity research, and controversies and disagreements in the phenomenology of will and intentionality within cognitive science (see Ebert and Wegner, 2011). The work of the late cognitive scientist Daniel Wegner,22 for example, explores how the phenomenology of will, intentionality
146
Haunted Data
and automaticity might be produced through specific ‘entangled agencies’ (Ebert and Wegner, 2011: 23). As he has argued, conscious will is a trick or illusion, which is not attributable to a bounded human subject exerting agency or intentionality (the usual psychologized explanation that Bohr reproduced). Conscious will is produced or enacted through particular material assemblages of matter and meaning. Bodies are indeterminate and extended by the relations they enter into. These relations are often experienced in intensive registers – that is below the threshold of conscious awareness and attention and even as ‘alien phenomenologies’. This is the phenomenology of will and intention that Wegner and others are exploring, including critical questions regarding what it means to be or feel moved by someone or something else. These traditions of phenomenological and post-phenomenological approaches to automaticity do not subscribe to the kind of human-centred psychological ontology that Barad is in conversation with via Bohr. Postphenomenological approaches to will and intentionality also focus on forms of ‘strange causality’ that have an unexplored kinship with Barad’s queer critters or what she also calls nature’s queer performativity (see Barad, 2012, 32). They provide different starting points which recognize that psychology is not a unified discipline and that there are extensive critiques within and outside psychology, which have also challenged human-centred, individualistic notions of will and intentionality. These can be found in those traditions of psychological research historically, and in the present, which displace the borders and boundaries between the psychic and the psychological.
Clairvoyance As an example of this close yet disavowed alliance between the psychic and the psychological, I will draw on two stories from Vicki Kirby’s (2011) work that Barad uses to illuminate what she calls nature’s queer performativity. This includes the apparent ‘clairvoyance of cellular communication and lightning strokes’ (Kirby, 2011: 13), both of which are taken to be ‘possessed of some mysterious clairvoyance’ (Kirby, 2011: 9). Both stories confound ‘the logic of origins and causality’ (Kirby, 2011: 10) offering examples of entities or ‘queer critters’ that seem to be able to anticipate the future before it has arrived. This foresensing confounds notions of distance, linear time and distinctions
Open Science and Quantum Matters
147
between sender and receiver. Clairvoyance in this context is used to refer to mysterious or what Barad terms ‘paradoxical forms of communication’, which queer concepts of time, space, matter and causality. Although Barad displaces the human and human-centred cosmologies, she does not consider work in quantum physics that takes psychic phenomena as its subject. This is perhaps surprising given her interest in clairvoyance and paradoxical communication. This is important as this work has the potential to reopen important questions about experimentation and radical notions of human indeterminacy that can be found in psychology’s past and possible futures. However, at present and as I will go on to illustrate, conservative notions of subjectivity re-emerge and close down on future possibilities.23 As I hope to have shown throughout the book, the historicity and historiality of psychological phenomena also provide openings to approaches and practices, which displace cognitive human-centred cosmologies. They find inspiration from entities, processes and practices that have been largely occluded by legitimate science and straight psychology. The confounding logic of clairvoyant causality is central to the revised notions of subjectivity that can be found and which have the potential to contribute to a speculative science – to a science that is more open, inventive and adventurous (also see Savransky, 2016). It is at this nexus that we might take seriously the liveliness of mindmatter relations as well as the liveliness of matter or materiality. I have termed the complexity of mind-matter relations understood in this way through the concept of immateriality. I argue that immateriality has not been given enough attention within rethinkings of matter and materiality (see Blackman, 2012). In order to illustrate what might be at stake I want to re-move an encounter between quantum physicists and parapsychologists also interested in paradoxical forms of communication and clairvoyant causality that hovers on the edge of this controversy and which goes nowhere. In 2011 a conference was held in San Diego hosted by the American Association for the Advancement of Science. It brought together physicists and parapsychologists, including Daryl Bem, under the title ‘Quantum Physics and Parapsychology: Two views of the same thing’?24 Why despite bringing together leading figures in quantum physics with parapsychologists did the quantum physicists present on the first day and then leave the parapsychologists to largely talk among themselves on the second day as they had another physics symposium to go
148
Haunted Data
to?25 They left the parapsychologists ample material for illuminating their own interests: backward-in-time causality, time-symmetric quantum theories, retrocausal influence and weak measurement. This was described as a historic meeting, which although productive was rather one-sided where the talks by parapsychologists were largely unheard as the physicists had left by the second day, which was devoted to parapsychology. Although not wanting to endorse parapsychology, I do want to draw attention to the epistemic uncertainties and foreclosures, which circumscribe these areas and the submerged narratives and disavowed or displaced actors and agents who do not intervene and change the setting. These are all questions opened up by the controversy and which are left going nowhere – a dead end perhaps – their potentiality existing as a matter of concern but not a matter of relevance for the experimental apparatuses and events that might bring them into being.
Moralizing into the future – or can you replicate something that doesn’t exist? The main theoretical criticisms stand on the premise that ‘precognition’ does not exist and hence all evidence supporting it is surely based on flawed procedures or interpretations. It is clear that this attitude is based on a prejudiced old Newtonian ideology rather than an examination of the state of the debate. As mentioned above, serious scientists are exploring retrocausation with obvious implications for the possibility of precognition. More generally, quantum mechanics with its basis in probability theory, non-locality, and entanglement effects is so strange and counter-intuitive, that outlawing parapsychological phenomena because they contradict the so-called laws-of-physics seems somewhat premature.26
In no way wanting to end with a fixed conclusion I will lay some future trails by exploring some of the debates laid by those psi researchers who exist at the interstices of physics (particularly quantum mechanics) and the psy sciences (whether parapsychology, cognitive science or anomalistic psychology). As we will see, it is much easier and accepted within science and the mainstream science press for quantum time-travel to be entertained as a possibility, than it is for psychic precognition to be debated in a similar scientific tone.27 Although not wanting to either endorse or cultivate a sceptical position in relation to
Open Science and Quantum Matters
149
this issue, I raise this particularly as it bears on what is and is not allowed into discussions of the extraordinary and impossible, and how we evaluate the historical a prioris that are already in place and have shaped what comes to matter and be materialized as precognition. I started the book with a commitment to refracting these controversies through my own interest in affect theories. As I argued the book is an attempt to take this strategy into the realm of science and to mine, poach and exploit the potential of weird science for affect studies. This diffraction of psychology through the weird, strange, ridiculous and ludicrous is offered as a queer strategy for the playful contamination of science. On this basis, what does this controversy and its re-moving bring to affect studies and to future research directions? One of the issues that I have raised in relation to both controversies is the importance of attending to both historicity (the historical specificity of phenomena that are experienced as affective), and to historiality (those repressed or displaced narratives that open up to future directions in science and science-in-the-making). As a way of tentatively ending this chapter, I will ask a historical question about the kinds of boundaries or ‘spacetimematterings’ that are already in place in this, and the John Bargh priming controversy, which mitigate against thinking, feeling or experimenting with the impossible, improbable or extraordinary? As I hope to have shown, the proto-performative approaches to experimentation that can be found in the histories of psychic research, and which are re-moved in the present in the form of haunted data, demonstrate how impoverished our understandings of affective phenomena are. We have seen throughout this and the John Bargh priming controversy that certain boundaries are already within and articulating the parameters of both controversies and what counts as legitimate and illegitimate science. This includes distinctions drawn historically between accepted and weird science, and with what therefore is considered outside of human reason. This can be found in those experiences and phenomena, which register primarily as exceptions, oddities and anomalies to a particular ontological conception of the psychological subject. These paradoxical forms of communication, including ‘non-local causalities’ have historically been figured as abnormal perceptions, puzzles or sometimes as signs of psychopathology. Priming and precognition are both already associated with weird science, sometimes with
150
Haunted Data
‘mad scientists’ and certainly with distinctions drawn between reason and unreason, the legitimate and illegitimate, the normal and the abnormal and with the odd, the outlier and the anomalous. These are historical distinctions, which became central to the making of psychology and its boundary-making practices. As psychology professionalized as a discipline throughout the early twentieth century, it separated from sociology, philosophy and with its earlier close and intimate relationship with psychic research (see Luckhurst, 2002; Valentine, 2012). Thus these distinctions are based upon a prioris, which already specify that psychic phenomena are outside of known laws of nature and the universe, and are therefore based upon theoretical presuppositions, which are ridiculous, ludicrous, unbelievable and unscientific; as some commentators have put it they rest on ‘irrational belief systems’.28 The phenomena in question are those produced once a boundary is in place, rather than reflecting on what phenomena might become if these boundaries were not seen as given, ‘but rather as the effect of particular boundary-drawing practices’? (Barad, 2012: 33) In other words, what seismic shift in experimentation would need to happen for researchers to recognize that the entities they investigate are the effects rather than the causes of practices? This is what Barad (2012: 39) terms, ‘iterative interactivity’. This is a crucial point and one that provides an opportunity for rapprochement between the humanities and the sciences, and for the invention of creative collaborative experimentation across the arts and sciences that might open to what Derrida termed ‘archives of the future’.29 In a curious twist, which takes seriously the proto-performativity of scientific experimentation in the present, what has been deemed outside of human reason and psychology by the human sciences now forms part of contemporary imaginaries (molecular, quantum, etc.), which are shaping computational forms of artificial intelligence. This includes quantum cryptology, quantum teleportation technologies and artificial precognition based on quantum entanglement (see introduction to part 2).30 These automated practices can ‘see’ patterns that exceed human perception and consciousness, and prime and shape potential-futures through practices and processes of quantum aggregation; or so the data-driven dreams go. These are the imaginaries that do not describe what ‘we’ (that is humans) supposedly do naturally31 but rather mediate the potential non-knowing that hovers on and across the boundaries
Open Science and Quantum Matters
151
or fringes of what can and can’t be seen or known through so-called conscious practices of (human) perception and attention. The already existing boundary between the psychic and the psychological and the human and the technical, made possible by a historical distinction between particular forms of reason and unreason, produces the kinds of moralizing discourse, which researchers located at the intersection of the psi and the quantum have to contend with. At best these surface in discussions of methodology and statistics (replication, protocols etc.), but at worse they surface in attacks on Daryl Bem’s character, motivations and past-life (as a magician or putting the present down to him having had a ‘rebellious youth’32), for example. One of the issues that Bem has brought up in relation to this often aggressive and active de-bunking is that parapsychology as a field of debate is overly policed by sceptics – those who often will not entertain the possibility of the impossible. As he says about one prominent sceptic, Ray Hyman, ‘No amount of data will convince them.’33
Conspiracy theories This active de-bunking co-exists alongside those who entertain some link between quantum retrocausation and precognition, some of which ends up in the realm of conspiracy theory and very weird science. An example of this can be found in a blog written by Jim Euclid called Regolish. In a post on 13 June 2012 titled ‘The Titanic Effect of Retrocausality’ ,34 there is a lucid discussion of quantum entanglement, quantum foam and psychological retrocausality, alongside links (many now broken) to conspiracy theories referring to 9/11 being known about long before it happened (link now broken)35 – or the relationship between presidents of the United States and time-travel. There also exist links to apparent anomalous artefacts found on archaeological digs, including a 100,000-year-old plug and a Swiss watch apparently found in a 400-year-old tomb (link now broken36)! There is also a brief mention of quantum cryptology, an example of a quantum imaginary forming the basis of new ways of encrypting information.37 Conspiracy theories are the places where discussions outlawed by reason and rationality often take place, so these (broken) links are perhaps not surprising. They fuel fears and fascinations and provide a way of making political critique,
152
Haunted Data
which falls outside of established parameters.38 There are some exceptions to be found in quantum neuroscience, which provides a positivist reading and potential closing down on the questions and issues I have raised. One example includes a high-ranking article, written by a quantum physicist, Henry P. Stapp, who worked with Werner Heisenberg and is now located at the Lawrence Berkeley National Laboratory. The article is called ‘Apparent Retrocausation As a Consequence of Orthodox Quantum Mechanics Refined to Accommodate The Principle of Sufficient Reason’ and is supported by a grant from the US Department of Energy. This article is a difficult read and perhaps shows what quantum mechanics becomes when a psychology of intentionality is brought into discussions of quantum mechanics and not refracted through a materialist deconstruction and post-human account of performativity (Barad, 2007).
Quantum mind-matter relations Stapp opens the article with a discussion of Bem’s ‘feeling the future’ experiments and particularly refers to a ‘heated discussion in the New York Times’ (2011: 1) and the claims of one discussant, Douglas Hofstadter, which will be very familiar to readers by now: If any of his claims were true, then all of the bases underlying contemporary science would be toppled, and we would have to rethink everything about the nature of the universe.
Stapp attempts to refute this claim by arguing that quantum mechanics does indeed allow a theory of the nature of the universe, which would allow Bem’s experimental results to be made intelligible. Stapp’s account of quantum mechanics is rather different to Barad’s and I don’t feel qualified to evaluate those differences comprehensively. What however is obvious is that Stapp’s account of nature or matter is one that does not subscribe to ontological indeterminacy but rather to the ‘principle of sufficient reason’ (1). Rather than ascribing capriciousness to nature, one where nature operates randomly or according to chance (one version that he discusses and which I suspect Barad would replace with indeterminacy), he rather argues that there are also psychophysical processes (choice, intentionality, reason), which modulate
Open Science and Quantum Matters
153
quantum effects. In other words he brings a conservative and rather straight human-centred psychology of intentionality and will back into the picture. Stapp attempts to develop a ‘rationally coherent ontological account compatible with the standard computational rules and predictions’ (10). In other words, he suggests that nature has reasons for why it makes certain choices and these choices modulate quantum effects. Stapp’s arguments demonstrate that as in any field there are competing interpretations of quantum physics. As Barad has made clear, for example, her interpretation is one that is aided by Butler, Foucault, Derrida and other ‘co-workers’, such as Vicki Kirby. Stapp provides complicated mathematical formulas to elucidate how nature might ‘choose’ to actualize certain quantum experiences (rather than other potentialities) and that these choices would manifest in the statistical probabilities that Bem found. Rather than discuss Stapp’s theoretical edifice in detail I will focus on the conclusion to Stapp’s study which foregrounds the importance of what he calls ‘backward-in-time causal effects’. This is a very strange and mysterious form of clairvoyant causality, which he suggests explains Bem’s data. Numerous reported seemingly backward-in-time causal effects are naturally explainable within forward-in-time orthodox quantum mechanics, provided the orthodox input of pure chance is replaced by the input of sufficient reason, with two such reasons being the promotion of positive, and the suppression of negative, feelings, or their subconscious relatives generated by subliminal stimuli. (p. 22)
Stapp’s account of quantum mind-matter relations is a form of panpsychism, which imputes the qualities of intentionality (ascribed to the mind) to all levels of matter. He has developed a quantum approach to neuroscience, which suggests that mind and matter are dynamically and inextricably interdependent (in non-deterministically causal ways) and draws on discussions of so-called practices of ‘self-directed neuroplasticity’ (mindfulness, meditation, cognitive reattribution) to make his arguments (see Schwartz, Stapp and Beauregard, 2004: 2). He also draws on process philosophy (James, Whitehead) to advance his thesis. In this account post-human performativity is replaced with active observing agents or participants, reducing intra-action to the choices experimenters set on the parameters of experiments. As Schwartz et al., argue, ‘How one acts on a system would, in general, be expected to affect it’ (7).
154
Haunted Data
Agency is not distributed but rather imputed to human decisions or choices, and to a psychology of intentionality underpinned by a particular interpretation of quantum mechanics. ‘Quantum theory thereby converts science’s concept of us from that of a mechanical automaton, whose conscious choices are mere cogs in a gigantic mechanical machine, to that of agents whose conscious free choices affect the physically described world in a way specified by the theory’ (8). This is intentionality writ large and positions the human knower as masterful vis-a-vis the quantum systems they are interacting (rather than intra-acting) with. Quantum processes are imputed ‘within’ (to the brain, to neurotransmitters, ion cells, etc.) in an attempt to displace what are considered mechanistic accounts of neurophysiology (mind as an epi-phenomenon of brain processes, for example). There is a commitment to the evolution of scientific progress and knowledge, which is based upon a particular psychology of attention and will. This is human-centred and based upon particular ontological assumptions that reify the consciously, choosing subject as the pinnacle of human reason. Stapp’s approach is characteristic of a quantum approach to neuroscience that can be found in consciousness studies and which foregrounds ‘backwardin-time causal effects’. Another example of this trend within quantum biology and neuroscience can be found in an article published in the journal Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, ‘How Quantum Brain Biology can Rescue Conscious Free Will’ (Hamerof, 2012). This article also debates what is cast as the worrying determinism of quantum mechanics, which displaces free will and human-centred control and agency. This is one of the key distinctions between Barad’s post-human approach to quantum mechanics, and the radical implications of indeterminacy when a post-human approach is foregrounded. Hamerof ’s approach is an attempt to rescue free will and agency through an incorporation of the possibilities of ‘backwards-in-time causalities’ to human consciousness. Hamerof discusses the capacity of human consciousness or what is cast as conscious choice to affect experimental events and matter more generally. Attention is turned to molecular processes in brains, which disclose quantum properties (including cytoskeletal microtubules) to foreground a quantum consciousness where mind-matter relations are considered dynamic, non-local and non-causal.
Open Science and Quantum Matters
155
Within this account there is considered no before and after (mind does not react after brain action). This is known as the ‘half-second delay’ within neuroscience associated with the reaction-time experiments of the neuroscientist Benajamin Libet. Libet’s experiments have been brought into cultural theory and have also been used to authenticate what has come to be known within affect theory as the ‘half-second delay’ between thinking and action (Thrift, 2007). As Leys (2011: p. 454) argues, these experiments have been taken up across the humanities ‘to prove’ that ‘material processes of the body-brain generate our thoughts and that conscious thought, or intention’, arrives after a ‘half-second delay’. Libet and his associates published the article based on their studies in the journal Brain. They assumed that they were exploring ‘free will’ or intentionality by measuring what is known as ‘readiness potential’ (RP). They used electrodes that were placed on six college students scalps and asked the students to engage in certain tasks and activities, or to elicit voluntary actions in a spontaneous manner; spontaneity was of course circumscribed by the parameters of the experiment. They were able to measure the activation of brain processes several 100 milliseconds before the experimental subjects performed particular acts, and on this basis they concluded that ‘the brain decides to act before awareness of, or conscious initiation of that act’ (Rose and Abi-Rached, 2013). This has become known as the half-second delay between thought and action, or even cognition and affect. This a priori, which as Rose and AbiRached (2013) argue is ‘alive and well’ (p. 211) in the neurosciences has also taken on an after-life within affect theory. It is a statement that disperses a variety of approaches across a range of disciplines to explore the potential of affect to open up the question of what counts as a body; and how matter might be figured as lively, processual and indeterminate. As Celia Lury (2015) suggests in Affective Methodologies: Developing Cultural Research Strategies for the Study of Affect, edited by Britta Timm Knudsen and Carsten Stage, there is neuroscientific support in Libet’s studies, for example, for the autonomy of affect and for what she calls following Patricia Clough, the realm of the infraempirical. Nikolas Rose and Abi-Rached in their recent book, Neuro: The New Brain Sciences and the Management of Mind refer to the positivity of Libet’s studies and of this statement as a form of Libetism (with the ism perhaps suggesting
156
Haunted Data
some kind of distinctive practice, style, system of beliefs, collective ideology, movement, affliction or even dogma). As a form of historiality Hamerof ’s account is of interest. Rather than accept this statement as a truism, he rather links Libet’s reaction-time experiments to evidence of ‘temporal non-locality’ (Hamerof, 2012: 2). This is described as a backwards and forwards in time, which undoes any sense of duality. Libet’s theories of reaction time are reread through non-local retrocausality and he argues that the supposed halfsecond delay between stimulus and response is non-quantum and reliant on a classic theory of physics.39 Hamerof displaces this account through quantum physics, which he suggests introduces non-local knowing into molecular mind-matter relations. Mind-matter relations are topological spaces that are looped, nonlinear, which switch, encounter and make choices in ways, which the author suggests undoes any notion that consciousness comes ‘too late’ or after brain activation. Discussing the much-cited experiments on readiness potential (RP) by Libet, Hamerof makes the following claim: Libet et al. used a rapidly moving clock and asked subjects to note when on the clock they consciously decided to move their finger. This conscious decision came 200ms before actual finger movement, hundreds of milliseconds after onset of the RP. Libet and many authorities concluded that the RP represented non-conscious determination of movement, that many seemingly conscious actions are actually initiated by nonconscious processes, and that conscious intent was an illusion. Consciousness apparently comes too late. However, … temporal non-locality enabling backward time referral of (quantum) information from the moment of conscious intent can account for necessary RP preparation.
Hamerof (2012) argues that Libet’s own interpretations of RP drew on notions of time-travel or backwards-in-time assertions to explain his results. This interpretation was based on a rather queer ontology of time, one where time is bent, distorted, telescoped, goes sideways and back to front, and where the psychic dimensions of timespacematter are reconfigured. Libet argued, in short, that it was not that the brain reacts before conscious experience or interpretation; rather, the future can retroactively influence the past. He did not subscribe to a temporal delay between brain and cognition, non-conscious and consciousness, affect and will (with both the temporal delay’s potential for indeterminacy but also the linear ordering of relations between brain/cognition,
Open Science and Quantum Matters
157
non-conscious/conscious, affect/intentionality and even past/present, before/ after, then/now etc). Libet suggested that the evidence for different temporalities could not be understood in such dichotomous terms. There were conflicts in his experiments which did not so easily map onto the half-second delay, or at least to how this might be understood. Rather, he suggested more of a loopingeffect where on a moment-to-moment basis we automatically transport our own consciousness backwards through time to experience something before we could scientifically ever have actually experienced it! In other words mind, whatever mind is, has an anti-dating capacity or is ahead of brain, or mind has time-travelling tendencies. Or to quote a contemporary neuroscientist, So the first set of findings 1 to 3 are to the effect that neural adequacy for any sensory experience is achieved only after a certain delay, about half a second. But finding 4, which is indeed crucial, appears to conflict with this. That is, the ordering by subjects of their experiences is this: conscious-exper ience-owed-to-later-skin-stimulus came before conscious-experience-owed -to-earlier-cortical-stimulus. (Honderich, 2005: 75)
These quantum interpretations are far more curious and certainly queer the science that is often drawn upon by many affect theorists to endorse their philosophical approaches (see Papoulias and Callard, 2010). These interpretations were however ridiculed by Libet’s interlocuters resulting in their mutation into a much more palatable statement that will be familiar to affect scholars – that non-conscious processes act prior to and are separate from conscious attention. Hamerof provides three lines of evidence, which he suggests refute the ‘half second delay’ and which return the vexed question of consciousness to quantum mechanics: Can (human) consciousness affect quantum matters? Can quantum backward referral happen in the brain? (p. 11). As well as Libet’s disavowed time-travelling explanations of reaction-potential, Hamerof also cites quantum delayed choice experiments,40 and Bem’s feeling the future experiments on precognition, to argue that conscious choice can affect ‘behaviour of previously measured, but unobserved, events’ (p. 9). These discussions of retrocausality and the proposed evidence to support his claims for backwards-in-time effects take the discussion into process philosophy and a discussion of Whitehead’s (1929) concept of ‘actual occasions’ or occasions of experience.41
158
Haunted Data
There are many competing interpretations of Whitehead’s philosophy, which underpin debates within and across object-oriented ontologies and speculative philosophies within the humanities (Bogust, Harman, Shaviro).42 This controversy opens up leads to these debates and to the question of what it might and could mean to ‘think with’ Whitehead (see Stengers, 2014; Savransky, 2016, 2018). This is an opening to future possibilities that this controversy re-moves and I will leave this as a detour that others might want to follow. In this last section I finish by showing how the distinction between the theoretical and the empirical, which was central to early psychic research has been lost in these perspectives. Rather than approach43 psychic phenomena as mere designations and the experimental practices as practices of simulation (with their proto-performative orientation) we see attempts to explain psychic phenomena through specific quantum explanations of mind-matter relations. They are based on a return to an ontology of human consciousness, and subjectivity, which is based on imputing the values of free will, autonomy, choice and reason to the question of what it means to be human. The traces of the human and technical practices, and their complex iterative histories that have shaped what takes form as retroactive causation, are displaced and replaced with a more positivist understanding. Hamerof, for example, uses Whitehead and William James’s concept of the ‘specious present’ to unify these different traditions in relation to his proposed theory of quantum consciousness, known as Orchestrated Objective Reduction (also known as Orch OR). This is a theory of the world, life, the human and everything and arguably closes down on the creative and technical possibilities of experimenting with psychic and psychological indeterminacy.
Orch OR It is probably fair to say that researchers attempting to unify psi entities with quantum mechanics emphasize those aspects of process philosophy that have been refused, disqualified, ignored or disavowed within contemporary humanities theorizing. As I have argued in previous work, William James research is generally used to refer to process, relationality, the virtual and nonhuman ignoring perhaps his own interests in the problem of unity or synthesis
Open Science and Quantum Matters
159
and what he called ‘the problem of personality’ – how we live singularity in the face of multiplicity. This was also termed the problem of the ‘one and the many’ and this problematic appeared in the nineteenth and early twentieth centuries across economics, psychology, philosophy, sociology, medicine, and so on (see Blackman, 2012). It was far from settled or resolved. These issues recur in this controversy and appear in attempts to translate the quantum dynamics of photons, electrons and atoms into understandings of psychological processes that are seen to operate non-locally. Hamerof cites the example of ‘quantum delayed choice experiments’. These experiments draw on understandings of quantum entanglement, where, as Hamerof argues, ‘entanglement is a feature of quantum mechanics in which unified quantum particles are separated but remain somehow connected, even over distance. Measurement or perturbation of one separated-but-stillentangled particle instantaneously affects the other, what Einstein referred to (mockingly) as ‘spooky action at a distance’ (2012: p. 9). This principle forms the basis of experiments involving human subjects whose decisions to measure the atoms as either unified or separated is connected to the decision of a third observer and his (stet) decisions, even if these are decided after the decisions of the other two researchers have been made. This is another example given as evidence of ‘backwards in time effects’ (p. 9) and this forms the basis of the question Hamerof asks: ‘How can backward time effects be explained scientifically’? (2012). Hamerof ‘thinks with’ particular readings of William James, Whitehead, St Augustine, Buddhism and the effects of long-range gamma synchrony in the brain to argue that ‘consciousness could be identified with sequences of quantum state reductions’ (2012: p. 10). Quantum state reductions relate to those rules and principles generally accepted within quantum mechanics; this includes that objects or particles exist in or as multiple co-existing possibilities (quantum superposition) – that is they can exist in unified states that remain separated (in space) yet entangled in time. These relations in time upset linear conceptions of time, as we have seen, and raise questions about what Barad has called strange forms of causality, which confound origins and foundations. The crucial question for Hamerof is whether these relations can be seen or even produced macroscopically and the answer he suggests is a resounding yes.
160
Haunted Data
Orch OR In order to ‘scale up’ from the molecular to the molar, Hamerof turns to theories of consciousness and panpsychism and to those that explicitly bring consciousness into physics. This includes the work of Penrose (1987, 1989, 1994) who coined the theory of Orch OR, which draws on algorithmic quantum computing and spacetime geometry. This theory is given neurobiological underpinnings by turning to the brain and explanations of the actions of neurons, dendrites and proteins, which provide the possibility for non-local quantum thresholds in the transmission of information throughout the brain (or how neurons fire through non-local conductance pathways). Orch OR is a speculative philosophy and one which attempts to theorize what are taken to be the minimal conditions under which quantum explanations of matter and nature can be translated into a neuropsychology of consciousness underpinned by quantum brain biology. It is also an attempt to ‘rescue’ free will from what is anxiously considered a form of quantum determinism. This attempt closes down the potential of quantum indeterminacy, which has become a key feature of new materialisms across the humanities. However, what I want to argue is that the particular psychologies of free will and intentionality that quantum brain biology inherits are those which are based on a human-centred agential subject. Barad is right to displace this cosmology through post-human approaches to the subject. However, there remain other collaborative possibilities and starting points to be found in all those displaced, occluded and submerged narratives, practices, entities and phenomena – particularly those that assume that psychic and psychological processes are always indeterminate, relational and contiguous with the technical, symbolic, historical, material and immaterial. These return in both the priming and Feeling the Future controversies and show the importance of pursuing the historicity of matter and the matter of historicity in the field of affect studies, new materialisms, speculative philosophies and the biohumanities.
Conclusion: The materiality of history and the historicity of materiality As we have seen throughout the book, although the concepts of time-travel, precognition, or even quantum retrocausation are weird or strange propositions,
Open Science and Quantum Matters
161
in different ways they have formed the basis of regimes of speculative forecast and anticipation that are part of the defining logic of computational cultures as they operate within a capitalistic mode. The Feeling the Future controversy connects to a dispersed set of surfaces of emergence, where the quantum scale of matter, is enacted, imagined, performed and materialized in order to shape possible futures. The controversy is part of a broader set of cultural imaginaries and discourses, which are entangled with new strategies of power based on future shaping and anticipation, which attempt to govern through nonlinear and distributed psychologies of time. This includes techniques and practices of pre-emption, foresight, foreseeing and premediation. These techniques are ‘strategic imaginaries’, which are manifest and becoming instantiated in computing (particularly programming and software development), biology and the neurosciences, practices of mediation within the context of communication technologies, business strategy, finance capitalism, and in the conduct of war, terrorism, politics and public health responses to global threats. They underpin and are shaping the development of future technologies, some of which are based on quantum mechanics and theories of quantum entanglement. Examples of these include quantum teleportation and quantum cryptology, and algorithms, which attempt to change the past within open systems, sometimes called programming in the subjunctive (including retroactive update). These are algorithms, which attempt to change computational pasts and are therefore seen to step sideways in time. The collision of these different imaginaries, theories, propositions, forms of experimentation and statistical imaginaries shape some of the displaced narratives, actors, agencies, spectres, entities and puzzling anomalies that are re-moved within this book. As Rheinberger has cogently argued, scientific controversies although considered settled at particular times carry the potential to be re-moved at different historical moments – what he terms the historical movement of a trace. As he has also argued, science always contains more stories than can be told at any one time. His concept of historiality returns the storytelling to science and provides a cogent argument as to why our engagements with science need to be qualified through the important question of how we understand and analyse the different philosophies, histories and technical materialities of experimentation at play. Post-publication-peerreview becomes an interesting source of data in order to speculatively explore
162
Haunted Data
these potentialities. What I hope to have shown throughout the book, and particularly in this and the last two chapters, is how a genealogical approach to data refracted through hauntology provides an interesting lens through which to engage with contemporary science as it intersects with the contemporary field of affect studies. Within the context of the Feeling the Future controversy what comes into view are radically different approaches, philosophies and multiple genealogies of experimentation – positivist (orch OR), proto-performative (including simulation and experimenting with the counterfactual), empirical and nontheoretical (what we might call a version of post-truth politics and practices). I hope that this novel approach to data adds weight to arguments made by sociologists and cultural theorists who are urging humanities scholars to open up to some of the problems as well as possibilities with engaging with science. The two controversies analysed in the book return us to four key areas of vexed discussion: how to analyse what it means to be human within the context of data and computation; how to understand and analyse mind-matter relations; and how to understand particularly the ontological definition of bodies as being defined by their capacities to affect and be affected. And lastly, what kinds of approaches to science, scientific data and experimentation can be shaped at the intersection of the sciences and humanities? Callard and Fitzgerald (2015) use the term ‘ebullience’ to describe a mode of interdisciplinary engagement within the context of the neurosciences, which tends towards an acceptance or assumption by humanities and social scientists of ‘experimental results and theoretical statements from the neurosciences as more-or-less true – with little contest or context, and in the absence of a sense of the wider, often fierce, epistemological and ontological debates within those sciences’ (p. 11). Thus before we endorse any account of affect with scientific theories, concepts and evidence we would be wise to develop a more critical, creative and speculative approach to different and sometimes hidden philosophies of experimentation and statistics; to the social technologies and devices of experimental apparatuses; and to the different psychological and psychic imaginaries that are assumed, shaped and bear the marks of particular technological and material histories. Scientific data is a very curious entity and its conditions of existence, possibility and emergence require the very kinds of critical ‘reading against
Open Science and Quantum Matters
163
the grain’ as well as reparative readings that we have developed so persuasively within the humanities, feminism and queer theories. As Hannah Landecker (2016) has argued in relation to antibiotic resistance, and the concept of bacteria when conventionally understood as an ontologically distinct entity, we need work that can be attentive to the materiality of history as well as the historicity of matter. Landecker’s writing skilfully uses historical archives and puts them to work by re-moving or putting them back into circulation in the present. As she cogently shows the historical records, which trace the circulation of antibiotics into what she calls an ‘industrialized biological’, disclose how its history has become inscribed into the biology of bacteria itself. Revealing that history and biology are thoroughly entangled and matter to each other in ways that have serious consequences for antibiotic and human futures, she develops the concept of the ‘biology of history’, to demonstrate how ‘human historical events and processes have materialized as biological events and processes and ecologies’ (p. 21). Biology not only matters in this example, but what comes to be recognized and produced as biological data already bears the material traces of human and technological histories that cannot be divorced or separated from what takes form. As she suggests, ‘The bacteria of today are not the bacteria of yesterday, whether that change is registered culturally, genetically, physiologically, ecologically or medically’ (p. 21). This raises important questions about what exactly counts as biological data given the historicity of matter and the materiality of history and suggests that the important question of antibiotic resistance might be shifted away from ‘bacteria’ (understood as a ontologically distinct entity) to a new more relational ontological register. In a similar vein I am arguing that we are still in urgent need of developing accounts of the psychic and the psychological (or mind-matter relations more broadly) that are attentive to the historicity of matter and the materiality of history. What the Feeling the Future and John Bargh Priming controversies show us is how impoverished our understandings of psychic and psychological indeterminacy are. What takes form as phenomena, including precognition, pre-emptive arousal, priming, clairvoyance, telepathy, retroactive causation and so forth, already bear the traces of human and technological histories, many of which have become displaced, occluded, erased, disqualified and forgotten.
164
Haunted Data
This is perhaps a good place to temporarily end or draw a cut in this controversy, as it brings contemporary debates in the humanities into the scene and offers a bridge to some fundamental re-workings of what might be at stake. I have followed those data-trails, some of which appear marginal, insufficient, sometimes through detours and back tracking, operating sometimes below the radar or through the intensification of affective dynamics on display. These are all what I have termed ‘haunted data’, which has entered into the scenes of entanglement staged throughout the book. I started with an initial feeling that both controversies would and should be of interest to humanities scholars and that there was more in both controversies than meets the eye. This has been borne out. What I hope to have demonstrated is just how rich engaging with science controversies within mediatized environments and across digital platforms can be. This engagement has been shaped by approaches to the philosophies of science, which have explored the close interdependence and interrelationship between cultural configurations of matter-meaning and the materialities of scientific cultures. For those scholars interested in affect and the challenges of computational cultures for studies of media and mediation, what has been opened up is a complicated set of historical relations and conditions of possibility, which shape the parameters of what can and can’t be said. What can be seen and not seen, what can be articulated and what remains in-articulable, but can sometimes be felt, managed and produced. In other words, the hauntological dimensions of both controversies and the sense that there is something more to be said have provided the affective draw and resonance for the thoughtspace opened up in this book. Both controversies stage timely questions about science, computation and data analytics, mediation, power, truth, experimentation, statistical imaginaries, networked affect, subjectivities, ontologies, and what kinds of alliances we might and could enact across the sciences and humanities. They respond to current discussions in relation to networked affect and how to understand media contagions. They also speak to current theories of anticipation and affect, to the capacity to feel the future, or to be moved by someone or something else – what I have described throughout the book as ‘alien phenomenologies’. One central issue raised by the book is that the shaping and emergence of more speculative psychologies and philosophies
Open Science and Quantum Matters
165
within science will only come about through collaboration with scientists, artists and humanities scholars who can ‘think together’ as part of a collective enterprise. I argue that this will only happen effectively if one is attentive to both science and data’s potential for historicity and historiality. This more speculative approach aims to return the storytelling to science, and recognizes the capacity of digital archives and post-publication-peer-review to re-move the excess to what is currently told. I will return to these issues in the short conclusion to the book and open up to what I think are some of the interesting leads and directions for research that this hauntology feeds-forward.
7
Conclusion: Affect and Archives of the Future Hauntological media This book has developed a hauntological approach to data and computation, which suggests that data bear the traces of human, material, technical, symbolic and imaginary histories that are often displaced and occluded in data metrics. The hauntological analysis developed in this book, with its particular focus on science and computational culture, explores how both science and computational culture are haunted by the histories and excesses of their own storytelling. In the context of the book these excesses have surfaced in ‘queer aggregations’ or haunted data to be mined, poached and put to work in newly emergent contexts and settings.1 The book points to the propensity of straight or legitimate science to sanitize, excise or even exorcise narratives, actors, agents and entities, which ‘contaminate’ it with queerness. I have provided a host or home for these queer aggregations and allowed them to commune with displaced and submerged actors, entities, processes and practices. Starting with two high impact articles and the post-publication-peer-review associated with the articles, I have followed data-trails across time and space, the visible and not-quite-visible, the legitimate and the weird, the accepted and archives of the future, in order to open up some critical questions. These questions speak to current concerns within affect studies and approaches to mediation, truth and post-truth across media and cultural studies. Before I move to some conclusions to feed-forward in future research I will situate the hauntological approach to data within the context of hauntology as it has been developed within the context of other archives and media forms. This will make a further argument as to why hauntological approaches to science and computational culture can be so productive, and particularly for those experiences, phenomena and entities, which are considered weird, odd, strange, freakish and aberrant.
Conclusion
167
As we saw in Chapter 2, hauntology is often associated with Jacques Derrida (1994) and particularly his meditation on the fate of Marxism following the so-called fall of communism at the end of the 1990s across Europe, which was articulated within a ‘discourse of the end’. This was a melancholic moment for many left academics who lamented such a fall. They wondered at the costs of this supposed ending to critical (Marxist) thinking. The refrain that is central to Derrida’s reflections is the feeling of time being out of joint – what he terms a ‘disjointed now’ (p. 1). Derrida sets out to explore this ghostly and melancholic feeling and to conjure and summon the spectre of Marxism in the present by engaging with various ghostings – the ghosts within Marx’s writing itself, as well as the way in which fictional writing (Shakespeare’s Hamlet, for example) has staged apparitions and called forth, interpreted and interrogated ghostings. Derrida’s main site of reflection is the spectral quality of language itself, which Derrida mines to perform the different temporalities that language can evoke. The radical untimeliness of language, Derrida suggests, performs the myriad of ways in which a feeling of radically disjointed time can be conjured. This, he suggests, can create the sense of ‘being-with spectres’ (xvii). This might include time being ‘off its hinges, time is of course, beside itself, disadjusted’ (p. 20) – in other words, ‘does not walk straight, or goes askew’ (p. 23). As well as ghosts or revenants always returning and coming back, language itself is structured, Derrida suggests, by relationships of condensation, displacement, metonym and metaphor, which perform temporalities ‘beyond the living present’ (p. xix). Derrida’s writing itself is akin to a magical incantation, which calls forth the spectral qualities of language to question what it might mean to live and to learn to live with ghosts. This question is aligned to a ‘politics of memory, of inheritance, and of generations’ (1994: xvii) – to those he goes on to suggest are not present: ‘Those who are not yet born or who are already dead’ (p. xvii). They might also exist as an absent-presence.
Haunted lives Rather than the concept of media life that has become so central to discussions of contemporary media forms and practices, this work extends what counts
168
Haunted Data
as media in the context of haunted lives. As an example of this distinction, David Marriot (2007) explores the work media do in the context of the endless repetition and consumption of colonial fantasies within media cultures. The book starts with video images of the murdered black British schoolboy Damiola Taylor, and it invites the reader to consider what we were being invited to recognize as these images were endlessly repeated and circulated within news broadcasts in November 2000. Marriot equates the work these images do to a form of ‘nonvision’ (p. xiv), where he suggests these images are haunted by the ‘future-to-come’ – what we already know – which is that they remain the last images of the boy before his brutal murder. He suggests that images do not work indexically, but rather they are anticipatory, enacting a ‘future memorialized, and yet unseen’ (2007: xv). The theme of Marriot’s book and his analysis of visual culture and black modernity is ‘what cannot appear, by what is missing’ (2007: xvi) – that is how TV, photographic images and other media can create a form of mediated perception which allows one to see and experience the un-seeable. Marriot’s focus is on the relationship between the spectral and visible and what he calls the ‘occult presence of racial slavery’ (2007: xxi) – ‘the way in which colonial fantasies create a form of ‘ghosted consciousness’ (2007: xxi). He uses this term to refer to the histories of oppression and trauma created by slavery, which are displaced by what comes to stand in for this disappearance – the endlessly repeated colonial fantasies which produce the colonial subject as an ambivalent object of both fear and desire (BhaBha, 2004; Fanon, 1967). He suggests that this is a ‘blinded mediatised seeing’ (2007: 2), which he equates to the ‘telepathic life of race’ (2007: 4). Marriot’s hauntological approach to the politics of racial disavowal creates what he calls a ‘shared hallucination’ (p. 8), which constitutes particular ways of ‘seeing’ blackness. In this sense, like Derrida (1994), Marriot views television as a haunted technological medium, extending the reciprocal and recursive relationships between the haunted imaginary of media and locating this within the context of racial discrimination and colonial racism. Through an analysis of film, television, modernist literature and the practices of black gay artists, such as Issac Julien, he develops the argument that media technologies are haunted by the phantom of racial slavery. Where Marriot suggests that the temporal, or the now, must be grasped in and through the time of racist historicity, Rey Chow (2012) asks the important
Conclusion
169
question of what happens to time as it is experienced and enacted within what she terms transmediality. Referring to the entangled relations of the web, and the ‘rapid disappearance of time-honoured intervals’ (Chow 2012: 10) her focus is on how hypermediatization and the ‘new regime of abstract touch – the pinch, the click, the tap, the slide, and the finger swipe’ (Chow 2012: 25) have created a sense of illusionism and automatism. This is perhaps a familiar account of media time, which focuses on immediacy where everything appears connected yet experienced as temporal – as part of a perpetual present. As she asks, ‘What happens to memory when images, in which past events are supposedly recorded and preserved, become instantaneous with the actual happenings?2 When conventional time shifts vanish as a result of the perfecting of the techniques of image capturing? When time loses its potential to become fugitive or fossilized – in brief, to become anachronistic?’ (Chow 2012: 5). The entanglement characteristic of transmediality is understood through images such as traps, knots, masks, mazes, parasites and shadows, creating a form of ‘topological looping’ (Chow 2012: 1). Her question, which is also situated within colonial relations, is to ask how does one engage in critical practice and the potential to think otherwise ‘when a particular form is involved?’ (Chow 2012: 23; also see Rotman, 2008). This question is also central to the hauntological method developed in this book and the question of how digital and software media are changing the nature of science controversies and their potential to accrue contagious potential. Chow’s focus is on the radical potential of what she terms ‘shadow media’ in the context of the proliferating ‘visibilities-in-flux, made up of user-generated, often mediocre-quality images’ (Chow 2012: 166). She suggests that this creates flows, criss-crossings and multiple becomings that provide ways of enacting and performing the present through ‘the small act of capture’ (Chow 2012: 166). She suggests that shadow media – the portable, atomized and individualized media that extend capacities for connectivity and interactivity – allow for the potentially wide dissemination of small moments. This potentially releases the image from the memorial of a time past and puts into motion new possibilities. This is equated to a move from captivity to captivation. Chow’s work is useful to think about how ‘information-objects’ (Chow 2012: 165) can become stagings or scenes of entanglement, scenes where new linkages and enmeshments might accrue ‘supplemental time-space’ (Chow
170
Haunted Data
2012: 12). This might enable a form of critical reflexivity, which is dramatized within a particular media form. This draws attention to the apparatus itself, but does not reduce the capacity to captivate to the media technology itself, nor to its use. Rather the critical reflexivity is shaped through the specific entanglements that might be staged that include multiple temporalities that overlap, cross, loop and create the event of capture. To that extent Chow’s focus is on the unhinging of time and its perpetual looping that she argues characterizes transmediality and opens up the potential for new forms of critical reflexivity. Drawing on Deleuze’s rereading of Foucault’s approach to disciplinary power, captured in Deleuze’s concept of the ‘control society’ (1992), she suggests that there has been a breakage within so-called old and new media, or medial and transmediality. This is represented by new forms of time shifting, or the collapse of what she terms a time lag. Chow suggests that this is leading to new battles for the media frame. All of these approaches focus on media as scenes of entanglement understood as performative re-movals or compositional forms of ‘seeing’ which move beyond representationalism.3 That is they stage entangled relationships (here Chow refers to strategies such as collage, montage and tableau, for example), which have the potential to open up or contribute to existing ‘epistemic foreclosures’ (2012: 22). Chow’s concept of scenes of entanglement is also captured by a number of other medial concepts that include ‘situation, dramatization, staging, picture, frame (and) window’ (ibid: 12). The supplementation of the concept of the cut or the ‘critical aperture’ (ibid: 12) with the concept of ‘supplemental time-space’ (ibid: 12) also introduces different conceptions of media time into the analysis. As we have seen in the discussion in this section, these media times can be ghostly but also introduce a politics to ghosting, which situates medial or transmedial forms of haunting within specific racist and colonial imaginaries. Hauntological analysis therefore extends across the arts and humanities, has a particular place in philosophy and has entered into discussions of media, such as film, photography and television. Hauntology has a particular place in the lives of oppressed and marginalized peoples and those suffering from traumatic memories that blur the historical and the personal and the past and present (also see Walkerdine and Jimenez, 2012). Avery Gordon (2008) suggests that hauntological analysis is a way of focusing on how people sense,
Conclusion
171
intuit and experience the complexities of modern power. It focuses on ‘what is usually invisible or neglected or thought by most to be dead or gone’ (Gordon 2008: 194). There is a sense that ‘we’ already know, in some form or way, what haunts as a ghostly presence, but that the paradigms we have for animating these ghosts operate at the limits of what is taken to be understandable. Gordon suggests that as researchers, readers, citizens and persons we are implicated in particular hauntings, but that we might need companions in thought, such as novels, photographs and other media to help articulate our concerns (also see Ahmed, 2014). Hauntologies are also histories of the present – of how particular presents have materialized in relation to specific and contingent normative horizons and what is relegated, excluded, disavowed or consigned to pathology. In that sense, hauntologies have a relationship to work which has explored how social norms become social goods or truths and the social figures who carry what is excluded from such norms – the feminist killjoy, the melancholic migrant, the unhappy queer and so forth (Ahmed, 2010). However, the aforementioned figures might be said to have a hyper-visibility in that they can be found, tracked, analysed and allowed to speak through the animation of particular archives of wilfulness perhaps (see Ahmed, 2014). The ghost might be said to be a different kind of social figure, which is both ‘there and not there, past and present, force and shape’ (Gordon, 2008: 6). It takes form and crops up in places, relations and shapes, which exert agency or an affective force without obvious definition. The ghost requires a host, someone, something or perhaps a controversy, which allows it to surface and demand our attention. Haunting can impose and then retreat; it can leave traces of its visit and invite us to take notice and ‘imagine beyond the limits of what is already understandable’ (Gordon 2008: 195).
Media time(s) and haunted socialities The focus of this book has been on the forms of extended perception and mediation made possible by social and digital media, and with the way in which scientific controversies are increasingly performed within digital environments. It has staged new directions and explored how the archive of science and its proper object is extended, contested and re-animated by the
172
Haunted Data
re-moval of science’s pasts and possible futures within an archive of haunted data. These new media environments disclose the dynamism of controversies and allow for the mapping and representation of the complexity of science, which is usually covered over by positivist forms of science writing. As Venturini (2010) has argued, positivist forms of writing and argumentation often present disagreements as technical points – problems with replication, the experimental set-up, statistical analysis and interpretation – but rarely as ‘conflicting visions of the world’ (p. 808). In this sense the controversies have investments in the shaping of particular visions of the future and in that sense are timely expressions of a broader cultural imaginary where science is being repositioned as renewing interest in possible futures-yet-to-come. They are all also examples of what, following Avery Gordon (2008), might be termed ‘haunted socialities’. The concept of haunted socialities points towards the different times that are present within what I term ‘transmedial time’. Within the controversies that I perform and animate within the book, different temporalities and space– time configurations collide and clash. The linear development of science as a narrative of progress towards truth is radically disrupted. The idea that media time is pure duration – movement, change and transformation – until stabilization cuts into temporal flow does not quite work. Instead, what we witness are different times and temporalities moving in and out of each other, sparking moments of discord, conflict, tension, anxiety, fear, fascination and inchoate feelings. The affective dimension and the hauntological potential of both controversies gesture towards half-hidden things, people, entities and agents. They unsettle and are unsettled. Although beyond the scope of this concluding chapter to include in any detail, there are many different examples and practices of hauntology across the arts and humanities. This includes the seminal work of the black British artist and filmmaker John Akomfrah who has used the term ‘hauntology’ to describe his own films and aesthetic. Akomfrah’s practice combines archival footage (often from sanctioned media archives such as those of the BBC including documentary TV and radio), with imaginative re-stagings and enactments of the gaps, silences and absences in such footage. These require the action of myth, fiction and fantasy as companions in thought. In a commentary in relation to a 2012 art installation of Akomfrah’s work, which mobilized
Conclusion
173
hauntology as its title, the author acknowledges Derrida’s use of such a term and its reference to those multiple pasts in the present, which remain as absentpresences4 – what is termed the ‘many pasts present in the present’. Akomfrah’s subject is the colonial histories of Jamaican and West Indian migrants to the United Kingdom and a staging of those stories erased by official histories and forms of mediated perception. Akomfrah’s film of Stuart Hall,5 the late and great black British scholar and public intellectual, whose story was told through the conjoining of archival footage and interviews, punctuated by the music of Miles Davies, shows just how effective and important hauntology as a political and artistic method can be. Hauntological approaches to the many pasts in the present are also carried by the figure of the ghost as a sociocultural and political phenomenon. This assumption is part of the curation of an ongoing interdisciplinary arts project led by the British artist Sarah Sparkes. GHost Hostings6 was initiated in 2008 and has to date curated fourteen events, bringing together artists, performers, dancers, academics, filmmakers and other kinds of ghost-hunters to conceptually interrogate and manifest the idea of the ghost. GHost takes its title from Marcel Duchamp’s (1953) aphorism and artwork, ‘A GUEST + a HOST = A GHOST’.7 As Sparkes argues, the guest is a host inside the ghost, which points towards the success and effectivity of the experimental apparatus to stage or summon ghosts for interrogation. In March 2014 GHost Hostings 14 staged three performance pieces, which all used sound, embodied performance and mediated images in order to conjure the experience of displacement through forced migration and traumatic histories. This included the work of Stasis 738 who animated found archival images and testimonies of people forced to migrate, sometimes by foot, or train or boat, to another place, leaving behind abandoned buildings, homes and villages, personal items, lives and the dead. Through a form of mediated perception they attempt to stage the unspoken trauma of such displacement through image and sound, in order to raise the ghosts and re-animate what they term the dead camera-eye, which captures such images frozen in time. They return perhaps captivation to captivity. In my development of an affective methodology that brings hauntological analysis into studies of data and computation, my focus was on returning storytelling to science or what Rheinberger termed historiality. I have explored
174
Haunted Data
this in the context of new forms of scientific data that blur fact and fiction, self and other, human and technical and the popular and the scientific. The data that are generated within post-publication-peer-review have provided the archive within which I have worked and have allowed a productive and I hope inventive way of attending to a statement’s liveliness. One of the conclusions I want to advance is that if we are to work and collaborate with science and scientists as humanities scholars and artists, it is important to recognize how contested science is. Papoulias and Callard (2010) have argued that not enough attention has been paid by affect theorists to the circuits of debate, legitimation, contestation and authorization that circumscribe scientific practices, theories and data. In other contexts humanities scholars such as Elizabeth Wilson (2015) argue that we, and particularly feminist scholars, should take scientific data more seriously. This book is an attempt to do both but to explore an alternative realm of scientific data production that for some scientists has the potential to shape a more open, democratic and participatory science. For others it is a source of anxiety and consternation potentially threatening the integrity of science and opening it up to the wrong kinds of public. As with other scholars who have crossed the sciences and humanities, I have been influenced by arguments that suggest there is always more to be said, that science always contains more stories waiting to be told, and that the paths to a more inventive, creative and curious science are to be found at the interstices of all those gaps, cracks, loops, dead ends, fracture-lines, detours and anomalies, which can be found in contemporary science controversies. As we have seen, the science controversies analysed in this book are more-than-one and splice together and re-move earlier historical controversies once thought resolved. As they move and migrate into new contexts they accrue agencies and enter into new scenes of entanglement. I have provided a temporary host for these voices, apparitions and ghosts who I believe have something very important to say to some of the current ontologies across the humanities and the sciences, which are questioning what makes us human. They foreground process, indeterminacy, relationality and creative experimentation in the context of the field of affect studies, and to those phenomena which displace the rational, self-enclosed, bounded, distinctly human subject. As a conclusion to this book I will outline seven areas germane to the book, which I hope will be useful to affect theorists, media scholars and to all those
Conclusion
175
interested in philosophies of science, and in the relations between science and storytelling, and to the possibility of more speculative sciences. I hope that these seven points will provide a stimulus for readers and researchers to imagine future possibilities in their own research, and to help open science up to new publics and the proliferation of new visibilities, new entities and new forms of experimentation, authorization and data analysis.
Seven points to feed-forward. Speculative manifesto for affect and the more-than-human: Loving the alien 1. Open Science: Open science should not just be confined to a discussion of how digital communication is transforming the communication of science and therefore how science engages in practices of verification, legitimation and authentication. The challenges of digital and social media to science cannot be contained by discussions of where, how and what to publish, nor with what kinds of practices pre- and post-publication might modify what was once considered the static, unmodifiable published journal article. These of course are important questions, which relate to open access publishing, the nature of peer review, reputational economies, citation metrics, and the way science might and is transforming within the context of the public communication of science and what it means to engage and publish research that will attract impact. Important as all these issues and questions are open science will need to confront bigger ontological questions about the nature of scientific progress, innovation, evolution and transformation. Science and scientists need to become more adventurous. Scientists need to confront the nature of scientific archives as they are and might be transformed across time. What might be considered the ‘proper object’ of any scientific archive is challenged by the kinds of transmedial times (nonlinear, criss-crossed, looped, back-tracked, telescoped, bent etc.), which are re-moved by contemporary science controversies as they form and are re-formed within and across digital media worlds. Derrida, Rheinberger, Barad, Haraway, Despret, Stengers and many others have foregrounded the historicity of times and the historiality (Rhineberg) of science with its potential for dynamism and creative indeterminacy. Scientists and philosophers of
176
Haunted Data
the nineteenth century knew this and what we have forgotten comes back insistently and often in submerged and disguised forms in the present opening to lost-futures. Open science needs the humanities and philosophy. That is why many of those who in my view write the most interesting work about the history, philosophy and practice of science have already crossed disciplines from science to the humanities and philosophy and sometimes back again. These crossings and borrowings, and their potential to open to archives of the future must not be forgotten in the contemporary calls for more rapprochement with the sciences and particularly the cognitive, biological, psychological and neurosciences. 2. We can still do critical research within the context of the data and computational turn. There is no ‘end of theory’ nor should we resign ourselves to inertia, resignation and defeat in the context of the softwaredriven and increasingly automated media worlds that we live within and through. These worlds often perform rather impoverished imaginaries that remove wonder from the world and enact some of the most unimaginative and restrictive psychological imaginaries about what it means to relate, connect, influence, know, forget, anticipate and feel about one another. Although we might scrape and visualize some of the data produced through these imaginaries, showing a keen awareness of the affordances of the application programming interfaces that shape the data, this must not exhaust the other critical openings that software and computational practices make possible. Many of the theories adapted to speak to these new automated media worlds draw from social psychological theories (Goffman for example), or overlay discussions of networked performativity with unexamined psychological assumptions about the nature of self, creativity and authenticity for example. There are other theories and practices we might use which served marginalized communities well in critical analyses of analogue and broadcast-media worlds. This includes the queer practice of ‘reading against the grain’ and attention to the hauntological forces at work in media; what I have termed the potential for ‘queer aggregations’. The latter approaches can be found in discussions of racist historicity, queer lives, displaced communities and those people, practices, entities, phenomena and experiences, which have been submerged, displaced, disqualified and
Conclusion
177
rendered insufficient or nonsensical. These practices can and are being extended and amplified by and through social and digital media, which is allowing collective psychic apparatuses to be assembled and made to speak as a powerful authorial voice. We can see this in relation to sexism and sexual harassment (think of the Everyday Sexism Project #metoo); to critical race studies in the context of #blacklivesmatter; to the transformation of mental health approaches in the context of the Hearing Voices Movement; and to, particularly, the link between trauma, abuse and voice hearing (see Blackman, 2016b). Digital practices are enabling the linking up of the inchoate, the marginal, the small and those traces and fragments which have been made to exist as outliers and as non-performatives. They can be re-moved, animated and kept alive through processes and practices of coding and re-coding, which allow something new to emerge and that refuse to stay silent or go away. It is through the connecting up of fragments across space and time that a new collective storytelling machine can and could take form. I have tried to adapt these distributed practices to science and to my feeling that science ought to be transforming, not least in those areas of science (psychology, the cognitive neurosciences etc.), which are supposed to help, support and transform people’s lives. We live in a world that is riven by inequalities, oppression and unequal distributions of everything. The kind of psychology I want (what Valerie Walkerdine and I have called ‘psychologies of survival’) must be one that can listen to its own pasts in order to open to lostfutures and futures-yet-to-come. In just these two science controversies there is much that opens to creative indeterminacy and speculative thinking about what could and might become, including the counterfactual and the relationship between what counts as truth and post-truth. Sadly these possibilities are left under-explored and do not cross the threshold where they might appear as a matter of concern or relevance. They are often closed down at the very moment that they are opened up insistently, affectively, hauntologically. 3. Future-Psychology/Speculative Psychologies. We need many! My work has been influenced by many different critiques of psychology that have been shaped in different conjunctures: critiques made in the 1980s by black psychologists entering the discipline; critiques made by feminist psychologists; philosophers of science; critical and discursive psychology; psychosocial
178
Haunted Data
studies; Foucauldian ‘histories of the present’; archaeologies and genealogies of perception, attention, suggestion, etc. This critical work rarely enters into the new rapprochements being forged between the humanities and the sciences. We must not simply either endorse or reject science but find ways to work in the gaps, silences, contradictions and absent-presences – with those versions of the psychological that have been disqualified, but which importantly do not go away. We have developed theories of materiality, but what is left under-explored and analysed are mind-matter relations, the incorporeal, and specifically those experiences that often end up as the subject matter of psychology particularly as it intersects with weird science. We need, as Isabelle Stengers has so cogently called for, more ‘innovative propositions’. For the last twenty-five years I have worked in the area of voice hearing and through the efforts and labour of voice hearers and those professionals willing to listen and learn we have begun to effect change.9 We must not do this by giving up on our critical theories, methods and practices. 4. New materialisms. The human subject is not over or post and subjectivity is not obsolete. We should not banish the human from discussions of the digital subject or consciousness, but we do need radically revised notions of body– world-consciousness relations and theories of the human compatible with twenty-first-century media. What it means to be human has continually been qualified and re-qualified when we look at other contexts and conjunctures. Some of what has created the displacement of the subject within new materialisms and related perspectives comes out of a refusal of conservative psychological theories of will and intentionality, which shaped other disciplines, such as quantum mechanics. Certain psychologies have travelled while others remain at the level of fantasy, fiction, myth or impossibility. Psychology needs a makeover in order to make good on the promises of new materialisms to return the dynamism to matter. This issue returns most insistently in the Feeling the Future controversy and should demand our attention. If we do not pay attention we reinstate problematic mind-matter relations, which end in panpsychism and other forms of speculative realism. Objects might indeed feel, think or refuse our concerns but what human subjects might and indeed could become in our ‘humanicity’ (Kirby, 2011) is one that opens to speculative thinking that challenges the ‘post’ in all its forms.
Conclusion
179
5. Affect. This in my view remains a productive area of research and inquiry, which crosses the arts, humanities and sciences and is returning the invention to radical thinking. However, this in my view will only be realized if we recognize the long traditions of inventive experimentation that have been gathered together under the designation of the subliminal, non-conscious or pre-conscious. This does return us to archives of experimentation which have a close association with psychic research and which return in both controversies staged within the book. These archives contain protoperformative approaches to experimentation; imaginative discussion and use of statistics and the shaping of statistical imaginaries; as well as reconfiguring rather queer psychic dimensions of space, time and matter. They haunt vitalist philosophy, new materialisms and speculative realism. Rather than engage in creative misreadings of science or speculative what if scenarios, we might also engage with what is excluded from legitimate science. This often opens to the historicity of affect and with the indeterminacy of affective potentials and therefore the potential to enter into new relations with others, human and nonhuman. What it means for the ‘social’ to go all the way down – to the visceral, anticipatory, phenomenological, to gut instinct returns in both controversies. I have called this following the work of Hannah Landecker (2016), the matter of history and the historicity of matter (referring to what she terms the biology of history and the history of biology). We must be prepared to displace what has become ‘black-boxed’ within affect theories to avoid ending up in accounts of mediation, which often feel too behaviourist, too neuro-reductionist and remove wonder from the world. 6. Governing through the affective/governing through the psychological. One of the concerns of the book has been to explore those psychic and psychological imaginaries that have become materialized within specific practices of governance and regulation, including nudge politics (priming), and anticipatory forms of speculative forecast (feeling the future). I hope to have shown that psychological theories and data are part of population management and that computational cultures enact psychological imaginaries as much as older forms of power and regulation have. They do not describe what we do naturally but rather perform imaginaries, which attempt to recruit particular psychological propensities and capacities in order to shape,
180
Haunted Data
augment, orchestrate and modulate what we see, do and experience. As I hope to have shown, many of the proprietorial forms of algorithmic power are still based on rather impoverished psychological imaginaries that in many cases are non-performative or counterfactual. Given the lack of understanding of the ontological indeterminacy and radical historicity of those experiences gathered together under the designation of the subliminal, suggestive or affective, digital subjects are not captured by these practices and the question of what becomes captivating is still little known and understood. However, some of the more radical practices that are experimenting with anticipation and feeling the future do seem more attuned to this radical ontological complexity and what might be possible. The current conjuncture therefore holds opportunities for shaping more open and creative forms of experimentation, and working in the gaps, silences, interstices and contradictions to shape radical politics and possibilities. 7. Haunted Data. I hope that the book provides a corpus of data, which challenges more instrumentalist notions of data. The data re-moved in this book is not immediately available for conversion into revenue or capital, but does I hope show the value of developing new ways of resisting such formulations and showing where data becomes compromised (see Langlois, Redden and Elmer 2015). There are many adjectives to describe data – big, beautiful, small, smart, anticipatory, aggregated, false, raw, cooked, compromised, lively, inert and my chosen adjective haunted. This adjective connects the analysis staged in this book to genealogical work, which has confronted hauntologies, which works with a sense that there is something more to say. I end this book at a time when debates about data mirror some of the utopian and dystopian debates shaped in the 1990s which saw the new media worlds then (virtual reality, for example) as either changing everything (for the better) or closing down on possibilities for human agency and change. We can look back now and see that neither of those poles could adequately capture what difference such technologies might make to our lives. The challenges of analysing mediation within computational and increasingly automated media worlds has come back to haunt us and I hope that this book has provided one small path through what in most contexts would be considered small or insignificant data.
Conclusion
181
The controversies and the data animated, re-moved and kept alive by this staging open up to what I think remain as some of the most challenging and vexed questions troubling the humanities, philosophy and sciences, as well as politicians, economists, designers, artists and many other interested publics. It is perhaps apposite therefore to finish with the words of the White Queen in Lewis Carroll’s Through the Looking Glass. As well as an inspiration to Daryl Bem, the White Queen also encourages readers to open to the impossible, the improbable and the extraordinary. In the context of Haunted Data and a post-truth landscape, this is not an invitation to endorse ‘weird science’, but rather to urge the shaping of more innovative and inventive propositions. We so desperately need these to explore what it means to be human and to what might be possible at this particular juncture, specifically as they operate at the ‘edges of consciousness’. This is situated within a context where linear conceptions of time, space and matter, and boundaries between the human and non-human, material and immaterial, fact and fiction, and media and mediation seem increasingly blurred, unstable and riven with possibilities and potentialities.
182
Notes Preface 1 http://www.etymonline.com/index.php?term=haunt 2 For readers not familiar with the field of affect study I will briefly outline some of its distinctive markers and characteristics. This includes a critical re-appraisal of the sciences (and particularly the psychological and neurosciences) by the humanities; a critical and creative re-engagement with ontological as opposed to epistemological concerns; a grounding of what might have passed as immaterial within a neo-materialist reading of what a body is capable of doing, and to that end a radical re-conceptualization of embodiment, often beyond a distinctly singular, phenomenologically experiencing human subject. Of course these concerns have pre-histories that affect acts as an attractor for and pick up on more long-standing debates surrounding power, agency, subjectivity and biopolitics, and on how to invent methodological and conceptual apparatuses that allow a purchase on the question of power, subjectification and the complex problematic of subjectivity. This is often but not always set within a destabilization of what it means to communicate beyond the context of (human) talk, discourse and conversation. This includes an exploration of theories that are sensitive to nonhuman agencies, entanglements and thresholds, which confound and unsettle humanist and sometimes post-humanist beliefs and sentiments (see Clough, 2008a; Gregg and Seigworth, 2010). Affect theories and the field of affect studies has been a very influential interdisciplinary focus of research and thought, which can be found across a range of disciplines, including literature, philosophy, cultural theory, media studies, film studies, art and curatorial study, queer theory, feminism and critical race studies. One of the orientations of affect theories that is examined in this book is a statement that has become something of a truism across the field of affect studies – that is that there is a half-second delay between affect and cognition, There is a growing edifice being assembled on the basis of this assumption, which has become something of a ‘black box’ across affect theories (Latour, 1998). The statement itself might be considered part of a surface of emergence, which has led to critique and counter-critique, and the mobilization of certain theories and
184
Notes
theorists (such as Silvan Tomkins) for example to authorize and extend its reach. It also opens affect theories to the cognitive and neurosciences and relates to an area of scholarship within cognitive science known as automaticity research. 3 This quote is taken from Ruth Leys’s (2011) critique of affect theory that was published in Critical Inquiry. Although she is sympathetic to such a move she is also critical of the current assumption that affect is independent or autonomous from meaning and signification. Also see Leys (2017). 4 The paradoxes that govern discussions of network culture for example ask how and why do certain contagions spread and intensify across social and digital media. This problem is seen by some to be usefully addressed by turning to theories of suggestion, imitation and automaticity found within experimental social psychology and the cognitive sciences. One book, which explicitly draws on such theories and concepts to explore the virality or viral logic of network culture, is by the cultural theorist, Sampson (2012). He argues that contagion and theories of imitation found within the past and present of experimental social psychology and the neurosciences might provide important heuristics for rethinking communication processes beyond the human, singularly bounded, cognitive subject. This is an attempt to grasp what is termed a ‘contagious relationality’ (p. 3), which he relates specifically to biopolitical strategies in the present that are taken to work pre-emptively. Sampson suggests that pre-emption tendencies attempt to modulate and exploit emotion and affect, as well as to ‘affectively prime social atmospheres, creating the conditions for increasingly connected populations to pass on and imitate the suggestions of others’ (p. 5; also see Massumi, 2009). Sampson draws a lineage with the concerns of nineteenth and early-twentieth-century psychologists and sociologists and specifically with the potentially unconscious, instinctual or even affective bases of contagion (also see Blackman and Walkerdine, 2001; Reicher, 2001; Blackman, 2012; Borch, 2012). These theories of the crowd, imitation and suggestion are brought into dialogue with explicitly contemporary concerns – with how communication is intensified and spreads within networked populations. The question for Sampson, as with many others, is, what is it that spreads? Clearly, it is not just information as understood within traditional media theory, or cybernetics, as what spreads includes political rumours, fads, fashions, trends, gossip, hype, emotions, feelings, affects, sensations and moods, for example – forms of contagious communication that, as many people have argued, take us back to the nineteenth-century concerns and potentially to those theorists, such as Gabriel Tarde and Gustave Le Bon, whose interests in contagion underpinned their own models of sociality (see Blackman, 2012).
Notes
185
Sampson announces the present as an ‘age of contagion’ (p. 1), and brings the past, primarily the French micro-sociologist and criminologist Gabriel Tarde’s work described as ‘Tarde’s imitation-suggestion thesis’ (p. 13) into dialogue with contemporary neuromarketing and post–Second World War experimental social psychology (specifically the work of Stanley Milgram and his famous obedience experiments). Tarde, like Whitehead in the writings of Hansen, is reread as a contemporary media theorist. In different ways all these theories and experimental practices are seen to potentially explain, animate, articulate, dramatize, make visible and allow a purchase on the modulation of processes, which exist, below, beyond and to the side of cognition (see Protevi, 2009). These processes are taken to reveal our fundamental relationality with others human and non-human. On this basis, Sampson argues for a ‘revised notion of subjectivity’ based on a refiguring of Tarde’s ‘sonambulistic subjectivity’ embedded within ‘technological network relations’ (p. 13). Tarde’s sonambulism relates to his oftquoted assumption, that suggestion rather than rationality, or reason is the basis of sociality. This assumption discloses Tarde’s interest in hypnotic suggestion and contagious forms of communication. 5 The term ‘psychomediation’ is a term that resonates with Patricia Clough’s (2008a) concept of ‘biomediation’ to describe the interlocking of the biological with the technical. The concept of psychomediation refers to the interlocking of the psyche with processes of mediation understood as simultaneously material, immaterial, technical, symbolical, affective, biological, historical, etc. The focus on the psyche attempts to introduce a revised notion of subjectivity into debates, which have tended to reinstate or even overstate distinctions between psyche and body – for example the ‘bio’ in biomediation is often overlaid by a rather flat neurophysiological body (see Blackman, 2012, Immaterial Bodies: Affect, Embodiment, Mediation, chapter 1). This tendency across the field of affect studies has been subject to critique (see Leys, 2011; Wetherell, 2013, for example), as it assumes a rather reductionist account of subjectivity, replacing unconscious desires and motivations with a neurophysiological model of personhood. This has opened the door to many cultural and media theorists to make bridges with and join alliances with psychology and the neurosciences to model ontologies of the human subject. Many of these ‘new’ ontologies are seen to be compatible with various turns – the affective, new materialist, non-human and so forth. This often involves overlaying the performative with the neurological or the neurobiological in order to specify the ontological materialities of psychic processes. As Wetherell (2013: 350) argues, the study of affect and bodily affectivities is not just marked out as a particular research area, but is ‘registered as various
186
Notes paradigmatic breaks’ with the past. These breaks, ruptures or turns include a break with psychoanalysis (and therefore subjectivity), a break with work on discourse and representation, and a break with the privileging of the human as the investigative site of interpretive activity. Affect emerges from assemblages of material, social, biological and cultural processes, which create intensities, atmospheres, resonances and so forth. These are pre-personal or trans-personal and relate to processes viewed as ‘beyond, below and past discourse’ – affect as excess (ibid: 350). Mediation is akin to forms of embodied immersion, which are not immediately knowable and communicable and therefore readily verbalized. As Wetherell (ibid) argues, affect and discourse are pitted against each other in an ambivalent relationship. Affect is seen to rescue the body from ‘dumb materiality’, an accusation launched at those who have attempted to explore performativity through a conjoining of Foucault’s work on discourse, with psychosocial understandings of psychic processes (see Butler, 1993 for example). Affective approaches it is argued are capturing the more captivated and lively bodily processes, which are implicated, channelled and modulated through mediation. These are often framed at the level of the brain or nervous system for example.
With this in mind I will explore what a turn to affect articulated in this way might open up, while also being mindful that what affect is pitted against covers over the complexity of perspectives: the diversity and difference of what came before and still exists in the present as a ghostly presence. These genealogies cannot be reduced to and summarily dismissed as approaches that reduce mediation to discourse or subjectivity for example. The media theorist Richard Grusin (2010: 7) is explicit that his approach to mediality is counter to poststructuralist approaches to discourse and to psychoanalytic models, which he suggests were ‘favoured by most contemporary cultural and media theorists’. Affect theory is positioned as offering something different and allows one to engage with the ‘materiality or agency of mediation’ 2010: 19). This materiality is tied to affect systems (often understood as precognitive and located in the brain), which can be modulated, shaped and channelled by remediation and premediation strategies. Neurobiological approaches are often preferred over accounts of subjectivity and fantasy and desire, as the problematic of bodily affect is taken to be foregrounded, a materiality which is seen to have been displaced by psychoanalytic conceptions of and theories of the psyche and psychic processes. This non-conscious, rather than unconscious approach to mediation, assumes that affect is imbricated with mediality, the experience or feel of which falls below the threshold of conscious awareness.
Notes
187
6 The focus on storytelling and inventive writing practices has been a key characteristic of new forms of writing being developed across the field of affect and non-human studies. See Kathleen Stewart’s (2007) Ordinary Affects, Duke University Press, and Laurent Berlant and Kathleen Stewart’s joint project, The One Hundreds, see http://supervalentthought.com/tag/berlant/ Also see the poetic writing of Patricia Clough in her performative memoir writing (see Clough, 2008b). 7 Clough et al. frame big data as the ‘performative celebration of capital’s queer captures and modulations’. The queerness of such queer capture and modulation is aligned in the reach of big data beyond number to the incalculable. This book engages in a different form of ‘queer capture’ and modulation, which attends to those ‘queer aggregations’ which are present in a corpus of data associated with post-publication-peer-review, but which are discarded from attempts to ‘storify’ or modulate the data within specific algorithmic and computational practices, including the Google PageRank algorithm for example. 8 Mark Hansen (2015) has argued for example that ‘agency is resolutely not the prerogative of privileged individual actors’. He suggests that this has always been the case but is revealed more closely in twenty-first-century media. This is aligned specifically to the capacities of computational media to make visible, articulate, extend and animate ‘environmental agencies’. The focus on the ‘environmental’ is not at the expense of a blanket dismissal of the human, but rather is one that argues for the profound need for a requalification of experience and subjectivity within the context of process, indeterminacy and radical contingency. This includes the development of approaches that can do justice to twenty-first-century media and that move beyond psychological individualism and the distinctly human subject as an agential centre of experience. 9 See the important writings of Tiziana Terrenova for example. 10 What Clough et al. have also termed the ‘unconscious drive’ of disciplines, such as sociology, for example. 11 See especially Despret (2004a, b, 2015). 12 Specifically see Jack Halberstam’s (2010) concept of undisciplinarity in the Queer Art of Failure. Durham: Duke University Press. 13 Specifically see their book, Callard and Fitzgerald (2015). The book is open access and can be read and downloaded for free following this link: http://www .palgrave.com/us/book/9781137407955 14 Wilson (2015). 15 See Rose and Abi-Rached (2013), Wilson (2015), Meloni (2016).
Notes
188
16 Also see http://link.springer.com/article/10.1057/s41286-016-0022-6, Blackman (2017). doi:10.1057/s41286-016-0022-6 17 See here the important research of Beverley Skeggs and Simon Yuill (2015) who have developed what they call a ‘life method’ in order to understand the rhythms and encounters that become entangled within Facebook intra-action (following Kember and Zylinska, 2012 and Barad, 2007).
Chapter 1 1 These words included worried, Florida, wrinkled, lonely, old, grey, bingo, wrinkle and forgetful. 2 https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/monkey-cage/wp/2015/12/30/race-aff ects-opinions-about-whether-college-athletes-should-be-paid-heres-how/ 3 The controversy re-moves long-standing problems with the individualization of racism, viewed as a property of individual minds found in the concept of racial prejudice (see Henriques et al., 1984). The question of how racism might be shaped by indirect suggestions is perhaps better understood through the analysis of already existing affective economies of racism, where racism is the product of cultural practices, rather than psychological beliefs or states of mind (see Ahmed, 2004). One of the crucial questions for priming studies is where is mind located? How is mind shaped, produced, changed and transformed, etc.? As we will see one response is that we need better questions and more innovative propositions. 4 http://researcher.watson.ibm.com/researcher/view_group.php?id=2862 5 http://science.howstuffworks.com/science-vs-myth/everyday-myths/quantu m-cryptology4.htm 6 As well as a range of popular books either critiquing or extolling the virtues of the happiness industry, there is also an important corpus of work appearing across feminism, queer theory, critical race studies and critical psychology. This work has refused the claims of the more humanistic side of psychology to represent the inherent freedoms and desire of the subject to change and transform themselves in order to achieve success in relationships, work, health, wealth and well-being, for example. In brief these arguments suggest that the positing of the psychological subject (often described through a fictional image of the autonomous self), as the agent of change and transformation obscures the workings of power and the social exclusions, inequalities and oppressions, which are created, reinforced and maintained. The complex machinations of power
Notes
189
are translated into psychological capacities to be lived, harnessed, discovered, coached or managed into being in order for change to be effected. Sara Ahmed’s important book, The Promise of Happiness (2010) reframes these psychological desires, concepts and techniques often found within positive psychology as social norms rather than social facts. She explores who is excluded from these norms, including the social figure of the unhappy queer, melancholic migrant and feminist killjoy, for example. The feminist Barbarah Ehrenreich (2010), in her book Bright-Sided. How the Relentless Promotion of Positive Thinking has Undermined America, argues that positive thinking is a tyranny, which enters into how individuals are enlisted to manage their own health and fitness for work in the context of unequal health care systems, bad luck, poverty and austerity, for example. Within the context of self-health a cancer diagnosis is reframed as an opportunity for self-improvement, or the chance for a creative makeover, or even as the lease for new life. The cancer sufferer must never get angry or breakdown, where positive thinking should also extend to accepting one’s lot with humility and dignity (also see Stacey, 1990). Positivity is intimately linked to practices of neo-liberal governmentality where self-health or healthism constitutes practices of self-determination as normalizing strategies of self and social regulation. 7 This includes a number of influential books, which explore the genealogical question of what does it mean to be human within the context of the psychological sciences. More recent versions of thinking on psychological governance include Whitehead et al. (2017) and Pykett, Jones and Whitehead (2017). 8 See the book by Potter and Wetherell (1987). 9 See Richards’s important books: Putting Psychology in its Place: Critical Historical Perspectives (2010); Race, Racism and Psychology: Towards a Reflexive History (1997). 10 Specifically see Blackman and Walkerdine (2001); Blackman (2001); Blackman (2012). 11 The collective included Diana Adlam, Julian Henriques, Wendy Hollway, Nikolas Rose, Couze Venn and Valerie Walkerdine. 12 See a discussion in the founding editorial essay of the journal Subjectivity, which has a direct lineage to Ideology and Consciousness via the work of the British psychosocial studies theorist Valerie Walkerdine (Blackman et al., 2008: 6). 13 I am not alone in these observations and arguments. Please see the important genealogical work of Roger Luckhurst (2002) on telepathy; Asprem (2010) on the unexplored links between psychic research, neovitalism, eugenics and Lamarkism within psychology; the historian of psychology, Elizabeth Valentine (2012); and
Notes
190
the writings of Andreas Sommer (2014) on the close yet disavowed relations between psychology and psychic research. 14 Although arguably psychology took the wrong path or direction in its pursuit of the human self-determined bounded agential subject as its normative subject matter, the banished process psychologies and their concern with ‘threshold phenomena’ moved into other disciplines, areas and practices. One example of this migration can be found in media archaeological accounts exploring the intimate and close interrelationship between ‘processual psychological imaginaries’ associated with the psychic, occult, supernatural and paranormal and different media’s technical forms. Stephan Andriopoulos (2013), for example, has explored how ghosting took a particular turn between 1750 and 1930, in the context of reciprocal and interdependent relationships constructed between nineteenth-century psychic research and the development of modern media technologies, such as TV, radio, cinema, telegraphy and the printing press. In his media archaeology of ghostly apparitions, he explores the reciprocal and recursive relationships between engineering, the gothic novel, philosophy (particularly German idealism) and psychic research, which shaped particular apparitional forms. Rather than see technical media creating the possibilities of psychic research – what he terms the ‘primacy of technology over culture’ (2013: 13) – he rather explores how material objects and discursive figures (in the form of particular psychic imaginaries and instruments) were central to the invention of particular media practices. As he suggests, what we witness are the permeable boundaries between philosophy, science, media and culture in the realization of what came to be. This media archaeological approach takes a particular archive – in this case moments of interchange and connection between the philosophies of Hegel, Kant and Shopenhauer, the gothic novel and the occult – to explore what he terms the juxtapositions, nuances and range of cultural and technological preconditions which shaped the invention of TV for example. This is a particular delimited archive, read through the resonances, confluence and interchange across such spheres, and reading back through those epistemic figures and instruments that are part of modern media’s conditions of existence. Andriopoulos’s own interests in apparitions come from what he sees as the lack of attention by many media archaeologists to the centrality of the occult, psychic and the spiritual to the mediums and materialities of technical media forms. These are often considered ‘marginal cultural materialities’ (2013: 155), which are left out or erased in the desire to explore the contingency of the present and what could have been.
Notes
191
The controversies that form the case studies of the book also concern marginal cultural materialities that are culturally significant in the shaping of contemporary computational media and their conditions of existence and interchange with psychic and psychological imaginaries. In different ways these imaginaries also speak back to some of the key points of critique and countercritique governing debate within the field of affect studies – particularly the assumed distinction between cognition and affect, which has become such a productive area of debate and disagreement. 15 Also see Kember and Zylinska’s (2012) use of the cut as a critical and creative media practice. 16 See Blackman (2015) for a further discussion of the importance of ethos in the context of affect studies. 17 also see https://thepsychologist.bps.org.uk/volume-19/edition-9/special-issue-l ife-margins
Chapter 2 1 http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/neuroskeptic/2012/11/02/john-barghs-trans ient-and-ephemeral-blogs/#.WOuHzFPys8Y 2 Social media can be hauntological in the same way as media forms and practices that have come before can be despite arguments, which suggest that this is not the case (see Fisher, 2012). As Avery Gordon (2008: 195) reminds us, ‘The not there is a seething presence’ and these absences are performed within relations and structures, which can be actively lived and felt. They may also account for some of the stark contradictions in theorizing identity and subjectivity within social media, which often foreground performance as central to the work the self (individual and collective) does within this context, but at the same time falls back on concepts of authenticity, where performances are judged for truth, veracity, and the extent to which they give voice to personal sentiment and feeling (see Papacharissi, 2012). 3 The subtitle for the introduction is taken from the title of a journal article by Ioannadis (2012), published in Psychological Inquiry: A Journal for the International Advancement of Psychological Theory, which responds to an article published in the same journal in 2010 by Nosek and Bar-Anan, ‘Scientific Utopia: 1. Opening Science Communication’, which calls for the development of what they call open science. Nosek and Bar-Anan’s article is essentially a
Notes
192
call for scientists to abandon forms of science publishing which they argue are anachronistic in the context of digital communications (that is the published journal article, which they link to seventeenth-century technologies and practices of publishing). see http://lib.hku.hk/cdblog/?p=15025 4 See an important book by van Mourik Broekman et al. (2014). 5 See the journal Ada: A Journal for New Media, Technology and Gender http:// adanewmedia.org/ 6 This includes strategies known as ‘wikithons’ and ‘hackathons’. 7 As Bernhard Rieder (2012) has cogently shown, software and programming practices enact particular forms of sociality, which often originate within fairly traditional mainstream psychological theories. One theory of ‘social influence’, for example, which is one of the conditions of possibility that shaped PageRank, the Google algorithm that ranks connection and influence, draws from theories of sociometry associated with the writings of the social psychologist Leon Festinger (1949). The concept of influence that is enacted is of a particular kind; it enacts a particular ontology of the network which stages connections and links based on status, prestige, hierarchy and canonization, rather than popularity per se. This will become apparent in this chapter, when we explore the relationship between Google PageRank, and what will come to the reader’s attention if they search for the ‘John Bargh priming controversy’. As Betty Bayer (2008), the feminist psychologist has argued, Festinger (1957) was committed to securing the idea of a rational psyche to matters of subjectivity. He was interested in contagion, and groups, processes and practices – such as religious sects, cults and prophecies – whose beliefs spread rapidly in ways that were difficult to understand. Despite these interests he nevertheless produced a rational theory of cognitive dissonance to explain such phenomena. We will return later in the chapter to the issue of contagion, as this has also found its way into discussions of social media (also see Knudsen and Stage, 2012). The fascination with networked virality or networked affect (see Sampson, 2012; Hillis, Passonen and Petit, 2015) – the capacity for particular communication forms to spread and circulate with a speed and rapidity – is an issue that puzzles contemporary researchers and practitioners. What spreads through social networks, how and why? These questions are the subject of new business and advertising models, which attempt to harness virality to produce maximum profit and attention. What is clear from Rieder’s (2012) and Bayer’s (2008) genealogical approach to psychology is that the psychological models of sociality instated and performed by algorithms are often limited and constraining, removing wonder
Notes
193
from the world. As she asks, one ‘wonders instead what social psychology and psychology more broadly traded off historically in its accounts of the world and psychological life’ (2008: 163). These accounts have found their way into the forms of sociality that are made possible within and across social media platforms, although they may of course not exhaust its potential. 8 (see Blackman, 2015 and 2016a). 9 Also see the important work of Grace Cho (2008) and her concept of ‘diasporic vision’, which is a distributed, mediated form of perception, which requires many eyes and ears, human and non-human. 10 Although a lot of discussion of data is framed in relation to the concept of ‘big data’, the data that form the subject of this book are small in comparison. As boyd and Crawford (2012: 663) argue, big data is a rather poor term, which ‘is less about data that is big than it is about a capacity to search, aggregate, and cross-reference large data sets’. The data that I will re-animate in this book are not searchable in the way that data shaped by specific API’s (Application programme interfaces) might be, notwithstanding the problem of the reliability and validity of big data samples and data sets. As boyd and Crawford (2012) have convincingly argued, Twitter data, for example, might be relatively easy to ‘scrape’ using particular software tools, but this does not mean the data are representative. As they argue, big data are not always better data, although this does underpin some of the claims that are being made for data-driven analytics. The data in this book might be considered by some to be ‘small data’ (2012: 670) and therefore overlooked in the rush to scrape and visualize big data sets derived from platform API’s such as Twitter or Facebook. 11 http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/notrocketscience/2012/03/10/failed-replic ation-bargh-psychology-study-doyen/ 12 By Neuroskeptic | 2 November 2012 11:54 am ‘John Bargh’s Transient and Ephemeral Blogs’. http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/notrocketscience/2012 /03/10/failed-replication-bargh-psychology-study-doyen/Also see https://twitter .com/psychscientists/status/394172505168314369?lang=en 13 https://hardsci.wordpress.com/2012/03/12/some-reflections-on-the-bargh-doye n-elderly-walking-priming-brouhaha/ 14 http://www.reddit.com/r/psychology/comments/qxspe/failed_replication_of_fam ous_elderlypriming_walk/ Reddit is described on Wikipedia as an online bulletin board, which is part social networking, part entertainment, part news and to that extent is a good example of the remediated digital environments transforming scientific debate and discussion. It is a good example of what Bolter and Grusin
Notes
194
(2000) term the ‘genealogy of affiliations’ between so-called old and new media. see https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Reddit 15 One of the paradoxes recognized by many theorists, is that Twitter, for example, is both ‘individualistic and communal’ (Murthy, 2013: 151). There is a tension between tweets, for example, which are essentially forms of self-promotion, and those which become an ‘event’, that is those that gain a reach and traction, and get taken up by broadcast media or that bring people to the street, for example. As Murthy argues, although social media is often considered a revolutionary medium, it should be considered alongside the role different forms of media have played in social change, protest and activism. Anabelle Srebeny-Mohammedi (1994) has talked about ‘small media’, such as cassette tapes, Xeroxes, tape recorders and telephones that allowed the spread and circulation of the Ayatolla’s words of revolutionary inspiration from France to Iran during the 1978–9 Iranian Revolution. This happened across time and space as the messages spread while he was under exile in France. Similarly, we might consider the role that video technology and art installations and practices (such as the Quilt project) played in activist movements, such as ActUp in the United States and Europe during the 1990s. ActUP mediated protests, which took the private anguish of dying individuals (from HIV and Aids) onto the streets, to the broadcast media and into people’s homes, and to the Bush administration and the insurance and pharmaceutical companies. Different forms of media, including DIY video technology of the time, were used to mediate collective action against governments and pharmaceutical companies. The alternative media of the time and its circulation within particular networks acts as an interesting precursor to YouTube (and the uploading of documentaries, videodiaries, etc.). These media carried feeling, passion, imagination, longing, anguish and hope, as well as being embedded and circulating within social networks, which were performatively linked, creating a new entity, the PwA (person with Aids). This entity blurred the personal and political in effective ways, and was staged via forms of direct action. These strategies memorialized the genocide, which occurred among the many, mainly gay communities during that time. The alternative media produced were often passed to the broadcast media and journalists gaining more mainstream media attention and public sympathy towards the plight. Jim Hubbard and Sarah Schulman have brought together some of these archives in a moving documentary, United in Anger: A History of ActUp (2012), and Schulman (2012) has written an important critical memoir of this period and her own memories and experiences in the book, Gentrification of the Mind: Witness to a Lost Imagination. These genealogies suggest something
Notes
195
important about the collective amnesia that occurs when social media is always considered distinctly ‘new’. They also open up different questions about the nature of change and transformation, which bring the politics into feeling, and show how the evolution of science is not purified from the networks of publics and communities linked by broadcast and alternative media. These potentially bring new entities, objects and practices into being. These remarks also take us back to the concerns of crowd psychology and to the important writings of the crowd psychologist Serge Moscovici (1985). He argued that fascism in the late nineteenth and twentieth centuries appealed to a feeling body, recognizing that appeals to reason and rationality, didactic command and instruction and staged forms of persuasion would often miss the mark and make followers more resistant to change and transformation. As Moscovici (1985: 104) proclaims, ‘The age of the crowd was the age of the imagination, and he who rules there rules by imagination.’ Similarly, Stuart Hall (1988), the important black cultural studies theorist, recognized that Thatcherism in the United Kingdom in the 1970s and 1980s appealed to particular fantasies, which have become embedded as social truths and social goods. Thus Thatcherism’s vision extends well beyond Thatcher and even the Conservative governments of then and now in the United Kingdom. He supplemented his approach to mediation with psychoanalytic concepts of fantasy and desire in order to draw attention to the complexity of processes of self and social change. We would be wise therefore to situate the potentially affective dimensions of social media within the context of these long histories of mediation, fantasy, public imagination and protest. As Murthy (2013: 102) suggests, ‘Even Martin Luther King generally needed more than 140 characters to capture people’s hearts!’ 16 With its title nod to Sandra Bernhard, the American lesbian comedian whose stage show of the same name parodied the rumour about her own affair with Madonna in the 1990s. 17 See (Blackman 2014a and b, 2015, 2016a). 18 https://docs.google.com/document/d/1wuu8URArgZusJELXF5j4xpM26ESkF fOveYoGKBf0CHo/edit?pli=1The original blog entry was posted on Psychology Today on 23 March 2012, later removed. It has been replaced with this post https ://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/the-natural-unconscious/201203/nothing-in-t heir-heads 19 Interested readers can follow the influence of Libetism across affect studies in my article, published in the journal, Subjectivity, ‘Affect and Automaticity: Towards an Analytics of Experimentation’ http://www.palgrave-journals.com/sub/journal/ v7/n4/pdf/sub201419a.pdf
Notes
196
20 https://twitter.com/psychscientists/status/394172505168314369?lang=en https:// muckrack.com/link/YByP/angry-birds-psychology-today 21 DOI 10 April 2018. 22 Despret (2015) uses the term dispositive to refer to the specificity of different practices of experimentation, including their philosophies of experimentation and assumptions about mind and matter, that bring particular entities, objects and processes into being. 23 http.//blogs.wsj.com/ideas-market/2012/03/12/failure-to-replicate-famous-st udy-causes-furore/?mod=google_news_blog 24 http.//www.livescience.com/27262/psychology-studies-questioned.html 25 https://digest.bps.org.uk/2014/09/19/the-10-most-controversial-psychology-s tudies-ever-published/ 26 http://andrewgelman.com/2016/02/12/priming-effects-replicate-just-fine-than ks/http://andrewgelman.com/2016/09/22/why-is-the-scientific-replication-cri sis-centered-on-psychology/http://www.slate.com/articles/health_and_science/ science/2016/10/why_the_replication_crisis_seems_worse_in_psychology.htmlh ttp://discovermagazine.com/2016/sept/2-the-replication-crisishttp://osc.centerfo ropenscience.org/2014/03/12/previous-episodes/http://www.sciencemag.org/car eers/2015/01/scientists-under-microscope 27 As a blog on the Wall Street Journal has argued, the basis of Bargh’s attack does not stand up to scrutiny. As they suggest, ‘Bargh called PLoS ONE a “for-profit” operation that disdains peer review and whose articles should be viewed as “essentially self-published”. He suggested that anyone with $1,350 can get his or her article published. In fact, as the publisher of PLoS ONE wrote in the comments section, the online journal is peer reviewed, non-profit, rejects 31% of articles submitted – and the fee structure resembles that of other openaccess journals. Fees, which are waived when necessary, help substitute for the high subscription fees that it does not charge. You might raise eyebrows at the article-acceptance rate of 69%, but much of Bargh’s claims about PLoS ONE seemed to evaporate on inspection.’ See http.//blogs.wsj.com/ideas-market/2 012/03/12/failure-to-replicate-famous-study-causes-furore/?mod=google_ news_blog 28 One can start to piece together the response from long quotations from Bargh’s original response reproduced on related blogs. The following extract was posted on The Situationist, a blog that is attached to The Harvard Law School. The link to Bargh’s original post that this reproduces is now lost. The reader is again taken to Psychology Today and met with the automated response, Page Not Found.
Notes
197
29 See Blackman (2014, 2015, 2016). 30 Interested readers can follow a storify of this Twitter conversation by Cedar Rieners on his blog, http://riener.us/WP/uncategorized/put-your-head-up-to-t he-meta-a-peer-reviews-post-post-publication-peer-review-a-bargh-f ull-of-li nks/ 31 26 May 2017. 32 For an extended discussion of this redemption narrative and the work it performs, the reader is directed to my article: Blackman (2016). 33 http://riener.us/WP/uncategorized/put-your-head-up-to-the-meta-a-peer-revie ws-post-post-publication-peer-review-a-bargh-full-of-links/. 34 https://cedarsdigest.wordpress.com/2012/03/21/put-your-head-up-to-the-meta- a-peer-reviews-post-post-publication-peer-review-a-bargh-full-of-links/
Chapter 3 1 (See Curran, Fenton and Freedman, 2012). In an overview of these theories, Couldry and Hepp (2013) have argued that this triumvirate model never really stood up to scrutiny. It is even less convincing now media are considered dispersed, pervasive, ubiquitous, embedded, embodied and part and parcel of the texture of experience (also see Deuze, 2012). 2 See the important work of the late queer theorist Jose Munoz (2009) and feminist scholars such as Ann Cvetkovich (2003) and Heather Love (2007). 3 See Despret (2004a) for a good description of the experiments and their retelling of the Clever Hans effect. In later work Despret (2015) has reopened the case of ‘Clever Hans’ and explored what was discarded, overlooked, missed and actively disqualified and disavowed in Pfungt’s experiments. As she argues, ‘Pfungst’s solution, despite its coherence and all the accumulated experimental evidence, leaves here and there unexplained residues, peculiarities, details that do not settle well with the whole and which suggest that there might be another investigation’ (p. 78). 4 The Elberfeld horses were horses owned by Herr Krall, an Elberfeld manufacture who had been enthralled by Hans and von Osten’s feat and who continued this tradition and his own interest in psychic animals training his horses, including Hans to undertake marvellous feats. Following von Osten’s fall into disrepute he sold Hans to Herr Krall, who continued nurturing Han’s equine celebrities and prodigious talents, where Hans lived on with two other
Notes
198
horses, known collectively as the Elberfeld horses. If readers want to read a fascinating account of the Elberfeld horses they are directed to an online book by Maurice Maeterlinck, The Unknown Guests, and particularly Chapter IV, The Elberfeld horses http://www.learnlibrary.com/unknown-gues t/index.htm This book also contains a chapter, The Knowledge of the Future (chapter III), which discusses precognition, the subject of Chapters 4, 5 and 6. 5 Also see the contemporary writing of the controversial scientist Rupert Sheldrake (1999) and particularly his book, Dogs who Know when their Owners are Coming Home and Other Unexplained Powers of Animals. 6 http://www.bbc.co.uk/programmes/b04vf36p This episode of the BBC Radio 4 All in the Mind series was broadcast on Tuesday, 23 December 2014, as part of an episode called, ‘Hypnoticism; Automatic Writing, Magic and Memory’. 7 http://www.biology-online.org/dictionary/Biocoenosis 8 In this article he considers what it means to become a nose within the perfume industry when human and technical are not consider separate, pre-existing entities. 9 The following blog identifies some interesting issues with foregrounding the body’s capacity to affect and be affected through the concepts of device, activation, test etc. The author suggests that this language is instrumental despite the aim to go beyond instrumental approaches to the body https://blogs.brown. edu/engl-2761k-s01-fall-2016/2016/10/13/being-withbeing-for-instrumentality -and-embodied-experiments/ 10 http://www.scoop.it/t/eponastribe/?tag=%23horsewhisper+%23cleverhan+%2 3smartequines the link to this issue is now broken DOI 26 May 2017. 11 Also see a fascinating book by Nosworthy (2013). 12 https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Biocoenosis 13 Despret’s (2015) reopening of the case cogently shows how productive the anomalies and residual traces of other explanations might be. As she argues, what is left are critical questions about ‘a dual intelligence, involving bodies, attentions to the other, desires and wills, consciousness capable of splitting, of being relocated, edges of consciousness that bring about effective actions’ (p. 83). She suggests that Pfungst is closer to these traces when he operates more as an ethologist, rather than psychologist, showing that he was working at a critical juncture in the history of psychology where the subject of psychology had not been stabilized.
Notes
199
Part 2 1 Particularly for those who have identified with left politics or are part of queer, critical race, de-colonial, feminist or disability activism and movements, for example. 2 Read more: http://www.oprah.com/world/Futurist-David-Houle-Investigates-the- Future-of-Shopping#ixzz2rn2s6iKo 3 Also see the writing of Zylinska (2014), http://quod.lib.umich.edu/cgi/p/pod /dod-idx/minimal-ethics-for-the-anthropocene.pdf?c=ohp;idno=12917741.0 001.001 4 See Seigworth and Tiessen’s (2012) analysis of the mythology of financial liquidity, what they also call the ‘illusion of liquidity’ (p. 64) and its future orientation and colonization of possible futures. As they go on to argue, ‘The liquidity crisis is only one expression drawn from out of a whole web of credit driven colonizations that mortgage the future to fund today’s human and morethan-human desires’ (p. 68). 5 H. G. Wells is often cited as one of the founding figures of Future studies, for example. He is considered a notable seer whose fiction imaginatively staged possible futures and potentially predictive scenarios, which anticipated futures-yet-to-come. As a journalist, popularizer and novelist Wells blurred fact and fiction in his own writing, publishing novels and short serialized stories of possible futures, which held wide appeal. Within the context of future studies, his volume, Anticipations of the Reactions of Mechanical and Scientific Progress upon Human Life and Thought, (1902) is often staged as a key moment and cornerstone in the development of Future Studies as a modern discipline. 6 Cited 670 times at the time of writing on 10 April 2018.
Chapter 4 1 http://redux.slate.com/cover-stories/2017/05/daryl-bem-proved-esp-is-real-s howed-science-is-broken.html 2 http://tanclab.org/feeling-the-future-of-bems-findings/ 3 http://www.techcentral.ie/ibm-watson-goes-precognitive/ 4 http://www.datanami.com/2015/04/24/ai-developer-touts-artificial-precognition/
Notes
200
5 http://rationalwiki.org/wiki/Feeling_the_Futurehttp://tanclab.org/feeling-the-fu ture-of-bems-findings/ 6 (http://www.dbem.ws/FeelingFuture.pdf 7 http://psychsciencenotes.blogspot.co.uk/2010/11/brief-note-daryl-bem-and-pr ecognition.html 8 http://blog.practicalethics.ox.ac.uk/2010/11/against-open-mindedness/ 9 Some readers may be able to access the clip from the show by following this link: http://www.cc.com/video-clips/bhf8jv/the-colbert-report-time-traveling-porn ---daryl-bem 10 http://www.imdb.com/title/tt1930067/ 11 http://www.telegraph.co.uk/comment/columnists/bryonygordon/8147953/Fra nkly-the-future-is-all-too-predictable.html 12 http://www.dailymail.co.uk/sciencetech/article-1330596/Humans-psychic-power s-New-study-proves-future.html#comments 13 http://www.huffingtonpost.com/cassandra-vieten/esp-evidence_b_795366.html 14 http://www.dazeddigital.com/artsandculture/article/16893/1/can-we-see-into- the-future 15 noetic.org/sites/default/files/uploads/files/PK_BuzzonBem.doc 16 see http://curatescience.org 17 see Sandra Bem’s (1998) autobiography An Unconventional Family for an account of their life together, including their commitment to ‘egalitarian partnering’ and ‘feminist child-rearing’ practices. 18 http://beforeitsnews.com/opinion-conservative/2011/04/the-top-5-mad-scienti sts-in-the-world-567089.html 19 Dossey (2011), http://www.alice.id.tue.nl/references/dossey-2011.pdf (accessed on 6 March 2015). 20 Despret (2015) uses the term dispostive to describe the a priori’s that shape different experimental configurations. She argues that psychology as it operates in experimental modes does not reveal pre-existing subjects, objects or entities, but rather is a technology of transformation that produces what is being looked for. In this case she argues that what is often dismissed in psychology as an ‘experimental effect’ actually reveals something much more interesting about the capacities of subjects to be affected and to affect experimental apparatuses and what we might conclude from this effect. The positivity of experimental apparatuses and their capacity to shape the experimental scene often closes down on the potential of experimental subjects to enter into more interesting relationships that are removed, buried,
Notes
201
re-routed or disallowed. As Betty Bayer (2008) has argued psychology removes wonder from the world often mechanizing experimental subjects and enacting impoverished notions of ‘influence’ that foreclose other explanations, routes and possibilities. 21 This referred to the apparent ordinariness of evil in the wake of the actions of Adolf Eichmann during the Holocaust https://www.theguardian.com/comment isfree/2011/aug/29/hannah-arendt-adolf-eichmann-banality-of-evil 22 http://www.nature.com/polopoly_fs/7.6716.1349271308!/suppinfoFile/Kahn eman%20Letter.pdf 23 http://www.skepticforum.com/viewtopic.php?t=23315http://journals.plos.org/p losone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0033423http://tanclab.org/feeling-the-fut ure-of-bems-findings/
Chapter 5 1 http://www.reed.edu/reed_magazine/june2011/articles/features/bem/bem2.html 2 This episode was screened in the United States on 27 January 2011. http://the colbertreport.cc.com/videos/bhf8jv/time-traveling-porn-daryl-bem 3 The main story emerging at the time of writing is that the controversy discloses the importance of replication. The reader will find a link here to a more submerged story about replication, which Bem says is being overshadowed by the voices of particular sceptics who are more newsworthy. http://www.dailygrai l.com/Mind-Mysteries/2014/1/Is-Precognition-Real-Positive-Replications-Dary l-Bems-Controversial-Findings Also see http://www.abovetopsecret.com/forum/ thread993701/pg 4 http://nymag.com/news/features/esp-instructions-2011-3/The ESP test is made available to a general public in a New York Times article published on 27 February 2011 with the headline, ‘53.1% of You Already Know What This Story’s About. Or Do You? Need a Hint?’ see http://nymag.com/news/features/ bem-esp-2011-3/ 5 http://nymag.com/news/features/esp-instructions-2011-3/ 6 http://rhineonline.blogspot.co.uk/2011/02/daryl-bem-precognition-in-mainstr eam.html 7 http://deanradin.blogspot.co.uk/2014/04/feeling-future-meta-analysis.html 8 http://f1000research.com/articles/4-1188/v1 http://tanclab.org/feeling-the-futu re-of-bems-findings/ http://www.dailygrail.com/Mind-Mysteries/2012/8/Not-Fee
Notes
202
ling-the-Future-New-Bem-Replication-Fails-Find-Evidence-Psi https://weilerp siblog.wordpress.com/2014/05/05/the-bem-precognition-meta-analysis-vs-those -wacky-skeptics/ http://psychsciencenotes.blogspot.co.uk/2011/05/failing-to-r eplicate-bems-ability-to.html http://www.richardwiseman.com/BemReplications .shtml 9 See Chapter 6, scene 3 for a curious form of non-local causality or time travel, which queers the statement that there is a ‘half-second delay between anticipation and reaction’ that has become associated with Libet’s formulation of reaction time. As we will see, Libet drew from more quantum explanations of time-travel and retrocausality that have been obscured by what has become black-boxed in relation to these experiments – for example, the so-called half-second delay (in cognitive science and affect studies, for example). 10 The four ‘known psychological effects’ that Bem reverses (what he terms ‘time-reversed effects’) in the nine experiments include precognitive approach to erotic stimuli and precognitive avoidance of negative stimuli; retroactive priming; retroactive habituation; and retroactive facilitation of recall (Bem, 2011: 1). 11 http://www.fastcompany.com/1705108/predicting-future-porn 12 http://mentalfloss.com/article/27014/predicting-future-or-least-predicting-where -naked-people-are 13 http://www.scientificamerican.com/article/extrasensory-pornception/ 14 www.skeptic.com 15 http://nymag.com/news/features/bem-esp-2011-3/index1.html 16 http://www.scientificamerican.com/article/extrasensory-pornception/ 17 See Ian Hacking’s (1990) book, The Taming of Chance, for a genealogy of statistics and probabilistic thinking. 18 See http://politicalticker.blogs.cnn.com/2010/04/23/sec-staffers-watched-porn-a s-economy-crashed/ 19 This extends theories of ‘cultural invitation’, which I argued in Chapter 3 are overly cognitivist, relating perception to theories of mind and ‘folk psychology’, rather than taking a more ecological approach to affect, perception, sensation, etc. 20 At the time of writing the keyword ‘porn’ on a Google browser brought up 140,000,000 results. 21 Bem had a reason for selecting porn: He figured that if people did have ESP, then it would have to be an adaptive trait – a sixth sense that developed over millions of years of evolution. If our sixth sense really had such ancient origins,
Notes
203
he guessed it would likely be attuned to our most ancient needs and drives. In keeping with this theory, he set up the experiment so that a subset of the hidden images would be arousing to the students. Would the premonition of a pornographic image encourage them to look behind the correct curtain?’ http: //redux.slate.com/cover-stories/2017/05/daryl-bem-proved-esp-is-real-showed -science-is-broken.html 22 At the time of writing there is an interesting discussion about the link between porn and the kinds of porn watched on handheld mobile devices. This is linked to a male-identified teenager’s (over) use of Viagra to remedy what are considered new forms of sexual psychopathology. These primarily register within the language of addiction. See http://www.mirror.co.uk/news/uk-news/britains-youn gest-viagra-addict-aged-8626707
Chapter 6 1 http://www.ejwagenmakers.com/2012/Wagenmakers2012Horrors.pdf 2 http://www.ejwagenmakers.com/ 3 http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/the- natural-unconscious/201203/not hing-in-their- heads 4 This is an important argument made by Mark Hansen in his book Feedforward; he argues that consciousness is a function of hosting and that literally consciousness is ‘in the machine’ when understood as elemental. Hansen borrows the term ‘elemental’ from Galloway and Thacker (2007) to describe the networked function of twenty-first-century media. Galloway and Thacker use the term to describe all those processes that operate above and below the human subject and that we do not directly control or manipulate. This account takes us beyond either technological determinism or media-use to explore the entangled body-world-consciousness relations that twenty-first-century media amplify and make visible. These arguments will be revisited later in this scene when we encounter quantum physics and quantum biological accounts of consciousness as they are re-moved by this controversy. 5 Aggregation is also an interesting concept to ‘think with’ when considering Derrida’s (1984) arguments regarding grammatology and its differences from semiology. Vicki Kirby (2011) provides a thought-provoking elucidation of these differences in the context of her seminal book, Quantum Anthropologies: Life at Large. As she argues, ‘Whereas semiology envisaged an aggregation or
Notes
204
assemblage of different systems that would somehow communicate with each other across the distance that identified them as separate, grammatology regards these different “entities” as articulations of the system’ (p. 7). 6 http://www.thefreedictionary.com/counterfactual 7 http://www.forgottenbooks.com/readbook_text/Journal_of_the_Society_for _Psychical_Research_1887_1000694001/41 8 The reader might find the following summary of the significance of Bem’s Feeling the Future experiments in an article written by Daniel Engber, with the provocative headline, ‘Daryl Bem Proved ESP is real: Which Means Science is Broken’. This article was written in May 2017 and draws attention to one argument made about Bem’s studies. If he was showing us that it is possible to prove statistically that precognition exists, what does this suggest about the truth or validity of broader psychological studies. In other words if Bem could prove the existence of ESP using the scientific method then did it not also suggest that you could find almost anything using the method? As the author suggests, ‘Daryl Bem had seemed to prove that time can flow in two directions – that ESP is real. If you bought into those results, you’d be admitting that much of what you understood about the universe was wrong. If you rejected them, you’d be admitting something almost as momentous: that the standard methods of psychology cannot be trusted, and that much of what gets published in the field – and thus, much of what we think we understand about the mind – could be total bunk.’ http://redux.slate.com/cover-stories/2017/05/daryl-bem-proved-esp-is-r eal-showed-science-is-broken.html 9 https://thepsychologist.bps.org.uk/volume-25/edition-5/replication-replication-r eplicationIn this edition of The Psychologist, Stuart J. Ritchie, Richard Wiseman and Chris French (all sceptics and anomalistic/cognitive scientists) argue that replication is the key issue, which emerges from both the John Bargh priming controversy (see Chapters 2 and 3) and the Feeling the Future controversy. As they argue, ‘The wide-ranging discussion of Bem’s paper has raised questions regarding the limits of science, our current statistical paradigm, the policies of academic journal publishing, and what exactly a scientist needs to do to convince the world that a surprising finding is true. In this article we outline the “Feeling the future” controversy, our part in it, and highlight these important questions about scientific psychology.’ Also see their article, Ritchie, Wiseman and French (2012) 10 http://www.reddit.com/search?q=Daryl+Bem discussion portal in relation to Feeling the Future http://osc.centerforopenscience.org/2014/06/25/a-skepti
Notes
205
cs-review/\http://cristiannegureanu.blogspot.co.uk/2010/11/proof-of-extra-d imensions-possible-next.htmlhttp://www.telegraph.co.uk/news/science/science -news/8244419/Extrasensory-perception-paper-published-in-respected-jou rnal.htmlhttp://www.skepticforum.com/viewtopic.php?f=7&t=23315http ://futureandcosmos.blogspot.co.uk/2014/04/feeling-future-study-replicated-a s.htmlhttp://www.csicop.org/specialarticles/show/response_to_alcocks_back_f rom_the_future_comments_on_bemhttp://www.alice.id.tue.nl/references/do ssey-2011.pdf 11 http://daniellakens.blogspot.co.uk/2014/05/a-pre-publication-peer-review-of -meta.html 12 https://koestlerunit.wordpress.com/study-registry/registered-studies/ 13 http://arxiv.org/abs/1205.1055 14 See Chapter 1 and my discussion of embodied hauntology. 15 http://neurocritic.blogspot.co.uk/2012/03/how-much-of-neuroimaging-literatu re.html http://www.danielbor.com/dilemma-weak-neuroimaging/ 16 http://www.alice.id.tue.nl/references/dossey-2011.pdf 17 http://www.alice.id.tue.nl/references/dossey-2011.pdf Dossey (2011) (quotation from p. 127). 18 This was also a key focus of my book Immaterial Bodies. 19 As Barad herself has cautioned, sociology should not be reduced to biology and people should not be reduced to atoms. I take seriously Barad’s lesser repeated statement and caution that people are not particles and that drawing such analogies is not her business (24). See Blackman, 2014 http://www.transform ationsjournal.org/issues/25/01.shtml 20 http://www.pbs.org/wgbh/nova/next/physics/retrocausality-could-send-informa tion-back-to-the-future/ 21 http://link.springer.com/search?query=Loving+the+Alien+Lisa+Blackman 22 http://wegner.socialpsychology.org/publications 23 http://psiresearch.org/tsirelson/ 24 http://mediumofexpression.blogspot.co.uk/2013/08/dispelling-quantum-spooks- clue-that.html 25 A major purpose of this meeting was to enable interaction and conversations between the two groups, but this was only partially successful. Unfortunately, another symposium of interest to physicists was also taking place on the second day, and some of the best talks about psi phenomena were missed by the physicists for whom they would have been most pertinent. Still, there were many useful discussions both inside and outside the seminar room, and the
Notes
206
organizers (Sheehan primarily) should be commended for creating another fascinating, productive, and perhaps even historic meeting. (see http://med iumofexpression.blogspot.co.uk/2013/08/dispelling-quantum-spooks-clue-that. html) 26 http://tanclab.org/feeling-the-future-of-bems-findings/ 27 See an article in The New Scientist debating quantum time-travel, with the headline, ‘Quantum Time-Travel: Black Hole not needed’. https://www.newscien tist.com/article/mg20827871.400-quantum-time-travel-black-hole-not-required /?full=true 28 This was a comment left on a blog of the psychologist Richard Wiseman, written on 10 November 2012, with the title ‘Bem’s ESP research: Quirky stuff ’. The comment was left by the user Anaglyph on 10 November 2010 at 4.17pm. In a previous comment left on 19 November 2010 at 6.21pm, the same user argues that ‘the thing that bugs me most about this whole affair is how readily it gets equated with “quantum” effects. It seems to me that many people think:“Quantum = weird These results = weird Quantum = real, therefore, These results are quite possibly genuine.”’ It is of course bereft of logic, but numerous commenters raise this idea, and many of the sites that carried the New Scientist article were champing at the bit to point it out. The association between these results and anything to do with quantum physics is a conjecture entirely separate from the experiment at hand. The word ‘quantum’ associated with some kind of paranormal event is inevitably a red flag in my experience’. 29 http://jacquelene-drinkall.squarespace.com/journal-of-integral-telepathies/2015/ 7/15/2q5bb3nwt4zn4ud8cceyloeu30s6kr 30 This also includes Mark Zuckerberg’s claim that telepathy is the future of communication http://www.itpro.co.uk/strategy/24908/zuckerberg-says-telepa thy-is-the-future-of-communication 31 This is a reference to Nick Couldry’s (2014) arguments about big data. 32 https://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/one-among-many/201202/bem-in-quan tum-space 33 http://www.skeptiko.com/daryl-bem-responds-to-parapsychologydebunkers/ 34 http://regolish.blogspot.co.uk/2012/06/titanic-effect-of-retrocausality.html 35 http://www.bibliotecapleyades.net/sociopolitica/sociopol_blackproject02.htm 36 http://newsflavor.com/world/asia/swiss-watch-found-in-400-year-old-tomb/
Notes
207
37 http://www.bibliotecapleyades.net/ciencia/time_travel/esp_ciencia_timetravel42. htm 38 See http://www.wired.com/2012/01/obama-mars/ an article which explores claims that President Barack Obama was part of a secret CIA intergalactic project in the 1980s where he was teleported to Mars! 39 We encountered this assumption of a half-second delay earlier as it appears across affect theories and also in assumptions that priming is linked to the modulation of the supposed delay between stimulus and response. 40 Quantum-delayed choice experiments are those which extend forced choice designs (yes or no) into the realm of the nonlinear. Examples of these will be outlined later in the chapter. 41 ‘Whitehead introduces a new metaphysically primitive notion, which he calls an actual occasion. For Whitehead, an actual occasion (or actual entity) is not an enduring substance, but a process of becoming. As Whitehead puts it, actual occasions are the “final real things of which the world is made up”, they are “drops of experience, complex and interdependent”’ (1929c, Pt 1, Ch. 2, sec. 1, p. 27). See https://plato.stanford.edu/entries/whitehead/ 42 https://larvalsubjects.wordpress.com/2010/08/02/harman-responds-to-shaviro/ 43 Also see a special issue of the journal SubStance devoted to the work of Isabelle Stengers (Savransky, 2018) https://muse.jhu.edu/issue/38165
Chapter 7 1 Clough et al. frame big data as the ‘performative celebration of capital’s queer captures and modulations’. The queerness of such queer capture and modulation is aligned in the reach of big data beyond number to the incalculable. This book engages in a different form of ‘queer capture’ and modulation, which attends to those ‘queer aggregations’ which are present in a corpus of data associated with post-publication-peer-review, but which are discarded from attempts to ‘storify’ or modulate the data within specific algorithmic and computational practices, including the Google PageRank algorithm, for example. 2 An example of what Chow is pointing towards can be found in the political efficacy of mobile phone footage associated with the #blacklivesmatter circulated across social media of black men being killed by white police officers in the United States.
208
Notes
3 See Patricia Clough’s (2015) description of Kathleen Stewart’s writing as compositional. 4 http://www.bfi.org.uk/news/review-john-akomfrah-s-hauntologies 5 The Stuart Hall Project http://www.bfi.org.uk/news-opinion/news-bfi/interviews/ stuart-hall-project-john-akomfrah-interview 6 http://www.ghosthostings.co.uk 7 http://archives.carre.pagesperso-orange.fr/Duchamp%20Marcel.html 8 www.stasis73.com 9 I am part of a Wellcome-funded interdisciplinary collaborative project called ‘Hearing the Voice’, which is bringing together cognitive scientists, philosophers, cultural theorists, artists, professionals and voice hearers. See http:// hearingthevoice.org/ Also see https://www.madinamerica.com/2016/09/sociolog ist-offers-new-perspective-on-voice-hearing/
Bibliography Adams, Vincanne, Murphy, Michelle and EClarke, Adele (2009) ‘Anticipation: Technoscience, Life, Affect, Temporality’. Subjectivity 28: 246. doi:10.1057/ sub.2009.18. Adlam, Diana., Henriques, Julian., Rose, Nik., Salfield, A., Venn, Couze and Walkerdine, Valerie. (1976) ‘Psychology, Ideology and the Human Subject’. Ideology and Consciousness 1: 1–26. Ahmed, Sara (2014) Wilful Subjects. Durham: Duke University Press. Ahmed, Sara (2010) The Promise of Happiness. Durham: Duke University Press. Ahmed, Sara (2004) The Cultural Politics of Emotion. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. Anderson, Chris (2008) ‘The End of Theory: The Data Deluge Makes the Scientific Method Obsolete’. Wired Magazine 16(7). Accessed 15 June 2014. http://archive. wired.com/science/discoveries/magazine/16-07/pb_theory. Andriopolous, Stefan (2013) Ghostly Apparitions: German Idealism, the Gothic Novel, and Optical Media. Cambridge, MA and London: The MIT Press. Andriopolous, Stefan (2008) Possessed: Hypnotic Crimes, Corporate Fiction, and the Invention of Cinema. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Angell, J. R. and Moore, A. W. (1896) ‘Studies from the Psychological Laboratory of the University of Chicago: 1. Reaction-Time: A Study in Attention and Habit’. Psychological Review 3: 245–58. http://www.brocku.ca/MeadProject/Angell/Angel l_Moore_1896.html. Ansfield, M. E. and Wegner, Daniel. (1996) ‘The Feeling of Doing’. In: P. M. Gottwitzer and J. A. Bargh (eds), The Psychology of Action: Linking Cognition and Motivation to Behaviour. New York and London: Routledge. Asprem Egil. (2010) ‘A Nice Arrangement of Heterodoxies – William McDougall and the Professionalization of Psychical Research’. Journal of the History of the Behavioural Sciences 46(2): 123–43. Auge, Mark (2009) Non-Places: Introduction to an Anthropology of Super-modernity. London: Verso Books. Barad, Karen (2012) ‘On Touching The Inhuman that Therefore I am’. Differences: A Journal of Feminist Cultural Studies 23(3): 206–23. Barad, Karen (2010) ‘Quantum Entanglements and Hauntological Relations of Inheritance: Dis/continuities, SpaceTime Enfoldings, and Justice-to-Come’.
210
Bibliography
Derrida Today 3(2): 240–68, ISSN 1754-8500. Available Online November 2010. http://dx.doi.org/10.3366/drt.2010.0206. Barad, Karen (2007) Meeting the Universe Halfway: Quantum Physics and the Entanglement of Matter and Meaning. Durham, NC: Duke University Press. Bargh John. A., and Morsella E. (2008) ‘The Unconscious Mind’. Perspectives on Psychological Science : A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science 3(1): 73–79. Bargh, John., Chen, M. and Burrows, L. (1996) ‘Automaticity of Social Behaviour: Direct Effects of Trait Construct and Stereotype Activation on Action’. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 71(1): 230–44. Bayer, Betty (2008) ‘Wonder in a World of Struggle’. Subjectivity 23(1): 156–73(18). Beer, David (2013) Popular Culture and New Media: The Politics of Circulation. Basingstoke and New York: Palgrave. Beer, David and Burrows, Roger (2013) ‘Popular Culture, Digital Archives and the New Social Life of Data’. Theory, Culture & Society 30(4): 47–71. Bem, Daryl (2011) ‘Feeling the Future: Experimental Evidence for Anomalous Retroactive Influences on Cognition and Affect’. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 100(3): 407–25. Bem, D., Tressoldi, P., Rabeyron, T. and Duggan, M. (2015) ‘Feeling the Future: A Meta-Analysis of 90 Experiments on the Anomalous Anticipation of Random Future Events. F1000Research 4: 1188. doi:10.12688/f1000research.7177.2. Bem, Daryl, Utts, Jessica, and Johnson, Wesley O. (2011b) ‘Must Psychologists Change the Way they Analyse their Data?’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 101(4): 716–19. Bem, Sandra (1998) An Unconventional Family. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Bem, Sandra Lipsitz (1981) ‘Gender Schema Theory: A Cognitive Account of Sex Typing’. Psychological Review 88(4): 354–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X .88.4.354. Berlant, Lauren (2011) Cruel Optimism. Durham, NC: Duke University Press. BhaBha, Homi (2004) The Location of Culture. London and New York: Routledge. Blackman, Lisa (2017) ‘Loving the Alien: A Post-post-human Manifesto’. Subjectivity 10(1): 13–25. Blackman, Lisa (2016a) ‘Social Media and the Politics of Small Data: Post Publication Peer Review and Academic Value’. Theory, Culture & Society 33(4): 3–26. Blackman, Lisa (2016b) ‘The Challenges of New Biopsychosocialities: Hearing Voices, Trauma, Epigenetics and Mediated Perception’. The Sociological Review Monographs 64: 256–73. doi:10.1002/2059-7932.12024. Blackman, Lisa (2015) ‘Researching Affect and Embodied Hauntologies: Exploring an Analytics of Experimentation’. In: B. T. Knudsen and C. Stage (eds), Affective
Bibliography
211
Methodologies: Developing Cultural Research Strategies for the Study of Affect. Basingstoke and New York: Palgrave. Blackman, Lisa (2014a) ‘Affect and Automaticity: Towards an Analytics of Experimentation’. Subjectivity 7(4): 362–84. Blackman, Lisa (2014b) ‘Immateriality, Affectivity, Experimentation: Queer Science and Future Psychology’. Transformations: Journal of Media and Culture 25. New Immaterialities: http://www.transformationsjournal.org/wp-content/uploads/201 6/12/Blackman_Transformations25.pdf. Blackman, L. (2013) ‘Habit and Affect: Revitalizing a Forgotten History’. Body & Society 19(2&3): 186–216. Blackman, Lisa (2012) Immaterial Bodies: Affect, Embodiment, Mediation. London and New York: Sage. Blackman, Lisa (2001) Hearing Voices: Embodiment and Experience. London and New York: Free Association Books. Blackman, Lisa and Walkerdine, Valerie (2001) Mass Hysteria: Critical Psychology and Media Studies. Basingstoke and New York: Palgrave. Blackman, Lisa, Cromby, John, Hook, Derek, Papadopoulos, Dimitris and Walkerdine, Valerie (2008) ‘Creating Subectivities’. Subectivity 22(1): 1–27. Bogost, Ian (2012) Alien Phenomenology, or What It Is Like to Be a Thing. Minnaepolis: University of Minnesota Press. Bolter, Jay and Grusin, Richard (2000) Remediation: Understanding New Media. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Borch, C. (2012) The Politics of Crowds: An Alternative History of Sociology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. boyd, danah and Crawford, Kate (2012) ‘Critical Questions for Big Data: Provocations for a Cultural, Technological, and Scholarly Phenomenon’. Information, Communication and Society 15(5): 662–79. Brown, S. (2012) ‘Experiment: Abstract Experimentalism’. In: C. Lury and N. Wakeford (eds), Inventive Methods: The Happening of the Social. London and New York: Routledge. Brown, Steve and Stenner, Paul (2009) Psychology Without Foundations: History, Philosophy and Psychosocial Theory. London and New York: Sage. Butler, Judith (2010) ‘Performative Agency’. Journal of Cultural Economy 3(2): 147–61. Butler, Judith (1993) Bodies that Matter: On the Discursive Limits of ‘Sex’. London and New York: Routledge. Butler, Judith (1990) Gender Trouble: Feminism and the Subversion of Identity. London and New York: Routledge. Callard, Felicity and Fitzgerald, Des. (2015) Rethinking Interdisciplinarity Across the Social Sciences and the Neurosciences. Basingstoke and New York: Palgrave.
212
Bibliography
Carroll, Lewis (2006) Alice in Wonderland. London: Penguin Books. Cho, Grace (2008) Haunting the Korean Diaspora: Shame, Secrecy, Silence and the Forgotten Korean War. Minneapolis: Minnesota Press. Chow, Rey (2012) Entanglements, or Transmedial Thinking about Capture. Durham, NC and London: Duke University Press. Chun, Wendy Hui Kyong (2016) Updating to Remain the Same: Habitual New Media. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Chun, Wendy Hui Kyong (2006) Control and Freedom: Power and Paranoia in the Age of Fiber-Optics. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Clark, Andrew (2008) Supersizing the Mind: Embodiment, Action and Cognitive Extension. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Clark, W. A. V. and Avery, K. L. (1976) ‘The Effects of Data Aggregation in Statistical Analysis’. Geographical Analysis 8: 428–438. doi:10.1111/j.1538-4632.1976.tb00549.x. Clough, Patricia Tincento (2018) The User Unconscious. On Affect, Media and Measure. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. Clough, Patricia Tincento (2008a) ‘The Affective Turn: Political Economy and the Biomediated Body’. Theory, Culture & Society 25(1): 1–24. Clough, Patricia Tincento 2008b ‘De-coding the Subject in Affect’. Subjectivity 23: 140–55. Clough, Patricia Tincento., Gregory, K., Haber, Benjamin and Scannell, Josh. (2015) ‘The Datalogical Turn’. In: Phillip Vannini (eds), Non-Representational Methodologies: Re-envisioning Research. London and New York: Routledge. Coleman, Rebecca (2009) The Becomings of Bodies: Girls, Image, Experience. Manchester: Manchester University Press. Coombs, S. and Smith, I. (2003) ‘The Hawthorne Effect: Is It a Help or Hindrance in Social Science Research?’ Change: Transformations in Education 6(1): 97–111. ISSN 1441-9319. Coole, Diana and Frost, Samantha (2010) New Materialisms. Durham, NC: Duke University Press. Couldry, Nick (2014) ‘The Myth of “Us”: Digital Networks, Political Change and the Production of Collectivity’. Information, Communication & Society 18(6): 608–26. Couldry, Nick and Hepp, Andreas (2013) ‘Conceptualizing Mediatization: Contexts, Traditions, Arguments’. Communication Theory 23(3): 191–202. Curran, James, Fenton, Natalie and Freedman, Des (2012) Misunderstanding the Internet. London and New York: Routledge. Cvetkovich, Ann (2003) An Archive of Feelings: Trauma, Sexuality and Lesbian Public Cultures. Durham: Duke University Press. Daston, Lorraine and Galison, Peter cited in Halpern, Orit (2015) Beautiful Data: A History of Vision and Reason since 1945. Durham: Duke University Press.
Bibliography
213
Davies, Will (2015) The Happiness Industry: How the Government and Big Business Sold Us Well-Being. London: Verso Books. De Landa, Manuel (1991) War in the Age of Intelligent Machines. New York: Zone Books. Deleuze, Gilles (1992) ‘Postscript on the Societies of Control’. October 59(Winter): 3–7. Deleuze, Gilles and Guattari, Félix (1980) A Thousand Plateaus. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. Derrida, Jaques (1995) ‘Archive Fever: A Freudian Impression’. Diacritics 25(2): 9–63. Published by: The Johns Hopkins University Press. doi:10.2307/465144, Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/465144. Derrida (1994) Specters of Marx: The State of the Debt, The Work of Mourning and the New International. London and New York: Routledge. Despret, Vinciane (2016) What Would Animals Say If We Asked the Right Questions? Minneapolis: Minnesota Press. Despret, Vinciane (2015) ‘Who Made Clever Hans Stupid’. Angelaki: Journal for the Theoretical Humanities 20(2): 77–85. Despret, Vinciane (2013) ‘Responding Bodies and Partial Affinities in HumanAnimal Worlds’. Theory, Culture & Society 30(7–8): 51–76. Despret, Vinciane (2008) ‘The Becomings of Subjectivity in Animal Worlds’. Subjectivity 23: 123–39. Despret, Vinciane (2004a) ‘The Body We Care for: Figures of Anthropo-ZooGenesis’. Body & Society 10(2–3): 111–34. Despret, Vinciane (2004b) Our Emotional Make-up: Ethnopsychology and Selfhood. New York: Other Press. Deuze, Mark (2012) Media Life. London and New York: Polity. Dewey, Caitlin ‘Mark Zuckerberg Thinks Telepathy is the Future, Here’s How It Could Actually Work’. Sydney Morning Herald, July 2, 2015. Dossey, Larry. (2011) ‘Why are Scientists Afraid of Daryl Bem?’. Explore 7(3): 127–37. Doyen, S., Klein, O., Pichon, C.-L. and Cleeremans, A. (2012) ‘Behavioral Priming: It’s All in the Mind, but Whose Mind?’ PLoS ONE 7(1): e29081. https://doi.org /10.1371/journal.pone.0029081 Ebert, Jeffrey P. and Wegner, Daniel M. (2011) ‘Bending Time to One’s Will’. In: W. Sinnott-Armstrong and L. Nadel (eds) Conscious Will and Responsibility. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Ehrenreich, Barbara (2010) Bright-Sided: How the Relentless Promotion of Positive Thinking has Undermined America. New York: Picador. Erickson, Milton and Rossi, Eric. (1981) Experiencing Hypnosis: Therapeutic Approaches to Altered States. New York: Irvington. Evans, Elizabeth (2011) Transmedia Television: Audiences, New Media, and Daily Life. New York: Taylor and Francis.
214
Bibliography
Faber, Roland and Goffey, Andrew (eds) (2014) The Allure of Things: Process and Object in Contemporary Philosophy. London and New York: Bloomsbury Academic. Fanon, Franz (1967) Black Skin, White Masks. London and New York: Pluto Press. Festinger, Leon (1957) A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Festinger, Leon (1949) ‘The Analysis of Sociograms Using Matrix Algebra’. Human Relations 2(2): 153–8. Fisher, Mark (2017) The Weird and the Eerie, London: Repeater Books. Fisher, Mark (2012) Ghosts of My Life. London: Zero Books. Foucault, Michel (2003) Society Must Be Defended: Lectures at the Collège de France, 1975–76. New York: Picador. Foucault, Michel (1980) Power/Knowledge: Selected Interviews and Other Writings. USA: Pantheon Books. Franklin, Sarah, Lury, Celia and Stacey, Jackie (2000) Global Nature, Global Culture. London and New York: Sage. Freeman, Elizabeth (2010) Time Binds: Queer Temporalities, Queer Histories. Durham, NC: Duke University Press. Frost, Samantha (2016) Biocultural Creatures. Durham, NC: Duke University Press. Fuller, Matt (2009) http://www.spc.org/fuller/texts/active-data-and-its-afterlives. Galloway, Alexander R. and Thacker, Eugene (2007) The Exploit: A Theory of Networks. Minneapolis: Minnesota Press. Game, Ann (2001) ‘Riding: Embodying the Centaur’. Body & Society 70(4): 1–12. Gerlitz, Carolin and Lury, Celia (2014) ‘Social Media and Self-Evaluating Assemblages: On Numbers, Orderings and Value’. Distinktion: Journal of Social Theory 15(2): 174–88. Giraud, Eve (2015) ‘Subjectivity 2.0: Digital Technologies, Participatory Media and Communicative Capitalism’. Subjectivity 8: 124–46. Gitelman, Lisa (ed.) (2013) Raw Data is an Oxymoron. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Gitelman, Lisa and Virgina Jackson (2013) ‘Introduction: Raw Data Is an Oxymoron’. In: Lisa Gitelman (ed.), Raw Data Is an Oxymoron. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press, 1–14. Gladwell, Malcolm (2007) Blink: A Study of Thinking Without Thinking. New York: Back Bay Books. Gordon, Avery (2008) Ghostly Matters: Haunting and the Sociological Imagination. Minneapolis: Minnesota Press. Gough, Brenda and McFadden, Majella. (2001) Critical Social Psychology: An Introduction. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan. Gregg, Melissa (2011) Work’s Intimacy. London and New York: Polity Press.
Bibliography
215
Gregg, Melissa and Gregory, Seigworth (eds) (2010) The Affect Theory Reader. Durham: Duke University Press. Grosz, Elizabeth (2017) The Incorporeal: Ontology, Ethics and the Limits of Materialism. Columbia: Columbia University Press. Grusin, Richard (2015) ‘Radical Mediation’. Critical Inquiry 42(1): 124–48. Grusin, Richard (2010) Premediation: Affect and Mediality after 9/11. Basingstoke and New York: Palgrave. Gutting, Gary (2015) What Philosophy Can do? London and New York: W.W. Norton and Co. Hacking, Ian (1990) The Taming of Chance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Hacking, Ian (1988) ‘Telepathy: Origins of Randomization in Experimental Design’. Isis 79(3): 427–51. doi:10.1086/354775. Halberstam, Jack (2010) Queer Art of Failure. Durham: Duke University Press. Halberstam, Jack (2005) In a Queer Time and Place: Transgender Bodies, Subcultural Lives. New York: New York University Press. Halewood, Mick (2011) A. N. Whitehead and Social Theory: Tracing a Culture of Thought. London and New York: Anthem Press. Hall, Stuart (1988) The Hard Road to Renewal: Thatcherism and the Crisis of the Left. London: Verso. Halpern, Orit (2015) Beautiful Data: A History of Vision and Reason since 1945. Durham: Duke University Press. Hameroff, S. (2012) ‘How Quantum Brain Biology can Rescue Conscious Free Will’. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience 6: 93. http://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2012.00093 Hansen, Mark B. N. (2015) Feedforward: On the Future of Twenty-First-Century Media. Chicago: University Of Chicago Press. Hanson, Mark (2015) Feed-forward: On the Future of 21st Century Media. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Haraway, Donna (2013) ‘SF: Science Fiction, Speculative Fabulation, String Figures, So Far’. Ada. A Journal of Gender, New Media and Technology. No.3. doi:10.7264/ N3KH0K81. Haraway, Donna (2007) When Species Meet. Minneapolis: Minnesota Press. Haraway, Donna (2003) The Companion Species Manifesto: Dogs, People and Significant Otherness. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Haraway, Donna (1997) Modest_Witness@Second_Millennium.FemaleMan_Meets_ OncoMouse. Feminism and Technoscience. London and New York: Routledge. Haraway, Donna (1990) Primate Visions: Gender, Race and Nature in the World of Modern Science. London and New York: Routledge. Henriques, Julian, Hollway, Wendy, Urwin, Cathy, Venn, Couze and Walkerdine, Valerie (1984) Changing the Subject: Psychology, Social Regulation and Subjectivity. London: Methuen Press.
216
Bibliography
Hillis, Ken, Passonen, Susanna and Petit, Michael (eds) (2015) Networked Affect. Cambridge, MA and London: The MIT Press. Hochman, Nadav; MANOVICH, Lev. Zooming into an Instagram City: Reading the local through social media. First Monday, [S.l.], june 2013. ISSN 13960466. Available at: http://firstmonday.org/ojs/index.php/fm/article/view/4711 (accessed 11 May 2017). doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.5210/fm.v18i7.4711. Honderich, Ted (2005) On Determinism and Freedom. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. Hornaday, William T. (1922) The Mind and Manners of Wild Animals. New York: C. Scribner’s Sons. James, William (1890) The Principles of Psychology. New York: Holt Press. Jeffereys, Sheila (2009) The Industrial Vagina: The Political Economy of the Global Sex Trade. London New York: Routledge. Jenkins, Henry (2006) ‘Searching for the Origami Unicorn: The Matrix and Transmedia Storytelling’. Convergence Culture: Where Old and New Media Collide. New York: New York University Press. Johnstone, John (1999) ‘Machinic Vision’. Critical Inquiry 26: 27–48. Kahnman, Daniel (2011) Thinking Fast and Slow. London: Penguin. Kember, Sarah and Zylinska, Joanna (2012) Life After New Media: Mediation as a Vital Process. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Kirby, Vicki (2011) Quantum Anthropologies: Life At Large. Durham, NC: Duke University Press. Knorr Cetina, Karin and Bruegger, Urs (2002) ‘Traders Engagements with Markets’. Theory, Culture & Society 19(5–6): 161–85. Knudsen B. T. and Stage C. (2012) ‘Contagious Bodies: An Investigation of Affective and Discursive Strategies in Contemporary Online Activism’. Emotion, Space and Society 5(3): 148–55. Lachapelle, Sophie and Healey, Jenna (2010) ‘On Hans, Zou and the others: Wonder Animals and the Question of Animal Intelligence in Early Twentieth-Century France’. Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological & Biomedical Sciences 41: 12–20. Landecker, Hannah (2016) ‘Antibiotic Resistance and the Biology of History’. Body & Society 22(4): 19–52. Langlois, Ganaele, Redden, Joanna and Elmer, Greg (2015) Compromised Data: From Social Media to Big Data. London and New York: Bloomsbury. Latour, Bruno (2005) Reassembling the Social: An Introduction to Actor Network Theory. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Latour, Bruno (2004) ‘How to Talk about the Body? The Normative Dimension of Science Studies’. Body & Society 10(2–3): 205–30. Latour, Bruno (1987) Science in Action. Harvard: Harvard University Press.
Bibliography
217
Lenoir, Tim (2010) ‘Introduction’. In: Hans-Jorg Rheinberger (ed.), An Epistemology of the Concrete: Twentieth Century Histories of Life. Durham: Duke University Press. Leys, Ruth (2017) The Ascent of Affect: Genealogy and Critique. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Leys, Ruth (2011) ‘The Turn to Affect: A Critique’. Critical Inquiry 37(Spring): 434–72. Leavenworth Lindgren, Maria (2011) ‘Transmedia Texts and Serialized Narratives’. In: Cyber Echoes (ed.), ‘Textual Echoes,’ special issue, Transformative Works and Cultures, no. 8. http://dx.doi.org/10.3983/twc.2011.0361. Love, Heather (2007) Feeling Backward: Loss and the Politics of Queer History. Harvard: Harvard University Press. Luckhurst, Roger (2002) The Invention of Telepathy 1870–1901. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Luhrmann, Tanya (2011) ‘Hallucinations and Sensory Overrides’. Annual Review of Anthropology 40: 71–85. Lury, Celia (2015) ‘Postscript: Beside(s) the Empirical’. In: B. T. Knudsen and C. Stage (eds), Affective Methodologies: Developing Cultural Research Strategies for the Study of Affect. Basingstoke and New York: Palgrave. Mackenzie, Adrian (2013) ‘Programming Subjects in the Regime of Anticipation: Software Studies and Subjectivity’. Subjectivity 6(4): 391–405. Manovich, Lev (2013) Software Takes Command. London and New York: Bloomsbury Academic. Marriot, David (2007) Haunted Life: Visual Culture and Black Modernity. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press. Martin, Emily (2013) ‘The Potentiality of Ethnography and the Limits of Affect Theory’. Current Anthropology 54(S7)(October): S149–S158. doi:10.1086/670388. Massumi, B. (2009) ‘National Enterprise Emergency. Steps towards an Ecology of Powers’. Theory, Culture & Society 26(6): 153–85. Massumi, Brian (2016) ‘Such as It Is: A Short Essay in Extreme Realism’. Body & Society 22(1): 115–27. Massumi, Brian (2015) Ontopower: War, Powers and the State of Perception. Durham: Duke University Press. Massumi, Brian (2002) Parables for the Virtual: Movement, Affect, Sensation. Durham, NC: Duke University Press. McCarthy, Anna (2008) ‘Stanley Milgram, Allan Funt and Me: Post-war Social Science and the First Wave of Reality TV.’ In: S. Murray and L. Ouellete (eds), Reality TV: Remaking Television Culture. New York: New York University Press. McDougall, Edward (1910) An Introduction to Social Psychology. London: Methuen. McRobbie, A. (2008) The Aftermath of Feminism. London and New York: Sage.
218
Bibliography
Meloni, M. (2016) Science and Social Values in Human Heredity from Eugenics to Epigenetics. Basingstoke and New York: Palgrave. Meloni, M., Williams, S. and Martin, P. (2016) ‘Biosocial Matters: Rethinking Sociology-Biology Relations in the Twenty-First Century’. The Sociological Review Monographs 64: 256–73. doi:10.1002/2059-7932.12024. Michaels, Mike and Rosengarten, Marsha (2012) ‘Medicine, Experimentation, Politics, Emergent Bodies’. Editorial for a special issue of Body & Society Medicine, Bodies, Politics: Experimentation and Emergence 18 (3–4): 1–17. Millard, Kathryn (2014) ‘Revisioning Obedience: Exploring the Role of Milgram’s Skills as a Filmmaker in Bringing his Shocking Narrative to Life’. Journal of Social Issues 70: 439–55. doi:10.1111/josi.12070. Moscovici, Serge (1985) The Age of the Crowd: A Historical Treatise on Mass Psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Munoz, Jose (2009) Cruising Utopia: The Then and There of Queer Futurity. New York: New York University Press. Murthy, Dhiraj (2013) Twitter: Social Communication in the Information Age. London and New York: Polity Press. Nosek, Brian A. and Bar-Anan, Yoav (5 May 2012) ‘Scientific Utopia: I. Opening Scientific Communication’. Psychological Inquiry. Available at SSRN: https://ssrn. com/abstract=2051047. Nosworthy, Cheryl (2013) A Geography of Horse-Riding: The Spacing of Affect, Emotion and (Dis)ability Identity through Horse-Human Encounters. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge University Press. Orr, Jackie (2006) Panic Diaries: A Genealogy of Panic Disorder. Durham, MA: Duke University Press. Paasonen, Susanna (2014) ‘Between Meaning and Mattering: On Affect and Online Porn’. Porn Studies 1(1–2): 136–42. Paasonen, Susanna (2011) Carnal Resonance: Affect and Online Pornography. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Palmas, Karl (2011) ‘Predicting What You’ll Do Tomorrow: Panspectric Surveillance and the Contemporary Corporation’. Surveillance and Society 8(3): 338–54. Papacharissi, Zissi (2012) ‘Without You I’m Nothing: Performance of the Self on Twitter’. International Journal of Communication 6: 189–206. Papoulias, Constantina and Callard, Felicity (2010) ‘Biology’s Gift: Interrogating the Turn to Affect’. Body & Society 16(1): 29–56. Papanicolaou, Andrew C. and Gunter, P. A. Y. (1987) Bergson and Modern Thought: Towards a Unified Science: Models of Scientific Thought. Chur, Switzerland: Harwood Academic.
Bibliography
219
Parisi, Luciana (2013) Contagious Architecture: Computation, Aesthetics and Space. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Penrose, R. (1994) Shadows of the Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Penrose, R. (1989) The Emperor’s New Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Penrose, R. (1987) ‘Newton, Quantum Theory and Reality’. In: S. W. Hawking and W. Israel (eds), 300 Years of Gravity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 17–49. Peters, John Durham (1999) Speaking into the Air: The History of the Idea of Communication. Chicago: Chicago University Press. Pfungst, Oscar (1911) Clever Hans (the Horse of Mr von Osten): A contribution to American Animal and Human Psychology. New York: Henry Holt and Co. Potter, Jonathon and Wetherell, Margaret (1987) Discourse and Social Psychology: Beyond Attitudes and Behaviour. London and New York: Sage. Preciado, Paul (2013) Testo-Junkie: Sex, Drugs and Biopolitics in the Pharmacopornographic Era. New York: The Feminist Press. Protevi, J. (2009) Political Affect: Connecting the Social and the Somatic. Minneapolis: Minnesota Press. Pykett, Jessica, Jones, Rhys and Whitehead, Mark (eds) (2017) Psychological Governance and Public Policy. London: Routledge. Rabinow, Paul (1996) Essays on the Anthropology of Reason. Princeton: Princeton University Press. Radin, Dean (1997) The Conscious Universe; the Scientific Truth of Psychic Phenomena. San Francisco: Harper Edge. Reicher, S. (2001) The Psychology of Crowd Dynamics. In: Blackwell Handbook of Social Psychology: Group Processes. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing, 182–208. Rheinberger, Hans-Jorg (2010) An Epistemology of the Concrete: Twentieth Century Histories of Life. Durham: Duke University Press. Rheinberger, Hans-Jorg (1994) ‘Experimental Systems: Historiality, Narration and Deconstruction’. Science in Context 7(1): 65–81. Rhine, J. B. (1937) New Frontiers of the Mind. New York: Fararr and Rinehart. Rhine, J. B. and Rhine, Louisa E. (1929a) ‘An Investigation of a ‘Mind-Reading’ Horse’. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 23: 466. Rhine, J. B. and Rhine, Louisa E. (1929b) ‘Second Report on Lady’. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 24: 292. Richards, Graham (1997) Race, Racism and Psychology: Towards a Reflexive History. London and New York: Routledge. Rieder, Bernhard (2012) ‘What is a PageRank? A Historical and Conceptual Investigation of a Recursive Status Index’. Computational Culture: A Journal of Software Studies. http://computationalculture.net/what_is_in_pagerank.
220
Bibliography
Ritchie, S. J., Wiseman, R. and French, C. C. (2012) ‘Failing the Future: Three Unsuccessful Replications of Bem’s “Retroactive Facilitation of Recall” Effect’. PLoS ONE 7(3): e33423. Rose, Nikolas (1990) Inventing Ourselves: Psychology, Power and Personhood. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Rose, Nikolas (1989) Governing the Soul: The Shaping of the Private Self. London and New York: Free Association Books. Rose, Nikolas (1985) The Psychological Complex: Psychology, Politics and Society in England, 1869–1939. London and New York: Routledge. Rose, Nikolas and Abi-Rached, Joelle. (2013) Neuro: The New Brain Sciences and the Management of Mind. Princeton: Princeton University Press. Rosen, Sidney (2010) My Voice Will Go with You: The Teaching Tales of Milton H Erickson. London: W. W. Norten and Co. Rosenthal, Robert (1966) Experimenter Effects in Behavioral Research. East Norwalk, CT, US: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Rossi, E. L. and Erickson, M. (eds) (1980) Innovative Psychotherapy The Collected Papers of Milton H. Erickson on Hypnosis, Vol. IV. New York: Irvington. Rotman, Brian (2008) Becoming Beside Ourselves. Durham: Duke University Press. Ruppert, Evelyn, Law, John and Savage, Mike (2013) ‘Reassembling Social Science Methods: The Challenge of Digital Devices’. Theory, Culture & Society 30(4): 22–46. Samhita, L. and Gross, H. J. (2013) ‘The “Clever Hans Phenomenon” Revisited’. Communicative & Integrative Biology 6(6): e27122. doi:10.4161/cib.27122. Sampson, Tony (2012) Virality: Contagion Theory in the Age of Networks. Minneapolis, MN: The Minnesota Press. Savransky, Martin (2018) ‘Isabelle Stengers and the Dramatization of Philosophy’. Special issue of the journal SubStance: A Review of Theory and Literary Criticism 47(1): issue 145, 3–172. Savransky, Martin (2016) The Adventure of Relevance: An Ethics of Social Inquiry. Basingstoke and New York: Palgrave Macmillan. Schrader, Astrid (2012) ‘Haunted Measurements: Demonic Work and Time in Experimentation’. Differences: A Journal of Feminist Cultural Studies 23(3): 119–60. doi:10.1215/10407391-1892916. Schulman, Sarah (2012) Gentrification of the Mind: Witness to a Lost Imagination. Oakland: University of California Press. Schwartz, Jeffrey M., Stapp, Henry P. and Beauregard, Mario (2005) ‘Quantum Theory in Neuroscience and Psychology: A Neurophysical Model of Mind/Brain Interaction’. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 360(1458): 1309–27. Sconce, Jeffrey (2000) Haunted Media: Electronic Presence From Telegraphy to Television. Durham, MA: Duke University Press.
Bibliography
221
Seigworth, Gregg and Tiessen, Matthew. (2012) ‘Mobile Affects, Open Secrets, and Mobile Illiquidity: Pockets, Pools and Plasma’. Theory, Culture & Society 29(6): 47–77. Shaviro, Stephen (2015) Discognition. New York: Repeater Books. Shaviro, Stephen (2010) Post-Cinematic Affect. London and New York: Zero Books. Sheldrake, Rupert (1999) Dogs who Know when their Owners are Coming Home and Other Unexplained Powers of Animals. New York: Crown. Skeggs, B. and Yuill, S. (2015) ‘Capital Experimentation with Person A/formation: How Facebook’s Monetization Refigures the Relationship between Property, Personhood and Protest’. Information, Communication & Society 19(3): 380–96. Smail, Daniel (2007) On Deep History and the Brain. Los Angeles: University of California Press. Solomons, L. and Stein, G. (1896) ‘Normal Motor Automatism’. Psychological Review 3: 492–512. Sommer, Andreas (2014) ‘Psychical Research in the History and Philosophy of Science: An Introduction and Review’. Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences 48: 38–45. Srebeny-Mohammedi, Anabelle (1994) Small Media, Big Revolution: Communication, Culture and the Iranian Revolution. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. Stapp, Henry P. (2011) ‘Apparent Retrocausation as a Consequence of Orthodox Quantum Mechanics Refined to Accommodate the Principle of Sufficient Reason’. https://arxiv.org/abs/1111.6584. Steiner, Rudolf (1988) Bee-Keeping: Nine Lectures on Bees. Blauvelt: Garber Communications. Stengers, Isabelle (2014) Thinking with Whitehead: A Free and Wild Creation of Concepts. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Stengers, Isabelle (2000) The Invention of Modern Science. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press. Stengers, Isabelle (1997) Power and Invention: Situating Science. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press. Stengers, Isabelle and Despret, Vinciane (2014) Women who Make a Fuss: The Unfaithful Daughters of Virgina Woolf. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. Taylor, Richard and Sunstein, Cass (2009) Nudge: Improving Decisions about Health, Wealth and Happiness. London: Penguin. Terrenova, Tiziana (2012) ‘Attention, Economy and the Brain’. Culture Machine, North America, 1–19. Terranova, Tiziana (2004) Network Culture: Politics for the Information Age. London and New York: Pluto Press. Thrift, Nigel (2007) Non-Representational Theory: Space, Politics, Affect. London and New York: Routledge.
222
Bibliography
Valentine, Elizabeth (2012) ‘Editorial’. Special issue on the relations between psychical research and academic psychology in Europe, the USA and Japan. History of the Human Sciences 25(2): 1. Van Mourik Broekman, P., Hall, G., Byfield, T., Hides, S. and Worthington, S. (2014) Open Education: A Study in Disruption. London: Roman and Littlefield. Venn, Couze (2010) ‘Individuation, Relationality, Affect: Rethinking the Human in Relation to the Living’. Body & Society 16(1): 129–62. Venturini, Thomas (2010) ‘Diving in Magma: How to Explore Controversies with Actor-Network Theory’. Public Understanding of Science 19(3): 258–73. Wagenmakers, Eric–Jan, Wetzels, Ruud, Borsboom, Denny and van der Maas, Han L. J. (2011) ‘Why Psychologists must Change the Way they Analyze their Data: The Case of psi: Comment on Bem (2011)’. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 100(3): 426–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0022790 Walkerdine, Valerie and Jimenez, Luis (2012) Gender, Work and Community after De-industrialisation: A Psychosocial Approach to Affect. London: Palgrave Macmillan. Walsh, E., Mehta, M., Oakley, D. A., Guilmette, D. N., Gabay, A., Halligan, P. W. and Deeley, Q. (2014) ‘Using Suggestion to Model Different Types of Automatic Writing’ Consciousness and Cognition 26(N/A): 24–36. doi:10.1016/j. concog.2014.02.008. Watt, Peter (2006) Blindsight. New York: Tor Books. Wetherell, Margaret (2012) Affect and Emotion: A New Social Science Understanding. London and New York: Sage. Wetherell, M. (2013) ‘Affect and Discourse – What’s the Problem? From Affect as Excess to Affective/Discursive Practice’. Subjectivity 6: 349. https://doi. org/10.1057/sub.2013.13 North Whitehead, Albert (1929) Process and Reality: An Essay in Cosmology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Whitehead, Mark Jones, Jones, Rhys, Lilley, Rachel, Pykett, Jessica and Howell, Rachel (2017) Neuroliberalism: Behavioural Government in the Twenty-First Century. London: Routledge. Wilson, E. (2015) Gut Feminism. Durham: Duke University Press. Yerushalmi, Y. (1991) Freud’s Moses. New Haven: Yale University Press. Zylinska, Joanna (2014) Minimal Ethics for the Anthropocene. Ann Arbor: Open Humanities Press.
Index affect vii, viii, x, xi, xii, xiii, xiv, xv, xvi, xvii, xviii, xxi, xxii, xxiii, 3–4, 6–12, 15–17, 20, 23–4, 28, 30, 32–3, 36–7, 40, 42, 51–3, 62, 65–6, 70, 72, 75–7, 79, 85, 89, 91, 94, 96, 103, 105, 110–11, 113–14, 116–22, 135–6, 143, 149, 155–7, 160, 162, 164, 166, 171–3, 177–80, 183–8, 191, 195–6, 198, 201–3 affect studies 51, 130, 135, 149, 155, 157, 162, 166, 174–5, 183, 191, 196, 202, 207. See also half-second delay; networked affect aggregation 55–6, 126–7, 150, 204 Ahmed, Sara xiv, 15, 72, 85, 171, 188–9 algorithms xx, 9, 31, 33, 49, 56, 81, 85, 114–15, 119, 161, 193 alien phenomenologies vi–viii, 4, 30, 146, 164 archives of the future xviii, 17, 25, 27, 32, 49, 67, 134, 143, 150, 166, 176 artificial intelligence 150. See also machine-learning automaticity vi–vii, 21, 30, 32, 44, 65, 71, 111, 145, 146, 184, 196 Barad, Karen xii, xvi, 3, 18, 23–4, 39–40, 90, 129, 135, 138–42, 144–7, 150, 152–4, 159–60, 175, 188, 205 Berlant, Lauren 14, 15, 141, 187 big data xix, xx, xxi, 90, 104, 119–20, 126, 187, 193, 206–7 biohumanities xv, xviii, xxiv, 17, 65–6, 91, 105, 135, 142, 160 Butler, Judith 14, 72, 85, 139, 140–2, 153, 186 Chow, Rey 21–3, 77, 104, 168–70, 208 Clever Hans xiii, 28, 34–5, 42, 52–3, 58, 60–2, 66, 68–71, 76, 128, 198. See also Hans the Horse conspiracy theories 123, 151
contagion vii, ix, x, xxi, 11, 29, 51, 66, 88, 103, 111, 113, 114, 120, 164, 184, 185, 192. See also suggestion counterfactual 123, 126, 131, 145, 162, 177, 180 cultural invitation 65–6, 203 Despret, Vinciane xii, xiii, 16–18, 46, 52, 66–7, 69–74, 76, 87, 98, 99, 117, 144, 175, 187, 196, 198–9, 201 dispositive 44, 46, 76, 116, 196 experimental device 98 extra-sensory perception vi, 94 futurology 79, 82–3 Gordon, Avery 16, 20, 170–2, 191 governmentality 12, 189. See also psychological governance; psychomediation half-second delay 110, 155, 157, 183, 202, 207. See also affect Hans the Horse 44, 52–3, 58–9, 70, 75. See also Clever Hans Haraway, Donna xi, xii, xvi, 18, 39, 66, 90, 175 hauntology xxii, 17, 18, 19, 21, 23, 33, 43, 54, 57, 76, 162, 165, 166, 167, 170, 172, 173, 205 Hawthorne effect 45–6 historiality xxi, 41, 60, 75, 77, 147, 149, 156, 161, 165, 173, 175 hypnosis xiii, 5, 11, 30, 63–5, 72 inventive propositions 17, 102–3, 105, 181 machine-learning 81. See also artificial intelligence McRobbie, Angela 14
224
Index
Martin, Emily 32, 117, 121 material-semiotic apparatus 69, 97, 102, 115, 121, 129, 130, 141 mediation viii, ix, x, 3, 16, 20, 22, 26, 30, 32, 50–1, 53–4, 57, 83–4, 90, 95, 101, 110, 113, 115, 118–20, 135, 137, 139–40, 142, 161, 164, 166, 171, 179–81, 185–6, 195 mediatisation 101, 140, 169 networked affect 29, 164, 192. See also affect new materialism(s) xii, xvi, xvii, xviii, xxiii, 3, 17, 122–3, 129, 135–6, 139–40, 142–3, 160, 178–9 non-local consciousness 4, 122, 123, 134–6 Nudge politics 7, 30, 179. See also priming Open science 26, 31, 86, 123, 132, 175–6, 191 Papacharissi 36–7, 191 pharmacopornographic era 119 pornception 104–7, 112–21, 124, 130. See also time-travelling porn post-truth x, xxii, 80–6, 88, 95, 103, 121, 162, 166, 177, 181 precognition 66, 79, 83, 88, 90, 92, 96–7, 106–7, 109, 111, 117, 120, 123–4, 127, 131, 145, 148–51, 157, 160, 163, 198, 204. See also retroactive causation priming 4, 7–9, 28–30, 32, 34–8, 40, 42, 44–50, 52–3, 67, 71, 75–6, 83, 101–2, 109–10, 124, 128, 134, 149, 160, 163, 179, 188, 192, 197, 202, 204, 207. See also Nudge politics proto-performative 15, 66, 128, 140, 149, 158, 162 psychic animals 28, 62, 71, 128–9, 198 psychological governance 7, 189. See also governmentality; psychomediation psychological individualism vii, xiv, 66, 121, 135, 143, 187 psychomediation x, 7, 11, 185. See also psychological governance
quantum entanglement 9, 84, 95, 150–1, 159, 161 queer aggregations xi, 24, 25, 41, 56, 88, 126, 176, 187, 207 queer theory 12, 14, 77, 139, 188 racism(s) 8, 80, 168, 188–9 regimes of anticipation 79, 81, 83, 113. See also speculative forecast re-moving 19, 39, 41, 45, 58, 94, 135, 149, 163 replication 28, 34–6, 39, 44–5, 47–8, 50–1, 91, 102, 109, 123–5, 131–2, 135, 151, 172, 201, 204 retroactive causation 158, 163. See also precognition Rheinberger, Hans-Jorg xii, xxi, 39–41, 43, 51, 60, 69, 102, 161, 173, 175 scenes of entanglement 23, 41, 51, 57–8, 84, 164, 169, 170, 174 shadow media 23, 104, 169 speculative forecast xxi, 81–2, 85, 96, 130–1, 161, 179. See also regimes of anticipation statistical imaginaries 124, 161, 164, 179, 121, 123 statistical reason 127, 130–1 Stengers, Isabelle xii, xiii, 11, 16, 17, 66, 97, 99, 129, 137, 158, 175, 178, 207 suggestion vii, ix, xvi, xxi, 7, 10, 11, 45, 63, 64, 65, 66, 67, 72, 73, 110, 129, 178, 184, 185, 188. See also contagion telepathy 5, 62–4, 66, 71, 107, 123, 126, 128–9, 163, 189, 206 Terrenova, Tiziana 30, 187 time-travelling 60, 90, 103, 105–7, 109, 118, 120, 157 time-travelling porn 105–7, 109, 120. See also pornception transmediality 21–3, 169–70 transmedial 11, 21–2, 51, 54, 77, 169–70, 172, 175 Whitehead, Albert, North viii, xii, 97, 136, 137, 138, 153, 157, 158, 159, 185, 207
225
226
227
228
229
230