230 44 38MB
English Pages 552 [560] Year 2004
Handbook of m
f-'*.
i f^
itedby
RANDALL W. ENGLE
Boston Public Library Boston iviA U2>16 »
i
Handbook of
Understanding and Measuring Intelligence
Handbook of
Understand ng and Measuring i
Edited by
OLIVER WILHELM • RANDALL W. ENGLE Humboldt-University, Berlin,
Germany
Georgia Institute cf Technology
®SAGE Publications New Thousand Oaks
London
Delhi
© 2005 by Sage Publications,
Copyright
All rights reserved.
No
part of this
Inc.
book may be reproduced or
utilized in
any form or by any means, electronic
or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, or by any information storage and retrieval system, without
permission
in writing
from the publisher.
For information:
Sage Publications,
Inc.
2455 Teller Road
Thousand Oaks, California 91320 E-mail: [email protected] Sage Publications Ltd. 1
Oliver's Yard
55 City Road
London EC lY ISP United Kingdom
Sage Publications India
Pvt. Ltd.
B-42, Panchsheel Enclave Post
Box 4109
New
Delhi 110 017
India
Printed in the United States of America
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
Handbook of understanding and measuring
intelligence
/
edited by Oliver
Wilhelm, Randall W. Engle. p.
cm.
Includes bibliographical references and index.
ISBN 0-7619-2887-1 1.
Intellect. 2.
(pbk.) Intelligence tests.
I.
Wilhelm, Oliver.
II.
Engle,
Randall W.
BF431.H3 188 2004 153.9—dc22 2004013896
04
05
06
07
Acquiring Editor:
10
9
7
6
5
3
Jim Brace-Thompson
Editorial Assistant:
Karen Ehrmann
Production Editor:
Diana E. Axelsen
Copy
Gillian Dickens
Editor:
4
Typesetter:
C&M Digitals (P) Ltd.
Indexer:
David Luljak
Cover Designer:
Michelle Lee Kenny
2
1
Contents
Preface
1.
vii
Intelligence:
A
Diva and a Workhorse and Randall W. Engle
1
Oliver Wilhelm 2.
Assessing Problem Solving Patrick C. Kyllonen
3.
Mental Speed:
in Context and Soonmook Lee
On Frameworks,
1
Paradigms,
27
and a Platform for the Future Vanessa Danthiir, Richard D. Roberts,
Ralf Schulze, and Oliver Wilhelm 4.
Cognitive Mechanisms Underlying Intelligence:
47
Defense of a Reductionist Approach Andrew R. A. Conway 5.
Working Memory Capacity, Attention Control, and Fluid Intelligence Richard P. Heitz, Nash Unsworth, and Randall
61 W. Engle
6.
Emotional Intelligence: An Elusive Ability? Gerald Matthews, Moshe Zeidner, and Richard D. Roberts
1
Metacognition and Intelligence Christopher Hertzog and A. Emanuel Robinson
101
Knowledge and
125
.
8.
Phillip L. 9.
Intelligence
Ackerman and Margaret
Full Frontal Fluidity?
Looking
in
E. Beier
on the Neuroimaging
of Reasoning and Intelligence
Michael 10.
J.
11.
A
141
Kane
Behavioral Genetics and Intelligence Stephen A.
79
165
Petrill
Dialectical Constructivist
View
of Developmental Intelligence
Juan Pascual-Leone and Janice Johnson
177
12.
Development of
Intellectual Abilities in
From Age Gradients
Old Age: 203
to Individuals
Martin Lovden and Ulman Lindenberger 13.
Group Differences
in Intelligence
and Related Measures
223
Werner W. Wittmann 14.
Modeling Structures of Intelligence
241
RalfSchulze 15.
Item Response Theory and the Measurement of Cognitive Processes
265
Florian Schmiedek 16.
g Factor: Issues of Design and Interpretation
279
Lazar Stankov 17.
Capturing Successful Intelligence Through Measures of Analytic. Creative, and Practical Skills P. J.
18.
Henry; Robert
J.
Sternberg,
and Elena
295 L.
Grigorenko
Faceted Models of Intelligence
313
Heinz-Martin Sufi and Andre Beauducel 19.
Assessing Intelligence: Past. Present, and Future Richard D. Roberts, Pippa M. Markham,
333
Gerald Matthews, and Moshe Zeidner 20.
The Role of Domain Knowledge David
21.
Z.
in
Higher-Level Cognition
361
Hambrick
373
Measuring Reasoning Ability Oliver Wilhelm
22.
The Measurement of Working Memory Capacity
393
Klaus Oberauer 23.
Working Memory, H. Lee
24.
and Learning Disabilities
Cognitive Ability in Selection Decisions Deniz
25.
Intelligence,
409
Swanson
S.
431
Ones, Chockalingam Viswesvaran, and Stephan Dilchert
Understanding Intelligence:
A Summary
and an Adjustable-Attention Hypothesis
469
Nelson Cowan 26.
To g
or
Not
to
g
—That
Is the
Question
489
Nathan Brody
About the Editors
503
About the Contributors
504
Author Index
509
Subject Index
529
Preface
The
test
in the
of a first-rate intelligence
mind
same
at the
time,
and
is
the ability to hold
still
two opposed ideas
retain the ability to fimction. Scott Fitzgerald
F.
Although there has been great progress in intelHgence research over the past years,
and although there
and what this
book,
it
we want
might
is
some consensus on how
reflect, there are
many
and measuring intelligence. Besides an overview of the
we
perspective on prospective
work
approach serves two goals.
On one
in
both understanding
status
quo of knowledge
also want to provide a comprehensive
remains to be accomplished. This
in the field that
hand, intelligence assessment
making
tance for students interested in
100
measure intelligence
important questions unanswered. In
been achieved
to highlight progress that has
about individual differences in intelligence,
to
is
of major impor-
a career in psychology, education, or other
behavioral sciences. Being familiar with fundamental issues of understanding and
measuring intelligence
is critical
construct of intelligence
is
and outside of psychology.
emerging problems
Some of outline how
for
many
practical activities.
A
the chapters focus
be helpful for
many
applied and scientific decisions.
on the theoretical and construct
the understanding of intelligence can be
logical
in
where we
felt
it
Proposals for cal
intelligence,
was
These chapters
and how dealing with these aspects helps us to measure statistical
intelli-
and pragmatic
measuring intelligence and outline methodological and techno-
improvements and how these contribute
ligence. In this preface,
level.
improved by incorporating certain
gence more thoroughly. Other chapters focus on fundamental assessment problems
the other hand, the
basic understanding of the established consensus and
in the field will
theoretical perspectives
On
of relevance for several other scientific disciplines, within
we
briefly
go though
to
all
an improved understanding of
the chapters, grouping
intel-
them together
possible, to give an idea about the scope and content of this book.
new
constructs such as critical thinking, situational judgment, practi-
and emotional intelligence need
to
be evaluated thoroughly (see
Chapter 2 by Kyllonen and Lee; Chapter 17 by Henry, Sternberg, and Grigorenko: and
Chapter 6 by Matthews, Zeidner. and Roberts). Assessing and predicting these constructs
can be a promising new
problems
to
field.
As with
all
new developments,
be solved, on both the theoretical and operational
going to be very hard because established intelligence
been investigated for a long time.
We
maintain our standards before accepting
there are difficult
levels. Initially, this is
tests are very successful
and have
should applaud and support these efforts but
new
constructs as reflecting intelligence.
iii
.
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE In our efforts to understand intelligence,
we
are interested in unequivocally assigning
psychological meaning to terms such as intelligence, abstract thinking, reasoning, and so on. However, there
currently no clear consensus of just
is
exactly. Constructs such as mental speed (see
what these terms mean
Chapter 3 by Danthiir. Roberts, Schulze,
and Wilhelm), controlled attention (see Chapter 5 by Heitz, Unsworth, and Engle), and meta-cognition (see Chapter 7 by Hertzog and Robinson) are all candidates for important intelligence-related constructs
when
and candidates for explanations of what happens
test. The basic assumptions and the explanatory mechanisms proposed through such constructs to explain identical, similar, or related intelligence variables need to be explicit and falsifiable (see Chapter 4 by Conway). Focusing strictly on intelligence as cognitive processes and general resources seems to have implicitly excluded the area of knowledge. The measurement of knowledge is crucial because there can be no doubt that knowledge is required to succeed in specific (real-life) domains or in complex problem solving by virtue of an interaction with attentional capabilities. Knowledge is a key concept in understanding real-life performance, and its measurement has long been neglected in intelligence research. Focusing on domain-specific knowledge and its acquisition as an important component of intelligent behavior is likely to become more important in the future (see Chapter 8 by Ackerman and Beier, as well as Chapter 20 by Hambrick). The physiological substrate thought to be most crucial for intellectual functioning is
cognitively
taking an intelligence
the frontal lobes. Relating the functioning and malfunctioning of the frontal lobes to intelligence into
human
is
a specific
form of reductionism and can provide important new insights
cognition in general and individual differences in particular (see Chapter 9
by Kane).
The promise of fundamental new controversial technology of genetics intelligence (see Chapter 10
by
insights gained through the rapidly developing is
likely to
Petrill).
of the most promising completely
have a big effect on
how we
Research on the genetics of intelligence
new developments
and
think about is
one
in intelligence research.
Intelligence research has always profited from developmental contributions to the field. Intelligence testing, as
gogy.
On
Ebbinghaus and Binet invented
the other side, Piaget's developmental theory has
it.
its
has
its
origins in peda-
roots in the analysis of
errors of subjects in Binet's laboratory. Central concepts such as fluid
and crystallized
intelligence gained initial support largely by demonstrating different developmental trajectories in older age.
Two
chapters (Chapter
1 1
by Pascual-Leone and Johnson;
Chapter 12 by Lovden and Lindenberger) address the major recent psychological and methodological contributions of developmental psychology to our understanding of intelligence.
The observafion of group differences in intelligence is an important and pervasive The implications are far-reaching and of great importance in public
issue in testing. policy.
It
is
essential to develop a scientifically
sound understanding and provide
professional advice for policymakers. Although very controversial,
it
is
necessary to
discuss the issue, address scientifically established causes, and outline possible inter-
ventions (see Chapter 13 by Wittmann).
The methodological fundamentals of intelligence
testing
must be understood
any conclusions about theories of intelligence. The methods for dealing with traditionally twofold.
On
a test or subtest level, relations
interest in theories of intelligence structure (see
observational level, item response theory ric
is
between specific
Chapter 14 by Schulze).
crucial in test construction
to
draw
tests are
tests are
On
of
the lowest
and psychomet-
analysis (see Chapter 15 by Schmiedek). In both these areas, there has been sub-
two decades, and this is already affecting how we test our a basic knowledge of these two areas, it is pointless to try and
stantial progress in the past
theories.
Without
at least
Preface
work through many of
the chapters of this
book or any other advanced
treatise
on
intelHgence.
One
particularly controversial
problem
importance of the so-called general abstraction of a general factor, it
factor.
good
tests to
are necessary prerequisites to assess the
research
in intelligence
is
the nature and
Besides assigning meaning to the
statistical
and adequate methods
to extract
measure
it
magnitude and relevance of
this
concept (see
Chapter 16 by Stankov). Faceted models of intelligence,
initially
research, have developed into an arsenal of
intended as a specific theory of intelligence
methods and procedures
that are particularly
measurement approaches. Faceted models helped to put the focus of research efforts on the psychological level of intelligence research in that these models provided the methodological background for discussing intelligence tasks with respect to their psychological demands (see Chapter 18 by SuB and Beauducel). The influence of available intelligence tests on research can hardly be overestimated. useful in conceptualizing
Within intelligence research, as well as when intelligence measures are used areas of psychological research, the availability of measures constraint that has a strong impact on research
However, there
is
tests
established tests actually measure
Reasoning
outcomes as well as applied work.
is
of cognitive
abilities.
intelli-
Gaining insight into what
crucial in understanding
what
test results
mean
by Roberts, Markham, Matthews, and Zeidner).
ability is included in
every structural theory. In hierarchical, higher order,
and topographical theories of intelligence, reasoning ment. Similarly,
in other
an important pragmatic
an enormous gap between theoretically established models of
gence research and widely used (see Chapter 19
is
in
ability is central to the ability arrange-
applied settings, reasoning ability
is
usually the most potent predictor.
The structure of reasoning tests is debatable, however (see Chapter 21 by Wilhelm). Working memory is currently the most prominent candidate for the explanation of many crucial intelligence aspects. Working memory has been particularly fruitful in predicting and explaining intelligence. However, currently there is an inflation of working memory measures, and many of these measures (old and new) are psychometrically unsound and in desperate need of improvement (see Chapter 22 by Oberauer). Learning disabilities are an applied area of intelligence research with rapidly growing importance and relevance. The complicated problem of understanding and measuring specific learning disabilities can contribute importantly to our understanding of
unimpaired intelligence,
to distinguishing learning disabilities
from other cognitive
impairments, and to providing insight into details of the measurement and diagnosis of learning disabilities (see Chapter 23 by Swanson).
Another applied area deals with the validity and implications of intelligence. The contribution of intelligence to the explanation and prediction of real-life
been demonstrated field
in
many
fields.
phenomena has
These contributions are specifically important
in the
of personnel selection (see Chapter 24 by Ones, Viswesvaran, and Dilchert).
We
are particularly glad that
in the field
we were
able to convince
two outstanding researchers
of cognition and intelligence to be discussants for this book. The various
approaches to the understanding of intelligence are discussed and assessed
from Nelson Cowan (Chapter the presented approaches
is
25).
The focus here
likely to
in a
chapter
on the identification of which of deepen and improve our understanding of intelliis
The chapter discusses the contributions of Roberts, Markham, Matthews, and Zeidner; Conway; Heitz, Unsworth, and Engle; Kane; Oberauer; Wilhelm; Hertzog and Robinson; Ackerman and Beier; Hambrick: Pascual-Leone and Johnson; Lovden and Lindenberger; and Swanson. The contributions to the measuring of intelligence are discussed and assessed by Nathan Brody (Chapter 26). The focus here is on gence
in the future.
what we have learned from the measurement of intelligence so
far,
how we
should do
•
ix
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE it.
and how we can improve
it.
This chapter discusses the contributions of Danthiir.
Roberts. Schulze. and Wilhelm: Ones, Viswesvaran. and Dilchert; Petrill; Schulze;
Schmiedek: Stankov; Wittmann; SuB and Beauducel: Kyllonen and Lee; Henry, Sternberg, and Grigorenko; and Matthews, Zeidner, and Roberts. These two discussions conclude the book. Primarily, the
book
is
intended for use by advanced undergraduate and graduate
students, researchers, and professionals in psychology
meant
to provide a
background on recent research
and education. The book
in intelligence
develop a sound understanding of results and perspectives
More
generally, the
book
is
supposed to provide
and
is
to help readers to
in intelligence research.
scientists with
broad interests
in indi-
vidual differences, cognitive abilities, intelligence, educational measurement, thinking,
reasoning, or problem solving with a comprehensive description of the status quo and
prospects of intelligence research.
A number of individuals have helped
us substantially in the task of editing this book.
Vanessa Danthiir made important contributions
in skillfully
proofreading
all
chapters
and providing constructive suggestions well beyond simple orthography, layout
for
improvements where necessary, and careful copyediting. Jim Brace-Thompson. Alison Mudditt. Karen Ehrmann, and Gillian Dickens reacted flexibly and supportively to suggestions
we made
all
an effort to improve the book. Their constant support and great
in
editorial service helped us in focusing
on the content side of the book.
Editing this handbook has been an interesting, thought-provoking, and rewarding
experience.
We
would
like to express
of-the-art science to a broad audience to
our gratitude to the authors for providing
and for helping
be a lasting contribution to intelligence research.
this
volume helpful
human
intelligence.
in
to
We
make hope
this
state-
book what we hope
that all readers will find
gaining valuable insights into the theory and measurement of
We
will
be rewarded
provokes new and innovative research on
if this
book advances our
field
and
if
it
this topic.
— Oliver Wilhelm and Randall W. Engle
I
1 Intelligence:
A Diva and a Workhorse Oliver Wilhelm
Randall W. Engle
This
introduction will try to set the stage
for the subsequent chapters.
We
first dis-
cuss a few issues related to the measure-
ment of
intelligence before
commenting on
understanding of intelligence.
We
and behaviors.
and by no means exhaustive
A comprehensive of adjectives
list
includes the following: abstract, accurate, agile,
the
analytic, artistic, astute, attentive, aware, block-
do so because
headed, brainy, bright, broadminded. bubble-
intelligence testing has always been a very prag-
matic job. In an attempt to satisfy the ability testing, the
attributes, states,
demand
for
development, use, and evalua-
measurement tools has been a focal activof researchers working in the area. In the
headed,
captivating,
clumsy,
complex, confused, contemplative,
cerebral,
clever,
clod,
crafty, creative, cultured, deep, detail-oriented,
tion of
distractible,
ity
dull,
ditsy,
flawless,
doddery, doltish, dreamy,
exacting,
fanciful,
foolhardy,
early years of empirical psychological research,
foolish, foresighted, forgetful, fuddled, gifted,
was so success-
hot-headed, ignorant, imaginative, impercepti-
the
measurement of
ful,
with approaches related to
intelligence
its
understanding
so far from being satisfactory, that the
latter
was
overly neglected for a long period. Following these historical lines, efforts to the
we postpone more
recent
toward the understanding of intelligence
end of
ble,
imprudent,
instinctive, tive,
able,
impulsive,
innovative,
nious,
intellectual,
intuitive, inventive,
learned,
inattentive,
inquisitive,
logical,
intelligent,
inge-
insightful,
introspec-
jumbled, knowledge-
madcap, meditative,
mental, meticulous, mindless, musical, oafish,
this introduction.
original,
perceptive,
philosophical,
plodding,
poetic, pondering, pragmatic, precise, prudent,
The Measurement of Intelligence
questioning, quick-witted, reasonable, reckless, refined, responsive,
no lack
In English, as in other languages, there
is
of adjectives to describe intelligent
activities.
AUTHORS' NOTE:
Correspondence regarding
of Psychology. Humboidt-University.
this
scholarly, shallow, sharp-
witted, slow, sly, smart, sophisticated, talented,
uncreative,
uninhibited,
unreflective,
vigilant.
chapter should be addressed to Oliver Wilhelm. Department
Rudower Chaussee
18.
12489 Berlin. Germany. E-mail: oliver.wilhelm@
rz.hu-berlin.de. 1
2
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE
.
visional^', well-read, wise,
are just a 100 of
and
many more
in ever>'day
be used for self-reports
These
v\orldly.
adjectives that can
language as
well as in scientific studies of personality to
this
is
intuitively
Cronbach (1949)
plausible.
introduced the distinction between measures of typical
and maximal behavior. The distinction
between both forms of measurement
is
strongly
We
some more or
less dispositional fea-
associated with the content of a measure.
tures of individuals that
somehow have some-
usually assess typical behavior with self-reports
describe
of preferences and valences.
thing to do with intelligence.
As with many assessment procedures, we can use factor analysis and other multivariate
methods
to
summarize data on such
and we would
self-reports,
we
likely find various factors
might label motor
abilities,
precision in working
and thinking, knowledge, reasoning, memory, and the
attention,
on
self-ratings
like. Similarly,
collect
how
comother respondents, they would do on
to provide us with estimates on
pared to
we can
by asking respondents
abilities
We
certain tests.
well,
can help respondents provide
more accurate estimates by explaining ability constructs and giving them examples from actual
ability
We
tests.
to provide estimates of
can ask respondents
how many
ability test they solved correctly.
rely
items of an
We
can also
on the confidence with which individuals
respond to individual items of ability (Pallier et
al..
tests
We
ior
A
behavior,
with measuring
achievement, and proficien-
abilities, aptitude,
cies.
Maximal
side, is associated
prototypical measure of
maximal behav-
can be characterized by several features. The
assessed person
aware of the performance
is
appraisal and willing to perform as well as he
The standards
or she can.
for evaluating the
performance of a person are
explicit,
and the
assessed person has to exert effort to succeed
&
(see Sackett. Zedeck. lar
conceptualization).
Fogli, 1988. for a simi-
The pervasive
distincfion
between measures of maximal and measures of typical
behavior remains despite attempts to
assess constructs from one side of the divide
with methods from the other side of the divide.
There are other fundamental differences between measures of maximal and typical behavior.
Once
is
it
ensured that participants are
&
motivated to do well and have understood what
can also ask individuals to
they are asked to do. precision of measurement
2002; Stankov, 2000: Stankov
Crawford. 1997).
on the other
provide us with self-reports about their typical
engagement (Ackerman 1994: Goff & Ackerman, 1992), need
&
intellectual
Goff.
in the ability
tion,
there
domain can be very high. In addino need to be afraid of faked
is
for cogni-
answers after taking care of routine precautions
&
Jarvis,
because you can hardly pretend to be more able
1996). and openness for new ideas (Costa & McCrae. 1992: Saucier. 1992: Trapnell, 1994). However, empirical data suggest that none of
than you actually are by doing better than you
the above approaches provides an acceptable
and vulnerability
approximation to the actual intelligence
bias, is threatened so strongly that they
tion
as
(Cacioppo, Petty, Feinstein,
measured by standardized
correlations
level,
ability tests.
The
and standardized
ability tests are
tests.
well they do in a broad \ariety of ability
Apparently,
intellectual
asking
for
preferences in
engagement, self-ratings of
ability,
use of
proficiency measures
on the
how
such as response
is
jusfified
validity
and
ability, apfitude,
by the abundant evidence ufility
of such measures.
There can be no serious doubt that
ability tests
provide incrementally useful information that
would improve judgment and decision making. Still, ability tests
are not used for
all
decisions in
which they could provide useful informadon. For example, there are important cultural
from intelligence
ences
does not bridge the gap
betw een putati\e personalit}' and putative measures.
From
a psychometric
abilit>
perspective.
and
for practically important
or confidence rafings of responses to problems tests
cannot
Apart from the lack of vulnerability to some
decisions
to assess
to artifacts
critical biases, the
ronment constantly providing us with information about what we can accomplish and what we
humans do poorly w hen asked
the other hand, the structure of
be applied sensibly for many practical purposes.
usually disappointingly low. Despite our envi-
cannot,
On
self-report measures, as well as their validity
of self-reports of intellect, self-
estimates of ability, and confidence ratings for ability tests
actually can.
in the
differ-
frequency with which ability mea-
sures are used. These differences are not caused
by
cultural differences in the validity
and
utility
Intelligence:
A
Diva and a Workhorse
•
3
of intelligence measures: Those are quite stable.
possible to raise fundamental fairness problems
make use of intelligence measures
not only in the case of ability tests (Willingham,
It is
policy to
for high-stakes decisions in in
one culture but not
&
another (see Schuler, Frier,
Kauffmann,
1999) but also for other forms of information
used
high-stakes decisions affecting the lives
in
not clear, then, what causes
1993, for a comparison in the personnel selec-
of individuals.
tion context). Leaving the decision uninformed
and maintains the differences across countries
about the
of those
abilities
of the decision
likely to
is
who
are the subject
be less precise, less
with respect to the use of ability measures
social
norms and
it
may
be that cultural and
implicit policies place
more
importance on some aspects of selection than on abilities, the utility
of information provided by
can be considered a certainty across
ability tests
A somehow
It
frequently pointed out that predictions
is
based on ability
and
diction,
statement
is
it
tests
do not allow perfect pre-
important to realize that this
is
and
true
for a long time to
is
very likely to stay true
the frequency
is
and the validity of such measures. Rationally, is
—
ceteris paribus
to use the
to
most valid
you are
sions
—evident
in
that
you
it
want
will
of predictors for deci-
set
charge of Now, the knowledge
(set of) predictor(s) is actually
be maximally predictive
is
going
no secret science.
Psychologists around the world have argued and literally
thousands of studies demonstrate the
point over and over again:
You would
not want
to neglect intelligence as a predictor in almost
by such constructs as moti-
any selection decision because the validity of
vation and personality, all
problem
come. Because human behav-
ior is also affected
account for
related
with which various selection methods are used
of which
cultures.
in
high-stakes decisions.
dependable, subject to more biases, and less accurate. Although
It is
will
abilities
never
the \'ariance in performance. In
the prediction
is
going to be worse than
it
would
be with consideration of intelligence. Empiri-
gap between what
addition, to justify using ability measures, one
cally, there is a surprising
should demonstrate that they improve the pre-
should be done and what
diction of success, that they cannot be replaced
Leboyer, 1994; Schuler
by other predictors or combinations thereof,
most countries, most selection decisions are not
that
the costs are sufficiently smaller than the benefits,
and
that there
no fundamental problem
is
made using
done (Levy-
is
et al., 1993). In fact, in
the best predictors for success.
The most
surprising aspect about test use
with fairness in practical applications. Perceived
described above seems to be that no one
fairness of procedures in practical selection con-
surprised, worried, or shocked about those facts.
texts
might be the cause for differences
in the
use of psychometric ability measures across cultures. If this
were
should be a wide-
—a country
have not heard of
of "Test
initiatives
kid" in countries where ability testing
is
too
my
not a
default hurdle in access to higher education.
Nor
which
have we heard of fan clubs for college admis-
measures are used rarely for practical
sion tests or similar ability tests in countries
spread belief in ability
true, there
We
is
selection
—
that
Germany ability
in
measures are unfair
if
they are used in high-stakes decisions such as
admission to higher education or employment. Similarly, in a country with intense
and frequent
where
ability testing is a default
many critical decisions. From all the individual that
procedure for
differences constructs
have been investigated and established
use of ability measures for crucial decisions,
within the past 100 years, the constructs related
such as the United States, there should be a
to intelligence, ability, aptitude,
widespread belief to
that using ability is a fair
way
such decision procedures. Available
assist
ment have a
and achieve-
special status. Ability testing
of the big success stories of psychology.
is
It is
one not
evidence suggests that the cultural differences in
so easy to assign credit for "inventing" intelli-
the evaluation of various selection procedures
gence
either
from applicants or jobholders are too
small (Marcus, 2(X)3: Phillips Steiner
&
Gilliland,
huge differences between
&
Gully, 2002;
1996) to account for the
in the use
of ability
cultures. In addition,
it
is
tests
found
of course
to patriarchs or leaders in a field of intel-
ligence research. There are two famous and
well-acknowledged traditions
in
intelligence
research. First, the methodological innovation
of using the then rather
new
correlation coeffi-
cient to express the association
between two
4
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE
.
was
tests
a brilliant idea. Extending this idea to
tetrad analysis
and
later to factor analysis is cer-
one of the most crucial developments for psychology as a scientific discipline, and it can tainly
be traced to Charles Spearman most prominently. Second, the
initial
pragmatic use of
intel-
ligence measures (Binet, 1903) and, only a few
years
later,
method
in
widespread use as a selection
its
World War
I
are clearly the other side
of a unique success story. Both traditions later joined
when
it
came
to standardizing the condi-
tions of administration
and the comparisons of
individual test results with a group of reference subjects,
validating
other test value, and utility
by various means,
tests
mostly correlating some
test
when
value with
some
the demonstration of
of measures was warranted.
The successful use of practical purposes
constructs they are supposed to reflect,
panacea
to
the
raised above.
It
not a
is
problem of indetermination that
is
lack of precision in
deriving measures that caused the inflation of available tasks.
As
a result, frequently the psy-
chological interpretation of the meaning of a specific test score takes not
than two or three lines.
much more
space
A related problem is that
our interpretation of intelligence constructs, as assessed by several intelligence
tests,
frequently
does not go beyond paraphrasing supposed
communality of
content.
test
Although the
mathematics of factor analysis has developed rapidly
and there
decent software widely
is
good factor analysis remains an art much more than a technology because the
available,
composition and nature of the included variables
ability tests for
many
are so crucial.
and the lack of unequivocal
guidelines in deriving indicators from a theory are responsible for the manifold of measures
available today.
thought to
elicit
The number of
responses that allow an evalua-
tion of the observed behavior as
more or
intelligent is hard to estimate, but
it
rising. Carroll's
less
high and
is
(1993) review of individual dif-
ferences research in intelligence
lists
and
classi-
good proportion of tasks proposed so far. One apparent problem with almost all of those tasks is that the gap between what the task is supposed to measure and what it actually measures is not bridged by strict derivation of the
fies a
For example, specification of the measure-
task.
ment
The Understanding of Intelligence
distinct tasks
intention for a task supposed to
measure
mental speed usually leaves countless degrees
The
rapid development of statistical methods
and the overwhelming demand
for the wide-
spread use of intelligence tests caused an imporneglect:
tant
cognitive
trying
vidual differences initial
to
understand the basic
mechanisms responsible
we
label
for the indi-
intelligence.
The
roots of intelligence research are cogni-
tive in nature. Specifically.
Ebbinghaus (1896-
1897) and, to a lesser degree, Binet and Henry
(1898) were fundamentally interested
in
cogni-
processes and their implications. These
tive
were almost completely neglected
roots first
in the
eight decades of the 20th century.
The
of freedom for
rediscovery of cognition in intelligence research
tion takes
how exactly the operationalizaplace. Many of the decisions that need
and the related developments caused a number
be made before having a task that can be used
of important changes that might not be apparent
to
to assess individuals are likely to affect the indi-
vidual differences that can be observed. Thus, is
not clear what exactly individual differences
on the task
reflect.
One can
then ask, "Speed of
what?" With any individual
task, there is thus
always a problem of identifying exactly what the task actually measures.
using
multiple
certainly
is
measures
are
communality of several pretty
personnel selection. There are. however,
more is
many
areas in which the assessment of abilities
a crucial aspect. Granting access to higher
education, college and university admissions,
construct
diagnosis of learning disabilities, diagnosis of
in
a
addressing the
psychometric problems associated with the use of single indicators. However, analyzing the
The validity of ability assessment has many The most prominent examples come from
faces.
The approach of for
major progress
to laypersons.
it
things, all of
which
fuzzy operationalizations of the
all
forms of dementia, identifying
intellectual
and giftedness, diagnosis of attentional disorders, and diagnosis of mental retardation talent
and specific disorders associated with tual
intellec-
problems are just some of the most
Intelligence:
prominent applications. Corresponding with this
manifold of applications
of
mental
is
the
prominence
throughout
abilities
Intelligence
is
our
condition for success in school and college. Intelligence
is
the job, and
it
name
is
associated with income, health,
of daily li\ing. to
activities
few of the abundant correlates.
just a
However,
these valuable data
all
what we do when we behave is
on
the best predictor for success
and instrumental
do not
tell
intelligently,
us
what
required to solve a specific problem, what
can be done to do better on some form of prob-
some
lem, what will help to improve
how our
ability,
brain operates to solve problems, what
processes are accomplished while solving intelligence problems, what
makes a problem harder
than another one. what
we
can accomplish and
why. what makes one individual smart and another one not. and
many more
efforts (Carpenter. Just.
questions that
Consider a widely used form of an
ability test
such as matrices items. This form of
been used for a long time and
is
A
group of researchers took a more
larger
explain fluid intelligence. Early vidual differences in working strated a strong link
and correlational work, individual in working memory have been
mental
differences
established as a crucial construct of
(Conway,
intelligence
tive resources that are
The developments we
just
sketched for
matrices items can be found for
many forms of
traditional intelligence assessment. Categorical
syllogisms or relational inferences, for example,
factor theory
that items of this type required eduction of rela-
tions
and of correlates as pivotal and
role
of
that items of this
that the
specifically,
prior
Cattell thought
type required dealing with
novel situations, although he had
some sympa-
thy for Spearman's notion of eduction. Multivariate analysis unequivocally demonstrates that
items of this
typ)e assess
something
that is at the
core of established intelligence tests (Carroll.
& Snow, 1983; Marshalek. 1984). However,
1993; Marshalek. Lohman.
Snow, Kyllonen.
&
fundamental for human
have a long and scattered histor} in intelligence
Spearman (1938) thought
—would be minimal.
human
Kane, Miyake,
intelligence.
research, too (Baddeley. 1968; Carter. Kennedy,
Werdelin. 1958).
knowledge
Jarrold,
& Towse. in press; Miyake & Shah. 1999). This body of research regresses fluid intelligence and other intelligence constructs on functions of working memor>' and provides interesting evidence about cognitive processes and cogni-
& Bittner.
—
Christal,
highly recom-
test
the construct of fluid intelli-
abilities
&
1990). Subsequently, in a mixture of experi-
subsumed under
other
indi-
fluid intelligence
and working memory (Kyllonen
(Jensen. 1998; Spearman. 1938) but can also be
(Cattell. 1963).
between
work on
memory demon-
has
mended by proponents of a general
gence
Shell. 1990) arrived at
construct-oriented approach in their attempts to
directly relate to the cognition involved in ability testing.
&
similar conclusions.
lives.
a necessary though not sufficient
A Diva and a Workhorse
on
1981; Sternberg. 1980; Storing. 1908;
We
interesting
this
describe two
more
refrain
from elaborating
work and want \er\'
briefly
to
contemporar\ appro-
aches toward the understanding of intelligence.
The
first
approach
is
through recent work on
interference control and behavioral and cognitive inhibition.
The second approach is through neuropsychology and brain
evidence from imaging.
Beginning with task classes well known cognitive
in
psychology, individual differences
were investigated
in
an attempt to find
promising constructs of human
new and
abilities.
that cause conflicts in the cognitive
Tasks
system and
its widespread use. many things were unknown about such matrices items a few years ago. It is in more recent efforts that some deter-
require cognitive processes to cope with these
minants of difficulty have been established
observed with such tasks can be considered to
Other work on matrices items has
reflect interference control, cognitive inhibition,
despite
(Primi, 2001
).
conflicts have tions.
The
been used
in several investiga-
individual differences that can be
Among
used specific models of item response theory
and behavioral inhibition (Nigg. 2000).
(Embretson, 1995) and provided us with
these tasks are the stop-signal paradigm, anti-
pretations
of two-person
parameters
inter-
(general
control processing and working memor>' capacity) for
such measures. Earlier, more experimental
saccadic eye movements, switching a task
set,
the flancker task, the Stroop test, directed forgetting,
and proactive interference,
to
name just
6
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE
.
and the
More
important, however,
a few. The results of investigations analyzing
tailored,
individual differences on such tasks have been
the appearance of established intelligence tests
disappointing so differences
are
far.
not
The observed individual very
across
stable
task
classes, or they are not correlated sufficiently
&
with relevant criteria (Friedman
Miyake.
&
2004; Kramer, Humphrey, Larish, Logan,
Miyake
Strayer, 1994;
&
Atkinson,
et al..
2000; Salthouse,
Berish, 2003; Shilling,
& Rabbitt, 2002).
It is
hoped
Chetwynd,
that future investi-
gations will provide hints for communalities
among
individual differences in coping with
cognitive conflicts, as assessed by various task classes. If intelligence research
successful,
we can add
on
this topic is
an important ability to
our set of usual suspects
important
in predicting
like).
might change because ordinary intelligence tests are
hard to develop, and what they assess
is
hard to describe. Forms of assessment that are
&
easier to compile (Kyllonen
2002)
Irvine.
because determinants of difficulty and other item
known and
statistics are
subject to experi-
manipulations might be established.
mental
Similarly, taxonomies of
human
might
abilities
change substantially over the years because we succeed
developing a more profound under-
in
standing of constructs such as fluid intelligence, crystallized intelligence, or mental speed.
These
efforts are not likely to provide us with tests that
beat existing
ones hands down. They just
replace traditional forms of assessment that had
criteria.
The second approach focuses on logical level.
the physio-
Fundamental changes for
intelli-
emerge from
some
many
basic flaws but
applied victories
with forms of assessment that are more thor-
the
oughly developed, psychologically profound,
rapidly developing fields of neuropsychology
but as successful as their ancestors. In the con-
and brain imaging. Not only are we
text of this
gence research are
likely to
likely to
work,
will
it
be desirable to do some
more about the neural basis of general intelligence (Duncan et al., 2000), but there is
fine-tuning on constructs that count, by and
also neuropsychological evidence about the dis-
measurement and
learn
sociation of
more
1996) or on
et al.,
specific abilities (e.g..
how emotion and
Awh
cognition
interact in decision-making tasks (e.g., Sanfey, Rilling.
Aronson. Nystrom.
These new developments
more appropriate and
&
Cohen. 2003).
tive processes. little
by
and profound as
we
should have clear expec-
and similar
issues,
tations.
not very likely that
It is
we
will
improve
of ordinary and available intelligence tests in
to investigate cogni-
These methods are
informed
structs are as refined, solid,
they should be. In our attempts to address these
human
likely to
be of
use for our understanding of intelligence
unless
details of
the predictions achieved through the application
correct models of
methods
all
interpretation of these con-
will help us to invent
intelligence and provide an important addition to our arsenal of
because not
large, as established
psychological
theory.
many
applied settings to a large degree through
improving the form of traditional assessment.
Many
outstanding psychologists have put a
of effort into establishing what will work certain applied context
and what
lot
in
a
will not. This is
Without solid psychological theory, there is no way to understand cognitive processes. Just
not to say that modifying the appearance of
recording physiological data that accompany
slightly
some
tional
cognitive process will not be instrumental.
We do
need psychological theory and a psycho-
logical understanding before
we can make
sense
of some physiological correlate of intelligence behavior. This objection does not
make physiowe need to
logical data any less interesting, but
be careful and sensible It is
in their interpretation.
possible that, in the long run, the appear-
ance of intelligence stantially.
tests
will
change sub-
This could be attributed to new
computerized forms of
new forms of
test
test
presentations and
compilation
(adaptive.
intelligence tests
not worthwhile.
is
improve the
validity
intelligence
acceptance
tests,
in fields
and
We
utility
might
of tradi-
we might improve
where intelligence
testing
was not accepted as useful information in decision making, and we might have sounder instruments
that are easier to
It is
oping
develop and maintain.
we succeed in develnew abilities. There are establish new constructs of
also possible that
tests that assess
several attempts to intelligence,
and they are addressed
chapters of this book.
We
supportive with such efforts because difficult to satisfy the
in various
should be patient and it
is
truly
necessary requirements
.
A
Intelligence:
before a skeptical crowd of scientists
new
is
willing
possible with these
Diva and a Workhorse
•
7
new developments. As
a
in the field
consequence, the basic science of cognition and
of intelligence. To meet the set of requirements,
the technology of intelligence testing are not
to accept that there is a
we
player
need to have close collaborations
will
between cognitive psychology,
differential psy-
connected
properly
been connected
— indeed,
at
have only
they
very beginning.
their
The
chology, psychometrics, and possibly additional
rediscovery of cognition in intelligence research
disciplines.
is
The various
correlates and possible conse-
quences of individual differences
in intelligence
have provoked many heated debates and intense political controversies.
enough
own
sake
is
important
what we do, what awareness we
for
develop, what knowledge
we
important to note
is
It
that intelligence for its
we
compile, and what
understand, discover, and create throughout
our lives to justify almost any level of attention
much more
than
fashion.
a
Without an
informed cognitive understanding of intelligence as a construct, the technology of intelligence testing
going to make
is
to
little
On
no progress.
the other side, testing of cognitive abilities
is
one
of the most important applied fields for cognitive
psychology, and the neglect of this aspect likely to
is
be causal for the lack of implications
cognitive psychology has in applied settings.
Hence,
we would
encourage psychol-
like to
realize that the individual differences in intelli-
more psychometric background to gain some detailed knowledge about the cogni-
gence and their relevance do not simply disap-
tive
devoted to the construct.
It is
also important to
ogists with a
processes underlying intelligent behavior.
with causes, conse-
we would like psychologists with a more cognitive or experimental background to make more use of applied knowledge from
quences, and correlates of individual differences
psychometric research, including individual-
pear
if
we remove
attention
from them. The
opposite can be. and frequently there
is
a need to deal
true.
is,
Hence,
so that policies regarding
in intelligence
how
to use such information can be discussed most
The
coherently.
future
is
Similarly,
book
We
methodology.
differences
hope
that
this
contributes to serve this purpose.
likely to create prob-
lems with an even higher propensity to trigger fundamental conflicts. What would our advice
be when
it
comes
to
problems related
ing for genes associated with different levels of
Would we recommend
intelligence?
the use of
devices and drugs that augment intelligence?
There are many similar questions ahead, and a profound understanding of intelligence
is
a
the
one of
a substantial irony in that
is
most successful constructs of psychological
research resists a consensual definition. Similarly,
there
is
Ackerman,
P.
L..
& Goff, M.
no broad consensus
in the scien-
(1994). Typical intellec-
engagement and personality: Reply
tual
to
Rocklin (1994). Journal of Educational Psycho150-153.
logy, 86,
Awh,
E., Jonides, J.,
Koeppe, R. A.,
prerequisite for sensible advice.
There
References
to screen-
Smith, E. E., Schumacher, E. H.,
&
Katz, S. (1996). Dissociation
and rehearsal
of storage
in
verbal
working
memory: Evidence from positron emission tomography. Psychological Science, Baddeley, A. D. (1968).
A three
7,
25-3 1
minute reasoning
test
community about the conceptualization of intelligence and measurement methods for intelligence testing. This is why we would label intelligence a workhorse and a diva: The construct is extremely useful, but we do not have a
Binet, A. (1903). L' Etude experimentale de I'intelli-
proper definition of what
Binet, A.,
tific
it
is
and what
it
is
not.
Despite this lack of a consensual definition,
our understanding of the principles by which
human minds is
rapidly
advancing
operate, develop, or
progressing. at
Cognitive
malfuncdon science
is
a very fast rate-much faster than
the practical realization of
what
is
or could be
based on grammatical transformation. Psycho-
nomic Science,
10,
341-342.
gence [The experimental study of intelligence]. Paris: Schleicher.
&
Henry, V. (1898). La fatigue intellectu-
elle [Intellectual tiredness]. Paris: Schleicher.
Cacioppo,
W.
J.
T,
Petty, R. E., Feinstein,
J.
A.,
& Jarvis,
B. G. (1996). Dispositional differences in
cognitive
motivation:
individuals
varying
The in
life
and times of
need for cognition.
Psychological Bulletin, 119, 197-253.
8
.
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE
Carpenter,
A., Just,
P.
M.
A..
&
Shell,
one intelligence measures:
processing in the Raven
of the
Matrices
account
Progressive
Review;
Psychological
test.
What
(1990).
P.
A theoretical
97.
404-431. Carroll.
A
Human
(1993).
B.
J.
New
survey of factor-analytic studies.
Cambridge University
Grammatical yardstick. Cattell. R. B.
Press.
Factors, 23. 587-591.
A
critical
experiment. Journal of
Educational Psychology. 54. 1-22.
Conway, A. R. A.,
& Towse, J.
J.,
Miyake,
(in press). Variation in
working
memory. Oxford. UK: Oxford University Press. Costa.
P T,
NEO
&
Jn,
McCrae. R. R. (1992). Revised
personality and five factor inventory-
professional manual. Odessa. FL: Psychological
Assessment Resources. Cronbach, L.
New York:
testing.
Duncan, H.,
Seitz, R.
J..
Ahmed,
Harper
&
Kolodny,
J..
A., et
al.
Row. J.,
(2000).
neural basis for
general intelligence. Science. 289.
Ebbinghaus,
Methode zur ihre
bei Schulkindern
for testing mental abilities
with schoolchildren]. Zeitschrift
[On and
fiir
new
a its
use
Psycho-
und Physiologic der Sinnesorgane.
logic
13.
401^57. Embretson.
S. E. (1995).
The
role of
intelligence. Intelligence, 20.
among
working
mem-
&
P.,
in
inhibition
and interference control functions:
A latent variable analysis. Journal of Experimental &
methods:
A partial
replication and extension
German sample. Applied Psychology: An
in a
International Review, 52, 515-532.
Marshalek. B.. Lohman. D. F.
&
Snow. R. E. (1983).
The complexity continuum models of
hierarchical
in
radex and
the
intelligence. Intelligence,
107-127.
7,
P..
Emerson, M.
& Wager. T D.
A. H.. Howerter. A..
J.,
Witzki,
(2000).
The
their contributions to
complex
frontal lobe tasks:
A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 49-100.
& Shah, P.
Miyake. A.. ory.
Nigg.
(
999).
1
Models of working mem-
Cambridge. UK: Cambridge University Press.
T
J.
(2000).
On
inhibition/disinhibition
in
developmental psychopathology: Views from cog-
and personality psychology and a working
nitive
taxonomy. Psychological Bulletin.
inhibition
Wilkinson. R.. Danthiir. V. Kleitman.
G.
Stankov.
L.. et al. (2002).
The
S.,
role
Journal of General
confidence judgments.
Psychology. 129. 257-299. Phillips.
J.
M.. to
&
Gully. S.
personnel
M.
(2002). Fairness reac-
selection
techniques
in
Assessment of typical
Journal of
London: Praeger
Strayer. D. L. (1994).
Beyond
a
unitary
in attention.
Human
Resource Management,
13,
1186-1205. Primi. R. (2001). Complexity of geometric inductive
reasoning tasks: Contribution to the understanding of
Kramer. A. F. Humphrey, D. G.. Larish,
9,491-512.
G,
Knezevic.
relations:
ability.
processing
Pallier.
Singapore and the United States. International
Psychology. 84. 537-552.
&
ogists Press.
L. (1992). Personality-
Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g factor: The science of
G. D.,
ed.. Vol. 4,
CA: Consulting Psychol-
Marcus, B. (2003). Attitudes towards personnel selec-
tions
engagement. Journal of Educational
intellectual
bition:
and organisational psychology (2nd
&
industrial
P
Ackerman,
intelligence
mental
Handbook of
(Eds.).
of individual differences in the accuracy of
169-189.
Miyake, A. (2004). The relations
Psychology: General, 133. 101-135. Goff. M.,
M. Hough
126. 220-246.
ory capacity and general control processes
Friedman. N.
Europe. In H. C. Triandis, M. D. Dunnette,
L.
41.
Priifung geistiger Fahigkeiten und
Anwendung
method
457^60.
Uber eine neue
(1896-1897).
H.
Levy-Leboyer, C. (1994). Selection and assessment
unity and diversity of executive functions and
Bor, D., Herzog,
A
Mahwah. NJ: Lawrence
development.
test
389^33.
& Irvine, S. H. (2002). Item generation
Miyake, A., Friedman. N.
(1949). Essentials of psychological
J.
more than) working-memory
Erlbaum.
tion
C. Kane, M.
A., Jarrold,
C,
P.
Reasoning
Christal, R. E. (1990).
(little
pp. 173-190). Palo Alto,
and crystallized
fluid
is
capacity?! Intelligence, 14,
Kyllonen,
in
S.,
Theory of
1963).
(
R.
Human
intelligence:
York:
& Bittner, A. C. (1981). reasoning: A stable performance
C. Kennedy,
Carter. R.
ability
for cognitive abilities:
&
P C,
Kyllonen.
J. F..
Logan.
Aging and
inhi-
view of inhibitory
Psychology and Aging,
human
intelligence.
Intelligence,
30,
41-70. Sackett.
P
tions
R..
Zedeck.
S..
&
Fogli. L. (1988). Rela-
between measures of typical and maximal
job performance. Journal ofApplied Psychology, 73,
482-486.
Intelligence:
&
Salthouse. T. A., Atkinson. T. M..
Berish. D. E.
Executive functioning as a potential
(2003).
mediator of age-related cognitive decline
normal
in
Journal of Experimental Psy-
adults.
chology: General, 132, 566-594. Sanfey. A. G.. Rilling, L. E..
K., Aronson,
J.
& Cohen. J.
A., Nystrom.
Openness versus
Personalauswahl
Much
&
Kauffmann, M. (1993).
itn
europdischen Vergleich
Chetwynd.
A..
&
Rabbitt,
inconsistency
Individual
An
sures of inhibition:
R M.
A.
across mea-
investigation of the con-
of inhibition
in
older adults.
Kyllonen,
R C., & Marshalek,
The topography of tions.
In
R.
J.
abilities.
W. (1996). Fairness
Gilliland. S.
Sternberg. R.
J.
81,
134-141.
(1980). Representation and process in
linear syllogistic reasoning. Journal
of Experi-
ability
B. (1984).
and learning correla-
Sternberg (Ed.), Advances
the psychology of
human
pp. 47-103). Hillsdale. NJ:
in
iiber
einfache
Schlussprozesse
investigations of simple
[Experimental
inference processes].
Trapnell,
A
lexical left turn.
nality, 8,
Werdelin,
11, 1-27.
D. (1994). Openness versus
P.
I.
intellect:
European Journal of Perso-
273-290. (1958).
The mathematical
ability:
Lund:
Gleerups.
Willingham. W. W. (1999). test fairness. In S. J.
A
Messick
systematic view of (Ed.),
Assessment
in
intelligence (Vol. 2,
higher education: Issues of access, quality, student
Lawrence Erlbaum.
development, and public policy (pp. 213-242).
Spearman, C. (1938). Measurement of intelligence. Scientia, 64. 75-82.
Storing, G. (1908). Experimentelle Untersuchungen
Experimental and factorial studies.
Neuropsychologia, 40. 605-619. E.,
of cognitive
Archiv fiir die gesamte Psychologic,
Gottingen: Hogrefe.
Snow, R.
&
Psychology intellect:
[Personal selection in European comparison].
validity
Steiner, D. D.,
tests
93-109.
mental Psychology: General, 109, 119-159.
Schuler. H., Frier, D..
struct
and performance on
France and the United States. Journal of Applied
381-386.
(2002).
121-143.
& Crawford, J. D. (1997). Self-confidence
ultimatum
the
in
ado about nothing. European Journal of Person-
Shilling, V. M.,
fluid intelligence. Intelligence, 28,
Stankov. L.,
reactions to personnel selection techniques in
game. Science. 300. 1755-1758.
ality.: 6.
9
D. (2003). The neural basis of
economic decision-making
Saucier, G. (1992).
•
Stankov, L. (20(K)). Complexity, metacognition, and
Intelligence, 25, J.
A Diva and a Workhorse
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
2 Assessing Problem
Solving
Context
in
Patrick C. Kyllonen
SooNMOOK Lee
test-taking
Introduction
more than
skills
kinds
the
of
real-world learning and performance activities Last year, one of us received a call from a
graduate school colleague
who was working
for
an international strategic consulting firm (hereafter.
ISCF).
to help
if
ETS might
(e.g.,
but recent college graduates from a vari-
increasingly viewed as a social activity operat-
system for
their selection
who work on
small teams and help
solve problems for business clients.
It is
essen-
problem-solving job, ISCF staff told
us.
said the job did not require
knowledge
of standard solutions but instead,
flexibility,
They
"aptitude"
creativity,
and the
through
ability to think things
and devise novel solutions. They also told us
that
19, this
volume), but the tide
within
ing
a
social-cultural-political
trial
testing circles, this has led to a
closely reflect the
activities that test scores are
designed to predict.
develop
tests that
These include measures of such constructs as practical intelligence (Sternberg, 1997; see also
volume), situational judgment
with a standardized assessment that "looks like
(Motowidlo, Dunnette,
an IQ
critical
had served
their purposes, but they
wanted something more up-to-date, and, more important, they wanted a test that
was more
directly relevant to the business analyst job itself.
A common
criticism
of intelligence
and
aptitude tests used for personnel selection and
program admissions abilities out
lack
is
that
they
of context. The view
authenticity
and
that
is
measure that
they
such
reflect
movement
more
to
Chapter
test." It
context
(Gipps, 1999). In both educational and indus-
previously they had been screening applicants
tests
more basic
Lohman, 1993; or even Chapter
is moving toward more contextualized assessments. Assessment is
analysts.
ety of majors
tially a
be able
ing out the virtues of measuring
Business analysts are not
them redesign
business
MBAs
He asked
they are intended to assess (Frederickson, 1984;
Wiggins, 1993). There are counterviews point-
17, this
thinking
&
(Derry,
Carter,
Levin,
&
1990), and
Schauble,
1995). These are constructs reflecting problem-
solving competencies in
compared tude
to traditional
more
practical settings,
intelligence and apti-
tests.
In this chapter,
we
review these constructs
along with methods for measuring them.
We
focus on their definitions and comparisons, as well as address the issue of
why
there
is
so
11
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE much
interest in
problem solving
context in
in
both industry and higher education.
We
also dis-
with ways to teach mathematics concepts based
on understanding rather than
rote memorization.
cuss the process of developing and scoring mea-
Polya suggested
sures of such constructs and their statistical and
broken down into four phases: understanding
psychometric properties.
that
problem solving could be
problem, devising a plan, carrying out
the
the plan, and looking back.
associated with
Problem Solving, Situational
approach
Judgment, Practical Intelligence,
course,
is
more
it
not
Each phase has
specific issues. Polya's
limited
to
mathematics, of
and others have suggested similar schemes for problem solving in general. For
AND Critical Thinking as Constructs
example. Bransford and Stein (1984) proposed a
We
used a standard problem-solving approach
(e.g.,
Polya, 1957) for thinking about the job of
the business analyst and designing the screening
assessment. But in the course of thinking about relevant problem-solving constructs,
we
judgment, practical intelligence, and
critical thinking. All of these approaches have
been used
to assess
problem solving
in
reflect different perspectives.
A
two frameworks. Table 2.1
Problem-Soh ins Schemes
Brandford and Stein (1984) -IDEAL"
Pdlxa {1957)
Identify the problem
Understand the
Define and represent
Devise a plan
one way
or another. But they stem from difterent origins
and
(Identify. Define.
also con-
sidered research done under the headings of situational
IDEAL
scheme they called
Explore. Act. Look). Table 2.1 presents these
question
is,
are these constructs or approaches?
problem Explore possible strategies
Act on the chosen strategy
Carr\ out the plan
Look back and
Look back
Problem Solving
From
The construct of problem solving in psychology is often aligned with thinking and contrasted with memorization. An influential view was
evaluate
interviews
analysts from ISCF.
we conducted with business we found that they \ iewed
problem solving on
the
remarkably close
We
their jobs
in
terms
to the Polya-Bransford-Stein
present the categories and vari-
expressed by the mathematician Polya (1957).
framework.
who, following the psychologists Duncker (1935) and Wertheimer (1945). was concerned
ous subcategories emerging from our interviews
Table 2.2
in
Table
2.2.
Problem-Solving Framework
"How
important to know prior to employment?"
(40 statements
in the following
seven categories)
Assessing the nature/scope of a problem
(2/4)
Identifying issues and generadng hypotheses
(3/3)
Structuring the problem and developing plan of action
(3/4)
Gathering information
(10/10)
Undertaking analyses
(8/10)
Synthesizing findings
(2/5)
Implementing a solution
N=
96 (21Tf return
rate
[Web
survey]).
The
importance rating of 2.5 or more (on a scale from within that category. analysts'
The percentages
recommendations
21% 14%
(0/4)
(40)
Total Note:
13% 15% 14% 17%
for
1
ratio indicates the
=
number of statements within
not important to 5
=
6%
100%
the category receiving an
very important), over the total
number of statements
are the target content coverage for the assessments, based on importance ratings, and on
how many
questions from each categor>' there ought to be on the assessment.
3
Assessing Problem Solving in Context
With
Because of the apparent agreement on what constitutes
problem solving, as seen by compar-
ing Tables 2.1 and 2.2.
it
seems plausible
that
would be a problem-solving construct
there
identified in the individual-differences literature.
However,
review of the abilities
in his definitive
literature. Carroll (1993) found no evidence for
such a
acknowledge
factor. Carroll did
blem solving
is
frequently mentioned in discus-
sions of what intelligence
Detterman. 1986) and
is
difference
Sternberg
is (e.g.,
&
the
between reasoning and
psychology,
in
practical use today
ing classroom examinations cations).
Bloom proposed
One
still
in
creat-
(among other appliknowledge
that one's
could be assessed
at
from knowledge
(e.g., recall)
various levels of abstraction
through compre-
hension, application, analysis, synthesis, and
Bloom
words
also provided probe
knowledge,
define,
list,
or label something; for evaluation, summarize,
judge, or explain something).
A more
problem
that
is
solving implies reasoning within a domain. For
is
by college instructors
for each level (e.g.. for
that
difficult to
is
critical thinking.
taxonomy of educational objectives
evaluation.
believe
it
1
view was Bloom's (1956), whose
influential
We
problem solving, as these terms are most com-
monly used
essence of
often treated synony-
mously with reasoning. essential
that pro-
these perspectives,
all
sort out the
•
was
recent treatment of the topic
provided by Facione (1990),
who was commis-
example, domain-specific analogies are com-
sioned by the American Philosophical Associa-
monly employed to achieve inductive reasoning in real problem solving (see Holyoak &
tion
Thagard. 1989. 1995). Reasoning, or general
cal thinking experts
fluid ability, tent,
report
on the assessment of
critical
46
criti-
from philosophy and other
can be measured with minimal con-
academic disciplines. Through the use of the
a domain-
Delphi procedure, the experts argued for and
in a specific
achieved some consensus on the makeup of a taxonomy of critical thinking. The results of their efforts are summarized in Table 2.3.
but problem-solving ability
specific proficiency, best
domain of
to
thinking. Facione assembled a panel of
activity
is
measured
such as psychology, biology,
mathematics, physics, or business. Examples of this relationship are seen in the work of
Wittmann and SuB (1999; see Chapter
volume),
13, this
also SuB, 1996;
who showed
the role
Table 2.3
Critical
Thinking Skills
Critical Thinking Skills
of general reasoning in domain-specific problemsolving activities. Thus, to think
it
may
not be as useful
of someone possessing problem-solving
ability as
possessing problem-solving
Identify relationships
Interpretation
Comprehend
Evaluation
Assess others' credibility
Inference
Query evidence, draw
Explanation
State
Thinking
movement gained
critical
Analyze and evaluate
thinking
one's judgments
force as a reaction to the
There has been some effect from
(perhaps caricaturized) educational practice of
memorization and regurgitation. Cri-
thinking
tion circles.
topic
is
a popular topic in higher educa-
There are numerous books on the
(Amazon bookstore
books with
and justify
one's reasoning Self-regulation
tical
meaning
conclusions
Like problem solving, the
uncritical
the
skills in
some domain.
Critical
Analysis
lists
critical thinking
close to 6,000
in the
title)
and
related efforts
on
critical
this
thinking that one can find on college
campuses. But
this
work
is
almost completely
divorced from psychological studies of abilities.
and
on courses and programs of study
For example, there
is
no
human
critical think-
even a foundation (www.criticalthinking.org).
ing factor represented in Carroll's (1993) or any
sometimes contrasted with
other abilities model or taxonomy. Also, large-
in that critical thinking involves
scale systematic efforts to assess critical think-
CriUcal thinking
problem solving "reasoning unlimited
in
is
an open-ended manner, with an
number of solutions"
of Education. 1995), but this
agreed-upon distinction.
is
(U.S. Department
not a universally
ing,
although
at
ETS developed which
is
one time showing promise "Tasks
in
(e.g.,
Critical Thinking,"
being used in college outcomes studies;
see Erwin,
1998),
seem not
to
have thrived.
14
.
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE Sample Item From
Table 2.4
the Educational Testing Service's
MFT-MBA Test
Bertha Sunshine entered the travel business after having traveled regularly to the Caribbean and becoming familiar with the businesses in the region.
She discovered a market need and,
Sunny Fun
in the 1960s, started
Destinations, a travel agency that specializes in providing air and ground transportation, as well as hotel and for locations in the Caribbean.
meal arrangements
...
Bertha
is
may be more vulnerable than wonders how responsible the tour operator should be
faced with an ethical dilemma. Her attorney has cautioned her that she
she realizes,
at least
from
a publicity perspective. Bertha
when she only provides arrangements Fun Destinations does not check
for transportation, lodgings,
and food. However, she admits
the age of travelers or whether minors are in the
that Sunny company of chaperones.
Bertha has decided to use demographic segmentation to divide her market before she gives any additional consideration to redesigning her product offerings or communications strategies.
Which would be
the
most useful demographic segmentation variable, given Bertha's present situation?
(A) Religion (B) Education
(C)
Age
(D) Socioeconomic status
Perhaps
due
this is
to the
expense of assessing
The key idea underlying
and scoring the typically open-ended
critical think-
practical intelligence
due
to the lack of
from standard analytic
ing examinations; perhaps
it
is
consensus on what constitutes effective
critical
thinking. It
gence that
be that
it
is
simply an item type. For
example, ETS's Major Field Test for Masters of
(MFT-MBA)
Business Administration
is
an out-
comes assessment measure (Gonzales, 2002). Each item is said to measure critical thinking, as contrasted with recall. One way it does that is by
tacit
knowledge and
it is
distinguishable
that
intelligence, the intelli-
measured by conventional
is
and academic achievement
tests
may
is
ways.
First,
it
is
tests,
ability in
two
typically not taught explicitly
but must be inferred
—
think of "street smarts."
To get a sense for what this is, consider McCormack's (1994) catchy-titled best-seller. What They Don 't Teach You at Harvard Business School, which, according to the author, covers
presenting business situations requiring exami-
topics important to success in business that
nees to apply domain knowledge
finance,
outside the standard curriculum, such as reading
develop solutions to
people, running meetings, negotiating, and cre-
management, marketing) problems presented item
is
shown
in
to
(e.g.,
in the scenario.
An example
Table 2.4.
ating
good
first
impressions. Second, unlike typ-
ical intelligence measures, tacit
practical intelligence tive,
Practical Intelligence
skills
The concept of
practical intelligence
grew
fall
may
knowledge and
include social, affec-
and conative aspects. This could include such as managing your time, your career,
yourself,
and others (Wagner
may
&
Sternberg,
out of research conducted on tacit knowledge
1990), which
by Wagner and Sternberg (1986). Practical intelligence is a major component of Sternberg's
a motivation component.
(1985, 1997) triarchic theory of intelligence, or
was conducted by Hedlund
successful intelligence, which divides intelli-
first-year
gence into analytic, creative, and practical
University of Michigan's Masters of Business
ties.
A
abili-
comprehensive account of practical
intelligence, including
its
and methods for measuring Sternberg et
al.
(2000).
history, it,
motivation,
can be found
in
A
well involve a social
skill,
or
large-scale study on practical intelligence
business
Administration
et al.
school
(MBA)
(2001
students
program. As a
)
on 422 in
test
the
of the
viability of the practical intelligence concept,
Hedlund
et al.
— —based on
compared a new measure
Successful Intelligence Assessment
the
5
Assessing Problem Solving in Context
•
1
"Personnel Shortage" Scenario (One of Six Scenarios)
Table 2.5
Personnel Shortage" Scenario
The
setting
is
a manufacturing plant facing a personnel shortage.
of overtime and morale
is
Employees
low. (5-15 pages of further description.)
You
are
are the
working excessive amounts
Human
Resource Manager.
What do you do? I.
Situational
Judgment Problems (Abbreviated Paraphrases)
For the "personnel shortage" scenario,
A.
1.
1
to 7 scale.
Immediately hire 200 new workers Meet with unit heads to discuss strategies possible responses
Dealing With Employee Turnover
C.
of the following solutions on a
Dealing With Understaffing
2.
B.
rate the quality
possible responses
Dealing With Employee Morale
possible responses
(Responses are scored by correlating the mean vector of 30 experts' responses with the examinee's response vector across
all
30
to
60 responses, yielding a single score
II.
[a correlation coefficient] for
each scenario.)
Case Scenario Problems (Abbreviated Paraphrases)
For the "personnel shortage" scenario, write a response to the following questions:
1.
What
2.
What would you do
3.
What information
4.
What outcome do you expect? What
is
the
main problem? to address the
main problem?
did you focus on? obstacles?
(Responses are rated by 8 to 10 experts on overall quality and also along 10 dimensions, such as problem identification, solution generation, information use,
Source: Hedlund et
practical
al.
(2001).
intelligence
currently
and outcome identification.)
ideas with the measure
used for admissions decisions, the
General Management Aptitude Test
(GMAT),
to those problems. Table 2.5 presents
from a sample scenario and types and
how
an excerpt
illustrates
they are scored.
A
both item
total
of six
which consists of items commonly found on
scenarios were developed, covering problems
higher educations admissions assessments, such
related to a personnel shortage, strategic deci-
and general logical
sion making, dealing with a poorly performing
as reading comprehension,
and quantitative reasoning. Hedlund
(2001)
subordinate, a consulting challenge, interdepart-
developed two kinds of items, a "situational judg-
mental negotiations, and project management.
ment problem" and a "ca.se scenario problem." The situational judgment problem required
Scenario settings varied and included a bever-
examinees to
to rate the quality
problems presented
the case scenario
in
et al.
of various solutions
business scenarios, and
problem required examinees
write short essays
to
on how they would respond
age company, a nonprofit organization, and a communications technology firm. Examinees were given four of the six scenarios, two in the situational
the case
judgment
(rating) format
and two
problem (short essays) format.
in
16
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE
.
Correlation Matrix of Predictor Measures and Graduate (First- Year Business School) Grades
Table 2.6
GMAT U-GPA
U-GPA
SJT-1
.18
SJT-1
.08
.04
SJT-2
-.01
.13
.26
G-GPA
.44
.30
.20
.23
G-Project
.06
.14
.18
.24
Source: Hedlund
Note:
G-GPA
SJT-2
.13
(2001).
et al.
N = 422. U-GPA
= undergraduate grade-point average; SJT-1 = situational judgment problem score based on profile = case scenario problem score based on holistic scoring; G-GPA = graduate (first-year business average; G-Project = graduate (t'lrst-year business school) project rating (1—5 scale).
correlation scoring: SJT-2
school) grade-point
Table 2.6 presents findings. There were three
key ones.
two new measures were
the
First,
independent of
fairly
they were getting
at
GMAT,
suggesting that
different kinds of
skills.
intelligence proves to be predictable
duplicative
established
other
of)
from
(or
cognitive
constructs, such as general fluid and crystallized abilities,
may
it
nevertheless be useful both as
GMAT correlated higher with
an approach to developing assessments and for
first-year business school grades
(G-GPA) than new measures, a multiple regression suggested that the new measures did add to GMAT
uses other than higher education admissions or
did the
job selection, such as job preview and outcomes
Second, although
(e.g.,
from Rr =
.27 with
GMAT and undergrad-
uate grade-point average to R-
adding the situational judgment ilar results for the
GMAT
=
when
.30,
test score;
-
did {r
=
on
findings,
develop inexpensive simulation
judgment and
a
results
Hedlund
et al.
But the
is
psychology
at least
of job tasks, such as typing tests for clerical
workers or "in-basket"
tests for office
man-
presenting their
agers to select those most qualified for the job.
(2001) did not adjust
Motowidlo et al. (1990) suggested the idea of a situational judgment test as an inexpensive simulator, in which situations would be described
in
results are at least
promising
(in
high
reflect skills
impor-
success in higher education settings
The most important
is
that there is
evidence for the independence of practical
fidelity.
situational
Since then, there have been
judgment
tests
developed
in
many
in areas as
intellittle
paper-and-pencil to video-based tests (Weekley
assessments.
There have been criticisms of practical
words) rather than acted out or simulated
diverse as crew resource management (Hedge, Hanson, Borman, Bruskiewicz, & Logan, 1996) and army leadership (Legree, 1995), from
that are not reflected in conventional cognitive
ligence.
personnel
within
for using fairly simple, inexpensive simulations
must be interpreted cautiously
GMAT scores, and
long history
judgment approach may to
tests for select-
.18, .24 for situational
in suggesting that a scenario-based situational
tant
grew out of psychology to
judgment
ing job applicants in selected fields. There
for this range restriction effect due to direct selection.
testing
in industrial-organizational
Situational
work
new measures
because examinees were selected based partly
Judgment
.08), but the
at all (r
case problem scores, respectively).
These
aid.
Situational
sim-
case problem scores). Third,
did not predict first-year project quality
very well
assessment
grades
in predicting first-year business .school
intel-
ligence from other established cognifive factors
& Jones,
1997).
Situational
judgment and
tests
have been used
and they cover
such as general cognitive ability or job knowl-
for both selection
edge
both cognitive and socio-affective dimensions
(e.g.,
Brody, 2003; Gottfredson, 2003;
Jensen, 1993;
Hunter,
Ree & Earles, 1993; Schmidt & However, even if practical
1993).
(McDaniel, Hartman,
Nguyen, 2001).
training,
& Grubb, 2003; McDaniel &
In general, criterion validities
7
Assessing Problem Solving in Context
1
Sample Item From ISCF Assessment
Table 2.7
Headliners Concerts seeks your advice on creating a
Concerts
•
the middle of negotiating with
is in
Web
agreed that for any
divide the remainder.
its
Web
site to
transaction, Headliners Concerts gets
Company
promote
its
30 franchises. Headliners
The company and
franchises.
35%
the franchises have tentatively
of the profits, and the franchises evenly
representatives are worried that this arrangement might
franchises think that the gain in visibility
is
The
fall apart.
small compared to the potential lost revenues from ticket sales and
concessions.
Which of the following (a)
(b)
Can you help the Can the Web site
(d)
How How
(e)
Can
(c)
for
questions are most important for this case? (Choose the best two)
parties negotiate an
can you include the franchises can you publicize the the
company
Web
in decisions
site for
general
mately
p
cognitive
=
.34
tend
tests
to
be
ability
[see
(approxi-
tests
McDaniel, Morgeson,
& Braverman, 2001] vs. Schmidt & Hunter, 1993). Some of
Finnegan, Campion,
p
= .51
[see
the difference
may be due
to the relatively
of situational judgment
reliabilities
tests,
low
on
average (Legree, 1995). As context (situation)-
dependent measures,
made regarding
Web
the design of the
it
may
site?
concert fans to generate interest?
moderately high but perhaps not as high as those for
will like?
acquire the technical capabilities needed to build and maintain a
judgment
situational
agreement the franchises
generate substantial revenue?
also be due to mul-
we made
items, and
Web
three forms of such items
from the information. Each form
B
(34 items),
C
site?
—A (35
(35 items), and
D
items),
(comprising
32 items from the three forms for equating purposes)
—consisted of
Each
three scenarios.
scenario consisted of approximately 12 items.
A
sample scenario and items are presented
Table 2.7
(this is
in
not an actual item but has a
similar look and feel to actual items used in the
A
ISCF's assessment). Forms
D
through
were
tidimensionality in situational judgment tests,
administered to 159, 58, 163, and 153 appli-
with each
situation
cants, respectively.
Situational
judgment
introducing tests are
uniqueness.
thought to have
lower disparate impact than general cognitive ability
tests
(e.g.,
judgment
particularly
istered
tests are primarily
cogni-
lower (22.8 out of 35 items, standard deviation
rather than "what's the best
course of action" (McDaniel Ployhart
We in
&
et
al.,
2003;
Ehrhart, 2003).
developing the assessment for the
inter-
national strategic consulting firm (ISCF).
We
ISCF consultants to find when working for a client, and
interviewed
out what they did
that resulted in the stages of
problem-solving
framework described above (see Table 2.2). We identified actual problems and solutions from further staff
interviews.
ETS
working with ISCF
tions of
The key
[SD]
test
development
staff turned those descrip-
problems and solutions
into actual test
finding
Form
= 4.8)
A
{n
is
that the applicants
=
admin-
159) scored significantly
than the incumbent sample
(27.0 out of 35 items,
SD =
4.6).
evidence for the validity of the
(n-
This
test;
is
191)
some
we were
not
able to gather any additional evidence. Also, as is
adopted a situational judgment approach
initially
and incumbents.
1996;
framed with a prompt of "'what
would you do"
to
2001).
tive ability tests, but they also reflect personality,
was administered
comparing applicants
& Schmitt, & Kabin,
Pulakos
Sackett. Schmitt, Ellingson,
Situational
A
Form
191 incumbents also for
et
common al.,
moderate A,
with tests of this type (see McDaniel
2003, Table
B,
(r^^.
C,
=
1),
reliabilities
.73, .64, .67,
were only
and .64 for Forms
and D, respectively). Examinees
reported liking the assessment, finding
it
quite
or extremely interesfing (68%), and almost
thought
it
all
served as a good job preview (94%).
Situational
judgment
tests
such as
this are
not limited to business contexts or even to prob-
lem solving. For exainple, we are currently developing a video-based communication
skills
assessment for the Association of American
18
.
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE
Medical Colleges
which the context
in
&
medical setting (see Ettienne
The assessment
is
is
the
Problem Solving
Julian, 2001).
similar to the
ISCF
Despite the fact that problem solving
assess-
domain-specific activity
ment,
in that
it
consists of several scenarios,
each of which
accompanied by several
is
lems
items of varying types. material
—both
It
differs in that the
the scenario and the problems
and possible responses
—
is
presented in video
squabbles
elements, as
and sis,
format.
we
— there
are
illustrated
2.2). In his reanalysis
above (see Tables
However, he did not even attempt validity
for
Instead, he argued that
Are the constructs
just reviewed here
—problem
solving, critical thinking, practical intelligence,
and situational judgment
—
in fact constructs, or
are they something else, such as labels for a set
of loosely related tasks, or even assessment
methods? One something
which
it
validity.
for judging
criterion
a valid construct
is
is
whether
the degree to
possesses convergent and discriminant
Convergent validity
among
correlations
is
indicated by high
various, disparate measures
of the construct. For example, sures of problem solving
—
if
disparate
mea-
from multipleof business problem solving to
choice tests
say,
Likert scale items of problem solving in a
—
problem-solving construct. Discriminant validity is
measurement independence construct from other established ability
indicated by
of the
constructs, such as general cognitive ability (g).
For example,
if
measures of problem solving
were more highly correlated among themselves than they were with measures of something else such as inductive reasoning or learning
—
ability
— then
that
would
constitute evidence for
is
The higher
this context.
(Campbell
Fiske,
This suggests that problem solv-
ability factor.
ing
is
not a valid construct per se but rather a
description of a cognitive activity associated
with the general fluid ability factor
—one
pos-
sessing the features listed in Tables 2.1 and 2.2.
This description
is
consistent with other non-
individual-differences approaches
psychol-
in
ogy in which reasoning ability is seen as being employed in problem-solving tasks or activities (Holyoak
&
Critical
Thinking
Thagard, 1989, 1995).
There are domain-independent measures of critical thinking,
such as ones by Watson and
Glaser (1980). Ennis (1996). and Facione and
Facione (1990). There are also domain-specific thinking, such as
ETS's
Tasks of Critical Thinking and ETS's
MFT-
measures of
MBA
critical
programs. Neither kind of measure
domain
specific or general
—has been submitted
to the extensive construct validity analysis that its
con-
among
Carroll (1993) did not even include measures of
the correlations
lower the
evidence for construct validity
&
599) was rather a description of the
kind of task characterizing the general fluid
vergent or discriminant validity. Consequently,
correlations with other-construct measures, the
the
with induction, reasoning, and visual perception; see p.
a relative expression in
same-construct measures and the
greater
problem solving.
problem solving (along
would warrant making statements on
discriminant validity.
Highly correlated
valid-
to identify
med-
communications context were highly correlated, that would be evidence for a broad ical
2.1
based on factor analy-
Carroll (1993) suggested that problem-solving
discriminant
Constructs or Methods?
a
human relasome common
measures did indeed possess convergent ity.
is
math prob-
very different from solving
is
tions
— solving
1959).
particularly confirmatory
Factor analysis,
factor analysis, can
critical
thinking in his review. There
considerable interest in critical
shown by
is
certainly
thinking,
number of books and Web
the
sites
as
on
the subject, the existence of an annual international conference
on
critical
thinking (now in
its
be treated as a formal method for evaluating
24th year), and the number of tests being sold to
construct validity via convergent and discrimi-
measure
nant validity
sus on what critical thinking
criteria.
Let us consider each in turn.
of the
constructs
therefore
it.
There also
may
is
somewhat of is.
a consen-
Critical thinking
perhaps be most usefully thought
of as a set of intellectual activities, such as
9
Assessing Problem Solving in Context
and evaluation, and perhaps an
critical analysis
approach
as well as a set of ideas for, guid-
to.
ing the development of an assessment. This
how we
in fact,
MFT-MBA — as
oping the
is,
treated critical thinking in devel-
an approach to guide
item writing.
or so, there
is
1
a sufficient database to have
conducted meta-analyses on them. The key findings have been that, validity
their criterion
first,
reasonably high, almost as high as
is
that of general cognitive ability. is
Second, there
no consensus on the construct intelligence
practical
validity of
judgment. The construct could be
situational
Practical Intelligence
now
•
&
(Wagner
Sternberg,
knowledge for success (Oswald, Schmitt, Ramsay, Kim, & Gillespie, 2002), or multiple dimensions (Chan & Schmitt, 1997). Some contend that situational judgment tests could just be a measurement method rather than a specific construct (McDaniel et al., 2001; Weekley & Jones, 1997). Third, there is a smaller difference between performances of Black and White examinees than is found on 1993), basic
There are now numerous measures of
practi-
cal intelligence specially tailored to occupations
as diverse as military
commander, salesperson,
school principal, and college student (Sternberg 2000). Extensive studies also have been
et al..
done on the convergent and discriminant
validity
of practical intelligence measures. For example, Sternberg et lations
al.
(2000) reported fairly low corre-
between
Knowledge of
their
measure
Military Leaders
.18-25
(pp.
Tacit
1996; Sternberg, 1985), but these
but the situational specificity of such measures
=
.14-.42 with
that these findings
have is
independent of general cognitive abihty or that a general practical intelligence fac-
tor (Gottfredson, 2003).
And
indeed, the
(2001) study presented above
is
Hedlund
not clear-
cut in that respect. Multidimensional scaling has
been applied Horvath
et al.,
tive ability
a link with
—
analyses have been mostly limited to total scores
must be considered an important
and have not sufficiently determined whether
Different studies
practical intelligence
is
uni- or multidimensional.
Further research will be needed to settle the issue.
For example,
it
would be useful
conduct a
to
structs
number of
gence measures and batteries
diverse practical intelli-
(b) several cognifive ability
consisting of both
fluid
(reasoning,
In that
difficulties
even
summary, there may be some evidence
measures of situational judgment and pracdistinguishable from mea-
far
from being clearly established. However, if
such measures are not clearly distinct
from other established measures by standard
pendent of the already established factors of
validity criteria, they are
general fluid and crystallized abilities.
able by the
way
in
somewhat
Judgment
Because numerous situational judgment have been developed over the past decade
tests
distinguish-
which they are developed.
Indeed, Hanson, Horgen, and
Situational
of meta-
sures of general cognitive ability, but the case is
factor inde-
Jones, 1997), suggesting
interpretation
tical intelligence are
measures. Such a study could establish whether
was a general
&
likely cause.
different con-
analyses.
problem-solving) and crystallized (knowledge)
practical intelligence
may measure
(Weekley
possible
large study involving the administradon of both (a) a large
be a
to practical intelligence data (e.g.,
Some have argued
et al.
13, this
to
measures show (there may even be emodonal intelligence; see Chapter 6, this volume). These findings, which can be viewed as providing evidence for discriminant validity of situational judgment tests, support their construct validity. However, as with practical intelligence, there is a weakness in the evidence for convergent validity McDaniel et al. (2001) found a great deal of variability in the validity coefficients of situational judgment tests. This may be due to a number of causes,
/"
not proven the case that practical intelligence
is
Chapter
196-197). indicating discriminant
and correlations of
there even
(cf.
and fourth, there seems
volume),
greater correlation with personality than cogni-
with r
ability,
effectiveness ratings, indicating convergent validity.
cognitive ability measures
=
measures of general verbal
validity,
of
(TKML) and
Borman (1998)
concluded that situational judgment "best viewed as a
tests were measurement method, rather
than measures of a distinct individual differ-
ences construct."
20
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE
.
Methods and Item Types for Measuring Them
which we referred
experts),
we
workshops. For these,
to as item-writing
(the item writers
and a
psychologist) reviewed the transcripts and pre-
Developing contextualized assessments
—whether
they be problem solving, critical thinking, situational
at the transcripts
judgment, or practical intelligence
involves interacting with subject matter experts or people
who do
knowledge,
the activities requiring the
skills,
and
designed to measure.
abilities
the
test
is
necessary to interact
It is
with such experts to get a sense for what they do, as well as the words they use in describing
what they do, and
to write actual items. This is
required for content validation of the assess-
many different approaches to doing this, but here we describe the one we used in developing the ISCF assessment. Figure 2.1 presents the method we used. We began with some test specifications in this ment. There are
Table 2.1. 8
ISCF
We
framework
listed in
then presented this framework to
consultants in two-at-a-time interviews
and asked them whether
main phases of
this characterized the
their job and. if not. to correct
and express the phases of words. This resulted
their
job
in their
own
a modified problem-
in
included
several
listed), for a total
more
Each of these phases
specific
phases (not
of 40 specific phases.
We
then
taxonomy out to 468 consultants and received back 96 completed forms. The question we asked them concerned the degree to which they engaged in (or the amount of time they spent on) each of the 40 specific phases of the job. We then retained the phases for which more than 50*7^ of the consultants said they regularly engaged in that sent the modified problem-solving
activity
and used those
to guide the
We
as our test specifications
then interviewed 25 consultants on the
telephone (the third set of experts). For each interview,
worked and
to
we asked them
on, to describe
to recall a case they
what the case was about,
walk through the choices they were con-
set
of
that the
This
for.
where they
fourth group of experts suggested
thought the key problems and decision points
were
in the scenario,
and what they thought the
would
best approach to those problems
some
be. In
cases, their suggestions followed the item
writers'
decisions:
in
other cases, the experts
made new suggestions both about where sion points were and
were.
The
the deci-
what the best responses
fourth group of experts also suggested
foils in addition to the correct responses.
During
would
this time,
we
also considered
We made
deliver the items.
and-pencil and
Web
versions of the
considered the various item types
and
settled
on three
—multiple
how we
both papertest.
We
we would
also
use
choice, multiple
response (multiple choice with more than one correct answer), and rank ordering (the
examinee
ranks options rather than selecting the best one). quite
is
It
common
for situational
judgment
and practical intelligence item development
to
include a fourth item type, the Likert scale, in
which examinees
on a 4-
indicate,
to 9-point
how effective a particular response would be (or how likely it would be for them to select scale,
a particular response).
(Open-ended or con-
structed response formats are also possible, of
course.) However,
we
did not include the Likert
scale item type in the
ISCF assessment. We
believe that Likert scale items can be quite useful in there to
development of items.
come up with and
item writers had already written items
solving taxonomy, the major phases of which are presented in Table 2.2.
of scenarios that the third
experts had already
—
case, the problem-solving
The
liminary items with another set of experts.
process involved the experts giving a second look
is
many
assessments, particularly
less than a
when
consensus among experts as
what the key (correct answer) ought
to be. In
ISCF assessment, there agreement among experts (and
the particular case of the
was
sufficient
between experts and the keys,
ETS
and we thought
item writers) about it
therefore useful to
include only non-Likert scale item types.
fronted with and the decisions they made. These
cases were then transcribed, and ers
(who
specialized in business,
ing, or verbal reasoning)
from the third
ETS
item writ-
math item
wrote preliminary items
situation transcripts.
We
Scoring
writ-
then began a
round of interviews (with a fourth group of
An
important theme
lem solving
is
in
that experts
contextualized prob-
may
best response to a situation.
disagree on the
Making a decision
1
Assessing Problem Solving in Context
•
2
Item-Type \
Compendium
\\
Item Types
&
Web
Delivery
(or Other)
Delivery '' 1
—
Test
—
Content
»•
Specifications
*
—
Keying
Test
>
Assembly
Elicitation
Use Construct Frameworks
"
t
1
Expert
L
Expert Polls
Reviews & Revisions
Interviews 1
1
Modify Wording
Credit
Model
Through Expert Interviews 1
Figure
The Test Development Process
2.1
way
about which call
to
respond
no clear
with
is
or
right
Rank-ordering items present similar credit
may
pre-
sent difficulties not typically encountered with
multiple-choice knowledge and
There may be several useful dimensions characterizing scoring issues for these kinds of
measures.
One dimension
with the
fore-mentioned (and omitting con-
structed response items
types
are
is
item type: Keeping
from
multiple
this discussion),
multiple
choice,
response, rank order, and Likert scale.
dimension
"d'"
is
assigned?
credit
assignment problems.
For the ISCF assessment, keys were unanimous (agreed upon by item writers and
and so multiple-choice item scoring was straightforward. However, with the other two item types rank ordering and multiple response we had to deal with the issue of partial credit. In practice, various schemes we experts),
ability tests.
item
how
"c" and
wrong answers.
Consequently, scoring such measures
conventional
— then
often a judgment
the degree of consensus
is
A
second
on the key,
—
—
experimented with did not make much
differ-
we
tested.
ence in the scores of the samples Thus,
we
opted for a binary scoring scheme in
which one
or,
in
some
cases,
more than one
ranging from unanimity to variability. Multiple-
response pattern (either ranking or set of multiple-
choice items for conventional ability tests are
response selections) was given
unanimously keyed. But they do not
typically
have to be.
If
experts disagreed, then
it
might
make sense to give partial credit in proportion to the number of experts who chose a particular key. For example, if
"a" and
40%
ing "a" selecting
60%
of the experts chose
chose "b," then examinees select-
would get .6 credit, and examinees "b"' would get .4 credit. Multiple-
response and rank-order item types present even greater
challenges.
is
there
is
example, "b,"
20%
and
The
Likert scale item type will almost always
have the problem of key variability
—
it
would
be rare to have experts unanimously agree to a rating of a response to a 7-point scale. Thus, various,
problem on,
complex
schemes have become commonplace ments using Likert
scales.
say, a
partial-credit in assess-
The schemes can be
divided into two general approaches, distancescoring schemes and profile conelation schemes.
Distance-scoring schemes involve scaling the
"a" and "b" and the examinee selects
difference between experts' ratings and exami-
"a" and "c," if
multiple-response
full credit,
other responses were given no credit.
the issue of partial matching. If
items, there the key
With
all
is
how much
variability in
70%
credit
is
warranted? Or,
what experts
select
—
for
of the experts selected "a" and
selected "a" and "c," and
10%
selected
nees" ratings. For example, one can take the
absolute value of the difference or the square root of the difference, the square root of the
sum
of the squared differences, and so forth. For set
22
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE
.
problems
—
that
ones
is,
which several Likert
in
and the examinee's
scale items follow a prompt,
task
is
each response
to rate
tions can also be used.
the correlation
—
These involve treating
between the pattern of expert
score for that item
due
set.
rat-
Profile correlations elim-
to experts
recruitment tools.
Another advantage
profile correla-
ings and the pattern of examinee ratings as the
inate bias
previews" and can therefore serve as valuable
and examinees using
assessments aids
Teaching to the
how
can be valuable training
learning or job situation
for the
nees
to highly contextualized
that they
is
amounts
test
to teaching
itself.
exami-
respond to the kinds of problems
to
they will actually face on the job or in the learning situation. Contextualized assessments such
different parts of the scale.
another dimension along which scoring
as these can also be used as self-assessments,
schemes can be arrayed concerns which group actually does the keying. Thus far, we have con-
allowing a student or prospective employee to
Still
sidered experts as the
key (perhaps
But
in consultation
some
in
ones who
capabilities in the domain, enabling the obser-
with item writers).
vation of increased efficacy over time. Further-
when
circles, particularly
there
is
considerable variability in experts' judgments as to the correct key, the population itself
feedback on his or her problem-solving
get
determine the
can determine the key,
more, because contextualized measures such as this are
a result of training or experience, such measures
a "majority
can serve as outcome measures as well as
in
rules" kind of scheme. This approach could be
predictor measures.
applied to any of the scoring schemes discussed thus
far,
although typically
this is
thought to reflect knowledge acquired as
of examinees
only applied
Still
another advantage of some of the con-
textualized problem-solving assessments
is that
to certain items in the emotional intelligence
they appear to be associated with smaller differ-
unusual judgments, such as
ences between subgroups, particularly male-
literature requiring
the emotional expression of a piece of art (for a discussion, see Davies. Stankov,
1998; see also
&
Roberts,
MacCann, Roberts, Matthews,
&
female and Black-White.
sample size
sufficient
ences
in
to
We
did not have a
examine these
differ-
our studies with the ISCF. However,
reduced subgroup differences have been found
Zeidner, 2004).
repeatedly with both situational judgment tests
(McDaniel
et
2001) and practical
al.,
intelli-
Advantages and Disadvantages
gence measures (Hedlund
Even if contextualized assessments did not show increased validity (or even incremental
tages to these kinds of assessments as well.
On
validity)
over conventional ability measures,
they possess several advantages over conven-
measures
tional
could warrant their use
that
operational settings. that there is a strong
what
One major advantage is user demand for tests with
traditionally has
ity," that is,
in
been called "face valid-
for tests that closely resemble the
et al.,
the other hand, there are
major one
2001).
some disadvanThe
point appears to be that their
at this
reliability is consistently
found
to
be substan-
lower than the reliability of conventional
tially
ability
measures. This means that contextual-
ized measures will take
ventional
measures
same precision
much
longer than con-
provide scores
at
the
level as conventional scores.
For
to
example, using the Spearman -Brown formula, a with a reliability of .60 would have to be
subsequent learning, training, or job perfor-
test
mance
longer by a factor of 6 to reach a reliability of
situations that successful
soon face. In our
ISCF
staff,
it
was
initial
examinees
will
discussions with the
clear that they
wanted an
assessment that consisted of problems that
—
.90
for example, a 20-minute test
to be
120 minutes.
A
second disadvantage, and perhaps one
closely resembled the kinds of problems that
related to the
business analysts would later encounter on the
worked out
job.
There simply was no
abstract,
"IQ
bulk of the
interest for the kind of
test-like" tasks that abilities literature.
make up
the
Particularly in
business contexts, such tests double as "job
first, is
As
simply not
schemes
for these
illustrated above, there are
possibilities, but there is very little research
on the topic thus been
that the field has
the best scoring
kinds of measures.
many
would have
tried
far.
Scoring schemes that have
have ranged from simple
to
complex.
Assessing Problem Solving in Context
we
but
simply do not have enough experience
with these approaches to say which ones might
work
which ones may be more or
best,
less sus-
ceptible to clever coaching strategies (e.g., for
Likert scale items, answering at the extremes
lower scores), and
likely to result in
how
it
be to communicate the scoring schemes to
will
examinees
to
make
the assessment
that
the balanc-
ing of multiple considerations rather than simply the recall of predetermined answers. Still,
number of challenges remain. The
a
mary one
pri-
that using contextualized assess-
is
ments may require considerably more testing time than
fair.
judgment and
requires thinking and
is
easy
and fuzzier side of decision making
softer
23
•
tings
due
allocated in current selection set-
is
lower
to their
reliability.
Perhaps
this
increase can be justified as a result of the addi-
Conclusions and Future Directions
tional benefits of such assessments
— such A
"job preview" and training benefits. In recognition of the validity ceiling
from con-
as the
second
major challenge concerns exploring issues that
ventional cognitive assessments for personnel
come along with widespread
selection and admissions, researchers have sug-
such as their suscepfibility to coaching.
new
gested considering various
constructs to
Among
supplement current measures.
the ones
have received the most attention are mea-
that
sures of problem solving, critical thinking, situ-
operational use, Sfill
another major challenge concerns developing robust and fair scoring schemes for assessments in
which experts themselves might not agree on
what constitutes a correct answer.
and practical intelligence. All
ational judgment,
these stem from different research traditions but
how
overlap considerably in
they are used and
what they mean. Based on the
of possessing convergent and discriminant
ria
validity,
it
is difficult
to
make
the case that these
constructs are in fact valid constructs in the psy-
chological sense. sures possess a
On
the other hand, these
number of
References
traditional crite-
mea-
distinct advantages
over their traditional counterparts.
Among
these
are that because of their contextualized nature,
they possess face validity and therefore are appeal-
Bloom, B.
S. (Ed.). (1956).
goals:
Taxonomy of educational
The classification of educational
objectives:
Handbook I,
Bransford,
D.,
J.
&
Stein, B. S. (1984).
problem solver: learning,
Brody,
N.
and
A
creativity:
New York:
Construct
(2003).
Sternberg Triarchic Abilities Test: reanalysis. Intelligence, 31,
for multiple purposes besides
most notably
tion
as educa-
and training materials and as outcome
measures to evaluate the effects of various ventions.
may
such measures have
Importantly,
shown fewer subgroup
inter-
differences and therefore
see increased use as a
means
to reduce
disparate impact of existing selection procedures.
For these reasons,
we
are likely to see grow-
ing use of contextualized assessments of types, even
if
all
such measures do not carry with
them any increase
in predictive validity. All else
Campbell, D.
T.,
B. (1993).
J.
And one
Human
Cambridge University Chan, D.,
&
cognitive abilities:
New
Press.
paper-and-pencil method of assessment tional test
A
York:
Schmitt, N. (1997). Video-based versus
judgment
tests:
in situa-
Subgroup differences
in
performance and face validity perceptions.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 143-159. Davies, M., Stankov.
construct.
potential users.
& Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and
survey of factor-analytic studies.
tages of contextualized assessments over con-
many
Comment and
319-329.
matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 56, 81-105. Carroll,
tional
ventional ability assessments will outweigh their
of the
discriminant validation by multitrait-multimethod
being equal, or even slightly unequal, the advan-
disadvantages for
IDEAL
Freeman.
validation
as doubling as '"job previews." Also, they can
more easily be used
The
guide for improving thinking,
ing to clients and users for several reasons, such
selection or admissions,
New York:
cognitive domain.
Longmans, Green.
S.,
&
Roberts, R. (1998).
In
search
Emo-
of an elusive
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology Derry,
L.,
intelligence:
75,
Levin,
J.
989-1015. R.,
&
Schauble,
L.
(1995).
can argue that the unexplored territory remaining
Stimulating statistical thinking through situated
research concerns the increasingly
simulations. Teaching of Psychology, 22, 51-57.
in
abilities
24
.
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE
Duncker. K.
(
1935). Ziir Psychologie des produktiven
Denkens [To
psychology of productive
the
Ennis. R. H. (1996). Critical thinking.
Upper Saddle
and assessment
Definitions
thinking, problem solving,
methods of
critical
and
Washington. DC: U.S. Department
writing.
R
M..
&
Julian. E. R. (2001). Identifying
behaviors of successful medical students and
Washington, DC: Association of
residents.
American Medical Colleges. Facione,
P.
A
(1990). Critical thinking:
assessment and instruction.
Facione,
P.,
statement of
Academic
Millbrae,
CA:
& Facione, N. (1990). California Critical Thinking Skills
Academic
California
Frederickson. N. (1984).
Horvath,
Thagard,
MIT
A..
J.
Millbrae,
Test.
CA:
J..
Forsythe. G. B..
Bullis. R.
C.
Williams, W. M.,
knowledge
in military leader-
J..
et al. (1996). Tacit
Army Technical Report 1042). Washington. DC: Army Research Institute for the Behavioral
U.S.
and Social Sciences.
knowledge." Current Directions Science.
Legree.
P.
2.
real test bias: Influences
ment. Review of Research in Education, 24.
unveils industry's first out-
comes assessment for MBA programs. Princeton, NJ: ETS.
develop fluid
abilities.
intelli-
claims and evidence. Intel-
343-397.
M.
E.,
&
Borman, W. C.
November). Situational judgment: An
and at
(1994).
Hanson, M. A., Borman, W. C,
&
Logan, K. K. (1996,
May). Predicting the crew resource management skills
of Air Force pilots (Institute Report
No. 283). Tampa, FL: Research Hedlund,
J.,
Personnel
Decisions
Institutes, Inc.
Plamondon.
Ashford, Practical
S.,
&
McDaniel. M. A., Hartman, N.
K..
Wilt.
intelligence for
J.
J..
Annual Society
for Industrial
Nebel.
K..
(2001, April).
business:
beyond the GMAT. Paper presented
Going
at the
18th
and Organizational
Psychology Conference, San Diego.
S.,
&
Grubb, W.
(2003, April). Situational judgment
L..
tests,
knowledge, behavioral tendency. Paper presented at the
1
8th Annual Conference of the Society
and Organizational Psychology.
Orlando, FL.
McDaniel, M. A., Morgeson,
&
A.,
situational
performance:
M.
McDaniel,
Situational
A
F. P.,
Finnegan, E. B.,
Braverman, E.
judgment
R
(2001).
job
tests to predict
clarification of the literature.
A.,
&
judgment
Nguyen, tests:
A
N.
T.
(2001).
review of practice
and constructs assessed. International Journal of Selection
Motowidlo. (1990).
Sternberg, R.
they don't teach you
Journal of Applied Psychology, 86(4). 730-740.
Testing Association. Pensacola. FL.
Bruskiewicz. K. T,
What
Hanard Business School. New York: Bantam.
Use of
W.,
Personality
tests.
Individual Differences, 36, 645-662.
ment (Symposium AB-34D). Paper presented
J.
&
weighting of performance-based
option
Campion, M.
the annual meeting of the International Militar\
Matthews, G.,
Emotional Intelligence (EI)
alternative approach to selection test developat
D.,
(2004). Consensus scoring and empi-
for Industrial
Hanson, M. A., Horgen. K.
to
Educational Researcher,
MacCann. C. Roberts, R.
III.
Gottfredson, L. S. (2003). Dissecting practical Its
Teaching and testing
(1993).
F.
McCormack, M.
355-392.
gence theory:
lAl-lbd.
testing procedure. Intelligence, 21,
Lohman. D.
rical
ETS
Psychological
(1995). Evidence for an oblique social
J.
Psychologist. 39. 193-202.
Gonzales, K. (2002).
in
9-10.
22. 12-23.
Press.
The
Gipps, C. (1999). Socio-cultural aspects of assess-
Hedge,
(1995). Mental leaps.
P.
Press.
Sternberg. R.
P
Sweeney,
Zeidner,
(1998,
&
J..
of testing on teaching and learning. American
ligence, 31,
Cognitive
intelligence factor established with a Likert-based
Press.
Thinking Dispositions Inventory and California Critical
(1989). Analogical
satisfaction.
Jensen. A. R. (1993). Test validity: g versus "tacit
expert consensus for purposes of educational
California
R
Thagard.
ship: Supporting instrument development (U.S.
of Education. Ettienne.
Holyoak. K.
Cambridge:
River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
(1998).
&
J..
mapping by constraint Science, 13, 295-355.
thinking]. Berlin: Springer.
Erwin. T. D.
Holyoak, K.
and Assessment,
S. J.,
An
Dunnette,
M.
9(1-2), 103-113. D..
&
Carter, G.
alternative selection procedure:
low-fidelity
simulation.
W.
The
Journal of Applied
P.'
activity occurs in other brain regions, too, but that activity
is
quickly supplanted by an inter-
fering stimulus. Thus, a pattern of activity, sus-
PFC. can represent task goals and.
tained in the
in turn, bias activation in
which a weaker neural path-
tant in situations in
way must be
particularly impor-
is
activated to achie\e a goal and a
more frequently used pathway, must
stronger,
be suppressed. According to Miller and Cohen's (2001) model, the biasing influence of the
and
functional
PFC
fMRI environment. They back
of stimuli
the respondent
Thus,
in the
stream
long-term
memory). As an
this biasing
can affect behavior,
When
consider the Stroop color-naming task.
presented with a conflicting stimulus, such as
word red printed
the
response
habitual
respond
"red."'
in
green ink, the stronger,
word and
read the
to
is
but the goal
to
is
name
the ink
must be repre-
color. Therefore, the task goal
M
and
task,
B
the one that
three back. Lures typically
is
the letter either n
Duncan, Burgess,
control,
with
&
Freer, 1996). Interestingly, patients
PFC damage,
lesser
WMC,
(Kane
&
tend to have less difficulty in the
when
Stroop task
as well as individuals with
the trials are incongruent
all
2003) than when the task
Engle,
includes congruent
trials.
When
incongruent, the task goal color)
is
all
(i.e..
the trials are
name
reinforced by the environment
seeing a conflicting stimulus on every therefore performance
is
sustained activation of a tion. In contrast,
tion
when
of congruent
less
the ink (i.e..
trial),
by
and
dependent on the
PFC
goal representa-
there are a high propor-
trials,
respondent can
the
temporarily forget the task goal and "get
away
with" responding on the basis of habit
(i.e.,
reading the word). ily forgotten,
If the task
and the
PFC
goal
is
temporar-
representation
is
-
1
back or n +
more
trials are
et al.
et al..
1
match
difficult to reject
2003).
(2003) reasoned that cognitive
and therefore prefrontal
activity, will
because there
is
a potential conflict between
and the task goal. They therefore
familiarity
examined the relationship between PFC and general
for lure trials
was a
there
activity
fluid ability. Indeed,
significant correlation
(r
=
ciated
with
lure
trials
and Raven's scores.
Importantly, this relationship
when PFC trials
was
was not diminished
activation associated with nonlure
partialled out.
Note how
this project
introduced both experimental control vs.
nonlure
(i.e.,
trials)
may
of individual
as well as statistical control
partialling out nonlure activity).
Genetic Mechanisms
no
differences
It
has been clear for quite
some time now
to
variation
in
to
80%;
50%
see Plomin
(estimates range from
&
that
g,
is
40%
Spinath, 2002). In other
PFC
words, genetic variation accounts for approxi-
behavioral
mately half of the variance in general intelligence
intelligence suggests that the
contribute
lure
(i.e.,
approximately
study
.54)
between the event-related signal change asso-
incorrectly to a conflicting stimulus.
recent
be
particularly important for lure trial performance
the heritability of general intelligence, or
A
it
back, and
longer active, the individual will then respond
in general
the
matches a recently presented stimulus but not
Gray
Johnson,
in a
B-R-X-M-B,
considered a "lure" because
is
than nonlure foils (Gray
Duncan. Emslie, Williams.
more
foils are
For example,
three-back task, in the stream
uation referred to as "goal-neglect"), then an
1995:
some
difficult to reject than others.
second
B
a match (target),
is
(foil).
Within the /!-back
task. If the goal is temporarily "forgotten" (a sit-
Emslie.
and
three-back task, the second
B-R-X-B-M
nonmatch
a
is
in a
indeed, these
&
letters),
must indicate whether the cur-
sented and must be sustained throughout the
incorrect response occurs (see
words,
(e.g.,
stream (n typically varies from one to
three).
therefore
in the
matches the one presented /7-back
rent stimulus
and the hippocampus
(and
outside the
In the A2-back task, a series
presented
is
therefore attention), motor systems (and there-
example of how
is,
then performed the n-
measure of working memory,
task, a
fMRI environment.
in the
fore response selection),
magnetic resonance
imaging (fMRI) measures. Subjects performed
pervasive, moderating sensory systems (and
is
assessed using
activity,
the Raven's task offline, that
other brain regions.
This top-down biasing
PFC
sive Matrices,
event-related
58
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE
.
An
population of healthy adults.
in the
new development
exciting
Conclusion
in the field of behavioral genet-
ics is multivariate genetic analysis
examines the extent
to
(MGA), which
which the variance
that
been a great deal of
In conclusion, there has
work aimed
determining
at
the
cognitive
determines heritability of one cognitive task cor-
and biological mechanisms
relates with the variance that determines the her-
performance on cognitive tasks and
itability is
of another cognitive task. For example,
it
two
possible for the heritability estimate for
understand
the
to better
between cognitive
relation
mechanisms and
that contribute to
This chapter
intelligence.
tasks to be equivalent, but that says nothing of the
reviews some general strategies that should be
"genetic correlation" between the two tasks. That
adopted
is,
the genetic basis for one heritability estimate
may be completely independent
of the genetic
which
basis for another heritability estimate (in
one
case, the variance that determines
heritability
in this endeavor.
research
It
also calls for future
both experimental and
exerts
that
and for future research
statistical control
that
examines not only cognitive mechanisms but neurological and genetic mechanisms as well.
estimate will be unrelated to the variance that
determines the other heritability estimate). In
MGA,
between two tasks
the genetic correlation
can vary from
MGA,
to
&
see Plomin
Spinath. 2002).
examine
therefore possible to
It is
correlation
among measures
Ackerman. the genetic
of cognition and
between measures of cognition and
intelligence.
For example, one could examine the genetic correlation Similarly, lation
among
WMC.
measures of
different
one could examine the genetic corre-
between measures of
intelligence.
WMC and measures no such studies
Unfortunately,
have been conducted.
However,
it
are
higher
&
(Plomin
when
the
task
is
tests
more complex
Spinath. 2002). For instance, the
heritability estimates for a Sternberg
scanning task were 0%, 35%, and sizes
that heri-
on elementary cognitive
tability estimates
of one.
and
three,
five,
memory-
47%
for set
respectively.
Similarly, the heritability estimate for a Posner
was 24% when matching was 60% when based on their name identity,
letter-matching task
physically identical letters but
matching
letters
despite the letters' physical difference. Interestingly, a parallel result
that
the
from psychometrics
tasks and general intelligence task
is
more a
is
higher
more complex (Ackerman.
Perdue, 2002). task
It
is
is
between so-called speed
correlation
when
the
Beier,
&
therefore possible that the
demands executive
attention, the
greater the heritability estimate, and the greater
between the task and
the correlation
remains to be seen with complexity
is
is if
g.
What
the rise in heritability
related to the rise in correla-
tion with complexity.
M.
Beier,
P. L.,
E.,
& Perdue, M. B. (2002).
Individual differences in working a nomological tual
speed
memory
within
network of cognitive and percep-
abilities.
Journal of Experimental
Psycholo}iy: General, 131. 567-589.
Anderson,
R..
J.
&
Lebiere, C. (1998). The atomic
components of thought. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Cantor,
J..
&
Engle. R. W. (1993).
capacity as long-term
worth noting here
is
References
(for a detailed description of
1
individual-differences
Working-memory
memory
activation:
An
Journal of
approach.
Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory,
and Cognition, Conway. A. R.
Cowan.
A..
Therriault. D.,
1101-1114.
19,
&
S. (2002).
variable analysis of working short term
memory
and general
Bunting.
N..
Minkoff,
memory
M. F,
A
latent
capacity,
capacity, processing speed,
fluid intelligence. Intelligence, 30,
163-183.
Conway. A. R.
A..
memory and inhibition
&
W.
Engle, R.
retrieval:
model.
A
(1994).
Working
resource-dependent
Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General, 123. 354-373.
Cowan. N. (1988). Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, straints within the
and
their
mutual con-
human information processing
system. Psychological Bulletin. 104, 163-191.
Cowan, N. (1995). Attention and memory: An integrated framework. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Cronbach. L. tific
J.
(1957).
The two
disciplines of scien-
psychology. American
671-684.
Psychologist,
12,
Cognitive
&
Daneman. M..
Carpenter,
dift'erences
A. (1980). Individual
P.
working memory and reading.
in
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior.
J.
I.
(2001).
Human
intelligence differences:
Towards a combined experimental-differential Trends
approach.
Cognitive Sciences,
in
5,
Dennett, D. C. (1995). Danvin's dangerous idea:
Evolution and the meaning of
life.
New
York:
Duncan,
J..
Burgess,
intelligence
&
P.,
Emslie, H. (1995). Fluid
lobe
frontal
after
lesions.
Neuropsychologia, 33, 261-268.
Duncan,
J.,
Emslie, H., Williams,
Freer, C.
P.,
(1996). Intelligence
and the
&
frontal
Cognitive Psychology, 30, 257-303.
Engle, R. W., Cantor,
&
J.,
Carullo.
A
18,
memory and
972-992. J.
E..
&
short-
general fluid intelligence:
P. S.
A
Journal of Experimental
477^85.
14,
R., Chabris. C. F.,
&
Braver, T. S. (2003).
Neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence.
Nature Neuroscience,
Hambrick, D.
Z.,
&
6,
Engle, R.
W.
(2002). Effects of
and age on cognitive performance: of the
An
capacity. investiga-
knowledge-is-power hypothesis.
Cognitive Psychology, 44, 339-384.
Resnick (Ed.), The nature of intelligence
(pp.
Kane, M.
237-260). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. J.,
Engle,
Bleckley, R.
&
J.,
memory
Engle,
M.
K.,
W. (2001).
Conway. A. R.
A
637-671.
9,
W. (2003). Working-
R.
capacity and the control of attention:
and task
petition,
set
neglect, response
com-
Stroop interference.
to
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,
Kane, M.
Hambrick, D.
J.,
Wilhelm, O., Payne,
T.
Z.,
Tuholski.
W.,
&
S.
Engle, R.
W..
W.
(2004). The generality of working memory
A
latent-variable approach to verbal
133, 189-217.
Lachman,
Lachman.
R.,
An
processing:
L.,
J.
&
Butterfield, E. C.
and information
introduction.
A..
&
controUed-attention
Hillsdale,
NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum. Miller, E. K.,
&
Cohen,
J.
D. (2001).
An
integrative
of prefrontal cortex function. Annual
theory
&
Miyake, A.,
Shah,
P
24, 167-202.
(1999).
Models of working
memoiy: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. New York: Cambridge University Press. J.
S.
term:
(2002).
The case
Remembering over against
The blank
Pinker, S. (2002).
of human nature.
&
slate:
New York:
Spinath,
F.
M.
general cognitive ability Sciences,
Turner.
M.
L.,
6,
the short-
model.
standard
the
Annual Review of Psychology,
Plomin, R.,
Hunt. E. (1976). Varieties of cognitive power. In L.
Kane, M.
Naime,
316-322.
domain knowledge, working memory
tion
individual-differences perspective. Psyclumo-
Review of Neuroscience,
(1995). Cellular basis of work-
memory. Neuron,
J.
An
(1979). Cognitive psychology
Psychology: General, 128, 309-331.
Goldman-Rakic,
role
capacity,
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,
Conway, A. R. A. (1999). Working memory,
latent variable approach.
The
(2002).
working-memory
memory and
Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin,
Gray,
W.
R.
in
executive attention, and general fluid intelligence:
capacity:
of four hypotheses.
test
Memory, and Cognition,
ing
Engle,
and visuo-spatial memory span and reasoning.
J.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
term
&
J.,
of prefrontal cortex
(1992).
J.
Individual differences in working
comprehension:
130,
132, 47-70.
Johnson. R.,
lobe: The organization of goal-directed behavior.
59
169-183.
The contributions of goal
Allen Lane.
•
of Experimental Psychology: General,
mic Bulletin and Review,
164-170.
Intelligence
view of working-memory capacity. Journal
Kane, M.
450-466.
19,
Deary.
Mechanisms Underlying
53, 53-81.
The modern denial Viking.
(2002). Genetics and
(g).
Trends
W.
(1989).
in
Cognitive
169-176.
&
Engle, R.
Is
working
memory capacity task dependent? Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 127-154.
5 Working Memory
Capacity,
Attention Control,
AND Fluid Intelligence Richard
P.
Heitz
Nash Unsworth Randall W. Engle
Or take the power of attention. Is this wholly, or partly, or not at gence? All three views are widely held in the literature.
all the
same as
intelli-
Spearman (1927,
The
idea that attention
intelligence
length. tion
is
not
is
important to
novel.
Indeed,
Spearman (1927) discussed this issue at However, the relationship between atten-
and intelligence was contemplated even
earlier,
with some of the
first
empirical evidence
provided by Burt (1909). Binet (1903), the father
of intelligence testing,
AUTHORS' NOTE:
also
recognized the
p. 13)
importance of attention to general intelligence (Sternberg, 1982). William too,
attend to the
James (1890/1981),
wrote that "the number of things is
we may
altogether indefinite, depending on
power of
the individual intellect" (p. 405).
However, despite nearly a century of research, the
above question
of mental
ability,
still
pervades the science
and Spearman's comment
This research was supported by Grants F49620-97-1 and F49620-93- 1-0336 from the Air
Force Office of Scientific Research. Correspondence concerning
this
Atlanta,
GA
R NW.
chapter should be addressed to Richard
Heitz or Randall W. Engle, School of Psychology, Georgia Institute of Technology, 654 Cherry Street.
30332. E-mail: [email protected], [email protected], or randall. engle
@psych. gatech.edu. 61
62
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE
.
appears to be as tme today as
it was in 1927. However, converging evidence from both exper-
and factor-analytic methodologies
imental
intelligence
—
is
fluid
the ability to reason abstractly in
difficulty
studying the complex
in
attention-intelligence relationship
is
one
partly
of semantics. Both constructs are latent factors,
elude direct observation and must
in that they
Thus, one must cau-
be measured
indirectly.
tiously select
measurement
Because
and intelligence
of attention
theory
most
tools that are
sensitive to the constructs of interest.
one's
guides the selection of measures, observed cor-
depend on one's under-
relations will partially
lying theory. For this reason,
we
take particular
care in defining our conception of attention and intelligence. In the sections that follow,
and
just that,
theory,
attention
is
we do
related
one's
to
potential
for
There are three general types of theories the psychometric study of intelligence.
The
idea of "positive manifold"
observation that individuals
one intelligence intelligence
as the
cognitive
tasks;
test
tests.
But
structure).
extraction of ^ it
is
of
need not be the case. The
this is
just as
statistical
cal
methodologies, one can support either
Spearman's g factor or Thurstone's specific intelligences. It is important, therefore, to have
sound theoretical grounds for proposing the existence of
g,
Gf, Gc, or any other manifes-
tation of intellectual factors.
who
in
first
—
Measuring Intelligence AND Attention
the
score high on
Exactly what constitutes a good measure of been,
specific
intelligences
others hold a
Still
(e.g.,
somewhat
Sternberg. 1985).
One
such view that has become widely accepted
model of
developers
is
is
Cattell (1943, 1963).
that
g
is
actually
com-
it
who
validate their
one's ability to reason abstractly and perform
well in novel environments, whereas
Gc
corre-
and knowledge accumulated
over time, as might be measured by tests of fac-
knowledge. Evidence
performance
tasks (e.g.. Engle.
leads
us
to
Gf
consistently
in higher-order cognitive
Kane,
believe
that
that
&
Tuholski,
attention
1999)
exerts
its
is
This,
what
much
of
some form.
In
This circular logic appears in
the current literature, at least in
examining the relationship between working
memory (WM) and
intelligence
experimental research),
it is
not
(at
least
uncommon
in
that
researchers use a single intelligence measure
Raven Progressive Matrices) as the cri(e.g.. Brewin & Beaton, 2002). This begs
(e.g., the
the following question:
reflects
tests.
has been measured by past intelligence tests"
terion
Gf
admonishes measure by
with other intelligence
(Gc) and general-fluid (Gf) intelligence (see 1993). Accordingly,
earlier
in
he says, "takes the view that intelligence
posed of two components: general-crystallized
to abilifies
particularly
intelligence research. Cattell (1943)
of
Cattell's viewpoint
has
intelligence
research, rather post hoc. Criticizing practices in
test
intermediate view
relates to
a part of theory as
tend to score high on other
(Thurstone, 1938).
tual
much
methodology. For example,
Cattell (1943) notes that, using different statisti-
(p. 159).
sponds
generally
are
This idea, originated by
variety
also Carroll,
factors
measures showing the highest loadings (simple
proposing no single psychometric factor but
the hierarchical
lesser
manner consistent with those
interpreted in a
correlating
a
principal
first
Spearman (1904, 1927), suggests that there is a single intellectual ability on which people differ. At the other end of the spectrum are theories instead
factor-
in
Commonly, g emerges component or the highest
analytic methodology.
that one's ability to control
abstract reasoning.
stresses the
also disagreement as to
is
what constitutes psychometric g
our guiding
interest of
the
in
we show
our discussion
limit
order factor extracted from a battery of broad
novel environments and situations.
The
on Gf, and we
This aside, there
is
beginning to support the notion that attention
one of the major determinants of general
strongest effect
to this construct.
What
is
special about
Raven test that other measures lack? The answer to this question lies in the fact that the measure consistently loads highly on g and the
appears to be quite sensitive to individual differ-
ences (Marshalek. Lohman.
Although
this
is
appropriate
&
Snow. 1983).
for
small-scale
experimental research, more sensitive involve the extraction of a this reason,
we have used
Gf the
tests
latent factor.
Raven
test
For
alone
— Working Memory Capacity, Attention Control, and Fluid Intelligence in
experimental research and. in addition, have
included multiple measures of
Gf
in
our factor-
A
similar
problem
exists in defining atten-
William James's (1890/1981) adage
tion.
"everyone knows what attention true
on the
is" is certainly
level of introspection, but deriving a
on proves
definition that all can agree
Some
that
difficult.
only to main-
attest that attention serves
tain information through activation (Anderson.
Reder,
&
Lebiere. 1996).
some renounce
&
(Hasher
tion in favor of inhibition
&
1977).
Shiffrin.
We
ciation
more
between controlled and automatic pro-
cessing, with the former being effortful and
some type of (attentional) capacity And the manner in which control
subject to limitation.
operates
due
likely
is
to
specialized mecha-
Our
nisms, such as activation and inhibition. stance, then, untary,
is
that attentional control is a vol-
cognitive
effortful
act
serves to
that
maintain information through activation of relevant brain
circuitr\'.
task
ilance
is
respond
to quickly
to the
The attention required in a somewhat different than
vigthat
required on, say, a divided-attention task, in
which participants must perform two tasks simultaneously. is
another variety of attention
Still
manifest in selective-attention tasks, in
which
channels, of information, and they must select
a disso-
is
onset of targets.
participants are presented multiple sources, or
take a
comprehensive view. Certainly, there
are able to maintain attention to the task at
hand are more able
Zacks,
attention as simply controlled cognifion (e.g.,
Schneider
Those
activa-
and others remain agnostic, treating
1988),
the
in
service of detecting an infrequent target.
who
analytic studies, reported below.
must maintain attention
participants
63
•
and
inhibit the irrelevant
on us
one and disregard another. Although
it
might be
argued that attention serves different roles vigilance,
we would
attention tasks,
common
and
divided-attention,
argue that what
to all is the control
in
selectiveis
of attenfion. In
other words, attention can be put to use in a variety of ways, but the act of voluntarily
employing
on capacity-limited exec-
attention always relies
we
utive control. For example,
find that both
selective-attention tasks, such as dichotic listen-
Cowan.
ing (Conway,
&
Bunting. 2001). and
divided-attention tasks (often a cognitive task
incorporating a secondary load; e.g.,
Kane
&
Engle. 2000) reliably differentiate individuals
at
rated high or low in attentional control ability,
any one time, and suppress prepotent response
but tasks that can be performed automatically do
distracting information that impinges
tendencies that are task irrelevant.
keep
tant to
different
in
from a
mind
is
It
impor-
that this is qualitatively
situation in
which attention
is
automatically captured, such as by a flashing light or
theory
is
an unexpected sound. Note that far reaching.
type of information that
due
to
By our definition, any may be lost over time
decay or interference must be maintained
through attentional control. This
form of a grocery fic,
or
this
it
may be
list
may
take the
while driving in busy
the flashing
is
cue" when
the
&
Conway,
&
Engle. 2001;
Baylis, 2001).
apparent that individuals
who
ability to use controlled attention
From
this,
it
differ in their
may be
disso-
ciated using a variety of paradigms. In other
memory
words, working
by our
capacity
(WMC)
logic, attention control are not task
and.
depen-
dent but are a factor in any task that requires attention control (Turner
One
traf-
a response production such as
"Look away from
not (Kane. Bleckley.
Tuholski, Engle.
activity
individuals
is
that
&
Engle, 1989).
does not differentiate
automatic attention capture
certainly a special case of attentional
phenomena.
flashing cue tends to reflexively capture atten-
Here, an innate mechanism directs attention,
tion or the control of a socially unacceptable
effortlessly
implicit
attitude
to
prevent
it
from being
expressed in publicly observable behavior.
Such a stance
is
consistent with a
so-called "varieties of attention."
a
number of researchers
(e.g.,
number of
As noted by Parasuraman.
lus
and without
volition, to
some stimu-
such as the sudden appearance of some
object in the periphery or any feature such as flicker that suggests
movement. This
is
certainly
an adaptive response, as suddenly appearing objects
may be hazardous
or helpful, predator
1998), attention serves different types of func-
or prey. Although automatic attentional capture
depending on task demands. One variety
does not require control, resisting attention cap-
tions
of attention comes into play during sustained
ture
attention tasks, such as vigilance. In such a task.
(Kane
by a
salient
et al..
event does require control
2001). That
is.
executive control
64 is
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE
.
important for the endogenous prevention of
exogenous capture from
both internal
and exter-
nal distractors.
To understand
we must
issue,
attention-intelligence
the
first
two constructs, as the
these factor
is
how we measure
explain
validity of a latent
only as good as the manifest variables
comprising
it.
thought to tap
WMC
reliably
ability to control attention.
Following
As we
apparent that
some way
We
and
we sig-
fluid
illustrate in this chapter,
it is
WMC and fluid intelligence are in
related but clearly not isomorphic.
submit that the relationship
ability to control attention
by both
tion tapped
gence
this,
show a
WMC
between
one's
reflect
describe macroanalytic studies that nificant relationship
review
suggesting that tasks
microanalytic research
intelligence.
we
In the next section,
is
due
to the
—an aspect of cogni-
WMC
and
fluid intelli-
we
consider such
tests. In a final section,
alternative explanations as processing speed
and
WMC
by the number of items complex span task, the scores on such measures reflect controlled-attention abilquantitatively
recalled on
ity
rather than the
mation
that
Accordingly, it}'
number of
can be held
in a
we propose
that
"units'"
of infor-
short-term store.
WMC
is
an abil-
which an individual
reflecting the extent to
is
able to control attention, particularly in situations
involving
from competing
interference
information, activated representations, or task
demands. Before citing evidence for the controlled attention
WMC.
view of
how
clear exactly
it
WMC
is
necessary to
is
make
assessed and the
rationale for this particular methodology. In line
with the view that
WM consists of both domain-
specific storage buffers
central executive central
control
decrease
in the
be maintained.
and a domain-general
mechanism, tasks
that load the
mechanism should lead to a amount of information that can In
other
words, including a
heavy processing component should tax the
task complexity.
attentional system to such an extent that to-be-
recalled information
Microanalytic Studies OF Working
Memory
tions are often
AND Controlled Attention
is
to this end.
And,
inter-
appear to capture
ability that is related to g.
Fogarty and
tory tasks requiring maintenance and/or manip-
is
clear in recent
(e.g..
1996). but
also evident in earlier models. In particular.
Atkinson and Shiffrin (1968) noted that infor-
mation held
in
the
short-term
store
(STS)
decays over time, unless control processes are invoked to refresh these traces using rehearsal. Thus, attention needs to be directed to information held online to
view
this
is
keep
it
active. Admittedly,
quite different
from theories of
immediate memory focusing on the number of units of information that can be maintained over
G. A. Miller, 1956). But, as
time
(e.g.,
tend.
WMC
se.
is
not about a
number
or an
we
con-
amount
Using extreme group methodology, we
have observed numerous dissociations between individuals rated high and low in
simply cannot be explained tion
lost
working memory system
the
models proposed by Baddeley
per
be
Dual-task situa-
Stankov (1982) presented individuals with audi-
an attentional construct. This
it is
employed
likely to
important to realize that the central execu-
component of
tive
stores.
estingly, dual-task situations
some It is
more
is
from domain-specific
in
WMC
that
terms of a limita-
based on a number of units. The
differences
ulation of tones or letters. In one condition, participants
another,
performed these tasks alone:
in
these tasks were performed as dual
They found that the tasks correlated more when they were components of a dual task than when they were performed alone, suggesting that some additional ability became necessary when performing the two tasks simultaneously that was not required by either task alone. In addition, Fogarty and Stankov examined the first principal component (representing all of the tasks). As Fogarty and Stankov argue, the nature of the tasks employed suggests that the first principal component can be considered a Gf factor. They found that dual tasks load higher on the first principal component than single tasks, suggesting that they were better meatasks.
strongly
sures of
Gf
In other words,
showed higher
competing tasks
positive manifold than
single
are intrinsically qualitative rather than quantita-
tasks (Stankov. 1983; but see Stankov. 1989).
we measure
Recent neuroimaging research strengthens the
tive.
In
other words,
although
Working Memory Capacity, Attention Control, and Fluid Intelligence
view
dual
that
measure an important
tasks
between
Shin (1995) had participants perform single or
that
in a
functional magentic resonance
imaging (fMRl) scanner. They found
marked increases
tasks elicit
between
link
both complex span and reading comprehension
performance are predicated on word knowledge,
and executive
prefrontal cortex
Kane
&
Engle, 2002, for a review;
see also Chapter 9, this volume) and (b) our
view
dual-task
that
situations
Known
heavy
place
demands on executive control. The first widely accepted test of constructed by
WMC
was
(1980).
as the reading span task (RSpan), it
it is
consists of both
storage and processing components. In the orig-
RSpan
were asked
task design, participants
to six sentences (each set size presented
three times), participants last
were cued
word of each. The point
at
then the
were
that if this
WMC-reading comprehension
should
but
words
high-frequency
with
disappear
words. They found that the
true,
correla-
tion should be strong with low-frequency
WMC-comprehen-
was somewhat stronger with low-frequency words but still very much significant with high-frequency words. From this, Engle et al. (1990) argued that although word knowledge does play some role in the
to recall the
which the
partic-
WM WM
span-comprehension correlation, a general capacity limitafion
was
the important predictor.
Dual tasks such as RSpan and OSpan appear
from
to read sets of sentences out loud. After
two
leading to a correlation. Engle, Nations, and
Cantor (1990) reasoned
sion correlation
Daneman and Carpenter
inherently a dual task in that
inal
individuals simply have larger lexi-
cons than others. This hypothesis argued that
that dual
did not. This would be expected given (a) the
control (see
WMC and reading comprehension was
some
in prefrontal cortex
whereas either task performed alone
activity,
65
Another possible explanation of the conelation
executive ability. D'Esposito. Detre. Alsop. and
dual tasks
•
to
measure an important aspect of real-world
high-level cognition, as they are found to correlate
with such tasks as language comprehension,
ipants could accurately recall the final words
following directions, vocabulary learning, rea-
two out of three times was taken as their WMC. was significantly correlated
soning,
with higher-level comprehension measures,
between dual-task
such as reading comprehension and the ability
such as simple digit or word span,
Critically.
WMC
to identify a
noun
referent for a later occurring
Daneman and Carpenter
pronoun.
Daneman
&
Tardif,
(1980, 1983;
1987) argued that
WMC
reflects processing efficiency involved in reading. Accordingly,
cient
those individuals with
reading comprehension
available
more
residual
WMC
to use for
words.
taining
the
to-be-recalled
against
the
processing efficiency
from a study using a
WMC
effi-
processes have
idea
task in
ticipants solved arithmetic strings
main-
Evidence
came
which par-
and remem-
complex
(Engle. Kane, et
beyond
ing
it
more
However,
tasks.
The
that
some
by mak-
WM.
individuals
are simply better at multitasking in dual-task
being able to strategically allocate
situations,
resources toward the
primary and secondary
components of the task
in
an effort to maximize
span scores. These same individuals
may
also be
complex cognitive WMC-higher-order cognition
strategic during
lation.
tion hypothesis,
tasks,
corre-
This stance, termed the strategic alloca-
was evaluated by Engle, Cantor,
and Carullo (1992). To evaluate
(1980). indicating that the correlation between
Engle
WMC
assess the
not depen-
task,
that dual
difficult to refresh traces in
higher with measures of comprehension than
is
is
by simple span
might also be
it
(OSpan)' score correlated just as well and often
and higher-order cognition
critical difference
and a single
hypothesis, occupies the central executive,
leading to a
by Daneman and Carpenter
The
situations
that required
and Engle (1989) found
those reported
1999).
extra processing load incurred by these tasks,
more
span
and note taking
learning,
tasks require additional processing above and
bered the word that followed each string. Turner that this operation
al..
et al.
this hypothesis,
used a moving window technique to
amount of processing given
to different
WMC task, with amount of pro-
dent on the similarity of the processing com-
segments of the
ponent with the task being predicted; thus, the
cessing operationally defined as viewing time
RSpan-comprehension correlation could not
(VT).
be due to efficiency of reading processes, as
allocation,
Daneman and Carpenter
ficant negative correlation
asserted.
If
high spans are more strategic in resource
one would expect
to
observe a signi-
between
VT
on the
66
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE
.
processing component and span score. The
Analogous data are provided by Kane
et al.
authors found that viewing times did not correlate
(2001), using the anti-saccade task. In these
with span scores, leaving the strategic allocation
studies, participants
hypothesis untenable.
saccade (an overt eye movement) toward (pro-
Tasks such as RSpan and
OSpan might
appear to reflect individual differences
number of items
that can be
in the
maintained
in
an
attention-demanding dual-task situation. However,
view advanced
is
matically
P,
(anti-saccade) a flashing
to identify a briefly presented
or R. Because exogenous cues auto-
capture
attention,
anti-saccade
the
number of elements
response as well as a controlled saccade in the opposite direction. There are two ways perfor-
WMC
work
is
that indi-
reflect a relatively
mance is hindered in the anti-saccade condition. The first is through interference: Participants
stable characteristic of an individual's ability
who
to control attention to maintain information in
prepotent response will be slow to
an active or easily accessible
rect
state in interfer-
are not adept at inhibiting the automatic
saccade
(requiring that one look
distracting information.
cue).
Evidence for
this
claim
vided by
Conway
found
is
One
(2001
et al.
in a
number
elegant test )
is
pro-
using the vener-
The second
is
ing left look right")
tend to
make
away from
through a loss
tenance. For example,
make
anti-saccade
the
in
ence-rich contexts as well as block irrelevant,
of different paradigms.
a
misguided. Again, the
in the present
vidual differences in
B,
letter:
make
condition required the inhibition of a prepotent
focusing on a limitation in the
can be maintained
away from
exogenous cue
to
WMC
evidence makes clear that a theory of
that
saccade) or
were required
if
the goal
is lost,
in
a cor-
condition a flashing
goal main-
("When
flash-
participants should
a rapid error by looking toward the
phenomenon." Moray's
flashing cue, with their behavior guided by the
(1959) dichotic listening paradigm, originally
prepotent response tendency to look toward a
used by Cherry (1953). provided evidence that
flashing stimulus. Following from our theory,
able
party
"cocktail
particularly salient information
tended channel (see
filter
Moray's to
is
also
from an unat-
but should differ in anti-saccade performance:
In
were asked
a relevant channel while
point, the participants
own name was
spo-
unattended message. Moray found
ken
in the
that
approximately
33%
of participants reported
name when
it
was presented
to the
Conway et al. reasoned that if working memory capacity is related to the ability to
ear.
control attention in the service of selecting
relevant input and inhibiting irrelevant input,
then those rated high in
WMC
likely to report hearing their
should be less
own name
in the
irrelevant channel during a dichotic listening
The results confirmed that high spans were indeed more proficient in inhibiting the irrelevant channel: Low-span individuals were much more likely (65%) to report hearing their own
task.
name
pro-saccade performance
als
1995).
ignoring information in an irrelevant channel. At
unattended
it
and low-span individu-
allowed through an attentional
classic study, participants
hearing their
that high-
Wood & Cowan,
shadow words from
some
was expected
than were high-span individuals (20%).
This suggests that high-span individuals were at blocking distractors from the message they were instructed to ignore, whereas low-span individuals were less able to block the
indeed better
distracting information.
should not differ
When
in
an anti-saccade
performed correctly,
is
low spans should be slower, indicating a culty in resolving conflict.
commit more
Low
diffi-
spans should also
errors, indicative of goal neglect
(De Jong. Berendsen,
&
when an
should be
error occurs,
ing that the behavior
it
Cools,
1999).
And
fast, indicat-
was indeed automatic.
These hypotheses were confirmed: High- and low-span participants did not differ
in
pro-
saccade performance
in
both
but
did
differ
latency and error rate for anti-saccade perfor-
mance. fast,
And when
an error did occur,
it
was very
indicafive of goal neglect.
Interestingly,
in
an additional experiment,
Unsworth, Schrock, and Engle (2003) showed
do emerge in a pro-saccade you force individuals to perform them in a controlled manner. Unsworth et al. intermixed pro- and anti-saccade trials within the same block. Each trial began with a symbol that span differences
condition
if
that designated
or anti-saccade.
whether the
Under
trial
required a pro-
this situation,
low spans
were both slower and more error prone on both pro- and anti-saccade trials. Unsworth et al.
Working Memory Capacity, Attention Control, and Fluid Intelligence argue that this manipulation placed a premium
participants differ
on actively maintaining the goal of the creating a situation in which both pro- and
conflict
saccade
WMC
if
WMC
task, they
should show greater differences in the Stroop
when incongruent
task
are
trials
relatively
infrequent.
This hypothesis was supported.
based on the maintenance of
When 75%
of the
to
difficult
some number of items with such is,
resolve
to
ability
their
in
and maintain the goal of the
reconcile a
would be very
It
anti-
must be performed with control.
trials
theory of
task,
67
•
a finding. That
were due
limitations
to a lack of
trials
were congruent, highs
and lows differed markedly not
in
accuracy rate but
RT, suggesting that the manipulation
in
it
should
increased goal neglect for low spans. However,
not play a role in the anti-saccade task.
As we
the Stroop task
available "slots" to hold information,
common mechanism
argue, the
anti-saccade
mance tion.
is
performance and
condition likely to favor high
WMC
as previously discussed.
perfor-
employ controlled
the ability to
atten-
appears that in the anti-saccade task,
It
span differences
response time (RT) are due
in
or no compatible
RT. That
is,
reinforced
orienting response and execute a planned sac-
trials,
in the
ability to exert tion, as
we
of an
the ink,
endogenous control over
atten-
to
define
it.
Also
in
controlled attention theory
of low
ity
is
when
the task goal
was continually
by presenting many incongruent
presumably because
to say the color it
of
took them longer
resolve the conflict between the response
name
agreement with the
tendencies to say the word and to say the
an apparent inabil-
of the color. These findings illustrate two impor-
maintain the
spans to actively
response production "Look away from the flashing cue." Loss of this production
may
be the
WM:
tant functions of
relevant information
through
anti-saccade condition.
(incongruent Stroop
These observations led Engle and colleagues propose a two-factor theory of executive
maintenance of goal-
(i.e.,
name
the color
and
not the word) and dealing with interference
locus for span differences in error rate in the
to
were few
there
WM span groups did not
low spans were slower
—aspects
opposite direction
trials,
WMC individuals,
When
differ in accuracy rate but did differ slightly in
to a differential ability to inhibit the reflexive
cade
also rich in interference, a
is
affecting both
that
of
inhibition
controlled
require
responses
prepotent
trials).
In
summary, tasks
attention
to
maintain
information, inhibit distractor information, or
to increa-
block prepotent response tendencies seem to
sed latency and goal neglect leads to fast errors.
dissociate individuals scoring low or high on
Lending further credence
dual tasks such as the
attention,
study by
whereby interference leads
to this theory
Kane and Engle (2003).
is
performed a color-word Stroop task under ferent
cates
(e.g.,
that
Logan
&
individuals
incongruent Stroop
Zbrodoff, 1979) indi-
make more
trials
them
when
relative to
there
is
errors
on
word red naming the ink
(saying the
printed in green ink instead of
color only)
dif-
Previous
proportions of congruency.
research
a
a small proportion of
congruent Stroop
interpretation for this observation
The
Participants
One
trials. is
that
repeated presentation of an incongruent
the trial
dissociations
OSpan
or RSpan.
between high and low
WMC individuals described above employ relatively low-level cognitive tasks.
However, these
differences are true for higher-level tasks as well.
Low
spans suffer more than high spans in
a proactive interference task (Kane
2000), in a degree-of-fan task (Cantor 1993;
Conway
&
& &
Engle, Engle,
Engle, 1994), and in a cate-
gory generation paradigm that requires suppression (Rosen
&
Engle, 1997; see also Rosen
&
Engle, 1998).
(such as the word blue presented in red ink) reinforces the goal of the task. Conversely,
incongruent lose
trials
are
infrequent,
when
participants
Motivation
maintenance of the response production
"Respond to the color, not the word" because most of the time, they can perform well by relying on relatively automatic word reading. The prediction, then, following from the antisaccade findings,
is
that if high-
and low-span
The prevalence and leaves
open the
direction of span differences
possibility
that
some
factor
other than differences in ability to control attention differentiates the is
often
the
case
two groups. Namely,
that
it
high-span individuals
68
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE
.
outperform low spans; thus, the suggestion that
some dimension such
the groups differ along
as
"motivation" cannot be ignored, though there is
good evidence against such
example, high- and low-span groups differ theoretically meaningful
They do not
for both groups. In other words, monetary incentive encouraged both groups to "try" harder.
The above experimental
For
view.
a
amounts
in
and predictable ways:
differ in a typical pro-saccade task,
make
studies
WMC.
strong case for the hypothesis that
a as
measured by dual-task situations such as OSpan and RSpan. reflect one's ability to control atten-
when
of a proactive
tion.
This capability
interference task (before interference has built
tion
and interference place a premium on
in visual search, or in the first trial
up),
as mentioned
above.
If
low spans were
simply unmotivated, one would not expect them to
perform as well as high spans
A thorough
test
in
any condition.
of this explanation was
made
maintaining
is
important
information,
particularly
ence. If ity to
we
are correct in
control attention
is
assuming
that the abil-
made up of
WM
latent factor
strongly related to a latent variable
while individuals found to be high and low
Gf
spans (measured using OSpan) performed the
analytic methodology.
to
task. Pupillary dilation has
the
important to Gf, then a
by Heitz, Schrock. Payne, and Engle (2003).
RSpan
in
presence of strong internal or external interfer-
They measured task-evoked pupil
dilations
distrac-
measures. To
test this,
tasks should be
we
made up of
turn to factor-
been shown
be sensitive to mental effort and processing
load (Hess
&
1966). Thus,
Kahneman
Polt, 1964: if
&
Beatty,
span groups do differ along a
Macroanalytic Studies of AND Fluid Intelligence
WMC
dimension such as motivation, one would expect larger
task-evoked pupillary responses from
high spans,
who may
simply expend more effort
during task performance.
Of
course,
could
it
also be that high spans are simply
more
information
Daneman
processors
(e.g..
efficient
&
Carpenter, 1980) than low spans, which would predict smaller pupil dilations from high spans.
Ahern and Beatty (1979) found
Interestingly, that this
was
true for high-
and low-intelligence
One could pose regarding
memory and
a
number of
intelligence
fluid
questions
initial
between working
relationship
the
microa-
that
nalytic research cannot address. First,
WMC
various
tasks
reflect
a
do the
common
con-
Second, does that construct show con-
struct?
struct validity in predicting
some other
criterion
behavior as well as discriminant validity? third,
what
is
the relationship
between
And
WMC
individuals (high intelligence exhibiting smaller
and other constructs such as short-term memory
pupil dilations), although there are problems
(STM)? We argued
with their methodology (see Heitz et
situations tap an attentional control ability that
Heitz
et al.
al.,
span individuals consistently recall more correctly than
more mental
2003).
(2003) found that although highletters
do low spans, they do not exert
effort. Specifically,
high spans did
is
important to higher level cognition. If
indeed true that
and beyond
STM
and
ing of the
letter,
better
fit
critically,
when
recalling the letters.-
pupillary dilation
was
or.
However,
it
is
WMC tasks tap an ability above by simple span tasks, model including separable
that required
a structural equation
not exhibit larger pupil dilations during encod-
while reading the sentence,
previously that dual-task
WM
constructs
to the data than a
should provide a
model consisting of
a single latent factor. In addition,
show good discriminant
these con-
if
validity, a
STM
sensitive to processing
structs
load in this study, as there
latent factor should not correlate significantly
in pupil size as set sizes
was a linear increase grew larger. In addition,
with a
baseline pupil size measurements, recorded at the beginning of each
trial,
related with the set size
suggesting that
previously
how
affected
were strongly cor-
on the previous
trial,
"hard" one had to work one's
pupil
size
later.
Gf factor,
but a
WM latent factor should.
Obviously, no cognitive measure pure,"
meaning
or only
WMC.
that
is
Quite to the contrary,
that both simple
"process
no task measures only
we
STM assert
span tasks and complex span
tasks reflect similar constructs but in different
simple span or
STM
Furthermore, a monetary incentive reliably
proportions. That
increased baseline
measure, by and large, domain-specific storage
pupil
size
but
in
equal
is.
tasks
Working Memory Capacity, Attention Control, and Fluid Intelligence
.64
.63^^
.79
RSPAN
>
4Q*
WM
-f
^
CSPAN
^^^^^.^
.6r>^
^\
R .81*
y^A X_y f
.74
>
y^
BSPAN
67*
.41
RAVEN
29*/
^
common)—
gF
.67
.74*
CATTELL
^85* .79*
.61
V-
FSPAND .71*^^
.70
* FSPANS
Note: The latent
WM
,'-'.12
^ Model From Engle, Tuholski,
Structural Equation
Figure 5.1
69
.^^^77*
OSPAN
.78
•
variable
still
correlates with the latent
Gf
(1999)
et al.
variable after partialling the \ariance due to
represent manifest, or obser\ed. variables; circles represent latent factors. Paths
marked with an
STM. Boxes
asterisk are significant at
.05. WM = working memory: STM = short-term memory: Gf = general fluid intelligence: OSPAN = operation span: RSPAN = reading span: CSPAN = counting span: BSPAN = backwards digit span: FSPAND = forward digit span dissimilar: FSPANS = forward digit span similar: RAVENS = Ravens Progressive Matrices: CATTELL = Cattell's Culture-Fair Test.
p
six-back flict
event-related
for
activity. In the a priori search, lure trial activity
parietal,
activity.
in
a
— LPFC,
lateral posterior
anterior
cerebellum
cingulate,
—and,
and
second, via
lure
and target
activity
LPFC
trials, this
correlation with brain
was much weaker. Despite and
parietal
the fact that
areas generally
their activity in the /7-back task
increased
compared
to
baseline, none of this sustained brain activity
correlated significantly with Gf, thus recalling the
analogy of distinguishing basic braking
158
.
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE ability.
Consistent with
some work from
the
neuropsychological
tra-
0.8
dition, in
which behav-
ioral deficits are linked
0)
O)
c
with particular patterns
to
of brain damage,
LPFC
areas appear to be par55
0.0
-
important to
ticularly
Gf
the perfonnance of In
tasks.
both
verbal
and nonverbal domains, inductive,
and
relational,
sometimes gF (Raven's
APM
score)
(and
transitive
deductive)
problems
reasoning
LPFC
elicit
special
Of
activity.
importance, no
other brain regions have
been identified
High gF 0.2
literature
-
O)
c
across
the
so
involved
consistently
0)
in
are
that
of
variety
the
(0
reasoning and stimulus
o 15
domains reviewed
c
O)
0.0
here.
This research area
is
growing and producing interesting
ble
and
replica-
However,
results.
-0.2
from the perspective of
LowgF
trying
—
1
2
1
4
3
1
1
6
5
TR =
2.36
inform
understanding of
8
7
gence, Delay (scan frames,
to
our
r-
1
s)
it
intelli-
time
is
to
move beyond examining brain activity during
Figure 9.5
Neuroimaging Data From Gray, Chabris, and Braver (2003)
individual
reasoning
The
multivariate
tasks. Note: (A) Correlation between Raven's Advanced Progressive Matrices
(RAPM)
score and
functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) signal from lateral prefrontal cortex
during correctly rejected three-back lure
LPFC
trials.
during correctly rejected three-back lure
mechanisms from sources of braking In
sum, only
WM
Time course of fMRI
signal
struct
from the
some connection be made between activity
high- versus low-Gf participants.
and performance
variation.
processes that were tied
directly to interference control
by Gf-related brain
(B)
trials for
were supported
task and activity
and performance
recent study by
Gray
field activity.
Gf condemands that
nature of the
(LPFC)
et al.
in
one
in others.
A
(2003) moves the
forward by linking reasoning performance
and brain
activity across multiple tasks
and by
using marker tasks of theoretically important
Conclusion
constructs, such as
WM
and attention control.
Their important findings support the behavioral
Neuroimaging research has begun
to
reveal
the brain structures important to general fluid
associations structs
among
and suggest
WM. that
attention,
PFC
and Gf con-
activity related to
Neuroimaging of Reasoning and Intelligence is what binds these constructs work that continues to combine Future
Tetris.
together.
and whole-brain metabolism during
and technique
theory
science
promise
great
advancing our understanding of not
in
how minds work
only
holds
how
but also
they differ
1
59
Although the correlation between Raven's score
attention control
cognitive-behavioral, psychometric, and neuro-
•
mance showed
=
tice (naive r
perfor-
Tetris
the predicted reduction following prac-
=
.77, practiced r
Raven's scores
.09),
continued to predict Tetris performance (naive r
=
practiced r
.41
LPFC
and
),
=
.36,
activity during Tetris did
not change with practice, remaining substantial at both
from one another and why.
time periods.
No
were reported between
correlations
Raven's performance and regional brain activity
Notes
in
either naive or practiced performers; only naive-to-
practiced change measures were reported. In parallel with the figural task,
1.
Duncan
et al.
(2000) also tested participants with an odd-man-out Service
modification of the
Educational Testing
(ETS)
(Ekstrom, French, Harman,
letter series test
Dermen. 1976). Here, four were presented
sets of letter
and
in a row,
same
structed according to the
but one
all
sequences
was con-
abstract rule. Again,
a control version of the task served as a baseline, in
which the to-be-discovered
rule
was
relatively obvi-
ous (three sequences were alphabetical). In contrast to the figural task, the letter task yielded only left
lateralized
acuvity (10/46. 46, 47). Although the
verbal nature of the task ization,
was
it
may account
its
lateral-
also true that the verbal reasoning and
more
control tasks had
similar g factor loadings to
each other than did the spatial tasks
and so the control condition
What
is
(LPFC)
less appropriate
that both the
activity similar to that seen in the
Raven's Progressive Matrices
The
2.
first
I
test
(Gur
(RPM)
The
et al., 1994).
34 adults
in
an analogies
spatial resolution for the
regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) measurements
was
poor, but one detector appears to have been
placed over
LPFC Area
9; in fact, this area,
with other prefrontal cortex (PFC) structures,
compared 3.
showed elevated
Berman (1999) so
it
is
4.
and posterior
activity during the task
rCBF
Kirkby, Van
Horn, Ellmore. and
did not report a relative measure of
like that
from Haier
unclear whether that finding Haier.
Siegel.
et al. (1988). is
also replicable.
Tang. Abel, and
(1992) tested participants
in
and
Buchsbaum
the Raven's
Advanced
Progressive Matrices test in a session outside the scan-
M. O.
Boyle,
mem-
Journal of Exper-
abilities.
Acuna. B. D., Eliassen, Sanes,
J.
J.
C, Donoghue,
N. (2002). Frontal and
P.,
J.
&
parietal lobe
acdvation during transidve inference in humans.
Cerebral Cortex,
12,
1312-1321.
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford,
UK: Oxford
University Press.
D.,
&
In G. A.
Hitch.
G.
Bower
(Ed.),
New York: Academic Barbas, H.,
Working
(1974).
The psychology
of learning and motivation (Vol.
8, pp.
47-89).
Press.
& Mesulam, M. M. (1985). Cortical afferent
input to the principalis region of the rhesus 15,
619-637.
&
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., S.
W.
Anderson,
(1998). Dissociation of working
memory
from decision making within the human prefrontal cortex. Journal
Berker, E.,
of Neuroscience,
& Smith, A.
and other factors
in
18,
428^37.
(1988). Diaschisis,
site,
dme,
Raven performance of adults
with focal cerebral lesions. International Journal
of Neuroscience, 38, 267-285.
Berman, K. F, lllowsky, B. P,
to a resting baseline.
Esposito,
regional
along
&
E.,
imental Psychology: General, 131, 567-589.
monkey. Neuroscience,
test.
imaging study of verbal reasoning
am aware of examined
that
and perceptual speed
memory.
is
M.
Beier,
L.,
ory within a nomological network of cognitive
may
inductive reasoning tasks evoked lateral prefrontal
cortex
P.
(2002). Individual differences in working
Baddeley, A.
in the letter task
however,
clear,
Ackerman,
(in a pilot study),
have been more demanding and a baseline.
for
References
&
&
Weinberger. D. R.
(1988). Physiological dysfunction of dorsolateral
prefrontal
cortex
in
schizophrenia:
IV.
Further evidence for regional and behavioral specificity.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 45,
616-622. Braver, T. S., Cohen,
Smith, E.
E..
&
J.
D.. Nystrom, L. E., Jonides,
Noll, D. C. (1997).
A
ner,
and these participants were given extensive prac-
study of prefrontal cortex involvement
tice
on a computerized visuospatial-motor game called
working memory. Neurolmage,
5,
J.,
parametric in
49-62.
human
160
.
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE M., Colombo, N.. Delia Sala,
Brazzelli,
&
S.,
Spinnler, H. (1994). Spared and impaired cogni-
damage.
frontal
bilateral
after
abilities
tive
P.
M.
A., Just,
one intelligence
& Shell, P. (1990). What
A.,
test
A
measures:
account of the processing
in the
theoretical
Raven Progres-
Psychological Review, 97,
sive Matrices test.
404-^31.
&
Goldberg.
(1982).
J.
Operational efficiency and the growth of short-
memory
span. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 33, 386-404.
&
Chorover, S. L, tion
(1966). Delayed alterna-
in
patients
with
cerebral
K.,
J.
Rostrolateral
Dorfman,
Holyoak, K.
Zhao,
J..
Z.,
(2001).
et al.
J.,
cortex involvement in
prefrontal
relational integration during reasoning.
Neurolm-
age, 14. 1136-1149. J.
B.
D.,
J.,
Forman,
S.
D., Braver, T. S., Casey,
&
D.,
D.
Noli,
C.
(1994). Activation of the prefrontal cortex in a
nonspatial working
memory
task with functional
MRI. Human Brain Mapping, A.,
&
Bulletin
A
meta-
Review,
3,
Cowan,
N.,
293-304.
1,
&
The cogni-
(Ed.),
MIT
Press.
&
Burgess, P,
J.,
Emslie. H. (1995). Fluid
intelligence after frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsy-
chologia, 33, 261-268.
Duncan,
Emslie, H., Williams,
J.,
The organization of
lobe:
Johnson, R.,
P.,
&
(1996). Intelligence and the frontal
goal-directed behav-
Cognitive Psychology, 30, 257-303.
ior.
J.,
Seitz, R.
Ahmed,
Kolodny,
J.,
A., et
Bor, D., Herzog, H.,
J.,
(2000).
al.
A
neural basis for
general intelligence. Science, 289, 457-460.
Duncan.
Williams.
J..
Brown,
(1993).
I.
Nimmo-Smith,
P..
The
&
I.,
control of skilled behav-
Learning, intelligence, and distraction. In
Meyer
D. E.
&
Komblum
S.
(Eds.), Attention
and performance XIV: Synergies
in
psychology, artificial intelligence,
experimental
and
cognitive
MIT
neuroscience (pp. 323-341). Cambridge: Press.
M.
Bunting,
M. Gazzaniga
neurosciences (pp. 721-733). Cambridge:
tive
ior:
Servan-Schreiber,
Conway. A. R.
Working
(1996).
(1995). Attention, intelligence, and the
J.
frontal lobes. In
Duncan,
lesions. Neuropsychologia, 4, 1-7.
Christoff, K., Prabhakaran, V.,
Cohen,
Duncan,
Freer, C.
M.
Cole.
performance
Kroger,
Psychonomic
analysis.
Duncan.
Case, R., Kurland, M. D..
term
P M.
Merikle.
memory and language comprehension: 422^33.
Cortex, 30, 27-51.
Carpenter,
&
Daneman. M.,
F.
Dureman,
&
I.,
Salde, H. (1959). Psychometric
and
phenomenon
revisited:
experimental methods for the clinical evaluation
The importance of working memory
capacity.
of mental functioning. Stockholm: Almqvuist
(2001).
The
cocktail party
Psychonomic Bulletin and Review;
Conway, A. R.
Cowan,
A.,
Therriault, D.,
&
Minkoff,
variable analysis of
memory
short term
and general
N.,
8.
331-335.
Bunting,
S. (2002).
M.
A
and Wiksell. F.,
latent
referenced cognitive
tional Testing Service.
fluid intelligence. Intelligence, 30,
A.,
Kane, M.
J.,
&
Engle, R. W.
Working memory capacity and
its
rela-
tion to general intelligence. Trends in Cognitive 7.
D'Esposito. M.. Ballard, D.. Zarahn. E..
&
Aguirre.
role of prefrontal cortex in sen-
memory and motor preparation: An eventrelated fMRI study. Neurolmage, II, 400-408. sory
D'Esposito, M., Detre, Atlas, S.,
&
J.
A., Alsop, D.
C.
Shin. R. K..
Grossman. M. (1995). The neural
basis of the central executive system of working
memory. Nature, 378, 279-281.
Daneman, M.,
&
Carpenter,
differences in working
P.
S.
450-466.
E.
The
(1995).
An
W.
M.
H.,
&
of factor-
Princeton, NJ: Educa-
of working
role
capacity and general control processes
169-189.
in intelligence. Intelligence, 20,
Engle. R.
Working memory and
(1996).
approach.
inhibition-resource
memory and
reading.
&
E. R. Stoltzfus,
R.
T
memory and human
New York: Engle, R.
W.
ity? In
In
Zacks
Oxford University
(2001).
What
(Eds.).
retrieval: J.
T.
E.
is
H. L. Roediger.
Working
(pp. 89-119).
cognition
Press.
working memory capacJ.
A. M. Suprenant (Eds.).
S.
Naime.
I.
Neath.
&
The nature of remem-
bering: Essays in honor of Robert G. (pp.
A. (1980). Individual
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19,
Embretson,
tests.
kit
Richardson, R. W. Engle, L. Hasher, R. H. Logie,
547-552.
G. K. (2000). The
W., Harman,
J.
working memory capacity,
memory
Conway, A. R.
Sciences,
B.. French,
Dermen, D. (1976). Manual for
capacity, processing speed,
163-183.
(2003).
Ekstrom. R.
Crowder
297-314). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association Press. Engle. R.
W.
(2002).
executive
Working memory capacity
attention.
Current
Directions
Psychological Science, 11. 19-23.
as in
Neuroimaging of Reasoning and Intelligence Kane. M.
Executive
(2004).
J.
working memory capacity, and
attention,
Ross
The psychology of learning and motiva-
New York: Academic
tion (pp. 145-199).
M.
Engle. R. W.. Kane.
&
J..
Individual differences in working ity
and w hat the\
W.
Tuholski. S.
Press.
(1999).
memory
capac-
us about controlled attention,
tell
general fluid intelligence and functions of the
&R
Miyake
prefrontal cortex. In A.
Shah
and
(Eds.),
NeuroReport.
reasoning.
inductive
Goel.
Gold. B.. Kapur,
v..
&
S..
Houle,
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
ing.
P
Goldman-Rakic.
(1987). Circuitry of primate
S.
prefrontal cortex and regulation of behavior
memory.
representational
In
F.
Physiological Society.
University Press. E..
&
A
memory and general fluid
intelligence:
Journal of Experi-
latent variable approach.
mental Psychology: General, 128. 309-331.
P
Eslinger.
J..
&
disturbance of higher cognition after bilateral frontal lobe ablation: Patient
Neurology,
&
M..
S..
Van Horn.
Berman, K.
J.
D.. Ellmore.
Context-
(1999).
F.
dependent, neural system-specific neurophysio-
Mapping PET
logical concomitants of ageing:
correlates
during cognitive
activation.
Brain,
122, 963-979.
D.
&
London: Smith
&
vidual Dijferences,
Goel,
M.
J.
New
and Indi-
3,
The
(1980).
prefrontal
cortex.
C.
Frith.
gistic reasoning. v.,
C.
&
Dolan. R.
Neurolmage.
J.
(2000).
12.
504—514.
in
an inductive infer-
ence task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,
Goel.
&
Dolan. R. (2001). Functional neuro-
anatomy of three-term
relational
reasoning.
tion
&
Dolan. R.
J.
of reasoning
v..
S.
M.. Skolnick. B.
L., et al. (1994). Lateral-
spatial tasks: Rela-
and Cog-
tionship with performance level. Brain
244-258.
nition, 24,
Haier, R. E.,
J.,
Siegel, B.
Wu.
C.
J.
V, Nuechterlein. K.
Pack.
J.,
et al.
H.. Hazlett.
(1988). Conical glu-
cose metabolic rate correlates of abstract reason-
Siegel.
J.,
Buchsbaum, M.
199-217.
12.
V. Tang. C. Abel.
B. S.
&
and
glucose metabolic
in regional cerebral
Hebb, D. O. (1945). Man's
L..
Intelligence
(1992).
415^26.
rate following learning. Intelligence. 16.
frontal lobes:
and
A
critical
Psychiatry,
54, 10-24.
& Penfield, W.
after extensive bilateral
(1940).
Human
behavior
removal from the frontal
and Psychiatry.
lobes. Archives of Neurology
Hitch, G.
Towse,
J.,
J.
limits children's retical
44.
N.,
& Hutton. U.
(2001).
What
working memory span? Theo-
accounts and applications for scholastic
ogy: General. 130. 184-198. Institute for Personality
Champaign,
Ability Testing.
belief.
Cognition,
87.
Jacobsen. C.
&
Houle.
An imagine
S.
( 1
997).
The
studv of deductive
F.
and Ability Testing. (1973).
intelligence with Culture Fair tests.
by
Gold. B.. Kapur. S.
seats of reason?
tomography. Intelligence,
(2003). Explaining modula-
B11-B22. Goel.
Muenz.
ized increases in cerebral blood flow during
Measuring
Neuropsychologia, 39. 901-909. Goel. v..
J..
development. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
110-119.
v..
316-322.
421-438.
& Dolan, R. (2000). Anatomical segregation
of component processes
12.
E., Jaggi,
Hebb, D. O.,
Dissociation of mechanisms underlying syllo-
Goel,
6.
Ragland. D.. Resnick.
review. Archives of Neurology
AQ1-A21.
York: Raven.
Buchel.
v.,
C.
changes
Putnam.
Stankov. L. (1982). Competing tasks
as an index of intelligence. Personality
Fuster.
Gur. R.
Haier. R.
Elder.
The functions of the brain (2nd
New York:
ed.).
Fogarty. G..
Braver. T. S. (2003).
ing and attention studied with positron emission
The functions of the brain.
(1876).
Ferrier, D. (1886).
&
F.
R.. Chabris. C.
J.
Neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence.
performance of verbal and
Esposito, G.. Kirkby. B. T.
EVR.
1731-1741.
35.
to 17.
187-208. Gray.
Nature Neuroscience,
Damasio. A. R. (1985). Severe
MD: American
damage. Journal of Psychology.
frontal lobe
Conway. A. R. A. (1999). Working memory, short-term
by
(Ed.),
The mental changes due
Goldstein. K. (1944). J.
Plum
Handbook of physiology: The nervous system (Vol. 5. pp. 373-417). Bethesda.
New York: Cambridge
10,
293-302.
maintenance and executive control
Engle. R. W.. Tuholski. S. W.. Laughlin.
S. (1998).
Neuroanatomical correlates of human reason-
Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active (pp. 102-134).
8,
1305-1310.
a two-
factor theory of cognitive control. In B. (Ed.).
PFerrier.
L '
&
Engle, R. W..
161
IL:
Institute
for
Personality and
(1935). Functions of the frontal asso-
ciation area in primates. Archives of
and
P.sychialiy. 33.
558-569.
Neurology
162
.
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE
Jacobsen. C.
F.
1936). Studies of cerebral function in
(
primates:
I.
tion area in
The functions of the
monkeys. Comparative Psychology-
Monographs, Kane. M.
K., Conway, A. R. A.. & A controlled-attention view
M.
W. (2001).
Marshalek. B.. Lohman. D. P.
models of
archical
mental activation Stroke, 11.
&
mem-
Engle, R. W. (2000). Working
ory capacity, proactive interference, and divided
on long-term memor>
attention: Limits
retrieval.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Memory, and Cognition, Kane, M.
&
J.,
Engle, R.
cortex
frontal
W.
26.
333-358.
(2002).
The
role of pre-
capacity,
May, C. P, Hasher,
McFie,
J.,
Kane, M.
J.,
&
Engle. R.
631-61
Revie^^•, 9,
W. (2003). Working-memor\
The
capacity and the control of attention:
L.,
& Kane, M. J. (1999). The role
contri-
Memory and
lesions. Brain, 75,
292-311.
Mcintosh, A.
Grady. C.
R..
L.
B.
6,
Cohen,
J.
An
D. (2001).
integrative
and task
Review of Neuroscience. 24, 167-202.
Stroop interference. Journal of
Kroger.
K.. Sabb,
J.
F
Cohen, M.
S. Y.,
W.. Pales, C. L., Bookheimer, S.,
&
Holyoak, K.
J.
(2002).
Recruitment of anterior dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in
human
reasoning:
A
parametric study
of relational complexity. Cerebral Cortex. 12,
477-485. Kyllonen,
R
Miller,
E.
&
K..
Desimone, R. (1994).
information processing:
A
test
of the four-
Science, 263, 520-522. Miller,
E.
K.,
memory
&
Erickson. C. A..
mechanisms of
(1996). Neural
16,
& Fazio, F.
S..
in
Neuropsychologia, 36, 369-376.
& Christal,
R C. is
(little
more
R. E. (1990). Reasoning than)
working-memory
capacity?! Intelligence. 14. 389—433.
Larson, G. E., Haier, R.
J.,
LaCasse,
L.,
K.
(1995). Evaluation of a "mental effort" hypothesis for correlations
between
cortical
metabolism
New York:
Luria. A. R.
New
(
1966).
Oxford University
in
man.
Perret. E. (1974).
J.
The
11,
(2001).
left
New
brain systems
reasoning.
probabilistic
954-965.
left frontal
lobe of
man and
the
suppression of habitual responses in verbal cate-
Pierrot-Deseilligny.
D.. Stenger, V. A.,
&
1991
(
C. ).
Rivaud.
V.,
Smith.
Glover. G. H.,
S..
Gaymard.
B..
&
Cortical control of reflexive visu-
ally-guided saccades.
Prabhakaran,
York: Basic Books.
MacDonald, A. W.. lU. Cohen.
Osherson, D.
versus
deductive
Cerebral Cortex,
Agid. Y.
Press.
Higher cortical functions
&
M.,
L.
evidence for distinct right and
gorical behavior. Neuropsychologia, 12, 323-330.
and intelligence. Intelligence, 21, 267-278. Lezak, M. D. (1983). Neuropsychological assessment.
deductive versus probabalistic reasoning.
Parsons,
for
& Hazen.
Schnur. T.
(1998). Distinct brain loci
120. 375-105.
ability
working
5154-5167.
Osherson. D.. Perani. D.. Cappa. Grassi, F,
Desimone. R. visual
of the macaque.
in prefrontal cortex
sources model. Journal of General Psychology,
Kyllonen,
Parallel
neuronal mechanisms for short-term memory.
Journal of Neuroscience.
C. (1993). Aptitude testing inspired by
faces.
of prefrontal cortex function. Annual
theory
set to
V..
571-584.
butions of goal neglect, response competition,
Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 47-70.
J.
(1996).
working memor\' task for
Cerebral Cortex.
&
Haxby,
L..
Horwitz,
and prefrontal functional
limbic
in
Miller, E. K.,
&
G..
interactions in a
1
J.
& Piercy, M. F. (1953). Intellectual impair-
Changes
individual-differences perspective. Psycho-
Risberg,
Cognition, 27. 759-767.
An
and
7,
normo- and hypercapnia.
in
of interference in memor>' span.
Ungerieider,
Bulletin
hier-
342-347.
executive attention, and general fluid intelligence:
nomic
&
I.,
ment with localized cerebral
working-memory
in
and
intelligence. Intelligence,
Cerebral hemodynamic response to
(1980).
imental Psychology: General, 130, 169-183. J..
Snow. R. E. (1983).
in the radix
Maximilian, V. A., Prohovnik,
of working-memory capacity. Journal of Exper-
Kane, M.
&
The complexity continuum 107-127.
13. 1-68.
Bleckley,
J..
Engle. R.
frontal associa-
&
114. 1473-1485.
fira/>j, J.
A.
L..
Gabrieli.
Desmond, J.
J.
E..
D. E. (1997).
An fMRI
Carter, C. S. (2000). Dissociating the role of the
Neural substrates of fluid reasoning:
dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cor-
study of neocortical activation during f)erfor-
tex in cognitive control. Science, 288, 1835-1838.
mance of
Malmo.
R. B. (1942). Intert'erence factors in delayed
response
in
monkeys
after
removal of frontal
lobes. Journal of Neurophysiology, 5, 295-308.
the Raven's Progressive Matrices test.
Journal of Cognitive Psychology. 33. 43-63. Rainer.
G.
Asaad. W. F.
&
Miller. E. K. (1998).
Selective representation of relevant information
L
Neuroimaging of Reasoning and Intelligence by neurons
in
primate prefrontal cortex.
the
Nature. 393, 577-579.
Raven,
J.
C.
Raven,
J.
H. (1998). Pro-
J.
UK: Oxford
gressive matrices. Oxford,
Psychol-
Risberg,
&
J.,
&
Ingvar, D. H. (1973). Patterns of acti-
Schulze, R.
Working-memory
(2002).
capacity explains reasoning ability bit
—and
a
little
more. Intelligence. 30, 261-288.
Sweeney,
ogists Press.
163
SUB, H.-M., Oberauer, K., Wittmann, W. W., Wilhelm, O.,
& Court.
E.,
•
M.
M.
A., Mintun,
J.
Brown, D.
B.,
Kwee.
A.,
Wiseman.
S.,
Rosenberg, D. R.,
L.,
et al.
vation in the grey matter of the dominant hemi-
(1996). Positron emission tomography study of
sphere during memorizing and reasoning. Brain.
voluntary saccadic eye
Risberg,
J.,
Maximilian. A.
Changes of
cortical
V..
& Prohovnik,
I.
patterns
activity
habituation to a reasoning
test.
(1977).
during
Neuropsycholo-
793-798.
gia, 15.
C.
Roberts. R. D.. Beh. H.
&
Stankov. L. (1988).
Hick's law, competing task performance and inlelVigence. Intelligence, 12.
Ruff, C.
C.
111-131.
Knauff. M.. Fangmeier.
(2003). Reasoning and working
mon and
T..
&
&
J.
memory: Com-
distinct neuronal processes.
Neuropsy-
Miyake. A. (1996). The separability of
and language processing: approach.
An
Journal
spatial thinking
individual differ-
of Experimental
Psychology: General, 125, 4—27. Shallice, T..
strategy
damage
&
Burgess,
application in
M.
L..
&
P W.
frontal
lobe
man. Brain, 114, 121-1 A\.
(1989). Is working
Waltz,
J.
A..
Knowlton, B.
K. B., Mishkin,
in
(1999).
human
F.
A
J.,
S.,
Holyoak, K.
J.,
Boone,
de Menezes Santos, M.,
system for relational reasoning
prefrontal
Psychological
cortex.
Science, 10. 119-125.
Weinstein,
S..
&
Teuber, H.-L. (1957). Effects of brain
injury
on intelligence
test
scores. Science, 125, 1036-1037.
Wharton, C. M., Grafrnan, K.. Brauner,
J.,
J.,
Flitman, S.
Marks. A.,
S..
Hansen, E.
et al. (2000).
neuroanatomical models of analogy:
A
Toward positron
dimensionality of a task. Intelligence. 16.
Cognitive Psychology, 40, 173-197.
Wittenbom,
J.
R. (1943). Factorial equations for tests
of attention. Psychometrika,
Spilsbury. G. (1992). Complexity as a reflection of the
31^5.
Stankov, L. (1983). Attention and intelligence. Jour-
nal of Educational Psychology, 75,
W.
emission tomography study of analogical mapping.
(1991). Deficits in
following
Engle. R.
memory capacity task dependent? Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 127-154.
penetrating
working memory resources for
ences
spatial
75, 454--*68.
Turner.
et al.
Spreer.
chologia. 41. 1241-1253.
Shah. P.
movements and
working memory. Journal of Neurophysiology,
737-756.
96,
471^90.
Zysset,
S., Miiller, K.,
Lohman,
8,
G.,
19-35.
&
von Cramon,
D. Y. (2001). Color- word matching Stroop
task:
Separating interference and response conflict.
Neurolmage,
13.
29-36.
10 Behavioral Genetics
AND Intelligence Stephen A. Petrill
For
decades, theories of intelligence and
cognitive development have
swung wildly
DZ)
(dizygotic,
on average.
share
50%
If identical
of the same genes,
twins are more similar
nativism, followed
than fraternal twins on a particular outcome,
by eras of strict environmentalism. Over the past
then genetic influences are assumed. Similarly,
30 years, a more balanced view has emerged
if
recognizing that both nature and nurture are
logical parents
between eras of
important.
strict
genetic
Quantitative
have
studies
suggested that genes account for a substantial
of the variance
portion
throughout the that the
life
in
cognitive
ability
span but have also suggested
environment
is
also essential to under-
adopted children are more similar to their bio-
and siblings than
also implicated. Heritability (h-) measures the
extent to which individual differences on an out-
come measure
are influenced by genetic differ-
ences in the sample. Shared environment
when
among
The purpose of this chapter is to examine the ways in which behavioral genetics methods may be used to better
on a particular outcome
understand intelligence and
found among family members
standing
intelligence.
its
development. In
implied
to
DZ
the correlation
twins or
when
is
who
the proportion of variance in an
and
gene-environment processes on intelligence.
The chapter explores behavioral
genetic
the implications of the
literature
with
respect
to
molecular genetics, theories of intelligence, and
enced by environments held or attending the
same
nonshared environment genetic
The the cal
factors
that
of the
same genes, whereas
MZ)
influ-
in the
same
school. Finally, the
(e-)
measures non-
make family members
heritability
of intelligence
most consistent tlndings literature,
one
that
in the
has
is
one of
psychologi-
been replicated
across twin/adoption designs, across countries,
and across ages (Bouchard Identical twins (monozygotic,
outcome
common among
different, including error.
the assessment of intelligence.
Univariate Genetic Analyses
in
family members, such as growing up
home
is
are biologi-
environment measures
cally unrelated. Shared
genetic analyses of cogni-
twins
a significant correlation
particular, the chapter addresses the following
tive ability, longitudinal genetic analyses,
(c") is
MZ
similar in magnitude
topics: univariate genetic analyses of cognitive ability, multivariate
their adoptive
parents and siblings, then genetic influences are
share
fraternal
100%
Chipuer, Rovine,
twins
across
all
&
&
McGue,
1981;
Plomin, 1990). Collapsing
available studies, the heritability of
165
li
166
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE
.
intelligence
around
is
intelligence but also for
(Plomin,
abilities
.50,
both for general
most
specific cognitive
DeFries,
intelligence
is
&
McClearn,
McGuffin, 2001). However, the
heritability
not stable at .50 across the
^
of life
span. Genetic influences increase in importance
with the age of the sample
when examining cog-
Verbal Ability
development. In general, the heritability
nitive
Twin
Spatial Ability in
in
Twin 2
1
20% in 80% in old
of intelligence increases, from around infancy to
60%
in early
age (Boomsma, Bernieri,
adulthood to
1993; McCartney, Harris,
McGue, Bouchard,
1990;
lacono,
& &
Cross-Twin Correlation
Figure 10.1
Lykken, 1993; Plomin, 1986; Plomin, Fulker, Corley,
&
DeFries, 1997). The heritability of
intelligence remains substantial in old-old
ples
(McClearn
sam-
et al., 1997). In contrast, these
same twin and adoption
studies suggest that
shared environmental influences are important
two
this case,
cence.
Nonshared environmental influences
skills) is
differences in intelligence
become
is
the cross-twin correlation (see Figure 10.1). In
childhood but reduce to zero by adoles-
individual
and envi-
into genetic
dence. The essence of multivariate genetics
in early
(including error) remain significant. In general,
outcomes
different
ronmental sources of covariance and indepen-
Tw in
1
score on Variable
's
compared
to
Twin
2's score
1
(verbal
on Vari-
able 2 (spatial skills). If identical twins are
more
similar than fraternal twins, then genetic
increasingly influenced by genes as the age of
influences are hypothesized to influence the
the sample increases. This increase in heritabil-
correlation between verbal and spatial skills. If
also found in specific cognitive abilities
identical twins are equally similar to fraternal
ity is
such as verbal,
spatial, perceptual
memory. This increase
in heritability is
indirect evidence for the
fially
active
and/or
speed, and
evocative
poten-
emergence of
gene-environment
processes. This possibility
will
be discussed
are implied. Figure 10.2 demonstrates a bivariate genetic
example of such an approach. In between verbal skills
this case, the correlation
and
spatial
genetic
in
later in the chapter.
shared environmental influences
then
twins,
skills
(A),
is
influenced
shared (C),
by overlap
and nonshared
The independence of measured by residual shared environmental (c), and non-
(E) sources of variance.
Multivariate Genetic Analyses
spatial
skills
genetic
(a),
is
also
shared environmental
The
described
analyses
univariate
above
To
(e)
sources of variance.
the extent that the cross-twin covariance in
identical twins
is
greater than fraternal twins,
long-debated question of whether genes or
A will be
To
the extent that the cross-twin
environment
covariance
are
important
because
are
they
important
differences in intelligence.
address
to
the
individual
The answer
to this
twins.
C
large. is
similar in identical and fraternal
will be large.
To
the extent that verbal
correlate within individual
question appears to be that both genes and
and
environments are important for individual
twins but not across twins,
dif-
spatial
skills
E
will be large.
To
ferences in intelligence. Muldvariate genetics
the extent that spatial skills are independent
research has taken the next step to examine
from verbal skills, a, c, and e will be large. Although more sophisticated models have been postulated, they are based on this general approach. These models have been employed
how genes and environments affect opment of cognitive skills. Where genetic analyses
decompose
the devel-
univariate
the variance of a
outcome into genetic and environmental components of variance, multivariate genetic methods decompose the correlation between single
to
examine several theoretically
central issues
to the understanding of intelligence.
be discussed
in turn.
These
will
I
Behavioral Genetics and Intelligence
1
•
1
67
.OMZ, 0.5DZ
1.0MZ, 0.5DZ
Figure 10.2
Bivariate Genetic
Model
Molarity Versus Modularity
(as
measured by Bayley scores) are correlated
with their biological parents' general cognitive
A
longstanding and continuing debate
intelligence literature
is
best conceived as a molar or (see Chapters 14
and
in the
whether intelligence
16, this
is
modular construct volume).
A
molar
ability but not their parents' specific cognitive
These
abilities.
results suggest that genetic influ-
ences are mediated by a general
factor.
Reznick,
Corley, and Robinson (1997) found that the
among
verbal and nonverbal
system implies that a singular unitary process
intercorrelations
or processes function across a wide variety of
tests
cognitive tasks. Conversely, a modular system
environmental factors. Similarly, Price, Eley,
involves independent and uncorrelated cognitive
processing
units.
If
different
dimensions of
cognitive ability, such as verbal ability, spatial ability,
speed of processing, and memory, are
were driven by both genetic and shared
Stevenson, and Plomin (2000) found substantial
shared
and genetic
environmental
overlap
between language and non-language-based cognitive ability. Petrill, Saudino, Wilkerson,
and
influenced by the same set of genes and envi-
Plomin (2001) conducted a multivariate genetic
ronments, then a molar explanation best
analysis on the individual cognitive tests that compose nonverbal intelligence using the same sample as Price et al. The results of this study
data. If different
fits
the
dimensions of cognitive pro-
cessing are each associated with independent sets
of genes and environments, then a modular
explanation best
fits
Multivariate genetic studies suggest that the
covariance
among
different cognitive skills
is
influenced largely by shared genes, whereas the
discrepancy between cognitive
suggest
that,
similar to Price et
shared environment tend to
the data.
skills
is
ties similar,
al.,
genes and
cognitive abili-
whereas the nonshared environment
(and error) contributes to the discrepancy cognitive
among
skills.
Many more
influ-
enced largely by the nonshared environment. Put
make
studies
examine the issue of
molarity/modularity in early and middle child-
another way. the influence of genes on cognitive
hood. Rice, Fulker. and DeFries (1986) con-
molar, but the influence of non-
ducted a multivariate genetic analysis of verbal,
skills is largely
shared environments
is
ExamThompson,
largely modular.
ining infancy and early childhood,
spatial, perceptual speed, in
4-year-old adoptees.
and memory
abilities
The data suggest a
strong
Plomin, and DeFries (1985) suggest that cogni-
general genetic intelligence factor and a weaker
and 24-month adoptive children
shared environmental factor. Looking at these
tive skills in 12-
168
.
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE
same adoptees
in
middle childhood, molarity
becomes even more pronounced Alarcon. Plomin. (
&
& DeFries.
influenced
and almost
Adult Development (Finkel. Pedersen. McGue.
of the covariance
among
specific cognitive
lar
&
McCleam.
1995). Finally. Petrill et
al.
(1998)
found even more pronounced general genetic
abilities.
Twin
was
abilities
results were obtained when comparing Swedish data to the Minnesota Twin Study of
1998; Cardon, Fulker,
for a large portion of the variance all
cognitive
Plomin, 1992). Genetic g accounts
Fulker, Corley,
DeFries,
among
primarily by the nonshared environment. Simi-
studies in early and middle childhood
Thompson
yield similar results. Luo. Petrill. and
(1994) examined the importance of genetic g in a sample of 6- to 13-year-old twins. In this study, the covariance
among
Wechsler
1 1
(WISC-R)
ligence Scale for Children-Revised subtests
Intel-
effects
when examining nondemented
80-i-year-
old twins recruited as part of the Octotwin
Study (McCleam
et al., 1997).
Genetic molarity
accounted for a large proportion of the variance, as well as almost in specific
all
total
of the covariance
cognitive abilities.
mazes) was examined.
(excluding
Results suggested that the cross-twin covariance
among
was
these subtests
substantial. In fact,
Longitudinal Genetic Studies
genetic effects could largely be explained by
a single genetic factor across
all
1
subtests.
1
Shared environmental influences contribute
to
In addition to
examining the relationship among
cognitive skills at a given time point, behavioral
molarity, and nonshared environment contributes
genetic research has also examined the influ-
and Fulker 1995)
ence of genes and environments on the develop-
to modularity. Casto. DeFries.
obtained similar results
(
when examining
ple of 7- to 15-year-old twins
Colorado Reading
the
childhood suggest that molarity
middle
may be found
standardized measures of scholastic perfor-
mance
as well.
These studies suggest
that the
correlation between reading performance and
general cognitive ability
is
influenced solely
by shared genetic factors (Brooks. Fulker. DeFries. 1990; Thompson. Dettennan.
1991; Wadsworth,
obtained
1994).
when examining
the
&
Plomin.
comorbidity disability.
than 507c of the observed comorbidity
between reading and mathematical
may
&
Similar effects are
between reading and mathematical
More
Chapter
12.
approach has already been described
One
chapter the
—examining
heritability
across the
life
in
this
age-related differences in
of general cognitive ability span. Although this approach
examines whether the magnitude of genetic and environmental effects varies with age,
this
approach does not provide any data on whether
same genetic and environmental variance is life span or whether new
the
operating across the
sources of genetic and environmental variance
"tum on" or "tum
off" at different ages. This
general issue has been analyzed in two ways. First,
research has examined stability
instability of intelligence
— whether
and
the genetic,
&
shared environmental, and nonshared environ-
Genetic molarity appears to become even
genetic, shared environmental, and nonshared
(Knopik, Alarcon.
DeFries, 1997; Light
mental influences
DeFries, 1995).
more pervasive in adulthood and old age. Tambs, Sundet, and Magnus (1984) suggest that
back
when
is
between the same
(WAIS) subtests old Norwegian
McCleam
in a
sample of 30-
Scale
to 57-year-
Pedersen. Plomin. and
twins.
(1994) found that most but not
all
of
the genetic variance in specific cognitive abilities
can be attributed to effects
were
zero,
g.
Shared environmen-
whereas the discrepancy
one age are related at
later ages.
to the
Going
to cross-twin correlations, genetic stability
among Wechsler Adult
Intelligence
at
environmental influences
genes account almost entirely for the similarity
tal
skills (also see
volume, for a detailed discussion of ways
this
disabilities
be attributable to shared genetic influences
&
ment of cognitive
of examining cognitive development).
Project.
In addition, behavioral genetic data in
in
a sam-
drawn from
inferred
the test
remain stable Genetic
when
cross-twin correlations
measured across time instability
is
inferred
genetic influences can be found at a single
points in time but cross-twin correlations are low. In other words, there are genetic influences at a
given time, but they do not persist across
time. Studies have suggested that the genetic
Behavioral Genetics and Intelligence
among
covariance
cognitive skills across age
becomes increasingly important. Although older research alluded to this issue (e.g.. Cherny et al., 1994; Fulker, Cherny, & Cardon, 1993). more recent work has examined this issue in longitwin (Bartels, Reitveld. van Baal.
tudinal
&
Boomsma, 2002) and twin/adoption (Bishop 2003:
et al..
Petrill et al.. in press) studies
suring cognitive
mea-
from early childhood
skills
•
1
69
Endophenotypes and General Intelligence The
genetic
quantitative
described
results
above are important because they more comprehensively specify the phenotypic relationships
among
and longitudinally mea-
concurrently
sured cognitive
However, these data, by
skills.
how genes
themselves, do not explain
To address
influ-
through adolescence. In general, these studies
ence cognitive
suggest that the cross-twin correlations between
researchers have taken reductionistic approaches
the
same
tests
measured over time are largely
stable. In other
among
to
ability.
examine the pathways from genes
words, the genetic covariance
and consistent
between endophenotypes or more basic cogni-
cognitive skills
is
stable
processes thought to mediate the relation-
tive
Shared environmental covariance across age
ship between genes
important
also
One
to g.
approach has been to examine the relationship
though much of childhood and adolescence.
is
this question,
childhood but
early
in
is
Chapter
skills (see
3,
and complex cognitive this volume, for a more
detailed discussion). Researchers have
ultimately nonsignificant by adolescence.
exam-
approach
ined the genetic and environmental covariance
examines the magnitude of the covariance of
between "elementary" processes, such as reac-
Whereas
the
stability/instability
and decision
genetic and environmental influences at differ-
tion time, stimulus discrimination,
examined change as a phenotype. This approach examines the genetic,
time, and general cognitive ability. Although
shared environmental, and nonshared environ-
genetic influences
mental influences on the rate and trajectory of
tant for the overlap
change
and psychometric
ent ages, others have
in cognitive abilities across age. In his
"developmental
classic
synchronies" paper,
Wilson (1983) suggested that the trajectory of developmental milestones (physical, cognitive, etc.) in identical
twins
is
more
similar than in
fraternal twins, suggesting genetic influences
growth. ies
More
have used
on
recently, behavioral genetic studlatent
growth curve and multilevel
modeling strategies
to
test
more
explicitly
genetic and environmental influences on change (e.g..
McArdle. 1986: McArdle. Prescott. Ham-
agami,
&
Horn. 1998:
2002; Neale Finkel. Gatz.
McGue &
Christensen,
& McArdle. 2000; Reynolds, & Pedersen. 2002). These studies
there are fewer studies, like psychometric tests,
become
increasingly impor-
between elementary tasks
tests
of intelligence.
ined
the
between
relationship
mentary" cognitive stimulus
ability (e.g., reaction time,
sample
of
found
that
between psychometric and
ele-
discrimination)
in
the covariance
studies
of adulthood,
shared environmental
influences are zero, but genes remain important to
understanding the relationship between ele-
mentary cognitive tasks and psychometric
& Ho.
Somsen, 1991: Ho. Baker.
McGue. Bouchard. Lykken.
ies
important to note that these stud-
have typically examined very young children
(e.g.,
McArdle.
McGue & 2002).
1986) or older adults
Christen.sen, 2002:
Reynolds
(e.g..
et al.,
The magnitude of genetic influences on
change has not been systematically examined intermediate ages. Thus,
it
is
in
unclear whether
common
genetic and shared environmental factors. In
nongenetic influences are implicated it is
al.
mentary measures was influenced by
primarily responsible for the intercept, whereas
However,
a
6- to 13-year-old twins. Petrill et
ligence (Baker. Vernon.
change.
Wechsler
1 1
subtests (excluding mazes) and 6 tests of "ele-
have suggested that genetic influences are
in
Petrill,
Luo, Thompson, and Detterman (1996) exam-
1991:
intel-
Boomsma &
& Decker. & Feuer.
1988; 1984:
& & Boomsma,
Neubauer, Spinath. Riemann, Angleitner, Borkenau, 2000; Rijsdijk, Vernon,
1998: Vernon. 1989). Other studies have begun
examine
activity,
the genetic and environmental between brain structure, brain and intelligence (see Boomsma.
Anokhin,
&
to
covariance
de Geus, 1997; Pennington
et al.,
Boomsma, & de Geus, de Geus, & Boomsma, 1998:
genetics are important to change in later child-
2000; Posthuma, Neale,
hood, adolescence, and early adulthood.
2001; van Baal,
f.ji
170
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE
.
van
this
Molenaar,
Beijsterveldt,
Boomsma,
&
Geus,
de
1998a, 1998b; see also Chapter
9,
volume).
In general, these studies have suggested sub-
on elementary cog-
stantial genetic influences
and brain
nitive tasks, brain structure,
activity.
However, the conclusions one can derive from analyses of these endophenotypes are unclear.
some measures of "elementary" show moderate correlations
In particular,
cognitive
ability
with general cognitive ability (Neubauer
2000;
1996). However,
Petrill et al.,
known how "elementary" in
comparison
and
structure
solving, but
et al., is
not
these tasks really are
psychometric measures of
to
measures of brain
Conversely,
intelligence.
it
show very
these studies
phenotypic correlations between these
small
10.3
Genes,
Environments,
and Their
Influence on Cognitive Skills
do not require problem
activity
many of
Figure
measures and general cognitive
ability
(e.g.,
two more independent Chomey, Lubinski, Thompson, &
since failed to replicate in
samples
(Hill,
Plomin, 2002).
& Eysenck, 1992; Haier, Siegel, Tang, Abel, & Buchsbaum, 1992; Matarazzo, 1992; McGarry-Roberts, Stelmack, & Campbell, 1992; Posthuma eta!., 2001; Reed & Jensen, 1992; Rijsdijk & Boomsma, 1997; Vernon &
ated with g
Mori, 1992).
2003, for a discussion). Although
Barrett
What
is
emerging
from
this
may number
important
genes associ-
literature is a realization that the
in the several
hundreds
and may have very small effect sizes
below
1%
(at
or
of the variance per gene) (see Plomin, this is sober-
ing from a reductionistic, mechanistic perspective, the possibility
Molecular Genetics and General Intelligence addition
In
studying endophenotypes,
to
others have attempted to identify genes associ-
ated with cognitive replicated
findings
There are two well-
skills.
—an
association
dementia and the apolipoprotein Farrer et
al.,
between
E gene
et al.,
2002).
DNA
with normal variation ability are also
D
in
(e.g.,
markers associated general
cognitive
beginning to emerge: cathepsin
(CTSD; Jacobs
et al.,
2002),
cholinergic
explain from a mechanistic perspective. Intelli-
gence appears
be "general" because of
to
genetic variance, especially after adolescence.
Figure
10.3
depicts the relationship between
two cognitive measures. There of overlap
among
is
a high degree
individual sources of genetic
variance but a low degree of overlap
among
individual sources of environmental variance.
Molecular genetics research suggests variance
genetic
is
that this
composed of numerous
genes each with a very small effect
size.
Thus,
2002), and a functional polymorphism
cognitive processing are likely to be influenced
receptor
Malhotra
et al.,
potential findings, that
explain
any two behaviorally measurable components of
2
(VAL158MET) in the cathechol 0-methyltransferase (COMT) enzyme system. (Egan et al., 2001;
may
(CHRM2; Comings
muscarinic et al.,
(e.g.,
1997) and linkage between markers
located near 6p21 and reading disability
Kaplan
of hundreds of small-effect
why g is so easy to find from a psychometric perspective but so hard to size genes
it
is
2002).
Despite these
important to remember
replication has been elusive for general
by a
common
set
of genes, leading to a genetic
correlation. This multicomponential
model of
general intelligence has been postulated previ-
ously
at
cognitive
the level of independent phenotypic
processes
(e.g.,
Detterman,
1987).
study found evidence for an association between
However, the independent components of general intelligence may be even more basic, at the
IGF2R and
level of
cognitive ability. For example, one longstanding
general cognitive ability in two
separate samples
(Chomey
et al.,
1998) but has
hundreds of individual genes interacting
with one another
in probabilistic
and pleitropic
k
— Behavioral Genetics and Intelligence
Experience
An
we may
Etiology
Environment as Experience Versus Environment as Etiology
Figure 10.4
ways.
optimist
may
reasonably argue that
eventually understand the mechanisms
by which genes yield
g.
A
may argue we may never be
pessimist
with equal reasonableness that
complex and
able to characterize the infinitely
pathways through which individual
varied
171
•
differences
in
genes
individual
influence
differences in intelligence.
environments (see Plomin, Asbury,
&
Dunn,
2001). This approach has led to mixed results
environment (Reiss, Neiderhiser,
family
for
Hetherington,
&
Plomin, 2000) and
much
con-
troversy about the efficacy of examining the
nonshared environment as a useful predictor of developmental outcomes
2001 Turkheimer
et al.,
;
Plomin, Asbury,
(e.g.,
& Waldron,
2000).
One
of the major obstacles using current methods that the
error).
important questions concerning the
mechanisms of how genes influence
intelli-
gence, quantitative genetic results have shown that
genes are increasingly important, not only
for individual
is
is
defined as
anything that makes twins different (including
Implications Despite
nonshared environment
measures of cognitive
also for the links
between cognitive
ability but
At the
skills.
Thus,
it is
unclear
ences between cognitive
skills
currently or longitudinally
is
environment or measurement less,
understanding
the differ-
measured con-
"true" nonshared error.
Neverthe-
how environmental
ences affect development
in
the
influ-
context
increasingly large and stable genetic factors
behavioral level, these results have important
one of the central issues
implications for developmental theory, particu-
genetic research.
how we
how much
of is
in current behavioral
conceptualize the environments
However, one of the major limitations of the
associated with general cognitive ability. For
current debate on the genetics and the role of
example, because shared environmental
larly in
vari-
the environment has been a misunderstanding of
some have
the distinction between the environment as expe-
argued that early experiences are ultimately
rience and the environment as etiology. Figure
unimportant to the study of individual differ-
10.4 presents
ance
ences
is
negligible by adolescence,
in
development
Harris,
(e.g.,
Others have argued that what
more systematic examination of
is
1998).
necessary
is
a
the child-specific
an environmental experience
shared reading between parent and child. This
experience
is
clearly
environmental from a
phenotypic perspective. However, the individual
172
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE
.
differences in the
shared reading
amount of time and
may
quality of
through distinct etiolo-
arise
To the extent that shared reading is influenced by genes associated with child reading skills, shared reading will show a genetic component. If this is the case, parents will be more likely to spend similar amounts of time with gies.
their identical twins than their fraternal twins. In
addition, shared reading
may
operate through
to
be compatible with their genetically influenced
characteristics.
tions
starting
A Gene x Environment interaction is inferred when
there
a nonlinear association
is
Rowe, Jacobson, and Van den Oord (1999)
found
heritability of verbal
measured environments associated with cog-
cents.
environmental etiology.
have
DNA.
Over
sample of adoles-
of IQ was higher
in
An
SES
experience does not
coming is
low
families.
processes
may
explain the decrease in "shared
environmental" influences
two decades, behavioral
high
in
Developmentally. passive gene-environment
into
likely to
mediated by genotype. past
heritability
in a
socioeconomic status (SES) families than
but the probability of
the
IQ
a genetic as well as an
contact with a particular experience
be partially
The
moderated the
parental education
that
cognitive skills are genetically mediated, the
may have
between
genetic and environmental influences. For example,
nitive skills
come with new
school and puberty,
children and adults.
shared and nonshared environmental influences. that to the extent that
transi-
opportunities to meet and interact with other
The important point
is
The major developmental
childhood and adolescence, such as
in
same home
living in the
more
in childhood.
more
are
Children
likely to expe-
when
genetic studies have highlighted the importance
rience
of gene-environment (G-E) processes. There
younger and different environments when they
are
two general classes of G-E process
—gene-
studies have
ment
heritability.
(1977) described three types of Passive
G-E
correlation
is
G-E
and Loehlin correlation.
found when a child
provided an environment that
is
is
correlated with
the parents" (and. hence, the child's) genes.
An
example of passive G-E correlation is the number of books in the home and child IQ scores (Plomin & Bergeman. 1991). Parents with more education and higher cognitive performance scores provide more books for their children. Their biologically related children are
not only exposed to
more books but
are also
employed
a sibling design to derive
shared environmental estimates, and
nonshared environmental estimates, the sibling design confounds passive shared environment. for
why
G-E
One
correlation and
of the possibilities
shared environment becomes so low
because passive
G-E
correlation
is
childhood but declines as active and evocative
G-E correlation and interaction effects become so much stronger. Second, differential experience
in siblings
has been unnecessarily equated
with the nonshared environment.
experiences
Differential
may also be a funcG-E correlation. For
older children
in
tion of active or evocative
are contributing to higher cognitive test perfor-
example, the heritability of cognitive
mance
increases to
correlation exists
between child cognitive out-
comes and
the
A
G-E correlation is a second found when an environment is pro-
number of books
in the
home.
reactive or ex'ocative
type and
is
is
operating in
provided with some of the parents' genes that scores. Thus, a passive gene-environment
they are
Although many behavioral genetic
are older.
environment correlation and Gene x Environinteraction. Plomin. DeFries.
similar environments
adulthood
.80 in
(McGue
ability et al.,
1993). However, because these estimates are typically derived
from a
variate estimates of effects are
sibling design using uni-
heritability.
subsumed
nonpassive
G-E
in the heritabilitv estimate.
vided to an individual by others as a reaction to his or her genetically influenced behavior.
Active
G-E
correlation occurs
when
a child
Conclusion
seeks environments based on his or her geneti-
influenced characteristics. Scarr and McCartney (1983) proposed that these effects become increasingly salient over the course of development as children gain more control over situations and experiences that are more likely cally
It
is
difficult to seriously dispute that intelli-
gence has both genetic and environmental components. However, there is still much work to be
done
to delineate
what
this
for theories of intelligence
information means
and the assessment
Behavioral Genetics and Intelligence
we can draw
of intelligence. For now. general conclusions. First, strong genetic rently
component
that
and longitudinally. This
difficult to fact. It is
tation of
three
intelligence has a is
instead possible that g
makes
is
important (see Chapter 16. this
volume). Measured specific cognitive abilities
indeed correlated but yet are factorially
are
As described above,
distinguishable.
seem
distinctions
to be
References
these
shaped mainly by the
Alarcon. M.. Plomin, R.. Fulker. D. W.. Corley. R.. DeFries.
of specific cognitive abilities data
age
Genetics. 28(4), 255-263.
Baker. L. A.. Vernon. genetic
&
A..
P.
Ho. H. Z. (1991). The
between intelligence and
correlation
speed of information
Genetics. 2/(4). 351-368.
closer to the
below
the
level,
operate
—
of measured behavior.
level
another way. intelligence
some
may
of individual genes
far
Put
may be modular
at
but at the level of psychometric
12 years of
at
Colorado Adoption Project. Behavior
in the
reductionism required to find truly orthogonal processes intluencing intelligence
&
T.
Barrett. P.
Behavior
processing.
Eysenck. H.
J.
(1992). Brain evoked
and intelligence: The Hendrickson
potentials
paradigm. Intelligence. 76(3^), 361-382.
M.
Bartels. M.. Reitveld.
Boomsma. D.
van Baal. G. C. M..
J. J.,
mental intluences on the development of
molarity because of genes.
gence. Behavior Genetics. 32(A). 237-249.
Second,
this
genetic stability also has an
The
ences.
quantitative genetic data suggest that
genes are important for cognitive that
gene-environment effects are likely
operating
when examining
the
and
skills
to
be
measured en\
ronments associated with cognitive
i-
skills.
Quantifying the connections between gene-
environment process and development will provide a
more complete
that yield individual
picture of the
mechanisms
differences in cognitive
Third,
behavioral
genetics
has
important
implications for the assessment of intelligence. It is
a mistake to assume that just because gene-
important to intelligence that the envi-
ronment does not matter. The average intelligence
1998).
In
level of
has increased over time (Flynn, addition,
some
interventions
have
been shown to improve intellectual functioning
(Campbell 1991).
& Ramey,
However,
differences in
it
1994: is
intelligence
genetic differences. Thus, to
assume
that
Ramey
clear
it
& Campbell.
that
individual
are
influenced by
is
also a mistake
environments and interventions
&
C.
J.
intelli-
Corley. R.. Plomin. R..
S.,
Heuitt.
K. (2003). Develop-
J.
ment genetic analysis of general cognitive ity
from
to
1
1
abil-
2 years in a sample of adoptees,
biological siblings, and twins. Intelligence. 31.
31-19.
Boomsma.
D.
(1993). Current status and future
I.
prospects in twin studies of the development
of cognitive T. J.
Infancy to old age. In
abilities:
Bouchard
Jr.
&
Propping (Eds.). Twins
P.
as a tool of behavioral genetics (pp. 67-82).
UK:
Chichester.
ability.
tics are
Bishop. E. G., Chemy. S. DeFries.
&
(2002). Genetic and environ-
I.
measures, there will likely be some degree of
impact on potential gene-environment influ-
&
C. (1998). Multivariate path analysis
J.
nonshared environment. However, the level of
level
problems
associated with cognitive functioning.
the manifes-
hundreds of genes of very small effect
abilities are not
treat
73
it
statistical arti-
This does not mean that specific cognitive
size.
and
detect, prevent,
1
stable concur-
stability
dismiss g as merely a
on how we
•
Boomsma, D.
Wiley.
Anokhin. A..
I..
&
de Geus. E. (1997).
Genetics of electrophysiology: Linking genes,
and behavior. Current Directions
brain,
in
Psychological Science. 6(4), 106-110.
Boomsma, D.
I..
&
Somsen. R.
measured
tion times
in a
J.
M.
(1991). Reac-
choice reaction time
and a double task condition:
A
small twin study.
Personalin and Individual Differences. 12(6),
519-522. Bouchard.
J.
T.
Jr..
&
McGue. M.
studies of intelligence:
A
(1981). Familial
review. Science. 212.
1055-1059. Brooks. A.. Fulker. D. W..
&
DeFries.
J.
C. (1990).
associated with intelligence occur in a genetic
Reading pert'ormance and general cognitive
vacuum. Instead, understanding the mechanisms through which genetics and environment
ability:
affect intelligence will
have important implications
data.
A
multivariate genetic analysis of twin
Personality
11(2). 141-146.
and Individual
Differences,
174
.
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE
CampbeU, F. A.,
& Ramey, C. T. (1994). Efifects of early
intervention on intellectual and academic achieve-
ment:
A
income
follow-up study of children from low
684-698.
families. Child Development, 65,
Comparison of Minnesota and
twins:
adult
Swedish
Behavior
data.
Genetics,
25(5),
421^31. Flynn,
R. (1998).
J.
IQ gains over
Toward
time:
find-
&
ing the causes. In U. Neisser (Ed.). The rising
Plomin, R. (1992). Multivariate genetic analysis
curxe (pp. 25-66). Washington, DC: American
Cardon, L. R., Fulker, D. W.. DeFries,
C,
J.
of specific cognitive abilities in the Colorado
Adoption Project
age
at
Intelligence,
7.
16,
Casto, S. D.. DeFries.
C,
J.
&
W.
Fulker, D.
(1995).
Multivariate genetic analysis of Wechsler Intelli-
gence Scale for Children-Revised (WISC-R)
Chemy.
A
Behavior Genetics, 25{ 1 ), 25-32.
S. S.. Fulker.
(1994).
S., et al.
J.
developmental genetic analysis of continuity
and change
in the
Bayley Mental Development
MacAnhur
Index from 14 to 24 months: The
Longitudinal Twin Study. Psychological Science, 5(6), 354-360.
LISREL
J.,
Daniels, (1998).
J..
&
Plomin. R. (1990).
modeling: Genetic and environmental
influences on
Chomey, M.
IQ
revisited. Intelligence. 14,
Chomey.
K., Seese, N.,
McGuffin.
J.,
A quantitative
cognitive
ability
P.,
Thompson,
trait
1-29.
L. A., et
J.,
al.
E.,
Wu.
McCleam
G. E.
J..
Siegel. B.. Tang.
baum. M.
C,
Association.
S. (1992). Intelligence
ing learning. Intelligence, 76(3-4), Harris,
way
children turn out the
they do.
Chomey, M.
L. A.,
&
Lubinski, D., Thompson,
J.,
A
Plomin, R. (2002).
quantitative
(2002).
S., et al.
Molecular Psychi-
10-11.
A
children:
failure to replicate. Psychological
Science, 7i(6), 561-562.
Ho, H.
&
Z., Baker, L. A..
Jacobs, N., Rijsdijk, F,
Decker,
ligence and mental retardation.
American Jour-
E.,
Kolachana. B.
S..
H., Mazzanti. C. M., Straub, R. E.,
COMT
Vall08/158 Met
genotype on frontal lobe function and risk for
Proceedings of the National
Academy of Science, B.,
A..
Mayeux,
J.
L.,
Hyman,
R., etal. (1997).
Effects of age. sex. and ethnicity on the association
between apolipoprotein E genotype and
Alzheimer disease:
A
R., et al. (2002).
population twin
meta-analysis.
Kaplan, D.
D., Olson,
R
J.,
Ahn.
J..
.K., et al. (2002).
7(4),
Won,
Pauls, link-
APOE
6p2 1.3-22. American Journal of Human Genetics,
70(5), 1287-1298.
Knopik, V.
S.,
Alarcon, M.,
Comorbidity deficits:
of
&
DeFries,
mathematics
J.
C. (1997).
and
reading
Evidence for a genetic etiology. Behav-
and
Light,
J.
G.,
&
DeFries.
J.
C. (1995). Comorbidity
of reading and mathematics
disabilities:
ing Disabilities, 28(2), 96-106.
Luo, D.,
Petrill, S. A.,
& Thompson, L. A.
analysis of cognitive
278(16), 1349-1356.
Reserve Twin Project. Intelligence,
& McCleam. abilities in
Genetic
and environmental etiologies. Journal of Learn-
Journal of the American Medical Association.
G. E. (1995). Heritability of cognitive
gen-
368-374.
T W.,
Evidence for
exploration
McGue, M..
A
study of the causes of
Molecular Psychiatry,
Gayan,
E..
Child psy-
ability:
Alzheimer Disease Meta Analysis Consortium.
Finkel, D., Pedersen. N. L.,
(1988).
ior Genetics, 27(5), 447-453.
9S(12), 6917-6922.
Farrer, L. A., Cupples, L. A., Haines,
Kukull, W.
M.
age and association with reading disability on
nal of Mental Deficiency, 92(1), 2-11.
schizophrenia.
S.
Derom. C, Danckaerts, M.,
Derom,
Thier)', E.,
association.
etal. (2001). Effect of
trait
locus not associated with cognitive ability in
influences. Behavior Genetics, 18(2). 247-261.
in cognition.
T
Why York:
cognitive processing: Genetic and environmental
Detterman, D. K. (1987). Theoretical notions of intel-
J.
New
Free Press.
eral
Callicott.
in
415^26.
R. (1998). The nurture assumption:
J.
(CHRM2)
Egan, M. F, Goldberg,
Buchs-
and changes
regional cerebral glucose metabolic rate follow-
chopathology and lower cognitive
7,
&
Abel. L..
Role of the cholinergic muscarinic 2 receptor gene
(Eds.), Nature,
psychology (pp. 77-97). Washington,
Psychological
Rostamkhani, M., McGue,
S..
&
Covariation between intelligence and speed of
M., Lacono, W. G.. Cheng, L.
atn,;
L. R. (1993).
locus associated with
children.
in
1
Owen, M.
Science, 9(3), 159-166.
Comings, D.
& Cardon.
S. S..
DC: American Psychological
Hill, L.,
M.
Chipuer. H. M., Rovine.
&
Plomin
In R.
nurture
Haier. R.
D. W.. Emde. R. N.. Robinson.
Corley, R. R, Reznick.
J.,
Chemy.
Continuity and change in cognitive development.
383-400.
factors.
Psychological Association. Fulker. D. W..
of genetic
Malhotra, A. K., Kestler, L. J.
A., Goldberg, T. E.,
g:
(1994).
Hierarchical
An
factor
data from the Western
J.,
&
18,
Mazzanti,
335-347.
C,
Bates,
Goldman, D. (2002).
.
Behavioral Genetics and Intelligence
A
polymorphism
functional
and performance on a
test
in the
COMT
gene
of prefrontal cogni-
American Journal of Ps\chiatr\\ 759(4),
tion.
J.
D. (1992). Biological and physiological
76(3^),
correlates of intelligence. Intelligence,
257-258.
McArdle.
(1994).
G
there
Is
P
Kennedy. D., Simon,
A
(2000).
A., Lefly, D., Chhabil-
MRI
twin
human
L. (1998).
J.
Hamagami,
Prescott, C. A..
A
&
F..
contemporary method
for
Petrill.
brain.
S.
Plomin. R.. Hewitt.
P.
Lipton.
A..
S.
Chemy.
Corley. R. P. et
S.,
al. J.
C.
Journal of Cognitive Neuro-
the
science. 27(1). 223-232.
J.,
H., et
J.
study of size variations in
76(1). 163-2(X).
Horn.
E.
genetic
(Is there
133-143.
abihty'?) Intelligence, 18,
Pennington. B. F. Filipek,
behavior genetic models. Behavior Genetics,
J.
McCleam, G.
beyond g?
75
influence on specific cognitive abilities indepen-
das. N..
(1986). Latent variable growth within
J. J.
McArdle,
1
dent of genetic influences on general cognitive
652-654. Matarazzo,
&
Pedersen, N. L., Plomin, R..
•
al.
(in
J..
press).
developmental-genetic analyses of age changes
Genetic and environmental contributions to gen-
Developmental Neuro-
eral cognitive ability through the first 16 years of
in
intellectual
abilities.
psychology. 74(1). 69-114.
McCartney.
M.
K., Harris,
A
(1990).
F.
Developmental Psychology.
Petrill. S. A.,
Luo. D., Thompson, L. A..
of general intelligence by elementary cognitive
E..
226-237.
tasks:
Johansson. B.. Berg.
Ahem. F.
L..
apart:
(1997). Sub-
on cognitive
80 or more years
in twins
Pedersen,
S..
Petrill. S. A., et al.
stantial genetic influence
abilities
old. Science, 276,
1560-1563. P.
A.. Stelmack, R. M.,
& Campbell,
K. B. (1992). Intelligence, reaction time, and event related potentials. Intelligence,
76(3^).
289-314.
Lykken, D. cognitive
T. J..
&
A
G. E.
Jr..
lacono,
W.
&
G.,
Behavioral genetics of
T. (1993).
ability:
R. Plomin
ton.
Petrill.
S. A..
Plomin. R.. Berg,
Pedersen, N., Ahern,
lifespan
McCleam
and psychology
perspective.
In
(Eds.), Nature,
(pp. 59-76).
DC: American Psychological
80 years and
older.
Psychological Science, 9(3),
Petrill. S. A..
Saudino. K.
Washing-
T.
J.,
Jr.,
Association.
Lykken. D. T.
twins reared apart. Intelligence. 76(3^).
289-314.
& Plomin. two-
in
Plomin. R. (1986). Development, genetics, and per-
Lawrence Erlbaum.
sonality. Hillsdale, NJ:
Plomin, R. (2003). Genetics, genes, genomics, and g.
Molecular Psychiatry,
&
8,
1-5.
Dunn.
J.
Nonshared en\ironment a decade
Why
(2001).
same family so
different?
later.
Cana-
dian Journal of Psychiatry. 46. 225-233.
& Christensen,
K. (2002). The heritabil-
of level and rate-of-change in cognitive func-
tioning in Danish twins aged 70 years and older.
Experimental Aging Research, 28(4),
&
M. C,
McArdle,
growth
Research,
Neubaurer. A.
3,
curves
J.
J.
for
435^5
1
(2000). Structured
twin
data.
Twin
165-177.
C.
Angleitner, A.,
Spinath.
&
Plomin. R..
&
Bergeman, C.
Borkenau,
M..
P
Riemann.
R..
(2000). Genetic
speed of information processing and their
psychometric intelligence:
rela-
Evidence
S.
(1991).
nurture: Genetic influence
The nature of
on "environmental"
measures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
14,
373^27. Plomin. R.. DeFries.
J.
C.
&
Loehlin.
J.
(1977).
Genotype-environment interaction and cortelation in the analysis of
F.
and environment influences on two measures of
to
Wilkerson. B..
year-old twins. Intelligence, 29, 31-43.
are children in the
tion
S.,
and modularity' of cognitive functioning
Feuer, D. (1984). Information processing abili-
latent
The
(1998).
183-189.
Plomin. R.. Asbury. K.,
McGue, M.,
Johansson, B..
S.,
et al.
genetic and environmental relationship between
&
McGue. M.. Bouchard. ties in
F.,
R. (2001). Genetic and environmental molarity
McGue, M.. Bouchard.
nurture,
Genetic and environmental influences.
Behavior Genetics, 26(2), 135-148.
general and specific cognitive abilities in twins
McGarry-Roberts.
Neale,
Detter-
meta-analysis of twin studies. Psychological
McCleam. G.
ity
&
man. D. K. (1996). The independent prediction
Bulletin. 107.
N.
Bemieri,
developmen-
Growing up and growing tal
life.
&
J.,
human
behavior. Psycho-
logical Bulletin. 84, 309-322.
Plomin, R.. DeFries,
McGuffin. ed.).
P.
J.
C, McCleam,
G.
E..
&
(2001). Behavioral genetics (3rd
New York:
Worth.
Plomin, R., Fulker, D. W., Coriey, R.,
& DeFries,
J.
C.
from the German Observational Study of Adult
(1997). Nature, nurture, and cognitive develop-
Twins. Intelligence. 28(4). 267-289.
ment from
1
to
16 years:
A
parent-offspring
I
I
176
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE
.
adoption study. Psychological Science.
S(6),
Posthuma, D., Neale, M. C. Boomsma, D. Geus, E.
Child
moderator.
as
Development,
70(5).
How
make
1151-1162.
AA2-AA1.
C. (2001
J.
).
I..
&
de
Are smarter brains running
& McCartney.
Scarr, S.. their
own
faster? Heritability of alpha
peak frequency, IQ,
environment
and
Behavior Generics.
424-435.
intercorrelation.
their
Tambs.
31(6), 567-579.
C.
Price. T. S.. Eley. T.
Stevenson.
&
J..
A
&
M..
J.
people
theory of genotype-
Child Development.
effects.
Sundet.
K..
K. (1983).
environments:
54.
Magnus. R (1984).
Plomin. R.
Genetic and environmental contributions to the
(2000). Genetic and environmental covariation
covariation between the Wechsler Adult Intelli-
between verbal and non-verbal cognitive devel-
gence Scale (WAIS) subtests:
opment
Intelligence. 16{4).
infancy. Child Development.
in
77(4).
Thompson,
948-959.
Ramey, C.
&
T.,
Campbell.
early childhood education tence:
Reed, T.
E..
&
and scholastic achievement: Genetic overlap but
velocity in a brain nerve
158-165.
2,
pathway of normal
Thompson.
Plomin. R..
A.,
L.
&
and specific cognitive
gence. 76(3^). 259-272.
Adoption Project. Intelligence.
&
Hetherington, E. M..
Turkheimer.
&
E..
environment:
A
phering genetic and social influences on adoles-
quantitative
review.
MA:
Cambridge.
Harvard
&
Pedersen.
N. L. (2002). Sources of influence on
rate
of
cognitive change over time in Swedish twins:
An
application of latent growth. Experimental
Aging Research. 28(4). 407-433. Reznick.
A
J.
S..
Corley. R.,
Robinson,
J.
(1997). in
second year. Monographs of the Society
for Research
in
Child Development.
62{
1
).
1-154. Rice.
T..
&
DeFries,
J.
C. (1986).
Multivariate path analysis of specific cognitive abilities
the
in
Colorado Adoption Project.
Behavior Genetics. 76(1). 107-125. Rijsdijk.
E
V.,
&
Boomsma.
D.
I.
(1997). Genetic
nerve conduction velocity and
IQ.
Behavior
Genetics, 27(2). 87-98. Rijsdijk, F. V., Vernon.
The
P
(
&
Boomsma. D.
I.
genetic basis of the relation between
speed-of-information processing and IQ. Behavioural Brain Research, 95(1), 77-84.
Rowe, D. C, Jacobson. K. C.
& Van den
J.
C.
& Boomsma. D.
I.
1998). Longitudinal study of genetic influences
on ERP-P3 during childhood. Developmental Neuropsychology, 14(1). 19^5. van Beijsterveldt. C. E. M.. Moienaar.
C,
J.
&
C. M.. de
P.
Boomsma. D.
I.
(1998a).
EEG
Genetic and environmental influence on coherence. Behavior Genetics. 28(6).
van Beijster\eldt. C. E. M.. Moienaar.
Geus. E.
J.
C.
&
Boomsma. D.
I.
P.
443^53. C. M.. de
(1998b). Indi-
P300 amplitude:
A
genetic
study in adolescent twins. Biological Psychology. 47(2).
Vernon.
91 -\ 20.
A. (1989).
P.
The
and Individual
heritability of
measures
Vernon.
P.
tion
A..
&
time,
Differences. 10(5) 573-576.
Mori,
M.
Wadsworth, J.
S.
J.
J.
(1994).
76(3^). 273-288. School achievement. In
C. DeFries. R. Plomin.
&
D. W. Fulker (Eds.).
Nature and nurture during middle childhood (pp. 86-101).
Oord. E.
(1992). Intelligence, reac-
and peripheral nerve conduction
velocity. Intelligence.
A..
and
Psychological Bulletin.
of speed of information processing. Personality
mediation of the correlation between peripheral
(1998).
theoretical, methological.
vidual difference in
Fulker. D. W..
9(1). 1-13.
726(1). 78-108.
Geus, E.
twin study of intelligence
longitudinal
the
&
C.
Colorado
abilities in the
van Baal. G. C. M. de Geus. E.
Reynolds. C. A.. Finkel. D.. Gatz. M..
J.
Waldron. M. (2000). Nonshared
Plomin, R. (2000). The relationship code: Deci-
University Press.
DeFries.
(1985). Parent-infant resemblance for general
adults correlates with intelligence level. Intelli-
cent development.
Plomin. R.
environmental difference. Psychological Science.
University Press.
J.,
&
L. A.. Detterman, D. K..
(1991). Associations between cognitive abilities
Cambridge,
project.
Jensen. A. R. (1992). Conduction
Reiss. D., Neiderhiser,
study of twins.
and academic compe-
The Abecedarian
UK: Cambridge
A. (1991). Povern,
F.
A
475-487.
Cambridge,
MA:
Blackwell.
Wilson. R. S. (1983). The Louisville Twin Study:
C. G. (1999). Genetic and environmental influ-
Developmental synchronies
ences on vocabulary IQ: Parental education level
Development. 54. 298-316.
in behavior.
Child
11 A Dialectical constructivist view of
Developmental Intelligence Juan Pascual-Leone Janice Johnson
Dedicated to the
memory
of Robbie Case
Separate causal factors operate through development to affect somewhat separately cognitive processes
and produce
to elucidate the nature
individual differences.
.
.
.
Further evidence
is
needed
of these factors.
NollandHorn(1998, g stands unassailed as a big concretion of mental triumph and a cognitive enigma.
p.
280)
a psychometric
test variance. It is
Deary (2002.
p.
176)
In summary, empiricism begins with the record of plain facts, science denounces this
evidence to discover hidden laws. There
is
no science but of that which
Bachelard (1949.
William Stem (1914/1977), contemporary of Binet
person's
problem
J.
Pascual-Leone)
38; translation by
hidden.
situations.
This coping synthesizes
or constructs performance by
composing pre-
concept. defined intelligence as the
viously acquired processes (schemes) in novel
successfully coping with novel
ways. Thus, Burt and Stankov defined intelligence
skill in
AUTHORS" NOTE; two authors from
anonymous
and inventor of the IQ
p.
is
Preparation of this chapter was facilitated by an operating grant (#410-2001-1077) to the
the Social Sciences
referee,
whose
insightful
and Humanities Research Council of Canada.
comments guided us
We
thank the editors and an
in the final revision.
177
178
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE
.
as "the 'integrative function of the mind' that
encompasses processes 2002,
p. 25).
at all levels"
assume
all
and
adulthood.
of
beginning
its
third approaches.
that intelligence
increases with age in normal persons, up to
spite
explanation and process analysis in the second
Other definitions have been offered,
often less apt, but
In
cognitive science, which contributes to causal
6.
(Stankov,
Binet's
(in
and
Because intelligence as a construct is rooted in individual and age differences, psychometric intelligence
and developmental intelligence might
intel-
be the two main contenders. However, the other
ligence often suffer from lack of integration
four are essential for clarification and construct
Spearman's work), current conceptions of with cognitive-developmental research.
Ever
since Reuchlin (1962. 1964). researchers have
validity.
Many
believe that factor analysis
descripti\e tool, a
neo-Piagetian theory with psychometric con-
ture available in data (e.g.. Burt.
ceptions of intelligence
Demetriou,
Case,
Anderson. 1999;
(e.g..
&
Platsidou,
Pascual-Leone, 1969). The hope
mental theory and
its
Kazi,
is
2001;
that develop-
empirical methods can
2000;
is
way of condensing
recognized the need to integrate Piagetian or
Reuchlin.
1941; Deary,
and think
1962).
a general the struc-
other
that
approaches, such as development, serve to anchor causal interpretations of factors
&
Horn
(e.g.,
Horn, 1998;
Noll. 1994; Reuchlin. 1964). Develop-
help "to devise measures that are theoretically
mental intelligence addresses the ontogenetic/
grounded and can be used with equal
psychogenetic evolution of the person's informa-
all
age groups" (Anderson. 1999.
we summarize an
chapter,
toward
this
integration
and
p.
facility in
316). In this
attempt to work
progressive emergence of noninformational organi-
to other
smic processes (resources, factors of mamration),
relate
it
ongoing attempts.
all critical
A Theories. Models, and Varied
for causal theorizing.
ways
is
to express the
tural findings
construals of intelligence
descriptive when it phenomena and strucencountered. It is causal when its
theory or model
offers
constructs are distinct from the descriptive constructs to be explained,
The
first
obstacle to integration of different
views about intelligence
We
components) and the
tional processes (schemes,
is
epistemological.
can be independently
anchored on experience via experimentation,
and
the
causal
constructs
are
capable
of
distinguish six substantive viewpoints from
accounting for change that descriptive con-
which intelligence can be investigated (PascualLeone & Goodman, 1979);
experience, maturation, or organismic change.
undergo as a
structs (and data)
result of events,
Descriptive and causal theories or models, in 1
the innate potential or capacity for development (a genotype,
i.e..
Hebb"s [1949] Intelligence A);
turn,
can be either local or general (Pascual-
Leone,
1978.
2.
the high-cognitive
(i.e..
These
1980).
of theories/models
are
all
distinct
jointly
sorts
needed.
intellective/intellectual)
competence of a partly grown or mature person
Combined, they tion
(an intellectual phenotype.
i.e..
(i.e..
yield
two dimensions of
varia-
local/general and descriptive/causal)
Hebb's [1949] that
can be crossed. Thus. simplif> ing. there are
Intelligence B);
local 3.
psychometric intelligence, indexed by cognitive tests
4.
(Vernon's [1969] Intelligence
developmental intelligence,
and Piaget
Q:
causal
(Pepper's
[1942]
or models. This
is
complexity of
and
is.
the
more
distinctly
intelli-
normally achie\ing individuals (Pascual-Leone
& Goodman's
local
important because the more
which, as Binet
general a causal theory did. scales the
descriptive,
'"mechanistic"),
general causal (Pepper's "organismic") theories
gent performances by the age of the youngest
differentiated
it
will
structural theories
dinate,
it
and the more theories
be from the descriptive
means
to explain
and coor-
local, the less differentiated
[1979] Intelligence D);
causal 5.
general
descriptive,
will
neuroscience, which provides a new criterion
descriptive
of construct validitv; and
distinction to collapse.
theories,
tend to be from eventually
their
leading the
A
From
Gardner's (1993)
perspective,
this
Dialectical Constructivist View of Developmental Intelligence
multiple intelligences theory appears as a collection of local descriptive theories,
hierarchical
1998;
Carroll,
analysis
factor
models
process
descriptive
the
(e.g.,
schemes, working mental space, short-term
memory) on which
mechanisms
these causal
theories
(e.g..
apply (as Oberauer also suggests
Horn,
1998;
this
1998;
Cattell,
and various
with
179
•
Chapter 22,
in
volume).
Horn & Noll. 1994; Noll & Horn. 1998) appear as more sophisticated and better structured
intelligence has to be
general descriptive accounts. Causal theoreti-
processes distinct from psychometrics, or gen-
may be
interpretations
cal
theories, but often there
adjunct
to
these
no clear distinction
is
We
believe that a general causal theory of
grounded on organismic
developmental psychology, but
eral descriptive
These constructs should
interpretable in them.
between causal and descriptive constructs,
be molarly defined so that they can serve as
perhaps due to lack of organismic general causal
technical
models
analysis of tasks, or test items, in a principled
can generate theory-based {funda-
that
mental) measurement of key causal constructs
language
for
conducting
process
manner.
(Deary. 20(X)).
A tant
its
explain
to
Therriault,
Laughlin.
emerged
size has
the
intelligence factor
SiiB,
focused on work-
line of causal theorizing
memory and
ing
as impor-
of a general
existence
(Conway, Cowan, Bunting,
& Minkoff. & Conway,
1999; Kyllonen, 2002;
2002; see also Chapter
&
Schulze,
We
volume).
5, this
are
sympathetic with this interpretation, which in a different language agrees with an early predic-
tion of Pascual-Leone
1970; Pascual-Leone
(Pascual-Leone.
&
1969,
Effortful Structural Learning The most
memory (WM) Engle
(Pascual-Leone.
It
2003) exhibits
1970; Pascual-Leone
1969,
&
discussed below) executive
when
number of
first
units that
retain in terms of
WM
simultaneously can
schemes or chunks.
schemes or
structures that the individual possesses. Indeed,
Pascual-Leone (1969) was the
mem-
Following Miller (1956), these working
ory theories, as well as our own, count the
coupled with the repertoire of effortful (or LM, as
al.,
Smith, 1969) pioneered:
mental-attentional
of the person but only
capacity
(A/)
(Cowan, 2001;
as a construct
1999; Ruchkin et
four characteristics that our developmental theory
1.
maximum
corresponds to the
et al.,
conception of working
interesting
Goodman, 1979) with
developmental intelligence:
to
OF Mental Attention and
2(X)2; Engle, Tuholski,
Oberauer, Wittmann, Wilhelm,
reference
Intelligence as a By-Product
2.
Tacitly, they reject the idea
memory and
("central")
researcher to
of
WM as a separate WM to
instead posit
point out that neo-Piagetian theory might offer a
be a state of hyperactivation (of information-
"good platform" from which
carrying units) within long-term
tion of
to attempt integra-
developmental and psychometric
gence (Case
et al..
2001.
p.
intelli-
327). Considerable 3.
research has
shown
that
Piagetian and neo-
Piagetian tasks (examples are described later in this
Focal attention and
its
assumed
mechanism
be the
to
emergence of
chapter and in neo-Piagetians references
given) load on fluid general intelligence (Gf) or
memory (LTM
or repertoire of schemes).
4.
At
least
in
WM
limited capacity are that
causes
in the brain.
Cowan
(2001) and Engle
new conception
et al.
WM
general intelligence factors (see Lautrey, 2002;
(1999), this
Vernon, 1969). From our perspective, however,
from short-term memory (STM) by claiming
the construct of a separate ("central") working
that
memory
unclear under scrutiny and possibly
(as
Embretson,
expresses the impact of controlled attention on
false
is
(see
2(XX), in press;
1995;
Ruchkin, Grafman, Cameron,
Bemdt, 2003: SuB
et al.,
2002). Indeed,
conflate or confuse causal (e.g.,
Pascual-Leone,
it
&
STM
is
distinguishes
the set of activated units in
Hebb. 1949. thought of
it),
whereas
LTM
WM
STM.
tends to
dynamic mechanisms
mental or endogenous/executive attention)
In this section,
we show how our developmental
theory explicates these four points, the
first
two
180
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE
.
addressing the problem of a psychological unit
and the and
last
two
the construct of mental attention
STM
relation to
its
and
WM.
analytical schemes produced with mental effort (initially in the left tic
hemisphere), global or holis-
automatized schemes (right hemisphere), and
so forth.
Schemes as Psychological
A
Units.
chunk
is
Schemes
an
are
ill-defined but necessary psychological unit that
tems
can be explicated by the scheme concept.
representations
move from one memory
Information does not
store to another; rather,
by
it
is
carried or mediated
distinct collections of neurons, often distrib-
uted over the brain, that are cofunctional (vis-a-
and coactivated
vis certain activities)
We
tasks.
these
call
schemes. Schemes situated
beyond
collections
the organism are
in
(i.e..
determine
conjointly
self-propelling
dynamic
sys-
produce plans, patterns of action, or
when
they
They
apply.
are
abstracted across situations for a given sort of
They must be
praxis.
internally consistent to be
formed. Their conditions and/or effects can, in turn,
be constituted by (copies of) other schemes.
Experiences are only possible because schemes produce, assimilate, or structure them.
distinct.
situation specific) causal factors
(i.e.,
overdetermine
that
functional
some
in
that
the need) manifest pertbrmance. and their
The Hidden Operators of the Mind's Brain. With the theory of schemes alone, one cannot general organismic constraints
explain
(i.e..
proper recognition facilitates process and task
those applying across kinds of schemes) such
analysis (Pascual-Leone,
as
Leone
&
1995; Pascual-
1970,
Goodman. 1979; Pascual-Leone
&
Johnson. 1991. 2004; Pascual-Leone, Johnson. Baskind, Dworsky.
&
WM
"central"
capacity
mechanisms,
inhibitory
"central"
limits,
structural versus con-
schemes'
tent learning, "central" resolufion of
competition in the network, or the emergence
Severtson. 2000).
Schemes overdetermine performance because
of truly
performances via unplanned
novel
they are self-propelling (Piaget"s assimilation
dynamic syntheses.
function) and tend to apply to the situation
prefrontal lobes take care of this by controlling
fire as
(i.e..
collections of neurons) under minimal
processes elsewhere
—
conditions of activation. Every scheme has a
areas)
dual reality: as a functional package of qualita-
Ruchkin
tive characteristics that
form
into) experience
its
parameter
learned:
chies
recursive
etc.),
It
that
the
scheme
will
set
(via neuronal
we
procedures
and can be
common to have scheme hierarschemes of schemes of schemes,
is
(i.e..
which we
the coordinated
call structures or
schemas
"program" or "script"
that
(e.g..
gov-
erns our behavior in restaurants or within familiar sorts
of problem solving).
LTM
can be best
explicated as a manifold repertoire of schemes
or structures sorted in kinds: executive schemes (prefrontal), operative tive
schemes
or linguisdc
(posterior is
limited.
metaphor of a
attentional "pointers."
A
restricted
better for-
mulation might be to postulate that the brain, in
synchonization) to cause performance. Schemes like
that
(2003) and others have expressed
with the
idea
number of
cortex
the
power
addition to a repertoire of schemes, has a small
(i.e.,
become hyperactivated and apply are
this
et al.
thought that
activation
probability
the
tions
inject
(i.e..
applies and as a
it
in
controlling
is
neural network area) that condi-
\ariable quantitative
weight for
can inform
when
a
often
It
(i.e..
—
schemes
(frontal). ^^?//ra-
perceptual or representational
occipital, parietal, or temporal),
coordinated packages of automatized opera-
of general-purpose functional resources that
hidden or
call
Leone,
1987,
Johnson.
We
silent
1995.
1991.
operators (Pascual-
2000; Pascual-Leone
2004; Pascual-Leone
&
et al.,
them operators because they are mechanisms of brain "hardware," defined as molar procedures whose computational details are unspecified and that apply on (constrain) schemes to change or produce new schemes or to synthesize truly novel performances. These operators are hidden because they lack substantive content referents, which 2000).
call
functional
schemes have sentational,
(i.e..
etc.).
purely relational
perceptual,
Instead,
motor, repre-
operators
multivariate
express
constraints
surprising patterns or "anomalies" (exhibited
tive/motor schemes (frontal right hemisphere,
under specific empirical circumstances)
basal ganglia, and cerebellum), affective/emotion
pure theory of schemes cannot explain. They
schemes (broadly defined limbic
are our attempt to formulate, as categories of
system).
that a
— A
Dialectical Constructivist View of Developmental Intelligence
•
181
Table 11.1
TCOs
Operator
Description
Brain Rei^ion
A
Set of affective processes that intervene in motivation and attentive arousal
Limbic lobes
C
Both the process of content learning and the schemes derived from
Broadmann primary
Hidden Operators Listed
in
Order of Their Likely Evolutionary Emergence
and secondary
associative content learning
areas
The ^eW
F
which
operator,
mechanism manner
acts as the brain's binding
that brings
All areas
closure to mental representations in a neo-Gestaltist
LC
The process of automatized
logical-structural learning derived from
C
Right hemisphere
learning through overpractice
T
Temporarily and effortlessly collates sequences of schemes, thus facilitating
Occipitotemporal
the coordination that constitutes distal objects
S
Facilitates
emergence of spatial schemes or schemas by coordinating
relations of coexistence
among
activated
schemes
Occipitoparietal
effortlessly within the
situation
The
attentional interrupt,
which corresponds
unwanted schemes activated
inhibition of
power of
to the
Prefrontal
central active
in the situation
M
Mental
LM
Logical-structural learning caused by the effortful use of mental-attentional
attentional capacity of the individual
Prefrontal
Left hemisphere
capacity
tertiary areas
Executive schemes
in the
person's repertoire for the task
constructs within a psychological theory,
some
purely organismic constraints that the brain's cortical architecture
imposes on psychological
processes and behavior.
with permission, summarizes the operators that
we
10 hidden
currently consider. These 10
categories of operators (which
we
operators for short) are ordered
in
call
hidden
Table 11.1
according to their evolutionar}' emergence, as
we
speculate
to be.
it
We
have best methods of
hand
Prefrontal
&
1998: Johnson. Fabian.
Pascual-Leone,
&
Leone
1970,
Pascual-Leone. 1989:
1987,
Baillargeon,
1994;
2000). Development of
et al..
Table 11.1, modified from Arsalidou (2003)
at
endogenous growth of mental mental
theory,
attention
among
(i.e.,
2001;
Pascual-
Pascual-Leone
M
explains the
attention. In our
endogenous, executive)
from dynamic interactions
results
four different sorts of processes:
ator (the
scheme
M oper-
activation resource), / operator
(attentional interrupt, "central" inhibition
anism),
F operator
mech-
(neo-Gestaltist internal field
assessment for the eighth operator, mental (M)
mechanism, often known as minimum-principle
the key hidden oper-
or stimulus-response [S-R] compatibility), and
attentional capacity. This
ator
is
underlying Cowan's (2001)
et al.'s
and Engle
(1999) concept of working memory.
M
capacity appears empirically as a purely relational pattern relating task
viduals'
age.
That
is,
as
complexity to indi-
we
parametrically
E operator (the tive
allocates
functions.
M
.set
of execu-
or / to relevant/
M, and E are prefrontal We believe F results from local lateral
irrelevant schemes).
/,
inhibition processes in the cortex. This system
increment the number of aspects to be enter-
is
our model for mental attention.
that this mental attentional
system com-
age of children capable of passing the
bines capacity and other dynamic constructs to
M
explain what others would call the central exec-
task or item increases predictably. That
capacity
(often
neo-Piagetians)
called
is,
working memory by
increases
endogenously with
chronological age until adolescence
(e.g..
Case,
utive (see Chapter 4, this
attention
(.see
volume) or controlled
other chapters in this volume).
To
explain cognitive development, one needs other
182
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE
.
hidden operators as well. For instance, there content
or the
learning,
substantive)
(i.e.,
is
solving, cognitive development, and emotional/
C
interpersonal
operator, and logical-structural/relational learn-
L
ing, or the
The L operator encom-
operator.
development, and they exhibit
individual cognitive-style differences
by the
ability
indexed
cope well with them. For
to
two varieties: one obtained by automatization from C learning, called LC learning, and another obtained from repeated acts of mental attention (M), which we call
cope well, but field-dependent persons, despite having good developmental intelligence, often
LM learning.
2002; Pascual-Leone, 1989; Wapner
passes
at least
Witkin's field-independent persons
instance,
cope badly (Goode, Goddard, 1991; Witkin
Misleading Versus Facilitating Situations and
&
& Pascual-Leone, & Demick,
Goodenough, 1981).
Facilitating situations
those
are
in
which
Their Importance. The power of mental atten-
only task-relevant schemes are activated, so
by the number of organismically
automatized schemes are often sufficient to
tion is indexed
distinct aspects (task-required
schemes)
that the
person simultaneously can entertain within mis-
A
leading situations.
when
it
elicits
situation
schemes
misleading
is
that are unsuitable for
solve
the
Application of automatized
task.
schemes does not require use of mental
effort.
Furthermore, acquisition of automatized schemes is
much more dependent on
a suitable socio-
hand and. when they apply, lower the
cultural milieu (learning opportunities) than
on
probability of the subsequent activation of task-
maturational processes, such as growth of
M
schemes (Pascual-Leone, 1969, 1987, 1989; Pascual-Leone & Baillargeon, 1994;
capacity.
the task at
relevant
Pascual-Leone
& Johnson, 2004). In misleading
situations, a processing conflict usually
emerges
reliable
One
Strategy Y.
unsuitable but
is
well-learned
to
The other
which we
is
X,
is
by
cross-sectional developmental traces of perfor-
M and
/
as
&
ing the situation into a facilitating one. Stable
executive schemes and
Baillargeon, 1994). Because
in
misleading situations, the unsuitable Strategy
Y
is
more or
less
automatized or overlearned.
this is the first strategy to
be mobilized and
acti-
vated. Consequently, for the suitable Strategy to determine performance,
ing Strategy (i.e.,
X
one must avert apply-
Y by using active, central
mental-attentional
inhibition
interruption
operator), and concurrently. Strategy
X
—our
Y
embedded
in
it)
situations
are
pre-
this
leading (Pascual-Leone,
paradigm are mis-
1969,
1980) because
the appearance of a clay ball being compared, for instance, with a long sausage of clay (which
in
the intellective process that corrects this error
and breaking down
common
first
substance, weight, and
(i.e..
volume). The items of
when
Piaget's conservation
creating this illusion function as Strategy Y, and
the perception of the manifest drawing. Mis-
leading
of physical matter
is
to
the relevant figure. Strategy X,
partly inhibiting
sented to respondents,
hiding (has
process that succeeds in seeing the relevant
by
well-known developmental
most children the illusion that the ball has less amount (this is due to the greater perceptual surface of the sausage). The low cognitive schemes
{M
in contrast, is the executive mental-attentional
figure
A
must be
compels the respondent is
leading situations.
task paradigm exhibiting stages,
contains equal substance) perceptually suggests
operator). For instance, in an embedded-figures
see only the manifest drawing that
stages of development exist but (contrary to Piaget's claims) are found reliably only in mis-
/
boosted by mobilizing mental attention test item. Strategy
to disappear with repetitions
suitable but needs
capacities (Pascual-Leone. 1989, 1995;
Pascual-Leone
mance, which tend
as respondents learn to cope with the task, turn-
be effortfully boosted by mental-attentional
mechanisms such
dis-
opera-
field factor
strategy,
Misleading situations typically exhibit
continuous or stagewise trajectories in their
facilitated
—our F
only in mislead-
ing ones.
call
automatized schemes (often
or
congruent with the tor).
strategy,
domain-free stages of development
in facilitating situations but
between two or more different implicit or explicit strategies.
One, therefore, should not expect
in
problem
functions
shows the
as
Strategy X. This example also
that ability to interrupt (centrally inhibit)
misleading schemes does not suffice to
explain a developmental process
(i.e..
applica-
tion of Strategy X); sufficient mental-attentional
capacity also
is
needed.
When
Piaget increased
A
Dialectical Constructivist View of Developmental Intelligence
the task complexity by changing the
from a
paradigm
of conservation of substance to one
test
of weight and then of volume, the developmen-
should consult Engle
model also explains
theoretical
lem. Task analyses show (Pascual-Leone, 1976)
difi'erent interpretation.
difficulty increased
to 10 years
M
that
1
rently increased
meaning of
this notation is
Within I,
this
model of mental
attention
F>, working memory (WM),
stood by
Cowan
'?
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology.
77,
138-154.
M. Chapman
Pascual-Leone,
J.,
I.
E. Sigel (Ed.).
Mahwah.
A
rein-
permanence. In M. Candler
(Eds.), Criteria
for competence
Lawrence Erlbaum.
(1999).
A dialectical
Development of mental represen-
and
applications (pp. 169-200).
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Pascual-Leone.
&
J..
Johnson.
J.
Manual
(2001).
manuscript. York University. Toronto, Ontario,
Canada. J..
&
Johnson.
(2004). Affect, self-
J.
motivation, and cognitive development: tical
A dialec&R.
constructivist view. In D. Y. Dai
S.
J.
Integrative perspectives on intellectual
nition:
functioning and development (pp. 197-235).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Dworsky,
J..
Baskind.
S.,
&
August). Misleading situa-
and
tions, active inhibition,
the fair assessment
Paper presented
of mental capacity:
at
the
Behaviour, and Cognitive
(2001). If the magical
number
for operations
is 4,
within
(in
J.
press).
Hidden operators of
mental attention applying on sion of a separate working
LTM
give the
illu-
memory. Behavioral
Pascual-Leone,
&
J.,
Johnson,
&
Baillargeon, R. (1994). Develop-
mental measure of mental attention. International
Journal of Behavioral Development, J.,
&
Goodman, D.
gence and experience: Instructional Science.
17,
(1979). Intelli-
A neoPiagetian 8,
161-200.
301-367.
approach.
J.,
Baskind,
S.,
Dworsky,
Severtson, E. (2000). Culture-fair assess-
ment and the processes of mental Kozulin
&
Rand
Y.
(Eds.),
attention. In A.
Experience of medi-
An impact of Feuerstein 's
theory
education and psychology (pp. 191-214).
New York: Pascual-Leone,
Pergamon. J.,
&
of water level: review.
Sciences.
Ottawa,
Science,
Ontario, Canada.
in
J.,
Johnson.
J..
S. (1998,
ated learning:
Pascual-Leone,
The psycho-
mental attention, executives, and symbols. In
working memory? Behavioral and Brain Sciences,
and Brain
(1991).
J.
role in task analysis:
& Johnson, J.
24, 136-138.
Pascual-Leone,
its
(pp. 155-187). Hillsdale. NJ:
S.,
how does one account
Pascual-Leone.
Johnson,
terpretation of object
&
35-66).
(pp.
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Hillsdale, NJ:
meetings of the Canadian Society for Brain,
843-844.
Pascual-Leone,
from
Pascual-Leone.
To appraise developmental
not enough. Behavioral
Pascual-Leone,
in
Pourchot (Eds.),
Sternberg (Eds.). Motivation, emotion, and cog-
processing capacity. International Journal of
Pascual-Leone,
& T.
Adult learning and development: Perspectives
Pascual-Leone.
(1996). Vygotsky, Piaget, and the
J.
problems of
Pascual-Leone,
models of learning
C. Smith
for FIT: Figural Intersections Test Unpublished
as dialectical factors in cognitive growth.
Pascual-Leone.
M.
tation: Theories
An experimentalist's underchildren. Human Development. 37,
J. (
Irwin, R. (1998). Abstraction,
constructivist view of representation: Role of
University Press.
Pascual-Leone,
adulthood. In
Pascual-Leone,
Press.
(1990b). Reflections on life-span
J.
intelligence, consciousness
stages of
that
Wisdom:
Sternberg (Ed.),
and development
on wisdom:
essay
Towards organismic processes ble. In R. J.
&
J.,
the will, the self, and
(1990a).
J.
Pascual-Leone,
Morra,
A
Advances
S. (1991). Horizontality
neoPiagetian developmental in
Child Development and
Behaviour, 23, 231-276.
Pascual-Leone,
J.,
&
Smith.
J.
(1969).
The encoding
and decoding of symbols by children: experimental
A new
paradigm and a neo-Piagetian
model. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
8,
328-355.
— A
&
Pennings. A. H..
M. G.
Hessels.
measurement of mental
Dialectical Constructivist View of Developmental Intelligence
(19%). The
P.
attentional capacity:
of change
23, 59-78.
in children
Oxford University
neo-Piagetian de\elopmental study. Intelligence,
Stankov,
Pepper, S. C. (1942).
Emerging minds: The process
Siegler, R. S. (1996).
A
World hypotheses: A study
R.
L.
&
thinking. Oxford,
's
A
E. L.
diminutive general.
Grigorenko (Eds.),
of evidence. Berkeley: University of California
general factor of intelligence:
Press.
(pp. 19-37).
Piaget.
(1983). Piaget's theor\'. In
J.
(Series Ed.)
&
W. Kessen
of child psychology: Vol.
methods (4th
1.
New York:
John
the history of psychology
Brown
&
K.
J.
Thampy.
Trans.). Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
(Original
work
ical theory':
A
New
systematizing attempt.
York:
M.
(1962). Les methodes quantitatives en
psychologic [Quantitative methods
in
psychol-
ogy]. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.
Reuchlin,
M.
(1964).
L'intelligence:
genetique operatoire
et
Conception
conception factorielle
[Intelligence: Operational genetic
and
factorial
conceptions]. Revue Swiss de Psychologic. 23,
113-134.
Ruchkin. D. R.
S.
S.,
(in
systems:
A
SuB,
H.-M.,
Oberauer,
&
Wilhelm, O.,
and a
Tolman,
press). state
J.,
Cameron,
W. W.,
Wittmann,
capacity explains reasoning ability
little bit
E.
K.,
Schulze, R. (2002). Working-
&
C,
more. Intelligence, 30, 261-288.
Brunswik, E. (1935). The organism
Psychological Review, 42, 43-11. Vernon,
E. (1969). Intelligence
P.
K..
Working memory
&
Bemdt,
retention
of activated long-term memory.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26. 109-111.
and
cultural envi-
ronment. London: Methuen.
Vuyk, R. (1981). Oven lew and critique of Piaget's epistemology
genetic
New Wapner.
1965-1980
(Vol.
1).
York: Academic Press.
S.,
& Demick,
J.
dence-independence.
Grafman,
(Series B.
and the causal texture of the environment.
International Universities Press.
Reuchlin.
1750-1920
Washington, DC: University Publications
memory
published 1975)
Rappaport. D. (1960). The structure of psychoanalyt-
Trans.). In
of America. (Original work published 1914)
(1985). The equilibration of cognitive struc-
J.
M. Whipple,
D. N. Robinson (Ed.), Significant contributions to
4{A)).
tures (T.
How general is it?
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
testing intelligence (G.
and
Wiley. Piaget,
In 772^'
Stem, W. (1977). The psychological methods of
Handbook
History, theory,
103- 128).
ed.. pp.
Mussen
H.
P.
(Vol. Ed.),
UK:
Press.
(2002). g:
Sternberg
J.
201
•
(Eds.). (1991). Field
Hillsdale,
NJ:
depen-
Lawrence
Erlbaum. Witkin,
H.
A.,
&
Goodenough, D.
Cognitive styles: Essence
E.
(1981).
and origin. New York:
International Universities Press.
12 Development of Intellectual
Old Age
Abilities in
From Age Gradients
to Individuals
Martin Lovden
Ulman Lindenberger
The
present
chapter
objectives: (a) to ric
has
main
two
summarize psychomet-
theorizing and evidence about intel-
Cattell,
theor>';
1971:
Horn,
Cattell, 1966, 1967), the
PPIK
1996; see also Chapter
8, this
1982:
Horn
&
theory (Ackerman.
volume), and the
ligence in adulthood and old age and (b) to
decomposition of cognition into mechanics and
promote a person-centered (idiographic) approach
pragmatics (Baltes, 1987; Baltes. Lindenberger.
psychometric study of adult intellectual
to the
development. Both objectives can be framed the context of
two-component models of
span cognition. These models posit that
development of
life
in
life
span
intellectual abilities reflects
two
fundamental and dynamically interacting influences, the biological and the cultural. Historical
examples of such models can be found
in
&
Staudinger, 1998).
Though
the scope, termi-
nology, and details vary considerably across the different
they
versions,
all
share fundamental
assumptions beyond mapping intelligence onto
two underlying components
(see Baltes et
al..
1998; Lindenberger. 2001).
models assume
In general, these
ontogeny, there
is
component
that,
during
an "investment" of the bio-
Tetens's (1777) differentiation between relative
logical
and absolute mental capabilities and
Hebb's
tors related to neurophysiological status) into
(1949) distinction between intellectual power
bodies of biographical ly acquired knowledge
and
through processes of socialization, experience,
intellectual products
in
(Hebb, 1949). Typical
contemporary examples include the theory of fluid
and crystallized intelligence
AUTHORS' NOTE: Institute for
(i.e.,
Address correspondence
Human Development.
Gf-Gc
to
and education 1971).
(i.e..
(i.e.,
heredity and other fac-
investment theory; Cattell,
These investment processes lead
Martin Lovden, Center for Lifespan Psychology,
Max
to
Planck
Lentzeallee 94, D-14195 Berlin, Germany; e-mail: loevden@mpib-berlin.
mpg.de 203
204
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURE^G INTELLIGENCE
.
and intraindividual differences
inter-
in
the
acquisition and organization of procedural and
The
declarative knowledge.
nent and
its
biological
compo-
mental correlates are held to decline
after maturity.
The
component, however,
cultural
continues to increase over the
span as long as
life
knowledge maintenance and knowledge acquisition outweigh losses in the biological component.
Two-component
thus
theories
dispute
hypothesis
is
framed
we
Furthermore,
in terms of these interactions.
point out that theories about
development are generally anchored
intellectual
at the intraindividual level.
Therefore, the use of
methods relying on interindividual differences should be complemented by methods that are better suited for assessing
change
this intraindividual level.
We
in structure at
draw
attention to
the
a select toolbox of intraperson, or idiographic,
validity of a unitary general intelligence con-
psychometric methods and note that their appli-
struct
(i.e..
intellectual
g) in understanding the dynamics of life
span development: that
two broad categories of
least
deemed necessary
is,
at
ability factors are
to capture the basic proper-
of intellectual
first
category of ability factors represents mea-
life
surable outcomes of the influence of the biolog-
component on development.
itself in
It
manifests
novel information
and transformation of
(i.e.,
reasoning or Gf; Cattell,
&
1971; Horn, 1982; Horn
processes, including working
memory, process-
ing speed, and aspects of coordination and con-
et
of processing
al.,
(e.g..
1998; Horn,
Bakes,
1985).
1987; Baltes
Henceforth, these
processes are referred to as the "mechanics" of intelligence (cf. Baltes, 1987).
more disparate category of to procedural
mon
The second and
ability factors refers
and declarative knowledge com-
to a given culture
(i.e..
Gc), to specialized
knowledge such as occupational expertise Chapter
8,
this
(e.g..
volume), and to knowledge
about the meaning and conduct of Baltes et
life
(e.g.,
1998). Henceforth, these applica-
al.,
Cross-Sectional Evidence
The
Cattell, 1966, 1967)
or in broader ensembles of basic information
trol
The Age Gradients of the Mechanics and the Pragmatics
cognitive processes involving extrapola-
reorganization,
tion,
allow researchers to investigate the
amount and nature of heterogeneity in patterns of change and to gauge the validity of methods based on interindividual differences.-
span development. The
ties
ical
may
cation
is
Study
(SLS;
perhaps the most com-
prehensive source on adult age gradients of intelligence.
From young adulthood to old age,^ SLS display
the cross-sectional findings of the
continuous decrements for four mechanic abilconstructs:
ity
perceptual
speed,
inductive
reasoning, spatial orientation, and verbal ory.
In contrast, the
verbal
in
more pragmatic
mem-
abilities,
ability, show an young adulthood with an asymptote
knowledge and numerical
increase in
middle adulthood, followed by a plateau
modest decrements are discemable
The steady negative age for the
mechanics
tions are collectively referred to as the "prag-
rated by an
matics" of intelligence.'
evidence.
we summarize
Longitudinal
Seattle
Schaie. 1994, 1996)
in the
until
in old age.
differences observed
SLS
study are corrobo-
enormous mass of cross-sectional
In
their
large-scale
meta-analysis,
the available
Verhaeghen and Salthouse (1997) reported age
cross-sectional and longitudinal evidence per-
correlations of -.52 for processing speed, -.40
In this chapter,
taining to the
mechanic and pragmatic age gra-
dients in adulthood and old age. In this vein,
show
that
improvements
in the
we
understanding of
these gradients require efforts to
decompose
the
for reasoning, -.38 for spatial ability, -.33 for
episodic memory, and -.27 for short-term ory.
Importantly,
significant
were also observed,
mem-
nonlinear trends
at least for
reasoning, pro-
time/age dimension into constituent compo-
cessing speed, and episodic memory, suggesting
nents, such as terminal decline, selective attri-
accelerating age-related decline over the adult
tion, retest effects,
addition,
recent
interactions
and cohort differences. In
attempts to directly
model
between the mechanics and prag-
matics in old age are described (Ghisletta
&
Lindenberger, 2003), and the dedifferentiation
life
span. For the pragmatics, the cross-sectional
young adulthood followed by stability is present in most studies, but the age of peak performance differs somewhat pattern of growth during
across studies
(e.g..
Nilsson
et al..
1997: Park
Development of Intellectual Abilities 2002; Ronnlund, Nyberg, Backman,
et al.,
Nilsson, 2003). Figure
may
(2002),
12.1.
from Park
et
&
et al.,
al.
large
serve as an example of the typical
age gradients observed. The figure depicts cross-sectional age gradients based on multiple
Working memory, short-term memory, episodic memory, and processing speed display monotonic decline, whereas verbal knowledge shows increase indicators for five abilities.
•
205
2002) or are suggestive of relatively decrements appearing within, but not
&
before, old age (e.g., Baltes
1997; Lindenberger others at
Old Age
in
report
&
Lindenberger, 1994,
Baltes,
age-related
differences
about age 50 (Backman
&
Wechsler, 1997). In any case,
1997), starting
Nilsson, it
1996;
seems safe
to
conclude that the preservation of the pragmatics often observed for groups of young-olds does
or stability at least into the 70s. This inter-
not generalize to groups of old individuals
action constitutes the "classic aging pattern"
Backman
et al.,
(cf.
2000).
(Botwinick, 1977) or the "hold versus no-hold pattern" of adult intellectual development (e.g.,
W.
Jones
1949; Wechsler,
Hunt,
L.
&
Longitudinal Evidence
1955; see also
The
Conrad, 1933).
The major source of discrepancy cross-sectional
amount of
data concerns
decline in the
in available
onset
the
and
pragmatics during the to old age
longitudinal findings from the
show decelerating young adulthood, peaks in
construct
abilities
from young-old Backman, Small, Wahlin, & Larsson, 2000, for review; cf. Salthouse, 2003). Whereas some studies report small age differences during this
continuous decrements beginning
transition
period
(e.g.,
Baltes
&
Lindenberger,
Christensen, 2001; Nyberg et
al.,
1997;
2003; Park
study
mechanic and pragmatic age gradients. The latent
increases
(see
SLS
suggest a greater degree of similarity between
across
middle age, and accelerating declines
As
thereafter.
a notable exception, perceptual speed shows in early adult-
hood. The general pattern of rising and falling
span curves
life
is
discemable
in
most other
Working Memory Short-term
^—
—
*
Memory
Long-Term Memory
Speed
of
Processing
Verbal Knowledge
20
40
30
Figure 12.1
Cross-Sectional
Age
60
50
Age groups
(years)
Gradients for Composite Measures of Working Memory, Short-Term
Memory, Long-Term Memory, Speed of Processing, and Verbal Knowledge Source: Adapted with permission from Park
et al.
(2002). Copyright
(APA). Note: Each composite
is
constructed from three
tests.
©
2002 by
(n
the
= 345)
American Psychological Association
206
.
tt4NDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE Zonderman.
Studies (e.g.. Giambra, .'Vrenberg.
Kawas.
&
Wilson
et al..
Costa. 1995: Ronnlund et
2002: Zelinski
&
2003:
al..
Stewan. 1998).
headings that have become increasingly familiar to researchers in the field: cohort differences,
and selective
retest effects,
attrition.
For the pragmatics, the bulk of evidence sug-
SLS
gests that the
observation of an onset of
decline in as early as middle adulthood atypical.
The dominant
pattern
is
is
one of remark-
ably stable, or even increasing, performance until
young-old or old age (Christensen. 2001:
Ronnlund
et
al..
2003; Singer, Verhaeghen.
& Baltes. 2003: Wilson
Ghislena. Lindenberger. et al..
At
2002). first
Cohort Differences
rather
glance, large amounts of evidence on
Substantial f)erformance improvements have
been observed on various
tests
functioning
past
example,
at
during
the
of intellectual century
— for
time of recruitment into military 1987:
1998: Raven.
service
(Flynn.
2000).
These "time-lagged" improvements,
Neisser.
together with the evidence of mostly positive
SLS
longitudinal developmental gradients of intelli-
cohort gradients provided the
gences are available. Consider, for example, an
1994. 1996). point to the potential severity of
and recent study by McArdle.
impressive
Hamagami. and Woodcock (2002). including individuals (n = 1.193) from early Ferrer-Caja.
By combining
childhood to old age. sectional latent
ics
and longitudinal information
in
a
growth cur\e modeling framework, the
authors to
cross-
showed
that different
growth cur\es had
be specified for the mechanics and pragmatof intelligence
(e.g.. for
Gf and Go.
This
evidence clearly underscores the multidirectionality
of intellectual de\elopment predicted by
tw o-component theories of cognition.
confounding
comparing age differences historical time.
later-bom cohorts
a single point in
they favor
if
(i.e..
a constant rate), cross-
at
may
sectional age gradients
overestimate the
magnitude of true age-related changes (see Baltes. Reese. illustration).
& Nesselroade.
Furthermore,
cohort differences
may
it
1977. for an early
conceivable that
is
originate
may
from multiple
operate additi\eh.
opposing directions, or interactively: and
they
may
differ
constellation
in
that
and nature
across cohorts and abilities. In the SLS. for
average
plotting
age
gradients,
cross-sectional or longitudinal, as a function of
chronological age ser\es imjX)rtant descripti\e
However,
purposes.
at
For example, when cohort gradi-
ents are linear and positive
in
Clearly,
w ithin-cohort de\ elopmental change secular trends when
between-cohort
with
sources: that the effects
Summary
(Schaie.
it
is
well
known
that
example, approximately continuous improve-
bom from 1907 to 1966 were found for inductive reasoning, verbal memory, and spatial orientation, amounting to ments across cohorts
standard deviation (SD) for the for-
a fallible and incomplete
around a
1.5
index of developmental change. To substan-
mer two
abilities. In contrast, for verbal ability,
chronological age
tively
interpret
is
and understand de\elopmental
of cognition, one must carefully
gradients
unpack the ingredients contributing
to a particu-
lar score, for a particular individual, at a partic-
ular point in time. Aside
from components with
a relatively strong intrinsic relation to chronological age, such as maturation
cognitive
and senescence,
developmental gradients are
influ-
numerical
ences
in
vein,
(
2(X)0 obser\ ed )
to
closely linked to chronological age. In the fol-
lowins.
we
discuss these influences under three
amount of data. effects
on
on the Ravens Progressive Matrices test Thomdike. 1977). Hence, if one were
distance from death, onset of pathology, and less
a great
modest cohort
the Mill Hill vocabulary scale but substantial
(see also
are
early in the
cohort effects, and then even disadvan-
when summarizing
Raven effects
of which
bom
tages for later-bom generations. In a similar
sources such as learning histon., health status.
all
and perceptual speed, there for cohorts
20th centur\. followed by a p)eriod of no differ-
enced by a wealth of additional de\ elopmental
nonnormative events,
ability,
was an advantage
adjust
cross-sectional
age differences for
cohort effects, these gradients would probably
change
in
ways
that
were more complex than a
constant reduction across abilities and ages.
Development of Intellectual Abilities (i.e.,
Retest effects denote the possibility that, in
repeated-measures designs, prior exposure to
may
alter
performance
at retest,
either through practicing task-relevant elements
of
or through a broad range of reactive
skill
effects such as general familiarization with the
negative) alter-
or (possibly
situation
testing
207
•
a composite of cued and free recall) in two population-based samples of adults from
Retest Effects
a test or task
Old Age
in
10 cohorts 35 to 80 years old at baseline (total A^
=
The measurement
1,788).
was
interval
5 years. Adjusting for retest effects critically
observed longitudinal gradients.
the
altered
Average
observed for episodic
effects
retest
memory were
reliable (.15 SD).
More
important,
of these effects accentuated the
statistical control
ations in motivation and interest. Thus, if retest
decline observed for older groups of individuals
operate in studies of intellectual age
and converted the continuous increments from
important to estimate their direc-
age 35 to age 60 to a pattern of stable perfor-
effects
gradients,
it is
magnitudes, and correlates. For example,
tions,
mance
across this age period. In contrast, retest
longitudinal findings showing a late onset of
effects for the semantic
decline in the mechanics might be a result of
ligible (.04
retest effects; that
decrements
in the
beyond
(e.g.,
the
may mask
these effects
is,
underlying ability dimensions level-practiced elements of
Retest effects have typically been regarded as threats to the internal validity of longitudinal studies.
However,
it
is
with
ated
6 Rabbitt,
1998; Wilson et
taking and
individual
differences,
may Lowe
to explicitly
together,
2002), and failure
al.,
instances (see also Rabbitt et
test
with
model them may underestimate the
magnitudes of average decline
designs.
problem structures may be associ-
systematic
may appear even
retest effects
long retest intervals such as 5 years, they
For
example, prior acquaintance with the nature of
raw longi-
tudinal curves of growth and decline in any major
way. Thus,
quite possible that they
operate in cross-sectional
also
were neg-
factor
affect the
vary in magnitude across abilities (see also
Salthouse, 1991, 2000).
skill; cf.
memory
SD) and did not
some
in
al.,
but not
evident that retest effects,
it is
may
unanalyzed,
all
2001). Taken
when
left
limit substantive conclusions
based on raw longitudinal data.
thereby constituting a major ingredient in cohort effects. In this respect, longitudinal
theoretically
superior because,
designs are
with proper
Selective Attrition
A common
design precautions and analysis techniques, retest effects
can be estimated and
statistically
controlled. In practice, however, this has rarely
been done (but see Lovden, Ghisletta, Lindenberger, in press; McArdle et Rabbitt. Diggle. Smith, Holland,
2001; Ronnlund
et
al.,
2002;
al.,
&
&
Mclnnes,
2003; Schaie,
1988,
1996; Wilson etal., 2002).
One method retest effects
on the sample
level is to
compare
the performance in a group of returnees with that
of a cohort-matched and not previously
tested
sample of individuals (Schaie, 1988).
If
attrition effects (see next section) are negligible or,
better
account,
still,
then
also estimated and taken into
differences
between samples
should reflect retest effects (and error). In a recent study using this method,
(2003) examined semantic
mance
(i.e.,
Ronnlund
memory
et al.
perfor-
a composite of verbal fluency and
knowledge) and episodic memory performance
who drop
is
out between measure-
ment occasions perform, on average,
at
lower
levels than returnees (e.g., Lindenberger, Singer,
& &
Baltes, 2002;
Bumight,
who drop decline
of estimating the magnitude of
finding in longitudinal studies
that individuals
to
during
2003; Zelinski
measurement intervals prior Bosworth & Schaie, 1999;
(e.g.,
& Wallace,
1979; Singer et attrition
et al.,
Furthermore, individuals
out often evince greater cognitive
dropout
Colsher
Ronnlund
1997).
effects
1991; Siegler
al.,
(i.e.,
variables of interest)
&
2003). Such attrition
is
may occur
Botwinick.
nonrandom related
to
for a variety
of reasons that can be grouped into two additive constituents:
mortality-related
experimental selectivity (Baltes
selectivity
& Labouvie,
and 1973;
for computational separation, see Lindenberger et al.,
2002).
Experimental selectivity occurs whenever individuals
who are alive but
unable or unwilling to
continue participation differ systematically from
208
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE
.
who do
those
participate. MortaUty-associated
on the other hand, occurs whenever
selectivity,
for old than for
individuals in close proximity to death (e.g..
who do
participants
not return because they are
young-old subgroups of the
total
sample. Clearly, selectivity limits the generalizability
of the results obtained
in longitudinal studies
deceased) differ on relevant attributes from
because the observ ed sample
individuals with a longer distance to death. In
sentative of the original sample. Experimental
may
principle, both types of selectivity attrition
refer to
selectivity poses a serious threat to the external
population validity of longitudinal findings as
selectivity poses an
and, subsequently, correlations of the variables
ability level, or both.
it
internal \alidity threat to longitudinal studies,
as
of the experimenter
reflects the inability
it
randomly assign surviving individuals
to
no longer repre-
change of
related to level of ability,
Whereas experimental
is
a returnee status, mortality-related selectivity
to is
To capture
a player in the population league.
KJeemeier
this population-heterogeneity aspect,
(1962) coined the term tenninal decline, argu-
may
affect the
under study 2000). tivity,
means, \ariances. covariances. Lindenberger.
(e.g.. Little.
The existence of
on the other hand,
found heterogeneity
&
Maier.
mortality-related selec-
suggestive of pro-
is
development and issues
in
warnings against the uncritical practice of averaging individual differences
in intelligence
Backman
a panicular age/time (cf
et al..
over
2000).
ing that aging-related changes in old age should
For example, when rigorous health screening
be distinguished from the changes associated
applied, old age groups
&
with impending death (see also Riegel
Riegel.
1972). Specifically, changes associated with ter-
minal decline
may
reflect other causal structures
than aging-induced changes
—
for example, spe-
cognition-influencing diseases and global
cific
may
of younger age groups, and the difference
tive
sample composition may
is
not be representa-
result in
in
an underesti-
mation of cross-sectional population gradients. That
is.
individuals sur\iving into late
life
who
are ready to participate in cross-sectional studies
may
constitute a select group in terms of multi-
1996:cf. Thaler. 2002).
ple,
correlated
Mounting evidence also points to an association between performance on intelligence tests
and cognitive
breakdowns of the biological system
and longevity
(e.g..
Singer
et al..
(e.g..
Berg.
2003: Small
Biickman. 1997). as well as between changes
and mortality
intelligence
Schaie.
&
1999: Maier
Bosworth
(e.g..
&
2003: see Berg, 1996; Small
et al..
(/?
=
that, in a
for the Berlin
the
first
Aging Study (BASE), those
in
when
(because mortality risk
al.
nme who
died within 6 years after cognitive performance
was
initially
initial
measured
(n
= 277) showed lower
performance compared to individuals
who were
still
alive
6 years
later (n
=
229).
Funhermore. greater a\erage decrements over 4 years
the
after the first
were observed for the
4
to
measurement occasion
indi\ iduals
6 years after the
initial
who
died during
measurement on
measures of perceptual speed and knowledge, as
compared
to those
who
survived and partici-
pated in the 6-year follow-up. Attrition due to factors other than mortality
was
(i.e..
related to only the initial le\
experimental)
el.
nitudes of both selectivitv effects
may
&
group of individuals
= 70-103) measured
516: age
the other hand,
also be overestimated, particularly in old age.
Backman.
1999. for overviews). For example. Singer et
(2003) reported
On
the magnitude of age-related decrements
&
1999: Singer
Smith.
socioeconomical. biological,
characteristics.
and the magwere greater
That
selectivity effects increase dramatically is
associated with age).
when averaging performance
is,
tively healthy individuals
of rela-
and individuals expe-
riencing terminal decline, aging-related changes
and dying-related changes are confounded. there
vein,
this
is
prolonged preclinical phase of dementia
when
(i.e.,
cognitive deficits are present but have
not yet have reached a diagnostic threshold: Elias et
al.,
Winblad.
&
cf.
2000: Small. Fratiglioni. Viitanen,
Backman. 2000). Because
dence of dementia age.
In
persistent evidence for a
including
an
is
the inci-
markedly higher
in old
unknown proportion of
individuals with preclinical signs of dementia
may
again
overestimate the
"pure"
aging-
intelligence relation (Slivvinski. Lipton. Buschke.
&
Stewart, 1996: but see
Wahlin. again, as ity
Small. is
&
Backman, Laukka,
Fratiglioni.
2002).
And
true for age-associated heterogene-
phenomena,
the admixture of individuals
with signs of terminal decline and preclinical
Development of Intellectual Abilities
dementia will increase variances and correlations, especially
in
Backman
age-heterogeneous samples
el al.,
2002; Sliwinski
(e.g..
sum, two opposing selectivity effects appear to operate in aging populations, one In
resulting in
tions in old age
owing
rela-
dementia
to preclinical
209
•
(differentiation) during maturation (e.g., Garrett,
becomes compressed
1946). and
(dedifferenti-
ated) again in old age (e.g.. Bakes, Cornelius,
et al., 1996).
exaggerated age-performance
Old Age
in
&
Nesselroade,
Spiro.
As
1970).
such,
1980; Reinert,
Willis,
hypothesis conveys
the
dynamic view of the structure of and
abilities
information-
underlying
the
a
intellectual
to
processing mechanisms. During childhood and
attenuated age-performance relations because
old age. the operations and expressions of the
and impending death and the other leading
may be more
individuals in older age groups
younger
positively selected than indi\iduals in
age groups
(cf.
Backman
2000). Clearly,
et al..
common
these considerations deconstruct the
assumed
diversity of cognitive abilities are
to
be subject to strong and fundamental systemic constraints
developmental status of
the
(i.e..
biological substrates of intelligence). In con-
assumption of a homogeneous population and
trast,
underscore the need to describe and explain the
common
during maturation and constraint
adulthood, this
in
relaxed, and other factors,
is
and occupational/
developmental characteristics of the individuals
such as
who compose
educational opportunities, occupy more promi-
the samples.
interest, motivation,
nent roles as determinants of intellectual devel-
opment, leading to greater diversity
Summary Apparently,
understanding
the
mental gradients of intellectual
abilities
is
a
impede
generalization across age groups and abilities,
and developmental changes appear
to
be hetero-
geneous by nature. Various influences may work simultaneously in opposing directions, and their
magnitude may
differ as a function of
study. Thus, an
improved understanding of the
age and
developmental gradients of intelligence requires consideration of
all
these factors in concert and,
as a best-case scenario,
latent
w ithin
method-
a unitar>'
framework. Recent developments
in
growth cur\e modeling and multilevel
modeling are promising candidates for such
frameworks (see Collins Liang,
&
Zeger,
& Sayer, 2001;
1994;
Little,
The dynamic view of
develop-
tricky business. Internal validity threats
ological
in levels
of
functioning in different abilities.
Diggle,
Schnabel.
&
early findings of a decrease in the
variance accounted for by g and decreases in
hood
to early maturity (e.g., Garrett, 1946), as
well as by corresponding findings of increases in old this
age
Reinert, 1970; see Chapter 14,
(e.g.,
volume, for a general treatment of factorial
invariance).
With respect
entiation in old age,
to
covariance dediffer-
more recent empirical
(e.g..
Bakes
&
Lindenberger,
Hertzog, Dixon,
Mitrushina Willis,
&
&
&
1991;
Satz,
Intrieri,
some
are
et al.,
1997; Hultsch,
Small, 1998; Li
&
1998; see Li
also
Lindenberger.
et al.,
(e.g.,
extent, the
somewhat mixed evidence methodological problems
ferentiation hypothesis
and the comparison of
Dedifferentiation Hypothesis
results across studies, including factors
Couched
restriction in range (e.g..
life
such as sample composition, unsuitable age groups, and
span intellectual
development, the differentiation/dedifferentiation hypothesis asserts that the functional organization of intellectual abilities
is
relatively
Juan-
2002). At least
surrounding the empirical testing of the dedif-
Old Age: The
terms of
2004;
Maitland,
some prominent
available
2002; Park
et al.,
Schaie,
may emanate from
to
in
evi-
dence has generally bolstered the early findings
Espinosa
in
from child-
correlations across different abilities
exceptions
Interactions Between Mechanics
of
structure
was sparked by amount of
hypothesis
dedifferentiation
1999, for an overview), but
Baumert, 2000).
AND Pragmatics
the
intelligence conveyed by the differentiation/
com-
pressed in childhood, undergoes decompression
An
Deary
et al.. 1996).
essential corollary of old-age dedifferen-
tiation is the hypothesis that the entire
intellectual abilities
by a
common
is
source
space of
increasingly dominated (or
an
ensemble of
210
.
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE
common
old-age decrements in pragmatic abilities are
performance levels for the pragmatics. However, in old age, negative age gradients generally prevail for both the mechanics and the pragmatics. Hence, in old age, the difference in
be induced by the mechanic decline
the directionality of the age gradients vaporizes
&
(for a longitudinal analog, see
Framed
sources) of intellectual development.
terms of the distinction between
in
mechanics and pragmatics of cognition,
the
assumed
to
(Ghisletta
Lindenberger,
2003).
other
In
Ronnlund
et al.,
words, the biological changes reflected by the
2003). This pattern, together with the findings
decline in mechanic abilities eventually limit
of covariance dedifferentiation,
the expression and accumulation of pragmatic
with the notion that the deteriorating functional
knowledge ties fall
in
old age
when
levels of
mechanic
the
below certain threshold
levels.
abili-
At higher
mechanic functioning, development of
the pragmatics
is
more dependent on and
experiential factors ical factors (but see
cultural-
dependent on biolog-
less
&
Hambrick
This dynamic account should perhaps be distinguished from the alternative nondynamic dedifferentiation notion of a
common
develop-
limit
to
starts
the
expression of the pragmatics in old age.
So
far,
evidence pertaining to the
direct
proposition that old-age decrements in prag-
matic abilities are driven by the mechanics
has
Engle. 2002).
mechanics
of the
level
consistent
is
been lacking.
Recently,
Lindenberger (2003)
dynamic
ing a recently developed structural equation
and
Ghisletta
lacuna by apply-
filled this
(i.e.,
lead-lag)
modeling method, the dual-
(DCSM; McArdle,
mental cause operating with constant force
change score model
throughout the adult
given that individual differences in rates of
McArdle & Hamagami. 2001; McArdle. Hamagami, Meredith. & Bradway. 2000). to
aging are present, rank ordering of individuals
combined longitudinal and cross-sectional data
should be
span.
life
increasingly
Specifically,
saturated
by aging-
related variance across age/time. Accordingly, if
age-related changes in different abilities are
common
driven by a
source
(i.e.. if
rates of aging
in different abilities are associated), correlations
across abilities should increase as a function of
time/age (Hofer
& Sliwinski, 2001; see also Li &
Schmiedek. 2002). Thus,
speaking, the
strictly
notion of an increased dominance of constraint
is
covariance
common
not necessary to explain a pattern of
However,
dedifferentiation.
the
BASE
from
and knowledge (Vocabulary and Spot-
Pictures)
a-Word) were used
DCSM
The
between
abilities
—
that
is,
among
participants
higher level of performance 1996; Spearman,
lower correlations
(e.g..
with a
Deary
et al.,
1927). Furthermore, as will
become evident below,
several
ings support the notion of a
more recent
common
find-
source that
linear models,
One
line of
ferentiation
evidence for the dynamic dedif-
account can be subsumed under
the heading of directionality dedifferentiation
(Baltes et
al..
2003). That
1998; Ghisletta is,
in the period
&
Lindenberger,
from young
to
considered as a variant
random
known
(LGM)
or
as hierarchical
coefficient models, or
mixed-effects models). Although these models
emanate from differ
different statistical traditions
in
down
to boil
to these
implementation
technical
to the
we
models as
same underlying
statistical
henceforth collectively refer
LGMs
(for detailed descrip-
tion of these models, see Collins
&
Sayer, 2001;
Diggle
et al.,
LGMs
estimate a time-based gradient for a
cific
1994; Little et
al.,
2000). In general,
group and represent individual
from
this gradient,
unobserved heterogeneity tories to error.
and
(e.g.,
& Ghisletta. in press), they all seem
model; therefore,
as deviations
varies in strength as a function of age.
may be
multilevel models (also
Lindenberger
non of divergence
mechanics and
growth curve models
of latent
being able to explain differentiation during mat-
2004) and the phenome-
to index the
the pragmatics, respectively.
may
al.,
= 516; age range = 70-104).
(n
Processing speed (Digit Letter and Identical
dynamic notion has the additional benefit of uration (e.g., Li et
2001;
sp)e-
trajectories
thereby allowing
in the individual trajec-
be represented rather than treated as
Extending these models to the multivariate
case allows for estimation of the extent to which levels
and changes are associated across
differ-
young-old age, the cross-sectional findings
ent variables, at the level of latent (true) scores.
summarized above reveal steady monotonic
The beauty of
decline in the mechanics but stable or increasing
extends the time/age-locked and symmetrically
the
DCSM.
however,
is
that
it
— Development of Intellectual Abilities
modeled associations between different variables (e.g., associations between slopes of change over 70
such as the one
60
conveyed by the dedifferentiation hypothesis.
DCSM,
other words, with the
low
that
the proposition
mechanic functioning
levels of
In
—and thus
temporally precede) decline is
amenable
answer
20
knowledge
different
.
80
by
from
that of
by
statis-
Knowledge
knowledge speed?
Ghisletta
-
60
-
"^"""^
-
and
o 50 o 2 40-
-
30 on 20
the lagger within this specific system of variat /-I
70
CD
cessing speed was the leader and knowledge was
processing speed
110
Age
Lindenberger (2003) clearly showed that pro-
is,
'
1
100
90
to the following
in processing
reported
results
ables; that
1
70
(i.e.,
speed on subsequent change
on subsequent change
The
-
30
the magnitude of the influence of
Is
level of processing in
''''''^'''^
-
pragmatics
to direct empirical testing
tically evaluating the
question:
in the
-
o 50 o ^ 40-
limit
drive
21
•
"I
the acquisition, expression, or maintenance of
the pragmatics in old age
Old Age
Speed
a certain time interval) to allow for empirical testing of lead-lag relations
in
'
70
80
100
90
time exerted
110
Age
a substantially stronger influence on change in
knowledge from
t-l to
r
than knowledge at t-\
did on subsequent change in processing speed
&
Gradients for Processing Speed and Knowledge
Lindenberger, 2003, for
statisti-
cal details). In other words, declines in
knowl-
(see Ghisletta
when and
edge,
if
they occur, are temporally
preceded by lower levels of processing speed (for similar results regarding younger participants,
McArdle
see
et al..
2000).
It
follows that, with
advancing age, interindividual variance
edge
increasingly
is
spjeed.
To
knowl-
by variance
saturated
in
suggesting that pragmatic abilities are
increasingly
ation.
in
composed of mechanic
we
return to the Singer et
al.
(2003) study,
516
sample was followed up two times, approx-
imately 4 and 6 years after the
Figure
12.2
depicts
initial
three
measure-
differently
calculated age gradients for processing speed
and knowledge, respectively. For our purposes, these measures
may
again serve as indicators of
the mechanics and pragmatics of cognition.
The
black lines represent the cross-sectional
gradient extracted
at
the
first
occasion for the longitudinal is,
Verhaeghen. Ghisletta,
Note: The thin black lines represent the cross-sectional gradient extracted
at the first
longitudinal sample (n
=
measurement CKcasion
132).
The dashed
for the
line describes the
corresponding cross-sectional age gradient for the initial
sample
-
{n
516).
The
the longitudinal convergence gradient
sectional
total
thick black line represents (i.e.,
combined
cross-
and longitudinal information) for the longitudinal
=
132).
further dissect the pattern of dedifferenti-
individuals ages 70 to 103 at baseline and that
thin
Singer,
Lindenberger. and Bakes (2003).
sample (n
Attrition." Recall that this study included
ment.
Source: Adapted from
variance.
described under the heading of "Selective
this
Three Differently Calculated Age
Figure 12.2
for those individuals (n
quently
survived
and
=
measurement sample
132)
who
participated
—
that
measurement
repeated individuals
are
highly
occasions.
select
owing
These to
both
experimental and mortality-associated reasons.
The dashed
line
describes the corresponding
cross-sectional age gradient for the total initial
sample (n = 516). This gradient presumably describes a sample including
more
participants
from cognition-associated health orders and terminal decline. The thick black suffering
disline
represents the longitudinal convergence gradient
(i.e.,
combined cross-sectional and
longitu-
dinal information) for the longitudinal sample. If
we
start
by considering the cross-sectional
gradient of the total sample,
it
is
clear that the
subse-
negative gradients prevail in both processing speed
the
and knowledge. In contrast, the cross-sectional
in
212
.
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE longitudinal
2003; see also Wohlwill. 1973: for a summary,
sample are more idiosyncratic: Knowledge remains stable, whereas processing speed
rumbles have been overheard by mainstream
decreases. In other words, there
developmental research.
gradients
describing
indicating a
select
the
is
Sample x Ability x Age
a pattern
interaction,
may be
suggesting that decline in the mechanics
normatively age related, whereas decline pragmatics, which presumably
in the
induced by
is
see Li. 2003). However, the echoes of their
cognitivist stereotypes,
In is
it
contrarv'
fact,
fair to
Skinnerian operant psychology, with
state its
to
that
careful
and manipulation of behavioral
description
repertoires o\ er time,
was much more germane
to
mechanic decline, may also be associated with
intraindividual research practice and theorizing
impending death (see also Small,
than most of today's developmental endeavors
&
von Strauss.
Fratiglioni,
Biickman. 2003). The shape of
the longitudinal gradient
is
with this conclusion. Specifically,
in
very old
age (> 90 years), negative gradients are for both processing speed this age,
it
likely that,
is
the effects of
e\ ident
and knowledge. At
even
(cf.
in a select
impending death are
sample,
large, relative
Baer. 1970; Richelle. 1993).
Two
largely consistent
interrelated insights are at the core of
the intraperson developmental research
agenda
(cf Chapter 14. this \olume: see Borsboom.
& van Heerden, 2003. for a review
Mellenbergh.
from a general perspective). interindividual
analyses of
First,
differences do not necessarily
aging-induced changes. Taken together, then,
inform the study of how
the pressure of biology-related factors, such as
within individuals. Recently, Molenaar. Huizenga.
specific diseases, in
and terminal decline increase
old age and may, at least in part, act as driving
forces of directionality dedifferentiation in old age. This conclusion
is
further bolstered by find-
ings suggesting that health-related and biologylinked variables such as sensory functioning
may
\'ariables are related
and Nesselroade (2003) elegantly addressed matical-statistical theory
a process having the to intra-
—
ergodicity
same
this
from mathe-
issue by applying a concept taken
—denoting
structure with respect
and interindividual variances. As a
rule
of thumb, a process can only be ergodic when
be related more strongly to cognitive function-
means, variances, and covariance are invariant
ing in old age. especially pragmatic abilities
across time.
(e.g..
Bakes
&
Lindenberger, 1997).
Intraindividual Patterns of
Change
As should be evident from the findings summarized above, developmental processes
related
to
godic.
From
are
intelligence this point
inherently
of view,
ble, if not likely, that analyses
seems
it
nonerplausi-
of interindividual
differences deliver results that are unrelated to It
is
a truism that the primary objective for
the structure of intraindividual differences. This
analogous to the conclusion
research on the aging of intelligence, and for
point
developmental psychology
aging-related changes from cross-sectional data
in general, is to
under-
is
made
stand the nature of developmental processes at
is
the level of individuals. Yet. standard multivari-
confounds
applied to hypotheses concerning
selectivity.
ate statistics
concerned with associations among variables across, rather than within, individuals.
long time,
this blatant
For a
discrepancy between con-
by interindividual-differences
the
in
form of cohort
The second message
development, such as standard cross-sectional
and longitudinal factor analyses, are notoriously
difficult
that inferring
effects
pertains to a critical
assumption of standard multivariate that
structural
and
among
relations
statistics:
variables
are
invariant across individuals. In light of the sus-
picion that developmental processes
may
lead
cepts and methods has been noted by a loosely
to interindividual differences in intraindividual
connected family of approaches, such as advo-
patterns of change, this assumption strikes a dis-
cates of a person-oriented view on development
cordant note.
(e.g..
Block, 1971; Magnusson. 1998; Valsiner,
1984); by
life
span theoreticians and methodol-
ogists (e.g.. Baltes et al.. 1977);
&
Nesselroade. 1979: Baltes
and by developmental systems the-
oreticians (e.2..
Lemer. Dowling.
&
Lara Roth.
Some
classics are worth revisiting
in this context. Baltes et al. (1977), for
suggested that the aim of tal
psychology should be
life
example,
span developmen-
to describe
and explain
intraindividual differences in behavior,
accompa-
nied bv the studv of interindividual differences
Development of Intellectual Abilities
and commonalities
in
these
intraindividual
changes. The developmental path of each individual tal
depicted as a multivariate developmen-
is
trajectory or, put ditferently, as a multivariate
pattern of intraindividual change.
drawn between studying ries
A
sharp line
is
intraindividual trajecto-
as the analytical building blocks
and the
study of the differences between them, implying that these differences
step
down on
might be substantial. To
the abstraction ladder, a similar
message appears every now and then as
critiques
dedifferentiation; that
sive
which
common
1985).
Such
gradients, exemplified in this chapter
trajectory.
The age
at
vary from person to person. Thus, an age-based
may
analysis
mask developmental
actually
terns that are truly
common
pat-
across individuals
because the biological timing of transition peri-
may
ods
ences
Age
differ across individuals.
in the onset
differ-
of adolescent growth spurts
example
sort (Wohlwill. 1970).
growth (see Estes,
"normally aging" and
developmental transition begins will
this
are the classical
of developmental
213
•
groups but denote, to some degree, different
time periods of a
across indi\ iduals in studies of learning and in
1956. for an early appearance; see also Hertzog,
Old Age
"terminally declining" individuals are not exclu-
against the heavy reliance on simple aggregates
studies
is,
in
for
phenomena of
this
Thus, to discern develop-
mental commonalities across individuals, one
may need
to substitute
age with neurobiological
and cultural-social constructs
that are close to
by the longitudinal and cross-sectional gradients
the developmental process of interest (see, e.g.,
of intellectual de\elopment. contain a mix of
Li
and interindividual variances and
intraindividual
mask
patterns of growth that are unique to the
individual
(cf.
& Nesselroade.
Jones
1990). In the
&
Schmiedek. 2002; Nesselroade
&
Schmidt
McCollam, 2000; Wohlwill. 1973). heterogeneity in patterns of change exists,
If
why
did standard multivariate techniques such
extreme case, one might end up with a gradient
as standard factor analysis not reveal
describing none of the individual growth curves
ence? As shown by Molenaar (1999; see also
make up the gradient. To illustrate, let us return
Molenaar
that
(2003) study for the
As deduced from
last
Singer
to the
time
(i.e..
et al.
Figure 12.2).
the absence of directionality
is
et al..
relatively
its
pres-
2003). standard factor analysis
insensitive to substantial
hetero-
geneity in patterns of intraindividual change; that
is.
solutions based on interindividual differ-
may
yield acceptable
dedifferentiation for the select cross-sectional
ences
sample, the pattern of dedifferentiation often
although the data correspond to individuals
observed
are very heterogeneous in terms of the structure
cross-sectional
in
studies
might be
driven by a subset of individuals in close prox-
of a person entering the termi-
nal decline phase
may move toward
a one-factor
to the
intellectual aging,
like to see increased use
one would
of methods that sepa-
solution (e.g., of measures of the mechanics and
rate intraindividual variability
interindividual differences, as well as
structure of a "normally aging" person of the
that
same age may be more
and transition periods
in patterns
the risk of sidestepping
many
differentiated. Statistical
made
up of mixtures of these two types of individuals
we
in these direcfions.
at
best,
mixture. Thus, a focus on the individual, or at
on
relatively
individuals, (e.g.,
is
homogeneous subgroups of to capture the dynamic
of intellectual development. if
some of the causal
of aging. At
useful approaches,
describe three broad avenues that might lead
Person-Oriented Approach With Pattern-Based Methods
needed
nonstationary. time-dependent) properties
Of course,
methods
an unknown
proportion of the individuals constituting this
least
and change from
have the potential to reveal heterogeneity
yield a picture of intellectual development in old
age that approximates,
who
In conclusion, to enhance the validity of our
knowledge about
the pragmatics), whereas the intellectual ability
parameters obtained from samples that are
data,
of their intraindividual changes.
imity to death. Put differently, the intellectual ability structure
fits
Instead of focusing on associations between variables,
pattern-based
relationships structures are
to
among
methods focus on aim
individuals, with the
group individuals on the basis of
indeed mortality related, one would expect most
Similarities and dissimilarities
individuals to sooner or later evince a pattern of
als are
similarity.
among
individu-
based on the profile of values on the
214
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE
.
to turn the
manage with this approach (see Bergman. 1998; Bergman et al.. 2003). Typically, classifications
conventional orientation of the cross-sectional
are carried out at each time, followed by the
under study. In terms of Cattell's
variables
(1952) data box. the general idea
is
data matrix 90 degrees, factoring people over variables
Q
(i.e..
technique), rather than factor-
ing variables over people
R
(i.e.,
technique).
study of individuals' group membership over
time with the aim to find typical and ami typical (cf von Eye, 1990) transition paths across the
Popular examples of model-based variants
groups established
include categorical factors, such as latent class
Bergman
analysis and
longitudinal extension, latent
its
When
transition analysis.
the observed variables
are continuous, the corresponding technique
called
Exploratory
analysis.
profile
latent
is
et al.,
at
each cross section
(e.g.,
2003). Panel designs lend them-
selves to this kind of analysis; for example, group
may map
transitions
theoretical model.
onto a stage-sequential
However, often,
at least
with
regard to intellectual development in old age.
pattern-based methods include configural fre-
transition
quency analysis (von Eye. 1990) and variants
process that deserves more direct and intense
of cluster analysis. Bergman, Magnusson, and
El-Khouri (2003)
may
be consulted for a com-
study.
might be a relatively slow and gradual
The next approach seems promising
in
this regard.
prehensive overview of the methods and the
behind a pattern-oriented
rationale
theoretical
approach, accompanied by an accessible descrip-
LGMs Allowing for Differences Parameters Between Subgroups
in the tion
of the technical
exploratory
(e.g..
implementation of the
in a closer
match between the
theoretical
model
and the measurement model. Specifically, principle,
there
is
no variance accounted
by the pattern-oriented approach
that
in
for
cannot be
explained by a standard variable-oriented factor
approach
(e.g.,
based methods
This approach merges the variable-oriented
non-model-ba.sed) alternatives.
The advantage of using a pattern-based approach lies perhaps not so much in statistics as
Horn, 2000). However, pattern-
may
offer a
more
direct reflection
LGM the
approach used
model-based
to structure
change with
methods
pattern-oriented
described above. Categorical latent variables are
used to describe groups of changing individuals
homogenous within and heterogeneous The notion is that each group
that are
across groups.
corresponds to a subpopulation with set
its
own
of parameters. These features are then com-
bined with the conventional growth modeling
of the theoretical suspicion that development
approach, which provides the parameters. To
may produce
recapitulate,
not only quantitative interindivid-
LGM
is
an attractive approach to
ual differences but also qualitatively different
analyze longitudinal data, relating an observed
developmental paths for different individuals. In
variable to time or to
other words, these methods
may provide a more way of focusing on the individual and. therefore, a way of grasping the extent and
such as age.
direct
estimated under the assumption that
nature of sample heterogeneity in patterns of
population, and a continuous latent variable
change.
used to capture any heterogeneity
A
further advantage of this approach
is
the
some time-related variable However, a mean growth curve is
uals in the sample are
all
drawn from
individ-
the
same is
in individual
trajectories of growth.
standard multivariate approaches offer ways to
The combination of the two approaches way to gauge the validity of the mean growth curve. By using an outcome variable
represent heterogeneity by including interaction
measured
at
terms or by employing multigroup factor models,
model
formed
the pattern-based approach offers a viable alterna-
correspond to different growth curves of the
capacity to structure and explore unobserved population heterogeneity'. Specifically, whereas
tive for structuring
unknown
geneity and exploring
its
many
sources of hetero-
Although the pattern-based approach offers a subgroups, the analysis of change
is
outcome
multiple time points, a latent class in
which the
latent classes
variable. In other words, individuals
are assigned to different groups based
correlates.
natural avenue for establishing
offers a
homogeneous is
not easy to
on
their
longitudinal trajectory. For example, one group
may have a linear growth, and may have a quadratic growth. Individual
of individuals
another
Development of Intellectual Abilities differences in the trajectories within the groups
by growth factor variances for each
are captured
group
(as
LGM). Muthen and
traditional
in
see also Muthen. 2001) provide
Muthen (2000;
in
Old Age
215
•
differences, individuals with similar patterns of
change can be aggregated into
intraindividual
homogeneous groups on (Nesselroade
tests
&
the basis of statistical
Molenaar, 1999). In
this
accessible overviews of this framework, and
way, generalization across individuals can be
Raudenbush (2001) describes similar
approached, and interindividual differences
tech-
niques within the multilevel modeling tradition.
in
intraindividual processes can be explored.
Multivariate,
Concluding Remarks
Replicated, Single-Subject,
Repeated-Measures Designs The methods described so
To capture
far are useful tools
for analyzing typical data sets in studies of intellectual aging
—
individuals,
relatively
that
data sets including
is,
few variables, and,
in
stand
if
among each
they do not differ
other only because they happen to be
bom
at
a
similar point in time nor construe intelligence as
a unitary and static construct. In younger adult-
hood, the biology-based mechanics and the
research design, as well as in data-
culture-based pragmatics of intelligence display
analytical procedure.
may
cal shift
individuals as
old age, one can neither treat
contrast,
the approach described shift
at
in
requires a profound
few measurement occasions. In
best, a
many
complexities of intellectual
the
development
As argued above,
this radi-
well be necessary to better under-
dynamics of
structural
the
now
intellectual
aging. Specifically, the designs and data-analytic
relatively loose couplings
age gradients. In
mechanics
late
and display different
senescence, decline in
limits pragmatic functioning, perhaps
The
due, at least in part, to terminal decline.
techniques described in this section are based on
onset and nature of this transition are likely to
the intensive longitudinal study of single indi-
differ
The degree of
viduals.
generalizability across
from person
in patterns
ties
to person.
General regulari-
of change are likely to exist
individuals can be assessed after patterns of
but can only be discerned with certainty after
change have been analyzed
intraindividual time-dependent structures have
level.
Nesselroade
1990; Nesseb-oade
and Molenaar strong
(e.g..
it
Molenaar
(e.g..
a
offers
intraindividual
Jones
&
Nesselroade,
& Schmidt McCollam,
advocates of
Because
at the intraindividual
to
to collect
several
many
variables
and individual-oriented perspective.
for
a
many times within may be to explore
of variables are related across time
single
individual;
may
that
is,
P-technique
be employed (variables over
Notes 1.
among
examine lagged
variables within persons (see
relations
Kim
&
Nesselroade, 2(X)3, for a recent application).
When vidual
the individual's structure of intraindi-
change has been clearly understood
without being contaminated by interindividual
studies
on
age have used verbal fact
we
join
those
(e.g., E.
Hunt, 2000) arguing for the use of a
wider range of pragmatic markers. 2.
to
in old
knowledge as a marker of the pragmatics. This
ment
analysis,
development
will be reflected in this chapter, although
com-
P-technique factor analysis, called dynamic
The overwhelming majority of
intellectual
bination of multivariate time-series analysis and
occasions). Molenaar (1985) suggested a
factor
age requires a multivariate, dynamic,
separate
Factor analysis
performed on the longitudinal data
factor analysis
intellectual develop-
approach.
clearly
in old
and interindividual differences,
individuals.
how groups
improved understanding of
ment
more widespread applications are desirable. Empirically, the main feature of this approach is
been observed, analyzed, and compared. Thus,
2003) are
et al..
general
this
way
2000)
In this chapter, in
we
discuss intellectual develop-
adulthood and old age from a psychometric
perspective. This perspective
is
complementary rather
than opposed to other perspectives, such as cognitivebehavioral
Chapter
1
(e.g., 1
,
childhood
this
Zacks. Hasher,
&
Li,
2000; see
volume, for an example concerning
development),
neurocognitive
Reuter-Lorenz. 2002), or neurophysiological Raz, 2000).
(e.g.. (e.g.,
216
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE
.
3.
Because any valid developmental stages are
both generation and person specific Smith, 2003), partitioning the
life
we
likely to
is
when nothing
a hopeless endeavor. Therefore, explicitly stated,
Baltes
&
span into substan-
based on chronological age
tive stages
is
(e.g..
be
else
use the labels young (ages
20-40). middle age (ages 40-60), young-old age (ages
Baltes.
B.. Lindenberger, U.,
P.
Life-span
(1998).
developmental
1029-1 143).
R
Baltes,
B.,
& Nesselroade.
rationale
replace chronological age with words.
Nesselroade
R.
J.
of longitudinal
&
(
P
Baltes.
A
L. (1996).
theory of adult intellectual
development: Process, personality,
and
interest,
knowledge. Intelligence, 22. 227-257.
Backman,
Laukka, E.
L.,
J.,
& Fratiglioni. L. (2002).
J..
Influences of preclinical
dementia and impending death on the magnitude of age-related cognitive deficits. Psychology and Aging. 17.
Backman,
435^W2.
&
L.,
memory
functioning across the adult
European Psychologist,
Backman,
L.,
(1996).
Small. B.
J..
1.
Semantic span.
life
27-33.
Wahlin, A.,
Larsson,
M.
(2000). Cognitive functioning in very old age. In
F
M. Craik
I.
&
T.
A. Salthouse (Eds.), The
handbook of aging and cognition (2nd ed., pp. 499-558). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
M.
Baer. D.
An
(1970).
age-irrelevant concept of
development. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly.
16,
B. (1987). Theoretical propositions of
P.
On
span developmental psychology: ics
J.
the
life-
dynam-
between growth and decline. Developmental
Psychology Baltes,
P.
23,
&
Willis. S. L. (1980). Integration versus
differentiation of tluid/crystallized intelligence in
old age. Developmental Psychology. Baltes.
P
B..
&
opment of
Labouvie. G. V. intellectual
explanation,
M.
P.
Lawton
(
(Eds.),
6,
625-635.
1973). Adult devel-
performance: Description,
modification.
opment and aging
In
C.
Eisdorfer
&
The psychology of adult de\'el-
(pp. 157-219).
Washington. DC:
P.
B..
&
Lindenberger, U. (1997). Emergency
of a powerful connection between sensory and cognitive functions across the adult
A
R.
J.
methods.
Monterey,
&
Smith.
young old
(2003).
J.
From
New
dilemmas of
to the
frontiers in the
successful aging of the the fourth age.
Gerontology 49. 123-135. Berg.
Aging, behavior, and terminal
(1996).
S.
&
E. Birren
J.
W. Schaie
K.
(Eds.),
Handbook of the psychology of aging (4th pp. 323-337). New York: Academic Press. L. R. (1998).
A
ed..
pattern-oriented approach
to studying individual
development: Snapshots
and processes. In R. B. Cairns, L. R. Bergman,
&
Kagan
J.
(Eds.),
Methods and models
for
.uudying the individual (pp. 83-121). Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Bergman.
L. R..
Magnusson.
D..
&
El-Khouri. B. M.
Studying individual development
(2003).
in
new window
to the
Psychology and Aging.
life
span:
study of cognitive aging? 12,
approach. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Block,
(1971). Lives through time. Berkeley,
J.
1
2-2
1
CA:
Bancroft. D.. Mellenbergh, G.
(2003).
The
J.,
&
van Heerden.
J.
theoretical status of latent variables.
Psychological Review, 110, 203-219.
&
Bosworth. H. B..
Schaie. K.
W.
(1999). Survival
effects in cognitive function, cognitive style,
sociodemographic Longitudinal
Study.
and
the
Seattle
Experimental
Aging
variables
in
Research. 25, 121-139.
Botwinick. J.
J.
E. Birren
(1977).
&
Intellectual
abilities.
K. W. Schaie (Eds.).
In
Handbook
of the psychology of aging (pp. 580-605).
American Psychological Association. Baltes.
B.,
Borsboom,
611-626.
B.. Cornelius. S. W.. Spiro. A.. Nesselroade,
R..
Nesselroade,
an interindividual context: A person-oriented
238-245. Baltes.
P
future of aging:
Bergman.
&
&
research
to
and develop-
York: Academic Press.
CA: Brooks/Cole.
decline. In
Nilsson, L.-G.
R.
J.
development psychology:
Life-span
Introduction
Baltes.
Wahlin. A.. Small, B.
New
Reese, H. W.,
B..
In
B. Baltes (Eds.), Longitudinal
P.
(pp. 1-39).
(1977). P.
York:
1979). Histor>' and
research.
research in the study of behavior
ment
Ackerman,
New
John Wiley.
60-80). and old age (age > 80) only as means to
References
Handbook of
(Ed.),
Theoretical models of
1.
(pp.
M.
Staudinger, U. in
Lemer
psychology. In R. M.
child psychology: Vol.
human development
&
theory
New Cattell.
York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.
R.
B.
(1952).
The
three
basic
factor-
analytic research designs: Their interrelations
and
derivatives.
499-520.
Psychological Bulletin, 49,
Development of Intellectual Abilities R.
Cattell,
(1971).
B.
Their
Abilities:
striutiire.
growth, and action. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Christensen. H. (2001).
What
cognitive changes can
be expected with normal ageing? Australian
and New Zealand Journal of
Psychiatry,
35,
768-775. Collins,
&
M..
L.
Sayer. A.
New
(2001).
(Eds.).
methods for the analysis of change. Washington,
DC: American Psychological Colsher,
P. L.,
& Wallace,
Association.
R. B. (1991). Longitudinal
defined population of community-dwelling
e\der^.
Deary,
J.,
I.
C. R.,
Annals of Epidemiology, Egan,
&
V.,
215-230.
1,
Gibson, G., Austin, E.
Kellaghen. T. (1996). Intelligence and
M. F.
R.
F.,
Zeger, S. L. (1994).
of longitudinal
Oxford University Elias.
&
Liang, K.-Y..
for assessing
the
interdependence of ageing-
A.,
Au,
R..
Horn,
L. (1982).
J.
A
intelligence
cohort.
J.
tests
Bulletin, 101,
F
M. Craik
I.
Ghisletta,
really
IQ gains
&
in 14 nations:
measure. Psychological
Horn,
A developmental theory of intel372-378.
1,
Lindenberger, U. (2003). Age-
in
the
Berlin Aging
Study: Direct evidence for ability dedifferentiation in old age.
Manuscript submitted for
publication.
&. Costa,
span changes
in
P T,
Jr.
(1995). Adult
B., life
immediate visual memory and
verbal intelligence. Psychology
and Aging,
10,
123-139.
Hambrick, D.
&
Engle, R. W. (2002). Effects of
domain knowledge, working memory and age on cognitive f)erfonnance: tion
of the knowledge
is
An
capacity, investiga-
power hypothesis.
Cognitive Psychology, 44, 339-387.
Hebb. D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior.
New
Horn,
Handbook of
York: John Wiley.
(2000).
L.
J.
New
York:
theory.
B. B.
In
Wolman
intelligence (pp. 267-300).
Comments on
integrating
924-930.
24,
Horn.
&
L.,
J.
and
R.
Cattell.
57,
Horn.
J.
(1966). Refinement
B.
of the theory of fluid and crystallized
test
of Educational Psychology,
253-270.
&
L.,
Cattell. R. B. (1967).
Age
differences
and crystallized intelligence. Acta
fluid
Psychologica, 26, 107-129.
C. Dixon, R. A., & Small, B. J. Memory change in the aged. New York:
Hultsch. D. F, Hertzog.
Cambridge University gence. Learning
Press.
for crystallized intelli-
it
and Individual
Difference, 12,
123-129. Hunt, W. L. (1949). The relative rates of decline of
Wechsler-Bellevue "hold" and "don't hold"
Journal of Consulting Psychology,
&
J.,
Nesselroade,
J.
tests.
440-443.
13,
R. (1990). Mulfi-
variate, replicated, single-subject, repeated
mea-
sures design and P-technique factor analysis:
A
review of intraindividual change studies.
Experimental Aging Research. Jones, H. E.,
Z.,
Aging and
York: John Wiley.
Jones, C.
Giambra, L. M., Arenberg, D., Zonderman, A.
Kawas, C,
(Eds.),
Remodeling old models of
Gf-Gc
Hunt, E. (2000). Let's hear
based structural dynamics between perceptual speed and knowledge
Trehub
S.
(1985).
L.
J.
intelligence:
(1998).
American Psychologist,
R,
&
cognitive processes (pp. 137-178).
in
171-191.
Garrett, H. E. (1946). ligence.
Psychological
134-140.
R. (1987). Massive
crystal-
concepts of
to
cognitive psychology and aging in adulthood. In
intelligence. Journal
W. K. (1956). The problem of inference from
What IQ
relation
in
and
fluid
Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research,
22-year
Framingham
curves based orb group data.
Flynn,
The theory of
White.
Archives of Neurology, 57, 808-813.
Bulletin, 53,
341-352.
related changes. Gerontology, 47,
person-centered and variable-centered research
D'Agostino, R. B. (2000). The preclin-
prospective study of the
Estes,
Under-
evaluation of research designs
on problems associated with the use of alcohol.
P
phase of Alzheimer's disease:
ical
(2001).
J.
An
York:
Press.
Beiser. A.. Wolf.
&
New
data.
M.
Sliwinski,
7-45.
7,
standing aging:
New
J.,
&
Hofer, S. M.,
105-132.
F
differences
individual
Research on Aging,
in gerontology.
(Ed.),
Analysis
217
•
perspective: Implications for cognitive research
the differentiation hypothesis. Intelligence, 23,
Diggle,
An
(1985).
Old Age
Plenum.
Brand,
J.,
C.
lized
application of cognitive function measures in a
Hertzog,
in
& Conrad, H.
16,
S. (1933).
decline of intelligence:
A
171-183.
The growth and
study of a homoge-
neous group between the ages of ten and Genetic Psychology Monographs,
Juan-Espinosa, Rebollo,
I..
M.,
Garcia,
Colom.
R..
L.
13,
F, Escorial.
& Abad. F
J.
(2002).
dedifferentiation hypothesis: Evidence
WAIS
III.
Intelligence, 30,
sixty.
223-275.
395-408.
S..
Age
from the
218
.
Kim,
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE
J. E.,
& Nesselroade,
among
R. (2003). Relationships
J.
and well-
social support, self-concept,
being of older adults:
dynamic
A
study of process using
factor models. International Journal of
Behavioral Development, 27. 49-65.
L.
&
Backman.
New
Nilsson (Eds.),
L.-G.
frontiers in cognitive aging. Oxford.
UK: Oxford
University Press.
Lindenberger, U., Singer.
&
T..
Longitudinal selectivity
R
Baltes.
B. (2002).
aging populations:
in
Kleemeier, R. W. (1962). Intellectual changes in the
Separating mortality-associated versus experi-
senium. Proceedings of the American Statistical
mental components in the Berlin Aging Study
Association,
290-295.
1,
Lemer, R. M., Dowling,
(BASE). Journal of Gerontology: Psychological
&
E.,
Lara Roth,
S. (2003).
Contributions of lifespan psychology
of developmental
elaboration
future
M. Staudinger
theory. In U.
&
to
the
systems
U. Lindenberger
Understanding human development:
(Eds.),
The
interplay
plasticity across levels:
of biology and culture
behavior across the
&
Baumert
A
computational exploration of the
between deterioration of neurotransmittor
systems and dedifferentiation of cognitive abihties in
old age. In L.-G. Nilsson
(Eds.),
&
H.
J.
Markowitsch
of memory
neuroscience
Cognitive
(pp. 103-146). Gottingen:
Hogrefe
&
Huber.
S.-C, Lindenberger, U., Hommel. B.. Aschersleben. G.. Prinz. W..
&
R
Baltes.
B. (2004). Lifespan
transformations in the couplings between intellectual
abilities
and
processes.
constituent
&
Schmiedek.
sarily aging:
F.
(2002).
Age
is
not neces-
Another step toward understanding
the "clocks" that time aging. Gerontology, 48,
Lindenberger. U. (2(X)1). Lifespan theories of cogni-
development. In N.
J.
Smelser
&R
&
Schnabel. K. U..
Little. T. D..
Baumert.
J.
(2000).
Modeling longitudinal and multilevel data.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Cognition
press).
(BASE): The psychology.
Lowe, C,
&
first
&
of the
ropsychology cal issues.
Lindenberger. U. (in
the
Aging Study
Berlin
Aging,
years.
ten
Neuro-
Cognition.
Rabbitt.
reliability
&
P..
in
R M.
A.. (1998). Test-re-test
CANTAB
ISPOCD
and
batteries: Theoretical
and
neu-
practi-
Neuropsychologia. 36. 915-923.
Magnusson. D. (1998). The
logic
and implication of a
person approach. In R. B. Cairns, L. R. Bergman.
&
J.
Kagan
(Eds.).
Methods and models for
studying the individual (pp. 33-63). Thousands
Maier, H.,
B. Baltes
& Smith, J. (1999).
of mortality
Psychological predictors
Journal of Gerontology:
in old age.
Psychological Sciences. 54B, P44-P54. J.
J.
(2001).
A
latent
score
difference
approach to longitudinal dynamic structural equation analyses. In R. Cudeck.
S. duToit,
&
of the social
D. Sorbom (Eds.). Structural equation modeling:
behavioral sciences (pp. 8848-8854). Oxford.
Present and future (pp. 342-380). Lincolnwood,
(Eds.). International encyclopedia
&
Mahwah, NJ:
(pp. 187-200).
Lawrence Erlbaum.
McArdle,
5-12.
tive
&
Modeling longitudinal and
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Psychological Science, 15, 155-163.
S.-C.
(Eds.).
Lovden. M.. Ghisletta.
Lindenberger. U. (1999). Cross-level
unification: link
Psychological
span.
life
171-194.
Bulletin. 129,
S.-C,
shaping the mind and
in
longitudinal
in
of continuers and
effects
dropouts. In T. D. Little. K. U. Schnabel,
multilevel data
York: Kluwer Academic.
opmental
Li.
the
New
of devel-
Li,
On
research:
Maier, H. (2000).
and generalizability
Selectivity
J.
Li. S.-C. (2003). Biocultural orchestration
&
Lindenberger, U.,
Little. T. D..
Dialogues with lifespan psychology (pp. 4 3^22). 1
Li,
Sciences, 57B, P474-P482.
UK:
IL: Scientific Software International.
Elsevier.
Lindenberger. U..
&
R
Baltes.
B. (1994). Sensory
functioning and intelligence in old age:
connection. Psychology
Lindenberger. U..
&
Baltes.
and Aging,
R
9,
A
strong
339-355.
B. (1997). Intellectual
functioning in old and very old age: Crosssectional results
from the Berlin Aging Study.
Psychology and Aging. Lindenberger. U..
12.
410-432.
& Ghisletta. P. (in press). Modeling
McArdle.
J.
J..
Ferrer-Caja. E..
Hamagami. F.
&
Woodcock. R. W. (2002). Comparative longitudinal structural analyses of the
of multiple intellectual
growth and decline
abilities
over the
life
span. Developmental Psychology. 38, 115-142.
McArdle,
J.
J..
&
Hamagami.
E
(2001).
Linear
dynamic analyses of incomplete longitudinal data. In L.
M.
Collins
&
A. Sayer (Eds.),
New
longitudinal changes in old age:
From covariance
methods for the analysis of change. Washington,
dynamic systems.
In R. A. Dixon,
DC: American Psychological
structures to
Association.
Development of Intellectual Abilities McArdle,
Hamagami. F. Meredith. W..
J.,
J.
Bradway, K.
(2000). Modeling
P.
&
dynamic
the
hypotheses of Gf-Gc theoiy using longitudinal
and Individual
life-span data. Learning
Differ-
ences. 12. 53-80.
&
M..
Mitrushina,
Nessekoade,
Stability
(1991).
P.
of
(2000).
Development. 24. 295-300.
Betula health,
M.
C.
P.
(1985).
A
dynamic
factor
model
of multivariate time series.
analysis
the
Psychometrica. 50. 181-202.
Molenaar, H.
Ader
J.
M.
C.
P.
Mellenbergh (Eds.). Research
in the social, behavioral,
and
life
sciences (pp. 143-167). London: Sage.
Molenaar. J.
C. M.. Huizenga. H. N..
P.
R.
The
(2003).
A
variability:
&
U. M. Staudinger
Emgrund,
L.,
et
cohort
prospective
K..
Nyberg,
study:
L..
The
(1997).
al.
Memory,
and aging. Aging, Neuropsychology, and
Cognition,
Nyberg,
1-32.
4.
Maitland, S. B., Ronnlund. M., Backman, L.,
(2003). Selective
et al.
adult age differences in an age-invariant multifactor
model of declarative memory. Psychology
and Aging.
C,
Park, D.
149-160.
18,
Lautenschlager, G., Hedden. T, Davidson,
&
R
Nesselroade,
N.
between the
Models of visuospatial and verbal memory
and intraindividual
theoretical and empirical vindica-
developmental
of
tion
&
relationship
structure of interindividual
L..
Dixon, R. A., Wahlin. A.,
(1999). Longitudinal analysis. In
& G. J.
methodology
Backman.
Adolfsson, R., Bucht. G.,
individuals. Brain Dysfunction, 4, 174-181.
for
Schmidt McCollam, K. M.
processes. International Journal of Behavioral
cognitive functions in young-old versus old-old
Molenaar.
219
•
Putting the process in developmental
Nilsson, L.-G..
Satz.
&
R.,
J.
Old Age
in
systems
theory.
In
Smith, A. D.,
S..
across the adult
life
Smith.
K. (2002).
span. Psychology
and Aging.
299-320.
17.
R M. A.,
Rabbitt,
Diggle, R, Smith, D., Holland,
P.,
&
Lindenberger (Eds.),
Mclnnes, L. (2001). Identifying and separating
Understanding human development: Dialogues
the effects of practice and of cognitive ageing
with
New
U.
psychology
lifespan
(pp. 339-360).
Muthen, B. (2001). Second-generation
structural
equation modeling with a combination of cate-
and continuous
gorical
opportunities
for
latent
class/latent
&
New
variables:
latent
modeling. In L. M. Collins
growth
A. Sayer (Eds.).
methods for the analysis of change Washington, DC: American
A^^K-
(pp. 291-322).
&
B..
Muthen. L. K. (2000). Integrating
person-centered and variable-centered analyses:
Growth mixture modeling with classes. Alcoholism: Clinical
latent trajectory
and Experimental
Research. 24, 882-891. Neisser, U. (Ed.). (1998).
gains
in
IQ and
The
related measures. Washington,
DC: American Psychological Nesselroade,
J.
comes of
R., &. Ford.
Neuropsychologia,
Raudenbush,
work
S.
W.
(2001).
comparing
for
change. In L.
M.
Toward
39,
a coherent frame-
trajectories of individual
& A.
Collins
Sayer (Eds.),
methods for the analysis of change
DC:
Washington,
American
New
(pp. 35-64).
Psychological
Association. J.
(2000).
Change and
The Raven's Progressive Matrices: stability
over cultures and time.
Cognitive Psychology, 41, 1-48.
Raz, N. (2000). Aging of the brain and
its
impact on
cognitive performance: Integration of structural
and functional findings. rising cun'e: Long-tertn
study of elderly
longitudinal
residents.
532-543.
Raven,
Psychological Association.
Muthen.
during a large
community
York: Kluwer Academic.
T.
In F.
I.
M. Craik
&
A. Salthouse (Eds.), The handbook of aging
and cognition (2nd
ed., pp. 1-90).
Mahwah,
NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Association.
D. H. (1985). P-technique
age: Multivariate, replicated, single-
Reinert,
G. (1970). Comparative factor analytic
studies of intelligence throughout the life span.
&P
Bakes
subject designs for research on older adults.
In L. R. Goulet
Research on Aging.
developmental psychology: Research and theory
Nesselroade.
J.
R..
&
7.
46-80.
Molenaar,
R
C.
M.
(1999).
Pooling lagged covariance structures based on short,
multivariate
dynamic factor
time-series
analysis
for
H.
Hoyle
(Ed.). Statistical strategies for small
sample
analysis.
research (pp. 223-250).
Sage.
In
R.
Newbury
Park.
CA:
(pp.
476-484).
Reuter-Lorenz,
mind and
P.
New
B.
(Eds.), Life-span
York: Academic Press.
A. (2002).
New
visions of the aging
brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 6,
394^00. Richelle,
M.
news on
(1993).
Du nouveau
sur I'esprit? [Any
the mind?]. Paris: Presses Universitaires
de France.
220
HANDBOOK OF UNDERSTANDING AND MEASURING INTELLIGENCE
.
&
Riegel, K. F.
Riegel, R.
M.
(1972). Development,
drop, and death. Developmental Psychology,
6,
306-319. L..
Backman,
&
L..
Nilsson,
and decline
L.-G. (2003). Stability, improvement,
adult life-span development of declarative
memory: Cross-sectional and longitudinal data
from a population-based
Manuscript
study.
Salthouse, T. A. (1991). Theoretical perspectives on cognitive aging. Hillsdale. NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Salthouse, T. A. (2000). Methodological assumptions
aging research. In
in cognitive
F.
1.
M. Craik
&
A. Salthouse (Eds.). The handbook of aging
and cognition (2nd
467^98). Mahwah.
ed., pp.
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Salthouse. T.
A.
(2003).
knowledge,
&
of aging,
Interrelations
performance.
In
Lindenberger (Eds.).
U.
Understanding human development: Dialogues
W.
L.
(1988).
of adult
studies
M.
(pp.
265-288).
York: Kluwer Academic.
K.
Schaie,
psychology
lifespan
New
Howe
Internal
cognitive
threats
validity
development.
in
In
C.
in
adulthood: Progress in cognitive
Brainard (Eds.), Cognitive
J.
development research (pp. 241-272).
New
York:
(1994).
development.
Intellectual
304-313.
development
in
Willis.
B..
L..
S.
&
(
&
C.
Botwinick.
J.
and Aging, (1979).
A
13,
8-20.
long-term
The matter of
selective subject attrition.
Journal of Gerontology, 34. 242-245.
in
T, Verhaeghen. R.
&
Baltes.
R
Ghisletta.
in
Psychological Science,
Small. B.
8,
to death
168-172. Winblad.
Fratiglioni, L.. Viitanen, M..
J..
Backman.
L. (2000).
impairment
B..
The course of cognitive Alzheimer's disease:
in preclinical
and 6-year follow-up of a population-based
3-
sample. Archives of Neurology, 57, 839-844. Small,
B.
Fratiglioni,
J.,
Backman.
performance
nitive
von Strauss,
L.,
E.,
&
and cog-
L. (2003). Terminal decline
very old age: Does cause
in
18,
193-202.
Spearman. C. (1927). The
abilities
of man. London:
Macmillan. Tetens,
N. (1777). Philosphische versuche iiber die
J.
menschliche
und
natur
and
development].
its
entwicklung
ihre
human
Leipzig:
nature
Weidmanns
Erben und Reich. Thaler.
D.
Design of an aging
(2002).
S.
brain.
Neurobiology of Aging, 23, 13-15.
Thomdike. R.
L. (1977). Causation of Binet
14,
Valsiner,
IQ decre-
197-202. J.
(1984).
frameworks
in
Two
alternative epistemological
psychology: The typological and
modes of
variational
5.
&
Verhaeghen. R.
thinking. Journal of
Mind
449^70.
Salthouse. T. A. (1997). Meta-
B. (2003).
The
the Berlin
Stewart.
W.
(1996).
The
B..
in
aging.
in
cross-classification.
Wechsler. D. (1955).
Buschke.
H..
&
effects of preclinical
Journal of Gerontology:
Psychological Sciences. 5 IB. P217-P225.
to
configural
frequency analysis: The search for types and
fate of cognition
dementia on estimates of normal cognitive functioning
von Eye. A. (1990). Introduction
Intelligence
Cambridge,
University Press.
Manual for
New
Scale.
the Wechsler Adult
York:
The Psycho-
logical Corporation.
Wechsler. R.
models. Psychological Bulletin.
UK: Cambridge
Aging, 18. 318-331. Lipton.
structural
122. 231-249.
antitypes
Aging Study (BASE). Psychology and
M.,
and
Lindenberger.
P..
very old age: Six-year longitudinal findings in
Sliwinski,
Time
and cognitive performance. Current Directions
Estimates of linear and non-linear age effects
C.
longitudinal study of intellectual ability of older
U.,
309-313.
12.
L. (1999).
1998). Longitudinal invariance of
adults:
Singer.
a
adults.
psychometric ability factor structures
across 7 years. P.sychology I.
from
old
analyses of age-cognition relations in adulthood:
Intrieri, R.
adult
Backman.
and Behavior
York: Cambridge University Press.
Schaie. K. W.. Maitland. S.
Siegler.
adult intellectual
The Seattle Longitudinal Study.
adulthood:
New
The course of
/\/?7fnca/j Ps}chologi.